Lay Dispositionism and Implicit Theories of Personality
Transcription
Lay Dispositionism and Implicit Theories of Personality
Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/97/S3.00 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1997, Vol.73, No. I, 19-30 Lay Dispositionism and Implicit Theories of Personality Chi-yue Chiu Ying-yi Hong University of Hong Kong Hong Kong University of Science and Technology Carol S. Dweck Columbia University Lay dispositionism refers to lay people's tendency to use traits as the basic unit of analysis in social perception (L. Ross & R. E. Nisbett, 1991). Five studies explored the relation between the practices indicative of lay dispositionism and people's implicit theories about the nature of personal attributes. As predicted, compared with those who believed that personal attributes are malleable {incremental theorists), those who believed in fixed traits (entity theorists) used traits or trait-relevant information to make stronger future behavioral predictions (Studies 1 and 2) and made stronger trait inferences from behavior (Study 3). Moreover, the relation between implicit theories and lay dispositionism was found in both the United States (a more individualistic culture) and Hong Kong (a more collectivistic culture), suggesting this relation to be generau'zable across cultures (Study 4). Finally, an experiment in which implicit theories were manipulated provided preliminary evidence for the possible causal role of implicit theories in lay dispositionism (Study 5), Lay dispositionism refers to the use of traits as the unit of analysis in social perception (L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991). The inferential practices that have been shown to be associated with lay dispositionism include (a) viewing behavior as reflecting its correspondent disposition (Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Jones, 1990; Jones & Davis, 1965; Nisbett & Ross, 1980), (b) the belief that behavior in a particular situation can be predicted from knowledge of the relevant trait (Kunda & Nisbett, 1986; L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991), and (c) the expectancy that behavior will be consistent across situations (Kunda & Nisbett, 1986; cf. Wright & Mischel, 1987). Some writers (e.g., Jellison & Green, 1981) believe that in the United States, most individuals are brought up to assume that behaviors reflect personal dispositions. Indeed, the fact that people may spontaneously infer traits from behavior has led some social psychologists to believe that dispositional inference is an automatic outgrowth of perceptual experience (Baron & Misovich, 1993). However, research has indicated that the likelihood and the meaning of dispositional inferences may vary with the perceiver's inferential goals (Krull, 1993; Krull & Dill, 1996; Lee, Hallahan, & Herzog, 1996), cultural experiences (Cousins, 1989; Lee et al., 1996; Miller, 1984), and the perceiver's construal of the person-situation relation (Shoda & Mischel, 1993). Such situational variability and individual differences argue against the view that trait inference is an inevitable consequence of perceptual experience. In the present research we sought to further understand the psychological processes that may underlie individual differences in lay dispositionism, focusing on the perceiver's conceptions of the nature of personal attributes. Recently, there has been increasing recognition among psychologists that an individual's judgments are influenced by the implicit theories the individual has about the self and other people (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995a; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; M. Ross, 1989; see also Heider, 1958; Kelly, 1955). These theories are referred to as implicit theories because unlike most scientific theories, these theories are rarely explicitly articulated. Nonetheless, they may set up an interpretive frame within which information is processed. In this article, we propose that lay people have implicit theories about human nature and that the kind of implicit theory a person holds is related to the tendency to subscribe to lay dispositionism. More specifically, we predicted that the tendency to use traits as the basic unit of analysis would be greater among those who believe that an individual's personality consists of fixed, static traits {entity theorists) than among those who believe that it consists of dynamic personal qualities that can be changed and developed (incremental theorists). Chi-yue Chiu, Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong; Ying-yi Hong, Department of Psychology, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology; Carol S. Dweck, Department of Psychology, Columbia University. Preparation of this article and the research reported in it were supported in part by University of Hong Kong Research Grants 337/014/ 0010 and 337/014/0015. We thank Nino Sidari and Catherine Parker for their assistance in data collection. We also thank Sheri Levy for the information on the expanded measure. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Chiyue Chiu, Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong or to Carol S. Dweck, Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New "Vbrk, New \brk 10027. Electronic mail may be sent via the Internet to [email protected],hk or to [email protected]. The idea that perceivers' implicit theories may guide the way they make social inferences was first articulated by Jones and Thibaut (1958), who proposed that perception of a target' 'will tend to be imposed on the stimulus person by the perceiver; the missing link in the inference chain will be supplied by the 19 20 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK perceiver's own 'theory' of personality" (p. 166). In the present research we focused on how implicit theories about some structural properties (fixedness or malleability) of personal attributes may provide an epistemic context for trait inferences and the meaning of traits. As an overview, the present research consisted of a series of five studies designed lo test the predicted relation of implicit theories and lay dispositionism. First, the proposed relation between implicit theories and the inferential practices indicative of lay dispositionism was systematically investigated in Studies 1, 2, and 3. In Study 4, we attempted to establish the crosscultural generalizability of this relation. Finally, in Study 5, participants' implicit theories were manipulated to explore possible causal relations between implicit theories and lay dispositionism. Background: Implicit Theories and Self-Judgments Previous research by Dweck and her associates (see Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Hong, Chiu, & Dweck, 1995) has shown that individuals' beliefs about the malleability of self-attributes predict judgments of the self (see also Robins & Pals, 1996). For example, in the domain of academic achievement, those who believe their intelligence to be a fixed entity (entity theorists) tend to focus on judging their level of intelligence. That is, they are concerned with how much of this fixed commodity they possess. They also tend to see academic failures as indications of their intellectual ability, ascribing lo themselves global, stable, negative ability traits on the basis of a limited number of failure experiences. By contrast, those who view their intelligence as a malleable trait (incremental theorists) tend to place the emphasis on developing versus judging their ability. In the face of obstacles, they tend to focus on the factors that mediate improved performance and increased ability (e.g., effort, problemsolving strategies) rather than on self-judgment. This relation appears to hold in the domain of social interaction as well (Erdley, Cain, Loomis, Dumas-Hines, & Dweck, 1997; see also Benenson & Dweck, 1986; Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1994; Goetz & Dweck, 1980). Holding an entity versus incremental theory of one's personality predicts a focus on selfjudgment and a tendency to attribute global, stable negative traits to oneself in the face of rejection. In short, a belief in fixed self-attributes appears to be associated wilh a greater tendency to process information in terms of traits and trait evaluation. By contrast, a belief in malleable self-attributes is associated with a greater tendency to process information in terms of process variables that mediate performance. ality is viewed as a set of fixed traits, then understanding their personality may involve assessing their fixed underlying trails. Thus, entity theorists may view the task of person perception as being to judge or diagnose these underlying traits from the available behavioral information. Moreover, because these (raits are believed to be fixed, a diagnosis made at one time should be predictive of behavior at a later time. In contrast, incremental theorists, who believe in more dynamic, malleable qualities that can vary systematically over time and perhaps across contexts, may view the task of person perception as being to understand the dynamics of behavior, rather than simply to judge dispositional traits. In doing so, they may be more likely to take account of the actor's states (e.g., goals, intentions) as well as relevant aspects of the situation (Hong, 1994). If this is true, then they should display weaker trait inferences and be more sensitive to the effects of the situation on behavior. Past research has provided support for the relation between entity versus incremental theory and one of the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism; namely, the tendency to draw global trait inferences from behaviors even when there is a plausible situational explanation for the behaviors. Specifically, Erdley and Dweck (1993, Experiment 2) assessed late grade school children's entity versus incremental theory of personality and later showed them a narrated slide show of a ' "new boy in school," who, in an attempt to make a good impression and to hide his ignorance, performed a variety of transgressions (e.g., trying to cheat on some work, appropriating another child's leftover materials). None of these transgressions hurt others, and the situational pressures were emphasized throughout. After the slide show, children were asked to make a number of judgments about the boy. The results revealed that entity theorists were significantly more likely to make global trait inferences ("bad," "mean," and "nasty") from these behaviors than were incremental theorists. The five studies reported in this article further explored the relation between implicit theories and the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism. Studies 1, 2, and 3 were designed to provide further evidence for the link between implicit theories and judgments of others by addressing the inferential links that characterize the lay oppositionist model of behavior depicted in Figure 1. As shown in the figure, in the lay dispositionist model, behaviors across situations are assumed to be mediated by enduring dispositional traits. According to L. Ross and Nisbett (1991), if perceivers believe that behaviors across situations are manifestations of the same underlying trait. Implicit Theories and Social Perception This research sought to extend previous work on implicit theories and judgment, focusing on the relation between implicit theories and the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism. Tn the domain of social perception, research by Dweck and her associates (Erdley & Dweck, 1993; see also Dweck, Hong, & Chiu, 1993) suggests that just as a fixed view of the self is associated with a greater emphasis on evaluating selfattributes, a fixed view of others may be associated with a greater emphasis on judging others' traits. When others' person- implies (Study 3), Dispositional Trait Behavior in One Situation Figure I. predicts (Study 1) predicts JStudy 2) Behavior in a New Situation A lay dispositionist model of behavior. 21 LAY DTSPOSITIONISM perceivers will expect a relatively high level of cross-situational consistency in these behaviors. For example, knowing that Jack is more aggressive than John in one situation, a lay dispositionist would predict Jack to be more aggressive than John in another situation as well. Thus, if entity theorists believe more strongly than incremental theorists in a trait analysis of behavior, they should expect greater cross-situational consistency in trait-relevant behaviors. This hypothesis was tested in Study 1. In addition, when presented with information about a person's trait, a lay dispositionist should be fairly confident that this information can reliably predict this person's behavior in a new situation (L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991). For example, if Jack is known to be more aggressive than John, on average, a lay dispositionist will predict with relatively high confidence that Jack will act more aggressively than John in any particular situation. In Study 2 we addressed the relation between subscribing to an entity versus incremental theory and the tendency to make confident predictions of specific behavior from knowledge of a person's traits. If entity theorists ascribe a more important role to traits than do incremental theorists, they should also be more confident in making these predictions. Study 3 focused on making confident judgments of aperson's traits on the basis of knowledge of the person's behavior (e.g., if Jack hits John in a particular situation, to what extent can it be concluded with confidence that Jack is an aggressive person?). As Figure 1 illustrates, when presented with a person's behavior, a lay dispositionist should be quite confident that this behavior is indicative of the person's traits (L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991). Accordingly, in this study, we predicted that relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists would be more ready to make trait inferences from behaviors. Study 4 examined the cross-cultural generality of the relation between implicit theories and lay dispositionism. In relatively individualistic cultures (e.g., American culture), people are socialized to identify themselves as individuals who can leave groups at will and are expected to behave according to personal motivation. Thus, these cultures may focus more on the individual as the origin or agent of social actions (Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994). By contrast, in relatively collectivistic cultures (e.g., Indian and Chinese cultures), people are expected to identify themselves as group members and behave according to such things as role expectations and group norms (Chiu & Hong, in press; Ho & Chiu, 1994; Hofstede, 1980). These cultures may thus focus on role expectations, group norms, or other contextual constraints as the impetus for social action (Morris & Peng, 1994). Will the relation between entity versus incremental theory and lay dispositionism hold in individualistic cultures only, or will it hold in both individualistic and collectivistic cultures? In other words, are entity and incremental theories indigenous cultural constructions developed to give meaning to social behavior in individualistic cultures, in which the emphasis is on personal agency (Miller, 1984; Shweder & Sullivan, 1990)? Or are they more general worldviews that individuals in all cultures tend to develop to make sense of the stability and variability in humatfactions (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995b)? Study 4 was designed to address this issue. Finally, to establish the causal relation between implicit theories and the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism, in Study 5, we manipulated participants' implicit theo- ries by asking them to read a (fictitious) Psychology Todaytype article that presented persuasive evidence for an entity or incremental theory. We predicted that, compared with those who read an incremental theory article, participants who read an entity theory article would be led to make stronger global trait inferences from behaviors. In summary, a number of phenomena in person perception are associated with lay dispositionism. The present research tested the hypothesis that the tendency to exhibit some of these phenomena is linked to a specific theory of personality. Specifically, having a static, entity theory was predicted to orient an individual toward using traits as the unit of analysis and toward subscribing to the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism. In contrast, holding a dynamic, incremental view was expected to attenuate the strength of lay dispositionism and make its allied inferential practices less likely. In addition, the cross-cultural generalizability and causal nature of these relations were explored. The idea that implicit theories may provide an interpretive framework for social inferences dates back to Jones and Thibaut (1958). In line with this research tradition, in the present research we further explored the role of perceiverinduced constraints in social perception by placing the perceiver's implicit theories of personality at the heart of person perception. Study I One important index of lay dispositionism is the expectancy that trait-relevant behaviors will be consistent across situations (the behavior-to-behavior link depicted in Figure 1). As L. Ross and Nisbett (1991) posited, if perceivers believe that behaviors are mediated by traits, they will expect a person who possesses a particular trait to act in a manner consistent with this trait across even very different situational settings. Thus, a lay dispositionist is inclined to make confident predictions of a person's behavior in new situations on the basis of knowledge of this person's trait-relevant behavior in a single prior situation.' In this study, consistent with the view that entity theorists are more likely to be lay dispositionists, we predicted that relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists would make more confident predictions of a person's behavior on the basis of knowledge of that person's behavior on one prior occasion. Method Participants The participants were 40 undergraduates (11 men and 29 women) at Columbia University who participated in the study to obtain course 1 In this study, we were interested more in entity versus incremental theorists' relative tendency to make confident predictions about crosssituational behavioral consistency than in how accurate their predictions were. Note, however, that in Kunda and Nisbett (1986), a strong expectancy of cross-si tuational consistency was regarded as a prediction error, given the consistent finding in personality research that the correlation of trait-relevant behavior across different situations is typically around .10 and seldom exceeds .20 (see Mischel, 1968;Mischel & Peake, 1982; Peterson, 1968). 22 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK requirement credit. Because no gender effects were found in this or any of the other studies, gender is not included in the analyses reported. Measures Measure of implicit person theories. Although implicit theories can be domain specific (e.g., a person can have an entity theory about moral character and an incremental theory of intelligence; see Dweck et al., 1995a), and although it is most desirable to make domain-specific assessments and predictions, the stimulus materials we adapted from Kunda and Nisbett (1986, see below) cut across the domains of morality, sociability, and intelligence. Therefore, we used a more domain-general measure of implicit theories. This implicit person theory measure was embedded in a battery of other measures designed to assess styles of coping with negative events and consists of three items: "The kind of person someone is is something very basic about them and it can't be changed very much;" "People can do things differently, but the important parts of who they are can't really be changed;'1 and "Everyone is a certain kind of person and there is not much that can be done to really change that." Respondents indicated their extent of agreement with each item on a 6-point scale with responses ranging from 1 {very strongly agree), 2 (agree), 3 (mostly agree), to 6 (very strongly disagree). Unlike individual-difference measures that tap generalized needs or cognitive styles, our measure taps one simple, unitary belief. The items are intended to have the same meaning, and continued repetition of the same idea becomes bizarre and tedious to the respondents. Thus, only three items were included. Respondents' implicit person theory was indexed by their mean score on these three items. Several issues pertaining to the format of the theory measure should be addressed. First, items depicting an incremental theory were not included in this measure because previous studies (Boyum, 1988; Leggett, 1985) and our pilot studies found that, even for respondents who endorsed items depicting entity theories, there was a strong tendency to endorse items depicting the opposite, incremental, theory, as well as a tendency to drift toward incremental choices over items. This suggests that the incremental items are highly compelling and perhaps more socially desirable as well. However, to ensure that disagreement with the entity theory statements can be taken to represent agreement with the incremental theory, in one study (see Dweck et al., 1995b), respondents were given the theory measure and asked to explain their choices. Those who disagreed with the entity statements gave clear incremental theory justifications for their responses. Also, in past studies in which implicit theories have been experimentally manipulated (Bergen, 1991; Dweck, Tenney, & Dinces, reported in Dweck & Leggett, 1988), participants given an incremental theory have behaved in ways that are similar to those who disagreed with the entity theory statements on our scale (see also Study 5 in the present article). Further, in the domain of intelligence, in which the theory measure has the same format, an incremental theory (i.e., disagreement with the entity theory) has been linked to a whole network of theoretically related variables, such as goals, attributions, affect, and behavior (see Dweck, 1991, for a review). Finally, Levy and Dweck (1996) have just obtained direct evidence, with an expanded implicit theory measure, that those who disagree with an entity theory on the present measure are the ones who agree with the incremental theory on the expanded new measure. The expanded new measure has avoided the overly compelling nature of past incremental items by presenting a very strong form of the incremental theory (e.g., "Everyone, no matter who they are, can significantly change their basic characteristics" and "All people can change even their most basic qualities" ). On the expanded measure, the correlation between the entity items (which are basically the same as the ones on the present measure) and the incremental items was - . 7 3 . with a sample size of 652. In a sample of 101 participants who took both the present measure and the expanded measure, with a delay of 1 week or less between the two measures, the correlation between the present measure and the expanded measure was .83. Of those who were classified as incremental theorists on the present measure, 88% were classified as incremental theorists on the expanded measure and only 9% were classified as entity theorists. Of those who were classified as entity theorists on the present measure, 90% were classified as entity theorists, and none were classified as incremental theorists on the new measure. In short, with the awareness that our present implicit theory measure does not provide a direct assessment of incremental theory, we have obtained evidence to show that disagreement with an entity theory (the belief that attributes cannot be changed) can be taken to represent agreement with an incremental theory (the belief that attributes can be changed). For these reasons, and for consistency with our past work, in the present article, we refer to those who disagreed with the entity items as "incremental theorists," but with appropriate caveats. Second, in the current format, although endorsement of an entity theory entails agreement with the items, previous studies have shown that agreement with these statements does not represent an acquiescence set. For example, MacGyvers (1992) found that there was no relation between respondents' endorsement of an entity theory and the tendency to agree with arbitrary items presented in a forceful and compelling way. In a similar vein, in a number of studies (e.g., Chiu, Dweck, Tong, & Fu, in press; see also Study 3 in the present article), we included in the measure or in the study control items that entailed agreement or disagreement. Entity theorists never showed a greater tendency to agree with items that are not theoretically relevant. Finally, Dweck et al. (1995a, 1995b) found that even though the implicit person theory measure has the same format as measures of implicit theories about nonhuman attributes (e.g., implicit theory about the malleability of the world), they are statistically independent. Previous studies (Dweck et al., 1995a) have shown that this scale is a reliable measure, with a high internal reliability (alphas ranging from .90 to .96 in our previous studies and from .73 to .90 in the present research). Test-retest reliability for a 2-week interval was .82. As far as the construct validity of the measure is concerned, the measure does not correlate with academic aptitude (verbal and quantitative SAT ' scores) or with standard measures of socially desirable responding (the Paulhus, 1984, Social Desirability Scale) and self-presentation (the Snyder, 1974, Self-Monitoring Scale). This indicates that the measure is not confounded with intellectual ability or self-presentation concerns. Also, it does not correlate with optimism about human nature, the Coopersmith ( 1967) measure of self-esteem, or with the Altemeyer (1981) Right-Wing Authoritarianism Scale and the Kerlinger (1984) measures of conservatism and liberalism (see Dweck et a!., 1995a). Thus, it is not confounded with positivity or negativity about the self and others or with the respondents' ideological rigidity or political stance. Finally, recall that the defining dimension of implicit theories and their assessment is whether personal attributes are believed to be fixed or malleable. The theories reflect one simple belief and do not contain in their definition a processing style component or a motivational component. They are thus distinct from other process-oriented individual differences variables such as personal need for structure (Neuberg & New som, 1993), atlributional complexity (Fletcher, Danilovics, Fernandez, Peterson, & Reeder, 1986), and need for cognition (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). In a justcompleted study by Levy and Dweck (1997) the correlations between implicit theories and these process-oriented individual differences variables were only between .17 and .24. In the present study, respondents' ratings on the three items were averaged to yield an overall theory score. To ensure that only participants who had a clear theory were included, data from 10 participants (25%) who scored between 3.0 and 4.0 were excluded from the theory group comparison analyses but were retained in the correlational analyses, in which we treated endorsement of entity theory as a continuous variable. Participants who scored at 3.0 or below (n = 15) were classified as 23 LAY DISPOSITIONISM entity theorists, and those who scored at 4.0 or above were classified as incremental theorists (n = 15). The behavioral prediction measure. The Kunda and Nisbett (1986) questionnaire, consisting of four questions, was used to assess participants' behavioral predictions. Each question asks for a prediction of a target's behavior in a particular situation on the basis of knowledge of the target's behavior in one completely different prior situation. Two of the four questions ask for behavioral predictions in the social domain (honesty and friendliness), and two ask for behavioral predictions in ability domains (academic performance and performance in a basketball game). An example is, Suppose you observed Jack and Joe in one particular situation and found that Jack was more friendly than Joe. What do you suppose is the probability that in a completely different situation, you would also find Jack to be more friendly than Joe? Respondents are asked to indicate their prediction on a probability scale from 0.00 to 1.00. Clear instructions were included to ensure that the participants understood how to use the probability scale. The theory measure was given to the participants together with a battery of measures designed for another study. Then the Kunda and Nisbett (1986) questionnaire was presented to the participants as an unrelated study. Results and Discussion Participants' predictions within each domain (social traits vs. ability) were averaged. Agreement with entity theory (treated as a continuous variable) correlated positively with cross-situational consistency predictions in the social domain (r — .61, p < .001), in the ability domains (r = .45, p = .01), and in the two domains combined (r — .49, p < .01). We also subjected the average predictions in each domain to a 2 X 2 (Implicit Theory X Domain) analysis of variance (ANOVA). As expected, the implicit theory main effect was significant, F( 1, 28) = 12.96, p - .001. Entity theorists made stronger predictions than incremental theorists both in the social domain (M ~ .57 v s . M = .42),?(28) = 4.03,/?< .001, and in the ability domains (M = .55 vs. M = .44), r(28) = 2.70, p = .01. Interestingly, the overall prediction of incremental theorists was significantly below .50 (95% confidence interval [CI] = .4275 ±.0687). This indicates that incremental theorists, for whom an individual's personality does not consist of fixed traits, did not believe that exhibiting more trait-relevant behavior than another person in one situation meant that the individual would exhibit that kind of behavior more than the other person in another situation. Indeed, for them, if a person was found to be friendlier than another person in one situation, this relation would more likely than not be reversed in a different situation. In contrast, entity theorists' overall prediction was significantly greater than .50 (95% CI = .5583 ± .0348). For them, if one person was found to be friendlier than another person in a particular situation, this relation would more likely than not generalize to a completely different situation. It thus appears that entity theorists expect trait-relevant behavior to be consistent across situations. It also appears that incremental theorists, when given no information other than the relative occurrence of a certain kind of behavior in a single situation, tend not to predict the same relative likelihood of displaying that kind of behavior in a different situation. On the contrary, lacking other information, they may expect this relative likelihood to "even out" across situations (cf. Tversky & Kahneman, 1982). Study 2 Another inferential practice associated with lay dispositionism is the expectation that knowledge of a person's traits is a highly reliable basis for making behavioral predictions (the trait-to-behavior link in Figure I). 2 In this study, we thus predicted that relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists would make stronger predictions of a target's behavior in a particular situation on the basis of information regarding the target's traits. Method Participants The participants were 50 undergraduates (18 men and 32 women) at the State University of New %rk at Purchase who were paid $4 for their participation in the study. They all completed the implicit person theory measure described in Study 1. On the basis of the same criteria as in Study 1,21 participants were classified as entity theorists; 11 participants were classified as incremental theorists; and data from 18 participants, who did not have a clear theory, were excluded from the group comparison analyses but were retained in the correlational analyses in which endorsement of entity theory was treated as a continuous variable. Measures In addition to the implicit person theory measure, the participants were given a 10-item questionnaire. On each item, trait information about a target was presented, and participants were asked to make a prediction about the target's behavior in a particular situation. One example is, "Henry is more aggressive than Edward on average. What do you suppose is the probability that Henry would act more aggressively than Edward in a particular situation?" Participants were given the same probability scale as in Study 1 (0.00 to 1.00) to indicate their predictions. The 10 items covered positive and negative traits, as well as traits with unclear valence: defensiveness, talkativeness, aggressiveness, dishonesty, helpfulness, shyness, xtudiousness, conscientiousness, boastfulness, and spontaneity. Results For each participant, the 10 predictions were averaged to give an overall prediction score. Agreement with entity theory (treated as a continuous variable) was positively correlated with the overall prediction score (r — .38, p < .05). Theory group 2 Again, we were not interested in whether entity theorists were more accurate in their predictions than were incremental theorists, or vice versa, although it should be noted that personality research has found knowledge about a person's trait to be a rather unreliable predictor of a person's behavior in a particular situation (see Mischel, 1968, 1973; L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991). Specifically, the correlation between a person's trait measured by a personality inventory and that person's traitrelevant behavior in a particular situation seldom exceeds .30 (see Mischel, 1968, 1973). Indeed, the correlation coefficient of .30 has been referred to in the literature as the personality coefficient (see Mischel, 1973). 24 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK comparisons performed on this overall score revealed a significant implicit theory effect, /(30) = 2.30, p < .05. As predicted, relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists made stronger predictions about the targets' behavior in a particular situation on the basis of information about the targets' traits (M = .78 vs. M = .64). Study 3 Tn summary, Studies 1 and 2 provided support for the hypothesis that entity theorists would exhibit a greater tendency than incremental theorists to display the inferential practices of the lay dispositionist model depicted in Figure 1. Study 1 showed that if a trait-relevant behavior is known to have taken place on one occasion, entity theorists tend to expect the same type of behavior to recur on other occasions in different situations (the behavior-to-behavior link in Figure 1). In Study 2 we found that compared with incremental theorists, entity theorists tended to make stronger predictions of a person's behavior in a particular situation on the basis of knowledge of this person's traits in the relevant domain (the trait-to-behavior link). In Study 3 we tested entity and incremental theorists' relative tendency to make a confident inference about a person's trait on the basis of knowledge of behavior in a single situation (the inferential practice specified by the behavior-to-trait link in Figure 1). If this hypothesis is confirmed, this would further indicate that for entity theorists more than for incremental theorists, dispositional traits mediate behaviors and thereby lend consistency to behaviors across situations. Method Participants The participants were 37 college students (23 men and 14 women) at the State University of New YorY. at Purchase who volunteered to take part in the study during a class meeting. None of them had participated in Study 2. Measures Measure of implicit theory of character. Recall that implicit theories are domain specific (see Dweck et al., 1993) and that judgments in a particular domain are best predicted by implicit theories of attributes in the same domain. In Studies 1 and 2, we were interested in the participants' predictions of behavior across domains (e.g., morality, intelligence, and sociability). We therefore measured the participants' general theory about the fixedness or malleability of basic human attributes. In this study, we were interested in how participants made judgments of a person's moral character on the basis of a single morally relevant behavior. Therefore, instead of assessing participants' general theory as we did in Studies 1 and 2, we assessed their implicit theory in a more specific domain—moral character. The measure of implicit theory of character consists of three items, each depicting an entity theory of character: (a) "A person's moral character is something very basic about them and it can't be changed much,'" (b) "Whether a person is responsible and sincere or not is deeply ingrained in their personality. It cannot be changed very much," and (c) "There is not much that can be done to change a person's moral traits (e.g., conscientiousness, uprightness, and honesty)." Participants were required to indicate on a 6-point scale, with responses ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree), how much they agreed with each statement. Evidence attesting to the reliability and validity of the measure is reviewed in Dweck et al. (1995a). Very briefly, the internal reliability coefficients of the measure obtained from past studies ranged from .85 to .94. Test-retest reliability for a 2-week interval was .80. In addition, the measure did not correlate with a pessimistic or negative view of human nature, social presentational tendencies as measured by the Paulhus ( 1984) Social Desirability Scale and Snyder's (1974) Self-Monitoring Scale, or with intellectual competence (SAT scores). Finally, the measure did not correlate with locus of control factors (Levenson, 1974), ideological rigidity such as authoritarianism (Altemeyer, 1981), or conservatism and liberalism (Kerlinger, 1984). Among the 37 participants in this study, 10 scored 3.0 or below on this measure and were classified as entity theorists; 20 scored 4.0 or above and were classified as incremental theorists. The remaining 7 participants did not have a clear theory, and, as in Studies 1 and 2, their data were excluded from the theory group comparisons but were retained in the correlational analyses. Measure nf trait judgments. To assess participants' trait judgments, a list of 35 behaviors was compiled. These behaviors ranged from clearly positive behaviors (e.g., "risking one's life for another") to mildly positive behaviors (e.g., "making one's bed in the morning"'), mildly negative behaviors (e.g., "interrupting someone who is speaking"), and clearly negative behaviors (e.g., "stealing a car"). The participants were asked to indicate on a rating scale the extent to which each of these behaviors was indicative of the goodness or badness (moral character) of the person who displayed the behavior. The scale ranged from 1 (not a! all indicative) to 5 (very indicative). The degree of rated indicativeness was taken as an index of trait judgments. In addition to the indicativeness judgments, participants were also asked to judge the positivity and negativity of each behavior on a 9point scale with responses ranging from 1 (verypositive), to 5 (neutral), to 9 (very negative). This rating allowed us to test whether any difference between entity theorists and incremental theorists on the indicativeness judgment was due to a difference in the perceived positivity or negativity of the behavior. For example, if entity theorists made more extreme indicativeness judgments than did incremental theorists, it could be because entity theorists perceived the behavior to be more extreme than did incremental theorists. Results and Discussion As predicted, entity theorists perceived the behaviors to be more indicative of the actors' moral character than did incremental theorists. Moreover, as analyses of the evaluation of the behaviors revealed, this was not because entity theorists perceived the behaviors as being more extreme than did incremental theorists. To construct an index of the perceived extremity of the behaviors, we recoded the participants' evaluation of each behavior's negativity versus positivity from the original scale (from 1 to 9) to a scale that ranged from —4 to 4. The absolute value on the transformed scale was then used to index how extremely positive or negative the behavior was perceived to be. We then took the average of the extremity scores for all 35 behaviors. The correlation between the extremity score and the implicit theory measure (treated as a continuous variable) was not significant (r = .01, ns). Theory group comparison analyses performed on the average extremity score revealed no significant differences between entity and incremental theorists, ?(28) = 0.07, p = .95. The average extremity scores for entity theorists (M = 2.58) and incremental theorists (M = 2.56) were virtually 25 LAY DISPOSITIONISM identical. Indeed, the two theory groups did not differ in their evaluation of the positivity or negativity on any of the 35 behavioral items. This finding also suggests that incremental theorists were not less inclined than entity theorists to make evaluative judgments as long as these judgments were attached only to the behaviors and not to the performers of the behaviors (cf. Bassili, 1989; Trope, 1986, 1989; Trope & Liberman, 1993). Despite the fact that entity theorists and incremental theorists made identical evaluations of the overall extremity of the behaviors, they differed significantly in how much they perceived these behaviors to be indicative of a person's character. Agreement with entity theory and the average of the 35 trait indicativeness judgments was significant ( r = — . 5 0 , p < .01).Theory group comparisons performed on the average trait indicativeness judgments revealed a clear difference between entity and incremental theorists, r(28) = 3.09, p < .005. Entity theorists, to a significantly greater extent than incremental theorists, believed that a person's character could be revealed in a single behavior this person performed (M = 3.62 vs. M = 2.96 on a 1 - 5 scale). Was this effect independent of the valence of the behaviors, or was it localized in judgments of either negatively or positively valenced behaviors? To answer this question, we divided the 35 behaviors into three categories on the basis of the participants' evaluations of the behavior on the original 9-point scale. Specifically, among the 35 behaviors, 15 had an average score between 1 and 3.5 and were classified as positive behavior, and 16 had an average score between 6.5 and 9 and were classified as negative behavior. The remaining 4 behaviors had an average negativity score between 3.5 and 6.5 and were classified as behaviors with weak valence. The average trait indicativeness rating for each category of behavior was then computed, and a 2 X 3 (Implicit Theory x Valence) ANOVA, with the second factor as a within-subject variable, was performed on the average trait indicativeness score. The results revealed a clear implicit theory main effect, F ( l , 28) = 12.19, p < .005. The Implicit Theory X Valence interaction was not significant, F(2, 56) — 1.20, p = .30. The insignificant interaction effect suggested that the difference between entity theorists and incremental theorists in the perceived trait indicativeness of a behavior was independent of the valence of the behavior. Indeed, as Table 1 shows, relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists judged a behavior to be more indicative of a person's character regardless of whether the behavior was clearly positive, clearly negative, or only weakly valenced. Thus, entity theorists' more extreme trait indicativeness judgments were not localized in posi- Table 1 Trait Indicativeness Judgment Scores: The Effect of Implicit Theories Entity theorists tive or negative behaviors but may reflect the general framework for social inference their theory has set up.3 In summary, together with the findings from Studies 1 and 2, the findings from the present study suggest that relative to incremental theorists, entity theorists believe more strongly in the lay dispositionist model of behavior. First, they are more inclined to make confident predictions of a person's behavior in a particular situation on the basis of knowledge of this person's relevant traits (Study 2). In addition, they believe more strongly that even a single behavior is indicative of a person's character (Study 3) and expect a higher level of cross-situational consistency in behavior (Study 1). Study 4 Studies 1, 2, and 3 showed that, at least among American participants, an entity theory of personal attributes is systematically linked to the tendency to subscribe to lay dispositionism. However, different cultures may construct different models of social inference (Cousins, 1989; Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994). Whereas the individual is seen as the primary agent of social actions in individualistic cultures (e.g., American culture), role expectations, situational norms, and contextual constraints are seen as primary regulators of social actions in collectivistic cultures (e.g., Chinese culture; Hong, Chiu, & Kung, in press; Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 1994). Will the relation between an entity theory of human attributes and the tendency to subscribe to lay dispositionism be found in cultures that put less emphasis on the person in understanding social events? To address this question, Study 3 was replicated with a sample of Hong Kong Chinese students and a sample of American students. Method The participants were 196 (54 men and 142 women) Chinese students from the University of Hong Kong and 124 students (47 men and 77 women) from Columbia University who had not participated in any of the previous studies. One week after the Chinese students filled out the implicit theories, they were asked to fill out the trait judgment measure in an "unrelated" study. This was the same measure as the one used in Study 3, in which descriptions of behaviors were presented and participants were asked to rate on a 9-point scale (from 1 to 9) the extent to which each of these behaviors was indicative of the moral character of the person who displayed the behavior. The American participants filled out the implicit theories measure and the trait judgment measure in the same experimental session, but the two measures were separated by a number of filler questionnaires. The implicit theories measure, the trait judgment measure, and the filler questionnaires were presented to the American participants as a battery of measures put together by investigators from different research projects for convenience of administration. To ensure that the relation between implicit theories and lay dispositionism was not mediated by participants' confidence in other people's morality, we also assessed the degree to which participants viewed peo- Incremental theorists 3 Behavior Positive behavior Weakly valenced behavior Negative behavior M SD M SD 3.82 3.15 3.54 0.61 0.77 0.86 3.05 2.14 3.08 0.85 0.77 0.69 There was a significant valence main effect, F(2, 56) = 11.55, p < .001. Independent of the participants' implicit theory, behaviors with clear valence were judged to be more indicative of a person's character dian were behaviors with unclear valence (M = 3.31 for positive behaviors, M = 3.23 for negative behaviors, and M — 2AS for behaviors with weak valence), r(29) = 6.19, p < .0001. 26 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK pie as basically moral or immoral. The confidence measure consists of three pairs of items (e.g., ' 'I believe that most people will take advantage of others if they can" vs. "Ibelieve that most people are trustworthy"). Participants were asked to choose one statement from each pair and rate it using a 3-point scale with responses ranging from 1 (very True for me) to 3 (sort of true for me). Responses to each item were recoded into a 6-point ( 1 - 6 ) scale with a higher score indicating higher confidence. The confidence measure was formed by taking the average of the three items. Finally, both the Chinese and the American participants were asked to fill out the 16-item Brown Collectivism Scale {Brown et al., 1992) after they had completed the trait indicativeness measure. Examples of items from the Brown Collectivism Scale are "It is more important that my group does well than that I do well as an individual" and "Working with others is usually more trouble than it's worth" (reverse scoring). Participants indicated the extent of their agreement with each of these statements on a 6-point scale with responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The collectivism score was the average agreement with the 16 items. Results Compared with the American participants, the Chinese participants scored higher on collectivism (M = 3.71 vs. M - 2.19 on a scale from 1 to 6), ?(316) = 27.04,/? < .0001, and had greater confidence in others' morality (M = 3.37 vs. M = 2.94), r(318) = 6.76, p < .001. The two samples did not differ in their tendency to endorse an entity theory of morality (M = 3.15 and M = 3.12, respectively), *(316) = 0.33, ns. To test the effects of cultural groups and implicit theories on trait indicativeness judgments, we performed a multiple regression analysis. In this analysis, average trait indicativeness judgment was regressed on cultural groups (American vs. Chinese), implicit theories, and the Cultural Groups x Implicit Theories interaction. In addition, morality confidence and collectivism were included as covariates. The implicit theories main effect was significant, f (1, 312) = 14.98,/? < .001. In line with our hypothesis, for both American and Chinese students, endorsement of an entity theory was positively related to average trail indicativeness judgments (r — .22, p < .01 for American students, r = .24, p < .01 for Chinese students, and r — .23, p < .001 for American and Chinese students combined). The Cultural Groups X Implicit Theories interaction was not significant, F( 1, 312) = 0.70, ns, indicating that the relation between implicit theories and trait indicativeness was not moderated by the participants' cultural group membership. The cultural group effect was also significant, F ( l , 312) = 5.97, p < .05. The least squares means indicated that after the effects of implicit theories, confidence in others' morality, and collectivism had been partialled out, American students still exhibited a slightly greater tendency to make trait indicativeness judgments than did Hong Kong Chinese students (M = 3.14 vs. M = 2.84). Confidence in others' morality was not related to trait indicativeness judgments, F ( l , 312) = 0.03, m (r = .06 for Chinese students and r = —.14 for American students). Recall that implicit theories predicted trait indicativeness judgments even after the effect of confidence was partialled out in the multiple regression analyses. Together, the results suggest that confidence in others' morality did not mediate the relation between implicit theories and trait judgment.4 In summary, there was support for the cross-cultural general- ity of the association between implicit theories of morality and the tendency to make moral trait inferences from behavior. Study 5 Thus far, we have shown a consistent association between implicit theories and dispositional judgments. However, our underlying model posits a causal relation, with the theories setting up a framework within which different types of inferences are plausible. It is therefore important to test the hypothesized causal relation. In two past studies (Dweck, Tenney, & Dinces, 1982, cited in Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Bergen, 1991), theories of intelligence have successfully been manipulated by means of reading passages. In the second case, with adults, Bergen provided participants with a Psychology Today -type article that compellingly presented either the entity or the incremental theory. The article vividly described and quoted from extensive research purporting to document the theory. In both studies the theory manipulation produced significant effects on major dependent measures that were in no way touched on in the passages. In the present study, theories of personality and character were manipulated. Each participant was given a short article tn read that presented either the entity or the incremental theory. Although participants might arrive with strong predispositions toward one theory or the other, both theories may represent basic modes of thought that are at some level familiar to most individuals, and the manipulations were expected to lead participants to adopt that particular mode of thought. The entity article informed participants that human nature used to be thought of as a bundle of potentialities, each of which could be developed, but that scientists, through years of rigorous research, had arrived at the view that people possessed a finite set of rather 4 In this multiple regression analysis, collectivism was positively related to trait indicativeness judgments, F(l, 312) = 14.54, p < .001. More collectivistically oriented individuals tended to make stronger trait judgments from behavior (r = .21, p < .01 for American students, r = .13,/J = .06 for Chinese students, and r = .16,/> < .001 for both groups combined). However, as noted, past studies (Lee et al., 1996; Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994) have found that, compared with individualists, collectivists, who are more sensitive to interpersonal influences on behavior, are less likely to attribute behavior to internal causes. The positive relation between collectivism and dispositional judgment suggests that although both dispositional inference and internal attribution are important aspects of lay dispositionism, they may be different cognitive processes; that is, one can infer a trait from behavior but not see it as the cause of the behavior (Hamilton, 1981). It is possible that, relative to individualists, collectivists, keeping group welfare in mind, place more responsibility on individuals to regulate their social behavior for the common good and are thus more likely to judge people's morality on the basis of whether they do or do not do so. That is, their character judgments will be based on the degree to which individuals perform socially positive versus socially negative actions. However, collectivists may still place more emphasis than individualists on social-situational factors as causes of behavior. For example, to collectivists, a morally weak person may simply be someone who is more susceptible to negative situational forces. Thus, although they may judge this person as immoral, they may still view the causes of the actor's behavior as residing more in social factors than do individualists (Miller & Bersoff. 1994). This issue would be an interesting one for future research. 27 LAY DISPOSITIONISM fixed traits. In contrast, the incremental article informed participants that the view of human nature as a set of fixed traits was now outmoded and had been replaced by the view of dynamic human potentialities that could be cultivated and developed over a lifetime. Each view was buttressed with vivid descriptions of research and case studies. No mention was made in the passage of anything directly relevant to any of the dependent measures. For example, one paragraph of the entity article said, In his talk at the American Psychological Association's annual convention held at Washington D.C. in August, Dr. George Medin argued that 'in most of us, by the age of ten, our character has set like plaster and will never soften again.' He reported numerous large longitudinal studies which show that people 'age and develop, but they do so on the foundation of enduring dispositions.' The same paragraph in the incremental article read, In his talk at the American Psychological Association's annual convention held at Washington D.C. in August, Dr. George Medin argued that 'no one's character is hard like a rock that cannot be changed. Only for some, greater effort and determination are needed to effect changes.' He reported numerous large longitudinal studies which show that people can mature and can change their character. He also reported research findings showing that people's personality characteristics can be changed even in their late sixties. Method Forty-six students (14 men and 32 women) from Columbia University, who had not participated in any of the previous studies, were recruited as participants. They were told that they were participating in three studies that were grouped together for efficiency. Each part was given in a separate booklet, with a separate study name, and a separate consent form. The first part, the theory manipulation, was called a study of social comprehension and memory (comprehension of and memory for personrelevant information). The second part, a filler task, was called a lexicaldecision task, and the third part was called a social judgment task. Participants were told after they finished reading the article that the comprehension and memory tests on the article would come later in the session. Then, they were presented with a filler task, which was a 15min lexical-decision task, and were asked to decide whether a string of letters presented on a computer screen was a word. Then, they filled out a four-item short form of the behavioral prediction questionnaire used in Study 2 (e.g., "Tina is more defensive than Mara on average, what do you suppose is the probability that Tina would act more defensively than Mara in a particular situation?") and a six-item short form of the trait judgment measure used in Study 3. Finally, as manipulation checks, four questions were included to assess the degree to which the participants found the article difficult to understand, credible, and persuasive and the degree to which they agreed with the views espoused in the article. Participants were asked to indicate their responses to each of these questions on an 8-point scale with responses ranging from 1 (not at all) to 8 (very much). To probe for suspiciousness, we had participants write down what they thought the research hypotheses were in this research. None of them saw a connection between the reading comprehension task and the judgment task. Thus, the findings from this study could not be due to the participants' effort to apply what they had read from the passage to the judgment task. Finally, previous research (Lord, Lepper, & Preston, 1984; see also McGuire, 1964) has shown that exposing people to arguments from both sides reduces the biases produced by a one-sided persuasive message. We therefore showed participants in the debriefing session both articles and discussed with them both viewpoints. They were told that, actually, both views were widely held, and they were given references for further reading. It was hypothesized that participants who were led to believe in fixed traits would tend to view behaviors as indicative of such traits and would tend toward dispositional judgments more than those who received the incremental passage. Results and Discussion In general, participants found the article easy to understand (Af - 1.42, SD = 0.70), credible (M = 5.98, SD = 1.22), and persuasive (M = 5.67, SD - 1.34). They also tended to agree with the views espoused in the article (Af = 6.71, SD — 0.72). The theory manipulation (entity vs. incremental) did not have a differential effect on these manipulation checks (all fs < 1). Thus, the two articles were equally effective in leading the participants to agree to the implicit theory presented in the article. As predicted, participants who were led to believe in an entity theory of personality scored higher on the behavioral prediction questionnaire than did participants who were led to believe in an incremental theory (Af = 77% vs. M = 69%), F ( l , 44) = 4.27, p < .05. Thus, compared with incremental participants, entity participants made stronger predictions about the targets' behavior in a particular situation on the basis of information about the targets' traits. Entity participants also scored significantly higher on the trait indicativeness judgment measure than did incremental participants (Af = 3.72 vs. M = 3.27), F ( l , 44) = 5.18, p < .05, indicating that entity participants made stronger trait judgments from trait-relevant behaviors than did incremental participants. General Discussion Although lay dispositionism is often thought of as a tendency that is shared by those in American culture, the present research shows that it is more characteristic of some people than others. It appears that for entity theorists—those who believe in fixed traits—more than for incremental theorists, traits are the basic unit with which social events are analyzed and understood. Specifically, in comparison to incremental theorists, entity theorists seem to be more confident that trait-relevant behaviors will be consistent across situations (Study 1). They also appear to believe more strongly that (a) knowing a person's traits allows one to make confident predictions about that person's behaviors in a new situation (Studies 2 and 5) and (b) knowing how a person behaved in a particular situation allows one to make confident inferences about this person's traits in the relevant domain (Studies 3, 4, and 5). Furthermore, the relation between implicit theories and dispositional inferences was found in cultures characterized by markedly different patterns of social understanding (Study 4 ) . This suggests that entity and incremental theories are not simply indigenous cultural constructions people in individualistic societies develop to understand social behavior. Instead, they may be more general worldviews individuals in all cultures can develop to give meaning to their social experiences. Finally, there is initial evidence that implicit theories can partially explain the tendency to subscribe to lay dispositionism (Study 5). What might mediate the relation between an entity theory 28 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK and the tendency to subscribe to lay disposicionism? Could it be the theory group differences in pessimism, response biases, or cognitive astuteness? Or is it the different interpretive frames the two theories set up to understand social events, as we proposed? It is evident from Study 4 that confidence in others' morality does not mediate the relation between implicit theories and trait judgments because confidence in others did not predict trait judgments, and implicit theories predicted trait judgments even after the effect of confidence was statistically controlled. Moreover, the results from Studies 1-3 show clearly that compared with incremental theorists, entity theorists tended to make more positive, as well as more negative, disposition-related inferences. If the findings in the present research were due to response biases on measures of social perception, then we should expect entity theorists to display more extreme responding than incremental theorists on all of the social perception measures, but that was not the case. In Study 3, although entity theorists made more extreme trait indicativeness judgments than did incremental theorists, their evaluations of the behaviors in the same study were identical to those of incremental theorists. In short, entity theorists displayed more extreme responding than incremental theorists only when our theory predicted that they would do so. Furthermore, as previously noted, our past research (see Dweck et al., 1995a, 1995b) showed that endorsing an incremental theory is not associated with social desirability or selfmonitoring as measured by the Paulhus Social Desirability Scale (Paulhus, 1984) and the Synder Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder, 1974). Thus, it is not the case that incremental theorists are more concerned with self-presentation than are entity theorists and, therefore, tend not to admit making extreme trait inferences. Endorsing an incremental theory is also not related to the respondents' performance on standardized tests of conceptual ability (e.g., the Academic Promise Tests; Bennett et al., 1965) or tests of academic aptitude (SAT scores) or to their selfperceived intellectual ability. This shows that incremental theorists are not more cognitively competent and hence more able to deal with social information in a complex way than entity theorists are. Future research will continue to search for specific mediators of the implicit theories effects. Our proposal is that an individual's implicit theory about the fixedness or malleability of personality sets up an interpretive framework for understanding the social world. As the present research suggests, along with entity theorists' belief in fixed traits, there appears to be a belief in traits as the unit of social understanding and a belief in a close correspondence between traits and behaviors. These beliefs allow entity theorists to make the three kinds of inferences illustrated in Figure 1. In contrast, incremental theorists, for whom traits are malleable and personality is, thus, dynamic, tend not to weigh static traits as heavily in understanding people and their behaviors. Hence, they are less likely than entity theorists to display the inferential practices associated with lay dispositionism. Instead, as previous research (Chiu, 1994; Hong, 1994) has shown, incremental theorists tend to put relatively greater emphasis on the dynamic psychological factors that mediate behaviors (e.g., goals, intentions, expectancies, and affect). For example, Chiu (1994, Study 1) found that when trait and goal information were explicitly presented, entity theorists used traits, whereas incremental theorists used goals as the major basis for organizing impressions of people. Similarly, Hong found that when college students were presented with behavioral descriptions (e.g., "Alexis stole the bread from the bakery shop") and were asked to generate causal explanations for the behaviors, entity theorists offered appreciably and significantly more trait responses (e.g., "Alexis stole the bread from the bakery shop probably because he was a thief or a dishonest person") than did incremental theorists. Incremental theorists, by contrast, had a greater tendency to explain the behaviors by referring to the actor's internal psychological states (e.g., "Alexis stole the bread from the bakery shop probably because he was hungry or desperate"). In short, there appear to be clear differences between entity and incremental theorists in their relative emphasis on traits versus psychological states as explanations. An entity versus incremental theory, as a general epistemic principle, may have wide implications for various inferential practices. Fur example, entity and incremental theorists may be differentially liable to trait-based stereotyping. Given entity theorists' greater readiness to assign traits to people, will they be more ready to assign traits to social groups as well? In two recently completed studies, Levy and Dweck (1996) found that entity theorists endorsed existing stereotypes to a significantly greater degree than did incremental theorists. Moreover, Levy, Stroessner, Dweck, Hong, and Chiu (1997) found that entity theorists assigned significantly stronger traits to novel groups, whether the group was a largely positive or a largely negative one. To conclude, our findings are anticipated by many early theories in personality and social psychology. Kelly (1955) posited that how individuals interpret and predict future events is to a large extent guided by their implicit constructions of the social world. Heider (1958) also proposed that individuals' implicit theories of personality set up a framework for understanding social events (see also Trope & Higgins, 1993). Consistent with these broad theoretical postulates, our research suggests that one implicit belief that may guide social inferences is the belief about the fixedness versus malleability of human attributes. In our view, having an entity theory sets up an emphasis on traits and hence creates a greater propensity for the inferential practices indicative of lay dispositionism. In contrast, having an incremental theory increases the relevance of situational and psychological mediators in social understanding and hence moderates the propensity for lay dispositionism (Dweck et al., 1993). Indeed, the idea that at least some perceivers may adopt a dynamic approach to understanding people and their behavior has received serious attention in recent models of social cognition (see Shoda & Mischel, 1993; cf. L. Ross & Nisbett, 1991). In line with this growing emphasis and the longstanding tradition represented by Heider and Kelly, our analysis and our findings suggest that (a) having an entity versus incremental theory of personality may set up a different processing framework in the realm of social perception and (b) lay dispositionism, a construct that has been associated with a rich set of empirical phenomena, may be differentially fostered by the perceiver's implicit theories. References Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada: The University of Manitoba Press. LAY DISPOSITION1SM Baron, R. M., & Misovich, S.J. (1993). Dispositional knowing from an ecological perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 79, 541-552. Bassili, J. N. (1989). Traits as action categories versus traits as person attributes in social cognition. In J. N. Bassili (Eds.), On-line cognition in person perception (pp. 61-89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Benenson, J., &Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of trait explanations and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. Child Development, 57, 1179-1189. Bennett, G. K., Bennett, M. G., Glendenen, D. M , Deppelt, J. E., Ricks, J. H., Jr., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (1965). Academic promise tests. New "Vbrk: Psychological Corporation. Bergen, R. (1991). Beliefs about intelligence and achievement-related behaviors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urban a - Ch ampai gn. Boyum, L. A. (1988). Students' conceptions of their intelligence: Impact on academic course choice. Unpublished master's thesis. University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Brown, R., Hinkle, S., Ely, P. G., Fox-Cardamone, L., Maras. P., & Taylor, L. A. (1992). Recognizing group diversity: Individualist-collectivist and autonomous-relational social orientations and their implications for intergroup processes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 327-342. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131. Chiu, C. (1994). Bases of categorization and person cognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York. Chiu, C , Dweck, C. S., Tong, Y. Y., & Fu, H. Y. (in press). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Chiu, Y., & Hong, Y. (in press). Justice from a Chinese perspective. In H. S. R. Kao & D. Sinha (Eds.), Asian perspectives on psychology. New Delhi, India: Sage. Chiu, C , Hong, Y, & Dweck, C. S. (1994). Toward an integrative model of personality and intelligence: A general framework and some preliminary steps. In R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 104-134). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. New York: Freeman. Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124-131. Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self-theories and goals: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1990 (pp. 199-255). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C , & Hong, Y. (1995a). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6. 267-285. Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C , & Hong, Y. (1995b). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322-333. Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y, & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644-656. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109-116. Erdley, C , Cain, K., Luomis, C , Dumas-Hines, E, & Dweck, C. S. ( 1997). The relations among children's social goals, implicit personality theories and responses to social failure. Developmental Psychology, 33, 263-272. Erdley, C. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1993). Children's implicit personality theories as predictors of their social judgments. Child Development, 64, 863-878. 29 Fletcher, G. J. O., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Peterson. D., & Reeder, G. D. (1986). Attributioual complexity: An individual differences measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 8 7 5 884. Gilbert, D. T, & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraints: Interpretation of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 269-280. Goetz, T. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). Learned helplessness in social situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 2 4 6 255. Hamilton, D. L. (1981). Cognitive representations of persons. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 135-159). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Ho, D. Y. E, & Chiu, C. (1994). Component ideas of individualism, collectivism, and social organization: An application in the study of Chinese culture. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C, Kagitcibasi, G. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory* method and applications (pp. 137-156). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA; Sage. Hong, Y. (1994). Predicting trait versus process inferences: The role of implicit theories. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York. Hong, Y, Chiu, C . & Dweck. C. S. (1995). Implicit theories of intelligence: Reconsidering the role of confidence in achievement motivation. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency and self-esteem (pp. 197216). New York: Plenum. Hong, Y., Chiu, C , & Kung, M. (in press). Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition. In K. Leung, Y. Kashima, U. Kim, & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology (Vol. 1). Singapore: Wiley. Jellison, J. M., & Green, J. (1981). A self-presentation approach to the fundamental attribution error: The norm of internality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 643-649. Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New "fork: Freeman. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219-266). New York: Academic Press. Jones, E. E., & Thibaut. J. W. (1958), Interaction goals as bases of inference in interpersonal perception. In R. Tagiuri &. L. Petrullo (Eds.), Person perception and interpersonal behavior (pp. 151 — 178). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press. Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton. Kerlinger, F. N. (1984). Liberalism and conservatism: The nature and structure of social attitudes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Krull, D.S. (1993). Does the grist change the mill? The effect of the perceiver's inferential goal on the process of social inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 340-349. Krull, D. S., & Dill, J. C. (1996). On thinking first and responding fast: Flexibility in social inference processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 949-959. Kunda, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The psychometrics of everyday life. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 195-224. Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T (1996). Explaining real-life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 732-741. Leggett, E. L. (1985, March). Children's entity and incremental theories of intelligence: Relationships to achievement behavior. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston. 30 CHIU, HONG, AND DWECK Levenson, H. (1974). Activism and powerful others: Distinction within the concept of internal-external control. Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, 377-383. Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (1996, June). The relation between implicit person theories and beliefs in stereotypes. Paper presented at the Eighth Annual Convention of the American Psychological Society, San Francisco. Levy, S., & Dweck, C, S. (1997). Implicit theory measures: Reliability and validity data for adults and children. Unpublished manuscript, Columbia University, New York. Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y. Y, & Chiu, C. Y. (1997, April). Implicit person theories and stereotype formation. Paper presented at the 68th Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Lord, C. G., Lepper, M. R., & Preston, E. (1984). Considering the opposite: A corrective strategy for social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1231-1243. MacGyvers, V. (1992). Implicit theories and real world outcomes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, UrbanaChampaign. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 568-579. McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion: Some contemporary approaches. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 191-229). New Tfark: Academic Press. Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961 — 978. Miller, J. G.( & Bersoff, D. M. (1994). Cultural influences on the moral status of reciprocity and the discounting of endogenous motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 592-602. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252-283. Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond deja vu in the search for cross-situational consistency. Psychological Review, 89, 730-755. Morris, M. W, & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949-971. Neuberg, S. L,, & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure; Individual differences in the desire for simple structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113-131. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598609. Peterson, D. R. (1968). The clinical study of social behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Robins, R. W., & Pals, J. L. (1996, June). Implicit self-theories in the academic domain: A longitudinal study. Poster presented at the Eighth Annual Convention of the American Psychological Society, San Francisco. Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The personal and the situation. New York: McGraw-Hill. Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341-357. Shoda, Y, & Mischel, W. (1993). Cognitive social approach to dispositional inferences: What if the perceiver is a cognitive-social theorist? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 574-585. Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. (1990). The semiotic subject of cultural psychology. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 399-4)6). New "fork: Guilford Press. Snyder, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526-537. Trope, Y.*( 1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239-257. Trope, Y. (1989). Levels of inference in dispositional judgment. Social Cognition, 7, 296-314. Trope, Y, &. Higgins, E. T. (1993). The what, when, and how of dispositional inference: New answers and new questions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 493-500. Trope, Y, & Liberman, A. (1993). The use of trait conceptions to identify other people's behavior and to draw inferences about their personalities. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 5 5 3 562. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 3 22). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Wright, J. C , & Mischel, W. (1987). A conditional approach to dispositional constructs: The local predictability of social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 454-469. Received March 10, 1994 Revision received October 9, 1996 Accepted October 25, 1996 •