Unit Two Understanding Adolescent Development
Transcription
Unit Two Understanding Adolescent Development
Unit Two Understanding Adolescent Development Unit Goal To provide officers with a basic understanding of healthy adolescent development across multiple domains and how adolescent development can affect youth behavior Scope In this unit, officers will gain an understanding of how the brain develops and receive an overview of adolescent social, emotional, and physical development. Officers will learn about how development across these various domains impacts youth behavior and what the most recent adolescent brain research reveals about adolescent behavior. Performance Objectives At the conclusion of the unit, officers will be able to: 2-1 Name the developmental processes that take place during adolescence. 2-2 Describe the impact of brain research on understanding adolescent behavior. 2-3 Identify specific developmental disruptions often occurring in adolescence. Materials Video 2.1: The Wiring of the Adolescent Brain (clip length: 5:58) from Frontline’s Inside the Teenage Brain series, which is described at the end of the module Case Study: Tony Case Study: Henry Video 2.2: What fMRI Scans Tell Us About the Adolescent Brain (clip length: 3:24) * Handout: MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice Issue Brief 3: Less Guilty by Reason of Adolescence Video 2.3: Peer Influence and Adolescent Behavior (clip length: 4:04) * * Requires access to the Internet 9 Unit Outline I. Objectives II. Defining Adolescence III. Adolescent Development a. Cognitive Development b. Moral Development i. Case Study (Tony) c. Social and Identity Development d. Physical Development i. Case Study (Henry) e. Brain Development IV. Differences between an Adolescent and an Adult a. Self-Control b. Short-Sightedness c. Susceptibility to Peer Pressure V. Important Considerations VI. Disruptions in Normal Development VII. Implications a. Purpose of Criminal Punishment b. Mitigation, Not Excuse 10 Training Aids Slide 2-1 Understanding Adolescent Development Slide 2-2 Objectives Describe and discuss basic adolescent development across multiple areas (cognitive, moral, social, physical) Describe the most recent brain development research and what it reveals about adolescent behavior Explore developmental disruptions and their potential impact on behavior Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Describe your expertise in adolescent development. Review the objectives of this unit, which are to provide information on the developmental process of adolescence and to identify areas that influence that process. Definition of adolescence Identification of stages and key areas of adolescent development: cognitive, moral, social and identity, and physical Examination of key developmental changes at each stage of adolescence Impact of new brain development research and information on juvenile justice policy and practice Exploration of disruptions in normal adolescent development This unit will: familiarize officers with the developmental processes that take place in adolescence; enhance officers’ ability to identify changes that occur during adolescence that will impact interaction with youth; and foster officers’ understanding of how to promote positive outcomes for youth when officers are called to respond and intervene. 11 Training Aids Slide 2-3 Video 2.1: The Wiring of the Adolescent Brain Slide 2-4 Defining Adolescence What does adolescence mean to you? What words would you use to describe this stage of life? Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Training Aid: Video 2.1 – The Wiring of the Adolescent Brain (clip length: 5:58) Introduce video clip from Frontline’s episode, Inside the Teenage Brain. The video clip introduces key concepts involved with adolescent development. In particular, it explains early brain development and how this relates to risk and behavior. (More information on the video is provided at end of module.) As stated by Dr. Laurence Steinberg, head of the MacArthur Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice, before the U.S. Senate Judiciary Committee in 2007: “We have always known that adolescents behave differently than adults. Young people are more impulsive, more shortsighted, more willing to take risks, and more susceptible to the influence of their peers. Anyone who has raised a teenager, taught a teenager, counseled a teenager, or been a teenager knows this. Scientific discoveries about brain development have helped us understand why this is true, but they haven’t changed the basic story line. Those who founded a separate system of juvenile justice in America some 100 years ago had it right, even without the benefit of brain scans, when they made a commitment to treating young people who have violated the law differently than how we treat adults. It is a commitment that we need to reaffirm today. If brain research helps us do this, I’m all too happy to tell you what scientists are learning.” Ask officers to work with others at their table to write a definition of adolescence and choose a spokesperson to present the definition. Allow three minutes to complete this task. After three minutes, ask each spokesperson to present definition. Write down key words on board/flip chart. 12 Training Aids Slide 2-5 Adolescent Development Adolescence begins around ages 10 to 13 and involves changes in cognitive development (thinking), moral development, social and identity development, physical appearance (puberty), and brain development. Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Explain that there are a number of beliefs about adolescence as a phase of development between childhood and adulthood, as well as a number of beliefs on when adolescence begins and ends. Most people view adolescence as a complex “growing up” period between childhood and adulthood. The dictionary defines adolescence as the period of physical and psychological development between puberty and maturity. “Psychological” often refers to thinking and feeling and “puberty” to physical and emotional changes. Adolescence begins around the ages of 10 to 13, ends between19 to 21, and involves significant changes in thinking, feeling, social skills, physical appearance, and moral beliefs. During this period, youth are gearing up for adult responsibilities. (Refer back to the officers’ ideas on defining adolescence and demonstrate how their terms relate.) Adolescence involves moving toward independent thinking and living. Remind officers that adolescents are on their way; they are not there yet. They need structure, supervision, safety, support, and guidance for healthy development. Slide 2-6 Adolescent Development Areas can overlap, develop, and grow at different rates in individual youth. Therefore, we must consider the individual characteristics of each adolescent. Emphasize to officers that each of these areas can overlap, develop, and grow at different rates. Explain that because each dimension of development can develop differently in any adolescent, youth may present very differently over time and have not yet formed the “package” of individual characteristics they will have as adults. A physically mature adolescent may function as a younger person cognitively, socially, or emotionally. A physically immature adolescent may be more mature intellectually or morally. Because the pace of maturity and the interactions of the developmental dimensions 13 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes are specific to each individual, each adolescent must be considered individually. Slide 2-7 Cognitive Development Changes in the way individuals learn and think about the world around them, how they take perspective on their interactions with others, and how they make decisions Slide 2-8 Cognitive Development During this period, adolescents become better able to take the perspective of others; consider alternatives or likely consequences; be reflective in their decision-making; and begin to question assumptions and perceptions. 14 Cognitive development refers to the way individuals learn and think about the environment/ world around them (Seifert & Hoffnung, 1994). If a 4-year-old child doesn’t follow signs posted on a bus, is he or she held responsible? No, because the child’s cognitive development has not formed enough to allow reading. At about age 11 or 12, adolescents can think about that which cannot be seen, heard, tasted, smelled, or touched (abstract ideas). They begin to think about their own thoughts and focus on what could be (Craig, 1999). Adolescents can imagine, for example, what they would be like if they were born into a rich versus a poor family (Seifert & Hoffnung, 1994). They have a developing ability to plan and think ahead. However, this ability is not fully formed. Adolescents’ ability to take the perspective of others is still developing, as is the ability to identify relevant information or to consider likely short- and longer term consequences for their decisions. However, these are developing abilities that are still maturing. As they mature, adolescents may be uneven in their ability to take the perspectives of others or in the thoughtfulness of their decisions. Training Aids Slide 2-9 Cognitive Development Other characteristics of cognitive development that can compromise a youth’s ability to “think like an adult” include the following: Errors in perceptions of risk Prone to sensation-seeking Short-sighted, “present-centered” thinking Self-centered, egocentric thinking Sense of personal invulnerability Unrealistic, even “magical” thinking Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Adolescents also share developmental qualities that make their thinking and decision-making different than those of mature adults. Adolescents tend to perceive and weigh risks differently; are more prone to sensationseeking; and are more likely to be shortsighted than adults. For example, they are more likely to engage in a thrill-seeking or risky experience “in the moment” without considering the consequences. Adolescents tend to be more self-centered in their thinking and believe that bad consequences for actions or decisions will not actually happen to them. For example, many teens can tell you it is a bad idea to drive without a seatbelt, but they are less likely to believe that they personally would be injured in a car accident because they were not wearing a seatbelt. Adolescents also have a greater tendency to “convince themselves” that their perceptions or judgments are the right ones, even in the face of much contradicting information. Note to Instructor: Head injury can have a tremendous effect on the way people develop cognitively and morally. Ask officers if they know anyone who has suffered from brain damage (e.g., a head injury sustained in a car accident). Discuss how that person differs in thinking and comprehending. Point out that it is likely that officers will encounter a youth who has suffered head injury and it is certain that they will encounter adolescents whose brains have not fully developed and may appear very similar to adults with brain injury. Slide 2-10 Moral Development Moral development has to do with the values (personal beliefs) on which to base decisions concerning right and wrong or good and bad (moral decisions) (Daeg de Mott,1998). 15 Training Aids System of values (personal beliefs) on which to base decisions concerning right and wrong Gradual move from moral decisions based mostly on rules or consequences to themselves …. - “I am supposed to…” - “I will get in trouble if I…” …to decisions based on empathy with others or principles. - “How would I feel if I was in this situation?” - “What would be fair in this situation?” Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Ask officers what influences a youth’s values and beliefs (e.g., peers, church, parents, grandparents, school, coaches, etc.). Point out that many of these things or people influence the teens with whom officers work, but sometimes such influences are largely absent. Between the ages of 11 and 12, adolescents begin to try to work out moral problems by seeking feedback and reinforcement from the people around them. As adolescents mature, they shift from caring about what others think to what society says is right or wrong, and they rely more on their personal moral beliefs (Vasta, Haith & Miller, 1995). Research shows that adolescent offenders often function at lower levels in their moral development than adolescents of the same social background who did not commit crimes. If adolescents are engaged in moral decisionmaking with others who have not reached higher levels of moral development, then they, too, are unlikely to reach higher levels of moral development. 16 Training Aids Slide 2-11 Moral Development Moral development is often deeply shaped by the values and moral decisions of parents, extended family members, peers, community members, and others. This is one reason for the variation in adolescents’ senses of right and wrong, fairness and justice, loyalty or obligation, accounting for the needs of others, and the morality of certain behaviors. Slide 2-12 CASE STUDY: Tony Tony is a 16-year-old boy. His cousin robs a local convenience store and later asks Tony to hide him. Tony agrees and is subsequently charged with “Accessory After the Fact.” He is offered a plea deal, but only if he will testify against his cousin. Tony feels protective of his cousin. Tony also believes that the convenience store has been a front for drug dealing, which he thinks at least some local police officers know and have been “on the take” from the store owner. Tony’s mother tells him to “do the right thing, tell the truth” and to testify. His lawyer tells him to take the plea and let his cousin fend for himself. Tony’s older brother tells him to be loyal to his cousin and refuse the plea agreement. Tony worries that if he is perceived as a “snitch,” people might hurt him or his Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Adolescents build their sense of moral development from many sources, but most powerfully from observing the values and moral decisions of their families, peers, and others whom they admire or respect. Adolescents don’t always receive a consistent message about what is “right,” what is “wrong,” and what to consider in making moral decisions. Many have to reconcile competing messages about values, morals, and moral decision-making. As a result, many adolescents make different decisions at different times, depending upon who they are with or the moral direction they have received. How many of us have experienced a “basically good kid” who made a selfish, misguided, or even tragic decision that involved “right and wrong,” not considering the impact of his/her behavior upon others? Consider asking an officer to read aloud the case study of Tony (presented as a handout at the end of this module.) The purpose of the case study is to elicit officers’ thoughts on the many potential influences on an adolescent’s decision. In this scenario, Tony is receiving conflicting advice from different sources with whom he has different kinds of relationships. He must also consider a range of possible consequences resulting from his decision. Ask officers to comment on: As a 16-year-old boy, how might Tony look at this situation? What might play a role in his decision: - Tony’s relationship with his cousin, including Tony’s resentment for putting him into this situation? - The advice from Tony’s mother? - The advice from his attorney? - The advice from his brother? 17 Training Aids family. He resents that his cousin got him into this situation in the first place. Slide 2-13 Social and Identity Development Adolescents undergo changes in how they perceive and experience themselves and others, including themselves as members of a broader community with ethnic and cultural, socioeconomic, political, and other factors to their identity; their ability to function on their own; relationships with family, peers, and community; and themselves as emerging adults, including as persons with sexual identities and interests. Content/Instructional Delivery Notes - Tony’s perception that some police are corruptly involved? - His fear of retaliation if seen as a “snitch”? A core developmental task for adolescents is to develop a consistent sense of both social and individual identity. Adolescents typically explore means by which they experience themselves as individuals and how others perceive them. Adolescents construct a sense of the broader social world and how they fit into that world. They develop social identities shaped by their understanding of cultural, ethnic, socio-economic, political, and other factors. They can and do think about how their lives would be different if they were of a different race or ethnicity, were richer or poorer, were of another family or community, or lived in a different time or place. Adolescents begin to explore their ability to function on their own, even as they remain connected in different ways with their families, peers, and members of their community. They begin to try different roles and identities and to consider how others respond to them when they adopt these new roles. This also involves thinking about themselves as young adults, including the emergence of themselves as individuals with sexual identities and interests. If time permits, consider with officers how adolescents explore and experiment with social and personal identities (e.g., embracing or rejecting clothing and hairstyles, different kinds of friends, preferences in music or leisure activities, and values and beliefs). Ask officers how an adolescent’s exploration of identity might be shaped by cultural, social, economic, or other factors. 18 Training Aids Slide 2-14 Social and Identity Development Sense of purpose Participating in activities and gaining acknowledgement for participation Pro-social behavior rewarded; deviant behaviors punished Sense of control The youth’s sense of control moves from external to internal. (“My world is controlled by others” changes to “My behaviors determine what happens to me.”) Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Facilitate discussion of how activities, such as sports, arts, hobbies, community service, and jobs, contribute to a sense of purpose. Gaining acknowledgment is powerfully reinforcing and the rate of reinforcement is a powerful predictor of behavior. Schools can offer positive experiences in academics, sports, or other extra-curricular activities. Communities can offer experiences through church activities, clubs, and organizations. All of these can help build children’s self-confidence. Ask officers how their interaction with juveniles can promote “purpose” in their lives. Discuss the idea of control and the importance of consistency in an adolescent’s life. The more an adolescent can anticipate the results of behavior, the greater his/her sense of “control.” Adolescents are in the process of learning how to “think before doing.” A sense of control is promoted when adults consistently reward or recognize pro-social/positive/good behaviors and consistently discipline or ignore deviant/ destructive/wrong behaviors. Consistent feedback lets adolescents correctly predict the result of their actions. They know exactly what to expect before they act. CONSISTENCY is key and requires adults who are actively engaged in the lives of youth. Point out how a youth’s sense of control changes from the external to the internal as a developmental process, moving from “others decide for me” to “I decide.” Ask officers how their interventions with youth can promote a sense of “control” for the developing youth. 19 Training Aids Slide 2-15 Social and Identity Development Belonging Monitoring and supervising activities Talking about interests and problems Personal Identity Youth’s own characteristics and competencies Youth’s experience of being valued and successful in relationships for themselves and what they can bring to relationships Content/Instructional Delivery Notes A good connection with positive adult role models often predicts healthy development. A sense of belonging comes from the belief that parents and/or other adults from the community are interested and want to be involved. Adults can nurture this sense of belonging by monitoring and supervising adolescents’ activities and encouraging adolescents to talk about their interests and problems. Adolescents who have a sense of belonging or connection with good adult role models are less likely to engage in delinquent behaviors and are more likely to do well in school. Define the term “identity.” In adolescence, issues such as “Who am I?” and “Where am I going?” are important. Feeling competent and useful contributes to a positive identity. In addition to a sense of control, a positive sense of self grows from relationships, community involvement, parental and adult support, and respect for individual opinions in the family (CSR, Inc., 1997). Slide 2-16 Social and Identity Development Sexual Identity Understanding one’s own sexual orientation and interests and how they relate to one’s social environment Developing ways of expressing one’s own sense of masculine or feminine and of sexuality Social Skills Developing ability to express needs, create mutual relationships, manage conflicts Sense of capacity to bring personal characteristics to relationships and to experience success, affirmation 20 Developing a sexual identity is an important aspect of development. Youth develop an understanding of their own sexual orientation and interests, often while sorting through competing messages about sexuality from sources as diverse as parents and family members, peers, community members, and mass and electronic media. Adolescents develop a fundamental identity as heterosexual, lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgendered. In doing so, they may explore different kinds of sexual activity with different partners. They also sort out how their sexual identity fits into their immediate and broader social environment. This includes developing a sense of how accepted their own sexual identity is among those around them and in the Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes community. Youth who experience intolerance or even direct hostility to themselves or others who share their sexual identity can be at increased risk for depression, substance abuse, and even suicide. Adolescents develop their own understanding of what it is to be masculine or feminine, and how to express their sexuality. They often rely upon social/cultural expectations, media images, and role models as they develop an identity. For example, adolescent males must determine what it is to be “a man” and what attitudes, values, beliefs, and behaviors best express “manhood.” As children move into adolescence, social interactions change. Different social skills are needed as new levels of friendship are formed, relationships with parents and family change, and new social situations arise. Adolescents seek acceptance by a peer or groups they admire. Instead of turning to the family for support, they begin to turn to friends. Adolescents begin to “date” and engage in romantic relationships – a major marker of social development in adolescence (Van Hasselt & Hersen, 1987). Adolescents with persisting problem behaviors often lack the social skills necessary to develop more appropriate conduct. Studies indicate that delinquent adolescents often lack adequate social and problem-solving skills and tend to have more problems finding good social situations. They may be less likely to problemsolve well due to poor social judgment and more difficulty resisting bad peer influences. Ask officers what other major social events occur in adolescence, discussing events such as jobs, school socials/sports, and the ability to drive. Discuss what connections youth will make if there are no concerned, engaged adults. Where will 21 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes adolescents seek belonging and acceptance (gangs, other delinquent youth)? Slide 2-17 Physical Development What happens during adolescence? There is an increase in hormone production in both boys and girls, which triggers changes in brain development. Hormonal changes are reflected in physical changes, but also contribute to experiences that impact emotional development. Development occurs at different rates: some early, others later. Slide 2-18 Physical Development Factors influencing onset of puberty include individual differences (e.g., genetics), nutritional status and medical conditions, gender differences, and racial/ethnic differences. 22 Explain that adolescent development differs among individuals and between genders. Ask officers what physical changes happen at around age 10 to 12 and what activates these changes. (Write responses down on a flip chart.) Physical changes in adolescents are activated by the increase of hormone production at about age 10½ for girls and 12 to 13 for boys (Craig, 1999). For girls, this includes a growth spurt, menstruation, pubic hair growth, development of breasts, etc. Girls are now ovulating and capable of getting pregnant. For boys, the physical changes include a growth spurt, enlargement of the testes, deepening of the voice, and pubic hair growth. Between the ages of 11 and 16, male adolescents experience the first discharge of semen (sperm). Boys are now always fertile and can produce a child if sexually active. Youth enter puberty at all different ages, from late childhood into mid-adolescence. Factors influencing the onset of puberty for any given adolescent include individual genetic heritage, nutritional status (poorly nourished children tend to have delayed puberty), and gender differences. While there is broad variation among individuals, girls tend to begin puberty sooner than boys, as noted earlier. Racial/ethnic differences are also associated with different average ages for onset of puberty. In summary, the onset of puberty and subsequent physical changes associated with puberty are highly individual. Training Aids Slide 2-19 Physical Development The onset of puberty impacts emotions and mood-regulation; need for increased sleep; physical maturity and sexual development; family, peer, and community relationships; and expectations of others for “more mature” ways of thinking and behaving. Content/Instructional Delivery Notes The onset of puberty has a broad impact on adolescents. Since puberty involves significant changes in hormones and hormone levels, adolescents typically have greater variation in their emotions and more challenges in regulating their moods. Adolescents need more sleep to support the significant physical changes that are underway. Many adolescents end up getting less sleep than they need or sleep for prolonged periods during less scheduled times (e.g., on weekends) to “catch up” on missed sleep. Family, peer, and community relationships change as youth become increasingly more “adult-like” in physical appearance. This often includes expectations for “more mature” ways of thinking and behavior on the part of the adolescent. However, as we have seen, physical maturity does not necessarily indicate that the youth has achieved similar levels of cognitive, social, or emotional maturity. Slide 2-20 Physical Development Early maturing boys Popular Good in sports Positive self-esteem More likely to be leaders IMPACT of early maturing for a boy can be: Expected to behave as if older May slip into risky behavior earlier Discuss what happens when boys mature early physically in terms of thinking, social behavior, emotions, or moral understanding. Early maturing boys typically gain social and athletic advantages. They’re popular and are good in sports. Many report positive selfesteem. They like who they are and are more likely to be leaders of their peer groups (Craig, 1999; White, 1999). But these advantages come with the potential disadvantage of being expected to behave as if they are more mature in all developmental dimensions than they really are. Boys with early physical maturation are at risk of adopting “older” behaviors earlier – such as sexual behavior, experimentation with alcohol or drugs, or spending time with older youth who 23 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes may be engaging in risky behaviors themselves. Ask officers to consider what might be happening to a boy developing more slowly. Slide 2-21 Physical Development Late maturing boys Initially smaller and less muscular – disadvantage in sports Treated as younger than age; perceived as less competent by adults Lower social status among peers IMPACT of late maturing for a boy can be: May be more prone to anti-social behavior Compared to both male and female peers, the late maturing boy is usually smaller, less muscular, not as athletic, and has lower social status among his peers. He may be perceived as a younger child by adults and treated as less competent or intelligent. In reaction or to compensate, he may act in an immature, dependent manner to get special “breaks” for behavior from adults. Alternatively, he may display what he believes to be status-gaining behavior, including high levels of aggression, to establish himself (Craig, 1999; White, 1999). May have lower self-esteem due to being teased by same-age peers May be childish, dependent, and immature Can become highly aggressive in an effort to establish himself Slide 2-22 Physical Development Early maturing girls Earlier puberty and menstruation Taller and more developed than peers IMPACT of early maturing girls can be: Harder to find female friends they can relate to More popular with older boys and date earlier 24 The opposite often occurs for girls. Early maturing girls are taller and more developed, report feeling attractive, are more popular with older peers, and are more likely to “date” than their late maturing peers. However, girls who mature early often have less in common with their friends and experience distress regarding the physical changes of puberty, resulting in feeling isolated. (Girls who experience their menstrual period early and who were not prepared by their parents describe it as traumatic) (Craig, 1999; White, 1999). They may be sexually teased or approached by male peers and older boys. Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes More likely to experience stress about physical changes (menstrual period) Slide 2-23 Physical Development Late maturing girls Later onset of puberty/physical maturation Late maturing girls, on the other hand, are more popular among peers, report higher self-esteem, and are more focused on academics (White 1999). Facilitate discussion on why this might be the case. IMPACT of late maturing girls can be: Later onset of puberty means development more closely parallels that of boys Adults are more protective of them More popular among peers Higher self-esteem Tend to be more focused on academics Slide 2-24 CASE STUDY: Henry Henry is a 15-year-old white male who is small for his age. His mother and aunt refer to him as their “little man.” Henry has few friends and has never played any type of organized sports or participated in clubs/organizations at school or in the community. Teachers report that Henry has a history of being aggressive toward other youth and teachers, and appears to be slower to understand school information than his classmates. Ask officers to read just the first paragraph of the case study handout and underline all items related to the physical aspects of adolescents previously mentioned. (The case study is presented as a handout at the end of this module. Paragraphs 2 and 3 will be discussed later). Invite the officers to share and discuss their findings and point out elements they may have missed. 25 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Slide 2-25 Brain Development Brains of adolescents begin a complex and prolonged process of change and development that will persist into young adulthood. Monitoring and supervising activities Talking about interests and problems Many of the characteristics of adolescents reflect these brain changes. Slide 2-26 What Science Tells Us About the Teen Brain Functioning of the frontal lobes is not at adult levels. (Steinberg, 2008) Perhaps the most important development in recent years for understanding adolescents has been research on adolescent brain development. This research is what some people call a “paradigm shift” or a “game changer.” Research on adolescent brain development provides evidence of biological underpinnings of the characteristics of adolescence. Adolescents are not just miniature adults. The ways in which they differ from adults in thinking, feeling, deciding, and behaving are reflected in the maturation of their brains for over a decade (or more) after the onset of puberty. The most important conclusion to emerge from recent research is that brain development continues long after childhood, well into the early adult years. In fact, scientists now believe that adolescence may be as important in brain development as the first three years of life, a developmental stage that has received far more attention from researchers, journalists, and policymakers (Steinberg, 2007). Interacting with teens can be frustrating because they are so impulsive and often don’t plan. Brain research, brain scans (e.g., MRI), and other developmental science research offer explanations. Physically, the brain of a teenager is still developing. The size of the brain is as big as it is going to get, but it is not fully developed. Why is this important? Teens may simply not be able to handle structured planning (otherwise referred to as “executive functioning”) because the area of the brain (note arrow on slide) that coordinates impulse control is the last part of the brain to develop. Ask officers to respond to the following: What implications does knowing that the brains of adolescents are not fully developed have on your work with them? 26 Training Aids Slide 2-27 Suggested Video 2.2: What fMRI Scans Tell Us About the Adolescent Brain Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Note to Instructor: To explore brain development in more detail, consider playing the What fMRI Scans Tell Us About the Adolescent Brain video (clip length: 3:24). This video requires access to the Internet. Start the video by clicking on the screenshot on PowerPoint slide 2-27, which links to the video on the Internet. Introduction to suggested video: Teens’ developing ability to engage in structured planning is described in this video clip, which is from episode two of the Brains on Trial with Alan Alda series (Chedd-Angier Production Company, 2013). In this clip, Dr. Jay Giedd of the National Institute of Mental Health shares what he has learned from his many years studying adolescent brain development. Slide 2-28 Implications Because the brains of teenagers are not fully developed, some of their behaviors may reflect this immaturity. Specifically, the frontal lobe plays a critical role in planning, The frontal lobe plays a critical role in planning, problem-solving, reasoning, and emotional regulation. Although it plays such a critical role in these key functions, it is the last part of the adolescent brain to mature. Research indicates that the frontal lobe continues to mature in boys until the mid20’s. problem-solving, reasoning, and emotional regulation. Slide 2-29 Implications Recall your own teenage behavior: Did you ever do anything that could have gotten you stopped by the police? Or that could have resulted in harm to you or somebody else if things had gone wrong? Note to Instructor: The bullet points on the slide are rhetorical questions; don’t ask for selfdisclosure by the officers. Since the frontal lobe is the biological underpinning for “adult-like” thinking and emotional regulation and since it continues to mature through early young adulthood, it is not realistic to expect an adolescent to be capable of behaving as an adult. However, adolescents may be more capable of thinking and managing 27 Training Aids How is adolescent brain development likely to be reflected in adolescent behavior? Content/Instructional Delivery Notes their emotions like adults when they have support, have reliable feedback, and are not being asked to make decisions “in the moment” of emotionally-charged circumstances. List how the adolescent brain differs from an adult brain (decision-making in risky situations, social situations, and legal situations). Discuss how these differences impact how we interact with adolescents. Slide 2-30 HANDOUT: MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice Issue Brief 3: Less Guilty by Reason of Adolescence Describe the research by the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice chaired by Dr. Steinberg. Researchers in the network studied over 900 individuals ages 10 to 30 (NOT in the juvenile or criminal justice systems). The participants took part in performance and selfreport measures of planning, preference for immediate gratification, impulsivity, risk processing, sensation-seeking, and susceptibility to peer pressure. The findings, as detailed on the next several slides, demonstrate differences in how teens think and are influenced as compared to adults. Slide 2-31 Types of Thinking that Will “Change” between Adolescence and Adulthood Dr. Larry Steinberg found key differences between adolescent and adult perception and thinking that correlate with adolescent brain development: Self-control (impulsivity) Short-sightedness (risk-taking) Susceptibility to peer pressure and other factors that contribute to rapidly made, often poor, decisions in emotional circumstances 28 Teens are more prone to risk-taking because they are more impulsive. They are less able to understand or anticipate consequences. They are motivated by social approval and affiliation. Fitting in with their peers sometimes becomes more important than almost anything else in their lives. Specific examples are depicted on the following slides. Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Slide 2-32 Self-Control: Impulsivity Declines with Age Adolescents and young adults were given a test to self-report their willingness to react in a quick, rash manner or more carefully think through their next decision/move. From this study of “normal” teens, we see that impulsivity declines with age, with a notable reduction in impulsivity around age 16-17, but still has a substantial impact well into early adulthood (ages 26-30). (Steinberg et al., 2008) Slide 2-33 Self-Control: Time Spent Thinking about Problems Increases with Age (Steinberg et al., 2008) In a measure of impulsivity, participants were given problems that were both simple and complex in nature. They were then timed to see how long they took to solve those problems. On the simpler problems, all age groups were very similar, taking about the same amount of time to decide their move. However, on the more complex problems, only the older age category of young adults (18-30) took the additional time to decide their move. The younger age groups (10-15) took virtually the same amount of time to decide to move, ignoring the difficulty and moving impulsively instead of strategically. This is a great example of why so many teens when asked, “What were you thinking?” respond with “I don’t know” or “Nothing.” They are telling the truth. They probably weren’t thinking at any more of a sophisticated level when they engaged in that behavior then when they were deciding what to eat for breakfast that morning. 29 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Slide 2-34 Sensation-Seeking Declines with Age And, not surprisingly, as youth get older, they not only spend more time thinking about their problems and what to do, they also are less prone to thrill or novelty seeking. Adolescents between the ages of 12-17 are more likely to choose/endorse sensation-seeking options, but as youth get older, they are less likely to engage in sensation-seeking behavior. (Steinberg et al., 2008) Slide 2-35 Risk Perception Declines and Then Increases after Mid-adolescence Adolescents are more prone to taking risks. In addition, their perception of risk is faulty. As adolescents’ interests in and preference for engaging in sensation-seeking or risky situations peaks (see Slide 2-34), their perception that the activity is actually risky is declining. In other words, they are engaging in more dangerous behavior, but fail to recognize that danger. Given this information, it’s not surprising that insurance rates are so high for teens and that law enforcement spends so much time working teenager-related traffic situations. (Steinberg, 2009) Slide 2-36 CASE STUDY: Henry Henry has been arrested in the past for theft and possession of marijuana. He’s on probation for those delinquent acts. When Henry went through the local Juvenile Assessment Center, testing showed that Henry had difficulty thinking about if-then and what-if situations. When asked about where he would like to be in two or three years, 30 Exercise: Using the case study of Henry, ask officers to read and underline all the thinking and moral aspects they can identify. Review with the officers and point out any that they may not have identified. Briefly highlight Henry’s challenges in realizing the consequences of his decisions, his vague thoughts about the future, and how peers influenced his behavior – as all of these issues are delineated on the next few slides. Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Henry stated, “I’m not sure, maybe going to school.” Henry stated that he took clothes from a local shopping mall because, “Everyone had jacked some really cool stuff and I was the only one that didn’t have anything. They said I was a momma’s boy. The only problem was I was the one stupid enough to get caught.” His response for smoking marijuana was, “I don’t know why everyone is freaking out and making a big deal out of this with me. Everyone does it.” Henry has very little contact with his father. His father has been in and out of jail since Henry was a year old and is currently serving time in another state. Henry’s mother has been in and out of jail herself for crimes such as possession of drugs and worthless check writing. Henry has lived with his grandmother, his aunt, and/or his uncle when his mom was in jail. Slide 2-37 Considering Impact of Current Decisions on Future Increases with Age When participants were asked to consider matters in the future, those in mid-adolescence had the least view/perspective into the future. This is good evidence as to why they are much more worried about the game on Friday night, the dance, what’s for lunch, or how they appear to their peers right now than for things in the future like college, jobs, family, a criminal record, etc. Think of this in a practical application. A teenager gets into a fight and gains “status” as the victor among his/her peers. Adults threaten him with expulsion, arrest, legal proceedings, detention, etc. Which holds more weight for that youth right then and there? (Steinberg et al., 2009) 31 Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Slide 2-38 More Resistant to Peer Influence With Age Referring back to the MacArthur study of those 900 “average” (i.e., not exclusively delinquent) subjects, point out that when placed in situations with peer influence, the ability to resist peer influence increases with age, moving into average adult levels in very late adolescence. (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007) Slide 2-39 Peers Increase Risky Driving among Teenager and College Students, but Not Adults Reiterate that as teens’ desire for risk-taking behavior increases, their view of a situation as risky is at one of its lowest points. This slide looks at a driving situation with peer influence. The MacArthur study looked at driving behaviors in youth 10-12 years old, teens 13-18 years old, and adults 20-30 years old. When put in the driving simulation alone, all three age groups do equally well in managing the driving situations. However, as peers are added to the simulation, adolescents’ risk-taking behavior doubles, youth’s increases (but not as much), and the adults remain unaffected. (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005) This demonstrates how influential peers are on adolescent decision-making and risk-taking behavior. As many of you are aware, some states have limited and/or forbid peers in the cars with beginning drivers without the presence of an adult. Point out that there is a heightened need for healthy supervision and guidance right at a point when many youth are desperately attempting to push their parents/guardians away. 32 Training Aids Slide 2-40 Suggested Video 2.3: Peer Influence and Adolescent Behavior Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Note to Instructor: To supplement the information just discussed, consider playing the Peer Influence and Adolescent Behavior video (clip length: 4:04). This video requires access to the Internet. Start the video by clicking on the screenshot on PowerPoint slide 2-40, which links to the video on the Internet. Introduction to suggested video: This video features Dr. Steinberg discussing the risky driving tendencies among adolescents described on the previous slide. It is from episode two of the Brains on Trial with Alan Alda series (Chedd-Angier Production Company, 2013). Slide 2-41 What Were You Thinking? Think of a situation in which you were astounded by the poor judgment or self-control exercised by an adolescent. What differences existed between how you saw the situation and how the adolescent saw the situation in terms of self-control or the perceived need for selfcontrol? perception of risk to self or others? perception of the likely outcomes of the situation? Ask for a volunteer to describe a situation in which he/she was astounded by the poor judgment or self-control of an adolescent. Use the exercise to emphasize the differences between how the volunteer assessed the situation and how the adolescent assessed the same situation. Summarize the discussion by pointing out the differences between how the volunteer and the adolescent assessed the same situation in terms of self-control, risks to self or others, the likely outcomes in the situation, the consequences for the youth for the decision that he/she made, and the role of peers in decision-making. impact of the decision on the youth’s future? impact of peers on decision-making in the situation? Slide 2-42 Important Considerations Age-norms in the study for youth are based on the average in the general community. Discuss the implications of considering delinquency in the context of development. Based on the MacArthur study, age-norms are based on “average” youth in the general community. These were kids from average 33 Training Aids Most youth seen by juvenile courts are not “average.” Greater intellectual deficits More developmental delays Greater prevalence of mental disorders Content/Instructional Delivery Notes schools and young adults from average colleges and neighborhoods. Most youth seen by juvenile courts are not “average.” They tend to have greater intellectual deficits, more developmental delays (recall the key areas of development: physical, cognitive, social, moral), and a greater prevalence of mental disorders (anxiety, depression, bipolar, substance abuse, trauma disorders, etc.), which will be discussed in the next unit. ALL of these factors increase the risk for poor decision-making, so the population encountered by law enforcement would likely have amplified results if given the same tests administered in the MacArthur studies. Also, many “delinquent behaviors” are actually normative during adolescence because of their preference for risk, peer pressure, shortsightedness, etc. Alcohol use is normative for the majority of teens (NOT DESIRABLE). Delinquent behavior, such as stealing, trespassing, some drug use (particularly unauthorized use of over-thecounter/prescription medications and marijuana), is normative (NOT DESIRABLE). Aggressive behavior is normative (NOT DESIRABLE). Ask officers (but don’t expect an answer), “What were you doing when you were 16?” An adolescent’s involvement in the juvenile justice system is often the result of a combination of individual, family, environmental, and systemic factors, rather than adolescence alone. Slide 2-43 Disruptions in Normal Development 34 Sometimes normal adolescent development is disrupted by some type of traumatic experience, typically referred to as an “adverse childhood experience.” These adverse childhood Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes Traumatic experiences can disrupt normal adolescent development – at the very least, they create additional challenges. experiences can include recurrent physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, and emotional and physical neglect. Adverse childhood experiences include the following: Recurrent physical abuse Recurrent emotional abuse Sexual abuse Emotional or physical neglect Caretaker instability (e.g., multiple foster placements) Parental mental illness, substance abuse, criminality Exposure to domestic violence The cumulative effect of these experiences can influence a youth’s development across a range of symptoms and can have a fundamental impact on brain development, learning, and perceptions. These disruptions can add to the existing risks for poor decision-making that all teenagers experience by virtue of being adolescents. All of these create/exacerbate risks of poor decision-making. Slide 2-44 Summing Up Adolescents Ask officers what important facts from this unit will impact how they will interact with adolescents as part of their job. Adolescents are less able to control impulses and more driven by thrill. Adolescents tend to be short-sighted and oriented to immediate gratification. Adolescents are less able to resist pressure from peers. Psychosocial maturity continues to develop into early adulthood, long after adolescents have become as “smart” as adults. Slide 2-45 Implications Adolescents may not have all the neural “hardware” in place for adult behavior. This “hardware” is especially important in non-routine situations when judgment calls need to be made and emotional influences are high. Dr. Steinberg’s presentation of these and other findings to the U.S. senate judiciary committee impacted the decision for youth to not be eligible for the death penalty. His research continues to influence decisions by states to reconsider the lower ages of criminal court transfer and to keep youth in the juvenile court process (and out of the adult system). Emphasize this key point to officers: Although general policy statements can be formed based 35 Training Aids Like other general trends of development, these brain trends can say little about a specific individual. Content/Instructional Delivery Notes on this information, individual differences in development will still bring into question the level of culpability in given delinquent situations. Pose these questions: As law enforcement officers, what do you do with this information when responding to a situation? How do you take into account a youth’s limitations in thinking? What about the heightened impact of emotional influences of peers in the situation? Is it safe to assume every youth is the same and equally punishable as a person of any age who may have broken the law? Briefly highlight one of the essential elements of a CIT-Y program: diverting youth from the juvenile justice system to appropriate treatment programs when it is appropriate to do so. Slide 2-46 Purpose of Criminal Punishment To prevent crime – through deterrence, incapacitation, or rehabilitative intervention To “do justice” – to impose justly deserved punishment in proportion to offender’s blameworthiness Slide 2-47 Mitigation, Not Excuses Juvenile offenders are not adult criminals. “…..immaturity is morally relevant to blameworthiness and should have mitigating weight.” Our focus is on mitigation, not excuse. 36 Acknowledge that we all want safe homes and communities before asking: “How do we achieve this in light of a system of ‘justice’ and what we understand about adolescent development?” “How blameworthy is a youth who may not have an adult’s intellectual capacity to make decisions?” According to Steinberg (2007), “There are several important implications of this brain research for juvenile justice policy and practice.” First, adolescents are less responsible for their behavior than are adults. In a legal system like ours, which punishes in proportion to an offender’s responsibility for his actions, juvenile offenders should not be punished as harshly as we punish mature adults, even when they have committed comparable crimes. The juvenile system does not excuse youth of their crimes; Training Aids Content/Instructional Delivery Notes rather, it acknowledges the development stage and its role in the crimes committed, and punishes appropriately. Second, teenagers are still “works in progress,” and many of them do things out of youthful impetuousness (i.e., impulsivity, recklessness) that they would not do just a few years later, when their brains are more fully developed. It is therefore important that we treat adolescents who have broken the law in ways that will not impair their subsequent development. We have the capacity to hold juveniles accountable for their misdeeds in ways that get them back on track, instead of punishing them in ways that derail their transition to productive adulthood. In the U.S. Supreme Court ruling of 2012 that barred mandatory life sentences for juveniles, Justice Kagan wrote, “Mandatory life without parole for a juvenile precludes consideration of his chronological age and its hallmark features – among them immaturity, impetuosity, and failure to appreciate the risks and consequences” (Supreme Court of the United States, 2012). Finally, and perhaps most importantly, we know that adolescent behavior is not driven by the brain alone. In supervised and supportive environments, teenagers behave better. That’s why we should focus our efforts on creating family and community contexts that help protect teenagers from their own immaturity. One thing we know for certain is that preventing delinquency in the first place is less costly and far easier than responding to it after the fact. 37 Materials Video Inside the Teenage Brain Length: 60 minutes Frontline Instructions This video consists of several sections that can be used to supplement an instructor presentation, help instructors prepare for this module, or to provide more information for officers. It can be found online at http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/teenbrain/ or by searching online for “Inside the Teenage Brain.” Sections: The Teen Brain is a Work in Progress How Much Can Science Tell Us? Do your Teens Seem Like Aliens? From Zzzzzzzzz’s to A’s - Sleep Summary of Video In “Inside the Teenage Brain,” FRONTLINE chronicles how scientists are exploring the recesses of the brain and finding some new explanations for why adolescents behave the way they do. These discoveries could change the way we parent, teach, or perhaps even understand our teenagers. New neuroscience research has shown that a crucial part of the brain undergoes extensive changes during puberty – precisely the time when the raging hormones often blamed for teen behavior begin to wreak havoc. The vast majority of brain development occurs in two basic stages: growth spurts and pruning. In utero and throughout the first several months of life, the human brain grows at a rapid and dramatic pace, producing millions of brain cells. “This is a process that we knew happened in the womb, maybe even in the first 18 months of life,” explains neuroscientist Dr. Jay Giedd at the National Institute of Mental Health. “But it was only when we started following the same children by scanning their brains at two-year intervals that we detected a second wave of overproduction.” 38 This second wave – occurring roughly between ages 10 and 13 – is quickly followed by a process in which the brain prunes and organizes its neural pathways. “In many ways, it’s the most tumultuous time of brain development since coming out of the womb,” says Giedd. Confronted by these new discoveries, academics, counselors, and scientists are divided on just what all this means for children. “The relationship between desired behaviors and brain structure is totally unknown,” John Bruer tells FRONTLINE. He is president of the James S. McDonnell Foundation and author of The Myth of the First Three Years. “This simple, popular, newsweekly-magazine idea that adolescents are difficult because their frontal lobes aren’t mature is one we should be very cautious of.” This FRONTLINE report also looks at research that is helping scientists understand another puzzling aspect of adolescent behavior – sleep. Mary Carskadon, director of the E.P. Bradley Hospital Sleep Research Laboratory at Brown University, has spent years mapping the brains of sleepy teens. She has calculated that most teens get about seven and a half hours of sleep each night, while they need more than nine. Some say these sleep debts can have a powerful effect on a teen’s ability to learn and retain new material – especially abstract concepts like physics, math, and calculus. Despite all the new scientific research, “Inside the Teenage Brain” suggests that there is a consensus among experts that the most beneficial thing for teenagers is good relationships with their parents. Even Dr. Giedd wonders about the kinds of lessons parents can draw from his science. “The more technical and more advanced the science becomes, often the more it leads us back to some very basic tenets. ... With all the science and with all the advances, the best advice we can give is things that our grandmother could have told us generations ago: to spend loving, quality time with our children.” Ellen Galinsky, a social scientist and the president of the Families and Work Institute, has seen scientific fads come and go. But she says her research for a book about children shows there are enduring lessons for parents. Drawing on her interviews with more than a thousand children, she found that, to her surprise, teens were yearning for more time and more communication with their parents, even when they seemed to be pushing them away. She told FRONTLINE, “Even though the public perception is about building bigger and better brains, what the research shows is that it’s the relationships, it’s the connections, it’s the people in children’s lives who make the biggest difference.” American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: http://www.aacap.org/cs/root/facts_for_families/facts_for_families 39 Materials Case Study: Tony Tony is a 16-year-old boy. His cousin robs a local convenience store and later asks Tony to hide him. Tony agrees and is subsequently charged with “Accessory After the Fact.” He is offered a plea deal, but only if he will testify against his cousin. Tony feels protective of his cousin. Tony also believes that the convenience store has been a front for drug dealing, which he thinks at least some local police officers know and have been “on the take” from the store owner. Tony’s mother tells him to “do the right thing, tell the truth” and to testify. His lawyer tells him to take the plea and let his cousin fend for himself. Tony’s older brother tells him to be loyal to his cousin and refuse the plea agreement. Tony worries that if he is perceived as a “snitch,” people might hurt him or his family. He resents that his cousin got him into this situation in the first place. 40 Materials Case Study: Henry Henry is a 15-year-old white male who is small for his age. His mother and aunt refer to him as their “little man.” Henry has few friends and has never played any type of organized sports or participated in clubs/organizations at school or in the community. Teachers report that Henry has a history of being aggressive toward other youth and teachers, and appears to be slower to understand school information than his classmates. Henry has been arrested in the past for theft and possession of marijuana. He’s on probation for those delinquent acts. When Henry went through the local Juvenile Assessment Center, testing showed that Henry had difficulty thinking about if-then and what-if situations. When asked about where he would like to be in two or three years, Henry stated, “I’m not sure, maybe going to school.” Henry stated that he took clothes from a local shopping mall because, “Everyone had jacked some really cool stuff and I was the only one that didn’t have anything. They said I was a momma’s boy. The only problem was I was the one stupid enough to get caught.” His response for smoking marijuana was, “I don’t know why everyone is freaking out and making a big deal out of this with me. Everyone does it.” Henry has very little contact with his father. His father has been in and out of jail since Henry was a year old and is currently serving time in another state. Henry’s mother has been in and out of jail herself for crimes such as possession of drugs and worthless check writing. Henry has lived with his grandmother, his aunt, and/or his uncle when his mom was in jail. 41 Materials MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice Issue Brief 3: Less Guilty by Reason of Adolescence In 2005, in a landmark decision, the U.S. Supreme Court outlawed the death penalty for offenders who were younger than 18 when they committed their crimes. The ruling centered on the issue of culpability, or criminal blameworthiness. Unlike competence, which concerns an individual’s ability to serve as a defendant during trial or adjudication, culpability turns on the offender’s state of mind at the time of the offense, including factors that would mitigate, or lessen, the degree of responsibility. The Court’s ruling, which cited the Network’s work, ran counter to a nationwide trend toward harsher sentences for juveniles. Over the preceding decade, as serious crime rose and public safety became a focus of concern, legislators in virtually every state had enacted laws lowering the age at which juveniles could be tried and punished as adults for a broad range of crimes. This and other changes have resulted in the trial of more than 200,000 youth in the adult criminal system each year.1 Proponents of the tougher laws argue that youth who have committed violent crimes need more than a slap on the wrist from a juvenile court. It is naïve, they say, to continue to rely on a juvenile system designed for a simpler era, when youth were getting into fistfights in the schoolyard; drugs, guns, and other serious crimes are adult offenses that demand adult punishment. Yet the premise of the juvenile justice system is that adolescents are different from adults, in ways that make them potentially less blameworthy than adults for their criminal acts. The legal system has long held that criminal punishment should be based not only on the harm caused, but also on the blameworthiness of the offender. How blameworthy a person is for a crime depends on the circumstances of the crime and of the person committing it. Traditionally, the courts have considered several categories of mitigating factors when determining a defendant’s culpability. These include impaired decision-making capacity, usually due to mental illness or disability, the circumstances of the crime—for example, whether it was committed under duress, and the individual’s personal character, which may suggest a low risk of continuing crime. Such factors don’t make a person exempt from punishment – rather, they indicate that the punishment should be less than it would be for others committing similar crimes, but under different circumstances. Should developmental immaturity be added to the list of mitigating factors? Should juveniles, in general, be treated more leniently than adults? A major study by the Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice now provides strong evidence that the answer is yes. 42 The Network’s Study of Juvenile Culpability The study of juvenile culpability was designed to provide scientific data on whether, in what ways, and at what ages adolescents differ from adults. Many studies have shown that by the age of sixteen, adolescents’ cognitive abilities – loosely, their intelligence or ability to reason – closely mirror that of adults. But how people reason is only one influence on how they make decisions. In the real world, especially in high-pressure crime situations, judgments are made in the heat of the moment, often in the company of peers. In these situations, adolescents’ other common traits – their short-sightedness, their impulsivity, their susceptibility to peer influence – can quickly undermine their decision-making capacity. The investigators looked at age differences in a number of characteristics that are believed to undergird decision-making and that are relevant to mitigation, such as impulsivity and risk processing, future orientation, sensation-seeking, and resistance to peer pressure. These characteristics are also thought to change over the course of adolescence and to be linked to brain maturation during this time. The subjects – close to 1,000 individuals between the ages of 10 and 30 – were drawn from the general population in five regions. They were ethnically and socioeconomically diverse. The study’s findings showed several characteristics of adolescence that are relevant to determinations of criminal culpability. As the accompanying figure indicates, although intellectual abilities stop maturing around age 16, psychosocial capability continues to develop well into early adulthood. Short-Sighted Decision-Making One important element of mature decision-making is a sense of the future consequences of an act. A variety of studies in which adolescents and adults are asked to envision themselves in the future have found that adults project their visions over a significantly longer time, suggesting much greater future orientation. These findings are supported by data from the Network’s culpability study. Adolescents characterized themselves as less likely to consider the future consequences of their actions than did adults. And when subjects in the study were presented with various choices measuring their preference for smaller, immediate rewards versus larger, longer-term rewards (for example, “Would you rather have $100 today or $1,000 a year from now?”), adolescents had a lower “tipping point” – the amount of money they would take to get it immediately as opposed to waiting. How might these characteristics carry over into the real world? When weighing the long-term consequences of a crime, adolescents may simply be unable to see far enough into the future to make a good decision. Their lack of foresight, along with their tendency to pay more attention to immediate gratification than to long-term consequences, are among the factors that may lead them to make bad decisions. 43 Poor Impulse Control The Network’s study also found that as individuals age, they become less impulsive and less likely to seek thrills; in fact, gains in these aspects of self-control continue well into early adulthood. This was evident in individuals’ descriptions of themselves and on tasks designed to measure impulse control. On the “Tower of London” task, for example – where the goal is to solve a puzzle in as few moves as possible, with a wrong move requiring extra moves to undo it – adolescents took less time to consider their first move, jumping the gun before planning ahead. Network research also suggests that adolescents are both less sensitive to risk and more sensitive to rewards—an attitude than can lead to greater risk-taking. The new data confirm and expand on earlier studies gauging attitudes toward risk, which found that adults spontaneously mention more potential risks than teens. Juveniles’ tendency to pay more attention to the potential benefits of a risky decision than to its likely costs may contribute to their impulsivity in crime situations. Vulnerability to Peer Pressure The law does not require exceptional bravery of citizens in the face of threats or other duress. A person who robs a bank with a gun in his back is not as blameworthy as another who willingly robs a bank; coercion and distress are mitigating factors. Adolescents, too, face coercion, but of a different sort. Pressure from peers is keenly felt by teens. Peer influence can affect youth’s decisions directly, as when adolescents are coerced to take risks they might otherwise avoid. More indirectly, youth’s desire for peer approval, or their fear of rejection, may lead them to do things they might not otherwise do. In the Network’s culpability study, individuals’ reports of their vulnerability to peer pressure declined over the course of adolescence and young adulthood. Other Network research now underway is examining how adolescent risk-taking is “activated” by the presence of peers or by emotional arousal. For example, an earlier Network study, involving a computer car-driving task, showed that the mere presence of friends increased risk-taking in adolescents and college undergraduates, though not adults. 2 Although not every teen succumbs to peer pressures, some youth face more coercive situations than others. Many of those in the juvenile justice system live in tough neighborhoods, where losing face can be not only humiliating but dangerous. Capitulating in the face of a challenge can be a sign of weakness, inviting attack and continued persecution. To the extent that coercion or duress is a mitigating factor, the situations in which many juvenile crimes are committed should lessen their culpability. Confirmation from Brain Studies Recent findings from neuroscience line up well with the Network’s psychosocial research, showing that brain maturation is a process that continues through adolescence and into early adulthood. For example, there is good evidence that the brain systems that govern impulse control, planning, and thinking ahead are still developing well beyond age 18. There are also several studies indicating that the systems governing reward sensitivity are “amped up” at puberty, which would lead to an increase in sensationseeking and in valuing benefits over risks. And there is emerging evidence that the brain systems that govern the processing of emotional and social information are affected by the hormonal changes of puberty in ways that make people more sensitive to the reactions of those around them – and thus more susceptible to the influence of peers.3 44 Policy Implications: A Separate System for Young Offenders The scientific arguments do not say that adolescents cannot distinguish right from wrong, nor that they should be exempt from punishment. Rather, they point to the need to consider the developmental stage of adolescence as a mitigating factor when juveniles are facing criminal prosecution. The same factors that make youth ineligible to vote or to serve on a jury require us to treat them differently from adults when they commit crimes. Some have argued that courts ought to assess defendants’ maturity on a case-by-case basis, pointing to the fact that older adolescents, in particular, vary in their capacity for mature decision-making. But the tools needed to measure psychosocial maturity on an individual basis are not well developed, nor is it possible to distinguish reliably between mature and immature adolescents on the basis of brain images. Consequently, assessing maturity on an individual basis, as we do with other mitigating factors, is likely to produce many errors. However, the maturing process follows a similar pattern across virtually all teenagers. Therefore it is both logical and efficient to treat adolescents as a special legal category – and to refer the vast majority of offenders under the age of 18 to juvenile court, where they will be treated as responsible but less blameworthy, and where they will receive less punishment and more rehabilitation and treatment than typical adult offenders. The juvenile system does not excuse youth of their crimes; rather, it acknowledges the development stage and its role in the crimes committed, and punishes appropriately. At the same time, any legal regime must pay attention to legitimate concerns about public safety. There will always be some youth – such as older, violent recidivists – who have exhausted the resources and patience of the juvenile justice system, and whose danger to the community warrants adjudication in criminal court. But these represent only a very small percentage of juvenile offenders. Trying and punishing youth as adults is an option that should be used sparingly. Legislatures in several states have begun to reconsider the punitive laws enacted in recent decades. They have already recognized that prosecuting and punishing juveniles as adults carries high costs, for the youth and for their communities. Now we can offer lawmakers in all states a large body of research on which to build a more just and effective juvenile justice system. 1 Allard, P., & Young, M. (2002). Prosecuting juveniles in adult court: Perspectives for policymakers and practitioners. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 6, 65-78. 2 Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk-taking, risk preference, and risky decision-making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625-635. 3 Nelson, E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E., & Pine, D. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163-174. For More Information MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice Temple University, Department of Psychology Philadelphia, PA 19122 www.adjj.org The Research Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice is an interdisciplinary, multi-institutional program focused on building a foundation of sound science and legal scholarship to support reform of the juvenile justice system. The network conducts research, disseminates the resulting knowledge to professionals and the public, and works to improve decision-making and to prepare the way for the next generation of juvenile justice reform. 45 References Chedd-Angier Production Company. (2013). Peer influence and adolescent behavior. Brains on Trial with Alan Alda: Deciding Punishment. Available at http://brainsontrial.com/watchvideos/video/episode-2-deciding-punishment/ Chedd-Angier Production Company. (2013). What fMRI scans tell us about the adolescent brain. Brains on Trial with Alan Alda: Deciding Punishment. Available at http://brainsontrial.com/watchvideos/video/episode-2-deciding-punishment/ Craig, G. J. (1999). Human development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. CSR, Inc. (1997). Understanding youth development: Promoting positive pathways of growth. Washington, D.C.: Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Families and Youth Services Bureau. Daeg de Mott, D. K. (1998). Moral development. Gale Encyclopedia of Childhood and Adolescence. Gale Research. Frontline. (2002). The wiring of the adolescent brain. Inside the Teenage Brain. Available at http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/video/flv/generic.html?s=frol02sfa9q392&continuous=1 Gardner, M. & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk-taking, risk preference, and risky decisionmaking in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625635. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Adolescent Development & Juvenile Justice (2006). Less guilty by reason of adolescence. Issue Brief 3. Chicago, IL: John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation. Seifert, K. L. & Hoffnung, R. J. (1994). Child and adolescent development, Third Ed. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Steinberg, L. (2007). Senate judiciary committee briefing. June 11, 2007. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Senate. Steinberg, L. (2008). Development & criminal blameworthiness: Bringing research to policy & practice. MacArthur Foundation Models for Change Annual Conference. December 9, 2008. Washington, D.C. Steinberg, L. (2009). Should the science of adolescent brain development inform public policy? American Psychologist, 64, 739-750. Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S. & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764-1778. Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E. & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28-44. Steinberg, L. & Monahan, K. (2007). Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1531-1543. 46 Supreme Court of the United States. (2012). Miller v. Alabama. 567 U.S. ___ Van Hasselt, V. B. & Hersen, M. (1987). Handbook of adolescent psychology. New York: Pergamon Press. Vasta, R., Haith, M. M. & Miller, S. A. (1995). Child psychology: The modern science, Second Ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. White, B. (1999). Understanding adolescent behavior: Knowledge, skills, & interventions. Trainer’s Manual. Louisiana State University in Shreveport. Division of Continuing Education and Public Service. 47 48