A requirement for the immediate early gene Zif268 in the
Transcription
A requirement for the immediate early gene Zif268 in the
© 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles A requirement for the immediate early gene Zif268 in the expression of late LTP and long-term memories M. W. Jones1, M. L. Errington1, P. J. French1, A. Fine1, T. V. P. Bliss1, S. Garel2, P. Charnay2, B. Bozon3, S. Laroche3 and S. Davis3 1 Division of Neurophysiology, National Institute for Medical Research, Mill Hill, London NW7 1AA, UK 2 Biologie Moléculaire du Développement, INSERM U-368, Ecole Normale Supérieure, 75230 Paris, France 3 Laboratoire de Neurobiologie de l’Apprentissage, de la Mémoire et de la Communication, CNRS UMR 8620, Université Paris-Sud, 91405 Orsay, France Correspondence should be addressed to S.D. ([email protected]) The induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus is associated with a rapid and robust transcription of the immediate early gene Zif268. We used a mutant mouse with a targeted disruption of Zif268 to ask whether this gene, which encodes a zinc finger transcription factor, is required for the maintenance of late LTP and for the expression of long-term memory. We show that whereas mutant mice exhibit early LTP in the dentate gyrus, late LTP is absent when measured 24 and 48 hours after tetanus in the freely moving animal. In both spatial and non-spatial learning tasks, short-term memory remained intact, whereas performance was impaired in tests requiring long-term memory. Thus, Zif268 is essential for the transition from short- to long-term synaptic plasticity and for the expression of long-term memories. Distinct cellular and molecular mechanisms underlie different temporal phases of hippocampal LTP: induction requires elevated postsynaptic calcium1, early maintenance is mediated by the activation of protein kinases2, and later phases depend on gene transcription3,4 and protein synthesis5–7. Consistent with the presumption that synaptic plasticity provides the neural basis for long-term information storage in the brain, several forms of longterm memory involving both hippocampal and cortical structures also depend on protein synthesis8–10. Following induction of LTP in the dentate gyrus, a cascade of genes is activated at different time points11,12. The transient activation of immediate early genes is believed to be a critical step in these successive waves of gene expression. Zif268 (also known as Krox-24, EGR-1 or NGFI-A) is one such immediate early gene, encoding a zinc finger transcription factor13–15. In the dentate gyrus, Zif268 mRNA is transiently upregulated between ten minutes and two hours following LTP-inducing stimulation16–18, a time course that precludes a role for Zif268 protein in the earliest stages of LTP, but that is consistent with an involvement in the transition from the early phase of LTP to the protein synthesisdependent late phase19. Expression is also upregulated in the insular cortex following LTP-inducing stimulation of afferents from the basolateral amygdala20, suggesting a role for Zif268 in activity-dependent neuronal function in other brain regions. The association between Zif268 activation and LTP has been known for over a decade. More recent studies have provided evidence that Zif268 mRNA is upregulated in the hippocampus in the behaving rat following exposure to a novel stimulus21, and in the inferior temporal gyrus of the monkey during associative learning22. However, it remains uncertain whether the activation of Zif268 is necessary for the generation of late LTP, or for the formation of long-term memories. To address these questions, nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 we examined short- and long-term synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus of mice with a targeted inactivation of the Zif268 gene23, using both anesthetized and freely moving animals. We have also examined a variety of hippocampus-dependent and independent tasks that make demands on short- and/or longterm memory. Our results demonstrate that activation of Zif268 is required for the expression of long-term but not short-term synaptic and behavioral plasticity. RESULTS Hippocampal anatomy in Zif268–/– mice We used several histochemical and immunohistochemical markers to examine hippocampal anatomy in control and mutant mice. Nissl staining (data not shown) and NeuN immunoreactivity (which labels a neuronal-specific DNA-binding protein24; Fig. 1a) were indistinguishable in wild-type (Zif268+/+) and homozygous mutant (Zif268–/–) mice. Parvalbumin labels a calcium-binding protein in a subpopulation of GABAergic interneurons in the hippocampus25, again showing similar staining in both Zif268 +/+ and Zif268 –/– mice (Fig. 1b). Synaptophysin immunoreactivity in synaptic layers also appeared normal in Zif268–/– mice (Fig. 1c). Thus, basic neuronal architecture was not affected by the Zif268 mutation. Short-term plasticity and LTP in anesthetized mice We measured LTP and short-term plasticity of both the field excitory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) and population spike in urethane-anesthetized mice. Paired-pulse stimulation (inter-pulse interval, 10–100 ms) at intensities sub-threshold for a population spike resulted in facilitation of the fEPSP, maximal at interpulse intervals of 10–20 ms, in both Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice (Fig. 2a). Paired-pulse facilitation of the fEPSP is attributable to 289 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com a b c Fig. 1. Gross hippocampal anatomy is normal in Zif268–/– mice. (a) NeuN immunoreactivity, (b) parvalbumin staining and (c) synaptophysin immunoreactivity all appeared similar in Zif268–/– (right) and Zif268+/+ (left) mice. presynaptic mechanisms26. Paired-pulse stimulation at intensities just above threshold for evoking a population spike resulted in complete suppression of the population spike at short interstimulus intervals (10–25 ms) and spike facilitation at longer intervals in both Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice (Fig. 2b). Spike facilitation peaked at inter-pulse intervals of 60–80 ms in all mice tested. This profile of spike depression at short inter-stimulus intervals followed by facilitation at longer intervals reflects recurrent inhibition and disinhibition and is therefore, in part, a postsynaptic phenomenon. Thus, short-term presynaptic plasticity and network excitability in the dentate gyrus seem to be normal in Zif268 mutant mice. Following tetanic stimulation, both genotypes showed significant and similar potentiation of the population spike 50–60 minutes after tetanus (2.8 ± 0.5 mV increase in Zif268+/+ and 2.8 ± 0.7 mV in Zif268–/– mice, p < 0.05 versus baseline; Fig. 2c). LTP of the fEPSP slope was also similar in the two genotypes (23 ± 5.3% in Zif268+/+ and 21 ± 6.1% in Zif268–/– mice, p < 0.05 with respect to baseline; Fig. 2d). Fig. 2. Short-term synaptic plasticity and LTP in the dentate gyrus of anesthetized Zif268 mutant mice. (a) At low stimulus intensities, paired-pulse facilitation of EPSP amplitude shows the same dependence on inter-stimulus interval (ISI) in Zif268–/– (, n = 5) and Zif268+/+ mice (, n = 4). Sample responses show facilitation at 15 ms ISI in a Zif268–/– mouse. (b) At stimulus intensities just above threshold for evoking a population spike, paired-pulse depression and facilitation of the population spike also show the same dependence on ISI in Zif268–/– and Zif268+/+ mice. Sample responses show depression (10 ms ISI) and facilitation (50 ms ISI) from a Zif268–/– mouse. (c, d) LTP of the population spike (c) and the field EPSP (d) in Zif268–/– and Zif268+/+ mice (, n = 5, and , n = 4, respectively). Mean changes in the amplitude of the population spike (c), and percentage changes in the slope of the field EPSP (d), plotted with respect to the mean values in the 10 min preceding tetanic stimulation (arrows). Test stimuli were given to the perforant path at a frequency of 1 per 30 s. A similar level of potentiation of the population spike and EPSP was seen in Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice for the first hour after tetanus. 290 Long-lasting synaptic plasticity in freely moving mice We investigated LTP over several days in freely moving mice. Test stimulus intensities required to evoke responses with comparable fEPSP slopes and population spike amplitudes were not different between genotypes (Fig. 3a). Following two days of baseline recording, tetanic stimulation of the perforant path induced significant potentiation of the population spike in all genotypes (p < 0.05 versus baseline for spike amplitude 50–60 minutes after tetanus in Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice, p < 0.01 in Zif268+/– mice). The decrease in the latency of the population spike after tetanic stimulation made it impractical to compare the initial slope of the fEPSP before and after LTP induction (Fig. 3a). Thus, as in anesthetized mice, potentiation in mutants was similar to that in wild types in the first hour following induction. Wildtype mice maintained significant spike potentiation 24 and 48 hours following the tetanic stimulation (p < 0.05 versus control). In contrast, neither Zif268+/– nor Zif268–/– mice supported significant potentiation at these time points (p > 0.06 versus control values, p < 0.05 versus Zif268+/+ mice; Fig. 3b). Thus, the Zif268 gene is necessary for the expression of the later phases of LTP in the dentate gyrus. Lack of expression of Zif268 in the hippocampus We examined levels of Zif268 mRNA in Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice by in situ hybridization. The results confirmed a complete absence of Zif268 mRNA in the Zif268–/– mice, whereas in the a b c d nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles Fig. 3. LTP in the dentate gyrus of awake Zif268 mutant mice. (a) Stimulus intensity required to evoke field potentials with comparable EPSP slopes and population spike amplitudes was independent of genotype. Sample responses are shown for mice of each genotype before (dotted line) and 10 min after (solid line) tetanic stimulation. Values for the mean amplitudes (± s.e.m.) of the population spike are given 1 h and 48 h after the tetanus (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 compared to pre-tetanus amplitudes). (b) Time course of LTP in the awake animal. Baseline responses were recorded for 2 days (20 min per day). A tetanus was delivered (arrow) on the second day, and responses were monitored for 1 h, and again for 20 min per day for the next 2 days. Zif268+/+ (, n = 7), Zif268+/– (, n = 11) and Zif268–/– (, n = 6) mice all showed significant potentiation of the population spike 1 h after tetanic stimulation, but only Zif268+/+ mice maintained this potentiation over the following 48 h. a Zif268+/+ mice, Zif268 expression was normal and LTP inducible (Fig. 4). The construct used to inactivate Zif268 in the mutant mice23 involved the insertion of a lacZ cassette downstream of the promoter; in situ hybridization demonstrated normal constitutive and LTP-regulated expression of the lacZ gene in the Zif268–/– mice (Fig. 4), suggesting that the activity of the Zif268 promoter was not affected by the mutation. This was confirmed by in situ hybridization with a common probe derived from the 5´ untranslated region of Zif268, and located upstream of the site of insertion of lacZ. As expected, the common probe detected equivalent levels of LTP-inducible mRNAs in Zif268+/+ and Zif268–/– mice (Fig. 4). b Learning and memory deficits in Zif268 mutant mice We next examined whether the deficits in synaptic plasticity in Zif268 mutant mice, particularly evident in the late phases of LTP, were paralleled by corresponding deficits in learning and memory. We first evaluated short-term, working memory by testing spontaneous alternation in a T-maze, a spatial behavior that depends on the integrity of the hippocampus27. All three genotypes (n = 6 per genotype) showed normal alternation between the arms visited on the first and second runs of a trial when a 30-second delay was imposed between runs (Zif268+/+, 75 ± 6.8%; Zif268+/–, 76 ± 3.0%; Zif268–/–, 69 ± 5.1% alternation; p < 0.05 versus chance in each case). Mice were also tested with a ten-minute delay between runs; again, there was no significant difference in the levels of alternation among genotypes (Zif268+/+, 70 ± 4.4%; Zif268+/–, 71 ± 7.7%; Zif268–/–, 60 ± 6.1%; n = 12 per group). All mice made a similar mean number of entries into the arms during habituation, indicating a similar level of exploratory behavior and locomotion. This type of spatial working memory can therefore occur in the absence of Zif268 expression. To assess the role of Zif268 in longer-lasting forms of spatial and associative memory, we trained mice in four other behavioral tasks. First, we tested spatial learning and memory in an open-field water maze, using a massed training protocol where the acquisition phase was conducted within a two-hour period. All genotypes took the same time to find the escape platform in the first training trial (Fig. 5a). As the training progressed, there was a reduction in escape latencies for all three genotypes. However, both Zif268+/– and Zif268–/– mice took significantly longer than Zif268+/+ mice, suggesting they had a learning deficit (F2,24 = 8.14; p < 0.01). A corresponding memory deficit was evident in a probe trial done 48 hours after training, during which Zif268+/+ mice showed a significant preference for the target quadrant (F3,32 = 6.01, p < 0.01), whereas Zif268+/– and Zif268–/– (F < 1 for both genotypes) mice showed no spatial bias for the training quadrant (Fig. 5b). Swim speeds were similar for all genotypes, and mice did not exhibit floating behavior. The deficits observed in the acquisition and Fig. 4. Expression of Zif268 in the dentate gyrus. In situ hybridization shows that mRNAs for Zif268 (in Zif268+/+ mice) and lacZ (in Zif268–/– mice) are expressed in the dentate gyrus 1 h following LTP-inducing stimulation of the perforant path. (Compare hybridization signal in the left (tetanized) and right (untetanized) dentate gyrus in each case.) A hybridization probe for a region of the Zif268 gene transcribed in both Zif268–/– and Zif268+/+ mice (‘common’) revealed similar expression in mice of both genotypes (right). nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 291 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles a b c d retention (and/or recall) of this task could not therefore be explained by aberrant motor behavior. Complex tasks can often be more efficiently learned with extended and distributed training28. Using a protocol in which training was distributed over ten days, all genotypes showed similar acquisition curves (Fig. 5c), and all groups spent more time in the training quadrant in a probe trial given eight days later (all p-values < 0.01; Fig. 5d). Thus, with extended training, Zif268 mutant mice show normal acquisition and long-term recall. As spatial learning takes place over many trials, we examined performance in three non-spatial tasks in which learning is Fig. 6. Conditioned taste aversion. Aversion indices (volume sucrose consumed/total volume consumed; mean ± s.e.m.) for Zif268+/+ (n = 8), Zif268+/– (n = 10) and Zif268–/– (n = 10) mice, following exposure to LiCl or to NaCl. When sucrose was followed by an intraperitoneal injection of lithium chloride, Zif268+/+ mice avoided sucrose and preferentially drank water 24 h after conditioning (*p < 0.05). In contrast, neither Zif268–/– nor Zif268+/– mice showed a significant aversion to sucrose at 24 h. Control mice in which drinking the novel sucrose solution was followed by a sodium chloride injection (n = 4 for each genotype) drank similar volumes of sucrose and water. Fig. 5. Spatial navigation in the water maze. (a) During massed training, all mice took the same amount of time to escape the water on the first trial (Tr1). During acquisition, all mice learned to locate the hidden platform, although the Zif268+/– (, n = 9) and Zif268–/– (, n = 9) were slower to learn the task than the Zif268+/+ mice (, n = 9). (b) In a probe trial given 48 h later, Zif268+/+ mice showed a spatial bias for the training quadrant (*p < 0.01), but this was not the case with Zif268+/– or Zif268–/– mice (F < 1 for both genotypes), which distributed their time equally among the 4 quadrants. (c) If extended and distributed training was given, all mice (Zif268+/+, n = 11; Zif268+/–, n = 13; Zif268–/–, n = 10) learned the task at similar rates and (d) showed retention of the learning in a probe trial given 8 days later (*p < 0.01). achieved after a single trial (conditioned taste aversion and social transmission of food preference) or after a brief training period (novel object recognition). In conditioned taste aversion, waterdeprived mice learn to associate a novel taste (15% sucrose solution) with the malaise induced by injection of lithium chloride29. When subsequently offered the choice between water and sucrose solutions, animals tend to avoid the sucrose. This task is not hippocampus-dependent, but requires structures including the basolateral amygdala and insular cortex. Zif268 mRNA is upregulated following induction of LTP in the pathway connecting these two structures20. We found that 24 hours after LiCl injection, mice showed significant aversion to the sucrose (p < 0.05 versus NaClinjected Zif268+/+ mice; Fig. 6). However, neither Zif268+/– nor Zif268–/– mice exhibited significant aversion at 24 hours (p > 0.1). Analysis of variance confirmed a significant difference in aversion index between genotypes (F2,24 = 9.58; p < 0.01 for three LiCl-injected groups). The same effects were also observed 48 hours after LiCl injections (data not shown). The three groups that were injected with NaCl showed no preference for sucrose or water, either 24 or 48 hours after injection. These data show that Zif268 is necessary for generating a long-term memory of an association formed in a single trial. Because of the malaise induced by injections of LiCl, we were unable to test the mice at short intervals, so we could not determine whether short-term memory of the association was affected in Zif268 mutants. Therefore, in two other experiments, we used a within-animal protocol to test for both short- and longterm memory. In the first task, we used social transmission of food preference to test one-trial learning both immediately and 24 hours after learning. This is an olfactory discrimination task in which rodents show preference for a novel food that has recently been smelled on the breath of another (demonstrator) animal30. Food-deprived demonstrator mice were given either cocoa- or coriander-scented food to eat over a period of two hours and then allowed to interact with observer mice for 20 minutes. Food-deprived observer mice were then given free 292 nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles Fig. 7. Discrimination learning at short and long delays. Olfactory discrimination in social transmission of food preference (a, b) and object recognition (c, d) is normal at short time intervals but impaired at long intervals in Zif268 mutant mice. (a) Preference index exhibited by the observer mice for the scented food eaten by the demonstrator mouse (demonstrated food consumed/total food consumed) at 30-s delay (a) and 24-h delay (b) after interaction between demonstrator and observer mice. All mice (n = 10 for each genotype) learned the discrimination when there was minimal delay (*p < 0.05), showing a significant preference above chance level for the food eaten by the demonstrator mice. When a 24-h delay was imposed, only Zif268+/+ mice (n = 10; *p < 0.05) retained the memory for the odor. Neither Zif268+/– (n = 7) nor Zif268–/– (n = 9) mice showed a significant preference for the flavor to which they had been exposed during the interactive period. (c, d) Percent time spent exploring the novel object (time spent exploring the novel object/total time × 100) at a 10-min and 24-h delay in the object recognition task. (c) All mice (n = 8 for each genotype) spent significantly more time exploring the novel object at the 10 min delay (*p < 0.05). (d) After a 24-h delay, Zif268+/+ and Zif268+/– mice spent more time exploring the novel object (*p < 0.05), whereas Zif268–/– mice no longer showed a preference for the novel object. a b c d choice between cocoa and coriander either 30 seconds after the interaction, or 24 hours later. Thirty seconds after the interaction, all genotypes showed a significant preference for the food they had smelled on the breath of the demonstrators (all p-values < 0.05; Fig. 7a). Twenty-four hours later, however, whereas Zif268+/+ mice still showed a preference for the demonstrated food, Zif268+/– and Zif268–/– mice consumed similar amounts of both food types (Fig. 7b). Analysis of the consumption of each scented food (F1,53 = 1.05; p > 0.05) showed no difference among genotypes, and the amount of food consumed by demonstrators (F2,52 = 1.95; p > 0.05) was equivalent, suggesting that there was no bias toward a particular food, and that the demonstrators for each genotype consumed enough food to transmit the smell. These data show that Zif268 is important for the formation of long-term memory for an olfactory event, whereas short-term memory for the same event is not dependent on Zif268. Rodents tend to explore a novel object in preference to a familiar object. Novel object recognition is hippocampus dependent31, and we used this as a second task in which to assess both short- and long-term memory in the same groups of animals. Mice were allowed to explore 2 objects for 20 minutes. Following a 10-minute or 24-hour delay, one of the familiar objects was replaced with a novel object, and the time spent exploring the novel and familiar objects was measured. At the ten-minute delay, mice of all genotypes spent significantly longer exploring the novel object than the familiar object (all p-values < 0.05; Fig. 7c), indicating similar levels of motivation and short-term memory. In contrast, with a 24-hour interval between exploring the familiar and novel objects, Zif268+/+ and Zif268+/– mice still preferentially explored the novel object (p < 0.01) whereas Zif268–/– mice did not (p > 0.05; Fig. 7d). Thus, as with social transmission of food preference, Zif268+/+ mice were able to retain information acquired 24 hours earlier, whereas Zif268–/– mice could not. The performance of Zif268+/– mice was again intermediate, although, in contrast to their performance in social transmission of food preference, they exhibited significant retention at 24 hours. nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 DISCUSSION In this paper, we demonstrate an absence of late phase LTP in the dentate gyrus of freely moving mice with a targeted inactivation of the immediate early gene Zif268. The observation that LacZ reporter mRNA is upregulated after tetanic stimulation suggests that signaling events upstream of Zif268 transcription are not affected in mice with the mutant gene. We conclude that the absence of late LTP is due to a failure in the synthesis of downstream effector proteins encoded by genes for which Zif268 is an obligatory transcription factor. As a consequence of pituitary and ovarian defects, homozygous Zif268 mutant mice have a reduced body size and are sterile, although heterozygous mice are phenotypically normal in terms of size and fertility23. However, LTP decayed at a similar rate in both heterozygous and homozygous mice, which also showed similar deficits in two of the learning tasks that placed demands on long-term memory, suggesting that endocrine dysfunction does not contribute to the observed phenotype. Histological examination using cellular, neuronal and presynaptic markers confirmed similar cell densities and hippocampal architecture in wild-type and mutant mice, showing that disruption of Zif268 had no gross effects on hippocampal circuitry. Basal synaptic transmission, neuronal excitability and shortterm plasticity were normal in mutant mice. All genotypes showed an equivalent magnitude of LTP for the first hour following its induction. However, whereas LTP was maintained for at least 48 hours after induction in wild-type mice, LTP in both heterozygous and homozygous mutant mice decayed to baseline within 24 hours. The level of Zif268 mRNA in heterozygous animals is approximately half that seen in wild-type mice (data not shown), suggesting that this is insufficient to achieve the levels of Zif268 activation required for the successful expression of late LTP. Other immediate early genes have also been implicated in LTP and learning. For example, mRNA for Homer, a protein that 293 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles binds metabotropic glutamate receptors, is upregulated following LTP induction32,33. Arc (also known as Arg 3.1), another mRNA species upregulated after LTP34,35, is necessary for both late LTP and spatial learning36, as shown by antisense techniques. Together with our results, these data suggest that several immediate early genes act together to establish late LTP. To examine the involvement of Zif268 in short- and longterm memory, we used a variety of behavioral tasks making use of single or repeated training, different types of reinforcement and the processing of spatial or non-spatial information. Some of these tasks are hippocampus dependent, and some are not. Our results provide evidence that at least some forms of short-term memory are intact in the Zif268 mutant mice, because they demonstrated normal levels of spontaneous alternation, an innate behavior that relies upon spatial working memory. In common with early LTP, spontaneous alternation requires activation of the NMDA receptor and phosphorylation of protein kinase C37, events that are upstream of gene transcription, and that seem to be sufficient to mediate shortterm memory. Consistent with the notion that loss of functional Zif268 does not affect short-term memory processes, Zif268 mutant mice were also able to perform olfactory discrimination in social transmission of food preference, and visual discrimination in an object recognition task, when minimal delay was imposed. In contrast to the lack of effect on short-term memory, longterm memory in Zif268 mutant mice was severely impaired. In three forms of learning, conditioned taste aversion, olfactory discrimination and novel object recognition, homozygous mice exhibited no significant recall when tested 24 hours later. These findings support the notion that Zif268 is critical in the expression of the long-term memory trace. Spatial navigation is a more complex type of learning, requiring several trials to reach criterion. During massed training, both heterozygous and homozygous mice showed impaired acquisition and a severe deficit in long-term spatial memory assessed in probe trials 24 hours later, suggesting that Zif268 is required either for memory consolidation during and following the process of learning or for retrieval. We found that spatial memory deficits in Zif268 mutant mice could be completely rescued by extended and distributed training. Similar deficits in the consolidation of learning and the potential for rescue by extended training have been reported in mice with targeted disruption of the gene encoding the cAMP response element binding protein CREB38. In addition, genetic studies in Drosophila have suggested that the ability to form longterm memories following disruption of CREB signaling is influenced by the temporal structure of the training schedule39. The upstream regulatory elements of the Zif268 gene include six serum response elements (SRE) and two cAMP response elements (CRE)40,41, and LTP-dependent transcriptional regulation of Zif268 is controlled by the mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) pathway42. Both CREB and MAPK signaling have been implicated in synaptic plasticity43,44 and certain types of learning45,46. Our data therefore strengthen the evidence that Zif268 is an essential participant in the signaling cascade required for synaptic and behavioral plasticity, with the proviso that in the absence of Zif268, other unidentified signaling pathways can be recruited under certain conditions (such as distributed training) to allow information to enter an accessible long-term memory store. In summary, activation of Zif268 is essential for stabilizing synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus and for the expression of hippocampal and non-hippocampal forms of long-term mem294 ory. Our results also establish an isomorphism between hippocampal LTP and hippocampus-dependent learning with respect to the effects of the Zif268 mutation: short-term plasticity and short-term memory are unaffected, whereas long-term plasticity and long-term memory are impaired. METHODS Animals. Mice were generated using 129/SV ES cells injected into C57BL/6J blastocysts and backcrossed onto a C57BL/6J background23. The mutation involved insertion of a lacZ-neo cassette between promoter and coding sequence, and addition of a frameshift mutation at the level of an Ndel restriction site corresponding to the beginning of the DNA-binding domain. Age-matched (2–8 month old) Zif268 +/+ , Zif268+/– and Zif268–/– littermates were tested in several behavioral tasks, with the sequence of tasks randomized. A subset of mice was used for electrophysiology. All procedures carried out at CNRS were conducted in accordance with recommendations of the EU (86/609/EEC) and the French National Committee (87/848) and those at NIMR in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act (1986). Spontaneous alternation. Mice were habituated to the T-maze for 10–20 min/day for two days. During testing (4–8 trials/day for three days), they were placed in the start box for 30 s and then given a forced choice run (randomly assigned). On entering the open arm, they were held there for 30 s, then returned to the start box, held for 30 s or 10 min and released, with both arms accessible. The percent number of trials on which mice alternated was subjected to angular transformation and differences analyzed with Student’s t-test. Spatial navigation. The water maze (1.5 m diameter) contained an escape platform (10 cm) in a fixed position. After a 1-day habituation period (2 blocks of 4 trials with the hidden platform in the center of the maze) massed trials were given the following day (5 blocks of 5 trials; 120 s intertrial interval; 60 s maximum swim time; 15 min inter-block interval). A 60 s probe trial was run 48 h post training. In distributed training, mice were given 1 block of 4 pre-training trials, and, during acquisition, 2 blocks (5-h interval) of 4 trials (60-s interval; maximum swim time of 90 s) a day for 10 days. A 90-s probe trial was given 8 days later. ANOVA was conducted on escape latencies (acquisition) and time in each quadrant (probe trial). Conditioned taste aversion. Water-deprived mice were trained for 3 days to drink ad libitum from 2 identical water bottles in the home cage (30 min/day). Bottles were weighed to measure consumption. On the conditioning day, mice received 15% sucrose from 2 identical bottles for 30 min. One hour later, mice were either injected with 0.9% saline, or 0.3 M LiCl (10% body weight i.p.). At 24 and 48 h after conditioning, mice could choose between water and sucrose for 30 min. The aversion index (volume of sucrose consumed × 100/total volume consumed) was subjected to angular transformation and analyzed with Student’s t-test (aversion index) and ANOVA (group difference). Social transfer of food preference. Mice were habituated to test cages and placed on a food-restricted diet for several days. On test days, demonstrators were given scented food (coriander, 0.3%, or bitter cocoa, 2.0% by weight) for 2 h. Observers were then exposed to demonstrators for 20 min, and placed in test cages either 30 s or 24 h later, where they had free access to both foods for 20 min. Both male and female mice were used in this task, and as performance was similar between sexes, animals were grouped for analysis. Food consumed was calculated by weighing, before and after testing. The preference ratio (demonstrated food eaten × 100/total food eaten) was analyzed using Wilcoxon ranked scores test. Object recognition. Mice were habituated to test boxes (30 × 20 × 10 cm) for 20 min per day for 3–4 days. Two objects were placed in the box in fixed positions, and mice were allowed two 10-min exploration sessions, with a 10-min interval in home cages. We quantified the time mice spent with both forelimbs orientated toward and within 4 cm of either object. nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com During the test phase (10 min or 24 h later), one object was replaced by a novel object, and time spent exploring the objects was recorded. Exploration indices (time exploring object × 100/total exploration time) were compared using Student’s paired t-test. Surgery and electrophysiology. Acute experiments were done under urethane anesthesia. A concentric bipolar stimulating electrode was positioned in the perforant path, and a glass micropipette recording electrode in the hilus of the ipsilateral dentate gyrus. Pairs of pulses (inter-pulse intervals, 10–100 ms) were used to study paired-pulse facilitation at two intensities, one to evoke a pure EPSP and the other a population spike of approximately 1 mV. Three responses were collected at each and averaged. Test responses were evoked by monophasic stimuli (1/30 s; 100–300 µA, 50–60 µs). Pulse width was doubled during tetani, which consisted of 6 series of 6 trains of 6 stimuli at 400 Hz, 100 ms between trains, 20 s between series. Long-term plasticity was studied in the dentate gyrus of freely moving mice as described47. Following surgery, animals were allowed to recover (at least seven days) before recording, and were habituated to the recording chamber. Test responses elicited by monophasic pulses (80 µs, 26–250 µA, 1/30 s) were recorded for 20-min periods on consecutive days at an intensity that evoked a 1–3 mV population spike. Following two days of stable baseline, a tetanus was delivered to the perforant path (same parameters as above, except pulse duration 100 µs). Responses were measured for 60 min after tetanus and again, for 20 min at 24 h and 48 h after tetanus. Data are expressed as change in amplitude (mV) relative to the baseline measured over the 10 min before tetanic stimulation. Student’s t-test was used to compare mean levels of potentiation. In situ hybridization. One hour after tetanic stimulation, brains were removed, frozen on dry ice and stored at –70°C. Sections (14 µm) were mounted on poly-L-lysine coated glass slides, and in situ hybridization was done as described17. After fixation, sections were hybridized overnight at 42°C in 100 ml buffer containing 50% formamide, 4× SSC (150 mM sodium chloride/50 mM sodium citrate), 10% dextran sulphate, 5× Denhardt’s, 200 mg/ml acid-alkali cleaved salmon testis DNA, 100 mg/ml long-chain polyadenylic acid, 25 mM sodium phosphate (pH 7.0), 1 mM sodium pyrophosphate and 100,000 c.p.m. radiolabeled probe (∼1 ng/ml) under parafilm coverslips. Sections were washed in 1× SSC at 55°C (30 min), 0.1× SSC at room temperature (5 min), dehydrated in 70% and 95% ethanol, and exposed to autoradiographic film. [35S]ATP end-labeled probes (NEN) were generated using terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase (Promega, France) according to manufacturer’s instructions. A 50-fold excess of unlabeled oligonucleotide was used as negative control. Oligonucleotides had a unique sequence (Oswel, UK). Probe sequences were as follows: Zif268, CCGTGGCTCAGCAGCATCATCTCCTCCAGTTTGGGGTAGTTGTCC, complementary to nucleotides spanning amino acids 2–16 (ref. 13). LacZ, TTGGTGTAGATGGGCGCATCGTAACCGTGCATCTGCCAGTTTGAG, complementary to nucleotides 261–305. The probe against the 5´ UTR of Zif268, common to both wild-type and mutant mice, was GGGTTACATGCGGGGTGCAGGGGCACACTGCGGGGAGT, complementary to nucleotides 90–128 upstream of the AUG start codon. Histology. Mice were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS and brains post-fixed overnight. Free-floating sections (40 µm) were washed 4 times in PBS (0.1 M, 10 min per wash). Cresyl violet was used as a general cell stain, anti-NeuN (1/6000, Chemicon) to mark viable cells, antisynaptophysin (1/100, Boehringer Mannheim, France) to mark presynaptic boutons, and anti-parvalbumin (1/1000, Sigma). Non-specific epitopes were blocked by incubation in 10% normal goat serum and 0.1% Triton X-100 in PBS for 1 h. Sections were incubated with the primary antibodies for 48 h at room temperature and then washed three times in PBS. Secondary antibody was applied for 1 h at room temperature, and the sections washed in PBS (×3). Immunostaining was visualized using an ABC elite system (Vector Labs, France) and a VIP revelation kit (Biosystems, France). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported in part by CNRS PICS programme (N°756). We thank P. Veyrac and M. Guegan for doing the immunohistochemistry, and S. Hiard for rearing and genotyping the mice. nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001 RECEIVED 23 OCTOBER 2000; ACCEPTED 16 JANUARY 2001 1. Lynch, G., Larson, J., Kelso, S., Barrionuevo, G. & Schottler, F. Intracellular injections of EGTA block induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Nature 305, 719–721 (1983). 2. Soderling, T. R. & Derkach, V. A. Postsynaptic protein phosphorylation and LTP. Trends Neurosci. 23, 75–80 (2000). 3. Nguyen, P. V., Abel, T. & Kandel, E. R. Requirement of a critical period of transcription for induction of late-phase LTP. Science 256, 1104–1107 (1994). 4. Frey, U., Frey, S., Schollmeier, F. & Krug, M. Influence of actinomycin D, an RNA synthesis inhibitor, on rat hippocampal neurons in vivo and in vitro. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 490, 703–711 (1996). 5. Bliss, T. V. P. & Collingridge, G. L. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361, 31–39 (1993). 6. Frey, U., Krug, M., Reymann, K. G. & Matthies, H. Anisomycin, an inhibitor of protein-synthesis, blocks late phases of LTP phenomena in the hippocampal CA1 region in vitro. Brain Res. 452, 57–65 (1988). 7. Otani, S. & Abraham, W. C. Inhibition of protein synthesis in the dentate gyrus, but not the entorhinal cortex, blocks the maintenance of long-term potentiation in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 106, 175–180 (1989). 8. Meiri, N. & Rosenblum, K. Lateral ventricle injection of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin impairs long-term memory in a spatial memory task. Brain Res. 789, 48–55 (1998). 9. Squire, L. R. & Barondes, S. H. Memory impairment during prolonged training in mice given inhibitors of cerebral protein synthesis. Brain Res. 56, 215–225 (1973). 10. Rosenblum, K., Meiri, N. & Dudai, Y. Taste memory: the role of protein synthesis in gustatory cortex. Behav. Neural Biol. 59, 49–56 (1993). 11. Fazeli, M. S., Cobet, J., Dunn, M. J., Dolphin, A. C. & Bliss, T. V. P. Changes in protein synthesis accompanying long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus in vivo. J. Neurosci. 13, 1346–1353 (1993). 12. Davis, S. & Laroche, S. A molecular biological approach to synaptic plasticity and learning. C. R. Acad. Sci. III 321, 97–107 (1998). 13. Milbrandt, J. A nerve growth factor-induced gene encodes a possible transcriptional regulatory factor. Science 85, 7857–7861 (1987). 14. Lemaire, P., Revelant, O., Bravo, R. & Charnay, P. Two genes encoding potential transcription factors with identical DNA-binding domains are activated by growth factors in cultured cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 85, 4691–4695 (1988). 15. O’Donovan, K. J., Tourtellotte, W. G., Milbrandt, J. & Baraban, J. M. The EGR family of transcription-regulatory factors: progress at the interface of molecular and systems neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 22, 167–173 (1999). 16. Cole, A. J., Saffen, D. W., Baraban, J. M. & Worley, P. F. Rapid increase of an immediate early gene messenger RNA in hippocampal neurons by synaptic NMDA receptor activation. Nature 340, 474–476 (1989). 17. Wisden, W. et al. Differential expression of immediate early genes in the hippocampus and spinal cord. Neuron 4, 603–604 (1990). 18. Richardson, C. L. et al. Correlation between the induction of an immediate early gene, zif268, and long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 580, 147–154 (1992). 19. Abraham, W. C., Dragunow, M. & Tate, W. P. The role of immediate early genes in the stabilization of long-term potentiation. Mol. Neurobiol. 5, 297–314 (1991). 20. Jones, M. W., French, P., Bliss, T. V. P. & Rosenblum, K. Molecular mechanisms of long-term potentiation in the insular cortex in vivo. J. Neurosci. 19, RC36, 1–8 (1999). 21. Hall, J., Thomas, K. L. & Everitt, B. J. Rapid and selective induction of BDNF expression in the hippocampus during contextual learning. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 533–535 (2000). 22. Miyashita, Y., Kameyama, M., Hasegawa, I. & Fukushima, T. Consolidation of visual associative long-term memory in the temporal cortex of primates. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70, 197–211 (1998). 23. Topilko, P. et al. Multiple pituitary and ovarian defects in Krox-24 (NGFI-A, Egr-1)-targeted mic. Mol. Endocrinol. 12, 107–122 (1998). 24. Wolf, H. K. et al. NeuN: a useful neuronal marker for diagnostic histopathology. J. Histochem. Cytochem. 44, 1167–1171 (1996). 25. Kosaka, T., Katsumaru, H., Hama, K., Wu, J. Y. & Heizmann, C. W. GABAergic neurons containing the Ca2+-binding protein parvalbumin in the rat hippocampus and dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 419, 119–130 (1987). 26. Katz, B. & Miledi, R. The role of calcium in neuromuscular facilitation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 195, 481–492 (1968). 27. Gerlai, R. A new continuous alternation task in T-maze detects hippocampal dysfunction in mice: a strain comparison and lesion study. Behav. Brain Res. 95, 91–101 (1998). 28. Rasmussen, M., Barnes, C. A. & McNaughton, B. L. A systematic test of cognitive mapping, working memory and temporal discontiguity theories of hippocampal function. Psychobiology 17, 335–348 (1989). 29. Garcia, J., Kimmeldorf, D. J. & Koelling, R. A. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122, 157–158 (1955). 30. Strupp, B. J. & Levitsky, D. A. Social transmission of food preference in adult hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. Comp. Physiol. 98, 257–266 295 © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com © 2001 Nature Publishing Group http://neurosci.nature.com articles (1984). 31. Rampon, C. et al. Enrichment induces structural changes and recovery from nonspatial memory deficits in CA1 NMDAR1-knockout mice. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 238–244 (2000). 32. Brakeman, P. R. et al. Homer: A protein that selectively binds metabotropic glutamate receptors. Nature 386, 284–288 (1997). 33. Kato, A. et al. Novel members of the Vesl/Homer family of PDZ proteins that bind metabotropic glutamate receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 273, 23969–23975 (1998). 34. Link, W. et al. Somatodendritic expression of an immediate early gene is regulated by synaptic activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 5734–5738 (1995). 35. Lyford, G. L. et al. Arc, a growth factor and activity-regulated gene, encodes a novel cytoskeleton-associated protein that is enriched in neuronal dendrites. Neuron 14, 433–445 (1995). 36. Guzowski, J. F. et al. Inhibition of activity-dependent Arc protein expression in the rat hippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and the consolidation of long-term memory. J. Neurosci. 20, 3993–4001 (2000). 37. Walker, D. L. & Gold, P. E. Intrahippocampal administration of both the Dand L- isomers of AP5 disrupts spontaneous alternation behavior and evoked potentials. Behav. Neural Biol. 62, 151–162 (1994). 38. Kogan, J. H. et al. Spaced training induces normal long-term memory in CREB mutant mice. Curr. Biol. 7, 1–11 (1996). 39. Yin, J., Del Vecchio, M., Zhou, H. & Tully, T. CREB as a memory modulator: induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances 296 long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 81, 107–115 (1995). 40. Changelian, P. S., Feng, P., King, T. C. & Milbrandt, J. Structure of the NGFI-A gene and detection of upstream sequences responsible for its transcriptional induction by nerve growth factor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 86, 377–381 (1989). 41. Sakamoto, K. M. et al. 5´ upstream sequence and genomic structure of the human primary response gene, EGR-1/TIS8. Oncogene 6, 867–871 (1991). 42. Davis, S., Vanhoutte, P., Pagès, C., Caboche, J. & Laroche, S. The MAPK/ERK cascade targets both Elk-1 and cAMP response elementbinding protein to control long-term potentiation-dependent gene expression in the dentate gyrus in vivo. J. Neurosci. 20, 4563–4572 (2000). 43. Impey, S., Obrietan, K. & Storm, D. R. Making new connections: role of ERK/MAP kinase signaling in neuronal plasticity. Neuron 23, 11–14 (1999). 44. Bailey, C. H., Bartsch, D. & Kandel, E. R. Toward a molecular definition of long-term memory storage. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 13445–13452 (1996). 45. Silva, A. J., Kogan, J. H., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. CREB and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 127–148 (1998). 46. Blum, S., Moore, A. N., Adams, F. & Dash, P. K. A mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in the CA1/CA2 subfield of the dorsal hippocampus is essential for long-term spatial memory. J. Neurosci. 19, 3535–3544 (1999). 47. Davis, S., Bliss, T. V. P., Dutrieux, G., Laroche, S. & Errington, M. L. Induction and duration of long-term potentiation in the hippocampus of the freely moving mouse. J. Neurosci. Methods 75, 75–80 (1997). nature neuroscience • volume 4 no 3 • march 2001