Exploration behaviour and behavioural flexibility in orb
Transcription
Exploration behaviour and behavioural flexibility in orb
Current Zoology 61 (2): 313–327, 2015 Exploration behaviour and behavioural flexibility in orb-web spiders: A review Thomas HESSELBERG* Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1 3PS, UK Abstract Orb-web spiders and their webs constitute an ideal model system in which to study behavioural flexibility and spatial cognition in invertebrates due to the easily quantifiable nature of the orb web. A large number of studies demonstrate how spiders are able to modify the geometry of their webs in response to a range of different conditions including the ability to adapt their webs to spatial constraints. However, the mechanisms behind this impressive web-building flexibility in these cognitively limited animals remain poorly explored. One possible mechanism though may be spatial learning during the spiders’ exploration of their immediate surroundings. This review discusses the importance of exploration behaviour, the reliance on simple behavioural rules, and the use of already laid threads as guidelines for web-building in orb-web spiders. The focus is on the spiders’ ability to detect and adapt their webs to space limitations and other spatial disruptions. I will also review the few published studies on how spatial information is gathered during the exploration phase and discuss the possibility of the use of ‘cognitive map’-like processes in spiders. Finally, the review provides suggestions for designing experimental studies to shed light on whether spiders gather metric information during the site exploration (cognitive map hypothesis) or rely on more simple binary information in combination with previously laid threads to build their webs (stigmergy hypothesis) [Current Zoology 61 (2): 313–327, 2015]. Keywords 1 Animal cognition, Spider web-building, Stigmergy, Cognitive maps, Exploration behaviour, Behavioural plasticity Introduction 1.1 Spatial cognition and cognitive maps The ability to sense and gain knowledge of the local physical environment is a requirement for survival in most animal species. The immediate surroundings provide a source of food, mating opportunities and other resources such as shelter, building materials and direction cues, but is also where predators might arrive from. Thus formation of a coherent picture of the local habitat is adaptive for a wide range of animals. Spatial cognition is defined as the ability to gather and learn spatial information of directions and distances. However, our understanding of spatial cognition remains poorly explored, although the processing of spatial information and learning appears to have different computational requirements than temporal associative learning (Shettleworth, 2010). Most of what we know about spatial cognition relates to navigation and orientation. Much orientation and short-distance navigation is based on less cognitive demanding abilities such as auditory, light, chemical and pheromone gradients, path integration and ideothetic memory, while long-distance navigation requires the ability to integrate, and prioritise among, Received Nov. 1, 2014; accepted Feb. 5, 2015. Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] © 2015 Current Zoology multiple cues and abilities such as the use of visual landmarks, polarised light and magnetic gradients in addition to the above mentioned abilities (Mouritsen, 2001; Frost and Mouritsen, 2006; Shettleworth, 2010). However, the most computational demanding, but also most controversial, aspect of spatial cognition is the ability to form a map-like representation of the environment. This concept is known as cognitive maps, which are defined as the ability to learn metric properties of space, i.e. distances and directions, and perform mental operations on the learned properties to follow novel routes to well-known locations (Tolman, 1948; Shettleworth, 2010). The concept of cognitive maps was first defined by Tolman (1948), who studied rats’ problem solving ability in mazes. He found that they learn and store spatial relationships of their surroundings in a field map, which is distinct from, and more complicated than, forming simple sequential stimulus response connections of the correct path through a maze. Since his seminal studies, cognitive maps and the ability to remember relations between geometrical landmarks have been found in a number of visually orientating vertebrates (see Tommasi et al., 2012 for a review). The idea that invertebrates 314 Current Zoology possess a cognitive map that allow them to judge distances and directions between geographically separate points has been discussed for some time particular in relation to navigation in social insects. Several authors have argued that honey bees are capable of making novel short cuts in a familiar habitat and thus may possess a spatial memory of their surroundings akin to a cognitive map based on previous exploratory flight experience (Gould, 1986; Menzel et al., 2005). However, another theoretical study on neural networks in desert ants suggests that such novel short cuts can be explained by separate decentralised memory subsystems relying on landmark guidance and path integration without the need to postulate a centralised spatial memory system that relies on cognitive maps (Cruse and Wehner, 2011). In contrast, a very recent study found that honey bees with shifted sun-compass caused by anesthesia readily orient towards home (Cheeseman et al., 2014a), which the authors interpret as a proof of the honeybees’ possession of cognitive maps, although their interpretation has been questioned (Cheung et al., 2014; Cheeseman et al., 2014b). Thus despite a relatively large number of studies, there is still no clear consensus as to whether insects possess a cognitive map or not. We know even less about spatial cognition in other invertebrates although spiny lobsters are proposed to employ a magnetic map for navigation (Boles and Lohmann, 2003). Spiders usually do not need to make longdistance journeys to and from fixed locations and thus do not engage in complicated navigation tasks with a few exceptions; the jumping spider Portia fimbriata is capable of making detours that temporarily involves it moving away from the target prey (Tarsitano and Jackson, 1992; Jackson and Pollard, 1996) and the males of the desert huntsman spider Leucorchestris arenicola make long nocturnal journeys searching for females while usually returning to their burrows in a straight line after each journey probably by relying on a combination of path integration and local cues (Nørgaard et al., 2003; 2007). Nonetheless navigation is not the only task where possessing a cognitive map might be advantageous. Web-building spiders assess the suitability of a site for their webs based on prior exploration (Vollrath, 1992). 1.2 Web-building behaviour and orb-web spiders Web-building spiders in general and orb-web spiders in particular are excellent model organisms for studies of cognitive abilities in terrestrial non-insect invertebrates since they leave a physical record, i.e. the web, of their foraging behaviour (Vollrath and Selden, 2007). Vol. 61 No. 2 Orb webs are furthermore highly regular geometric twodimensional structures, which make them easily quantifiable. The typical orb web consists of a hub from which radii radiate outwards towards the frame which is connected to the surrounding vegetation via anchor threads (Fig. 1). Enclosed within the frame and connected to the radii is the sticky capture spiral which functions to trap intercepted prey long enough for the spider to rush out from the hub to catch them (Eberhard, 1990a). The spiders are usually resting motionless in the hub or on nearby vegetation while connected to the hub through a signal thread from where they will initiate prey attack behaviour when vibratory signals from the struggling prey reach the hub via the radii (Masters et al., 1986; Landolfa and Barth, 1996). In most webs, the hub is not located in the exact centre of the web, but usually displaced upwards to give asymmetric webs with a larger lower part reflecting the fact that spiders, due to gravity, can run faster downwards than upwards (Fig. 1; ap Rhiziart and Vollrath, 1994; Zschokke and Nakata, 2010). Almost 4,500 species of orb-web spiders are known from all continents, except Antarctica, belonging to 7 families (Anapidae, Araneidae, Nephilidae, Symphytognathidae, Theridisomatidae, Tetragnathidae and Uloboridae). Some spiders include a missing sector in the corner of the web, where a signal thread runs from the hub to the spider’s retreat in the nearby vegetation, while many spiders that always reside in the hub include web Fig. 1 An orb web of Cyclosa caroli digitised from a photograph The figure shows the anchor threads (Anc) that connects the web to the surrounding vegetation, the radii (Rad) radiate outwards from the hub in the centre of the web until they reach the frame (Fr). The sticky capture spiral (Cap) is connected to the radii. HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders decorations made of silk or prey remains and debris primarily used to camouflage the spiders or attract prey (Herberstein et al., 2000a; Bruce, 2006; Nakata, 2009; Walter and Elgar, 2012). Further advantages of using orb-web spiders as model organism for cognitive studies include that most orb-web spiders readily built webs in frames under laboratory conditions (Zschokke and Herberstein, 2005) and that they are highly motivated to build webs even under adverse physiological and environmental conditions such as when missing legs (Reed et al., 1965; Pasquet et al., 2011), when under the influence of pesticides and neurotoxic drugs (Witt, 1971; Samu and Vollrath, 1992; Hesselberg and Vollrath, 2004; Benamú et al., 2010; 2013), when exposed to space limitations (Ades, 1986; Vollrath et al., 1997; Barrantes and Eberhard, 2012; Hesselberg, 2013) and when exposed to weightlessness in space (Witt et al., 1977). Once the site has been found to be suitable, orb-web spiders construct their webs in four more or less stereotypical phases. In phases I and II, which is covered in more detail later, they identify the location of the web, build the radii, frame and anchor threads. In phase III they construct the auxiliary spiral (also known as the temporary or non-sticky spiral) that starts at the hub and logarithmically spirals outwards, where it mechanically stabilises the scaffolding structure of the web and, among other cues, acts as a guideline for the construction of the sticky spiral (Peters, 1970; Zschokke, 1993; 2011; Eberhard and Hesselberg, 2012). The auxiliary spiral is usually removed during construction of the sticky spiral although some members of the Nephilidae family retain it in the intact web, presumably to strengthen the web (Kuntner and Agnarsson, 2009; Hesselberg and Vollrath, 2012). In contrast, miniature orb-web spiders in the Anapidae family do not build auxiliary spirals at all (Eberhard, 1987a). Phase IV takes the longest for the spider to finish and is where the geometric capture spiral is constructed. The spider starts from the near the frame and build towards the hub attaching each spiral loop to every radius that it crosses. The capture spiral is built as a geometric spiral ensuring that the distance between the spiral loops is more or less regular across the web (although see Eberhard, 2014). Numerous studies have investigated web-building behaviour and the geometry of the completed web in detail under a wide variety of environmental and physiological conditions revealing a surprisingly degree of behavioural flexibility as we shall see in the next section. However, we know next to nothing about the exploration behaviour preceding the onset of the web-buil- 315 ding behaviour. In this paper, I first review some examples of remarkable cognitive abilities in orb-web spiders including behavioural flexibility and learning. In the following section I go into details with what we know about the exploration behaviour and the early web building behaviour while discussing the implications for spatial cognition in orb-web spiders. The review builds on Vollrath’s excellent review paper (Vollrath, 1992) on exploration and web-building behaviour, which first discussed the possibility that orb-web spiders may construct spatial maps. I therefore focus especially on studies conducted after 1992 as well as linking spatial cognition more explicitly to neuroethology and behavioural flexibility. In the final section, I introduce two extreme hypotheses in terms of cognitive demand; the stigmergy and the cognitive map hypotheses, discuss and evaluate their predictions, present some preliminary data and suggest further experiments that might shed light on the type of information obtained by the spiders during their exploration behaviour. 2 Cognition, Behavioural Flexibility and Spatial Constraints 2.1 The spider brain and behavioural limitations Given the current and historic fascination of spider webs and spider web-building behaviour from both scientists and lay people, we know surprisingly little about the underlying cognitive processes. The central nervous system of orb-web spiders, like that of all spiders, consists of two compact ganglia; the subesophageal and the supraesophageal ganglion (Foelix, 2011). The subesophageal ganglion, which as the name implies is located below the esophagus, is the main centre for linking sensory input to motor output and consists of the fused ganglia of the appendages. The supraesophageal ganglion, also referred to as the ‘brain’, is the most complex part of the central nervous system and consists of several different structures including the protocerebrum, which contains the central body, and the optic nerves (Babu, 1975; Park et al., 2013). As the supraesophageal ganglion receives sensory input from the eyes, it is most highly developed in spiders with good vision such as jumping spiders and wolf spiders (Foelix, 2011). Orbweb spiders on the other hand have poor vision and can build perfectly normal orb webs in total darkness and instead primarily rely on mechanosensory cues for building their webs and capturing their prey (Spronk, 1935; Land, 1985; Foelix, 2011). Although their supraesophageal ganglion is not as complex as in some other spiders, it has (and the central body in particular) none- 316 Current Zoology theless attracted much attention in connection with webbuilding behaviour. Volumetric measurements, however, failed to find any differences in relative size of the central body between the orb-web spider Nephila clavipes and three web-less spiders (Weltzien and Barth, 1991). Nonetheless, the central body remains at present the most likely candidate for the brain structure that governs web-building behaviour (Park et al., 2013). This is supported by older studies on the orb-web spider Araneus diadematus displaying disruption to normal webbuilding behaviour after its protocerebrum was damaged with laser light (Witt and Reed, 1964; Witt, 1969). Since there appears to be no large differences between the central nervous system of adult web-building and web-less spiders, perhaps the cognitive demands of web-building can instead best be explored by comparing the webs of early instar and adult spiders. The size limitation hypothesis proposes that early instar juveniles, which can be up to 1,000 times smaller than the adults (Eberhard, 2011; Hesselberg, 2010), should, due to their resultant miniature brains, be less precise, more behaviourally limited or commit more errors during webbuilding than adults. However, three experimental studies comparing early instar juveniles and adults of four species of orb-web spiders fail to provide any support for this hypothesis (Eberhard, 2007; Hesselberg, 2010; Eberhard, 2011). Although this can partly be explained by early instar spiders enlarging their central nervous system by expanding it into their legs (Quesada et al., 2011), the failure to find any kind of behavioural differences, and the findings that other non-web-building kleptoparasitic spiders show a similar brain enlargement in early instars (Quesada et al., 2011) supports the conclusion from the neurophysiological studies that webbuilding behaviour is achieved by following relatively simple rules based on guidance from previously laid threads (Peters, 1939; Zschokke, 1993; 2011; Eberhard and Hesselberg, 2012). This is further supported by computer models of web-building behaviour that while following a few simple rules successfully simulate completed webs built by both normal spiders (Krink and Vollrath, 1998) and by spiders with shorter regenerated front legs (Krink and Vollrath, 1999). 2.2 Learning and memory As we saw above, juvenile spiders do not commit more errors than adult spiders, but do early instar spiders build perfect webs from the start or do they improve their web-building behaviour through practice? The acquisition of complex motor skills through experience is well known from humans and other vertebrates, but Vol. 61 No. 2 there is also growing evidence that motor learning and plasticity may be widespread in invertebrates (Chittka and Niven, 2009; Wolf et al., 1992). Bees for instance learn specific motor sequences while foraging in a complex artificial environment (Collett et al., 1993) and fruit flies, without prior flight experience, show less precise steering control compared to more experienced flies (Hesselberg and Lehmann, 2009). However, a study of spider web-building behaviour revealed no differences in web geometry between inexperienced and experienced spiders (Reed et al., 1970). In general orbweb spiders do not seem to improve their web-building skills with experience and show no major differences in web geometry throughout life (Pasquet et al., 2014), except that old spiders build webs that contain more errors (Anotaux et al., 2012). However, some minor differences are found in that older heavier, spiders build more vertically asymmetric webs, although this difference is likely caused by older spiders also being heavier (Herberstein and Heiling, 1999; Venner et al., 2013; Hesselberg, 2010; Nakata, 2010; Kuntner et al., 2010; Gregoric, 2013) and that early instar juveniles of species that as adults include a free sector often build webs without a free sector (Hesselberg, 2010). Although webbuilding behaviour in spiders appears to be innate, this does not mean that they do not show learning in other aspects. The golden orb-web spider Nephila clavipes has recently been shown to remember the amount (and possibly the number) of recently caught and stored prey (Rodríguez et al., 2013; 2014), the trashline orb-web spider Cyclosa octotuberculata learns the spatial location of recently caught prey in the web and selectively pulls radii in that sector (Nakata, 2013), and the missing sector orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata has been shown to rely on memory of the direction to its retreat when returning from excursions onto the web (LeGuelte, 1969). Evidence furthermore suggests that spiders rely on memory of distances travelled and the location of previously laid spiral threads during construction of the auxiliary and capture spirals (Eberhard, 1988a; Eberhard and Hesselberg, 2012). Finally, orb-web spiders modify the geometry of their webs based on the type and size of previously caught prey (Schneider and Vollrath, 1998; Venner et al., 2000; Tso et al., 2007; Blamires et al., 2011) and also on the presence of potential prey in their environment (Pasquet et al., 1994). 2.3 Behavioural flexibility and cognitive abilities Notwithstanding the examples of learning discussed above, one of the orb-web spiders’ most impressive cognitive abilities is probably their extensive beha- HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders vioural flexibility when it comes to the size, shape and geometry of their webs. A very wide range of environmental and physiological factors affect the completed orb webs, but we need to separate factors that limit the spider’s ability to construct normal webs (i.e. factors that induce errors) from factors that result in adaptive changes to the web as a result of behavioural flexibility (i.e. factors that result in modified webs that are better adapted to a given situation). However, it is not always easy to make this distinction. While some changes to orb web geometry are clearly non-adaptive such as those caused by neurotoxins and pesticides (Witt, 1971; Samu and Vollrath, 1992; Hesselberg and Vollrath, 2004; Benamú et al., 2010; 2013), by old age (Anotaux et al., 2012) and by ectoparasites (Eberhard, 2000; 2010; Gonzago and Sobzcak, 2011), others are less straightforward. Rotating the frame while spiders are building their webs, for example, results in suboptimal webs probably due to disruption to the gravitational cue (Vollrath, 1986; 1988), but at higher rotations webs become more normal suggesting that the spider shifts to rely on centripetal forces instead which confirms the findings of earlier studies that spiders in space apparently can modify their use of gravitational cues (Witt et al., 1977). Similarly humidity seems to influence the orb web by reducing its size and minimising the distance between spiral turns at lower humidity (Vollrath et al., 1997), which could be an adaptation to the reduced stickiness of the capture spiral threads at lower humidity (Edmonds and Vollrath, 1992; Opell et al., 2011). Losing one or several legs also affect geometry of the web (Pasquet et al. 2011), but this constitute a more obvious example of adaptation, since all legs normally play a Fig. 2 317 role during web-building (Reed et al., 1965; Vollrath, 1987; Eberhard, 1990b; 2012), so the construction of functional webs, even when missing 2‒4 legs (T. Hesselberg, Pers. Obs.), suggests a high degree of plasticity in the construction rules. Clear examples of behavioural flexibility additionally include the matching of size and geometry to the available silk supply (Eberhard, 1988b), the increase in the distance between capture spiral turns at lower temperatures to decrease web-building time and/or to better target the larger insects still active (Vollrath et al., 1997) and the decrease in web size and overall thread length and increase in stiffness and distance between spiral turns that reduces drag in wind-exposed webs (Vollrath et al., 1997; Liao et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2013). One of the most striking examples of adaptive behavioural flexibility, however, is some orb-web spiders’ ability to adapt the size and shape of their webs to spatial constraints in the space available for building their webs in (Fig. 2). Some, but not all, orb-web spiders readily build webs in limited spaces with less than a fourth of the area of their normal webs in the field (Vollrath et al., 1997; Hesselberg, 2013), with Leucauge argyra even building in containers that allow only webs less than a tenth of the size in the field (Barrantes and Eberhard, 2012), while also vertically or horizontally elongating the shape of their webs to match the available space (Vollrath et al., 1997; Krink and Vollrath, 2000; Harmer and Herberstein, 2009; Harmer et al., 2012; Hesselberg, 2013). Interestingly, though this and the previously described examples of web-building flexibility all appear to be examples of activational behavioural plasticity, which is defined as an immediate response that relies on Webs built by Eustala illicita in experimental frames in the laboratory The highlighted black spirals show the location and shape of the auxiliary spiral reconstructed from silk remnants left in the completed web. A. Webs built in control frames show the normal shape and geometry. B. Webs built in vertical frames show significantly vertically elongated webs. C. Webs built in horizontal frames show significantly horizontally elongated webs. D. Webs built in small square frames show smaller but normal shaped and slightly compressed webs. E. The size of the experimental Perspex frames in which the spiders built their webs. All measurements are in cm and all frames were 5 cm wide. The white horizontal scale bars represent a length of 5 cm. Figure from Hesselberg (2013) with kind permission from Springer Science+Business Media. 318 Current Zoology pre-configured neural pathways rather than developmental behavioural plasticity that relies on modification of neural pathways through learning from experience (Snell-Rood, 2013; Hesselberg, 2014). Thus spiders tested in the experimental frames in Fig. 2 showed an immediate adaptation to the frames and did not improve their shape or area utilisation in subsequent webs build in the same frames (Hesselberg, 2014). 3 Exploration Behaviour, Construction of Attachments Points and Anchor Threads 3.1 Exploration behaviour In the previous section we learned that the present evidence supports the notion that despite the structural complexity of orb webs, their construction requires no more cognitive abilities than any other foraging behaviour and possibly significantly less than other specialised invertebrate behaviours. Therefore in order to explore the cognitive demands of web-building and search for the potential use of cognitive maps, we should instead focus on the behaviour that precedes the more stereotypical radial and spiral construction behaviour. As mentioned in the introduction we can divide the web-building behaviour into four more or less distinct stages (exploration, frame and radii construction, temporary spiral construction and capture spiral construction). The exploration behaviour stage is the one we know the least about and the little we do know comes from laboratory studies, where webs are built in a much more limited and structurally simpler space than in nature (Eberhard, 1972; Zschokke, 1996). The function of site exploration is probably to assess if the site contains large enough unobstructed space for the construction of a web and to determine the potential attachment points of the anchor threads that connects the finished web with the vegetation (Ebehard, 1972; Vollrath, 1992). Orbweb spiders have very poor eyesight (Spronk, 1935; Land, 1985; Foelix, 2011), so such as an assessment must rely on other sensory cababilities. There is some evidence that spiders associated with specific plant species could use chemical signals to determine the suitability of a site (Krell and Krämer, 1998; Romero et al., 2008; Hesselberg and Triana, 2010; Styrsky, 2014) and spiders may also assess the quality in terms of foraging potential by picking up vibrations transmitted through the air by flying insects (Pasquet et al., 1994; Herberstein et al., 2000b). However, in order determine the suitability of the space for constructing a web, the spiders Vol. 61 No. 2 need to physically explore the vegetation. In nature spiders probably rely on a combination of walking around and using airborne lines to traverse gaps in the vegetation (Vollrath, 1992; Foelix, 2011). Spiders release silk into the air from their spinnerets by raising their abdomen or by descending a silk thread, which ensures that wind will carry the light and fluffy threads to nearby vegetation (Eberhard, 1987b; Peters, 1990). Once the airborne thread has attached itself the spider will tighten the thread and walk across it while replacing it with stronger silk. In the absence of wind in the laboratory (and with the much smaller distances involved), spiders move around the edges of the frame of the cage continuously trailing silk threads (Fig. 3; Eberhard, 1972; Vollrath, 1992; Zschokke, 1996). Spiders building vertical webs will then often move along horizontal threads in the upper part of the frame while occasionally descending vertically until they reach the ground upon which they double back (Fig. 3; Eberhard, 1990b; Zschokke, 1996). The function of this behaviour could be to ensure that the space is free of obstacles and is therefore probably also found during web-building in nature. The problem with observing exploration behaviour in the wild is that it often takes place several hours before the actual web construction starts and the exploration behaviour often consists of short periods of activity followed by long periods of inactivity (Eberhard, 1972; 1990b) making it virtually impossible to recognise it as exploration behaviour before actual webbuilding commences. However, an exception is a study on the complete web-building sequence in the wild of the Darwin’s bark spider (Gregorič et al., 2011). The Darwin’s bark spider builds large webs spanning rivers with anchor thread lengths often exceeding 10 m. It crosses the river by releasing silk threads into the wind as described above, but interestingly once it has established a silk thread across the river, it builds its web directly underneath this initial thread over the middle of the river with only one further long anchor thread below the web attaching it to one of the river banks (Gregorič et al., 2011). This species does not appear to perform any further site exploration once the initial river spanning silk thread is established. The Darwin bark’s spider probably gets away with drastically reducing the exploration stage since spaces above water bodies are usually free of any obstacles and since the use of only three anchor points automatically ensures that the web is built in one plane (Gregorič et al., 2011). In general, it is important to note that although site exploration only takes place when spiders relocate to a new location or com- HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders pletely lose their webs due to wind or other disturbances (Zschokke, 1996; Wherry, 2009), the fitness consequences of selecting a high-quality web site are so significant that we would expect natural selection to have optimised this behaviour. Usually spiders take down their old webs by ingesting the silk threads and recycling the proteins before rebuilding a new web at the same site without displaying exploration behaviour and reusing at least some of the anchor threads from the previous web (Breed et al., 1954; Peakall, 1971; Zschokke, 1996; Zschokke and Vollrath, 2000). 3.2 Early stages of web construction Once the site is found acceptable for web-building (the exact mechanism for how the spider does this is unknown, but I will discuss a few potential mechanisms in the next section), the spider starts, after a period of time, to construct the web. During exploration the spider, by trailing its dragline, has left some threads crisscrossing the free space and the spider uses the intersection of these threads as a proto-hub to add radii from (Zschokke, 1996). This first simple structure sometimes takes the form of a Y (Peters, 1937; 1939; Foelix, 2011), but can also consists of 4-7 protoradii running from the surrounding vegetation or structure and meeting in the proto-hub (Zschokke and Vollrath, 1995; Zschokke, 1996). The spider then starts to build the frame in the upper part of the web sometimes removing previous threads and constructing new anchor threads before it moves the proto-hub by cutting and reattaching the proto-radii into a new (usually more central) position that then becomes the hub (Eberhard, 1990b; Zschokke and Vollrath, 1995; Zschokke, 1996). This is followed by the addition of a few more radii in different sections of the web as well as simultaneously finishing the construction of the frame and finalising the position of the Fig. 3 The tracks of an Araneus diadematus spider showing the exploration behaviour of two subsequent webs built in the laboratory Tracks were recorded with computer-automated tracking software. In the first web (A) the spider spent less time exploring than during the second web (B). The vertical descents are visible in both figures, but especially during the exploration prior to the second web (B). Figure from Zschokke (1996) with kind permission from the Museum of Natural History in Geneva and the Swiss Zoological Society. 319 anchor threads. During this process the spider attempts to keep the structure in one plane by usually, although not always, attaching the anchor threads within the same plane (Zschokke and Vollrath, 2000). It is important to keep the web as much as possible in one plane, since this ensures the largest possible projection area (and thus effective capture area) per web area and distribute the forces from prey impact and wind more equally (Wainwright et al., 1976; Denny, 1976; Lin et al., 1995). After the frame has been completed the spider builds the remaining radii by identifying large gaps between existing radii and then moving out along an existing radius (the exit radius) and some distance down the frame where it attaches the trailing dragline to the frame and moves along this new radius back to the hub (Eberhard, 1990; Zschokke, 1996). A preliminary study suggests that the exact distance moved on the frame, and thus the attachment point for the new radius, might be determined by the spider relying on path integration to locate the direction to the hub (Vollrath et al., 2002). During this process the new radius is usually placed immediately below the exit radius and gaps are filled in alternate sections of the web, presumably to ensure mechanical stability of the structure during construction (Wirth and Barth, 1992; Zschokke, 1996). Once all gaps have been filled the spider strengthens the hub before building the auxiliary spiral and the capture spiral. In some species additional radii are sometimes added to the structure during the construction of the auxiliary spiral (Zschokke and Vollrath, 1995) 4 Cognitive Maps, Preliminary Studies and Suggestions for Further Experiments In the previous section we saw how spiders obtain essential information on the quality of a potential webbuilding site through exploration. To successfully determine if a web should be build, the spider requires information on i) the size of the available space and the presence (and potentially the location) of suitable anchoring points in the surrounding vegetation and ii) whether the available space is free of obstructions or not. There are several ways that such information can theoretically be obtained varying in the cognitive demands made on the spider brain. Below I outline two extremes; the cognitively very demanding spatial awareness and cognitive map hypothesis (from now on referred to as the cognitive map hypothesis) and the much less cognitively demanding binary information and stigmergy hy- 320 Current Zoology pothesis (from now on referred to as the stigmergy hypothesis), before I discuss their predictions and evaluate the admittedly limited evidence for and against them. 4.1 The cognitive map hypothesis The cognitive map hypothesis proposes that spiders construct some form of metric map during site exploration providing information on the size and shape of available space as well as possibly on the location and direction of potential anchor thread attachment points. The potential use and nature of cognitive maps in the practically blind orb-web spiders would though be different from the cognitive maps we discussed earlier used for navigation in visually orientated animals. There is relatively limited evidence for the generation of nonvisual spatial maps and for non-visual navigation in general among animals. However, bats are known to use acoustic landmarks in orientation (Jensen et al., 2005) and the ability to generate olfactory spatial maps are proposed to exist in both vertebrates and invertebrates (Jacobs, 2012). Furthermore, magnetic maps are known from both sea turtles (Putman et al., 2011) and spiny lobsters (Boles and Lohmann, 2003) and the Mexican blind cave fish uses its lateral line organs to encode the geometry and location of landmarks (Burt de Perera, 2004). In the latter study, the results suggested that the cave fish develop cognitive maps based on exploration of the environment without at any time being able to perceive the whole environment at once. Spiders could similarly rely on some simple form of a cognitive map based on memories formed during spatial exploration of the habitat. There is currently no information available on how such metric information could be gathered. Intriguingly, however, the vertical descents discussed above could be used to gather metric information on the size and shape of the available free space in spiders that build vertically inclined webs. If the spider kept a more or less constant horizontal distance between each vertical descent, it could estimate the available space in the horizontal direction by counting the number of vertical descends it can make without encountering obstacles. As discussed previously, numerosity has been reported in jumping spiders and might also be present in orb-web spiders (Nelson and Jackson, 2012; Rodríguez et al., 2014). Distances in the vertical direction could similarly be estimated relatively easily by timing the vertical descents and using that as proxy for distance covered and thus free space. Although perhaps more unlikely, spiders could also store the position of potential attachment points encountered during site exploration in a manner Vol. 61 No. 2 similar to how landmarks are stored in social insects (Cruse and Wehner, 2011; Shettleworth, 2010). During the early phases of web-building spiders could then use this stored information to guide their attachment of anchor threads in order to ensure building a web as planar as possible. 4.2 The stigmergy hypothesis Stigmergy, a type of emergent property or self-organised behaviour, is the expression of complex behaviour based on interactions with, modifications of and feedback from the environment relying on simple rules. The construction of circular walls built by grains around the brood in a species of ants arises by stigmergy. Individual worker ants deposit grains based on the distance to the brood and the local density of already placed grains (Franks and Deneubourg, 1998). Similarly, spiders modify their environment by laying down silk threads which again modifies the behaviour of the spiders encountering them (Krafft and Cookson, 2012). Social spiders and spiderlings of solitary species appear to aggregate in response to specific dragline attachment patterns and silk densities (Saffre et al., 1999; Jeanson et al., 2004). Orb-web spiders could thus rely purely on information obtained from the silk threads that it trails behind during site exploration (Zschokke, 1996), similar to how they primarily rely on information gained from the distance to the auxiliary spiral and to previously laid sticky spiral threads for the exact placement of threads during construction of the sticky spiral (Hingston, 1920; Peters, 1970; Eberhard, 2012; Eberhard and Hesselberg, 2012). A stigmergy model could in combination with cognitively simple binary information provide the spider with all necessary information. Vertical descents could provide yes/no information on whether the space was obstacle free or not, while size suitability (suitable or not) could be provided from the number or density of silk threads (i.e. denser silk could mean the spider has passed through the area many times during exploration suggesting the site is small). Remote sensing using chemical and vibrational stimuli could even provide some metric information. The spider could for example perhaps obtain length information by plucking a thread similar to how it obtains information on the location and size of prey trapped in its web by plucking radii (Klärner and Barth, 1982; Foelix, 2011). 4.3 Predictions arising from the stigmergy and cognitive map hypotheses At present we do not have enough data to firmly reject or support either of the two hypotheses. However, in Table 1 I have compiled some of the information and HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders predictions that in theory would allow us to distinguish between them. Exploratory webs: An exploratory web is a smaller and less planar first web hypothesised to be built at a new site (Zschokke and Vollrath, 2000). The spider then slowly improves the quality of its web in subsequent iterations. The presence of an exploratory web might explain how spiders without relying on spatial memory of the relative location of attachment points manage to construct almost perfectly vertical and planar webs. In contrast the cognitive map hypothesis postulates that exploratory webs are not necessary as the spider has gathered information on the location of suitable attachment points during exploration which it can rely on during the early construction phases of the web. There is some laboratory evidence that some spiders build a smaller and less planar exploratory web on the first attempt to build a web at a new site (Zschokke and Vollrath, 2000; Nakata, 2004). However, studies on other species of orb-web spiders found no evidence for an exploratory web (Nakata, 2004; Hesselberg, 2013; 2014) and the degree to which orb-web spiders in general rely on exploratory webs under natural conditions remains currently unknown. Frequency of relocation after first web: If spiders do not possess any information on the size of the available space and instead rely on exploratory webs, we would expect them to frequently have to relocate, when the site where they build their smaller exploratory webs proves not to be suitable for building the larger normal webs. Unfortunately there is to my knowledge no available observational or experimental data on the frequency of relocations in the wild after construction of the first web. Abandoning site during exploration: The cognitive map hypothesis predicts that spiders continuously gather information on the size and suitability of the site as they explore it, while the stigmergy hypothesis predicts that spiders first make an assessment after exploration Table 1 Predictions of the cognitive map and the stigmergy hypotheses Predictions/hypotheses Cognitive map Stigmergy Exploratory webs Rare Always Frequency of relocation after first web Rare Often Abandoning site during exploration Often Rarely Complex proto-hub structure present Rarely Always Removal of all threads leads to full exploration Rarely Always 321 based on the number and density of threads laid down. Both hypotheses though allow that site exploration can be interrupted if obstacles in the free space are encountered. Nonetheless we would expect to see spiders leave a site during exploration behaviour more often under the cognitive map hypothesis than under the stigmergy hypothesis. Again I have not been able to find any data on this. Complex proto-hub always present: According to the stigmergy hypothesis we would always expect a complex multi-stranded proto-hub to be present prior to web-building as the silk threads contain the information as to whether or not to construct the web as well as what size and shape of web to construct. As mentioned in the section above, the building of the web follows the construction of a proto-hub with either a simple Y-shaped structure of three proto-radii or more commonly a proto-hub with up to 7 proto-radii (Peters, 1937; Eberhard, 1990b; Zschokke, 1996) and it is difficult to imagine how such a simple structure in itself could provide adequate information on size and anchor point locations. During site exploration the spider might attach threads to particular good anchor points which end up being incorporated into the proto-hub structure. However, this would not work with a Y-structure since typical orb webs in the field contain 4–10 anchor threads (7.3 ± 1.6 (mean ± std. dev., n = 130) based on rawdata of four species from Hesselberg 2013). In contrast the cognitive map hypothesis makes no predictions of the complexity of the proto-hub. More quantitative information from the wild on the proto-hub structure both during initial web-building, but also during web-renewals is required. Removals of all threads lead to full exploration: The stigmergy hypothesis postulates that the the role of exploration behaviour is to leave silk threads behind which will then be used subsequently to assess the quality of the site, while the primary aim of site exploration according to the cognitive map hypothesis is to gather and store spatial information. Thus if all silk threads were removed following site exploration, the former hypothesis requires a complete new exploration to be carried out, while the latter hypothesis only require a much shorter and more focussed renewal of key silk threads to be rebuilt. Similarly if new spiders replace original spiders after the latter have explored a site (i.e. laid down trailing silk threads), but before they have actually built a web, the stigmergy hypothesis predicts that these new spiders could build a web without first exploring it. This prediction could relatively easily be tested in laboratory studies if spiders were replaced after 322 Current Zoology they had taken their old web down, but before they had built a new one. Although as mentioned above, we do not have enough experimental evidence to reject any of the hypotheses, a number of studies suggest that spiders may require some sort of spatial awareness and memory to successfully build their webs. Firstly, while the simple stigmergy hypothesis may be used by the spider for determining if the space is suitable or not for a web, it cannot easily explain (unless the silk threads or remote sensing can provide some rough metric information) the fact that many orb-web spiders manage to match the shape of their webs to horizontal and vertical elongated spaces (Fig. 2; Vollrath et al., 1997; Krink and Vollrath, 2000; Hesselberg, 2013). Secondly, as we saw in previous sections spiders adjust the size and shape of their webs according to a host of environmental and previous prey capture experiences (Heiling and Herberstein, 2000) and so probably have a target size and shape in mind prior to initiating web-building, which they need to ensure the available space can accommodate. This is further supported by a study that found that spiders match the size of their webs to their available silk reserves (Eberhard, 1988b). It is possible though that capture spiral size can be regulated independently of frame size by constructing the web such that the area enclosed by the frame is significantly larger than the area enclosed by the capture spiral (Eberhard, 1972). Nonetheless the spider would still need to ensure that there is enough available space to build the minimum size of frame structure allowing it to build its target sized web. 4.4 Suggestions for further experiments and results of a preliminary study As evident from the previous subsection, we first and foremost require observational and experimental data to test the predictions of the two hypotheses. However, in addition we need more information on how site exploration is conducted by different spider species under natural conditions in the wild. Most of our knowledge comes from the spatially limited and structurally simple conditions in the lab, but spiders in the wild may show a very different behaviour. It is also likely that a proportion of the exploration performed by spiders in the lab prior to web-building is done to look for ways in which to escape the cage rather than to evaluate the site for future web-building. While we have no way of knowing when and where new webs will be constructed in the field, one possible method would be to remove all silk threads from existing webs on the site and release spiders caught from elsewhere onto it in the hope that they Vol. 61 No. 2 will construct new webs there. Data could be obtained by recording with one or more video cameras. In addition, in spiders that rest in retreats such as spiders in the genus Zygiella and many tetragnathids, it might be possible to modify the surrounding vegetation while the spider is in its retreat and then record the ensuing exploration and web-building behaviour in order to assess the importance of key attachment sites. Most of our limited information on exploration behaviour comes from spiders building vertical webs. It would be very interesting to contrast and compare the behaviour of the vertically building araneid spiders with spiders that build more horizontal webs such as those in the genera Gasteracantha (Araneidae), Leucauge (Tetragnathidae) and Uloborus (Uloboridae), where for example vertical descents such as those shown in Figure 3B would be useless. However, in spiders that build vertical webs, the possible role of the vertical descents is particular intriguing. The first step to evaluate their importance is to determine the degree to which they are performed during construction of webs in the lab and in the field. If the presence of vertical descents, as expected, is widely confirmed, the second step would be to assess their role in gathering metric and/or binary information from the number of descents and descent duration as discussed above. One way to do this would be to force spiders to build webs in quadratic frames with long narrow vertical shaft extensions that prevent spiders from extending their webs into them, but would increase the total drop time and compare the size and shape of the resulting webs with webs build in quadratic frames alone and with webs build in quadratic frames with long narrow horizontal shafts. Another method would be to look at how spiders deal with smaller obstacles in the available space to estimate the potential spatial resolution of the exploration behaviour. A preliminary study doing just this with the orb-web spider Argope argentata is shown in Fig. 4 (T. Hesselberg, Unpublished data). Three cylindrical obstacles of different diameters were inserted (by tethering them to wires) into the free space of the experimental frames. The very preliminary results suggest that the spiders adapt their web-building differently to large obstacles (Fig. 4C) than to smaller obstacles (Fig. 4A, B). Webs built in the presence of large obstacles were built around the obstacle with the frame curving around it, whereas webs built in the presence of smaller obstacles showed no web-wide adaptation to the obstacles and had the frame beyond the obstacle; in some cases even completely enclosing it within the web (Fig. 4B). One HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders explanation for the observed web geometries in Fig. 4 could be that the spider noticed the large obstacle during exploration and adapted its web accordingly prior to the start of web-building, whereas the smaller obstacles were first detected and responded to during radii and spiral construction. This could again imply that some kind of low-resolution spatial maps of obstacles were formed during site exploration. However, much more experimental work, including recordings of the actual behaviour of the spiders prior to and during web-building, are required before any conclusions can be drawn. It is also important to note that these experiments were carried out in the lab where the spider had no option to relocate. A different approach to estimate the information gathered during exploration would be to record the behaviour and web geometry of webs built in situations where the available space is reduced or enlarged in between the time the spider finishes exploration and starts web-building. This could potentially be achieved by getting spiders to build in frames with adjustable sides although care would be required to ensure that any proto-radii maintained their tension. Finally, although as highlighted above, more experimental work from both the field and the laboratory is essential, computer simulations may also offer vital insights into the role of exploration behaviour. Computer models of the construction of the auxiliary spiral (Gotts and Vollrath, 1991) and of the capture spiral (Eberhard, 1969; Krink and Vollrath, 1997; 1998) have previously shed light on the rules governing these construction behaviours. Agentbased models, similarly to the one produced by Cruse and Wehner (2011) to model the memory elements and neural processing of navigation in desert ants in connection with the cognitive map hypothesis, might help determine the level of information that spiders are required to collect during the exploration phase to successfully construct optimal orb webs at new sites. Fig. 4 5 323 Conclusion In this review we have seen how spiders despite a relatively small and simple central nervous system show a variety of complex cognitively demanding behaviours including learning from previous prey capture, memory of spiral turns, selective attention, potential numerosity and an astounding behavioural flexibility allowing them to adapt their webs to a wide range of internal and external factors. However, we have also seen how webbuilding itself although resulting in the highly structured and complex orb web may actually not be that cognitively demanding and may instead rely on relatively simple rules of which many make use of previously constructed silk threads. The most cognitively demanding task facing orb-web spiders may instead relate to the initial assessment of new web-building sites. However, too few studies have been conducted on exploration behaviour to date to draw any firm conclusions. Although it is probably unlikely that spiders possess cognitive maps as advanced as those whose existence is currently being hotly debated in social insects, which are hypothesised to give bees and ants the ability to infer and take novel short cuts between known locations (Gould, 1986; Menzel et al., 2005), it is equally difficult to imagine how spiders would be able to build well adapted orb webs at novel sites without relying on some form of spatial memory involving basic metric distances obtained during site exploration, especially given their ability to match the size and shape of their webs to confined spaces. One surprising outcome of this review is that limited progress has been made in the study of spatial cognition, exploration behaviour and the early stages of web construction since Vollrath’s review paper (Vollrath, 1992). This is particular remarkable given the substantial progress achieved in our understanding of other aspects of web-building beha- Webs built in experimental frames (30 × 30 × 5 cm) with obstacles by juvenile Argiope argentata The photos show the first web built in a clean frame with an obstacle, so spiders are expected to have explored the frames prior to web-building. A. A web built in a frame with a single small cylindrical obstacle (diameter of 2 mm). B. A web built in a frame with an intermediary obstacle (diameter of 7 mm). C. A web built in a frame with a large obstacle (diameter of 25 mm). The white horizontal scale bars represent a length of 5 cm. 324 Current Zoology viour and its flexibility and in our understanding of how the web-building behaviour and the resulting web geometry interacts with the biomechanical properties of the silk to create an optimal strong and light-weight aerial trap (Vollrath and Selden, 2007; Harmer et al., 2011). Recently there has been a renewed interest in the otherwise neglected area of spider neurophysiology with a number of anatomical studies on brain size and structure (Quesada et al., 2011; Park et al., 2013). Similar methods could be used to compare structures in the supraesophageal ganglion between spiders with extensive site exploration to spiders with hardly any, such as the Darwin’s bark spider (Gregoric et al., 2011), in order to estimate the possible computational requirements of the behaviour. Even more promising is the potential of looking at the central nervous system of live spiders as they perform behaviours with new and more sophisticated electrophysiological and neuroimaging techniques. Finally technological progress may also soon provide us with GPS tags of small enough size and a high enough spatial resolution to directly track large orb-web spiders such as species in the Nephila and Argiope genera during their site exploration behaviour in the wild. I hope with this review to have provided some avenues for further research on the role of exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders and to have highlighted the rich potential offered by these spiders and their webs for enhancing our understanding of cognition, behavioural flexibility and spatial learning in invertebrates. References Ades C, 1986. A construção de teia geométrica como programa comportamental. Ciéncia e Cultura 38: 760–775. Anotaux M, Marchal J, Chaline N, Desquilbet L, Leborgne R et al., 2012. Ageing alters spider orb-web construction. Animal Behaviour 84: 1113–1121. Ap Rhiziart A, Vollrath F, 1994. Design features of the orb web of the spider Araneus diadematus. Behavioral Ecology 5: 280– 287. Babu KS, 1975. Post embryonic development of the central nervous system of the spider Argiope aurantia (Lucas). Journal of Morphology 146: 325–346. Barrantes G, Eberhard WG, 2012. Extreme behavioral adjustments by an orb-web spider to restricted space. Ethology 118: 438– 449. Benamú MA, Schneider MI, Sánchez NE, 2010. Effects of herbicide glyphosate on biological attributes of Alpaida veniliae (Araneae, Araneidae), in laboratory. Chemosphere 78: 871– 876. Benamú MA, Schneider MI, Sánchez NE, 2013. Short and longterm effects of three neurotoxic insecticides on biological and behavioural attributes of the orb-web spider Alpaida veniliae (Araneae, Araneidae): Implications for IPM programs. Eco- Vol. 61 No. 2 toxicology 22: 1155–1164. Blamires SJ, Chao Y-C, Liao C-P, Tso I-M, 2011. Multiple prey cues induce foraging flexibility in a trap-building predator. Animal Behaviour 81: 955–961. Boles LC, Lohmann KJ, 2003. True navigation and magnetic maps in spiny lobsters. Nature 421: 60–63. Breed AL, Levine VD, Peakall DB, Witt PN, 1954. The fate of the intact orb web of the spider Araneus diadematus CL. Behaviour 23: 43–60. Bruce MJ, 2006. Silk decorations: Controversy and consensus. Journal of Zoology 269: 89–97. Burt de Perera T, 2004. Spatial parameters encoded in the spatial map of the blind Mexican cave fish Astyanax fasciatus. Animal Behaviour 68: 291–295. Cheeseman JF, Millar CD, Greggers U, Lehmann K, Pawley MDM et al., 2014. Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111: 8949–8954. Cheeseman JF, Millar CD, Greggers U, Lehmann K, Pawley MDM, et al., 2014. Reply to Cheung et al.: The cognitive map hypothesis remains the best interpretation of the data in honeybee navigation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Early View. Cheung A, Collett M, Collett TS, Dewar A, Dyer F et al., 2014. Still no convincing evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Early View. Chittka L, Niven J, 2009. Are bigger brains better? Current Biology 19: R995–R1008. Collett TS, Fry SN, Wehner R, 1993. Sequence learning by honeybees. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 172: 693–706. Cruse H, Wehner R, 2011. No need for a cognitive map: Decentralised memory for insect navigation. PLoS Computational Biology 7: e1002009. Denny M, 1976. The physical properties of spider's silk and their role in the design of orb-webs. Journal of Experimental Biology 65: 483–506. Eberhard WG, 1969. Computer simulations of orb-web construction. American Zoologist 9: 229–238. Eberhard WG, 1972. The web of Uloborus diversus (Araneae: Uloboridae). Journal of Zoology 166: 417–465. Eberhard WG, 1987. Web-building behavior of anapid, symphytognathid and mysmenid spiders (Araneae). Journal of Arachnology 14: 339–356. Eberhard WG, 1987b. How spiders initiate airborne lines. Journal of Arachnology 15: 1–9. Eberhard WG, 1988a. Memory of distances and directions moved as cues during temporary spiral construction in the spider Leucauge mariana (Araneae: Araneidae). Journal of Insect Behavior 1: 51–66. Eberhard WG, 1988b. Behavioral flexibility in orb web construction: Effects of supplies in different silk glands and spider size and weight. Journal of Arachnology 16: 295–302. Eberhard WG, 1990a. Function and phylogeny of spider webs. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 21: 341–372. Eberhard WG, 1990b. Early stages of orb construction by Philoponella vicina, Leucauge mariana and Nephila clavipes (Araneae, Uloboridae and Tetragnathidae) and their phylogenetic implications. Journal of Arachnology 18: 205–234. HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders Eberhard WG, 2000. Spider manipulation by a wasp larva. Nature 406: 255–256. Eberhard WG, 2007. Miniaturized orb-weaving spiders: Behavioural precision is not limited by small size. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 274: 2203–2209. Eberhard WG, 2010. New types of behavioral manipulation of host spiders by a parasitoid wasp. Psyche 2010, Article ID 950614. Eberhard WG, 2011. Are smaller animals behaviourally limited? Lack of clear constraints in miniature spiders. Animal Behaviour 81: 813–823. Eberhard WG, 2012. Cues guiding placement of the first loop of the sticky spiral in orbs of Micrathena duodecimspinosa (Araneidae) and Leucauge mariana (Tetragnathidae). Bulletin of the British Arachnological Society 15: 224–227. Eberhard WG, 2014. A new view of orb webs: Multiple trap design in a single structure. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 111: 437–449. Eberhard WG, Hesselberg T, 2012. Cues that spiders (Araneae: Araneidae, Tetragnathidae) use to build orbs: Lapses in attention to one set of cues because of dissonance with others? Ethology 118: 610–620. Edmonds DT, Vollrath F, 1992. The contribution of atmospheric water vapour to the formation and efficiency of a spider's capture web. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 248: 145–148. Foelix R, 2011. Biology of Spiders. 3rd edn, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Franks NR, Deneubourg JL, 1997. Self-organizing nest construction in ants: The behaviour of individual workers and the properties of the nest’s dynamics. Animal Behaviour 54: 779– 796. Frost BJ, Mouritsen H, 2006. The neural mechanisms of long distance animal navigation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16: 481–488. Gregoric M, Agnarsson I, Blackledge TA, Kuntner M, 2011. How did the spider cross the river? Behavioural adaptations for river-bridging webs in Caerostris darwini (Araneae: Araneidae). PLoS ONE 6: e26847. Gregoric M, Kiesbuy HC, Quiñones Lebron SG, Rozmann A, Agnarsson I, et al., 2013. Optimal foraging, not biogenetic law, predicts spider orb web allometry. Naturwissenschaften 100: 263–268. Gonzaga MF, Sobzcak JF, 2011. Behavioral manipulation of the orb-weaver spider Argiope argentata (Araneae: Araneidae) by Acrotaphus chedelae (Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae). Entomological Science 14: 220–223. Gotts NM, Vollrath F, 1991. Artificial intelligence modelling of web-building in the garden cross spider. Journal of Theoretical Biology 152: 485–511. Gould JL, 1986. The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science 232: 861–863. Harmer AMT, Blackledge TA, Madin JS, Herberstein ME, 2011. High-performance spider webs: Integrating biomechanics, ecology and behaviour. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 8: 457–471. Harmer AMT, Herberstein ME, 2009. Taking it to extremes: What drives extreme web elongation in Australian ladder web spiders (Araneidae: Telaprocera maudae)? Animal Behaviour 78: 325 499–504. Harmer AMT, Kokko H, Herberstein ME, Madin JS, 2012. Optimal web investment in sub-optimal foraging conditions. Naturwissenschaften 99: 65–70. Heiling AM, Herberstein ME, 2000. Interpretations of orb-web variability: A review of past and current ideas. Ekológia 19: 97–106. Herberstein ME, Craig CL, Coddington JA, Elgar MA, 2000a. The functional significance of silk decorations of orb-web spiders: A critical review of the empirical evidence. Biological Reviews 75: 649–669 Herstein ME, Gasket AC, Glencross D, Hart S, Jaensch S et al., 2000b. Does the presence of potential prey, affect web design in Argiope keyserlingi (Aranea, Araneidae)? Journal of Arachnology 28: 346–350. Herberstein ME, Heiling AM, 1999. Asymmetry in spider orb webs: A result of physical constraints? Animal Behaviour 58, 1241–1246. Hesselberg T, 2010. Ontogenetic changes in web design in two orb-web spiders. Ethology 116: 535–545. Hesselberg T, 2013. Web-building flexibility differs in two spatially constrained orb spiders. Journal of Insect Behavior 26: 283–303. Hesselberg T, 2014. The mechanism behind plasticity of webbuilding behavior in an orb spider facing spatial constraints. Journal of Arachnology 42: 311–314. Hesselberg T, Lehmann F-O, 2009. The role of experience in flight behaviour of Drosophila. Journal of Experimental Biology 212: 3377–3386. Hesselberg T, Triana E, 2010. The web of the acacia orb-spider Eustala illicita (Araneae: Araneidae) with notes on its natural history. Journal of Arachnology 38, 21–26. Hesselberg T, Vollrath F, 2004. The effects of neurotoxins on webgeometry and web-building behaviour in Araneus diadematus Cl. Physiology & Behavior 82: 519–529. Hesselberg T, Vollrath F, 2012. The mechanical properties of the non-sticky spiral in Nephila orb webs (Araneae, Nephilidae). Journal of Experimental Biology 215: 3362–3369. Hingston RWG, 1920. A Naturalist in the Himalaya. London: Small, Maynard & Co. Jackson RR, Pollard SD, 1996. Predatory behavior of jumping spiders. Annual Review of Entomology 41: 287–308. Jacobs LF, 2012. From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 10693–10700. Jeanson R, Deneubourg JL, Theraulaz G, 2004. Discrete dragline induces aggregation in spiderlings of solitary species. Animal Behaviour 67: 531–537. Jensen ME, Moss CF, Surlykke A, 2005. Echolocating bats use acoustic landmarks for spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Biology 208: 4399–4410. Krafft B, Cookson LJ, 2012. The role of silk in the behaviour and sociality of spiders. Psyche 2012: Article ID 529564. Krell F-T, Krämer F, 1998. Chemical attraction of crab spiders (Araneae, Thomisidae) to a flower fragrance component. Journal of Arachnology 26: 117–119. Krink T, Vollrath F, 1997. Analysing spider web-building behavior with rule-based simulations and genetic algorithms. Journal of Theoretical Biology 185: 321–331. 326 Current Zoology Krink T, Vollrath F, 1998. Emergent properties in the behaviour of a virtual spider robot. Proceeding of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 265: 2051–2055. Krink T, Vollrath F, 1999. A virtual robot to model the use of regenerated legs in a web-building spider. Animal Behaviour 57: 223–232. Krink T, Vollrath F, 2000. Optimal area use in orb webs of the spider Araneus diadematus. Naturwissenschaften 87: 90–93. Kuntner M, Agnarsson I, 2009. Phylogeny accurately predicts behavior in Indian Ocean Clitaetra spiders (Araneae: Nephilidae). Invertebrate Systematics 23: 193–204 Kuntner M, Gregoric M, Li D, 2010. Mass predicts web asymmetry in Nephila spiders. Naturwissenschaften 97: 1097–1105. Land MF, 1985. The morphology and optics of spider eyes. In: Barth FG ed. Neurobiology of Arachnids. Berlin: SpringerVerlag, 53–78. Landolfa MA, Barth FG, 1996. Vibrations in the orb web of the spider Nephila clavipes: Cues for discrimination and orientation. Journal of Comparative Physiology 179: 493–508. LeGuelte L, 1969. Learning in spiders. American Zoologist 9: 145–152. Liao C-P, Chi K-J, Tso I-M, 2009. The effects of wind on trap structural and material properties of a sit-and-wait predator. Behavioral Ecology 20: 1194–1203. Lin LH, Edmonds DT, Vollrath F, 1995. Structural engineering of an orb-spider's web. Nature 373: 146–148. Masters WM, Markl HS, Moffat AJM, 1986. Transmission of vibration in a spider's web. In: Shear WA ed. Spiders: Webs, Behavior and Evolution. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Menzel R, Greggers U, Smith A, Berger S, Brandt R et al., 2005. Honey bees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 3040– 3045. Mouritsen H, 2001. Navigation in birds and other animals. Image and Vision Computing 19: 713–731. Nakata K, 2009. To be or not to be conspicuous: The effects of prey availability and predator risk on spider's web decoration building. Animal Behaviour 78: 1255–1260. Nakata K, 2010. Does ontogenetic change in orb web asymmetry reflect biogentic law? Naturwissenschaften 97: 1029–1032. Nakata K, 2013. Spatial learning affects thread tension control in orb-webs. Biology Letters 9: 20130052. Nakata K, Ushimaru A, 2004. Difference in web construction behavior at newly occupied web sites between two Cyclosa species. Ethology 110: 397–411. Nelson XJ, Jackson RR, 2012. The role of numerical competence in a specialized predatory strategy of an araneophagic spider. Animal Cognition 15: 699–710. Nørgaard T, Henschel JR, Wehner R, 2003. Long-distance navigation in the wandering desert spider Leucorchestris arenicola: Can the slope of the dune surface provide a compass cue? Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 189: 801–809. Nørgaard T, Henschel JR, Wehner R, 2007. Use of local cues in the night-time navigation of the wandering desert spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae, Sparassidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 193: 217–222. Opell BD, Karinshak SE, Sigler MA, 2011. Humidity affects the extensibility of an orb-weaving spider's viscous thread droplets. Journal of Experimental Biology 214: 2988–2993. Vol. 61 No. 2 Park YK, Kim HJ, Kim H, Moon MJ, 2013. Fine structure of the CNS ganglia in the geometric spider Nephila clavata (Araneae: Nephilidae). Entomological Research 43: 330–343. Pasquet A, Anotaux M, Leborgne R, 2011. Loss of legs: Is it or not a handicap for an orb-weaving spider? Naturwissenschaften 98: 557–564. Pasquet A, Marchal J, Anotaux M, Leborgne R, 2014.Does building activity influence web-construction and web characteristics in the orb-web spider Zygiella x notata (Araneae, Araneidae). Zoological Studies 53: 11 (1–7). Pasquet A, Ridwan, A, Leborgne R, 1994. Presence of potential prey affects web-building in an orb-weaving spider Zygiella x notata. Animal Behaviour 47: 477–480. Peakall DB, 1971. Conservation of web-proteins in the spider Araneus diadematus. Journal of Experimental Zoology 176: 257–264. Peters HM, 1937. Studien am Netz der Kreuzspinne Aranea diadema. II. Über die Herstellung des Rahmens, der Radialfäden und der Hilfsspirale. Zeitschrift für Morphologie und Ökologie der Tiere 33: 128–150. Peters HM, 1939. Über das Kreuzspinnennetz und seine Probleme. Naturwissenschaften 47: 777–792. Peters HM, 1990. On the structure and glandular origin of bridging lines used by spiders for moving to distant places. Acta Zoologica Fennica 190: 309–314. Peters PJ, 1970. Orb web construction: interaction of spider (Araneus diadematus CL.) and thread configuration. Animal Behaviour 18: 478–484. Platnick NI, 2014. The World Spider Catalog, Version 15. American Museum of Natural History. http://research.amnh.org/iz/ spiders/catalog_15.0/INTRO1.html Putman NF, Endres CS, Lohmann CMF, Lohmann KJ, 2011. Longitude perception and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Current Biology 21: 463–466. Quesada R, Triana E, Vargas G, Douglass JK, Seid MA et al., 2011. The allometry of CNS size and consequences of miniaturization in orb-weaving and cleptoparasitic spiders. Arthropod Structure & Development 40: 521–529. Reed CF, Witt PN, Jones RL, 1965. The measuring function of the first legs of Araneus diadematus CL. Behaviour 25: 98–119. Reed CF, Witt PN, Scarboro MB, Peakall DB, 1970. Experience and the orb web. Developmental Psychobiology 3: 251–265. Rodríguez RL, Briceño RD, Briceño-Aguilar E, Höbel G, 2014. Nephila clavipes spiders (Araneae: Nephilidae) keep track of captured prey counts: Testing for a sense of numerosity in an orb-weaver. Animal Cognition, Epub ahead of print Rodríguez RL, Kolodziej RC, Höbel G, 2013. Memory of prey larders in golden orb-web spiders Nephila clavipes (Araneae: Nephilidae). Behaviour 150: 1345–1356. Romero GQ, Vasconcellos-Neto J, Trivelin PCO, 2008. Spatial variation in the strength of mutualism between a jumping spider and a terrestrial bromeliad: Evidence from the stable isotope 15N. Acta Oecologica 33: 380–386. Saffre F, Furey R. Krafft B, Deneubourg JL, 1999. Collective decision-making in social spiders: Dragline-mediated amplification process acts as a recruitment mechanism. Journal of Theoretical Biology 198: 507–517. Samu F, Vollrath F, 1992. Spider orb web as bioassay for pesticides side effects. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 62: HESSELBERG T: Exploration behaviour in orb-web spiders 117–124. Schneider JM, Vollrath F, 1998.The effect of prey type on the geometry of the capture web of Araneus diadematus. Naturwissenschaften 85: 391–394. Shettleworth SJ, 2010. Cognition, evolution and behavior. 2nd edn. New York: Oxford University Press. Snell-Rood EC, 2013. An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity. Animal Behaviour 85: 1004–1011. Spronk F, 1935. Die Abhängigkeit der Netsbauzeiten der Radnetz-Spinnen Epeira diademata und Zilla x notata von verschiedenen Aussenbedingungen. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie C22: 604–613. Styrsky JD, 2014. An orb-weaver spider exploits an ant-acacia mutualism for enemy-free space. Ecology and Evolution 4: 276–283. Tarsitano MS, Jackson RR, 1994. Jumping spiders make predatory detours requiring movement away from prey. Behaviour 131: 65–73. Tolman EC, 1948. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55: 189–208. Tommasi L, Chiandetti C, Pecchia T, Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G, 2012. From natural geometry to spatial cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 36: 799–824. Tso I-M, Chiang S-Y, Blackledge TA, 2007. Does the giant wood spider Nephila pilipes respond to prey variation by altering web or silk properties. Ethology 113: 324–333. Venner S, Bel-Venner ML, Pasquet A, Leborgne, R, 2003. Body mass dependent cost of web-building behaviour in an orbweaving spider Zygiella x notata. Naturwissenschaften 90: 269–272. Venner S, Pasquet A, Leborgne R, 2000. Web-building behavior in the orb-weaving spider Zygiella x notata: Influence of experience. Animal Behaviour 59: 603–611. Vollrath F, 1986. Gravity as an orientation guide during webconstruction in the orb spiders Araneus diadematus (Aranea, Araneidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 159: 275– 280. Vollrath F, 1987. Altered geometry of webs in spiders with regenerated legs. Nature 328: 247–248. Vollrath F, 1988. Spiral orientation of Araneus diadematus orb webs built during vertical rotation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 162: 413–419. Vollrath F, 1992. Analysis and interpretation of orb spider exploration and web-building behaviour. Advances in the Study of Animal Behavior 21: 147–199. Vollrath F, Downes M, Krackow S, 1997. Design variability in web geometry of an orb-weaving spider. Physiology & Behavior 62: 735–743. Vollrath F, Nørgaard T, Krieger M, 2002. Radius orientation in the cross spider Araneus diadematus. In: Toft S, Scharff N ed. European Arachnology 2000. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press, 327 107–116. Vollrath F, Selden P, 2007. The role of behavior in the evolution of spiders, silks and webs. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 38: 819–846. Wainwright SA, Biggs WD, Currey JD, Gosline JM, 1976. Mechanical Design in Organisms. London: Edward Arnold Ltd. Walter A, Elgar MA, 2012. The evolution of novel animal signals: Silk decorations as a model system. Biological Reviews 87: 686–700. Weltzien P, Barth FG, 1991. Volumetric measurements do not demonstrate that the spider brain "central body" has a special role in web-building. Journal of Morphology 208: 91–98. Wherry T, Elwood RW, 2009. Relocation, reproduction and remaining alive in an orb-web spider. Journal of Zoology 279: 57–63. Wirth E, Barth FG, 1992. Forces in the spider orb web. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 171: 359–371. Witt PN, 1969. Behavioral consequences of laser lesions in the central nervous system of Araneus diadematus Cl. American Zoologist 9: 121–131. Witt PN, 1971. Drugs alter web-building of spiders: A review and evaluation. Behavioral Science 16: 98–112. Witt PN, Reed CF, 1964. Laser lesions and spider web construction. Nature 201: 150–152. Witt PN, Scarboro MB, Daniels R, Peakall DB, Gause RL, 1977. Spider web-building in outer space: Evaluation of records from the skylab experiment. Journal of Arachnology 4: 115–124. Wolf R, Voss A, Hein S, Heisenberg M, 1992. Can a fly ride a bicycle? Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 337: 261–269. Wu C-C, Blamires SJ, Wu C-L, Tso I-M, 2013. Wind induces variations in spider web geometry and sticky spiral droplet volume. Journal of Experimental Biology 216: 3342–3349. Zschokke S, 1993. The influence of the auxiliary spiral on the capture spiral in Araneus diadematus Clerk (Araneidae). Bulletin of the British Arachnological Society 9: 169–173. Zschokke S, 1996. Early stages of orb web construction in Araneus diadematus Clerck. Revue Suisse de Zoologie 709–720. Zschokke S, 2011. Spiral and web asymmetry in the orb webs of Araneus diadematus (Araneae: Araneidae). Journal of Arachnology 39: 358–362. Zschokke S, Herberstein ME, 2005. Laboratory methods for maintaining and studying web-building spiders. Journal of Arachnology 33: 205–213. Zschokke S, Nakata K, 2010. Spider orientation and hub position in orb web. Naturwissenschaften 97: 43–52. Zschokke S, Vollrath F, 1995. Web construction patterns in a range of orb-weaving spiders (Araneae). European Journal of Entomology 92: 523–541. Zschokke S, Vollrath F, 2000. Planarity and size of orb-webs built by Araneus diadematus (Araneae: Araneidae) under natural and experimental conditions. Ekólogia 19: 307–318.