Childhood and the Future
Transcription
Childhood and the Future
No. 30 30 Childhood and the Future New realities, new challenges Pau Marí-Klose Marga Marí-Klose Elizabeth Vaquera Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham Presentación Social Studies Collection SOCIAL projects. The spirit of ”la Caixa”. Social Studies Collection No. 30 Childhood and the Future New realities, new challenges Pau Marí-Klose Marga Marí-Klose Elizabeth Vaquera Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham With the collaboration of Alba Lanau Sánchez Publication: Obra Social Fundació “la Caixa” Authors: Pau Marí-Klose Marga Marí-Klose Elizabeth Vaquera Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham Translated by: Jed Rosenstein Design, layout and printing: cege Coordination of publication: Area for Grants, Universities and Social Studies ©Pau Marí-Klose, Marga Marí-Klose, Elizabeth Vaquera, Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham ©The ”la Caixa” Foundation, 2010 Av. Diagonal, 621 - 08028 Barcelona Pau Marí-Klose is a professor of sociology at the University of Barcelona and the director of social science projects at the Institute of Childhood and the Urban World (CIIMU). In addition he is a researcher for the Spanish National Research Council (CSIC). He holds a PhD in Sociology from the Autonomous University of Madrid, Master of Arts from the University of Chicago and Master in Social Sciences from the Center for Advanced Studies in the Social Sciences (Juan March Institute). He has studied and done research at the Universities of Berkeley, Harvard, Michigan, Essex, Lancaster and Oslo. Included among his recent publications are: Matrimonios y Parejas Jóvenes [Marriage and Young Couples] (2009), Informes de la Inclusión Social en España [Reports on Social Inclusion in Spain] (2008 and 2009), Temps de les Famílies [Family Time] (2008) and Edad del Cambio: Jóvenes en los Circuitos de la Solidaridad Intergeneracional [Changing Age: Young people in the Circuit of Intergenerational Solidarity] (2006). His sociological research is focused primarily on the family, childhood, youth, education and social exclusion. Marga Marí-Klose is an assistant professor of sociology at the University of Barcelona. She has a PhD in Sociology from the University of Barcelona, a Masters in Social Policy Research from the London School of Economics and a Masters in Gender and Development from the Complutense University of Madrid. She has coordinated various research projects at the Institute of Childhood and the Urban World (CIIMU) and has studied and done research at Harvard University and the University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA). She is the author of diverse publications on gender and family policy, sociology of the life cycle, education, social exclusion and poverty. EliZabeth Vaquera is an assistant professor in the Department of Sociology of the University of South Florida. She has a PhD in Sociology from the University of Pennsylvania. She has participated in different projects in the areas of the sociology of adolescence and youth, the sociology of education and immigration studies. She is co-author of the book, Educational Outcomes of Immigrants and Their Children in the U.S (2010), in addition to diverse publications in books and journals. Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham is a professor in the Department of Global Health at Emory University (Atlanta). She earned a PhD in Demography and Sociology from the University of Pennsylvania. In her research she has examined the determinants of various dimensions of childhood well-being, with special attention on the influence of interpersonal relationships on the risks of obesity and unhealthy habits. Her current research is being funded by the US National Institute of Health (NIH). CONTENTS CONTENTS Presentation 7 I. Introduction 1.1.Growing up in the Risk Society 1.2.Childhood and the future 9 11 14 II.Fathering and Mothering 2.1.Children in parents’ common life project 2.2. Family and fertility in the new social settings 2.3.The difficulties of having and raising children 2.4.The ethic of family care and the model of a good childhood 17 17 19 22 I II.Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age 3.1.When children arrive: new motherhood and fatherhood 3.2.Schedules in families with young children 3.3.The quality of time provided to children 3.4.External childcare IV.Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds 4.1. Parental influence on childhood 4.2. Joint activities 4.3.Managing the expression of affection and disapproval 4.4. Family learning cultures V. Uses of free time 5.1.Structured and unstructured after school time 5.2. Parents’ involvement in their children’s free time 5.3.Time with friends 5.4.Involvement of grandparents and siblings 88 89 96 98 98 30 I.The emergence of social risks in childhood V 6.1.A weighty problem: the road to obesity 6.2.Raising socially and emotionally competent children 6.3.The origins of school disengagement 103 106 116 125 36 Conclusion 137 38 45 52 58 Bibliography 143 Index of graphs and tables 153 Methodological Appendix 157 Survey on Inter and Intra-generational relationships in Childhood (2010) 157 63 63 65 74 83 Presentation Presentation The children of today will become the adults of tomorrow. The care children receive is, therefore, not only a determinant of their present well-being but also a guarantee for the future and a solution to potential problems that have their genesis in early childhood. Concern for children has been constant for at least the past century; guided by this concern, tremendous effort and resources have been invested in improving children’s living conditions. Traditionally, effort has been focused on overcoming the most urgent problems that put children’s lives at risk or seriously limit their opportunities for development. These include problems of infant mortality, childhood diseases and situations of poverty and social exclusion. Thanks to these efforts, the infant mortality rate has fallen considerably and the number of children living in extreme poverty has decreased or at least not increased in the developed countries, although there still remains much work to be done in this area even in the most advanced societies. Beyond these challenges others have appeared as a result of the important transformations our society has undergone in recent decades. These are changes related to the massive incorporation of women into the workforce, the redefinition of the role of men in bringing up children, the appearance of new models of the family or the increasingly intercultural society in which we live, brought about by migratory processes unknown until just a few years ago. These transformations, which directly affect adults, have inevitable consequences for children as well. They affect decisions about when and how many children to have, the amount of time parents spend with children and the activities they share with them; they also shape the importance that other social agents have (the school, grandparents, friends, etc.) in the lives of children. Unfortunately, despite their importance, there has been little research focused on how these social transformations directly affect children. This is precisely the focus of the current study, to address the organization of children’s lives in society today and to contribute important data regarding who is responsible for their upbringing, in which activities they are involved, how they relate to their With the addition of this study to the Social Studies collection, the ”la Caixa” Foundation seeks to stimulate debate on the needs and challenges of childhood today. Children do not passively live through changes in their families, in parents’ work lives, or in their own forms of recreation, but they are fundamentally influenced and affected by them. Deepening our understanding of these issues enables us to plan effective programmes to help families and contribute to the well-being of children today as well as to provide the tools needed to build a better future. Jaime Lanaspa Gatnau Executive Director of ”la Caixa” Social Projects and Chief Executive Officer of the ”la Caixa” Foundation Barcelona, December 2010 Presentation peers, and to what extent they are affected by factors such as parents’ economic situation and employment status, family structure and immigration. In addition, this study examines the appearance and occurrence of problems such as obesity, school failure and the lack of socio-emotional competencies–problems which begin in childhood and may have consequences later in adult life. Introduction I. Introduction In this book two different visions regarding childhood in Spain coexist: one that is optimistic and hopeful and another much more concerned and questioning. In the following chapters, the reader will find sufficient evidence to lend credence to one or the other perspective, or conversely, to get a taste of a complex reality that does not permit a simple diagnosis.Through this research we seek to take the analysis of childhood beyond conventional approaches by including ages and measuring dimensions that have generally received little attention in the sociological research published in Spain up until now. The focus of our research is on some of the primary social risks and vulnerabilities experienced before the onset of adolescence, as well as on the factors that cause them. The risks experienced by the young have been the object of considerable sociological research, but by and large most of it (beginning with the classic studies) has focused on the later stages of childhood and the transition to adulthood. In these stages adolescents often become a «social problem» for adult society because of the collective implications of the behaviours they manifest, their educational choices or the difficult transition from school to work. Our research is an attempt to look more closely at the reality of the youngest children (from 0 to 10 years old), who have traditionally received much less attention from the social science disciplines (with the exception of specialities focused on childhood development, such as psychology or psychiatry). Thanks to a survey designed for this study (la Encuesta de Relaciones Inter e Intrageneracionales en la Infancia [the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood]), we have the opportunity to use a wide range of specific factors in analyzing the processes we are interested in. This has not often been the case in other studies on the social reality of childhood that have been carried out in Spain, which usually have had to rely on secondary data collected for other purposes. In recent decades, throughout the developed world many indicators of child well-being have improved. Infant mortality has been reduced, and the occurrence of factors of physical vulnerability in the early years of life (such as low birth weight) has also been reduced considerably. Extreme poverty responsible for malnutrition and health problems has been eradicated. Universal schooling of children starting at the age of six has been achieved (in Spain, effectively, schooling for the majority of children begins at age three). Some of the «old risks» threatening child social Despite these advances in education, health, the physical protection of children and the recognition of rights, concern for children is at its peak. There are some important reasons for this (as well as others which are more outlandish). Alarm over the situation of children in society has cultural roots frequently tied to processes of recognizing, defining, and constructing social discourses and narratives which tend to overdramatize certain childhood experiences. Some of these narratives are based on biased or partial readings of reality which make isolated episodes or incidents seem to be the norm. In contemporary society moral panic periodically arises over different issues; often threats to child well-being such as bullying in school, addictions, and pederasty are the targets of such alarmist discourse. Within such discourse, children are usually presented as either victims of an out of control ego that pushes them toward behaviours contrary to their own interests (or that is simply unacceptable in society), or they are presented as the passive prey of unscrupulous adults taking advantage of their innocence. In both cases, these are stories that can mobilize momentary interest in the vulnerability of children, feeding protectionist impulses and initiatives to increase adult control over children’s lives at home or in the public sphere–in school, on the streets, in places of leisure, etc. –, but they tend to remain indifferent to more general problems that affect children’s lives and opportunities. Beyond these outbreaks of fragmented attention to childhood, the growing concern in society for the situation of children is based on a number of objective realities with great significance and representativeness, which are endangering the welfare of a large number of children in the first world. In addition to some of the «old risks» which caused apprehension among families in industrial societies–unemployment, illness, or the main breadwinner becoming disabled–, today there are «new risks»– new relationships of parents to the labour market, the dissolution and reconstitution of families, immigration–, whose effects are particularly damaging for children. This combination of old and new risks is creating new profiles of vulnerability which have not yet been adequately studied. Many researchers agree that the Introduction welfare have been largely eliminated. A very small percentage of children now face adverse situations linked to the death of their parents: the mother’s death in childbirth or the subsequent death of a parent in the early years of life. The legal system has recognized the right of children to be protected from physical or sexual abuse and defines childhood as a stage of life free from any form of work, forced or voluntary. For the first time, concerns for the well-being of children transcend all borders. There is universal agreement on what constitutes a «good childhood», at least on paper, thanks to the efforts of children’s rights activists, social scientists and international organizations. The basic principles for the protection of children are guaranteed by widereaching international agreements such as the United Nations’ International Convention on the Rights of the Child or the Charter of Fundamental Rights of the European Union. Children, who currently do not have a voice or the vote, have benefitted from the social progress achieved by other groups, in particular mothers, who have enjoyed a greater capacity to place their needs and demands on the public agenda. Feminist movements struggling for the recognition of women’s work in the private sphere have sensitized society, leading to social responses to situations of risk, such as the increasing poverty in households headed by single mothers or the existence of domestic abuse and violence. The proposal of the present study is to advance our understanding of this new configuration of social risks affecting the lives of children. Our aim is to describe the nature of these risks, to analyze how they are expressed in the daily lives of the very youngest (0 to 10 years of age) and how they can come to affect children’s health and welfare and even condition their trajectories during this stage of their lives. However, recognizing the existence and sociological relevance of these risks is not the equivalent of assuming that their emergence necessarily involves a worsening of children’s living conditions or their future opportunities. What some authors (Giddens, 1991, 1999; Beck, 1992) have come to call the risk society, is one in which individuals are exposed to new forms of «manufactured» risks (in contrast to the «external» risks that had concerned humanity for centuries). However, this risk society is also reflexive and in transformation, rethinking and reconstructing itself constantly, generating in this process new capacities to mitigate risks and new forms of organization to alleviate their effects. We live in a society with a growing concern for security and well-being which provides itself with more and better instruments to procure them. As Giddens reminds us (1999:3), living in the risk society does not necessarily mean living in a more insecure or dangerous society but rather in a society in which these dangers can be anticipated and prevented, where the decisions of individuals and families, as well as the very collective management of risks, influence the way in which risks are expressed. We live in a reality which we try to unravel and control in order to build a better future. We often fail, and in many cases, outcomes are unevenly distributed. In the risk society, not all risks receive equal attention, nor do all vulnerable groups receive the same level of priority. 1.1. Growing up in the Risk Society An element that is increasingly present in the discussions about childhood is the process of ageing in developed societies. In recent years, social scientists have become more aware of the social and political implications of increasing longevity with the consequential increase in the demographic weight of the elderly in society. In a pioneering article, the demographer Samuel Preston warned in 1984 that the demographic advantage of the elderly in the United States was enabling them to dominate in the competition for public resources. Thanks to this, their economic situation had improved substantially, giving them a comparative advantage over other social groups, above all, children. In the same article, Preston noted the gradual deterioration in the living conditions of children. The deterioration of the situation of minors has been described in other countries immersed in the demographic process of ageing (Dang, 2001; OECD, 2008). Social spending on programmes primarily oriented toward the elderly (pensions, health care) absorb a growing portion of public resources, thereby reducing the chances of developing other social programmes. In this Introduction well-being of children is suffering from the effects of the social change taking place in developed societies. Children are affected by the social dynamics in spheres in which they do not directly participate (such as in the labour market), by decisions made by parents for their own personal benefit and self-fulfilment (such as getting divorced or finding a new partner), or by policy decisions that give political priority to issues that concern other groups. It is often argued that the increased levels of participation of women in the workforce are the primary form of protection against child poverty. Having two sources of income in the household decreases the risk of economic vulnerability, but only when families find ways to balance women’s employment and childcare without incurring excessive costs. This is not always possible. The high costs of formal childcare in day-care centres or of after-school activities can considerably reduce the benefits of a second income and even dissuade many mothers from participating in the workforce. Additionally, when there are large differences in men’s and women’s salaries, the incentives for mothers to continue working (instead of staying home and taking care of the children) decrease considerably. In this scenario, child poverty–characteristic feature of underdevelopment–is experiencing an unexpected increase. International data published in recent years reveal that in a large number of countries in the developed world, the risks of child poverty have increased since the end of the 1980. In almost all of them, child poverty rates are higher than the poverty rates for the overall population (UNICEF Innocenti, 2007; OECD, 2008). Spain is no exception: in recent years, the percentage of children under 16 years of age living in poverty is between two and five percentage points above that of the overall population. Child poverty in developed countries continues to be in many cases the result of old risks dragging families into situations of vulnerability, such as the loss of a job or the low salary of the main breadwinner in the family; but increasingly it is a consequence of the new work and family trajectories of parents, which are diverse, unstable and irregular and can expose children to economic and social adversity at different stages of the family cycle. Parents’ changing patterns of participation in the labour market (particularly mothers) directly affect other dimensions of child wellbeing. The increase in the number of hours that children spend away from one of their parents (especially the mother) has triggered anxiety and concern in some sectors, who feel that the foundations for the responsible socialization of children will be shaken if it is transferred to non-family agents (childcare professionals) or if the amount of time children spend under an adult’s supervision is reduced. This concern has crystallized in genuine popular myths, such as the so-called latchkey kids, who spend hours alone waiting for their parents to get home from work, while they are exposed to risks of all sorts: domestic accidents, harmful television programmes, the excessive and pernicious use of new technologies, the growing role of the peer group as a substitute form of socialization, etc. Social research has cleared up most of the doubts and questions related to the desirability of mothers working. The vast majority Introduction context, demands for aid for families have received little support for a quite some time. The justification for concentrating resources on providing economic aid to the elderly population, who have contributed resources to the public system through their social security payments and taxes throughout their lives, has been much more powerful and persuasive than ethical appeals to support children. The contributory nature of many welfare state benefits establishes the right of the elderly to be the major beneficiaries of public services. In recent decades, welfare states have turned into what John Myles has called «welfare states for the elderly,» in which guaranteeing the welfare of children is considered a private responsibility, the costs of which must be borne by the families who choose to have children. This does not mean that we should disregard the influence of new labour scenarios on children’s welfare. A growing number of children live in households in which labour market flexibility has become a source of distress. Along with the economic risks threatening households during unfavourable economic times due to the greater likelihood of job loss, there is also the problem of parents finding the time to carry out their parental duties. In some sectors of the labour market there are a growing number of jobs requiring commitments that are not compatible with the times that children need or expect parental care: shift work, weekend work, overtime, etc. Flexible working hours become a factor of uncertainty for children and adults, forcing families to find makeshift solutions that can generate confusion and frustration in children and feelings of guilt and anxiety in parents (primarily in mothers) (Roppelt, 2003; Klammer, 2006). Along with these transformations, structural changes in households, which can affect child welfare, should also be noted. In recent decades the size of families has been decreasing; as a result, parents have been able to dedicate more attention to their children, as the time they have available is distributed among fewer children. The decrease in fertility (and especially of second and third births) means that a greater number of children will grow up without siblings, which on the one hand may strengthen the connection with parents, but on the other hand, can deprive children of the enriching experience of sociability in childhood. There has been little social research done to measure the effects of this trend. It must also not be forgotten that the decrease in family size has also been a result of the gradual disappearance of other adults residing in the home (generally relatives) and therefore, of the presence of other agents of socialization capable of providing support to parents in the exercise of their childrearing duties. But without a doubt the primary structural change that is happening today in many households is the weakening of family bonds through separation or divorce. A growing number of children go through the experience of parents separating at some point and therefore the disruption of life with one parent. The process of divorce or separation often places children in a situation of economic risk. Due to the persistence of important differences in the dedication of men and women to paid work, the salary gaps between men and women and the often inadequate financial support mothers who get custody receive from ex-partners, households headed by single mothers usually have lower incomes than two parent households. The exposure of these households to the risk of poverty is therefore much higher. Aside from economic problems, the breakup of the Introduction of empirical research strongly concludes that women working has no negative effects on the well-being of children, except when it is in the first year of the child’s life (Waldfogel et al., 2002). The reality is more complex and suggests that parents usually make up for the deficits in their own supervision and time with their children through supervision by other adults in formal and informal contexts (early childhood education centres, grandparents, babysitters, etc). In addition, in many households the mothers’ absence from the home has led to fathers taking on childcare responsibilities which were traditionally assumed by mothers. On balance, there is no reason to think that the massive incorporation of women into the workforce has a direct negative effect on child well-being. In fact, there are indications that the opposite is the case. 1.2.Childhood and the future In a society prone to reflect on itself, to reconsider and reconstitute itself, the tendencies pointing toward a worsening of children’s living conditions are the object of growing public concern, although this does not always crystallize in specific initiatives to correct such conditions. In recent years, a new set of arguments has been articulated supporting proactive public investment in childhood. These new discourses understand that «investing» in children is investing in the future of our society. The orientation of such aims is fundamentally futuristic. From this perspective, social policy should contribute to generating economic dynamism, preparing individuals to take advantage of available job opportunities throughout their lives and improving their productivity. In this new paradigm of social policy (insistently emphasized by international organizations and governments), public spending on children does not represent a social cost; rather, it is the main axis of socially productive investment oriented toward the future. The goal is to take preventive measures against all situations of risk that could jeopardize the life prospects of children; in other words, their possibilities of becoming prepared and flexible adults capable of effectively adapting to the changing demands of the market. Backed by an enormous amount of social research, this new paradigm calls for intensified efforts to combat child poverty, to support families with difficulties balancing work and family responsibilities and to encourage participation in preschool education for families with low cultural and educational resources. The fruits of these initiatives should not only improve the competitiveness of economies by providing them with flexible and prepared workforce but also contribute to the sustainability of the old architecture of the welfare state. Children will become the guarantors for the provision of public services in the future if we are able to convert them into productive adults, who are capable of making significant financial contributions to the funds that sustain the system of public welfare provision. This will only be possible if they have the opportunities today to grow up in adequate conditions and if risks threatening their educational and personal development can be overcome. Thanks to this new paradigm, childcare now occupies a privileged place in the range of social policies employed in a growing number of countries. In these countries, society has become aware of the living conditions of children in precarious economic situations and the difficulties of families in balancing work and family responsibilities. The discourses and prescriptions that are Introduction couple’s relationship involves situations that can result in distressful experiences for children. Divorce or separation of the parents disrupts the daily access of the child to one of the parents (usually the father), which in many cases ends up weakening the bond with that parent. The appearance of new adult figures in the home–if the mother initiates a new sentimental relationship– opens new scenarios for the child, which are not always satisfactory (Fustenberg and Cherlin, 1991; Cherlin, 2010). In recent years, society has been equipping itself with new instruments to adapt to these changing realities. There has been a trend in many countries to revise the laws administering family breakups with the objective of better serving the interests of the child. One example of this has been the debate over shared custody or measures directed toward encouraging the involvement of parents who do not live with the child in his or her education. In some countries such as ours, the discourses described in the previous section have barely begun to take root. This is not because the situation of children is so promising. Despite the fact that some international indicators of child well-being place Spain in a rather privileged position compared to other developed countries–for example, in indicators related to emotional and relational well-being–, in dimensions which are key to well-being today and for the future, Spanish children suffer significant deficits. In a well-known UNICEF report published in 2007, two disturbing facts were noted (Innocenti, 2007). The first is that the economic and material situation in which many Spanish children are living may be denying them essential opportunities for social participation, thereby conditioning their life prospects. For example, in 2007 Spanish children were located in 17th place out of 21 countries surveyed in terms of child poverty rates. Another area of concern is formal education. The early dropout rates from school are very high, and the level of knowledge and capacities of students as measured by international standardized tests–PIRLS or PISA–is quite low. These results suggest that many Spanish children are entering into adulthood in conditions of educational vulnerability hardly conducive for the adequate management of the economic and social scenarios that lie ahead in the future. The hallmarks of identity of the Spanish family, traditionally referred to as «familism», are disappearing and with them some of the conditions on which the welfare and security of children in the context of the traditional family have been based - primarily, the mother’s exclusive dedication to domestic responsibilities and childcare. However, this new scenario does not necessarily increase childhood vulnerability because its appearance has coincided with the emergence of new mechanisms and strategies for children’s well-being as efficient (or even more) than the previous ones. In recent years, we have seen the appearance of new family structures and new ways of organizing daily life and domestic culture, which are laying the groundwork for new models of family well-being. This book aspires to provide a clear picture of the reality of these new families in order to contribute to identifying the strengths and weaknesses in the emerging profiles of family sociability for child welfare. In chapter 2, we will analyze what it means for mothers and fathers to have children; what challenges are involved today in parenthood and what tensions for children are caused by the attempt to balance work and family. In chapters 3 and 4 we will analyze intergenerational relationships between parents and children, focusing on the 0-4 years of age and 5-10 years of age groups respectively. The ways and strategies of parents in relating to children will be discussed: how much time they dedicate to caring for their children, how this time is distributed and what they do during this time, how reward and punishment is used to guide children’s behaviour, how parents involve themselves in their children’s formal education. A secondary objective of these chapters is to trace emerging forms of inequality, stemming from socialization practices in the home. Chapter 5 looks at children’s «free time» in and outside the home, differentiating structured (after school) from non-structured (with Introduction being applied are varied and in some cases subject to very strong criticism (see for example, Lister, 2006). However, it is undeniable that the new paradigm of social investment stands as a necessary counterweight to the gerontocratic tendencies of the old welfare states, perhaps the only moderately effective one in the context of the growing demographic, economic and political weight of the oldest generations. Our work is based on different sources of both primary and secondary information. We have utilized data from international databases (OECD or Eurostat) and from available surveys (the Survey of Income and Living Conditions of Spain’s National Statistics Institute (INE), different studies of the Spanish Centre for Sociological Research (CIS), the World Values Survey and the European Values Survey). However, it is important to point out that a large part of the data in chapters 3, 4, 5 and 6 comes from a telephone survey carried out in February 2010 in which 2,200 mothers and fathers with children between the ages of 0 and 10 were interviewed. The questionnaires contain specific modules for families with children of different ages (methodological details are provided in the appendix). The fact that this survey was designed to address our specific concerns has given us considerable scope to come closer to realities that have barely been analyzed in previous studies in this country. Introduction friends, siblings, relatives) activities. Chapter 6 provides an analysis of the implications of some of the dynamics studied in the appearance of different situations of childhood distress. We focus specifically on three dimensions of childhood that play an important role in the well-being and personal development of children: problems related to overweight, and obesity emotional difficulties, and those related to adaptation to the demands of school in the early years of compulsory education. Few institutions have changed as dramatically as the family in recent decades. With the exception of certain circumstances, family demographic parameters evolve slowly. But in Spain this has not been the case. In recent times, there has been a notable increase in the cohabitation of unmarried couples, separation, divorce, singleparent families and international adoption. Homosexual marriage has been recognized and legalized and as a result, so have families with homosexual parents. All of this has been accompanied by a continuing increase in the number of women in the workforce (especially those who are mothers), the transformation of intimate relationships between couples, the emergence of new practices regarding the distribution of domestic responsibilities and childcare, a decrease in fertility, the postponement of the emancipation of young people from the parental home, and the growing personal and residential autonomy of the elderly. In this context of change, important legislative reform has been passed affecting family and kinship relationships. In recent decades the principles regulating divorce, consensual union, transmission of surnames, assisted reproduction, adoptions, or most recently, shared custody have been revised and brought up to date, in some cases more than once, in order to adapt to a reality that no longer fits the existing legal framework. This reality has pushed Spanish people to change, and these changes have not come without tensions and contradictions. 2.1. Children in parents’ common life project We live in a society in which the patriarchal family is being transformed. The massive incorporation of women into public life, in both the educational sphere and the workforce has created a new scenario that is bringing about a redefinition of the role of women in the family. Women are no longer the cornerstone of a family model in which the responsibilities for disabled and dependent members falls disproportionately on them. The number of women who think that being a housewife can be as rewarding as having a profession is shrinking every day, while the number of women who believe that domestic responsibilities are a barrier to their career advancement is growing (Rivero Recuento et al., 2005; Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009). Since the 1980s, sociological studies based on surveys carried out by Spain’s Centre for Sociological Research have traced the evolution of the Fathering and Mothering II. Fathering and Mothering In this context, the primary bond supporting the family structure –the emotional relationship and commitment uniting the couple–has also changed. In contrast to the past, when the bond between the couple was primarily a means to achieve economic (self-sufficiency in the family) and institutional (the survival of lineage and transmission of heritage) ends without the need for a romantic bond between the two partners, intimate relationships in more recent times have become more and more based on personal expectations and aspirations. In what North American sociologist Andrew Cherlin (2010) has called «individualized marriage,» the value of being in a couple is rated above all according to the daily satisfaction it brings. The individual right to happiness is the driving motor of everything: it is the mechanism that sets in motion a life in partnership and that terminates it the moment that the relationship no longer satisfies the aspirations of its members. The promise of happiness lies in the quality of the intimate relationships one is able to build. When that promise loses credibility, there are few obstacles to prevent the dissolution of the relationship. Coinciding with the consolidation of this model of relationship, which gives unprecedented centrality to living with a partner, the value placed on children has also changed. For many couples, having children is no longer one of the most important aspects of happiness. Other dimensions such as respect and mutual affection, faithfulness, mutual understanding, a satisfactory sexual relationship, sharing in household chores, having a sufficient income and a nice home have gained in importance. Being able to maintain the quality of the relationship requires increasing effort and attention. The challenge is for each partner to offer the other the best version of him or herself, to find intimate spaces for a mutually rewarding exchange and to constantly try to inject something restorative into the relationship (a gift, an intimate dinner, a surprise trip, etc). The centrality given to the partnership experience tends to relegate to second place the bond that usually weaves together the fabric of the family: the union between parents and children. This does not mean that its importance is not recognized under the new circumstances. Bringing up and educating children is one of the main functions of the family for all Spanish society (Meil, 2006) Raising children can in fact be perfectly compatible with the aspirations of «individualized couples» if such an experience opens up intimate spaces in which the couple can satisfy its desire for «special» emotional experiences that strengthen the couple’s interpersonal bond. However, there is no doubt that some of the traditional aspects of childrearing may be in conflict with the priorities of this type of couples. Qualities such as stability, altruism or self-sacrifice for the benefit of the child become less valued. For a sizeable number of young couples, the couple comes first, and children must not get in the way of their happiness and plans. Thus, according to the results of a recent study of the Fundación SM [SM Foundation] in which two of the writers of this book Fathering and Mothering preferences and orientations of Spanish men and women regarding the distribution of roles in the family. A growing number of men and women are in favour of an egalitarian model in which both share household chores and childcare. Especially notable is the evolution in the attitudes of men. Those in favour of a traditional model, with a strict definition of roles, are a declining minority that is being rapidly replaced by new cohorts of men who express a preference for an egalitarian model. Experiencing the relationship with a partner as a project that must be constantly renewed and reaffirmed, a sort of «daily plebiscite,» creates situations of uncertainty in which it becomes difficult to decide to have children. There are many situations that discourage women from having children: those that live with a partner but are unmarried wait to decide until they are sure that the relationship is permanent; married women who have doubts about the future of their relationship are reluctant to have children for fear of the consequences of a possible break-up; women who have uncertain job prospects postpone the decision to have children as they wait for better times; women with a high level of education and who are career oriented have reasons to be concerned about the implications that having children might have on their professional future. The decline in fertility in Spain in recent decades largely reflects decisions to postpone pregnancy as a result of giving greater importance to personal aspects such as educational, professional or emotional factors than to fertility projects. But beyond these decisions, we find a voluntary infertility among women who, already mothers, are choosing to interrupt their fertility plans as preferences related to ideal family size change. Some mothers with only one child decide to have no additional children, perhaps contrary to their initial desires, discouraged by the difficulties encountered in raising the first. The sociological research reveals agreement in presenting a panorama with some bright spots and quite a few dark ones. The immense majority of women are satisfied with having had children, but a significant percentage express fear and anxiety regarding how much of an obstacle children represent for their professional lives, the limitations they place on the quality of life for the couple and about not having enough time to take care of them. In addition, there is the concern felt by many mothers and fathers, especially with older children and adolescents, about whether or not they will be able to exercise the kind of authority needed to keep their children away from harmful influences (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009; Meil, 2006). 2.2. Family and fertility in the new social settings The family has invariably been the most highly valued dimension of personal life since survey information on Spanish attitudes and values began to be gathered. The family receives a very positive valuation among those who have children, and particularly, among parents of small children. According to the data from Study 2.578 (2004) of the CIS, 86 percent of those who have a child under six years of age consider their family to be «very important to them,» an attitude shared by 68 percent of those who do not have children at that age. Spain has often been characterized as a family-oriented country in which private life is primarily thought of as family life and family obligations are placed before personal ambitions. Within this sphere, bringing up children is often seen as one of the main functions of the family. For the vast majority of couples having children continues Fathering and Mothering participated, 52 percent of young couples (under 40 years of age) agree that «having children limits the parents’ freedom too much.» Contrary to widespread belief in the past, having children should not be an obstacle to separation or divorce. Only 11 percent of those interviewed thought that «when there are children, parents shouldn’t separate even if they don’t get along» (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009: 89). Beyond the deep-seated sense of duty felt by many, the main reasons people give for having children are emotional. Having children produces intangible rewards that compensate for the sacrifice. According to the previously cited Fundación SM study, 66 percent of young adults under the age of 40 who live with their partner state that they strongly agree that «watching children grow up is one of the greatest pleasures in life,» and an additional 21 percent «agree» with that statement (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009:89). That «pleasure» is essentially emotional in nature. As can be seen in table 2.1, when asked about the two main reasons for having a child or children, the main reason given is emotional in nature (CIS, Study 2,639). Other considerations such as the preservation of lineage («to see the family continue in the future») or facing old age with security («children make it less likely that one will be alone in old age») have lost the importance they may have had in the past. It is also interesting to note that only a minority mention that «having children strengthens the relationship with one’ spouse/partner.» However, measuring the value of children in Spanish society requires greater attention to nuances. The assumption that the Spanish give centrality to children in the plans of the couple is debatable if certain attitudes are examined in light of comparative evidence. A significant proportion of the Spanish currently believe that it is legitimate and possibly even satisfactory for those who do so, to choose not to have children. For example, according to data from the World Values Survey (1990 and 1999) and the European Values Survey (2008), the proportion of Spanish who responded affirmatively to the question «Do you think that a woman has to have children in order to be fulfilled?» is significantly lower than found in other countries in the south of Europe, such as Greece, Portugal or Italy, and even lower than that found in the countries of central and northern Europe, such as Germany, France or Denmark. In the last decade, this proportion has come closer to that of countries that are less parentally oriented. On the other hand, the differences among cohorts is substantial–greater than in the majority of other countries–even though in recent years the generation gap has decreased as a result of the incorporation on the margins of old age of groups that one or two decades ago were already less parentally oriented. Thus, while in 1999, 62 percent of those interviewed over 50 years of age were in agreement with the statement, «a woman needs to have children to feel fulfilled» and only 32 percent of those under 30 years of age agreed, in 2008, the difference had declined significantly (to 52 percent and 34 percent respectively). Fathering and Mothering to be an essential part of their plans as a couple. Mothers and fathers are called on to make every effort to make this plan come to pass. Related to this, a high proportion of the Spanish (around 80 percent according to different waves of the World Values Survey) think that parents should make sacrifices for the welfare of their children, even if it is at their own expense. This is a widespread opinion among all social groups with only minor differences related to education and religion. who do not have children but want to have them and for women who already have children but would like to have more In percentages most important reason for having a child primary reason secondary reason Having children is very rewarding feeling 39 25 It is good to see children grow up and develop 21 23 Children increase one’s sense of responsibility 16 13 It is satisfying to see the family continue 14 25 Children make being alone in old age less likely Having children strengthens the relationship w/ partner 4.9 4.7 4.1 9.1 Total 100 Number of cases (2,760) Source: Calculations based on data from Study 2,639 of CIS, 2006. The attitude of the majority of Spanish young people has moved away from their parents’ conception which considered childlessness nothing less than a curse. Far removed are the times in which the majority of the population thought that a marriage without children was not really a marriage.1 Today, only a small 1 In a survey from the magazine Cambio 16 (October 1978, no. 560), only 27% of those surveyed thought that a married couple that had decided not to have children constituted a family (cited by Campo and Navarro, 1985: 121). percentage of Spaniards express negative judgments with respect to life projects that exclude having children. For example, in the survey of the Fundación SM, only 23 percent of the persons interviewed agreed or totally agreed with the statement that «people who do not have children lead empty lives» (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009:89). Parallel to these changes in attitude related to childlessness, the number of children couples want to have has also declined sharply. The evolution in the figures is telling. In the 1966 FOESSA Report, the ideal number of children for Spaniards was 3.3 (average value). A 1968 DATA study yielded a similar figure (3.4), while the 1970 FOESSA Report showed a value of 3.1. These figures were even lower than the actual number of children women were having. This pattern has reversed in recent decades. The 1975 FOESSA report had already detected figures below the threshold of 3: 2.83 for women; 2.66 for men (del Campo and Navarro, 1985:118). In the 1980s and the first half of the 1990s, these figures would decline slightly more than half a point more, reaching at the lowest point, 2.15, in 2000. In recent years there has been a rebound placing the ideal number of children at around 2.3. This figure is well above the actual fertility rate of women in advanced ages in the reproductive life span: in 2006, the average number of live births to women between 40 and 44 years of age was 1.79 (data from the Encuesta de Fecundidad, Familia y Valores [Fertility, Family and Values Survey] of the Centre for Sociological Research by Margarita Delgado, 2007:108). Even though at the end of the reproductive cycle, women who have apparently fulfilled their aspirations to have children are dominant, a significant group of women have not done so. One out of three women between 40 and 44 years of age without children would like to have a child Fathering and Mothering TABLE 2.1: Most important reasons for having a child among women Overall, evidence suggests that the attitudes of Spaniards with respect to fertility have changed rapidly. Those who have children and who want to have them continue to dominate. But the alternative of not having children is perfectly acceptable. The pressure to have children has diminished and the preference for large families is increasingly a minority position. It is difficult to explain what has brought about these changes. One factor that has probably had an influence in this process is the adaptation of attitudes and preferences to a reality in which options are constrained. Some Spaniards would take the position of the fox in Aesop’s fable who, unable to reach the grapes, walks away claiming he did not want them anyway because they were not ripe. 2.3. The difficulties of having and raising children Although the changes in ideas regarding childlessness and the number of children can be attributed to diverse factors and processes, it is not unimportant to point out the fact that these changes coincide with a recognition of the problems posed by the arrival of children for their parents. If we examine the attitudes of women from 25 to 39 years of age who do not have children and would not like to, it is striking to note the high number who brandish the difficulties involved in having children and raising them. Reasons such as «personal uncertainty,» the «lack of confidence in the future» and the problems and worry that come with having children weigh heavily on their attitudes toward fertility and childrearing. The immense majority of these young women put forward adverse reasons of this type. Among mothers between 25 and 39 years of age who have children and do not wish to have any more, the percentage of those who talk about «personal uncertainty» and «lack of confidence in the future» is much lower. Obviously, this is a group of women who, despite being in the same age group as those who do not have children, have already crossed the threshold of motherhood and have overcome the uncertainties and doubts regarding the future, which are always there at the beginning of a family project. Even so, a significant proportion state –whether as the first or second reason given– that raising children involves a lot of worry, is expensive and takes time away from doing other important things in life. Fathering and Mothering (33 percent). The percentage of those who already have a child but would like to have more is lower (15 percent). Women between 25 and 39 years of age without children and who do not want to have them In percentages MOST IMPORTANT REASONS FOR NOT HAVING A CHILD PRIMARY PRIMARY AND REASON SECONDARY REASONS Raising children involves a lot of worry and problems 24 33 Personal uncertainty 18 27 Lack of confidence in the future 14 22 Children make it more difficult for the woman to work 11 18 Children are expensive, especially when they’re growing up 10 14 Children take time away from other important things in life 10 20 Age 7.0 10 Health reasons 3.9 5.2 Pregnancies, birth are hard on the woman 0.9 6.7 I have as many as I want 0.8 1.1 My house isn’t big enough 0.0 0.5 Total 100 Number of cases (123) Source: Based on data from Study 2,639, CIS 2006. The difficulties of having and raising children become even more important in the context of the restructuring of the roles of women. In what Esping-Andersen (2009) has called an «incomplete revolution,» the life course of women has been masculinised, driven by the massive incorporation of women into the labour market and the tendency to remain in it after marriage and giving birth. This growing orientation toward productive work is part of economic as well as cultural processes. From the economic standpoint, the increase in the participation of women in the workforce coincides with the expansion of educational systems –where a growing proportion of women are found at the highest educational levels– and with the transformation of the economic structures in post-industrial societies, which has created new spaces of production that no longer require are based on a sexual division of labour. In these new scenarios leaving work to dedicate oneself to raising a family and domestic life is no longer the most profitable option for the family economy, especially in the case of women with high levels of qualifications and salary. Fathering and Mothering TABLE 2.2: Most important reasons for not having children. Women between 25 and 39 years of age who have children but do not want to have more In percentages MOST IMPORTANT REASONS FOR NOT HAVING A CHILD PRIMARY REASON PRIMARY AND SECONDARY REASONS I have as many as I want 39 50 Children are expensive, especially when they’re growing up 14 25 Raising children involves a lot of worry and problems 14 29 Health reasons 5.1 8.4 Lack of confidence in the future 4.7 11 Children make it more difficult for the woman to work 4.6 10 Pregancies, birth are hard on the woman 4.5 8.8 Age 4.4 11 Children take time away from doing other important things in life 3.5 8.0 Personal uncertainty 3.1 9.2 My house isn’t big enough 2.6 5.4 Total 100 Number of cases (644) Source: Based on data from Study 2,639, CIS 2006. From a cultural standpoint, women’s employment activity takes on new meaning in the context of the processes of individualization that are taking place in Western societies. In the new culture of individualism, work stands as a central element in the life project: providing the necessary economic resources to carry out that project (and avoid situations of dependency), opening avenues for social participation in areas acquiring growing importance in the shaping of individual identity (most notably in the world of consumption), and enabling the affirmation of individual achievement in the framework of the autonomous construction of the individual’s own life (Beck-Gernsheim, 2003; Lipovetsky, 2003). In the case of women, the rise in individualism has brought with it a strengthening of the orientation toward education, having a job, and being free to focus on oneself throughout one’s life, defying traditional conceptions that demand women’s dedication to collective obligations –primarily caring for others. The old order based on the «ideology of sacrifice,» which celebrated the values of self-denial and altruism for women, has entered into direct confrontation with the development of the consumer society, which has spread throughout the world the values of well-being and individual pleasure and the right to pursue them openly. We find ourselves before a social earthquake which is transforming women’s desires and ambitions as well as the power relations in the couple and the family. This dynamic of change does not flow, however, without obstacles that often put the care of children at the epicentre of contradictions, frustrations and conflicts. On the one hand, the destruction of the system of traditional values is not complete. There are still men and women attached to traditional values who seek to reconcile their beliefs with social dynamics which are gradually making traditional life economically and socially unsustainable. Thus, in these new Fathering and Mothering TABLE 2.3: Most important reasons for not having children. On the other hand, the adaptation of actors, institutions and social structures to the new realities is also not complete. Women who have joined the labour market face, first of all, a lack of commitment by many men unwilling or unable to adjust their levels of involvement in housework and childcare in response to their partners’ employment. Although working outside the home (and the income it provides for household) gives women negotiating power in relation to their partners, the level of effective co-responsibility in households where both partners work is limited. A high proportion of men do household chores that are typically male (such as paying the bills, doing repairs) and a growing number participate in tasks that are less specialized (weekly shopping or cleaning), but the number of men who assume equal responsibility for all household tasks continues to be low (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009). According to some analyses, as a result of the incorporation of women into the workforce, the gap between the total number of hours that women and men work –whether in the home or on the job– far from decreasing has actually increased (England, 2006). This situation is especially evident after the birth of children. The limited development in our country of public services for early childhood care and for aid to families represents a factor of tension of first order between productive and reproductive work. Despite young women’s strong committment to work for many of them the birth of a child still involves abandoning the labour market or a reduction in the number of hours worked and has a significant impact on their professional advancement (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009: 103). The economic impact of these changes can be considerable. Esping-Andersen (2009:85-86) has estimated, for example, that interruptions in work for Spanish women with two children have an economic cost equivalent to 20 percent of their total income earned over the course of their lives. This Fathering and Mothering contexts, women’s employment has not only become a central element of the new female identity but it is also a conditio sine qua non to begin an economically sustainable family with a partner, which assumes access to basic goods –starting with their own place to live– and protection against economic adversity. Today the formation of new families normally requires two incomes. Families with only one breadwinner are at a very high risk of poverty, which is accentuated during economic downturns (Marí-Klose et al., 2008a). In addition, those who follow traditional models face, on the one hand, an increasingly hostile institutional environment that neglects their needs and demands, and on the other hand, hegemonic discourses that rebuke their practices as anachronisms of the past that should be gradually left behind. In this regard, public policies in many countries are clearly immersed in a shift that seeks to offer protection and aid to families that have moved away from traditional models of status and gender roles. Economic aid for families is increasingly directed toward those in which both parents work (through, for example, subsidies for working mothers) and the expansion of services is aimed at the «defamiliarization» of caregiving activities which have traditionally been carried out by women. This process coincides with a gradual devaluation of the role of the housewife. Exclusive dedication to homemaking and childcare has lost much of the social status it once enjoyed; little by little it is becoming a social anomaly defying widely held social expectations and which does not fit with the new schemes for social protection. From an individual standpoint, the lack of a meaningful career path condemns the woman whose relationship ends or whose spouse dies to greater risk of social exclusion. From the perspective of society, the persistence of a group that does not directly contribute to the public system of social provision is seen as a threat to its financial sustainability. The arrival of children also intensifies the separation of roles in the home along traditional lines. Among couples with young children, the difference in hours dedicated to housework between women and men has increased and the distribution of domestic responsibilities has become more unequal. Obviously, this pattern is related to the decrease in the time mothers spend working outside the home. But it is also related to the increase in the proportion of men who increase the time they dedicate to work. Today for many young couples the arrival of a child brings with it adjustments in the levels of work for both the woman and the man (generally in the opposite direction) TABLE 2.4: Distribution of domestic responsibilities in homes in which the women work, by couples with and without children(a) In percentages DOMESTIC RESPONSIBILITIES SHE DOES IT ALWAYS / USUALLy COUPLE SHARES TASK EQUALLY NO CHILDREN has CHILDREN NO CHILDREN HAS CHILDREN Doing the laundry 56 76 39 19 Preparing meals/cooking 53 67 32 24 Making beds 45 59 47 34 Deciding what to eat the following day 40 66 51 30 Doing the cleaning 34 50 62 42 Washing the dishes 28 48 61 43 Paying the bills 23 31 60 53 Doing the shopping 22 35 69 55 Doing minor repairs in the home 9.9 9.0 19 12 Taking the car to be repaired 9.8 7.6 22 19 Note: a) The data correspond to women’s answers: Number of cases: 1,532; women who live with partner and are under 40 years of age. Source: Iglesias de Ussel et al. (2009) p. 106. Fathering and Mothering cost is four times greater than that suffered by Danish women, who have greater opportunities for balancing motherhood and a professional career thanks to the existence of public services to support families. There is widespread awareness of these costs. Fifty-eight percent of Spaniards understand that «having children is an obstacle for the woman’s career»; this increases to 69 percent among persons with children under three years of age (CIS, Study 2,578, 2004). The proportion of women who are aware of these obstacles is somewhat higher than that of men, although a majority of men do perceive that these obstacles exist. In these conditions, women with children face high opportunity costs. These costs are higher the greater the monetary value of their productive work and the more highly they regard their career. Some authors have pointed out that women become fully aware of the magnitude of these costs the moment they decide to have a second child. Their inclination to have more children after the birth of the first increases if they believe that they can count on a partner who is going to be co-responsible in raising the second or subsequent children and that they will, therefore, be able to reduce the opportunity costs associated with motherhood. The experience in raising the first child is, as a result, the deciding factor (Cooke, 2003; Mills et al., 2008). The birth of a child no longer only implies «bringing joy and hope», crowing and stabilizing the family project, as in the past. As we point out in a recent study, organizing childcare usually involves considerable difficulties that crystallize in stressful situations (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009). Couples with small children tend to express greater disagreement regarding the division of housework as well as higher levels of dissatisfaction with the opportunities available to enjoy interpersonal relations with their partner. Possibly women and men experience this situation in different ways. For example, in the study just mentioned, we found that women with children have a greater probability of feeling less in love at the time of the interview in comparison to when they began to live with their partner, than women who do not have children (holding time cohabiting constant). This is not the case for men: there is no statistical difference in terms of the «risk of falling out of love» between men with children and those without. Combining work and family life when there are children is a complicated issue in which women’s new orientations, professional ambitions and autonomy enter into conflict with the daily dynamics of their relationships with their partners. In the process of adaptation to these imbalances situations of tension may arise that cannot always be adequately dealt with. A significant percentage of parents interviewed in the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, carried out for this study, indicate that in their homes situations of tension arise related to the issue of balancing work and family life. Thus, 63 percent of households mention episodes of tension connected to the lack of personal time to relax and disconnect (13 percent indicated that these episodes happened often); 49 percent recognized situations of tension caused by the distribution of housework (9 percent said that this took place often), and 28 percent referred to tension over childcare (2.8 percent said that this happened often). Only 16 percent stated that in their household there were never situations of tension associated with any of these factors. Situations of tension are more frequent in homes where the mother works and has ambitious career aspirations (women with high educational credentials). They are also more common if the children are under three years of age (particularly if the tension is related to caring for the children) and when the involvement of the father in the care of the children is low (table 2.5). The influence of this factor is significant. However, as can be seen, the employment status of the father or his education level has little affect on the atmosphere in the home. Fathering and Mothering in order to face the demands for attention and care which young children require, while at the same time maintaining the family unit’s purchasing power (Iglesias de Ussel et al., 2009: 103). TABLE 2.5: Persons who say that there is tension in the home for different reasons, by socio-demographic characteristics In percentages. Homes with children from 0 to 10 years of age THERE IS TENSION CAUSED BY Sociodemographic characteristics DIVISION OF DOMESTIC RESPONSIBILITIES CARING FOR THE CHILD ESTRÉS EN EL TRABAJO NO DISPONER DE TIEMPO PARA RELAJARSE Yes Often Yes Often Yes Often Yes Often Primary 39 8.1 25 3.1 48 6.4 53 12 Secondary 51 11 26 1.9 53 6.8 64 11 University 54 7.8 33 3.7 60 7.2 69 11 Works full-time 55 9.6 28 2.8 61 7.3 68 11 Works part-time 55 11 29 2.2 56 8.6 68 13 Doesn’t work 40 7.3 27 3.2 47 5.5 56 10 Very involved 49 8.7 29 2.7 54 6.8 64 10 Moderately involved 50 10 27 3.2 56 7.1 63 13 Little or not involved –(a) – – – – – – – Primary 47 10 28 3.3 53 6.4 58 12 Secondary 49 8.6 27 1.5 54 7.2 65 11 University 53 8.2 31 3.9 58 6.6 67 11 Mother’s characteristics Education level Employment status Mother’s level of involvement with child Father’s characteristics Education level Fathering and Mothering of parents and by child’s age (Continue) Works full-time 50 8.6 29 2.6 55 6.9 64 11 Works part-time 43 7.8 26 2.0 54 6.9 66 11 Doesn’t work 53 12 28 2.7 54 5.4 61 11 Very involved 46 6.8 25 1.9 53 5.4 61 8.5 Moderately involved 52 10 30 2.6 56 7.2 65 13 Little or not involved 58 17 39 8.7 61 15 66 19 0-3 years of age 51 9.0 35 3.1 54 6.9 68 13 4-7 years of age 46 9.6 25 3.1 53 7.6 61 11 8-10 years of age 50 8.0 23 1.9 57 5.7 60 9.2 Father’s level of involvement with child Child’s characteristics Age Note: a) Fewer than 20 cases per category. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Graph 2.1 shows the probability that situations of tension connected to the division of housework, care of the children and lack of time to relax will be produced in three different household settings. The first is the traditional setting, where the mother does not work, has a basic education, and the involvement of the father in childcare is low. The second setting is one of transition: the mother works, has a higher education and the involvement of the father is low (an incomplete revolution).The third is similar to the second setting but with a high degree of paternal involvement (a complete revolution). These are ideal types which, independent of their degree of representativeness in the population, permit us to develop an idea of what occurs given a specific constellation of situations. Fathering and Mothering Employment status homes with children from 0 to 10 years of age for different reasons by different models for the division of responsibilities by gender 2.4. The ethic of family care and the model of a good childhood PROBABILITY 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 BECAUSE OF DIVISION OF DOMESTIC RESPONSIBILITIES BECAUSE OF CHILDCARE BECAUSE OF LACK OF TIME TO RELAX REASONS FOR TENSION Traditional setting works and has career aspirations but cannot count on the support of her partner for childcare, tends to produce more situations of tension. Transitional setting Egalitarian setting Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) There is tension in the home for different reasons; 0) There isn’t tension in the home. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, mother’s occupational status, mother’s education level, if the child has siblings, father’s level of involvement. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The results of multivariable analysis suggest that the traditional setting and the egalitarian are clearly more desirable than the transition model. The traditional model displays the lowest probability of tension in the home related to the division of housework and lack of time for relaxation, while the egalitarian model is especially advantageous for preventing situations of tension associated with the care of children. In all cases, the second setting in which the mother Social demand for policies to reconcile work and family life is very widespread. This reflects the desire of many parents to spend more time with their children, especially when they have not yet begun school. Spain is usually included by researchers in that group of countries that is seen as tied to the traditional cultural model in which family care, supported by an ethic of responsibility and moral obligation is considered superior to care provided by paid caregivers –whether it be through the state or by for profit services– as they do not guarantee the same degree of commitment (PfauEffinger, 2006). In this model a «good childhood» is one which takes place in the home, under the supervision of a family member (generally the mother). These criteria should prevail especially when children are at pre-school ages. Data obtained during the decade of the 1990s and the first years of the present decade support this view. Spain is a country in which, comparatively, a high proportion of the population revealed negative attitudes toward mothers working when their children were at pre-school age. Such an attitude was especially widespread among older age groups. Thus, according to data from the 1990 World Values Survey, 71 percent of Spaniards over the age of 50 agreed or totally agreed with the statement «a pre-school child is likely to suffer if his/her mother works.» The fact that only 41 percent of those between the ages of 15 and 29 years of age shared that opinion can now be seen as heralding imminent change. Fathering and Mothering Graph 2.1: Probability that there will be situations of tension in be a high percentage who view part-time work while the children are very young positively (table 2.6). A CIS survey in 2003 allows us disaggregate this data by age cohorts. TABLE 2.6: Opinions on whether women should work full-time, part-time or not work in different situations, by age group In percentages Age FROM 18 TO 34 FROM 35 TO 49 FROM 50 TO 64 65 AND OVER Work full-time 82 83 74 64 Work part-time 15 12 18 19 Not work 2.3 5.0 8.2 17 After marriage and before having children Total 100 When there is a child under school age Work full-time 25 22 15 9.9 Work part-time 51 50 40 32 Not work 24 29 46 58 Total 100 After the youngest child (or the only child) has started school Work full-time 46 43 30 23 Work part-time 47 47 49 44 Not work 7.4 10 21 34 100 (836) (236) (500) (512) Total Number of cases Source: Based on data from Study 2,529, CIS, 2003. Fathering and Mothering And that change has been dramatic. A growing percentage of the Spanish population, primarily those belonging to the younger generations, understand that the care of children cannot continue to depend on mothers leaving work, although there continues to The youngest Spanish adults no longer show any reluctance at leaving their children under the care of others in preschool centres. The overwhelming majority consider it necessary for children between the ages of three and six to attend school or a day-care centre, and a significant percentage (71 percent) see it as necessary that one and two year old children should be in day-care, although the majority consider this necessary only «in some cases.» One out of every three even believe that it is acceptable in certain cases for infants under one to be in day-care, something that not only clashes with the maternalist discourse of the traditional family but is also discouraged by many child psychologists (Belsky et al., 1988). TABLE 2.7: Opinions on whether it is necessary for children to attend school or day care, by different age groups of children In percentages. Persons under 50 years of age In all cases In some cases NEVER Total From 3 to 6 years of age 81 17 2.2 100 From 1 to 2 years of age 17 54 29 Under one year old 5.3 31 64 Age of child Source: Based on data from Study 2,788, CIS, 2009. Behind these considerations one can clearly see the impact of time pressures stemming from balancing work and family life. Sixtyfive percent of those interviewed who think that a child of one or two years old should be in day-care cite as the main reason that in this way «parents will be able to resolve the problem of taking care of the child.» Eighty-two percent of those who consider it necessary for a child under one to be in day-care give the same reason. If we limit the analysis to parents whose child is or has been in day-care before the age of three, the majority indicate once again that it was for reasons related to reconciling work and family life in homes where both parents work. The interests of the child appear to be secondary. Eighteen percent of those interviewed indicated that the main reason for putting the child in day-care is or was «so the child can be with other children.» Thirteen percent put forward «so that he or she can learn and develop» as the reason for placing their child in day-care. Fathering and Mothering Another clear indicator of the evolution in attitudes is the growing perception of legitimacy among Spaniards of the use of social services in support of families with children under three as an alternative to the care of the child in the home. In this regard, CIS Study 2.788 (2009) provides a rare opportunity to examine different attitudes toward the role of day-care centres in the care of children. Asked if it is better for children to be at home or to attend nursery school, 54 percent of those interviewed opted for the latter. There are marked differences among cohorts. Of those under 35 years of age, 66 percent preferred the second option, compared to only 42 percent of those over 65. In percentages: Persons under 50 years of age Because my spouse and I work 63 So child can be with others 18 So child can learn and develop 13 Because I don’t have family to take care of the child 2.8 Other 3.2 Total 100 Number of cases (467) Source: Based on data from Study 2,788, CIS, 2009. However, this does not mean that they do not believe that preschool education has some pedagogical and social benefits for the child. A high percentage of respondents think that school before the age of three can play an important role in the development of the child. They believe that day-care is more effective at teaching children under three years of age «to relate and communicate with others» than the family; in addition, it is better for developing capacities of observation and exploration and for learning rules and routines. These attitudes appear in all social groups with only slight variations by education. The results of this analysis suggest that in recent years an adaptation to new realities is taking place, especially among those age groups which are experiencing in their own lives the problems and worries of raising young children. As a consequence of this change in attitudes, social pressure on women who decide to continue working fulltime and/or take advantage of social services to balance work and family life has decreased. A growing number of Spanish men and women believe that a young child can have «a good childhood» spending lots of hours away from the mother and in the care of childcare professionals. This quiet transition to a new model has gathered supporters with few dissenting voices challenging the course followed either from an ideological standpoint or on the basis of their expertise. Fathering and Mothering TABLA 2.8: Why child is or was in day care before three years of age In percentages. Persons under 50 years of age WHERE DOES A CHILD LEARN THE FOLLOWING ABILITIES BETTER? EDUCATION LEVEL Primary Secondary University Family 57 59 61 School 24 26 25 Both equally 18 15 15 Knowing how to express basic health and well-being needs Total 100 Being able to feed him/herself and take care of personal hygiene Family 52 49 51 School 28 37 33 Both equally 20 15 16 Total 100 Controlling his/her own body (maintaining balance, not falling down, standing up) Family 62 56 52 School 20 24 26 Both equally 19 20 22 Total 100 Relating to and communicating with others Family 19 13 15 School 64 72 70 Both equally 18 15 15 Total 100 Fathering and Mothering TABLE 2.9: Opinions on whether children under three develop certain abilities better in school or in the family, by education level (Continue) Family 28 24 24 School 43 49 46 Both equally 29 29 30 Total 100 Learning routines and rules (schedules, obedience) Family 30 26 30 School 50 50 48 Both equally 20 24 22 Total Source: Based on data from Study 2,788. CIS 2009. 100 Fathering and Mothering Observing and exploring his/her world (being curious, asking questions) Love is spelled T-I-M-E particularly when it comes to relationships between parents and children. It is in the time shared with parents that small children learn life skills, internalize expectations of achievement, develop feelings of confidence and security and find love and stability. This time is a form of social investment which, despite its absence from the traditional indicators of productivity or national accounts, has evident socioeconomic implications for children and for society in general. The quantity and especially the quality of this time is one of the main determinants of children’s well-being and development and, therefore, of their opportunities and productive capacity across the lifespan (Büchel y Duncan, 1998; Cooksey y Fondell, 1996). Sociologists coined the term social capital to refer to the benefits obtained from social connections (Bourdieu, 1983; Coleman, 1988). Intergenerational relationships between parents and children constitute one of the modalities of social capital that generate the highest individual and social returns. However, now more than ever families cannot amass as much social capital due to the difficulties encountered in dedicating the amount of time they would like to their children. On the one hand, the increase in couples breaking up has placed limits on the possibility of interaction with the non-custodial parent. On the other, an image has spread in which parents, and especially mothers, carry out an impossible balancing act to reconcile their dual family and work roles, getting trapped in what some authors have referred to as the «second shift» or «endless work day» (Durán, 1986; Hochschild, 1989). On the flipside, there are children getting out of school becoming «orphans at 5 o’clock,» «latchkey children,» or being rushed from one after-school activity to another. In such conditions it seems that intergenerational relationships are being affected by an increasingly overbooked day, and growing concerns appear to be justified. In contrast to these images, there is solid international evidence –based on analysis from rigorous surveys that have carefully recorded time use in families– that suggests exactly the opposite (Sandberg y Hofferth, 2001; Gauthier et al., 2004). Many mothers and fathers spend more time today with their children than their parents did with them. Their parenting practices, are informed by new childcare ethics and models of what it means to be a «good mother» or a «good father,» which encourage them to extend parental responsibility beyond the spaces and activities where Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age III. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age This value is, in the first place, emotional. As we indicated in the previous chapter, young couples of the «individualistic type» see their sentimental relationships as a way to achieve happiness. The emotional aspects of family life have come to have unprecedented importance. There is firm belief that the expression of love and tenderness towards children can help create an environment and support the kinds of experiences needed to achieve the goal of self-actualization. This conviction has even been embraced by many men, who traditionally renounced direct contact with their children. It is possible that men’s desire to participate in caring for their children is in part a response to having to move away from a traditional model of fatherhood that could arouse the distrust of their partner and prevent them from maintaining the atmosphere of trust and affection which is today the basis of relationships (now much more fragile than in the past). Or perhaps, men’s greater involvement in childcare comes from a genuine desire to experience fatherhood in a different way, consistent with new conceptions of masculinity that allow men to openly express their feelings, show weakness and value the personal rewards of intimacy, physical contact and caring for others (Alberdi and Escario, 2007). Secondly, in recent decades fears have intensified regarding risks threatening the physical safety of children. Urbanization, the growth of cities and the increase in social diversity combined with the erosion of community ties have generated concerns that did not exist in the past, when children played alone in the streets and plazas without parents worrying about their safety. Today it is understood that the home is the only safe haven against the new dangers awaiting them. Nowadays parents accompany their children to many of the activities that in the past they would have gone to alone: to school, to meet with their friends, or to afterschool activities. If they do not accompany them, they run the risk of being labelled irresponsible or negligent. The household, increasingly outfitted with all kinds of gadgets and technologies to entertain –technologically sophisticated toys, televisions in the bedroom, internet connection, etc.– have turned into true «gilded cages» where children spend many hours near their parents. A third element that has influenced fathers to spend more time with their children is the recognition of the pedagogical value of their involvement in the education of their children. The decline in the birth rate has reduced the number of children per home, and as a result, it has increased the capacity of parents to concentrate resources (primarily time and money) on their children, investing in their quality (Becker and Lewis, 1973). The anxiety of many parents in the face of the uncertain future of their children in an unpredictable world, where it is increasingly more difficult to assure that children maintain the same social status of the family, has encouraged many parents to become intensely involved early on in their children’s upbringing. Behaving as a «good father» or «good mother» means enrolling one’s children in the best early childhood education centres, offering them opportunities to express and develop their talents from the earliest age in organized activities, but also dedicating quality time to them to stimulate their cognitive aptitudes and support their learning. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age it had traditionally been exercised in the past. Caring for a child no longer means just «being present» or «responding diligently» to the child’s needs. Sharing quality time with children through involvement in interactive activities that require attention has now acquired a value that was unrecognized in the past. 3.1. When children arrive: new motherhood and fatherhood There is evidence in Spain suggesting that men spend more time taking care of their children (Larrañaga et al., 2004). However, there is still clear evidence of an imbalance in the division of this responsibility. In many homes with small children, taking care of the children is now a joint activity, but in the majority of cases, it TABLE 3.1: Working day of couples between 25 and 49 years of age by age of child In percentages ALL COUPLES COUPLES WITHOUT CHILDREN COUPLES WITH AT LEAST ONE CHILD UNDER 6 COUPLES WITH CHILDREN OVER 6 woman working full-time 44 63 38 42 woman working part-time 14 8.4 17 14 woman not working 30 16 35 33 woman working full-time 0.7 1.0 0.7 0.6 woman working part-time 0.5 0.7 0.5 0.4 woman not working 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 Woman working full-time 4.1 5.6 3.2 4.4 woman working part-time 1.5 1.1 1.5 1.5 woman not working 4.3 3.7 4.3 4.5 COUPLE FORMED BY Man working full-time and Man working part-time and Man not working and Total 100 Source: Economically Active Population Survey, INE [National Statistics Institute], 2008. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age The engine behind these ideological changes has been the deep sociodemographic transformations that began in the latter part of the 20th century. In this chapter and the following one, we will examine new ways of experiencing motherhood and fatherhood and, therefore, intergenerational relationships. We will also analyze the participation of other persons and specialized services in the care of children. In this chapter we will focus on caregiving for children from 0 to 4 years of age. Women have massively entered into the labour market and contribute financially to the family. In barely three decades, the employment rate for women between 25 and 49 years of age has almost doubled, increasing from 36 percent in 1990 to 63 percent in 2010, according to the Spanish Labour Force Survey (EPA) data. However, employment statistics reveal significant differences in women’s commitment to work depending on their family situation. As can be seen in the following table, in three out of five couples with children under six, the women work outside the home. This is a high proportion if we compare it to the situation found just two decades ago. But it is significantly lower than the number of women working who live with their partner and do not have children (80 percent). Another important difference lies in the percentage of women working part-time (19 percent of women with children under six and 10 percent of those without children). (a) Resignation and renunciation For many women the arrival of children has important implications for their life course and career; this may be because they leave their jobs, temporarily or permanently, relinquish positions of responsibility or reduce their working hours, etc. Scaling back is one of is one of the strategies often adopted by couples with two incomes as a way to manage the care of the children. But rather than a strategy, in the majority of families it becomes a necessity. According to data from our Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 23 percent of women compared to only 4.8 percent of men with children under five years of age recognize, since the birth of their child, having had to leave a job, studies or some other form of training because they had problems in finding a centre or a person to take care of their child. The cost of this withdrawal from work or study –assessed according to different factors, such as contribution to family income, potential to develop a successful career, etc– influenced the decision. The educational level of the women is the principal factor determining this cost. Graph 3.1 shows the proportion of women that have had to abandon an activity after the birth of their child is substantially lower among women with university degrees. Along with education, it is important to consider employment conditions before and after the birth of a child. Leaving the workforce is more likely when working conditions are precarious. In these situations many women feel pushed to quit jobs due to time limitations which make it difficult to balance work and new family responsibilities, or because of employer discrimination (Azmat et al., 2003). Among those who decide to continue working after the birth of children, it is not unusual to find situations of downward mobility in terms of quality of work, income or responsibility (Gutierrez-Doménech, 2002). Women who are not employed (whether because they have lost their jobs or because they are inactive) usually remain in this situation. Even though they may want to work, finding a job that adjusts to their needs to balance work and family becomes a very difficult task. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age is still not carried out equally. In contrast to what happened in the past, when the father’s commitment to childcare was much less, the exercise of motherhood for the majority of women today is part-time. after the birth of their child by educational level Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age PERCENTAGE inactive at the time of the birth of their child: the vast majority of these women still do not work (80 percent inactive and 8 percent unemployed). The labour market is an inhospitable territory for mothers who are not in it at the time of giving birth. TABLE 3.2: Mother’s current employment status by status 35 at the time of the birth of child 30 25 In percentages. Mothers of children between 0 and 4 years of age 20 EMPLOYMENT STATUS AT TIME OF BIRTH 15 10 5 0 PRIMARY OR NO EDUCATION SECONDARY UNIVERSITY WOMAN’S LEVEL OF EDUCATION CURRENT EMPLOYMENT STATUS IS IS IS WORKING UNEMPLOYED INACTIVE Was working 78 13 8.7 Was unemployed 28 42 30 Was inactive 12 8.0 80 Total 100 Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. In table 3.2 we show the current employment status of mothers with children between 0 and 4 years of age with respect to their status at the moment of the birth of the child. At that time, 70 percent of the current mothers were working. Of these, almost four out of five are still working. A fairly high percentage of these women (13 percent) are now unemployed and 8.7 percent have become inactive. At the time of the birth of their child, 18 percent of the women were unemployed. At present, approximately three out of every four of these women continue to be out of work (42 percent unemployed; 30 percent inactive). For these women there is a shortage of jobs that are compatible with their family needs. Finally 12 percent were Motherhood usually jeopardizes one’s position in the labour market. The percentage of mothers with children between 0 and 4 years of age who were working when their child was born but were unemployed at the time of the survey (13 percent) far exceeds the percentage of their partners who are unemployed but had work at the time of the birth of their child (6.5 percent). The mothers most at risk of losing their job are those who have lower educational qualifications. Twenty-two percent of mothers with just a basic education and who were working at the time of the birth of their child are currently unemployed (11 percent of the men with this educational level have gone from being employed to unemployed after the birth of their child). The same Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age GRAPH 3.1: Women that have had to abandon an activity In short, there is little evidence that women who abandon the labour market do so deliberately with the intention of dedicating themselves fulltime to parenting. The profile of the woman who gives up her career driven by strong convictions about motherhood (what has become an actual movement in the U.S. called Mothers opting out) represents, in any case, a small minority. The majority of mothers who find themselves in this situation have been forced into it (or at least pushed) by circumstances. Leaving the workforce is probably not permanent. Many will reincorporate into the labour market when the opportunity for a job that meets their needs becomes available or when the employment situation in the country improves. A slightly lower percentage of new mothers move from working to inactivity. In some cases, this probably follows an intermediate period of unemployment. Often, inactivity is the result of a decision not to look for employment in situations in which it is difficult to find work that is compatible with caring for a young child (rather than the mother’s desire to dedicate herself exclusively to parenting). The costs of work opportunities can be especially high for mothers likely to earn low pay. As a result, mothers with lower educational qualifications are also the ones who are most likely to move from work to inactivity. Fifteen and a half percent of mothers who have only a basic educational level and were working at the time of their child’s birth are currently in a situation of inactivity. This is the same for 12.1 percent of mothers with secondary school diplomas. Among women with university degrees this shift is less common, affecting only 4.1 percent. It is likely that many of these women leave their jobs because of a genuine desire to dedicate themselves to the upbringing of their children. The reshaping of policy on parental leave conforms to new ways of understanding motherhood and fatherhood, incorporating into these experiences a new range of choices and dilemmas that require negotiated and thoughtful solutions. These leaves constitute one of the main chapters in the so-called traditional reconciliation policies designed to make it possible for women to balance work with maternity and care when the children are in most need of it. But for some years these policies have pursued more than this. The introduction of paternity leave (or the possibility that the mother transfers part of this right to the father) represents a new formula to encourage fathers to be involved in the care of their children from birth. (b) Maternity and paternity leave Leave policies in developed countries have been a powerful support tool for women who want to continue working after giving birth (making their return to work under the same conditions as when Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age is true for 17 percent of those mothers who completed secondary school (in comparison to 8.2 percent of the men with this level of education). Even those mothers with higher educational levels face a significantly higher risk of unemployment than their partners (7.4 percent and 0.9 percent respectively). Thus, the vulnerability of mothers reflects, «the market value» of their degrees but also business strategies of getting rid of female workers when they decide to have children. Many businesses are probably influenced by the belief that these mothers are going to be less productive when they have to assume the main responsibility for parenting after the birth of the child. Data support the idea often expressed by feminist theorists and researchers that gender inequalities in the home are the main obstacle to women in the pursuit of full equality in the labour market (see, for example, England, 2000). In Spain, 91 percent of the women who work outside the home take maternity leave after giving birth. The data we examined in our survey suggest that the use of this right is widespread, even among the most vulnerable groups. Even so, there are small but significant differences in the proportion of women who do not take maternity leave based on different socioeconomic factors. The variable that best indicates the possibility of enjoying this right, as well as how much leave time is used, is household income. Although the vast majority of working women from more disadvantaged sectors take advantage of their right to maternity leave, a considerable percentage of these women have not been able to do so: 16.5 percent (see table 3.3). These figures point to a more likely precarious participation in the labour market. It is possible that many women who give up their right to maternity leave do so in order not to lose a job that is precarious or they are forced to return early to some type of self-employment. TABLA 3.3: Women who took maternity leave by age, education level and household income In percentages. Households with children between 0 and 4 years of age MOTHER’S CHARACTERISTICS PERCENTAGE Age 18 to 35 92 36 to 40 92 Over 40 86 Education level Primary 86 Secondary 90 University 93 Household income erase level per person Less than €320 83 Between €321 and €500 89 Between €501 and €800 92 Over €800 94 Total 91 Number of cases (675) Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age they took leave possible) and for protecting newborns. A large body of evidence suggests that babies who do not receive direct care from their parents during the first twelve months of life are likely to experience adverse situations in their development (Waldfogel et al., 2002). The majority of European countries grant parental leave which ranges from four to six months, although income replacement rates vary. The inadequate implementation of these initiatives can have unexpected consequences. Too short a maternity leave cuts off the benefits intended for mothers and infants prematurely. Faced with these restrictions, many women prefer to completely abandon the labour market in order to better care for their children. For example, in the Netherlands where maternity leave extends four months, 25 percent of women leave the workforce (Gustafsson and Kenjoh, 2004). In Spain, where maternity leave is of a similar duration, women abandoning the labour market is also high. However, when maternity leave is too long the human capital of mothers (their knowledge, competencies and skills) erodes and makes reincorporation into the labour market difficult (OECD, 2007). The reforms that encourage fathers to take paternity leave have been relatively recent in Spain. In our country, working women (who meet certain minimum established requirements for eligibility) are eligible for 16 weeks of maternity leave, which can be transferred in part to the father (up to a maximum of 10 weeks), with the exception of the first six weeks after birth, which are available only for the woman. Until three years ago, no paternity leave existed similar to maternity leave. Men could get two days off from work for the birth of a child, while women with work contracts had the right to a leave from work for 16 weeks, as long as they fulfilled the contributory requirements. Women could (and still can) give up a part of this leave time (up to ten weeks) and transfer it to the father. With the recent Law of Equality (March, 2007) a new paternity leave was introduced, fifteen days of leave from work independent of the mother’s employment status and that is personal and non transferable to the woman. Thus, a significant number of the fathers who responded to this survey were among the first to take advantage of this new measure. The data analyzed in the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood reveal that, on average, men take two and a half weeks of leave and women take 16 and a half weeks, which is basically the amount of time stipulated by the law. The approval of this law has brought about a substantial increase in paternity leave. According to our survey, the proportion of fathers that took advantage of paternity leave almost quadrupled (increasing from 15 percent to 58 percent). Table 3.4 provides some interesting points for of reflection to clarify whether the men who take paternity leave correspond to a specific profile and if this has changed since the law came into effect. Fathers more often take this new paternity leave, and in the vast majority of cases shared with their wives, when their partners work. The beginning of life with a child is an opportunity for delight that many men do no want to give up, especially with the firstborn. Data indicate that younger fathers take paternity leave more often. This tendency consolidates patterns of behaviour already observed before the law came into effect. Apparently, new generations of fathers are incorporating innovative behaviours. The extension of paternity leave represents social support–certainly still not sufficient–for new masculine aspirations, which distance fathers from a model which kept them away from witnessing and enjoying the first days of life with their newborn child. It is worth noting that the relationship between the variable level of education and paternity leave has changed since the approval of the law. Before its approval, the proportion of fathers who took leave time was much higher among those with university studies. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age Paternity leave has a much shorter history. It is a response to a new understanding of parenting, which aims to promote a more effective and balanced participation of fathers in the home environment. There is growing evidence that suggests that paternity leave encourages the involvement of fathers in the care of their children and a more equitable distribution of domestic responsibilities, or at least it prevents the traditionalization of roles (the accentuation of differences based on gender) which usually occur after the birth of children. For example, Nepomnyaschy and Waldfogel (2007), using a sample of longitudinal data of North American families, found that the probability of the father actively participating in the different activities related to childcare at the end of the first nine months is much greater if he has taken two weeks or more of leave time at the time of birth. Regardless of the strength of the long-term effects, paternity leave has laid the foundation for a new form of fathering. opened a door that had been kept closed for one group of fathers who are now willing to assume new forms of fatherhood. TABLE 3.4: Fathers who took paternity leave before and after the Law of Equality entered into effect In percentages. Households with children between 0 and 4 years of age PATERNITY LEAVE AFTER LAW OF EQUALITY(a) PATERNITY LEAVE BEFORE LAW OF EQUALITY Father’s socio-demographic characteristics Age at fatherhood 19 to 34 years old 61 17 35 to 40 57 15 Over 40 54 8.0 Primary or no education 55 12 Secondary 61 12 University 56 21 Education level Type of work contract Employee with permanent contract 64 –(b) Employee with temporary contract 58 – Employer or professional with employees 42 – Self-employed or profession without employees 40 – 17 to 31 years old 65 14 32 to 37 57 15 Mother’s socio-demographic characteristics Age at motherhood Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age After the law’s approval, other fathers seem to be participating almost equally in this right. In this sense, the law seems to have Over 37 52 12 Primary or no education 53 9.6 Secondary 59 15 University 58 17 Working 62 – Not working 42 – Child has siblings 53 11 Firstborn child 64 21 Total 58 15 (307) (67) Education level Employment status at time of birth Household characteristics Number of cases Note: a) The Organic Law 3/2007 of 22 March for the effective equality of women and men established a two week paternity leave. b) Less than 20 cases per category. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The results also illustrate that, in spite of the step forward that the new law meant, economic and employment barriers persist for many fathers. Fathers who are employed with permanent contracts take advantage of paternity leave to a greater extent than any other occupational group. They are closely followed by those with temporary contracts, suggesting that the institutionalization of the right is not in conflict with business practices that might be interested in restricting it (covertly or overtly wielding the possibility of not renewing a temporary contract). Businessmen or the self-employed (with or without employees) are those least likely to take paternity leave. Doing so for them would entail much higher costs than for other groups. 3.2. Schedules in families with young children Taking care of children takes time. The time that fathers and mothers are able to invest in the care of their children depends Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age (Continue) On the other hand, the dimension of time which orders events as a succession of activities, each of which occupies a quantifiable amount of time, does not allow access to certain social qualities of the phenomenon of caregiving. Some authors have pointed out that taking care of someone is not only an activity bounded by time, but it is above all a mental state (Folbre and Bittman, 2004): implying responsibilities, organization, continuous availability, and time to be «attentive to someone.» The tools to gather information on how time is employed in certain activities also do not reflect all of the organizing conflicts derived from the necessities of providing care. There are difficulties in adequately recording simultaneous tasks or those that are carried out in combination with other tasks, those in which care is sometimes recorded as a secondary activity or is not accurately recorded because of its being considered a normal and undervalued activity. The Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood is not a survey specifically about time use, although parents were asked about how many hours they spent with their children the previous day.1 We realize that by only recording the number of hours parents and children spend together many of the nuances making up the time parents share with their children are lost. We did not obtain information on the intensity of contact or on the specific attention the children receive from each parent. We could not record the range of gestures that accompany a shared activity, nor the expression of affection and feelings to which it gives rise to. But parents’ answers provide us with a clear picture of the time framework supporting the bond between parents and children. It is a necessarily incomplete picture, but one that makes it possible to trace basic patterns in the management of the care of children and the inequalities in carrying it out. It has also offered us the opportunity to analyze subjective perceptions of time in the light of objective dedication. Our initial objective has been to examine the quantity of time in hours that mothers and fathers spend with their children differentiating work days and weekends - and the valuation that they merit. In the analyses we take into account four factors, commonly cited in the specialized literature, that can condition shared time: the characteristics of the child (age and sex); individual characteristics of the parents (sex, education level); 1 Time Use Surveys–which quantify the amount of time members of a household dedicate to different activities through diaries in which individuals record what they do throughout the day–are without a doubt the best instrument that the social sciences have to analyze reproductive work. However, despite the methodological sophistication of these surveys, their usefulness is limited. It is an extremely expensive instrument. Due to the cost, researchers are not able to adminster them at sufficient intervals to track the changes that take place in the daily organization of activities and time distribution in households. The last (and only) survey on time use that we have for Spain was carried out in 2002-2003, which makes it an inadequate instrument to analyze social processes that are changing continuously and rapidly. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age on objective constraints such as their work schedules or the availability of services for early childhood care. But equally or more influential in the time parents dedicate to caring for their children are their convictions and personal preferences, their attitudes with respect to gender role distribution and their convictions related to what a «good childhood» consists of and what role other social agents (formal and informal) play in achieving this. Time distribution in attending to and caring for children is a complicated task which every family carries out as well as it can and knows how to, primarily (but not exclusively) in the domestic sphere. Reproductive work takes place within a private context, which confers an «invisibility» that other sociological phenomena occurring in the public sphere do not have. This has traditionally entailed difficulties for its study. The time that mothers and fathers spend with their children depends to a great extent on the children’s age. Younger children need more attention given that can do virtually nothing by themselves. The majority of younger children spend many hours under the direct supervision of their parents, whose dedication to providing care during these years is very intense. Graph 3.2 shows the average amount of time mothers and fathers are with their children on work days and weekends based on the age of the child. The first thing that can be seen is that the patterns of time shared with the child are very different on work days and weekends. After the first years of life, the time fathers and mothers dedicate to the child during the week steadily decreases, coinciding with the majority of children at three or four years of age entering the school system. In contrast, during the weekends, the time spent together is high, independent of the age of the child. GRAPH 3.2: Average time fathers and mothers spend with children by child’s age. Week days and weekends Hours per day. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age NUMBER OF HOURS 14 Week days Weekends 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 CHILD’S AGE Father Mother Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 14 12 10 8 Both during weekdays and weekends, children spend more time with their mothers than with their fathers. On average, mothers spend 8.4 hours with their children and fathers 5.7 hours. Intergenerational time is primarily feminine time during the work week. The weekend is family time. Therefore, the average difference in hours that mothers and fathers spend with their children during the week is almost three hours, while on weekends the difference Madre is 1.4 hours. The imbalance between genders is greater in the first Padre two or three years of the child’s life, when providing care continues to be primarily the mother’s responsibility. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age employment profiles of the couple; and the contribution of outside caregivers (grandparents, relatives, babysitters). The employment profiles of the couple are without a doubt the factor that has the most effect on availability and organization of time in family life. Thus, on work days, as expected, parents who work outside the home spend less time with their children than those who do not. However, on weekends, they seem to want to compensate for their absence as they invest as much or more time in their children as parents who do not work outside the home. GRAPH 3.3: Average time fathers and mothers spend The incorporation of women into the paid workforce has not corrected the gender imbalance in time dedicated to childcare that is found when only the father works. In families in which both parents work, mothers continue to spend more time taking care of the children than does the father, especially on work days: on average, spending 2.3 hours more per day with the child during the week than the father. This difference is 4.7 hours more when the mother does not work. For the mother, having a job means on average spending almost four hours less in childcare than a mother who does not work, hours the child is normally with someone from outside the home. The amount of time fathers spend with children in homes where the mother works does not compensate for the hours that the mother is not available. In fact, there are no differences in the amount of time that fathers spend with their children in families where the mothers work outside the home and in those where they do not. with children by parent’s educational level. Week days and weekends Hours per day. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age NUMBER OF HOURS 14 Week days Weekends 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 PRIMARY SECONDARY UNIVERSITY PRIMARY SECONDARY UNIVERSITY PARENT’S EDUCATION LEVEL Average no. hours father spends with child Average no. hours mother spends with child Source: Based on data from the Survey on Childhood and Inter and Intragenerational Relationships, 2010. During weekends, the time that mothers and fathers spend with their children increases considerably, although independent of the employment status of the couple, it continues being the mother who on average dedicates a little more time to being with the child. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age Besides gender, there is also a series of sociodemographic characteristics which condition the time parents spend with their children. Thus, for example, the higher the educational level of the parent, especially of the mother, the less time spent with the child. The opportunity cost of spending time with children is higher for parents with higher levels of education. However, on weekends practically no differences are seen related to the educational level of the parents. GRAPH 3.4: Average time fathers and mothers spend with children by couple’s employment status. Week days and weekends Hours per day: Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age NUMBER OF HOURS 14 Week days Weekends 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 BOTH MEMBERS OF COUPLE WORK ONLY FATHER WORKS Average time spent by father BOTH MEMBERS OF COUPLE WORK ONLY FATHER WORKS Average time spent by mother Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Perception of a lack of time together The vast majority of mothers and fathers value positively the opportunity to spend time with their children, and they consider it necessary for the adequate growth and development of their children. Some authors have pointed out that the cultural standards that establish the amount of time that is «necessary» for parents to spend with their children are more demanding today than in the past, which in context of growing difficulties in reconciling work and family life can cause frustration and anxiety (Bianchi, 2000; Daly, 2001). Numerous studies have analyzed the time that mothers and fathers spend with their children and how they organize this time, but less is known about how they perceive the time they spend with their children and what factors influence these perceptions. According to data from The Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood approximately one third of parents recognize that in general they do not spend enough time with their children (graph 3.5). There are sharp differences between what fathers think and what mothers think: almost twice as many fathers as mothers think that they do not spend enough time with their children. In fact, there is strong agreement in the figures obtained when examining self-perceptions and the perceptions of those interviewed about the time that their partners spend with their children: 23 percent of the men interviewed think that their partners do not spend enough time with their children, and 45 percent of the women think the same of their partners. Thus, there is a shared and widespread perception among men and their partners that fathers are not present enough in the lives of their children. The data about the quantity of time that men and women share with their children appears to confirm these opinions. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age Our analysis does not show that the help of outside caregivers (grandparents, other family members, babysitters) has a very significant influence on the time that parents spend with their children. On average, mothers dedicated 7.4 hours on work days when they had outside help available and 8.2 hours when they did not (which means approximately four hours less by the end of the work week). Fathers dedicated 4.6 hours daily if there were outside caretakers and 4.8 hours if there were not. they spend with their children Household with children from 0 to 4 years of age PERCENTAGE 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 MORE THAN ENOUGH ENOUGH Father NOT ENOUGH Mother Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. We do not know how much time mothers and fathers have to spend with their children for them to consider it to be «enough», but we know the amount of time they usually spend together. The fathers who responded that, in general, they do not spend enough time with their children had spent on average 4.2 hours with them the previous day (if it was a work day). In the case of mothers, they had spent an average of 6.7 hours with their children. These differences suggest that there is a significant imbalance in standards regarding time spent with children between men and women. Mothers who thought they spent enough time with their children spent on average 8.4 hours with them; in other words, a full work day. In contrast, fathers who felt they spent sufficient time with their children had spent 5.7 hours with them. The objective amount of time (in hours) that mothers and fathers spend with their children is not, therefore the only determinant of perceptions about the adequacy of their dedication. The probability of mothers thinking they spend insufficient time with their children is related to individual estimates of what is appropriate (which are not distributed socially in a uniform manner) and to cultural expectations (whose influences are also unequal). The participation of mothers in the workforce, especially if it is full-time, has a powerful effect on their perceptions. Women who work tend to be concerned about the time they dedicate to their children, even though objectively speaking they spend the same amount of time with them.2 The perception of spending less time than necessary with children is also more common, all other things being equal, when the educational level of the woman is lower. It is likely that women with fewer educational resources question more the convenience of working, given that the opportunity cost of doing so is higher. Perhaps their families have a more traditional idea of the role of the woman, with higher levels of expectations regarding the amount of time that should be spent with the children, behaviours that deviate from these expectations being more severely criticized. 2 Probabilities have been calculated with a logistic regression model, which isolates the statistical effects of the explanatory variables, controlling for the influence of sociodemographic factors and the number of hours dedicated to their children. The explanatory variables used are indicated in the footnote to graph 3.6. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age GRAPH 3.5: Parent’s evaluation of the amount of time GRAPH 3.6: Probability of mother believing she does not spend enough time with her child by different levels of partner’s co-responsibility Mothers with children from 0 to 4 years of age PROBABILITY 0.70 0.60 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 MOTHER DOESN’T WORK, FATHER’S TIME INSUFFICIENT MOTHER WORKS PART-TIME, FATHER’S TIME INSUFFICIENT MOTHER WORKS FULL-TIME, FATHER’S TIME INSUFFICIENT MOTHER WORKS PART-TIME, FATHER’S TIME SUFFICIENT MOTHER WORKS FULL-TIME, FATHER’S TIME SUFFICIENT Note: The probabilities are calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) feels she doesn’t spend enough time with her children and 0) feels she spends enough or more than enough time with her children. The following independent variables have been introduced into the model: child’s age, mother’s level of education, hours mother spends with child, if child is an only child, if child attends child care centre, perception of the amount of time father spends with child, mother’s employment status. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 3 In statistical terms, there is an interaction between the effect attributed to the mother’s working hours and the perception that the mother has regarding the father’s involvement. Beyond theses aspects, it is necessary to address two other explanatory factors which, have an influence on mothers’ attitudes. On the one hand, the most «inexperienced» mothers (with only one child) tend to show more concern about the time they dedicate to their child (all other things being equal). On the other hand, Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age But the most decisive factor affecting women’s perceptions about the amount of time they spend with their children is their perception of the amount of time their partners spend with the children. Mothers who feel that their partners do not spend enough time with the children are much more likely to feel that they also do not, holding all other things constant. In other words, the perception they have about their dedication depends to a great extent on what their partner does (or at least on their perception of this). They are more negative, regardless of the hours shared with their child, if they feel that their partner is not involved enough and therefore the child is not receiving enough attention. The co-responsibility of the partner also significantly attenuates mothers’ feelings of guilt for participating in the workforce.3 Working does not cause mothers great anxiety when there is co-responsibility (graph 3.6). If women who work full-time have a partner who is sufficiently involved in the children’s lives, the likelihood of feeling that they do not dedicate enough time to their children is similar to that of mothers in traditional situations (mothers who do not work and have partners who are not sufficiently involved in their children’s lives). 3.3. The quality of time provided to children The quality of life in childhood depends to a great extent on the quality of the interaction between parents and children. Specialized research points out the volume of time, measured in hours, can, in general terms, be beneficial for the well-being of the child, but it is not an adequate indicator to measure the contribution of the parents to the development of the child’s skills and learning abilities. The quality of the interactions between parents and children is a stronger determinant than the quantity of time. The accumulated evidence is abundant. For example, children exposed regularly to language stimuli by their mothers during the first years of life already begin to demonstrate considerable differences in linguistic competence from those who receive poorer stimulation (Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Hart and Risley, 1999). Research shows that stimulation that pursues a response from the child, the strengthening of appropriate behaviours through expression of approval and affection, the effort to communicate with the child from the first months on (inviting the child to say words through songs or reading aloud) and activities directed toward developing skills and competencies, all contribute to adequate cognitive and socioemotional development at this stage (Ramey and Ramey, 2000; Zuckerman and Kahn, 2000). Some studies suggest that the active involvement of the father is particularly important. In this vein, a recent study by Bronte-Tinkew et al. (2008), focused on the analysis of babbling and exploration capacities, showed that children whose fathers are more involved in their care and supervision are less likely to suffer cognitive delays. Along with this work focused on early child development, there is growing evidence that relates the educational activities of parents with their children in the first years of life with school performance in later years (Neidell, 2000; Sylva et al., 2010). In this section the differences in the types of activities mothers and fathers do with their children and their intensity are analyzed in relation to the socioeconomic profiles of the parents. Given the importance of these activities to the present and future development of children, this is key to understanding the mechanisms involved in the reproduction of social inequalities. Caregiving is a multi-sided activity. It activates diverse faculties and emotions and can have different objectives as its aim, in isolation or simultaneously. Caregiving activities have a primary purpose, which is to offer support to a dependent person, but many times in the process, other objectives of no less importance are achieved (either deliberately or as a byproduct of the primary activity): creating an atmosphere of affection or emotion, educating the person being taken care of and/or oneself, building bonds of trust, etc. There are a great variety of typologies for distinguishing and Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age mothers who place their children in early childhood education centres or day-care do not have more negative perceptions regarding the time dedicated to their children, even when they are very young (under two years of age). As was noted in the previous chapter, a growing number of Spanish men and women are not critical of the idea that a child can commence a «good childhood» spending many hours away from its mother under the care of childcare professionals. The fact that mothers who place their children in these centres do not express greater dissatisfactions regarding the time they dedicate to their children is further proof of the growing legitimacy given to this strategy. of three parents. A little more than half the mothers and fathers state that they take their child for a walk or to the park daily or almost daily and a similar percentage state that they run errands with the child. Finally, four out of ten families take their children to visit relatives daily or almost daily, and a similar proportion do crafts with their child with this same frequency. In a questionnaire like ours, which addresses different issues, the capacity to thoroughly analyze the range and frequency of possible activities that parents could share with their children is limited. Our intention has been to confine the analysis to the most common spaces of intergenerational sociability at the ages we are examining, and within them the most representative indicators. Thus, some of the questions in the survey record activities that require direct, explicit and individualized interaction with the child, involving intellectual stimulation of the child’s capacities –such as reading stories, teaching letters or numbers, singing or teaching songs or doing crafts–, while others capture expressions of love and affection which the parents express to the child, such as hugging, kissing or playing. We have also included shared activities that do not necessarily require direct and explicit interaction between parents and child, such as visiting relatives, going for a walk or to the park, and going out with the child to run errands. GRAPH 3.7: How often parents did different activities with their child in the previous week In percentages. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age TOOK CHILD TO VISIT RELATIVES TOOK CHILD TO THE PARK TOOK CHILDTO RUN ERRANDS KISSED, HUGGED, TICKLED CHILD PLAYED WITH CHILD DID CRAFTS WITH CHILD TAUGHT CHILD SONGS OR MUSIC TAUGHT CHILD LETTERS, WORDS OR NUMBERS TOLD CHILD A STORY 0 Daily or almost daily Of all the shared activities from which we obtained information, two of them–«playing with the child» and «kissing or hugging»– are daily or almost daily activities among practically all the mothers and fathers interviewed. The analysis of differences in frequency of interaction does not yield any significant result (see graph 3.7). The activities of intellectual stimulation (with the exception of crafts) are carried out daily or almost daily by two out Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age categorizing the childcare activities that parents do with their children. Some authors differentiate activities by the purpose of the interaction (educative, recreational, basic physical care, etc.), the priority given to the interaction (primary or secondary); or they distinguish between active care (that involves direct interaction with the child) and passive care (supervision, control). 10 20 30 At least once 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Not once Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Given the importance of shared activities in the development of the child, it is key to trace their differentiated prevalence across family contexts. Our analysis focuses on the influence of two explanatory Diariamente Alguna vez Ninguna v educational resources and who work spend, at the end of the day (excluding weekends), fewer hours with their children than those who do not work , which raises the question of the implications of these patterns for the well-being and development of the child. TABLE 3.5: How often someone living in household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by mother’s education level In percentages. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age MOTHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL DAILY OR ALMOST DAILY OCCASIONALLY NEVER Tells or reads a story Primary 49 42 9.6 Secondary 62 29 8.7 University 72 20 7.5 Primary 59 31 10 Secondary 67 24 9.3 University 66 24 9.2 Primary 58 34 7.9 Secondary 65 32 3.3 University 64 31 5.2 Primary 39 44 16 Secondary 43 41 16 University 46 42 12 Teaches child letters, words or numbers Teaches child songs or music Does crafts with child Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age factors: parents’ educational level and their employment status. As was seen in the previous section, these are factors with a significant impact on the number of hours that fathers and, above all, mothers spend with their children. Parents with greater Families with lower levels of educational resources are more prone to create spaces of social interaction between parent and child which are less intensive. In other words, face to face care coupled with other activities. For example, in these families it is more common to go out with the child to visit family or simply to run errands. In these cases, the interaction with the child is not the primary or exclusive purpose of the activity (Craig, 2006). A particularly controversial debate is the possible effects of women’s paid work on child development. It is often argued that women who have paid work spend less time with their children. In light of the data examined, there is no doubt that those who make this claim are right. However, it seems doubtful that mothers’ paid work reduces the quality of the bond they have with their children. Table 3.6 shows that in households where the mothers work there are the same standards of stimulation as in those where the mothers dedicate themselves exclusively to domestic responsibilities. Perhaps we may detect a negative impact –although slight– in other activities such as visiting family or time shared with children going out to run errands. The intensive care given to children in those households where it occurs tends to be conceived of as a shared activity that both parents are committed to. In almost half of the households, men take co-responsibility for these activities, either because they share equally in them or because they are the ones who take the initiative. Male involvement is especially important in homes where the men have university level studies (see table 3.7). In these households, for example, 43 percent of the fathers share equally in the activity of reading stories to their children, and 16 percent of them take primary responsibility. In homes where the parent has a primary school level of education, the degree of co-responsibility is less: 29 percent of fathers participate equally in this activity, and only 8.9 percent take primary responsibility for this task. A more equal sharing of such activities probably helps increase joint efforts and therefore leads to greater benefits for the child, as the father’s level of commitment rises to the standards of the mother’s, rather than the opposite. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age But the results of our analysis suggest that the time they share with their children is of a better quality. As can be observed in table 3.5, the level of education of the parents is an explanatory factor in their involvement in intellectually stimulating activities. Thus, for example, in two out of three households where the mothers have university studies, one of the parents reads or tells a story every day to the children. This does not happen in half of the households where the mother only has a primary school level education. If we examine other activities, the differences are smaller, but always in the expected direction. TABLE 3.6: How often someone living in household does cognitively stimulating activities and outside activities with the child, In percentages. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age MOTHER’S EMPLOYMENT STATUS DAILY or ALMOST DAILY OCCASIONALLY NEVER Cognitively stimulating activities Tells or reads a story Works 66 27 7.7 Doesn’t work 60 31 9.4 Works 64 26 9.6 Doesn’t work 66 25 9.1 Works 64 31 5.1 Doesn’t work 62 33 4.8 Works 43 44 13 Doesn’t work 44 40 15 Works 57 37 6.0 Doesn’t work 56 39 5.5 Works 41 54 5.6 Doesn’t work 45 51 4.5 Works 49 42 9.6 Doesn’t work 55 36 9.1 Teaches child letters, words or numbers Teaches child songs or music Does crafts with child Activities outside the home Take child to the park Takes child to visit relatives Takes child to run errands Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age by mother’s employment status TABLE 3.7: Men who take primary responsibility for cognitively stimulating activities or take equal responsibility, In percentages. Households with children from 0 to 4 years of age RESPONSIBILITY FOR COGNITIVE ACTIVITIEs FATHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL PRIMARY SECONDARY UNIVERSITY 8.9 13.8 16.1 Both parents equally 29.4 36.9 42.9 Total 38.3 50.7 59 4.1 5.6 6.8 Both parents equally 38.5 48.5 53.8 Total 38.5 48.6 53.9 5.4 5.0 6.3 Both parents equally 33.3 40.9 37.9 Total 33.4 41 38 6.0 7.8 7.8 Both parents equally 30.8 40.1 39 Total 30.9 40.2 39.1 Tells or reads a story Father does it Teaches child letters, words or numbers Father does it Teaches child songs or music Father does it Does crafts with child Father does it Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age by their educational level 3.4. External childcare In Spain, where families traditionally have taken on considerable responsibility in providing well-being, care for the youngest children has been no exception. As Constanza Tobío (2001) reminds us, many families in which both parents work have been able to rely on «substitute mothers» –usually a relative who lives nearby– to balance work and family life. The transfer of responsibilities of childcare to relatives –especially grandmothers– has often been seen as the main resource working mothers count on to take care of their children (see, for example, Moreno, 2002 and Tobío et al., 2010). Our survey has corroborated what has so often been proclaimed: the provision of childcare by available relatives plays an important role in families’ strategies to combine work and family life. But we believe that it would now be an exaggeration to claim, perhaps contrary to what may have happened in the past, that this is the main conciliation strategy for families. The data that have emerged from our analysis suggest that family help is primarily thought of as complementary or for emergencies, something to address momentary problems that arise, but it is not available for the majority of families regularly and systematically. According to the data examined, 55 percent of the families with children from 0 to 2 years of age have relied on the help of someone who does not live in the home to take care of the child within the previous month (table 3.8). Of these, 76 percent were able to count on grandparents, and 17 percent received help from other relatives. A slightly lower percentage (9.2 percent) relied on the help of neighbours or friends, and 7.3 percent hired a babysitter. Overvall only 42 percent of families with children in this age group received help in the previous month from a grandparent to take care of small children. As expected, this help is more common when the mother works and has more intense ties to the labour market. The data in our survey show that in the majority of cases help is occasional. Of those families who receive help, 39 percent say that this help is daily or almost daily, which means 21 percent of all of the families. In other words, the percentage of families who regularly have at their disposition «substitute mothers» is limited. The majority of households must rely on alternative strategies. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age The results suggest that families where the parents work more intensely compensate for the possible lack of time they can spend with their children by increasing quality activities with them. Children in the greatest situation of vulnerability are those who live in homes in which this compensation does not occur. This can happen if the parents’ level of dedication to their jobs is high and/ or they lack the ability to provide this compensatory stimulation. The majority of experts in early childhood education believe that in these situations early schooling is highly recommendable, as it can contribute to reducing cognitive disadvantages before compulsory education begins (Sylva et al., 2010). For this to occur, it is necessary to provide easy and affordable access to schooling to those families who most need it and offer enough quality services to guarantee exposure to such cognitive stimulation to children who may not receive it at home. assistance in the previous month from someone who does not live in the home, by mother’s characteristics In percentages. Households with children from 0 to 2 years of age MOTHER’S SOCIODEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS RECEIVED HELP RECEIVED HELP FROM GRANDPARENTS 18 to 35 years old 57 46 36 to 40 56 39 Over 40 54 35 Primary or no education 46 36 Secondary 54 40 University 60 47 Mother’s age Education level Employment status In recent years, formal childcare services have become more important in the provision of early childhood care. According to data from the Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport (2010), there were 6,947 early childhood education centres registered in Spain in the academic year 2009-2010. Between the academic years 20002001 and 2009-2010, the number of children between 0 and 3 years of age enrolled in centres authorized by the Ministry of Education multiplied by 3.8, reaching 384,000 children. Enrolment rates have increased dramatically in recent years, as can be observed in table 3.9, although there continue to be important variations by region: thus, for example, while more than half of the children in the Basque Country are enrolled in early childhood education centres, only 2.4 percent are in Castilla-La Mancha (Tobío, 2010). However, experts in the area believe that the real percentages are higher than those revealed by official statistics (Balaguer et al., 2004, 2008). TABLE 3.9: Evolution of enrolment rates in early childhood education Percentage of children in age group enrolled in school Works 62 47 AGE Unemployed 45 35 Under 1 1,1 2,5 4,9 5,6 Inactive 42 33 1 year old 5,4 10,1 17,3 19,8 2 years old 13,4 22,1 32,6 35 3 years old 72,5 94,7 96,8 97,5 Working day Full-time 61 48 Part-time 65 47 Total of those who received help 55 42 (338) (258) Number of cases Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 1997-1998 2002-2003 2006-2007 2007-2008 Source: Datos y Cifras 2009-2010, Ministry of Education. The data from the survey offers us a description of the patterns of participation of children from 0-2 years of age in early childhood education centres based on the testimony of their parents. The results Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age TABLE 3.8: Households that have received some kind of child care If we disaggregate the figures by age group, we can see that among families with children between the ages of 0 and 2, 10.3 percent of children under one year old, 46.1 percent of one year olds and 70 percent of two year olds currently attend day-care or pre-school. The survey also asked parents of 5 to 10 year olds if their children had been in day-care or pre-school when they were 0-2 years of age (based on a larger sub-sample of 1,148 cases). Eleven point nine percent of these parents stated that their child was in day-care or preschool when he or she was between 0 and 6 months old, 22.8 percent when the child was 7 months old to one year old, 57.6 percent when the child was one year old and 61 percent when the child was two years old. Even though these figures must be read with caution, as they are based on remembering events from several years before, the differences with official figures are glaring, and they point in the same direction as the results analyzing the current data for children from 0 to 2 years of age. In both cases, the central role of these services in the provision of childcare in early childhood is confirmed. The majority of the parents who place their children in an early childhood education centre or day-care centre count on these services for the 4 The interviewer specified that «regular attendance» means a minimum of once a week during the past month. care of their children for a significant number of hours. 51 percent have their children in these centres for more than five hours a day, and 23 percent for eight hours or more. The probability of children from 0 to 2 years of age attending a preschool is greater in households where the mother works full-time. These centres are a crucial instrument for balancing work and family life in families where both parents have paid employment. But beyond this, other conditions influence the decision to use these centres. The main factor is economic (see graph 3.8). Families in disadvantaged economic sectors are less likely to take their children to day-care or pre-school, all other things being equal. In other words, our analysis (based on logistic regression models that control for the influence of other explanatory factors) indicates that regardless of the degree of availability of the mother to provide childcare, families in more precarious economic situations tend to rely less on formal childcare for children between 0 and 2 years of age. This may be because some of them count on outside help (from relatives, neighbours or friends), which makes it unnecessary to turn to formal childcare services, or it may be because of cultural resistance to leaving the child in the care of strangers. However, the analysis (controlling for possible intervening variables) does not allow us to draw either conclusions. Therefore, the most plausible explanation is that early childhood education services are economically inaccessible for many families with lower incomes, depriving them of a resource which can contribute to resolving their need to balance work and family life and –as indicated in the previous section– benefit the cognitive development of their children. This explanation is consistent with aggregate data published recently in an OECD report on these issues, which indicates that the cost of Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age of the analysis support the idea that under-reporting of attendance is a major problem. According to the data from out survey, based on a subsample of 615 parents of children from 0-2 interviewed during February 2010, 43.6 percent stated that their children were currently regularly attending a day-care centre or school for early childhood education.4 These percentages suggest that a good proportion of the use of these services is submerged, possibly because of the existence of unauthorized private centres or because of the use of «play centres» as spaces which also offer childcare. GRAPH 3.8: Probability of parents placing children in a nursery school, pre-school or day are centre, by household income Households with children from 0 to 2 years of age PROBABILITY 0.70 0.60 An additional issue with implications for the welfare of children is the quality of services offered. There are diverse indicators to measure the quality of day-care centres and pre-schools. The most frequently used indicator is the ratio between the number of childcare professionals and children, which offers a simple indication of the potential level of contact between children and those responsible for their care. The majority of European countries have established regulations which centres must comply with, ranging from five to seven children per childcare professional. In Spain the ratios are usually higher and are established by each autonomous community. The data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, based on the responses from parents whose children are in day-care, indicates that the ratio of seven children per childcare professional is complied with in only about half of the centres. In 17 percent of day-care centres, the ratio is more than 10 children per childcare professional. 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 UP TO €320 PER PERSON BETWEEN €321 AND €500 BETWEEN €501 AND €800 OVER €800 PER PERSON MONTHLY INCOME PER PERSON IN HOUSEHOLD Note: The probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) child currently attends early childhood edication centre and 0) does not attend. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: child’s age, mother’s working day, mother’s education level, level of income per person in the home. Calcat Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. textos picats If we use this ratio as an indicator of quality in pre-schools our analysis does not reveal that children from more disadvantaged families who are able to take advantage of these services are in the centres with the worst ratios. The ratio is primarily associated with the age of the child (as provided for by the corresponding legislation in each autonomous community). Controlling for this effect, we find that children whose families have lower income levels are in schools with a slightly more unfavourable ratio, but the difference is not statistically significant. Responses to the survey suggest that in general parents are satisfied with the pre-schools that they send their children to, without significant differences related to social background. About nine out Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age services could be prohibitively expensive for less affluent families in Spain (OECD, 2007: 151-152). According to the data in this report, in Spain the average monthly cost of childcare for a child 0-2 years of age in an authorized pre-school is equivalent to 30.3 percent of the average salary of a worker. For the complete group of OECD countries studied, the figure is 16.3 percent. In the Nordic countries (Sweden, Finland, Denmark and Norway), the cost of childcare is equivalent to 10 percent of average salary. The data examined demonstrate that the system of provision of childcare to the youngest children is changing rapidly. In recent years formal childcare has played a very important role from very early ages (1 and 2 years of age). We are possibly witnessing the eclipse of a model in which traditional family mechanisms of micro solidarity (having «substitute mothers») were effective. The availability of possible candidates to act as substitute mothers (primarily grandmothers) has been reduced, in part because of the growing participation of women in the workforce, and as a result, their availability to help out has decreased. On the other hand, it is possible that the increase in the prices of housing in recent years has pushed many couples to move away from the areas where their parents and in-laws live, making it difficult to help out with childcare. To all of this must also be added the increasing value broad sectors of the population place on the activities offered to children in pre-school. In this new scenario formal childcare is slowly becoming the primary resource for many families to deal with problems balancing work and family. However, the fact that the solution works for many does not mean that it is for everyone, nor even the great majority. Our analysis shows that families with fewer economic resources have more difficulty in accessing these services. The difficulty of accessibility 5 This information is from the subsample of parents with children from 5 to 10 years of age who had their children in pre-school when they were pre-school age. has dual implications. On the one hand, it means that the problems of reconciliation are concentrated in these families, placing them in nearly impossible situations if they need two incomes (and therefore, to have both parents working) or because the mothers do not want to give up their jobs to take care of their children. On the other hand, it means that pre-school education cannot help alleviate possible deficits of cognitive stimulation in those families where, according to the indications of all of the international studies, such deficits are more common. Not having access to this schooling means not being able to reduce the gaps in capacities and skills presented at a very early age by these children and that will later on affect their academic performance. The children who, could benefit the most from pre-school education are those with the lowest levels of participation. Providing care to children from 0 to 4 years of age of ten parents interviewed who placed their children in a pre-school in recent years stated that they were satisfied with the number of teachers per classroom, their preparation and with the timetable of the centre.5 As was seen in the previous chapter, the time that parents spend with their young children is an asset distributed in an unequal manner. Numerous studies have concluded that the quantity of time parents spend with their children at an early age, and even more so the nature of the interaction, positively affect the wellbeing of the child at later stages of his or her life and can be crucial for the development of personal skills that will facilitate his or her later insertion into the educational system. During the first years of life, parental childcare practices and intergenerational interaction turn out to be crucial for the child’s development, given that the health and well-being of the child (at the stage of consolidating physiological and psychological profiles) and the adequate development of his or her social, cognitive and linguistic skills largely depend on the dedication and skill of the parents (Waldfogel et al., 2003; Ramey and Ramey, 2000; Heckman and Lochner, 2000). In this chapter, we shift our focus to a new stage in children’s lives: the ages between 5 and 10. During this stage the vast majority of children are in school, which means that many studies have focused on what happens within the school context. This is not the focus of the current chapter. We are convinced that the im- portance of the intergenerational bond in early childhood does not outweigh its importance at later stages. 4.1. Parental influence on childhood Attention and care are crucial in early childhool, but their benefits are not limited to just these early years. There is abundant research to suggesting, that the disruption of life with a parent due to divorce or the death of a parent can lead to deficits in «the social capital of the family» (that is, reductions in the quantity and quality of time a child interacts with a parent), negatively impacting the child’s path of personal and educational development, which, prior to that may have been more balanced. In the same way, homes in which frequent and deep disagreements arise over the education of the children do not provide a climate conducive for their well-being. The primary mechanism often invoked to explain these adverse developments is the weakening of the «social control» that parents exercise over their children. Social control theories propose that parental monitoring and supervision of children’s behaviour and the exercise of authority for educational purposes Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds IV. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds esteem, self-control, less likelihood of developing behavioural problems or participating in risky behaviours) than those who grow up in authoritarian homes (which subject children to a high degree of supervision and control while not looking after or ignoring other aspects), permissive homes (with high degrees of affection and dialogue but little control), or negligent homes (where control and parental involvement are lacking). In other words, parental control is beneficial for the child when it is not overwhelming and is carried out in conjunction with dialogue and affection between parents and their children. However, most specialized research coincides in pointing out that, without denying the need for social control for the adequate psycho-social development of children, some forms of control can have negative consequences. There is academic consensus that the recurring use of coercion by the parents through physical punishment or psychological manipulation is damaging to the child and may have consequences in later stages of development. The exercise of parental control tends to generate anti-bodies if unconditional obedience is expected, if it is not accompanied by efforts of persuasion and dialogue, or if it is exercised in a family climate lacking warmth and affection toward the child. Since the beginning of the 1970s, a significant number of studies have consistently shown that socalled authoritative parenting styles, which combine a higher degree of parental involvement in the life of children with significant levels of control (although not excessively high) have beneficial effects on the psycho-social development of children (Baumrind, 1970; Steinberg, 2001). Children who are raised in authoritative family atmospheres have more positive values for a wide range of indicators (such as basic competencies, self- Beyond parenting styles, there are other important factors in family contexts. Certain socioeconomic characteristics of the parents, such as their educational level, have an important influence on the educational path of the children. It has been demonstrated that the level of education of the mother is the main explanatory variable for the academic performance of children as well as for the likelihood of the child continuing to study after finishing compulsory education (Marí-Klose et al., 2009; Fernández Enguita, 2010). The mechanisms for the transmission of these advantages are increasingly known. Some families have greater capacity than others to provide resources that facilitate educational success, but not all resources have the same value. Economic resources, for example, have limited importance. More affluent families can make investments in their children’s education that are not possible for disadvantaged families. But this investment explains only a small part of the variability in educational outcomes. The evidence is quite conclusive that the most decisive resources to explain educational achievement are related to the more intangible qualities of families, often described as their cultural capital (DiMaggio and Mohr, 1982; Esping Andersen, 2009). Cultural capital includes knowledge Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds are fundamental for the successful socialization of the child. They curb the indiscrimate needs and desires of the infantile ego, favour the internalization of rules and expectations and help to separate children from harmful influences (see formulations of this theory in Hetherington, 1979; McLanahan and Bumpass, 1988). From this point of view, the «parenting from a distance» by the parent who does not live with the child, or conflicts caused by differences between parents about how to raise the child can erode the bases of parental authority and hinder the exercise of adequate levels of supervision and control. 4.2. Joint activities It is a truism that parents seek their children’s well-being. It is widely agreed that parents have to spend time with their children to achieve this goal, but they find it very difficult to do so. Frequently we hear parents complain that their obligations keep them from fulfilling their desire to spend more time with their children. Some fathers and many mothers end up having to adapt their work to family life. From many different institutions, work schedules that are compatible with family needs are being demanded, and governments are taking action on the matter by adopting more or less ambitious measures in order to facilitate balancing work and family. However, when asked directly, only a minority of parents concede that the amount of time they spend with their children is not enough. In our Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, 17 percent of mothers of children between the ages of 5 and 10 and 35 percent of the fathers considered the amount of time to be insufficient. The emergence and growth of concern about the amount of time parents dedicate to children is in part related to the drastic increase of women in the workforce and to mothers continuing to work after giving birth. This trend, accompanied by the growing residential autonomy of seniors, has deprived families of their primary source of childcare in an (extended) familybased system–that is, one based on mothers and grandmothers providing childcare–without, apparently, having anyone to substitute for them. The fear of the void these women have left in the home has combined with and been reinforced by other contemporary fears: childhood obesity (caused, according to some discourses, by food not being prepared at home or parents’ excessive tolerance of their children’s dietary whims), the concern caused by discipline problems at an early age (whose source is seen to be the erosion of authority in the home) and school failure (in the view of many, rooted in the parents’ lack of involvement in the formal education of their children). However, as we pointed out in the previous chapter, the reality is somewhat more complex than suggested by these discourses. The socioeconomic processes that can have negative repercussions on parents being able to dedicate time to their children are offset by other processes– which usually do not receive as much attention– that counter the more harmful effects of the former. This second package of social processes includes sociodemographic tendencies Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds and competencies in matters that the school converts into learning objectives. In turn, this allows parents to offer their children continuous support–but also, a capacity to transmit skills, habits and evaluation criteria that increase the productivity of their children in school. Families with greater cultural capital are more able to interact with their children through intellectually stimulating activities, reading to them, talking to them about a wide range of issues, or guiding them more effectively in order to develop their creativity (with educational games, teaching them to draw, doing crafts, etc.). Children who belong to these families internalize values and work habits that are valued by their teachers, and they better understand what the school expects from them at every step (Farkas et al., 1990). Learning the value of education at home and recognizing prestigious aesthetic and artistic experiences (such as reading, visiting museums or playing an instrument) facilitate their adaptation to the school culture, where these attitudes and predispositions are rewarded (Lareau, 2000). (a) Education Studies published in the United States have demonstrated that parents with higher levels of education tend to sacrifice personal time in order to spend more time with their children (in spite of greater time spent in paid jobs) and to invest this time in activities that are more enriching for the children than parents with only primary school education. Thanks to the extraordinary increase in the educational level of the population that took place in the second half of the twentieth century, in aggregate terms there has hardly been a change in levels of parental involvement in providing care to their children (Sayer et al., 2004). There is no evidence suggesting that this pattern cannot be extrapolated to our country, although we do not have longitudinal data to confirm it. The educational expansion in Spain in recent decades has been extraordinary, especially among women. Thus, while in the cohort born in the post-war period (1941-1950), who had their children approximately 25-35 years later (around the time of the transition to democracy), 13 percent of the women graduated from high school, and 10 percent had university degrees, while in the cohort born thirty years later (1971-1980), 71 percent of women graduated from high school, and 45 percent had university degrees (Marí-Klose et al., 2009: 193). The children of this new cohort are growing up in completely different family settings: a significant proportion of mothers and fathers are clearly aware of the benefits of investing time in their children. Data from our survey reveal results that point in this direction. The additional years mothers spend in the educational system moderates the effect working outside the home has on the amount of time dedicated to their children. The following tables contrast the degree of parental involvement in children’s lives based on a number of indicators among three different groups defined by educational level. First of all, we map the participation of all the family members in a daily activity, such as meals, which gives children and parents the opportunity to communicate and exchange experiences throughout the day in a structured way that favours face-to-face intergenerational interaction. Secondly, we analyze two activities that involve intense interaction within the home and two activities of lower intensity carried out outside the home. Finally, we examine the subjective perceptions of mothers and fathers with respect to their own degree of involvement and that of their partner (understanding degree of involvement as the time that each one spends with the child, cares for him or her, takes care of his/her needs and pays attention to him or her). At first glance, table 4.1 suggests that in the households where the mothers have a higher educational level, the children do not have more opportunities to spend time with their parents at meal times, but the data merit a closer look. A higher proportion of children with university educated mothers have breakfast with their parents every day or almost every day, but the percentage of those who never do is also slightly higher. A significantly Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds (or «compositional» changes) and cultural changes (which affect the behaviour of parents). The first of these that must be pointed out is the overall increase in parents’ educational levels. Regarding cultural changes, it is necessary to mention, on the one hand, the consolidation of new models of parenting, and on the other, new «processes of choosing» fatherhood/motherhood that tend to separate out from this experience those individuals who are less willing to make sacrifices for a child. In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age MOTHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL DAILY OR OCCASIONALLY ALMOST DAILY NEVER Total 100 odss RATIO(a) ODDS adjusted RATIO(b) (312) 1 1 NUMBER OF CASES Breakfast Primary 20 71 8.9 Secondary 24 64 11 (472) 1.25 1.29 University 31 55 14 (363) 1.41* 1.62* Primary 48 48 3.4 Secondary 40 57 2.4 0.71* 0.59** University 24 70 5.6 0.36*** 0.89 Primary 84 15 1.3 Secondary 81 18 University 82 15 Lunch 100 1 1 Dinner 100 1 1 1.5 0.81 0.86 2.8 0.76 0.78 * Significance level of 5%. ** Significance level of 1%. *** Significance level of 1‰. Note: a) logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) eating breakfast,lunch and dinner together daily and 0) not doing so daily. b) In the adjusted model, in addition to controlling for the mother’s education level, the mother’s employment status is also controlled for. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. lower percentage of children of mothers with university degrees have lunch with their parents every day or almost every day, and the same percentage has dinner with their parents. These figures can be explained in part by the greater dedication to paid work among mothers with a higher level of education (and possibly of their partners as well). Thus, to measure their effort it is necessary to determine the probability adjusted to their work circumstances and that of their partners (in other Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds TABLE 4.1: Frequency of eating breakfast, lunch and dinner together, by mother’s education level the workforce, no statistically significant differences are then observed with the households where the mother has a primary school education. The probability of having dinner together is the same for both groups. Table 4.2 provides information on activities that involve a higher degree of cognitive stimulation and individualized interaction. Results suggest that there are significant differences in the number of parents who read stories to their children daily or TABLE 4.2: How often someone living in the household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by mother’s education level In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age MOTHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL DAILY OR ALMOST DAILY OCCASIONALLY NEVER Total 100 ODDs RATIO ODDS ajusted RATIO Tells or reads a story Primary 50 30 19 1 1 Secondary 58 25 17 1.35* 1.45* University 64 20 15 1.79*** 1.96*** Primary 32 54 14 1 1 Secondary 31 57 12 0.94 1.15 University 33 54 13 1.03 1.35 Does crafts 100 * Significance level of 5%. ** Significance level of 1%. *** Significance level of 1‰. Note: a) logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) doing cognitively stimulating activities daily or almost daily and 0) not doing them daily. b) In the adjusted model, in addition to controlling for the mother’s education level, the mother’s employment status is also controlled for. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds words, all other things being equal). The last column in the table indicates the odds ratio of being together every day or almost every day (versus not being together) when, in a multivariable model, we control for the time dedicated to employment. The adjusted results confirm that in households where the mother has a university degree the adjusted probability of having breakfast together is higher. The probability of having lunch together is lower, but once we control for the effects that must be attributed to mother’s and their partner’s participation in The following table shows the involvement of mothers and fathers in joint outings with their children which involve parental supervision, but that are not necessarily activities involving cognitive stimulation. The results suggest that these activities (especially visiting relatives) are more common among families with lower educational levels. The multivariable analysis demonstrates that these differences persist basically intact when we control for levels of participation of parents in the workforce. TABLE 4.3: How often someone living in the household does outdoor activities with the child, by mother’s education level In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age MOTHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL DAILY OR ALMOST DAILY OCCASIONALLY Primary 40 56 3.5 Secondary 30 64 5.3 0.65** 0.67* University 27 67 6.6 0.55*** 0.58** Primary 50 42 7.1 1 1 Secondary 46 47 6.4 0.84 0.92 University 44 49 6.9 0.78 0.92 NEVER Total ODDS RATIO ODDS ADJUSTED RATIO Takes child to visit relatives 100 1 1 Takes child for a walk or to the park 100 * Significance level of 5%. ** Significance level of 1%. *** Significance level of 1‰. Note: a) Logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) Doing cognitively stimulating activities daily or almost daily and 0) not doing them daily. b) In the adjusted model, in addition to controlling for the mother’s education level, the mother’s employment status is also included in the model. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds almost daily based on the parents’ level of education, but no differences are observed in the number that do crafts with their children. Multivariable analyses confirm that, with employment conditions being equal, the mother’s level of education increases the relative probability that stories are read daily or almost daily in the home, but the adjustment does not translate into significant statistical differences in the probability of doing crafts (although this increases slightly). (b) New models of fatherhood In recent years, diverse studies have confirmed that although women continue to assume most of the domestic responsibilities, in a growing number of households they are almost equally distributed. The roots of this equalizing process are difficult to trace. The first sign of changes ahead is found in the recent change in attitudes of men toward the division of gender roles in the family. The magnitude of the changes is obvious in table 4.4, which gathers an historic series of data on the same indicator from different studies carried out by the Centre for Sociological Research (CIS). In 1990, 42 percent of men in Spain opted for a symmetrical model for the division of gender roles, whereas in 2004 the percentage had gone up to 66 percent. In less than two decades, the equitable model had clearly been imposed as the ideal over the traditional model. Needless to say, discourse is usually ahead of reality. The change in men’s attitudes has created divisions in many households between their orientations and their effective participation in domestic responsibilities. The family in which the man does not do any domestic chores is now unusual. According to a recent study on young couples (Iglesias de Ussel et al, 2009:177), only in 20 percent of the households do men spend less than a fifth of the time their partners do on domestic tasks. But in the majority of cases important inequalities persist in the division of domestic responsibilities, and this is often the case even in the households where the man claims to believe in equal roles. Changes have come slowly and unequally, but there is no doubt that they have taken place in many households and deserve a closer examination. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds The most significant differences appear when subjective perceptions are analyzed. Women with higher educational levels are more likely to respond that they are very involved in their children’s lives. They are also more likely to see their partners as «very involved.» This perception does not seem particularly justified based on the amount of time and activities they share with their children in comparison to the groups with lower levels of education, as the results suggest that there are no marked differences. One possible explanation is that the high assessment of their involvement in their children’s lives reflects in part the effort required to reconcile this involvement with their work. Maintaining the standards of involvement socially expected from a «good mother» demands extra effort on the part of mothers who work, mothers who perhaps have more ambitious professional goals than those with lower levels of education and less attractive employment prospects. The adjusted probabilities examined point in this direction. When we control for the degree of parent’s participation in the labour market, the probability that they will have breakfast with their children, read them stories and do crafts with them daily or almost daily is greater; the probability that they will have lunch or dinner with their children or that they will go to the park with them is the same. Only the probability that they will visit relatives is lower. An alternative possibility is that mothers evaluate in an especially positive way the quality of this involvement because they understand that through their effort, they will be able to transmit advantages (cultural capital) to their children. In this case, the «degree of involvement» would have less to do with the quantity of time invested than with an evaluation of its quality. for division of responsibilities by gender in the family In percentages MODEL OF IDEAL FAMILY(a) 1990 1994 2004 2010(b) Single breadwinner model 29 25 17 13 Unbalanced two breadwinner model 24 21 14 15 Balanced model 42 50 66 69 Don’t know/No answer 5.0 4.0 3.0 1.7 Other Total Number of cases 1.2 100 (1,260) (1,184) (1,203) (1,223) Note: a) The question asked was: «Nowadays there are different kinds of families. Of the three possibilities, could you tell me which is closest to your ideal of the family?» The three types of families considered in the question correspond to the following categories: – Single breadwinner model: a family in which only the man works outside the home and only the woman takes care of domestic tasks and cares for the child/children. – Unbalanced model with two breadwinners: a family in which the woman works fewer hours outside the home and therefore, has more domestic and child care responsiblities. – Balanced model: a family in which both the man and the woman work fewer hours outside the home and divide the domestic responsiblities and child care. b) In the 2010 survey the possible answers were formulated in a slightly different way without specifying the sex of the couple who occupied the role in the different models. It also included an additional possible response: «None of these types of families.» Source: Data from the Center for Sociological Research, from 1990, Study 1,867; from 1994, Study 2,107; from 2004, Study 2,556 and from 2010, Study 2,831. In this sense, it is necessary to point out that, based on diverse quantitative and qualitative studies published in Spain, the involvement of the father in caring for the children seems to be significantly greater than in other areas of domestic responsibilities (Brullet and Roca, 2008; Iglesias de Ussel, et al., 2009). Some research has ventured to highlight the appearance of men who fully share responsibilities in caring for the children with their partners. In this vein it is worth looking at the study of Inés Alberdi and Pilar Escario (2007). In an excellent qualitative study on new attitudes and identities among young middle class fathers (elaborated based on eleven focus groups), the authors described three profiles of fathers committed to the care of their children: 1) the intense father, fully dedicated to the care of his children, equally or more so than the mother; 2) the responsible father, who wants to share equally with the mother the responsibilities for and care of the children, and 3) the adaptive or complementary father, who rejects the traditional model and supports the mother from the outside in everything she needs, but believes that men cannot replace the preeminent role of the mother in the relationship with the children. It is not possible to create a typology like that of Alberdi and Escario from the indicators in our survey, and our approach in no way captures the wealth of nuances Alberdi and Escario proposed. A similar intention is far from our reach. But we believe that the indicators of our survey provide an approximate idea of the numerical importance of the phenomena of new fatherhood. The following classification is based on the responses of mothers with children from 5 to 10 years of age to four questions: 1) What is your level of involvement in your child’s life?; 2) And the level of involvement of the father?; 3) Do you consider the amount of time you spend with your child to be more than enough, enough or not enough?; 4) And the amount of time the father spends with the child? The classification is based on the opinions of the mothers who responded to the questionnaire both for sampling Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds TABLE 4.4: Evolution in male opinions about the ideal model To create typologies we take into account the absolute position of the father (in other words, his degree of involvement and participation in terms of time in caring for his child) as well as his relative position (in relation to his partner). In our classification, if the mother responded that the father «is very involved» and spends «more than enough» time with the child, this is categorized as «intense fatherhood.» If the mother states that her partner is a «very involved» father who dedicates «enough time» to the child, we also categorize this as intense fatherhood, as long as the mother is not «very involved» and does not dedicate «more than enough» time to the child (and is, therefore, more involved in caring for the child than the father). In such a case, we identify this as «responsible fatherhood.» «Responsible fatherhood» also includes fathers who are «quite involved» and who spend enough time with their children. Fathers who are «quite involved» but who do not spend enough time with their children and whose partners are primarily responsible for them, are categorized as «adaptive.» We have created two additional categories in order to cover the full range of possible situations. «Pre-disposed» fatherhood refers to those fathers who are judged to be «very involved» by the mother but who do not spend enough time 1 Thus, 51 percent of the men considered themselves to be «very involved» in the life of their child, but only 41 percent of the women categorized their partners’ level of involvement in this way. However, the men were more likely to evaluate the time they spent with their children as not enough. 35 percent responded in this way. Only 17 percent of the women saw it in this way. with their children, presumably because of things outside of their control. «Traditional» fatherhood includes all those cases of little or no involvement by the father and in which the mothers are «very» or «quite» involved. Finally, there is also another category which includes all of the cases in which both the mother and father present low levels of involvement and dedication of time: these fathers we have designated as «uncommitted.» The percentage distribution of fathers is the following: GRAPH 4.1: Types of fatherhood In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age Adaptive Traditional 19.8 12.8 Uncommitted Predisposed Intense 10.2 6.7 6.5 Responsible 43.9 Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The results show that around half of the fathers fit into the categories of intense (6.7 percent) or responsible (43.7 percent) fatherhood. The rest are distributed among the other modalities. In 19.8 percent of the homes, the mothers carry the weight of the responsibility for raising the children though with some help from the father (adaptive fatherhood). In 12.8 percent of homes, the division of responsibilities is traditional: male support is scarce or does not exist. In 10.2 percent there is a predisposed fatherhood, Adaptativa Predispuesta Responsable Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds reasons (they constitute a sufficient number) and because of our understanding that men’s answers could be influenced by a tendency towards providing socially desirable responses. The answers of the men tended to be more indulgent with their level of involvement in the life of their child.1 Tables 4.5 and 4.6 give us an idea of the impact of the transition from the family with a traditional division of domestic responsibilities to other family types in which intense, responsible and predisposed fatherhood models have become more common. In homes with these kinds of fathers, the children enjoy TABLE 4.5: How often someone living in the household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by types of fatherhood In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age TYPES OF FATHERHOOD DAILY OR ALMOST DAILY OCCASIONALLY NEVER Total NUMBER OF CASES 100 (139) Reads or tells a story Intense 63 19 17 Responsible 62 22 16 (474) Predisposed 64 23 14 (136) Adaptive 54 22 24 (157) Traditional 57 28 15 (108) Uncommitted 47 29 24 (73) Intense 38 50 12 Responsible 27 59 14 Predisposed 38 56 6,3 Adaptive 32 50 18 Traditional 34 54 12 Uncommitted 27 53 20 Does crafts Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 100 Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds formed by fathers whose partners describe them as «very involved» but who do not dedicate enough time to their children. In 6.4 percent of households, the mothers describe their partners as not very involved and not spending much time with their children (uncommitted), but they see themselves in the same way. TABLE 4.6: How often someone living in the household does outside activities with the child, by types of fatherhood In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age TYPES OF FATHERHOOD DAILY OR OCCASIONALLY NEVER ALMOST DAILY Takes child to visit relatives Intense 40 56 3.8 Responsible 34 61 5.0 Predisposed 41 53 6.3 Adaptive 28 66 5.2 Traditional 35 58 7.0 Uncommitted 39 55 5.9 Intense 56 37 7.7 Responsible 48 44 8.2 Predisposed 50 45 5.0 Adaptive 45 48 7.7 Traditional 45 48 7.0 Uncommitted 37 55 7.8 Takes child for a walk or to the park Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The analysis presented in this section questions the assumption that the weakening of the traditional family threatens intergenerational interactions between parents and children. The fear of the void left in the home when mothers work (or when grandmothers do not live near their daughters) has fed exaggerated concerns. Certainly, the increase in mothers working outside the home represents a challenge to maintain standards for care and attention provided to children. But the data suggest that the majority of families facing this situation resolve the difficulties that arise without the time and opportunities for intergenerational interaction being affected. It is an error to assume that more hours spent by parents working means less of an investment in providing care to their children. In recent years historic changes of great significance have taken place which have led parents to strengthen their commitment to their children, even in families in which the time pressures are great. Thus, the increase in the educational levels of the parents and the consolidation of new models of being a «good father» are opening new outlooks for building intergenerational bonds in the home. We are seeing the formation of a new logic regarding parental responsibility, more equitable and informed than that which sustained past models, which is enough (and even more than enough) to prevent the erosion of intergenerational bonds which could otherwise be caused by the increased participation of women in the workforce. 4.3. Managing the expression of affection and disapproval In modern societies, the family and especially the intergenerational bond, is associated with love and tenderness. To get to this point Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds the same or greater opportunities for intergenerational interactions thanks to both parents’ commitment and dedication of time, which compensates for the possible deficits created by their greater involvement in their professions. differences by sex: 31 percent of the women said they did this more often; 67 percent said this was done equally by both parents and only 2.9 percent said that their partners did this more often. In contrast, when the men were asked, the response was slightly different: 8.3 percent recognized that their partner expressed affection more often; 82 percent stated that it was equal and 9.3 percent believed that they expressed affection more often. TABLE 4.7: Who cuddles with the child (hugs and kisses) more often, by types of fatherhood In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age In our society the norm that requires mothers and fathers to shower their children with expressions of tenderness is very strong. This figures in the forefront of any prescription for good parenting. It is a norm that is beyond question and to do so invites suspicion. Perhaps because of this, when questioned about how often someone in the home «has kissed, hugged, or tickled» the child, 97 percent of the mothers and fathers interviewed in the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood said that this happened daily or almost daily, without our being able to differentiate the responses of men and women. This is an example of a «desirable» answer, which almost certainly does not reflect the reality for many parents. However, when asked who lavished this kind of attention most often, there emerged clear 2 In fact, the social historian Edward Shorter (1977) is the author of a classic book that suggests that before the 19th century in Europe, the treatment of children by their mothers was usually rough and insensitive. Far from the maternal instincts that children awaken in mothers today, the women of the 17th and 18th centuries showed little interest in the welfare of their children, did not attend to their cries, bound them from their feet to their shoulders so they could not move, and when possible, they would send them away for long periods of time to wet nurses who would raise them with their own children. The experience of motherhood did not represent something special, which merited greater dedication and effort in the lives of those mothers. TYPES OF FATHERHOOD FATHER MOTHER BOTH EQUALLY Intense 7.9 11 81 Responsible 4.0 16 80 Predisposed 2.5 31 67 Adaptive 1.9 64 34 Traditional 6.6 19 74 Uncommitted 9.7 19 71 Total 4.8 23 72 Note: The responses are from the mothers. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Despite the differences which appear in the survey, the results are clear that in the majority of households the man participates actively in the framework of affection and emotions that is woven around the child. This emotional involvement is found across all social Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds the evolution of attitudes and practices have been extraordinary. As the historian Philippe Ariès pointed out in his classic work Centuries of Childhood (1960), for many centuries children were not the object of any special sort of sentimental treatment. Far from what one might imagine, the expression of affection toward children–kisses, hugs, caresses, etc.–was not a universal practice or a core assumption of the successful socialization of any civilization. Maternal and paternal love, as understood today, was an unknown sentiment in the majority of human societies in the past.2 It is a social construction that nowadays has acquired a social importance that it did not have in our own country just a few decades ago. Expressions of tenderness are only one side of the emotional dynamics between fathers and children. Educating the child demands placing limits, controlling their indiscriminate needs and demands and channelling their behaviour in a manner that is conducive to a positive family atmosphere and the child’s learning. Instilling habits and values is not an easy task. It requires dedication and consistency. There are no recipes that always work, but there are common experiences that most parents go through sooner or later. Socialization strategies among parents include communication as well as rewards and punishment. Differences lie in the importance that is given to each of these ingredients. Not long ago in Spain, parental socialization in traditional homes (which constituted the majority) was based on authoritarianism and a division of roles in the provision of reward and punishment. The father imposed the rules hardly providing explanations or taking into account the child’s arguments or ideas. When the rules were broken, fathers restored balance through reproach or punishment, including corporal punishment. The mother’s role was more ambiguous and limited. Usually without questioning the father’s authority, she often specialized in giving positive reinforcement to the child which at times contributed to containing the more despotic forms of authority exercised by the father. Times have changed, and with them, the attitudes of Spaniards toward dialogue with their children and the administration of punishment. We live in a democratic society where dialogue and negotiation are highly appreciated values. Dialogue is especially important in spaces where an authoritarian logic previously prevailed, such as in intergenerational relationships. The vast majority of people consider dialogue to be a necessary and effective instrument to raise their children. According to the work of Iglesias de Ussel et al. (2009:90), already introduced elsewhere in this volume, 82 percent of young adults who live with a partner agree or totally agree that «if you explain things, all children will understand their parents’ reasoning.» In this same study, the authors found that only 5 percent agreed with the old adage, «Spare the rod, spoil the child.» However, there is also a clear awareness of the importance of discipline. In the study, 80 percent of young people living with a partner agreed with the idea that discipline is the key to success in education. Asked to rate the importance of discipline as a quality that parents try to inculcate in their children, the Spanish gave it, on average, an 8.6 (on a scale of 1 to 10). It was rated slightly lower by younger people: those 35 and under rated it at 8.3, while those 65 and over rated it 9.2. Even so, the rating remains Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds groups (defined by educational level, income and age of the parents), but not among all styles of fatherhood. In this area, our analysis found great differences in the role of fathers in these spaces of intimacy. Fathers who practice new models of parenting (intense or responsible) have commitments similar to those of their partners. As Alberdi and Escario also point out in their study, these new fathers harbour the desire to overcome the old discourses and habits that associated fatherhood with the exercise of power and a certain emotional distance from the children. They have opted instead for a deepening of emotional relationships through an initial physical communication with their child (through hugging, kissing, caressing) and the hope of maintaining an emotional and intellectual closeness later on in subsequent stages of the child’s development. The data show the limited role of the traditional father in these spaces. In households where fatherhood follows traditional forms, the mother is clearly the primary provider of affection. TABLE 4.8: Opinions on the best method to raise children, by age groups In percentages AGE FROM 18 FROM 35 FROM 50 OVER TO 34 TO 49 TO 64 65 It is better to reward good behaviour than to spank Tend to agree 83 86 80 77 Tend to disagree 9.7 5.9 9.5 11 Neither agree or disagree You have to teach children to obey from an early age, even if it means punishing them Tend to agree 7.0 8.4 11 12 58 58 64 69 Tend to disagree 31 34 24 20 Neither agree or disagree 11 7.5 12 12 Tend to agree 49 57 71 77 Tend to disagree 41 35 22 18 Neither agree or disagree 9.4 8.5 7.0 4.6 A spanking at the right moment can avoid bigger problems Source: Based on data from Study 2,621, CIS 2004. 3 Spaniards gave it greater importance than other qualities. For example, sensitivity received an average rating of 8.3; simplicity, 8.2; a sense of thrift, 8; imagination, 7.9; independence, 7.7; competitiveness, 7.1; leadership, 6.5 and religiousness, 5.9. Evidently, the distribution of values is not the same with all of the variables. The most legitimate manner to achieve this objective is to reward appropriate behaviour. 82 percent of the Spanish under 50 years of age agree that «it is better to reward appropriate behaviour than to spank» (CIS Study 2,621, 2005). But this does not mean completely giving up the use of punishment. The majority of young adults in Spain (around 58 percent) believe that it is necessary to teach children to obey, even if it is by punishing. Around half of parents accept the use of mild forms of physical wpunishment (spanking). Table 4.9 presents a picture of the use of reward and punishment to deal with the behaviour of their 5 to 10 year olds. In the survey there is a battery of questions asking parents about their actions during the week prior to the interview. The answers reveal a range of considerable activity. The vast majority of families employed various actions directed toward getting the child to conform to parental expectations. The strategy utilized most frequently was positive reinforcement: congratulating the child for doing a good job. This, as we have seen, is considered the most legitimate strategy to discipline, and therefore, there is the risk of answers being influenced by a tendency to provide socially desirable responses. It is also necessary to highlight the high number of parents who ask their children to think about their behaviour, which indicates a high propensity to trust in the child’s capacity to reason in response to an explanation. Young parents understand that their child must be treated as an active subject who can respond constructively to parental disapproval. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds consistently high.3 Discipline does not have to mean unconditional obedience. The majority of parents want to build relationships with their children based on affection and dialogue, but in which the children follow codes of acceptable behaviour. In percentages. Household with children from 5 to 10 years of age FREQUENCY DAILY almost daily OCCASIONALLY NEVER Congratulate child on doing things well 43 37 20 0.5 Give child time to reflect on what he/she has done 26 20 49 4.0 Raise voice or shout at child 10 17 64 8.4 Threaten to punish child 10 13 62 15 Punish child (has to stay in his/her room, not allowed to watch TV,use computer or play videogames) 2.4 4.4 73 20 Spank child 0.2 0.1 36 64 Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The least used resource for disciplining is «spanking.» Even so, 36 percent of parents recognized that they had done this in the previous week. This is a high figure, but it is plausible. This rate is not far from results obtained in previous surveys in Spain, as well as in other developed societies (e.g. Save the Children, 2005). But in light of the plurality of attitudes of Spaniard families towards punishment, these figures corroborate the general belief that such practice is not widespread. The Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood provides a unique opportunity to determine the social place of physical punishment based on a representative sample of Spanish families with children between 5 and 10 years of age. A number of nationally representative studies–fundamentally in Anglo-Saxon countries–that have analyzed the use of corporal punishment by parents have come to a series of common conclusions. The first is that the use of corporal punishment tends to be more common when it occurs under the umbrella of social norms and values that view it as legitimate and effective. Since 2007, in Spain these norms and values have lost legal protection. Corporal punishment in the home was prohibited by an amendment to the Civil Code in 2007. In its article 154, the Code had previously recognized the «right» of parents and guardians to utilize «reasonable and moderate» forms of «correction.» The data presented in tables 4.8 and 4.9 suggest that the legal reform reflects only partially what has occurred in the terrain of attitudes and practices of the Spanish population. Corporal punishment in its more «reasonable and moderate» versions continues to figure in the catalogue of admissible strategies to socialize children, although its legitimate use is reserved only Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds TABLE 4.9: How often parents used reward and punishment in the previous week A second set of possible factors determining the use of corporal punishment, according to the literature, are certain parental profiles. Researchers on these issues have not found a strong relationship between the educational level or the economic situation of the parents and the use of corporal punishment, except in situations of extreme precariousness (Dietz, 2000; Giles-Sims et al., 1995). Instead, the research demonstrates the importance of parenting styles (Simons et al., 1994; Socolar and Stein, 1995). In these studies, corporal punishment is more common among families that neglect other dimensions in the child’s education and development, all other things being equal. For example, Grogan, Kaylor and Otis (2007) found a negative relationship (statistically significant) between activities of cognitive stimulation and resorting to corporal punishment. A third group of factors includes the behaviours and characteristics of the child. Obviously, corporal punishment usually arises in response to the child’s behaviour. It is not surprising that the statistical models tend to show that children who, in the eyes of the parents, are frequently conflictive or simply hard to control are more likely to face corporal punishment, while those who are more passive are less so (Grogan-Kaylor and Otis, 2007). Age is also an important determinant. The probability of resorting to corporal punishment decreases as the child gets older. Sex also has an influence on this likelihood. Some research leads us to believe that parents tend to punish sons more than daughters (Giles-Sims et al., 1995), although other research does not reveal significant differences) (Grogan-Kaylor and Otis, 2007). We have tested these hypotheses analyzed in these specialized Anglo-Saxon studies, using a sample of parents interviewed for the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood. The analysis, based on the construction of logistic regression models with categorical dependent variables, estimates the TABLE 4.10: Factors related to a child being spanked in the previous week Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age Logistic regression INDEPENDENT VARIABLES CHILD being spanked(a) Characteristics of child Sex 0 Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds for parents. The administration of any form of physical abuse, no matter how mild, by other adults responsible for children–in institutions such as schools, boarding schools or civic centres–is seen today as aberrant and unacceptable. (Continue) 5 years old + 6 years old + 7 years old + 8 years old + 9 years old 0 Psychological characteristics Conflictive(b) + Low state of mood(c) 0 Parents’ Characteristics Mother’s education level (reference primary) Secondary 0 University 0 Mother’s occupational status (reference does not work) Works full-time – Works part-time 0 Immigrant origin – Father’s level of involvement (reference high level of involvement) Medium involvement 0 Low involvement 0 Father’s age – Household Characteristics Child has siblings + Household income per person (reference 1st quartile)(d) 2nd quartile 0 Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds Age (reference 10 years old) 3rd quartile + 4th quartile + Characteristics of inter and intra-generational relationships There is tension in the home(e) Related to care of the child + Related to lack of time to relax + Degree of cognitive stimulation of the child(f) – Notes: a) The signs +/- represent the direction of the influence of the factors on the dependent variable in a logistic regression model that includes them simultaneously. The ‘0’ indicates that the coefficcient is not statistically significant with a confidence level of 95 %. The number of cases that the model includes is N=1,021. b) An index was created to measure the conflict level of the children based on a series of questions parents were asked about their behaviour. The index adds up those cases in which the parents agreed or strongly agreed with the following statements about their children: he/she gets into conflicts or fights; he/she likes to bother others; he/she loses control easily; he/she can’t stay still. c) An index was created to measure mood based on a series of questions parents were asked about their behaviour. The index adds up those cases in which the parents agreed or strongly agreed with the following statements about their children: he/she is sometimes sad; he/she sometimes feels lonely; she/he is shy; she/he is sometimes afraid of things or people. d) Household income is divided into 4 quartiles. The 1st corresponds to income per person up to 320 euros; the 2nd from 321 to 500 euros; the 3rd from 501 to 800 euros and the 4th, over 800 euros. e) The cases in which there is often or there is sometimes tension in the home have been combined. f) The degree of congnitive stimulation is an additive index with values between 0 and 4 that shows the frequency with which parents read stories to their children or do crafts with them. The response categories are: daily or almost daily; occasionally or never. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. influence of a series of explanatory factors in the variable of interest (resorting to corporal punishment within the past week), all other things being equal. That is to say, we use a model which makes it possible to isolate the statistical effect of each factor on the explanatory variable, filtering out the relationship it may have with third variables included in the statistical model. The main results are presented in table 4.10. The analysis confirms the weak and insignificant relationship between the education of the parents and the use of punishment among Spanish families. The importance of the child’s age is also corroborated. The risk of experiencing punishment follows a clear descending path as the child gets older. We also detected a strong relationship between behavioural problems and the use of corporal punishment. There are also indications of an association between parental styles and the use of corporal punishment. A robust relationship between the degree of parental involvement with the child and the use of spanking was not apparent. The coefficients are in the expected direction, but the effect is not statistically significant at conventional confidence levels. Apparently, the amount of involvement of the parents is not significant, but rather it is how they are involved that matters. As can be seen in graph 4.2, parents who are more committed to cognitive stimulation activities are less likely to use corporal punishment, all other Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds (Continue) GRAPH 4.2: Probability of having been spanked in the previous week, by degree of cognitive stimulation Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.50 0.45 0.40 Our analysis introduces an innovation with respect to previous studies. We estimate if tensions in the household, due to a lack of personal time or related to care of the children, affect the probability of parents using corporal punishment. As we pointed out in chapter 3, in a society such as the Spanish one, in which the patterns found in the division of responsibilities are in transition, certain norms and expectations are contradictory, which lead to situations of ambiguity and tension in many households. Our objective is to determine the influence of such a family atmosphere in the generation of violent situations, caused by the relaxing of social inhibitions which in normal conditions deter the use of physical force. Our hypothesis is inspired by theories of violence that relate this phenomenon to situations of anomie (Durkheim, 1897) or adaptation to situations of stress (Coser, 1967; Gelles, 1974). 0.35 The results of the analysis are unequivocal. In households where situations of tension linked to a lack of time or disagreement over the care of the children are found, the propensity for corporal punishment is greater (graph 4.3). 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 NO STIMULATION LOW STIMULATION MID STIMULATION MID-HIGH STIMULATION HIGH STIMULATION DEGREE OF COGNITIVE STIMULATION Note: The probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) the child has been spanked in the previous week; 0) the child has not been spanked. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of the child; sex of the child; origin of the parents; level of father’s involvement; tension in home over childcare; tension in home over lack of time to relax; mother’s education level; household income per person; index of child’s level of conflictiveness; index of mood of child; and index of cognitive stimulation. For a more detailed description of the variables see table 4.10. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds things being equal. The effect is robust. The fact that in households where the mother works there is less corporal punishment also suggests that families with a more traditional division of roles are more prone to violent forms of disapproval, which is consistent with the affinity of this model with an authoritarian style of socialization. by existence of situations of tension in home for various reasons Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.45 0.40 0.35 0.30 analysis have found very weak evidence for this). One possible explanation would be that adults in affluent households are less tolerant of the frustration caused by the inappropriate behaviour of their children. Economic success could contribute to increasing expectations regarding children’s behaviour and achievements and perhaps the level of parental demands, which would increase the vulnerability of children who are unable to fulfill their parents’ expectations. To confirm this hypothesis would require a deeper empirical analysis than we have undertaken here. 0.25 0.20 4.4. Family learning cultures 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 NO TENSION OF ANY TYPE TENSION DUE TO LACK OF TIME TO RELAX TENSION DUE TO CHILD CARE Note: The logistic regression model includes the same variables as graph 4.2. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. One finding difficult to explain is that, with all things being equal, corporal punishment is more common in households with higher levels of income.4 This is a counterintuitive finding (but statistically robust), given that a good number of specialized studies have speculated on the possibility that in homes in economically precarious situations the use of corporal punishment would be more likely (although, on the other hand, the findings of empirical 4 The statistical effect is weakened when we eliminate variables from the model that measure parental style and situations of tension in the home. Intergenerational relationships in the family are a powerful mechanism in the reproduction of educational inequalities. The quantity, and especially quality, of these interactions are decisive in placing students at different levels throughout their educational career, which determine to a great extent their future life’s course beyond the educational system. The quality of these interactions is determined primarily by what has come to be called the family’s «cultural capital.» Families are differentiated by their capacity to cognitively stimulate their children from an early age, transmitting to them the cultural knowledge that will help them in their education and encourage and support the learning process in school. These practices and initiatives are worthwhile for children because they favour their adaptation to the demands of school and thus, they constitute a form of «capital.» Many studies have shown the importance of families’ cultural capital, which operates in different ways, in explaining academic performance and educational success (DiMaggio, 1982; Esping-Andersen, 2009). Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds GRAPH 4.3: Probabilty of having been spanked in the previous week, Today almost no one questions the social value of education. Having a child who gets bad grades or who does not make adequate progress in school is a cause of worry for virtually all parents, without large differences among socioeconomic groups. The democratization of access to higher education has generated greater ambitions and expectations for the educational attainment of children among groups traditionally excluded from education. For example, according to data from the Longitudinal Study of Families and Childhood, in a survey from the Institute of Childhood and the Urban World based on a sample of 3,000 Catalan adolescents from 13 to 16 years of age and their parents, the vast majority of the parents stated that they wanted their children to go to university. In families in which the mother had a primary school education, 80 percent of the parents expressed this desire (although only a fraction of them believed it would be possible). In almost two out of three households, parents had encouraged their adolescent children to go to college. Differences based on parents’ educational level were non-existent (Marí-Klose et al., 2008b). Parents’ education also has a limited power in explaining involvement in activities in support of the formal education of their children. The data from the Longitudinal Study of Families and Childhood suggest that students whose parents have university studies receive a little bit more help from their parents with school work, but the differences with parents who have lower educational levels are not significant. There are also no appreciable differences in the degree of involvement in parents’ associations. The results are consistent with those obtained from a subsample of parents with children between 6 and 14 years of age, the universe of analysis in this case being the whole of the Spanish population (CIS Study 2,621). It should be noted that due to small number of cases available for the analysis, it is necessary to be cautious in interpreting data. Cultural capital is an intangible quality. It is not as easy to measure it unlike other forms of capital, which can be measured by economic resources, educational credentials or even social connections. Cultural capital is formed by sensitivities, dispositions, attitudes, which, when they can be activated, cultivate in children forms of being and self-presentation in different social spaces. These forms of being and presenting oneself are especially important in school, as they are key to help children in their academic work so they can be more in-tune with teachers’ preferences and demands. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds Cultural capital is correlated with the educational level of the family, but it is far from being a perfect relationship. Parents with higher educational levels tend to encourage activities that contribute to the cognitive stimulation of their children. In addition, they transmit more cultural knowledge and skills, are more sensitive to the value of prestigious cultural products, usually take a greater interest in their children’s school activities and have more fluid and effective contact with their children’s teachers. However, the variability within the same educational group is considerable. Educational credentials are an imperfect indicator of family cultural capital. Studies on educational performance show that indicators of cultural capital usually have an independent impact on grades and educational achievement, which remains firm when the effects of parents’ educational level is controlled for. In percentages. Households with children from 7 to 14 years of age TASKS PARENTS CARRY OUT OR REMEMBER HAVING CARRIED OUT MOTHER’S EDUCATION LEVEL PRIMARY Help child at home with school work or studying Participate in activities of parents association in the school SECONDARY UNIVERSITY TOTAL NUMBER OF CASES 86 92 96 89 (384) 44 46 44 45 (192) Source: Based on data from the study 2,621, CIS 2004. TABLE 4.12: How often someone living in the household engages in reading activities with the child In percentages. Households with children from 5 to 10 years of age READING ACTIVITIES OFTEN OCCASIONALLY never Stops reading and asks child what he/she sees in the illustrations 72 23 5.7 Stops reading and points out letters 67 17 16 Asks child to read 79 12 8.3 Talks about what happens in the story after reading it 79 18 2.8 Stops reading and asks child what he/she sees in the illustrations 49 39 11 Stops reading and points out letters 44 35 22 Asks child to read 58 27 15 Talks about what happens in the story after reading it 66 33 0.8 Read or tell stories to child daily or almost daily read Sometimes reads or tells a story Source: Based on data from the Survey on Childhood and Inter and Intragenerational Relationships, 2010. Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds TABLE 4.11: Parental involvement in children’s school work, by mother’s education level also certain non-cognitive qualities that are useful for success in school (motivation, self-control, perseverance, capacity to plan and postpone gratification). In this sense, one of the most interesting indicators of family «cultural capital» consists in the activities of stimulation such as story reading. In table 4.12 we can see that this activity is what many families use to stimulate the analytical capacity of their children, their reading comprehension or to help them learn letters and vocabulary. Families directly involved in cognitive stimulation activities in the home or that encourage participation in after school activities designed to stimulate children’s aptitudes and interests also tend to be more involved in helping their children with school work. The PIRLS Survey (Progress in International Reading Literacy Study) by the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement to evaluate reading comprehension of students in the fourth-grade of primary school offers us an opportunity to estimate the effects of these learning cultures on the level of reading competency of children at the age of 10. In 2006, a total of 40 countries participated in the survey, among them Spain. This is one of the only databases available that gathers information on the socioeconomic context influencing the reading competence of the student before the end of compulsory education (unlike the PISA studies, which tests different competences when students are 15 years of age). It is important to point out, before describing the data, that because of factors that have to do with the way in which the surveys of families were carried out in Spain, the response rate was low (59 percent) and therefore not representative. It is reasonable to suspect that the questionnaires that were answered correspond to those parents who are most interested in the education of their children (Spanish PIRLS Report, 2006). Other results of our analyses suggest that there is an association between involvement in cognitive stimulation activities in the home and the likelihood that the child will receive help with homework or participate in after school activities. Children who live in homes with greater amounts of cultural capital usually benefit from multiple «quality» interactions with their parents or other adults, in the course of which they improve their cognitive profiles (knowledge, analytical capacity and linguistic skills), but Taking into consideration these limitations, our analysis shows a clear social gradient in the results on the reading competency of children at the age of 10. For example, there are 73 points of difference in the indicator for reading competency between Spanish children with parents who have university studies (552) and those who only have a primary school education (479) (educational level is based on the parent with the highest educational level in the household). Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds The data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood sheds light on some of these dimensions of cultural capital and their interrelationship with parents’ support of the formal education of their children at early ages. Although the indicators available are limited, the data show that in Spanish families there is considerable variability in «learning cultures,» configured by different intellectually stimulating activities and the consumption of cultural products. Our analyses indicate that these «learning cultures» are the result, but only in small part, of the educational level of the parents. They are made up of different pieces connected by a common thread. Children who enjoy a cumulative range of opportunities for learning enter into a virtuous spiral which favours their academic success. As the economist and Nobel Prize winner James Heckman has suggested, «learning begets learning» (see, for example, Heckman and Masterov, 2007). In the homes with a positive family learning culture, the biblical principle from Matthew comes to pass: «because to anyone who has something, more will be given, and he will have more than enough. But from the one who doesn’t have anything, even what he has will be taken away from him.» Evidence such as this impels us to rethink the causes of phenomena that are generating great social alarm, such as school failure and educational inequality. As recent studies have warned (Fernandez Enguita, 2010; M. Marí-Klose et al., 2010), the road to failure and dropping out is long. GRAPH 4.4: Level of reading comprehension of 10 year old children, by how often parents read to them up until they were three years of age In percentages PERCENTAGE LEVEL OF READING COMPREHENSION 90 550 80 540 70 530 60 520 50 510 40 500 30 490 20 480 10 470 0 460 OFTEN OCCASIONALLY NEVER/HARDLY EVER FREQUENCY OF READING STORIES Frequency of reading stories Source: Based on PIRLS data, 2006. Reading comprehension Intergenerational relationships among 5 to 10 year olds PIRLS provides information about the activities and practices carried out by parents with their children that fosters knowledge of language and reading during the early stages of childhood. Among other aspects, PIRLS asks about the frequency with which the parents or another adult in the home read stories to the child before the beginning of primary school. As can be seen in graph 4.4, there is a relationship between reading performance at 10 years of age and this type of cognitive stimulation: there is a 48 point difference in the reading performance of children whose parents stated that they often read to their children (45 percent of the parents) with respect to those whose parents said they had never or almost never done so (only 7 percent). However, the differences are also considerable with respect to those who responded that they had done so sometimes (almost half of the parents): the reading performance of their children was 36 points below the children whose parents read them stories often. These gaps can be explained only in part by the education of the parents. When educational resources in the home are controlled for in a multivariable model, the differences are somewhat reduced (33 points between children who come from families that often read stories and families without this practice), but they continue to be statistically significant. Uses of free time V. Uses of free time There are certain activities considered to be basic in the life of children, such as eating, sleeping and learning. Experts from all fields (doctors, psychologists, educators, etc.) have been prolific in the publication of precepts and prescriptions to help parents guarantee that their children eat well, sleep enough and learn quickly. In contrast, for a long time other activities associated with free time and play have received much less attention. In fact, throughout history, free time and play have often been considered unimportant aspects, even a waste of time, in children’s lives. Thinkers such as Locke or Kant were pioneers in pointing out that play is a mental activity that contributes to the balanced development of the child’s personality and promotes his or her learning processes (Chudacoff, 2007). Later, some of the first scholars to study educational processes vigorously defended play as a method of exploration that allowed children to learn about society and nature, and as a result, they recommended incorporating it into the learning process (Dewey, 1900). Today, we have abundant empirical evidence demonstrating the importance of leisure time and play for healthy childhood development, and in particular the development of children’s motor skills, organizational capacities and capacities for social interaction (Pellegrini and Smith, 1998; Razza Blair, 2009). Although play now occupies a central place in the life of children, for many generations, childhood was spent working (Ariès, 1960; Heywood, 2001). This continues to be the case in many countries in Africa, Asia and Latin America. The right of children to play, to be happy while they make use of their free time, to not have responsibilities that infringe on these rights is a dimension of childhood unknown in many societies, and which has only been recognized in our own society recently. In contemporary societies, play has become a central element in what defines childhood identity, what precisely separates it from adult life. Play is connected to a concept of childhood as a stage of life in which children are still not prepared to venture into the world of romantic relationships, the working world or political participation. Child’s play implies immaturity and irresponsibility. In this respect, as Michael Wyness (2006) pointed out, child’s play also becomes a justification for maintaining the separation between adulthood and childhood, thus legitimizing the subordinate and dependent status of children. In this chapter we offer an overview of how children from 5 to 10 years of age spend their free time. To do this, we analyze the information contributed by their parents in the Survey on Inter and Intra- 5.1. Structured and unstructured after school time One issue that raises much debate is if after school time should or should not be programmed and planned. Should parents let their children spend this time playing freely or should they encourage them to participate in structured activities that are healthy, safe and educational? Should there be an adult supervising the activities of children at all times? Underlying this question there is a basic tension that arises when children become aware of their individuality: the «power struggle» between parents and children. In early childhood, parents make almost all of the decisions related to their children: when, what, where and how much they eat, sleep, play, are alone, etc. As children come to recognize themselves as individuals, they begin to demand greater autonomy. On occasion, this translates into small conflicts in which children refuse to eat what they are given or go to bed at a certain time or in tantrums to get what they want or because play time has ended (Belden et al., 2008). Children look for small openings to try to assert their own desires in lives which are directed by others; trying in this way to increase their decision making capacity in relation to those who normally have control. As an example, research shows that children more often refuse to eat when it is the mother who is feeding them – if she is the person normally in charge of doing so – than if other family members do so (Faith et al., 2004). In our analysis of time use among children from 5 to 10 years of age, we differentiate between organized and unstructured activities following the scheme of Mahoney et al. (2005). Under the term «organized», we refer to activities that are characterized by a structure imposed through the presence of an adult. Compared with free play, in structured activities, adults have more input, for example initiating the activity, controlling the resources available for the activity, or intervening or participating during the course of the activity. The emphasis is often on skill-building and activities are generally characterized by challenges that need to be met as the complexity of the activity increases with children’s growing mastery of the activity (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). In contrast, non-structured activities refer to leisure time and improvised play shared with friends, whether in the park, on the street or at home, such as playing hide-and-seek, riding bicycles, or playing on the swings at the playground, etc. In some cases, adults participate or supervise these activities, but they do not direct them. This can also refer to time children spend alone reading, playing an instrument or watching television. (a) Participation in structured activities One of the patterns of modern parenthood is the attempt by parents to enrich and entertain their children enrolling them in all kinds of tutored activities (sporting, artistic, etc.). Starting at an early age, many children are enrolled in a wide range of extracurricular activities. These activities are considered critical not only for the healthy development of children, but also for their educational opportunities, Uses of free time generational Relationships in Childhood. Some of the key aspects that we address in this chapter are the types of activities children participate in during programmed and non-programmed time, with whom they spend this time, and what the level of parent’s involvement and supervision is in these activities. The patterns of behaviour observed in Spain are framed in broader contexts related to processes of children’s socialization and development in the world today. It is important to highlight that participation and frequency of attendance is approximately the same among boys and girls (about 78 percent participate in at least in one activity, regardless of gender, and in both cases they do so an average of twice a week). However, as can be seen in graph 5.1, there are differences in the activities in which they participate. Girls are more likely to participate in artistic activities and language classes, boys in sports. The differences in other types of activities are fewer. Even so, it seems that parents provide their sons with opportunities oriented toward physical activities and their daughters those oriented toward the development of creativity and educational competencies. GRAPH 5.1: Children enrolled in after school activities, by sex and type of activity In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 SPORTS ARTISTIC (DANCE, ART, MUSIC…) LANGUAGE CLASSES Boys REMEDIAL CLASSES AFTER SCHOOL ACTIVITIES REQUIRING PARENTAL PARTICIPATION OTHER ACTIVITIES Girls Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Practiced with the same assiduity these activities three or more times per week are involved in organized sports or remedial classes. As can be seen in graph 5.2, the majority of the activities are carried out twice a week. Uses of free time exposing children to an enriched upbringing and preparing them to face the challenges of the modern world (Education, Audiovisual & Culture Executive Agency, 2009). From our data, it is evident that parents in Spain feel the necessity to involve their children in various types of organized activities. Even among children as young as those in our sample (aged 5 to 10), involvement in extracurricular activities is very common. In our survey we asked parents if their 5-10 year olds were signed up in any of these six extracurricular activities: (1) organized sports, (2) artistic (such as dance, art, or music), (3) language classes, (4) remedial classes, (5) scholastic activities, or (6) other, nonspecified. According to parents, these children are signed up for an average of 2 activities. About three quarters of children participate in at least one activity, which means that a sizeable 28 percent of children are not engaged in any type of organized activity. Over half of the children (55 percent) are enrolled in either one or two activities and 19 percent are engaged in three or more. As can be seen in the graph below, among the organized activities for which we have information available, organized sports were the most common activities, with about 6 in ten children involved. Next most common were art-related activities, with about 1 in 4 of the children who participate in any type of activity engaged in arts. In addition, about 1 in 4 of all children aged 5-10 are taking language courses outside of school. Other activities like remedial courses or scholastic activities are less common. In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE family obligations (such as taking care of other children or elderly or disabled family members). Finally, some parents simply have fewer social connections or are less socially inclined than others (Burdette and Whitaker, 2005). 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 SPORTS ARTISTIC (DANCE, ART, MUSIC…) Once a week LANGUAGE CLASSES Twice a week REMEDIAL CLASSES AFTER SCHOOL ACTIVITIES REQUIRING PARENTAL PARTICIPATION OTHER ACTIVITIES Three or more times a week Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Participation in after school activities, especially when this requires travel or the parents’ participation is linked to the availability of various types of family resources. Not all families are able to provide the time and opportunities for after school activities for their children (or opportunities for them to play with other children outside of school). In some cases this is because they lack the economic means to meet the expenses involved. Other times it is because of parents’ long work day or work schedules which leave them with no time available at sociable hours. Parents may also have other responsibilities or excessive 0,8 0,7 0,6 Graph 5.3 shows the percentage of families who do not enroll their children in any organized activity. The black line indicates the 28% of the sample that is not involved in any activity, so comparison of the bars with the black line indicates whether a particular subgroup of the population is below or above the average in terms of involvement. As can be seen, children who are less likely to participate in extracurricular activities come from disadvantaged environments lacking in economic resources or from families with difficulties in finding time to manage their children’s participation in these activities. The first factor seems to be the most important, although we do not have information on the economic effort that these families must make (expenses related to supplies, equipment, uniforms and registration). Families with lower levels of income have much lower participation rates in after school activities than more affluent families. There also appears to be a significant gap between immigrant and nativeborn children, and a slightly smaller gap between single parent families and two-parent families. Families with higher educational levels tend to enrol their children more often in after school activities, although it is difficult to clarify if this is due to having the economic resources necessary or because of their belief that such activities are beneficial for the children. Probably both of these play a part in their decision. Our data suggest that the majority of parents who take their children to after school activities do so because the child asks Tres o más Dos veces Uses of free time GRAPH 5.2: Frequency of participation in after school activities graph 5.3: Children who do not participate in any after school activity, by household characteristics and family socioeconomic situation Household structure Parents’ origin Father Parents’ education Mother Father Parents’ employment status Mother Monthly income (in euros) Single-parent Two-parent Immigrant Native-born Primary Secondary University Primary Secondary University Does not work Works part-time Works full-time Does not work Works part-time Works full-time <1200 1201-2000 2001-3000 3001-5000 5001+ 0 5 Uses of free time In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age 10 15 20 25 25 30 35 AVERAGE Not enrolled in any activity 40 45 0 5 10 15 20 25 25 30 35 AVERAGE Not enrolled in any activity Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 40 45 For some parents, their children’s participation in after school activities is a way of balancing their work schedules with the necessities of childcare, but this is not usually the case. Only 8.9 percent of the parents who have their children in after school activities said that their work hours played a very important part in it. 14 percent said that it was quite important. These results and the previous data suggest that, in contrast to the image of children pushed to spend their days far from the family based on the needs of the parents, children’s wishes and initiative to participate in these programmes play a role that analysis has often ignored. (b) Participation in unstructured activities According to widely held opinion, children must have the opportunity to entertain themselves outside of activities programmed by parents. Free time has an intrinsic value for children and cannot be evaluated only from an outlook based on improving the child’s development or skills. The overzealous parent programming his or her child’s time has sparked a groundswell of opinion in support of recovering free time for children, invoking the need for unrestricted unsupervised play for the healthy development of the child. In this regard, the American Pediatric Association has recently warned about the risks to the mental health of children if they do not have the freedom to play in childhood (Gibbs, 2009: 56). In the rest of this section and the following we focus on an analysis of the unstructured time that children spend at home or with a family member as well as the time they spend in social activities with friends. In general, un-structured activities do not require previous planning and are usually free. The questionnaire asked about the following activities: watching television or videos; going to the cinema, theatre or a museum; playing in the street or practicing some sport or physical activity; playing video games or on the computer, and going to visit relatives. Graph 5.4 shows the frequency of children’s participation in these activities. It should be highlighted that watching television is the most widespread activity among children in this age group: 84 percent watch television daily or almost daily. In addition, many children spend even more time in front of a screen because another frequent activity is playing video games (20 percent of children doing so daily and an additional 65 percent doing so occasionally). Physical activity is also very common, about half of the children (48 percent) do physical activities on a daily basis and another 45 percent occasionally. The majority of children visit relatives at least occasionally (65 percent), but only 20 percent do so several times a week. Uses of free time them to do so. Thirty nine percent stated that the child’s request weighed heavily in their decision, and 34 percent stated that this was quite important. Only 17 percent said they had signed their children up to an after school activity without taking into account the child’s wishes. Based on the parents’ testimony, 60 percent indicated that their child liked very much attending an after school activity or programme, and 35 percent stated that the child quite liked the activity. Parents who force their children to go to after school activities which the children do not like are a very small minority. Evidently, the reliability of the responses may be affected by the perspective of the respondents, but it does not seem likely that the children’s perspective if it were possible to ask them would be that different. In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 VISITS RELATIVES PLAYS VIDEO OR COMPUTER GAMES OR SIMILAR Daily or almost daily DOES SPORTS OR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY GOES TO CINEMA, THEATRE OR MUSEUM WATCHES TV OR VIDEOS Occasionally PLAYS MUSICAL INSTRUMENT The childhood use (or abuse) of new technologies is a topic of debate and often appears in the media as a source of alarm. Hence, we have looked at the relationship between certain family characteristics and the use of TV and video games. To do this, we have compared this more sedentary form of entertainment with the frequency with which children practice a sport or physical activities, usually considered beneficial for their age. To simplify the analysis we have focused exclusively on families in which children do both types of activity daily or several times a week. Table 5.1 shows the differences are small, but reveals a pattern in terms of parental education and income. Children in families with a higher socioeconomic level tend to watch television and play video games less often. They are, instead, more likely to practice sports and physical activities daily–with the exception of those children in families with the highest levels of income; among this group we find the lowest percentage of children that do so. Never Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Ninguna vez Alguna vez Cada día o casi cada día Practica otros sola, familiares ordenador algún deporte Va o juevos al cine, o actividad similares teatro Miraolafísica museo TV Toca o videos un instrumento visitar a otros familiares Uses of free time GRAPH 5.4: Frequency of participation in unstructured activities In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age WATCHES TV OR VIDEOS PRACTICES/DOES SPORTS OR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY PLAYS VIDEO GAMES Single-parent 78 48 24 Two parents 85 54 20 Immigrant 80 45 23 Native-born 85 49 20 Primary 85 42 23 Secondary 86 48 20 University 80 54 19 Does not work 86 46 22 Works part-time 82 43 24 Works full-time 83 53 17 Does not work 82 46 25 Works part-time 88 54 21 Works full-time 84 48 19 SOCIOECONOMIC CHARACTERISTICS Household structure Parents’ origin Mother’s characteristics Education level Employment status Father’s characteristics Employment status Uses of free time TABLE 5.1: Activities child does daily or almost daily, by different socioeconomic characteristics of household (Continue) Primary 87 49 24 Secondary 85 43 20 University 79 56 16 Less than €1,200 82 46 24 From €1,201 to €2,000 84 45 21 From €2,001 to €3,000 86 49 21 From €3,001 to €5,000 86 56 16 More than €5,000 72 40 22 Household income Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 5.2. Parents’ involvement in their children’s free time As we pointed out in previous chapters, despite the assumption often held being the opposite, boys as well as girls receive more attention and emotional support from their parents today than children did in the past. Parents are driven by the desire to do the right thing for their children, following both their own criteria and that of the watchful society in which they live. Parents develop a set of guidelines about what is right for a child based on their own experience as children, observations from the rearing of siblings or other relatives, what they have read or been told, or what they feel based on common sense. Driven by a sense of urgency that any misstep in following these guidelines could lead a child to having nutritional, sleep or educational deficiencies, they set out to direct the child at every step according to these guidelines. This includes what and how much the child should eat; when, where and how long the child should sleep; how much time he or she should spend with adults and with other children; which activities are appropriate and which ones are not allowed; and what the rules are for these activities and interactions. In addition, parents feel outside pressure to demonstrate to the world that they are good parents by following norms and freely offered directives from relatives and other adults about what children should and should not do, receive, and be engaged in. Even when these norms are not in full accord with the parents’ own guidelines, parents must publicly demonstrate their good parenting. This includes demonstrating that the child is supervised and not allowed to roam on his/her own, that the child is clean and Uses of free time Education level In a society in which people are having fewer children, but at the same time are investing more in them (in terms of health, education, toys and after school activities), the norms that parents establish as well as the social pressures, seem to be changing. There is growing concern about the safety of children. Parents today are less permissive than their own parents were or their grandparents when it comes to allowing children to play outdoors without adult supervision. This concern for childhood safety is due to the objective increase of certain risks (for example, being hit by a car), but much of the perception of risk is inspired by new currents of collective anxiety which place childhood at the centre of obsessions about safety. This anxiety is embodied in the fear of risks that are negligible (such as children being kidnapped by a stranger or being abused by a paedophile) while ignoring many of the risks to children’s physical and emotional safety (including sexual abuse) that tend to originate in the home or the immediate environment. Paradoxically, the obsession over risks, which has caused many parents to limit their children’s freedom to go out alone, has been related to the origin of new risks, real or perceived. Phenomena such as childhood obesity or young people’s lack of initiative have been attributed to childhood experiences in homes converted into «gilded cages,» where children lack opportunity for entertainment. It must be remembered that the data analyzed refer to young children (between 5 and 10 years of age), so that their «free time» tends to be supervised by adults. As can be observed in table 5.2, parents participate in the majority of their children’s activities. They are present when the children go out, whether to the cinema, to the TABLE 5.2: Persons with whom the child usually does different activities(a) In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age ALONE WITH YOU OR YOUR PARTNER SIBLINGS OTHER RELATIVES Play video games 26 22 32 1.8 2.6 15 Practice/do sports or physical activity 15 20 8.0 1.1 49 7.5 Go to cinema, theatre or museum 0.3 78 2.0 2.4 4.0 13 Watch TV or videos 17 54 26 1.7 0.5 0.9 Visit relatives 1.3 92 1.5 1.1 0.0 3.9 ACTIVITIES Note: a) The percentages are calculated based on the total number of children that do the activity. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. FRIENDS DOESN’T DO THIS ACTIVITY Uses of free time neat, that the child has been taught basic manners, and that he or she is receiving adequate social and educational stimulation. immigrant families than among native families. By level of income, the difference is in the number of times per week that children spend in the homes of friends: in homes with higher incomes, the proportion of parents who say their children have friends over one time per week is higher; in contrast, in homes with a lower economic level, they have friends over more often. 5.3. Time with friends To understand what happens in children’s free time, we cannot ignoretheroleofotherfamilymembers. Inmanyfamiliesgrandparents provide important support. For a long time, grandparents have taken care of their young grandchildren, and they still do this today. In different developed countries, many parents rely on their own parents to take responsibility for the children after school, whether it is to pick them up, give them something to eat, supervise them or simply be with them. In a country such as Spain, where families tend to live near their relatives, it is common for the grandparents to share in the responsibilities of taking care of the children (Borra and Palma, 2009). Three generations living together (parents, grandparents and grandchildren) in the same home is not very common today. However, living together is not a requirement for the grandparents to take care of their grandchildren. According to recently published data, in Spain one out of every four grandparents takes care of their grandchildren. The European average is somewhat higher with one out of every three. However, Spanish grandparents that take care of their grandchildren do it more intensively than in the rest of Europe: they dedicate on average seven hours a day to the care of their grandchildren, two hours more than the European average (Badenes Pla and Lopez Lopez, 2010). Time with same-aged peers peaks in adolescence, especially between ages 14 to16 years, however, it is common for children to spend time outside of school with other children even at these younger ages. The importance of creating these connections, also known as peer cultures has been demonstrated to benefit child development (Corsaro, 1985). To analyze the time spent with friends, we have focused on two aspects: how often the child has friends over to his or her home and the level of supervision by parents when there are friends over. As can be seen in table 5.3, spending time with friends at home is something that is relatively common but does not happen every day. This is an expected result, taking into account that these are children between the ages of 5 and 10 and, therefore, requiring adult supervision. It is even likely that the parents plan these encounters with other children. According to our data, in four out of every ten families, the children do not invite friends to their home. Moreover, only 11 percent of the families responded that their children spend time in the homes of their friends three or more times per week. Although there are not marked differences, it is more common among girls to have friends over to play. It is less common among 5.4. Involvement of grandparents and siblings Uses of free time theatre or to visit relatives, and inside the home, while the children play an instrument or watch television. Our data contradict the vision of parents leaving their children in front of the television (a «safe» activity that does not require supervision) in order to work or do other tasks. More than half of the children in our sample usually watch television accompanied by their parents. The only activities that they commonly do with their siblings and friends are playing video games and sports or doing physical activities. In percentages: Children from 5 to 10 years of age NUMBER OF DAYS A WEEK FRIENDS COME TO PLAY SOCIOECONOMIC CHARACTERISTICS 0 1 2 3 or more Single-parent 38 32 14 15 Two parents 43 34 13 10 Boy 46 31 12 10 Girl 39 36 13 12 Native-born 42 33 14 11 Immigrant 47 35 7.7 10 Primary 38 32 15 15 Secondary 44 34 12 10 University 45 36 12 7.0 Does not work 40 35 13 13 Works part-time 52 27 17 3.8 Works full-time 43 34 13 11 Household structure Sex of child Parents’ origin Father’s characteristics Education level Employment status Uses of free time TABLE 5.3: How often child has friends over to play, by household socioeconomic characteristics (Continue) Education level Primary 41 27 16 16 Secondary 44 34 12 10 University 43 38 11 7.7 Does not work 38 33 14 14 Works part-time 48 31 13 8.1 Works full-time 44 35 12 8.8 Less than €1.200 43 28 17 13 From €1,201 to €2,000 38 33 16 13 From €2,001 to €3,000 46 37 10 7.2 From €3,001 to €5,000 43 36 11 10 More than €5,000 47 44 3,1 6.3 Total 43 34 13 11 Employment status Household income Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. As we have seen, grandparents, just like older siblings, can be a resource in taking responsibility for the supervision of the youngest children and, in general, for being with them, especially when the parents are working, preparing meals or doing other household chores. But in certain situations, grandparents and siblings can also take away from the time parents are able spend with their young children. This is the case when the grandparents have health problems or when the youngest child’s siblings are young themselves; it is possible that they require time, effort and resources that otherwise the parents could dedicate to the youngest. In 8.4 percent of the families interviewed, one of the grandparents lives in the home. Forty-two percent of the households received help from the grandparents in caring for the children. Table 5.4 shows that in households that count on the grandparents’ help, whether because they live with them or because they help with Uses of free time Mother’s characteristics TABLE 5.4: Number of after-school activities child participates in, by composition of household In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age GRANDPARENTS NUMBER OF AFTER-SCHOOL ACTIVITIES CHILD IS ENROLLED IN(a) RESIDE IN HOME SIBLINGS DO NOT RESIDE in home (b) ( HELP DO NOT HELP RESIDE IN HOME DO NOT RESIDE in home Total NUMBER OF CASES None 28 26 16 29 26 28 (324) 1 27 26 17 30 21 27 (313) 2 or more 45 48 67 41 53 45 (510) 100 (1,147) 100 Note: a) After school activities: language classes, art classes, remedial classes, sports, school activities or other organized activities. b) The category «does not reside» includes cases in which the child does not have this family member and those in which he/she does but family member does not live in the home. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. childcare, the children tend to do fewer after school activities. This fact suggests that the provision of care offered by grandparents attenuates the necessity of many families to resort to these activities to resolve the scheduling problems they have. However, the fact that three out of four children that have grandparents «available» participated in at least one after school activity (and many in two or more) indicates that it would be a serious error to think that children’s participation in these activities is simply a way to reconcile work and family life for those who have no other means of taking care of their children. Almost 70 percent of the families interviewed have more than one child living in the home. Children between 5 and 10 years of age who live in families with two or more children participate on average in fewer organized activities. It is possible that children who have siblings do not ask as often to participate in activities outside the home as they can be entertained playing with their siblings. In addition, the increase in the number of children also increases the effort that parents must make to offer the opportunity to participate in different after school activities, both from an economic perspective (possible fees) and in regards to the time necessary to manage all these different activities. All of the data presented in this chapter suggest that new modalities for managing children’s free time (with a growing emphasis on organized activities) cannot be considered simply strategies Uses of free time (grandparents and siblings living or not living in the home) Secondly, we are witnessing the consolidation of the role of the child in choosing his or her educational and entertainment activities, which is not necessarily in contradiction with the processes described in the previous paragraph. Until very recently, it was understood that adults had to decide for children («for their own good») how their time should be organized (to be spend primarily in school), reserving spaces for them of unsupervised time after school in which they could enjoy a considerable amount of freedom with their peers. The growing fear of parents of the risks for the physical safety of children in public space has reinforced their ties to spaces controlled by adults. But this does not necessarily mean that their autonomy has disappeared completely. In contrast to an image that emphasizes activities planned by adults, the evidence is otherwise; children are involved in the selection of the activities they do, and the majority of them enjoy these activities. Although the nature of these activities can be quite varied, there are reasons to think that in many of them children find spaces for interaction and entertainment that offer opportunities to affirm their identities as agents (Näsman, 1998). Uses of free time resulting from parents pressured by a lack of time. It is necessary to interpret them in at least two other ways. First of all, they are linked to the growing concern among parents to provide «optimum support» to their children (Beck-Gernsheim, 2003). Starting at the end of the twentieth century, the progress in medicine, psychology and pedagogy combined with the decrease in the number of children per family, has fuelled new expectations of «improvement» for children within the family through an adequate channelling of resources. New discourses equate not taking advantage of possibilities for children’s development with neglect, resulting in growing demands placed on parents. The creed of the new attitude toward children, which the data suggest extends to a greater or lesser degree among all social groups, is that parents can promote children’s faculties (and correct their defects) by providing them with the opportunity to get off to a good start. After-school activities occupy a privileged position in the catalogue of strategies for improvement that parents are called on to adopt. The situation of children in the developed world is awakening concern. The majority of international reports offering an analysis of this population’s quality of life and its evolution are in agreement in finding a vulnerability to risks concentrated in early childhood. The risks which have generally threatened childhood are those related to health, poverty and other economic difficulties. With regard to health, in the past century that of children’s has greatly improved throughout the world. For centuries childhood was seen as an unhappy and vulnerable period in life as children often suffered from illness. Children that were fragile and victims of chronic illness were part of the daily life of many families. The lack of hygiene, the affect of infectious diseases and hunger took a terrible toll in childhood, claiming many lives. We know, for example, that at the end of the nineteenth century in certain years (1880, 1882-1883, 1885 and 1900) one quarter of all newborns in Spain died. Childhood illness and death were a part of daily life, and adults exhibited a certain fatalism in facing them. In many testimonies from that time (in novels, health reports, the press, etc.), the idea that children’s lives hung by a thread was accepted with resignation (A. de Miguel, 1998: 195-197). The increase in life expectancy during the past century has been extraordinary. At the beginning of the twentieth century the infant mortality rate in Spain was 203 deaths for every 1,000 children. Since then, it has continuously declined. From 19601970 when the parents of our Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood were growing up, the infant mortality rate in Spain was already then below 40 deaths for every 1,000 births. According to most recent estimates, the infant mortality rate has gone down to 3.5 deaths for every 1,000 births (World Bank, 2008). This decline in infant mortality has been accompanied by a decrease in morbidity. Respiratory infections and diseases which are common in developing countries are rare and seldom deadly among children in develped countries. However, this does not mean that the health problems of childhood have been eradicated. New illnesses, no longer mortal, are appearing related to lifestyle and unhealthy habits. The situation with respect to poverty and economic difficulty continues to be of concern. For example, in the majority of countries in the European Union the poverty rates among children are higher than those among adults (European Commission, The emergence of social risks in childhood VI.The emergence of social risks in childhood The degree of family participation in the labour market is the principal factor determining its economic situation. Thus, seven out of ten children under ten years of age from families where none of the economically active members are working are poor. However, economic precariousness does not only affect households in which no one works. When one person works, the risk of poverty is reduced considerably, although it continues to be high: affecting three out of ten children from 0 to 10 years of age. The traditional family model with just one breadwinner, usually the man, does not assure protection from economic exclusion for many families with dependent children. A very high proportion of children from 0 to 10 years of age who are poor come from these families (58.5 percent). The poverty rate for children from 0-10 years old goes down by 20 percentage points when two people in the household work (8.6 percent), which indicates that today more than in the past, it is necessary to have two incomes in the home to avoid economic vulnerability in childhood. In addition to work, the second factor that has a strong impact on the living conditions of children is household structure. Large families and single parent families are overrepresented among households in which children are exposed to poverty: 49 percent of the children who live in large families and 43 percent of those in single parent families are poor. When the mother is the head of a single parent household and does not work the risk of poverty increases. While 30 percent of single parent households in which the mother works are living in poverty, in those where the mother does not work this goes up to 69 percent (data from the Quality of Life Index, 2008). It is also necessary to look at the situation of children of immigrant origin. According to the Survey of Income and Living Conditions (2008), approximately four out of ten children under 10 years of age whose mother or father was born outside of the EU-25 are living in poverty, twice the rate of autochthonous children. High poverty rates and severe poverty rates follow similar patterns: The high poverty rate is 15.6 percent among children of foreign origin and 6.2 percent among autochthonous children, and the severe poverty rate is 6 percent among those of foreign origin and 3.5 percent among autochthonous children. The limited data available for the recent years of economic crisis suggest that the poverty rates, measured according to available income, have not varied much since 2007, but the perception of hardship has.1 According to data gathered in the Survey of Income 1 At the time of publication of this book, the INE (Spain’s National Statistics Institute) has preliminary results from the Living Conditions Survey for 2010. According to these results the poverty rate for children under 16 is 24.6 percent, 4.3 points above the risk of poverty for the population over 16 years of age. If we subtract the «imputed rental value» (a measure of the rental value of the dwelling), the poverty rate (22.2 percent) is much greater than that for all other age groups (for example, 9 points greater than for the population over 65 years of age). The emergence of social risks in childhood 2009; OECD, 2008). Spain forms part of the group of European countries with the highest levels of child poverty. Whereas in the EU-27, one out of every five children under 18 years of age is poor, in Spain poverty affects one out of four, a figure which situates Spain in a worse position than the majority of EU countries, with the exception of Italy, Latvia, Bulgaria and Romania (Eurostat, 2008). If we focus specifically on the age group of our study (children from 0 to 10 years of age), the outlook is not encouraging: 21.7 percent of Spanish children are poor (data from the 2008 Survey of Income and Living Conditions of the National Statistics Institute). These findings show that the childhood poverty rate is two percentage points higher than the overall poverty rate for Spain (19.6 percent). Regarding over-indebtedness, according to the 2008 Survey of Income and Living Conditions, only 4.2 percent of the Spanish population with bank accounts stated that they had an overdraft or outstanding balance due to economic difficulties. In households with children 10 years old and under, the proportion is somewhat higher (7.2 percent). When the economic situation worsens, some households find it difficult to pay their bills. Half of the families with children 10 years old and under recognized that in 2008 the total cost of housing represented a heavy burden for the household, and 8.3 percent of homes with children from 0-10 had got behind in paying their mortgage or rent in the previous year. The impact of the cost of housing is more noticeable in households with children under 10 years of age than in households headed by older persons because these children tend to belong to younger families who are still covering the costs of housing that they were likely to have rented or purchased during the recent great increase in the price of housing. In fact, the percentage of children living in poverty increases by 11 points (from 22 percent to 33 percent) after subtracting families’ housing costs from household income. Besides the traditional risks of illness or economic hardship to the well-being of children, throughout the twentieth century new causes for concern emerged related to the physical, emotional and educational vulnerability of children. In this chapter we utilize our Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood to analyze the influence that factors examined in the previous chapters–household structure, socioeconomic conditions, styles of parenting or after school time use–have on key aspects of the well-being and health of children in contemporary society. We focus our analysis on three indicators that generate considerable social concern today: body weight, socio-emotional competence and educational engagement. The experiences of childhood play a crucial role in people’s lives. There is growing empirical evidence to support the idea that the problems children face affect their physical and emotional development and their learning capacity, endangering not only their well-being but also their quality of life and opportunities when they are adults. In this sense, the components analyzed here are central aspects of children’s health and well-being. International studies on children’s weight, socio-emotional competencies and their response to the demands of school have shown that problems related to these factors can have long-lasting effects that are very difficult to reverse. The studies on these issues that have been carried out in Spain have tended to approach these problems once they have fully developed, generally in adolescence when they take on new forms: isolation and marginalization, emotional distress and school failure. Rarely has there been data available on earlier stages of childhood, making it difficult to get to the root of these problems. Our survey offers us this opportunity. A good understanding of the mechanisms involved in the generation of these problems is one of the keys for families as well as governments to be able to channel their energy, time and money The emergence of social risks in childhood and Living Conditions (2008), in the preliminary phases of the crisis, 26 percent of the Spanish population said that in the previous year they had already suffered a significant decrease in income. In households with children 10 years of age and under, the percentage was somewhat higher: 31 percent. The economic crisis could also have accentuated conditions of economic hardship, which are measured based on indicators such as over-indebtedness or the impact of the cost of housing on the risk of poverty. 6.1. A weighty problem: the road to obesity Body weight reflects the balance between energy intake (the ingestion of food) and energy expenditure. When children are very young, they are able to regulate the proper intake, but as they get older the influence of external social factors becomes more important (Birch, 1987). Childhood is a crucial stage in life in terms of defining food preferences and tastes as well as for acquiring habits that promote physical activity. Parents prepare most of the meals children eat (when they are not eating at school), decide on their distribution throughout the day and decide on the type and quantity of food the child is going to eat; they are in charge of and monitor energy intake, and they are responsible for teaching children about the qualities of foods and the behaviours that must be observed during mealtimes. In reality, what affects the health of the child is not so much weight but the amount of body fat or adiposity. Indeed, the most effective treatments to improve children’s health have focused on reducing adiposity without altering weight (American Dietetic Association, 2006). Although there are complex ways to measure adiposity, the most common is the body mass index (BMI), which is calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in centimetres squared. In addition, when sex and age are taken into account, the calculation of body mass index provides a very rough measure of body fat (Johnson-Taylor and Everhart, 2006). In adults, body weight is measured using the BMI. However, in the case of children this is more problematic since weight gain and changing shape are part of the growth process itself. Taking into account that the BMI varies according to sex and age in childhood, studies have generally utilized for their calculations the BMI percentiles or the standard deviations (Z score) (Johnson-Taylor and Everhart, 2006). In recent years, there has been a considerable increase in the prevalence of overweight in childhood. Today one out of every ten school-age children in all of the world is overweight (Lobstein et al., 2004). In the United States, it is estimated that 17 percent of the children between 6 and 11 years of age are obese (in other words, their body mass index is above the 95th percentile) and one out of three are overweight (BMI ≥ 85th percentile), according to data from 2003-2006 (Ogden et al., 2006). In Spain, using international measurement standards of reference for weight and height according to age, Aranceta-Bartrina et al., (2005) calculated that in the year 2000, 14.5 percent of the children between 6 and 9 years of age were above the 85th percentile, but below the 97th percentile, and 15 percent had a weight above the 97th percentile (Aranceta-Bartrina, et al., 2005).2 Childhood obesity has been shown to be associated with diverse health problems, such as asthma and sleep apnoea, early onset of type 2 diabetes or menstrual irregularity (Must et al., 2003). The physiological consequences in childhood tend to last long, multiplying the risks of obesity in adulthood or suffering from chronic diseases (Serdula et al., 1993; Thompson 2 In general, one is considered overweight if the BMI is above the 85th percentile and obese if above the 95th percentile for the same age and sex, according to recommendations of the European Childhood Obesity Group. But some authors have chosen different thresholds, such as the 90th and 97th percentiles to define overweight and obesity, respectively. Because of this, on occasion it is difficult to establish comparisons between the findings of different studies given that the criteria employed to define overweight and obesity are not the same: not everyone uses the same tables as a reference or the same cut-off point. The emergence of social risks in childhood in the most efficient way possible and in this way be able to promote changes that contribute to the well-being of children. sample of 847 with plausible measurements for children’s weight and height.3 The average height of children in the sample is 128 cm, and the average weight is 30.3 kilograms. If we compare these figures with the international reference established by the WHO (International reference population), the children of our sample are slightly taller and weigh about half a standard deviation more. Their body mass index is above that of the reference population by over half a standard deviation. This indicates that approximately half of the children (46 percent) are in the normal range, while 14 percent are underweight, 24 percent are overweight; 12 percent obese and 4.6 percent suffer from morbid obesity. TABLE 6.1: Distribution of boys and girls in weight categories, Our study is based on data provided by parents about their children. In studies with a large sample like ours, this method turns out to be more viable than direct measurement. However, the declared weights and heights are usually biased, requiring caution in the interpretation of the results (Bogaert et al., 2003). To construct the BMI we have utilized the Child Growth Standards for schoolage children of the World Health Organization (WHO Reference, 2007). Children who are placed two standard deviations above the mean are considered obese. The data from the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, like other studies, suggests that parents are more likely to know their child’s weight than the height. In the analysis carried out, taking into account age and sex, the cases with a biologically impossible weight and height were eliminated. From the total of 1,148 parents of children between 5 and 10 interviewed, we got a by body mass index Children from 5 to 10 years of age WEIGHT CATEGORIES NUMBER OF CASES PERCENTAGE total GIRLS Underweight 115 14 12 14 Normal weight 390 46 54 40 Overweight 202 24 23 24 Obesity 101 12 9.0 15 39 4.6 2.0 7.0 847 100 100 100 Morbid obesity Total BOYS Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 3 Given that we do not have a very large sample, and in order to maintain the reliability of our results, we minimize the number of controls we introduce into our analysis to maintain adequate representation of families and children in each of the categories. The emergence of social risks in childhood et al., 2007). Obesity in childhood also has psychological and social consequences. Much research has documented the stigmatization and marginalization suffered by children with overweight (Sweeting et al., 2005; Storch et al., 2007), as well as the emotional consequences of being discriminated against by peers, with the consequential increase in the risk of suffering from problems of low self-esteem or depression (Storch et al., 2007). In the worst of cases, obesity can lead to a downward spiral of socio-economic, relationship and mental problems that feed on each other. Children who are obese often find themselves on the first steps of a path that can lead to diverse complications in the future, not only of a medical nature but also social and psychological (Ferraro and Kelly-Moore, 2003; Crosnoe and Lopez-González, 2005). In the next section we will analyze some of the primary determinants of weight for Spanish children between 5 and 10 years of age based on the sample from our survey. With the goal of facilitating the interpretation of the findings and given the limitations of the sample, we will differentiate solely between obese and nonobese children. We have designated as «obese» the 16.7 percent of children who have levels of obesity or morbid obesity. All of the rest (including those who have lower than normal weights) are categorized as «non-obese». (a) Fathers’ and mothers’ weight One of the most important predictors of the body mass index of children is the body mass index of their parents. There is a genetic component in the relationship between one’s own weight and the weight of family members. In our sample, mothers tended to maintain normal weights more often than fathers. The average BMI of mothers was 23.6 (normal weight), while that of the fathers entered in the category of overweight, with an average of 26.3. In fact, only 27 percent of the children had both parents in the normal category. 5 percent had at least one parent with underweight (in 80 percent of the cases this was the mother), and one out of five children had at least one obese parent (in almost 70 percent of the cases, the father). Many studies suggest that the mother’s weight has more influence than does the father’s weight on the weight of the child. However, in our sample the relationship is the opposite, and the child’s weight appears to be slightly more correlated with the weight of the father (r=0.16) than with that of the mother (r=0.14). These correlations are also lower than those reported in other studies: the BMI of the father and mother explains approximately 4 percent of the variation in the BMI of the children. Still, a child with an obese father has almost 3 times higher odds of also being obese, and a child with an obese mother has 80 percent higher odds of being obese compared with a child whose mother is normal weight. (b) Socioeconomic characteristics and household structure In addition to the genetic predisposition to overweight, we must also look at the influence of new habits and behaviours as well as other social factors on the increase in childhood obesity that has taken place in recent years. As seen in earlier chapters, children’s development during the first stages of childhood is marked by their home environment. This is especially true in the case of weight. When children are small, the parents have an influence over their diet, the physical or sedentary character of their activities, and expectations about the body and the importance given to taking care of it (Golan et al., 2004). To better understand the influence of socioeconomic and attitudinal factors on childhood obesity we will return to some of the factors analyzed in previous chapters. The emergence of social risks in childhood Although a large part of the child population has a normal weight, boys tend to weigh more and have a greater probability of being in the problematic categories (both for underweight and obesity). The differences are especially marked for morbid obesity, which affects 7 percent of boys and 2 percent of girls. The odds ratio for being obese for girls is half that of boys of the same age. Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.16 0.14 0.12 0.10 0.08 0.06 0.04 0.02 0.00 NORMAL WEIGHT OVERWEIGHT Mother OBESITY Father Note: The probability is calculated based on logistic regression models in which the dependent variable had two values: 1) The child is obese; 0) The child is not obese. The following variables have been introduced into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day, mothers’ weight and father’s weight. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. In earlier chapters, we looked at diverse evidence on the relationship between the socioeconomic characteristics of families and children’s well-being. The children of well-off families tend to grow up in contexts more favourable for their development. However, the following tables indicate that the relationship between household income level and obesity is not very strong. Although descriptive data suggest that there are Padre a lower proportion of obese children among those who come from families with higher incomes, the differences are not statistically significant. In fact, when we control for other factors (mother’s level of education and employment status), the relationship disappears, which suggests that the differences must be explained by other characteristics of the child and his or her family (see table 6.2). More than income, without a doubt, the most important explanatory factor is the educational level of the parent. As we will see later on, this is a relationship repeated with the other indicators analyzed in this chapter: socio-emotional competence and the response to demands of school. Children whose parents have completed secondary education have a lower odds ratio for being obese than children who come from households with lower educational levels. If the parents have a university education, the odds ratio for being obese is even lower. In this case, the educational level of the mother has greater explanatory power than that of the father. If we control for the influence of parental education, other socioeconomic factors, such as parental income level and employment status, do not explain the variation in obesity among children (as can be seen in the last column). Another socioeconomic factor significantly associated with risk of obesity is immigrant status. The percentage of children of immigrant origin having problems with obesity is greater than that of autochthonous children (21 percent and 16 percent respectively). If we control for the effect of other sociodemographic factors, children between 5 and 10 years of age who are of immigrant origin have an 80 percent higher risk of suffering obesity than the children of Spanish parents. Madre Madre Padre The emergence of social risks in childhood Graph 6.1: Probability of children being obese, by parents’ weight In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age SOCIOECONOMIC CHARACTERISTICS Household income Less than €1,200 From €1,201 to €2,000 From €2,001 to €3,000 From €3,001 to €5,000 More than €5,000 Parents’ origin Native Immigrant Mother’s characteristics Employment status Does not work Works part-time Works full-time Education level Primary Secondary University Father’s characteristics Employment status Does not work Works part-time Works full-time PERCENTAGE ODDS RATIO(a) ODDS ADJUSTED RATIO(b) 18 20 17 15 4.2 1 1.28 0.97 0.82 0.20* 1 1.32 1.25 1.36 0.35† 16 21 1 1.45 1 1.78* 18 17 15 1 0.88 0.74 1 0.99 0.91 22 17 17 1 0.69† 0.48** 1 0.66† 0.46** 26 18 16 1 0.87 0.74 1 0.92 0.81 The emergence of social risks in childhood TABLE 6.2: Obesity in children, by socioeconomic characteristics of household Education level Primary Secondary University 18 17 14 1 0.9 0.71 1 0.89 0.74 † Significance level of 10% * Significance level of 5% ** Significance level of 1% *** Significance level of 1‰ Note: a) Probability of child being obese. b) Probability of child being obese, socioeconomic conditions being equal. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. An important aspect to bear in mind in relation to the family situation is family structure; that is, which people live with the child and thus, form part of his or her daily life. One of the most studied aspects is whether the absence of one parent in the home affects indicators of well-being. Research on family structure and obesity is limited. Even so, the assumption is that children who live with only one parent run a greater risk of having unhealthy eating habits because of being subject to lower levels of parental control, due to the difficulties involved in doing this job alone. In the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood we have a very small sample of singleparent families, which limits our capacity to reach relevant conclusions. Only 7.1 percent of our sample of children from 5 to 10 years of age lives in a single-parent home. The results of our analysis, however, point in the expected direction: children who live with only one parent have a greater probability of being obese than those who live with both parents, although the differences are not statistically significant. A large number of international studies have concluded that the presence of other adults in the home can be beneficial for childhood health. Among the most significant evidence for this is that it is often pointed out that children who live with or often spend time with a grandmother tend to obtain better scores on different indicators of well-being, including survival (Mace, 2000) and growing up without health problems (Duflo, 2003). In the United States, Pope (1993) related living with a grandmother to higher cognitive capacities, better behaviour and health at three years of age. Regarding nutrition and risk of obesity, the presence of additional adults in the home, above all grandmothers, could be especially important given that they are often in charge of caring for the children and the preparation of meals. Their presence in the home can increase the levels of supervision of children’s habits and promote meal preparation at home. That said, it is also possible that grandparents are more tolerant of their grandchildren eating more, or they are more likely to give them fattening foods (sweets, candy, etc.) and that in general, they are The emergence of social risks in childhood (Continue) In line with these results, Spanish children who live with grandparents have a significantly higher probability of being obese (table 6.3). Childhood obesity is a relatively recent problem in Spain, about which older generations are not well-informed. In addition, perhaps the fact that many of them experienced food shortages in their own childhood (or at least serious restrictions in the range of products available) could reduce their concern about obesity in their grandchildren. It is doubtful, therefore, that the presence of grandparents in the home can contribute to increasing the levels of control over their grandchildren’s habits and limit the amount they ingest. In fact, research carried out in other countries suggests that grandmothers put a lot of pressure on mothers when they think that their grandchildren are too thin (Bruss et al., 2003). TABLE 6.3: Obesity in children, by composition of household In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age COMPOSITION OF HOUSEHOLD PERCENTAGE ODDS RATIO(b) ODDS ADJUSTED RATIO(c) Household structure Two parents 16 1 1 Single-parent 22 1.44 1.34 Do not reside in the home(a) 17 1 1 Reside in the home 16 0.92 0.91 Do not reside in the home 16 1 1 Reside in the home 26 Siblings Grandparents 1.86* 1.73† † Significance level of 10% * Significance level of 5% ** Significance level of 1% *** Significance level of 1‰ Note: a) The category «do not reside» combines the cases in which the child does not have siblings and those in which he/she does, but they do not reside in the home. b) Probability of child being obese. c) Probability of child being obese, socioeconomic conditions being equal. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The emergence of social risks in childhood less concerned about the negative consequences of childhood obesity (Jiang et al., 2007). (c) Parental styles Graph 6.2: Probability of child being obese, by parents’ perception Daily routines and sedentary habits have a considerable influence on the risk of being overweight. The time children spend in front of the screen (television, computer or video console) and childhood obesity have increased simultaneously in recent years. Despite this, research has not yielded clear findings regarding the relationship between obesity and the time that children spend watching television or playing video games. Apparently, the relationship becomes stronger after 10 years of age (Marshall et al., 2004; ReyLopez, 2008). In line with this, our analysis does not reveal the existence of a relationship between the activities children do at home–that is, the frequency with which they watch television, play video games, practice sports or eat with their parents–and the risks of obesity among children between 5 and 10 years of age. Looking at parents’ assessments of the amount of time they share with their children–independent from how they spend that time–we find that this does have an influence on the risk of obesity. Graph 6.2 shows that the probability of suffering from obesity is higher in children whose parents state that they do not spend enough time with their children. The amount of time the father dedicates is apparently more crucial than that of the mother, especially when it is not enough time. 0,25 0,20 Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 NOT ENOUGH ENOUGH Mother’s perception about amount of time father spends with child MORE THAN ENOUGH Mother’s perception about amount of time spent with child The emergence of social risks in childhood of the amount of time they spend with him/her Note: The responses of mothers on their perception of the amount of time they spend and the father spends with the child were collected. Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) the child is obese; 0) child is not obese. The following variables have been introduced into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day, mother’s perception of time father spends with child and perception of time she spends with child. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Today there are still many questions about the way in which the family influences the weight of its youngest members. In fact, it seems that the parents’ commitment to promoting healthy eating can end up backfiring. For example, Galloway (2006) has found that parental pressure on children to eat healthy food is not usually very successful. Other research has even indicated that childrenPercepción who are del tiem Percepción del tiem Graph 6.3: Probability of child being obese, by type of fatherhood Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 INTENSE RESPONSIBLE ADAPTIVE TRADITIONAL PREDISPOSED UNCOMMITTED TYPES OF FATHERHOOD Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) the child is obese; 0) child is not obese. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mothers’ education level, mother’s working day and parenting styles. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. (d) Friendships and free time 4 These types of fatherhood are defined in Chapter 4. See pages 91-93. The negative social and psychological ramifications of adolescent obesity may be as damaging as its consequences for physical health. Obese children face social problems such as teasing, systematic discrimination, mistreatment, exclusion and chronic victimization, even more than children with other stigmatized attributes (Storch, 2007; Sweeting, 2005). Children have negative The emergence of social risks in childhood not allowed to eat or are restricted from eating certain harmful foods tend to eat those more and gain weight (Clark et al. 2007). At the other extreme, an excessive permissiveness or lack of supervision of children’s eating habits can lead to situations of persistent obesity and/or nutritional deficiencies (Brann and Skinner, 2005). Our research supports these findings. The risk of suffering from obesity between 5 and 10 years of age in Spain is related to the patterns of control and permissiveness of parents. In this regard, the father’s involvement can be crucial. Utilizing data from our survey, there is evidence that in the homes where the father participates intensely in caring for his children there is a lower probability of the children being obese. In contrast, children whose fathers are «traditional,» «pre-disposed,» or «uncommitted,» where only the mother or neither parent is involved in the child’s life, have a significantly higher risk of obesity, socioeconomic factors being equal.4 found in the study carried out in the United States. Children described by their parents as shy, sad and with a tendency to be fearful have a higher probability of being obese (see graph 6.4). Obesity in childhood can lead to an accumulation of adverse situations. Obese children have a greater probability of suffering from psychological problems such as depression and low selfesteem, as well as expressing dissatisfaction with their own bodies (Strauss, 2000; Storch et al. 2007; Haines and NeumarkSztainer, 2006). Moreover, they tend to be more withdrawn, to consider themselves bad students, to have lower educational expectations and to suffer a greater number of attempted suicides (Falkner et al. 2001). In previous studies carried out in the United States, we studied the direction of causality with longitudinal studies (Jackson and Argeseanu, 2010). With a sample of North American children, we found that (non-obese) children who have been diagnosed with depression have a higher risk of becoming obese three years after the initial diagnosis of depression, but children diagnosed with obesity were not more likely than nonobese children to develop depression in the same time period (Papadopoulos et al. 2010). These findings suggest that low socio-emotional competence and depression are at the origin of obesity and not the reverse. With the results from our Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood it is not possible to reconstruct the direction of causality given that this was not a longitudinal study. Symptoms of emotional distress and measurements of weight were recorded in a single moment in time. But the findings do reveal evidence of the same associations Children from 5 to 10 years of age 0,30 0,25 Graph 6.4: Probability of child being obese, by level of symptoms of depression PROBABILITY 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 MINIMUM LEVEL MAXIMUM LEVEL DEPRESSION SYMPTOMS INDEX Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) the child is obese; 0) child is not obese. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and level of child’s symptoms of depression. The depression symptoms index is the sum of parental responses of agreement or disagreement with the following statements about their child: «he/she is sometimes sad,» «she/he is sometimes afraid of things or people,» «he/she gets angry with others.» The index has values from 0 (disagrees with the statement) to 8 (strongly agrees with the statement). Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. To summarize, the analysis we carried out supports the idea that obesity is the product of genetic factors and socioeconomic probability The emergence of social risks in childhood attitudes towards obese children, and these negative feelings are held even by children who are themselves obese (Staffieri, 1967; Cramer and Steinwert, 1998). As children enter adolescence, parental influence on body-image weakens, and instead, the importance of peer opinion becomes stronger. In adolescence, weight becomes a major yardstick to measure self-worth. 6.2. Raising socially and emotionally competent children With the improvement in children’s health described at the beginning of this chapter, society’s attention to the well-being of children has expanded to include broader components beyond physical health, such as mental health and social adjustment. Happiness and sociability are common indicators of child wellbeing and are primary concerns for today’s parents, healthcare practitioners, and researchers. Many authors have stated that interactions with other people in childhood provide opportunities to develop skills such as social reasoning (Piaget, 1932; Rubin et al. 2009). In other words, in the context of socialization with a group of peers, children have opportunities to question their ideas, negotiate and discuss diverse points of view and decide which arguments they keep and which ones they reject. These experiences interacting with peers are positive for the development of adaptive behaviours, including among others, the ability to empathize with the thoughts and feelings of others. Appropriate social and emotional competencies gives us the ability to manage difficult situations that otherwise could lead to fights, anger, or offensive responses (Steiner, 1998). Along these lines, some authors in Spain have highlighted the importance of including socio-emotional education in the design of educational curricula in order to minimize discipline problems, violence, risk behaviours and prevent their appearance (Darder, 2001; Izquierdo, 2000). In addition, the latest research shows that psycho-social wellbeing is important not only for a healthy childhood but that it can also have long-term consequences. Children with emotional or behavioural problems are at higher risk of developing psychological and social problems in adulthood related to mental health, sexual relationships, education and employment, and they are more likely to engage in criminal activities or consume addictive substances (Fergusson et al. 2005). In this regard, the OECD has recently recognized the need to extend the attention dedicated to «cognitive» competencies (in studies such as the PISA) to also include other types of skills that favour the development of a balanced personality and that promote learning processes, such as assertiveness, self-motivation and the capacity to handle different levels of social relationships. Children restricting access to shared activities (i.e., the act of rejecting other children and refusing to let them join in play) is a consistent feature of social interaction among young children and The emergence of social risks in childhood inequalities, but also of other psycho-social processes that take place in the world in which the child lives. The dynamics of the social interaction between parents and their children as well as between children and their peers play a role in this. It is important to point out that our analysis did not find evidence that women working outside the home negatively affects the risk of obesity (other things being equal), but there is a correlation between the degree of parental involvement in the life of a child and the risk of obesity. With respect to this, it is important to note the role that new forms of co-responsible parenting can play in the prevention of obesity. We also did not find evidence that children who were enrolled in pre-school from 0-3 years of age or who are now in after school programmes have a higher risk of developing obesity than those who have not utilized these services. Children who are repeatedly refused admittance into shared activities are classified as “rejected children”, and repeated rejection may be due to, or may cause, either aggressive or withdrawn behavior. Children who are not rejected, but still do not take active part in social interaction, are called “neglected children” (Corsaro, 1997). Both rejected and neglected children are often at a disadvantage in developing friendships and social competence, with negative consequences lasting into adulthood (Parker, 1997). Many studies have confirmed the importance of socio-emotional competencies in childhood. For example, Gilliam (2005) showed that children with many socio-emotional problems in primary school have a three times greater risk of being expelled from school than those who do not have these problems. Initial disadvantages can have fairly long-range effects. Children who have been bullied in pre-school by other children have a greater probability of engaging in risky behaviours in adolescence and adulthood (Gagnon et al., 1995). Mahoney et al. (2003) found that social competence during pre-adolescence is associated with higher educational expectations in adolescence and higher educational achievement at the age of 20. Social and emotional competencies, which are manifested in the ability to interact positively with others, improve individuals’ employability. In personnel selection processes the evaluation of these types of qualities is increasingly more common with the objective of creating dynamics of mutual understanding among members of work teams and therefore contributing to the generation of a positive working environment. For the analysis of emotional and behavioural problems in childhood developed in this section, we have used an adapted version of Achenbach’s Child Behaviour Checklist (1992). In our survey parents were asked to what extent they agree with the following thirteen statements with respect to their children: he/ she likes spending time with other people; he/she gets into conflicts or fights; he/she likes to bother others; he/she likes to laugh; he/ she is sometimes sad; he/she sometimes feels alone; he/she is normally in a good mood; he/she loses control easily; he/she can’t be still; he/she is shy; he/she likes to try new things; he/she is sometimes afraid of things or of people; and he/she often gets angry with others. For each behaviour, the parents had three possible response choices; «strongly agree,» «partly agree,» «disagree.» In the literature, behaviours and attitudes similar to those listed above fall under the umbrella of socio-emotional competence (Rubin et al., 2009). Based on parental responses we created a scale in which the highest values indicate a greater tendency to manifest positive social and emotional behaviours. Although it would be equally as interesting to analyze the different attitudes and behaviours separately, this is beyond the possibilities of this study. Because of space considerations and to facilitate interpretation we have opted for an index to unify and analyze the responses. The index permits us to classify children into three groups, based on the scores obtained from the thirteen questions The emergence of social risks in childhood not necessarily problematic. It is when these antisocial behaviors become the norm that children may be experiencing a problem requiring intervention. Acquiring strategies to overcome rejection is essential for children. Strategies such as approaching a group, observing their common task or topic of conversation, and finding a way to appropriately insert oneself into the interaction are utilized and refined throughout childhood and adulthood. In the following pages, following the same schema as in previous sections, we will analyze the association between problems of socio-emotional competence and family characteristics and social environment. The tables in this section include the percentages in the first two columns and the odds ratio in the next two. The first odds ratio measures the direct association established between the index of socio-emotional competence and the characteristic being analyzed. Finally, in the last column we show the odds ratio adjusted for family socioeconomic conditions (that is, other things being equal, for parents’ educational level, employment status, the sex and age of the child and national origin). In this way, controlling for the influence which other variables may have, we can know what effects specific family characteristics have on socio-emotional competence. (a) Socioeconomic characteristics and household structure Some studies have suggested that there is a direct relationship between family socioeconomic conditions and levels of socioemotional development in children: in families with greater economic and educational resources children have greater social skills (Mahoney, 2003). It is argued that families with greater economic and educational resources are more likely to encourage their children to argue and defend their ideas, and in these families children are more accustomed to interacting with people with whom they do not share family ties than children from more disadvantaged social classes (Lareau, 2002). Perhaps parents in families with greater educational resources are better prepared to understand the emotional changes their children go through and to find solutions to problematic situations. It is also possible that this association between social class and socio-emotional competence can be attributed to that fact that children who live in homes in more precarious situations are exposed to a wider range of social risks and destabilizing factors which may affect the development of their competencies, such as family break-ups caused by separation or divorce, unemployment and economic insecurity (McLeod and Shanahan, 1996). Our data support this idea. Table 6.4 shows that the non-adjusted odds ratio of a child having low socio-emotional competence is lower in homes with a higher economic level than in those in more insecure economic situations, although the result is not sufficiently robust after controlling for other socioeconomic factors. The best predictor of socio-emotional competence is the educational level of parents. According to the results of our analysis, economic and employment conditions being equal, The emergence of social risks in childhood formulated. The children included in the group with «low socioemotional competence» are those who received scores at least one standard deviation below the mean, which includes 30 percent of the children; in other words, these are children whose parents responded that they strongly agreed with at least nine of the statements regarding problematic behaviours and attitudes. Another 40 percent of the children fall into the group of «medium level socio-emotional competence» formed by those who answered that their children manifested negative behaviours between five and eight times. Finally, 30 percent were in the group of «high socio-emotional competence,» that is, children whose parents responded that they disagreed when asked if their children were sometimes sad, angry with others, or shy and that they agreed that their children had positive behaviours (such as laughing or normally being in a good mood) in at least 9 out of the 13 behaviours analyzed. In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age LOW SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE MID SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE HIGH SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE Less than €1,200 38 34 28 From €1,201 to €2,000 31 45 24 0.73† 0.81 From €2,001 to €3,000 30 41 30 0.69† 0.84 From €3,001 to €5,000 21 42 37 0.44*** 0.66 More than €5,000 9.7 45 45 0.17* 0.31† Native 28 42 30 Immigrant 38 38 24 Does not work 32 40 28 1 1 Works part-time 31 40 29 0.97 1.11 Works full-time 25 44 31 0.72* 0.88 Primary 36 41 23 1 1 Secondary 31 40 29 0.78† 0.83 University 21 44 35 0.46*** 0.60* Does not work 34 39 26 1 1 Works part-time 34 44 22 0.98 0.97 SOCIOECONOMIC CHARACtERISTICS ODDS RATIO ADJUSTED ODDS RATIO(a) Household income 1 1 Parents’ origin 1 1.60** 1 1.67** Mother’s characteristics Employment status Education level Father’s characteristics Employment status The emergence of social risks in childhood TABLE 6.4: Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by socioeconomic characteristics of household Works full-time 27 42 31 0.72* 0.85 Primary 33 41 27 1 1 Secondary 31 42 27 0.86 0.98 University 20 42 38 0.49*** 0.69† Education level † Significance level of 10% * Significance level of 5% ** Significance level of 1% *** Significance level of 1‰ Note: a) Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, with respect to probability of having average or high socio-emotional competence, socioeconomic conditions being equal. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. there is lower risk of low socio-emotional competence in homes with greater educational resources; in other words, parents with higher levels of education tend to raise more sociable children, less prone to have behavioural problems (such as getting into fights) or attitudinal problems (sadness, shyness). Economic resources or the employment status of parents are no longer significant, educational conditions being equal. It is interesting to note that the employment status of parents does not seem to significantly affect the level of socio-emotional competence. Concretely, it is striking that the children of mothers who work full-time are at lower risk for socio-emotional problems than those of mothers who do not work, although the effect is not significant after controlling for other socioeconomic factors. Children of immigrant origin also face additional difficulties related to socio-emotional competence. The percentage of children with low socio-emotional competence is higher in the homes of the foreign population than in Spanish homes (38 percent and 28 percent, respectively). The adverse economic situations experienced in these homes could partially explain this difference (Marí-Klose et al., 2008ª). It should be noted, however, that the disadvantage of children of immigrant origin is maintained even when they are compared to children who come from families in similar socioeconomic conditions. This finding is consistent with the evidence gathered in other countries with significant levels of immigration. It is probably symptomatic of the great effort required to adapt to new social norms, and often a new language. Some children of foreign origin might have gone through the migratory process themselves, which, even if this occurs at an early age, tends to have a destabilizing effect. Along with the difficulties involved in integration when there are cultural differences, changing one’s country brings among other things changes in routines, separation from family and friends, and on occasion, having to face ridicule, hostility or exclusion from other The emergence of social risks in childhood (Continue) Table 6.5 shows the relationship between socio-emotional competence and factors related to the structure in the home. 35 percent of the children who live in single-parent homes fall into the category of low socio-emotional competence, compared to 29 percent of those who live in two parent homes. But the differences are not statistically significant (probably because of the low representation of single-parent families in the sample). Neither are the differences significant between homes in which the child has siblings and those where he/she does not, or between homes where the child lives with grandparents and those where he/she does not. TABLe 6.5: Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by composition of household In percentages. Children from 5 to 10 years of age COMPOSITION OF HOUSEHOLD LOW SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE MID SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE HIGH SOCIOEMOTIONAL COMPETENCE ODDS RATIO(b) ADJUSTED ODDS RATIO(c) Household structure Two parents 29 42 30 1 1 Single-parent 35 41 24 1.34 1.19 Do not reside in the home(a) 29 45 26 1 1 Reside in the home 29 40 31 0.98 1.02 Do not reside in the home 29 42 30 1 1 Reside in the home 34 39 27 1.29 1.16 Siblings Grandparents Note: a) The category «do not reside» combines the cases in which the child does not have siblings and those in which he/she does, but they do not reside in the home. b) Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, with respect to probability of having average or high socio-emotional competence. c) Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, with respect to probability of having average or high socio-emotional competence, socioeconomic conditions being equal. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The emergence of social risks in childhood children. There is abundant research about immigrant children and adolescents in the United States and other countries with a longer migratory history than Spain, which describes these destabilizing effects in what has been called the 1.5 generation– children of immigrant origin who arrive in another country at an early age (Suarez-Orozco and Todorova, 2008; Kasinitz et al., 2008). As important or more important than socioeconomic conditions in the household are interpersonal interactions. The family atmosphere in which the child grows up is determined to a great extent by the interpersonal relationships between parents and children (shaped in turn by different ways of fathering and mothering) and the interpersonal relationships between the parents. In this regard, situations of tension can negatively affect the emotional development of the child (Anthony et al., 2005; Deater-Deckard, 1998). We will begin our analysis by examining the influence of fatherhood on children’s level of socio-emotional competence. Table 6.6 shows that children who live in homes where the fathers are less involved in their lives (with «adaptive» and «traditional» forms of fatherhood, and in the remaining category where both mothers and fathers are not very involved in children’s lives) tend to have lower levels of socio-emotional competence than those in homes where the fathers exercise «intense» or «responsible» fatherhood. The effect is not very robust after controlling for socioeconomic factors but the anticipated direction remains. TABLE 6.6: Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by parenting styles In percentages: Children from 5 to 10 years of age LOW SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE MID SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE HIGH SOCIO-EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE Intense 23 44 33 1 1 Responsible 27 40 32 1.28 1.24 Adaptive 34 37 29 1.79* 1.68† Traditional 36 42 22 1.88* 1.64† Predisposed 22 48 30 0.97 0.91 Uncommitted 39 41 20 2.13* 1.84 TYPES OF FATHERHOOD ODDS RATIO † Significance level of 10% * Significance level of 5% Note: a) Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, with respect to probability of having average or high socio-emotional competence, socioeconomic conditions being equal. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. ADJUSTED ODDS RATIO(a) The emergence of social risks in childhood (b) Parenting styles and interpersonal relationships in the home Graph 6.5: Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, by degree of exposure to cognitively stimulating activities Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.40 0.35 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 NO COGNITIVE STIMULATION LOW MID MID-HIGH HIGH COGNITIVE STIMULATION DEGREE OF EXPOSURE TO COGNITIVELY STIMULATING ACTIVITIES Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) the child has low socio-emotional competence; 0) child does not have low socio-emotional competence. See detailed definition on pages 158-159. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and degree of exposure to cognitively stimulating activities. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. between cognitive stimulation and socio-emotional capacity also remains significant, socioeconomic conditions in the home being equal. There are two elements related to the interaction between parents and children that have been shown to be key in explaining low socio-emotional competence in children: the use of sanctions (such as the threat of punishment, giving them time to reflect on their actions, punishing them by different forms of deprivation, yelling at them or spanking them) and level of stress that is felt by parents. As can be seen in the following graphs, the use of sanctions is clearly associated with low socio-emotional competence. The use of positive reinforcement (congratulating the child for things done well) reveals no relationship to the probability of having low socio-emotional competence (although, significantly, there does exist a positive correlation with the likelihood of high socioemotional competence). In contrast, the relationship between the frequency of sanctions and low socio-emotional capacities remains for different disciplinary methods. Evidently, the data must be interpreted with caution because we cannot disentangle the direction of causality. We can appeal here to other research on these issues. Studies carried out in other countries with longitudinal samples support the idea that the abuse of disciplinary methods is at the origin of emotional and behavioural problems, and they minimize the importance of the alternative hypothesis (Gershoff, 2002; Grogan-Kaylor, 2005). The emergence of social risks in childhood One interesting finding is that the benefits of cognitive stimulation activities described in chapter 3 are not exclusively cognitive but also social and emotional. As can be seen in graph 6.5, parents who do cognitive stimulating activities with their children (reading them stories and doing crafts with them) have children who are less vulnerable from a socio-emotional perspective. The relationship Graph 6.7: Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, competence, by frequency of use of rewards and punishments by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes Children from 5 to 10 years of age Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY PROBABILITY 0.70 0.60 0.60 0.50 0.50 0.40 0.40 0.30 0.30 0.20 0.20 0.10 0.10 0.00 0.00 NEVER OCCASIONALLY Raise voice or shout at child Punish child (has to stay in his/her room, not allowed to watch TV,use computer, etc) Congratulate child on doing things well ALMOST DAILY NEVER DAILY Threaten to punish child Spank him/her Give child time to reflect on what he/she has done Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) The child has low socio-emotional competence; 0) child does not have low socio-emotional competence. See detailed definition on pages 116-118. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and frequency of use of positive and negative reinforcement. There is insufficient data to estímate the probability that a child will have low socio-emotional competence when his/her parents spank him/her daily or almost daily. OCCASIONALLY OFTEN FREQUENCY OF TENSION IN THE HOME Tension over division of domestic responsibilities Tension due to economic difficulties Tension caused by job stress The emergence of social risks in childhood Graph 6.6: Probability of child having low socio-emotional Tension over child care Tension due to not having time to relax Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) The child has low socio-emotional competence; 0) child does not have low socio-emotional competence. See detailed definition on pages 116-118. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and frequency of tension due to different causes. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Interactions between parents and children are part of a wider which these situations are produced (in different modalities) and network of relationships in which there can be situations of tension the socio-emotional competence of the child. The data show that caused by different social and environmental circumstances. independently of the socioeconomic conditions of the child and 0,6 Graph 6.7 describes the relationship between the frequency withreflexionar family, said tensions affect the development of these competencies. Tensión por estrés e Le hace 0,5 Le felicita Tensión por no dispo 0,4 Le da un cachete Tensión por dificulta 6.3. The origins of school disengagement The third and final aspect related to well-being in childhood that we will analyze is interest in studying and being in school or its opposite, the processes leading to school disengagement and difficulties with staying in school. Interest in studying and involvement in school (engagement) have been defined as consisting of a constellation of interrelated attitudes and behaviours that promote learning in an academic environment (Marks, 2000; Newmann et al., 1992). School engagement requires concentration, dedication and effort. In contrast, school disengagement involves a lack of attention, little interest and limited investment. The problems of school disengagement limit both the capacity to learn and educational performance. School engagement is central for bringing about academic success since it fosters the achievement of academic goals and prevents dropping out (Newmann et al., 1992; Rumberger, 1987). Students tend to obtain better marks when they show an interest in educational tasks (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2000). Students who show high levels of commitment to school tend to learn more, be more satisfied with the time spent in school and continue to study after ending their compulsory education (Marks, 2000). School disengagement, on the other hand, has been linked to learning delays and difficulties in keeping up with the prescribed academic rhythm, which in turn can lead to a spiral that ends with the student dropping out (Finn, 1989; Marks, 2000; in Spain, see Fernández-Enguita, 2010). Disengagement can begin at an early age and can condition the educational development of the child. For example, Alexander et al. (1997) related lack of school engagement in the first year of primary school to the decision to drop out in secondary school. There are also studies which have found an association between school engagement and other dimensions in children’s trajectories. For example, Manlove (1998) found evidence that there is a lower incidence of adolescent pregnancy among students with high levels of school engagement. Helping children to develop an interest in their studies and to identify with educational goals is without a doubt a challenge for educators, parents and policy makers. Although the symptoms of disengagement that have been most analyzed (absenteeism, grade retention, dropping out) usually occur later on in school, the process that leads to this is forged during the first years of schooling. Understanding the nature of the determinants of school engagement as well as the contextual influences and the interpersonal relationships involved is imperative in order to detect and prevent problems caused when engagement is lacking. Although dropping out of school and educational performance have been the object of numerous studies, the problems related to disengagement have not received sufficient attention. Our Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood, which has a section dedicated to the education of children from 5 to 10 years old, offers us a rare opportunity to look closely at that stage in which boys and girls may begin to disconnect from school and at a time when their destiny has not yet been written. At this earlier stage, problems with concentration, lack of interest The emergence of social risks in childhood In homes where situations of tension occur often, the probability of the child having low levels of socio-emotional competence is higher. A significant number of parents in our survey revealed problems of disengagement among their children: between 15 percent and 30 percent, depending on the indicator used and the age and sex of the child. Our analysis has confirmed research carried out in other countries which shows that at all stages of compulsory education, boys suffer higher levels of disengagement than girls (Lee and Smith, 1994). In 31 percent of homes, the parents interviewed indicated that the teacher had commented that their sons had problems with concentration, but only 19 percent said the same thing about their daughters; 27.1 percent of parents said that their sons did not like studying, whereas 18.7 percent said the same about their daughters. Finally, 19.5 percent of boys versus 15.7 percent of girls had problems following certain material or a certain subject in school (see graph 6.9). Earlier studies have also pointed out that disengagement increases as children get older (Jacobs et al., 2002). Our data partially corroborate this. As can be seen in graph 6.10, there is no linear relationship between age and disengagement for the different components analyzed. The percentage of children who do not like to study or who have problems with certain material or a subject is low before eight years of age (when the amount of educational material is still low), and somewhat higher later on, but the prevalence of problems with concentration reveals more erratic behaviour. Graph 6.8: Children with problems of disengagement from school, by sex In percentages: Children from 6 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 PROBLEMS CONCENTRATING (a) DON’T LIKE TO STUDY Boys PROBLEMS KEEPING UP WITH SUBJECT IN SCHOOL Girls Note: a) Children from 5 to 10 years of age. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The emergence of social risks in childhood or difficulties in keeping up with subjects in school can condition students’ progress, but they still do not constitute determining factors for the future. Even with these problems, some students are still able to get good marks (Martinez et al., 2004). Studying school engagement or the problems of disengagement makes it possible to analyze the educational process from a new focus, affects children’s lives and well-being beyond their educational implications. Based on the responses given by parents, we selected three indicators of school disengagement at early stages in children’s education trajectory: a) «the teacher has commented that the child has problems with certain material or a subject in school»; b) «the teacher has said that the child has problems with concentration or paying attention»; and c) «the child does not like to study». These responses capture different dimensions: the academic dimension (a) as well as others of a more psychological or attitudinal nature (b and c). by age In percentages: Children from 5 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 5 YEARS Problems concentrating 6 YEARS 7 YEARS 8 YEARS Don’t like to study 9 YEARS 10 YEARS Problems keeping up with subjects in school Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. (a) Socioeconomic characteristics and household structure The level of engagement children have in school can be considered the result of a combination of natural inclinations and predispositions, the satisfaction (or dissatisfaction) their educational experiences bring them, and the expected returns they hope to gain in the future (Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). Parents, through the 30attention and stimulation they provide their children, play a very important role in the formation of their children’s preferences and 25 attitudes. Parental influence is manifested in different ways. The 20 15 most studied is the association between the socioeconomic level of the parents and the level of engagement or disengagement of the children (Lee and Smith, 1994). This relationship is consistent with a theory of educational attainment, according to which parents’ education is the main predictor of the educational attainment of children (Sewell and Shah, 1968). Much subsequent research has traced the mechanisms responsible for producing this relationship. The findings of the Survey on Inter and Intragenerational Relationships in Childhood confirm the relationship between parents’ education and children’s interest in studying and engagement in school. Data unequivocally suggest that problems with concentration are less common among children whose parents have high levels of education: 29 percent of all the parents with a primary school level education said that the teacher had told them their son or daughter had problems with concentration, while this had occurred with only 19 percent of the parents who had a university degree. It is also more common to find children whose teachers say they have problems keeping up with a subject coming from families with lower educational levels: in families where the parents only had a primary school level education, the odds ratio for having this type of problem is four times greater than if at least one of the parents had university studies (other socioeconomic conditions being equal). Among children of parents with secondary school education, the odds ratio is twice as high. These findings remain statistically robust even when adjusted for different socioeconomic factors in the household (participation in the labour market and origin of the parents, as well as age and sex of the child). The results indicate that the influence the level ofparents’ education has on disengagement is not simply the product of the The emergence of social risks in childhood Graph 6.9: Children with problems of disengagement from school, age are probably better prepared to maintain their attention and adapt to the learning dynamics in school than those children who have not had those kinds of experiences. The third indicator analyzed (if the child likes studying) has a weaker level of association with the educational level of the parents. Even so, it can be seen in table 6.7 that in households with higher levels of education, the children are less likely to demonstrate TABLE 6.7: School disengagement by parents’ education level In percentages. Children from 6 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE ODDS RATIO(a) adjusted odds RATIO(b) Mother’s education level Has problems concentrating(c) Primary 29 1 1 Secondary 28 0.97 0.91 University 19 0.59** 0.57** Has problems keeping up with subject Primary 30 1 1 Secondary 16 0.45*** 0.46*** University 10 0.26*** 0.28*** Primary 27 1 1 Secondary 24 0.89 0.88 University 18 0.61* 0.60* Does not like to study The emergence of social risks in childhood unequal distribution of the material and social conditions which produced the educational credentials of the parents. As we have seen in previous chapters, parents with higher levels of education tend to dedicate more quality time to their children, participate more often in cognitive stimulation activities (and when the children are at an earlier age), and understand better the educational value of after school programmes and choose them accordingly. Children who have benefitted from after school programmes from an early (Continue) Has problems concentrating Primary 29 1 1 Secondary 26 0.80 0.80 University 18 0.52*** 0.63* Primary 25 1 Secondary 17 0.63** 0.88 University 9.0 0.30*** 0.52* Primary 27 1 Secondary 21 0.70* 0.77 University 19 0.59** 0.69 Has problems keeping up with subject 1 Does not like to study 1 * Significance level of 5% ** Significance level of 1% *** Significance level of 1‰ Note: a) Probability of child having some problems with school disengagement, with respect to probability of not having problems. b) Probability of child having some problems with school disengagement, with respect to probability of not having problems, socioeconomic conditions being equal. c) Children from 5 to 10 years of age. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. negative attitudes toward studying. It is to be expected that the families in which the parents have spent more years in school would place greater value on studying and would tend to put more emphasis on instilling this value in their children from an early age. However, the fact that this variable has a weaker relationship to the parents’ education points to the possibility that this dimension of school engagement is more difficult to transmit and mould at this age, and perhaps responds to deeper personality traits of the child. Table 6.8 provides information on the influence of the parents’ employment situation on the different aspects of school engagement examined. As in the preceding table, we offer the results for mothers and fathers separately. Some of the The emergence of social risks in childhood Father’s education level situations of tension and uncertainty that have repercussions on the well-being of the children. More striking is the effect of women working. In the households where the mother works full-time the children tend to have fewer problems of disengagement. The effect is not very robust, but it helps to shed light on the supposed negative consequences of women’s participation in the labour market. TABLE 6.8: School disengagement by parents’ employment status In percentages, Children from 6 to 10 years of age PERCENTAGE ODDS RATIO(a) adjusted odds RATIO(b) Mother’s employment status Has problems concentrating(c) Does not work 26 1 1 Works part-time 29 1.18 1.33 Works full-time 23 0.87 1.06 Does not work 24 1 1 Works part-time 15 0.61* 0.72 Works full-time 14 0.58** 0.87 Does not work 24 1 1 Works part-time 22 0.88 0.96 Works full-time 22 0.90 1.06 Has problems keeping up with subject Does not like to study The emergence of social risks in childhood findings are to a certain extent counterintuitive; others are more predictable. Among these, it should be highlighted that in homes where the father does not work the children tend to have more problems, especially regarding difficulties with a school subject, other conditions being equal. The experience of a father being unemployed or economically inactive can be a destabilizing element in the family, given that it often leads to (Continue) Has problems concentrating Does not work 30 1 1 Works part-time 27 0.75 0.90 Works full-time 24 0.87 0.78 Does not work 23 1 1 Works part-time 27 1.17 1.22 Works full-time 17 0.53** 0.62* Does not work 28 1 Works part-time 12 0.36* 0.38* Works full-time 22 0.75 0.78 Has problems keeping up with subject Does not like to study 1 * Significance level of 5% ** Significance level of 1% Note: a) Probability of child having some problems with school disengagement, with respect to probability of not having problems. b) Probability of child having some problems with school disengagement, with respect to probability of not having problems, socioeconomic conditions being equal. c) Problems with concentration calculated with children from 5 to 10 years of age. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Finally, it should be mentioned that a slightly higher proportion of children of immigrant origin have problems keeping up with a subject or material in school. 22.6 percent of the parents of these children said the teacher had commented that their children had these difficulties, while this was the case with 16.7 percent of children of Spanish origin. This difference is statistically significant when other socioeconomic factors are controlled for. No significant differences are observed, however, in the probability of children of immigrant origin having problems with concentration or not liking to study. (b) Parenting styles and involvement Parental involvement and monitoring of their children in school have repeatedly been identified as crucial in determining the adolescents’ educational results. Starting with the first studies The emergence of social risks in childhood Father’s employment status Graph 6.10: Probability of child having problems keeping up with a school subject, by degree of parental involvement Children from 6 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.50 0.45 0.40 0.35 0.30 Parental involvement in the educational and psychological development of their children could be considered one of the primary responsibilities of parenting. However, not all forms and levels of involvement have the same effect. Graph 6.11 illustrates the effect of parental involvement on the probability of a child between 6 and 10 years of age having problems with a subject or certain material in school, sociodemographic conditions being equal. It is easy to see that the lower the level of parental involvement (especially of the mother), the greater the risk of suffering from these problems. The degree of parental involvement also has an effect on the probability of the child not liking to study (graph 6.2). In contrast, we have not detected a significant effect from parental involvement on the probability of the child having problems with concentration. 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 LITTLE OR NOT INVOLVED MODERATELY INVOLVED VERY INVOLVED DEGREE OF INVOLVEMENT Mother Father Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) Teacher has said that child has difficulty keeping up with a subject; 0) Teacher has not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and degree of parents’ involvement. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. A second aspect that was analyzed was the influence the different forms of fatherhood identified in chapter 4 have on school disengagement. The findings again support the importance of the father’s involvement in the prevention of disengagement problems. Children whose fathers practice «intense fatherhood» or «responsible fatherhood» tend to have less difficulty in following a subject or material in school. Other conditions being equal, The emergence of social risks in childhood done by Coleman (1988), several authors have suggested that school performance depends to a great extent on family characteristics, questioning the explanatory capacity of school factors. Although these approaches may overestimate the influence of family factors, it seems reasonable to think that the behaviours and attitudes of mothers and fathers, the values they transmit, and their styles of parenting are decisive, particularly at early ages, when parents exercise a higher level of control on their children’s activities. Graph 6.11: Probability of child not liking to study, by degree of parental involvement Children from 6 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY The influence of the family on children’s attitude toward school extends to other areas as well. Thus, we find that the quality of the couple’s relationship and the tensions stemming from work or economic difficulties can also affect the atmosphere in the home and as a result, the well-being of the child. Our study analyzed the influence of various types of tension on the risk of school disengagement: division of domestic responsibilities, care of the children, economic difficulties, and lack of personal time to relax or disconnect. Graph 6.12: Probability of child having problems keeping up with 0.50 a subject, by types of fatherhood 0.45 0.40 Children from 6 to 10 years of age 0.35 0.30 PROBABILITY 0.25 0.30 0.20 0.15 0.25 0.10 0.20 0.05 0.00 LITTLE OR NOT INVOLVED MODERATELY INVOLVED VERY INVOLVED DEGREE OF INVOLVEMENT Mother Father 0.15 0.10 0.05 Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) Child has said he/she does not like to study; 0) Child has not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and degree of parents’ involvement. 0.00 INTENSE RESPONSIBLE ADAPTIVE TRADTIONAL PREDISPOSED UNCOMMITTED TYPES OF FATHERHOOD Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) Teacher has said that child has difficulty keeping up with a subject; 0) Teacher has not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and type of fatherhood. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. The emergence of social risks in childhood the risks are greater in traditional families in which the mother is involved in childcare and the father is not, and in households in which both parents have a low level of commitment. by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes Children from 5 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY 0.40 0.35 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 Graphs 6.14, 6.15 and 6.16 show that there is a positive relationship between the probability of having problems of disengagement and frequent tension in the home. This relationship is maintained, socioeconomic conditions in the home and characteristics of the child (parents’ level of education, sex and age of child) being equal. The form of disengagement that seems to be most closely linked to tensions in the home is difficulty in keeping up with a subject in school, which is specifically closely linked to tensions caused by distribution of childcare responsibilities. Problems with concentration are more related to tensions resulting from employment-related stress. These findings support the hypothesis that family tension, whether or not related to the children, directly affect children’s engagement with school. 0.05 0.00 NEVER OCCASIONALLY OFTEN FREQUENCY OF TENSION IN THE HOME Tension over division of domestic responsibilities Tension due to economic difficulties Tension caused by job stress Tension over child care Tension due to not having time to relax Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) The teacher has said the child has problems concentrating; 0) the teacherhas not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and frequency of tension in the home due to different causes. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. Tensión por estrés en el trabajo Tensión por no disponer de tiempo personal para relajarse Tensión por dificultades económicas The emergence of social risks in childhood Graph 6.13: Probability of child having problems concentrating, Graph 6.15: Probability of child saying he/she does not like to study, subject, by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes Children from 6 to 10 years of age Children from 6 to 10 years of age PROBABILITY PROBABILITY 0.55 0.45 0.50 0.40 0.45 0.35 0.40 0.30 0.35 0.30 0.25 0.25 0.20 0.20 0.15 0.15 0.10 0.10 0.05 0.05 0.00 0.00 NEVER OCCASIONALLY OFTEN NEVER FREQUENCY OF TENSION IN THE HOME Tension over division of domestic responsibilities Tension due to economic difficulties Tension caused by job stress OCCASIONALLY OFTEN FREQUENCY OF TENSION IN THE HOME Tension over child care Tension due to not having time to relax Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) The teacher has said the child has problems keeping up with a subject; 0) The teacher has not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and frequency of tension in the home due to different causes. Tension over division of domestic responsibilities Tension due to economic difficulties Tension caused by job stress The emergence of social risks in childhood Graph 6.14: Probability of child having problems keeping up with a Tension over childcare Tension due to not having time to relax Note: Probability is calculated based on a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable has two values: 1) The child has said he/she doesn’t like to study; 0) The child has not said this. The following variables have been introduced simultaneously into the model: age of child, sex of child, origin of parents, mother’s education level, mother’s working day and frequency of tension in the home due to different causes. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Childhood and Inter and Intragenerational Relationships, 2010. Source: Based on data from the Survey on Inter and Intra-generational Relationships in Childhood, 2010. 0,45 0,40 0,35 0,30 Tensión por estrés en el trabajo Tensión por estrés en Tensión por no disponer de tiempo personal para relajarse Tensión por no dispo Tensión por dificultades económicas Tensión por dificultad The emergence of social risks in childhood In short, the findings of our analysis demonstrate the powerful influence of intergenerational relationships on school disengagement observed at early ages. Evidence such as this suggests the need to focus our attention in dealing with school failure and dropping out on earlier stages of childhood, which are often ignored in analyses of educational problems. The gap between educational success and failure begins to appear before the problem is fully expressed in adolescence. In light of the evidence, there seems to be little doubt that the setting where things start to go wrong is in the family home. CONCLUSION CONCLUSION Our society has an ambivalent attitude toward children: on the one hand, they are seen as fragile and innocent, needing to be cared for and protected and on the other, they are out of control (even perverse) little ones who must be socialized or they will end up wayward and failures– they are angels or demons. Historically, the function of caring for children and protecting them has been the domain of the family, and that of educating them, the domain of the school. No one could better protect children than their own parents. The state should intervene only as a last resort, when the family had clearly shown itself to be negligent, incapable of attending to the basic needs of the child or egregiously abusive. In contrast, with respect to education, most states have had the primary responsibility for educating children for more than a century. Having well educated children who can adequately exercise their citizenship is for the state too important a goal to leave in the hands of adults who are often incapable of maintaining their own lives in order. Schools (and compulsory education) symbolize this public interest in controlling the education of the citizenry. However, the basis of the relationship among children, families and the state has changed in recent years. Gradually states have colonized spaces of protection traditionally reserved for families, but that families–immersed in a process of dramatic transformation–are incapable of handling effectively. At the same time, many families have become increasingly more willing to hand over certain aspects of protection. More and more families believe that state aid to families must be increased both in terms of economic support as well as childcare services. Families recognize that they are no longer the self-sufficient entities they may once have been and need public support in order to effectively carry out their role. In this process, certain traditional beliefs have disappeared (at least among the majority of the population)– such as the belief that a child must be raised by the mother (or grandmother)–and prejudices regarding the participation of fathers or childcare professionals have been overcome. Paradoxically, however, it has been the absence of children (or at least their scarcity) that has led to them, and therefore the families who decide to have them or not, becoming an object of public concern. As we have seen in the previous pages, having children and raising them is a rewarding experience. They enrich parents’ lives, contributing to their self-realization. But there is also a The findings of our study reveal that what parents have to give up is not something that is evenly distributed throughout the population. Children continue to be taken care of primarily by mothers, who, as a result, are the ones who make the sacrifices that dedication to children demands. Some mothers leave the workforce, reduce their work hours, or change jobs in order to take care of their children. But in contrast to what happened in the past, there are fewer mothers willing to definitively give up their career aspirations. We find ourselves before a new generation of mothers, the majority of whom work while taking care of their children; in the first place because they want to, and secondly because they feel they have to (in order to have a sufficient income to sustain their families, to have financial security in their old-age, or to face the consequences of divorce). Our research has sought to highlight the extent to which this radical change in the architecture of family relationships has been accompanied by other transformations (what is known in statistics as intervening variables), which have increased or mitigated the implications of this change in the lives of children. In this regard, the initial conclusion of our research contradicts some of the quasi-apocalyptic expectations about the consequences of this social shift. The evidence we have presented - and which corroborates research done in other countries - clearly shows that households where the mothers work are not cold heartless places, where children spend long hours attended by substitute mothers who are less committed to their care or alone waiting for their absent parents. Families do often rely on substitute mothers, generally someone from the close family circle (many times grand mothers), who gives a dose of dedication and commitment equal to what would be expected of a mother. For example, 55 percent of the families interviewed with children between 0 and 2 years old had resorted to someone who was not living in the home to take care of their child in the previous month. But this was not always possible and, perhaps, increasingly less so. For this reason, families must often find other solutions, without these shaking the foundations of a good upbringing for their child. In fact, in light of the data, there is some great news. In the first place, we must welcome with optimism the increasing commitment of fathers to children taking place in recent years. The changes observed in the attitudes of men toward the division of gender responsibilities, partially endorsed in daily practice in the home, is contributing to forging new ways of understanding masculinity. Some of the greatest progress has been made in changes in the exercise of fatherhood. Our study provides evidence of some of the benefits of these new forms of fatherhood. The beneficiaries of this change are the partners, the atmosphere in the family, and ultimately the children. With respect to the first, mothers, the analysis indicates that fathers who show a greater level of involvement with their children help alleviate to a great CONCLUSION personal cost to raising children, which is sometimes quite high. For this reason, a growing number of couples do not have children or decide not to have a second or third child, or in other cases wait to have children until the burden of costs no longer affects other aspects of their personal well-being that they consider to be important. Even so, on the terrain of sentimental relationships individuals are rarely guided by cost benefit analysis. The vast majority of couples end up having children and assuming the responsibility that this entails, often in adverse circumstances. In such situations, the proper care of children necessitates giving up certain things. Secondly, there is no reason for alarm regarding the extraordinary increase in the number of families relying on professional childcare services. The substitution of a system which rested on the sacrifice of mothers and grandmothers by another one in which the children are taken care of by childcare professional does not necessarily have negative repercussions for the well-being of children, as long as those services have high quality standards and are accessible to disadvantaged groups. On the one hand, it is a mistake to assume that care provided by a mother who does not work or a grandmother substituting for her guarantees in all cases the best development of the child. The analysis presented in this book provides evidence of advantages (or at least the absence of disadvantages) in indicators of well-being among the children of women who work full-time, other socioeconomic conditions being equal. It also shows that grandmothers do not always provide the best care for their grandchildren, as is revealed, for example, in the association between the presence of a grandmother in the home and a higher risk of obesity. Moreover, a large volume of research has shown that attending a day-care centre can have a beneficial effect on children’s cognitive and social development, especially if they come from disadvantaged environments. In our analysis of the impact attending a day-care centre has on indicators of health and childhood well-being, we did not detect significant differences among children between 5 and 10 years old who had attended or had not attended day-care. Nine out of ten mothers and fathers who placed their children in day-care expressed satisfaction with the services received, although it must also be added that there was reasonable suspicion that a minority of day-care centres–between 10 and 20 percent–provide services in need of improvement. Approximately three out of four parents thought that the day-care centres offered opportunities for the children to develop cognitive and social competencies that either could not be developed at home or that could complement those learned in the family. Based on the data examined in this study, there are no reasons to question this. CONCLUSION extent the anxiety and guilt felt by mothers who work because of their not being able to dedicate more time to their children. If a woman who works full-time has a partner who does not dedicate enough time to the children, it is 3.4 times more likely that she will feel she does not dedicate enough time to the children herself than if her partner’s involvement is greater (other conditions being equal). Regarding the benefits to the atmosphere in the family, men being co-responsible is essential in order to prevent situations of tension derived from the division of housework and care of the children in the home. For example, in the homes where both parents work, the probability of tensions arising over caring for the children is 52 percent higher when only the mother is very involved than when both parents are fully co-responsible. If this were not enough, the results of the statistical analysis point to the time dedication of intense and responsible fathers having positive repercussions on the health of the child, on his or her socio-emotional competence, and on his or her engagement with school. The probability of a 5 to 10 year old having problems of obesity is, other conditions being equal, almost two times greater when the father does not dedicate enough time than when his degree of commitment is intense. The magnitude of the observed gap is similar when we look at the probability of the child having problems keeping up with a subject in school in relation to the degree of dedication of the father. Clearly, these findings do not support the hypothesis that children today are becoming a generation at risk. However, as we warned in the introduction, not all is rosy. In the context of an ageing population, children who are in situations of vulnerability are the forgotten ones in systems of public protection. Child poverty rates and indicators of educational attainment in Spain are, in general, bad. Some of the groups most vulnerable are those which have gained the most demographic weight in recent years as a result of the great social changes taking place in Spain, such as children living in single-parent families and those of immigrant origin. Of particular concern is the situation of immigrant children, who systematically are ranked highest regarding vulnerability in terms of poverty level, obesity, socio-emotional competence and school failure. Approximately four out of ten children of immigrant origin under 10 years of age live in situations of poverty; 21 percent of 5 to 10 year olds in this group have problems with obesity, 38 percent have low levels of socio-emotional competence and 23 percent have problems keeping up with a subject in school. The accumulation of situations of risk and adversity in childhood threatens to open a social divide which will be difficult to reverse. Faced with this situation it becomes urgent to ask what systems of service provision–which begin with the work of government, but also increasingly involve a host of different agents engaged in these services– can do to address new forms of social exclusion that begin in childhood and condition individuals’ life course. This question is of great relevance at a time in which the outlook ahead of an ageing population and the challenges this presents threaten to eclipse discussion about the needs and demands of groups in the early stages of life. Now more than ever it we need to reflect on the measures necessary to promote the «best interest of the child». This book cannot offer a prescription for how to do this, but with the arguments presented–based on our research and the evidence accumulated in similar studies in Spain and other countries–it aspires to serve as a guide for concrete social action. In this regard, it should be noted that: 1.Mothers’ paid work is crucial to preventing situations of economic exclusion in childhood, especially among the more vulnerable groups. Promoting women working implies creating conditions that favour this, especially among mothers whose opportunity costs from working (instead of staying at home and taking care of their children) are higher. Unequal access to childcare centres, detected in the present CONCLUSION Similarly, there is no reason to be alarmed over the participation of children between 5 and 10 years old in organized after school activities. According to the responses of their parents, children tend to be very or quite satisfied with the after school programmes they attend. Although the possibility cannot be ruled out that parents’ responses reflects a certain amount of self-deception, two out of three parents stated that the decision to put their children in after school programmes was «very» or «quite» influenced by what their children wanted; a minority said that the decision was based on the necessity of reconciling work and family life (slightly less than 25 percent of parents said that their work hours were very important or quite important in the decision). In households today, children are not passive subjects of decisions made unilaterally by parents and without consultation. Children value the opportunity for learning and entertainment provided in participating in these activities and their evaluation, in the context of a growing recognition of the individuality of the child, is a factor that parents do not ignore. have become irreversible. The return to a world where women assumed the role of taking care of and educating the children is blocked and any attempt to return to it (even partially) would not be productive and possibly even counterproductive for children. Consequently, it is necessary to make known the advantages of male involvement in these responsibilities, not only to improve the family atmosphere in homes where both parents work, but also in the interest of the general well-being of children. Legislative initiatives should aim to support shared responsibility among parents from the moment a child is born, eliminating rights founded on sexist assumptions about which parent is best able to be in charge of the child at different stages of his or her life. Beyond these initiatives, it is important to complement the extension of rights and opportunities of men, encouraging a culture of co-responsibility which reinforces their disposition to be involved in the care and education of their children. 2.The dedication of mothers and fathers to their children is a very important investment which favours the present wellbeing and the social and cognitive development of children. As we have been able to show, this dedication is not simply expressed in terms of «quantity of time,» but has more to do with how mothers and fathers use this time. It is clear, however, that parental commitment is not feasible if parents feel «overwhelmed.» The existence of intergenerational shared time does not depend merely on the length of the work day, but also that fathers and mothers’ work schedules are predictable and regular and that shared time can be really experienced as «family time,» without the distorting factor of overtime or parents bringing work home. Having dinner together or other family rituals, such as watching television together or spending time together outdoors on the weekend, are important links for intergenerational bonding that need to be protected from the interference of the working world. The starting point of «reconciliation policies» is to promote mothers and fathers and children having time in those moments that they need it (as in the Anglo-Saxon expression, having time at the right time). This implies the expansion of new rights and labour practices that promote the possibility of workers being able to reorganize their work schedules to meet the needs of the family. 4.Health in childhood is sine qua non for the equality of opportunity in life. Child health has social determinants which must be considered and addressed at an early age (some even from birth). Phenomena such as obesity, responsible for a large number of diseases and health problems in adulthood, have their origin in childhood. Similarly, distress in childhood and low socio-emotional competence hinder children’s personal progress and educational opportunities. Early detection of these situations is crucial to prevent these consequences. 3.New forms of fatherhood have clear benefits for the child in a context in which the transformations in the lives of women 5.The educational system is not able to correct the effects of social inequality on academic achievement and success CONCLUSION study, creates effects which are contrary to those that are desirable. Similarly the phenomena of wage discrimination and underemployment are disincentives for working mothers. In order to achieve all of the above, it is essential that budgetary allocations in Spain for family services reach the same level, in comparison to other countries, as the allocations for services for the elderly, for health care or for the unemployed. We are at the back of the line among OECD countries in terms of policies for the protection and support of families, especially for those who have dependent children. This anomaly reveals a lack of commitment to these forms of protection and reflects historical inertia. The future of our society demands a change in direction. In this world in which we must live, not investing in childhood is a luxury that we can no longer afford. CONCLUSION in school. Said inequalities are caused to a great extent by factors outside of school, and it is probably there where some of the new solutions are to be found. It is difficult to change parenting styles which are responsible for certain inequalities or to give those families which do not have it, the cultural capital that would empower their children to assure them success in their educational journey. Even recognizing such difficulties, parents have to understand the role they play in the educational path of their children and find encouragement, support and advice to help them be responsible parents. However, the stimulation of cognitive and other capacities essential for educational achievement is not an objective that can be reached only through the family and school. The role of other formal services (preschools, after school programmes) can be key provided that those who could most benefit from these services have access to them. Bibliography Bibliography Achenbach, T.M. (1992): Manual for the child behavior checklist / 2-3 and 1992 profile, Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. Argys, L.M., E. Peters, J. Brooks-Gunn and J.R. Smith (1998): «The impact of child support on cognitive outcomes of young children», Demography, 35, 159-173. Alberdi, i., and P. Escario (2007): Los hombres jóvenes y la paternidad, Bilbao: Fundación BBVA. Ariès, Ph. (1960): L’enfant et la vie familiale sous l’Ancien Régime, Paris: Seuil. Alexander, K.L, D.R. Entwisle and C.S. Horsey (1997): «From first grade forward: early foundations of high school dropout», Sociology of Education, 70(2), 87-107. American Dietetic Association (2006): «Position of the American Dietetic Association: individual-, family-, school-, and community-based interventions for pediatric overweight», Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 106, 925-945. Anthony, L.G., B.J. Anthony, D.N. Glanville, D.Q. Naiman, C. Waanders and S. Shaffer (2005): «The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behaviour and preschoolers’ social competence and behaviour problems in the classroom», Infant and Child Development, 14, 133-154. Aranceta-Bartrina, J., L. Serra-Majem , M. Foz-Sala, B. Moreno -Esteban and Grupo Colaborativo Seedo (2005): «Prevalencia de obesidad en España», Medicina Clínica, 125(12), 460-466. Argeseanu, S.A., and S.L. Jackson (2010): «Is social competence associated with child obesity?», manuscript presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Sociological Association, Atlanta, USA. Azmat, G., M. Güell and A. Manning (2003): «Gender gaps in unemployment rates in OECD countries», Discussion Paper No. 4307, London: Centre for Economic Policy Research. Online: www.cepr.org. Badenes Pla, N., and M.T. López López (2010): Doble dependencia: abuelos que cuidan a nietos en España, Navarra: Civitas. Baker, A., C. Piotrkowski and J. Brooks-Gunn (1998): «The effects of the home instruction program for preschool youngsters (HIPPY) on children’s school performance at the end of the program and one year later», Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(4), 571-588. Balaguer, I., and E. Arderiu (2008): Calidad de los servicios para la primera infancia y estimación de la demanda, Madrid: Ministerio de Asuntos Sociales. — (2004): Situación de la oferta de servicios existente para menores de tres años durante la jornada laboral de sus padres, Madrid: Ministerio de Trabajo y Asuntos Sociales. — (1998): Mothers, babies and health in later life, Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. Beck, U. (1992): Risk society: towards a new modernity, London: Sage. Beck-Gernsheim, E. (2003): La reinvención de la familia. En busca de nuevas formas de convivencia, Barcelona: Paidós. Becker, G., and H.G. Lewis (1973): «On the interaction between the quantity and quality of children», Journal of Political Economy, 81(2), 279-288. Belden, A.C., N.R. Thompson and J.L. Luby (2008): «Temper tantrums in healthy versus depressed and disruptive preschoolers: defining tantrum behaviors associated with clinical problems», Journal of Pediatrics, 152(1), 117-122. Belsky, J., and M. Rovine (1988): «Nonmaternal care in the first year of life and the security of infant-parent attachment», Child Development, 59(1), 157-167. Bianchi, S.M. (2000): «Maternal employment and time with children: dramatic change or surprising continuity?», Demography, 37(4), 401-414. — J. Robinson and M.A. Milkie (2006): Changing rhythms of American family life, New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Birch, L.L., L. Mcphee, B.C. Shoba, E. Pirok and L. Steinberg (1987): «‘Clean up your plate’: effects of child feeding practices on the development of intake regulation», Learning and Motivation, 18, 301-317. Bogaert, N., K.S. Steinbeck, L.A. Baur, K. Brock and M.A. Bermingham (2003): «Food, activity and family: environmental vs biochemical predictors of weight gain in children», European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 57, 1242-1249. Borra, C., and L. Palma (2009): «Child care choices in Spain», Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 30(4), 323-338. Bourdieu, P. (1983): «Forms of capital», in J.C. Richards (ed.): Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education, New York: Greenwood Press. Brann, L.S., and J.D. Skinner. (2005): «More controlling child-feeding practices are found among parents of boys with an average body mass index compared with parents of boys with a high body mass index», Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 105(9), 1411-1416. Bronte-Tinkew, J., J. Carrano, A. Horowitz and A. Kinukawa (2008): «Involvement among resident fathers and links to infant cognitive outcomes», Journal of Family Issues, 29(9), 1211-1244. Brullet Tenas, C., and C. Roca (2008): «Tener y cuidar hijos: estrategias, tiempos, redes sociales y políticas de apoyo a la crianza», in C. Brullet y C. Gómez-Granell (coords.): Malestares: infancia, adolescencia y familias, Barcelona: Graó, 21-86. Bruss, M.B., J. Morris and L. Dannison (2003): «Prevention of childhood obesity: sociocultural and familial factors», Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 103(8), 1042-1045. Büchel, F., and G.J. Duncan (1998): «Do parents social activities promote children’s school attainments? Evidence from the German socioeconomic panel», Journal of Marriage and Family, 60(1), 95-108. Burdette, H.L., and R.C. Whitaker (2005): «A national study of neighborhood safety, outdoor play, television viewing, and obesity in preschool children», Pediatrics, 116(3), 657-662. Campo, S., and M. Navarro (1985): Análisis sociológico de la familia española, Barcelona: Ariel. Centro de Investigaciones Sociológicas (2010): Estudio 2.831, Barómetro marzo de 2010, Madrid. Online: www.cis.es. — (2009): Estudio 2.788, Barómetro febrero de 2009, Madrid. — (2006): Estudio 2.639, Estudio fecundidad y valores en la España del siglo XXI, Madrid. — (2005): Estudio 2.621, Estudio sobre las actitudes y opiniones sobre la infancia, Madrid. Bibliography Baumrind, D. (1970): «Socialization and competence in young children», Young Children, 26(2), 104-119. — (1985): Friendship and peer culture in the early years, Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing. Cherlin, A. (2010): The marriage go-round: the state of marriage and the family in America today, New York: Vintage. Coser, L.A. (1967): Continuities in the study of social conflict, New York: Free Press. — (2004): «The deinstitutionalization of American marriage», Journal of Marriage and Family, 66(4), 848-861. Craig, L. (2006): «Does father care mean fathers share? A comparison of how mothers and fathers in intact families spend time with children», Gender & Society, 20(2), 259-281. — (1999): «Going to extremes: family structure, children’s well-being, and social science», Demography, 36(4), 421-428. Chudacoff, H.P. (2007): Children at play: an American history, New York: New York University Press. Clark, H.R., E. Goyder, P. Bissell, L. Blank and J. Peters (2007): «How do parents’ child-feeding behaviours influence child weight? Implications for childhood obesity policy», Journal of Public Health, 29(2), 132-141. Coleman, J.S. (1988): «Social capital in the creation of human capital», American Journal of Sociology, 94, 95-120. Coltrane, S. (1996): Family man: fatherhood, housework, and gender equity, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cooke, L.P. (2003): «The South revisited: the division of labor and family outcomes in Italy and Spain», IRISS Working Paper Series, 2003-12, Luxembourg: CEPS/Instead. Cooksey, E.C., and M.M. Fondell (1996): «Spending time with his kids: effects of family structure on fathers’ and children’s lives», Journal of Marriage and Family, 58(3), 693-707. Cramer, P., and T. Steinwert (1998): «Thin is good, fat is bad: how early does it begin?», Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19, 429-451. Crosnoe, R., and L. Lopez-Gonzalez (2005): «Immigration from Mexico, school composition, and adolescent functioning», Sociological Perspectives, 48(1), 1-24. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999): «Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity», in R.J. Sternberg (ed.), Handbook of creativity, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 313-335. — (1990): Flow: the psychology of optimal experience, New York: Harper & Row. — and B. Schneider (2000): Becoming adult: how teenagers prepare for the world of work, New York: Basic Books. Daly, K.J. (2001): «Deconstructing family time: from ideology to lived experience», Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(2), 283-294. Dang, T.T., P. Antolin and H. Oxley (2001): «The fiscal implications of ageing: projections of age-related spending», OECD Economics Department Working Papers, 305. Cooney, T.M., F.A. Pedersen, S. Indelicato and R. Palkovitz (1993): «Timing of fatherhood: is ‘on time’ optimal?», Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55(1), 205-215. Darder, P. (2001): «Repensar l’educació des de les emocions», Perspectiva Escolar, 256, 4-11. Corsaro, W. (1997): The sociology of childhood, Thousand Oaks, California: Pine Forge Press. Datcher, L. (1988): «Effects of mother’s home time on children’s schooling», The Review of Economics and Statistics, 70(3), 367-373. Bibliography — (2004): Estudio 2.578, Estudio sobre las opiniones y actitudes sobre la familia, Madrid. European V alues S tudy (2008): Databases of the Tilburg University and GESIS Leibniz Institute of Social Sciences. Online: http://www. europeanvaluesstudy.eu/evs/data-and-downloads. De Miguel, A. (1998): La España de nuestros abuelos, Madrid: Espasa Calpe. Deater-Deckard, K. (1998): «Parenting stress and child adjustment: some old hypotheses and new questions», Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 314-332. Delgado, M. (coord.) (2007): Encuesta de fecundidad, familia y valores 2006. Opiniones y actitudes 59, Madrid: Centro de Investigaciones Sociológicas. Dewey, J. (1900): The school and society, Chicago: The Chicago University Press. Dietz, T.L. (2000): «Disciplining children: characteristics associated with the use of corporal punishment», Child Abuse and Neglect, 24(12), 1529-1542. DiMaggio, P. (1982): «Cultural capital and school success: the impact of status culture participation on the grades of U.S. high school students», American Sociological Review, 47(2), 189-201. Duflo, E. (2003): «Grandmothers and granddaughters: old age pension and intra-household allocation in South Africa», World Bank Economic Review, 17, 1-25. Durán, M.A. (1986): La jornada interminable, Barcelona: Icaria. Durkheim, E. (1897 [1995]): El suicidio, Madrid: Akal. Education, Audiovisual & Culture Executive Agency (2009): Arts and cultural education at school in Europe, European Commission, Eurydice. England, P. (2006): «Toward gender equality: progress and bottlenecks», in D. Blau, M.C. Brinton and D.B. Grusky (ed.): The declining significance of gender?, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 245-264. Esping-Andersen, G. (2009): The incomplete revolution. Adapting to women’s new roles, Cambridge: Polity Press. e uropean c ommission (2009): Joint report on social protection and social inclusion 2009, Luxembourg: Publications office of the European Union. Eurostat (2008): Statistics on income, social inclusion and living conditions. Online: http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/portal/page/portal/income_ social_inclusion_living_conditions/introduction. Faith, M.S., K.S. Scanlon, L.L. Birch, L.A. Francis and B. Sherry (2004): «Parentchild feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status», Obesity Research, 12(11), 1711-1722. Falkner, N.H., D. Neumark-Sztainer, M. Story, R.W. Jeffery, T. Beuhring and M.D Resnick (2001): «Social, educational, and psychological correlates of weight status in adolescents», Obesity Research, 9, 32-42. Farkas, G., R. Grobe, D. Sheehan and Y. Shuan (1990): «Cultural resources and school success: gender, ethnicity, and poverty groups within an urban school district», American Sociological Review, 55, 127-142. Fergusson, D.M., L.J. Horwood and E. Ridder (2005): «Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood», Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(8), 837-849. Fernández Enguita, M., L. Mena and J. Riviere (2010): Fracaso y abandono escolar en España, Barcelona: Obra Social Fundación ”la Caixa”. Ferraro, K.F., and J.A. Kelley-Moore (2003): «Cumulative disadvantage and health: long-term consequences of obesity?», American Sociological Review, 68(5), 707-729. Finn, J. (1989): «Withdrawing from school», Review of Educational Research, 59(2), 117-142. Finn, J.D., and D.A. Rock (1997): «Academic success among students at risk for school failure», Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 221-234. Bibliography — (1982): «Effects of community and family background on achievemen», The Review of Economics and Statistics, 64(1), 32-41. — (1991): Modernity and self-identity. Self and society in the late Modern Age, Cambridge: Polity Press. Folbre, N., and M. Bittman (ed.) (2004): Family time: the social organization of care, New York: Routledge. — (1990): The consequences of Modernity, Cambridge: Polity Press. Furstenberg, F.F., and A. Cherlin (1991): Divided families: what happens to children when parents part, Cambridge: Harvard University Press. —, P. Morgan and P. Allison (1987): «Paternal participation and children’s well-being after marital dissolution», American Sociological Review, 52(5), 695-701. G agnon , C., W.M. C raig , R.E. T remblay , R.M. Z hou and F. V itaro (1995): «Kindergarten predictors of boys’ stable behavior problems at the end of elementary school», Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23(6), 751766. Galloway, A.T., L.M. Fiorito, L.A. Francis and L.L. Birch (2006): «‘Finish your soup’: counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect», Appetite, 46(3), 318-323. Gauthier, A., T. Smeeding and F.J. Furstenberg (2004): «Are parents investing less time in children? Trends in selected industrialized countries», Population and Development Review, 30(4), 647-671. Gelles, R.J. (1974): The violent home: a study of physical agression between husbands and wives, Beverly Hills: Sage. Gershoff, E.T. (2002): «Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviours and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review», Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 539-579. G ibbs , N. (2009): «The growing backlash against overparenting», Time, 20/11/2009. Online: http://www.time.com/time/nation/ article/0,8599,1940395,00.html Giddens, A. (1999): Runaway world: how globalization is reshaping our lives, London: Profile Books. Giles-Sims, J., M.A. Straus and D.B. Sugarman (1995): «Child, maternal and family characteristics associated with spanking», Family Relations, 44(2), 170-176. Gilliam, W.S. (2005): Pre-kindergarteners left behind: expulsion rates in state prekindergarten system, New Haven, CT: Yale University Child Study Center. Golan, M., and S. Crow (2004): «Parents are key players in the prevention and treatment of weight-related problems», Nutrition Review, 62, 39-50. Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2005): «Corporal punishment and the growth trajectory of children’s antisocial behavior», Child Maltreat, 10(3), 283-292. — and M. Otis (2007): «The predictors of parental use of physical punishment», Family Relations, 56, 80-91. Gustafsson, S.S., E. Kenjoh and C.M.M.P. Wetzels (2002): «Postponement of maternity and the duration of time spent at home after first birth: panel data analysis comparing Germany, Great Britain, the Netherlands and Sweden», OECD Labour Market and Social Policy Occasional Papers, 59, Paris: OECD. Online: www.oecd.org/els/workingpapers. Gutiérrez-Domènech, M. (2002): «Employment penalty after motherhood in Spain», CEP Discussion Paper 546, London: Centre for Economic Performance, LSE. Haines, J., and D. Neumark-Sztainer (2006): «Prevention of obesity and eating disorders: a consideration of shared risk factors», Health and Education Research, 21(6), 770-782. Hallal, P.C., C.G. Victora, M.R. Azevedo and J.C.K. Wells (2006): «Adolescent physical activity and health: a systematic review», Sports Medicine, 36(12), 1019-1030. Bibliography Fleisher, B. (1977): «Mother’s home time and the production of child quality», Demography, 14(2), 197-212. Hardy, R., D. Kuh and M. Wadsworth (2000): «Smoking, body mass index, socioeconomic status and the menopausal transition in a British national cohort», International Journal of Epidemiology, 29(5), 845-851. Hart, B., and T.R. Risley (1999): The social world of children learning to talk, Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. H eckman , J., and D.V. M asterov (2007): «The productivity argument for investing in young children», Review of Agricultural Economics, 29(3), 446-493. — and L. Lochner (2000): «Rethinking myths about education and training: understanding the sources of skill formation in a modern economy», in S. Danzinger and J. Waldfogel (ed.): Securing the future: investing in children from birth to college, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 47-83. H etherington , E.M. (1979): «Divorce: A child’s perspective», American Psychologist, 34, 851-858. Iglesias de Ussel, J., P. Marí-Klose, M. Marí-Klose and P. González Blasco (2009): Matrimonios y parejas jóvenes, España 2009, Madrid: Fundación SM. Instituto Nacional de Estadística (2007, 2008): Encuesta de condiciones de vida (ECV), Madrid: INE. Izquierdo, C. (2000): «Comunicación interpersonal y crecimiento emocional en centros educativos: un modelo interpretativo», Educar, 26, 127-149. Jacobs, J.E, S. Lanza, W. Osgood, J.S. Eccles and A. Wigfield (2002): «Changes in children’s self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve», Child Development, 73(2), 509-527. Jiang J., U. Rosenqvist, Wang H., T. Greiner, Lian G. and A. Sarkadi (2007): «Influence of grandparents on eating behaviors of young children in Chinese threegeneration families», Appetite, 48(3), 377-383. Johnson, M.K., R. Crosnoe and G. Elder (2001): «Students’ attachment and academic engagement: the role of race and ethnicity», Sociology of Education, 74(4), 318-340. Heywood, C. (2001): A history of childhood: children and childhood in the West from medieval to modern times, Cambridge: Polity Press. Johnson-Taylor, W.L., and J.E. Everhart (2006): «Modifiable environmental and behavioral determinants of overweight among children and adolescents: report of a workshop», Obesity, 14(6), 929-966. Hill, M. (1992): «The role of economic resources and remarriage in financial assistance for children of divorce», Journal of Family Issues, 13(2), 158178. Kasinitz, P., J.H. Mollenkopf, M.C. Waters and J. Holdaway (2008): Inheriting the city: the children of immigrants come of age, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hochschild, A.R. (1995): «The culture of politics: traditional, post-modern, cold-modern, and warm-modern ideals of care», Social Politics, 2(3), 331-345. Klammer, U. (2006): «Work life balance from the children’s perspective», in J. Lewis (ed.): Children in the context of family and welfare state change, Cheltenham: Edward Elgar. — (1989): The second shift: working parents and the revolution at home, New York: Viking Penguin. Kuntsche, E.N., and G. Gmel (2004): «Emotional wellbeing and violence among social and solitary risky single occasion drinkers in adolescence», Addiction, 99(3), 331-339. Huttenlocher, J., W. Haight, A. Bryk, M. Seltzer and T. Lyons (1991): «Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender», Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236-248. Lareau, A. (2002): «Invisible inequality: social class and childrearing in black families and white», American Sociological Review, 67(5), 747-776. Bibliography Han, W., J. Waldfogel and J. Brooks-Gunn (2001): «The effects of early maternal employment on later cognitive and behavioral outcomes», Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(2), 336-354. Larrañaga, I., B. Arregui and J. Arpal (2004): «Reproductive or domestic work», Gaceta Sanitaria, 18, supl.1, 31-37. Online: http://scielo.isciii.es/scielo. php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0213-91112004000400007&lng=es&nrm=iso. M arí -K lose M., C. G ómez -G ranell , A. L anau , and P. M arí -K lose (2010): Acompanyament a l’escolaritat: pautes per un model local de referència, Barcelona: Diputació de Barcelona (forthcoming). M arí -K lose , P., M. M arí -K lose , F. G ranados , C. G ómez -G ranell and À. Martínez (2009): Informe de la inclusió social a Espanya 2009, Barcelona: Obra Social Caixa Catalunya. L ee , V.E., and J.B. S mith (1994): High school restructuring and student achievement. Issues in restructuring schools (Report No. 7), Madison: University of Wisconsin-Madison, Center on Organization and Restructuring of Schools. —, M. Marí-Klose, Ll. Flaqué, C. Sánchez and L. Morató (2008a): Informe de la inclusió social a Espanya 2008, Barcelona: Obra Social Caixa Catalunya. L ipovetsky , G. (2003): La era del vacío: ensayos sobre el individualismo contemporáneo, Barcelona: Anagrama. —, C. Gómez-Granell, C. Brullet and S. Escapa (2008b): Temps de les famílies: anàlisi sociològica dels usos del temps dins de les llars catalanes a partir de les dades del Panel de Famílies i Infància, Barcelona: Departament d’Acció Social i Ciutadania, Generalitat de Catalunya. Lister, M.R.A. (2006): «Children (but not women) first: new labour, child welfare and gender», Critical Social Policy, 26(2), 315-335. Lobstein, T., L. Baur and R. Uauy (2004): «Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health», Obesity Review, 5, supl. 1, 4-104. Marks, H.M. (2000): «Student engagement and instructional activity: patterns in elementary, middle, and high school years», American Educational Research Journal, 37(1), 153-184. Mace, S.R., and I.A. Mcgregor (2000): «Maternal grandmothers improve nutritional status and survival of children in rural Gambia», Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences, 267, 1641-1647. M arshall , S.J., S.J. B iddle , T. G orely , N. C ameron and I. M urdey (2004): «Relationships between media use, body fatness and physical activity in children and youth: a meta-analysis», International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 28(10), 1238-1246. Magnuson, K.A., C. Ruhm and J. Waldfogel (2007): «Does prekindergarten improve school preparation and performance?», Economics of Education Review, 26(1), 33-51. Martinez, J.R., D.S. Degarmo and J.M. Eddy (2004): «Promoting academic success among Latino youth», Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 26, 128-151. Mahoney, J.L., R.W. Larson and J.S. Eccles (ed.) (2005): Organized activities as contexts of development: extracurricular activities, after-school and community programs, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. McLanahan, S., and G. Sandefur (1994): Growing up with a single parent: what hurts, what helps?, Cambridge: Harvard University Press. —, B.D. Cairns and T.W. Farmer (2003): «Promoting interpersonal competence and educational success through extracurricular activity participation», Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 409-418. Manlove, J. (1998): «The influence of high school dropout and school disengagement on the risk of school-age pregnancy», Journal of Research on Adolescence, 8(2), 187-220. — and L. B umpass (1988): «Intergenerational consequences of family disruption», American Journal of Sociology, 94(1), 130-152. McLeod, J.D., and M.J. Shanahan (1996): «Trajectories of poverty and children’s mental health», Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(3), 207-220. Meil, G. (2006): Padres e hijos en la España actual, Barcelona: Obra Social Fundación ”la Caixa”. Bibliography — (2000): Home advantage: social class and parental intervention in elementary education, Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield. Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia (2007): PIRLS 2006: estudio internacional de progreso en comprensión lectora de la IEA. Informe español, Madrid: Instituto Nacional de Evaluación y Calidad del Sistema Educativo. M oreno , L. (2002): «Bienestar mediterráneo y ‘supermujeres’», Revista Española de Investigaciones Sociológicas, 2, 41-57. Must, A., and S.E. Anderson (2003): «Effects of obesity on morbidity in children and adolescents», Nutrition in Clinical Care, 6(1), 4-12. Näsman, E. (1994): «Individualization and institutionalization of childhood in today’s Europe», in J. Qvortrup, M. Bardy, G. Sgritta and H. Wintersberger (ed.): Childhood matters: social theory, practice and politics, Aldershot: Averbury. Neidell, M. (2000): «Early parental time investments in children’s human capital development: effects of time in the first year on cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes», Working paper, 86, Department of Economics, University of California, Los Angeles. Nepomnyaschy, L., and J. Waldfogel (2007): «Paternity leave and fathers’ involvement with their young children», Community, Work & Family, 10(4), 427-453. N ewmann , F.M., G. W ehlage and S.D. L amborn (1992): «The significance and sources of student engagement», in F.M. Newmann (ed.): Student engagement and achievement in American secondary schools, New York: Teachers College Press, 11-39. OECD (2008): Growing unequal? Income distribution and poverty in OECD countries, Paris: OECD. — (2007): Babies and bosses: reconciling work and family life, Paris: OECD. — (2005): The definition and selection of key competences. Executive summary. Paris: OECD. Online: http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/47/61/35070367.pdf. Ogden, C. I., M.D. Carroll, L. Curtin, M.A. McDowell, C.J. Tabak and K.M. Flegal (2006): «Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 19992004», Journal of the American Medical Association, 295, 1549-1555. Papadopoulos, A.S., E. Vaquera and I. Qi (2010): Depression and obesity in Medicaid-enrolled adolescents: trends in service use, cost and outcomes, Tampa, FL: Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute, University of South Florida. Pellegrini, A.D., and P.K. Smith (1998): «The development of play during childhood: forms and possible functions», Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 3(2), 51-57. Pfau-Effinger, B. (2006): «Cultures of childhood and the relationship of care and employment in European welfare states» in J. Lewis (ed.), Changing families and welfare states, Cheltenham: Edward Elgar. Piaget, J. (1932): The moral judgement of the child, New York: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich. Pope, S.K., L. Whiteside, J. Brooks-Gunn, K.J. Kelleher, V.I. Rickert, R.H. Bradley and P.H. Casey (1993): «Low-birth-weight infants born to adolescent mothers: effects of coresidency with grandmother on child development», Journal of the American Medical Association, 269, 1396-1400. R amey , C.T., and S.L. R amey (2000): «Early childhood experiences and developmental competence», in J. W aldfogel and S. D anziger (ed.): Securing the future: investing in children from birth to college, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 122-155. R azza , R.A., and C. B lair (2009): «Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function and social competence: a longitudinal analysis», Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 332-343. Rey-Lopez, J.P., G. Vicente-Rodriguez, M. Biosca and L.A. Moreno (2008): «Sedentary behaviour and obesity development in children and adolescents», Nutrition, Metabolism & Cardiovascular Diseases, 18(3), 242-251. Bibliography Mills, M., L. Mencarini, M.L. Tanturri and K. Begall (2008): «Gender equity and fertility intentions in Italy and the Netherlands», Demographic Research, 18, 1-26. Online: http://www.demographic-research.org/Volumes/ Vol18/1/default.htm. Roppelt, U. (2003): «Kinder-Experten ihres Alltags?», in A.M. Frankfurt et al. (ed.) (2004): Familie bringt Gewinn. Innovation durch Balance von Familie und Arbeitswelt, Gütersloh: Bertelsmann. Rubin, K.H., R. Coplan and J. Bowker (2009): «Social withdrawal and shyness in childhood and adolescence», Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141-171. Rumberger, R. (1987): «High school dropouts: a review of the issues and evidence», Review of Educational Research, 57(2), 1001-1121. Sandberg, J.F., and S.L. Hofferth (2001): «Changes in children’s time with parents: United States, 1981-1997», Demography, 38(3), 423-436. Save the Children (2005): Annual report, Connecticut: Save the Children Federation. Sayer, L.C., S.M. Bianchi and J.P. Robinson (2004): «Are parents investing less in children? Trends in mothers’ and fathers’ time with children», American Journal of Sociology, 110(1), 1-43. S erdula , M.K., D. I very , R.J. C oates , D.S. F reedman , D.F. W illiamson and T. Byers (1993): «Do obese children become obese adults? A review of the literature», Preventive Medicine, 22(2), 167-177. Sewell, W.H., and V.P. Shah (1968): «Social class, parental encouragement and educational aspirations», American Journal of Sociology, 73(6), 559-572. Simons, R.L., C. Johnson and R.D. Conger (1994): «Harsh corporal punishment versus quality of parental involvement as an explanation of adolescent maladjustment», Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 591-607. Socolar, R.R.S., and R.E.K. Stein (1995): «Spanking infants and toddlers: maternal belief and practice», Pediatrics, 95(1), 105-111. Steinberg, L. (2001): «We know some things: parent-adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect», Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11(1), 1-19. Steiner, L. (1998): «Organizational dilemmas as barriers to learning», The Learning Organization, 5(4), 193-201. Storch, E.A., V.A. Milsom, N. DeBraganza, A.B. Lewin, G.R. Geffken and J.H. Silverstein (2007): «Peer victimization, psychosocial adjustment, and physical activity in overweight and at-risk-for-overweight youth», Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(1), 80-89. Strauss, R.S. (2000): «Childhood obesity and self-esteem», Pediatrics, 105(1), 15. Suárez-Orozco, C., M. Suárez-Orozco and I. Todorova (2008): Learning a new land: immigrant students in American society, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sweeting, H., C. Wright and H. Minnis (2005): «Psychosocial correlates of adolescent obesity, slimming down and ‘becoming obese’», Journal of Adolescent Health, 37(5), 409-417. Sylva, K., et al. (2010): Early childhood matters: evidence from the effective preschool and primary education project, New York: Routledge. Thompson, D.R., E. Obarzanek, D.L. Franko, B.A. Barton, J. Morisson, F.M. Bro, S.R. D aniels and R.H. S triegel -M oore (2007): «Childhood overweight and cardiovascular disease risk factors: the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute growth and health data», Journal of Pediatrics, 150(1), 18-25. Tobío Soler, C. (2002): «Conciliación o contradicción: cómo hacen las madres trabajadoras», Revista Española de Investigaciones Sociológicas, 97, 155-186. — et al. (2010): El cuidado de las personas: un reto para el siglo XXI, Barcelona: Obra Social Fundación ”la Caixa”. Unicef (2009): «Trends in infant mortality rates (1960-2008)», in Childinfo: Monitoring the Situation of Children and Women – Unicef. Online: http://www. childinfo.org/mortality_imrcountrydata.php. Unicef Innocenti Research Centre (2007): Child poverty in perspective: an overview of child well-being in rich countries, Innocenti Report Card 7, Florence: The United Nations Children’s Fund. Bibliography Rivero Recuento, A. (dir.) et al. (2005): Conciliación de la vida familiar y la vida laboral: situación actual, necesidades y demandas (Informe de resultados), Madrid: Instituto de la Mujer, Ministerio de Igualdad. Waldfogel, J. (2002): «Child care, women’s employment, and child outcomes», Journal of Population Economics, 15(3), 527-548. — (1997): «The effect of children on women’s wages», American Sociological Review, 62(2), 209-217. —, W.J. Han and J. Brooks-Gunn (2002): «The effects of early maternal employment on child cognitive development», Demography, 39(2), 369392. —, J. Hill and J. Brooks-Gunn (2003): «Sustained effects of high participation in an early intervention for low-birth-weight premature infants», Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 730-744. World Bank (2008): «Mortality rate, infant (per 1,000 live births)», The World Bank, consulted in July 2010. Online: http://data.worldbank.org/ indicator/SP.DYN.IMRT.IN. World Health Organization (2007): WHO reference 2007: growth reference data for 5-19 years, World Health Organization. World Values Survey (1990, 1999, 2008): ASEP/JDS database, Madrid. Online: http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org. Wyness, M. (2006): Childhood and society: an introduction to the sociology of childhood, Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan. Zuckerman, B., and R. Kahn (2000): «Pathways to early child health and development», in S. Danziger and J. Waldfogel (ed.): Securing the future: investing in children from birth to college, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 87-121. Bibliography Voelkl, K.E. (1995): «School warmth, student participation, and achievement», The Journal of Experimental Education, 63(2), 127-138. Graphs 2.1Probability that there will be situations of tension in homes with children from 0 to 10 years of age for different reasons by different models for the division of responsibilities by gender 30 3.1 Women that have had to abandon an activity after the birth of their child by educational level 3.2Average time fathers and mothers spend with children by child’s age. Week days and weekends 3.3Average time fathers and mothers spend with children by parent’s educational level. Week days and weekends 3.4Average time fathers and mothers spend with children by couple’s employment status. Week days and weekends 3.5Parent’s evaluation of the amount of time they spend with their children 3.6Probability of mother believing she does not spend enough time with her child by different levels of partner’s co-responsibility 51 3.7How often parents did different activities with their child in the previous week 53 3.8Probability of parents placing children in a nursery school, pre-school or day are centre, by household income 61 4.1Types of fatherhood 72 4.2Probability of having been spanked in the previous week, by degree of cognitive stimulation 82 4.3Probabilty of having been spanked in the previous week, by existence of situations of tension in home for various reasons 83 4.4Level of reading comprehension of 10 year old children, by how often parents read to them up until they were three years of age 87 40 47 48 49 50 Index of graphs and tables Index of graphs and tables 6.8 Children with problems of disengagement from school, by sex 126 90 6.9 Children with problems of disengagement from school, by age 127 5.2Frequency of participation in after school activities 91 6.10Probability of child having problems keeping up with a school subject, by degree of parental involvement 132 6.11Probability of child not liking to study, by degree of parental involvement 133 109 6.12Probability of child having problems keeping up with a subject, by types of fatherhood 133 6.2Probability of child being obese, by parents’ perception of the amount of time they spend with him/her 113 6.13Probability of child having problems concentrating, by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes 134 6.3Probability of child being obese, by type of fatherhood 114 6.14Probability of child having problems keeping up with a subject, by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes 135 6.15Probability of child saying he/she does not like to study, by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes 135 5.3 Children who do not participate in any after school activity, by household characteristics and family socioeconomic situation 5.4Frequency of participation in unstructured activities 6.1Probability of children being obese, by parents’ weight 6.4Probability of child being obese, by level of symptoms of depression 6.5Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, by degree of exposure to cognitively stimulating activities 6.6Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, by frequency of use of rewards and punishments 6.7Probability of child having low socio-emotional competence, by frequency of tension in the home due to different causes 92 94 115 123 Tables 124 2.1 Most important reasons for having a child among women who do not have children but want to have them and for women who already have children but would like to have more 21 124 2.2 Most important reasons for not having children. Women between 25 and 39 years of age without children and who do not want to have them 23 Index of graphs and tables 5.1 Children enrolled in after school activities, by sex and type of activity 57 3.8Households that have received some kind of child care assistance in the previous month from someone who does not live in the home, by mother’s characteristics 59 3.9Evolution of enrolment rates in early childhood education 59 4.1Frequency of eating breakfast, lunch and dinner together, by mother’s education level 67 4.2How often someone living in the household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by mother’s education level 68 4.3How often someone living in the household does outdoor activities with the child, by mother’s education level 69 4.4Evolution in male opinions about the ideal model for division of responsibilities by gender in the family 71 4.5How often someone living in the household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by types of fatherhood 73 4.6How often someone living in the household does outside activities with the child, by types of fatherhood 74 4.7 Who cuddles with the child (hugs and kisses) more often, by types of fatherhood 75 4.8Opinions on the best method to raise children, by age groups 77 3.5How often someone living in household does cognitively stimulating activities with the child, by mother’s education level 54 4.9How often parents used reward and punishment in the previous week 78 3.6How often someone living in household does cognitively stimulating activities and outside activities with the child, by mother’s employment status 4.10Factors related to a child being spanked in the previous week 79 2.4Distribution of domestic responsibilities in homes in which the women work, by couples with and without children 2.5Persons who say that there is tension in the home for different reasons, by socio-demographic characteristics of parents and by child’s age 2.6Opinions on whether women should work full-time, part-time or not work in different situations, by age group 24 26 28 31 2.7Opinions on whether it is necessary for children to attend school or day care, by different age groups of children 32 2.8 Why child is or was in day care before three years of age 33 2.9Opinions on whether children under three develop certain abilities better in school or in the family, by education level 3.1 Working day of couples between 25 and 49 years of age by age of child 3.2 Mother’s current employment status by status at the time of the birth of child 3.3 Women who took maternity leave by age, education level and household income 3.4Fathers who took paternity leave before and after the Law of Equality entered into effect 34 38 40 42 44 56 Index of graphs and tables 3.7 Men who take primary responsibility for cognitively stimulating activities or take equal responsibility, by their educational level 2.3 Most important reasons for not having children. Women between 25 and 39 years of age who have children but do not want to have more 4.12How often someone living in the household engages in reading activities with the child 5.1Activities child does daily or almost daily, by different socioeconomic characteristics of household 112 6.4Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by socioeconomic characteristics of household 119 6.5Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by composition of household 121 6.6Level of socio-emotional competence of children, by parenting styles 122 6.7School disengagement by parents’ education level 128 6.8School disengagement by parents’ employment status 130 A.1Autonomous Community by age of the child 158 A.2Autonomous Community by size of municipality 159 85 85 95 5.2Persons with whom the child usually does different activities 97 5.3How often child has friends over to play, by household socioeconomic characteristics 99 5.4Number of after-school activities child participates in, by composition of household (grandparents and siblings living or not living in the home) 6.3Obesity in children, by composition of household 101 6.1Distribution of boys and girls in weight categories, by body mass index 107 6.2Obesity in children, by socioeconomic characteristics of household 110 Index of graphs and tables 4.11Parental involvement in children’s school work, by mother’s education level To carry out the present study a specific survey was designed. What follows are the characteristics of this survey. tested in research on these matters in studies carried out in other countries. The telephone interviews were carried out by Random Estudios de Opinión, Márketing y Socioeconómicos SA. Survey on Inter and Intra-generational relationships in Childhood (2010) Date of survey: January and February, 2010. Scope: Spain. Design: The survey is based on two questionnaires, structured in function of the age of the child living in the home: the first, for children up to four years of age; the second, for children from five to ten years of age. In the case of more than one child at these ages in the home, the interviewer asked questions only about one of the children. In such cases, the selection was random. The average duration of the interview was 15 minutes and it was carried out over the telephone (CATI) with one of the parents. The questionnaire for children up to four years of age included 57 questions; that for children from five to ten years of age, 62 questions. One block of questions was common to both questionnaires; other questions were specific for the different age groups. The questionnaire was developed by the research team for this study. To the extent possible, the survey was based on standard questions previously Universe: The universe was composed of a sample of fathers and mothers that live with their children of 0 to 10 years of age. The survey was responded to by 611 fathers and 1,595 mothers. The responses referred to 1,058 children from 0 to 4 years of age and 1,148 children from 5 to 10 years of age. In total, information was gathered for 1,160 boys and 1,046 girls. Sample Size: 2,206 families with children up to ten years of age (based on a prevision of 2,200 families). Sampling procedure: Representative geographic distribution. Quotas based on the size of municipality in each autonomous community in Spain, sex and age of the child, and the economic Methodological Appendix Methodological Appendix Sampling error: ±2,13% Questionnaires and data files are available to researchers upon request. ([email protected]). TaBlE A.1: Autonomous Community by age of the child from 0 to 3 years of age from 4 to 6 years of age from 7 to 10 years of age 145 106 139 390 Aragon 23 16 21 60 Asturias 18 14 18 50 Balearic Islands 20 14 19 53 Basque Country 40 28 35 103 Canary Islands 38 29 40 107 Cantabria 11 8 10 29 Castilla and León 41 31 41 113 Castilla-La Mancha 33 25 33 91 138 95 119 352 Extremadura 17 13 18 48 Galicia 47 34 46 127 La Rioja 5 4 5 14 Madrid 126 87 110 323 Murcia 26 18 23 67 Navarre 11 8 10 29 Valencian Community 90 64 83 237 3 2 2 7 832 596 772 2,200 Andalusia Catalonia Ceuta and Melilla Total Total Methodological Appendix status of the mother. Random selection of households based on data updated semi-annually by the CMT. Below, the number of interviews forecast according to age of the child and size of municipality is presented. 5.001-10.000 Andalusia 39 34 50 64 58 145 390 Aragon 14 3 6 3 2 32 60 Asturias 3 3 8 5 7 24 50 Balearic Islands 3 5 6 16 3 20 53 Basque Country 11 9 16 19 11 37 103 Canary Islands 3 8 12 27 19 38 107 Cantabria 6 3 5 3 3 9 29 Castilla and León 34 10 7 9 16 37 113 Castilla-La Mancha 27 12 13 10 21 8 91 Catalonia 36 28 38 58 45 147 352 Extremadura 16 7 5 6 7 7 48 Galicia 17 18 22 21 17 32 127 La Rioja 3 2 1 1 0 7 14 Madrid 7 11 13 19 37 236 323 Murcia 1 2 9 17 7 31 67 Navarre 9 4 4 3 0 9 29 20 19 27 61 31 79 237 0 0 0 0 7 0 7 249 178 242 342 291 898 2,200 Valencian Community Ceuta and Melilla Total 10.001-20.000 20.001-50.000 50.001-100.000 MORE THAN 100.000 0-5.000 Total Methodological Appendix TaBlE A.2: Autonomous Community by size of municipality Social Studies Collection Available on the internet: www.laCaixa.es/ObraSocial 1. FOREIGN IMMIGRATION IN SPAIN (Out of stock) Eliseo Aja, Francesc Carbonell, Colectivo Ioé (C. Pereda, W. Actis and M. A. de Prada), Jaume Funes and Ignasi Vila 5. THE SPANISH FAMILY AND ATTITUDES TOWARD EDUCATION (Out of stock) Víctor Pérez-Díaz, Juan Carlos Rodríguez and Leonardo Sánchez Ferrer 2. VALUES IN SPANISH SOCIETY AND THEIR RELATION TO DRUG USE (Out of stock) Eusebio Megías (director), Domingo Comas, Javier Elzo, Ignacio Megías, José Navarro, Elena Rodríguez and Oriol Romaní 6. OLD AGE, DEPENDENCE AND LONG-TERM CARE (Out of stock) David Casado Marín and Guillem López and Casasnovas 3. FAMILY POLICIES FROM A COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE (Out of stock) Lluís Flaquer 7. YOUNG PEOPLE AND THE EUROPEAN CHALLENGE Joaquim Prats Cuevas (director), Cristòfol-A. Trepat and Carbonell (coordinator), José Vicente Peña Calvo, Rafael Valls Montés and Ferran Urgell Plaza 4. YOUNG WOMEN IN SPAIN (Out of stock) Inés Alberdi, Pilar Escario and Natalia Matas 8. SPAIN AND IMMIGRATION (*) Víctor Pérez-Díaz, Berta ÁlvarezMiranda and Carmen GonzálezEnríquez 9. HOUSING POLICY FROM A COMPARATIVE EUROPEAN PERSPECTIVE Carme Trilla 10. DOMESTIC VIOLENCE (Out of stock) Inés Alberdi and Natalia Matas 11. IMMIGRATION, SCHOOLING AND THE LABOUR MARKET (*) Colectivo Ioé (Walter Actis, Carlos Pereda and Miguel A. de Prada) 12. ACOUSTIC CONTAMINATION IN OUR CITIES Benjamín García Sanz and Francisco Javier Garrido 13. FOSTER FAMILIES Pere Amorós, Jesús Palacios, Núria Fuentes, Esperanza León and Alicia Mesas 14. PEOPLE WITH DISABILITIES AND THE LABOUR MARKET Colectivo Ioé (Carlos Pereda, Miguel A. de Prada and Walter Actis) 15. MOSLEM IMMIGRATION IN EUROPE Víctor Pérez-Díaz, Berta ÁlvarezMiranda and Elisa Chuliá 16. POVERTY AND SOCIAL EXCLUSION Joan Subirats (director), Clara Riba, Laura Giménez, Anna Obradors, Maria Giménez, Dídac Queralt, Patricio Bottos and Ana Rapoport 17. THE REGULATION OF IMMIGRATION IN EUROPE Eliseo Aja, Laura Díez (coordinators), Kay Hailbronner, Philippe de Bruycker, François Julien-Laferrière, Paolo Bonetti, Satvinder S. Juss, Giorgio Malinverni, Pablo Santolaya and Andreu Olesti 18. EUROPEAN EDUCATIONAL SYSTEMS: CRISIS OR TRANSFORMATION? Joaquim Prats and Francesc Raventós (directors), Edgar Gasòliba (coordinator), Robert Cowen, Bert P. M. Creemers, Pierre-Louis Gauthier, Bart Maes, Barbara Schulte and Roger Standaert 19. PARENTS AND CHILDREN IN TODAY’S SPAIN Gerardo Meil Landwerlin 20. SINGLE PARENTING AND CHILDHOOD Lluís Flaquer, Elisabet Almeda and Lara Navarro 21. THE IMMIGRANT BUSINESS COMMUNITY IN SPAIN Carlota Solé, Sònia Parella and Leonardo Cavalcanti 22. ADOLESCENTS AND ALCOHOL. THE PARENTAL VIEW Eusebio Megías Valenzuela (director), Juan Carlos Ballesteros Guerra, Fernando Conde Gutiérrez del Álamo, Javier Elzo Imaz, Teresa Laespada Martínez, Ignacio Megías Quirós and Elena Rodríguez San Julián 23. INTERGENERATIONAL PROGRAMMES. TOWARDS A SOCIETY FOR ALL AGES (*) Mariano Sánchez (director), Donna M. Butts, Alan HattonYeo, Nancy A. Henkin, Shannon E. Jarrott, Matthew S. Kaplan, Antonio Martínez, Sally Newman, Sacramento Pinazo, Juan Sáez and Aaron P. C. Weintraub 24. Food, consumption and health (*) Cecilia Díaz Méndez y Cristóbal Gómez Benito (coordinators), Javier Aranceta Bartrina, Jesús Contreras Hernández, María González Álvarez, Mabel Gracia Arnaiz, Paloma Herrera Racionero, Alicia de León Arce, Emilio Luque and María Ángeles Menéndez Patterson 25. VOCATIONAL TRAINING IN SPAIN. TOWARD THE KNOWLEDGE SOCIETY (*) Oriol Homs 26. Sport, health and quality of life (*) David Moscoso Sánchez and Eduardo Moyano Estrada (coordinators), Lourdes Biedma Velázquez, Rocío FernándezBallesteros García, María Martín Rodríguez, Carlos Ramos González, Luís Rodríguez-Morcillo Baena and Rafael Serrano del Rosal 28. CARING FOR OTHERS A CHALLENGE FOR THE 21ST CENTURY (*) Constanza Tobío, M.ª Silveria Agulló Tomás, M.ª Victoria Gómez and M.ª Teresa Martín Palomo 29. SCHOOL FAILURE AND DROPOUTS in Spain (*) Mariano Fernández Enguita, Luis Mena Martínez and Jaime Riviere Gómez 30. Childhood and the Future: new realities, new challenges (*) Pau Marí-Klose, Marga Marí-Klose, Elizabeth Vaquera and Solveig Argeseanu Cunningham 27. The rural population in Spain. from disequilibrium to social sustainability (*) Luis Camarero (coordinator), Fátima Cruz, Manuel González, Julio A. del Pino, Jesús Oliva and Rosario Sampedro (*) English version available on the internet This study examines the activities and relationships of children under 10 years old and how they are being affected by recent social transformations, such as the appearance of new types of families, the massive incorporation of women into the workforce and new forms of fathering and mothering. The authors also trace the origins of phenomena that can become an obstacle to children’s development, such as obesity, socioemotional problems and school disengagement. The findings of this study are an invitation to reflect on this critical stage in which the experiences children live through will have a decisive influence on their future lives. In addition, they contribute to the development of initiatives aimed at helping families and improving the well-being of children. With this study, ”la Caixa” Social Projects seeks to promote research on and analysis of the social factors that shape individuals’ opportunities throughout their lives. Evidence such as that presented in this book can help in the development of instruments to prevent and correct situations of exclusion and vulnerability.