Département de la détention et la protection des animaux
Transcription
Département de la détention et la protection des animaux
Département de la détention et la protection des animaux de la Faculté Vetsuisse Université de Berne (Directeur: Prof. Dr. A. Steiger) Travail fait sous la surveillance du Dr. Sabine G. Gebhardt-Henrich Reliability and validity of a test of character in Hovawart dogs Thèse inaugurale pour l'obtention du titre de Docteur de la Faculté Vetsuisse Université de Berne présentée par Caroline Paroz de Saicourt, BE 2007 1 Travail accepté comme thèse par la Faculté Vetsuisse Université de Berne, sur proposition du Prof. Dr. M. Vandevelde Berne, le Le Doyen de la Faculté Vetsuisse Université de Berne 2 Index 1 Abstract, Zusammenfassung, Résumé ............................................................................. 5 1.1 Abstract .................................................................................................................. 5 1.2 Zusammenfassung ................................................................................................. 5 1.3 Résumé .................................................................................................................. 6 2 Reliability and validity of a test of character in Hovawart dogs ......................................... 8 Abstract ............................................................................................................................ 8 2.1 Introduction ................................................................................................................ 8 2.2 Materials and methods ............................................................................................. 10 2.2.1 Test of Character (TC) ....................................................................................... 10 2.2.2 Subjects ............................................................................................................. 13 2.2.3 Questionnaire .................................................................................................... 14 2.2.4 Reliability ........................................................................................................... 14 2.2.5 Statistical analyses ............................................................................................ 14 2.3 Results ..................................................................................................................... 15 2.3.1 Agreement between judges ............................................................................... 15 2.3.2 Comparison between problematic and control dogs .......................................... 18 2.4 Discussion ................................................................................................................ 21 2.4.1 Agreement between judges ............................................................................... 21 2.4.2 Comparison between problematic and control dogs .......................................... 23 Acknowledgments .......................................................................................................... 23 References ..................................................................................................................... 25 3 Appendix ......................................................................................................................... 27 3.1 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC of juvenile dogs . ........................... 28 3.2 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC for the breeding approval ............. 29 3.3 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in French .................................................... 41 3 3.4 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in German .................................................. 56 3.5 Appendix: Pictures during the TC in spring ........................................................... 72 4 1 Abstract, Zusammenfassung, Résumé 1.1 Abstract The reliability of 6 judges who assessed the behaviour of dogs simultaneously in the test of character (TC) of the Swiss Hovawart Club (SHC) was examined in spring, summer and autumn 2006. The reliabilities were calculated using the Spearman-Brown prediction formula and Spearman rank correlations for quantitative traits and the kappa coefficient for binomial variables. Secondly, in a retrospective study the validity of the TC was studied by comparing the results of TC of dogs which (later) showed behaviour problems related to aggression with the results of behaviourally inconspicuous dogs. Problematic dogs were reported to the club because they had to be euthanised or rehomed with different owners because of their behaviour problems. The behaviour problems were quantified by using a questionnaire for the owner who had reported the problems. Generally, the reliability between the 6 judges was high, but there were differences in the degree of agreement on the three different dates. The result of the retrospective study indicated that the behaviourally inconspicuous dogs had generally better scored in the TC than problematic dogs. When dogs with a problem of aggressiveness and their matched control dogs were compared, there were significant differences in the trait "playing" in the questionnaire and the TC. Dogs who later displayed aggressive behaviour played less enthusiastically than dogs who remained non-aggressive. However the other traits which were assessed in the TC were not useful to predict which dogs will develop problems of aggressiveness later. 1.2 Zusammenfassung Erstens prüften wir die Zuverlässigkeit von 6 Richtern, die gleichzeitig das Verhalten von Hunden an Wesensprüfungen (WP) (Junghundebegutachtung und Körung) des schweizerischen Hovawart Klubs (SHC) im Frühling, Sommer und Herbst 2006 einschätzten. Zweitens studierten wir in einer retrospektiven Studie die Validität der Junghunde-Begutachtung durch Vergleich der Resultate von Wesenprüfungen von Hunden, welche (später) Aggressivität zeigten, mit den Resultaten von Hunden, die nie Verhaltensprobleme zeigten. 47 Hunde, die während des Jahres 2006 entweder an Junghunde-Begutachtungen oder an Körungen getestet wurden, haben am ersten Teil dieser Studie teilgenommen. Jeder Hund wurde gleichzeitig von 6 Richtern beobachtet und nach jeder Prüfung mussten die 6 Richter allein das Protokoll der Prüfung auszufüllen. Nachher haben sie die Resultate besprochen und das offizielle Protokoll der Prüfung zusammen ausgefüllt. Für den zweiten Teil wurde 15 Hunde genommen, die laut Klub wegen Aggressionsverhalten euthanasiert oder umplatziert werden mussten. Wir verglichen diese Hunde mit 15 Hunden, die keine Verhaltensprobleme hatten, um eine Kontrollgruppe zu formen. Für jeden Hund mit einem Problem fanden wir einen Kontrollhund desselben Alters, desselben Geschlechtes, und welcher die WP am selben Tag machte. Die Verhaltensprobleme wurden durch Verwenden eines Fragebogens für den Halter, der über die Probleme berichtet hatte, eingeschätzt. Wir konnten dann die Resultate der 5 Fragebogen mit den Resultaten der Junghunde-Begutachtung vergleichen. Die Zuverlässigkeit wurde mit der Spearman-Brown Voraussage-Formel berechnet, der Spearman Korrelationskoeffizient wurde für quantitative Merkmale benützt und der Kappa Koeffizientl für binomische Variablen. Im Allgemeinen war die Zuverlässigkeit zwischen den 6 Richtern hoch, es gab aber Unterschiede der Übereinstimmung an den drei verschiedenen Testdaten. Das Resultat der retrospektiven Studie zeigt, dass die unauffälligen Hunde allgemein bessere Noten für die Junghunde-Begutachtung hatten als die problematischen Hunde. Wenn Hunde mit Aggressivität und die Kontrollhunde in Bezug auf den Fragebogen und die JunghundeBegutachtung verglichen wurden, gab es signifikante Unterschiede im Verhalten "Spielen". Hunde, die später aggressives Verhalten zeigten, spielten weniger enthusiastisch als Hunde, die nicht aggressiv blieben. Jedoch waren die anderen Merkmale, die mit der Junghunde-Begutachtung eingeschätzt wurden, nicht brauchbar, um vorauszusagen, welche Hunde später Probleme mit Aggressivität entwickeln werden. Die Art, wie das Prüfungsprotokoll ausgefüllt wird, und die Anzahl von 6 Richtern beim SHC ist optimal. Es soll aber immer eine genügende Zahl von erfahrenen Richtern bei jeder Prüfung haben, und sie müssen die Möglichkeit haben, die Bewertungen zu besprechen. Das beeinflusst die Übereinstimmung zwischen den Richtern und die Aussagekraft der Bewertung positiv. Die Wesenprüfungen sind ein gutes Instrument, um eine erste Selektion der Hunden auszuüben; es gibt eine hohe Wahrscheinlichkeit, dass ein Hund, welcher bereits in diesem Alter Schwierigkeiten zeigt, ein problematischer Hund zu werden. Das Verhalten "Spielen" schien als einziges Verhaltensmerkmal relevant für die Risikobewertung, ob ein Hund später ein Problem mit Aggressivität zeigte. 1.3 Résumé Premièrement, nous avons examiné la fiabilité de 6 juges qui évaluaient le comportement des chiens pendant les test de caractère (TC) (le TC du jeune chien et le TC pour l'approbation à l'élevage) du club suisse du Hovawart (SHC) au printemps, été et automne 2006. Deuxièmement, nous avons étudié la validité de l'examen du jeune chien dans une étude rétrospective par la comparaison des résultats du TC des chiens qui ont montré (plus tard) de l'agressivité, avec les résultats des chiens qui n'ont jamais montré de problème de comportement. Les 47 chiens qui ont été testés pendant 2006, soit au TC du jeune chien soit au TC pour l'approbation à l'élevage, ont participé à la première partie de cette étude. Chaque chien était observé simultanément par 6 juges et après chaque examen ces 6 juges devaient remplir seul le procès-verbal de l'examen. Plus tard, ils ont discuté les résultats et ont rempli ensemble le procès-verbal de l'examen. Pour la deuxième partie, 15 chiens, qui selon le SHC ont dû être déplacés de leur famille ou euthanasiés pour cause d'agressivité, ont été retenu. Nous avons comparé ces chiens avec 15 chiens qui n'ont eu aucun problème de comportement jusqu'à ce jour, pour former un groupe de contrôle. Pour chaque chien problématique, nous avons trouvé un chien de contrôle du même âge, du même sexe, et qui a fait le TC le même jour. Les problèmes de comportement ont été évalués grâce à un questionnaire adressé au propriétaire du chien, qui a rapporté le ou les problèmes. Ensuite, nous avons pu comparer les résultats des questionnaires avec les résultats du TC du jeune chien. La fiabilité a été calculée avec la formule de prédiction Spearman-Brown, le coefficient de 6 corrélation de Spearman a été utilisé pour les caractéristiques quantitatives et la mesure de la conformité par Kendall pour les variables binômes. En général la fiabilité a été grande entre les 6 juges, mais il y a eu des différences de concordance aux trois diverses dates. Le résultat de l'étude rétrospective a montré que les chiens de contrôle ont eu en général de meilleures notes pour le TC du jeune chien que les chiens problématiques. Quand des chiens agressifs et avec leur contrôle ont été comparés par rapport au questionnaire et le TC du jeune chien, il y a eu des différences significatives pour la caractéristique "Jouer". Les chiens qui ont montré plus tard de l'agressivité, ont joué avec moins d'enthousiasme que les chiens de contrôle. Cependant les autres caractéristiques qui sont évaluées par le TC du jeune chien n'ont pas été utiles pour prédire quels chiens développeront plus tard des problèmes d'agressivité. La manière, dont le procès-verbal des TC est rempli, et la quantité de 6 juges au SHC est optimale. Néanmoins il doit toujours avoir un nombre suffisant des juges expérimentés à chaque examen, et ils doivent avoir la possibilité de discuter entre eux les évaluations. Cela influence positivement la concordance entre les juges et la signifiance de l'évaluation. Les TC sont un bon instrument pour effectuer la première sélection des chiens; il y a une forte vraisemblance qu'un chien qui montre déjà à cet âge des difficultés, devienne un chien problématique. La caractéristique "Jouer" semble être l'unique signe important pour l'évaluation du risque, si un chien montra plus tard un problème d'agressivité. 7 2 Reliability and validity of a test of character in Hovawart dogs Caroline Paroz, Sabine G. Gebhardt-Henrich, Andreas Steiger Division of Animal Housing and Welfare, Vetsuisse-Faculty of University of Berne, Bremgartenstrasse 109a, CH 3012 Berne, Switzerland (Manuscript submitted to Applied Animal Behaviour Science) Abstract The reliability of 6 judges who assessed the behaviour of dogs simultaneously in the test of character (TC) of the Swiss Hovawart Club (SHC) was examined in spring, summer and autumn 2006. The reliabilities were calculated using the Spearman-Brown prediction formula and Spearman rank correlations for quantitative traits and the kappa coefficient for binomial variables. Secondly, the validity of the TC in a retrospective study was studied by comparing the results of TC of dogs which (later) showed behaviour problems related to aggression with the results of behaviourally inconspicuous dogs. Problematic dogs were reported to the club because they had to be euthanised or re-homed with different owners because of their behaviour problems. The behaviour problems were quantified by using a questionnaire for the owner who had reported the problems. Generally, the reliability between the 6 judges was high, but there were differences of agreement on the three different dates. The results of the retrospective study indicated that the behaviourally inconspicuous dogs had generally better scored in the TC than problematic dogs. When dogs with a problem of aggressiveness and their matched control dogs were compared, there were significant differences in the trait "playing" in the questionnaire and the TC. Dogs who later displayed aggressive behaviour played less enthusiastically than dogs who remained non-aggressive. However the other traits which were assessed in the TC were not useful to predict which dogs will develop problems of aggressiveness later. Keywords Hovawart dog, test of character, behaviour, reliability, number of judges, agressiveness 2.1 Introduction The behaviour of dogs is of growing public concern. In today‘s world, dogs are expected to behave without endangering or annoying people. It is important to identify a potentially problematic dog early, possibly during a test of character (TC). Many studies have been done on this subject, but the researchers studying the character of dogs come from varied backgrounds and publish in a broad range of journals (Jones and Gosling, 2005). In their review Jones and Gosling (2005 p. 49) stressed that future research should assess the 8 reliability and validity of behavioural coding. Standardized behaviour tests for the purpose of evaluating the character or temperament of dogs in regard to people and the environment have been applied in Switzerland in various breeds for breeding purposes. Every breed club uses its own test with different request profiles, so that there are as many tests as clubs (Scherrer, 2007). Besides their use as selection criteria, TCs are also used as aptitude tests. The objectives of aptitude tests are to predict the suitability and future performance of the dogs, e.g. as police dogs (Slabbert, 1999), guide dogs for the blind (Wilsson and Sundgren, 1997a,b, 1998)), family dogs (Venzl et al., 1989), and shelter dogs (Weiss and Gary, 1997; van der Borg et al., 1991). A particularly important factor is the judge, because he/she must objectively and accurately interpret the behaviour of the dogs. The SHC is probably the only breed club in Switzerland where each dog is evaluated by 6 judges simultaneously. The judges decide which dogs will be allowed to breed; therefore their decision influences the future of the breed. A fundamental topic in psychology regarding behavioural/personality tests in particular is the extent to which two observers or judges agree with one another in their impressions of a common target (Gosling et al., 2003; Lloyd et al., 2007; Kenny, 1991). The agreement question is closely related to the accuracy question. In social perception, exact accuracy is rare, and only partial accuracy is the norm. It is possible for two judges not to agree, with both being partially accurate. Technically, agreement is neither a necessary nor a sufficient condition for accuracy; however, agreement and accuracy are linked. Six factors can determine agreement (Kenny, 1991): 1. Acquaintance: It means the sheer amount of information to which the judge is exposed. The more the six judges see of a dog‘s behaviour, the more they will agree. Judges who are still in training might not observe exactly what an experienced judge can observe and will thus differ slightly in their judgement. 2. Overlap: It is the extent of the set of target behaviour which the judges observe at the same time. It plays a pivotal role in determining the degree of agreement. The six judges are probably not always concentrated simultaneously and consequently do not share the same observations of a dog. Since they are not at the exact same place they observe the dog from different angles and distances. 3. Shared meaning system: It means to what extent an act is given the same meaning by the six judges. If six judges see a dog engaged in a behaviour they may not label that behaviour in the same way, especially when judges still in training take part in the evaluation. 4. Consistency: It measures how consistent the dog‘s behaviour is across situations, e.g. dogs might behave differently at home than during the TC. The six judges observe the dog only during the test and must describe its behaviour with the best possible reliability. 5. Extraneous information: this means to what extent the judges rate the target on the basis of extraneous information, that is information not based on the dog‘s behaviour during the test. The majority of the judges know the kennels and breeders of the examined dogs. It is difficult to disregard personal opinions and feelings, although the judges must remain as objective as possible. 6. Communication takes place when the judges share their impressions of the dogs with each other. Six judges can be in agreement because they communicate their impressions and mutually influence one another which can give us a more objective judgement. In this situation it is likely that agreement can be used to forecast accuracy (Kenny, 1991). The aim of this project was to examine the TC of the Swiss Hovawart Club (SHC). The Hovawart is a fairly large (58-70 cm) and strongly built working and companion dog. In 9 Switzerland the SHC promotes this breed solely as a companion dog. We analysed two aspects of the tests: The first part dealt with the reliability of the judges. Breeding approval is an important decision for the future of the dog and the breed and it is based on the result of the TC. If TCs should have any value they must predict every-day behaviour in the real world (Gosling and Vazire, 2002). Therefore, the second part was a retrospective study, in order to examine whether the behaviour test achieved the objectives laid down by the SHC. We compared the TCs of dogs which (later) showed behaviour problems related to aggression with behaviourally inconspicuous dogs. We wanted to see if these dogs with behaviour problems (at a later date) had been judged differently at the TC than the unproblematic dogs. If it is possible to recognize such dogs promptly and instruct the dog leader regarding potential future problems some incidences of euthanasia or re-homing might be avoided. 2.2 Materials and methods 2.2.1 Test of Character (TC) Since 1960 the Swiss Hovawart Club has applied two standardized TCs which are based on Seiferle and Leonhardt (1984). One test examines the juvenile dog between 7 and 13 months of age and the other test evaluates the aptitude for breeding from 18 months of age onwards. The TC of the juvenile dog does not influence the breeding approval. During each test six judges observe the dogs‘ behaviour in detail and describe the character of the dogs. The parts of the test proceed one after another without a pause and they increase in intensity to test the stability of the behaviour. Besides these various test elements, the performance of the dog is judged generally during the entire exam (temperament and relationship with the handler for the juvenile and the older dogs, excitability and calming down only for the older ones). The TC is divided into 5 different parts for the juvenile dogs and 10 parts for the breeding approval. 2.2.1.1 TC of juvenile dogs 2.2.1.1.1 Encounter with a single person The dog (not on the leash) and its handler go for a walk approximately 20 meters outside the area and suddenly an unknown person (a judge) behind a tree becomes visible to the dog. The judge greets the handler and the dog with physical contact. Then they go back together to the test area. With this test the judge evaluates the behaviour of the dog during contact with a stranger in an unusual environment and can thereby determine its fearlessness and self-confidence in front of human beings. 2.2.1.1.2 Play behaviour First the handler plays with the dog while holding a tug toy in his hands, inciting the dog to bite, grip and dragging it. Then the judge takes the toy and continues the animation. 2.2.1.1.3 Broad lane Dog and handler walk through a broad lane of standing people (between 6 and 15 persons including the 6 judges). The judges can stroke or touch the dog at any time while she is passing. 2.2.1.1.4 Optical and acoustical test in broad lane Several optical and acoustical objects (umbrella, cloth, cow bell, horn, cans filled with 10 stones ...) are presented to the dog in a playful manner in the broad lane. There should be no provocation or intimidation; it should be a game for the dog. These objects are then thrown on the ground, so that the dog has the opportunity to make contact with them. The desired behaviour is a solved, attentive attitude which expresses its control faced with all these phenomena and its interest in these objects. The encounter with these objects should turn into a kind of game. It is important to judge the behaviour in its entirety and not to emphasise an isolated reaction. 2.2.1.1.5 General behaviour a) Temperament (physical flexibility and intensity of reaction to different environmental stimuli): the dog should be normal to lively in temperament and interested in the environment. b) Relationship with the handler: the dog should show a good to pronounced relationship with his handler, without sticking to her. 2.2.1.2 TC for the breeding approval 2.2.1.2.1 Encounter with a single person The judge greets the handler and the dog (not on a leash) with physical contact. He/she explains the procedure of the test to the handler. 2.2.1.2.2 Broad and narrow lane Dog and handler walk through a broad lane of standing people (between 6 and 15 persons including the 6 judges). The judges can stroke and touch the dog at any time while he is passing. Then, the people move to form a narrow lane (less than a metre). The dog should pass with his handler and should not show signs of anxiety or fear. 2.2.1.2.3 Optical and acoustical test in broad lane The same test as for the juvenile dogs (see 2.2.1.1.4) 2.2.1.2.4 Holding of the dog by a stranger The judge holds the dog by the collar, while the handler moves approximately 15 meters away. The dog must keep quiet. 2.2.1.2.5 Circle a) Call into the circle: the handler goes into the middle of a circle of a maximum of 10 people, who are facing outwards. The handler calls his dog once. The dog should come to him inside the circle directly or after a detour. b) Closing of the circle: the circle is opened and the people turn inwards. The handler and the dog stand in the middle. Suddenly the 10 people run towards the dog and make the circle smaller. The dog can be frightened; she must, however, overcome this fear of the people shortly afterwards. 2.2.1.2.6 Bang effect The reaction to a bang is tested with a starting clapboard. The judge walks approximately 10 metres in front of the dog and his handler, suddenly he starts up the clapboard, without the dog seeing him, he puts the clapboard on the ground and continues to walk. The handler stops when he hears the bang, and then he walks to the site of the clapboard. The dog can be surprised by the bang but should not be afraid of the clapboard or the judge. 11 2.2.1.2.7 Play behaviour The same test as for the juvenile dogs (see 2.2.1.1.2) 2.2.1.2.8 General behaviour a) Excitability: during the whole test the dog should be able to control itself although the pressure rises. He should react neither nervously nor over sensitively. b) Calming down: during the whole test the dog should be able to control herself although the pressure rises during the test. She should be able to calm down after every test element. c) Temperament (physical flexibility and intensity of reaction to different environmental stimuli): the dog should be normal to lively in temperament and interested in the environment. d) Relationship with the handler: the dog should show a good to pronounced attachment to his handler, without sticking to him. The test is always done at the same place in Switzerland. A cord (streamer of building site) defines the sector of examination which is approximately 60 m2. During the examination the dog is free in this area; she should not cross the border and should not be at the heel of the handler. As few commands as possible should be given, to enable the dog to react independently in the various situations. Some tests can be repeated until the reaction of the dog can be clearly judged. Each dog is judged simultaneously by 6 judges and after each examination these 6 judges meet in order to discuss the results and to fill out the official protocol of the examination together (see Appendix 1). The protocol is a subjective grading recorded verbally; words and expressions which vary in intensity are used. The most experienced judge underlines the expressions of the protocol which correspond to the character of the dog by considering the comments of the 5 other judges. To be able to analyse the judgment of the various judges, we transformed the verbal grades into numerical scores (Tab. 1). Table 1: Transformation of the verbal grades into scores for several traits. Traits with the answer "yes" or "no" (presence of aggression, result: pass/fail) were not transformed. a) TC for the juvenile dog (Desired scores are shown in bold) Score self-confidence play with handler play with stranger temperament relationship with the handler 1 looking threatened, impressed no interest, does not bite lethargic is lacking, is under pressure 2 slightly impressed bites with inhibition sluggish very independent, indifferent 3 feeling insecure bites slightly playfully slightly lively loose 4 slightly feeling insecure bites playfully calm good 5 curious plays slightly enthusiastically normal pronounced 12 6 self-confident 7 plays enthusiastically lively very pronounced plays very enthusiastically very lively, great drive of exercise confined nervous dog sticks to the handler 8 b) TC for the breeding approval (Desired scores are shown in bold) Self-confidence, play with handler and stranger were coded as in juvenile dogs relationship calming Score excitability temperament with the down handler 1 Too difficult to excite rapidly calmed down sluggish is lacking, is under pressure 2 difficult to excite normal slightly lively loose, very independent 3 normal slow to calm down normal good 4 quickly excited hesitatingly lively calming down 5 too slow too quickly calming excites down 6 cannot be calmed down very lively pronounced dog sticks to the handler dog does not separate 7 The examination of the juvenile dogs gives only an indication of their behaviour, temperament and character. In this way, owners are advised about the important points on which they should concentrate while training their dog. The TC for the breeding approval ends with the result "pass" or "fail". It is possible to repeat this exam, but only in exceptional circumstances; the term ―postponed‖ may be used. The judges are not the same people for every exam, but a core of three experienced judges is desired. Three other judges are either judges in training, or people with a good knowledge of the behaviour of Hovawarts. Under two particular conditions, it is possible that the number of 6 judges is not achieved: When a judge is the owner of the examined dog or when a judge is the breeder of the examined dog he/she is excluded. 2.2.2 Subjects 2.2.2.1 First part The subjects of this study were 23 dogs of the breed Hovawart which took the TC of juvenile dog and 24 dogs which took the TC for the breeding approval in 2006. For the study the 6 judges received the protocol of judgment after every dog and they had to fill it out independently of the others. After that, they judged the dog together as they normally do. 13 2.2.2.2 Second part For the second part, we selected 15 Hovawart dogs which had to be euthanised or rehomed with a new family because of aggressiveness. We matched those with 15 dogs, which had no problem of behaviour to form a group of control. For every dog with a problem we found a control dog of the same age, the same sex and which took the TC on the same day. We collected the reports of the TC of juvenile dog for these 30 dogs. Only 6 TCs for the breeding approval could be obtained, therefore they were not analysed. 2.2.3 Questionnaire Using a questionnaire to collect the behavioural data is based on the assumption that the owner (or primary handler) of a dog usually knows more about its typical behaviour than anyone else. We used the validated questionnaire of Hsu and Serpell (2003) with the additional questions of Svartberg (2005) which is known as the University of Pennsylvania Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (PennBARQ)(Appendix 3.3 – 3.4). The items were worded to address the dog's typical responses to specific situations and events; the owners were requested to describe how their dogs responded to these specific situations. For simplicity, items were grouped into the following general categories: training and obedience, aggression, fear and anxiety, separation-related behaviour, excitability, attachment and attention-seeking behaviour, sociability, miscellaneous. For categories in which responses were most readily evaluated in terms of frequency of a particular response (i.e., sociability, trainability, separation-related behaviour, attachment and attention-seeking behaviour, and miscellaneous), owners were asked to score their dogs with 5-point frequency-scales (i.e., 0 = never, 1 = seldom, 2 = sometimes, 3 = usually, 4 = always). For categories in which responses were most readily evaluated in terms of intensity of particular behaviours (i.e., aggression, fear and anxiety, and excitability), owners were asked to score their dogs with 5-point qualitative (semantic differential) rating scales (i.e., 0 = no signs of the behaviour, 1 to 3 = mild to moderate signs of the behaviour, and 4 = severe signs of the behaviour). For these rating scales, each section of the questionnaire included a brief explanation describing the sorts of behavioural signs that respondents could use when scoring their dogs. For every item, respondents also had the option of not scoring if the dog's response to the particular situation was not known or if the item was not applicable to their dog for some reason. In the analyses, these responses were treated as missing values. 2.2.4 Reliability We used a transformed Spearman-Brown prediction formula to determine the reliability of the six judges (http://www.med-ed-online.org/rating/reliability.html, accessed 17.04.07). We corrected the obtained results for 6 judges by the formula found on http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Spermann-Brown prediction formula (Solomon, pers. comm. 21.09.06). 2.2.5 Statistical analyses SAS Version 9.1 was used for data analyses. Spearman rank correlation coefficients were used to estimate the level of agreement between judges for quantitative variables. All 15 possible correlations among the 6 judges were calculated and the median of those correlations was taken for each date. The kappa coefficient was used for qualitative variables (presence of aggression, pass/fail). 14 2.3 Results 2.3.1 Agreement between judges Only 11 dogs out of 24 (45.8%) passed the examination as to suitability for breeding. Eight dogs failed and 5 were postponed. In the breeding aptitude test, correlations between the judges were generally high and significant except for "self-confidence", "play with handler" and "calming down" at the summer test (Tab. 2). Table 2: Medians of the Spearman rank correlation coefficients between the judges for several traits on the three dates for the breeding approval (ns = non significant, * P<0.01, ** P<0.001, *** P<0.0001). Median Self -confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Calming Exciting Bond Temperament Spring 0.87*** 0.65*** 0.58*** 0.98*** 0.92*** 0.56** 0.71*** Summer 0.13 ns 0.22 ns 0.92*** 0.19 ns 0.61* 0.5*** 0.82*** Autumn 0.66*** 0.57*** 0.73*** 0.81*** 0.82*** 0.21* 0.54*** Likewise, the correlations between the individual judges and the official result were generally high (Tab. 3). Table 3: Means (± standard deviations) of the Spearman rank correlation coefficients (result individual judge correlated with the official result) for several traits on the three dates. a) juvenile dogs Trait Self-confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Temperament Relationship Spring 0.50 (± 0.25) 0.68 (±0.24) 0.58 (±0.33) 0.83 (±0.11) 0.69 (± 0.23) b) breeding approval Trait Spring Self-confidence 0.85 (± 0.14) Play with handler 0.69 (±0.14) Play with stranger 0.72 (±0.0.21) Excitability 0.91 (± 0.06) Calming down 0.99 (±0.01) Fall 0.74 (±0.13) 0.79 (±0.10) 0.78 (±0.13) 0.77 (±0.15) 0.62 (±0.30) Summer 0.13 (±0.48) 0.35 (0.48) 0.95 (±0.21) 0.71 (± 0.37) 0.44 (±0.7) Fall 0.78 (±0.17) 0.63 (±0.24) 0.77 (±0.22) 0.88 (± 0.07) 0.86 (0.01) 15 Temperament Relationship 0.84 (±0.21) 0.66 (± 0.34) 0.89 (±0.12) 0.75 (± 0.28) 0.73 (±0.20) 0.19 (±0.34) Only two young dogs were tested on the summer date, therefore no statistics are given for this date. In juvenile dogs, "calming down" and "excitability" were not tested. The mean correlations between the six judges and the official result differed among traits and among the three dates. The correlations at the summer test were generally lower than those at the spring or the autumn test but the dates only differed significantly for selfconfidence (both tests), temperament and relationship with the owner (breeding approval)(Tab. 4). Table 4: Influence of date and experience of the judges on the Spearman rank correlation coefficients (result individual judge correlated with the official result) for several traits. A GLM was used. For the juvenile dogs, "play with the owner" and "play with stranger" were transformed by arc sinus. For the breeding approval test "calming down" was transformed by arc sinus, "play with the owner" and "self-confidence" were square transformed. Correlations of the trait "play with stranger" had too many 1‘s (perfect correlations) and were analysed by a Friedman test. Neither date nor experience were significant, the results are not shown in this table. In juvenile dogs, "calming down" and "excitability" were not assessed. When interactions had P-values over 0.20 they were pooled and listed as "none" (* P > 0.05, ** P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001). a) juvenile dogs Trait Date (F-value) Self-confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Experience (F-value) Interaction (F-value) 12.21* 2.55 3.31 4.26 1.5 2.7 0.23 2.83 none Temperament 0.05 0.08 2.89 relationship 0.18 0.5 none b) breeding approval Trait self-confidence Date (F-value) Experience (F-value) Interaction (F-value) 7.50*** 0.13 none play with handler 0.57 4.53* none excitability 1.02 1.71 none calming down 1.84 1.96 none Temperament 6.32* 3.13 6.86** relationship 8.29** 1.76 2.21 The kappa coefficients as a measure of agreement between the judges for the trait 16 "aggressiveness" and the pass/fail result were also high and significant (Tab. 5). Table 5: Kappa coefficients for the measure of agreement between the judges for the trait "aggressiveness" and the pass/fail result (SD = standard deviation). Result Aggressiveness Average SD 0.79 0.33 0.71 0.44 P-Value 0.014 0.011 The average scores of all judges and the official scores did not differ significantly for any trait (Tab. 6). Table 6: P-values of the calculated difference between the average of the judges and the official score (Wilcoxon Test). Self-confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Excitability Calming down Temperament relationship P-Value (median) 0.25 0.47 0.54 1.00 0.25 0.12 1.00 The trait "self-confidence" correlated significantly negatively with "excitability" and "calming down", but "excitability" and "calming down" were significantly positively correlated. A dog which was judged "very excited", was also judged "slow to calm down" and with little "selfconfidence" (Tab. 7). Table 7: Spearman rank correlation coefficients between several traits. Above the diagonal are the correlation coefficients and below the diagonal the P-values (ns = non significant). Selfconfidence Play handler Play stranger Excitability Calming down Temperament Relationship Selfconfidence 1.00 0.58 0.42 -0.85 -0.78 0.46 0.22 Play with handler 0.00 1.00 0.50 -0.48 -0.50 0.44 -0.05 Play with stranger 0.05 0.02 1.00 -0.36 -0.46 0.47 -0.35 < .0001 0.02 ns 1.00 0.89 -0.18 -0.14 Excitability 17 Calming down < .0001 0.01 0.03 < .0001 1.00 -0.23 -0.20 Temperament 0.02 0.03 0.03 ns ns 1.00 0.00 ns ns ns ns ns ns 1.00 Relationship We did not find significant differences between experienced judges and judges who were still in training concerning their correlations with the official score for the TC of juvenile dog and the breeding approval except for "play with handler" (Table 4). The high inter-rater reliabilities were confirmed by the inter-rater reliability calculator for the juvenile dogs and for the breeding approval (Tab. 8). All the reliabilities calculated for the various traits were over 0.8, which is excellent. Table 8: Reliabilities between judges calculated with the inter-rater reliability calculator for TC of juvenile dogs and for the breeding approval. Number of raters denotes the harmonic mean number of raters per dog. The mean and the standard deviation (SD) are based on the averaged rating for each dog. a) juvenile dogs Trait Self-confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Temperament Relationship number of rater 6 5.91 5.91 5.96 5.91 mean rating 4.26 5.31 5.74 5.7 5.07 SD rating 1.03 1.51 1.47 0.76 0.96 reliability 0.8 0.95 0.94 0.89 0.87 b) breeding approval Trait Self-confidence Play with handler Play with stranger Excitability Calming down Temperament Relationship number of rater 5.92 5.92 5.58 5.83 5.83 5.87 5.87 mean rating 3.92 3.54 3.36 3.48 2.8 3.34 3.18 SD rating 1.29 1.05 1.63 0.78 1.06 0.54 0.66 reliability 0.89 0.89 0.97 0.97 0.95 0.81 0.92 2.3.2 Comparison between problematic and control dogs Thirty dogs of the group of problematic dogs had to be euthanised and 20 were placed into a new family. We eliminated 21 dogs for various causes: primary owner could not be found, dog died a long time ago, or no TC. Thus we finally sent 14 questionnaires to owners of dogs which were euthanised and 15 questionnaires to owners of dogs which were placed in a new family. We received 7 usable questionnaires for the euthanised dogs and 8 for the re-homed dogs. Fourteen questionnaires were returned empty. We classified these refusals into three categories: 1) the dog had died a long time ago; 2) the dog was not euthanised/re-homed because of a behaviour problem but because of pathology or personal problems; 3) unknown reasons. One of the principal causes was the length of time since the death of the dog. The questionnaire was not filled out if the owners felt they 18 did not remember how the dog acted in the various situations. When we compared the results of the TC of juvenile dogs with a problem of aggression and their controls we observed that the problematic dogs and the matched controls did not differ in the traits "aggression", "temperament", "self-confidence", "relationship", "type of relationship", "meeting with a foreigner" and "play with the owner". We therefore could not differentiate between these two categories of dogs by the results of the TCs. However, there was a significant difference (P = 0.0084) for the trait "play with stranger". We examined the difference between the sums of scores received in the TC which were given to dogs presenting problems and the control dogs; control dogs received significantly better scores (Fig 1). Figure 1: Difference of the sums of scores for the TC of juvenile dogs for the dog with problem of aggressiveness and their dog of control. 19 100 total sum of scores 90 80 self confidence Play_handler Play_stranger 70 Temperament Relationship 60 50 aggressiv dogs normal dogs We correlated the scores of the TC for the juvenile dogs with the various items of the questionnaire (Tab. 9). The trait "play with handler" of the TC correlated significantly negatively with these traits from the questionnaire: aggression to foreigners, aggression to the owner, stranger-directed fear, problem of separation. This means that dogs that played more with their owners during the test of character were less likely to have shown aggression to people, to be afraid of strangers and to have separation anxiety. The trait "play with stranger" correlated significantly negatively with the items aggression to the owner, stranger-directed fear, non-social fear, and excitability from the questionnaire. Table 9: Spearman correlation coefficients for several traits of the TC of juvenile dogs and the questionnaire. Selfconfidence Play handler Play stranger Temperament Relationship 20 Aggression against strangers Aggression against owners Aggression against dogs Aggression against familiar dogs -0.19 ns -0.58 * -0.41 ns -0.48 ns -0.38 ns -0.28 ns -0.62* -0.59 * 0.07 ns -0.47 ns -0.19 ns -0.23 ns -0.44 ns -0.47 ns 0.17 ns 0.46 ns 0.45 ns 0.38 ns 0.36 ns 0.04 ns Trainability -0.14 ns 0.28 ns 0.17 ns 0.39 ns 0.11 ns Chasing -0.38 ns -0.25 ns -0.07 ns -0.73** -0.09 ns Stranger directed fear -0.09 ns -0.65* -0.56 * -0.09 ns -0.28 ns Non-social fear -0.35 ns -0.49 ns -0.61* -0.23 ns -0.06 ns Separation problems -0.22 ns -0.58* -0.39 ns -0.38 ns -0.5 ns Touch sensitivity -0.11 ns -0.17 ns -0.3 ns -0.25 ns 0.19 ns Excitability -0.02 ns -0.39 ns -0.67* -0.23 ns 0.11 ns Attention seeking 0.1 ns -0.02 ns -0.3 ns -0.04 ns 0.27 ns Energy 0.12 ns -0.03 ns -0.31 ns 0.34 ns 0 ns When dogs with a problem of aggressiveness and their matched control dog were compared, using the questionnaire that the handler had filled out and the TC, problematic dogs almost always played significantly less. 2.4 Discussion 2.4.1 Agreement between judges The Swiss population of Hovawarts with pedigrees was represented well in this study, because the SHC encourages the owners of young Hovawarts to present their dogs at these two tests of character, even if the dogs in question will not be intended for breeding. It is for that reason that castrated or sterilized dogs participated in these tests, which is unusual. About 90 % of the young Swiss Hovawarts appear at the tests of character every year. The results showed a good general agreement between the 6 judges. The correlations between the judges and the official result and the reliabilities between the judges were high and most of the time significant. The mean interobserver agreement correlation often exceeded the mean reported in a review of studies of animal personalities and of humans (Gosling and Vazire, 2002). At the first date of this study in spring, we asked the judges to fill out the protocol without consulting, something they were not used to. Although our orders were clear several judges cheated a little, something we did not see later in summer and in autumn. 21 The agreement between the judges in July was worse than on the two other occasions (see Tables 1 and 5). Several explanations are possible. Maybe the composition of 6 judges of the summer 2006 was not ideal. We know that precisely for this session the requirement of having three experienced judges was not respected. The weather could have had an influence; it was very hot that day, which might have disturbed the concentration of the judges. As another possibility, the most experienced judge had his arm in a cast and this might have influenced his concentration. We also know that one week before this session all the judges participated in a day of training. They were made attentive to certain factors which can affect their judgment during the exam (e.g. a shared meaning system, acquaintance, etc.). This might have also influenced their judgements. Agreement coefficients are known to vary between traits (Gosling and Vazire, 2002). Certain traits like "self-confidence", "play with a stranger", "temperament" and "relationship" seemed more difficult to judge than others. This was most noticeable in the summer when the agreement was generally lower. It is difficult to explain why these traits were more difficult to judge than the others. It might be difficult to see the same things during the examination and to describe them in the same way, with the same specific words. The 6 judges have to remain concentrated during the entire examination and analyse every situation already by thinking of the protocol. If we take the example of "play with a stranger", we know that the stranger is played by the most experienced judge who is also the judge who fills out the protocol and who acts as a leader in the discussion. This trait is not difficult to judge normally, because the leader describes how the dog played with him without the 5 other judges intervening. These 5 judges are only observing and do not play with the dog, they are not therefore required to describe this behaviour, which can falsify the data relative to this trait. Furthermore, it is the last test for the dog; the concentration of the judges worsened, we even observed certain judges who did not look conscientiously at this test. In all cases, it should be realized that various factors can have serious consequences which are not always evident at first sight. Even if behavioural traits were well defined, the grading of the performance of a dog would always be subjective to a certain degree. In studies with several judges, an additional source of variation will affect the grading and inflate the total variance. Wilsson and Sundgren (1997) suggested to keep the number of people evaluating the dog's behaviour down to a minimum and to have routines that regularly coordinate the standard of these experts. In the SHC, there are only 9 available judges to evaluate the tests of character and they are all officially present at least once a year. Furthermore, the three experienced judges should be present every time. The results of table 3 demonstrated that the official score and the scores by all judges did not differ significantly. When we look for example at the spring results, we see that judges C and D never put in the same score but the average of these two judges is very close to the average of all 6 judges and also to the official note. So the more judges, the higher the probability that an error committed by one judge will be weak. Thus, the total variance is not going to amplify, but on the contrary to diminish. However, the composition of the six judges is important, there should always be a sufficient number of experienced judges in every session of the examination and the judges should have the possibility of communicating between themselves and to discuss the results. According to Fowell et al. (2006) a minimum of 6 judges is required when the judges are allowed to discuss their judgments. Their results indicate that a discussion phase improves the reliability and reduces the number of judges. Roberts et al. (2006) also showed that the optimal number of judges to obtain reliabilities better than 0.8 is between 5 and 7. While increasing the number of judges is important, reliability could also be improved. For example in one of the tests, all the judges, independently of the others, judged that the dog fled the situation. 22 When we looked at the official protocol, they agreed that the dog remained quiet during the situation. We do not know if it was an error on behalf of all the judges, or if a discussion on this subject made them all change their opinion. During a test of breeding approval, the judges had to vote because they did not agree concerning the final result of a dog and even during the discussion they did not find a common ground. So a group of experienced people instead of a single person decided on the dog‘s breeding approval or not. We observed that certain traits were strongly significantly correlated positively or negatively. (see Table 5). The judgements of the traits which correlate strongly with each other are not independent of each other. These are therefore not independent traits of character, but describe a type of character. 2.4.2 Comparison between problematic and control dogs The reliability of a TC does not predict its validity. The TCs made by the SHC are two assessments of the behaviour of the dog at two fixed moments. The dog is not constant in its behaviour; it can modulate its character all the time, influenced by the situations that it meets in its life. If the dog did not show aggressiveness during the one or/and the other TC, we cannot classify this dog as being exempt from aggressiveness forever, but only for this time in this situation. The dog might behave very differently at home with his family. The TC of the juvenile dog can help the owners to work with their dog in a way more targeted on certain aspects of the character. The persons who participate in a TC for juvenile dogs are generally interested in the advice of the judges. Mostly, they are also going to participate in various lessons of sociability and of obedience. In the study of Bennett and Rohlf (2006), dog owners who engaged in dog training activities reported that their dogs were less disobedient and more friendly/less aggressive towards strangers and familiar people than other dogs. Bennett and Rohlf (2006) established that participation in training activities is associated with a significantly reduced prevalence of canine behaviours that are potentially problematic for dog owners. Dogs at their TC of juvenile dogs are not yet mature sexually and are thus still very malleable in their character. We observed that the results of the TC for juvenile dogs of the control dogs were generally better than those of the problematic dogs. This TC is thus a good measure to practise a first selection of dogs; there is a high probability that a dog which already shows difficulties behaving in an adequate way at this age will become a problematic dog. On the other hand this TC cannot be used to predict the future aggressive behaviour of the dogs because it is not sensitive enough (same as in Svartberg, 2005). We obtained only one single significant difference between the results of problematic dogs and their controls for the trait "play with stranger". The problematic dogs did not play with the judge with so much enthusiasm as their controls. When dogs with a problem of aggressiveness and their matched control dog were compared, using the questionnaire to the handler and the TC, problematic dogs almost always played significantly less than their controls. However, not all dogs playing less at the TC will develop a problem of aggression. Considering this limitation, these TCs seem to assess important aspects of dog personality in a valid way which supports their uses as an instrument in dog breeding, in selection of individual dogs for different purposes and they allow to keep track of the characters of the Swiss population of Hovawarts. Acknowledgments The authors thank the judges of the SHC for their help with the data collection during the TC and especially Denise Gaudy, who also helped us with the dog-owners addresses and 23 the access of old TCs. Christine Wittwer told us about the six judges at the TC of the SHC and thus gave us the idea for this study. Many thanks also to Dr. James A. Serpell, who kindly put the CBARQ questionnaire at our disposal, and let us use it in this study. 24 References Bennett, P. C. and Rohlf, V. I. 2006. Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2007: 65-84. Fowell, S. L., Fewtrell, R. and McLaughlin, P. J. 2006.Estimating the minimum number of judges required for test-centred standard setting on written assessments. Do discussion and iteration have an influence? Advances in Health Sciences Education (in press). Gosling, S. D. and Vazire, S. 2002. Are we barking up the right tree? Evaluating a comparative approach to personality. Journal of Research in Personality 36: 607-614. Gosling, S. D., Kwan, V. S. and John, O. P. 2003. A dog's got personality: a cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of personality and social psychology 85: 1161-9. Hsu, Y. and Serpell, J. A. 2003. Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medicine Association 223: 12931300. Jones, A. C. and Gosling, S. D. 2005. Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): A review and evaluation of past research. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95: 1-53. Kenny, D. A. 1991. A general model of consensus and accuracy in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review 98: 155-163. Lloyd, A. S., Martin, J. E., Bornett-Gauci, H. L. I. and Wilkinson, R. G. 2007. Evaluation of a novel method of horse personality assessment: Rater-agreement and links to behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105: 205-222. Roberts, C., Cromarty, I., Crossley, J. and Jolly, B. 2006. The reliability and validity of a matrix to assess the competed relective personal development plans of general practitioners. Medical Education 40: 363370. Scherrer, C2007. Wesenprüfungen bei Rassehundeklubs in der Schweiz - Ergebnisse einer Befragung. Diss. med. vet., Vetsuisse-Fakultät der Universität Bern, Bern. Seiferle, E. and Leonhardt, E., 1984. Wesensgrundlagen und Wesenprüfung des Hundes Erni Satz und Druck AG: 8722 Kaltbrunn, Switzerland. Slabbert, J. M. and Odendaal, J.S.J. 1999. Early prediction of adult police dog efficiency - a longitudinal study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 64: 269-288. Svartberg, K. 2005. A comparison of behaviour in test and in everyday life: evidence of three consisten boldness-related personality traits in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 91: 103-128. van der Borg, J. A. M., Netto, W. J. and Planta, D. J. U. 1991. Behavioural testing of dogs in animal shelters to predict problem behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32: 237-251. Venzl, E., Unshelm, J. and Oldigs, B. 1989. Der Campbell-test: eine Wesensprüfung beim Hund, durchgeführt an der Rasse Beagle. In: Aktuelle Arbeiten zur artgemässen Tierhaltung. Darmstadt, Giessen. 134-152. Weiss, E. G., Gary. 1997. Service dog selection tests: Effectiveness for dogs from animal shelters. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53: 297-308. Wilsson, E. and Sundgren, P.-E. 1997a. The use of a behaviour test for selection of dogs for service and breeding. II. Heritability for tested parameters and effect of selection based on service dog characteristics. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54: 235-241. 25 Wilsson, E. S., Sundgren P.-E. 1997b.The use of a behaviour test for the selection of dogs for service and breeding, I: Method of testing and evaluating test results in the adult dog, demands on different kinds of service dogs, sex and breed differences. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53: 279-295. Wilsson, E. and Sundgren, P.-E. 1998.Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58: 151 - 162. 26 3 Appendix 3.1 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC of juvenile dogs ............................ 28 3.2 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC for the breeding approval ........... 29 3.3 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in French...................................................... 41 3.4 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in German .................................................... 56 3.5 Appendix: Pictures during the TC in spring ............................................................ 72 27 3.1 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC of juvenile dogs (JunghundeBegutachtung). The judges underline the several words to describe the behaviour of the dog during the TC. 28 3.2 Appendix: Protocol of examination for the TC for the breeding approval (Körung) 29 3.3 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in French Questionnaire d’évaluation et de Recherche sur le Comportement (PennBARQ) Les questions suivantes sont destinées à la compréhension du comportement de votre chien au cours des mois précédents (c.à d. pendant les derniers mois avant l'euthanasie/la séparation avec le chien) De préférence, veuillez répondre à toutes les questions, en ne cochant qu‘une seule réponse. Mais, si pour une quelconque raison, vous n‘êtes pas en mesure de répondre correctement ( p.ex. vous n‘avez jamais vu votre chien confronté à la situation évoquée) alors, ne répondez pas. Section 1: Formation et obéissance Quelques chiens sont plus obéissants et aptes à la formation que d‘autres. En cochant les cases qui conviennent, veuillez indiquer comment, au cours des derniers mois, votre chien s‘est comporté dans les situations suivantes: Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 1. En liberté, revient-il immédiatement à l‘appel ? 2. Obéit-il immédiatement à l‘ordre de s‘asseoir ? 3. Obéit-il immédiatement à l‘ordre ''pas bouger'' ? 4. Est-il très attentif à ce que vous dites ou faites ? 5. Est-il réceptif ou indifférent à la punition ou à la correction ? 41 6. Est-il lent à apprendre de nouveaux jeux ou de nouvelles tâches ? 7. Est-il facilement distrait par les odeurs, par les bruits ou par ce qu‘il voit ? 8. Rapporte-t-il ou essait-il de rapporter bâtons, balles ou objets ? Section 2: L’Agressivité Quelques chiens font preuve d‘un comportement agressif, de temps en temps. Les signes typiques d‘agressivité modérée incluent : aboiements ou grognements, le chien peut même montrer les dents. L‘agressivité la plus grave inclut généralement : claquement de dents, menace de sauter sur la personne, intention évidente de mordre. Cochez le chiffre correspondant au degré d‘agressivité sur l‘échelle suivante (0= pas agressif - 4= très agressif). Veuillez indiquer la tendance de votre chien à l‘agressivité dans les situations suivantes : Pas du tout agressif: Aucun signe visible d‘agressivité Agressivité modérée: Grognements / aboiements & montre ses dents Forte agressivité: Claque des dents, mord ou essaie de mordre 0…..……..…………..1…………….………..2………..………..…..3…………….………..4 9. Quand vous ou quelqu‘un de son entourage le punissez ou le grondez (avec des mots) 10. Quand il est approché par un adulte inconnu alors qu‘il est en laisse. 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 42 11. Quand il est approché par un enfant inconnu alors qu‘il est en laisse. 0 1 2 3 4 12. Quand il est approché par un inconnu alors qu‘il est dans votre voiture (ex. à la pompe à essence). 0 1 2 3 4 13. Quand quelqu‘un de son entourage lui enlève ses jouets, son os ou tout autre objet. 0 1 2 3 4 14. Quand quelqu‘un de son entourage lui donne un bain ou fait sa toilette. 0 1 2 3 4 15. Quand un inconnu vous approche ou approche un autre membre de votre entourage à votre domicile. 0 1 2 3 4 16. Quand un inconnu vous approche ou approche un autre membre de votre entourage à l‘extérieur. 0 1 2 3 4 17. Quand quelqu‘un de la maison l‘approche pendant qu‘il mange. 0 1 2 3 4 18. Quand le facteur ou des livreurs approchent de votre maison. 0 1 2 3 4 19. Quand quelqu‘un de la maison lui retire sa gamelle. 0 1 2 3 4 20. Quand des inconnus passent devant la maison alors qu‘il est dans le jardin 0 1 2 3 4 21. Quand un inconnu essaie de le toucher ou de le caresser. 0 1 2 3 4 43 22. Quand des inconnus courent, font du vélo ou du roller etc.. près de la maison, lorsque le chien est dehors ou dans le jardin. 0 1 2 3 4 23. Quand il est approché franchement par un autre mâle alors qu‘il est tenu en laisse 0 1 2 3 4 24. Quand il est approché franchement par une autre femelle alors qu‘il est tenu en laisse. 0 1 2 3 4 25. Quand vous le fixez dans les yeux. 0 1 2 3 4 26. Envers un autre chien qui visite votre maison. 0 1 2 3 4 27. Envers les chats, les écureuils ou tout autre animal qui se trouve dans 0 1 2 3 4 votre jardin. 28. Envers des inconnus qui vous rendent visite. 0 1 2 3 4 29. Quand un chien inconnu aboie, grogne ou saute sur lui 0 1 2 3 4 30. Quand quelqu‘un de la maison l‘enjambe 0 1 2 3 4 31. Quand vous ou quelqu‘un de la maison lui reprenez un objet ou de la nourriture volée. 0 1 2 3 4 32. Envers un autre chien de la maison. (Ne pas cocher s‘il n‘y a pas d‘autre chien chez vous) 0 1 2 3 4 44 33. Quand un autre chien de la maison l‘approche alors qu‘il dort dans son panier préféré. 0 1 2 3 4 34. Quand un autre chien de la maison approche de sa gamelle. 0 1 2 3 4 35. Quand un autre chien de la maison l‘approche alors qu‘il joue avec son jouet préféré ou un os. 0 1 2 3 4 Y-a-t-il d‘autres situations où votre chien se montre parfois agressif ? Si oui, veuillez les décrire : Section 3: La Peur ou l’Anxiété Quelques chiens manifestent de la peur ou de l‘anxiété par rapport à certains sons ou bruits particuliers, devant certains objets, certaines personnes ou certaines situations. Les signes typiques d‘une peur modérée incluent : évitement du regard, évitement de l‘objet qui inspire la peur, il s‘aplatit sur le sol, se tient avec la queue basse ou queue entre les pattes, gémit ou couine, se tétanise, se secoue ou tremble. La peur extrême est caractérisée par des tremblements exagérés, par une tentative vigoureuse de fuite pour s‘éloigner rapidement de l‘objet ou de la situation engendrant sa peur. En utilisant l‘échelle suivante (0=Pas de peur, 4= Peur extrême) indiquez la tendance de votre chien en ce qui concerne une éventuelle réaction de peur dans les situations suivantes 45 Pas du tout peureux: Forte anxiété: Anxiété modérée: Aucun signes visible d‘anxiété Se recroqueville; recule ou se cache, etc. 0…..……..…………..1…………….………..2………..………..…..3…………….………..4 36. Quand une personne adulte inconnue s‘approche franchement de lui alors qu‘il est loin de la maison. 0 1 2 3 4 37. Quand un enfant inconnu s‘approche de lui franchement alors qu‘il est 0 1 2 3 4 loin de la maison. 38. Réaction aux bruits soudains (ex. aspirateur, pétarades de moteurs, marteau-piqueur objets qui tombent etc...) 0 1 2 3 4 39. Quand des inconnus viennent en visite à la maison 0 1 2 3 4 40. Quand un inconnu essaie de toucher ou de caresser le chien. 0 1 2 3 4 46 41. Réaction devant un important trafic de voitures ou de circulation 0 1 2 3 4 42. Réaction vis-à-vis d‘objets étranges ou inattendus sur ou à côté du trottoir (ex. sacs de poubelles, feuilles, détritus, drapeaux qui claquent 0 1 2 3 4 43. Réaction quand il est examiné par le vétérinaire. 0 1 2 3 4 44. Pendant les orages 0 1 2 3 4 45. Quand un chien inconnu (de la même taille ou plus grand que lui) l‘approche franchement 0 1 2 3 4 46. Quand un chien inconnu plus petit que lui l‘approche franchement 0 1 2 3 4 47. Réaction devant des situations nouvelles (ex. première fois en voiture, 0 1 2 3 4 en ascenseur ou première visite chez le vétérinaire). 48. Réaction aux rafales de vent ou aux objets emportés par le vent. 0 1 2 3 4 49. Pendant la coupe des ongles par quelqu‘un de la maison 0 1 2 3 4 50. Pendant le toilettage ou le bain par quelqu‘un de la maison 0 1 2 3 4 51. Quand il est enjambé par quelqu‘un de la maison 0 1 2 3 4 52. Quand quelqu‘un de la maison essuie ses pattes 0 1 2 3 4 53. Quand des chiens inconnus viennent à la maison. 0 1 2 3 4 47 54. Quand un chien inconnu aboie ou saute sur lui 0 1 2 3 4 Section 4: Comportement vis-à-vis de la Séparation. Quelques chiens manifestent de l‘anxiété ou un comportement anormal lors d‘une séparation même brève. Dans un passé récent combien de fois votre chien a-t-il montré un comportement anormal (voir les éléments ci-dessous) avant ou pendant une séparation. (Cochez les cases appropriées) Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 55. Frémissement ou tremblement 56. Excès de salive 57. Fébrilité ou agitation 58. Gémissement 59. Aboiement 60. Hurlement 61. Ronge ou gratte les portes, les fenêtres, les rideaux etc 62. Perte de l‘appétit. Y-a-t-il d‘autres situations au cours desquelles votre chien est peureux ou anxieux ? Si oui, veuillez les décrire : 48 Section 5: Excitation Certains chiens manifestent peu de réaction face à des situations qui pourraient être énervantes, d‘autres au contraire, sont très excités face à de nouvelles situations. Les signes d‘un excitation modérée incluent : un certain intérêt pour approcher cette nouveauté ainsi que quelques aboiements. L‘excitation extrême se caractérise par une tendance à des réactions excessives. Le chien hyper-excité aboie / jappe hystériquement à la moindre chose, court vers ou autour de chaque nouvelle source d‘excitation ; il est très difficile de le calmer. En utilisant l‘échelle (0=Calme, 4=Excitation extrême ), indiquez la tendance de votre chien quant à son comportement d‘excitation dans les situations suivantes : Extrêmement excitable: Calme: peu ou pas de réaction spéciale Nervosité faible-modérée: Réagit de manière excessive, difficulté à se calmer 0…..……..…………..1…………….………..2………..………..…..3…………….………..4 63. Quand vous ou un autre membre de votre entourage revenez après une courte absence. 0 1 2 3 4 64. En jouant avec vous ou un autre membre de votre entourage. 0 1 2 3 4 65. Quand la sonnette retentit. 0 1 2 3 4 66. Juste avant la promenade. 0 1 2 3 4 67. Juste avant une sortie en voiture. 0 1 2 3 4 49 68. A l‘arrivée d‘invités à la maison. 0 1 2 3 4 Y-a-t-il d‘autres situations dans lesquelles votre chien devient surexcité ? Si oui, veuillez les décrire. Section 6: Attachement et besoin d’Attention. La plupart des chiens sont fortement attachés à leur famille et certains ont besoin de plus d‘attention et d‘affection que d‘autres. Dans un passé récent combien de fois votre chien a-t-il montré, par les signes suivants, son besoin d‘attachement ou son besoin d‘attirer votre attention : Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 69. A-t-il montré un fort attachement pour quelqu‘un en particulier. 70. A-t-il tendance à vous suivre (ou à suivre quelqu‘un de votre entourage) de pièce en pièce dans la maison. 50 71. A-t-il tendance a s‘asseoir près de vous, ou à chercher le contact avec vous (ou avec quelqu‘un d‘autres) lorsque vous êtes assis. 72. A-t-il tendance à donner un coup de patte ou de museau (à vous ou à quelqu‘un d‘autre) pour attirer votre attention quand vous êtes assis. 73. Devient-il agité (gémit, saute ou essaie d‘intervenir) quand vous (ou quelqu‘un d‘autre) montrez de l‘affection pour une autre personne. 74. Devient-il agité (gémit, saute ou essaie d‘intervenir) quand vous (ou quelqu‘un d‘autre) montrez de l‘affection pour un autre animal ou un autre chien Section 7: Jeu et comportement social Beaucoups de chiens jouent très volontiers et continuellement avec les gens et/ou avec d'autres chiens. Les visiteurs vont souvent être acceuills. Dans le passé, votre chien a-til manifesté certains comportements de jeu ou de salutations suivants. Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 75. Joue très volontiers avec les membres de la famille 76. Joue très volontiers avec des personnes inconnues 77. Apporte des jouets et demande à jouer 78. Joue très volontiers avec des mâles 51 79. Joue très volontiers avec des femelles 80. Se chamaille, se bat volontiers pour jouer 81. Engage rapidement le jeu lorsque d' d'autres chiens l'y poussent 82. Joue volontiers au jeu du ''tir à la corde'' avec des personnes inconnues 83. Poursuit volontiers les balles lancées 84. Veut être le centre d'attention 85. Accueille des adultes en visite d'une façon amicale 86. Accueille des enfants en visite d'une façon amicale 87. Accueille des chiens en visite d'une façon amicale 88. S'approche très volontiers d'une façon amicale d'adultes en dehors de chez lui 89. S'approche très volontiers d'une façon amicale des enfants en dehors de chez lui 52 Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 90. S'approche très volontiers d'une façon amicale des chiens en dehors de chez lui 91. Se réjouit d'être caressé par des personnes inconnues Section 8: Observations diverses Certains chiens manifestent d‘autres problèmes de comportement que ceux déjà indiqués dans ce questionnaire. Dans un passé récent, votre chien a-t-il, dans son comportement, manifesté certains des signes suivants. Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 92. Court après les chats 93. Court après les oiseaux 94. Court après les écureuils, les lapins, etc... 95. Se sauve de la maison ou du jardin et erre longtemps 96. Se roule dans ses propres excréments ou ceux des autres ou dans des déchets malodorants 97. Mange ses propres excréments ou ceux des autres chiens 98. Ronge des objets inappropriés 53 99. ―grimper‖ (comportements sexuels) sur certains objets, meubles ou personnes. 100. Réclame à table. 101. Vole la nourriture 102. A peur des escaliers (montée ou descente) 103. Tire excessivement sur la laisse. 104. Urine contre les objets/ meubles dans la maison. 105. Urine à l‘approche de quelqu‘un, ou quand il est caressé, touché ou porté. 106. Urine quand il est seul la nuit ou le jour. 107. Fait ses besoins quand il est seul la nuit ou le jour. 108. Est hyperactif, agité, a du mal à trouver sa place. 109. joueur et turbulent. Jamais Rarement Parfois Régulièrement Toujours 110. Est actif, énergique et toujours en mouvement. 111. Fixe son regard sur quelque chose 54 d‘invisible. 112. Essaie de mordre des insectes invisibles. 113. Tourne sur lui-même pour attraper sa queue. 114. Chasse / suit les ombres. 115. Aboie excessivement. 116. Se lèche de manière excessive. 117. Lèche les gens ou les objets de manière excessive. 118. Manifeste d‘autres comportements*, bizarres, étranges ou répétitifs *Veuillez les décrire : 55 3.4 Appendix: PennBARQ questionnaire in German Fragebogen über Hundeverhalten zur wissenschaftlichen Auswertung (C–BARQ) Anhand dieses Fragebogens können Sie beschreiben, wie sich Ihr Hund in der letzten Zeit (d.h. während der letzten Monate bevor Ihr Hund umplaziert/euthanasiert wurde) verhalten hat. Bitte kreuzen Sie pro Frage nur ein Kästchen an. Bitte versuchen Sie, alle Fragen zu beantworten. Lassen Sie eine Frage nur offen, falls Sie sie aus irgendwelchen Gründen nicht beantworten können (z.B. wenn Sie Ihren Hund nie in der beschriebenen Situation beobachtet haben). Teil 1: Training und Gehorsam Einige Hunde sind gehorsamer und leichter trainierbar als andere. Bitte kreuzen Sie das geeignete Kästchen an, wie trainierbar oder gehorsam Ihr Hund in den folgenden Situationen in der letzten Zeit gewesen ist. nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 1. Unangeleint kommt er/sie sofort, wenn er/sie gerufen wird 2. Gehorcht dem ―Sitz‖ Kommando sofort 3. Gehorcht dem ―Bleib‖ Kommando sofort 4. Scheint aufmerksam alles zu verfolgen/hören, was Sie sagen oder tun 5. Reagiert langsam auf Korrektur oder Strafe; ‗dickhäutig‘ 6. Lernt neue Tricks und Aufgaben langsam 56 nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 7. Lässt sich leicht von interessanten Dingen, Geräuschen oder Gerüchen ablenken 8. Apportiert (oder versucht zu apportieren) Stöcke, Bälle oder sonstige Objekte 57 Teil 2: Aggression Einige Hunde zeigen von Zeit zu Zeit aggressives Verhalten. Typische Zeichen für gemässigte Aggression sind Bellen, Knurren und Zähne zeigen. Ernsthaftere Aggression besteht gewöhnlich aus Schnappen, sich auf jemanden Stürzen, Beissen oder dem Versuch zu Beissen. Umkreisen oder unterstreichen Sie eine Nummer auf der folgenden 5-Punkte-Skala rechts aussen (0 = keine Aggression, 4 ernsthafte Aggression), die der Tendenz ihres Hundes entspricht, in den folgenden Situationen aggressives Verhalten zu zeigen: Keine Aggression: Gemässigte Aggression: Knurren/Bellen—Zähne Fletschen Keine sichtbaren Zeichen von Aggression Ernsthafte Aggression: Schnappen, Beissen oder Versuche zu Beissen. 0............................1............................2............................3............................4 9. Wenn der Hund von Ihnen oder einem Familienmitglied mit Worten korrigiert oder bestraft wird (Schimpfen, Anschreien, u.s.w.) 0 1 2 3 4 10. Wenn sich ein unbekannter Erwachsener dem Hund direkt nähert, während der Hund an der Leine geführt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 11. Wenn sich ein unbekanntes Kind dem Hund direkt nähert, während der Hund an der Leine geführt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 12. Wenn sich unbekannte Personen dem Hund nähern, während der Hund in Ihrem Auto ist (z.B. an der Tankstelle). 0 1 2 3 4 13. Wenn Spielzeuge, Knochen oder andere Objekte von einem Familienmitglied weggenommen werden. 0 1 2 3 4 58 14. Wenn der Hund von einem Familienmitglied gebadet oder gebürstet wird. 0 1 2 3 4 15. Wenn sich eine unbekannte Person Ihnen oder einem Familienmitglied zu Hause nähert. 0 1 2 3 4 16. Wenn sich eine unbekannte Person Ihnen oder einem Familienmitglied ausserhalb ihres Hauses/Wohnung nähert. 0 1 2 3 4 17. Wenn sich ein Familienmitglied direkt dem Hund nähert, während der Hund frisst. 18. Wenn sich der Postbote oder andere Boten Ihrem Haus/Ihrer Wohnung nähern. 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 19. Wenn sein/ihr Futter von einem Familienmitglied weggenommen wird. 0 1 2 3 4 20. Wenn Fremde an Ihrem Haus vorbeigehen, während Ihr Hund draussen oder im Garten ist. 0 1 2 3 4 21. Wenn eine unbekannte Person versucht, Ihren Hund zu berühren oder zu streicheln. 0 1 2 3 4 22. Wenn Jogger, Velofahrer, Rollerbladers oder Skateboarders an Ihrem Haus vorbei laufen/fahren, während Ihr Hund draussen oder im Garten 0 1 2 3 4 ist. 23. Wenn sich ein fremder Rüde direkt nähert, während der Hund an der Leine geführt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 24. Wenn sich eine fremde Hündin direkt nähert, während der Hund an der Leine geführt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 25. Wenn Ihr Hund von einem Familienmitglied direkt angestarrt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 59 26. Gegenüber unbekannten Hunden, die Ihr Haus/Ihre Wohnung besuchen. 0 1 2 3 4 27. Gegenüber Katzen, Eichhörnchen oder anderen kleinen Tieren, die in 0 1 2 3 4 Ihren Garten kommen 28. Gegenüber unbekannten Personen, die Ihr Haus/Ihre Wohnung besuchen. 0 1 2 3 4 29. Wenn Ihr Hund von einem anderen (unbekannten) Hund angebellt oder angeknurrt wird, oder sich der unbekannte Hund auf ihn stürzt. 0 1 2 3 4 30. Wenn ein Familienmitglied über Ihren Hund stolpert. 0 1 2 3 4 31. Wenn Sie oder ein Familienmitglied Futter oder Objekte zurückholt, die der Hund gestohlen hatte. 0 1 2 3 4 32. Gegenüber einem anderen (bekannten) Hund in Ihrem Haushalt (nicht ausfüllen, wenn keine anderen Hunde in Ihrem Haushalt leben). 0 1 2 3 4 33. Wenn sich ein anderer (bekannter) Hund dem Hund an seinem Lieblings(schlaf)platz nähert (nicht ausfüllen, wenn keine anderen Hunde in Ihrem Haushalt leben). 0 1 2 3 4 34. Wenn sich ein anderer (bekannter) Hund dem Hund nähert, während er/sie 0 1 2 3 4 frisst (nicht ausfüllen, wenn keine anderen Hunde in Ihrem Haushalt leben). 35. Wenn sich ein anderer (bekannter) Hund dem Hund nähert, während er/sie mit dem Lieblingsspielzeug, Knochen, oder einem Objekt spielt oder es benagt (nicht ausfüllen, wenn keine anderen Hunde in Ihrem Haushalt leben). 0 1 2 3 4 60 Gibt es irgendwelche andere Situationen, in denen Ihr Hund manchmal aggressiv ist? Falls ja, beschreiben Sie es bitte kurz: Teil 3: Furcht und Ängstlichkeit Hunde zeigen manchmal Zeichen von Ängstlichkeit oder Furcht, wenn sie bestimmten Geräuschen, Objekten, Personen oder Situationen ausgesetzt sind. Typische Anzeichen für geringe bis mässige Angst wären: Vermeidung von Augenkontakt, Meiden des gefürchteten Objekts, Kauern oder Kriechen mit herabhängenden Schwanz oder mit dem Schwanz zwischen den Beinen; Winseln oder Jaulen, Erstarren und Zittern oder Schlottern. Extreme Angst zeigt sich durch übertriebenes Kauern und/oder kräftige Versuche auszubrechen, sich vor dem gefürchteten Objekt, der Person oder der Situation zurückzuziehen oder zu verstecken. Benutzen Sie bitte die folgende 5-Punkte Skala rechts aussen (0=keine Angst, 4=extreme Angst) und bezeichnen Sie die Tendenz Ihres Hundes, in den folgenden Umständen ängstliches Verhalten zu zeigen: Keine Ängstlichkeit: Gemässigte Ängstlichkeit: Keine sichtbaren Zeichen von Furcht Ernsthafte Ängstlichkeit: kauert; zieht sich zurück oder versteckt sich, usw. 0............................1............................2............................3............................4 36. Wenn sich ein unbekannter Erwachsener fern von zu Hause dem 61 Hund direkt nähert. 0 1 2 3 4 37. Wenn sich ein unbekanntes Kind fern von zu Hause dem Hund direkt nähert. 0 1 2 3 4 38. Als Reaktion auf plötzliche oder laute Geräusche (z.B. Staubsauger, Fehlzündungen von Autos, Strassenbohrungen, herunterfallende Objekte, 0 1 2 3 4 u.s.w.). 39. Wenn unbekannte Personen Ihr Haus/Ihre Wohnung besuchen. 0 1 2 3 4 40. Wenn eine unbekannte Person versucht, Ihren Hund zu berühren oder zu streicheln. 0 1 2 3 4 41. In dichtem Verkehr 0 1 2 3 4 42. Als Reaktion auf seltsame oder unbekannte Objekte auf oder nahe dem Trottoir (z.B. Kehrrichtsäcke, Blätter, Abfall, flatternde Fahnen, u.s.w.) 0 1 2 3 4 43. Wenn Ihr Hund von einem Tierarzt untersucht oder behandelt wird. 0 1 2 3 4 44. Während Gewittern, Feuerwerk, oder ähnlichen Ereignissen 0 1 2 3 4 45. Wenn sich ein unbekannter gleich grosser oder grösserer Hund direkt 0 1 2 3 4 nähert. 46. Wenn sich ein unbekannter kleinerer Hund direkt nähert. 0 1 2 3 4 47. Wenn Ihr Hund zum ersten Mal unbekannten Situationen ausgesetzt wird (z.B. die erste Autofahrt, das erste Mal im Lift, der erste Besuch beim 0 1 2 3 4 Tierarzt, u.s.w.) 48. Als Reaktion auf Wind oder im Wind treibende Objekte. 0 1 2 3 4 62 49. Wenn die Nägel von einem Familienmitglied geschnitten werden. 0 1 2 3 4 50. Wenn Ihr Hund von einem Familienmitglied gebürstet oder gebadet wird. 0 1 2 3 4 51. Wenn ein Familienmitglied über Ihren Hund stolpert. 0 1 2 3 4 52. Wenn seine/ihre Pfoten von einem Familienmitglied abgetrocknet werden. 0 1 2 3 4 53. Wenn unbekannte Hunde Ihr Haus/Ihre Wohnung besuchen. 0 1 2 3 4 54. Wenn Ihr Hund von einem anderen (unbekannten) Hund angebellt oder angeknurrt wird, oder sich der unbekannte Hund auf ihn stürzt. 0 1 2 3 4 63 Teil 4: Verhalten bei Trennungen Einige Hunde zeigen Anzeichen von Angst oder abnormalem Verhalten, wenn sie, selbst für relativ kurze Zeit, allein gelassen werden. Wenn Sie an die letzte Zeit denken, wie oft hat Ihr Hund die folgenden Anzeichen von Trennungsverhalten gezeigt, wenn er oder sie allein gelassen wurde oder dabei war, allein gelassen zu werden (Kreuzen Sie bitte die entsprechenden Kästchen an): nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 55. Zittern 56. Vermehrtes Speicheln 57. Unruhe/Aufregung/Hin-und-herlaufen 58. Winseln 59. Bellen 60. Heulen 61. Nagen/Kratzen an den Türen, Boden, Fenstern, Vorhängen, u.s.w. 62. Appetitlosigkeit Gibt es irgendwelche andere Situationen, in denen Ihr Hund ängstlich ist? Falls ja, beschreiben Sie es bitte: Teil 5: Erregbarkeit Einige Hunde reagieren relativ wenig auf plötzliche oder potenziell aufregende Ereignisse und Störungen in ihrer Umgebung, während sich andere wegen der 64 kleinsten Neuheit sehr aufregen. Anzeichen für geringe oder gemässigte Erregbarkeit umfassen zunehmende Aufmerksamkeit, Annäherung an die Ursache des Neuen und kurze Bellphasen. Extreme Erregbarkeit ist gekennzeichnet durch eine generelle Tendenz zur Überreaktion. Der erregbare Hund bellt und kläfft hysterisch bei der kleinsten Störung, rennt zu der Ursache der Aufregung und umkreist sie und ist schwierig zu beruhigen. Benutzen Sie bitte die folgende 5-Punkte Skala rechts aussen (0=ruhig, 4=extrem erregbar) und bezeichnen Sie die Tendenz Ihres Hundes, in den folgenden Umständen erregt zu reagieren: Ruhig: Mild-gemäßigte Erregbarkeit: wenig oder keine spezielle Reaktion Äußerst erregbar: Reagiert über, schwerig sich zu beruhigen 0............................1............................2............................3............................4 63. Wenn Sie oder andere Familienangehörige nach einer kurzen Zeit der 0 1 2 3 4 Abwesenheit nach Hause kommen. 64. Wenn Sie oder andere Familienangehörige mit dem Hund spielen. 0 1 2 3 4 65. Wenn es an der Haustüre klingelt. 0 1 2 3 4 66. Kurz vor dem Spaziergang. 0 1 2 3 4 67. Kurz vor der Autofahrt. 0 1 2 3 4 68. Wenn Besucher zu Ihnen nach Hause kommen. 0 1 2 3 4 Gibt es sonst irgendwelche andere Situationen, in denen Ihr Hund manchmal überreagiert? Falls ja, beschreiben Sie es bitte kurz: 65 Teil 6: Bindung und Suche nach Bindung Die meisten Hunde sind stark mit ihren Besitzern verbunden und einige verlangen viel Aufmerksamkeit und Zuneigung von ihnen. Wenn Sie an die letzte Zeit zurückdenken, wie oft hat Ihr Hund die folgenden Anzeichen für Bindung oder Suche nach Bindung gezeigt? nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 69. Zeigt eine starke Bindung an ein bestimmtes Familienmitglied. 70. Tendiert dazu Ihnen (oder einem anderen Familienmitglied) im Haus zu folgen, von Raum zu Raum. 71. Tendiert dazu nahe oder in Berührung mit Ihnen (oder anderen) zu sitzen, wenn Sie sich hinsetzen. 72. Tendiert dazu Sie anzustupsen, mit der Schnauze oder mit der Pfote zu berühren, um Aufmerksamkeit zu erzeugen, wenn Sie sich hinsetzen. 73. Regt sich auf (winselt, springt hoch, versucht einzugreifen) wenn Sie (oder andere) Zärtlichkeit für eine andere Person zeigen. 74. Regt sich auf (winselt, springt hoch, versucht einzugreifen) wenn Sie (oder andere) Zärtlichkeit für einen anderen Hund oder Tier zeigen. Teil 7: Spiel und soziales Verhalten Viele Hunde spielen gerne und ausdauernd mit Menschen und/oder anderen Hunden. Besucher werden oft begrüsst. Wenn Sie an die letzte Zeit denken, wie oft hat Ihr Hund folgende Elemente des Spiels oder Begrüssungen gezeigt? nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 66 75. Spielt eifrig mit Familienangehörigen 76. Spielt eifrig mit Fremdpersonen 77. Bringt Spielzeug und fordert zum Spiel auf 78. Spielt eifrig mit Rüden 79. Spielt eifrig mit Hündinnen 80. Rauft gern spielerisch 81. Geht schnell auf Spielaufforderungen von anderen Hunden ein 82. Liebt Zerrspiele mit bekannten Personen 83. Läuft gern hinter geworfenen Bällen her 84. Möchte im Mittelpunkt der Aufmerksamkeit stehen 67 nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 85. Begrüsst erwachsene Besucher auf eine freundliche Art 86. Begrüsst besuchende Kinder auf eine freundliche Art 87. Begrüsst besuchende Hunde auf eine freundliche Art 88. Nähert sich eifrig Erwachsenen ausserhalb des Zuhauses auf eine freundliche Art 89. Nähert sich eifrig Kindern ausserhalb 90. Nähert sich eifrig Hunden ausserhalb des Zuhauses auf eine freundliche Art 91. Freut sich, wenn er/sie von Fremden gestreichelt wird 68 Teil 8: Verschiedenes Hunde zeigen eine grosse Vielfalt an Problemverhalten zusätzlich zu dem, was in diesem Fragebogen schon abgedeckt wurde. Wenn Sie über die letzte Zeit nachdenken, wie oft hat Ihr Hund irgendwelche der folgenden Verhaltensweisen gezeigt: nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 92. Jagt oder würde bei Gelegenheit Katzen jagen 93. Jagt oder würde bei Gelegenheit Vögel jagen 94. Jagt oder würde bei Gelegenheit Eichhörnchen, Kaninchen und andere kleine Tiere jagen 95. Reisst aus oder würde bei Gelegenheit ausreissen 96. Wälzt sich im Kot oder anderen ‚stinkigen‘ Substanzen 97. Frisst den eigenen Kot oder den von anderen Tieren 98. Kaut an ungeeigneten Objekten 99. ‚Besteigt‘ (aufreiten) Objekte, Möbel oder Leute 100. Bettelt ständig am Tisch, wenn die Leute essen 101. Stiehlt Essen 102. Nervös oder ängstlich auf Treppen 103. Zieht besonders stark an der Leine 69 nie selten manchmal gewöhnlich immer 104. Uriniert auf Objekte / Einrichtungen in Ihrem Haus/Ihrer Wohnung 105. Uriniert bei Annäherung, Streicheln, Berührung oder Aufheben 106. Uriniert, wenn er/sie in der Nacht oder am Tag allein gelassen wird 107. Setzt Kot ab, wenn er/sie in der Nacht oder am Tag allein gelassen wird 108. Hyperaktiv, unruhig, hat Probleme, sich zu beruhigen 109. Verspielt, welpenhaft, ungestürm 110. Aktiv, energetisch, immer in Aktion 111. Starrt aufmerksam auf Unsichtbares 112. Schnappt nach (unsichtbaren) Fliegen 113. Jagt den eigenen Schwanz/Hinterteil 114. Jagt/verfolgt Schatten, Lichtpunkte, u.s.w. 115. Bellt unaufhörlich, wenn alarmiert oder aufgeregt 116. Leckt sich übertrieben 70 117. Leckt Leute oder Objekte übertrieben 118. Zeigt anderes bizarres, seltsames oder repetitives Verhalten * * Bitte beschreiben: 71 3.5 Appendix: Pictures during the TC in spring 72