another look at gender identity
Transcription
another look at gender identity
ANOTHER LOOK AT GENDER IDENTITY IN PREADOLESCENCE by Jonathan Perle A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Charles E. Schmidt College of Science in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton, Florida May 2008 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. David Perry for his guidance and encouragement throughout my thesis and my parents for giving me constant support and providing me with the numerous opportunities they’ve given me. iii ABSTRACT Author: Jonathan Perle Title: Another Look at Gender Identity in Preadolescence Institution: Florida Atlantic University Thesis Advisor: Dr. David G. Perry Degree: Masters of Arts Year: 2008 This thesis examined relations of multiple dimensions of gender identity (same- gender typicality, other-gender typicality, gender contentedness, gender oppression, felt pressure to conform, and gender centrality) to children’s adjustment (global self-worth, narcissism, depression, internalizing behaviors, externalizing behaviors, and prosocial behaviors). Participants were 237 fourth through eighth graders (108 males, 129 females; M age = 11 years, 4 months). Each measure of gender identity related to children’s adjustment in a unique way. Findings also showed gender centrality to moderate relations of other gender identity variables to adjustment, suggesting that how central gender is to a child influences the impact of other gender self-appraisals on the child’s development and adjustment. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables…………...…………………….………………………………………… vii Introduction…………………….………………………………………………………… 1 Conceptualizations of Gender Identity……………….………………………... 2 Additional Gender Identity Variables…………….……………………………. 5 Adjustment Variables………………….……………………………………….. 7 Present Study……………………..……………………………………….……. 8 Same- and Other-Gender Typicality…….……..……………………… 9 Gender Contentedness………………………………………….…….. 10 Felt Pressure………………..………………………………………… 10 Gender Oppression…………..…………………………………..…… 11 Methods…………...………………………………………………………………..…… 12 Participants…………..…..……………………………………………………. 12 Measures…………………………………………………………….………… 12 Results………………….………………………………………………………………. 18 Age and Sex Differences in Measures…………….…………………..……….. 18 Intercorrelations of Gender Identity Measures………………………………… 19 Relations Between Gender Identity and Adjustment Measures…………..…… 20 Gender Centrality as a Moderator……………………………………………… 23 Discussion………………………………..……………………………….…………….. 26 v Other-Gender Typicality…………….…………………………………………. 26 Gender Oppression……………………….……………………..……………… 28 Gender Centrality as a Moderator……………………………………………… 29 Limitations and Future Directions…..…………………………….…………… 30 References……………………………………..…………………………….…………. 32 vi LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations of Measures…………………………... 36 Table 2: Correlations Among Gender Identity Variables and Adjustment Measures………………………………………………………...……… 37 Table 3: Correlations Among Measures by Sex………………………………..… 39 vii Introduction Gender identity, or one’s conception of the self as either male or female, is a key phenomenon that arises early in a child’s development. By 2 to 3 years of age, a child can correctly answer whether he or she is a boy or girl. However, it is not until about age 6 to 7 that the child achieves gender constancy, or the knowledge that his or her gender will not change (Egan & Perry, 2001). This developmental process and identification with one’s gender are crucial to a child’s adjustment, as gender affects many aspects of one’s life, including recreational, academic, occupational, and relationship activities (Egan & Perry, 2001). It also affects self-esteem and other aspects of adjustment (Yunger, Carver, & Perry, 2004). Although most children develop their gender identity and have minimal problems with adjustment, it seems that while growing up, some children question their gender and reflect on their existence as a male or female by asking themselves such questions as, “How well do I fit with my gender category?” For these reasons, it is important to identify what comprises one’s gender identity and the mechanisms underlying its influences on children’s current and future behavior and adjustment. The present project is concerned with adding new variables to an existing multidimensional conceptualization of gender identity with the goal of furthering knowledge relating gender identity to adjustment. 1 Conceptualizations of Gender Identity Gender identity is a central construct in many accounts of psychosocial development and mental health. However, over the years gender identity has been conceptualized in diverse ways. Kagan (1964) believed gender identity to be the degree to which one perceives the self as conforming to cultural stereotypes for one’s gender. Kohlberg (1966) and Zucker et al. (1993) viewed it as knowing that one is a member of one sex rather than another. Bem (1981) defined gender identity, or what she called “gender schematicity,” as the degree to which one internalizes societal pressures for gender conformity. One popular practice (originating with Bem) that has been proven problematic is to assess gender identity in terms of self perceived personality traits (i.e., instrumental and expressive traits). The concern with such an approach is these traits do not relate to other gender phenomena, and therefore it is inadvisable to infer an individual’s gender identity from self-perception of these traits alone (Spence & Helmreich, 1980). Although it is possible that each of the varying views of gender identity has some degree of merit and application, it seems unlikely that gender identity can be captured by a single measure. Egan and Perry (2001) conceptualized gender identity as comprising multiple facets, and they advanced a multidimensional model that took into account different forms of gender identity, each of which is believed to serve different psychological functions and to have different influences on adjustment. As defined by Egan and Perry (2001), gender identity is a construct encompassing an individual’s (a) membership knowledge (knowledge of one’s membership in a gender category), (b) 2 gender typicality (how typical a member of one’s gender the individual feels the self to be), (c) gender contentedness (how happy one is with his or her gender category), (d) felt pressure for gender conformity (pressure from parents, peers, teachers, and the self for conforming to gender stereotypes), and (e) intergroup bias (the idea that one’s own sex is superior to the other). From this conceptualization, gender identity goes beyond basic measures that have previously been used and is defined as a collection of all of these variables that can form a patterned knowledge of the self in relation to gender categories. Egan and Perry’s (2001) model has been shown to be consistent with other multidimensional models (e.g., Ashmore, Deaux, & McLaughlin-Volpe, 2004). It also fits with Spence’s (1985) idea that people not only generate domain-specific selfperceptions of specific aspects of sex-typed behavior (e.g., play with dolls versus trucks) but also integrate diverse information about gender and self to create a more abstract creation of gender identity (e.g., “Overall, am I a good fit within my gender?”). With their multidimensional conceptualization of gender identity, Egan and Perry (2001) hypothesized that higher levels of gender typicality and contentedness, but lower levels of felt pressure and intergroup bias, would benefit adjustment. This hypothesis has received support not only in their own studies but also in studies by others (Carver, Yunger, & Perry, 2003; Smith & Leaper, 2004; Yunger, et al., 2004) and further work by Egan and Perry (2001) also suggested the operation of certain causal pathways. For example, they suggested that gender contentedness motivates gender typing. They stated that it is possible for gender contentedness to influence children’s adoption of sex-typed attributes which would enhance their feelings of gender typicality which would, in turn, 3 influence adjustment. Additionally, it was initially shown that intergroup bias was detrimental to children’s peer relations. However, this relation has not been reliably replicated. Based on their conceptualization of gender identity, Egan and Perry (2001) looked at differences between males and females in gender identity and found substantial differences. Boys have higher scores than girls on gender typicality, gender contentedness, and felt pressure for gender conformity. These results conform to other work (Huston, 1983, Ruble & Martin, 1988) and showcase how boys are more genderidentified than girls and often experience more pressure to conform to their sex role. In society, it is generally deemed unfavorable for a boy to wear dresses or other clothes that are typically worn by girls or to engage in feminine tasks, and those who do are typically ridiculed and made to cease either by parents or peers. Girls seem to be more open to explore cross-gendered acts. If a girl dresses like a boy, she is deemed a “tomboy,” but she is unlikely to be ridiculed to the same high degree as a boy who dresses like a girl. Girls have also been shown to be more negatively influenced by pressure than boys (e.g., felt pressure is associated with low self-esteem mainly for girls). This could be due to girls being more likely to take self-limiting social criticism and negative feedback to heart and to become depressed by it, but it could also reflect the fact that felt pressure to avoid cross-sex behavior is more limiting for girls than for boys because boys’ options are generally more likely to lead to status and rewards than are girls’. Overall, Egan and Perry (2001) feel that children’s adjustment is optimized when (a) they are secure in their conceptions of themselves as typical members of their sex yet (b) feel free to explore 4 cross-sex options when they so desire. Additional Gender Identity Variables This thesis aims to further the multidimensional conceptualization of gender identity proposed by Egan and Perry (2001) by seeing whether new gender identity variables also contribute to children’s adjustment. For the purposes of the current study, six measures of gender identity were used. These were: (a) felt same-gender typicality (self-perceived similarity to one’s own sex), (b) gender contentedness, (c) felt pressure, (d) felt other-gender typicality (self-perceived similarity to the other sex), (e) gender oppression, and (f) gender centrality. The first three measures are from Egan and Perry’s (2001) model. However, the last three are candidates for new additions to the model. Other-gender typicality is a measure of how similar one feels to the other gender (e.g., how much a boy feels like a girl), gender oppression is how much one feels dismayed or feels that it is unfair that they are not allowed to do things of the other gender even though they may like to, and gender centrality is how central (important) gender is to one’s identity. All of these gender identity variables will be analyzed to see they relate to adjustment. Other-gender typicality may be important to measure because it is possible that some children are constantly comparing themselves to members of the opposite sex and wondering if they are sufficiently different. It is important to investigate how a child who feels more typical to the opposite sex would develop in relation to adjustment. Do children who experience this feeling of typicality have poor adjustment? Also, including 5 a measure of other-gender typicality will permit seeing how this measure relates to samegender typicality. Are these self-perceptions independent or do they operate as opposite ends of a single continuum (i.e., are the two measures negatively correlated)? Felt gender oppression was included for a number of reasons. First, it is important to measure how oppressed a child feels by gender-role restrictions independently of felt pressure to conform. While these two are likely to be related, they may contribute independently to adjustment. It is hypothesized that felt pressure and gender oppression will be correlated so that if there was higher gender oppression, the child would also feel higher levels of felt pressure to conform. A second reason to include gender oppression is due to a suggestion by Bigler (2006) that Egan and Perry’s (2001) measure of gender contentedness does not actually measure contentedness with belonging to one gender category but rather measures contentedness with a restricted gender role (i.e., gender oppression). In other words, Bigler claimed that Egan and Perry’s scale of gender contentedness assessed mainly children’s contentment with the fact that cross-gender activities are off limits. Indeed, certain items of the original Egan and Perry contentedness scale were open to this interpretation. In the present study, gender contentment was measured with items less open to this interpretation (e.g., “Are you glad you’re a girl?”), and a new set of items was written to tap gender oppression (e.g., “Do you feel it is unfair that some activities are off limits because they are done mainly by other-sex children?”). It was hypothesized that gender contentedness would relate to favorable signs of adjustment but that felt gender oppression would relate to negative signs of adjustment. 6 The third new variable added to the model was gender centrality. Centrality is a concept put forward in a recent paper by Ruble et al. (2007). Ruble et al. conceptualized centrality as the extent to which one’s group is a salient and pivotal factor in self-concept. They believed this to be important because many studies have documented how identification with a particular social category can promote a sense of belonging, connectedness, and increased positive evaluation with the group. However, these factors may only be important if centrality is important to the individual. Ruble et al. found that higher levels of stability and understanding with one’s gender were associated with higher own-sex centrality and positive evaluation. For these reasons, it can be seen that centrality is an important measure. However, Ruble et al. only measured centrality in relation to age and gender constancy. For the present study, centrality will be added as a gender identity variable and will be used to predict adjustment. Also, as will be explained later, gender centrality was expected to moderate relations of other gender identity variables to adjustment. Adjustment Variables Along with gender identity variables, Egan and Perry (2001) measured various levels of adjustment to test the effects of the gender identity variables. For the purpose of this thesis, six adjustment variables were used: (a) global self-worth, (b) externalizing behaviors, (c) internalizing behaviors, (d) prosocial behaviors, (e) narcissism, and (f) depression. Global self-worth, as proposed by Harter (1998), is one’s overall self-regard or self-esteem. Three adjustment variables were self-reported (global-self worth, 7 narcissism, and depression), and three were peer-reported (externalizing behaviors, internalizing behaviors, and prosocial behaviors). Externalizing behaviors include aggression, dishonesty, disruption, and conflict-proneness. These types of behaviors have been found to be more common to boys. The opposite can be said for girls, who have been shown to internalize more (e.g., be more depressed and anxious). It would be beneficial for an individual to have low levels of both externalizing and internalizing as either represents problematic adjustment. Prosocial behaviors (e.g., getting along with others) are behaviors that are looked upon as positive and have been shown to predict positive adjustment variables. The final two measures are narcissism and depression, which have not been used often in studies linking adjustment and gender identity. Narcissism is important to examine because it could reflect inflated, unrealistically high self-esteem which could be detrimental to the individual in the long run (e.g., by motivating aggression; Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996), and high levels of certain gender identity variables may be conducive to a puffed up self-concept, as explained later. Depression too is an adjustment measure for which low levels would be beneficial, and it too might be influenced by certain kinds of gender identity (e.g., felt gender oppression and felt pressure). Present Study The present study was designed to gather further support for Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional model of gender identity while adding the above mentioned new measures of gender identity. One interest was in seeing whether the new measures of 8 gender identity would contribute to adjustment as individual predictors. A second interest was to see whether the gender centrality measure would moderate relations of other gender identity variables to adjustment: Do the usual measures of gender identity (e.g., gender typicality) relate more strongly to adjustment for children for whom gender is an especially central and salient aspect of identity? It is expected that higher centrality will magnify the contribution of same-gender typicality, gender contentedness, other-gender typicality, and felt pressure to adjustment. This is based on the logic that if gender is salient to the child, then the other components of gender identity should also assume greater importance. Associations were also studied as a function of sex and age. Predicted trends and hypotheses are outlined below. Same- and Other-Gender Typicality It has been shown that when children feel dissimilar to others of their gender they experience dismay. Children who see themselves as atypical members of their own gender may have a constant fear of ridicule, potential rejection from their social group, and possible denial of privileges (Caporael & Brewer, 1991). Due to this, these children will often have lower self-esteem and may be depressed. They could also score higher on internalizing problems, something that might contribute to these children being victimized by peers. Furthermore, if it is the case that the child is high on other-gender typicality (i.e., identifies with the other group), then it is hypothesized that they will be lower on self-esteem and higher on depression and internalizing behaviors, as they are unsure about themselves (Carver, et al., 2003). 9 It is also predicted that the above relations (distress among children who feel gender atypical or similar to the other gender) will be stronger for children with high gender centrality. That is, if the child is high on centrality then high same-gender typicality will especially lead to better adjustment, and high other-gender typicality will especially lead to poor adjustment. These effects are expected to hold for both sexes and for children of all ages. Gender Contentedness It is important for children to feel content with their gender so that they do not worry about themselves and their acceptance by peers. Contentedness has been shown to be a predictor of global self worth. It has also been shown that children who are dissatisfied with their gender are often unhappy and socially maladjusted. It is expected that children who feel discontent with their gender will be especially distressed if they are also high in gender centrality. Similar results are expected for children of different ages and sexes. Felt Pressure Felt pressure has been shown to be negatively correlated with psychological well being and self-regard. Those who feel high pressure will be limited in what they do, due to a fear of what others might think or do to them. Those (especially girls) who have high felt pressure also tend to exhibit higher levels of internalizing behaviors and less prosocial behaviors. Furthermore, it is hypothesized that if gender centrality is salient, 10 then high levels of felt pressure will be especially associated with adjustment problems. Boys are believed to experience higher levels of felt pressure as they are highly sex typed, but gender centrality should moderate effects of felt pressure similarly for both sexes and children of different ages. Gender Oppression Although little work has been done in regard to gender oppression, certain trends are predicted. As noted, high feelings of resentment that other-sex activities are off limits should be depressing to children and contribute to lower self-esteem and internalizing problems. Also, if a child is high on gender centrality and also has high oppression, then he or she will experience strong adjustment problems, as feelings of oppression should be especially salient to such a child. 11 Methods Participants The sample included 237 children (129 females and 108 males) in the fourth through eighth grades of a state university school. Children received parental consent to participate in the study, and the children also signed an assent form acknowledging that the experimenters had explained what is expected of them before the testing began. The racial and ethnic composition of the sample was 53.2% White, 20.3% Black, 18.1% Hispanic, 3.8% Asian, and 4.6% other. Children averaged 11 years 4 months (136.16 months) of age in the fall of the school year. Measures Instruments measuring five aspects of gender identity (same-gender typicality, other-gender typicality, felt pressure for gender conformity, gender contentedness, and gender oppression) were administered via self-report questionnaires. One additional gender identity variable, gender centrality, was also included, especially to see if it moderates relations of the other gender identity variables to the dependent (adjustment) variables. Thus a total of six measures of gender identity were included. The dependent variables, which tapped diverse aspects of personal adjustment, included global self worth, externalizing behavior, internalizing behavior, prosocial behavior, narcissism, and 12 depression. Gender identity measures. Six measures of gender identity were included, the first five of which derived from Egan and Perry (2001), or were adapted from them. The same-gender typicality scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient of .62, 6 items, average inter-item correlation .22) assessed the degree to which a child feels he or she is a typical member of his or her own gender category. A sample item from the boy’s form is: Some boys have the same interests that other boys have Very true for me BUT Sort of true for me Other boys don’t have the same interests that other boys have. Sort of true for me Very true for me The other-gender typicality scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient of .84, 6 items, average inter-item correlation .46) measured the degree to which a child feels that he or she is a typical member of the other gender. A sample item is: Some boys have the same interests that girls have Very true for me BUT Sort of true for me Other boys don’t have the same interests that girls have. Sort of true for me Very true for me The gender contentedness scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient of .77, 5 items, average inter-item correlation .45) assessed the extent to which a child is happy or content with his or her gender category. A sample item is: 13 Some boys feel lucky that they are a boy Very true for me BUT Sort of true for me Other boys don’t feel lucky that they are a boy. Sort of true for me Very true for me The felt pressure for gender conformity (Cronbach alpha coefficient of .86, 7 items, average inter-item correlation .46) measured the degree to which a child feels pressure from parents, peers, and the self to avoid other-gender conformity. A sample item is: The boys I know would be upset if I wanted to play with girls’ toys. Not at all true for me A little true for me Pretty true for me Very true for me The gender oppression scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient of .83, 6 items, average inter-item correlation .45) measured the degree to which a child feels it is unfair or is distressed because he or she is not allowed to partake in other-gender activities. A sample item is: Some boys are upset that they’re not allowed to do all the things that girls are allowed to do Very true for me BUT Sort of true for me Other boys don’t mind that they’re not allowed to do all the things that girls are allowed to do. Sort of true for me Very true for me The gender centrality measurement was measured through a card sorting task consisting of three phases. The measure was adapted from Ruble et al. (2007). Thirteen 14 index cards with computer printed words were presented to the children. The index cards had the words: American, student, fourth grade (or the grade of the student), boy, girl, son, daughter, Hispanic, Black, Asian, White, Arabic, and Native American. The child is initially presented with the thirteen cards, one at a time. A card (e.g., “American”) is presented and put in front of the child on the table, and the experimenter asks the child if the identity on the card applies to him or her (e.g., “Are you an American? Yes or no.”). The “yes” cards are put into one stack for later use, and the “no” cards are discarded. If the child is asked if he or she is an American and the answer is no, he or she is asked to tell his or her nationality, and this nationality is written by the experimenter on a blank card and substituted for the American nationality card. Also, if a child chooses more than one ethnic/racial identity (e.g. Hispanic or White), the child is asked which is more important to him or her (e.g. “Is being Hispanic or being White more important to you?”). After the child has gone through the cards, six remain (nationality, son/daughter, student, boy/girl, grade, and ethnic/racial group). The six cards are then displayed on the table, all at the same time in a rectangle formation. The researcher then asks the child, “Which of these words is the most important thing about you, so important that without it you could no longer be yourself?” After the child has selected the card, it is removed and the researcher asks, “Just looking at the cards that are left, can you tell me now which word is the most important thing about you?” This card is then removed and the experimenter asks the final question of, “Now, looking at the cards that are left here, which word is the least important thing about yourself?” The measure is scored by giving the most important identity a 2, the second most a 1, and the least important identity a -1. 15 The other three identities receive a 0. Adjustment measures. Six adjustment measures were included: global self-worth, narcissism, depression, externalizing behavior, internalizing behavior, and prosocial behavior. The global self-worth scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient .76, 6 items, average inter-item correlation .350) assessed the overall self-esteem of the child. A sample item is: Some kids don’t like the way they’re leading their life Very true for me Other kids do like the way they’re leading their life. BUT Sort of true for me Sort of true for me Very true for me The Narcissism scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient .73, 17 items, average interitem correlation .143) measured a child’s narcissistic tendencies, or belief that they are the best at everything and better than everyone else. A sample item is: I can usually talk my way out of anything. or I try to accept what happens to me because of my behavior. The Depression scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient .75, 10 items, average inter-item correlation .248) measured the depression of a child and how unhappy they are. These questions were from the Beck Children Depression Inventory. A sample item is: □ I am sad once in a while. □ I am sad many times. 16 □ I am sad all the time. The externalizing behavior, internalizing behavior, and prosocial behavior scales were derived from a peer nomination inventory. The child was presented with a grid that listed 16 behaviors (e.g., He makes fun of people.) along the left side of the page, and along the top was a list of names of the other children in that student’s class. There were male and female peer nomination inventories, and male nominations were made only by the males and female nominations were made only by the female students. The children’s scores were calculated as the proportion of the same-sex classmates who checked the child’s name for the item; scale scores were calculated by averaging across items. Externalizing behavior measured the peer’s perception of every child in the class in relation to externalizing behaviors such as physical aggression. A sample item is: “He hits and pushes others around.” Internalizing behavior measured the peer’s perception of every child in the class in relation to internalizing behaviors such as anxiety. A sample item is: “He seems unhappy and looks sad often.” Prosocial behavior measured the peer’s perception of every child in the class in relation to prosocial behavior such as helping others or sharing. A sample item is: “He is good to have in a group because he shares things and gives other people a turn.” The instruments were administered in one individual testing session in a private room at the school. The sessions averaged about one hour per student. All questions and instructions were read aloud to the child to make the testing uniform, and the children followed along and marked their answers. 17 Results Age and Sex Differences in Measures Means and standard deviations of the measures are given separately by sex and age in Table 1. For these analyses the younger children group included the fourth and fifth grades while the older group consisted of the sixth through eighth grades. Initial analyses treated each measure (gender identity and adjustment measures) as a dependent variable with sex and age entered on the same step in a multiple regression analysis. This analysis was used to determine if there are age and sex differences. All results were analyzed using a p value of .05. The results showed that there were significant age differences in the gender identity variables of same-gender typicality (p < .001, increasing as the child gets older), other-gender typicality (p < .01, decreasing as the child gets older), and gender contentedness (p < .001, increasing as the child gets older). There were age differences in the adjustment measures of depression (p < .02, decreasing as a child gets older) and prosocial behavior (p < .001, decreasing as the child gets older). There were significant sex differences in the gender identity variables of other-gender typicality (p < .001, higher for girls), gender oppression (p < .001, higher for girls), and felt pressure (p < .001, higher for boys), and in the adjustment measures of narcissism (p < .02, higher for girls), and depression (p < .01, higher for girls). These results also demonstrate that gender centrality and the adjustment measures of global self-worth, internalizing behavior, and externalizing behavior do not appear to have significant age 18 or sex differences. These effects of age and sex indicated a need to control for these variables in subsequent analyses. Age and sex were also examined as possible moderators of certain other relations under study. Intercorrelations of Gender Identity Measures Relations among all the study variables may be found in Table 2. This table displays the partial correlations with sex and age controlled as well as the zero-order correlations among the variables. The partial correlations are presented on the top row, while the zero-order correlations are presented directly below. Many of the associations adhere to previous findings by Egan and Perry (2001) and Carver et al. (2003). For example, same-gender typicality was significantly positively correlated with gender contentedness, but not so highly correlated as to consider them the same measure. One of the primary aims of the intercorrelation table is to see the relationships between the old gender identity variables (same-gender typicality, gender contentedness, and felt pressure) and the new ones (other-gender typicality, gender oppression, and gender centrality). Correlations split by sex are presented in Table 3. The new gender identity variables are presented first. Each of these analyses controls for age and/or sex, as appropriate. Other-gender typicality was significantly negatively correlated with same-gender typicality ( pr = -.45, p < .00), gender contentedness (pr = -.41, p < .001), felt pressure (pr = -.25, p < .001) and gender centrality (pr = -.13, p < .049). Other-gender typicality was positively correlated with gender oppression (pr = .53, p < .001). 19 Gender oppression was negatively correlated with same-gender typicality (pr = .30, p < .001), gender contentedness (pr = -.28, p < .001) and felt pressure (pr = -.14, p < .04) and positively correlated with other-gender typicality (pr = .53, p < .001). Gender centrality was negatively correlated with other-gender typicality (pr = .13, p < .05) and positively correlated with gender contentedness (pr = .16, p < .03) and felt pressure (pr = .17, p < .01). Same-gender typicality was significantly moderately negatively correlated with other-gender typicality (pr = -.45, p < .001) and gender oppression (pr = -.30, p < .001). Same-gender typicality was positively correlated with gender contentedness (pr = .34, p < .001). Gender contentedness was negatively correlated with other-gender typicality (pr = -.41, p < .001) and gender oppression (pr = -.28, p < .001) and positively correlated with same-gender typicality (pr = .34, p < .001) and gender centrality (pr = .16, p <.03). Felt pressure was negatively correlated with other-gender typicality (pr = -.25, p < .001) and gender oppression (pr = -.14, p < .04) and positively correlated with gender centrality (pr = .17, p < .01). Relations Between Gender Identity and Adjustment Measures A main purpose of this study was to examine if the three new gender identity measures related to adjustment. Relations between the gender identity variables and the adjustment variables may be found in Table 2. Significance tests of interaction terms were conducted to see if any relations of gender identity and adjustment depend on sex or 20 age. This was done by testing each adjustment variable as a dependent variable and then entering age, sex, and a gender identity variable on step 1 and age X a gender identity variable and sex X a gender identity variable on step 2 of a multiple regression. No significant interactions with age were found. However, there were three sex differences which indicated a need to split the file by sex when looking at the correlations. The three interactions that indicated a need to split the file were between gender contentedness testing for depression, felt pressure testing for global self-worth, and felt pressure testing for depression. All analyses controlled for age and sex except for the three split by sex which only controlled for age. Results are discussed below. Other-gender typicality was found to be negatively correlated with global selfworth (pr = -.18, p < .01) and not positively correlated with any adjustment measure. Gender oppression was negatively correlated with global self-worth (pr = -.19, p < .001) and positively correlated with depression (pr = .16, p < .02). Gender centrality was not correlated with any adjustment measures. Same-gender typicality was negatively correlated with depression (pr = -.21, p < .001) and positively correlated with global selfworth (pr = .21, p < .001) and narcissism (pr = .16, p < .02). Gender contentedness was positively correlated with global self-worth (pr = .36, p < .001). As noted, there was a need to examine the correlation of gender contentedness and depression by sex. Girls yielded a significant negative correlation (pr = -.19, p < .03) between the variables. Boys did not yield a significant correlation between these variables. Felt pressure was negatively correlated with global self-worth (pr = -.27, p < .001) and prosocial behavior (pr = -.19, p < .001) and positively correlated with 21 depression (pr = .14, p < .04). As noted, there was a need to examine the correlations of felt pressure with global self-worth and depression separately by sex. For girls, felt pressure was negatively correlated with global self-worth (pr = -.35, p < .001) and positively correlated with depression (pr = .28, p < .001). For boys, felt pressure was negatively correlated with global self-worth (pr = -.21, p < .03) but not significantly correlated with depression. A final set of analyses tested each new variable (other-gender typicality, gender oppression, and gender centrality) to see if each contributed to adjustment directly beyond the three original (same-gender typicality, gender contentedness, and felt pressure) Egan and Perry (2001) measures. These analyses were conducted through multiple regression analyses with Step 1 entering age and sex, Step 2 entering the three old gender identity variables, and Step 3 entering one of the three new gender identity variables. These analyses were run separately for each adjustment variable. None of the new variables reached a significance level of p < .05. F change values were also calculated to see if, when all three new gender identity variables were added on Step 3, the three together predict something unique. These analyses showed that when the three new variables were added, the F change for the adjustment variable of depression is significant (p < .04). However, the value was not significant for any of the other five adjustment variables. These results demonstrate that not only do the old measures of gender identity correlate in a similar fashion as found by Egan and Perry (2001) and Carver, Younger, and Perry (2003), but the new gender identity measures have trends in expected ways and 22 contribute to adjustment and therefore might be incorporated into Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional conceptualization of gender identity. Gender Centrality as a Moderator A second purpose of this study was to see if gender centrality moderates relations of the other gender identity variables to adjustment. The moderation effects were tested through multiple regressions. A series of five-step multiple regressions was run that was set up as: Step 1 with age and sex; Step 2 with one gender identity and gender centrality; Step 3 with the interaction of gender identity X gender centrality; Step 4 with the interaction of gender identity X sex and gender centrality X sex; and Step 5 with the interaction of gender identity X gender centrality X sex. Additional analyses were run replacing sex with age to see age effects. In no analysis with sex was the three-way interaction entered on Step 5 significant. However, on Step 3, the two-way interaction between gender centrality and the gender identity variable was significant in several analyses. These interactions were explored with an Aiken and West regression analysis to look at the relation of gender identity to adjustment at different levels of gender centrality: low (-1 SD), medium (0 SD), and high (+1 SD). The first interaction was for gender contentedness predicting global self-worth. As gender centrality increased from low to high, gender contentedness becomes increasingly positively paired with global self-worth, showing that as gender becomes more central to a child, the stronger the positive relation of gender contentedness to selfesteem (βs = .27, p < .01; .42, p < .001; .57, p < .001). The second effect was other- 23 gender typicality interacting with gender centrality to predict narcissism. As gender centrality increased from low to high the relation of other-gender typicality to narcissism reversed (βs = .16, ns; .02, ns; -.13, ns). At low gender centrality, other-gender typicality was positively related to narcissism but at high gender centrality, other-gender typicality was negatively related to narcissism. The third moderating effect was the interaction between gender contentedness and gender centrality predicting narcissism. As gender centrality increased the relation of gender contentedness to narcissism reversed (βs = .13, ns; .02, ns; .17, ns). At low gender centrality, gender contentedness was negatively related to narcissism but at high gender centrality, gender contentedness was positively related to narcissism. Similar analyses were run replacing sex with age to see if there were differences between younger and older children in the interaction of gender identity and gender centrality when predicting adjustment measures. Younger children consisted of fourth and fifth grade children, while the older children consisted of sixth through eighth graders. For these analyses there were three significant two-way interactions and two significant three-way interactions. The three two-way interactions were essentially identical to the three two-way interactions described in the preceding paragraph. The first of the three-way interactions was that of age X gender contentedness X gender centrality predicting global self-worth. This interaction was broken down by examining the two-way interaction of gender centrality and gender contentedness for older and younger children separately. For younger children, as centrality increased, gender contentedness became increasingly associated with higher levels of self-esteem 24 (βs = .19, ns; .47, p < .001; .76, p < .001). The two-way interaction was not significant for older children. The second three-way interaction effect was that of age X felt pressure for conformity X gender centrality predicting externalizing behavior. There was no significant two-way interaction for younger children. For older children, as centrality increased the relation of felt pressure to externalizing behavior became increasingly evident (βs = -.07, ns; .22, p < .05; .51, p < .001). These results showcase the moderating effects of gender centrality. This conforms to the expectation that centrality moderates the relation of other gender identity variables to children’s adjustment. These results indicate that if a child sees gender as being central, other aspects of gender identity become more of a deciding factor in the child’s adjustment. 25 Discussion The results go beyond the original conceptualization created by Egan and Perry (2001) and demonstrate that the new gender identity variables of gender oppression, other-gender typicality, and gender centrality are also related to a child’s adjustment. There were two primary goals of the current study, first to see if the added new gender identity variables were predictors of adjustment, and second to see if gender centrality moderates the relationships between the other gender identity variables and adjustment. The new measures correlated with the old measures in expected ways, although some unexpected results were present that will be outlined below. The new gender identity variables will be discussed in turn. Additionally, gender centrality was shown to moderate some relationships between other gender identity variables and adjustment. This issue is also discussed below. Other-Gender Typicality The hypothesis that children who feel dissimilar to their own gender and more similar to the other gender may feel dismay and could display lower self-esteem was supported. By associating the self with the other gender, a child may come to fear peer rejection and other problems with in turn could lead to loneliness, anxiety, depression and poor self-esteem (Dijkstra, Lindenberg, & Veenstra, 2007). Other-gender typicality was found to be moderately negatively related to same-gender typicality but related to 26 adjustment in somewhat different ways. Although same-gender typicality may predict positive adjustment measures like high self-esteem and gender contentedness, othergender typicality can be used to predict negative adjustment such as depression. While it is of obvious importance to measure a child’s same-gender typicality, the data collected hint that measuring other-gender typicality might yield new insights into different aspects of a child’s adjustment. For these reasons it may be important to keep other-gender typicality as a gender identity variable and add it to Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional conceptualization. Regardless of retaining other-gender typicality in the battery of gender identity measures, the data collected are informative in that they reveal the correlation between same-gender typicality and other-gender typicality to be negative. This is important because it has sometimes been claimed that Egan and Perry’s (2001) measure of gender typicality is independent of gender, that is, that felt same-gender typicality simply captures how similar to all other children (same-sex and other-sex) a child feels (DiDonato et al., 2006). However, if that is the case, then one would expect same-gender typicality and other-gender typicality to be positively correlated, which they are not. The negative correlation discredits the notion that the measures being used are independent of gender and provides further validity for the measures as they are measuring what they are supposed to measure. An interesting finding is that felt pressure for gender conformity was negatively correlated with other-gender typicality. Previous studies have found that gender typicality and felt pressure are uncorrelated (Carver, et al., 2003). Of course, in previous studies 27 gender typicality tapped only perceived similarity to the same gender, while in the current study gender typicality was split between same- and other-gender typicality. It is unclear, however, how one should interpret the finding that as a child feels more typical to the other gender, he or she will feel low levels of felt pressure to conform. To explain this finding, it might be hypothesized that those who are high on other-gender typicality do not find gender identification as important and do not place as much emphasis on it as others. Therefore, due to this lack of emphasis, they do not feel the great pressure that others would. Of course, an alternative hypothesis is that feeling little pressure for gender conformity allows children to participate in cross-gender activities which in turn fosters a sense of cross-gender typicality. Longitudinal data will be required to test the alternative explanations. Gender Oppression Although gender oppression has seldom been included in gender identity models, it was hypothesized that children high in gender oppression would experience adjustment problems, especially reduced self-esteem. This hypothesis was confirmed. Although reduced self-esteem would seem bad for a developing child, and therefore gender oppression would appear bad on the surface, it could be beneficial for some children in the long-term. It is possible that the oppression that a child feels will cause him or her to become more radical in his or her thoughts and developing opinions on societal structures. These radical feelings could lead the child to grow up and fight for equal rights through such philosophies as feministic rights. For these reasons, it is possible that 28 while gender oppression may initially appear bad, it could be good in the long run by altering typically passive views. A second reason that gender oppression was added was to investigate Bigler’s (2006) suggestion that gender contentedness does not actually measure contentedness with one’s gender category but rather measures contentedness with gender oppression. Indeed, Egan and Perry’s (2001) original measure of gender contentedness was open to this criticism. The present study, however shows that gender contentedness is not simply the lack of feeling of gender oppression. The variables of gender contentedness and gender oppression were only minimally negatively correlated. It may be that a lack of feeling of resentment at the existence of cross-sex options that are off limits (i.e., low felt gender oppression) is simply one factor that contributes to satisfaction with one’s gender category. Gender Centrality as a Moderator It was found that gender centrality did not contribute directly to adjustment but did moderate relations between other gender identity variables and adjustment. As hypothesized, gender centrality magnified the contributions of other gender identity variables. If gender was central to a child then the gender identity variables became more salient and therefore more likely to influence the child’s adjustment. Due to its importance as a moderator between gender identity and adjustment, gender centrality should be included in Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional model of gender identity. 29 Although it was found that none of the new measures contributes directly beyond the old measures to adjustment, the interactions of these new variables with centrality were sometimes significant. For example, other-gender typicality interacted with gender centrality to predict narcissism in children. While it could be said that only the new variables that interact with centrality should be kept and incorporated into Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional model of gender identity, the possible long term effects of some of the new variables as outlined about could be a reason to keep them for future studies. Limitations and Future Directions This study has its limitation. One limitation is that most of the measures presented were self-reported. For future studies, parent and teacher reports would help to validate the children’s reports. Although direct observation might also be a good alternative, this would be very time consuming. Another limitation is in the design. This study is a concurrent correlational design. A longitudinal design is needed in order to decipher the likely direction of causality of the relationships between the gender identity variables and the adjustment measures. Future research could look at the motivating roles of racial or ethnic identities in the adoption of gender-identity related characteristics. One could collect data from different cultures such as collectivistic cultures like China versus individualistic cultures of America. The data could then be compared and analyzed to see if there are any differences. While the current participant pool had a variety of races and ethnicities, they 30 were all young children and can be considered influenced more by American values than elsewhere regardless of where they are from. By examining other cultures, research can investigate whether the results found are unique to America or more universal. Another point of future interest is to investigate the result that felt pressure is negatively correlated with other-gender typicality. While possible reasons for this relationship are outlined above, future study could focus specifically on this association of how centrality influences the amount of felt pressure a child feels when they feel more typical to the other gender. By doing so researchers could gain new insights into why this relationship may occur. Gender oppression should also be examined through a longitudinal study. Although gender oppression has been found to be bad for developing adolescents and can cause poor adjustment, it is suggested that the oppression may be beneficial for some over time. If it is the case that the oppression can cause some children to develop radical views and lead them to fight for equal rights, then this could be viewed as a positive adjustment outcome. However, only a longitudinal design would allow for such answers. A final recommendation for future studies it to examine the direction of influence with respect to gender centrality through a longitudinal design. Although this study found that gender centrality can act as a moderating variable between other gender identity variables and adjustment measures, it is still unclear as to whether other gender identity variables and the adjustment measures can be used to predict gender centrality. A longitudinal design will help decipher this directionality. 31 References Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80-114. Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex-typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354-364. Bigler, R. S. (2006, April). Gender Development Research Conference Debate. Caporael, L. R., & Brewer, M. B. (1991). The quest for human nature: Social and scientific issues in evolutionary psychology. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 1-9. Carver, P. R., Yunger, J. L., & Perry, D. G. (2003). Gender identity and adjustment in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 49, 95-109. DiDonato, M., Fesi, B., Backer, A., Ravago, C., Ndimbie, Y., Roberts, S., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Predictors and specificity of gender identity. Poster presented at the Conference on Gender Development, San Francisco, CA. Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. (2007). Same-gender and cross-gender peer acceptance and peer rejection and their relation to bullying and helping among preadolescents: Comparing predictions from gender-homophily and goal32 framing approaches. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1377-1389. Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: A multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 451463. Green, R. (1974). Sexual identity conflict in children and adults. New York: Basic Books. Harter, S. (1998). The development of self representations. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, pp. 553-617). New York: Wiley. Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In E. M. Hetherington (Eds.) Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development(Vol. 4, pp. 338467. New York: Wiley. Kagan, J. (1964). Acquisition and significance of sex typing and sex role identity. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman (Eds.). Review of child development research (Vol. 1, pp. 137-168. New York: Russell Sage. Kohlberg, I. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.). The development of sex differences (pp. 82173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-380). Skokie, IL: Rand McNally. Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development 33 (Vol. 3, pp. 933-1016). New York: Wiley. Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). The role of gender constancy in early gender development. Child Development (In Press). Slaby, R. G., & Frey, K. S. (1975). Development of gender constancy and selective attention to same sex roles. Smith, T. E., & Leaper, C. (2004). Self-perceived gender typicality and the peer context during adolescence. Poster presented at the Gender Development Research Conference, San Francisco. Spence, J. T. (1985). Gender identity and implications for concepts of masculinity and femininity. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.). Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Psychology and gender (Vol. 32, pp 59-96). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (1995). Masculinity and femininity: Defining the undefinable. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power and communication in human relationships (pp. 105-138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1980). Masculine instrumentality and feminine expressiveness: Their relationships with sex role attitudes and behaviors. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5, 147-163. Yunger, J. L., Carver, P. R., & Perry, D. G. (2004). Does gender identity influence children’s psychological well-being? Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 572-582. Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Sullivan, C. B. L., Kuksis, M., Birkenfeld-Adams, A., & 34 Mitchell, J. N. (1993). A gender identity interview for children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 442-456. 35 Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations of Measures Boys Girls Sex Differences Age Differences F F Younger M SD Older M SD Younger M SD Older M SD 1. Same-gender typicality 2.8 .70 2.9 .52 2.5 .71 3.0 .63 2.7 20.7*** 2. Other-gender typicality 1.5 .46 1.5 .35 2.5 .72 2.3 .51 170.4*** 6.3* 3. Gender contentedness 3.8 .37 3.8 .27 3.6 .52 3.8 .29 3.8+ 11.7** 4. Gender oppression 1.7 .51 1.8 .52 2.7 .74 2.5 .64 119.2*** 1.5 5. Felt pressure 2.7 .82 2.8 .76 1.9 .65 1.8 .58 104.5*** .010 6. Gender centrality 2.7 1.2 3.3 1.2 2.8 1.3 3.0 1.3 .259 3.7 7. Global self worth 3.3 .58 3.5 .48 3.3 .66 3.5 .57 .206 2.1 8. Narcissism .19 .14 .23 1.5 .24 .20 .28 .17 5.8* .42 9. Depression 1.2 .19 1.2 .20 1.4 .35 1.3 .21 7.2** 5.6* 10. Internalizing behavior .18 .18 .25 .25 .18 .16 .22 .20 .18 .50 11. Externalizing behavior .19 .19 .26 .20 .20 .17 .27 .20 .40 .99 12. Prosocial behavior .48 .20 .40 .23 .53 .20 .42 .18 1.5 13.3*** Measure Note: F values indicate the significance of the sex difference with age controlled and the age difference with sex controlled. + p<.06 * p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.000 36 Table 2: Correlations Among Gender Identity Variables and Adjustment Variables Measures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1. Same-gender typicality - -.45*** .34*** -.30*** .04 .08 .21** .16* -.21** -.11 .07 .01 -.41*** .38*** -.30*** .08 .10 .22** -.15* -.25*** -.09 .08 -.06 - -.41*** .53*** -.25*** -.13* -.18** .01 .01 -.01 -.07 .05 -.40*** .70*** -.52*** -.09 -.12+ .10 .13+ -.03 -.03 .10 - -.28*** .03 ..16* .36*** -.01 -.12 -.02 -.00 .11 -.30*** .09 .16* .37*** -.01 -.17* -.01 .01 .05 - -.14* -.11 -.19** .05 .16* .12 -.01 .01 -.42*** -.07 -.14* .13* .23*** .08 .02 .06 - .17* -.27*** .11 .14* -.02 .05 -.19** .12 -.25*** -.00 .02 -.00 .01 -.19** - .02 .09 .05 -.07 .03 .03 .03 .10 .04 -.07 .04 .01 2. Other-gender typicality 3. Gender contentedness 4. Gender oppression 5. Felt pressure 6. Gender centrality 37 Table 2: Correlations Among Gender Identity Variables and Adjustment Variables (Continued) Measures 1 2 3 4 5 7. Global self-worth 8. Narcissism 9. Depression 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 - -.13* -.50*** -.05 -.10 .15* -.12 -.50*** -.05 -.09 .13+ - .03 -.11 .37*** -.31*** .04 -.11 .37*** -.30*** - .34*** .06 -.11 .32*** /06 -.06 - -.21** -.07 -.20** -.08 - -.52*** 10. Internalizing behavior 11. Externalizing behavior -.51*** 12. Prosocial behavior - Note: Entries on the top row are partial correlations with age and sex controlled and entries on the bottom row are zero-order correlations. + p<.06 * p<.05 ** p<.01 *** p<.000 38 Table 3: Correlations Among Measures by Sex Measures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1. Same-gender typicality - -.31** .26** -.28** .11 .02 .23* .08 -.17 -.06 -.02 -.08 2. Other-gender typicality -.52 - -.53*** .33*** -.30** -.20* -.30** .03 -.01 -.02 -.18 .09 3. Gender contentedness .37*** -.36*** - -.33** .21* .26** .27** -.07 .05 .08 .05 .03 4. Gender oppression -.29** .60*** -.24** - -.09 -.12 .33*** .09 .20* .00 -.02 .03 5. Felt pressure -.02 -.26** -.11 -.20* - .34*** -.21* .26** -.05 -.05 -.02 -.10 6. Gender centrality .10 -.08 .06 -.10 .00 - .04 .16 .11 .01 -.02 .03 7. Global selfworth .20* -.13 .43*** -.12 -.35*** .00 - -.15 -.31** -.02 -.06 .16 39 Table 3: Correlations Among Measures by Sex (Continued) Measures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 8. Narcissism .22* -.01 .03 .02 -.03 .05 -.12 - -.14 -.15 .33*** -.26** 9. Depression -.21* -.01 -.19* .12 .28** .03 -.61*** .09 - .47*** -.08 .02 10. Internalizing behavior -.13 -.02 -.09 .19* .01 -.14 -.08 -.09 .26** - -.25** .05 11. Externalizing behavior .19* -.03 -.02 -.03 .11 .09 -.13 .39*** .12 -.18* - -.54*** 12. Prosocial behavior .08 .05 .17 .02 -.29** .03 .15 -.35*** -.19* -.19* -.50*** - Note: Entries are partial correlations by sex with age controlled. Boys are above the horizontal and females below the horizontal. + p<.06 * p<.05 ** p<.01 *** p<.000 40