Infant Behavior and Development
Transcription
Infant Behavior and Development
Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development What helps the mother of a preterm infant become securely attached, responsive and well-adjusted? Tracey Evans a,∗ , Koa Whittingham b,a,c , Roslyn Boyd b,c a School of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Queensland Cerebral Palsy and Rehabilitation Research Centre, The School of Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia c Queensland Children’s Medical Research Institute, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia b a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 12 July 2011 Received in revised form 28 September 2011 Accepted 18 October 2011 Keywords: Preterm infant Maternal attachment Maternal psychological symptoms Maternal responsiveness a b s t r a c t Objective: To investigate the relationship between the predictor variables of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support, and the criterion variables of maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness, after controlling for birth weight. Design: A quantitative survey study. Method: The participants were 127 mothers of preterm infants (delivery prior to 37 weeks gestation, <24 months corrected age) recruited through parent support organisations. The web-based survey included measures of: demographics, postpartum support and prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience)—all designed for this study, as well as maternal attachment, (MPAS) maternal responsiveness (MIRI), experiential avoidance (AAQ), maternal psychological symptoms (DASS-21) and relationship satisfaction (RQI). Three standard multiple regression analyses were conducted. Results: The combined effects of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support accounted for a significant 21% of variance in maternal attachment, Fch (4,121) = 8.01, p < .001, a significant 38% of variance in maternal psychological symptoms Fch (4,121) = 18.38, p < .001, and a significant 11% of variance in maternal responsiveness, Fch (4,121) = 3.78, p = .013 after controlling for birth weight. Conclusion: The four predictor variables predicted maternal attachment, psychological symptoms and responsiveness after controlling for birth weight, with experiential avoidance being the most important predictor, followed by prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience), relationship satisfaction, then postpartum support. This has implications for designing interventions to optimise attachment and responsiveness, and minimise psychological symptoms, in mothers of infants born preterm. © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. ∗ Corresponding author at: Queensland Cerebral Palsy and Rehabilitation Research Centre, The University of Queensland, Level 7, Building 6, Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, Herston, Brisbane 4029, Australia. Tel.: +61 7 3636 5361. E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Evans). 0163-6383/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.10.002 2 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 1. Introduction 1.1. Preterm birth In Western societies premature births account for approximately 5–9% of all live births (Greco et al., 2005). Preterm birth is associated with an elevated risk of neonatal complications that can cause significant mortality or morbidity (Laws & Hilder, 2008). The survival of preterm infants is promoted by admission to a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) (Goldberg, Perrotta, & Minde, 1986; Goldenberg & Rouse, 1998). The intense medical environment of the NICU can be difficult for the parents of preterm infants and the medical condition of the infant can enforce the separation of the parent from their infant. Parents may find themselves adopting a passive role as health professionals manage the day to day care of their infant and may find themselves experiencing feelings of guilt, helplessness and anxiety (Hughes, McCollum, Sheftel, & Sanchez, 1994; Miles, Funk, & Casper, 1992; Zeanah, Canger, & Jones, 1984). Preterm birth can have health and developmental consequences for infants not only immediately following the birth but also in the long-term (Aylward, Pfeiffer, Wright, & Verhulst, 1989; Clark, Woodward, Horwood, & Moor, 2008; Greco et al., 2005; Laws & Hilder, 2008; Lorenz, Wooliever, Jetton, & Paneth, 1998; McCormack, McCarton, Tonascia, & BrooksGunn, 1993). With preterm infant survival rates increasing (Goldenberg & Rouse, 1998) there will be a greater number of parents experiencing the difficulties adjusting to parenthood in the NICU environment, as well as the ongoing health and developmental problems more common in the very preterm infant. Mothers of infants born preterm are at risk for attachment difficulties (Amankwaa, Pickler, & Boonmee, 2007; Borghini et al., 2006; Nagata, Nagai, Sobajima, Ando, & Honjo, 2004; Tideman, Nilsson, Smith, & Stjernqvist, 2002), increased psychological symptoms (Davis, Edwards, Mohay, & Wollin, 2003; Ellestad et al., 2007; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001), and decreased responsiveness (Amankwaa et al., 2007; Feely, Gottlieb, & Zelkowitz, 2005; Landry, Smith, Swank, Assel, & Vellet, 2001; Zelkowitz, Bardin, & Papageorgiou, 2007). 1.2. Attachment Attachment is a reciprocal bond between parent and infant (Bowlby, 1988). The way in which these bonds develop and are organised in infancy has a key role in determining mental health in adulthood. Insecure attachment patterns are more likely if the mother is less responsive (Bowlby, 1958; Mills-Koonce, Gariepy, Sutton, & Cox, 2008; Van Ijzendoorn, Goldberg, Kroonenberg, & Frankel, 1992) or has increased psychological symptoms (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Anisfeld, Casper, Nozyce, & Cunningham, 1990; Bowlby, 1988; Mills-Koonce et al., 2008; Van Ijzendoorn et al., 1992). Mothers and preterm infants are at risk of attachment difficulties (Borghini et al., 2006; Forcada-Guex, Borghini, Pierrehumbert, Ansermet, & Muller-Nix, 2010; Tideman et al., 2002) because preterm birth disrupts normal physical contact between the mother and the infant (Amankwaa et al., 2007) and mothers may withdraw from their preterm infant due to distress (Miles, Holditch-Davis, & Burchinal, 1999). This may inhibit the mother’s caregiving behavioural system, as her desire and ability to provide protection for her preterm infant is interrupted. This can affect the mother’s attachment representations and the child’s attachment patterns (George & Solomon, 2008). Borghini et al. (2006) found at 6months only 20% of mothers with preterm infants had a secure attachment representation compared to 53% for the term comparison group. At the 18-month follow-up, the figures were 30% and 57%, respectively. 1.3. Maternal psychological symptoms Preterm birth is a risk factor for a mother developing postnatal depression (Psychosocial Paediatrics Committee, 2004) and increased psychological symptoms (Davis et al., 2003; Ellestad et al., 2007; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001), with preterm birth accounting for 24% of the total variance in depression (Blumberg, 1980). This increased risk is likely due to a combination of factors including the elevated infant risk of mortality and morbidity (Laws & Hilder, 2008), the enforced separation of the parent from their infant, an extended hospital stay and costs (Kaufman & Fairchild, 2004) and the crisis atmosphere of the NICU (Zeanah et al., 1984). Increased maternal psychological symptoms are a risk factor for attachment difficulties and decreased maternal responsiveness (Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Nagata et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). 1.4. Maternal responsiveness Maternal responsiveness or sensitivity is a sequence of synchronous mutual exchanges or expressions between the mother and her infant (Landry et al., 2001). Infancy is a particularly critical time where mothers need to respond in a sensitive and warm manner as this provides a strong basis for the child’s later development including the development of secure attachment patterns (Ainsworth et al., 1978). A preterm birth can also have a negative impact on maternal responsiveness because the behavioural cues of a preterm infant may be less noticeable and more difficult to understand (Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). Eckerman, Hsu, Molitor, Leung, T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 3 and Goldstein (1999) found only 33% of very low birth weight (VLBW) and 21% of low birth weight (LBW) infants (n = 47) showed strong positive arousal cues and weak negative arousal cues in a standardised peekaboo game compared to 69% of full-term infants in the comparison group (n = 32). Over 61% of the preterm infants failed to show a full smile compared to less than 16% of full-term infants. Responsiveness may also be disrupted by maternal psychological symptoms (Amankwaa et al., 2007; Feely et al., 2005; Landry et al., 2001; Zelkowitz et al., 2007) for which mothers of infants born preterm are at risk (Davis et al., 2003; Ellestad et al., 2007; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). Three studies (Feely et al., 2005; Zelkowitz et al., 2007; Zelkowitz, Papageorgiou, Bardin, & Wang, 2009) using different sample groups and different time measurement periods of two weeks, nine months and 24 months, showed that mother’s of preterm infants with high levels of anxiety displayed low levels of maternal responsiveness when mother–infant interaction was observed. In order to effectively intervene it is important to identify predictors in the relationship between prematurity and maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. One potential predictor from a psychosocial perspective is experiential avoidance. Experiential avoidance is a person’s unwillingness to experience certain private experiences, for example, emotions, images, memories, thoughts and bodily sensations (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996). The person takes action to alter the form, frequency or contexts in which the experiences occur, even when the avoidance can cause behavioural harm (Hayes et al., 1996). Increased levels of experiential avoidance can lead to increased levels of depression, anxiety, psychopathology and trauma (Greco et al., 2005; Marcks & Woods, 2005; Reddy, Pickett, & Orcutt, 2006; Sloan, 2004; Spira et al., 2007). Greco et al. (2005) examined the responses of sixty-six mothers of preterm infants who completed the Parental Stressor Scale (PSS), Parenting Stress Index-Short Form, Post traumatic Stress Checklist for Civilians and the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ) when their infants were aged between 3 and 36 months-of-age. They found maternal experiential avoidance partially mediated the relationship between NICU stress and maternal well-being after discharge, with a positive correlation between experiential avoidance and maternal stress and trauma symptoms (Greco et al., 2005). From a behavioural systems perspective, marital satisfaction may also be a predictor as research has shown marital satisfaction to increase maternal sensitivity and attachment (Belsky, Crnic, & Gable, 1995; Feldman, Weller, Sirota, & Eidelman, 2003). In addition, mothers reported as depressed expressed a decrease in marital satisfaction (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 1998). Further, preterm birth can impact upon relationship satisfaction (Belsky, 1985; Zelkowitz et al., 2007). Zelkowitz et al. (2007) found that for both mothers and fathers the quality of the marital relationship was significantly, negatively related to anxiety and positively related to parental responsiveness. Another possible predictor arises from the psychosocial association between prenatal expectations and a negative transition to parenthood, when the mother had underestimated the personal impact of the birth of the infant (Feldman & Nash, 1984) or the mother’s experience was negative compared to her prenatal expectations (Harwood, McLean, & Durkin, 2007). A preterm birth is likely to be completely different to the positive, romanticized expectations held during pregnancy (Feldman & Nash, 1984). Again from a behavioural systems perspective, postpartum support is a potential predictor of maternal attachment, responsiveness and adjustment. Postpartum support refers to all assistance received by the mother in the postpartum period (Logsdon, McBride, & Birkimer, 1994). It is one of the most important predictors of postpartum depression and maternal responsiveness in mothers of newborn infants (Beck, 2001) including mothers of infants born preterm (Younger, Kendell, & Pickler, 1997). Further, postpartum support may play a key role in assisting mothers of infants born preterm in achieving mastery in parenting tasks. 1.5. The current study The aim of this study was to test four potential predictors of maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. Four potential predictors that may be modifiable by intervention were chosen, experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (as compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. It was hypothesised that after controlling for birth weight, these predictors would predict the quality of maternal attachment, psychological symptoms and responsiveness. Specifically, as experiential avoidance increased, maternal psychological symptoms would increase and the quality of maternal attachment and responsiveness would decrease; as relationship satisfaction decreased, the quality of maternal attachment and responsiveness would decrease and maternal psychological symptoms would increase; as the difference between prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) increased, maternal psychological symptoms would increase and the quality of maternal attachment and responsiveness would decrease; and as postpartum support decreased, the quality of maternal attachment and responsiveness would decrease and maternal psychological symptoms would increase. Giving birth to a preterm infant can have a negative impact on maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. These negative effects can have both short- and long-term implications for both the mother and the child, and on the relationship that exists between them. It is important therefore to investigate factors that may improve the mother’s psychological symptoms, responsiveness and attachment to potentially decrease the negative effects for the mother–infant dyad. 4 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 Table 1 Demographics of mother and infant in the preterm sample (n = 127). Variable (mother) M SD Age (years) Ethnicity (%) Caucasian European Asian Marital status (%) Married De facto Education (%) University postgraduate University undergraduate Trade/apprenticeship High school grade 12 High school grade 10 2 Less than grade 10 Income (%) Less than $25,000 $25,000–50,000 $50,000–70,000 $70,000–100,000 Over $100,000 Employment status (%) Employed full-time Employed part-time Home duties Unemployed History of preterm births, n (%) yes History of miscarriages, n (%) yes 32.47 7.63 Variable (infant) Gender, n (%) of male Gestational age at birth (weeks) Birth weight, n (%) <1000 (g) >1000–<1500 (g) >1500–<2500 (g) >2500 (g) Birth order First born Second born Third born or later Twins, n (%) Duration of hospitalisation (days) Number 117 (93%) 8 (6%) 1 (1%) 102 (80%) 25 (20%) 29 (23%) 43 (34%) 31 (24%) 10 (8%) 9 (7%) 5 (4%) 2 (1%) 9 (7%) 31 (24%) 41 (32%) 43 (34%) 18 (14%) 33 (26%) 76 (60%) 0 (0%) 20 (16%) 21 (17%) M SD 30.40 3.53 Number 74 (54%) 36 (26%) 34 (25%) 55 (40%) 13 (9%) 82 (66%) 24 (19%) 19 (15%) 22 (16%) 58 6.14 2. Method 2.1. Participants The study included 127 mothers who had given birth to preterm infants (gestational age < 37 weeks, M = 30.40, SD = 3.53). They were eligible for the study if their child/children were less than 24 months corrected age, to ensure the preterm birth was relatively recent when recalling the experience in the self-report sections of the questionnaire. In addition, they had to be married or in a defacto relationship so they could complete the Relationship Satisfaction measure. They were recruited via preterm parent support groups within Australia, for example, the Preterm Infants’ Parents’ Association in Queensland, and also by word-of-mouth. Participant characteristics are displayed in Table 1. 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Demographics questionnaire The demographics questionnaire was designed for the purposes of this study and included basic demographic information, details of birth and hospital stay as well as baby characteristics. No participants were excluded on information given for other diagnoses or significant health complications. T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 5 2.2.2. Postpartum support questionnaire The researchers designed the postpartum support questionnaire for the purposes of this study as one did not previously exist. Questions were scored on a five-point Likert scale from ‘1 strongly disagree’ to ‘5 strongly agree’, with a score range of 9–45 for the total score. It included questions on how easy or difficult the hospital environment and staff made it for the mother to visit her baby, and the amount of emotional and practical support the participant received from their partner, family and friends in the postpartum period. Internal consistency for this study was high, ˛ = .81. 2.2.3. Prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) questionnaire The researchers of this study designed the prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) questionnaire to determine the difference between the participant’s prenatal expectations of their experience with their baby, to their actual experience with their baby following the preterm delivery. Questions were scored on a five-point Likert scale from ‘1 strongly disagree’ to ‘5 strongly agree’. Prenatal and postnatal item scores were added together separately, and then the prenatal scores were subtracted from the postnatal scores producing a score range for the total score of −40 to 40. This resulted in a ‘difference’ score, with negative scores reflecting a large difference and positive scores reflecting a small difference between prenatal expectations and postnatal experiences. Internal consistency for this study was high for both prenatal expectations ˛ = .89 and postnatal experience ˛ = .87. 2.2.4. Maternal postnatal attachment scale (Condon & Corkingdale, 1998) This self-report scale quantitatively measures a mother’s emotional response to her infant by assessing the intensity and frequency of her subjective experiences in four indicators of attachment. These indicators are pleasure in proximity, tolerance, need-gratification and protection, and knowledge acquisition. The scale consists of 19 self-report items scored on a five-point scale, with 1 indicating low attachment and 5 indicating high attachment. Scores for each item are added together to form an attachment score ranging from 19 to 95, with higher scores indicating higher attachment levels. The scale has high internal consistency ˛ = .78, ˛ = .79 and ˛ = .78 at four weeks, 4 months, and 8 months, respectively (Condon & Corkingdale, 1998). Test–retest reliability is also high with Pearson’s correlation coefficient reported at .86. The scale was found to be a valid measure of attachment as it was significantly positively related to the AQS, which is a behavioural indicator of caregiver–infant attachment. Internal consistency for this study was high, .79. 2.2.5. Maternal infant responsiveness instrument (MIRI; Amankwaa & Pickler, 2007) This instrument is a 22-item self-report questionnaire measuring maternal responsiveness to infant cues. Specifically, it measures the mother’s recognition of her own responses, the mother’s recognition of her infant’s responses to her, and any difficulties she notices in responsiveness. It is scored on a five-point Likert scale from ‘1 strongly agree’ to ‘5 strongly disagree’, with a score range of 22–110. Face and content validity were established using advanced practice nurse practitioners and maternal child nursing experts (Amankwaa & Pickler, 2007). The scale has good internal consistency ˛ = .86 (Amankwaa & Pickler, 2007) and ˛ = .83 (Drake, Humenick, Amankwaa, Younger, & Roux, 2007). Internal consistency for this study was high, ˛ = .84. 2.2.6. The acceptance and action questionnaire (AAQ; Hayes et al., 2004) The AAQ is a nine-item standard measure of experiential avoidance. Each item is scored on a 7-point Likert scale from ‘1 never true’ to ‘7 always true’, with a score range of 16–112. It focuses on five main areas: taking action when inhibitory thoughts or feelings are being experienced, the presence of anxiety, worry or negative evaluations of private events and the control of these, the ability to distance yourself from the actual content of negative evaluations, using worry or day-dreaming to regulate behaviour, and using negative comparisons to cope with your life. The questionnaire is a good measure of experiential avoidance with good internal consistency ˛ = .70, a test–retest reliability of .64, and several significant correlations with related constructs, including thought suppression and social desirability indicating high construct validity (Hayes et al., 2004). Internal consistency for this study was high, ˛ = .71. 2.2.7. The depression anxiety stress scale-21 (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) The DASS-21 is the reduced version of the DASS-42 (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). It contains 21 self-report items reflecting the frequency or severity of the participant’s experiences with depression, anxiety and stress over the past week. Each item is rated on a four-point Likert Scale from ‘0 Did not apply to me at all’ to ‘3 Applied to me very much, or most of the time’, with a score range of 0–63. The DASS-21 has been shown to have high internal consistency ˛ = .83, ˛ = .78 and ˛ = .87 for depression, anxiety and stress, respectively (Norton, 2007). The scale has high convergent validity with other measures of similar constructs: r = .76 between the DASS depression scale and the BDI, r = .74 between the DASS anxiety scale and the Beck Anxiety Scale and r = .74 between the DASS stress scale and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Gloster et al., 2008). Internal consistency for this study was high, ˛ = .93. 2.2.8. Relationship quality index questionnaire (RQI; Norton, 1983) This six-item questionnaire has been adapted from the quality marriage index (Norton, 1983), and contains evaluative variables of relationship satisfaction. Each item is measured on a 7-point Likert scale from ‘1 very strongly disagree’ to ‘7 very strongly agree’, with a score range of 6–45. Responses are added together with low scores representing less relationship 6 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations between MA, MPS and MR, and BW, EA, RS, PE and PS. MA MPS MR BW EA RS PE PS M SD M BW EA RS PE PS 81.05 33.50 98.17 2.34 56.48 36.07 14.61 36.54 7.82 10.50 9.48 .96 11.89 10.08 9.77 6.50 1 1 1 −.08 −.01 .02 1 −.39*** .49*** −.33*** .01 1 .18* −.41*** .09 −.03 −.26* 1 −.28* .34*** −.09 −.26 .23* −.14 1 .15 −.32*** .12 −.25 −.29*** .35*** −.15 1 MA: maternal attachment; MPS: maternal psychological symptoms; MR: maternal responsiveness; BW: birth weight; EA: experiential avoidance; RS: relationship satisfaction; PE: prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience); PS: postpartum support. * p < .05. *** p < .001. satisfaction and high scores representing greater relationship satisfaction. The median internal consistency for the six items is ˛ = .76 (Norton, 1983). Convergent and discriminant validity was significant at the p < .01 level with the DAS and the Relationship Satisfaction Questionnaire (Heyman, Sayers, & Bellack, 1994). Internal consistency for this study was high, ˛ = .97. 2.3. Procedure Ethics clearance was obtained from the University of Queensland, School of Psychology, ethics clearance number: 10PSYCH-4-72-JM. A web-based questionnaire was created and uploaded onto a website specially created for the study. Emails were distributed to 20 preterm support organisations, requesting their help in the recruitment of mother’s of preterm infants by distributing an information letter to parents. The information letter invited the mothers to participate in the study by completing the web-based questionnaire. The web-site address was also included in the letter as well as the researchers contact details for any further information. A web-based distribution method was chosen to maximise participant convenience. The questionnaire took participants approximately 20 min to complete. Participants were informed their responses would be anonymous and would include questions relating to their expectations before the preterm birth, their experiences during hospitalisation, coping strategies, their feelings, their spousal relationship, and their current feelings about parenting. 2.4. Analysis Three hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted, with birth weight entered first to control for the severity of the prematurity, and then experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support entered next as the predictor variables. Maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness were entered separately as the criterion variables. The analysis was conducted using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 17.00. 3. Results The assumptions were found not to be violated. The pattern of missing data was random and formed less than 5% of the data set. Where participants omitted a single response on a questionnaire, mean substitution was performed by inserting that participant’s mean score for that questionnaire as a conservative procedure for obtaining a value (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Logarithmic transformations to account for skewness and kurtosis did not change the interpretation of the data, nor did the removal of outliers and therefore the original data are reported. 3.1. Hierarchical regression one with maternal attachment as the criterion variable There were significant correlations between the predictor variables of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction and prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience), and the criterion variable maternal attachment. Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for correlations between the variables. Results of the analysis of regression at step 1 showed F (1,125) = .75, p = .388, indicating a non-significant 6% (−2% adjusted) of variance in maternal attachment was due to birth weight. At step 2 the analysis showed Fch (4,121) = 8.01, p < .001, indicating a further significant 21% (18% adjusted) of variance in maternal attachment was due to the inclusion of the combined effects of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. At step 2 with all the predictors in the analysis, it showed F (5,121) = 6.59, p < .001, indicating a significant amount of variance in maternal attachment was due to the combined effects of birth weight, experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. Experiential T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 7 Table 3 Hierarchical multiple regression statistics with BW, EA, RS, PE and PS as predictor variables and MA, MPS and MR as the criterion variables at step 1 and 2. MA B ˇ sr2 BW EA RS PE PS S1 S2 −1.19 −.22 .06 −.19 −.06 −.15 −.33*** .08 −.24* −.05 .02 .09 .00 .05 .00 MPS B BW EA RS PE PS S1 S2 MR BW EA RS PE PS S1 S2 .16 .30 −.27 .24 −.15 B .28 −.26 −.01 −.00 .06 ˇ sr2 .02 .34*** −.26* .22* −.09 .00 .10 .06 .04 .01 ˇ sr2 .03 −.32* −.01 −.00 .04 .00 .09 .00 .00 .00 R R2 Adj R2 R2 ch Intercept .08 .46 .01 .21 .00 .18 .21*** 98.82 R R2 Adj R2 R2 ch Intercept .01 .62 .00 .38 −.01 .35 .38*** 27.77 R R2 Adj R2 R2 ch Intercept .02 .33 .00 .11 −.01 .08 .11* 110.39 MA: maternal attachment; MPS: maternal psychological symptoms; MR: maternal responsiveness; BW: birth weight; EA: experiential avoidance; RS: relationship satisfaction; PE: prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience); PS: postpartum support; S1: step 1; S2: step 2. * p < .05. *** p < .001. avoidance had a significant 9% unique contribution to the variance for maternal attachment, t (127) = −3.80, p < .001. Prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) had a significant 5% unique contribution to the variance for maternal attachment t (127) = −2.70, p = .008. Birth weight t (127) = −1.67, p = .098, relationship satisfaction t (127) = .87, p = .384 and postpartum support t (127) = −.50, p = .617, did not have a significant unique contribution to the variance for maternal attachment. There was no additional shared variance. The size and direction of the relationships indicates higher levels of experiential avoidance and a negative difference between prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) are associated with lower levels of maternal attachment. The 95% confidence intervals were calculated for birth weight (−2.600 to .223), experiential avoidance (−.332 to −.105), relationship satisfaction (−.075 to .194), prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) (−.328 to −.050) and postpartum support (−.276 to .164), showing experiential avoidance and prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) to have significant ˇ weights. The relative weights for each predictor were calculated to indicate the relative importance of each predictor. It was found birth weight had a weighting of 4%, experiential avoidance 60%, relationship satisfaction 6% prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) 30%, and postpartum support 0%. Table 3 shows the unstandardised regression coefficients (B), the standardised regression coefficients (ˇ), the squared semi-partial correlations (sr2 ), R2 , adjusted R2 , R2 ch and intercept. 3.2. Hierarchical regression two with maternal psychological symptoms as the criterion variable There were significant correlations between the predictor variables of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support, and the criterion variable maternal psychological symptoms. Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for correlations between the variables. Results of the analysis of regression at step 1 showed F (1,125) = .01, p = .914, indicating 0% (−8% adjusted) of variance in maternal psychological symptoms was due to birth weight. At step 2 the analysis showed Fch (4,121) = 18.38, p < .001, indicating a further significant 38% (35% adjusted) of variance in maternal psychological symptoms was due to the inclusion of the combined effects of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. At step 2 with all the predictors in the analysis, it showed F (5,121) = 14.71, p < .001, indicating a significant amount of variance in maternal psychological symptoms was due to the combined effects of birth weight, experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. Experiential avoidance had a significant 10% unique contribution to the variance for maternal psychological symptoms, t (127) = 4.43, p < .001. Relationship satisfaction had a significant 6% unique contribution to the variance for maternal psychological symptoms, t (127) = −3.31, p = .001. Prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) 8 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 had a significant 4% unique contribution to the variance for maternal psychological symptoms, t (127) = 2.81, p = .006. Birth weight t (127) = .19, p = .851 and postpartum support t (127) = −1.13, p = .259 did not have a significant unique contribution to the variance for maternal psychological symptoms. There was no additional shared variance. The size and direction of the relationship indicates high levels of experiential avoidance, low levels of relationship satisfaction and a negative difference between prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) are associated with higher levels of maternal psychological symptoms. The 95% confidence intervals were calculated for birth weight (−1.524 to 1.846), experiential avoidance (.168 to .439), relationship satisfaction (−.430 to −.108), prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) (.069 to .400) and postpartum support (−.413 to .112), showing all the variables except for postpartum support to have significant ˇ weights. The relative weights for each predictor were calculated to indicate the relative importance of each predictor. It was found birth weight had a weighting of 0%, experiential avoidance 44%, relationship satisfaction 28%, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) 20% and postpartum support 8%. Table 3 shows the unstandardised regression coefficients (B), the standardised regression coefficients (ˇ), the squared semi-partial correlations (sr2 ), R2 , adjusted R2 , R2 ch and intercept. 3.3. Hierarchical regression three with maternal responsiveness as the criterion variable There was a significant correlation between the predictor variable experiential avoidance and the criterion variable maternal responsiveness. Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for correlations between the variables. Results of the analysis of regression at step 1 showed F (1,125) = .04, p = .847, indicating 0% (−1% adjusted) of variance in maternal responsiveness was due to birth weight. At step 2 the analysis showed Fch (4,121) = 3.78, p = .013, indicating further a significant 11% (8% adjusted) of the variance in maternal responsiveness was due to the inclusion of the combined effects of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. At step 2 with all the predictors in the analysis, it showed F (5,121) = 3.04, p = .13, indicating a significant amount of variance in maternal responsiveness was due to the combined effects of birth weight, experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support. Experiential avoidance had a significant 9% unique contribution to the variance for maternal responsiveness, t (127) = −3.48, p = .001. Birth weight t (127) = .30, p = .763, relationship satisfaction t (127) = −.12, p = .907, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience), t (127) = −.02, p = .984 and postpartum support, t (127) = .40, p = .694 did not have a significant unique contribution to the variance for maternal responsiveness. There was no additional shared variance. The size and direction of the relationship indicates low levels of experiential avoidance are associated with higher levels of maternal responsiveness. The 95% confidence intervals were calculated for birth weight (−1.541 to 2.098), experiential avoidance (−.404 to −.111), relationship satisfaction (−.184 to .163), prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) (−.180 to .177) and postpartum support (−.227 to .340), showing only experiential avoidance to have a significant ˇ weight. The relative weights for each predictor were calculated to indicate the relative importance of each predictor. It was found birth weight had a weighting of 0%, experiential avoidance 96%, relationship satisfaction 0%, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) 0% and postpartum support 4%. Table 3 shows the unstandardised regression coefficients (B), the standardised regression coefficients (ˇ), the squared semi-partial correlations (sr2 ), R2 , adjusted R2 , R2 ch and intercept. 4. Discussion This study found after controlling for birth weight, the combined effects of experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction, prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) and postpartum support accounted for a significant amount of variance in maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. This is consistent with previous research in mothers of infants born preterm (Amankwaa et al., 2007; Davis et al., 2003; Ellestad et al., 2007; Feely et al., 2005; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Nagata et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Zelkowitz et al., 2007, 2009). Experiential avoidance was found to be a significant unique contributor to maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. Previous research suggests that preterm birth is a stressor for the mother of the preterm infant (Nagata et al., 2004; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Zeanah et al., 1984) The results of this study suggest that mothers who attempt to cope with this stress by engaging in experiential avoidance may put themselves at risk of increased psychological symptoms, consistent with previous research (Greco et al., 2005; Marcks & Woods, 2005; Reddy et al., 2006; Sloan, 2004; Spira et al., 2007). Further, this study suggests that they also may put themselves at risk of attachment difficulties and decreased responsiveness to their infant. To the author’s knowledge this is the first study to demonstrate a relationship between experiential avoidance, attachment and responsiveness. This relationship makes sense. A mother’s experiential avoidance attempts around cognitions and emotions regarding the preterm birth may decrease the mother’s sensitivity to infant cues, thus decreasing maternal responsiveness and impacting on the ability to form a secure bond with her infant. Relationship satisfaction was found to be a significant unique contributor to maternal psychological symptoms only. This is consistent with previous research establishing a relationship between relationship satisfaction and psychological symptoms (Cicchetti et al., 1998; Zelkowitz et al., 2007). A positive marital relationship acted as a buffer in the stressful situation of a preterm birth. T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 9 Prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) were also found to be a significant unique contributor to maternal attachment and maternal psychological symptoms. Consistent with previous research (Harwood et al., 2007) mothers experienced a better outcome in terms of attachment and psychological symptoms if their prenatal expectations were consistent with their postnatal experiences. This research has important implications for interventions to support mothers of preterm infants. Interventions to prevent problems with maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness could be implemented whilst the infant is in the NICU. Mothers could be screened for experiential avoidance and relationship difficulties with interventions implemented to improve the marital relationship and decrease experiential avoidance. A brief behavioural marital therapy intervention and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) could be utilised for this purpose. ACT is a cognitivebehavioural intervention that targets experiential avoidance as a key mechanism of change. Further research including a randomised controlled trial of such interventions to establish efficacy in the prevention of psychological symptoms, attachment difficulties and decreased responsiveness in mother of infants born preterm should be conducted. Another implication of this research would be the inclusion of information on preterm birth in standard antenatal care to provide mothers with some expectation and knowledge of the implications of a preterm birth. A correct balance needs to be found here between alarming low-risk expectant mothers and providing appropriate preparation to women who will go on to have a preterm birth. The information should be presented in matter of fact, positive light that normalises the experience of preterm birth. As this study found no significant results for postpartum support, it is not a recommendation of this paper to address this area as mother’s of preterm infants indicated they felt adequately supported by the hospital staff and the hospital environment. It is likely that significant results were not found for postpartum support because efforts are made by hospital staff in the existing NICU environment to support parents well. A limitation of this study was that the results relied solely on maternal self-report measures, potentially allowing for response bias and shared method variance. In addition, this study is correlational only, representing a snapshot in time. Therefore it is impossible to draw definite conclusions about causality. Also the results may not be generalizable to mothers of preterm infants as there is a possible self-selection bias due to the sample being recruited through parent support organisations via the web. This sample also reported to be of a high socioeconomic status. Future research taking a longitudinal approach would be beneficial. A further limitation was the absence of a child attachment measure to assess the attachment of the child to the mother. It is not possible from this study to predict whether the attachment of the mother to the infant also reflected the attachment of the infant to the mother. Future research could utilise Ainsworth’s Strange Situation paradigm when the infant was 12 months-of-age to assess the attachment pattern of the infant to the mother. 5. Conclusion A preterm birth impacts negatively on maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. After controlling for birth weight, this study has identified experiential avoidance, relationship satisfaction and prenatal expectations (compared to postnatal experience) as predictors of maternal attachment, maternal psychological symptoms and maternal responsiveness. This suggests that mothers of infants born preterm could be better supported by providing Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and behavioural marital therapy in the NICU and in ensuring appropriate prenatal expectations are present in antenatal care. This could potentially improve the mother’s attachment and responsiveness to her infant, and decrease the negative psychological symptoms she may experience. Future research could focus on exploring the most effective methods of delivering these interventions to the mother whilst she is still caring for her preterm infant and establishing the efficacy of such interventions. The ultimate aim of these interventions would be to improve the relationship between preterm infants and their mothers, therefore promoting better outcomes for the mother–infant dyads. Acknowledgements We acknowledge the mothers who participated in our study, and the following preterm baby support groups: Feather Weight Club, Preterm Infant’s Parents Association (PIPA) (QLD), Central Queensland Premmies, Life’s Little Treasures, L’il Aussie Prems, Bub Born Early, Bonnie Babes Foundation and The Bub Hub. References Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E. & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum. Anisfeld, E., Casper, V., Nozyce, M. & Cunningham, N. (1990). Does infant carrying promote attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the development of attachment. Child Development: 61., (5), 1617–1627. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130769 Amankwaa, L. & Pickler, R. (2007). Measuring Maternal Responsiveness. ABNF Journal: 18., (1), 4–15. Retrieved from http://web.ebscohost. com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Amankwaa, L. C., Pickler, R. H. & Boonmee, J. (2007). Maternal responsiveness in mothers of preterm infants. Newborn and Infant Nursing Reviews: 7., (1), 25–30. doi:10.1053/j.nainr.2006.12.001 Aylward, G., Pfeiffer, S. I., Wright, A. & Verhulst, S. J. (1989). Outcome studies of low birth weight infants published in the last decade: A meta-analysis. Journal of Pediatrics: 115., (4), 515–520. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Beck, C. (2001). Predictors of postpartum depression. Nursing Research: 50., (5), 275–285. Retrieved from http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Belsky, J. (1985). Exploring individual differences in marital change across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family: 47., (4), 1037–1044. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au 10 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 Belsky, J., Crnic, K. & Gable, S. (1995). The determinants of co-parenting in families with toddler boys: Spousal differences and daily hassles. Child Development: 66., (3), 629–642. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Blumberg, N. L. (1980). Effects of neonatal risks, maternal attitude, and cognitive style on early postpartum adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology: 89., (2), 139–150. Retrieved from http://csaweb114v.csa.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Borghini, A., Pierrehumbert, B., Miljkovictch, R., Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M. & Ansermet, F. (2006). Mother’s attachment representations of their premature infant at 6 and 18 months after birth. Infant Mental Health Journal: 27., (5), 494–508. doi:10.1002/imhj.20103 Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis: 39., 350–373. Retrieved from http://www.pepweb.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Bowlby, J. (1988). Developmental psychiatry comes of age. The American Journal of Psychiatry: 145., (1), 1–10. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A. & Toth, S. L. (1998). Maternal depressive disorder and contextual risk: Contributions to the development of attachment insecurity and behaviour in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology: 10., (2), 283–300. doi:10.1017/S0954579498001618 Clark, C. A. C., Woodward, L. J., Horwood, L. J. & Moor, S. (2008). Development of emotional and behavioural regulation in children born extremely preterm and very preterm: Biological and social influences. Child Development: 79., (5), 1444–1462. doi:10.1111/j. 1467-8624.2008.01198.x Committee, P. P. (2004). Depression in pregnant women and mothers: How children are affected. Paediatrics and Child Health: 9., (8), 575–583. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Condon, J. T. & Corkingdale, C. J. (1998). The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology: 16., (1), 57–76. doi:10.1080/02646839808404558 Davis, L., Edwards, H., Mohay, H. & Wollin, J. (2003). The impact of very premature birth on the psychological health of mothers. Early Human Development: 73., (2), 61–70. doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(03)00073-2 Drake, E. E., Humenick, S. S., Amankwaa, L., Younger, J. & Roux, G. (2007). Predictors of maternal responsiveness. Journal of Nursing Scholarship: 39., (2), 119–125. doi:10.1111/j.1547-5069.2007.00156.x Eckerman, C. O., Hsu, H., Molitor, A., Leung, E. H. L. & Goldstein, R. F. (1999). Infant arousal in an en-face exchange with a new partner: Effects of prematurity and perinatal biological risk. Developmental Psychology: 35., (1), 283–297. Retrieved from http://csaweb114v.csa.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Ellestad, S., Meyer, W., Sinclair, T., Swamy, G., Heine, P. & Murtha, A. (2007). Maternal psychological impact of very preterm birth. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology: 197., (6), 94–102. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2007.10.317 Feely, N., Gottlieb, L. & Zelkowitz, P. (2005). Infant, mother and contextual predictors of mother-very low birth weight infant interaction at nine months of age. Journal of Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics: 26., (1), 24–32. Retrieved from http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Feldman, S. S. & Nash, S. C. (1984). The transition from expectancy to parenthood: Impact of the first-born child on men and women. Sex Roles: 11., (1), 61–78. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Feldman, R., Weller, A., Sirota, L. & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Testing a family intervention hypothesis: The contribution of mother–infant skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) to family interaction, proximity and touch. Journal of Family Psychology: 17., (1), 94–107. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.17.1.94 Forcada-Guex, M., Borghini, A., Pierrehumbert, B., Ansermet, K. & Muller-Nix, C. (2010). Prematurity, maternal posttraumatic stress and consequences on the mother–infant relationship. Early Human Development: 87., 21–26. George, C. & Solomon, J. (2008). The caregiving system: A behavioural approach to parenting. (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 833–856). New York: Guilford. Gloster, A. T., Rhoades, H. M., Novy, D., Klotsche, J., Senior, A., Kunik, M., et al. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 in older primary care patients. Journal of Affective Disorders: 110., (3), 248–259. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.01.023 Goldberg, S., Perotta, M. & Minde, K. (1986). Maternal behaviour and attachment in low-birth-weight twins and singletons. Child Development: 57., (1), 34–46. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Goldenberg, R. L. & Rouse, D. J. (1998). Prevention of premature birth. The New England Journal of Medicine: 339., (5), 313–320. Retrieved from http://proquest.umi.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Greco, L. A., Heffner, M., Poe, S., Ritchie, S., Polak, M. & Lynch, S. K. (2005). Maternal adjustment following preterm birth: Contributions of experiential avoidance. Behaviour Therapy: 36., (2), 177–184. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Harwood, K., McLean, N. & Durkin, K. (2007). First-time mother’s expectations of parenthood: What happens when optimistic expectations are not matched by later experiences? Developmental Psychology: 43., (1), 1–12. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.1 Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record: 54., (4), 553–578. Retrieved from http://proquest.umi.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M. & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology: 64., (6), 1152–1168. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152 Hughes, M., McCollum, J., Sheftel, D. & Sanchez, G. (1994). How parents cope with the experience of neonatal intensive care. Child Health Care: 23., 1–14. Retrieved from http://web.ebscohost.library.uq.edu.au Heyman, R. E., Sayers, S. L. & Bellack, A. S. (1994). Global marital satisfaction versus marital adjustment: An empirical comparison of three measures. Journal of Family Psychology: 8., (4), 432–446. Retrieved from http://ft.csa.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Kaufman, D. & Fairchild, K. D. (2004). Clinical microbiology of bacterial and fungal sepsis in very-low-birth-weight infants. Clinical Microbiology Review: 17., (3), 638–680. doi:10.1128/CMR.17.3.638-680.2004 Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A. & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children’s development or is consistency across childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology: 37., (3), 387–403, doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.387. Laws, P. J., & Hilder, L. (2008). Australia’s mothers and babies 2006. Perinatal statistics series no. 22. Cat. No. PER 46. Sydney: AIHW National Perinatal Statistics Unit. Retrieved from http://www.aihw.gov.au/. Logsdon, M., McBride, M. & Birkimer, J. (1994). Social support and postpartum depression. Research in Nursing and Health: 17., (6), 449–457. doi:10.1002/nur.4770170608 Lorenz, J. M., Wooliever, D. E., Jetton, N. P. & Paneth, M. P. H. (1998). A quantitative review of mortality and developmental disability in extremely premature newborns. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine: 152., (5), 425–435. Retrieved from http://archpedi.ama-assn.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Lovibond, P. F. & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy: 33., (3), 335–343. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U Marcks, B. A. & Woods, W. D. (2005). A comparison of thought suppression to an acceptance-based technique in the management of personal intrusive thoughts: A controlled evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy: 43., (4), 433–445. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.005 McCormack, M. C., McCarton, C., Tonascia, J. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1993). Early educational intervention for very low birth weight infants: Results from the Infant Health and Development Program. Journal of Pediatrics: 123., (4), 527–532. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Miles, M. S., Funk, S. G. & Kasper, M. A. (1992). The stress response of mothers and fathers of preterm infants. Research in Nursing Health: 15., 261–269. Retrieved from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.library.uq.edu.au Miles, M., Holditch-Davis, D. & Burchinal, P. (1999). Distress and growth in mothers of medically fragile infants. Nursing Research: 48., (3), 129–138. Retrieved from http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gariepy, J., Sutton, K. & Cox, M. J. (2008). Changes in maternal sensitivity across the first three years: Are mothers from different attachment dyads differentially influenced by depressive symptomatology? Attachment and Human Development: 10., (3), 299–317. doi:10.1080/14616730802113612 Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A. & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–child interactions. Early Human Development: 79., (2), 145–158. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002 T. Evans et al. / Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 1–11 11 Nagata, M., Nagai, Y., Sobajima, H., Ando, T. & Honjo, S. (2004). Depression in the early postpartum period and attachment to children—In mothers of NICU infants. Infant and Child Development: 13., 93–110. doi:10.1002/icd.339 Norton, P. J. (2007). Depression anxiety and stress scales (DASS-21): psychometric analysis across four racial groups. Anxiety, Stress and Coping: 20., (3), 253–265. doi:10.1080/10615800701309279 Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family: 45., (1), 141–151. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au/stable/i214975 Poehlmann, J. & Fiese, B. H. (2001). The interaction of maternal and infant vulnerabilities on developing attachment relationships. Development and Psychopathology: 13., (1), 1–11. doi:10.1017/S0954579401001018 Reddy, M. K., Pickett, S. M. & Orcutt, H. K. (2006). Experiential avoidance as a mediator in the relationship between childhood psychological abuse and current mental health symptoms in college students. Journal of Emotional Abuse: 6., (1), 67–85. doi:10.1300/J135v06n01 04 Sloan, D. M. (2004). Emotion regulation in action: Emotional reactivity in experiential avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy: 42., (11), 1257–1270. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.006 Spira, A. P., Beaudreau, S. A., Jimenez, D., Kierod, K., Cusing, M. M., Gray, H. L., et al. (2007). Experiential avoidance, acceptance, and depression in dementia family caregivers. Clinical Gerontologist: 30., (4), 55–64. doi:10.1300/J018v30n04 04 Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Pearson Education. Tideman, E., Nilsson, A., Smith, G. & Stjernqvist, K. (2002). Longitudinal follow-up of children born preterm: The mother–child relationship in a 19-year perspective. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology: 20., (1), 43–55. doi:10.1080/02646830220106785 Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P. M. & Frankel, O. J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Development: 63., (4), 840–858. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Younger, J. B., Kendell, M. J. & Pickler, R. H. (1997). Mastery of stress in mothers of preterm infants. Journal of the Society of Pediatric Nurses: 2., (1), 29–35. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6155.1997.tb00197.x Zeanah, C. H., Canger, C. I. & Jones, D. (1984). Clinical approaches to traumatised parents: Psychotherapy in the intensive-care nursery. Child Psychiatry and Human Development: 14., (3), 158–169. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com.ezproxy.library.uq.edu.au Zelkowitz, P., Bardin, C. & Papageorgiou, A. (2007). Anxiety affects the relationship between parents and their very low birth weight infants. Infant Mental Health Journal: 28., (3), 296–313. doi:10.1002/imhj.20137 Zelkowitz, P., Papageorgiou, A., Bardin, C. & Wang, T. (2009). Persistent maternal anxiety affects the interaction between mothers and their very low birthweight children at 24 months. Early Human Development: 85., 51–58. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.06.010