Backyard Toxicology
Transcription
Backyard Toxicology
Backyard Toxicology Overview of the Course Toxicology is all around us, it’s constantly in the news, and toxins affect us on a day to day basis. In New York City, this is a particularly meaningful subject because one in four New Yorkers lives within a mile of a state Superfund site. In some areas of the Bronx, twenty-five percent of children suffer from asthma as a result of air toxins. These issues are on the minds of high school students, and make toxicology an appealing subject. Backyard Toxicology is a course designed to develop the critical thinking and scientific writing skills necessary to bridge the gap between Regents-level science and college-level science courses. The course was designed based on personal observations from a Regents Chemistry course comprised of Einstein* students. The idea was to create a course that will prepare students for any college-level science course, rather than focusing on a specific subject matter. The course uses an interdisciplinary subject as a way of exposing students to the scientific thought process. The course synthesizes the students’ existing knowledge of biology and chemistry concepts and then integrates these basic concepts with critical reading of research articles and authentic toxicology research. Goals and Objectives The curriculum for Backyard Toxicology is based on the CREATE method developed by Sally Hoskins. The course is designed to make scientific writing and research accessible to students, allowing them to think critically about research. During the course of reading scientific articles, the students are pushed to understand core concepts in Toxicology and make connections between new concepts and even other disciplines of science. Harnessing their new understanding of toxicology research, students then design and conduct their own experiments. Organization of the Course In the first Unit of the course, students read a series of three articles from a single lab, following the course of their experiments from one to the next. By breaking the articles down into individual components, they become accessible to the students. The use of concept mapping and cartooning allows students to form connections between key concepts in the paper and make the complicated scientific methods more accessible. Students then analyze the data collected by the researchers, figure-by-figure and table-by-table, empowering the students to think critically about data and research. They develop their own hypotheses for the published figures and draw their own conclusions about the data before comparing what they have concluded to the real conclusions of the researchers. The students then propose the next experiment. Review panels consisting of groups develop criteria for successful projects and select a project from the proposals of their peers. The students then see the next experiment the researchers actually did and the process repeats itself two more times. The timing of this unit is flexible, and the progression should flow with the class. Articles can be added or removed to adjust for a fast or slow moving class. Unit two is the experimental unit of the course. The students are challenged with the task of transforming their understanding of toxicology into authentic toxicology research. Students are first introduced to the concept of serial dilutions, a topic generally regarded as challenging in college Chemistry and Molecular Biology. Students utilize this new skill in a lab testing the effects of commonly encountered chemicals (nicotine, caffeine and alcohol) on California Blackworms. Finally, students prepare proposals for an experiment, and like the CREATE method, student-comprised panels select the best proposal. Each group then acts as a research team and conducts the experiment selected. There are daily assignments in the course that will allow the instructor to gauge students’ progress. In addition, there are in depth group discussions, facilitated by instructors that challenge the students push the boundaries of their knowledge and empower them to think critically about science. Students are asked to express their ideas in different formats which help build skills that are relevant in today’s scientific community. Course Contents Unit One – CREATE Toxicology Unit Two – Independent Toxicology Research Course Materials Articles • Bozic, J., DiCesare, J., Wells, H., Abramson, C.I. 2007. Ethanol levels in honeybee hemolymph resulting from alcohol ingestion. Alcohol (41): 281-284. • Bozic, J., Abramson, C.I., Bedencic, M. 2006. Reduced ability of ethanol drinkers for social communication in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.). Alcohol (38):179-183. • Mixson, T.A., Abramson, C.I., Bozic, J. 2010. The behavior and social communication of honey bees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.) under the influence of alcohol. Psychological Report 106(3):701-717. Analysis Template Toxicants and Worms Packet Credits Lauren Esposito was a fellow in the CASE GK-12 program from 2008-2010, is a current College NOW fellow and doctoral candidate in Biology at CUNY. Her research, based out of the American Museum of Natural History is on scorpions, an animal notorious for toxins. This course was developed using lesson plans written by Susan Conklin and the Society of Toxicology, and the CREATE methodology developed by Sally G. Hoskins, Leslie M. Stevens, and Ross H. Nehm. Unit One – CREATE Toxicology This unit introduces students to the field of toxicology by exploring a series of three papers investigating the effects of ethanol on honeybees. Teachers are strongly urged to read the article ““But if it’s in the newspaper, doesn’t that mean it’s true?” Developing critical reading and analysis skills by analyzing newspaper science with C.R.E.A.T.E.” by Sally G. Hoskins (American Biology Teacher 72(7): 415–420) before beginning this unit. The methodology created by Dr. Hoskins is the cornerstone of this unit, and teachers should be familiar with it for this course to be successful. Total Time Allotment – 50 hours Goals / Objectives – Students will: • Become familiar with toxicology terminology • Learn how to read scientific literature • Learn how to interpret results and data from scientific studies • Become objective participants in the scientific process Materials – 1. Journal articles (3) 2. Vocabulary notebook 3. Analysis Template 4. Markers, poster paper, white paper 5. Toxicants and Worms Lab Packet, California blackworms, recovery containers for blackworms (petri dishes), disposable Beral pipettes (with tips cut off) or eyedroppers, 4 chambers per group (petri dishes), distilled water, filter paper, toxicant solutions, waste beaker, probes, black permanent marker Activity #1 – Blackworm Lab 1 This is the first day and first lab of the course. It is intended to be an inquiry lab, where the students are immediately immersed into the science of toxicology through an authentic toxicology investigation. This lab looks at the effects of ethanol on California blackworms. Activity #2 – Article 1 Concept Mapping This activity begins as a post-lab exercise from Activity 1. It then transfers what the students have learned about concept mapping to reading scientific articles. Activity #3 – Read This activity continues breaking down Article 1 into accessible components. This activity tackles the results and methods sections in the form of cartoons and captions. Activity #4 - Elucidate the Hypothesis This activity continues breaking down Article 1 into accessible components. Here students use all the interpretive work they have done on the article sections to figure out what the actual hypothesis (or hypotheses) being tested is. Activity #5 – Analyze and Interpret the Data This activity continues to analyze Article 1. Here students look at the data and determine for themselves what is it saying, forming their own conclusions about the research. This step is very enabling. It transforms the article from something that is taken as scientific fact because it has been published, to something that is simply one person’s (or a few people’s) point of view. The students will realize that they can be participators in science rather than being passive observers. Activity #6 – Think of the Next Experiment This activity is a follow up to Article 1. Here students think about what the next step from this research is. They design an experiment that they feel would build on the research from Article 1 to ask a new question or test a different hypothesis. They then form Review Panels and review all of the proposals created by the students. This is similar to how funding agencies and journal reviewers operate in the world science research. The students are tasked with forming criteria and selecting the best proposal. Activity #7 – Blackworm Lab 2 This activity is a follow up to Activity 1. Here students do the next step in the experiments to determine of the effects of toxicants on blackworms. This time after the experiments are concluded the students think of the next experiment and their peers review the proposals, like they did in Activity 6. The concept mapping and research proposal skills are reinforced in an authentic experiment. Activity #8 – Article 2 This activity is a repeat of Activities 2-6 using the second in a series of articles on the effects of ethanol on honeybees, written by a single research group. The goal is to scaffold the skills of CREATE while allowing the students to see the actual progression of scientific research in a laboratory. Activity #9 – Blackworm Lab 3 This activity is a follow up to Activities 1 and 7. Here students go on to the next step in the blackworm experiments, and determine of the effects of caffeine. As in Activity 7, after the experiments are concluded the students think of the next experiment and their peers review the proposals. The concept mapping and research proposal skills are again reinforced in an authentic experiment. Activity #10 – Article 3 This activity is a repeat of Activities 2-6 using the third in a series of articles on the effects of ethanol on honeybees, written by a single research group. The goal is to scaffold the skills of CREATE while allowing the students to see the actual progression of scientific research in a laboratory. Unit Two – Toxicology Research In unit two, the students will take what they have already learned about Toxicology, add some practical lab skills, and combine them to design and conduct an experiment of their own. Total Time Allotment – 30 hours Goals / Objectives – Students will: • Learn to work as a research team • Learn how to perform serial dilutions • Learn how to design and conduct their own experiment • Learn how to present scientific results to their peers Materials • Lab Notebooks • CuSO4, H20, Scale, Graduated cylinder, Test tubes, Pipettes, Stirring rods, Volumetric flask • Materials as needed by students for Independent Projects Activity #11 – CuSO4 Serial Dilutions This activity is an essential skill for toxicological research. The activity is simply intended to serve as a skill builder for the subsequent activity. Students should learn to successfully make serial dilutions of a solution. Activity #12 – Experiment Proposals This activity begins the second phase of the course: CREATE research. Students will propose their own research projects based on the articles they have read and experiments they have performed in class. The class will then form Review Panels and each panel will develop criteria and then select the best project from another group. Activity #3 – Research Projects This activity continues the second phase of the course: CREATE research. Students will conduct the Panel selected experiments they proposed in Activity 12. Before beginning their research, students must each summarize and present an article to their group. They must then write an introduction section to be submitted to the instructor along with their supplies ‘order’ for their experiment. Activity #4 - Reporting Results Students should draw from their knowledge of the articles they read during the CREATE process to create appropriate tables and figures of their results. Conclusions should be drawn from the results just as in the articles with the CREATE method. Each group will then present their projects to the class. Backyard Toxicology Spring, 2008 Meister Hall, Room G03C Mondays and Wednesdays: 4:30- 6:00 PM Instructor: Lauren Esposito E-mail: [email protected] Office Hours: After Class or Scheduled by Appointment Phone: (212) 769-5614 I. Rationale: Toxins are all around us, in the air we breathe and the food we eat. Toxicology, the study of how toxins interact with organisms, involves aspects of physics, biology, chemistry, and geology. During this course you will be introduced to Toxicology through scientific literature. You will then use what you have learned to conduct your own research project and design a scientific experiment. This course will show you how to make the connections between learning about science, thinking about science and doing science. II. Course Aims and Objectives: Aims This course will help you prepare for advanced courses in all disciplines of science by allowing you to start thinking about science as a researcher rather than memorization of terms and concepts. By “thinking like a toxicologist”, you will build skills to help you succeed in any college-level class. Specific Learning Objectives: The goal of this course is to read scientific literature, design a toxicology experiment, conduct the experiment, analyze the results, and present the findings in a scientific format to your peers. By the end of this course, students will: Understand what toxicology is and how it is connected to other science disciplines, themselves and their environment. Learn how to actively read scientific articles and form conclusions from data presented. Learn how to propose research projects based on previous experiments. Learn how to perform serial dilutions of chemicals in a lab using beakers and graduated cylinders. Understand why every step of the scientific process is critical to evaluating the success of scientific experimentation. Learn how to present scientific information so that it can be evaluated and replicated by other scientists. III. Format and Procedures: Group work: Most of the assignments for this class will be done in groups. ALL GROUP MEMBERS MUST BE ABLE TO SHOW EVIDENCE THAT THEY PARTICIPATED IN ASSIGNEMENTS. The work is expected to be evenly divided. In the event that one member of the group is not contributing, she or he will receive a zero for that assignment. Lectures: This course will not include any traditional lectures! Every class requires active participation by every person. Article readings: Three articles will be read during this course. Each requires that you read and analyze each section. Some of this must be done at home, and it is critical that you come to class prepared or you will negatively affect the grades of all your group members. Labs: Labs will be performed in groups throughout this course. Lab Safety rules and procedures are to be followed AT ALL TIMES! The lab will typically begin with a brief discussion of the lab activity by the professor. The group will designate ONE “materials” person to collect the necessary materials. Everyone must participate in every lab, and each person must record notes in their own lab notebook. The group is responsible for cleaning their lab station when the experiment is complete, and the “materials” person must return all the equipment. Notebooks: Vocabulary notebooks should be kept with definitions of unknown terms from the articles. They will be turned in at the end of the semester for a grade, so it is important to make sure they contain entries. Research: For the final experiment, each student must find, read and summarize at least 1 SCIENTIFIC reference to contribute to their group. The experiments will require organization, cooperation and participation of all group members to be successful. A successful project must have all components: a summarized article for each member of the group, an introduction, a materials list, a cartooned methods section, a hypothesis, data tables, results tables and figures, a conclusion, and a list of references. Additionally, each group must design and carry out a scientific experiment. Presentations: This course will end in a presentation of the results of your experiments. Each group will make and present a research poster. Every group member must participate in both the poster and the presentation. The format for the poster will be discussed in class. IV. My Assumptions “The important thing in science is not so much to obtain new facts as to discover new ways of thinking about them.” ~William Lawrence Bragg This class is intended to help you to succeed not only as a toxicologist, but in any field. By integrating things that you learn in class with actual experiments, you apply the things that you learn rather than simply memorizing them. Often, there is a disjunction between things taught in the classroom and their actual uses in the real world. Understanding that the things we learn about are relevant to our lives is an important lesson that brings greater meaning to our studies. V. Course Requirements: 1. Class attendance and participation policy: Every student must come to each class and be ON TIME. In the event you are absent, you must bring a note from a doctor or health professional. Unexcused absences will NOT be tolerated, and you will receive a zero for any assignments or quizzes that day. Please contact me in advance if you need to be absent due to a conflict. Make-up tests must be scheduled IN ADVANCE. Every student is expected to participate in every class. Come prepared, and ready to learn! You are allowed a maximum of three (3) absences for the semester whether they are excused or unexcused. If you miss more than three classes you will be removed from the class and receive a “WU” which is calculated as an “F” in your college GPA. 2. Course readings: Articles will be handed out in class. 3. Assignments: Many of the assignments for this course will be handed in as a group. Everyone is expected to contribute to the group assignment. Each individual must be prepared to show proof of their contribution. You must come to class with all of your assignments, either individual or group, completed and ready to hand in. VI. Grading Procedures: Grades will be based on: 1. Participation 33.3% 2. Major Assignments (Labs, Article assignments) 33.3% 3. Final Experiment and Presentation 33.3% VII. Academic Integrity Each student in this course is expected to abide by the Code of Academic Integrity. This means that any work submitted by a student in this course for academic credit will be the student's own work, and cannot be copied from any other work without the proper citation. Cheating will not be tolerated, any student caught cheating will be referred to the Office of the Vice President for Student Affairs for disciplinary action. VIII. Tentative Course Schedule: Date Topics Materials Assignments 25-Feb Toxicology Concept Map Syllabus HW: Familiarize yourself with the class policies 27-Feb Blackworm Lab 1 Blackworm Lab Packet HW: Finish answering lab questions 3- Mar Concept Mapping Article 1 Introduction HW: Look up term definitions 5-Mar Concept Mapping ctd. Article 1 Introduction HW: Look up term definitions 10-Mar Reading the Results Article 1 Methods & Results sections HW: Look up term definitions 12-Mar Reading the Results ctd. Article 1 Methods & Results sections HW: Look up term definitions, work on table and figure annotations. 17-Mar Elucidate the Hypothesis Article 1 Methods & Results sections HW: None! 19-Mar Analyze and Interpret Data 24-Mar 26-Mar 31-Mar 2-Apr Analyze and Interpret Data ctd. Think of the next Experiment Blackworm Lab 2 Blackworm Lab 2 ctd. Article 1, all sections; CREATE analysis template Article 1, all sections; CREATE analysis template Research Proposals Blackworm Lab Packet, Lab 1 Blackworm Lab Packet, Lab 1 HW: Complete analysis templates HW: Cartoon the next experiment HW: None! HW: Cartoon the next experiment HW: Read Article 2 Introduction 7-Apr Concept Mapping Article 2 Introduction HW: Read Article 2 Results and Methods 9, 14Apr Reading the Results Article 2 Methods & Results sections HW: Look up term definitions, work on cartoons, table and figure legends, table and figure annotations 16-Apr Elucidate the Hypothesis Article 2 Methods and Results Sections HW: Complete CREATE analysis templates 21-Apr Analyze and Interpret Data Article 2- all sections, CREATE analysis templates HW: Read discussion section 23, 28 Apr NO CLASS!! 30-Apr Analyze and Interpret Data Article 2- all sections, CREATE analysis templates HW: Cartoon the next experiment 5-May Think of the Next Experiment Research Proposals HW: None! 7-May Blackworm Lab 3 Blackworm Lab Packet, Lab 1, Lab 2 HW: Cartoon the next experiment 9-May Blackworm Lab 3 ctd. Blackworm Lab Packet, Lab 1, Lab 2 HW: Read Article 3 Introduction 12-May Concept Mapping Article 3 Introduction HW: Read Article 3 Results and Methods 14-May Reading the Results Article 3 Methods & Results sections HW: Look up term definitions, work on cartoons, table and figure legends, table and figure annotations 19-May Elucidate the Hypothesis 21-May Analyze and Interpret Data Article 3 Methods and Results Sections Article 3- all sections, CREATE analysis templates 26-May Think of the Next Experiment Research Proposals HW: None! 28-May Serial Dilutions Lab Materials HW: Think about toxicants in your life 2-June Proposals Labs, Articles HW: Cartoon a research proposal, write hypothesis 4-June Review Panels Research Proposals HW: Find, read, summarize an article relevant to your project. 9-June Research Projects Research Proposals, Article summaries HW: Complete Introduction, lab order, data sheets 11, 16, 18, 23 June Research Projects Research projects materials HW: Make Tables, Figures, write Conclusion, make a list of references, prepare presentation Presentation Research Presentations 25June Spring Break HW: Complete CREATE analysis templates, Read discussion HW: Cartoon the next experiment Adv Physiol Educ 33: 17–20, 2009; doi:10.1152/advan.90184.2008. A Personal View Learning our L.I.M.I.T.S.: less is more in teaching science Sally G. Hoskins1 and Leslie M. Stevens2 1 Department of Biology and the Graduate Center, City College of the City University of New York, New York, New York; and 2Section of Molecular Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Texas, Austin, Texas Submitted 3 September 2008; accepted in final form 21 November 2008 primary literature; undergraduate; nature of science undergraduate cell biology some 25 years ago, the 1-semester lecture course was packed with information. Cell structure, gene expression, membrane proteins–all these hot topics were covered in great detail. We finished the course confident we had learned a lot, or at least committed a great deal of cell biology to memory. But that was then. The past 3 decades have seen an explosion of biological discovery, with methodological breakthroughs allowing the analysis of cellular and genetic mechanisms in unprecedented detail. Twenty-first century biologists have a rapidly deepening understanding of the molecular basis of evolution, development, and disease, and the recent emergence of the field of genomics has set the stage for continued exponential progress in this century. Much of what we teach now was discovered long after we received our PhDs. The semester, however, is still 14 wk long, and while some of the new findings have replaced the content that we learned in college, a fair amount of the “old” knowledge has held up. If we try to cover that material while adding highlights of the past few decades, we find ourselves packing some 40 wk worth of information into a single semester. We hope our readers will agree that teaching, and especially learning, that amount of material is impossible in a single course. Furthermore, an intense focus on transmitting content is completely at odds with the recommendations of science education reform panels, which encourage us to foster scientific thinking in the classroom (12–14, 16). WHEN WE TOOK Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: S. G. Hoskins, Dept. of Biology, City College, City Univ. of New York, Marshak 607, Convent Ave. at 138th St., New York, NY 10031 (e-mail: [email protected]). This situation has serious consequences. Research published a decade ago showed that undergraduates drop out of Biology majors largely due to a sense of being “overwhelmed” by detailed and “boring” content (15), an unfortunate situation that persists today (6). Despite the growth in biomedical research in the United States, the fraction of American undergraduate students who undertake graduate training in biology is declining (10). College biology teaching clearly needs to change (1, 7, 11, 17). But how? Most professors have much more on their minds than science education. Tenure and promotion are largely tied to “productivity”–a measure of grant dollars and published research–while teaching expertise rarely factors into the equation. However, only a tiny proportion of first-time grant applications are funded, requiring time-consuming multiple submissions for virtually every grant awarded. Under these circumstances, why would faculty members change the way they teach, especially now that the Instructor’s Version of the textbook comes with handy PowerPoint slides and test banks? We suggest that by adopting a “less is more” philosophy and limiting the quantity of information they try to impart, physiology faculty members will improve both their own experience in the classroom and their students’ understanding of physiology–the field, not just the facts. We used this philosophy in designing and evaluating a new upper-level biology course for undergraduates focused on journal articles rather than textbooks [see Ref. 8 for details of the approach and assessment data and Ref. 9 for more on adapting the “CREATE” (for Consider, Read, Elucidate the hypotheses, Analyze and interpret the data, and Think of the next Experiment) approach to the classroom]. Primary literature brings the reader inside the 1043-4046/09 $8.00 Copyright © 2009 The American Physiological Society 17 Downloaded from ajpadvan.physiology.org on May 18, 2010 Hoskins SG, Stevens LM. Learning our L.I.M.I.T.S.: less is more in teaching science. Adv Physiol Educ 33: 17–20, 2009; doi:10.1152/advan.90184.2008.—The rapid and accelerating pace of change in physiology and cell biology, along with the easy access to huge amounts of content, have altered the playing field for science students, yet most students are still mainly taught from textbooks. Of necessity, textbooks are usually broad in scope, cover topics much more superficially than do journal articles, and present the scientific process as a linear string of successful experiments, largely ignoring the reality of rejected hypotheses, unanticipated discoveries, or surprising findings that may shift paradigms. We suggest that a more narrow focus on scientific thinking, using a new method for reading a series of journal articles that track the evolution of a single project over a period of years, can more realistically convey the excitement and challenges of research science and perhaps stimulate some students to consider research careers for themselves. Our approach, termed “CREATE” (for Consider, Read, Elucidate hypotheses, Analyze data, and Think of the next Experiment), has proven successful at both demystifying the scientific literature and humanizing science/scientists in undergraduate biology courses (8), and we suggest that it could be profitably expanded to physiology courses. A Personal View 18 LESS IS MORE IN TEACHING SCIENCE making use of their advanced training and reducing the amount of preclass preparation required. In the CREATE classroom, less is more. We initially withhold article titles, abstracts, and discussion sections and provide only the introduction, methods, and results pages for article 1 of the four-article series, challenging students to grapple directly with the data, analyzing and interpreting the findings as if they had been generated in the students’ own laboratories. This intensive encounter with primary literature is the first time many students have been encouraged to delve beyond textbook summaries to the nuts and bolts of scientific discovery. After the first article has been fully analyzed in class, and before seeing what experiments were in fact done next by the team of researchers, each student designs two possible “next experiments” to carry out in the system. The class then compares the student-designed experiments and debates possible research directions for the project in an exercise that models the decision-making process typical of bona fide scientific grant panels. The analysis process repeats with each article in the series. Many faculty members are reluctant to change what they do in the classroom (typically lecture) for fear that taking time for class discussion will result in less content being covered during the semester. We find that in regard to coverage of content, less is plenty in the CREATE classroom. To read and understand Table 1. Summary of CREATE tools CREATE Classroom Tool Using the Tool Encourages Students to: Concept mapping ● ● ● Relate old and new knowledge Define what they do and don’t know about a topic Review to fill gaps in knowledge Cartooning ● ● ● ● Visualize the experiments by representing “what went on in the laboratory” Link specific methods to specific data Triangulate information in methods/figure or table legends/narratives Construct a context for the data Elucidating hypotheses ● Define, in their own words, the question being asked or hypothesis being tested in experiments related to each figure or table Annotating figures ● Actively engage with data Determine the significance of each figure Closely read figure legends and narratives Prepare for in-class analysis of the data’s significance ● ● ● Analyzing data using templates ● ● ● ● Determine the logic of each experiment Define controls and determine their role Relate data presented to results derived Debate the significance of the data, defend their own ideas, and intelligently criticize the authors’ interpretations Designing a followup experiment ● ● ● Recognize research as a neverending process Exercise creativity in experimental design Consider that multiple options exist; science is not necessarily linear and predictable Grant panel exercise ● ● ● ● Consider how research funding decisions are made Use critical analysis to rank student-designed experiments Develop verbal communication abilities by pitching/defending particular experiments Learn to work in small groups and reach a consensus ● ● ● ● See scientists as humans much like themselves, not stereotypes of pop culture Make personal connections to research/researchers Get their own questions answered Recognize the diversity of personalities that can all be “scientists” E-mail interviews of article authors CREATE stands for for Consider, Read, Elucidate hypotheses, Analyze data, and Think of the next Experiment. See Refs. 8 and 9 for additional information on how these tools are used in the CREATE classroom. Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 33 • MARCH 2009 Downloaded from ajpadvan.physiology.org on May 18, 2010 authors’ laboratory by presenting the actual methods used and data generated. Making sense of these data is where much of the excitement of scientific discovery resides. Our CREATE approach brings this excitement to the classroom. We designed CREATE to demystify scientific literature and humanize science, with the goals of improving students’ critical thinking ability, content understanding, attitudes toward science/scientists, and personal interest in the research process. Rather than relying on a textbook, CREATE uses guided analysis of a series of four journal articles, produced sequentially from a single laboratory, to highlight the evolution of a research project over a number of years. Students prepare for the class at home, using an assortment of pedagogical tools (Table 1), first to orient themselves in the topic area and define background subjects for review and then to break down the mass of information in the article, reassemble it into component experiments, and critically interpret the data illustrated. Applying the CREATE pedagogical tools in preparation for the class frees students to spend their classroom time on instructor-led in-depth discussion of the experimental findings and their implications. We find that CREATE students are empowered by rising to the intellectual challenge of deciphering the logic of each article and realizing, often for the first time, that they can “think like scientists.” At the same time, CREATE instructors are able to run the class more like a laboratory meeting than a lecture, A Personal View LESS IS MORE IN TEACHING SCIENCE develop a facility with data-analysis skills rather than focusing on memorizing or engaging only superficially with content that will rapidly age. We evaluate students based on 1) the concept maps, cartoons, figure annotations, and other homework assigned in preparation for class and collected in student notebook/portfolios, 2) participation in class discussion and analysis of experiments, contributions to small-group work, and participation in grant panel debates, and 3) performance on open-book/opennotes exams. As preclass homework, students construct concept maps and cartoon experiments to define for themselves “what went on in the laboratory” (as opposed to “what was found,” i.e., the results presented in the figure), define hypotheses in their own words, and annotate figures. Students also draw conclusions from the data, summarizing their interpretations on template forms of our design. The templates prompt students to take the final step of defining control and experimental cases, determining which panels of figures or lanes on gels, for example, should be compared directly, and coming to their own conclusions about what the data mean. We give two open-book/open-notes exams and twice per semester collect and examine the notebook/portfolios in which students compile their data analyses, homework assignments, and experiments they designed. An example of a typical homework assignment would be asking students to take the data from a table in one of the articles and represent it in graphical form and then interpret the graph that they sketched. Exam grades, notebook grades, and class participation factor equally into the final grades. Keeping up with the notebook/portfolios by applying the CREATE tools to each article is the key to student success, as these at-home activities prepare students to think on their feet as they critically analyze the data in class. Working through the sequential CREATE steps demystifies the process of reading and analyzing an article, helping students achieve fluency in the universal language of data analysis. At the same time, well-prepared students free faculty members from the drudgery of describing every experimental detail, allowing the instructor to coach students in discussion of the significance of the findings, contributing insights and sidebar stories from their own research experiences. Open-book tests reflect the reality that no working biologist we know walks into their laboratory and carries out a series of experiments based exclusively on memorized information without looking at any written material, logging on to a computer, or conversing with anyone. Research science is an open-book activity. The exam questions, often short-essay questions or requests for critical analysis of data, reflect issues previously discussed in class. Successful answers are ones that demonstrate that students have the data-decoding skills and analytical ability that lead to genuine understanding of the article’s findings. A unique feature of the CREATE approach is that it goes beyond data analysis to give students insight into the personalities and motivations of researchers. Late in the semester, students generate an e-mail survey for the articles’ authors, posing questions aimed at providing a behind-the-scenes look at the people behind the papers. Students’ questions range from personal (What made you decide to become a research scientist? How do you deal with rejection of a grant or paper?) to broader issues (Do you have to be a straight-A student to Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 33 • MARCH 2009 Downloaded from ajpadvan.physiology.org on May 18, 2010 virtually any cell biology article, students must review aspects of gene expression, immunology, cell structure, and cell signaling. Thus, students assigned a series of four 20-page articles, rather than ten to fifteen 20-page chapters of a textbook, still integrate a great deal of content as a basis for intelligent discussion of the data. What is different about content coverage in the CREATE class is that it is contextual, directly related to a particular experimental situation. For example, understanding of methodology is reinforced by brief content reviews. A basic method like in situ hybridization with a digoxygenin-labeled probe detected via alkaline phosphatase histochemistry, used in many studies for mRNA localization, can trigger a quick review of probe production and binding (plasmids, RNA synthesis, and nucleotide specificity), bonds (covalent linkage of digoxygenin to a nucleotide and hydrogen bonding of probe and target nucleotides), antibody/antigen recognition (amino acid R groups, protein shape, and hydrogen/van der Waals/ ionic bonds), the basis of specific binding, and enzyme/substrate interactions. Brief, narrowly focused reviews of content allow the instructor to rapidly determine students’ depth of understanding of “the basics” (information they theoretically mastered in prerequisite courses but often don’t fully understand), fill any conceptual gaps (e.g., what, exactly, makes antibodies “specific?”), and then return to the problem at hand: what do we learn from the patterns of gene expression revealed by the experiment illustrated in Fig. X? Some in-class figure analysis focuses on canonical experimental designs seen in many studies. These include timecourse experiments, dose-response curves, controls for antibody specificity, how “n” for a study is determined, and when or how to use statistical methods. As an example, one of our module articles contains a figure showing the characterization of a new bioassay for axonal growth cone collapse in response to a topical application of ephrin. In “old-style” teaching, we might have spent a mere 90 s on the entire figure, simply telling the students what dose was chosen, what time point was selected, and what controls were performed. In the CREATE classroom, in contrast, we challenge the students to dissect each aspect of the experiment and figure out how each of these parameters was determined. We consider why the collapse assay needed to be characterized empirically (i.e., you can’t buy a commercial kit for analysis of a phenomenon you discovered yourself) and how, specifically, the authors designed, performed, and interpreted their dose response, time course, and antibody specificity tests. Taking time over this figure, which is merely a precursor to the bulk of the article’s data, helps students see how novel phenomena are actually approached in laboratory situations. We do not expect students to remember the particulars of this experiment long term; rather, the next time they see a dose-response figure in a different article, we expect they will recognize and understand it. In this way, the CREATE method is constructivist (5), aiming at coaching students in building their own understanding of the approaches taken in a given series of experiments. In the CREATE classroom, more time is spent on activities at higher levels on the Bloom scale (analyzing, debating, and designing) than on the less cognitively challenging lower levels (naming, classifying, and defining) typical of many lectures (3, 4). With the ongoing explosion of information in 20th–21st century biology, we feel it important that students learn approaches that can be adapted to new information as it arises and 19 A Personal View 20 LESS IS MORE IN TEACHING SCIENCE “think like a biologist/physiologist/chemist/geologist.” They have written and rewritten manuscripts, grants, and book chapters in response to criticism from colleagues, critically analyzed numerous articles, and given talks at conferences. Professors using CREATE spend class time posing challenging questions, moderating discussion, and guiding students in clarifying their explanations and defending their ideas. By limiting lecture in favor of active analysis, the CREATE instructor can devote class time to modeling the scholarly thinking characteristic of their field–something all of us already know how to do. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank the National Science Foundation (Grant DUE 0618536) Course, Curriculum, and Laboratory Improvement program for support. We also thank students of the City College of New York Bio 31206 and 355 classes for participation (this study was approved by the City College of New York Institional Review Board). REFERENCES 1. Alberts B. A wakeup call for science faculty. Cell 123: 739 –741, 2005. 2. American Association for the Advancement of Science. Science for all Americans: a Project 2061 Report on Literacy Goals in Science, Mathematics, and Technology. Washington, DC: AAAS, 1989. 3. Anderson LW, Krathwohl DR. (editors). A Taxonomy for Learning, Reaching and Assessing: a Revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives (complete edition). New York: Longman, 2007. 4. Bloom BS, Krathwohl DR. Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: the Classification of Educational Goals, by a Committee of College and University Examiners. Handbook 1: Cognitive Domain. New York: Longman, 1956. 5. Brooks J, Brooks M. The Case for Constructivist Classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, 1993. 6. Cech T, Kennedy D. Doing more for Kate. Science 310: 1741, 2005. 7. Handlesman J, Ebert-May D, Beichner R, Bruns P, Chang A, DeHaan R, Gentile J, Lauffer S, Tighlman S, Wood W. Scientific teaching. Science 304: 521–522, 2004. 8. Hoskins S, Stevens L, Nehm R. Selective use of primary literature transforms the classroom into a virtual laboratory. Genetics 176: 1381– 1389, 2007. 9. Hoskins S. Using a paradigm shift to teach neurobiology and the nature of science–a C.R.E.A.T.E.-based approach. J Undergrad Neurosci Educ 6: A40 –A52, 2008. 10. Kennedy D. Science teaching roundup. Science 317: 17, 2007. 11. Knight J, Wood W. Teaching more by lecturing less. Cell Biol Educ 4: 298 –310, 2005. 12. National Research Council. National Science Education Standards. Washington, DC: National Academies, 1996. 13. National Research Council. Inquiry and the National Science Education Standards: a Guide for Teaching and Learning. Washington, DC: National Academies, 2000. 14. National Research Council. BIO 2010: Transforming Undergraduate Education for Future Research Biologists. Washington, DC: National Academies, 2003. 15. Seymour E, Hewett N. Talking about Leaving: Why Undergraduates Leave the Sciences. Boulder, CO: Westview, 1997. 16. Siebert E, McIntosh S. College Pathways to the Science Education Standards. Arlington, VA: National Science Teachers Association, 2001. 17. Steitz J. Commentary: Bio 2010 –new challenges for science educators. Cell Biol Educ 2: 87–91, 2003. Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 33 • MARCH 2009 Downloaded from ajpadvan.physiology.org on May 18, 2010 become a researcher? What would be your “dream discovery?”). The range of responses received from authors (including graduate students, postdocs, and professors) reveal “scientists” to be a varied group of individuals with diverse attitudes and motivations, much like the students themselves. This aspect of CREATE humanizes science, helping to dispel students’ preconceptions of scientists as antisocial geeks and of research as an activity open only to geniuses. In addition, the scientists who responded to our survey seemed to appreciate this “outreach” opportunity to share their experiences with our students. Like the “use what you have” home decorating shows, where the style maven, without spending a dime, reconfigures your living room using furniture you already own, our approach allows Biology faculty members to capitalize on skills they already have but may only rarely bring to the undergraduate classroom. Biology professors know how to design research studies, evaluate scientific findings, and run laboratory meetings. The CREATE class takes advantage of these abilities, running as an active discussion in which methods are deciphered, results presented, and interpretations debated. Because students prepare on their own for class, the CREATE professor need not review every basic issue. Instead, the professor is freed to model “thinking like a scientist”– using sophisticated logic and data analysis skills developed over years of study– during every class. Faculty members using CREATE shift the challenge of learning to the students, who construct their own understanding as they work through the steps of the process. Our approach aligns well with recommendations that students must take charge of their own learning (5, 16) as well as with the call for science teaching to focus on the research process (2, 7, 12–14, 16). CREATE faculty members do not “describe biology” through lecture but instead establish a classroom environment within which students discover biology for themselves. Students decode the biological research process through their own efforts, as they work to critically analyze, interpret, and understand data. Despite the relatively narrow content focus, students reported that they reviewed “all the biology and cell biology I ever learned” during the semester, said the CREATE approach helped them with scientific reading in general, and developed more positive attitudes about research/researchers (see Ref. 8 for student interview excerpts). We suggest that the CREATE approach should be applicable to all areas of biology and encourage physiology faculty members to consider a parallel approach: limiting content, reading related articles in sequence, withholding summaries in favor of close analysis in class, and guiding students in getting their own questions answered through e-mail interviews of authors. Abandoning the lecture format may initially feel like stepping off of a cliff, but there are practical as well as cognitive advantages to emphasizing the analytical approaches typical of one’s subject in the undergraduate classroom. Professors already know the logic of their discipline and how to Copyright Ó 2007 by the Genetics Society of America DOI: 10.1534/genetics.107.071183 Genetics Education Innovations in Teaching and Learning Genetics Edited by Patricia J. Pukkila Selective Use of the Primary Literature Transforms the Classroom Into a Virtual Laboratory Sally G. Hoskins,*,1 Leslie M. Stevens† and Ross H. Nehm*,§ *Biology Department and The Graduate Center, The City College of the City University of New York, New York, New York 10031, † Section of Molecular Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712 and §School of Education, The City College of the City University of New York, New York, New York 10031 Manuscript received January 22, 2007 Accepted for publication April 25, 2007 ABSTRACT CREATE (consider, read, elucidate hypotheses, analyze and interpret the data, and think of the next experiment) is a new method for teaching science and the nature of science through primary literature. CREATE uses a unique combination of novel pedagogical tools to guide undergraduates through analysis of journal articles, highlighting the evolution of scientific ideas by focusing on a module of four articles from the same laboratory. Students become fluent in the universal language of data analysis as they decipher the figures, interpret the findings, and propose and defend further experiments to test their own hypotheses about the system under study. At the end of the course students gain insight into the individual experiences of article authors by reading authors’ responses to an e-mail questionnaire generated by CREATE students. Assessment data indicate that CREATE students gain in ability to read and critically analyze scientific data, as well as in their understanding of, and interest in, research and researchers. The CREATE approach demystifies the process of reading a scientific article and at the same time humanizes scientists. The positive response of students to this method suggests that it could make a significant contribution to retaining undergraduates as science majors. D ESPITE the stunning success of research science in the last half of the 20th century, there is a general consensus that the teaching of science to college students has not made parallel gains (Chickering and Gamson 1987; Felder 1987; American Association for the Advancement of Science 1989; Seymour and Hewett 1997; Glenn Commission 2000; McCray et al. 2003; National Research Council 2003; Handlesman et al. 2004; Alberts 2005; Cech and Kennedy 2005). Indeed, the vast increase in scientific knowledge has potentially contributed to this problem, because instructors feel compelled to teach their students an evergrowing body of facts, and students spend more time honing their memorization skills than they do learning how to understand and evaluate scientific data. The 1 Corresponding author: Biology Department, The City College of New York, Marshak Hall 607, 138th St. and Convent Ave., New York, NY 10031. E-mail: [email protected] Genetics 176: 1381–1389 ( July 2007) sense of discovery felt by the scientists involved in generating this new information is unfortunately rarely communicated to undergraduates. Textbooks, for example, typically present the growth of scientific knowledge as a gradual increase of information over time, ignoring the blind alleys, digressions, and unexpected findings that in fact characterize research science. Although laboratory courses are often proposed as a complement to lecture classes that rely on textbooks, students in lab classes too often test hypotheses developed by others, perform experiments for which the results are known, and fail to become intellectually invested in their results. Many undergraduate science majors do not have the opportunity to carry out individual laboratory research projects; even for those that do, the short-term nature of most such projects makes it difficult for students to visualize how their work fits into the overall scientific progress of the laboratory. As a consequence, many undergraduates have little sense of how scientific 1382 S. G. Hoskins, L. M. Stevens and R. H. Nehm knowledge is generated, how research projects progress over time, or of how scientists think about and actually do research. These factors often combine to induce disappointed students to drop out of science majors (Seymour and Hewett 1997; Alberts 2005; Cech and Kennedy 2005), a problem that is exacerbated for minority students, who remain underrepresented at all levels of academic science (National Science Foundation 2002; American Council on Education 2003; Bok 2003, Atwell 2004; total enrollment by gender/race/ ethnicity is at http://www.aamc.org/data/facts/2003/ 2003school.html). As one approach to addressing these problems, we have developed CREATE (consider, read, elucidate hypotheses, analyze and interpret data, and think of the next experiment), a teaching method that involves students in reading and analyzing the primary scientific literature while simultaneously exposing them to the intellectual excitement and challenges experienced by the scientists who carried out the work under discussion. In contrast to other approaches that use single or partial journal articles in the undergraduate classroom ( Janick-Buckner 1997; Herman 1999; Muench 2000; Chowe and Drennan 2001; Klemm 2002; Herreid 2004), CREATE focuses on a sequence of articles that reports a single line of research from one laboratory as it developed over a period of years. In addition to promoting the development of skills that students need to understand and analyze scientific information, the CREATE approach introduces students to issues regarding the nature of science (Lederman 1992; Schwartz et al. 2004) and to the creative roles played by individuals in scientific research. CREATE is not meant to substitute for standard lecture classes and hands-on research projects, but rather to supplement and complement such classes. Consistent with the recommendations of recent reform documents (American Association for the Advancement of Science 1989; Bransford et al. 1999; Glenn Commmission 2000; National Research Council 1999, 2000, 2003), CREATE involves in-depth study of a single line of scientific research, which takes advantage of the narrative nature of science (Muench 2000; Kitchen et al. 2003). A CREATE module consists of four articles, published in sequence from the same lab, that are read and analyzed sequentially, providing insight into the evolution of ideas as a project develops over time. As outlined below, CREATE employs a unique combination of pedagogical tools and active classroom approaches that facilitate learning (Bransford et al. 1999; Siebert and McIntosh 2001; Chin et al. 2002; Zohar and Nemet 2002; Osborne et al. 2004). We had two overall goals. The first was to develop each student’s ability to think like a scientist in terms of designing experiments, analyzing and interpreting data, and critically evaluating results as well as proposed follow-up experiments. Our second goal was to increase the stu- dents’ interest in science and scientific research by providing them with insights into the experiences, both intellectual and personal, of working scientists. We tested the ability of CREATE to meet these goals in an elective course for juniors and seniors that required Genetics and Cell Biology as prerequisites. The 3-credit CREATE class met twice weekly for 75 min/class with a single instructor (S. G. Hoskins), and the class size ranged from 12 to 25 students in the three separate classes (51 students overall) that are discussed in this report. We focused on a module of four articles from the laboratory of Christine Holt (Cambridge, UK) (Nakagawa et al. 2000; Mann et al. 2002, 2003; Williams et al. 2003) that analyze the role of ephrin/eph-mediated signal transduction in axon guidance during optic nerve development. Our assessments indicate improvements both in the ability of CREATE students to think scientifically and in their confidence in their abilities. Importantly, CREATE students also developed a new appreciation for science and for scientists as individuals. THE CREATE METHOD In our previous experience, when students were assigned to read research articles, they often read only the abstract, introduction, and discussion, merely glanced at the figures and tables, and accepted the authors’ conclusions without developing a thorough understanding of the experimental results on which they were based. To avoid this problem, we do not initially provide CREATE students with the articles’ titles, abstracts, discussion/ conclusion sections, or the authors’ names. Using the specific exercises outlined below, we challenge the students to understand the methods, explain the experimental designs, and interpret the data as if they had made these findings themselves. Class discussion focuses on figure-by-figure data analysis and interpretation, with the professor acting as ‘‘lab head’’ and discussion leader, guiding students through evaluation of experiments and in synthesis and application of scientific concepts— higher-level cognitive activities known to facilitate understanding (Bloom et al. 1956). Mini-lectures of 10–15 min are occasionally used to review essential background material, but most class time is spent in whole-class or small-group discussion. After analyzing each article, the students generate their own proposals for what the next experiment would be if they were carrying out the research themselves. They then discuss and debate their ideas with other students in an exercise meant to model the peer review that real science undergoes. As the CREATE process repeats with each module article, students experience how an actual research project develops over time. To enhance the students’ understanding of the personal experience of scientists carrying out research, students communicate with some of the article authors by e-mail, in which they pose their own questions Genetics Education about researchers’ motivations and experiences. Sequential steps of the CREATE process are summarized below: Consider: We explain to the students that, as they read each article, our goal is for them to work through the data as if they had generated it themselves. To facilitate this, they are given each section (Introduction, Results and Methods, Discussion) sequentially and they are not provided with the title and abstract of the article nor with the names of the authors. Although some students may try to circumvent this process by using the Internet to obtain the complete article prematurely, we did not find this to be a problem in our CREATE classes. Even if students do ‘‘look ahead,’’ it does not significantly interfere with their learning experience because most of the CREATE activities require the students to think for themselves. The students are introduced to the principles of concept mapping (Good et al. 1990; Novak 1990, 2003; Allen and Tanner 2003). They are then assigned to read the Introduction section of the first article and to construct a concept map of it by defining key terms and creating appropriate diagrammatic linkages between them. Such maps highlight the range of issues that the article addresses and alert students to concepts that they need to review in preparation for reading and analyzing the article. This exercise empowers the students to take charge of their own learning (Novak and Gowin 1984; Brooks and Brooks 1993). Read: Students read the Methods and Results sections of the article. Then they are instructed to go through the Results section figure by figure and, using the information in the Methods section, ‘‘work backwards’’ from the data presented in each figure (or table) to determine how the results were obtained, that is, what experiment was performed. Students (1) diagram each experiment in a cartoon format that illustrates the methods used, (2) annotate the figures by adding clarifying labels, and (3) write their own descriptive titles for each cartoon and each figure. We emphasize that the cartoons are meant to depict what was physically done in each experiment (see Figure 1 for an example of a CREATE student’s cartoon), not to show what the results were or to restate what the authors said about the experiment. We require the students to draw a sketch for this step, rather than a flow chart. We find that creating a visual representation of what was done in each experiment is critical for the students’ ability to interpret the resulting data. In the annotation step, the students use the information from the figure legend to instructively label each panel in the figure. They note which panels serve as controls and which are experimental and also categorize the type of experiment depicted, e.g., ‘‘dose-response histogram.’’ To carry out this step, students must look closely at the figures and their legends to determine exactly what is represented in each panel. Finally, writing their own titles for the figures as well as their cartoons gives the students a sense 1383 Figure 1.—Sample student cartoon. To link the information given in the Methods sections with the data represented in figures and tables of the Results, students make sketches to illustrate the experiments that were performed. The student cartoon above illustrates an experiment in which mouse L cells were transfected with frog ephrinB2 and then frog retinal explants were challenged to extend axons on membrane carpets made from the L cells. Creating these visual representations facilitates understanding of the hands-on lab work that led to the results reported. of ownership of the material and can help them to distill the essential information. For example, one student rewrote ‘‘Ephrin-B overexpression at the chiasm induces precocious ipsilateral projections in the early tadpole’’ as ‘‘Early Ephrin-B drives axons ipsilaterally.’’ Each of these activities promotes the development of conceptual linkages between what was actually done in each experiment and the data that were obtained. These methods encourage visualization and abstraction as well as integrative and synthetic thinking, all of which facilitate learning (Bloom et al. 1956; Kozma and Russell 1997; Foertsch 2000, Zull 2002; Yuretich 2004). These steps—the cartooning, annotation, and retitling—are done by students as homework in preparation for class. Thus, students arrive in class familiar with the article and ready to participate actively in class discussion. Elucidate the hypotheses: Research articles typically involve numerous individual experiments, each of which plays a role in the final conclusions. Introductions to articles, however, tend to emphasize one major finding, and the Materials and Methods sections often describe the methods without linking them to individual figures or tables. The students triangulate between their cartoons, annotated figures, and rewritten figure/table titles to dissect the ‘‘anatomy’’ of the study by identifying each individual experiment and defining the specific hypothesis that it tested or the question that it addressed. The student-generated hypotheses or questions are written above the figure or table to which they apply. 1384 S. G. Hoskins, L. M. Stevens and R. H. Nehm Analyze and interpret the data: Students analyze each figure using CREATE analysis templates (supplemental Figure S1 at http://www.genetics.org/supplemental/), which build on the work done in the previous steps and guide them in determining which panels in a figure (or numbers in a table) should be compared directly. As they fill in the templates, students compare the control and experimental panels that they identified during figure annotation, relate the results to the hypothesis or question that the experiment addresses, and begin to draw conclusions. Students also explicitly relate the findings to the hypotheses previously elucidated, judge how convincing they find the data to be, and note any questions that they would like to ask the authors. Templates filled out as homework prepare students for active discussion of the outcomes of experiments. Templates are always used for article 1 of the module. Some students continue to use them for subsequent articles while others are able to generate their own analyses after their initial experience with the templates. Class discussion of the articles focuses on data analysis, and the instructor runs the discussion much like a lab meeting. Some analysis is done in small groups, with students charged to work together and then to report their conclusions back to the class. When all of the figures have been analyzed and thoroughly discussed in class, students record their overall interpretations and conclusions as a list of bulleted points—points that they think would be worth including in a Discussion section. Only after completing their own lists are students provided with the actual Discussion section of the article. After reading it, they make a similar list of points based on the authors’ conclusions. Comparing the two lists highlights the role of interpretation in science, showing that data may be interpreted from several different or even opposing viewpoints (Germann and Aram 1996). Finally, students make a summary concept map, this time using the articles’ figures and tables as central concepts and creating linkages between them that indicate the logical flow of ideas in the article. After the intense and detailed analysis of individual experiments, this is an opportunity for the students to step back and weave the individual parts of the article into a ‘‘big picture.’’ Think of the next experiment: Each student imagines that he or she is an author of the article just analyzed and asks: What experiments should be done next? The students diagram two of their proposed experiments in cartoons that are discussed in class. To model the scientific peer-review process, the class collaboratively devises criteria for judging proposals and then divides into several three- or four-person ‘‘grant panels,’’ each of which selects one of the student experiments to ‘‘fund.’’ Often, different groups choose different ‘‘best’’ experiments. Such an outcome contrasts with some students’ preexisting views of scientific research as a linear path with one obvious step after another. Grant panel discussions help students hone data interpretation and verbal logic skills (Vanzee and Minstrell 1997; Marbach-Ad and Sokolove 2000; Zohar and Nemet 2002) and foster an understanding of how science works by modeling the discussions and debates that are characteristic of research laboratories (Steitz 2003) and actual grant panels. Final steps and reiteration of the CREATE process: After the CREATE methods are applied to the first article, the process is repeated with each additional module article, although in these cases there is the added excitement of discovering whether the experiments reported in the subsequent articles match any of the students’ proposed experiments. For students who independently had the same idea as the authors, the experience reinforces the idea that they are learning to think like scientists. For students who have different ‘‘next experiments,’’ the experience underscores the idea that real projects can move in many different directions. This realization contrasts with some students’ previously held beliefs that science is very predictable and that scientists always know what their results will be (Table 1 and supplemental Table S1 at http://www.genetics.org/supplemental/). Analysis of the subsequent articles generally proceeds more rapidly because the students are now familiar with the experimental system as well as with the CREATE tools. Interviews with scientist–authors: At the conclusion of the module, our first class of CREATE students prepared a survey of 12 questions (supplemental Table S2 at http://www.genetics.org/supplemental/) that was e-mailed to each author of the four articles, a group that included technicians, graduate students, postdoctoral fellows, and principal investigators. One author visited the class and was interviewed directly in a session that was videotaped. Subsequent CREATE student cohorts read the e-mail interviews and viewed the videotape generated by the first class; thus, authors were contacted only once. CREATE students’ questions ranged from scientific (‘‘How did you choose your research area?’’) and ethical concerns (‘‘Have you ever encountered any ethical issues and how were they resolved?’’) to more personal issues (‘‘Did you ever wake up and just want to give up? How did you deal with it?’’). The range of responses from 10 different authors (50% response rate) to the same questions highlighted for students that scientists are individuals with different motivations and goals. Especially important for our students was the realization that their previous stereotypes of scientists as ‘‘antisocial’’ and as ‘‘geniuses’’ were inaccurate (Table 1 and supplemental Table S1 at http://www.genetics.org/ supplemental/), which evoked comments such as: ‘‘I realized ½for the first time that scientists are people like me. . . . if I wanted to, if I worked at it . . . I could become a scientist’’ (supplemental Table S1 at http://www.genetics. org/supplemental/). Genetics Education Assessment: Many studies that describe methods for engaging undergraduate students with the primary scientific literature have been published (see, for example, Janick-Buckner 1997; Herman 1999; Muench 2000; Chowe and Drennan 2001; Mangurian et al. 2001; Klemm 2002; Herreid 2004). We did not directly compare the CREATE approach with these other methods because we did not design CREATE solely as a method for reading the primary literature. Instead, the CREATE approach uses a linked sequence of articles as a portal into the research laboratory such that the students experience many of the cognitive activities that scientists use in their daily work. CREATE students also had the opportunity to learn about the personal experiences of the scientists involved in the work. Our goal was to achieve a synergy between the intellectual and personal aspects of research science that would enhance students’ interest in science as well as their abilities to read and understand scientific literature. For these reasons, we chose to use pre- and post-course testing, an established approach in science education, to determine whether the students made gains in these specific areas (Edwards and Fraser 1983; McMillan 1987; RuizPrimo and Shavelson 1996; Stoddart et al. 2000; Bissell and Lemons 2006; Bok 2006). To determine whether there were improvements in the students’ ability to critically read and interpret data, we administered critical thinking tests (CTTs; adapted from http://www.flaguide.org/) pre- and post-course. CTT questions required the use of general data analysis skills and were not specific to the CREATE module. To determine whether the CREATE approach facilitated the ability of students to understand and integrate concepts related to the module content, we carried out pre- and post-course assessments in which students constructed concept maps based on seed terms (Novak 2003). (Note that these assessment maps were distinct from previously described concept maps used as learning tools in the CREATE classroom.) Finally, to explore students’ understanding of the nature of science and their attitudes toward science and scientists, we used oral interviews (Glaser and Strauss 1967; Novak 1998; Ary et al. 2002) and an online, anonymous SelfAssessed Learning Gains survey (http://www.wcer.wisc. edu/salgains/instructor/). The latter two assessments also provided information on the students’ own perceptions of how their critical thinking and data analysis skills had changed and gave us feedback on students’ reactions to the course format. CREATE, in all three implementations, was demonstrated to improve students’ critical thinking skills (Figure 2) and their ability to read/analyze scientific literature and understand complex content (Figure 2 and supplemental Figures S2 and S3 at http://www. genetics.org/supplemental/). Students taught using the CREATE method self-reported increased confidence in their reading and analysis abilities, as well as enhanced 1385 Figure 2.—Summary of results on CTT. Students who took CREATE classes demonstrated gains in their ability to critically analyze data and draw logical conclusions. CTTs requiring data interpretation were administered pre- and postcourse. The CTT was a 30-min closed-book activity, designed on the basis of the Field-tested Learning Assessment Guide (http://www.flaguide.org/), in which students read and responded to six ‘‘story problems’’ requiring them to interpret data presented in charts or tables and explain why they did or did not agree with the conclusions stated in the problem. This test was not based on material covered in the CREATE modules. Horizontal bars with asterisks indicate questions on which significant increases in numbers of logical justifications (statements) or decreases in numbers of illogical justifications were seen post-course, compared to pre-course, in written responses to CTT questions (***P , 0.001; **P , 0.02; *P , 0.05; paired t-test). These data suggest that students’ critical thinking and data analysis abilities improved during the CREATE semester. For each question, we included only data for students who answered the same question in both pre- and post-tests; thus, the N is smaller for questions 4, 5, and 6. Error bars indicate standard error. Questions 1–3, N ¼ 48; question 4, N ¼ 42; question 5, N ¼ 34; question 6, N ¼ 24. Additional information regarding the CTT appears in the supplemental data at http://www.genetics.org/supplemental/. skills that transferred from the CREATE class to other science classes (supplemental Figure S4 at http:// www.genetics.org/supplemental/). They also exhibited improved understanding of the nature of science, increased interest in science participation, enhanced personal engagement with science, and more positive views of science and scientists (Table 1; supplemental Figure S4 and supplemental Table S1 at http://www. genetics.org/supplemental/). Thus, CREATE students experienced gains in both their academic skills and their perception of the scientific enterprise. I expected: they had a theory, they proved that theory; that’s it. ½Now I see it’s more like a blind person placed in a room and trying to feel around as to what they think a structure may be; and even when they think a structure is ‘‘that’’ they’re still not sure as to what it is, but they can kind of come up with what it is, based on the shape, and touching it, and comparing it to another structure that’s next to it. . . .(S4) I walked away with skills that are going to help me in every single class I take again, and even in life, really. I feel like I can take on my own taxes this year! ½laughs Just being able to sit down and focus and not get bogged down. (S6) Before ½the CREATE class I used to read the entire paper and then go to the pictures. Now. . .it’s figures first, then text. . . . I’m more of a detective now. I could, like, pinpoint certain words and look it up. . . . I could find the main part of the figure. And I wasn’t able to do that before, I would just read, to read. . . . I would only get it ½the overall point of the experiment or article after I read the whole paper, and probably went through it with like, a professor or something, or in class? But now I think I’m good on my own. I could decipher what the message is, on my own. (S2) Class 1 (continued ) As far as research, I learned that one answer can lead to so many different things, and every person has their own ideas about where the ideas will lead. And I thought that was like the coolest thing—you know, you could have, like six different groups doing the same research, and get the same result; then go in different directions. And I thought that was interesting, because I ½had always thought everybody would go in the same direction. (S3) I thought ½before from lab to lab they had to buy each others’ things; like if I needed a knockout mouse I would have to buy it? But it actually turned out to be a give and take, like ‘‘my stuff is your stuff’’—I didn’t know that. Another thing is. . .a lot of revision goes into these papers. . . . which shows you that it’s not all cut and dried; it’s not all clear; and that even top grade scientists can make mistakes. . . . It’s like a circle; if those papers weren’t passed around and read from one person to another, from one student to another, new ideas won’t come up. . . . It’s a network. It’s a network of thoughts. (S10) Understanding of how scientific research is carried out I took microbiology—we read a paper then—if I read it again it would probably be totally different now, with this approach. . . . If I had to do it all over again, I’d probably want to take this course after the first two intro courses—because it allowed me to interpret information differently; ‘‘think outside the box’’ so to speak. . . . This pretty much required us to do all the work ourselves. . . .It wasn’t just a bunch of facts that we just had to accept. We had to actually question it. (S8) It’s the most effective way of teaching that I’ve ever had, especially in a science course. Because in ½real science you’re not really given exams, and you’re not asked, like, to memorize things—you’re asked to analyze and understand—and I think this class really focused on analyzing and understanding, as opposed to memorizing. (S3) Class 3 I had a paper for one of one of my other classes—called Muscle and Nerve—I had to do a review paper on a presentation I did. And ½because of the CREATE class I have never read through papers so quickly than I did those five papers, and I could actually sit there and say, ‘‘Oh, this makes sense; OK. This is dose response to creatine in embryonic umbilical cords. I’m like, oh, that was so great! I didn’t think I would apply it so quickly . . . (S7) Transfer of the CREATE approach to other classes I would recommend it to every student taking a science major or nonscience major, and I think this class should be a prerequisite in order to graduate with a science degree. I think it’s a very strong class that expands all the students’ minds. . . . And I think that this course will definitely help anyone who wants to pursue a career a science, whatever that might be, because it helps you really really understand what science is about. It’s not just a textbook that you read and memorize things; you actually learn so much. (S1) Reaction to the CREATE approach Class 2 Post-course interviews: student comments about CREATE and its effect on their views of science and their own abilities TABLE 1 1386 S. G. Hoskins, L. M. Stevens and R. H. Nehm I myself could be a scientist now. Before I was like ‘‘½Only some kinds of people can be scientists’’ and it has to be like these geniuses, who were, you know, like eight times smarter—I learned that it can be anybody. Anyone can be a scientist; it has to do with having a passion to do research, and just a drive, and not to get bogged down by failed experiments and things not going right, but just to go through a process, because there’s a thinking process you have to go through, of elimination, and trying, and experimenting. (S5) I feel like this course . . . has made me become less intimidated. . . . because these people are just like you and I—you can sit here and wonder why the sky is blue—and you take your question and you put it to the test. . . . This course has definitely helped me with knowing that scientists are just people, just like you and I. Whatever work they’re doing. . . . I can do it myself, you know? . . . I’m not a scientist, ½but I’m contemplating if I should be! . . . I’m starting to like research. What I love about it is the creative part of it. And I’m questioning. . . . can I still have that while being a medical doctor? This class has kind of changed my life in a way. (S3) Increased interest in becoming a scientist I got to see they’re like everyone else. . . . Growing up, you think of scientists as geniuses like Einstein or whatever—but they have a job just like anyone else, and the concepts aren’t that difficult . . . anyone could think of another experiment. You don’t have to have a super-high IQ. It’s just thinking up the work and being truthful with the data and not tweaking your results to match what you want it to be, but just coming up with a hypothesis, running the experiments, writing the data as you see it and trying to analyze it as best as possible. (S4) My scope of thinking has been widened. First, I have more ideas pulled together and I’m using my initiative much more. And I’m being innovative and I’m being very active; right on my feet thinking; and I have more interest in doing the work. My interest has been boosted up. It’s very easy; it’s made open for me to bring out the best out of me and then to join with others. The other thing too; in that class we learned to work together. We had different types of people in the groups, so we were learning something else. In addition to our individual capacities we were learning how to exchange ideas with others. (S2) Class 3 I definitely have changed my perspective of scientists. Before I thought being a scientist was a job where you were antisocial; cooped up in a lab, and that’s it. And I always figured people who become scientists are not ‘‘people people,’’ and I don’t think that any more. (S11) Personal connection to science and scientists You need to be skeptical in science—not just take the data for what they say it is. A lot of times ½before, we study papers, we just look at the discussion and the title and take that for granted, and this class has taught us to be skeptical—to be scientists, and look at the data and try to analyze it for ourselves and see if we get the same conclusion that they got . . . from the data. (S6) Thinking like a scientist Class 2 Representative comments of CREATE students about the CREATE approach and its effect on their views of science and their own abilities. Interviews lasting 20 min were conducted at the end of the semester and audiotaped. Interview questions examined students’ overall reaction to the CREATE approach, whether the course had affected their ability to ‘‘think like a scientist,’’ whether their views of science or scientists had changed over the course of the semester, whether their scientific reading skills had been affected, and whether their confidence in their ability to participate in science research had changed. We made transcripts of the interviews from the first class and used a constant comparison approach to categorize responses on the basis of broad themes that emerged in multiple interviews (Glaser and Strauss 1967; Novak 1998; Ary et al. 2002). We then analyzed interviews of subsequent cohorts on the basis of the similarity to or difference from the initially defined emergent categories (centered headings). The number following each quote corresponds to a distinct student (S) participant. Class 1: n ¼ 12; class 2: n ¼ 13; class 3: n ¼ 12. I think I’m a little bit more confident that I could do it if I really wanted to go in that direction. I think the human aspect of it and the way that research is carried out by individuals, that whole experience has changed for me. I feel that if I wanted to do it I could. Like, there’s somany people that help you along the way and get you started, and just the whole thought process, to think of experiments and to do them, is interesting to me. (S8) I believe scientists are people; they are like everyday people. Anyone can be a scientist. . . . Before I used to think that scientists were rigid people who wore lab coats and didn’t talk to anyone ½laughs but from the class and from the responses we got from the e-mails I see that scientists are ‘‘people persons,’’ everyday people; meaning they didn’t have to be super-geniuses or really up in status to become a scientist; with connections or anything like that. (S1) I think the biggest, kind of like enlightenment for me ½laughs is that you can have your own ideas . . . and you can come up with your own interpretation of things and not necessarily be ‘‘wrong.’’ I think there is a lot more creativity behind science than most people are aware of. . . . Through this course, the creativity, for me it’s been like ‘‘Wow I can really think about these things and not just take in this data and say ‘‘OK this is it; I can’t question it.’’ For me that was the biggest insight. It was like ‘‘I can question it and maybe come up with an alternate explanation. And it might be, it might not be, but at least I’m ‘allowed’ to do so; there’s no big law against that.’’ (S12) Class 1 (Continued) TABLE 1 Genetics Education 1387 1388 S. G. Hoskins, L. M. Stevens and R. H. Nehm CONCLUSIONS To our knowledge, CREATE is the only multiply assessed educational method shown to increase both understanding of and interest in scientific research among undergraduate students. In this regard it is also notable that 64% of our CREATE students were members of minority groups that are traditionally underrepresented among students progressing on to careers in science. We anticipate that the CREATE method will benefit students from a variety of backgrounds, however. Our data suggest that the CREATE approach could significantly alleviate the well-documented disengagement of many college students from science (Seymour and Hewett 1997; National Science Foundation 2002; Alberts 2005; Cech and Kennedy 2005). It is also important to note that the CREATE approach does not require any significant financial expenditure and therefore will be accessible to instructors at many different types of institutions. We believe that the CREATE curriculum, which encourages students to think of themselves as scientists, will complement and enhance students’ experience of traditional lecture-based science teaching and inquiry lab classes. Although CREATE was initially developed for use in an upper division elective course with relatively few students, we believe that elements of the CREATE method can be effectively adapted for use in lower division and larger science classes. The approach is adaptable to content in any area of science, and articles can be chosen to be accessible to students at a variety of levels. Earlier exposure to CREATE analytical approaches may help students to develop critical analysis skills early in their college careers so that they can benefit from them throughout their college coursework (Braxton et al. 2000). Correspondingly, the earlier that students develop an appreciation for the creative nature of scientific investigation and, in particular, recognize that they, too, could make an important contribution to science, the less likely it is that they will drop out of science majors. In contrast to K–12 teachers, most instructors at the college level have not had formal training in how to teach effectively. Many faculty members in the sciences obtain academic positions and promotions on the basis of their research accomplishments. In this respect, we believe that the CREATE approach can benefit instructors as well as students because, rather than requiring instructors to learn a completely new teaching method, it encourages faculty members to use skills that many employ in their laboratories every day. The CREATE class is very similar to a lab meeting in which methods are described, results reported and analyzed, interpretations discussed, and future directions debated. In short, by using primary literature as a portal into the activities of working scientists, and by guiding class discussions rather than lecturing, instructors can create a virtual laboratory in which every student is a scientist. We thank David Eastzer, Shubha Govind, and David Stein for their valuable discussions and advice during the development and implementation of this project. We thank Ruth Ellen Proudfoot for advice on statistical analyses, Christina Nadar and Arturo de Lozanne for help with graphics, and two anonymous reviewers for insightful comments on a previous version of the article. We also are very grateful to Carol Mason for her participation in a group interview, to Christine Holt for her encouragement of the project, and to all of the article authors who responded by e-mail to our students. Finally, we thank all of the City College of New York students who participated in the CREATE classes. This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF) under grant no. 0311117 to S.G.H. and L.M.S. (CoPrincipal Investigators). We thank the NSF for support and the NSF Course, Curriculum and Laboratory Improvement program officers for helpful discussions during the development and implementation of the CREATE project. LITERATURE CITED Alberts, B., 2005 A wakeup call for science faculty. Cell 123: 739–741. Allen, D., and K. Tanner, 2003 Approaches to cell biology teaching: mapping the journey—concept maps as signposts of developing knowledge structures. Cell Biol. Educ. 2: 133–136. American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1989 Science for All Americans. AAAS, Washington, DC. American Council on Education, 2003 20th Anniversary: minorities in higher education annual status report, pp. 85–86. American Council on Education, Washington, DC. Ary, D., L. Jacobs and A. Razavieh, 2002 Introduction to Research in Education, Ed.6. Wadsworth/Thomson Learning, Belmont, CA. Atwell, R., 2004 The long road ahead: barriers to minority participation persist, pp. 7–9 in Reflections on 20 years of Minorities in Higher Education and the ACE Annual Status Report. American Council on Education, Center for Advancement of Racial and Ethnic Equity, Washington, DC. Bissell, A., and P. Lemons, 2006 A new method for assessing critical thinking in the classroom. Bioscience 56: 66–72. Bloom, B., M. Englehart, E. Furst, W. Hill and D. Krathwohl, 1956 A Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: Handbook 1—Cognitive Domain. McKay, New York. Bok, D., 2003 Closing the nagging gap in minority chievement. Chron. High. Educ. 24: B20. Bok, D., 2006 Our Underachieving Colleges. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Bransford, J., A. Brown and R. Cocking (Editors), 1999 How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School, Expanded edition. National Academy Press, Washington, DC. Braxton, J., J. Milem and A. Sullivan, 2000 The influence of active learning on the college student departure process. J. Higher Ed. 71: 569–590. Brooks, J., and M. Brooks, 1993 The Case for Constructivist Classrooms. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, Alexandria, VA. Cech, T., and D. Kennedy, 2005 Doing more for Kate. Science 310: 1741. Chickering, A. W., and Z. Gamson, 1987 Seven principles for good practice. AAHE Bull. 39: 3–7. Chin, C., D. Brown and B. Bruce, 2002 Student-generated questions: a meaningful aspect of learning in science. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 24: 521–549. Chowe, S., and P. Drennan, 2001 Analyzing scientific literature using a jigsaw group activity. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 30: 328–330. Edwards, J., and K. Fraser, 1983 Concept maps as reflectors of conceptual understanding. Res. Sci. Educ. 13: 19–26. Felder, R., 1987 On creating creative engineers. Eng. Educ. 77: 222–227. Foertsch, J., 2000 Models for undergraduate instruction: the potential of modeling and visualization technology in science and math education, pp. 37–40 in Targeting Curricular Change: Reform in Undergraduate Education in Science, Math, Engineering and Technology. American Association for Higher Education, Washington, DC. Genetics Education Germann, P., and R. Aram, 1996 Student performances on the science processes of recording data, analyzing data, drawing conclusions, and providing evidence. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 33: 773–798. Glaser, B., and A. Strauss, 1967 The Discovery of Grounded Theory. Aldine Publishing, Chicago. Glenn Commission, 2000 Before It’s Too Late: A Report to the Nation from The National Commission on Mathematics and Science Teaching for the 21st Century. U. S. Department of Education, Washington, DC. Good, R., J. Novak and J. Wandersee (Editors), 1990 Special issue: perspectives on concept mapping. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 27: 921–1074. Handlesman, J., D. Ebert-May, R. Beichner, P. Bruns, A. Chang et al., 2004 Scientific teaching. Science 304: 521–522. Herman, C., 1999 Reading the literature in the jargon-intensive field of molecular genetics. J. Coll. Sci. Teach. 28: 252–254. Herreid, C. F., 2004 Can case studies be used to teach critical thinking? J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 33: 12–14. Janick-Buckner, D., 1997 Getting undergraduates to critically read and discuss primary literature. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 27: 29–32. Kitchen, E., J. Bell, S. Reeve, R. Sudweeks and W. Bradshaw, 2003 Teaching cell biology in the large-enrollment classroom: methods to promote analytic thinking and assessment of their effectiveness. Cell Biol. Educ. 2: 180–194. Klemm, W., 2002 FORUM for case study learning. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 31: 298–301. Kozma, R., and J. Russell, 1997 Multimedia and understanding: expert and novice responses to different representations of chemical phenomena. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 34: 949–968. Lederman, N., 1992 Students’ and teachers’ conceptions of the nature of science: a review of the research. J. Res. Sci. Teaching. 29: 331–359. Mangurian, L., S. Feldman, J. Clements and L. Boucher, 2001 Analyzing and communicating scientific information. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 30: 440–445. Mann, F., S. Ray, W. A. Harris and C. E. Holt, 2002 Topographic mapping in dorsoventral axis of the Xenopus retinotectal system depends on signaling through ephrin-B ligands. Neuron 35: 461– 473. Mann, F., E. Miranda, C. Weinl, E. Harmer and C. E. Holt, 2003 B-type Eph receptors and ephrins induce growth cone collapse through distinct intracellular pathways. J. Neurobiol. 57: 323–336. Marbach-Ad, G., and P. Sokolove, 2000 Can undergraduate biology students learn to ask higher level questions? J. Res. Sci. Teaching 37: 854–870. McCray, R., R. Dehann and J. Shuck (Editors), 2003 Improving Undergraduate Education in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics Report. Committee on Undergraduate Science Education, National Research Council, National Academy Press, Washington, DC. McMillan, J., 1987 Enhancing college students’ critical thinking: a review of studies. Res. Higher Educ. 26: 3. Muench, S., 2000 Choosing primary literature in biology to achieve specific educational goals. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 29: 255–260. 1389 Nakagawa, S., C. Brennan, K. G. Johnson, D. Shewan, W. A. Harris et al., 2000 Ephrin-B regulates the ipsilateral routing of retinal axons at the optic chiasm. Neuron 25: 599–610. National Research Council, 1999 Transforming Undergraduate Education in Science, Mathematics, Engineering, and Technology. NRC Committee on Undergraduate Science Education, National Academy Press, Washington, DC. National Research Council, 2000 How People Learn: Bridging Research and Practice. National Academy Press, Washington, DC. National Research Council, 2003 Bio 2010: Transforming Undergraduate Education for Future Research Biologists. National Academy Press, Washington, DC. National Science Foundation, 2002 Science and Engineering Degrees, by Race/Ethnicity of Recipients: 1991–2000. Arlington, VA. Novak, J. D., 1990 Concept mapping: a useful tool for science education. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 27: 937–949. Novak, J. D., 1998 Creating and Using Knowledge: Concept Mapstm as Facilitative Tools in Schools and Corporations. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ. Novak, J. D., 2003 The promise of new ideas and new technology for improving teaching and learning. Cell Biol. Educ. 2: 122–132. Novak, J. D., and D. B. Gowin, 1984 Learning How to Learn. Cambridge University Press, New York. Osborne, J., S. Enduran and S. Simon, 2004 Enhancing the quality of argumentation in school science. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 41: 994–1020. Ruiz-Primo, M., and R. Shavelson, 1996 Problems and issues in the use of concept maps in science assessment. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 33: 569–600. Schwartz, R., N. Lederman and B. Crawford, 2004 Developing views of nature of science in an authentic context: an explicit approach to bridging the gap between nature of science and scientific inquiry. Sci. Educ. 88: 610–645. Seymour, E., and N. Hewett, 1997 Talking About Leaving: Why Undergraduates Leave the Sciences. Westview Press, Boulder, CO. Siebert, E., and W. Mcintosh, 2001 College Pathways to the Science Education Standards. National Science Teachers Association Press, Arlington, VA. Steitz, J., 2003 Commentary: Bio 2010—new challenges for biology educators. Cell Biol. Educ. 2: 87–91. Stoddart, T., R. Abrams, E. Gasper and D. Canaday, 2000 Concept maps as assessment in science inquiry learning: a report of methodology. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 22: 1221–1246. Vanzee, E., and J. Minstrell, 1997 Using questioning to guide student thinking. J. Learning Sci. 6: 229–271. Williams, S. E., F. Mann, L. Erskine, T. Sakurai, S. Wei et al., 2003 Ephrin-B2 and EphB1 mediate retinal axon divergence at the optic chiasm. Neuron 39: 919–935. Yuretich, R., 2004 Encouraging critical thinking. J. Coll. Sci. Teaching 33: 40–45. Zohar, A., and F. Nemet, 2002 Fostering students’ knowledge and argumentation skills through dilemmas in human genetics. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 39: 35–62. Zull, J., 2002 The Art of Changing the Brain. Stylus Press, Alexandria, VA. Communicating editor: P. J. Pukkila Backyard Toxicology: Activity 1- Blackworm Lab 1 Activity Description: This is the first day and first lab of the course. It is intended to be an inquiry lab, where the students are immediately immersed into the science of toxicology through an authentic toxicology investigation. This lab looks at the effects of ethanol on California blackworms. Process Skills Goals: Concept mapping, team work, safe animal handling, data recording, critical thinking. Knowledge Goals: Dose-response; the definition of toxicology and its connections to other sciences, real world, their own lives; how to conduct a toxicological assay. Estimated Length of Activity: 2 class periods (2-3 hours) Pre-activity: Concept map the term ‘toxicology’ as a class on the board. Major Assignment: Conduct a toxicological assay of ethanol on California blackworms, draw a doseresponse curve from the data points and write a summary of the findings. Assessment: Non-formative- participation with the group and class discussion Formative- Summary of findings and doseresponse curve Materials: Blackworm lab packet, California blackworms (available at aquarium/ pet stores where live fish food is sold), ethanol, graduated cylinders, 5 petri dishes per group, probes Activity Instructions: **The teacher should prepare the ethanol concentrations according to the instructions provided in the blackworm lab packet prior to class. It will save time if the teacher assembles the probes as well. Begin the class by introducing concept mapping. Write the term ‘toxicology’ on the board and encourage the class to make connections to themselves, their environment and what they know about toxins and environments from other classes. Follow the lab activity instructions in the packet. Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms In this investigation, participants work in groups to determine the normal behavior of California blackworms (Lumbriculus variegatus). They then determine how various concentrations of assigned toxicants affect the worms’ behavior. This investigation introduces testing of potential toxicants, an important component in environmental health science. After testing different toxicants and concentrations, participants will investigate exposure pathways, nature of effects, acute and chronic exposure, and reversible and irreversible effects. The participants discuss and analyze their data, observe physiological effects, and present their findings to the class. Through extensions, participants can develop new investigations based on their findings. £ The activity is written for workshop participants and may need modification for classroom use. Suggested Background Reading • An Introduction to Toxicology National Science Education Standards for Grades 5–12 Science as Inquiry • Abilities Necessary to Do Scientific Inquiry Conduct scientific investigations. Students conduct an investigation that tests the behavioral effects of various concentrations of alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine on California blackworms. Formulate and revise scientific explanations using logic and evidence. After considering the data while answering a series of questions and participating in group discussions, students formulate explanations for the worms’ behavior and suggest future experiments based on scientific knowledge, logic, and investigational evidence. Life Science • The Behavior of Organisms Organisms have behavioral responses to internal changes and external stimuli. Students recognize how organisms respond to external stimuli through exposure to environmental changes caused by the introduction of toxicants. Science in Personal and Social Perspectives • Personal and Community Health Personal choice concerning health involves multiple factors. By observing, understanding, and discussing biological consequences of products such as alcohol and tobacco, students will be able to make more informed decisions about personal health practices. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 1 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms Safety As the instructor, you are expected to provide participants with the necessary safety equipment (including personal protective equipment such as goggles, gloves, aprons, etc.) and appropriate safety instruction to allow them to work safely in the laboratory. Always follow local, state, and school policies. Read and follow all precautions on labels and MSDSs provided by the manufacturer for all chemicals used. Materials For Getting Ready Per class • materials to make a probe § narrow rubber bands § electrical tape § applicator stick or bamboo skewer • containers for preparing, measuring, and storing stock solutions • Bunsen burner or hot plate • glass stirring rod • distilled water or untreated spring water • toxicants § 80-proof vodka (for ethanol) § Vivarin® (for caffeine) § cigarettes of regular length and strength (for nicotine) £ For the Procedure Per class • California blackworms California blackworms may be ordered from Carolina Science & Math (800/334-5551; #B3-L412). The worms may also be available at local tropical fish stores. • £ recovery container for blackworms Per group • disposable Beral pipets (with tips cut off) or eyedroppers • 4 chambers Chambers can be small weighing dishes or petri dishes placed over a white sheet of paper. Any small, clear container that will allow at least a 1-cm depth for the solutions will work. • • • • distilled water filter paper toxicant solutions (prepared in Getting Ready) waste beaker Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 2 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms • • • probes (prepared in Getting Ready) stopwatch or other timer black permanent marker Getting Ready Solution Preparation In each case, make the stock solution first and then use it for dilutions. Use ONLY distilled water or spring water for making all dilutions and holding the worms. Tap water may kill the worms because they are very sensitive to chlorine. Ethanol Do not use denatured ethanol, methanol, or isopropyl alcohol. Vodka is recommended because it is clear and nearly odorless. To prepare the stock solution of ethanol, to 100 mL 80-proof vodka (40% ethanol), add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 400 mL (10% ethanol). Use this stock solution to prepare the following solutions: • • • Solution #1: To 5 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL (0.25% ethanol). Solution #2: To 50 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL (2.5% ethanol). Solution #3: Measure out 200 mL stock solution (10% ethanol). Caffeine Vivarin is recommended as the source for caffeine rather than NoDoz® tablets, which contain a mint flavoring. To prepare the stock solution of caffeine, place two Vivarin tablets (total 400 mg caffeine) in an Erlenmeyer flask and add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 400 mL. Heat while frequently stirring to dissolve the tablets. It helps to gently break the tablets by tapping them with a glass rod while the solution is being heated. Do not let the solution boil. Use this stock solution to prepare the following solutions: • • • Solution #1: To 16 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL. Solution #2: To 66 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL. Solution #3: Measure out 200 mL stock solution. Nicotine Use any generic or name-brand cigarette that is regular length and strength (do not use menthol, 100’s, or ultralights). To prepare the stock solution of nicotine, stir the tobacco from two cigarettes (total 2.2 mg nicotine) in 500 mL very warm distilled water for 15–20 minutes. Strain or filter the solution after soaking. (You will lose about 50 mL of solution through straining). This process makes about 450 mL (0.011 mg/mL). Use this stock solution to prepare the following solutions: Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 3 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms • • • Solution #1: To 10 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL. Solution #2: To 50 mL stock solution, add distilled water to bring to a final solution volume of 200 mL. Solution #3: Measure out 200 mL stock solution. The intent of this investigation is to determine what behavior changes occur at different sublethal concentrations. However, as a demonstration you may wish to expose one worm to a double-strength alcohol solution and one to a double-strength nicotine solution. Both of these are usually fatal. Probes Make probes by cutting a 2.5-cm piece of a narrow rubber band and then tape it to the end of an applicator stick or bamboo skewer. Use electrical tape to attach the rubber band. About half of the rubber band should hang from the end of the stick (Drewes, 1997). Procedure Notes and Outcomes In this investigation, participants work in collaborative groups of three or four to observe California blackworms and establish normal behavior. This baseline observation is used for comparison when toxicants are tested. Participants then test their worms with their assigned toxicant: alcohol, caffeine, or nicotine. Three concentrations are used and the participants are assigned observation criteria for the worms during the exposure. After answering a series of questions and participating in a group discussion, participants present their data, proper explanations, and suggest future experiments to the class. This investigation can range from structured to open-ended, depending on the intended grade and ability level as well as on the length of the class period and the time devoted to the study. For instance, participants can mix their own dilutions and calculate the concentrations, or the solutions can already be prepared. Participants can use more concentrated stock solutions to test concentrations that determine the range for lethal, sublethal, or no effect. Participants can also investigate the physiological effects of these concentrated solutions on organisms. The time of exposure can be changed. First, participants must to learn to manipulate the worms and to observe normal behaviors. They will soon develop their own “system” for handling the worms. They could be assigned to read the article in Carolina Tips (see References) to complete their introduction to the worms. For all experiments, use full-length worms that are uniform in color. Worms that are dark with lighter sections have recently undergone regeneration and should not be used. Use care in handling the worms with the pipet so as not to fragment them. The worms should be “probed” only with the special probes made for this activity, never the tip of the pipet or forceps. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 4 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms Participants should determine the anterior end from the posterior end. The anterior end is blunter and more darkly pigmented than the posterior end. It is also the end that will move first. If several worms are in the chamber, they will clump together in a ball, as they like to “cling” to things. This in itself is a normal behavior that the participants will want to note. Participants can separate the group by gently probing the worms or pipetting with water. Once the anterior and posterior ends are established, participants should begin observing the swimming behavior. Touch the probe to the posterior end. The worm will swim forward in a corkscrew fashion, alternating clockwise and counterclockwise. When the worm is probed on the anterior end, it will coil and reverse its position. Both these movements are quite rapid, and it may take some time to note the differences. Next, participants can observe crawling behavior. The worm can be placed in a petri dish or weighing dish on moistened filter paper (remove all excess water). Again they should probe both the posterior and anterior ends of the worm. In each case the worm will move by peristaltic crawling (successive waves of muscle contractions) in the opposite direction. Results that might be noted for the suggested toxicants are listed below. These sample results represent some of the behavior changes that participants have observed using the given concentrations. You may note different observations on different days, depending on how participants observe and perceive the changes as well as on the size and health of the worms used. Sample Data In ethanol, the worms will be less likely to clump and will become rather inactive as the concentrations increase. In the highest concentration, they may straighten out in the middle but have their ends curled. The anterior area will be more affected. The worms may need to be probed several times to stimulate a response. They will have less skill in swimming, although they will still be able to crawl. They should begin to recover in 15 minutes after exposure to the first two solutions. Although the worms in the first two solutions will recover completely within 24 hours, recovery may not be complete after 24 hours with the third solution. In caffeine, the worms become very active as the concentration increases, but they may try to clump at the lower concentration. They will show a greater sensitivity to probing both in swimming and crawling. At the higher concentration they may first curl in a ball and then stretch out. Some recovery should be seen after 15 minutes in the lower concentrations, and all should fully recover within 24 hours. With nicotine, the worms may twitch in the solution. The tail may curl with loss of response. In the highest concentration, paralysis will occur. With paralysis, the worm will stretch and just seem to float in the water. There should be some recovery at the lower concentration in 15 minutes, and all worms should recover within 24 hours. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 5 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms Plausible Answers to Questions 1. Exposure occurs when an organism comes in contact with a toxicant. Exposure frequency refers to how often, exposure duration refers to how long, and exposure concentration refers to how much. Using this terminology, describe each for your investigation. Answers will vary. 2. Two types of toxicity tests can be performed. Acute toxicity tests are a high single exposure for a brief duration. Chronic toxicity tests are usually a persistent and longer exposure (depending on the organism’s lifespan) at a lower concentration than the acute test. a. Based on this information, which type of test was done in this investigation? This was an acute toxicity test. b. What are the benefits of using an acute toxicity test? It saves time, one can see immediate responses to the toxicant; and one can learn the per dose limit of tolerance to a toxicant. c. What are the benefits of using a chronic toxicity test? One can learn the cumulative effects of the toxicant; it more closely approximates normal exposure to toxicants. 3. Using the data from your assigned toxicant, design a chronic toxicity test that you might perform on the blackworms. Predict what your results might be. One might begin by exposing the worms to the lowest alcohol concentration used in this experiment, as well as exposing them to even lower concentrations. Since the exposure needs to be chronic, the experiment would last over a much longer period of time (months or even years). The exposure could be constant or periodic. Periodic exposures, meaning the worms are given some recovery time in between exposures, would be more representative of “real life” exposures. 4. The exposure pathway is how a toxicant enters the body. What was the exposure pathway for your toxicant? The exposure pathway is through the surface membrane of the worm. It is unlikely that for a water-soluble toxicant, such as alcohol, entry would be through the mouth. Ingestion of a toxicant by these worms would probably have to be associated with food. (You can demonstrate this to the participants by removing the head and the tail of a worm and showing that the toxicant still has a dramatic systemic effect.) 5. Toxicity is affected by both intrinsic and extrinsic factors. Extrinsic factors, such as temperature or barometric pressure, occur outside the body. Intrinsic factors, such as age, metabolism, and genetic differences, are inherent to an individual organism. Using the following factors, predict how you think each could affect the results with your toxicant. Be specific! Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 6 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms a. temperature b. age c. metabolism d. genetic difference Answers will vary. 6. Your concentrations represent sublethal concentrations of the toxicant. Explain what you think this means. The concentration of the toxicant is not high enough to kill the worms during the period of time they were exposed. 7. The potency of a toxicant is the measure of its strength. Paracelsus (1493–1541) is quoted as saying “The dose makes the poison.” The more potent the toxicant, the less it takes to evoke a response. Based on the concentrations listed above and your observations, which toxicant do you think is the most potent and why? Answers will vary. 8. Based on your toxicant, what body systems do you think were affected and why? Answers will vary. 9. At the end of the 24-hour recovery, you can generally determine whether the effects of your toxicant are reversible or irreversible. Based on the toxicant that you used, tell whether the effects were reversible or irreversible at each concentration. All of the worms recovered within 24 hours, so the effects of these toxicants are reversible. The effects of the strongest ethanol solution is a possible exception. 10. Did all of your worms (at each concentration) demonstrate the same behavior? Assume that one worm demonstrated normal behavior and the other four demonstrated abnormal behavior. How would you explain this? No, all of the worms at each concentration did not demonstrate the same behavior. This can be explained by variability, or differences, between the worms. For example, there may be genetic, age, health, or size differences between the worms. 11. The investigation that you did was a controlled experiment. a. What was the control? A set of blackworms whose behavior was observed but to which no toxicant was applied; the blackworms in only distilled water. b. Why is a control necessary in a scientific experiment? To be able to determine what is “normal” so we can compare and determine deviations from normality. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 7 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms 12. Risk assessment of a toxicant is the estimate of severity and the likelihood of harm to human health or the environment that occurs from exposure to a risk agent (toxicant). The toxicants that you tested are relevant to human health. Name some toxicants you might test that would harm the environment and thus pose a threat to the worms. Ammonia, chlorine, or other forms of alcohol that are commonly released into the environment, such as antifreeze. 13. How do lifestyles play a part in risk assessment of human health toxicants? Lifestyles affect an individual’s health, metabolism, and limits of exposure to toxicants. For example, risk increases if a person is in poor health. Limits of exposure to toxicants can be affected by actions such a frequency of exposure to alcohol or career choice. Risk increases as the frequency of alcohol (ethanol) exposure increases. Also, a mechanic will probably be exposed to more toxicants than a secretary; thus a mechanic’s risk for exposure to certain chemicals would be higher. Remember, exposure does not necessarily mean the toxicant has entered the person’s body. If the mechanic is very careful (which is also a lifestyle choice), the dose of the toxicants would be zero. 14. Can the results of your tests be applied to humans or other vertebrates? Why or why not? Not directly. Effects can only be extrapolated from an experimental system to another system if the two types of systems can be shown to be sufficiently similar in relevant characteristics and behavior. 15. Based on what you have learned from your investigation and your answers to the questions above, analyze your data and summarize any conclusions that can be drawn from the results. Answers will vary. 16. As a group, discuss your findings. Using reference material, look up any information about your toxicant that will help in further analyzing your data. Suggest additional investigations using your toxicant and make a group presentation of your findings to the class. Answers will vary. Extensions Participants may test other toxicants. They will need to determine the initial concentration for their stock solution by trial and error. The goal of the experiment is to determine three sublethal concentrations that produce the following results: 1. low concentration—evokes little or no response 2. middle concentration—evokes a near maximum response 3. high concentration—is sublethal and evokes a maximum response Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 8 Instructor Notes Toxicants and California Blackworms The following are just some examples of toxicants that might be tested: • Aleve®, Nuprin®, aspirin, Tylenol®, Excedrin® PM • melatonin, Nytol®, Dexatrim®, vitamins • Benadryl®, Sudafed® • saccharin, Nutrasweet® • chlorinated water • pesticides, antifreeze, detergents • UV radiation References Drewes, C.D. “Those Wonderful Worms,” Carolina Tips. 59 (3). Carolina Biological Supply: Burlington, NC, 1996. Drewes, C.D. “Screening for Sublethal Behavioral Effects in a Freshwater Oligochaete, Lumbriculus variegatus.” Lab for Dr. Drewes’ toxicology class, 1995. Lesiuk, N.M.; Drewes, C.D. “Blackworms, Blood Vessel Pulsations, and Drug Effects,” The American Biology Teacher. 1999, 61 (1), 48–53. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 9 Activity Instructions Toxicants and California Blackworms This investigation represents a model for testing potential toxicants on organisms. You will determine the behavioral changes that occur when blackworms are exposed to different concentrations of an assigned toxicant through a controlled experiment. At the end of the investigation, you will analyze your data, present your findings to the class, and suggest other possible experiments. Safety In a laboratory setting, you are ultimately responsible for your own safety and for the safety of those around you. It is your responsibility to specifically follow the standard operating procedures (SOPs) which apply to you, including all local, state, and national guidelines on safe handling, storage, and disposal of all chemicals and equipment you may use in the labs. This includes determining and using the appropriate personal protective equipment (e.g., goggles, gloves, apron). If you are at any time unsure about an SOP or other regulation, check with the course instructor. Procedure You will work in groups and be assigned a toxicant to test. 1. Label four chambers as follows: “Control,” “#1,” “#2,” and “#3” along with the name of the toxicant you’ve been assigned. Pour distilled water into each chamber to a depth of 1 cm. 2. Using a pipet, transfer 20 worms to your control chamber. Be sure to choose worms that are full length and uniform in color. Do not select worms that have distinct lighter patches. 3. Transfer five worms from the control chamber to each of the other chambers (#1, #2, and #3). Observe the behavior of the worms in all four chambers based on your assigned criterion from Table 1. Also look for physical changes such as bulging in the center, bleeding, or fragments breaking off. If any worms die, record the mortality. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 10 Activity Instructions Toxicants and California Blackworms Table 1: Observation Criteria Behavior Procedure Clumping behavior Note whether the worms tend to clump together in a ball. Swimming behavior Use the probe to touch the anterior end (blunter and more darkly pigmented) of the worm and observe the movement. Then touch the posterior end and again observe the movement. Make note if all worms exhibit the same behavior or if some remain normal while others exhibit a change. Crawling behavior Use the pipet to transfer the worm onto moistened filter paper in another chamber and remove all excess water. Probe the worm first on the anterior end and then on the posterior end to make your observations. Use a different worm at each 3-minute interval. Also make note of activity level (faster or slower than normal) and individual position in the water, such as stretched out, curled in a ball, and ends curled, as well as anything else that might be considered an unusual response. 4. Gently draw your worms from each chamber (except the control) back into a pipet. (If this is too difficult, get another chamber with some distilled water and put your worms there temporarily.) Discard the distilled water and then add your assigned toxicant solution in the corresponding chamber to a depth of 1 cm. Transfer your worms back into the chamber. Expose the worms to the toxicant for at least 15 minutes. During the exposure period, make your assigned observations every 3 minutes on all four chambers and record them by 3-minute intervals on your data sheet. 5. After the exposure period has ended, allow the worms to recover. Draw your worms into a pipet or transfer them to a clean chamber. Empty the toxicant into the waste beaker and fill the chamber to a depth of 1 cm with distilled water. Add the worms. (Try to expel as much toxicant as possible out of the pipet first if they have been held in the pipet.) Again observe the worms at 3-minute intervals on all four chambers for at least 15 minutes during the recovery period and record your observations just as you did before. Be sure to make notes about any recovery (a return to normal behavior) that takes place. 6. Leave the worms in their chambers overnight and observe again the next day. Be sure to record the number of mortalities that occurred overnight. Return fully recovered worms to the original large container after 48 hours. Any worms that don’t appear to be in good health or fully recovered after 48 hours should go in a separate container marked “Recovery.” Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 11 Activity Instructions Toxicants and California Blackworms Questions Collaborate with your group to obtain data for all concentrations of your toxicant. Complete your data table on the last page with the following information on toxicant concentrations and then answer the questions. Table 2: Toxicant Concentrations Ethanol Caffeine Nicotine Solution #1 0.25% 0.08 mg/mL 0.00055 mg/mL Solution #2 2.5% 0.33 mg/mL 0.00275 mg/mL Solution #3 10% 1.0 mg/mL 0.011 mg/mL 1. Exposure occurs when an organism comes in contact with a toxicant. Exposure frequency refers to how often, exposure duration refers to how long, and exposure concentration refers to how much. Using this terminology, describe each for your investigation. 2. Two types of toxicity tests can be performed. Acute toxicity tests are a high single exposure for a brief duration. Chronic toxicity tests are usually a persistent and longer exposure (depending on the organism’s lifespan) at a lower concentration than the acute test. a. Based on this information, which type of test was done in this investigation? b. What are the benefits of using an acute toxicity test? c. What are the benefits of using a chronic toxicity test? 3. Using the data from your assigned toxicant, design a chronic toxicity test that you might perform on the blackworms. Predict what your results might be. 4. The exposure pathway is how a toxicant enters the body. What was the exposure pathway for your toxicant? 5. Toxicity is affected by both intrinsic and extrinsic factors. Extrinsic factors, such as temperature or barometric pressure, occur outside the body. Intrinsic factors, such as age, metabolism, and genetic differences, are inherent to an individual organism. Using the following factors, predict how you think each could affect the results with your toxicant. Be specific! a. temperature b. age c. metabolism d. genetic difference 6. Your concentrations represent sublethal concentrations of the toxicant. Explain what you think this means. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 12 Activity Instructions Toxicants and California Blackworms 7. The potency of a toxicant is the measure of its strength. Paracelsus (1493–1541) is quoted as saying “The dose makes the poison.” The more potent the toxicant, the less it takes to evoke a response. Based on the concentrations listed above and your observations, which toxicant do you think is the most potent and why? 8. Based on your toxicant, what body systems do you think were affected and why? 9. At the end of the 24-hour recovery, you can generally determine whether the effects of your toxicant are reversible or irreversible. Based on the toxicant that you used, tell whether the effects were reversible or irreversible at each concentration. 10. Did all of your worms (at each concentration) demonstrate the same behavior? Assume that one worm demonstrated normal behavior and the other four demonstrated abnormal behavior. How would you explain this? 11. The investigation that you did was a controlled experiment. a. What was the control? b. Why is a control necessary in a scientific experiment? 12. Risk assessment of a toxicant is the estimate of severity and the likelihood of harm to human health or the environment that occurs from exposure to a risk agent (toxicant). The toxicants that you tested apply to human health. Name some toxicants you might test that would harm the environment and thus pose a threat to the worms. 13. How do lifestyles play a part in risk assessment of human health toxicants? 14. Can the results of your tests be applied to humans or other vertebrates? Why or why not? 15. Based on what you have learned from your investigation and your answers to the questions above, analyze your data and summarize any conclusions that can be drawn from the results. 16. As a group, discuss your findings. Using reference material, look up any information about your toxicant that will help in further analyzing your data. Suggest additional investigations using your toxicant and make a group presentation of your findings to the class. Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. 13 Risks & Choices, Center for Chemistry Education, Miami University (Ohio) www.terrificscience.org—Permission granted to copy for classroom use only. Conc:_____ Solution #3 Conc:_____ Solution #2 Conc:_____ Solution #1 Control Observed behavior b __________ efore exposure Toxicant: 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 3 min 6 min 9 min 12 min 15 min 6 min 6 min 9 min 3 min 3 min 9 min Observed behavior during recovery (total recovery time ____) Observed behavior during exposure (total exposure time ____) Observed behavior after 24 hours recovery Data Sheet Toxicants and California Blackworms 14 Backyard Toxicology: Activity 2- Concept Mapping Activity Description: This activity begins as a pos-lab exercise from Activity 1. It then transfers what the students have learned about concept mapping to reading scientific articles. Process Skills Goals: Concept mapping, teamwork, reading primary scientific literature. Knowledge Goals: How to read an introduction from a scientific paper, how to translate scientific research into a concept map combining prior and new knowledge of the topic, the importance of understanding of the affect of toxins on a model organism. Estimated Length of Activity: 2 class periods (2-3 hours) Pre-activity: Concept map the term ‘toxicology’ as a class on the board. Ask the students to draw on their lab to build on the concept map from Activity 1. Major Assignment: Individually read the introduction of a scientific article and as a group create a concept map about the topic of focus. Search for definitions to unknown terms and record them in a vocabulary notebook. Assessment: Non-formative- participation with the group and class discussion Formative- Concept map, current vocabulary notebook. Materials: Article 1 introduction, large paper, markers, vocabulary notebook Activity Instructions: The teacher should reintroduce the term toxicology and ask the class to help create a concept map on the board, drawing from the first concept mapping exercise and student observations in the blackworm lab. Provide the class with the introduction (only!) of the article. Ask the students to read the article in class and construct a concept map in groups from the introduction. There should be a significant number of toxicology terms in their concept maps that need to be defined. Encourage them to find these definitions for themselves, and record the terms and their definitions in their vocabulary notebooks. Have students share their concepts maps and definitions with the class in a teacher led class discussion. Alcohol 41 (2007) 281e284 Ethanol levels in honeybee hemolymph resulting from alcohol ingestion Janko Bozica, John DiCesareb, Harrington Wellsc, Charles I. Abramsond,* a Department of Biology, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK 74104, USA c Department of Biology, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK 74104, USA d Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, 215 N. Murray, Stillwater, OK 74078, USA Received 24 February 2007; received in revised form 4 April 2007; accepted 4 April 2007 b Abstract Our previous work on a social insect model of ethanol-induced behavior focused on behavioral studies of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). We now investigate the dependence of honeybee blood ethanol concentration on both the amount of ethanol consumed and time elapsed since ingestion. Blood ethanol level was determined using gas chromatograph using hemolymph taken from harnessed bees. Significantly increased levels of ethanol in honeybee hemolymph were detected within 15 min of feeding bees alcohol. Within 30 min, ethanol concentration increased 2.7 times. The concentration of ethanol ingested also had a significant effect on blood ethanol level. However, postfeeding times greater than 30 min did not significantly increase ethanol concentration in bee hemolymph. This study integrates with our behavioral data on the effect of ethanol on honeybees. Our laboratory and field experiments show a correlation between the time frame for behavioral changes and significant increases of blood ethanol levels shown in this study. Ó 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Honeybee; Apis mellifera; Gas chromatograph; Ethanol; Hemolymph Introduction Investigations of social insect models of induced ethanol behavior have concentrated on the behavioral effects of ethanol on honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Previous results from this laboratory have shown several effects that are common among humans and bees. These include the ability to self-administer ethanol and disruptions in both locomotion and Pavlovian conditioning (see Abramson et al., 2007 for a review). Our most recent work has extended the behavioral analysis to more complicated aspects of honeybee behavior. Drinking ethanol will disrupt, for example, both complex decision processes in free-flying forager bees and social communication within the hive (Abramson et al., 2005; Bozic et al., 2006). We now turn our attention to the physiology of ethanol consumption in the honeybee. The present paper provides some physiological background for the possible action of ethanol through the blood on the central nervous system. We investigate the dependence of blood ethanol concentration on postingestion time and on concentration of ethanol consumed. In the phylum Arthropoda, some data exist on how ethanol is metabolized in insects. Alcohol dehydrogenize in the fruit fly Drosophila is from the type II short-chain dehydrogenises (Benach et al., 2005). It can bind both ethanol and acetaldehyde. Other studies using Drosophila suggest that there also exists acetaldehyde dehydrogenize (Fry et al., 2004). However, it remains controversial whether this enzyme contributes to the metabolism of ingested ethanol in fruit flies. The majority of data on blood ethanol levels and its physiological influence is available from studies using mammals and humans (Grant et al., 2000; Jones et al., 1991; Norberg et al., 2000). Blood concentration of ethanol depends on transport from the digestive track to the blood and catabolism of ethanol in the body. Ethanol conversion to acetaldehyde is catalyzed by alcohol dehydrogenize in these organisms (Sherman et al., 1994; Umulis et al., 2005). Acetaldehyde is then oxidized to acetate by acetaldehyde dehydrogenize. Materials and methods Bees preparation and hemolymph sampling * Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1-405-744-7492; fax: þ1-405-7448067. E-mail address: [email protected] (C.I. Abramson). 0741-8329/07/$ e see front matter Ó 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2007.04.003 Honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) were collected at random as they departed the hive 1 day before an experiment. Each 282 J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 41 (2007) 281e284 bee was placed in a glass vial in an ice water bath. When a bee became inactive, it was immediately removed from the vial and placed into a restraining harness. After regaining consciousness, each subject was fed a 1.5 M sucrose solution (commercial sugar and distilled water) until it would no longer extend its proboscis; the subjects were then left until the following morning (for further details, see Abramson et al., 1997). Prefeeding ensured that all subjects had the same motivation to eat and pass food along their digestive tracks. Different sets of subjects were used for each of the experiments described. On the following day (postfeeding day), bees were randomly assigned to treatment groups and fed with 10 ml of a test solution. Test solutions were always 1.5 M sucrose with different concentration of ethanol. Solutions were made by diluting 95% ethanol (Pharmco, Brookfield, CT ethyl alcohol, 190 proof) with distilled water to make the final concentrations for feeding bees. After feeding, a bee was set aside, still in the harness, for the required postingestion period before its hemolymph was sampled. A harnessed bee was briefly narcotized with CO2 immediately before blood sampling. The bee’s thorax cuticle was then punctured at the abdominal side of the wings’ base and the blood collected in a 1-ml disposable glass microcap (Drummond Scientific Co). This procedure was successfully used in previous honeybee blood studies (Bozic and Woodring, 1997, 1998), and insures that neither the honey stomach nor the intestine is punctured during hemolymph sampling. The 1.00 ml of hemolymph taken from a bee was mixed with 9.00 ml of a 1-butanol stock solution to afford 10 ml of 0.502 mM 1-butanol solution, which was immediately frozen at 20 C until analyzed by gas chromatography. Gas chromatography analysis Ethanol concentration in the honeybee hemolymph was determined by gas chromatography analysis using 1-butanol as an internal standard. A standard curve was prepared with 1.000 mM 1-butanol and 5.000, 1.000, 0.500, 0.100, and 0.050 mM aqueous ethanol solutions resulting in a Rsquared value of 0.999. The standards and honeybee blood samples were analyzed on a Hewlett Packard 5890 GC equipped with a Flame Ionization Detector (FID), Peak Simple analysis software, and a 30-m J&W Scientific capillary column (0.25 mM DB-Wax). The analysis conditions were isothermal at 80 C for 5 min with 1 ml/min of helium carrier gas and a head pressure of 16 psi. Experiments Time dependence First, we wanted to determine how concentration of ethanol in blood changes over time since consumption. We used 10 ml of a test solution containing 5% ethanol, 1.5 M sucrose to feed bees (t 5 0 min). This concentration was selected because it is known from our previous work that a 5% ethanol solution will disrupt honeybee behavior under both laboratory and field conditions. Bees were sampled at 15, 30, 60, 120, 240, and 480 min postethanol ingestion (treatments). A control group was fed 10 ml 1.5 M sucrose with no ethanol and sampled 10 min after feeding. For each test treatment, we used six to eight bees (n 5 48 total). Half the bees for each treatment were tested on day 1 and the remaining tested on day 2. Concentration dependence We used a 30 min postethanol ingestion time to test the effect of ingestion of different ethanol concentration on ethanol blood levels. Bees were fed 10 ml of 0, 1, 2.5, and 5% ethanol in 1.5 M sucrose. These are the same concentrations that were used in our behavioral experiments (see review: Abramson et al., 2007). We assigned three to four bees to each treatment on two separate experimental days. Tests were run in parallel for all ethanol food concentrations (n 5 22 total). Data analysis Data were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with Tukey comparison of mean values. In addition, linear regression was used to show dose dependence of blood ethanol concentrations on concentration of ethanol in food. All statistics were applied using SPSS. Results Ethanol was separable based on the gas chromatography protocol used. A linear relation existed between ethanol concentration in standards and levels detected in the gas chromatography analysis (GC-mM 5 0.951 StandardmM þ 0.0440; significant of regression F 5 10,358.01, df 5 1.3, P !.0001). Time dependence Onset of ethanol effects on the central nervous system of the honeybee is related to the time taken for ethanol to enter the blood. Our shortest experimental time was 15 min. At that time, we find a significant amount of ethanol in the honeybee’s blood (24.6 6 9.2 mM, Fig. 1, ANOVA: F 5 6.11, df 5 4.21, P 5 .002; Tukey test). Average ethanol concentration in the blood increased 2.7 times during the next 15 min. Postingestion times greater than 30 min did not significantly increase the ethanol concentration in the blood (average ranged from 64 to 77 mM: Fig. 1). We observed a relatively steady state mean ethanol value for the resting periods longer than 1 h. Based on the slope between 15 and 30 min (slope 5 2.75 mM/min) extrapolated to the x-axis the estimated delay of onset of ethanol in the honeybee’s blood is 6.1 min. J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 41 (2007) 281e284 100 Blood EtOH (mM) Blood EtOH (mM) 100 50 0 283 0 2 4 6 8 Time (hours) Fig. 1. Effect of resting time after feeding bees with 10 ml of 5% ethanol sugar solution on ethanol blood concentration. Mean values and standard error are plotted for each data point. Concentration dependence Ethanol concentrations in food had a significant effect on ethanol concentration in the blood of honeybees 30 min after consumption of the ethanol solution (F 5 7.58, df 5 3.18, P 5.0017). The largest differences were observed at lower concentrations of ethanol in sugar solution (Fig. 2). A positive correlation was found between an increase in blood ethanol concentration and the amount of ethanol consumed (F 5 24.56, df 5 1.20, P !.0001); amount consumed was equal to the ethanol concentration 10 ml droplet. Discussion This study correlates well with our behavioral data on the effect of ethanol on honeybees. Our laboratory and field experiments were in the same time frame as the significant increases of blood ethanol level observed in our sample. Just 15 min postingestion there were elevated ethanol levels. In foraging situations, the elevated levels are likely to increase even faster due to the anatomy and physiology of the honeybee. Nectar consumed by a forager is regurgitated from its honey stomach to be processed into honey by hive mates and stored in wax cells for future use. Of course, foragers must use some of the nectar collected for metabolic purposes to stay alive. This occurs by passing nectar from the honey stomach into the intestines where sugar is absorbed (Morse and Flottum, 1990). Nectar flow from honey stomach to intestine is highly regulated in honeybees, and inversely related to hemolymph sugar levels (Rocess and Blatt, 1999). Activities that increase sugar metabolism have been shown to increase the rate at which nectar passes from the honey stomach to the intestine 50 0 0 2 4 6 % EtOH Fig. 2. Effect of concentration of ethanol in sugar solution on blood ethanol concentration 30 min after feeding. Mean values and standard error are plotted for each data point. (Rocess and Blatt, 1999). So, higher energetic demands of bees increase flow of nutrients from the honey stomach to the intestine, and from the intestine to the hemolymph. We suspect that ethanol absorption is correlated with that of sugars and, like mammals, increased flow from the stomach to the intestine increases the rate of absorption of ethanol. The key to this flow in honeybees is metabolic demand, which is elevated by flight activity. In our foraging experiment (Abramson et al., 2005; Bozic et al., 2006), we found dramatic effects of ethanol consumption occurring within 15 min postingestion when an artificial feeder contained ethanol at the foraging site. The frequency of social behavior naturally occurring inside of the hive also changed because of ethanol consumption. Most prominent was disruption of waggle dance communication, which has been shown to correlate with the dopamine system in honeybees (Bozic and Woodring, 1998). Dopaminergic, GABA, and neuropeptide neuron transmission and modulation have been recognized to control in animals centrally generated motor patterns (Marder and Bucher, 2001). A fertile area for future research is the neurophysiologic background of ethanol effects on fixed motor patterns generated by the central nervous system. In comparison to vertebrate models, ethanol blood concentration was high and remained elevated for an extended period of time (Matthews et al., 2001). These results were reflected in a study that complements the work presented here (Maze et al., 2006). The Maze study used an enzyme assay to estimate ethanol in bee blood. Its focus was higher concentrations of ethanol (5e50%), and looked at blood alcohol levels starting 30 min after ingestion (then at 6, 12, 24, and 48 h). Our study focus was on lower concentrations of ethanol (1e5%) that are more likely to be encountered naturally by honeybees. We also looked more closely at onset (15 min; 284 J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 41 (2007) 281e284 then at 30 min, 1, 2, 4 and 8 h), and considered chronic alcoholism in addition to a single meal. As expected, 50% ethanol produces much higher blood alcohol levels (over 100 mM) than does 1% ethanol (approximately 5 mM) in a bee’s diet. However, the blood alcohol levels we report for 5% ethanol consumption are consistent with those reported earlier based on the variation among bees observed in both studies (SE bars of the studies meet at about 50 mM). At the lower ethanol concentrations we used, ethanol level in the blood can be seen to be an accelerating function of ethanol amount ingested. This is probability related to the maximum rate a bee can process ethanol (Fig. 2). The Maze study does not report a drop in blood ethanol until 12 h postingestion. The correlation in results from both studies suggests that these results are either artifacts of an analytical technique or artifacts of sampling bees from a particular hive. With respect to time, notably, both studies show that ethanol remains extremely elevated in the hemolymph for a prolonged period. This is most likely due to the rate of nectar passing from the honey stomach into intestine, which is under control of blood glucose level (Rocess and Blatt, 1999). That is, unlike mammals that deal with excess glucose by sequestering it after it has entered the blood stream, honeybees sequester the sugar by holding it in the honey stomach so that it never reaches the hemolymph (Rocess and Blatt, 1999). Honeybee metabolic rate in resting bees is about 1/25 that of foragers in flight (Rothe and Nachtigall, 1989; Wood et al., 2005), Thus, the nectar flow from the honey stomach during our experimental time would be expected to be low since we used resting bees. Although data do not exist on whether ethanol can be digested before passing into the blood, it has been shown that alcohol dehydrogenase and acetaldehyde dehydrogenase are among the proteins secreted by honeybee feeding glands (Santos et al., 2005). Further, there are several different alleles of alcohol dehydrogenize in honeybees (Martins et al., 1977). The transport model from stomach to intestine links ethanol with sucrose absorption and predicts differences in both blood alcohol levels and duration of inebriation base on metabolic rate and nectar sugar concentration. Obviously, much more work is required in this area and how enzyme system relates to the ecology of honeybees. Acknowledgments Janko Bozic participation was supported by Fulbright Scholarship at Oklahoma State University. References Abramson, C. I., Aquino, I. S., Silva, M. C., and Price, J. M. (1997). Learning in the Africanized honey bee: Apis mellifera L. Physiol. Behav. 62, 657–674. Abramson, C. I., Sanderson, C., Painter, J., Barnett, S., and Wells, H. (2005). Development of an ethanol model using social insects V: honey bee foraging decisions under the influence of alcohol. Alcohol 36, 187– 193. Abramson, C. I., Wells, H., and Bozic, J. (2007). A social insect model for the study of ethanol induced behavior: the honey bee. In R. Yoshida (Ed.), Trends in Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Research (pp. 197– 218). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. Benach, J., Winberg, J. O., Svendsen, J. S., Atrian, S., GonzalezDuarte, R., and Ladenstein, R. (2005). Drosophila alcohol dehydrogenase: acetate-enzyme interactions and novel insights into the effects of electrostatics on catalysis. J. Mol. Biol. 345, 579–598. Bozic, J., Abramson, C. I., and Bedencic, M. (2006). Reduced ability of ethanol drinkers for social communication in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.). Alcohol 38, 179–183. Bozic, J., and Woodring, J. (1997). Effect of activity on the haemolymph sugar titres in honey bees. J. Apic. Res. 36, 33–39. Bozic, J., and Woodring, J. (1998). Variations of brain biogenic amines in mature honeybees and induction of recruitment behavior. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 120, 737–744. Fry, J. D., Bahnck, C. M., Mikucki, M., Phadnis, N., and Slattery, W. C. (2004). Dietary ethanol mediates selection on aldehyde dehydrogenase activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Integr. Comp. Biol. 44, 275–283. Grant, S. A., Millar, K., and Kenny, G. N. (2000). Blood alcohol concentration and psychomotor effects. Br. J. Anaesth. 85, 401–406. Jones, A. W., Jonsson, K. A., and Neri, A. (1991). Peak blood-ethanol concentration and the time of its occurrence after rapid drinking on an empty stomach. J. Forensic Sci. 36, 376–385. Marder, E., and Bucher, D. (2001). Central pattern generators and the control of rhythmic movements. Curr. Biol. 11, R986–R996. Martins, E., Mestriner, M. A., and Contel, E. P. (1977). Alcohol dehydrogenase polymorphism in Apis mellifera. Biochem. Genet. 15, 357–366. Matthews, D. B., Overstreet, D. H., Rezvani, A. H., Devaud, L. L., and Morrow, A. L. (2001). Effects of sweetened ethanol solutions on ethanol self-administration and blood ethanol levels. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 68, 13–21. Maze, I. S., Wright, G. A., and Mustard, J. A. (2006). Acute ethanol ingestion produces dose-dependent effects on motor behavior in the honey bee (Apis mellifera). J. Insect Physiol. 52, 1243–1253. Morse, R. A., and Flottum, K. (1990). The ABC & XYZ of Bee Culture, 40th ed., Medina, OH: The A.I. Root Company. Norberg, A., Gabrielsson, J., Jones, A. W., and Hahn, R. G. (2000). Withinand between-subject variations in pharmacokinetic parameters of ethanol by analysis of breath, venous blood and urine. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 49, 399–408. Rocess, F., and Blatt, J. (1999). Hemolymph sugars and the control of the proventriculus in the honey bee Apis mellifera. J. Insect Physiol. 45, 221–229. Rothe, U., and Nachtigall, W. (1989). Flight of the honey bee. J. Comp. Physiol. B 158, 739–749. Santos, K. S., dos Santos, L. D., Mendes, M. A., de Souza, B. M., Malaspina, O., and Palma, M. S. (2005). Profiling the proteome complement of the secretion from hypopharyngeal gland of Africanized nurse-honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Insect Biochem. Mol. Biol. 35, 85–91. Sherman, D. I., Ward, R. J., Yoshida, A., and Peters, T. J. (1994). Alcohol and acetaldehyde dehydrogenase gene polymorphism and alcoholism. EXS 71, 291–300. Umulis, D. M., Gurmen, N. M., Singh, P., and Fogler, H. S. (2005). A physiologically based model for ethanol and acetaldehyde metabolism in human beings. Alcohol 35, 3–12. Wood, W. A., Heinrich, B., and Stevenson, R. D. (2005). Honeybee flight metabolic rate: does it depend on air temperature? J. Exp. Biol. 208, 1161–1173. ANALYSIS TEMPLATE—Fill one in for each figure or table Figure or Table Number: _____ 1) “Official” title for this figure or table (from the caption): 2) My (simplified, decoded, in regular language) title for this figure or table: 3) The specific hypothesis being tested, or specific question being asked in the experiment represented here is: ANALYSIS: First, refer to your cartoon of what the experimenters did, and to your annotated figure, and to the information you wrote in above. Then, answer the following for each figure or table: 4a) For descriptive studies, If we compare panel(s)__________ and __________, or columns __________ and ___________, we learn about________________________________________ If we compare panel(s)__________ and __________, or columns __________ and ___________, we learn about________________________________________ If we compare panel(s)__________ and __________, or columns __________ and ___________, we learn about________________________________________ 4b) For experimental tests, The controls in this experiment are: They are represented (in which part of the chart or graph, or what figure panels?) The experimental groups are: They are represented: We need to compare the controls in _________ with the experimental groups in ____________to find out____________________________________________ We need to compare the controls in _________ with the experimental groups in ____________to find out____________________________________________ (Continue if there are more experiments in the figure): We also need to compare ____________________ with _______________ to find out ______________ When we do this, we learn that: 5) Overall, what we learn from this figure is: 6) The following issues are ones of concern to me: (these can be things you don’t understand, criticisms of the method, questions for the authors, or anything else that comes to mind) Backyard Toxicology: Activity 3- Reading the Results Activity Description: This activity continues breaking down Article 1 into accessible components. This activity tackles the results and methods sections in the form of cartoons and captions. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, reading primary scientific literature, cartooning Knowledge Goals: How to read the results and methods sections of a scientific paper, how to visualize scientific research in the form of a cartoon, how to rephrase scientific jargon to get at the underlying information presented. Estimated Length of Activity: 2 class periods (2-3 hours) Pre-activity: Read results section and look for definitions for unknown terms. Major Assignment: Individually read the results and methods sections of a scientific article, and search for definitions to unknown terms. Create a cartoon of the experiments presented in the results sections. Rewrite the captions of figures and tables so they can be better understood, and add informative labels. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation and participation in class discussions Formative- Cartoon, current vocabulary notebook, captions. Materials: Article 1 results, material and methods sections, white paper, markers, vocabulary notebook Activity Instructions: Provide the class with the Methods and Results sections of the first article. Ask students to read the Results Section and, using the Methods section for more information, do the following: a) Make a cartoon diagram of each experiment from the results section. The idea is to draw what the experiment may have actually looked like in cartoon form. b) Annotate all of the figures and tables in the results section by adding clarifying labels. The students will need to really study the figures and the legends in order to do this. They should be sure to clearly identify the control group and each experimental group in each figure and table. They should feel free to write on their copy of the sections when adding labels or even to make notes. Activity Instructions ctd: c) Ask students to write their own descriptive titles for each cartoon and each figure. The students should come up with new titles that are meaningful and informative to them. d) This process may need a few iterations to get the students on board. Small group in-class working sessions may help students through this process the initial time. They can get ideas from one another, but should complete the assignment individually. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 4- Elucidate the Hypothesis Activity Description: This activity continues breaking down Article 1 into accessible components. Here students use all the interpretive work they have done on the article sections to figure out what the actual hypothesis (or hypotheses) being tested is. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, reading primary scientific literature, analytical thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 1 class period (1-1.5 hours) Pre-activity: Review concept map, cartoons, figures and tables. Major Assignment: As a group, determine the hypothesis tested for each table and figure in the paper. Contribute justified arguments to the class discussion. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation and participation in class discussion Knowledge Goals: Using scientific information presented in a publication to determine the hypothesis of the study (which is not always clearly indicated in scientific literature). Formative- Hypotheses. Materials: Article 1 introduction, results, material and methods sections, concept maps, cartoons, table and figure legends and annotations, vocabulary notebook Activity Instructions: In this activity the students need bring all their hard work together and look for clues to the actual hypotheses being tested. Students should look at every figure or table given and identify the hypothesis that was tested. This should be done in class in small groups, followed up by a class discussion of each group’s ideas. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 5- Analyze and Interpret the Data Activity Description: This activity continues to analyze Article 1. Here students look at the data and determine for themselves what is it saying, forming their own conclusions about the research. This step is very enabling. It transforms the article from something that is taken as scientific fact because it has been published, to something that is simply one person’s (or a few people’s) point of view. The students will realize that they can be participators in science rather than being passive observers. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, reading primary scientific literature, critical scientific thinking Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to evaluate scientific literature and use critical thinking to form conclusions based on results and hypotheses, how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers Estimated Length of Activity: 2-3 classes (2- 4.5 hours) Pre-activity: Review hypotheses created in groups. Major Assignment: Using the hypotheses from Activity 4 and the CREATE analysis templates students will analyze the data results presented in each table and figure and draw their own conclusions. They will then be given the discussion section of the article containing the conclusions of the authors. They will make a list of the author’s conclusions and compare their conclusions to those of the authors Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation and participation in class discussions Formative- student conclusions, list of authors’ conclusions and completed analysis templates. Materials: Article 1 introduction, results, material and methods, and discussion sections, vocabulary notebook, CREATE analysis templates Activity Instructions: Using the CREATE analysis templates, the students will go through every single figure and table and analyze the results for themselves, drawing their own conclusions from the real data. 1. This activity should be done in small groups and the teacher should act as a facilitator of the discussion within each group, circling around the room. 2. Have students individually fill out a CREATE analysis template for each table and figure. 3. Students will compare each figure to data tables and/or other figures. They should then relate their findings to the hypothesis they had previously determined for each figure. Their findings should be analyzed and discussed among the group. 4. The students should then be able to form their own conclusion from the data. They should make a list of the conclusions drawn from each figure and table. Activity Instructions ctd: 5. Once their lists have been completed, the students should be provided with the Discussion section from the Journal article. 6. Ask the students to make a list of the conclusions made by the authors and compare it to their own conclusions. 7. If time allows, have everyone briefly discuss their findings as a class. Did they differ from the findings of the paper? Why or why not? Backyard Toxicology: Activity 6- Think of the Next Experiment Activity Description: This activity is a follow up to Article 1. Here students think about what the next step from this research is. They design an experiment that they feel would build on the research from Article 1 to ask a new question or test a different hypothesis. They then form Review Panels and review all of the proposals created by the students. This is similar to how funding agencies and journal reviewers operate in the world science research. The students are tasked with forming criteria and selecting the best proposal. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 1-2 classes (2- 3 hours) Pre-activity: If desired, assign students the cartooning activity for homework. Otherwise this can be done individually in class. Major Assignment: Building on Article 1, the students come up with a new research project proposal. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation and participation in class discussions Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, how to evaluate scientific research, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers Formative- Student proposals, group proposal criteria. Materials: Article 1, vocabulary notebook, white paper, markers, poster paper Activity Instructions: Direct students to take over the role as lead researcher and think of the next experiment they would do as a follow up to the journal article. 1. Every student should individually (for homework or in class) design the next experiment and draw a cartoon (with very minimal words) showing what the next experimental design/methods would be. 2. In class have students assemble in groups. Each group is now a Proposal Review Panel that will review the proposed experiments and decide which it will fund. 3. The first step of the review process is for each Panel to come up with a list of criteria for funding a successful project. What things would a project proposal need to show for the panel to approve of it? Activity Instructions ctd: 5. Next the Panels should review all of the experiments and decide on one to fund based on the criteria they established. 6. Each Panel should then share their criteria and their selection with the class. Students should realize that not only can research take off in a wide range of directions, but what makes a “good” experiments depends on how you look at it. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 7- Effect of Nicotine on Blackworms Activity Description: This activity is a follow up to Activity 1. Here students do the next step in the experiments to determine of the effects of toxicants on blackworms. This time after the experiments are concluded the students think of the next experiment and their peers review the proposals, like they did in Activity 6. The concept mapping and research proposal skills are reinforced in an authentic experiment. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 2 classes (2- 3 hours) Pre-activity: As a class, concept map ‘nicotine’ Major Assignment: Effects of nicotine on California Blackworms, research proposals of a follow-up experiment Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, how to evaluate scientific research, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers Formative- lab questions, student proposals (homework), group proposal criteria Materials: nicotine portion of Toxicants and Worms Lab Packet, results from Activity 1, California blackworms, probes, petri dishes, graduated cylinders, nicotine solutions, white paper, markers, poster paper Activity Instructions: Introduce the term ‘nicotine’ and ask the class to help make a concept map on the board as in Activity 1. **The teacher should prepare the nicotine solutions in advance for the Blackworm Lab, as in Activity 1 (see preparation instructions in the lab packet). 1. Ask the students to form groups and conduct the nicotine portion of the experiment from the Toxicants and Worms Lab Packet, and answer the questions based on their observations of the effects of nicotine and ethanol. They should compare and contrast the effects. Activity Instructions ctd: 5. For homework, ask the students to ‘think of the next experiment’ based on what they have done in the activity and make a cartoon diagram (just as they did in Activity 6). 6. The next experiments should be reviewed in panel, just as the students have already done (in Activity 6). Make sure the groups define their criteria for a successful project before reviewing the next experiments. 7. Share the group project selections and criteria as a class. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 8- CREATE Article 2 Activity Description: This activity is a repeat of Activities 26 using the second in a series of articles on the effects of ethanol on honeybees, written by a single research group. The goal is to scaffold the skills of CREATE while allowing the students to see the actual progression of scientific research in a laboratory. Estimated Length of Activity: 5-10 classes (5- 15 hours) Pre-activity: Process Skills Goals: teamwork, concept mapping, cartooning, critical thinking Major Assignment: CREATE process using Article 2 Knowledge Goals: how to read the results and methods sections of a scientific paper, visualize scientific research in the form of a cartoon, rephrase scientific jargon to get at the underlying information presented, use scientific information presented in a publication to determine the hypothesis of the study formulate scientific research projects, evaluate scientific research, engage in active scientific discussion with peers, evaluate scientific literature and use critical thinking to form conclusions based on results and hypotheses Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Formative- concept maps of introductions, cartooning results, table and figure captions and annotations, hypotheses, CREATE analysis templates, students conclusions, list of author conclusions, student proposals, group proposal criteria, vocabulary notebooks Materials: Article 2, CREATE analysis templates, white paper, poster paper, markers, vocabulary notebooks Activity Instructions: The teacher should gauge the participation level and enthusiasm of the class and assign portions of the CREATE process for homework accordingly. Refer to the Activities 2-6, and the Hoskins articles for detailed instructions on each step. C- Concept map the introduction R- Read the results, cartooning experiments, captioning and annotating tables and figures E- Elucidate the hypothesis (for each table and figure) A- Analyze and Interpret the data (using the CREATE Analysis template) TE- Think of the next Experiment- cartoon a proposal, Review Boards select the best one Alcohol 38 (2006) 179e183 Reduced ability of ethanol drinkers for social communication in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.) Janko Bozica,*, Charles I. Abramsonb, Mateja Bedencica a Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, Vecna pot 111, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia b Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74078, USA Received 30 April 2005; received in revised form 17 October 2005; accepted 27 January 2006 Abstract Foraging behavior was evaluated in honeybees trained to fly to a feeder containing sucrose only, 1% ethanol, 5% ethanol, or 10% ethanol. The results indicated that exposure to ethanol disrupted several types of honeybee social behavior within the hive. Consumption of ethanol at the feeding site reduced waggle dance activity in foraging bees and increased occurrence of tremble dance, food exchange, and self-cleaning behavior. These ethanol-induced changes in behavior may reflect effects on the central nervous system similar to the previously observed effects of food poisoning with sublethal doses of insecticides. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Honeybee; Waggle dance; Tremble dance; Apis mellifera; Social behavior; Ethanol 1. Introduction This is the sixth in a series of behavioral experiments testing the suitability of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) as an animal model for the study of alcoholism. Previous results from this laboratory have shown several alcoholrelated effects in bees that share properties in common with similar effects in humans. These include self-administration, disruption of learning, locomotion, and decision making, preferences for commercially available alcoholic beverages, the ability of an emetic to limit consumption of ethanol, and an increase in aggression (Abramson et al., 2000, 2003, 2004a, 2004b, 2005). This paper continues the search for common behavioral effects of ethanol on human and honeybee behavior by asking the question whether ethanol consumption influences the foraging honeybee behavior. It is known that consumption of ethanol in humans can cause cognitive dysfunction, aggression, and other abnormal social behavior (see recent papers Giancola, 2004; Herzog, 1999; Peirce et al., 2000). In the interest of further developing our social insect model, this study evaluated the influence of ethanol consumption on behavior of the foraging bee inside of the hive. The effects of ethanol consumption on the waggle dance and other * Corresponding author. Tel.: þ386-1-42-333-88; fax: þ386-1-25-73390. E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Bozic). 0741-8329/06/$ e see front matter Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.01.005 behaviors of foraging bees were assessed after they returned to the hive following a foraging trip. After a foraging trip, a honeybee shares collected nectar among nestmates by mouth-to-mouth food exchange, also called trophalaxis (Winston, 1987). Excited upon finding a nectar source, the foraging bee performs a waggle dance on the comb inside of the hive. This dance contains information about the location of a food source (von Frisch, 1965). Bees that follow the dancer are in the best position to pick up dance information (Bozic & Abramson, 2003). It is also known that bees that are disrupted at a feeding site will emit a tremble dance rather than a waggle dance. The tremble dance consists of irregular movements in all directions (Seeley, 1992). We expected that ethanol will affect waggle dance behavior and other related behavior inside of the hive during foraging activity. 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Procedure Honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.) were reared in a two frame observation hives during spring 2003. For 2 days, we individually marked potential foragers at the hive entrance using numbered tags. After tagging, we trained bees to forage to a feeder located 250 m from the hive. The feeders were custom made from six, 200 ml plastic 180 J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 38 (2006) 179e183 cups. This training took 1 day. The following morning we started the first day of actual experimental trials. In the morning of the first experimental day, bees were offered a 1.5 M sucrose solution. After a minimum of 10 marked bees were consistently returning to the feeder for 90 min, we switched the 1.5 M solution to a solution containing 10% (v/v) ethanol. The 10% solution remained in place until no marked bees appeared for 15 min. After this 15-min period, the 10% solution was removed and the feeder remained empty for 4 h. The 10% solution reduced the number of foraging visits so dramatically in a half an hour (last recorded visit of the marked bee in 25th min) that we were not able to continue with 10% test solution (no marked bee in next 15 min). The rationale behind the 4-h time period was to ensure that bees were not suffering aftereffects from the 10% solution when we switched to a new ethanol solution. Following the 4-h period, we renewed the feeder with 1.5 M sucrose and within 30 min the 10 marked bees were returning. The sucrose-only feeder remained in place for 60 min at which time we replaced the sucrose-only feeder with one containing 1% (v/v) ethanol. The 1% ethanol feeder remained in place for 90 minduntil 30 min before sundown. The experiment was continued the following day. On the second day of experiment, we started with a 1% ethanol solution for 60 min, then switched to a 5% (v/v) solution for 60 min, and finally returned to a sucrose-only solution. At the feeding site, we tried to remove all unmarked bees on the first day. On the second day, we could not do this because of the large number of unmarked foragers. The solution in the feeder was replaced as needed. At the feeding site, we recorded the time of arrival of any marked bee. Once inside the observation hive, we recorded the behavior of the marked bees with a Cannon MV600i miniDV camcorder. 2.2. Data analysis We calculated the number of visits, number of returns, and duration of returns for each feeder solution. For all marked bees, which were observed at the feeder and inside of the hive, we counted the number of trophalaxis encounters, waggle dances, tremble dances, self-cleaning, attending and following of the waggle dancers, and walking or resting on the comb inside of the hive in 30-min time periods. Return times between successive treatments at the feeding station were tested with one-way analysis of variance with post hoc multiple Tukey comparison at 0.05 significance level. The number of visits at the feeder between successive 30-min time periods was tested with c2 goodness-of-fit test. We compared counts of behavior inside of the hive with the number of visits at the feeder during the same time periods. Our hypothesis was that occurrence of observed behaviors inside of the hive was under influence of foraging visits. Possible independence of in-hive behavior counts was tested with two by two Fisher’s exact test, because many observed frequencies were less than five. The same test was used to evaluate independence of occurrence of waggle dance to other hive behavior. The waggle dance behavior is the only behavior that can be clearly related to the foraging visits between observed behaviors inside of the hive, and therefore was chosen for comparison with other behavior on the comb. All statistics were applied using SPSS for Windows 12.0. 3. Results 3.1. Foraging activity Foraging activity was significantly affected by the presence of ethanol in the feeding solution. Return time of the foragers (Fig. 1) was significantly affected by exposure to the ethanol solution in the feeder [F(6, 853) 5 7.33, P !.001], even by the 1% ethanol solution (Tukey multiple comparison test, P !.05). Ethanol exposure significantly increased return time of foragers when exposed to 1% ethanol solution at the feeder compared to solution containing no ethanol, and also when exposed to 5% ethanol solution at the feeder compared to 1% ethanol solution or to the solution containing no ethanol (Fig. 1). We were not able to show significantly longer time for 10% ethanol solution because only 10 bees returned for a total of 17 times to the feeder. We also observed that when exposed to 10% ethanol solution, some bees actually were not able to fly back into the hive. In contrast, foraging visits were affected only by 5% and 10% ethanol solution (Fig. 2). Significant decreases in number of visits were observed in 10% ethanol exposure on the first day (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 16.3, df 5 1, P !.001) and 5% ethanol on the next day (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 23.4, df 5 1, P !.001). We observed 55 visits in 30 min of feeding on sugar solution containing no ethanol before bees were exposed to 10% ethanol solution. We counted only 20 visits during feeding on ethanol solution in next 30 min. Actually, most visits (12 out of 20) occurred in the first 10 min. Due to the low frequency of visits, we stopped exposure to 10% ethanol after 30 min of data collection. When the feeder was removed for 4 h and subsequently filled with sucrose only, we counted 36 visits in next 30 min of data collection. This was significantly higher than that when the animals were exposed to the 10% ethanol solution (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 4.57, df 5 1, P 5.033; Fig. 2). In the next 30 min, we observed a significant increase of foraging activity, up to 72 visits (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 16.08, df 5 1, P !.001). On the next day, we observed a significant drop in visitsdfrom 36 in the first 30 min of foraging on 5% ethanol solution to only five visits in the next 30 min of feeding on 5% ethanol solution (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 23.4, df 5 1, P !.001). We were not able to observe any additional J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 38 (2006) 179e183 181 Fig. 1. Effect of ethanol on return time of 135 marked foragers. Generally, longer return times to the feeder were observed at higher concentrations of ethanol. There were significant differences between successive trials: 0e1%, 1e5%, and 5e1%. Star indicates significant Tukey comparison at P ! .05 for successive trials. At the top are indicated days, number of the bees, and number of returns to the feeder. The vertical line divides 2 observation days. visits of marked bees in the last 10 min of exposure to 5% ethanol solution. Bees regained normal foraging rate (25 visits) in the next 30 min after replacing 5% ethanol solution with sucrose only (goodness-of-fit test, c2 5 13.3, df 5 1, P !.001). 3.2. Behavior of foraging bees inside of the hive We observed quantitative and qualitative changes in behavior inside of the hive when bees foraged at different ethanol solutions. In general, ethanol reduced waggle dance activity and increased tremble dance activity, but it also affected other observed behavior patterns (Fig. 2). After exposure to the 10% ethanol solution, we observed a significant increase in frequency of trophalaxis, tremble dance, and self-cleaning when compared to the frequency of feeder visits (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001, for all three tests; Fig. 2). Frequency of tremble dance and self-cleaning also significantly increased relative to the foraging visits after exposure to 1% ethanol when bees were exposed to 5% ethanol solution on the second experimental day (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001 tremble dance, P 5 .027 self-cleaning). Frequency of tremble dance also significantly increased relative to the foraging visits between the 30-min periods during exposure to 5% ethanol solution (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001). The number of waggle dances decreased after application of ethanol. In the first exposure to 10% ethanol, no waggle dances were observed. That drop in frequency was correlated with the lower frequency of feeder visits (Fig. 2). On one other occasion, when bees were exposed to 5% ethanol after 1% ethanol solution, we observed a significant decrease in waggle dance frequency (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001; Fig. 2). When we replaced the ethanol solutions with a sucroseonly solution, the frequency of the tremble dance significantly decreased when compared to the number of visits (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001). Frequency of trophalaxis and self-cleaning significantly decreased only after exposure to 10% ethanol when compared to the number of visits (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001, both comparisons; Fig. 2). An increase of waggle dance activity compared to the number of feeder visits was observed after the 10% ethanol solution was replaced with sucrose only (Fisher’s exact test, P !.001; Fig. 2). The frequency of waggle dances also significantly increased when compared to the feeder visits between 30-min periods of the exposure to the 1% ethanol solution during the second experimental day (Fisher’s exact test, P 5.023). Diminishing waggle dance behavior after exposure to 10% and 5% ethanol solutions was always correlated with a significant increase of frequency of trophalaxis, tremble dance, self-cleaning, and walking-and-resting activities (Fisher’s exact test, P !.015 walking-and-resting at 5% ethanol, P !.001 all other cases; Fig. 2). When the 10% ethanol solution was replaced with sucrose only, the frequencies of the same behaviors to the values was similar to the frequencies observed before exposure to 10% ethanol (Fig. 2). After replacing the 5% ethanol solution with sucrose only, only the frequency of tremble dance significantly decreased when compared to the waggle dances (Fisher’s exact test, P 5 .038). When bees were exposed to a 1% ethanol solution after removal of the sucrose-only solution, the decrease in the frequency of the waggle dance was significantly negatively correlated with an increase of tremble dance frequency (Fisher’s exact test, P 5.007). Increase in the frequency of the waggle dance when exposed to 1% ethanol solution on the second day was negatively correlated with trophalaxis (Fisher’s exact test, P 5 .009) and self-cleaning (Fisher’s exact test, P 5 .004). 4. Discussion The most striking change in behavior following consumption of ethanol was a reduction of waggle dance activity and a corresponding increase in tremble dance activity (Fig. 2). Moreover, when bees reduced foraging activity, 182 J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 38 (2006) 179e183 Fig. 2. Frequencies of behaviors observed in 170 marked bees that were recorded at the feeder and in the hive. Increase of ethanol concentration in the feeding solution decreased frequency of the feeder visits, and decrease of ethanol had opposite effect. Diamonds mark significant differences between successive feeder concentrations, c2 goodness-of-fit test, P !.05. There were also observed significant changes of hive behaviors of foragers when compared to the foraging visits () or to the waggle dance activity of the foragers (stars), Fisher’s exact test, P !.05. At the top are indicated days and number of bees. The vertical line divides 2 observation days. The experimental trials were divided into equal half an hour periods to enable proper comparison of behavior counts. the foraging bees that returned stayed inside the hive for a longer time. This could explain our observed longer return times (Fig. 1) as well as the fact that our bees were having difficulty flying when exposed to ethanol. This was especially pronounced in bees that consumed a 10% ethanol solution. We also observed variations in tremble dance activity following exposure to ethanol. Of particular interest, bees were observed exchanging food during the tremble dance. This is the first time that food exchange during trembling has been reported in the literature. Bees are known to initiate the tremble dance in times of stress; for example, an J. Bozic et al. / Alcohol 38 (2006) 179e183 increase of tremble dance activity is induced in foraging bees after visiting a poisoned food source (Biesmeijer, 2003; Schmuck, 1999) or a feeder crowded with bees (Thom, 2003). Our study suggests that at least some levels of ethanol exposure may increase tremble dance activity due to increased stress conditions for the foraging bees, in a manner like the poisoned food source. As in our previous papers exploring the social insect model, it is emphasized that only in the honeybee can such a relatively large concentration of ethanol be tested in animals. Moreover, in our free flying experiment it is easy to overlook the fact that they are wild animals that are actually self-administering the ethanol solution. The wide repertoire of social behavior, learning and orientation capabilities of honeybees make them an excellent invertebrate model for the study of ethanol effects. In addition to what is known about honeybee physiology and behavior, a total genome library (http://www.hgsc.bcm.tmc.edu/projects/honeybee/) is available, complemented by increasing knowledge of honeybee functional genetics (Rueppell et al., 2004). Such a database will provide new opportunities for studying the molecular basis of ethanol-induced behavior of bees. Acknowledgments This research was funded by Slovenian Research Agency by project L1-5213 and research program P1-0184. References Abramson, C. I., Fellows, G. W., Browne, B. L., Lawson, A., & Ortez, R. A. (2003). The development of an ethanol model using social insects II: The effect of antabuse on consumatory responses and learned behavior of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Psychol Rep 92, 365–378. 183 Abramson, C. I., Kandolf, A., Sheridan, A., Donohue, D., Bozic, J., Meyers, J. E., & Benbassat, D. (2004a). The development of an ethanol model using social insects III: Preferences to ethanol solutions. Psychol Rep 94, 227–239. Abramson, C. I., Place, A. J., Aquino, I., & Fernandez, A. (2004b). Development of an ethanol model using social insects: IV Influence of ethanol on aggression of Africanised honey bees, Apis mellifera L. Psychol Rep 94, 107–115. Abramson, C. I., Sanderson, C., Painter, J., Barnett, S., & Wells, H. (2005). Development of an ethanol model using social insects V: Honey bee foraging decisions under the influence of alcohol. Alcohol 36, 187– 193. Abramson, C. I., Stone, S. M., Ortez, R. A., Luccardi, A., Vann, K. L., Hanig, K. D., & Rice, J. (2000). The development of an ethanol model using social insects I: Behavior studies of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24, 1153–1166. Biesmeijer, J. C. (2003). The occurrence and context of tremble dancing in free-foraging honey bees (Apis mellifera). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53, 411–416. Bozic, J., & Abramson, C. I. (2003). Following and attendingdtwo distinct behavior patterns of honeybees in a position to collect the dance information. Mellifera 3e6, 48–55. von Frisch, K. (1965). Tanzsprache und Orientirung der Bienen. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Verlag. Giancola, P. R. (2004). Executive functioning and alcohol-related aggression. J Abnorm Psychol 113, 541–555. Herzog, T. A. (1999). Effects of alcohol intoxication on social inferences. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 7, 448–453. Peirce, R. S., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., Cooper, M. L., & Mudar, P. (2000). A longitudinal model of social contact, social support, depression, and alcohol use. Health Psychol 19, 28–38. Rueppell, O., Pankiw, T., Nielsen, D. I., Fondrk, M. K., Beye, M., & Page, R. E. Jr. (2004). The genetic architecture of the behavioral ontogeny of foraging in honeybee workers. Genetics 167, 1767–1779. Schmuck, R. (1999). No causal relationship between Gaucho seed dressing in sunflowers and the French bee syndrome. Pflanzenschutz-Nachr Bayer 52, 256–299. Seeley, T. D. (1992). The tremble dance of the honey bee: messages and meanings. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 31, 375–383. Thom, C. (2003). The tremble dance of honey bees can be caused by hiveexternal foraging experience. J Exp Biol 206, 2111–2116. Winston, M. L. (1987). The Biology of the Honey Bee. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 9- Effect of Caffeine on Blackworms Activity Description: This activity is a follow up to Activities 1 and 7. Here students go on to the next step in the blackworm experiments, and determine of the effects of caffeine. As in Activity 7, after the experiments are concluded the students think of the next experiment and their peers review the proposals. The concept mapping and research proposal skills are again reinforced in an authentic experiment. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 2 classes (2- 3 hours) Pre-activity: As a class, concept map ‘caffeine’ Major Assignment: effects of caffeine on California Blackworms, research proposals on a follow-up experiment Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, how to evaluate scientific research, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers Formative- lab questions, student proposals (homework), group proposal criteria Materials: caffeine portion of Toxicants and Worms Lab Packet, results from Activities 1 and 7, California blackworms, probes, petri dishes, graduated cylinders, caffeine solutions, white paper, markers, poster paper Activity Instructions: Introduce the term ‘caffeine’ and ask the class to help make a concept map on the board as in Activity 1. **The teacher should prepare the caffeine solutions in advance for the Blackworm Lab, as in Activity 1 (see preparation instructions in the lab packet). 1. Ask the students to form groups and conduct the caffeine portion of the experiment from the Toxicants and Worms Lab Packet, and answer the questions based on their observations of the effects of caffeine, nicotine and ethanol. They should compare and contrast the effects. Activity Instructions ctd: 5. For homework, ask the students to ‘think of the next experiment’ based on what they have done in the activity and make a cartoon diagram. 6. The next experiments should be reviewed in panel. Make sure the groups define their criteria for a successful project before reviewing the next experiments. 7. Share the group project selections and criteria as a class. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 10- CREATE Article 3 Activity Description: This activity is a repeat of Activities 26 using the third in a series of articles on the effects of ethanol on honeybees, written by a single research group. The goal is to scaffold the skills of CREATE while allowing the students to see the actual progression of scientific research in a laboratory. Estimated Length of Activity: 5 classes (5- 7.5 hours) Pre-activity: Process Skills Goals: teamwork, concept mapping, cartooning, critical thinking Major Assignment: CREATE process using Article 3 Knowledge Goals: how to read the results and methods sections of a scientific paper, visualize scientific research in the form of a cartoon, rephrase scientific jargon to get at the underlying information presented, use scientific information presented in a publication to determine the hypothesis of the study formulate scientific research projects, evaluate scientific research, engage in active scientific discussion with peers, evaluate scientific literature and use critical thinking to form conclusions based on results and hypotheses Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Formative- concept maps of introductions, cartooning results, table and figure captions and annotations, hypotheses, CREATE analysis templates, students conclusions, list of author conclusions, student proposals, group proposal criteria, vocabulary notebooks Materials: Article 3, CREATE analysis templates, white paper, poster paper, markers, vocabulary notebooks Activity Instructions: The teacher should gauge the participation level and enthusiasm of the class and assign portions of the CREATE process for homework accordingly. Progressively more homework than Atricle 2. Refer to the Activities 2-6, and the Hoskins articles for detailed instructions on each step. C- Concept map the introduction R- Read the results, cartooning experiments, captioning and annotating tables and figures E- Elucidate the hypothesis (for each table and figure) A- Analyze and Interpret the data (using the CREATE Analysis template) TE- Think of the next Experiment- cartoon a proposal, Review Boards select the best one Psychological Reports, 2010, 106, 3, 1-17. © Psychological Reports 2010 THE BEHAVIOR AND SOCIAL COMMUNICATION OF HONEY BEES (APIS MELLIFERA CARNICA POLL.) UNDER THE INFLUENCE OF ALCOHOL1, 2 T. ANDREW MIXSON AND CHARLES I. ABRAMSON Oklahoma State University JANKO BOŽIČ University of Ljubljana Summary.—In this study, the effects of ethanol on honey bee social communication and behavior within the hive were studied to further investigate the usefulness of honey bees as an ethanol-abuse model. Control (1.5 M sucrose) and experimental (1.5 M sucrose, 2.5% w/v ethanol) solutions were directly administered to individual forager bees via proboscis contact with glass capillary tubes. The duration, frequency, and proportion of time spent performing social and nonsocial behaviors were the dependent variables of interest. No differences in the relative frequency or proportion of time spent performing the target behaviors were observed. However, ethanol consumption significantly decreased bouts of walking, resting, and the duration of trophallactic (i.e., food-exchange) encounters. The results of this study suggest that a low dose of ethanol is sufficient to disrupt both social and nonsocial behaviors in honey bees. In view of these results, future behavioral-genetic investigations of honey bee social behavior are encouraged. Ethyl alcohol, or ethanol, is best known for its role as the intoxicating agent in alcoholic beverages. In humans, impairments of memory and learning (Moulton, Petros, Apostal, Park, Ronning, King, et al., 2005; Soderlund, Parker, Schwartz, & Tulving, 2005; Mintzer, 2007), visual acuity and depth perception (Watten & Lie, 1996; Nawrot, Nordenstrom, & Olson, 2004; Souto, Bezerra, & Halsey, 2008), fine and gross motor skills (McNaughton & Preece, 1986; Breckenridge & Berger, 1990; Marczinski, Harrison, & Fillmore, 2008), and decision-making abilities (Field, Christiansen, Cole, & Goudie, 2007; Phillips & Ogeil, 2007; Stoner, George, Peters, & Norris, 2007) are common short-term effects of acute ethanol consumption. Long-term ethanol consumption places individuals at an elevated risk of several chronic health problems including hepatic damage (Ekstedt, Franzén, Holmqvist, Bendsten, Mathiesen, Bodemar, et al., 2009; Hatton, Burton, Nash, Munn, Burgoyne, & Sheron, 2009), pancreatiAddress correspondence to Dr. Charles I. Abramson, Laboratory of Comparative Psychology and Behavioral Biology, Departments of Psychology and Zoology, Oklahoma State 116 N. Murray, Stillwater, OK 74078 or e-mail ([email protected]). 2 Mr. Mixson was supported by a Lew Wentz Foundation Research Project Award. The authors thank Dr. James W. Grice for his suggestions and comments regarding the statistical analyses. 1 DOI 10.2466/PR0.106.3. ISSN 0033-2941 2 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. tis (Webster, 1975), and sexual dysfunction (Mandell & Miller, 1983; Cocores, Miller, Pottash, & Gold, 1988; Grinshpoon, Margolis, Weizman, & Ponizovsky, 2007). Alcohol consumption during pregnancy can also damage the developing human fetus. The Centers for Disease Control (2005) estimate that the fetal alcohol syndrome rate in the United States ranges from 0.2 to 1.5 per 1,000 live births. The effects of alcohol abuse on society are deep-reaching. Annually claiming an estimated 79,000 lives, excessive alcohol use constitutes the third leading lifestyle-related cause of death in the United States (Centers for Disease Control, 2008). According to the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2008), nearly one-third of all traffic fatalities in 2006 and 2007 were related to alcohol-impaired driving. Moreover, toxicology testing conducted by the Centers for Disease Control revealed that one-third of those who committed suicide in 2003 tested positive for alcohol (Karch, Crosby, & Simon, 2006). Social pressures from friends and peers play a key role in the prevalence of alcohol abuse and underage drinking, which suggests that some individuals may be at risk of forming a dependence on alcohol at an early age (Graham, Marks, & Hansen, 1991). The above-mentioned problems represent only a small fraction of the total number of public health issues that stem from alcohol misuse. However, the experimental study of ethanol’s effects on human health and behavior is limited by obvious ethical and moral constraints (Dolinsky & Babor, 1997). This is problematic in the development of effective treatments and recovery programs for alcoholics. As a result, researchers have extensively developed invertebrate models to study the pharmacology of alcohol tolerance, dependence, and abuse. Research has demonstrated that the molecular architecture of invertebrate and vertebrate nervous systems share several similarities (Abramson, Sanderson, Painter, Barnett, & Wells, 2005). For example, both types of organisms have ligand- and voltage-gated ion channels, and Gprotein coupled receptors (Harris, 1999; Holmes, Barhoumi, Nachman, & Pietrantonio, 2003). Studies focusing on the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, and the roundworm, Caenorhabditis elegans, have made impressive strides in elucidating the metabolic pathways implicated in ethanolinduced intoxication. Results generated from research with these model organisms have also demonstrated high symmetry with findings from vertebrate studies (Lovinger & Crabbe, 2005). For example, Singh and Heberlein (2000) demonstrated that the concentrations of ethanol required to induce locomotor behaviors in D. melanogaster are similar to those described in humans. On a molecular level, cAMP-associated signaling proteins have been implicated in effects related to acute and chronic ethanol exposure in both fruit flies and mice (Moore, DeZazzo, Luk, Tully, HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 3 Singh, & Heberlein, 1998; Thiele, Willis, Stadler, Reynolds, Bernstein, & McKnight, 2000). Work with C. elegans has also improved understanding of the genetics of alcohol-induced effects on behavior. Davies, Pierce-Shimomura, Kim, VanHoven, Thiele, Bonci, et al. (2003) discovered that ethanol sensitivity in C. elegans is dependent on neuronal expression of the slo-1 gene. This gene derives its name from the slowpoke ortholog gene in D. melanogaster, which is known to encode a large conductance calcium-activated BK potassium channel (Wang, Saifee, Nonet, & Salkoff, 2001; Davies, et al., 2003). In C. elegans, slo-1 loss-of-function mutations result in ethanol resistance, while slo-1 gain-of-function mutations result in the depression of locomotion and egg-laying behaviors (see Davies, et al., 2003). Several studies have demonstrated that mammalian BK potassium channels are also sensitive to ethanol (Dopico, Anantharam, & Treistman, 1998; Gruss, Henrich, König, Hempelmann, Vogel, & Scholz, 2001). Interestingly, BK potassium channels also mediate muscle contraction, neurotransmitter release, and hormonal secretion in mammals (Sah, 1996; Davies, et al., 2003). It should be noted that the above-mentioned studies represent only a small fraction of the massive amount of alcohol research conducted with D. melanogaster and C. elegans. These studies highlight the usefulness of a simple systems approach in elucidating the mechanisms that mediate ethanol’s various biochemical, genetic, and molecular-level modes of action. Continued research with these organisms is sure to involve the discovery of many other parallels between mammalian and invertebrate nervous systems. Nevertheless, research with these organisms is somewhat limited to fairly simple motor responses (Wolf & Heberlein, 2003). This shortcoming arguably reduces the comparative value of behavioral research conducted with these organisms. At first glance, the honey bee may seem an unlikely organism for alcohol research. However, in contrast to the abovementioned invertebrate ethanol models, honey bees have a very expansive behavioral repertoire that can be analyzed following acute or chronic ethanol consumption (Wolf & Heberlein, 2003; Abramson, et al., 2005). Much is also known about the natural history, physiology, genetics, and behavior of this species. Given the social structure and caste differentiation of honey bees, foraging workers rely heavily on social behaviors such as the waggle dance to relay information about nearby food sources (von Frisch, 1965). This complex social structure and division of labor exhibited by the honey bee has also garnered much attention from behavioral geneticists (Ruppell, Pankiw, & Page, 2004; Whitfield, Cziko, & Robinson, 2003). Indeed, considerable advances in the understanding of the genetic mechanisms which underlie honey bee foraging and nest-defense behaviors have been made 4 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. recently (Giray & Robinson, 1996; Ruppell, et al., 2004; Hunt, Amdam, Schlipalius, Emore, Sardesai, Williams, et al., 2007; Alaux, Sinha, Hasadsri, Hunt, Guzman-Novoa, DeGrandi-Hoffman, et al., 2009). Despite the novelty of eusocial insects, it should be emphasized that the social structure of honey bees is markedly different from the social nature of humans. Cross-species comparisons regarding the social nature of individual humans and honey bees indeed warrant cautious interpretation. This seemingly limits the value of studying ethanol’s effects on honey bee behavior. However, it should be noted that an important facet of eusocial insect life is that the behavior of individuals is exceedingly simplified, compared to the behavioral repertoire of solitary insects (Oster & Wilson, 1978). As a result, the experimenter is provided with added experimental control that is not available when working with organisms that exhibit a higher prevalence of idiosyncratic differences in behavior. This should not imply, however, that similarities do not exist between humans and honey bees, and that informative cross-species comparisons cannot be made. For example, it has been demonstrated that ethanol induces aggression and the disruption of locomotive, learning, and decision-making abilities in both humans and honey bees (Abramson, Stone, Ortez, Luccardi, Vann, Hanig, et al., 2000; Abramson, Place, Aquino, & Fernandez, 2004; Abramson, et al., 2005; Božič, Abramson, & Bendencic, 2006). Like humans, honey bees also demonstrate self-administration of ethanol and exhibit preferences for commercially available alcoholic beverages (Abramson, Kandolf, Sheridan, Donohue, Božič, Meyers, et al., 2004). Moreover, Antabuse® (disulfarim) is also capable of limiting ethanol consumption in honey bees (Abramson, Fellows, Browne, Lawson, & Ortiz, 2003). Despite these interesting findings, knowledge of ethanol’s effects on the relative frequency and duration of social and nonsocial behaviors within the colony is rudimentary. Previous work has established that a 5% ethanol solution significantly reduces the foraging activity of worker bees, with a 1% ethanol solution causing an increase in the return time of foragers from the food source to the hive (Božič, et al., 2006). Significant increases in the frequency of trophallaxis, tremble dancing, and self-cleaning within the hive occur relative to foraging visits from artificial feeders containing increasing concentrations of ethanol. That is, the frequency of these behaviors increases with increasing concentrations of ethanol up to 10%. The same study revealed that the frequency of waggle dancing is negatively correlated with the consumption of increasing concentrations of ethanol. The primary aim of this study was to expand current knowledge of ethanol’s effects on honey bee behavior by identifying its actions on the following three dependent variables, i.e., the measures of individual honey bee behavior listed in Ta- HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 5 ble 1: (1) the relative frequency of occurrence of behaviors, (2) the overall proportion of time spent performing each type of behavior, and (3) the duration of individual behavior displays. Given the results of previous research, it was hypothesized that relative to sucrose-treated foragers, ethanol-treated foragers will undergo a decrease in the magnitude of each of the abovementioned measures of those behaviors listed in Table 1. Method Subjects Carniolan honey bees (Apis mellifera carnica) were reared in a twoframe observation hive during May 2008 at the University of Ljubljana, Slovenia. Forager bees (N = 400) were captured at the hive entrance, marked with numbered identification tags, and reintroduced into the hive. Application of the ID tags was accomplished by capturing individual bees at the hive entrance with a plastic vial as they flew out of the hive. Each bee was then placed under a plastic mesh screen, a small amount of adhesive was applied to the thorax using a toothpick, and a plastic identification tag measuring about 4 mm in diameter was immediately applied to the adhesive. Identification tags of various colors with numbers ranging from 1 to 99 were utilized in this study. A small amount of pressure was applied to the tag with the toothpick for approximately 15 sec. to allow the adhesive to cure. After applying the identification tag, the plastic mesh screen was removed, and the bees were released. These tags can be easily acquired from a company that sells beekeeping equipment, as beekeepers typically use them to aid in visually identifying the queen. Experiments did not begin until two days following the application of all identification tags. The behaviors of 26 individual marked bees (control: n = 13; experimental: n = 13) bees observed. This small sample resulted from the fact that individual bees were not included in the experiment unless they were observed to successfully follow a waggle dance. Typically, bees located around a waggle dancer will exhibit attending or following the dance. Following is distinguished from attending, in that the former requires the bee to follow the dancer’s abdomen for two complete turns of the figure-eight waggle dance (see Božič & Abramson, 2003). This stringent criterion was utilized to ensure that the bees included in the experiment were in fact forager bees, and not nurse bees. Both forager and nurse bees are colloquially referred to as worker bees, but worker bees do not typically leave the hive to forage for pollen and nectar until they are about 20+ days old (Seeley, 1982). Failure to control for age-related differences in behavior would result in ambiguous results and make it nearly impossible to attribute any experimental effects to the ethanol treatment. 6 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. Measures The behaviors listed in Table 1 were recorded for 1 hr. with a Canon MV600i miniDV camcorder for each of the experimental and control group subjects. The postfeeding videos were analyzed to document the frequency and duration (in sec.) of the 14 target behaviors described in Table 1. Procedures Following a waggle dance, marked foragers were fed either a control (1.5 M sucrose) or experimental (2.5% w/v ethanol in 1.5 M sucrose) solution via direct proboscis contact with 100 mm glass capillary tubes containing the solutions. A 2.5% (w/v) ethanol concentration in 1.5 M sucrose was utilized because it is known from previous work that a 5% ethanol solution will significantly disrupt honey bee behavior both under laboratory and field conditions (Božič, et al., 2006). Furthermore, ethanol concentrations equal to or greater than 10% severely inhibit movement and foraging activity (Božič, et al., 2006), while a 5% ethanol solution significantly impairs Pavlovian conditioning of the proboscis extension reflex (Abramson, et al., 2000). In other words, the 2.5% w/v concentration was selected to further specify the alcohol tolerance threshold on honey bee behavior and to prevent a severe disruption of behavior that would otherwise occur with a higher ethanol concentration. This procedure took place for a 1-hr. period, and each bee was fed for approximately 2 sec. or until it stopped consuming the solution as indicated by retraction of the proboscis, whichever occurred first. It should be noted that this methodology did not allow measurement of the total volume of liquid imbibed by each bee. However, each individual was allowed to imbibe the control or experimental solution ad libitum after it was placed into contact with the proboscis. This is a unique aspect of the experimental design, in that it minimizes experimenter interaction with the hive and enhances the value of the experiment as a field study. In other words, by requiring individual bees to consume a fixed volume of the control or experimental solution, excessive disruption of the naturally occurring behaviors of the subject would have occurred. The use of capillary tubes to administer the solutions also allowed selective treatment of the control and experimental group subjects with the proper solutions. Moreover, from a comparative perspective, it is critical to remember that humans also exhibit individual differences in the amount of alcohol they will consume voluntarily in a given period of time (Weafer & Fillmore, 2008). Previous work with harnessed honey bees revealed that ethanol concentrations in the hemolymph approached 23 mM approximately 30 min. after bees consumed 10 μL of a 2.5% ethanol solution in 1.5 M sucrose (Božič, et al., 2006); for resting periods longer than 1 hr., blood ethanol Swarm chain Trophallaxis Trail follow Tremble dance Attending tremble dance Waggle dancing Attending a waggle dance Following SC FE TF TD ATD D A F RS Resting In cell Self-cleaning Walking Walking after following a waggle dance Resting in swarm Behavior R INC CL W WF Abbreviation Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No No No No Social Description Bee remains stationary. Bee deposits nectar or pollen in a comb cell. Bee removes pollen particles from its body. Bee traverses the hive. Walking activity that occurs after a bee follows a waggle dance; the interval of time that elapses before the bee exits the hive after following a waggle dance. Bee remains stationary but clings to other bees with its legs as they traverse the hive. Bee interlocks its legs with bees above and below it. Bee shares nectar with another bee via proboscis contact. Bee follows the walking path of another bee. Irregular movements in all directions. Bee attends a tremble dance. Bee performs a waggle dance. Bee attends a waggle dance. Bee follows a waggle dance (bee must follow the dancer for two complete cycles of the figure-eight movement). TABLE 1 Behavior Descriptions and Abbreviation Key HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 7 8 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. concentrations remained relatively stable for at least 8 hr. following ethanol ingestion. Immediately following the 1-hr. feeding period, the hive was filmed for 1 hr. with a Canon MV600i miniDV camcorder. A total of seven feeding and postfeeding trials were conducted. To control for calendar variables, bees from both groups were included in each feeding trial. No more than two feeding trials and observation periods were conducted each day, in order to keep from compounding the honey bees’ blood ethanol concentrations. The postfeeding videos were analyzed to document the frequency and duration of the 14 target behaviors described in Table 1. Analysis The above-mentioned measures of the behaviors identified in Table 1 were analyzed with the nonparametric, Mann-Whitney U statistical test to identify significant differences between control (sucrose-treated) and experimental (ethanol-treated) group subjects. Data were analyzed with this statistic, because an exploratory data analysis revealed that most of the three measures of the behaviors listed in Table 1 violated assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance. Following the recommendations of Field (2005, p. 532), effect size estimates for the Mann-Whitney U test were calculated by converting the z-score reported by SPSS into an effect size estimate, r. To make quantitative comparisons across the three measures of the behaviors listed in Table 1 possible, sample sizes, z-scores, and effect size estimates are reported in addition to the U statistics and p values in Tables 2, 3, and 4. All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS 16.0 for Windows. Results The following measures of each social and nonsocial behavior listed in Table 1 were analyzed for each individual bee: (1) the relative frequency of occurrence of each behavior, (2) the proportion of total time spent performing each behavior, and (3) the duration of each individual behavioral display. Individual behavior displays (N = 994; control: n = 486; experimental: n = 508) were observed. The duration of each of these behavioral displays was also recorded, yielding a total of 11.4 hr. of behavioral data. Ethanol-treated foragers did not differ from sucrose-treated foragers in terms of their relative frequency (see Table 2) or proportion of total time (see Table 3) spent performing each of the target behaviors listed in Table 1. However, compared to sucrose-treated foragers (Mdn = 32.5 sec.), ethanol-treated foragers (Mdn = 14.5 sec.) had significantly shorter instances of resting (U = 534.5, p < .001, r = .35, two-tailed test). Ethanol-treated foragers also spent significantly less time per instance of walking (Mdn = 31.9 sec.) than did sucrose-treated foragers (Mdn = 33.8 sec.; U = 23289.5, p = .037, r = .10, two-tailed test). In other words, although sucrose- and ethanol- C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E C E R 0 0 .02 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 .41 .42 0 0 0 0 0 0 Min. 0 0 .06 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 .03 .04 .08 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 .43 .47 0 0 0 .03 Q1 0 0 .04 0 .09 .10 0 0 0 0 0 0 .07 .06 .13 .09 0 0 0 0 .02 0 .45 .47 0 0 .05 .03 0 .03 Median .13 .20 .04 .08 0 0 0 0 0 .08 .10 .17 .14 .01 0 0 0 0 .15 .07 .08 .11 .01 .01 .47 .52 .04 .05 Q3 .23 .25 .17 .12 0 .24 .20 .44 .16 .07 .05 .75 .75 .08 .07 .06 .03 .08 .04 .16 .14 .25 .20 .03 .21 .04 .16 .07 Max. n 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 Z r .20 −1.00 −1.26 .25 .22 .01 −.06 −1.13 .03 .09 −.44 −.16 .11 .11 −.56 −.57 .03 .16 .37 .04 .26 .15 −.16 −.81 −1.90 −.19 −1.33 −.75 U 61.5 62.5 78.0 84.0 82.0 76.0 73.5 77.0 82.5 69.5 47.5 81.5 59.0 71.0 p .207 .258 .317 .956 .870 .663 .567 .576 .870 .418 .058 .851 .184 .451 Note.—C = control group subjects (1.5 M sucrose); E = experimental group subjects [1.5 M sucrose, 2.5% (w/v) ethanol]; Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; Q1 = first quartile (25%), Q3 = third quartile (75%); n = number of bees: control = 13, experimental = 13; Z = z statistic reported by SPSS; r = effect size estimate. Refer to Table 1 for behavior key. All minimum, median, maximum, and quartile values are reported as frequencies. F A D ATD TD TF FE SC RS WF W CL INC Group Behavior TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics and Mann-Whitney U Results of the Relative Frequency of Occurrence of Each Behavior HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 9 Group Min. Q1 Median Q3 Max. n Z r U p C 0 0 .01 .12 .33 13 −.70 .14 72.0 .485 E 0 0 0 .08 .27 13 INC C 0 0 .02 .03 .26 13 −.81 .16 69.0 .420 E 0 0 .01 .03 .15 13 CL C 0 0 0 0 .01 13 −.19 .04 81.5 .851 E 0 0 0 0 .02 13 W C .41 .55 .81 .84 .99 13 −1.00 .20 65.0 .317 E .60 .73 .84 .93 .97 13 WF C 0 0 0 0 .04 13 −.86 .17 68.5 .388 E 0 0 0 .02 .10 13 RS C 0 0 0 0 .08 13 −.08 .02 83.5 .935 E 0 0 0 0 .06 13 SC C 0 0 0 0 .26 13 −.63 .12 76.0 .526 E 0 0 0 0 .11 13 FE C 0 0 0 .01 .03 13 −.39 .08 77.0 .696 E 0 0 .01 .01 .03 13 TF C 0 0 .02 .03 .07 13 −.08 .02 83.0 .939 E 0 .01 .01 .02 .05 13 TD C 0 0 0 0 .01 13 −.10 .02 83.0 .922 E 0 0 0 0 .03 13 ATD C 0 0 0 0 .01 13 −.06 .01 84.0 .956 E 0 0 0 0 .04 13 D C 0 0 0 0 .03 13 −1.00 .20 78.0 .317 E 0 0 0 0 0 13 A C 0 .01 .02 .03 .06 13 −.39 .08 77.0 .700 E 0 .01 .02 .03 .06 13 F C 0 0 0 .02 .08 13 −1.5 .30 57.5 .132 E 0 0 .02 .04 .06 13 Note.—C = control (1.5 M sucrose) group subjects; E = experimental [1.5 M sucrose, 2.5% (w/v) ethanol] group subjects; Q1 = first quartile (25%), Q3 = third quartile (75%); n = number of bees: control = 13, experimental = 13; Z = z statistic reported by SPSS; r = effect size estimate. Refer to Table 1 for behavior key. All minimum, median, maximum, and quartile values are reported as proportions. R Behavior TABLE 3 Descriptive Statistics and Mann-Whitney U Results of the Overall Proportion of Time Spent Performing Each Behavior 10 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. Behavior Group Min. Q1 Median Q3 Max. n Z r U p C 4.43 19.8 32.5 58.4 668 45 −3.22 .35 534.5 .001† E 1.13 5.04 14.5 41.7 285 40 INC C .460 1.53 3.85 6.21 88.2 32 −.35 .04 455.0 .725 E .900 1.89 3.19 13.3 53.6 30 CL C 1.07 2.14 5.97 6.93 10.0 7 −1.43 .40 11.0 .153 E 3.23 6.02 8.17 16.0 20.0 6 W C .240 14.5 33.8 81.9 583 216 −2.08 .10 23,290 .037* E .533 7.18 31.9 70.9 1686 243 WF C 1.77 6.07 7.78 10.5 31.9 8 −.62 .15 33.0 .534 E 1.27 6.12 10.4 22.3 138 10 RS C 4.86 23.9 30.0 104 130 6 −.33 .12 5.00 .739 E 8.67 51.2 93.7 136 179 2 SC C 27.8 89.3 128 273 522 7 −1.25 .40 5.00 .210 E 22.4 23.9 25.3 169 313 3 FE C .200 1.63 3.53 10.8 43.8 37 −1.94 .24 416.0 .052 E .267 1.03 2.33 4.68 22.9 31 TF C .960 3.35 6.52 8.99 26.2 54 −.13 .01 1,170 .895 E 1.73 3.90 5.90 9.73 16.8 44 TD C .700 2.75 4.70 9.08 18.4 4 −1.43 .25 32.0 .151 E 1.07 1.50 1.92 2.23 7.0 29 ATD C 10.7 10.7 10.7 10.7 10.7 1 −.66 .25 2.00 .513 E 2.13 2.48 4.10 9.72 17.2 7 D C 37.3 37.3 37.3 37.3 37.3 1 na na na na E 0 0 0 0 0 0 A C 1.50 5.27 8.12 12.3 45.6 50 −.12 .01 911.5 .908 E 2.40 4.73 7.57 10.7 27.7 37 F C 6.36 13.8 17.8 23.6 39.2 10 −.44 .08 90.0 .660 E 5.10 10.9 18.1 24.8 108 20 Note.—C = control group subjects (1.5 M sucrose); E = experimental group subjects [1.5 M sucrose, 2.5% (w/v) ethanol]; Q1 = first quartile (25%), Q3 = third quartile (75%); na = not available; n = behavior frequency (the total number of individual behavior displays produced by each of the thirteen bees in each group); Z = z statistic reported by SPSS; r = effect size estimate. Number of bees included in experiment: control = 13, experimental = 13. Refer to Table 1 for behavior key. All minimum, median, maximum, and quartile values are reported in seconds. The U- and p-values of dance (D) behavior could not be computed because only a single instance of this behavior occurred. *p < .05. †p < .01. R TABLE 4 Descriptive Statistics and Mann-Whitney U Results of the Duration of Individual Behavior Displays HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 11 12 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. treated foragers did not differ in the total amount of time walking or resting, when the inebriated forager bees walked or rested, they did so for relatively shorter periods of time. The mean duration of individual instances of trophallaxis between control and experimental group subjects was not significant, but of substantial effect size; ethanol-treated subjects displayed shorter bouts of food exchanges with hive mates (Mdn = 2.33 sec.) than did sucrose-treated subjects (Mdn = 3.53 sec.; U = 416, p = .052, r = .24, two-tailed test). Additional Mann-Whitney U tests revealed that the lengths of individual displays of the remaining behaviors did not differ between control and experimental group bees (see Table 4). Discussion No significant effects of ethanol on the relative frequency or proportion of time spent performing each of the target behaviors were observed (see Tables 2 and 3). This null effect may be attributable to insufficient sample sizes for both the control and experimental groups. It should also be noted that only a single instance of waggle dancing was observed by a marked forager in the 11.4 hr. of behavioral data collected from the postfeeding video recordings. This low rate of waggle dancing is indicative of depressed foraging activity that may have been induced by weather and seasonality. Negative results are never appealing; however, the lack of any effect on the relative frequency or proportion of time spent performing the target behaviors minimally implies that ethanol treatment does not significantly disrupt the division of labor within the hive. That is, the forager bees included in the experiment continued to perform similar activities within the hive regardless of whether they consumed ethanol. However, several significant effects were identified when considering the length of individual behavior displays. For example, the duration of bouts of walking and resting were significantly lower in the experimental group subjects (see Table 4). These shorter resting and walking behavior events suggest a higher frequency of behavior changes in ethanol-treated bees. These results also suggest that a low dose of ethanol stimulates mobility and movement in honey bees, as indicated by the decreased duration of resting and walking displays in the ethanol-treated subjects. Interestingly, lower doses of ethanol stimulate walking speed in Drosophila, or fruit flies, while higher doses reduce movement and mobility (Wolf & Heberlein, 2003). Previous work has demonstrated that high doses of ethanol also reduce movement and mobility in honey bees (Abramson, et al., 2000). The concentrations of ethanol required to stimulate locomotion and reduce coordination in Drosophila are remarkably similar to those required to cause the same effects in humans (Singh & Heberlein, 2000). HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 13 In terms of social behavior, previous research demonstrated a significant overall increase in trophallactic activity when compared to feeder visits in a colony that was allowed to forage freely from a 10% ethanol solution (Božič et al., 2006). In the present study, bees fed a 2.5% ethanol solution did not exhibit an increase in the frequency or proportion of time spent engaged in trophallactic encounters. Nevertheless, experimentalgroup (i.e., ethanol-treated) subjects did perform shorter food exchanges with surrounding bees relative to control subjects (see Table 4). Hence, it appears that a low dose of ethanol is sufficient to also induce a higher frequency of change in the display of social behaviors. This result does warrant great caution in interpretation, as other social behaviors including waggle dancing, attending, and following were not affected in the present study following ethanol consumption. This finding does indicate, however, that the social behaviors of honey bees are differentially affected by ethanol. This could be attributed to the likely explanation that different social behaviors in honey bees are controlled by the expression of different sets of genes and gene products. Much of what is known about the genetic bases of ethanol tolerance and metabolism in invertebrates has been elucidated by research efforts focusing on D. melanogaster and C. elegans as ethanol models. However, the behavioral repertoire of these organisms is limited to simple motor responses (Wolf & Heberlein, 2003; Abramson, et al., 2005) and these species lack a complex social structure and related social behaviors. In this respect, the honey bee (Apis mellifera) has the potential to complement behavioralgenetic studies with Drosophila and C. elegans, especially in the study of ethanol’s effects on the genetic underpinnings of socially-related behaviors. Several genes implicated in the actions of ethanol have been mostly conserved between arthropods and vertebrates (Heberlein, Wolf, Rothenflush, & Guarnieri, 2004). Given the conservative nature of these genes, future investigations of the molecular-genetic and neural bases of the ethanol-induced behaviors observed in the present study are warranted. A total genome library of the honey bee is available online at http://www.hgsc.bcm.tmc.edu/projects/honeybee/. Coupled with increasing knowledge of the functional genetics of D. melanogaster and C. elegans, such a database will provide new opportunities to elucidate the molecular bases of ethanol-induced behaviors in the honey bee. This type of future research is absolutely critical if cross-species comparisons of the effects of acute ethanol intoxication on social behaviors are to be made between humans and honey bees. Behavioral-genetic studies with the honey bee have the potential to substantiate its efficacy as a valid ethanol model for the comparative purpose of better understanding alcohol’s various modes of action in humans. 14 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. REFERENCES Abramson, C. I., Fellows, G. W., Browne, B. L., Lawson, A., & Ortiz, R. A. (2003) Development of an ethanol model using social insects: II. Effect of Antabuse® on consumatory responses and learned behavior of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Psychological Reports, 92, 365-378. Abramson, C. I., Kandolf, A., Sheridan, A., Donohue, D., Božič, J., Meyers, J. E., & Benbassat, D. (2004) Development of an ethanol model using social insects: III. Preferences for ethanol solutions. Psychological Reports, 94, 227-239. Abramson, C. I., Place, A. J., Aquino, I. S., & Fernandez, A. (2004) Development of an ethanol model using social insects: IV. Influence of ethanol on the aggression of Africanized honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). Psychological Reports, 94, 1107-1115. Abramson, C. I., Sanderson, C., Painter, J., Barnett, S., & Wells, H. (2005) Development of an ethanol model using social insects: V. Honey bee foraging decisions under the influence of alcohol. Alcohol, 36, 187-193. Abramson, C. I., Stone, S. M., Ortez, R. A., Luccardi, A., Vann, K., Hanig, K., & Rice, J. (2000) The development of an ethanol model using social insects: I. Behavior studies of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 1153-1166. Alaux, C., Sinha, S., Hasadsri, L., Hunt, G. J., Guzman-Novoa, E., DeGrandi-Hoffman, G., Uribe-Rubio, J. L., Southey, B. R., Rodriguez-Zas, S., & Robinson, G. E. (2009) Honey bee aggression supports a link between gene regulation and behavioral evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1540015405. Božič, J., & Abramson, C. I. (2003) Following and attending: two distinct behavior patterns of honeybees in a position to collect the dance information. Mellifera, 3, 48-55. Božič, J., Abramson, C. I., & Bedencic, M. (2006) Reduced ability of ethanol drinkers for social communication in honey bees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.). Alcohol, 38, 179-183. Breckenridge, R. L., & Berger, R. S. (1990) Locus of control and perceived alcohol ingestion in performance of a fine motor skill. Psychological Reports, 66, 179-185. Centers for Disease Control. (2005). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Retrieved January 4, 2010, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/factsheets/FAS.pdf. Centers for Disease Control. (2008). Alcohol and public health. Retrieved January 4, 2010, from http://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/. Cocores, J. A., Miller, N. S., Pottash, A. C., & Gold, M. S. (1988) Sexual dysfunction in abusers of cocaine and alcohol. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 14, 169-173. Davies, A. G., Pierce-Shimomura, J. T., Kim, H., VanHoven, M. K., Thiele, T. R., Bonci, A., Bargmann, C. I., & McIntire, S. L. (2003) A central role of the BK potassium channel in behavioral responses to ethanol in C. elegans. Cell, 115, 655-666. Dolinsky, Z. S., & Babor, T. F. (1997) Ethical, scientific, and clinical issues in ethanol administration research involving alcoholics as human subjects. Addiction, 92, 1087-1097. Dopico, A. M., Anantharam, V., & Treistman, S. N. (1998) Ethanol increases the activity of Ca++-dependent K+ (mslo) channels: functional interaction with cytosolic Ca++. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 284, 258-268. HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 15 Ekstedt, M., Franzén, L. E., Holmqvist, M., Bendsten, P., Mathiesen, U. L., Bodemar, G., & Kechagias, S. (2009) Alcohol consumption is associated with progression of hepatic fibrosis in non-alcoholic fatty liver disease. Scandinavian Journal of Gastroenterology, 44, 366-374. Field, A. (2005) Discovering statistics using SPSS. (2nd ed.) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Field, M., Christiansen, P., Cole, J., & Goudie, A. (2007) Delay discounting and the alcohol stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. Addiction, 102, 579-586. Giray, T., & Robinson, G. E. (1996) Common endocrine and genetic mechanisms of behavioral development in male and worker honey bees and the evolution of the division of labor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 11,718-11,722. Graham, J. W., Marks, G., & Hansen, W. B. (1991) Social influence processes affecting adolescent substance use. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 291-298. Grinshpoon, A., Margolis, A., Weizman, A., & Ponizovsky, A. M. (2007) Sildenafil citrate in the treatment of sexual dysfunction and its effect on quality of life in alcohol dependent men: preliminary findings. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 42, 340-346. Gruss, M., Henrich, M., König, P., Hempelmann, G., Vogel, W., & Scholz, A. (2001) Ethanol reduces excitability in a subgroup of primary sensory neurons by activation of BKCa channels. European Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 1246-1256. Harris, R. A. (1999) Ethanol actions on multiple ion channels: which are important? Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, 1563-1570. Hatton, J., Burton, A., Nash, H., Munn, E., Burgoyne, L., & Sheron, N. (2009) Drinking patterns, dependency, and life-time drinking history in alcohol-related liver disease. Addiction, 104, 587-592. Heberlein, U., Wolf, F. W., Rothenflush, A., & Guarnieri, D. J. (2004) Molecular genetic analysis of ethanol intoxication in Drosophila melanogaster. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 44, 269-274. Holmes, S. P., Barhoumi, R., Nachman, R. J., & Pietrantonio, P. V. (2003) Functional analysis of a G protein-coupled receptor from the Southern cattle tick Boophilus microplus (Acari: Ixodidae) identifies it as the first arthropod myokinin receptor. Insect Molecular Biology, 12, 27-38. Hunt, G. J., Amdam, G. V., Schlipalius, D., Emore, C., Sardesai, N., Williams, C. E., Rueppell, O., Guzmán-Novoa, E., Arechavaleta-Velasco, M., Chandra, S., Fondrk, M. K., Beye, M., & Page, R. E., Jr. (2007) Behavioral genomics of honeybee foraging and nest defense. Naturwissenschaften, 94, 247-267. Karch, D., Crosby, A., & Simon, T. (2006) Toxicology testing and results for suicide victims: 13 states, 2004. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 55, 1245-1248. Lovinger, D. M., & Crabbe, J. C. (2005) Laboratory models of alcoholism: treatment target identification and insight into mechanisms. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 14711480. Mandell, W., & Miller, C. M. (1983) Male sexual dysfunction as related to alcohol consumption: a pilot study. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 7, 65-69. Marczinski, C. A., Harrison, E. L., & Fillmore, M. T. (2008) Effects of alcohol on simulated driving and perceived driving impairment in binge drinkers. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 1329-1337. McNaughton, L., & Preece, D. (1986) Alcohol and its effects on sprint and middle distance running. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 20, 56-59. 16 T. A. MIXSON, ET AL. Mintzer, M. Z. (2007) The acute effects of alcohol on memory: a review of laboratory studies in healthy adults. International Journal on Disability and Human Development, 6, 397-403. Moore, M. S., DeZazzo, J., Luk, A. Y., Tully, T., Singh, C. M., & Heberlein, U. (1998) Ethanol intoxication in Drosophila: genetic and pharmacological evidence for regulation by the cAMP signaling pathway. Cell, 93, 997-1007. Moulton, P. L., Petros, T. V., Apostal, K. J., Park, R. V., II, Ronning, E. A., King, B. M., & Penland, J. G. (2005) Alcohol-induced impairment and enhancement of memory: a test of the interference theory. Physiology and Behavior, 85, 240-245. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2008) Vehicle safety research. Retrieved January 5, 2010, from http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/Pubs/811016.pdf. August. Nawrot, M., Nordenstrom, B., & Olson, A. (2004) Disruption of eye movements by ethanol intoxication affects perception of depth from motion parallax. Psychological Science, 15, 858-865. Oster, G. F., & Wilson, E. O. (1978) Caste and ecology in the social insects. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Phillips, J. G., & Ogeil, R. P. (2007) Alcohol consumption and computer blackjack. Journal of General Psychology, 134, 333-353. Ruppell, O., Pankiw, T., & Page, R. E., Jr. (2004) Pleiotropy, epistasis, and new QTL: the genetic architecture of honey bee foraging behavior. Heredity, 95, 481-491. Sah, P. (1996) Ca2+-activated K+ currents in neurones: types, physiological roles, and modulation. Trends in Neurosciences, 19, 150-154. Seeley, T. D. (1982). Adaptive significance of age polyethism schedule in honeybee colonies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 11, 287-293. Singh, C. M., & Heberlein, U. (2000) Genetic control of acute ethanol-induced behaviors in Drosophila. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 1127-1136. Söderlund, H., Parker, E. S., Schwartz, B. L., & Tulving, E. (2005) Memory encoding and retrieval on the ascending and descending limbs of the blood alcohol concentration curve. Psychopharmacology, 182, 305-317. Souto, A., Bezerra, B. M., & Halsey, L. G. (2008) Alcohol intoxication reduces detection of asymmetry: an explanation for increased perceptions of facial attractiveness after alcohol consumption? Perception, 37, 955-958. Stoner, S. A., George, W. H., Peters, L. M., & Norris, J. (2007) Liquid courage: alcohol fosters risky sexual decision-making in individuals with sexual fears. AIDS and Behavior, 11, 227-237. Thiele, T. E., Willis, B., Stadler, J., Reynolds, J. G., Bernstein, I. L., & McKnight, G. S. (2000) High ethanol consumption and low sensitivity to ethanol-induced sedation in protein kinase A-mutant mice. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1-6. von Frisch, K. (1965) Tanzsprache und orientirung der bienen. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Wang, Z. W., Saifee, O., Nonet, M. L., & Salkoff, L. (2001) SLO-1 potassium channels control quantal content of neurotransmitter release at the C. elegans neuromuscular junction. Neuron, 32, 867-881. Watten, R. G., & Lie, I. (1996) Visual functions and acute ingestion of alcohol. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 16, 460-466. HONEY BEES: ALCOHOL-CONSUMPTION MODEL 17 Weafer, J., & Fillmore, M. T. (2008) Individual differences in acute ethanol impairment of inhibitory control predict ad libitum alcohol consumption. Psychopharmacology, 201, 315-324. Webster, I. P. (1975) Secretory and metabolic effects of alcohol on the pancreas. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 252, 183-186. Whitfield, C. W., Cziko, A., & Robinson, G. E. (2003) Gene expression profiles in the brain predict behavior in individual honey bees. Science, 302, 296-299. Wolf, F. W., & Heberlein, U. (2003) Invertebrate models of drug abuse. Journal of Neurobiology, 54, 161-178. Accepted April 21, 2010. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 11- Serial Dilutions Activity Description: This activity is an essential skill for toxicological research. The activity is simply intended to serve as a skill builder for the subsequent activity. Students should learn to successfully make serial dilutions of a solution. Estimated Length of Activity: 1 class (1- 1.5 hours) Pre-activity: Process Skills Goals: teamwork, following laboratory instructions Major Assignment: In groups, make serial dilutions of CuSO4 Knowledge Goals: how to measure using a balance, how to measure using a graduated cylinder and pipette, how to make serial dilutions. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Formative- none Materials: Lab Instructions, CuSO4, H2O, Balance, Graduated cylinder, Test tubes, Pipettes, Stirring rods, Volumetric flask Activity Instructions: Tell the class that CuSO4 (copper sulfate) makes a blue solution in water. As the concentration of dissolved particles decreases, the value of the color becomes lighter because there are fewer particles to reflect the light. This is similar to adding water to a glass of soda, the more water you add, the more dilute the soda becomes because there the ratio of soda particles to water particles increases. Monitor group progress. Serial Dilutions of CuSO4 In this exercise you will learn how to perform serial dilutions of chemicals. Background CuSO4 (copper sulfate) makes a blue solution in water. As the concentration of dissolved particles decreases, the value of the color becomes lighter because there are fewer particles to reflect the light. Materials CuSO4 H2O Balance Graduated cylinder Test tubes Pipettes Stirring rods Volumetric flask Procedure Start with the most concentrated solution. Decide what final volume you want for each solution, and begin with twice that. (In this example, we’ll use 10 mL for each treatment, so start with 20 mL of your most concentrated solution.) Take half this amount (10 mL) and add it to the same volume of water (10 mL). This is your first dilution, and its concentration is half that of the starting solution. Repeat by removing half the volume of this solution, and again, adding it to the same volume of water. Repeat until all the dilutions are made. You will have to discard half of the volume of your last dilution to make the final volumes all equal. (Try 5‐fold or 10‐fold serial dilutions to cover a broader range of doses.) 1. Make 100.0 mL of a 0.5 CuSO4 molar solution in a volumetric flask. 2. Using a pipette, transfer ________ this solution to a graduated cylinder and add distilled water up to 20.0 mL. Put into test tube. 3. Now take _______ of this second solution and transfer it to another 10.0 mL graduated cylinder and add water to make a 10.0 mL solution. 4. Continue transferring 1.0 mL of each solution and diluting it four more times or until the solution is colorless. This is called serial dilution. 5. Show calculations for making the original solution, and calculate the molarity of each of the other six solutions. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 12- Experiment Proposals Activity Description: This activity begins the second phase of the course: CREATE research. Students will propose their own research projects based on the articles they have read and experiments they have performed in class. The class will then form Review Panels and each panel will develop criteria and then select the best project from another group. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 2 classes (2- 3 hours) Pre-activity: Major Assignment: groups will concept map ‘toxicants in the environments’, for homework students will cartoon a research proposal, groups will evaluate proposals from other groups in a Phase I Review Panel Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups in class discussions Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, how to evaluate scientific research proposals, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers Formative- group concept maps, individually cartooned research proposal, group review panel criteria Materials: previous activities, articles, white paper, markers, poster paper Activity Instructions: 1. In class, have the groups concept map toxicants in their environment. 2. For homework, each student is assigned the task of cartooning an experiment idea just as they did for the CREATE articles. They can draw from any resource for ideas and are NOT limited to the next blackworm experiment. The only rule is that the toxicant cannot be hazardous for handling by the students and the organism must be either a plant or an invertebrate. a. In addition to the cartoon the students should clearly define the hypothesis being tested, the control and experimental groups, and a list of the materials needed. Activity Instructions ctd: 1. In class have groups exchange their cartooned experiments. Each group now becomes a Phase I Panel reviewing the proposals from one other group. The Panel will select the best proposal from the group and must also make recommendations to improve the practicality or feasibility of the project. The group (that the proposal originated from) will then conduct the experiment from the proposal selected taking the recommendations into consideration. ** The teacher must review the selected proposal and make feasibility recommendations to ensure the projects are actually possible for classroom research. This is phase II of the review process. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 13- Research Projects Activity Description: This activity continues the second phase of the course: CREATE research. Students will conduct the Panel selected experiments they proposed in Activity 12. Before beginning their research, students must each summarize and present an article to their group. They must then write an introduction section to be submitted to the instructor along with their supplies ‘order’ for their experiment. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, how to synthesize a scientific article, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers, how to write an introduction to a scientific article, how to conduct (and modify) a scientific experiment Estimated Length of Activity: 5-10 classes (5- 15 hours) Pre-activity: Find, read and summarize an article on the topic of your research Major Assignment: Formulate and conduct a research project in groups. First read, summarize and present an article to the group. Then as a group write an introduction section from the summarized articles and prepare a supplies order for your project. Then prepare appropriate data tables for recording the data collected. Then conduct the experiment, refine and repeat. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups Formative- individual article summaries, group introductions, group supply orders, group data tables Materials: lab supplies as requested in group supply orders Activity Instructions: 1. The groups should prepare an “order” of the materials needed for their selected project. 2. For the project selected, the students in the group should each find one related background article in google scholar and present a summary of the article to their group members. The group should then prepare a 1-2 page Introduction for their project. **The teacher should obtain the materials needed by the class during this period. 3. Data collection tables should be prepared by groups for each part of the experiment. Once the teacher approves them, the students can proceed with their experiment. 4. Groups should be encouraged to modify and repeat experiments as needed and as time allows. Backyard Toxicology: Activity 14- Reporting Results Activity Description: Students should draw from their knowledge of the articles they read during the CREATE process to create appropriate tables and figures of their results. Conclusions should be drawn from the results just as in the articles with the CREATE method. Each group will then present their projects to the class. Process Skills Goals: teamwork, critical scientific thinking Estimated Length of Activity: 2-5 classes (4- 7.5 hours) Pre-activity: Clean up data tables from experiment Major Assignment: Formulate results and draw conclusions from research. Present a research project to peers in groups. Assessment: Non-formative- individual participation in groups Formative- group tables and figures, group conclusions, group presentations Knowledge Goals: an understanding of how to formulate scientific research projects, and how to engage in active scientific discussion with peers, how to write results and conclusion sections of a scientific article, how to present the results of a scientific experiment Materials: data sheets, introductions, researched articles and summaries Activity Instructions: 1. Students should prepare data tables and figures for their results and make conclusions. 2. They should write a conclusion section and a prepare a list of references for the articles that were summarized. 3. Each group should then prepare a presentation of their results, and present it to the class. The format for presentations is at the discrepancy of the teacher: PPT or Scientific Poster.