Defence Research Reports
Transcription
Defence Research Reports
DR RDC No. CR R2012-077 A REVIEW OF O SOCIA AL SCIENCE LITER RATURE ON SOCIAL IDEN NTITY DY YNAMICS S AND SC CIENTIFIC C FUNDAM MENTALISM byy: Irene Cheung, Yvonne DeW Wit, Emily-A Ana Filardo,, Michael H H. Thomson n, and Barbara D D. Adams Hum mansystemss® Incorporatted 111 Farq uhar St., Guelph, ON N N1H 3N4 Project M Manager: Michael H. Thomson (519) ( 836 59 911 ext. 301 PWGSC P Con ntract No.: W W7711-088136/001/TOR R Call-up No .: 8136-11 half of On Beh ATIONAL D DEPARTM MENT OF NA DEFENCE as repressented by Canadian Force Le eadership In nstitute Canadian Defe ence Academ my PO Box 17000, Station Forcces Kiingston, Onttario, Canada K7K 7B4 Scientific A Authority: Afzal Upal 416-635-200 4 00, ext. 2170 0 March 2012 Authorr Mic chael H. Th homson ® Humansystems Incorporate d The scientific or technical va alidity of this conttract report is enttirely the responssibility of the Con ntractor and the ccontents do not necessarily have th he approval or en ndorsement of D Defence R&D Can nada. © Her Ma ajesty the Que een as repres sented by the Minister of N National De © Sa ma ajesté la reine e, représentée e par le ministre de la Défe Abstrract The main purpose p of thee Human Terrrain Visualizaation and Sim mulation (HTV Vis) project iss to develop com mputer tools that t can help Canadian deccision makerss envision andd simulate asppects of human terraain. The preseent report rev views literaturre from Sociaal Identity Theeory (SIT; Taajfel and Turner, 197 79) to help thee developmen nt of such com mputer tools ffor use in Cannadian Forcess (CF) training inittiatives. This review was guided g by threee primary quuestions: 1. How do sociostrructural belieffs influence ssocial identityy managemennt strategies foor high and low w status group ps? 2. How do sociostrructural belieffs influence inntergroup perrceptions? 3. How do social id dentity manag gement strateegies influencce intergroup pperceptions? ow that belieffs about socioostructural varriables (i.e., pperceptions off status The papers reviewed sho stability, legitimacy, and d permeability y) can influennce identity m management strategies (i.e., social competition n, individual mobility, m or social s creativiity). Researchh suggests thaat, even thouggh high and low staatus groups may m have diffeerent motivatiions, they mayy use similar or different sstrategies. But this is often o dependeent on the lev vel of identificcation with thhe ingroup andd their percepptions of the sociostrructural conteext. Results off the literaturee review also showed som me research exxamining the impact of sociostructtural beliefs on o intergroup perceptions. There is veryy little researcch that addresses th he impact of identity i manaagement strateegies on interrgroup percepptions. Howevver, there is research considering other o kinds off strategies, ouutside of the tthree core strategies, and hhow these impact percceptions. Ano other goal of the t project waas to examinee psychologiccal literature ppertaining to secular or o scientific fu undamentalism m and to idenntify any relevvant psychom metric scales. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page iii R Résum mé Le projet de Visualisation et de simulatio on de la dimen nsion humainne (HTVis) a pour objet dee m mettre au pointt des outils in nformatiques pouvant p aiderr les décideurrs canadiens à visualiser ett à siimuler des asp pects de la dim mension hum maine. Le présent rapport exxamine la doccumentation ssur la thhéorie de l’ideentité sociale (TIS; Tajfel et e Turner, 197 79) afin de peermettre la miise au point dd’outils innformatiques qui serviront dans le cadree des initiativees d’instructioon des Forcess canadienness (FC). C Cet examen se fondait sur trrois grandes questions q : ociostructurellles ont-elles sur les stratéggies de gestioon de 1. Quellee influence less croyances so l’identtité sociale dees groupes à statut supérieu ur et inférieurr? 2. Quellee influence less croyances so ociostructurellles ont-elles sur les percepptions intergrroupes? 3. Quellee influence less stratégies dee gestion de l’identité sociaale ont-elles ssur les percepptions intergrroupes? D D’après les documents exam minés, les croyances relativ ves aux variabbles sociostruucturelles (c.--à-d. lees perceptionss en matière de d stabilité du u statut, de lég gitimité, et dee perméabilitéé) peuvent innfluencer les stratégies s de gestion g de l’id dentité (c.-à-d d. la concurreence sociale, lla mobilité inndividuelle ou u la créativité sociale). La recherche r ind dique que, bieen que les grooupes de statuut suupérieur et infférieur puisseent avoir des motivations m différentes, d ceeux-ci peuvennt utiliser des sttratégies semb blables ou diffférentes. Maiis cela dépend d souvent du degré d’identtification avecc l’endogroupe et e de la percep ption du conteexte sociostru ucturel. Les réésultats de l’eexamen de la doocumentation n ont permis de d cerner certaains travaux de d recherche pportant sur l’iimpact des crroyances sociiostructurelless sur les perceeptions interg groupes. Il exiiste très peu dde travaux porrtant suur l’incidencee des stratégiees de gestion de d l’identité sur s les percepptions intergrooupes. Cependdant, onn trouve des travaux t exam minant d’autress sortes de strratégies, en deehors des troiis stratégies dde base, ett l’influence que q celles-ci ont o sur les perrceptions. Le projet visait également unn autre objectiif : exxaminer les publications dee psychologiee relative au fondamentalis f sme séculier oou scientifiquue et reecenser tout barème b psycho ométrique perrtinent. Paage iv Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Execu utive Summa S ary Social Id dentity Dy ynamics and Scienttific Funda amentalis sm eung, Yvonn ne DeWit, Emily-Ana Fiilardo, Mich hael H. Thom mson, & Ba arbara D. Irene Che £ Adams, Humansyste H ems Incorp porated; DR RDC Toronto o No. CR2012-XXX; De efence R&D Cana ada – Toron nto; April 20 012. This researcch was carried out by Hum mansystems® IIncorporated in support off the Human T Terrain Visualizatio on and Simulation (HTViss) project beinng conducted at Defence R Research and Developmeent Canada – Toronto T (DRD DC – T). Thee main purposse of the HTV Vis project is tto develop computer to ools that can help h Canadian n decision maakers envisionn and simulatte aspects of hhuman terrain. Thee present repo ort reviews thee psychologiccal literature ppertaining to Social Identitty Theory (SIT), to heelp inform thee developmen nt of computerr tools for Caanadian Forcees (CF) traininng initiatives. t people’s beliefs about themselves aare partly baseed on the social groups thaat they SIT posits that belong to an nd identify with, w and that being b part of a group invollves beliefs annd knowledge about the group, evaluations e ab bout the group, and behaviiours that theyy should adheere to as a meember of the group. A key assump ption of SIT is that people are motivatedd to see their social groupss positively and a distinctiveely (positive distinctivenes d ss), and whenn people’s belliefs about theeir group are threaten ned, they can use various strategies to m manage their cconcerns (i.e.,, social comppetition, social creattivity, or indiv vidual mobilitty). The strateegies that peoople select deppend on socioostructural variables (i.e., the stabiliity and legitim macy of the soocial differenntial, and the ppermeability oof the boundaries between grou ups) that are related r to theiir beliefs abouut the social pposition of their group. Understand ding how peop ple function in an intergrouup context caan aid in the ddevelopment oof a social simulation system of soccial identity dynamics d for C CF training. ure review on SIT was guid ded by three pprimary questtions: The literatu 1. How do sociostrructural belieffs influence ssocial identityy managemennt strategies foor highand d low status groups? g 2. How do sociostrructural belieffs influence inntergroup perrceptions? 3. How do social id dentity manag gement strateegies influencce intergroup pperceptions? The results of the literatu ure review ind dicated that thhe impact of sociostructural beliefs on ssocial identity maanagement strategies was mediated m by hhow closely iddentified an inndividual wass with their group. Key finding gs with respecct to the stabillity and legitim macy of sociaal structures iindicated that among those who arre highly iden ntified with thheir group, ann unstable soccial structure iis experienced d as threatenin ng and stressfful for membbers of a high status group and challengiing by members off low status groups. g Underr unstable or iillegitimate soocial structurees, highly ideentified group mem mbers are likelly to use sociaal competitionn strategies (ee.g., bolsterinng) in order too either maintain th heir status (in the case of hiigh status grooups) or challeenge the statuus (in the casee of low status group ps). When staatus is deemed d to be stable or legitimatee, the predom minant manageement strategy is likely l to be so ocial creativitty (e.g., selecttive devaluingg) among higghly identifiedd members of both high h and low staatus groups. When W the bounndaries betweeen groups are seen to be ppermeable, highly iden ntified membeers of low stattus groups aree likely to usee social comppetition (e.g., collective action) strategies to man nage social ideentity. Highlyy identified m members of hiigh status grouups are Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page v likkely to use mobility m strateg gies (i.e., recaategorization)) to maintain their distinctiiveness. Reseearch allso indicates that t the impacct of the socio ostructural vaariables are addditive. The ppredominant sstrategy ussed by those who w are weak kly identified with their low w status groupp, irrespectivve of sociostruuctural vaariables, is individual mob bility (e.g., ou utgroup favourritism, ingrouup derogationn) as they are likely too see changing g their group in order to im mprove their social s status aas the most effficient strateggy. W With respect to o the influencce of sociostru uctural beliefss on intergrouup perceptionns, the researcch inndicates that highly h identifi fied group members are lik kely to displayy outgroup deerogation and inngroup favourritism in respo onse to unstab ble social stru uctures. Whenn social structture is stable,, these inntergroup percceptions are likely to only be evident when w using imp mplicit measurres. When a soocial sttructure is bellieved to be leegitimate, hig gh status group p members arre likely to evvaluate outgrooups neegatively and d feel less guillt or threat about the sociall structure. Thhe less legitim mate they findd the soocial structuree, the more lik kely they are to perceive discrimination d n from outgrooup members. Inntergroup relaations are only y impacted by y the permeab bility of the ggroup boundarries in combinnation w with the impacct of other socciostructural variables. v Forr example, perrmeable grouup boundariess are asssociated with h less ingroup p identificatio on and more outgroup o idenntification amoong low statuus grroups when th he status struccture is perceived to be staable and legitiimate. W With respect to o the impact of o social identtity managem ment strategiess on intergrouup perceptionns, the reesearch indicaated that the use u of mobilitty strategies such as superoordinate recattegorization reeduced neegative outgro oup perceptio ons, especially y by lower staatus groups. O On the other hhand, social coompetition, esspecially in th he face of lim mited resourcees was likely tto lead to neggative intergrooup peerceptions succh as feelingss of threat, biaas against the outgroup, annd downward social compaarisons. O Overall, the literature review w shows that there are com mplex relationns among the variables in tthe soocial identity model and th he current repo ort aims to clarify these re lations to suppport the HTV Vis prroject. Paage vi Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Somm maire Social Id dentity Dy ynamics and Scienttific Funda amentalis sm eung, Yvonn ne DeWitt, Emily-Ana E F Filardo, Mic chael H. Tho omson, & Barbara Irene Che £ D. Adams s, Humansy ystems Inco orporated; DRDC Toro onto No. CR R2012-XXX; Defence R&D Cana ada – Toron nto; Avril 20 012. umansystems® Incorporateed à l’appui duu projet de Cette recheerche a été efffectuée par Hu Visualisatio on et de simullation de la diimension hum maine (HTViss) mené actueellement par R Recherche et développ pement pour la défense Can nada – Toronnto (RDDC – T). Le projet HTVis a pouur principal objet de meettre au point des outils infformatiques ppouvant aider les décideurss canadiens à visualiser et à simulerr des aspects de d la dimensiion humaine. Le présent raapport examinne les publicaations de psychologiee portant sur la l théorie de l’identité l sociiale (TIS), afi fin de guider lla mise au poiint des outils inform matiques desttinés aux initiiatives d’instrruction des Foorces canadieennes (FC). D’après la TIS, T l’opinion n que se font les gens de leeur propre perrsonne se fonnde en partie ssur les groupes socciaux auxquells ils appartieennent et auxqquels ils s’ideentifient. La T TIS pose égaleement en principe qu ue pour faire partie p d’un gro oupe, on doitt avoir des crooyances et dess connaissancces concernant ce groupe, faaire l’évaluatiion du groupee, et adopter lees comportem ments qui s’im mposent en tant que meembre du grou upe. Une des hypothèses cclés de la TIS est que les geens sont motiivés à voir leurs group pes sociaux dee façon positiv ve et distinctiive (caractèree distinctif positif) et que, llorsque leurs croyan nces à l’égard d de leur grou upe sont menaacées, ils peuvvent avoir reccours à diversses stratégies pour p gérer leurrs inquiétudess (c.-à-d. con currence sociiale, créativitéé sociale ou m mobilité individuellee). Les stratég gies choisies par p les gens ssont fonction des variabless sociostructurrelles (c.à-d. la stabiilité et la légittimité de l’écart social et laa perméabilitté des limites qui séparent les groupes) liéées à leurs cro oyances à l’ég gard de la possition sociale de leur grouppe. La comprééhension du fonction nnement des gens g dans un contexte c interrgroupes peutt aider à l’élabboration d’unn système de simulatio on sociale de la dynamique de l’identitéé sociale aux fins de l’instrruction des m membres des FC. d ouvrages sur la TIS se fondait sur trrois grandes qquestions : L’examen des 1. Qu uelle influencee les croyancees sociostructturelles ont-ellles sur les strratégies de geestion de l’id dentité socialee des groupes à statut supéérieur et infériieur? 2. Qu uelle influencee les croyancees sociostructturelles ont-ellles sur les peerceptions inteergroupes? 3. Qu uelle influencee les stratégies de gestion dde l’identité ssociale ont-ellles sur les perrceptions inteergroupes? Les résultatts de l’examen de la docum mentation dém montrent que l’incidence ddes croyances sociostructu urelles sur less stratégies dee gestion de l’’identité sociaale est influenncée par le deegré auquel une personne s’id dentifie à son groupe. Les résultats clés relatifs à la sstabilité et à laa légitimité des d structures sociales dém montrent que, ppour les persoonnes qui s’iddentifient forttement à leur groupee, une structurre sociale insttable peut connstituer une m menace et êtree source de strress (membres de d groupes dee statut supériieur) et paraîtrre difficile (m membres des ggroupes de staatut inférieur). Lorsque L la strructure sociale est instable ou illégitimee, il y a de forrtes chances qque les personnes qui q s’identifieent fortement à leur groupee aient recourrs à des stratéggies de concuurrence Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page vii soociale (p. ex. le renforcemeent) dans le but soit de maiintenir leur sttatut (dans le cas des grouppes à sttatut supérieur), soit de con ntester le statu ut (dans le cass des groupess à statut inférrieur). Quandd le sttatut est jugé stable s ou légiitime, il est po ossible que laa stratégie de ggestion prédoominante soit la crréativité sociaale (p. ex. la dépréciation d sélective) s chezz les personnnes qui s’identtifient fortem ment à leeur groupe, qu ue le statut soit supérieur ou o inférieur. Si S les limites sséparant les ggroupes sont coonsidérées co omme étant peerméables, alo ors les person nnes qui s’ideentifient fortem ment à leur grroupe dee statut inférieur vont prob bablement avo oir recours au ux stratégies dde concurrencce sociale (p. ex. l’action collecttive) pour gérrer l’identité sociale. s Les membres m qui ss’identifient ffortement à dees grroupes à statu ut supérieur vont probablem ment employeer des stratégiies de mobilitté (c.-à-d. la reecatégorisatio on) pour préseerver leur caraactère distincttif. La rechercche indique aaussi que les inncidences des variables socciostructurellees s’additionn nent. La stratéégie prédominnante utilisée par ceeux qui s’iden ntifient peu à leur groupe à statut inférieeur, quelles qu que soient les variables soociostructurellles, est la mo obilité individ duelle (p. ex. le l favoritismee exogroupe, lle dénigremennt enndogroupe), car c il y a de fo ortes chances que ces perso onnes jugent que la meilleure stratégie ppour am méliorer leur statut social sera s de chang ger de groupe.. En ce qui a traiit à l’influencce qu’ont les croyances c socciostructurellees sur les percceptions inntergroupes, la recherche in ndique que lees personnes qui q s’identifieent beaucoup à leur groupee risquent fort dee dénigrer les autres group pes et de faire du favoritism me à l’intérieuur de leur grouupe en rééaction à l’insstabilité des sttructures sociiales. Quand la l structure soociale est stabble, ces percepptions inntergroupes riisquent de n’aapparaître quee lorsque des mesures impllicites sont apppliquées. Quuand ils crroient en la léégitimité de laa structure socciale, les mem mbres des grooupes à statut supérieur ontt de foortes chances d’évaluer less autres group pes négativem ment et de resssentir moins dde culpabilité à l’égard de la sttructure sociaale ou de se seentir moins menacés m dans lle cadre de ceelle-ci. Moinss les geens croient en n la légitimitéé de la structu ure sociale, plu us ils risquentt de percevoirr de la diiscrimination de la part dess membres dees autres grou upes. Les relattions intergrooupes ne sont afffectées que par p la perméab bilité des limiites du groupee combinée à l’incidence dd’autres variaables soociostructurellles. Par exem mple, les limites perméables d’un groupee sont associéées à une moiindre iddentification à l’endogroup pe et à une plu us grande iden ntification à ll’exogroupe cchez les grouppes à sttatut inférieurr quand la stru ucture du statu ut est jugée sttable et légitim me. En ce qui a traiit à l’incidencce des stratégies de gestion n de l’identitéé sociale sur lees perceptionns inntergroupes, la recherche démontre d que le recours aux stratégies dde mobilité, coomme la reecatégorisatio on surordonnéée, réduit les perceptions p néégatives de l’’exogroupe, ssurtout chez lees grroupes à statu ut inférieur. D’autre D part, laa concurrencee sociale, partticulièrement lorsque les reessources sont limitées, risque fort d’enttraîner des peerceptions néggatives interggroupes, notam mment lee sentiment d’’être menacé, de faire l’objjet de préjugéés de la part dde l’exogroupe, et des coomparaisons sociales s desceendantes. D Dans l’ensemb ble, l’examen des publications démontree l’existence dde relations ccomplexes enttre les vaariables du modèle m de l’ideentité sociale,, et le présentt rapport vise à clarifier cess relations à ll’appui duu projet HTV Vis. Paage viii Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Table e of Co ontents s ABSTRACT.................................................................. ..................... ..........................................................III RÉSUMÉ..................................................................... ..................... ..........................................................IV EXECUTIVESSUMMARY............................................... ..................... ..........................................................IV SOMMAIRE............................................................... ..................... .........................................................VII TABLEOFCO ONTENTS................................................. ..................... ..........................................................IX LISTOFFIGU URES........................................................ ..................... ..........................................................XI LISTOFTABLES.......................................................... ..................... .........................................................XII 1. PROJECTOVERVIEW........................................... ..................... ...........................................................1 1.1 1.2 1.3 BACKGROUNDANDDSCOPE................................... ......................................................................................1 WORKITEMS.................................................... ......................................................................................1 DELIVERABLES................................................... ......................................................................................1 2. METHO ODANDRESULT TS....................................... ..................... ...........................................................3 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 MINDMAPANDKEYWORDS E .................................. ......................................................................................3 DATABASES....................................................... ......................................................................................4 SELECTIONOFARTTICLES....................................... ......................................................................................4 STRUCTUREOFTHE T EREPORT................................. ...................................................................... ................4 3. SOCIALIDENTITYAND DSOCIALIDENTITYMANAGEEMENT.....................................................................5 3.1 DEFINITIONSANDRELEVANTTHEORRETICALFRAMEW ORKS..............................................................................5 4. HOWDO OSOCIOSTRUC CTURALBELIEFFSOFSTABILITTY,LEGITIMAC CY,ANDPERM MEABILITYINFLLUENCE SOCIALIDEN NTITYMANAGEMENTSTRAT TEGIESFORHIG GHANDLOWSSTATUSGROU UPS?(4.7.3)..................11 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 STABILITYAND T IDENTITYMANAGEM MENTSTRATEGIES ..................................................................... ..............11 LEGITIMACYAND E IDENTITY D MANAGEMENTSTRATEGI ES................................................................................28 PERMEABILITYANDDIDENTITYMANAAGEMENTSTRATEEGIES.............................................................................40 OTHERRESEARCHEXAMININGSOCIOSTRUCTURALBEELIEFSANDIDENTTITYMANAGEMEN NTSTRATEGIES.................50 SUMMARY........................................................ ....................................................................................55 5. HOWDO OSOCIOSTRUC CTURALBELIEFFSͲSTABILITYY,LEGITIMACY,,ANDPERMEA ABILITYͲINFLU UENCE INTERGROU UPPERCEPTION NS?(4.7.4).......................... ..................... ..........................................................57 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 5.5 STABILITYAND T INTERGROUPPERCEPPTIONS............. ....................................................................................57 LEGITIMACYAND E INTERGROUP N PERCCEPTIONS.......... ....................................................................................60 PERMEABILITYANDDINTERGROUPPERCEPTIONS E ....... ....................................................................................69 OTHERRESEARCHEXAMININGSOCIOSTRUCTURALBEELIEFSANDINTERGGROUPPERCEPTIONS..............................70 SUMMARY........................................................ ....................................................................................79 6. HOWDO OSOCIALIDEN NTITYMANAGEMENTSTRATTEGIESINFLUENCEINTERGRO OUPPERCEPTIIONS? (4.7.5)........................................................................ ..................... ..........................................................81 6.1 SUMMARY........................................................ ....................................................................................96 7. FEEDBA ACKLOOP–HO OWDOIDENTITYMANAGEM MENTSTRATEG GIESIMPACTID DENTITYANDG GROUP STATUSRELA ATIONS(SOCIO OSTRUCTURALLBELIEFS)?(4..7.6)........................................................................97 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page ix 8.. SYNOPSIS(LESSTHAN3,0 000WORDS–5PAGES)OFM METHODOLOG GIESUSEDTOM MANIPULATEA AND M MEASURESOCIO OCULTURALBELIEFSANDID DENTITYMANA AGEMENTSTRA ATEGIES......................................99 8.1 8.2 8.3 MEASSURESOFSOCIOSTTRUCTURALVARIAABLES....................................... ........................................................99 MANIPULATIONSOFTH HESOCIOSTRUCTU URALVARIABLES.......................... ......................................................101 MEASSURESOFSOCIALIDENTITYMANAG GEMENTSTRATEGIES.................. ......................................................104 9.. SCIENTIFICSTATUSOFTH HECONCEPTO OFSECULARAN NDSCIENTIFIC FUNDAMENTA ALISM..................113 SECULLARANDSCIENTIFFICFUNDAMENTAALISM....................................... ......................................................113 9.1 9.1.1 Fu undamentalism m............................................................................................ ..............................113 9.1.2 Seecularfundameentalism................................................................................ ..............................114 9.1.3 Scientificfundam mentalism............................................................................. ..............................114 IMPACCTOFNONͲRELIGIIOUSBELIEFSYSTEEMSONHEALTH,W WELLͲBEINGANDCOGNITION......................................115 9.2 9.3 SCALEESPERTAININGTOSECULARANDSCCIENTIFICFUNDAM MENTALISM........ ......................................................116 9.3.1 Scientism....................................................................................................... ..............................116 9.3.2 EvvolutionandCrreationism............................................................................ ..............................117 9.3.3 ReelatingScienceandReligion........................................................................ ..............................118 9.3.4 StrrengthofBelieefs......................................................................................... ..............................119 9.3.5 AtttitudetowardScience................................................................................ ..............................119 9.3.6 FreeeWillandDeeterminism............................................................................ ..............................120 9.4 SUMM MARYOFFINDINGS................................................................... ......................................................121 REEFERENCES........................................................................................... ..................... ............................123 ANNEXA–SCIEENTIFICFUNDA AMENTALISMSSCALE................................. ..................... ............................131 ANNEXB–CREA ATIONISMSCA ALEVERSIONS.......................................... ..................... ............................133 ANNEXC–STREENGTHOFBELLIEFSMEASUREES....................................... ..................... ............................135 COMMITMENTTTOBELIEFS(CTB;;MAXWELLͲSMITTH&ESSES,2012 2)................... ......................................................135 WORLDHEALTH HORGANIZATIONQUALITYOFLIFE–SPIRITUAL,RELIGIOUS,ANDPERRSONALBELIEFS((WHOQOLͲSRP PB).136 ANNEXD–SCALESMEASURIN NGATTITUDESSTOWARDSCIENCE............ ..................... ............................138 MENIS,1989)................................................... ......................................................138 ATTITUDETOWAARDSSCIENCE(M ANNEXE–FADͲͲPLUS(PAULHUS&CAREY,2 2011).................................. ..................... ............................139 Paage x Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® List of o Figures FIGURE1.THEEROLEOFSOCIOSTTRUCTURALBELIEFSINIDENTIFICATTIONANDIDENTITTYMANAGEMENTTSTRATEGIES(TAJJFEL& TURNERR,1986)......................................................... ......................................................................................7 FIGURE2.INTEERGROUPHELPING GRELATIONSASA AFFECTEDBYPERCCEIVEDLEGITIMAC CYANDSTABILITYOFPOWERRELATTIONS BETWEENGROUPS(NADLLER,2002;CITED DINNADLERAND HALABI,2006,PP.99)...................................... ..............12 FIGURE3. VC,,CO,ANDTPRAASAFUNCTIONOFGROUPSTATUSAANDSTATUSSTABBILITY.(SCHEEPERS,2009,P.231)).............18 FIGURE4. RELLATIONSHIPSBETW WEENDIFFERENTFFUNCTIONSANDFFORMSOFINGROU RTIN UPBIAS,SELFͲESTTEEM,ANDEFFOR EXPERIM MENT3.STANDAR RDIZEDCOEFFICIEN NTSAREEXPRESSEEDALONGTHELIN NES(LOWSTATUSS/HIGHSTATUS);TTHOSEIN BOLDAR RESIGNIFICANTATTP<.05.ID=IDEENTITYͲEXPRESSIV VEFUNCTION;INSST=INSTRUMENTTALFUNCTION;PCSE= PRIVATEESUBSCALEOFCOLLECTIVE O SELFͲEESTEEMSCALE;M MIP=MAXIMUMINGROUPPROFITT;MJP=MAXIMU UMJOINT PROFIT;MD=MAXIMUM MDIFFERENTIATIO ON.(SCHEEPERSETTAL.,2006,P.9 954)......................................................25 FIGURE5.THEESTATUSVALUEASYMMETRYSHOW WNWITHGROUPBBASEDONSCHOO OLAFFILIATIONINSTUDY1.(SCHMADERETAL., 2001,PP.786)........................................................... ....................................................................................29 FIGURE6.STATUSILLEGITIMACYYMODERATESTHESTATUSVALUEAASYMMETRYINSTTUDY3.(SCHMADDERETAL.,P.792)..........30 FIGURE7.POSSITIVEAFFECTASAAFUNCTIONOFLO OWEFFORTATTRI BUTIONSANDEXPPERIMENTALLEGIITIMACY.(COSTARELLI,2012, P.54)....................................................................... ...................................................................... ..............38 KESSLERANDMU MMENDEY,20022,P.76)................................................50 FIGURE8.INTEEGRATEDSITͲANDDͲRDTMODEL(K FIGURE9.INTEERACTIONBETWEENIDENTIFICATIO ON×EXPERIMENTTALCONDITION.D EPENDENTVARIA ABLE:IMPLICITOU UTGROUP EVALUAT TION.(VEZZALIETTAL.,2012,P.37)................... ...................................................................... ..............59 FIGURE10.APATHͲANALYTICM MODEL,WITHPRO OTOTYPICALITYAS SEXOGENOUSVAR RIABLEANDLEGITIMACY,THREAT,G GUILT,AND ROUPATTITUDESA ASENDOGENOUSVARIABLES.(FRO OMWEBER,MUM MMENDEY,ANDWALDZUS,2002, P.456)..62 INTERGR FIGURE11.ATTRIBUTIONSTOD T DISCRIMINATIONA AMONGHIGHSTA ATUS(EUROPEANAMERICAN)ANDLOWSTATUS(LA ATINO AMERICAN)GROUPMEMBERSASAFUNCTTIONOFREJECTIONNCONTEXTANDBBELIEFININDIVIDU UALMOBILITY(STTUDY2). (MAJORRETAL.,2002,P.275)..................................... ....................................................................................67 FIGURE12.MINORITYANDMAJJORITYGROUP’SBBIASINGENERALRREWARDALLOCATTIONSINEXPERIM MENT2.(GONZA ALEZ& BROWN,2006,P.763).............................................. ....................................................................................84 FIGURE13.IDENTIFICATION×IDENTITYTHREATINTERACTIONPREEDICTINGPERCEPTTIONOFINGROUPPVARIABILITY(PAAGLIAROET AL.,201 12,P.44)....................................................... ....................................................................................90 FIGURE14.IDENTIFICATION×IDENTITYTHREATINTERACTIONPREEDICTINGAMBIVAALENCETOWARDTTHEINGROUP(PAAGLIAROET 12,P.44)....................................................... ....................................................................................91 AL.,201 FIGURE15.SUPPRESSORMODE U ELTESTINGRELATIONSHIPSBETWEEENTHEPERCEIVED DGROUPDEVALUA ATIONMANIPULA ATION,GROUP IDENTIFICATION,INTERGR ROUPDIFFERENTIA ATION,ANDINTRA AGROUPRESPECT .(JETTENETAL.,2005,P.212)...............93 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page xi L List of Tables s TAABLE1.KEYWORD DS........................................................................................ ..........................................................3 TAABLE2.DATABASSES........................................................................................ ..........................................................4 TAABLE3.THE3×2TAXONOMYOFIIDENTITYMANAGEMENTSTRATEGIEESFOLLOWINGNEEGATIVESOCIALIDDENTITY(BLANZEETAL., 1998,P722 2)........................................................................................ ..........................................................8 TAABLE4.SUMMARRYOFIDENTITYMAANAGEMENTSTRAATEGIESINRESPONSETOSOCIOSTRRUCTURALSTABILIITY............................27 TAABLE5.SUMMARRYOFIDENTITYMAANAGEMENTSTRAATEGIESINRESPONSETOSOCIOSTRRUCTURALLEGITIM MACY.........................39 TAABLE6.SUMMARRYOFIDENTITYMAANAGEMENTSTRAATEGIESINRESPONSETOSOCIOSTRRUCTURALPERMEAABILITY......................49 TAABLE7.CORRELATTIONSBETWEENTTRAITCOMPONEN NTSOFTHESITͲAN NDͲRDTMODEL. (KESSLERANDM UMMENDEY,20 002,P. 83)........................................................................................................ ........................................................52 TAABLE8.SUMMARRYOFRESEARCHCOMBININGALLTH HREESOCIOSTRUC CTURALDIMENSIO ONSINRELATIONTTOIDENTITY MANAGEMEN NTSTRATEGIES........................................................................ ........................................................55 TAABLE9.SUMMARRYOFINTERGROUPPERCEPTIONSIN NRESPONSETOSOCIOSTRUCTURALLSTABILITY.........................................60 TAABLE10.SUMMAARYOFINTERGROUPPERCEPTIONSINRESPONSETOSOCIOSTRUCTURAALLEGITIMACY...................................69 TAABLE11.SUMMAARYOFRESEARCHCOMBININGALLTTHREESOCIOSTRU UCTURALDIMENSIIONSINRELATION NTOINTERGROUP P PERCEPTIONSS.......................................................................................... ........................................................78 TAABLE13.SUMMAARYOFRESEARCHASSESSINGTHERELATIONSHIPBETWEENSOCIALIDDENTITYMANAGEEMENTSTRATEGIEESAND INTERGROUPPPERCEPTIONS........................................................................ ........................................................95 TAABLE14:SUBSCAALEDESCRIPTIONSS,SAMPLEITEMS,ANDRELIABILITYYFORPOLINGANDDEVAN’S(2004))MEASURE...............118 Paage xii Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 1. Project Overv view 1.1 Ba ackground d and Sco ope The Canadiian Forces need to understand the expedditionary enviironment in oorder to effecttively communicaate their messages and win n the hearts annd minds of addversary popuulations. Signnificant scientific an nd technologiical breakthro oughs in the fi fields of sociaal psychologyy, social influeence, and cultural antthropology are needed to achieve a a preddictive model of social inflluence that alllows decision maakers to plan, execute, and d measure effeectiveness of various typess of non-kinettic operations. The goals off the HTVis prroject are to iidentify the m most well deveeloped and w well tested social scien nce models off the dynamics of the formaation and maiintenance of ppeople’s sharred social beliefs and to build comp puter models that can be uused by Canaddian decision makers to beetter understand complex socio-cultural beeliefs of targett populationss in expeditionnary environm ments. To identify succh models, wo ork is needed along the folllowing two llines of researrch: (1) underrstanding the dynamics of social id dentity beliefs fs and (2) the sscientific stattus of the conncept of scientific/seecular fundam mentalism. 1.2 Wo ork Items The followiing work item ms were underrtaken: x Parrticipated in a start-up meeeting with the scientific autthority. x Wo orked with thee scientific au uthority to deffine the scopee of the invesstigation. x Con nducted a tho orough search h of the psychhological literaature for research pertaininng to the key y questions x Sellected scholarrly publication ns (i.e., journnal articles). x Rev viewed literatture pertainin ng to the key qquestions andd integrated seelected publiccations into o a literature review. r 1.3 De eliverables s A comprehensive review w of the literatture related too social identtity theory andd the conceptt of scientific fu undamentalism m. The reportt will include : x A summ mary of databaases, methodss and keyworrds employed to conduct thhe literaturre review; x Summarry of key theo ories and conccepts; x Review of the literatu ure on social identity theorry and scientiific fundamenntalism and a su ummary of thee findings. x Draaft of the literrature review.. x Fin nal literature review r to be submitted. s Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 1 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 2 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 2. Method M and Results R s 2.1 Miindmap an nd Keywo ords To begin, a Mindmap waas generated to t provide ann illustration oof the major cconstructs andd other research areeas relevant to o social identtity, pertaininng specificallyy to the role oof sociostructuural beliefs in identity management m strategies and d intergroup pperceptions. T This process iinvolved a brainstormiing session with w members of the HSI£ rresearch team m, and relied oon their cumuulative knowledge and experien nce with the pertinent psychhological dom mains. From tthe Mindmapp, a set of keywords was w developed d to focus thee literature seaarch. The team m establishedd a number off core concepts an nd primary keeywords, as sh hown in Tabl e 1. Table 1. K Keywords Co ore Concept Primary Keyw words Meeaning Sysstems Consistent/ cooherent meaningg syste*, worldview, belief system m, system justification, meaning m maintennance, meaning making, meaninng framework, global meaninng, situational meeaning, value syystem, search foor meaning Meeasurement (related to meeaning sysstems) Tolerance for ambiguity, needd for structure, need for cognitionn, need for control, personal uncertainty, need for clarity, personal inconssistency, need foor meaning Meeasures scale, questionnaire, inventory ry, test, indices, iindicators Social identity identi*, social identi*, relationaal identi*, social role*, collective identi*, relationaal sel*, intergrouup contact theoryy, intergroup relaations, sociostructural beliefs, identity managgement, identity bolstering, idenntity change, inteergroup status, intergroup perrceptions, identitty formation, ideentity maintenancce, shared sociaal belief, self-cattegorization, grooup membership, self-enhancem ment, individual mobility, sociaal creativity, soci al competition, ssocial comparisoon, group status, group structurre, group identificcation, intergrouup inequality, inteergroup conflict, sociocultural beliefs, b group leggitimacy, group stability, group ppermeability, group boundaaries, group dynaamics, group meembership, low sstatus group, higgh status group, lower/ higher staatus groups Sciientific/ Secular Fundamentalism Scientific belieefs/orientation, ssecular beliefs/orrientation, humaanistic beliefs/orientaation, fundamenttalis*, dogmatic, dogmatism, scieentism Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 3 2.2 Databases A As shown in Table T 2, the fo ollowing datab bases were the most relevaant for searching the scienttific litterature relev vant to social identity i theorry (i.e., sociosstructural beliiefs, identity m management sttrategies, and intergroup peerceptions) an nd scientific fundamentalis f sm. Ta able 2. Data abases D Database Deescription P PsycINFO The PsycINFO dattabase is a collection of electronically stored bib liographic refereences, often withh maries, to psychoological literaturee from the 1800ss to the present. The available litterature absstracts or summ inccludes material published p in 50 countries, c but is all a presented in English. Books and chapters puublished woorldwide are alsoo covered in the database, as weell as technical reeports and disseertations from thhe last sevveral decades. JJSTOR JSTOR is a not–for–profit service that t allows individuals to accesss a wide range oof content in a truusted diggital archive of ovver one thousannd academic jourrnals and other sscholarly contennt. JSTOR includdes a varriety of topics inccluding economiics, history, polittical science, socciology, and psyychology, as well as othher key fields in the t humanities and a social servicces. G Google S Scholar A specialty s search engine maintainned by Google which w contains a cademic articless and presentatioons. Maany disciplines are represented, and sources incclude articles, boooks, and abstraacts from academ mic publishers, professsional societies, online repositories, universities,, and other web sites. Google sccholar atteempts to rank doocuments basedd on the full text of each documeent, where it wass published, whoo it was wriitten by, how reccently it was cited, and how oftenn it was cited in other scholarly lliterature 2.3 Selec ction of Articles A The databases were systemaatically search hed using the core conceptts specified abbove. We alsoo iddentified articcles cited in th he reference liists of the artiicles obtainedd for the revieew on the bassis of thheir potential relevance to the t key questions. The reseearch team reeviewed the ar articles generaated frrom the search hes and scann ned each for relevance. r On nce a number of potentiallyy suitable articles w were identified d (approximattely 100), we reviewed thee abstracts andd selected thoose that addreessed thhe core concep pts on social identity and scientific s fund damentalism and gave prioority to the arrticles thhat were mostt recently pub blished (since 2000). 2.4 Struc cture of th he Report The report is broken b down into i two majo or parts. The first f part conssists of those articles that w were foound relating to understand ding the dynaamics of sociaal identity beliiefs. This parrt of the reporrt is fuurther divided d into four secctions addresssing each of th he key questiions. These caan be found inn chhapters 4, 5, 6, 6 and 7. The second part consists c of tho ose articles reelating to the sscientific stattus of thhe concept of scientific fun ndamentalism m. A summary y is provided rregarding the status of the cooncept. This summary s can be found in chapter c 9. Paage 4 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 3. Social Identity y and S Social Identiity Manage M ement 3.1 De efinitions and Relev vant Theo oretical Fra ameworks s Human bein ngs are essentially social and, a as a resullt, belong to a number of ssocial categorries or groups. Succh belonging is in some caases voluntaryy (e.g., husbannd, mayor, Jeewish, Canadiian) and in other cases involuntary (e.g., ( female, black, “babyy boomer”). E Either way, social identificaation occurs wheen individualss associate witth a valued grroup, accept aand integrate the values annd norms of the group p, and believee that membership is an im mportant elem ment of their seelf-definitionn. Social identity is a product of our o relationshiip to a group and the adherrence to its paarticularities. It differs from one’s personal iden ntity, which can c be definedd as our uniquue traits or chharacteristics ((e.g., neuroticism m, extraversion n) that disting guishes us wiithin the groupp. Social idenntities, rather,, emerge when the an nswer to the question q ‘Who am I?’ derivves from an aassociation with internalizeed group membership p and a subseequent manifeestation of thee group’s valuues and norm ms (Haslam, Elllemers, Reicher, Reeynolds, and Schmitt, S 2010 0). The emph asis here is beelonging to annd integration with others and collective c belliefs, attitudess and actions to form our sself-concept. Central to social s identity y is its functio onal value or ““membershipp has its privilleges”. For exxample, Brewer (19 999) argues th hat social iden ntity satisfies our longing ffor social incllusion. She staates that the “expecttations of cooperation and security prom mote positive attraction tow ward other inggroup members an nd motivate adherence a to ingroup i norm ms of appearannce and behavvior that assurre that one will be reco ognized as a good g or legitim mate ingroupp member” (p.. 433). As succh, members rreceive privileges or o entitlementts that others do not. Peoplle can predictt their social eenvironment m more effectively and also locaate themselvess in relation too others withhin the social sspace (Kundaa, 1999). Social identity theorists Tajfel and Tu urner (1986) aalso emphasizze the functioonal value of bbelonging to social groups. They hold that identtification withh valued sociaal groups enhhances an indiividual’s social stand ding, self-esteeem, and positive identity. A key assum mption of sociaal identity theeory (SIT)1, therrefore, is that people are geenerally motivvated to mainntain a positivve self-conceppt, which can be acco omplished by invoking a po ositive sociall identity. However, forming f and maintaining m a positive soci al identity is often dependdent on ingrouup bias, i.e., favouraable comparissons showing g the positive ddistinctivenesss of one’s inggroup relativee to the outgroup on n some relevaant dimension n or characterristics (Hewsttone, Rubin & Willis, 20022). According to Brewer (19 999, p. 433-4), “[s]ymbolss and behaviors that differeentiate the inggroup from local outgro oups become particularly important…to i o reduce the rrisk that ingrooup benefits w will be inadvertenttly extended to outgroup members, m and to ensure thaat ingroup mem mbers will recognize one’s own entitlement e to o receive beneefits. Assimillation within aand differentiiation between groups is thus mutu ually reinforccing, along wiith ethnocentrric preferencee for ingroup interactions aand institutions.” Through so ocial compariison, group m members may perceive them mselves to havve an Subsequent refinements em mphasized self-caategorization theeory (SCT; e.g. TTurner, Hogg, O Oakes, Reicher aand Wetherell, 19887) in order to emphasize the intragroup processses as well as thhe intergroup prrocesses relevannt to identity. These two theeories, social ideentity theory (Tajjfel and Turner, 1986) and self-ccategorization theory have sincee been integrated intoo the social identity approach (e.g., Haslam, 20001). 1 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 5 addvantaged staatus on specifi fic dimensionss in relation to o an outside ggroup. In this way, one’s ggroup iddentity gains a particular sttatus (e.g., hig gh status vs. low l status). R Research has shown s that ing group biases can c occur in response r to booth real distinnctions (e.g., one is m more likely to favour memb bers of one’s social s groups) as well as inn response to arbitrary diistinctions. Fo or example, seminal sociall psychological research byy Tajfel (19788) showed thaat even diividing group ps on the basiss of an arbitraary distinction n (e.g., eye coolour – minim mal group paraadigm) caan give rise to o ingroup vs. outgroup disttinctions. Deffinitions of onneself versus others are inttended too be “relationaal and compaarative” (Tajfeel, 1978), as they t are most meaningful iin comparisonn to otther categoriees (e.g., the caategory “youn ng” receives much m of its m meaning in oppposition to the caategory “old””). Events or occurrences o th hat increase th he salience off social categoorization proccesses caan enhance in ngroup biases (Turner & Haslam, H 2001). A According to Tajfel T and Turrner (1979; ciited in Postmees and Bransccombe, 2010)), ingroup biaas is not neecessarily seeen as an inevittable outcome of social categorization, but relies on the followingg 3 coonditions: x x x The deegree to which h identificatio on with the in ngroup definees one’s self-cconcept; The ex xtent to which h the context allows a for com mparison andd competitionn between groups; and The peerceived relev vance of the comparison c ou utgroup (in teerms of both rrelative and abbsolute status)). Iff these 3 cond ditions are meet, they argue,, categorizatio on or compariison processees may lead too inngroup bias. It is believed, therefore, thaat one can pro otect or even eenhance theirr sense of worrth and beelonging by psychological p lly amplifying g similarities among ingrouup members aand/or emphaasizing ouutgroup differrences (Turneer, Oakes, Haaslam & McG Garty, 1994). H However, bolstering one’s social s identity y may come at a a cost. Reseearch shows ssuch ingroup faavouritism can n be sufficien nt to elicit disccriminatory behaviour b andd attitudes in ffavour of the inngroup at the expense of th he outgroup (T Turner & Hasslam, 2001). F For example, viewing the inngroup as morrally superiorr to outgroupss can facilitatee discriminatiion or hostilitties (Brewer, 1999). Inngroups also develop d stereo otypes of outg groups. Stereeotypes repressent our beliefs regarding tthe chharacteristics,, attributes, an nd behaviourss of others (H Hilton & von H Hippel, 1996)), and are bassed on thhe knowledgee, beliefs, and expectations we hold abou ut a group (K Kunda, 1999). They can guide our exxpectations ab bout group membership m an nd can colour (more often nnegatively) hhow we interppret ouutgroup mem mbers’ behavio ours and traitss (Kunda). Co onsidering thee context of inngroup biasinng, inntergroup con nflict is especiially amplified when group p identities coome under thrreat or when coompeting for scarce resourrces (Brewer; Fisher, 2000). Research aalso shows thaat under condditions off uncertainty, those who sttrongly identify with their ingroup are m more likely too derogate an ouutgroup (McG Gregor, Haji, & Kang, 200 08). So those who w feel goodd about their own group sttatus m may make outg group membeers feel badly about theirs. Indeed, reseaarch shows thhat low status groups seee their own group g less possitively than members m of high h status grooups (Brown,, 1978, 1984; Brown & Wade, 1987 7). Im mportantly, so ocial identity theory also considers how w group identiification can sshape social cchange, esspecially wheen addressing a negative so ocial identity. Considered ffrom an inverrse perspectivve, i.e., soocial compariisons yielding g unfavourablle results (e.g.., an ingroup sees itself as inferior to ann ouutgroup on reelevant dimen nsions), accord ding to SIT, group g membeers can adopt strategies to allleviate negative attribution ns stemming from affiliation with a neggatively consttructed groupp. In SIIT, ingroup id dentification is i described as a a variable that t mediates the relation bbetween the Paage 6 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® sociostructu ural variabless and identity managementt strategies (e..g., Mummenndey, Kessler et al., 1999; Mum mmendey, Klin nk et al., 1999 9). When stattus relations aare perceived as unstable aand illegitimatee, dominant grroup memberrs should expeerience a decrrease in ingrooup identificaation whereas no on-dominant members m shou uld experiencce an increasee in ingroup iddentification. Understood d as identity management m strategies, s Tajjfel and Turner (1986) idenntify three – iindividual mobility, so ocial creativitty, and social competition. However, thhe strategy grooup members adopt is dependent on o perception ns of sociostru uctural beliefs fs. Thus, it is iimportant to ddefine these hhere before describing the t identity management m strategies. s There are th hree sociostru uctural variab bles that are prrominent in thhe psychologgical literaturee (Tajfel & Turner, 198 86), including g: x x x Sta ability of statu us: the extent to which grouup positions aare static and unchanging oover time; Leg gitimacy of status: the exteent to which hhigh and low status groupss accept the sttatus stru ucture as bein ng valid; and Perrmeability of group bound daries: the exttent to which group membeers can leave a group and d become partt of another group. g As the mod del in Figure 1 illustrates, adopting a an iddentity managgement strateggy derives froom perceptionss of these sociiostructural beliefs. Figure 1. The role r of socio ostructural beliefs in id dentificatio on and identtity management m t strategies s (Tajfel & T Turner, 1986 6) For examplle, individual mobility refeers to the movve from one ggroup to anothher group to ppromote social status. However, for f this to occcur, group bouundaries musst be permeable. Social creativity, on the other haand, is a strateegy employed d when groupp boundaries aare viewed ass impermeablee, but group status is relatively y stable. Even n though ingrooup members are not able tto move beyoond the group, they y increase theiir own distincctiveness by m making creativve comparisoons between thheir own group and the t outgroup (e.g., ( making positive evalluations of the ingroup bassed on atypicaal dimensionss or devaluing g the importan nce of charactteristics that rreflect poorlyy on the ingrouup). This strategy esssentially requires redefinin ng the comparrative contextt by introducing new dimennsions of comparison n, flaunting in ngroup traits, and shifting tto new compaarison groupss to bolster grooup identity stattus (Ellemers, van Rijswijk k, Roefs, & S Simons, 1997)). In responsee to impermeaability or Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 7 w when status ineequality is seeen as unstablee, groups can employ sociaal competitionn. This strategy reeflects collecttive action on the part of diisadvantaged group membbers (van Zom meren, Postmees and Sppears, 2008). The strategy employed is a result of thee perceptionss of sociostrucctural beliefs for hiigh and low status s groups. The model also suggests that t ingroup iidentification mediates the reelation betweeen the sociosttructural variaables and iden ntity managem ment strategiees (e.g., M Mummendey, Kessler et al., 1999; Mummendey, Klin nk et al., 19999). W Work by Blanzz, Mummendey, Mielke, and Klink (199 98) has gone beyond the m model createdd by Tajfel and Turn ner (1986) an nd suggested that t the strateegies employeed by individuuals to managge their soocial identity can be classiffied along two orthogonal dimensions. The first axiss in their taxonomy iss termed “Chaange of Comp parison Param meters”. Here they assess w whether or nott a strategy aiims to chhange charactteristics aboutt the ingroup or the outgro oup. For exam mple, social coompetition dooes not aiim to change the characteristic of eitherr group, but raather the relattive positions of the groups. A Assimilation, on o the other hand, h aims to change the co omposition off both the inggroup and the ouutgroup. The second dimen nsion they pro opose is referrred to as the “Response M Mode”, which diistinguishes between b strateegies at the beehavioural lev vel (e.g., alterring one’s behhaviour in ordder to im mprove their relative r statuss) versus those at the cogniitive level (e.gg., changing oone’s attitudee about thhe importancee of the characcteristics on which w the gro oups differ). B Based on thesee dimensions, Blanz ett al. hypothesize six catego ories of identiity managemeent strategies,, which are ouutlined in Tabble 3. T Table 3. The 3 × 2 taxo onomy of identity mana agement strrategies folllowing neg gative social s identiity (Blanz ett al., 1998, p 722) Noote. Table 3 showss assignments of identity managem ment strategies to the t cells of the moodified taxonomy. Change of Compaarison Paarameters differenntiates responses to t negative social identity accordingg to their specific cchanges in the com mparison subject ((the inggroup) and/or the comparison objecct (the outgroup). Response R Mode considers c whether strategies are eithher predominantlyy beehaviour-related or primarily cognitioon-oriented. Each cell of the taxonomy contains the laabel of the respecttive cluster (underrlined woords) as well as thhe single strategiess assigned to each cluster. The cell Behavioural Channge of Comparisoon Object has been derived theeoretically based on o the results of thhe present study. Itt should be no oted that the strategies listeed go beyond those discusssed by Tajfel and Turner (1986) annd are known n by various names n across the t various reesearch prograams. Howeveer, each strateegy lissted by Blanzz et al. (1998)) can be discu ussed as a form m of one of thhe three strateegies suggesteed by Tajfel and Turn ner. For exam mple, employiing a new com mparison grouup can be thoought of as a fform of soocial creativitty or superord dinate recateg gorization can n be understoood as an indivvidual mobilitty sttrategy (see Kessler K and Mummendey, M 2002 2 and Horrnsey and Hoggg, 2002 in C Chapter 4). Throughout thee report, iden ntity managem ment strategiess will be categorized, as beest possible, acccording to Tajfel and Turn ner’s three brroad descriptiions – individdual mobility, social creativvity, annd social com mpetition. H Having describ bed in brief th he critical asp pects of sociall identity (how w it arises, hoow it is bolsteered, ettc.) and relevaant theoreticaal frameworkss, we can now w examine thee current scienntific literaturre Paage 8 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® pertaining to t the role of sociostructural beliefs in i dentity manaagement strateegies and inteergroup perceptionss. The followiing investigattion looks speecifically at how high and low status grooup members un nderstand thee intergroup sttructure, and how the diffeerence in understanding influences social identtity and manaagement strateegies to mainttain, promotee or change soocial identities. It was also pertineent to uncoverr any research h that looked at the impactt of behaviourrs associated with identity maanagement strategies had on social identtification andd the sociostruuctural beliefss. Finally, this investig gation consid dered the psycchological meethodologies uused to manippulate and meeasure sociostructu ural beliefs an nd managemeent strategies. Also, as partt of this reporrt, the scientiffic status of the concept of secularr and scientifiic fundamentaalism was revviewed, includding identifyiing existing meeasures related d to these con nstructs. The final chapter also investigaates how a seecular, st world view scientific orr humanistic fundamentali f w might influeence health annd well-beingg as well as cognition n. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 9 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 10 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 4. How do o socio ostructtural be eliefs o of stab bility, le egitima acy, an nd perm meabillity inflluence e so ocial id dentity y mana ageme ent stra ategies s for high and low status s group ps? (4..7.3) Sociostructtural beliefs in nfluence the identity i manaagement strateegies memberrs of high andd low status group ps use to main ntain or bolstter their sociall identity in ccomparison too other groupss. And there is evid dence that beliefs about the legitimacy, stability, andd permeabilityy of the interggroup structure will differ betw ween high and d low status ggroups (Verkuuyten & Reijeerse, 2008). Foor example, when w group bo oundaries are construed as permeable, m members from m high status ggroups members, may be threeatened by thee prospects off expanded paarticipation inn their group by outgroup m whereas people in low sttatus groups may m see this aas an opportuunity for upwaard social mobility. When the status structurre is seen as sttable and legiitimate, membbers from higgh status grouups may feel secure in their positiion. Those fro om low statuss groups, on tthe other handd, may perceiive less opportunity y for collectiv ve action. As such, s high annd low status ggroups may uuse different sstrategies to manage their t social id dentity concerrns. The researcch reviewed in n this chapterr considers hoow sociostrucctural beliefs oof stability, leegitimacy, and permeaability influen nce social iden ntity strategiees to manage threats to soccial standing ffor high and low staatus groups. The T following sections con sider researchh that specificcally examinees each sociostructu ural belief. Th he first section looks at thee impact perceeptions of staability have onn identity managemen nt strategies, the t second seection looks aat the influencce of legitimacy, and the thhird permeabilitty. In cases where w research hers examine m more than onne sociostructuural belief in the same study, the article a is categ gorized accord ding to the m most predominnant variable. It should alsoo be noted that many researchers r usse terms and concepts c otheer than those uused by Tajfeel and Turner (1986), i.e., individ dual mobility, social creativ vity, and sociial competitioon, to describee identity mannagement strategies (ee.g., Blanz et al., 1998). As best possiblle, we subsum me these undeer Tajfel and T Turner’s classificatio on system. 4.1 Stability and d Identity Managem ment Strattegies A number of o recent stud dies have look ked at the imppact stability hhas on social identity and managemen nt strategies in nvoked when n the status is unstable. Forr example, in a series of stuudies, Nadler and Halabi (2006 6) examined low l status grooup members’ willingness to seek and rreceive help from high h status meembers, and tested whetherr their willinggness is depenndent on the eextent to which they perceive stattus differences as being staable, their level of ingroup identity, andd the kind of help thatt is offered. They T argued th hat, in helpingg relations, thhere are inhereently unequall status relations su uch that the heelper has suffficient resourcces to offer thhe recipient annd the recipieent is reliant on th he goodwill of o the helper. This inequaliity, however, can be potenttially threatenning to those who are a on the recceiving end off help (Nadlerr & Fisher, 19986; cited in N Nadler & Hallabi). Research on n helping relaations has sho own that indivviduals can reespond negatiively to help w when they feel threatened (Nadler & Fisher, 198 86; cited in Naadler & Halabbi). Previous research show ws that recipients of o help tended d to evaluate themselves t m more negativelly, if the helper was from a dominant group g (e.g., Haalabi, 2003, as a cited in Naddler & Halabi). Nadler andd Halabi sugggest that accepting help h in this cirrcumstance may m reinforce an existing sttatus differennce. Low statuus Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 11 reecipients, they y continue, may m be threaten ned as a resullt. However, the kind of hhelp offered caan alter thhe perception of threat (Naadler, 1997, 19 998; cited in Nadler N & Hallabi). A According to Nadler N and Haalabi (2006), dependency-o oriented help consists of ooffering the reecipient thhe full solution to a problem m, implying the t recipient has h difficulty resolving thee problem on their ow wn. On other hand, autono omy-oriented help consists of offering a partial solutiion to a probllem, suuch as giving hints or instrructions, and this t implies th hat the recipieent has the tools and skillss to soolve the probllem on their own o with min nimal assistance (Brickmann et al., 1982; cited in Nadler & H Halabi). Based d on these find dings, Nadlerr and Halabi predicted p that when low staatus group meembers arre offered dep pendency-orieented help, bu ut not autonom my-oriented hhelp from higgher-status meembers, thhey may perceeive the help to be in confllict with theirr motivation too attain equall status. Thosse who iddentify more strongly s with their ingroup p tend to respo ond to identitty threat moree defensively,, for exxample, show wing more identification wiith their threaatened ingroupp, more ingrooup favouritissm, m more stereotyp ping against th he outgroup (Ellemers, ( Speears, & Doosjje, 1999; cited in Nadler & H Halabi), and viiew ingroups and outgroup ps as more homogenous (R Rothgerber, 19997, as cited iin N Nadler & Halaabi). Nadler an nd Halabi preedicted that lo ow status mem mbers who higghly identify with thheir ingroup would w be more reluctant to seek and receive help from m higher-statuus members coompared to th hose who weaakly identify with w their ingroup, but onlyy when the heelp is dependdencybuut not autonom my-oriented. Moreover, th hey argued thaat this would be impacted when low staatus m members perceeived status reelations as un nstable, makin ng them relucctant to seek oor receive deependency-orriented help from fr high status members. A summary oof their interggroup helpingg reelations modeel is presented d in Figure 2. Figure 2. Intergroup helping rellations as affected a by p perceived le egitimacy a and stability of o power re elations betw ween group ps (Nadler, 2 2002; cited in Nadler a and Halabi, 2006,, p. 99) Paage 12 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® In general, Nadler and Halabi H (2006) expected low w status membbers to experiience social iddentity threat when n they perceiv ved status diffferences as unnstable and w when their deppendence on hhigh status members fo or help is inco onsistent with h their goal too improve theiir social posittion. They exppected low status members m to reespond to identity threat byy expressing low affect annd by striving to g against andd devaluing thhe high status outgroup, positively distinguish d theemselves by discriminating d and by percceiving ingrou up and outgro oup members as more hom mogeneous (Sttudies 1 and 22). They also expected that this paattern of findiings would hoold for high bbut not low iddentifiers (Stuudy 3). Finally, low w status memb bers who high hly identify w with their ingrroup, they arggued, would bbe the least likely to waant help from high status member m whenn status relatioons are seen aas unstable annd dependency y-oriented hellp is offered (Study ( 4). Low w status, highh ingroup idenntifiers may bbe more willing to reject help and d rely more on n their own reesources to m manage identitty threat. Givven Nadler and Halabi’’s study 4 buiilds on the fin ndings from S Studies 1-3 annd enables a fu full test of the predictions of the intergrroup helping model illustraated above, oonly Study 4 is described inn detail. 6) used a 2 (high ingroup iddentification vs. control) × 2 (stable vs.. unstable Nadler and Halabi (2006 relations) between-partic b cipants design n for their fouurth study. Paarticipants weere 56 high schhool students fro om a midsize town in north hern Israel. Inn the first partt of the study,, the experimenter was introduced to participantts as an emplo oyee of the Issraeli Ministryy of Educatioon and told theem that they were taking part in a study exam mining differennt forms of pssychometric ttesting involvving assessments of verbal an nd analytical abilities. a Partiicipants weree told that theiir high schooll and another hig gh school, thatt was prestigiious and reputtable, were seelected to reppresent northeern Israel. Participantss were then prresented with h the status staability manipuulation. In thee stable conddition, participantss were told th hat comparativ ve analyses ovver the past 5 years showeed that, in genneral, their school had performed worse than the prestigious s chool on num merous criteriaa (e.g., entry into selective un niversity prog grams). In the unstable conndition, particcipants were ttold that compparative analyses ov ver the past 5 years showed d that the gap between theiir school and the more prestigious school was becoming naarrower. Participants were then presenteed with the iddentity manipuulation. As part of the cover c story th hat the study was w about psyychometric teesting, particippants were assked to read a shortt article that they t would latter be tested oon to assess thheir verbal abbilities. In thee high ingroup ideentification co ondition, partiicipants read an article aboout their high school that w was allegedly pu ublished in th he local comm munity newsppaper. The artiicle describedd the history oof their school and praised the scchool, remark king on past aand current stuudents’ comm mitment to thee school. In the contrrol condition, participants read r an articl e about envirronmental issuues. Afterwarrds, participantss were asked to t answer queestions pertainning to the arrticle and alsoo completed manipulatio on check quesstions. Following this, t Nadler and a Halabi (20 006) told partticipants that their interactiive analytic skills would be asssessed, but in n actuality their willingnesss to seek helpp was assesseed. They weree given 5 index cardss with math prroblems and were w requiredd to solve eacch of them in 660 seconds. T Two of the problems were w easy to so olve, whereass the other thrree were unsoolvable. Beforre participantts moved on to the “interactive” paart of the stud dy, they were asked to pickk one of threee choices for eeach of the problems th hat they were unable to sollve: (a) Not wanting help from a student s who aattended the oother school aand is also woorking on the same problem m (i.e., avoid dance of seekiing help); (b) Wanting the solution to th he problem frrom the other student (i.e., seeking depeendencyorieented help); or o (c) Wanting a hiint from the other o student tto help them ssolve the probblem (i.e., seeeking auttonomy-orientted help). Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 13 Foor each of theese options, th he number off times the parrticipant seleccted each of thhe responses was coomputed so th hat scores can n range from 0 (i.e., never choosing thiss option) to 3 (i.e., choosinng this opption for all th hree unsolvab ble problems)). Prreliminary an nalyses showeed that the staability and ideentification m manipulations were successful. N Nadler and Haalabi (2006) co onducted a tw wo-way ANOVA was condducted on avooidance of hellpseeeking, and reesults showed d that there waas a significan nt interactionn between stabbility and iddentification. Follow-up an nalyses showeed that particiipants who highly identifieed with their inngroup were more m likely to o avoid asking g for help wheen status relattions were unnstable. Particcipants inn the control condition c show wed no differrences in theirr tendency to avoid help-seeking betweeen the sttable and unsttable conditio ons. For depen ndency-orientted help-seekking, a t-test w within each iddentification condition c wass conducted because b at leasst one cell hadd a value of zzero. Results shhowed that peeople who hig ghly identified d with their in ngroup soughht more depenndency-help w when sttatus relationss were stable versus v unstab ble. No differeences were obbserved betw ween the two sstability coonditions amo ong those in the t control co ondition. Baseed on the finddings for avoiddance and deependency-orriented help, it i would not be b possible fo or Nadler and Halabi to finnd a significannt innteraction for the autonomy y-oriented help because paarticipants weere required too select one oof the thhree kinds of help. h A Across the fou ur studies, Nad dler and Halaabi (2006) fou und support foor their predicctions. Their findings indicaated that low status s membeers are reluctaant to seek annd receive helpp from high sstatus m members when n the relationss between thee two groups are a perceivedd as unstable aand when helpp is deependency-orriented. And this t is most ch haracteristic of o low status m members whoo highly idenntify w with their ingro oup. Furtherm more, under conditions of unstable u statuus relations, loow status mem mbers reeported less positive affectt, more likely to show discrrimination aggainst high staatus outgroupp m members (or more m ingroup favouritism), more likely to t hold negatiive evaluationns about the ouutgroup, and more likely to o perceive thee outgroup ass more homoggenous, but onnly when the high sttatus outgroup p offered help p. O One could argu ue that receiv ving help from m a high statuss member maay be particulaarly threateniing to loow status mem mbers’ social identity, becaause it may bee seen as reinnforcing the exxisting statuss sttructure, If seeeking to imprrove their social position frrom within, loow status highh identifiers m may reeject help and d express greaater ingroup faavouritism (as Nadler and Halabi’s resuults suggests,)), thhereby promo oting their own n group. Nadler and Halab bi’s (2006) ressults suggest that when help is offfered, low status high iden ntifiers engag ge in social co ompetition esppecially whenn the social sttructure is vieewed as unstab ble. O On the other haand, offering dependency--oriented help p to a low stattus group mayy be a way for high sttatus members to maintain their social standing s when n confronting challenges frrom low statuus ouutgroups. Nad dler, Harpaz-G Gorodeisky, and a Ben-David (2009) refeer to this as ddefensive helping, i.ee., “help that is proffered to t a member of o an outgroup p that poses a threat to thee ingroup’s staatus annd that is used d with the purrpose of mitig gating that thrreat” (p. 823)). They argue that defensivve heelping enablees the threaten ned high statu us group to re--establish its ppositive distinnctiveness, thhereby m maintaining the existing soccial order. Because defensiive helping iss used to neutr tralize social iidentity thhreat and man nage challengees to identity,, Nadler et al.. explain that threatened hiigh status grooup m members should offer defen nsive helping irrespective of o recipients’ needs. Defennsive helping should inn theory prom mote dependen ncy. Howeverr, this is a risk ky strategy, beecause pre-em mptively offerring heelp might sign nal to low staatus recipientss that they aree viewed as innadequate andd incompetennt by hiigh status gro oup members (Nadler et al., 2009). Paage 14 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Across threee studies, Naadler and colleeagues (2009)) showed thatt defensive heelping was ussed to avert Study 1) or amelioraate social iden ntity threat, ev voked by an eequal status group in a minnimal group (S and a real-g group (Study 2) context. Th he findings frrom their firstt two studies generally suppported defensive helping h as a sttrategy to deaal with threatss to social idenntity. In Studdy 1, the greattest amount of help h was prov vided to the outgroup whenn participantss identified with the ingrouup and when the ou utgroup threaatened their so ocial identity.. Also in Studdy 1, after parrticipants werre given the opportu unity to offer help h to their source s of threeat, they expreessed less neeed to derogatee the outgroup. Defensive D help ping served as a a strategy too maintain poositive distincctiveness. Naddler et al.’s second d study replicated the findiings from theiir first study, using real grooups. The greeatest offering waas given to thee target that was w threateninng to participaants’ social iddentity. The rresults also indicated th hat when partiicipants’ sociial identity waas threatened,, those who hhighly identifyy with the ingroup ten nded to use deefensive helpiing regardlesss of whether tthe help targeet needed assistance. The furtherr finding that collective (an nd not personaal) self-esteem m moderated the associatioon between ideentity threat and a helping behaviour provvides evidencce that the diffferent levels of defensive helping h show that individuaals’ social ideentities are beeing threateneed to differentt degrees. Nadler et all. (2009) also examined deefensive helpiing in an interrgroup contexxt, specificallyy to determine if i an unstable social contex xt will lead hiigh status grooup members to offer moree dependency y-oriented hellp to a lower--status group tto manage id entity threats. Again, a thrreat to the existing soccial order mig ght elicit moree dependencyy-oriented hellp than autonoomy-orientedd help from the high staatus group. In n addition, Nadler et al. expplored the role of social caategorization pprocesses, such that when w a commo on identity beetween the inggroup and outtgroup is induuced, high staatus members sh hould be less threatened th han when the ttwo groups arre induced to have unique or separate ideentities. They y thought that a common iddentity shouldd elicit more aautonomy-oriiented help, whereeas separate id dentities shou uld elicit moree dependencyy-oriented hellp to essentiallly maintain diistinctiveness. Because Naadler and collleagues’ (2009) third studyy examined thhe effects of uunstable status relations on defensiv ve helping and d the use of defensive d helpping by high sstatus group m members to m manage social identtity threat, wee have describ bed the methoodology in deetail. The thirdd study was a 2 (stable vs. unstablee status hierarrchy) × 3 (sep parate identitiies vs. commoon identity vss. control) bettweensubjects dessign. Participants were 92 students whoo attended a pprestigious higgh school in IIsrael and were told th hat they woulld be part of a research proogram to deveelop new psycchological toools to be used in the Defence Forcces’ unit. Theey were also ttold that the reesearch was cconducted bettween pairs of hig gh schools in the t same region and that thhe pairs were selected at raandom. The sstudents learned thatt the school th hey were pairred with was iinferior in theeir academic rreputation annd less prestigious relative to th heir own schoo ol based on thhe rankings oof 10 educatorrs. p of study, Nadler et al. (2009) preseented participaants with the self-categorizzation In the first part manipulatio on. In the separate identities condition, participants rread about hoow their high school was uniquee relative to otther high scho ools in Israel.. In the comm mon identity coondition, theyy read about the sp pecial qualitiees of the high h schools in thheir region. Inn the control ccondition, theey read about a topic unrelated to high schoolls. Participantts then answeered some queestions based on what they read. In I the second part of the stu udy, Nadler eet al. presenteed participantss with the maanipulation for status sttability of inteergroup relatiions. In the sttable status coondition, partticipants weree given information n that the perfformance of th he lower-statuus outgroup hhas not changged over the ppast five years, wherreas in the unsstable status condition, theey were told tthat the performance of thee outgroup has been prrogressively im mproving oveer the past fivve years. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 15 Paarticipants weere then given n 14 analogy problems, 6 of o which weree marked shoowing that a sttudent frrom the other high school had h difficulty y solving the analogy. a Nadller et al. then informed paarticipants thaat this phase of o the study focused f on decision-makinng behaviour rrather than prroblemsoolving abilitiees, and so they y were presen nted with the correct answeer to each of tthe analogies. For eaach of the 6 marked m analog gies that the other o student had h difficultyy solving, partticipants had to deecide if they wanted w to pro ovide the stud dent with the full f solution ((i.e., dependenncy-oriented help), a hint that wou uld help the sttudent solve the t analogy (i.e., autonomyy-oriented hellp), or no helpp at all. N Nadler and collleagues (2009) found that when high sttatus memberrs’ social posiition was threeatened byy an unstable status contex xt, they offereed defensive helping h to thoose that posedd a threat. Thee effect off status stability on defensiive helping was w moderated d by the level of categorizaation. When sstatus reelations were perceived as unstable, high status mem mbers who werre induced to think about ttheir inngroup and th he outgroup ass distinct from m one anotherr (separate ideentities) tendeed to offer moore deependency-orriented help compared to th hose that werre induced to tthink that thee two groups w were siimilar (comm mon identity) or o those in thee control cond dition (Nadlerr et al.). Undeer unstable staatus reelations, high status members offered more m autonomy y-oriented heelp when theyy perceived a coommon identiity between th heir ingroup and a the lowerr-status outgrooup comparedd to those thaat peerceived uniq que identities and those in the t control co ondition. Baseed on these fiindings, it seeems thhat defensive helping can be b used by hig gh status grou ups to managee threats to thheir social identity. O Offering help when w the statu us context is unstable u serves to maintainn positive disstinctiveness ffor hiigh status gro oups and to maintain the staatus quo. Sccheepers (200 09) has also examined e the impact of stability on idenntity managem ment strategiees. In onne study, he considered c if threat or challlenge would be experiencced by high orr low status ggroups w when differencces in status were w stable veersus unstablee during interggroup compettition, and whhat sttrategies that might m provok ke. Past researrch has shown n that when loow status grooups are challeenged, thhey may engaage in function nal coping, which w can inclu ude maintain ing commitm ment to the grooup (E Ellemers, Speears, & Doosjee, 1997), exerrting more eff ffort to better tthe group’s social standingg (O Ouwerkerk, De D Gilder, & De D Vries, 200 00), and acting g to disconfirrm negative sttereotypical exxpectation (K Kray, Thompso on, & Galinsk ky, 2001; citeed in Scheepeers, 2009). For Scheepers, m members in low w status grou ups in stable conditions would be most llikely to experience threatss to thheir social ideentity, becausee they lack a strong positiv ve group distiinctiveness. H However, he beelieved this would w change under unstab ble conditions. He argued th that threats to social identitty may tuurn into a challlenge, because the unstablle condition may m present aan opportunityy to improve their sttatus. Functional coping might m be descriibed as sociall competition . In contrast, high status grroup m members may feel threateneed by status in nstability beccause they aree confronted w with a challennge by a low status group and as su uch their supeerior social staanding becom mes tenuous (ee.g., Bettencoourt, C Charlton, Doorr, & Hume, 2001). When status s is stablee, high status members weere not expectted to feeel challenged d because theiir position in the status stru ucture would be positive aand secure. To test his hyp potheses, Scheeepers (2009)) randomly asssigned 40 unndergraduate sstudents to a 2 (G Group Status: Low vs. Hig gh) × 2 (Statuss Stability: Un nstable vs. Sttable) design with stabilityy as the w within-subjectss factor. He used u three card diovascular in ndices to meaasure threat annd challenge, inncluding ventrricular contraactility (VC) (the ( force of heart h pumps);; cardiac outpput (CO) (amoount of bllood the heartt pumps in on ne minute); an nd total periph heral resistannce (TPR) (ressistance of thee arrterioles). Acccording to Scheepers, threaat is marked by b relatively llower cardiacc performancee (VC annd CO) and higher h vasculaar resistance (TPR), ( whereaas challenge iis marked by higher cardiaac peerformance an nd lower resistance. Senso ors for physiological recordding were putt on participannts and baaseline cardio ovascular resp ponses were recorded. r Paage 16 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Scheepers’ (2009) particcipants were told t that the s tudy examineed reasoning aand problem--solving ability. Theey were told th hat people wiith inductive oor deductive reasoning skiills were betteer problem solvers. Parrticipants com mpleted a reassoning test thaat supposedlyy categorized them as being better at either inducctive or deducctive reasonin ng, when, in ffact, they werre all told thatt they were innductive reasoners. Participants P were w then askeed to complette two problem m-solving tassks. The first tassk was a num mber-counting g task which w was used to m manipulate grooup status. Paarticipants were asked to count as quickly q as posssible the num mber of times a specific nuumber would aappear on the computer screen, and d they were to old that their pperformance would be bassed on the acccuracy of their respon nses. Those in n the low statu us condition w were told thatt their group ((i.e., inductivve reasoners) performed p wo orse on the task than did thhe outgroup (ii.e., deductivee reasoners). In the high status condition, paarticipants were told the oppposite. To innduce status sttability, particcipants were then told that the seecond task, a letter-countinng task, was vvery similar to the previouus task. They comp pleted a letter--counting task k which used a similar form mat to the firsst task exceptt they counted lettters instead of numbers. Peerformance onn the first tassk would be a good indicattor of how they would perform on the t second tassk. Participannts then movedd on to the thhird task, whicch was a word-findin ng task (see Blascovich, B Mendes, M Hunteer, Lickel, & K Kowai-Bell, 22001, as citedd in Scheepers, 2009, for a siimilar task). They T were tolld that this tassk was quite ddifferent in foormat than the numberr- and letter-co ounting taskss. As such, theeir performannce on the preevious two tassks would not be a goo od predictor of o how they would w do on tthis task. Thiss information was intendedd to give participantss the perceptio on that group p differences iin status woulld be unstablee during this final task. Participantss were given 5 minutes to find f as many words as posssible in a maatrix of letterss. Again, Scheepers’ (2009 9) main depen ndent measuree was the carddiovascular reeactivity (VC C, CO, TPR) durin ng the letter-co ounting and word-finding w tasks to assesss reactions too stable and uunstable status differrences respecctively. The nu umber of worrds that particcipants foundd during the w wordfinding task k was used ass a performancce measure. M Manipulation check items were adminisstered after each manip pulation, and the t results ind dicated that thhe manipulatiions were effeective. To testt the main hypotheses, the three carrdiovascular indices i were ssubmitted to a 2 (Group Sttatus: Low vss. High) × 2 (Status Sttability: Unstaable vs. Stablle) repeated m measures analyysis, with thee correspondinng physiologiccal measure frrom the first task t as a covaariate (to conttrol for indiviidual differennces). As shown in Figure 3, for alll three indicees, there was a significant interaction beetween Groupp Status and Status Stability. S Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 17 Figure 3. VC, CO, an nd TPR as a function of o group sta atus and sta atus stabilitty. (Sche eepers, 200 09, p. 231) Sppecifically, when w status differences werre stable, low status membbers showed loower cardiovascular reeactivity (VC and CO) and d more resistan nce (TPR), in ndicating thatt they felt threeatened. In coontrast, hiigh status mem mbers showed higher card diovascular reactivity and llower resistannce, indicatingg that thhey felt more challenged by y the situation n. This latter finding was nnot expected. However, furrther annalyses showeed that CO an nd TPR levelss were not sig gnificantly diffferent from zzero, meaningg that it iss difficult to discern d whether threat or ch hallenge was experienced. When status differences w were unnstable, low status s members felt more challenged c (m marked by highher VC and C CO, lower TP PR), w whereas high status s members felt more th hreatened (marked by low wer VC and CO O, higher TPR R). Low status gro oup members performed beetter on the word-finding w taask than did hhigh status grroup m members, but performance p was w not correelated with an ny of the cardiiovascular reaactivity indices. Sccheepers and Ellemers (20 005) argued th hat studies examining the iimpact of threeat on social iddentity, measu ure threat at one o point in time, typically y after a statuss manipulationn, and this maay reeflect responsses to the statu us quo rather than responsees to possiblee changes in ssocial status. A As suuch, they exam mined threat during d a perfo formance situaation. They prredicted that members of llow sttatus groups would w show a physiologicaal threat respo onse when evaaluating theirr inferior posiition (i.e., status quo o), whereas members m of high status grou ups would shhow a physioloogical threat reesponse when n evaluating a possible chan nge in their su uperior positiion (i.e., statuus loss). Theyy appplied Blasco ovich and Tom maka’s (1996; cited in Scheepers & Elleemers) bio-pssycho-social m model off arousal regu ulation to und derstand interg group relation ns. Past researrch using thiss model has shhown thhat there are validated v physsiological ind dices of threatt and challengge when indivviduals are reqquired too perform a taask, known ass “motivated performance p situations”. s F For this experiiment, Scheeppers annd Ellemers used u systolic blood b pressurre (SBP) and the t mean arteerial pressure (MAP) to meeasure thhreat responsees. The formeer indicates th he maximum pressure p in thhe artery after each heartbeeat, w whereas the lattter indicates the mean preessure in the artery a during a complete caardiac cycle. Paage 18 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Scheepers and a Ellemers (2005) used minimal m grouups for this stuudy. Status diifferences bettween the ingroup and d outgroup weere manipulatted by providding participannts with falsee feedback onn a group task. Participants were to old that their group either pperformed beetter (high staatus conditionn) or worse (low status condition) th han the outgro oup. Participan ants’ blood preessure was m measured beforre they completed the t group task k (baseline measure), m afterr they receiveed the feedbacck for the grouup task (response to o existing stattus differencees), and beforre an unexpeccted second roound of the grroup task (response to o perceived in nstability of status s quo). A second rounnd of the groupp task servedd as “renewed competition” c between b the ingroup and thhe outgroup. According too Scheepers annd Ellemers, th his introducess the possibiliity that the hi gh status grouup may lose ttheir superior position, leading them m to defend their t position against groupps that can thrreaten their sttatus. In contrrast, renewed co ompetition sho ould be less th hreatening to the low statuus group; afterr all, they havve “nothing to o lose”. In factt, Schmader and a Major (19999; cited in S Scheepers & Ellemers, 20005) suggest that, in such situ uations, low sttatus memberrs may disenggage from thee task if the grroups are not perceived as importaant or the task k is novel. Folllowing the reenewed comppetition, Scheepers and Ellemers asssessed ingrou up identification, personal self-esteem, and collectivve self-esteem m, before completing g the second ro ound of the taask. Results con nfirmed Scheeepers and Elleemers (2005) predictions. P Participants iin the low stattus condition sh howed higherr blood pressu ure after receiiving feedbacck about the ggroup task, whhereas those in thee high status condition c show wed higher b lood pressuree before a second round off the group task was an nnounced. Consistent with Scheepers (22009), low staatus members were more thhreatened by the statu us quo (stabiliity), whereas high status m members weree more threateened by potenntial status loss (instab bility). Scheep pers and Ellem mers also founnd that the staatus manipulaation did not aaffect personal self-esteem, bu ut did affect co ollective self--esteem. Speccifically, highh status membbers reported greeater collectiv ve self-esteem m than low staatus memberss. Results alsoo indicated thhat group identificatio on moderates the effects off status on thrreat responses, such that only those whoo highly identified with w their grou up showed a threat t responsse when they were not ablee to obtain a ppositive social identtity (in the low w status cond dition) or wheen there is the possibility thhat they may lose their positive soccial identity (iin the high staatus conditionn). Though hig gh and low staatus memberss can both expperience threaat in a perform mance situatioon, the results from m Scheepers (2009) and Sccheepers and E Ellemers (20005) show the basis of theirr threat responses differ. d Moreov ver, sociostru uctural beliefss can not onlyy affect outcom mes for attainning a more positiive social iden ntity, but they y can also affe fect individualls’ motivationnal processes that help them attain that goal. In cases where status s is perceeived as unstaable, lower sttatus groups w who exhibit incrreased functio onal coping (ee.g., socially ccompete) mayy better their social standinng, and hence their social identitty. Scheepers (2009) show wed that ingrouup identificattion and ingrooup bias can be expllained not onlly in terms off threat, but allso in terms oof challenge. Scheepers, Ellemers, and d Sintemaarteensdijk (20099) expanded oon their earlierr work to inveestigate further strattegies (e.g., collective c esteeem) to managge identity thhreat. They exxamined identtity threat among high h status memb bers using botth explicit sellf-report and pphysiologicall measures. A Again, they used Blasco ovich and Tom maka’s (1996 6; cited in Schheepers et al.)) bio-psycho-social model to measure ch hanges in physsiology as a result r of threaat, i.e., increasses in systolicc blood pressuure (SBP) and pulse pressure p (PP). They also measured diastoolic blood preessure (DBP)) because this is not consistently y related to sttress. In additiion, they expllored the assoociation betw ween affect andd blood pressure. nt, Scheepers et al. (2009) eexamined ressponses to soccial identity thhreat for For their firrst experimen high status members (staatus artificiallly created in tthe lab) invollving stable veersus unstablee status Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 19 coonditions. Parrticipants receeived false feeedback on a group g reactionn time task, w which categorrized thhem into a hig gher status gro oup. Status sttability was th hen manipulatted by inform ming participaants thhat they were required to do the task agaain, but with modified m stim mulus materiaals, and that thheir peerformance on the first task k was a stron ng (stable cond dition) or weaak (unstable ccondition) preedictor off the second task. t Participaants’ blood prressure readin ngs were takenn before and after the m manipulation. Then they com mpleted meassures that assessed positivee and negativve affect, as w well as coollective self--esteem. They y hypothesizeed that particip pants in the uunstable statuss condition w would shhow higher SB BP and PP co ompared to th hose in the staable conditionn. However, thhey believed that thhere would bee no difference in collective self-esteem m measures (a variable oftenn used to asseess soocial identity threat) for paarticipants in the t two condiitions, becausse participantss being in a reelatively high status group,, members in this status ten nd to respondd defensively.. M Manipulation checks c indicaated that particcipants correcctly identifiedd their group m membership (i.e., hoolistic) and th hat those in th he stable cond dition perceiveed more statuus stability thaan those in thhe unnstable condition. Reactiviity scores for blood pressu ure and pulse ppressure weree calculated bby suubtracting thee baseline score (average reeadings beforre the manipuulation) from tthe postexxperimental score s (averagee readings after the manipu ulation). Scheeepers et al. (22009) found ssome suupport for theeir hypothesess. Results sho owed that SBP P and PP weree higher for pparticipants inn the unnstable compared to stablee conditions. No N differencees between coonditions weree found for D DBP. R Results also in ndicated that th here were no differences in n positive andd negative afffect between coonditions. Ho owever, when specific emo otions were an nalyzed, Scheeepers et al. foound that paarticipants in the unstable condition feltt more upset and a hostile thhan participannts in the stablle coondition. Feelling upset waas also positiv vely correlated d with SBP annd PP, whichh supports theiir arrgument that SBP and PP are a indicatorss of threat. Mo oreover, posittive emotionss did not correelate w with SBP or PP P, providing further f support that the inccrease in bloood and pulse ppressure was nnot due too positive task k engagementt or general ex xcitement. Reesults on the m measure of coollective self--esteem inndicated that there t was no difference d bettween the two o conditions. Inn Experiment 2, Scheepers et al. (2009) used gender as the contexxt to examine how existingg or They focusedd on men, the higher chhanging interg group status relations r impaacts social ideentity threat. T sttatus group, in n response to women, the lower l status group. g Scheeppers et al. arguued that gendder reelations can be seen in sociiety as relativ vely unstable, with legislatiions and policcies supportinng eqqual rights forr men and wo omen, or as reelatively stablle, with such legislations aand policies having onnly limited im mpact within society. s Using g both self-report and physsiological meeasures, they prredicted that perceptions p of o status instab bility would be b associated with more deefensive and sttrategic conceerns, leading individuals i to o fail to reportt experiences of threat eveen when they show phhysiological indications i to the contrary.. They also prredicted that w when individuuals interact w with ouutgroup mem mbers, they wo ould experience more threaat to their soc ial identity, bbut that their exxperience of threat t may no ot be conveyed in self-repo ort measures. M Men and women participateed in either an n intragroup or o an intergrouup discussionn on one of thhree toopics: gender neutral, conservative, or progressive. p For the experim mental sessioons, the intraggroup coondition conssisted of two same-sex s partticipants, wheereas the interrgroup condittion consistedd of one m male and one female f and paarticipants weere randomizeed assigned too debate topiccs. The gender neeutral topic was w about legaalizing drugs. Participants were random mly assigned too take the oppposing orr defending position. The conservative c topic t was cen ntred on tradittional gender roles (womenn sttaying home to t care for a child, c rather th han the fatherr), so that menn would perceeive their stattus to bee stable. In th he intergroup condition, meen were asked d to defend thhis position annd women weere assked to opposse it. In the intragroup cond dition, particiipants were raandomly assiggned to defennd or opppose the possition. The pro ogressive topic (subsidies for childcare so that womeen can work) was Paage 20 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® designed to o make changiing gender relations saliennt. In the interrgroup condition, women w were asked to defend th he position an nd men were asked a to oppoose it. In the iintragroup conndition, particcipants were randomly assigned d to either defe fend or opposee the positionn. Blood presssure was takenn at the start of the study (to estaablish baseline) as well as bbefore and affter each debaate session. Sccheepers et al. also in ncluded meassures of modeern sexism (Sw wim, Aikin, H Hall, & Hunteer, 1995; citedd in Scheepers et e al.) and attiitudes toward d affirmative aaction to deteermine if partiicipants respoond strategically y to self-report measures of o gender relaations. They bbelieved that m modern sexism m would be greatest for men, wheereas affirmattive action woould be stronggly endorsed by women inn an intragroup versus v intergrroup context. Scheepers et e al. (2009) found f that meen showed thee highest PP dduring the proogressive topiic and women sho owed the high hest PP during g the conservaative topic. Thhe results alsoo indicated thhat men showed hig gher PP and SBP than wom men for the prrogressive toppic in the interrgroup contexxt, but there was no n difference between b men n and women iin the intragrooup conditionn. No effects were found for DBP. D For the modern m sexism m scale, menn expressed m more sexism thhan women inn the intragroup context, c but there was no difference d in tthe intergroupp context. Forr attitudes tow ward affirmative action, womeen were someewhat more liikely to suppoort affirmative action than men in the intragro oup context, th han the interg group contextt. According tto Scheepers and colleaguees, men appeared more m threateneed when they evaluated chaanging gendeer relations, shhown in their levels of PP and SBP P, which tend ded to be high her in the pressence of wom men, the low sttatus group, thhan in the presence off other men. This, T they argued, implies m men may perrceive themselves to be in ddanger of losing theirr high status position. p However, Scheepers S et al. a (2009) phy ysiology meassures did not parallel the self-report meeasures. In words, men n tended to su uppress their expression e off sexism in ann intergroup coontext compaared to an intragroup context. c Wheen women evaluated changging gender rrelations, theyy did not show w the same pattern as men. m This provides supportt that the physsiological chaanges in men were due to tthe possibility of o status loss rather than general arousaal due to unstaable group rellations. Wom men showed thee highest PP when w they disccussed the coonservative toopic in the preesence of menn (although this was not statistically different from m their PP whhen they weree discussing tthe progressivve topic). Women alsso expressed less l support for fo affirmativee action in thee intergroup tthan intragrouup context. Scheepers et e al (2009) sh howed that an n intergroup ccontext can inncrease physioological respoonses based on grroup status an nd the stability y of the statuss, and this, thhey argue, resuults from sociial identity threat. They y further show wed that in an n intergroup ccontext, indivviduals might be cagey withh respect to how one actually feelss. Men expresssed less sexiism whereas w women expreessed less suppport for affirmation n action in the presence of outgroup o mem mbers. Alterinng the expression of these attitudes may have served as a waay to avoid neegative evaluaations from ooutgroup mem mbers. Being sseen as a male chauv vinist or as a radical r feminiist implies cerrtain negativee stereotypicaal expectationns. People may be morre cautious an nd strategic when w it comess to articulatinng their feelinngs to others. Such an approach might m help to manage m one’ss social identiity in the facee of threat. Again, Scheepers et al. (2009) ( demon nstrate that higgh status grouups are more threatened byy the possibility that t they may y lose their su uperior status,, whereas low w status groupps are more thhreatened by the possibility that the status quo will w remain unnchanged. Mooreover, responsiveness too changes in status staability invokees different ap pproaches to m manage this, aand this is deppendent on w whether one belongss to a high staatus group or a low status ggroup. Scheeepers et al. shhowed that peoople’s feelings maay be inconsisstent with their attitudes annd perceptionns, and this coould be intentiionally strategic. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 21 O Other research h has looked at a the impact of o the percepttion of stabiliity and the funnctional use oof inngroup bias (ffavouritism) in maintaining g or promotin ng social identtity. Scheeperrs, Spears, Dooosje, annd Manstead (2006) argued d that ingroup p bias could serve s two funnctions. An ideentity-expressive fuunction encou urages group members m to voice v the valu ue of one’s grooup (e.g., boaasting), whereeas an innstrumental fu unction of ing group bias inv vokes change of a group’s relative sociaal position (e.g., soocial competition). They arrgued that thee status of thee group makess it more likely that a partiicular foorm of ingrou up bias might be favoured over o another. Specifically, for high statuus groups, thee fuunction of ing group bias is likely l identity y expression since s they havve no desire ffor social channge. O On the other haand, Scheepers et al. believ ve that memb bers of a low sstatus groups might use ingroup biias in order to o mobilize soccial change. And A they look ked at how staability impactted these two fuunctions. Sccheepers et all. (2006) argu ue that while high h status grroups who seee their conditiion as stable m may usse ingroup biaas as a meanss of identity expression, it is i unlikely thaat high statuss groups who see thhis condition as a unstable would w use ingrroup bias. Theey argue that this might aleert lower statuus grroups to the precariousnes p s of the situattion and mobiilize them to aaction. Sociall mobilizationn w would threaten n the high stattus group’s po osition. For lo ow status grouups, subtlety is important. In caases where th he conditions are a unstable and a their statu us can changee, low status ggroups shouldd be m more likely to use more sub btle forms of ingroup i bias so s as not to allert the higherr status groupp of thheir desire forr change. On the t other hand d, Scheepers et al. argue thhat when low w status groupss find thhemselves in stable s conditiions, where th heir status is unlikely u to chhange, they haave nothing too lose annd are thus more m likely to use u more extrreme forms of ingroup biass (i.e., identitty-expressive fuunction). Thesse assumption ns were exam mined in a seriies of studies.. Sccheepers et all. (2006) firstt assessed wheether or not group g status annd the stabilitty of this statuus m might determin ne the form of ingroup biass participants were likely tto exhibit in a minimal grooup paaradigm. Partticipants weree ostensibly assigned to a group g based oon their abilityy to estimate the nuumber of dotss on a screen. They were to old that there were two cattegories of peerceivers (globbal and deetailed). Scheeepers et al. to old all of theirr participants that they werre detailed peerceivers baseed on thheir previous task t performaance. They were further told that the grooup (global oor detailed peerceivers) wh ho performed best on the do ot estimation task would bbe entered intoo a draw for a reeward. Particiipants were giiven either neegative feedbaack about thee performancee of detailed peerceivers (low w status condiition) or posittive feedback k (high status ccondition). T They were thenn told thhat the perform mance in prev vious sessionss had fluctuatted a great de al from one rround to anothher (uunstable cond dition) or had remained stab ble from one round to anotther (stable coondition). Paarticipants in Scheeper’s et e al.’s (2006) study were also a told that, during the seecond sessionn, there w would be an op pportunity to receive feedb back that wou uld help their pperformance.. They wouldd decide foor other particcipants (both ingroup and outgroup) o how w much feedbback they wouuld receive. T This w was done using g Tajfel matriices that pitted four differin ng allocation strategies ag ainst one anoother. A m maximum join nt profit (MJP) strategy maaximizes the resources in b oth the ingrouup and outgrooup. A faairness (F) strrategy allocatees equal resou urces to the in ngroup and ouutgroup otherrs. A maximuum inngroup profit (MIP) strateg gy aims to maaximize the reesources alloccated to the inngroup and a m maximum diffe ferential (MD)) strategy max ximizes the reelative differeence betweenn the resources of thhe ingroup and outgroup ev ven at the pottential cost to the ingroup pprofit. Scheeppers et al. repoort the M MD is considered the most aggressive an nd most blatan nt form of inggroup bias. U Using the Tajfe fel m matrices, pull scores s were calculated c thatt represented material ingrroup bias (i.e.., the bias for prroviding moree material ressources to the ingroup rather than the ouutgroup). Sym mbolic ingrouup bias w was measured with six item ms representing either ingro oup favouritissm (e.g., “Thee detailed percceivers grroup is a supeerior group”) or outgroup derogation d (e..g., “Global pperceivers aree born losers”)). Paage 22 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Scheepers et e al. (2006) conducted c a 2 (status: highh vs. low) × 2 (stability: staable vs. unstabble) multivariatee analysis of variance v (MA ANOVA) to aassess both the symbolic inngroup bias annd the two material ing group bias strrategies (MIP and MD). Thhey found a significant twoo-way interacction between staatus and stability for both the t material ssymbolic ingrroup bias and material MIP P. Participantss showed the greatest amou unt of symboolic ingroup bias in the stabble, high statuus condition and showed th he greatest am mount of mateerial MIP in thhe unstable, loow status conndition. As such, ingrou up bias strateegies fulfilled different funnctions for thee different grooups. As preddicted, high status group ps in stable co onditions exh hibited ingrouup bias expresssively, whereeas low statuss groups in unstable co onditions used d ingroup biass instrumentallly. Scheepers and a colleaguees (2006) wan nted to investiigate the diffeerent functionns of ingroup bias more directly and d to investigatte the impact of the audiennce on this strrategic use. They argued thhat the audience fo or ingroup straategies mightt alter the funcction of the inngroup bias, eespecially whhen considering g the status an nd stability off the situationn. Scheepers eet al. argued thhat when ingrroup bias is visible to o members off the outgroup p, it is likely too have an ideentity-expressive function. Instrumentaal ingroup acttions in front of the outgrooup are counteerproductive, they argued, as it underscoress mobilization n, risking outtgroup mobiliization and coompetition (Ellemers et al.,, 2000; cited in Sch heepers et al, 2006). Imporrtantly, Scheeepers et al. bellieved that thhe awareness oof the audience with w regard to the t use of ing group favourittism would bbe most evidennt when the cconditions are unstablee. They explaained that, in the stable conndition, wherre change is uunlikely, grouups have little to losee by expressin ng instrumenttal ingroup biias in front off the outgroupp. They reasonned that ingroup meembers would d pay little atteention to wheether or not ouutgroup mem mbers were aroound so long as the ingroup bias served its insstrumental funnction of inciiting action w within the ingrroup. p were w randomlyy assigned to a 2 (communnication conteext: For their seecond study, participants intragroup vs. v intergroup p) × 2 (group status: low vvs. high) × 2 (stability: unsttable vs. stablle) between-grroups design. Scheepers et al. (2006) askked participannts to read a sshort scenarioo and imagine theemselves as th he main charaacter, “X”. Thhe story descrribed a handbball game betw ween two teams (“DD DV” and “Pap pendrecht”). At A one point, X, who was ppart of the DD DV team, madde a derogatory comment abo out the Papen ndrecht team. In the intragrroup communnication condiition, this statement was w made only y to the otherr DDV playerrs. In the interrgroup commuunication conndition, the statement was w made and d it was clear that t memberss of the Papenndrecht team could hear. W Within the low status condition, c parrticipants read d that the DD DV team was rranked 9th andd the Papendrrecht team was ranked d 1st. In the hig gh status cond dition, the rannkings were rreversed. In thhe stable conddition, participantss were told th hat this rankin ng was the sam me as it has beeen for years, whereas, parrticipants read that th he ranking had d fluctuated a great deal ovver the past feew years in thhe unstable coondition. Scheepers and a colleaguees found a sig gnificant threee-way interacttion for the innstrumental fuunction measure. Within W the low w status condittion, when thhe statement w was made so tthat only the iingroup could hear it (intragroup p condition), the t statement was seen more instrumenttally in the unnstable condition compared to th he stable cond dition. On thee other hand, when the stattement was m made so that the oth her team could d also hear it (intergroup ( coondition), thee statement w was believed too have less of an instru umental functiion in the unsstable than thee stable condiition. There w were no differrences between thee low status, stable s conditiions regardlesss of whether the statemennt was intergrooup or intragroup. In this case, it was though ht that the grooup had nothinng to lose whhether or not tthe other d the statemen nt. Within thee high status ccondition, theere were no diifferences in tthe group heard attribution of o instrumenttal function reegardless of ccommunicatioon and stabilitty. With regarrd to the identity-exp pressive functtion of the staatement, Scheeepers and coolleagues (20006) found a siignificant interaction between statu us of the grou up and the stabbility of the ssituation. Withhin the stablee conditions, it was believ ved that the staatement madee by the high status group served more of an Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 23 iddentity-expresssive function n than when th he statement was w made by a member off the low statuus grroup. This diffference was not n significan nt within the unstable u condditions. Sccheepers et all.’s (2006) seccond study sh howed that ingroup bias seerved an identtity-expressivve fuunction among stable, high h status group ps. It further sh howed that w within unstablee, low status grroups, ingrou up favouritism m as an instrum mental functio on was more likely to be m manifest whenn the auudience was the t ingroup on nly. Expressin ng ingroup biias instrumenntally in front of the outgrooup was onnly likely witthin low status groups for whom w this staatus was perceeived to be sttable, since thhey had noothing to losee by alerting the t outgroup of o their attem mpts to improvve their positiion. In unstable seettings, low sttatus groups are a wise to mo obilize with stealth, s when attempting too move beyonnd the exxisting status structure. Foollowing the procedures off the first stud dy, the third study s conductted by Scheeppers et al (20006) asssessed the in nteraction betw ween stability y (stable vs. unstable) u and status (low vss. high) on inngroup biias. However, by making participants p ostensibly justtify their resouurce allocatioons to either inngroup m members or bo oth ingroup an nd outgroup members m (inteergroup conteext), this studyy also assesseed coommunication n context. Un nlike the first study, participants were allso asked to ccomplete a serries of otther measuress, including: x x x x generaal indicator off instrumentall function: wiillingness to innvest effort inn the second rround (2 item ms) generaal indicator off identity-exprressive functiion: Private sccale of Luhtaanen and Croccker’s (1992)) Collective Self-esteem S Sccale (PCSE; cited c in Scheeepers et al, 20006) (4 items)) expliciit measure off material ingrroup bias: 5 ittems – 3 meassured instrum mental functioon, 2 measu ured identity-eexpressive fun nction expliciit measure off symbolic ing group bias: 5 items i – 3 meaasured instrum mental functioon, 2 measu ured identity-eexpressive fun nction B By using 2 (co ommunication n context: intrragroup vs. in ntergroup) × 2 (status: low vs. high) × 2 (sstability: stablle vs. unstablee) ANOVAS and MANOV VAS, Scheepeers et al. founnd that there w was a thhree-way interraction when assessing thee MIP strategy y. Specificallyy, within the intragroup coondition, thosse in the low status s group who w were in the t unstable ccondition scorred higher on the M MIP than did the low status participants in i the stable condition. c Thhere were no ddifferences, hoowever, betw ween the low status, s stable participants p and a the low sttatus, unstablee participants in the inntergroup con ndition. Furtheer, there weree no effects fo or stability or communicatiion within thee high sttatus participaants. For the more m aggressiive, MD strategy, there waas an interactiion between sstatus annd stability su uch that particcipants in the stable, low sttatus conditioon used this sttrategy signifficantly m more than did those in the unstable, u low status conditiion. There waas no such diffference betw ween the sttable and unsttable high stattus participan nts. An assesssment of the m measure of syymbolic ingroup bias inndicated that there t was a siignificant inteeraction betweeen status andd stability. W Within the stabble coonditions, parrticipants in th he high statuss group displaayed more sym mbolic ingrouup bias than ddid the loow status partticipants. Thiss difference between low and a high statuus groups wass not evident iin the unnstable condition. Sccheepers et all. (2006) also found a significant main effect e of statuus for the meaasure of the innstrumental fu unction of thee material ingrroup bias as well w as the wiillingness to iinvest effort, aand the iddentity-expresssive function n of the symbo olic ingroup bias. b Participaants in the low w status conddition w were more likeely than high status particip pants to indiccate that they used materiall ingroup biass for its innstrumental fu unction and to o indicate thatt they would be willing to exert more efffort in the seecond roound. Conversely, participaants in the hig gh status cond dition were m more likely thaan their low sstatus Paage 24 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® counterpartts to indicate that t they had used symbollic ingroup biaas for its idenntity-expressivve function. nificant interacction betweenn status and sstability on thee measure of collective Finally, theere was a sign self-esteem m, an indicatorr of the identity-expressivee function of iingroup bias. While there w was no difference between b partiicipants in thee unstable connditions regarrdless of statuus, Scheepers et al.’s (2006) participants in the stable condition who were part of the high status ggroup scored significantlly higher than n their stable, low status coounterparts. In addition to all of the above a analysees for study 3 , Scheepers aand colleagues (2006) condducted a multisample path analyssis to assess th he different rooutes from thee functions off ingroup biass to the expression of ingroup biias (structurall equation moodel depicted in Figure 4). n different ffunctions an Figure 4. Relationshiips between nd forms off ingroup bias, selfa effort in n Experimen nt 3. Standa ardized coeffficients are e expressed d along esteem, and the lines (low ( status//high status s); those in bold are significant att p < .05. Id = identity- expressive e function; f In nst = instrum mental func ction; PCSE E = Private s subscale of Collecttive Self-Estteem Scale; MIP = max ximum ingro oup profit; MJP = max ximum joint profiit; MD = maximum diffe erentiation.. (Scheeperrs et al., 200 06, p. 954) They found d that the mod del representin ng the path frrom identity-eexpression to self-esteem w was stronger wiithin the high status particiipants and thaat this path waas mediated bby symbolic inngroup bias. On thee other hand, the pathwayss from the insstrumental funnction of ingrroup bias to M MIP and effort were stronger for low l status parrticipants thann for high staatus participannts. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 25 The final study y conducted by b Scheepers et al (2006) assessed a ingrooup bias usingg a 2 (group sstatus: loow vs. high) × 2 (stability: unstable vs. stable) s × 2 (fo orm of ingrouup bias: materrial vs. symboolic) m mixed model design d with reepeated measu ures on the laast factor. Thee procedures m mirrored thosse of sttudies 1 and 3. 3 Symbolic in ngroup bias, as a with material ingroup biias, was meassured using T Tajfel m matrices. For the symbolic ingroup i bias, the matrices were used to allocate poinnts to other paarticipants. It was stressed to participan nts that these points p had “noo value in thee estimation coompetition”. The T pull scores of the various ingroup and a outgroup matrices resuulted in four ffactors (m material MD, symbolic MD D, material MIP, M and symb bolic MIP). A An ANOVA conducted on the M MD scores ind dicated that th here was a sign nificant threee-way interacttion between status, stabiliity and foorm of ingrou up bias. When n assessing thee material ing group bias, paarticipants in tthe stable, low w sttatus condition scored high her on the MD D measure thaan did particippants in the uunstable, low sstatus coondition. There was no succh difference among particcipants in the high status coondition. When asssessing the teendency to usse MD with sy ymbolic ingro oup bias, the researchers fo found that paarticipants in the stable, high status con ndition scored d higher than ddid participannts in the stabble, low sttatus condition. There was no such diffeerence among g participants in the unstabble conditions. A siimilar ANOV VA conducted on the MIP scores s showed d no significaant main effeccts or interacttions. Sccheepers and his colleaguees (2006) fourr studies indiccated that inggroup bias waas not always the saame thing tim me and again. Rather, the fo orm and functtion differed, and this depeended largelyy on the coontext in whicch it was foun nd. They foun nd that abstracct, symbolic iingroup bias iis used most bby hiigh status gro oups for whom m this status is stable. Partiicipants strovve for maximuum ingroup prrofit in unnstable, low status s groups,, especially when w this was done anonym mously or in fr front of other inngroup memb bers. Moreoveer, the more harsh h form of ingroup bias,, maximum diifferentiation, was foound most oftten among low w status group ps for whom this status waas stable, perhhaps because they haad nothing to lose by trying a radical strrategy even iff that strategyy would be knnown to the hiigh sttatus group. Symbolic S ingrroup bias serv ved an identity y-expressive ffunction whille material inggroup biias served an instrumental function. Thee functional use u of ingroupp bias varies aaccording to ccontext annd group mottive. In cases where w low staatus groups seee the existin g status quo aas unstable, inngroup faavouritism can n be construeed as a call forr social mobillization (com mpetition). At the same timee, Sccheepers et all. argue that in ngroup favou uritism serves a social mobbilization funcction in stablee coonditions too,, given that th he low status groups g have nothing n to losse and make ssalient their m mobilization sttrategy. Table 4, summ marizes the ressearch pertain ning to the im mpact of stabillity on identityy managemennt sttrategies. Paage 26 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Table 4. Summary S off identity managementt strategies in respons se to socios structural stab bility Reference Outccome Managemennt strategy Nadler & Halabbi (2006) Ͳ Loow status, high ideentifiers are less w willing to accept heelp from high status meembers and negattively perceive higgh status memberss who offer deependency-orienteed help under unsttable conditions. Ͳ Loow status members challenge statuss relations by refussing help while higgh status memberrs try to maintain thheir position by offfering deependency-orienteed help. Social competition Nadler, HarpazzGorodeisky, & BenB David (2009) Ͳ Higgh status, high ideentifiers maintain ppositive distinctiveeness by offering deependency-orienteed (defensive) helpp to low status meembers regardlesss of whether or not heelp is needed espeecially when statuss relations are unnstable. Ͳ When members of a high status groupp think about an iddentity they have in coommon with low status members, theey are more likelyy to offer autonomyyoriiented than depenndency-oriented (ddefensive) help. Defensive heelping (Social competition) Scheepers (20009) Ͳ Loow status groups experienced e a stabble status as threaatening, as evvidenced by lower cardiovascular re activity and higher vascular ressistance. Ͳ Unnstable status wass perceived by low w status groups ass a challenge, as evvidenced by higherr cardiovascular reeactivity and lower vascular ressistance. Ͳ Higgh status groups experienced e an unnstable status as tthreatening, as evvidenced by lower cardiovascular re activity and higher vascular ressistance. Functional cooping Scheepers & Ellemers (2005) Ͳ Loow status group members felt threattened (higher bloood pressure) by thee staatus quo (stable situation) whereas high status groupp members felt thrreatened by statuss loss (unstable siituation). Ͳ Thhis was only true foor those who weree highly identified with their group. Ͳ Loow status group members reported lless collective selff-esteem than highh staatus members thoough personal self--esteem was unafffected. Individualizaation (Individual m mobility) Scheepers, Elleemers, & Sintemaartensddijk (2009) m experiencce unstable statuss as more stressfuul Ͳ Higgh status group members (ass measured by systolic blood press ure and pulse preessure) and are moore upset and hosstile than stable staatus. Ͳ Higgh status group members m experiencce an unstable staatus as particularlyy thrreatening when low w status group meembers are prese nt. Ͳ In unstable situationns, both high and l ow status group m members are moree strrategic about theirr responses that m might be interpreteed as ingroup bias in intergroup situatioons. Ingroup biass (Social competition) Scheepers, Speears, Doosje, & Mansstead (2006) Ͳ Inggroup bias served an identity-expresssive function for high status groups paarticularly when thaat status was stabble. Ͳ Inggroup bias served an instrumental ffunction for low staatus groups and toook a more aggresssive form when thee status was stab le. Ͳ When the status is unstable, u low statuus group memberss use ingroup biass to serve an instrumeental function onlyy in intragroup situuations whereas s they will usee ingroup bias to sserve an whhen the status is stable, insstrumental functionn in either intragrooup or intergroup ssituations. Social competition/ Realistic com mpetition, ingroup bias (Social competition) Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 27 4 4.2 Legittimacy an nd Identity y Managem ment Strattegies There is some research look king at percep ptions of the sociostructura s al belief of leggitimacy, andd how thhis leads to vaarious identity y managemen nt strategies. Schmader, S Maajor, Ecclestoon, and McCooy (22001) examin ned a social crreativity identtity managem ment strategy, devaluing or selective devvaluing. U Understood as “a process off reducing thee perceived im mportance of a domain in aan effort to prrotect onneself againstt negative outtcomes” resullting from a social compariison in that ddomain (Schm mader, M Major, Ecclestton, & McCoy y, 2001, p. 78 82), individuaals may engagge they state inn selective deevaluing. Seleective devaluing essentiallly shifts the dimension of ccomparison too protect the ggroup iddentity and self-esteem from m a negative comparison to t an outgroupp. Schmader and colleaguees arrgue that devaaluing a domaain is influencced by group status and peerceived legitiimacy of the inntergroup com mparisons. Ho owever, they also a argue thaat willingnesss to devalue a domain depeends on hoow much peo ople value the given domain, and how much m knowleddge they havee regarding whho exxcels in that domain. d B Based on previious research (Jost & Banaaji, 1994; citeed in Schmadeer et al., 20011), Schmader and coolleagues (2001) work from m the assump ption that peop ple perceive sstatus differennces as legitim mate unnless they aree given inform mation to sugg gest otherwisee. Referred too as status vallue asymmetry ry, w when differencces are perceived as legitim mate, individu uals from highh status groupps place less vvalue onn domains in which low sttatus groups excel, e and putt higher valuee on domains in which highh status grroups excel in n comparison. However, when w differencces are perceiived as illegittimate, individduals arre expected to o devalue dom mains in whicch their ingrou up is outperfoormed by a hiigher status ouutgroup. In th his way, perceeived legitimaacy moderatess when indiviiduals devaluue a domain. Scchmader et all. (2001) hypo othesized thatt status value asymmetry iss mediated byy people’s peerceptions of how useful th he domain is for gaining reewards that arre relevant to their status. L Low sttatus groups may m be likely to value dom mains in which h a high statuus outgroup exxcels becausee they beelieve that if they t do well in i those domaains, they willl be able to reeap the rewarrds enjoyed byy the hiigh status outtgroup membeers. These domains are hig gh in expectedd utility. Dom mains where thhe low sttatus group ex xcels, individu uals may be less l likely to see s those as im mportant because they are peerceived to bee low in expected utility. Schmader S et al. a expected thhat when statuus differencess are leegitimate, individuals’ percceived utility of domains sh hould be baseed on who exxcels in that doomain, buut they should d rely less on this knowled dge when statu us differencess are illegitim mate. In the laatter caase, individuaals would be free f to devalu ue the domain ns in which thheir ingroup pperforms poorrly coompared to th he higher statu us outgroup. Scchmader et all. (2001) cond ducted three studies s that ex xamined selecctive devaluinng among higgh and loow status grou up members. In all three sttudies, participants compleeted a test thatt measured a fictitious positiive personalitty trait called surgency2 an nd received grroup feedbackk indicating thhat thheir group had d done either better or worrse than an ou utgroup on thiis trait. Studiees 1 and 2 useed a 2 (rrelative group p status: high or low) × 2 (g group feedbacck: ingroup suuccess or outggroup success) beetween-subjects design. In n Study 1, diffferences in staatus were creaated by askinng participantss to coompare students’ personaliity scores from m their own school s (UCSB B) to those at a local city ccollege (lower in status) or Stanford d University (higher ( in stattus). While sttudents were w waiting to seee how thheir group sco ored on the peersonality testt, they were given g a data shheet showing differences beetween their own o group an nd the outgrou up in domainss such as incoome and careeer advancemeent to 2 P Participants were never providedd with a specific definition of surrgency, and weree only told that itt is a positive peersonality traait. Paage 28 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® reinforce sttatus differencces. Participants then receiived feedbackk about how ttheir own grouup scored relative to the t outgroup and a were told d that their personal score w was not incluuded in the grooup score. In the ingro oup success co ondition (den noting outgrouup failure), paarticipants weere told that thhe average for their un niversity was 72.5 7 and for the t comparisoon university was 34.5. In the outgroup success condition (d denoting ingrroup failure), the scores weere reversed ssuch that theirr own universsity had scored 34.5 5, whereas thee comparison university haad scored 72.55. Schmader et e al.’s (2001)) participants then respondded to three iteems exploringg the extent too which they valued d the surgency y trait. Participants also annswered items about their pperceived utility of the surgency trrait as status-rrelevant (for achieving a careeer success) aand as status-irrelevant (foor achieving social s success). Three item ms assessed car areer utility annd three itemss assessed soccial utility. Participantss also indicateed how they defined d the doomain of surggency by ratinng what traits individuals are likely to have in conju unction with ssurgency, succh as competeence (e.g., inteelligent, ocial warmth (e.g., sociablle, friendly). T They measureed self-appraiisals of surgenncy on lazy) and so three items. These two measures m weree used to test alternative exxplanations too perceived uutility as a mediator fo or the status value v asymmeetry. Schmadeer et al. then aadministered m manipulationn checks for group feeedback and group g status, which were eeffective. Finaally, they asseessed particippants’ perceived leegitimacy of status differences betweenn groups on thhree items (e.g., “Do you bbelieve that it is acccurate or inacccurate to say that Stanfordd [or UCSB] rreally is superior to UCSB B [or City College]?”)). Results sup pported the sta atus value asyymmetry accoount. Consisteent with Schm mader et al.’s (2001) predictions, there was a significant in nteraction betw ween group status and grouup feedback oon how much particcipants valued d the domain of surgency ((see Figure 5)). Figure 5. The status value asym mmetry show wn with gro oup based o on school a affiliation in Study 1. (Schmade er et al., 200 01, p. 786) s conditio on valued thee domain signnificantly morre when their ingroup Participantss in the high status scored high her than the lo ower status grroup compareed to when thee outgroup sccored higher tthan the ingroup. Ho owever, when n participants were in the llow status conndition, there was no differrence between ho ow much they y valued the domain d based on how their ingroup scorred relative too the outgroup. No N other signiificant effectss were found. Further analyyses testing thhe effects of sstatus within grou up feedback conditions sho owed that in thhe ingroup suuccess conditiion, both highh and low status group ps valued the domain equaally. In the ouutgroup success condition ((or ingroup faailure Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 29 coondition), parrticipants in th he high statuss condition peerceived the ddomain as signnificantly lesss im mportant than n did those in the low statuss condition. Results R also shhowed an inteeraction betw ween grroup status an nd group feed dback for statu us-relevant uttility of the doomain. No maain effects weere foound. The dom main was percceived as morre useful for gaining g careeer success wheen UCSB scoored hiigher than Citty College, bu ut not when City C College scored s higher than UCSB. Similarly, peerceived utilitty for surgenccy was higherr when Stanfo ord scored higgher than UCSB, but not w when U UCSB scored higher h than Stanford. Med diational analy yses followingg the Baron aand Kenny (19986; ciited in Schmaader et al.) meethod showed d that perceiveed career utiliity mediated tthe value statuus assymmetry on personal valu ue of the dom main. For statu us-irrelevant uutility, there w was only a maain efffect of group p feedback succh that when participants’ ingroup scoreed higher thaan the comparrison grroup, they vallued the domaain more relevant to when they scored llower. Scchmader et all. (2001) cond ducted two fo ollow on studiies. For Studyy 2, rather thaan using groupps that w were situationaally constructted, they used d gender as staatus differencce as it tends tto be chronic and m more salient. The T findings in Study 2 rep plicated those in Study 1. Inn Study 3, Scchmader and coolleagues used a similar deesign as in thee previous two studies but also manipullated the legittimacy off group statuss differences and a groups were w based on school affiliaation (as in Sttudy 1). The ddesign w was a 2 (group p status: high vs. v low) × 2 (group ( feedbaack: ingroup ssuccess vs. ouutgroup succeess) × 2 (legitimacy of group status: legitimate vss. illegitimate) between-subbjects designn. As in the prrevious sttudies, before participants found out how w their group p scored on thhe surgency peersonality test, they w were given a data d sheet desccribing how their t group differed from thhe comparisoon group. How wever, atttached to the data sheet was also a fictiitious research h article that eelaborated onn the details m mentioned in th he data sheet.. In the illegittimacy condittion, the reseaarch suggested that studentts at thhe two schools are equivaleent in both their academic and intelligennce potential,, whereas in thhe leegitimacy con ndition, studen nts at the high her status scho ool have betteer academic aand intelligennce pootential than those t at the lo ower status scchool. Resultss showed thatt all three mannipulations w were efffective. Scchmader et all. (2001) pred dicted that thee value status asymmetry w would be elim minated when inndividuals recceived inform mation that stattus differencees were illegittimate. A maiin effect of grroup feeedback was observed, o butt this effect was w qualified by b the significcant three-waay interaction,, as shhown in Figurre 6. Figure 6. Status S illeg gitimacy mo oderates the e status valu ue asymme etry in Study y 3. (Sch hmader et al., p. 792) Paage 30 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® When statu us differences were perceiv ved as legitim mate, the statuss value symm metry is observved consistent with w Studies 1 and 2. However, when sttatus differencces were percceived as illeggitimate, the status value v symmetrry is eliminated. Specificallly, participannts in the low w status condittion valued the surgency s dom main less when they scoredd lower than tthe outgroup ccompared to w when they scored high her than the ou utgroup. Partiicipants in thee high status condition vallued the domaain equally regardless of o whether theeir ingroup orr the outgroupp scored highher on the dim mension. No oother significant effects were observed. o howed a signiificant main eeffect of Statuus Legitimacyy and a Further anaalysis on expeected utility sh significant Group Statuss × Group Feeedback interacction, both quualified by a ssignificant thrree-way interaction. Schmader ett al.’s (2001) results r lookinng at perceiveed utility repliicated the finddings from Study 1 in the t legitimacy y condition. Participants P inn the higher status conditioon perceived tthe domain to be b more usefu ul for career success s compaared to those in the lower status conditiion. When participantss were in the low l status con ndition, they perceived greeater utility inn the domain for career success when the higherr status schooll scored higheer on the traitt than their ow wn school. In contrast, when statuss was perceived as illegitim mate, there waas only a maiin effect of grroup status. The perceived utility u of surgeency for careeer success waas higher for pparticipants inn the high staatus compared to the low stattus condition.. Mediationall analyses shoowed that percceived career utility significantlly mediated th he effects of status, s feedbaack, and legitiimacy on valuuing the domaain. No significant effects were found f for the social utility of surgency. gs from Schm mader et al.’s (2001) ( studiess suggest thatt it is importaant to considerr both The finding group status and percepttions of the leegitimacy of th that status on devaluing proocesses. Indivviduals from low sttatus groups do d not alwayss devalue dom mains in whichh their ingrouup performs ppoorly relative to a high status group. g Ratherr, there is eviddence that theey devalue doomains when there is reason to beelieve that staatus differences are illegiti mate, and thiis can be consstrued as a soccial creative meeans to manag ge a negative identity. Highh status groupps are also inffluenced by sstatus legitimacy. Members fro om high status groups tendd to devalue a domain when status differrences are legitimate and a when they y do worse in n the domain tthan a lower status group. This suggests that high status group p members vaalue another domain d as a sstrategy to maaintain the exiisting social sstatus, when it mig ght be in jeop pardy. In a field sttudy, Dumontt and van Lill (2009) consiidered the imppact of percepptions of statuus legitimacy and identity management m strategies s for dominant annd non-dominaant groups. Past research haas shown that dominant gro oup members try to maintaain their groupp’s position bby using group-levell strategies an nd resort to in ndividual-leveel strategies w when it seems likely that thheir group will lose their social adv vantage (e.g., Bettencourt eet al., 2001). F For members of non-dominant groups, on the other han nd, they usuallly prefer an inndividual mobbility strategyy, unless it is impossible for them to use u this strateg gy such as whhen group bouundaries are iimpermeable (e.g., Ellemers ett al., 1988, citted in Dumon nt and van Lil l, 2009). How wever, if statuus relations arre viewed as insecure (i.e., illegitim mate and unsttable), membeers of the highh status groupp would be unnlikely to use group-llevel strategiees to maintain n their social pposition. Dumont and van Lill (20 009) predicted d that under innsecure condditions, dominnant group meembers would show w less ingroup p identificatio on and be lesss likely to act collectively tto preserve thheir social position. In n contrast, non n-dominant grroup memberrs should show w more ingrooup identificattion and be more lik kely to collecttively mobilizze (i.e., sociall competition)) to improve ttheir social sttanding when statuss relations aree insecure. Co onsidering ressearch by Nieens and Cairns (2003; citedd in Dumont and van Lill), Dumont D and van v Lill also ppredicted that ingroup idenntification shoould be Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 31 poositively asso ociated with more m use of co ollective strategies and neggatively assocciated with thhe use off individual sttrategies regaardless of indiividuals’ statu us position. To test their hy ypotheses, Du umont and van n Lill (2009) conducted a field study inn South Africaa, foollowing the first f democrattic elections in 1994, i.e., the t first time tthe black majjority gained poolitical powerr.3 According g to Gibson an nd Gouws (19 999; cited in D Dumont and vvan Lill), raciial grroups in Soutth Africa are highly h salientt categories an nd people stroongly identifyy with them. Paarticipants in Dumont and van Lill’s fieeld study weree 300 studentts, each membbers of one off the thhree racial gro oups (white, black, b and colloured). Each group had 1000 members. The field studdy ussed a 3 (Raciaal Group: Wh hite vs. Black vs. Coloured d) × 2 (Compaarison Group:: one of the tw wo raacial groups to o which participants did no ot belong) bettween-subjecct design. Partticipants first inndicated their perceptions of o the transforrmation proceess and their eeconomic staatus position. Then paarticipants ind dicated their perceptions p of o legitimacy, stability, andd level of ingrroup identification. N Next, participaants indicated their level off agreement with w items asssessing four iddentity managgement sttrategies, as fo ollows: x x x x social competition (e.g., ( “Ingrou up people shou uld demonstraate that they aare the more successsful group in terms of econ nomic status””), outgro oup favouritism m (e.g., “If neew jobs arise in the next feew years, ingrroup people w will make sure s that thesee jobs will bee filled with outgroup peopple rather thann with ingroupp peoplee”), individ dualization (ee.g., “I regard myself as a single s person rather than ass a member oof a certain n group of peo ople”), and supero ordinate recateegorization (ee.g., “I consid der myself as South African an” and “I connsider myselff as black/white/coloured” score based on o differencee between thesse two items)). R Results showed d that White group g membeers considered d their ingrouup as the curreent dominant group, w whereas memb bers of the Co oloured and Black B groups considered c theeir ingroup ass the current nnondoominant grou up relative to the t compariso ons. As such, members of bboth Coloureed and Black ggroups inn South Africaa were consid dered non-dom minant. For th he dominant W White group m member, percceived illlegitimacy off economic staatus was asso ociated with a decrease in iingroup identiification. Thee oppposite was trrue for non-dominant grou up members. When W they viiewed the stattus structure aas illlegitimate, th hey reported in ncreased iden ntification witth their groupp. D Dumont and vaan Lill (2009)) also found systematic s diffferences betw ween the dom minant and nonndoominant grou ups in terms of the choice of o identity straategies they sselected to maanage soociostructurall beliefs. For dominant d gro oup members, perceived leggitimacy posiitively prediccted soocial competition strategies (demonstratting their prow wess) and neggatively predicted outgrouup faavouritism (m maintaining the social orderr); whereas illlegitimacy waas associated with less inggroup iddentification. The opposite was found fo or non-domin nant groups, i..e., illegitimaccy was associiated w with greater no on-dominant ingroup i identtification. Alsso, for South A African Blackks, perceived leegitimacy possitively prediccted outgroup favouritism. Legitimacy ddid not predicct the choice oof sttrategies for th he Coloured racial r group. And A across alll three raciall groups, theree was a signifficant neegative correllation between ingroup ideentification an nd the use of iindividual-levvel strategies (i.e., inndividualization and supero ordinate recattegorization). For dominannt group mem mbers, a positiive coorrelation between ingroup p identificatio on and social competition, c a collective sstrategy, was shown. 3 A At the time, the government’s g main priority was to t initiate a transsformation proceess to establish social and economic eqquality among thhe racial groups to t form a non-raacial society (Alexander, 1999; ccited in Dumont aand van Lill, 20009). Paage 32 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Based on Dumont D and van Lill’s find dings, it is safee to argue thaat ingroup ideentification results in less individ dual strategiess (in their casee, individualiization and suuperordinate ccategorizationn) and more collecctive action. Another div visive politicaal context to understand u iddentity managgement strateggies for dominnant and non-dominaant groups is Northern Irelland. In short,, the conflict in Northern IIreland comprrised individuals who wanted to remain a part p of the Unnited Kingdom m (Protestantss) and those w who wanted to be b integrated with w the Repu ublic of Irelannd (Catholics). Most peoplle in Northernn Ireland identify eith her as a Catho olic or Protestant (Cairns & Darby, 19998; cited in Niiens & Cairnss, 2002), and categorrize other cou untrymen as belonging b to oone of those tw wo denominaations (Cairnss, 1980; cited in Nieens & Cairns)). The politicaal situation thhroughout the 1970s, 80s annd early 90s w was viewed as illegitimate (aand hence unsstable) mainlyy by the Cathoolic communnity, which proovoked a wave of soccial unrest and paramilitary y violence (soocial competiition). The Protestants had largely maintained political and social domin nance in Northhern Ireland, at the expensse of the Com mmunity (e.g., Cairn ns & Mercer, 1984; 1 cited in n Niens & Caiirns). “The Trroubles”, as tthey were callled, was essentially a collective action a arising from a desiree to change thhe disparate coonstitutional rights and treatment of o the Catholicc communitiees across Nortthern Ireland,, and separatee from Great B Britain. Political an nd military hostilities ended d with the Goood Friday Peeace Accord 11998. This ressulted in a cessation off sectarian vio olence, param military disarm mament, and sshared politiccal power. Deespite the social and political p changes, there rem mains today a smattering oof political vioolence. Niens and Cairns C (2002)) developed sccales to meassure different identity manaagement strattegies based on th he political context in North hern Ireland aand examinedd the relationss with severall sociostructu ural variabless, including peerceptions off legitimacy annd stability, aas well as percceptions of ingroup identification n. They argued d that the Prootestant comm munity (dominnant group) m may view the current political situaation as illegiitimate and unnstable as a coonsequence oof greater poliitical and social engagement of thee Catholic com mmunity. Forr obvious reassons, social iddentities will be particularly y salient in inttergroup confflicts like thatt witnessed inn Northern Ireeland over thee last 4 decades. An nd Niens and Cairns (2002 2) identified ffive particularr identity mannagement straategies most relevaant to this con ntext, includin ng: x x x x x ind dividualization n (e.g., “If som meone attackks my commuunity, I usuallyy do not take it perrsonally”), soccial competitio on (e.g., “I want w my comm munity to dem monstrate that it is the supeerior one”), chaange of comparison dimensions (e.g., “F For my comm munity, there aare more impportant com mparisons witth the other co ommunity thaan having moore political innfluence”), tem mporal compaarisons (“For my m communiity, it is most important to compare its ssituation tod day with its sittuation 2 yearrs ago”), and sup perordinate reecategorization (e.g., “Firstt and foremosst, I regard myyself as Europpean rath her than as a member m of my m denominatiional communnity”). Like Dumo ont and van Liill (2009), ind dividuation annd superordinnate recategorrization are coonsidered individual strategies s for managing ideentity, whereaas social com mpetition, channge of compaarison dimensionss, and temporaal comparison ns are consideered collectivve strategies, tthe latter two falling under sociaal creativity. Participantss, both Protesstant or Catho olic, were giveen a questionnnaire that assessed their peerceptions of the legitiimacy and staability of the situation, s leveel of ingroup identificationn (from Brow wn, Condor, Mathews, Wade, W & Willliams, 1986; cited c in Nienss & Cairns, 20002), and the use of speciffic identity managemen nt strategies. Niens N and Caairns found thhat stronger inngroup identiffication was aassociated Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 33 w with more sociial competitio on as well as diminished d in ndividualizatioon and superoordinate reecategorizatio on. However, they also fou und that greateer perceived llegitimacy waas associated with leess ingroup id dentification, less l social competition, an nd more indiv idualization. And these rellations am mong the variiables remain ned the same when w Protestaants and Cathholics were exxamined separrately, exxcept for percceptions of staability. For Prrotestants, mo ore stability w was associatedd with less ingroup iddentification and a more indiividualization n. In contrast, for Catholicss, more stabiliity was assocciated w with somewhat more ingrou up identificatiion, but was not n associatedd with individdualization. Fuurther analysees showed thaat Catholics perceived p the situation in N Northern Irelaand as less leegitimate and more stable, reporting greeater ingroup identification i n than did Prootestants (Nienns & C Cairns, 2002). This suggests that, despitee the strides made m since thee Good Fridaay Peace Accoord, the siituation in No orthern Ireland d may not be fully resolved d in the eyes of the Cathollic communityy. To m manage the ten nuous situatio on, it may be that t the non-d dominant grouup strongly iddentifies withh their grroup as an ideentity manageement strategy y, since accorrding to Nienns and Cairns’’ study no diifferences weere found betw ween the two groups when it came to iddentity managgement strateggies. Inndeed, althoug gh perception ns of legitimacy, stability, and a ingroup iidentification were expecteed to coorrelate with all of the man nagement straategies, this was w not the ca se. Specificallly, change off coomparison dim mensions and d temporal com mparisons weere not relatedd to any of thhe variables inn the sttudy, and as such may be related r to otheer concepts no ot measured hhere. Howeveer, the social iidentity vaariables were correlated with individuall strategies (i..e., individuallization and ssuperordinate reecategorizatio on) and one co ollective strattegy (i.e., sociial competitioon). Niens andd Cairns sugggest thhat social iden ntity theory may m be better at a explaining individual ass opposed to ccollective idenntity m management sttrategies. O Ouwerkerk and d Ellemers (2 2002) also con nsidered the leegitimacy of the context or circumstancce of soocial compariison, and how w this influencces strategies to protect or bbolster sociall identity. Within thhe process of social compaarison, they arrgued, people compare the circumstancees under whicch the grroups perform med, and thesee circumstancces can have an a influence iif group mem mbers attributee their reelative successs or failure to o the groups general g superiior or inferiorr ability, respeectively. In tuurn, thhese judgments can influen nce a group’s perception off the extent too which their ability on thee peerformance dimension is leegitimate and d stable, i.e., ju ustified and uunlikely to chhange. Ouwerkkerk annd Ellemers also a predicted d that these kin nds of intergrroup comparissons would innfluence inndividuals’ afffective and motivational m reesponses. To test their hy ypotheses, Ou uwerkerk and Ellemers (20 002) examinedd people’s aff ffective and m motivational reesponses to po ositive or neg gative intergro oup comparissons and varieed their persoonal coontribution ass well as the circumstances c s surrounding g the outgroupp’s performannce. They used a 2 (C Comparison: Favourable F vs. Unfavourab ble) × 2 (Outg group: disadvvantage vs. addvantage) × 2 (R Responsibility y: Low vs. Hiigh) between--subjects design. Ouwerkerrk and Ellemers’ participaants first completed d a modified dot d estimation n task (see Jettten, Spears, & Manstead, 1996; cited inn O Ouwerkerk and d Ellemers) and were then allegedly cattegorized intoo one of two sstyles of perceeiving (eeither “detaileed” or “globall” perceivers)). All participants were toldd that they weere “detailed”” peerceivers and that the purp pose of the stu udy was to compare the perrformance off the detailed aand gllobal perceiveers on a “Group Recognition Word Task k,” an anagraam task, which measures a grroup’s ability y to recognize words in an unusual u conteext quickly annd concurrenttly. They werre also toold that the grroup’s perform mance on the task can pred dict their succcess within ann organizationnal coontext. A After participaants completed d the word task, participan nts in the low responsibilityy condition w were toold that their personal p scoree could not bee included in their group’ss score due to a computer eerror. Paage 34 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® In the high responsibilityy condition, participants p diid not receivee this messagee. Then the sccores for both groupss were presen nted to particip pants. To mannipulate comp mparison, the pperformance oof participantss’ own group was held con nstant, but in tthe unfavouraable comparisson conditionn, the outgroup sccored 7 pointss higher, whereas in the favvourable com mparison conddition, the outtgroup scored 7 po oints lower. To manipulate outgroup (diis)advantage ((legitimacy vvs. illegitimacyy), participantss in the outgro oup disadvan ntage conditioon were told thhat the anagraams completeed by the outgroup were w more diffficult, whereaas those in thee outgroup addvantage conddition were toold that the anagrams completed c by the outgroup were easier tthan the ones that their inggroup completted. This essentially made the circcumstances un nfair or illegittimate. k and Ellemerss’ (2002) asseessed particippants’ affectivve responses to four negattive (e.g., Ouwerkerk disappointm ment, shame) and four posiitive emotionns (e.g., pride,, contentmentt) in response to their group’s perrformance on the word task k. Participantts were told thhat both the ddetailed and gllobal perceiver groups would be given the opportunity o too improve theeir performannce on the woord task by completing g another 20 anagrams a and their motivattional responsses were thenn assessed on tthree measures. First, F participaants’ collectiv ve efficacy foor performancce improvemeent was measuured that assessed thee magnitude and a strength judgments j (m modeled after self-efficacy scale –Banduura, 1977; cited in Ouw werkerk & Elllemers). Colllective efficaccy magnitudee assessed how w much participants believed thaat their group p could improve on the taskk. They were presented wiith seven succcessively higher scores (e.g., 69, 70, 7 71) and assked to indicaate whether thheir group cann attain that sccore (with yes/no resp ponses). Colleective efficacy y strength asssessed particippants’ confideence that theiir group can attain each e of the performance scores (scale froom 0 to 100). The second motivation m measure examined participants’ p tendency to hiinder the futuure performannce of the outggroup. Particiipants were told th hat for the wo ord task they would w be com mpleting laterr, the difficultty level for haalf of them would be seelected by thee computer, but the difficuulty level for tthe other halff would be selected by the outgrou up. Similarly, they would also a be able too determine thhe difficulty llevel for half the anagrams to o be completeed by the outg group. Difficuulty level for the anagramss range from w words with 5 to 8 letters, and th he mean difficculty level fo r the 10 anaggrams served aas the measurre for their tendency to o hinder the ou utgroup’s futu ure performannce. Then, paarticipants didd the word tassk again. Following this, t Ouwerkeerk and Ellem mers had partiicipants answer the third m motivation meeasure which required them to report r their efffort on the seecond word taask by responnding to three statements (e.g., “I was strongly s motiivated at the ssecond Groupp Word Recoggnition Task””). Finally, participantss completed th he manipulatiion check item ms, which weere effective. Results sho owed that, for positive affect, there was a significant main effect ffor relative group performancce and outgrou up (dis)advan ntage. Particippants experiennced more poositive affect w when their group perfo ormed better than t the outgrroup and wheen the outgrouup was advanntaged rather tthan disadvantag ged. In additio on, there was a significantt interaction bbetween relatiive group perfformance and outgrou up (dis)advan ntage. When th he comparisoon between thhe ingroup andd outgroup w was unfavourab ble, participan nts reported more m positive aaffect when thhey were com mpared to an advantaged d versus a disaadvantaged ou utgroup. Wheen the comparrison betweenn groups was favourable, there were no n differences in positive a ffect when thhe outgroup w was advantageed or disadvantag ged. For negaative affect, main m effects foor relative grooup performannce and outgrroup (dis)advanttage were sign nificant. Partiicipants show wed less negattive affect folllowing a favoourable versus unfaavourable com mparison and when the outtgroup was addvantaged verrsus disadvanntaged. The interaction between the relative r perfo ormance and ooutgroup (dis))advantage w was not signifiicant, but showed a siimilar pattern n as positive affect. a Participantss’ belief and confidence c th hat their groupp’s performannce would impprove on the second word task were w aggregatted to form a composite meeasure of colllective efficaccy for perform mance Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 35 im mprovement. Results from Ouwerkerk and a Ellemers (2002) showeed that there w was a significcant m main effect forr relative grou up performance, such that they t reportedd higher efficaacy for im mprovement after a an unfav vourable ratheer than a favou urable compaarison. There was also a siignificant inteeraction betweeen relative group g perform mance and outtgroup (dis)addvantage. Sppecifically, when w the outgrroup was adv vantaged, an unfavourable u comparison lled to stronger coollective efficcacy beliefs fo or doing betteer on the task compared to a favourable comparison, but no diifferences weere observed when w the outg group was disadvantaged. R Responses fo r the self-repoort m measure of mo otivation show wed the same interaction paattern as the ccollective effi ficacy measuree, and noone of the maain effects weere significantt. Ouwerkerk and Ellemerss also showedd that collectivve effficacy mediaated the interaaction between relative gro oup performannce and outgrroup (dis)advantage onn self-reporteed effort. It sh hould be noted d that there were w no signifi ficant effects ffor personal reesponsibility, which is imp portant to show w that intergrroup comparissons elicit afffective and m motivation resp ponses becau use of their inffluence on ind dividuals’ soccial identity. Foor outgroup hindering, h Ou uwerkerk and Ellemers (2002) found a ssignificant maain effect for reelative group performance and outgroup p (dis)advantaage. Participaants were morre likely to hinnder thhe outgroup’s future perforrmance after an a unfavourab ble versus favvourable com mparison and w when thhe outgroup had h been advaantaged versuss disadvantag ged. There waas also a signiificant interacction beetween outgro oup (dis)advaantage and personal respon nsibility, indiccating that paarticipants in tthe hiigh responsib bility condition n showed a greater g tenden ncy to hinder tthe outgroup’’s performancce w when the outgrroup was advantaged vs. disadvantaged d d, but no diffeerences were oobserved in thhe low reesponsibility condition. c Th he interaction between relattive group peerformance annd outgroup (ddis)advantagee was marginaally significan nt, but the patttern parallelss the findings for the other two m motivation measures. Imporrtantly, outgro oup hindering g did not corrrelate with colllective efficaacy or seelf-reported motivation. m O Ouwerkerk and d Ellemers’ (2 2002) study show s that wheen individualss compare theeir ingroup too an addvantaged as opposed to a disadvantageed outgroup, they t experiennce more positive and less neegative affecttive responsess. This impliees that there are potential bbenefits to beiing disadvanttaged ass group ability y comparison ns can be percceived as unju ustified in the particular cirrcumstance. B Being a diisadvantaged ingroup in illlegitimate con ntexts may motivate m membbers to demonnstrate their aability byy working harrder (social competition) when w their rellevant identityy dimensions are threateneed or chhallenged, and there is an opportunity o to o change the existing e statuus hierarchy. IIndeed, not onnly w would participants hinder th he future perfformance of an n outgroup affter doing woorse than the ouutgroup on a task, but also when their group g had beeen previously disadvantageed. Ouwerkerkk and Ellemers’ findiings, thereforre, suggest thaat construal of particular ddimensions (grroup task abillity) w will be impacteed by legitimacy judgemen nts, and this may m lead to soocial competiition in an efffort to boolster or main ntain social id dentity. C Costarelli (201 12) looked at how h individu uals use sociall group attribuutions as a strrategy to mannage thheir identity (ssocial creativity). The attriibutions that individuals i m make to explaiin poor perforrmance off their ingroup p can help theem manage an ny identity th hreat associateed with this pperformance. IIf they seee their group p’s poor perfo ormance as a product p of low w effort, for eexample, this is likely to haave leess negative im mpact on theiir social identtity than if theey attribute thhe poor perforrmance to low w grroup ability, as a ability seem ms harder to change c than effort. e And Coostarelli thougght that this m might bee emphasized d with high as opposed to low group ideentifiers. Speccifically, he hyypothesized tthat hiigh group identifiers would d be more likely to make low effort thann low ability attributions ffor pooor group perrformance wh hen the status differential iss illegitimate and unstablee in order to m manage iddentity threat and re-establiish positive affective a reacttions. Costareelli argued thaat, for low grooup iddentifiers, poo or group perfo ormance was not expected to threaten soocial identityy, and thereforre, they Paage 36 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® would not show s bias in their t attributio ons. He manippulated levels of legitimaccy and identiffication (individual vs. social/gro oup) and used d a continuouss measure of status stabilitty to predict ccausal attributionss and positive affect. The design d was a 2 (Legitimacyy: low vs. higgh) × 2 (Selfcategorizatiion: individuaal vs. social/g group) × contiinuous measuure of perceivved temporal sstability of status between-subjects design. udy were Italiaan secondaryy students. Inggroup and outtgroup Participantss in Costarellii’s (2012) stu status was emphasized e by b highlightin ng the differennces in curricuulum betweenn two types oof schools in Italy (foccus on math and a physics vss. focus on Laatin), and thiss was intended to promote the perception that group bo oundaries werre impermeabble. Self-categgorization wass manipulatedd by asking\participants to lisst either six ad djectives that described theem as “uniquee” individuals (individual)) or as an ingrroup memberr (social). Leggitimacy of loow ingroup staatus was mannipulated by telling participants p th hat postsecond dary academicc achievemennt for their grooup memberss tended to be worse th han outgroup members, and d that this infformation wass either basedd on 15 years of research fro om 10 nationaal universitiess (high legitim macy) or 3 yeears of researcch from 3 natiional universitiess (low legitim macy). Costareelli then askedd participantss to complete a questionnaiire exploring th heir attributio ons (i.e., perceeived temporaal stability) abbout the low status of theirr ingroup on one bipo olar item, and d their attributtions for theirr ingroup’s pooor performannce on three bbipolar items (abiliity, effort). Reesponses to th hese three item ms were aggrregated to form m a compositte attribution score. Next, participants p co ompleted a m measure of possitive affect (i.e., glad, prooud, calm, cheerful, saatisfied). Finaally, participan nts answered items that assessed the efffectiveness off the selfcategorizatiion and legitimacy manipu ulations. Resuults indicated that the maniipulations weere successful. Costarelli (2012) conduccted a hierarchical regressiion analyses tto test the maain predictionss with n self-categoriization, legitiimacy, and staability. Results indicated a attributionss regressed on significant three-way intteraction amo ong the predicctors. Follow--up analyses iindicated thatt within the high leg gitimacy cond dition, there were w no signifficant effects or interactionns. However, within the illegitimacy y condition, th here was a sig gnificant mainn effect for seelf-categorizaation in the soocial identity con ndition, as weell as a signifiicant interactiion between sself-categorization and stabbility. Specifically y, when sociaal identity wass salient and sstatus differennces were perrceived as tem mporal (unstable), participants were w more lik kely to attribuute their groupp’s poor acadeemic perform mance to low effort than t to low ab bility attributiions. Regression analyses werre also conduccted with the positive affecct index regreessed on attribbutions, 4 self-categorrization, and legitimacy. l Results R indicaated that theree was a signifficant main efffect for attributionss, a significan nt interaction between b attribbutions and leegitimacy andd between attrributions and self-cattegorization (Costarelli, ( 20 012). As expeected a signifiicant three-waay interactionn emerged. Follow-up analyses a indiccated that theere were no siignificant effeects or interacctions within tthe individual identity i condiition. Howeveer, within thee social identitty condition, there was a simple main effectt for attributio ons and legitim macy, but moore importantlly, there was a significant simple interaction between attriibutions and legitimacy (seee Figure 7). Stability wass entered as a coovariate, but beccause no significcant effect emergged, the analysees were re-analyyzed without including it. 4 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 37 Figure 7. Positive P affe ect as a function of low w effort attriibutions an nd experime ental legitimacy y. (Costarelli, 2012, p. 5 54)5 A Although theree was no association betweeen positive affect a and attriibutions in thhe high legitim macy coondition, therre was a signifficant associaation in the lo ow legitimacyy condition. G Greater low efffort atttributions weere related to more m positivee affect in thee latter (illegittimate) condittion. C Costarelli (201 12) found thatt when individ duals’ group identity is maade salient (w which should inncrease group p identification n), whether th hey attribute poor p group peerformance too low group eeffort orr low group ability a is consttrained by soccial reality. Low L effort attrributions are m made possible when sttatus differencces are perceiived as illegittimate and unstable. Costarrelli’s findinggs suggest thaat using soocial creativitty to manage identity threaats, i.e., shiftin ng evaluationns to positive rrather negativve diimensions, seerves to preserrve one’s social identity an nd generate p ositive affectt. And this proocess w was observed under u an illeg gitimate statuss structure. O Other research h showed the link l between legitimacy an nd identity maanagement strrategies. For exxample, Betteencourt, Dorr,, Charlton, an nd Hume’s (20 001) meta-annalysis showed that perceivved leegitimacy mod derated the efffect of group p status. Speciifically, high status groupss showed morre faavourable evaaluations towaard their ingro oup compared d to outgroupps on both com mparisons thaat were reelevant to the status differeence and thosee that were no ot, irrespectivve of perceiveed legitimacy. In coontrast, low status groups tended t to show more ingro oup bias on diimensions thaat were imporrtant to thhe status diffeerence when th hey perceived d status differrences as illeggitimate. Thiss latter findingg suuggests that when w status diifferences are not valid, low w status grouups tend to seee themselves more faavourably on status-relevan nt dimensionss to compete with w the highh status group for positive diistinctivenesss. Under such conditions, lo ow status may y be more likkely to use soccial competitiion as a sttrategy to add dress their iden ntity concern ns (Bettencourrt et al.). Furthhermore, wheen status diffeerences w were perceived d to be illegitiimate, low staatus groups ev valuated theirr ingroups moore positivelyy on diimensions thaat were not im mportant to staatus differencces, which sugggest that theey may be moore likkely to use so ocial creativity y for positivee distinction from fr high stattus groups. FFigure plotted ussing unstandardiized ȕ weights frrom final regresssion equation. D Dashed line repreesents the high leggitimacy conditioon and the solid line represents the low legitimaccy condition. 5 Paage 38 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® See Table 5 for a summaary of the reseearch discuss ed in this secction. Table 5. Summary S off identity managementt strategies in respons se to socios structural legitim macy Reference Outco ome Managemennt strategy Schmader, Major, Eccleston, & McCoy (2001) Ͳ Whhen status is perceeived to be legitimaate, higher status groups devalue a dom main in which theyy compare unfavouurably to a lower sstatus group. Ͳ Low wer status groups devalue a domainn in which they compare unffavourably to a higgher status group w when the status ddifference is perrceived to be illegittimate, but not whhen it is perceived to be legitimate. Ͳ Whhen status is perceeived to be illegitim mate, dominant grooups display less ingroup identification and more outgrouup favouritism, whhereas low status grooups display more ingroup identificattion. Ͳ Whhen status is perceeived to be legitimaate, dominant grooup members enggage in more sociaal competition (es pecially when indiividuals are highlyy identified with the grooup) and less outggroup favouritism, while nonminant groups dispplay more outgrouup favouritism. dom Ͳ Am mong both dominannt and non-dominaant groups, those who are highly identified with the inggroup are unlikely to use individual iidentity management strategies such as individdualization and suuperordinate recategorization. Ͳ In Northern N Ireland, Catholics C perceivee the status differeentiation with Prootestants to be less legitimate and m more stable than P Protestants. Ͳ In Northern N Ireland, Catholics C report grreater ingroup identification than Prootestants. Ͳ Am mong both Catholiccs and Protestantss in Northern Irelannd, stability is possitively associated with ingroup idenntification, but amoong Protestants stability is also assocciated with individuualization. Ͳ Am mong both high andd low status groupps, legitimacy is neegatively asssociated with ingrooup identification aand social compettition, but positively asssociated with indivvidualization. Ͳ Ingroup identificationn is negatively assoociated with indiviidualization and supperordinate recategorization, and poositively associateed with social com mpetition. Ͳ Low w status groups (i.e., poor performeers) experience moore positive affect andd less negative afffect when the statuus differentiation is illegitimate (i.e., the outgroup gets an advantage compaared to the ingrouup) rather than legitimate (i.e., the ouutgroup does not rreceive an advanttage compared to the ingroup). w status groups arre more motivatedd to improve their pperformance and Ͳ Low hindder the outgroup’ss performance wheen the status diffeerential is illeggitimate, especially if they feel somee personal responnsibility for the ingroup’s performancce. Ͳ Whhen status is perceeived to be illegitim mate as well as unnstable, poor group perrformance is attributed to low effort rrather than low abbility by those whoo identify with the group and this leads too more positive afffect. c on d imensions that aree important to Ͳ Whhen groups make comparisons stattus differences, high status groups sshow more ingrouup bias regardlesss of the legitimacy of thhe status differencce whereas low staatus groups show w more ingroup bias onnly when the statu s difference is ille gitimate. Ͳ Whhen groups make comparisons c on d imensions that aree irrelevant to stattus differences, high status groups sshow ingroup biass when group bouundaries are perm meable regardless of the legitimacy oor stability of the struucture. Ͳ Whhen group boundarries are impermeaable and status diffferences are perrceived to be illegittimate, high and loow status groups show similar levells of ingroup bias on dim mensions that aree irrelevant to statuus differences. Selective deevaluing (Social creattivity) Dumont & van Lill (2009) Niens & Cairns (2002) Ouwerkerk & Ellemers (2002) Costarelli (20122) Bettencourt, Doorr, Charlton, & Hum me (2001) Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Social comppetition Outgroup favvouritism (Individual m mobility) Social comppetition Individualizaation, superordinatte recategorizaation (Individual m mobility) Social comppetition Ingroup attribution bias (Social creattivity) Social comppetition, ingroup biass (Social competition) Page 39 4 4.3 Perm meability and a Identity Manage ement Strrategies There is also so ome evidencee of the impacct the percepttion of permeaability has onn social identiity m management sttrategies. Forr example, von n Hippel (200 06) wonderedd if group bouundaries weree seen ass permeable, would w low staatus memberss would adoptt a different ssocial identityy, i.e., switch rather thhan fight. Thiss desire, she argued, a mightt be reflected in greater ouutgroup favouuritism. To tesst this nootion, von Hip ppel surveyed d temporary and a permanen nt employees to examine thheir implicit aand exxplicit intergrroup attitudes toward their co-workers. Past researchh has shown thhat temporaryy em mployees are viewed as haaving lower sttatus than perrmanent emplloyees (e.g., B Boroughs, 19994; D Davidson, 1999; both cited in von Hippeel). Moreover,, temporary eemployees apppear to gain pprestige thhrough their affiliation a with h permanent employees e (D Davis-Blake & Uzzi, 1993;; cited in von H Hippel), suggeesting that their relative soccial standing to t permanentt employees iss legitimate. IIn adddition, becau use temporary y employees usually u have the t opportuniity to becomee permanent em mployees, gro oup boundariees between th hese two grou ups can be desscribed as perrmeable. Baseed on thhis intergroup p relation, von n Hippel prediicted that perm manent emplooyees would show more inngroup faavouritism, bu ut that temporrary employees would also o show more ooutgroup favoouritism in thhe hope off upward mob bility. voon Hippel (20 006) asked tem mporary and permanent p em mployees from m various orgganizations inn the U United States to t fill out a su urvey about th heir attitudes toward t their jjob and cowoorkers. Tempoorary em mployees werre asked to in ndicate their agreement on measures thaat assessed theeir perceptionns of thhe relative staatus between temporary t and d permanent employees e annd their perceeptions of grouup peermeability. Permanent P em mployees weree asked to ind dicate the perccentage of theeir temporaryy w workers who would w like to obtain o permanent employm ment. Explicitt bias was asssessed using aan alllocation matrrix (adapted from f Tajfel, Billig, B Bundy,, & Flament, 1971; cited inn von Hippel)) to exxamine behav vioural indicaators of ingrou up favouritism m. Participantts were askedd to read two hyypothetical sccenarios that required r them m to distributee a bonus betw ween a tempoorary and perm manent em mployee and were given a different cho oice matrix for each scenarrio. One of thee matrices alllowed paarticipants to allocate the bonus b equally y between the two memberrs, whereas thhe other forcedd paarticipants to favour eitherr temporary orr permanent employees. e Im mplicit bias w was assessed uusing thhe Linguistic Intergroup I Biias (LIB; Maaass, Salvi, Arcuri, & Seminn, 1989; citedd in von Hipppel). The broad ideaa of the LIB iss that when people are ask ked to describee the behavioour of ingroupp and ouutgroup mem mbers, the conccreteness or abstraction a wiithin their desscription is likkely to naturaally vaary. When peeople describee the positive behaviours of ingroup mem mbers they arre likely to doo so m more abstractly y and more co oncretely for outgroup mem mbers. On thee other hand, when peoplee deescribe the neegative behav viours of ingro oup members they are likeely to do so m more concretelly and m more abstractly y for outgroup p members. These T differen nces emphasizze the positivve traits (and diiminish the im mportance of negative behaaviours) of in ngroup memb ers and highlight the negattive trraits of outgro oup members while lesseniing the impacct of positive bbehaviours (i.e., by limitinng their im mpact to a speecific event orr situation). A total of fourr scenarios weere used to asssess implicit bias, tw wo of which involved a tem mporary emplloyee and two o of which invvolved a perm manent emplooyee as thhe target. The scenarios varried in terms of whether th he employee pperformed weell or poorly. Paarticipants in von Hippel’ss (2006) study y were random mly assigned to read the sccenarios that eeither innvolved the teemporary or permanent p em mployee. Afterr participants read each off the scenarioss, they w were presented d with four deescriptive stattements aboutt the target’s bbehaviour rannging from thhe least too most abstracct, and indicatted their agreement with each statementt. R Results for allo ocation matricces showed th hat temporary y and permaneent employeees favoured thhe peermanent emp ployees, but permanent p em mployees show wed more biaas than temporrary employeees. Paage 40 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Results of the t LIB show wed that both temporary t andd permanent eemployees prreferred moree abstract descriptive statements when w they evaluated the po sitive scenariios, but this prreference wass stronger for a permaanent compareed to a tempo orary employeee, despite peerforming the same positivee behaviour. However, forr the negativee scenarios, booth types of eemployees preeferred more concrete statements, but this prefeerence was stronger for a ppermanent em mployee who performed thhe same negative beehaviour as a temporary t em mployee. Accoording to vonn Hippel’s (20006) findings,, both temporary and a permanen nt employees are implicitlyy biased towaard permanent employees. His results also indicated thaat among temp porary emplooyees, there w was a negativee correlation bbetween explicit and d implicit meaasures such th hat bias towarrd permanent employees oon the explicitt measure also shows bias toward permanent p em mployees on thhe implicit m measure. This association w was not observed am mong perman nent employeees. It is imporrtant to note tthat studies onn intergroup rrelations usually sho ow more bias on the impliccit than the exxplicit measurres as implicitt measures lesssen social desirability y effects. von Hippel specu ulates that perm manent emplooyees may haave been strattegic in their respon nses on the ex xplicit bias meeasure and exxaggerated theeir bias againnst temporary employees. poses of this investigation, von Hippel’’s (2006) studdy suggests thhat when grouup For the purp boundaries are permeablle and status relations r are ssecure, low sttatus memberrs are unlikelyy to be highly iden ntified with th heir ingroup. Her H research sshowed that llow status meembers displayy outgroup faavouritism on n both explicitt and implicitt measures. Too overcome a negative soccial identity, low w status mem mbers will opt for change inn group membbership (indivvidual mobility) when the opportu unity to join a high status group g arises. Blair and Jo ost (2003) alsso looked at th he impact perrceptions of ppermeability hhad on identitty managemen nt strategies. To T this end, they t conducteed an experim mental simulattion in which individuals had to choosse between ind dividual and ccollective forrms of advanccement, a situuation workers oftten deal with in the workpllace. They weere particularlly interested iin the forms oof advancements that peoplle would select based on thheir opportunnity to improvve their sociall standing and their level of identiffication with the t group. Peoople are oftenn confronted w with decidingg between what is bestt for themselv ves versus wh hat is best for their group ((e.g., Dawes, 1980; Krameer & Brewer, 1984, both as ciited in Blair & Jost). This ddecision is paarticularly diffficult for mem mbers of lower statuss or failing grroups, becausse they are oftten faced withh the decisionn between leaving their group, optin ng for individ dual mobility strategies, orr staying loyal to their grouup, dealing with their situation co ollectively (e.g g., Ellemers, Spears, & Dooosje, 1997; T Tajfel, 1975; cited in Blairr & Jost). Research haas shown thatt if individualls have the oppportunity to improve theirr social standiing (i.e., group boun ndaries are perrmeable), they will most liikely choose tto further their own status rather than staying g with their group g (e.g., Elllemers, Wilkke, & van Kniippenberg, 19993). Howeveer, research has also sho own that indiv viduals who strongly s identtify with theirr group tend bbe more inclinned to remain loyaal (e.g., Jamess & Cropanzaano, 1994; Veeenstra & Hasslam, 2000; ccited in Blair & Jost). Blair and Jo ost (2003) preedicted that efffects of grouup permeabilitty on decisionns for individdual versus collective strategies s for improvement i t would be mooderated by inndividuals’ leevel of group identificatio on. Specificallly, group perrmeability efffects would bee stronger forr those who w weakly identify witth their group p relative to th hose who stroongly identifyy with their grroup. Weak iddentifiers would be ex xpected to tak ke advantage of permeablee group bounddaries in ordeer to elevate thheir social standing, whereas w high identifiers i wo ould stay loyaal to their grouup, even if inddividual mobility is an available an nd attractive option. o To tesst their hypothhesis, they coonducted a 2 ((Group Bounddaries: Permeable vs. Impermeaable) × 2 (Ing group Identificcation: Low vvs. High) betw ween-subjectss factorial design. Partticipants weree given the im mpression thaat there was annother experim mental groupp in the study, whicch enabled theem to consideer themselves as a memberr of the assignned group. Grroups Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 41 asssigned to thee low identificcation condition were asked to solve a sseries of probllems indepenndently (ggroup membeers interacted very little) an nd essentially competed wiith their groupp members too win a prrize for the beest individuall solution. Theey were told that t they wouuld have to prrovide writtenn exxplanations an nd justificatio ons for their solutions. s In contrast, c group ups assigned too the high iddentification condition c werre asked to wo ork on the pro oblems interddependently (ggroup membeers w were socially engaged) e and competed wiith other grou ups to win a prrize for the beest group soluution. They were told d that they wo ould have to explain e and ju ustify their solutions to thee other group m members. Nex xt, participantss completed a filler task an nd afterwards were told thaat they wouldd need too solve anotheer set of probllems, and if th he team earnss 100 points, tthen the mem mbers will be eentered foor a prize. Prrior to solving g the problem ms, participantts in Blair and d Jost’s (20033) study weree told that theiir grroup was rand domly assigneed to the “Lab bour” position n, whereas thhe other groupp was assigneed to thhe “Manager”” position. Meembers of the Manager possition were inntended to be higher in stattus, to eaarn more poin nts for every correct c probleem solved, an nd were reportted to have allways earned ennough points for the prize lottery in the past. Particip pants were alsso told that thhese group diifferences refflected real-liffe work situattions. Then paarticipants weere presented with a groupp booundaries maanipulation an nd given indiv vidual mobilitty and collecttive action options (the grooup booundary maniipulation wass presented in the individuaal mobility opption). In the ppermeable coondition for in ndividual mobility, particip pants were to old that they ccan join the M Management ggroup % of baased on their performance on a prelimin nary set of pro oblems and nneeded to earnn less than 50% thhe total pointss (15 out of 35 5). In the imp permeable con ndition, they hhad to earn 1000% of the tootal pooints. For the collective acction option, Blair B and Jostt told participaants that theyy could contribbute all off their points to a group po ool, and if the pool meets a certain numbber of points based on the total nuumber of people in the gro oup pool, mem mbers could advance a to thee Managemennt group. Thiss prresented the opportunity o to o stay or leavee. Before partticipants actuually started thhe series of prroblems, they y had rated thee appeal of th he two optionss and were reequired to tell the experimeenter w which option th hey preferred d with no furth her chance to change theirr decision. Finnally, particippants w were presented d with the man nipulation ch heck items. A According to Blair B and Jost (2003), participants in thee impermeabl e condition w were less likelly to beelieve that a sufficient s num mber of pointss would be acccumulated inn the group poool to move uup to thhe management level comp pared to thosee in the permeeable conditioon. Moreover,, there was a siignificant inteeraction betweeen group ideentification an nd group bounndary permeaability for colllective acction when paarticipants ratted the attracttiveness of thee two strategiies. Specificallly, participannts in thhe low identiffication/ high permeability condition ratted the collecttive action strrategy as the least atttractive optio on, compared to ratings of the other threee conditions.. The other thhree conditionns were eqquivalent in attractiveness. a . For individu ual mobility, there t were noo differences bbetween condditions. Inn terms of parrticipants’ acttual choice off strategy, the results indicaated that theree was an interraction beetween group p identification n and group permeability, p such that parrticipants in thhe low iddentification/h high permeab bility condition were less liikely to choosse collective aaction compaared to thhe other three groups, and again a the otheer three group ps did not diff ffer from eachh other. The findings frrom Blair and d Jost’s (2003 3) research sho ow that whenn individuals hhave the oppportunity to improve their social stand ding, when gro oup boundariies are seen ass permeable, tthose w who weakly id dentify with th he group will most likely choose c to leavve their groupp. Collective aaction too these individ duals is the leeast attractive strategy com mpared to indiividual mobiliity when their group iss low in statuss. On the otheer hand, thosee who strongly y identify witth their groupp will most likkely sttay loyal to th heir group, even if they bellieve that indiividual mobiliity will be moore successfuul than coollective actio on. Loyalty to o a group, therefore, is relaated to how cllosely one ideentifies with tthat Paage 42 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® group. Wheen group boun ndaries are seeen as permeaable and one bbelongs to a llow status grooup, it is more probaable that weak k identifiers will w adopt indiividual mobillity as their prreferred identtity managemen nt strategy to improve their social statuss. In a series of o studies, Vaan Vugt and Hart H (2004) allso examined the role of soocial identity in promoting group g loyalty y under a perm meable situatiion. They propposed the soccial glue hypoothesis of social identtity, which theey believed would w be suppported by eviddence that higghly identifiedd group members would w display more group loyalty l than thhose who weaakly identify with their grooup (as found by Blair and Jost, 2003), especially when thhey could benefit by leavinng the group thhus fostering grroup stability and integrity y. They also exxamine three kinds of reassons why peoople’s social identtity may influ uence group lo oyalty. First, iindividuals’ ppast investmeents in the grooup may mediate thee relation betw ween group id dentity and grroup loyalty, w which is referrred to as the selfperception hypothesis. A second med diator may be individuals’ extremely poositive percepttions of ferred to as thee group-perceeption hypothhesis. Finally,, a third mediiator may their group, which is refe originate from a generic social norm such s as feelinng obligated tto help other ggroup membeers or doing what other group members m mig ght do, which is referred too as the norm--perception hy hypothesis. The researcchers tested th heir hypothesees in three stuudies. The ressults of studiees 1 and 2 aree summarized d and Study 3 is described d in detail becaause it buildss on the findinngs from the ffirst two studies. V and Harrt (2004) founnd that when there was an attraction exiit option In Studies 1 and 2, Van Vugt (permeable boundaries), participants who stronglyy identified wiith the group were more likkely to stay with th he group than those who weakly w identifi fied with the ggroup, thus prromoting grouup integrity. When W the exit option was un nattractive (bboundaries weere impermeaable), there waas no difference in i group loyallty between high h and low iidentifiers. Thhe findings w were not mediated by the amount of time participants were willing to invvest in their ggroup, failing to support a sselfperception explanation for f the social glue hypothe sis. They did find, howeveer, that particiipants’ positive perrceptions, butt not past inveestments, meddiated the relaation betweenn group identiification and group loyalty, l such that t higher id dentification w was related too more positivve group perceptions, which in tu urn, was relateed to more gro oup loyalty, pproviding suppport for the ggroup-percepttion hypothesis. d a 2 (group iddentification: low vs. high)) × 2 (other’s choice: In Study 3, the researcheers conducted stay vs. leav ve) between-ssubjects facto orial design. P Participants arrrived at the eexperimental session in groups of six and were to old that the sttudy was con ducted by theeir own univeersity and the university’ss rival. Particiipants were either told thatt the researchhers were inteerested in how w well groups of sttudents from each universiity perform thhe task (high identificationn condition) oor that the researchers were interestted in how individual studdents perform on the task (llow identificaation condition). p in a comp puter-mediateed group investment task w with the other Next, particcipants took part participantss in their expeerimental sesssion. At the bbeginning of eeach trial, partticipants receeived an endowmentt that could eiither be invessted in the grooup or themseelves. If the ggroup’s investtment reached a certain value, then all of thee participantss would get a return regarddless of what tthey had invested du uring the trial.. Bogus feedb back was giveen to participaants after eachh trial, and thhe results suggested to participantss that the grou up had failed in the majoriity of trials. A After the sixthh trial, participantss received a message m sayin ng that they w were half way through the iinvestment tassk and were given the opportun nity to leave th he group to w work on their oown for the reemainder of tthe task. If they decideed to work on their own, th hey would be given a fixedd amount of m money for eacch trial which woulld be more th han if they stay yed with theiir group. Thenn they were toold that the otther group Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 43 m members woulld have to maake a decision n to stay or leaave the groupp and that for tthe rest of thee task thheir group wo ould still need d at least four contributors in i order to eaarn a bonus. W While participaants were decciding whetheer they should d stay or leavee the group, thhey were ablee to see thhe decision off one other gro oup member. Participants were assigne d a letter from m (A-F) and deecisions weree always madee in alphabetiical order and d the participaant was alwayys assigned too “M Member B”. Participants P either e obtained d feedback th hat Member A chose to stayy in the groupp (oother stay con ndition) or to leave l the grou up and work independently i y (other leavee condition). Paarticipants theen answered the t dependen nt measures, which w includeed an item aboout emotionall looyalty (“I feell loyal to this group”), behaavioural loyalty (“For the nnext trials, I w want to remaiin a m member of thiss group”), and d items assesssing external attributions ( e.g., “Was it because the ttask is diifficult to und derstand?”) an nd internal atttributions (e.g g., “…people are not pullinng their weighhts for thhe group?). Fiinally, they in ndicated how attractive theey found the ooption to leave the group. A two-way AN NOVA showeed that there was w a main efffect of group identificationn for behavioural looyalty, such th hat those who o strongly iden ntified with th he group werre more willinng to stay withh the grroup than those who were weakly identtified. In addittion, participaants’ decisionn to stay with the grroup was not influenced by y another grou up member’s choice (norm m-perceptionss). Additionall annalyses showeed that emotio onal loyalty significantly s mediated m the rrelation betw ween identificaation annd behaviouraal loyalty, succh that the mo ore participan nts identified w with their grooup, the moree loyal thhey felt to their group, which in turn, waas associated with their deccision to stayy with the grouup. Fuurther analysees indicated that t participan nts who highly identified w with their grouup were moree likely too make extern nal attribution ns and less likely to make in nternal attribuutions for theeir group’s faiilure reelative to thosse who weakly y identified with w their grou up. Taken togetherr, the findingss demonstratee that social id dentity can bee beneficial bby keep groupps intact w when the boun ndaries betweeen groups aree permeable. When W leavingg the group beenefits the sellf, inndividuals wh ho highly (vs. weakly) iden ntify with theiir group are m more likely to stay with thee grroup, displayiing their grou up loyalty. Th hus, social ideentity can be pperceived as tthe social glue that keeep groups to ogether that may m otherwise dissolve. If individuals i w were to leave thheir group, thhey w would also tak ke valuable ressources (i.e., monetary con ntributions) frrom the groupp. Further, inndividuals red duced threat in nflicted on th he group (i.e., attractive exiit option) by sshowing stronnger grroup loyalty. Thus, social identity i is a way w to maintaain stability annd integrity inn groups. Finally, grroup loyalty seems s to be a result of evalluating the grroup positivelly (group-percception) ratheer than hoow much onee has invested d into the grou up (personal-p perception) orr how other ggroup memberrs are beehaving (norm m-perception)). H Hornsey and Hogg H (2002) examined e superordinate reccategorizationn as a social iidentity m management sttrategy (mobiilization) for low l status gro oup memberss, and consideered how this might bee adopted wh hen group bou undaries were not dependen nt on permeabbility. They eexplained thatt when inndividuals aree part of a low w status group p, they often have h a supero rdinate category that they share w with other subg groups, and th hey may try to obtain a mo ore positive iddentity by cattegorizing thhemselves as part p of the su uperordinate group g rather th han the subgrroup to whichh they belong.. They arrgued that this strategy is an a efficient way w of managiing negative iidentity, becauuse it is not deependent on permeable p gro oup boundariees. Rather, it involves shift fting the levell of inclusivenness of thhe individualss’ group categ gory to be viewed differenttly by themseelves and otheers. However,, when m members of low w status grou up use this straategy, they may m feel threattened by a lacck of positivee diistinctivenesss from other groups g and do omination by members m from m the higher sstatus subgroups (M Mummendey & Wenzel, 1999; cited in Hornsey & Hogg). H As succh, this strateggy may lead Paage 44 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® members off low status groups g to expeerience a confflict between enhancing thheir identity aand defending their t distinctiv veness among g other group s. Hornsey an nd Hogg (2002 2) examined if people whoo perceive theeir group as loow in status teend to identify mo ore with superrordinate cateegories than thhose who perrceive their grroup as high iin status. They also examined e the effect of cateegorization onn how membeers respond too status inform mation by emphasizin ng either their superordinatte category orr both their suuperordinate aand subgroup categories. This can test whether threats to subgrouup distinctiveeness influencces the strateggies used by low statu us group mem mbers. Hornseey and Hogg hypothesizedd that individuuals induced tto categorize themselves t ass a superordin nate member w would show ggreater interggroup bias thaan those induced to categorize theemselves as both b a superorrdinate and suubgroup mem mber. In additiion, the viduals perceiv ved their subg group status, the more theyy would categgorize themseelves at lower indiv the superordinate level. Hornsey an nd Hogg (2002 2) tested theirr hypotheses in two studies. In Study 1,, they manipuulated level of group caategorization (superordinat ( te vs. simultanneous) and m manipulated suubgroup status (high status vs. lo ow status) viaa false feedbacck. In generall, they found support for thheir hypothesses. However, th he status man nipulation had d little indepeendent effect oon participantts’ perceptionns of their ingroup. Ho ornsey and Hogg reasoned d that some paarticipants maay not have taaken the falsee feedback seriously, and a easily dism missed or ratiionalized anyy type of negaative feedbackk. Therefore, tthey measured perceptions p off subgroup staatus and also manipulated level of groupp categorizatiion in Study 2. s maths--science studeents completeed a questionnnaire about In Hornsey and Hogg (2002) second study, des toward being a memberr of their facuulty area, andd one of the iteems assessed their their attitud perceptionss of relative su ubgroup statu us (e.g., “Thinnk for a moment about the relative statuus of maths-scien nce and humaanities studentts at the Univversity of Queeensland”). N Next, participaants were asked to thiink about a go overnment prroposal to turnn a vacant pieece of land intto a park. Parrticipants were given a list of objeccts and servicces and askedd to indicate thhe items that they think woould lead to a well-fu unctioning parrk. They weree presented w with the categoorization mannipulation. In the superordina ate condition, participants were given innstructions thhat made them m focus on theeir university membership m and a less so on n themselves aas individualss by asking thhem to choosee the label that applied d to them (“U UQ student” vss. “town plannner”) and preesenting them m with the univversity logo on carrdboard. They y were told that the researcchers were intterested in loooking at univeersity students’ reesponses becaause past research has show wn that they ccan have insigghts into probblems that town planners may not have. h In the siimultaneous ccondition, afteer receiving tthe instructionns from the superordinate condition, they werre also told th at the researcchers were intterested in com mparing responses between b math hs-science and d humanities sstudents. Studdents in the ddifferent faculties were given differrent coloured pen to fill ou ut their responnse sheet, whiich enabled a symbolic disstinction between thee two facultiees without phy ysically separrating them innto groups. Thheir responsee sheets allowed theem to circle th he labels that represented ttheir superorddinate group ((“UQ student””) or their subgroup (““maths-sciencce” or “human nities”). Agaiin, group bouundaries were not dependennt on permeabilitty. Hornsey an nd Hogg (2002 2) measured the t manipulattion check iteem, participannts level of identificatio on with the faaculty area an nd the universsity (e.g., “In terms of yourr general attittudes and beliefs, how w much do yo ou feel similarr to other peoople in your faaculty area?; adapted from m Hains, Hogg, & Duck, 1997; cited in Hornseey & Hogg), aand the extentt to which theey wanted to bbe categorized d at the supero ordinate or su ubgroup level.. Finally, inteergroup bias w was assessed oon three dimensionss: representatiion bias (the difference d bettween feelingg good about tthe superordinnate group Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 45 beeing represen nted by all hum manities vs. all a maths-scien nce students) , contact biass (the differennce beetween how much m fun it would w be to wo ork on a task with humanitties vs. mathss-science studdents), annd likeability bias (the diffference betweeen general po ositivity towaard humanities vs. maths-sccience sttudents). R Results showed d that the cateegorization manipulation m was w effective.. Hornsey andd Hogg (20022) coonducted regrression analysses with the categorization c n and status vaariables and ttheir interactioon as thhe predictor variables, v and identification n, self-categorrization, and intergroup bias as the criteerion vaariables. Anallyses for participants’ leveel of identificaation with theeir faculty areea showed thaat there w was a significaant interaction n between thee two predicto ors. In the supperordinate coondition, the hhigher inn status mathss-science stud dents viewed themselves, t th he more they identified wiith their facullty arrea, whereas for f those in th he simultaneo ous condition,, there was noo relation betw ween status annd iddentification. For participan nts’ preferred d level of selff-categorizatioon, there was only a main eeffect off status indicaating that the lower in statu us maths-science students pperceived theeir faculty to bbe, the m more they prefferred to be co onsidered a member m of thee university. Foor intergroup bias (the threee bias measu ures were com mbined into a single index)), Hornsey and H Hogg (2002) found fo a main effect e of statu us, but this waas qualified bby the two-waay interaction.. The innteraction sho owed that, in the t superordin nate condition n, the higher iin status studdents perceiveed their faaculty to be, th he more ingro oup bias they displayed. In n the simultanneous conditioon, there was no siignificant asso ociation betw ween the statuss and ingroup p bias. Furtherr analyses connsisting of a m median spplit on the status variable showed s that high h status parrticipants in thhe superordinnate conditionn shhowed more ingroup i bias than t high status participantts in the simuultaneous conddition as welll as loow status partticipants in bo oth categorizaation conditions. The findings frrom Hornsey and Hogg’s (2002) ( two stu udies indicatee that superorrdinate reecategorizatio on, essentially y a mobilizatio on strategy, may m be used bby low status members maanage thheir identity concerns. Likee von Hippel (2006) and Blair B and Jost (2003), if inddividuals see aan oppportunity to adopt anotheer social identtity to improv ve their currennt status, theyy may do this if grroup boundarries are permeeable. In the case c of Hornsey and Hogg,, individuals m managed theiir low sttatus by assum ming a supero ordinate categ gory as this grroup permeabbility was not restricted. Onn the otther hand, Ho ornsey and Ho ogg showed th hat high statu us members w will protect theeir subgroup diistinctivenesss by showing more ingroup p bias (negativ ve perceptionns of the low sstatus group) when thhey are catego orized at the superordinate s level with lower status subbgroups. No ingroup bias was prresent when the t distinctiveeness of the su ubgroups wass upheld. Highh status groupps, therefore, might siimply reject th he mobilizatio on strategy, superordinate s recategorizattion, in order to maintain thheir hiigh status iden ntity and distinctiveness. B Boen, Vanbeseelaere, and Co ool (2006) ex xamined the reelation between the perceivved status of an orrganization affter a merger and employees’ level of id dentification w with the new organization. When a merger occurrs between tw wo organizatio ons, employeees are often faaced with a siituation that innvolves a chan nge in group membership from the pre--merger organnization to thee new organizzation. Inn addition, thee new organizzation includees the pre-merrger ingroup and the pre-m merger outgrooup. Inndividuals bellonging to a low status verrsus high statu us pre-mergerr organizationn might have diifferent motiv ves after a meerger. The form mer group maay be more atttuned to enhaancing their sstatus (bbolstering), whereas w the lattter may be more m attuned to protecting ttheir status, especially wheen the neew merger orrganization is perceived as high in statuss. As this is a merger, shift fting group booundaries sho ould be underrstood as perm meable, and to o bolster theirr social identiity, low statuss group m members migh ht adopt an ind dividual mobility strategy and embrace their new poost-merger orrganization id dentity. Paage 46 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Boen et al. (2006) hypotthesized that employees’ e pperceptions off their status bbefore and aftter the merger wou uld influence their level off identificationn with the post-merger orgganization. Sppecifically, among emp ployees of thee high status pre-merger p orrganization thhere should bee a positive reelation between theeir perceived post-merger status and theeir level of iddentification w with the post-m merger organization, but this asssociation, Boeen et al. argueed, should bee stronger amoong employeees of the low status pre-merger p orrganization. Itt was also hyppothesized thhat employeess’ pre-merger status and post-merger identificatio on would influ uence post-m merger identifiication. Speciifically, high status premerger emp ployees would d show a posiitive associatiion between ttheir pre- andd post-merger levels of identificatio on, but this asssociation wo ould be strongger for low staatus pre-mergger employeess. Boen et al. also exp pected their peerceived statu us and identifiication after tthe merger woould be positiively related but only for employees who weakly w identiffied with the ppre-merger orrganization annd not for those that strongly identiified with thee pre-merger oorganization. Finally, the rresearchers teentatively hypothesizeed that pre- an nd post-merger status and pre-merger iddentification w would interacct to influence post-merger id dentification, such that the relation betw ween post-merrger status annd postmerger iden ntification wo ould be for weeakly identifi ed employeess of the low sstatus pre-merrger organization. ( adminiistered a quesstionnaire to eemployees froom two To test their predictions,, Boen et al. (2006) companies. Company A had six different sites andd 460 employeees, whereas Company B hhad one site, 102 em mployees, and d was in serious financial tr trouble. Basedd on this inforrmation, Com mpany A was higher in status than n Company B. B Company A was in the pprocess of taking over Com mpany B, which impllied that Com mpany B would d cease to exiist and that itss site would bbe another sitee for Company A. A The questio onnaire was distributed d to employees inn Company B and employeees from two sites in n Company A. In all three, the majority of respondennts were blue--collar employyees. However, site s A1 had th he greatest num mber of whitee-collar emplloyees compaared with sitess A2 and B because it had h productio on, commerciaal, and adminnistrative unitss. The three ssites did not ddiffer in terms of thee proportion of o men and women w and disstribution of aage. Participantss were asked to indicate th heir level of iddentification w with the pre-m merger organiization on three items (e.g., “Before the merger I felt very connnected to coompany A [coompany B] beefore the merger”; ad dapted from Boen, B Vanbesselaere, De W Witte, and Luijjters, 2003; ciited in Boen eet al.) and level of identification wiith the post-m merger organizzation on threee items simillar to those ussed to measure pree-merger iden ntification (e.g., “I feel verry connected tto company A after the meerger”). They were also asked to indicate theirr perceived prre-merger staatus (e.g., “Thhe prestige off company A before th he merger, com mpared with company c B bbefore the merrger was…” rrated on a 5-ppoint scale where 1 = much m lower an nd 5 = much higher) and ppost-merger sttatus (e.g., “C Compared witth other corrugated board produccing organizattions, the presstige of comppany A after tthe merger is at the moment…”” rated on a 5-point scale where w 1 = mucch lower and 5 = much higgher). Boen et al. (2006) found d that employeees of site B pperceived thee status of their pre-mergerr organization as significaantly lower than those of siites A1 and A A2. For post-m merger status,, employees from site A1 perceived thee post-mergerr status as siggnificantly low wer than pre-m merger status, wherreas employeees from site B perceived thhe post-merger status as siignificantly hhigher than pre-merger status. And employees e fro om site A2 peerceived no chhange in statuus from pre- tto postmerger. Forr pre-merger identification n, employees ffrom site A2 showed signiificantly less identificatio on with the prre-merger org ganization thaan did those fr from site A1. Additionally,, there were no diffferences in prre-merger ideentification am mong employyees from sitees A1 and B. Finally, employees from site B sh howed signifficantly less p ost-merger iddentification ccompared to tthose from sites A1 and A2, and theese latter two groups did noot differ from m each other. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 47 B Boen et al. (20 006) conducteed regression analyses to teest their hypottheses, with ppre-merger staatus, poost-merger staatus, and pre--merger identtification as th he predictor vvariables and post-merger iddentification as a the criterion variables. Results R showeed that the thrree-way interaaction was noot siignificant. Ho owever, all off the predicted d two-way intteractions werre significant. The interacttion beetween pre-m merger status and a post-merg ger status sho owed that amoong low statuus pre-merger em mployees, thee higher they perceived thee new organizzation to be, thhe more they identified wiith it. N No relation bettween post-m merger status and a post-merg ger identificattion was obseerved for highh status prre-merger em mployees. The interaction between b pre-m merger status and pre-mergger identificattion shhowed that fo or both high- and a low status pre-merger organizationss, the more em mployees idenntified w with their pre-m merger organ nization, the more m they also o identified w with the post-m merger organiization. H However, this relation was stronger for employees e fro om the high sttatus pre-merrger organizattion. Fiinally, the intteraction betw ween post-merrger status an nd pre-mergerr identificationn showed thaat em mployees weaak in pre-merrger identificaation showed a positive rellation betweenn post-merger status annd post-mergeer identificatiion, whereas employees e high in pre-merrger identificaation showedd no siignificant relaation between n the two variaables. The findings frrom Boen et al. a (2006) stud dy suggest th hat for low staatus employeees, their level of poost-merger id dentification was w strongly influenced i by y the status off the post-merrger organizattion. A Also, for high status group, it may have been b easier to o transfer theiir identificatioon from the prem merger to the post-merger p organization, o which w may ex xplain why thhere was no reelation between preannd post-mergeer identificatiion. For the lo ow status grou up, they expeerienced the ggreatest amounnt of orrganizational changes and their identificcation with th he pre-mergerr organizationn may have trransferred to the t post-merg ger organizatio on to some ex xtent given thhat their workk location andd cow workers remain ned the same, which might explain the positive p relattion between ppre- and postm merger identifiication. In terrms of the relaation between n post-mergerr status and prre-merger iddentification, employees who w do not feeel closely tied d to their pre-m merger organization may bbe m more likely to psychologicaally disengagee from it, the more m they vieew the post-m merger organization ass higher in staatus, as such the t higher thee perceived status of the neew organizatioon, the more they iddentify with itt. In contrast, for those thatt do feel closeely tied to theeir pre-mergerr organizationn no reelation betweeen post-mergeer status and post-merger p identification i may have beeen observed, beecause they are a more likely y to remain faaithful to the pre-merger o rganization thhan to be perssuaded In the end, moving byy the possibility of status enhancement. e m up by individual m mobility mightt be a goood identity management m strategy s when n group bound daries are perrmeable. There is other research that connects the perception off permeabilityy with identitty strategies. F For exxample, Betteencourt et al.’s (2001) metaa-analysis sho owed that whhen comparisoons were madde on diimensions thaat were irrelev vant to status differences, high h status grroups generallly favoured thhe inngroup over reelevant outgro oups. Howev ver, there weree also interacttive effects off the sociostruuctural vaariables on th hese dimensio ons. When gro oup boundariees were perceeived as perm meable, high sttatus grroups showed d more ingrou up bias than lo ow status grou ups, regardlesss of stabilityy or legitimacyy. H However, the difference d bettween the hig gh and low staatus groups w was smaller whhen the groupp booundaries were impermeab ble. Also, wheen group bou undaries were impermeablee and when sttatus diifferences weere illegitimate, high and lo ow status grou ups showed s imilar levels of ingroup biias. B Based on Betteencourt et al.’’s meta-analysis, it appearss that, under m most conditioons for high sttatus grroups, they may m be more liikely to use social creativitty strategies ffor positive inngroup diistinctivenesss by evaluatin ng their own group g more po ositively on ddimensions that are irrelevaant to thhe status hieraarchy. Table 6, summ marizes the ressearch pertain ning to the im mpact of percep eptions of perm meability on iddentity manag gement strateg gies. Paage 48 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Table 6. Summary S off identity managementt strategies in respons se to socios structural ability permea Reference Outccome Managemennt strategy von Hippel (20006) Ͳ Loow status group members m (i.e., tem mporary employeess) who feel that grroup boundaries are a permeable andd that the social sttructure is stable, shhow greater outgrooup favouritism annd ingroup derogaation in an effort too chhange their group membership. Outgroup favouritism, ingroup deroogation (Individual m mobility) Blair & Jost (20003) Ͳ Inndividuals who aree weakly identified with their group aand perceive group booundaries to be peermeable are lesss likely to favour coollective action and arre more likely to faavour leaving theirr group if the oppoortunity arises. Ͳ Inndividuals who aree highly identified w with their group arre likely to stay looyal to their group even if they perceeive the boundariees between the grroups to be permeeable. Group loyallty (Individual mobility) Van Vugt & Hart (2004) Ͳ Inndividuals who aree strongly identifiedd with their group show more groupp looyalty to their group when there is ann attractive optionn to leave the grouup annd this is mediatedd by their positive perceptions of thee group rather thaan hoow invested they feel f in the group. Ͳ Group G loyalty may be b used by high iddentifiers as a straategy to avert exxternal threat to thhe group to maintaain the stability an d integrity of the grroup when group boundaries b appeaar to be permeablee. Group loyaltty (Individual mobility) Hornsey & Hoggg (2002) Ͳ Irrespective of perm meability, low statuus group memberss can adopt suuperordinate recattegorization as a m mobilization strateegy to improve theeir sttatus as this is nott dependent on grooup boundary perrmeability. Ͳ High status group members m maintainn their distinctiveneess from low statuus grroup members by focusing on their subgroup rather t han superordinatee grroup identity, and perceive low statuus group memberss more negativelyy when w they are categorized with both superordinate andd subgroup iddentities. Superordinaate recategorizaation (Individual m mobility) Boen, Vanbeseelaere & Cool (2006) Ͳ When W merging one organization into another and grouup boundaries are peermeable, membeers of the lower staatus organization identify with the neew organization more m strongly whenn their perceived sstatus in the new orrganization is highher. Ͳ Those in a high status organization w who were more strrongly identified with w the organizatioon pre-merger are also more identifiied post-merger. This is not true of loow status organizaation members. Ͳ Foor those who are weakly w identified w with their organizaation pre-merger, thheir identification post-merger p is asssociated with their perceived status. Organizationnal identificationn (Individual mobility) Bettencourt, Doorr, Charlton, & Hum me (2001) Ͳ When W group bounddaries are permeabble high status grooups show more inngroup favouritism on dimensions thhat are both relevaant and irrelevant tto thhe group distinction, regardless of thhe legitimacy or sttability of the statuus sttructure. Ͳ When W group bounddaries are seen to be permeable, low w status group members m favour thee outgroup if theree are opportunitiess for individual mobility, m but favour the ingroup if thesse opportunities aare not available. Ͳ When W group bounddaries are impermeeable and status ddifferences are peerceived to be illeggitimate and unstaable, high and low w status groups shhow similar levels of ingroup bias onn dimensions that are irrelevant to sttatus differences. Ingroup biass (Social competition)) Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Positive disstinctiveness (Social creativity) Page 49 4 4.4 Othe er Researc ch examin ning Socio ostructura al Beliefs and Identtity Mana agement Strategies S s Soome research h specifically examined e all three sociostrructural belieefs and the choice of identiity m management sttrategies. Forr example, Keessler and Mu ummendey’s ((2002) longituudinal study exxamined chan nges in percep ptions of interrgroup relatio ons and hypotthesized causaal sequence oof the prrocesses positted by social identity theorry and relative deprivationn theory (RDT T; e.g., Ellemeers, 19993; cited in Kessler K and Mummendey) M ). They explaiin that both thheories postullate a causal seequence wherreby individuaals’ perceptio ons of intergro oup relations influence ideentity processes, and thhese processess affect the id dentity manag gement strateg gy that is usedd. Despite theeir similaritiess, the tw wo theories diiverge with reespect to diffeerent mediatin ng processes. Specifically,, SIT emphasiizes coognitive variaables related to t aspects of group g membeership, such aas level of grooup identificattion (B Brown & Rosss; cited in Keessler and Mu ummendey), whereas w RDT T emphasizes affective variiables reelated to grou up deprivation n, such as feellings of resenttment towardd groups (e.g.,, Cook, Crosbby, & H Hennigan, 197 77; cited in Keessler and Mu ummendey). The unification n of East and West German ny provided the t social conntext backdropp for Kessler and M Mummendey’ss (2002) study y. This mergeer was expected to bring toogether two grroups of uneqqual sttatus under a single s commo on identity (i..e., Germans). They explaiined that Eastt Germans aree tyypically perceeived to be infferior to Westt Germans. Kessler K and M Mummendey teested the relattions beetween the major m variabless outlined in SIT S and RDT T. Figure 8 shoows their moddel which is aan exxpanded versiion of Tajfel and Turner’s (1986). Figure 8. Inte egrated SIT T-and-RDT model m (Kess sler and Mu ummendey, 2002, p. 76 6) Inn general, perceptions of th he sociostructtural characterristics were eexpected to prredict the idenntity m management sttrategies used d, but to be mediated m by grroup identificaation (derivedd from SIT) oor reesentment (deerived from RDT). R Kesslerr and Mummeendey (2002) also includedd a third mediiator in thhe model, colllective efficaccy, which ind dicates expecttations of impprovement or deterioration in inntergroup dyn namics. Collecctive efficacy y is also derived from RDT T. Overall, theey hypothesizzed that Paage 50 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® the perceptiions of the intergroup struccture would iinfluence leveel of group identification, ffeelings of resentment,, and collectiv ve efficacy. In n addition, Keessler and Muummendey exxpected identtification to predict collective efficcacy and reseentment, as grreater identification with a group is assoociated with more collective c effiicacy (e.g., Mummendey M eet al., 1999) aand increases the possibilityy of experiencin ng group-baseed emotions (e.g., Smith, 11993; cited in Kessler and M Mummendeyy). Their prediction was w less clearr for the relatiion between ccollective effiicacy and reseentment. To examinee the effectiveeness of identtity managem ment strategiess, Kessler andd Mummendeey (2002) assessed thrree classes off strategies. Fiirst, individuaal strategies w were assessedd by social moobility (individualss try to leave the ingroup for f another grroup) and recaategorizationn at a higher llevel (ingroup an nd outgroup members m beco ome part of a superordinatee category – rrecall Hornseyy & Hogg, 2002). Seco ond, collectiv ve strategies in ncluded sociaal competitionn (conflict oveer favourablee evaluationss) and realistic competition n (conflict oveer real resourrces, such as m money and lannd). Finally, creeative strategies included with w preferencce of temporaal comparisonns (placing moore importance on the comparison of pastt and present situation of E East Germanss than on the comparison n between Easst and West Germans) G andd re-evaluation of the material or econoomic dimension (perceiving ( th he material co omparison dim mension as leess important than the sociaal dimension for f a positive social identitty). The threee strategies deelineated by K Kessler and Mummendeey essentially y map onto Taajfel and Turnner’s (1986) tthree, individuual mobility, social competition n, and social creativity c resp pectively. All of Kesssler and Mum mmendey’s (20 002) participaants were peoople who weree born and stiill resided in East Gerrmany. Repeaated measures were taken aat four differeent time pointts, spaced onee year apart. In each questionnaaire, participaants were askeed to indicatee whether theiir status was iinferior, equal, or su uperior compaared to West Germans. G Theen they were asked to indiicate their perrceptions of intergrou up stability (ee.g., “I think th he relationshiip between Eaast and West Germany willl remain stable for th he next few years”), y legitim macy (e.g., “T The West Gerrmans are enttitled to be better off than the Eaast Germans”)), and permeaability (e.g., “IIn principle, iit is not difficcult for an Easst German to be consid dered as a Weest German.”)). There weree three items ffor each socioostructural vaariable and they were all a answered on o a 5-point rating scale froom 1 (compleetely disagreee) to 5 (complletely agree). Theen Kessler and d Mummendeey rated the m material groupp status of Eaast Germans (ee.g., standard off living, econo omic status) on o a 5-point raating scale ran anging from 1 (worst situattion) to 5 (best situatiion). Participaants answered d questions asssessing sociaal identification (e.g., “I iddentify with East Germans”), G resentment (e.g g., “Being facced daily withh the situationn of the East G Germans one can onlly become ann noyed”), and collective eff fficacy (e.g., ““We East Gerrmans can chaange the relation to the t West Germ mans by our own o effort”). Three items were used to assess each oof these variables. mmendey (20002) had threee items for inddividual To measuree individual sttrategies, Kesssler and Mum mobility (e.g., “I make any a effort to be b consideredd as West Germ man”). For reecategorizatioon at a higher level they calculaated the differrence betweenn two items (““I regard mysself as Germaan” and “I regard myself as East Geerman”). Sociial competitioon was assesssed on three ittems (e.g., “W We will show the West W Germanss that we are the t more efficcient Germans”) as was reaalistic compettition (e.g., “If neew jobs arise in i the next fiv ve years, we E East Germanss will have too make sure thhat these jobs will bee established in i East Germany rather thaan in West Geermany”). Keessler and Muummendey took the diffference betw ween two item ms to measure re-evaluationn of the materrial or econom mic dimension (“The ( East Germans consiider economicc wealth as: uundesirable-deesirable” and “The West Germ mans consider economic weealth as: undeesirable-desiraable” both onn 5-point scalees). Similarly, preference p of temporal com mparisons wa s measured by taking the ddifference bettween two items (“Acccording to your opinion, ho ow importantt is it for Eastt Germans to compare them mselves Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 51 w with the situatiion before thee unification?”; “According g to your opinnion, how im mportant is it ffor the East Germans to compare th hemselves with West Germ mans?”). Theyy explained thhat higher num mbers inndicated stron nger preferencce for that straategy. W With respect to o high and low w status, Kesssler and Mum mmendey (20002) found thaat for group sttatus, ovver 80% of paarticipants ratted West Germ mans as being g superior to East Germanns, whereas thhe reemaining partticipants rated d the two grou ups as being equal. e Very feew participannts rated East G Germans as beeing superior to t West Germ mans. Over the four measurrements, therre was a margginally siignificant incrrease in the nu umber of partticipants ratin ng East and W West Germanss being equal in sttatus. Means for f the variab bles in the model were subm mitted to a reppeated-measuures ANOVA A to exxamine wheth her the meanss changed oveer time and whether the ch anges were liinear, quadrattic, or cuubic. All chan nges were lineear unless oth herwise noted d. The results showed that E East Germanss peerceived increeases in their material grou up status, stattus stability, aand status legiitimacy, sugggesting thhat they believ ve their materrial situation improved i and d that the inteergroup relatioons were secuure (i.e., stable and d legitimate), so further im mprovements in i status may be slow or unnlikely to occcur. In adddition, there were decreasses in both ideentification with w East Germ mans and reseentment towaard the inntergroup relaationship. Forr perceived co ollective efficaacy, there weere significantt changes oveer the coourse of the study (cubic trrend). In term ms of preferences for collecctive strategiees, both sociall and reealistic compeetition decreaased over timee. None of thee other identitty strategies cchanged over time, exxcept for re-evaluation of material m dimeension (cubic trend). K Kessler and Mummendey (2 2002) also conducted trait--state analysees, which invoolves decompposing vaariables into trait t (stable diifferences am mong participaants) and statee (fluctuatingg variance duee siituation influeences of meassurement erro or) componentts. The correllations betweeen the trait coomponents sh howed that ideentification iss positively co orrelated withh resentment aand stability, and neegatively corrrelated with permeability p and a individuaal strategies (i .e., mobility aand reecategorizatio on). Results allso showed reesentment was correlated w with all sociosstructural varriables annd collective strategies. Th here was an asssociation bettween identifi fication and reesentment. Thhe coorrelations aree presented in n Table 7. T Table 7. Corrrelations be etween traitt componen nts of the S SIT-and-RDT T model. (Ke essler and Mu ummendey, 2002, p. 83 3) Inn terms of thee effectivenesss of identity management m strategies, s botth trait and sttate componennts inndicated that, for almost alll strategies, th here was no evidence e that they had posiitive effects. T There w was some evid dence showing g that recategorization as Germans G was associated w with more posiitive peerceptions of group materiial status. How wever, this asssociation didd not show anyy long-term bbenefits. Paage 52 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® In general, the dynamicss between traiit and state coomponents didd not fit Kesssler and Mum mmendey’s (2002) hypo othesized seq quential proceess predicted bby SIT and R RDT. In additiion, there wass no evidence from the analyses for the ex xpected relatioons between vvariables. Theese findings cchallenge the assumptions of SIT and a RDT, wh hich suggest thhat the psychoological process of selectinng an identity maanagement strategy is lineaar and sequenttial. Instead, tthe findings pprovide suppoort for parallel pro ocesses, in wh hich people haave a stable configuration of beliefs. In the intergrouup context, the belief sy ystem may in nclude variablles, such as peerceptions abbout the statuss structure, grroup identificatio on, and preferrences for ideentity manageement strategiies. Kessler aand Mummenddey’s research suggests that th he representatiion of these vvariables overrlaps with eacch other so thaat changes in one variaable (e.g., gro oup permeabillity) may prom mote changess in other variiables (e.g., leevel of ingroup ideentification, sttrategy preferrence). Thereffore, the variaables are not structured in such a way that presum mes a specific order in whicch the variablles influence oone another, bbut rather theey represent a belief system that conssists of severaal distinct dim mensions that mutually inflluence each oother. V (2002) also examined the impact of all three sociostrructural belieefs and Boen and Vanbeselaere how these affected a low status s group members’ m respponse to a neegative social identity. Based on SIT, they expectted an interaction between permeabilityy and stabilityy and betweenn permeabilityy and legitimacy for strategic management m choice. Howeever, social m mobility may nnot always bee an option or even dessired, and low w status memb bers may be eespecially unliikely to acceppt their inferioor status when statuss relations aree unstable and d illegitimate.. As such, theey argued thatt an interactioon should occur amon ng all three so ociostructural variables. To test their hypotheses,, Boen and Vaanbeselaere (22002) used a 2 (legitimacyy: legitimate vvs. illegitimatee) × 2 (stabilitty: stable vs. unstable) u × 2 (permeabilityy: permeable vs. impermeaable) × 2 (individual ability feedback: high vs. low) betweenn-subjects dessign. Participants were stuudents from primaary school classes (ages 11-12) in variouus primary schools in Flannders, Belgium m. Each class was raandomly assig gned to one leevel of each iindependent vvariable. Studdents participaated during theirr regular classs times and were w instructedd to work on their own. Thhey were giveen the impression that they werre competing with anotherr class to test a computer prrogram that w was being developed by b a universitty professor. Participants P w were told thatt the class witth the highest score on an empathy y test would be b evaluating and providingg feedback onn the program m, which was described as a pleasan nt task. All off the participaants were toldd that their claass performedd worse on thee empathy test comparred to the otheer class, and were w thereforre the lower sttatus group. Boen and Vanbeselaere V (2002) manip pulated the thrree sociostrucctural beliefs as well as peerformance ability. To manipulate m leegitimacy, parrticipants werre told either the other classs followed thhe same testing proccedures as theey did (legitim mate) or the oother class hadd an opportunnity to work collaborativ vely because the t test admin nistrators leftt the classroom m for a few m minutes (illegiitimate). To manipullate stability, participants were w told thatt there was a bbig differencee between thee scores from their class c and the other o class, an nd that it is v ery likely thaat the classes w would attain similar scores if theey were to do o the test again n the next dayy. The test addministrator laabelled the diifference as “unchang geable” and wrote w the worrd on the blacck board. In thhe unstable coondition, participants were told th hat there was a small differrence betweenn the scores oof the two claasses, and thatt if they were to do the test again n the following day, their class could scoore higher thaan the other cclass. The test adminisstrator labelleed the differen nce as “changgeable” and w wrote the wordd on the blackk board. For permeaability, low staatus students were told thaat the high staatus students w would be willling to accept students from theeir group, if th hey did well oon a retest of tthe empathy ttest. The expeerimenter wrote on th he board that the t other class was “open”” (i.e., permeaable) for thosee who scored high on the retest. In the imperm meable conditiion, low statuus students weere told that thhe high statuss class would not be b willing to accept a any members from their class regardless of thheir performannce on the Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 53 reetest. The exp perimenter wrrote on the board that the other o class waas “closed” foor those who sscored hiigh on the retest. The studeents in each class c were ran ndomly assignned to receivee high or low inndividual ability feedback. In the high ability a conditio on, the word “good” was w written on theeir test, w whereas in the low ability condition, the word “weak”” was written on their test. The commennts w were intended to communiccate to particip pants whetherr they were suuited or not suuited to judgee the prrogram respecctively. B Boen and Vanb beselaere (2002) presented d participants with five posssible responsses (identity m management sttrategies) to the t situation, including: i x x x x x Individ dual normativve action: Stu udents can ask k to do an inddividual retestt of the empatthy test (using a similar butt different testt) for an oppo ortunity to joinn the high staatus group. (T Then the perrmeability maanipulation was w presented to students.) Collecctive normativve action: Stu udents can req quest their enttire class to doo a retest of thhe empath hy test and try y to perform better b the oth her class, so thhat they can juudge the com mputer prograam. Individ dual nonnorm mative action: Students can n sign a prepaared personal pprotest letter stating that they should be included in th he high statuss group withoout doing anotther individuaal retest. This was presented as non nnormative by y telling studeents that univversity professsors would not be pleaseed with this op ption becausee it goes againnst the rules. However, stuudents have chosen c to do th his in the past and were su uccessful. Collecctive nonnorm mative action: Students can n sign a preparred collectivee protest letterr stating g that their claass should jud dge the compu uter program instead of thee other class. Similaar to individuaal nonnormatiive action, theey were told tthat this actioon was againsst the rules but b has been successful s in the t past. Accepttance: Studen nts can acceptt the result of the test and aacknowledge that they wouuld not be judg ging the comp puter program m. Paarticipants ressponded to fo our items as manipulation m checks c (all off these were eeffective). Theey also raated the exten nt to which theey experiencee relative grou up deprivationn (e.g., “I havve been unjusstly trreated in comp parison with other o pupils of o my class”).. Finally, ingrroup bias wass measured byy assking studentss to do a mon netary division n task by allo ocating funds to their own cclass and to thhe otther class as a reward for th heir cooperattion up to the current pointt in the study. Foor the response alternativee, both individ dual and colleective nonnorm mative actionn were combinned innto a single faactor of nonno ormative actio on. Boen and Vanbeselaeree’s (2002) annalysis of studdents’ reesponse options using lineaar modelling showed that there t was a siignificant maiin effect for aall four inndependent vaariables. Thosse in the perm meable conditiion were morre likely to chhoose individuual noormative actio on, accepted the t results, an nd were less likely l to chooose collective normative annd noonnormative action compaared to particiipants in the im mpermeable condition. Aggain, perceptiions of peermeability motivate m indiv viduals to swittch groups in an effort to aadvance their social status. Those inn the unstable condition weere more likelly to choose collective c norrmative actionn and less likeely to chhoose individ dual normativee action comp pared to thosee in the stablee condition. T There was no diifference betw ween stability y conditions fo or the other tw wo response aalternatives. P Participants inn the illlegitimate con ndition were somewhat mo ore likely to pick p collectiv e nonnormatiive action andd less likkely to accept the results compared c to th hose in the leegitimate conddition. And participants in the illlegitimate con ndition showeed more ingro oup bias than those in the llegitimate conndition. Theree were noo differences between the two t condition ns for the otheer two responnse alternativees. Finally, thhose in thhe high ability y condition were w more likeely to pick ind dividual norm mative action aand less likelyy to piick collectivee normative an nd nonnormattive action co ompared to paarticipants in tthe low abilitty Paage 54 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® condition. There T was alsso a main effeect of ability. High ability sstudents displlayed more inngroup bias than th hose with low w ability. No differences d weere observed for acceptancce. Using logiistic regression, Boen and Vaanbeselaere allso tested wheether feelingss of relative ggroup deprivattion mediated th he relation bettween legitim macy (based oon manipulatioon check item m) and collecttive nonnormatiive action and d the relation between legittimacy (basedd on manipulation check ittem) and ingroup biaas. The resultss showed thatt relative grouup deprivationn mediated booth relations. Boen and Vanbeselaere’ V s (2002) find dings show thaat the sociostrructural variaables do not innteract to predict low w status group members’ prreference for iidentity manaagement strategies. Insteadd, they seem to hav ve an additivee rather than an a interactivee effect. Table 8 sum mmarizes the findings of th hose studies th that examinedd together all three sociostrructural beliefs and the ensuing identity i manaagement strateegies. Table 8.. Summary of research h combining g all three s sociostructu ural dimens sions in relation to identity ma anagement s strategies. Reference Outccome Management strategy Kessler & Mum mmendey (2002) Ͳ Inggroup identificationn is more likely to predict the use off social mobility thaan is perceived peermeability. Ͳ Peermeability is negaatively associated with ingroup identtification, ressentment, realisticc competition, re-eevaluation of compparison dimensionns, and perceived stabillity and positively associated with thheir group’s materrial staatus within the Easst German populaation. Ͳ Peerceived stability of o the social structuure is positively reelated to ingroup ideentification and ressentment and neggatively associatedd with social moobility and superorrdinate recategorizzation. Ͳ Thhe perception of a legitimate social sstructure is negativvely associated witth resentment andd realistic competittion and positivelyy associated with maaterial group status. Ͳ Thhe relationship betw ween the sociostrructural dimensionns and managemeent strrategies are not linnear. Superordinaate recategorizaation (Individual m mobility) Ͳ Meembers of low stattus groups that peerceived the groupp boundaries to bee peermeable were moore likely to employy individual normaative action and accceptance and lesss likely to employ collective normati ve action and noonnormative actionn than those who pperceived the bouundaries to be impermeable. Ͳ Meembers of low stattus groups are moore likely to choosee collective noormative action andd less likely to chooose individual noormative action whhen status relationns are perceived too be unstable. Ͳ Meembers of low stattus group are morre likely to accept their status when thee status structure is legitimate. Ͳ Illeegitimate social strructures are moree likely than legitim mate social strructures to lead to collective non-noormative action andd ingroup bias andd theese effects are meediated by percepttions of relative grroup deprivation. Ͳ Soociostructural variaables have an addditive rather than innteractive effect on choice of managemeent strategy. Individual noormative action, indivvidual nonnormative action mobility) (Individual m Boen & Vanbesselaere (2002) 4.5 Temporal coomparison, Re-evaluatioon of comparison dimension (Social Creaativity) Social comppetition, realistic com mpetition (Social competition) Collective noormative action, colleective nonnormative action (Social competition)) Acceptance Su ummary Overall, thee research ind dicates that high status grouups find unstaable conditionns more threaatening and are morre likely to reespond to thesse conditions in a defensivve manner. Foor example, thhey are Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 55 likkely to increaase their opiniion of the ing group, while decreasing d theeir opinions oof the outgrouup. Iddentity manag gement strateg gies, such as defensive d hellping, help higgh status grouups reinforce the sttatus quo. On the other han nd, when low status groupss perceive thee status structuure as unstable, they viiew this situattion as an opp portunity to challenge the status s structur ure to further ttheir standingg. Low sttatus groups might m invoke ingroup bias for example to t mobilize thheir group in an effort to im mprove thheir social stattus. Low statu us groups perrceive stable conditions c as threatening, bbut still may select grroup mobilizaation as an ideentity manageement strateg gy since there is little to losse in trying too im mprove their social s standin ng. Sttatus structurees that are perrceived to be legitimate arre less threatenning to both hhigh and low status grroups. Low sttatus groups are a more acceepting of a staatus structure as legitimate when the higgh sttatus group co ompares moree favourably on o domains th hat are importtant to the staatus structure.. When sttatus structuree is illegitimate, however, low l status gro oups elect grooup mobilizattion as an identity m management sttrategy, favou uring their ow wn group. Hig gh status grouups are likely to distance thhemselves fro om their group p (e.g., by sho owing outgrou up favouritism m), if they feeel that the stattus sttructure is illeegitimate. W When status sttructures are believed b to bee permeable, high h status grroup memberss act in a defeensive m manner (e.g., showing s ingro oup bias, focu using on subg groups rather tthan superorddinate groups,, etc.). Low status gro oup members who believe the boundariees between grroups are perm meable will liikely leeave their grou up and join a higher group p, but only if they t weakly iidentify with ttheir group. H Highly iddentified mem mbers of low status s groups are likely to remain r loyal to their groupp regardless oof the exxpected utility y of joining a higher statuss group. Thosse who are nott highly identtified with theeir inngroup are lik kely to use ind dividual strateegies to impro ove their situaation under peermeable coonditions. Paage 56 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 5. How do o socio ostructtural be eliefs - stability, le egitima acy, an nd perm meabillity - in nfluenc ce in ntergro oup pe erceptio ons? ((4.7.4) This chapteer explores ho ow individuals’ perceptionns of the legitiimacy and staability of the sstatus structure an nd beliefs abo out the permeaability of grouup boundariees influence peerceptions off their own group and other o groups. It should be pointed p out thhat there weree not many arrticles specificcally examining the relationsh hips between the sociostrucctural beliefs and intergrouup perceptionns. Following a similar struccture as the previous chaptter, research iis organized aaccording to eeach sociostructu ural belief and d its influencee on intergrouup perceptionns. We begin w with researchh focusing on perceptions of stabilitty. 5.1 Stability and d Intergro oup Perce ptions There is som me evidence that t perceptio ons of stability ty shape interggroup perceptions. For exaample, Vezzali, An ndrighetto, Trrifiletti, and Visintin V (20122) examined tthe effect of inngroup identiification on explicit and implicit intergroup i atttitudes, and w whether this efffect is moderrated by perceptions of status stabillity. There is evidence thatt status stabiliity moderatess the link betw ween identificcation and ingroup biaas (Doosje, Sp pears, Ellemers, 2002). Speecifically, low w status mem mbers who higghly identify witth their ingrou up tend to perrceive both thhe ingroup annd outgroup ass homogenouus when social status is seen as am menable to ch hange. Vezzaali et al. exam mined perceptions of the rellation between tw wo nonconflicttual groups (IItalians and A Americans) froom the perspeective of mem mbers of the low stattus group, i.e.., Italians6. Th hey argued thhat current chaanges in the ppolitical and eeconomic situation may lead peoplle to think thaat the internattional status hhierarchy migght change, whhich may influence th heir attitudes toward other countries. Buut because theese two groupps are nonconnflictual, Vezzali et al. a thought thaat this may en nhance the sallience of equality social noorms, makingg it more difficult to express preju udiced attitudees. Instead, peeople in noncconflictual arrrangements, tthey argued, may y be more lik kely to engagee in ingroup eenhancement rrather than ouutgroup deroggation. Vezzali and d colleagues (2012) ( specifiically exploreed explicit andd implicit attiitudes. With rrespect to the former, they predicteed that stronger ingroup iddentification w would be assoociated with ggreater ingroup biaas, but this rellation would be b moderatedd by social staability. They ppredicted thatt ingroup bias would be stronger when w status was w perceived to be unstablle compared tto a control coondition in which the stability s of gro oup status is not n mentionedd. But this biaas was expectted to be drivven specifically y by ingroup enhancement e rather than ouutgroup deroggation. Similaarly, for impllicit attitudes, Vezzali V et al. expected e that the relation bbetween ingrooup identificaation and bias would be stronger wh hen status is unstable u comp pared to a conntrol condition. However, tthey hypothesized that this effect would w be driv ven by both in ngroup enhanccement and ooutgroup deroogation, as it w would be more difficult to control expressions of o prejudice iin implicit meeasures as passt research suuggests (Devine, Pllant, & Blair, 2001; Nosek k, 2005, 2007;; cited in Vezzzali et al.). 6 Past researcch has shown Itaalians as a lowerr status group coompared to Ameericans (see Aleexander, Brewerr, & Livingstone, 2005; 2 cited in Veezzali et al, 2012). Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 57 To test their hy ypotheses, Veezzali et al. (2 2012) asked undergraduate u e students at thhe Universityy of Paadova to participate in a sttudy about atttitudes related d to media com mmunicationn. They first coompleted meaasures of natio onal identificcation (Capozza, Brown, A Aharpour, & F Falvo, 2006; ccited in V Vezzali et al.) and social do ominance orieentation adaptted for Italianns (SDO; Aielllo, Chirumboolo, Leone, & Prattto, 2005; see Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; both b cited in V Vezzali et al.)). Vezzali et all.included SD DO because it has been shown to influen nce intergroupp relations (e..g., Pratto, Siddanius, & Levin, 2006 6; cited in Vezzzali et al.). N Next, Vezzali and a colleaguees (2012) rand domly assigned participantts to one of tw wo experimenntal coonditions. In the t status insttability condition, participaants read an aarticle (adapteed from The N New Y York Times an nd made to ap ppear in the most m popular Italian newspaaper) about thhe U.S. declinning in thhe economic domain, d but also a stated thaat several nations were on tthe rise in thee same domainn, esspecially Italy y. In the contrrol condition, participants read r an articlle unrelated too the relation beetween the U.S. and Italy and a discussed d how daily ph hysical activiity has health benefits. Theen paarticipants co ompleted man nipulation check items relatted to the stattus (in)stabilitty of Americaan and Ittalian econom my and their sttatus in generral as well as the t quality off the article (tto dismiss allternative exp planations duee to the text reead in the artiicle). Paarticipants theen completed d the Go/No-G Go Associatio on Task (GNA AT; Nosek & Banaji, 20011, as ciited in Vezzalli et al., 2012)), to assess im mplicit attitud des separately for two targeet groups. Theere w were four categ gories of stim muli used. Forr the ingroup and a outgroupp categories, 110 Italian (e.gg., M Marco, Anna) and 10 English (e.g., Keviin, Abbey) typ pical names w were used, booth categories m matched on typ picality and word w length. For F the attribu ute categoriess, 10 positive (e.g., peace, ennjoyment) and d 10 negativee (e.g., cancerr, prison) words were used,, both categorries matched on faamiliarity and d word length. The GNAT explored the relationship bbetween the sspeed of respoonses too each categorry (Italian or American) when w paired with either possitive or negattive valence w words ass an indicatorr of implicit atttitudes.7 Vezzzali et al. then had particippants complette explicit meeasures off ingroup and d outgroup evaaluations on five f semantic differential sscales (e.g., unndesirable/desirable) on a 7-point scale with 1 repressenting the neegative pole, 7 representingg the poositive pole, and a 4 represen nting “neitherr/nor”. The items were agggregated for ingroup and ouutgroup evalu uations separaately. Finally,, participants completed a ssocial desirabbility measuree addapted for thee Italian conteext (Manganeelli, Rattazzi, Canova, and Marcorin, 20000; cited in V Vezzali ett al.). V Vezzali et al.’ss (2012) prelim minary analyses on the maanipulation chheck items inddicated that thhe m manipulation of o status (in)sttability was effective. e In ad ddition, theree were no diffferences betw ween coonditions for perceptions of o status and the t quality off the article, suuch that both conditions raated Ittalians as lower in status th han American ns and had equ uivalent qualiity ratings. Exxplicit ingrouup bias w was calculated d by subtractin ng the outgrou up evaluation n from the inggroup evaluatiion. The ingrooup w was evaluated more positiveely than the outgroup o on th his measure. TThe GNAT used in this study had four critical bloocks, which weree randomly pressented to particippants. At the top of the sccreen in each bloock, two target-laabels were show wn in the upper leeft and right of thhe screen to rem mind participantss of the caategories they shhould be thinkingg about. In the experimental blocck, the four targeets were: Italian names + positivve woords, Italian nam mes + negative words, w American names + positivve words, and A American names + negative wordds. Paarticipants were presented with stimuli s in the midddle of the screeen and asked to press the spaceebar (Go) as quickly as a poossible if the stim muli belonged to the categories at a the top of the screen (signal), or to do nothingg (No-go) for stim muli that beelonged to other categories. 7 Paage 58 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® For implicit attitudes, a sensitivity s ind dex (d’) basedd on signal deetection theorry (Green & S Swets, 1966; cited in Vezzzali et al., 20 012) was com mputed for eacch experimenntal block, andd then the sennsitivity scores weree submitted to o a 2 (Target-concept: Italiian vs. Ameriican) × 2 (Attrribute: Positivve vs. Negative) repeated r meassures ANOVA A. A significaant interactionn emerged annd simple effeects tests indicated th hat Italians weere associated d with more ppositive than nnegative wordds. In contrasst, Americans were associiated with mo ore negative th han positive w words. Three indices were computed froom the sensitivity scores: s ingrou up evaluation n (difference inn d’s betweenn Italian-positive and Italiaan-negative blocks), outtgroup evaluaation (differen nce in d’s bettween Americcan-positive aand Americann-negative blocks), and d ingroup biaas (difference between ingrroup and outggroup evaluatiion indices). Hierarchicaal regression was w used to teest Vezzali ett al.’s (2012) main hypotheeses, with thee experimenttal conditions, identificatio on, and SDO aas predictors, with explicitt and implicit ingroup and outgrou up evaluation ns and ingroup p bias as the ddependent varriables, and soocial desirabiility as a covariate. For F explicit atttitudes, the reesults showedd that particippants with stroonger identifiication evaluated th heir ingroup more m positiveely and showeed more ingrooup bias. No oother significaant effects were observ ved. Thereforre, the data paartially suppoorted their firsst hypothesis, such that thee relation between ideentification an nd ingroup biias and was ddriven by ingrroup enhancem ment. Howevver, this effect was not n moderated d by status (in n)stability. Foor implicit atttitudes, the results showed that there was a significant interaction between identificationn and experim mental conditiion for evaluaation of the outgrou up. Tests of simple slopes (see ( Figure 9)) indicated thaat in the statuus instability ccondition, participantss’ outgroup ev valuation decreased with hhigher ingroupp identificatioon, whereas thhe effect was not reliiable in the co ontrol conditiion, providingg support for their second hhypothesis. Figure 9. 9 Interactio on between Identificatio on × Experiimental con ndition. Dep pendent variable: v im mplicit outgrroup evalua ation. (Vezzzali et al., 20 012, p. 37) 8 The finding gs for explicitt attitudes rev veal that whenn individuals hhighly identiffy with a grouup, and in this case, Ittalians identiffying with a lo ow status grouup, they tendded to show m more positive iingroup evaluationss and ingroup bias regardleess of the stabbility of the sttatus structuree. Low status group 8 High and low w scores of identtification are preesented at one sttandard deviatioon above and beelow the mean. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 59 m members, thereefore, may prromote ingrou up identificatiion through inngroup enhanncement ratheer ouutgroup derog gation. In con ntrast, for imp plicit attitudess, when the sttatus structuree was unstablee and w when participaants identified d more strongly with their ingroup, i theyy engaged in m more intergrooup coompetition by y derogating the t outgroup. To the extentt that stabilityy is usually coorrelated withh leegitimacy (seee Bettencourt et al., 2001), this finding is i consistent w with the preddictions of SIT T in w which low stattus groups aree more likely to engage in intergroup i coompetition whhen the status sttructure is unsstable, and thaat this would be especially y likely for thoose who stronngly identify with thheir ingroup. Taken T togetheer, the stabilitty of the statu us structure caan have differrential effectss on booth explicit an nd implicit in ntergroup percceptions. Table 9 provid des a summary y of the findin ngs pertaining g to perceptioons of sociostr tructural stabiility nt intergroup perceptions. p annd subsequen Table 9. Summary of o Intergrou up Perceptio ons in Resp ponse to So ociostructurral Stability y Reeference Outcome Veezzali, Andrighettoo, Trrifiletti, & Visintin (2012) 5 5.2 Ͳ Status stability s has no impact on explicit ingroup bias and peerceptions when people are highly iddentified with theirr ingroup. Ͳ Particippants who are strongly identified withh their group perceeive the ingroupp more positively and a show more inggroup bias on expllicit measures of attitudes. s is unstable, people who strongly identify with thheir ingroup Ͳ When status exhibit more implicit outgroup derogation compared c to thosee who do not stronglyy identify with theirr ingroup. Ͳ Status stability s has differeential effects on im mplicit and explicit measures of intergrooup attitudes. Intergroup Perceeptions Ingroup bias Outgroup derogaation Legittimacy an nd Intergro oup Perce eptions O Other research h looked at thee impact of so ociostructurall beliefs regarrding legitimaacy of status diifferences on intergroup peerceptions. Weber, W Mumm mendey, and W Waldzus (20022) hypothesizzed that peerceptions of legitimacy deepend on how w individuals perceive the rrelative protootypicality of the inngroup for thee inclusive cattegory. The prototype p of th he inclusive ccategory provvides a standaard aggainst which to t compare su ubgroups. As such, subgro oups that are ccharacteristic of the inclusiive caategory shoulld be evaluateed positively, whereas subg groups that arre less characteristic shouldd be evvaluated negaatively. Addittionally, differrences in prottotypicality shhould substanntiate differennt enntitlements (W Wenzel, in preess; cited in Weber W et al., 2002). 2 Acrosss three studiess, Weber et all. test w whether relativ ve prototypicaality influencees perceptions of legitimaccy and whether the valencee of the prrototype mod derates the efffect. Inn Weber et al..’s (2002) firsst study, participants were students from m universities in Germany.. Traditionally, a university education e is reegarded as mo ore prestigiouus than an appplied educatioon. H However, with h companies requiring r emp ployees with more m applied education and an interest iin prromoting and d expanding Polytechnic P scchools, status differences bbetween univeersities and poolytechnic sch hools are becoming more and a more unsstable. Weberr and colleaguues recruited B Business Administration (B B.A.) students from the Uniiversity of Jenna to take parrt in the studyy. Seenior studentss (those in thee 3rd to 5th yeaar of the prog gram) were re cruited to enssure that theyy would exxperience suffficient ingrou up identificatiion, and only those who inndicated in a ppre-test that thhey peerceived haviing high ingro oup status werre included in n the analysess. Paage 60 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Participantss completed a questionnairre that asked tthem the exteent to which thhey agreed w with the statement “independent “ of whether it is justified orr not, graduattes with a B.A A. degree from ma university generally g have higher presttige than gradduates with a B.A. degree from a polyteechnic school” to assess a their peerception of status s differennces. To assesss relative proototypicality, Weber et al. (2002) asked a particip pants to come up with four characteristiccs that were tyypical of B.A A. university students s relatiive to B.A. po olytechnic stuudents, and similarly, four characteristiccs that were typicaal of B.A. poly ytechnic relattive to B.A. uuniversity studdents. Then thhey rated how w typical the characteeristics were of o B.A. studeents in generaal.9 Next, percceived legitim macy was meaasured by asking partiicipants to ind dicate their ag greement on ffour statemennts (e.g., “I thhink it is justiffied that B.A. graduaates from the University have a higher pprestige than B.A. graduattes from the Polytechnicc School”). Th hen, participaants indicatedd the extent too which they w would feel guuilty about comparing themselves with w outgroup members on two items (e.g., “I feel guuilty when I coompare p with h that of the Polytechnic P ggraduates”) annd the degree of threat theyy would our career prospects experience if polytechnic students beccame a true thhreat to their status on onee item (“I wouuld experience it as threatening if the Polytechnic studdents would bbecome true competitors foor us”). Next, particcipants indicaated their interrgroup attituddes for four ddifferent conceepts each meaasured with three items: i x x x x sym mpathy (e.g., “I like B.A. students s from m the Polytechhnic School”);; reaadiness to con ntact (e.g., “I think t it is impportant to havve contact witth B.A. studennts from the Polytechnic School”); selff-observed beehaviour (e.g., “If I talk to somebody wh who has sentim ments against the Pollytechnic Sch hool I usually defend the Poolytechnic stuudents”); andd toleerance (e.g., “In “ their diffeerences B.A. sstudents from m the Universiity and from tthe Pollytechnic Sch hool complem ment each otheer very well”)). Finally, parrticipants indiicated their leevel of identiffication with ttheir ingroup on three item ms (e.g., “I identify witth B.A. studen nts that study y at the Univeersity”) and w with their incluusive group oon two items (e.g., “I identify with w B.A. stud dents in generaal”). Results from m Weber et al. a (2002) show wed that partiicipants perceeived their inggroup status tto be more prestigious than B.A. stu udents from a polytechnic school and iddentified withh both B.A. unniversity students and B.A. studen nts in general. Consistent w with the reseaarchers’ hypottheses, protottypicality was positiv vely associated d with perceiv ved legitimaccy of being inn a high statuss position, inddicating that the more participantts thought thaat their ingrouup was the typpical B.A. stuudent relative to B.A. polytechnicc students, thee more they believed b that ttheir higher sttatus was legiitimate. The rresearchers also found that t perceptio ons of legitim macy was negaatively associiated with guiilt and attitudees toward the outgrou up, and positiv vely associateed with feelinngs of threat. C Consequentlyy, when particcipants perceived th heir higher sttatus to be mo ore legitimate, the less guillty they felt abbout the sociaal comparison n, the more neegative attitud des they expreessed toward the outgroupp, and the lesss threatened they t felt by th he outgroup. Weber et all. (2002) cond ducted a path analysis and found that reelative prototyypicality posittively predicted leegitimacy, and d in turn, legiitimacy was nnegatively rellated to interggroup attitudes, consistent with w the correelational findiings. Similarlyy, relative proototypicality also indirectly Prototypicality was computeed by summing thhe scores for thee ingroup attribuutes, summing thhe scores of the outgroup attributions, and then subtractting the second sum from the firrst sum. 9 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 61 prredicted guilt and threat viia legitimacy. Finally, neith her prototypiccality, guilt, nnor threat, dirrectly prredicted intergroup attitudees (see Figuree 10). Figure 10. A path-analytic model, with proto otypicality a as exogenous variable and legitimacy, threat, guilt, and interrgroup attitu udes as end dogenous v variables. (F From Webe er, Mummen ndey, and Waldzus, W 200 02, p. 456) Evidence from m Weber et al.’s (2002) first study suggeested that percceptions of reelative prrototypicality y are associateed with percep ptions of legitimacy. Furthhermore, percceptions of leegitimacy are associated with different outcomes, o succh as intergrooup attitudes aand emotions. H However, they y argued that the t relation beetween protottypicality andd legitimacy sshould only be obbserved when n the inclusivee category (i.ee., B.A. studeents in generaal) is evaluateed positively. This w was expected because b being g similar to a comparison standard s shouuld only be poositive if that coomparison staandard is judg ged positively y. Therefore, individuals i caan only justify fy a high statuus poosition if the group is view wed as prototy ypical of a positive inclusivve category aand that categoory is im mportant to th heir identity. To T examine th his, Weber et al. manipulatted the valencce of the incluusive caategory in Stu udy 2. The intergroup p context for their t second study s involved d Germans ass the ingroup,, Poles as the ouutgroup, and Europeans ass the inclusivee category. Po oles were seleected as the ouutgroup becauuse thhey are Germaany’s direct neighbours, n making m them a salient and rrelevant outgrroup. As welll, reesearch has co onsistently sh hown that Gerrmans perceiv ve Germany too hold a highher status posiition thhan Poland wiithin Europe (no ( references provided by y Weber et al.., 2002). Furtthermore, at thhe time thhe study was conducted, c Po oland was parrt of Europe but b not the Euuropean Unionn. It remainedd a poossibility thatt Poland woulld become a member m of thee European U Union in the fu future. Particippants w were from a German univerrsity and valen nce of the incclusive categoory was manippulated by asking paarticipants to think about either e positivee or negative characteristic c cs of Europe. Then, relativee prrototypicality y, status, legitiimacy, intergroup attitudess, and ingroupp identificatioon were assesssed ussing similar measures m as in n Study 1, and d the intergroup context w was changed too include Gerrmans annd Poles. W Weber et al.’s (2002) prelim minary analyses showed thaat participantts perceived G Germans to occcupy a hiigher status position than Poles, P and theey perceived Germans G to b e more protottypical of Eurrope thhan Poles. To test their maiin prediction, multiple regrression analyyses were condducted with peerceived relattive prototypiicality, the vallence of the in nclusive categgory, and theeir interaction as prredictors, and d perceived legitimacy as th he criterion. The T analyses revealed no ssignificant maain efffects, but a siignificant inteeraction betw ween the prediictors indicatiing that the reelation betweeen prrototypicality y and legitimaacy is dependeent on the vallence of the innclusive categgory. Simple reegressions forr the positive and negative inclusive categories show wed that when participants thhought about the t positive aspects a of the inclusive cateegory, perceivved relative pprototypicalityy was asssociated with h an increase in perceived legitimacy off status differeences (marginnally significant), Paage 62 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® whereas wh hen participan nts thought ab bout the negattive aspects oof the inclusivve category, pperceived relative pro ototypicality was w associated d with a decreease in perceiived legitimacy (not signifficant). Finally, leg gitimacy was negatively n co orrelated with intergroup atttitudes, suchh that more peerceived legitimacy of status diffeerences was associated a witth less positivve intergroup attitudes. he results from m the second study, Weberr et al. (2002)) concluded thhat high statuus Based on th members ju ustify their su uperior status when w they peerceive their oown group to be prototypiccal of a positive incclusive catego ory. However, when the in clusive category is evaluatted negativelyy, high status mem mbers are less likely to justiify their superrior status. Thhese findings highlight thee importance of the valencce of the inclu usive categoryy when makinng judgmentss about the leggitimacy of status rellations between subgroupss. In Study 3, Weber et al. (2002) investtigated whethher individualls’ ingroup staatus position moderated the relation n between relaative prototyp picality and peerceived legittimacy. They also manipullated prototypicaality so that th he ingroup is either e high orr low in protootypicality of tthe inclusive category compared to the outgrou up. Weber et al. a hypothesizzed that, for hhigh status grooups, when thheir ingroup is more m prototyp pical of the in nclusive categgory than the outgroup, theey would percceive status differrences as legiitimate, whereeas when the outgroup is m more prototyppical, they woould perceive staatus differencces as illegitim mate. For low w status groups, the oppositte pattern wass predicted. When W the ingrroup is more prototypical oof the inclusivve category thhan the outgrroup, status differrences should d be perceived d as illegitimaate, whereas w when the outggroup is moree prototypicaal of the inclusive category y, status differrences shouldd be perceivedd as legitimatee. Weber et al. prediccted that this would w also im mpact intergrooup perceptioons. For high status groups, illegitimatee status relatio ons would be associated wiith guilt and ppositive interggroup attitudees, whereas forr low status groups, g illegitiimate status rrelations woulld be associatted with angeer and less positive inttergroup attitu udes. Again, stud dents from a German G polyttechnic schoo l participatedd in Weber et al.’s (2002) tthird study. They were assigned to arrtificial group ps based on thheir performaance on tasks that assessedd their perceptual style, s which categorized c th hem into one of two styles: “verbalizerss” or “visualizzers”. All participantss received falsse feedback in ndicating thatt they were “vvisualizers” aand told that tthe two types of perrceptual stylees structured visual v informaation differenntly. Participaants then com mpleted a second visu ual test and were w told that there t are two perceptual sttyles (“figure--based” or “ggroundbased”) and d each subgro oup was charaacterized diffeerently based on five charaacteristics.10 Participantss selected the set of characcteristics that tthey thought best describeed the group tto which they belong ged. The expeeriment was programmed p tto provide parrticipants withh feedback thhat confirmed their t selection n. This self-asssignment proocedure was uused to ensuree ingroup ideentification and provideed a set of traaits to use for the prototypi cality manipuulation. Status was manipulated by telling parrticipants thatt career prosppects were relaated to percepptual style and that eith her their own n group or thee outgroup hadd a better chaance of obtainning good empployment. Participantss were also to old that this finding was baased on scienttific surveys aand that it waas unclear why there is a link betweeen perceptuaal style and caareer prospeccts, but they only know thaat the two groups diffe fered in terms of their perso onality traits tthat were presented earlierr in the study.. Weber et al. (2002) manipulated m prototypicality p y by presentinng participantts with graphss of the ingrooup, outgroup, and a inclusive category, com mparing how each group sccored on eachh of the 10 atttributes There weree a total of ten atttributes. Each paarticipant was prresented with tw wo sets of five atttributes, and thee sets of attributes werre randomly seleected by the com mputer. In additioon, the attributess were pretested and are equivallent on valence. 10 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 63 annd to judge th he status diffeerence. In the ingroup proto otypicality coondition, 8 ouut of the 10 grraphs shhowed that th he inclusive caategory was more m similar to t the ingroupp, whereas in the outgroup prrototypicality y condition, 8 out of the 10 graphs show wed that the innclusive categgory was moree siimilar to the outgroup. o Thee remaining tw wo graphs sho owed that thee ingroup and outgroup weere eqquivalent on the t two attribu utes. Then, paarticipants weere presented with a manippulation checkk of thhe prototypicaality manipulaation asking them t to indicaate which subbgroup was m more prototypiical for viisualizers in general. g W Weber and colleagues (2002 2) then had paarticipants an nswer four item ms that assessed legitimaccy of sttatus differencces. The word ding was chan nged to reflecct their self-asssigned ingrouup and the staatus of thheir ingroup. Group-based G emotions werre assessed by y asking partiicipants to annswer three iteems m measuring guillt and three items measurin ng anger, whiich were comb mbined into a ssingle factor. Inntergroup attittudes were asssessed with three t items measuring feeliings of sympaathy, readinesss to coontact, and co ooperation. Th he first two ittems were reliiable and com mbined into a new factor. Iddentification with w the ingro oup was meassured with thrree items. Idenntification wiith the inclusiive caategory was measured m usin ng similar item ms as in the in ngroup identiification meassure. Finally, peerceptions of status was asssessed consissting of seven n vertical “step eps”, where, thhe highest steep reepresents the best b status po osition and thee lowest step represents th e worst statuss position. Paarticipants haad to assign eaach subgroup to one of thee steps, and thhe difference bbetween the suubgroups’ status position was w used as a manipulation n check. W Weber et al. (2 2002) reported d that the man nipulation forr status was efffective, but tthe prototypiccality m manipulation did d not have th he intended effects, e such th hat the manippulation only worked for thhe inngroup prototy ypicality cond dition (i.e., paarticipants perceived their own group ass more protottypical off the inclusivee category), but b not for thee outgroup pro ototypicality condition (paarticipants perrceived thhe ingroup and the outgrou up as equally prototypical). p . Because the prototypicaliity manipulattion w was not effectiive, analyses using u the man nipulation willl not be summ marized. Onlly the analysees using thhe prototypicaality manipulaation check measure m will be b described. Paarticipants’ perceived prottotypicality raatings were diichotomized uusing a mediaan split. The m main hyypotheses weere tested usin ng a 2-way AN NOVA with perceived p proototypicality rratings and the status m manipulation as a the indepen ndent variablees and legitim macy as the deependent variaable. The resuults shhowed that th here was a sign nificant main n effect of stattus and signifficant interacttion between status annd prototypicality. Furtherr analyses sho owed that high h status groupps perceived sstatus differennces to bee more legitim mate when theeir ingroup was w prototypiccal and less leegitimate wheen the outgrouup was prrototypical (m marginally sig gnificant). Forr the low statu us groups, thee findings weere reversed. T They peerceived statu us differencess to be more leegitimate wheen the outgro up was prototypical and peerceived statu us differencess to be less leg gitimate when n their own grroup was prottotypical (m marginally sig gnificant).11 The T correlations between leegitimacy andd the group-based emotionns and beetween legitim macy and inteergroup attitudes were not supported. N No significant correlations w were foound, which may m be due to o the artificiall nature of thee groups. Thuus, the findinggs from Webeer et all.’s third study y provide sup pport that the relation betw ween prototypiicality and leggitimacy diffe fers for hiigh and low status s groups. When the prototypicality manipuulation was usedd as the indepenndent variable, a significant interraction between prrototypicality andd status emergedd for only one off the four legitimaacy items. The i nteraction reveaaled that prototyppicality afffected legitimacy evaluations for high-status grooups, but not for low status groupps. Low-status ggroups perceivedd status diffferences as illeggitimate regardleess of perceivedd prototypicality of o the ingroup annd outgroup. 11 Paage 64 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Overall, Weber et al.’s (2002) studiess demonstratee the link betw ween legitimaacy of status sstructure and intergro oup perceptio ons. Specificaally, they foun und that, for m members of a hhigh status grroup, the perceived relative protottypicality of their t characterristics was poositively relatted to their peerceptions of the legitiimacy of the social s structure, and the m more legitimatee they believeed the social sstructure to be, the leess guilt they experienced about a the exissting structurre and the morre negative thheir intergroup attitudes a weree toward outg groups. This m may be becauuse perceptionns of a more legitimate social struccture also led members m of a high status ggroup to repoort feeling more threatenedd by lower status group ps potentially y encroaching g on this statuss. Moreover, Weber et al. found that staatus differences are perceived d to be legitim mate by high status groupss when their ggroup is typicaal of a positive incclusive catego ory, but illegittimate when tthe category iis negative. Major and colleagues c (2002) believed d that individuuals can varyy in how muchh they endorse legitimizing g ideologies that t justify ex xisting differe nces betweenn groups, and the extent to which these ideolo ogies are endo orsed can inflluence how thhey perceive iintergroup peerceptions andd interactionss. One legitim mizing ideolog gy is the belieef that one cann improve theeir social stannding by becoming part p of a higheer status grou up (i.e., indiviidual upward mobility). Beeliefs in indivvidual mobility sh hould have diffferent impliccations for how w high and loow status grouups respond tto ambiguous intergroup in nteractions, beecause their ggroup memberrship can inflluence how thhey interpret thee interaction. Major et al.. (2002) exam mined whetherr group statuss and endorseement of legittimizing ideollogy predicted whether w indiviiduals perceiv ved themselvees as targets oof discriminattion in an inteergroup interaction. They examin ned whether individuals i w would attributee rejection froom an outgrouup member to discriminatio on. Major et al. a argued thatt individuals w who belong too a low statuss group may be lesss likely to perrceive themseelves as targetts of discriminnation when tthey are rejeccted by someone from a high staatus group. In n fact, they maay perceive thhe response frrom the high status individual as a fair becausse they feel less deserving and inferior iin status. In coomparison, inndividuals who belong g to a high staatus group maay be more likkely to perceiive themselvees as targets oof discriminattion because they t feel moree deserving annd superior too low status m members, whiich can make the reejection seem m unfair. This set of predicttions is referreed to as the sttatus-legitimaacy hypothesis. d their predicctions in threee studies usingg different meethodologies (survey Major et al.. (2002) tested vs. experim ment) and grou ups (ethnic vss. gender grouups). In Studyy 1, they survveyed people ffrom high status (Euro opean Americcan) and low status (Africaan American or Latino Am merican) ethniic groups to examine the relation between b belieefs in individuual mobility aand perceptionns of personaal discriminattion (i.e., discrimination baased on their eethnic group)). Among highh status groupp members, th hey found thaat the more th hey endorsed the legitimiziing ideology, the more they reported being targets of personall discrimination. Among loow status group members, they found thhat the more they endorsed e the ideology, i the less they rep orted being taargets of disccrimination. T These findings aree consistent with w the statuss-legitimizingg hypothesis. M Major et al. aalso found thaat the more low status group g membeers identified with w their inggroup, the moore they perceeived personall discriminattion, but no asssociation waas found betw ween the two vvariables for hhigh status group members. d study, Majorr et al. (2002)) wanted to exxamine responses of high aand low statuus In a second members to o a situation in nvolving a rejjection by eitther an ingrouup or outgroupp member. Peerceptions of discrimin nation should d be more likeely when indivviduals are reejected by an outgroup rathher than an ingroup meember becausee status differrences are moore relevant annd status legiitimizing ideoologies would moree likely be acttivated. Thereefore, beliefs in legitimizinng ideologiess were expecteed to Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 65 m moderate attrib butions to disccrimination when w individu uals are rejectted by an outggroup membeer, but noot when they are rejected by b an ingroup p member. European Ameerican (high status) and Laatino Americaan (low status)) students weere invited to the laaboratory to participate p in a study aboutt work group development d and performaance. Prior too the seession, Majorr et al. (2002) asked them to t complete prre-testing meeasures whichh assessed their beeliefs in indiv vidual mobilitty and their leevel of identiffication with ttheir ingroup. In each exxperimental session, s studen nts believed that t they weree participatingg in the studyy with two othher sttudents and were w told they were seated in i the other cu ubicles. How wever, this wass a fabricationn. Paarticipants weere told that th he three of th hem would be working on a series of prooblem-solving tasks inn a structured work team co onsisting of a manager, co-manager, annd a clerk. Thee actual particcipant w was told that one o of the otheer participants was random mly assigned tto the role of the manager, who w would be respo onsible for assigning the ro oles of co-maanager and cleerk. Participannts were given deescriptions off the co-manaager and clerk k positions, wiith the formerr position desscribed as moore deesirable. The experimenterr took a digitaal picture of th he participantt and ostensibbly of the otheer two paarticipants. Paarticipants weere then asked d to fill out ap pplication maaterials that w would be givenn to the manaager. A Among the maaterials, they were w asked to o provide som me demographhic informatioon, which inclluded thheir ethnicity. While the paarticipants weere waiting for the managerr’s decision, tthey were preesented w with the picturres of the three people who o were in the session s on theeir computer screen. In thee ouutgroup rejecction condition, the manageer and the oth her applicant w were the sam me sex but diffferent inn ethnicity relative to the acctual participant. In the ing group rejectioon condition, the manager was thhe same sex and ethnicity as a the particip pant, whereas the other appplicant was thhe same sex bbut frrom a differen nt ethnic grou up than the acttual participant. All particiipants “accideentally” heardd a coonversation between b the manager m and th he experimen nter over the inntercom (in aactually, this w was sccripted and prre-recorded). The managerr expressed to o the experimeenter that s/hee did not feel that thhey would hav ve a good chaance of winnin ng the prize with w the actuaal participant, and then the exxperimenter informed the actual a particip pant that s/he has been ass igned to the cclerk positionn. Paarticipants theen completed d the primary dependent vaariable, an attrribution meassure, which asssessed thheir belief thaat the managerr’s decision was w discriminatory. They w were also askeed to rate the extent too which the manager’s m decision was bassed on their raace/ethnicity, manager’s peersonal preferrences, annd aspects of their written statement fro om their job ap pplication. To test the stattus-legitimacy y hypothesis, Major et al. (2002) ( examinned whether sstatus, rejectiion coontext, and beeliefs in indiv vidual mobilitty influenced attributions too discriminattion (controlliing for paarticipants lev vel of ingroup p identificatio on). Regressio on analyses shhowed that thhere were no siignificant maiin effects or 2-way 2 interacttions among the t predictorss, but there w was a significaant thhree-way interraction. Follo ow-up analysees showed thaat there was a different patttern of resultss for thhe two rejectio on conditionss (see Figure 11). Paage 66 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Figure 11. Attributio ons to discrimination a among high h status (Eu uropean Am merican) and low status s (Latin no America an) group m members as a function of rejection n context and belief in individual mobility m (Stu udy 2). (Ma ajor et al., 20 002, p. 275)) 12 In the outgrroup rejection n condition, greater g endorssement of a leegitimizing iddeology was aassociated with more attributions a to o discriminatiion among Euuropean Amerricans, but lesss attributionss among Latino Ameerican studentts. In the ingrroup rejectionn condition, sttatus but not iideology influuenced attributionss to discriminaation. Latino American stuudents were m more likely too attribute the rejection by an ingro oup manager to t discriminattion than Euroopean Americcans, in whichh a high statuus versus a low status applicant a wass favoured resspectively. A third stud dy conducted by Major and d colleagues ((2002) extendded these finddings to statuss differences based on gen nder rather thaan ethnicity aand to a conteext that involvves discriminaation in one’s favou ur. Past researrch has shown n that beliefs in legitimizinng ideology innfluence peopple’s sense of entitlemeent (Major, 19 994; cited in Major M et al). People who bbelong to a loow status grouup may feel that theey deserve thee inferior position, where tthose who bellong to a highh status groupp may feel that they arre entitled as a consequence of their supperior positionn. Then, for hhigh status meembers, their belief in legitimizin ng ideologies may preventt them from feeeling unfairlly advantagedd because of the group p to which they belong and d its ensuing entitlements. Using a sim milar methodo ology as in Stu udy 2, Major et al. (2002) randomly asssigned men (hhigh status) and women (low status) into one o of two connditions. Onee condition waas the outgrouup 12 + p < .10 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 67 reejection condiition which iss similar to th he one in Stud dy 2, and the ssecond condittion was an inngroup seelection condiition in which h a same-sex manager (ing group) picks thhe participantt to be in the com manager role rather r than an opposite-sex x applicant (ou utgroup). Sim milar to Studyy 2, Major andd coolleagues pred dicted that in the outgroup p rejection con ndition, the m more men endorsed legitim mizing iddeology, the more m likely th hey would perrceive rejectio on by an outggroup memberr as based on diiscrimination. For women,, on the other hand, the mo ore they endorrsed the legitiimizing ideollogy, thhe less likely they t would peerceive the reejection as due to discriminnation. In the ingroup selecction coondition, the more m men bellieved in the ideology, i the less likely thhey would attrribute an ingrroup m member’s deciision to discriimination, wh hereas women n were not exppected to show this patternn. A Again, Major et e al. (2002) tested t the statu us-legitimacy y hypothesis uusing regressiion with gendder, beelief in indiviidual mobility y, and contextt as predictorss and attributiions to discrim mination as thhe crriterion (contrrolling for lev vel of ingroup p identificatio on). The resultts showed thaat there was aan efffect for conteext, such that participants were w more lik kely to perceiive discriminaation for outggroup reejection comp pared to ingro oup selection. The three preedictors also iinteracted andd revealed a diifferent patterrn of results for f the two context conditio ons. In the ouutgroup rejecttion conditionn, both sttatus and ideo ology influencced attribution ns. Consisten nt with Major et al.’s predicctions, the moore m men believed in i individual mobility m (i.e.,, the legitimizzing ideologyy), the more liikely they perrceived thheir rejection by a woman as a discriminaation. Women n showed the oopposite patteern, such thatt the m more they believed in the leegitimizing id deology, the leess likely theyy perceived thheir rejection by a m man as discrim mination. In th he ingroup sellection condittion, the moree men endorsed individuall m mobility, the more m they den nied that discrrimination waas the reason w why a male m manager seleccted thhem over a wo oman for the co-manager position, p and they also percceived the maanager as morre quualified and more m competeent. In contrasst, for women n, endorsemennt of the ideollogy was not asssociated with h perceptions of discriminaation or evalu uations of the manager. Taken togetherr, the findingss from Major et al.’s (2002 2) studies suppport their stattus-legitimacyy hyypothesis. Th heir results sho ow the extentt to which ind dividuals endoorse status leggitimizing iddeologies is asssociated with h less perceiv ved discriminaation among m members from m low status ggroups, buut is associateed with more perceived disscrimination among a those from high staatus groups, aafter beeing rejected from an outg group memberr. What is parrticularly impportant is that there were noo efffects of ideollogy when ind dividuals werre rejected by y an ingroup m member. Thiss means that leegitimizing id deologies are important i onlly in specific and relevant contexts and have differenntial efffects on percceptions of disscrimination, depending on n the status off the individuual who endorrses thhose ideologiees. Finally, en ndorsed legitimacy appearss to prevent m members from m high status ggroups frrom recogniziing that they have h been unffairly advantaaged because of their grouup membership in inntergroup relaations. Table 10 summ marizes those studies invesstigating the im mpact of legiitimacy on inttergroup peerceptions. Paage 68 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Table 10. Summa ary of Interg group Perce eptions in R Response to o Sociostruc ctural Legitim macy Reference Outco ome Intergroup P Perceptions Weber, Mummeendey, & Waldzus (2002) Ͳ Forr members of a higgh status group, thhe perceived relattive prototypicalityy of their t characteristiccs on an inclusive category is positivvely related to theeir perrceptions of the legitimacy of the soocial structure and the more leggitimate they feel thhe social structuree is, the more negative their inteergroup attitudes, and the less guilt they experience aabout the structuree. Ͳ Perceptions of a morre legitimate sociaal structure also leead members of a higgh status group to feel more threatenned by lower statuus groups pottentially encroachiing on this status. Ͳ Staatus differences arre perceived to bee legitimate by highh status groups onlly when their group is typical of a poositive inclusive caategory, but illegitimate when thee category is negattive. p a more leegitimate structuree when the ingroupp Ͳ Higgh status groups perceive is more m typical of an inclusive categoryy than the outgrouup whereas low staatus group perceive a more legitimatte structure when the outgroup is moore typical of an incclusive category thhan the ingroup. Prototypicality, guilt, threat, interggroup attitudes Major, Gramzow w, McCoy, Levin, Schmader, & Sidanius (2002) Ͳ Thee more members of a high status grroup endorse indivvidual mobility (leggitimizing ideologyy), the more they pperceive that theyy are targets of discrimination. Ͳ Thee more members of a low status grooup endorse indivvidual mobility, the less they perceive thhat they are targetss or discriminationn. Ͳ Am mong members of the low status grooup, personal disc rimination is perrceived to a greateer extent among thhose who are highhly identified with thee ingroup. Ͳ Whhen high status grooup members are rejected by an ouutgroup member, bellief in a legitimizingg ideology is assoociated with more attributions of discrimination whereeas it is associatedd with fewer attribuutions of discrimination amongg low status groupp members. Ͳ Whhen members of a high status groupp are rejected by aan ingroup meember, belief in a legitimizing ideologgy is negatively asssociated with perrceptions of discrim mination. Ͳ A member m of a low status s group who i s rejected by an inngroup member iss moore likely to perceivve this as an act oof discrimination thhan members of a higgh status group whho are rejected by an outgroup mem mber. Ͳ Leggitimizing ideologies do not accountt for perceived disscrimination when donne by an ingroup member m among loow status groups. Discriminatioon 5.3 Pe ermeability y and Inte ergroup Perception ns A search off the literaturee revealed thaat there is lim mited research that focussess only on perm meability and intergro oup perceptio ons. One study y conducted bby Leong (20008) did include perceived permeabilitty as one of th he variables of o interest inveestigating attiitudes towardds immigrantss. This investigatio on used secon ndary data, an nd as such the methodologyy for the data collection waas not reported. Results R from a multiple hierrarchical regrression showeed that permeaability did noot significantlly predict attittudes toward immigrants, rrather for Leoong’s study perceived threat and intergroup contract c weree found to preedict the interggroup attitudees. It is possibble that there is a relationship p between perrmeability and d attitudes tow wards immigrrants that is m mediated by pperceived threat and intergroup con ntact. Other research (e.g. Verkuyten, 22005; Verkuytten & Reijersse, 2008; Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 69 Joohnson, Terry y, & Louis, 20 005) examinin ng the relation nship betweeen sociostructuural beliefs annd inntergroup percceptions is discussed in thee following seection, and thhese efforts innclude permeaability. 5 5.4 Othe er Researc ch examin ning Socio ostructura al Beliefs and Interrgroup Perceptions O Other research h centred on more m than onee sociostructurral belief andd the impacts oon intergroupp peerceptions and are, therefo ore, included in i this section n. One study bby Verkuytenn (2005) exam mined thhe implication ns faced by ad dopting an ideeology of mullticulturalism m versus an ideeology of collourbllindness (as exhibited e by assimilation). a Though theree are proponeents of multicculturalism annd its poositive impacct on intergrou up relations, there t are thosee who argue iit indulges inddividual cultuural diifferentiation,, thus exacerb bating culturaal conflict. Acccording to Veerkuyten, the impact of m multiculturalism differs acro oss minority and a majority groups. Whille multiculturralism allows m minority group ps to maintain n and affirm th heir ethnic id dentity, it mighht prove threaatening to thee m majority culturre for whom assimilation a provides p justiffication. V Verkuyten (2005) conducted four studiess investigating g the effects oof multiculturralism and asssimilation on n group identiity and ingrou up and outgro oup evaluationn among minority (Turkishh) and m majority (Dutcch) groups witthin the Netherlands was examined. e Thhe series of stuudies was aim med at asssessing both the multicultturalism hypo othesis and claaims made byy SIT. He prooposed that addopting a multiculturall ideology willl lead to a mo ore accepting attitude towaards other culltural groups. On the otther hand, SIT T argues that interethnic id deologies affeect group interractions diffeerently dependding on w whether one is a member off the minority y or majority group. g Speciffically, SIT prroposed that enndorsing a mu ulticultural id deology would d lead to increeased ethnic iidentificationn and a more ppositive evvaluation of th he ingroup fo or members off the minority y Turkish grouup. On the otther hand, enddorsing a multiculturall ideology wo ould lead to weaker w ethnic identification i n and more poositive outgroup evvaluations am mong the majo ority Dutch grroup. Embraccing a particullar ideology iis essentially an addmission of itts legitimacy. He also conssidered how perceptions p off stability andd permeabilityy w would impact intergroup i peerceptions. Inn the last two of his four sttudies Verkuy yten (2005) ussed an experim mental surveyy method in w which Study paarticipants weere primed wiith either a mu ulticultural id deology or ann assimilationiist ideology. S 3 also included d a neutral con ntrol conditio on that primed d leisure timee and environm mental issuess. Fuurthermore, Study S 4 also in ncluded meassures of perceeived permeabbility of the ggroups and peerceived stabiility of the eth hnic relationss. Inn both of these studies, Verrkuyten (2005 5) found that within the muulticulturalism m condition, Turkish particiipants were more m likely to endorse multticulturalism tthan were Duutch participannts. A Also, for Turkiish participan nts, ethnic gro oup identificattion was highher in the mullticultural conndition thhan the other two t condition ns combined in i Study 3, an nd higher thann the assimilaation conditioon in Sttudy 4. Theree was also a positive associiation betweeen multiculturralism and ethhnic group iddentification for f the Turkissh participantss, in both stud dies. Converssely, within thhe assimilation coondition, Dutch participantts were signifficantly more likely than T Turkish participants to endoorse asssimilation. Furthermore, F in i both studiees, ethnic grou up identificatiion was higheer for Dutch paarticipants in the assimilatiion condition n than in the multicultural m ccondition. Thhere was also a poositive associiation between n assimilation n and ethnic group g identifiication for Duutch participannts in booth studies. On O the other hand, h there waas a negative association a beetween assim milation and etthnic grroup identificcation for Turrkish participaants, howeverr, this associaation was onlyy significant iin Sttudy 3. Paage 70 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® With respecct to the socio ostructural vaariables, Verku kuyten (2005) found that D Dutch participaants felt that the ethn nic groups weere more perm meable than T Turkish particcipants, and thhat the intergrroup relations weere more stab ble. Across co onditions, therre was a signiificant stabilitty × ethnic grroup interaction such that, am mong the Dutcch participantss, there was a positive assoociation betw ween stability and d group identtification. On the other hannd, there was no significannt relationshipp between stability and d ethnic grou up identificatio on among thee Turkish partticipants. Thuus, when the sstability of the intergro oup relations were w perceiveed to be stablee, Dutch partiicipants, but nnot Turkish participantss, were much more likely to t closely idenntify with theeir group. Theere was also a significant permeability × ethnic grou up interactionn such that theere was a negative associattion between peermeability an nd ethnic grou up identificatiion among thee Turkish parrticipants, butt no relationship p within the Dutch D particip pants. In otherr words, whenn membershipp was perceivved to be permeable, Turkish partiicipants were much less likkely to closelyy associate thhemselves witth their ot true for Duttch participannts. group. The same was no meability and stability weree not significaantly related tto each other,, permeabilityy was While perm significantlly negatively related r to mu ulticulturalism m in the multiccultural condiition and posiitively related to assimilation in n the assimilattion conditionn (Verkuytenn, 2005). Therrefore, when eethnic boundaries were seen as more flexible, there was a decreased suupport for muulticulturalism m, but increased su upport for asssimilation. Peerceived stabiility was onlyy marginally ppositively relaated to assimilation n and not relaated to multicu ulturalism. Thhese results w were similar aacross both Dutch and Turkish parrticipants. Turning to the ingroup and a outgroup evaluations, M MANOVAs w were conductted. In both Sttudies 3 and 4, as in n Studies 1 an nd 2, there was a positive a ssociation between ethnic group identiffication and ingroup p evaluations. There was no n associationn between grooup identificattion and outggroup evaluationss in either stud dy. In Study 4, 4 there was aalso a negativve association between percceived stability and d ingroup evaaluations. All of these finddings were sim milar for bothh Turkish and Dutch participantss. There was a significant multivariate m ethnic e group × experimental condition iinteraction in both Studies 3 an nd 4. Dutch participants p teended to havee a somewhat more positivee evaluation oof the outgroup an nd somewhat less positive evaluation off the ingroup in the multicuulturalism conditions compared to the assimilaation conditio ons in both stuudies. Converrsely, the Turrkish participaants tended to have a somewh hat more posiitive evaluatioon of the ingrroup and less positive evalluation of the outgrou up in the multiculturalism condition. c Thhese findings w were also mirrrored in the multivariatee multiculturaalism × ethnicc group interaaction. Amonng Dutch partiicipants in Stuudy 3, there was a positive asso ociation betweeen their endoorsement of m multiculturalism and their outgroup evaluationss. In Study 4, this finding was w extended to ingroup evvaluations succh that a stronnger endorsemen nt of multicullturalism amo ong the Dutchh participants was associateed with a morre negative ing group evaluattion. Howeveer, the findinggs for Turkishh participants were not signnificant for either ingro oup or outgrou up evaluation ns in either stuudy. Taken toogether, it apppears that enddorsing multiculturalism in itselff may promotte more positiive perceptionns toward a loow status outggroup. d a significantt interaction bbetween the enndorsement oof assimilationn and Verkuyten (2005) found up for both ing group and outtgroup evaluaations. Howevver, this was oonly true in S Study 4for ethnic grou both ingrou up and outgroup evaluation ns. For Dutchh participants, a stronger enndorsement off assimilation n was positiveely related to ingroup evalluations, and nnegatively rellated to outgrroup evaluationss. There was no n association n between thee endorsemennt of assimilattion and eitherr ingroup or outgroup p evaluations among Turkiish participannts. However, among Turkiish participannts, there was a positive associatio on between th he belief in thee permeability ty of the groupps and outgrooup Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 71 evvaluations. Th he more perm meable the Turrkish participants found thhe groups, thee more positivvely thhey felt about the Dutch. W Within Study 4, 4 Verkuyten (2005) also examined e ingrroup and outggroup evaluattions of traits tyypically assocciated with eitther the Dutch h (competencce) or the Turkks (morality).. Dutch particcipants w were more likeely to be seen as competen nt, both by theemselves (ingrroup evaluatiions) and by tthe Turkish particiipants (outgro oup evaluation ns) and Turkss were more llikely to be seeen as highly moral booth by themseelves (ingroup p evaluations) and by the Dutch D particippants (outgrouup evaluationns). These findingss were not asssociated with either multicu ulturalism or assimilation. Perceptions of peermeability did interact wiith ethnic grou up for both morality m ratinggs and competence ratings.. For Turkish particiipants, there was w a significcant positive association a beetween permeeability and I other wordss, the more peermeable the Turkish partiicipants believved the ouutgroup moraality ratings. In grroups to be, th he more moraal they found the Dutch gro oup. On the oother hand, foor Dutch paarticipants, th here was a sig gnificant negaative associatiion between ppermeability aand outgroup coompetence ratings. Thus, the t more perm meable the Du utch participannts believed tthe groups to be, the leess competentt they felt Turrkish people were. w Stability y had no effeccts on either ccompetence oor m morality rating gs. V Verkuyten and d Reijerse (2008) also exam mined the com mbined effectss of the three sociostructurral vaariables on grroup identification, stereoty ypes, and gro oup feelings aamong high annd low status group m members. In paarticular, they y compared reesponses of Dutch D people ((high status ggroup) and TuurkishD Dutch people 13 (low status group) in the Netherlands.. Measures asssessed particiipants’ percepptions off all three socciostructural variables, v ingrroup identificcation, and ouutgroup identiification amonng low sttatus members (i.e., Turkissh-Dutch indiv viduals’ identtification withh Dutch). To assess group sttereotypes, the attributions of Dutch and d Turkish-Duttch participannts were also used as meassures. There were 10 positive attriibutes in total: five were reelevant to the status distincction, but morre chharacteristic of o the Dutch culture c (e.g., efficient, e achiievement orieented), and fivve were irreleevant to thhe distinction,, but more chaaracteristic off the Turkish culture (e.g., hospitable, trradition mindded) (ssee Verkuyten n, 2005). Finaally, the feelin ngs of both grroups of partiicipants towarrd several inggroups annd outgroups (Dutch, Turk ks, Surinamesse, Moroccanss, and Antilleeans) were alsso captured. A According to Verkuyten V and d Reijerse (20 008), results for f beliefs aboout the status structure shoowed thhat members of o the low staatus group perrceived the strructure as les s legitimate, m more stable, aand as haaving less perrmeable group p boundaries than did mem mbers of the hhigh status grooup. In additiion, a leegitimate interrgroup structu ure had differrent meaningss for the two ggroups. For loow status mem mbers, a legitimate strructure was asssociated with h less stability y and more p ermeability, w whereas for hhigh sttatus members, a legitimatee structure waas associated with more staability and less permeabiliity. B Because high and a low statuss groups percceive the statu us structure diifferently, Veerkuyten and R Reijerse (2008) analysed thee two groups separately fo or the other deependent meaasures. Resultts shhowed that members m of thee low status group g had high her ingroup iddentification than memberrs of thhe high status group. Also for the low sttatus group, th heir level of T Turkish identiification was neegatively relaated to their leevel of Dutch identification n. For the low w status groupp, regression annalyses indicaated that the sociostructura s al variables prredicted their identificationn with Turks. All of thhe main effectts and two-waay interaction ns were signifficant, and weere qualified bby a three-waay innteraction. Th his interaction n showed that when the stattus structure w was perceivedd as stable annd These are actuaally Turkish participants, but beccause ethnic minnorities tend to hhave dual identitiies – they identiffy with booth their ingroup and the nationaal category – they are referred too as Turkish-Duttch (e.g., Verkuyyten, 2005; cited in Veerkuyten & Reijeerse, 2008). 13 Paage 72 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® relatively leegitimate, thee permeability y of group bouundaries was negatively reelated to Turkkish identificatio on and positiv vely related to o Dutch identtification. In tthis context thhen more perm meability was related d to less ingrou up identificattion and moree outgroup higgher status iddentification. W When the status structure was percceived as less stable and reelatively legitiimate, there w was no relatioon between permeabilitty and ingroup p identificatio on. When the status structuure was low inn legitimacy and regardless of o the level off stability, theere was no asssociation betw ween permeabbility and inggroup identificatio on. For the hiigh status grou up, perceivedd legitimacy hhad a positivee effect on inggroup identificatio on, such that the t more statu us differencess were perceiived to be legitimate, the m more high status group p members id dentified with Dutch. Theree were no siggnificant effeccts for stabilitty or permeabilitty, and there were w no significant interacctions. Importantly y, Verkuyten and Reijerse (2008) discovvered some evvidence that ssociostructuraal beliefs influenced intergroup peerceptions. Fo or example, paarticipants peerceived statuus irrelevant ddimensions T the low wer status grouup. And for thhe status relevvant dimensioons, high as more stereotypic of Turks, status mem mbers perceiveed the dimenssions as more stereotypic oof their ingrouup rather thann their outgroup, whereas w low status s memberrs perceived ttheir ingroup and outgroupp as equivalennt on those dimensionss. Among low w status memb bers, Verkuyteen and Reijerrse regressionn analyses on the status irrelevant dimensions d sh howed that thee main effectss for stability and legitimaacy were signiificant, and their in nteractions with permeabiliity were also significant. T The effects weere qualified bby a significant three-way intteraction amo ong the sociosstructural variiables for the status irrelevvant dimensionss. Similar to th he findings fo or ingroup ideentification, w when status diifferences weere perceived as a stable and relatively r legiitimate, there was a negativve associationn between permeability and ingroup p stereotypes.. Therefore, in n this intergrooup context, w when group bboundaries weere seen as more permeeable, stereoty ypes on irreleevant dimensiions were seeen as less posiitive for the inngroup. For the otheer combinatio ons of stability y and legitim macy, no relatiion was obserrved for perm meability and ingroup p stereotypes.. For the statu us relevant dim mensions, theere were signiificant negativve effects of permeab bility and legittimacy, as weell as an interaaction betweeen stability annd legitimacy. When status differrences were relatively r legiitimate, more stability wass associated w with more positive outgroup sttereotypes. When W status diffferences werre illegitimatee, more stability was assocciated with less positive outgroup sttereotypes. h status group p members, Verkuyten V andd Reijerse (20008) found ann effect of leggitimacy For the high for group sttereotypes on n the status irrrelevant dimennsions. The pperceived legiitimacy of thee status structure was positively associated wiith more posiitive ingroup stereotypes annd less positive outgroup sttereotypes. Hiigh status mem mbers ascribeed more posittive attributioons to themsellves. The status relev vant dimension ns showed a main m effect o f stability andd an interactioon between leegitimacy and stability y. When statu us differencess were perceivved as relatively legitimatee, more stabillity was associated with w less posiitive outgroup p stereotypes,, whereas wheen differences were perceiived as illegitimatee, there was no o association between stabbility and outggroup stereotyypes. Verkuyten and Reijerse (2008) also examined e the impact percepptions of sociiostructural beliefs would havee on ingroup feelings. f Regrression analysses showed thhat for low staatus group members, there was a positive effeect of stability y, such that m more perceivedd stability was associated w with more positive ing group feelingss. For feelings toward the ooutgroup, theere was a posiitive effect for permeabilitty. Verkuyten n and Reijersee showed that more perceivved permeabiility in group boundaries was related to t more positiive feelings tooward the Duutch outgroup. There was aalso a legitimacy by stability in nteraction forr outgroup feeelings. When status was peerceived as leggitimate, more stability was assocciated with mo ore positive fe feelings towarrd the Dutch. In contrast, w when status was perceived p as illegitimate, i more m stabilityy was associatted with less ppositive feelinngs toward the Dutch. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 73 A Analyses on th he ratings for feelings of lo ow status mem mbers toward other minoritty groups (sum mmed raatings for Surinamese, Morroccans, and Antilleans) A sh howed that thhere was a thrree-way interaaction am mong the sociostructural variables. v Wheen the intergrroup context w was perceivedd as legitimatte and sttable, there was a negative association between b perm meability and ffeelings towaard minority ggroups, suuch that a more permeable group bound dary with the higher h status group was reelated to less poositive feeling gs toward oth her minority groups. g The reelation betweeen permeabiliity and feelinngs tooward minoritty group mem mbers was nott significant for fo the other ccombinations of legitimacyy and sttability. With respect to thee high status group’s g feelin ngs toward otther minority outgroups, reesults inndicated that there t was a neegative main effect for botth stability annd legitimacy,, but these efffects w were qualified by an interacction between n legitimacy and a stability. V Verkuyten annd Reijerse (2008) foollow-up anallyses showed that when thee intergroup context c was pperceived to bbe legitimate, more sttability was asssociated with h less positivee outgroup feeelings. Howeever, when thee context wass illlegitimate, th here was no asssociation bettween stability y and outgrouup feelings. F Finally, legitim macy haad a positive effect on ingrroup feelings,, such that wh hen status wass perceived as legitimate, hhigh sttatus group members m reporrted more possitive feelingss toward theirr ingroup. V Verkuyten and d Reijerse (2008) underscore the importaance of the innterplay amonng perceived sttability, legitim macy, and peermeability fo or understandiing intergroupp relations. W When intergrouup reelations were legitimate an nd stable, low status group members tennded to perceiive the outgrooup faavourably on stereotypes th hat were relev vant to differeences in sociaal status. Wheen the relationns were peerceived as illegitimate, ho owever, low status s members showed lesss stereotypinng on the relevvant diimensions and d less positivee feelings tow ward the higheer status grouup the more sttable the statuus sttructure was perceived p to be. b These find dings indicatee that when staatus differencces are not vaalid, loow status grou up members may m question differences in n status betweeen their ownn group and thhe high sttatus group. When W high staatus group possition was perrceived as desserved and juustified, high status m members show wed more ingrroup identification, more positive p feelinngs toward theeir ingroup, aand peerceived feweer differencess between theiir ingroup and d the outgrouup on dimensiions that weree irrrelevant to th he status structure. In Verku uyten and Reeijerse’s studyy, group statuss stability also m moderated the effects of leg gitimacy. Wheen the intergrroup context w was perceivedd as valid, moore grroup stability was related to t more negattive evaluations of the outggroup on statuus relevant diimensions and d more negatiive feelings to oward lower status groupss. When the differences in status w were not seen as a deserved or o justified, staability was no ot related to ooutgroup evalluations or atttitudes. V Verkuyten and d Reijerse mak ke an importaant contributio on to understaanding the differential effeects of soociostructurall beliefs on in ntergroup percceptions. Joohnson, Terry y, and Louis (2005) ( also in nvestigated thee effects of m multiple sociosstructural belliefs on inntergroup percceptions. Specifically, John nson and collleagues were interested in negative intergroup peerceptions, naamely prejudiice and stereo otyping. Theree has been rennewed publicc awareness of raacism and preejudice as a peervasive prob blem in Austraalia. As such,, the study exaamined the prredictors of blatant and sub btle prejudicee as well as stereotyping foor majority W White Australiaans tooward minoritty Asian Austtralians. Three sociostructu ural beliefs w were hypothesized to predicct prrejudice: instaability, permeeability, and legitimacy. l In n addition, higgher ingroup sstatus and auuthoritarianism m were included as predicctors. Johnson n and colleaguues had three specific hyypotheses. Firrst, it was preedicted that when w perceptio ons of instabiility are high, perceptions oof peermeable grou up boundaries would be po ositively relatted to blatant and subtle prrejudice againnst A Asian Australians unfavorab ble stereotypiing, particularrly among W White Australiaans who perceeived hiigh ingroup sttatus. Second d, it was prediicted that the previously p deescribed interractive effectss beetween stabiliity and permeeability on preejudice and sttereotyping w would also be strong amongg W White Australiians who percceived high leegitimacy of their t status. L Lastly, it was ppredicted thatt high Paage 74 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® perceptionss of status and d the legitimaacy of status pposition wouldd have interacctive effects oon prejudice an nd stereotypin ng. To test their hypotheses,, Johnson and d colleagues (22005) mailedd out questionnnaire packages that included measures of au uthoritarianism m, perceived iinstability, peerceived statuus, perceived legitimacy, subtle and bllatant prejudice, and positiive stereotypiic perceptionss of Asian Auustralians. A total of 265 2 Australian n residents retturned the quuestionnaire package (respoonse rate = 311%) and 10 participaants who did not n identify as a White Austtralian were eexcluded from m analysis. Foor the most part, the scaales used dem monstrated accceptable reliaability (Į= .699 - .83). Due tto methodologgical concerns, th he stability measure m was dichotomized tto contrast reespondents whho perceived White Australianss’ position as unstable and changing (n = 144) versuss those who ddid not (n = 977). Results of regression r mo odels simple slope s analysiss demonstrateed that higherr perceptions oof ingroup status and permeability p was w linked wiith stronger bblatant prejudiice and less eendorsement oof positive stereotypes when the soccial positions were view ass unstable, suupporting Johnnson et al.’s ((2005) first hypothesis. When the so ocial position was viewed aas high and sttable, percepttions of permeeable group boun ndaries were unrelated u to blatant prejudiice, weakly liinked to less ffavourable stereotyping, and associaated with low wer subtle biass against Asiaan Australianss. Similarly, JJohnson and colleag gues’ second hypothesis h waas supported. White Austraalians who viiewed their soocial position to be unstable and a the intergrroup boundarries to be perm meable demonnstrated increeasing blatant prejudice the more they perceived their grooup status to bbe legitimate. Together, thhese interactionss imply that majority m group p members “eexperience inccreased threaat when they pperceive that their su uperior status position is in nsecure, particcularly when the intergrouup boundariess are perceived to o be permeab ble and their dominant d statuus position is judged to be high or legitiimate.” (Johnson ett al., p.66) Th hese results also suggest thaat this increassed perception of threat leaads to greater outg group bias. Johnson and colleagues’ (2005) also found f significcant interactioon effects forr legitimacy aand status and these were w significan ntly associateed with both ssubtle and blaatant prejudice. In other woords, mate demonsstrated increassed blatant White Austtralians who viewed v their status s to be hiigh and legitim and subtle prejudice. p Ov verall, the inteeractive effeccts of instabiliity, permeabiility and legitiimacy in their study highlights thee importance of examiningg the interplayy among socioostructural beeliefs in the predictiion of intergro oup perceptio ons, such as prrejudice. Although Jaackson (2002 2) did not speccifically lookk at sociostrucctural beliefs, his research eexamined group identtity, conflict and a prejudice, so it is incluuded here for interest. In brrief, he argued that the relation bettween group identity i and in ngroup bias w would be stronnger for majoority ethnic grroup members raather than min nority ethnic group membeers. Jackson rreasoned that majority grouup members’ ethnic e identity y should be saalient if they pperceive confflict with anoother group annd should elicit ingrou up bias. In co ontrast, ethnic minority grooup members have a varietty of reasons ffor identifying with their gro oup, such as the t salience oor visibility off their group, and not limitted to perceived conflict. c Thereefore, a relation between ggroup identityy and prejudicce is predictedd but this relation sho ould be strong ger for majoriity than minorrity group meembers. And tthe perceivedd intergroup conflict c is priimarily respon nsible for thiss relation. Jackson (20 002) administtered a questio onnaire that aassessed the vvariables of innterest. Particcipants belonged to o one of threee ethnic group ps: White Am merican, Africaan American,, or Asian Am merican. There weree six different versions of th he questionnaaire to reflectt six different ingroup/outggroup situations based b on the participants’ p ethnicity. e Thuus, each ethnicc group (e.g., White Amerrican) had to two outg groups (Africaan American and Asian Am merican), andd, as such, theere were two vversions of the question nnaire for eacch group. Furtthermore, Jacckson assessed two major ddimensions of group Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 75 iddentity: interd dependency (iidentifying wiith ingroup members m regullarly) and inggroup attractioon (ppride in ingrou up). He also assessed a perceptions of con nflict with thee outgroup (oobtaining outggroup gooals prevents obtaining ing group goals). Finally, threee dimensions of prejudice w were assessedd: (1) beehavioural inttentions, by using u social diistance items (e.g., the exteent to which tthey would accept ann outgroup member m as a go ood friend); (2 2) affect, by asking a particiipants to indiccate how theyy w would feel if th hey were at a party and a derogating d jok ke was told abbout [the outggroup] (scenaario addapted from Bynes B and Kiger, 1988, as cited in Jacksson) using sem mantic differential scales ((e.g., reesentment—aacceptance); and a (3) cognittion, by asking g participantss to indicate hhow they wouuld reeact if they weere at a dinneer and some of the guests stated that [ouutgroup membbers] are the rreason foor many of tod day’s social problems p (sceenario adapted d from Byness and Kiger, 11988; cited in Jaackson) using g bipolar scalees (e.g., unfair—fair) and close-ended c ittems (e.g., “M Most [outgrouup m members] havee the drive an nd determinatiion to get aheead”). Higher scores indicaate more prejuudice annd all three prrejudice meassures were po ositively correelated. Jaackson (2002)) found that African A Amerricans showed d the strongesst group identtification folloowed byy White Ameericans and then by Asian Americans. A Id dentification w was significanntly differentt for all thhree groups. African A Amerricans also perrceived the grreatest amounnt of intergrouup conflict foollowed byy Asian Amerricans and theen by White Americans. A African A Ameriicans significaantly differedd from A Asians and Wh hite American ns, but the diffference between the latterr two groups w was only marginal. A Additional anaalyses showed d that the high hest level of conflict c was fo for African Am mericans withh reespect to White Americanss as the outgro oup. When th he two dimenssions were annalysed separaately, A African Ameriicans showed more ingroup p attraction th han both the A Asian Americcan and Whitee A American samp ples, but the latter l two gro oups did not differ d from eacch other. In aaddition, Asiaan A Americans sho owed less inteerdependency y than did Wh hite Americanns, and there w was no differeence beetween the White W and Afriican American n groups. M More importan ntly, Jackson (2002) found d prejudice lev vels were fairrly low amongg all participaants. A 6 (intergroup context) c × 3 (P Prejudice type) ANOVA with w the latterr factor as witthin-subjects w was coonducted on the t prejudice scores. Theree was a signifficant interacti tion between tthe two factorrs reevealing that the t intergroup p context had d no effect on expressions oof affective prrejudice, but did haave an effect on prejudiced d behaviourall intentions an nd cognitions . For prejudicced behaviourral inntentions, Asian Americans reported lesss prejudice to oward White Americans coompared to A African A Americans. Fo or prejudiced cognitions, c African A Ameriicans showed more prejudiice toward W White A Americans com mpared to Asiian American ns, and White Americans shhowed more pprejudice tow ward A African Ameriicans compareed to Asian Americans. A Jaackson’s (200 02) analysis of the relation between grou up identity annd perceived iintergroup coonflict shhowed that th his relation waas significantlly stronger fo or White Ameericans compaared to Asian and A African Ameriicans. A simillar pattern of results was fo ound for the rrelation betweeen group ideentity annd prejudice. When the gro oup identity dimensions d were examinedd separately, tthe correlationnal findings were more m consisteent with the in nterdependen ncy than ingrooup attraction dimension. A Additional anaalyses on the ingroup i attracction dimension showed thhat for White Americans, inngroup atttraction was positively asssociated with prejudiced co ognitions agaainst African A Americans annd prrejudiced beh haviours again nst Asian Am mericans. Thesse two correlaations were noo longer signiificant w when interdepeendency and perceived p con nflict were tak ken into accoount in a regreession analysiis. For booth minority groups, g ingroup attraction was not signiificantly correelated with prrejudice againnst ouutgroups. On the interdepeendence dimension, analysses generally sshowed that ffor White A Americans, intterdependencee was positiveely and signifficantly correelated with alll three types oof prrejudice for both b African and a Asian Am mericans (exceept for prejuddiced cognitioons for this lattter grroup). When ingroup attracction and percceived conflicct were controolled for in a regression annalysis, Paage 76 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® the correlattions were stilll significant except for preejudiced behaavioural intenntions toward African Americans.. For African Americans, interdependenncy was not siignificantly ccorrelated withh the three types of preejudice againsst White Ameericans or Asiian Americanns. For Asian Americans, thhere were no significaant correlation ns between in nterdependenccy and the thrree types of pprejudice for bboth outgroups. For White Am mericans and African Ameericans, perceeived conflictt was the best predictor for all threee types of prejjudice (contro olling for bothh dimensionss of group identity). Jackson’s (2002) study supports s the notion n that thee relation betw ween group iddentity and prrejudice depends on n the status off the groups in nvolved. For hhigh status grroup memberss, stronger inggroup identificatio on was associiated with mo ore negative pperceptions off low status group memberrs, whereas forr low status group g memberrs, ingroup iddentification w was not correllated with intergroup perceptionss. Jackson sho owed that perceived confli ct was the best predictor oof prejudice foor White and African n Americans. Moreover, in ngroup interdeependency was a better preedictor of preejudice compared to ingroup attrraction. The relationship r bbetween confllict and interggroup perceptiion such as prejudicee should be ex xamined with h variances inn sociostructurral beliefs as these will likkely be associated. ummarizes thee research thaat assesses alll three sociosttructural dimeensions in rellations to Table 11 su intergroup perceptions. p Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 77 Table 11. Summary of research combining all a three soc ciostructura al dimensions in relation to o intergroup p perceptio ons. Reeference Outcome Intergroup Percceptions Veerkuyten (2005) Ͳ Multicultturalism is more strongly endorsed by a minority grouup whereas assimilaation is more strongly endorsed by a majority group. Ͳ Multicultturalism is associaated with a strongeer ethnic group ideentification and more poositive ingroup evaaluations among minority m groups whhereas among majorityy groups it is assocciated with weakerr ethnic group idenntification, more neegative ingroup evvaluations and morre positive outgrouup evaluations. Ͳ Assimilaation is associatedd with more negative outgroup evaluuations among majorityy group members. Ͳ Majorityy group members perceive p a more stable status structture and more permeable boundaries thaan minority group members and thiss is strongest when asssimilation is endoorsed. Ͳ When boundaries are perrceived to be perm meable, there is lesss support for milation. multicultturalism and moree support for assim Ͳ Stabilityy is associated withh stronger group iddentification amonng majority group members m whereas permeability is asssociated with weaaker group identificaation among minoority group membeers. Multiculturalism, Ͳ Low status groups perceivve a legitimate soccial structure as leess stable and more peermeable than an illegitimate social structure. Ͳ High staatus groups perceiive a legitimate soocial structure as m more stable and lesss permeable than an illegitimate soccial structure. Ͳ Within loow status groups, when status struccture is perceived to be stable and legitimate more perm meability is associaated with less ingrooup identificaation and more ouutgroup identification. Ͳ High staatus groups are moore likely to identiffy with their groupp when the structuree is perceived to be b legitimate regarrdless of stability oor permeability. Ͳ Low status groups expresss less positive inggroup stereotypes on status irrelevannt dimensions wheen the boundaries between groups aare permeable. Ͳ High staatus groups expresss more positive inngroup stereotypees and less positive outgroup stereotyypes on status irreelevant dimensionss when the status structure is seen ass legitimate. Ͳ On statuus relevant dimenssions, low status groups g express moore positive outgroup stereotypes andd less positive ingrroup stereotypes w when the b stable rather than unstable, wherreas within the structuree is perceived to be high status group, when status s differences were perceived too be legitimate, more staability is associateed with less positivve outgroup stereootypes. Ͳ In stablee, illegitimate, and impermeable situuations, low statuss groups stress their distinctive ethnic identity and distance themselves from other low status groups. Ingroup identificaation Ͳ Higher status s and more permeability is assoociated with moree blatant prejudicce and less endorssement of positive stereotypes whenn the status structuree is perceived to be b unstable. Ͳ Under stable status conditions, high status and permeability w were unrelated to blatannt prejudice, weakkly related to less favourable f stereottyping, and associatted with lower subbtle bias. Ͳ The more permeable grouup boundaries aree perceived to be bby members of more legitimate a high status group underr an unstable sociaal structure, the m the struccture is believed too be and the moree blatant prejudicee that is displayeed. Ͳ Memberrs who believe theey are part of a higgh status group whho believe their status too be legitimate dissplay both subtle and a blatant prejudiice. Ͳ Ingroup attraction and inggroup interdependeency in majority g roups are associatted with prejudice towards the minoority outgroup wheereas ingroup Subtle prejudice Veerkuyten & Reijersse (2008) Joohnson, Terry, & Loouis (2005) Jaackson (2002) Paage 78 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Assimilation, Ingroup evaluatioon, Outgroup evaluaation, Ethnic group identification Outgroup favourritism Distinctiveness Blatant prejudicee Positive outgroup stereotypes Intergroup prejuddice Humanssystems® Reference Outco ome Intergroup Perceptions attraction and interdeependency are nott associated with pprejudice towards the outgroup among minority group meembers. Ͳ Perrceived intergroup conflict is related to ingroup identityy for majority grouup members, but not forr minority group m embers. Ͳ Thee best predictor of prejudice, especi ally among the maajority group, is perceived intergroup conflict. 5.5 Su ummary In assessing g how sociosttructural belieefs impact inttergroup perceeptions, the reesearch has inndicated that among high status groups, g strong ger ingroup iddentification is related to m more negative outgroup perceptionss (e.g., intergrroup prejudicee). The impacct of status staability on evaaluations of thhe outgroup arre only eviden nt when the evaluations aree measured im mplicitly. Whhen status is uunstable, and individ duals are stron ngly identified d with their inngroup, they aare more likely to exhibit nnegative implicit outtgroup evaluaations. Explicit evaluationss are only imppacted by the identificationn with the ingroup, no ot the stability y or instability y of the statuss. The perceptions of whatt constitutes a legitimate str tructure vary ddepending onn group statuss. Specifically y, low status groups g perceiive a legitimaate social struccture to be unnstable and peermeable, whereas hig gh status grou ups perceive a legitimate sttructure to bee stable and im mpermeable. This also has implicaations for outg group percepttions. When tthe status struucture is perceeived by low status groups to be stable and legitimate, l permeability is associated w with more posiitive outgroupp perceptionss. On the otheer hand, legitim macy is assocciated with m more negative outgroup evaaluations among high h status group ps. Legitimacy y is also asso ciated with leess feelings off guilt and moore feelings of threat from th he lower statu us group. n ideology thaat justifies thee hierarchical relationship among groupps (i.e., a legittimizing Belief in an ideology, su uch as individ dual mobility) moderates th the relationshiip between sttatus and interrgroup perceptionss. While low status s groups who endorsee the legitimaccy of the struccture are less likely to perceive disscrimination from the outg group, higher status group members whho endorse leggitimizing ideologies are a more likelly to feel disccriminated aggainst by the lower status ooutgroup. When statu us structure is perceived to be more stabble and less peermeable, assiimilation is aadvocated especially by b high statuss groups. Supp porting assim milation (or coonversely showing little suupport for multiculturalism) leads to t a decrease in outgroup eevaluations am mong high staatus groups, w whereas support for multiculturallism increasess outgroup evvaluations. W When the position of high sttatus groups is po otentially threeatened by su uggestions of instability annd permeabilitty in legitimaate social structures, protective p ressponses are deemonstrated iin the form off both blatant and subtle prrejudice, and less fav vourable stereeotyping. When statu us is perceived d to be stable and impermeeable, but illegitimate by loow status groups, they are likely to o emphasize their t distinctiv veness from oother low stattus groups in order to elevaate their own status relative to thee other group. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 79 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 80 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 6. How do o socia al identtity ma anagem ment sttrategiies inflluence e interg group pe ercepttions? (4.7.5)) An extensiv ve search of existing e psych hological literrature was connducted to innvestigate how w various identity maanagement strategies identiified by Tajfeel and Turner (1986) can innfluence interrgroup perceptionss. This search revealed thatt there is littlee research dirrectly examining this relatiionship. However, th here were som me articles that look at othher identity m management sttrategies and tthe impact these have on intergroup p perceptions that are incluuded in this reeview. a Brown (2 2006) were innterested in thhe effects of tw wo strategies For examplle, Gonzalez and individualizzation (decateegorization) and a superordinnate recategoorization (“com mmon group recategorization”) on inttergroup bias during and b eyond a contaact situation. Recall, Blanzz et al. (1998) conssidered chang ge of categorization to be a cognitive iddentity management style uunderstood as individuaalization, sup perordinate reccategorizationn, and suborddinate re-categgorization. Individualizzation occurs when low staatus memberss no longer deefine themsellves as members of a specific gro oup, but ratheer as unique in ndividuals whho are not affe fected by grouup evaluationss (Ng, 1989; cited in Blanz et al.). a Again, thee superordinaate re-categorrization strategy involves fformer ingroup and d outgroup members m defining themselvees in terms off a common, hhigher level iingroup (e.g., Kessler & Mummeendey, 2002; Hornsey & H Hogg, 2002). IIn contrast, thhe subordinatee recategorization strategy y involves div viding the form mer ingroup iinto subgroupps. They weree also interested in n a third man nagement strattegy, dual ideentities. Gonzzalez and Brow wn explain thhat individuals can maintain n their subgroup identities (enables grouup distinctivenness) within aan inclusive co ommon identiity with outgrroup memberrs (enables ouutgroup membbers to be seenn as ingroup meembers). In th he context of Gonzalez G andd Brown’s ressearch, this strrategy was viewed as a strategy forr minimizing bias toward an a outgroup. L Low status m members who eendorse dual identities simultaneou usly could use their higherr status membbership of thee superordinatte group to baalance the low status of o their subordinate group. Gonzalez and Brown (20 006) also won ndered if the eeffects of these categorizattion strategies would be moderated by group size s and status. With respeect to the form mer, minority group members may be more con ncerned with the distinctiv veness of theirr group relatiive to majorityy group mem mbers, and therefore, should be mottivated to eng gage in ingrouup favouritism m to preserve their group iddentity. For group status, s low staatus memberss may be moree concerned w with identity eenhancementt whereas high status members maay be concerneed with identiity protectionn. The need foor high status members to protect th heir identity should s lead to o more ingrouup favouritism m. As such, G Gonzalez and B Brown predicted th hat minority and a high statu us group mem mbers would sshow more biaas than majorrity and low status group g membeers. Gonzalez and Brown (20 006) also had d predictions rregarding the effects of thrree categorizaation strategies. When W de-cateegorization orr individualizaation is used as a strategy, outgroup meembers should be viewed v as indiividuals ratheer than memb ers of a speciific group. Thherefore, the ggroup distinctionss should have lost their meaning and inddividuals shouuld benefit froom a contact situation, outside of their t group staatus. Howeveer, the benefitts of de-categoorization mayy not be transferred beyond the contact situaation, because individuals m may not be pssychologicallyy linked to ouutgroup members orr the outgroup p members may m be perceivved as excepttions to the grroup. Gonzaleez and Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 81 B Brown predicteed that group size and statu us would hav ve no effect onn de-categorizzation both duuring annd beyond a contact c situatiion. W When common n recategorizaation or superrordinate iden ntity is used aas a strategy, iindividuals reecaategorize theiir group membership so thaat they belong g to the same group as theiir outgroup m members. Gon nzalez and Bro own (2006) point p out that the t benefits fr from the contaact situation m may noot be generaliized beyond this t situation because b differences in gro up size or staatus make subbgroup iddentities salien nt. For minorrity members, they may feeel more threattened with reccategorizationn beecause becom ming part of th he same identtity as majoritty members ccan mean that they lose theeir grroup distinctiveness. Mino ority memberss may show more m intergrouup bias both dduring and beeyond a coontact situatio on. For high status s group members, m they y may not waant to be subsuumed into thee same grroup identity as low status members and d try to protecct their status, especially iff the group booundaries seeem permeablee. Thus, Gonzzalez and Brow wn predicted that high stattus minority m members woulld avoid recattegorization and a show morre intergroup bbias relative tto low status m minority memb bers. For low status group members, theey may be mootivated to ennhance their iddentity w when group bo oundaries seem m permeable,, because sharring an identiity with high status membeers can booost their colllective self-essteem. Gonzaalez and Brow wn predicted tthat both majoority and minnority loow status grou up members would w show more m outgroup p favouritism m after recateggorizing their iddentity. For majority m memb bers, recatego orizing their id dentity shouldd not pose a tthreat to the diistinctivenesss of their grou up identity, so o should show w less intergrooup bias than minority mem mbers. W When group sttatus is taken into account, low status majority m membbers can potenntially enhancce their iddentity by bein ng part of thee superordinatte identity, wh hereas high sttatus majorityy members acctually reepresent the su uperordinate identity and may m subsequeently view low w status majoority memberss neegatively. Go onzalez and Brown predicteed that the hig gh status maj ority would bbe more biaseed than loow status majority. Fiinally, invokiing dual identtity as an iden ntity managem ment strategy should be moost important for m minority high status s and min nority low staatus group meembers indep endent of grooup status. A According to Gonzalez G and Brown (2006 6), the formerr should be abble to protect their status annd the laatter should bee able to beneefit from bein ng part of an in nclusive cate gory and maiintain their suubgroup identtity, and thus would be exp pected to show w less intergrroup bias. Theey argued thaat high sttatus majority y group memb bers would bee expected to show more inntergroup biass and low stattus m majority group p members wo ould be expeccted to exhibitt more outgrooup favouritissm, particularrly w when differencces in status are a perceived as stable and legitimate. To test their hy ypotheses, Go onzalez and Brown B (2006) conducted tw wo experimennts to look at tthe efffectiveness of o the three caategorization strategies on intergroup biias, attitudes ffavouring inggroups ovver outgroupss, during and beyond a con ntact situation n, and how theese strategies were affected by grroup size and d status. In Ex xperiment 1, th hey examined d whether thee categorizatioon strategies w were m moderated by group g size. In n Experiment 2, they exam mined whetherr strategies weere moderatedd by grroup size and d group status.. Because the two Experim ments used a ssimilar paradiigm, only Experiment 2 is i described. ntal design used by Gonzallez and Brown n (2006) for E Experiment 2 was a 3 The experimen Categorization n Strategy: Seeparate Indiviiduals vs. Onee Group vs. D Dual Identities) × 2 (Groupp Size: (C M Minority vs. Majority) M × 2 (Group ( Statuss: High vs. Lo ow) factorial ddesign. Particcipants were ttold thhat the experim ment was abo out decision making. m Each experimentall session invoolved six unnacquainted participants. p They T first com mpleted a task k that was alleegedly used too identify theeir prroblem solvin ng thinking sttyle, and weree assigned eith her an “Analyytic” or “Syntthetic” probleem soolving style. For the group p size manipu ulation, two off the participaants were cateegorized into one of Paage 82 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® the styles fo orming the minority m group p, and the rem maining four pparticipants w were categorized into the other style forming the majority m group p. Participantts were told thhat the group sizes in the ssession reflected naatural proporttions in the po opulation, whhich was a 20//80% split. Annalytics weree the minority grroup for half of o the session ns. For the grooup status maanipulation, paarticipants weere told that one of the problem solving s group ps was successsful at complleting about 668% of the tassks relative to the t other grou up at about 61 1%. Analyticss were describbed as the higgh status grouup in half of the sessions. Participaants were told d that the infoormation abouut the two stylles was foundded on research an nd all of them were given a badge to weaar with eitherr “Analytic” oor “Synthetic”” on it. Each subgroup worked independently i y on a group ddecision-makking task (Winnter Survival Task; Johnson & Johnson, 197 75; cited in Go onzalez & Brrown, 2006) th that was desiggned to increaase group nd group iden ntity. Afterwaards, participaants indicated their level off identificatioon with cohesion an their group on a scale wiith items such h as “Do you feel strong tiees with your fellow group ndor, Matthew ws, Wade, & Williams, 19986; Ellemers, Kortekass, & members?”” (Brown, Con Ouwerker, 1999; both ciited in Gonzalez & Brown)). Participants were then aasked to compplete a cooperativee task that inv volved ranking g a set of traitts that would be most ideaal for a successsful leader. Each h batch of 6 participants p were w randomlyy assigned to one of three ccategorizationn strategies, which w were crreated using various v manippulations suchh as seating aarrangements,, perceptual cues, c group laabels, photographs, etc. Paarticipants theen allocated reewards to thee people in their session (local reward allocation)) based on theeir contributioons (not incluuding the self)) by distributing g 100 chips, and their alloccations were cconfidential. N Next, to see w whether their attitudes from the co ontact situatio on were generralized outsidee of the situattion (general reward allocaation), they watcheed two videottapes that werre allegedly m made earlier oof Analytic annd Synthetic ggroups working tog gether (order was counterb balanced) andd allocated rew wards to the iindividuals inn the video using 100 chips. c Particip pants then ansswered manippulation checkk items (whicch were effecttive), and rated the qu uality of the contact c situation (e.g., coopperative, frienndly, close) annd their perceeptions of differences in group stattus. i , Gonzalez an nd Brown (20 06) found a ssignificant intteraction betw ween group For group identification, status and size. s Specificaally, those in the minority low status coondition identtified less stroongly with their subgro oup than did those t in the minority m high status and thee majority low w status conddition. No other signifficant differen nces were fou und between cconditions. Reesults also shhowed a signifficant main effectt for categorizzation strategy y. Post hoc teests indicated that participaants in the decategorizzation conditio on rated the contact c situatiion less positiively than didd those in the dual identity con ndition. Particcipants’ rating gs in the recattegorization ccondition, how wever, did noot differ from those in either the separate s indiv viduals or duaal identity connditions. A siignificant inteeraction between gro oup size and status was alsso revealed. S Simple effectss tests indicatted that membbers of minority low status grou ups rated the contact c situatiion less positiively than didd those in the majority low status groups. g Gonzalez and Brow wn did not obbserve any othher differencees between grroups. For both local and generral reward allo ocations, Gonnzalez and Brrown (2006) ccomputed an ingroup bias index by b subtracting g the number of chips alloccated to outgrroup memberrs from ingrouup members (values can n range from -100 to +100; negative valuues mean outtgroup favourrability, zero m means no bias, positiv ve values meaan ingroup favourability). Analysis reveealed a main effect of grouup status on local reward allocations.. Participants in the high sttatus groups sshowed more bias than thoose in the g A maain effect of grroup size wass also observeed such that pparticipants inn the low status groups. minority grroups were mo ore biased thaan those in thhe majority grroups. There w was no main eeffect for categorizatiion strategy. However, H all three strategiies limited inttergroup bias during the coontact situation an nd none of thee means were significantlyy different fro m zero. Categgorization strrategy did Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 83 noot interact witth group size or status. Forr general rewaard allocationn, a significannt three-way innteraction wass observed am mong strategy y, group size and a status (se e Figure 12). Figure 12 2. Minority and a majority group’s bias b in gene eral reward allocations s in Experiment 2. 14 (Gonzalez & Brown, 2 006, p. 763)) Sttarting with th he minority groups g in the top t figure, hig gh status mem mbers showedd significantlyy more inngroup bias th han low statuss members in both the sepaarate individuuals and one ggroup conditioons. H However, statu us had no effeects in the duaal identity con ndition. Theree were no diff fferences amoong low sttatus members. In majority y groups in the bottom figu ure, high statuus members shhowed signifi ficantly m more ingroup bias b than low status members for all thrree strategy coonditions in thhe majority ggroups. Fiindings from Gonzales and d Brown (200 06) show that all three cateegorization strrategies used during thhe contact situ uation limited d intergroup bias b (bias was near zero). H However, grouup size and grroup sttatus affected levels of inteergroup bias regardless r of categorization c n strategy. Hiigh status andd m minority memb bers showed more m bias, deemonstrating that t group sizze and group sstatus may bee more poowerful than the strategiess being used and a can remov ve any positivve benefits gaained from thee coontact situatio on between th he groups. G Generalizing beyond b the contact situation n, Gonzalez and a Brown (22006) found thhat group stattus also haad an effect on o intergroup bias such thaat members off high status ggroups were ssimply motivaated to m maintain their status. Howev ver, when ind dividuals weree allocating rrewards to meembers they hhad not enncountered, group g size did d not seem to matter. m Resullts showed thaat for membeers of minorityy grroups, the duaal identity straategy worked d best such thaat there was aalmost no genneralized biass for 14 Note. *Significantly different from zero at p < .01. Paage 84 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® high or low w status group ps. In contrast, Gonzalez annd Brown fouund that high status seemedd to exhibit ingrroup bias and d low status members m seem med to exhibit outgroup biaas in the separrate individuals and one grou up conditions respectively.. Contrary to what they hadd expected, liittle difference was w found bettween the reccategorizationn and dual ideentity conditioons. For majoority members, none n of the strrategies limiteed bias for hiigh status grouups, which suuggests that oother strategies may m be requireed. Overall, Gonzalez G and Brown demoonstrated that identity mannagement strategies, such s as chang ges in categorrization, have potential to aaffect intergrooup perceptioons such as ingroup and d outgroup bias. Schmitt and d Maes (2002 2) were also in nterested in pperceptions off ingroup biass, particularlyy in East Germans. Unlike U Gonzaalez and Brow wn (2006) thatt viewed ingrooup bias as ann outcome thaat should be managed d, Schmitt and d Maes actually viewed it as an identityy managemennt strategy. Sinnce the unification of Germany, the distinctio on between Eaast Germans and West Gerrmans has creeated a here, on many y objectively measured dim mensions, succh as econom mic success, East situation wh Germans arre left feeling inferior to West W Germanss, thus creatinng a negative ssocial identityy (see Kessler and d Mummendeey, 2002). To improve theirr negative soccial identity, Schmitt and M Maes suggest that East Germaans could emp ploy social creeativity as a m management sstrategy, and shift comparison n dimensions in their favou ur. They arguee that these ddimensions shhould be deem med important to o the ingroup p and difficultt to measure oobjectively (e.g., cooperatiion, social conntact, helping). Scchmitt and Maes M proposed d that, in manaaging their iddentity, East G Germans wouuld “attribute higher h scores on o socially deesirable persoonality traits tto themselves than to Westt German” (p. 311). Th his would be particularly p trrue of those w who identify sstrongly with the East Germ man identity as the t inferior so ocial identity would be moore personallyy threatening tto them. Becaause West Germans seee themselvess as the higherr status groupp, Schmitt andd Maes argueed that they w would not feel the neeed to engage in i the same leevel of ingrouup bias. Like Kessleer and Mumm mendey (2002 2), Schmitt annd Maes (20022) consider noot only SIT, bbut also the impact of relative deeprivation on intergroup i peerceptions. Unnderstood as tthe differencee between what the paarticipants feltt they ought to t have versuss what they fe felt they did haave with resppect to quality of liife (work and d labour, prosp perity, humann relations, hoousing and citties, and the nnatural environmen nt), they argued that relativ ve deprivationn would havee a causal imppact on percepptions of ingroup biaas. Schmitt an nd Maes exam mined ingroupp bias on a serries of 38 adjeectives. A facctor analysis of the 38 items revealed a thrree factor struucture: sympaathy, personall integrity, annd competencee. Taking the difference off ingroup ratinngs and outgrroup ratings yyielded a meaasure of ingroup biaas. Social iden ntity was asseessed only on the second (aand final) measurement occcasion of their longitu udinal study, and asked paarticipants to rrate themselvves on the deggree to which they considered themselves East E or West Germans. G Witth regard to inngroup bias, S Schmitt and M Maes (2002) foun nd that both East E and Westt Germans fouund their ownn group to be somewhat m more sympatheticc and compettent compared d to the outgrooup. Howeveer, on the integgrity scale, East Germans raated themselv ves as much higher than theeir West Germ man counterpparts. This waas particularly y true of thosee who identifiied closely wiith their East German ingroup. On the oother hand, West Germ man participan nts did not sho ow any ingrouup bias on inttegrity. In facct, they rated tthe East Germans ass much higherr in integrity than West Geermans. This outgroup bias negates the ingroup bias seen on n the other tw wo dimensionss (Schmitt & Maes). Accoording to Schm mitt and Maess, these findings pro ovide evidencce that East Germans G invokked social creeativity to maanage their ideentity, enhancing their t status on n dimensions that appear too be both impportant and haard to assess objectively. Moreover, th hey found thaat East Germaans perceivedd more fraternnal deprivatioon in all of the quality of life dimensions measured, except forr the natural eenvironment ddimension. T This Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 85 deeprivation waas somewhat lower l at Timee 2 than it waas at Time 1. O On the other hhand, West G Germans rated d themselves as a highly frateernally privileeged at both T Time 1 and T Time 2. To test whetheer relative dep privation resullted in an incrrease in ingrooup bias, Schm mitt and Maees (22002) conducted a longitud dinal regressio on analysis prredicting ingrroup bias at T Time 2 from inngroup biias at Time 1 (to control fo or bias tenden ncies) and relaative deprivattion at Time 11. They foundd that reelative deprivation at Timee 1 significanttly predicted ingroup i bias aat Time 2, buut this only acccounted for 1% of the varriance in ingro oup bias at Tiime 2. These findings are aalso consistennt with a longitudinal trend t analysiss Schmitt and d Maes condu ucted that show wed that relattive deprivatiion for East Germans decreased fro om Time 1 to Time 2. B Based on Schm mitt and Maess (2002) study y of high and low status grroups, East Geermans reporrted m much more relative deprivation in almost all domains compared to West Germaans, while Weest G Germans reporrted relative privilege. p Morreover, ingrou up bias existeed only withinn the East Gerrman grroup, specificcally on the diimension of in ntegrity. Schm mitt and Maees’ study sugggests that invooking a paarticular identity managem ment strategy, in this case social s creativiity, can influeence intergrouup peerceptions. C Caricati and Monacelli M (201 10) were interrested in interrgroup bias ass a result of soocial competiition. Inn particular, th hey were inteerested in the effects of low w, intermediatte, and high ggroup statusess on inntergroup biass. The inclusion of an interrmediate grou up is notable aas most resear arch on social iddentity theory y has typically y examined reesponses to status differencces between hhigh- and low w status grroups. Little research r has actually a exam mined responses from groupps with an inttermediate staatus – thhat is, they aree neither at th he upper end nor n the lower end of the soocial structuree, but are hiierarchically between b a hig gher and loweer status group p on a specifiic dimension of comparisoon. C Caricati and Monacelli M (201 10) suggest th hat intermediaate-status grouup members m may choose tto prrotect or imprrove their soccial position depending d on their perceptiions of the strructural conteext. Inntermediate sttatus group members m may be more likelly to protect ttheir status in situations thaat innvolve high so ocial competiition and cond ducive to losin ng their interm mediate statuus. In contrastt, these m members may be more likelly to improvee their social standing s and ccompare them mselves to thee hiigher-status group g when so ocial competition is low. In ntermediate status group m members wouuld be exxpected to sho ow more biass toward the lo ower status grroup when soocial competittion is high annd m more bias towaard the higherr status group p when compeetition is low.. To test their hy ypotheses, Caaricati and Mo onacelli (2010 0) provided pparticipants w with false feedbback abbout their thin nking style an nd were categ gorized into lo ow, intermediiate, or high sstatus groups. To m manipulate soccial competition, participan nts were asked to completee a memory taask in which reesources weree limited or no ot limited. Th he former con ndition was exxpected to eliccit greater soccial coompetition (e.g., Brewer, 1986; 1 cited in n Caricati & Monacelli). M M More specificaally, in the lim mited reesource condiition, particip pants were ask ked to allocatee 90 seconds of extra timee among threee annonymous meembers from each e of the th hree groups, whereas w thosee in the not lim mited resourcce coondition weree able to give up to 90 seco onds to each of o the three m members. The extra time would bee used for a su ubsequent meemory task. The T study desiign was thereefore a 3 (ingrroup status: loow vs. m moderate vs. high) h × 2 (reso ources: limited vs. not limiited) betweenn-subjects design. The depeendent m measures in the study were ingroup identtification (e.g g., “How mucch do you idenntify with thee group off Analytic/Miixed/Holistic thinkers?”) and a intergroup p biases (diffeerences betweeen ingroup aand ouutgroup allocation scores).. Foor intergroup bias, each grroup in Caricaati and Monaccelli (2010) sttudy was com mpared to the other tw wo groups and d, therefore, the t mean of th he two intergrroup biases inndicates overaall intergroupp bias. Paage 86 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Intergroup bias was anallysed as a 3 (iingroup statuss) × 2 (resourrces) × 2 (outgroup target: more vs. less advantaaged) with th he last factor as a within-subjjects. Results revealed a siignificant stattus main effect. The high status grroup showed less intergrouup bias comppared to the inntermediate annd low status group ps (the latter two t groups diid not differ ffrom each othher). In additioon, there wass also a target main n effect in whiich there was more bias aggainst the advvantaged grouup than the disadvantag ged group. Th here was also a marginallyy significant innteraction am mong the threee factors. Among low w status group p members, th here was a tarrget main effeect, such that tthey were moore biased against the high than inteermediate staatus group. Am mong intermeediate status ggroup membeers, there was a target main effect,, which was qualified q by a significant innteraction bettween target aand resources. The T interactio on showed thaat, within the limited resouurces conditioon, intermediaate status group mem mbers were mo ore biased agaainst high stattus group, whhereas in the nnon-limited reesources condition, they t were equ ually biased against the higgh and low staatus groups. F Finally, amonng high status group p members, th he target and resources maain effects weere both signifficant indicatiing that they were more m biased against a the inttermediate thaan low status group and moore biased whhen resources were w limited th han when they were not lim mited. Overall, Caaricati and Mo onacelli’s (20 010) study dem monstrated thhat status affects intergroupp bias. When resou urces were no ot limited, meembers from tthe intermediaate status grouup were moree biased against the high than thee low status grroup, as they predicted. Hoowever, whenn resources w were limited, meembers from the t intermediaate group werre equally biaased against thhe high and loow status groups, sho owing a chang ge in intergrou up perceptionn as a result oof social comppetition. Cariccati and Monacelli argued a that in ntermediate grroup memberrs may feel thhreatened by a potential shiift in status from both directio ons, i.e., high and low statuus groups. Thheir research thhen shows a connection between iden ntity managem ment strategiees and intergrroup perceptioons. ossible way id dentity manag gement strateggies can influeence intergroup perceptionns is by Another po mediating responses r to group g threat or o disadvantagge. Reactionss to threat to oone’s ingroup can lead to group-baased anxiety (e.g. ( Wohl, Brranscombe, aand Reysen, 22010) and angger (e.g. Van Z Zomeran, Spears, and d Leach, 2008 8), which can then lead to nnegative perceptions towarrd outgroups. Social identity maanagement strategies, such as social com mpetition, mayy emerge to m mitigate thesee reactions to ingroup threat, t thereby y influencing g intergroup pperceptions. W Wohl and colleagues (20100) were specifically y interested in n understandin ng how extincction threats aabout one’s inngroup wouldd affect group-based anxiety (or collective angst) and intraagroup behaviiour. They arggued that extiinction threat is a special s case off distinctiveness threat, annd hypothesizeed that, whenn invoked, woould foster a desire to strengthen s thee ingroup agaainst possible future threatss. To test thiss hypothesis, tthey conducted three t studies and a found thaat extinction tthreats resulteed in collectivve angst and ssupport for ingroup streengthening beehaviours, reg gardless of whhether the exttinction threaat stemmed froom the physical or symbolic dem mise of a univ versity, threattened culturall extinction of French Cannadians, or from threats related to hiistorical extin nction of the JJewish Holoccaust. Wohl ett al.’s research suggests that strategiies, such as co ollective actio on or social ccompetition, uused to managge identity thrreat may promote neegative intergrroup perceptions as a conssequence in orrder to preserrve social idenntity. In their own words, w “a poteential negativee consequencce of extinctioon threat is thhat by elicitingg collective angst, a the stag ge may be set for justifyingg aggressive aaction towardd adversarial groups…W When collectiv ve angst is exp perienced, acttions taken too strengthen aand protect thee ingroup are likely to o be endorsed d. At the extreeme, ingroup strengtheningg may serve tto legitimize ooutgroup harm doing g, that is, harm m committed in i the name oof protecting tthe ingroup frrom possible extinction.”” (p. 907). Another em motional respo onse to ingrou up threat or coollective disaadvantage is aanger. Van Zoomeran and colleag gues (2008) ex xamined the relationship r bbetween groupp-based angerr, group identtity and Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 87 coollective actio on (i.e., sociall competition n) in response to collective disadvantagee. Specificallyy, they prredicted that for f members of a disadvan ntaged group, group identitty would incrrease group-based annger which would w lead to collective c actiion based on emotion-focuused coping. O On the other hhand, m members of a disadvantaged d d group who did not identiify closely wiith the group would be lesss likely too engage in co ollective actio on, unless they y feel that thee collective acction would bbe of benefit tto them annd the group (group ( efficaccy). Group effficacy is then n conceptualizzed as problem m-focused cooping. A According to Van V Zomeran et al. (p. 357 7), “we expectt the relevancce of group iddentity to facillitate em motion-focused coping and d to moderatee problem-foccused coping with collectivve disadvantaage”. Inn the first stud dy, Van Zomeeran et al (200 08) assessed protesters p at a demonstratiion against finnancial cuuts to higher education. e Ussing survey daata, they evaluated particippants’ level of identificatioon with the sttudent group (e.g., ( “I see myself m as a stu udent”), group p-based angerr (e.g., “I feell angry becauuse of this prroposal”), gro oup efficacy (e.g., ( “I think together we are a able to chhange this situuation”), and collective acction tendencies (e.g., “I would w particip pate in a futuree demonstratiion to stop thiis proposal”).. To assess whetther group-baased emotionss mediate the relationship bbetween idenntification andd collective acction tendencies, a series of o multiple reg gression analy yses were connducted as suuggested by B Baron and K Kenny (1986; cited in Van Zomeran Z et all., 2008). Van n Zomeran et al. found thaat group identiification prredicted colleective action tendencies t as well as group p-based angerr. However, w when group-bbased anger w was entered intto the equatio on, predicting g collective acction tendenciies, group ideentification was no loonger a signifficant predicto or. Together, these t results imply i that grooup-based annger does meddiate the reelationship beetween group identification n and collectiv ve action tenddencies. To assess whetther group ideentity moderaated the impacct of group effficacy on colllective actionn, Van Zomeran et al (2008) follow wed the procedures suggestted by Aiken and West (19991; cited in V Van Zomeran et el.). Specifically y, they ran a multiple m regreession analysis to predict ccollective actiion teendencies from m group identtification, gro oup-based ang ger, and groupp efficacy as well as the grroup iddentification × group-based d anger and group g identificcation × groupp efficacy intteraction term ms. The reesults showed d that there waas a significan nt main effectt of group-baased anger, suuch that those who w were higher in group-based anger indicatted a greater tendency t tow wards collectivve action. Fuurthermore, th here was a sig gnificant grou up identificatiion × group eefficacy interaaction. Speciffically, am mong those who w had a low w identificatio on with the gro oup, strongerr collective acction tendenciies w were associated with strong ger beliefs abo out group effiicacy. On the other hand, thhere was no asssociation bettween group efficacy e and collective c actiion among paarticipants whho were highlyy iddentified with h the group. Inn the second study, s Van Zo omeran et al (2008) ( manipu ulated the sallience of partiicipants’ grouup veersus their personal identitty. They did th his by getting g participants to write abouut the typical day in thheir life as eith her a student (group identiity condition) or as an indivvidual (persoonal identity coondition). Aftter being rand domly assigneed to conditio on, participantts were told thhat universityy board haad plans to raaise the annuaal college feess paid by stud dents and that this increase would be deccided w without input from f studentss themselves. Group-based d anger, groupp efficacy, andd collective aaction teendencies werre then assesssed using simiilar measures as in Study 11. V Van Zomeran et al (2008) found fo that gro oup-based ang ger and collecctive action teendencies werre hiigher among participants p in n the group id dentity condittion than in thhe personal iddentity condittion. In teesting the hyp pothesis that group-based g anger a mediateed the relationnship betweenn identificatioon and coollective actio on tendencies, the results of o Study 1 weere replicated.. In other worrds, the relatioonship beetween group p identity and collective acttion tendenciees was signifi ficantly reduceed by the incllusion off group-based d anger, therefore indicatin ng that group--based anger ddid mediate thhe relationshiip beetween group p identity and collective acttion tendenciees. Paage 88 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® To test the hypothesis th hat group efficcacy moderatted the relatioonship betweeen group identtity and collective action a tendenccies, the analy yses from Stuudy 1 were repplicated. Oncce again, Van Zomeran et al. (2008) found f that gro oup-based ang ger predicted collective acttion. They alsso found that group efficacy preedicted collecctive action. However, H thiss was qualified by an identtity salience × group efficacy intteraction. Speecifically, amo ong those in tthe personal iidentity condiition, greater group efficacy waas associated with w more collective actionn tendencies. This associaation was not significant among partticipants in th he group identtity condition . Thus, the hyypothesis thatt group identiity moderates the relation nship between n group efficacy and collecctive action w was supported,, as it was in Study 1. The results of the two stu udies conducted by Van Z Zomeran and ccolleagues (20008) indicate that groupbased angerr is a significaant response to t a group thrreat or disadvvantage. Specifically, groupp-based anger was found to t mediate thee relationship p between idenntification annd collective aaction tendenncies. Furthermorre, results dem monstrated thaat individual’ s group identtity will impaact how that inndividual copes with group disadv vantages. Indiv viduals with a strong grouup identity willl feel a strong sense of group-based anger aboutt injustices, which w would llead to a greatter likelihoodd of collectivee action. Though this research did d not specificaally involve ggroup-based aanger towardss an outgroupp, it demonstrates the importance of group p-based angerr in relation too collective aaction. Both thhis research by Van Zom meran and colleagues (200 08) and the re search by Woohl and colleaagues (2010) demonstrate that group-b based emotions leads to id dentity managgement actionn strategies, suuch as collecttive action (orr social comp petition). How wever, only Wohl W et al. sugggests that in choosing a pparticular idenntity managemen nt strategy, su uch as social competition, c iimplies negattive intergrouup perceptionss. Another intteresting line of research lo ooks at expresssing an ambivalent attitudde toward onee’s own ingroup to manage m identtity threat. Pag gliaro and collleagues (20112) consideredd ambivalencce as a form of psychologiccal disengageement from th he group, whi ch is used to manage a neggative social iidentity. They y were also in nterested in th he use of this strategy in reesponse to grooup threat. Sppecifically, thhey predicted that when identity threatt is high (vs. low), l low stattus members w who are weakkly identified with the group woulld perceive th he ingroup as more heteroggeneous, and iin turn, expreess more ambiivalence toward it. Pagliaro P et al. also tested whether w perceiived ingroup variability mediates the efffect of identificatio on and identitty threat on am mbivalence tooward the inggroup. They reasoned that when individuals are asked to evaluate theirr own group uunder high iddentity threat, those with loow group identificatio on would psy ychologically disengage froom the group by stressing greater intraggroup variability, which leads to t a negative evaluation off the group. T This negative evaluation, however, would be co ontrasted agaainst their gen neral tendencyy to evaluate tthe ingroup ppositively. Thuus, holding both positiv ve and negativ ve evaluation ns would lead to an ambivaalent attitude ttoward the inngroup. In contrast, those with high h group identiification woulld emphasize intragroup hoomogeneity aand express more ingrou up favouritism m by evaluatiing the group positively. To test their hypotheses,, Pagliaro et al. a (2012) connducted a studdy using undeergraduate stuudents from a med dium-sized un niversity. Partticipants answ wered four item ms that were used to assesss their level of ing group identificcation (e.g., “Being “ Italiann is important to me”; Ellem mers, Pagliaroo, Barreto, & Leach, 2008; as cited in Pagliaro ett al.) and werre then presennted with the iidentity threaat manipulatio on. In past ressearch, Italian ns have been sshown to connsider themsellves as havingg higher status than Senegalese an nd lower statu us than Britissh (Mucchi-Faaina, Pacilli, P Pagliaro, & A Alparone, 2009, as citted in Pagliaro o et al.). To manipulate m ideentity threat, Pagliaro et all. asked particcipants to read fictitio ous data that compared c Itallians’ econom mic situation tto either Seneegalese or Brittish. In the high identitty threat cond dition, particip pants were tolld that Italianns were dramaatically disadvvantaged in terms of average salarries/unemploy yment compaared to Britishh. In the low iidentity threatt condition, participants p were w told the opposite o that IItalians were much better off comparedd to Senegalese. Next, participants answerred a manipullation check iitem regardinng the econom mic and Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 89 occcupational conditions of Italians I comp pared to Britissh/Senegalesee. Participantss also indicateed the exxtent to which h they considered Italians to t be differen nt from each oother in econoomic/occupational teerms to assesss perception of o intragroup variability. v Fiinally, particiipants indicateed their opiniion of Ittalians on 12 unipolar u item ms which consisted of six po ositive (e.g., aapproval) andd six negativee (e.g., diisapproval) items. Responsses to these ittems were useed to computee an ambivaleence score.15 Paagliaro et al. (2012) used multiple m regreession analysiis to test the eeffects of ingrroup identificcation and iddentity threat, and their inteeraction terms as predictorrs of perceptioon of intragrooup variabilityy and am mbivalence to oward the ing group. Resultss of a regressiion analysis reevealed that ffor perceptionn of inntragroup variiability, there was a signifiicant main efffect of ingrouup identificatiion, but not foor identity thhreat. As pred dicted, there was w a significaant interaction n between inggroup identifiication and iddentity thhreat. Simple slopes analyssis indicated that t low-identtifiers of the iingroup perceeived less hom mogeneity inn the high com mpared to the low identity threat conditiion. In contraast, high-identtifiers perceivved more hoomogeneity in n the high com mpared to thee low identity y threat condittion (see Figuure 13). predicting perception of Figure 13 3. Identifica ation × Identtity Threat interaction i 12, p. 44) ingroup variabiility (Pagliarro et al., 201 Foor ambivalence toward thee ingroup, ideentification waas a significannt predictor. A As anticipated, thhere was a sig gnificant interraction betweeen identificatiion and identtity threat. Sim mple slopes annalysis shhowed that low-identifiers reported morre ambivalencce toward thee ingroup in thhe high compared to thhe low identity y threat condition. In contrrast, high-iden ntifiers report rted less ambiivalence in the high coompared to th he low identity y threat condition (see Fig gure 14). Ambivalence sccores were calcuulated using the following formula: (P + N)/2 - |P – N|, where P = positive attitudee score annd N = negative attitude score. A constant of 1.55 was added to the scores to avooid negative respponses (see Thoompson, Zaanna, & Griffin, 1995). 1 15 Paage 90 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Figure 14 4. Identificattion × Identtity Threat in nteraction p predicting a ambivalenc ce toward the ingro oup (Pagliarro et al., 201 12, p. 44) Mediation analysis a tested whether perrception of inntragroup variiability mediaated the effectt of the identificatio on and identitty threat interraction on am mbivalence tow ward the ingrooup. As menttioned previously, the interactio on effect sign nificantly preddicted intragrooup variabilitty and ingroupp ambivalencce. Perception n of intragroup p variability aalso significaantly predictedd ingroup ambbivalence. When the in nteraction and d intragroup variability v weere entered ass predictors siimultaneouslyy, intragroup variability v rem mained a sign nificant predicctor of ambivvalence towarrd the ingroupp, but the interaction did not, providing supportt for mediatioon. The invertted model witth ambivalencce as the mediator off the interactio on on intragro oup variabilitty was tested and not signiificant. It appears th hen that when n social identity is threatenned, individuaals who are loow-identifierss with the ingroup psy ychologically y disengage from the ingrouup by exaggeerating their pperception of iintragroup variability, which leads to t more ambiivalent evaluaations of the iingroup (Paglliaro et al., 20012). Overall, thiis research shows that wheen individualss belong to a ddisadvantagedd group, the identity managemen nt strategy thaat they use to cope with id entity threat ((such as ambiivalence) can influence their overalll evaluations of the ingrou up. Pagliaro aand colleaguess suggest thatt expressing ambivalencce toward the ingroup can be b consideredd a social creaativity strateggy used to maanage identity threeat. So though h this researcch does not exxamine interggroup perceptiions, it does cconsider ambivalencce as an identiity managemeent strategy, w which can leaad weak identtifiers to view w their own ingroup as more m heterog geneous and hold h both posiitive and negaative perceptiions about thee ingroup as a conseq quence. Reseaarch showing how ambivallence toward one’s ingroupp impacts inteergroup perceptionss is needed. However, H it is appropriate tto say that thee kind of strattegy one invokes to manage ideentity threat in nfluences inteergroup perceeptions. Similarly, Jetten, J Schmittt, Branscomb be, and McKiimmie (2005)) examined w whether social creative strategies can help group p members su uppress the neegative effects of devaluatiion on group identificatio on. They exam mined two social creative sstrategies thaat may help inndividuals oveercome threats to th he worth of th heir ingroup: intergroup i diffferentiation aand intragrouup respect. Inttergroup differentiation involves highlighting h the t differencees between thhe ingroup andd the relevantt comparison n groups. Separating the in ngroup from oother groups eenables indiviiduals to makke intergroup as a opposed to o interpersonaal comparisonns and to estabblish a clear aand common identity with other ingroup i mem mbers. As such h, group mem mbership can pprotect individduals from thhe negative effects of in ngroup devalu uation threat by b emphasiziing the differeences betweenn their own ggroup and Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 91 otther groups. Research R has recently ackn nowledged thee importance of intragroupp factors, suchh as an inndividual’s staatus within th he group or th he amount of respect r an inddividual receiives from theiir inngroup memb bers (e.g., Bran nscombe, Speears, and Elleemers, 2002; JJetten, Spearss, & Mansteadd, 19997; both citeed in Jetten ett al.). Jeetten et al. (20 005) were parrticularly interested in intraagroup respecct, because thhey believe that w when the valuee of a group iss threatened, the respect th hat an individuual receives ffrom their inggroup m members can help h them man nage the threaat to their soccial identity. A According to Biernat, Vesccio, annd Green (199 96; cited in Jeetten et al.), in ntragroup resp pect is a sociaal creativity sstrategy becauuse it heelps individuaals focus on th he positive asspects of theirr group membbership to hellp them cope with vaalue threats to o their ingrou up. Moreover, past work haas shown thatt intragroup reespect can prootect grroup identificcation and inccrease loyalty to the group (e.g., Branscoombe et al., 22002, as citedd in Jeetten et al.). Jeetten and colleagues (2005) hypothesizeed that the soccial creative sstrategies wouuld counteracct the neegative effectts of a value threat. t In otheer words, valu ue threats of thhe group wouuld lead to inccreased peerceptions of intergroup diifferentiation and intragrou up respect, whhich should thhen lead to ann inncrease in gro oup identificattion. The hyp potheses suggeest that valuee threats to a ggroup can havve a diirect negativee effect on gro oup identificaation, but that social creativve strategies ccan have a poositive efffect, and as such s the strateegies suppress the negativee effects. Thiss pattern of fiindings is connsistent w with a suppressor model, wh hich is a speccial case of mediation (Barron & Kenny,, 1996; cited iin Jeetten et al.). To test their hy ypotheses, Jettten et al. (200 05) provided participants w with false feeedback about hhow otther groups perceived theirr residential state s (Queenslland, in Austrralia). To mannipulate valuee thhreat, participants were told d that Australlians in other states generaally viewed Q Queensland as either neegative (high value threat condition) c or positive (low w value threat condition). T Then, effectivveness off the manipulation (e.g., “O Overall, otherrs consider Qu ueenslanders as good”; adapted from puublic seelf-esteem scaale, Luhtanen n & Crocker, 1992; 1 cited in n Jetten et al.)) intergroup ddifferentiationn (e.g., “T There are man ny differences between Qu ueensland and d other states””), intragroupp respect (e.g., “In geeneral, I feel valued v by oth hers in my gro oup”; adapted d from Deauxx, Reid, Mizraahi, & Cottingg, 19999; cited in Jetten J et al.), and group ideentification (ee.g., “I identiffy with other Queenlanderrs”; addapted from Jetten, J Bransccombe, Schmiitt, & Spears, 2001; cited iin Jetten et al..) were assesssed. A Analyses show wed that the vaalue threat maanipulation had no effect oon group idenntification, buut did afffect intergrou up differentiaation and intraagroup respecct. Specificallly, those in thhe high value tthreat coondition show wed more inteergroup differrentiation and d more intragrroup respect ((this latter efffect w was only marg ginal) than did d those in the low value thrreat conditionn. Mediation aanalyses using reegression show wed that theree was no effeect of value th hreat on groupp identificatioon. In additionn, reelation betweeen value threaat and identifi fication contro olling for the two social creeative strateggies, shhowed that th hreat to the value of a group p led to increaased percepti ons of intergrroup differenttiation annd intragroup p respect, whicch in turn, enhanced group p identificatioon. Also, by aadding the twoo social crreative strateg gies as predictors, value threat negativelly predicted ggroup identifiication. Takenn toogether, Jetten n et al.’s (200 05) findings providing supp port for the reesearchers’ suuppressor moddel (ssee Figure 15)). Paage 92 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 1 Suppres ssor model testing rela ationships b between the e perceived d group Figure 15. devalu uation manipulation, grroup identiffication, inttergroup diffferentiation n, and intragroup res spect. 16 (J etten et al., 2005, p. 21 12) Jetten et al.’s (2005) find dings demonsstrate that botth intergroup differentiatioon and intragroup respect serv ve an identity y-enhancing management m sstrategies wheen individualss’ ingroup is tthreatened. Furthermorre, it is possib ble that individ duals who doo not engage iin these social creative straategies may distancce themselvess from the ing group under vvalue threat, aand may turn to more direcct strategies, such s as indiviidual mobility y, to manage ttheir identity threat. Furtheer research shhould examine if higher identiffication with the ingroup aas a result of tthese manageement strategiies leads to negative inttergroup percceptions and evaluations. e Roberts, Seettles, and Jelllison (2008) examined e the factors that iinfluence the strategies inddividuals use to manaage a devalueed social identtity, includingg response to recent discrim mination. Thiis research focused on two particulaar types of strrategies that inndividuals caan use to manaage their negaative social identtity. First, the disadvantageed group mem mbers can usee social recateegorization strrategies in which indiv viduals try to avoid being categorized c innto the devaluued group, buut instead beinng categorized d into an altern native group that is more ffavourable as individuals ooften belong tto multiple social group ps and have multiple m identtities (e.g., Doovidio, Kawaakami, & Gaerrtner, 2000; ccited in Roberts et al.). a This can involve activ vely suppressiing the devaluued category (e.g., avoidinng behaviours that make thee devalued caategory more salient) and ttrying to seem m more similaar to the members off the favourab ble group. Seccond, individduals can use ppositive distinnctiveness strrategies in which grou up members trry to foster a more m positivee perception oof the devalueed group by ppublicly acknowledg ging their mem mbership in the t group, infforming otherrs about the faavourable quaalities of the group, or o bolstering the t group (e.g g., Ellemers eet al., 2002; M Major, Quintoon, McCoy, & Schmader, 2000; cited in n Roberts et al.). a a (2008) preedicted that in ndividuals woould use sociaal recategorizaation and posiitive Roberts et al. distinctiven ness if they peerceive that th heir social grooup is devalueed because booth types of sstrategies aim to conttrol others’ im mpressions about the devaluued group. H However, theyy may be moree inclined to use one over o the otherr depending on o the contextt. The choice of strategy m may also depennd on how important th he social iden ntity to a grou up member’s self-concept ((e.g., Ashmorre, Deaux, & McLaughlin n-Volpe, 200 04; cited in Ro oberts et al.) bbecause, in cllaiming the deevalued identtity, individuals are permitted d to behave in n a way that iss consistent w with their percceptions abouut themselves. It is probablly that those who w attach moore centralityy to their devaalued social iddentity 16 Path weightts are standardizzed. *p < .05, **pp < .01, ***p < .0001, one-tailed. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 93 m may be more liikely to use positive p distin nctiveness and d less likely too use social reecategorizatioon. On thhe other hand,, individuals who w attach more m centrality y to an alternaate social idenntity that is viiewed m more favourab bly may be mo ore likely to use u social recaategorization to downplay their membeership w with the devalu ued group. R Roberts and co olleagues (200 08) tested their hypothesess across two qquestionnaire studies. In Sttudy 1, thhey examined d how women in a science--related field strategically s m managed concerns relatingg to Roberts et al..), women aree often thheir gender id dentity. Accorrding to Barbeercheck (2001 1; as cited in R faaced with a co ontradiction between b what is expected of o them as woomen (e.g., nuurturing and em motional) and d as a scientist (e.g., being rational and objective),.Th o hese conflicting expectatioons m may lead to geender discrimiination from male m colleagu ues and, as suuch, lead to thhe perception that thhey are less vaalued than theeir male coun nterparts in science-related domains (Keeller, 2001; ciited in R Roberts et al.). W Women who were w studying or working in a science-reelated field w were recruited for Roberts eet al.’s (22008) study. First, F they asssessed how offten participan nts engaged inn the two typpes of strategiees. Foour items ask ked about the extent to whiich they used social recateggorization as a strategy to m manage their gender g identity y concerns by y decreasing the t salience oof their gender or increasinng the saalience of oth her identities. Positive distinctiveness waas also assesssed on four iteems asking abbout thhe frequency with w which th hey tried to in ncrease otherss’ positive perrceptions of w women by eduucating otthers about th heir gender orr by acting as an exemplar of the group. Both of thes e measures w were addapted from Morgan’s M (2002; cited in Roberts R et al.) Positive Disttinctiveness ssubscale in thee Soocial Identity y-Based Impreession Manag gement Scale. R Roberts et al. (2008) ( then measured m the predictors p of the t strategies . Participants provided deemographic in nformation, in ncluding racee (woman of colour c vs. whhite woman) aand professionnal sttatus (worker vs. student). Next, particip pants’ recent experiences oof gender disccrimination w were asssessed by ind dicating the frequency fr of those experien nces and how w bothersome they felt (adaapted frrom Daily Racist Hassles Scale, S Harrell, 1994, as citeed in Robertss et al.). For exxample, they were assked whether any classmattes (or colleag gues) had receently insultedd them or advvised them to switch too another disccipline becausse of their gen nder. Then, geender centraliity and scienttist centrality were ments asssessed by ask king participaants to indicatte their agreem ment with a sseries of statem coorresponding to being a wo oman (e.g., “B Being a woman is an impoortant reflectioon of who I aam”) annd being a sciientist respecttively. Both centrality c meaasures were addapted from tthe Centralityy suubscale of thee Multidimenssional Invento ory of Black Identity I (Sell ers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, 1997 7; cited in Rob berts et al.). U Using regressio on analyses, Roberts R et al. (2008) found d that the use of both sociaal recategorizaation annd positive diistinctiveness were associaated with morre recent expeeriences of geender discrimiination annd when partiicipants were more bothereed by those ex xperiences. P Participants w who reported thheir geender as being more imporrtant to their self-concept s compared c to bbeing a scienttist were som mewhat leess likely to use social recaategorization (marginally ( significant) s annd more likelyy to use posittive diistinctivenesss. In contrast, the more partticipants iden ntified as a sciientist the moore likely theyy soocially recateg gorized (marg ginally signifi ficant). The fin ndings from S Study 1 mostly supported the R Roberts and co olleagues’ hyp potheses. How wever, they did d not find a ssignificant relation betweeen geender centraliity and social recategorizattion, and the relation r betw ween scientist centrality andd social reecategorizatio on did not reacch significancce. Inn a second stu udy, Roberts et e al. (2008) largely replicaated the findinngs from Studdy 1 by exam mining B Black medical students and their experiences of being g negatively stereotyped (i..e., being lesss quualified for medical m schooll than non-Black students)). In general, tthey found thhat participantts were Paage 94 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® more likely y to use sociall recategorizaation if they pllaced less impportance on rracial centrality and more imporrtance on theiir professionaal identity. Peerceptions of rracial stereotyyping, howevver, did not predict the use of social recategorizattion. Participaants were morre likely to usse positive distinctiven ness the more they perceiveed themselve s to be negatiively stereotyyped and the m more they valued theirr race. The more m participan nts used posittive distinctivveness, the lesss depressed tthey reported, th he less likely they t were to consider c quittting medical sschool, and thhe more comm mitted they were to their mediccal career. Theese outcomess were not obsserved for soccial recategorrization. gs from Roberrts and colleaagues (2008) sstudy suggestt that women and Black m medical The finding students aree unlikely to be b passive tarrgets of discriimination andd stereotypingg. Those indivviduals who belong g to a “devalu ued” social gro oup do, howeever, seek to cchange otherss’ perceptionss of them by correctin ng inaccurate perceptions. Intergroup peerceptions can an be altered, ttherefore, usiing identity maanagement strategies like so ocial recategoorization and positive distiinctiveness. Table 13 su ummarizes thee research exp plored in this chapter, inveestigating the influence of identity managemen nt strategies on o intergroup perceptions. Indeed, the reesearch abovee suggests thaat strategies can shape intergroup percep ptions. Table 13. Summary of o Research h Assessing g the Relatio onship Betw ween Socia al Identity Management Sttrategies an nd Intergrou up Perceptions Reference Outco ome Identity Management Strategy Gonzalez & Broown (2005) Ͳ In contact c situations, high status and m minority group mem mbers are more biassed, but the use of o identity manageement strategies lim mit this bias. Ͳ Outtside of contact sittuations, dual idenntity categorizationn reduces bias forr minnority group membbers only. Decategorizzation, recategorizaation, dual identity (Indiividual mobility) Schmitt & Maess (2002) Ͳ Ingroup bias by mem mbers of a low stattus group is related to decreases in perrceptions of relativve deprivation ove r time. Ͳ Ingroup bias moderates the impact of rrelative deprivatioon on mental health succh that when indiviiduals exhibited inngroup bias relativve deprivation does not impact mental heealth whereas wheen individuals do nnot exhibit ingroupp biass, greater feelingss of relative deprivvation are associatted with poorer mental health. Ingroup biass (Social creativity) Caricati & Monaacelli (2010) Ͳ Ingroup identificationn is associated withh the ability to maake downward soccial comparisons. Ͳ Inteermediate status group g members arre more biased aggainst high status grooups than low statuus groups when reesources are unlim mited, but equally biassed against both when w resources a re limited. Negative ouutgroup bias (Social competition) Wohl, Branscom mbe, & Reysen (2010) Ͳ Ingroups experience more angst and ddisplay more ingrooup strengthening behhaviours when thee potential extinctioon of the group orr threats to disttinctiveness are made m salient. Ͳ Thrreatening a groupss existence may leead to strong proteective activities that may include harmingg the outgroup to m maintain the ingrouup’s disttinctiveness. Ingroup streengthening behaviours ((Social competition)) Van Zomeren, Spears, S & Leach (2008)) Ͳ Stroongly identified group members aree likely to feel grouup-based anger at injuustice and engage in collective actioon whereas weaklyy identified group members will only enngage in collectivee action if they fee l that the group caan actually do something about the injustiice (i.e., group effiicacy). Collective acction (Social competition)) Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 95 Paagliaro, Alparone, Paacilli, & Mucchi-Faaina (2012) mbivalence Ͳ Members of a low status group are more likkely to express am towardss their ingroup in high h social identity threat situations w when they are weakly identified with the ingroup. Ͳ Members of a low status group who are strrongly identified wiith their ingroup express less ambbivalence towards their ingroup in hiigh social identity threat situations. e are mediateed by intragroup variability v (i.e., in hhigh threat Ͳ These effects situationns, weak identifierrs see more ingrouup dissimilarity leaading to greater ambivalence). Ambivalence tow wards ingroup (Individuual mobility) Jeetten, Schmitt, Brranscombe, & MccKimmie (2005) Ͳ Threats to the value of ann ingroup increasee the use of social creative strategiees such as intergrroup differentiationn and intragroup reespect and these buuffer against decreeased ingroup ideentification in respoonse to threat. Intergroup differeentiation, intragroup respect (Social creativity) Rooberts, Settles, & Jeellison (2008) Ͳ Members of a devalued group g use both soccial categorizationn and positive mbership to this grroup is important tto their selfdistinctivveness, but if mem conceptt, they are more likkely to use positivee distinctiveness. Ͳ Positivee distinctiveness was w associated with positive outcom mes, unlike social reecategorization. Social recategorization (Individual mobility) 6 6.1 Perceptions of inngroup variability (Social creativity) Positive distinctivveness (Social creativityy) Summary The research has h indicated that t the use of individual mobility m strateegies, such ass employing a dual iddentity, reduces the likeliho ood of negativ ve outgroup perceptions p am mong low staatus groups. T The use off identity man nagement straategies (e.g., ingroup i bias) by low statuss groups, reduuces their feelings off relative deprrivation and increases i their mental heallth over time. Furthermore, the emphasiis on poositive distincctiveness is more m likely to be associated d with positivve outcomes eespecially wheen grroup identity is important to t the individ dual. Inn situations where w group id dentity is threatened, strong g ingroup ideentification results in more anger annd collective action whereaas weaker ing group identification resultss in more ambbivalence tow wards thhe ingroup and a greater lik kelihood of em mphasizing dissimilarities d with the ingrroup. Howeveer, if soocial creative strategies, su uch as intergro oup differentiiation and intrragroup respeect, are used, these thhreats can bufffer against deecreased ingro oup identificaation and leadd to more positive outcomees. Paage 96 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 7. Fe eedback Loo op – Ho ow do Identity Manage M ement Strategies Im mpact Identitty an nd Gro oup Sta atus R Relation ns (S Socios structural Beliefs)? ? (4.7.6 6) The previou us chapters fo ocused on wh hether the sociiostructural vvariables affeccted the use of identity managemen nt strategies and a intergroup p perceptionss, and whetherr identity mannagement straategies affected inttergroup perceeptions. In co ontrast, this chhapter focuses on the feedbback loop – thhat is, how identity maanagement strategies influeence individuaals’ social ideentity and theeir perceptionss of status differences. The literaturre search reveealed very litttle empirical eevidence thatt directly addrressed this feedback lo oop. Thereforee, in the sectiion below, ressearch findinggs that might provide insigght into the feedback lo oop are discusssed. There is som me indirect ev vidence from m Boen et al. (22006) indicatting that identtity managem ment strategies can affect sociial identity. Their T study sh owed that whhen employeees have an oppportunity to move fro om a lower-status to a high her-status orgaanization duee to a merger, they may psychologiccally disengag ge from their old organizat ation the higheer in status thhey view the nnew, postmerger orgaanization and d the more theey tend to idenntify with this new organizzation. Althouugh Boen et al. only speculated s thaat individuals distanced theemselves from m the old orgaanization, this suggests that individ duals select strrategies that enable e them tto not only im mprove their ssocial status, bbut that may also make m it easier for f them to id dentify with thheir new sociial group. Unffortunately, thhey did not assess group g relation ns following the t merger. Other work k suggests thaat identity man nagement straategies may nnot be effectivve at all for m managing negative ideentity concern ns, at least in the field (Keessler and Muummendey, 20002). The finddings from Kessler and d Mummendeey’s longitudin nal study on tthe unificatioon of East andd West Germaany show that there arre correlation ns between thee strategies annd sociostrucctural variablees, but there iss little evidence fo or the causal influence i of id dentity managgement, with the exceptionn of social recategorization. This strrategy tended d to foster perrceptions of im mproved mateerial status foor the lower-statu us group (Eastt Germans), although a theree were no longg- term positiive effects forr using this strategy y. Kessler and d Mummendeey challenge tthe assumptioon of a linear and sequentiaal process for variablees within the social s identity y model in thaat individualss do not necesssarily experience a sequence off processes when w dealing with w a negativve social idenntity. Instead, they believe that individuals may have a configuration c of beliefs thaat try to balannce out their ssociostructuraal beliefs, identity maanagement preeferences agaainst identificaation and percceptions of thhreat or opporrtunity. In sum, emp pirical research is necessarry to understaand the impacct of identity m management strategies on identity and group staatus relations and how thesse strategies w work with thee other variables in the social identtity model. Ho owever, at present, there iss very little loooking specifi fically at this ffeedback loop. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 97 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 98 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 8. Synops sis (les ss than n 3,000 0 words s–5 pa ages) of Metthodolo ogies Used tto Manipu M late an nd Mea asure S Socioc cultura al Beliefs B and Id dentity Manag gemen nt Strategiies 8.1 Me easures of o sociostrructural va ariables Variable Methhod Scale Studies using ssimilar scales Stability mmendey et al. (1999); ( Kessler Mum and Mummendey (22002) 1. I think the relationship between East E and West Germany G will remain stable forr the next years. T current relationship betweenn 2. The East E and West Germany G will not change c easily. 3. The T current relationship betweenn East E and West Germany G is just teemporary (R) 1 = do noot agree at all/ completelly disagree, 5 = agreee very much/com mpletely agree Verkuyten & Reeijerse, 2008 Verkuyten, 20005 (Temporal) Sttability m from McAuley, Duncan, & Item Russels (1992) caussal dimension scale: Is thhe cause permannent or temporarry? 1 = permaanent, 6 = tempoorary Costarelli, 20122 mmendey et al. (1999), ( Kessler Mum and Mummendey (22002) Legitimacy Humansystem ms® T West Germaans are entitled tto 1. The be b better off thann the East Germans G (R). 2. Itt is justified that the West Germaans are a currently doinng better than thhe East E Germans (R R). 3. We W East Germanns can demand tto be b as well off as the West Germans. G 1 = do noot agree at all/ completelly disagree, 5 = agreee very much/com mpletely agree Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 99 Vaariable Method Leegitimacy Costarelli (2012) and Scchmader et al., (2001) r / 1. I think it is justified / right mate that Italian high school legitim studeents are consideered worse in their past sports perfformance than Frencch ones 2. Do yoou believe it is accurate a or inacccurate that Stanfford is really superior to UCSB? Leegitimacy Dumont and van Lill (2009) 1. Curreent economic diffferences betweeen ingroup andd outgroup peopple are just. 2. It is juustified, that ingrroup people are doing d better in ecconomic matteers than outgrouup people. Peermeability Peermeability Paage 100 Mummeendey et al. (1999); Kessler and Mum mmendey (2002) 1. In principle, it is not difficult d for an East German to be considered c as a Westt German. 2. For an a East German it is nearly impossible to be regaarded as a Westt German (R). 3. No matter m what effortt one makes, an Eaast German will never become a Weest German (R). Scale For question 11: 1 = not at all, 6 = very muchh For question 22: 1 = strongly ddisagree, 7 = strongly aagree Stuudies using similaar scaales Cosstarelli, 2012; Schhmader et al., 20001; Weeber et al., 2002 No informatio n about scale providedd. 1 = do not agrree at all/ completely dissagree, 5 = agree very ry much/complettely agree Verrkuyten, 2005 von Hipppel (2006) 1. I am working as a tem mporary employee in hope of finding a manent position. perm 2. I think I have a good chance of gettinng a permanent job j through tempporary assignmennt. Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 8.2 Ma anipulatio ons of the sociostru uctural va ariables Variable Manipulation Example Legitimacy Research basis for status differences Legitimacy Fictitious research articlle comparing o performance of ingroup and outgroup Legitimacy Procedural fairness Costarelli (2012, Study 2) : Legitimacyy of low ingroup status was mannipulated by tellinng participantts that postseco ndary academicc achievement foor their group mem mbers tended too be worse than outgroup membbers, and that thhis information w was either basedd on 15 years of research from f 10 national universities (higgh) or 3 years off research from f 3 national uuniversities (low)). Schmaderr et al. (2001, Sttudy 2): Participannts were random ly assigned to a high- or low staatus group by receiving r false feeedback on a peersonality test. B Before participantts found out how w their group scoored on the perssonality test, they were w given a daata sheet describbing how their grroup differed froom the comparisson group. Attacched to the data sheet was also a fictitious reseaarch article that eelaborated on the details meentioned in the ddata sheet. In thee illegitimacy conndition, the researrch suggested thhat students at thhe two schools aare equivalentt in both their accademic and inteelligence potentiaal, whereas inn the legitimacy condition, studeents at the higheer status schhool have better academic and inntelligence potenntial than thosee at the lower staatus school. Boen and Vanbeselaere ((2002): Students were w told that th eir performancee on a test wouldd be comparedd to those from aanother class, annd depending onn the results of this t test, the bettter performing cclass would be selected too judge a task thhat was in the prrocess of being developedd by a professor and that the tassk is also a pleassant one. Studeents were alwayys told that their class was outperform med by the outgrroup. In the legittimate condition,, participantts were told thatt the rival class hhad followed thee same testing proocedures as theyy did (e.g., givenn same amount of time and students did the test iindependently). The experimentter also stressed that the other claass had attained its higher scoree in a fair way. Inn the illegitimatee condition, participants were told that during thee testing session of the rival grouup, the test administraators had to leavve the room for a few minutes annd when theyy returned, somee of the studentss were working together on o the test, and ttheir collaboratioons were stopped immediateely. The experim menter told the sttudents that the outgroup had h attained a hhigher average uunfairly, but that s/he was not inn the position to ddo anything aboout the situation. Studiees using similar methoodology Ouwerkerk and Ellemers ((2002) Participannts were told thatt they would worrk as a group onn a Group Woord Recognition TTask and that thheir performancee as a group wouuld be comparedd to other groupss. The task was more or less diffficult depending on the number of letters in the anagram. Participants we re told that the ccomputer would randomly assign twenty a nagrams to eachh group. The meean Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 101 Vaariable Maanipulation Leegitimacy Taask leader asssignment Sttability Paast peerformance as baasis for stability of status diffferences Example difficulty of thee anagrams that the participant’ss group completeed was always 6.5. Upon compleetion of the anag rams, participannts were informedd of the performaance of their gro up (always 68 points) and thee performance of o the outgroup. In the low statuss condition participants were told that the other group had scoreed 7 points higheer (i.e., 75 points) whereas in thee high status condition, theyy were told that the t outgroup hadd scored 7 pointts lower (i.e., 61 points). Legitimaacy was manipuulated by alteringg the supposed difficulty of the task t completed bby the outgroup.. In the legitimate condition, particcipants were toldd that the averagge difficulty of thee outgroup’s taskk was 7.1 whereeas in the illegitimate conndition, the difficculty was said to be 5.9. Burke et al. (22007): In a group of four f made up of two males and ttwo females, thee experimenter randomly assignned one person ssitting at the endd of the table to be the moderatoor of a four discuussion questionss, half of them peertained to malee issues and halff of them pertainned to female issues.of the table too be the moderaator of a four discussion queestions, half of thhem pertained too male issues annd half of them peertained to femaale issues. Scheepers (20009): Using a within-subjects designn, participants w were randomly w status group bby receiving falsse assigned to bee in a high- or low feedback on a reasoning test. Then they comppleted three taskks that would be allegedly used to t measure theirr problem solvingg t first two taskks being similar aand the third task abilities, with the being differentt in nature. Afterr the first task, paarticipants were told that they would w be compleeting a second taask that is very similar to the first f task. As succh, the first task w would be a goodd indicator of hoow they would peerform on the seecond task. This suggests that differences in sttatus would be sstable during thee P thenn moved on to thhe third task and second task. Participants were told that their performancce on the previoous two tasks would not be a good predictorr of how they woould do on this task. This information was inteended to give paarticipants the perception thaat group differencces in status woould be unstable during this finaal task. Studies ussing similar methodoloogy Scheeperss, Ellemers, and Sintemaarrtensdijk (2009) Halabi Nadler & H (2006) Boen and Vannbeselaere (20022): In the stable condition, c studennts were told thatt their class had scored a 5 outt of 10 whereas another class haad scored an 8 oout of 10 on a testt. The experimennted explained too the students thhat this was a big difference between the two classses, and that it is very likely thatt the classes would attain simila r scores if they were to do thee test again the next n day. The exxperimenter labelled this difference as “uncchangeable” andd wrote the wordd on the blackbooard. In the unsttable condition, sstudents were toold that their classs scored a 7 out of 10, whereas the other class scored an 8 ouut of 10 on the teest. The experim menter told the students that this t difference was w small and if tthe classes weree Paage 102 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Variable Stability Manipulation Degree of fluctuation or change in passt performance of o groups Stability Degree of change compared withh other groups Stability Emphasizing traditional versus changing roles. Humansystem ms® Example to do the test t again the neext day, it is posssible that their class would scoore higher than thhe outgroup. The experimenter labelled thhis difference as “changeable” and wrote the woord on the blackbboard. Scheeperss, Spears, Doossje, and Mansteaad (2006): Participannts were random ly assigned to bbe in a high- or loow status group using false feeedback on an eestimation task w which showed thhat their ingroup either performeed better or worse than the outgrooup respectively.. Perceived stabbility of group staatus was manippulated by tellingg participants thaat group scores either fluctuated a lot (unstable ccondition) or flucctuated a little (sstable condition) in previous sesssions. Studiees using similar methoodology Nadleer and Halabbi (2006); Nadleer, HarpazGoroddeisky, & Ben-D David (2009) a (2002): Major et al. In the stabble condition, paarticipants were ttold that comparrative analyses over o the past 5 yyears showed thhat, in general, thheir school hadd performed worrse than the prestigious school oon numerouss criteria (e.g., enntry into selectivve university programs)). In the unstablee condition, participants were told that comparative analyses oveer the past 5 yeaars showed that the gap betweeen their school and the more prrestigious schoool was becoming narrower. A Trifil etti, and Visintinn (2012) Vezzali, Andrighetto, In the stattus instability conndition, Italian paarticipants read an article (adapted from The New York Timees and made to aappear in the mosst popular Italiann newspaper) abbout the U.S. decclining in the economic domain, bbut also stated thhat several natioons me domain, especially Italy. In thhe were on thhe rise in the sam control condition, participaants read an article unrelated to the relation beetween the U.S. and Italy and discussed how daaily physical activity a has healt h benefits. Scheeperss, Ellemers, andd Sintemaartensddijk (2009) In an exam mination of gendder relations, parrticipants were aasked to debate about three topiics. They first staarted with the neeutral gender deebate topic, “Harrd drugs should bbe legalized, andd be sold by phharmacies,” and participants werre randomly assigned to take thee opposing or deefending positionn. For the conservative topic, the goal was to makke traditional gennder roles saliennt so w perceive ttheir status to bee stable. The toppic was that men would “After a chhild is born the m mother (rather than the father) shhould stay at home to care for it .” The progressive topic was designed would to make changing genderr relations salientt, so that men w feel threattened due to thee instability of theeir status. The toopic was “Moree subsidies for dday care centers should becomee available so s that women ccan stay workingg when they havee a child.” Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 103 Studies ussing similar methodoloogy Vaariable Maanipulation Example Peermeability Eaase with which meembers of a low w status group caan be part of higgh status grooup. Blair and Jost (2003): In the permeable condition, paarticipants were told that they caan join the managgement group (hhigher-status grooup) if they earneed 15 out of a totaal of 35 on a sett of problems, whhereas in the impermeable condition, c they had h to earn all 355 points to join thhe management group. g Peermeability Willingness of higgher status grooup to accept meembers of low wer status grooup Boen and Vannbeselaere (20022): In the permeable condition, low w status studentts were told thatt the high statuss students wouldd be willing to acccept students from their grouup if they did well on a retest of tthe initial test thaat was used to determine the staatus of the two c lasses. The w on the boaard that the otheer class was experimenter wrote “open” for thosse who scored high h on the retesst. In the impermeable condition, c low sttatus students w were told that thee high status claass would not bee willing to acceppt any members from their classs regardless of their t performancce on the retest. The experimenter wrote on thee board that the other class wass “closed” for thoose who scored high on the reteest. (Participantss performance on o the test was randomly r assignned to be good vvs. weak.) 8.3 Meas sures of Social S Iden ntity Mana agement S Strategies s Vaariable Individual Mobility Individual Mobility Paage 104 Measure Mummendey et al. (1999); Keessler and Mummendey (2002) W 1. I make anyy effort to be connsidered as a West German. 2. It is my very wish to belong to the West Germans. 3. I try to livee as a West Germ man rather than as an East Germ man. Major et al. (22002) 1. America iss an open societty where individuuals of any ethniccity can achieve higher status 2. American society is possibble for individuals of all ethnic grooups 3. Individual members of a loow status ethnic group h status (R) have difficculty achieving higher 4. Individual members of cerrtain ethnic groupps are often unabble to advance inn American society (R) Scaale Studies 1 = do not agree at all/ com mpletely disagreee, 5 = agree very mucch/completely agreee 1 = strongly disagreee, 7 = strongly agree Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Variable Measure Scale Social Competition Mummenndey et al. (1999); Kessler and M Mummendey (2002) W Germans th at we are the 1. We will show to the West e Germanns. more efficient 2. It is ouur goal not to be taught by the W West Germaans, but to teachh them ourselvess. 3. We froom the “new fedderal states” will vvery soon show more initiative and a commitmentt than the West Germans. Dumont and a van Lill (20009) 1. Ingrouup people shouldd demonstrate thhat they are the more successful group g in terms off economic statuss. 2. I wantt ingroup peoplee to demonstratee that they are the suuperior group in terms t of econom mic status. Niens & Cairns C (2002) 1. I wantt my community to demonstrate that it is the superrior one. 2. I wantt my community to demonstrate that it is culturaally superior. 3. My coommunity will very soon show m ore initiative than the other commuunity. 4. I wantt my community to demonstrate that it is the betterr one. Caricati and a Monacelli (2010): To manippulation social coompetition, particcipants were asked to complete a mem mory task in whicch resources l and the fformer were limitted or were not limited, condition was expected too elicit greater ssocial competitioon (e.g., Brewerr, 1986, cited in C Caricati & Monacelli, 2010). Specificcally, in the limiteed resource condition, participants weere asked to alloocate 90s of a mem mbers from extra timee among three anonymous each of thhe three thinkingg styles groups, w whereas those in the t not limited reesource conditionn, were able to give upp to 90s to each of the three mem mbers. The extra timee would be usedd for a subsequeent memory task. 1 = do not agreee at all/ completely disaagree, 5 = agree very much/completeely agree Social Competition Social Competition Social Competition Manipulation Humansystem ms® Studies No information about scale provided. 1 = disagree strrongly, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither, 4 = agree, 5 = agree stronngly Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 105 Vaariable Measure Scaale Reealistic Coompetition Mummendey et al. (1999); Keessler and Mummendey (2002) 1. If new jobss arise in the nexxt five years, wee East Germans will w have to makke sure that these jobs will be estaablished in East Germany rather than in West Germ many. 2. By now, enough has been n invested in West Germany. We East Germaans have to fightt for future inveestments being exclusively e madee in East Germany. 3. In the nexxt five years, we, the East Germaans, will speak up in favor of the diversion of the raare training allowances and grants from Westt to East Germany.. Mummendey et al. (1999); Keessler and Mummendey (2002) Difference beetween items (1-22): 1. I regard myself m as a Germ man. 2. I regard myself m as an Eastt German. Social Recateegorization subsscale (adapted frrom Morgan, 20022) Four items weere reworded to assess strategiees that actively suppress the saliencee of a group (e.gg., avoiding discussions of gendeer) Positive Distinnctiveness subscale (adapted frrom Morgan, 20022) Five items weere reworded to assess strategiees that increase the positive p perceptiion held by others regarding thee participants soccial group (e.g., educating othhers about womeen) Mummendey et al. (1999); Keessler and Mummendey (2002) Difference beetween the itemss indicate the East Germans (low wer-status groupp) devaluation off the material dimeension (2-1): 1. The East Germans considder economic weealth as: undesirabble-desirable. 2. The West Germans consider economic wealth w as: undesirabble-desirable. Mummendey et al. (1999); Keessler and Mummendey (2002) Difference beetween items (1-22): 1. Accordingg to your opinion, how important is it for the East Germans G to compare themselvess with the situation before the unnification? 2. Accordingg to your opinion, how important is it for the East Germans G to compare themselvess with West Germ mans? 1 = do not agree at all/ mpletely disagreee, com 5 = agree very mucch/completely agreee Soocial Reecategorization Soocial Reecategorization Poositive Diistinctiveness Ree-evaluation of Material Diimension Prreference for Teemporal Coomparison Paage 106 Studies 1 = do not agree at all/ com mpletely disagreee, 5 = agree very mucch/completely agreee 0 = not at all, 5 = a great deal 0 = not at all, 5 = a great deal 1 = do not agree at all/ com mpletely disagreee, 5 = agree very mucch/completely agreee 1 = do not agree at all/ com mpletely disagreee, 5 = agree very mucch/completely agreee Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Variable Measure Scale (De)valuation of Dimensions Adapted from f Valuing Suubscale in Major & Schmadeer’s (1998) Intelleectual Engagem ment Inventory to assesss individuals’ perrsonal value of a novel personality trait (i.e., surggency), used in S Schmader et al. (2001)): 1. Being high in surgenccy is very importaant to me. 2. I care a great deal aboout being high inn surgency. 3. It doesn’t matter to mee one way or thee other if I am o low in surgenccy (R). high or Dumont and a van Lill (20009) 1. If new w jobs arise in thee next few yearss, ingroup peoplee will make suree that these jobs will be filled with outgroup o people rather than with ingroup peoplee. 2. Southh Africa has long been invested iin Black Econoomic Empowerm ment. Ingroup pe ople will fight for outgroup people coontinuing this invvestment also inn the future. 1 = strongly dissagree, 7 = strongly agrree Ingroup/outgrooup favouritism Nadler annd Halabi (2006)) Allocationn of funds to ingrroup and outgro up. Prejudice: Behavioural intentions Participants givven 7 possible allocattion choices, three oof which represented inggroup favouritism (moore resources alloccated to ingroup than outgroup), one of which represennted equal allocationns for the ingroup andd outgroup, and tthree of which represennted outgroup favouritism (more resourcees allocated to outtgroup than ingroup) 1 = definitely yees 4 = neither/it deepends 7 = definitely noo Jackson (2002) ( Participannts rated how likkely they would bbe to accept an outgrooup member as their t child’s playymate, good friend, meember of churchh or club, work suupervisor, United Sttates president, date, d coworker, child’s teacher, family f physician, state governor,, sibling’s spouse, and a own spouse. Jackson (2002) ( (adapted from Byrnes annd Kiger, 1988) 7-point semantic scale Participannts were asked to t imagine a sceenario where they are at a a party and soomeone tells a dderogatory joke abouut the outgroup and a many peoplee laugh. They werre asked to indiccate how they woould feel in this situattion on five semaantic differential scales: resentmeent-acceptance, funny-unamusin f ng, trivialserious, cruel-kind, c and smart-stupid. s Outgroup favouritism Prejudice: Affective dimension Humansystem ms® Studies No information about scale provided. Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 107 Vaariable Measure Scaale Prrejudice: Coognitive dimension Jackson (20002) (adapted from m Byrnes and Kiger, 1988) w asked to im magine a scenario where Participants were they are at a party and someoone comments that t the mbers are causinng many of todaay’s outgroup mem social problem ms. They were thhen asked to inddicate how they wouuld feel about this statement on five f bipolar scaless: agreeable-oppposed, unfair-fair, knowledgeabble-ignorant, closse minded-open minded, and sympatheetic –unsympathhetic. Jackson (20002) (adapted from m Katz and Hasss, 1988) Six items, for example: 1. Most [outggroup members] have the drive and a determination to get aheadd (reverse scoreed). 2. This counttry would be better off if more [inngroup members] were willing to recognize r the goood things aboout [outgroup] cuulture (reverse sccored). Dumont and van v Lill (2009) 1. I regard myself m as a singlee person rather than t as a member of o a certain groupp of people. 2. I would rather have nothinng to do with anyy of the racial grouups in South Africa, including myy own. 3. I usually do d not consider myself m as belongging to any racial group. Niens & Cairnns (2002) 1. I usually doo not consider myself as belonging to any group. 2. In situations involving the Northern N Irish conflict, I m community’s side. automatically tend to take my mmunity, I usually do not 3. If someonee attacks my com take it personally. 4. I would rathher not have anyything to do with any of the two com mmunities in Norrthern Ireland. Dumont and van v Lill (2009) Difference beetween items (2-1): 1. I considerr myself as Southh African. 2. I considerr myself as blackk/white/coloured”” Niens & Cairnns (2002) 1. In this dayy and age with thhe European Unnion, nationalism m and patriotism m are out of datee. 2. First and foremost, f I regarrd myself as Eurropean rather than as a member of o my denominaational communitty. 7-pooint bipolar scalee Prrejudice: Coognitive dimension Individualization Individualization Suuperordinate ree-categorization Suuperordinate ree-categorization Paage 108 Studies 1 = strongly disagreee 7 = strongly agree No information about sca le provided. 1 = disagree stronggly, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither, 4 = agree, 5 = agree strongly No information about sca le provided. 1 = disagree stronggly, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither, 4 = agree, 5 = agree strongly Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Variable Measure Categorizationn Manipulation Hornsey and a Hogg (20022, adapted from H Hornsey & Hogg, 1999, 2000): Participannts are presenteed with a task annd asked to make deccisions, such as what should objjects and services should s be includded for successfuul functioning of a new park. In the supeerordinate condiition, u membber was participannts’ status as a university emphasizzed whereas theeir status as indivviduals was not. In thee simultaneous condition, c particiipants’ status as a univeersity member (ssuperordinate caategory) and their statuus as a member of a specific facculty (subgroupp category) weree emphasized. Gonzalezz and Brown (2005): Participannts were random mly assigned to oone of three categorization conditions: 1. Separrate individuals – no group categgory should be sallient (e.g., particcipants wore shirrts with their namess on it and worked on task indeppendently) 1. One group g – a commoon group identityy should be salient (e.g., participants wore same-ccoloured worked on shirts with their univerrsity logo, and w task collaboratively) c 2. Dual identity – both thhe common and subgroup identitties should be saalient (e.g., parti cipants wore t-shirtss with a university logo, but subggroups wore differeent coloured t-shhirts) Tajfel, Flaament, Billig, andd Bundy (1971):: Participannts allocated ressources among m members of the ingrouup and outgroupp using Tajfel maatrices. Categorizationn Manipulation Material ingrooup bias (**instrumentaal function of ingroup bias, which fosters intergroup competition – Scheepers et al. 2006)) Symbolic ingroup bias (**instrumentaal function of ingroup bias, which fosters intergroup competition) Humansystem ms® Scale Can also be measured byy looking asking participants to allocatee rewards to inggroup and outgrooup memberss. Scheeperrs et al. (2006) Agreement with ingroup favouring f (e.g., ““The detailed perceiverrs group is a supperior group”) annd outgroup derogatinng (e.g., “Global perceivers are bborn losers”) statements. Studies Nadleer, HarpazGoroodeisky, & Ben-David, 2009 In general, “pullscores” are calcculated to assess the tendency for individuals to usse a particular strateegy or favour a particuular group. Scheeepers et al., 20066; von H Hippel, 2006; Gonzzalez and Brow wn, 2005; Cariccati and Monaacelli, 2010 0 = not at all, 100 = very mucch w Tajfel matricees. Can also be measured with Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 109 Vaariable Measure Ingroup bias Go/No-Go Asssociation Task (GNAT; ( Nosek and a Banaji, 2001) Used to assess implicit attituddes separately foor two target groupss. Sensitivity scores were calculaated to assess impliccit ingroup bias, as a well as impliccit ingroup and outgroup o evaluattions. Nadler, Harpaaz-Gorodeisky, and a Ben-David (2009): ( Participants were w asked to raate the ingroup and outgroup on 5 bipolar adjectivve scales (industrious/laazy, competitive/cooperative, cuurious/not curious, creattive/not creative,, and intelligent/nnot intelligent) byy marking a range on a 574-pixell axis showing where they think moost group membeers fall under. Ingroup bias Group Identification Manipulation Chhange of coomparison dimension Teemporal coomparisons Im mplicit bias Paage 110 Van Vugt & Hart H (2004) Participants were w told that theeir performance on an investment taask would be com mpared to other students (low group ideentification) or sttudents from anoother university (higgh group identificcation. Niens & Cairnns (2002) 1. Compared with the other coommunity, my community might be in a worse economic situation, s but we do not n consider ecoonomic situation important. 2. Compared with the other coommunity, my community might be in a worse political situuation, but we do not n consider political situation im mportant. 3. Compared with the other coommunity, my community might be in a worse social situaation, but we do not consider c social situation s important. Niens & Cairnns (2002) 1. For my com mmunity, it is impportant to compaare its situation today with its situaation two years ago. a mmunity, comparisons with the situation s 2. For my com two years ago a are more important than comparisonns with the otherr community. Linguistic Inteergroup Bias (LIB B; Maass, Salvi, Arcuri, & Semin, 19889) The broad ideea of the LIB is that t when peoplee are asked to desccribe the behavioor of ingroup andd outgroup mem mbers, the concrreteness or absttraction within their deescription is likely to naturally vaary. When peoplee are likely to desscribe the positivve behaviours off ingroup membeers they are likelly to do so more abstractly. For exam mple, von Hippel (p. 536) e of how the t act of helping provides an example someone acrooss the street coould be interpreted differently inteerpreting on wheether it is perform med by an ingroup orr outgroup membber. For examplee, the Scaale Studies Vezzalli eet al. (2012) Thee midpoint of thee rangge was taken ass partticipants’ evaluaation of ggroup members oon eacch of the scales. Thee midpoints weree addded together to creaate an evaluativee sco re for ingroup annd outggroup members.. Schmitt aand Maes, 20002 1 = disagree stronggly, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither, 4 = agree, 5 = agree strongly 1 = disagree stronggly, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither, 4 = agree, 5 = agree strongly Stattements varyingg on leveel of abstractnesss andd concreteness werre rated on a 10-poinnt scale with “desscribes very pooorly” andd “describes veryy welll” as the anchorss. Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism von Hippel, 2006 Humanssystems® Variable Behavioural Responses Collective Acttion Tendencies Humansystem ms® Measure Scale positive behaviour b of an ingroup memberr could vary from the relatively r abstracct “Brad is helpfuul” descriptioon to a more conncrete descriptioon when attributingg this positive beehaviour to an ouutgroup member “Brad “ helped thee blind person crross the street” (p. 536). Similarly,, people are alsoo likely to describe the negative behhaviours of outg roup memberss in a more abstrract way, but aree likely be more conncrete when desccribing the negaative behaviours of ingroup meembers. These ddifferences emphasizze the positive trraits (and diminissh the importancce of negative beehaviours) of inggroup memberss and the highlighht the negative ttraits of outgroup members while lessening the im mpact of positive behaviours b (i.e., by limiting their impact to a specific event e or situationn). Boen andd Vanbeselaere (2002): Context: students s from a class are told thhat they performed worse (low staatus class) on a ttest relative to students from another class (high statuus class). Students are given the foollowing as actionns that they can take: Studies 1 = not at all, 7 = very much a which invvolves a 1. Individdual normative action, requeest to take the tesst again so that they have the oppportunity to join the high status class 2. Collecctive normative action, a which invvolves a requeest for the entire class to be retessted and hopeffully improve their performance 3. Individdual nonnormative action, whichh involves signinng a prepared peersonal protest leetter to be part of o the high statuss class without a retest 4. Collecctive nonnormatiive action, whichh involves signinng a prepared coollective letter whhereby the class should take the place of the hig h status class without the classs doing a retest 5. Accepptance, which invvolves acceptingg the test outcome and being okk with not being able to do the task that the high status group woould be doing. van Zomeeran, Spears, annd Leach (2008) : Collectivee action tendenccies 1. I would participate in a future demonsstration to stop thhis proposal. 2. I would participate in raising r our collecctive voice to stop thhis proposal. 3. I would do something together with feellow students to stopp this proposal. 4. I would participate in some s form of coollective actionn to stop this proposal. 5. I would sign a petitionn to stop this pro posal. Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 111 Vaariable Measure Scaale Deependencyorriented vs. auutonomyorriented help Nadler and Halabi (2006); Participants asked a to select one o of three choiices after working on a problem that theey could not solvve: 1. not wantinng help from a sttudent who attennded the other schoool and is also working w on the saame problem (ii.e., avoidance of o seeking help); 2. wanting thhe solution to thee problem from the t other student (i.e., seeking depeendency-oriented help); 3. wanting a hint from the othher student to heelp them solve the problem p (i.e., seeeking autonomyyoriented help). h ms Thee number of item on w which participannts chooose to provide oor askk for dependencyyvs. autonomy-oriented helpp. Exxpressing am mbivalent atttitude toward inggroup Ingroup favouritism Deefensive Heelping Intergroup Diifferentiation (social creative strategy) Intragroup reespect (social creative strategy) Paage 112 a Ben-David (2009): ( Nadler, Harpaas-Gorodeisky, and Similar optionns were providedd to participants (as in the measure above), but in thhe position of a helper. h Pagliaro, Alpaarone, Pacilli, annd Mucchi-Fainaa (2012) Participants inndicated their oppinion of Italians on 12 unipolar itemss which consisteed of 6 positive (e.g., approval) andd 6 negative (e.gg., disapproval) items. Ambivalent sccores were calcuulated using the formula (P + N) / 2 - | P – N|, where P = positive attituude score and N = negaative attitude scoore, and then a constant c of 1.5 was addded to the scoree in order to prevvent a negative resuult (see Thompsoon, Zanna, & Griffin, 1995). Nadler, Harpaaz-Gorodeisky, and a Ben-David (2009): ( Participants were w asked to asssign four roles to t ingroup and outgroup o membeers that differed in their level of prestige in a student newspaper n (e.g.., chief editor, marketing manager, ettc.). Nadler, Harpaaz-Gorodeisky, and a Ben-David (2009): ( In a 9 out of 12 1 trials, participants were told thhat an outgroup mem mber was havingg difficulty with thhe problems, andd were asked if they t wanted to help h the outgroup mem mber. Jetten, Schmitt, Branscombe, and McKimmiee (2005): 1. There are many differencees between Queeensland and other states. 2. Queenslannd is very differeent from other sttates in Australia. Jetten, Schmitt, Branscombe, and McKimmiee (2005; adapted from Deaux, Reid, Mizrahi, M and Cotting, 1999): 1. In generall, I feel valued byy others in my group. 2. Queenslannders treat me positively. p 3. I feel respected by other Queenslanders. Q 4. I feel fully accepted by Quueenslanders. Studies 1 = not at all, 6 = strongly 0 = extreme ingroupp favoouritism, 7 = equal allocationn to the ingroup and outggroup, 14 = extreme outgrooup favoouritism Thee number of timees thatt participants choose to hhelp the outgroupp mem mber. 1 = strongly disagreee, 9 = strongly agree 1 = strongly disagreee, 9 = strongly agree Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 9. Scientiffic Stattus of the Co onceptt of Secularr and Scientif S fic Fun ndame entalism m Included in n this chapter is a review off the scientificc status of thee concept of ssecular and sccientific fundamentaalism. Existin ng measures related r to thesse constructs are also identtified. Furthermore, it investigatess how a secular, scientific or humanisticc fundamentaalist world vieew might influuence health and well-being w ass well as cogn nition. 9.1 Se ecular and d Scientific Fundam mentalism 9.1.1 Fundamentaliism In order to explore the meaning m of sciientific or seccular fundameentalism, it iss important to first understand the meaning of fundamentalism in gen eral. Merriam-W Webster’s online dictionary y offers two deefinitions of ffundamentalissm, includingg: x x “a movement m orr attitude stresssing strict annd literal adheerence to a sett of basic prinnciples.” (“F Fundamentalissm,” n.d.) “a movement m in 20th century y Protestantism m emphasizinng the literallyy interpreted B Bible as fun ndamental to Christian C life and teachingg; the beliefs oof this movem ment; adherennce to such beliefs.” (“Fund damentalism,”” n.d.) nition emphassizing adhereence to a set oof principles ppresents a relaatively The first paart of the defin neutral view w of fundameentalism, and makes no asssumptions aboout the types of principles that are typical. Thee second part of the definittion shows thhe close relatioon between reeligion and fundamentaalism, as exprressed in a wiide body of reesearch explooring religiouss fundamentalism (e.g., Altemeyer and Hunsberg ger, 1992). According to C Calhoun (20022; cited in Piggliucci, 2005,, p. 1106), the Oxford Dictionary of the Social Sciences S definnes fundamenntalism as “a m movement thaat asserts the primacy y of religious values in soccial and politi cal life and caalls for a retuurn to a ‘fundaamental’ or pure form m of religion””. Other descrriptions of fun ndamentalism m make more negative assuumptions arouund the naturee of adherence to t basic principles. For exaample, Pigliuccci (2005) arggues that at itts core, fundam mentalism is a specificc form of “ideeological intraansigence”. Inn his view, fuundamentalism m extends beyyond religion to political p and social s views as a well, and hhe cites extrem me positions ttaken by ‘environmeentalists’ or an nimal-rights activists a as otther exampless of fundamenntalism. In thiis case, then, fundaamentalism seeems to be desscribed as adhherence to a sset of principlles that are m more extreme thaan moderate, and perhaps not n open to chhange. Other definitions seeem to indicaate that fundamentaalism implicaates the relatio onship betweeen oneself andd others, perhhaps making ppeople more motiv vated to give preference p to their own belliefs over thoose of others oor making theem less likely to be tolerant of otthers. Vail an nd colleagues state (2010, pp. 89) that “fuundamentalism m entails a fortress mentality in whiich security iss maintained bby continuallly affirming thhe superiorityy of one’s own beliefss over all otheers”. This chaaracteristic off fundamentalism can be eqqually appliedd to religious an nd secular (no on-religious) fundamentali f sm. As Seitz (2010, p. 5) aargues, “a bassic and strongly held part of any y type of fund damentalism iis the view thaat the ‘believers’ are in poossession of the truth and that they y have the righ ht, indeed dutty to make evveryone acceppt this and enfforce Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 113 coonforming beehaviour. Com mpliance migh ht be achieved d through perrsuasion, psycchological m manipulation, economic, social and polittical pressure as well as bruute force.” These definitio ons illustrate the t most com mmon descripttion of fundam mentalism as a negative chharacteristic. This observattion is echoed d in the follow wing descriptiion: “Whetheer applied to reeligious or seccular thinking g, the ‘fundam mentalist’ labeel carries as ppejorative connnotation. Oftten ussed loosely an nd without cleear definition n, the label can n be used to m mark a personn, group, or innstitution as in n some respecct intolerant, militant m of oth herwise danggerous.” (Connkle, 1996, p. 5). The majority of o the availablle definitions,, then, describ be fundamenttalism in prim marily negativve teerms, as repreesenting extrem me views thaat are often diffficult to channge, a sense oof superiority and geeneral intolerance and attempts to forcee the complian nce of others. 9.1.2 Secullar fundame entalism This use of thee term secularr fundamentallism seems to o be more freqquently used iin popular culture. It haas, however, also been useed to label thee extreme fasccist and comm munist movem ments of the eearly to m middle twentieeth century ass well as the French F Revolu ution (Schlesiinger, 1995; C Carrington, 1993; ciited in Conklee, 1996). All of o these moveements involv ved totalitariaan governmennts that used ttheir exxtreme politiccal beliefs to justify j the exeecution of inn nocents and oother crimes. W When used inn coontext, the neegative associations with th he fundamentalist label aree clear. Inn a more geneeral and less extreme e mann ner, secular fu undamentalism m has been ussed to describbe an iddeological intrransigence off political beliiefs such as political liberaalism. In this context, secuular fuundamentalism m is understood as rejectin ng religion or spiritual beliefs as a sourcce of truth in tthe puublic domain and instead emphasizing e the t use of reason and logicc (Campos, 19994; Conkle, 1996). Prroponents of secular fundaamentalism do o not reject reeligion entirelly, but insteadd seem to arggue that it has no place in the public domain, as evidenced e by the t followingg quote, “To tthe extent thatt reeligion has tru uth value, it iss a matter of private p truth, a form of trutth that lacks ppublic significance” (C Conkle, 1996,, p. 348). C Conkle (1996) used the term m “comprehen nsive secular fundamentaliism” to descrribe a secular fuundamentalism m that appliess to both the public p and priivate domainss. Comprehennsive secular fuundamentalism m argues thatt all questionss of truth and meaning can only be answ wered by secuular raationalisation and modern science, and that t there are no truths to bbe gained from m any other soources. Comp prehensive seccular fundameentalism com mpletely rejectts the possibillity of the suupernatural, or o that any tru uths or insightts can be gain ned from religgion. 9.1.3 Scien ntific fundam mentalism Sccientific fund damentalism, on the other hand, h is rarely y mentioned iin the academ mic literature, and it iss very difficullt to obtain deefinitions of th his construct other than in the popular cculture literatuure. Sccientific fund damentalism can c be defined d as the belieff that “empiriically based kknowledge is the onnly reliable way w of knowin ng reality” (A Appleby & Maarty, 2008, p. 16). Similarlyy, Seitz (20100, p.7) arrgues, “[a] fun ndamental an nd very imporrtant believe (sic. ( belief) off scientific fuundamentalism m is the asssumption thaat Science pro ovides objectiive knowledg ge that is univ ersally appliccable and is abble to soolve most, if not n all, probleems”. When compared c to the t final definnition of secular fundamenntalism prresented in th he previous seection, there are a no evidentt conceptual ddifferences beetween the tw wo coonstructs (i.e., secular and scientific fun ndamentalism m). A Another term “scientism” “ has been identtified in the litterature that m may be a morre establishedd term foor the same co onstruct as scientific fundaamentalism. Scientism S has been definedd as having “ttwo Paage 114 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® major attrib butes: uncond ditional belieff in science, a view mainlyy held and authhorised by sccientists which they unconsciouslly force the general g publicc in any culturral setting to aaccept and shhare; and uncritical conviction of valuing v appliccation of scieence into life--world settinggs, originatingg in the Western traaditional value system” (M McConnel, 20003, p.3). Anotther author haas conceptuallized scientism as: x x x x A single-minded s d adherence to o only the em mpirical or testable; A strictly s scientiific worldview w; Rejjects most, if not all, metap physical, phillosophical andd religious claaims becausee they can nnot be tested d through the scientific s metthod; and Vieews science as a the absolutee and only jusstifiable accesss to the truthh about the woorld and reaality (Seitz, 20 010). As demonstrated by this description and a through ccomparing theese definitions with the onees in previous seections, the terrms “secular fundamentaliism”, “scientiific fundamenntalism” and “scientism”” seem to be defining d a verry similar connstruct. In genneral, howeveer, there is relatively little detail available abo out them, and these construucts appear too be quite undderdeveloped in the available litterature. The next two secttions further eexamine the sstate of the litterature by investigatin ng the impact of non-religious belief sysstems on believers’ health and well-beinng (e.g., “happier”) or cognition. Scales relevaant to scientiffic fundamenttalism are alsoo identified. 9.2 Im mpact of no on-religio ous belief systems o on health, well-bein ng and co ognition One particu ular area of in nterest is the im mpact non-reeligious belieff systems (e.gg., rigid seculaar, scientific, or o humanistic fundamentalist world view ws) have on hhealth and weell-being as w well as cognition. There T is somee evidence in the available literature of connections aamong religioon, spirituality,, and health. Several S studiees demonstratte that religiouus devotion aand spiritualitty are associated with w longevity y, psychologiical adjustment, improved recovery from m illness, copping skills, health-relatted quality off life, and redu uce anxiety annd depressionn (e.g. Pargam ment, 2001; Sm mith, McCulloug gh, &Poll, 200 03; Mueller, Plevak, P & Rum mmans, 20011; Sloan, Bagiiella, & Poweell, 1999; Sloan & Baagiella, 2002; McCullough h, Hoyt, Larsoon, Koenig, & Thoresen, 2000; Luskin, 2000; Levine & Targ, T 2002, Seeeman, Dubin n, Seeman, 20003). Howeveer, there is a ggreat deal of ccriticism of the meassures used in this t research (cf. Hall, Meador & Koennig, 2008; Büsssing, Matthieessan, & Ostermann,, 2005), and this t could lim mit the conclussions that cann be drawn froom existing reesearch on the positivee impacts of religion on health and well-being. A search off the availablee literature loo oking at the iimpacts of nonn-religious orr non-spirituaal belief systems on health outcom mes revealed very little ressearch. . Evenn though studdies investigatting the relationship p between reliigion and heaalth outcomess often includee individuals who do not identify themselves as being relig gious, analysees do not ade quately incluude this imporrtant group whhen considering g the relationsship between a more neutraal, secular belief system annd health outccomes. Ross (1990 0) argues that people in thiss group are, aat best, used aas a comparisoon group rathher than tapping their unique persspective on why w they identtify as non-reeligious and hhow this benefits them regarding th heir health an nd well-being. Studies ofteen combine thhese individuals with participants who claim very v weak beeliefs, or exclu ude them from m analysis alttogether. In R Ross’ investiggation of the effects of o religion on n psychologiccal distress, shhe emphasizedd the importaance of undersstanding the perspecctives of both religious and d non-religiouus individuals. She states, Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 115 “Peoplle who say they have no reeligion do nott say it lightlyy; they are nott indifferent. They have made m a consciious choice to o reject religio on, and this iss quite differeent from sayinng, for examp ple, that one iss Protestant but b has only a very weak beelief. Those w who claim "noo religio on" or "no belief" are an im mportant comp parison groupp… Past literaature leads us to expectt that those wiith no religion n should havee high distresss levels, sincee they are maarginal and lacck the meanin ng and comm mitment that co ome from reliigious belief. On the other hand, they may m have mad de commitmen nts to other no on-religious iinstitutions thhat serve an emotio onal function similar to thaat of religion. We might th en expect thaat both personns with strong religious belief and person ns who have rejected r religgion would haave low distreess levels”” (p. 237). Inndeed, this ressearch showed that individ duals reporting no religiouss beliefs had tthe lowest levvels of diistress when compared c to individuals i with w religious beliefs b (Ross,, 1990). Indivviduals who reported sttrong religiou us beliefs show wed similarly y lower distresss levels to thhose reportingg weak religioous beeliefs. Other analyses a expllored whetherr the type of religion endorrsed impactedd on well-beinng. R Results showed d that the psy ychological diistress of indiv viduals who rreported no reeligion was nnot siignificantly diifferent from that of Protesstants, whereaas individualss who identifiied themselvees as C Catholic or Jew wish showed significantly higher levels of distress. H However, evenn this study ddid not exxamine the co ontent or conv viction of the beliefs of peo ople who idenntified themselves as non-religious. A search of thee available litterature revealed no existin ng empirical sstudies that innvestigate the reelationship beetween specifi fic non-religio ous beliefs, su uch as atheism m, agnosticism m, and scientiism and w well-being. Insstead, studies group all of the t different non-religious n belief system ms into a one-diimensional “n non- religiouss” variable (cff. Weber, Parrgament, Kuniik, Lomax, & Stanley, 20111), w which does nott delineate tho ose who are non-religious n to those whoo are secular oor scientific fuundamentalistts. This lack of o specificity severely limiits the conclussions that cann be drawn froom reesearch that in ncludes a “non-religious” variable. v Thiss oversight haas not gone unnnoticed by oother reesearchers. Fo or example, Whitley W (2010 0) argued abou ut the importaance of investtigating the effect of attheism on heaalth. As he ex xplained, “[t]h hough not a ‘rreligion’, atheeism can be ann orienting w worldview thatt is often conssciously chossen by its adheerents, who ¿¿rmly believe in the ‘truth’ of attheism… Ath heism (just lik ke theism) is an a appropriatee domain of sstudy for sociaal and culturaal pssychiatrists (aand allied soccial scientists)) interested in n exploring soocio-environm mental stressoors and buuffers relating g to mental heealth” (p.190,, emphasis ad dded). O Overall, the rellationship bettween strong, committed, non-religious n s worldviews and health annd w well-being doees not seem to o have been em mpirically inv vestigated in the available literature. 9.3 Scale es pertain ning to sec cular and scientific c fundame entalism The following sections revieew potentially y relevant meeasures from tthe available peer-revieweed litterature and, to the extent possible, p willl compare theem to the Upaal and Legaultt (2011) scienntific fuundamentalism m (SF) scale. Due to the laack of empiriccal research oon scientific fu fundamentalissm and sccientism, only y one scale that directly meeasures the co onstructs of innterest could bbe identified,, but a nuumber of otheer scales that are potentiallly relevant to scientific funndamentalism m were also coonsidered. Fo or a full list off items of the SF scale, seee Annex A. 9.3.1 Scien ntism A As discussed in n the previous section, scieentism seems closely relateed to the conccept of scienttific fuundamentalism m. Fulljames,, Gibson, and Francis (199 91) developedd a 5-item Likkert scale meaasuring Paage 116 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® scientism, which w they deefined as “thee extent to whhich scientificc views are abbsolutely certaain” (p.174). Th he items from this measure are as follow ws: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Sciience will eveentually give us u complete ccontrol over tthe world. Theeories in scien nce can be prroved to be deefinitely true. Thee laws of scieence will neveer be changedd. Theeories in scien nce are neverr proved with absolute certtainty. (Reverrse scored) Nothing should be believed unless u it can bbe proved scieentifically. odified the scaale by addingg an additionaal item “Sciennce will eventtually give Stolberg (2007) later mo us completee understandiing of the worrld”. Howeveer, the originaal 5-item meassure from Fullljames and colleag gues demonstrrated poor intternal consisteency with an alpha of 0.566, while the 6--item measure used by Stolberrg (2007) dem monstrated accceptable reliaability with ann alpha of .700. Astley & Frrancis (2010)) also created a modified veersion of the scale to moree accurately ccapture more recent conceptualizzations of scientism with tthe addition oof two items: ““Science can give us absolute tru uths”and “Sciience alone caan provide truuths about natture”. The ressultant 7-item m scale had a reliability y coefficient of o 0.77, demonstrating impproved reliabiility. The originaal scientism measure m was found f to be siggnificantly reelated to perceeptions of Chhristians as creationistss (Fulljames et e al., 1991). In I Francis andd Greer’s (20001) investigattion the roles of scientism and creationism m on adolescents’ attitudees towards science and religgion, this meaasure was also significcantly negativ vely related to o participantss’ endorsemennt of creationnism and posittively related with h attitudes tow ward science. The 7-item vversion used bby Astley andd Francis (20110) demonstrated an identicaal pattern of relationships r iin an investiggation of the eeffects of scieentism and creationism m on attitudes toward science and religioon. Specificallly, it was signnificantly neggatively related to crreationism an nd positively related r to attiitudes toward science and rreligion. . The scientissm scales sho ow some relattionship to thee SF scale (U Upal & Legaullt, 2011) as thhe SF scale also contain ns items assesssing the view w that scientiffic methods aand scientific ttheories can aattain absolute tru uth. 9.3.2 Ev volution and d Creationis sm As mention ned in the prev vious section, scientism haas been investtigated in tanndem with belliefs about creationism m and evolutio on. Since indiividuals who aadhere to scieentific fundam mentalism woould completely reject creatio onism in all fo orms and enddorse evolutioon, measures oof beliefs in eevolution and creation nism may also be relevant. The measurres used in the previous section that werre correlated with scientism m measure w were a measure off Creationism developed by y Francis and Greer (1999aa). Astley andd Francis (2010) used a modified veersion of the scale s that rem moved all refeerences to the Bible as it is a religious teext that is specific to Christianity. C These T two veersions of the Creationism scale are founnd in Annex B B. Another pro omising meassure that was identified in the literature was one deveeloped by Polling and Evans (200 04). This meassure contains a wider specttrum of beliefs about creattionism and eevolution and includees seven consttructs: common descent, crreationism, aadaptation, intterconnectednness, religiosity, paranormal beliefs, b and ex xtinction. Forr a descriptionns, sample iteems (completee list of items was not n published)), and reliabillity of these suubscales see Table 14. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 117 Table 14:: Subscale Description D ns, Sample Items, I and Reliability ffor Poling a and Eva an’s (2004) Measure M Suubscale Description Samplle items R Reliability Coommon Descent (11 items) Focused on thhe Darwinian theorry of common deceent “All sppecies have develooped from a comm mon ancestor” and “D Dogs, coyotes andd wolves all have a common ancestor.” Į = .87 Crreationism Measured the degree of belief that all w created by Good and living things were that the univerrse has not changed since creation “All living creatures weree created by God”” and “Our univerrse was created byy God.” Į = .75 “Species undergo changges as a result of pressure from thhe environment” aand “If a grass covvered island becom mes desert-like, soome animals might develop the ability to store water (likke camels) and theey would pass this abbility on to their offfspring” Į = .77 (8 items) Assessed the opinions on the iddea that species changge over time in ressponse to environmenntal change Intterconnectednesss (3 items) Focused on thhe interconnectivityy of species “If all members m of one sspecies migrated too a different area, members m of otherr species left behinnd would be affecteed” and “All speciees are completely separate from one o another” (reveerse coded). Į = .66 Reeligiosity Measured enddorsement of a corre set of religious belieffs that transcend religious r boundaries “Religiion is one way thaat we can explain tthings that Į = .73 Assessed beliefs in paranormal phenomenon “Certaain individuals can read the thoughtss of others” and “S Some people can ppredict the future”” Į = .77 Measured genneral knowledge about extinction “Certaain species have bbecome extinct eveen though humanns have never ide ntified them” and “If an animal’s environment iss suddenly changeed, the entire speciees might disappeaar” Į = .68 (7 items) Addaption (5 items) Paaranormal Beliefs (8 items) Exxtinction (8 items) otherw wise have no explaanation” and “Religious writings provide guidelines for coorrect moral behavior” d promising psychometric properties, witth some evideence of conveergent The measure demonstrated annd divergent validity v and th he majority reeliabilities off the subscaless ranging from m .73 - .87. U Unfortunately, no further ev vidence of vallidation and reliability r exisst as the meassure appears tto have onnly been used d in the one sttudy 9.3.3 Relatiing Science e and Religiion Inn Stolberg’s (2 2007) investigation of the attitudes of primary p teacheers toward religion and sciience, a new five-item m measure ex xplored differeent ways of reelating sciencce and religionn. These quesstions arre not intendeed to be used as a scale; each item is sup pposed to reprresent a diffeerent way of reepresenting th he relationship p between relligion and scieence. These iitems are listeed below. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Both science s and reeligion are im mportant for hu uman well-beeing Conflict between sccience and relligion is ineviitable Sciencce and religion n should be kept k completely separate Deep down d science and religion are one and the t same Interacction between n science and religion can be b of benefit tto both Paage 118 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Though nott directly a measure of scieentific or secuular fundamenntalism, this m measure is pootentially relevant as individuals who w endorse scientific s funddamentalism rreject religionn as a possible source of truth or meaning. m As a result, thosee who score hhigh on the SF F scale (Upal & Legault, 22011), would stron ngly disagree with items th hat indicate thhat religion haas some impoortance (itemss 1, 4, and 5). The SF scale includes items aboutt the conflict bbetween sciennce and religiion and the roole of religion succh as “Wheneever science and a religion coonflict, sciencce is right”, ““Science will one day explain all aspects of reaality including g religion”, annd “Religion is needed to explain thosee aspects of the univeerse that scien nce cannot an nd will never bbe able to expplain” (reversse scored). Ass such, individuals with high sco ores on the SF F scale may ppotentially enndorse one or both of itemss 2 and 3 of the relatiing science an nd religion measure. Howeever, these sppeculations aree subject to empirical validation. 9.3.4 eliefs Strrength of Be As discusseed in the prev vious section, the potential health benefiits of religiouus beliefs mayy largely depend on the t strength of o these belieffs. Consequenntly, measurinng the strengtth of non-religgious beliefs is allso important in order to in nvestigate the relationship between non--religious bellief systems and d health outco omes. By defi finition, scienttific fundameentalists are sttrongly comm mitted to their beliefss. As a result,, individuals who w score higgh on the SF sscale may alsso score high on scales measuring strength s of beeliefs. Few studiess directly inveestigate the generic strengtth of beliefs. Existing reseearch demonsttrates that some studiees measure strrength of beliiefs using dom main-specificc scales (e.g. rreligious fundamentaalism; Altemeeyer & Hunsb berger, 1990 aas cited in Maaxwell-Smithh & Esses, 20112) or a one-item measure m that diirectly asks th he participantt to rate the thheir commitm ment to a speciific belief (i.e., Ross, 1990). Two longer potentiially relevant scales measuuring the strenngth of beliefs fs include the Commitment to Beliefs scale (CT TB; Maxwell- Smith & Essees, 2012) andd the World H Health Organizatio on Quality of Life – Spiritu ual, Religiouss, and Personaal Beliefs moodule (WHOQ QOL – SRPB; WH HOQOL SRPB B Group, 2002). The comm mitment to beeliefs scale meeasures “the ddegree to which an in ndividual generally feels it is important to follow his or her value--expressive beeliefs” (Maxwell-S Smith & Essees, p.195). Thee scale is com mprised of twoo subscales, bbelief centraliity and belief transcendence. Th he measure haas overall dem monstrated goood reliabilityy and validity (Maxwell-S Smith & Essees) and directlly measures sttrength of belliefs. For a fuull list of item ms included in the CTB scale see Annex C. The WHOQOL W –S SRPB scale m measures strenngth of beliefs fs less directly. Ho owever, it hass extensive crross-cultural vvalidity, and iit measures peersonal and sppiritual beliefs in general (WHO OQOL SRPB Group, 2006)). In addition,, the WHOQO OL-SRPB waas developed to t assess the direct d relevan nce of spirituaal, religious annd personal bbeliefs to heallth-related quality of liife and outcom mes (WHOQOL SRPB Grroup, 2002). F For a full list of items incluuded in the WHOQ QOL scale see Annex C. 9.3.5 Atttitude toward Science Scientific fundamentalis fu sm is adopted as a result off strong or firm m attitudes annd beliefs onee has regarding th he power of science s and itss capacity to define our woorld and placee in it. Individduals that endorse a scientific fund damentalist beelief system vview science aas important aand infallible. Attitudes toward scieence have beeen examined, specifically inn terms of stuudents’ attituddes toward sccience, and identified in n the literaturre (c.f., Blalocck et al., 20088). For examplle, Menis (198 89) developed d a 23-item m measure of atttitudes towardd science withh subscales assessing the importance i off science, attittude toward sscience as a career, and sciience in Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 119 thhe school currriculum. The attitude toward science as a career and science in thee school curriiculum sm. Howeverr, the importaance of sciencce arre not likely to be relevant to scientific fundamentali f suubscale may be b related to the t SF scale (Upal & Legaault, 2011) as individuals w who are high iin sccientific fundaamentalism are a likely to raate science as being very im mportant. Thiis 10-item subbscale m measuring the importance of o science has also been useed on its ownn in studies annd is reportedd to haave demonstrrated reliabilitty and validity y (Francis & Greer, 1999bb; Stolberg & Fulljames, 20003; Sttolberg, 2007 7). For a full list l of items, see s Annex D.. M Measures havee also been deeveloped to deetermine how w attitudes tow ward science cchange over ttime suuch as the Change in Attitu ude about the Relevance off Science queestionnaire (C CARS; Siegel & R Ranney, 2003)). This scale contained c item ms such as “Sccience helps m me think thinngs through” aand “S Science can help h me to maake better cho oices about vaarious things iin my life (e.gg., food to eatt, car to buuy)” (Siegel & Ranney, p. 769). As such, it is intend ded to addresss how integrall science is too one’s liffe in general. Individuals with w high scorres on the SF scale (Upal & Legault, 20011) would bee likely too score high on o this measurre as well. Ho owever, the uttility of the C CARS instrum ment may be liimited byy its specificity for an educcational settin ng. 9.3.6 Free Will W and Determinism Siimilar to evollution, individ duals who adh here to scienttific fundamenntalism are likkely to have sstrong oppinions aboutt free will and d determinism m. Because off the emphasiss that scientiffic fundamenttalism pllaces on explaanations baseed on science or rationalizaation, these inndividuals woould be likely to enndorse free will, w whereas determinism d or o belief in fatte would be vviewed as “unnscientific” annd faatalistic (e.g., predestinatio on). Paaulhus and Cary (2011) deeveloped a meeasure of freee will and deteerminism callled the FAD-pplus. The FAD-plus contains 27 items, i uses a five-point Lik kert scale, andd is reported to have prom mising pssychometrics. The scale is composed off four subscalles: Free Willl, Scientific D Determinism, Faatalistic Determinism, and d Unpredictab bility. Scientiffic determinissm measures bbelief in bioloogical annd environmeental determin nants of humaan behaviourss whereas Fattalistic Determ minism measuures beelief in fate. For F a full list of items on th he FAD-plus scale see Annnex E. Sccores on the SF S scale woulld likely be positively relatted to scores on the free w will subscale aand poossibly the sccientific determ minism scale. Paage 120 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® 9.4 Su ummary of Findings s In sum, the concept of seecular or scientific fundam mentalism has received verry little attentiion in the existing sciientific literatu ure. Literaturre that does coover non-religgious fundam mentalism has been largely phillosophical or theoretical in n nature. Therre is some lim mited experim mental researchh that investigatess constructs th hat are related d to secular o r scientific fuundamentalism m. This researrch has for the most paart been limiteed to the educcational field and focuses oon the attitudees of teacherss and students tow ward science and religion. Due to the limited l researrch on scientiific fundamenntalism and reelated construucts, there is little research inv vestigating th he characteristtics of individduals who adhhere to non-reeligious belieefs. Strong religious beeliefs have beeen linked to positive p healtth outcomes, bbut no researcch has investiigated if commitmen nt to non-relig gious belief sy ystems impaccts positively or negativelyy on health. T Though some researrch on religio on and health has included individuals w who do not iddentify with anny religion as a comparrison group, the t personal beliefs b of thesse individualss (e.g., such aas if they idenntify with atheism or scientific fun ndamentalism,, or the strenggths of their bbeliefs) has noot been investtigated. Similarly, although a reseaarch has identtified social aand cognitive characteristiccs that are asssociated with religio ous fundamen ntalism, no ressearch that innvestigates thee social or coggnitive characcteristics of individuaals who espou use non-religiious belief syystems was nooted during ouur searches. As a result of the limited d body of literrature on nonn-religious bellief systems, tthere are few existing measures th hat measure scientific fund damentalism. Upal and Leggault’s (2011) SF scale adddresses an important gap g in existing g research. Th he conceptuall framework oof the scale iss promising as items seem to tap p identified ch haracteristics of scientific ffundamentalissm. Howeverr, some of the current items includ de more than one concept in the same ittem, and this could make iit confusing ffor participantss if they have different opin nions about thhe different cconcepts in thhe item. For exxample with the item “We have to be a little skeptical s of soome of the sccientific claim ms because sciientists also have th heir own agen ndas” an indiv vidual may aggree with the first part of thhe statement (“We have to be a little skepticcal of some sccientific claim ms”) but disaggree with the reason (“…bbecause scientists allso have theirr own agendass”). Future w ork may conssider the indivvidual items oon the SF scale to dettermine if exaamples such as a the one provvided above w will impact pparticipants’ understandiing of these (ii.e., what partt of the item iis guiding theeir response). Future revisiions will likely strengthen the valiidity and reliaability of the scale. Follow wing validatioon, the SF scalle will contribute to t future reseaarch addressin ng the prominnence of non--religious belief systems annd the impact on health h and weell-being and cognition. c Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 121 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 122 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® REFE ERENC CES ALTEMEY YER, Ǻ, & HU UNSBERGER R, Ǻ. 1992. A Authoritarianism, religious fundamentallism, quest, and prejudice. p Thee Internationa al Journal forr the Psycholo logy of Religioon, 2, 113-133. APPLEBY, R.S. & MAR RTY, M.E. 20 002. Fundam mentalism. Forreign Policy, 128, 16-18+220-22. J & FRANC CIS, L.J. 2010 0. Promoting ppositive attituudes towards science and rreligion ASTLEY, J., among sixth h-form pupilss: Dealing witth scientism aand creationissm. British Joournal of Reliigious Education, 32(3), 189-20 00. ON, K., DORR R, N., & HUM ME, D. L. 20001. Status diffferences BETTENCOURT, B. A.., CHARLTO p bias: A meta-analytic exaamination of the effects off status stabiliity, status legitimacy, and ingroup and group permeability. p Psychologica al Bulletin, 1227(4), 520-5442. BLAIR, I. V., V & JOST, J. J T. 2003. Ex xit, loyalty, annd collective action amongg workers in a simulated business b envirronment: Interactive effectts of group iddentification aand boundary permeabilitty. Social Justice Research h, 16(2), 95-1 08. BLALOCK K, C., LICHTE ENSTEIN, M., M OWEN, S.., PRUSKI, L L., MARSHAL LL, C., & TOEPPERW WEIN, M. In pursuit of vaalidity: A com mprehensive review of scieence attitude instrumentss 1935-2005. Internationall Journal of SScience Educaation, 30(7), 9961-977. BLANZ, M., M MUMMEN NDEY, A., MIELKE, M R., & KLINK, A A. 1998. Respponding to neggative social identtity: A taxono omy of identitty managemeent strategies. European Joournal of Sociial Psychologyy, 28, 697-729 9. BOEN, F., & VANBESE ELAERE, N. 2002. The reelative impactt of socio-struuctural characcteristics on behaviorral reactions against a memb bership in a loow status grouup. Group Prrocesses & Intergroup Relations, 5(4), 5 299-318 8. BOEN, F., VANBESEL LAERE, N., & COOL, M. 22006. Group status as a deeterminant of organizational identificaation after a taakeover: A soocial identity pperspective. G Group Processses & Intergroup Relations, 9(4), 547-560. ogy of prejudiice: Ingroup llove or outgrooup hate? Jouurnal of BREWER, M. B. 1999. The psycholo 9-444. Social Issuees, 55(3), 429 BROWN, R. R 1978. Divided we fall: An A analysis o f relations beetween sectionns of a factoryy workforce. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), ( Differeentiation betw ween social grroups (pp. 3955-429). San D Diego, CA: Academic Press. P BROWN, R. R J. 1984. Th he effects of in ntergroup sim milarity and cooperative veersus competiitive orientation on intergroup p discriminatiion. British Joournal of Soccial Psychologgy, 23. 21-33. R & WADE, G. 1987. Su uperordinate ggoals and inteergroup behavviour: The efffect of role BROWN, R., ambiguity and a status on intergroup atttitudes and taask performannce. Europeann Journal of SSocial Psychologyy: 17, 131-142 2. BÜSSING, A., OSTERM MANN, T., & MATTHIES SSEN, P.F. (22005). Role oof Religion annd spirituality in medical paatients: con¿rrmatory resultts with the SppREUK questtionnaire. Heaalth and Quality of Life L Outcomes, 3(10):1–10 0. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 123 C CAMPOS, P.F F. 1994. Secullar fundamentalism. Colum mbia Law Revview, 94(6), 1814-1827. C CARICATI, L. AND MON NACELLI, N. 2010. Social hierarchies aand intergroupp discriminatiion: The case of thee intermediatee status group p. British Jourrnal of Sociall Psychology,, 49(3), 637–6646. C CONKLE, D.O O. 1996. Secu ular fundamentalism, relig gious fundam mentalism, annd the searchh for trruth in contemporary Am merica. Journa al of Law and d Religion, 122(2), 337-370.. C COSTARELLII, S. 2012. Co oping with inttergroup threaat via biased attributions too low group eeffort: The moderatin ng roles of ing group identificcation, legitim macy, and insstability of inttergroup statuus. Soocial Psychollogy, 43(1), 47-59. D DOOSJE, B., SPEARS, S R., & ELLEMER RS, N. 2002. Social identiity as both cauuse and effectt: The deevelopment of o group identtification in reesponse to antticipated and actual changges in the interrgroup sttatus hierarchy. British Jou urnal of Socia al Psychologyy, 41(1), 57-766. D DUMONT, K., & VAN LIL LL, B. 2009. Dominant D and d non-dominaant groups ressponses to social chhange: The ecconomic transsformation prrocess in Soutth Africa. Souuth African Joournal of Ps Psychology, 39 9(4), 432-447 7. ELLEMERS, N., N SPEARS, R., & DOOS SJE, B. 1997. Sticking togeether or fallinng apart: In-grroup iddentification as a a psycholog gical determinant of group p commitmennt versus indivvidual mobilitty. Joournal of Perrsonality and Social S Psycho ology, 72(3), 617-626. ELLEMERS, N., N VAN RIJS SWIJK, W., ROEFS, R M., & SIMONS, C. 1997. Biass in intergrouup peerceptions: Balancing grou up identity wiith social reality. Personallity and Sociaal Psychology Bulletin, 23, 18 86–198. ELLEMERS, N., N WILKE, H., H & VAN KNIPPENBER K RG, A. 1993. Effects of thhe legitimacy of low grroup or indiviidual status on n individual and a collectivee status-enhanncement strateegies. Journaal of P Personality an nd Social Psycchology, 64(5 5), 766-778. FIISHER, R. J. 2000. Intergrroup conflict. In M. Deutscch, P. T. Coleeman, & E. C C. Marcus (Edds.), nd Th The handbook of conflict reesolution: Theeory and pracctice. (2 ed., pp. 176-196)). San Francissco: Joossey-Bass. FR RANCIS, L.JJ., & GREER R, J.E. 1999a. Attitudes tow wards creationnism and evollutionary theoory: the deebate among secondary pu upils attending g Catholic and d Protestant sschools in Noorthern Irelandd. P Public Understanding of Sccience, 8, 93-1 103. FR RANCIS, L.JJ., & GREER R, J.E. 1999b. Attitude towaard science am mong secondary school puupils in N Northern Irelan nd: relationsh hip with sex, age, a and relig gion. Researchh in Science aand Technoloogical E Education, 17((1). FR RANCIS, L.JJ., & GREER R, J.E. 2001. Shaping S adoleescents’ attituddes towards sscience and reeligion inn Northern Ireeland: the rolee of scientism m, creationism m and denominnational schools. Researchh in Sccience & Tech hnological Ed ducation, 19(1), 39-53. FU ULLJAMES,, P., GIBSON N, H.M., & FR RANCIS, L.J.. 1991. Creatiionism, scienttism, Christiaanity annd science: A study in adolescent attitud des. British Educational E R Research Jourrnal, 17(2), 1771-190. FU UNDAMENT TALISM. (n.d d.). In Merria am-Webster Dictionary D online. Retrieveed from htttp://www.meerriam-websteer.com/diction nary/fundameentalism Paage 124 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® GONZALE EZ, R., & BRO OWN, R. 200 06. Dual idenntities in interggroup contactt: Group statuus and size moderate th he generalizattion of positiv ve attitude ch ange. Journaal of Experimeental Social Psychologyy, 42, 753–767 7. HALL, D.E E., MEADOR R, K.G., KOEN NIG, H.G. 20008. Measurinng religiousneess in health rresearch: Review and d critique. Jou urnal of Relig gion and Heallth. 47(2), 1344–163. HASLAM, S. A. 2001. Psychology P in n organizationns: The sociaal identity appproach. Londoon, England: Saage. HASLAM, S. A., ELLEMERS, N., REICHER, R S. D, REYNOL LDS, K. J., & SCHMITT, M M. T. 2010. The social s identity y perspective tomorrow: O Opportunities and avenues ffor advance. IIn T. Postmes, & N. R. Bransccombe (Eds.), Rediscoveriing social ideentity: Key reaadings in sociial psychologyy (pp. 357-380 0). New York k, NY: Psychoology Press. HEWSTON NE, M., RUBIN, M., & WILLIS, H. 20002. Intergroup up bias. Annuaal Review of Psychologyy, 53, 575-604 4. HILTON, J. J L. & VON HIPPEL, W. 1996. Stereottypes. Annuaal Review of P Psychology, 47, 237272. Y, M. J. & HO OGG, M. A. 2002. 2 The effeects of status on subgroup relations. Briitish HORNSEY Journal of Social S Psycho ology, 41(2), 203-218. JACKSON N, J. W. 2002. The Relation nship Betweenn Group Idenntity and Interrgroup Prejuddice Is Moderated by Sociostrucctural Variation. Journal oof Applied Soccial Psycholoogy, 32, 908–9933. NSCOMBE, N. R., & MC CKIMMIE, B. M. 2005. Suuppressing JETTEN, J., SCHMITT,, M. T., BRAN valuation on group identiffication: The rrole of intergrroup differenttiation and the negativee effect of dev intragroup respect. r Journ nal of Experim mental Sociall Psychology,, 41(2), 208-2215. JOHNSON N, D., TERRY Y, D.J., & LOU UIS, W.R. 20005. Perceptioons of the inteergroup structture and anti-Asian prejudice p amo ong White Au ustralians. Grroup Processees & Intergrooup Relations,, 8(1), 5371. MMENDEY, A. A 2002. Sequuential or paraallel processees? A longituddinal field KESSLER,, T., & MUM study conceerning determ minants of iden ntity-manageement strategiies. Journal of Personalityy and Social Psycchology, 82(1), 75-88. KUNDA, Z. Z (1999). Soccial cognition: Making sennse of people. Cambridge, M MA: MIT Preess. LEONG, C.-H. C 2008. A multilevel research framew work for the analyses of atttitudes towarrd immigrantss. Internationa al Journal off Interculturall Relations, 322, 115-129. LEVINE, E.G. E & TARG G, E. 2002. Sp piritual correllates of functiional well-beiing in womenn with breast cancer. Integrativve Cancer Theerapies, 1(2), 166-174. F 2000. A review of thee effect of reliigious and sppiritual factorss on mortalityy and LUSKIN, F.M. morbidity with w a focus on o cardiovascu ular and pulm monary diseasse. Journal off Cardiopulmoonary Rehabilitation, 20(1), 8--15. MAJOR, B., B GRAMZOW W, R. H., MC CCOY, S. K.,, LEVIN, S., SCHMADER R, T., & SIDA ANIUS, J. 2002. Perceeiving person nal discrimination: The rolee of group staatus and legitiimizing ideoloogy. Journal of Personality P and a Social Psyychology, 82((3), 269-282. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 125 M MAXWELL-S SMITH, M.A.., & ESSES, V.M. V 2012. Assessing A indiividual differeences in the ddegree too which peoplle are committted to follow wing their belieefs. Journal oof Research inn Personalityy, 466(2), 195-209 9. M MCCONNEL F. 2003. Com mpliance, cultture, and the health h of Indiggenous peoplle. Rural and Remote Health h, 3(190). Avaailable: http:///www.rrh.org g.au M MCCULLOUG GH, M. E., HOYT, T. H., LARSON, L D. B., KOENIG G, H. G., & T THORESEN, C. (22000). Religio ous involvement and mortaality: A meta--analytic review. Health P Psychology, 199, 211– 2222. M MCGREGOR,, I., HAJI, R.,, KANG, S-J. 2008. Can in ngroup affirm mation relieve outgroup deerogation? Jo ournal of Expeerimental Soccial Psycholog gy, 44, 1395--1401 M MENIS, J. 198 89. Attitudes towards t schoo ol, chemistry students andd science amonng upper secoondary chhemistry stud dents and scien nce among up pper secondarry chemistry students in thhe United Stattes. Research in Sccience & Tech hnological Ed ducation, 7, 183–190. M MUELLER, P., PLEVAK, D., D & RUMM MANS, T. (20 001). Religiouus involvemennt, spiritualityy, and m medicine: Implications for clinical c practiice. Mayo Cliinic Proceedinngs, 76, 12255-1235. M MUMMENDE EY, A., KESS SLER, T., KL LINK, A., & MIELKE, M R. 11999. Strateggies to cope w with neegative sociall identity: Preedictions by social identity theory and reelative deprivvation theory.. Joournal of Perrsonality and Social S Psycho ology, 76(2), 229-245. M MUMMENDE EY, A., KLIN NK, A., MIEL LKE, R., WEN NZEL, M., & BLANZ, M. 1999. Socio-sttructural charaacteristics of intergroup reelations and id dentity managgement strateggies: results ffrom a field study in East E Germany y. European Journal J of Soccial Psycholoogy, 29(2-3), 2259–285. N NADLER, A. & HALABI, S. 2006. Interrgroup helpin ng as status reelations: Effeccts of status sttability, identiification, and type of help on receptivity y to high statuus groups hellp. Journal off P Personality an nd Social Psycchology, 91(1), 97-110. N NADLER, A., HARPAZ-G GORODEISKY Y, G., & BEN N-DAVID, Y Y. 2009. Defennsive helping: Threat to group p identity, ing group identifiication, statuss stability, andd common grooup identity aas deeterminants of o intergroup help-giving. h Journal J of Peersonality andd Social Psychhology, 97(5)), 8238334. N NIENS, U., & CAIRNS, E. 2002. Identitty managemen nt strategies iin Northern Irreland. The Jo Journal off Social Psych hology, 142(3 3), 371-380. O OUWERKERK K, J. W., DE GILDER, D., & DE VRIE ES, N. K. 200 0. When the ggoing gets touugh, thhe tough get going: g Social identification n and individu ual effort in inntergroup com mpetition. P Personality an nd Social Psycchology Bulleetin, 26(12), 1550-1559. 1 O OUWERKERK K, J. W., & ELLEMERS, E N. N 2002. The benefits of bbeing disadvanntaged: Peerformance-related circum mstances and consequences c s of intergroupp comparisonns. European Joournal of Soccial Psycholog gy, 32(1), 73-91. PA AGLIARO, S., S ALPARON NE, F. R., PA ACILLI, M. G., G & MUCCH HI-FAINA, A A. 2012. Manaaging a soocial identity threat: Ambiv valence toward the ingroup p as psycholoogical disengaagement. Social Ps Psychology, 43 3(1), 41-46. PA ARGAMENT T, K. I. (2001). Religious struggle s as a predictor p of m mortality. Arcchives of Interrnal M Medicine, 161,, 1881–1885. Paage 126 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® PAULHUS S, D.L., & CA AREY, J.M. 2011. The FAD D-Plus: Meassuring lay belliefs regardingg free will and related constructs. Journal J of perrsonality asses essment, 93(1)), 96-104. CI, M. 2005. Science S and Fundamentalis F sm. EMBO reeports, 6(12), 1106 – 1109. PIGLIUCC POLING, D.A., D & EVAN NS, E.M. 200 04. Religious belief, scienttific expertisee, and folk ecoology. Journal of Cognition C and d Culture, 4(3 3), 485-524. POSTMES, T. & BRAN NSCOMBE, N. N 2010. Sourrces of social identity. In T T. Postmes, & N. Branscomb be, (Eds.) Red discovering So ocial Identity:: Core Sourcees. Psychologgy Press. ROBERTS, L. M., SETT TLES, I. H., & JELLISON N, W. A. 20088. Predicting tthe strategic iidentity managemen nt of gender and a race. Iden ntity: An Interrnational Journal of Theorry and Researrch, 8, 269-306. E. Religion and Psychological Distress. 1990. Journaal for the Scientific Study of Religion, ROSS, C.E 29(2), 236-245. RS, D. 2009. Turning social identity thrreat into challlenge: Status stability and SCHEEPER cardiovascu ular reactivity y during inter--group compeetition. Journnal of Experim mental Social Psychologyy, 45(1), 228-2 233. SCHEEPER RS, D., & EL LLEMERS, N. N 2005. Whenn the pressuree is up: The asssessment of social identity threeat in low and d high status groups. Journnal of Experim mental Sociall Psychology, 41(2), 192-200. RS, D., ELLE EMERS, N., & SINTEMA AARTENSDIJJK, N. 2009. Suffering from m the SCHEEPER possibility of o status loss:: Physiologicaal responses tto social idenntity threat in hhigh status grroups. European Journal J of Soccial Psycholo ogy, 39(6), 10075-1092 SCHEEPER RS, D., SPEA ARS, R., DOO OSJE, B., & M MANSTEAD D, A. S. R. 20006. Diversity in ingroup bias: Structural faactors, situatio onal features, and social fuunctions. Jourrnal of Personnality and Social Psycchology, 90, 944-960. 9 SCHMADE ER, T., MAJO OR, B., ECCL LESTON, C. P., & MCCO OY, S. K. 2001. Devaluing domains in responsee to threatenin ng intergroup comparisons: Perceived leegitimacy andd the status vaalue asymmetry. Journal of Personality P an nd Social Psyychology, 80(55), 782-796. S, J. 2002. Steereotypic ingrroup bias as sself-defense aagainst relativve SCHMITT,, M. & MAES deprivation n: evidence fro om a longitud dinal study off the German unification prrocess. Europpean Journal of Social S Psycho ology, 32, 309 9-326. SEEMANN N, T., DUBIN N, L.F., & SEE EMANN, M. 2003. Religiosity/Spirituaality and Heallth. A critical reviiew of the eviidence for bio ological pathw ways. Americaan Psychologgist, 58(1), 533-63. SEITZ, A. 2010. 2 Is scien nce the new fundamentalis fu sm? Public leccture presentted at the exisstentialist society. Easst Melbourne, Australia. SIEGEL, M.A., M & RANNEY, M.A. 2003. 2 Developping the channges in attitudde about the reelevance of science (CARS) ( questtionnaire and assessing tw wo high school science classses. Journal oof Research in n Science Tea aching, 40(8),, 757-775. SLOAN, R. R P., & BAGIIELLA, E. (20 002). Claims about religioous involvemeent and healthh outcomes. Annals A of Beh havioral Med dicine, 24, 14––21. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 127 SL LOAN, R. P., BAGIELLA A, E., & POW WELL, T. (199 99). Religion,, spirituality, and medicinee. Laancet, 353, 66 64–667. SM MITH, T.B., MCCULLOU UGH, M. E., & POLL, J. (2003). Religiiousness and depression Evidence for a main effect and a the moderating influen nce of stressfuul life events.. Psychologiccal Bulletin, 129(4 4), 614 – 636 6. ST TOLBERG, T. T 2007. The Religio-scien ntific Framew works of pre-s ervice primarry teachers: A An annalysis of their influence on o their teachiing of sciencee. International Journal off Science Educcation, 299(7), 909-930 0. ST TOLBERG, T. T L., & FUL LLJAMES, P. 2003. An anaalysis of the cconceptual fraameworks utiilised byy undergraduate theology students when n studying science & religgion. Discoursse, 2(2), 167––199. TAJFEL, H. 19 978. Social caategorization,, social identiity and social comparison. Reprinted in T. Poostmes, & N. R. Branscom mbe (Eds.). (2010). Redisco overing sociaal identity: Keey readings inn social pssychology (pp p. 119-128). New N York, NY Y: Psycholog gy Press. TAJFEL, H. & TURNER, J. J C. 1986. Th he social identtity theory off intergroup b ehaviour. In S S. W Worchel, & W. W G. Austin (E Eds.), Psycho ology of interg group relationns (pp. 7–24)). Chicago, IL L: N Nelson-Hall. TURNER, J. C., C & HASLA AM, S. A. 200 01. Social iden ntity, organizzations, and leeadership. In M M. E. Turner (Ed.). Groups G at worrk: Theory an nd research (p pp. 25-65). M Mahwah, NJ, U US: Lawrencee Erlbaum Assocciates Publish hers. TURNER, J. C., C HOGG, M. M A., OAKES S, P. J., REICH HER, S. D., & WETHERE ELL, M. S. 19987. Rediscovering the social gro oup: A self-ca ategorization theory. Oxfoord, England: Blackwell. TURNER, J. C., C OAKES, P. P J., HASLAM M, S. A. & MCGARTY, M C C. A. 1994. Self and collecctive: C Cognition and social contex xt. Personalityy and Social Psychology P B Bulletin, 20, 4454-463. U UPAL, A., & LEGAULT, L L. L 2011. Scien ntific fundameentalism scalee. V Vail, K. E., III, Rothschild, Z. K., Weise, D., Solomon n, S., Pyszczyynski, T., & G Greenberg, J. (22010). Terrorr managemen nt theory and religiosity. Personality P annd Social Psycchology Revieew, 14, 844-94. V VAN VUGT, M., M & HART T, C. M. 2004.. Social identiity as social gglue: The origgins of group looyalty. Journa al of Personality and Socia al Psychologyy, 86(4), 585--598. V VAN ZOMER REN, M., POS STMES, T., & SPEARS, R. R 2008. Towaard an integraative social iddentity m model of collective action: A quantitative research syn nthesis of threee socio-psycchological peerspectives. Psychological P l Bulletin, 134 4, 504–535. V VAN ZOMER REN, M., SPE EARS, R., & LEACH, L C. W. W (2008). Exxploring psychhological m mechanisms off collective acction: Does reelevance of grroup identity influence how w people coppe with coollective disad dvantage? Brritish Journal of Social Psyychology, 47(22), 353-372. V VERKUYTEN N, M. 2005. Ethnic E group identification and group evvaluation amoong minority aand m majority group ps: Testing thee multiculturaalism hypotheesis. Journal of Personality ty and Social Ps Psychology, 88 8(1), 121-138 8. V VERKUYTEN N, M., & REIJJERSE, A. 20 008. Intergrou up structure an and identity m management among etthnic minority y and majority y groups: Thee interactive effects e of percceived stabiliity, legitimacyy, and peermeability. European E Jou urnal of Socia al Psychologyy, 38(1), 106-1127 Paage 128 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® VEZZALI, L., ANDRIG GHETTO, L., TRIFILETTII, E., & VISIN NTIN, E. P. 22012. Perceivving status (in)stability y in a low stattus group: The effects of iddentification oon explicit annd implicit inttergroup attitudes. So ocial Psychollogy, 43(1), 33-40. VON HIPP PEL, C. D. 2006. When peo ople would raather switch thhan fight: Ouut-group favorritism among temp porary emplo oyees. Group Processes & Intergroup R Relations, 9(4), 533-546. WEBER, U., U MUMMEN NDEY, A., & WALDZUS S, S. 2002. Perrceived legitimacy of interrgroup status differrences: Its preediction by reelative ingrouup protypicality. Europeann Journal of SSocial Psychologyy, 32(4), 449-4 470. WEBER, S.R., S PARGAM MENT, K.I., KUNIK, M.E E., LOMAX, J.W. II, & ST TANLEY, M.A. Psychological distress am mong religiou us nonbelieveers: A systematic review. JJournal of Religious Health, 51, 72-86. Y, R. 2010. Attheism and meental health. H Harvard Reviiew of Psychoology, 18(3), 190-194. WHITLEY WHOQOL SRPB Group p. 2002. WHO OQOL SRPB uusers manuall: Scoring andd coding for tthe WHOQOL SRPB field teest instrumentt. Geneva: W World Health O Organization. WHOQOL SRPB Group p. 2006. A cro oss-cultural sttudy of spirituuality, religioon, and personnal beliefs as componeents of quality y of life. Sociial Science & Medicine, 622, 1486–14977. WOHL, M.. J. A., BRAN NSCOMBE, N. N R., & REY YSEN, S. 2010. Perceivingg your group’ss future to be in jeoparrdy: Extinctio on threat indu uces collectivee angst and thhe desire to sttrengthen the ingroup. Personalityy and Social Psychology P Bu ulletin, 36(7) , 898-910. Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 129 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 130 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Annex A – Scienti S ific Fundame entalis sm Sca ale Scientifiic Fundam mentalism m Scale - U Upal and L Legault (20 011) 1. Wheneveer science and d religion con nflict, sciencee is right. 2. Evolution n is just a theeory similar to o intelligent ddesign.* 3. I don't reeally understaand how any reasonable r peerson can doubbt the scientiffic consensuss on issues such as global g warmin ng. 4. Science will w one day explain e all asp pects of realitty including rreligion. 5. I detest th he thought th hat we are justt physical enttities with no spiritual or m metaphysical ccomponent to our beeing.* 6. Religion is needed to explain thosee aspects of thhe universe thhat science caannot and willl never be able to ex xplain.* 7. Over tim me, modern scientific and raational thinkiing will continnue to replacee religion andd other superstitiious ideas. 8. Rational thought and scientific metthod are the oonly way to discover truthss about realityy. ubt science. 9. It is unreeasonable for people to dou 10. Of all th he theories an nd opinions off reality, sciennce is the besst and most rigorous set off principles we’ve got. g 11. Sciencee is only one of o many meth hods through w which to undderstand the w world around uus.* 12. It’s morre important to t rely on ourr personal opiinions and observations thaan to rely on tthe recomm mendations off the scientificc community .* 13. In orderr to make progress in life, we must consstantly fight ffor teaching oof science to sschool children n. 14. People should consid der all possiblle perspectivees of the worlld – spiritual, personal, artistic, etc – not justt the view of scientific s reasson.* 15. Althoug gh scientific theories t someetimes need too be revised, tthe ability of science to deescribe our world is the only reaal way to mak ke firm concluusions about rreality. 16. Sciencee is humanity’’s best hope for f progress. 17. It’s imp possible to reffute that scien nce is responssible for the ggreatest advanncements in hhuman existence. oud of my rattional thinking and my scieentifically infformed worldd view. 18. I am pro 19. We hav ve to be a littlee skeptical off some of the scientific claiims because sscientists alsoo have their ow wn agendas.* 20. Most allternative med dicine approaaches such as homeopathy are junk sciennce because tthey have not beeen shown to work w better thaan placebos. *: reverse scored s Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 131 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 132 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Annex B – Creatio C onism Scale Versio ons Creation nism Scale e - Francis s and Gee er (1999b)) I believe in n the scientificc view of the origins of thee world* God created d the world ass described in n the Bible Everything in the world was made by y natural forcees* The world was w made by God in 6 day ys each of 24 hours God created d the universee, including liiving creaturees, out of nothhing I accept thee idea of evolu ution creating g everything oover millions of years* God formed d man out of the dust of th he Earth God made woman w out off man’s rib God rested on the seventth day after he h had finishedd his work off creation Science dissproves the biblical accoun nt of creation** Scientists have h discovereed how the world w was madde* *Reverse co oded Creation nism Scale e – Astley y and Fran ncis (2010 0) The animalls and plants we w know todaay have evolvved from earliier species* All the adap ptations of liv ving things caan be explaineed by natural selection* I accept thee idea of evolu ution creating g everything oover millions of years* God created d all the speciies of animalss and plants ddirectly I believe th hat God made the world in six days of 244 hours God made woman w out off man’s rib *Reverse co oded Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 133 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 134 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Annex C – Streng S gth of B Beliefs s Meas sures Commitment to Beliefs B (CT TB; Maxwe ell-Smith & Esses, 2012) Belief Centtrality Nothing is more m importaant to me than n following m my beliefs My beliefs are the most important parrt of how I deefine myself aas a person My beliefs are very impo ortant to me Living the lifestyle l sugg gested by my beliefs b is my top priority My beliefs influence how w I spend my y time (e.g., thhe groups, asssociations andd/or events thhat I participate in) My beliefs influence thee important ch hoices I makee in my life My beliefs are reflected in the way I behave b I am confid dent that my beliefs b are true and valid I feel uncom mfortable wheen I do sometthing that goees against myy beliefs I act accord ding to my beliefs even if those t around m me think thatt I shouldn’t My beliefs do not have anything a to do o with who I am as a persoon. (reversed)) My beliefs offer the mosst accurate an nd ‘‘true’’ refllection of reallity I would nott hesitate to arrgue in favor of my beliefss if called upoon to do so Belief Tran nscendence My primary y concern in life l is to abidee by my belieefs; all other cconcerns are ssecondary Without my y beliefs, I wo ould have notthing Pursuing my m beliefs is of paramount importance, i eeven if someoone (possibly myself) losess their life in the proceess Those who hold beliefs opposite o to my m own are miisguided I would actt in accordancce with my beeliefs even if iit meant harm ming others People need d to adopt my y beliefs in orrder to see thinngs clearly When I believe in sometthing, it is wo orth going to aall possible leengths to defeend that belieff The potentiial consequen nce of hurting others wouldd not stop me from followiing my belieffs I give up my m free time in n order to eng gage in activitties related too my beliefs I spend my money in acccordance with h my beliefs It is difficult to convincee me that som mething I belieeve in is wronng Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 135 W World Health Organization Qu uality of Life L – Spirritual, Reliigious, an nd P Personal Beliefs B (W WHOQOL- SRPB) S P Please note: th his measure was w designed to t be used in conjunction w with the Worlld Health O Organization Quality Q of Life fe-100 (WHOQ QOL-100) Insstrument. In nstructions: The following questions ask k about your spiritual, s relig gious or persoonal beliefs annd how these beliefs haave affected your y quality of o life. These questions aree designed to bbe applicablee to people cooming frrom many diffferent culturees and holding g a variety off spiritual, reliigious or perssonal beliefs. If you foollow a particular religion, such as Judaaism, Christian nity, Islam orr Buddhism, yyou will probably annswer the folllowing questiions with you ur religious beeliefs in mind . If you do noot follow a paarticular reeligion, but still believe thaat something higher h and more m powerful exists beyonnd the physicaal and m material world d, you may answer the follo owing questio ons from that perspective. For example,, you m might believe in i a higher sp piritual force or o the healing g power of Naature. Alternaatively, you m may haave no belief in a higher, spiritual s entity y, but you maay have strongg personal belliefs or follow wings, suuch as beliefs in a scientific theory, a peersonal way of o life, a particcular philosopphy or a moraal and etthical code. W While some off these questio ons will use words w such as spirituality pplease answerr them in term ms of yoour own perso onal belief sy ystem, whetheer it be religio ous, spiritual oor personal. The following questions ask k how your beeliefs have afffected differeent aspects off your quality of life inn the past two o weeks. For example, e one question askss "To what exxtent do you ffeel connectedd with yoour mind bod dy and soul?" If you have experienced e th his very muchh, circle the nnumber next too "very m much". If you have not exp perienced this at all, circle the number nnext to "Not aat all". You shhould ciircle one of th he numbers in n between if you y wish to in ndicate your aanswer lies soomewhere bettween "N Not at all" and d "very much h". Questionss refer to the last l two week ks. Ittems: SP P1.1 To whatt extent does any a connectio on to a spiritu ual being helpp you to get thhrough hard tiimes? SP P1.2 To whatt extent does any a connectio on to a spiritu ual being helpp you to toleraate stress? SP P1.3 To whatt extent does any a connectio on to a spiritu ual being helpp you to underrstand others?? SP P1.4 To whatt extent does any a connectio on to a spiritu ual being provvide you withh comfort / reeassurance? SP P 2.1 To whaat extent do yo ou find meaniing in life? SP P2.2 To whatt extent does taking t care off other peoplee provide meaaning of life ffor you? SP P2.3 To whatt extent do yo ou feel your life has a purpo ose? SP P2.4 To whatt extent do yo ou feel you aree here for a reeason? SP P5.1 To whatt extent do yo ou feel inner spiritual s streng gth? SP P5.2 To whatt extent can you find spiritu ual strength in n difficult tim mes? SP P8.1 To whatt extent does faith f contribu ute to your weell-being? SP P8.2 To whatt extent does faith f give you u comfort in daily d life? SP P8.3 To whatt extent does faith f give you u strength in daily d life? SP P3.2 To whatt extent do yo ou feel spirituaally touched by b beauty? SP P3.3 To whatt extent do yo ou have feelin ngs of inspirattion / excitem ment in your liife? SP P3.4 To whatt extent are yo ou grateful fo or the things in n nature that yyou can enjoyy? SP P7.1 How hopeful do you feel? SP P7.2 To whatt extent are yo ou hopeful ab bout your life?? Paage 136 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® SP3.1 To what w extent aree you able to experience aw we from yourr surroundinggs? (e.g. naturre, art, music) SP4.1 To what w extent do o you feel any y connection bbetween yourr mind, body and soul? SP4.3 To what w extent do o you feel the way you livee is consistentt with what yoou feel and thhink? SP4.4 How w much do you ur beliefs help p you to creatte coherence bbetween whaat you do, thinnk and feel? SP5.3 How w much does spiritual s streng gth help you to live better?? SP5.4 To what w extent do oes your spirittual strength hhelp you to feeel happy in llife? SP6.1 To what w extent do o you feel peaaceful within yyourself? SP6.2 To what w extent do o you have inn ner peace? SP6.3 How w much are yo ou able to feell peaceful wh en you need tto? SP6.4 To what w extent do o you feel a seense of harmoony in your liife? SP7.3 To what w extent do oes being optiimistic improvve your qualiity of life? SP7.4 How w able are you u to remain op ptimistic in tim mes of uncerttainty? SP8.4 To what w extent do oes faith help you to enjoy life? SP4.2 How w satisfied are you that you have a balannce between m mind, body annd soul? As part of the t “About yo ou” portion off the WHOQO OL-100 instruument there aare five items about religion/spiirituality/personal beliefs: To what ex xtent do you consider yoursself to be a reeligious person? To what ex xtent do you consider yoursself to be partt of a religiouus communityy? If so, which h religious community are you a part off? To what ex xtent do you have h spiritual beliefs? To what ex xtent do you have h strong peersonal belieffs? Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 137 A Annex D – Sc cales measu m ring A Attitude es Tow ward Science S e A Attitude to owards Sc cience (Me enis, 1989) Imporrtance of Sciencce Subscale Sccience is useful for solving the problems p of everyyday life Sccience has ruineed the environmeent (reverse scored) Sccience is very im mportant for a country’s developm ment Money spent on science s is well worth spending Much of the anxieety in modern socciety is due to sccience (reverse scored) Sccientific inventions improve our standard s of livingg Sccientific inventions have increaseed tensions betw ween people (revverse scored) Sccience will help to t make the worrld a better placee in the future Sccientific discoverries do more harrm than good (reeverse scored) Sccience and technnology are the cause of many off the world’s problems (reverse sscored) Attitude towaard Science as a Career Subs cale W Working in a sciennce laboratory would w be an interresting way to eaarn a living In the future most jobs will requiree a knowledge off science Peeople who understand science are a better off in our o society It is important to know k science in order o to get a goood job In my future careeer, I would like too use the sciencee I learned in school Iw would like to beccome a science teacher t when I leave school Attitude toward Science in the School Curricculum Sccience in an enjooyable school suubject Thhe science taughht in school is intteresting Sccience is a difficult subject (reverse scored) Sccience is difficultt when it involves calculations (reverse scored) Sccience is difficultt when it involves handling appaaratus (reverse scored) s Thhere are too manny facts to learn in science (reveerse scored) Sccience is relevannt to everyday liffe Paage 138 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® Annex E – FAD-pl F lus (Pa aulhus s & Carrey, 20 011) For each staatement below w, choose a number n from 1 to 5 to indiccate how mucch you agree oor disagree. +1 +2 +3 +4 +5 Strongly disagree Strongly aggree 1. I believe that the futurre has already y been determ mined by fate. 2. People’s biological makeup m determ mines their tallents and perssonality. 3. Chance events e seem to o be the majo or cause of huuman history. 4. People have completee control overr the decisionss they make. 5. No matteer how hard you y try, you caan’t change yyour destiny. 6. Psycholo ogists and psy ychiatrists willl eventually ¿ ¿gure out all human behavvior. 7. No one can c predict wh hat will happeen in this worrld. 8. People must m take full responsibility y for any bad choices they make. 9. Fate already has a plan n for everyon ne. 10. Your geenes determin ne your futuree. 11. Life seeems unpredicttable—just lik ke throwing ddice or Àippinng a coin. 12. People can overcomee any obstaclees if they trully want to. 13. Whatev ver will be, wiill be—there’s not much yyou can do aboout it. 14. Sciencee has shown how h your pastt environmentt created yourr current intellligence and personality. 15. People are unpredicttable. 16. Criminaals are totally y responsible for f the bad thhings they do. 17. Whetheer people like it or not, myssterious forcees seem to moove their livess. 18. As with h other animalls, human beh havior alwayss follows the laws of naturre. 19. Life is hard h to predicct because it is almost totallly random. 20. Luck pllays a big rolee in people’s lives. 21. People have complette free will. 22. Parents’ character will determine the characterr of their childdren. 23. People are always att fault for theiir bad behavioor. 24. Childho ood environm ment will deterrmine your suuccess as an aadult. 25. What haappens to peo ople is a matteer of chance. 26. Strength h of mind can n always overrcome the boddy’s desires. 27. People’s futures cann not be predictted. Subscales 4 8, 12, 16, 21, 2 23, 26 Free Will: 4, Scienti¿c Determinism: D 2, 6, 10, 14, 18, 22, 24 Fatalistic Determinism: D 7 1, 5, 9, 13, 17 Unpredictab bility: 3, 7, 11, 15, 19, 20, 25, 27 Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 139 TH HIS PAGE IN NTENTIONALLY LEFT B BLANK. Paage 140 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems® DO OCUMENT CO ONTROL DATA A (Security classification of title,, body of abstract and a indexing annootation must be enttered when the oveerall document is cclassified) 1. ORIGINATOR R (The name and add dress of the organizattion preparing the doocument. Organizations for whom the docum ment was prepared, e.g g. Centre sponsoringg a contractor's rep port, or tasking agenccy, are entered in secction 8.) 2.. Humansy ystems Incorp porated 111 Farqu uhar Street Guelph, ON O N1H 3N4 3. SECURITY CL LASSIFICATION (Overall securityy classification of thee document including speciall warning terms if appplicable.) UNCLASS SIFIED (NON-CON NTROLLED G GOODS) DMC A GCEC June 2 2010 REVIEW: G TITLE (The co omplete document title as indicated on thee title page. Its classiification should be inndicated by the approopriate abbreviation ((S, C or U) in parentheses after the title.) A Revie ew of Socia al Science Literature o on Social IIdentity Dyn namics and Scientific Fundame entalism 4. AUTHORS (laast name, followed by initials – ranks, titlles, etc. not to be useed) Irene Che eung; Yvonne e DeWit; Emily y-Ana Filardo o; Michael H. Thomson; Ba arbara D. Ada ams 5. DATE OF PU UBLICATION (Month and year of publication of document.) d March 2012 7. 6a. NO. OF PAG GES 6b. NO. OF REF FS (Total containning information, (Total cited in document.) including Annnexes, Appendices, etc.) 15 54 100 0 DESCRIPTIV VE NOTES (The cateegory of the documen nt, e.g. technical repoort, technical note orr memorandum. If apppropriate, enter the ttype of report, e.g. interim, prrogress, summary, an nnual or final. Give th he inclusive dates whhen a specific reportinng period is covered.) Contract Report 8. SPONSORIN NG ACTIVITY (The name n of the departmeent project office or llaboratory sponsorinng the research and deevelopment – includee address.) Defence R&D R Canada – Toronto 1133 She eppard Avenu ue West P.O. Box 2000 O M3M 3B9 Toronto, Ontario 9a. PROJECT OR R GRANT NO. (If appropriate, a the appliicable research and developmeent project or grant nu umber under which the t document was written. Pllease specify whetherr project or grant.) 9b. CONTRACT NO. (If appropriate,, the applicable numbber under which the docuument was written.) W7711-0 088136/001/T TOR 10a. ORIGINATOR R'S DOCUMENT NUMBER N (The officiial document number by whiich the document is identified i by the originating activity. This number n must be uniqu ue to this document.)) [if used] 10b. OTHER DOC CUMENT NO(s). (A Any other numbers w which may be assigned this ddocument either by thhe originator or by thhe sponsor.) DRDC To oronto CR 20 012-077 11. DOCUMENT AVAILABILITY (An ny limitations on furtther dissemination off the document, otherr than those imposedd by security classificcation.) Unlimited 12. DOCUMENT ANNOUNCEMENT T (Any limitation to the bibliographic annnouncement of this ddocument. This will nnormally correspondd to the Document Avaailability (11). Howev ver, where further distribution (beyond thhe audience specifiedd in (11) is possible, a wider announcemeent audience may be b selected.)) Unlimited Humansystem ms® Social Iden ntity Dynamicss and Scientificc Fundamentaliism Page 141 13. ABSTRACT (A A brief and facttual summary of o the documen nt. It may also a appear elsewh ere in the bodyy of the document itself. It is highly desirable that th he abstract of classified c docu uments be unclassified. Each paragraph of the abstract sha all begin with an a indication off the security cl assification off the informatio on in the paragraph (unless the docum ment itself is un nclassified) represented as (S S), (C), (R), or ((U). It is not necessary to include herre abstracts in both b official lan nguages unless s the text is bili ngual.) The main purpose of the Human n Terrain Visualiz zation and Simula ation (HTVis) pro oject is to develo op computer toolss that can help Canadian decision mak kers envision and d simulate aspec cts of human terrrain. The presentt report reviews literature from Social S Identity The eory (SIT; Tajfel and Turner, 197 79) to help the de evelopment of su uch computer too ols for use in Canadian n Forces (CF) tra aining initiatives. This review was guided by three primary questio ns: 1. 2. 3. How do d sociostructura al beliefs influenc ce social identity management strrategies for high and low status group ps? How do d sociostructura al beliefs influenc ce intergroup perrceptions? How do d social identity y management sttrategies influenc ce intergroup perrceptions? The papers revie ewed show that beliefs about soc ciostructural variables (i.e., perce eptions of status stability, legitima acy, and permeability y) can influence identity managem ment strategies (i.e., ( social comp petition, individua al mobility, or soccial creativity). Rese earch suggests th hat, even though high and low sta atus groups mayy have different m motivations, theyy may use similar or different strategies s. But this is often dependent on the level of identtification with the e ingroup and the eir he sociostructura al context. Resultts of the literature review also sh owed some rese earch examining the perceptions of th impact of socios structural beliefs on intergroup pe erceptions. There e is very little ressearch that addre esses the impactt of identity manage ement strategies on intergroup pe erceptions. Howe ever, there is rese earch considerin ng other kinds of strategies, outside of the three core c strategies, and a how these im mpact perceptionss. Another goal o of the project was to examine psycho ological literature e pertaining to secular or scientific c fundamentalism m and to identify any relevant psychometric sc cales. Le projet de Visualisation et de simulation s de la dimension d humaine (HTVis) a po our objet de mett re au point des o outils informatiques po ouvant aider les décideurs canad diens à visualiserr et à simuler dess aspects de la d dimension humaiine. Le présent rapport examine la docu umentation sur la a théorie de l’iden ntité sociale (TIS S; Tajfel et Turne r, 1979) afin de permettre la mis se au point d’outils informatiques qui serviront dan ns le cadre des i nitiatives d’instru uction des Forces canadiennes (FC C). Cet examen se fondait sur tro ois grandes ques stions : 1. 2. 3. Quelle e influence les crroyances sociosttructurelles ont-e elles sur les straté égies de gestion n de l’identité socciale des groupes à statut supérieur s et inférrieur? Quelle e influence les crroyances sociosttructurelles ont-e elles sur les perce eptions intergrou upes? Quelle e influence les sttratégies de gesttion de l’identité sociale s ont-elles sur les perceptio ons intergroupess? D’après les docu uments examiné és, les croyances s relatives aux va ariables sociostru ucturelles (c.-à-d . les perceptionss en matière de stabiilité du statut, de légitimité, et de perméabilité) pe euvent influencerr les stratégies de e gestion de l’ide entité (c.-à-d. la concu urrence sociale, la mobilité individ duelle ou la créattivité sociale). La a recherche indiq que que, bien que e les groupes de statu ut supérieur et in nférieur puissent avoir des motiva ations différentess, ceux-ci peuven nt utiliser des stra atégies semblables ou différentes. d Mais cela dépend sou uvent du degré d’identification d avvec l’endogroupe e et de la percepttion du contexte sociosttructurel. Les rés sultats de l’exame en de la docume entation ont perm mis de cerner cerrtains travaux de recherche portant sur l’impact de es croyances soc ciostructurelles sur s les perception ns intergroupes. Il existe très peu u de travaux portant sur s l’incidence des stratégies de gestion de l’iden ntité sur les perce eptions intergrou upes. Cependantt, on trouve des trava aux examinant d’autres sortes de stratégies, en dehors des trois sstratégies de basse, et l’influence que ations de psycho celles-ci ont sur les perceptions. Le projet visait également é un au utre objectif : exa aminer les publica ologie relative au fonda amentalisme séc culier ou scientifiq que et recenser tout t barème psyychométrique perrtinent. 14. KEYWORDS, DES SCRIPTORS or IDE ENTIFIERS (Technically meaningful terrms or short phrases tthat characterize a doocument and could bbe helpful in cataloguin ng the document. Theey should be selected d so that no security classification c is requiired. Identifiers, suchh as equipment modeel designation, trade naame, military project code name, geograp phic location may also o be included. If posssible keywords shouuld be selected from a published thesaurus,, e.g. Thesaurus of En ngineering and Scien ntific Terms (TEST) and that thesaurus iddentified. If it is not ppossible to select indexing terms whicch are Unclassified, th he classification of each should be indicaated as with the title.)) social identity y; human terrrain; socio-culltural modelin ng Paage 142 Social Ideentity Dynamiccs and Scientiffic Fundamentaalism Humanssystems®