Flexible workers: how about their distractions and concentration?
Transcription
Flexible workers: how about their distractions and concentration?
Master Thesis Human Resource Studies Flexible workers: how about their distractions and concentration? A quantitative study in a general New Way of Working setting I.M. Molendijk, BSc. Anr: 885367 Supervisor: Prof. Dr. M.J.P.M. van Veldhoven Second assessor: Dr. T.A.M. Kooij Project Period: January 2011 – September 2011 Project theme: Get the balance right Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table of Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................................................... 4 1. Introduction .............................................................................................................................................................. 5 2. Theoretical Framework ......................................................................................................................................... 7 3. 2.1. Concentration on work tasks .......................................................................................................................... 7 2.2. Distractions........................................................................................................................................................ 8 2.3. Personal characteristic of flexibility ............................................................................................................... 9 Method ..................................................................................................................................................................... 12 3.1. Research context ............................................................................................................................................. 12 3.2. Procedures ....................................................................................................................................................... 12 3.3. Description of the sample ............................................................................................................................. 13 3.4. Instruments...................................................................................................................................................... 14 3.4.1. Diary ................................................................................................................................................................. 14 3.4.2. General Questionnaire ................................................................................................................................... 17 4. 5. 3.5. Control Variables ............................................................................................................................................ 18 3.6. Quality of the data .......................................................................................................................................... 19 3.7. Statistical analysis ............................................................................................................................................ 20 Results ...................................................................................................................................................................... 21 4.1. Variable differences between Home Days and Office Days ................................................................... 21 4.2. Correlations ..................................................................................................................................................... 23 4.3. Sequential multiple regression analyses and control variables................................................................. 29 4.4. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 1 ............................................................................ 29 4.5. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 2 ............................................................................ 31 4.6. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 1 ............................................................................ 33 4.7. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 2 ............................................................................ 34 4.8. Additional analysis on the interaction effects for both Home Days and both Office Days .............. 36 Conclusion and Discussion ............................................................................................................................... 39 5.1. Conclusion ....................................................................................................................................................... 39 5.2. Discussion of the findings............................................................................................................................. 41 5.3. Limitations and implications for further research ..................................................................................... 43 5.4. Practical implications ..................................................................................................................................... 45 2 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk References ........................................................................................................................................................................ 47 Appendix I. General questionnaire and daily diary.................................................................................................... 51 Appendix II. Factor analysis.......................................................................................................................................... 58 Appendix III. Exclusion of control variable Age ...................................................................................................... 72 Appendix IV. Results of two-way ANOVA tests for each day separately............................................................. 74 Appendix V. (Non-) significant results of the independent-samples t-test for equality of means ..................... 78 3 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Abstract The New Way of Working: a notable new-fashioned arrangement in which work is increasingly disconnected from traditional office space and buildings and employees are allowed to perform their work in more convenient or suitable places. The aim of this research was to investigate which kind of working days (home- or office days) seem to be more effective in relation to concentration on work tasks, work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions and in which way these variables interdepently cohere. A booklet which contained a general questionnaire and six daily questionnaires was answered by 120 respondents working in a general New Way of Working setting. Analyses were conducted at two office days and two home-days and indicated that non-work-related distractions have a higher negative impact on concentration on work tasks than work-related distractions do. Moreover, the personal characteristic of flexibility moderated for home day 1 the relationship between work- and non-work-related distractions on concentration on work tasks. This indicated that respondents who scored high on the personal characteristic flexibility, made the direct negative relationship between distractions and concentration on work tasks less negative. Future research should examine the above stated relationships with a bigger and more diverse sample and should consider multilevel research, because in such a way statements can be made about within-person effects. Key words: The New Way of Working, concentration on work tasks, work-related distractions, nonwork-related distractions, personal flexibility. 4 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 1. Introduction Psychological literature shows that enhanced concentration on work tasks leads to higher levels of performance (Demerouti, Taris & Bakker, 2007; Lieberman et al., 2006; Van der Linden, Keijsers, Eling & Van Schaijk, 2005). Nevertheless, recent research by Jurofoon (2011) showed that nearly 70% of the Dutch employees experience concentration problems due to loud-talking colleagues, noise due to devices and outside noise (http://www.fmm.nl/nieuws/nieuws/hard-pratende-collegas-zorgen-voor.73828.lynkx). As Jett & George (2003) explain in their research about interruptions in organizational life, the physical and psychological work environment can foster interruptions. Nowadays informal work climates and open office layouts are a trend and are designed to promote flexibility, space, and bring people closer together. However this also increases the likelihood of unscheduled interactions with others, which consumes time that could be spent on critical tasks and leaves a person with insufficient time to meet a deadline, achieve a goal or simply complete a task (Breuer, 1995). Breuer (1995) therefore notices that distractions in the workplace are problematic, as they may affect productivity in the end. At the moment, new information and communication technology at the office and at home has made organizations less dependent on time and space demands. Work is increasingly disconnected from traditional office space and buildings and employees are allowed to perform their work in more convenient or suitable places (Baane, Houtkamp & Knotter, 2010; Vittersø, Akselsen, Evjemo, Julsrud, Yttri & Bergvik, 2003). This New Way of Working is a vision to make work more effective, efficient, but also more pleasant for both the organization as well as the employee (Bijl, 2009). This vision can be realized by giving the space and freedom needed for an employee to let him determine how, where and when he works, and with what attributes and with whom (Bijl, 2009). Research state that the home environment fosters better concentration than the office (Montreuil & Lippel, 2002; Van Sell & Jacobs, 1994). For work-related distractions this would seem to be logical, because fewer work interruptions are likely to take place. However, when considering non-work related distractions, this phenomenon on concentration on work tasks can be considered as being doubtful. Sullivan & Lewis (2001) showed that when people had the opportunity to work from home they often had the tendency to combine work and non-work tasks. Whether this is beneficial to obtain high concentration levels can be questionable. Another aspect towards home telework that might be considered is whether concentration is more likely to happen if the number 5 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk of hours an employee is working at home are increasing. In literature, telework is frequently measured as a dichotomous variable, whereby the number of the exact hours or even days an employee is working is becoming guessable. Pitiful, because in this way it can be that the effects of telework are wrongly interpreted. For instance, with the New Working Practices, employees regularly work a few hours in the morning to avoid traffic jams (Bijl, 2009). During these hours they often read and respond their emails, before departing to the office (Baane, Houtkamp & Knotter, 2010; Bijl, 2009). This is an activity which mostly does not require a high level of concentration, which is in contrast to the previously mentioned main reason that working at home is an arrangement whereby enhanced concentration is obvious to be reached. Moreover, today’s dynamic organizations are in need of a workforce that is able to adapt to change(s) (Griffin & Hesketh, 2005; Pulakos, Arad, Donovan & Plamondon, 2000). For individuals it is often hard to stay self-disciplined and continue a task without yielding to changes such as distractions. Therefore this research will also take the personal characteristic of flexibility into account to see if this will buffer the relationship between work-related and non-work-related distractions and concentration. It is hypothesized that personal flexibility will benefit an individual to react properly to these types of distractions, which in turn makes it possible for individuals to achieve an optimal level of concentration to accomplish the work task at hand. As can be seen from the preceding introduction, flexible work arrangements, such as The New Way of Working, are becoming increasingly interesting for both employers as employees in the present quick information gaining Twenty-Four-Hour Society of the 21st century. Therefore, the aim of this research is to contribute to the current literature in this field by examining the relationship between working days at home and working days at the office and concentration, with the extra dimension of distractions in this relationship and the moderating variable flexibility, which can probably buffer the relationship between work- and non-work-related distractions on concentration. From a practical point of view, the aim of this research is to give organizations insight in the efficiency of which kind of days (home- or office days) seem to be more effective in relation to concentration. Also the uniqueness of non-work-related distractions and work-related distractions in combination with concentration will be highlighted. The research question of this study therefore implies: To what extent do work- and non-work-related distractions influence an employee’s concentration on work tasks? Furthermore, does the personal characteristic flexibility make a difference in this respect? 6 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Next, the theoretical framework of this study will include definitions of the previously mentioned variables and will deepen the understanding of the relationships between the main concepts by the use of scientific theory and empirical articles. Also the differences between these variables between home days and office days will result in another explorative question, which is stated at the end of the theoretical framework. The method section will provide insight into how the conceptual model shall be tested and furthermore how the variables are operationalized. 2. Theoretical Framework 2.1. Concentration on work tasks For years, the boundaries between work and home have mostly been separated. However, with the advent of flexible work arrangements, the boundaries inevitably shifted from separating home and work to a way of integrating home and work (Nippert-Eng, 1996). Nevertheless, with this shift it is recognized that individuals may be actively participating in one role while simultaneously feeling distracted by thoughts, emotions or demands that belong to another role (Cardenas, Major & Bernas, 2004; Ashforth, Kreiner & Fugate, 2000; Nippert-Eng, 1996). Ultimately, such distractions will reduce performance efficiency and an effective possibility to enhance this performance is by taskrelevant concentration (Demerouti, Taris & Bakker, 2007). Overall, concentration has to do with an individual’s ability to direct thinking in whatever direction this individual would intend. Many forms of work involve individual cognitive activities, which require long periods of uninterrupted time during which one can concentrate (Perlow, 1999). This also applies for employees working under The New Working Conditions, which imply high levels of autonomy, being managed by objectives, and mostly facing strict deadlines (Peters, den Dulk & van der Lippe, 2009). Therefore, it can be said that working under such conditions requires a high absorptive ability, or in other words, an uninterrupted concentration, of an individual (Carsky, Dolan & Free, 1991). In several studies it states that working at home enhances the ability to concentrate on work for most individuals, if not all (Montreuil & Lippel, 2002; Vittersø et al., 2003; Van Sell & Jacobs, 1994). Findings suggest that the home environment fosters better concentration than the office, where noise, regular interruptions, open-space lay-out and dubious air quality have a negative impact on employees (Montreuil & Lippel, 2002). Moreover, Peters and Wildebeest (2010) notice that telework can be seen as a resource and therefore a positive relationship between telework and 7 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk positive work outcomes will exist. Peters and Wildebeest (2010) also emphasize the importance of the amount of teleworking. In their research on telework, flow and exhaustion, they found that substantial teleworking, or differently said working more than one day per week at home, significantly increases the amount of flow at work tasks. Flow consists of absorption, job satisfaction and intrinsic motivation. Whereas this first term, absorption, refers to a mental condition of full concentration. Taken together, these previously mentioned researches (Peters and Wildebeest (2010); Montreuil & Lippel, 2002; Vittersø et al., 2003; Van Sell & Jacobs, 1994) conclude that working from home results in higher concentration levels, whereas working at the office results in lower concentration levels. Despite this conclusion, this research expects something quite different. Today’s society is namely all about flexibility, multitasking, being available preferably twenty-fourhours a day, which makes it impossible to be or stay fully concentrated on work tasks. Also, the location from which an employee works seems to be irrelevant, because quick (online) messages, phone calls or online meetings are a common used method to gather relevant information. It is certainly possible that therefore the efficiency will decrease, whereas the likelihood of errors will increase. However, this seems to be irrelevant to organizations (http://www.gezondweb.be/ gezondweb/rubrieken/ziekteaandoeningen/zenuwstelsel/concentreren/wat-is-concentratie.htm). Therefore, in this study it is expected that the level of concentration on work tasks for employees working at the office and for employees working at home is not that different from each other as is investigated by previous research. 2.2. Distractions Distractions are defined as psychological reactions triggered by external stimuli or alternative activities that interrupt focused concentration on a primary task (Jett & George, 2003). Distractions are generally encouraged by competing activities or environmental stimuli that are irrelevant to the relevant task and they affect a person's cognitive processes by diverting attention that might otherwise have been directed to that task (Jett & George, 2003). Therefore, distractions consume time that could be spent on critical tasks, and these distractions can leave a person with insufficient time to meet a deadline, achieve a goal, or simply complete a task (Jett & George, 2003). As a consequence, distractions are often regarded as annoying and frustrating, because they keep employees from their work. However, according to Zijlstra, Roe, Leonarda and Krediet (1999), distractions can also be seen as welcome distracters in boring and monotonous work tasks. 8 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk For an employee it is possible that while having a family dinner, he or she may get a call from a coworker or simply be distracted by thinking about a work project while having this family dinner (Cardenas et al., 2004). As can be seen by this example, distractions can be both behavioral (f.i. intrusions) and/ or psychological (f.i. thoughts; Jett & George, 2003). Therefore in this research, both behavioral and psychological distractions will be taken into account. Disagreements in literature exist about the effectiveness of working at home to attain the most optimal level of concentration. Some research notice that when an employee is conducting work at home on one or more days per week, better concentration is likely to happen (Vittersø et al., (2003); Montreuil & Lippel, 2002; Van Sell & Jacobs, 1994). According to Van Sell and Jacobs (1994) this is explainable, because fewer work-related interruptions will occur at home, which causes this improved concentration. However, Sullivan & Lewis (2001) also explain that distractions at home while working will occur in a different way, namely due to non-work-related distractions, such as children running around, unexpected visitors coming over, and doing household chores. Deliberately combining work tasks with such non-work tasks seems a common strategy for teleworkers (Sullivan & Lewis, 2001) and because of this, obtaining absolute concentration is almost unthinkable. Therefore, it can be concluded that non-work-related distractions are more common to occur in the home environment, whereas work-related distractions seems to occur more frequently in the office domain. In sum it seems that distractions, in whatever form they may be, result in negative consequences for an individual. This is especially the case when work is complex, demanding, requires learning and an individual’s full attention (Jett & George, 2003). As stated earlier, this is often the case for an employee working under the New Working conditions. The hypotheses concerning work-related and non-work-related distractions therefore state: H1: The more work-related distractions an employee experiences, the less concentration on work tasks this employee will experience. H2: The more non-work-related distractions an employee experiences, the less concentration on work tasks this employee will experience. 2.3. Personal characteristic of flexibility Personal flexibility can be described as the ability to cope quickly with changing circumstances or environmental uncertainty (Gupta & Goyal, 1989). Moreover, there is a strong link between 9 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk flexibility and adaptability (Karuppan, 2004). Individuals who perform well in a changing task context are namely said to be highly adaptable, whereas those who do not perform well in a changing context are considered to have low adaptability (Le Pine, Colquitt & Erez, 2000). As a consequence, one can argue that human beings are unique in their adaptability or their ability to find routes toward desired ends (Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010). Therefore in this research, because of the overlap in the preceding definitions, both personal flexibility and adaptability will be used interchangeably. It can be said that telework is not appropriate for all employees (Lamond, Daniels & Standen, 2003). Generic task competencies may namely be highly influential in adaptation to telework, which among others include a persons’ level of flexibility towards tasks, time management skills and the ability to take independent decisions (Lamond, Daniels & Standen, 2003). Therefore, when a teleworker is being distracted by work-related- or non-work-related issues, it is crucial to react flexible, and adapt to the situation the best way possible. This is also confirmed by Jett and George (2003), who state that the level of flexibility, or its counterpart rigidity, can affect an individual’s response to distractions. For example, when a person is facing a rise in job demands, this persons’ coping skills are likely to stretch, which in turn can lead to reduces in personal adaptability (O’Connell, McNeely & Hall, 2008). As individual adaptability has numerously been indicated as a factor for organizational success (Le Pine et al., 2000), it must be clear that being flexible and adaptable is an important facet. For an employee each work task has an optimum to perform the task most optimally. The body mobilizes energy to let the employee focus and concentrate to perform this task (Gaillard, 2003). Because an individual is not capable of staying highly concentrated all the time, this person can take independent decisions to adapt to distractions or not. As previously mentioned, Zijlstra, Roe, Leonarda and Krediet (1999) state that distractions can also be seen as welcome interruptions of the task at hand. This can be the case when an employee is not longer highly concentrated on the work task and a distraction therefore can be considered as a nice break to momentary escape the work situation. Therefore it is hypothesized that: H3: The personal characteristic flexibility will buffer the relationship between work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks. H4: The personal characteristic flexibility will buffer the relationship between non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks. 10 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk The conceptual model to be tested in this research, as derived from the preceding four hypotheses, is shown in figure 1. This model will be examined on ‘home days’, being the two days employees worked most of the time at home, and on ‘office days’ , which refers to two days employees worked most of the time at the office. Furthermore, as derived from the preceding theory in which it was stated that in this study it is expected that concentration on work tasks will be more or less the same on home days and office days, work-related distractions will be higher for office days than for home days and lastly non-work-related distractions will be higher for home days than for office days, it is of interest to explore the following question: To what extent does the level of concentration on work tasks and the level of work-related and non-work-related distractions vary between home days and office days? The research question and the added above stated explorative question will be answered in the conclusion section, after all analyses have been conducted. Personal Flexibility Work-Distractions - Concentration on work tasks Non-Work Distractions - Figure 1. Conceptual Model 11 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 3. Method 3.1. Research context With the aim to examine the aforestated four hypotheses and to give answer to the added explorative question about home days and office day differences, a quantitative research was conducted. Furthermore, a longitudinal design was used, because data was collected on a six-day basis and therefore causal conclusions could be made between the days. However, for this study no causal interpretations between the days were made and therefore the data-analysis can be considered to be cross-sectional. The data for this research was collected at several organizations in The Netherlands which all were familiar with The New Way of Working or which offered flexible work arrangements such as teleworking. Moreover, data collection took place by the procedure of snowball sampling. Connections at the abovementioned organizations were used to distribute a general questionnaire and the six-day-diary. In this way, the sample would build up and therefore enough useful data would be gathered for this study. Despite the fact of using a snowball sampling procedure, this research managed to get an approximately same amount of males and females and an equal variance of respondents in all age groups. In this way generalizations could be made, because the amount of outliers was reduced. The diary was kept six working days by each respondent individually, from which three days at home and three days at the office. In this way it was possible to compare workdays from home to each other and to workdays at the office and the other way around. The total number of respondents that were approached for this research was 145, whereas the total number of participants who returned the general questionnaire was 120. This therefore made the response-rate 82,7%. The amount of participants who answered the six-day-diary fluctuated, which can be seen in table 2. Based on this, it was decided to not run regressions for home day three (n=77) and office day three (n=103), because of the decreasing amount of respondents, compared to the other days. Therefore only two working days from home and two working days at the office will be discussed in this study. 3.2. Procedures The diary and the general questionnaire were combined in a booklet in which also a brief introduction to the research was given. Moreover, the diary and the questionnaire for the respondents was set up in Dutch. In this way no translations of the Dutch scales had to be considered and therefore no misinterpretations in a different language have occurred. The contact 12 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk person of every organization received a letter with an explanation and procedure of the study. To this person it was asked to contribute and distribute the diary and questionnaire among other suited employees. In the letter to the contact person also the importance of confidentiality was emphasized. Respondents could hand in the booklet in a closed envelope and put it in a common, beforehand postmarked, envelope, which after a few weeks was send by the contact person to the researcher. Furthermore, to all respondents clarity was given about when, or differently said, at what time of the day, the diary should be filled in. Because of the fact that a working day under The New Way of Working conditions could also end at other times than a normal working day from nine till five, the best suitable option for all respondents was to fill in the diary one hour before going to sleep. Finally, it was emphasized that there were no wrong or right answer possibilities. The booklet can be found in Appendix I. 3.3. Description of the sample The participants in this research were employees working at various organizations and in different industries across the Netherlands. All 120 respondents worked in a way that was related to The New Way of Working conditions. There were 45% women and 55% men included in the sample. The age of the respondents varied from 21 years till 61 years, whereas the average age was 36,1 years with a standard deviation of 10.8. The majority of the sample population was highly educated, namely 49,2% had a university of applied sciences degree, whereas 37,5% of the population was in the possession of an university degree. Furthermore, most of the respondents lived together with a partner or with a partner and children (both 35%), whereas 27,5% of the respondents lived alone and only 2,5% of the sample population lived alone with children. Of all respondents, 57,5% worked in an operational position, 33,3% in a management position and only 9,2 % hold a board of directors position. The average contractual hours worked by the sample population were 36.6 hours per week, ranging from 15 hours till 40 hours a week. 13 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the sample population Variable Total /Mean Men 55% Women 45% Average age (in years) 31,6 University 37,5% University of applied sciences 49,2% Operational level 57,5% Management level 33,3% Board of director level 9,2% Contractual hours 36,6 N=120 3.4. Instruments In this study, next to a survey, also a diary method was used to look at the concepts more in depth. In this way better understandings of the day to day experiences of the relationship between working at home or at the office and distractions and concentration were examined. As Ohly, Sonnentag, Niessen and Zapf (2010) state, diary methods give insight in thoughts, feelings, and behaviors within the work context as well as characteristics of the work situation which can fluctuate on a daily basis. The diary consisted of three items, namely work-related distractions, non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks, whereas the survey consisted one original item, namely personal flexibility and a scale about general level of concentration on work tasks. This was done to check for the respondents consistency in the answer-pattern in daily and general feelings of concentration on work tasks. This will be discussed next. 3.4.1. Diary Operationalization Working day from home and Working day from the office The first question of the diary stated: ‘I consider this day as a: 1. A day working from home, or: 2. A day working at the office. So in total six working days were measured with the diary, from which 14 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk only four were used. As explained earlier, with The New Way of Working it is of frequent occurrence that employees work a few hours at home in the morning to avoid traffic jams. Therefore, as an instruction respondents had to select three days they worked more than half of the working day at home, which was considered to be a working day from home, and the other way around, namely, to select three days they worked more than half of the working day at the office, so this was considered to be a working day at the office. To check the hours a respondent worked at home or at the office, the questions ‘How many hours did you work at home today’ and ‘How many hours did you work at the office today’, were also included in the diary. The questions can be found in Appendix I. Operationalization day-level Concentration on work tasks Concentration was measured by four items, of which three came from the Flow State Scale by Jackson and Marsh (1996). Originally this scale consisted of four items, but in imitation of Beard and Hoy (2010) and Quinn (2005) the choice was made to develop a three-item concept. Reason for this was that the study on flow by Jackson and Marsh (1996) focused mostly on sports and leisure and thus on physical activity, whereas the studies by Beard and Hoy (2010) and Quinn (2005) were totally focused on knowledge work and thus on intellectual activity, which also suites this research better. Furthermore, an additional item was added, namely ‘My thoughts were wandering to other things during the task’. This item came from the study by Demerouti, Taris and Bakker (2007) on the critical link of concentration in the relationship between need for recovery, home-work interference and performance, and was a reverse-coded item. The concept of concentration measures the degree to which employees have a complete focus upon their tasks on a seven-point Likert-type scale, anchored by 1= ‘strongly disagree’ to 7= ‘strongly agree’. Statements such as ‘My attention was focused entirely on what I was doing’ and ‘I was completely focused on the task at hand’ had to be answered six working days on this sevenpoint Likert-type scale. Higher scores on the scale showed that respondents were more concentrated on work tasks than respondents with lower scale scores. The study by Beard and Hoy (2010) reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.792, whereas the study by Quinn reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.8. The study by Demerouti, Taris and Bakker (2007) reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.78 on time 1 and a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83 on time 2. For this research the internal consistency of the scale had Cronbach’s alpha’s of respectively .908 for home day one, .900 for home day two, .905 for office day one and .892 for office day two. 15 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Principal factor analysis for concentration on work tasks for the two days from home and the two days at the office showed a clear one-factor model, which declared 80,16% of the variance. This became evident by the intial eigenvalues and the screeplot (Pallant, 2007). Furthermore, all correlations for all days were above .3, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy (KMO) value was .824 for home day one, .815 for home day two, .815 for office day one, and .816 for office day two, whereas the Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was significant (.00) for all days. Therefore all statistical requirements for this scale were met (Pallant, 2007). The results of the factor analyses can be found in Appendix II. Operationalization day-level Work-related distractions and Non-work-related distractions A self-designed scale has been used to measure the two types of distractions. Respondents were asked to indicate how much they felt distracted by work-related and non-work-related distractions on three days they worked from home and on three days they worked at the office. A starting point for this concept was a question created by Cardenas et al. (2004), namely: ‘Estimate the total number of hours per day you feel distracted by work thoughts or interruptions while at home or with family’. However, as distractions are not easy to interpret in hours or even minutes, a continuous scale was made with five answering categories. First, a short introduction to the scale was given to give the respondent more insight in what is considered a thought and an action to work-related- and nonwork-related distractions. Second, a common question was asked, namely: ‘to what extent did you feel distracted during your work tasks today?’ Two questions were asked about thoughts to workrelated distractions as real actions to work-related distractions. These two questions were: Today, how much did you feel distracted by: thoughts to work tasks other than the primary task (1) and actions to work tasks other than the primary task (2). Furthermore, two questions were asked about thoughts to non-work-related distractions and real actions to non-work-related distractions: namely: Today, how much did you feel distracted by: thoughts to private matters (1) and actions to private matters (2). All five questions had to be answered on a five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1= never to 5= often. Principal factor analysis for the total distractions scale for both working days from home and the two working days at the office showed a clear two-factor structure, which meant a clear contradiction between the non-work related distractions and the work-related distractions (Appendix II). For both working days from home and office day 2 the component correlation matrix showed correlations above .3 and therefore the Oblimin rotation was reported (Pallant, 2007). 16 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk For office day 1 the Varimax rotation was reported, because the components were correlated lower than .3. When conducting a separate factor analysis for work-related and non-work related distractions, for all days the correlations in the correlation matrix were all above .3, the KMO values were .500 and the Bartlett’s Tests of Sphericity were .00, so significant (Appendix II). The internal consistency of the two-item scale for work-related distractions had a Cronbach’s alpha of: .680 for home day one, .737 for home day two, .794 for office day one and .813 for office day two, whereas the Cronbach’s alpha’s for the two-item scale of non-work-related distractions were respectively: .894, .813, .699 and .779. 3.4.2. General Questionnaire Operationalization personal characteristic Flexibility Flexibility is considered to be a fixed personal characteristic of a respondent and as a consequence will not fluctuate per day; therefore this item was measured with a survey. Personal flexibility was measured by its counterpart, the rigidity scale, which is part of the Dutch Personality Questionnaire2 by Barelds, Luteijn, van Dijk and Starren (2007). This scale consisted of twenty items, from which two items were reverse-coded and had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.84. However, for this study only eight out of the twenty items were considered to be interesting for this research. These eight items seemed to be more in line with and relevant to feelings of distractions instead of the also measured characteristics of appearance and being neatly. Overall, the rigidity scale measures how orderly and dutiful a person is. Someone who scores high on this scale is very attached to fixed rules and habits and is very conscientious. Someone who scores low on this scale is very flexible, adapts easily to any given situations and would like to solve problems. Therefore, in this study, the total scale had to be reverse-coded so that respondents who scored high on this scale were seen as being highly flexible and adaptable.The scale for this study existed of eight items, which had to be answered with three answer possibilities, namely: ‘True’, ‘don’t know’, or ‘False’. Statements such as ‘I don’t like to interrupt the task I am working on’ and ‘I often do things in a fixed order’ had to be answered. Principal factor analysis for the eight-item flexibility scale for the two working days from home and the two working days at the office showed that two of the eight items did not met the requirement for the correlation coefficients being higher than .3 (Pallant, 2007). These items were items 75 and 79 and were deleted from the scale. For the remaining six items principal factor 17 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk analysis showed one clear eigenvalue higher than one, which declared 47,49% of the variance. The KMO value for these six items was .809 and the Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was significant (.00) (Appendix II). The internal consistency of this six-item scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of .770, which was indeed higher than the eight-item scale (α = .758). Operationalization general-level Concentration To check for consistency among the respondents answers on the scale of concentration on work tasks, this scale was also added to the general questionnaire (Appendix I). In this way the common and daily experiences of this concept were measured and compared to each other. The general level of concentration scale was the same scale as the daily concentration scale, except for the fact that the items did not start with ‘today…’. The scale existed of four items which together made a Cronbach’s alpha of.914. In this research this general-level of concentration will be used as a control variable. 3.5. Control Variables To show whether the relationships between the variables in this research were not caused due to any disturbing factors, five control variables were included in this study. General level of concentration. With controlling for the general level of concentration, a consistency check in the answering of the respondents on the daily questionnaire is made possible. Gender. Research has shown that gender is related to working from home. As Sullivan & Lewis (2001) notice, women tend to work from home because of domestic reasons, whereas men have the tendency to work from home because of work-related- and individual reasons. Therefore it can be argued that gender also has an impact on concentration on work tasks. According to O’ Connell et al. (2008) gender also seems to affect adaptability. Age. Age also has an effect on working from home. The amount of teleworkers is the highest in the age category 35 to 44 years (CBS, 2005). Furthermore there seems to be significant differences between concentration abilities in old and young people (Grady, Springer, Hongwanishkul, McIntosh & Winocur (2006). Grady et al. (2006) state that it is also known that older adults are more easily distracted. Also adaptability is likely to be influenced by age (O’ Connell et al., 2008). As O’Connell et al. (2008) state, gender and age may set expectations to social norms and create differences in treatment in the workplace. So for all variables in this research, age seems to be relevant. Family life stage. When someone is living alone, fewer distractions by the family domain are likely to occur. However, when this is not the case, distractions by co-residents are likely to happen 18 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk when working at home. According to Sullivan & Lewis (2001), these co-residents are crucial actors in the construction of the home as a workplace. Moreover, in this study the expectation is that concentration depends on the amount of distractions an employee experiences. Therefore, an understanding of the impact of an employee’s family life stage should be taken into consideration. Occupational level and educational background. It seems that employees with a managerial position are more likely to work from home than employees who do not have a managerial position (Tijdens, Wetzels & Van Klaveren, 2000). Also higher educated employees seem to work more from home, because they have to do more thinking and writing than lower educated employees (CBS, 2005). Therefore, attaining high levels of concentration on work tasks seem to be more relevant to higher educated employees. 3.6. Quality of the data The general questionnaire was filled in by 120 respondents. The daily questionnaires in its turn had to be restructured, due to the fact that no fixed order was given in the way the home and office days had to be filled in by the respondents. Furthermore, not all respondents ideally followed up the instructions in the questionnaire. For instance, some respondents did not fill in all the daily questionnaires and others filled in more than three office days and less than three home days. However, to conduct the regression analyses, a clearly structured SPSS datafile was needed. Therefore the data was entered in SPSS in the following order: general questionnaire, home day 1, home day 2, home day 3, office day 1, office day 2 and office day 3. When further analyzing the datafile, a difference was found in ‘clear’ home and office days and ‘unclear’ home and office days. It was decided to only keep the ‘clear’ home and office days in the datafile. This decision was based on the rule that a respondent had to work at least twice the amount of hours at home compared to the hours at the office on a day in order to be scaled as a real home day and the other way around to be indicated as a real office day. Moreover, when for instance, a respondent filled in more than three office days, this fourth ‘clear’ office day was also removed from the datafile. The previously mentioned rules led to a removal of a total of 54 daily questionnaires from the datafile and the decision to only analyze the data for the first two home days and the first two office days. The exact numbers of respondents that filled in clear home- and office days can be found in table 2. 19 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 2 Number of respondents for each day separately Type of day Home day 1 No. of respondents 115 Home day 2 102 Home day 3 77 Office day 1 116 Office day 2 112 Office day 3 103 3.7. Statistical analysis After all data was collected, a standard statistical program, SPSS, was used to enter and analyze the data. First, the frequency tables were used to detect errors and missing values (Pallant, 2007). Secondly, the reversed coded total scale of flexibility and the fourth item of the concentration scale were recoded, so that high answers on the flexibility scale meant high scores on being flexible and high scores for the fourth item of the concentration on works tasks scale meant being highly concentrated. Finally, the data set was checked on assessing normality and outliers (Pallant, 2007). To test the hypotheses in the preceding conceptual model, hierarchical multiple regression analyses on the dataset were conducted for each day separately. This was done because this type of analysis, also known as sequential multiple regression analysis, is frequently used in research using linear- and interaction effects (Keith, 2006). Before these hierarchical multiple regression analyses were done, all variables which were used in the interaction effects had to be centered (Voeten & van den Bercken, 2004). Then all variables were entered one by one into the regression equation. This was done in the following sequence: I. Control variables II. Work-related distractions, non-work-related distractions and personal flexibility III. Work-related distractions x personal flexibility and non-work-related distractions x personal flexibility. In sum, the conceptual model was tested four times, so that the results of two working days at home could be compared to each other and to the results of the two working days at the office. In 20 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk such manner conclusions were drawn about the effects of the location (home or the office) on work- and non-work-related distractions and concentration, with the moderating variable of personal flexibility. The results of these analyses are discussed in the Results section. 4. Results 4.1. Variable differences between Home Days and Office Days Before testing the actual hypotheses, the variables concentration on works tasks, work-related- and non-work-related distractions will be tested separately for home and office days. This will be done with a paired-samples t-test, which is applied when data is collected from one group of respondents under two different conditions (Pallant, 2007). With SPSS it is possible to test whether the average degree of the variables significantly differ from each other. First, a paired-samples t-test will test whether the average degree of work-related distractions on an office day is significantly higher than on a home day. Moreover, with another paired-samples t-test it is tested whether the average degree of non-work-related distractions is significantly higher on home days than on office days. Lastly, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to look for significant differences between home and office days for the variable concentration on work tasks. In this way the explorative question stated earlier in this research can be answered: To what extent does the level of concentration on work tasks and the level of work-related and non-work-related distractions vary between home days and office days? The results of the three paired-samples t-test are shown in table 3.1, 3.2, and 3.3. Table 3.1. Results of the paired- samples t-test for Work-related distractions Mean Std. Deviation t df Sig. (2tailed) Eta squared Pair 1 Home day 1 – Home day 2 .058 1.023 ,581 101 .563 Pair 2 Office day 1 – Office day 2 .117 .9389 1,314 110 .192 Pair 3 Home day 1 – Office day 1 -.708 1.210 -6,222 112 .000 0.26 Pair 4 Home day 1 – Office day 2 -.610 1.210 -5,263 108 .000 0.20 Pair 5 Home day 2 – Office day 1 -.810 1.224 -6,616 99 .000 0.31 Pair 6 Home day 2 – Office day 2 -.722 1.271 -5,594 96 .000 0.25 21 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Foremost, as can be seen from table 3.1, work-related distractions do not significantly differ from each other on the same kind of days. Therefore, it can be concluded that between home day 1 (M=2,726) and home day 2 (M=2,667) and between office day 1 (M=3,437) and office day 2 (M=3,320), there is no significant difference in work-related distractions. The difference between the mean on home day 1 (M=2.726) the mean on office day 1 (M=3,437) and the mean on office day 2 (M=3,320) seem to be significant at p<0.001. Also, the difference between the mean on home day 2 (M=2,667) and the means on the office days seem to be significant at p<0.001. In general, it can be concluded that work-related distractions seem to be lower on home days compared to office days and the effect size between these days can be considered to be quite large, according to the eta squared, which is higher than .14 (Pallant, 2007). Table 3.2. Results of the paired- samples t-test for Non-work-related distractions Mean Std. Deviation t df Sig. (2tailed) Eta squared Pair 1 Home day 1 – Home day 2 .100 1.052 ,951 99 .344 Pair 2 Office day 1 – Office day 2 -.041 .9562 -,447 110 .656 Pair 3 Home day 1 – Office day 1 .558 1.308 4,515 111 .000 0.16 Pair 4 Home day 1 – Office day 2 .514 1.218 4,385 107 .000 0.15 Pair 5 Home day 2 – Office day 1 .520 1.176 4,402 98 .000 0.17 Pair 6 Home day 2 – Office day 2 .417 1.160 3,519 95 .001 0.12 Next to table 3.1, table 3.2. also shows that non-work-related distractions do not significantly differ between days of the same kind. Furthermore, the differences between the mean of home day 1 (M=2.896) and the mean of office day 1 (M=2.284) and office day 2 (M=2.324) seem to be significant at p<0.001. This also yields for home day 2 (M=2.796) and the office days. In sum, this means that non-work related distractions seem to be higher on home days in comparison to office days, which is also reflected by the eta squared. The effect size between the two home- and the two office days can be considered as relatively large, because the eta squared is approximately around .14. 22 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 3.3. Results of the paired- samples t-test for Concentration on work tasks Mean Std. Deviation t df Sig. (2tailed) Eta squared Pair 1 Home day 1 – Home day 2 -.005 1.276 -,039 101 .969 Pair 2 Office day 1 – Office day 2 .016 1.238 ,134 111 .894 Pair 3 Home day 1 – Office day 1 .192 1.727 1,177 111 .242 0.01 Pair 4 Home day 1 – Office day 2 .252 1.707 1,543 108 .126 0.02 Pair 5 Home day 2 – Office day 1 .245 1.803 1,358 99 .177 0.02 Pair 6 Home day 2 – Office day 2 .342 1.720 1,967 97 .052 0.04 Finally, the results of the paired-samples t-test in table 3.3. show that concentration between home days do not significantly differ from each other, suggesting that the levels of concentration are the same on days working from home. The same result yield for office days, because of p>.05, the concentration levels on office days can be considered the same. However, when comparing home days with office days, small differences are found. This can be explained as concentration being more of a fixed personal characteristic, which does not fluctuate radically. A significant difference between the mean of home day 2 (M=4.728) and the mean of office day 2 (M=4.507) is found with p<.05. However, the difference between home day 2 and office day 1 (M=4.522) was only significant at a p<.10. This was also the case for the difference between home day 1 (M=4.723) and office day 2. For the difference between the means of home day 1 and office day 1, no significant result was found, which suggests that the concentration levels did not differ for these days. In sum it can be said that concentration on work tasks differs in some cases, but overall concentration seems to be the same for different kind of days. Moreover, when taking a look at the eta squared, the effect sizes between home days and office days are relatively small (relatively: .01, .02, .02, .04), suggesting that even if there were differences between the home days and the office days, these differences are quite small. 23 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 4.2. Correlations In the tables 4.1, 4.2, 4.3 and 4.4, the number of respondents, the means, standard deviations and Pearson’s correlations for all variables are shown. Correlations concerning the control variables education, living situation and work level should be interpreted with caution, because in this study’s sample respondents did not score evenly on the answering categories. For example, in this study almost all respondents were highly educated. Moreover, the correlation matrixes were checked for multicollinearity issues between the variables work-related- and non-work-related distractions. As a guideline correlations of .3 till .49 were considered to be good (Cohen, 1988). These correlations for the two working days from home were respectively, r=.375, and r=.355. For both office days these correlations were respectively: r=.263 and r=.488. So, in sum, there seemed to be no disruptive multicollinearity issues. When taking a look at the dependent and independent variables, for all days, a negative correlation between work-related distractions and concentration on works tasks is found (r=-.509, r=-.-.381, r=--.398, r=-.535, all with p < .01). The same, but somewhat higher, negative correlation yields for non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks for all days as well (r=-.662, r=-.658, r=-.524 and r=-.629, all with p < .01). Another remarkable and significant correlation exists between the interaction effect of non-work-related distractions and flexibility on concentration (r=.456, r=-.502, r=-.399, r=-.475, all with p < .01). These correlations seem to be much higher than the interaction effect of work-related distractions and flexibility on concentration (r=-.255, p < .01 , r=-.231, p < .01 , r=.176, n.s., r=-.280, p < .01). As can be seen by these results, being flexible in combination with either work-related-or non-work-related distractions decreases the direct correlation these variables have with concentration on work tasks. This conclusion suggests that when experiencing distractions of one or both kinds and being flexible is beneficial for concentration. Overall, for all days, non-work-related distractions seem to effect concentration on work tasks more than work-related distractions do. 24 Table 4.1 Correlation Matrix Home Day 1; Number of Respondents (N), Mean(M), Standard Deviation(SD) and intercorrelations Variables N M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 76 8 9 10 1. Concentration 114 4.730 1.310 2. Work Distractions (WD) 115 2.726 .911 -.509** 3. Non Work Distractions (NWD) 114 2.833 1.114 -.662** .375** 4. Flexibility 120 2.161 .590 .155 -.194* -.255** 5. Interaction WD * Flexibility 115 -.255** .701** .078 .522** 6. Interaction NWD * Flexibility 114 -.456** .157 .729** .421** .391** 7. General Concentration level 120 .381** -.083 -.220* .166 .051 -.119 8. Gender 120 .098 -.063 -.061 -.072 -.099 -.122 -.020 9. Age 120 36.13 .102 .012 -.055 -.036 .015 -.105 .272** -.179 10. Education 120 5.22 -.045 -.025 -.165 .100 .042 -.074 -.046 -.039 -.082 11. Living Situation 120 2.78 -.135 .134 .167 .109 .151 .176 .097 -.096 .470** -.078 12. Work Level 120 1.52 -.021 .147 .234* -.241** -.073 -.013 .033 -.176 .240** -.232* 4.758 1.232 10.805 11 12 .148 ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 25 Table 4.2 Correlation Matrix Home Day 2; Number of Respondents (N), Mean(M), Standard Deviation(SD) and intercorrelations Variables N M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 96 10 1. Concentration 102 4.727 1.361 2. Work Distractions (WD) 102 2.667 .945 -.381** 3. Non Work Distractions (NWD) 101 2.797 1.070 -.658** .355** 4. Flexibility 120 2.161 .590 .075 .027 -.146 5. Interaction WD * Flexibility 102 -.231** .769** .126 .622** 6. Interaction NWD * Flexibility 101 -.502** .253** .760** .490** .456** 7. General Concentration level 120 ,256** -.236* -.154 .166 -.118 -.027 8. Gender 120 -.036 -.132 .053 -.072 -.088 .066 -.020 9. Age 120 36.13 .041 .080 .001 -.036 .003 -.050 .272** -.179 10. Education 120 5.22 .047 -.218* -.222* .100 -.130 -.160 -.046 -.039 -.082 11. Living Situation 120 2.78 -.131 .206* .182 .109 .164 .154 .097 -.096 .470** -.078 12. Work Level 120 1.52 -.112 .166 .214* -.241** -.060 .024 .033 -.176 .240** -.232* 4.758 1.232 10.805 11 12 .148 ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 26 Table 4.3 Correlation Matrix Office Day 1; Number of Respondents (N), Mean(M), Standard Deviation(SD) and intercorrelations Variables N M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 77 8 9 10 1. Concentration 116 4.569 1.306 2. Work Distractions (WD) 116 3.414 .931 -.398** 3. Non Work Distractions (NWD) 116 2.254 .917 -.524** .263** 4. Flexibility 120 2.161 .590 .130 -.110 -.306** 5. Interaction WD * Flexibility 116 -.176 .632** -.059 .674** 6. Interaction NWD * Flexibility 116 -.399** .147 .743** .368** .379** 7. General Concentration level 120 .471** -.172 -.286** .166 .009 -.186* 8. Gender 120 .107 -.124 .001 -.072 -.110 .003 -.020 9. Age 120 36.13 .177 -.108 -.065 -.036 -.132 -.098 .272** -.179 10. Education 120 5.22 -.096 -.127 -.069 .100 -.010 -.011 -.046 -.039 -.082 11. Living Situation 120 2.78 -.030 -.020 .048 .109 .058 .113 .097 -.096 .470** -.078 12. Work Level 120 1.52 -.032 .267** .214* -.241** -.044 -.042 .033 -.176 .240** -.232* 4.758 1.232 10.805 11 12 .148 ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 27 Table 4.4 Correlation Matrix Office Day 2; Number of Respondents (N), Mean(M), Standard Deviation(SD) and intercorrelations Variables N M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 76 8 9 10 1. Concentration 112 4.507 1.289 2. Work Distractions (WD) 111 3.320 1.048 -.535** 3. Non Work Distractions (NWD) 111 2.324 1.048 -.629** .488** 4. Flexibility 120 2.161 .590 .278** -.338** -.429** 5. Interaction WD * Flexibility 111 -.280** .623** .064 .490** 6. Interaction NWD * Flexibility 111 -.475** .263** .725** .255** .437** 7. General Concentration level 120 .376** -.141 -.249** .166 -.012 -.178 8. Gender 120 .139 -.113 .005 -.072 -.124 .003 -.020 9. Age 120 36.13 .113 .028 -.043 -.036 -.030 -.100 .272** -.179 10. Education 120 5.22 -.036 -.095 -.170 .100 .007 -.113 -.046 -.039 -.082 11. Living Situation 120 2.78 -.078 .076 .186 .109 .136 .268** .097 -.096 .470** -.078 12. Work Level 120 1.52 -.153 .364** .328** -.241** .038 .077 .033 -.176 .240** -.232* 4.758 1.232 10.805 11 12 .148 ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 28 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 4.3. Sequential multiple regression analyses and control variables In order to test the influence of work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions, personal flexibility and the interaction effects of these variables on concentration on work tasks, hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to test the four hypotheses for two home days and two office days independently. On the grounds that the sample size of this study was not quite large (N=120), the choice has been made to use a maximum number of five independent variables that will be included in the analyses. Meyers, Gamst & Guarino (2006) recommended to have a sample size that is twenty times the number of predictors. Since the respondents vary from 102 till 120 per day, it seems that the best solution is to apply five independent variables. In this study this means that only two control variables could be entered into the regression analyses. Because of the fact that the last three control variables; education, living situation and work level, did not vary equally between the corresponding answer categories and research about the combination of gender and concentration and/or gender and distractions is less pronounced, the choice have been made to include age and general level of concentration as the most important control variables in this research. Nevertheless, as a conclusion only general feelings of concentration appear to have a significant effect on the dependent and the independent variables, whereas age does not (Appendix III) Therefore, this non-significant control variable age will not be taken into account when further analyzing the data. 29 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 4.4. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 1 Table 5.1 Results Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 1 Concentration on work tasks Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 .381*** .256*** .261*** Work distractions -.312*** -.293*** Non-work distractions -.509*** -.471*** Personal Flexibility -.077 -.066 General Concentration level Work distractions * Flexibility .078 Non-work distractions * Flexibility .087 R² .145 R² Change F Change 18,843*** .581 .600 .436*** .019 37,492*** 2,517*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). As can be seen by table 5.1, 14,5% of the variance in concentration on work tasks is explained by Model 1, or differently said, by the control variable general concentration level. The beta (ß= .381, p<0.001) suggests that the higher a respondents general level of concentration is, the higher this respondents concentration level is on home day 1. Model 2 explains an additional 43,6% to the variance in concentration on work tasks. This model shows that work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions have a negative effect on concentration (respectively: ß= -.312, p< 0.001 and ß= -.509, p< 0.001). Therefore hypotheses one and two are confirmed for home day 1. In addition, Model 3 explains only an additional 1.9% of the variance in concentration on work tasks. In this model the moderator effects of personal flexibility are added. However, no significant effects of neither the interaction effect between work-related distractions and personal flexibility nor the interaction effect of non-work-related distractions and personal flexibility were 30 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk found. Therefore, hypotheses three and four are rejected for home day 1. In figure 2.1 the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for home day 1 are shown. Personal Flexibility .087(n.s) Work-Distractions -.078(n.s) -.312*** Concentration on work tasks Non-Work Distractions -.509*** Figure 2.1. Results of linear regression analysis for home day 1 4.5. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 2 In table 5.2 the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for home day 2 are shown. As can be seen from this table, 6,5% of the variance in concentration on work tasks is explained by a respondents general level of concentration. Furthermore, an additional 45.8% to Model 1 of the variance in concentration on work tasks is explained by Model 2. Model 2 shows that work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions have a negative effect on concentration on work tasks (respectively: ß= -.137, p< 0.05 and ß= -.593, p< 0.001). Therefore hypotheses one and two are also confirmed for home day two. In addition, just like home day 1, Model 3 for home day 2 only explains an additional small non-significant variance to Model 2 (R² Change=1%). In other words, no significant interactions effects were found. In other words, the interaction effect of work-related distractions and the personal characteristic flexibility on concentration on work tasks and the interaction effect of nonwork-related distractions and the personal characteristic flexibility on concentration on work tasks were not found. Therefore, hypotheses three and four are also rejected for home day 2. Figure 2.2 shows the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for home day 2. 31 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 5.2 Results Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Home Day 2 Concentration on Work Tasks Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 .256*** .137* .126 Work distractions -.137* -.136 Non-work distractions -.593*** -.632*** Personal Flexibility -.031 -.045 General Concentration level Work distractions * Flexibility -.121 Non-work distractions * Flexibility .048 R² .065 R² Change F Change 6,939** .475 .485 .410*** .010 25,007*** ,895*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). Personal Flexibility .048(n.s) Work-Distractions -.121(n.s) -.137* Concentration on work tasks Non-Work Distractions -.593*** Figure 2.2. Results of linear regression analysis for home day 2 32 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 4.6. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 1 Table 5.3 shows the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis of office day 1. As can be noticed, 22,2% of the variance in concentration on work tasks is explained by Model 1, namely by the respondents general level of concentration. The beta of general level of concentration (ß= .471, p<0.001) suggests that the higher the general level of concentration of a respondent is, the higher this respondents concentration on work tasks for office day 1 is. Model 2 demonstrates significant negative effects of work-related distractions and nonwork-related distractions on concentration on work tasks (respectively: ß= -.248, p< 0.001 and ß= .385, p< 0.001). Therefore, hypotheses one and two for office day 1 are confirmed. Moreover, Model 3 shows to be a non-significant addition to the variance already explained by Model 2. Both interaction effects stated in this model can be considered neglectable (ß= 0.001, p= not significant and ß= 0.048, p = not significant). This means that hypotheses three and four are rejected for office day 1. Figure 2.3 shows the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for office day 1 graphically. Table 5.3 Results Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 1 Concentration on Work Tasks Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 .471*** .330*** .336*** Work distractions -.248*** -.241*** Non-work distractions -.385*** -.384*** Personal Flexibility -.069 -.072 General Concentration level Work distractions * Flexibility .001 Non work distractions * Flexibility .048 R² .222 R² Change F Change 32,505*** .447*** .449 .225*** .002 15,054*** ,224*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 33 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Personal Flexibility .048(n.s) Work-Distractions .001(n.s) -.248*** Concentration on work tasks Non-Work Distractions -.385*** Figure 2.3. Results of linear regression analysis for office day 1 4.7. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 2 Table 5.4 demonstrates the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for office day 2. Model 1 appears to explain a percentage of 14.1 of the variance in concentration on work tasks. In Model 2, work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions both have negative direct effects on concentration on work tasks (respectively: ß= -.306, p< 0.001 and ß= -.447, p< 0.001). Therefore, it can be concluded that both hypothesis one and hypothesis two are confirmed for office day 2. Moreover, for this office day, the interaction effect of work-related distractions and personal flexibility seems to be significant ( ß= -.138, p<.05). However, the interaction effect of non-workrelated distractions and the personal characteristic flexibility appears to be non-significant (ß= -.019, p= not significant) and therefore hypothesis four is already rejected. Figure 2.4 displays the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis for this office day 2. 34 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 5.4 Results Hierarchical multiple regression analysis Office Day 2 Concentration on Work Tasks Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 .376*** .231*** .231*** Work distractions -.303*** -.315*** Non-work distractions -.447*** -.467*** Personal Flexibility -.054 -.074 General Concentration level Work distractions * Flexibility -.138* Non-work distractions * Flexibility -.019 R² .141 R² Change F Change 17,948*** .515 .531 .373*** .016 27,175*** 1,775*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). Personal Flexibility -.019(n.s) -.138* Work-Distractions -.303*** Concentration on work tasks Non-Work Distractions -.447*** Figure 2.4. Results of linear regression analysis for office day 2 35 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk To take a closer look at the significant interaction effect of work-related distractions and personal flexibility on concentration on work tasks for office day 2 (hypothesis 3), an interaction plot has been made. The border between high and low outcomes was at the middle point of the actual responses given to the scale of personal flexibility and work-related distractions. However, as can be seen in figure 2.5, the opposite of what is expected happens. Namely when an individual scores high on the personal characteristic of flexibility in combination with experiences of high work-related distractions he or she has a lower concentration on work tasks instead of a higher concentration on work tasks. It is therefore, hypothesis three in this study is rejected for office day 2. Moreover, in the figure it is indicated that there seems to be almost no differences in the levels of concentration on work tasks for individuals who experience low work-related distractions and scoring either high or low on personal flexibility. This is logical to occur, because when an individual does not experience any or low distractions, he or she does not always have to react to them and in turn can stay highly concentrated on the task at hand. Figure 2.5. Interaction plot of Work-related distractions and Personal Flexibility for office day 2 4.8. Additional analysis on the interaction effects for Home Days and Office Days As was found by the separate hierarchical regression analyses, none out of the eight possible interaction effects have been confirmed. It is therefore expected that there is no clear linear effect between both types of distractions and personal flexibility. Overall, with hierarchical multiple 36 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk regression analysis it is expected that effects will be found on two combinations. The first one being high distractions and low personal flexibility, which will have disadvantageous consequences for concentration on work tasks. The second one being high distractions and high personal flexibility, which will have better consequences for concentration on work tasks than not being flexible at all. These effects are shown in figure 3. However, when one of these expectations does not occur, no interaction effects will be found in hierarchical multiple regression analysis. Therefore, to take a more detailed look at the seven other non-significant interaction effects, a two-way univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted. With two-way ANOVA the effect of two independent variables on the dependent variable and the interaction effect can be simultaneously tested (Pallant, 2007). Within this analysis the variables work-related- and non-work-related distractions and personal flexibility are beforehand transformed into categorical variables. In this way, six groups are formed: a group scoring high and low on work-related distractions, a group scoring high and low on non-work-related distractions and a group scoring high and low on flexibility. The border between high and low was at the midpoint of the actual responses on the scale for each of these groups, in order to get the most equal distribution. In such a way all combination effects for high and low work-related distractions, high and low nonwork-related distractions and high and low personal flexibility levels on concentration on work tasks will become clear by looking at the mean of concentration on work tasks for these combined groups separately. Concentration on work tasks HIGH - 0 + + (non-)work Distractions LOW LOW HIGH Personal Flexibility Figure 3. Expected linear interaction effects of both work and non-work distractions and personal flexibility For all days this two-way between-groups analysis of variance was conducted to explore the impact of personal flexibility and distractions on concentration of work tasks. Per day two ANOVA-tests were done, one with the independent variable concentration on work tasks, personal flexibility and 37 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk work-related distractions and another one with the dependent variable concentration on work tasks, personal flexibility and non-work-related distractions. As can be seen in figure 4, for almost all days it seems that concentration was indeed the lowest among respondents whom experienced high distractions (both work and non-work) and who scored low on the personal characteristic flexibility. However, just like the interaction effect of work-related distractions and personal flexibility on concentration on work tasks on office day 2, there was a same kind of exception, namely for office day 1 and the level of non-work-related distractions. On that specific day respondents had lower concentration levels when experiencing high distractions, but also whom scored high on the personal characteristic of flexibility. In Appendix IV, the results of the two-way ANOVA are given for each day separately both for work-related- as for non-work-related distractions. Moreover, the results of the independent-samples t-test are shown in Appendix V. In this Appendix (non-)significant differences between the two most relevant groups for this study are shown, namely group 1: low personal flexibility and high work-related- and non-work-related distractions and group 2: high personal flexibility and high work-related- and non-work-related distractions. These two groups are the relevant ones, because when an individual experiences none to low distractions his or hers concentration on work tasks will without a doubt be higher than someone who experiences high distractions. Then in turn, personal flexibility seems irrelevant in this case, because this concept measures how well an individual copes with distractions or with irregularities at work. This is measured and reflected in the lower quadrants of figure 4. As can be seen in the results in Appendix V, only home day 1 shows a significant interaction effect of work-related distractions and personal flexibility on concentration on work tasks (p<.05) and a significant interaction effect of non-work-related distractions and personal flexibility on concentration on works tasks (p<.05). However, even with the fact that concentration on work tasks generally was the lowest among respondents who experienced high distractions and who were low on personal flexibility, these results can be considered neglectable for the remaining three days when comparing these results with the results of respondents who scored high on distractions and high on personal flexibility (p>.05). Therefore, in this study, hypotheses three and four are confirmed for home day 1, but still rejected for home day 2 and the two office days. 38 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Concentration on work tasks HIGH Low (non-)work Distractions LOW Low/ Moderate High High LOW HIGH Personal Flexibility Figure 4. Two-way ANOVA; results for all days except for office day 1; non-work-related distractions 5. Conclusion and Discussion 5.1. Conclusion The aim of this study was to give an appropriate answer to the research question stated in the introduction: To what extent do work- and non-work distractions influence an employee’s concentration on work tasks? Furthermore, does the personal characteristic of flexibility make a difference in this respect? Four hypotheses were formulated to see if there were indeed relationships between work-related distractions, non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks and the possibility of a moderating effect of personal flexibility. The first hypothesis stated that work-related distractions have a negative effect on concentration on work tasks; this hypothesis was confirmed for all four days, which were two home days and two office days. The second hypothesis stated that non-workrelated distractions have a negative effect on concentration on work tasks; this hypothesis was confirmed for all days as well. The third hypothesis concerned the moderating effect of personal flexibility on work-related distractions on concentration on work tasks. This hypothesis was rejected for all days, although for office day 2 a negative significant effect (ß =-.138, p<.05) was found. However, this interaction effect was not in the way that it was expected to be and therefore it was decided to reject this hypothesis for office day 2. The fourth and last hypothesis stated that the 39 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk personal characteristic flexibility will buffer the relationship between non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks. However, this hypothesis was rejected with hierarchical multiple regression analysis for all days. Because for all days none out of the eight possible interaction effects have been found in the regression analyses, it was expected that there was no clear linear effect between both types of distractions and personal flexibility. Therefore a univariate analysis of variance was conducted for all days. As expected, the results of this test showed that concentration on work tasks was generally the lowest for respondents who experienced high distractions and low flexibility. Nevertheless, the differences between these respondents and respondents who also experienced high distractions but scored high on flexibility can be considered as being not significant and therefore neglectable for home day 2 and for both office days. However, for home day 1 the third and fourth hypothesis was confirmed with p<.05, being the interaction effect of personal flexibility on work-related distractions and non-work-related distractions on concentration on works tasks. This indicates that when an individual is personally flexible this in turn diminishes the direct negative relationship between workrelated and non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks. Next, to see if there were indeed differences between the variables for the two home days and the two office days an explorative question was stated in this research: To what extent does the level of concentration on work tasks and the level of work-related and non-workrelated distractions vary between home days and office days? The expectation was that work-related distractions would be higher on office days compared to home days and that non-work-related distractions would be higher for home days compared to office days. Moreover, it was expected that concentration on work tasks would be more or less the same for office days and home days, because with the New Way of Working and the Twenty-FourHours-Society it has become normal to communicate, whenever, wherever and without actually meeting each other. Three different paired-samples t-tests were conducted for all days to see if these expectations were met: one considered the variable work-related distractions; another considered the variable non-work-related distractions; whereas a last paired-samples t-test considered the variable concentration on work tasks. For all three variables it seemed that the same type of days, so home days or office days, did not significantly differ from each other, so they can be considered to be the 40 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk same. Furthermore, the paired-samples t-test for both types of distractions showed that home days do significantly differ from office days, in such a way that work-related distractions are higher at office days compared to home days and non-work-related distractions are higher on home days compared to office days. The corresponding eta squared for these pairs of distractions had an overall large effect, which means that the differences for these variables between different kinds of days were large. Concentration on work tasks, on the other hand, seems to slightly differ for the pair home day 2 and office day 2, however, only a small effect size was found (eta squared =0.04). For the other three pairs, no significant effect of p<0.05 was found. Therefore, it can be said that concentration seems to be a stable factor, which does not fluctuate radically between different locations (home or the office). It also seems not to fluctuate radically within a person. This was based on the control variable general level of concentration in the regression analyses, namely, when a respondents general level of concentration was high, his or hers daily concentration seemed to be high as well (respectively ß= .256, p<.001, ß= .137, p<.05, ß= .330, p<.001, ß= .231, p<.001) This finding suggests that concentration on work tasks, when influenced by distractions, more or less can be labeled as a stable personal characteristic and should not fluctuate radically between days, locations or time. In sum it can be said that the beforehand made expectations for the three different variables between different types of locations, being the home and the office, were fulfilled. 5.2. Discussion of the findings The most striking finding in this study is the fact that concentration on work tasks appeared to be largely the same for home days as for office days. This is not in line with previous research, but for this research, which was held in a generally New Way of Working environment, it was exactly what was beforehand expected. However, it can be that this finding was influenced by the living situation of the respondents who lived alone. It is this group, from which it was expected to have the highest possibility to stay or be concentrated on work tasks in comparison to the other groups, namely the groups: living alone with children, living together or living together with children. The reason why this group could have higher concentration levels at home is because they experience no distractions of other family members who live in the same house and therefore they can plan their own working schedule, because they do not have to take into account other persons wishes and/or demands. Nevertheless, the group of respondents that lived alone only consisted of 33 respondents out of the 120 respondents. Therefore, as this group is somewhat underrepresented in this study, it can be that 41 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk the differences between home days and office days is not significant. Next, the degree of work-related distractions appears to be the highest on office days in comparison to home days, as this was tested with a paired-samples t-test. As mentioned in the theory section, research found that working from home fosters better concentration than working at the office, because less work-related interruptions should take place at home (Montreuil & Lippel, 2002; Van Sell & Jacobs, 1994). However this seems not necessarily the case when taking a look at the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analyses. Especially, when taking a closer look at home day 1, concentration on work tasks for that day is besides non-work-related distractions (ß= .509, α = 0.001) also highly effected by work-related distractions (ß= -.312, α = 0.001). Nevertheless, home day 2 shows a much lower negative effect for work-related distractions on concentration on work tasks (ß= -.137, α = 0.05). For the office days the differences between the effects of non-workrelated distractions and work-related distractions on concentration on work tasks seemed to be much lower, so more in balance, than the home days. In contrast, the degree of non-work-related distractions appears to be the highest for home days, which was also tested with a paired-samples t-test. As Sullivan & Lewis (2001) already explained, this is logical to occur, because employees working at home can give more way to nonwork-related distractions’ activities, whereas office employees generally cannot (f.e. doing the dishes, taking the children to school, taking the dog for a walk). This in turn means that working from home can be considered disadvantageous for an employee to obtain high concentration levels. When testing this hypothesis 2, it certainly showed that the relationship between non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks on home days was quite high (ß= -.509, p<.001 and .593, p<.001). However, this same relationship was also tested on office days, and showed quite high negative effects as well (ß= -.385, p<.001 and -.447, p<.001). The unexpected part was the fact that for all days, so also for the two office days, the relationship between non-work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks was higher than the relationship between work-related distractions and concentration on work tasks. This in turn can be explained by the emotional value non-workrelated distractions, or differently said private or personal distractions, have on an individual. In such times, when private distractions take place, it is hard to stay focused, because worrying about private issues will without a doubt have consequences for an employee’s concentration on work tasks. This is better known as strain-based home-work interference (HWI), which means that anxiety and fatigue caused by the home domain will make it more difficult to perform well in the work domain (Demerouti et al., 2007). 42 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk After the execution of the regression analyses only one significant interaction effect out of the eight possible ones emerged, being the one of personal flexibility and work-related distractions on concentration on work tasks for office day 2. Although this hypothesis was still rejected for office day 2, because its effect was not in the way it should have been for this research, the nonsignificant effects for the other seven hypotheses can be explained more in depth. An explanation for this remarkable result of the non-existence of interaction effects can be found in the limited distribution of the answers of the respondents on both the personal flexibility scale and the distractions scales, which makes it clear why there were no linear effects found with the regression analyses. On the flexibility scale the mean score was 2,16, where the top score was 3, suggesting that the amount of respondents in general considered themselves being flexible. For non-work-related distractions the highest mean respectively was 2,83, and the lowest mean was 2,25, whereas for work-related distractions the highest mean was 3,41 and the lowest mean had a value of 2,67. This in turn can be considered quite low, because the scale’s top score was 5. As an explanation it can be that respondents did not feel like answering the questionnaire at the end of ‘heavy days’, which possibly are the days they felt they were highly distracted. In conclusion this indicates why with linear multiple regression analysis no effects were found on high work-related and high non-workrelated distractions and low personal flexibility. A solution to find more significant effects was an additional execution of ANOVA for all days separately. Although with these univariate analyses of variance tests, two significant interaction effects were found, there still were five non-significant differences found between the two relevant groups of this study. A presumably explanation for these non-significance differences can be the fact of the limited sample size of this study. As Keith (2006) explains: to have a power of .80, you will at least need 800 respondents to find a sequential addition of an interaction effect to the regression. This in turn is quite a large difference with the 102 till 120 respondents which participated in this study. 5.3. Limitations and implications for further research A general point of discussion concerning this research is the size of the sample. Although this sample size was acceptable to find direct effects, this may not be the case for finding interaction effects. Therefore, to find out if the interaction effects of personal flexibility on work related- and non-work related distractions on concentration on work tasks is present, a research of approximately n=800 is needed in future research (Keith, 2006). Also a more balanced sample should be considered. In this research most of the respondents were highly educated and had quite high work 43 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk levels, which in turn made it difficult to state about their exact effects. Furthermore, according to Ohly, Sonnentag, Niessen and Zapf (2010), a diary study should at least consist of five days to decrease bias and increase generalizability. Unfortunately, in this study the third home day was not properly answered by enough respondents (n=77), when handling the rule of n=100. Because of this, the third office day (n=102) have not been taking into account either. It is therefore, further research should keep in mind the arduousness of finding enough respondents who are willing to participate in such a demanding study. Also the fact that for this study no data was collected about the type of industry, the types of jobs of the respondents and the kind of tasks these respondents had to perform, reduces the strength of this study. New research about the New Way of Working should mention these control variables, so more detailed insights can be given about New Way of Working employees. For this study, just like in other research, the risk of social desirability was inevitable. Respondents might not have been completely honest when answering the questions in the booklet. However, this study emphasized the anonymity of the respondents and the confidential treatment of the data in both the diary and the general questionnaire. Also the perception of distractions may have been different between persons. Although beforehand a short introduction was given in the diary of what distractions are, this still might have been interpreted differently between persons. Further research should therefore map this and also the perceptions of the differences between positively experienced- and negatively experienced distractions in more detail. Because of the fact that for employees working at home the amount of non-work-related distractions was much higher than for employees working at the office, more insight is needed in what kind of non-work-related distractions for both the home and office days seem to matter. Qualitative research in this case is needed. Also in this light, the limitation of a proper distractions scale has to be mentioned. For this study a self-designed scale was used to map work-and non-work related distractions. As with every self-designed scale, limitations can occur. For instance, the scale used in this research only consisted of two items for work-related distractions and two items for non-work-related distractions. Further research should therefore develop more items per scale, which in turn will make these scales stronger. Another scale-issue relates to the scale of personal flexibility. The scale of rigidity, which was constructed by Barelds et al. (2007), was used as a starting point for the personal flexibility scale in this research. However, just like the original scale the personal flexibility scale used a three-point answering category, with ‘2’ being ‘don’t know’. As Furr (2011) explains, researchers should avoid 44 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk using this category as a midway between two poles of a dimension, because such an answering category can reflect two different outcomes, namely: a lack of knowledge or a lack of opinion, which are two completely different things. Moreover, future research about this study’s topic should consider using multilevel analysis. Especially because of the fact that more and more employees are starting to work in organizations that apply New Way of Working practices. In this case it would be possible to use this within-person design to give more insight in the way hours at home are used for work and in that way deepen the insight about the often used dichotomous variable working from home. 5.4. Practical implications The New Way of Working, the integration of home and office, flexibility, multitasking, the feeling of being available twenty-four-seven hours a day, seven days a week; just a few concepts that are hot topics in today’s society. However, as also became clear by this study, these topics have some efficiency problems for their employees, which seem to be irrelevant or less of a priority to organizations (http://www.gezondweb.be). For example, with The New Way of Working, or other flexible arrangements, open-office layouts are a must for a lot of organizations. Nevertheless, with these layouts distractions are more likely to happen and more difficult to counteract. Therefore, when considering the nowadays frequently introduced open-office layouts, trade-offs should be made by organizations if this indeed is as efficient as it thought to be. From the results of this study, which generally was set out in a New Way of Working setting, it may well be that such open office layouts hindered concentration, which in turn can hinder performance (Demerouti et al., 2007; Lieberman et al., 2006; Van der Linden et al., 2005). In such settings, distractions, of work-related nature, could occur more frequently than in the case of separate office rooms. Therefore, it is recommended to introduce concentration rooms next to the open-office layouts, in which employees can focus on important work-related aspects. For employees working at home, same kind of arrangements should be considered, because overall, the non-work-related distractions experienced at home seem to have a quite negative effect on concentration. Therefore, a separate working room at home seems to be a wise decision as well. In general, when it may be the case that an employee wants to welcome distractions, he or she can just leave or open the room in both the home- as the office environment. Furthermore, in many job advertisements nowadays organizations ask for employees which have to be highly flexible, because such organizations want their employees to adapt quickly to 45 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk changes when they emerge (Griffin & Hesketh, 2005; Pulakos, Arad, Donovan & Plamondon, 2000). As individual adaptability is considered to be one of the factors that explain organizational success (Le Pine, et al., 2000), organizations should focus more on the importance of this personal characteristic as well. For example, when recruiting employees, a personality questionnaire can be taken to see how rigid, or the opposite, how flexible an applicant is (Barelds, et al., 2007). Or, an assessment test can be done to see how well an applicant deals with changing tasks. Subsequently, these results can be included in the application process to find the best match for an organization. In conclusion, it seems that with the integration of the home and the office as the work domain, distractions are more difficult to counteract. However, as being flexible and being concentrated seems to be crucial for the survival of organizations and individuals, it is of high necessity to counter less important distractions by leaving crowded and noisy rooms to focus on tasks which requires high levels of concentration. As became clear by the theoretical framework of this study, such tasks appear to be highly relevant to a New Way of Working employee. 46 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk References Ashforth, B.E., Kreiner, G.E., & Fugate, M. (2000). All in a day's work: Boundaries and micro role transitions at work. Academy of Management Review, 23, 472-491. Baane, R., Houtkamp, P., & Knotter, M. (2010). Het nieuwe werken ontrafeld: Over bricks, Bytes & Behavior. Assen: Koninklijke Van Gorcum BV. Barelds, D.P.H., Luteijn, F., Dijk, van H., & Starren, H. (2007). NPV-2: Nederlandse Persoonlijkheidsvragenlijst-2. Amsterdam: Harcourt Assessment B.V. Beard, K.S. & Hoy, W.K. (2010). The nature, meaning, and measure of teacher flow in elementary schools: A test of rival hypotheses. Educational Administration Quarterly, 46 (3), 426-458. Bijl, D. (2009). Aan de slag met Het Nieuwe Werken. Zeewolde: Par CC. Breuer, N. L. (1995). Minimize distractions for maximum output. Personnel Journal, 74 (5), 70-76. Cardenas, R., Major, D., & Bernas, K. (2004). Exploring work and family distractions: antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Stress Management, 11, 346-365. Carsky, M.L., Dolan, E.M., & Free, R.K. (1991). An integrated model of homebased work effects on family quality of life. Journal of Business Research, 23, 37-49. Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (CBS) (2005). De digitale economie 2004. Voorburg/ Heerlen: Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Demerouti, E., Taris, T.W., Bakker, A.B. (2007). Need for recovery, home-work interference and performance: Is lack of concentration the link? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 71, 204-220. Furr, R.M. (2011). Scale Construction and Psychometrics: For Social and Personality Psychology. London: SAGE Publications Ltd. Gaillard, A. (2003). Stress, productiviteit en gezondheid. Amsterdam: Uitgeverij Nieuwezijds. Gezondweb. Retrieved August 22, 2011, from http://www.gezondweb.be/gezondweb/rubrieken/ ziekteaandoeningen/zenuwstelsel/concentreren/wat-is-concentratie.htm Grady, C.L., Springer, M.V., Hongwanishkul, D., McIntosh, A.R., & Winocur, G. (2006). Agerelated changes in brain activity across the adult lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(2), 227–241. Griffin, B., & Hesketh, B. (2005). Are conscientious workers adaptable? Australian Journal of Management, 30(2), 245-259. Gupta, Y.P., & Goyal, S. (1989). Flexibility of manufacturing systems: concepts and measurements. 47 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk European Journal of Operational Research 43, 119–135. Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 17–35. Jett, Q. R., & George, J.M. (2003). Work interrupted: A closer look at the role of interruptions in organizational life. Academy of Management Review, 28(3), 494-507. Jurofoon. Retrieved March 1, 2011, from http://www.fmm.nl/nieuws/nieuws/hard-pratendecollega-s-zorgen-voor.73828.lynkx Karuppan, C.M. (2004). Strategies to foster labor flexibility. International Journal of Productivity and Performance Management, 53(5 & 6), 532-547. Keith, T.Z. (2006). Multiple Regression and Beyond. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Lamond, D., Daniels, K., & Standen, P. (2003). Teleworking and Virtual Organisations: The Human Impact. In: Holman, D., Wall, T.D., Clegg, C.W., Sparrow, P. & Howard, A. (2003). The New Workplace, A guide to the human impact of modern working practices. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. Le Pine, J.A., Colquitt, J.A., & Erez, A. (2000). Adaptability to changing task contexts: Effects of general cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and openness to experience. Personnel Psychology, 53, 563-593. Lieberman, H. R., Niro, P., Tharion, W. J., Nindl, B. C., Castellani, J. W., & Montain, S. J. (2006). Cognition during sustained operations: Comparison of a laboratory simulation to field studies. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 77, 929–935. Meyers, L.S., Gamst, G., Guarino, A.J. (2006). Applied Multivariate Rsearch: Interpretation and Design. Newbury Park, California: Sage Publications Inc. Montreuil, S., & Lippel, K. (2002). Telework and occupational health: a Quebec empirical study and regulatory implications. Safety Science, 41, 339–358. Nippert-Eng, C.E. (1996). Home and work: Negotiating boundaries through everyday life. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. O’Connell, D.J., McNeely, E., & Hall, D.T. (2008). Unpacking personal adaptability at work. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 14(3), 248-259. Ohly, S., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., & Zapf, D. (2010). Diary Studies in Organizational Research: An Introduction and Some Practical Recommendations. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9 (2), 7993. 48 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS version 12. Berkshire, UK: Open University Press. Perlow, L.A. (1999). The Time Famine: Toward a Sociology of Work Time. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(1), 57-81. Peters, P., Dulk, den L., Lippe, van der T. (2009). The effects of time-spatial flexibility and new working conditions on employees’ work-life balance: the Dutch case. Community, Work and Family, 12(3), 279-297. Peters, P. & Wildenbeest, M. (2010). Telewerken als hulpbron? ‘Flow’ en uitputting onder twee telewerkcategorieën vergeleken. Gedrag en Organisatie, 23(2), 97-117. Pulakos, E.D., Arad, S., Donovan, M.A. & Plamondon, K.E. (2000). Adaptability in the workplace: Development of a taxonomy of adaptive performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(4), 612–624. Quinn, R.W. (2005). Flow in knowledge work: High performance experience in the design of national security technology. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 610-641. Sullivan, C., & Lewis, S. (2001). Home-based telework, gender, and the synchronization of work and family: Perspectives of teleworkers and their co-residents. Gender, Work and Organization, 8(2), 123-145. Tijdens, K.G., Wetzels, C.M.M.P., & van Klaveren, M. (2000). Wie kan en wil telewerken: Een onderzoek naar de factoren die de mogelijkheid tot en de behoefte aan telewerken van medewerkers beïnvloeden. Amsterdam: AIAS. Van der Linden, D., Keijsers, G. P. J., Eling, P., & Van Schaijk, R. (2005). Work stress and attentional difficulties: An initial study on burnout and cognitive failures. Work & Stress, 19, 23–36. Van Sell, M., & Jacobs, S.M. (1994). Telecommuting and quality of life: A review of the literature and a model for research. Telematics and Informatics, 11(2), 81-95. Vittersø, J., Akselsen, S., Evjemo, B., Julsrud, T.E., Yttri, B., & Bergvik, S. (2003). Impacts of homebased telework on quality of life for employees and their partners. Quantitative and qualitative results from a European survey. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 201–233. Voeten, M.J.M., & Bercken, van den, J.H.L. (2004). Regressieanalyse met SPSS: Een handleiding voor lineaire regressieanalyse met SPSS. Nijmegen: Radboud Universiteit. 49 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Zijlstra, F.R.H., Roe, R.A., Leonarda, A.B., Krediet, I. (1999). Temporal factors in mental work: Effects of interrupted activities. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 72, 163 -185. 50 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Appendix I. General questionnaire and daily diary Onderzoek naar de effecten van flexibel werken op het welzijn van werknemers In opdracht van: Universiteit van Tilburg Faculteit Sociale Wetenschappen Inhoud boekje: 1 algemene vragenlijst 6 dagelijkse korte vragenlijsten Onderzoek door: Irene Molendijk (uw contactpersoon) E-mail: [email protected] Tel.: 06-15411155 51 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Algemene informatie Allereerst hartelijk dank voor uw medewerking aan dit afstudeeronderzoek van vijf Master studenten Human Resource Studies aan de Universiteit van Tilburg. Het onderzoek gaat over uw beleving van het werk en het effect daarvan op uw welbevinden. Instructie Op de volgende pagina start de algemene vragenlijst. Het invullen van deze vragenlijst zal ongeveer 15 minuten in beslag nemen. Wij willen u vragen om deze op een afzonderlijke dag in te vullen alvorens aan de zes andere vragenlijsten te beginnen. Beantwoord de vragen vlot en geef het antwoord dat als eerste in u opkomt. Lees ook steeds goed de betekenis van de antwoordcategorieën. Wij zijn geïnteresseerd in uw eigen mening, er zijn dus geen goede of foute antwoorden. Naast het beantwoorden van de algemene vragenlijst, verzoeken wij u om de zes dagelijkse vragenlijsten in te vullen. Per dag zal dit vijf minuten van uw tijd in beslag nemen. De instructie hiervoor treft u op de pagina na de algemene vragenlijst. Vertrouwelijk We willen benadrukken dat uw antwoorden strikt vertrouwelijk worden behandeld, niemand binnen uw organisatie krijgt uw antwoorden te zien. Contact informatie Mocht u vragen hebben, neemt u dan gerust contact op met uw contactpersoon. Deze staat vermeld op de voorzijde van dit boekje. Retourneren Nadat u zowel de algemene vragenlijst als de zes korte vragenlijsten heeft ingevuld, verzoeken wij u om dit boekje vóór 8 mei te retourneren in bijgevoegde envelop. Controleert u alstublieft goed of u alle zeven vragenlijsten volledig heeft ingevuld. 52 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Algemene vragenlijst 1. Wat is uw geslacht? Man / Vrouw 2. Wat is uw leeftijd? ........... 3. Kruis aan wat uw hoogst genoten opleidingsniveau is. o Basisonderwijs o VMBO/MAVO o HAVO/VWO o Middelbaar beroepsonderwijs o Hoger beroepsonderwijs o Wetenschappelijk onderwijs 4. Kruis aan welke thuissituatie voor u momenteel van toepassing is. o Alleenstaand o Alleenstaand met kind(eren) o Samenwonend met partner o Samenwonend met partner en kind(eren) 5. Kruis aan in welke positie u momenteel werkt. o Uitvoerend o Management o Directie 6. Hoeveel uur per week werkt u volgens uw contract? Vul s.v.p. het aantal uur per week in (bijv. 24 uur of 40 uur): ................. uur 53 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Concentratie tijdens werktaken 1= helemaal mee oneens 2= mee oneens 3= een beetje mee oneens 4= neutraal 5= een beetje mee eens 6= mee eens 7= helemaal mee eens 60. Over het algemeen is mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig ben. 1 2 61. Over het algemeen heb ik totale concentratie. 1 2 62. Over het algemeen ben ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig ben. 1 2 63. Over het algemeen dwalen mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens een taak. 1 2 Flexibiliteit 3 4 5 6 7 3 4 5 6 7 3 4 5 6 7 3 4 5 6 7 1= onjuist 2= weet niet 74. Een regelmatig levenspatroon bevalt me het best. 3= juist 1 2 3 75. Ik begin pas ergens aan als ik weet hoe het zal aflopen. 1 2 3 76. Ook bij onbelangrijke dingen moet ik eerst nadenken voor ik wat doe. 1 2 3 77. Als ik uit mijn dagelijkse regelmaat word gehaald hindert mij dat. 1 2 3 78. Ik houd er niet van een taak waar ik mee bezig ben te onderbreken. 1 2 3 79. Ik moet lang van tevoren weten waar ik aan toe ben. 1 2 3 80. Het kost me moeite om van een plan af te wijken. 1 2 3 81. Ik doe dingen vaak in een vaste volgorde. 1 2 3 54 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Instructie dagelijkse vragenlijsten U heeft op een voorgaande dag de algemene vragenlijst ingevuld. Dit boekje bevat verder zes korte vragenlijsten welke bedoeld zijn om in te vullen op zes aparte werkdagen. De zes vragenlijsten zijn identiek aan elkaar. Zes dagen Wij verzoeken u om zes dagen te selecteren, waarvan: - Drie dagen waarop u meer dan de helft van uw werktijd thuis heeft gewerkt. Drie dagen waarop u meer dan de helft van uw werktijd op kantoor heeft gewerkt. N.B. De dagen waarvoor u een lijst invult hoeven niet opeenvolgende werkdagen te zijn. Instructie Het is de bedoeling dat u aan het eind van elk van deze zes dagen één vragenlijst invult, circa een uur voordat u naar bed gaat. Beantwoord de vragen vlot en geef het antwoord dat als eerste in u opkomt. Lees ook steeds goed de betekenis van de antwoordcategorieën. Wij zijn geïnteresseerd in uw eigen mening, er zijn dus geen goede of foute antwoorden. Vertrouwelijk We willen nogmaals benadrukken dat uw antwoorden strikt vertrouwelijk worden behandeld, niemand binnen uw organisatie krijgt uw antwoorden te zien. 55 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Dagelijkse vragenlijst 1. Datum van vandaag: ………… - ………… - 2011 2. Vandaag heb ik meer dan de helft van mijn werktijd thuis / op kantoor gewerkt. (omcirkel wat van toepassing is) 3. Hoeveel uur heeft u vandaag thuis gewerkt? ………….. uur 4. Hoeveel uur heeft u vandaag op kantoor gewerkt? ………….. uur Van tijd tot tijd wordt iedereen wel eens afgeleid van zijn/haar werk, bijvoorbeeld door pratende collega’s, huishoudelijke verplichtingen, het ophalen van uw kinderen van school of een telefoontje van een collega, uw partner of uw kind. Ook kunt u worden afgeleid door gedachten, bijvoorbeeld aan uw kinderen, aan een ruzie met uw partner, of aan vakantieplannen. Ook kunnen uw gedachten afdwalen naar werkgerelateerde zaken, anders dan het werk waar u op dat moment mee bezig bent. Hieronder vindt u een aantal vragen over de mate waarin u vandaag werd afgeleid tijdens uw werk. Geef telkens aan wat voor u van toepassing is door het juiste getal te omcirkelen. Afleiding tijdens het werk 1=nooit 2= bijna nooit 3= neutraal 4= regelmatig 7. In hoeverre werd u vandaag tijdens u werk afgeleid? 5= vaak 1 2 3 4 5 Indien u vandaag thuis heeft gewerkt, kunt u dan aangeven in hoeverre u vandaag tijdens uw werk thuis werd afgeleid door de volgende zaken: 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 56 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 2 3 4 5 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken 1 2 3 4 5 Indien u vandaag op kantoor heeft gewerkt, kunt u dan aangeven in hoeverre u vandaag tijdens uw werk op kantoor werd afgeleid door de volgende zaken: 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 2 3 4 5 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 2 3 4 5 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 2 3 4 5 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken Concentratie tijdens werktaken 1= helemaal mee oneens 2= mee oneens 5= een beetje mee eens 6= mee eens 1 2 3 4 5 3= een beetje mee oneens 7= helemaal mee eens eens 31. Vandaag was mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig was. 32. Vandaag had ik totale concentratie. 4= neutraal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 33. Vandaag was ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig was. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 34. Vandaag dwaalden mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens de taken. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Bedankt voor het invullen van deze vragenlijst Hartelijk bedankt voor uw deelname aan dit onderzoek. Wij verzoeken u om dit boekje in bijgevoegde envelop te retourneren. 57 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Appendix II. Factor analysis Table 1 Component matrix Concentration on work tasks Home Day 1 Item Nr. Item Component 1 31 Vandaag was mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig was. ,952 32 Vandaag had ik totale concentratie. ,942 33 Vandaag was ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig was. ,918 34 Vandaag dwaalden mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens de taken. ,756 Extraction Method: Principal Component analysis. a. 1 components extracted. Table 2 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Concentration on work tasks Home Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 400,363 df 6 Sig. ,824 ,000 Table 3 Component matrix Concentration on work tasks Home Day 2 Item Nr. Item Component 1 31 Vandaag was mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig was. ,943 32 Vandaag had ik totale concentratie. ,933 33 Vandaag was ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig was. ,913 34 Vandaag dwaalden mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens de taken. ,746 58 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Extraction Method: Principal Component analysis. a. 1 components extracted. Table 4 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Concentration on work tasks Home Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 324,123 df 6 Sig. ,815 ,000 Table 5 Component matrix Concentration on work tasks Office Day 1 Item Nr. Item Component 1 31 Vandaag was mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig was. .942 32 Vandaag had ik totale concentratie. .938 33 Vandaag was ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig was. .943 34 Vandaag dwaalden mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens de taken. .708 Extraction Method: Principal Component analysis. a. 1 components extracted. Table 6 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Concentration on work tasks Office Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 409,426 df 6 Sig. ,815 ,000 59 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 7 Component matrix Concentration on work tasks Office Day 2 Item Nr. Item Component 1 31 Vandaag was mijn aandacht volledig gericht op waar ik mee bezig was. ,946 32 Vandaag had ik totale concentratie. ,932 33 Vandaag was ik volledig gericht op de taak waar ik mee bezig was. ,923 34 Vandaag dwaalden mijn gedachten af naar andere dingen tijdens de taken. ,696 Extraction Method: Principal Component analysis. a. 1 components extracted. Table 8 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Concentration on work tasks Office Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,816 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 358,704 df 6 Sig. ,000 Table 9 Oblimin Pattern Matrix for Distractions Home Day 1 Factor Item 7. In hoeverre werd u vandaag tijdens uw werk afgeleid? 1 2 ,596 ,448 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,828 ,942 60 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,902 ,970 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 5 iterations. Table 10 Factor correlation matrix for Distractions Home Day 1 Factor 1 2 1 1,000 ,411 2 ,411 1,000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. Table 11 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Distractions Home Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,752 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 271,827 df 10 Sig. ,000 Table 12 Oblimin Pattern Matrix for Distractions Home Day 2 Factor Item 7. In hoeverre werd u vandaag tijdens uw werk afgeleid? 1 2 ,633 ,391 61 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,787 ,933 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,952 ,900 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 6 iterations. Table 13 Factor correlation matrix for Distractions Home Day 2 Factor 1 2 1 1,000 ,367 2 ,367 1,000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. Table 14 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Distractions Home Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 195,036 df 10 Sig. ,751 ,000 62 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 15 Varimax Rotated Component Matrix for Distractions Office Day 1 Factor Item 1 2 7. In hoeverre werd u vandaag tijdens uw werk afgeleid? ,824 ,448 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,866 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,850 ,882 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,888 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 3 iterations. Table 16 Factor correlation matrix for Distractions Office Day 1 Factor 1 2 1 1,000 ,287 2 ,287 1,000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. Table 17 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Distractions Office Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,710 63 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 184,552 df 10 Sig. ,000 Table 18 Oblimin Pattern Matrix for Distractions Office Day 2 Factor Item 1 7. In hoeverre werd u vandaag tijdens uw werk afgeleid? ,898 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,855 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 2 ,893 ,909 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,919 Extraction Method: Principal Component Factoring. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 4 iterations. Table 19 Factor correlation matrix for Distractions Office Day 2 Factor 1 2 1 1,000 ,497 2 ,497 1,000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Factoring. Rotation Method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. 64 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 20 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Distractions Office Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,779 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 245,415 df 10 Sig. ,000 Table 21 Correlation Matrix Work-related Distractions Home Day 1 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 ,515 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,515 1 Table 22 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Work-related Distractions Home Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 34,672 df 1 Sig. ,500 ,000 65 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 23 Correlation Matrix Work-related Distractions Home Day 2 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 8. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 ,584 10. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,584 1 Table 24 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Work-related Distractions Home Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,500 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 41,539 df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 25 Correlation Matrix Work-related Distractions Office Day 1 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,660 66 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,660 1 Table 26 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Work-related Distractions Office Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,500 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 64,846 df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 27 Correlation Matrix Work-related Distractions Office Day 2 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 12. Gedachten gerelateerd aan andere werkzaken dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. 1 ,685 14. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan werk, anders dan de taken waarmee u op dat moment bezig was. ,685 1 Table 28 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Work-related Distractions Office Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square ,500 68,735 67 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 29 Correlation Matrix Non-Work-related Distractions Home Day 1 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 ,808 ,808 1 Table 30 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Non-Work-related Distractions Home Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,500 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 117,758 df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 31 Correlation Matrix Non-Work-related Distractions Home Day 2 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 9. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 11. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 ,686 ,686 1 Table 32 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Non-Work-related Distractions Home Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling ,500 68 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 62,599 df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 33 Correlation Matrix Non-Work-related Distractions Office Day 1 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 ,550 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,550 1 Table 34 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Non-Work-related Distractions Office Day 1 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy ,500 Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 40,845 df 1 Sig. ,000 Table 35 Correlation Matrix Non-Work-related Distractions Office Day 2 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 13. Gedachten gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. 1 ,644 15. Bezigheden gerelateerd aan privé-zaken. ,644 1 69 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 36 KMO and Bartlett’s Test for Non-Work-related Distractions Office Day 2 Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 58,035 df 1 Sig. ,500 ,000 Table 37 Component matrix Personal flexibility Item Nr. Item Component 1 74 Een regelmatig levenspatroon bevalt me het best. ,618 76 Ook bij onbelangrijke dingen moet ik eerst nadenken voor ik wat doe. ,596 77 Als ik uit mijn dagelijkse regelmaat word gehaald hindert mij dat. ,781 78 Ik houd er niet van een taak waar ik mee bezig ben te onderbreken. ,615 80 Het kost me moeite om van een plan af te wijken. ,726 81 Ik doe dingen vaak in een vaste volgorde. ,773 Extraction Method: Principal Component analysis. a. 1 components extracted. 70 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 38 KMO and Bartlett’s Test Personal flexibility Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 168,081 df 15 Sig. ,809 ,000 71 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Appendix III. Exclusion of control variable Age Table 39 Regression analysis Home day 1 with control variable Age Concentration on Work Tasks Age Model 1 Model 2 .102 .073 Flexibility -.043 Work Distractions -.311*** Non Work Distractions -.553*** R² .010 R² Change .525 .515*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). Table 40 Regression analysis Home day 2 with control variable Age Concentration on Work Tasks Age Model 1 Model 2 .041 .055 Flexibility -.006 Work Distractions -.173* Non Work Distractions -.598*** R² R² Change .002 .461 .460*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 72 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 41 Regression analysis Office day 1 with control variable Age Concentration on Work Tasks Age Model 1 Model 2 .177* .117 Flexibility -.034 Work Distractions -.270*** Non Work Distractions -.456*** R² .031 R² Change .362 .331*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). Table 42 Regression analysis Office day 2 with control variable Age Concentration on Work Tasks Age Model 1 Model 2 .113 .100 Flexibility -.032 Work Distractions -.311*** Non Work Distractions -.486*** R² R² Change .013 .475 .462*** *** p < .001 (2-tailed), ** p < .01 (2-tailed), * p < .05 (2-tailed). 73 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Appendix IV. Results of two-way ANOVA tests for each day separately. Table 43.1 Univariate analysis of variance Home Day 1 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 22,15 4,00 13 Low-High 20,84 3,96 44 High-Low 15,17 5,92 23 High-High 17,74 4,91 34 Sig .050 Table 43.2 Univariate analysis of variance Home Day 1 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (non-work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 23,69 2,53 13 Low-High 21,06 3,78 48 High-Low 14,36 5,15 22 High-High 16,97 4,83 30 Sig .004 74 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 44.1 Univariate analysis of variance Home Day 2 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 19,68 5,09 22 Low-High 21,47 4,79 34 High-Low 15,36 5,39 14 High-High 17,22 5,12 32 Sig .973 Table 44.2 Univariate analysis of variance Home Day 2 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (non-work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 22,00 3,16 14 Low-High 21,59 4,49 37 High-Low 15,24 5,31 21 High-High 16,62 5,14 29 Sig .374 75 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 45.1 Univariate analysis of variance Office Day 1 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 22,38 4,10 8 Low-High 20,60 4,10 20 High-Low 17,20 4,82 30 High-High 17,47 5,48 58 Sig .394 Table 45.2 Univariate analysis of variance Office Day 1 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (non-work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 20,93 5,50 14 Low-High 19,89 4,87 53 High-Low 16,75 4,23 24 High-High 14,84 4,62 25 Sig .662 76 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 46.1 Univariate analysis of variance Office Day 2 Dependent variable: Concentration on work tasks Group (non-work distractions - flexibility) Mean Std. Deviation N Low-Low 19,82 3,30 17 Low-High 19,91 4,37 58 High-Low 13,45 4,81 20 High-High 14,63 4,79 16 Sig .569 77 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Appendix V. (Non-) significant results of the independent-samples t-test for equality of means Table 47.1 T-test for equality of means Home day 1(high work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Home Day 1 Equal variances assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) .317 -1.777 55 ,081 -1.714 41,295 ,094 Equal variances not assumed Note. p<.05, one-tailed Table 47.2 T-test for equality of means Home day 1(high non-work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Home Day 1 Equal variances assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) .727 -1.867 50 ,068 -1.848 43,677 ,071 Equal variances not assumed Note. p<.05, one-tailed 78 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 48.1 T-test for equality of means Home Day 2 (high work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Home Day 2 Equal variances assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) ,780 -1.118 44 ,270 -1,095 23,736 ,285 Equal variances not assumed Table 48.2 T-test for equality of means Home Day 2(high non-work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Home Day 2 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) ,971 -,926 48 ,359 -,921 42,390 ,362 79 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 49.1 T-test for equality of means Office Day 1(high work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Office Day 1 Equal variances assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) ,283 ,152 94 ,880 ,156 73,792 ,876 Equal variances not assumed Table 49.2 T-test for equality of means Office Day 1(high non-work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Office Day 1 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) ,496 1,506 47 ,139 1,509 46,899 ,138 80 Master Thesis Human Resource Studies | Irene Molendijk Table 50.1 T-test for equality of means Office Day 2(high non-work-related distractions – personal flexibility) Concentration Office Day 2 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assumed Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) ,662 -,730 34 ,470 -,730 32,343 ,470 81