“You are not normal if you won`t be scared”: A Psychosociolinguistic
Transcription
“You are not normal if you won`t be scared”: A Psychosociolinguistic
“You are not normal if you won’t be scared”: A Psychosociolinguistic Study of Coping and Narrative Processing in the Discourse of Israeli Bus Drivers who Experienced Terror Attacks Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of “DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY” by Alison Stern Perez Submitted to the Senate of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev August 30, 2014 Be’er Sheva “You are not normal if you won’t be scared”: A Psychosociolinguistic Study of Coping and Narrative Processing in the Discourse of Israeli Bus Drivers who Experienced Terror Attacks Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of “DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY” by Alison Stern Perez Submitted to the Senate of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev Approved by the advisors: Date: Date: Approved by the Dean of the Kreitman School of Advanced Graduate Studies: August 30, 2014 Be’er Sheva This work was carried out under the supervision of: Professor Shifra Sagy (Emeritus, Department of Education) and Professor Yishai Tobin (Department of Foreign Literatures and Linguistics) and Professor Dan Bar-On (z”l) (Department of Psychology) in the Department of Psychology, in the Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev. ii Research Student’s Affidavit when Submitting the Doctoral Thesis for Judgment I, Alison Stern Perez , whose signature appears below, hereby declare that (please mark the appropriate statements): X I have written this Thesis by myself, except for the help and guidance offered by my Thesis Advisors. X The scientific materials included in this Thesis are products of my own research, culled from the period during which I was a research student. This Thesis incorporates research materials produced in cooperation with others, excluding the technical help commonly received during experimental work. Therefore, I am attaching another affidavit stating the contributions made by myself and the other participants in this research, which has been approved by them and submitted with their approval. Date: Student’s name: Signature: iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to express my deep gratitude to the participants in this study, the bus drivers I interviewed in Jerusalem and Be’er Sheva, who volunteered so much of their precious time and effort to sit with me and share some of the most intimate and difficult experiences of their lives. My thanks to Egged Bus Company for granting me access to their organization and allowing me to interview their employees and personnel. I am grateful, in particular, to Dr. Yael Dover, (at that time) the managing psychologist and organizational consultant for the Jerusalem region, and Yael Ayalon, the social worker for the Jerusalem region, for their professional support and cooperation in the participant recruitment process, and for offering me their own insights into the life and world of the Israeli bus driver. I am indebted to my advisors, Professor Yishai Tobin and Professor Shifra Sagy, for their dedicated, thoughtful, and tireless efforts to guide me through the doctoral research process. It was a long road—perhaps the longest of any of their students—and I am so grateful that they were willing to travel it with me (even when they probably weren’t!). Shifra took me on although the project was a bit outside her ‘wheelhouse,’ and helped me to mold it into something of which I am truly proud. She supported and encouraged me in my (often seemingly utterly misguided) dream of creating a truly interdisciplinary piece of research. This task sometimes seemed impossibly large and unconquerable, but Shifra was always there to ‘talk me down’ and show me the ‘big picture.’ And I will be forever grateful to Yishai, who spent untold hours teaching me an approach and methodology that was entirely new to me, and kindly guiding and correcting me through my fledgling attempts to be a linguist. From the first moment I timidly knocked on his office door, seeking an explanation to interview data of which I could not make sense on my own, I knew I was in the right place. Yishai’s utter dedication to the cause, and to me, continually bowled me over. In sickness and in health, he returned drafts to me within mere hours of my (sometimes late-night) emailed submissions to him, always thoroughly read and commented upon. Although we began this journey together from totally different worlds, we slowly but surely found a middle ground— which was ground-breaking in its own way, and just “simply WERKED!” Plenty of people pretty much gave up on me over the course of this ‘extended’ process—and I don’t blame them a bit because I, too, was tempted to give up on myself many, many times—but Yishai never did, and never even considered it. He had 100% (often seemingly blind, and possibly unfounded) faith in me, and I cannot imagine having gotten through this without him. Above all else, Yishai supported me through countless crises of all sorts, and has become, for me, a true mentor for life. I would also like to acknowledge the tremendous contribution of Professor Dan Bar-On (z”l), who was one of the driving forces of the project from its inception, and one of my original iv advisors before his untimely passing. It is likely that I would not even have been accepted into Ben-Gurion University—and perhaps not have made aliyah at all—if it wasn’t for Dan, so I owe him quite a lot indeed. Professor Golan Shahar, too, had an important hand in the early part of the process, helping me shape the topic of the project and the initial analytic perspective, in addition to encouraging me to widen the scope and continue on to a doctorate. Many of the important conclusions arrived at in this research study would not have revealed themselves without the academic and emotional support of a number of other mentors, teachers, and colleagues, and for that I am forever grateful. Professor Ruthellen Josselson was gracious enough to meet with me many times over the years just to let me ‘talk things out,’ providing valuable insight and calm reassurance. Professor Amia Lieblich provided an ‘extra eye’ in the analysis phase, and often, just with a simple word or smile, gave me validation and motivation at critical points in the process. And Dr. Gabriela Spector-Mersel served as a valued teacher, advisor, supporter, and friend. Much of our conversations over the years cemented key concepts in the study, and had an indelible impact on the final product. I see Ruthellen, Amia, and Gabriela as true role models for brilliance, rigor, and thoughtfulness in academic qualitative work, and I thus often shaped my analyses and writings around what I perceived as “what would [the trifecta] do” (but of course, any errors of fact, style, or judgment herein are purely my own). And this project would never have gotten ‘off the ground’ without the approval of my doctoral proposal committee, Professor Amia Lieblich and Professor Hanoch Flum. Through their academic courses, their frequent encouragement and support, and their simple presence at BGU as representative of the sometimes seemingly-impossible combination of womanmother-professor-successful qualitative researcher, Professor Michal Krumer-Nevo, Professor Lea Kacen, and Dr. Maya Lavie-Ajayi served as great inspirations for me throughout my studies. And the Reflexivity Group at BGU—in particular, Dr. Nitsan Almog, Irit Porat, and Dr. Eitan Shahar— supported me in many ways when and how I needed it the most. Dr. Emanuel Schattner, Dr. Dafna Israel Weinberg, and Dr. Dana Tzur-Bitan were by my side from the beginning, helping me through the intricacies of the early proposal and committee, and providing emotional and moral support throughout. Dr. Liat Amar-Arran, Hagit Rodrigues Garcia, and Dr. Pnina Dolberg held my hand up to the very end, empathic in ways that only fellow anguished doctoral students could ever know. This project would have been impossible to complete (or at least stalled indefinitely) without the tireless efforts of Keren Siegler-Lotan, whose absolute dedication to perfection and academic integrity is matched only, perhaps, by my own. Her hours of work, transcribing and retranscribing and then double- and triple-checking the interviews, in addition to countless hours processing and mining the data, were simply invaluable. Her deep suffering throughout the process was, again, matched only by my own. My thanks also to Ela Stoliar, Hamutal Raber, Michal v Yermish Levi, and Liav Malka, my other students in my two Guided Research courses, whose hard work and valuable insights contributed significantly to this project. Along the way, I have participated and presented in a number of extracurricular courses and groups, all of which infused into this research a tremendous amount of knowledge and important input from colleagues. My years at Machon Mofet introduced me to Professor Bracha Alpert, Professor Irit Kupferberg, and Dr. Liron Dushenik, all of whom helped me think through preliminary findings and interpretations. My participation in the “Trauma and Resilience: Theory and Practice from the Israeli Experience” course by The Israel Center for the Treatment of Psychotrauma and Hebrew University taught me a great deal, and Professor Danny Brom and Dr. Ruthy Pat-Horenczyk became mentors for me in more ways than they probably know. Methods workshops led by Professor Gabriele Rosenthal, Professor Miriam Raider-Roth, and Dr. Tom Wengraf all helped me to expand and develop my qualitative analytical skills to a level that has made this study rigorous in a manner of which I am truly proud. And the various conferences at which I presented constituted an essential influence on the study and the research journey. I thank, in particular, Professor Catherine Riessman, Professor Matti Hyvärinen, Professor Molly Andrews, and Professor Brian Schiff for giving me invaluable comments and encouragement along the way. Early concepts and versions of the research study were also presented to and/or discussed with Professor Niza Yanay, Professor Orit Tykocinski, Professor Rivka Tuval-Mashiach, Professor Orit Nuttman-Schwartz, and Professor Michal Komem, and all offered me important insights and guidance. Thanks also to Dr. Igor Dreer, for ‘assisting’ me with the statistical analyses, and for the laughs throughout. Many people also helped me in my never-ending quest to understand Israeli society in spite of my ‘outsider-ness,’ and I would like to thank, especially, Professor Gadi BenEzer, Dr. Galia Glasner-Heled, Dr. Alon Lazar, Professor Julie Cwikel, Dr. Ariel Ayalon, Dr. Limor Danon-Meoded, Dr. Lisa Rochman, Bosmat Shalom-Tuchin, Noga Einy, Sonia FabrikantLivne, and Tamar Walfisch. And I could not have gotten through the final month of writing without the invaluable help of Michal Shemesh. I have been blessed with significant academic organizational and financial support throughout my doctoral studies, all of which provided a firm base from which I was able to advance on this project. First of all, I would like to thank the Kreitman Fellowship Foundation at BenGurion University of the Negev for accepting me as a fellow. My four years in the program gave me the confidence and courage to even begin thinking of myself as a doctoral student. In addition, I am grateful to the Center for Training and Research on Resilience and Coping with Trauma at Sapir Academic College, the Israel Trauma Coalition for Response and Preparedness, and the Association of Qualitative Researchers in Israel, as the generous scholarships I received further enabled this doctorate to come to fruition. The Israeli Center for Qualitative Research of People vi and Societies (ICQM) has been my academic home, supporting me intellectually, emotionally, and spiritually, throughout the years of this doctoral journey. Particularly after the passing of Professor Bar-On, I had a great need for guidance in the qualitative realm of the project, and the ICQM and its Doctoral Forum provided a strong resource for me at every turn. Although being a doctoral student is often assumed to be merely an academic post, this project was, for me, quite often emotionally draining in more ways than twenty. I simply could not have gotten through it without the support system of my Israeli family and friends—they all know who they are, and how important they are to me. Tziki and Aya Aud are the best grandparents on this continent, and their love for my kids kept my guilt at bay during my working weekends. Limor Niego advised and supported me at every twist in the road, assuring me that it would all be worth it in the end. Tali Ben Shitrit-Machlev also helped push, pull, kick, and rally me to the finish line, as only a great coach can. And none of this would have been possible without the presence in my life of Gideon Goldstein, who never stopped challenging, encouraging, and believing in me. He is perhaps the biggest part of the reason I made aliyah in the first place, and one of my best models of ‘Israeli resilience’ in action. I still can’t believe he managed to convince me even to apply to advanced degree programs a mere ten months after I immigrated, and I still can’t believe I have come this far since. I simply never would have managed as long as I have here without him. Finally, my deep appreciation and gratitude go to my husband, Michael, for being unflinchingly by my side and behind the scenes throughout this seemingly endless process. It was never easy; in fact, it was pretty much always gut-wrenchingly painful. And truthfully, I’m not sure if I would still be standing with me if I had a choice in the matter. But he took the brunt of it with as much patience and support as humanly possible, in addition to endless hours of bleary-eyed parenting and back-breaking amounts of work to keep our household running while I was attempting to focus on my work. I am grateful and honored that he is still standing with me to see the finish line, and I hope to be able to pay him back for his sacrifices. Coming through this journey successfully (albeit slightly later than planned), while simultaneously upholding my standards and values as a self-actualized woman, wife, and mother, is perhaps my greatest accomplishment. And so, my concurrent thanks and apologies go to my first three babies, Amit, Assaf, and Matan, for graciously accepting less time (and at least a bit less patience) from me as I worked on the birth of this ‘little’ baby. This dissertation was written almost exclusively in 20- to 90-minute installments between nursing sessions, play sessions, cuddle sessions, blowout-diapering sessions, bathing sessions, booboo-kissing sessions, marathon book-reading sessions, and tantrum/ screaming/sibling-rivalry sessions. It was one of the most challenging multi-tasking endeavors I’ve ever had to do, and I wouldn’t have had it any other way. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS Statement of supervision .................................................................................................................................... ii Research student’s affidavit ..............................................................................................................................iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .............................................................................................................................. iv LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................................... xv LIST OF FIGURES ..................................................................................................................................... xvii LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS.....................................................................................................................xviii GUIDE TO TRANSCRIPTIONS ................................................................................................................ xix ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................... xxi Introduction............................................................................................................................................. xxi Purpose .................................................................................................................................................xxiii Data collection ...................................................................................................................................... xxiv Conceptual and methodological frameworks......................................................................................... xxv Basic linguistic background ................................................................................................................xxviii Central patterns and guiding hypotheses.............................................................................................. xxix Results and discussion ........................................................................................................................ xxxiv Contribution ......................................................................................................................................... xxxv PART 1: Introduction, Background, Methods, and Preliminary Findings 1 2 INTRODUCTION TO THE RESEARCH STUDY AND THE DISSERTATION 1.1 INTRODUCTION TO THE RESEARCH STUDY ....................................................................................... 1 1.1.1 Background and overview................................................................................................... 1 1.1.2 The research question and central assertion ........................................................................ 2 1.1.3 The central findings............................................................................................................. 3 1.1.4 The importance and contributions of the study ................................................................... 4 1.2 INTRODUCTION TO THE DISSERTATION ............................................................................................ 5 1.2.1 The organization of the dissertation .................................................................................... 5 1.2.2 Several important caveats ................................................................................................... 7 LITERATURE REVIEW: THE CONCEPTUAL, THEORETICAL, AND CONTEXTUAL FRAMEWORKS OF THE RESEARCH STUDY 2.1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................................... 10 2.2 THE PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY .............................. 10 2.2.1 Salutogenesis and the ‘ease/dis-ease’ continuum: A paradigmatic shift ........................... 10 2.2.2 Stress, coping, resilience, and working through................................................................ 13 2.3 THE PSYCHOLINGUISTIC CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY ........................... 18 2.3.1 Narrating trauma and traumatic memory .......................................................................... 18 viii 2.3.2 2.3.3 3 2.3.1.1 Memory, identity, and trauma ........................................................................... 18 2.3.1.2 Translating traumatic memory into narrative memory..................................... 20 2.3.1.3 Characteristics and signals of ‘trauma narratives’ .......................................... 22 2.3.1.4 The relationship between narrating trauma and health ................................... 25 Retelling and narrative processing over time .................................................................... 27 The relationship between the use of pronouns and the health of the speaker ................... 35 2.4 THE MACRO-LEVEL PSYCHOSOCIAL CONTEXT OF THE RESEARCH STUDY ..................................... 37 2.4.1 Terror, war, and chronic stress in Israel ............................................................................ 38 2.4.1.1 Overview and statistics ..................................................................................... 38 2.4.1.2 The phenomenological daily experience and its effects: Communal acute and chronic stress and trauma.............................................. 41 2.4.1.3 Israeli resilience ............................................................................................... 44 2.4.1.4 Military vs. civilian trauma: Universal belonging vs. isolation and marginalization ..................................................... 46 2.4.2 Israeli societal pressure: The hegemonic cultural discourse of ‘The Israeli Warrior’ ...... 49 2.5 THE MICRO-LEVEL PSYCHOSOCIAL CONTEXT OF THE RESEARCH STUDY....................................... 54 2.5.1 Egged Bus Company: History, demographics, and the psychosocial context .................. 54 2.5.1.1 History of the company and its culture ............................................................. 55 2.5.1.2 Demographics and terror-related statistics ...................................................... 57 2.5.1.3 The psychosocial context surrounding and within Egged ................................ 59 2.5.2 Being an Egged bus driver in Israel: The personal context ............................................... 62 2.5.2.1 The phenomenological context and experience of Egged drivers ..................... 62 2.5.2.2 The fundamental dilemma of the Egged driver ................................................. 66 2.6 CONCLUSION .................................................................................................................................. 69 THE METHODOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY 3.1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................................... 70 3.2 THE PARTICIPANT POPULATION AND RESEARCH CONTEXT ............................................................ 70 3.3 THE DATA COLLECTION METHOD ................................................................................................... 72 3.4 THE REFLEXIVE RESEARCHER: WHERE I AM COMING FROM .......................................................... 73 3.5 THE METHODOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE STUDY: INTEGRATION OF THREE DISTINCT CONTENT WORLDS ................................................................... 76 3.5.1 Social and discursive psychology: A focus on the individual and the surrounding societal context ........................................................................................ 77 3.5.2 Sign-oriented semiotic linguistics: A focus on the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages of the individual’s words ................................................... 79 3.5.2.1 The pronoun system and the importance of gender in Modern Israeli Hebrew................................................................................. 80 3.5.2.2 The interlocked tense system in Modern Israeli Hebrew .................................. 84 3.5.2.3 The systems of focus and deixis and prepositions in Modern Israeli Hebrew................................................................................. 85 3.5.3 Qualitative inquiry and the interpretive narrative paradigm: A focus on the individual’s stories and the story-telling context.................................................... 88 3.5.3.1 An organizing model for narrative analysis ..................................................... 92 ix 3.5.3.2 3.5.4 4 Primary qualitative analytical influences on the methodological framework .................................................................... 93 The need for and importance of an integration of these disciplines .................................. 95 3.6 THE ANALYTICAL TOOLS: A SPIRALING MIXED-METHOD TECHNIQUE ........................................... 97 3.6.1 Quantitative data analysis methodology ........................................................................... 99 3.6.2 Qualitative data analysis methodology ........................................................................... 101 3.6.2.1 Central macro- and micro-level analytical questions..................................... 101 3.6.2.2 Holistic and categorical content processing................................................... 102 3.6.2.3 Pronoun ‘poetry’............................................................................................. 104 3.6.2.4 Longitudinal comparative analysis of mechanisms of narrative selection ..... 106 3.6.2.4.1 Preparation of parallel narrative tables ........................................ 106 3.6.2.4.2 Spector-Mersel’s model of narrative selection ............................ 108 3.6.2.4.3 Parallel narrative comparison: A new analytical model .............. 110 3.7 METHODOLOGICAL, LOGISTICAL, AND ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS ............................................. 111 3.7.1 Translation and transcription in a non-fluent and micro-analytical context.................... 111 3.7.2 Confidentiality and anonymity in a media-infused context ............................................ 113 3.7.3 The follow-up interview protocol ................................................................................... 113 3.7.4 The effects of the interviews and potential conflicts ....................................................... 114 3.8 ‘VALIDITY’ AND TRUSTWORTHINESS: THE QUALITY OF THE RESEARCH STUDY ......................... 115 3.8.1 Worthy topic.................................................................................................................... 115 3.8.2 Rich rigor ........................................................................................................................ 116 3.8.3 Sincerity .......................................................................................................................... 118 3.8.4 Credibility ....................................................................................................................... 119 3.8.5 Resonance ....................................................................................................................... 122 3.8.6 Significant contribution ................................................................................................... 124 3.8.7 Ethics ............................................................................................................................... 124 3.8.8 Meaningful coherence ..................................................................................................... 126 PRELIMINARY RESULTS AND FUNDAMENTAL HYPOTHESES 4.1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................................. 127 4.2 FUNDAMENTAL PSYCHOLINGUISTIC FINDINGS: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL AND HIS INNER PSYCHOLOGICAL WORLD ......................................... 128 4.2.1 4.2.2 4.2.3 4.2.4 Personalization and possession: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and phrases ..................................................... 129 Depersonalization and universalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun and/or third-person ‘others’ ............................................... 130 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun............ 131 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun ...................................... 133 4.3 PSYCHOSOCIOLINGUISTIC INTERPRETIVE HYPOTHESES: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL, HIS INNER PSYCHOLOGICAL WORLD, AND HIS PERCEPTIONS OF THE SURROUNDING SOCIAL DISCOURSE ............................................................ 134 4.4 SOCIOPSYCHOLOGICAL LONGITUDINAL COMPARATIVE GUIDELINES: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL AND HIS NARRATIVE OVER TIME ....................... 137 x PART 2: Baseline Analyses 5 6 YOSSI: “YOU (FS) SWALLOW ALL OF THE ATROCITY” – BRINGING THE AUDIENCE IN TO TRAUMATIC EXPERIENCE AND ‘UNACCEPTABLE’ WEAKNESS 5.1 INTRODUCTION TO YOSSI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS.................................... 144 5.2 YOSSI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ......................................... 147 5.2.1 Personalization, possession, and separation: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases ........................................ 147 5.2.2 Depersonalization, generalization, and universalization: Use of third-person ‘others’ and the second-person masculine singular pronoun .......... 148 5.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun............ 151 5.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun ...................................... 156 5.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN YOSSI’S BASELINE TAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT ...................................................... 158 5.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF YOSSI’S BASELINE TAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ................................................................. 159 5.5 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 165 DANI: “YOU (MS) ARE NOT NORMAL IF YOU (MS) WON’T BE SCARED” – AMBIVALENCE ABOUT AND UNIVERSALIZATION OF THE LESS ‘ACCEPTABLE’ EXPERIENCE 6.1 INTRODUCTION TO DANI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS ..................................... 167 6.1.1 Dani’s ‘non-narrative’ narratives .................................................................................... 169 6.2 DANI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT .......................................... 171 6.2.1 Personalization, possession, and separation: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases ........................................ 172 6.2.2 Depersonalization, universalization, and normalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun ................................................... 172 6.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun............ 178 6.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun ...................................... 182 6.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN DANI’S BASELINE TRAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT ..................................................... 184 6.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF DANI’S BASELINE TRAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ................................................................. 186 6.5 THE CONNECTION BETWEEN THE MS ‘YOU’ AND THE LIVED EXPERIENCE: AMBIVALENCE ABOUT PERSONAL VS. SOCIETAL APPRAISALS ..................................................... 192 6.5.1 The form and content of Dani’s entire main TAN: The ‘less acceptable’ way ............... 192 6.5.2 ‘Validation’: Dani’s own words about the ‘acceptable’ way .......................................... 197 xi 6.6 7 8 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 199 MOTTI: “THE HERO DRIVER” vs. “I FEEL ABOUT MYSELF THAT I AM VERY WEAK” – CLAIMING OWNERSHIP OF ‘ACCEPTABLE’ ACTIONS AND A WIDER RANGE OF EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCES 7.1 INTRODUCTION TO MOTTI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS................................... 202 7.2 MOTTI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ........................................ 203 7.2.1 Personalization vs. collectivization: Use of first-person possessive pronouns vs. third-person pronouns ............................... 204 7.2.2 Depersonalization and universalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun and distancing discourse .................................................... 205 7.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun............ 208 7.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun ...................................... 211 7.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN MOTTI’S BASELINE TAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT ..................................................... 215 7.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF MOTTI’S BASELINE TAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ................................................................. 218 7.5 THE CONNECTION BETWEEN THE ‘I’ AND THE LIVED EXPERIENCE: WILLINGNESS TO OWN A WIDER AND DIFFICULT RANGE OF EXPERIENCE ................................... 222 7.6 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 227 YOSSI, DANI, AND MOTTI IN CONTEXT: HOW INDIVIDUAL DISCOURSE REFLECTS AND IS SHAPED BY PERCEPTIONS OF SOCIOCULTURAL DISCOURSE SURROUNDING ‘ACCEPTABLE’ BEHAVIOR 8.1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................................. 229 8.2 PRONOUN USE, MEANINGS, AND MESSAGES: QUANTITATIVE AND QUALITATIVE FINDINGS AGGREGATED AND JUXTAPOSED .......................... 229 8.3 CONNECTIONS BETWEEN THE PARTICIPANTS’ PRONOUN USE AND PERSONAL EXPERIENCES ...... 237 8.3.1 Yossi: “You (FS) are spineless” ..................................................................................... 237 8.3.2 Dani: “The society pities him in my opinion” ................................................................. 238 8.3.3 Motti: “I don’t think of myself as a hero” ...................................................................... 240 8.3.4 Connections and conclusions across the cases of Yossi, Dani, and Motti ...................... 242 8.4 ‘VALIDATION’: CONNECTIONS BETWEEN THE HYPOTHESES, THE FINDINGS, AND THE INTERVIEWEES’ OWN WORDS ........................................................................................ 243 8.4.1 Individual coping ............................................................................................................ 243 8.4.2 Societal pressure, collective coping, and perceived weakness ........................................ 247 8.4.3 The myth and reality of ‘The Israeli Warrior’................................................................. 249 PART 3: Longitudinal Comparative Analyses xii 9 10 YOSSI: FROM “YOU (FS) SWALLOW ALL OF THE ATROCITY” TO “I EXPERIENCED ALL OF THAT TERROR ATTACK” – AN EXEMPLAR OF ‘IMPROVED’ NARRATIVE PROCESSING 9.1 RE-INTRODUCTION TO YOSSI: BACKGROUND AND IMPRESSIONS ................................................ 253 9.2 YOSSI’S FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ...................................... 253 9.3 QUANTITATIVE COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS: HOW MUCH AND WHEN YOSSI SAID IT ..................... 254 9.3.1 Life narratives and spontaneous terror attack narratives within them ............................. 254 9.3.2 Pronoun and tense use distribution within the terror attack narratives ........................... 259 9.4 YOSSI’S FOLLOW-UP PRONOUN ‘POETRY’: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT ................................................................. 262 9.5 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF MECHANISMS OF NARRATIVE SELECTION: HOW YOSSI CHOSE WHAT TO SAY AND HOW TO SAY IT ............................................................... 266 9.5.1 Inclusion .......................................................................................................................... 267 9.5.2 Sharpening ...................................................................................................................... 271 9.5.3 Omission ......................................................................................................................... 274 9.5.4 Silencing.......................................................................................................................... 277 9.5.5 Flattening ........................................................................................................................ 279 9.5.6 ‘Appropriate’ meaning attribution .................................................................................. 282 9.5.7 Transferring ..................................................................................................................... 285 9.6 A NOTABLE EXCEPTION: YOSSI’S FOLLOW-UP SPONTANEOUS MINI-TAN ................................... 288 9.7 ‘VALIDATION’: YOSSI’S OWN WORDS ABOUT WHERE HE ‘STANDS’ ............................................ 291 9.8 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 296 ELI: “SO THAT’S IT, WHAT WE WENT THROUGH THAT DAY, IT WAS NICE…” AND SHIMI: “AND THAT’S IT…IT PASSED WITHOUT A PROBLEM” – TWO EXEMPLARS OF ‘STATIC’ NARRATIVE PROCESSING 10.1 INTRODUCTIONS TO ELI AND SHIMI ............................................................................................. 302 10.1.1 Introduction to Eli: Background, context, and impressions ............................................ 302 10.1.2 Introduction to Shimi: Background, context, and impressions ....................................... 304 10.2 COMPARATIVE DISCUSSION OF THE LIFE NARRATIVES OF ELI AND SHIMI ................................... 306 10.2.1 Eli’s baseline and follow-up life narratives..................................................................... 306 10.2.2 Shimi’s baseline and follow-up life narratives ................................................................ 308 10.3 QUANTITATIVE AND QUALITATIVE COMPARATIVE ANALYSES OF ELI’S TANS ........................... 310 10.3.1 Quantitative distribution of pronouns and tenses within Eli’s TANs: How much and when he said it ....................................................................................... 311 10.3.2 Pronoun ‘poetry’ of Eli’s TANs: The essence of what he said and how he said it ......... 315 10.3.2.1 Eli’s baseline TAN .......................................................................................... 315 10.3.2.2 Eli’s follow-up TAN ........................................................................................ 325 xiii 10.4 QUANTITATIVE AND QUALITATIVE COMPARATIVE ANALYSES OF SHIMI’S TANS....................... 333 10.4.1 Quantitative distribution of pronouns and tenses within Shimi’s TANs: How much and when he said it ....................................................................................... 333 10.4.2 Pronoun ‘poetry’ of Shimi’s TANs: The essence of what he said and how he said it .... 336 10.4.2.1 Shimi’s baseline TAN ...................................................................................... 336 10.4.2.2 Shimi’s follow-up TAN .................................................................................... 339 10.5 COMPARATIVE ANALYSES OF MECHANISMS OF NARRATIVE SELECTION: HOW ELI AND SHIMI CHOSE WHAT TO SAY AND HOW TO SAY IT ................................................. 342 10.5.1 Mechanisms of narrative selection in Eli’s TANs .......................................................... 344 10.5.2 Mechanisms of narrative selection in Shimi’s TANs ...................................................... 349 10.6 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 351 PART 4: Discussion and Conclusion 11 DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION: A GLOBAL PSYCHOSOCIAL AND COMPARATIVE VIEW ON THE RESEARCH STUDY AND ITS CONTRIBUTIONS 11.1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................................. 357 11.2 FINAL COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION ...................................................................... 358 11.3 THE ‘BIG PICTURE’: THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL, HIS NARRATIVE, AND THE SURROUNDING CONTEXT .................................................................... 365 11.3.1 The ‘narrative identity card’............................................................................................ 365 11.3.2 The dynamic relationship between language, narrative, culture, and media................... 369 11.4 THE CONTRIBUTIONS AND IMPLICATIONS OF THE STUDY ............................................................ 373 11.4.1 The NMAPP model and tool ........................................................................................... 373 11.4.2 Theoretical basis and support for the NMAPP................................................................ 376 11.5 THE LIMITATIONS OF THE RESEARCH STUDY AND AREAS FOR FURTHER INVESTIGATION ........... 379 11.6 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................................ 383 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................. 384 APPENDIX 1: Interview Questions Guide ................................................................................................... 401 APPENDIX 2: Informed Consent Form ....................................................................................................... 403 APPENDIX 3: Biographical Questionnaire .................................................................................................. 405 APPENDIX 4: Endnotes (All Original Hebrew Quotations) ........................................................................ 407 תקציר....................................................................................................................................................................ג הצהרה על מנחים..................................................................................................................................................... ב xiv LIST OF TABLES 3-1 3-2 3-3 3-4 3-5 3-6 The pronoun system in Modern Israeli Hebrew .................................................................................. 81 The sign-oriented interlocked tense system in Modern Israeli Hebrew .............................................. 84 Common MIH prepositions according to their relative deictic level .................................................. 87 Demonstrative example of pronoun ‘poetry’ .................................................................................... 105 Raw excerpt pair from parallel narrative table .................................................................................. 107 Excerpt pair from parallel narrative table, broken into component cells .......................................... 108 4-1 Guidelines for characteristics of narrative processing in a follow-up interview, relative to the respective baseline interview ..................................................................................... 142 5-1 5-2 5-3 5-4 5-5 Actual (generalized) quotes and possible (more personalized) alternatives ..................................... 150 Pronoun and tense distribution in Yossi’s baseline TAN ................................................................. 159 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Yossi’s baseline TAN ................................................................................ 160 Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Yossi’s baseline interview ...................... 165 Suggested ‘main messages’ of Yossi’s baseline interview ............................................................... 166 6-1 6-2 6-3 6-4 Pronoun and tense distribution in Dani’s baseline TrAN ................................................................. 185 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Dani’s baseline TrAN ................................................................................ 186 Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Dani’s baseline interview........................ 200 Suggested ‘main messages’ of Dani’s baseline interview................................................................. 201 7-1 7-2 7-3 7-4 7-5 7-6 7-7 Discursive pattern comparison between Motti’s and Dani’s parallel texts ....................................... 214 Primary pronoun and tense distribution in Motti’s baseline TAN .................................................... 216 Expanded pronoun and tense distribution in Motti’s baseline TAN ................................................. 217 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Motti’s baseline TAN ................................................................................ 218 Content regarding difficulty and indescribability in Motti’s baseline interview .............................. 225 Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Motti’s baseline interview ...................... 227 Suggested ‘main messages’ of Motti’s baseline interview ............................................................... 228 8-1 8-2 8-4 8-5 8-6 8-7 8-8 8-9 Relative comparison of pronoun use in the main narratives of Yossi, Dani, and Motti.................... 231 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases ...................................................................... 231 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of third-person ‘others’ and distancing discourse.................................................................................. 232 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of the MS ‘you’ ................................................... 233 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of the FS ‘you’ .................................................... 234 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of ‘I’ .................................................................... 235 Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of ‘we’ ................................................................. 236 Comprehensive pronominal extra-linguistic ‘main messages’ ......................................................... 236 Comparison of Yossi’s, Dani’s, and Motti’s definitions of “an Israeli man” ................................... 250 9-1 9-2 9-3 9-4 9-5 9-6 9-7 9-8 9-9 Comparative summary of LNs and spontaneous TANs in Yossi’s interviews ................................. 255 Summary of topics and their lengths (by number of lines) in Yossi’s LNs ...................................... 256 Spontaneous openings of the within-LN TANs in Yossi’s interviews ............................................. 258 Codes and characteristics of all of Yossi’s TANs ............................................................................. 259 Pronoun and tense distribution in all of Yossi’s TANs ..................................................................... 259 Pronoun and tense distribution percentages in Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs ........................................ 260 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Yossi’s follow-up ‘complete’ TAN (FUR) ............................................... 263 Inclusion within Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs....................................................................................... 268 Inclusion within Yossi’s follow-up TANs ........................................................................................ 269 8-3 xv 9-10 9-11 9-12 9-13 9-14 9-15 9-16 9-17 9-18 9-19 9-20 9-21 9-22 9-23 9-24 9-25 9-26 Sharpening in content and form across three of Yossi’s TANs ........................................................ 272 Sharpening in form across three of Yossi’s TANs ............................................................................ 273 Omission in content within Yossi’s follow-up ‘complete’ TAN ...................................................... 275 Omission or silencing in content within Yossi’s life narratives ........................................................ 277 Silencing in content within Yossi’s baseline life narrative ............................................................... 278 Flattening in content within Yossi’s follow-up TANs ...................................................................... 280 Flattening in form within Yossi’s follow-up ‘complete’ TAN ......................................................... 281 ‘Appropriate’ meaning attribution across three of Yossi’s TANs .................................................... 283 Transferring of the FS ‘you’ within Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs ........................................................ 285 Transferring of ‘I’ from ‘past’ to ‘present’ tense within Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs ......................... 287 Summation statements from Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs .................................................................... 292 Statements from Yossi’s interviews about getting back on a bus ..................................................... 293 Statements from Yossi’s interviews about psychological and overall functioning ........................... 294 Comparative summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Yossi’s interviews .............. 297 Comparative summary of discursive findings and interpretations of Yossi’s LNs and TANs ......... 298 Comparative summary of findings and interpretations regarding uses of mechanisms of narrative selection in Yossi’s interviews ..................................................... 299 Suggested ‘main messages’ of Yossi’s interviews ............................................................................ 301 Primary pronoun and tense distribution in Eli’s TANs ..................................................................... 311 Primary pronoun and tense distribution percentages in Eli’s TANs ................................................. 311 Expanded pronoun and tense distribution in Eli’s TANs .................................................................. 313 Expanded pronoun and tense distribution percentages in Eli’s TANs .............................................. 313 The first-person pronoun ‘poetry’ of Eli’s baseline TAN ................................................................. 315 The third-person pronoun ‘poetry’ of Eli’s baseline TAN ................................................................ 317 The first-person pronoun ‘poetry’ of Eli’s follow-up TAN .............................................................. 325 The third-person pronoun ‘poetry’ of Eli’s follow-up TAN ............................................................. 327 Pronoun and tense distribution in Shimi’s TANs.............................................................................. 334 Pronoun and tense distribution percentages in Shimi’s TANs .......................................................... 334 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Shimi’s baseline TAN ............................................................................... 336 The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Shimi’s follow-up TAN............................................................................. 339 Inclusion, omission, and flattening across Eli’s TANs ..................................................................... 344 Inclusion, sharpening, flattening, and meaning attribution in Eli’s follow-up TAN ........................ 346 Inclusion and meaning attribution across Eli’s TANs ...................................................................... 348 Inclusion across Shimi’s TANs ......................................................................................................... 349 Omission in Shimi’s follow-up TAN ................................................................................................ 350 Summary statements in Shimi’s TANs ............................................................................................. 351 Comparative summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Eli’s and Shimi’s interviews ............................................................................................................. 352 10-20 Comparative summary of discursive findings and interpretations of Eli’s and Shimi’s LNs and TANs...................................................................................................... 353 10-21 Comparative summary of findings and interpretations regarding uses of mechanisms of narrative selection in Eli’s and Shimi’s interviews ...................................... 354 10-22 Suggested ‘main messages’ of Eli’s and Shimi’s interviews ............................................................ 356 10-1 10-2 10-3 10-4 10-5 10-6 10-7 10-8 10-9 10-10 10-11 10-12 10-13 10-14 10-15 10-16 10-17 10-18 10-19 11-1 11-2 Relative comparison of biographical, discursive, and psychosocial data among all five participants, as of their baseline interviews .............................................................. 359 Relative comparison of biographical, discursive, and psychosocial data among the three relevant participants, as of their follow-up interviews ........................................... 363 xvi LIST OF FIGURES 2-1 2-2 Occurrence of suicide bombing attacks within the Green Line, 1994-2011 ....................................... 40 Timeline of suicide bombing attacks within the Green Line, 1994-2007 ........................................... 40 5-1 Pronoun-tense collocations in Yossi’s baseline TAN ....................................................................... 159 6-1 Pronoun-tense collocations in Dani’s baseline TrAN ....................................................................... 185 7-1 7-2 Primary pronoun-tense collocations in Motti’s baseline TAN.......................................................... 216 Expanded pronoun-tense collocations in Motti’s baseline TAN....................................................... 217 8-1 Comparison of pronoun use in the main narratives of Yossi, Dani, and Motti ................................. 230 9-1 9-2 Distribution of volume dedicated to life periods in Yossi’s LNs ...................................................... 255 Pronoun-tense collocations in Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs ................................................................. 261 10-1 10-2 10-3 Primary pronoun-tense collocations in Eli’s TANs .......................................................................... 312 Expanded pronoun-tense collocations in Eli’s TANs ....................................................................... 314 Pronoun-tense collocations in Shimi’s TANs ................................................................................... 335 11-1 Schematic diagram showing comparative hypotheses and interrelationships among all five participants, as of their baseline interviews .............................................................. 362 Schematic diagram showing comparative hypotheses and interrelationships among the three relevant participants, as of their follow-up interviews ........................................... 364 11-2 xvii LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS BL Baseline BLI Baseline interview BLL (Yossi’s) Baseline within-LN TAN C. Cell / cells (followed by number, denoting cell(s) of comparative table being referenced) E. Entry / entries (followed by number, denoting entry(ies) of pronoun poem being referenced) EP End point FS Feminine singular (pronoun or verb form) FU Follow-up FUI Follow-up interview FUL (Yossi’s) Follow-up within-LN TAN FUR (Yossi’s) Follow-up requested (‘complete’) TAN FUS (Yossi’s) Follow-up spontaneous (mini-)TAN IDF Israel Defense Forces L. Line / lines (followed by number, denoting line(s) of text excerpt being referenced) LN Life narrative MIH Modern Israeli Hebrew MNS Mechanism of narrative selection MP Masculine plural (pronoun or verb form) MS Masculine singular (pronoun or verb form) NMAPP Narrative Method for Assessment of Psychosocial Processing of Stressful Life Events NII Israel’s National Insurance Institute, or Bituach Leumi PPr Proximate-present (tense form) PTSD Post-traumatic stress disorder RF Remote-future (tense form) RPa Remote-past (tense form) TA Terror attack TAN Terror attack narrative TrA Traffic accident TrAN Traffic accident narrative xviii GUIDE TO TRANSCRIPTIONS The following is a guide to the (relatively conventional) rules utilized in the transcriptions of the interview material gathered as part of this study. All original Hebrew quotations are included as endnotes, organized by chapter, at the conclusion of this dissertation. All italics in quotations are mine, added to the texts to emphasize the particular linguistic phenomena under discussion. TEXT SIGN (example(s)) SIGNIFIES underlined text speech that is relatively louder (than the rest of the speech in the given interview) boldface text speech that is relatively more emphatic {words within curly brackets} speech that is relatively softer or quieter ^words within upward arrows^ speech that is relatively more rapid <words within outside arrows> speech that is relatively slower words with repeated letters (“I thiiink”) speech in which the indicated syllables or parts of these words were elongated in some manner a part of a word with a hyphen (“thi-”) a word that was cut off, spoken only partially, or interrupted in some manner (in most cases it was possible to understand or hypothesize with reasonable certainty as to the intended word) a part of a word with an apostrophe, usually conjoined with the following word (“th’people”) a word or multiple words that were spoken quickly or connected together (a frequent phenomenon in MIH of speakers connecting words as a ‘shortcut’ to save linguistic time, and often, skipping a few vowels in the process) ITALIC CAPITAL LETTERS word(s) that were originally spoken in English (numbers within parentheses) the amount of seconds that a particular pause in speech lasted ((notes within double parentheses)) (“((sharp intake of breath))”, “((chuckling))”, “((weeping))”, ((car horn honking))”) extra-linguistic or contextual clues, such as tone, breathing, emotional expressions, or external noises \((notes within slashes))\ a type of speech, tone, or emotional expression that continued for the entire stretch of talk within the slashes [notes within square brackets] additions of words or explanations for the reader with regard to potentially unclear phrases in the discourse, or an indication of a personal detail camouflaged or removed to protect the interviewee’s confidentiality (“I was driving on [bus] line [19]”) (“[you/they/we (MP)] walked”) NOTE: this includes cases when the specific pronominal form is not indicated with its collocated verb, whereby the pronoun equivalent (or possible equivalents, where certainty is not possible) is noted in [square brackets] preceding the verb “…” the omission of a small section of the original interview text that is inconsequential to the discursive phenomenon under discussion, such as an affirmative utterance by the interviewer or extra-linguistic contextual information xix In addition, it may be necessary to note several even more basic transcription rules: TEXT SIGN (example(s)) SIGNIFIES period (“I walked on the street.”) falling tone at end of statement, suggesting completion of the given message question mark (“Did I walk on that street?”) rising tone at end of statement, suggesting question or uncertainty in the given message comma (“I walked, then stopped, then walked some more.”) pause lasting less than one second, with similar tone before and afterwards, suggesting a continuation of the given message after the pause And finally, it is important to note that the issue of dialogue and quotation marks was not a minor one in the context of this study, and involved a number of subjective, interpretive decisions on the part of the transcribers and myself. The final conclusion reached is reflected in the following rules: TEXT SIGN (example(s)) SIGNIFIES double quotation marks three options, according to the interviewee’s discourse and to the best of my interpretation: (“I said, “Look where you’re going!” ”) 1) dialogue that was likely actually spoken, or that the interviewee seems to believe was actually spoken, at the time of the experience (“You (MS) say, “Look where you’re going!” ”) 2) dialogue that could have been spoken out loud at the time of the experience (e.g., generalized ‘you (MS)’-based external dialogue) (“I said to myself, “What are you doing?” ”) 3) self-dialogue that was or could have been spoken out loud single quotation marks three options, according to the interviewee’s discourse and to the best of my interpretation: (“You say, ‘What am I doing?’ ”) 1) dialogue that was likely not actually spoken, or that the interviewee seems to believes was not actually spoken, at the time of the experience (“I thought to myself, ‘Don’t drive so fast’ ”; “You (MS) think to yourself, ‘Don’t drive so fast’ ”) 2) self-dialogue that was not spoken out loud at the time of the experience (e.g., generalized ‘you (MS)’-based internal dialogue) (“God says to you, ‘I’ll take care of you’ ”) 3) imagined dialogue that was not spoken out loud (e.g., any God-related dialogue) xx ABSTRACT Introduction For much of the last three decades, the Israeli population has been exposed to significant and continuing stress, primarily from the ever-present threat of terror attacks. The First and Second Intifadas (1987-1993 and 2000-2004/2005, respectively) took place predominantly within the confines of the Green Line border of Israel (as of the eve of the Six Day War in 1967); with the hardest-hit targets highly populated civilian entities such as restaurants, cafés, malls, and buses. Besides conscripted soldiers, therefore, the ‘front lines’ of these conflicts involved three primary populations: security guards, restaurant service staff, and bus drivers. Each post is defined by its own job responsibilities, and must negotiate its own crises when confronted with a security threat. However, in Israel, neither security guards nor restaurant staff operate on a daily basis in a context as complicated and problematic as that of bus drivers. Indeed, as a result of a convoluted interplay of factors, including their personal senses of identity, cultural background, military experience and mentality, worldview and value systems, and the occupational and societal expectations of them, Israeli bus drivers appear to exist in the crux of a fundamental dilemma. The present research study focuses on this population and on these critical issues. In Israel, (most often armed) security personnel stand guard outside stores, malls, restaurants, movie theaters, bus stations, universities, and nearly all other public establishments— charged with the singular task of protecting those who enter. They are required to inspect the personal belongings of everyone, either by physically opening backpacks and bags or by supervising their placement into an x-ray machine. They are allowed and encouraged to request identification if they deem it warranted, and to take any other measures necessary to inspect suspicious individuals. These men and women, often fresh out of the army, are presented with a clear job description and are fully aware that part of it requires them to sacrifice their lives in order to protect the public, if the need arises. Indeed, if they were to see a man walking toward them with a bomb, their duty would obligate them to jump on him or subdue him using any means necessary. These ‘means’ might likely entail losing their own lives. On the other hand, restaurant service people exist on the opposite side of the spectrum. Their singular task is to serve their clientele, with no particular regard to their safety or any possible threat posed. Waiters and waitresses rarely feel any significant degree of responsibility for their customers, and have no obligation to protect their patrons. Their job responsibilities do not involve anything related to (national or individual) security concerns; thus, if a suspicious individual were to somehow manage to enter a restaurant or café, the staff would likely run in the opposite direction and feel no guilt for doing so. There is no set societal expectation (spoken or otherwise) for their behavior, and rarely has a media report mentioned a “hero waiter.” Bus drivers in this context, however, exist in a near-impossible dilemma—caught between xxi responsibilities and expectations to both serve and protect. When faced with a daily reality that involves the possibility of a potential suicide bomber boarding their bus at any moment, they must constantly heed both of these duties and be vigilant at every moment. Indeed, the bus drivers interviewed herein expressed that they often find themselves functioning in multiple roles at the same time: as a driver, security guard, policeman, and even army commander. Although this confluence of roles often is not necessarily disadvantageous, the separate functions can contradict each other, creating a serious conflict of interests and a consequent sense of guilt and inadequacy in the drivers. That most drivers served in the army during (multiple) wars and military campaigns, and have internalized sociocultural expectations of ‘The Israeli Warrior’—particularly given the “centrality of military matters” in Israel (Lomsky-Feder & Ben-Ari, 1999)—also contributes significantly to their belief systems, behaviors, and subsequent attributions about their actions. Indeed, the Zionist ethos of the new Israeli ‘superman’ is such an ingrained part of the Israeli belief system and collective identity that it has continued to permeate successive generations (Ben-Ya’acov, Amir, Arzy, & Kotler, 2005; Solomon, 1993). Modern Israeli soldiers tend to idealize themselves and their peers as “immune, invulnerable and free from weaknesses” (BenYaacov et al., 2005, p. 7), and there is an essentially ‘unwritten rule’ that anything less would be considered unacceptable and even threatening to the national identity. Israeli male bus drivers grew up and continue to exist in this sociocultural reality, whereby a premium is placed on selfless heroism and courage and strength ‘no matter what.’ Idalovichi (2004), following Golani (2003) and Nimni (2003), referred to a likening in modern Israeli society of the war ethos as “almost the only Zionist ethos,” and a perception of heroism in combat as “the ultimate heroism” (p. 623). Israel and its society indeed exist within a complex and emotionally charged set of psychosocial contexts and discourses. With its nationhood having been built upon the Holocaust and its victims and survivors, Israel’s very reason for being and national purpose is intricately related to a collective and historical trauma of unprecedented, and personal, proportions. The protracted sense of wartime and conflict, as well as the constant sense of existential threat since its establishment, has created a national identity as both a victim and a fighter, and has engendered a society of soldiers, warriors, and brave heroes (and those who aspire to be so). It is within this context that terror in Israel has come to be “part of a collective consciousness,” essentially “institutionalized” on a national level (Coetzee & Rau, 2009, p. 3), and for this reason that the psychosocial discourse surrounding it feels imposed on all, by all. Israel’s and Israeli Jews’ existential struggle for survival as a nation and as individuals, against the backdrop of the ‘old Jew’’s essential failure to survive in Europe, has thus led to tremendous pressure and a fundamentally unattainable set of expectations placed upon modern Israeli Jewish men. They believe that they must fight (on the front lines), win against all odds, be level-headed and proactive under pressure, be brave and heroic, compensate for past losses, vanquish future enemies, and do all this without becoming traumatized or showing weakness, passivity, or emotional vulnerability. xxii The myth of The Israeli Warrior is as deeply believed in as it is impossible to attain, and thus, has a powerful hold on those who live their lives in its shadow and attempting to fulfill its prescription. However, Israeli bus drivers are not policemen and are neither (active) soldiers nor warriors; they do not carry weapons, and there are no clear and set rules regarding their official responsibilities under conditions and in emergency situations such as those that frequently emerge during an Intifada. While their job description specifies that they are responsible for “transporting their passengers safely and efficiently from their point of origin to their point of destination,” this duty becomes infinitely complicated (and ultimately, can be entirely compromised) during periods of high security threat. Drivers tend to feel a much greater degree of responsibility towards their jobs and their clientele than that which is ‘officially’ (or unofficially) sanctioned under normal circumstances; this sense of duty is only further heightened when the national threat of terror attacks increases. Officially, they are instructed to “use their judgment,” and are afforded a certain degree of autonomy and ex post facto legitimacy for their decisions vis-à-vis security, safety, and professional authority. Unofficially, however, many of their job responsibilities, duties, and capabilities are simply not defined, with the consequence that already permeable boundaries appear even more complex and ‘fuzzy’ during periods of violence escalation. This dynamic places the drivers into a situation that ultimately is fraught with ambivalence and uncertainty, engendering doubt about their behavior (and what the ‘correct behavior’ may even be) and guilt about any range of possible outcomes that may result. It is this confluence of factors that creates an exceedingly difficult situation for bus drivers in this context. The importance and fundamentality of their identities as drivers is coupled with their heightened sense of responsibility for their buses and passengers, and interacts with a lessthan-clearly defined job description but attendant perceptions of impossibly high sociocultural expectations of their behavior. All of this, in turn, creates a powerful potential dynamic of guilt, an exaggerated sense of culpability, coping dilemmas, and a desire to achieve the elusive (but likely unattainable) ‘hero status.’ Israeli bus drivers thus represent a complex and highly illuminating case for the study of the interplay between individual and societal discourse, and between the narrative and communicative ‘product,’ on the one hand, and the narrator’s inner psychological world and coping processes, on the other. Purpose This phenomenological, qualitative, narrative research study endeavors to investigate the nature and manifestation of individual narrative processing and of the complex and dynamic relationship between personal and societal discourse, on both a short-term and longitudinal basis. The study seeks to do this through an examination of the discourse and communicative strategies of male Jewish Israeli bus drivers who experienced terror attacks while on duty. In-depth, semistructured, open-ended qualitative interviews were conducted with eight drivers in Jerusalem and Be’er Sheva. Five of the participants were then re-interviewed three years later, following the same xxiii interview technique and structure. In addition to garnering life stories and narratives on the terror attack(s) experienced, the overarching topics of the interviews surrounded issues of identity, worldview, systems of coping and meaning-making, sociocultural norms, and overall belief and attribution structures. The principal research question of this study is essentially: “How are the narratives of male Jewish Israeli bus drivers who experienced terror attacks both reflective of and shaped by their perceptions of their own coping, resilience, and heroism, and by their perceptions of the surrounding sociocultural discourse on these topics?” In its exploration of this question, the study involves four central domains. The first is an integrative quantitative and qualitative discourse and narrative analysis of each participant’s baseline interview data, from which emerged a focus on each individual’s differential uses of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages. The second is an integrative quantitative and qualitative discourse and narrative analysis of each participant’s follow-up interview data, which allowed for a focus on each individual’s longitudinal trends in the use of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages. The third involves comparative analyses among participants, from which emerged a focus on the similarities and disparities across the drivers with regard to their uses of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages, as well as their experiences, attitudes and beliefs, and the influence of the societal discourse on their personal discourse. Finally, the fourth focus involves comparative analyses within participants, from which emerged an understanding of the longitudinal processes involved, and a capacity to recognize and assess ‘improvement’ vs. ‘deterioration’ vs. ‘static-ness’ in narrative processing over time. Analysis of the transcribed interview texts involves an exploration of the personal, familial, communal, societal, and cultural forces that are at a constant interplay in the physical and psychological lives of these individuals. The goal of this study is to offer a multidimensional perspective on narrative processing in the specific context of Israeli bus drivers. Furthermore, the underlying presumption is that the findings can contribute to broader postulations about the manners in which members of Israeli society at large may negotiate, cope with, and process their varied experiences within a context of ongoing and widespread life stress. It is proposed here that the communicative strategies shown in the form and content of these individuals’ discourse can reveal their feelings, attitudes, and attributions with regard to their perceptions of the prevailing sociocultural discourse surrounding them. In the case of male Israeli bus drivers, this discourse seems to facilitate and encourage a collective perception of an obligation to act and cope resiliently and ‘heroically’ in the face of threat, and to discourage ‘less acceptable’ reactions. Data collection Interviews were conducted with bus drivers who experienced at least one terror attack— including bombings, shootings, stone-throwings, and Molotov cocktails, perpetrated or attempted in xxiv a suicidal and/or homicidal manner—either directly or as a first-hand witness. All of the participants were male and Jewish, and between the ages of 29 and 64 and of various marital statuses at the time of initial contact. Participation in the study was entirely voluntary and without compensation, and interviews were conducted at the participants’ homes, at their convenience. The interviews were conducted, transcribed, and analyzed in Hebrew. Because of the micro-analytical nature of the research, the transcription process was meticulous, including changes in tone, volume, and speed of speech; lengths of pauses; any signs of emotion (e.g., laughing, crying, audible inhaling or exhaling of breath); non-verbal, extra-linguistic, or contextual information (e.g., a siren outside, or sound of the participant pounding on the table, or a comment made by the participant’s wife); and all utterances that held information about the interviewee-interviewer dynamic as well. Excerpts have been translated to English only when necessary for publication and for this dissertation. Most of the interviews took place during a relatively calm period in Israel vis-à-vis busrelated terror attacks, although many other types of attacks took place both within Israel and on several of its borders—including bulldozer attacks, bombings at roadblocks and shopping malls, a particularly horrific shooting spree at a yeshiva in Jerusalem, and a number of Qassam and Grad rocket attacks on private houses and populated civilian areas (Israel Ministry of Foreign Affairs). This period also encompassed the Second Lebanon War, in the summer of 2006, and Operation Cast Lead in Gaza, in the winter of 2008-9. This national context set the scene for many of the topics discussed in the interviews, and often played an integral part in the participants’ discourse. Conceptual and methodological frameworks This research study is interdisciplinary at its foundation, as the conceptual, theoretical, and methodological frameworks and contexts are drawn from a wide variety of fields. The overarching conceptual framework rests on the salutogenic paradigmatic orientation, and adopts the ‘ease/disease’ continuum as the lens through which to view ‘progress’ (in either direction) in the interviewees’ narrative processing development over time (rather than utilizing more dichotomous terms, such as ‘improvement’ or ‘deterioration’) (Antonovsky, 1979, 1987). In addition, the literature on stress and coping and the dominant salutogenic concepts therein, such as resilience, hardiness, and ‘working through,’ provide the psychological perspective most heavily relied upon. The primary message is that, although this research deals with individuals who experienced an undoubtedly stressful and potentially traumatic life event, there is no presumption that they were traumatized by it, nor that their recounting of the experience should fall under the category of a ‘trauma narrative.’ Instead, the salutogenic perspective emphasizes the manners in which these drivers are coping well, behaving resiliently, and drawing upon their existing psychic and emotional resources to work through these difficult experiences in the most adaptive manner of which they are capable. Alongside of this, the psycholinguistic conceptual framework draws on the literature on xxv narrating trauma and the properties and dynamics of traumatic memory, the processes and nature of retelling and narrative processing over time, and the relationship between such narrations (and various discursive strategies therein, such as pronominal patterns) and the health of the narrator. The macro-level psychosocial context surrounding the bus drivers is also taken into account, from both a theoretical and phenomenological perspective. This involves a discussion of the political and security realities of living in Israel, under the near-constant threat of war, terror, and violent tensions, and the effects this can have on its populace. Recent literature on the psychological effects of both acute and chronic stress and trauma—in general, and in its shared and communal nature in the Israeli case, in particular—is reviewed herein. Here, too, the salutogenic perspective is essential, allowing for an exploration of both the potential negative effects as well as the possibility for resilient responses to these challenges, and the unique strain of ‘Israeli resilience.’ The context of the study also involves a focus on experiences and perceptions surrounding the disparity between military and civilian trauma in the Israeli context, with particular emphasis on the isolation and marginalization expressed by many of the individuals interviewed herein. A discussion of the primary characteristics of and hegemonic myths in Israeli culture is also provided, viewed through the emphasis on masculinity, strength, and heroism; the ‘culture of power’ and centrality of the military; and peer pressure to cope and act ‘acceptably.’ The linguistic methodological framework of the study revolves around sign-oriented semiotic theory and discursive analytical techniques associated with the Columbia School approach. These are utilized to understand the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages encoded in the discourse, reflecting the personal and societal factors that may impact these individuals’ processes of meaning-making and narrative processing. In particular, a theoretical background in sign-oriented linguistics and a semiotic approach to Modern Israeli Hebrew are relied upon to investigate the non-random distribution of pronouns and tenses in these narratives, in relation to the reflected degree of personalization vs. generalization and proximity vs. distance with regard to the speaker/narrator. At the heart of this study lies the central assertion that there is a relationship between the manner in which one tells and processes his personal narrative, on the one hand, and the nature and influence of the sociocultural discourse surrounding him, on the other. The study methodology is naturalistic and phenomenological, with a focus on the participants’ particular, constructed realities and meanings. It is situated within the interpretive or constructivist family of paradigms, which is based on a relativistic ontological view with a nonstatic, specific, and localized focus. A number of techniques and approaches are employed— separately as well as in concert—throughout, to facilitate an in-depth analysis of the content, form, and attendant meanings of the interviewees’ discourse. Together, they permit an intricate exploration of both the participants’ lived experiences and their processes of narrating them, as well as a located view of the experience of the investigator and the dynamic and mutual interplay between analyst, analysis, and the analyzed text. The analytical approaches used here are largely xxvi abductive; that is, the hypotheses emerge from the data and the ‘field,’ in relation and response to the material itself, rather than from a pre-determined theory. The method of this study is also interdisciplinary and mixed, with the particular operationalization of mixed methods employed herein involving the use of both qualitative and quantitative techniques to analyze solely qualitative data. Different analytical contexts and questions often called for different methodologies and tools; thus, throughout the analyses, the analytical magnifying glass was widened and narrowed, constantly redefining the analytical unit in focus. Analysis was conducted primarily on the narratives of the terror attacks experienced by the participants. However, there were also cases when the focus was placed on another narrative within a participant’s interview; or his life narrative; or multiple narratives on a related topic within a single interview; or ‘parallel’ narratives across both of a participant’s interviews; or multiple narratives on a similar topic across several participants’ interviews. One of the central organizing principles of the psychosocial theoretical and methodological framework of this research is the analytical dichotomy of content vs. form; that is, what the interviewees said vs. how they said it. Many of the analytical methods utilized herein spiral between these foci, while also shifting between holistic and categorical viewpoints on the qualitative data. The basic questions with regard to content were: 1) How was the terror attack perceived, comprehended, and narrated by the interviewees?; 2) How did the interviewees describe their strategies of coping with fear and stress, and processes of meaning-making therein?; 3) How did the interviewees view their life as a whole and the place of the TA they experienced within it, and what meaning did they attach therein?; and 4) When, how, and why did certain societal perceptions and cultural discourses (e.g., behavioral expectations such as masculinity and heroism) emerge manifestly in the interviewees’ discourse, and what was the attendant meaning of these issues to the interviewees? These questions were asked on both the individual and collective/comparative levels; that is, both within each participant’s pair of interviews and among all of the interview texts. This level of questioning remained very much on the surface of the interviewees’ discourse, focusing almost solely on what they specifically said about these topics and performing very little, if any, interpretive analyses or inquiring as to what they may have meant to express ‘between the lines.’ The basic questions with regard to form were: 1) How was the narrative of the TA constructed (e.g., what was included vs. omitted or silenced; chronological vs. thematic organization)?; 2) Which discursive signs and patterns were repeated throughout a narrative or interview as a whole, and what meaning may be ascribed to these patterns?; and 3) How did central themes or issues express themselves latently rather than manifestly in the discourse? This level of analysis existed almost entirely below the surface of the spoken discourse, searching for semiotic patterns that likely were not or could not be apparent to the interviewee himself, and require multifaceted interpretation and the proposing of hypotheses with regard to motivation and significance. xxvii On the micro-level and within the operational implementation of the analyses, the following five questions were considered at all points and stages throughout: 1) Why is this word or phrase used here, and not in other places?; 2) What is the meaning, both in this sentence/context and in the larger picture, of the usage of this word or phrase here?; 3) How else could this be phrased, and how might the meaning change as a result?; 4) Are there any discernible patterns of usage of certain words or phrases?; and 5) What does this discursive distribution and/or communicative strategy express or reflect about this individual, and about the society and culture in which he lives? Basic linguistic background Tobin (1990, 1995) claimed that “successful linguistic communication can only be achieved through the combined effort of an encoder and a decoder cooperating together” (p. 48, p. 11). As such, this research relies on a sign-oriented understanding of the Modern Israeli Hebrew pronoun system in terms of its invariant meanings: encoder (first-person), decoder (second-person), and other than the encoder/decoder or outsider (third-person). This pronoun system has distinct forms distinguishing singular and plural for all three persons, and gender-specific forms for second- and third-person pronouns. In Modern Israeli Hebrew, the first-person pronominal forms (‘I’ and ‘we’) are gender-neutral, as the speaker(s) are readily identifiable by means of the collocated verbs. Contrarily, the second- and third-person pronominal forms are marked for both gender (masculine/feminine) and number (singular/plural). The masculine pronouns are the unmarked forms, as they can be used to refer to either male or female objects, and verbs are generally characterized by the base form. Feminine forms, on the other hand, require additional obligatory suffixes, and are used to denote feminine objects only. The masculine singular form can be applied to a specific male and/or as an inclusive gender form for either or both genders together. Essentially, the unmarked masculine forms can be used in all contexts (all-male, mixed, and even all-female groups), while the marked feminine forms are customarily used only in all-female groups or in relation to a specific female decoder(s) (and even then, the feminine forms can and frequently are replaced by the unmarked masculine forms for use in expressing generic and/or general messages). The first-person singular pronoun (‘ani’ in Hebrew; ‘I’ in English) signifies speaker/writer (‘encoder’), referring to ‘the one who speaks here and now.’ Because it is ego-centered, it is considered the most proximate and personal pronoun. It is unmarked for (that is, neutral to) gender, and is the most specific or ‘known’ pronoun—as its use leaves little ambiguity as to the identity or nature of the speaker. This form is used to relate to personalized events, actions, or states, and cannot represent a relation to anything or anyone but the encoder. As Pennebaker (2002) noted, “The use of 1st person singular (I, me, my)…provides insight into people’s social identity and ‘ownership’ of their speaking or writing topic” (p. 8). The second-person masculine singular (MS) pronoun (‘atah’ in Hebrew; ‘you’ in English) signifies audience/listener/reader (‘decoder’), and, as it is unmarked, is considered one of the most xxviii general and neutral pronouns (Tobin, 2001). It has two functions: 1) its traditional usage, to address a specific male decoder; and 2) a more generic, general, and impersonal usage corresponding roughly to the non-gendered and non-numbered ‘you’ in English. In the latter case, it functions as the unmarked form, customarily used in non-gendered discourse or in relation to a non-specific subject (similar to, in English, “When one does this type of work…” or the generalized, “When you wake up in the morning…”). However, the MS ‘you’ is also used in discourse that does not—or does not necessarily—call for the unmarked form. Indeed, feminists have reported cases, both in private conversations and the media, of two women using the MS ‘you’ in discourse with each other, even in such obviously gendered cases as, “When you (MS) are pregnant…” or, “When you (MS) are nursing your (MS) baby…” (Tobin, personal communication). Thus, it has been hypothesized that the use of the unmarked MS ‘you’ serves to create a depersonalized or universalized sense of meaning, allowing relation to ‘one’ or ‘every/anyperson.’ The second-person feminine singular (FS) pronoun (‘at’ in Hebrew; ‘you’ in English) also signifies decoder, and also has two functions: 1) to address a specific female decoder; and 2) a generic and/or general manner of usage between two females or in all-female groups, and specific or generic (female) gendered situations and contexts (i.e., one might hear, “When you (FS) are pregnant…” in this context). This second function has moved more into popular (and, some say, even provocative) use in the past few years—primarily by feminists and the younger generation— but is not viewed as conventional or widely accepted across generations or population sectors. As the FS ‘you’ is marked for gender and number (Tobin, 2001), it is customarily utilized only in person- or gender-specific discursive situations. Thus, the choice to use it (rather than the standard neutral, unmarked MS form) may create a sense of greater proximity and less neutrality toward the female individual being addressed. In the context of this study, it is suggested that a male bus driver who uses the FS ‘you’ while speaking to a female interviewer about his own experiences may be demonstrating an entirely different communicative strategy—one that has particular implications regarding his attitude toward what he is describing and/or toward the specific decoder. Central patterns and guiding hypotheses Essentially, the foundation for this study is the linguistic assumption that the non-random distribution of language within the drivers’ discourse may hold a specific set of meanings and messages—which may, in turn, offer insight into their inner psychological worlds. In particular, the guiding hypothesis is that their patterned use of pronouns and tenses may reflect their feelings, attributions, and perceptions surrounding the terror attack they experienced, and its place within their life narrative and life history. Specifically, this research asserts the following: 1) there exists a number of specific, non-random patterns in these drivers’ discourse; 2) there exists a relationship between each individual driver’s discursive patterns and his relevant perceptions and attributions; 3) there exists a meta-connection between this relationship and each driver’s perceptions of his xxix surrounding societal discourse, particularly with regard to coping and resilience, masculinity and heroism, and ‘acceptable’ behavior; and 4) this multifaceted dynamic relationship between the individual’s discourse, his inner psychological world, and his perceptions of the surrounding societal discourse can be utilized as a window to assess his longitudinal process of narrative processing of the event he experienced. Four central discursive patterns were found across the interviews conducted for this study. First, first-person singular possessive pronouns (such as, ‘me,’ and, ‘my’) and phrases were found to frequently be the subject of the sentence when the encoder was either: 1) describing an experience that he saw as personalized for himself; or 2) attempting to personalize the experience for himself. Second, the second-person MS decoder was found to frequently be the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: 1) describing an experience that he saw as depersonalized from himself; or 2) attempting to distance the experience from himself; and/or 3) describing an experience in which he behaved passively and/or was being acted upon (particularly by the perpetrator of the terror attack). Third, the second-person FS decoder was found to frequently be the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: 1) describing difficult, stressful, and/or upsetting sensory experiences (particularly relating to the terror attack and its aftereffects); and/or 2) describing something that he perceived as particularly important for the interviewer to know, understand, and/or vicariously experience. And fourth, the first-person singular encoder (‘I’) was found to frequently be the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: 1) describing an experience in which he was performing in an active, powerful, and/or ‘heroic’ manner; and/or 2) describing an experience or behavior of which he approved, or saw as ‘acceptable.’ Furthermore, from a basic quantitative assessment of the pronoun and tense collocations in the interviewees’ central narratives, three clear patterns emerged. First, the MS ‘you’ was collocated most frequently with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense. Second, the FS ‘you’ was collocated almost exclusively with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense. And third, the ‘I’ could be collocated with all tenses, with the following interpretive sub-patterns therein: 1) collocation of the ‘I’ with the ‘past’ (remote + experience-able) tense seemed to indicate a higher level of narrative processing and (appropriate) psychological distance; and 2) collocation of the ‘I’ with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense seemed to indicate a lower level of narrative processing and a greater sense of proximity and salience (‘alive-ness’ of the narration). These linguistic patterns were further explored, leading to a forging of a connection between them and the stated personal feelings and attitudes of the interviewees about their experiences and their perceptions of their surrounding societal discourse. Consequently, this research ultimately asserts four central psychosociolinguistic interpretive hypotheses regarding the communicative strategies of the drivers interviewed herein. First, it is hypothesized that first-person singular possessive pronouns and phrases are used in contexts in which the encoder feels particularly connected to or responsible for the experience he xxx is describing. This can be viewed as a discursive technique that can pinpoint the speaker’s sense of responsibility and/or guilt (or lack thereof) regarding the narrated experience, and identification with facets therein (e.g., the victims—both animate and inanimate, such as the bus itself). A heightened focus on the speaker’s personalized sense of the experience appears to substitute for an acknowledgment of the collective nature of it in actuality. Second, it is hypothesized that the unmarked MS ‘you’ pronoun is used predominantly in contexts in which the encoder is making an attempt to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize what he is describing, out of a need to fit in with his surrounding society (and its norms). This can be viewed as a technique aimed at encouraging, assisting, or requesting of the decoder/ audience to relate in a generalized manner to the speaker himself and/or to particular aspects of the described experience. This may be utilized in neutral contexts, but may also come into play in connection to ‘less acceptable’ aspects of the experience, which are often steeped in ambivalence and uncertainty for the driver. This may particularly involve moments during which, he apparently believes, he could or should have acted, thought, or felt differently—and thus, sees himself as having not lived up to certain internalized personal, cultural, and/or societal expectations. The MS ‘you’ thus helps the encoder to send the message that the given issue or experience should be universal, or that he wishes it were universal. In this sense, if the behavior or feeling is presented as universal and ‘normal,’ then it can at least be a candidate for reinterpretation by the audience as ‘acceptable,’ in spite of the speaker’s ambivalence about it. Third, it is hypothesized that the marked FS ‘you’ pronoun is used predominantly in the most difficult experiential contexts, in which the encoder is making an attempt to connect specifically and personally with me (the young, female, ‘new immigrant,’ advanced psychology student and interviewer), and bring me in to what he is describing. This can be viewed as a technique aimed at dealing with the most difficult and emotionally charged aspects (the ‘unacceptable’ parts) of the described experience. These facets often simply do not lend themselves to universalization, generalization, or normalization, as they are frequently the most specific and specialized details of the terror attack, such as the experience of seeing blood and smelling burning flesh. Because discursive ownership of these descriptions (by means of the ‘I’ pronoun) may be viewed, in the drivers’ eyes, as an acknowledgment of all the psychological ‘baggage’ that would likely accompany such an experience, the FS ‘you’ is most frequently employed to displace these difficult and charged statements, or to share them with the decoder. In this sense, the FS ‘you’ takes on all the facets of the terror attack experience that are viewed as ‘unacceptable’ within Israeli society—the trauma, the weakness, the moments of paralysis before the ‘correct’ measures were taken. This is perhaps the most complicated of the interpretive hypotheses, because it carries with it five potential sub-hypotheses with regard to motivation: a) an attempt to both distance the given feeling, statement, or experience from himself and project it onto me—as represented in the xxxi (hypothetical) statement, “I bring you in to it so you can take it away from me”; b) an attempt to cause me to personally identify with or experience vicariously the given feeling, statement, or experience—as represented as, “I bring you in to it so you can share it with me”; c) an attempt to cope with the given feeling, statement, or experience—as represented as, “I bring you in to it so it won’t be so hard for me to handle/so you can take some of the burden off me”; d) an attempt to educate me—as represented as, “I bring you in to it as much as I can so you can try to understand it, because no one can understand it without going through it”; and e) an attempt to connect, for the simple sake of connection—as represented as, “I bring you in to it as much as I can, because I feel so alone with it.” All of these motivational hypotheses are equally valid and plausible, and it may not be possible to discern with certainty which may have specifically contributed to a given interviewee’s use of the FS ‘you’ at any given discursive moment. Furthermore, this communicative strategy prompts the fundamental question as to whether a visceral, discursive connection with one’s audience—regardless of the motivation for doing so—can, in itself, help the narrator in some way to cope with or process the difficult aspects of a stressful life experience, such as a terror attack. It is possible that each of the interviewees used this discursive strategy for different reasons; each nonetheless forged a connection with his audience in the process, and by doing so seemed to find some relief from the troubling effects of the experience and from telling about it. It is nonetheless important for the purposes of this study to take notice of the particular statements and contexts in which the FS ‘you’ was utilized by each of the interviewees, in order to detect where their particular difficulties with the experience may lie. Fourth, it is hypothesized that the ‘I’ pronoun is used predominantly in relation to actions, feelings, or experiences that the encoder is willing and able to claim as his own, out of a degree of personal acceptance and/or a sense that the surrounding society would approve (particularly with regard to his behavior). This can be viewed as a technique aimed at calling attention to the ‘acceptable’ parts of the described experience, and sending the message to the audience that, “This is what I did right.” This is most commonly collocated with active and agentic behavior at the immediate scene of the terror attack, which positions the driver as masculine, powerful, heroic, and unfailingly Israeli (in his perception of the definitions of these terms). While he was in shock or passively experiencing the event, the ‘I’ is most often silenced, and transferred to either the MS or FS ‘you.’ But when he was acting, helping, or saving, the ‘I’ is recruited to express this unambiguously and unabashedly. Finally, extending these hypotheses about the interpretive relationship between the interviewees’ discourse, their inner psychological world, and their perceptions of the surrounding societal discourse, the following three macro-level (across-participant) hypotheses are proposed: 1) the driver(s) who perceive(d) themselves or were perceived as having behaved most actively and ‘heroically’ before, during, and/or after the terror attack use ‘I’ at a higher rate than the other xxxii interviewees; 2) the driver(s) who are/were most affected (psychologically, emotionally, and/or physically) by the terror attack use the FS ‘you’ at a higher rate than the other interviewees; and 3) the driver(s) who are/were most ambivalent about the ‘acceptability’ of their behavior before, during, and/or after the terror attack use the MS ‘you’ at a higher rate than the other interviewees. Thus, the first major goal of this study is to analyze the drivers’ discourse in their baseline interviews only, investigating the relationship between their patterned communicative strategies, on the one hand, and their beliefs and attributions about the ‘acceptability’ of their behavior and feelings surrounding the stressful event they experienced, on the other. Building upon the fundamental psycholinguistic findings and the extrapolated psychosociolinguistic micro- and macro-level hypotheses delineated here, the second principal objective of this study is to explore the drivers’ longitudinal processes of coping with, working through, and processing of the terror attack experienced, as evident in their narrations of it. This stage of analysis attempts to explore and define longitudinal development in psychological processing from a comparative perspective on ‘parallel’ narratives told at two different points in time. It is proposed that this can be accomplished by an analysis of the participants’ uses of six mechanisms of narrative selection (Spector-Mersel, 2011)—inclusion, omission, silencing, sharpening, flattening, and ‘appropriate’ meaning attribution—in both the form and content of both their baseline and follow-up interviews. This may elucidate where the individual stands at the present moment (that is, at the time of the second interview), with regard to: 1) the terror attack experience and its potential psychological traumatic effects; 2) the process of narrating, processing, and working through the experience, and where and how it stands in the life narrative (and life itself); 3) his perceptions and appraisals of his behavior before, during, and after the terror attack and related emotional attributions, such as guilt and responsibility; 4) his perceptions of his surrounding society’s perceptions and appraisals regarding coping, resilience, and heroism (both in general and in relation to his particular case); and 5) his sense of emotional and functional wellbeing vs. isolation within the experience and its effects. To this end, comparative analyses were conducted on primarily the life narratives and terror attack narratives of each participant’s baseline and follow-up interview pair, according to six central narrative guidelines. To summarize, the proposed operationalization of a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing involves a follow-up terror attack narrative that is more distanced and less proximal and personalized than the parallel narrative in the baseline interview. The follow-up narrative is less vivid, salient, and ‘alive,’ as represented by a heightened use of the remote ‘past’ tense and less use of the proximate ‘present’ tense. There is more talking about, as opposed to talking within, the terror attack experience, as represented through less emphasis and dialogue. And there is less transferring of the most difficult, upsetting, and/or ambivalence-causing facets of the experience from the ‘I’-voice to either of the ‘you’-voices. There is a sense that there is more to the individual’s life (history and story) than the terror attack, as represented by its taking xxxiii up a smaller volume within the life narrative, and by a less stark division or no partition at all between the periods of life before, during, and after the terror attack. There is more evidence of coping and processing, as represented in an increased capacity to discuss and work through issues that may not be so ‘black and white.’ In contrast, the operationalization of a trajectory toward more ‘dis-ease-ful’ narrative processing involves a follow-up terror attack narrative that is more personalized and proximate, more vivid and descriptive, and more ‘alive’ (narratively and psychologically) than the respective narrative in the baseline interview. There is more representation of this in the discourse, as the narrator uses more emphasis and dialogue to bring the story to ‘life’ for himself and his audience. This will include more transferring of any troubling or traumatic aspects of the terror attack experience away from the ‘I’-perspective and onto either the MS or FS ‘you,’ depending on the motivation for the transfer. There is a sense that the individual’s life has become condensed and reduced to little more than the terror attack, as demonstrated in a substantial increase in the volume occupied by the terror attack narrative within the life narrative. The terror attack narrative may also stand alone or (discursively) outside of the life narrative, in a reflection that it is perceived by the individual as standing outside of his ‘normal’ life history. In this sense, the more the terror attack conspicuously protrudes from the life narrative, the less likely it is that it has been ‘ease-fully’ processed, worked through, and coped with by the narrator. Finally, the individual who shows evidence of greater ‘dis-ease’ in his narrative processing will show a decreased willingness and capacity to explore the range of possible reactions to a stressful event such as this. He may instead demonstrate a stricter adherence to more rigid attitudes about ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ norms, and may find it difficult to discursively claim any of his own behavior that he perceives as not fitting these requirements. Results and discussion The fundamental contention of this study is that the manners in which these bus drivers tell their narratives (particularly those of the terror attack and/or other especially emotionally charged life event(s)) are influenced by three predominant factors: 1) their psychological stance—that is, where they stand with regard to the terror attack experience; their own appraisals of their behavior before, during, and after the attack; and their location on the continuum of narrative processing development; 2) their sociological stance—that is, how they view the expectations of them held by their surrounding society; how they perceive their society’s appraisal of their behavior before, during, and after the attack; and how they have internalized the societal discourse on coping, resilience, heroism, and ‘acceptability’; and 3) the complex and dynamic relationship between these two stances—that is, whether the individual’s psychological stance is consistent with, rejects or disapproves of, or is ambivalent about his sociological stance, at any given time or position. The results of the analyses validated all of the central hypotheses postulated above, and from this emerged two central findings. The first is that a comprehensive and integrative xxxiv psychosociolinguistic analysis of the participants’ discourse (in particular, their uses of pronouns and tenses) can reveal both their personal beliefs and attributions about their behavior vis-à-vis the terror attack they experienced, and their perceptions about the attributions their society has made about the same behavior. Specifically, the interviewees’ choices to utilize the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’) or either the second-person singular masculine (‘you’ (MS)) or feminine (‘you’ (FS)) pronouns can reveal whether they are willing and able to ‘own’ the described facet of their experience, or whether they are more motivated to universalize or depersonalize it, or to bring in or project onto another subject. Each of these discursive decisions essentially reflects their perception of whether their society ‘approves’ or ‘disapproves’ of the behavior, emotion, or belief. The first set of findings examines the discourse of three participants: one who utilized the FS ‘you’ pronoun in the most prominent manner; one who utilized the MS ‘you’ pronoun in the most prominent manner; and one who utilized the ‘I’ pronoun in the most prominent manner. As nouns and pronouns in Modern Israeli Hebrew are marked for gender, and verbs are also conjugated according to gender, person, and number, the gender choices in each interviewee’s discourse constitute a crucial aspect of the analysis. This places a focus on the dynamics between the individual, on the one hand, and his perceptions of the expectations and attributions of his surrounding society, on the other. It is claimed that coping and narrative processing exists in—and can be encouraged and/or hindered by—the relationship between these two elements. The second central finding is that a deep, multifaceted analysis of the participants’ use of discourse (particularly, but not restricted to, their uses of pronouns and tenses) over time can provide insight into their longitudinal coping and their narrative processing of the difficult event they experienced. This set of results draws from comparative analyses of both the baseline and follow-up interviews of three participants: one whose psychosocial functioning and processing of the event ‘improved’ during the period between the interviews, and two others whose functioning and processing remained relatively static during this period. These findings led to the proposition of this unique type of psychosociolinguistic discourse analysis as a new and innovative qualitative method for assessment of psychosocial functioning after a stressful or traumatic life event. Contribution The potential contributions of this research study are threefold: theoretical, methodological, and practical. First of all, it involves an in-depth exploration of significant topics heretofore underresearched and under-represented in the relevant literature: 1) the population of Israeli bus drivers, who provide a unique and complex picture of a cohort on the ‘front lines’ of a well-documented stressful time period in the nation’s (and world’s) history; 2) semiotic facets of Modern Israeli Hebrew—in particular, the use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun—that may reflect important sociopsychological phenomena, on both the individual and societal levels; and 3) longitudinal comparative narrative, or ‘retelling,’ research, which is considered a key window into understanding issues of narrative identity and narrative development over time. This research xxxv aspires to contribute to the literature in all of these areas. In addition to these more specific theoretical contributions, the present study proposes a number of more general theoretical, methodological, and practical innovations, all of which have the potential for a significant impact in the relevant fields. First, a new and much-needed interdisciplinary link between the psychological, sociological, and linguistic spheres is proffered, in an attempt to bridge the gaps and draw out the strengths in each discipline. This study focuses on the use of language (primarily, pronouns and tenses) in narratives, in an attempt to understand the larger relationship between individual discourse and societal discourse. This involves a threepronged exploration, of: 1) the individual narrator/encoder and his narratives; 2) his particular psychological world and perceptions of himself; and 3) his perceptions of his surrounding society and its expectations of him. A thorough investigation of this nature clearly requires multiple disciplines, perspectives, and approaches. Research on the use of pronouns and tenses in discourse is relatively sparse in the domain of academic psychology; the work that does exist is unfortunately unidisciplinary, with little connection to the relevant literature in other fields. This research does exist, in abundance, within the disciplines of linguistics, semiotics, and literature, but the majority of these studies similarly make little attempt (and apparently find little need) to connect their respective findings to the fields of psychology or sociology. Thus, a significant gap in the literature exists due to this lack of interdisciplinary work. This doctoral research study, with its foundation in a methodical integration of a sociopsychological conceptual and theoretical framework, a narrative paradigmatic and discourse analytical methodological framework, and theory and analytical tools from sign-oriented linguistics, aims to fill this gap and make a meaningful theoretical contribution to all three academic fields. This is a truly interdisciplinary work, with multiple research domains providing both theory and method, as well as informing the manner in which the findings are interpreted in the final stage of analysis. In addition, research on and methodology related to ‘retelling’—that is, the same interviewer requesting a ‘parallel’ narrative from the same individual in a similar context at different points of time—is equally sparse and underdeveloped. The longitudinal facet of this study presents the opportunity to delve into an array of issues and discursive phenomena revealed in the participants’ retellings, and to make connections between the form and content of their narratives, on the one hand, and their processing and meaning-making surrounding the event they experienced, on the other. This may lead to topical contributions to the literatures on coping with terror in Israel; narrative methods and representations of coping; processes of coping and meaning-making over time; and the relationship between individual and societal discourse. Second, this study offers a two-pronged methodological contribution: 1) a new type of ‘mixed methods,’ represented in the integration of sociopsychological discourse analysis and signoriented semiotic analytical tools; and 2) a new model for a longitudinal comparative analysis of xxxvi uses of mechanisms of narrative selection and a variety of other discursive and communicative strategies. The central methodological contribution lies in the expansion of Spector-Mersel’s (2011) analytical method based on mechanisms of narrative selection, for use in longitudinal comparative analyses. A focus on these selection mechanisms allows for a deep analysis of the similarities and differences in the retellings, and a perspective on the possible motivations and narrative ‘end points’ involved. Finally, and as a result of both of these theoretical and methodological innovations, perhaps the greatest potential contribution of the current study is the proposal of a qualitative tool for narrative assessment of psychosocial functioning after a stressful or potentially traumatic life event. The Narrative Method for Assessment of Psychosocial Processing of Stressful Life Events (NMAPP) involves a single, simple probe: “Tell me the story of what you experienced.” This tool may hold the capacity to ascertain a reliable picture of where the individual stands at the moment of telling, with regard to: 1) the experience and its potential traumatic effects; 2) the process of processing and narrating the experience, and where and how it stands in his/her life narrative; 3) the individual’s assessment of his/her behavior during the experience and related emotional attributions, such as guilt and responsibility; and 4) the individual’s sense of emotional and functional well-being vs. isolation within the experience and its effects. The NMAPP can potentially be used as either a single-use diagnostic tool or as a comparative assessment tool. In the former case, it could provide an acute assessment—for instance, upon an individual’s arrival at a hospital after an accident or terror attack—in order to allow care providers to gauge the level of susceptibility to post-traumatic symptoms or full-blown PTSD. In the latter case, requests for the relevant narrative before and after a given therapy, program, study, or other intervention could provide reliable evidence that longitudinal change has occurred in the processing of the narrated experience. In both cases, a request for the narrative may be therapeutic in and of itself. Indeed, it may assist the individual in organization of what may be very chaotic events and experiences, and in the process of the creation of coherence from what may be highly incoherent facets of experience—particularly in the acute post-event context. The NMAPP analysis phase involves a mixed quantitative and qualitative analysis of the narratives, with a three-pronged focus on: 1) the use of pronouns; 2) the use of tenses; and 3) the use of mechanisms of selection. The focus on pronouns explores the discursive phenomena of personalization vs. universalization, distancing vs. bringing in the interviewer/audience/society, and isolation vs. collectivization. The focus on tenses explores the proximity vs. distance and level of experience-ability of the discourse. And the focus on mechanisms of narrative selection places under examination discursive phenomena such as emphasis (and over- and under-emphasis), repetition, absences and silences, and related narrative markers of meaning and significance. Perhaps most importantly, this latter focus provides a wider view of the position and prominence of the narrative of the given emotionally charged experience within the individual’s larger life xxxvii narrative. This can then offer a perspective on the individual’s belief system and attributions about, and processing of, the experience. There are a number of significant advantages to this approach, which potentially constitute justification for choosing the NMAPP over a more quantitative and positivistic approach. First, the basis in the salutogenic perspective encourages less of a focus on symptoms and pathology, and more empathy for the individual and his/her unique set of circumstances and psychic resources. Consequently, this provides a greater sense of proximity to his/her lived experience. Second, the qualitative approach proffers a welcome alternative to the lengthy batteries of impersonal and potentially distressing questionnaires that are often the fundamental basis of any positivistic study of, or intervention in, a traumatic context. An added benefit of the non-reliance on standardized quantitative assessment measures is that, because the analysis involved is often of subconscious or unconscious selection mechanisms and choice patterns, it would be nearly impossible for the narrator to ‘hide’ symptoms or facets of his experience or process that he might actively choose not to report on a questionnaire. Third, the narrative approach offers an empathic, connecting experience for the individual, which can be therapeutic in itself and can provide a ‘jump-start’ for a therapeutic relationship. Indeed, asking an individual who has just experienced a difficult life event to simply tell his/her story is an empathic request. Contrarily, administering a pile of quantitative questionnaires, especially immediately after a potentially traumatic event, can easily be experienced as quite the opposite. Finally, the interdisciplinary dimension of the NMAPP-based approach can reveal a highly comprehensive picture of psychological processing, coping, and resilience, on both the individual and societal levels. The combination of perspectives culled from the content worlds of clinical and social psychology and linguistics offers this wider view of such phenomena. Ultimately, it may be possible to gain a clear picture of an individual’s resilience and processes of meaning-making and coping simply by asking him/her—before and after a given therapy or intervention, or at two given points in time—to tell his/her story of what he/she experienced. This could contribute significantly to the field of psychological assessment, to research on coping and resilience, and to the basic goal of understanding the human lived experience. KEY WORDS: psychosociolinguistic, social psychology, discursive psychology, sign-oriented semiotic linguistics, qualitative inquiry, narrative research, discourse analysis, phenomenology, content vs. form, retelling, salutogenesis, ‘ease/dis-ease’ continuum, resilience, coping, working through, life stress, trauma, terror, heroism, Israeli bus drivers, Modern Israeli Hebrew, pronouns, tenses, communicative strategies, pronoun poetry, mechanisms of narrative selection, personal discourse, psychosocial discourse, sociocultural discourse, behavioral attributions, meaning-making, psychosocial functioning, narrative processing, narrative assessment xxxviii PART 1 Introduction, Background, Methods, and Preliminary Findings CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION TO THE RESEARCH STUDY AND THE DISSERTATION 1.1 INTRODUCTION TO THE RESEARCH STUDY This chapter offers an overview of the research study and of the dissertation itself. It begins with a brief summary of the study and its diverse surrounding contexts, and then details the research question and four central domains through which the question is explored, as well as the fundamental assertion of the study. The main findings of the research are then presented, as an organizing summary to guide the reading of the dissertation. The importance, worthiness, and contribution of the study are then discussed. The organization of the dissertation is then detailed, by means of a brief description of each chapter and its principle contents. This is followed by a discussion of several critical caveats to be kept in mind throughout the reading of the dissertation, as the audience may be varied and certain key assumptions may not be taken for granted. 1.1.1 Background and overview For much of the last three decades, the Israeli population has been exposed to significant and continuing stress, primarily from the ever-present threat of terror attacks (TAs). The First and Second Intifadas (1987-1993 and 2000-2004/2005, respectively) took place predominantly within the confines of the Green Line border of Israel (as of the eve of the Six Day War in 1967); with the hardest-hit targets highly populated civilian entities such as restaurants, cafés, malls, and buses. Because of this, Israeli bus drivers constitute one of the central populations that have existed on the ‘front lines’ of these conflicts. This interdisciplinary, phenomenological, qualitative, narrative research study endeavors to investigate the nature and manifestation of narrative processing and of the complex and dynamic relationship between personal and societal discourse, on both a shortterm and longitudinal basis. The study seeks to do this through an examination of the discourse and communicative strategies of male Jewish Israeli bus drivers who experienced TAs while on duty. In-depth, semi-structured, qualitative interviews were conducted with eight drivers in Jerusalem and Be’er Sheva. Five of the participants were then re-interviewed three years later, following the same interview technique and structure. In addition to garnering life stories and narratives on the TA(s) experienced, the overarching topics of the interviews surrounded issues of identity, worldview, systems of coping and meaning-making, sociocultural norms, and overall belief and attribution structures. A salutogenic orientation provides the conceptual framework, and a psychosocial theoretical and methodological perspective drives the exploration of both the form and content of the narratives told by the drivers. Sign-oriented semiotic linguistic theory and discursive analytical techniques associated with the Columbia School approach are utilized to understand the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages encoded in the discourse, reflecting the personal and societal factors that may impact these individuals’ processes of meaning-making and narrative 1 processing. In particular, a theoretical background in sign-oriented linguistics and a semiotic approach to Modern Israeli Hebrew (MIH) are relied upon to investigate the non-random distribution of pronouns and tenses in these narratives, in relation to the reflected degree of personalization vs. generalization and proximity vs. distance with regard to the speaker/encoder. At the heart of this study lies the central assertion that there is a relationship between the manner in which one tells and processes his personal narrative, on the one hand, and the nature and influence of the sociocultural discourse surrounding him, on the other. Analysis of the transcribed interview texts involves an exploration of the personal, familial, communal, societal, and cultural forces that are at a constant interplay in the physical and psychological lives of these individuals. The goal of this study is to offer a multidimensional perspective on narrative processing in the specific context of Israeli bus drivers. Furthermore, the underlying presumption is that the findings of this study can contribute to broader postulations about the manners in which members of Israeli society at large may negotiate, cope with, and process their varied experiences within a context of ongoing, widespread life stress. It is proposed here that the communicative strategies shown in the form and content of these individuals’ discourse can reveal their feelings, attitudes, and attributions with regard to their perceptions of the prevailing sociocultural discourse surrounding them. In the case of male Israeli bus drivers, this discourse seems to facilitate and encourage a collective perception of an obligation to act and cope resiliently and ‘heroically’ in the face of threat, and to discourage ‘less acceptable’ reactions. 1.1.2 The research question and central assertion The principal research question of this study is essentially: “How are the narratives of male Jewish Israeli bus drivers who experienced terror attacks both reflective of and shaped by their perceptions of their own coping, resilience, and heroism, and by their perceptions of the surrounding sociocultural discourse on these topics?” In its exploration of this question, the study involves four central domains. The first is an integrative quantitative and qualitative discourse and narrative analysis of each participant’s baseline interview (BLI) data, from which emerged a focus on each individual’s differential uses of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages. The second is an integrative quantitative and qualitative discourse and narrative analysis of each participant’s follow-up interview (FUI) data, which allowed for a focus on each individual’s longitudinal trends in the use of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages. The third involves comparative analyses among participants, from which emerged a focus on the similarities and disparities across the drivers with regard to their uses of pronouns, tenses, other communicative strategies, and discursive meanings and messages, as well as their experiences, attitudes and beliefs, and the 2 influence of the prevailing societal discourse on their personal discourse. Finally, the fourth focus involves comparative analyses within participants, from which emerged an understanding of the longitudinal processes involved, and a capacity to recognize ‘improvement’ vs. ‘deterioration’ vs. ‘static-ness’ in narrative processing. The central assertion of this study is that the manners in which these bus drivers tell their narratives (particularly those of the TA and/or other especially emotionally charged life event(s)) are influenced by three predominant factors: 1) their psychological stance—that is, where they stand with regard to the TA experience; their own appraisals of their behavior before, during, and after the attack; and their location on the continuum of narrative processing development; 2) their sociological stance—that is, how they view the expectations of them held by their surrounding society; how they perceive their society’s appraisal of their behavior before, during, and after the attack; and how they have internalized the societal discourse on issues related to coping, resilience, heroism, and ‘acceptability’; and 3) the complex, dynamic relationship between these two stances—that is, whether the individual’s psychological stance is consistent with, rejects or disapproves of, or is ambivalent about his sociological stance, at any given time or position. 1.1.3 The central findings From this research study emerged two central findings. The first is that a comprehensive and integrative psychosociolinguistic analysis of the participants’ discourse (in particular, their uses of pronouns and tenses) can reveal both their personal beliefs and attributions about their behavior vis-à-vis the TA they experienced, and their perceptions about the attributions their society has made about the same behavior. Specifically, the interviewees’ choices to utilize the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’) or either the second-person singular masculine (‘you’ (MS)) or feminine (‘you’ (FS)) pronouns can reveal whether they are willing and able to ‘own’ the described facet of their experience, or whether they are more motivated to universalize or depersonalize it, or to bring in or project onto another subject. Each of these discursive decisions essentially reflects their perception of whether their society ‘approves’ or ‘disapproves’ of the behavior, emotion, or belief. The first set of findings in this dissertation examines the discourse of three participants: one who utilized the FS ‘you’ pronoun in the most prominent manner; one who utilized the MS ‘you’ pronoun in the most prominent manner; and one who utilized the ‘I’ pronoun in the most prominent manner. As nouns and pronouns in MIH are marked for gender, and verbs are also conjugated according to gender, person, and number, the gender choices in each interviewee’s discourse constitute a crucial aspect of the analysis. This places a focus on the dynamics between the individual, on the one hand, and his perceptions of the expectations and attributions of his surrounding society, on the other. It is suggested that coping and narrative processing exists in— 3 and can be encouraged and/or hindered by—the relationship between these two elements. The second central finding is that a deep, multifaceted analysis of the participants’ use of discourse (particularly, but not restricted to, their uses of pronouns and tenses) over time can provide insight into their longitudinal coping and their narrative processing of the event they experienced. This section of results draws from comparative analyses of both the BLIs and FUIs of three participants: one whose psychosocial functioning and processing of the event ‘improved’ during the period between the interviews, and two others whose functioning and processing remained relatively static during this period. These findings lead to the proposition of this unique type of psychosociolinguistic discourse analysis as a new and innovative qualitative method for assessment of psychosocial functioning after a stressful or potentially traumatic life event. 1.1.4 The importance and contributions of the study This research study is valuable, necessary, and important for several reasons. First of all, it involves an in-depth exploration of significant topics heretofore under-researched and underrepresented in the relevant literature: 1) the population of Israeli bus drivers, who provide a unique picture of a cohort on the ‘front lines’ of a well-documented stressful time period in the nation’s (and world’s) history; 2) semiotic facets of MIH—in particular, the use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun—that may reflect important sociopsychological phenomena, on both the individual and societal levels; and 3) longitudinal comparative narrative, or ‘retelling,’ research, which is considered a key window into understanding issues of narrative identity and narrative development over time. This doctoral study aspires to contribute to the literature in all these areas. In addition to these more specific theoretical contributions, the present study proposes a number of more general theoretical, methodological, and practical innovations, all of which have the potential for a significant impact in the relevant fields. First, this dissertation suggests a new and much-needed interdisciplinary link between the psychological, sociological, and linguistic spheres, in an attempt to bridge the gaps and draw out the strengths in each discipline. This may lead to topical contributions to the literatures on coping with terror (in Israel and throughout the world); narrative methods and representations of coping; processes of coping and meaning-making over time; and the relationship between individual and societal discourse. Second, this study offers a two-pronged methodological contribution: 1) a new form of ‘mixed methods,’ represented in the integration of sociopsychological discourse analysis and sign-oriented semiotic analytical tools; and 2) a new model for a longitudinal comparative analysis of uses of mechanisms of narrative selection (MNSs) and other discursive and communicative strategies. Finally, this research has produced a potential practical contribution, in the form of a qualitative assessment tool: the Narrative Method for Assessment of Psychosocial Processing of Stressful Life Events (NMAPP). 4 Because the number of participants in this study is small, because the TAs experienced were of disparate types and magnitudes, and because the analyses were primarily micro-focused— essentially a collection of case studies—the results and conclusions should be taken cautiously. That said, great effort was made to relate the specific findings to the more general context, and it is hoped that this research will shed light on the larger phenomena involved—from linguistic, psychological, and sociological perspectives. Ultimately, perhaps the greatest contribution of the study lies in its attempt to integrate all of these perspectives, facilitating a comprehensive understanding of both the specific interviewees and the larger context around them. 1.2 INTRODUCTION TO THE DISSERTATION 1.2.1 The organization of the dissertation This dissertation is divided into four parts. Part 1 (Chapters 1-4) encompasses the introduction and background, literature review, methods, and preliminary results and fundamental hypotheses of the research study. Part 2 (Chapters 5-8) consists of analyses of the baseline data of three participants and an across-participant comparative conclusion. Part 3 (Chapters 9-10) presents within-participant longitudinal comparative analyses of three participants. Finally, Part 4 (Chapter 11) encompasses the discussion and conclusion. This first chapter provides an introduction to the research study and to this dissertation, and delineates several important caveats and logistical information to be kept in mind throughout the reading of this work. Chapter 2 offers a comprehensive review of the relevant theoretical literature and conceptual frameworks upon which the study relies, and presents a discussion of the microand macro-level contexts—psychological, sociological, cultural, and linguistic—within which the participants are located and findings interpreted. Chapter 3 presents the data collection and analytical methods of the study, including a description of the participant population and microcontext and a brief introduction to my reflexive position as the researcher. The interdisciplinary methodological framework of the study is described in detail, and theoretical overviews of the three primary content worlds—social and discursive psychology, sign-oriented semiotic linguistics, and qualitative inquiry and the interpretive narrative paradigm—are proffered. Methodological, logistical, and ethical considerations specific to this study are then discussed, and the research is justified with regard to its quality, validity, and trustworthiness. Chapter 4 concludes Part 1, with a delineation of the fundamental psycholinguistic preliminary findings and the predominant psychosociolinguistic interpretive hypotheses driving the research. Part 2 presents three separate baseline case studies. Each chapter begins by introducing the specific participant and giving a summary of his demographic background and of the TA he experienced, as well as a discussion of my initial impressions, the interview contexts, and our 5 interactions. Then, there is a detailed presentation of the pronominal discourse of the interviewee’s BLI, with particular focus on the pronoun used more predominantly. This is followed by a quantitative analysis of the distribution of pronouns and tenses in the central analyzed narrative(s) of the BLI, and then by a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of the same narrative(s). Each chapter concludes with a discussion of the central findings and main messages illustrated in the analysis. Part 2 focuses on each of these three participants as an exemplar of predominant usage of a different pronoun. Chapter 5 presents Yossi, who used the second-person FS pronoun the most. Chapter 6 presents Dani, who used the second-person MS pronoun the most. Chapter 7 presents Motti, who used the first-person singular pronoun the most. Finally, Chapter 8 offers a comparative across-participant analysis of all three, from a psychosociolinguistic macro-level perspective. Comparative focus is placed on their pronoun and tense use, meanings, and messages, as well as their relevant personal experiences that may place these discursive findings into greater context. Finally, connections are demonstrated between the study hypotheses, findings, and the interviewees’ own words, particularly related to the larger topics of individual and collective coping and societal pressure and expectations. Part 3 presents three separate longitudinal comparative psychosociolinguistic case studies, but is arranged slightly differently than Part 2. Chapter 9 returns to the case of Yossi, this time analyzing his material from a longitudinal perspective, while Chapter 10 presents two similar cases together. Here, Yossi is presented as a demonstration of ‘improvement’ in narrative processing, while Eli and Shimi provide an illustration of unchanged, or static, narrative processing. As in Part 2, both of these chapters begin with a holistic introduction and short summary of the given participant’s demographic background and of the TA he experienced, as well as a brief discussion of my impressions and the interview and interactional contexts. Chapter 9 builds on Chapter 5, providing a quantitative analysis of the distribution of pronouns, tenses, and topics in the central analyzed narratives of Yossi’s FUI. This is followed by a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of the central TAN, and then a deep comparative analysis of Yossi’s uses of MNSs throughout both of his interviews. Chapter 10 provides a condensed version of this analysis in the cases of Eli and Shimi, presenting a comparative discussion of their life narratives (LNs) and central TA-related narratives. Both quantitative and qualitative comparative analyses are shown for the baseline and follow-up data of both participants, in addition to brief analyses of their predominant uses of MNSs across both interviews. Conclusions are presented for each participant with regard to the discourse, meanings, and main messages presented in their interviews. Part 4 encompasses the final chapter of the dissertation. Chapter 11 endeavors to draw the study findings into a dialogue with the relevant theoretical and conceptual foundations, with an interpretive focus on the relationship between language, identity, narrative, culture, and media. The 6 contributions and implications of the study—most importantly, the proposal of the NMAPP model and tool—are also presented, and the limitations and directions for future research are delineated. 1.2.2 Several important caveats As may already be apparent, this research study is interdisciplinary in nature, attempting to integrate three substantial content worlds that do not generally enter into extended dialogue with each other. Even within each discipline, there are certain topics, ideologies, and methodologies that are considered mainstream, while others have been relegated to the periphery. The possibility for conflict or contradiction between some or all parts of this equation, therefore, is likely and must be acknowledged. To that end, it is critical to locate myself and this dissertation within this complex interdisciplinary forum, stating clearly which specific theoretical, methodological, and academically ideological decisions have been made and why. The first point of divergence regards the ‘voice’ of this dissertation. As evident herein, the ‘I’—my voice, as the primary researcher, interviewer, analyst, interpreter, writer, and experiencer of this study—is fully present, not silenced, and even dominant at times. The discourse of reflexivity is a primary language of, and permeates, this work. Its essential consideration of my role, perspectives and biases, and influence within and upon the methods, analysis, and writing of the study drives frequent discussions of the context surrounding the co-construction of the interviews conducted, references to my own background and what this may have contributed to the study, and reflections on the potential problematic nature of my non-native status. The positivistic-minded schools of psychology, sociology, and linguistics might likely find this type of discourse and writing, with its highly personal and subjective tone, unconventional, to say the least, and perhaps even ‘unscientific’ or ‘un-academic.’ They tend to favor, instead, a more ‘objective,’ distanced language that aims to underplay the role and influence of the researcher on the research. The ideology of narrative research is founded upon the centrality of reflexivity and the use and consideration of the ‘I’-voice as the co-constructor of the research context, however, and most qualitative researchers would find anything less entirely unacceptable and untrustworthy. It is this latter academic ‘home’ in which this study is most firmly situated, and which, therefore, mediated the conflict between these two fundamentally opposed discourses. It should be stated here, then, that my stance as a narrative researcher, and the stance of this work as a narrative inquiry, is based upon the contention that there is no objective ‘truth.’ Indeed, every part of this work is subjective, interpretive, and bound to a particular and situated context. In this sense, no justice would be done to this fundamental assumption, and no academic gains made, by referring to “the researcher” as an objective, generic, and generalizable being. The second point of divergence involves the decision to situate this research within a 7 salutogenic conceptual framework. Many positivistic psychologists might argue that because the population under study here experienced a potentially traumatic event (or, in their view, a ‘traumatic event’), their discourse should be viewed only from within a conceptual, theoretical, and analytical perspective based on trauma, post-traumatic effects, and trauma discourse. In this view, not invoking the ‘trauma prism’ every time I discuss the participants’ various uses of discourse and attributions about their experiences could be seen as ‘missing’ a large piece of the analytical puzzle. After hearing the stated opinions of the interviewees themselves, however, that the TAs they experienced were undoubtedly difficult—but not necessarily traumatic—events for them, I felt it necessary to widen my conceptual and analytical perspective. It was at this point in the research process that the salutogenic approach, which actually views the pathogenic orientation as ‘missing’ an important part of the story, became central. I continue to believe that this was the right choice for this particular study, although I do not deny that a trauma-focused view can be necessary in many contexts. Indeed, even in this dissertation, there are certain points at which a discussion of trauma and its related discursive phenomena was necessary and valuable. A pathogenic research ‘worldview,’ however, was not adopted for the conceptual framework of the study. The third decision, or what could be seen as a point of convergence, involves the overarching ideology around conducting this study as a multidisciplinary piece of research, bringing in a variety of perspectives, approaches, and bodies of literature. In this sense, for instance, particular aspects of the relevant literature within both the areas of sociopsychological discourse analysis and linguistic discourse analysis are relied upon, and valuable contributions from both perspectives are taken into account. In addition, the qualitative narrative paradigm provides both ideology and methodology that seemed to be lacking in some of the other paradigms. It is in this multidisciplinary manner that each approach can build on and enhance each other, with one providing a view or tools that may be missing from the other, and vice versa. Indeed, one of the basic goals of this study is to identify the points of convergence between various fields and attempt to allow them to complement each other in order to arrive at a more comprehensive perspective. A final, and related, decision that had to be made for this dissertation dealt with terminology and accessibility issues. Because this work is interdisciplinary and may attract and be relevant to a large and varied audience, I found myself constantly in a dilemma between making the language of the study comprehensible to individuals outside of one or more of the fields under discussion, as opposed to writing the dissertation in the most succinct manner possible. For instance, many linguistic terms and notations had to be explicitly explicated, and a detailed discussion of a variety of relevant semiotic facets of the MIH language was required as basic background for a nonlinguistic (and non-Hebrew-speaking) audience. The choice was made to refer to the various pronouns in a more lay-friendly manner, and transcription rules were modified and simplified from 8 the linguistic conventions, all to facilitate greater understanding across the academic fields. However, interviewee quotations were still kept predominantly faithful to the actual utterances (rather than, for instance, taking out ‘unnecessary’ commas, elongated syllables, pauses, or extralinguistic clues, as many transcribers often do) because of their potential analytical significance. Similarly, terms such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are commonplace and widely understood in the psychological field, but needed to be defined for a linguistic audience. And many fundamental concepts in the qualitative narrative domain, such as reflection, narrative identity, and trustworthiness, required long introductory discussions in order to ground any potential readership in the ideological building blocks of this study. All of this has the consequence, of course, of a significantly longer (and, at times, more arduous and potentially repetitive) dissertation. This was a difficult decision to make, but, I believe, a necessary one, as a primary goal of an interdisciplinary piece of work is to bring a variety of fields (and audiences) together and into dialogue with one another. It is, in many ways, only by making this study fully accessible to all, that it may have the potential to make an impact on any. It is my hope that the length of this dissertation will not make it prohibitive to read by any and all. A final note regards the fact that six articles were published over the course of the conducting and writing up of this research study and dissertation—four (Perez & Sagy, 2011; Perez & Tobin, 2009; Perez, Tobin, & Sagy, 2010; Tobin & Perez, 2009) directly related to the research and findings discussed here and two (Perez & Tobin, 2010a, 2014) that utilized a form of integrative psychosociolinguistic analysis similar to that used and proposed in this study, but on other qualitative or narrative material. Several small parts or findings of this dissertation have thus been published already, some in more or less the same form and some having undergone relatively major revisions in the ensuing years. Because the writing of the articles and dissertation was both ongoing and concurrent, I rarely cited myself in either source. In addition, much of the same material and findings were discussed in various presentations given at conferences in both Israel and abroad over the same time period (Golub, 2007; Perez, 2010a, 2010b, 2012; Perez & SpectorMersel, 2012a, 2012b; Perez & Tobin, 2010b, 2012), and so should be acknowledged as part of the research process. Much of this process was, in fact, dynamic and reciprocal, as the act of thinking about and writing up my findings for an article or conference presentation, and then receiving feedback from external readers and peers, had a significant influence on my analyses and on the versions of the dissertation chapters I was writing up at the time. Therefore, citing one or the other source seems impossible, as all were constantly in dialogue with each other. 9 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW: THE CONCEPTUAL, THEORETICAL, AND CONTEXTUAL FRAMEWORKS OF THE RESEARCH STUDY 2.1 INTRODUCTION This chapter presents the conceptual, theoretical, and psychosociolinguistic frameworks and contexts of this study, from within a review of the relevant literature in a wide variety of fields. It begins with a conceptual review of the salutogenic paradigm and the ‘ease/dis-ease’ continuum, as well as a brief discussion of the field of stress and coping and of the dominant terms therein, such as resilience, hardiness, and ‘working through.’ The next section presents the psycholinguistic conceptual framework, through a review of the literature on narrating trauma and the properties and dynamics of traumatic memory, the processes and nature of retelling and narrative processing over time, and the relationship between the use of pronouns and the health of the narrator. The subsequent section offers a discussion of the macro-level psychosocial context of the research, focusing on Israeli society at large and its most pressing threats—terror, trauma, and war—with a view into the daily phenomenological experience of shared, communal, chronic stress. Particular focus is placed here, too, on the salutogenic perspective, and the unique strain of ‘Israeli resilience’ is explored. A discussion of the primary characteristics of and hegemonic myths in Israeli culture is also provided, viewed through the emphasis on masculinity, strength, and heroism; the ‘culture of power’ and centrality of the military; and peer pressure to cope and act ‘acceptably.’ The chapter concludes with a glimpse into the micro-level psychosocial context within which this research takes place. The history, company culture, and demographics of Egged Bus Company are discussed, in addition to the psychosocial discourse and experiences surrounding the drivers. Particular attention is paid to what is viewed through the perspective of this study as the fundamental dilemma of being an Egged driver, and where and how these bus drivers exist in their day-to-day reality within their jobs and within the security situation in Israel. 2.2 THE PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY This section presents a discussion of the overarching psychological conceptual and theoretical framework of this study, organized around the salutogenic orientation. The primary message here is that, although this research deals with individuals who experienced an undoubtedly stressful and potentially traumatic life event, there is no presumption that they were traumatized by it, nor that their recounting of the experience should fall under the category of a ‘trauma narrative.’ Instead, this study’s salutogenic perspective emphasizes the manners in which these drivers are coping well, behaving resiliently, and drawing upon their existing psychic and emotional resources to work through these difficult experiences in the most adaptive manner of which they are capable. 2.2.1 Salutogenesis and the ‘ease/dis-ease’ continuum: A paradigmatic shift The field of psychological research on coping with stress and trauma has traditionally been based on a pathogenic view, which focuses on what is wrong with a given individual rather than on 10 what is working well, even in the face of other problems or psychopathologies. In the 1970s, Aaron Antonovsky proposed an innovative approach and dramatic shift in thinking about stress and its consequences, which he labeled salutogenesis (Antonovsky, 1979, 1984, 1987; Sagy & Antonovsky, 1996, 2000). This perspective relies on the assumption that humans live in an environment that is more often than not filled with stress and chaos, and are prone to experience a great deal of challenge in the normal course of their lives. As such, under this conceptualization, health is viewed as not a ‘natural’ or automatic state of being, but rather, one that must constantly be renewed and reestablished in the face of threats that come in the form of stress or disruption. Indeed, as Antonovsky (1987) asserted, a primary source of the salutogenic orientation is “the fundamental assumption of heterostasis, disorder, and pressure toward increasing entropy as the prototypical characteristic of the living organism” (p. 2). Essentially, this is predicated on the assumption that disease (and trauma and stress) is not at all an unusual occurrence; as opposed to the pathogenic orientation, which assumes that the living organism is generally in a state of homeostasis that only occasionally becomes disregulated. Antonovsky (1979, 1987) proposed that the factors that promote and enable healthy functioning and coping with this stress be focused upon more intently—certainly no less so than the causes of pathology and dysfunction. Indeed, he suggested that clinicians and theoreticians conceive of their patients and clients not (just) as being currently sick, but as people who could be, and naturally strive to be, healthy. An important corollary of this perception holds that everyone is both potentially vulnerable to, as well as possibly protected from, stress and trauma-related effects (Sagy & Dotan, 2001). Thus, it must be asked not only what causes illness, but what contributes to health. The salutogenic orientation, which seeks to understand the origins of health, thus asks, “why are people located toward the positive end of the health ease/dis-ease continuum, or why do they move toward this end, whatever their location at any given time?” (Antonovsky, 1987, p. xii). Out of a rejection of the traditional conceptualization of a fundamental dichotomy between ‘healthy’ and ‘sick,’ Antonovsky (1979, 1987) proposed this multidimensional health ease/dis-ease continuum instead, noting that it can be fluid and dynamic. The salutogenic orientation thus focuses on exploring the location of a given individual, at any given time, on this continuum, rather than a rigid classification of illness or health. Perhaps the greatest advantage of this model, as expressed by Antonovsky (1987), is that “It keeps us from falling into the trap of focusing solely on the etiology of a given disease rather than always searching for the total story of a human being, including his or her sickness” (p. 12). This leads to a greater focus on the coping resources at the individual’s disposal, and his or her ability to deal with and work through a stressor, rather than just the stressor itself. Antonovsky (1987) also recommended placing emphasis on an exploration of the factors or resources that could 11 lead to the individual staying in the same place on the continuum or moving in an ‘ease-ful’ direction. Indeed, as Antonovsky (1987) suggested, any life stressor inevitably leads to a state of tension or disruption, and “whether the outcome will be pathological, neutral, or salutary depends on the adequacy of tension management” (p. xii). It is in this domain that the present research lies—exploring, essentially, the participants’ discursive methods of ‘tension management’; that is, how the participants cope with the stress of the terror attack (TA) they experienced, through narrating their stories. A corollary of this examines whether their narrations and particular uses of communicative strategies may actually assist them in their coping and meaning-making processes. The conceptualization of a stressful life event as potentially but not necessarily problematic is a particularly useful building block for the present study. Antonovsky (1987) noted that “…the pathogenic orientation invariably sees stressors as pathogenic, as risk factors, which at best can be reduced, inoculated against, or buffered,” (p. 7), while the salutogenic orientation makes no such assumptions; rather, taking a larger, holistic view of the individual’s circumstances and perceptions about the stressor before crystallizing a view. Granted, as Antonovsky (1987) contended, “some stressors—for example, the ax descending on one’s head—are very predictably destructive of health, irrespective of one’s coping resources. But the assumption that ‘stressors are inherently bad’ is tenuous” (p. 7). A TA could conceivably be considered to be more analogous to this “ax”—a major stressor that could have lasting implications for the life and health of the experiencer. However, as the majority of the participants of this study demonstrate, a TA may not always cause indelible trauma. And even when it does, the natural drive toward health ‘ease,’ together with the use of the array of coping mechanisms and adaptive tools at one’s disposal, will often lead to a more resilient response than the pathogenic orientation might predict. Indeed, Antonovsky (1987) asserted, a salutogenic perspective is based on the assumption that there is generally, and more often than not, “good theoretical reason to predict positive health consequences” (p. 8). Many life stressors, therefore, may actually have salutary effects in the long run. Although Antonovsky’s (1979, 1987) original conceptualization of the salutogenic orientation was aimed primarily at the medical field, much can be gleaned and adapted to suit a psychosocial study such as this. For the purposes herein, the ‘ease/dis-ease’ continuum has been adopted as a model for health in narrative processing. Thus, rather than discussing a participant as having ‘improved’ during the period between his interviews, he will be referred to as located closer to the ‘ease-ful’ end of the narrative processing continuum. Ultimately, Antonovsky (1987) suggested that the salutogenic perspective should not eclipse, or cause us to abandon, the pathogenic orientation; rather, both outlooks should be viewed as complementary to each other. In the current study, this may mean that if an individual has clearly been suffering from post-traumatic symptoms as a result of the TA he experienced, this 12 should be acknowledged and taken into account with regard to his life, narratives, and discourse. And yet, trauma should not be assumed a priori. As Denham (2008) asserted: “It is important to recognize that traumatic events do not always result in psychiatric distress; individuals, as well as societies, differ in the manner in which they experience, process and remember events. Thus, distress resulting from a trauma experience is not due to the traumatic event per se, but the response attributed to, or meaning derived from the trauma experience (Erikson, 1995; Garland, 1998). Accordingly, it is important to differentiate between an event that may potentially cause a trauma response and an individual’s actual response to trauma. It is impossible to predict and irresponsible to assume that a particular event, no matter how (in)significant, will affect two individuals or cultural groups in the same way” (p. 395). Indeed, a focus on the positive factors in the participants’ lives and their various potential coping resources allows a larger picture to emerge and a deeper analysis of the factors involved. In contrast, simply assuming that every narrative they tell about the TA they experienced is ‘a trauma narrative’ can lead to a narrow focus and a tendency toward potentially limiting classification, which can cause a loss of perspective on the other ‘non-traumatic’ facets or characteristics present in the narrative. In Antonovsky’s (1987) view, perhaps the most important implication of salutogenically-oriented thinking is that it “not only opens the way for, but compels us to devote our energies to, the formulation and advance of a theory of coping” (p. 13). In many ways, the present study places a magnifying glass on the interviewees’ manners and manifestations of discursive coping, by means of viewing their experiences and narratives through a salutogenic lens. 2.2.2 Stress, coping, resilience, and working through The emergence of salutogenesis and other similar models created a paradigmatic shift, representing an entirely new outlook and approach to research on stress and coping. Wholly new and different questions came to the forefront as a result of this focus on how individuals cope with stressors, manage well in the face of adversity, and achieve health in spite of constant challenges— which led to the emergence of a variety of new constructs, such as hardiness, resilience, and posttraumatic growth (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 1998; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). Perhaps the most valuable finding that emerged was the salutogenic possibility of remaining ‘unscathed,’ or even simply ‘growing out’ of the effects of a prior stressor (Almedom, 2005). Parallel to the salutogenic paradigm appeared the concept of psychological resilience, the definition and operationalization of which have undergone a number of shifts since the 1970s. Resilience was initially conceptualized as a static, individualized, and built-in characteristic that one either does or does not have, with frequent references in early definitions to “invulnerability,” “invincibility,” and “stress resistance” (Anthony, 1987; Bolig & Weddle, 1988; Maluccio, 2002; Masten, 2001). Some of the more general perspectives referred to resilience as simply “the ability to recover from trauma,” or, even more vague, “overcoming the odds,” citing positive outcomes and successful adaptation (Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990; Murphy & Moriarty, 1976; Werner & Smith, 1982, 1992). In these early inquiries, resilience was conceptualized as trait-based or related 13 to specific coping styles, and synonymous with “bouncing back” and “optimal functioning” (Higgins, 1994). A central tenet of these definitions was that the “resilient individual” must not have any signs of post-traumatic stress, and if there were symptoms in the past, there must have been a full (and preferably swift) recovery. Within this focus on the individual, contextual and ecosystemic contributing factors were wholly unexamined (Waller, 2001). Later conceptions of resilience related to particular genetic and/or personality protective factors that defended an individual from succumbing to the effects of stressful life events, and some researchers discussed environmental protective factors that could enhance personal resilience. Rutter (1987) conceptualized vulnerability factors (which exacerbate an individual’s non-adaptive reaction to negative situations) and protective factors (which mitigate the harmful effects of a stressor) as being on opposite ends of a continuum, proposing an interactional and transactional view, with these mechanisms perceived as key turning points rather than attributes. In light of this, operational definitions of resilience began to appear that emphasized specific protective factors that seemed to allow an individual to be more likely to show resilience in the face of or after experiencing a stressful life event. In this sense, resilience was seen as the absence of serious negative effects of difficult life circumstances, and it was suggested that self-esteem and selfefficacy are the most important traits in resilient people (Turner, Norman, & Zunz, 1995). One of the more enduring early conceptualizations in this field was hardiness (Kobasa, 1979; Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982; Kobasa & Puccetti, 1983), which suggested that those with so-called “hardy personalities” were more predisposed to cope more successfully with stress and to emerge with less serious, if any, psychopathological aftereffects. In particular, Kobasa (1979) discussed three core concepts within this personality type: 1) a commitment to the self and to one’s surrounding atmosphere, including one’s overall sense of worth, purpose, and meaningfulness within life; 2) a perception of change as a challenge rather than hardship or threat; and 3) a belief that events can be controlled (that is, an internal locus of control). Alongside of this, and as an outgrowth of his salutogenic approach, Antonovsky (1979) coined the term, sense of coherence, defined as “a global orientation that expresses the extent to which one has a pervasive, enduring, though dynamic, feeling of confidence that one’s internal and external environments are predictable and that there is a high probability that things will work out as well as can reasonably be expected” (p. 132). Out of this came a well-known and widely-utilized theoretical and practical tool conceptualized by Antonovsky (1979, 1993, 1994), which has been used and evaluated in a wide variety of contexts, populations, and dimensions (Antonovsky & Sagy, 1986; Cohen & Kanter, 2004; Eriksson & Lindström, 2005, 2006; Flannery, Perry, Penk, & Flannery, 1994; Frenz, Carey, & Jorgensen, 1993; Holmberg, Thelin, & Stiernström, 2004; Johnson, 2004; Lustig, Rosenthal, Strauser, & Haynes, 2000; McSherry & Holm, 1994; Sagy & Antonovsky, 1992; Sagy, 14 Antonovsky, & Adler, 1990; Strümpfer & Mlonzi, 2001; Yeheskel, 1995). This perspective on individual personality factors paved the way for a further shift in the dominant definitions of resilience, from trait-centered to process-centered (Sullivan, 1993). A focus on resilient mechanisms as opposed to resilient characteristics appeared, allowing for the possibility of an individual to be resilient in one area of life and perhaps less resilient in another. This threw into question the idea of a “resilient individual” at all, relativizing the concept and permitting the view that nearly any individual can engage in resilient processes at one time or another, to some degree and in some capacity. It was suggested that, “Protection does not reside in the psychological chemistry of the moment but the ways in which people deal with life changes and in what they do about their stressful or disadvantageous circumstances” (Rutter, 1987, p. 329). Resilience thus became a property of the situation, the environment, and the coping processes that are utilized to allow the successful negotiation of such circumstances. At this point in the literature, resilience was no longer viewed as a specific, quantifiable category, but rather as a continuum—allowing an exploration of how, rather than if, an individual was resilient. This opened up the possibility for an individual to be more or less resilient within a given domain of functioning, more or less resilient in a given stressful event, and more or less resilient in one environmental or social context than in another. In this way, “Depending on the different definitions, different people can appear to be resilient. Resiliency in one arena of life does not ensure resiliency in all other domains” (Grossman, Cook, Kepkep, & Koenen, 1999, p. 14). The focus then shifted yet again, with a claim that the environment in which the individual attempts his coping tasks is also essential to the development of resilience. Thus, a transactional framework was proposed for understanding the concept of resilience (Grossman et al., 1999), with the basic assumption that it does not exist in a vacuum; rather, this interaction between an individual and his or her social and physical environment is reciprocal in its effects on both sides. In this sense, resilience was viewed as a mechanism that draws on a variety of processes and has the capacity to develop through biological, psychological, and environmental avenues (Grossman et al., 1999). Indeed, the multidimensional nature of resilience has been evidenced by its ability to utilize both personal and environmental dynamics (Rutter, 1987). Although the current literature is far from showing widespread agreement, and indeed, the frequently changing definitions and conceptualizations have led to some confusion and lack of clarity in the literature (Humphreys, 2003), there is currently a tentative consensus regarding a number of facets of the resilience concept. For one, resilience is widely viewed as dynamic and fluid (Bonanno, 2004). In this sense, the belief that “resilient people” respond to all stressors in the same manner, or even that all stressful life events can produce the same type and style of resilience, holds little justification anymore. Indeed, it is now fairly well established that, more than specific 15 personality traits or a particular lifestyle, the tendency toward resilience may lie in a set of protective factors that individuals can possess inherently or actively adopt (Bonanno, 2004). This research field is now at a place and time in which it is not seeking to define, or even to prove the existence of, the resilient individual, or the resilient context, in isolation. In fact, resilience is now generally conceptualized as the successful dyadic, interactional relationship between the individual and his or her ecosystemic context, those developmental and dynamic processes that both define and impact resilience and its effect on the individual under stress. This dynamic conceptualization allows for growth and change and a constant reshaping of the concept within its context. Indeed, as Waller (2001) notes, “There is growing recognition that resilience is multidimensional and multidetermined, and can best be understood as the product of transactions within and between multiple systemic levels over time (Walsh, 1998)” (p. 294). The present research attempts to contextualize resilience in this manner, with an implicit hypothesis that this interaction is essentially triangular, involving not only the individual and his context, but the event as well. This tridirectional relationship is the meeting point at which this study takes place. Within the context of references to coping with stress, this dissertation uses a number of terms and concepts not wholly interchangeably, but in concert and in dialogue with each other. That is, much discussion is conducted about the drivers’ coping processes and abilities, narrative and psychological processing, and resilience. A final useful concept that is also drawn upon at certain points in the analysis is that of working through, a term which was first coined by Freud (1930) to describe the long and intensive therapeutic process that unfolds between a therapist and patient, particularly within the context of repressed childhood experiences. The assertion was that this process is the conduit to bring any repressed content into the conscious domain, in order to release it from continuing to create a disturbance in functioning or cause interference with the individual’s emotions, behavior, and/or attitudes (Novey, 1962; Bar-On & Kassem, 2004). The concept of working through has also undergone a number of developmental shifts over the years, with perhaps the most general current definition prescribing a basic form of internal processing or “learning to live with the painful past better than one has up to now” (Bar-On & Kassem, 2004, p. 290; Bar-On, 1995; Lehman, Wortman, & Williams, 1987). It has been applied and appropriated in a variety of contexts and studies; most prominently, with individuals who underwent traumatic experiences and suffer from PTSD (Danieli, 1988), with Holocaust survivors (Bar-On & Kassem, 2004; Maoz & Bar-On, 2002) and vis-à-vis the intergenerational traumatic effects on their children (Bar-On, 1995; Danieli, 1988) and grandchildren (Litvak-Hirsch & BarOn, 2006), and with descendants of Nazi perpetrators (Bar-On, 1989, 1990; Bar-On & Kassem, 2004). It has proved a valuable concept particularly regarding long-term coping after profoundly traumatic events, in an attempt to understand deeply-rooted psychological wounds, loss, and 16 feelings of helplessness, and the processes involved in coming to terms with these indelible effects (Danieli, 1988; Davidson, 1980). With regard to the third generation’s process of working through familial Holocaust experiences, Bar-On (1990) proposed a typology of five stages, characterized by: 1) knowledge of what happened to their family during the Holocaust; 2) understanding of these events, by placing the newfound knowledge of the facts “within a meaningful human, historical, social, or moral frame of reference”; 3) emotional response to this knowledge and understanding, which can include anger, fear, shame, and/or pride; 4) the formation of a particular attitude about these familial experiences, based on the knowledge, understanding, and emotional response, and their implications; and 5) the behavior enacted as a result of all of these factors in dynamic relationship with each other, in relation to the past, present, and future. While much of this is specific to the particular events and experiences involved in the Holocaust and the intergenerational familial transmission of its aftereffects, some facets of these stages may be applicable to the current research study. That is, the drivers’ processes of working through their TA experiences often may involve, or begin with, gathering knowledge about what factually transpired during the attack (either because they may have been unconscious at some point, or because of added information about the terrorist uncovered only after the fact, by the police or other intelligence sources). They also may have a need to place these ‘facts’ into a social or political context, in order to make sense and meaning out of the experience. There is undoubtedly an emotional response as well as set of attitudes, beliefs, and attributions involved in the drivers’ processes of coping with and working through these experiences. Finally, the drivers develop a variety of behaviors in response to all of these stages and processes. Perhaps most relevant to the present study is their set of narrative behaviors—the discursive patterns and communicative strategies evident in their interviews, which are reflective of the influence of these working-through factors and stages. In many ways, therefore, the concept of working through has proven useful in this context, and is employed alongside and sometimes interchangeably with discussions of coping and processing. Ultimately, as Denham (2008) asserted with regard to trauma narratives, “embedded within…were numerous strategies for resilience, or a non-pathological adaptive response and ability to maintain or ‘spring back’ to a stable equilibrium after experiencing adversity (Bonanno, 2004; Conner, Davidson, & Lee, 2003; Dion Stout & Kipling, 2003; Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000)” (p. 392). It is this salutogenic focus on adaptive responses rather than pathological ones, on resilience rather than ‘falling apart,’ and on equilibrium rather than trauma, that drives the present research study and the analyses and interpretations herein. 17 2.3 THE PSYCHOLINGUISTIC CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY This section focuses on the psycholinguistic literature relevant to this research, surrounding the topics of traumatic memory and narratives, retelling and narrative processing, and the relationship between such narrations (and various discursive strategies therein) and the health of the narrator. The primary message is that, although this study does not presuppose trauma or traumatic effects in the lives of these drivers, it is necessary to review the full range of possible mechanisms at work in their processes of narrating, processing, and making meaning out of their experiences. Thus, these narratives may not be characterizable as ‘trauma narratives,’ but may nonetheless show some signs or signals of trauma, and these should be identifiable to this audience. In addition, attention is paid to the properties of ‘healthy’ narratives of traumatic experiences, and the adaptive processes involved in creating such narratives. 2.3.1 Narrating trauma and traumatic memory Although this study does not presume that the TAs experienced by the participants were necessarily traumatic, or that each participant necessarily experienced post-traumatic symptoms as a result, a TA such as those discussed here can, and should, be considered a potentially traumatic event. As such, some of the characteristics of what are referred to as ‘trauma narratives’ may be found in some of the narrative material presented herein, while others may not be relevant. This research holds as an ideological tenet that a narrative told by an individual who experienced a potentially traumatic event should not necessarily be automatically characterized as a ‘trauma narrative,’ and that a ‘narrative of trauma’ is not the same as a ‘trauma narrative.’ Nonetheless, it is necessary to present a brief discussion of the literature on processes and manners of narrating trauma, beginning with a basic review of the interplay between memory, identity, and trauma. 2.3.1.1 Memory, identity, and trauma Humans use memory to relate their present to their past, to understand the world around them, and to create an interpretation of both themselves and others that utilizes this experience and knowledge. As Hirsch (1995) claimed, “As an individual reconstructs his or her biography through memory, that biography becomes the basis for identity,” adding, “The connection between memory and identity is dialectical because memory both shapes the content of what is communicated by the socialization process and is formed by that process. Ultimately, the self does not develop in a vacuum” (p. 133). Indeed, as Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) asserted, “People infuse meaning into their lives, into the events they have experienced, and into the choices they have made, through the stories they tell about themselves” (p. 281). In essence, memory is the fundamental force behind identity formation and self-understanding. Without childhood memories, one would not know how one came to be, what one likes or dislikes, or why one thinks and feels a certain way in response to 18 a certain situation. Without memories of life events and circumstances such as marriage, the birth of a child, or the death of a parent, there can be no cohesive understanding of one’s life course. Personal identity is created out of personal memories, and both reciprocally influence each other as well as the life story itself. In this sense, the life story both reflects and expresses the identity of the narrator, as well as “shap[ing] and influenc[ing] the transformations of that identity…because it is through their stories that individuals come to know themselves or to reveal themselves to others” (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004, p. 281; Lieblich, Tuval-Mashiach, & Zilber, 1998). The nature of human memory is such that particular and increased attention is often paid to those events that are more significant for some reason—whether they are especially emotional, meaningful, exciting, or traumatic. The events that have the most impact on an individual are thus most often and likely the ones that are remembered with the most clarity and longevity. Brown and Kulik (1977) coined the term, flashbulb memory, to refer to this type of extremely vivid and essentially permanent memory, generally produced by an unexpected and/or emotionally important or arousing event. These memories are often almost photographic in nature, acting to preserve the event in the mind as a flashbulb captures a photograph. Because traumatic memories are encoded in this different fashion and context, the retrieval of traumatic memories also occurs in an atypical manner. The very act of remembering these events can be difficult, painful, and intrusive, because of the strong sensory and visual content. As Herman (1992) described this process: “Long after the danger is past, traumatized people relive the event as though it were continually recurring in the present. They cannot resume the normal course of their lives, for the trauma repeatedly interrupts. It is as if time stops at the moment of trauma. The traumatic moment becomes encoded in an abnormal form of memory, which breaks spontaneously into consciousness, both as flashbacks during waking states and as traumatic nightmares during sleep. Small, seemingly insignificant reminders can also evoke these memories, which often return with all the vividness and emotional force of the original event” (p. 37). In this sense, traumatic memories do not always need retrieval cues, or at least, related retrieval cues; rather, they can intrude into daily consciousness without warning or invitation. In addition, with regard to meaning-making and emotional management in the face of protracted stress and/or directly after a potentially traumatic event, Shalev (2004) asserted that events such as TAs can have a particularly unthinkable nature, which can make it difficult to cope with, manage, and make sense of them. Furthermore, Shalev (2004) noted, “stressful events become traumatic when they include novel and incongruous experiences, such as exposure to extreme brutality, disfigured dead bodies, people jumping out of windows, or major loss,” and result in an experience of horror for the survivor. In this sense, while not all stressful life events are experienced as traumatic, the existence of any of these components may increase the likelihood that an event may become traumatic for the experiencer. In fact, Shalev (2004) claimed that individuals who experienced a TA in Jerusalem suffered from “extreme forms of intrusive recollections” directly after the experience, and that: “These ‘flash bulb’ recollections typically consist of repeated visual images of the most horrific fragment of the event. Some survivors find it difficult to close their eyes, because of disturbing mental images of horror. 19 For some, the intrusive images consist of scenes acquired in the aftermath of a blast, that is, during rescue. … For them, the event has became [sic] traumatic because of its ‘informational load’ ” (p. 175). Some of the interviewees in this study described exactly these types of experiences, memories, and difficulties with ‘flashbacks’ and intrusive recollections. This “informational load” apparently caused them much distress, often expressed in both the content and form of their discourse. All of these characteristics of traumatic memory present problems for the development and organization of a cohesive life narrative (LN) based upon a coherent set of memories. Because, as Herman (1992) claimed, these types of memories “are not encoded like the ordinary memories of adults in a verbal, linear narrative that is assimilated into an ongoing life story” (p. 37), individuals who have experienced trauma tend to have greater difficulty integrating the traumatic memories with their other memories, and with the rest of their lives. As Harvey (1996) asserted: “Simply stated, traumatic events wreak havoc with the individual’s ability to recall and make use of the past. Trauma survivors are often plagued both by the absence of salient information about their experience and by traumatic intrusions which disable and terrify even as they elude meaningful appraisal” (p. 11). This relative absence of informational memory, coupled with the unwelcome intrusions of the more vivid, traumatic memory, often leaves the individual who has experienced trauma with powerful and upsetting memory experiences that are often not cohesively retrievable and may not lend themselves to narrative processing and expression. Thus, in an examination of narratives—of an event and/or of the life surrounding it—of individuals who have experienced a trauma, it is crucial to remember the differences between normal memory and traumatic memory. Although these disparities exist in most trauma contexts and can be manifested in different ways, the clearest illustration can be seen in the LN itself and how it is told. Following Janet (1919), Herman (1992) discussed the explicit differences between the mechanisms of normal memory and traumatic memory, and how they can be exhibited in the rehearsal of the life story: “[Normal memory,] like all psychological phenomena, is an action; essentially it is the action of telling a story… A situation has not been satisfactorily liquidated…until we have achieved, not merely an outward reaction through our movements, but also an inward reaction through the words we address to ourselves, through the organization of the recital of the event to others and to ourselves, and through the putting of this recital in its place as one of the chapters in our personal history… Strictly speaking, then, one who retains a fixed idea of a happening cannot be said to have a ‘memory’…it is only for convenience that we speak of it as a ‘traumatic memory’” (p. 37). In this sense, traumatic memories may be essentially incompatible with the concept of ‘normal’ memory, and less capable of integration in an LN filled with ‘normal’ memories likely arranged in a coherent and linear fashion. Traumatic memories, in this view, stand alone and are destined to exist outside the scope of normalcy and of the cohesively recited narrative. 2.3.1.2 Translating traumatic memory into narrative memory For individuals who have experienced trauma, therefore, the construction of cohesive narratives can be a complex, difficult, and painful process. In his research on Holocaust survivors, 20 LaCapra (1998) asserted, “Especially for victims, trauma brings about a lapse or rupture in memory that breaks continuity with the past, thereby placing identity in question to the point of shattering it” (p. 9). The Holocaust is perhaps the most extreme example of a traumatic life event, having caused a dramatic disruption and fragmentation, and leaving memories that were fundamentally opposed to anything before and after them. A potentially traumatic event such as a TA can cause equally disruptive processes in memory and narrative production. Indeed, one of the interviewees in the current study demonstrated a similar ‘before vs. after’ pattern in the structure of his LN. Along these lines, Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) asserted that, “Trauma, by its nature, breaks the continuity and smooth flow of daily life. This break is expressed in the stories trauma survivors tell about themselves and about their lives” (p. 281). Along these lines, trauma has been contended to cause a primary disruption in narrative processing in two central areas: specifically, a disruption of the narrative of the traumatic event itself, and more generally, a disruption of the entire life story of the experiencer (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004; Wigren, 1994). The impact of trauma on LNs can be reflected in the narrator’s life itself (or perhaps, the impact of the trauma on the individual’s life can reflect itself in his or her LN). Indeed, as Tuval-Mashiach and her colleagues (2004) claimed, “The healthy individual is capable of holding a coherent, meaningful, and dynamic narrative of himself. According to this perspective, a person whose story is unavailable, flawed, or partial is prone to psychological and emotional difficulties” (p. 281). Thus, there appears to be a constant, complex, reciprocal, and dynamic interrelationship between a traumatic experience, the individual who experienced it, and his or her identity, memory, life story, and psychological functioning. There may also be a healing or restorative quality to narrative in this context; as Crossley (2000) has suggested, “when disorder and incoherence prevail, as in the case of trauma, narratives are used to rebuild the individual’s shattered sense of identity and meaning” (p. 527). In many ways, then, narrative and the properties of memory may both reflect and repair the individual in traumatic circumstances. An essential part of coping with a traumatic life experience involves the process of putting it into words, processing and digesting it into some form of narrative, whether initially coherent and cohesive or not. Van der Merwe and Gobodo-Madikizela (2007) asserted that, in order to fully recover from a traumatic experience and regain a sense of control, an individual must complete a process of transforming a traumatic memory into a narrative memory. Following LaCapra (2001), Schiffrin (2003) contended, “when the past becomes accessible to recall in memory, and when language functions to provide some measure of conscious control, critical distance, and perspective, one has begun the arduous process of working over and through the trauma” (p. 539). This process of ‘translating’ a trauma into narrative can actually create order and coherence, transforming a chaotic, visceral, and nonlinear memory into a cohesive and organized verbal expression of it. 21 The act of remembering and narrating is interactive, involving “a triangulation between the experiences of the past, the set of circumstances within which these experiences occur and the way in which the individual reflects on these experiences,” including the circumstances surrounding the narrator in the given moment of the recalling/telling (Coetzee & Rau, 2009, p. 3). Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) pointed to the operation of two crucial coping mechanisms—narrative and cognitive— involved primarily in the period immediately following a trauma, which function interactively to facilitate the individual’s creation of a narrative of the experience and of his life as including the experience. Furthermore, they suggest that “the construction of stories is mediated by different factors—psychological (e.g., personality), cultural (norms and values), and situational (to whom is the story told)” (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004, p. 282). Indeed, part of the process of understanding, processing, and coping with a traumatic event involves ascribing meaning to it. This is a highly individualized and personal process, which can be dynamic and intimately tied to the individual’s psychosocial context. It may facilitate or inhibit recovery at any stage, and interacts with the process of narrating the experience as well. As Fullerton (2004) asserted: “The meaning of a traumatic experience to any one person results from the interaction of his or her past history, present context, and physiological state. The ascribed meaning will then direct individual behaviors of what to do, what to fix, and whom or what to blame” (p. 62). The meaning attached to the experience by the experiencer then has an impact on the resulting psychological consequences, including the individual’s set of beliefs, attitudes, and attributions about himself, those surrounding him, and his society. TAs and war, in particular, can have deep negative impacts on the survivor’s sense of safety, security, and faith that his world is comprehensible and just (Fullerton, 2004; Holloway & Fullerton, 1994). Indeed, following North et al. (1999), Benyakar and Knafo (2004) stated, “Disruptions caused by terrorism…constitute abrupt assaults to the individual and collective psyche” (p. 87). These disruptions, as part of a constant, daily state of fear, helplessness, defenselessness, and powerlessness (Krystal, 2004), can bring great difficulty in the meaning-making process, and in turn, in the process of translating traumatic memory into narrative memory. 2.3.1.3 Characteristics and signals of ‘trauma narratives’ When traumatic experiences are expressed in an individual’s personal narrative, they tend to appear in a particular fashion and be distinguished by specific markers (BenEzer, 1999). Indeed, it has been claimed that ‘trauma narratives’ hold their own unique characteristics, which often stand out and can be identifiable linguistically. It has been found, for instance, that, relative to written narratives about neutral topics, written narratives about traumatic events tend to contain more emotionally charged words (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986), more words that relate to emotions and 22 cognition (Cohn, Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2004; Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003; Pennebaker & Francis, 1996), and more negative emotion words (Fernandez, Paez, & Pennebaker, 2009; Freitag, Grimm, & Schmidt, 2011; Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003). Following Rullkoetter et al. (2009), Freitag et al. (2011) generalized further, stating, “Narratives of negative life events with an emotional impact to the individual were characterized by an increased number of words, a greater number of sentences and they contained more emotional words compared to events without emotional impact” (p. 43). Boals and Perez (2009) found a higher percentage of cognitive word use in oral narratives of Holocaust experiences as opposed to non-Holocaust experiences, and suggested that cognitive words “can help provide links between events and emotions that allow an individual to come to a better understanding of the experience” (p. 1). This is particularly critical in narrations of stressful or traumatic life experiences, which fundamentally involve unpredictability, chaos, and a violation of expectations (Janoff-Bulman, 1992). In fact, a number of studies have found a greater degree of cognitive words in narratives that describe a variety of stressful experiences (Boals & Klein, 2005; Cohn et al. 2004; Klein & Boals, 2010; Lyons, Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2006; Pennebaker & Lay, 2002). In addition, a relationship has been found between this increase in cognitive words and successful coping (Alvarez-Conrad, Zoellner, & Foa, 2001; Pennebaker, Mayne, & Francis, 1997), as well as with increased posttraumatic growth (Ullrich & Lutgendorf, 2002). An elevated use of cognitive words, as opposed to emotion-related words, was also found to hold a stronger association with improvement in health (Pennebaker, Mehl, & Niederhoffer, 2003). Thus, it may be possible that use of cognitive words when narrating stressful or traumatic life experiences may be both adaptive and reflective of positive adaptation. There have been a limited number of in-depth studies of trauma utilizing a life story or narrative interviewing methodology, and all focused primarily on the content and thematic properties of the narratives, rather than the form or linguistic properties (Agger, 1994; Bar-On, 1989, 1995; Dwork, 1991; Lieblich, 1994; Lomsky-Feder, 1995; Rosenthal, 1991). To fill this lacuna, BenEzer (1999) proffered a focus on the “verbal and non-verbal trauma signals” within life stories, suggesting that “traumatic experiences, even if the person has come to terms with them, are narrated in a unique way within narratives, and…these ways could be ‘charted’ and detected within the stories” (p. 30). These are: To fill this lacuna, BenEzer (1999) proffered a focus on the “verbal and non-verbal trauma signals” within life stories, suggesting that “traumatic experiences, even if the person has come to terms with them, are narrated in a unique way within narratives, and…these ways could be ‘charted’ and detected within the stories” (p. 30). These are: 1) self-report, whereby the individual states outright that a particular event was traumatic, painful, horrific, distressing, wounding, negative, or unsettling; or describes an “image of ultimate horror,” which may serve as a single symbol of a series of related traumatic experiences; 23 2) the existence in the life history of a “hidden” event, which was not mentioned in the main, uninterrupted life narrative, but which emerged during the question-and-answer portion of the interview, and which may involve “distressing emotions such as mourning, grief, shame or guilt”; 3) the occurrence of long silence(s) directly preceding or following a narration of a particular event or facet thereof, which appears to have “a particularly painful or tormenting quality” for the narrator; 4) a sudden loss of emotional control in connection with narrating an event, often expressed in rage or crying or other displays of emotion that appear out of ‘character’ with the rest of the life story; 5) emotional detachment or numbness while reporting events or facets thereof that seem to have had a horrifying quality or consequences for the narrator, often appearing as “an unvarying tone of voice and ‘frozen’ facial expression and body gestures,” frequently described in the psychological literature as “psychic numbness’; 6) repetitive reporting of a particular distressing experience in its entirety or with “an extraordinary reiteration of its minute details” over and over, as if the narrator is finding it difficult to “move on”; 7) the experience of the narrator seeming to lose him or herself in the traumatic event, appearing to “disappear from the reality of the interview” or “sink into themselves” while narrating—often expressed as extended periods of silence with clear signs that the narrator is simply “not there” in the current moment or situation; 8) the emergence of intrusive images of a traumatic event or facet thereof, which seem to appear as ‘flashes’ that intrude into, interrupt, and distract the narrator’s train of thought and narrative flow; 9) the narrator’s forceful argumentation regarding his or her conduct within an event, emphasizing the reasons behind their behavior rather than describing or recounting the behavior itself, and apparently offering a justification for their conduct in an attempt to prevent the interviewer from reaching any conclusions on his or her own; 10) the appearance of cognitive-emotional disorientation, which may be “characterized by a disappearance of the boundaries between the event which is being recounted and the situation of the interview,” often expressed as a type of loss by the narrator of a sense of exactly where he or she is (within the story and within reality)—for instance, “relating to the interviewer as a figure within the story (without the ‘as if’ quality that is sometimes used in recounting)”; 11) an inability of the narrator to tell a story at all, often getting stuck immediately at the opening point of a narrative (which may cause the interviewer to have to intervene, offering questions to help the narrator tell the story in some form); 12) changes in voice, such as tone, pitch, or “colouring”; and 13) changes in body language, such as facial expressions, posture shifts, and hand or leg movements (BenEzer, 1999, p. 34-36). Any or all of these “linguistic behaviors” may appear in a given narrative, signaling the existence of a traumatic life event in the life story. Many of them are apparent in the texts analyzed in the present study, and particular attention will be paid to #10 above, which can be manifested in BenEzer (1999) describes as “relating to the interviewer as a figure within the story (without the ‘as if’ quality that is sometimes used in recounting)” (p. 35). This research, however, relates to this as more of a psychosociolinguistic phenomenon that may reflect the narrator’s emotions, beliefs, and attributions; rather than a reflection of disorientation or psychic confusion. In addition, Lieblich et al. (1998), in their proposal of a qualitative analytical method for assessing the extent to which a narrative may be emotionally charged, offered a list of “the formal aspects of emotional or mental disturbance,” as a guide to “elements that may appear in an emotionally charged narrative” (p. 156). These elements are: 24 1) adverbials (e.g., “suddenly”) that may indicate whether an event was expected or unexpected; 2) mental verbs (e.g., “I thought,” “I understood”) that may indicate “the extent to which an experience is in consciousness and is undergoing mental processing”; 3) denotations of time and place that may indicate an attempt to either distance or bring into closer proximity a particular event or facet of an event; 4) past, present, or future forms of verbs, and transitions between them, which may indicate the narrator’s “sense of identification” with the described event; 5) shifting between first-, second-, and third-person narrative subjects, which may indicate “a split between the speaking self and the experiencing self,” as a result of difficulty surrounding the experience and memory; 6) passive and active forms of verbs, which may indicate the narrator’s perceptions and attributions surrounding agency; 7) intensifiers (e.g., “really,” “very”) or deintensifiers (e.g., “maybe”), which may indicate the magnitude of the experience vs. expressions of helplessness and inability to cope; 8) a break in the chronology or causal progression of narrative events, as signified in the narrative by regressions, digressions, leaps in time, or silences, which may indicate avoidance of difficult or sensitive subject matter; 9) repetition (i.e., of words, syllables, sentences, and/or ideas), which may indicate an emotional charge relating to the topic at hand; and 10) detailed descriptions of events, which may indicate an avoidance of or inability to describe difficult emotional content (Lieblich et al., 1998, p. 156). In general, all of these characteristics and signals should be kept in mind when analyzing a narrative of a potentially traumatic event, as well as a life story that may include such an experience. In the present study particular focus will also be placed on #5 above, alongside a claim of deeper significance and a new interpretation of the meaning therein. In many ways, these signals are important not only as a possible method of ‘detection’ of a traumatic experience, but, perhaps more significantly, because “the language that people use to describe past traumas can reveal how they think about and organize the experience” (Boals & Perez, 2009, p. 3). This research delves into the interviewees’ manners of thinking about and creating meaning out of such experiences, as signified by many of these characteristics and viewed through a psychosociolinguistic lens. 2.3.1.4 The relationship between narrating trauma and health BenEzer (1999) writes that “trauma is a very intimate experience” (p. 29), and, following Klein (1976) and Garbarino, Kostelny, and Dubrow (1992), contends that “traumatization…is related to, and imbued with, the meaning of the event for the individual” (p. 32). This individualized meaning can emerge from a complex interplay of factors such as the individual’s unique life history, personal beliefs and biases, and particular priorities and sensitivities. There is an inevitable meaning-making process involved in narrating trauma and, in many cases, the meaning held by traumatic events over the course of an individual’s life can change many times, with these transformations often reflected and revealed in the individual’s LN. In many ways, then, there is a dynamic, interactional relationship between the processes of meaning-making, narrating, 25 and coping, and all are tied to health. Indeed, this is a new and growing field of research; as Boals and Perez (2009) wrote, “Researchers have recently begun to examine language use in descriptions of traumatic experiences in hopes that analyses of language use can shed insight into successful and unsuccessful coping strategies” (p. 1). Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) have pointed to three crucial factors involved in successful coping, all of which are intimately connected with the creation of the narrative: 1) continuity and coherence, within both the narrative of the traumatic experience and the LN as a whole; 2) an active process of creation of meaning related to the trauma experience; and 3) a process of self-evaluation with regard to the trauma experience, which may include issues of control, guilt or responsibility, and active or passive conduct. Continuity and coherence in both the narrative of the trauma and the LN were found to be associated with better recovery (Omer & Alon, 1994; Wigren, 1994), and the extent of control within self-evaluation vis-à-vis the traumatic event was also found to be correlated with successful coping (Foa, Zinbarg, & Olasov Rothbaum, 1992; Steptoe, 2000). In fact, TuvalMashiach et al. (2004) found “a preliminary relationship between narrative, cognitive measures, and symptoms of Post Traumatic Stress Disorder,” such that PTSD symptoms were significantly lower “when the narrative was well built, with a coherent story, significance, and a positive selfimage,” while a less coherent narrative was correlated with increased symptoms (p. 290). Along these same lines, Pennebaker (1993), when analyzing trauma-related writings, found that the individuals who used a larger proportion of negative emotion words, as opposed to positive emotion words, actually showed a greater improvement in health overall. Indeed, as Pennebaker (1993) contended, “Independent of verbal emotion expression, the increasing use of insight, causal, and associated cognitive words over several days of writing is linked to health improvement” (p. 539). In this sense, then, it is suggested that both coherence and a significant expression of negative emotions can be an important, healthy, and therapeutic part of narrative construction for individuals writing about traumas they experienced. Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) also discussed what they refer to as “a natural process of recovery,” the spontaneous development of a coherent story accompanied by developmental and longitudinal shifts in the individual’s perceptions of his behavior, his self-image, and the significance of the traumatic experience in his life. When this process occurs ‘naturally’ and individually, the narrator experiences increased significance and logic, as well as fewer symptoms of PTSD (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004). Conversely, when this process does not occur or is only partially completed, the result is an incoherent or less coherent narrative that may hold less or ambivalent significance for the narrator. In this case, the individuals may not be able to fully integrate the traumatic experience into their overall LN and may have difficulties with meaningand sense-making (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004). This, in turn, may contribute to a far greater 26 likelihood of developing PTSD or experiencing more post-traumatic symptoms. 2.3.2 Retelling and narrative processing over time Narrative analysis holds as its primary domain of inquiry the question of how individuals tell their stories. Inquiry into how individuals retell their stories, however, is sorely underexplored and underrepresented. And yet, the investigation of how narratives may (and/or may not) change over time lies at the heart of narrative identity theory (McAdams, 1985; Singer, 2004), wherein an individual’s stories are viewed as a primary conduit for understanding and exploring his self and identity (Alasuutari, 1997; McAdams, 1993; Polkinghorne, 1991; Spector-Mersel, 2011). Bamberg (2008) referred to “the long tradition of using the narrative interview as a site for tapping into the narrator’s sense of self and identity,” labeling this as “one of the most influential contributors in the turn to narrative…” (p. 184). Spector-Mersel (2011, 2012) also discussed the process of claiming a sense of self and narrative identity through life stories—even going further to assert that the process is reciprocal, stating, “If a sense of identity is attained through the stories we tell ourselves and others, not only is identity expressed in narrative, but also importantly, it is also constructed by it” (p. 173). Bamberg (2008) pointed to the “identity work” conducted during the process of telling stories, which produces one or more particular “displays” of a sense of self. Similarly, LitvakHirsch and Bar-On (2006), following Bruner (1990) and Gergen (1992), asserted a view of the individual as “creator of stories and as constructor of his identity through his life story” (p. 467). Along these lines, Mishler (2004) contended that “research participants are the historians of their own lives. They tell and retell their stories in variant ways and, thereby, continually revise their identities” (p. 101). In this sense, then, exploring how an individual retells his stories over time may allow a unique view into the degrees and modes of coherence and continuity therein, which, in turn, provides an opportunity to understand how narrative identity may develop over the life course. Bamberg (2008) discussed the value of retellings and this burgeoning research area, as a conduit to more clearly understanding the manners in which an individual may put together, or ‘claim,’ and continually revise a sense of self at different moments in time. Indeed, Bamberg (2008) followed Shweder et al. (2006) in asserting that “the perfect material for an empirical inquiry into the topic of identity formation processes would be the twice-told tales of the same person over a period of time” (p. 186). In addition, Norrick (1998) contended that “Retold stories are of interest both for the study of narration and for research on repetition in discourse” (p. 76). Bamberg (2008) also suggested that story retelling is of interest to linguists and cognitive researchers as well, as potential findings could illuminate processes of memory encoding, storage, and transformation. In many ways, then, this is an important area of inquiry. Research on retelling of stories, however, is sparse, at best. 27 While many narrative researchers have mentioned the importance of studying how individuals tell the ‘same’ stories over time, they also admit that this type of research has thus far simply not been done on any large scale (Bamberg, 2008; Greenspan, 2003; Norrick, 1998). Mishler (2004) stated, “Very little is known about the factors that affect how we tell our story on each next time, about what varies or stays the same in form or content” (p. 103). This doctoral research has as one of its primary aims an attempt to fill this lacuna and contribute to the literature on retelling. While this study focuses less on the participants’ identity per se, central emphasis is placed on the interplay between personal and societal discourse, and on a deep exploration of how one’s manner of story-telling may reflect processes of meaning-making, narrative processing, and coping over time. Most of the few studies that have been conducted on the topic have found that retellings are generally different from each other. Mishler (2004), for instance, asserted that it is “unarguable” that “the content, form, and performance of the so-called same story is always different,” pointing to general memory processes and the passage of time between tellings, in addition to assumed differences in telling contexts and intentions as well as audiences, as likely explanations for these variances (p. 103; Chafe, 1998; Ferrara, 1994; Norrick, 1998; Schiffrin, 2003). Along these lines, Litvak-Hirsch and Bar-On (2006) cited as their motivation for re-interviewing the women in their study the theoretical view that “narratives are continually reconstructed as the life course progresses (Cohler & Grunebaum, 1981; Josselson, 1995)” (p. 465). Bamberg (2008) claimed that in “a natural, conversationally embedded setting, and in the condition of the same audience, the way the story is told is likely to change… However, we would expect the gist – the backbone of what happened – to remain pretty much the same” (p. 185). Bamberg (2008) also asserted that time having elapsed between the tellings could have a significant influence on what is told and how it is told, perhaps as a result of reinterpretation or “reworking the ‘memory bank’” (p. 185). At the heart of the motivation for the present study lie inquiries into the impact of the passage of time on a given narrative; what is involved in the process of narrative processing and reinterpretation of a given experience over time; and the manner in which the ‘packaging’ of and discursive techniques utilized in multiple story-tellings can reflect the individual’s inner world and perceptions. The finding that multiple tellings can be highly disparate has a direct and problematic impact on the degree of confidence a researcher can reasonably place on a single telling of a life event and how much can be hypothesized about the teller’s identity therein. In light of this, Mishler (2004) referred to the problem of retellings, asserting that analytical trust is a core issue and stating, “Here is the crux of the matter: If researchers use stories as data for making claims about identity, then what are they to do when the stories turn out to be different?” (p. 103). In this sense, it becomes crucial to relate to a plurality of identities, or a contextual, fluid, and co-created process of claiming identity through narrative, rather than a fixed and monolithic concept of a single, 28 immutable identity (Spector-Mersel, 2011; Mishler, 2004). Andersson (2008), too, contended that the idea of a “solid and united” self-identity should be viewed post-structurally only as an interactive discursive notion, utilized as a means to presenting an ideal version of the self—that “There is no ‘real’ self or ‘authentic’ identity independent of the discursive environment” (p. 143). Indeed, in Mishler’s (2004) seminal study, faced with multiple (highly divergent) versions of the ‘same’ story and myriad possible explanations for the motivations therein, rather than questioning which Betty is “the real Betty,” he suggested that it might be more prudent to view the two tellings as “representing two versions of her, neither one more or less true than the other” (p. 116). Multiple versions, or identities, may be seen as congruent or in tension with each other, but nonetheless able to exist together without a need to consider them contradictory or problematic. Bamberg (2008), in an analysis of the same material, asserted that his analysis explored “how Betty hails two different subjectivities (or ‘identities,’ if you want to) into being,” or the means by which Betty constructed the two versions of the story, which led to the assigning of the multiple identities (p. 201). Josselson (2009), too, referred to a “multiple self,” stating that “Memories, in this sense, operate as texts whose meaning changes as the dialogue within self changes. The meanings of past memories, rather than their contents, are reshaped to hold aspects of a layered, multiple self” (p. 647). The analytical implication is that explorations of retellings must reject the assumption of a ‘true version’ of a narrative or a single identity of a teller, embracing instead this conceptualization of a plurality of identities and the possibility of myriad narrative versions and truths therein. Given the dearth of research studies on the topic of retelling, it is necessary to look closely at each analysis that does exist in the literature in order to both understand its contribution and recognize what may still be needed to advance the field. Those investigations that hold the most parallels with the present study will be focused on in more detail, to glean more directly useful analytical recommendations as well as to highlight the contribution and innovation of this study. Research on retelling emerged for the first time in the 1930s, and in earnest only in the 1980s; it is only now gathering much more traction and interest. Perhaps the first study of its kind, by British psychologist Frederic Bartlett (1932), involved participants writing down remembered versions, at several points in time, of a folktale they had read. Norrick (1998) referred to several initial studies in various cultural contexts (Sherzer, 1981; Hymes, 1981, 1985; Bauman, 1986), and there also exist a small number of studies of retellings of stories by children (Scollon & Scollon, 1984; Romaine, 1984). These analyses, along with Chafe’s (1980) and Tannen’s (1980, 1984) investigations of ‘retellings’ of a non-dialogic film, primarily investigated the differences between oral, written, and cinematic versions of a story. There was little focus on autobiographical narratives, and some referred to ‘retelling’ as the process of essentially repeating a story previously told to the participant by someone else (as in a child ‘retelling’ a fairy tale that had been read to 29 him). Ferrara’s (1994) study of three versions of a story provided by a client within the context of therapy sessions appears to have more commonalities with the present research. However, as was most often the case in the few other studies on record as well, analytical focus was placed more on the structural and functional elements in the stories, rather than on a deep comparative microanalysis of both the form and content. There is a very small number of notable longitudinal follow-up studies on women (Cohler & Grunebaum, 1981; Josselson, 1995, 2000, 2009; Litvak-Hirsch & Bar-On, 2006), which involve comparisons of similar narratives, content, or themes over a series of interviewing occasions. All place emphasis on developmental processes surrounding identity, interpersonal relations, and/or personal processes of growth and change over time. McAdams et al. (2006), in a three-year study of the written memories of emerging adults, found that while most of the participants gave different autobiographical memories at each data collection point, overall their memories demonstrated a significant degree of continuity in content and themes therein. In addition, Thorne, Cutting, and Skaw (1998) conducted a study involving memories told to an interviewer on two separate occasions, with six months between them. Although most often the memories provided at each interview were not the same, a good degree of thematic consistency was found across the material. Special focus was placed on the few ‘twice-told’ narratives that appeared, but the analysis was not particularly deep and because of the lack of relevant material, few useful conclusions were reached. Norrick (1998) presented a study of retold conversational stories, exploring three different retelling contexts: 1) immediate retelling by a single teller for a new listener; 2) retelling of the same story by a single teller for different audiences on different days; and 3) group reconstruction, or co-narration, of a story that had already been told to the audience in attendance. As explanation for the motivation of the study, Norrick (1998) suggested that a focus on the moments in which a narrator tells a story more than one time for separate audiences provides a clearer perspective on what is repeated, which allows for an analysis of separate performances, as identified as multiple versions of the same story. Following Polanyi (1981), Norrick (1998) also asserted that “it is through separating the story qua underlying structure from the real-time performance that we can be said to be retelling ‘the same story’” (p. 77). It is unclear whether the author intended to include issues of motivation, context, and audience influence within the purview of the “performance” that supposedly needs to be separated, but in any case this assertion appears problematic, at best. Norrick (1998) did refer to “the distinctions between a distilled kernel story and those aspects of the narration tied immediately to the local context,” suggesting that “retold tales” can offer a “special perspective on conversational storytelling” in light of their ability to place these distinctions into focus (p. 94). Ultimately, a similar number and manner of parallel clauses were found in all versions of the retold stories, leading the author to conclude that “a story can remain 30 substantially intact from one telling to the next…[suggesting] that tellers redesign a basic story for the audience present, rather than reconstructing a narrative from the ground up for each new audience” (Norrick, 1998, p. 75). This constitutes a meaningful contribution to the literature on conversational story-telling, and an important building block in the literature on retelling overall. Schiffrin (2003) conducted an analysis of four separate oral history tellings , with a range of two to eleven years between them, provided by a single Holocaust survivor, in an attempt to explore “knowledge and action in narrative, life story and oral history” (p. 537). Analytical focus was placed on structural and evaluative changes within the narratives, as well as on shifts in the form and meaning of a specific phrase marking a turning point. Changes were found primarily in the mode of evaluation utilized across the tellings, and Shiffrin (2003) concluded that the narrative had become (more) performative over the years. Particular reference was made to “the double arrow of time,” and the pervasive involvement of this process within the “ever-widening context of later experiences that provides gradual understandings of ‘what happened’ and leads to reconstruction of the meanings of past experiences” (Shiffrin, 2003, p. 540). Despite a deep emphasis on the longitudinal meaning-making processes undergone by the teller, perhaps the most prominent problematic issue in this study was that each of the four tellings involved different audiences and contexts, and that this was not a topic of significant analytical focus. Continuing in the genre of Holocaust testimony, Greenspan (2003) wrote about an exercise conducted within the framework of a university course, in which he presented three versions of a specific narrative told in three different interviews conducted by himself with a single Holocaust survivor. Greenspan (2003) noted that, in general, he is “centrally concerned with how survivors retell: not only their use of narrative form but also survivors’ specific, situated uses of repetition, silence, and voice” (p. 101), and his research explored the ways in which the testimonies of survivors evolve over the course of time, over retellings, and as part of an interplay with the relationship between the teller and the audience. Highlighting a difference between his study and Schiffrin’s (2003), Greenspan (2003) emphasized that testimony is not equivalent to oral history, in that the former is not primarily concerned with documentation or ‘objective’ eyewitness accounts. In this sense, Greenspan’s (2003) study constitutes a closer parallel with the present research, as it involved narratives of an autobiographical experience told within a personalized interview context; however, the former was ultimately primarily descriptive in nature, discussing the students’ reactions to the various tellings and general content and thematic issues. The survivor’s story was referred to as “a kind of monument—a memory that is also a memorial” (Greenspan, 2003, p. 108), and little narrative micro-analysis was conducted on the form and content of this “monument.” Litvak-Hirsch and Bar-On (2006) presented a study of the life stories of three generations of women—a Holocaust survivor and her daughter and granddaughter—conducting a “double 31 analysis” of their stories, 12 years apart, in order to explore “the transformations of relationships over time” and the centrality of these transformations within their personal development (p. 465). Analytical emphasis was placed on themes surrounding the women’s personal processes (both long- and short-term) and relationships with themselves, their significant others, and each other. The analysis was primarily thematic and focused on the content, exploring inclusions and omissions in each life story ‘version,’ in addition to what was given more or less emphasis. Direct micro-level comparisons of the data were not presented, however, and the lack of quotations from the interviewees made it difficult to judge the level and credibility of the comparisons. Josselson (2000) conducted a long-term analysis of early memories, using interview material collected as part of a longitudinal study of identity formation in women. A question about the participants’ earliest two memories was included in each of three interviews, which took place with 10-12 years separating each of them, and the responses (some verbal and others in writing) were analyzed with a view to exploring change over time and the internal stability of the structure of personality across developmental time periods. In the thematic analysis conducted, three patterns were explored, in particular: consistent themes, developmental change, and unique memories. Within this perspective, Josselson (2000) also looked at how the affective tone of the narrated memory may reflect the individual’s resolution of an issue at the given point in time, and how the individual’s encoded defenses or coping mechanisms may undergo transformations in relation to the existence or degree of resolution. It was found that most of the participants’ earliest memories showed a high degree of consistency and stability, suggesting the existence of core themes that emerge within the process of consolidation of personality over the life course. However, Josselson (2000) also noted, “While the criteria for ‘sameness’ required that the memory narrate essentially the same event, the affective tone and deeper thematic material often show changes from one time period to another” (p. 468). It is, indeed, in these changes in tone, theme, and form—as well as content—that the present research resides, exploring the meanings, messages, and end points of what often emerges as patterns of divergences over time. Using the same interview material, Josselson (2009) investigated a single autobiographical episode in one woman’s life, told on four separate occasions over a period of 35 years. Analytical emphasis was placed on exploration of the manners in which the present (re)constructs the past, as the author asserted that “the process by which the self adjusts memory over time is not yet well understood” (Josselson, 2009, p. 648). Ultimately, it was found that “across the life span it is the meanings assigned to memory rather than the contents of the memory itself that are reworked in order to anchor, illuminate, counterpoint, disavow, or otherwise enter into dialogue with current self-experience” (Josselson, 2009, p. 664). Josselson’s (2009) study was particularly close in goals, analytical groundings, and findings with this doctoral research, and the insights and conclusions 32 therein have influenced much of how the present study was conducted. Perhaps the most well-known and well-read explorations of retelling, with much similarity to the longitudinal comparative analyses conducted in the current study, can be found in Mishler’s (2004) analysis and Bamberg’s (2008) paper on “Twice-told tales,” both of which analyze the same data. The material analyzed came from a woman, “Betty,” who was videotaped telling two versions of the ‘same’ story, at the same location, to the same (anonymous) audience, under similar circumstances, with a few hours between the tellings. The content of each telling was purportedly, as Bamberg (2008) writes, “exactly the same,” but the manners in which each was told and performed were dramatically different from each other. Mishler (2004)’s analysis was highly detailed, with great attention to the words Betty used, in an attempt to locate the significant contrasts between the two versions, mainly in lexical and syntactic choices. There were some very minor references to pronouns, but no deep linguistic interpretation of Betty’s use of various discursive and semantic tools. Meanwhile, Bamberg (2008) performed “a small story, microanalytic analysis on this seemingly monologic data” (p. 189), analyzing each version independently and then conducting a thorough comparative analysis. Bamberg’s (2008) analysis was mostly content-focused, exploring ‘classic’ narrative ingredients such as plot, sequence of events, complication, and resolution. Processes of meaning- and sensemaking in the narratives were also explored, although there was no evident capacity to look at the surrounding contexts of the tellings and Betty’s embeddedness within them. With regard to the form of the tellings, Bamberg’s (2008) analysis contained some focus on narrative clauses and devices, but little to no attention was paid to semantic discourse, such as pronouns and tenses. Finally, Chafe’s (1998) research constitutes the closest parallel—theoretically, conceptually, and methodologically—to this doctoral study, providing much valuable insight into the utility of a methodology comparing two tape-recorded tellings, fifteen months apart, of the ‘same’ experience by the same individual. Similar content was presented juxtaposed together, and the transcriptions included notations of pauses, pitch, voice contours, and lengthened syllables. Narrative mechanisms such as settings, complications, climaxes, denouements, and coda were identified and analyzed, with attention paid to the similarities and differences between the two tellings. Chafe (1998; see also 1973) also related to “preoccupations” of the speaker, which, he claimed, caused some topics to be repeated narratively, as a reflection of their repetitive intrusive presence in the speaker’s consciousness. Overall, significant focal, thematic, structural, and chronological stability was found between the tellings, although analyses of prosodic, syntactic, and space-time orientational patterns showed some inconsistencies. Perhaps the greatest contribution of Chafe’s work on retelling, with regard to the present study, was his assertions about sentence inconsistency: “the information brought together in a sentence seems not to represent any cognitively stable unit of 33 perception, storage, or remembering. Rather, sentence boundaries appear when a speaker judges, during a particular telling, that a coherent center of interest has been verbalized at that point. There are a variety of grounds for judging such coherence, and those grounds are subject to variation in repeated verbalizations of the same subject matter” (Chafe, 1994, p. 145). Indeed, as will be apparent in the analyses presented in this dissertation, although the sentence structure often varied slightly between tellings, the content, form, and narrative meanings frequently showed significant within-participant stability. Thus, as Chafe (1998) determined, juxtaposing retellings across time for the purposes of a psycholinguistic comparative analysis of narrative and psychic development, and of manners of verbalization of underlying phenomenological experience, can be a valuable and fruitful endeavor. In summary, each of these seminal articles made valiant attempts to explore various facets and contexts of retelling. Norrick (1998) investigated conversational retellings, sometimes (but not always) by the same teller, and sometimes (but not always) to the same audience. Shiffrin (2003) looked at oral history retellings, each to different audiences and from within different motivations and purposes. Litvak-Hirsch and Bar-On (2006) explored life story retellings, in similar contexts and with similar emphases but to different audiences. Josselson’s (2000) study dealt with early memories told by the same teller to the same interviewer on three different occasions, with similar thematic content and some of the ‘same’ narratives, and (2009) research investigated the case of a single teller providing content on the same topic to the same audience, in four different sessions over a span of 35 years. Mishler’s (2004) and Bamberg’s (2008) analyses dealt with two ‘identical’ stories told by the same individual to the same (anonymous) audience over the duration of just a few hours. And Chafe (1998) conducted a largely linguistic comparative analysis of two tellings about the ‘same’ experience told by the same individual to a similar audience, over fifteen months. All of these studies, and those that preceded them, are invaluable contributions to the literature, and I contend that the present study can offer an equally important and necessary contribution. In many ways, indeed, this study combines the best of the previous literature with a focus on some of the most important facets lacking from them. In its analyses of retellings of the ‘same’ (or rather, ‘parallel’) narratives from the same individuals, told to the same audience, in the same interview context and for the same research purposes, across a period of three years, and with its interdisciplinary focus on both psychosocial and linguistic elements, as well as both the form and content, of the discourse, this study attempts to both rely and build upon this valuable literature. From within the research goal to investigate multiple narrative truths and the longitudinal process of meaning-making, narrative researchers have repeatedly called for more and deeper studies of retellings, of precisely the nature that characterizes this doctoral study. Mishler (2004), for instance, asserted that in the current state of longitudinal narrative research, there is “little opportunity for participants to respond to the same question/situation from different perspectives” 34 (p. 119), and offered a recommendation regarding the value involved in narrative researchers asking multiple versions of a question about the ‘same’ event or experience. Perhaps most compellingly, Josselson (2009) claimed: “Single tellings of a given memory, while offering a window into current personality organization and concern, do not illuminate the processes by which self and memory are narratively co-constructive. What has not been investigated, in part due to the logistical difficulties of long-term longitudinal studies, is the way in which the same memory may be reevaluated over long periods of time in light of ongoing experience and the restructuring of the self” (p. 649). Along these lines, it may be possible that the dearth of research on retelling is, at least in part, a result of a lack of an appropriate or effective methodological tool that could enable a systematic and in-depth comparison of ‘parallel’ narratives. Bamberg (2008), in particular, recommended that, given that life (and narrating a life) is a continuous process, “we may have to rethink qualitative methods, and narratives [sic] methods in particular, in terms of how they capture and do justice to the constant changes that take place” (p. 203). Indeed, the present research study attempts to do exactly this, rethinking and expanding a number of narrative methods currently in use, and proposing new manners of viewing and analyzing the retelling of narratives over time. This type of investigation is important not only from a theoretical perspective, but also from a methodological and practical standpoint, as much can be gained and understood from studying the longitudinal development of narrative processing and meaning-making. 2.3.3 The relationship between the use of pronouns and the health of the speaker The area of research utilizing quantitative and/or qualitative analytical methods to study discourse on a micro-level, and particularly to explore connections between the use of various semiotic elements and the health of the speaker, is also still relatively sparse. Within the literature that does exist, James Pennebaker and a number of his students and colleagues have emerged as the leaders therein. Pennebaker (2007) differentiated between content words (nouns, regular verbs, and most adjectives and adverbs), which signify “what people are talking about,” and style words (pronouns, prepositions, articles, etc.), which “provide clues to how they are talking and socially connecting to their audience” (p. 4). He asserted that style words are “more intriguing” than content words, as recent research has found them to be related to a number of psychological states that can, in turn, influence health (Pennebaker, 2007, p. 3). Many studies have been conducted on the associations between pronoun use and affective states (see Chung & Pennebaker, 2007, for a review of these findings), but most have treated the discursive strategies under investigation as little more than a predictive or associative variable, rather than a semiotic phenomenon worthy of study in and of itself (Boals & Perez, 2009). As Pennebaker et al. (2003) wrote: “Use of first person singular, for example, is associated with age, sex, depression, illness, and more broadly, self-focus. First person plural can variously be a marker of group identity and, on occasion, a sign of emotional distancing (Pennebaker & Lay, 2002). Second and third person pronouns are, by definition, markers to suggest that the speaker is socially engaged or aware” (p. 570). 35 Indeed, the extent of most of Pennebaker’s statements on the linguistic properties of pronouns is represented in the following: “Particles serve as the glue that holds content words together. But particles are more than mere glue. They are referential words that have tremendous social and psychological meaning. To use a pronoun requires the speaker and listener to share a common knowledge of who the referent is. … Pronoun use requires a relatively sophisticated awareness of the audience’s ability to track who is who. Prepositions are also referential. … Particles, then, can be construed as having tremendous social implications” (Pennebaker et al. 2003, p. 570). While these are important assertions within the world of discursive psychology, they fall short in the linguistic domain, failing to provide meaningful interpretations of the psychosemiotic phenomena at work. Indeed, Chung and Pennebaker (2007), relying on fundamental assumptions that the use of the ‘I’ pronoun simply “suggests that the speaker is briefly paying attention to the self” (p. 352), while “most other pronouns implicitly or explicitly suggest that the person is attending to other individuals” (p. 351), then linked with the literature on social support, which claims that greater discursive reference to others is associated with adaptive coping and consequently better physical health. Pennebaker (2002) stated that in this emerging area of study, psychologists have found many “intriguing” patterns related to a variety of parts of speech, but the most revealing, and the “category that is yielding the most remarkable links to mental and physical health is pronouns” (p. 8). Along these lines, Pennebaker (2002) suggested: “The ways that people refer to themselves and others is highly diagnostic of their mental state. The use of 1 st person singular (I, me, my) versus 1st person plural (we, us, our) provides insight into people’s social identity and ‘ownership’ of their speaking or writing topic. By the same token, references to other people suggest an awareness and, often, integration with others” (p. 8). Several studies have found an association between the use of the first-person singular pronoun and personal distress (Boals & Klein, 2005; Pennebaker & Lay, 2002) and negative affective states (see Chung & Pennebaker, 2007, for a review of these findings). In particular, it has been reported that depressed or suicide-prone individuals show a much greater tendency to make ‘I’-statements than do individuals who do not suffer from depression (Ramírez-Esparza & Pennebaker, 2006; Stirman & Pennebaker, 2001). As Chung and Pennebaker (2007) noted, two such studies found that patterns of pronoun use actually functioned as a better marker of depression in the participants than did their use of words that signified negative emotions. The authors essentially argued that the use of the first-person singular pronoun signifies attention to the self, too much of which has been found to be associated with depression (Boals & Perez, 2009; Chung & Pennebaker, 2007). Furthermore, Campbell and Pennebaker (2003) found a correlation between a change in pronoun use over time and an improvement in physical health. As Boals and Perez (2009) claimed, “A change in use of pronouns is believed to reflect a change in social perspective” (p. 3), and may, thus, serve as a possible predictor of psychological states. This field is clearly emerging as an important area of interest, but the sparse focus upon 36 second-person pronouns in any of the studies conducted thus far is perplexing. This is most likely a result of the semiotic structure of the English language, which includes only a single second-person pronoun (‘you’), rather than four separate forms as in Modern Israeli Hebrew (masculine singular and plural, and feminine singular and plural). However, even when involving only a single secondperson form, the qualitative difference between the narrative phrases, “I went through that trauma,” and “You went through that trauma,” is substantial and meaningful, and deserving of research attention. While Pennebaker and his colleagues have emphasized the import of a switch from firstperson singular to first-person plural, a dedicated focus on pronoun switching between first- and second-person seems equally warranted. Along these lines, research on the connection between the other underrepresented pronouns and the speaker’s inner psychological world, psychosocial functioning, and overall health is also lacking. The present study aims to fill this lacuna as well. 2.4 THE MACRO-LEVEL PSYCHOSOCIAL CONTEXT OF THE RESEARCH STUDY This section addresses the psychosocial context surrounding the bus drivers who participated in this study, on a macro-level and from both a theoretical and phenomenological perspective. The first sub-section presents the political and security realities of living in Israel, under the constant threat of war, terror, violent tensions, and stress, and the effects this can have on its populace. The section begins with an overview of Israel’s history of war and conflict, and statistics on the impact of the Second Intifada. The phenomenological daily experience is then discussed, with a focus on the shared and communal nature of the stress experienced. Recent literature on the psychological effects of both acute and chronic stress and trauma—in general, and in the Israeli case, in particular—is then reviewed, with an equal focus on the potential negative effects as well as the salutogenic possibility for resilient responses to these challenges. Finally, a brief discussion is presented of the experiences and perceptions surrounding the disparity between military and civilian trauma in the Israeli context, with particular focus on the isolation and marginalization expressed by many of the individuals interviewed in the present study. The second sub-section offers an overview of the psychosociocultural context surrounding the bus drivers, with regard to the hegemonic myths in Israeli society vis-à-vis masculinity, power, heroism, and pressure to act courageously under all circumstances and against all odds—idealized in the prescriptive myth of ‘The Israeli Warrior.’ The overall goal of this review is to locate the participants of this study within their societal context, while also bringing the reader in to understand it more intimately. It may well be impossible to fully understand every facet of the drivers’ discourse, as so much of it may be deeply intertwined with this psychosocial context, but an attempt must be made to explore the factors at work. Again, while this research study does not assume that these individuals were traumatized by 37 their experiences, it is nonetheless critical to understand the potentially traumatic properties and negative effects of terror, in addition to the possibility for resilient responses. All of the factors discussed here may play a part in the interviewees’ discursive choices and narrative strategies, through their influence on the meaning-making processes and attribution systems of the narrators. 2.4.1 Terror, war, and chronic stress in Israel1 2.4.1.1 Overview and statistics Terrorism, war, conflict, and chronic exposure to potential trauma, in one form or another, have been pervasive components of the fabric of Israeli society since the official establishment of the State in 1948, and well beforehand. Israel has been involved in a total of fourteen wars and military operations since its independence was declared, and because these events have been spaced out relatively evenly, there is a general sense of seemingly constant tension and conflict—either in actual war or in expectation of the next armed confrontation or terrorist incident, locally or abroad. The past three decades in particular, encompassing the First and Second Intifadas (1987-1993 and 2000-2004/20052, respectively), have had a significant effect on the Israeli populace (Baum, 2004; Bleich, Gelkopf, & Solomon, 2003; Kaplan et al., 2005). Indeed, particularly during the Al-Aqsa Intifada (a.k.a., Second Intifada), which began in late September of 2000 and was characterized by frequent and deadly TAs, the day-to-day reality in Israel was filled with palpable fear, anxiety, and apprehension (Bleich et al., 2003). Many of the attacks were carried out by suicide bombers, targeting public places such as restaurants, buses, supermarkets, shopping malls, and various venues of entertainment and popular leisure sites. Other types of attacks included detonations of roadside bombs; intrusions into private homes, restaurants, hotels, and public buildings; kidnappings and hostage-takings; drive-by shootings; and various ‘random’ injuries and killings, such as those using knives, stones, or Molotov cocktails (Bleich et al., 2003; Zeidner, 2005, 2006b). Victims included civilians as well as security forces, of all ages and walks of life. Kaplan et al. (2005) described the atmosphere in great and accurate detail: “In the cities in those areas of Israel that are bounded by the Green Line (i.e., conform to Israel’s borders on 1 2 It may be necessary to state from the outset that there are, of course, two sides to the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, and both have suffered devastating and deadly losses throughout. Both sides have acted as aggressors as well as suffered as victims, and it is beyond the scope of this dissertation to enter into a discussion of the deeply complex and convoluted politics of the conflict. It should, nonetheless, be acknowledged that there is no mention here of the ‘Palestinian side’—that is, of the fatalities and injuries suffered and the daily situation in Gaza and the West Bank, that is just as fraught with tension, fear, and violence as is the daily reality within the boundaries of the Green Line on the Israeli side. This lack of discussion is a reflection not of a political position that the ‘other side’ of the conflict is less important, less tragic, or less worthy of mention, but only of the fact that the focus of this study is on the experience of terror by (Jewish) Israeli bus drivers, within the very specific psychosocial context surrounding them. It is difficult to pinpoint the precise end of the Second Intifada, as there was no declaration as such, or signing of any kind of treaty or agreement. The violence simply seemed to slowly decrease, perhaps as a result of the gradual progression in building the security fence/wall, or perhaps as a result of greater intelligence and anti-terrorism efforts on the part of the Israeli army. The lack of successful attacks did not appear to be the result of a lack of or decreased attempts on the part of the intended terrorists. 38 the eve of the Six-Day War in 1967), the terror attacks have characteristically taken place in crowded public venues, with shootings and suicide bomb attacks on buses and in markets, shops, and discotheques. In the areas of maximal contact between the Palestinians and the Israelis—the West Bank, the Gaza Strip, and Jerusalem—the atmosphere is even more one of a war zone. The constant military/paramilitary presence on both sides, the road-blocks, the sound of gunfire by day and nightly mortar shelling, all create a very different atmosphere to that within the Green Line. Within the Green Line, the attacks are less frequent, unexpected, and often on a larger scale, i.e., more costly in terms of casualties. The events are immediately, extensively, and vividly (some claim luridly) reported in real time in all of the media” (p. 1147). There were, indeed, highly differential experiences throughout different regions of Israel during the Second Intifada; although generally, attacks were more focused on the three major metropolitan areas of Jerusalem, Tel Aviv, and Haifa, as well as the three smaller cities of Hadera, Netanya, and Afula3 (Somer et al., 2005). Statistics show that during 2002, 20% of the TAs that occurred were in Jerusalem, while 8% took place in Tel Aviv, and only 3.2% occurred in northern Israel and 1.1% in Haifa (Zeidner, 2006b). Across the entire span of the Intifada, Jerusalem was, by far, the hardest hit, with a total of 22.7% of the total number of TAs having taken place there (Zeidner, 2006b). The data varies with regard to the precise number of TAs, injuries, and casualties during these periods. Perliger, Pedahzur, and Zalmanovitch (2005) claimed that there were nearly 200 suicide bombings in Israel between 1994 and 2005. In Jerusalem alone, there were over 30 suicide bombings and hundreds of other violent incidents over the course of that decade, with the first suicide bombing that caused fatalities (and second overall) in Jerusalem having taken place in August of 1995. The four to five years of the Second Intifada comprised a particularly intense and violent period, however, involving near-constant incidents of terror-related violence. Indeed, from September of 2000 to October of 2003, there was not a single month in which fatalities did not occur in Israel as a result of terrorism (Yechiam, Barron, & Erev, 2005). Some sources indicate that there were approximately 132 suicide bombings between October of 2000 and April of 2004, 85 of which occurred within the Green Line between 20012003 (Kaplan, Mintz, Mishal, & Samban, 2005; Shalev et al., 2006; Sharlin, Moin, & Yahav, 2006). These attacks reportedly caused the deaths of 666 and injuries to 4,447 Israeli civilians, and the deaths of 276 and injuries to 1,843 individuals in security forces—a total of 0.11% of the Israeli population at the time (Shalev et al., 2006). Other sources include a wider range of types of attacks; including knife and gun attacks, home intrusions, and drive-by shootings. Citing the Israel Ministry of Foreign Affairs, for instance, Bleich et al. (2003) reported that, as of the end of April of 2002, “472 persons (318 civilians) had been killed in terrorist attacks and 3846 persons (2708 civilians) had been injured (totaling 0.067% of the population of 6.4 million),” with a total of 560 TAs having occurred within the Green Line (p. 612; Zeidner, 2005, 2006b). Others relate more generally to “thousands of men, women, and children” killed or wounded on both sides (Sharlin et al., 2006, p. 97) or a “massive” casualty rate of “over 1000 Israelis” killed during the Second 3 These three cities are closest to the Green Line, the unofficial border between Israel and the Palestinian territories. 39 Intifada alone (Somer, Ruvio, Soref, & Sever, 2005, p. 166), although the source of this claim is not clear. The graph below presents data on the occurrence of suicide bombings only within the Green Line during 1994-2011, as gleaned from the official Israel Ministry of Foreign Affairs website. Figure 2-1: Occurrence of suicide bombing attacks within the Green Line, 1994-2011 In addition, the figure below represents a timeline illustration of the suicide bombing attacks that occurred before, during, and after the Second Intifada. It was at the height of this period of time that the drivers who participated in this study were working full-time on a daily basis, and toward the end of which they were interviewed for this study. Figure 2-2: Timeline of suicide bombing attacks within the Green Line, 1994-20074 4 The dates written in red above the timeline represent TAs that occurred on or involving buses, while those below the timeline represent TAs that occurred in other venues. Numbers in parentheses signify multiple attacks (either double or triple bombings that occurred together or directly in succession). 40 I created this timeline illustration early in the research process, as a means to attempt to enter into the psychological mindset of the drivers I was interviewing. And indeed, from just a brief glimpse at the peak period of TAs, recognizing that there were often multiple attacks in a single week, or even in successive days (not to mention successive attacks on the same day), one can perhaps begin to imagine the nature of stress, fear, and tension in which these drivers (and the Israeli population at large) must have been functioning. In addition to this, the Israeli populace was bombarded with constant media coverage of these attacks, most often filled with gory images of burning shells of buses, bloodied and charred bodies hanging out of burnt window frames, and pools of blood surrounding scenes of carnage and destruction. Indeed, as Somer et al. (2005) wrote, “Television, radio, internet and printed media coverage of these events was immediate, graphic, and extensive, contributing to a sense of a massive shared national crisis that engulfed survivors and other civilians alike” (p. 166). This research cannot be comprehended fully without attempting to understand this daily reality confronting Israeli bus drivers during this period. 2.4.1.2 The phenomenological daily experience and its effects: Communal acute and chronic stress and trauma This period in Israel’s history was thus terribly difficult on a daily basis, with ongoing and unpredictable TAs, particularly leveled at civilian populations, causing an all-encompassing sense of threat to individuals and to the society as a whole (Zeidner, 2005). As Baum (2004) wrote, “Riding a bus, sitting in a cafe, attending a celebration in a hall, driving along certain roads—all these and many other ordinary activities have become fraught with danger and a source of a constant underlying apprehension” (p. 396), and referred to the existence of “a perpetual sense of threat, which permeates people’s daily lives” (p. 396). Furthermore, Ayalon (2004) noted, “When airplanes, buses, and trains are targeted and when shops and restaurants are exploding, daily mobility can no longer be taken for granted. The threat is contagious—there is a lurking danger that any train or bus can become a death trap” (p. 176). In many ways, for the past few decades at least, the Israeli population has subsisted under constant threat and both chronic and acute stress. Indeed, for this reason, Israelis are known as avid followers of the news—watching, reading, or listening to broadcasts several times a day to keep constantly abreast of the security situation. Terror in Israel has thus been referred to as not only a personal challenge, but a national political, societal, and communal trauma, as it is experienced in a shared manner by so many facets of the population (Nuttman-Shwartz, Lauer, & Offir, 2002; Laqueur, 1999; Raviv et al., 2000; Zeidner, 2006a, 2006b). The demographic makeup and small size of the country, as well as the strong focus on the “cohesive traditional family structure” among the Israeli Jewish population, all serve to create a sense of personal vulnerability with regard to TAs, even in the absence of ‘direct’ involvement (Brody & Baum, 2007, p. 10; Somer et al., 2005). These periods in Israel’s history 41 can thus be viewed as having caused a “group trauma,” defined by Kimbles (2006) as a process by which “each member of the group recognizes that something happened at the level of the group psyche…that has profound implications for issues of belonging and identity” (p. 104). Indeed, as Noy (2004) asserted, trauma that occurs on a larger scale—that is, beyond the realm of a single individual—can have far-reaching implications; being, as it is, “not confined to the individual’s narrative,” but rather, having the capacity to actually “destroy social structure and order” (p. 212). With regard to the overall Israeli population’s rate of primary victimization from TAs during the Second Intifada, the statistics actually show very low numbers. As of 2006, only 0.01% had been killed and 0.07% injured (Zeidner, 2006b). However, Bleich et al. (2003) found that in a representative sample of over 500 Israeli men and women, “terrorism has had a substantial impact. Almost two thirds of the sample (60%) reported that they felt that their lives were in danger and more than two thirds (67.9%) that they felt that the lives of their friends and family were in danger” (p. 619). This involves widespread effects, affecting Israelis of all groups and in all walks of life. As Zeidner (2006b) contended, “Even for those not directly impacted, the [effects of the] past 5 years of ongoing terror attack…are still reverberating, and causing concern about personal safety in their day-to-day life routines” (p. 309; Zeidner, 2006a). Indeed, even in the absence of direct exposure to a terror event, most Israelis tend to feel and report that they ‘experienced’ the terror of these periods (Kaplan et al., 2005). Many have a friend or acquaintance who was directly involved, and many can recall a ‘close call’ when they ‘almost’ got onto a bus that later exploded, or ‘just left’ a restaurant before an attack. Somer et al. (2005) found that approximately 28% of their subjects responded that they “felt that they narrowly escaped an attack only by sheer luck” (p. 177). The chronic and near-constant nature of terror and conflict facing Israel over past decades can thus be characterized as “a traumatic national disaster situation,” involving thousands of fatalities and injuries, an onset that has both sudden and unexpected but yet protracted elements, and a pattern of severely disrupted daily functioning and coping (Zeidner, 2006b, p. 307). One of the most prominent factors in the widespread experience of stress and the communal sense of national crisis during these periods in Israel was the print and televised media coverage after each TA that occurred. Kaplan et al. (2005), describing the Al-Aqsa Intifada period, wrote that “Almost everyone follows the events closely in the media, and many report a vicarious form of intense emotional involvement in the events” (p. 1147). National news coverage of TAs in Israel is often aired only minutes after they have occurred, and is often graphic and disturbing in itself. Somer et al. (2005) referred to “the extensive traumatizing sounds and images broadcast on national radio and television” (p. 166), and pointed to emerging evidence of “a significant association between exposure to television coverage of catastrophic events and subsequent psychological symptoms” (p. 178; Cantor, Mares, & Oliver, 1993; Pfefferbaum et al., 2001; Schuster et al., 2001). 42 This additional type of indirect exposure to potentially traumatic images and scenes can indeed cause stress reactions and even long-term psychological effects. Especially when exposure of this nature can happen daily or repeatedly over a protracted period of time, as in the case of heightened periods of terror or war in Israel, a host of negative effects can result. It is well-known and well-documented in the psychological literature that exposure to trauma is “a pathogenic factor known to cause and exacerbate somatic and psychiatric distress and disorder including depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)” (Solomon, Gelkopf, & Bleich, 2005, p. 947; Freitag et al., 2011). Trauma can be caused by multiple stressful life experiences over a short period of time, or a series of even milder stressors accumulated over a longer period of time, or a single stressful event at one point in time (Denham, 2008). War and TAs are considered among the most distressing, painful, and potentially traumatic of life stressors, particularly due to the overwhelming sense of threat and possibility of death or lifelong disfigurement or impairment (Zeidner, 2006a). Terror, in particular, can cause a flood of anxiety, intense fear, and feelings of helplessness, overwhelming the victim’s sense of safety and security and rendering his defenses powerless, leading him to perceive the world around him as hostile and dangerous (Baum, 2004; Friedland & Merari, 1986; Herman, 1992; Zeidner, 2005, 2006a). These effects can be long-lasting and deeply disturbing, posing a great challenge to the individual’s ability to adapt and cope. As Zeidner (2006b) contended: “terror poses a threat to one’s physical and psychological integrity, one’s possessions and other entities, one’s sense of what is familiar and controllable, and a threat to cherished values, beliefs, commitments, and assumptions. In addition, terror attack leads to the curtailment of daily pursuits and routine activities, such as work, family, and leisure activities” (p. 297). Citing a host of studies, Sharlin et al. (2006) asserted that war and TAs, representing “a complex mixture of psychologically traumatic events,” can have a “wide-ranging and multi-faceted” impact that “may affect all areas of personality” (p. 98). In addition to post-traumatic stress symptoms, this can include depression, anxiety, and mental disorders; behavioral problems and difficulties in the social, familial, and occupational spheres; and difficulties with concentration, learning, and memory (Sharlin et al., 2006). These symptoms and/or disorders can appear in both individuals who directly experienced an attack and those in the general population who were indirectly exposed (Galea & Resnick, 2005; Sharlin et al., 2006). As Shalev et al. (2006) noted, “Terrorism, and the efforts to contain it, can also disrupt people’s daily living (e.g., by affecting transportation and other forms of freedom) and thereby create a secondary stressor” (p. 667). Living under, and the constant need to invest mental and psychic resources in coping with, such conditions of unremitting stress and political tension for protracted periods of time can take its toll. Following Horowitz and Lissak (1989), Zeidner (2006a) suggested that Israelis experience a “depletion of emotional resources over time and…social 43 burnout and potential increase in social tension and pathology” (p. 779; Zeidner, 2006b). In addition, it has been found that individuals who have been through potentially traumatic events tend to have “a less positive view regarding the world,” perceiving their world as less safe, their selfworth as lower in value, and their world as imbued with less meaning (Tuval-Mashiach et al., 2004, p. 282; Janoff-Bulman, 1992). In this sense, TAs can represent a fundamental challenge to coping and meaning-making, shattering a number of basic assumptions upon which humans rely to make sense of their world, and causing dramatic breaks and shifts in their worldview. Shalev et al. (2006) discussed an important facet of the distinction between acute and chronic trauma, or between a single stressful event and chronic and continuous stress, asserting that “under continuous threat, the symptoms that are currently subsumed as posttraumatic and therefore purposeless and somewhat exaggerated (e.g., avoiding previously dangerous places and situations, responding emotionally to threat signals, remaining vigilant and ‘on guard’) may reflect anticipation and self-protection” (p. 667). Thus, behaviors that would ‘normally’ be characterized as possible symptoms of PTSD may not reflect distress or dysfunction, but rather, may be seen as adaptive and even necessary under these difficult conditions. Indeed, when one is faced with the threat of terror on a daily basis, and unable to change the external circumstances or one’s inevitable exposure to them, the only useful, active, and adaptive methods of coping may be essentially protective methods—that is, “acceptance of the situation and getting on with life, cautiously planning one’s actions, and suppressing dangerous activities one normally performs” (Zeidner, 2006b, p. 308). Essentially, therefore, Israelis today may be subsisting in a state labeled by the field of psychology as “the new normal,” characterized by “persistent, widespread distress that, while not diagnostically significant, is significant in terms of social functioning and adaptation” (Steury, Spencer, & Parkinson, 2004, p. 158; North & Pfefferbaum, 2002). While the potential effects of chronic stress and conflict on a national scale are undeniable, however, there is another possible perspective—that of a salutogenic view of the resilience of the Israeli populace. 2.4.1.3 Israeli resilience In light of the state of protracted conflict over the past few decades, many studies have investigated the effects of trauma and war in various population groups in Israel (Murthy & Lakshminarayana, 2006). Indeed, the continuous and constant nature of the threat posed by TAs throughout the Second Intifada has provided an important and unique opportunity to study the effects of prolonged violence and chronic stress on a civilian population (Somer et al., 2005; Zeidner, 2006a). The present research is based, in part, on the hypothesis that the bus drivers interviewed herein, many of whom have withstood the ever-present stress of a looming TA on a near-daily basis for decades, have been dealing with these threats to their physical and emotional 44 lives with resilience. Indeed, the Israeli case may allow a salutogenic examination of the forces within the society that help and/or hinder its populace in their efforts to be resilient. Although the effects of protracted periods of TAs have been researched in many facets, individual responses and processes of coping and adjusting to these stressors are still not fully understood (Zeidner, 2006a, 2006b). As discussed in the resilience literature, there is significant variability in the reactions of those exposed to (even the same) traumatic events. Significant differences also exist in various conditions and contexts, and involving many personal, communal, and societal variables. Sharlin et al. (2006) presented a valuable review of the relevant literature, stating that the degree of stress experienced by an individual after a difficult event can be influenced by the sociocultural and political context surrounding the individual, the involvement of the media in the event and its aftermath, the social support system available, personal factors such as level of social-emotional and cognitive development, and the situational factors involved in the event itself. Following Rubonis and Bickman (1991), Zeidner (2005) noted that in general, a large proportion of a population exposed to a potentially traumatic event such as a TA tends to suffer from a mild form of stress for a relatively brief period of time. While it has been found that the Israeli population at large has indeed been affected by these long periods of conflict, the consensus has been that “Israeli adults appear to respond adaptively to the security crisis, manifesting rather low levels of somatic symptoms and post-traumatic stress reactions” (Zeidner, 2006a, p. 779; 2006b). For instance, Bleich et al. (2003) found that while 76.7% of a national sample of Israeli adults who had experienced war- or terror-related trauma were suffering from at least one symptom of traumatic stress, only 9.4% met the diagnostic criteria for PTSD. Somer et al. (2005) also found only a “moderate level of emotional distress,” with 18% of “noteworthy sadness” and 50% of “noteworthy tension” among a randomly-sampled population of Israelis (p. 177). Furthermore, it was found that individuals who had been directly affected by a traumatic event showed no greater likelihood of developing a negative mood than those who had been affected only indirectly or not at all, and in fact, the general mood in the areas of the country that had been the hardest hit by TAs was actually better than in the less-affected areas (Somer et al., 2005). Under the conditions of daily tension in Israel, it was contended, the levels of distress reported among the population were decidedly lower than what could have been expected. Indeed, Bleich et al. (2003) noted that there were few requests for professional help, that most of the participants appeared to be employing constructive and flexible coping strategies, and that “a majority also expressed optimism about their personal future and the future of Israel, and a sense of self-efficacy in the event of a terrorist attack” (p. 619). This rate is also lower than several other populations who experienced trauma, and may be considered particularly remarkable in light of the ongoing and chronic nature of the stress facing the Israeli population (Bleich et al., 2003). 45 Indeed, studies have found that Israelis tend to show a high degree of adaptation to whichever crisis situation is at hand, and “are commonly thought to be experts in survival” (Zeidner, 2006b, p. 300). It is possible, therefore, that this population, having been exposed to war, continuous violence, and ongoing conflict and tension for such an extended period of time, “may come to adopt a problem-focused attitude and to deal with stressful encounters in active ways even under the most dire of conditions” (Zeidner, 2006b, p. 300). There also has been found to be an accommodation, habituation, or inoculation effect at work in the Israeli population, as a result of the prolonged periods of stress they routinely endure (Baum, 2004; Bleich et al., 2003; Solomon, 1995). Shalev (2004) suggested that this unusually low incidence of PTSD in Israelis may illustrate the possibility of “some form of immunity,” perhaps conferred by having survived such long and repeated periods of trauma (p. 174). In addition, it has been claimed that “the strong community solidarity and support during times of war and political violence in Israeli society, may serve as sources of social support for Israelis” (Zeidner, 2006a, p. 779). For some time, then, Israeli civilians have been thrust into a position of having to cope with highly stressful events and their effects and literally build (or at least accept) this into the daily structure of their lives (Baum, 2004; Nuttman-Shwartz et al., 2002). As Baum (2004) noted, “Despite the repeated deadly attacks on the civilian population, Israelis have gone on with their daily lives and routines with a determined, somewhat fatalistic, business-as-usual approach and no large scale panic” (pp. 395-396). Indeed, following Breznitz and Eshel (1983), Somer et al. (2005) asserted that “although a higher baseline of pressures and stressors is characteristic of a large part of the Israeli population, the repeated exposure to intense stress can foster adequate modes of adjustment, based on learning from these experiences and subsequent habituation” (p. 178). This type of adaptation and accommodation may be one of the most powerful tools in the individual and collective arsenal of Israeli society, and is a prominent part of the societal discourse surrounding coping, heroism, and resilience. In addition, the collective coping of Israeli society with the national trauma of the Intifadas has created and hinged on a sense of communal psychological bond and a basic group cohesion (Nuttman-Schwartz et al., 2002; Zinner, Williams, & Ellis, 1999), reinforced by norms, symbolic meanings and identities, and ideological myths that all set forth prescriptions for behavior, beliefs, and emotional reactions. 2.4.1.4 Military vs. civilian trauma: Universal belonging vs. isolation and marginalization The social discourses on coping, heroism, and resilience are intimately connected to the centrality of the military in Israel, and the differential experiences of ‘military trauma’ vs. ‘civilian trauma.’ Several studies have focused on the experience of trauma and the emergence of posttraumatic symptoms in soldiers, and much research has been conducted on the same issues in 46 civilian populations. Israel, however, represents a particularly complex case, as its military system is based on a strongly involved civilian army—over 80% of the eligible Israeli population has served or is serving in the Israel Defense Forces (IDF), according to the Mahal website. This, along with the many wars, military operations, and TAs that have taken place in Israel over its very short history as a nation, means that essentially all Israelis have experienced some degree and amount of potentially traumatic experiences, whether as soldiers or civilians. Indeed, Ben- Ya’acov, Amir, Arzy, and Kotler (2005) emphasized “the uniqueness of Israeli society and the special place that military and civilian trauma occupies therein,” asserting, “The realities of Israeli life entail numerous traumas in both military and civilian settings” (p. 8). Ben-Ya’acov et al. (2005) suggested that in the Israeli case, active participation in combat can be considered to be essentially synonymous with ‘trauma exposure,’ as “a front-line soldier in a war, the Intifada, or any operational activity will almost always have been involved in, or witnessed, substantial military activity with injuries and/or life threatening events” (p. 3). Indeed, wars and operations in Israel tend to be very short, limited geographically, and intense (often lasting only a matter of weeks), and the Intifadas both involved near-daily incidences of violence over a period of years. Because of this level of intensity, the IDF employs a policy of rotation for all combat units—with the result that generally, soldiers do not spend more than three consecutive months in a front line combat role. Therefore, it is highly unlikely that combat soldiers in the army during these periods did not experience at least some trauma (Ben-Ya’acov et al., 2005). Many studies have noted differences between traumatic events that occur in a military, as opposed to civilian, setting. Rosenheck and Fontana (1994), for instance, claimed that stressors that occur in a military setting are of a higher intensity and are correlated with higher levels of posttraumatic symptoms than are non-military stressors. Helzer, Robins, and McEvoy (1987) found that individuals who experienced traumatic events as civilians generally suffer fewer symptoms, particularly hyper-alertness and disturbances in sleep. However, it was not clear whether this difference has to do with disparities in training and psychological ‘preparation,’ or due to what may be inherent differences in the nature and intensity of the given trauma. It was suggested that the disparity may be the result of situational characteristics, as traumatic events in a military setting generally occur within a more prolonged combat situation, whereas civilian traumas tend to be more sudden, unexpected, and surprising (Helzer et al., 1987). While in recent years, the IDF has become more aware of the need for attention and possible intervention after traumatic events, widely implementing a policy of ‘debriefing’ after operations and otherwise difficult incidents, there is no such set of routine procedures to deal with the effects of a trauma in a civilian setting in Israel. As Ben-Ya’acov et al. (2005) wrote, “trauma victims in a civilian setting often find themselves alone, with no kind of establishmental support in 47 the short range—no authority to organize, support and structure the experience in a crisis situation” (p. 7). In particular, Bituach Leumi5, the government institution responsible for attending to civilians who have been involved in a traumatic event, has been severely criticized for the inadequate system of treatment, compensation, and/or rehabilitation presently available to Israeli trauma survivors. Many studies have found, in a variety of cultures and traumatic contexts, that social support can have a mitigating impact on post-traumatic symptoms, and contrarily, that lack of social support is associated with higher degrees of PTSD (Barrett & Mizes, 1988; Hendin & Haas, 1984; Sarason, Shearing, Pierce, & Sarason, 1987; Solomon & Mikulincer, 1990; Solomon, Mikulincer, & Waysman, 1991). In light of this, then, the isolation that Israeli civilian TA survivors often feel, from both their governmental institutions and their surrounding society, can have a deep impact on their experiences, perceptions, and coping after experiencing a TA. Exacerbating this disconnect is Israel’s practice of worshipping and rallying around its war heroes, exalting them to an essentially mythical status. Israeli society memorializes its fallen soldiers with monuments, candle-lighting, a siren sounding throughout the country to mark a moment of silence, and a national Remembrance Day ceremony; there are no such rituals or government-sanctioned collective mourning processes for terror victims (Nuttman-Schwartz et al., 2002). As Ben-Yaacov et al. (2005), noted, “injuries in a civilian context generally bear no heroic aura whatsoever, but are considered fruitless and are associated with chance, bad luck, fate and the like, and not with values, a contribution to society or personal challenge” (p. 7). Many of the bus drivers interviewed for this study also seemed to exist in a ‘grey area’ between civilian and soldier, referring to themselves as ‘soldiers out of uniform’ and as ‘captains’ of their bus domains, but nonetheless aware that they were perceived by their society as nothing more than bus drivers. Thus, they found themselves caught in a psychological dilemma, unsure of whether they were ‘worthy’ of a “heroic aura” or destined to be seen as “fruitless” and without value to their society. Thus, in many ways, a powerful disconnect exists within Israeli society in this case— between military trauma and civilian trauma, between war hero and trauma victim, and between the expectations in place of the individuals existing on both sides of this chasm. Nuttman-Schwartz et al. (2002), in their study of group therapy conducted with Israeli individuals who had been injured in TAs and the difficulties involved in their processes of working through their experiences, found a stark dichotomy set up by the group members between terror survivors and those wounded in military contexts, noting that the participants “expressed a great deal of envy and anger at persons who had been disabled during their army service, and enjoyed greater prestige than other disabled citizens because their disability was recognized as a sacrifice for the nation” (p. 54). It is indeed the case that Israeli army veterans are entitled to a number of highly desirable and special benefits 5 The Israeli National Insurance Institute (NII), which provides basic, compulsory universal health care to all citizens. 48 that are not offered to individuals wounded or affected by terror. Furthermore, while injured soldiers are placed under the treatment and responsibility of the Ministry of Defense, individuals who underwent a TA receive their compensation and care through the NII, well-known as the institution responsible for injuries sustained in work or traffic accidents. The group members in Nuttman-Schwartz et al.’s (2002) study expressed great dissatisfaction and fury toward their society and government for this perceived discrimination and hypocritical distinction. Within Israeli society, different standards exist also for coping after stressful experiences in both spheres, as well as for societal treatment and acceptance of them. ‘Military trauma’ is viewed as nearly universal—a rite of passage of sorts—and so there is a degree of belongingness involved among Israeli men who have fought in wars, particularly those who were in combat units. ‘Civilian trauma,’ on the other hand, is not at all a common experience, despite the amount of chronic stress and tension across the Israeli population. In fact, many of the participants in the present study expressed a sense of isolation, loneliness, and marginalization as a result of their TA experiences and the relative lack of others who experienced similar things. Nuttman-Schwartz et al. (2002) found comparable sentiments in their study, noting that many of the participants “emphasized their difference from the rest of Israeli society, stating and restating the feeling that ‘nobody understands us,’ that ‘only those injured like us can understand,’” while also expressing “strong feelings of impotence, helplessness, and envy of the unscathed” (p. 53). In many ways, the authors suggested, the difficulties in coping and meaning-making that emerged within this small group process may reflect the difficulties faced by Israeli society at large in its own psychosocial process of working through the terror, protracted conflict, and frequent wars that have plagued it over the years (Nuttman-Schwartz et al., 2002). 2.4.2 Israeli societal pressure: The hegemonic cultural discourse of ‘The Israeli Warrior’ The State of Israel was established, in May of 1948, essentially due to and out of the shadow of the Holocaust, and this played a significant part in shaping the identity and mentality of the new society and its members (Bar-On, 1995; Ramon, 2004; Segev, 1991), nearly a third of whom were Holocaust survivors themselves (Klar, Schori-Eyal, & Klar, 2013; Yablonka, 1999). The Holocaust has thus constituted an omnipresent memory, image, force, political myth, and guiding discourse and principle from the early days of Israeli statehood up until today (Liebman & Don-Yihya, 1983), invoking four driving prescriptions to the Israeli populace: “1) never be a passive victim; 2) never forsake your brothers; 3) never be [a] passive bystander; and 4) never be a perpetrator” (Klar et al., 2013, p. 126). The emphasis on Israel as representative of the rebirth and redemption of the Jewish people, and as compensation for the Holocaust, created a powerful dichotomy between ‘the new Jew’ and those who purportedly went submissively to their death ‘like 49 sheep to the slaughter’ (Klar et al., 2013). This dichotomy placed a tremendous amount of pressure on Israel’s ‘new Jews’ to behave actively rather than passively, and to be heroic rather than fearful, weak, and emotionally affected. Much of this societal pressure and these behavioral prescriptions remain today, and have already shaped generations of Israelis—particularly, Israeli men. In addition to the influence of the Holocaust on the fledgling state’s societal development, Israel’s status as a country of immigrants from all over the world, with a highly heterogeneous population (Bleich et al., 2003; Solomon et al., 2005), had an equally fundamental impact on the cultural discourses and myths that were created. The post-Holocaust and pre-State ‘machine’ expended great efforts to build a system of cultural myths that could serve to bind these highly disparate sub-populations together. As Andersson (2008), following Miller (2002), claimed, “Myths direct how people live their lives at an unconscious level and are related to culturally shared assumptions” (p. 145)—thus guiding their self-understandings, meaning-making processes, and symbolic and values systems. Furthermore, following Kiesling (2005), cultural discourses are defined as “widely shared ‘background’ assumptions or ‘truths’ about how the world works” (Andersson, 2008, p. 140). While they may or may not actually be ‘true,’ the concept of these discourses relates to the hegemonic dominance of the belief that they are true. That is, whether or not all ‘normal’ Israeli men act heroically and cope resiliently is irrelevant, in light of the widelyheld assumption that they do, and, perhaps even more importantly, the societal belief and expectation that they should. In an effort to present a picture (albeit partial, at best) of the psychosocial context in which the participants of the present study were brought up and currently exist, this section provides a brief discussion of the hegemonic and quasi-mythological cultural discourses on masculinity, machismo, power, and heroism in modern Israeli society. The dominant Israeli ethos of this time focused on pioneering spirit, collective resilience, and great individual sacrifice for the common good (BenEzer, 2002; Paine, 1993). These central themes of Jewish identity, suffering, and bravery and inner strength have persisted over the generations, which, as BenEzer (1999) contended, constitute primary dimensions of self-concept, in addition to corresponding to “the three main aspects and myths prevalent in Israeli society during the 1980s” (p. 31; 2002). A particularly telling indication of the cultural and social pressures at work herein lies in the following refrain, which was written in 1943 by Miriam Yallan-Shtekelis as a poem and quickly became a popular children’s song: Dani is a Heroi Mommy told me: Dani / My wise and heroic son / My son who will never cry / Like a foolish child I don’t cry. Ever. / I’m not a crybaby. / Then why, Mommy, why / Do the tears cry by themselves? It’s only the tears…the tears…they / Cry by themselves. This song was and still is widely beloved by Israelis across social and cultural strata, and was put into the educational programs of kindergartens and schools even prior to the declaration of 50 statehood (Tobin, personal communication). It remains popular and ingrained in the culture to this day. Perhaps even more significant is an alternate version for the final verse: “She went to play with a boy – With another boy.” That is, Dani’s mother chose to go spend time with another boy, who was presumably less foolish, less of a crybaby, and more heroic than her son. This poem represents a widely believed and deeply rooted discourse in Israeli society: that real men don’t cry; that men who do cry are weak and foolish; and that, although the tears may “cry by themselves,” Israeli men must be strong, powerful, courageous, and heroic. Put even more plainly, Israeli men are expected to suppress and/or disown their emotions, and to be heroes against all odds. Whitehead (2005) suggested that this discourse is even more pervasive, claiming that ‘hero’ is a symbol of manhood itself, and that masculinity can be defined, at its very essence, as heroism. Idalovichi (2004) referred to Israel as existing in “an extended period of wartime,” a situation that has brought with it “crises in identity and values” since before its inception, and “constant uneasiness in Israeli society” (p. 621). Indeed, with such high societal expectations of courageousness and heroism, the frequent wars and military operations have created a stressful and challenging daily reality. Idalovichi (2004) also suggested the existence of a modern Israeli phenomenon of an “addiction to power,” which “appears on centre stage in terms of both public life and the Israeli psyche and resulted in the rise of the concept of power in the Israeli ethos,” presumably making “the use of military power unavoidable” (p. 619). What is often seen as a “culture of power” exists on the national, societal, and individual levels, cutting across strata and backgrounds (Golani, 2003; Nimni, 2003; Idalovichi, 2004). There is also a ‘trickle-down’ phenomenon, whereby the Israeli leadership’s often-strong preference for the use of force to ‘resolve’ conflicts just as often shows itself within the beliefs and behaviors of private individuals. Strength (both physical and emotional), power, bravery, and resilience have become the dominant expectations and discourse in and about Israel and Israelis. Alongside of this, as Nuttman-Schwartz et al. (2002) discussed, Israeli society has been referred to as one that “sees itself as a victim of terror, fighting for its existence” (p. 51). Terror in Israel has been exalted to the status of a “chosen trauma” (Volkan, 1997), used collectively and iconically to evoke previous disasters (such as the Holocaust) or myriad existential threats, bringing up all the emotional import and collective psychic wounds from generations of pain and trauma (Nuttman-Schwartz et al., 2002). This type of shared trauma, which has been described as historical trauma by Denham (2008), is generally never fully worked through and thus, passed on through the generations. Thus, Israeli society can be seen as caught in a dilemma between seeing itself as a victim or a survivor; war-weary and wounded or a fighter and a hero. In this sense, many Israeli men, such as those interviewed for this study, may view ‘fighting terror’ (and ‘winning’) as the ultimate heroism, compensating for generations of collective and historical trauma. 51 Perhaps as a result of these cultural discourses and psychosocial processes, Idalovichi (2004) noted, Israeli culture is a military culture, pointing to a “gradual process in which segments of Israeli society are adopting more and more signs of militarism,” and an influence of the culture of power on “the development of Israeli society’s system of norms and values as well as its human infrastructure” (p. 625). In Israel, most citizens must, by law, serve a three-year term in the army from the age of 18 years; military service is a deeply ingrained facet of society, and Israeli men often define themselves and others by their type of service, unit, and army experiences. As Kaplan (2003) asserted, “The IDF is more than just the major state organization. standpoint, it is the state of Israel” (p. 113). From a cultural Military culture is often seen as essentially synonymous with ‘Israeli-ness,’ and the hegemonic model of masculinity is particularly dominant in Israeli culture because of the central role and presence of the military (Kaplan, 2003). As Lomsky-Feder and Rapoport (2003) noted, in societies “which epitomize the commanding and rigid nature of the masculinity regime, hegemonic masculinity is firmly established and thoroughly institutionalized even more than in others” (p. 114). Perhaps the most significant and influential discourse and set of expectations aimed at Jewish men within Israeli society is the concept of The Israeli Warrior. Lomsky-Feder and Rapoport (2003) explicated this term and its historical origins and meanings thusly: “Soldiering and masculinity are fused in constituting the ideal of manhood (Arkin and Dobrofsky 1978; Morgan 1994; Mosse 1996). In Israeli society, the convergence between them is particularly strong and shapes the hegemonic model of masculinity—‘The Israeli Warrior.’ It is this model that normalizes the meaning of masculinity for the entire society by defining its standards and proper manifestations. The ideal of the warrior is deeply rooted in the Zionist revolution. Zionist ideology that negates the Diaspora and the ‘exilic Jew’ aims to create, instead, a ‘new Jew’ in the land of Israel, distinct from the old in both body and spirit. While the new Jew is characterized as strong, bound to the soil, tough, devoted to his nation, and committed to defend the land, the ‘exilic Jew’ is characterized as effeminate, weak, and rootless (Boyarin 1996; Gilman 1991). The warrior is both the symbol of the new nation and the means of fulfilling the revolution (Almog 1997; Biale 1992). Such a man ideally possesses a disciplined body, emotional control, and a willingness to forsake personal fulfillment and to take risks (Ben-Ari 1998; Gal 1986). The warrior model that governs Israeli masculinity, both inside and outside the army, ranks groups of men in accordance with its precepts and establishes male hierarchy” (p. 115). Much of this ethos is present in the discourse of the bus drivers interviewed for this study, particularly in the manners in which they speak about their army service and behavior under pressure. While military service is mandatory for Jewish Israelis, Ben-Ya’acov, Amir, Arzy, and Kotler (2005) asserted, “Service in the IDF is perceived by the Israeli public as an individual obligation and a privilege—defending the borders of one’s country. Soldiers, especially combat troops, are believed to be imbued with a sense of deliverance” (p. 7). In general, Israelis who served in combat positions declare this proudly, while those who did not often offer profuse explanations and justifications as to why or how they ‘almost made it’ or ‘should have’ been in a different unit. Wetherell and Edley (1999) related to this as part of a process of negotiating a heroic position—that is, providing a particular narrative production of one’s self whereby those 52 parts that are most consistent with images of masculinity, machismo, bravery, strength, and heroism are actively included and emphasized. The Zionist ethos that the new Israeli man is a ‘superman’ is such an ingrained part of the belief system and collective identity that it has continued to permeate subsequent generations (BenYa’acov et al., 2005; Solomon, 1993). Modern Israeli soldiers tend to idealize themselves and their peers as “immune, invulnerable and free from weaknesses” (Ben-Yaacov et al., 2005, p. 7), and there is an essentially ‘unwritten rule’ that anything less would be considered unacceptable and even threatening to the national identity. The bus drivers interviewed for this study grew up and continue to exist in this sociocultural reality, whereby a premium is placed on selfless heroism and courage and strength ‘no matter what.’ Idalovichi (2004), following Golani (2003) and Nimni (2003), referred to a likening in modern Israeli society of the war ethos as “almost the only Zionist ethos,” and a perception of heroism in combat as “the ultimate heroism” (p. 623). Indeed, as Lomsky-Feder and Rapoport (2003), following Maisels, Gal, and Pishof (1989), noted, “being a warrior is still viewed as a sine qua non for Israeliness, and combat service is still considered superior to any other” (p. 116). In this sense, Israeli men who served in combat positions sense strict expectations to hold up to superior standards of courageousness and heroism, while those who served in non-combat positions may already feel an ingrained sense of inadequacy and need to prove their ‘worth’ to their society. Before the TA even occurred, therefore, these bus drivers may have been ‘set up’ to behave and to perceive their behavior in a certain way. These perceptions expose themselves clearly in their narrative discourse. Solomon (1993) also related to a powerful culturally-influenced psychological denial process, whereby weakness and vulnerability are denied or disowned as having no place in the Israeli cultural belief system. Thus, much of the discourse of the participants of this study should be interpreted as having been shaped and influenced by the cultural discourse surrounding them, pushing and pressuring them to act heroically, react courageously, and disown any ‘less than acceptable’ emotions. Israel and its society indeed exist within a complex and emotionally charged set of psychosocial contexts and discourses. With its nationhood having been built upon the Holocaust and its victims and survivors, Israel’s very reason for being and national purpose is intricately related to a collective and historical trauma of unprecedented, and personal, proportions. The protracted sense of wartime and conflict, as well as the constant sense of existential threat since its establishment, has created a national identity as both a victim and a fighter, and has engendered a society of soldiers, warriors, and brave heroes (and those who aspire to be so). The shared experience of terror, particularly throughout the Intifadas, has become a “chosen trauma,” bringing up even more visceral reactions, reopening wounds created generations ago and evoking traumas experienced by the Jewish people since biblical times. In addition, Israelis have come to feel a 53 sense of community, fellowship, shared identity, and even spiritual kinship as a result of their shared communal trauma (Erikson, 1994). It is in this manner that terror in Israel has come to be “part of a collective consciousness,” essentially “institutionalized” on a national level (Coetzee & Rau, 2009, p. 3), and for this reason that the psychosocial discourse surrounding it feels imposed on all, by all. Israel’s and Israeli Jews’ existential struggle for survival as a nation and as individuals, against the backdrop of the ‘old Jew’’s essential failure to survive in Europe, has thus led to tremendous pressure and a fundamentally unattainable set of expectations placed upon modern Israeli Jewish men. They believe that they must fight (on the front lines), win against all odds, be level-headed and proactive under pressure, be brave and heroic, compensate for past losses, vanquish future enemies, and do all this without becoming traumatized or showing weakness, passivity, or emotional vulnerability. All are judged according to these criteria, and everyone surrounding them is a potential jury member. The impossibility of fully living up to these standards is obvious to anyone outside of the Israeli cultural discourse, but just as vehemently denied among those within it. The Israeli Warrior is, therefore, as deeply believed in as it is mythical, and thus, has a powerful hold on those who live their lives in its shadow and attempting to live up to its prescription. The bus drivers interviewed in this study have been and continue to be profoundly affected by these psychosocial forces and cultural discourses, and their own narrative discourse must be viewed from within this lens. 2.5 THE MICRO-LEVEL PSYCHOSOCIAL CONTEXT OF THE RESEARCH STUDY This section deals with the immediate context surrounding these bus drivers at the time of their terror experiences: their workplace. The historical, demographic, and phenomenological culture of Egged Bus Company is discussed, both in general and with regard to the day-to-day workings therein. The experience of working as a bus driver during a period of high stress and terror threat, as well as the psychological reactions of the drivers, is described, from the perspective of the managerial and professional staff at Egged. The fundamental dilemma of the Egged bus driver is also delineated, as perhaps the most critical influence on the attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors of the study participants. All of this attempts to shine light on the unique personal, interpersonal, professional, and societal context within which these individuals existed at the time of the TA they experienced, and at the moment in which they narrated these experiences to me. 2.5.1 Egged Bus Company: History, demographics, and the psychosocial context6 6 Because little has been written or theorized about this specific population in Israel, much of this discussion was gleaned from statements from a number of key personnel (both managerial and social service-oriented) within the Egged organization, with whom I conducted multiple ‘fact-finding’ interviews, as well as from the interviewed bus drivers themselves. The primary sources were Yael Dover, the managing psychologist and organizational consultant for the Jerusalem region; Yael Ayalon, the social worker for the Jerusalem region; Pini Rosenberg, the managing 54 2.5.1.1 History of the company and its culture Egged Bus Company is the national bus carrier and largest transportation service in Israel, and by far the most well-known and prolific. While there is a very small number of other intracity and intercity carriers, Egged is the only company whose bus lines traverse the entire country. The term, Egged, is thus essentially synonymous with bus travel in Israel. The company was established as a cooperative in 1933, as the product of a merger between four separate public transportation companies, and subsequently began a process of privatization in 2005. Today, Egged operates 55% of Israel’s public transportation service lines, and employs approximately 4,000 drivers nationally, in three central service areas: around 1,500 in Jerusalem; 1,000 in the northern part of the country; and 1,500 in the southern region. Each geographical partition has its own collection of units, or branches. Jerusalem, for instance, has six branches, with about 250 drivers belonging to each one. Each unit operates semi-independently, with its own scheduling process, cultural activities, new driver education, and network of interrelationships between management and workers. The drivers interviewed, as well as the managerial staff of Egged, referred to a rather rich ‘cultural life’ and a sense of community and family within the branches. According to the company’s website, Egged provides service through 2,893 regular bus lines, 58 bulletproof bus lines (which primarily service the more dangerous areas of the country, such as those that approach or go over the Green Line), 25 minibus lines, and 3,507 alternative bus lines. Approximately 720,073 kilometers are travelled every day within the confines of 25,267 daily service line trips, and approximately 960,000 passengers are served per day. The official policy has always been that Egged drivers are not allowed to work more than twelve consecutive hours per day, and in recent years this law has been enforced more strictly by both the police and Egged management and organizational staff. From a popular perspective, in the 1960s and 1970s (when many of the participants in this study were in their formative years), being a member of Egged was viewed by Israelis as a symbol of prestige and social value. The work was considered safe and secure, and the salary was sufficient. Many of the drivers interviewed herein referred to a sense of pride, influence, respect, and honor associated with being an Egged driver, and a perception of a widespread sentiment in society at that time that ‘any girl would be lucky to marry an Egged man.’ Ayalon and Dover confirmed that Israeli mothers from the middle-class and blue-collar socioeconomic status level at this time would often tell their daughters to marry an Egged driver, and young sons of drivers grew up wanting to follow in their fathers’ footsteps. The social status attached to being an Egged driver has steadily declined since the late 1970s and early 1980s, primarily as a result of growing social worker for the southern region; and Yigal Shlomof, an Egged member, retired bus driver, and manager of the medical department for the Jerusalem region. Multiple drafts of this entire section were submitted to and discussed with Yael Dover to ensure full accuracy, and this version reflects all edits and clarifications requested by her. 55 dissatisfaction with blue-collar jobs and an increasing interest in more academic pursuits across the social classes in Israel. The shift in Egged’s policy, from employing mostly cooperative members to taking on more contract workers, has also played a central role in the ‘fall from glory’ of both the company and the profession as a whole. Today, many of the interviewed drivers seemed fairly dissatisfied with their jobs, citing difficult working conditions, troublesome passengers, unsatisfactory pay rates, and high levels of daily stress stemming from what they view as unrealistic expectations on the managerial level (particularly related to timetables and traffic). Ayalon asserted that the population of drivers at Egged today can be categorized into three groups. The first is comprised of men of the ages of 35-45 who did, indeed, dream of working in the same profession as their fathers, and who entered the organization from within a type of ‘family legacy’ mentality. Some of these drivers are the third generation in their families to be employed at Egged, and are generally proud of this fact. They have, however, witnessed and felt a decrease in their social standing in recent years, and voice their dissatisfaction about this. The second group contains individuals whose socioeconomic status and education level brought them into the profession, and who remain out of a sense that it is the “best job they can get.” The final group is made up of drivers who simply love the work and the people they meet in the process. These are the drivers who tend to talk most about feeling “like a family,” and Ayalon contended that a worker from this group, who truly loves being a bus driver, is immediately identifiable in every way. Egged is still a cooperative company, but the beginning of the process of privatization in 2001 was a major event in the company’s history, creating a rather dramatic transformation in company culture, discourse, policy, and operative behavior. In 2001, the strategic program was introduced, whereby it was decided that the company would shift from an operational to a business model, within which there would be a greater focus within the entire organization on service and profitability. The process commenced on the managerial and administrative levels, and the effects of the program began to be felt palpably by the drivers only around 2005. The government is still deeply involved, frequently performing checks of the quality of service and overseeing efficiency levels, precision of service, and accessibility, while attending to any customer complaints received. Many of the drivers in this study mentioned this as a defining moment in their own employment timeline, although the centrality of the topic in their discourse may have been impacted by the fact that the process was at its height during the exact period of time wherein most of the initial interviews were conducted. Most discussed the process as difficult and many expressed bitterness about the effects on their own jobs. Whereas in the 1960s and 1970s, the majority of drivers were members and thus held stock and power within the company, now only approximately 40% of Egged drivers overall hold the status of cooperative members, while 60% are simply contract employees. By 2001, only approximately a third of drivers in the Jerusalem branch 56 were members, and today this stands at only a fifth. Furthermore, of the approximately 350 Jerusalem members, 150 of them hold managerial or administrative positions. Dover mentioned that in the early 2000s, many of the drivers seemed to be more concerned and fearful about their job stability than about the possibility of a TA, and had been feeling progressively more stress and uncertainty in the face of the large-scale organizational and “deep cultural” changes occurring throughout the company. As a result of this process, the standards and demands placed on the drivers became increasingly strict and rigid, particularly vis-à-vis levels of service provided and reduction in wastefulness across the board. Thus, these drivers were often facing high expectations from their workplace as well as their society. 2.5.1.2 Demographics and terror-related statistics Given that the population under study here is comprised predominantly of male, Jewish, native-born Israelis, it cannot be considered generally representative of Israeli society at large. The population of interviewees is fairly representative of the general population of Israeli bus drivers, however, in the categories of gender, age, marital status, cultural background, and army service. Although exact statistics are not available, it is estimated that of the approximately 4,000 drivers employed by Egged, over 90% are male and approximately 75% are Jewish. About 30% of the drivers in Jerusalem are Arab, but the percentage is lower in the other two service areas. Up until recently, there were no female drivers at all, but as a result of a new program aimed at encouraging their recruitment, there are now twelve women working as drivers in Jerusalem, with a similar number currently in training. However, the numbers of female drivers remain quite low nationally. While historically, the overriding culture of Egged has been decidedly masculine, in recent years changes have been steadily occurring, with a shift in image and an organizing motto focused more on passenger service and less on any characteristics of the drivers themselves. The majority of bus drivers employed by Egged are married, with children. The ages of the drivers range from 23 to 70, with a fairly even distribution across the range. Members of Egged, if they have accumulated 35 years of seniority, are permitted to retire at the age of 57; although many members choose to continue working as part-time or contract drivers after their ‘retirement.’ Contract workers are not allowed to retire until the age of 67. While official statistics on the drivers’ countries of origin were not attainable, Dover noted that the general population of Egged drivers can be considered a microcosm of any blue-collar profession in Israel. That is, the population is comprised of a large number of Russians (and in particular, many new immigrants from the former Soviet Union), some Ethiopians, and a small number of religious Jews. Essentially all Jewish Egged drivers have served a compulsory three-year period of military service, in addition 57 to an average of 20-30 days per year in reserve duty7, and fought in multiple wars and/or served in the territories over the course of their duty. While most of the interviewees served in combat units, it is not known whether this is representative of the larger population of Egged drivers. Bus drivers in Israel have existed on the ‘front lines’ of both Intifadas. According to data from the Israel Ministry of Foreign Affairs, and as viewed in Figure 2-1, 58 (35%) of a total of 165 suicide and other bombing attacks between 1994 and 2007 were on or directly involved buses, as opposed to 21 (13%) having occurred in restaurants and cafés. This is a significant percentage, which seems to be reflected in the public opinion in Israel, as well as throughout the world. 8 The bus drivers interviewed herein were acutely aware that buses seem to be associated in the public view with bombings, and that this association may be irreparable, as reflected in the fact that even the nearly complete decline in bus bombings since 2005 has not managed to obliterate this public perception (which has not necessarily impacted the tendency of the Israeli public to ride buses). It was not possible to obtain exact official statistics on the incidence of TAs in relation to Egged and its drivers, but some estimates were ascertained. Over the course of Egged’s history, there have been approximately 40 suicide bombings involving or very proximate to Egged buses, with twelve having actually occurred on an Egged bus. Only one Egged bus driver has died in a TA, although a number have been physically wounded. Dover claimed that the majority of Egged bus drivers who experienced serious TAs, whom she treated during her tenure as a psychologist at Egged (2001-2012), were not able to continue working as drivers. During the Second Intifada in particular, as Dover remembered, of three Egged drivers who experienced a suicide TA on their bus, one died, one managed to escape the attack and was able to work despite being significantly traumatized, and one experienced the entire attack and was not able to return to work as a driver. Ayalon also reported that most of the drivers who were physically wounded in TAs were not able to return to work as drivers, and one whose wounds were primarily emotional and psychological also did not return. It was estimated that, between 1986-2005, approximately 20 Egged drivers in total stopped working as drivers as a result of experiencing serious TAs. Dover cited as the primary reason for this traumatic psychological effects that made it too difficult for them to spend a long (or, in some cases, any) period of time on a bus. According to Dover, however, only one of the drivers in the Jerusalem region who did not return to work was diagnosed with full-blown PTSD. When the ‘net’ is widened to include all types and severities of TAs, however, it appears 7 8 Israeli Jewish men are generally required to serve in IDF reserve duty yearly until the age of 50-52. In just a brief scan of the online headlines nearly two years since the last prior bus bombing in Israel, I came across the following from an article on a popular American news site: “Between 2001 and 2004, at the height of PalestinianIsraeli fighting, Jerusalem was a frequent target of Palestinian attacks, including suicide bombings on buses” (Associated Press. Israel signals it’ll talk despite seminary attack [Electronic version]. Retrieved in March of 2008, from http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/23515835/). Similarly, in a lecture I attended in February of 2008, an accomplished trauma researcher made a passing reference to “that exploding box which in Israel we call a bus.” 58 that the majority of Egged bus drivers who experienced an attack (including rocks or a Molotov cocktail thrown at their bus) have continued to work as drivers, and have not evidenced a great degree of trauma or psychological effects due to the event(s). Indeed, Dover reported that, over the course of her tenure at Egged, she met with every driver who had experienced any type of terrorrelated incident—approximately 50-60 drivers in total. While five were diagnosed with full PTSD and while several exhibited some symptoms (some that were initiated by the TA, and others that were brought on by a related re-experiencing of previous traumas), only a small fraction of these drivers stopped working for any period of time. All underwent a process of treatment and rehabilitation, the length of which varied from a single session with Dover to years of therapy. 2.5.1.3 The psychosocial context surrounding and within Egged According to its website, Egged holds five central objectives with regard to its operations and service: “1) focus on the customer needs and improve service; 2) stress safety and professionalism of drivers; 3) keep company profitable on the competitive market; 4) take steps of business development through diverse revenue sources; and 5) preserve the environment.” While all these goals are important and necessary for the overall profitability of the company, the policies and workings of the organization become far more complex and difficult to navigate during hightension and conflict periods such as the Intifadas. Indeed, the concepts of “stress[ing] safety…of the drivers” and “focus[ing] on the customer needs” take on entirely new meanings under such conditions. In order to be employed by Egged, candidates undergo a significant amount and degree of evaluation and assessment, and then participate in an intensive driving instruction program. Psychometric testing is also conducted to insure that a minimum requirement has been met for psychological stability. However, the stress, fear, and anxiety involved in driving a bus during an Intifada, when buses seem to be exploding daily all around the country, can create a challenging set of circumstances for even the most seasoned driver. Egged employs a psychologist and social worker, as well as an organizational physician, for each region. The psychologist, in particular, is responsible for providing a variety of services to drivers who have been through difficult experiences—including, but not limited to, TAs—as well as organizational consulting and managing the various counselors that work in the field. Because the NII cannot always be counted on to provide what the drivers view as appropriate, sufficient, or timely care, Egged has invested much energy, particularly during the height of the Intifadas, into helping all drivers who have been affected by terror and/or other stressful events while on the job. Indeed, according to Shlomof, Egged prides itself on being “part of everything”—that is, helping with all parts of their drivers’ lives if necessary, including injuries sustained while off-duty, family illnesses, and any personal issues that may affect their work. Whereas many other organizations in 59 Israel have been criticized for their policies regarding first response to TAs and their effects on their staff, Egged stands out in its willingness to help its workers and investment in doing so. Typically, after a significant TA or any other potentially traumatic event (e.g., a fatal or near-fatal traffic accident), the organizational policy is to send a medical team manager and social worker to the hospital to visit and assess the driver, while the psychologist and organizational consultant team seek out the other drivers in the affected unit—particularly those who drive on the line involved in the incident. At this point, drivers exhibiting any post-traumatic symptoms are encouraged to talk and allowed to take time off from work if they so request. Great effort has been made by the organizational psychological services in the past decade to facilitate open discourse and legitimize the drivers’ feelings, and significant progress has occurred in this domain, with many of the younger drivers showing much more willingness to speak openly and freely than their older counterparts. However, ‘peer pressure’ within the unit and society at large may still serve to undermine these efforts and create a sense of obligation to behave as the others are behaving, including continuing to work under what may be tremendous stress and anxiety. If the affected driver was not hospitalized, Egged’s official policy is that he is required to have at least one conversation with the organization’s psychologist, either by phone or face-to-face. In this sense, it is assured that there is some basic level of follow-up for every (even theoretically minor) stressful event. Wives of drivers are also welcome to see the psychologist, and when necessary, therapeutic activities are conducted with the entire family unit. There are a variety of organizational mechanisms in place to facilitate the driver’s coping process, and each case is evaluated on its own basis, with treatment and recommendations tailored individually to each set of circumstances. The decision to allow or encourage a driver to return to work on a bus after such an event is dependent on a variety of factors, such as willingness and desire, physical and emotional capability, and particular conditions such as the specific bus line, geographical area, and level of stress involved (e.g., peak traffic hours or area that has been hit by terror repeatedly in the past). Dover asserted that for the few days directly following a TA, there is a patterned significant increase in talk about and concern with the attack, its aftereffects, and the larger context of the security situation in Israel, followed by an immediate significant decrease, whereby the drivers return to their ‘normal’ routines, conversations, and behaviors.9 Processing and social sharing takes 9 This is in line with several studies of social sharing after a traumatic event, which found that affected individuals talked about the event and its effects on them at significantly elevated rates throughout the first two weeks following it, after which their rates of talking dropped to nearly zero. As Gortner and Pennebaker (2003) asserted, a trauma that affects an entire community can produce experiences of shock and grief in the entire collective. Thus, after most communal traumas, such as natural disasters or TAs, the immediate reaction is that community members tend to ‘band together’ to share and talk about the experience (Rimé, Mesquita, Philippot, & Boca, 1991). Gortner and Pennebaker (2003) contended that this social sharing permits a process of “construct[ing] a collective script of the disaster, highlighting aspects that will facilitate the processing of the trauma” (p. 581). Notably, during such periods of upheaval and communal stress, they write, “social norms that prohibit talking with strangers in public places are 60 place within the Egged branches through these mechanisms, and these populations of drivers can be seen as communities within themselves. While the drivers in any given unit are not at all strangers to each other, the norms seem to change and the discourse shifts, allowing a discussion of the event and the politics surrounding it (if not a deep discussion of their attendant emotions). In this communal spirit, the other drivers in the affected unit tend to be highly supportive of the driver who experienced the TA, particularly if he has returned to work. (If he does not return, he will likely be much more in contact with the treating staff of Egged and fall out of touch with his fellow drivers.) This is a relatively recent development, however, and the result of a great deal of work on the part of Dover, who has labored to build a support mechanism within the company to deal with these types of events and resultant issues. As part of this, group activities aimed toward coping and stress relief have been conducted over the years at various units, and a variety of interventions were instituted for acute stress events. Before this, and even as late as the early 2000s, there was no company-wide structure in place to deal with the occurrence of potentially traumatic experiences within the context of the drivers’ work, and there was, according to Dover, an overarching culture of silence, loneliness, and powerlessness in the face of these experiences. Dover contended that at the managerial level of Egged, there is a general perception that the drivers are strong and have coped well with stress. Rosenberg echoed this, stating that he sees resilience in the daily routines and reactions of the drivers in his charge, infused in “everything they do.” While this may be true, there was also a degree of denial at this level, with many managers initially showing significant resistance to Dover’s attempts to facilitate more open discussion of topics of fear, stress, and trauma. Some even told her not to “put fears into the drivers’ heads,” and suggested that encouraging them to talk may actually reinforce a range of non-adaptive behaviors, which may, in turn, damage the productivity of the company as a whole. Fortunately, much has changed within Egged over the past decade, and upper management now fully believes in the process that was undertaken (and the need for it), whereby an official organizational mechanism altered and a natural, interpersonal coping process emerges” (Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003, p. 581). Pennebaker and Harber (1993) thus developed a social stage model of coping, comprised of three stages. In the first stage, the emergency phase, affected individuals think and talk about the experience they went through at very high levels, experiencing obsessive thoughts and much emotional upheaval. They may discuss what they are going through with anyone who will listen, and do so with excessive openness. In the second stage, the inhibition phase, which occurs approximately two weeks after the event, individuals still think about what they went through but demonstrate a significant decrease in their outright talking about it. This phase may coincide with the emergence of various physical and/or social problems, such as nightmares, interpersonal conflicts, or other behavioral difficulties. The third and final stage, the adaptation phase, generally appears between six and twelve weeks following the event, and brings a basic adjustment to the situation, with little to no increased rumination about the experience and no increased talking about it (Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003; Pennebaker & Harber, 1993; Stone & Pennebaker, 2002). Gortner and Pennebaker (2003) found that these dramatic decreases in social sharing were “not gradual nor linear”—rather, people went from talking about these traumatic, affecting events multiple times per day to not discussing them nearly at all. In many ways, this pattern is reflected in the Israeli case, although the stages seem to follow a much more rapid trajectory. Perhaps this is a result of the frequency of occurrence of TAs—Israelis may simply not have six to twelve weeks to ‘spare’ in order to fully process these events, before the next one occurs. 61 was established and adopted for coping with TAs and other traumas and their aftermath. 2.5.2 Being an Egged bus driver in Israel: The personal context 2.5.2.1 The phenomenological context and experience of Egged drivers Perhaps the most essential component in the immediate psychosocial context surrounding Egged drivers, and that which seems to have the greatest impact on their perceptions, beliefs, and behaviors, is their individual branch. Rosenberg reported that there is, in fact, a great deal of group cohesion and sense of togetherness, partnership, and belongingness within the units. Shlomof agreed, contending that the drivers see themselves as part of a family, and close relationships are often created, with many attending each other’s weddings and other special occasion celebrations. Much of these dynamics may come from a military influence, as the drivers are used to this sense and experience of being part of a unit, relying on each other, and existing communally under difficult and stressful conditions. This collectivity may also have a significant influence on the drivers’ perceptions of what is expected of them in these stressful periods, increasing the sense and impact of a type of social peer pressure to behave courageously and resiliently. Indeed, Ayalon and Shlomof claimed that this pressure is subtle but very much present across the branches. Many drivers express, for instance, a belief in a kind of existential ‘game of chicken’—that is, “If I give up (i.e., stop driving entirely or don’t come in to work today because I’m scared), I have no right to exist in this country.” The overriding sentiment among the drivers is that they are no different from anyone else, and have no right to distinguish themselves as such. They understand they are permitted to call in sick the day after a TA, for instance, but often respond by asking, “What about the next time, and the next time, and the next time? And what if we all called in sick? Who am I to allow myself to give in to my fears, when everyone else is in ‘the same boat’?” It is, in many ways, this communal belongingness, with the attendant sense of peer pressure, that can be both a source of comfort (helping them feel less alone) and stress (making them feel a behavioral expectation that can be difficult to cope with, much less fulfill). Ayalon suggested that the drivers she has worked with are, in fact, under a tremendous degree of pressure and stress. She believes that they thought about the threat of terror and their fears very frequently during the Intifada, but that they simply could not tolerate the alternative—in their words, “sitting at home doing nothing.” On a very practical level, Ayalon claimed, the drivers also generally lack the self-confidence to look for other work, and often express a belief that they are “just simple drivers,” who are not smart enough to find a different job. Some also express a feeling that they have no choice, and that they are “stuck” in their jobs, whether they like it or not. Above all else, Ayalon pointed to a number of essential reasons in the drivers’ ultimate decision to keep working in the face of an often unbearable level of fear and stress: 1) the pressing, 62 daily need and obligation to support their wives and children; 2) the desire to keep their families from worrying about them, and to not become a burden on them; 3) the concern about how those around them might react to their decision to stop working; 4) the sense of responsibility to their (micro- and macro-level) collective, and ingrained sense of ‘survivorship’; 5) the perception that they need to prove themselves, prove their masculinity and ‘man-ness,’ prove that they are adequate, ‘acceptable’, and ‘less than’ no one, and show that they are the same as everyone else; and 6) the desire to prove (to their society, to the enemy, and perhaps to the world) that they will not surrender to terror, or ‘let the terrorists win.’ Ayalon contended that ultimately, the drivers are highly practical about these issues: they have mortgages, so they must work. Whether they were hurt or traumatized or fearful, they simply must work. In some ways, this represents a tremendous amount of pressure, but in others and for some individuals, it may actually have a ‘freeing’ effect. Indeed, if there is no choice, why dwell on the issue at all? According to Dover, in her experience and observation, the drivers do experience a significantly heightened degree of fear and anxiety for a number of weeks directly following a TA in their area. Some admit it and some don’t, and some change their behavior while others don’t. There is generally particular pressure from the wives of the drivers, who become more anxious about their husbands’ safety and more adamant that they work the bare minimum of hours and refuse extra shifts or positions. Often, the drivers’ children will ask them not to go to work, and show increased stress and anxiety about the risks involved in their fathers’ jobs. Dover stated that she has heard a variety of sentiments with regard to the drivers’ worldviews, with some showing a more realistic perspective, others more fatalistic, and still others seeming to exhibit an entirely helpless outlook. Many refer to the workings of “fate,” asserting that “whatever happens, happens,” and “life goes on, no matter what.” There is a general perception that “there just isn’t a choice”—these drivers must work, must bring income home to their families, and because of this, simply must find a way to cope with the daily stresses and fears involved in their jobs. In many ways, it can be suggested, and corroborated by Dover, that there is something profoundly masculine and Israeli about the behavior of these drivers. The fundamentally military mythological axiom of ‘carrying on no matter what’, and of ‘staying in the field’ (or ‘returning to battle’ as quickly as possible) because that is simply ‘what one does’ is strong and ingrained in the Israeli mindset and unspoken code of conduct. This set of beliefs and actions may lead to great isolation and silencing of one’s innermost emotions, but is widely practiced and rarely questioned. Dover claimed that in the immediate wake of a TA, the drivers tend to look for meaning, as well as distraction, in a variety of other domains, and Ayalon suggested that the stress “must be released somehow.” There are often many political arguments the day after a TA, with the overriding theme of placing blame on the Israeli government. Political involvement tends to 63 increase temporarily, and the drivers (as well as most Israelis) often spend much more time than usual watching televised news programs. Ayalon contended that drivers may watch sports event broadcasts in a more ‘extreme’ manner, yelling at the screen or expressing more anger at certain outcomes. Some may throw themselves more into their own sports or exercise routines, and others may show more nervous behaviors or emotional responses. Reactions are concrete and seemingly in response to concrete stimuli, with no discussion on any symbolic or emotional level. Indeed, with regard to voicing real concern and feelings such as anxiety, according to Dover, most often there is merely silence and silencing—with an unspoken ‘don’t ask, don’t tell’ policy. The drivers simply attempt to go about their days as normally as possible while avoiding the ‘elephant in the room.’ In the days immediately following a serious TA in the vicinity, there is also a significant drop in number of passengers, as the Israeli population at large is also affected by the events. So buses run ‘as usual,’ but are noticeably half- or almost completely empty, and no one says a word. Indeed, Dover discussed the role of denial within Egged as a vital part of the daily job of being a bus driver. She recalled one incident, when she was talking with a manager the day after a TA on a bus line that belonged to his branch. He told her that he had heard from at least twenty drivers who were calling in sick, and when she suggested that there might be a relationship between this and the TA, he was surprised and completely denied the possibility. While “too much denial hurts the organization,” she contended, “one needs at least some denial—as it is an essential part of their resilience.” Because many of the drivers seem to perceive that they do not have much choice in the matter of whether to continue driving, they appear to have reached a precarious balance between acceptance, fatalism, and proud resistance. Indeed, the phrases, “What can you do? Life must go on”; “Whatever is going to happen is going to happen—I have no control over it”; and “We won’t/can’t stop living” swirled through and permeated the discourse of the interviewees. Dover asserted, however, that there is a sense in the organization that the younger drivers come across as more open and willing to discuss their feelings, and seem more in touch with their fear and stress levels. In general, Dover saw a qualitative difference between those drivers who experienced TAs in the 1990s-early 2000s, and those who experienced TAs before that. Perhaps this is related to the social perceptions and cultural discourse on the Intifadas and terror, and regarding the security situation in Israel in general, or perhaps significant shifts in attitude and perspective have taken place across the board. Indeed, Dover also claimed that soldiers today are simply “not the same” as soldiers thirty years ago, particularly in their possession of much more psychological awareness and development and a greater willingness to explore and discuss their feelings. She also believes there is less machismo in the Israeli sociocultural discourse today, and more legitimacy given to talking about fear, anxiety, and emotions in general. Thirty years ago, Israeli men were simply not able or permitted to give voice to such topics, and furthermore, the 64 surrounding society was not willing to listen to such a voice. Today, however, Dover believes, Israel exists in a “more psychologically-minded era,” allowing its population to explore a much wider range of feelings and experiences. The interviewees in the present study exist on the complex and problematic boundary between these two sides of the hegemonic discourse—having spent their formative years under the shadow of an emotional silencing discourse, while now attempting to cope with their TA experiences in a more open and facilitative sociocultural context. Dover also suggested that today, in many ways, the drivers’ processes of coping are similar across the board, regardless of the particular event that caused stress. Indeed, whether a driver has experienced a TA or traffic accident, the feelings and behaviors triggered and mechanisms involved in dealing with it are often the same. In fact, Dover mentioned, she had seen a number of drivers after they had been through a traffic accident, and it slowly emerged that the root of much of their anxiety and fear was actually related to the security situation and the constant threat of terror. With regard to political beliefs, according to Dover, the majority of bus drivers employed by Egged are right-leaning, with some more moderate and only a very small number of left-leaning individuals. The population of Egged drivers is thus fairly representative of the larger Israeli society, with regard to socioeconomic status, education, and political attitudes. Dover suggested that there may be a political component involved in the discourse surrounding what is ‘good’ and ‘acceptable’ behavior in this context. An ardent resistance to changing one’s lifestyle and routines, lest ‘the terrorists win,’ is often invoked in rightist ideologies, and can engender a greater degree of denial of the actual security realities. In these cases, drivers may stubbornly refuse to avoid certain areas or routes, or to stop working even when it is financially feasible to do so. While this can have the benefit of an ideological ‘immunization’ against fear and anxiety, it may also have the consequence of encouraging unsafe and unwise decisions. Indeed, for instance, Egged’s (and the Israeli police’s) official policy requires that drivers inspect their bus thoroughly at the beginning and end of every shift for any suspicious objects. Dover recounted an anecdote from a particular month at the height of the Second Intifada, when a manager noted that Egged had received an increased number of fines from the police for infractions of this rule. The manager was concerned about and unable to understand the rise in the drivers’ lack of willingness to check their buses. Dover explained that in their denial of the security situation and fervor to ‘keep on living normally,’ the drivers actually discounted very real security threats and ultimately could have put themselves and their passengers in greater danger, due to their actually checking their buses less thoroughly (or not at all) than during less stressful periods.10 Thus, in times of greater stress and threat of terror, there actually emerges a paradox among 10 Of course, Egged took this phenomenon very seriously, and did not allow drivers or passengers to be endangered at any time. 65 the drivers, whereby they are both more alert and internally anxious but less cautious and outwardly fearful. Dover, having witnessed this phenomenon, claims that drivers under these circumstances are both afraid and in denial; hypervigilant but stubborn in their rigidity and faithfulness to their ‘normal’ routines. Indeed, even when many drivers were fined by the company for not inspecting their buses for suspicious objects, the behavior did not decrease or cease. Dover contended that this seeming act of defiance may, in fact, simply be an expression of the defense mechanism of denial, which may, in many ways, be more valuable to the drivers as a coping strategy than the loss of their wages. Thus, while there may be a high price to the drivers’ decision to defy this policy, this behavior can be considered a sign of their coping strategies, however harmful or dysfunctional. 2.5.2.2 The fundamental dilemma of the Egged driver The Intifadas were experienced primarily within and by the civilian population in Israel, with the most targeted entities being highly populated restaurants, cafés, malls, and buses. Besides conscripted soldiers, therefore, the ‘front lines’ involved three primary populations: guards, restaurant service staff, and bus drivers. security Each post is defined by its own job responsibilities, and must negotiate its own crises when confronted with a security threat. However, neither security guards nor restaurant staff operate on a daily basis in a context as complicated and problematic as that of bus drivers. Indeed, as a result of a convoluted interplay of factors, including their personal senses of identity, cultural background, military experience and mentality, worldview and value systems, and the occupational and societal expectations of them, Israeli bus drivers appear to exist in the crux of a fundamental dilemma. In Israel, (most often armed) security personnel stand guard outside stores, malls, restaurants, movie theaters, bus stations, universities, and nearly all other public establishments— charged with the singular task of protecting those who enter. They are required to inspect the personal belongings of everyone, either by physically opening backpacks and bags or by supervising their placement into an x-ray machine. They are allowed and encouraged to request identification if they deem it warranted, and to take any other measures necessary to inspect suspicious individuals. These men and women, often fresh out of the army, are presented with a clear job description and are fully aware that part of it requires them to sacrifice their lives in order to protect the public, if the need arises. Indeed, if they were to see a man walking toward them with a bomb, their duty would obligate them to jump on him or subdue him using any means necessary. These ‘means’ might likely entail losing their own lives. On the other hand, restaurant service people exist on the opposite side of the spectrum. Their singular task is to serve their clientele, with no particular regard to their safety or possible threat posed. Waiters and waitresses rarely feel any significant degree of responsibility for their 66 customers, and have no obligation to protect their patrons. Their job responsibilities do not involve anything related to (national or individual) security concerns; thus, if a suspicious individual were to somehow manage to enter a restaurant or café, the staff would likely run in the opposite direction and feel no guilt for doing so. There is no set societal expectation (spoken or otherwise) for their behavior, and rarely has a media report mentioned a “hero waiter.” Bus drivers in this context, however, exist in a near-impossible dilemma—caught between responsibilities and expectations to both serve and protect. When faced with a daily reality that involves the possibility of a potential suicide bomber boarding their bus at any moment, they must constantly heed both of these duties and be vigilant at every moment. Indeed, the bus drivers interviewed herein expressed that they often find themselves functioning in multiple roles at the same time: as driver, security guard, policeman, and army commander. Although this confluence of roles often is not necessarily disadvantageous, the separate functions sometimes contradict each other, creating a serious conflict of interests and a consequent sense of guilt and inadequacy in the drivers. That most drivers served in the army during (multiple) wars and military campaigns, and have internalized sociocultural expectations of ‘The Israeli Warrior’—particularly given the “centrality of military matters” in Israel (Lomsky-Feder & Ben-Ari, 1999)—also has a significant influence on their belief systems, behaviors, and subsequent attributions about their actions. However, Israeli bus drivers are not policemen and are neither (active) soldiers nor warriors; they do not carry weapons, and there are no clear and set rules regarding their official responsibilities under conditions and acute situations such as those that frequently emerge during an Intifada. As part of Egged policy, every driver attends at least one ninety-minute seminar, covering basic emergency systems and routines and prescribing basic manners of dealing with certain circumstances. They receive a company manual containing the pertinent information, and there is a yearly ‘refresher’ course covering these topics. And yet, while their job description specifies that they are responsible for “transporting their passengers safely and efficiently from their point of origin to their point of destination,” this duty becomes infinitely complicated (and ultimately, can be entirely compromised) during periods of high security threat. Drivers tend to feel a much greater degree of responsibility towards their jobs and their clientele than that which is ‘officially’ (or unofficially) sanctioned under normal circumstances; this sense of duty is only further heightened when the national threat of terror attacks increases. Officially, they are instructed to “use their judgment,” and are afforded a certain degree of autonomy and ex post facto legitimacy for their decisions vis-à-vis security, safety, and professional authority. Unofficially, however, many of their job responsibilities, duties, and capabilities are simply not defined, with the consequence that already permeable boundaries appear even more complex and ‘fuzzy’ during periods of violence escalation. This dynamic places the drivers into a situation that ultimately is 67 fraught with ambivalence and uncertainty, engendering doubt about their behavior (and what the ‘correct behavior’ may even be) and guilt about any range of outcomes resulting from their actions. Indeed, the drivers are expected to drive carefully and defensively, while simultaneously taking great care to keep to the timetable of their route, even during peak traffic periods (an almost impossible task in itself, and the source of much frustration). Meanwhile, they are expected to be courteous and respectful to their passengers while also keeping a constant eye out for ‘suspicious’ individuals, behavior, and objects. They have a legal right, but no obligation, to inspect any passengers or belongings that they deem suspicious. Additionally, if a dangerous situation does arise, these drivers believe that they are expected to prevent all possible tragedy, to save the lives of their clientele if need be, and to subdue or kill the perpetrator at all costs. They perceive security situations that may arise as under their purview and see themselves as responsible for initiating a solution, but also have a keen understanding of the thin line they must straddle between protecting their passengers overall while not offending some by requesting identity papers or an inspection. Part of the complexity of the dilemma lies in the fundamental nature of what it means to be a bus driver in Israel. It was clear from the interviews conducted for this study, and corroborated by Dover and Shlomof, that the identity of these individuals as drivers is a substantial and predominant part of who they are and how they see themselves. This is particularly the case with the veteran members, who have viewed themselves as ‘belonging to Egged’ for significant periods of time, sometimes even the majority of their lives. As many of these individuals began working young and financed their weddings and homes through this work, the sense of personal investment and connection is often deep, essential, and ingrained. Because they spend the majority of their workdays on ‘their’ bus, and because many of them drive on the same routes for years at a time, they come to view their bus as their ‘second home’ or as a personal possession—indeed, as an extension of their identity and their selves (Hyvärinen, personal communication). Many drivers also become familiar and even quite friendly with ‘their’ regular passengers, and refer proudly to special relationships with some—even having attended birthday celebrations and weddings. Related to their ingrained identities as drivers, many also mention a sense of personal responsibility—as if they are the ‘captain of their ship’—which stems from their perception of their bus as their ‘territory’ that must be protected against threat. Rosenberg echoed this statement, expressing that “the driver is the (army) commander of his bus.” This can translate into a high level of alertness with regard to security, as well as personal decisions to add unofficial duties that are decidedly not part of their job description. For instance, some drivers make it a point to request identity papers from all dark-skinned individuals attempting to board their bus, while others admit to purposely skipping stops at which an individual whom they perceive as suspicious is waiting. In light of all this, a primary feeling experienced by the drivers when an attack occurs is 68 guilt. In particular, the branch sadran (the driver in charge of assigning the other drivers to specific bus lines and scheduling their shifts) tends to feel the most guilt, under the perception that he was the one who placed the ‘victim’ into the exact time and spot of the attack. In general, according to Dover, drivers who experienced a TA tend to feel a great deal of guilt vis-à-vis their passengers, and constantly question what they should or could have done to keep them out of harm’s way. Many drivers seem to feel they should have acted differently; or rather, perfectly: that they should have identified the terrorist, taken him out of commission, protected every one of their passengers, and prevented any and all possible acts of terror. It is this confluence of factors that creates an exceedingly difficult situation for bus drivers in this context. The importance and fundamentality of their identities as drivers is coupled with their heightened sense of responsibility for their buses and passengers, and this interacts with a lessthan-clearly defined job description but attendant perceptions of impossibly high sociocultural expectations of their behavior. All of this, in turn, creates a powerful potential dynamic of guilt, an exaggerated sense of culpability, coping dilemmas, and a desire to achieve the elusive (but likely unattainable) ‘hero status.’ Israeli bus drivers thus present a complex and highly illuminating case for the study of the interplay between personal and societal discourse, and between the narrative and communicative ‘product,’ on the one hand, and the narrator’s inner psychological world and coping processes, on the other. 2.6 CONCLUSION This chapter has presented a comprehensive overview of the content worlds and contexts within which this study is grounded. Context and culture are particularly critical facets of this research; as Tuval-Mashiach et al. (2004) claimed, culture and meaning-making structures can have a significant impact on an individual’s narrative. I suggest that this use of the term, “culture,” should be understood on both a macro- and micro-level: the Israeli culture, the masculine culture, the military culture, the Egged culture, and the specific cultural context of familial, communal, interpersonal, and psychosocial forces at work in each individual’s life. Coetzee and Rau (2009) referred to the “intersubjectively constituted nature of memory,” arguing: “to interpret narratives of suffering one needs to take into account their socio-cultural as well as their personal dimensions. To understand trauma narratives one needs to incorporate all individual, intersubjective and collective facets that play a role in the process of remembering. These facets weave together to form our reflections on the past” (p. 16). Only in this multidimensional manner can the discourse of the bus drivers interviewed for this study be understood, as both reflective of a process of coping and narrative processing, and as a product of myriad psychosociocultural contexts that constitute significant and dynamic influences on the meanings and messages expressed. 69 CHAPTER 3 THE METHODOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE RESEARCH STUDY 3.1 INTRODUCTION This chapter addresses the methodological basis of this research study, while making a fundamental distinction between methodology, defined by Strauss and Corbin (1998) as “a way of thinking about and studying social reality,” and methods, defined as “a set of procedures and techniques for gathering and analyzing data” (p. 3). Both are presented here, while describing and honoring the multidisciplinary framework upon which the study is based. Indeed, as Guba and Lincoln (2005) stated, “Methodology is inevitably interwoven with and emerges from the nature of particular disciplines…and particular perspectives” (pp. 191-192). Thus, an attempt has been made to explicate both the fundamental assumptions upon which these disciplines rest, while also discussing the points at which they intersect with each other and with the present inquiry. The following two sections of this chapter deal with research design, first with regard to the participants and the research context surrounding them and me, as the interviewer; and then, to the data collection method, including ethical obligations to the participants. The next section discusses more explicitly and reflexively my stance and positioning as a researcher in the context of this study, and the attendant advantages and disadvantages of being an ‘outsider.’ The fifth section presents the interdisciplinary methodological framework of the study, which integrates the content worlds of social and discursive psychology, sign-oriented semiotic linguistics, and qualitative inquiry and narrative analysis. This section concludes with a discussion of the necessity of this integration, both in general and concerning the present research goals. The sixth section focuses on the practical analytical tools and methods involved in the execution of this research, which include both quantitative and qualitative approaches and a spiraling manner of analysis. The penultimate section explicates the methodological, logistical, and ethical considerations involved, and the chapter concludes with a discussion of the trustworthiness and quality of the research. 3.2 THE PARTICIPANT POPULATION AND RESEARCH CONTEXT This study is based on qualitative interviews conducted with Israeli bus drivers who experienced at least one terror attack (TA), either directly or as a first-hand witness. The types of TAs included bombings, shootings, stone-throwings, and Molotov cocktails, perpetrated or attempted in a suicidal and/or homicidal manner. All of the participants were male and Jewish, and between the ages of 29 and 64 and of various marital statuses at the time of initial contact. With regard to securing the data collection field and research population, a number of meetings between myself and both the managing psychologist/organizational consultant and the social worker for the Jerusalem region at Egged Bus Company took place before the study commenced. After a substantial review process, official approval was obtained, a legal consent form was signed in January of 2005, and an agreement was reached for collaborative work. 70 Potential participants were approached first and spoken with about the study by the psychologist and/or social worker, and then, if interest was shown and consent obtained, by myself. The aims of the research were then explicated in more detail at the time of the interview, and official informed consent was obtained. Participation in the study was entirely voluntary and without compensation. All but one of the participants (who was found through a personal connection in Be’er Sheva) were recruited through these Egged clinicians. There was neither opportunity nor necessity for any particular sampling decisions, given that the entire pool of possible participants was relatively minimal. Drivers in Jerusalem were targeted for participation first, as this city had been the hardest hit by bus bombings and thus had the highest number of possible participants. However, as it gradually became more difficult to find participants in Jerusalem, personnel at the Egged branches in Tel Aviv, Haifa, and Be’er Sheva were also approached in a similar fashion, in addition to contacting drivers directly whenever possible. A great deal of energy over the first five years of the study was spent in an attempt to recruit more participants, with little success. Qualitative researchers often discuss the concept of “saturation”—the point at which new data does not provide any new insights, and only confirms interpretations that have already been reached—as the ideal ending point for recruitment of participants (Charmaz, 2006; Josselson & Lieblich, 2003), and my initial hope was to recruit at least twenty interviewees in an attempt to reach this point. However, one of the challenges of this study was finding individuals willing to be interviewed on such a sensitive topic and for what is, for them, such a lengthy amount of time. This challenge only increased as the security situation inside of Israel continued to improve and the Israeli public became further removed from the tense security realities of the Second Intifada. Ultimately, eight complete baseline interviews (BLIs) were conducted, and five of the participants agreed to follow-up interviews (FUIs), which took place after almost exactly three years. The BLIs took place between August of 2005 and March of 2007, which was a relatively calm period in Israel vis-à-vis bus-related TAs. The FUIs took place between July of 2008 and May of 2009, during which time there was a small number of TAs involving buses and bulldozers. It is generally agreed that the Second Intifada came to a close by 2005, perhaps because this year involved the fewest TAs and the fewest fatalities, on both sides, since its start five years prior. However, the period of 2005-2009 was not entirely calm, with many other types of attacks both within Israel and on several of its borders—including bombings at roadblocks and shopping malls, a particularly horrific shooting spree at a yeshiva in Jerusalem, and a number of Qassam and Grad rocket attacks on private houses and populated civilian areas (Israel Ministry of Foreign Affairs). This period also encompassed the Second Lebanon War, in the summer of 2006, and Operation Cast Lead in Gaza, in the winter of 2008-9. This national context set the scene for many of the topics discussed in the interviews, and often played an integral part in the participants’ discourse. 71 3.3 THE DATA COLLECTION METHOD In-depth, semi-structured, open-ended qualitative interviews were conducted with each of the participants. The initial interview questions guide was informed by a number of ‘fact-finding’ meetings with Egged personnel, after which a single pilot interview was conducted to assess the efficacy of these questions and to allow for preliminary analyses and minor modifications in methodology. After this first interview was transcribed and assessed, various questions were added, removed, or modified, and the rest of the interviews were conducted according to the revised questions guide (see Appendix 1), which consists of fourteen open-ended questions relating to the general topics under initial investigation (i.e., identity, worldview, systems of meaningmaking, cultural norms, and overall belief structures). The interviews generally followed Rosenthal’s (1993, 2005) two-part biographical-narrative interview style, which distinguishes between the period of main narration, during which the interviewee is asked a single initial narrative question and the interviewer takes care not to interrupt, intervene, or direct, and the questioning period, which allows for both internal and external narrative questions. Each interview began with a general request for a life story (“Please tell me about yourself,” or, if necessary, “Please tell me about yourself and your background”1) and included a specific request for the narrative(s) of the TA(s) experienced. Although I took great care to ensure that all the topics in the questions guide were represented to some degree, the interviews generally flowed in an open-ended manner according to the narratives told by the participant, with additional probing depending on the order, amount, depth, and scope of the narratives. The FUIs were conducted in a manner as similar as possible to each participant’s BLI. As many as possible of the same specific questions were asked, as well as the same general topics covered. If a particular narrative was partially covered within the context of another narrative, I made a separate request for the entire narrative at a later point in the interview. I spent a great deal of time reviewing each participant’s BLI directly prior to his FUI, and so was prepared to ask for retellings of narratives that were particular to him (i.e., if he had told a narrative in the BLI about how he met his wife, I requested the narrative again at the FUI). Essentially, the primary purpose of the FUI was to garner as many ‘parallel narratives’2 as possible. In addition, several new questions were included to explore whether any changes had taken place over the three years that 1 2 The more direct request, “Please tell me your life story,” was attempted in multiple interviews, but more often than not, proved too abstract for the interviewees, seemingly causing anxiety and uncertainty as to what to include and from where to begin, often expressed in multiple questions and demands directed to me as to what I wanted to hear. My rephrasing of the question into these versions garnered greater success, as the interviewees seemed to feel less pressure. Although these versions do not follow the classic wording prescription for a “life story interview,” they did seem to be the only possible means to obtaining a life story from these participants. This is a term I have coined to refer to retellings of the ‘same’ narrative, by the same teller, to the same interviewer, in the same physical venue and in as similar of an interpersonal context as possible, at two or more different points in time. Bamberg (2008) used the term, “twice-told-tales,” to refer to the ‘same’ narratives told by the same person over a period of time, but allowing for the option of different audiences. 72 had passed between the interviews. This could involve physical, psychological, emotional, social, or any other notable developments, such as a new job or change in work status; a change in relationship or marital status; any new traumatic events or life stressors; a change in worldview, spirituality, or degree of religiousness; a greater or lesser need for any therapy or psychological intervention; and/or any new perspective on the TA or any other previous life event. Interviews were conducted at the participants’ homes, at their convenience. Interviewees were requested to sign an informed consent form (see Appendix 2) at the beginning of the interview, and then asked to fill out a short biographical questionnaire (see Appendix 3). They were asked before the interview to reserve at least three hours, so that the interview would not be rushed or limited in any way; however, multiple meetings were scheduled when necessary. Interviews were recorded using cassette tape and MP3 recorders. All tapes and MP3 files, as well as any files containing the participants’ personal details, have been kept confidential and passwordprotected, with no names or identifying information connected to them. The interviews were conducted, transcribed, and analyzed in Hebrew. Excerpts have been translated to English only when necessary for publication and for this dissertation. All research materials that may have been in the hands of others (i.e., outside transcribers, student research assistants) were kept confidential and then destroyed after work with them was completed. The research design, consent form, and all materials presented to the participants received official approval from the Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev in July of 2005. All names used here, as well as in any and all publications resulting from this research, are pseudonyms, and as many relevant personal details as possible have been changed to protect the identities and confidentiality of the participants. Those biographical and personal details which have not been altered are considered essential to a full understanding of the individual (at the time of analysis), and should be assumed to have been recorded at the time of the BLI—unless significant details have changed since then, in which case they will be mentioned specifically—and thus, may be outdated or even incorrect as of the current point in time. 3.4 THE REFLEXIVE RESEARCHER: WHERE I AM COMING FROM One of the most essential components of qualitative research is the awareness of one’s own reflexive perspective as a researcher, and the relationship between this consciousness and the experiences and perspectives of those individuals being studied. As Charmaz (1990) asserted: “the researcher actively shapes the research process. The researcher creates an explication, organization, and presentation of the data rather than discovering order within the data. The discovery process consists of discovering the ideas the researcher has about the data after interacting with it” (p. 1169). Finlay (2002) concurred, stating, “the researcher is a central figure who influences the collection, selection, and interpretation of data” (p. 531). Along these lines, Myerhoff (1992) wrote, “I could 73 never imagine trusting my own or anyone else’s work as fully again without some signposts as to how the interpretations were arrived at and how the [researcher] felt while doing so” (p. 295). To this end, I will now discuss my positioning as the interviewer and researcher in this context, as this is the perspective through which both the process and results of this research will be viewed. Although the unique and co-constructed interpersonal interview contexts are discussed in more specific detail in the holistic introduction to each participant in the individual results chapters, it is necessary to note here a number of basic biographical facts about myself, all of which were readily apparent to the participants. First and foremost, I am female, Jewish, and youthful-looking. At my first contact with the interviewees, I was between 28 and 30 years old and introduced myself to them as a Master’s student in psychology. I was between 31 and 33 years old at the time of the FUIs, and I re-introduced myself as a doctoral student in psychology. The biographical detail about myself that is, perhaps, most relevant to this study is that I was born and grew up in the United States, and immigrated to Israel in October of 2003. While some of the interviewees asked about the specifics of my immigration and some did not appear interested, my status as a ‘new immigrant’ was obvious. My Hebrew was sufficient at the time of the BLIs and more fluent at the FUIs, but my American accent was unmistakable. In addition, I unwittingly offered myriad nonverbal clues (e.g., not dressing ‘like an Israeli’) and missed social cues (e.g., refusing a hot beverage or not eating ‘enough’ of the cake and cookies served to me)— only some of which I am even aware as of this writing—which made my non-native status and our consequent lack of a basic shared cultural context likely immediately evident to the participants. The status and label of new immigrant is commonplace in Israel—used even to refer to those who have been in the country for ten years or more—and carries with it significant import, along with a wide number and variety of (possibly accurate) stereotypes (e.g., Zionist, naïve, not knowledgeable of Israeli society). This status, along with my sex and clearly youthful appearance, undoubtedly placed me at a clear contrast to the participants, and the possibility that their discourse and communicative strategies may have been influenced by these dynamics cannot be ruled out or ignored. Indeed, the interviewees may have been particularly motivated to educate, enlighten, or connect in some way with me, simply because of my identity as an ‘outsider.’ On the other hand, the opposite could be just as plausible—that the interviewees may have felt alienated in some way or put off by my inability to identify with them as an Israeli. The related fact that I did not live in Israel during the majority of either of the Intifadas also likely had an effect on the interview dynamic, as well as on the analysis and research process overall. Although I was studying in Israel as a tourist at the time of the outbreak of the Second Intifada, and I arrived as an immigrant shortly after its height, I do not believe I can ever claim to know what it was like to live under such a constant, daily siege of terror. I have now been in the 74 country through several small-scale wars and military operations over the past few years, some of which ‘hit closer to home’ than others, and I did live in Jerusalem toward the end of the Second Intifada. These experiences actually affected me deeply, perhaps because the circumstances were so new to me and I was new to the country, and perhaps because I had not yet developed any useful coping mechanisms for such stressors. Still, I simply do not have an intrinsic capacity or frame of reference to fully identify with the experiences of these bus drivers. Of course, one must then ask, can anyone “fully identify” with these experiences? Even those who have directly experienced a TA have nonetheless experienced it in their own unique context and through their own perspective. Perhaps my willingness to admit that I have no possibility of identifying with the participants allows me to be privy to a different type of narrative or discourse from them. In any case, I can be considered very much an outsider regarding both the participant population and the society under study. Beyond conducting a thorough review of the literature on various aspects of Israeli society and consulting with my advisors, colleagues, and native Israeli friends to get their (own socially influenced) perspectives, I have no innate way of understanding the intricacies of the social phenomena that I am attempting to study. I was nonetheless drawn to the topics of resilience, coping, and narrating and making meaning out of trauma, and how these phenomena express themselves in Israeli society, since well before I immigrated. In this sense, my motivation to understand these issues is both academic and personal, and strong and intrinsic. Throughout the study process, I have discovered both advantages and disadvantages to my background and positioning. As all interviews are a product of the relationship between the interviewer, interviewee, and interview context (Mishler, 1991)3, it is undeniable that the interviews I conducted were influenced by my ‘outsider-ness.’ In general, I felt that the participants were willing to be open with me and in response to my (often seemingly strange and intrusive) questions because I am an outsider, and they also tended to invest time and energy to come ‘down to my level’ to explain certain issues and phenomena that may be taken for granted by a native Israeli. I also noticed that some of the interviewees were very concerned with making sure that I understood what they were trying to express, and they often went so far as to attempt to translate certain words for me (without my asking them to do so or making any indication that I didn’t understand) or using the little English of which they had mastery to encourage or clarify my understanding. I would claim that my relatively ‘fresh’ eyes may see most clearly phenomena that seem commonplace and unremarkable to native Israelis. Indeed, the first question many of the interviewees posed to me upon our meeting was, “Why are you interviewing me?”—seeming to imply that whatever they have done or suffered or experienced is simply normal and familiar to 3 Another conceptualization of this same idea, using Fabian’s (1971) terms, is that the producer, process, and product are interconnected in their influence on each other (Myerhoff & Ruby, 1992). 75 Israelis. They often expressed surprise and shock at my suggestion that getting back onto a bus and going back to work just hours or days after experiencing a TA may be unusual, or perhaps even admirable. Whether or not this is the case, even the topic and population of this research study might not have been chosen by a cultural ‘insider,’ and in fact, has thus far not been studied. As I am constantly attuned to the possible benefits as well as disadvantages involved in my being an outsider in the eyes of the interviewees, I have invested much effort into ‘filling in the blanks,’ through both reflexivity and debriefing with native Israelis and/or veteran immigrants to Israel. These practices will be discussed in more detail later in this chapter, and all of these interactional issues should be kept in consideration when interpreting the results presented in this dissertation. 3.5 THE METHODOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE STUDY: INTEGRATION OF THREE DISTINCT CONTENT WORLDS This section presents the overarching methodological framework of the study from a general theoretical perspective, while the following section will focus on the specific analytical tools and how they were used. The analytical approach employed herein combines both theory and methodology from sign-oriented linguistics and sociopsychological discourse analysis with an interpretive narrative approach. The goal of this interdisciplinary method is to understand the phenomenological experiences of these bus drivers by analyzing both the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages of their discursive choices. This analytical view, focused on the individual as well as his perceptions of his own experiences and of his society and its expectations, forges a connection between the narrator’s text, his inner psychic world, and the attributions and meanings he attaches to his experiences. This is accomplished by identifying the non-random distribution of linguistic signs in the discourse and hypothesizing about the connection between these specific uses of language and their extra-linguistic messages, and between these meanings and the larger individual and psychosocial implications. Indeed, it is suggested in this dissertation that the communicative oppositions and choices made within the text, distinguished by clear discourse markers, may reflect both the narrator’s worldview and his reciprocal relations with the world and society surrounding him. This interdisciplinary analytical approach asks questions of the discourse, such as: “How could this have been said differently?” and, “How might the choice of alternative linguistic forms and their meanings affect the extra-linguistic message of the text?” The procedure of placing the narrator’s discursive choices into comparative context with a range of possible alternative choices allows a multidimensional and open-ended interpretative perspective. The analytical framework and methodology stays bound to the text at every point and every level, exploring what is being said in the text and how it is being said. Later, interpretative techniques are employed to ask why it was said, with the proposal of possible empirically76 motivated explanations for the extra-linguistic messages that emerged from the linguistic analysis. It is asked why this particular use of language exists here and not there, why the narrator may have chosen to use these specific language forms, and why this particular extra-linguistic message seems to be important to him, both from a personal and societal perspective. This method interprets both the form and content of the text, on both the micro- and macro-levels, in a cohesive manner in order to arrive at a comprehensive and holistic narrative analysis. Below is a detailed discussion of the three central content worlds involved in this interdisciplinary approach: social and discursive psychology; sign-oriented semiotic linguistics; and qualitative inquiry and narrative analysis. 3.5.1 Social and discursive psychology: A focus on the individual and the surrounding societal context One of the primary theoretical foundations for this study is the assumption that the individual exists, acts, and understands and narrates his experiences within a social context. Indeed, the phenomena under study here exist within an interactional and dynamic interplay between personal, familial, communal, societal, and cultural forces and contexts. Both linguistics and discursive social psychology investigate the choice of language forms by individuals within their social contexts, but each does so in different ways and from different angles. Linguists explore discourse in order to understand the intricacies of the choices and meanings of the linguistic forms, while social psychologists explore the user of the discourse and how the language reflects his/her attendant behavior and attributions. As Kroger and Wood (1998) wrote, “At the most general level, the topic of social psychology is discourse because it is in and through discourse that the specific topics of interest (e.g., attribution, social comparison) are constituted” (p. 269). Thus, discourse is viewed by traditional social psychology as the means rather than the ends; the reflection of (or conduit toward understanding) a personal, interpersonal, or societal reality, rather than a phenomenon to be understood in and of itself. Discourse analysis is the term most often used within the social sciences for the type of text analysis routinely conducted within the purview of semiotic linguistics, which is, albeit, more focused on interpersonal and societal meanings and on the social construction of these meanings. Widdowson (2007) claimed that discourse analysis is comprised of two broad areas: “how people make meaning and make out meaning, in texts,” on the one hand, and how meanings are “socially constructed so that expressing them is effectively a kind of social practice,” on the other (p. xv). Wetherell (1998) went even further, contending that discourse can be equated with the overarching human process of making meaning itself. The use of discourse analysis generally relies upon an explicit methodology grounded in discursive psychology, a larger academic theory of social psychology, which holds as its central assumption that “the phenomena of interest in social and psychological research are constituted in and through discourse” (Kroger & Wood, 1998, p. 269; 77 Edwards & Potter, 1992; Potter & Wetherell, 1987). Furthermore, within the post-structural view, there is a greater focus on the interactive and dynamic relationship between identity and discourse. Andersson (2008) claimed that self and identity do not exist outside of the discursive context, and narrators are positioned as selves by their talk. This is, in many ways, an important point of convergence between psychosocial discourse analysis and linguistic theory. Discourse analysis in the social sciences purports to study the intricacies of the language employed, but analyses can frequently appear less comprehensive and less rooted in a specific wellgrounded linguistic approach or theory. Kroger and Wood (1998) referred, for instance, to a reliance on “an extensive set of devices or strategies for examining discourse in a variety of ways” (p. 270), but the focus therein is most often placed more on the speaker than on the speech, and more on the larger social meanings and contexts than the specific use of language at a specific point in time. Indeed, Kroger and Wood (1998) also stated that “discourse analysis attempts to elucidate the social functions and consequences of discourse” (p. 271). While this brings in an important perspective, it leaves the exploration of the language itself somewhat incomplete. Kroger and Wood (1998) thus suggested that the field of discursive psychology is in current need of a conceptual shift toward the idea that talk itself is the “event of interest,” as it is the discourse itself that constructs the reality illustrated in the aforementioned “phenomena of interest.” Indeed, discursive psychology theoretically views language as constituting social practice, rather than merely a communicative or descriptive tool. Similarly, Potter (2003) pointed to “a positive recognition of the primacy of discourse as a medium for action,” suggesting the necessity for a combined analytical focus on language and behavior as intimately connected (p. 785). Furthermore, Kroger and Wood (1998) called for a new method within discursive psychology, one that involves “increased conceptual and methodological rigour,” rather than “a decline into a mushy, relativistic, touchy-feely methodology,” and that “must be suitable to penetrate beyond the common-sense appearances of social interaction” (p. 270). They suggested going “back to the data, to the initial utterance, to the performative force of these initial actions,” seeking and identifying patterns in the discourse and requiring that hypotheses be supported by evidence (Kroger & Wood, 1998, p. 270). This research study has attempted to do just that, placing a psychosociolinguistic lens on the discourse of the bus drivers interviewed herein, in order to understand their beliefs, attitudes, and perceptions about their personal experiences as well as their surrounding societal and cultural discourses. The interactional dynamic between these factors stands at the focus of the analyses conducted and the interpretive conclusions reached. Indeed, the individual’s discourse is never analyzed within a vacuum; rather, it is viewed as an integral part of a context that includes his life history, his particular TA experience, his beliefs about his behavior before, during, and after the 78 attack, and his attributions about how he was perceived and what might have been expected of him throughout. Social psychology thus provides a foundation for this research, but lacks the requisite tools for a comprehensive understanding of the phenomena. An interdisciplinary methodological approach is thus necessary, and the following two content worlds ‘complete’ this objective. 3.5.2 Sign-oriented semiotic linguistics: A focus on the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages of the individual’s words As a central part of its methodological and analytical framework, and out of an attempt to provide a deeper empirical basis for the discourse analysis employed herein, this research study relies on the semiotic, or sign-oriented, linguistic approach originally inspired by Saussure (1983 [1916]). Tobin (1990, 1995) expanded on Saussure, espousing a definition of language as, “A system of systems composed of various sub-systems…which are organized internally and systematically related to each other and used by human beings to communicate” (p. 7, p. 47). In this sense, the structure of language is seen as being shaped by its communicative function and by the characteristics of its users. Complex semiotic systems are viewed as composed of linguistic signs—defined by Tobin (1990, 1995) as signals associated with their own individual and invariant (and relatively abstract) meanings—which must share a common semantic denominator in order to fit into the same semantic and grammatical systems (Dreer, 2007, 2011). While each linguistic sign has only one single, invariant meaning—which motivates the distribution of the sign in language— this consistent and unitary meaning can produce myriad and variable messages in diverse linguistic and extra-linguistic situational contexts. Tobin (1990, 1995), for example, referred to the sign, “to drive,” whereby the invariant meaning could be defined as, “to lead/transport X,” or, “to cause X to move,” while the various specific contextual messages (involving a car or not) can be reflected in, “to drive her home”; “to drive her crazy”; “the drive to win”; and “to put your car in drive.” The sign-oriented semiotic approach defines communication as the creation of oppositions by means of linguistic signs. This can be demonstrated in the following examples: BOY ≠ BOYS Grammatical number opposition (singular vs. plural) BOY ≠ MAN Gradient age opposition BOY ≠ GIRL Polaric gender opposition BOY IS ≠ BOY WAS Tense opposition (present vs. past) The choice of each member in these binary oppositions directly affects the extra-linguistic message the speaker/encoder aims to convey (Kirsner, 1979). The unitary, invariant meaning of each linguistic sign thus motivates its non-random distribution in language to create different messages, and the analysis of the non-random distribution of signs allows an understanding of the extralinguistic message(s) of a text and the motivations behind the narrator’s choices. The Columbia School approach to sign-oriented linguistics was first used in analyses of 79 specific grammatical systems, validated by qualitative explanations of sentences in context. Diver’s (1969) seminal article on the Homeric verb validated the meanings of the Homeric Greek verb tenses according to a quantitative analysis of their total distribution within The Odyssey, thus presenting the first use of holistic narrative theory within linguistics. The analysis demonstrated that the sentences with high-relevance verb forms contained all the important events of the plot and the fundamental details of all the major characters, and the sentences with low-relevance verb forms contained all the elaborative descriptive and background details. Diver’s (1969) introduction of qualitative and quantitative analyses and motivated explanations of the non-random distribution of linguistic forms, which could be directly related to the empirically postulated extra-linguistic messages of texts, represented an early integration of linguistics and narrative analysis. The interdisciplinary method used in this study rests on both this holistic linguistic theory and methodology, along with a sociopsychological narrative approach. The analysis herein rests on the fundamental assumption that the linguistic sign can be analyzed as an individual unit within a number of potentially interrelated semiotic systems in order to gain insight into the larger messages of a narrator’s discourse. Human beings tend to assume that we know the signs and signals of our own language a priori, but Tobin (1990) contended that we—humans, in general, and linguistic analysts, in particular—must begin ‘from scratch’ each time we confront a potential sign, creating hypotheses anew with regard to validating both the signal and its invariant unitary meaning, and the manner in which both are employed in creating new extralinguistic messages by different speakers/encoders in diverse linguistic and situational contexts. 3.5.2.1 The pronoun system and the importance of gender in Modern Israeli Hebrew Tobin (1990, 1995) claimed that “successful linguistic communication can only be achieved through the combined effort of an encoder and a decoder cooperating together” (p. 48, p. 11). As such, the pronoun system in Modern Israeli Hebrew (MIH) must be understood, first and foremost, in terms of its invariant meanings: encoder (first-person), decoder (second-person), and other than the encoder/decoder or outsider (third-person). Below is a table delineating the MIH pronoun system from a sign-oriented perspective, explicating both the invariant meaning and conventional usage characteristics and extra-linguistic messages of each pronoun. 80 Table 3-1: The pronoun system in Modern Israeli Hebrew PERSON 1st ‘I’ NUMBER Singular ‘We’ Plural PRONOUN ‘You (MS)’ 2 nd 3rd Singular GENDER Neutral Neutral INVARIANT MEANING Encoder (Speaker / Writer) Masculine ‘You (FS)’ Singular Feminine ‘You (MP)’ Plural Masculine ‘You (FP)’ ‘He’ ‘She’ Plural Singular Singular Feminine Masculine Feminine ‘They (MP)’ Plural Masculine ‘They (FP)’ Plural Feminine Decoder (Audience / Listener / Reader) Other (Outsider) CHARACTERISTICS / MESSAGES Most proximate and personal – ego-centered Unmarked – generic singular form Most general and neutral Customarily used in non-gendered discourse or in relation to a non-specific subject Marked for feminine Customarily used only in person- or genderspecific discursive situations Creates a sense of greater proximity and less neutrality toward female being addressed Unmarked – generic plural form Customarily used in mixed-gender groups Marked for feminine Unmarked – generic singular form Marked for feminine Unmarked – generic plural form Customarily used to refer to mixed-gender groups Marked for feminine The MIH pronoun system has distinct forms distinguishing singular and plural for all three persons, and gender-specific forms for second- and third-person pronouns. The first-person pronominal forms (‘I’ and ‘we’) are gender-neutral, as the speaker(s) are readily identifiable by means of the collocated verbs. Contrarily, the second- and third-person pronominal forms are marked for both gender (masculine/feminine) and number (singular/plural). The masculine pronouns are the unmarked forms, as they can be used to refer to either male or female objects, and verbs are generally characterized by the base form. Feminine forms, on the other hand, require additional obligatory suffixes, and are used to denote feminine objects only. The masculine singular form can be applied to a specific male and/or as an inclusive gender form for either or both genders together. Essentially, the unmarked masculine forms can be used in all contexts (all-male, mixed, and even all-female groups), while the marked feminine forms are customarily used only in all-female groups or in relation to a specific female decoder(s) (and even then, the feminine forms can and frequently are replaced by the unmarked masculine forms for use in expressing generic and/or general messages). Unlike in English, all verbs (and adjectival forms) in MIH are inflected and explicitly and obligatorily conjugated for person (first/second/third), number (singular/plural), and gender (masculine/feminine). This makes the collocation of ‘pronoun + conjugated verb’ optional in most 81 cases, as the conjugated verb itself overtly displays the information found in the pronoun. 4 In addition, the gender and number agreement in all verbs always allows a clear identification of the first-person encoder. Only in the present tense is the pronoun-verb collocation frequently necessary for full comprehension of the identity of the encoder, decoder, and other. The first-person singular pronoun (‘ani’ in MIH; ‘I’ in English) signifies speaker/writer (‘encoder’), referring to ‘the one who speaks here and now.’ Because it is ego-centered, it is considered the most proximate and personal pronoun. It is unmarked for (that is, neutral to) gender, and is the most specific or ‘known’ pronoun—as its use leaves little ambiguity as to the identity or nature of the speaker. This form is used to relate to personalized events, actions, or states, and cannot represent a relation to anything or anyone but the encoder. As Pennebaker (2002) noted, “The use of 1st person singular (I, me, my)…provides insight into people’s social identity and ‘ownership’ of their speaking or writing topic” (p. 8). The first-person plural pronoun (‘anaxnu’5 in MIH; ‘we’ in English) signifies a plural or collective speaker/writer (‘encoder’), which is also egocentered but overtly indicates more than a single individual encoder. This then requires further interpretation of the identities of those who comprise this collective entity. The second-person masculine singular (MS) pronoun (‘atah’ in MIH; ‘you’ in English) signifies audience/listener/reader (‘decoder’), and, as it is unmarked, is considered one of the most general and neutral pronouns (Tobin, 2001). It has two functions in MIH: 1) its traditional usage, to address a specific male decoder; and 2) a more generic, general, and impersonal usage corresponding roughly to the non-gendered and non-numbered ‘you’ in English.6 In the latter case, it functions as the unmarked form, customarily used in non-gendered discourse or in relation to a non-specific subject (similar to, in English, “When one does this type of work…” or the generalized, “When you wake up in the morning…”). However, the MS ‘you’ is also used in discourse that does not—or does not necessarily—call for the unmarked form. Indeed, feminists have reported cases, both in private conversations and the media, of two women using the MS ‘you’ in discourse with each other, even in such obviously gendered cases as, “When you (MS) are pregnant…” or “When you (MS) are nursing your (MS) baby…” (Tobin, personal communication). Thus, it has been hypothesized that the use of the unmarked MS ‘you’ serves to create a depersonalized or universalized sense of meaning, allowing relation to ‘one’ or ‘every/anyperson.’ The second-person feminine singular (FS) pronoun (‘at’ in MIH; ‘you’ in English) also 4 5 6 The phenomenon of using only the conjugated form of a verb, without its collocated pronoun, is referred to as “prodrop” in the formal-syntactic linguistic literature. All transliterations in this dissertation follow the conventional guidelines of the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), wherein the x stands for the voiceless velar fricative, sometimes written in lay circles as ch. In English, the second-person ‘you’ is unmarked for both number and gender (i.e., there is no difference between the masculine singular, feminine singular, masculine plural, and feminine plural forms), while as noted here, there are four distinct forms in MIH for the second-person ‘you,’ each marked for both gender and number. 82 signifies decoder, and also has two functions: 1) to address a specific female decoder; and 2) a generic and/or general manner of usage between two females or in all-female groups, and specific or generic (female) gendered situations and contexts (i.e., one might hear, “When you (FS) are pregnant…” in this context). This second function has moved more into popular (and, some say, even provocative) use in the past few years—primarily by feminists and the younger generation— but is not viewed as conventional or widely accepted across generations or population sectors. As the FS ‘you’ is marked for gender and number (Tobin, 2001), it is customarily utilized only in person- or gender-specific discursive situations. Thus, the choice to use it (rather than the standard neutral, unmarked MS form) may create a sense of greater proximity and less neutrality toward the female individual being addressed. In the context of this study, it is suggested that a male bus driver who uses the FS ‘you’ while speaking to a female interviewer about his own experiences may be demonstrating an entirely different communicative strategy—one that has particular implications regarding his attitude toward what he is describing and/or toward the specific decoder. The second-person masculine plural (MP) pronoun (‘atem’ in MIH; ‘you’ in English) functions as the unmarked form in all-male and mixed-gender groups. The presence of a single male in even a large group of women is sufficient to call for the use of the MP form, while the marked feminine plural (FP) pronoun (‘aten’ in MIH; ‘you’ in English) is conventionally used only in all-female groups. This is particularly true in entirely female-only contexts, such as basic training courses for female soldiers or in religious (gender-separated) schools; however, even then, the MP form can be used and fully accepted. Only the most feminist-minded individuals in recent generations have begun a revolution of sorts, consciously utilizing the marked FP rather than the unmarked MP pronominal form in mixed groups with ‘only’ a majority of women (Tobin, 2001).7 In addition, the use of the MP conjugated form of verbs, with or without the inclusion of the pronoun itself, provides an even more generalized, generic, and depersonalized form. When the MP form of a verb is being used as the unmarked, generalized form, it generally need not be collocated with a pronoun, and therefore, could potentially refer to ‘you’ (MS), ‘they’ (MP), or even ‘we.’ This is essentially the most distant, impersonal, and passive verb form in the system, as it requires the most added information to ascertain its linguistic message. As may already be clear from this discussion of the pronoun system, one of the most central elements of MIH is gender and the constant linguistic choices that must be made in order to conform to the use of obligatory gender(ed) forms. As Tobin (2001) contended, “Gender is inherent, integral, and ubiquitous” in MIH, as all nouns, pronouns, adjectives, verbs, and prepositions either have an intrinsic grammatical gender or must be inflected to agree with their 7 Tobin (personal communication) noted that this extreme ‘politicized’ and ideological use has not spread to the general Hebrew-speaking population and remains unusual and strange-sounding to native (non-feminist) speakers. 83 collocated grammatical counterpart. Indeed, Tobin (2001) claimed, “structurally speaking, gender (biological and grammatical) is almost always present at all levels of word and utterance formation in Modern Hebrew” (p. 192). This centrality of gender carries with it an ever-present requirement for speakers to select the appropriate form to use throughout their discourse. Analytically speaking, therefore, it is crucial to pay attention to the structure and nature of these usages and choices in spontaneous discourse—particularly if patterns emerge as unusual or unconventional, or predominantly consistent or inconsistently distributed. 3.5.2.2 The interlocked tense system in Modern Israeli Hebrew According to sign-oriented linguistic theory, the tense system in MIH should also be understood in terms of its invariant meanings and potential interpretive messages. Unlike in Biblical Hebrew, this system primarily revolves around signs indicating what are typically referred to as ‘traditional simple’ past, present, and future tenses (Dekel, 2010). This conventional analysis, however, does not accurately encompass all the varied uses of each tense, as all can cross over in both time (e.g., the so-called ‘present’ tense can be used to refer to actions, states, and events in the past as well as future) and function (e.g., the so-called ‘past’ tense can be used to refer to hypothetical actions, states, and events in both the present and future, and the so-called ‘future’ tense can function as an imperative). Despite these phenomena, Dekel (2010) referred to a sparcity of research on the tense system of spoken Hebrew, noting Borochovsky Bar-Aba’s (2008) discussion of “inconsistencies in the verb tenses” (p. 267), with no further explication therein. In a sign-oriented perspective on these perceived “inconsistences,” Tobin (1989b) proposed a linguistic sign-oriented analysis centered on a Space-Time-Existence system, one “whose semantic substance deals with the placing of events/actions/states in their spatio-temporalexistential relation to the speaker at the point (or the ‘here and now’) of speaking” (p. 63). Within this system, the invariant meaning of the ‘present’ tense is proximate, while that of the ‘past’ and ‘future’ tenses is remote with regard to the encoder. Furthermore, Tobin (1989b) suggested that the Space-Time-Existence system should be interlocked with the Experience system, the “semantic domain” of which is “the speaker’s ability to perceive or experience an event/action/state” (p. 63). Within this system, the invariant meaning of the ‘present’ and ‘past’ tenses is experienced or able to be experienced, while that of the ‘future’ tense is not experienced or not able to be experienced. Table 3-2: The sign-oriented interlocked tense system in Modern Israeli Hebrew TENSE ‘Present’ ‘Past’ ‘Future’ PROXIMITY Proximate Remote Remote EXPERIENCE-ABILITY Experienced / experience-able Experienced / experience-able Not experienced / not experience-able 84 Thus, within this framework of Tobin’s (1989b) two interlocked semantic sub-systems of the MIH tense system, the description of an event, action, or state using the ‘past’ tense signals the meanings remote and (able to be) experienced; the use of the ‘present’ tense signals the meanings proximate and (able to be) experienced; and the ‘future’ tense signals the meanings remote and not (able to be) experienced.8 In this sense, as Tobin (1989b) suggested, the use of particular tenses in an individual’s discourse may not be—and in fact, is rarely—determined by the actual time of occurrence of the described event/action/state, but rather by the encoder’s perception of these events/actions/states with regard to proximity and ‘experience-ability.’ Thus, a speaker may use one tense sign rather than another as a motivated expression of his beliefs, emotions, or attributions vis-à-vis what he is describing. Furthermore, as Tobin (1989b) contended, “the consistent and systematic preference of one verb tense over another…may tell us something about the encoder’s attitude toward those actions, states, or events, and/or indicate his particular world view” (p. 65). Therefore, for instance, an interviewee’s clear tendency to use the proximate-present (PPr) tense to describe experiences that occurred in the past, or to use a particular tense with a particular pronoun, may suggest a non-random choice and a specific communicative strategy at work—especially if the pattern is found to be relatively consistent across individuals, experiences, or contexts. 3.5.2.3 The systems of focus and deixis and prepositions in Modern Israeli Hebrew Pronouns are essentially substitutions or shortcuts, used to refer to (usually) previously mentioned persons or entities, and therefore, there is a need for additional semantic markers to instruct the decoder as to how and where to relate to given pronouns as and after they are identified. The semantic systems of focus and deixis exist to fulfill this need within the pronominal system, and are closely associated with it. The focus system—defined as whether or not a particular subject is in the center of the speaker’s interest (Kirsner, 1979)—regards the linguistic sign’s worthiness of attention, and thus, is binary (i.e., either in or not in focus). The deictic system—defined as “the force with which the hearer is instructed to find the referent” (Garcia, 1975, p. 65)—regards the degree of attention the linguistic sign is worth, and thus, can be denoted on a relative scale, either numerically or as ‘high,’ ‘medium,’ or ‘low.’ In this sense, while subject pronouns (e.g., ‘I,’ ‘you,’ ‘we,’ ‘they’) are in focus and high-deixis, object pronouns (e.g., ‘me,’ ‘us,’ ‘them’) are not in focus and low-deixis. In the sentence, “You gave the book to me,” for example, the subject (“you”) is in focus and high-deixis, as it is performing the action of the sentence. The object pronoun, “(to) me,” is not in focus and mid-deixis, as it is receiving the action in the sentence. The semantic meanings 8 Consequently and henceforth, this dissertation will refer to the ‘present’ tense as the proximate-present (PPr) tense, to the ‘past’ tense as the remote-past (RPa) tense, and to the ‘future’ tense as the remote-future (RF) tense. The traditional terms for these tenses will be used sparingly but interchangeably, and only when necessary for maximum comprehension or readability. 85 of each pronominal form within the focus and deictic systems serve to assist both the encoder and decoder in determining the function and importance of the represented entities within the linguistic and situational discourse contexts in which they appear (Tobin, personal communication). Many of the prepositions in MIH are essentially identical to those in English, although their conventional usage may vary slightly (e.g., the English phrase, “he recommended me,” would be strictly translated from MIH as, “he recommended on me”; the English phrase, “he helped me,” would be, “he helped to me”). In addition, while both English and MIH contain the words, ‘me,’ and, ‘mine,’ MIH also possesses a unique semantic preposition that does not exist in English, which is used constantly in daily conversation and indeed appeared frequently in the discourse of these drivers. The invariant meaning of this word, ‘etzel’, according to Tobin (personal communication), is essentially, “within the boundaries/realm of X.” An exact translation into English is impossible (although it is comparable to ‘chez’ in French), but it will have to suffice to explain that ‘etzli’ (the conjugated form for ‘me’) can be understood as ‘at me,’ ‘by me,’ ‘on me,’ or ‘in me,’ depending on the context. For example, while one could say, “The book is at my house,” the addition of ‘etzli’ would change the translation to, “The book is at me at my house’—or, alternatively, ‘etzli’ could be used to replace the phrase, “my house,” entirely, allowing the sentence to be simply, “The book is at me,” which is a perfectly acceptable conventional usage of the term (loosely meaning, “I have the book”). The extra-linguistic meaning of this term is highly personalized, more so than if one were to use any of the above alternative prepositions. Because the interviewees utilized both this preposition and a variety of semi-equivalent alternatives, it will be claimed that unique and significant meaning lies in these choices and their non-random distribution. To this end, I have attempted to translate these instances of discourse as literally (albeit often not as elegantly or eloquently) as possible, in hopes of conveying these distinctions clearly. Indeed, there are a number of other prepositions in MIH that require definition here, as their invariant meanings impact the messages sent by the encoders: for instance, ‘l-’, the meaning of which is orientation in some manner or form; ‘b-’, the meaning of which is circumstance; and ‘el-’, the meaning of which is goal/destination/in the direction (of) (Tobin, 2008). The conjugated form of ‘l-’ for ‘me’—‘li’—can be translated as ‘to me,’ ‘for me,’ ‘on me,’ or ‘mine/belonging to me,’ depending on the verb and surrounding context, and has the literal meaning of ‘orientation to self.’ The sign-oriented meaning also interlocks with the Participation/Contribution system, such that it can be seen as higher on a personalization level than, say, ‘el-’, which is not a part of this system. Both ‘li’ and ‘etzli’ can be interpreted as sending the generalized extra-linguistic message of ‘I am/was involved in this event,’ and both terms are not in focus and mid-deixis. However, ‘l-’ is understood as more personalized than ‘etzel’ and than many of the alternatives (e.g., “They got onto me” vs. “They got onto my bus”). The choice of preposition at every point in an individual’s 86 discourse is, on some level, motivated by their invariant meanings, and should not be ignored. The text quotations presented throughout this dissertation highlight these differentiations in the interviewees’ discourse as clearly as possible—with the unfortunate, but necessary, consequence of slightly more clumsy phrasings in English. The table below shows the relative levels of deixis among several of the most common prepositions in MIH, all of which appeared frequently in the interviewees’ speech. Given that all are object-related prepositions (that is, for instance, “on me,” rather than “I”), all are fairly low in deixis in general. However, there is a significant variance in degree of deixis relative to each other, and so the discursive choice to use one over the other often had a meaningful influence on the semantic message sent by the encoder. Table 3-3: Common MIH prepositions according to their relative deictic level PREPOSITION (CONJUGATED FOR ‘ME’) ‘li’ ‘bi’ ‘alai’ ‘elai’ ‘etzli’ ‘iti’ (None) EXEMPLAR DISCOURSE IN TEXT (FOR “GOT ON [THE BUS]”) ‘on me’ / ‘onto me’ ‘into me’ ‘on to me’ ‘on toward me’ ‘on at me’ / ‘on at my bus’ ‘on with me’ ‘got on’ or ‘got onto the bus’ RELATIVE DEIXIS 5 (Highest) 5 4 4 3 2 1 (Lowest) Again, the gendered nature and use of all of these interrelated systems in MIH requires constant choices regarding gender as well as number, in relation to both encoder and decoder. A fundamental assumption of this research is that these choices are not made arbitrarily, and the meanings of and relationships between the choices of marked and unmarked pronouns as well as tense may function as analytical ‘flags,’ thus spurring and facilitating a deeper analysis of the psychosocial discourse surrounding the individual or event. Viewed from a linguistic and discursive analytical perspective, the non-random distribution of pronouns and tenses within the texts is employed as both the source and the empirical support for the hypotheses offered. Analyses of both the pronoun and tense systems, along with the juxtaposed scales of focus and deixis where relevant, can give the basis for an understanding of their use in the bus drivers’ discourse, whereby the forms are chosen by their meanings within their respective systems, which interact and carry with them particular implications. This study focuses on the interviewees’ discursive choices at particular moments or in relation to particular topics and their extra-linguistic messages, alongside the meanings of these usages in their given contexts. Specifically, the focus on the bus drivers’ divergent uses of the FS ‘you’ pronoun and its motivations, meanings, and ramifications constitutes an innovative contribution to the linguistic literature. A view of these choices as communicative strategies utilized by the participants provides a background for an understanding of the individuals and the larger psychosocial implications and contexts within their lived-worlds. 87 3.5.3 Qualitative inquiry and the interpretive narrative paradigm: A focus on the individual’s stories and the story-telling context This research study is qualitative, naturalistic, and phenomenological, with a focus on the participants’ lived experiences and particular, constructed realities and meanings. Denzin and Lincoln (1994) defined qualitative research as “multimethod in focus, involving an interpretive naturalistic approach to its subject matter,” and as involving “the studied use of and collection of a variety of empirical materials…that describe routine and problematic moments and meaning in individuals’ lives” (p. 2). Qualitative researchers, in this domain, “study things in their natural settings, attempting to make sense of, or interpret, phenomena in terms of the meanings people bring to them” (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994, p. 2). While quantitative research is post-positivistic, seeking to learn about prediction and control of variables, qualitative research is generally interpretive and interested in achieving an understanding of those ‘variables.’ The central research philosophy was phenomenological—focused upon exploring the events and experiences of the interviewees in their day-to-day context from their own perspective and emphasizing the particular meanings they themselves attach to these experiences (Baum, 2004; Becker, 1992; Spinelli, 1996). This study is situated within the interpretive or constructivist family of paradigms, which is based on a relativistic ontological view with a non-static, specific, and localized focus (Guba & Lincoln, 1994, 2005). In this sense, there is no assumption of clear, simple, behavioral operationalizations for the phenomena under investigation. Rather, the epistemological research stance is entirely subjective, constructivist, and transactional in its view of the individual’s understanding of his reality, and of the co-constructed interpretive reality of the interview context. Indeed, as Josselson (2006) stated: “The practice of narrative research, rooted in postmodernism, is always interpretive, at every stage. From framing the conceptual question through choosing the participants, deciding what to ask them, with what phrasing, transcribing from spoken language to text, understanding the verbal locutions, making sense of the meanings thus encoded, to deciding what to attend to and to highlight – the work is interpretive at every point” (pp. 3-4). A facet of the investigation looks, therefore, at the participants’ particular and personal perceptions of the societal discourse surrounding them, rather than presuming the existence of a single, accepted discourse or any objective or universal ‘truths’ therein. The methodology adopted in this inquiry is hermeneutic and dialectical at its base, seeking a complex and deep understanding of the phenomena under study. The underlying goal is to expose and deconstruct discursive phenomena that may have been heretofore taken for granted, and to provide a new perspective and potential application for the insights garnered herein. At its essential core, this research study is narrative in nature. Narrative research, at its basic level, “begins in experience as expressed in lived and told stories” (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 5). In the case of the present study, narratives comprise the empirical material for investigation, 88 the interpersonal context for data collection, and the body of evidence for confirmation of interpretive hypotheses. Narrative inquiry is generally classified as a sub-type of qualitative inquiry (Chase, 2005), but Spector-Mersel (2010) recently made a persuasive case for a reinforced view of the narrative approach as a distinct qualitative paradigm, in all its diversity and multiplicity and complete with its own specific ontology, epistemology, and methodology. Too often, SpectorMersel (2010) argued, narrative inquiry is referred to as merely a method, a methodology, or an analytical approach. This dissertation views and utilizes narrative as an integral part of all its research facets: theoretical, conceptual, methodological, reflexive, and paradigmatic. The study relies upon and is a product of the “narrative turn” that has taken place over the past decades, across disciplines and throughout the academic and professional world, whereby a positivistic and realistic focus on quantitative data has shifted toward an interpretive and openended view toward qualitative texts of all kinds (Lieblich et al., 1998; Riessman, 1993, 2008; Spector-Mersel, 2010). This view called for a shift from looking for a single, objective, positivistic ‘truth,’ toward seeking meaning, subjective and constructed processes of understanding, and multiple interpretations and worldviews. In particular, this study’s positioning within the academic discipline of psychology follows an important tradition, as Josselson and Lieblich (1996) asserted: “Narrative modes of investigation, which take hermeneutics rather than logical-positivism as a philosophical base, are gaining in currency in psychology… In privileging analysis of the particular, in recognizing the role of the observer in all investigation, in honoring the importance of language as structuring understanding, narrative-based psychology has become a creative horizon in psychology” (p. 651). I contend that the integration of the disciplines of psychology, linguistics, and narrative inquiry within the methodological framework of this study constitutes an important, innovative “horizon.” With regard to the historical development of narrative inquiry, Pinnegar and Daynes (2007) discussed four “turns,” or changes in thinking from the positivistic stance, that have taken place: 1) the attention placed on the relationship forged between the researcher and the researched; 2) the move from numbers to words as data; 3) the move from a focus on the general and universal toward the particular and specific; and 4) the recognition of blurred genres of knowing. All four of these turns inform the methodology of this study, and so a brief discussion of each is presented here. In addition, Chase (2005) delineated a set of “five analytic lenses through which contemporary researchers approach empirical material” within narrative inquiry (p. 656): 1) narrative as a distinct discourse form; 2) narrative as verbal action; 3) narrative as impacted by social context; 4) narrative as socially situated interactive performance; and 5) the researcher as narrator. These perspectives also form the basis of the interpretive stance taken by this study, and can be viewed as fundamental assumptions for the methodology herein, and so they will be presented alongside of the turns. The first turn, which is evident in several areas throughout this chapter (most notably in the reflexive sections), contends that the relationship created in an interview context is interactional, 89 dynamic, and mutually impacting (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007). In contrast to the positivist-realist research perspective that a researcher conducts studies or experiments on ‘human subjects,’ the narrative view is that research is undertaken with ‘participants,’ and “both researcher and researched will learn” (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 14). This relationship, therefore, and the data garnered from it, is particular and unique to the time, space, and contexts involved in the interview experience, as well as to the specific identities, histories, and worldviews of the experiencers. The quality of the relationship is perhaps the most critical element; as Josselson (2009) asserted, “Narrative inquiry begins with a respectful, ethical, nonjudgmental, responsive, and empathic research relationship (Josselson, 2007)” (p. 650). Indeed, the level of trust and connection between the interviewee and interviewer is a driving force and has a fundamental influence on the quality of narrative data obtained in the research interview context (Josselson, 2009). Along these lines, Chase’s (2005) second analytic lens presents a view of narratives as “verbal action—as doing or accomplishing something” (p. 657). This view is centered on the basic assumption that when individuals present their narratives to others, they do so for specialized and specific reasons. Indeed, as Alasuutari (1997) contended, “Individuals do not have their readily narrated life stories in their back pockets or the back of their minds, waiting for a researcher to collect them. Any account of one’s personal past…makes a point and serves a function” (p. 6). We tell what we tell, how we tell it (and when and why), as a result of a complex interplay of personal, interactional, and contextual factors. Indeed, as Riessman (2008) asserted, narrative “involves persuading an audience that may be skeptical,” and “engages an audience in the experience of the narrator” (p. 9). Telling one’s story is also an interpretive act of sense- and meaning-making, for the self as well as vis-à-vis the surrounding society and world (Andersson, 2008; Dawson, 1994). Spector-Mersel (2011) referred to the function of self-narrative as a means toward claiming an identity, writing, “a sense of identity is attained through the stories we tell ourselves and others,” and suggesting that “not only is identity expressed in narrative, but also importantly, it is also constructed by it” (p. 173). Ultimately, as Chase (2005) contended, when narrative is perceived through this lens, the situated voice of the narrator is placed into a primary focus. This, in turn, leads to a privileging of the ‘what, how, and where’ of the particular narrator and told narrative, and allows an interpretive and constructivist approach to analysis of material of this nature. The second turn involves “a recognition that in translating experience to numeric codes researchers lose the nuances of experience and relationship in a particular setting that are of interest to those examining human experience” (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 15). In an early phase of this study, I considered including a quantitative questionnaire to attempt to more ‘objectively’ measure post-traumatic symptoms and overall health statuses of the participants. However, very quickly I realized that I was much more interested in the bus drivers’ narratives about their experiences and 90 coping processes, and their own subjective perceptions of their health, rather than their supposedly objective position on any external scale or typology. It then became obvious to me that narrative inquiry would provide “a deeper opportunity to establish the authenticity and trustworthiness” of my findings (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 21), along with the means and material to interpret these findings in a meaningful way. This turn also fits in with Chase’s (2005) first analytic lens, which calls for narrative researchers to “treat narrative—whether oral or written—as a distinct form of discourse” (p. 656). In this view, narrative is seen as an integral part of the human meaningmaking endeavor, allowing the narrator to place the narrated experiences into the context of his life story (Bruner, 1986). As Riessman (2008) stated, “In a dynamic way…narrative constitutes past experience at the same time as it provides ways for individuals to make sense of the past” (p. 8). The third turn is represented in a shift from a focus on general phenomena or ‘universal’ experiences toward specific, particular, and situated participants, contexts, and experiences (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007). Chase’s (2005) fourth analytic lens is in line with this, suggesting a treatment of narratives as “socially situated interactive performances—as produced in this particular setting, for this particular audience, for these particular purposes” (p. 657). In this sense, and alongside the aforementioned reflexive view of the co-construction of the interview context, each story told by each narrator is seen as “flexible, variable, and shaped in part by interaction with the audience” (Chase, 2005, p. 657). In addition, Chase’s (2005) third analytic lens leads narrative researchers to conceive of narratives as “both enabled and constrained by a range of social resources and circumstances” (p. 657). This is a particularly critical view for the present inquiry, in its focus on the interplay between personal and societal discourse, which can be deeply influenced by the individual’s physical, social, communal, organizational, cultural, and historical positionings. The fourth turn involves a shift “from one way of knowing the world to an understanding that there are multiple ways of knowing and understanding human experience” (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 25). Again, this involves a move away from ‘objectivity’ and positivistic conceptualizations of validity and reliability, and toward the more inclusive concepts of trustworthiness, authenticity, and resonance (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994; Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007; Tracy, 2010). In this sense, narrative researchers “accept and value the way in which narrative inquiry allows wondering, tentativeness, and alternative views to exist as part of the research account” (Pinnegar & Daynes, 2007, p. 25). Indeed, as Chase (2005) stated: “Contemporary narrative inquiry can be characterized as an amalgam of interdisciplinary analytic lenses, diverse disciplinary approaches, and both traditional and innovative methods—all revolving around an interest in biographical particulars as narrated by the one who lives them” (p. 651). The multidimensional and interpretive nature of this type of inquiry is perhaps its greatest strength, allowing for a rich, comprehensive, and dynamic understanding of phenomena under investigation. Alongside of this, Chase’s (2005) fifth lens acknowledges the role and stance of the researcher as 91 knower and narrator of the study findings. Not only is narrative research viewed as interpretive and as containing a multiplicity of perspectives on the issues under study, but the research report itself is seen as essentially a narrative of the study, with the researcher/writer as narrator. This privileges the voice of the investigator alongside the voices of the participants, and brings all of these lenses into play for all members of the research context. 3.5.3.1 An organizing model for narrative analysis One of the central organizing principles of the methodological framework of this research is the analytical dichotomy of content vs. form; that is, what an encoder says vs. how he says it. Many of the analytical methods utilized herein spiral between these foci, while also shifting between holistic and categorical viewpoints on the qualitative data. Lieblich et al. (1998) proposed a fundamental classification model for the myriad types of narrative analysis: a four-celled matrix created by the intersecting dimensions of holistic vs. categorical and content vs. form. This model is particularly useful as an organizing conceptualization for the various methods employed in this study. However, as Lieblich et al. (1998) suggested, many analytical methodologies can occupy middle points along these spectrums, integrating the diverse perspectives in a variety of ways. Lieblich et al.’s (1998) model presents four modes of reading a narrative: 1) holisticcontent; 2) holistic-form; 3) categorical-content; and 4) categorical-form. The first focuses on the individual’s entire story as the unit of analysis, looking at what is manifestly spoken and placing it into the holistic context. The second mode also places the analytical focus on the story in its entirety, but trains the lens on the plots and structure of the narrative; exploring, for instance, climaxes, turning points, and high and low peaks. The third type of reading is traditionally represented in content analyses, whereby short excerpts of text are extracted, classified, grouped, and defined according to the focus of the research question. There is little or no relating to context in this mode, and often the identity and specific life history context of the participant holds no importance to the research goals. The final reading mode “focuses on discrete stylistic or linguistic characteristics of defined units of the narrative,” and is generally utilized in discourse analytical methods (Lieblich et al., 1998, p. 13). Particular phenomena in the form of the text, such as active or passive phrasing, metaphors, or pronouns, are distinguished and delineated; here again, there is purportedly little reference to the larger holistic context of the narrative material. Indeed, conventional forms of discourse analysis, as practiced within the purview of many academic fields, are often seen as not fitting the description of holistic narrative analysis, due to their exploration of language through specific and categorized boundaries. However, the interdisciplinary, spiraling, and multidimensional methodology employed in this doctoral study is unique in its investigation of language distribution across entire narratives and on multiple levels, 92 uncovering comprehensive extra-linguistic messages from a holistic view of the linguistic meanings within. This basis in holism positions the methodological framework of this study firmly within the narrative paradigm (Spector-Mersel, 2010, 2011, 2012), which is holistic by definition and design. Alongside of this, the set of methods employed herein, taken as a whole and in collaboration, can be seen as crossing many of the boundaries of Lieblich et al.’s (1998) matrix. In general, this research study relies on a holistic view of the interview material at hand, focusing on each participant and each interview as a whole, in and of itself. When smaller sections of text are isolated for deeper analysis, they are interpreted within the context of the individual’s life history and experiences, and of the surrounding interview context. When categories are employed (e.g., in relation to distinct discursive phenomena), they remain situated within a larger holistic view of the interviewees’ discourse, and particularly, the narrative of the TA experience. This study is, however, primarily focused on the form of the participants’ discourse, out of an assumption that it often “seems to manifest deeper layers of the narrator’s identity” (Lieblich et al., 1998, p. 13). Along these lines, Josselson (2004) referred to Ricoeur’s (1970) conceptualization of the hermeneutics of faith vs. suspicion, distinguishing between interpretive approaches that take the interviewee’s discourse ‘at face value’—that is, trusted or believed ‘on faith’—and those that lead the analyst to ‘dig deeper’ to reveal what may be going on under the surface of the discourse, which may or may not contradict the narrator’s own stated meanings and interpretations. This distinction is relied upon in the methodological framework of this research, and frequently represented in the data as the central dialectic between content and form. In this study, therefore, the content of the narratives is often explored particularly as fodder for triangulation or ‘checking’ of hypotheses generated by the micro-level form-focused analyses. Indeed, multiple perspectives and tools are utilized, with a focus on both form and content, and an analytical stance that alternates between ‘zooming in’ and ‘zooming out’ from both categorical and holistic viewpoints. 3.5.3.2 Primary qualitative analytical influences on the methodological framework A number of techniques and approaches are employed—separately as well as in concert— throughout this research study, to facilitate an in-depth analysis of the content, form, and attendant meanings of the interviewees’ discourse. Together, they permit an intricate exploration of both the participants’ lived experiences and their processes of narrating them, as well as a located view of the experience of the investigator and the interplay between analyst, analysis, and the analyzed text. The analytical approaches used here are largely abductive; that is, the hypotheses emerge from the data and the ‘field,’ in relation and response to the material itself, rather than from a pre-determined theory. As Josselson (2009) asserted, “The work of narrative analysis is to extract, frame, and conceptualize meanings that can be demonstrated in the text. The focus of this analysis is on 93 intraindividual processes, meanings contextualized within the text, and the interrelationship of the elements of experience” (p. 652). The specific methods utilized will be discussed in detail below, but this section ‘sets the stage,’ presenting the more general influences from the content world of qualitative and narrative analysis that have informed the methodological framework of this study. Rosenthal’s (1993) thematic field analysis model, which suggests that particular attention be paid to changes in text type, transition words and statements, breaks in the discourse (either verbal or non-verbal), and topic and/or style changes, significantly influenced the present methodological framework. This approach proposes constantly forming and testing hypotheses, and suggests guiding these hypotheses through the use of, among others, the following questions: “Is the biographer generating a narrative or is he/she being carried along by a narrative flow in his/her story-telling?”; “Why is the autobiographer using this specific sort of text to present his/her experience or theme?”; and, “In which details are the single experiences or themes presented and why?” The purpose of this method is to continually question and problematize the content, form, manner, length, sequence, and absences of the told narratives. In this way, it is possible to watch as hypotheses materialize through and out of the discourse, and the analysis becomes more intricate through the attempt to formulate theories and garner evidence for them within the text. The present study continually questions the obtained narrative data in this manner. Wood and Kroger’s (2000) discussion of discourse analysis and a number of specific strategies therein were perhaps the most fundamentally influential upon the analyses conducted for this study. First, they mentioned the importance of considering one’s own reaction as one reads through and attempts to analyze a given text, and of being aware that one’s reaction can be a sign that “something interesting is going on” (Wood & Kroger, 2000, p. 92). Next, they cautioned that the obvious should not be ignored, as it may be important in some way—particularly from the interviewee’s perspective. This also means that literal meanings of a given statement or narrative may not be the most central or the most meaningful, and that analysis must go beyond the surface to investigate motives, relations, and modes through which meanings are reached and created. The authors also suggested that the actual content of a statement can frequently be less important than the very fact that the statement was given at all. Inclusions of particular issues can, of course, be just as important as exclusions. Wood and Kroger (2000) also contended that it is necessary to “play with the text” throughout analysis, asking oneself if the omission or exchange of certain phrases would affect the text, or how it would read if spoken by a different person or in a different context. In addition, they asserted that one must look at what is not there (i.e., what is missing from a text) as much as what is there, in both form and content. Silences and the absence of talk should rarely be considered “nothing,” and in fact, can be quite informative in an analysis of meanings and the importance of particular facets of a given narrative. 94 In line with Rosenthal’s thematic field analysis, Wood and Kroger’s (2000) stated that it is critical to explore “how the text is structured, shaped, and ordered in both individual segments and overall, because structures are ways of achieving both content and function” (p. 93). The authors noted that “multiple functions of discourse” should be paid attention to as well, as one statement can have many different themes and meanings attached to it (Wood and Kroger, 2000). They also mentioned the possibility of having to develop fresh terms or concepts for particular discourse contents, functions, or techniques, as the opportunity or need arises. All these methodological suggestions connect closely with the sign-oriented linguistic approach to discourse analysis, whereby a deep investigation of the non-random distribution of language is an analytical conduit to understanding the linguistic meanings and extra-linguistic messages of the given text and encoder. Finally, Wood and Kroger (2000) suggested that one arrive at discourse analysis “as a member of the culture, as a speaker-hearer and writer-reader of the language” (p. 95). This admittedly presents particular difficulty for me, as I am still very much a novice member of the culture within which these interviewees live. I have attempted to learn the language, and learn about the language, as fast and as comprehensively as possible, but deficiencies—linguistic, psychosocietal, and cultural—still remain. In this case, as previously mentioned, there are advantages and disadvantages to my ‘outsider-ness,’ as my lack of ‘native’ knowledge of certain cultural experiences and psychosocial phenomena may actually have assisted me in the analyses undertaken here. Indeed, just the fact that the language in which the interviews were conducted is not my mother tongue most likely illuminated the central findings of this research. 3.5.4 The need for and importance of an integration of these disciplines This study focuses on the use of language (primarily, pronouns and tenses) in the participants’ narratives, in an attempt to understand the larger relationship between individual discourse and societal discourse. This involves a three-pronged exploration, of: 1) the individual narrator/encoder and his narratives; 2) his particular psychological world and perceptions of himself; and 3) his perceptions of his surrounding society and its expectations of him. A thorough investigation of this nature clearly requires multiple disciplines, perspectives, and approaches. Indeed, the field of linguistics alone would supply a rich analysis of the first prong, but have very little to contribute to the second two. The field of clinical psychology, on the other hand, would provide an appropriate approach to the second prong, but would not have much to offer to the third and would have difficulty fully exploring the first on its own. Social psychology would furnish a good basis for an exploration of the third prong, but would not be able to adequately study the first two. Finally, narrative inquiry as a paradigm is a requirement for the first prong, and, in my opinion, strongly recommended as a methodology for the other two, but lacks a firm theoretical and 95 conceptual framework with which the main research questions could be explored. As aforementioned, research on the use of pronouns and tenses in discourse is relatively sparse in the domain of academic psychology; the work that does exist is unfortunately unidisciplinary, with little connection to the relevant literature in other fields. This area of research clearly exists on the border between the two academic disciplines, but there still seems to be a disconnect between the psychological and semiotic/linguistic literature. While this burgeoning field of research has made important contributions to the psychological literature, the linguistic assumptions that are made frequently appear superficial due to their lack of support from the semiotic content world, and may not do justice to the important findings therein. This is equally true for social psychology and sociology, wherein discourse analysis places far less emphasis on the specifics and mechanics of linguistic forms and patterns, and much greater focus on the manners in which people and things are constructed through talk and interaction. Furthermore, psychological discursive analysis focuses more on the extra-linguistic message by proposing hypotheses for the personal and social meanings, motivations, and significance within the discourse. Indeed, the sociopsychological study of discourse is essentially a study of extra-linguistic messages, and appears to have no direct need to be associated with a specific linguistic theory. The relevant research on specific linguistic forms and patterns does exist, in abundance, within the academic confines of linguistics, semiotics, and literature, but the majority of these studies similarly make little attempt (and apparently find little need) to connect their respective findings to the fields of psychology or sociology. Indeed, the sign-oriented approach to linguistics focuses on the non-random distribution of language and the invariant meanings therein. The extralinguistic message—a presumably plausible hypothesis that can be accepted and shared by encoders and decoders—not in itself considered part of the linguistic theory, is then inferred and proposed. While this extra-linguistic inference is often an important part of an analysis, it is not viewed as a requirement or as the ‘ultimate answer’ therein, and is generally seen as secondary to the linguistic findings. Thus, a significant gap in the literature exists due to this lack of interdisciplinary work. Linguistics and social psychology, as academic fields, generally function independently of each other and do not appear to find it necessary to even enter into a shared discussion. I propose, however, that both are missing essential parts of the ‘larger puzzle,’ and might thus complement each other well. The field of psychology seems to have begun to recognize the import of the type of discourse analysis that has been conducted within linguistic circles for years, and it appears that the time is ripe for an interdisciplinary turn within these academic domains. Most encouraging from the perspective of the present study is the claim of Pennebaker et al. (2003), that “Future research must begin exploring the nature of pronouns and other particles in much greater detail. … 96 Some very basic psychometric work is needed on pronouns and other particles at the word level to disentangle their mathematical and psychological meaning” (p. 571). This research study, with its foundation in a methodical integration of a sociopsychological conceptual and theoretical framework, a narrative paradigmatic and discourse analytical methodological framework, and theory and analytical tools from sign-oriented linguistics, aims to fill this gap and make a meaningful contribution to all three academic fields. Essentially, this study asks psychosociological questions about the psychological experiences of the participants, as reflected in the linguistic phenomena revealed in their discourse. The focus lies on both the patterned meanings of the linguistics and the larger individual and societal implications of the extra-linguistic messages within the discourse. The research question reflects the interdisciplinary nature of the study, as the focus is not only on the drivers’ discourse or communicative strategies (as might be the case in a strictly linguistic study), nor is it only on their coping strategies or resilience (as might be the case in a strictly psychological study), nor is it only on the societal norms and cultural discourses surrounding them (as might be the case in a strictly sociological study). Rather, each of these factors is a central ingredient in the attempt to understand the individual phenomenological experiences of these bus drivers who underwent TAs, and to explore their feelings and attitudes about these experiences and their behavior therein. The discursive patterns evidenced in their narratives can be viewed as a possible window into their perceptions regarding their behavior and what their society expects of them, and similarly, their comments about resilience and coping with fear can inform a greater understanding of the meaning and messages behind the communicative strategies they utilize. It is through this manner of spiraling attention to the multiple theories and methodologies that a deep comprehension of the holistic picture under study can be reached. 3.6 THE ANALYTICAL TOOLS: A SPIRALING MIXED-METHOD TECHNIQUE Keeping in mind the fundamental analytical ‘building blocks’ delineated above, this section presents the specific analytical tools used in this study. A variety of tools were employed—some in concert and some separately—just as a number of theoretical approaches and perspectives influenced the methodology overall. Although psychological studies are (at least) primarily quantitative and this methodology is overwhelmingly preferred in the ‘hard sciences,’ Kroger and Wood (1998) suggested that quantitative-minded researchers must ask what the numbers mean, rather than taking them at face value, and that discourse analysis should be entered into as a means to “warrant (‘validate’) claims made via statistical tests and to explicate the basis for those claims” (p. 270). In this manner, the conventional sequence involving qualitative analysis as a preliminary exploration in order to justify conducting a supposedly more rigorous quantitative analysis is 97 actually reversed, leading to a challenging of “the conventional privileging of quantification in the search for understanding” (Kroger & Wood, 1998, p. 270). The present study privileges qualitative analysis and ways of knowing, while utilizing a mixed methodology. The term, “mixed methods,” has come to refer to myriad analytical techniques and combinations, but the central criterion involves a concerted effort to integrate the quantitative and qualitative tools or methodological approaches utilized, rather than simply using them in sequence or with no ultimate dialogue between them. As Teddlie and Tashakkori (2009) stated, “A requirement for truly mixed designs is the presence of two or more clearly identifiable (sets of) inferences, each gleaned from the findings of a strand of the study, followed by a deliberate attempt to integrate these inferences” (p. 142). Part of the motivation for the choice of a mixed methodology came out of the nature of this research as multidisciplinary, which automatically opens it up to more diverse audiences. Given the varied perspectives and opinions across 9 disciplines , a greater attempt was made to allow the study to hold up to a wider range of standards for rigor and trustworthiness. The particular operationalization of mixed methods employed herein involves the use of both qualitative and quantitative techniques to analyze solely qualitative data, which falls under the category of what Denzin (1978) has termed methodological triangulation. The specific research design follows Morse’s (2003) notational representation of QUAL+quan, indicating a “qualitatively driven, quantitative simultaneous design” (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009, p. 143). The mixed design adopted here was not only simultaneous but also spiraling, involving an “oscillat[ion] between methods, both conceptually and practically” (Mendlinger & Cwikel, 2008, p. 280). Different analytical contexts and questions often called for different methodologies and tools, and throughout the analyses, I widened and narrowed the analytical magnifying glass, constantly redefining the analytical unit in focus. Analysis was conducted primarily on the narratives of the terror attacks experienced by the participants (TANs), and/or other significant emotionally charged narratives. However, there were cases when the focus was placed on another narrative within a participant’s interview; or his life narrative (LN); or multiple narratives on a related topic within a single interview; or ‘parallel’ narratives across both of a participant’s interviews; or multiple narratives on a similar topic across several participants’ interviews. The same methods were utilized in all analyses conducted, and the same set of analyses was 9 To wit, Kroger and Wood (1998) wrote, “Discourse analysis is primarily a qualitative methodology and is thus often treated with the same disdain that some psychologists have for all forms of qualitative research” (p. 270). Furthermore, Coetzee and Rau (2009) claimed, “All researchers interested in revealing human meanings and motives accept that interpretive, qualitative methodology provides the key to understanding how people perceive and experience their worlds. However, advocating the use of interpretive and qualitative approaches—either to supplement or even replace objective, quantitative approaches—brings its own set of challenges” (pp. 1-2). And Morse (2008) referred to the unfortunate but common dismissive criticism received by qualitative researchers by academics in a variety of other fields: “It’s only a qualitative study!” 98 conducted for all participants. However, I have attempted to present the final interpretive results and conclusions in the most thorough, well-grounded, and efficient manner—which has sometimes necessitated that different analyses were ultimately shown for different participants. Indeed, constant and countless methodological and interpretive decisions were made throughout this project, all of which were painstakingly considered and reconsidered. I cannot claim that any of these choices are not without possible problematic issues, or that another researcher might make the same ‘judgment calls.’ But wherever possible, I have attempted to make my decision-making process transparent and manifest, by presenting my reasoning and justifications. Below is a detailed delineation of the specific methods employed throughout this study, in various contexts and in a spiraling manner, in an attempt to best serve these analytical decisions. 3.6.1 Quantitative data analysis methodology The quantitative linguistic data analysis methods utilized in this research involve counting, tabulating, and mathematically processing the distribution of specific discursive phenomena within both the form and content of the participants’ discourse. Within the form, the primary focus is on the participants’ uses of pronouns and tenses; while in the content, the lens is trained on numbers of lines dedicated to particular topics or narratives. This semiotic methodology of discourse and text analysis has been developed over the past 35 years, and various aspects and applications of the method have been published in an extensive number of anthologies, monographs, and articles (see, for example, Andrews & Tobin, 1997; Aphek & Tobin, 1988, 1989/1990; Blum-Kulka, Tobin, & Nir, 1981; Contini-Morava & Sussman Goldberg, 1995; Contini-Morava & Tobin, 2000; Davis, Gorup, & Stern, 2006; De Jonge & Tobin, 2011; Dreer, 2007; Gorlach, 2004; Kirsner, ContiniMorava, & Rodriguez-Bachiller, 2004; Perez & Sagy, 2011; Perez & Tobin, 2009, 2010, 2014; Perez, Tobin, & Sagy, 2010; Reid, Otheguy, & Stern, 2002; Tobin, 1988, 1989a, 1993, 2002, 2005; Tobin & Perez, 2009; Waisman, 2010). This type of analysis has also been applied in many theses and dissertations to discourse of diverse styles and registers, in both spoken and written form, across many languages—from literary and poetic texts to fortune-telling sessions, from political speeches to Six Day War stories, from kibbutz assembly meetings to children’s games, and even in trance parties. The counting method used here is particularly similar to that used by Tobin (1989b) in his analysis of the textual use of the ‘present’ tense in the Holocaust memoir of Zivia Lubetkin, in which the uses of tense were tabulated and compared systematically throughout the narrative, in relation to all the characters. Tobin (1995) also employed this method in his examination of the non-random use of certain MIH root and conjugation systems in a wide variety of spoken and written texts, including well-known novels such as A. B. Yehoshua’s A Late Divorce. In the majority of the analyses conducted within this study, the primary unit of analysis was 99 each participant’s main TAN, and the primary pronominal foci were the first-person pronouns (‘I’ and ‘we’) and both singular second-person pronouns (‘you (MS)’ and ‘you (FS)’), in addition to the three primary tenses (‘past,’ ‘present,’ and ‘future’). Third-person pronouns were excluded from the analyses, for two related reasons: 1) they were not a focus of inquiry for the study; and 2) the rate of occurrence of these pronouns was most often nominal across all the TANs. Exceptional cases were analyzed differently when necessary and noted accordingly. Pronouns within each narrative under analysis were counted and re-counted at least three times, using a categorized table constructed for this express purpose. Pronouns within dialogue (e.g., the “you” within “And then I said, ‘You are crazy’”), as well as interpersonal talk directed specifically at me within the interview context (e.g., “Do you want coffee?” or “Do you understand?”) were tabulated separately, and not included in the totals. Again, exceptional cases were analyzed differently when necessary. When overall trends across entire interviews are referenced, the data are based on a computerized word tabulation, executed through the Atlas.ti qualitative analysis program. Because this function generated a simple list of every word and its occurrence in each interview, it was able to provide a representative, albeit very general and superficial, picture of pronominal use. Thus, these data were used only as a background for comparison or contextualization, and never as the primary basis for an analytical interpretation. The total number of lines of text in each narrative under analysis was also counted, which was often an interpretive endeavor in itself. Indeed, while some of the TANs and LNs had clear beginning points (e.g., whatever came after my request for the participant to tell the story) and ending points (e.g., the participant volunteering a concluding statement such as, “That’s that”), most of them were much more fluid and multifaceted. Some began spontaneously from within the LN, while others never really ended and simply flowed into a new narrative on a related topic, such as the phenomenon of terrorism in Israel, or coping with the emotional aftermath of the TA. Therefore, the very act of ‘deciding’ where a narrative began and ended often felt like a problematic subjective imposition upon the text, from my own position above and outside of it. This was a methodological issue with which I often grappled, and so was careful to make extensive reflexive notes as to my reasoning and justifications. The ratio of pronouns per line was also calculated for each TAN, relative to the total number of lines of the given narrative. Percentages of uses of each pronoun were then computed, relative to the total number of the four pronouns of inquiry used within the TAN, and percentages of uses of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of relevant tense-marked verbs within the TAN. Finally, a graph was produced in order to view the distribution of collocations of each pronoun with each tense. The graph included both the absolute values (represented by the bars themselves) as well as the computed percentages (as notations above each bar). Raw numbers 100 were included in order to provide a picture of the absolute data, and percentages were included in order to express the relative value as a fraction of the whole—thus providing a picture of the relationship between the quantitative data points (Dreer, 2011). With regard to quantitative content analyses, numbers of lines of text relating to a given topic were tabulated and percentages were computed relative to the entirety of the unit of analysis. For instance, some analyses involved determining how much of a participant’s LN was dedicated to the TA, and how much related to his life before or after the TA. Others focused on a comparison of text volume dedicated to various topics or mini-narratives within the TAN, across both the BLI and FUI of a given participant (in which case, the BLI narrative was considered 100% text volume, and the FUI was compared to that baseline). In general, tables were used to present the data, and graphs were generated when it appeared necessary or particularly useful to provide visual illustration. Teddlie and Tashakkori (2009) referred to “the process of converting [qualitative] data into numbers that can be statistically analyzed” as quantitizing data (p. 27), but I would label the process undertaken herein as more of a quantitized processing of the data. In this sense, qualitative data was tabulated quantitatively, but because the participant pool was so small, few psychometric statistics were conducted in this study. The Chi-square test with Yates correction for continuity has been shown to be useful to some degree when testing for independence in a contingency table, so this statistical test was utilized to judge the quantitative significance of the comparative analyses— particularly those among multiple participants, and between the BLIs and FUIs within the relevant participants. Ultimately, however, basic mathematical logic was employed wherever possible when interpreting the quantitative results. That is, if a participant used one pronoun across 85% of his TAN and another only 5% of the time, or if 70% of his LN was dedicated to the topic of the TA, these data were regarded as representative of substantial and meaningful disparities. In any case, other than where expressly noted as part of a statistical test result, the use of the word, “significant,” throughout this dissertation should be understood only in its lay meaning. 3.6.2 Qualitative data analysis methodology 3.6.2.1 Central macro- and micro-level analytical questions As discussed above, the primary dichotomy within the analytical process conducted herein is that of content vs. form, and thus the central questions guiding all levels and stages of analysis regarded what the interviewees said vs. how they said it. The basic questions with regard to content were: 1) How was the TA perceived, comprehended, and narrated by the interviewees?; 2) How did the interviewees describe their strategies of coping with fear and stress, and processes of meaningmaking therein?; 3) How did the interviewees view their life as a whole and the place of the TA they experienced within it, and what meaning did they attach therein?; and 4) When, how, and why 101 did certain societal perceptions and cultural discourses (e.g., behavioral expectations such as masculinity and heroism) emerge manifestly in the interviewees’ discourse, and what was the attendant meaning of these issues to the interviewees? These questions were asked on both the individual and collective/comparative levels; that is, both within each participant’s pair of interviews and among all of the interview texts. This level of questioning remained very much on the surface of the interviewees’ discourse, focusing almost solely on what they specifically said about these topics and performing very little, if any, interpretive analyses or inquiring as to what they may have meant to express ‘between the lines.’ Indeed, much of this level of analysis focused on the question-and-answer part of the interviews, as some of these topics came up only as a result of my direct questioning. The basic questions with regard to form were: 1) How was the narrative of the TA constructed (e.g., what was included vs. omitted or silenced; chronological vs. thematic organization)?; 2) Which discursive signs and patterns were repeated throughout a narrative or interview as a whole, and what meaning may be ascribed to these patterns?; and 3) How did central themes or issues express themselves latently rather than manifestly in the discourse? This level of analysis existed almost entirely below the surface of the spoken discourse, searching for semiotic patterns that likely were not or could not be apparent to the interviewee himself, and which require multifaceted interpretation and the proposing of hypotheses with regard to motivation and significance. On the micro-level and within the operational implementation of the analyses, the following five questions were considered at all points and stages throughout: 1) Why is this word or phrase used here, and not in other places?; 2) What is the meaning, both in this sentence/context and in the larger picture, of the usage of this word or phrase here?; 3) How else could this be phrased, and how might the meaning change as a result?; 4) Are there any discernible patterns of usage of certain words or phrases?; and 5) What does this discursive distribution and/or communicative strategy express or reflect about this individual, and about the society and culture in which he lives? 3.6.2.2 Holistic and categorical content processing After reading through the entirety of each transcript following its completion, while making extensive notes regarding my initial holistic impressions, the first main analytical task comprised creating a series of tables organized by general topic. Each entire transcript was read through again, this time extracting chunks of text and copying them into corresponding tables. Although this type of qualitative data processing would be classified by Lieblich et al. (1998) as within the categorical-content mode of reading a narrative, an attempt was made to follow a more holistic narrative approach within the categorization process. In this sense, the extracted text sections were 102 always lengthy and centered on the relevant narrative, and included the surrounding context (e.g., the prompting question)—never did they consist of a single sentence or description. In addition, any conclusions reached through this analytical step were always brought back into a holistic perspective on the individual and his life experiences, and ‘checked’ against the larger context. Some of the topics appeared across all of the interviews, in part because they were an integral part of the questions guide, while other topics were unique to each interview. Every interview generated tables for the TAN, the LN, and narratives of the participant’s experiences in the army. There were also generally tables on the topics of coping, fear, guilt and responsibility, fate, and feelings about being a bus driver in Israeli society. The total number of tables for each participant ranged from ten to twenty, and included comparisons between the BLIs and FUIs. Rather than providing categorized data for a full content analysis, these tables primarily formed the basis for triangulation after certain form-related hypotheses emerged. For instance, if my analyses of a given participant’s particular pattern of pronoun use seemed to suggest a sense of ambivalence surrounding his emotional responses to the TA he experienced, my next analytical step was to turn to the table of his discussions of fear and/or other emotions, or of coping in general, in order to attempt to understand his own manifest beliefs, attitudes, or perspectives on the issue. It was often from these tables that I chose which quotations to bring into the final write-up of each analysis, in order to best highlight the phenomenon under discussion. The majority of the tables were created initially by B.A. students of mine, within the framework of two successive individualized Guided Research courses in the Department of Psychology at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev. After being extensively familiarized with the project, its research goals, and its theoretical, conceptual, and methodological backgrounds, the students were then each assigned a single participant and charged with listening to the original interview recordings while checking over the existing transcripts for accuracy (with the secondary purpose of becoming familiar with ‘their’ participant). The students then worked on creating their own qualitative analyses of their case, according to the themes in content and/or form that emerged from the process of categorizing the interviews into these tables. All tables were submitted to me for examination and consultation on a weekly basis, and I worked closely and individually with the students throughout their analysis process. At the conclusion of the courses, all final versions of tables were turned over to me for further use in the study. Some of the students’ analyses ultimately proved a valuable resource for me throughout my own process, and often (with their express consent, of course) provided a jumping-off point for deeper analyses that I later conducted. 103 3.6.2.3 Pronoun ‘poetry’ The next method used is essentially the qualitative counterpart to the quantitative tabulation of pronouns and tenses, and is based upon an analytical step within the Listening Guide, which was initially proposed as a methodology by Carol Gilligan in the 1990s. It has been used in various forms and fashions by her students and colleagues over the years (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; see also Gilligan, Spencer, Weinberg, & Bertsch, 2003, for a review), but only recently fully explicated as a specific method for the analysis and interpretation of qualitative data. I learned of this unique technique in an advanced qualitative methods course, and later participated in a workshop dedicated to teaching the method, led by a student of Gilligan’s, Dr. Miriam Raider-Roth. The Listening Guide is a psychological analytical method that “draws on voice, resonance, and relationship as ports of entry into the human psyche,” with the primary framing questions: “Who is speaking and to whom, telling what stories about relationship, in what societal and cultural frameworks…?” (Gilligan et al., 2003, p. 157, 159). The methodology is feminist and relational at its foundation, and prescribes a series of (at least) four sequential listenings to analyze and interpret the range of perspectives and interpersonal dynamics, or voices, within a text. The first step is listening for the plot, which involves creating the ‘story’ of the interview, describing the relational view therein, and listening for and becoming conscious of the interviewer’s own response. The second step is listening for the voice of the self, or creating what have been referred to as ‘I poems’ (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Gilligan et al., 2003). The primary purpose therein is to focus on both the form (cadence and rhythm) and content of the ‘I’-voice of the narrator, and to expose an “associative stream of consciousness carried by a first-person voice, cutting across or running through a narrative rather than being contained by the structure of full sentences” (Gilligan et al., 2003, p. 163). The Listening Guide sets out two central rules for the construction of an ‘I poem’ from a section of text: 1) marking (by underlining, highlighting, or otherwise selecting) every occurrence of the ‘I’ pronoun along with the collocated verb and any other accompanying words that appear important for the given phrase; and 2) keeping all the marked phrases in their original sequence (Gilligan et al., 2003). All of the marked phrases are then extracted (in order) from the original text excerpt and placed together, each on a separate line, as if in a poem. Stanzas may be created and are often readily apparent, but are not necessary to the analytical method. The third step of the Listening Guide is listening for contrapuntal voices, wherein the various parts of the interview text that may be more directly related to the research question are identified and distinguished (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Gilligan et al., 2003). This may require multiple additional readings, each focused specifically on a particular aspect of the content or form of the told story. This listening is shaped most by the researcher’s questions, which are inherently connected to the theoretical framework of the study and/or any issues that emerged from the 104 previous listenings. Finally, the fourth step is composing an analysis, in which all the previous steps are synthesized into a coherent and comprehensive interpretation of the text and the narrator (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Gilligan et al., 2003). This doctoral research is closely aligned with the Listening Guide’s central assertion that multiple listenings are methodologically necessary because the human psyche is not monotonic, but rather, contrapuntal (Gilligan et al., 2003). Just as many voices and perspectives can co-occur in an individual’s mind, so can be the case in one’s told stories. These voices are fluid and constantly changing, and can be in harmony or in tension—with each other, with other relational and interpersonal voices, and with the surrounding culture or context (Gilligan et al., 2003). Building upon these assumptions, the analytical method used for this aspect of the study expands on the second step and combines it with the third, and is referred to here as pronoun ‘poetry’. This method, which might be classified by Lieblich et al. (1998) as within the categorical-form mode of reading a narrative, involves generating a categorical breakdown of the entire given text excerpt, by pronoun, tense, verb, and/or any other relevant systematic linguistic distinction. Each of the pronouns along with their collocated verb, and additional contextual information where necessary, are extracted from the full text and condensed into lists representing each pronominal ‘voice.’ For demonstrative purposes, a text excerpt is presented below, with the relevant pronoun ‘voices’ marked with respective underlines, followed by a table of the generated pronoun ‘poetry’: “Wow I became a media star. Everyone all of a sudden what where who II said how do [you (MP)] say? (1) It’s fun when [they (MP)] are interested in you (MS) tons of phone calls but you (MS) say to yourself (MS) {‘Wow, God loves you (MS)}. God loves you (MS).’ Because if you (FS) had seen the bus you (FS) would have said ‘My Lord, how did it, how is it alive?’ Because thirty bullets. You (FS) know what that is? On, on the windshield wiper on the windows now they went over me my head too. A taller person than me, 1.75 meters, 80, his story would be over he takes it here. You (FS) understand? The bullets over the head. You (FS) see the bus, afterwards I went to the [Egged bus] branch, because myy bag was still there everything, [they] took my’bag. Two weeks later, I, for no particular reason, am straightening up, I see tons of glass shards inside the bag. Tons of glass. (4) {In the end, and you (FS) see the bus, my God, it is full of holes, I am saying “Whoah, God, III, really,” I ow-, owe God my’life}. Understand (FS)? (3) A huge miracle, let’s (FS) say that. A big miracle happened here, in my case, that I wasn’t hurt, there are some who, you (FS) know, were killed. PLEASE, IIII ((chuckles)).”i Table 3-4: Demonstrative example of pronoun ‘poetry’ ‘I’ POEM (8) ‘PAST’ TENSE (4) ‘PRESENT’ TENSE (4) 1 I became a media star 10I…am straightening up 2 11 I said I see tons of glass shards 13 --I am saying “Whoah, 9 I went to the [Egged God…” 14 bus] branch I ow-, owe God my’life --17 I wasn’t hurt ‘YOU (MS)’ POEM (1) 3 You say to yourself ‘Wow, God loves you. God loves you.’ 105 ‘YOU (FS)’ POEM (2 (7)) 4 (if you had seen the bus) 5 (you would have said ‘My Lord…’) 6 (You know what that is?) 7 (You understand?) 8 You see the bus 12 you see the bus 15 (Understand?) 16 (let’s say [a huge miracle]) 18 (you know) For the basic purposes of this study, the singular first-person and second-person pronouns (‘I,’ ‘you (MS),’ and ‘you (FS)’) stand at the central focus of this analytical step, and tense is distinguished wherever relevant. Thus, for the majority of the analyses conducted on the participants’ texts, ‘poems’ were generated for the RPa ‘I,’ the PPr ‘I,’ the MS ‘you,’ and the FS ‘you.’ Other ‘poems’ were added or substituted wherever there was significant methodological justification for doing so. The entries in the ‘poems’ are numbered according to their order in the original text, following the Listening Guide’s sequential principle. In order to illustrate any apparent clusters in each ‘voice,’ three dashes signify a significant break10 in the given sequence, during which other pronouns or tenses were used. Paraphrasing is kept to a minimum, as great care is taken to preserve the original content, form, meaning, and feeling of each pronominal voice. Entries in parentheses signify direct talk with me within the narrative, as when the interviewee asked me a question (rhetorical or not) or interacted dialogically with me. This interpretive decision was a complex one, but emerged out of a desire to reflect the holistic picture of the text excerpt, including direct interaction with me, while also differentiating between analytically significant and insignificant uses of the various pronouns. Ultimately, the combination and expansion of the second and third steps of the Listening Guide provide the most useful and comprehensive analytical methodology for this facet of the research study. It allows a condensed view of each pronominal ‘voice,’ both separately and in context with the other ‘voices,’ and offers a qualitative view of the quantitative linguistic data already obtained from the text. This was often the most significant and interpretively meaningful step of analysis in this study, as the juxtaposition of the ‘poems’ made discursive patterns almost immediately apparent. Indeed, this type of processing of the data can almost effortlessly expose repetitions in content or form, vacillations between one or another pronominal ‘voice,’ and collocation patterns (e.g., ‘present’ tense with a particular pronoun, or ‘past’ tense with a particular topic or locus of action). Indeed, it was the first pronoun ‘poetry’ analysis of the first interview text that spawned the initial interpretive hypothesis that the FS ‘you’ is used predominantly in connection with sensory, traumatic, and/or emotionally charged content. 3.6.2.4 Longitudinal comparative analysis of mechanisms of narrative selection 3.6.2.4.1 Preparation of parallel narrative tables Through extensive comparative examination of the tables created in the content analysis step, all ‘parallel’ narratives (i.e., retellings of the ‘same’ narrative at different points in time) were 10 “Significance,” in this case, is relative and depends on the particular characteristics of the ‘poetry’ of each individual text. That is, if one text is characterized by many 3-entry gaps throughout its ‘poems’ but few gaps larger than that, then a 6-entry gap would be considered significant, while a 3-entry gap would not. If another text has few gaps at all, then a 3-entry gap would be considered significant in that case. 106 identified and highlighted for more intensive analysis. New tables were then created, juxtaposing each set of parallel narratives for direct comparison. This step focused exclusively on narratives, rather than argumentations or descriptive discussions. At minimum, each participant’s pair of interviews generated respective comparative tables for his BL and FU TANs and BL and FU LNs. Parallel narratives about coping, fear, and a variety of experiences as a bus driver were also often gleaned for most of the participants. In addition, lengthy narrative responses to certain questions (e.g., “What is an Israeli man, in your view?” or “How were your army experiences similar or different to your experience of the terror attack?”) which appeared in both interviews were also placed into comparative tables. Often, there were more than two parallel narratives, as certain narratives came up in different forms and elaborations multiple times across a given interview, and this generated comparative tables with three or even up to five columns. Given the relative dearth of literature on retelling and comparative qualitative analysis, this analytical technique has rarely been used, and thus can be considered innovative in many ways. Therefore, in an attempt to explain the various steps involved as clearly as possible, as analytical justification as well as offering a possible methodological contribution, a demonstrative sample of a comparative table is presented below. This illustrates the first step of this analysis, wherein parallel narratives are identified and juxtaposed. Table 3-5: Raw excerpt pair from parallel narrative table BASELINE INTERVIEW Let’s (FS) say like this I am driving, line [20], it was winter [February], rain. (2) Approach the [bus] stop, take on th’passengers, take, you (FS) know, [bus] passes money MONEY. Suddenly I see someone come to a standstill in front of {the bus. (2) I do [gesture] to himmm} like ‘Go past.’ (1) Because I want to put on the blinker and leave the [bus] stop. (1) And then he winks, winks his eye at me does like this ((winks at me)) I do to him like ‘What?’, [he] opens the zipper coat, takes ouut a rifle and ‘Brrrrrrrrrr’ ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)). ii FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW Uhh let’s (MS) say eight years, around 2001. A wintery day pretty wet, rain. I am driving. I remember that I passed [city center square] if you (FS) know Jerusalem I arrived next to the direction of [another square] at the [bus] stop, and then I see sommee somebody who is standing next to me. (3) /((chuckling)) And I do to him like go past./ Suddenly he takes out th’, zipper and Brrrrrr ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)) in automatic.iii Once the parallel narratives were placed next to each other, extensive work was done to break them down into parallel component cells, in order to most clearly view the similarities and differences across the texts. For instance, in the table of LNs, two parallel cells would distinguish the participant’s discussion in each narrative of his family; two others would differentiate his army service; and two others would signify, perhaps, his wedding. If a topic or statement did not appear in one narrative, that cell was left empty and the narrative would continue in the following cell. No part of any narrative was removed or taken out of its original order, and thus, if similar statements or topics appeared at different points in each narrative, this was still demarcated as an occurrence of parallel cells, but not in the same narrative order. In this manner, the holistic entirety of all of the 107 narratives was kept intact, allowing a relating to the surrounding discursive context of every cell and pair of cells. Each set of cells was also given a basic in vivo code or title (Charmaz, 2006), often including basic analytical notes as well, for my own ease in poring through the tables. This analytical step as a whole is illustrated in the table below. Table 3-6: Excerpt pair from parallel narrative table, broken into component cells ( CODE / NOTES ) “Let’s say” (both!) 8 years, around 2001 I am driving, winter, rain Get to the stop (more details about money in BL, more details about route in FU) I see someone standing, I do like ‘go past’ He winks at me He opens zipper and Brrrrrrr BASELINE INTERVIEW Let’s (FS) say like this I am driving, line [20], it was winter [February], rain. (2) Approach the [bus] stop, take on th’passengers, take, you (FS) know, [bus] passes money MONEY. Suddenly I see someone come to a standstill in front of {the bus. (2) I do [gesture] to himmm} like ‘Go past.’ (1) Because I want to put on the blinker and leave the [bus] stop. (1) And then he winks, winks his eye at me does like this ((winks at me)) I do to him like ‘What?’, [he] opens the zipper coat, takes ouut a rifle and ‘Brrrrrrrrrr’ ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)). FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW Uhh let’s (MS) say eight years, around 2001. A wintery day pretty wet, rain. I am driving. I remember that I passed [city center square] if you (FS) know Jerusalem I arrived next to the direction of [another square] at the [bus] stop, and then I see sommee somebody who is standing next to me. (3) /((chuckling)) And I do to him like go past./ Suddenly he takes out th’, zipper and Brrrrrr ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)) in automatic. Perhaps the most valuable contribution of this method lies in its ability to uncover and make readily apparent the highly analogous nature (when it exists) of two entirely separate narrative excerpts. In this research, as soon as the separate cells were created, a picture often immediately emerged of substantially similar content, structure, and chronological order, across narratives told three years apart. When this picture was achieved, the real analytical work began: a deep, microlevel investigation of patterns in both form and content, in what was told and what was not told, and in what was emphasized and what was minimized. To this end, after the parallel table was fully broken down into its component cells, the next step of the longitudinal comparative analysis involved rereading the parsed narratives both separately and as a series of comparative units in order to locate and identify any uses of mechanisms of narrative selection (Spector-Mersel, 2011). 3.6.2.4.2 Spector-Mersel’s model of narrative selection This analytical step is based on Spector-Mersel (2011)’s model for interpreting narratives, which views the construction of narrative identities as a process of selection from the vast repository of facts, events, and experiences in the narrator’s life history. In this sense, every selfnarrative is seen as the result of a series of (usually unconscious) decisions regarding what and how to tell. In particular, the narrative act is conceived of “as consisting of six mechanisms of selection through which biographical facts are chosen, filtered, and sorted, with the purpose of confirming a 108 particular end point” (EP) (Spector-Mersel, 2011, p. 172). These six mechanisms of narrative selection (MNSs) are delineated below, along with their most recognizable identifying characteristics, in both form and content: 1) Inclusion: Representation of facts, events, and periods that are compatible with the narrative’s EP, thereby confirming it 2) Omission: Non-reporting of facts, events, and periods that are irrelevant to the EP a) Content: Non-reporting of themes b) Form: Lack of representation of full life periods/stages 3) Silencing: Non-reporting of facts, events, and periods that contradict the EP (Appears identical to omission, but has a very different motivation) a) Content: Non-reporting of themes b) Form: Lack of representation of full life periods/stages 4) Sharpening: Emphasis given to facts, events, and periods compatible with the EP a) Content: Superlatives (e.g., “very,” “a lot,” “much”); greater detail b) Form: Repetition of words; emphasis in tone or body language (e.g., louder voice or yelling, use of body such as pounding on table); emphasis in emotion (e.g., crying, laughing, chuckling, intakes or outtakes of breath); greater length/volume (i.e., over-representation); dialogue; mininarratives; specific or demarcated placement (e.g., at beginning or end of narrative) 5) Flattening: Condensing of facts, events, and periods because either: a) they are irrelevant to or contradict the EP (In this case, flattening resembles omission or silencing, differing only in intensity—rather than telling nothing, telling only a little bit) Form: Shortened or condensed length/volume b) they relate to two opposing parts of the EP (In this case, flattening serves a dual function—mentioning a fact, event, or period while also asserting its insignificance) Content: Double message—superlative expressions juxtaposed with flattening expressions 6) ‘Appropriate’ meaning attribution: Conferring to facts, events, and periods (which perhaps in ‘real time’ had a different or no meaning) a significance that is compatible with the EP a) Content: Telling with a meaning that fits the EP b) Form: Use of different pronouns or active/passive phrasing; demarcation or setting-apart of particular narratives or parts of them Spector-Mersel’s (2011) model seeks to pinpoint the expressions of these MNSs within the narrative, in order to posit and determine the identity claimed therein by the narrator. Within this model, a single narrative can have multiple—and even contradictory—EPs, but each EP can be recognizable through a thorough analysis of the MNSs that are utilized to compose and reinforce it, and therein to claim a narrative identity. This method is holistic and interpretive by nature, and follows two fundamental principles. The first is treating the story as a whole unit, which presumes that the MNSs operate in concert and cohesion, complementing each other in service of claiming a particular EP (Spector-Mersel, 2011). This allows for an inclusive analysis under the assumption that the same mechanisms may appear throughout a narrative or interview, across topics and contexts, performing a similar function in their reinforcement of a given EP. The second principle is regard for content and form, which has already been discussed as a guiding tenet of this study. The method employed under Spector-Mersel’s (2011) narrative selection model is most 109 effective when a two-phased interviewing technique has been used for data collection, such as Rosenthal’s (1993, 2005) method, which produces a main narrative—the response to a single, open-ended question (e.g., “Tell me your life story”) with no further interruption or intervention by the interviewer—and a question-and-answer portion, which tends to generate more directed and abbreviated responses to more specific questions posed by the interviewer. Spector-Mersel’s (2011) analytical method involves both a “text mapping” of the main narrative, and a comparative analysis between the main narrative and the relevant responses in the question-and-answer section of the interview. For instance, if, after being asked to talk about his family, a participant effusively remarks that his wife and children are the most important people in his life and that family is “everything” to him, but had not mentioned them at all in his main life narrative, this would raise an analytical ‘red flag.’ Similarly, if certain experiences or details were expressed very differently in the life story than in the question-and-answer portion of the interview, this would, in SpectorMersel’s (2011) view, warrant a deeper investigation of the MSNs at work. In this manner, the main narrative and specific responses to pointed questions are systematically juxtaposed and analyzed in order to arrive at the EP(s), and in turn, to understand the identity being claimed. 3.6.2.4.3 Parallel narrative comparison: A new analytical model The related analytical method used in this study essentially proposes a new and expanded application for Spector-Mersel’s (2011) model, as a tool for comparatively analyzing two parallel narratives, rather than (or in addition to) investigating differences between the main narratives and the later responses to specific questions. In this case, the EP as a tool for claimed identity is not the central focus of the analysis; rather, the point of interest is essentially a longitudinal EP. Rather than comparing two pieces of discourse on the same topic in the same interview in an attempt to understand the message and identity of the narrator at that point in time, the goal here is to compare two parallel narratives given at two points in time, in order to understand the message of what has occurred in the interval between the tellings. A deep analysis of what has been included in one narrative but omitted or silenced in the other, or sharpened in one but flattened in the other, may suggest a great deal about the individual’s processes of coping and meaning-making over time. To this end, all tables of parallel narratives were analyzed intensively, with a focus on all six MNSs and their varied expressions in the texts. This analytical work often took months (or years) at a time, going over and over the pairs of cells, looking for patterns in both form and content, both within each narrative and across the comparative unit. In the analysis process, extensive notes were made and significant words, phrases, and cells were highlighted and interpreted in order to build evidence for emerging hypotheses. Representative examples of each MNS were shown and discussed in this dissertation. A comprehensive perspective was ultimately 110 reached regarding each participant’s narrative processing and location on the continuum between ‘ease’ (i.e., a generally ‘improved’ state of coping with and after the TA, from BLI to FUI) and ‘dis-ease’ (i.e., a generally ‘deteriorated’ state of coping with and after the TA, from BLI to FUI). The specific hypotheses that guided this analysis are discussed further in Chapter 4. 3.7 METHODOLOGICAL, LOGISTICAL, AND ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS 3.7.1 Translation and transcription in a non-fluent and micro-analytical context First and foremost, a central consideration of this study surrounds the fact that I, the primary investigator, am very much an outsider regarding both the study population and Israeli society and culture. This has already been mentioned and will be discussed more below, but it should suffice to say here that while I am now fluent in Hebrew and constantly learning and analyzing its usage in daily discourse, it is not my mother tongue. The fact that the interviews were conducted in Hebrew (before I was fully fluent) thus had the potential to create difficulties in my ability to fully understand the interviewees at the time, in my ability to understand and analyze the transcripts afterwards, and in the necessity to translate the texts into English for later publication. Following Nikander (2008), this brief section seeks to engage in a process of “acknowledging, embracing and discussing the dilemmas of translation and transcription,” in order to maximize my transparency and accountability as the primary researcher and translator, and attest to my attempts to ultimately produce “good enough transcripts” herein (p. 229). It has been suggested that the challenges involved in the creation of a “perfect translation” are “insurmountable,” as translations must simultaneously take into account issues of vocabulary, idioms, grammar, and conceptual levels of understanding (Ramírez-Esparza & Pennebaker, 2006, p. 216). Indeed, the extensive system of grammatical notations in this dissertation makes this clear—particularly in the need to distinguish the four forms of the ‘you’ pronoun, and in the lessthan-eloquent translations of the various Hebrew prepositions. However, the research area of translation in qualitative research, in general, and in discourse analysis, in particular, is sparse and under-theorized (Nikander, 2008; for an exception, see ten Have, 1999, pp. 93-4). Therefore, it is difficult to judge just how much of an impact (either positive or negative) these issues may have had throughout the conducting of this research (Riessman, personal communication). Each translation choice I made likely held a number of practical, ideological, theoretical, and analytical implications on the data and research process—only some of which have been possible to discern in a comprehensible way (Nikander, 2008). Although I always analyzed the original Hebrew transcripts and only translated for the final write-up, and while I spent a great deal of time consulting with native Israelis with regard to language use and equivalent translations, this nonetheless remains a potentially problematic issue. 111 The transcription process was also not without its complications, primarily because I did not feel qualified to perform it myself. I conducted the first interview not even two years after my immigration to Israel, and while I understood the vast majority of the participants’ speech at the time of each interview, not having a full vocabulary arsenal at my disposal while transcribing made my first attempt at doing so clearly inadequate. Nikander (2008) asserted that during the process of transcription, “practical compromises” often must be made between “the ideals of faithfulness to the original, the readability and accessibility of the final transcript, as well as time and space issues” (p. 226). Indeed, this was a difficult decision for me—as a researcher and as a ‘new immigrant.’ It felt highly problematic, and perhaps even inauthentic, to hand off such a critical stage of the research process to someone entirely external to it. Few researchers discuss this facet, but I have always viewed transcription as an integral part of the analysis process, rather than a precursor to it. However, I was far more concerned that my final transcriptions would be sub-par just because of my inadequate Hebrew, which would surely lead to far more serious damage to the analysis (and to the research project as a whole). Relatively early on in the data collection process, therefore, I decided to outsource the transcribing for the entire project. Finding qualified transcribers was a huge endeavor in itself, and the transcripts changed hands and underwent revisions many times throughout the study. Because of the micro-analytical nature of the research, the transcripts had to be particularly meticulous, including changes in tone, volume, and speed of speech; lengths of pauses; any signs of emotion (e.g., laughing, crying, audible inhaling or exhaling of breath); and extra-linguistic or contextual information (e.g., sound of a siren outside, or sound of the participant pounding on the table, or a comment made by the participant’s wife). Of course, it was also critical to include in the transcriptions all of my utterances, as these often held clues as to the interpersonal dynamic between myself and the interviewee, as well as allowing a more informed view of the flow of the narratives. Most importantly (and most problematic for the transcribers), because the MIH writing system is a consonant-based syllabary11 that indicates most vowels by diacritic marks (Dekel, 2010), all of the potentially ambiguous conjugated verb forms and pronominal inflections had to be notated with vowels. Thus, for instance, because the second-person MS and second-person FS forms of all verbs are written the same but pronounced differently, if the specific pronoun was not verbally collocated with the conjugated verb, vowels had to be inserted in order to allow a fully informed analysis of pronoun use within the transcripts. Because of all this, much time and energy was spent training and supervising the transcribers, checking their work and making joint interpretive decisions. This process, referred to by Nikander (2008) as “time consuming, messy, 11 Tobin (personal communication) explained that this type of writing system can be most clearly demonstrated in English through a popular advertisement from the New York City subway system, circa 1960, upon which was written, “IF U CN RD THS U CN B A SCRTRY.” 112 and imperfect” (p. 226), was extremely challenging for me and for the various transcribers. By the final version of each transcript, however, I generally felt highly confident about its accuracy. 3.7.2 Confidentiality and anonymity in a media-infused context The circumstances surrounding the often very specific details of each TA created some complications with regard to ensuring the confidentiality and anonymity of the participants. I camouflaged as many personal and potentially identifying details as possible, particularly in relation to the drivers themselves and their backgrounds, but doing so for the details of the TA each one experienced proved much more problematic. For instance, the particular date, season, exact location, and bus line number rarely comprised details that were crucial to understanding the narratives overall. However, the nature of each attack (e.g., suicide bombing on or near the bus, shooting, Molotov cocktail, or double bombing, as well as how many passengers were killed or wounded) was fundamental and often threaded throughout the narratives, and could potentially compromise the anonymity of the driver involved. Indeed, all of the TAs were reported, often extensively, in the Israeli television and print media at the time of their occurrence, and the drivers were all interviewed in person as well. In particular, because there have been only two bus bombings in Be’er Sheva to date, it would not be difficult to identify the drivers involved. A ‘rule of thumb’ of research with human participants is to make all efforts to camouflage personal details to the point that the participant himself would not be able to recognize himself in any write-ups of the research. This, unfortunately, was entirely impossible here, and throughout my process of writing up the results for this dissertation and for various publications, I have been plagued with concern as to whether enough of the privacy of the participants has been honored. Researching individuals who have spent even a limited amount of time in the public eye indeed presents some thorny ethical issues, which were grappled with consciously throughout this process. 3.7.3 The follow-up interview protocol In the interest of full disclosure for possible future users of the longitudinal comparative methodology presented here, it should be stated that the objective of collecting as many parallel narratives as possible in FUIs could potentially be problematic. I presented the purpose of the FUI to the participants as “checking in” with them and hearing about any changes in their lives in the interim between the interviews, and I also placed a greater emphasis on the narrative structure of the interview. The follow-up questions guide (to myself) included this introduction: “This time the interview will be structured a little differently. Last time we focused a lot on the terror attack that you experienced, and on your life as a bus driver. This time I would like to focus on you as a person, not just you as a bus driver. We will talk about the terror attack at some point, and I will ask you some of the same questions as I did last time, but mostly I would like to hear about you. … In general, this interview is 113 aimed at hearing your stories, not necessarily yes/no answers. I want to hear about you, about how you view the world, and about how you view your own life and life story.” I was initially concerned that the interviewees would question my motivations (e.g., whether I was ‘testing’ them, or whether I had lost the tapes from the first interview), or tell shorter narratives because they remembered or were influenced by what they had said before. I half-expected them to respond to my requests for these narratives with statements like, “Didn’t I tell you this already?” or “Why do you want to hear this again?” Thankfully and to my surprise, however, the participants never expressed any reservations about any of the questions I posed to them, and if they remembered what they had told me in their BLIs, they never expressed this. In fact, there were several occasions on which I had to remind them of something they had said (which carried its own potential ethical issues). I also often had to pretend as if I ‘just happened to remember’ some detail or narrative from the BLI, in an attempt to prompt them to talk about it. The participants were often surprised by my ‘good memory,’ and seemingly flattered that I had held such minute details in my mind over the course of such a long time. In reality, of course, I had spent hours (and years) poring over these transcripts, but could not actually express this to the participants. There were times that this felt inauthentic to me, but I was not able to find another way to explain the comparative angle of the study without possibly causing the interviewees to feel ‘tested.’ This part of the methodology may require further refining in future studies. 3.7.4 The effects of the interviews and potential conflicts There are ethical considerations involved in this research, such as the fact that it required interviewees—some of whom were interviewed at a relatively early juncture after their terror experience—to revisit painful and possibly traumatic memories, and in fact, frequently to relive them during their story-telling. This had the potential to cause more and fresh pain, and this was discussed with the participants as a possible outcome, prior to beginning their interview. I made sure to ‘check in’ with them, both throughout the interviews as well as afterwards, in order to monitor them for such influences on their psychological well-being. The consent form included contact information for professional resources, and the referring practitioner at Egged Bus Company was apprised of the interview date in order to do her own monitoring. I also found that the involvement of the psychologist and social worker at Egged was associated with its own complications. I often had to consider the conflict between their roles as recruiters of study participants and as providers of therapeutic care to these individuals. I have no way of knowing how the study was presented to potential participants, and whether one clinician pushed her clients to participate, or whether the other chose not to recommend participation at all at some point. In the beginning, both clinicians were also anxious to hear the results of the study, even before a thorough analysis was conducted, and at times this put a strain on our working 114 relationship. I found that I needed to be careful not to discuss certain aspects of the study with the clinicians, as they showed an inclination to use this information in their therapeutic meetings with the participants. All these ethical and logistical issues were kept under consideration throughout the conducting of this study, and should be held in mind while reading this dissertation as well. 3.8 ‘VALIDITY’ AND TRUSTWORTHINESS: THE QUALITY OF THE RESEARCH STUDY Throughout this chapter, I have described the various methodological decisions and spiraling nature of analyses conducted in this research study. I have discussed the analytical requirements of the study material, as well as the need for an interdisciplinary perspective overall. I have often included my own perspective as the researcher, within the process of making myriad interpretations and decisions and attempting to document them as thoroughly as possible. This type of transparency and reflexivity is of utmost importance in qualitative research, as the guidelines for ‘quality control’ in this context are much different from those commonly used within the quantitative research world. Indeed, conventional, positivistic terms such as reliability and validity—universally accepted and commonplace within quantitative research—represent much more complex and problematic issues within interpretive inquiry. There have been many attempts in past decades to set criteria for judging the academic rigor, interpretive ‘validity,’ “goodness,” and quality of qualitative research (Guba & Lincoln, 2005), with varying success and consensus. Indeed, much is written about best practices for qualitative research (see Tracy, 2010, for a review). Perhaps the most widely recognized and adopted set of criteria to purportedly measure whether “the research findings of an inquiry are worth paying attention to,” has been proposed under the umbrella concept of trustworthiness, comprised of four components: credibility, transferability, dependability, and confirmability (Lincoln & Guba, 1985, p. 290). For the purposes of judging the quality and worthiness of the present study, I have also found Tracy’s (2010) “eight ‘big-tent’ criteria for excellent qualitative research” to be most useful. In addition to essentially encompassing Lincoln and Guba’s (1985) basic benchmarks, perhaps the greatest import of this set of guidelines lies in its separation between means and ends; that is, that it proposes “common markers of goodness [that are not tied] to specific paradigmatic practices or crafts” (Tracy, 2010, p. 839). These eight criteria are: 1) worthy topic; 2) rich rigor; 3) sincerity; 4) credibility; 5) resonance; 6) significant contribution; 7) ethics; and 8) meaningful coherence (Tracy, 2010). This section delineates each of the criteria, and discusses how this research purports to meet each one. 3.8.1 Worthy topic According to Tracy (2010), the first criterion is met by a research topic that is relevant, timely, significant, and interesting. I assert that this dissertation deals with a number of topics that can be described as such, chiefly among them: coping with terror; markers of coping in the 115 discourse of narratives of stressful life events; the interplay between individual discourse and societal discourse; the meanings and mechanisms of retelling and meaning-making over time; and the use of pronouns and tenses as discursive markers of a narrator’s beliefs, attributions, and system of meanings. As mentioned previously, the period during which the study was conducted was actually calmer in terms of bus-related TAs in Israel, so in this sense it could be argued that there is less timeliness and even, perhaps, less relevance within this immediate context. However, I suggest that the majority of contentions put forth in this research are not at all necessarily dependent on type of TA (or even type of potentially traumatic experience), or on most of the specific characteristics of the participants. Research on dealing with terror and trauma is (unfortunately) becoming more acutely relevant and timely throughout the world, and to the extent that at least some of the interpretations in this study can be generalized, this topic is quite worthy. In addition, perhaps the greatest indication of the significance and ‘interestingness’ of the topic was apparent every time I presented my research to audiences of a variety of backgrounds and academic levels—B.A., M.A., and Ph.D. students, as well as professors and academics in disciplines such as psychology, sociology, anthropology, linguistics, social work, and narratology. Non-Israeli audiences often remarked that the work was “fascinating,” and I received many requests for copies of my published articles. Israeli audience members frequently remarked that they had never before been aware of the discursive phenomena I discussed, and I often witnessed smiles of realization as they became conscious of apparently long-taken-for-granted assumptions about their native tongue. Nearly every time I encountered Israelis who had previously heard me speak about my research, they told me they were now acutely sensitized to these discursive phenomena, and all of a sudden ‘saw’ them everywhere. As one of my reflexive concerns was that my status as an outsider to the Hebrew language may have caused me to be too sensitive to or literal about the discourse, these audience reactions felt particularly important and validating. 3.8.2 Rich rigor Tracy’s (2010) second criterion for excellent qualitative research, rich rigor, is met by “sufficient, abundant, appropriate, and complex” use of theoretical constructs, data and time in the field, sample(s), context(s), and data collection and analysis processes (p. 840). I submit that the comprehensiveness of this chapter attests to the rigor of the methodology and analysis process of this study. With regard to the sample, although my initial hope had been for at least five additional participants—and I still do believe that this would have enhanced the research—the circumstances did not comply, despite great efforts on my part. Serendipitously, the consequent choice to conduct FUIs actually added great richness and value to the research, as well as an unforeseen methodological contribution. The duration of a study is often seen as a measure of credibility, and 116 while I did not plan for it to last as long as it has, I claim that my prolonged engagement with the field, data, analysis, and write-up process produced a significantly rigorous piece of scholarship. Along these lines, Lincoln and Guba (1985)’s concept of confirmability refers to the degree to which the analysis and results are grounded in and supported by the data material garnered and whether inferences are logical (Schwandt, 1997; Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009). Similarly, Wood and Kroger (2000) contended that discourse analysis is centered not as much on the development of patterns and interpretations, as on “how you justify your identification of patterns, how you ground your interpretations” (p. 95). All hypotheses in this study emerged from countless hours of poring over both quantitative and qualitative data tables, and all interpretations are thoroughly grounded in the discourse. I have cited multiple and varied examples for each of my hypotheses, and have looked deeply into the texts for appropriate justifications on the grammatical, structural, and contextual levels. In any case, all versions of transcripts, tables, and tabulations, as well as consent forms, participant questionnaires and materials, and field notes, will be kept in my possession indefinitely, should the need for a confirmability audit arise (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009). In the interest of sincere (see below) and full disclosure, it feels necessary to note that I did not scrutinize to the same extent every single page of every transcript of the interviews I conducted for this study. As there were over 600 pages of transcribed text from just the five participants presented in this dissertation, early on in the analysis process I realized it was not realistically possible (at least for me, given my limitations in Hebrew) to become intimately familiar with every word of every interview. Although I initially invested many, many hours attempting to do so, I finally decided to reframe the primary analytical unit for the majority of the analyses as the TANs. This decision emerged, in part, out of my desire to conduct thorough, consistent, deep, microfocused, and grounded analyses, and out of the attendant logistical issues involved. However, methodological and theoretical justifications exist here as well. When a narrative researcher purports to focus on an entire interview (often upwards of 100 pages) as the unit of analysis, it may be interpreted as suggesting that a ‘complete’ understanding of the interviewee was achieved because the text was analyzed in its entirety. In this sense, for me to attempt to take into account every narrative and every argumentation on every page of the texts I procured from my participants is to assume that this is ‘enough’ information on which to base my interpretations. But the truth is that there is never ‘enough,’ and never will be. There is an infinite amount of additional potential ‘texts’—which may support, confirm, weaken, challenge, and/or contradict every bit of information provided in any given interview—existing every day within the interviewee himself. And the narrative researcher has no access to all this. So this study represents my best—‘good enough’—attempt to analyze the material at my disposal, to ground my hypotheses in this material, and to make interpretations accordingly, while never purporting to (and never suggesting that I 117 should) know ‘everything’ about the topic, the participants, or the phenomena under investigation. 3.8.3 Sincerity The third criterion, sincerity, requires “self-reflexivity about subjective values, biases, and inclinations of the researcher(s)” as well as transparency with regard to the methods and methodological challenges therein (Tracy, 2010, p. 840). This criterion is particularly relevant in this case due to the unique complexities of its interpersonal context. As previously discussed, this research brought together an American-born, primarily English-speaking, relatively new immigrant, ‘outsider’ researcher with native Israeli, Hebrew-speaking, ‘insider’ participants. This pairing involves a number of advantages and disadvantages, and questions and issues that I have grappled with throughout the research. For instance, how can a researcher who is not an inborn member of the culture or native speaker of the participants’ language presume to understand what they are speaking about? What might be overlooked because of these ‘deficiencies,’ and what might be ‘gained’ or viewed differently through non-native eyes? And how can an ‘outsider’ researcher successfully analyze (and translate for other audiences) not only the discourse used by her interviewees, but also the sentiments, unspoken assumptions, and dynamics of the interview? This research study relies on the claim that a sign-oriented semiotic analysis of language can illuminate discursive patterns and communicative strategies therein. Because the gendered pronouns at the heart of the patterns and strategies uncovered in this study do not exist in English, many of the most important and interesting findings would not have existed had the interviews not been conducted in the interviewees’ mother tongue. Of course, by the same token, not conducting the interviews in the interviewer’s mother tongue led to an inevitable loss of some level of understanding of the nuances and perhaps even outright parts of the ‘insider’ experience. This may have had an effect on the interview dynamic and on the interviewees’ perception of whether they were being fully understood. Ultimately, I claim that it is possible to wade one’s way through interviews and interactional phenomena in one’s foreign language and cultural context, and come out the other side with meaningful and valuable results. This should be undertaken, however, alongside a conscious and reflexive perspective on the problematic nature of this endeavor. While all of the various approaches and methods described throughout this chapter provide excellent sources of triangulation for the interpretations put forth, at the core of this research process has been my own primary attempt to trust my experience with and understanding of narrative inquiry, and to allow my own eye to guide, at least in part, the analysis and the decisions made therein. However, it is important to note that my ‘eye’ was not unassisted. Throughout the research process, I took part in several analysis groups comprised of M.A. and Ph.D. students utilizing qualitative methods, meeting frequently to collaboratively analyze our interview materials. 118 I also participated in a Reflexivity Group, where I was able to discuss with other qualitative-minded students a variety of dilemmas (personal, methodological, and ethical) emerging from our respective research processes. Shenton (2004) referred to frequent debriefing sessions and peer scrutiny of the research project as two important strategies for ensuring trustworthiness, and credibility in particular, in qualitative inquiries. Indeed, my participation in these groups allowed me to see how native Israelis read my interviewees’ discourse, and to clarify any issues or questions that may have been unclear to me because of my ‘outsider-ness.’ It also provided me with opportunities to check emerging hypotheses with others and receive feedback, from their perspectives both as researchers and as inborn members of the society under study. Perhaps the most valuable benefit of these groups lay in the ongoing opportunity to practice qualitatively analyzing texts and continually hone my skills, knowledge base, and capacity for reflexivity as a qualitative researcher, with all the inherent uncertainties and dilemmas. In this sense, I view my involvement in these academic communities as a fundamental and important part of the qualitative methodology of this research. Josselson (2000) described a process of consulting with colleagues regarding her various interpretations, as part of ensuring reliability; ultimately, however, she suggested “present[ing] several cases in great detail and mak[ing] my interpretive process apparent; the reader is thereby free to agree or disagree with my interpretations” (p. 466). In general, the multitude of decisions regarding what to analyze and how to analyze it were ultimately made by myself, after much deliberation and consultation with my advisors as well as an array of colleagues. The decisions regarding what to present in this dissertation were even more difficult, as I often found myself wanting to include every analysis, every insightful result, and every illustrative quotation. In particular, the decision to present the results in a participant-focused manner, essentially as minicase studies, and to focus on some of the participants more than others, was not an easy one. It should suffice to say that this dissertation, as lengthy as it is, actually represents only a small fraction of the analyses conducted and findings reached. But what is included is the most sincere possible representation of the study. Tracy (2010) stated, “Sincerity means that the research is marked by honesty and transparency about the researcher’s biases, goals, and foibles as well as about how these played a role in the methods, joys, and mistakes of the research” (p. 841). I have gone to great lengths to infuse this dissertation with this kind of honesty and transparency. 3.8.4 Credibility The fourth criterion for excellent qualitative research, credibility, is in line with Lincoln and Guba’s (1985) original definition of the term, which refers to whether or not the research findings, interpretations, and reconstructions are “credible to the constructors of the original multiple 119 realities” (p. 296). This is, essentially, a reflection of the strength, accuracy, and authenticity of the connection between the study participants’ lived experience and the representation of those experiences in the study report (Schwandt, 1997). Credibility is thought to be obtained by research using “thick description, concrete detail, explication of tacit (nontextual) knowledge, and showing rather than telling” (Tracy, 2010, p. 840). Thick description, a term first coined by Geertz (1973) within the discipline of anthropology, refers to a process of writing up the results of a qualitative research study using much detail, examples, and discussion of the relevant concepts, contexts, and data within their culturally-situated meanings. Thick description is most often achieved by showing—that is, providing enough information so as to allow the audience to arrive at their own conclusions and understandings about the phenomena—rather than simply telling the reader which assertions are being made and what to think about them (Tracy, 2010). Josselson (2009) concurred, claiming the importance of the researcher’s taking “interpretive authority (Chase, 1996)” for his/her own understandings of the participant’s words, while simultaneously “liberally reproduc[ing]” those words as substantiation for the credibility of his/her interpretations (p. 652). Only in this manner, Geertz (1973) suggested, can meanings and psychosocial phenomena be sufficiently communicated by or through the researcher, about the participants, to the reader or audience. Tracy (2010) also suggested an honoring of multivocality, the inclusion of multiple and varied voices in the qualitative research report, and Guba and Lincoln (2005) referred to the inquirer posture within the constructivist paradigm as “passionate participant” and “facilitator of multivoice reconstruction” (p. 196). As Tracy (2010) contended, “Qualitative researchers do not put words in members’ mouths, but rather attend to viewpoints that diverge with those of the majority or with the author” (p. 844). This study is principally interested in the particular situated reality of each participant, and seeks to identify patterns across participants only where the relevant experiences and/or contexts converged sufficiently to allow such collective conclusions. Multiple hypotheses and possible interpretations are always offered, and wherever evidence for a greater likelihood for a particular hypothesis is either present or lacking, this is stated. I claim that the abundance of participant material, deep analyses, and varied perspectives presented throughout this dissertation satisfy the criteria of thick description, showing rather than telling, and multivocality. In addition, triangulation of data and/or analyses is often employed in the service of obtaining credibility, in an attempt to garner similar or converging interpretations through multiple avenues and perspectives. Denzin (1978) defined credibility by means of triangulation as a convergence on the same conclusion by or through at least two different researchers, sources or types of data, or theoretical frameworks, and referred particularly to a ‘within-method’ or methodological triangulation, which utilizes multiple analytical tools and techniques for interpretation and hypothesis-testing. While this conceptualization of credibility remains popular in 120 the field of qualitative inquiry, its realist assumption that the “same conclusion” can be reached, particularly by two divergent researchers, seems problematic. I see narrative inquiry, specifically, as far too subjective, interpretive, and multidimensional to be able to achieve this criterion. However, an attempt to garner a multifaceted, cohesive, and comprehensive perspective on a research question is critical to quality qualitative research, and this study subscribes to Ellingson’s (2008) concept of crystallization, the practice of the utilization of multiple and various theoretical perspectives, conceptual lenses, types and sources of data, analytical methods, and researcher viewpoints, in order to arrive at a rich and multidimensional perspective, rather than a single “same conclusion.” This comes from a post-structural and performative viewpoint, and follows Tracy’s (2010) assertion that the goal of crystallization is “not to provide researchers with a more valid singular truth, but to open up a more complex, in-depth, but still thoroughly partial, understanding of the issue” (p. 844). The interdisciplinary and multifocal methodological framework and collection of analytical tools—both quantitative and qualitative, and focused on both content and form—employed in this research study attempts to accomplish this type of crystallization. Along with these components of credibility, Lincoln and Guba’s (1985) criterion of dependability refers generally to the “ability of the human instrument to yield consistent results” (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009, p. 296); that is, the likelihood that similar conclusions might be reached if the ‘same’ research process were to be conducted by someone other than the original researcher. While this also appears problematic within the view of narrative inquiry as particular, contextually-situated, and reflexively-influenced by the researcher’s identity, background, and stance, it does seem warranted to document the research process as accurately and comprehensively as possible. Yin’s (1994) suggestion that dependability is achieved through careful and detailed record-keeping with regard to research procedures, field notes, analytical and interpretive processes, and various consultations has thus been implemented throughout the conducting of this study. Again, all files related to the study will be kept in my possession indefinitely for the purposes of a dependability audit (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009). Furthermore, all analyses were conducted on the original Hebrew transcripts, and, according to Nikander (2008), “hiding the original data from the reader’s view clearly violates the ‘validity through transparency and access’ principle” (p. 227). Therefore, in an effort to maximize my own accountability as translator and in order to allow native speakers to audit (and challenge, if need be) my translations and analytical interpretations (Nikander, 2008), every translated excerpt presented in the body of this dissertation has attached to it an endnote denoting its original Hebrew quotation in its entirety (see Appendix 4). However, it must be noted that, while great care has been taken to document every step of the data collection and analysis procedures, this was done more in service of the criteria of rigor and sincerity than of the possibility of achieving dependability. Indeed, the research process 121 undertaken here was, in many ways, unmistakably unique to the interpersonal contexts at hand. The texts that were garnered and analyzed for this study emerged from a single irreplicable moment in time, between pairs of individuals who will never exist in that specific form, set of emotions, and mental state again; in a set of personal, interpersonal, familial, societal, and cultural contexts that will never exist in that exact form again; within a co-constructed relationship that will never exist again; about topics that will never appear, be experienced, or be perceived exactly the same way to or by either of us. This is, in part, because the interview experience itself changed us in infinite and indistinguishable ways. And so, within and taking into account all of these limitations, I made my analytical decisions, attempting to do so and document them as credibly as possible. 3.8.5 Resonance The fifth criterion, resonance, necessitates that the research “influences, affects, or moves” its audience through “aesthetic, evocative representation,” “naturalistic generalizations,” and/or “transferable findings” (Tracy, 2010, p. 840). In the case of this study, this criterion feels closely connected to two of the previous criteria: the worthiness of the topic and sincerity. I have already stated that this study has sparked great interest and even personal insights in large audiences of students, professors, and other academic colleagues of mine. That a single presentation of these research findings might have the power to create even a tiny shift in thinking or in one’s consciousness of his/her use of language is an indication of this criterion having been met. Regarding the intersection between sincerity and resonance, I myself have been deeply moved by Josselson and Lieblich’s (1996) call for an end to the use of “passive voice, bloodless prose, and hedged statements” in narrative research in psychology, and by their contention that the discipline of psychology might be better served by researchers who: “tell the real stories of their work—to consider their own role as coparticipants in designing the questions, choosing participants, shaping the context, and structuring the results; choosing the language that seems to them suitable for sharing what they learn; and, in general, reflecting on the complexities of the process of knowing” (p. 651). This served as one of my most important foci in my process of writing this dissertation, as I have striven for a balance between the personal and the professional; between the student of psychology and the doctoral researcher attempting to forge a new interdisciplinary path; and between the academic analyst and the new immigrant just hoping and trying to understand the foreign culture into which I have implanted myself. Tracy (2010) wrote that “The use of the first person voice (e.g., ‘I said,’ or ‘They reacted to me by…’) effectively and appropriately reminds readers of the researcher’s presence and influence in participating and interpreting the scene” (p. 842). I have thus used the first-person voice throughout this dissertation not only as a representation of myself, but as a conduit for the reflexivity, sincerity, and resonance of this piece of work. It is also, of course, the pronoun that most honors a research study on the uses of pronouns and their meanings. 122 Tracy’s (2010) reference to transferability and generalizability relates to “a study’s potential to be valuable across a variety of contexts or situations” (p. 845), and loosely correlates to Lincoln and Guba’s (1985) original conceptualization of transferability, which refers to the more specific potential for generalization or application of the data, interpretations, or conclusions, from the particular context of the given research study to other or larger contexts (Schwandt, 1997). As the use of this term in positivistic, quantitative research circles generally involves ‘objective’ statistics, random representational sampling, minimization of ‘noise’ or confounding variables, and isolation of phenomena under study from their culturally-situated context, this conceptualization is entirely irrelevant and of no use in a phenomenological, interpretive qualitative inquiry (Tracy, 2010). However, the aspiration of a quality naturalistic research study to garner findings that “can be extrapolated beyond the immediate confines of the site, both theoretically and practically” (Charmaz, 2005, p. 528) is important and relevant—but as an audience-driven, rather than researcher-driven, process (Tracy, 2010). In this sense, Tracy (2010) regarded transferability as having been achieved “when readers feel as though the story of the research overlaps with their own situation and they intuitively transfer the research to their own action” (p. 845). The present study provides a rich context for this, as the language phenomena under investigation here are universal in MIH and other gendered languages, and are readily apparent and abound in daily conversation. Similarly, while the focus is on bus drivers’ narratives of TAs, I claim that the discursive patterns found are not limited to extreme cases of (potential) trauma; rather, they may occur in a variety of types of narratives, particularly those describing myriad life stressors. Josselson (2006) discussed the complexities involved in the accumulation of knowledge gained through narrative research, and the manner in which the findings of this study are presented has been influenced by my own desire to maximize the potential for both naturalistic generalization (Stake & Trumbull, 1982) and amalgamation of conclusions with other relevant studies. Josselson (2006) suggested that narrative knowledge might potentially be merged through, among others, the linguistic ground of comparison; that is, focus should be placed on the ways in which researchers “report the language patterns of their participants as the participants, in different languages, try to give verbal shape to their experiences” (p. 8). Josselson (2006) also discussed the comparative ground of case studies, suggesting that the potential for amalgamation of narrative knowledge may lie in “layering studies in a way that establishes correspondence and difference,” and building “support for repeated patterns that remain situated rather than generalized” (p. 8). Although Josselson (2006) primarily referred to the accumulation of knowledge across narrative studies, I see these recommendations as entirely relevant within studies as well, and as having the potential to promote a research report that may lend itself more readily to transferability. I have thus attempted to follow both of these recommendations in this dissertation. Discursive 123 patterns and communicative strategies are uncovered in each individual participant’s narratives, and situated within his particular personal, experiential, and social context. Patterns across participants are also illustrated, in an attempt to understand which factors may cut across contexts and may thus be further transferable. The results themselves are arranged in a case study format—both separate and layered, pointing out both similarities and divergences, and seeking both the particular and the generalizable. Perhaps the greatest potential for transferability of the study’s findings exists in its theoretical, methodological, and practical contributions (discussed below), all of which are not at all bound to the specific context of the study population or research question. 3.8.6 Significant contribution The sixth criterion, significant contribution, can take place in the conceptual/theoretical, practical, moral, methodological, and/or heuristic domains, and asks, among other things, whether the study extends knowledge, improves practice, generates ongoing research, and/or liberates or empowers (Tracy, 2010). This research has the potential to do at least the first three items in this list, and, as it is not situated directly in a feminist, critical, or social action-oriented paradigm, does not attempt the latter. As was stated in the introductory chapter, this study offers theoretical, methodological, and practical innovations, all of which may potentially bring with them significant impacts in their respective domains. This research proposes an interdisciplinary bridge between the psychological, sociological, and linguistic fields, which may lead to topical contributions to the literatures on coping with terror; narrative methods and representations of coping; coping and meaning-making over time; and the relationship between individual and societal discourse. This study also proposes a two-pronged methodological contribution: 1) a new form of mixed methods, represented in the integration of sociopsychological discourse analysis and sign-oriented semiotic analytical tools; and 2) a new model for a longitudinal comparative analysis of mechanisms of narrative selection and a variety of other discursive and communicative strategies. And finally, and closely related, this research has produced a potential practical contribution: the Narrative Method for Assessment of Psychosocial Processing of Stressful Life Events (NMAPP). All of these contributions will, of course, be discussed further in the coming chapters. 3.8.7 Ethics The seventh criterion requires that excellent qualitative research be ethical, considering not only procedural ethics (e.g., accepted guidelines for research on ‘human subjects’), but also situational, cultural-specific, and relational ethics (Tracy, 2010). Tracy (2010) also referred to exiting ethics, which relates to the manner in which the researcher leaves the domain of the research and shares its conclusions with a wider audience. The various ethical considerations that were most prominent and problematic throughout the process of this research were already mentioned above, 124 but a discussion of the overall ethical stance of the study seems warranted here. This study followed all appropriate and necessary ethical guidelines, including undergoing a formal ethical review board; explaining the research goals to and obtaining informed consent from the participants; and following conventional protocol for ensuring confidentiality and anonymity. Having attended at least a dozen different university courses, workshops, and specialized trainings in qualitative research methods and interviewing techniques, I believe that I have sufficient qualifications and skill to “conduct an empathic, nonjudgmental and respectful interview,” in addition to having “thought through and given reasonable consideration to the possible effects the inquiry may have on the participants,” and being “prepared to deal with consequences in an ethically sound way” (Josselson, 2007, p. 558). However, some ethical issues simply remain thornily present within this study. My concerns about attempting to analyze and interpret often deeply culturally-ingrained issues and phenomena as an outsider to this culture continue to plague me. So too does the possibility that my ‘outsider-ness,’ or my age, or my gender, or my academic status may have influenced the interviews to an extent that may render my interpretations of the data and ultimate conclusions much less reliable than I contend. My inability to fully ‘read’ the interpersonal dynamics between myself and my interviewees, with regard to their assumptions and expectations about me as well as possible perceptions of my expectations of them, may have impacted the nature of relational ethics therein. As a researcher, I subscribe most closely to what Ellis (2007) referred to as an ethic of care, which “recognizes and values mutual respect, dignity, and connectedness between researcher and researched, and between researchers and the communities in which they live and work” (p. 4). Alongside of this, however, I continue to struggle with issues of exiting ethics, particularly surrounding publications and presentations of facets of the research study. Indeed, with regard to the aforementioned difficulties involving the ensuring of anonymity in a media-infused context, Chase (1996) pointed to similar issues when participants are members of the same sub-community, in which case any attempt to “disguise” them would most likely prove futile. Josselson (2007) referred to Amia Lieblich’s recommendation that: “researchers in these instances…take great care to collaborate with participants about what will be published and…be ready to rescind any material the participant feels might be injurious to others or to their relationships with others. If we suspect at the outset that there is no way of doing the research such that the participants or the important others in their lives can be sufficiently disguised so as to be unrecognizable to others, then one simply cannot publish the research unless all those significantly mentioned read what is to be published and agree to its dissemination” (p. 554). This feels sincere, ethical, and honorable within many research contexts. However, in this case it causes another thorny issue, which simultaneously may simplify the original problem but make the above recommendation impossible to fulfill. This dissertation is written in English, as are the majority of articles containing findings from this research published thus far. With little command 125 of the language, how might the participants go about reading and approving the reports and interpretations of their data? Of course, the language barrier also creates a near-zero possibility that they, or likely most of the members of their nuclear and extended community, will ever personally encounter these write-ups. Again, this simultaneously represents a relief and a concern for me. Ethically, I have done ‘the best I can’ in this domain, and practically, it is likely that no problems will ever arise. Yet, writing about someone who does not possess the ability to read what has been written about him does not feel quite right. Perhaps, in the end, I can do little else but take comfort in Josselson’s (2007) contention that “Many, if not most, perhaps all, narrative researchers end their studies with some questions about the absolute ethicality of what they have carried out, although only a few have said so in print... This seems to me to be the most ethical position of all” (p. 559). 3.8.8 Meaningful coherence The final criterion, meaningful coherence, aims at determining how well the research study achieves its purported purpose and deals sufficiently with its purported topic (Tracy, 2010). This includes a successful use of appropriate methods and procedures, as well as the forging of a meaningful interconnection between the literature and theoretical foundation, research questions, findings, and interpretations (Tracy, 2010). I have made all attempts to ensure that this dissertation and the research it presents “hangs together well” (Tracy, 2010, p. 848), incorporating and integrating multiple perspectives, disciplines, and methodologies and methods, in addition to the analyses and discussion of those results, into a cohesive and coherent final product. With regard to existing literature upon which to ground some of the interpretations, I have encountered a surprising dearth of theoretical and/or empirical studies in several specific areas relevant to this research—for instance, with regard to retellings of multiple ‘parallel’ narratives; or sufficiently substantiated psycholinguistic investigations into pronoun and tense use in discourse; or any research at all on bus drivers in Israel. I have reviewed the literature that is currently in existence to the absolute best of my abilities, and, in retrospect, perhaps this lacuna was necessary to produce this dissertation. This study thus offers theoretical, methodological, and practical innovations in an attempt to fill these gaps in the various literatures, and endeavors to do so with as much meaningful coherence as possible. 126 CHAPTER 4 PRELIMINARY RESULTS AND FUNDAMENTAL HYPOTHESES 4.1 INTRODUCTION The process of analysis for this study began soon after the first interview was conducted, and was ongoing throughout and beyond the data collection phase. I found it useful to shift back and forth between the collection and analysis phases—similar to what Mendlinger and Cwikel (2008) referred to as “spiraling” through the data—as frequently an insight found in one domain was used to inform and revise the other, and vice versa. Almost immediately after the analysis process began, however, I noticed that something interesting was going on in the interviewees’ talk, and I realized that some of the basic discursive phenomena that I had begun to uncover would require a deep exploration and dedicated explication. I felt, then, and still do assert, that these preliminary findings regarding the participants’ discursive patterns are the essential building blocks of this research. And while they are innovative and important findings in and of themselves, for this study they should be viewed more as the starting point, rather than the end result. Indeed, the four central discursive patterns that were found represent the foundation for the basic hypotheses 1 that were later explored throughout the study, and for the comparative longitudinal analyses that paved the way for the proposal of a narrative tool for the analysis and assessment of coping with and processing of stressful life events. Essentially, the foundation for this study overall, and for the initial analyses of the interview texts, is the linguistic assumption that the non-random distribution of language within the drivers’ discourse may hold a specific set of meanings and messages—which may, in turn, offer insight into their inner psychological worlds. In particular, the initial guiding hypothesis is that the drivers’ patterned use of pronouns and tenses may reflect their feelings, attributions, and perceptions surrounding the terror attack (TA) they experienced2, and its place within their life narrative (LN) and life history. The results of the initial set of analyses did, indeed, uncover a series of patterns in the drivers’ texts, and a set of preliminary claims emerged regarding a possible psycholinguistic connection between their discourse and some of their specific perceptions and attributions. These claims were then extrapolated to produce a set of secondary psychosociolinguistic interpretive hypotheses, in two distinct directions: within-participant and across-participant. Finally, building upon and alongside the process of exploring these hypotheses, a set of hypotheses was asserted with regard to the practical implementation of all of these assumptions and findings. 1 2 The use here of the term, hypothesis, should not invoke and does not reflect a positivistic or experimental stance. Rather, it refers to an interpretive theory or supposition—subjective in many ways and not absolute. As Josselson (2009), following Denzin and Lincoln (2000), Glaser and Strauss (1967), and Polkinghorne (1988), contended, “Narrative inquiry does not test hypotheses; instead, patterns are inductively adduced, documented, and conceptualized” (p. 650). As according to qualitative interpretive methodologies, analyses with regard to these hypotheses are discussed here in terms of possible support or evidence for a given premise, rather than proof or confirmation. This analytical phase is entirely inductive, and all of these hypotheses emerged from the analyses conducted in a ‘bottom-up’ approach, rather than being adopted from external sources in a ‘top-down’ approach. This is also relevant for other particularly emotionally charged narratives (of particularly difficult life events), such as in Dani’s case, which will be discussed in Chapter 6. 127 Namely, this research study asserts the following: 1) there exists a number of specific, nonrandom patterns in the discourse of these drivers; 2) there exists a relationship between each individual driver’s discursive patterns and his relevant perceptions and attributions; 3) there exists a meta-connection between this relationship and each driver’s perceptions of his surrounding societal discourse, particularly with regard to coping and resilience, masculinity and heroism, and ‘acceptable’ behavior; and 4) this multifaceted dynamic relationship between the individual’s discourse, his inner psychological world, and his perceptions of the surrounding societal discourse can be utilized as a window to assess his longitudinal process of narrative processing of the event he experienced. This chapter first outlines the fundamental psycholinguistic results, organized by pronoun use and their interpreted meanings and messages. This first section explores the relationship between the individual participant and his manner of processing and narrating the TA experience, with a particular focus on the motivations behind his discursive choices. The second section connects these findings to a macro-level societal view, to assert the essential psychosociolinguistic hypotheses that guide the individualized analyses of the participants. This involves an exploration of the relationship between the manner in which the participants tell their stories and their perceptions of the expectations and cultural discourses of their surrounding society. The final section presents the central hypotheses that provide the foundation for the longitudinal withinparticipant comparative analyses, which aim to define trajectories toward ‘ease’ and ‘dis-ease’ in coping with and processing the TA experience, as viewed through the narrations of the stories. 4.2 FUNDAMENTAL PSYCHOLINGUISTIC FINDINGS: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL AND HIS INNER PSYCHOLOGICAL WORLD This section briefly presents the four central discursive patterns evident in the interviewees’ texts, along with basic and generalized hypotheses about the linguistic meaning and extra-linguistic messages of each phenomenon. Each discursive pattern will be more deeply explicated and demonstrated through “exemplar” texts (Mishler, 1990), along with lengthy discussions of (more individualized) potential interpretive hypotheses, within the framework of the following three results chapters. These findings relate primarily to the manners in which these bus drivers narrated their experiences, in particular, and in which their perceptions of the social and cultural discourse surrounding them may influence the personal discourse of its members, in general. Of course, one could always suggest that the linguistic patterns highlighted here are just common, ‘normal,’ or simply ‘how people talk,’ particularly within the context of narratives of stressful life events. This may or may not be the case. Nonetheless, I suggest that these discursive choices are not arbitrary and certainly not meaningless, no matter how common, conventional, and socially accepted they may be. That is to say, there are specific moments and contexts within which certain grammatical, 128 discursive, and communicative techniques are used, and there are other moments and contexts within which others are used. Embedded within these choices, there is a meaning, a message (or multiple messages), and a significance—in both the patterns of usage and the contexts surrounding them. I propose that the discursive choices made and communicative strategies used—as reflected in the form as well as in the manifest content—by the interviewees in telling their narratives serve to express their feelings and perceptions about themselves, their bus and work, their society and culture, and their sense of responsibility with regard to the terror event(s) they experienced. 4.2.1 Personalization and possession: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and phrases The interviewees showed a discursive pattern of personalizing or owning certain statements in a way that appeared to demonstrate a deeper motivation or communicative strategy. Some referred to their bus or parts of their bus as theirs or even themselves (e.g., “the passengers got onto my bus,” “the passengers got on to me3,” or “my door was warped”). Some referred to those who were injured or killed in the TA as their casualties. Others showed a tendency to separate themselves from the collective aspect of the TA and focus only on their own singular experience, narrating the event as if they were the only victims or ‘experiencers’ and referring to it as “what happened to me.” This personalization or possession of certain statements or topics may be understood as a corollary of a phenomenon known as extension of identity (Hyvärinen, personal communication), within which each driver’s own bus is viewed as his home, an extension of himself (and his self). Indeed, as these drivers spend at least eight hours a day on ‘their’ bus, and as many of them have driven on the same routes for long periods of time, it is understandable that they would come to view their bus as their own, as a personal possession or an extension of themselves. Another hypothesis regards the sense of personal responsibility to which the interviewees seemed to be referring in their discourse. Viewed in this vein, the drivers may see their own bus as not only their possession but also their territory, which must be protected against threat. This sense of (possibly over-)protectiveness may lead them to feel guilty or at fault in the face of evidence that they were unsuccessful at preventing, avoiding, or acting more proactively during the TA. Indeed, much of the discourse of these interviewees was riddled with the theme of guilt, responsibility, and self-judgment, in many different forms and contexts—particularly apparent in the significant number of statements that began with, “I should have…”, “I could have…”, “I wish I had…”, and “I wanted to…” Several drivers also expressed that they still (and always will) consider themselves to be soldiers, just without a uniform at the moment. Their sense of ownership and protectiveness can be viewed within this perspective, as they perceive their bus as both their battleground and their 3 The pronoun used here, “etzli”, was discussed at length in Chapter 3 (Section 3.5.2.3). An exact translation into English is impossible (although it is comparable to ‘chez’ in French), but it can be understood as ‘at me,’ ‘by me,’ ‘on me,’ or ‘in me,’ depending on the context. 129 territory to protect. Indeed, the oft-made references to their casualties evoked an image of the drivers as commanders on a battlefield, referring to soldiers in their charge who had been hit by enemy fire, or as captains of a ship, responsible for all of their passengers. I assert that it is through the discursive choices to personalize the statements made at these specific contextual moments that the participants expressed—both latently and manifestly—their feelings and attributions about themselves, their work, and their sense of responsibility with regard to the terror event that occurred. It could be argued that personalized statements about their own unique experiences and emotions represent, in fact, the most ‘logical’ response, given that I was interviewing them individually and asking them to tell their own specific stories. Perhaps it is only ‘natural’ that they would talk about their experiences from their own personalized perspective, and make no attempt to enter into the minds or experiences of the other ‘experiencers.’ And yet, not all of the interviewees spoke in the same manner at the same discursive points or about similar facets of their stories; the discourse of at least one participant stands as a contrast to this pattern. Therefore, it is plausible that careful analytical attention paid to the moments and contexts in which the drivers chose to use first-person possessive pronouns and phrases can illuminate the facets of the experience about which they feel the most personalized, possessive, and/or responsible. 4.2.2 Depersonalization and universalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun and/or third-person ‘others’ The interviewees showed a tendency to depersonalize, generalize, universalize, neutralize, and/or normalize their statements at times, and used two distinct means of doing so. The first was characterized by narrative moments in which the interviewees were clearly telling about a personal experience of their own, but chose to utilize a third-person ‘other’ or outside entity (such as “a bus driver” or “he”) as the subject. The second was demonstrated frequently across the various interview texts and contexts in which the unmarked second-person masculine singular (MS) pronoun (‘you’ (MS), or the generic ‘one’) was used in the general, generic, and/or impersonal sense. This was most commonly apparent in statements such as, “When you (MS) drive the bus every day…” and “You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be scared.” The two statements directly above utilize the same discursive strategy, and because both emerge out of a choice against the use of the ‘I’ pronoun, both have a depersonalized extralinguistic message. I assert, however, that the motivations behind each of them are different. The former comes out of a basic, conventional, and widely accepted generalization technique in Modern Israeli Hebrew (MIH). The entire structure of the sentence is unmarked, generic, and habitualized, as represented by the use of the proximate ‘present’ tense and the phrase, “every day.” The motivation here appears to be a manner of encouraging, or helping, or even requesting of, the decoder to understand and relate to the driver’s daily experiences and, perhaps, to himself. In this 130 sense, the speaker is attempting to generalize and collectivize the described behaviors, actions, feelings, or attitudes by means of discursively distancing them from himself and the personalized realm. Indeed, the personalized alternative—“When I drive the bus every day…”—places the experience solely in the world of the speaker/encoder and disallows the forging of any connection with or identification by the audience. In contrast, the statement, “When you (MS) drive the bus every day…” allows the decoder to imagine the action being performed by the everyman—an infinite repository of subjects that could even include the decoder himself. This may open up the discursive opportunity for greater understanding by the interviewer and/or wider audience. The statement, “You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be scared,” represents another category of motivation behind this pattern of pronoun use, which was also evident across the interview texts. In this sense, the motivation for generalization and universalization appears to be related to a more specific agenda—that is, a need for not just understanding, but acceptance. This use of the MS ‘you’ often emerged when the interviewees were describing moments of passivity or ‘weakness,’ in which they apparently didn’t act in accordance to their perceptions and definitions of ‘manliness’ or heroism, or in which they were being acted upon (particularly by the perpetrator of the TA), through no fault of their own. The agenda therein is to universalize and normalize the experience, behavior, or emotion in order to explain, justify, and/or attempt to garner (societal) approval for their role within it. When the interviewees were not able or willing to discuss ‘weak’ emotions in an ‘I’-statement, they often utilized the MS ‘you’ in order to acknowledge them covertly, under the ‘safe cover’ of the collectivized and normalized experience of them. Within this interpretation of the statement presented here, then, the encoder seems to believe that if he can convince the decoder that the ‘standard’ reaction to a TA would be fear, even from a macho and ‘acceptable’ generic Israeli man, then he can feel secure in a societal ‘approval’ of his own fear reaction—even if he himself does not approve of or is ambivalent about his role and behavior. 4.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun One of the most notable discursive tendencies exhibited by these interviewees was their unusual patterned use of the second-person feminine singular (FS) pronoun, in a non-generic, marked discursive context. This is perhaps the most central and innovative preliminary finding, as the pattern is particularly unconventional and yet, highly consistent (albeit to varying extents) across the participants. The analyses revealed that the FS ‘you’ was used most frequently in conjunction with the most difficult or stressful experiences described by the participants (most often within the terror attack narratives (TANs)), and particularly within the sensory, bodily, and highly emotional facets therein—such as looking, seeing, hearing, feeling, and being (afraid, wounded, etc.). Perhaps most significantly, it was found that the majority of the interviewees 131 uttered nearly the exact same statement at least once in their interviews: “You (FS) see the blood.” As previously mentioned, grammatical and discursive conventions in MIH dictate that an encoder, when not speaking directly in ‘I’-statements, would utilize the MS ‘you’ for generalized or generic statements, and the FS ‘you’ only for specific statements or questions directed at me (e.g., “Are you (FS) familiar with Jerusalem?”). That is, standard MIH pronoun usage norms involve a wide variety of contexts and statements that can be collocated with the MS ‘you,’ regardless of the actual gender (and number) of the audience. Furthermore, as discussed in Chapter 3, the marked FS ‘you’ is reserved for either generic or personalized statements only in all-female contexts, in situations in which two females are talking to each other, or in unusual instances when at least the majority of an audience is female and the speaker is an ideological feminist. Even in these cases it is perfectly socially acceptable to use the customary unmarked MS ‘you’ instead, or interchangeably. I thus walked into the interviews conducted for this study—all with male interviewees, all but one of whom were over the age of forty, and none of whom had made a declaration of being a feminist—assuming I would hear the FS ‘you’ only in discursive contexts directed specifically at me, in the current moment of the interview framework (e.g., asking if I wanted coffee or if I understood what was being said). It is, therefore, highly unusual that the interviewees made use of the FS ‘you’ within the narrative context of their TANs, while discussing their own experiences from their own perspectives. The frequency and extent of these usage patterns varied by participant, but the discursive phenomenon appeared in the texts of all of them. My initial hypothesis, which actually emerged during the process of the first interview conducted for the study, was that while the MS ‘you’ seems to serve as a ‘first-degree’ distancing technique, the FS ‘you’ represents a second degree of distancing, or even projection. That is, the former provides a discursive shift from the personalized ‘I’ to the unmarked generic subject, and the latter facilitates an even more directed bridge from the generic to the specific marked FS subject, which in this case was me. I felt, at the time, that this interviewee was making a concerted effort to discursively pull me into and inside of his story. In a sense, this is really just the ‘flip side of the coin’—the more distant the experiences and emotions are from the encoder, the closer they can be to the decoder; and the more the narrator attempts to personalize his discourse for the audience, the more removed it may necessarily be from himself. It can thus be hypothesized that the use of the FS ‘you’ is motivated by a desire to bring the (female) decoder in to the narrated story and experience, and the following section presents several hypotheses as to reasons behind this motivation. In particular, it must be acknowledged that I, personally, typify a particularly complicated decoder in this case. Not only was I clearly at least ten and up to 35 years younger than the majority of the interviewees (and generally appear even younger than that), but I am also plainly not a native Israeli. My obvious American accent may 132 have made my interviewees dubious about my ability to understand their words at all, much less the meanings and possible cultural significance behind them. In addition, my declared status as an advanced-level psychology student may have introduced a differential power dynamic. At least one of the interviewees interpreted my ‘title’ as being on par with a licensed clinical psychologist, and thus, as having the potential to influence his struggle for reparations for his TA-related injuries. It thus must be acknowledged that the interviewees’ need to bring me in to their discourse, as represented by their patterned use of the FS ‘you,’ could be hypothesized to be only an ‘artifact’ of our unique interview dynamic and context. I assert, however, that this discursive phenomenon is much deeper and more universal, as will be apparent in the specific analyses and results to follow. 4.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun The final pattern apparent throughout the interviewees’ discourse was a consistent tendency to use ‘I’-statements at points in their narratives at which they were describing their specific actions and agency—particularly before, during, and after the TA. It was indeed extremely rare to find a use of the MS or FS ‘you’ in narrations about what the drivers did actively at the scene of the attack. Thus, as represented in the common types of statements found in the texts: “You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) are not scared,” and “You (FS) see the blood,” but “I opened the door of the bus,” and “I picked her up and told her not to worry.” I assert that the interviewees’ use of the ‘I’ pronoun signifies a willingness and capacity to claim the experience, thought, feeling, or behavior as their own and as worthy of their ownership. This, in turn, suggests a self-evaluation of acceptance or approval of the described events and circumstances and their role therein, and a belief that their audience (and their surrounding society at large) would agree with this appraisal. Indeed, the participants appeared much more comfortable claiming ownership discursively of an experience or facet therein if it involved them playing an active role, at least, or performing in a manner that could be perceived as courageous, powerful, and heroic, at most. Thus, while the most difficult, upsetting, ‘weak,’ and potentially traumatic facets of the TA experience were relegated to the FS ‘you,’ and the facets about which the interviewees were ambivalent or uncertain were ascribed to the MS ‘you,’ the ‘I’ received all the action, agency, positive self-imagery, heroism, and glory. I contend that these linguistic patterns in the individual discourse of these drivers may essentially reflect and be influenced by a perceived message sent by the hegemonic cultural discourse of Israeli society; namely, that it is not ‘acceptable’ to be affected by an event, or to be passively acted upon therein. Rather, one must actively affect the event itself—being the hero, not the victim; the strong and powerful man, not the weakling. In this perceived view, only when he has behaved ‘correctly’ can the actor/narrator be proud of his actions, and, in turn, be given the 133 social opportunity or right to ownership of the experience, both experientially and discursively. 4.3 PSYCHOSOCIOLINGUISTIC INTERPRETIVE HYPOTHESES: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL, HIS INNER PSYCHOLOGICAL WORLD, AND HIS PERCEPTIONS OF THE SURROUNDING SOCIAL DISCOURSE The previous section put forth the fundamental psycholinguistic patterns upon which all the analyses presented in the coming chapters are based. To summarize, the central discursive patterns found across the interviews conducted for this study were the following: 1) First-person singular possessive pronouns (such as, ‘me,’ and, ‘my’) and phrases were frequently the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: a) describing an experience that he saw as personalized for himself; or b) attempting to personalize the experience for himself. 2) The second-person MS decoder (‘you’ (MS)) was frequently the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: a) describing an experience that he saw as depersonalized from himself; or b) attempting to distance the experience from himself; and/or c) describing an experience in which he behaved passively and/or was being acted upon (particularly by the perpetrator of the TA). 3) The second-person FS decoder (‘you’ (FS)) was frequently the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: a) describing difficult, stressful, and/or upsetting sensory experiences (particularly relating to the TA and its aftereffects); and/or b) describing something that he perceived as particularly important for me (the young, female, ‘new immigrant,’ advanced psychology student and interviewer) to know, understand, and/or vicariously experience. 4) The first-person singular encoder (‘I’) was frequently the subject of the sentence when the encoder was: a) describing an experience in which he was performing in an active, powerful, and/or ‘heroic’ manner; and/or b) describing an experience or behavior of which he approved, or saw as ‘acceptable.’ Furthermore, from a basic quantitative assessment of the pronoun and tense collocations in the interviewees’ TANs, the following clear patterns emerged: 1) The MS ‘you’ collocated most frequently with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense. 2) The FS ‘you’ collocated almost exclusively with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense. 3) The ‘I’ could be collocated with all tenses, with the following interpretive sub-patterns therein: a) Collocation of the ‘I’ with the ‘past’ (remote + experience-able) tense seemed to indicate a higher (more ‘ease-ful’) level of narrative processing and (appropriate) psychological distance. b) Collocation of the ‘I’ with the ‘present’ (proximate + experience-able) tense seemed to indicate a lower (more ‘dis-ease-ful’) level of narrative processing and a greater sense of proximity and salience (‘alive-ness’ of the narration). The preliminary analyses that were conducted in order to explore these patterns then led to the emergence of four interpretive hypotheses, some of which were introduced in part above. Indeed, when connecting the linguistic patterns to the stated personal feelings and attitudes of the interviewees about their experiences and their perceptions of their surrounding societal discourse, the following psychosociolinguistic hypotheses for their communicative strategies can be asserted: 134 1) First-person singular possessive pronouns and phrases are used in contexts in which the encoder feels particularly connected to or responsible for the experience he is describing. This can be viewed as a discursive technique that can pinpoint the speaker’s sense of responsibility and/or guilt (or lack thereof) regarding the narrated experience, and identification with facets therein (e.g., the victims—both animate and inanimate, such as the bus itself). A heightened focus on the speaker’s personalized sense of the experience appears to substitute for an acknowledgment of the collective nature of it in actuality. 2) The unmarked MS ‘you’ pronoun is used predominantly in contexts in which the encoder is making an attempt to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize what he is describing, out of a need to fit in with his surrounding society (and its norms). This can be viewed as a technique aimed at encouraging, assisting, or requesting of the decoder/ audience to relate in a generalized manner to the speaker himself and/or to particular aspects of the described experience. This may be utilized in neutral contexts, but may also come into play in connection to ‘less acceptable’ aspects of the experience, which are often steeped in ambivalence and uncertainty for the driver. This may particularly involve moments during which, he apparently believes, he could or should have acted, thought, or felt differently—and thus, sees himself as having not lived up to certain internalized personal, cultural, and/or societal expectations. The MS ‘you’ thus helps the encoder to send the message that the given issue or experience should be universal, or that he wishes it were universal. In this sense, if the behavior or feeling is presented as universal and ‘normal,’ then it can at least be a candidate for reinterpretation by the audience as ‘acceptable,’ in spite of the speaker’s ambivalence about it. 3) The marked FS ‘you’ pronoun is used predominantly in the most difficult experiential contexts, in which the encoder is making an attempt to connect specifically and personally with me (the young, female, ‘new immigrant,’ advanced psychology student and interviewer), and bring me in to what he is describing. This can be viewed as a technique aimed at dealing with the most difficult and emotionally charged aspects (the ‘unacceptable’ parts) of the described experience. These facets often simply do not lend themselves to universalization, generalization, or normalization, as they are frequently the most specific and specialized details of the event, such as the experience of seeing blood and smelling burning flesh. Because discursive ownership of these descriptions (by means of the ‘I’ pronoun) may be viewed, in the drivers’ eyes, as an acknowledgment of all the psychological ‘baggage’ that would likely accompany such an experience, the FS ‘you’ is most frequently employed to displace these difficult and charged statements, or to share them with the decoder. In this sense, the FS ‘you’ takes on all the facets of the experience that are viewed as ‘unacceptable’ within Israeli society—the trauma, the weakness, the moments of paralysis before the ‘correct’ measures were taken. This is perhaps the most complicated of the interpretive hypotheses, because it carries with it five potential sub-hypotheses with regard to motivation: a) An attempt to both distance the given feeling, statement, or experience from himself and 135 project it onto me—as represented in the (hypothetical) statement, “I bring you in to it so you can take it away from me.” b) An attempt to cause me to personally identify with or experience vicariously the given feeling, statement, or experience—as represented as, “I bring you in to it so you can share it with me.” c) An attempt to cope with the given feeling, statement, or experience—as represented as, “I bring you in to it so it won’t be so hard for me to handle/so you can take some of the burden off me.” d) An attempt to educate me—as represented as, “I bring you in to it as much as I can so you can try to understand it, because no one can understand it without going through it.” e) An attempt to connect, for the simple sake of connection—as represented as, “I bring you in to it as much as I can, because I feel so alone with it.” All of these motivational hypotheses are equally valid and plausible, and it may not always (or ever) be possible to discern with certainty which may have specifically contributed to a given interviewee’s use of the FS ‘you’ at any given discursive moment. Furthermore, this communicative strategy prompts the fundamental question as to whether a visceral, discursive connection with one’s audience—regardless of the motivation for doing so—can, in itself, help the narrator in some way to cope with or process the difficult aspects of a stressful life experience, such as a TA. It is possible that each of the interviewees used this discursive strategy for different reasons; each nonetheless forged a connection with me in the process, and by doing so seemed to find some relief from the troubling effects of the experience and from telling about it. It is nonetheless important for the purposes of this study to take notice of the particular statements and contexts in which the FS ‘you’ was utilized by each of the interviewees, in order to detect where their particular difficulties with the experience may lie. 4) The ‘I’ pronoun is used predominantly in relation to actions, feelings, or experiences that the encoder is willing and able to claim as his own, out of a degree of personal acceptance and/or a sense that the surrounding society would approve (particularly with regard to his behavior). This can be viewed as a technique aimed at calling attention to the ‘acceptable’ parts of the described experience, and sending the message to the audience that, “This is what I did right.” This is most commonly collocated with active and agentic behavior at the immediate scene of the TA, which positions the driver as masculine, powerful, heroic, and unfailingly Israeli (in his perception of the definitions of these terms). While he was in shock or passively experiencing the event, the ‘I’ is most often silenced, and transferred to either the MS or FS ‘you.’ But when he was acting, helping, or saving, the ‘I’ is recruited to express this unambiguously and unabashedly. Finally, extending these hypotheses about the interpretive relationship between the interviewees’ discourse, their inner psychological world, and their perceptions of the surrounding societal discourse, I propose the following three macro-level (across-participant) hypotheses: 1) The driver(s) who perceive(d) themselves or were perceived as having behaved most actively and ‘heroically’ before, during, and/or after the event use ‘I’ at a higher rate than the other interviewees. 2) The driver(s) who are/were most affected (psychologically, emotionally, and/or physically) by the event use the FS ‘you’ at a higher rate than the other interviewees. 136 3) The driver(s) who are/were most ambivalent about the ‘acceptability’ of their behavior before, during, and/or after the event use the MS ‘you’ at a higher rate than the other interviewees. Thus, the first major goal of this study is to analyze the drivers’ discourse in their baseline interviews (BLIs) only, investigating the relationship between their patterned communicative strategies, on the one hand, and their beliefs and attributions about the ‘acceptability’ of their behavior and feelings surrounding the stressful event they experienced, on the other. The following four chapters, comprising the Baseline Analyses section of this work, present this exploration from an individualized participant-focused perspective on the interviewees who utilized each of the three main pronouns more than the other participants (Chapter 5 analyzes the BLI of Yossi, who used the FS ‘you’ more; Chapter 6 analyzes the BLI of Dani, who used the MS ‘you’ more; and Chapter 7 analyzes the BLI of Motti, who used the ‘I’ more). Finally, Chapter 8 offers a macro-level and contextualized psychosociolinguistic comparative analysis of the relationships between the experiences and discourses of all three participants. These four chapters attempt to systematically investigate each of the hypotheses set forth herein, in order to fully understand the dynamic relationship between the individual, his inner psychological world, and his perceptions of the expectations of the society and culture surrounding him. 4.4 SOCIOPSYCHOLOGICAL LONGITUDINAL COMPARATIVE GUIDELINES: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE INDIVIDUAL AND HIS NARRATIVE OVER TIME Building upon the fundamental psycholinguistic findings and the extrapolated psychosociolinguistic micro- and macro-level hypotheses delineated above, the second principal objective of this study is to explore the drivers’ longitudinal processes of coping with, working through, and processing of the TA experienced, as evident in their narrations of it. I propose that this can be accomplished by a comparative analysis of the participants’ uses of the six mechanisms of narrative selection (MNSs) (Spector-Mersel, 2011)4—inclusion, omission, silencing, sharpening, flattening, and ‘appropriate’ meaning attribution—in both the form and content of their BLI and follow-up interview (FUI). This may serve to elucidate where the individual stands at the present moment (that is, at the time of the FUI), with regard to: 1) the TA experience and its potential psychological traumatic effects; 2) the process of narrating, processing, and working through the experience, and where and how it stands in the LN (and life itself); 3) his perceptions and appraisals of his behavior before, during, and after the TA experience and related emotional attributions, such as guilt and responsibility; 4) his perceptions of his surrounding society’s perceptions and appraisals regarding coping, resilience, and heroism (both in general and in relation to his particular case); and 5) his sense of emotional and functional well-being vs. isolation within the experience and its effects. 4 Spector-Mersel’s (2011) model of narrative analysis and the six mechanisms of narrative selection were explicated in detail in Chapter 3 (Section 3.6.2.4.2). 137 Furthermore, I propose an additional narrative selection category relevant for the comparative analyses conducted within the purview of this study, which I have named transferring. Under this category falls the central psycholinguistic patterns upon which this study is based—that is, it refers to the systematic discursive shift from the use of one pronoun to another and/or one tense to another, in connection to a given topic, feeling, attitude, or experience or facet therein. Rather than constituting an additional MNS in itself, transferring can be viewed more as an intersection between silencing and ‘appropriate’ meaning attribution. The latter mechanism can generally subsume such form representations as motivated pronoun or other word use, and the former mechanism can also be viewed as involved, as a use of the ‘you’-voice can be interpreted as a related silencing of the ‘I’-voice on the given topic. However, transferring is focused more intently on the specific communicative strategies surrounding pronoun and tense use outlined in this study, and comprises both of these facets, thus warranting an additional integrative category. This stage of analysis attempts to define development in processing from a longitudinal perspective on parallel narratives. That is, comparative analyses are conducted on primarily the LNs and TANs of each participant’s BLI and FUI, according to six central hypotheses. Because, as discussed previously, this study is fundamentally based on a salutogenic conceptual framework, it is more appropriate to refer to a continuum of progression in either direction, rather than utilizing more dichotomous terms (such as ‘improvement’ or ‘deterioration’). Therefore, this comparative analysis model refers to trajectories toward ‘ease’ or ‘dis-ease’ in narrative processing. Below are outlined the six Narrative Guidelines5 for a progression toward more ‘ease-ful’ processing: 1) More inclusion (less omission and silencing) of non-TA-related content a) More discussion of life before and/or after the TA, or content beyond the TA, within the LN b) More space and attention focused on life before and/or after the TA, or content beyond the TA, within the LN The first sub-criterion refers primarily to the content of the text, while the second refers to the form and is represented in both raw line counts and percentages. That is, relative to the BL LN, it would be expected to see in the FU LN a greater number of lines of text, as well as a greater percentage of the whole, dedicated to non-TA-related topics. In this case, there is a complementary nature to the MNSs, as more inclusion necessarily infers less omission and less silencing. This guideline could be reflected, for example, in more talk about family, childhood, or current daily life. 2) More omission (less inclusion) of TA-related content a) Less discussion of the TA within the LN b) Less space and attention focused on the TA within the LN Again, the second sub-criterion, which is more form-focused, is represented in both raw line counts 5 The term, guidelines, is used here more or less synonymously and interchangeably with, and can be understood as, “hypotheses,” or, “criteria.” However, it is important to note that these are not rigid categories with specific cut-off points or quantitative requirements. Rather, they are general guidelines based on qualitative data and interpretive analyses, which should be used to garner a holistic picture of the functioning and processing of the encoder/narrator. 138 and percentages. That is, relative to the BL LN, it would be expected to see in the FU LN a lesser number of lines of text, as well as a smaller percentage of the whole, dedicated to the TA and TArelated topics. Here, more omission necessarily infers less inclusion, but no complementary inference can be made about the relationship (or lack thereof) with silencing within this ‘equation.’ In addition, it may be tempting to assume that Guidelines 1 and 2 complement each other as well— that is, it seems logical to assume that more inclusion of non-TA-related content necessarily leads to more omission of TA-related content. However, it must be remembered that, at least in absolute numbers, this is not representative of a negative correlational relationship. Relative to its respective BL LN, an FU LN can include both more TA-related content and more non-TA-related content— indeed, it could be four times as long as the BL LN. This would simply mean that that comparative narrative assessment met the first guideline for more ‘ease-ful’ processing, but not the second. These criteria are only complementary when viewed through the lens of percentages. It should be acknowledged that omission of TA-related content could, in some cases or contexts, indicate ‘dis-ease-ful’ processing, rather than the opposite. Indeed, in a therapeutic or psychodynamic setting, for instance, a refusal or inability to talk about a traumatic life event might constitute evidence of a maladaptive coping strategy, such as denial or repression. However, in the context of the present study, the purpose of which is primarily research-based, multiple parallel narratives are compared longitudinally and the MNS of omission is indicated only relative to prior or subsequent inclusion. Indeed, given the comparative nature of this analytic model, without inclusion of a topic in one of the interviews, there is actually no way of determining what was omitted in the other. And omission of a great deal of TA-related content in both the BLI and FUI would not qualify as meeting this guideline (and likely would indeed indicate denial or repression). Therefore, the contention here stands that greater omission of TA-related content in the FUI, relative to the BLI, may imply a progression toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing. Contrarily, more omission of TA-related content in the BLI, followed by greater inclusion of TArelated content in the FUI, may indicate a trajectory toward ‘dis-ease-ful’ processing. 3) Less silencing of the ‘I’-voice regarding particularly difficult and upsetting TA-related content a) More ‘I’-statements (preferably in the ‘past’ tense) about sensory experiences of the TA and TA-related traumatic effects (both during and after event) b) More ‘I’-statements about topics such as passivity, weakness, lack of heroism, and/or guilt This is not necessarily limited to the LNs as were the previous two guidelines, but rather, may be evident throughout a participant’s two interview texts. However, the strongest indication of a shift toward more ‘ease-ful’ processing would be the use of the ‘I’-voice in particularly sensory or emotionally charged statements in the FU TAN (especially those that were collocated with the FS ‘you’ in the BL TAN). This is a particularly complex hypothesis, as upon first glance it may appear that the criterion calls for more inclusion of difficult content surrounding the TA 139 experience—which would stand as contradictory to Guideline 2. On the contrary, Guideline 3 seeks not additional content in the FUI, but rather, a shifting (or transferring) of focus and discursive strategies within content that existed, in a different form, in the BLI. Indeed, this criterion seeks evidence that the individual has (at least) begun processing the most upsetting facets of the experience, as reflected in the shift from non-‘I’- to ‘I’-statements. I assert that this is one of the most powerful narrative representations of coping with and working through a difficult life event such as this. For clarification, if a participant did not discuss the topics of weakness or guilt at all in his BLI, and then devoted a great deal of time to them (whether with ‘I’-statements or not) in his FUI, this should be viewed as an example of greater inclusion of TArelated content—and thus, as evidence of a shift toward ‘dis-ease-ful’ processing, according to the second guideline. However, if a participant discussed these topics in the MS ‘you’-voice in his BLI, and then in the ‘I’-voice in his FUI, this is a good indication of Guideline 3. 4) Less sharpening (more flattening) of TA-related content a) Fewer mini-narratives and shorter narratives overall b) Less emphasis, superlatives, and repetition in form c) Less within-narrative dialogue This guideline is fully complementary in its foundation, as less sharpening is essentially more flattening, and vice versa. The representations therein can exist in both content and form, and are sought primarily within the main TAN and other TA-related mini-narratives. Sub-criterion 4(a) focuses predominantly on content, as represented by raw line counts and percentages. This is a logical offshoot of Guideline 2, as this facet of flattening differs from omission only in intensity. And Sub-criteria 4(b) and 4(c) seek a discursive shift in the form of descriptions—as in, “It was difficult,” as opposed to, “It was very very difficult”; and, “I told her to sit down,” as opposed to, “I told her, ‘Sit down’.” 5) More attributions of meanings consistent with ‘owning’ and processing a) More seamless incorporation of the TAN into the LN, rather than the TAN standing apart from or outside of the LN b) More acceptance of variety of roles and actions before, during, and after the TA c) More willingness and capacity to explore the ‘greys’ of coping and effects of the TA d) Greater focus on the collective nature of the TA This guideline involves both form and content. In Sub-criterion 5(a), evidence of the TAN ‘standing apart from or outside of the LN’ can be represented in content by demarcation marks or narrative breaks (e.g., “And that’s it”), or questioning surrounding the introduction to the TAN (e.g., “Do you want to hear about the terror attack now?”), and in form by significant pauses, sighs, or changes in tone of voice. In this sense, this sub-criterion seeks an LN that includes the TAN, but is not divided by it. The more ‘ease-fully’-processed LN should simply flow from one topic to the next, at some point flowing into the TA experience, and later flowing out of it to the next topic, with little fanfare or partitioning—similar to any other life event discussed within the LN. 140 As previously mentioned, this guideline is closely connected with both the silencing and the following transferring hypotheses. In Sub-criteria 5(b) and 5(c), the focus is primarily on the content of the discourse, although a fundamental assumption is that this content be expressed in more ‘I’- than non-‘I’-statements. In general terms, this guideline seeks less rigidity and strict divisions between ‘right’ and ‘wrong’ behavior; between ‘acceptable’ and ‘unacceptable’ feelings; and between ‘heroic’ and ‘non-heroic’ actions. The more ‘ease-fully’-processed narrative text will show a greater acceptance of the full variety of the speaker’s behaviors and emotions surrounding the TA experience, as represented in spoken content as well as in greater numbers of ‘I’-statements. This may include more open discussion of traumatic effects and symptoms; need for medication or therapy; participation in support groups; or consultations with mental health professionals. In addition, mini-narratives about coping or heroism—such as discussions of other drivers who had similar experiences and did or did not return to work, or fellow army buddies who fought in wars with the narrator—would be expected to be presented in less ‘black and white’ terms. That is, there should be an acknowledgment of the complexities of coping—that it may not always be ‘correct’ to ‘return to the field’ immediately after a difficult experience such as a TA, and that experiencing traumatic effects does not automatically imply weakness or lack of masculinity. Finally, Sub-criterion 5(d) seeks a greater focus on the collective nature of the TA, as represented in the form of the discourse as less personalization and more use of collective pronouns (e.g., “what happened to us,” as opposed to, “what happened to me”; and “we arrived at the bus stop,” as opposed to, “I arrived at the bus stop”). All of these sub-criteria may present evidence of greater ‘owning’ and more ‘ease-ful’ processing of even the most troubling aspects of the TA experience. 6) Less transferring of difficult, upsetting, or ambivalent TA-related content away from the ‘I’-voice a) More ‘I’-statements (of either ‘past’ or ‘present’ tense): i. instead of ‘present’ tense FS ‘you’-statements ii. instead of ‘present’ tense MS ‘you’-statements b) More ‘past’ tense ‘I’-statements instead of ‘present’ tense ‘I’-statements Again, closely related to the guidelines on silencing and ‘appropriate’ meaning attribution, this wholly form-related hypothesis seeks more specific discursive ‘owning’ of the potentially traumatic effects and most difficult facets of the TA experience. Thus, Sub-criterion 6(a) would be represented in the substitutions of statements such as, “I saw it,” for, “You (FS) see it”; and, “I was afraid,” for, “You (MS) are afraid.” Sub-criterion 6(b) focuses more specifically on tense use; as represented, for example, in the substitution of, “I did it,” for, “I do it.” All of these discursive shifts are indicative of a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing of these topics. Whereas all of these guidelines are fundamentally relative—in the sense that comparisons are conducted between the BLI and FUI of each participant separately, and no comparisons are made between participants—it is particularly critical to note the relativity of the transferring guideline. If, for instance, a participant employed the FS ‘you’ in 40% of his BL TAN pronouns 141 and in only 15% of his FU TAN pronouns, this can be viewed as a meaningful degree of transferring. If another participant’s use of the FS ‘you’ stood at 15% in his BL TAN and decreased to 3% in his FU TAN, this can also be a significant demonstration of transferring. This criterion focuses on the pronoun and tense uses delineated as the fundamental patterns found in this study, seeking any substantial shift away from depersonalizing and projecting techniques and toward owning and claiming techniques. I assert that a longitudinal shift toward heightened use of both the ‘I’ pronoun and the ‘past’ tense may constitute evidence of a greater degree of processing of the TAN, and thus, of working through and psychological coping with the TA experience itself. The above discussion has carefully defined the central hypotheses upon which an assessment of longitudinal ‘improvement’ in, or more ‘ease-ful,’ narrative processing, would rest. The inverse of this is ‘deterioration,’ or a trajectory toward more ‘dis-ease-ful’ processing, which essentially involves the opposite of each of these guidelines. The table below demarcates the specific characteristics of a given FUI fitting each assessment description, organized by MNS. Notation is present if the analytical focus lies predominantly on the LN or TAN. For clarity, the mechanisms of sharpening and flattening are presented together, as the end result in each cell is the same—that is, “less dialogue” is the consequence of less sharpening and more flattening in more ‘ease-ful’ processing, and of more sharpening and less flattening in more ‘dis-ease-ful’ processing. Table 4-1: Guidelines for characteristics of narrative processing in a follow-up interview, relative to the respective baseline interview Inclusion Omission Content Content Silencing Content Sharpening Content and Flattening Form ‘Appropriate’ meaning attribution Transferring Content and Form Form ‘EASE-FUL’ PROCESSING More non-TA-related content (in LN) Less TA-related content (in LN) More ‘I’-statements about difficult/upsetting facets of TA Fewer mini-narratives (in TAN(s)) and shorter TAN(s) overall Less emphasis, superlatives, and repetition (in TAN(s)) Less dialogue (in TAN(s)) TAN is integrated within LN More acceptance of varied roles and actions before, during, and after TA More exploration of ‘greys’ of coping and traumatic effects of TA Greater focus on collective nature of TA More ‘I’ and less ‘you’ overall (in TAN) More ‘past’ tense and less ‘present’ tense overall (in TAN) ‘DIS-EASE-FUL’ PROCESSING More TA-related content (in LN) Less non-TA-related content (in LN) More non-‘I’-statements about difficult/upsetting facets of TA More mini-narratives (in TAN(s)) and longer TAN(s) overall More emphasis, superlatives, and repetition (in TAN(s)) More dialogue (in TAN(s)) TAN stands apart from LN More ambivalence about and/or less acceptance of roles and actions before, during, and after TA More rigidity and ‘black and white’ attitudes about coping and traumatic effects of TA Greater focus on individual experience of TA Less ‘I’ and more ‘you’ overall (in TAN) Less ‘past’ tense and more ‘present’ tense overall (in TAN) Overall, for the purposes of this study, the operationalization of a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing involves an FU TAN that is more distanced and less proximal and personalized than the respective BL TAN. The FU TAN is less vivid, salient, and ‘alive,’ as 142 represented by a heightened use of the remote ‘past’ tense and less use of the proximate ‘present’ tense. There is more talking about, as opposed to talking within, the TA experience, as represented through less emphasis and dialogue. And there is less transferring of the most difficult, upsetting, and/or ambivalence-causing facets of the experience from the ‘I’-voice to either of the ‘you’voices. There is a sense that there is more to the individual’s life (history and story) than the TA, as represented by its taking up a smaller volume within the LN, and by a less stark division or no partition at all between the periods of life before, during, and after the TA. There is more evidence of coping and processing, as represented in an increased capacity to discuss and work through issues that may not be so ‘black and white.’ In contrast, the operationalization of a trajectory toward more ‘dis-ease-ful’ narrative processing involves an FU TAN that is more personalized and proximate, more vivid and descriptive, and more ‘alive’ (narratively and psychologically) than the respective BL TAN. There is more representation of this in the discourse, as the narrator uses more emphasis and dialogue to bring the story to ‘life’ for himself and his audience. This will include more transferring of any troubling or traumatic aspects of the TA experience away from the ‘I’-perspective and onto either the MS or FS ‘you,’ depending on the motivation for the transfer. There is a sense that the individual’s life has become condensed and reduced to little more than the TA, as demonstrated in a substantial increase in the volume occupied by the TAN within the LN. The TAN may also stand alone or (discursively) outside of the LN, in a reflection that it is perceived by the individual as standing outside of his ‘normal’ life history. In this sense, the more the TA conspicuously protrudes from the LN, the less likely it is that it has been ‘ease-fully’ processed, worked through, and coped with by the narrator. Finally, the individual who shows evidence of greater ‘dis-ease’ in his narrative processing will show a decreased willingness and capacity to explore the range of possible reactions to a stressful life event like this. He may instead demonstrate a stricter adherence to more rigid attitudes about ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ norms, and thus, may find it difficult to discursively claim any of his own behavior that he perceives as not fitting these requirements. The second section of results, comprising the Longitudinal Comparative Analyses section of this dissertation, explores these fundamental comparative hypotheses and guidelines in an attempt to assess narrative processing, again from an individualized participant-focused perspective on three of the interviewees. Chapter 9 analyzes both the BLI and FUI of Yossi, who best exemplifies a trajectory toward ‘ease’ in his longitudinal processing of the TA he experienced, while Chapter 10 analyzes both the BLIs and FUIs of Eli and Shimi, who both exemplify static longitudinal processing of the TAs they each experienced. Finally, Chapter 11 presents a comprehensive discussion of these results and the possible innovative contribution of this method of analysis as a qualitative narrative assessment tool for individuals after stressful and/or traumatic life events. 143 PART 2 Baseline Analyses Herein are presented three separate baseline case studies. Each chapter begins by introducing the specific participant and giving a summary of his demographic background and of the terror attack he experienced, as well as a discussion of my initial impressions, the interview contexts, and our interactions. Then, there is a detailed presentation of the pronominal discourse of the interviewee’s baseline interview, with particular focus on the pronoun used more predominantly. This is followed by a quantitative analysis of the distribution of pronouns and tenses in the central analyzed narrative(s) of the baseline interview, and then by a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of the same narrative(s). Each chapter concludes with a discussion of the central findings and main messages illustrated in the analysis. Three participants are focused upon, each as an exemplar of predominant usage of a different pronoun. Chapter 5 presents Yossi, who used the second-person feminine singular pronoun the most. Chapter 6 presents Dani, who used the second-person masculine singular pronoun the most. Chapter 7 presents Motti, who used the firstperson singular pronoun the most. Finally, Chapter 8 offers a comparative acrossparticipant analysis of all three, from a psychosociolinguistic macro-level perspective. Comparative focus is placed on their pronoun and tense use, meanings, and messages, as well as their relevant personal experiences that may place these discursive findings into greater context. Finally, connections are demonstrated between the study hypotheses, findings, and the interviewees’ own words, particularly related to the larger topics of individual and collective coping and societal pressure and expectations. CHAPTER 5 YOSSI: “YOU (FS) SWALLOW ALL OF THE ATROCITY” — BRINGING THE AUDIENCE IN TO TRAUMATIC EXPERIENCE AND ‘UNACCEPTABLE’ WEAKNESS 5.1 INTRODUCTION TO YOSSI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS At the time of the baseline interview (BLI), Yossi was 56 years old and married with three children. He was born in Iraq and immigrated to Israel with his family at the age of eighteen months. He completed twelve years of schooling and served in the Armored Corps during his compulsory army service. Yossi fought in the Six Day War (1967) and the War of Attrition (196870) during his mandatory service, and as a reserve soldier he fought and was wounded in the Yom Kippur War (1973) and the First Lebanon War (1982). Yossi worked as a bus driver in Jerusalem for approximately 35 years without a single break, even continuing to work part-time after his ‘retirement’ at the age of 49. Nineteen months prior to the BLI, at the age of 54, Yossi experienced a suicide bombing on his bus. The terror attack (TA) was extensive, with many casualties and injuries, and Yossi suffered a permanent partial loss of hearing as a result. He was not working at the time of the BLI, and appeared to be suffering from significant post-traumatic symptoms. I was referred to Yossi by the psychologist at Egged. It took quite a while to schedule the BLI, due in part to his inability or lack of willingness to plan in advance. Every time I called to ask him whether we could book a meeting in the upcoming week, he would always answer that the first day of the week would be “great” for him, but that it was “hard to plan beyond that.” I got the impression that it was related to a variable work schedule, such that sometimes he was busy in the mornings and other times in the afternoons. At one point it took me almost a week to catch Yossi on the phone, during which time his son answered one call and mentioned that his father was on “holiday.” All of this combined to cause my surprise when Yossi stated during the interview that he does not work anymore, and repeatedly attested that all he does is sit “right here on the couch.” He never directly addressed our scheduling difficulties and I never asked, but I later deduced that most of Yossi’s ‘“busy”-ness’ during this period was related to his struggles with the National Insurance Institute (NII), which became a central topic of his BLI and is discussed further below. The BLI lasted approximately three hours, and I found it difficult for two primary reasons. First, at the time of the interview Yossi was embroiled in a dispute with the NII over whether he could be recognized as disabled as a result of his injuries from the TA, and if so, how much compensation he would be eligible to receive. Most of the interview was dominated by Yossi’s long-winded and angry rants about this battle, which clearly was very upsetting for him. At the time, I found it frustrating and challenging to keep him ‘on-task’ as far as the (my) overarching objectives of the interview. Indeed, there were several moments when Yossi entered into what I could only describe as tirades on one or another topic—most often, his experiences with the NII and the way he perceives the government as having treated him after the TA, or his feelings on Arabs and terrorism in Israel. It was close to impossible to interrupt him when he was in the throes of these argumentations, and even more so to redirect him. The first time on each subject, I ‘let’ 144 him talk uninterruptedly as long as he chose, while in subsequent appearances of the same topic I allowed myself more leeway to interrupt and gently try to steer him to a new subject. However, I since came to realize that what Yossi did present was clearly important for him and an informative window into his life story in any case (and that perhaps I should have intervened less in this process). And yet, I also later noted that Yossi repeated each tirade at least one other time in the interview in nearly the same exact words and manner, leading me to believe that he may actually have some organic memory impairment. This may also have been influenced by his age and/or the repetitive rhetoric of Israeli conversation in general (Tobin, personal communication). It is also possible that these speeches had become rehearsed to the point of being almost rote; a likely result of having given them repeated times in the past, perhaps to anyone who would listen and empathize with him. As I perceived this apparent need for ‘a shoulder to cry on’ at the time of the BLI, I made great efforts to validate Yossi’s feelings throughout all of these speeches, by nodding and agreeing that these experiences must be very frustrating for him. However, I was left wondering if Yossi perhaps viewed me as a professional of some kind—thus, in his mind, deserving of hearing these tirades and perhaps responsible for alleviating his distress. My fears were further amplified after the formal interview had concluded, when he requested that I write up a report of my “professional” opinion of him and his condition. I replied that I am unqualified to write any report of a clinical nature, but he did not seem convinced. I still feel fairly certain that Yossi viewed me not as a student, but as a psychologist, and as such, part of the ‘establishment.’ The second reason for my discomfort during the BLI lay in Yossi’s manner of presentation of his terror attack narrative (TAN), which was extremely vivid, descriptive, and detailed—in fact, quite gruesome. He talked at length about the bodies of the victims, their dismembered limbs and heads, their twitching body parts, and the blood and pieces of flesh strewn all over the bus. It was as if he were putting on a performance, and he seemed to go out of his way to tell the story in the goriest way possible. Yossi also employed facial expressions, gestures, and body movements to act out parts of the narrative, pulling his body rigid and widening his eyes while describing the victims’ heads, and twitching and clenching his arms and fists while describing their detached limbs. From the viewpoint of the interviewer/researcher, I must admit that this narrative was incredibly difficult to deal with (to hear initially as well as to analyze for hours afterwards). I was highly disturbed throughout Yossi’s ‘performance,’ and found it nearly impossible to not be affected by the gruesomeness of his discourse. I was equally intrigued by the cognitive dissonance I was feeling. Indeed, within the framework of my B.A. thesis research, I conducted similarly indepth narrative interviews with twenty Holocaust survivors. I had borne witness to narratives of significant trauma and pain and descriptions of unspeakable atrocities, and I was no ‘stranger’ to the effects of these types of narratives. Yet, while I never had a problem keeping my composure in 145 any of those interviews, I found myself struggling not to be overcome with emotion during Yossi’s interview. While I had certainly expected that I would be exposed to unpleasant stories throughout this research project, I felt that something else was going on between me and Yossi that fell outside the purview of a simple imparting of a narrative by interviewee to interviewer. Indeed, Yossi mentioned a few times that these were the images that still follow and disturb him endlessly, with comments such as, “You (FS) would never forget it,” and, “You (FS) can’t work after something like this.” At some point, I began to get the sense that he was trying to affect me in the same way in which he was affected. It actually felt like Yossi was attempting to pull me into his story, and was using any means at his disposal to make his statements more personal for me and thus, easier for me to connect with. At the time of the interview, I thought that this could be reflected in Yossi’s choice to tell his narrative of the TA in such a vivid and descriptive manner. (Only in the analysis process did I realize that Yossi’s patterns of pronoun usage were significantly different from those of the other interviewees, and it was this realization that led to a major shift in the focus of the research study overall.) Suffice it to say, I left Yossi’s interview feeling as if I had a huge weight on my shoulders. I could viscerally feel the effects of what I viewed as his attempts to project his difficult experiences onto me, and it was only after a few days of feeling upset and emotionally drained that I felt as if I had fully recovered from the interview. I did not have this type of reaction to any of the other interviews, and it is of note that none of the other interviewees told their TANs in a manner even approaching the gruesomeness in Yossi’s telling performance. My overall impressions of Yossi after the BLI were that he was clearly depressed and affected by what he had experienced. It had been a little over a year and a half since the TA, and he was still very much upset. He made repeated references to what I would classify as anhedonia (an inability to experience pleasure in normally pleasurable activities) and it did appear that his symptoms at the time would have qualified as clinical depression and/or post-traumatic stress. This notwithstanding, Yossi was able to talk quite animatedly and at times I did see some flickers of positive emotions. His face literally lit up several times during the interview—first, at the mention of his wife and wedding, and later, at references to his granddaughter. I also observed that, even after saying that there aren’t too many stories to tell about his work (“I just work, that’s it, nothing special”), a narrative he subsequently told about an old man who took his bus every day came out almost effortlessly, and Yossi’s face looked almost enchanted as he told it. Following the BLI, I had ‘classified’ Yossi as decidedly not resilient, according to my research definitions at the time. Indeed, one of my primary ‘criteria’ had been the ability to return to work (or at least ride on a bus) after being in a TA, and Yossi was adamant about his refusal to go near a bus (as well as to let anyone in his family do so). However, Yossi did mention that he defines himself “one million percent” as a survivor, and that, at least before the TA, he saw himself 146 as strong and brave for the work he did. At the time, I wrote in my field notes that “perhaps there is something else in this interview that will make itself known as part of the ‘real story’.” And indeed, Yossi, both as an individual participant and as reflective of a greater psychosociolinguistic phenomenon, quickly became absolutely central to the research as a whole. 5.2 YOSSI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT Relative to the other interviewees and throughout his BLI, what most characterized Yossi’s discourse was his propensity for statements utilizing the second-person feminine singular pronoun (‘you’ (FS)). Nearly every time I asked a question that I viewed as clearly calling for a personalized and specific answer (e.g., “How was that experience for you?” or, “Were you afraid?”), he gave responses such as, “Listen, you (FS) are afraid of many things,” or “When you (FS) do…” There were discursive moments in which Yossi would put me (presumably, the ‘you (FS)’ in question) into the role of a psychologist, or an NII representative, or a bus driver, or a bus passenger, or, perhaps most significantly, himself as experiencer of the TA. Each use of the ‘you (FS),’ as well as of each of the other pronouns, in each context carries with it interpretations and implications of its own. It is, therefore, critical that attention be paid to specific examples of the discursive choices made by Yossi, within their specific contexts. Overall, Yossi showed four distinct patterns of pronoun choice throughout the BLI, all of which were reflected in his TAN and life narrative (LN). First, he utilized first-person possessive pronouns (such as, ‘me,’ and, ‘my’) and phrasing as a strategy toward creating a sense of personalization and possession of the topics or objects of reference. Second, he utilized a number of third-person ‘others’ (such as, “a bus driver”) as a strategy for creating a sense of depersonalization, or distance, and universalization. Third, he utilized the FS ‘you’ as a strategy to bring his audience in to the experience he was describing. Finally, he utilized the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’) as a strategy toward claiming ownership of the experience or role he was describing. Each of these strategies is discussed in turn below, with demonstrative examples. 5.2.1 Personalization, possession, and separation: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases From the beginning of his BLI, Yossi frequently used discourse that appeared to be personalizing his bus and events he had experienced through the use of first-person possessive pronouns, referring to certain objects as his own or even as himself.1 For instance, rather than saying that the door of the (or even his) bus had been gashed open, he said, “My door was warped, 1 This discursive mechanism is known in linguistics as metonymy, defined as a device in which one entity is used to stand for another associated entity, generally portraying some actual relationship between the things signified. In Yossi’s uses of this mechanism, the use of “me” most often refers to the bus he generally drove or the bus he was driving at the time of the TA. 147 gashed.”i At another point he said, “I thought that it’s a truck that ran into me hard,”ii as opposed to “…ran into my bus,” or, “…ran into the bus.” He also referred to both passengers on his bus and casualties that his unit suffered during the Yom Kippur War as his own, as in the following two statements: “Two girls got on me [my bus],”iii and, “Now I was wounded in the Yom Kippur War, (4) I received a direct hit, and I had two casualties (MP).”iv The latter sentence could be interpreted as Yossi’s inclusion of “[having]” two casualties under the umbrella of his own ‘personal’ wounds. That is to say, Yossi may not differentiate between the “direct hit” he personally suffered and the two casualties—both appear to be, in his mind, his wartime wounds. Similarly, he referred to his casualties during the TA almost as if he were a commander on a battlefield during a war, with soldiers under his command who had been hit by enemy fire. Yossi also referred to the TA itself as “what [they] did to me”v at one point, and also many times as “what happened to me.”vi At other points, however, he referred to it euphemistically, as simply “what happened.”vii Yossi showed a general tendency to focus more on himself and on his own role, responsibilities, and actions, and what he experienced and suffered at the time, rather than generalizing the event and recognizing that it did, indeed, involve and cause a great deal of stress to other (still living) people as well. Yossi spent much time discussing the bodies of, and in general, the victims of the attack, but scarcely mentioned the other survivors. From his discourse, it almost seemed as if the TA was an event solitarily experienced by him, nearly from beginning to end, and as if he was the sole survivor of it. In addition, the communicative strategy reflected in the use of first-person possessive phrases and pronouns may suggest a sense of guilt or responsibility for what happened, as if he expected to be able to protect his passengers from an attack happening on his bus. This notion of personal ‘experience-ability’ and responsibility under fire could be viewed as a byproduct of having served in the Israeli army, and of the social mentality surrounding the cultural supremacy of ‘The Israeli Warrior’ prevalent in Israel during the period in which Yossi grew up and fought in multiple wars (Tobin, personal communication). In this sense, the TA may be perceived by Yossi as another ‘wartime’ incident that he experienced, whether he was in uniform or not—and thus, his discourse surrounding both contexts is notably similar. 5.2.2 Depersonalization, generalization, and universalization: Use of third-person ‘others’ and the second-person masculine singular pronoun Atypically in relation to most of the other interviewees, Yossi did not use the second-person masculine singular pronoun (‘you’ (MS)) at all in his TAN. This finding makes sense, in light of the hypothesis that the use of this pronoun signifies universalization and collectivization, and given Yossi’s many statements about his overarching sense of isolation (both as an effect of the TA and as a result of his battles with the NII to have his injuries recognized and compensated). Notably, however, Yossi made use of a number of third-person ‘others’ (most specifically, “he,” “a person,” 148 and “a bus driver”), in discursive locations in which the MS ‘you’ might be expected as the conventional choice. He offered this exemplar account as part of the introduction to his TAN: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 “That’s it. I also worked (1) without taking any breaks. I would leave happy, I had those three hours, four hours, I would come back home. Like filled up my day. And uhh, of course, when youuu (FS) dr-, whenn a bus driver, it’s a person whoo knows his passengers, you (FS) know, you (FS) ride with him every day, so, uhh, he knows the problems. It is like for example a psychologist of people. People tell him what they have been through, and he tells them, and what is happening with them, and ‘Why are you (FS) sad?’ and ‘Why are you (MS) sad?’ and ‘What’s the problem?’ and all. When [they] get onto a bus everyone (MS) unwinds and tells his story. So over time you (FS) get to know people. So you (FS) get on routinely with him ont-, on-, onto the bus, and sit (FS) in one of the places, and you (FS) know hello [to] this one (FS), hello [to] that her, or hello [to] that him. Everyone (MS) gets on (MS), and sits down (MS), annd uhh, that’s it. So you (FS) know the people, and someti- and sometimes you (FS) are like this, like, like a sort of little family. You (FS) know the people, you (FS) know the- their problems, everyone (MS), and [they] keep riding, everyone (MS) is smiling, and everyone (MS) is running around [busy]. And, uhh, some of the people I didn’t know, and that was like every normal day. Like every normal day. Uhh, in the morning I got up, and usually we put on tefillin. (2) I prayed, and uhh, I went to work.” viii In this short section of text, Yossi utilizes three distinct pronominal techniques, with what at first appears as very little ‘logic’ with regard to his choices in a given moment or sentence. He begins his description2 with several specific statements about himself, using the ‘I’ pronoun—making it clear that these experiences happened to him, and that he is recounting them as himself. After this, however, the subject of the bulk of the report becomes the indefinite third person—“a bus driver” or “a person” who is nameless, universal, and non-personal. Yossi, in fact, likens this bus driver to a psychologist at one point, and then continues to refer to the subject as “he” or “him.” Alongside of this, Yossi introduces another subject: the second-person FS pronoun. It is unclear to whom he is referring, but it appears that he sees this FS ‘you’ as a prototypical passenger, as is most apparent in lines (L.) 3-4 and 7-9. Indeed, while Yossi is likely directing these statements to me specifically (as represented by the marked and personal use of the pronoun), it is also possible that he is introducing an unconventional generic and impersonal use of the FS ‘you,’ to relate to a non-specific female third party. Finally, Yossi completes this description— which, it becomes clear at this point, is only a prelude to his TAN—by switching back to ‘I’statements (L.14-15). He returns to become the subject of the narrative, and the ‘experiencer’ of the events he is describing, as opposed to the majority of this text section. Particularly discernible in Yossi’s choices of pronoun usage throughout this excerpt is the apparent inconsistency therein. Indeed, the boundary between objective description and subjective comment was frequently blurred, and the choice at certain points to employ the subjective function of the FS ‘you,’ collocated with the proximate-present (PPr) tense, for actions and events that 2 According to Rosenthal (1993), this text would be labeled as a condensed situation, which is a subcategory of description in her thematic field analysis typology (story, description, argumentation), as it is framed as one exemplary situation comprised of a compression of events that were frequently experienced by the interviewee. This type of discourse is generally collocated with a timeless or neutral tense (Tobin, personal communication)—as in, “This is what we would do every day,” as opposed to, “This is what we did”—and often with the use of words such as “usually” or “generally” to indicate the frequency and/or familiarity of the ‘compressed’ events. 149 actually took place in the past, was highly unusual. This, in addition to the various uses of the third-person forms collocated with the timeless use of the PPr tense, combined to create a picture of a mix of communicative strategies apparently aimed at both distancing and bringing in the decoder at different points in the discourse. As part of the analysis of this passage, therefore, it is critical to ask first, “How else could each statement have been expressed?” and second, “If there is a logic underlying the choices expressed here, what might it be?” The following table offers several alternative (i.e., more personalized) manners of expressing the statements that Yossi generalized. Table 5-1: Actual (generalized) quotes and possible (more personalized) alternatives ACTUAL QUOTE FROM TEXT “a bus driver, it’s a person who knows his passengers” (L.3) “you (FS) ride with him every day” (L.3-4) “People tell him what they have been through, and he tells them, and what is happening with them” (L.5) “So you (FS) know the people, and someti- and sometimes you (FS) are like this, like, like a sort of little family.” (L.10-11) POSSIBLE ALTERNATIVES “I knew/was familiar with my passengers.” “I knew/was familiar with the passengers on my bus.” “The passengers rode [on the bus] with me every day.” “I drove with some of the same passengers every day.” “People used to/would tell me what they have been through, and I used to/would tell them…” “I knew the people, and sometimes we were like a sort of little family.” Here, the various possible alternatives for some of Yossi’s statements show the different discursive atmosphere created by each. It seems that Yossi chose to generalize or distance many of his statements from himself, in addition to formulating the sentences in a habitualized framework (as seen in the use of the timeless function of the PPr tense). This creates the sense that his statements pertain to all bus drivers in a universalized context, rather than describing a specific situation, condition, or feeling that he personally experienced. It is possible that Yossi’s use of the timeless tense form collocates with his use of the generalized third-person pronouns, while other—perhaps more emotionally charged—tense forms tend to be utilized together with the less neutral—and also more emotionally charged and personal—pronouns (Tobin, personal communication). Yossi did utilize the MS ‘you’ in his BL discourse, albeit extremely sparingly. It was most often employed to refer discursively to male individuals other than himself, in the context of generalized discourse and yet, in a strangely specific manner. For instance, the following quotation came as part of a long diatribe about “Arabs who put bombs on buses,” and Yossi’s opinion of the difference between fighting heroically as a soldier and the “cowardly act” of bombing women and children in the midst of their daily lives: 1 2 3 4 “But you (FS) also start to hate these things. To hate these people, because, because. I am saying, at the moment that, that you (MS) get onto the bus, it is not a war, it is a crime. It is not a war. (2) In any case I would understand this war that you (MS) are going to kill a soldier, you (MS) are going to fight against soldiers, bomb, kill, you (MS) understand? Great. To appreciate that.” ix The next quotation employing the MS ‘you’ followed later in this speech: 1 2 “Listen (FS), it’s crazy! (1) It’s crazy! Imagine (FS) that you (FS) are going and taking a stroll in the old city, alone, nothing, without a weapon, nothing, without protection, you (FS) are not doing 150 3 4 5 6 7 8 anything. Anyway you (MS) come stab a soldier, uhh, I still kind of understand. But when you (MS) come stab a little girl, or a little boy, or a pregnant woman, or a man, or an old man who is going to, the what kind of crazy, despicable war is that? So this is what bothers me. This is what makes you (FS) angry. And then if they (MP) caught him, and he doesn’t have blood on his hands, {you (MS) let him go}. What are you (MP) crazy? What are you (MP) stupid? People like that shouldn’t be let go at all, no, no. They shouldn’t see the light of day. (2) At all.”x Here, Yossi continues to use the MS ‘you’ as a discursive stand-in, first for the ‘Arab aggressor’ (L.3-4), and later for the unacceptably-acting Israeli police force and/or judicial system (L.6-7). This again represents an unusual use of this pronoun, as it appears distanced from the ‘Israeli everyman.’ Indeed, Yossi’s use of the various third-person ‘others’ seemed to accomplish a function closer to that of the MS ‘you’ in the discourse of most of the other interviewees. The question remains with regard to what ‘logic’ he was employing (either consciously or unconsciously) within these discursive processes. Perhaps Yossi’s use of these third-person ‘others’ serves as a discursive representation of his sense of marginalization and disconnectedness. Or perhaps his use of these strategies simply reflects his view of these experiences as generalized, universalized, and ‘usual.’ Indeed, the text excerpt refers to a very conventional situation, one that Yossi experienced nearly daily for decades: driving on the same bus, on the same route, with many of the same passengers. Regardless of the myriad possible motivations for this discursive strategy, of primary importance here is the system of choices reflected—choices that tend to favor the ‘other’ over the ‘I’ in these generalized contexts. The continuation of the analysis below reveals which contexts and likely motivations led to discursive choices by Yossi favoring the FS ‘you’ and the ‘I.’ 5.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun Significantly more than any of the other participants, Yossi’s BL discourse was riddled with the FS ‘you’ pronoun, and indeed, a half hour into his interview, I had noticed clearly that in certain discursive contexts, he rarely used ‘I’-statements at all. Much of Yossi’s TAN had the FS ‘you’ as its subject; two nearly consecutive representative sections of text are presented below: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 “You (FS) look and you (FS) see that your (FS) eyes, it is from, it, it, it (2) the, everything like opens up to you (FS) in horror, in in in, it, you (FS) aren’t afraid, it iit is atrocity, it isn’t, it isn’t fe-, you (FS) are scared, it isn’t fear, it is something that is impossible to define. Because there is no, nothing to be afraid of, but, it is’it is atrocity that is impossible to describe. And you (FS) look (3), and no one gets near the bus. No one. The bus is going up in flames. (3) I look, and then I do like this with [my] ear- with my hands, with [my] head, (1) ‘Wow,’ I say to myself things like, ‘What happened?’ ‘It can’t be!’ You (FS), you (FS) hear silence, and then you (FS) hear from within the silence “Aii, aii, aii, it hurts, aii.” All sorts of shou- cries like that. And you (FS) look, you (FS) see people, (1) hanging. (2)”xi Here, the entire text section is in the PPr tense, which, according to Tobin’s (1989b) interlocked semantic tense sub-systems, signifies that this experience is perceived by Yossi as ‘close to his heart’ and experience-able. Except for a single, “I look” (L.5), he claims a statement for himself only when there is distinct action, such as when he does and says (L.5-6). Every other verb in this excerpt is attributed to the FS ‘you,’ and involves sensory/emotional statements—looking, seeing, 151 being (afraid or not afraid), and hearing. This pattern is maintained in the subsequent section: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 “You (FS) see people without heads. Some without arms, halves of bodies. Now you (FS) see all the stuff inside of the bodies. Everything is on the floor. If you (FS) can imagine, there are twelve bodies, twelve people who were pulverized, it’s not people who died and were whole. Because if you (FS) see a corpse, so you (FS) don’t, don’t think about it, you (FS) say that the person maybe is asleep, like, because there is no, uhh, sense like that. But it’s not like that. It is something else. You (FS) see mutilated people. You (FS) see hands moving. You (FS) see legs very few, it, it everything is hot. You (FS) see, everyything is throbbing, you (FS), everything everything is moving, everything, is boiling. People are like, the movements of the eyes too, of the hands too, everything is moving. Everything is moving. And now I started to push. When I had already recovered, (2) I started to push, limbs so that I wouldn’t go on top of them. To hear, I went to the middle of the bus, and I came back. And all of this I swallow, that one is moving hands here, moving legs. Everything, all the flesh, [it is] moving people, halves! Moving! I came back to the front of the bus a girl was lying there, she also had wounds, all over her too, full of blood. ‘Driver, help me.’ And so I picked her up. You (FS) get strength, that you (FS) don’t know from where. You (FS) get supernatural powers, it is really, impossible to define it. (2) My door was warped, gashed. Everything was gashed. I grasped the door, by force, I opened a little, and then, and then, only then, a head [of a security person] appeared.”xii Here, Yossi continues to bring in the FS ‘you’ subject, discursively placing the entire sensory experience of the bombing onto his audience (me). Rather than discussing the event as something he personally went through in the past—indeed, he was the one who saw all of these sights, heard all of these cries, and felt all of these feelings—Yossi uses the combined communicative strategies of the PPr tense and the second-person FS pronoun to denote his attitude toward these experiences. Notably, however, the above section includes a small interlude during which Yossi tells a mini-narrative with himself as the agentic ‘I,’ in a personalized and active role. From the sentence, “And now I started to push” (L.9), until the sentence, “And so I picked her up” (L.14), Yossi is the main character and there is no ‘you’ subject to whom he is talking or referring. Suddenly, the narrative takes on an entirely different feeling, with no detached, ‘other’ character passively seeing and experiencing all the carnage. The ‘I’ here is active, powerful, and even heroic. Significantly, and in an equally stark contrast to the rest of the excerpt, Yossi also uses the ‘past’ (RPa) tense— denoting a remote and experienced event—almost exclusively in this interlude, reinforcing the message that he definitively performed these actions. After this mini-narrative, Yossi returns to the PPr tense and to employing the FS ‘you,’ when he asserts that “you (FS) get strength,” and “you (FS) get supernatural powers” (L.14-15). Then, again, Yossi jumps back to ‘I’-statements and the RPa tense, again describing his specific actions at the time. Throughout Yossi’s entire BL TAN, the second-person FS pronoun was used most often to describe his own experiences rather than in collocation with any direct dialogue with me (as conventional MIH pronoun usage rules would dictate), and in the initial analysis it was difficult to determine whether there was a particular pattern or meaning therein. As Yossi’s discourse became more intense and descriptive, however, it became clear that nearly all of the statements therein utilizing the FS ‘you’ were sensory in nature. Complementing this pattern, it was found that the majority of statements with regard to seeing, feeling, touching, or hearing had the FS ‘you’ as their 152 subject. Yossi did attribute a comparatively small number of sensory statements to himself and his own personalized experience (e.g., “I look,” in L.5 of the first excerpt above; and “And all of this I swallow,” in L.11 of the second excerpt above—significantly, both collocated with the PPr tense). Notably, however, there were almost no statements in the TAN that were directed toward the FS ‘you’ and not sensory in content. Furthermore, the predominant pattern of collocation between the FS ‘you’ and the PPr tense is highly indicative of a particular communicative strategy at work. This strategy is likely aimed at bringing me in to the experience with him, although perhaps the most important question is why. Within the context of the TAN, the fact that I am most often discursively brought in when the focus of the statement is sensory seems to point to a particular need surrounding the most gruesome details of the TA—perhaps, a need to not experience them alone. Yossi himself may provide some insight to support the hypothesis that the manner in which a story is told may be influenced by the narrator’s sense of loneliness or isolation in the experience, and by his desire to have someone share in and empathize with his pain. At one point, Yossi took a short break from his TAN to explain: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “You (FS) don’t know what it is. I don’t wish on anyone to see it. Iit is impossible to describe it, it is something that even in hell is impossible to see a thing like this. (2) And also I still didn’t understand what happened. And then, like in some sort of horror film, I turn around, towards, the inside of the bus. Like, I turn back around, and then, like in some sort of horror film, you (FS) swallow all of the atrocity. It is something that is impossible to define. Difficult, very difficult. Difficult also to endure it.”xiii Here Yossi appears to exhibit a sense of frustration, as he attempts to both describe his experience and explain why it is impossible to describe and define. While on a manifest level, he verbally expresses that he does not “wish on anyone to see it,” the rest of his narrative—filled with vivid descriptions of blood, body parts, and the sensory experience of an incinerated and still-burning bus—seems to belie this statement, perhaps revealing a subconscious, very real desire to bring his audience in to his experience, for them/me to truly “see it” as he did. Indeed, this seems to represent a contradiction between the form and content of Yossi’s discourse. This ambivalence also shows in Yossi’s alternating use of ‘I’ and ‘you (FS)’ throughout this section, and particularly in his statement, “You (FS) don’t know what it is” (L.1). While on the one hand, this is an absolutely valid assertion and he is simply stating the obvious (that I personally have no idea what it is like to experience a bombing), Yossi’s use of the FS ‘you’ here may reveal again his desire, on some level, for me to learn (from him) “what it is.” The most significant message in this excerpt occurs when he says, “I turn around…I turn back around” (L.3-4), and follows it directly by, “you (FS) swallow all of the atrocity” (L.4-5). Revealed here again is Yossi’s system of choices—to use ‘I’ when describing his physical action of “turn[ing] around,” and to use ‘you (FS)’ in conjunction with the metaphorical action of “swallow[ing].” Thus, while the ‘I’ is agentic, the FS ‘you’ victim is passive, receiving or having “all of the atrocity” loaded 153 onto ‘her’ through no fault of ‘her’ own. Again, there are a number of potential explanations for these discursive patterns, and for the communicative strategy of using the second-person FS pronoun in general. While a basic contention of this research is that the FS ‘you’ connotes the speaker’s attempt to bring his audience in to the experience he is describing, the question of motivation still remains. I propose three possible hypotheses that appear to be the most likely, and because Yossi’s use of the FS ‘you’ in his TAN followed such a specific and systematic pattern (that of collocation with the PPr tense and sensory content), two additional excerpts from other places in Yossi’s BLI will be presented in order to show the variety of his uses of the FS ‘you’ and of the contexts in which they are situated. One hypothesis for Yossi’s motivation to bring me in to his discourse using the FS ‘you’ would suggest that he wants to personally educate me (the young, female, American ‘new immigrant,’ psychology student and interviewer) as to the ‘Israeli experience’ overall, and being in a TA, in particular. This would account for the prominent use of the pronoun throughout his entire interview, but does not specifically explain the particular FS ‘you’ focus on sensory content within his TAN. The following excerpt, which comprises the first part of his response when I asked him to tell me about a positive experience he had had as a bus driver, or something that he loved about being a bus driver, shows the more generalized and timeless use of the FS ‘you’: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 “…there are some people who love {their} job. But, we also, sometimes out of a lack of choice, because you (FS) have to support [your] family. … So I would ge- get up in the morning and go to my job, you (FS) have usually at Egged, about uhh, you (FS) have a fixed work schedule. You (FS) come, you (FS) have a fixed bus line, for instance, line [4] I would work. [I] would go out in the morning, do my line, know th’people, laugh/joke3, throw words out here, every girl, or every guy, whoever gets on, when you (FS) know him and you (FS) become friends, you (FS) like it. You (FS) get up [in the morning], and you (FS) always want to see those’people. Every morning [you (MP)] see them. Regularly. And you (FS) enjoy talking about it, it you (FS) don’t have, umm, uhh, experiences. For instance, sometimes many times I would drive in the morning. I would do-, at 4:30 I would leave here [the house], in the night, dark, rain, downpour, it would be coming down. I drove toward [area]. I would have to do a collection, of drivers, from [area]. In the morning, at 4:45. So I would leave the house aat 4:20, from here, in order to get to [area].” xiv Here, the FS ‘you’ is chosen as the subject when Yossi is making more universalized statements about his usual, or average, day. Again, this choice is significantly opposed to the conventional discursive strategies of Israelis, which would call for the use of the unmarked generic masculine form of this pronoun in this case (Tobin, 2001). There is also always the option to utilize the ‘I’ pronoun in this context, even collocated with the timeless function of the PPr tense—as these experiences did, after all, happen to him personally, no matter how familiar and timeless they have come to be perceived by him now. Indeed, it is evident in L.2, L.4-5, and L.9-12 that Yossi is willing and able to make ‘I’-statements about many of the generalized and timeless facets of this story. And in fact, directly after this Yossi proceeded to tell a detailed story about an old man 3 This is the same word in Hebrew, and thus, it is not possible to ascertain which of these words in English constitutes the best translation of Yossi’s message here. 154 whom he used to pick up every morning and drive to the Western Wall to pray, and how important this daily interaction was for him. Significantly, there was not a single FS ‘you’ in that narrative (which will be presented further below). So there is clearly a non-random choice mechanism at work, and the hypothesis that Yossi’s motivation for these choices lies in a desire to educate me about ‘a day in the life of an Israeli’ or the ‘Israeli experience’ may provide a piece of the puzzle. Another—more psychologically-oriented—hypothesis might be that Yossi’s sense of isolation within what he experienced (particularly due to his depressive symptoms and his battles to have his disability recognized by the NII) leads him to make attempts to bring me in to the experience with him, in order to alleviate some of his pain and solitude within it. This could be viewed as a desire for an ally, or confidante, or perhaps simply an empathic ‘shoulder to cry on’ (i.e., the ‘misery loves company’ syndrome). A possible reflection of this hypothesis can be found in the following excerpt from a lengthy speech into which Yossi launched directly after (or perhaps still as a part of) his TAN, about his struggles with the NII: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “Now against Bituach Leumi it totally became an ‘Arabian Nights’4 story, and still, they gave me twenty percent here, and I went to a psychologist, or to a psychiatrist, uhh, [name], [do you (FS)] know her? And she decided on at least thirty percent and above for me. And so you (FS) have to deal and tell them, what you (FS) are going through, and it doesn’t interest anyone. It doesn’t interest the government, or the, absolutely nobody is interested. Because you (FS) are stuck in a request here, and it doesn’t interest them. They don’t care about it.” xv Here, Yossi chooses to utilize the FS ‘you’ when referring to the need to “deal” with the bureaucracy of the NII and to discuss “what you (FS) are going through,” as well as when he mentions the isolation and sense of frustration he has experienced at the hands of this governmental institution (“you (FS) are stuck in a request”). In this excerpt, it seems clear that Yossi could have used any other pronoun as the subjects for these statements, which only further supports the communicative strategy view. Yossi’s choice to invoke the FS ‘you’ for this particular topic seems to validate the hypothesis that he wants to bring me in to his experience at points that are particularly painful and that may require some ‘extra support,’ in his eyes. Still another—sociopsychologically-oriented—hypothesis asserts that Yossi’s pronoun choice within any given context can be linked to the discursive act of claiming ownership of the feeling, state, or experience described. That is, when he feels uncomfortable, unable, or unwilling to claim certain things as his own or as having been experienced himself, he chooses to discursively ‘give’ them to his audience. In this sense, nearly all of the sensory statements surrounding the TA—which represent Yossi’s passive absorption of the most traumatic effects of it—were collocated with the FS ‘you’ and the PPr tense, in an effort to disown them from himself and 4 The literal translation of this term in Yossi’s text is, “One Thousand and One Nights,” which refers to a collection of Middle Eastern and South Asian folk tales, often referred to in English as “Arabian Nights.” The expression can be understood as a reference to a neverending story, which, according to Yossi, has been his experience of his seemingly endless dispute against the NII, with the aim of getting his disability recognized and compensated appropriately. 155 project them onto me. This may also explain why, in the excerpt directly above, he brought in the FS ‘you’ in his statements about having to talk openly with the NII about his personal problems and being stuck and isolated in his requests from them. Going even further into the realm of the societal and cultural discourse surrounding Yossi, this discursive strategy may be viewed as allowing him to claim the more ‘acceptable’ actions for himself—the strong, unflappable Israeli male—and to distance himself by ‘handing off’ the less ‘acceptable’ facets of his experience to me—perceived as the weak, vulnerable American female. This hypothesis is further validated below, in an exploration of the discursive contexts in which Yossi chose to use the ‘I’ pronoun. 5.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun Yossi’s use of the ‘I’ pronoun appears throughout his TAN discourse as particularly prominent in its stark contrast against his profuse use of the FS ‘you’ pronoun. Returning to a section of text within Yossi’s TAN that was discussed above with regard to the FS ‘you,’ a dedicated focus on the second half shows that it is much more ‘I’-centered: 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 “And now I started to push. When I had already recovered, (2) I started to push, limbs so that I wouldn’t go on top of them. To hear, I went to the middle of the bus, and I came back. And all of this I swallow, that one is moving hands here, moving legs. Everything, all the flesh, [it is] moving people, halves! Moving! I came back to the front of the bus a girl was lying there, she also had wounds, all over her too, full of blood. “Driver, help me.” And so I picked her up. You (FS) get strength, that you (FS) don’t know from where. You (FS) get supernatural powers, it is really, impossible to define it. (2) My door was warped, gashed. Everything was gashed. I grasped the door, by force, I opened a little, and then, and then, only then, a head [of a security person] appeared.”xvi Here, for perhaps the first time since he had begun to describe the bombing itself, Yossi appears as the central character, the actor-agent within his narrative. He “pushed” the limbs, “went” and “came back,” “picked [the girl] up,” and “grasped” and “opened” the door “by force”—all verbs that require one to be alive, potent, conscious, and able to use his wits and strength. Even Yossi’s use of the verb, “swallow,” seems more active here than in the aforementioned excerpt, perhaps because of his use of ‘I’ and perhaps because of all the active descriptive terminology surrounding it (although its collocation with the PPr tense and sensory content attests to a heightened emotional charge, relative to the other statements in this section). Notably, all but one use of the ‘I’ pronoun here is collocated with the RPa (unmarked narrative) tense, giving a strong sense that these actions were not general, usual, or timeless—but rather, were completed once, definitively, only by Yossi himself, and cannot be ‘transferred’ to any other actor or subject. In addition to the form, the content of Yossi’s narrative here also contributes much to an impression of him as the ‘heroic’ actor of the story. Indeed, amidst all the carnage, he hears a girl ask for his help and he picks her up, uses his strength to open the door, and not only brings her to safety but also brings reality and consciousness (in the form of security personnel) into the experience. The image presented is of Yossi as the hero and savior, of the girl and perhaps the rest 156 of the passengers as well, as he was apparently the first to open the door of the still-burning bus. A similar pattern is evident in the excerpt presented below (which directly followed the excerpt above), wherein the ‘I’ pronoun is again used with primarily active and ‘heroic’ deeds: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 “I said “Do me a favor buddy, help me get this woman down [off the bus].” We took her off [the bus], and I got [back on the bus] and I began to check. And all of this inside flames. After III saw that I can’t do anything else, there is nothing, whoever waas wounded jumped out of the bus, fell onto the ground, or flew outside. Because everything went up, everything was pulverized. … Annd uhh, (3) I got [back on the bus] again, I checked, and I stood inside the bus. And then, like, like someone poured tons of, of, of, of steel on you (FS). {When it fell [hit me], I began to cry}, I began, uhh, to scream, uhh, until suddenly, uhh, po-, security guards and policemen arrived, and an Egged member arrived, (2) who came, grabbed my hand. My whole strongbox, all of my things, had flown out. [He] grabbed my hand “Shitrit, what happened? Nothing happened buddy. You (MS) are in good hands.” [He] hugged me, “Come, come we are going to get off [the bus] very slowly.” I told him “But look what happened, look what they did to me”.”xvii Notably, here Yossi refers twice to his having gotten back onto the bus to check for more wounded passengers—it is unclear whether he is repeating himself (as a sharpening technique to emphasize his heroism) or whether he actually did get back on the bus twice. Nonetheless, in sharp contrast to the previous excerpts highlighting the use of the FS ‘you’ collocated exclusively with the PPr tense, this section is told completely in ‘I’-statements collocated exclusively with the RPa tense. Significantly, there are two statements here that can be viewed as both agentic and as representative of the traumatic effects of the TA (“I began to cry,” and, “I began…to scream,” in L.6-7). The examples above demonstrated that seeing, hearing, and sensorily experiencing the acute scene of the bombing was most often attributed to the FS ‘you,’ but here, Yossi’s bodily/emotional reaction to these sensory experiences is placed firmly in ‘I’-statements. This may provide another important clue as to the discursive choice mechanisms at work, in relation to both pronoun and tense. Below is the continuation of a text section above, which came as a response to my request for Yossi to tell a positive story about his time as a bus driver. After a lengthy generalized introduction, with a number of protestations that he didn’t really love his job and didn’t have any real experiences to tell me, he launched easily into this personal(ized) and quite touching narrative: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 “It’s like in the movies. I would meet an old man, (2) elderly, one hundred plus years old, at least. Bent over like this, and walking and walking and walking. A little, like, lit-. Screech, I would stop next to him “Grandpa, where are you going? My lord, in the rain, downpour, Grandpa, to where?” He tells me “My son, I am going toward the Western Wall.” Going to pray, with the praying shawl, with everything. “But my lord,” I tell him, “Rain.” “God will help, what should concern me,” he says. And it doesn’t bother him, he is full of water. “Okay, at least come, I will take you (MS) to, to the Jaffa Gate.” Every morning, I would wait for him. I know that at 4:20 I leave, and he will be walking there. There were days that I would be running a little late sometimes. I mean not late, I would wait there, in order to see him. There were days that I would see him, and there were… ((unintelligible)) and to this day so far, when since then, it was a long time ago, I haven’t seen him. And every morning I would wait for that man, to take him. Back then it was a kind of, kind of, like a kind of, uhh, don’t know, kind of, don’t know, some kind of nice, wonderful, feeling, to wait for that man, to take him toward the Western Wall there. There were beautiful things. There were (2) all kinds of dreams, of, uhh, that’s it, your (FS) daily job. You (FS) liked it, or you (FS) didn’t like it, but you (FS) would get up [in the morning] and go to work. Thirty-six years, or 37 years you (FS) worked on a bus, that you (FS) got on and you (FS) got off. And almost, the bus itself already knew th’way on its own.”xviii Most apparent here is Yossi’s clear ability to speak in the ‘I’-voice when he chooses, and when he 157 finds it necessary and warranted to do so. This narrative is highly personalized and proximate, and the use of the timeless PPr function reflects (and creates) that sense. Notably, Yossi shifts back into both generalized statements and the use of the FS ‘you’ in L.14-16, along with an abrupt shift in narrative mood—most apparent in the contrast between, “There were beautiful things” (L.13), and, “You (FS) liked it, or you (FS) didn’t like it” (L.14-15). This transition was prefaced by discursive clues suggesting some discomfort with the “nice, wonderful, feeling” and “beautiful things” that perhaps Yossi did not want to discuss, as represented by the stammering and repetition of “kind of” and “don’t know” (L.11-12) and summarization phrase, “that’s it” (L.14). The above excerpts, taken from contexts both within and outside of the TAN, show a pattern of choice with regard to when and how Yossi used the ‘I’ pronoun. While the use of the ‘I’ has traditionally been understood to connote a focus on the narrator and the placement of the speaker in the center of the narrative ‘action,’ something much deeper may be involved. Yossi’s collocation of the ‘I’ with the RPa tense in some cases and contexts, and with the PPr or timeless tense in others, along with several notable examples of systematic switching between the FS ‘you’ and the ‘I,’ seem to suggest a more complex communicative strategy at work. Once again, there may be more than one motivation for this complex and varied use of personal pronouns and tenses. Perhaps the most likely hypothesis, however, suggests that Yossi speaks in ‘I’-statements when he is describing something that he is comfortable claiming as his own, such as situations in which he acted in a manner deemed ‘appropriate’ by his own or societal standards. Israeli society has fairly set (and impossibly high) expectations of the behavior of its men—namely, that they take control in uncontrollable circumstances and act as heroes at any cost—and these expectations have become deeply ingrained and internalized, particularly in men of Yossi’s generation. It is therefore possible that Yossi’s choice of pronouns may reflect his beliefs about the ‘acceptability’—both in his own mind and in his perception of the appraisal of his surrounding society—of his behavior during the TA. Thus, when he is discussing a facet of the experience that involves him acting as he ‘should’ have, he uses the ‘I’ pronoun to claim the ‘acceptable’ behavior as his own. Contrarily, when he is referring to parts of the experience that are less fitting with the societal expectations of masculinity and invincibility—such as being affected by the trauma or being acted upon by the terrorist—he is more likely to choose not to use ‘I’-statements. It is within this view that it is suggested that the discursive choices and communicative strategies of the individual can be reflective of the larger social context and societal discourse surrounding him. 5.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN YOSSI’S BASELINE TAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT In order to give a comprehensive picture of the phenomena discussed above, the following table and graph present quantitatively the distribution and percentage computations of Yossi’s uses 158 of (the primary analytical foci) pronouns and tenses within his BL TAN. Table 5-2: Pronoun and tense distribution in Yossi’s baseline TAN ‘Past’ ‘Present’ ‘Future’ % use of each pronoun6 ‘I’ 56 18 1 63% ‘YOU (MS)’ 0 0 0 0% ‘YOU (FS)’ 0 40 0 33% ‘WE’ 4 1 0 4% % use of each tense5 50% 49% 1% There were 78 total lines of text in the TAN, and a total of 120 relevant pronouns (and therefore, 120 tense uses), and the ratio of pronouns per line was calculated to be 1.54. The graph below presents the patterns of collocations of each pronoun with each tense. Figure 5-1: Pronoun-tense collocations in Yossi’s baseline TAN7 With a view on the essential assertions of this study with regard to the extra-linguistic meanings of the uses of the various pronouns and tenses, this graph presents a picture of an individual who, at the time of his first interview, was claiming parts of the TA experience for himself nearly twothirds of the time (nearly half of the time in a less proximate manner), and spending a third of his TAN attempting to bring his audience in to those parts of the experience with him. 5.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF YOSSI’S BASELINE TAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT According to the method described in detail in Chapter 3 (Section 3.6.2.3), the following table is the ‘poetic’ result of the 78 lines of text of Yossi’s baseline TANxix. Poems were constructed only for the significantly-used pronouns, and thus, there are no columns for the MS 5 6 7 The use percentages of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of tense-marked verbs in the TAN. The use percentages of each pronoun were computed as relative to the total number of these pronouns in the TAN. The bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within the TAN. 159 ‘you’ or ‘we,’ as it was established above that these pronouns were virtually nonexistent herein. However, because Yossi often substituted a number of ‘others’ to achieve what was argued to be a comparable communicative strategy (generalization/universalization), a column for the ‘others’ poem is included here. The ‘you (FS)’ and ‘others’ poems are almost exclusively in the ‘present’ tense, and so are not separated by tense as is the ‘I’ poem, but ‘past’ tense entries are noted with a double-underline. It should be noted that all within-narrative dialogue was excluded from both the poems and the tabulations (i.e., the sentence, “I asked them, “Why are you sad?”” would be included in the ‘I’ poem, but “Why are you sad?” would not be included in the ‘you’ poem). Table 5-3: The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Yossi’s baseline TAN8 ‘I’ POEM (74) ‘PAST’ (RPa) (56) ‘PRESENT’ (PPr) (18) 1 32 I also worked I don’t remember the 2 I would leave happy exact hour anymore 3 37 I would come back home I remember it well ----25 53 I didn’t know some of the I still don’t know that an people explosion happened 26 54 I got up [in the morning] I still totally don’t know 27 I prayed that an explosion 28 I went to work happened 29 55 I went out [from the bus What I do know is that terminal] there is darkness 30 56 I did one trip I am all of me– 31 57 I drove I am like looking 33 59 I collected passengers I am looking forward 34 60 I continued to drive I am not looking 36 I got to [street] backward at the whole 38 I told her that “Sit (FS) bus 61 behind me” I am looking 39 68 I got to the [bus] stop I don’t wish on anyone to 40 I collected passengers see it 41 70 I left [the stop] I turn around towards the 42 I didn’t feel anything inside of the bus 43 I thought that it’s a truck that 71I turn back around 80 ran into me hard I look 44 81 I didn’t hear the explosion I do like this with [my] 45 I lost my hearing ear- with my hands, 46 I had lost my hearing already with [my] head 47 82 All of me flew I say to myself things 48 I lost consciousness like, ‘What happened?’ 49 All of me flew --50 105 I lost consciousness I swallow all of this 51 129 I woke up Could I identify you? 52 I got up --62 I woke up 63 I got up 8 ‘YOU (FS)’ POEM ‘OTHERS’ POEM ((42) (2)9) (13) 4 5 When you dr[ive]A bus driver, it’s a 6 (You know) person who knows his 7 You ride [the bus] with him passengers 8 every day He knows the problems 12 9 You get to know people He tells them [his 13 You get on [the bus] problems] 14 You sit in one of the places 10[They (MP)] get onto a 15 You know hello [to] this bus 11 one (FS) Everyone (MS) unwinds 18 You know the people and tells his story 19 You are like a sort of little 16Everyone (MS) gets on 17 family [Everyone (MS)] sits 20 You know the people down 21 You know their problems 22Everyone (MS) 23 --Everyone (MS) is 58 If you are looking smiling 67 You don’t know what it is 24Everyone (MS) is 72 You swallow all of the running around [busy] 35 atrocity Everyone (MS) smiled 73 You look --74 78 You see that your eyes– No one gets near the bus 75 79 You aren’t afraid No one 76 You are scared 77 You look 83 You–10 84 You hear silence 85 You hear from within the silence 86 You look 87 You see people hanging 89 You see people without heads 90 You see all the stuff inside of the bodies 91 If you can imagine 92 If you see a corpse The numbers to the left of each entry signify its location in the overall order of 131 pronominal entries. Three dashes (- - -) signify a significant break in the given sequence, during which other pronouns or tenses were used. Italics here signify a repetition in the discourse. 9 Entries in parentheses signify direct talk with me, as when he asked me a question (rhetorical or not) or interacted dialogically with me. These uses of the FS ‘you’ were not included in the pronoun-tense collocation analyses. 10 Pronouns not collocated with a verb were necessarily not included in the pronoun-tense collocation analyses. 160 You don’t think about it You say that the person maybe is asleep 95 You see mutilated people 96 You see hands moving 97 You see legs very few 98 You see 99 You– --108 You get strength 109 You don’t know from where 110 You get supernatural powers --117 (Did you see the bus?) 125 You don’t see 126 You don’t recognize 127 You don’t know 128 You could be a terrorist with a bag 130 Can you check everyone 64 93 65 94 I stood up All of me was– 66 I was all of me covered in blood, flesh 69 I still didn’t understand what happened --88 I knew a person --100 I started to push 101 I had already recovered 102 I started to push limbs 103 I went to the middle of the bus 104 I came back 106 I came back to the front of the bus 107 I picked her up 111 I grasped the door 112 I opened [the door] a little 113 I said ‘Do me a favor buddy, help me get this woman down [off the bus]’ 114 I got [back on the bus] 115 I began to check 116 I saw that I can’t do anything else 118 I got [back on the bus] again 119 I checked 120 I stood inside the bus 121 I began to cry 122 I began to scream 123 I told him ‘But look what happened, look what they did to me’ 124 Did I see that guy 131 I said ‘No, I really don’t recognize him’ The condensation and juxtaposition of each of these poems together allows a comprehensive picture of the discursive choices and communicative strategies at work in Yossi’s BL TAN. Most apparent here are the conspicuous holes—at different points, of course—in each poem. These discursive breaks, or shifts, may hold an important clue as to the motivations behind Yossi’s pronominal choices. The dominant poem, or viewpoint of the TAN, is clearly the RPa ‘I,’ as is generally the case in narratives told about an encoder’s own life experience. What is significant, however, is when and where and in what contexts Yossi chooses to leave the RPa ‘I’-perspective and describe what he is telling from the viewpoint of another pronoun and/or tense. This analysis discusses each poem separately (as much as possible), with a focus on the holes in each and the clusters in the others that fill those holes. The RPa ‘I’ poem opens the TAN itself and commences prior to the day of the TA, with three generalized introductory sentences about Yossi’s life as a bus driver before his life was changed forever. His phrase, “I would leave happy” (entry (E.) 2), seems to reflect the main 161 message he wants to send about this time—everything was good, and he was happy, before. There is then the first prominent and lengthy hole, during which Yossi switches to the FS ‘you’ and ‘other’ pronouns to continue telling the story of life before the TA. He then returns to the RPa ‘I’ to begin narrating the day of the attack—waking up, praying, and going to work. This part of the story is narrated systematically, with detail and seemingly nothing left out. Yossi continues, discussing the bus route, his collecting of passengers, and his arrival at the fateful bus stop. Then comes the explosion, and Yossi’s admissions that he didn’t feel or hear anything. At this point (E.45-52, 62-63), the poem is filled with repetition: “I lost my hearing,” twice successively; “All of me flew,” twice and, “I lost consciousness,” twice, interspersed with each other; and, “I woke up,” and, “I got up,” paired together once and then again ten entries later. Here appears the second hole—bookended by these two repetitions of, “I woke up,” and, “I got up”— which exists (almost literally) between Yossi’s loss of consciousness and his subsequent regaining of it. The discursive series directly before and after this hole seems to be a narrative climax for the RPa ‘I’-voice, as it is the only place in the poem where Yossi describes what happened to him and what he experienced in the acute context of the bombing. It makes sense, therefore, that the discourse here is filled with repetition and confusion. It is here, surrounding this climax, that Yossi makes his only descriptive reference to the gore of the scene from the viewpoint of the RPa ‘I’: “I was all of me covered in blood, flesh” (E.66). Directly after this is a nearly complete hole of over 30 entries, most of which is filled by the FS ‘you’ pronoun and all of the gruesome details of the bombing scene. When the narrative returns to the point of view of the RPa ‘I,’ its focus, perspective, and content have changed dramatically— from waking up, getting up, being “covered in blood,” and not “understand[ing] what happened,” to, “I started to push” (E.100). The ensuing series of seven entries (100-107) places Yossi in the center of the action, as the heroic agent who has recovered from the blast and has begun to ‘get to work.’ All of Yossi’s actions here—“push[ing] limbs” (repeated twice), going and coming “back to the front of the bus” (also repeated twice), and picking up a wounded girl—are placed firmly into the RPa ‘I’-voice. The only exception is a single statement in the PPr ‘I’ poem—“I swallow all of this” (E.105)—which, significantly, relates to metaphorical action rather than physical action. After a brief hole of only three lines, Yossi again returns to the RPa ‘I’-voice with another unbroken cluster of twelve entries (111-123), to continue the narrative of what he did on the bus directly after the bombing. The final three entries (121-123) in this series relate to the emotional effects of the experience, with Yossi describing how he “began to cry,” and “began to scream,” and began to realize what had taken place. And finally, the RPa ‘I’ poem ends with Yossi’s admission (to the security person who later interviewed him) that he did not identify the terrorist before, nor could he do so in any pictures afterwards. This concluding sentence seems particularly significant, 162 perhaps suggesting that Yossi is still struggling with feelings about his role in not preventing the TA. It is notable that, although the majority of the RPa ‘I’ poem deals with Yossi’s active, sentient behavior during the TA, the final sentence rests on his inability to perceive and act accordingly. The PPr ‘I’ poem begins late in the TAN (E.32), and already focused on the topic of the attack itself, with two single statements about Yossi’s remembering of facets of the TA experience —the first regarding what he does not remember and the second regarding how well he does remember. After a significant absence of entries (during which the narrative was centered around the RPa ‘I’) the poem then continues with a nearly uninterrupted sequence (E.53-61) that entirely fills the first hole of the RPa ‘I’ poem. This series of entries is occupied first with not knowing and knowing, and then with looking and not looking, broken only by a single FS ‘you’-statement also about looking (E.58). It seems that the climax of this poem exists here, in its only significant cluster, which is filled with the confused and confusing sensory experience of the moment directly following the bombing. The rest of the PPr ‘I’ poem consists of single or paired statements, primarily concerned with “turn[ing] around,” “looking,” making gestures with his hands, asking himself what happened, and “swallow[ing] all of [the atrocity].” Overall, this poem subsumes less ‘hard’ action than the RPa ‘I’ poem, but also less traumatic content than the ‘you (FS)’ poem. The ‘you (FS)’ poem begins in a generalized manner, interspersed intricately with the ‘others’ poem. Both begin by telling the story of the (also intertwined) experience of being a bus driver and a bus passenger in the generic and universal life before the TA. Everything was normal and pleasant, and everyone was smiling and talking and familial before. Notably, here Yossi seems to have chosen the FS ‘you’ and the various ‘others’ for nearly identical functions; that is, to present a generic and timeless picture of what was. Nearly every one of the entries up until the first holes in both poems could have their pronouns interchanged, and the meanings would not change. The ‘you (FS)’ poem then has an extensive hole of 37 entries, after which it offers two single statements punctuating the RPa and PPr ‘I’ poems and then two significant clusters (E.72-77 and 83-99) that encompass nearly all of the gruesome, sensory descriptions of the acute scene on the bus directly after the bombing. Here, the poem is exclusively in the ‘present’ tense and almost exclusively related to senses and emotions—looking, seeing, and hearing, in addition to (metaphorical) swallowing and being afraid/scared—as well as cognitive experiences such as imagining, thinking, and saying. Significantly, both of these clusters exist chronologically within the largest hole in the RPa ‘I’ poem, the boundaries of which are marked by, “I still didn’t understand what happened” (E.69), and “I started to push” (E.100). This suggests that all of these sensory and cognitive experiences constitute the narrative of what Yossi experienced between his immediate disoriented state and his later awakening and regaining of his ability to act (‘appropriately’). Indeed, nearly all of the trauma and distressing images are here, in the ‘you (FS)’ 163 poem—while the ‘heroic’ actions actually performed by Yossi are ‘allowed’ into the RPa ‘I’ poem. The only exception to this lies in entries 108-110, in which Yossi’s statements about “get[ting] strength” and “supernatural powers,” and not knowing from whence they come, are attributed to the FS ‘you.’ It could purportedly be expected that, according to the central hypotheses of this research, any discussion by Yossi of strength or power would be assigned to the ‘I,’ and so these three entries in the ‘you (FS)’ poem seem, at first glance, out of place. However, it is possible that, for Yossi, getting strength and powers—from an unknown source, no less—rather than organically having strength and power, may illustrate exactly the type of weakness that he does not want to own. Indeed, the hypothetical alternative statements, “I had strength,” or “I felt like I had supernatural powers,” feel very different in both form and content. Nonetheless, these three entries comprise the only place in the ‘you (FS)’ poem in which there is any mention of real action or strength. The rest of this poem comes toward the end of the overall TAN, with a short series of statements dealing with the inability of ‘you (FS)’ (ostensibly Yossi) to identify the terrorist. Here, Yossi alternatingly puts the FS ‘you’ into the role of the generic bus driver as well as of a terrorist, and seems to again be placing onto the female subject what he may view as his vulnerability. From this view, his possible sense of guilt, responsibility, or inadequacy stemming from his inability to have recognized the terrorist and, therefore, to have prevented the attack, may still be something Yossi has not fully processed or worked through. Ultimately, the entirety of each of these four ‘poems’ of Yossi’s BL TAN provide a condensed but comprehensive picture of his experience of the TA and the discursive messages he may be attempting to send while narrating it. It appears that the primary objective of the ‘others’ poem is to present the story of the ‘everyman,’ both the generic driver and passenger on a generic bus, that exist in Yossi’s pre-TA world and context. This poem serves to provide the counterbalance to the acute narrative of the TA; the before to the ‘I’ and ‘you (FS)’ poems’ during and after. In contrast, the primary objective of Yossi’s RPa ‘I’ poem seems to be to present his actions, both before the bombing and after he ‘recovered’ and managed to become an effective helping agent (that is, a ‘hero’), but not during the period of time in between (when he was presumably too affected to act ‘acceptably’). Related to this, it appears that the main purpose of Yossi’s PPr ‘I’ poem is to present a minimal explanation of what happened to him and why he was unable to act ‘appropriately’ during the immediate bombing experience and aftermath—indeed, the precise period of time ‘missing’ from his RPa ‘I’ poem. The concentration of this facet of the experience into the ‘present’ tense reflects a higher degree of emotional charge for him, and its status as more salient, ‘alive,’ and ‘close to his heart’ than those facets described in the RPa ‘I’ poem. Finally, it seems that the central aim of Yossi’s (wholly PPr) ‘you (FS)’ poem is to present all the traumatic and upsetting content that is almost entirely ‘missing’ from all of the other poems. 164 This largely consistent pattern suggests that the ‘acceptable’ (i.e., agentic, courageous, powerful, and heroic) acts are worthy of ownership by the ‘I,’ while the less ‘acceptable’ experiences (i.e., those demonstrative of vulnerability, emotional affectedness, and perhaps weakness) are brought in or passed off to the discursive domain of the FS ‘you’ subject—who, herself, may be perceived by Yossi as more weak, vulnerable, and worthy of ownership of less heroic behavior. This deep discourse analysis of Yossi’s BL ‘pronoun poetry’ thus indicates that his choices surrounding pronoun and tense use may be highly influenced by his perceptions of what his surrounding society deems as satisfactory behavior and emotions for an Israeli male such as himself. Because, at the time of the BLI, these perceptions may have contributed to Yossi being unable or unwilling to claim ownership of certain (particularly distressing) aspects of the TA experience, he may also have found it difficult to fully process, work through, and put into perspective these aspects or even the entirety of the experience. In this sense, Yossi’s discursive and narrative patterns may provide insight into his inner psychological world and functioning. 5.5 CONCLUSION This chapter began from a broad, holistic perspective on Yossi himself and the BL interview context and experience, then explored the overall patterns in his discourse, then showed a basic quantitative view on the non-random distribution of these patterns, and finally moved to a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of both the form and content of these patterns. This deep, spiraling analysis of Yossi’s outward discursive choices was employed in an attempt to understand his related inner psychological beliefs, attitudes, and perspectives. The following two tables summarize the central findings of Yossi’s BLI, within the discursive and narrative categories that were analyzed. The first focuses on Yossi’s patterns of pronoun (and tense) use. Table 5-4: Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Yossi’s baseline interview PRONOUN USE First-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases Third-person ‘others’ MS ‘you’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING Suggests sense of responsibility and guilt for his bus, for his passengers, for what happened Suggests solitary sense of experience—“what happened to me” Suggests generalized, ‘usual,’ timeless experiences with generic, universalized subjects— perhaps disconnected from them himself Relative lack suggests very little need or ability to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize (in)to a larger context world Suggests great and acute need to bring audience in to the most difficult, upsetting sensory facets of experience—either to educate, to join him in his pain and solitude, and/or to ‘take on’ the projected traumatic and/or passive (‘unacceptable’) facets instead of him Suggests desire and ability to claim ownership only of parts of experience in which he was acting ‘heroically’ (‘acceptably,’ in his perception of the viewpoint of Israeli society) Relative lack suggests very little to no acknowledgment of the collective nature of experience—“what I did”; “what happened to me” FS ‘you’ ‘I’ ‘We’ 165 As a final analytic summarizing exercise, presented here is a list of suggested hypothetical messages, or end points (Spector-Mersel, 2011), which appeared central to Yossi’s BL TA narrative and discourse. Some of them may have appeared as more conscious or less so, but all were either expressed manifestly or seemed present in some way between or underneath the lines of his interview text. While most are essentially my own formulations of what I understand and interpreted as Yossi’s ‘main messages,’ I believe they most succinctly summarize and illustrate the results of the comprehensive analyses conducted throughout this chapter—with regard to both form and content, and particularly related to the found pronoun and tense use patterns. Table 5-5: Suggested ‘main messages’ of Yossi’s baseline interview I experienced the ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. You (FS) experience the worst, most traumatic, and least ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. Various ‘others’ generally take on the generic/universalizable facets of the terror attack. You (FS) are traumatized by the terror attack. You (FS) are affected by the (many) sensory, most emotionally distressing facets of the terror attack. I was active and behaved ‘appropriately’ during parts of the terror attack experience. I did not act heroically or ‘acceptably’ enough during other parts. What happened to my passengers, on my bus, might have been my fault. I think there was a societal/cultural expectation that I could or should have done more to prevent the terror attack. I need and deserve the support of Bituach Leumi since the terror attack. I am in a constant battle with Bituach Leumi, and I feel helpless and angry. I was and am all alone in my experience of the terror attack and its aftereffects. I feel alienated and isolated from my surrounding community and society because of my sense of aloneness in my experience of the terror attack. The terror attack is a big part of my life—it is my life. The terror attack split my life into (before vs. after) pieces, and my life and I will never be the same. Yossi’s baseline interview provided a particularly rich opportunity for an investigation of the link between an individual’s hypothesized inner psychological world and his perceptions of the societal attitudes and cultural discourses surrounding him. Yossi was found to have used the FS ‘you’ in his discourse to a much greater degree than the other participants in this study, as well as to have been the most psychologically and emotionally affected by his terror experience. Thus, Yossi was presented here as an exemplar of a difficulty ‘owning’ the most traumatic and ‘weak’ (that is, least ‘heroic’) facets of his TA experience, and of a heightened need and attempt to bring me in to these facets, for various possible reasons. Yossi’s case will be revisited in Chapter 9, from a longitudinal comparative perspective on both of his interviews and on his narrative processing over time. 166 CHAPTER 6 DANI: “YOU (MS) ARE NOT NORMAL IF YOU (MS) WON’T BE SCARED” — AMBIVALENCE ABOUT AND UNIVERSALIZATION OF THE LESS ‘ACCEPTABLE’ EXPERIENCE 6.1 INTRODUCTION TO DANI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS At the time of the baseline interview (BLI), Dani was 29 years old and single. He is a native-born Israeli. He did not excel in his studies, and did not finish high school. He served in the Border Patrol in the Jerusalem area during his mandatory army service. Dani had been working as a bus driver in Jerusalem for six years, and he experienced three different terror attacks (TAs) over the course of his employment as a driver: once when a terrorist opened fire with an automatic weapon on his bus while it was standing at a bus stop, wounding a number of his passengers (henceforth referred to as the ‘main’ TA); once when he witnessed a bus in front of him explode from a bombing; and once when a single bullet was fired at his bus as he was driving. He also was involved in a traffic accident (TrA) while driving his bus that nearly fatally wounded a pedestrian, which he described as the most traumatic of all of these incidents. Dani was working as a bus driver at the time of the BLI, and he proudly reported that he had never taken more than a two-day break after any of the incidents he experienced. I was referred to Dani by the social worker at Egged. The BLI lasted approximately two hours, and was conducted in two parts (about a month apart). During the first session, he was an open, cooperative, and willing interviewee, although it was difficult at times to elicit unbroken narratives from him. Indeed, as much as I attempted to present only open-ended questions and probes that encouraged narrative responses, Dani often persisted in turning the interview into a ping-pong-style question-and-answer session. (On the other hand, the narratives that did emerge were fluent and extremely rich.) When we first met and I realized how close we were in age, I was initially concerned about possible potential interpersonal complications such as Dani trying to flirt with me—and in truth, at times I did feel that he was being more friendly and familiar with me than the other interviewees had been. In addition, because Dani was living with his parents at the time, both BLI sessions were conducted in his bedroom. This was initially off-putting for me, but he was polite and respectful of my boundaries throughout. In any case, I left the first session convinced that Dani had enjoyed the interview and his time with me, and that it would not be a problem to schedule and complete the second session. In actuality, I could not have been more wrong. While Dani initially seemed very willing to be interviewed and it was easy to schedule the first interview session, it took an enormous amount of (unrequited) effort on my part to schedule the second. Near the conclusion of the first session, Dani had made a comment that the questions I had asked had been incisive and had caused him to “think a lot.” I took this as a compliment, but over the course of the subsequent month, I began to question whether Dani saw this as a positive facet of our interview together. I had informed him at the time that we had not finished all the questions I had for him, and began attempting contact with him soon after the first session. To my chagrin, Dani frequently did not answer my calls or return 167 my messages, and repeatedly claimed that he was having “trouble” with his cell phone. However, when I blocked my phone number (so it appeared on his Caller ID as “anonymous”), he always answered right away—and then, upon realizing it was me, would profess to be busy and promise to call me back within the hour. Of course, he never did, and this process repeated itself several times over the course of the month following the first session. Once he offered to meet me in a coffee shop for a half hour, saying that was “all [he] could do,” but I persisted, telling him we would need a quieter atmosphere to conduct the conclusion of the interview. When we finally managed to schedule and conduct the second BLI session, Dani’s behavior was decidedly different than in the first. He was pleasant upon my arrival, but also seemed distracted. The television was on when I entered his room, and he avidly watched a soccer game while I set up my recording devices and prepared my materials. When I announced that I was ready for us to begin the interview, he was still watching television and did not make a move to turn it off. He seemed to want to keep it on, and perhaps to just mute it, throughout the interview, and when I requested that he turn it off he seemed a bit surprised. Although he gave the appearance and lip-service of being willing to talk, I couldn’t help but feel opposition in some of his behavior. It seemed that he made less eye contact with me than in the first session, and his responses to my questions were decidedly shorter and less descriptive. While there were enough interview questions left over from the first session to fill at least an hour (assuming he would have responded in a similarly open and cooperative fashion), the second session lasted a mere 22 minutes. I left the interview confused and frustrated, with no comprehension of what had caused this dramatic shift in Dani’s mood and willingness to speak and share with me. Lacking the courage (and uncertain if it was appropriate) to confront him directly about it, I could only assume at the time that this process of being asked such pointed questions and being required to “think a lot”— perhaps in ways with which he was unfamiliar and uncomfortable—about his experiences had been unpleasant for him, for reasons about which I could only speculate. My overall impressions of Dani after the BLI were that he was utterly committed to presenting a picture of himself as unaffected by the TAs he had experienced, and as a strong, ‘manly man’ who could handle any dangers that might be thrown at him. It was unclear to me, however, whether this picture actually reflected his internal experience, or whether he only wished it did. My initial instinct suggested the existence of a disconnect in this domain, or an ambivalence that created the discomfort that Dani seemed to be feeling in (at least the second, if not both) interview context(s). Although this chapter focuses predominantly on Dani’s BLI, the experience of and events surrounding his follow-up interview (FUI) were also illuminating and significant for the overall analytical process, and therefore, necessitate a brief discussion here. When I initially decided to conduct FUIs with the study participants, my biggest concern was Dani. I had spent a great deal of 168 time analyzing his BLI and focusing on the ambivalence that I felt was expressed significantly therein, and a number of important hypotheses and findings had emerged from this process. I felt desperate for the chance to hear how (or whether) his narratives had evolved over the ensuing three years, and to clarify some issues that had become critical to my developing research. Unfortunately, I was just as desperately convinced that, if (for whatever reason) Dani had been so reluctant to cooperate in the previous interview session, persuading him to participate in yet another one would be virtually impossible. And sure enough, although my first phone call to Dani after three years of no contact was met with warmth and enthusiasm, the familiar pattern of avoidance and empty promises commenced again shortly thereafter. Finally, after a (second) month of this, my desperation got the better of me and I left a (slightly overly) honest, pleading, and yet forceful message on his voicemail, to the effect of: “Hi, it’s Alison again. It seems clear that you do not want to participate in another interview with me, which is unfortunate but I understand. But when you tell me that you are willing, and to call you back, I cannot ignore that because I very, very much want to interview you again. If you are just being polite, then I will understand and not call you again. I don’t want to bother you unnecessarily, but I must try as hard as I can to get you to participate, because another interview with you will be so important to my research. Your first interview helped my work so much, and I really hope that you will be willing to be interviewed again, to help my project and the others out there who will benefit from this study.” To my great surprise, Dani called me back immediately, telling me he was so pleased to hear that his first interview had helped my work and that he would be “happy to help again.” We scheduled the FUI for the following week, and he complied fully. The FUI with Dani lasted approximately an hour and a half, and he was once again open, cooperative, willing, and forthcoming. He stated repeatedly how happy he was to be involved, and how proud he was to be a part of helping me and others. Several times he professed his belief that my doctoral project felt to him like his “own doctoral project” (which felt, to me, like a bit of an overstatement). Apparently, my phone message had had a significant effect on him, and I realized that through this lengthy interactional process I had learned a crucial lesson about Dani himself: it was of supreme importance to him to feel needed, useful, and valuable. This message is evident in Dani’s interview texts as well, but had I not been lucky enough to see it manifestly and learn it in our direct interactions together, it is quite possible that I would have missed it in my analyses. 6.1.1 Dani’s ‘non-narrative’ narratives Dani constituted a particularly complex case for analysis, as an ability and/or willingness to tell long, flowing, uninterrupted narratives—the type abundant in Yossi’s texts, for instance— simply was not apparent in his interviews. Most of his ‘narratives’ resembled reasonably lengthy 169 ‘answers’ to my pointed questions, which were asked out of necessity (stemming from the repeated blank stares and silences I received whenever I posed a more open-ended narrative request). It seemed that Dani was most uncomfortable with the open-endedness and abstract nature of the request for his “life story,” and simply did not appear willing or able to construct a global narrative of this nature. Indeed, his life story had no real narrative structure; rather, it was dominated by staccato bullet points with almost no elaboration, and filled with requests for information and elaboration by myself. (The opening set of statements of his life narrative was: “I am 29 years old. I was born in Jerusalem. Live in [neighborhood in Jerusalem]. (1) Single. (3) Work at Egged for six years.”i) On a number of occasions throughout his interview, I attempted to wait and allow the silence between us to encourage elaboration on Dani’s part, but it rarely came. More often than not, I would ask an open-ended question, he would respond with a bullet point or extremely succinct statement, and then we would look expectantly at each other until I finally ‘broke down’ and asked a follow-up question. The predominant majority of Dani’s texts were thus less than ideal for the purposes of this type of narrative research, and so I often found it difficult to decide which ‘narratives’ to analyze. Indeed, the conventional narrative approach guiding this research prescribes that, if a researcher’s aim is to understand and draw conclusions about an interviewee’s discursive choices, analysis must rest on his spontaneous, uninterrupted responses to the most open-ended interview questions (such as, “Tell me your life story,” or, “Tell me what happened”). This guideline dictates that this type of ‘untampered-with’ data can most credibly reveal the interviewee’s choices in both form and content, and the moment that an intervention (e.g., a new question posed by the interviewer) or interruption (e.g., any type of validation, such as “Really!”, or invalidation, such as “Are you sure?”) beyond basic ‘reflexive listening’ techniques (e.g., “Um-hmm”) takes place, these choices are consequently potentially influenced, redirected, and/or negated entirely. I believe in these methods, and wherever possible, I attempt to employ them. In analyzing Yossi’s interviews, for example, it was easy to locate and identify the lengthy, uninterrupted narratives and, because of their abundance, a focus on only a small number of these texts resulted in a deep analysis. In Dani’s case, however, I consistently felt it unavoidably necessary to widen the analytical ‘net.’ As mentioned previously, for instance, Dani’s life history included four potentially traumatic incidents—three separate TAs, as well as a TrA that he himself referred to as “traumatic.” The result was multiple ‘narratives’ of analytically significant content—each with varying degrees of ‘completeness,’ narrative coherence, depth, and volume; and none, unfortunately, entirely free of intervention by myself. All were punctuated by at least one additional probe from me (beyond, “Tell me about it”) to attempt to garner more details, and most included many questions of this nature. All of Dani’s narratives were relatively brief and ‘thin,’ characterized by much less 170 elaboration and more of a ‘report’ style than most of the other interviewees’ narratives. Because of all these issues, it felt neither appropriate nor useful for the analytic focus of this case to rest solely on one or another of Dani’s narratives of the terror attacks he experienced (TANs); and due to space constraints of this dissertation, it could not rest on all of his significant narratives. Therefore, this chapter utilizes a spiraling approach, not only among the levels and depth of analysis, but also with regard to the analytical unit itself. That is, as in Yossi’s case, the analysis herein begins with a general overview of Dani’s BLI and the specific pronominal patterns across the discourse. The ‘magnifying glass’ is then focused on Dani’s narrative of the traffic accident (TrAN), from both a quantitative and then qualitative ‘poetic’ perspective. Finally, a multilayered analysis is presented as a comprehensive interpretive exercise to gain deeper insight into Dani’s beliefs and attributions. In this final section, both the form and content of Dani’s narrative of the ‘main’ TA are analyzed from a holistic perspective, and then ‘validation’—from Dani’s own more direct statements on the relevant topics—is brought in to shed light on the interpretive claims put forth throughout the analysis. These analytical choices were not made easily, and may certainly be open to criticism or disagreement, but I believe that they allowed the most multidimensional and comprehensive view possible (within the space constraints) of Dani and his discursive messages, and of the theoretical and interpretive assertions central to this study. 6.2 DANI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT Relative to the other interviewees and throughout his BLI, what most characterized Dani’s discourse was his propensity for statements utilizing the second-person masculine singular pronoun (‘you’ (MS)) in its generic message function. He appeared significantly preoccupied with the universal ‘you’ and what the ‘normal’ Israeli man would or should have done in situations such as he described. Nearly every time Dani discussed how he had behaved during any of the many stressful events in his life history, his statements included declarations and/or suppositions about what “you (MS)” would do, or what he wished he had done, or what he felt he “should” have done. I claim that this pronominal pattern suggests an ambivalence in his own appraisal regarding his behavior, and an uncertainty in his perceptions of his surrounding society’s appraisal (that is, possible disapproval) of his behavior—particularly during the main TA Dani experienced. Overall, as in Yossi’s chapter, this section focuses on four prominent and distinct patterns of pronoun choice throughout Dani’s BLI, all of which were reflected in his various narratives. First, he utilized first-person possessive pronouns (such as, ‘my’) and solitary phrasing as a strategy toward creating a sense of personalization and possession of the topics or objects of reference. Second, as aforementioned, he utilized the MS ‘you’ as a strategy toward creating a sense of generalization, universalization, and normalization. Third, he utilized the second-person feminine 171 singular pronoun (‘you’ (FS)) as a strategy to bring his audience in to the experience he was describing. Finally, he utilized the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’) as a strategy toward claiming ownership of the experience or role he was describing. Each of these strategies is discussed in turn below, with demonstrative examples. 6.2.1 Personalization, possession, and separation: Use of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases Throughout the BLI, Dani frequently used discourse that appeared to be personalizing the events he had experienced and those who experienced it with him through the use of first-person possessive pronouns. Similar to Yossi, Dani spoke of his casualties almost as if he were on active military duty in the middle of a war, with soldiers in his command who were hurt on the battlefield. At one point, when discussing the passengers on his bus who had been struck by the shooter’s bullets, Dani said, “my people were wounded.”ii This signifies a form of personalization, particularly as the statement could be formulated in other ways (e.g., “There were wounded people on the bus”). In the context of the TA he witnessed, Dani also showed a tendency to separate himself and focus on his own solitary experience, rather than on the collective nature of the experience for all those who existed within it at the time. This is apparent in the following account: 1 2 3 4 “This is the third terror attack. (1) Like that I really saw LIVE LIVE I saw it LIVE you (FS) know, [line] [28] takes a left toward [street name], line [10] also takes a left, line [10] passed through th’intersection, stood at the bus stop, went past the stop a little bit, exploded went up, {and I am still standing here and seeing the whole scene, maybe 30 meters away from me, and hearing everything}.”iii From this excerpt alone, it sounds as if Dani was standing on a sidewalk, alone, watching these events unfold. In reality, however, the surrounding context of his account reveals that Dani was on duty at this time, at the wheel of a bus full of passengers who all witnessed the same event. There was a collective quality to this experience to which Dani’s discourse here does not attest; rather, the picture he paints is a wholly solitary one, with no mention of those who shared it with him. Indeed, similar to Yossi’s BL discourse, aside from mentioning those of his passengers who were wounded in the main TA on his bus, Dani rarely related to the other ‘experiencers’ of any of the TAs he narrated. This singular focus on himself may reflect Dani’s preoccupation with his feelings and attributions with regard to his behavior before, during, and after the TAs he experienced. 6.2.2 Depersonalization, universalization, and normalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun The most apparent and significant pattern in Dani’s BL discourse was his frequent use of the MS ‘you’ pronoun, almost exclusively in conjunction with statements existing on a more universal and generic plane, rather than those that could be tied uniquely to him and to his own experiences. His recurrent, patterned use of the MS ‘you’ seemed to have two predominant themes. With few exceptions, the MS ‘you’ was found to appear in Dani’s discourse when he was 172 describing an experience or feeling that: 1) was distanced from him or from which he was making an attempt to distance himself; and/or 2) involved him being passive or acted upon by some other (presumably more active and powerful) ‘character’ (e.g., a terrorist or bus passenger). There exists a complementary or interactional nature to these two themes, as often Dani seemed most invested in distancing himself from events or experiences during which, in his perception, he behaved in a passive (that is, in his mind, ‘unacceptable’) manner. The interpretive claim, therefore, is that Dani—most prominently so, of all the participants —utilized the MS ‘you’ to achieve two discursive goals: 1) as a distancing technique, either as a reflection of an experience or feeling that was already distanced from him or as an attempt to distance something that was ‘too close’ to him, for whatever reason; and 2) as a universalizing, collectivizing, and/or normalizing technique, used when Dani may have been uncomfortable with his own role in the experience or feeling he was describing, and thus as part of an attempt to place it into the realm of his surrounding society’s appraisal of ‘acceptable’ or ‘appropriate’ behavior and norms. In Dani’s texts, these discursive strategies were often utilized together, and at times it was difficult to discriminate between them. Because their purposes are essentially complementary, it often appeared unnecessary to even make this distinction. Yet, there were places in Dani’s discourse when each technique did appear separately, and these cases proved informative for the interpretive process overall. Presented below are several examples of Dani’s extensive use of these techniques, both separately and complementarily, in his patterned usage of the MS ‘you.’ Dani’s discourse surrounding the various TAs he experienced as well as the TrA in which he was involved was filled with the MS ‘you,’ particularly in places where the topics of responsibility and guilt surfaced. For instance, within the TrAN Dani stated: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “…you (MS) say “Oh no what did I do? What did I do? How did I not put on the brakes? How did I not avoid him?” But you (MS) go back over it you (MS) say “I didn’t see him at all, and what am I going to tell the investigator?” And your (MS) whole heart is shaking. Now gone is the driver’s license gone is the job, gone is the, you (MS) work that job. And you (FS) see him down there, he doesn’t get up, he doesn’t anything, tons of blood from the head, you (MS) killed a person that’s it you (MS) killed a person. (2)”iv Here, Dani’s use of the MS ‘you’ is apparent in its distancing function, particularly in relation to the elements of the narrative within which it seems most difficult for him to reconcile his own role. His internal dialogue with himself (“you say…”, in lines (L.) 1-2), his mental reconstruction of the circumstances (“you go back over it,” in L.2), and his emotional reaction (“your whole heart is shaking,” in L.3) are all placed onto the distanced, generic MS decoder. Perhaps most significant here is Dani’s repetitive declaration, “you (MS) killed a person” (L.5-6), in which he chooses to place a charged action onto the generic other. The emotional import of this is apparent, as the possibility that he could have been involved in a fatal accident is likely extremely distressing for Dani. As he seems unable to allow himself to discursively own this statement and this possibility, 173 the MS ‘you’ is recruited here again within its distancing/generic function, as Dani’s attempt to cope narratively with the experience. Later in the interview, I asked Dani to compare the TAs he had experienced with the TrA, to which he responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “Umm, it’s difficult. … Because it [a traffic accident] is something that you (MS), (1) {that it is up to you (MS)}. … Here [in a terror attack], {it isn’t up to you (MS)}. You (FS) understand? … {If you (MS) can prevent it, (1) so of course you (MS), (1) you (MS) will have a little something} onnn [your] conscience. … {Understand (FS)}? As long as it isn’t up to you (MS), {so okay, what? What can I do? It is the fate}. … I didn’t kill {th’people on my bus. It is the terrorist killed them, not me. Understand (FS)}?”v Rather than personalizing his statements (e.g., “If I could have prevented it, so of course I would have had a little something on my conscience”), Dani again employs the generic MS ‘you,’ giving a sense of distance and collectivity to his assertions, as if it is something that ‘we all’ would grapple with, and that has very little to do with him personally. Significantly, Dani does shift to ‘I’statements when he says that he did not kill the people on his bus (L.5), although he could easily have continued using the distancing language (e.g., “You didn’t kill the people on your bus”). This may lend further support to the hypothesis that patterned pronoun choice represents ability and willingness (or lack thereof) to claim ownership of the referenced experience, role, or emotion. Indeed, it could be hypothesized that it is easier for Dani to personalize statements related to his lack of culpability for the TA on his bus, while it may still be difficult for him to use personalized language with regard to his own role in the TrA that he was unable to prevent. It may thus be more comfortable for Dani to universalize the experience and position himself (as much as is possible discursively) as a member of a larger collective, to which any of these events could happen. The next excerpt exemplifies the use of the MS ‘you’ as a marker of passivity or being acted upon. When I asked Dani to talk about the worst part of being a bus driver for him, he responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “The worst? \((thinking to himself)) The worst, I think\ it’s the, it’s that [they (MP)] get on your (MS) nerves, that like {passengers} curse you (MS) out. … {[They (MP)] threaten you (MS) [they (MP)] spit at you (MS) that’s the worst in my opinion, like you (MS) are serving them annd sometimes you (MS) know, not everything has to be on the dot, so it happened that there is traffic, \((whispering)) he curses you (MS) out\}. … {((coughing)) That’s the worst thing in my opinion, that a passenger like curses you (MS) out and wants to hit you (MS) or raise his hand to you (MS)}.”vi Here, Dani utilizes the MS ‘you’ in its generic and generalizing function, while showing a consistent pattern of placing the bus passengers as the actors and the MS ‘you’ (himself, essentially) as the passive receiver/object of their actions. One could even suggest that Dani is placing the passengers in the role of the perpetrator and himself in the role of the victim. The behaviors of note are conventionally active—cursing, threatening, spitting, hitting, and handraising—as well as less so—such as getting on his nerves. Each of these phrases that utilize the MS ‘you’ could be formulated differently, in a manner that places Dani as more of a central character. This would be accomplished primarily by recruiting ‘I’-statements and placing the ‘perpetrators’ in the predicate of the phrase, utilizing the active (focus on agent) rather than passive 174 (defocus of agent) forms—as in, “I get cursed out by passengers,” instead of, “Passengers curse you (MS) out”; or, “I get spat at,” instead of “[They (MP)] spit at you (MS).” Thus, a distinct discursive choice is evident on Dani’s part, in positioning the main character in his description as the passive, generic male grammatical object, rather than as himself as the ‘I’ subject. Perhaps his apparent lack of willingness to own these experiences stems from some level of shame that he has been the receiver of such disgraceful behavior, or perhaps he is uncomfortable with the fact that his career as a bus driver places him in a serving role (perhaps perceived by him as inferior, as evident in, “you (MS) are serving them,” in L.3). Or perhaps Dani is simply attempting to send the message that all bus drivers—that is, the everydriver—experience such things from time to time. Another prominent content theme that was frequently associated with the MS ‘you’ in Dani’s discourse surrounded fear and its psychological repercussions, such as being hypervigilant or being reminded of the fearful experience afterwards. When Dani discussed the main TA he experienced and his perceptions of fear, his discourse showed even greater vacillations between uses of ‘I’ and the MS ‘you,’ as seen in this section of text: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “I was afraid, I was afraid. But when it is/was1 over, you (MS) kind of say ‘My God, (2) what a,’ like, if it had been possible, I would have wanted to go back and do the things better, maybe to kill him {too}. You (FS) understand? … I was afraid, it doesn’t mean that I wasn’t afraid I was shhhaking. There is no I don’t have, what is it? A pers-, ^a terrorist is shooting at you (MS) from four meters away, you (MS) won’t be scared? You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be scared. I screamed “Mama!” and everything^.” vii Above, L.1, in particular, exposes a certain level of confusion between the personalized and the generalized experiences—that is, between references to his own lived experience of the attack (“I was afraid, I was afraid”) and more universalized statements using the unmarked subject (“you (MS) kind of say…”). It is as if, while Dani is willing here to admit that he was afraid, he may have difficulty remaining ‘in the moment,’ within the personal experience and connection to the full realization of what could have happened to him. Directly after this, he shifts to statements about what he would have liked to have done or wished he had done—a method he utilized repeatedly throughout his interview (i.e., retreating to the ‘safety’ of assertions about what he could have done, after making more psychologically threatening or distressing statements about what he did not do). Here, too, is a demonstration of the generalizing/collectivizing technique, with which Dani attempts to convince his audience that, ‘Yes, it’s okay that I was afraid, because the MS ‘you’—that is, any normal person—would be afraid in this situation.’ Later in the BLI, when I asked Dani what he was feeling and thinking in the moment that he first got back onto a bus following this TA, he responded: 1 2 3 1 “The truuth? (1) On, on that specific [bus] line, when I passed by from thhe it, a bit {you (FS) know, you (MS) are looking, you (MS) are on alert, on alert}. You (FS) know what that is? … {On alert is liike, YOUR HEART BEATS, YOUR (2) yoouu (MS) are looking, you (MS), are expecting It is not possible to ascertain whether Dani was using the ‘past’- or ‘present’-tense morphological form of this verb. 175 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 something. A different [bus] line like it doesn’t matter what what but when you (MS) pass by there you (MS) also know you (MS) are reminded you (MS) say “Wow, God, what a miracle I had here.” You (MS) look you (MS) say “My God right here it happened to me right here that I pulled over right here” I am trying to}, to penetrate it. … You (FS) understand? To penetrate th’, mem- memory. … And it comes back, sometimes. {You (MS) say “My God”, suddenly} brrrrr, ((sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)) {you (MS) hear the, like} echoing in your (MS) head. Reminded. Yes? Sometimes [you/they/we (MP) are] reminded.”viii I then asked him if this still happens for him, to which he responded: 1 2 3 “Iin no. In, the pas- two years no no. (2) In the first year it would happen to me a bit. … {You (MS) are reminded a bit, the, the cries the you (MS), suddenly you (MS) know (FS) are reminded (MS) you (MS) start to uhh, (2) get}, (2) a memory kind of.” ix Both of these excerpts seem to demonstrate Dani’s attempts to distance himself from the emotional effects of the stressful experiences he has undergone, but the distinctly patterned usage of the MS ‘you’ appears to provide another important clue as to his narrative ‘agenda.’2 Indeed, keeping in mind the central contentions of this research regarding the FS ‘you’ pronoun and given the emotional charge of many of these statements of Dani’s (“you (MS) are reminded,” in the first quote above (L.5) and twice in the subsequent quote (L.1-2); “you (MS) hear the…echoing in your (MS) head,” in L.9), it might have been expected that these statements would have appeared in conjunction with the FS, rather than MS, ‘you’ pronoun. This might have reflected the emotionality and difficult psychological nature of these “remind[ing]” experiences, and an attendant need to bring me in to them. As was apparent in Chapter 5, this was a central hallmark of Yossi’s BL pronoun use patterns. So why might Dani be showing such a different pronominal pattern, by keeping these experiences in the generalized, universalized, distanced and generic MS ‘you’ discursive sphere? Perhaps they do not possess an emotional component for him, or perhaps he is attempting to deny their inherent emotionality. Or perhaps he is intent on not bringing me in to these experiences, for myriad potential reasons. Dani’s discursive patterns here warrant a deeper investigation, and the following excerpts provide added insight into his narrative choices. The first is from his response to my request that he tell me about specific incidents that happened to him during his army service: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 “There is, uhh an incident when a Molotov cocktail that [they (MP)] threw at us onto the Jeep, the Jeep [mistaken word], caught fire, (2) and we had to react fire extinguishers and extinguish th’Jeep, annd get all of theee equipment out [of the Jeep]. (1) Stones, rocks, shooting. A Frigidaire that falls on you (MS) from, do you (FS) know what a Frigidaire is? … A Frigidaire in a refugee camp, {[they (MP)] bring it down onto you (MS) on the po- from the porch, down}. If you (MS) don’t pay attention, the Frigidaire, crushes you (MS). … Understand (FS)? Dangers like those that you (MS) need to anticipate them. If you (MS) are complacent and laid-back, (1) so you (MS) will be surprised.”x The next text comes from Dani’s description of the first moments of the main TA, when the terrorist opened fire, and his attempt to stay level-headed and act ‘appropriately’: 1 2 2 “If right now I won’t find a solution and if right now I go into shock and if right now I get scared and my hands will shake, \((whispering)) as far as I’m concerned it’s over, the story is over.\ {Right now The term, agenda, here is employed and should be interpreted cautiously, as any motivations Dani may have for his narrative choices are likely subconscious or unconscious. 176 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I need to forget everything, make a cut, be me, and my head and not think about anything, think about what you (MS) are doing, \((whispering)) you (MS) and yourself (MS)\}. … {Bend down, underneath the steering wheel as he is still firing at you (MS), bullets are exploding [all around] you (MS), [he] keeps on firing, and fires and fires and you (MS) drive meanwhile and the whole foot is on the gas and fly out of there as fast as possible. Understood (FS)? … Because if you (MS) won’t do that, you (MS), and you (MS) killed another 20}. … {Because if he hurts you (MS) the story is over for the whole bus, because he messed with me, he didn’t mess with any’one \((whispering)) else\}.”xi Both of these quotations have one apparent theme in common: Dani’s professed perceptions of how one ‘should’ react—the ‘acceptable’ and normative behavior for an Israeli man who may find himself in such a situation. In the first text above, he describes what can happen to someone who isn’t paying attention (“the Frigidaire, crushes you (MS),” in L.5-6) or who is “complacent and laid-back” (“you (MS) will be surprised,” in L.7-8). The message here appears to be that a ‘good’ Israeli (male) soldier is vigilant, on guard, and constantly capable of anticipating possible dangers and reacting quickly, efficiently, and appropriately. In the second text section above, Dani begins with a series of ‘I’-statements, emphasizing his own personalized role and obligation to act ‘correctly’ at the time of the TA. But by the end of L.3, he has switched to the MS ‘you’ subject, which is employed exclusively for the remainder of the excerpt. Here, the MS decoder is positioned as both the passive receiver/object of the attack (“he is still firing at you (MS),” and, “bullets are exploding [all around] you (MS),” in L.5-6) and as the focus of Dani’s instructions as to the ‘appropriate’ manner of acting in such a situation (“think about what you (MS) are doing,” in L.3-4). Dani’s statements in L.7-8 are perhaps the most powerful, expressive, and demonstrative of this communicative strategy, sending the message: ‘If you (MS) don’t act the ‘right’ way, you (MS) will cause irreparable harm.’ He emphasizes this even further in L.8-9 with the message that, essentially, if he had been wounded during the TA, all of the passengers on the bus would have been killed. The responsibility that Dani has placed on his own shoulders here seems enormous, and this is also reinforced in L.9, when he states outright that the terrorist “messed” only with him, and not with anyone else. Based on these interpretations of Dani’s communicative strategies, the central underlying message of his BLI can be hypothesized as essentially: ‘The correct and acceptable way of being an Israeli man is to be brave, heroic, vigilant, constantly prepared and ready to act, and able to execute the correct solution immediately and without fear or paralysis.’ Accompanying and often in dialogue with this message seems to be Dani’s entrenched hope that he fits this description, despite possible evidence to the contrary. If the statements further above, in which Dani mentioned some of the psychological effects of the TA, are revisited, a more comprehensive understanding of his narrative choices can be achieved, as viewed from within the prism of Dani’s apparent deep need to prove that he fits his own prescription for the normative Israeli man. Indeed, when Dani refers to looking, being on alert, and expecting something, he seems to be sending the message that these are the ‘appropriate’ behaviors for a bus driver working during 177 an Intifada. And when he refers to being reminded of the TA and hearing the echoes of the terrorist’s weapon firing in “your (MS)” head, Dani appears to be attempting to express that this, too, is appropriate and acceptable. Dani seems intent on universalizing and collectivizing even these very personal and emotional experiences, because if they are able to be successfully placed into the sphere of the Israeli everyman, then it may be ‘tolerable’ that he experienced them himself. If, on the other hand, they discursively remain Dani’s own personal ‘owned’ experiences, they may be deemed reflective of pain, trauma, or weakness—which, in his perception, is ‘unacceptable.’ His narrative ‘agenda’ here appears to be to send the message: ‘A good Israeli man acts like A, C, and E.’ Dani’s actions and experiences—B, D, and F—are not exactly on this list, so he places them discursively onto the ‘good’ Israeli everyman, and seems to hope that they ‘stick.’ The result is that the expanded definition of a ‘good Israeli man’ includes actions and experiences A, B, C, D, E, and F, and Dani can then theoretically fit into it. This is Dani’s most overarching patterned discursive strategy and most prominent narrative message, appearing in various themes and forms throughout his interviews. Below is a discussion of how this influences his use of the FS ‘you.’ 6.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun While Dani employed the FS ‘you’ often, nearly all of these usages were recruited for direct dialogue with me or questioning directed toward me, such as, “you (FS) know…”, or, “Do you (FS) understand?” These phrases were frequently rhetorical in nature, or even seemingly used as ‘filler’ or phatic3 communicative discourse—indeed, more often than not, Dani did not appear to be seeking, and did not wait for, a response from me. Further analysis found that Dani’s use of the phrases, “Did you (FS) understand?”, “You (FS) understood?”, and, “you (FS) know,” was higher than that of the other participants. This, along with a more frequent use of English words throughout his discourse, seemed to reflect Dani’s apparently significant need for understanding (Perez & Tobin, 2009). This usage of the FS ‘you,’ however, is not the focus of this study. What is of significance here is the finding that Dani did use the FS ‘you’ in discursive contexts that did not involve direct talk with me—the FS ‘you’ of analytic importance for this research—but did so extremely rarely. There were so few of these usages throughout the BLI, in fact, that the pattern of collocation of the FS ‘you’ with statements of sensory content appears even more prominent and systematic. Indeed, although Dani used the FS ‘you’ much less frequently than did Yossi, when this pronoun does appear in Dani’s discourse, it is most often in sentences that were significantly similar to those made by Yossi. For example, as he talked about the passengers on his bus who had been wounded by the 3 In linguistics, a phatic expression or communication constitutes a speech act that has the sole function of performing a social task, as opposed to transmitting information. 178 terrorist, Dani said, “Becauuse some one of them took some two bullets to the foot, and so much blood, my God, you (FS) see the blood, you (FS) know the steps of the bus trickle drip drip drip, like some kind offf, stream, of, water of blood.”xii Later, Dani described his experience of witnessing a bus bombing, stating, “A second later you (FS) see his roof go up in the sky with a boom, like you (FS) never {heard} in your (FS) life.”xiii Here, the FS ‘you’ is employed by Dani along with sensory verbs, such as seeing and hearing, in the most difficult and emotionally charged contexts, such as the immediate lived experience of the TAs. As mentioned in Chapter 5, Yossi also used the phrase, “you (FS) see,” repeatedly, all followed by highly graphic images of the TA scene (e.g., “hanging people,” “mutilated people,” and “hands moving”). It is notable that, even with the dearth of (non-dialogic) FS ‘you’ pronouns in Dani’s discourse, the FS ‘you’ was used in nearly identical manners and contexts in both Dani’s and Yossi’s texts. Below is part of a narrative Dani told about going back to pick up his bag, which had been taken by the bus company for safekeeping after the TA, while Dani had been taken to the hospital: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “You (FS) see the bus, afterwards I went to the [Egged bus] branch, because myy bag was still there everything, [they] took my’bag. Two weeks later, I, for no particular reason, am straightening up, I see tons of glass shards inside the bag. Tons of glass. (4) {In the end, and you (FS) see the bus, my God, it is full of holes, I am saying “Whoah, God, III, really,” I ow-, owe God my’life}. Understand (FS)? (3) A huge miracle, let’s (FS) say that. A big miracle happened here, in my case, that I wasn’t hurt, there are some who, you (FS) know, were killed. PLEASE, IIII ((chuckles)).”xiv Here again, the phrase, “you (FS) see the bus,” appears twice in the exact same formulation (L. 1 and L.3). Dani shows a willingness and ability to use an ‘I’-statement to describe seeing glass shards inside his bag (L.2-3), in addition to declaring that he, and no one else, owes God his life (L.4). But concerning his experience of seeing his bus after the TA he experienced, Dani repeatedly chooses the collocated use of the FS ‘you’ and the proximate-present (PPr) tense. This pattern is reiterated as a consistent communicative strategy to distance the encoder (the actual ‘experiencer’) from the experienced events and draw in the FS decoder, while sending the message that the events described are both proximate and experience-able to the encoder (and decoder). As mentioned above and as in Yossi’s case, Dani’s choice to employ the FS ‘you’ most frequently coincides with sensory statements and highly charged or descriptive contexts. The following excerpt from Dani’s TrAN further reflects this: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “Someone jumped in front of me into the street, totally ran without looking, and I hit him in the corner I didn’t see him even, I didn’t step on the brakes didn’t anything like like a mosquito that gets into your (FS) car and you (FS), (1) no chance, all of a sudden [it is] smeared on your (FS) window, boom to the corner. (1) Really the truth really it I was scared totally it really, you (MS) say “My God, I killed the man,” and I didn’t kill him. {But you (FS) see him lying there, poor guy, covered in blood}.xv Here, Dani utilizes the FS ‘you’ in a highly vivid and descriptive simile, likening the collision of his bus and the pedestrian to what happens to a mosquito who gets trapped in “your (FS) car” and then smeared all over “your (FS) window” (L.2-3). Then, again, the phrase, “you (FS) see,” is employed in Dani’s description of the pedestrian lying on the ground after the accident, “covered in 179 blood” (L.5-6). Later in the narrative, Dani used this phrase again: “And you (FS) see him down there, he doesn’t get up, he doesn’t anything, tons of blood from the head, you (MS) killed a person that’s it you (MS) killed a person.”xvi These examples, in addition to those presented directly above, appear qualitatively divergent from the previous excerpts showing Dani’s use of the MS ‘you,’ and this reflects the communicative opposition created by the distinct discursive patterns and strategies at work here. Dani employs the FS ‘you’ in personalized contexts that entail experiences that were emotionally charged and difficult for him, and that he may not be ready or able to ‘own.’ There is no need or opportunity for generalization or universalization regarding these experiential elements, as there appears to be no societal agenda involved. In Dani’s apparent perception, these facets of the experience are those that the ‘good Israeli man’ should not undergo, because he should have been vigilant and heroic enough to prevent or avoid them in the first place. But once the mistakes have been made and the damage done, it is the FS ‘you’ who discursively experiences them and their effects. Indeed, being involved in a TrA in which Dani’s bus caused enough damage that he had to see the victim laying in a pool of his own blood can absolutely be traumatic, and Dani himself referred to the experience as such. So it makes interpretive sense that, similar to Yossi’s case, it would be the most traumatic and emotionally charged elements of the experience that Dani might feel the need to bring me in to or project onto me, as a coping mechanism. This is equally relevant to the texts above, in which the FS ‘you’ was collocated with experiences such as seeing the blood of his passengers trickle down the bus steps during the main TA; seeing and hearing the roof of a bombed bus explode into the sky; and seeing his bullet-riddled bus after the attack. These difficult experiences are brought into the discursive realm of the FS ‘you’ because, according to the central assertions of this research, the ‘ideal’ MS ‘you’ shouldn’t see, hear, or have them; and the ‘I’ doesn’t want to own that he did actually see, hear, and have them. Dani’s use of the FS ‘you’ in the following excerpt seems to even further support this hypothesis. When I asked him about his thought processes during the Intifada, and whether he still thinks “nothing bad can happen” to him or whether any fear did or does enter into his consciousness, he responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “IIII at at work no but sometimes you (FS) know that you (FS) are thiinking for no reason like all of a sudden you (FS) are alone with yourself (FS) {you (FS) say, (2) ‘Something like that can happen yes, that suddenly’} (1) but that only you (FS) like in in a muse like that like you (FS) doon’t, you (FS) are alone with yourself (FS), you (FS) and {your (FS) own personal} peace. {You (FS) like for no reason are sitting and thinking. That’s when I am thinking. If I am sitting down it happens to me a lot that I take a walk and III think}.” xvii Here, the FS ‘you’ is employed repeatedly, in one of the rare appearances in Dani’s interview of discourse surrounding introspection and contemplation. As Dani’s discourse is so much more often focused on doing, acting, and experiencing, both the content and form of the excerpt seems to 180 reveal that Dani is not entirely comfortable with questions (and answers) regarding his thought processes and consciousness. To that end, he once again places the actions and experiences that he is not willing or able to own onto the FS ‘you,’ where they can presumably be more easily and comfortably discussed and explored. Essentially, the topic here is vulnerability (as seen in the statement, “you (FS) say, ‘Something like that can happen yes…’, in L.2-3), and the FS ‘you’ is deemed by Dani the appropriate pronoun for such a sensitive topic. Significantly, the same pattern is evident in the following text section, which was Dani’s response when I asked about the atmosphere in his bus branch during the Intifada: 1 2 3 4 “There was tension. (2) There was also a bit of fear. People {like that you (FS) know}, diiidnn’t, didn’t laugh like they used to laugh there were terror attacks and all that, so the atmosphere a bit uhh, was kind of calm quiet. [They (MP)] saw that there is tension. (2) Annd uhh, but we went out to work and everything. But how do you say? You (FS) feel {the tension} in the air.”xviii Here, again, the FS ‘you’ appears exclusively in the sensory context of feeling, and in the PPr tense. Indeed, in Dani’s discursive view, “you (FS) feel the tension,” but, “we went out to work”—the Israeli men did what needed to be done in a difficult situation, and the female decoder deals with the emotional repercussions. This patterned collocation strategy again seems connected to vulnerability—as the distanced statement, “There was tension” (L.1), is imparted with no pronoun subject; and the generalized, “[They (MP)] saw that there is tension” (L.3), is placed onto the generic MP decoder; but the more emotionally charged statement, “You (FS) feel the tension in the air” (L.4), is projected onto the sensing, feeling, thinking, experiencing FS decoder. This hypothesis that vulnerability exists in Dani’s discourse almost exclusively in the feminine realm may be further reinforced in the following excerpt, from Dani’s description of having seen the terrorist for the first time directly before the main TA: “He [the terrorist] stood there, a regular guy with a coat it was rain, {and suddenly he took out th’M-16, you (FS) can’t do anything}.”xix Placing this text into dialogue with all the other examples presented here, a significant discursive contrast is evident between Dani’s perception of the MS decoder—the strong, capable, active, heroic Israeli male—and the FS decoder—the weak, helpless, powerless, ‘actedupon’ female. Whenever Dani is describing actions or experiences that fit into the latter category, he employs the FS ‘you,’ as he may not be able or willing to claim them for himself. Essentially, he couldn’t do anything at that moment, and he felt helpless and powerless. On the other hand, whenever Dani is describing actions or experiences he believes he should have or wishes he had done or had—that is, the ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ actions and reactions—he employs the MS ‘you,’ in an apparent possible attempt to make himself worthy of claiming them. And whenever Dani seems able to claim any actions or behaviors that fit into the description of the ‘ideal’ Israeli male, he does—using the ‘I’ pronoun. This discursive process is discussed below. 181 6.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun Dani’s use of the ‘I’ pronoun seemed to reflect an overarching preoccupation with his own role—particularly in the TAs and the TrA in which he was involved—which was a prominent theme in both the form and content of his BLI. Below is a lengthy section of Dani’s main TAN, in which he described in detail his own actions before and while the terrorist was shooting at his bus: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 “I am driving, line [20], it was winter [February], rain. (2) Approach the [bus] stop, take on th’passengers, take, you (FS) know, [bus] passes money MONEY. Suddenly I see someone come to a standstill in front of {the bus. (2) I do [gesture] to himmm} like ‘Go past.’ (1) Because I want to put on the blinker and leave the [bus] stop. (1) And then he winks, winks his eye at me does like this ((winks at me)) I do to him like ‘What?’, [he] opens the zipper coat, takes ouut a rifle and ‘Brrrrrrrrrr’ ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)). {And I didn’t se-, am I dreaming? I DREAM? What? What?} Straight away, like liike [they] taught me in the army, I bent down underneath the steering wheel, I am small. (1) The bullets passed me over my head he fired in a burst, innn automatic, not single shots, ONE ONE, in a burst. I went down underneath, with the steering wheel, and I started to drive, I wanted to run him over but he got away. Now I look, I see him pump full of bullets thhh’people who were at the [bus] stop. (3) What did I do I escaped,4 I went to the right towards [name of hospital], there was a hospital nearby, I went off the route, now my people were wounded, tons of blood on the bus. I said ‘I have to get th’bus out [of here], otherwise he is tearing us apart,’ understand (FS)?”xx This entire narrative section is told exclusively in ‘I’-statements, with not a single use of secondperson pronouns of either gender (save for direct dialogue with me), thus creating a sense that Dani was present, active, and agentic in this experience. Here, amidst all the chaos, he is able to think quickly enough to not only save his own life, but also to spring into action, extricate the bus and its passengers from the scene, and bring them to safety and to critical medical attention. As in Yossi’s similar texts, the image presented here is of the bus driver as the hero and savior of those in his charge. There is only a hint of remorse in Dani’s discourse here, in his statement about what he had wanted to do (L.10), which notably also makes use of the ‘I’ pronoun. This is one of many assertions of this nature, always told in a personalized and ‘owned’ manner, which reveals what may be an underlying sense of guilt and inadequacy with regard to his behavior during this event. Notably, the extra-linguistic messages of the pronouns Dani utilized here interact and correspond with the meanings of the tenses in his discourse. That is, there is a near-absolute split in the patterned tense use between the first and second halves (L.1-7 and L.7-14, respectively) of the above narrative, with the former being told in the PPr tense and the latter almost exclusively in the remote-past (RPa) tense. The primary dichotomy within the two sub-sections can be hypothetically stated as, ‘what happened to me’ vs. ‘what I did in response’ (or, more generally, ‘what was done to me’ vs. ‘what I did’). While Dani positions himself as the central character of the narrative overall, the use of the PPr tense in the first half suggests an even more proximate, salient, and ‘alive’ experience. This may be motivated by a number of possible, related issues: 1) this description of 4 There appears to be particular significance to Dani’s word choice here, as he could have said, “left/went out,” or, “drove,” or even, “I got the bus out of there.” The choice of the word, “escape,” here seems to suggest a value judgment Dani may be placing on his own behavior, and indeed, this was not the only time this word and/or its root was used. This will be discussed further, later in this chapter. 182 the terrorist’s active role in the incident may have been experienced by Dani as more upsetting and stressful, and this manner of telling the narrative may be a reaction to the painful effects of the event; 2) Dani may be attempting to make this part of the story more proximate to me and/or easier for me to imagine; and/or 3) Dani may not want to fully claim his role discursively in this part of the narrative because, as the terrorist was acting, Dani’s role was more passive and less ‘heroic.’ In the second half of this narrative section, the use of the RPa tense gives a stronger sense that what Dani is describing happened definitively, in the past, and that the experience is more distanced. This, coupled with the exclusive use of the ‘I,’ creates a narrative picture that Dani was present, active, and central in this experience. This section describes his response to the terrorist’s actions, and Dani presents himself as having acted immediately and decisively. He “bent down,” “went down,” “started to drive,” “got out of there,” “went to the right,” and “went off the route”— all immediate and effective actions, spurred by quick thinking and vigilant reaction to the emergent situation at hand. By comparison, although Yossi’s TAN had only a very small number of sections in which any ‘I’-statements were made, there is a significant similarity between his and Dani’s texts in this context, in their collocated use of the RPa tense with their own agentic actions. At the exact narrative moment in which both Yossi and Dani began to describe their own roles and actions within their respective TA experience, they switched from PPr to RPa tense, and showed an increased use of the ‘I’ pronoun. Dani’s only return to the PPr tense in this section (“Now I look, I see him pump full of bullets…”, in L.10-11) is again in relation to the terrorist’s actions, and not his own; that is, although the main character in the sentence is still Dani, the main actor is the terrorist. This essentially supports the third hypothesis offered above. The next example provides the larger narrative context of one of the shorter quotations that was presented above as a demonstration of Dani’s use of the FS ‘you’ (L.3-4 below). This larger excerpt, in which Dani discusses the bombing he witnessed on the bus in front of him, makes even more evident the specific contexts in which he makes his patterned pronominal choices: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 5 6 “Again I am telling you (FS), luck. Hhee, went through the traffic light which was turning yellow, went left, like passed through the intersection, and I God said to me, ‘Stay there. Don’t need to take it [the light], for what? So two more minutes what is wrong with that?’ A second later you (FS) see his roof go up in the sky with a boom, like you (FS) never {heard} in your (FS) life. Immediately I understood that it is a bombing, I made a U-turn I escaped from that place. (1) I escaped.5 You (FS) know what kind of a smell [there was]? My God. Smoke was coming out of its roof. The roof of the back section [of the accordion-style double bus], totally split into two. So you (FS) say luck? I believe in luck I believe in luck I be-, no doubt about it. Luck is a separate part of me. 6 You (MS) say that if I had stayed close to him, {I also would have gone}. Because that bombing demolished his whole back section, {that huge bus}.”xxi Again, Dani’s choice of the word, “escape,” here is notable (particularly as repetition is utilized as well), as again, a variety of alternatives were potentially available to him. The meaning and possible significance of this narrative choice will be discussed in further detail, later in this chapter. It is not entirely clear what Dani meant by this statement. From the context, it seems likely that Dani may have said or meant, “inseparable,” or, “integral,” rather than, “separate.” The original recording may not have been of a good enough quality to permit a correct hearing of this phrase, or there may have been some confusion on Dani’s part. 183 Dani again appears to make an unambiguous switch between the FS ‘you’ and the ‘I’ when the action and actor in the sentences shift from relatively passive and potentially emotionally difficult, to active and fitting of ‘correct’ Israeli male behavior. This can be observed in L.3-4, when the FS ‘you’ is used with seeing and hearing the effects of the bombing—presumably the most emotionally charged elements of this experience for him. This contrasts with the subsequent two sentences (L.4-5), in which Dani utilizes the ‘I’ in conjunction with the actions of understanding that a bombing had taken place, making a U-turn, and escaping (twice) from the immediate scene of the TA—all initiatives performed consciously and agentically by Dani, rather than by someone else and imposed upon Dani. According to the central hypotheses of this research, these actions may constitute the epitome of Dani’s perception of ‘acceptable’ behavior for an Israeli man in such a situation, and his combined use of the ‘I’ pronoun and the RPa tense reinforces this message. Most evident here is the pattern regarding which parts of the experience Dani is willing and able to claim, and which elements might be perceived as less ‘acceptable’ (that is, those exposing weakness or passivity, in his view) and are, therefore, projected onto a second-person decoder. 6.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN DANI’S BASELINE TRAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT As previously mentioned, Dani’s life history included four analytically significant incidents —three separate TAs, as well as a TrA that he referred to as “traumatic.” Theoretically, the analysis proposed in this study does not necessarily require a narrative of a TA, per se; rather, just a particularly emotionally charged narrative. It was thus particularly critical in this case to decide on the appropriate text unit upon which to focus the analytical ‘magnifying glass,’ in order to best explore Dani’s discursive patterns. It became clear throughout Dani’s BLI that the TrA he experienced was actually more difficult for him than any of the TAs, and upon analysis of the narratives of all four incidents, it emerged that Dani’s TrAN was indeed the most representative of the patterns discussed above, which were apparent across the BLI overall. That is, as Dani appeared to use the MS ‘you’ to a greater degree throughout his entire BLI than did the other participants, he showed the same proportional pronoun usage in his TrAN. Therefore, in order to give a comprehensive picture of the phenomena discussed above, the following table and graph present quantitatively the distribution and percentage computations of Dani’s uses of (the primary analytical foci) pronouns and tenses within his BL TrAN. 184 Table 6-1: Pronoun and tense distribution in Dani’s baseline TrAN ‘Past’ ‘Present’ ‘Future’ % use of each pronoun9 ‘I’ 19 13 0 52% ‘YOU (MS)’ 4 8 0 19% ‘YOU (FS)’ 1 (2)8 6 (5) 0 11% (11%) ‘WE’ 0 0 4 6% % use of each tense7 38% (3%) 43% (8%) 6% There were 43 total lines of text in the TrAN, and a total of 63 relevant pronouns (and therefore, 63 tense uses), and the ratio of pronouns per line was calculated to be 1.47. The graph below presents the patterns of collocations of each pronoun with each tense. The directed/phatic uses of the FS ‘you’ (those not of analytic significance) are not represented. Figure 6-1: Pronoun-tense collocations in Dani’s baseline TrAN10 With a view on the essential assertions of this study with regard to the extra-linguistic meanings of the uses of the various pronouns and tenses, this graph presents a picture of an individual who, at the time of his first interview, was claiming parts of the TrA experience for himself half of the time (nearly a third of the time in a less proximate manner); spending about one-eighth of his TrAN attempting to bring his audience in to those parts of the experience with him; and universalizing and/or normalizing parts of the experience nearly one-fifth of the time. 7 The use percentages of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of tense-marked verbs in the TrAN. The number outside the parentheses signifies uses of the FS ‘you’ that are of analytic significance (e.g., “You (FS) see the blood”), while the number inside the parentheses refers to phatic talk with me, as when he asked me a question (rhetorical or not) or interacted dialogically with me (e.g., “You (FS) understand?” or, “you (FS) know…”). 9 The use percentages of each pronoun were computed as relative to the total number of these pronouns in the TrAN. 10 The bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within the TrAN. 8 185 6.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF DANI’S BASELINE TRAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT Just as the above quantitative representation of these data is useful to uncover patterns, so the qualitative view is an essential part of the analytical puzzle. According to the method described in detail in Chapter 3 (Section 3.6.2.3), the following table is the ‘poetic’ result of the 43 lines of text of Dani’s baseline TrANxxii. Here, again, poems were constructed only for the significantlyused pronouns. The ‘you (MS)’ and ‘you (FS)’ poems are almost exclusively in the ‘present’ tense, and so are not separated by tense as is the ‘I’ poem, but ‘past’ tense entries are noted with a doubleunderline. And again, all within-narrative dialogue was excluded from both the poems and the tabulations. As discussed previously, Dani’s narratives were not uninterrupted, and the three questions that I asked herein are represented as breaks in the overall ‘poetry.’ Table 6-2: The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Dani’s baseline TrAN11 ‘I’ POEM (32) ‘PAST’ (RPa) (19) ‘PRESENT’ (PPr) (13) A: When you were driving? 6 You say “My God, I killed the man” 2 I hit him 3 I didn’t see him 4 I didn’t step on the brakes 5 I was scared 7 I didn’t kill him 9 I couldn’t…get back to the wheel right away 10 I got on [to the bus] 11 I said “I have to be at home a bit” 12 A: So tell me, after you did that, what did you do? 20 Maybe I am guilty You take the statements of 17 I am a bit guilty[?] the people who were 18 I am a lot guilty? with you 21 --You drove slowly 31 [That] I am not at all guilty 22[It’s his luck that] it was at all you 23 You say to yourself ‘Dammit, I’m not guilty and not anything, so why let him destroy my…, I’m not guilty. I did what I could do. I tried to get away, I didn’t succeed’ 32 You say ‘Damn, why let him screw up your…?’ What did I do? 13 What did I do? 14 I did a reconstruction in my head 15 [Where] I could have prevented it 19 I said ‘[Dani], you are not guilty at all’ --28 If I had been a bit guilty 30 [As soon as] I got myself out of the assumption 16 35 48 I almost fainted ‘YOU (MS)’ POEM (12) A: So what was traumatic? I don’t don’t doon’t, don’t 38You say ‘Oh no what did I 11 ‘YOU (FS)’ POEM ((7) (7)12) 1 (You know) --You see him lying there 8 24 You see You come to that situation [conclusion] 26 You say ‘Okay, I’m totally okay, I’ll move on. I don’t need to be, uhh, with a guilty conscience. And even if I had killed him, God forbid, like it, I couldn’t have done anything I didn’t do it on purpose of course and I didn’t, and I’m not guilty at all’ 27 (You understood?) 29 (You know) 33 (You understood?) 25 34 Did you go crazy? The numbers to the left of each entry signify its location in the overall order of 58 pronominal entries. Three dashes (- - -) signify a significant break in the given sequence, during which other pronouns or tenses were used. Italics here signify a repetition in the discourse. 12 Entries in parentheses here signify direct phatic talk with me, as when he asked me a question (rhetorical or not) or interacted dialogically with me (e.g., “You (FS) understand?” or, “you (FS) know…”). 186 I said “That’s it, I finished him” 37 I said “That’s it you can’t let [this] go” --47 I received a letter 36 think about it anymore even 50 I take it as a motto 51 I am called a nonchalant driver 52 I am a slow driver 53 I like calm 54 I am [on] line [28] 55 I like a few people 57 I like to be calm with myself 58 I work with a small amount of people do? What did I do? How did I not put on the brakes? How did I not avoid him?’ 39 You go back over it 40 You say ‘I didn’t see him at all, and what am I going to tell the investigator?’ 41 You work that job 43 You killed a person 44 That’s it you killed a person --You see him down there 45 (You know) 46 You go even more crazy 49 (You know where) --56 (You know all the Jerusalem [bus] lines) 42 Again, as in Yossi’s case, the dominant poem, or viewpoint of the TrAN, is clearly the ‘I,’ or ‘ego,’ as is generally the case in narratives told about an encoder’s own life experience. However, the RPa ‘I’ is not as dominant as might be expected, and in fact, none of the poems takes a principal piece of the narrative ‘pie.’ This analysis discusses each section separately (as much as possible), with a focus on the holes in each poem and the clusters in the others that fill those holes. After an initial single dialogic use of the FS ‘you,’ the RPa ‘I’ poem opens the TrAN, and tells the majority of this first section. The focus here is primarily on what Dani did and did not do: he hit the pedestrian, he was scared, he got on the bus, and he said; while he didn’t see the pedestrian, didn’t manage to brake in time, didn’t kill him, and couldn’t get back to work right away. The emphasis here is on actions—or rather, the dialectic between action and inaction. There is no thinking, no knowing, no sensing, and very little feeling. Significantly, while this section of the ‘I’ poem contains a reference to not killing the pedestrian (entry (E.) 7), the MS ‘you’ poem contains a single reference to the opposite—the possibility, expressed in imagined and generalized dialogue, that he had killed him (E.6). Similarly, while the ‘I’ poem contains the statement, “I didn’t see him” (E.3), the FS ‘you’ poem contains, “You see him lying there” (E.8). These direct oppositions seem to lend further support to the hypothesis regarding Dani’s possible discomfort with owning some of the most upsetting and difficult sensory facets of the experience—such as the realization that he could have killed the pedestrian, and the raw and presumably terrifying vision of seeing him on the ground after the impact (contrasted with not having seen him at all before the impact, and thus, having been unable to prevent the accident). In the second section of the TrAN, which was prompted by my request for a chronological recounting of the ensuing sequence of events, Dani continues in the RPa ‘I’-perspective, and in his focus on his own actions and non-actions. He repeats the phrase, “What did I do?” twice in a row (E.12-13), which appears to signal (in both content and form) a heightened emotional charge or distress. Dani then seems to attempt to calm himself by discussing the apparent impossibility of preventing the accident and using self-talk (this time, notably, with the additional usage of his own name) to reassure himself of his complete innocence—as seen in the use of the phrase, “at all,” in 187 E.19. Inserted between these attestations to his lack of guilt in the RPa ‘I’ poem exist three significant statements to the contrary—or at least, indications of ambivalence and uncertainty—in the PPr ‘I’ poem. Indeed, E.16-18 stand in opposition (again, in both content and form) specifically to the content of E.19, in their questioning and repetition surrounding the subject of guilt. Essentially, the hypothetical dialectic here might be phrased as: ‘I was not guilty at all, but maybe I am guilty to some degree.’ In this case, Dani’s shifts between the RPa and PPr tenses seem to signify his difficulty reconciling the technical fact that he could not have done anything differently with the equally reality-based fact that his bus (and by extension, he) nonetheless caused injury to this individual. That is, in Dani’s apparent perception, factually, he was not guilty of any wrongdoing; but that does not change a possible inner sense of culpability that harm was caused ‘on his watch.’ This seems to connect with one of the central messages of the discourse of Dani’s BLI overall: an apparent deep ambivalence regarding his personal expectations for himself and his perception of his surrounding society’s expectations of him. Here, while Dani “did a reconstruction in [his] head,” and independently concluded that he could not have prevented the accident and that he was “not guilty at all” (E.14-16), he still questions (tellingly, in the ‘present’ tense) whether he is nonetheless “a bit guilty,” or, “a lot guilty” (E.16-18). This may be a compelling representation of Dani’s personal appraisals interacting discursively with his perceptions of his society’s appraisals. At this emotional high point in the narrative, Dani’s discourse shifts abruptly to the MS ‘you’ poem, with four sequential entries (20-23) that seem to attempt to generalize and universalize the experience of the TrA. Most significant is the opposition between E.23 and E.19, whereby “You (MS) say to yourself ‘…I’m not guilty,” but, “I said ‘…you are not guilty…” This seems to provide further indication of Dani’s possible ambivalence and uncertainty surrounding the issue of guilt within this event; indeed, there is not a single entry that simply and unequivocally states, ‘I was not guilty.’ If Dani speaks in the ‘I’-voice about the possibility of guilt or lack thereof, he tends to employ the PPr tense, which suggests a more salient and emotional experience, and possible lack of full narrative processing. And if Dani speaks more definitively about his lack of culpability, he utilizes a mixture of various other pronouns and dialogue, which seems to obfuscate the ultimate message. This can be observed again in E.26, which presents a significant emotional contrast with E.23. While both entries are focused on what could or could not have been done, and both include the phrase, “I’m not guilty” (within dialogue), the MS ‘you’ entry mentions Dani’s attempt to not allow the event to “destroy [his life],” while the FS ‘you’ entry is more attentive to being “okay,” and not having a “guilty conscience.” Ultimately, Dani’s concerns about his culpability and his ambivalence about his actions are all presented in non-‘I’-statements (or rather, ‘I’-statements only within non-‘I’-dialogue), which seem to allow him to work more intensively through the attendant emotionality of the issues. 188 This section of the TrAN concludes with seven entries that switch, one after the other, from poem to poem, with no coherent sequentiality. Dani shifts from talking about being “not guilty at all” (E.19) to possibly being “a bit guilty” (E.28) in the RPa ‘I’ poem, and from possibly being “a lot guilty?” (E.18) to being “not at all guilty at all” (E.31) in the PPr ‘I’ poem. In the MS ‘you’ poem, Dani seems to reinforce his position of anger, adding to his first dialogic statement about “let[ting] him destroy my…” (E.23), by asking again (dialogically), “why let him screw up your…?” (E.32). Both statements remain unfinished, and both, significantly, place the prospective blame on the pedestrian (“him”), and seem to allude to fairly far-reaching potential effects of the TrA on Dani’s life and well-being. Finally, the FS ‘you’ poem completes its contribution to this section with three entries (27, 29, and 33) that seem to indicate a particular need for understanding, affirmation from, and co-construction of meaning with, me. It is as if Dani, herein mired in such confusion and ambivalence, may be expressing a request for support or empathy from his audience. The final section of this narrative comes after my direct question about the traumatic nature of the TrA experience, to which Dani himself had originally referred. His immediate response appears in the FS ‘you’ poem, in the form of a rather incredulous question, perhaps conveying his assumption (or hope) that anyone would or should find an experience of this nature traumatic. The next three entries exist in the RPa ‘I’ poem, wherein Dani presents another apparent emotional high point, albeit in mostly dialogic form. Although the emotional sentiment here is positioned in saying rather than directly feeling, it seems evident nonetheless. His repetitive use of, “I said,” and, “That’s it” (E.36-37), following his admission to having “almost fainted” (E.35), send a message indicating an emotionally charged moment. And Dani’s shifts between the ‘I’ and the ‘you’ in the dialogue of these entries offer further evidence of a sense of ambivalence and discomfort with his actions and their unfortunate consequences. At this moment in the narrative, Dani transfers his discourse to the MS ‘you’ poem, with a cluster of four entries (38-41). Again, here, there is an emphasis on saying, and lengthy dialogic discourses questioning what he did and did not do. And again, there is a mixing of the speaking ‘you’ with the dialogic ‘I.’ Dani could just as easily have placed these statements into an ‘I’ poem (as in, “I said ‘Oh no what did I do?...’” instead of, “You say ‘Oh no what did I do?...’”), and so their existence in the MS ‘you’ poem may be interpreted as an attempt to generalize a particularly stressful or ambivalent facet of the experience. The repeated questioning, self-blaming, and apparent remorse in E.38 and E.40 may again send a message that Dani is still wrestling with guilt and uncertainty regarding his role in the TrA. Here, he has placed these concerns into the discursive realm of the generic MS ‘you,’ in a possible attempt to universalize and normalize them. The TrAN is then punctuated by a significant entry in the FS ‘you’ poem, whereby Dani says, “You see him down there” (E.42). This builds on a repetition already set in place by E.8 189 (“You see him lying there”) and E.24 (“You see”), and further demonstrates a pattern observed across the participants in this study, in which the most frequent sensory use of the FS ‘you’ is collocated with the verb, “to see.” This phenomenon, which is significant on a very basic level, is open to a range of interpretations. Perhaps Dani’s experience of looking down from his perch on his bus and seeing the bloodied, immobilized body of a human being he had injured was the most traumatic facet of this overall traumatic event. Or perhaps because this was the most graphic and visceral part of the experience, Dani is most interested in bringing me in to it, and able to do so. It does appear that this may have been the most emotional, affecting, passive, and uncertain facet of the experience for Dani—that is, seeing the effects of his actions (or non-actions); not knowing the extent of the damage he caused; and, most notably, not immediately springing into ‘heroic’ action to save or care for the wounded man. Indeed, most conspicuously absent from the TrAN overall is any reference to Dani as a hero or as having acted on behalf of the pedestrian or his passengers. In fact, his perception that the entire event was caused by his ‘incorrect’ behavior may be the most traumatic part of the experience for him. This could explain the subsequent two entries in the MS ‘you’ poem, wherein Dani repeats the phrase, “You killed a person” (E.43-44)—which may build upon E.6 (“You say “My God, I killed the man””). This seems to represent a repeated attempt to generalize and universalize the horrifying experience for Dani of entertaining the possibility that he had killed the pedestrian. This also, as previously mentioned, is opposed to E.7 in the RPa ‘I’ poem, wherein Dani declared definitively, “I didn’t kill him.” Therefore, most evident here is a dialectic between the actual fact, presented declaratively from the RPa ‘I’-perspective, that Dani did not kill the man, and the imagined possibility, presented both dialogically and non-dialogically from the MS ‘you’perspective, that, “You killed a person.” While E.36 in the RPa ‘I’ poem presents the euphemistic possibility, “I finished him,” the most difficult and upsetting statement that the man could have been killed, is placed into the universalized and generalized discursive realm of the MS ‘you.’ Although the experience of potentially or nearly killing a human being is undoubtedly highly emotionally charged, significantly, Dani does not place it into the FS ‘you’-perspective. Perhaps this is because this may be viewed by him as a masculine act, or as an agentic behavior rather than a passive sensory experience (such as, “see[ing] him down there,” in E.42 in the FS ‘you’ poem). This interpretive hypothesis may be reinforced by the repetition employed in E.34 and E.46, wherein Dani twice places the experience of, “go[ing] crazy,” and, “go[ing] even more crazy,” into the discursive realm of the FS ‘you.’ Perhaps this is an experience or behavior perceived by Dani as quintessentially female? This may also be interpreted as further evidence for one of the fundamental claims of this study; namely, that the FS ‘you’ is often recruited to take on the passive sensory reactions and emotional effects of stressful, difficult, and upsetting experiences. 190 The final cluster that essentially summarizes the TrAN exists in the PPr ‘I’ poem, wherein Dani makes a series of statements about himself as a person and his personality as a bus driver. His final message here is that he “like[s] calm” (E.53 and E.57), and prefers to drive on the less crowded bus lines because he likes to work with fewer people (E.55 and E.58). This eight-entry cluster constitutes a significant percentage within the total number of entries in the TrAN, which can be interpreted to indicate the importance of this message to Dani, although the meaning behind it is less clear. Perhaps Dani is reiterating a message he began to make in E.21-22 of the MS ‘you’ poem, where he essentially stated that he had been driving slowly at the time of the TrA, and that the pedestrian was “lucky” that Dani had been driving at that pace, place, and time, because if it had been any other driver, the pedestrian would surely have been injured much more seriously. In this view, Dani’s summation statements surrounding his slow and nonchalant driving (E.51-52) and preference for calm could be interpreted as an attempt to remind his audience of the pedestrian’s “luck” and ingrain the message of Dani’s ‘acceptable’ behavior, represented as his ultimate success in preventing a greater tragedy. Dani may also simply have wanted to conclude this narrative with a final message regarding who he is as a person and that he is indeed a good bus driver, despite what may appear to be evidence to the contrary, given the content of this narrative overall. One final notable finding in the pronoun poetry of Dani’s TrAN regards his only (four) uses of the ‘we’ pronoun, which appeared sequentially within the dialogue of E.32 in the MS ‘you’ poem. The full dialogue was: “You say ‘Damn, why let him screw up your…? … We will get back on [the bus] and we will drive properly. And we will drive with confidence and we will do exactly what I would have done beforehand, and even more.’”xxiii This constitutes a significant and fairly unusual interjection of the first-person plural/collective pronoun, perhaps symbolizing a discursive attempt on Dani’s part to integrate himself and his actions at this time into his society’s collective. Indeed, the content here surrounds what appears to be Dani’s perception of appropriate, ‘correct,’ and ‘acceptable’ behavior in such a context—getting right back onto ‘the battlefield’ no matter how traumatic the experience; behaving “properly,” “with confidence,” and “exactly” as beforehand; and not allowing anything or anyone to affect our well-being and ability to perform as we should. Ultimately, most apparent is Dani’s ambivalence surrounding this event and his role and level of culpability within it. The RPa ‘I’ grapples with whether or not he was guilty (and in whose perspective) and what he did and didn’t do. The PPr ‘I’ similarly wrestles with guilt, but ultimately concludes with a strong, unambiguous message that he is a good person and a good driver. The MS ‘you’ takes on the preponderance of the possible blame for the perceived reprehensible act (whether accidental or not), and much questioning, anger, and uncertainty about whether Dani could have done more to prevent the accident. The FS ‘you’ is consistently employed in a search for empathy, understanding, and discursive sharing in the most passive, upsetting, and difficult effects of the 191 experience. And finally, the few but significant uses of the ‘we’ pronoun seem to be used in an additional attempt to universalize and collectivize, and emphasize the ‘acceptable’ actions that Dani did manage to perform, even in the face of what he seems to perceive as an ‘unacceptable’ event. 6.5 THE CONNECTION BETWEEN THE MS ‘YOU’ AND THE LIVED EXPERIENCE: AMBIVALENCE ABOUT PERSONAL VS. SOCIETAL APPRAISALS Dani has been presented here as an exemplar of an increased use of the unmarked MS ‘you,’ and as such, an analysis of his texts can provide a window into an understanding of the use of the MS ‘you’ by the other participants in this study as well. Unmarked word forms are generally used more frequently than marked forms and hold the most general meanings, which in turn confer them more potential extra-linguistic interpretations, in more and wider linguistic and situational contexts. The present study found that the unmarked MS ‘you’ is most often used by these interviewees when they are attempting to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize the topic at hand. The interpretive implication is that the MS ‘you’ is employed most often when the encoder may be uncomfortable or unable to claim the experience as his own, but does not seek to bring me in to it. The facets of an experience narrated within the discursive realm of the MS ‘you’ may be those that, the speaker feels, less exemplify ‘acceptable’ actions or behavior, or about which he may feel ambivalent or uncertain. This interpretation, in turn, suggests a likelihood that the unmarked MS ‘you’ may be employed when there is a conflict or contradiction between the encoder’s personal appraisals and his perceptions of his surrounding society’s appraisals. This section presents a final multidimensional analysis of a number of Dani’s texts, with a particular focus on his attributions, beliefs, and perceptions about what is ‘acceptable’ and ‘unacceptable’ behavior for an individual such as himself, in a societal context such as that in which he exists. 6.5.1 The form and content of Dani’s entire main TAN: The ‘less acceptable’ way Over the years during which I have conducted this research, I have performed several different analyses on the forthcoming text, as it is particularly rich and representative of many of the psychosociolinguistic patterns upon which this study is based. And yet, only after reflecting on the interview dynamic and the joint process of creating shared meaning and understanding (Perez & Tobin, 2009) did I begin to notice some unique and heretofore unrecognized phenomena in this text, causing me to question a number of facets that I had long since taken for granted. It indeed took a great deal of time to find the ‘missing link’ that would tie these various linguistic signs and discursive forms into a cohesive story explaining the ‘larger picture’ of Dani’s case. And it was only after deeply analyzing the content and form of Dani’s entire narrative of the main TA that he experienced that I was able to discern what I believe to be the central message underlying Dani’s BLI as a whole. This central message, in turn, has shed much light on the 192 messages of the other participants’ interviews, as will be seen in later chapters. Although various portions of this text have been displayed and analyzed above, the transcript section is presented here in its entirety, as the interaction between us and the process of his story-telling is particularly relevant. The dialogue began with Dani describing some of his (self-defined “dangerous”) experiences in the army, after which I asked him how this period had been for him. He responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 D: Fine, challenging, a challenge. I like th’dangers. (1) I like [them]. You (FS) know. (2) They say that, masochist kind of, masochism, you (FS) know? A: Hmm. D: I am like that. Like to put myself in danger, III, (1) don’t know. (2) In the attack that I had, (2) okay we will get back to the attack I believe, soo, ((sigh)), I will just tell you (FS). (1) If I had had a gun, I would have gotten off [the bus]. (1) And I wanted to get off I wanted like when I got th’bus away from the shooting, from the gunfire, from thhee danger, I wanted to go back to to that place, because he kept shooting. A: So— D: I understood and I was not afraid. A: Okay. Tell me about the first [terror attack], tell me from the beginning. D: Uhh, from which attack? From the shooting attack, let’s (FS) put it like this I am driving, line [20], it was winter [February], rain. (2) Approach the [bus] stop, take on th’passengers, take, you (FS) know, [bus] passes money MONEY. Suddenly I see someone come to a standstill in front of {the bus. (2) I do [gesture] to himmm} like ‘Go past.’ (1) Because I want to put on the blinker and leave the [bus] stop. (1) And then he winks, winks his eye at me does like this ((winks at me)) I do to him like ‘What?’, [he] opens the zipper coat, takes ouut a rifle and ‘Brrrrrrrrrr’ ((makes a sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)). {And I didn’t se-, am I dreaming? I DREAM? What? What?} Straight away, like liike [they] taught me in the army, I bent down underneath the steering wheel, I am small. (1) The bullets passed me over my head he fired in a burst, innn automatic, not single shots, ONE ONE, in a burst. I went down underneath, with the steering wheel, and I started to drive, I wanted to run him over but he got away. Now I look, I see him pump full of bullets thhh’people who were at the [bus] stop. (3) What did I do I escaped13, I went to the right towards [name of hospital], there was a hospital nearby, I went off the route, now my people were wounded, tons of blood on the bus. I said ‘I have to get th’bus out [of here], otherwise he is tearing us apart,’ understand (FS)? A: Hmm. D: And so, actually14, I wanted to go back, like, that’s it, I saved th’bus, now I want to go back to that place like, how do they say? To see whaat is going on with the terrorist, to see if they killed him didn’t kill him what he’s doing maybe it is possible to bring him under control. You (FS) understand? There is no fear in me I am not afraid. (1) I didn’t go into shock no nothing. You (FS) can ask [name of the psychologist at Egged]. They came, something like thirty reporters, [she] says to me “Are you able to talk?”, I told her “Yes.” And two days later I went back to work. (2) There is no fear in me, there is no fear in my lexicon, {I don’t have it}. (2) A: Okay. D: I am not afraid, I don’t know why, maybe I am not normal. A: Really, in, during the [actual moment]— D: I was afraid, I was afraid. But when it is/was15 over, you (MS) kind of say ‘My God, (2) what a,’ like, if it had been possible, I would have wanted to go back and do the things better, maybe to kill him {too}. You (FS) understand? A: Hmm. D: I was afraid, it doesn’t mean that I wasn’t afraid I was shhhaking. There is no I don’t have, what is it? A pers-, ^a terrorist is shooting at you (MS) from four meters away, you (MS) won’t 13 Again, it is important to note the possible significance of Dani’s word choice here, as he could have said, “left/went out,” or, “drove,” or even, “I got the bus out of there.” The choice of the word, “escape,” seems to suggest a value judgment Dani may be placing on his own behavior. 14 The literal translation of the phrase used here by Dani approximates, “the story is…” or, “the real story is…” It is an unusual term in Hebrew, and the intended meaning is not entirely clear. It seems likely, however, that Dani chose to utilize this phrase in order to lend added emphasis to the statement that follows it. Because the consequent statement could easily have been understood without this introductory emphatic phrase, its presence here can thus be interpreted as notable and meaningful—and as a discursive marker of significance for Dani. 15 It is not possible to ascertain whether Dani was using the ‘past’- or ‘present’-tense morphological form of this verb. 193 44 45 be scared? You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be scared. I screamed “Mama!” and everything^. But, (4) that’s it, how do they say? God tests us. xxiv Dani begins his response to my question by referring to his professed enjoyment of dangers and the challenges of his army role, and his narrative contains four separate declarations that he is and/or was not afraid and that he has no fear, neither in himself nor in his “lexicon” (L.10, L.31, L.34, and L.36). At first, these assertions come across as emphatic, appearing to reflect a sense of pride in what Dani seems to view as a notable accomplishment. In L.31-32, for instance, he elaborates that he experienced no traumatic effects as a result of the TA, and even assured me that I will receive a corroborating report if I check his statements with another source. (Only a few minutes later in the interview, Dani reframed this a bit, stating, “After that I was in the hospital, [they (MP)] came, (1) I wasn’t wounded physically, [they (MP)] said that I had a biiit of, you (FS) know, what is it called shock? ... Shock yes, something like that.”xxv) In L.36, however, Dani’s ‘façade’ seems to break slightly, as he questions why he has/had no fear, and expresses uncertainty with regard to whether he is “normal.” It is possible that Dani’s ultimate admissions of experiencing fear (L.38 and L.42) were the result of an interaction between myself and him, as my, “Okay,” (L.35) did not appear to be the response he was seeking. Perhaps he expected me to react in wonderment or admiration, or to consider the question ‘answered’ and move on to another topic; perhaps in these cases he might have continued to boast his lack of fear (that is, lack of weakness). But instead, at the time, I strove to maintain an emotionally ‘objective’ persona, even in my internal surprise and shock that this man sitting in front of me was suggesting that he experienced this TA with not a shred of fear. Dani seemed even more perplexed at my attempt to clarify his statements, as I pushed to make sure that he “really” was never afraid, even in the exact moment in which the TA was occurring (L.37). I scarcely said three words before he interjected, suddenly admitting readily, and equally emphatically, that he was indeed afraid at the time (L.38). Immediately after this, however, Dani distances himself discursively and continues to vacillate between statements about what he would have wanted to do (L.39-40) and his actual feelings of fear and even the physiological manifestation of his reaction at the time (L.42). In L.42, Dani appears to make an attempt to repeat his contention that “there is no fear in me…I don’t have it” (L.34), but does not complete it. He follows this by employing the unmarked, universal, and generic discursive technique reflected in the MS ‘you’ (L.43-44), finally imparting what I suggest is his summation statement: “You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be scared” (L.44). There appears to be an emotionally charged and highly ambivalent ‘play’ with words and meanings in Dani’s discourse, particularly surrounding the issues of fear and being “normal.” While, at one juncture, he states categorically that fear does not exist in his lexicon (L.34), only 194 seconds later, he asserts not only that he was afraid, but that what he had said previously “doesn’t mean that [he] wasn’t afraid” (L.42). Meanwhile, he questions whether he is normal because of his lack of fear (L.36); then admits that he was afraid (L.38 and L.42); then questions rhetorically whether the universal MS ‘you’ wouldn’t be scared if he were in Dani’s place (L.43-44); and finally agrees that this generic ‘you’ is indeed not normal if [he] wouldn’t be scared in a comparable situation (L.44). Here, Dani seems to qualify his statements about his own fear with a reference to the purportedly universal experience of fear, suggesting that the generic/normative MS ‘you’ must or should be scared in this context. By stating that the universal subject is “not normal” if he does not react as Dani did, he may be attempting to justify his own reaction, while collectivizing and normalizing it at the same time. There are several potential and plausible hypotheses with regard to Dani’s judgments about how the “normal” Israeli man should react in a situation of this nature, which could be viewed essentially as a reflection of Dani’s perceptions of Israeli society’s judgments of the behavior of its (male) members: 1) Dani may believe in his statement that “You are not normal if you won’t be scared,” and thus may believe that his own fear reaction was “normal”; 2) Dani may believe that Israeli society would perceive his own fear reaction as “normal,” but he may not agree that it was; 3) Dani may believe that his own fear reaction was “normal,” but may believe or fear that Israeli society would not agree and might disapprove of or be unwilling to justify his reaction, and Dani may thus feel a need to justify his feelings for himself and/or me; and/or 4) Dani may believe neither that his fear reaction was “normal” and acceptable, nor that the ‘average’ Israeli should or would react this way, but may be attempting to convince me otherwise and thereby justify his ‘abnormal’ reaction. Indeed, Dani demonstrated significant ambivalence with regard to his own fear reactions, which may provide a clue as to which of these hypotheses may be closer to explaining his lived experience and attributions therein. While it appears that Dani often directly contradicted statements he made mere sentences beforehand, there may indeed be an internal logic to his discourse. Perhaps what appear to be contradictions are actually signs of Dani’s attempts to make sense of his ambivalence and his own realizations that some of his statements may seem paradoxical. Indeed, this may be evidence of a working-through process functioning within the discourse—a gradual shift from lack of willingness to willingness to admit to his fear reactions and an attempt to accept his own behavior. This appears particularly perceptible through the patterned communicative strategies that may encourage collectivization and universalization. Viewed through this lens, a re-examination of the initial section of the above text makes apparent a significant disconnect between Dani’s summation statement and his earlier assertions. Before I had even mentioned the topic of TAs, and even coupled with an admission of his understanding that “we will get back” to this subject, Dani nonetheless presented me with the 195 opening statement of his TAN: “If I had had a gun, I would have gotten off [the bus]” (L.5-6). I claim that this sentence is essentially the title of Dani’s narrative of this event, and his placement of it at this juncture—preceding and outside of the TAN itself—may be his way of clarifying for me that this is, in his eyes, the most important facet of his story. Indeed, in the single unbroken text section presented above, Dani expressed in five different places and ways his feelings about what he wanted to do, would have wanted to do, and/or wished he had done, if the circumstances had been different (i.e., get off the bus if he had had a gun (L.56); go back to the scene because the terrorist kept shooting (L.7-8); run over the terrorist (L.22-23); go back to see if it was possible to bring the terrorist under control (L.28-30); and “go back and do the things better, maybe to kill him too” (L.39-40)). Notably, Dani’s ‘title sentence’ includes and is followed by the most elaborated expression of this (L.5-8). It seems to hold particular importance for him to make his point unequivocally and early on in—or even as a prelude to—the narrative, perhaps in order for me to fully comprehend the rest of it. Perhaps, in Dani’s eyes, I would only be able to understand or appraise or—possibly most importantly—accept his actual behavior during the TA if I view it from within a perspective already influenced by my knowledge of what he had wanted to do, and would surely have done, if he had had the resources and opportunity. Notably, in L.39, Dani stated that he would have wanted to “do things better,” which is his only fully articulated attestation to feeling that he did not “do things” in a manner that he (or perhaps his society) finds ‘acceptable.’ He also provided three reasons for having not acted in a “better” manner, assuring his audience that if these conditions had been in place, his “better” behavior would undoubtedly have followed: 1) Dani did not have a gun (L.5-6); 2) the terrorist got away (L.22-23); and 3) it was not possible (to go back to the scene and “do the things better”; L.39). While the first two reasons are very practical and specific, the third encompasses all possible conditions, essentially absolving Dani of any ‘less than’ behavior. Here, significantly, his most telling admission seems wrapped in a paradox: if it was not “possible” for him to return to the scene and “do the things better,” why is he so preoccupied with his failure to do so? It actually seems to be Dani himself who expresses an inability to accept or ‘make peace’ with his actions and perceived lack thereof, during this event. This is particularly visible in L.28, when his use of the phrase, “that’s it,” seems to send a distinct message as to what he and/or his society expected of him in this context, and that he did not live up to these expectations (i.e., ‘task #1: save the bus—done = ‘acceptable’); task #2: kill the terrorist—not done = ‘unacceptable’’). Indeed, it was found that all the interviewees made statements of this nature, revealing expressions of doubt, regret, guilt, and responsibility. Much of their discourse contained the interjections, “I should have…”; “I would have…”; “I wanted to…”; and/or, “I wish I had…”; as if they were apologizing for not being ‘heroic’ enough, or ‘man’ enough, or ‘Israeli’ enough. These drivers 196 seemed to feel a need to prove themselves as powerful, honorable, and ‘heroic’ (that is, productive and ‘acceptable’) members of their society. I suggest that these feelings extend into their work and into their TA experiences, as their discourse showed an almost overactive sense of obligation to protect and/or save their clientele, even under essentially impossible circumstances. In Dani’s case, this may show itself in his repeated choices to use forms of the word, “to escape,” which seems to hold much more judgment and emotional charge than, for instance, the word, “to leave.” A final, and particularly notable, piece of evidence of a possible preoccupation with ‘acceptable’ and ‘correct’ behavior, as perceived by the individual of the hegemonic societal expectations surrounding him, comes from Dani’s main TAN in his FUI. Rather than stating, as he had in his BLI, that he had wanted to run over the terrorist at the time of the attack, three years later Dani made the following statement: “The truth [is] I tried to run him over {I didn’t succeed}. I tried to run him over he escaped ttoo the shops over there…”xxvi I assert that this may be a significant demonstration of Dani’s desire and attempt to transform his TAN from an account of what happened (as viewed through his eyes) to at least a partial account of what he wished had happened.16 The BLI took place nearly five years after this TA, when Dani was apparently still focused on what he did and did not do, and what he wished he had done, during the event itself. At some point during the ensuing three years between the BLI and FUI, Dani apparently managed to convince himself or decided to try to convince me that, rather than simply wanting to run over the terrorist, he had actually tried to do so. That is, this part of his narrative shifted from initially discussing what he wished he had done, to later actually stating that he had done it. It is possible that, in Dani’s eyes, wanting to do something and trying to do something are actually interchangeable concepts. It may also be that, at the time of the BLI, he was more concerned with emphasizing his desire to run over the terrorist—without relating to whether or not he was able to implement or succeed in this desire—while in the FUI, he may have been more willing and/or able to admit and discuss that he had tried but ultimately failed. It is difficult to determine with certainty which motivations lie behind Dani’s discursive choice here. I nonetheless claim that the presence of this shift in word choice is significant, and does appear to be related on some level to Dani’s preoccupation with the ‘acceptability’ of his behavior during the TA, both in his own eyes and in his perception of the appraisal of his surrounding society. 6.5.2 ‘Validation’: Dani’s own words about the ‘acceptable’ way The above analyses rested on a deep interpretative discourse analysis of both the form and content of a number of Dani’s significant narratives. This uncovered several patterns and possibly 16 This is a process in which humans tend to engage throughout their lives as story-tellers, and should not prompt judgment or criticism. It is, nonetheless, intriguing to see what appears to be this process unfolding in ‘real time’ in this case. 197 unconscious motivations and messages that may exist in his discourse. In an attempt to further explore Dani’s perceptions of what is ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ behavior on the part of a man in the context of Israeli society, it may also be helpful to simply turn to Dani himself—that is, to bring in some of his more direct statements on this issue. During the question-and-answer session of his BLI, I asked Dani specifically whether he had felt that he had a responsibility to his passengers and/or obligation to act in a certain manner during the main TA he experienced. He responded: 1 2 3 “Of course. … I had to. To get the bus out of there. Had to. I said that that’s it, the responsibility is on my shoulders, that if I screw up I am like the captain of the ship if I, screw up, right now I am killing too, I am killing along with me another 20 {people}.”xxvii Here appears manifest evidence of Dani’s perceptions of his duties and responsibilities as a bus driver in the context of this TA. He speaks in absolutes, as seen in the repetitive use of the phrases, “had to,” and, “that’s it” (L.1). He also refers (twice) to a failure to act in the expected manner as “screw[ing] up” (L.2), and as inevitably leading to him “killing” his passengers (L.3). There is no ‘grey area’ in Dani’s discourse here, in which he could be viewed as unable or unwilling to act ‘appropriately’ but nonetheless not consequently a killer, or in which the terrorist could be viewed as the killer regardless of Dani’s own actions. Dani seems to view himself as having been totally responsible for the well-being of his passengers at the time of the TA, regardless of the fact that he, too, was technically a victim. His perception of himself as “captain of the ship” seems to confer an almost superheroic status, which, as evident throughout his text, apparently brings with it almost superhuman expectations. Indeed, while Dani did manage to get the bus out of the shooter’s range with admirable speed and agility, he still appears to suffer from a great deal of guilt about his failure to return to the scene to subdue the attacker. At another point in the interview, while Dani was talking about his army service, I asked him whether he sees any connections between what he experienced as a soldier and what he experienced in the various TAs he had been through. He responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 “Yes. Coping, pressure courage, in the army we would go into’uhh, {refugee camps, we would get rocks [thrown at us] we would gettt that. In the beginning in the army I was scared. In the beginning in the beginning. But afterwards}, as time went on I began like to [get into a] routine. Let’s say I got a rock [thrown] at the bus so {I diddnn’t didn’t pay it any mind, (1) okay a rock, two rocks no big deal, I got a lot more in the army}, army taught meee how to deal, (1) to be more, (1) more ‘with it,’ more functioning, to find solutions let’s (FS) say like that here [they (MP)] threw a rock at me so I {will drive faster so I will look for whoever threw th’rock and tryyy}, to get away from him and not. Things like that it the army taught me to deal with stressful situations and not to be afraid that’s the most important.”xxviii Here again, Dani expresses his perceptions of the ‘appropriate’ behavior in this type of context, and thus, what he seems to believe was expected of him. He states repeatedly that “in the beginning” (presumably, before he became a ‘correctly-performing’ soldier) he was scared (L.2-3)—but soon thereafter became accustomed to the appropriate routine. Dani employs several phrases to define what this routine entailed, and what the ‘correct’ behavior involved: not “pay[ing] any mind” to rocks being thrown at his bus (L.4); attributing this experience as “no big deal” (L.4); and being 198 “more ‘with it,’ more functioning…find[ing] solutions” (L.5-6). The ultimate message here can be discerned in L.5 and L.8, wherein Dani states that the “army taught [him] [how] to deal with stressful situations.” Indeed, Dani’s summation statement is his most unequivocal declaration: that he sees “the most important” thing as “not to be afraid” (L.8-9). In light of this assertion, the central message of Dani’s main TAN, and perhaps of his entire BLI—“You (MS) are not normal if you (MS) won’t be afraid”—seems fraught with dilemma, contradictions, and ambivalence. The question remains: whence do these (seemingly impossibly high) expectations come, and how and why do they exert such a powerful influence on the perceptions, attributions, beliefs, and experiences—and through these avenues, the discourse—of Dani and the rest of the interviewees? This is one of the central areas of inquiry of this study, and it will continue to be ‘worked through’ from different perspectives and analyses of the other interview materials. 6.6 CONCLUSION This chapter began from a broad, holistic perspective on Dani himself and the BL interview context and experience, then explored the overall patterns in his discourse, then showed a basic quantitative view on the distribution of these patterns, and then moved to a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of both the form and content of these patterns. Finally, a multilayered analysis of the content of Dani’s discourse, with a focus on his attributions and perceptions, was offered. This deep, spiraling analysis of Dani’s outward discursive and narrative choices was employed in an attempt to understand his related inner psychological beliefs and perspectives. The following two tables summarize the central findings of Dani’s BLI, within the discursive and narrative categories that were analyzed. The first focuses on Dani’s patterns of pronoun (and tense) use. 199 Table 6-3: Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Dani’s baseline interview PRONOUN USE First-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases MS ‘you’ FS ‘you’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING Suggests sense of responsibility and guilt for his passengers, for what happened Suggests separation of himself and solitary sense of experience—“what I am seeing in front of me” Suggests great need to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize facets of experience that he is uncomfortable claiming as his own Suggests great need to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize (in)to a larger context world—but unclear how inclusive this world is, in his eyes (all men, or all Israelis, or all Israeli males, or all Israeli male bus drivers, etc.) Suggests possible attempt to exclude me (specifically) discursively from the specific context or experience under discussion Suggests significant ambivalence and uncertainty with regard to the less ‘acceptable,’ ‘appropriate,’ or ‘correct’ facets of experience—such as his behavior and emotions Suggests possible conflict or contradiction between his appraisals of his own behavior and his perceptions of his surrounding society’s appraisals Suggests some need to bring audience in to the most difficult, upsetting sensory facets of experience, and much of the more emotional and passive experiences—such as feeling introspective, or helpless or powerless Directed (non-analytically significant) use suggests great need for my understanding, connection, and cooperation in our co-construction of meaning within the interview and in the co-creation of a shared experience, both discursively and emotionally Suggests desire and ability to claim ownership only of parts of experience in which he was acting in an unequivocally ‘heroic’ manner (‘acceptably,’ in his perception of the viewpoint of Israeli society) Relative lack suggests very little to no acknowledgment of the collective nature of the terror attack—“what I saw” Suggests minor attempt to build on the use of the MS ‘you’ as a further mechanism to align himself with the ‘acceptable’ collective and ‘fit in’ to the societal norms ‘I’ ‘We’ As a final analytic summarizing exercise, presented here is a list of suggested hypothetical messages, or end points (Spector-Mersel, 2011), which appeared fundamental to Dani’s central BL narratives and discourse. Some of them may have appeared as more conscious or less so, but all were either expressed manifestly or seemed present in some way between or underneath the lines of his interview text. While most are essentially my own formulations of what I understand and interpreted as Dani’s ‘main messages,’ I believe they most succinctly summarize and illustrate the results of the comprehensive analyses conducted throughout this chapter. 200 Table 6-4: Suggested ‘main messages’ of Dani’s baseline interview I acted during the ‘acceptable’ facets of these difficult experiences. You (MS) experience the parts of these difficult experiences about which I am more ambivalent and uncertain regarding their ‘acceptability.’ You (FS) experience the passive, weak, and ‘unacceptable’ parts of these difficult experiences. I was afraid during the main terror attack, but isn’t that okay? You (MS) are not normal if you don’t react emotionally in some way to a traumatic experience. You (FS) are affected by the (few) sensory, most emotionally distressing facets of these traumatic experiences. You (the interviewer) are my main conduit to understanding, connection, and possibly acceptance of even the less ‘acceptable’ parts of my behavior. We Israelis are supposed to behave courageously and actively and correctly. What happened to my passengers during the main terror attack might have been my fault. What happened to the pedestrian during the traffic accident might have been my fault. I set myself apart or feel set apart in my experience of the terror attack(s) and its aftereffects. I was active during parts of the main terror attack, but not heroic enough. I should have been more active and heroic during the main terror attack. If I could have, I would have been more active and heroic during the main terror attack. I think there was a societal expectation that I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the main terror attack. I think I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the terror attack—I should have gone back to kill the shooter. A real Israeli man is courageous, never afraid, strong, and always able to find solutions and act to prevent disaster or injury. I am highly uncertain and ambivalent as to whether I fit this description. The various terror attacks I experienced were no big deal; the traffic accident I was involved in upset me more. Dani’s baseline interview provided another rich opportunity for a further investigation of the link between an individual’s hypothesized inner attitudes and his perceptions of the societal attitudes and cultural discourses surrounding him. Dani was found to have used the MS ‘you’ to a greater degree than the other participants in this study, as well as expressing the most ambivalence with regard to the multiple difficult life events he had experienced and his roles therein. Thus, Dani was presented here as an exemplar of an apparent conflict between personal and perceived societal expectations, and as being most occupied with which facets of his various experiences could be seen as ‘acceptable’ and thus worthy of being claimed as his own. It was contended that he employed the MS ‘you’ most often, in an attempt to place the behaviors and feelings about which he was most ambivalent, into the universalized, collectivized, and normalized discursive realm. 201 CHAPTER 7 MOTTI: “THE HERO DRIVER” vs. “I FEEL ABOUT MYSELF THAT I AM VERY WEAK” — CLAIMING OWNERSHIP OF ‘ACCEPTABLE’ ACTIONS AND A WIDER RANGE OF EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCES 7.1 INTRODUCTION TO MOTTI: BACKGROUND, CONTEXT, AND IMPRESSIONS This chapter presents the case of Motti, the participant who most exemplified a heightened use of the ‘I’ pronoun in his discourse. Because this linguistic finding has already been discussed and illustrated in the context of both Yossi’s and Dani’s texts, Motti’s case is presented here in a more abbreviated fashion, essentially more as a window into a deeper across-participant understanding, rather than reiterating the entire analytical process conducted. At the time of the baseline interview (BLI), Motti was 45 years old and single. He is a native-born Israeli. He completed twelve years of schooling and served in the Armored Corps during his compulsory army service. He had worked as a bus driver in Be’er Sheva for fifteen years until the double terror attack (TA) he experienced, approximately 21 months prior to the interview. The bus next to his exploded from a bombing, after which he drove for about a block and then pulled over to let off some of his passengers, at their request. At this moment, his bus went up in an explosion as well, causing the deaths of five and injuries to seven of his passengers. Motti did not suffer significant physical injuries, but was affected emotionally by the experience. He did not work at all for approximately a year afterwards, and, having been unable to step foot on a bus since the attack, was working as a cab driver at the time of the BLI. One of the central parts of the experience for Motti, and a prominent theme throughout his BLI, surrounded the media’s interpretation of his actions at the time of the TA. Apparently, the first to arrive on the scene had been a journalist, who understood that Motti had let off a number of passengers before the explosion on his bus, thus ‘saving’ their lives. The journalist, and later, the rest of the Israeli print and television media ‘ran’ with this story, presenting Motti as “the hero driver” who purportedly had an intuition to pull over and save these individuals’ lives. Of course, the facts and context of the story makes this ‘scoop’ dubious at best, given that this was the first (and only to date) bus-related TA in Be’er Sheva, and that he had not chosen to get off the bus himself or usher the rest of the passengers off. Indeed, Motti spent a great deal of time in his interview protesting his heroic image, stating emphatically that he had no such premonition or feeling, and had simply heeded a request by his passengers. Nonetheless, this social status as a hero seemed firmly implanted in Motti’s experience, and, I claim, deeply impacted his discourse. I was referred to Motti by a fellow university student, a friend of a relative of his. The BLI lasted over three and a half hours (by far the longest of any of the study interviews) and was conducted in two parts (about a week apart). He was a friendly and enthusiastic interviewee, pleased to cooperate and willing to help “in any way.” His interview was filled with rich and lengthy narratives. He frequently went out of his way to physically substantiate his descriptions, bringing out a notebook of newspaper articles about the event and even going to his apartment window to point out a relevant landmark. Motti was, by far, the most open of all the participants, 202 particularly with regard to sharing details about his emotional states and telling elaborate narratives about his process of coping with the traumatic facets of his TA experience, including various therapeutic and psychotropic pharmaceutical interventions. Despite a number of references to what I would classify as depressive and post-traumatic symptoms (e.g., dysthymic affect, nightmares, and hypervigilance), Motti came across to me as exceedingly well-adjusted—coping resiliently with very difficult experiences and emotions, and apparently quite confident in his ability to do so. 7.2 MOTTI’S BASELINE INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT Relative to the other interviewees and throughout his BLI, what most characterized Motti’s discourse was his propensity for statements utilizing the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’), in both the proximate-present (PPr) and remote-past (RPa) tenses. He rarely used either of the secondperson singular pronouns, and so his discourse was most firmly positioned in his own claiming of the topics, experiences, actions, and emotions at hand. I claim that this pattern of pronoun use suggests a high level of comfort with and acceptance of his own behavior, and a perception that his surrounding society holds a positive appraisal of his behavior during the TA he experienced. As will be shown throughout this chapter, this stands in contrast to some of the pronominal patterns and appraisals of the other participants. This section briefly presents the four patterns of pronoun choice utilized by Motti throughout his BLI, some of which were reflected most predominantly in his main terror attack narrative (TAN) and others of which appeared in various other mini-narratives or descriptions in relation to the attack. First, Motti utilized some first-person possessive pronouns (such as, ‘my’) as a strategy toward creating a sense of personalization and possession of the topics or objects of reference. Alongside of this, however, he also used the first-person collective pronoun (‘we’) and third-person pronouns (such as ‘he,’ ‘she,’ and ‘they’) to describe many facets of the TA that involved the other ‘experiencers’ of it, either together with him or separate from him. Second, Motti utilized the second-person masculine singular pronoun (‘you’ (MS)), as well as distanced and non-personalized phrasing, in certain contexts as a strategy toward creating a sense of generalization and universalization. Third, he utilized the second-person feminine singular pronoun (‘you’ (FS)) as a strategy to bring his audience in to the experience he was describing. However, all of these pronouns were used relatively sparingly throughout the BLI overall, and some not at all in Motti’s TAN. Indeed, it is the final pronominal pattern—in which Motti utilized the first-person singular pronoun (‘I’) as a strategy toward claiming ownership of the experience, activity, or role he was describing—that was the most prominent in all dimensions. Each of these strategies is discussed in turn below, with demonstrative examples. 203 7.2.1 Personalization vs. collectivization: Use of first-person possessive pronouns vs. third-person pronouns As seen in Chapters 5 and 6, respectively, Yossi and Dani showed a pattern of personalizing many aspects of their bus, their role, and their TA experience overall—seemingly sending a message that, for them, the most important part was their own, solitary experience of the event. When referring to his bus, Motti used the same highly personalized formulations as did Yossi and Dani. At one point, Motti said, “There was one woman whose brother was killed at me1 on the bus…”i In addition, he began his TAN by setting up the scene, actually creating a model for me and pointing to the various components: “This is [bus] line [8], there was [sic] two buses, this is 8 and this is me.”ii He did not say, “This is 8 and this is my bus,” or, “This is 8 and this is 3.” In fact, directly after his first statement, I asked him which bus line he had been driving, after which he answered me and repeated his original statement: “[Three]. It’s like this, this is me, the blue one. … Now [8] needs to turn left…”iii Motti did not refer to the other bus as “him,” all the while persisting in referring to his own bus as “me,” rather than, “my bus,” or “[line] three.” In contrast to the discursive patterns of both Yossi and Dani, however, Motti referred frequently throughout his BLI to the other victims and survivors of the TA on his bus. He regularly used the ‘we’ rather than ‘I’ pronoun, as well as making many statements with third-person subjects (‘he,’ ‘she,’ and ‘they’). At one point, he said, “And then [they (MP)] evacuated us to the hospital in ambulances,”iv directly after which he repeated himself but personalized the statement, saying, “And then [they (MP)] evacuated me to the hospital.”v Motti offered multiple narratives throughout his BLI about encounters he has had with family members of victims, in which he was recognized as the driver of the ill-fated bus and was asked repeated questions about the victims’ last moments. Few of the other interviewees related in such detail to the other experiencers of the TA they underwent, and none with such rich narratives as Motti’s. From the very beginning of Motti’s TAN, he discussed the perspective and actions of the passengers on his bus: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 “Now, I look at the driver, and I do like this ((waving)) to him. For no particular reason [I] tell him hi, and as soon as I raise [my] hand it blows up. And I am here. Now, as soon as [it] blows up, I understood that it is a terrorist. No. I did like this “Wow, wow, wow,” this was my first reaction. And people from the windows, uhh, I am trying hard like, don’t know, it was an indescribable, sight. And now everything I am telling [you] it is a matter of seconds. It is an indescribable sight. Now all the people who were at me in the vehicle, and there weren’t many, they barely filled up the seats, there weren’t any people standing. There were only people sitting down. And then they came close to the windows here, to look [at] what is going on there. And [they (MP)] said “Wow, fast, it,” now [they (MP)] began, people began to run toward that vehicle, right here, difficult to describe what [you/they/we (MP)] see there, and people began to run to that bus, I went forward.”vi Here, Motti was referring to the first explosion in the double TA he experienced, which he and his passengers witnessed immediately preceding the explosion on their own bus. 1 This excerpt, The pronoun used here, “etzli”, was discussed at length in Chapter 3 (Section 3.5.2.3). An exact translation into English is impossible (although it is comparable to ‘chez’ in French), but it can be understood as ‘at me,’ ‘by me,’ ‘on me,’ or ‘in me,’ depending on the context. 204 therefore, can be compared analytically to Dani’s narrative of the bus explosion he witnessed, which was presented discursively as an event experienced only by him. Dani had stated, “…and I am still standing here and seeing the whole scene, maybe 30 meters away from me, and hearing everything.”vii Although the first part of Dani’s description is quite similar to Motti’s statement, “And I am here” (line (L.) 2), the rest of the excerpt above paints an entirely different picture. After a cursory mention of his own reaction (L.3), Motti’s narrative appears highly focused on the actions, reactions, and experiences of the other individuals on his bus at the time of the TA. This constitutes a significant opposition to the narrative strategies of Yossi and Dani, and may provide insight into the discursive patterns and attendant messages of all three participants. 7.2.2 Depersonalization and universalization: Use of the second-person masculine singular pronoun and distancing discourse Motti’s use of the MS ‘you’ was almost nonexistent in his BLI overall, and the pronoun was entirely absent from his TAN. This is a significant finding in itself, suggesting that Motti—similar to Yossi—did not demonstrate a need to depersonalize or universalize many aspects of his experiences, in the manner that Dani showed in abundance. This section provides just the few examples wherein Motti did use the MS ‘you’ pronoun, in order to highlight his very specific discursive choices therein. Indeed, microanalysis of the rare appearances of the MS ‘you’ across Motti’s entire BLI revealed the significant and consistent pattern of discursive depersonalization and universalization almost exclusively in relation to the topics of coping and responsibility. Motti employed both the MS ‘you’ and distanced, non-personalized phrasing in doing so. For example, when he first discussed his feelings about coping after the TA, Motti constructed a number of sentences in a universalized and distanced formulation. Particularly, his use of the phrase, “it is…”, is demonstrated below: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 “The the biggest difficulty is after what happened, after. It is the coping, and the thoughts, it is the fear of being in noisy hmm places, annd every time that [you/they/we (MP)] see a terror attack on television, annd [you/they/we (MP)] are not calm. Simply not calm (MP). And at night totally when I would hear any kind of sound I would jump. Annd really just like this, jump. Like, jump and hold onto something. Uhh, it is another life. It is a life of coping. Me personally, I take the coping on myself. I don’t try, uhh, don’t know how to tell you (FS) this, don’t try to get someone to support me, to help me.”viii Here, Motti vacillates between impersonal and personal, and between generalized and specific phrasing. Although it may be that the decoder is to simply understand that Motti is referring to his own life and his own experiences after the TA, Motti’s manner of expressing much of this experience creates a sense of dissociation and disconnection, as if he is standing with me outside of his life, looking in. His repeated use of the unmarked and unspecified MP form (“[you/they/we (MP)] see,” in L.2, and, “[you/they/we (MP)] are not calm,” in L.3), collocated with the PPr tense, similarly creates a universalized, habitualized, passive-like, and distant sense in his statements. 205 Only toward the end of the above excerpt does Motti refer specifically to his own coping, stating, “I take the coping on myself” (L.5-6). Instantly, both the content and the form of these sentences (L.5-7) seem to reflect a personalization, a shift from the collective experience and manner of expression. Motti could easily have universalized these statements (e.g., “You (MS) take the coping on yourself”), but here he makes a personal expression about his own individualized experience. One potential explanation for this could be that Motti believes that his choice to ‘take it on himself’ is unusual or not the ‘normal,’ standard, or universal choice of the normative Israeli man. Perhaps Motti is proud of himself for coping entirely on his own, and feels it necessary to help or make me take note of this ‘brave’ choice. Another theory could be that he feels isolated by this behavior and alone in his coping, and his use of ‘I’-statements reflects this. Possible further explanation for this discursive pattern can be found in a number of elaborative comments made by Motti later in his interview, all on the topic of coping. The first excerpt relates to his opinion on coping alone, as opposed to enlisting the help of “professionals”: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “The best in my opinion is to get help from- from psychologists, and if you (MS) feel that they are unnecessary so don’t go (MS) to them. Don’t go (MS) liiike to say that I was there. If you (MS) think that they are helping you (MS), so do go (MS). If not, don’t go (MS). If you (MS) think that you (MS) are strong enough to cope alone, don’t take (MS), not pills, not psychologists, not psychiatrists, not anything. Cope (MS) with it alone. In my opinion that’s the best. If you (MS) feel that you (MS) cannot cope alone, so go (MS) to professionals.”ix Here, Motti shows a degree of ambivalence, most evident in the opposition between his first sentence, where he suggests that “the best…is to get help” from professionals (L.1), and his statement toward the end of the excerpt, that “cop[ing] with it alone” is “the best” (L.5). Both of these suggestions are presented by Motti as his “opinion,” and this entire text section is positioned within the discursive realm of the MS ‘you.’ In this sense, Motti appears to be sending a message to the general, universal public (within his surrounding society), regarding his perception of the ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ way to cope. The crux of the problem, however, appears to be his seeming uncertainty about the ‘right’ answer. Motti appears to be sanctioning the practice of seeking help and support from professionals, be it talk or psychopharmaceutical therapy, when the acute need is present. But if this is “unnecessary,” or if “you (MS) think that you (MS) are strong enough” to not need or seek this help (L.3-4), all the better. That is, in Motti’s perception, if “you (MS) cannot cope alone” (L.6), then it’s acceptable—albeit not ideal—to get help; but if it is at all possible to “cope with it alone” (L.5), then that is the ultra-acceptable way. Motti seems to be attempting to reconcile what he perceives as his society’s norms and expectations (e.g., “coping alone”) with his own ambivalence in accepting the possible reality; that is, an inability to live up to these standards. This attempt at reconciliation between these two possibly contradicting sides of the issue can be seen in Motti’s references—few and far between, but still apparent in his BLI—to the difficulty of coping alone. While talking about the death of his 206 company commander during his army service, for instance, Motti stated, “When you (MS) cope alone, so it’s possible that [the] coping is harder.”x In addition, in the following narrative section, Motti gives a much more personalized perspective on his own experience coping after his TA: 1 2 3 4 5 “[They (MP)] are talking with you (FS) and media and people, and after that already three, four days, you (MS) already begin to cope with it alone. … The coping is {very difficult}, after a few days the coping is difficult and then suddenly someone again reminds you (MS) of it and again, you (MS) don’t want to tell him, on the other hand you (MS), he doesn’t leave you (MS) alone. And, okay, you (MS) try to avoid, and you (MS) again go and again for what to remind you (MS) of it, and again.” xi Here, as in many other points in his BLI, Motti refers repeatedly to his specific experience of being reminded of his TA, by both physical landmarks and people who either recognized him from the media reports or who were connected in some way to the passengers who were wounded or killed on his bus that day. Motti’s exclusive use of the MS ‘you’ here seems to suggest an attempt to normalize and universalize these very personal experiences—coping and coping alone; being reminded; not wanting to talk; not being left alone; trying to avoid; and going (presumably out of the house) and again being reminded. While he states that this coping process was “very difficult” (L.2), he nonetheless chooses to neither own it for himself nor bring me in nor project it onto me. Motti places this description of coping alone onto the generic, universal ‘you,’ which suggests a desire to generalize it. Perhaps he does not want to admit (in ‘I’-statements) to the magnitude of the difficulty he personally suffered throughout this process; preferring, instead, to keep it in the discursively distanced realm. Or perhaps he is attempting to garner support and approval from his collective, by placing himself discursively into the midst of his surrounding society. More possible evidence of Motti’s perceptions of his society’s expectations, and of his beliefs regarding the roles and responsibilities of the average Israeli bus driver, may be visible here: 1 2 3 4 “You (MS) are responsible for the bus. You (MS) are supposed to take care of, that everything will be okay. You (MS) need to, to take the people home. Suddenly something like this/that2 happens and you (MS) see the people cut up and, the bodies and, it’s an unbelievable thing. Simply… unbelievable. Something that (2), unbelievable. Simply unbelievable.”xii Motti’s use of the MS ‘you’ again seems to serve to highlight his apparent perception of the ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ behavior in this given context. He has already stated that “coping alone” is the “best” way to deal with the aftereffects of an experience such as this, but here he discusses the general expectations of a bus driver—being “responsible,” “tak[ing] care…that everything will be okay,” and making sure that one’s passengers arrive home safely. At another point in his interview, Motti directly juxtaposes these expectations (in the discursive form of MS ‘you’-statements) with his own appraisal (in the form of ‘I’-statements) of his actions at the time of the TA he experienced: “But despite that, that everybody in, during the driving itself was alert, not 100%. I, was not alert. In that case, because you (MS) need to be 2 This is the same word in Hebrew, and thus, it is not possible to ascertain which of these words in English constitutes the best translation of Motti’s message here. 207 careful. If I had been alert enough, maybe, not sure, I would have, identified the terrorist.”xiii Here, Motti’s apparent grappling with his perceptions of the expectations of him as a bus driver and as a male member of Israeli society comes across in these vacillating pronominal choices. The generic ‘correctly-behaving’ and ‘approved’ Israeli bus driver “need[s] to be careful,” but Motti himself was not “alert enough,” and, in his mind, this may have contributed to the TA that took place. There seems to be a sense of guilt and responsibility here, which may have been a motivating factor in Motti’s refusal to return to work as a bus driver. Indeed, when I asked him what he would do if he found himself forced to return to driving a bus, he responded: 1 2 3 4 5 “To be a bus driver one needs a certain responsibility. … In any case you (MS) are transporting sixty, seventy people even on the bus. A minimum of responsibility. So I looked at myself, I say, ‘How can I put my hands on the [steering] wheel after what happened, how can I? Regardless of a terror attack, how will I see.’ I first of all don’t look at the road, I look at who is getting on [the bus] all the time, look at who like this, who is on the bus…” xiv This excerpt constitutes a good exemplar of Motti’s sparse but patterned use of the MS ‘you,’ particularly where it is juxtaposed with his use of the ‘I’ pronoun. Essentially, across Motti’s discourse and in his view, the generalized, universalized, ‘acceptable’ MS ‘you’ is responsible, taking care of many people in his charge. If a terrorist should appear, the MS ‘you’ is alert enough and identifies him, presumably thwarting any potential attack or threat. And if a disaster does take place, the strong, resilient Israeli man copes alone. Motti, on the other hand, seems to struggle with this “minimum of responsibility”; questions whether he was “alert enough”; and expresses that if he were to drive a bus again, he would be much more concerned with issues of safety and security than with driving. As difficult as these ruminations may be for Motti, his ability to differentiate between what he perceives as ‘acceptable’ behavior and whether he sees himself as capable of meeting these expectations shows a level of working through that did not appear in the texts of Yossi and Dani. Indeed, Yossi came across as focused intently on the effects of the trauma and what had been taken away from him therein, while Dani seemed to acknowledge little trauma but appeared deeply preoccupied with what he should have done, how he wished he had behaved, and what would have been more ‘acceptable.’ Motti seems to simply recognize that the TA he experienced took a certain toll on him, and consequently there are certain things of which he is no longer capable. In his perception, the MS ‘you’ should be able to do these things, but Motti himself most likely cannot, and he has more or less accepted this limitation. The next section focuses on Motti’s usage of the FS ‘you’ and his perception of the discursive role therein. 7.2.3 Bringing in the audience: Use of the second-person feminine singular pronoun Motti’s use of the FS ‘you’ was also almost entirely nonexistent in his BLI. According to the hypotheses set forth in this study, this suggests that Motti—similar to Dani—did not have a great need to bring me in to or project onto me aspects of his experiences, in the manner that Yossi 208 showed in abundance. Below are shown the few examples wherein Motti did use the FS ‘you’ pronoun, in order to illuminate his specific discursive choices. As in Dani’s case, analysis of the rare appearances of the FS ‘you’ across Motti’s entire BLI serves to only further support the central finding that this pronoun is used predominantly in sensory, emotionally charged, and potentially traumatic contexts. The interpretive hypothesis, validated in the cases of Yossi and Dani and perhaps even more evident here in Motti’s texts, suggests that these contexts represent the most difficult facets of the TA experience for the narrators, and therefore, seem to require the recruitment of the audience in the coping and meaning-making endeavor. Their emotional charge also makes these parts of the experience the most difficult to claim through the use of ‘I’-statements, as they may be perceived as incompatible with the masculine, heroic, ‘acceptable’ persona. Motti’s only two (analytically significant) usages of the FS ‘you’ within his BL TAN occurred within a single sentence describing the direct aftermath of the explosion on his bus: 1 2 3 4 5 “I was sure that I went, and I waited a few seconds, all of me was full of dirt, anndd shrapnel, and [shattered] glass anndd some other things, thaatt believe (FS) me that to shower, three hours, and you (FS) think that there are still things on you (FS). It is difficult to describe what, what flew all over me. In short, and, if I tell it maybe technically I, but uhh, I got off the bus maybe a few minutes later and I had a bottle of water, I always take [with me] a bottle of water, I began to wash myself…” xv The FS ‘you’ is utilized here in relation to a highly sensory statement, and its collocation with the PPr tense makes it stand out distinctly among the surrounding RPa tense usages. This single sentence seems to hold particular power and meaning for Motti, apparently connected with a need or desire to bring me in to the experience. Indeed, while his request for me to “believe (FS)” him necessarily involved a direct relating to me, the other two FS ‘you’-statements could easily have employed other pronouns (e.g., “…and I thought that there were still things on me”). The experience of thinking (or feeling) that the thick and multilayered residue of the attack would not or could not be washed away represents a powerful metaphor here in Motti’s discourse, bringing to mind both physical and emotional remnants of trauma. It may be, in fact, that he is referring to these figurative “things on you (FS),” while attempting to bring me in to this emotional experience. Later in his interview, Motti returned to the subject of what he saw immediately after the explosion, and again narrated his first shower after returning home: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 “The images. The images don’t go away. Don’t go away. I never saw, didn’t ((clicking his tongue)) how will I tell you (FS), I never saw a person without an arm. Or without a leg, or his head on the ceiling. That I am right now talking with you (FS) what I saw. Uhh or that a woman that a moment before that paid me money, sat down and all of a sudden sheee, very difficult sights. Everything blood, everything, (2) I don’t want to, to gross out how do [you (MP)] say, like it’s not ((chuckling)) but you (FS) do like this you (FS) see someone’s hairs… (1) Pieces of flesh. I remember, I showered here, I was here at home, I didn’t want to get out of the shower. … Cleaning myself, cleaning, and standing and washing m’self, and saying “Maybe more and maybe more and maybe more,” I know. (2) Difficult, difficult, really difficult.” xvi Here, Motti refers literally to his attempts to cleanse himself after the TA, and mentions more specifically that the “things” that flew onto him in the explosion included hairs and flesh from the 209 other victims of the explosion. Again, the specific recounted memories are told almost entirely in RPa ‘I’-statements, with the real-time communiqués with me taking place in the proximate-present (L.2 and L.3). And again, there is a single phrase employing the FS ‘you’ twice, bringing me in to a very sensory and emotionally charged past and experienced experience of Motti’s (L.6). Indeed, he speaks in a general manner of “very difficult sights” (L.4), and uses the ‘I’ to refer to something he “never saw” (L.1), but when describing something he actually did see and experience—which was presumably significantly disturbing for him—Motti employs the FS ‘you’ and the PPr tense. These two text excerpts thus seem to suggest a very specific and non-random pattern to Motti’s discourse about these facets of the TA experience, similar to that in Yossi’s and Dani’s texts. This pattern is even more apparent in one of Motti’s only other uses of the FS ‘you,’ which actually appeared in the conjugated form of the ‘onto’ preposition, much later in his BLI. It came in his response to my question regarding whether he sees any connection between his experiences in the army and what he went through in the TA: “In the army of course I saw a body here and there, but I didn’t see arms, and legs detached, and thi- fly onto you (FS) onto y-, things fly that, difficult to think about it. Something. It, so much [that it causes] kind of nausea…”xvii Although the speech is halting in places, Motti here again chooses to use the feminine form when referring to “things” that “fly onto you (FS).” Given that the three usages of this pronoun mentioned in this section represent the majority of its distribution across Motti’s BLI text as a whole, this discursive pattern seems highly significant. It is indeed notable that there was such a sparing distribution of the FS ‘you,’ occurring at three completely separate and distinct places in the interview, and yet all were employed so consistently in relation to the same subject matter. The possible motivation for or psychological interpretation of this pattern is, nonetheless, a matter of speculation. Perhaps Motti’s use of the FS ‘you’ is related to what he may perceive as weakness; that is, facets of the experience that he sees as the most difficult or the least masculine. Or perhaps he was most invested in bringing me in to the experience discursively when discussing the most traumatic parts—those that made him feel the most isolated and alone. One possible piece of evidence as to the motivations for Motti’s discursive choices may lie in the following quotation, part of which was presented above as an illustration of his use of the MS ‘you’: “And yes, after, the first three days you (FS) don’t know where you (FS) are. [They (MP)] are talking with you (FS) and media and people, and after that already three, four days, you (MS) already begin to cope with it alone.”xviii Here, in the final example of Motti’s use of the FS ‘you,’ he employs the pronoun in what comes across as a profoundly weak context—not knowing where one is. In this case, the MS ‘you’ takes on the active, ‘appropriate’ coping, but the FS ‘you’ takes on the passive experiences of not knowing, being, and being talked to. The next section investigates Motti’s distributed use of the ‘I’ pronoun, in order to bring all of these discursive patterns into interactional context. 210 7.2.4 Claiming ownership: Use of the first-person singular pronoun Motti’s BLI was told almost exclusively in ‘I’-statements, across myriad contexts and subjects. As, in this case, there is no single specific pattern to speak of, this section presents an excerpt of his TAN for illustrative purposes, and then provides several examples of Motti’s ‘I’statements that can be compared and contrasted with similar statements made by Yossi and Dani. Because there were so many ‘I’-statements across Motti’s interview overall, and because this often simply ‘feels logical’ to the decoder (and analyst), it is particularly important to juxtapose what was said with what could have been said, and even with similar statements made by other interviewees. The purpose herein is to distinguish Motti’s use of the ‘I’ pronoun as ‘above and beyond’ that of the other participants, and then to hypothesize as to the possible motivations for this pattern. Motti’s narrative of the TA he experienced is nearly completely ‘I’-focused, and he is clearly the central character in his descriptions. When I initially asked him to tell his TAN, he sat forward on his couch, leaned down toward the coffee table, and picked up two remote controls and placed them side by side on the table. Referring to one of them as a model of his own bus, and the other as representing the bus that had been next to his—essentially creating visual aids to assist in my understanding of the circumstances—he said: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 “And we are standing like this. Now, I look at the driver, and I do like this ((waving)) to him. For no particular reason [I] tell him hi, and as soon as I raise [my] hand it blows up. And I am here. Now, as soon as [it] blows up, I understood that it is a terrorist. No. I did like this “Wow, wow, wow,” this was my first reaction. And people from the windows, uhh, I am trying hard like, don’t know, it was an indescribable, sight. And now everything I am telling [you] it is a matter of seconds. It is an indescribable sight. Now all the people who were at me in the vehicle, and there weren’t many, they barely filled up the seats, there weren’t any people standing. There were only people sitting down. And then they came close to the windows here, to look [at] what is going on there. And [they (MP)] said “Wow, fast, it,” now [they (MP)] began, people began to run toward that vehicle, right here, difficult to describe what [you/they/we (MP)] see there, and people began to run to that bus, I went forward. Because I said, ‘There is nothing I can do here.’ To help them I can’t help them, I am not a doctor, I am not anything, there is nothing I can do here. I will clear the, I wanted to clear th’field, I knew that ambulances would start coming, and this is all a matter of seconds, by the way. Now I drive this way, people on the bus, are starting to get nervous, “Driver, driver, driver driver.” I stopped [the bus] here. Something like that. And, I opened doors. I opened the doors and people began to get off [the bus]. I didn’t get off. Didn’t, didn’t, didn’t feelll, didn’t feel a ne-, a need to get off [the bus]. People began to get off [the bus], I estimate that ten people got off [the bus]. Suddenly {that} bus {exploded}. Explosion, impossible to describe it. And then III say to myself ‘So, Motti how are you (MS) dying?’ Straight away, I remember it up until today. ‘How are you (MS) dying?’ ”xix This excerpt is in line with the central discursive finding of this study that the ‘I’ pronoun, most often collocated with the RPa tense, is most often associated with agentic, powerful, and ‘heroic’ behavior. Indeed, here Motti looks, does/did, tells, raises his hand, understood, went forward, said/says, knew, drives, stopped the bus, and opened the doors—all active and potent behaviors. In this sense, Motti chooses to focus the content as well as the form of his narrative on his role as actor and agent. Alongside of this, as discussed above, Motti places a significant focus on the actions and experiences of the passengers on his bus, the fellow experiencers of the TA. Overall, Motti’s 211 grammatical choices here seem to create a sense of connectedness and lived experience within this text—a feeling that Motti was there and directly experienced these things, and is willing to talk about his personal experiences as such. There is very little need to distance himself, project his feelings onto others, or universalize or collectivize his experience within this section of discourse. To illustrate just one comparative example, both Dani and Motti claimed to have understood immediately after their respective explosions that it had been a TA. Indeed, Motti’s statement, “Now, as soon as [it] blows up, I understood that it is a terrorist” (L.2-3) is nearly identical to one that Dani had made: “Immediately I understood that it is a bombing…” This seems to present an image of both narrators as present-minded and prescient, thinking and understanding even in the most chaotic and stressful of circumstances—indeed, two individuals who showed courage under fire and strength under pressure. Significantly, from a discursive point of view, both statements have the bombing/explosion and/or terrorist positioned in the PPr tense, while the action of the encoder is in the RPa tense. Semiotically, this suggests that the TA is perceived by Dani and Motti as more proximate and salient than their understanding of its occurrence. Overall, Motti dedicated very little space in his TAN to describing the immediate scene or aftereffects of the TA on his bus, and showed a tendency throughout his BLI to avoid discussing ‘the gory details.’ Several times he stated that he didn’t want to “upset” me, or said, “You shouldn’t know about these things,” but it was not possible to ascertain his reasons for omitting these parts of the narrative. However, the omission of these details stands in contrast to Yossi’s inclusion of vivid and descriptive accounts of the immediate aftermath of the TA he experienced. In addition, Motti showed a contrasting strategy in the form of his discourse. While the majority of Yossi’s post-TA descriptions were told predominantly from the perspective of the FS ‘you,’ Motti, in one of the few descriptions of this nature, offered a more generalized report-style discourse: 1 2 3 4 “What I saw is difficult for me to talk about. Arms all around, and and and a person lying next to you (MS), not speaking. And, difficult sights, difficult sights. There was silence. Actually, I don’t know if there was silence or that my ears were plugged up, but everything is red from blood from people, everything is dirt and [shattered] glass and, end of the world, end of the world {it was}.” xx Particularly notable here is that Motti says, “What I saw…” (L.1), rather than, “What you (FS) see…” He also says, “There was silence” (L.2), which represents a significant semiotic opposition to Yossi’s, “You (FS) hear silence.” While Motti uses, ‘I,’ and, ‘my,’ here, he also once refers to the MS ‘you’ (L.1-2), in addition to using the PPr tense throughout the excerpt. Thus, in this proximate but non-personalized and report-style description, Motti shows much less of a tendency to utilize either of the discursive techniques employed widely by Yossi and Dani. This seems to suggest less of a need to distance himself from or bring his audience in to the described experience, which may indicate greater ‘ease’ and ability to own even these difficult facets. While he was noticeably taciturn with regard to describing the post-attack scene, Motti 212 showed a prominent tendency to provide much detail and speak freely about a wide variety of other related topics throughout his BLI. When I asked him, for instance, to describe the moment when he got off the bus after the TA, and if someone had helped him, he responded: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 “No nobody helped me. I got off [the bus] all by myself. I, I told you (FS) thaat in the first few seconds I thought that I am about to die, annd it is tangible. Because I heard rumors that people who were killed they stayed whole, and insiide, don’t know what happened. So I thought that that’s what is happening to me. After a few, don’t know to estimate time, maybe half a minute, ten seconds, or a minute, or something like that, I started to check myself. ((sounds of patting on his body)) Arms, legs, I started like this, to shake off what, ears to see that it’s okay, nose, like this I remember, I felt on myself the, and I started to clean myself I remember. And everything therre, where the driver, everythinngg was bent, and broken, and shattered glass, and. Slowly slowly I got up, I started to take m’self, to wash m’self, I got off the bus, just then that journalist came, I tell him, he’s the first that I saw when I got off the bus ((chuckling)) yes, don’t know, [he] pounced on me.” xxi This excerpt provides another prototypical example of Motti’s profuse use of the ‘I’ pronoun, even in a relatively sensory and emotionally charged context. This is, perhaps, the closest available context to the immediate post-bombing scene, and Motti chose to describe it and his actions elaborately and with abundant ‘play-by-play’ detail. A similar level of elaboration was evident in Motti’s account of his disembarkation from the bus: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 “…I got off the bus maybe a few minutes later and I had a bottle of water, I always take [with me] a bottle of water, I began to wash myself like this, to wash, right then apparently apparently a journalist came at that moment. [He] says to me “You are the driver right?” I told him, the first words that came out of my mouth, I said “What luck that I opened the doors. And peopllee escaped.” Whoever stayed in the vehicle, bit it. I think maybe ten, five, were killed. All the rest were wounded. But the rest escaped, and it is a little comforting, in some way. And then I told him “My God what luck that I opened the doors,” and I said it to him like, that’s what came out of my mouth, don’t know why. He made it a scoop. Liike, the hero driver, whose intuition let [him] open doors. It’s not true. But I said that to the media too. Like it’s not true, it wasn’t my intuition to open doors that maybe there is a bomber also at me. I didn’t think there is a bomber at me. I like to tell th’truth. I am not someone who, tries to, to get some award, I could have come and said, ‘Yes, I opened, I had a feeling,’ that I didn’t say that.”xxii This excerpt, which is almost exclusively ‘I’-centered, presents a notable illustration of the narrative selection mechanism of sharpening. Motti selects for inclusion in his TAN this significant mini-narrative about his interaction with the journalist, and Motti also makes the choice to repeat twice what he had told him (L.4 and L.6-7), employing dialogue. And yet, this text is filled with Motti’s repeated and vehement protestations that the wrong conclusions were reached and that he is not a “hero driver.” In this sense, Motti appears to be sending a dual message. That is, in the process of vociferously denying the media’s ‘misinterpretation’ of the events surrounding the TA and his possibly-heroic role therein, he is nonetheless rebroadcasting, and perhaps reinforcing, that very misinterpretation. This is particularly apparent in Motti’s use of repetition throughout, essentially sharpening the message that he is purportedly attempting to deny. A final comparison between excerpts from Motti’s and Dani’s BLIs provides perhaps the clearest demonstration of the disparities in their discursive patterns, particularly regarding uses of ‘I’ and the MS ‘you.’ Here, Motti discusses his continued post-traumatic symptoms and associated difficulties, while Dani talks about his own experiences with hypervigilance and being reminded of 213 the TA he experienced. It should be noted that the cells in the table below were divided according to analogous form rather than content. That is, the content of each pair of cells is not always parallel, but comparisons can be made between the manners in which the content was told. Table 7-1: Discursive pattern comparison between Motti’s and Dani’s parallel texts MOTTI “The the every time the I once went to the market, I, it’s hard for me to explain this to you (FS) because I M1 hope for you (FS) that you (FS) will not experience it. But, I went once to the market, my mother wanted a M2 few things. I got there, I went into the market, where there is a lot of people and, it seemed to me that at any moment something is about to explode. Like, I would walk and I like, here M3 it is coming now, here it is coming now. And I would buy quick quick quick and go, buy quick quick and go. Because at any moment that I M4 felt that it is about to happen, any moment I felt it is about to happen. I was at the market no more than a few minutes. So I say, ‘How can I be on, on a bus and I’? M5 I look at the picture now, I get on a bus and drive, and people get on and people that I don’t know get M6 on and I am afraid of them and I, I won’t be focused on the work, for sure. Thaat I definitely won’t be focused on the work. That is what I wanted to prevent because in any case, a minimum of responsibility to transport that kind of amount of people. I was sure that I am not focused on the work and then. (1) ((mumbling)) I M7 didn’t want to lie [to] myself. I didn’t want to lie [to] myself to say that nothing happened and I can drive. I will put the people in danger, I for sure that I would {put the people in danger} in some way.” xxiv DANI “The truuth? (1) D1 On, on that specific [bus] line, when I passed by from thhe it, a bit {you (FS) know, D2 you (MS) are looking, you (MS) are on alert, on alert}. You (FS) know what that is? … {On alert is D3 liike, YOUR HEART BEATS, YOUR (2) yoouu (MS) are looking, you (MS), are expecting something. A different [bus] line like it doesn’t matter what what but when you (MS) pass by there you (MS) also know you (MS) are reminded D4 you (MS) say “Wow, God, what a miracle I had here.” You (MS) look you (MS) say “My God right here it happened to me right here that I pulled over right here” I am trying to}, to penetrate it. … You (FS) understand? To penetrate th’, mem- memory. … And it comes back, sometimes. {You (MS) say “My God”, suddenly} brrrrr, ((sound of an automatic weapon firing in succession)) {you (MS) hear the, like} echoing in your (MS) head. Reminded. Yes? Sometimes [you/they/we (MP) are] reminded.”xxiii D5 D6 These two texts, when juxtaposed, show a contrast between personalized and generalized discourse. The overall content topic is essentially the same, but the communicative strategies utilized are entirely different. Motti speaks only in ‘I’-statements and mostly in the habitual conditional use of the PPr tense, referring to personalized feelings and experiences—as apparent in, “it seemed to me…” (cell (C.) M3), and, “I felt…” (twice in C.M4). He then extrapolates from his experience at the market to the possibility of his returning to work as a bus driver, admitting that he “won’t be focused on the work” (twice in C.M6; again in C.M7), and would thus “put [his potential passengers] in danger” (twice in C.M7). Motti’s excerpt comes across as thus personalized in both 214 form and content, as he speaks about his feelings from an ‘I’-focus and connects them to a very personal sense of responsibility. In contrast, the excerpt from Dani’s interview positions the entire mini-narrative onto the MS ‘you,’ leading to a significantly different discursive experience. The only use of the (nondialogic) ‘I’ pronoun comes in the opening sentence of the mini-narrative, collocated with the RPa tense, as he sets the scene. From then on, the experience is generalized and in the habitual use of the PPr tense, with the apparent attempted message that this is what the MS ‘you’ ‘anyperson’ might experience under these conditions. Some of the direct discursive parallels are evident in the table above, as in C.M3 and C.D3, when Motti relates to his own feelings but Dani refers only to the MS ‘you’ and his actions and feelings. This is apparent again in C.M4 and C.D4, when Motti talks of his actions from a specific and ‘I’-centered focus, while Dani refers to what the MS ‘you’ does in this generalized context. And again, a direct comparison is possible with regard to form in C.M5 and C.D5, when Motti states, “I say…”, while Dani repeatedly states, “you (MS) say…” The divergences in discourse are discernible in this comparative presentation, and suggest a distinct difference in perception and attribution within the post-TA experience for both participants. Indeed, it is possible to imagine Motti telling the above excerpt from Dani’s story, with the phrasing, “I would look, I would be on alert,” rather than, “you (MS) are looking, you (MS) are on alert”; and, “my heart would beat,” rather than, “your heart beats.” I contend that these discursive strategies reflect each individual’s inner experience and perception of the social discourse surrounding them. In the case of Motti, his high degree of ability and willingness to own nearly all facets of the TA experience, as demonstrated by the use of the ‘I’ pronoun, may indicate a greater level of comfort with his role therein. His relative lack of discursive generalizing, universalizing, and/or projecting onto me of most parts of the experience may thus be interpreted as a greater degree of comfort with and acceptance of a wider range of experiences surrounding the TA, both physical and emotional. The possible motivations for this pattern are discussed further below. 7.3 QUANTITATIVE DISTRIBUTION OF PRONOUNS AND TENSES WITHIN MOTTI’S BASELINE TAN: HOW MUCH AND WHEN HE SAID IT In order to give a comprehensive picture of the phenomena discussed above, the following tables and graphs present quantitatively the distribution and percentage computations of Motti’s uses of pronouns and tenses within his BL TAN. In the first set of analyses, only the first-person and both singular second-person pronouns are presented, as these are the primary foci of the acrossparticipant analyses. 215 Table 7-2: Primary pronoun and tense distribution in Motti’s baseline TAN ‘Past’ ‘Present’ ‘Future’ % use of each pronoun4 ‘I’ 48 27 1 95% ‘YOU (MS)’ 0 0 0 0% ‘YOU (FS)’ 0 1 1 3% ‘WE’ 0 1 0 1% % use of each tense3 60% 36% 2% There were 71 total lines of text in the TAN, and a total of 80 of these primary relevant pronouns (and therefore, 80 relevant tense uses), and the ratio of pronouns per line was calculated to be 1.13. The graph below presents the patterns of collocations of each pronoun with each tense. Figure 7-1: Primary pronoun-tense collocations in Motti’s baseline TAN5 With a view on the essential assertions of this study with regard to the extra-linguistic meanings of the uses of the various pronouns and tenses, this graph presents a picture of an individual who, at the time of his first interview, was claiming almost all of the TA experience for himself, with the majority of that claiming expressed discursively in a less proximate and more processed manner. There was virtually no evidence of a need to generalize or universalize, or of a need to bring me in to any significant part of the experience. As was discussed in the first section of results in this chapter, it was also found that Motti’s use of third-person pronouns in his TAN was significant, both in number and in light of its atypicality, relative to the pronominal patterns of Yossi and Dani. Thus, it seems necessary to present a widened view of the quantitative pronominal data; and to this end, the table below shows 3 4 5 The use percentages of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of tense-marked verbs in the TAN. The use percentages of each pronoun were computed as relative to the total number of these pronouns in the TAN. The bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within the TAN. 216 the first-, second-, and third-person pronoun and tense distribution in Motti’s BL TAN. Table 7-3: Expanded pronoun and tense distribution in Motti’s baseline TAN 48 27 1 ‘YOU (MS)’ 0 0 0 ‘YOU (FS)’ 0 1 1 54% 0% 1% ‘I’ ‘Past’ ‘Present’ ‘Future’ % use of each pronoun7 ‘WE’ ‘HE’ ‘SHE’ 0 1 0 10 6 1 3 5 1 ‘THEY’ (MP) 29 4 2 1% 12% 6% 25% % use of each tense6 64% 31% 4% This widened analytical lens brings the total pronoun count to 141 (and therefore, 141 tense uses), and the new ratio of pronouns per line was calculated to be 1.99. The graph below presents the expanded picture of pronoun-tense collocation patterns evidenced in Motti’s BL TAN. Figure 7-2: Expanded pronoun-tense collocations in Motti’s baseline TAN8 From this widened analytical view, it is apparent that, although the ‘I’ is still the dominant voice in Motti’s BL TAN, the variety of third-person characters—in particular, the generic MP ‘they’— occupy a significant place in the narrative as well. Indeed, this was observable in several of the excerpts presented previously, when Motti’s discourse was focused intently on the actions and experiences of the passengers on his bus and the others surrounding the scene of the first TA. Over and over, Motti made the choice to include them in his narrative and to position these ‘theys’ as 6 7 8 Here, again, the use percentages of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of relevant tense-marked verbs in the TAN. Here, again, the use percentages of each pronoun were computed as relative to the total number of the expanded set of pronouns in the TAN. Here, again, the bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within the TAN. 217 central characters therein. This discursive pattern was found to be unusual among the group of participants in this study, and particularly stands in contrast to the TANs of both Yossi and Dani, who repeatedly referred to their TAs as something they experienced solitarily or that happened to them alone. In this sense, while Motti’s BLI represents the most prominent use of the ‘I’ pronoun among all the participants, it also provides a unique case of significant third-person pronoun use. 7.4 PRONOUN ‘POETRY’ OF MOTTI’S BASELINE TAN: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT As was done for Yossi and Dani, and according to the method described in detail in Chapter 3 (Section 3.6.2.3), the following table is the ‘poetic’ result of the 80 lines of text of Motti’s baseline TANxxv. Here, again, poems were constructed only for the significantly-used pronouns, and thus, there are no columns for either the MS or FS ‘you,’ as it was established above that these pronouns were virtually nonexistent herein. However, because Motti placed such a significant focus on various third-person subjects, columns were included for singular and plural poems of these pronouns. The third-person poems were not separated by tense as was the ‘I’ poem, but ‘present’ tense entries are noted here with a double-underline. As previously, all within-narrative dialogue was excluded from both the poems and the tabulations. Table 7-4: The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Motti’s baseline TAN9 ‘I’ POEM (73) ‘PAST’ (RPa) (45) ‘PRESENT’ (PPr) (28) 7 1 I understood that it is a I need to keep going terrorist straight 8 2 I did like this “Wow, wow, I look at the driver 3 wow” I do like this to him 4 --[I] tell him hi 19 5 I went forward I raise [my] hand 20 I said, ‘There is nothing I can 6I am here 9 do here’ I am trying hard 24 10 I wanted to clear th’field [I] don’t know 25 I knew that ambulances would 11I am telling [you] start coming --28 21 I stopped [the bus] here I can’t help them 29 22 I opened doors I am not a doctor 30 23 I opened the doors I am not anything 32 26 I didn’t get off [the bus] I drive this way 33 I didn’t feel --34 36 I didn’t feel a need to get off I estimate 38 [the bus] I say to myself ‘So, Motti --how are you (MS) 40+41 I was sure that I went dying?’ 42 39 I waited a few seconds I remember it up until 43 All of me was full of dirt today 45 44 I got off the bus (if) I tell it maybe 47 I began to wash myself technically 9 3rd-PERSON POEMS (42) SINGULAR (21) PLURAL (21) 48 A journalist came at that 12All the people who were moment at me in the vehicle 49 13 [He] says to me “You They came close to the are the driver right?” windows here 56 14 He made it a scoop [They] said “Wow, fast, --it” 67 15 He was here [They] began 71 16 Someone told me People began to run afterwards toward that vehicle 75 17 Someone told me [You/they/we (MP)] see 76 Some doctor told me there 18 “Listen, [they] made People began to run to you into a hero…” that bus 82 She is an older woman --84 27 Someone, couldn’t People on the bus, are 92 My brother tells me starting to get nervous 93 31 (He) was at home People began to get off 94 My brother is older than [the bus] 35 me People began to get off 97 She is sitting down off [the bus] 98 37 She sees Ten people got off [the 99 She sees her son bus] 100 She began to cry, and to --62 yell They didn’t ask The numbers to the left of each entry signify its location in the overall order of 115 pronominal entries. Three dashes (- - -) signify a significant break in the given sequence, during which other pronouns or tenses were used. Italics here signify a repetition in the discourse. 218 50 I told him I said “What luck that I opened the doors…” 53 I told him “My God what luck that I opened the doors” 54 I said it to him 57 I said that to the media too 58 I didn’t think there is a bomber at me 61 I didn’t say that 63 I stopped [the bus] 64 I said “Whoever wants, should get off” 65 I didn’t think 66 I was here 69 I didn’t hear a thing 72 I called it a slaughterhouse 77 I didn’t know this 78 I wasn’t aware of anything 79 I wasn’t aware of anything 80 I couldn’t even answer the phone calls 81 I caught her [my mother] on the phone 83 I told her “Mom don’t worry…” --95 I told him “Stay with Mom…” --104 I lied to her 105 I lied for a reason 107 (I say that) I lied 108 I wanted to come home and then to tell her 110 I didn’t know how hard it [the coping] would be 111 I thought that okay, what happened happened 113 I was in a support group 51 46 I always take [with me] a bottle of water 52 I think maybe ten, five, were killed 55 (I) don’t know why 59 I like to tell th’truth 60 I am not someone who, tries to, to get some award --70 I don’t know how to estimate time --88 I know what 90 I say [it] in quotes 91 I don’t think of myself as a hero --106 I say (that I lied) --114 I am in treatment (till today) 101 She knew that [they/we (MP)] had tricked her 102 My brother calmed her down 103 [He] said that it’s okay 109 She sees me everything is okay 115 He calls me in, to see that everything is okay 68 [They] evacuated us to the hospital in ambulances 73 [They] evacuated me to the hospital 74 [They] are coming to see --85 They had to interview me 86 They asked 87 They pushed/pressured 89 They just asked to interview ‘the big hero’ 96 [They] are watching [the news] --114 [They] prepared me for it What is most apparent from the pronoun ‘poetry’ of Motti’s BL TAN, and what most distinguishes it from that of Yossi and Dani, is that the RPa ‘I’ poem contains almost all of the content of the narrative, albeit in a condensed form. In addition, it is predominantly active and agentic. Notably, it begins with Motti’s declaration that he “understood” that the explosion on his bus had been caused by a terrorist (entry (E.) 7), and then continues through Motti’s actions and responses to the attack. The poem has a matter-of-fact feel to it, with Motti describing the sequence of events from a relatively non-emotionally charged perspective. He repeats the phrase, “I opened [the] doors” (E.29-30), then repeats that he “didn’t feel” a need to get off the bus (E.33-34). This double repetition can be interpreted as signifying the emotional high point of the RPa ‘I’ poem, and seems to represent the central message of Motti’s narrative: that he opened the bus doors (saving the lives of many of his passengers), but did not do so because of any intuition or premonition. There are only a very few entries in the RPa ‘I’ poem describing the immediate aftereffects 219 of the TA—particularly the aspect that Motti thought he was dead for a few seconds (E.40-42) and that he was covered in dirt and debris (E.43)—and here, again, any detailed descriptions of the post-bombing scene are conspicuously absent. Motti then refers to washing himself (E.47), and then immediately turns to his interaction with the journalist. There is repetition here as well, of the phrases, “I told him…” (E.50 and E.53), and, “I said…” (E.51, E.54, and E.57). He also repeats the phrases, “I didn’t think…” (E.58 and E.65), and, “I wasn’t aware of anything” (E.78-79). Ultimately, when viewed in ‘poem’ form, what appear to be Motti’s core perceptions and attributions about his terror experience seem crystallized in these repetitions—opening the doors, not feeling, telling/saying, not thinking, and not being aware of anything. The final (triple) repetition lies in E.104, E.105, and E.107, wherein Motti refers to having lied to his mother about having been on the affected bus. The presumed import of these three statements can be directly reinforced through a view on the content within the third-person singular poem, wherein an emotional peak is evident in E.97-99 (with minor build-up in prior entries). Motti’s description of his mother sitting down in front of her television and seeing him on the news talking about having been in a TA, is told in the PPr tense, denoting the emotionality, proximity, and salience of this (imagined) event for him. Similarly, E.109 and E.115 are also told in the PPr tense, both of which express content relating to “everything [being] okay”; i.e., that Motti is okay after what he has experienced. In this sense, Motti’s reference to what he has constructed narratively as his mother’s experience of understanding Motti’s involvement in the TA and his admission of having lied to her can be interpreted as significant, meaningful, and emotional facets of the overall experience for Motti. Evidence for this interpretation can be found in both the repetition in the RPa ‘I’ poem and the PPr tense use in the third-person singular pronominal poem. The PPr ‘I’ poem contains the first six pronominal uses of the TAN and begins with the scene-setting for the overall TA experience, wherein Motti is driving his bus and interacting with the driver of the bus parallel to his. After shifts to the RPa ‘I’ and third-person plural poems, he returns to the PPr tense to discuss his inability to help the victims of the first bombing (E.21-23). Both of these foci involve an apparent lack or lesser degree of agency, as Motti’s role therein is primarily as a witness and passive bystander. He “raise[s] [his] hand” (E.5) to wave to the other driver and then sees his bus explode (presumably, as the action of seeing this does not exist in any of the poems). Motti “tr[ies] hard” (E.9), but “can’t help them” (E.21), because he is not a doctor or “anything” (E.22-23). As opposed to the RPa ‘I’ poem, in which Motti “understood,” “did,” “went forward,” “stopped,” and “opened the doors,” here in the PPr ‘I’ poem, Motti’s ‘character’ comes across as much more passive and less able. Much of Motti’s discourse in this poem also exists outside of the traditional time boundaries of the TA itself, as in his discussions of what he “estimate[s]” (E.36), “remember[s]” (E.39), and 220 “think[s]” (E.52); what he is telling me within the current interview context (E.44); what he always does (E.46); what he likes to do (E.59); and what he is (E.60). It is in this poem, in particular, that the most descriptive details of the immediate post-TA scene seem to be most lacking, contributing to a sense of lack of coherence and narrative order to the poem itself. There is little action and less description of the TA experience, and much more meta-discourse surrounding Motti’s present-day attributions about the experience and himself. Notably, the poem concludes with Motti’s reference to being in psychological treatment, from directly after the bombing up to “today” (E.114). This was also the penultimate statement of the TAN overall, suggesting an integration and cohesion between the TA and the consequent treatment, and the proximity of the treatment to Motti’s daily life. By way of hypothetical comparison, if Motti’s final sentence had been, “Since then, I have had to be in treatment,” this might send an entirely different message. Finally, the third-person plural pronominal poem illustrates a significant focus placed by Motti on the other experiencers of the TA, as was discussed above. Much of the overall emotional and physical reactions to the first bombing exist here, with the people exclaiming, beginning to run, seeing, getting nervous, and getting off the bus—as opposed to Motti, who understood, did, went forward, and realized that he couldn’t help anyone. Significantly, one of the only PPr tense entries in this poem concerns the people “starting to get nervous” (E.27), which appears to be the most emotional reaction in the poem. From E.12 to E.37, all of the third-person plural subjects were the people on Motti’s bus—the passengers who were ultimately saved from the bombing that took place on his bus after they got off. There is little to no mention of those passengers who fell victim to the second bombing, and they, consequently, essentially do not exist in Motti’s TAN. Then, from E.62 on, the third-person plural subjects were either generic and unspecified, or journalists wanting to interview Motti. Even when he refers to journalists, he does so in a nonspecific and collectivized way, sending a different message from that of the entries involving the passengers on his bus. While the latter are referred to in nearly every entry as “people,” the former are presented almost as an amorphous and impersonal “they” clan, clamoring for an interview, asking, pushing, and pressuring (E.85-87 and E.89). The only exception here is E.96, wherein Motti refers to his family, in the context of the previous mini-narrative in which his mother discovers his involvement in the TA. In consistent form, this entry, too, is presented in the PPr tense—further confirming that, regardless of the subject pronoun involved, this narrative seems to exist in the proximate realm for Motti, and was thus told as such across all four pronominal poems. Ultimately, in (or perhaps due to) the absence of any description of the immediate postbombing scene, and with no FS ‘you’ pronominal poem, it is possible to hypothesize that the circumstances surrounding Motti’s mother’s discovery of his having been in a TA and having lied to her may have been the most upsetting facet of the experience for Motti. The consistent patterns 221 of repetition and use of the PPr tense for the emotional climaxes, and the existence of threads of this mini-narrative across all of the major pronouns employed in his TAN, could be interpreted as evidence for this hypothesis. It should also not be forgotten that here in Motti’s case, the analytical lens was widened to include the two third-person pronominal poems because of the absence of both the MS and FS ‘you’ pronouns in his TAN. The significant predominance of the ‘I’-voice herein may thus provide the most substantial indication of a higher degree of working through, processing, and ultimate acceptance by Motti of the TA experience in his life and life story. Only within this primary interpretation can the relatively emotional and proximate mini-narrative surrounding his mother be perceived as significant (but secondary) on the scale of processing. 7.5 THE CONNECTION BETWEEN THE ‘I’ AND THE LIVED EXPERIENCE: WILLINGNESS TO OWN A WIDER AND DIFFICULT RANGE OF EXPERIENCE The fundamental interpretive finding that the ‘I’ pronoun is used predominantly when the encoder is discussing his actions and agency within the context of the TA he experienced, was evident across all of the interviews conducted for this study. While Yossi and Dani showed their own patterns in uses of the MS and FS ‘you’ pronouns, they still demonstrated this systematic discursive use of the ‘I’ pronoun. Motti, however, went ‘above and beyond’ all of the participants’ patterned usages, with a preponderance of ‘I’-statements across his entire BLI and in over 90% of his TAN (as compared with Yossi’s 62% and Dani’s 51%, when taking into account only the firstperson and second-person pronouns). The next chapter delves more deeply into a comparative analysis across the specific cases of Yossi, Dani, and Motti, but it is important here to provide some larger content-related context for Motti’s discursive choices and patterns. To this end, this section attempts to ‘validate’ the interpretations put forth throughout the present chapter, through a triangulation with some of Motti’s more direct statements about heroism, weakness, coping, and possible guilt. Several examples are presented of Motti’s willingness to discuss his image in the media, his feelings about his actions, the indescribability of some facets of the TA, his difficulties with coping and the prospect of returning to work as a bus driver, and his view of himself as weak. In analyzing Motti’s BLI, it became apparent that there exists some ambivalence between his image as “the hero driver” and his own perception of himself and his actions surrounding the TA. As discussed above, although Motti frequently and persistently protested the heroic image placed upon him by the media, this nonetheless was expressed through recurrent broaching of the topic of his heroism throughout the interview. And while he seemed to view himself as weak and decidedly non-heroic, his very willingness and ability to discuss this self-image may actually suggest the opposite. Indeed, Motti discussed his coping processes and difficulties after the TA more than any of the other interviewees, and did so from a perspective of openness and introspection. While Yossi offered myriad diatribes about his problems with the National 222 Insurance Institute and feeling abandoned to cope alone by his government, and Dani presented various prescriptions for how the generic MS ‘you’ should cope but did not speak much about his own coping, Motti spoke candidly and honestly about his own very personal struggles. Despite instances in which Motti employed the MS ‘you’ in his discussions of coping, the finding that the content exists at all in his discourse is what may be considered significant in this case. Motti spoke frequently throughout his BLI about the media having made him into part of a “scoop,” as “the hero driver” whose intuition caused him to save the lives of many of his passengers. He also offered a number of narratives about being recognized almost daily on the streets of his city, both by people who had seen him on television and those who were connected in some way to the passengers who had been on his bus that day. The apparent message of these anecdotes seemed to be that Motti understood his newfound societal role and the image that had been cast of him. This seems particularly evident in the following excerpt: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 “When I spoke on television I was very nervous, never in my life had I spoken on the radio suddenly I’m on, television on the news. And I spoke with a friend who works in the media, he says to me “Television always in every situation looks for, doesn’t matter if it’s a terror attack, if it’s a positive thing, if it’s soccer. They look for the hero of the moment. So you (MS) were in their eyes the hero of the moment. That you (MS) saved and you (MS) did and people survived and got off [the bus]. So everyone is looking for you (MS), they (MP) aren’t looking for someone else.” He says, “They (MP) are looking for the star of the, of the situation”…”xxvi Here may be manifest evidence of Motti’s belief that he was perceived, at least in the media’s eyes, as “the hero of the moment,” as having saved people and as having acted ‘acceptably.’ Later, he also presented an understanding of the ‘less acceptable’ option, as represented by the driver of the other bus, who was not perceived as a hero. Indeed, when I asked him whether the other driver had been interviewed, he responded that he had not, and explained: 1 2 3 4 5 “They grabbed me because I knew how to express myself, I don’t know if I express myself perfectly, but I knew how to express myself, to talk. And I also am the hero, you (FS) understand, he isn’t a hero. ((chuckling)) In quotes, again, I am not saying hero so that you (FS) will think that I am really a hero. … Everything is in quotes. So he wasn’t interviewed anywhere. It’s possible that he was interviewed in Russian on some radio station…” xxvii Again, the content here contains many protestations against his heroic image, but the form (particularly the repetition and sharpening of “hero”-related discourse and themes) seems to serve to actually reinforce the very perception to which Motti objects. It is possible that Motti’s sole motivation is simply humility, and that he truly rejects this position into which he was placed by the media and surrounding society. Nonetheless, given the volume of narratives throughout his BLI about the various other people who had seen him on television and been exposed to this “scoop,” it can be presumed that Motti sees this heroic image of himself as having been cemented in his society’s appraisal. This, in turn, can be interpreted as having a significant impact on his discourse. Thus, whether or not Motti agrees with this image, I contend that his placement in this category essentially ‘allows’ him to discuss the full range of surrounding issues—good, bad, and otherwise. Both Motti and Dani referred to wishes that they had acted differently at the time of the TA 223 they each respectively experienced. While Dani was most preoccupied with his desire to go back to the scene of the (main) attack and kill or neutralize the shooter, Motti was concerned with his lack of intuition (the very intuition whose existence the media presumed and publicized). At one point, Motti pointed to a position on his makeshift schematic of the geographic location of the attacks and stated: “I say, you (FS) know, if I could go back, I should have opened the bus here. At this part. Not to keep going, here.xxviii When I asked him why, he responded: 1 2 3 4 “Because there is li-, more people would have gotten off [the bus]. I mean, but I didn’t have the, the thought that there is someone in the vehicle at me, terrorist. … I didn’t have that thought. It, it is only because, again, only because of the pressure of the people on the bus, I stopped and opened the doors.xxix This was Motti’s only reference to any possible feelings of guilt or regret about his actions or lack thereof, and stands in contrast to Dani’s repeated ruminations on what he should have done, wished he had done, wanted to do, and tried to do. Motti’s owning of these feelings and statements, by means of ‘I’-statements, can be interpreted as an indication of a more ‘ease-ful’ level of processing. Overall, Motti’s ability and willingness to discuss a variety of his difficulties surrounding the TA, particularly from the discursive stance of the ‘I,’ can be viewed as reason to place him closer to the ‘ease-ful’ end of the narrative processing scale. His openness and forthcoming nature concerning topics such as fear, coping difficulties, psychological struggles and need for both psychopharmaceutical and talk therapy, membership in support groups, and his inability to drive a bus since the TA reached far beyond the related content in any of the other participants’ interviews. And nearly all of these discussions were presented in the ‘I’-voice. Some of Motti’s references to his difficulties coping with the TA and its aftereffects have already been discussed, in the context of his use of the MS ‘you’ pronoun, but the excerpt below constitutes one of the many examples of Motti’s candid ‘I’-voice in this context. It came as a response to my question of whether he might have been willing to return to driving a bus if he felt he was psychologically able to do so: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 “III, don’t know, III thought about that many times. … I thought about that, also during the nights, I would think about it. It’s difficult, it’s difficult, it’s difficult. It’s very difficult even. And also uhh, beyond the fact that it’s difficult for me, I am sure that I feel that I, that I will not give the level of driving toward the people, the safety/security10 of the people, it won’t be good. … Because the driving won’t be the same driving that I know, the fear will take control over me and, I will look more at the people than at the road, for sure my leg will shake while driving. That, also from a safety/security standpoint, that is also dangerous. And beyond the fact that for personal reasons it is very difficult. Very difficult. I thought about it, I said, maybe I, what happens if I will go to work on, I mean I imagine that I am at the [steering] wheel, driver… very difficult. From many standpoints. It is also safety/security, and also psychologically and. It is impossible even. Despite the fact that there are people uhh that went back to it.” xxx Here, Motti repeats over and over how difficult this proposition would be for him, discussing both potential emotional (“the fear will take control over me,” in L.5) and physical (“my leg will shake while driving,” in L.6) reactions. In addition to his lengthy discussions throughout his BLI of the 10 This is the same word in Hebrew, and thus, it is not possible to ascertain which of these words in English constitutes the best translation of Motti’s message here. 224 challenges he believes he would face if he returned to work as a bus driver, Motti also made many references to the difficulty and indescribability of what he experienced in the TA itself. Below is only a very partial list, collected in the span of under 90 (of a total of nearly 2,800) lines of text: Table 7-5: Content regarding difficulty and indescribability in Motti’s baseline interview 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 “[It is] difficult to explain to someone, who comes from outside, even thouugh maybe he saw, on television he sees …”xxxi “…I am trying hard like, don’t know, it was an indescribable, sight. And now everything I am telling [you] it is a matter of seconds. It is an indescribable sight.”xxxii “…[it is] difficult to describe what [you/they/we (MP)] see there…”xxxiii “Explosion, impossible to describe it.”xxxiv “It is difficult to describe what, what flew all over me.”xxxv “Yes, it is difficult, very difficult. It is very difficult. (2) That’s pretty much the gist, and afterwards, what was on the bus also, it is difficult for me to to explain it, I called it a slaughterhouse. … Really just like that. A slaughterhouse of (2) of all kinds of things. … It was a slaughterhouse what happened there.” xxxvi “Uhh, and afterwards the coping. In the beginning I didn’t know how hard it would be. I thought that okay, what happened happened, one or two days, and [they] prepared me for it…”xxxvii Both the form and content of these multiple excerpts seem to indicate that Motti experienced many facets of this event as extremely difficult, both to undergo and to express afterwards. There is a great deal of repetition of these themes throughout his BLI, and much introspection about the impact that the experience has had on his psyche and well-being. What is most significant and unusual here, given the central psycholinguistic assertions of this study and in relation to the other interviewees, is that the predominant majority of these issues and themes were discussed in the ‘I’voice and thus, owned and claimed by Motti. Perhaps the most significant piece of narrative evidence can be found barely twenty lines into Motti’s BLI, in his first reference to the TA, still within the confines of his life narrative. With no prompting from me and unrelated to either the prior or following subject matter, Motti stated: 1 2 3 “It is possible to say thaat my life has changed since the terror attack, despite the fact that many [people] don’t see that on me, but I, maybe it’s not good, many told me that, I cope more inside than outside. I don’t let out what I, feel inside. That’s also maybe not so good.” xxxviii If this is taken as Motti’s title statement with regard to his TA experience, most apparent is his preoccupation with the topic of coping and his introspection and self-evaluation on this issue. This was, indeed, evident throughout his BLI. When contrasted with Dani’s title sentence, “If I had had a gun, I would have gotten off [the bus],” which also represented much of Dani’s focus throughout his BLI, a very different image emerges of both individuals and their views of the most important facets of their respective experience. Motti appears accepting of the impact that the TA has had on his life, but questions whether he is coping ‘appropriately.’ Slightly later, he stated: “People see me on the street won’t think that I went through some terror attack, some kind of thing, but on the inside I, I am very weak, I am very weak, I feel about myself that I am very weak.”xxxix Herein lies what, I assert, may be the crux of the issue for Motti: the ambivalent dynamic between what he feels, on the one hand, and what he 225 perceives as the appraisal of him by his surrounding society, on the other. This seems reflected in his constructed split between the “inside” and the “outside”; what he believes and feels vs. what he shows; and how he sees and attributes his behavior during the TA vs. how the media viewed him. Dani’s BLI, too, contained some of these dialectic themes and a great deal of ambivalence on similar issues, much of which made its mark on his discourse in the form of an elevated usage of the MS ‘you.’ What is significant in Motti’s case, then, is his ability and willingness to grapple with these difficult issues from a self-focused position, shown by a discursive use of the ‘I’-voice. Indeed, even within his TAN, Motti was able to make this rather non-‘heroic’ statement: “…I went forward. Because I said, ‘There is nothing I can do here.’ To help them I can’t help them, I am not a doctor, I am not anything, there is nothing I can do here.”xl His candor about what he was able and not able to do, what he did and did not do, and what (in his opinion) does and does not qualify him as a hero, all discussed in ‘I’-statements, sets Motti apart from the other participants. I assert that this is not simply a reflection of difference in personalities or communication styles, or even due to the divergent TA experiences of the interviewees. Rather, it appears to be Motti’s certainty and assurance that he has been universally placed into the ‘correct,’ ‘appropriate,’ and ‘most acceptable’ social category—that of “the hero”—that may allow him the psychosocial freedom to discuss the widest range of emotional and psychological facets of what was clearly a difficult experience for him. Motti apparently has little need to bring me in or project much of the experience onto me, because he does not seem to fear being viewed as weak. Indeed, he himself is even able to proclaim outright that he is weak and feels that he is weak, purportedly because he knows that his heroic image will ‘trump’ these declarations. (Alternatively, it is theoretically possible that Motti omitted or silenced the descriptive details of the immediate post-TA scene from his TAN out of such trauma and upset that he has not even begun to process it. However, the excerpts presented above did demonstrate that these facets were not silenced entirely from Motti’s BLI, and his openness on so many related difficult topics appears to challenge this theory.) In addition, although Motti expresses many of the same ambivalences as Dani, he does not recruit the MS ‘you’ to the same extent—apparently indicating neither interest nor need to generalize, universalize, or normalize his experience. In fact, Motti seems to have more motivation to discuss these issues from the strong and claiming ‘I’-voice, as he exists in his own unique and ‘acceptable’ position. Thus, in many ways, while Motti may or may not agree with his society’s appraisal of his ‘heroism,’ the existence of the image itself may actually facilitate and allow Motti to process the experience more resiliently, taking into account the wider range of attendant emotions and uncertainties. Yossi’s and Dani’s discourse was much more black-and-white, in both form and content, as they seemed so intent and concerned with presenting their agenda and convincing me of their desired social position. Motti, I contend, appears to have no need for these 226 agendas, as his image has already been firmly entrenched in the ‘right’ place. Thus, he may be able to ‘let his guard down’ and open up his ‘I’-voice to discuss a variety of ‘less acceptable’ issues. This may, indeed, place him closest to the ‘ease-ful’ side of the narrative processing continuum. 7.6 CONCLUSION This chapter began from a holistic perspective on Motti himself and the BL interview context and experience, then explored the overall patterns in his discourse, then showed a basic quantitative view on the distribution of these patterns, and then moved to a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of both the form and content of these patterns. Finally, a content-focused analysis of Motti’s discourse, particularly regarding his attributions, perceptions, and emotions, was presented. This analysis of Motti’s outward discursive and narrative choices was employed in order to understand his related inner psychological beliefs and perspectives. The following two tables summarize the central findings of Motti’s BLI, within the discursive and narrative categories that were analyzed. The first focuses on Motti’s patterns of pronoun (and tense) use. Table 7-6: Summary of pronoun use findings and interpretations in Motti’s baseline interview PRONOUN USE First-person possessive pronouns and phrases Third-person ‘others’ MS ‘you’ and distancing discourse FS ‘you’ ‘I’ ‘We’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING Suggests some sense of responsibility and ownership for his bus, for what happened to the passengers at him Lack of solitary phrases suggests no solitary sense of experience—no focus on himself to the exclusion of the other experiencers Suggests great focus on the other characters and experiencers of the terror attack—“the people did…” Suggests acceptance of the shared nature of the experience and little to no need to personalize it—it did not happen only to him Suggests generalized, universalized, distanced experiences with unspecified or without subjects, creating a sense of dissociation and disconnection—“It is the coping” Suggests some ambivalence with regard to ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ coping vs. his own process of coping Suggests perception of ‘correct’ behavior as a bus driver surrounding responsibility, alertness, taking care of the situation and passengers—and uncertainty as to whether he fulfills this expectation Relative lack suggests almost no need to bring audience in to experience—highly equipped and willing to deal with most of the difficult facets of experience in an ‘owning’ fashion Only very few uses suggest that the most traumatic part of experience was finding human debris on himself afterwards and trying to wash it off Suggests much willingness and ability to claim ownership of a wide range of facets of the terror attack experience—including difficulties with coping, indescribability of experience, inability to work as a bus driver since, and view of himself as weak Suggests understanding that his image as a hero has been firmly entrenched in his society’s perception—he is ‘acceptable’ Suggests much ability to grapple with difficult issues and ambivalences, while still being introspective, open, and candid from the personal voice Suggests much acknowledgment of collective nature of experience—“they evacuated us” 227 As a final analytic summarizing exercise, presented here is a list of suggested hypothetical messages, or end points (Spector-Mersel, 2011), which appeared central to Motti’s BL TA narrative and discourse. Some of them may have appeared as more conscious or less so, but all were either expressed manifestly or seemed present in some way between or underneath the lines of his interview text. While most are essentially my own formulations of what I understand and interpreted as Motti’s ‘main messages,’ I believe they most succinctly summarize and illustrate the results of the comprehensive analyses conducted throughout this chapter—with regard to both form and content, and particularly related to the found pronoun and tense use patterns. Table 7-7: Suggested ‘main messages’ of Motti’s baseline interview I acted, felt, and experienced a range of behaviors, emotions, experiences, and facets of the terror attack, almost all of which are acceptable to me. You (MS) need to cope with the aftereffects of the terror attack in an appropriate way. You (FS) experience a single, more upsetting sensory facet of the terror attack. They (my passengers, and members of my family) experienced this together with me. It is best if you (MS) cope alone. I cope alone, inside. You (FS) are affected (only) by the single sensory, most emotionally distressing facet of the terror attack—the debris on you afterwards. I was seen by the media and my society as “the hero driver,” but I don’t agree with that appraisal. I don’t see myself as a hero, but on some level I’m glad that I’m seen this way. What happened to my passengers during the terror attack might have been my fault. Although I don’t feel pressure from anyone about this, I think I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the terror attack—I should have let the people off sooner. We experienced the terror attack together, my passengers and I. The other experiencers of the terror attack and its effects are just as important ‘main characters’ in the story as I am. The terror attack changed me and my life, and I accept that. I have had difficulties coping with the aftereffects of the terror attack, and I am comfortable talking about that. I say that I see myself as weak, but the way that I talk might show just the opposite. Motti’s baseline interview provided another rich opportunity for a further investigation of the link between an individual’s hypothesized inner attitudes and his perceptions of the societal attitudes and cultural discourses surrounding him. Motti was found to have used the ‘I’ pronoun to a much greater degree than the other participants in this study, and in conjunction with a much wider array of contexts and topics. He was also viewed by his society as the most ‘heroic.’ Thus, Motti was presented here as an exemplar of an ability and willingness to claim and ‘own’ the greatest range of facets of his terror attack experience. 228 CHAPTER 8 YOSSI, DANI, AND MOTTI IN CONTEXT: HOW INDIVIDUAL DISCOURSE REFLECTS AND IS SHAPED BY PERCEPTIONS OF SOCIOCULTURAL DISCOURSE SURROUNDING ‘ACCEPTABLE’ BEHAVIOR 8.1 INTRODUCTION This chapter brings together the central psycholinguistic hypotheses and findings of this study, through a comprehensive across-participant analysis. The pronominal findings of Yossi, Dani, and Motti will be juxtaposed, quantitatively and qualitatively, through form and content, and on the micro- and macro-levels, in order to explore the individual and societal messages therein. By comparatively examining possible explanations for the varied pronoun use patterns across these interviewees, a deeper understanding may be garnered of the psychosocial phenomena and motivations underlying their linguistic choices. To this end, this chapter asserts three main points that emerged from this line of analysis, represented in this first section of results (Chapters 5-7). First, it is contended that the data presented in this section validated the sign-oriented linguistic meanings for the Modern Israeli Hebrew pronoun and tense systems (Tobin, 1989b, 1990, 1995; as delineated in Chapter 3 (Sections 3.5.2.1 and 3.5.2.2)). Second, it is claimed that the frequency patterns shown in the discourse of the individual drivers interviewed herein provided support for the hypothesized, interpretive extra-linguistic meanings directly associated with these pronominal forms (as delineated in Chapter 4 (Sections 4.2 and 4.3)). And third, it is asserted that these patterned extra-linguistic messages, connected to the sign-oriented linguistic meanings of the pronoun and tense systems, appear to reflect the inner psychosocial values held by the encoders, as well as their perceptions of the psychosocial and cultural discourse surrounding them. The final message of the comparative analyses presented here is that societal discourse can have an impact on individual discourse, and this interaction can also shape and be shaped by the individual’s personal processes of narrative processing and meaning-making after a stressful life event. This chapter first shows the patterned pronoun findings from a comparative quantitative perspective, in both absolute and relative terms. Next is presented a summary of the qualitative findings and the central messages expressed therein, with discussions of the possible motivations behind these patterns. Following this, connections are forged between these discursive patterns and the specific personal experiences of each of the interviewees, in an exploration of the divergences between them and possible explanations. Finally, several more specific content comparisons are made between manifest views expressed by Yossi, Dani, and Motti on the topics of coping, societal pressure and discourse, and views on ‘the Israeli man’ and what may be more or less ‘acceptable.’ All these comparative findings, juxtaposed and integrated, allow a comprehensive perspective on the interplay between individual and sociocultural discourse and personal narrative processing. 8.2 PRONOUN USE, MEANINGS, AND MESSAGES: QUANTITATIVE AND QUALITATIVE FINDINGS AGGREGATED AND JUXTAPOSED While the same general patterns of linguistic meanings of the pronouns used in the drivers’ discourse were found across the texts of Yossi, Dani, and Motti, the actual frequencies and ratios of 229 pronoun usage varied significantly across the interviewees. The extra-linguistic messages of their pronominal discourse also varied. This section summarizes and juxtaposes the pronominal findings of these three drivers, in order to highlight the similarities and differences. Below is a comparative view of the quantitative data already presented in each individual chapter, of each participant’s pronoun usage in his main narrative of analysis (the most emotionally charged narrative for each one—that is, for Yossi and Motti, their respective TANs; and for Dani, his TrAN). Figure 8-1: Comparison of pronoun use in the main narratives of Yossi, Dani, and Motti1,2,3,4,5 Simplified slightly, the essential comparative data reveal the following: 1 2 3 4 5 The bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within each individual’s narrative. The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of uses of the ‘I’ pronoun (‘past’ and ‘present’ aggregated) between the participants. For Yossi’s vs. Dani’s uses of ‘I’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.2541—thus not statistically significant. For Yossi’s vs. Motti’s uses of ‘I’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0344—thus statistically significant. For Dani’s vs. Motti’s uses of ‘I’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0098—thus very statistically significant. The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of uses of the MS ‘you’ pronoun between the participants. For Yossi’s vs. Dani’s uses of the MS ‘you’, the Chi-square probability is less than 0.0001—thus extremely statistically significant. For Dani’s vs. Motti’s uses of the MS ‘you’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0003—thus extremely statistically significant. The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of uses of the FS ‘you’ pronoun between the participants. For Yossi’s vs. Dani’s uses of the FS ‘you’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0078—thus very statistically significant. For Yossi’s vs. Motti’s uses of the FS ‘you’, the Chi-square probability is less than 0.0001—thus extremely statistically significant. For Dani’s vs. Motti’s uses of the FS ‘you’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0522—thus not quite statistically significant. The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of uses of the ‘we’ pronoun between the participants. For Yossi’s vs. Dani’s uses of the ‘we’, the Chisquare probability equals 0.3984—thus not statistically significant. For Yossi’s vs. Motti’s uses of the ‘we’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.2331—thus not statistically significant. For Dani’s vs. Motti’s uses of the ‘we’, the Chi-square probability equals 0.1292—thus not statistically significant. 230 Table 8-1: Relative comparison of pronoun use in the main narratives of Yossi, Dani, and Motti USE OF ‘I’ Moderate (63%) Moderate (52%) High (95%) YOSSI DANI MOTTI USE OF ‘YOU (MS)’ None (0%) High (19%) None (0%) USE OF ‘YOU (FS)’ High (33%) Low (11%) None (3%) As evident here, each individual used one of the pronouns at a much higher rate than the others: Yossi, the feminine singular (FS) ‘you’; Dani, the masculine singular (MS) ‘you’; and Motti, the ‘I’. The possible motivations and explanations for these patterned differences will be explored further in the following section. Here, first, it is also necessary to view these findings from a qualitative comparative perspective. To this end, below is presented a series of tables, organized by pronoun, juxtaposing the main interpretive findings, suggested meanings, and hypothesized messages of the discourse of each of the interviewees. It may be necessary to state again that these ‘main messages’ are not direct quotations from any of the participants, but rather, a posited paraphrasing of the essential extra-linguistic message suggested by the particular set of pronoun findings. The first table deals with the use of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: Table 8-2: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of first-person possessive pronouns and solitary phrases ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI DANI Suggests solitary sense of experience—“what happened to me” What happened to my passengers, on my bus, might have been my fault. I think there was a societal/cultural expectation that I could or should have done more to prevent the terror attack. I was and am all alone in my experience of the terror attack and its aftereffects. The terror attack is a big part of my life—it is my life. What happened to my passengers during the main terror attack might have been my fault. Suggests sense of responsibility and guilt for his passengers, for what happened I think there was a societal expectation that I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the main terror attack. Suggests separation of himself and solitary sense of experience—“what I am seeing in front of me” I think I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the terror attack—I should have gone back to kill the shooter. I set myself apart or feel set apart in my experience of the terror attack(s) and its aftereffects. What happened to my passengers during the terror attack might have been my fault. Although I don’t feel pressure from anyone about this, I think I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the terror attack—I should have let the people off sooner. We experienced the terror attack together, my passengers and I. MOTTI Suggests sense of responsibility and guilt for his bus, for his passengers, for what happened Suggests some sense of responsibility and ownership for his bus, for what happened to the passengers at him (Lack of solitary phrases suggests no solitary sense of experience—no focus on himself to the exclusion of the other experiencers) 231 Here, the central similarity among the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti seems to surround a perceived sense of responsibility, guilt, and ownership for what happened to their passengers. There is some connection between the interviewees with regard to expectations of their behavior vis-à-vis preventing the terror attack (TA), with differences in their levels of guilt and sense of pressure. And perhaps the central difference among the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti seems to be their position on a hypothetical continuum from aloneness to connectedness, both in the experience of the TA and afterwards. While Yossi seems to feel isolated and abandoned by his country and government institutions, Dani seems to take it upon himself to set himself apart (both discursively and otherwise). Motti, on the other hand, comes across as connecting himself to his passengers—his ‘co-experiencers’ of the TA—in both the content and form of his discourse. The next table deals with the use of various third-person ‘others’ and other types of distancing discourse by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: Table 8-3: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of third-person ‘others’ and distancing discourse ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI Suggests generalized, ‘usual,’ timeless experiences with generic, universalized subjects—perhaps disconnected from them himself DANI (Relative lack suggests great focus on the MS ‘you’ as the generalization/collectivization discursive strategy of his choice) Suggests great focus on the other characters and experiencers of the terror attack—“the people did…” Suggests acceptance of the shared nature of the experience and little to no need to personalize it—it did not happen only to him MOTTI Suggests generalized, universalized, distanced experiences with unspecified or without subjects, creating a sense of dissociation and disconnection—“It is the coping” Various ‘others’ generally take on the generic/universalizable facets of the terror attack. I feel alienated and isolated from my surrounding community and society because of my sense of aloneness in my experience of the terror attack. (None) They (my passengers, and members of my family) experienced this together with me. The other experiencers of the terror attack and its effects are just as important ‘main characters’ in the story as I am. Here, distinct and prominent differences are evident between the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti. While Yossi employed a variety of third-person ‘others’ in an apparent attempt to distance himself and/or express a sense of alienation from his surrounding society, Dani did not use them at all, placing his entire discursive distancing strategy on the use of the MS ‘you.’ And Motti introduced a number of third-person characters to apparently express that the other experiencers of the TA, placed as narrative ‘characters’ alongside of himself, were just as important to the ‘plot.’ The next table deals with the use of the MS ‘you’ by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: 232 Table 8-4: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of the MS ‘you’ ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI DANI MOTTI (Relative lack suggests very little need or ability to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize (in)to a larger context world) Suggests great need to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize facets of experience that he is uncomfortable claiming as his own Suggests great need to generalize, universalize, collectivize, and/or normalize (in)to a larger context world—but unclear how inclusive this world is, in his eyes (all men, or all Israelis, or all Israeli males, or all Israeli male bus drivers, etc.) I was and am all alone in my experience of the terror attack and its aftereffects. I feel alienated and isolated from my surrounding community and society because of my sense of aloneness in my experience of the terror attack. You (MS) experience the parts of these difficult experiences about which I am more ambivalent and uncertain regarding their ‘acceptability.’ I was afraid during the main terror attack, but isn’t that okay? Suggests possible attempt to exclude me (specifically) discursively from the specific context or experience under discussion You (MS) are not normal if you don’t react emotionally in some way to a traumatic experience. Suggests significant ambivalence and uncertainty with regard to the less ‘acceptable,’ ‘appropriate,’ or ‘correct’ facets of experience—such as his behavior and emotions A real Israeli man is courageous, never afraid, strong, and always able to find solutions and act to prevent disaster or injury. Suggests possible conflict or contradiction between his appraisals of his own behavior and his perceptions of his surrounding society’s appraisals I am highly uncertain and ambivalent as to whether I fit this description. Suggests some ambivalence with regard to ‘correct’ and ‘acceptable’ coping vs. his own process of coping You (MS) need to cope with the aftereffects of the terror attack in an appropriate way. It is best if you (MS) cope alone. I cope alone, inside. Suggests perception of ‘correct’ behavior as a bus driver surrounding responsibility, alertness, taking care of the situation and passengers—and uncertainty as to whether he fulfills this expectation Here, the primary difference among the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti involves their perceptions of and relationship with the hegemonic social and cultural discourse of their surrounding society. Yossi’s relative lack of use of the MS ‘you’ seems to belie a sense of alienation—from his society, in general, and from his government and its institutions (specifically, the National Insurance Institute (NII)), in particular. Dani, on the other hand, seems to place great emphasis on his society and its appraisals of his behavior before, during, and after the difficult events he experienced, and comes across as deeply preoccupied with questions regarding whether he is ‘normal’ and ‘acceptable’ in the eyes of his society. Essentially, Dani appears to want to be connected to and viewed as an integral member of his society, but seems to fear that he has been rejected or denied membership into ‘the club.’ Finally, Motti, who has been granted this elusive status of “hero,” and knows he has found favor in the eyes of his society, seems to place more focus on his own coping, alone and without (perhaps rejecting) the help of his society, as he seems to believe it should be. 233 The next table deals with the use of the FS ‘you’ by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: Table 8-5: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of the FS ‘you’ ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI DANI MOTTI Suggests great and acute need to bring audience in to the most difficult, upsetting sensory facets of experience—either to educate, to join him in his pain and solitude, and/or to ‘take on’ the projected traumatic and/or passive (‘unacceptable’) facets instead of him Suggests some need to bring audience in to the most difficult, upsetting sensory facets of experience, and much of the more emotional and passive experiences—such as feeling introspective, or helpless or powerless Directed (non-analytically significant) use suggests great need for my understanding, connection, and cooperation in our coconstruction of meaning within the interview and in the co-creation of a shared experience, both discursively and emotionally Relative lack suggests almost no need to bring audience in to experience—highly equipped and willing to deal with most of the difficult facets of experience in an ‘owning’ fashion Only very few uses suggest that the most traumatic part of experience was finding human debris on himself afterwards and trying to wash it off You (FS) experience the worst, most traumatic, and least ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. You (FS) are traumatized by the terror attack. You (FS) are affected by the (many) sensory, most emotionally distressing facets of the terror attack. You (FS) experience the passive, weak, and ‘unacceptable’ parts of these difficult experiences. You (FS) are affected by the (few) sensory, most emotionally distressing facets of these traumatic experiences. You (the interviewer) are my main conduit to understanding, connection, and possibly acceptance of even the less ‘acceptable’ parts of my behavior. You (FS) experience a single, more upsetting sensory facet of the terror attack. You (FS) are affected (only) by the single sensory, most emotionally distressing facet of the terror attack—the debris on you afterwards. Here, there are both similarities and differences among the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti. As was apparent from the quantitative data, the central distinction here is in the volume of usage of this pronoun, with Yossi employing it the most, Motti the least, and Dani a minor amount of times. This is also visible in the qualitative messages, as Yossi’s message refers more to trauma, while those of Dani and Motti refer more to being affected. However, in keeping with the central psycholinguistic assertions of this study, all three employed the FS ‘you’ in similar contexts—that is, the most difficult and upsetting sensory facets of the most emotionally charged experience (their respective TAs as well as Dani’s traffic accident (TrA)). Even though Motti’s use of this pronoun came only in conjunction with a single part of his TA experience, it nonetheless followed in line with this fundamental hypothesis. The next table deals with the use of the ‘I’ pronoun by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: 234 Table 8-6: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of ‘I’ ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI DANI MOTTI Suggests desire and ability to claim ownership only of parts of experience in which he was acting ‘heroically’ (‘acceptably,’ in his perception of the viewpoint of Israeli society) Suggests desire and ability to claim ownership only of parts of experience in which he was acting in an unequivocally ‘heroic’ manner (‘acceptably,’ in his perception of the viewpoint of Israeli society) Suggests much willingness and ability to claim ownership of a wide range of facets of the terror attack experience—including difficulties with coping, indescribability of experience, inability to work as a bus driver since, and view of himself as weak Suggests understanding that his image as a hero has been firmly entrenched in his society’s perception—he is ‘acceptable’ Suggests much ability to grapple with difficult issues and ambivalences, while still being introspective, open, and candid from the personal voice I experienced the ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. I was active and behaved ‘appropriately’ during parts of the terror attack experience. I did not act heroically or ‘acceptably’ enough during other parts. I acted during the ‘acceptable’ facets of these difficult experiences. I was active during parts of the main terror attack, but not heroic enough. I should have been more active and heroic during the main terror attack. If I could have, I would have been more active and heroic during the main terror attack. I think there was a societal expectation that I could or should have done more to prevent (at least the scope of) the main terror attack. I acted, felt, and experienced a range of behaviors, emotions, experiences, and facets of the terror attack, almost all of which are acceptable to me. I was seen by the media and my society as “the hero driver,” but I don’t agree with that appraisal. I don’t see myself as a hero, but on some level I’m glad that I’m seen this way. I say that I see myself as weak, but the way that I talk might show just the opposite. The terror attack changed me and my life, and I accept that. I have had difficulties coping with the aftereffects of the terror attack, and I am comfortable talking about that. Here, again, the main messages of Yossi, Dani, and Motti revolve primarily around their relationship with their surrounding society, and their perceptions of its expectations of them. While Yossi and Dani appear preoccupied with questions relating to whether they were active enough during the TA, and whether they behaved ‘acceptably’ (that is, ‘heroically’), Motti is not—perhaps because he may be aware that he has already been appraised as ‘acceptable.’ Dani seems to invest great energy dealing with the ‘should’s and ‘would’s and ‘could’s of his behavior, perhaps in an attempt to convince himself and/or his society of his ‘acceptability.’ Yossi, on the other hand, confines his ‘I’-statements only to those actions or dimensions that he apparently is certain would be deemed ‘appropriate’ through the perspective of the surrounding sociocultural discourse. And Motti allows himself to discuss a number of ‘weaker’ facets of his experience, perhaps because he feels that his society’s acceptance of him into the ‘in-group’ of heroes permits him to do this. The final pronoun results table deals with the use of ‘we’ by Yossi, Dani, and Motti: 235 Table 8-7: Findings and suggested ‘main messages’ of uses of ‘we’ ‘MAIN MESSAGES’ INTERPRETIVE FINDING YOSSI DANI MOTTI Relative lack suggests very little to no acknowledgment of the collective nature of experience—“what I did”; “what happened to me” Relative lack suggests very little to no acknowledgment of the collective nature of the terror attack—“what I saw” Suggests minor attempt to build on the use of the MS ‘you’ as a further mechanism to align himself with the ‘acceptable’ collective and ‘fit in’ to the societal norms Suggests much acknowledgment of collective nature of experience—“they evacuated us” I was and am all alone in my experience of the terror attack and its aftereffects. The terror attack is a big part of my life—it is my life. I set myself apart or feel set apart in my experience of the terror attack(s) and its aftereffects. We Israelis are supposed to behave courageously and actively and correctly. We experienced the terror attack together, my passengers and I. The other experiencers of the terror attack and its effects are just as important ‘main characters’ in the story as I am. Here, a distinct difference is observable between the three interviewees, again in the realm of a continuum from isolation to connectedness. While Yossi’s use of this pronoun sends an apparent message that the TA happened (only) to him, and is a big part of (only) his life, Motti seems to send the opposite message: that the TA happened to him and the other experiencers together, and they share this common experience. On the other hand, Dani’s pronominal use here appears to send an ambivalent message—which may, in some ways, ‘bridge the gap’ between the messages of Yossi and Motti. That is, Dani suggests that he experienced the TAs (and TrA) alone, but apparently hopes to insert himself into the collective experience and societal ‘in-group’ through his attempts to appear courageous, heroic, and ‘acceptable,’ along with ‘all of us Israelis.’ The final table presents a juxtaposed summary of the hypothesized comprehensive extralinguistic ‘main messages’ expressed in the pronoun patterns in the participants’ baseline discourse: Table 8-8: Comprehensive pronominal extra-linguistic ‘main messages’ YOSSI DANI MOTTI I experienced the ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. You (FS) experience the worst, most traumatic, and least ‘acceptable’ facets of the terror attack. Various ‘others’ generally take on the generic/universalizable facets of the terror attack. I acted during the ‘acceptable’ facets of these difficult experiences. You (MS) experience the parts of these difficult experiences about which I am more ambivalent and uncertain regarding their ‘acceptability.’ You (FS) experience the passive, weak, and ‘unacceptable’ parts of these difficult experiences. I acted, felt, and experienced a range of behaviors, emotions, experiences, and facets of the terror attack, almost all of which are acceptable to me. You (MS) need to cope with the aftereffects of the terror attack in an appropriate way. You (FS) experience a single, more upsetting sensory facet of the terror attack. They (my passengers, and members of my family) experienced this together with me. 236 To clarify and simplify these distinctions even further, the following essential hypotheses can be claimed here: 1) the use of the ‘I’ pronoun can signify: “I may or may not approve of my actions/feelings—but I perceive that my society does approve”; 2) the use of the FS ‘you’ pronoun can signify: “I do not approve of my actions/feelings—and I perceive that my society does not approve either”; and 3) the use of the MS ‘you’ pronoun can signify: “I am not sure if I approve of, or I am ambivalent about, my actions/feelings—and I am not sure if my society approves, or I think my society is ambivalent about them as well.” In this sense, the individual’s perception of the sociocultural discourse surrounding him and approval or disapproval therein can have a profound effect on the manner in which he tells his narrative. This can, in turn (as a precursor and/or consequence), have an equally profound effect on the manner in which he processes this narrative, works through it, makes meaning out of it, and integrates it into his life and life story. 8.3 CONNECTIONS BETWEEN THE PARTICIPANTS’ PRONOUN USE AND PERSONAL EXPERIENCES Taking into account the central similarities and differences—both quantitative and qualitative—delineated above, this section presents hypotheses about the possible motivations underlying these patterns. In order to do so, the analytical lens is shifted to focus upon the interviewees’ individual personal experiences, and then widened to consider the larger societal and cultural context surrounding these individuals, and their attitudes about this context. Each participant is discussed here separately, placing into context the particularities of their experience and the specific pronoun use patterns they employed. 8.3.1 Yossi: “You (FS) are spineless” As seen in Chapter 5, Yossi came across as having had the most difficulty dealing with and recovering after what he experienced, and, at the time of their respective baseline interviews (BLIs), both he and Motti had been (psychologically) unable to board a bus since their attacks. Yossi fully admitted that he was unable to prevent the attack on his bus, repeatedly referring to the near-futility and impossibility of attempting to identify a potential suicide bomber. He was not working at all at the time of his BLI, and repeatedly mentioned difficulties with nightmares and recurrent vivid thoughts about the TA. Indeed, his ‘performance’ of his terror attack narrative (TAN) seemed to suggest that he was reliving the event on a visceral level during his retelling (he also told me specifically that this was the case). In this sense, Yossi’s pattern of pronoun usage suggests that he may still be very involved with the emotional and psychological effects of his TA experience. He showed the highest incidence of usage of the FS ‘you,’ indicating that he may have been the most motivated to attempt to draw me into these post-TA emotional experiences, either with him or in place of him. The moderate incidence of the ‘I’ pronoun in Yossi’s discourse can also be viewed against the 237 background of his stated inability to prevent the attack and respond immediately thereafter in a ‘heroic enough’ manner. He made frequent references to his inability to hear and general confusion and shock directly following the explosion, almost as if he was explaining why he did not do more. This may suggest that Yossi may not see his reactions or behavior in a positive light, and therefore, that he perceives himself as having less justification or ‘right’ to discursively claim these parts of the experience by speaking in the first-person singular voice. Indeed, later in his interview, when I asked Yossi whether he thought that “what drivers do” is courageous, he replied: 1 2 3 4 “Very courageous (MS), very courageous (MS). You (FS) need to be courageous (FS) in order to do those things. Because if you (FS) were, if you (FS) are spineless6, or something like that, you (FS) get up and say (FS) ‘Listen, I cannot (FS) drive {anymore}, I cannot (FS) be in that b-, in that box and tomorrow fly into the air’.”i Significantly, here Yossi continues to use a predominant majority of the FS ‘you,’ even as he describes, more or less, exactly what his own response was to the prospect of driving after the TA. This seems to reinforce the hypothesis that the FS ‘you’ is the discursive ‘main character’ when the topics of discussion are weakness, “spineless[ness],” inability to act, and lack of courageousness, power, strength, and heroism. In contrast, the generic and universalized MS ‘you’ is courageous. In that he has been so affected by the traumatic nature of his TA experience, Yossi’s overall relatively lesser amount of ‘I’-statements, along with his heightened use of the FS ‘you’ pronoun, may indeed suggest his perception that he is not following his society’s prescription for ‘successful’ (that is, courageous and heroic) coping and general behavior. 8.3.2 Dani: “The society pities him in my opinion” Viewed through this lens, Dani’s patterns of pronoun use also validate the hypotheses suggested in this study. He barely spoke of the emotional effects of either the TAs or the TrA he experienced, and spent most of his BLI attempting to deny the existence of any effects at all. It therefore fits that his use of the (analytically significant) FS ‘you’ was low, as he appeared to have little need or opportunity to attempt to bring me into his emotional or sensory experiences— because, in his eyes, they simply do not exist (or so he apparently wanted me to believe). Dani did make a number of ‘I’-statements with regard to the actions he did perform, in the context of both the TAs and the TrA. Indeed, although he also mentioned not being able to prevent the main TA, he did think and act quickly during it, possibly preventing much greater injury to his passengers by evacuating the bus immediately to a nearby hospital. And yet, Dani repeatedly stated that he wished he could have done more, and would have done more if circumstances had permitted. It supports the central claims of this study that the MS ‘you’ receives the most usage in Dani’s discourse throughout his BLI, in the contexts of the TAs as well as the TrA he experienced. 6 The phrase Yossi used here can be literally translated to, “weak of character.” 238 This pronominal pattern can be interpreted as a reflection of Dani’s efforts to ‘fit in’ with his society, to universalize his actions and the experiences themselves, and to present the argument that he is no better or worse than ‘the next guy,’ who is “not normal if [he isn’t] scared.” Dani’s ambivalence, however, seems to lie in his desire to indeed be better than ‘the next guy’—Dani wishes he had behaved more heroically and courageously, and he wishes he hadn’t been scared. In this sense, the consistent message of the majority of his BLI appears to be a presentation of his perception of ‘the ideal Israeli man,’ and his attempts to prove to me that he fits this description. The following unbroken four-part series of Dani’s responses to my direct questions aimed at understanding his perceptions and attributions in this area may shed light on this interpretation. I first asked Dani to define for me, a non-native Israeli, “what is an Israeli man,” and he responded: 1 2 3 4 “An Israeli man, it’s a person who deals. A person who, experiences things annd needs to give, needs to deal with them, things that are not nice things, (2) difficult things let’s (FS) say the army and it’uhh peop- people don’t know. (1) A man who, needs to deal with dangers and needs to find solutions for them let’s (FS) say like that. ^Always have to be on alert.^”ii Much of this constituted a reiteration of what he had already said about his own army experiences and what he had learned therein. I then asked Dani what kind of “solutions” he was referring to, to which he replied: 5 6 7 8 9 “\((apparently thinking to himself out loud)) {What kind of solutions? What kind of solutions?}\ For instance [they (MP)] come, \((apparently thinking to himself out loud)) {what kind of solutions?}\ you (MS) see let’s say a suspicious-looking person on the street, you (MS) have to don’t know what, to, to ask him something, ttoo investigate him that [he] woooon’t, you (FS) understand? That he won’t, go up explode, blow himself up.”iii This is a particularly significant excerpt, wherein Dani essentially offers his perception of the ‘acceptable’ and ‘correct’ Israeli man as one who takes action, even in a potentially highly dangerous scenario. The focus here is also on a prevention of disaster, rather than acting or coping well after a disaster has occurred. This holds meaningful implications for Dani’s views of his own behavior, both before and during the main TA he experienced, and surrounding the TrA that occurred on his watch. He has already shown that he is all too aware that he did not prevent either of these incidents, and that a number of individuals were harmed as a result. The lack of discussion here of any worthiness of ‘heroic’ action after a disaster already occurs, can reasonably be interpreted as further evidence that Dani may not view his own behavior at the time of these events as fitting into the definition of ‘correct’ or ‘approved’ Israeli male conduct. That is, thinking fast and springing into action quickly, managing to get the bus out of the line of fire quickly (and while still under fire), and racing his wounded passengers to the nearby hospital for treatment, is all simply not enough, in Dani’s appraisal, or in Dani’s view of his surrounding society’s appraisal. Following this line of thought in the interview, I then asked if Dani thinks there is pressure in Israeli society to be masculine or macho, and he responded: 10 11 “Umm, (1) don’t know. One needs to be masculine in, in this country in my opinion. … Because it’s a country, {it’s a country, how can I explain this to you (FS)}? (5) It’s a country that is like, that 239 12 13 14 [you/they/we (MP)] need in the, don’t get offended (FS) I men, courage. … People who aren’t afraid people, (1) who are built so that their hearts are strong {let’s (FS) say like that}. … Because if not {then it’s difficult to live in this country}, with everything that happens. … {Understood (FS)}?”iv Here, again, Dani expresses what appears to be his perception of the normative prescription for the universal Israeli man—courageous, not afraid, and built with a strong heart (lines (L.) 12-13). He presents this as a simple matter of survival in a country such as Israel, which is constantly beset by war and terrorism. Particularly notable here is Dani’s entreaty to me to not “get offended” (L.12) as he launches into his description of the kind of men this country needs. (I am still uncertain as to why he assumed this might offend me.) Finally, and perhaps as an unconscious response to this exchange, I asked Dani how, in his belief, Israeli society relates to someone who doesn’t fit the description he provided or live up to these standards. After a brief pause, he replied: 15 16 17 “The societtyyyy pities him in my opinion. … {Let’s (FS) say like this}, that someone [let’s] say who does not go back to work and suddenly in that7 so the society takes care of him of course, but it is from you (FS) know someone like that it’s, (1) pity already. … {Do you (FS) understand}?” v Herein, I contend, may lie the crux of the dilemma for Dani. He tries to admit that he wasn’t afraid, but ultimately admits that he was afraid, and then apparently attempts to justify his own fear by protesting that everyone would react in the same way. But he appears to strongly believe the exact opposite: that a real Israeli man copes no matter what; deals with dangers by finding solutions and not by falling apart; and is masculine, courageous, and unafraid. In Dani’s own words, a real Israeli man’s heart is strong, and he is built to withstand all the difficulties inherent in living in Israel. In Dani’s mind, anything less deserves pity, and is not worthy of any sense of personal ownership. Indeed, it could be claimed that hypothetically, if Dani had managed to go back to the scene of the main TA and kill the terrorist, or if he had killed him immediately or even prevented the shooting altogether, Dani’s employment of the MS ‘you’ throughout his BLI might have been significantly lower. In this case, Dani might have felt more justified claiming ownership (through ‘I’-statements) of more, if not all, of his experiences immediately before, during, and after the TA. Even if he had been emotionally affected by it, or by the TrA as well, he would likely have shown more discursive evidence of being willing and able to admit this, as he would have already established himself as worthy of sociocultural praise, rather than pity. 8.3.3 Motti: “I don’t think of myself as a hero” Possible support for this hypothesis may lie in Motti’s case. Similar to Yossi and Dani, Motti also was unable to prevent either the TA on the bus next to his or that which occurred on his own bus. However, because he opened the doors and let off some of his passengers (who requested that he do so), he was perceived by the media as a hero, as if he had had a ‘sixth sense’ to behave 7 It seems that Dani is referring here to experiencing post-traumatic stress symptoms after a difficult event such as a TA—as in, “someone who does not go back to work and is suddenly in trauma.” 240 how he did. In the moments directly following the TA and to this day, Motti has been portrayed in the press as “the hero driver,” who saved many people. The following two excerpts demonstrate both Motti’s perceptions of the media’s strong motivation to label him as such, and his fundamental discomfort with the image of himself as a hero, or as having acted in any way heroically: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 “…right then apparently apparently a journalist came at that moment. [He] says to me “You are the driver right?” I told him, the first words that came out of my mouth, I said “What luck that I opened the doors. And peopllee escaped.” Whoever stayed in the vehicle, bit it. I think maybe ten, five, were killed. All the rest were wounded. But the rest escaped, and it is a little comforting, in some way. And then I told him “My God what luck that I opened the doors,” and I said it to him like, that’s what came out of my mouth, don’t know why. He made it a scoop. Liike, the hero driver, whose intuition let [him] open doors. It’s not true. But I said that to the media too. Like it’s not true, it wasn’t my intuition to open doors that maybe there is a bomber also at me. I didn’t think there is a bomber at me. I like to tell th’truth. I am not someone who, tries to, to get some award, I could have come and said, ‘Yes, I opened, I had a feeling,’ that I didn’t say that.”vi 1 2 3 4 5 6 “The press already like, in quotation marks, was searching for the hero driver. Someone told me there, some doctor told me “Listen, [they] made you into a hero, you don’t know what is going on out there, all the press is talking about you.” I didn’t know this, I wasn’t aware of anything. I wasn’t aware of anything. Phone calls, I couldn’t even answer the phone calls. The whole world was calling. Everyone knew that it was me. … So three, four hours, they just asked to interview ‘the big hero,’ and again I say [it] in quotes, I don’t think of myself as a hero.”vii Motti’s candid statements here offer a possible explanation for why he used the first-person singular pronoun more than the other interviewees, as well as why Yossi and Dani did not. While all three drivers engaged in various actions that could have been viewed—both by themselves and by the media—as heroic, it was only Motti who was “made…into a hero.” Indeed, it appears that it was not the actual actions of the individual that dictated his perceptions of his level of worthiness or heroism, but rather, the societal perception that emerged thereafter, with the help of the news and popular media. And while Motti protested repeatedly that the image that was created of him was decidedly not accurate, these ‘facts’ were irrelevant to the press, and the public’s perception of Motti’s behavior had been firmly entrenched before the day of his TA had even ended. Indeed, Motti stated frequently in his BLI that even nearly two years after the TA, he was still getting stopped on the street and recognized in his taxi as “the hero driver.” Motti’s linguistic strategies—primarily his nearly exclusive use of the ‘I’ pronoun— throughout his BLI appeared to reflect his having been bestowed with this powerful label and consequent solid placement into his society’s ‘good graces’ (or at least, his belief that this is the case). While the content of his discourse was filled with protestations about not being a hero, much of the form was reflective of and influenced by an apparent internalization of his society’s acceptance of him. Indeed, Motti easily and readily discussed (in ‘I’-statements) the emotional effects of the TA, and even his visits to a psychologist because of psychosomatic symptoms. Similarly, he appeared comfortable attempting to reject the image of himself as a hero. In addition, it is analytically significant that, while Motti experienced a bus bombing of similar proportions to that experienced by Yossi—and perhaps even more intense, given that Motti 241 both witnessed a bombing on the bus next to his and then experienced a bombing on his own bus— Motti’s TAN contains none of the gruesome and highly descriptive detail that characterized Yossi’s TAN. Motti’s TAN comes across as personal, individualized, and experiential, while Yossi’s TAN seems almost like a script—essentially, his way of telling what someone who had been through a bombing might or should experience or feel. This may be another indication of owning and claiming, as opposed to distancing from or projecting, the difficult experience. Motti appeared able to express his experiences without a need for his audience to experience them with him or vicariously for him, and he was able to discuss a wider range of difficult feelings and experiences, from his own personalized perspective and while claiming them as his own. I contend that all of these characteristics of Motti’s discourse are direct corollaries of his having already been granted membership to the societal ‘in-group’ of heroes. This has allowed him the ‘freedom’ to shift and move within the existential confines of the group—to claim ownership of, organize and create coherence within, and internalize his TAN, with all its varied hues of experience. In this sense, I assert that Motti does not need, and has no motivation, to try to convince me of his heroism (as Dani did); this has already been done for him, and whether it was an accurate portrayal or not is now irrelevant. Motti also has no need to use the FS ‘you’ pronoun (as Yossi did), as he is ‘allowed’ to be emotionally affected. And Motti’s use of the MS ‘you’ is equally low, as he similarly has no reason or motive to universalize his experience or attempt to ‘blend in’ with the rest of his society. From the perspective of Israeli society, Motti occupies an even better (perhaps the best) niche: the courageous, intuitive, quick-thinking and fast-acting hero. 8.3.4 Connections and conclusions across the cases of Yossi, Dani, and Motti Essentially, from the discursive findings of these three cases emerges a pair of acrossparticipant interpretive claims. The first is that Motti was able to (unintentionally) accomplish what Dani seemed desperate to do: convince his society and perhaps himself (via his audience) of his heroism. Second, and as a direct result of this, Motti was also able to do discursively what Yossi was unable to do: claim and fully process as his own all the facets (even those that were difficult, traumatic, and ‘weak’) of the TA experience and its aftereffects. Perhaps, it could be hypothesized, if the TA that Dani experienced had involved a suicide bomber rather than a shooter who required disarming (which Dani did not have the ‘fortune’ of doing), he might have been perceived by the media as Motti was, and then Dani might not have had feelings of guilt about what he could or should have done. And perhaps if Yossi had been able to identify the bomber in the TA he experienced, he also might have been bestowed with the media’s (and society’s) praise. But these hypothetical scenarios did not take place, and Yossi and Dani were not let into the ‘in-group.’ Instead, they were left alone to cope with the effects of their experiences and even, as seen in their 242 discourse, an apparent sense that they had not lived up to the expectations set out by their society. Ultimately, the interpretive results of this study suggest that these interviewees expended significant discursive effort apparently attempting to convince their society that they had reacted, at the very least, in a ‘normal’ way; and at the most, in a powerful and heroic way. In service of this, they seemed to discursively distance and/or project onto the decoder and/or audience any possible sign of weakness (such as admitting fear or traumatic effects). Their use of the ‘I’ pronoun was visible almost exclusively when they were describing an active or heroic behavior on their part, similarly reflecting their attempt to make this ‘case.’ However, the far fewer instances of ‘I’statements in Dani’s and Yossi’s discourse, relative to Motti’s, may reveal that they were not able or willing to fully own what they seemed to view—and/or what they believed their society would view—as non-heroic actions. Thus, I claim, the discourse of these interviewees is both influenced by and reflects their perceptions of their surrounding sociocultural discourse: its expectations of its members; its approval of strength, power, masculinity, ‘courage under fire,’ and heroism; and its disapproval, and perhaps even disowning, of anything else or less. 8.4 ‘VALIDATION’: CONNECTIONS BETWEEN THE HYPOTHESES, THE FINDINGS, AND THE INTERVIEWEES’ OWN WORDS This chapter has presented the findings with regard to the interviewees’ various patterns of pronoun use, in both quantitative and qualitative form. A number of hypotheses have been proffered about the main messages of the discourse of each of the interviewees, and possible explanations for the motivations behind these patterns and messages have also been asserted. Although this is based on deep analyses and rigorous attention to the texts under investigation in this study, much of these contentions are essentially interpretive, likely influenced at least to some degree by my own analytical eye and worldview. This section attempts to make connections between these interpretations and the interviewees’ own words on the relevant subjects. This more direct view on the manifest discourse of Yossi, Dani, and Motti provides a complement and triangulation for the prior analyses that have focused primarily on the latent form of their talk. This final spiraling analysis, therefore, may provide a degree of ‘validation,’ in the interviewees’ own words, for the hypotheses put forth thus far. 8.4.1 Individual coping One of the topics prominent throughout all of the interviews conducted herein surrounded coping and resilience after the TA experience, as well as coping with terror and trauma in general in Israel. The subject surfaced often, sometimes as a result of a direct question posed by me, but more often spontaneously within various stories, discussions, explanations, and argumentations brought up by the interviewees themselves. Below is presented a single quotation each from Yossi, 243 Dani, and Motti, on the topic of coping. Each came at a different point and in response to a different question in the respective interview, but all can be considered representative of the general sentiment on the subject expressed by each interviewee throughout his BLI. In many ways, these three statements on coping, from each participant’s own manifest perspective, can be viewed as a microcosm of many of the findings and hypothesized explanations discussed above. With regard to his own coping, Yossi stated: 1 2 3 4 5 6 “/((slower and in a sad, despondent tone)) After what happened, my coping, it is very simple uhh, (2) I am not making peace with what happened to me. (1) Becauuse it really bothers me, it really, I am keeping a lot inside. Very, uhhhh, when something happens, a terror attack happens, it happens, aand I have no help. I have no hellpp from that perspective of thhee governmental institutions, that [they should] try to locate, to see what, what hurts, what is going on with me {and that}. That [they should] give me help, none./”viii Here, Yossi makes it clear that he has not reconciled with the TA he experienced and its aftereffects. He expresses some emotional difficulties (“it really bothers me,” and, “I am keeping a lot inside,” in L.2-3) and seems most upset by his perception that he has “no help” (L.4, twice). Even when purportedly discussing his personal coping, he quickly descends into an argumentation about what the Israeli governmental institutions should be doing to help him. This was a familiar and oft-repeated topic throughout his BLI, indicating much import to him and consequent distress. This fits, in many ways, with the connections made between Yossi’s pronoun use patterns and his personal experiences, as discussed above. It was contended that Yossi still appears highly preoccupied with the emotional and psychological effects of the TA, and thus may have been most motivated to bring me in to them, with or in place of him—through significant employment of the FS ‘you.’ This may be connected, in part, to Yossi’s struggles with the NII and persistent attempts to garner attention, sympathy, and both physical and financial support in the wake of the TA. In this sense, although it is not possible to determine whether Yossi’s heightened use of the FS ‘you’ is a cause or effect of his inability or lack of willingness to “make peace” with what happened to him, I suggest that, at the very least, there may be a relationship between these two phenomena. Yossi’s admission that he is still upset and internalizing his distress also fits the hypotheses put forth; particularly, that his pronominal patterns indicate a greater degree of psychological distress. Yossi’s choice to emphasize his lack of “help” when talking about his coping also seems significant, and may indicate why he narrates his TA experience in a manner that appears to constitute an attempt to recruit the help of his audience. In this sense, Yossi’s copious use of the FS ‘you’ could be interpreted as his request for my discursive cooperation, collaboration, and physical and emotional support. In the absence of any ‘real’ help from those who he feels should be helping him, perhaps he is seeking any help he can get, from anyone who is willing to listen and offer it. Dani, on the other hand, expresses his thoughts about coping thusly: 1 2 “/((seemingly thinking aloud to himself)) Coped?/ (1) If you (MS) take everything like that in, (1) calmly like, you (MS) are calm and youuu (MS), don’t get too much into a panic stress and all, in my 244 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 opinion that’s the best coping simply, to do your (MS) job {and not to think too much}. In my opinion most of the drivers are like this and there are some who are a bit more stressed out {so uhh}, [it’s] natural that they (MP), let’s (FS) say those, [are] more cow- cowards let’s (FS) say like that so they (MP), if there will be some kind of something so they (MP) let’s say let’s (FS) say wooon’t, (1) won’t function. {You (FS) understand}? There are those who most of them most of the drivers are, how do you say? [They (MP)] take everything iiin proportion, like if it’s there already then {there’s nothing that can be done}. … [They (MP) are] ready for everything. {Understood (FS)}?”ix Here, Dani focuses less on himself and his own coping processes and more on his suggested prescription for ‘best coping’ for his fellow Israelis. Even when I asked him more directly about his own coping, this was the type of response Dani repeatedly offered. Here, he frequently emphasizes that it is best to stay calm, “do your (MS) job,” and not think too much (L.2-3). Dani also points to “most of” the other drivers, who “take everything in proportion” (L.8), and are “ready for everything” (L.9). In Dani’s opinion, this is the way to keep functioning, to keep from becoming “stressed out” (L.4), and to keep from becoming a coward. This supports, in many ways, the connections made between Dani’s pronoun use patterns and his personal experiences, as discussed above. It was contended that Dani may be most preoccupied with his behavior and responses surrounding the TAs and TrA he experienced, and with questions of whether it was ‘acceptable’ to behave in that manner (e.g., to “escape” from the scene of the shooting and take his passengers to safety and medical care, without subduing the shooter; to be afraid during the attack; to almost kill the pedestrian who crossed in front of his bus, even though it wasn’t Dani’s fault). As Dani comes across as highly concerned with whether his actions measured up to those of his fellow ‘correct’ Israelis, it fits that his discussion of coping primarily involves his presentation of a specific list of what he perceives as ‘correct’ behaviors. The implication appears to be that Dani engaged in all of them in his attempt to cope with the aftermath of his difficult experiences. His coping prescription, most often collocated with the MS ‘you,’ can be interpreted as another attempt to align himself with the universal, generic, collective Israeli male ‘you,’ who acts ‘correctly’ and ‘acceptably.’ Although coping is generally viewed as an emotional process, there is no discussion of emotion in Dani’s quotation here. This, too, may validate the finding presented above—that Dani’s low use of the FS ‘you’ can be explained through his repeated denials of any emotional effects of what he experienced. Overall, Dani’s description of, or prescription for, coping seems to align considerably with his definition of the ‘Israeli man’: alert, ready for everything, dealing well with whatever comes up, and strong and courageous no matter what. This is, perhaps, why Dani may be fundamentally unwilling to discuss any emotional difficulties he may have had after any of these stressful events. Because his singular ‘agenda’ seems to be to send the message that he fits this description, Dani may perceive himself as being unable, or not allowed, to admit to even the existence of any weakness at all (projected onto me or not). In this sense, even discussing his own processes of coping may be seen by him as threatening, and therefore, much of his discourse 245 remains in the realm of the MS ‘you,’ prescribing the appropriate way to cope and behave. Finally, and as previously partially presented in Chapter 7, Motti makes the following statements about coping: 1 2 3 4 5 “People, their coping, and again I am telling you (FS), there are those whoo their coping is obvious, and there are those whose coping, and I feel this about myself, that it isn’t obvious at all for me. People see me on the street won’t think that I went through some terror attack, some kind of thing, but on the inside I, I am very weak, I am very weak, I feel about myself that I am very weak. I, it happens to me that I am walking to the side and crying.”x 1 2 3 4 5 6 “The best in my opinion is to get help from- from psychologists, and if you (MS) feel that they are unnecessary so don’t go (MS) to them. Don’t go (MS) liiike to say that I was there. If you (MS) think that they are helping you (MS), so do go (MS). If not, don’t go (MS). If you (MS) think that you (MS) are strong enough to cope alone, don’t take (MS), not pills, not psychologists, not psychiatrists, not anything. Cope (MS) with it alone. In my opinion that’s the best. If you (MS) feel that you (MS) cannot cope alone, so go (MS) to professionals.”xi These two quotations are very different in nature, but represent well the range of Motti’s views on this topic, which came up in many contexts and variations across his BLI. These excerpts, taken together, are both complementary and contradictory in some ways—a characteristic that seemed to reflect a central message of Motti’s discourse: that his life, experiences, and perceptions are not of a black-and-white nature; but rather, they are filled with nuance, uncertainty, and shades of grey. In the first excerpt here, Motti states emphatically and repeatedly that he feels and sees himself as “very weak” (L.4, three times), and even discloses that he weeps at times. These admissions, containing such an open expression of emotion, stand in distinct opposition to anything Yossi and Dani said on the topic at any point in their interviews. In many ways, this fits with the hypotheses put forth above—that Motti may be able to freely discuss a wider range of emotions and difficult experiences, and to do so from the ‘I’-voice, as he has already been identified and distinguished as “the hero driver.” In his perception, perhaps, a hero can be weak and even cry—as long as everyone has already agreed that he is a hero. Yossi and Dani are afforded no such reprieve, as they are still struggling for recognition for their experiences and actions (Yossi, from his government; and Dani, from the surrounding in-group of ‘acceptable’ Israeli men). In the second quotation here, Motti, similar to Dani, presents his prescription for ‘best coping.’ Indeed, some of his comments are nearly identical in form to those of Dani, although the content diverges (Dani: “…in my opinion that’s the best coping simply, to do your (MS) job and not to think too much”; Motti: “The best in my opinion is to get help from psychologists,” and, “Cope (MS) with it alone. In my opinion that’s the best.”). Both Motti and Dani employ the MS ‘you’ as the central subject in these prescriptions, and both seem to be grappling with whether they themselves followed this model ‘correctly.’ From previous statements, it was evident that Motti sees himself as having coped alone, for the most part, and feels alone and isolated in his coping. However, he also made a number of references to treatment (both talk and psychopharmaceutical therapy) he sought out and received after the TA, as well as membership in a support group. It 246 appears, therefore, that Motti may see himself as fitting into both of the categories of ‘best coping’ that he suggested, which may explain why he offered such an apparently contradictory prescription. This all fits, in many ways, with the connections made between Motti’s pronoun use patterns and his personal experiences, as discussed above. It was contended that Motti was able to claim a much wider range of experiences for himself, through the use of the ‘I’ pronoun, because of his ‘hero status.’ And indeed, his open statements professing his perception of himself as weak seem to support this hypothesis. While he employed a similar discursive form technique to that of Dani when describing coping—notably, offering a prescription for the generalized Israeli male ‘you’—the content of Motti’s prescription was quite different. Rather than describing a single, stereotyped, rigid picture of ‘correct’ coping behavior (i.e., be calm, don’t think too much, and do your job), Motti offered a wider range of possibilities (i.e., if you can cope alone, great, and if you can’t, get help). Here, again, Motti’s discourse seems to take into account the various ‘greys,’ and is not preoccupied with a strict and judgmental dialectic between ‘acceptable’ and ‘unacceptable.’ I contend that Motti’s ability and willingness to discuss the varied—and sometimes even ambiguous —dimensions of coping, resilience, heroism, and ‘acceptability’ in the eyes of his society stems from his perception that he has already been placed into the ‘acceptable’ sociocultural category. 8.4.2 Societal pressure, collective coping, and perceived weakness Another topic that appeared in all of the interviews centered on collective and societal coping, alongside Israeli sociocultural discourse on coping and weakness (as viewed through the participants’ eyes). This was another subject that showed a tendency to emerge spontaneously just as often as it was involved in a direct question asked by me. The interviewees sometimes discussed the pressure they felt by their society to be strong and macho and active, while also expressing their beliefs about their society and its coping, from a more collective perspective. These topics are essentially two sides to the same issue, as a society’s ability to cope and specific manners of doing so can likely become its prescription for its members, whether stated outright or not. For this reason, Yossi’s, Dani’s, and Motti’s discussions of their perceptions of characteristics such as strength and weakness was found to be highly illuminating, facilitating a greater understanding of both their self-perceptions and their views of their surrounding society. Below are presented quotations from Yossi, Dani, and Motti, on these topics. Once again, there are similarities and differences among the statements, but I contend that each presents further support for the various hypotheses suggested above. Yossi’s most prominent statement on the topic of societal pressure was: “There is, there is in Israeli society really the, that situation of pressure to be uhh, more, more courageous, more volunteering, more…”xii This admission of the demands he apparently felt surrounding his own 247 behavior before, during, and/or after the TA provides support for the relevant hypotheses put forth. In this sense, whatever actions in which Yossi engaged that did not fit this definition (e.g., not identifying the suicide bomber and not being able to prevent the TA), or opposed it entirely (e.g., being upset or traumatized by the experience) may serve to remind Yossi of his perceived inability to hold up to the expectations of his society. This, coupled with the isolation he feels from his society (in particular, the governmental institutions), is reflected in Yossi’s discourse as less ‘I’statements and more FS ‘you’ subjects, in an attempt to bring me in to his difficult experiences. Dani’s most prominent statement on the subject was: “In this country [they (MP)] love strong strong men not men who start to you (FS) know to, to cry about uhh, their bitter fate. … (2) {So you (MS) say, ‘I will keep it to myself’}.”xiii When I then asked him what his country does to weak men, he responded: “It steps all over them. … (4) {Steps all over them}.”xiv This constitutes another declaration as to Dani’s manifest perception of his surrounding society and its expectations of its members. In Dani’s eyes, a normative and ‘acceptable’ Israeli man must be strong and not cry, and keep anything upsetting to himself. If he does not manage to hold up to this, in Dani’s view of his society’s discourse, he is considered weak and will be trampled underfoot. The apparent rigidity and unforgiving nature of these expectations fits with all of Dani’s other sentiments on this topic, both manifest and latent, and provides further support and a possible explanation for his self-criticism and constant talk about what he should/could/would have done and wanted to do better during both the TAs and TrA. Dani’s view of his society’s strict dialectic between strong and weak, and perceived refusal to sanction anything ‘unacceptable,’ seems to validate the finding of his discursive preoccupation with the MS ‘you.’ Indeed, as the hypothesis suggests, this pronominal pattern may be connected with Dani’s desire and attempts to fit into his society and its ‘acceptable’ in-group, and this is accomplished through universalizing and normalizing his perceived ‘less acceptable’ behaviors and feelings. The more he may be able to convince his audience that his more ‘questionable’ actions (such as being afraid and not returning to the scene to subdue the shooter) still might fit into the definition of ‘acceptable’ (as even the MS ‘you’ would do/feel the same), the more of a chance Dani sees of being a member of this in-group. Motti, once again, provides a distinct contrast to both Yossi and Dani. He did not make any such clear-cut statements on the topic of societal pressure, which is a notable finding in itself. Motti did, however, express his opinions on the related subjects of societal coping and his own coping, which appear connected both to each other and to the above statements made by Yossi and Dani. When I asked how he thinks Israeli society has coped with the security situation over the years, Motti said: 1 2 3 “It has no choice, it has already become exhausted from the situation. … I think so, people already, before when there would be a terror attack, 20, 25 years agoooo, there would be a terror attack, wowww the whole country would jump to its feet. Today there is a terror attack, after a few hours 248 4 [they (MP)] clean up what was there, and that’s it, [you/they/we (MP)] forget.”xv When I then asked him whether he sees this as a sign of good or bad coping, he responded: 5 6 7 8 “It’s a sign of weakness. … That people have become weaker. People don’t care anymore. People uhh, have become more concerned only with their own interests. Like other people don’t interest them anymore. People don’t, dooon’t, don’t look right or left anymore, they look after themselves and that’s it. They dooon’t, don’t pay attention to other things. It seems that way to me.”xvi Here, Motti presents a description of weakness that diverges significantly from that of Dani, but which also differs from his own definition of himself as weak, as presented below: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 “I thought that I am strong and such. But it became clear to me that I on the inside am weak. Before thee thing [terror attack], I wouldn’t make a big deal. I was fine, working, and, I had financial security, and I had, everything was regular, and everything was fine. After the terror attack I found out that I became a weak person. Cry over nonsense. Uhh, (1) a lot, (2) uhh, (1) trembling. Uhh, identify with people. That is to say someone (FS) for example, who her mother passed away at me in the vehicle, that I would see her, now [she] cried on me once, I cried together with her. Like. Not strong. Wouldn’t, I wouldn’t define myself as a strong person, who, who, who can cope with serious things like that. I would more call myself a survivor of things like that, but not strong.”xvii In this final excerpt, Motti essentially provides his own definition of strength, or at least of being ‘normal,’ which involves not making “a big deal” (L.2), being employed and having financial stability (L.2-3), and living a life in which everything is “regular” and “fine” (L.3). Motti’s description of his own weakness here is very similar to Dani’s; i.e., crying and externalizing upset. However, significant here is Motti’s inclusion of “identify[ing] with people” (L.5) in his definition of his weakness—as in the previous quotation, he suggested that his society has become weaker because they are “concerned only with their own interests,” and “look [only] after themselves” (L.6-7, above). Thus, in Motti’s apparent view, being self-centered and uncaring is a weakness in others, but connecting with others and showing emotion with them is a weakness in himself. This provides further evidence of Motti’s possible ambivalence and uncertainty with regard to ‘the right answer.’ And rather than constituting a potential weakness or fault, I contend that this tolerance of ambiguity is perhaps Motti’s greatest strength, one which allows him to view the range of options and not see these issues in such stark and judgmental terms. This ability to see the ‘greys’ may be what helps him to accept his own range of behaviors. Indeed, while he emphasizes here that he “wouldn’t define [him]self as a strong person” (L.7), and repeatedly states that he sees himself as weak, he ultimately ‘corrects’ himself and states: “I would more call myself a survivor…” (L.8). In this sense, it may fit that Motti did not manifestly mention any societal pressure, and does not provide a specific, monolithic prescription for coping or strength or heroism. As he sits comfortably with his ‘hero’ label, he comes across as open to exploring the varied hues of all of these issues, and more ‘ease-fully’ working through his uncertainties and ambiguities regarding his perceptions and feelings about his behavior. 8.4.3 The myth and reality of ‘The Israeli Warrior’ The final direct content comparison discussed here involves a specific question that I asked 249 all of the interviewees8: “Can you define for me (a female, non-native Israeli), what is an Israeli man?” Across all the interviews I conducted for this study, this question was, by far, one of the most illuminating. In just briefly analyzing the varied responses, it quickly became apparent that the interviewees were essentially describing themselves, while being seemingly unconscious of doing so. The following table presents the responses of each of these three participants, and this section analyzes these statements—again, in context with each other and in relation to the main hypotheses of this study. Dani’s full response to this question was already discussed above, so only the main portion of his response is juxtaposed here with the responses of Yossi and Dani. Table 8-9: Comparison of Yossi’s, Dani’s, and Motti’s definitions of “an Israeli man” YOSSI “/((quiet tone)) ((exhaling)) What is an Israeli man? (6) Because we talked, and we said, that we are how did you (FS) say? (1) A melting pot. … (1) And the Israeli man aspires, or draws, (2) his consciousness, his ego, the, his learning, a bit from here a bit from there a bit {from there a bit}, understand (FS)? He lives within that atmosphere. It’s not a mentality let’s say’uhh, let’s (MS) see that is extreme or something like that, for instance Chinese that they are robots and they behave because they were told this is how to behave … Or Americans… (1) Here there is no such thing. Here they learned to be a bit more aggressive, (1), yes? Uhhm, (1) and, he is more, (1) let’s (MS) say, before the, before the wedding or before something like that, he is more authoritative, more liiike ‘I am the rooster.’ … ‘I am like, I am my own, I amm thhee man,’ in parentheses what is a man, I’don’t know, to the, it is like I, I have a higher authority over every{one}. And/ … This is before the wedding that he … Before the wedding that he is more authoritative, more’uh aggressive, more’uh uhh everything gets him hot under the collar, more they (MP) are more irritable more’uh, the Israeli man, annnd, his behavior is more’uh freer. … He can’uh, uh travel abroad on a whim…”xviii Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 Y5 Y6 Y7 Y8 Y9 Y10 Y11 Y12 Y13 Y14 DANI “An Israeli man, it’s a person who deals. A person who, experiences things annd needs to give, needs to deal with them, things that are not nice things, (2) difficult things let’s (FS) say the army and it’uhh peop- people don’t know. (1) A man who, needs to deal with dangers and needs to find solutions for them let’s (FS) say like that. ^Always have to be on alert.^”xix D1 D2 D3 D4 MOTTI “How would I define the Israeli man? (5) Like I know? Don’t know. Everyone with his own characteristics, everyone with his own personality, everyone with th-, with th-, his own wishes, don’t know. Haven’t, I haven’t thought about it, like. Not trying to think about it like that. Really doesn’t, it doesn’t interest me. Like, what does the society think about something. It doesn’t interest me, like. Again, I see everything in the big [picture]. That people with luck, people without luck. … Uhh, what else? … I don’t get too deep into little [things], like what does society think about this, what [it] thinks about that. It doeessn’t, it doesn’t interest me.”xx M1 M2 M3 M4 M5 M6 M7 I assert that, once again, the responses of each of these three interviewees to this single question diverge in ways that mirror the differences reflected in their discourse, and lend support to the hypotheses set forth in this research. In his definition, Yossi refers to the characteristics of aggression, authoritativeness, irritability, and freedom to make one’s own decisions. Significantly, he sets a partition between the Israeli man “before the wedding” (L.Y8 and L.Y11) vs. afterwards, suggesting that beforehand he is more of all of these things. This division into before vs. after seems to parallel the distinction Yossi made often in his BLI, between ‘before the terror attack’ and ‘after the terror attack.’ He 8 All of the participants except for Yossi were presented with this question in both their BLIs and FUIs. Yossi was asked in his FUI only, as the idea for the question emerged only after conducting and initially analyzing his BLI. 250 frequently referred to who and how he was ‘before,’ and stated that he was completely the opposite afterwards. In many ways, therefore, Yossi’s statements describing his idea of the “Israeli man” may send the message that he perceives himself as having fit the definition only before his TA occurred. Since then, he seems to see himself as having become a mere shell of what he was before. And this ‘shell’ does not appear to hold up, in Yossi’s perception, to the hegemonic sociocultural expectations of what an Israeli man is or should be. If this is, indeed, his belief about himself, Yossi appears to have essentially already placed himself—or believes he has been placed—into the ‘unacceptable out-group,’ which may explain his feelings of loneliness and isolation, and that no one is “helping” him. His heightened use of the FS ‘you’ may be interpreted as an attempt to bring me in to that group with him, or perhaps as a reflection of his belief that I, as a (non-native Israeli) woman, already exist there. His descriptions of and statements about weakness, emotion, and the difficult sensory facets of the TA are presented in this discursive realm—perhaps, as an expression of their ‘unacceptability.’ Thus, I contend, Yossi’s definition of the “Israeli man” essentially transmits the message that he perceived himself as having ‘fit the bill’ before having experienced the significant and life-changing TA, but now sees himself as a lesser version of himself, which makes him feel alone, abandoned, and in need of help. In his definition, predictably, Dani provides a list of prescriptive behaviors—among them, always being on alert; dealing with whatever comes, especially difficult things; and finding solutions for difficult and/or dangerous situations. There is no reference to emotion or being affected by any of these challenges, which reflects Dani’s own avoidance of these topics throughout his BLI. This definition is all about action, and appears carefully confined to actions in which Dani has successfully engaged. When juxtaposed with the significant volume of discussions dedicated in his BLI to his actions in the army and during the TAs and TrA he experienced, the similarity and correspondence is significant. Indeed, Dani’s definition of the “Israeli man” reflects, in some places word for word, the image he has attempted to present of himself. All other facets of these difficult experiences, or of being an Israeli man in itself, are disowned, downplayed, or normalized. In this sense, Dani’s heightened use of the MS ‘you’ may be seen as representing an attempt to emphasize the ‘acceptable’ parts of his ‘Israeli man-ness’ and deemphasize or universalize the less ‘acceptable’ or ambivalent facets. Thus, I assert, Dani’s definition of the “Israeli man” essentially sends the message that he perceives himself as ‘acceptable’ only when he has acted ‘acceptably,’ and is far more ambivalent about all other facets of his experiences. When he was afraid, or did not behave heroically ‘enough’ (in his eyes), or could have prevented a tragedy but was unsuccessful, Dani appears more uncertain and ambivalent about whether he belongs in the ‘acceptable’ in-group. It is at these points in his discourse that Dani brings in the MS ‘you,’ in an attempt to universalize and normalize these ‘less acceptable’ actions or lack thereof, so as to bring 251 himself more in line with the definition, or bring the definition more in line with his behaviors. Motti, in characteristic form, once again presents the third, non-black-and-white and more inclusive, perspective. He professes to have no idea how to define the “Israeli man,” and moreover, to have no desire or interest in doing so. In particular, his comment that “everyone [has] his own characteristics, everyone [has] his own personality” (L.M1-M2) shows the significant difference between Motti’s view on this question and the responses of the other two participants. There is no prescription—for either characteristics or behavior—and there are no universal, rigid, or stereotypical beliefs about a single, monolithic type. This may provide a possible explanation for, or reflection of, Motti’s ability to discuss (in ‘I’-statements) a wider range of his behaviors and emotions, and of the various aftereffects of the TA he experienced. Indeed, it seems Motti is able to contain and express a greater degree of complexity, ambivalence, and pluralistic experience in many more facets of his life, perceptions, and attributions. This is apparent throughout his discourse, in both form and content, and on both the macro- and micro-levels. Motti makes an even bolder statement when he expresses that “it doesn’t interest” him to attempt to ascertain “what…the society think[s] about something” (L.M4-M5 and L.M6-M7). His complete rejection of this issue and apparent refusal to grant it importance in his belief system stands out as unusual among the discourse of the entire group of interviewees in this study. It is not possible to determine whether this viewpoint may have emerged as a result of Motti’s having been deemed ‘acceptable’ by his surrounding society (and therefore, no longer in need of the knowledge of what is thought about him), or whether he may have felt this way before the TA he experienced. However, Motti’s repeated references to his having been labeled “the hero driver”—along with his repeated protestations that he did not deserve that label—do seem to indicate that he is aware of, and does have at least minimal interest in, his society’s perception of him. In this sense, Motti’s predominant and heightened use of the ‘I’ pronoun throughout his BLI discourse may be interpreted as representing both his acceptance of his society’s appraisal of him and his behavior, and the effect that this acceptance has had on his own belief systems. Because Motti appears to be so aware and convinced of his membership in the ‘acceptable’ and heroic ingroup, he is able to admit to, and claim ownership of, a much wider range of experiences. Motti is essentially released from the rigid prescriptions presented by Dani, as Motti is perceived as having already fulfilled them. And Motti is less concerned about any isolation that may result from admitting to emotional difficulties, as Yossi has experienced, because Motti is already in the ‘correct’ societal group. Thus, here, again, Motti shows the potential impact of societal discourse on individual discourse. The comparative analyses presented in this chapter have ultimately provided further evidence for and understanding of the interaction between sociocultural discourse, individual discourse, and personal processes of meaning-making and narrative processing. 252 PART 3 Longitudinal Comparative Analyses Herein are presented three separate longitudinal comparative psychosociolinguistic case studies. Chapter 9 returns to the case of Yossi, this time analyzing his material from a longitudinal perspective, while Chapter 10 presents two similar cases, Eli and Shimi, together. Here, Yossi is presented as a demonstration of ‘improvement’ in, or a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’, narrative processing, while Eli and Shimi provide an illustration of unchanged, or static, narrative processing. Both of these chapters begin with a holistic introduction and short summary of the given participant’s demographic background and of the terror attack he experienced, as well as a brief discussion of my impressions and the interview and interactional contexts. Chapter 9 builds on Chapter 5, providing a quantitative analysis of the distribution of pronouns, tenses, and topics in the central analyzed narratives of Yossi’s follow-up interview. This is followed by a qualitative ‘poetic’ analysis of the most central narrative, and then a deep comparative analysis of Yossi’s uses of mechanisms of narrative selection throughout both of his interviews. Chapter 10 provides a condensed version of this analysis in the cases of Eli and Shimi, presenting a comparative discussion of their life narratives and central terror-related narratives. Both quantitative and qualitative comparative analyses are shown for the baseline and follow-up data of both participants, in addition to brief analyses of their predominant uses of mechanisms of narrative selection across both interviews. Conclusions are presented for each participant with regard to the discourse, meanings, and main messages presented in their interviews. CHAPTER 9 YOSSI: FROM “YOU (FS) SWALLOW ALL OF THE ATROCITY” TO “I EXPERIENCED ALL OF THAT TERROR ATTACK” — AN EXEMPLAR OF ‘IMPROVED’ NARRATIVE PROCESSING 9.1 RE-INTRODUCTION TO YOSSI: BACKGROUND AND IMPRESSIONS This chapter returns to Yossi, here from a longitudinal comparative perspective, analyzing both his baseline interview (BLI) and follow-up interview (FUI) together. This section thus provides a brief reminder of the central biographical information from his life history. At the time of the FUI, Yossi was 59 years old and married with three children. As mentioned in Chapter 5, he was born in Iraq and immigrated to Israel with his family at the age of eighteen months. He completed twelve years of schooling and served in the Armored Corps during his compulsory army service. Yossi fought in the Six Day War (1967) and the War of Attrition (1968-70) during his mandatory service, and as a reserve soldier he fought and was wounded in the Yom Kippur War (1973) and the First Lebanon War (1982). Yossi worked as a bus driver in Jerusalem for approximately 35 years without a single break, even continuing to work part-time after his “retirement” at the age of 49. Nineteen months prior to the BLI, at the age of 54, Yossi experienced a suicide bombing on his bus. The terror attack (TA) was extensive, with many casualties and injuries, and Yossi suffered a permanent partial loss of hearing as a result. He was not working at the time of the BLI, and appeared to be suffering from significant post-traumatic and some depressive symptoms. He was adamant at the time about his refusal to go near a bus. The FUI with Yossi was conducted almost exactly three years following the BLI—in two parts, just under two weeks apart, lasting nearly three hours in total. It was decidedly easier to schedule both interviews with him this time, and he seemed pleased to see me again and willing to help (although he mentioned a few times during the interview that he does not enjoy talking about the TA and most often refuses requests to do so, out of his desire to put it behind him and “move on”). The FUI was much easier for me, and seemed easier for him as well—as reflected in a significant reduction in speeches about his struggles with the National Insurance Institute (NII), as well as in the much less performative nature of the terror attack narrative (TAN). Indeed, as will be seen below, both Yossi’s life narrative (LN) and TAN(s) were told in quite different manners in the FUI, relative to the BLI. In the FUI, Yossi also attested to fewer symptoms of depression and PTSD, and an improvement in functioning (most evident in his ability to ride buses again). 9.2 YOSSI’S FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW: WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT Yossi’s FUI was significantly different from his BLI, particularly with regard to his pronoun and tense choices in his LN and TANs. Indeed, perhaps the most prominent difference is that his FUI contained not just a single TAN as in the BLI, but three separate narratives that dealt with the TA—one mini-TAN within the LN, the main TAN provided at my request, and an additional spontaneous mini-TAN as a response to another of my questions. The characteristics and contents of each of these narratives are discussed further below. 253 With regard to the four patterns of discursive choices shown in his BLI, Yossi’s FUI discourse showed three central shifts. First, his use of first-person possessive pronouns (such as, ‘me,’ and, ‘my’) and phrasing decreased, reflecting an apparent move from personalization and possession to a technique more readily described as distanced reporting on what seemed to have become more of a shared experience, in his perception. Second, Yossi’s use of a number of thirdperson ‘others’ (such as, “a bus driver”) seemed to shift toward a greater use of inclusionary phrases (such as, “like everyone else”), reflecting an apparent move from depersonalization and distance toward repeated attempts to be included in—rather than excluded from—universalizing discursive techniques. Finally, Yossi’s extensive use of the second-person feminine-singular pronoun (‘you’ (FS)) in his BLI decreased to nearly nonexistent in the central narratives of the FUI, reflecting an apparent shift from a need to bring his audience in to the described experience, toward an increasing willingness and ability to claim even the distressing facets of the event as his own. This, in turn, led to a significant increase in Yossi’s use of the ‘I’ pronoun, as an apparent reflection of this more pronounced claiming and owning of a wider array of facets and feelings. 9.3 QUANTITATIVE COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS: HOW MUCH AND WHEN YOSSI SAID IT A quantitative comparative analysis of Yossi’s two interviews, with a focus on both form and content, reveals a significant convergence of phenomena from all angles. This section provides a broad quantitative picture of the central analyzed narratives (the LNs and TANs) of both interviews. First, data is presented on the form and content of the LNs and of the spontaneous TANs told within them. Then, the content of the LNs is further investigated, looking at the specific topics that Yossi discussed within them and how much attention he paid to each one. Focus is placed on how he commenced and concluded (or didn’t definitively conclude) each narrative; which narratives or mini-narratives were introduced in particular ways; which were given ‘closure’ within the discourse; and which stood out prominently from other parts of the narratives. Finally, a multidimensional quantitative analysis of Yossi’s uses of pronouns and tenses in all four of his TANs is presented. The two most ‘complete’1 TANs of each interview are then focused upon, from within a view on the hypotheses set forth in this study regarding ‘improvement,’ or a more ‘easeful’ trajectory, in narrative processing from the BLI to the FUI. 9.3.1 Life narratives and spontaneous terror attack narratives within them Yossi’s LNs in both interviews were highly similar to each other, covering almost all of the 1 The term, complete, is used herein not to represent a judgment regarding what should or should not have been told; rather, it describes a narrative that meets three simple criteria: 1) there was a definitive beginning, middle, and end; 2) it was substantially detailed and elaborated; and 3) there were no obvious ‘missing’ facets (as far as I could tell) and thus, no need to request more details or information. This term relates, in particular, to the spontaneous TAN told within Yossi’s BL LN, but not the TAN within his FU LN. It also characterizes the requested TAN in the FUI, as it was the only one of the three TANs therein that met all of these criteria. 254 same topics in almost the exact same order. The main disparity between the two LNs involved the amount of each that was spontaneously dedicated to the topic of the TA, as well as the structure and content of each of these TANs. The table below presents a summary of the basic differences between Yossi’s two LNs, with a particular focus on the spontaneous TANs within each. Table 9-1: Comparative summary of LNs and spontaneous TANs in Yossi’s interviews BASELINE (100%) – 93 lines (100%) – 78 lines 11% 84% 5% No Yes Yes Length of LN (relative to BLI) Length of spontaneous TAN within LN (relative to BLI) Percentage of LN occupied by pre-terror attack period Percentage of LN occupied by terror attack (TAN) Percentage of LN occupied by post-terror attack period ‘Closure’ to LN ‘Standing-out’ of spontaneous TAN within LN ‘Completeness’ of spontaneous TAN within LN FOLLOW-UP 115% – 107 lines 44% – 34 lines 25% 32% 43% Yes No No As evident here, in Yossi’s BLI, only 16% of his entire life story was dedicated to anything other than the TA—but this percentage increased to 68% in the FUI. It could be argued that this increase might be due to the addition of three years to his life (and presumably, life story), and this is likely at least part of the explanation. However, it is particularly significant that, while Yossi increased his telling about his post-TA life by over 800%, he also increased his talk about his pre-TA life by over 220%. This disparity is even more apparent when viewed in graph form, as seen below. Figure 9-1: Distribution of volume dedicated to life periods in Yossi’s LNs2,3 2 3 The bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of lines in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within each interview (that is, all of the percentages above the blue bars (representing the BLI) together equal approximately 100%, as do all of those above the red bars (representing the FUI)). The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of LN life period volume between Yossi’s BLI and FUI. For volume dedicated to the pre-TA period in the BL LN vs. FU LN, the Chi-square probability equals 0.022—thus statistically significant. For volume dedicated to the TA period (TAN) in the BL LN vs. FU LN, the Chi-square probability is less than 0.0001—thus extremely statistically significant. For volume dedicated to the post-TA period in the BL LN vs. FU LN, the Chi-square probability is less than 0.0001—thus extremely statistically significant. 255 What varied most frequently in these two LNs—often significantly—was not the insertion of new subjects or omission of old ones, but rather, the volume of attention paid to each topic, as well as to various periods, experiences, and issues in his life. Indeed, it is apparent, particularly from the graph above, how disproportionate was the amount of narrative ‘space’ dedicated to the TA in Yossi’s BL LN, and how significantly that proportion had shifted in his FU LN. The table below presents a summary of the subjects (generally paraphrased) covered by Yossi in each of his LNs, in order, with the amount of lines (or words) devoted to each. Particularly significant comparative findings are highlighted in orange. Table 9-2: Summary of topics and their lengths (by number of lines) in Yossi’s LNs TOPIC Introduction: School, army service During the army, service in wars Release from army Work after army, became bus driver Got married Service as reserve soldier in multiple wars, war injuries Got married, children Work at Egged and in army industry Spontaneous opening to TAN Description of life (general and specific) as driver before attack – all was good Morning of terror attack, prayers Bus route on day of terror attack Argumentation4 – lack of drivers’ authority, couldn’t have identified terrorist Argumentation – fate, who dies when, every person has a time and place written for his death Mini-narrative about young woman who gave up her seat to elderly man – she died and he survived Rest of ‘complete’ TAN – phenomenological experience of bombing Since the terror attack – lasting effects of trauma Since the terror attack – psychological treatment, battles with the NII Anger at release of suicide bombers Battles with the NII Need treatment for hearing problems, headaches, pain As of today, cannot get onto a bus Support groups and courses, support from family Difficulty coping, many stimuli that remind Don’t want to do anything, don’t care about anything Closer to religion after terror attack All the “attacks” experienced – warnings from God One of kids got married, loves grandchild Stated end of LN BASELINE INTERVIEW 2.25 3.50 1.00 0 1 word 0 0.25 2.75 5.00 11.50 1.00 1.50 0 FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW 1.50 9.50 1 word 1.75 0.25 9.00 1.25 3.50 2.25 2.50 4.25 0.75 8.50 0 7.00 6.25 7.25 52.25 5.00 0 23.00 19.75 4.75 3.00 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 3.00 4.25 3.00 0 0 0 5.25 8.75 4.50 1.00 13.00 3.25 0.75 Both LNs began from the same place in Yossi’s life history, and in both, any information about his birth (day or place), family of origin, and childhood was completely absent. Relative to his BL LN, Yossi spent much more time in his FU LN describing the period of his life spent as a 4 According to Rosenthal (1993, 2005), the term, ‘argumentation,’ which is a category within her thematic field analysis typology, is used to describe a section of text that includes reasoning, theorizing, or a declaration of ideas, and generally shows the narrator’s view(s) of himself and/or the world surrounding him. 256 soldier in the IDF and the wars in which he fought, in both his compulsory service and reserve duty. In the FU LN, Yossi almost completely omitted the lengthy description told in his BL LN of his experiences as a driver before the TA he experienced, but inserted two entirely new argumentations about his perception of a lack of bus drivers’ authority (particularly with regard to identifying and preventing potential TAs). In both LNs, Yossi told the same mini-narrative—with nearly identical lengths—about a young woman on his bus who gave up her seat to an elderly man. However, perhaps the most significant disparity between the two LNs lies in what followed this mini-narrative. In the BLI, it functioned as merely an introductory opening to the ‘complete’ TAN that followed—a lengthy, descriptive, elaborative narrative of Yossi’s full experience of the bombing. In contrast, in the FUI, this mini-narrative functioned essentially as the within-LN TAN itself, and Yossi did not volunteer any additional information in his LN about the TA. Significantly, both TANs were directly followed by a statement by Yossi about his psychological status following and as a result of the TA; but from this point on, the LNs diverged. In the BLI, Yossi immediately launched into a lengthy, politicized argumentation about Israel’s release of suicide bombers, directly followed by another long speech about his battles and frustrations with the NII and his need for treatment from them. The most problematic facet of Yossi’s BL LN was that it had no definitive ending. There was no conclusive point at which he switched to an unrelated topic, or paused, or asked for my next question, or (as is most common in interviews that involve a request for a life story) said, “That’s it.” His discourse simply flowed in and out of other subjects, which may or may not have been related in his mind to his life story. Therefore, I was left having to make my own determination as to the end of Yossi’s BL LN, which was admittedly fraught with dilemma. For the purposes of the initial analysis, I decided to ‘draw the line’ before the long speeches, as they seemed to have a different tone and feel to them. However, as is apparent in the above table, these two topics appeared also in Yossi’s FU LN—to a much lesser degree but definitively within the LN. Indeed, the key difference here is represented by the clear and stated conclusion to the FU LN: Yossi neared the end by talking about his granddaughter and then concluded by saying, “{and} that’s it you (MP) keep on going with the flow, I don’t know what, (1) that is roughly the story more or less generally speaking.”i This is, interpretively speaking, a significant addition, and I assert that it may be evidence of a greater sense of cohesion and coherence in Yossi’s FU LN, as compared to his BL LN. Furthermore, as already illustrated, in the FU LN there was a significant increase in attention paid by Yossi to his life after the TA, related to from within a focus on coping, support groups, greater religiosity, and his relationship with God. This contained the important inclusion of Yossi’s discussion of his child’s marriage and his loving relationship with his granddaughter. Notably, the topics of Yossi’s anger at the government (as viewed through his stated frustration 257 about the release of suicide bombers) and his battles with the NII received in the FU LN only 13% and 10%, respectively, of the space that they occupied in the BL LN. All this combines to present a picture of a fuller life story, and presumably, a fuller life, for Yossi. His perspective seemed to have widened—to include not only more facets of his life unrelated to the TA, but also less of the experience of the TA itself. The micro-level comparison of both the content and form of Yossi’s two LNs seems to reflect his gradual process of working through and processing the TA, and putting it into ‘its place’ within the totality of his life experiences and story. An in-depth comparative analysis of the spontaneous within-LN TANs of Yossi’s two interviews can illustrate this process most clearly. The first noticeable difference exists in how he started them, as seen below in a juxtaposed comparison of the openings of these two TANs. Table 9-3: Spontaneous openings of the within-LN TANs in Yossi’s interviews BASELINE INTERVIEW Y: …I continued working, and uhh, until the incident that happened. (3) And in the incident that happened, iit, do you (FS) want to hear now the story of what happened, or do you (FS) want to ask more questions? {I don’t know exactly 1 what}. A: Umm, it’s up to you. Y: Yeah? A: Whatever is comfortable for you. 2 Y: That’s it. I also worked (1) without taking any breaks. I would leave happy…ii FOLLOW-UP INTERVIEW Y: …And, I continued working as a bus driver. (3) It was good. (2) And, as usual, the tensions with thhe our neighbors with the our Arab neighbors, that every day, we had a new war pop up and another war and another war, (2) \((staccato)) and unfortunately,\ I as a bus driver, (1) there were many terror attacks in Jerusalem, (1) and like every bus driver, that we check (MP) and look (MP) at people whoever gets on (MS) to the bus, and say (MP) to ourselves, ‘It won’t happen to me, it won’t happen to me’, (3), uhh. (2) I had joie de vivre, I would always laugh/joke5, smile at work, (1) at home, with friends, (2)… iii Both of these excerpts begin the same way—with Yossi referring to his work as a bus driver. In the BLI, however, he then spontaneously introduces the topic of the TA, causing an abrupt ‘interruption’ of the LN by then asking me pointedly if I want to hear “the story of what happened” (BL cell (C.) 1). As the interviewer, I did my best to leave this decision to Yossi, at which point he began his description of the ‘good life’ he had as a driver before the TA—which was essentially his introduction to the main part of his (only and ‘complete’) TAN. On the contrary, there is no such abrupt moment in the FU LN; and because of this, the determination of the precise start of the spontaneous TAN therein also ultimately involved an interpretive decision. As apparent in FU C.1, Yossi’s discourse flows rather seamlessly from the topic of his work as a driver, to the political tensions in the region, to wars, and finally to an introduction of the subject of TAs in general in his city and the collective experience of bus drivers during this period. This generalized perspective and ‘we’-voice is particularly notable, as it stands in contrast to the corresponding section of the BL TAN, in which Yossi refers directly and immediately to “the incident that happened,” with no introductory reference to the larger social or 5 This is the same word in Hebrew, and thus, it is not possible to ascertain which of these words in English constitutes the best translation of Yossi’s message here. 258 political context of the time. Finally, the content of both of these TANs converges again, as Yossi shifts to a discussion of his happiness/joie de vivre in his life before the TA—although, as evident in Table 9.2, this section in the BLI is nearly five times longer than the parallel section in the FUI. Thus, there are three central differences vis-à-vis content in the spontaneous TANs within the LNs of Yossi’s two interviews: 1) the BL TAN was ‘complete,’ while the FU TAN was essentially just a short mini-narrative of one facet of the TA experience; 2) the BL TAN ‘stood out’ from the LN and was introduced by Yossi as such, while the FU mini-TAN flowed seamlessly from and back into related topics in the LN; and 3) the BL TAN comprised over four-fifths of the volume of its LN, while the FU mini-TAN occupied only one-third of its LN. 9.3.2 Pronoun and tense use distribution within the terror attack narratives This section presents a quantitative comparative analysis of the form of Yossi’s TANs, particularly his uses of pronouns and tenses. For the purposes herein, it is necessary to note again that while Yossi’s BLI contained only one TAN, there were three separate TANs in his FUI—all of which had different structures and were told as (or within) responses to different interview questions. Below is a brief description of each of the four TANs, along with their corresponding codes, which are utilized throughout the comparative analyses shown further below. Table 9-4: Codes and characteristics of all of Yossi’s TANs CODE BLL / BL TAN INTERVIEW CHARACTERISTICS INITIATION PROMPT / CONTEXT Baseline ‘Complete’ TAN Spontaneous Within the LN FUL Follow-up A few mini-narratives and argumentations Spontaneous Within the LN FUR Follow-up ‘Complete’ TAN Requested FUS Follow-up Mini-narratives as elaboration on specific points of experience Spontaneous Request to tell about the terror attack, from the beginning Question about relationship between army experiences and terror attack, how and why his reactions were different The tables below present quantitatively the distributions and basic data totals of the (primary analytical foci) pronouns and tenses across all four of Yossi’s TANs. Tables 9-5: Pronoun and tense distribution in all of Yossi’s TANs ‘I’ ‘Past’ ‘Present’ ‘Future’ ‘YOU (MS)’ ‘YOU (FS)’ ‘WE’ BLL FUL FUR FUS BLL FUL FUR FUS BLL FUL FUR FUS BLL FUL FUR FUS 56 18 1 13 12 0 51 8 1 12 16 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 40 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 26 11 4 1 0 1 5 0 7 0 0 0 0 0 Total lines of text Total of relevant pronouns / tenses Ratio of pronouns per line BLL 78 120 1.54 FUL 34 33 0.97 FUR 43 67 1.56 259 FUS 50 66 1.32 In order to understand the significant discursive differences among Yossi’s TANs, these numbers must be put into context by viewing their relative percentage tabulations. The following comparative analysis focuses primarily on the spontaneous TAN within the LN of Yossi’s first interview (the BLL), and the requested TAN in his second interview (the FUR), for two reasons: 1) these constituted the only ‘complete’ TANs, and thus, they provide the most similar context and overall content; and 2) although the FUR is slightly over half the length and contains slightly more than half the amount of pronouns (and tenses) than the BLL, the ratio of pronouns per line is essentially identical, thus facilitating the most comparable percentages. It could be argued that the FUL and the FUS should also be included in all comparative analyses. However, as these two TANs are essentially only mini-narratives about specific parts of Yossi’s TA experience, comparing them on the same level and dimensions with the more ‘complete’ TANs would be problematic, at best; and analytically meaningless, at worst. The FUL was discussed above, within the comparison between the LNs and within-LN TANs of both of Yossi’s interviews (Section 9.3.1); and the FUS was included within the above quantitative analyses, and is discussed further below as well. Furthermore, all four of Yossi’s TANs are utilized as material for a comparative analysis of his uses of mechanisms of narrative selection (MNSs), presented further below. Narrowing the analytical focus for the present time, however, the table directly below presents a percentage comparison of Yossi’s uses of (the primary analytical foci) pronouns and tenses within his two ‘complete’ TANs. The use percentages of each pronoun were computed as relative to the total number of these pronouns used within the given TAN, and the use percentages of each tense were computed as relative to the total number of tense-marked verbs within the given TAN. All percentages are relative within the given interview, not between them. Table 9-6: Pronoun and tense distribution percentages in Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs Uses of ‘I’ Uses of MS ‘you’ Uses of FS ‘you’ Uses of ‘we’ Uses of ‘past’ tense (RPa) Uses of ‘present’ tense (PPr) Uses of ‘future’ tense BLL TAN 63% 0% 33% 4% 50% 49% 1% FUR TAN 90% 0% 0% 10% 87% 12% 1% Evident here is a significant disparity between Yossi’s uses of the ‘I’ and the FS ‘you’ pronouns in each interview, and in his uses of the proximate-present (PPr) and remote-past (RPa) tenses. Indeed, it almost appears as if the numbers were exchanged, with Yossi transferring (represented with the arrows in the above table) all the FS ‘you’s in the BLL to ‘I’s in the FUR, and threefourths of his uses of the PPr tense in the BLL to the RPa tense in the FUR. The graph below presents the patterns of collocations of each pronoun with each tense. 260 Figure 9-2: Pronoun-tense collocations in Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs6,7 These data indeed indicate the pattern hypothesized in this research (specifically, in Section 4.4) as reflective of a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing of the TA experience. Yossi’s discourse within the ‘complete’ TAN of his FUI evidenced a significant increase in uses of ‘I’ in the RPa tense (from 47% to 76%); a slight decrease in uses of ‘I’ in the PPr tense (from 15% to 12%); a meaningful increase in uses of ‘we’ in the RPa tense (from 3% to 10%); and, most significantly, a decrease in uses of the FS ‘you’ from 33% to none. With a view on the essential assertions of this study with regard to the extra-linguistic meanings of the uses of the various pronouns and tenses, this graph presents a picture of an individual who, at the time of his first interview, was claiming parts of the TA experience for himself less than half of the time, and spending a third of his TAN attempting to bring his audience in to those parts of the experience with him. By the second interview, this picture had shifted significantly. Yossi was no longer discursively involving me in his ‘complete’ TAN at all, and was now claiming over three-fourths of the experience as his own—in a processed and appropriately distanced manner. There were still some facets of the experience that he narrated in a more proximal and salient nature, but overall, he did so much less than he had in his BL TAN. All of this 6 7 Again, the bars on the graph represent the absolute numbers of pronouns/verbs in each category, and the percentages above each bar are relative within each interview (that is, all of the percentages above the blue bars equal approximately 100%, as do all of those above the red bars). The Chi-square test with Yates correction (with 1 degree of freedom and one-tailed P) was conducted for each association of pronominal uses between Yossi’s BLI and FUI. For uses of the ‘past’-tense ‘I’ in the BLI vs. FUI, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0309—thus statistically significant. For uses of the ‘present’-tense ‘I’ in the BLI vs. FUI, the Chi-square probability equals 0.3871—thus not statistically significant. For uses of the ‘present’tense FS ‘you’ in the BLI vs. FUI, the Chi-square probability is less than 0.0001—thus extremely statistically significant. For uses of the ‘past’-tense ‘we’ in the BLI vs. FUI, the Chi-square probability equals 0.0628—thus not quite statistically significant. 261 comparative quantitative data, according to the central psychosociolinguistic hypotheses of this study, strongly indicate a more ‘ease-fully’-processed narrative of an event that has been put into more proportional perspective within the life of an individual with improved daily functioning. 9.4 YOSSI’S FOLLOW-UP PRONOUN ‘POETRY’: THE ESSENCE OF WHAT HE SAID AND HOW HE SAID IT To further deepen the comparative investigation of Yossi’s communicative strategies and narrative choices in both of his interviews, the analysis will now turn from a quantitative exploration of the form of his central narratives to a qualitative view on the content within the form. The forthcoming sections utilize two distinct qualitative analytical methods: the first focuses again on Yossi’s pronoun and tense use within his two ‘complete’ TANs, while the second delves comparatively into the discursive and narrative decisions made by Yossi throughout all of his TANs as well as his LNs in both interviews. Both analyses are aimed at gaining a comprehensive and triangulated understanding of Yossi’s motivations and manners of telling, and at investigating the link between his outward discourse and inner psychological world. The results of these analyses will then be viewed through a lens focused on Yossi’s current ‘state of affairs,’ with regard to his processing of the TA experience and his level of daily functioning. As was evident in Chapter 5, the pronoun ‘poetry’ for Yossi’s BL TAN included four poems: the RPa and PPr ‘I,’ the FS ‘you,’ and the ‘other.’ It was contended that the primary objective of the ‘others’ poem was to present the story of the ‘everyman,’ both the generic driver and passenger on a generic/universal bus, which existed in Yossi’s pre-TA world and context. This poem provided the counter-balance to the acute narrative of the TA; the before to the other poems’ during and after. In contrast, the primary objective of Yossi’s BL RPa ‘I’ poem seemed to be to present his actions, both before the bombing and after he ‘recovered’ and managed to become an effective helping agent (that is, a ‘hero’), but not during the period of time in between (when he was presumably too affected to act ‘appropriately’). Related to this, the main purpose of Yossi’s BL PPr ‘I’ poem appeared to be to present a minimal explanation of what happened to him and why he was unable to act ‘appropriately’ during the immediate bombing experience and aftermath—the precise period of time ‘missing’ from his BL RPa ‘I’ poem. The concentration of this facet of the experience into the PPr tense reflected a higher degree of emotional charge for him, and its status as more ‘alive’ and ‘close to his heart’ than those facets described in the RPa ‘I’ poem. Finally, the primary hypothesized objective of Yossi’s (wholly PPr) ‘you (FS)’ poem was to present all the traumatic and upsetting content that was almost entirely ‘missing’ from all of the other poems. The same analytical exercise was conducted with the ‘complete’ TAN from Yossi’s FUI, with some significant disparities. First, as aforementioned, there were no uses of the ‘you (FS)’ pronoun in this narrative, so that column was removed. Similarly, there is no significant ‘others’ 262 poem either; but because Yossi’s use of the ‘we’ pronoun (in the RPa tense) increased from 3% in the BL TAN to 10% in the FUR TAN, the ‘we’ poem is included here instead. The following table is thus the ‘poetic’ result of the 43 lines of text of Yossi’s FU ‘complete’ TANiv. Table 9-7: The pronoun ‘poetry’ of Yossi’s follow-up ‘complete’ TAN (FUR)8 ‘I’ POEM (59) ‘PAST’ (RPa) (51) ‘PRESENT’ (PPr) (8) 1 7 I said in the beginning I tend to lay tefillin in the 2 I told morning 3 16 I was late to work I don’t know exactly what [they 4 I was late study] 5 18 I took th’bus I know them usually 6 30 I drove quickly I know [the girl who sat behind] 8 34 I drove quickly I drive every morning on this bus 9 37 I managed to get there I don’t remember it [the 10 I laid quickly tefillin explosion] 11 I finished laying [tefillin] --12 63 I went on [my] way I still isolate myself [at home] 13 66 I took on passengers I am in treatment [therapy] 14 I got to the direction of [hospital] 15 I took on passengers 17 I went out in the opposite direction 20 I continued toward 21 I collected all of the passengers 23 I did not identify [the terrorist] 24 I got to [street] 25 I told her not to move 27 I got up 28 I continued to drive 29 I continued to drive 31 I continued along the trip 32 I saw [the one who let him sit down] 36 I collected passengers 38 I didn’t hear [the explosion] 39 I fainted 40 I flew outside 41 I woke up 42 I woke up 43 I heard cries 44 I [still] didn’t know what happened 45 I was with my face toward the front of the bus 46 I turned around toward [the inside of] the bus 47 I didn’t know that there had been an explosion 48 I looked into the bus 49 I saw things that [are] very very very very very atrocious 50 I heard screams 51 All of me is covered with flesh and blood 52 I hugged her [woman who cried for help] 53 I didn’t know what to tell her 54 I said “Nothing happened” 55 I opened th’door 56 I got really God-given strength 57 I opened th’doors 59 I returned 8 ‘WE’ POEM (7) 19 We drove 22 We went down toward [area] 26 We got to [street] 33 We got to, on [street] 35 We got to [street] --58 We took th’woman down [off the bus] 61 We were afraid that it would burn up The numbers to the left of each entry signify its location in the overall order of 66 pronominal entries. Three dashes (- - -) signify a significant break in the given sequence, during which other pronouns or tenses were used. Italics here signify a repetition in the discourse. 263 60 I continued to search for wounded people I experienced all of that terror attack 64 I laid there [in the hospital] 65 I didn’t give them the goods 62 The first prominent finding is that there are few significant holes in any of the poems, as there are no major clusters to take any meaningful degree of focus away from the RPa ‘I’ poem. From a cursory glance at the numbers, it is evident that the dominant poem here is clearly the RPa ‘I,’ and both the PPr ‘I’ and the ‘we’ poems pale in comparison. While the RPa ‘I’ was also the dominant voice in Yossi’s BL TAN, the presence of significant holes, wherein entire parts of the TA experience were predominantly unmentioned, served to create a feeling that something was ‘missing’—and these facets were often ‘found’ in the poems of the other pronouns or tense. There thus seemed to be a sense of disjointedness and lack of cohesiveness that is not at all present in Yossi’s FUR TAN, as evident below through a separate examination of each of these poems. While the ‘others’ poem in Yossi’s BL TAN dealt primarily with a generic idealized bus context, in the FUR TAN there is no such reference to this context. The ‘we’ poem herein ‘sets the scene’ in a similar manner, but is told in a personalized/collectivized way, wholly in the RPa tense, and exclusively relating to the immediate pre-TA context rather than a generalized or universalized set of circumstances. All but two entries in the ‘we’ poem, in fact, discuss the bus route directly preceding the bombing, and refer to the entire population of passengers on the bus that day. The final two entries, which exist in close succession toward the end of the overall TAN, seem to refer to a more specific “we”—presumably, Yossi and the security officer who together evacuated a wounded woman from the bus and who “were afraid,” also together. This ‘we’ poem, terse as it is, seems to send two distinct messages that did not exist in Yossi’s BL TAN. First, the increase in references to the collective nature of actions such as driving on the bus and arriving at various streets—which were most often referred to in the BL TAN as individual actions performed and experienced only by Yossi—seems to suggest that Yossi has become less focused on his previous perspective on the TA as personally victimizing, and more accepting of the fact that it was actually experienced collectively by all of the individuals involved, including but not limited to himself. Indeed, the fact that there was no significant ‘we’ poem in the BL TAN suggests that a discursive (and likely psychological) shift has taken place in this domain. Second, Yossi’s choice to ally himself discursively with the security officer in the last two entries of the FU ‘we’ poem seems to imply a newfound dimension to his characterizations of his strength and heroism at the time of the bombing. Yossi appears here as no longer only experiencing, feeling, and acting alone; but rather, he now (if only briefly) discursively pairs himself with a symbol of power and security, positioning himself as a member of a team. Although this increase in Yossi’s use of the ‘we’ pronoun overall is seemingly small (from 4% to 10%), I contend that it represents a 264 shift in narrative processing and psychological perspective that is not at all insignificant. Within the PPr ‘I’ poem, nearly all of the entries involve more generalized and timeless statements about Yossi’s life as a bus driver before the TA—as in, “I tend to lay tefillin in the morning” (entry (E.) 7), and, “I know them usually” (E.18)—and are evenly interspersed throughout. Significantly, here remembering the explosion or facets therein is placed by Yossi within the domain of the PPr ‘I’ (E.37), as was the case in his BL TAN as well. Here, also, the final two entries contain a significantly different nature and message from the rest of the poem, as they deal with Yossi’s current ‘state of affairs’ vis-à-vis his post-TA psychological and daily life. The use of the PPr ‘I’ seems discursively necessary, but it is nonetheless significant that Yossi concludes this poem with a statement of where he is presently as far as the effects of his experience. Besides (and, in fact, related to) the entire disappearance of the ‘you (FS)’ poem in Yossi’s FUR TAN, the RPa ‘I’ poem therein shows the most significant transformation, relative to the parallel poem in the BL TAN. Most apparent here, in addition to (and largely because of) the lack of any significant holes or ‘missing’ content, is that essentially the entire narrative is told within this poem—that is, it serves as a microcosm of the FUR TAN as a whole. Yossi begins with his late arrival to work, but managing to get there and have time to pray, then setting off on his route and taking on passengers. In E.28, E.29, and E.31, he repeats himself three times (“I continued to drive,” twice, and, “I continued along the trip”), possibly signifying an impending narrative climax. Almost immediately following this, there is indeed a lengthy and wholly unbroken series of entries (38-57)—beginning with Yossi’s not having heard the explosion and fainting, and continuing through his waking up (repeated twice in E.41-42), hearing cries, and turning around and looking into the bus. Notably, this cluster also includes Yossi’s having seen “things that [are] very very very very very atrocious” (E.49), as well as having “heard screams” (E.50) and being “covered with flesh and blood” (E.51). Then, the poem shifts to Yossi’s (heroic) actions, including getting “God-given strength” (E.56). What is most significant is that many of these statements— essentially all the emotional and traumatic facets of the TA experience—existed in the PPr ‘you (FS)’-voice in Yossi’s BL TAN, and here have been transferred into the RPa ‘I’-voice. Even the most sensory and descriptive imagery of the BL TAN has been brought into the FU RPa ‘I’ poem, causing these parts of the narrative to have thus become more condensed, organized, distanced, and discursively owned. Perhaps the most telling sentence of this poem exists in E.62, wherein Yossi states, “I experienced all of that terror attack.” This appears to be essentially the title for Yossi’s FUR TAN, in which the RPa ‘I’ is not only the dominant pronoun of use, but, in contrast to the BL TAN, encompasses the entirety of Yossi’s TA experience. Indeed, here the RPa ‘I’ is employed not only in reference to active and ‘heroic’ behavior, but also with some of the less pride-inspiring, more 265 distressing, and less ‘acceptable’ aspects of the experience. This seems to represent a significant shift in perception and processing. It is difficult to discern whether this change may exist in Yossi’s own beliefs about the experience and his behavior therein, and/or whether it may involve his attitudes about the larger societal discourse surrounding him. That is, might the decrease in Yossi’s use of the FS ‘you’ in relation to what he apparently viewed as the weak, vulnerable, and traumatic facets of the experience, signify that he no longer perceives these facets as weak and as necessitating the recruitment of the female subject, or that he no longer feels that Israeli society sees them (or him) that way? Regardless of the reasons for the discursive shifts demonstrated by Yossi, these patterns are evident and constitute an outward manifestation of an inner psychological change in Yossi’s processing of his TA experience. Below, further analysis will continue to delve into the narrative markers of Yossi’s psychological attitudes towards his experiences. 9.5 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF MECHANISMS OF NARRATIVE SELECTION: HOW YOSSI CHOSE WHAT TO SAY AND HOW TO SAY IT The following analysis was conducted in order to garner a more comprehensive picture of the similarities and differences between Yossi’s two interviews. Again, here, most of the excerpts come from his LNs and TANs, which are generally representative of the central differences across the interviews at large—particularly given that the FUI contained three separate TANs, all of which are excerpted here in various places. It may be necessary to reiterate that this analysis of the MNSs at work within the central narratives was driven by the six Narrative Guidelines (delineated fully in Section 4.4) hypothesized to determine a trajectory toward more ‘ease-ful’ narrative processing. Essentially, the claim is that ‘improvement’ in Yossi’s processing of the TA experience and ability and willingness to place it into perspective within the entirety of his life story (and life) would reflect itself thusly: 1) More inclusion (less omission and silencing) of non-TA-related content a) More discussion of life before and/or after the TA, or content beyond the TA, within the LN b) More space and attention focused on life before and/or after the TA, or content beyond the TA, within the LN 2) More omission (less inclusion) of TA-related content a) Less discussion of the TA within the LN b) Less space and attention focused on the TA within the LN 3) Less silencing of the ‘I’-voice regarding particularly difficult and upsetting TA-related content a) More ‘I’-statements (preferably in the ‘past’ tense) about sensory experiences of the TA and TA-related traumatic effects (both during and after event) b) More ‘I’-statements about topics such as passivity, weakness, lack of heroism, and/or guilt 4) Less sharpening (more flattening) of TA-related content a) Fewer mini-narratives and shorter narratives overall b) Less emphasis, superlatives, and repetition in form c) Less within-narrative dialogue 266 5) More attributions of meanings consistent with ‘owning’ and processing a) More seamless incorporation of the TAN into the LN, rather than the TAN standing apart from or outside of the LN b) More acceptance of variety of roles and actions before, during, and after the TA c) More willingness and capacity to explore the ‘greys’ of coping and effects of the TA d) Greater focus on the collective nature of the TA 6) Less transferring of difficult, upsetting, or ambivalent TA-related content away from the ‘I’-voice a) More ‘I’-statements (of either ‘past’ or ‘present’ tense): i. instead of ‘present’ tense FS ‘you’-statements ii. instead of ‘present’ tense MS ‘you’-statements b) More ‘past’ tense ‘I’-statements instead of ‘present’ tense ‘I’-statements While some of the analyses in this dissertation presented single sentences or small quotations extracted both from any order in their respective interview and often from their larger context, the excerpts presented below (parts of many of which have already been shown in previous analyses) are unbroken, sequential, and juxtaposed in tables sectioned into cells in order to make comparisons. Great effort was made to ‘cut’ the cells in such a way as to position parallel content from each interview side by side (while still keeping the narratives in their original chronology), in order to distinguish clearly the similarities and differences in both content and form. Below, each of the MNSs are presented, with examples and analysis to demonstrate how Yossi used each one and what can be understood from this usage and its context. The mechanisms are presented in separate sections, although there are often points of convergence and complementarity between them. Indeed, for each set of quotations shown as an exemplar of the main MNS at work, some of the other mechanisms involved may also be discussed as a way to provide further evidence for others of the Narrative Guidelines. Much support for Yossi having met the criteria put forth in Guidelines 1 and 2, as well as Guideline 5(a), has already been provided above, within the quantitative comparative analyses of his LNs and TANs (Section 9.3). In addition, evidence supporting Guidelines 3(a) and 6 was also found in the pronoun ‘poetry’ analysis of Yossi’s FUR TAN (Section 9.4). Overall, however, the six sub-sections below should be read on a holistic level, with an attempt to comparatively analyze Yossi’s texts globally and with all six guidelines in mind throughout. Particularly significant sections are highlighted in orange. 9.5.1 Inclusion The first MNS to be discussed here is inclusion, whereby Yossi chooses to report certain facets of or details about the TA he experienced. This comparative example brings in corresponding, or ‘parallel,’ text sections from the BL TAN and the FUR TAN. Here, Yossi’s use of inclusion within both of his ‘complete’ TANs is evident. 267 Table 9-8: Inclusion within Yossi’s ‘complete’ TANs BASELINE TAN (BLL) 1 I got to the [bus] stop, 2 I collected passengers, I left [the stop] (1) and then there was the explosion. 3 Now I didn’t feel anything. I thought that it’s a truck that ran into me hard. First of all, I didn’t hear the explosion. What happened it’s like I lost my hearing, there was a powerful blast. 5 So already I had lost my hearing, the uhh it, my [ears] exploded. Then uhh, and all of me flew, the whole seat, the whole windshield, it became dark. It became totally dark. I 6 lost consciousness. Then all of me flew onto the front of the bus, like, almost my whole body was outside, like this, hanging outside, I lost consciousness like this, 7 until I woke up, I got up, and, and uhh, 8 4 9 still I don’t know that an explosion happened on the 10 bus. Still totally don’t know that an explosion happened.v FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUR) We got to [street], and then at the [bus] stop… I collected passengers, (1), there was an explosion. (1) There was an enormous explosion, (2) that the truth is I don’t remember it. Because they tell me, the blast came before the noise/boom. (2) Then therre was, (1) an explosion in [my] ears, so I didn’t hear th- (1) {it}. I fainted and flew outside, ninety percent of my body was, bent over, toward the steering wheel. When I woke up, (2) I woke up I heard cries, on the bus, and a smell of burning, (1) and a smell of scorched flesh, (1) and then still I didn’t know what happened…vi There is significant similarity between these two excerpts, particularly in their content. Every chronological ‘step’ of the narratives is nearly the same, in the same order and with most of the same information. The only two cells of the FUR TAN that do not appear to have corresponding content in the BL TAN (“I heard cries…” (C.8), and, “and a smell of burning…” (C.9)) do indeed ‘match up’ with nearly identical statements slightly later in Yossi’s BL TAN. Indeed, Yossi chooses here to include nearly all of the same details, in much of the same language and discursive strategies as well (for instance, “almost my whole body” vs. “ninety percent of my body” in C.6). Still, there are several small but significant differences, particularly in the form of the discourse. First, in C.1 of both interviews, there is a shift in pronoun choice, from ‘I’ to ‘we,’ in the identical micro-context. Second, in C.10 and as discussed previously, Yossi’s references to initially not knowing what happened are made in the PPr tense in the BL TAN, and in the RPa tense in the FUR TAN. Finally, in general there is evidence of more inclusion of details and much more repetition in the BL TAN—as clearly represented in C.5, where Yossi repeats, “I (had) lost my hearing,” twice; in C.6, where he repeats, “all of me flew,” “it became (totally) dark,” and, “I lost consciousness,” twice each; and in C.10, where he repeats “still…don’t know” twice as well. Overall, the juxtaposed comparison of these two sections provides a number of prototypical discursive representations of Yossi’s process of working through the TA he experienced. Longitudinal narrative (and psychological) development appears evident in his discourse about the event, in three significant domains: 1) from, “what happened to me,” to, “what happened to us” (i.e., less personalization and more understanding of the collective nature of the experience); 2) 268 from his experience and reactions existing in the PPr tense (signifying a proximate and salient experience, existing in his ‘here-and-now’), to a discursive shift into the RPa tense (more distant and psychically manageable); and 3) from an apparent inner need to repeat himself and make his account of the event more ‘alive,’ to a more succinct and distanced reporting of what took place. The next comparative example brings in parallel excerpts from the BL TAN, the FUL TAN, and the FUR TAN. Here is demonstrated Yossi’s use of inclusion within only his FU narratives, but twice therein—as he included the exact same elements in both the within-LN mini-TAN and the ‘complete’ TAN (which were told approximately 80 minutes apart). Table 9-9: Inclusion within Yossi’s follow-up TANs BASELINE TAN (BLL) 1 2 Uhh, in the morning I got up, 3 4 and usually we lay tefillin. (2) 5 6 7 I prayed, and uhh, I went to work.vii 8 FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUL) \((Staccato)) That infamous day, (2) I got up in the morning and I was running late almost a little a bit\ to work. (2) I went, quickly to work, and with us [you/they/we (MP)] take th’buses and leave, fast. And usually, I arrive to work early, and lay tefillin, {every day}. (1) That day I didn’t manage [to do so]. (2) {Okay}. I drove quickly, to, [name of hospital where bus route begins] and from there I went out, (1) and in actuality I managed to get there ten minutes early, I laid tefillin, (2) and, I went on [my] way. (1)viii FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUR) On that day of the attack, I was late to work. (2) Uhh, I was late, not by much, because I had a tiny bit of time uhh. I took th’bus, and drove quickly, and usually I tend to lay tefillin in the morning. (2) So I drove quickly toward uhh, [name of hospital where bus route begins] Hospital. I managed to get there, about ten minutes before my departure time. I laid quickly, tefillin, (2), and finished laying, and I went on [my] way,ix First, as opposed to the first example, here is evident a shift from the collective to the personal, as demonstrated in C.4: “usually we lay tefillin,” vs. “usually I…lay tefillin,” and, “usually I tend to lay tefillin.” Because this shift exists within the exact same content and context across both interviews, and because it is uniquely consistent—as viewed in the two separate FU TANs—this can be considered a reliable discursive marker of a shift in Yossi’s processing of and perspective on the event he experienced. The question, then, remains with regard to the motivation for this shift. Most apparent in this comparative table is the inclusion of the entire mini-narrative that takes place in C.3-7 of both of the FU TANs—about Yossi running late to work, almost not having enough time to pray, having to drive quickly to the beginning point of his bus route, and ultimately managing to arrive in time to lay tefillin. Notably, each TAN corresponds with at least one other TAN (and most often, all three ‘match’) at specific points (“[getting] up in the morning,” in BLL and FUL C.2; “usually [laying] tefillin,” in BLL and FUL C.4, and praying/laying tefillin and going to work/on his way, in C.8 across all three TANs). And yet, in both FU TANs, told over four and a 269 half years after the TA he experienced, Yossi chose to include new (and quite intricate) details about the day it happened—details that he did not include in the interview conducted only one and a half years after it occurred. Indeed, if I had only interviewed Yossi once, I would never have known anything about this moment in his life beyond, “Uhh, in the morning I got up, and usually we lay tefillin. (2) I prayed, and uhh, I went to work.” x In this sense, the addition of the FUI brings with it an implication of the imperative to ask, ‘where is the ‘truth’ here?’ Perhaps this new mininarrative in Yossi’s FU TANs is a fabrication? How can one know which narrative to ‘trust’? These questions are thorny, and issues of this nature continue to plague narrative researchers. However, I assert that the unique opportunity provided by the current analytical method and implementation, due to its comparative nature and parallel contexts, may lend support to the contention that questions regarding ‘truth’ and ‘trust’ in a narrative context can and should be laid to rest. Concern, or even interest, need not be placed on ‘just the facts’ or ‘what may/may not have actually happened.’ Rather, interpretive analyses might best be served by asking, ‘why might the narrator have chosen to tell the narrative in one manner the first time, and in another manner the next?’ Indeed, rather than asking whether Yossi was ‘really’ late to work that morning, or when and where he was actually able to pray, the analysis should inquire as to what may have motivated him to leave this mini-narrative out of the BL TAN, while in the FU TANs, he not only included the detail that he was late but also elaborated intricately on the surrounding circumstances. From an interpretive perspective, I assert that the details and/or circumstances contained in this mini-narrative held special significance for Yossi at the time of the FUI, and did not at the BLI. The excerpts above illustrate a particular emphasis on the ‘near-miss’ nature of the praying; that is, that Yossi almost didn’t have enough time to pray, but he did his utmost and ultimately managed. Also apparent is a contrast between what he “usually” does (arriving to work early so as to have time to pray) and what he actually did on this (decidedly unusual) morning. Ultimately, this demonstration of inclusion only in the FUI actually runs counter to the hypothesis that Yossi was in a more ‘ease-ful’ state relative to the BLI, as it suggests that he is more focused on the details of the TA and expending more energy to make at least this section of the TAN more ‘alive.’ However, there may be another potential explanation for this added inclusion. Toward the end of Yossi’s FU LN, he stated: “And by the way when you (FS) uhh experience a terror attack, (1) I about myself, I don’t know [about] others, you (FS) get much closer to religion.”xi He then discussed the “three or four attacks” that he experienced over his life, including his injuries as a soldier. As further explanation of his greater religiosity, he then stated: 1 2 3 4 5 “But somewhere listen (FS) you (FS) say to yourself (FS), uhh, ‘Itt, right or not, why didn’t I get killed there, and why didn’t I get killed there, and why did they (MP) get killed, and why did those who were on the bus right next to me get killed but I wasn’t killed.’ So it means this is how it is. (3) And if I am a believing man, (1) as a believing man [they (MP)] are telling you (MS), (1) ‘Listen (MS) you (MS) have warning one, and warning two, and [they (MP)] are warning you (MS), and 270 6 7 8 9 10 again [they (MP)] are warning you (MS), here you (MS) could have, and again you (MS) could have and here it is again [they (MP)] are forgiving you (MS) and again yes and, so be (MS) careful,’ (4) even though I believe that I am not a saint, but I believe that I am okay. But anyway you (FS), say (MS) ‘Listen (MS), what didn’t, what what what didn’t I do {good}, what?’ (5) And meanwhile [you (MP)] go with the flow, (1) and you (FS) sit, (2) {that’s it}, wait (MS) for the next miracle.”xii Most noteworthy here is not only that Yossi spent a relatively significant amount of time discussing his religious beliefs with regard to why (he believes) he was both warned and spared so many times, but more importantly, that he did so within the discursive confines of his life narrative. Indeed, there were several places throughout both interviews that Yossi discussed this and similar topics, particularly as a response to my own questions about his beliefs in God, fate, and whether “things happen for a reason.” However, it is particularly significant here—and can be interpreted as evidence for his status at the time of the FUI—that Yossi chose to include this lengthy explication of his views on this topic as part of his life story. The significant decrease in attention and prominence paid to the TA within the LN of Yossi’s FUI has already been discussed. And if the analytical focus is trained on the places where Yossi does discuss the TA, it is apparent that he does so primarily as support for his argumentation that every person has a time and a place written for his death. This is most evident in the inclusion of the only real story dedicated to the TA within the LN—the mini-narrative about the young woman who gave up her seat to the elderly man and ultimately was killed, while the man was not. Thus, this newfound increased focus on religiosity and fate vis-à-vis “everyone’s time and place” within his FU LN could be interpreted to indicate that Yossi has become more occupied with these issues than he was at the time of his BLI. In this sense, it is possible that these issues have become more relevant and/or meaningful to him within his life now, which may have motivated the choice to include them in his life story. If this is indeed the case, Yossi may have come to view certain experiences or aspects therein very differently, with a new or changed focus on different details or information. Yossi’s inclusion of ‘new’ details about the morning of the TA— particularly details that clearly relate to his perception of fate and religion, and perhaps the reason for his survival—would make interpretive sense when viewed through this lens (as opposed to a lens focused solely on whether inclusion as an MNS indicates more ‘ease-ful’ processing or general functioning). In the FUI, Yossi may have been more occupied with his religiosity or issues regarding fate—which may, in turn, have caused him to be more ‘tuned in’ to these facets of a variety of his experiences, and particularly of the TA. This focus may have then influenced his consequent process of selecting what to tell and what not to tell, and the narrative reflected this. 9.5.2 Sharpening The second MNS is sharpening, whereby Yossi chooses to give special prominence or place emphasis on certain facets of or details about the TA. Sharpening is generally represented in 271 content by verbal emphasis and the provision of great detail, and in form as repetition of words, greater length of passages, and the telling of mini-narratives to illustrate points viewed as important in the narrator’s estimation. This comparative example brings in parallel text sections from the BL TAN, the FUL TAN, and the FUR TAN. Here is evident Yossi’s use of sharpening in both content and form across three TANs, as well as more specifically in the FUL TAN. Table 9-10: Sharpening in content and form across three of Yossi’s TANs BASELINE TAN (BLL) The one (FS) who sat down next to her, and let the old man, at the [bus] 1 stop that we went up, that it blew up, an old senior. 2 He sat down, 3 FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUL) The elderly man got on, the young, girl got up, let, like every good Israeli (MS), let that elderly man sit down. (2) and she went where? Went to her death. Went into the middle of the 4 bus, exactly standing up, went into the bus, 5 She got up, and went, into the middle of the bus. (2) And, what happened happened. That girl got killed and the elderly man is still alive. Soo it shows that a persson he has th’[his] [time and] place, she was supposed to die and he wasn’t. If she hadn’t gotten up he would have gone in, but she got up {let him, and she went into her thing [death]}. So that’s what I’m saying, and if only we would know exactly what is happening…xiii 6 FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUR) Uhh, the elderly man who got on at me, (2) the girl who sat behind who I know got up and let him sit down, I continued along the trip. And the one who I saw let him sit down. and then there was an 7 explosion…xiv We got to, on [street] to the [bus] stop where there was the explosion. (1)xv Yossi made the discursive choice to include this mini-narrative (aforementioned) about a girl on his bus who gave up her seat to an elderly man, both within his BL TAN and twice in his FUI, in both the within-LN TAN and the ‘complete’ TAN. The interpretive conclusion suggests that this story holds great importance for Yossi, and the possible explanations for such have already been discussed above. Particularly noteworthy, and demonstrable through this juxtaposed presentation, is the significant similarity in Yossi’s manners of telling across all three narratives. For instance, he introduced and concluded the mini-narratives within the two ‘complete’ TANs in nearly the exact same way (C.1 and C.7). It may be useful here to reiterate that it is always possible (and, as some researchers have suggested, inevitable (Mishler, 2004; Bamberg, 2008)) to tell the same story in a different way, even minutes after the first telling. For example, Yossi could have told this mini-narrative after describing the explosion or his being transported to the hospital, or outside the confines of the TAN entirely. But he chose to focus attention on this girl and this elderly man, situated narratively 272 before any of them (including Yossi) knew the implications of their actions—creating a sharpening effect for the message he apparently wanted to send. So the question remains, what might be Yossi’s message, or end point, here? Why might this mini-narrative be so important to him, at both points in time? I assert that Yossi’s statements in FUL C.6 most succinctly present his central sentiments on the topic. Yossi appears preoccupied with issues of fate and the “time and place” for his death, and the reasons for having been ‘saved’ so many times. These seem to be important subjects for him, and they consequently become important subjects within both his LNs and his TANs. It is in this sense that a deep analysis of the MNSs at work within his central narratives can reveal what is central for Yossi himself. Indeed, this analysis has already shown that his choices to include and/or sharpen certain facets of his narratives can provide significant evidence as to what may hold importance for him. The next comparative example brings in parallel excerpts from the BL TAN, the FUR TAN, and the FUS TAN. Here is apparent Yossi’s use of sharpening in form only, across the three TANs. This mini-narrative did not appear in Yossi’s first spontaneous TAN within the LN in the FUI (the FUL TAN)—which contributed to my decision to request a ‘complete’ TAN later in the interview. Table 9-11: Sharpening in form across three of Yossi’s TANs BASELINE TAN (BLL) 1 I came back to the front of the bus a girl was lying there, she also had 2 wounds, all over her too, full of blood. “Driver, help me.” 3 And so I picked her up.xvi FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUR) There was someone (FS) that iit, [she] was wounded beside me That someone (FS) who cried “Driver help me,” cried beside me “Driver, driver help me.” (2) And so I looked down, hugged her, and I am all of me full of blood and flesh. I hugged her, told her “Nothing happened,” what is that nothing happened? I am coming convincing her nothing happened. And she is wounded and my whole bus.xviii I hugged her 4 5 FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUS) and I didn’t know what to tell her very simply I said “Nothing happened.” (2)xvii What stands out most significantly here is, once again, the similarity in content and structure of these three separate mini-narratives. In addition, perhaps the best demonstration of sharpening exists in C.3, where Yossi utilizes dialogue as a discursive technique to make this facet of the story more vivid and ‘alive.’ In all three of these TANs, Yossi uses nearly the exact same phrasing and structures it as having come straight from the girl’s mouth, rather than paraphrasing or describing in a more distanced manner. The only difference, and it may be a significant one, is that Yossi introduces the girl’s action in both of the FU TANs (“who cried,” and, “cried beside me”), while in the BL TAN the dialogue simply appears ‘out of the blue’—which, it could be interpreted, was essentially how Yossi experienced it in the moment. 273 There are several other differences in both content and form among the three excerpts above. First, Yossi’s discourse was much less descriptive in the FU TANs than he was in the BL TAN, particularly about the girl who was lying on the bus floor (“a girl was lying there,” vs. “someone (FS),” in C.2). Second, and by contrast, Yossi’s discourse was decidedly more descriptive about himself in the FUS TAN (“I am all of me full of blood and flesh,” in C.4). Third, there is a significant divergence in verb choice in C.4, as in the BL TAN Yossi “picked her up,” while in the FU TANs (three times, consistently) he “hugged her.” Finally, there is an inclusion of greater detail toward the end of both of the mini-narratives in the FUI only, where Yossi elaborates twice on what he told the girl after taking her into his arms. This was, significantly, information that did not appear at any point in Yossi’s BLI. Ultimately, it seems evident that the sharpening mechanism was utilized here by Yossi to send the message that this particular story was a pivotal part of his experience of the TA—and this perspective has apparently not changed over the three years that passed between the interviews. Indeed, his use of (the same) dialogue in all three of these TANs strongly suggests that this particular part of the narrative/experience has been mostly unchanged by the passing of time. One possible explanation is that Yossi has no interest in changing or further processing this story. Indeed, here he appears as a central character, implored to help someone in distress, acting heroically to save (and, in the FUI, comfort) her. It is also notable that all three of these excerpts consistently employ the ‘I’ pronoun and are told nearly exclusively in the RPa tense—all of which further supports the hypothesis that Yossi is comfortable claiming this facet of the TA. This may be essentially, in Yossi’s eyes, a ‘good part’ of the narrative of the TA experience, and thus, it has been ‘cemented’ into the larger narrative as is, with no motivation or need for further processing. 9.5.3 Omission The third MNS is omission, whereby Yossi chooses not to report certain facets of or details about the TA he experienced. Omission is generally represented in content by a non-reporting of specific themes, and in form as a lack of representation of full parts of the event or experience. This comparative example brings in parallel text sections (or lack thereof) from the BL TAN and the FUR TAN. Notably, there was no corresponding excerpt within the FUL TAN, as the entire theme of the horrific nature of the immediate post-bombing scene was entirely absent from the first (mini-)TAN and from the LN overall—which is, in itself, a significant demonstration of omission. Here is presented Yossi’s use of omission in content within the ‘complete’ TAN of the FUI. 274 Table 9-12: Omission in content within Yossi’s follow-up ‘complete’ TAN 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 BASELINE TAN (BLL) And all of me was, my whole hands are hurting, my body, the thhe my clothes are torn. I was all of me covered in blood, flesh. Tons. A lot. You (FS) don’t know what it is. I don’t wish on anyone to see it. Iit is impossible to describe it, it is something that even in hell is impossible to see a thing like this. (2) And also I still didn’t understand what happened. And then, like in some sort of horror film, I turn around, towards, the inside of the bus. Like, I turn back around, and then, like in some sort of horror film, you (FS) swallow all of the atrocity. It is something that is impossible to define. Difficult, very difficult. Difficult also to endure it. You (FS) look and you (FS) see that your (FS) eyes, it is from, it, it, it (2) the, everything like opens up to you (FS) in horror, in in in, it, you (FS) aren’t afraid, it iit is atrocity, it isn’t, it isn’t fe-, you (FS) are scared, it isn’t fear, it is something that is impossible to define. Because there is no, nothing to be afraid of, but, it is’it is atrocity that is impossible to describe. And you (FS) look (3), and no one gets near the bus. No one. The bus is going up in flames. (3) … … You (FS) see people without heads. Some without arms, halves of bodies. Now you (FS) see all the stuff inside of the bodies. Everything is on the floor. … … You (FS) see mutilated people. You (FS) see hands moving. You (FS) see legs very few, it, it everything is hot. You (FS) see, everyything is throbbing. You (FS), everything everything is moving, everything, is boiling. People are like, the movements of the eyes too, of the hands too, everything is moving. Everything is moving.xx FOLLOW-UP TAN (FUR) I looked into the bus and I saw things, that [are] very very very very very, atrocious. (3) People in alll kinds of positions, in alll kinds of, uhh mutilated states, things that they are still completely alive, hands that are mooving, hearts that are beating, heads that are moving, halves of people, uhh, legs, everything is hanging in all kinds of ways, everything is jumbled together, covered in blood. (2)xix There are a number of significant differences between Yossi’s two ‘complete’ TANs, and this set of quotations shows perhaps the most prominent illustration of Yossi’s narrative process of working through the experience overall. The most apparent contrast lies in the length and scope of the two excerpts, as the first section is a total of nineteen lines while the second is a mere two lines. (Seven lines of the BL TAN—between C.10 and C.12—were removed from the table out of space considerations.) In his BL TAN Yossi chose to express what he experienced in a manner that involved great detail, vividness, and repetition; while in his FUR TAN he tended toward relatively concise and more distanced descriptions. This is most visible in C.5-6, wherein Yossi imparts the same information in both TANs, but in entirely different ways. The FU cell is not only half the length, but also told exclusively in the ‘I’-voice and in the RPa tense—while the parallel BL cells are exclusively in the PPr tense and contain alternating uses of the ‘I’ and FS ‘you’ pronouns. Significantly, up until the final cells of the above table, Yossi essentially compressed all of 275 the vivid description from the BL TAN into just the single sentence in C.5-6 of the FUR TAN. In doing so, Yossi entirely omitted a number of what were apparently key details for him in his BL TAN—e.g., that his “hands [were] hurting,” and, “clothes [were] torn” (C.1); that he was “covered in blood, flesh” (C.2); that the scene was indescribable and indefinable in its horror (C.3, C.7, and C.9); and that no one approached the still-burning bus (C.10). In the FUR TAN, rather than ‘walking’ me through every minute “atroci[ous]” descriptor—but not denying the horrific nature of the scene altogether—Yossi simply offered this single, fairly distanced, report-style sentence. However, it seems that Yossi was not able or willing to completely disregard the ‘facts’ of the scene in which he found himself directly after the bomb exploded on his bus—which may have led him to provide the descriptive discourse at the end of the FUR TAN excerpt above. Indeed, the most notable facet of this comparative example lies in C.11-12, where Yossi’s discourse imparts nearly identical pieces of information (content); but again, in entirely different manners (form). Significantly, there is much of the same choice of words, as in “halves of bodies/people,” “mutilated,” and “hands moving.” But most apparent here is the prominent shift in pronoun and tense use, likely signifying a corresponding shift in Yossi’s sense of psychological proximity to the event described. While these two cells in the BL TAN included a total of seven uses of the FS ‘you’ (all but one collocated with the verb, “see,” in the PPr tense), the corresponding FUR excerpt utilized not a single subject pronoun at all. The BLL cells contain an actor, an FS ‘victim’ who is seeing all of the atrocity—while the FUR cell describes the horror from a distanced, objective viewpoint. While neither of the excerpts involve an ‘I’ subject, this shift from the FS ‘you’ to a distanced, undefined observer is significant. The previous statement in the FUR TAN (“I looked into the bus and I saw things…”) does place the action of seeing into Yossi’s own, RPa-focused experience, which also seems to signify a notable shift in communicative strategy. A closer look at the entire BLL excerpt above (as well as the rest of the surrounding TAN) reveals a complete lack of employment of the ‘I’ pronoun with any sensory verb, such as seeing, looking, or smelling. I contend that this comparative example provides a prototypical illustration of Narrative Guideline 2, showing Yossi’s more ‘ease-ful’ progress in the narrative processing and claiming of (both the negative and positive facets of) his experience of the TA. Furthermore, according to the central assertions of this study vis-à-vis longitudinal narrative processing, I venture a prediction that if Yossi were to be interviewed again, three years following his FUI, this section of his TAN might not only be further condensed, with greater use of omission, but also that there might be an increase in the use of the RPa ‘I’ (perhaps instead of the undefined third-party observer). It is also necessary to reiterate that while the discourse in the first column was provided spontaneously by Yossi as part of the ‘complete’ TAN within his BL LN, none of this content was presented in the spontaneous TAN within his LN in the FUI. This finding, above all else, makes this comparative 276 example perhaps the most significant demonstration of the MNS of omission. 9.5.4 Sil
Similar documents
ben- gurion university of the negev the faculty of humanities and
sides in their pursuit of their respective goals and attitudes that reflect the experience of conflict and which at the same time affect the way we act and react to conflict. [Galtung 2009]. This b...
More information