Chapter 7 - Gender and communication
Transcription
Chapter 7 - Gender and communication
Contents 7 Gender and communication Sex or gender? Biology or culture? 7.2 Gender and communication: The state of play Clash of ideas: Feminism vs. evolutionary psychology 7.2 Brains and ability: Biology is destiny? 7.3 Gender and communication: Differences and similarities 7.7 The spoken word: Talk, talk, talk 7.7 Genderlects: Rapport talk and report talk 7.8 Rapport, aggression, indirect communication and relational bullying 7.8 Martians, Venusians and communication 7.9 Other speech differences between males and females 7.11 Body talk: Non-verbal communication 7.13 The written word: Men and women writing 7.16 Contents Male and female registers? 7.16 Man-made? Unloading language and becoming inclusive 7.16 Gender-neutral expression 7.17 Styles of communication and conflict 7.19 7.20 1. The differences are about power, not gender 7.20 2. There are no or few real differences anyway 7.21 3. The differences are real, but will unfair use be made of them? 7.21 4. The differences are real, and they indicate a better world (or perhaps the same old one . . .) 7.21 5. Specific communication patterns exist, but their causes are misinterpreted 7.21 Where to from here? 7.22 Summary 7.23 Student study guide KEY T ERMS 7.23 7. 23 R EVI EW QUES TI ON S APPL I ED AC TI V I TI ES 7. 24 7. 24 W HAT WO ULD YOU DO ? R EFER EN CES 7. 24 7.25 SUG G ESTED READI N G 7. 28 ACKN OW L EDG EMENT S 7. 29 7 Gender and communication After reading this chapter you should be able to: • Discuss the biological and cultural background of sex and gender • Identify perceived differences in female and male approaches to spoken, non-verbal and written communication • Explain the basis of communication problems and conflicts between genders • Articulate reservations about the validity of gender as a variable in human communication • Learn gender-neutral communication techniques Do men and women communicate in different ways? Is it possible, for example, that the spoken, written and non-verbal communication of humans can be understood in terms of their sex? These are the questions we will be addressing in this chapter. Such answers as we come up with may have implications for our personal and professional lives. It should be stressed from the outset that the field of gender communication, like those of a number of other specific types of communication discussed in this book, is very young in historical terms: apart from some early research (e.g. Jesperson 1922), the area of study is barely a few decades old, with much of the research carried out only in the past few years. The best work is still to come (and perhaps some of it will be done by you). The relative immaturity of the field, combined with the fact that many females and males hold strong opinions (and prejudices) about male/female differences in general and communication in particular, means that whatever conclusions we might reach will be tentative. For all that, they may be enlightening, and may even provide us with opportunities to recharge our sense of humour and our goodwill — precious qualities in any field of communication but perhaps particularly in this one. Sex or gender? Biology or culture? Constructivism: the idea that sex and gender are essentially different concepts, with sex defining inherited characteristics and gender referring to roles that are socialised or learned; sometimes carries the implication that learned behaviour is more socially significant than inherited behaviour Communicating in the 21st Century Essentialism: the idea that sex and gender are essentially the same; sometimes carries the implication that inherited behaviour is socially more significant than learned behaviour 7.2 Why do we use the word gender rather than sex? Surely ‘sex’ is what differentiates males and females? Not necessarily. A view that has emerged in the past few decades is that ‘gender’ is socially constructed rather than genetically transmitted. Indeed it is possible that gender will come to be used as a verb (to be gendered, or to gender something or someone). ‘Essentialists’ who oppose this view use the two terms interchangeably to speak of a product of evolution and biology. Constructivism and essentialism, then, distinguish between a belief in the pre-eminent influence of environment or nurture, on the one hand, and of heredity or nature on the other. Clash of ideas: Feminism vs. evolutionary psychology This clash of ideas has been driven by two major intellectual movements of the past several decades — feminism on the one hand, and the disciplines of evolutionary psychology on the other. Evolutionary psychology and sociobiology are science-based disciplines that attempt to explain current human behaviour in terms of evolutionary strategies of men and women developed in prehistoric and indeed prehuman times (e.g. Buss 2004; Browne 2002; De Wall & Maner 2008, Dunbar & Barratt 2009, Iredale, Van Vugt, & Dunbar 2008, Miller & Kanazawa 2008; Moir 2001; Pinker 2003; Thornhill & Gangestad 2009; Stenstrom, Stenstrom, Saad, & Cheikhrouhou 2008; see also chapter 8, ‘Non-verbal communication, pp. 262–3). • Animal primate behaviour gives us insight into human behaviour. • Much primate behaviour shows strong role specialisation, with females tending offspring, gathering plant foods and staying close to home base, and males operating in hunter bands far from the home base, using coordinated aggression to hunt prey. • Much female behaviour in these circumstances is concerned with establishing rapport with other females in food-gathering activities, while much male behaviour is concerned with goal-oriented behaviour of tracking and hunting animals, with minimal verbal interaction with other males except for exchanging problem-solving techniques and mapping out territory. • Men and women follow two different paths or strategies, as dictated by Darwinian natural selection: females invest energy and scarce biological resources on having one or a few partners and raising offspring to maximise the chances of that gene set being transferred to subsequent generations, while males invest energy and abundant biological resources in having offspring with as many mates as possible. Sex/gender differentiation may, in this view, go even further: Women, for their part, will compete for those men who invest in their children, thus raising their value. While the impact of these elements varies from one culture to the next, the pattern remains: Women invest in children more than men, and men, all other things being equal, prefer more sexual partners than do women. Given these facts, the EPs [evolutionary psychologists] suggest, men will naturally be inclined to pursue multiple partners. Their efforts, from an evolutionary point of view, will be put into finding a beautiful woman (genetically more appealing), finding as many partners as possible, and honing their physical skills in order to battle with other men for desirable women. Emotional skills that might lead to long-term relationships, in this view, have little role. Women, on the other hand, will pursue men who are more likely to stick around the morning after and help provide the food and protection mother and child will need to survive. But Barnett and Rivers (2004) find that attractive women do not necessarily produce more offspring, and that women in general are now more likely to be economically independent and have less need to establish relationships with older males to maintain security, wealth and status. Instead, research shows that economically independent women are more likely to prefer younger males as mates, who have qualities like empathy, understanding and the ability to bond with children. Does that rebut the essentialist argument? Perhaps, but we need to be careful not to discount the traditional older male/younger female model, which may be still embedded in whatever genetic programming we all have. The war over gender is complicated by a meta-gender factor: most writers developing an essentialist line seem to be men, while most writers pursuing a constructivist line seem to be women. This is not always the case, of course. Anne Campbell (2002), a psychologist specialising in the evolutionary background of female aggression, argues that females have always been more proactive in selecting partners and ensuring their own reproductive success, using strategies such as competition, rivalry and indirect aggression towards other females, and strategies of unfaithfulness towards men. Neurobiologist Richard Francis (2004) takes issue with pat sociobiological dictums (e.g. ‘Men won’t ask for directions because they believe in their own spatial abilities to navigate — developed in prehistoric hunting bands — and to do so would be to lose face’) and suggests that essentialism seriously overestimates the influence of prehistoric behaviour patterns in today’s world (see also Zuk 2004). Brains and ability: Biology is destiny? Baron-Cohen (2003) suggests that male and female brains have evolved differently, and today female brains can be categorised as E-brains (empathising brains) while male brains can be categorised as S-brains (systemising brains) (figure 7.1). Chapter 7 Gender and communication Upsetting to critics of EP theories is the suggestion, popularized in the press, that what is biological is unchangeable. ‘One of my objections to evolutionary psychology,’ explains Mary Gergen, Ph.D., professor of psychology and women’s studies at Pennsylvania State University–Delaware County and a hard-core believer that gender is primarily a social construct, ‘is that it tends to stabilize and justify existing patterns of social relationships. They say they are presenting “just the facts ma’am,” but it justifies the status quo.’ (Blustain 2000, pp. 42–4) 7.3 People with empathising brains are more prone to . . . People with systemising brains are more prone to . . . • Pick up non-verbal cues • Discuss problems with others rather than try to solve them alone • Get upset at seeing animals and other people in pain • Get emotionally involved in films and other people’s problems • Enjoy caring for other people. • Read non-fiction than fiction, and watch documentaries • Want to know technical details about cars and appliances • Look at structure in music and buildings • Engage in DIY activities • Like games involving high degrees of strategy. Figure 7.1: Empathising versus systemising brains Source: Adapted from Baron-Cohen (2003). Reduced levels of empathy can lead to greater aggression; increased levels of empathy can lead to better communication skills. Baron-Cohen suggests that different individuals have different mixes of empathy and systemising, although individuals do seem to specialise in one or the other. (Interestingly, he suggests that the male tendency to systemise and classify, and to score low on empathy and regard for others, leads to extreme male brain autism and Asperger syndrome, most sufferers of which are male.) Cameron takes Baron-Cohen and the essentialists to task here, suggesting that we are merely hearing a very old story of ‘separate spheres’ of accomplishment and genetic programming, and of ‘different but equal’. Such comforting dichotomies, she suggests, invokes that cosy weasel word of our times, ‘diversity’ (see chapter 16, ‘Intercultural communication’, pp. 533–5), and have in the past led to such ‘separate spheres’ as the women’s anti-suffrage movement (allowing women to vote would upset the natural order of things) and apartheid (not allowing black and white South Africans to mix). Cameron (2007) also sees the essentialism of Baron-Cohen and others as quite patronising to men: seeing them as bullies, or as Neanderthals sulking in their caves. She remarks, sardonically: Communicating in the 21st Century Perhaps men have realised that a reputation for incompetence can sometimes work to your advantage. Like the idea that they are no good at housework, the idea that men are no good at talking serves to exempt them from doing something which many would rather leave to women anyway (Cameron 2007, p. 11). 7.4 Kimura (1999) argues that there are clearly discernible differences in ability, often closely linked to hormonal levels, such as: • Males score better on mathematical ability and spatial ability than females. • Males are very much better at most kinds of targeting abilities, such as dart throwing and ball catching (although homosexual men have targeting abilities not significantly better than women’s). • Females score better than males on verbal memory and fluency. • Females score better than males on fine motor skills. What might such differences mean, however? Kimura notes: Some people believe that knowing more about sex differences in abilities will put women at a special disadvantage, since it is currently true that the largest established differences we know about favour men. Radical feminists are prone to the view that knowledge of sex differences should be suppressed. They suggest that the lower representation of women in the sciences, especially in the physical sciences and mathematics, is due primarily to the tendency to ‘gender’ occupations. They feel that accepting the reality of certain sex differences in cognitive function provides an excuse for keeping women out of science and math fields, notwithstanding the explanations for the smaller numbers of women in the physical sciences . . . Some people have argued that the gendering of science, and hence the disadvantaging of women, begins early, in high school or before. Their solution is to encourage women to take more science and math courses, so that they later qualify for positions in related fields. Simple encouragement, of course, is not a bad thing; but if it takes the form of making people act out of a sense of duty, or causes them to feel guilty about their life choices, we must question the use, and the ethics, of such pressures. [Researchers] report that even the girls in their highly math-talented samples often had more person-oriented interests than did the boys, so that they might not enter fields in which math is critical. It is begging the question to insist that their choices are mostly due to societal influences. (Kimura 1999, p. 184) Crawford and Chaffin (1997) and Fausto-Sterling (2001) contest much of the ‘different brains and abilities’ approaches, suggesting that the data does not in fact bear out many differences at all. Bing (1999) argues that: Hyde (2005) investigated a large number of ‘inbuilt gender differences’, such as maths ability, but also in a number of other areas (men are more aggressive, they offer helping behaviour less often, they smile less often, girls fall behind boys in self-esteem when at school). She found little evidence to support any of these propositions, thus strengthening what she called ‘the Gender Similarities Hypothesis’ (see also Barnett & Rivers 2004). Zurbriggen and Sherman (2007) claim that there is a link between political ideology and biologically influenced views of human nature. They assert that more conservative media tend to cover stories from a biological determinism or essentialist viewpoint, while more liberal media were less essentialist and more constructivist. They also noted that a mother’s perceptions of the difficulty in math(s) for her child predicted the child’s performance and interest in mathematics. If essentialist stories dominate the media (e.g. girls are weak at maths), it is therefore conceivable that a type of self-fulfilling prophecy or Pygmalion effect will take place. For example, in a 1960s psychology experiment, primary school teachers were told that certain students had above-average abilities (when in fact, they did not) and the marks of these students improved substantially. The researchers in this experiment concluded that the teachers had behaved differently to the students they believed to be ‘smarter than average’ by letting them know non-verbally and with variations in teaching, which then produced the students’ better results. Thinking made it so, and smarter they became (Rosenthal & Jacobson 1992). Lippa (2002) suggests a compromise, or synthesis, of the essentialist-nature-heredity view and the constructivist-nurture-environment view (figure 7.2). This model is not simply a weak compromise but a demonstration of the fact that environmental and hereditary influences Chapter 7 Gender and communication . . . reports of scientific research first show differences between groups and then, with no explanation, equate difference with deficit or inferiority, a ready-made explanation for limiting the opportunities of girls and women. This is a successful strategy because when one myth about biological essentialism becomes discredited, a new one can be quickly found to replace it. Current arguments for essential differences between the sexes (and, by implication, the inferiority of females) can be found in reports of brain research. What is troubling about current discoveries of differences between male and female brains is not the research itself, but the way this information is reported, distorted, and widely disseminated by the media and then used as a justification for discrimination . . . I argue that feminists should avoid the ‘difference debate’ altogether and suggest that the important issue is equal opportunity, not equal ability . . . (Bing 1999, pp. 4–5) 7.5 7.6 Source: Lippa (2002, p. 196). Reproduced with permission. Feedback loops 7. Behaviour 6. Emotions/ feelings/ 5. Cognition/ thought 4. Social cultural influences 3. Peer influences 2. Family influences 1. Biological/ genetic factors Communicating in the 21st Century Figure 7.2: Parallel tracks of gender development and their complex evolution 0 1 2 3 4 Prenatal social environment Prenatal hormones and uterine environment 5 6 8 9 Puberty and its hormonal and physical changes 40 50 60 Family roles and behaviour College/ university influences College and/or work cultures Departing from family Marriage, spouse influences, children Behaviour in close relationship Sexual behaviours Attitudes Gender ideologies and attitudes towards boys Sexual attractions and girls and orientations Sex-typed interests Gender segregation Sex-typed play Attitudes towards siblings Attitudes towards parents Gender Internal Gender labelling gender constancy standards Gender stereotypes Gender schemas Adolescent culture Social groups Teacher influences School influences Media influences Sex segregation; differing cultures of boys and girls 30 Marriage and parenthood, childbirth 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Family roles; sibling influences Independence and dependence training 7 Differential treatment of boys and girls Encouragement of sex-typed play Dress; room decor Organisational effects on nervous system Tracks of gender development Prenatal 70... feed back into each other all the time, often making it impossible to clearly assign one extreme pattern of causation or another. Examples of such feedback include (Lippa 2002, pp. 197–200): • Genes held in common by parents and children influence how parents treat their children and also how children respond to their parents. • Biological predispositions in girls and boys foster sex segregation, and conversely, sex segregation amplifies biological predispositions in girls and boys. • Parental socialisation moulds the way children interact with their peers. • Peer influences determine which TV shows children watch and the resulting gender messages children take from TV. • Parent and teacher stereotypes influence the educational choices of boys and girls, which then influence their subsequent occupational choices. • Work and educational settings influence individuals’ gender stereotypes, stress levels and even hormone levels. • Both biological predispositions and early social learning lead to sex-typed toy preferences in children, which in turn lead to sex differences in child–parent and child–peer interactions and to the development of different motor skills and cognitive abilities in boys and girls. • Individuals’ levels of sex hormones can be influenced by environments (e.g. by stress, success, or the presence or absence of members of the opposite sex) as well as by sex chromosomes. ASSESS YOURSELF Consider the continuum below. The extremes are Essentialism (according to which biology/ heredity/nature determines behaviour, and gender is a concept with little value) and Constructivism (according to which society/environment/nurture determines behaviour, and gender is a vital concept). Place a mark on the continuum that represents your belief or point of view. Compare it with the response of at least two other people. Essentialism Constructivism We have now completed a brief overview of the debate on sex and gender. Skimming the surface of the environmental and hereditary factors will have raised more questions than it has answered about why males and females understand and misunderstand each other. Let’s now look at communication processes in greater detail, examining in turn spoken communication, non-verbal communication and written communication. These three areas, of course, overlap one another considerably. We will concentrate here on real or apparent differences, but later we will consider similarities and assess the authenticity of these differences. Further, we will speculate on whether, if differences exist, the key reason for this might not be biological sex differences but something else entirely. The spoken word: Talk, talk, talk Non-written verbal communication is all about talk and the use of the voice, but it is also about silence and the withholding of talk. So what gender differences can we identify? Let’s begin by looking at two popular writers in the field, Deborah Tannen and John Gray. While Chapter 7 Gender and communication Gender and communication: Differences and similarities 7.7 not without their critics (e.g. Peterson 2000), these writers have done much to make the idea of gender and communication part of everyday discourse. Genderlects: Rapport talk and report talk Genderlect: a genderspecific dialect Rapport talk: relationshiporiented talk whose primary function is to build understanding and empathy within a wider group Report talk: task-oriented talk whose primary function is to produce solutions Troubles talk: talking about problems in order to share the experience rather than necessarily to generate a solution Tannen (1990, 1995, 2002) suggests that women and men talk in different genderlects, or gender-specific dialects (see also Loosemore & Galea 2008). Women tend to engage in rapport talk, or relationship-oriented talk, whose primary function is to build understanding and empathy within a wider group. By contrast, she argues, men tend to engage in report talk, or task-oriented talk, whose primary function is to produce solutions to problems. She suggests, in fact, that inter-gender communication is a form of intercultural communication (see chapter 16, ‘Intercultural communication’; see also Basow & Rubenfeld 2003). One particular type of rapport talk, suggests Tannen, is troubles talk: Ordinary troubles provide fodder for conversation that helps speakers think about life’s challenges — not just to find a solution (though it can certainly do that) but as a kind of philosophical investigation: How do others deal with situations like this? What is the best way to think about them? Most of all, just talking this way creates connection. A problem does not have to be overwhelming for a woman to bring it up for discussion; any problem can make a good topic by providing an opportunity to reaffirm mutual interest and connection. But many men assume that if someone brings up a problem, it must be serious. One woman complained daily about situations at work; her son said, ‘If you hate your job so much, why don’t you quit?’ She was puzzled; she liked her job. He misinterpreted her runof-the-mill stresses and strains as serious complaints because he would not talk about them unless they were major problems he wanted to solve. (Tannen 2002, p. 126) Troubles talk, then, while appearing to present a problem for solution, is really an exercise in solidarity and sharing of experience. Misunderstandings between males and females may therefore be the result of the collision between troubles talk and report talk, or the predisposition of males to provide solutions to any complaint. The presentation of those solutions can be misperceived by some women as the male not listening (see Gray’s ‘Mr Fix-It’ on page 7.9). Troubles talk can be interpreted by males as unproductive whining; feminist writers assert the consciousness-raising benefits of ‘bitching’ as a means of expressing emotion in bureaucratic environments — where the suppression of emotion is part of a broader system of control — and also of expressing the resistance of those who are less powerful. Communicating in the 21st Century Rapport, aggression, indirect communication and relational bullying 7.8 Relational bullying: aggression towards others that does not involve physical violence Are women then intrinsically non-aggressive, especially towards other women? Are all female-to-female interactions primarily concerned with forming extended networks of rapport and support? Not at all. While the evidence that males are more physically violent than females is overwhelming (Baron-Cohen 2003), physically violent and aggressive women are not unknown. Perhaps of greater interest, however, are subtler types of violence, especially those inflicted by females on other females. For example, relational bullying, a common female-to-female behaviour pattern, can involve insults, teasing, the silent treatment, note-passing, glaring, gossiping, ganging up, criticism of appearance and inconsistent behaviour (e.g. being nice in private but mean in public) (Goodwin 2002; Simmons 2002). Tannen has noted similar indirect aggressive tactics, such as repeating a critical remark made by someone else (‘with the for-your-own-good introduction “I think you should know” ’), and offering ‘helpful’ suggestions and mixed-stroke-type compliments (‘ “your presentation was excellent. It was much easier to follow than your last one” ’) (Tannen 1990, pp. 172–4). Women can sometimes be harsher on each other than men are (Trethewey 1999), and can be more aggressive than males in the race to succeed in organisations (Hollands 2001). To the extent that these behaviours are typical, they may confirm the contention that many females have traditionally been taught to suppress conflict and assertive, direct communication, and thus have had little alternative but to communicate in indirect passive or passive-aggressive (manipulative) styles. Of course, traditions are culture-specific: some communities are more open in the way they handle conflict than the Anglo-American cultures where most of the gender and communication research has been done, and within those cultures women often give as well as they get. ASSESS YOURSELF Interview at least two males and two females on the topic of bullying. Do the accounts vary according to gender? Martians, Venusians and communication When women talk about their problems, men lose points by interrupting and giving instant solutions. Women perceive this as a message that her male colleague does not care. The Office Improvement Committee refers to women’s tendencies to improve things, look around and make suggestions. This eagerness to please often turns men off. For example, if a woman says ‘you should clean up this office,’ her intention may be to help, but what the man hears is criticism . . . Women value caring, cooperation, collaboration, consensus, consideration and communication. Men value the bottom line, efficiency, effectiveness, accomplishment, achievement, independence and autonomy . . . There is a communication barrier because male and female hormones are different. In the workplace there are different reactions to stress. There are certain things that make men feel comfortable, certain things that make women comfortable. It’s as if we come from planets with different languages. Men and women share the same words, so they think they are being heard, but they are not. So they might feel disappointed and betrayed when Chapter 7 Gender and communication ‘Mr Fix-It’ syndrome: the predisposition of males to offer solutions to problems and complaints rather than to show empathy towards the complainant When John Gray (1992) suggested that ‘men are from Mars, women are from Venus’, he was humorously alluding to the communication difficulties males and females often experience. Among these differences he identifies the following: • Men prefer to brood and stew over problems and daily experiences in private (‘retreating to their caves’), whereas women like to talk about them. • Women like to talk about problems, often in detail, because they seek empathy and because it helps to talk them through, whereas men may simply try to offer solutions (the ‘Mr Fix-It’ syndrome). • Women offer advice and constructive criticism (‘The Office Improvement Committee’) as an act of love, whereas men may feel that women are trying to change them. • Women approach problems in an involved, intuitive fashion, whereas men tend to be more detached and less intuitive. These differences, he suggests, often cause communication breakdown, leading to conflict and misunderstanding: 7.9 they don’t get what they want. A woman on her planet will share feelings as a way of bonding or relieving stress. For example, a woman might say ‘what a crazy day it has been today.’ A man might look at her and think ‘why is she complaining?’ The man, instead of hearing a plea for emotional support, responds with a solution or makes a comment. He is not empathetic. A separation, rather than a connection, occurs between the two. (Gray, quoted in Casison 2002) Within both personal and professional realms of experience, crossed wires may lead to frustration and anger. Such misunderstandings can be aggravated when men and women are working under pressure: they may use the same words but in quite different ways, their language embedded in different sets of assumptions. Gray’s work has been extremely influential, but has attracted criticism that verges on the ferocious. In studying the rapid and wide spread use of the ‘Mars/Venus (M/V) metaphor’, Noonan (2007) draws upon Richard Dawkin’s idea of a ‘meme’ — something which is to culture what the gene is to biology — a unit of information with the ability to reproduce itself (Blackmore & Dawkins 2000). M/V dichotomies are often nothing whatever to do with gender, and yet have become part of common discourse. Buzzard (2002) points out that when Gray — ‘the Coca-Cola of psychology’ — produced the first version of the Mars/Venus book in 1993, it was a relative failure, as it was a data-heavy, self-published book. It was only after several years and sales had reached 50 000 that commercial publishers showed interest in mass publication. Part of the marketing strategy was to make Gray a brand, with a series of related products, such as television, audiovisuals, films, CD-ROMs and games. In other words, Buzzard puts much of the Gray phenomenon down to marketing genius: Communicating in the 21st Century Why did Gray’s book, written in the eighties, fail to catch fire until the nineties? Gray’s repackaging of the traditional gender types would seem to have captured the public imagination and sensibility of this era, puzzling scholars, reviewers and many readers alike. Is there a massive cultural need to hear that relationships between the sexes are so simple and so traditional? . . . Whether its success represents a backlash against changes in male– female relationships or illuminates important gender differences depends on the reader. At the heart of both of these beliefs is the assumption that demand automatically creates supply, that audiences get what they want, that Men Are From Mars, Women Are From Venus reflects the values, interests, and characteristics of its audience. However, it is pointless to speak of some vast cultural zeitgeist or psyche at work at a particular moment in history, which creates popular reception of a work when ordinary business factors suffice. (Buzzard 2002, p. 90) 7.10 Gray’s work has proven to be spectacularly popular, and has done much to stimulate discussion of gender/communication differences. Critics, however, have taken him to task for overemphasising differences between genders, for being too far to the essentialist end of the gender spectrum and for prescribing quick-fix solutions to complex human problems (Wood 2002; Buzzard 2002). Thus, Zimmerman, Haddock and McGeorge (2001) take Gray to task because he mishandles the negotiation of power in relationships: The book’s unprecedented sales are an indication that Gray’s . . . descriptions must have some degree of familiarity among the public and many therapists. However, Gray’s basic thesis — that men and women are instinctively different in all areas of life — and his recommendations for dealing with these differences serve to reinforce and encourage power differentials between men and women, thereby eroding the possibility of deep friendship and sustained intimacy in their relationships. As mentioned, this position is counter to a growing body of research that underscores the importance of shared power for achieving an intimate and effective relationship. (Zimmerman, Haddock & McGeorge 2001, p. 63) Gender communication is a field in its infancy: who knows what we will know in twenty years? Better communication techniques? Increased insight into the similarities between intercultural and gender communication? The knowledge that there is no such thing as gender communication, only interpersonal? Stay tuned. Other speech differences between males and females Bearing in mind certain limitations (for example, most research on gender communication has been conducted among white, middle-class Americans), some generalisations about gender-specific speech can be attempted (table 7.1). Characteristic Differences Voice pitch Women tend to have higher-pitched voices, which may be a liability in societies that associate a deep voice with authority, gravitas and credibility. Some women in powerful situations (politicians, newsreaders) may sometimes deliberately pitch their voices lower to counteract such a perceived disadvantage. Inflection Females may be more prone to high-rising tone (HRT), or upward inflection within sentences. Males and females both upwardly inflect at the end of question sentences in order to evoke a response, but inflecting several times within a sentence can suggest uncertainty and low assertiveness. Tone Men may have more monotonous speech: males tend to use approximately three tones when speaking, while females tend to use approximately five tones. Emphasis Men tend to use loudness to emphasise points, while females tend to use pitch and inflection to emphasise points. Pronunciation and enunciation Men tend to use sloppier pronunciation, mumbling more than women. Grammar and hypercorrection Women tend to use more precise grammar in speech than men do, and may be more likely to correct the usage of others. Fillers and discourse particles Men more than women tend to use speech fillers (‘like’, ‘um’, ‘er’ etc.). Delivery Men tend towards choppy and staccato delivery, while women tend towards more breathy and flowing delivery. Descriptions Men tend to describe colours broadly (‘green’, ‘blue’, ‘brown’), while women tend to use subtler descriptors (‘turquoise’, ‘chartreuse’, ‘bone’). Chapter 7 Gender and communication Table 7.1: Gender differences in spoken communication 7.11 Communicating in the 21st Century Table 7.1 (continued) 7.12 Characteristic Differences Diminutives and euphemisms Women may use diminutives (‘chocky bicky’, ‘Chrissy pressy’) and euphemisms (‘powder my nose’) more often than men. Aggression Men may be more likely to use profanity/obscenity, and to use teasing insults and playful put-downs either as indicators of affection and intimacy or as threatening and controlling behaviour. Interruptions and overlaps Men tend to interrupt women more than vice-versa. Interrupting can be a form of expressing dominance, and male-to-female interruption patterns tend to be of this kind. However, things are not that simple. Tannen (1994) notes that in all-female groups interruption levels can be higher than in mixed groups, but that these interruptions may be of a different type: she prefers to use the term ‘overlap’, because a person can speak while another is talking, not to change the topic or to attack the speaker, but in support — to establish rapport rather than assert power or disagreement. Turn taking When a person is waiting to speak, he or she generally waits for the other person to stop talking. Males tend to wait shorter times before speaking. This can lead females to think that males are pushy and uninterested, and males to think that females don’t have much to contribute. Hedging Females more than males tend to use qualifying or hedging phrases (‘maybe’, ‘perhaps’, ‘you know’, ‘in my opinion’, ‘it seems to me’). Expressive forms Women may make more use of expressive forms (‘lovely’, ‘beautiful’, ‘sad’, ‘divine’ — adjectives, rather than nouns or verbs, particularly those expressing emotional rather than intellectual evaluation) than men. Intensifiers or boosters Men tend to use quantifiers (‘always’, ‘never’, ‘all’, ‘none’), while women tend to use qualifiers (‘kind of’, ‘a bit’). Questions Males may be more likely to interpret conversations as win–lose contests in which you are either one up or one down. Males may be more likely to see questions as admissions of ignorance and therefore a one-down strategy. Females may be more likely to see questions as neutral information seeking or as rapport building. Some males may see female questioning as a sign of incompetence and uncertainty. Credit claiming Males may be more likely to take credit for their own achievements and even for the achievements of others. Bragging rituals are important in some male groups. Males may be more likely to use the singular pronoun ‘I’ than the plural ‘we’. Females may have traditionally been more reticent in claiming credit, although this is changing in some professional circles. Indirect expression Women tend to use a more indirect style than men. This can be out of tact and implicit communication or to avoid conflict. Feedback Women may provide more feedback (both verbal and non-verbal) in conversation. In professional situations, women may be prone to use more comprehensive but less direct feedback than males. This can sometimes lead to males feeling that females have missed the point, and for females to feel that males aren’t listening. Characteristic Differences Tag questions Females may sometimes be more prone to make tentative statements using ‘tag endings’ after making declarative statements, or to use upward inflections that make statements sound like questions (‘It’s a nice day, isn’t it?’ or ‘It’s a nice day?’). Males are more prone to make declarative statements (‘It’s a nice day.’). Tags may sometimes be used to include others and to elicit agreement. Both males and females do this. Ritual opposition Men may be more prone to engage in ritual opposition in arguments, while not being too emotionally involved in the conflict or its aftermath. Shifts Men may be more likely to change the topic than women. Women may be more likely to use conjunctions when changing topics (‘however’, ‘but’, ‘and’), while men may be more likely to use interjections (‘Oh’, ‘By the way’, ‘Listen’). Apologies Males may be less likely than females to apologise. Males may be more likely than females to see apologies as a one-down strategy, i.e. an admission of weakness. Frame of reference In arguments, females may be more prone than males to bring up things from the past rather than stick to the present matter of contention. Compliments Females tend to express compliments more easily than males. Males may be more likely to infer manipulative intent from compliments. Anger release Men may be more prone to yell, shout and swear to release anger, while women may be more prone to cry. Topics Men may be more prone to talk about things and activities such as cars, sports, jobs and mechanical things, while women may be more prone to talk about people, relationships, clothes, feelings and children. Requests Men may be more likely to make command or direct requests, using imperative verbal mood (‘Come here’), while women may be more likely to make compound requests or ‘wimperatives’, sometimes using subjunctive verbal mood (‘Please come here’, ‘If you could come here now, I’d be able to show you what’s wrong’). Body talk: Non-verbal communication Non-verbal communication or body language is a significant aspect of gender communication (see chapter 9). Two anecdotes may help to sketch out some of the factors at work in this arena (Cameron 2000): • A fast-food chain created a ‘script’ that all employees had to follow when farewelling diners; this involved certain words and a ‘cheery wave’. Some male employees found the wave gesture ‘effeminate’ and rebelled by turning the wave into a quasi-salute. Chapter 7 Gender and communication Sources: Adapted from Lakoff (1990); Glass (1992); Tannen (1990, 1994); Gray (1992); Dolan and Kropf (2004); Lizzio, Wilson, Gilchrist and Gallois (2003); DeCapua and Boxer (1999); Siegel (2002); Holmes (1995). 7.13 • A supermarket chain created a ‘superior service’ program that required employees to show customers friendliness, a personal interest and eagerness to please. A group of female workers complained at a union conference that male customers were misinterpreting this as indicating ‘romantic interest’ and inviting ‘lewd behaviour’. Some apparent gender differences in non-verbal communication are outlined in table 7.2. Communicating in the 21st Century Table 7.2: Gender differences in non-verbal communication 7.14 Characteristic Differences Personal space Males tend to take up more personal space than females when seated: they may sprawl, their legs wide, and may set up territorial markers (books, equipment) more quickly than women. Males are more easily stressed by crowding. Approach and orientation Women may be approached more closely than men. When males and females are forced into close proximity, males will tend to brush past by turning towards the female, while females will turn away from the male. Orientation Women may prefer to interact side by side, while men may prefer to interact face to face. Volume response Males may be less likely than females to stand back from a person who is talking loudly. Arm gestures A Women may keep their arms close to their body, while men hold their arms about 5–10 degrees away from the body. Women tend to use limp wrist and arm flapping gestures more than men. Arm gestures B Men may be more likely to use two-armed gestures and less likely to use one-armed gestures than women. Male gestures tend to be more forceful, angular and restricted, while female gestures are more likely to be fluid, easy and light. Arm gestures C Women may be more likely to keep their hands down on a chair than men. Women may play with their hair or clothing, place their hands in their lap, and drum their fingers more frequently than men. Men may use sweeping hand gestures, stretching the hands, cracking the knuckles, pointing, and using arms to lift the body from a chair or table more frequently. Hand gestures Males may be more likely to gesture with their fingers together or by pointing, while females may be more likely to gesture with their fingers apart and using curved hand gestures. Leg gestures A Males may be more likely to have their legs apart, at a 10–15 degree angle. Men may be more likely to jog their knees and tap their toes nervously, while women may be more likely to drum their fingers. Leg gestures B Women tend to cross their legs at the knees or cross their ankles with their knees slightly apart, while men tend to sit with their legs apart or with their legs stretched out in front of them and their ankles crossed. Posture A Women tend to walk with their pelvis pushed slightly forward, while men tend to walk with their pelvis rolled slightly back. Posture B Women may be more prone to move their entire body from neck to ankles as one entity when they walk, while men tend to move their arms independently and exhibit a slight twist in their rib cage. Posture C Males tend to assume more reclined positions when seated, while women tend to assume more forward positions. Men tend to fidget and shift their body position more than women do. Men tend to be more relaxed, while women tend to be more tense or alert. Body lean Women tend to lean their bodies less than men, while men tend to lean backward more. Differences Touching A Men tend to touch others less than women do. Men tend to perceive touching as an instrumental behaviour leading to sexual activity or as childish behaviour indicative of dependency and lack of manliness. Females may be more likely to view touch as an expressive behaviour which demonstrates warmth and affiliation. Touching B Men are less likely than women to engage in same-sex touching. Male same-sex touching tends to take the form of play fighting, triumph displays, backslapping or crushing handshakes. Listening Men tend to frown and squint when listening, while women tend to smile and head-nod when listening. Men tend to cock their head to one side and look obliquely at the person talking, while women tend to look directly at the person talking. Facial expressions A Men tend to suppress facial expressiveness and plasticity more than women; women tend to have more animated expressions. Facial expressions B Women tend to smile more than men. Men may smile less when there are other men around. Men may be less likely to return a smile than women. Women may be more attracted than men are to others who smile. Males who are more androgynous (sharing characteristics of stereotypical male and female extremes) may tend to smile more than males who are more stereotypically masculine; and more androgynous females may smile less than females who are more stereotypically feminine. Jaw Men may tend to open their jaw less than women do when speaking. Facial gesture leakage As males age, they may become less adept than females in suppressing leakage of emotions when lying. Eye contact Women may establish more eye contact than men do, but men may be more likely to stare while women may be more likely to avert their gaze. Female–female interaction is likely to involve much more eye contact than male–male interaction. Locomotion Men may be more likely than women to walk around when talking. Artefacts Males are more likely to use a one-handed grip to carry books or objects at their side while females are more likely to use a two-handed grip to carry books or objects against their chests. Clothing Males tend to dress to avoid disapproval, while women tend to dress to win approval. Men tend to wear darker colours and coarser textures than women. Adornment Women are more likely to groom and adorn themselves to emphasise smooth and nubile qualities (makeup, body hair reduction), while males may be more inclined to groom themselves to emphasise coarseness and toughness (facial hair, self-mutilation through piercing and tattoos). Body awareness There is greater cultural pressure for females than males to appear more fit and less fat. Females are more prone to take radical action to modify their body shapes. Responsiveness and awareness Overall, women tend to be more responsive than men in non-verbal communication, and tend to be more sensitive to the non-verbal communication of others — both when non-verbal messages reinforce verbal messages and when they contradict non-verbal messages . Men may be harder to ‘read’, providing less non-verbal feedback. Sources: Adapted from Lakoff (1990); Glass (1992); Tannen (1990, 1994); Gray (1992); Gamble and Gamble (2003); Stewart, Cooper, Stewart and Friedley (2002); Pearson, Turner and Todd-Mancillas (1995); Trethewey (1999). Chapter 7 Gender and communication Characteristic 7.15 The written word: Men and women writing Let’s now move from the spoken word and non-verbal communication to the written word. As already noted, the degree of overlap and interaction between these forms of communication can be very considerable, but it is nonetheless analytically useful to consider them separately. We will look at two aspects of written language — the question of female versus male style, and the broader question of whether the historical evolution of language has biased or loaded the language in favour of one gender or the other. Male and female registers? Do men and women write in different ways? Are there uniquely male and female styles or registers? Although this might at first seem unlikely, there is some evidence to support the case. Studies of email content suggest that female writers concentrate more on relationship than task topics, using the channel to build rapport and maintain friendship and kin networks, in ways similar to those used in face-to-face and telephone conversation (Boneva, Kraut & Frohlich 2001; Colley & Todd 2002). Comparisons of graffiti in female and male toilets reflect this tendency: graffiti in female toilets tends to be more polite and interactive, while that in male toilets is more argumentative and negative (Green 2003). Statistical analysis of fiction and non-fiction texts reveals similar trends, with female writers tending to use a more personal or involved style (particularly evident in the use of pronouns), while male writers seem to use a more informational and detached style. A correlation has even been made between male style and non-fiction genres, on the one hand, and female style and fiction writing on the other (Argamon, Koppel, Fine & Shimoni 2003). Man-made? Unloading language and becoming inclusive Spender (1990) and others have argued that language is ‘man-made’ — that is, because males have historically exercised power over females, language itself reveals loaded or biased structures. Examples include the use of generic pronouns, generic nouns or titles, spotlighting, diminutivisation, differential naming and featurism, all of which appear to suggest a world in which females play secondary roles to males (Romaine 2000; Goddard & Patterson 2000) (table 7.3). Such patterns, of course, occur in many languages other than English. Communicating in the 21st Century Table 7.3: Biased or loaded language phenomena 7.16 Generic pronoun: using the masculine pronouns ‘he’ and ‘his’ in all situations, irrespective of the gender of those referred to Generic noun or title: using nouns or titles with male connotations in all situations, even when females are involved Spotlighting: using a modifier to draw attention to a role adoption that challenges a gender-role stereotype Phenomenon Example Analysis Generic pronoun ‘If a ratepayer wants to pay this in person, he must bring payment to the Council Offices payment desk.’ Embodies assumption that both females and males can be adequately described by a male pronoun. Generic noun or title • • • • • chairman man-made manpower foreman salesman Historically, these roles or associations related to men; social changes are not necessarily reflected in title changes. Spotlighting • • • • • • lady doctor male nurse male prostitute career girl male model woman priest The use of an adjective draws attention to a role adoption that challenges a gender-role stereotype. Phenomenon Example Analysis Diminutivisation: attaching diminutive suffixes to names, implying childishness, lower status or affection Diminutivisation • • • • • Roles and names are formed by adding lower-status and/or affectionate suffixes to titles and names. Differential naming: using family names and titles for males and first names for females Differential naming ‘Mr Smith [the men from Accounts] and Jenny [the girls from Marketing] want to see you.’ Males may be referred to by adults titles, while females may be referred to by pre-adult titles. Featurism: drawing attention to the personal rather than role characteristics of female office-holders Featurism ‘Joe Breeze was today sworn in as President. He vowed to introduce a new era of freedom and prosperity.’ ‘Jo Breeze was today sworn in as President. Sporting a smart new pageboy cut and a mauve Armani jump suit, set off with turquoise pearls, she vowed to introduce a new era of freedom and prosperity.’ Focus on appearance of females tends to trivialise and demean. actress authoress mayoress Charlotte Paulette Gender-neutral expression Table 7.4: Strategies for gender-neutral writing Rule Unrevised form Revised form Use a pair of pronouns A manager needs to know what his project budget is going to be. Just ask any nurse what she thinks of her profession, and that might help your career planning. A manager needs to know what his or her budget is going to be. Just ask any nurse what she or he thinks of her or his profession, and that might help your career planning. Use the slash/ combined form The clerk will need to have a requisition form signed before he can obtain a modem. The clerk will need to have a requisition form signed before s/he can obtain a modem. The clerk will need to have a requisition form signed before he/ she can obtain a modem. Recast the sentence to omit the genderspecific pronoun/s The average student may end up spending too much of his money on software. The operator needs to equip himself or herself with these protective devices. The average student may end up spending too much money on software. The operator needs to become equipped with these protective devices. Use imperative mood of verbs He or she can load the floppy disk. Load the floppy disk. Chapter 7 Gender and communication How then can we express ourselves in a more gender-neutral way in our writing? Traditional sex or gender roles have undergone dramatic transformation in recent times, and this has meant that we need to think more carefully when we use gender-specific language, ensuring we have a language for humans, not just men. It makes more sense these days, therefore, to use more inclusive language when referring to persons of either gender. Some strategies that can be employed to create inclusiveness are shown in table 7.4. 7.17 Communicating in the 21st Century Table 7.4 (continued) 7.18 Rule Unrevised form Revised form Replace third-person pronouns with secondperson pronouns He or she must clean up the conference room at the end of each meeting. You must clean up the conference room at the end of each meeting. Use plurals The modern plumber knows that he cannot neglect the paperwork if his business is to thrive. He or she will find that the Z2000 model has a number of advantages when compared to its predecessor. Modern plumbers know that they cannot neglect the paperwork if their businesses are to thrive. Users will find that the Z2000 model has a number of advantages when compared to its predecessor. Repeat the noun The builder will find all the traditional lines of nails and screws, now in metric or SI. In fact, in converting from imperial measures, we have taken the opportunity to expand the product range, giving him or her more, not fewer, resources. The builder will find all the traditional lines of nails and screws, now in metric or SI. In fact, in converting from imperial measures, we have taken the opportunity to expand the product range, giving the builder more, not fewer, resources. Replace third-person pronouns with indefinite article (a, an) or definite article (the) The manager or his assistant . . . The manager or an assistant . . . Change secondperson pronouns to generic pronoun (sometimes occurs with subjunctive) — use sparingly) She will need to consider her position on this. One would need to consider one’s position on this. Change a nominal to a verbal expression A person who has in his possession such prohibited substances will in fact have broken the law. A person who possesses such prohibited substances will in fact have broken the law. Change if … then clauses to who/ which/that clauses If a staff member uses the scanner in such a manner, then he will damage it. A staff member who uses the scanner in such a manner will damage it. Change if … when clauses to on/upon phrases, or modifiers without expressed subjects When the manager has completed this procedure, he should have the blue form witnessed by another person. Upon completing this procedure, the manager should have the blue form witnessed by another person. Recast restricting/ spotlighting names, titles and roles • chairman • manhole • man-made • manpower • foreman • salesman • shopgirl • actress • authoress • woman/female/lady doctor • chair, chairperson • access hatch • artificial, synthetic, constructed • human resources, workforce • supervisor, team leader • sales person, representative • staff member, salesperson • actor • author • doctor Source: Adapted from Corbett (1990); and Troyka and Hesse (2007). ASSESS YOURSELF Rewrite the following passage, making it more gender-neutral. You may need to re-cast sentences. Consult online chapters 1 to 4 for clarification of some technical terms. The typical accountant today needs to have his wits about him when it comes to software. While his forefathers may have been satisfied with ink and paper records, the accountant with his eye on future trends knows that such a ‘hard copy’ approach is simply too inflexible. He must be able to manipulate data electronically, and his data must be compatible with other software packages. If, for example, his spreadsheet and data base files cannot be smoothly integrated with his assistant’s word processing package, then she will not be able to quickly and flawlessly integrate text and data into meaningful reports. The professional man of the future must understand these technological matters, or else he will end up on the organisational scrapheap. Styles of communication and conflict Table 7.5: Male versus female approaches to conflict Incident Analysis A female doctor had approached a male hospital administrator and simply described what she needs by way of space. The administrator gave her only part of what she wanted. She is annoyed; he is perplexed that she is annoyed. The administrator treated the doctor as he would any other staff member. He presumed her request was merely an artificially inflated opening bid in a negotiating ritual; she thought it was a straightforward rational process. In a venture capital firm a male negotiator, returning after putting together a deal, said he had brought home another pelt. A female negotiator at the same firm saw the same acquisition as completing a process that welcomed the new company into the community. The male sees the acquisition in terms of a hunting metaphor, a win–lose situation; the female sees it from a cooperative, win–win perspective. Male and female students ask questions of a female lecturer. The lecturer finds the females’ questions charming, but finds the males’ questions cheeky. The lecturer tells her husband of her views, but he supports the male students. The husband thought it natural to challenge an expert. The female lecturer, like the females in the class, thought that access to an expert was a chance to learn inside information and make personal contact. Sources: Adapted from Tannen (1990) and Kolb (1992). Chapter 7 Gender and communication Let’s now move back to the more general areas of communication to further pursue the concepts of gender conflict and misunderstanding. Tannen, Gray and others suggest that males tend to see life as a contest, where one’s worth has to be continually reasserted and defended against assault. This can mean a competitive rather than a cooperative approach to problem solving. Non-physical contests, such as arguments or negotiations, may become combative or antagonistic, with men seeing them as arenas for using language or logic as a weapon, and experiencing the thrill of a contest. This win–lose, zero-sum game mentality interprets life as a struggle to gain hierarchical dominance (see chapter 14, ‘Negotiation skills’, p. 441 and chapter 15, ‘Conflict management’, p. 484). Females, some researchers contend, experience reality in quite a different way, approaching disputes as an opportunity to seek cooperation (Gray suggests that many females have been socialised into a lose–win or sacrifice mentality). With men and women assuming diametrically opposed mind-sets, misunderstandings and confusion can arise, some examples of which are shown in table 7.5. 7.19 There may be profound biological and historical reasons to explain this male combativeness, although it may be based on personal insecurity as much as a perception of objective threats. Whether this mentality transfers to workplace success is another matter. Although it is routine now for people to endorse ‘aggressive’ business strategies, hyper-competitive behaviour does not always pay off. According to Tannen, ‘studies show that women and men who have been successful in the traditionally male fields of business and science are not very competitive. Rather, they excel in “work competence” or “work mastery.” They simply do their jobs extremely well’ (1990, p. 181). Women are defining new roles and new ways of behaving, from car saleswomen learning how to ‘attack nicely’ to women executives endeavouring to make workplaces less hostile and more cooperative (Lawson 1994; Portello & Long 1994). It has also been suggested that formal and ritualised conflict resolution systems such as the adversarial courts system and union–management grievance procedures are more likely to be used by men, whereas women may prefer ‘softer’ systems such as mediation (Astor & Chinkin 2002) (see chapter 15, p. 498). These developments are interesting, although we have to be careful of replacing one sexist view (‘Women are unassertive, manipulative and bitchy’) with another (‘A world run by women politicians and managers would automatically be a more peaceful and equitable place’). It may be that it is not sex or gender traits that determine approaches to conflict but rather role expectations: if a manager is rewarded for being a hostile bully, then a female manager appointed to the role will be under pressure to adopt similar methods; if a negotiator receives praise and prestige for being assertive and aggressive, then a female negotiator is likely to follow this lead (Power 1994). Gender and communication: The state of play We have covered a lot of ground in our consideration of gender and communication. The key questions remain: Are there any real differences between male and female styles of communicating, and if there are, are they best explained by the constructionist (environment/ nurture) model or the essentialist (heredity/nature) model? We can interpret the evidence we have discussed so far in a number of ways. Let’s consider each of them now. Communicating in the 21st Century 1. The differences are about power, not gender 7.20 Some analysts of gender and communication concede that there are differences in the way people in general communicate, but that these differences are best explained not by sex or gender but by power, context and the roles people play. Thus, communication patterns such as politeness, tag questions, hedges and deferential non-verbal communication are not unique to females but are used by people, irrespective of gender, in powerless or low-power situations, such as inexperienced court witnesses, unemployed persons, assembly-line workers, uneducated persons or servants (Hopper 2003, p. 184; Mills 2003, p. 205). Norms of politeness and courtesy may be a class phenomenon (Mills 2003). Crawford (1995) suggests that assertiveness training is fundamentally wrong-headed because it merely confirms the ‘woman-as-problem’ view, creating a set of techniques to mask the underlying reality of the lack of power that most women have in the roles to which they are restricted (see chapter 9, ‘Interpersonal skills 1: emotional intelligence, self-talk and assertiveness’, p. 295). Males raping and harassing females is often more about power than about sex, and concerns have been expressed that ‘miscommunication theory’ — the idea that men and women speak in such different languages that communication breakdown is inevitable — may be used to rationalise male assaults (Frith & Kitzinger 1997). 2. There are no or few real differences anyway Crawford and Chaffin (1997) argue that disproportionate attention is paid to differences (some of which are trivial anyway) and not enough attention to similarities. Romaine (2000) and Hyde (2005) argue that there are few, if any, context-independent gender differences in language. 3. The differences are real, but will unfair use be made of them? Fears have also been expressed that demonstrating widespread and systematic differences between men and women communicating might not necessarily be a good thing, especially for women. If there are differences, and the dominant norms relate to males, does that suggest that the ‘problem’ is women’s perceived inadequacies in direct communication (Weatherall 2002)? Might perceived differences in biology and communication style be used as excuses for new forms of discrimination (Bing 1999)? 4. The differences are real, and they indicate a better world (or perhaps the same old one . . .) Evans (2000) and Hollands (2001) advise women to take note of differences between the sexes, and then to succeed on their own terms, without sacrificing their femaleness in pursuit of a male definition of success. Hatcher (2003) notes that some management theorists have begun to speak of a ‘feminization of management’, whereby women are seen to have natural advantages in a world moving away from hierarchy, individuals and competition towards a world of flatter organisations, teams and cooperation (see chapters 19, ‘Organisational communication’ and 23, ‘Team communication’). In this brave new world, emotion and the ‘soft skills’ of communication, hitherto devalued or demonised, are now seen as panaceas. This view, however, has the potential to lead to a new form of enslavement rather than liberation: Similarly, the notion that women are better suited to team interaction may lead to a perpetuation of women’s inferior status (Benschop & Doorewaard 1998). Men may be adept in picking up and using soft skills simply to consolidate their position vis-à-vis women (Rutherford 2001) (see chapter 9, p. 258). 5. Specific communication patterns exist, but their causes are misinterpreted Some argue that ‘typically female’ communication patterns are not necessarily that at all, and to the extent that they are, the causes are often misinterpreted. Tannen (1995) suggests that Chapter 7 Gender and communication The attention to daily practices of speaking, listening and language use and a range of ‘softening devices’, including styles of handling conflict, techniques for decision-making and the capacity to take responsibility by connecting with others, rather than through controlling others, shores up a particular identity for the manager as an embodied subject. This particular set of images, or idealizations of women’s style of communication, assists in managing an increasingly significant part of the population through the regulation of these micro-practices in the workplace. This focus on understanding women’s ways is a mechanism to increase the self-regulation of both men and women, by making use of emotions in an instrumental way. (Hatcher 2003, p. 404) 7.21 indirect communication is often used by powerful males in work situations, and no-one who wishes to flourish alongside such males is going to misinterpret such signals. Tag questions are not always a sign of powerlessness, and indeed are sometimes a sign of power display (Preisler 1998) and of support and facilitation of others (Weatherall 2002). Interruption patterns vary from situation to situation, with women interrupting men more often than viceversa in some settings, and with some types of interruption being supportive and collaborative rather than hostile (Weiss & Fischer 1998). Where to from here? Many prescriptions for action have been proposed in this field, but if they are to work, they need to take account of the complexities of human interaction. For example, Weiss and Fischer (1998) consider the virtues of indirect styles of communication, noting along the way that western female managers may in fact fare better than western male managers when dealing with non-western, high-context cultures, where indirect rather than direct communication is favoured (although such cultures tend to be less gender-egalitarian than western ones). On the other hand, they quote Madeline Albright, newly appointed as US Secretary of State in 1997, who, when asked what advice she would give to ambitious women, replied, ‘Women need to interrupt’ (Weiss & Fischer 1998). The interconnections between gender communication and intercultural communication are extensive and interesting (see chapter 16). Tannen (1995) recalls teaching a case study about indirect communication: the context was an aircraft cockpit, where the (male) co-pilot had tried to suggest to the (male) pilot that the aircraft was unsafe; the pilot had ignored this indirect communication and had taken off — with fatal results for all. As a result of this actual event, airlines came to recognise the critical shortcomings of indirect communication — irrespective of gender — and began to demand assertiveness training for all air staff. One of Tannen’s Japanese students, however, pointed out that it would have been just as effective to train pilots to pick up on hints. Tannen remarked: Communicating in the 21st Century This approach reflects assumptions about communication that typify Japanese culture, which places great value on the ability of people to understand one another without putting everything into words. Either directness or indirectness can be a successful means of communication as long as the linguistic style is understood by the participants. (Tannen 1995, p. 147) 7.22 Training in assertiveness and in argumentation skills might help females to become more adept in communicating with males (Kosberg & Rancer 2001), but the reservations about assertiveness training by critics such as Crawford (1995) need to be borne in mind. The overwhelming focus in the gender and communication area is on women and females, with the common implication that men and males are either the default setting of communication or the tyrannical elite that must be overthrown. The ideological tenor of much of this work in this area is remarkable, with vested interests and entrenched positions on all sides, but given the rapid changes in sex and gender roles of the past few decades, such zeal is not surprising, and to a certain extent is to be welcomed: everyone has a strong interest in the way this is going to turn out. As the field is a relatively new one, there may be a long way to go before the smoke clears and we get a clearer view of reality. Indeed, we may come to the view that communication differences can be better explained by way of other constructs (e.g. power, leadership, biology or interpersonal skills). Perhaps the research has yet to produce its greatest insights. We will finish by considering advice on inter-gender communication by two of the most popular writers in the field: As a basic guideline [for communicating with your partner]: never argue. Instead, discuss the pros and cons of something. Negotiate for what you want but don’t argue . . . The differences and disagreements don’t hurt as much as the ways in which we communicate them. Ideally an argument does not have to be hurtful; instead it can simply be an engaging conversation that expresses our differences and disagreements. (Inevitably all couples will have differences and disagreements at times.) But practically speaking most couples start out arguing about one thing and, within five minutes, are arguing about the way they are arguing. Unknowingly, they begin hurting each other; what could have been an innocent argument, easily resolved with mutual understanding and an acceptance of differences, escalates into a battle. They refuse to accept or understand the content of their partner’s point of view because of the way they are being approached. (Gray 1992, p. 151) When one’s habitual style is not working, trying hard by doing more of the same will not solve problems. Instead, men and women could both benefit from flexibility. Women who avoid conflict at all costs would be better off if they learned that a little conflict won’t kill them. And men who habitually take oppositional stances would be better off if they broke their addiction to conflict . . . Eventually, perhaps, men therapists — and men in therapy — learn to talk like women. This is all to the good. Assertiveness training, on the other hand, teaches women to talk more like men, and this too is to the good. Women and men would both do well to learn strategies more typically used by members of the other group — not to switch over entirely, but to have more strategies at their disposal. (Tannen 1990, pp. 187, 121–2) Summary Student study guide KEY TERMS constructivism p. 7.2 differential naming p. 7.17 diminutivisation p. 7.17 essentialism p. 7.2 featurism p. 7.17 genderlect p. 7.8 generic noun or title p. 7.16 generic pronoun p. 7.16 ‘Mr Fixit’ syndrome p. 7.9 rapport talk p. 7.8 relational bullying p. 7.8 report talk p. 7.8 spotlighting p. 7.16 troubles talk p. 7.8 Chapter 7 Gender and communication In this chapter we considered the biological and cultural background of sex and gender. Exploring the way these two terms have been used and understood, we noted that sex and gender may be applied to the same thing or to two quite different things. Views about sex and gender can be understood as being situated on an essentialist–constructivist continuum, in a variant of the heredity–environment debate. We identified perceived differences in female and male approaches to spoken, non-verbal and written communication. We explored the basis of communication problems and conflicts between genders. We examined in particular the ideas of the popular writers Deborah Tannen and John Gray, including genderlect, report talk/rapport talk, trouble talk, and Mars/Venus type differences, while noting critiques of their work. The debate about sex, gender and communication tends to revolve around one or several of five positions — namely, the differences are about power, not gender; there are no or few real differences; the differences are real, but unfair use can be made of them; the differences are real, and they indicate a path to a better world (or perhaps the same old one . . .); and specific communication patterns exist, but their causes are misinterpreted. 7.23 REVIEW QUESTIONS 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Are sex and gender the same thing? What are the key differences between E-brains and S-brains? What are the key differences between rapport talk and report talk? What is relational bullying? Identify at least five perceived differences in the speech behaviour of males and females. Identify at least five perceived differences in the non-verbal behaviour of males and females. 7. Identify at least three perceived differences in the writing styles of males and females. 8. Give at least two explanations of why gender is not necessarily the cause of differences in the communication patterns of males and females. APPLIED ACTIVITIES 1. Consider the lists of perceived differences in male and female spoken and non-verbal communication tables 7.1 and 7.2). Create a checklist showing the categories in the lists. Without compromising the privacy or dignity of others, use this checklist to observe males and females in different settings (meetings, public places). Record what you perceive. Does your data confirm or disconfirm what is in the lists? 2. Still working with the lists in tables 7.1 and 7.2, try to think of at least two other spoken and two other non-verbal differences. 3. ‘Gender is a completely unnecessary category. Sex explains everything.’ Discuss. 4. Create a dialogue in which one person is using rapport talk and one person is using report talk. Don’t forget to include non-verbal communication — write in non-verbal aspects as you might do in stage directions in a script. Produce two versions of the script — one in which communication breaks down into conflict, and one in which there is a meeting of minds. 5. Write a passage that features extensive use of generic pronouns, generic nouns or titles, spotlighting, diminutivisation, differential naming and featurism, and then translate it, removing these phenomena. Compare the two versions. Communicating in the 21st Century WHAT WOULD YOU DO? 7.24 Mike is new to the job of area coordinator for the nurses union. He shows up on Monday morning to address a meeting of his members at Central General, the biggest hospital in his area. He knows that they have been unhappy with their treatment by the union head office, especially over their claim for a salary award 30 per cent above what the union is negotiating for. The union director, Marie Stevens, who used to work at Central General and does not have fond memories of the nursing, medical or management staff there, said to him this morning as he left, ‘Put your flak jacket on. Tell them what the policy is, and try to explain to them in words of one syllable what “solidarity” and “gradual gains” mean.’ Mike sits at the table and smiles at 16 grim and suspicious female faces. Lyn Marabel, the shop steward, begins by saying loudly, ‘If you think you can come in here as Marie’s messenger boy, you’ve got another think coming! Here’s what we want to talk about today!’ She shoves a sheaf of paper across the table, which Mike stops by slamming his hand down on it — perhaps harder than he had intended, as the sound reverberates through the table. A chorus of facetious ‘Oooh!’s goes up. Lyn says, ‘Well, men are from Mars, eh? Well mister, don’t try to intimidate us!’ What should Mike say? Ambady, Nalini, Conner, Brett & Hallahan, Mark 1999, ‘Accuracy of judgements of sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 77, no. 3, pp. 538–48. Argamon, Shlomo, Koppel, Moshe, Fine, Jonathan & Shimoni, Rachel 2003, ‘Gender, genre, and writing style in formal written texts’, Text, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 321–46. Astor, Hilary & Chinkin, Christine M 2002, Dispute resolution in Australia, 2nd edn, LexisNexis Butterworths, Sydney. Barnett, Rosalind C & Rivers, Caryl 2004, ‘Men are from Earth, and so are women. It’s faulty research that sets them apart’, The Chronicle of Higher Education, 3 September, www.psychology.uiowa.edu. Baron-Cohen, Simon 2003, The essential difference: man, woman and the extreme male brain, Allen Lane/Penguin, London. Basow, Susan A, & Rubenfeld, Kimberley 2003, ‘Troubles talk’: effects of gender and gender-typing’, Sex Roles, vol. 48. no. 3/4, pp. 183–187. Benschop, Yvonne & Doorewaard, Hans 1998, ‘Six of one and half a dozen of the other: the gender subtext of Taylorism and team-based work’, Gender, Work and Organization, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 5–18. Bing, Janet 1999, ‘Brain sex: how the media report and distort brain research’, Women and Language, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 4–13. Blackmore, Susan & Dawkins, Richard 2000, The Meme Machine, Oxford University Press, New York. Blustain, Sarah 2000, ‘The new gender wars’, Psychology Today, vol. 33, no. 6, pp. 42–9. Boneva, B, Kraut, R & Frohlich, D 2001, ‘Using e-mail for personal relationships: the difference gender makes’, American Behavioral Scientist, vol. 45, no. 3, pp. 530–49. Browne, Kingsley 2002, Biology at work: rethinking sexual equality, Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, NJ/ London. Buss, David 2004, Evolutionary psychology: the new science of the mind, 2nd edn, Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA. Buzzard, Karen S 2002, ‘The Coca-Cola of self-help: the branding of John Gray’s men are from Mars, women are from Venus’, Journal of Popular Culture, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 89–94. Cameron, Deborah 2000, ‘Styling the worker: gender and the commodification of language in the globalized service economy’, Journal of Sociolinguistics, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 323–47. —— 2007, The myth of Mars and Venus, Oxford University Press. Campbell, Anne 2002, A mind of her own: the evolutionary psychology of women, Oxford University Press, Oxford/New York. Casison, Jeanie 2002, ‘On different planets’, Incentive, vol. 176, no. 4, pp. 16–17. Chakrabarti, Rajesh, Jayaraman, Narayanan, & GuptuMukherjee, Swasti 2005, ‘Mars–Venus marriages: culture and cross-border M & A’, Social Science Research Network, November 21, http://papers.ssrn.com. Colley, Ann & Todd, Zazie 2002, ‘Gender-Linked differences in the style and content of e-mails to friends’, Journal of Language and Social Psychology, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 380–92. Corbett, Maryann Z 1990, ‘Clearing the air: some thoughts on gender-neutral writing, IEEE Transactions in Professional Communication, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 2–6. Crawford, Mary 2004, ‘Mars and Venus collide: a discursive analysis of marital self-help psychology’, Feminism & Psychology, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 63–79. Crawford, Mary 1995, Talking difference: on gender and language, Sage, London/Thousand Oaks, CA/New Delhi. Crawford, Mary & Chaffin, Roger 1997, ‘The meaning of difference: cognition in social and cultural context’, in Paula J Caplan (ed.), Gender differences in human cognition, Oxford University Press, New York. DeCapua, Andrea & Boxer, Diana 1999, ‘Bragging, boasting and bravado: male banter in a brokerage house’, Women and Language, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 5–23. DeWall, C. Nathan, & Maner, Jon K 2008, ‘High status men (but not women) capture the eye of the beholder’, Evolutionary Psychology, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 328–341. Dolan, J & Kropf, JS 2004, ‘Credit claiming from the U.S. house: gendered communication styles?’, Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 41–59. Dunbar, Robin, & Barrett, Louise (eds) 2009, Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology, Oxford University Press, New York. Evans, Gail 2000, Play like a man, win like a woman: what men know about success that women need to learn, Broadway Books, New York. Falling Buzzard, Karen S 2002, ‘The Coca Cola of self help: the branding of John Gray’s Men are from Mars, women are from Venus’, Journal of Popular Culture, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 89–102. Fausto-Sterling, Anne 2001, Sexing the body: gender politics and the construction of sexuality, Basic Books, New York. Francis, Richard C 2004, Why men won’t ask for directions: the seductions of socio-biology, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Frith, Hannah & Kitzinger, Celia 1997, ‘Talk about sexual miscommunication’, Women Studies International Forum, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 517–28. Gamble, Teri Kwai & Gamble, Michael 2003, The gender communication connection, Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MA. Glass, Lillian 1992, He says, she says: closing the communication gap between the sexes, Perigee/Penguin, New York. —— 2000, The complete idiot’s guide to understanding men and women, Alpha Books, Indianapolis, IN. Chapter 7 Gender and communication REFERENCES 7.25 Communicating in the 21st Century 7.26 Goddard, Angela & Patterson, Lindsey Meân 2000, Language and gender, Routledge, London/New York. Goodwin, Marjorie Harness 2002, ‘Building power asymmetries in girls’ interaction’, Discourse and Society, vol. 13, no. 6, pp. 715–30. Gray, John 1992, Men are from Mars, women are from Venus: a practical guide for improving communication and getting what you want in your relationships, HarperCollins, New York. —— 2003, How to get what you want at work: a practical guide for improving communication and getting results, HarperCollins, New York. Green, James A 2003, ‘The writing on the stall: gender and graffiti’, Journal of Language and Social Psychology, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 282–96. Guillaume, Cécile & Pochic, Sophie 2008, ‘What would you sacrifice? Access to top management and the work-life balance’, Gender, Work and Organization, DOI 10.1111/ j.1468-0432.2007.00354.x Hall, Judith A & Friedman, Gregory B 1999, ‘Status, gender and nonverbal behavior: a study of structured interactions between employees of a company’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 25, no. 9, pp. 1080–91. Harvey, Keith 2002, ‘Camp talk and citationality: a queer take on “authentic” and “represented” utterance’, Journal of Pragmatics, vol. 34, no. 9, pp. 1145–65. Hatcher, Caroline 2003, ‘Refashioning a passionate manager: gender at work’, Gender, Work and Organization, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 391–412. Hobbs, Pamela 2003, ‘The medium is the message: politeness strategies in men’s and women’s voice mail messages’, Journal of Pragmatics, vol. 35, pp. 243–62. Hollands, Jean 2001, Same game, different rules: how to get ahead without being a bully broad, ice queen or ‘ms.understood’, McGraw-Hill, New York. Holmes, Janet 1995, Women, men and politeness, Longman, London. Holmes, Janet & Meyerhoff, Miriam (eds) 2003, The handbook of language and gender, Blackwell, London. Homoly, Paul 1999, ‘Mars and Venus in the dental office: different patterns of behaviour between men and women’, Oral Health, vol. 89, no. 2, pp. 59–62. Hopper, Robert 2003, Gendering talk, Michigan State University Press, East Lansing, IL. Immerman, Ronald S & Mackey, Wade C 2003, ‘The feminist paradox: an index of cultural evolution’, Journal of Social, Political and Economic Studies, vol .28, no. 2, pp. 217–49. Hyde, Janet Shibley 2005, ‘The gender similarities hypothesis’, American Psychologist, vol. 60, no. 6, pp. 581–592. Iredale, Wendy, Van Vugt, Mark, and Dunbar, Robin 2008 ‘Showing off in humans: male generosity as a mating signal’, Evolutionary Psychology, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 386–392. Jesperson, Otto 1922, Language: its nature, development and origin, George Allen & Unwin, London. Kimura, Doreen 1999, Sex and cognition, MIT Press, Boston, MA. Kolb, Deborah 1992, Is it her voice or her place that makes a difference?: a consideration of gender issues in negotiation, Industrial Relations Centre, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario. Koppel, Moshe, Argamon, Shlomo & Shimoni, Anat Rachel 2002, ‘Automatically categorizing written texts by author gender’, Literary and Linguistic Computing, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 401–12. Kosberg, Roberta L & Rancer, Andrew S 2001, ‘Enhancing argumentativeness and argumentative behavior: the influence of gender and training’, in Linda Longmire & Lisa Merrill (eds), Untying the tongue: gender, power and the word, Praeger, Westport, CT/London. Krahe·, B, Waizenhöfer, E & Möller, I 2003, ‘Women’s sexual aggression against men: prevalence and predictors’, Sex Roles, vol. 49, no. 5–6, pp. 219–32. Lakoff, Robin Tolmach 1990, Talking power: the politics of language in our lives, Basic Books, New York. Lawson, HM 1994, ‘“Attacking nicely”: car saleswomen adapt to an incompatible role’, Sociological Viewpoints, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 1–15. Leap, William (ed.) 1997, Beyond the lavender lexicon: authenticity, imagination, and appropriation in lesbian and gay languages, Routledge, London. Lippa, Richard A 2002, Gender, nature and nurture, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ. Livia, Anna & Hall, Kira (eds) 1998, Queerly phrased: language, gender and sexuality, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Lizzio, Alf, Wilson, Keithia L, Gilchrist, Jan & Gallois, Cindy 2003, ‘The role of gender in the construction and evaluation of feedback effectiveness’, Management Communication Quarterly, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 341–79. Loosemore, Martin & Galea, Natalie 2008, ‘Genderlect and conflict in the Australian construction industry’, Construction management and economics, vol. 26, pp. 125–135. Magma, Anna 2003, Female terror: scary women, modern crimes, Virgin Books, London. McGregor, Michael W & Davidson, Karina 2000, ‘Men’s and women’s hostility is perceived differently’, Journal of Research in Personality, vol. 34, pp. 252–61. McHelhinny, Bonnie 2003, ‘Theorizing gender in sociolinguistics and linguistic anthropology,” in Janet Holmes & Miriam Meyerhoff (eds), The handbook of language and gender, Blackwell, London. Miller, Alan S, & Kanazawa, Satoshi 2008, Why beautiful people have more daughters: from dating, shopping, and praying to going to war and becoming a billionaire: two evolutionary psychologists explain why we do what we do, Perigee, New York. Mills, Sara 2003, Gender and politeness, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. gathering: an evolutionary perspective on sex differences in website preferences and navigation’, IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication, vol. 51. no. 2, pp 155–177. Stewart, Lea P, Cooper, Pamela J, Stewart, Alan D & Friedley, Sheryl 2002, Communication and gender, 4th edn, Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA. Tannen, Deborah 1990, You just don’t understand: men and women in conversation, William Morrow and Company, New York. —— 1994, Talking from 9 to 5: how women’s and men’s conversational styles affect who gets heard, who gets credit, and what gets done at work, William Morrow and Company, New York. —— 1995, ‘The power of talk: who get heard and why’, Harvard Business Review, vol. 73, no. 5, pp. 138–48. —— 2002, I only say this because I love you: how the way we talk can make or break family relationships throughout our lives, Virago, London. Thornhill, Randy, & Gangestad, Steven W, 2009, The evolutionary biology of human female sexuality, Oxford University Press, New York. Tomlinson, Asha 2002, ‘Working on different planets’, Canadian HR Reporter, vol. 15, no. 3, p. 3. Trethewey, Angela 1999, ‘Disciplined bodies: women’s embodied identities at work’, Organization Studies, vol. 20. no. 3, pp. 423–51. Troyka, Lynn Quitman, & Hesse, Douglas 2007, Simon & Schuster handbook for writers, 8th edn, Simon & Schuster, New York. Vanfossen, Beth 1996, ‘Gender differences in communication’, www.towson.edu. Weatherall, Ann 2002, Gender, language and discourse, Routledge, London/New York. Weiss, Edmond H & Fischer, Bronwyn 1998, ‘Should we teach women to interrupt? Cultural variables in management communication courses’, Women in Management Review, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 37–44. Wood, Julia T 2002, ‘A critical response to John Gray’s Mars and Venus portrayals of men and women’, The Southern Communication Journal, vol. 67, no. 2, pp. 201–11. Wood, Julia T 2003, Gendered lives — communication, gender and culture, 5th edn, Wadsworth, Belmont, CA. Zimmerman, Toni, Schindler, Haddock, Shelley A, & McGeorge, Christine R 2001, ‘Mars and Venus: unequal planets’, Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 55–68. Zurbriggen, Eileen L, & Sherman, Aurora M 2007, ‘Reconsidering “sex” and “gender”: two steps forward, one step back’, Feminism & Psychology, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 475–480. Zuk, Marlene 2004, Sexual selections: what we can and can’t learn about sex from animals, University of California Press, Los Angeles. Chapter 7 Gender and communication Moir, Ann 2001, Why men don’t iron: the fascinating and unalterable differences between men and women, Citadel, New York. Noonan, Jo Howarth 2007, Men are from Mars, women are from Venus: an analysis of a potential meme, Master’s thesis, Georgia State University, http://etd.gsu.edu. Pearson, Judy Cornelia, Turner, Lynn H & Todd-Mancillas, William R 1995, Gender & communication, 3rd edn, Wm. C. Brown/McGraw-Hill, Dubuque, IA/New York. Peterson, Valerie 2000, ‘Mars and Venus: the rhetoric of sexual planetary alignment’, Women and Language, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 1–8. Pinker, Steven 2003, The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature, Viking, New York. Portello, JY & Long, BC 1994, ‘Gender role orientation, ethical and interpersonal conflicts, and conflict handling styles of female managers’, Sex Roles, vol. 31, no. 11–12, pp. 683–701. Power, M 1994, ‘Does a woman negotiator have to be like a man?’, Australian Dispute Resolution Journal, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 49–61. Preisler, Bent 1998, ‘Deconstructing feminist linguistics’, Journal of Sociolinguistics, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 281–95. Quina, Kathryn, Harlow, Lisa L, Morokoff, Patricia J, Burkholder, Gary & Deiter, Pamela J 2000, ‘Sexual communication in relationships: when words speak louder than actions’, Sex Roles, vol. 42, no. 7/8, pp. 523–50. Reuther, Donald F 2003, Gaydar: the ultimate insider guide to the gay sixth sense, Crown, New York. Romaine, Suzanne 2000, Language in society: an introduction to sociolinguistics, 2nd edn, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Rosenthal, Robert, & Jacobson, Lenore 1992, Pygmalion in the classroom, expanded edn, Irvington, New York. Roush, Mark L 2007, ‘Lighting people are from Mars, LED folks are from Venus’, Lighting Design and Application, vol. 37, no. 12, pp. 57–60. Rutherford, Sara 2001, ‘Any difference? an analysis of gender and divisional management styles in a large airline’, Gender, Work and Organization, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 326–45. Siegel, Muffy EA 2002, ‘Like: the discourse particle and semantics’, Journal of Semantics, vol. 19, pp. 35–71. Simmons, Rachel 2002, Odd girl out: the hidden culture of aggression in girls, Harcourt, New York. Sotirin, Patty 2000, ‘“All they do is bitch, bitch, bitch”: political and interactional features of women’s office talk’, Women and Language, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 19–26. Spender, Dale 1990, Man-made language, 2nd edn, Routledge, London. Stenstrom, Eric, Stenstrom, Phillippe, Saad, Gad, & Cheikhrouhou, Soumaya 2008, ‘Online hunting and 7.27 Communicating in the 21st Century SUGGESTED READING 7.28 Allen, Louisa 2003, ‘Girls want sex, boys want love: resisting dominant discourses of (hetero) sexuality’, Sexualities, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 215–36. Annis, Barbara 2004, Same words, different language: why men and women don’t understand each other and what to do about it, Piatkus, London. Arliss, Laurie P & Borisoff, Deborah J (eds) 2000, Women and men communicating: challenges and changes, Waveland Press, Long Grove, IL. Ashcraft, Karen & Mumby, Dennis K 2004, Reworking gender: a feminist communicology, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA/ London. Backland, Philip M & Willams, Mary Rose 2004, Readings in gender communication, Wadsworth, Belmont, CA. Brizendine, Louann 2007, The female brain, Broadway, New York. Brock, Sabra & Dooley, Joseph 2005, Men head east, women turn right: how to meet in the middle when facing change, Adams Media, New York. Buzzanell, Patrice M, Sterk, Helen & Turner, Lynn H (eds) 2004, Gender in applied communication contexts, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA/London. Cameron, Deborah, & Kulick, Don (eds) 2006, The language and sexuality reader, Routledge, London and New York. Chrisler, Joan C 2007, ‘The subtleties of meaning: still arguing after all these years’, Feminism & Psychology, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 442–446. Cowlishaw, Bridget Roussell 2001, ‘Subjects are from Mars, objects are from Venus: construction of the self in self-help’, Journal of Popular Culture, vol. 35, no.1, pp. 169–184. DeFrancisco, Victoria Pruin, & Palczewski, Catherine Helen 2007, Communicating gender diversity: a critical approach, Sage, London. Elmore, Cindy 2007, ‘Recollections in hindsight from women who left: the gendered newsroom’, Women & Language, vol. 30, no. 2, pp 18–27. Eckstein, Daniel & Goldman, Alan 2001, ‘The couples’ gender based communication questionnaire’, Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 62–74. Glick, E, Garber, L, Holland, S, Balderston, D & Quiroga, J 2004 ‘New directions in multiethnic, racial and global queer studies’, GLQ: A Journal of Lesbian and Gay Studies, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 123–37. Hearn, Jeff, Kimmel, Michael S & Connell, Robert W (eds) 2005, Handbook of studies on men and masculinity, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Holmes, Janet & Stubbe, Maria 2003, Power and politeness in the workplace, Pearson, London. Holmes, Mary 2005, Sociology of gender, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Ivy, Diana K & Backlund, Phil 2004, GenderSpeak: personal effectiveness in gender communication, McGraw-Hill, New York. Jansen, Sue Curry 2002, Critical communication theory: power, media, gender and technology, Rowman & Littlefield, Lanham, MD. Kendall, Shari 2004, ‘Framing authority: gender, face and mitigation at a radio network’, Discourse and Society, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 55–79. Knöfler, Tobias, & Imhof, Margarete 2007, ‘Does sexual orientation have an impact on nonverbal behavior in interpersonal communication?’ Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, vol. 31, pp. 189–204. Krolokke, Charlotte & Sorensen, Ann Scott 2004, Contemporary gender communication theories and analyses: from silence to performance, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA/London. Lohan, M & Faulkner, W 2004, ‘Masculinities and technologies: some introductory remarks’, Men and Masculinities, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 319–29. Luchjenbroers, June 2002, ‘Gendered features of Australian English discourse: discourse strategies in negotiated talk’, Journal of English Linguistics, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 200–16. Meyerhoff, Miriam 2006, Introducing sociolinguistics, Routledge, London and New York. Miller, Gloria E 2004, ‘Frontier masculinity in the oil industry: the experience of women engineers’, Gender, Work and Organization, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 47–73. Paul, Elizabeth L 2002, Taking sides: clashing views on controversial issues in sex and gender, 2nd edn, McGrawHill/Dushkin, Guilford, CT. Payne, Kaye E 2001 Different but equal: communication between the sexes, Praeger, Westport, CT. Pease, Allan & Pease, Barbara 2001, Why men don’t listen and women can’t read maps: how we’re different and what to do about it, Broadway Books, New York. Peterson, Valerie 2000, ‘Mars and Venus: the rhetoric of sexual planetary alignment’, Women & Language, vol. 23 (XXIII), no. 2, pp. 1–7. Real, Terrence 2002, How can I get through to you? Reconnecting men and women, Simon and Schuster, New York. Reeder, Heidi M 1996, ‘A critical look at gender difference in communication research’, Communication Studies, vol. 47, no. 4, pp. 318–31. Schiffrin, Deborah, Hamilton, Heidi Ehernberger & Tannen, Deborah (eds) 2003, The handbook of discourse analysis, Blackwell, London/New York. Sunderland, Jane 2006, Language and gender: an advanced resource book, Routledge, London and New York. Watson, Tennessee Jane 2006, ‘The profound language of mothers and daughters: a Ms. conversation with Deborah Tannen’, Ms., vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 42–44. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Chapter 7 Gender and communication Quote, pp 7.4–5: © Dooren Kimura, ‘Sex and Cognitition'. Reproduced with permission of MIT Press. Quote, p. 7.5: ‘Brain sex: how the media report and distort brain research', © Janet Bing, Women and Language, 22 (2), 1999, pp. 4–12. Figure 7.2, p. 7.6: From ‘Gender, nature and nurture' by Richard A Lippa, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2002, p. 196. Reproduced with permission from Copyright Clearance Center. Quote, pp. 7.9–10: © 2008 Billboard. Reprinted with permission from ‘Incentive'. 7.29