(Full Article)
Transcription
(Full Article)
Arch.Geflügelk., 77 (1). S. 10–14, 2013, ISSN 0003-9098. © Verlag Eugen Ulmer, Stuttgart The effect of selection for high and low feather pecking on aggression – related behaviours of laying hens Der Einfluß der Selektion auf hohes und niedriges Federpicken auf das aggressive Verhalten von Legehennen W. Bessei, Hanna Bauhaus and Stefanie Bögelein Manuscript received 27 February 2012, accepted 12 May 2012 Introduction Feather pecking and aggressive pecking in chickens are considered to have different underlying motivations. Threatening, aggressive pecking, and fighting behavior is performed in the context of establishing and maintaining the social hierarchy in poultry flocks (GUHL, 1968). Aggressive pecks are delivered in an upright body posture and mainly directed towards the head of the birds, where they cause feather damage (BILCIK and KEELING, 1999). In some cases, when a submissive bird flees from the attack of a dominant bird, aggressive pecks are delivered to the back. Most feather damages of the body, however, are due to vigorous feather pecking. Therefore it is possible to differentiate between aggressive pecking and vigorous feather pecking. The latter is often followed by pulling and eating the feathers (MCKEEGAN and SAVORY, 1999). There is no clear tendency in the interrelationship between aggression or the position in the social hierarchy in a flock and feather pecking. WENNRICH (1975) suggested an interrelationship between feather pecking and aggressive pecking in so far as a high social rank may facilitate both, aggressive and feather pecking. Laying hens in 4-bird-cages, which showed poor feather conditions as a result of feather pecking, had lower social rank scores than those with better plumage conditions. The results, however, were not consistent in all groups (HUGHES and DUNCAN, 1972). Males in a flock have usually a higher social rank than females. But they are preferred targets for feather pecking of hens (BRAASTAD and LANGSTRAND, 1994). Indeed, the males high in rank received more feather pecks by hens than low ranking males (KATHLE et al., 1997). KJAER et al. (2001) found no relationship between feather pecking and aggressive pecking in lines selected for high and low feather pecking. The present study was carried out to compare aggressive interactions within and between White Leghorn lines selected for high and low feather pecking in a social environment. Materials and methods A total of 224 laying hens of a White Leghorn line, which have been divergently selected for high (HFP) and low (LFP) feather pecking behavior for 9 generations were Dept. of Farm Animal Behavior and Poultry Science, University Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany used in the present experiment. The selection criterion was the number of bouts of vigorous feather pecking in adult layers kept in groups. The selection was started in the Danish Institute of Animal Sciences, Foulum, Denmark, for the first 5 generations (KJAER et al., 2001) and then for 4 more generations at the Institute of Animal Husbandry and Breeding, University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart-Hohenheim, Germany. The pullets of the lines were raised in separate deep litter pens under conventional lighting and feeding programmes. At 26 weeks of age 114 HFP and 110 LFP birds were randomly assigned to five pens of 16 m2 each with half the birds of each pen of the HFP and LFP line. The pens were separated by a wire mesh grid and the birds could see and hear the birds of the other pens. All pens were equipped with round feeders, nipple drinkers, nests, perches and a litter area (1/3 of the total space). The litter area was covered with wood-shavings (10 cm depth). The litter was dry and friable throughout the experiment. Two third of the pen area were covered by a perforated plastic floor. The slats were raised at a height of 38 cm from the litter area. Artificial light was provided through incandescent bulbs from 3 am to 5 pm (14 hrs). There was additional natural light through transparent plastic material at the upper part of the side wall of the pen. Light intensity provided by the electric bulbs was 20 lux, and, depending on the irregular fluctuation of the natural light, increased occasionally up to 2500 lux on sunny days. Fresh air was supplied through a thermostatically controlled ventilation system. Room temperature was kept between 18 and 22°C. A conventional layer mash containing 16.6% crude protein and 11.4 ME/kg was provided ad libitum. The birds were weighed and individually identified by numbered plastic tags on their back before the start of the observations (25 weeks of age). The tags of soft plastic material (14 × 5.5 cm, length × width) were fixed with plastic bands at the humerus of both wings one week before the start of the observations. The birds adapted to the tags within a few hours and showed undisturbed locomotor behaviour including flying to the perches and nests. Gentle pecking of the tags by the pen mates was observed at a low frequency during the first days and disappeared thereafter. Each pen was visually observed in 15 sessions of 20 min each for aggressive behaviour and in 10 sessions of 20 min each for feather pecking behaviour. The observations were carried out from 26 to 35 weeks of age from 6:30 to 15:00 hrs. They were balanced for observation day and time of day. Criteria for aggression were the number of threats and aggressive pecks delivered and received. Threats were recorded when a hen visually fixed a pen mate in an upright body posture. Threats were followed either by avoidance or escape behaviour of the subArch.Geflügelk. 1/2013 Bessei et al.: Interrelationships between aggressive pecking and feather pecking in hens ordinate hen, by aggressive pecking or by fighting. Aggressive pecks are defined as pecks towards the head and the body of conspecifics in an aggressive posture of the aggressor or during a fight among hens. The winner and loser of the fights were recorded. With regard to feather pecking, only vigorous pecks or pulls delivered and received were recorded. This behavior is carried out in a non-aggressive posture of the feather pecking hen. The hen to be pecked either tolerates pecking or tries to escape. The differences of aggressive pecks delivered and received and feather pecks delivered and received were calculated and referred here as feather pecking index and aggression index. The latter has been described by LEE and CRAIG (1982) as a criterion of social rank in laying hens. Statistical analysis The JMP SAS, Version 7 programme was used for all statistical procedures (SAS, 2007: JMP 7 Statistics and Graphics Guide, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). The mean number per 20 minutes session of threats delivered and received, aggressive pecks delivered and received, bouts of vigorous feather pecks delivered and received of each bird were calculated over the 15 observation sessions for feather pecking and 10 observation sessions for aggressive pecking. Data were subjected to a two-factorial analysis of variance with pen and line as factors. The residuals were tested for normal distribution. All criteria with the exception of body weight, feather pecking index and aggression index significantly deviated from the Gaussian distribution. As the effect of pen was not significant for any of the criteria the line effects were analysed by a one-factorial design using non-parametric procedure (Wilcoxon-Test) for not normally distributed criteria and ANOVA and student’s t-test for criteria which followed a normal distribution. The Bartlett-Test was used to test the divergence of variances of the index of feather pecking and Aggression Index. The correlations between the criteria were calculated within lines by Spearmans rank correlation coefficient. 11 ered and received more aggressive pecks than LFP birds. There was no significant difference between the lines in the mean aggression index, while the feather pecking index was significantly higher in the HFP as compared to the LFP line. There was greater variation for both the aggression and feather pecking indices between individual birds in HFP than LFP (Figure 1a, b). HFP were significantly heavier than LFP, though the difference was relatively small. The correlations of the different behavioural criteria and body weight are shown in Table 2. There were significantly positive correlations between threats delivered, aggressive pecks and aggression index within both lines. The correlations between the aggressive pecks received and threats received were also significant within both lines (0.65 and 0.25). The correlations between aggressive pecks delivered and received, and threats delivered and received were close to zero in both lines. The aggression index was positively correlated with threats and aggressive pecks delivered and negatively correlated with threats and aggressive pecks received. The correlation coefficients were significant in all cases. The correlations of feather pecks delivered and received were low and not significant. Feather pecks delivered were positively and feather pecks received negatively correlated with the feather pecking index. The correlations were significant in both lines. The correlations between feather pecks delivered and received and the aggression index were not significant in the HFP and in the LFP. Aggressive pecks received and feather pecking index were negatively correlated, the effect was significant in the LFP only (–0.23 for LFP and –0.09 for HFP). The correlation of feather pecking index and aggression index was significant in the HFP only (0.19). Body weight was significantly positively correlated with aggressive pecks delivered, aggression index and feather pecks received within the HFP. Negative correlations appeared between body weight and feather pecks delivered, aggressive pecks received and feather pecking index within the HFP. All correlations between body weight and the behavioural traits were low and not significant in the LFP. Discussion Results The lines differed significantly in the number of threats and aggressive pecks delivered and received (Table 1). HFP deliv- The selection for high and low number of bouts of feather pecking has led to highly divergent lines in this criterion. The HFP birds in the present study delivered 7 times more feather pecks than the LFP birds (Table 1). But the number Table 1. Mean values *) of lines selected for high (HFP) and low (LFP) feather pecking for threats delivered and received, aggressive pecking delivered and received and feather pecks delivered and received; feather pecking index, aggression index and body weight (BW); Mittelwerte für ausgeteilte und erhaltene Drohungen (threats), Federpickschläge Fp)und aggressive Pickschläge (Aggr.) sowie für den Federpick-Index und Aggressionsindex (Differenz zwischen ausgeteilten und erhaltenen Pickschlägen) und Körpergewicht der auf hohes (HFP) und niedriges (LFP) Federpicken selektierten Linien Line N Threats delivered Threats received Aggressive pecks delivered Aggressive pecks received Aggression index**) Feather pecks delivered Feather pecks received Feather pecking index**) BW (kg) HFP LFP 114 110 224 0.99 0.50 p < 0.002 0.98 0.49 p < 0.004 3.46 1.19 p < 0.001 3.15 1.35 p ≤ 0.001 0.49 0.43 n.s. 7.57 1.05 p < 0.001 3.96 4.17 n.s. 3.61 –3.12 p ≤ 0.001 1.52 1.45 p ≤ 0.001 *) Mean number of events per 20 minutes based on 15 observation sessions of 20 minutes each Mittlere Anzahl von Ereignissen über 15 Beobachtungen zu 20 Minuten **) Number of aggressive pecks (resp. feather pecking bouts) delivered minus number of aggressive pecks (resp. feather pecking bouts) received Mittlere Anzahl ausgeteilter aggressiver Pickschläge (bzw. Federpickserien) abzüglich Anzahl erhaltener aggressiver Pickschläge. Arch.Geflügelk. 1/2013 12 Bessei et al.: Interrelationships between aggressive pecking and feather pecking in hens a 30 20 10 0 -10 -20 -30 b HFP LFP HFP LFP 110 90 70 50 30 10 -10 -30 Figure 1. a,b. Variation in the Aggression-Index (a) and Feather Pecking Index (b) between the high (HFP) and low (LFP) feather pecking lines (points show for each bird the difference between pecks delivered and pecks received) Variation des Aggressionsindex (a) und des Federpick-Index (b) der auf hohes (HFP) und geringes(LFP) Federpicken selektierten Linien (die Punkte bezeichnen die Differenz zwischen ausgeteilten und erhaltenen Federpickschlägen bzw. aggressiven Pickschlägen) of feather pecks received was nearly identical in the HFP and LFP lines and the correlation between feather pecks delivered and received was low and not significant within both lines (0.01 and –0.07). The interrelationships between feather pecks delivered and feather pecks received are not consistent in the studies reported in the literature. CUTHBERTSON (1978) assumed that high feather pecking hens be less frequently the target of being pecked. KJAER et al. (2001) observed the lines used in the present study in an earlier generation. As in the present study there was no difference between the lines in the number of pecking bouts received. The genetic correlation between feather pecking and being pecked within lines was not significant (KJAER and SØRENSEN, 1997). Although a genetic basis for feather pecks received has been reported in different studies (CUTHBERTSON, 1978; BESSEI, 1985) feather pecking and being feather pecked are obviously determined by different genetic mechanisms. Selection for high feather pecking has obviously not changed the risk of being feather pecked in the lines used in the present study. There was a high consistency among the aggression related criteria within both lines with the HFP showing higher means for threats and aggressive pecks delivered and received (Table 1). These results are in contrast to observations of KJAER et al. (2001) who found no difference in aggression between the same lines in earlier generations, and to other lines differing in feather pecking (BLOKHUIS and ARKES, 1984). The divergence in aggression between the HFP and LFP may have developed during the recent selection generations. It is also possible that the extended time spent in the observation of aggressive behaviour, and the concentration of the observer on aggression related criteria has uncovered differences in aggression which have not been recognized in the earlier studies. High rank in the social hierarchy may facilitate the approach to target birds for feather pecking. This could explain the higher aggression in the HFP lines of this experiment. MCKEEGAN and SAVORY (1999) reported a low non-significant correlation between non-aggressive feather pecks/pulls and aggressive pecks in layer flocks. The correlation between aggressive pecking, feather damages and feather eating events was significant. This correlation Table 2. Spearman‛s rank correlation coefficients between aggression and feather pecking related criteria and body weight of a high (HFP; above the diagonal) and low (LFP, below the diagonal) feather pecking line. Significant coefficients (p < 0.05) are marked with underlined figures. Spearman`s Rangkorrelationskoeffizienten zwischen den Merkmalen die mit Federpicken (ausgeteilte und erhaltene Federpickschläge, Federpick-Index) und Aggression (ausgeteilte und erhaltenen Drohungen, aggressiven Pickschlägen und Aggressionsindex) verbunden sind sowie dem Körpergewicht innerhalb der auf hohes und geringes Federpicken selektierten Linien. Signifikante Korreletionskoeffizienten (p < 0,05) in unterstrichenen Zahlen) Threats del Threats Rec. Aggr. Del Aggr. Rec. Aggr. Ind Fp del Fp rec Fp Ind BW Threats delivered Threats received Aggressive pecks delivered Aggressive pecks received Aggr. Index Feather pecks delivered Feather pecks received Feather Pecking Index Body weight – –0.12 0.00 0.48 –0.03 0.39 0.09 –0.06 0.09 0.10 – 0.60 –0.07 –0.09 0.25 –0.21 0.04 –0.10 0.10 –0.06 – –0.13 0.65 –0.15 –0.05 0.81 0.06 –0.04 0.06 0.09 – 0.49 –0.45 0.78 –0.74 –0.62 –0.14 0.19 –0.23 –0.15 – 0.01 –0.08 –0.05 –0.12 0.04 0.13 –0.15 0.19 0.15 – 0.03 –0.03 0.20 –0.07 0.18 0.01 –0.07 0.57 0.04 – 0.00 –0.07 0.11 –0.09 –0.02 0.95 –0.31 –0.86 0.09 – 0.12 –0.03 0.28 –0.18 0.30 –0.20 0.20 –0.25 –0.06 – Arch.Geflügelk. 1/2013 Bessei et al.: Interrelationships between aggressive pecking and feather pecking in hens was mainly based on the behaviour of two out of 12 groups, which showed the highest level of feather pecks and the highest level of aggression. The HFP of the present study may represent the conditions of these extreme groups of the MCKEEGAN and SAVORY (1999) experiment. The higher body weight of the HFP (Table 1) may also have assisted the birds to establish a higher social rank within the mixed groups. If high social rank and aggression facilitate feather pecking, we would expect that the HFP deliver more feather pecks than the LFP and that the LFP receive more feather pecks than the HFP. We would also expect a positive correlation between feather pecking and aggressive actions within the lines, and in the HFP in particular. However, the correlations between aggressive pecks delivered and feather pecks delivered were close to zero. In addition, there was a significantly positive correlation between aggressive pecks delivered and feather pecks received in the HFP line (0.20) which shows that the aggressive HFP received more feather pecks than expected under the above mentioned assumption. The present results show that the social behaviour in the groups of laying hens is not a univariate system of aggression and submission. The LFP, though less aggressive than the HFP, obviously are able to avoid aggressive encounters. This is clearly demonstrated in the variation of the aggression index (Figure 1). While the means of both lines are similar, the aggression index of the HFP birds showed a wider range than of the LFP and the LFP are neither presented at the higher nor at the lower end of the distribution. The fact that the LFP experience a significantly lower number of threats shows that the birds are able to anticipate and avoid social encounters. Aggression avoidance behaviour of the LFP may have developed in response to the large group keeping system. BESSEI et al. (1984) observed the behaviour of Leghorn hens after five generations of selection for high egg production under cage and floor pen systems. When tested in both environments, the hens were less aggressive in the system where they had been selected. The results have been explained by the development of aggression avoidance strategies. ENGELMANN (1968) suggested layers reduce aggression through avoiding visual fixation of pen mates. The behavioural strategies of avoiding aggressive interactions of the LFP it not known and requires further studies. The avoidance behaviour of the LFP was obviously not effective with regard to prevent receiving feather pecks. LFP received as many feather pecks as HFP. This can be explained by the non-aggressive posture that feather pecking hens when they approach target birds. The feather pecked hens not only tolerate the approach of the feather pecker, but also vigorous and damaging feather pulling. Conclusions Selection for high feather pecking has obviously led to increased aggression in the lines used in the present study. Selection for high feather pecking did not reduce the risk of being feather pecked. The LFP, though less aggressive than the HFP, obviously are able to avoid aggressive encounters. The avoidance behaviour of the LFP was not effective with regard to feather pecking behaviour. Acknowledgement The authors acknowledge the financial support of the studies on feather pecking of the German Research foundation (DFG) and Dr. J.B. Kjaer for supply of the lines selected for high and low feather pecking. Arch.Geflügelk. 1/2013 13 Summary Feather pecking and aggressive pecking of laying hens are generally considered to underlie different motivations. Divergent selection of a White Leghorn line for high (HFP) and low (LFP) feather pecking in a social group has lead to increased aggression in the HFP line. The aggressive behaviour (threats, aggressive pecking delivered and received) and vigorous feather pecks delivered and received have been observed in a total of 114 HFP and 110 LFP hens kept in 5 intermingled groups with half of the birds of each line. The difference between feather pecks and aggressive pecks delivered and received, referred here as feather pecking index and aggressive pecking index, was calculated. The HFP birds delivered significantly more feather pecks than LFP birds. There was no significant difference between the lines for the feather pecks received. The HFP lines showed higher frequencies in threats and aggressive pecks delivered and received than LFP birds. The feather pecking index was higher in the HFP than in the LFP. There was no difference between the lines in the mean aggression index. High positive correlations within lines were obtained for the threats, aggressive pecks delivered and fights won. The correlations between aggressive pecks delivered and received and feather pecks delivered and received were small and not significant. The means of aggression index were close to zero for both, HFP and LFP. But the variation was significantly higher in the HFP than in the LFP. This indicated that the overwhelming share of aggressive encounters happened among the HFP. The LFP not only showed a lower level of active aggression, but also successfully avoided aggressive encounters with the more aggressive HFP. The difference of the feather pecking index of the HFP and LFP and the similar frequency of feather pecks received in both lines indicated that the LFP were less successful in avoiding feather pecks. The results confirm the hypothesis that feather pecking and aggressive pecking are controlled by different motivations. The selection for high feather pecking not only increased the frequency in the selection trait but also in active aggressive behaviours. Key words Laying hens, feather pecking, aggression, genetics Zusammenfassung Die Beziehungen zwischen aggressivem Picken und Federpicken bei Legehennenlinien, die auf hohes und geringes Federpicken selektiert wurden Es wird allgemein angenommen, dass Federpicken und aggressives Picken bei Legehennen unterschiedlichen Motivationen folgen. In der vorliegenden Arbeit soll geprüft werden, ob die Selektion auf hohes Federpicken (HFP) und niedriges Federpicken (LFP) die Aggressionen beeinflusst. Das aggressive Verhalten (erhaltene und ausgeteilte aggressive Pickschläge und Drohungen) und heftige Federpickschläge (erhalten und ausgeteilt) wurden bei insgesamt 114 HFP und 110 LFP Hennen beobachtet. Die Tiere wurden in fünf gemischten Gruppen mit je zur Hälfte HFP und LFP gehalten. Die Differenz zwischen erhaltenen und ausgeteilten Federpickschlägen sowie aggressiven Pickschlägen wurde als Federpick-Index bzw. als Aggressionsindex bezeichnet. Die HFP teilten signifikant mehr Federpickschläge aus als die LFP. Es gab jedoch keine signifikante Differenz zwischen den Linien für erhaltene Federpick- 14 Bessei et al.: Interrelationships between aggressive pecking and feather pecking in hens schläge. Die HFP zeigten signifikant mehr Drohverhalten und aggressives Picken als die LFP. Gleichzeitig verzeichneten die HFP mehr empfangene Drohungen und aggressive Pickschläge als die LFP. Der Federpickindex war bei den HFP höher als bei den LFP. Die Mittelwerte des Aggressionsindexes lagen in beiden Linien nahe bei Null und unterschieden sich nicht signifikant. Aber die Variation des Aggressionsindexes war bei den HFP signifikant größer als bei den LFP. Innerhalb der Linien wurden hohe positive Korrelationen zwischen den Merkmalen Drohen und aggressives Picken vorgefunden. Die Korrelationen zwischen ausgeteilten und erhaltenen aggressiven Pickschläge sowie ausgeteilten und erhaltenen Federpickschlägen waren gering und nicht signifikant. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass der überwiegende Anteil der aggressiven Handlungen sich zwischen den HFP Tieren abspielte. Die LFP zeigten nicht nur einen geringeren Aggressionsspiegel, sondern konnten offensichtlich erfolgreich aggressive Handlungen von Seiten der HFP vermeiden. Die LFP waren jedoch weniger erfolgreich im Vermeiden von Bepickt-werden. Die Ergebnisse bestätigen die Hypothese, wonach Federpicken und aggressives Picken durch unterschiedliche Motivationen kontrolliert werden. Die Selektion auf hohes Federpicken erhöhte nicht nur die Häufigkeit im Selektionsmerkmal, sondern auch die Häufigkeit aggressiver Handlungen. Stichworte Legehennen, Federpicken, Aggression, Genetik References BESSEI, W., G. KLINGER, B. PEITZ, 1984: Das Verhalten von Legehennen unter dem Einfluß der Leistungsselektion in Boden- und Käfighaltung. Arch. Geflügelk. 48, 29-35. BESSEI, W., 1985: Pecking and feather loss – genetical aspects. Proc. 2nd Europ. Symp. Poultry Welfare, Celle, Germany, 10–13 June, 212-218. BILCIK, B., L. KEELING, 1999: Changes in feather condition in relation to feather pecking and aggressive pecking in laying hens. Brit. Poult. Sci. 40, 444-451. BLOKHUIS, H.J., J.G. ARKES, 1984: Some observations on the development of feather pecking in poultry. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 12, 145-157. BRAASTAD, B.O., H. LANGSTRAND, 1994: Experience with cocks of two breeds and two densities in aviaries with laying hens. 7th Nordic ISAE Winter Meeting, Nordseter, 5th– 6th December 1994. CUTHBERTSON, G.J., 1978: An ethological investigation of feather pecking. Ph. D. Thesis, Univ. Edinburgh. ENGELMANN, C., 1968: Der Einfluß der Intensivhaltung auf das Verhalten des Geflügels. Arch. Geflügelk. 17, 251-265. GUHL, A.M., 1968: Social inertia and social stability in chickens. Anim. Behav. 16, 219-232. HUGHES, B.O., I.J.H. DUNCAN, 1972: The influence of strain and environmental factors upon feather pecking and cannibalism in fowls. Brit. Poult. Sci. 13, 525-547. KATHLE, J., B. BRAASTAD, H. LANGSTRAND, 1997: Non-beaked laying hens housed in aviaries II: Behaviour of cockerels and their effects on hen performance. Norw. J. Agric. Sci. 10, 425-436. KJAER, J.B., P. SØRENSEN, 1997: Feather pecking in White Leghorn chickens – a genetic study. Brit. Poult. Sci. 38, 333-341. KJAER, J.B., P. SØRENSEN, G. SU, 2001: Divergent selection on feather pecking behaviour in laying hens (Gallus gallus domesticus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 71, 229-239. DOI: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00184-2. LEE, Y., J.V. CRAIG, 1982: The social rank index as a measure of social status and its association with egg production in White Leghorn pullets. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 8, 377-390. MCKEEGAN, D.E.F., C.J. SAVORY, 1999: Feather eating in layer pullets and its possible role in the aetiology of feather pecking damage. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 73-85. SAS JMP, 2007: JMP 7 Statistics and Graphics Guide. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA:660-680. WENNRICH, G., 1975: Beziehungen zwischen Ranghöhe, Federpickaktivität und Aggressivität von Haushennen (Gallus domesticus). Arch. Geflügelk. 39, 167-171. Correspondence: Prof. Dr. Dr. h.c. Werner Bessei: Dept. of Farm Animal Behavior and Poultry Science (470c), University Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany; e-mail: [email protected] Arch.Geflügelk. 1/2013