Developmentally Appropriate Practice: Philosophical and Practical
Transcription
Developmentally Appropriate Practice: Philosophical and Practical
Developmentally Appropriate Practice: Philosophical and Practical Implications Author(s): David Elkind Source: The Phi Delta Kappan, Vol. 71, No. 2 (Oct., 1989), pp. 113-117 Published by: Phi Delta Kappa International Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/20404083 Accessed: 02/02/2010 11:13 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=pdki. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Phi Delta Kappa International is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Phi Delta Kappan. http://www.jstor.org Approprite Developmentally Practice: Philosophical Practical Implications and True education reformwill come about only when we re place the reigning psycho metric educational psychology with a developmentally appropriate one, Mr. Elkind asserts. Unfortunately, the prospects for such a shift are not good. BYDAVID ELKIND [ Z HE IDEAOF develop mentally appropriate ed ucational practice - that the curriculum should be matched to the child's level of mental ability -has been favorably received in educa this positive re tion circles.' However, for de ception is quite extraordinary, velopmentally appropriatepractice de rives from a philosophy of education that to the "psycho is in total opposition metric" educational philosophy that now dictates educational practice in the ma jority of our public schools. Perhaps for this reason developmental appropriate ness has been honored more inword than in deed. Inwhat follows I highlight some of the differences between these two education al philosophies and contrast a few of their DAVID ELKIND is a professor of child study at Tufts University, Medford, Mass. He is a past president of theNational Associa tion for theEducation of Young Children and the author of The Hurried Child (Addison Wesley, 1981), All Grown Up and No Place toGo (Addison-Wesley, 1984), and Misedu cation: Preschoolers at Risk (Knopf, 1987). Illustrationby Kay Salem practical implications. My purpose in do ing so is to argue that true education re form will come about only when we re of education.The developmentalphilos ophy differs from thepsychometricphi losophy on all four counts. I should men place the reigningpsychometric educa tion that the developmental philosophy tionalpsychologywith a developmentally that I present here derives from the re appropriate one. TWO PHILOSOPHIES Any philosophy of education must in clude some conception of the learner, of the learning process, of the information to be acquired, and of the goals or aims search and theory of Jean Piaget.2 Conception of the learner. Within a de velopmentalphilosophyof education, the learner is viewed as having developing mental abilities.All individuals(with the exception of the retarded) are assumed to be able to attain these abilities, though not necessarily at the same age. For ex OCTOBER 1989 113 0 ample, we expect that all children will at tain the concrete operations that Piaget described as emerging at about age 6 or 7. These operations, which function much like the group of arithmetic opera tions, enable children who have attained them to learn and to apply rules. How ever, not all children will attain these operations at the same age. According ly, a developmental philosophy sees in in ability as differ dividual differences ences in rates of intellectual growth. This conception of mental ability con trasts sharply with that of a psychometric philosophy of education.According to the psychometric position, the learner is 0 0 ing," which in effect allows bright chil dren to go through the material more quickly than slower children. This psy chometric orientation also underlies the provision of special classes for the gift ed and for the retarded. Conception of the learning process. Within the developmental philosophy of education, learning is always seen as a creative activity. Whenever we learn anything, we engage the world in a way that creates something new, something that reflects both our own mental activi ty and the material we have dealt with. We never simply copy content; we al ways stamp it with our unique way of he developmental approach implies little or no automatic transfer of learning; the psychometric approach assumes spontaneous transfer. seen as having measurable abilities. This philosophy assumes that any ability that exists must exist in some amount and must, therefore, be quantifiable. For ex ample, intelligence tests - the flagships of the psychometric philosophy - are viewing the world. The child from Con necticut who heard the Lord's Prayer as "Our Father, Who art in New Haven, Harold be thy name" is not the excep tion but the rule. Everything we learn has both a subjective and an objective com designed to assess individualdifferences ponent. in the ability to learn and to adapt to new The conception of learning as a cre ative or constructive process has a very important practical implication. Itmeans that we cannot talk of learning independ ently of the content to be learned. The material to be learned will always inter act with the learning process in some spe cial way. Long after Piaget discovered the successive stages and organizations of mental operations, he continued to study the ways inwhich children attained different concepts, such as space, geom etry, time, and movement and speed.3 In so doing he emphasized the fact that merely knowing the stages of mental de velopment does not provide special in sight into how children use the operations at any given stage to attain any particu lar concept. The only way to discover how children go about learning a partic ular subject is to study children learning. By contrast, the psychometric philos ophy views learning as governed by a set of principles (e.g., intermittent reinforce ment) and consisting of the acquisition of a set of skills (e.g., decoding) that are in dependent of the content to be learned. Early workers in this tradition enunciat situations.A psychometric perspective regards individual differences in per in formance as reflecting differences amount of a given ability. Both of these opposing conceptions of human ability contain some truth. How ever, they have far different pedagogi cal implications. From a developmentalperspective, the important task for educators ismatching curricula to the level of children's emerg ing mental abilities: hence the principle of developmentalappropriateness.Cur riculum materials should be introduced only after a child has attained the level of mental ability needed tomaster them. This in turnmeans that curricula must be studied and analyzed to determine the level of mental ability that is required to comprehend them. From a psychometric point of view, the most important task for educators is matching children with others of equal ability. Bright children are assumed to be able to learn more in a given time than less bright children. In practice, this phi losophy leads to so-called "ability group 114 PHIDELTA KAPPAN ed such principles as "mass versus dis tributed" or "whole versus part" learning, which were presumed to operate inde pendently of the content to be learned. Indeed, early studies of memory em ployed nonsense syllables in order to eliminate the effect of content on the study of memory. The limitations of this approach were dramatically demonstrated by Jerome Bruner, Jacqueline Goodenough, and George Austin in their seminal work on problem solving.4Before thepublication of their work, problem solving was spo ken of in terms of "trial and error" or "sudden insight" because most of the work had been done with animals. What Bruner and his colleagues demonstrated was that human subjects, when present ed with complex problems, employ com plex problem-solving activities - in oth er words, strategies." Put differently, the content of the problem determines the level of the problem-solving activities that humans employ. this insight seems to have Nonetheless, been lost. The current interest in teach ing young children such things as think ing skills,5 learning strategies,6 or com puter programming7 reflects a regres sion to the idea that thought and content can be treated separately. It is assumed that - once children learn thinking skills or learning strategies or computer pro gramming - these skills will automati cally be transferred to different kinds of content. To be sure, transfer of training does occur, but it is far from automatic. Transfer happens when students are ac learners.8 But what tive, not passive, can we possibly mean by activity if not that students are consciously aware of the content they are thinking about or apply ing strategies to?Mental processes are al ways content-oriented. The developmental approach implies that there is little or no automatic trans fer from one subject to another, where as the psychometric approach assumes that the skills and strategies of thinking often transfer spontaneously to new areas. Conception of knowledge. From a de velopmental perspective, is knowledge always a construction, inevitably reflect ing the joint contributions of the subject and the object. This is far from a new idea, and it harks back to the Kantian resolution of idealist (all knowledge is a mental construction) and empiricist (all knowledge is a copy of an externally ex isting world) interpretations of how we 0 0 0M~o that are different from those of their peers come to know the world.9 Kant argued that themind provides the "categories" of not "wrong." It is, in fact, as developmen tally appropriate as the older child's grasp and teachers.Unfortunately, these ideas knowing, while the realworld provides of conservation. From thepsychometricpoint of view, are often regarded as wrong rather than as different and original. One bright is something that a child ac knowledge inde quires and that can be measured child, when asked to write something is thus always a the content. Knowledge construction of the mind's interaction with the world and cannot be reduced to one or the other. What Piaget added to the Kantian so lution - and what makes Piaget a neo Kantian - was the demonstration that the categories of knowing (themental oper ations of intelligence) are not as Kant had supposed. Rather, gories change with age. This a developmental dimension to constant, the cate idea adds the Kant pendently from theprocesses of acquisi about the color blue, wrote about Picas so's Blue Period and was teased and tion. This separation is reflected in the jeered. A greater appreciation for such distinctionbetween intelligencetestsand differenceswould make the lifeof bright achievement tests.One consequence of children in our schools a lot easier. Conception of the aims of education. the separation between learning and con tent is that knowledge ismeasured against an external standard that is independent of the learner. When compared to such an external standard, a child's responses can be assessed as being either "right" or The aims of developmental education are straightforward. If the learner is seen as 1650; two plus two equals four, not five. aims of education a growing individualwith developing abilities, if learning is regarded as a cre is seen ative activity, and if knowledge ianversion of theconstructionof knowl then the aim of educa as a construction, edge.As theirmental operationsdevelop, "wrong." tion must surely be to facilitate this de Certainly, there is a right and a wrong children are required to reconstructthe realities theyconstructedat theprevious with respectto some typesof knowledge. velopment, thiscreativeactivity, and this developmental level. In effect, a child The Bastille was stormed in 1789, not in construction of knowledge. Piaget put the creates and re-creates reality out of his from a developmental or her experienceswith theenvironment. We have to distinguish here between perspective thisway: what I have elsewhere termedfisdamen The reality of the young child - his tal knowledge, which we construct on or her knowledge of the world - is thus The principal goal of education is to our own, andderivedknowledge,which create men who are capable of doing different from the reality of the older child and adult. For example, young chil dren believe that a quantity changes in amount when it changes in appearance that, say, the amount of liquid in a low, flat container is greater when it is poured into a tall, narrow one. Older children, whose reality is different, can appreci ate the fact that a quantity remains the same in amount despite changes in its appearance. In other words, older chil dren recognize that quantity is conserved. From a developmentalperspective, the young child's conception of quantity is is constructed by others and which we must acquire at second hand. 10 The terms right and wrong are useful only in connectionwith derived knowledge. The developmental approach intro duces the idea that there can be differ ences in knowledge without any refer ence to "right" or "wrong." The idea of difference, ratherthanof correctness, is important not only with respect to fun but also with re damental knowledge, spect to creative thinking.For example, many bright children come up with ideas new things, not simply repeating what other generations have done - men who are creative, inventive, and dis coverers. The second goal of education is to formminds which can be critical, can verify, and not accept everything that is offered. The greater danger today is of slogans, collective opin ions, readymade trendsof thought.We have to be able to resist them individu ally, to criticize, to distinguish be tween what is proven and what is not. So we need pupils who are active, who learn early to find out by themselves, partly by their own spontaneous activi ty and partly through material we set up for them; who learn early to tell what is verifiable and what is simply the first idea to come to them." The aim of developmental education, then, is to produce thinkers who are cre ative and critical. This aim will not be achieved, however, by teachingthinking skills to children and adolescents. Rath er, the way to pursue this aim is by creat ingdevelopmentallyappropriatelearning PRESCHOOL that challenge the child's environments emerging mental abilities. Creative think ing and critical thinking are not skills to be taught and learned. They reflect ba sic orientations toward the self and the world that can be acquired only when children are actively engaged in con structing and reconstructing their physi cal, social, and moral worlds. The aim of psychometric education is to produce children who score high on tests of achievement. In other words, the OCTOBER 1989 115 0 0 0 From a developmentalpoint of view, the recommendation of the Holmes he developmental approach seeks to create stu dents who want to know, not students who know what we want. Group that we do away with the under graduate major in education and substi tute a year or two of graduate training and internshipwill not producebetter teach ers. There is a need for teacher training at the undergraduate level - not in tradi tionaleducationcourses, but in child de velopment. Curriculum. From a developmental the ac aim of education is to maximize of curious, active learnersmust precede point of view, there are several princi quisition of quantifiableknowledge and theacquisitionof particularinformation. ples thatshouldguide theconstructionof skills. Perhaps formerSecretaryof Edu To put thedifferencemore succinctly, the thecurriculum.First, a curriculummust cationWilliam Bennett stated this view developmentalapproach seeks to create be constructedempirically, not a priori. of the aims of education as well as any studentswho want toknow,whereas the There is no way to figure out how chil one: psychometricapproachseeks toproduce dren learna subjectwithout studyinghow they actually go about learning it. Thus studentswho knowwhat we want. We should want every student to know how mountains are made, and that formost reactions there is an equal and opposite reaction. They should know who said "I am the state" and who said "Ihave a dream." They should know about subjects and predicates, about isosceles triangles and ellipses. They should know where the Ama zon flows and what the First Amend ment means. They should know about the Donner party and about slavery, and Shylock, Hercules, and Abigail Adams, where Ethiopia is, and why there is a Berlin Wall. 12 it is truly a scandal that curriculum pub lishers not only fail to do research on the materials they produce, but also fail even IMPLICATIONS OF A DEVELOPMENTAL PHILOSOPHY Now to field-test them! Inno otherprofession that we have looked at these two contrastingeducationalphilosophies,we can review a few of the implications for the practice of education of adopting a de velopmentalperspective.Once again,my interpretationis largely based on the Piagetian idea of the development of in telligence. Teacher training.Studentsofmost dis ciplines must learn the basic material of their discipline. A physics studenthas In this statement Bennett echoes a theme that was also sounded in A Nation at Risk, which was published three years earlier and decried the poor performance of American students on achievement tests, especially when compared to the performance of children from other na tions.Moreover, Bennett's remarksfore shadowed the best-selling critiques of and education by Allan Bloom U.S. E. D. Hirsch, Jr., which charged that American education was failing to pro vide childrenwith the basic knowledge of western civilization.'3 Young people should certainly be ex posed to Shakespeare, they should know the basics of geography, and they should be familiar with current events. A de velopmental approachto educationdoes not deny the importance of such knowl edge. The difference between the two ap proaches is amatter of which acquisition comes first. Those who hold a develop mental philosophy believe thatchildren who are curious, active learners will ac that Ben quire much of the knowledge nett, Bloom, and Hirsch call for - and many other things as well. But, from a the creation developmental perspective, 116 KAPPAN PHIDELTA to learn about the rules that govern the physicalworld; a chemistrystudentmust learn how the basic chemical elements interact; a biology student must learn about plants and animals. Education is perhaps the only discipline wherein stu dents do not learn the basic material of the discipline at the outset. Students take courses in curriculum, in methods, in educationalphilosophy, in assessment, and in classroom management. They take only one (or at most two) courses in educationalor developmentalpsycholo gy. But the basic material of education is not curriculum. Nor is it assessment or methods. The basic material of education is children and youth. A teacher training program that is truly developmentally ap propriate would have its students major in child development. Trained in this way, a teacher would be, first and fore most, a child development specialist. Stu dents with a strong foundation in child can integrate what they development learn about curriculum, assessment, and management with what they know about how children of various ages think and learn. would we allow a product to be placed on themarket without extensive field testing. In a truly developmental system of edu cation, teacherswould have the oppor tunity to construct and test their own materials. They could see what works and what doesn't, and they could try out different sequences and methods. The way curriculum materials work will al ways depend on the specific group of in any given children in the classroom year. So a curriculum should never be final; it shouldalways be open, flexible, and innovative. Such a curriculum is ex citing for the teacher and for the pupils and makes both learning and curriculum innovationcooperative ventures. I believe that a curriculum Second, should be localized, for particularly elementary schools. I know that this is contrary to trends in other countries, which have uniform curricula for all chil dren. Japan and France are but two of the countries with such uniform national cur ricula. England, too, will be initiating a uniform national curriculum in 1990. But the such national curricula eliminate possibility of localizingmaterials to in clude particulars from the environment inwhich children actually live and learn. Such localized curricula hold a great deal of intrinsic interest for children. For example, in learning math, children liv ing in Hawaii might be asked to match coconuts and palm trees, whereas chil dren living in the Northeast might be asked to match acorns and oaks. Like it would add to children's enjoy wise, ment if the stories they read took place D 0D-DD_ 0 & in their own communityor one similar to it. In social studies, too, children are cooperatively andwho experimentwith curriculum materials are teaching as well trulybe developmentally appropriateif itsunderlyingphilosophy is psychomet ric. as learning. One way to highlight the difference How can we change that underlying between authentic teachingand psycho educationalphilosophy? Itmight seem be sure, childrenlike storiesaboutplaces metrically oriented teachingis to look at that what is required is a paradigm shift and events that are new to them. None how each typeof instructionhandles the of the sort described by Thomas Kuhn as theless, they also enjoy reading stories asking of questions. In psychometrical characterizingmajor scientific revolu that relate directly to the world they live ly oriented teaching, the teacheroften tions.14Yet neither the developmental in.Children,no less thanadults,appreci asks students questions to which the thinking of Freud nor that of Piaget has ate both fantasy and the realism of local teacheralreadyknows theanswers.The been sufficientto effect sucha shift.This reference. purpose is to determinewhether the stu may reflect the fact that educational prac Finally, we need to studycurricula to dents have the same information as the tice is dictatedmore by social, political, determine their level of developmental teacher.But asking questions towhich and economic considerations than it is by difficulty. Developmental difficulty is one already has the answers is not authen science. Unfortunately, amajor shift in quite different frompsychometric diffi tic behavior.A much more meaningful educational philosophy is more likely culty. The psychometric difficulty of a approach is to ask children questions to to come about as a result of economic delighted to find a picture of a building that they have actually been in, rather than one that they have never seen. To curriculum or a test item is determined by the number of children of a particu lar age who successfully learn the mate rial or who get the item correct. A cur riculumor test item isgenerally assigned to the grade or age level at which of the children can succeed. 75% Developmental difficulty, by contrast, must be determined by examining the ac tual "errors" children make in attempting to master a problem or task. For exam ple, when young children who have been taught the short a sound are asked to learn the long a as well, they have great diffi culty. The problem is that they are be ing asked to grasp the fact that the same letter can have two different sounds. Un that the same symbol can derstanding stand for two different sounds, however, requires the attainment of themental abil ities thatPiaget calls concreteoperations. A teacher who holds a developmental philosophy would thus avoid teaching phonics until he or she was quite sure that most of the children could handle con crete operations. Because the develop mental difficulty of any particular prob lem or task can be determined only by active investigation, part of the ex perimental work of teaching would be to explore the developmentaldifficulty of the available curriculum materials and to try out new materials that might work differently or better. Instruction.Developmentally speak ing, it is as impossible to separate the learning process from the material to be learned as it is to separate learning from instruction.This is authentic teaching. From this perspective, the teacher is al so a learner, and the students are also which one doesn't have the answers. Finding the answers can then be a learn than as a result of scientific in novation. ing experience for teacherand students alike. The authentic teacher asks ques tions to get information and to gain un not to test what students derstanding, know or understand. Such questioning reflects the fact that the authentic teach er is first and foremost an enthusiastic learner. Assessment. Developmental assess ment involvesdocumentingthework that a child has done over a given period of time. Usually this is done by having a child keep a portfolio that includes all of his or her writing, drawing, math ex plorations, and so on. In looking through such a portfolio, we can get a good idea of the quality of work that the child is capable of doing and of his or her prog ress over the given period. Psychometricassessmentinvolvesmeas uring a child's achievement by means of commercial or teacher-made tests. A child's progress is evaluated according to his or her performance on such tests. Unlike a portfolio of work, the psycho metric approach yields a grade that sym bolizes both the quantity and the quality of the work that the child has done over a given period of time. Although some testing can be useful, it is currently so overused thatmany children and parents are more concerned about grades and test scores than about what a child has learned. The documentation of a child's work tends to avoid that danger. I have tried to demonstrate that, while the idea of developmentally appropriate practice has been well received among educators, it really has little chance of teachers.The teacherwho experiments being widely implemented.Without a with the curriculum is learning about the change inunderlyingphilosophy, chang curriculum and about the children he or she teaches. And children who work necessity es in educational practice will be super ficial at best. No classroom or school can 1. Sue Bredekamp, Developmental^ Appropriate Practice National Association (Washington, D.C: for the Education of Young Children, 1987). 2. Jean Piaget, The Psychology of Intelligence & Kegan Paul, 1950). (London: Routledge 3. Jean Piaget and B?rbel Inhelder, The Child's Conception of Space (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1956); Jean Piaget, B?rbel Inhelder, and Alina The Child's Conception Szeminska, of Geometry (New York: Basic Books, 1960); Jean Piaget, The Child's Conception of Time (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1967); and idem, The Child's Concep tion of Movement and Speed (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1970). 4. Jerome S. Bruner, Jacqueline J. Goodenough, and George A. Austin, A Study in Thinking (New York: Wiley, 1956). 5. Joan Boykoff Baron and Robert J. Sternberg, Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Practice (New York: Freeman, 1987). 6. Edwin Weinstein and Richard Edwin Mayer, The of Learning Strategies," inMerlin Teaching C. Wittrock, ed., Handbook of Research on Teach ing, 3rd ed. (New York: Macmillan, 1986). 7. Seymour Papert, Mindstorms (New York: Ba sic Books, 1980). 8. David N. Perkins and Gavriel Salomon, Teach vol. 46, ing for Transfer," Educational Leadership, 1988, pp. 22-32. 9. Immanuel Kant, Critique of Pure Reason (New York: Wiley, 1943). 10. David Elkind, Miseducation: at Preschoolers Risk (New York: Knopf, 1987). 11. Quoted in Richard E. Ripple and Verne E. A Report of Rockcastle, eds., Piaget Rediscovered: on Cognitive Studies and Curricu the Conference :School of Educa lum Development (Ithaca, N.Y. tion, Cornell University, 1964), p. 5. 12. William First Lessons: A Report on in America (Washington, of Education, Department 1986), p. 3. 13. Allan Bloom, The Closing of the American Mind (New York: Simon & Schuster, 1987); and E. D. Hirsch, Jr., Cultural Literacy: What Every to Know American Needs (Boston: Houghton Elementary D.C: U.S. J. Bennett, Education Mifflin, 1987). 14. Thomas S. Kuhn, The Structure of Scientific 2nded. Revolutions, (Chicago: University of Chica go Press, 1970). K) OCTOBER 1989 117