dissertation final - Gradworks
Transcription
dissertation final - Gradworks
THE EFFECTS OF AN INTERDISCIPLINARY UNDERGRADUATE HUMAN BIOLOGY PROGRAM ON SOCIOSCIENTIFIC REASONING, CONTENT LEARNING, AND UNDERSTANDING OF INQUIRY Jennifer L. Eastwood Submitted to the faculty of the University Graduate School in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Indiana University, August 2010 UMI Number: 3423606 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. UMI 3423606 Copyright 2010 by ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This edition of the work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1346 Accepted by the Graduate Faculty, Indiana University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Doctoral Committee _______________________________________ Robert D. Sherwood, Ph.D. _______________________________________ Valarie Akerson, Ph.D. _______________________________________ Meredith Park Rogers, Ph.D. _______________________________________ Whitney M. Schlegel, Ph.D. May 10, 2010 ii © 2010 Jennifer L. Eastwood ALL RIGHTS RESERVED iii Acknowledgments I would like to thank my dissertation committee for their encouragement and guidance throughout this project. I am grateful to my advisor, Dr. Bob Sherwood, for giving careful feedback that strengthened my work. He always expressed confidence in me, which helped me to persist on the dissertation. I would like to thank Dr. Whitney Schlegel for sharing her passion for improving college science education and her creativity with the development of the Human Biology Program. I am grateful to have had the opportunity to work with Human Biology as well as to grow through sharing ideas with her over coffee. I would like to thank Dr. Valarie Akerson and Dr. Meredith Park Rogers for their feedback on my proposal, which helped me to design a better study, and their insightful comments on the dissertation, which will help guide my future research. I am grateful to all the Science Education faculty for their investment in me though teaching and research projects, which has helped me to learn the field. I would also like to thank Kristin Cook, Vanashri Nargund , and Cathy Smith who each provided special contributions to my project. Most of all, I would like to thank my family for supporting me throughout my entire degree. To my husband Bill, thank you for the countless times you put your work on hold so I could finish my dissertation! And thank you for not complaining when I rehashed my outline to you over and over. I could not have finished my degree without your support, flexibility, and wonderful care of our daughter. Thank you to Ashleigh for being a Science Ed kid and thank you to Andrew for arriving just on time for me to finish the defense! I love you all and thank you for inspiring me to finish my degree. iv Jennifer L. Eastwood The Effects of an Interdisciplinary Undergraduate Human Biology Program on Socioscientific Reasoning, Content Learning, and Understanding of Inquiry Preparing students to take informed positions on complex problems through critical evaluation is a primary goal of university education. Socioscientific issues (SSI) have been established as effective contexts for students to develop this competency, as well as reasoning skills and content knowledge. This mixedmethods study investigates the effects of an interdisciplinary undergraduate human biology program focused on the development of evidence-based reasoning to form personal commitments on SSI. Specifically, the study investigates how human biology majors differ from traditional biology majors in their reasoning with SSI, their perceptions of experiences with SSI, their understanding of scientific inquiry, their levels and perceptions of science content knowledge, and their general program perceptions. These outcomes were assessed through open-ended questionnaires on SSI and scientific inquiry and a basic biology concept test administered to 95 participants representing both programs and 16 semi-structured student interviews. Although the two groups did not differ significantly in their decisions or factors influencing their decisions in SSI, human biology majors showed higher levels of socioscientific reasoning, suggesting that learning contextualized in SSI helped them understand and reason with similar issues. While biology majors reported few experiences with socioscientific reasoning, human biology majors felt well equipped to reason with SSI and more likely to consider alternative perspectives in their decision making. Human biology majors also were more likely to view social science research as a form of inquiry and less likely to view scientific inquiry as purely experimental. No difference was found between groups in basic biology content knowledge, although human biology majors felt they were exposed to less detailed biology content. This exploratory study illustrates a novel approach to interdisciplinary, SSI-based science education at the college level. __________________________________________________ __________________________________________________ __________________________________________________ __________________________________________________ v TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACCEPTANCE PAGE ii COPYRIGHT PAGE iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS iv ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION v LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES x CHAPTER 1: Introduction 1 Rationale 1 Motivation for Research 3 Research Questions 5 CHAPTER 2: Background for Study 8 Theoretical Framework 8 Situated Cognition 8 Developmental Constructs in College Students 8 Literature Review 13 Socioscientific Issues 13 Interdisciplinary Learning 21 Collaborative Learning 27 Contextualized Learning 34 Relation to Study 43 CHAPTER 3: Method 45 Context of Study 45 SSI Group 45 Biology Comparison Group 55 vi Methodology 56 Worldview 56 Research Design 57 Participants 57 General Procedures of Data Collection 61 Data Collection for Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI 63 Data Analysis for Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI 64 Data Collection for Understanding of Scientific Inquiry 70 Data Analysis for Understanding of Scientific Inquiry 72 Data Collection for Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge 73 Data Analysis of Levels and Perceptions of Content Knowledge 74 Data Collection for General Perceptions of Major 75 Summary of Methods 76 CHAPTER 4: Results 77 Socioscientific Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI 77 Comparison of Decisions 77 Comparison of Factors Influencing Reasoning 78 Comparison of Reasoning 82 Comparison of Reasoning in DMQ Follow-up Questions 83 Perceptions of SSI in Majors 84 Understanding of Inquiry 89 Modified Views of Scientific Inquiry Questionnaire 89 Inquiry Portion of Interviews 102 vii Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge 106 Biology Concept Inventory 106 Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge Portion of Interview 107 Student Perceptions of Majors 110 Perceptions of Personal Outcomes 110 Perceptions of the Learning Environment 113 CHAPTER 5: Discussion 117 Review of Study 118 Socioscientific Issues 118 Socioscientific Reasoning 118 Consideration of Multiple Perspectives 122 Understanding of Scientific Inquiry 124 Views of Different Disciplines and Perspectives in Science 124 Views of the Scientific Method and Experiment 126 Data and Evidence 129 Tentativeness of Theory and Purpose of Theory 130 Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge 131 Perceptions of Majors 133 Perceptions of Personal Outcomes 133 Perceptions of the Learning Environment 136 Limitations of the Study 137 Major Findings and Implications 140 Future Directions 142 viii Conclusion 143 REFERENCES 145 APPENDIX A: Informed Consent Statement 152 APPENDIX B: Overhead Slide for Participant Recruitment 154 APPENDIX C: Demographic Sheet 155 APPENDIX D: Open-ended Questionnaire 157 APPENDIX E: Biology Concept Inventory 162 APPENDIX F: Semistructured Interview Protocol for Human Biology and Biology Majors 170 APPENDIX G: Coding Scheme for the Modified VOSI 172 APPENDIX H: Methods Matrix 178 CURRICULUM VITA 180 ix LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES Page Table 1. Demographic information for SSI and BIO participants. 60 Table 2. Rubric for reasoning and perspectives applied to DMQ 66 Table 3. Examples of scoring for reasoning scale. 67 Table 4. Examples of scoring for perspectives scale 68 Table 5. Assessment themes for stages of reflective judgment 70 Table 6. Percentages of SSI and BIO students by decision for questions on the modified DMQ 78 Table 7. Factors influencing reasoning in Climate change and policy question cluster of DMQ 80 Table 8. Factors influencing reasoning in Diet and health research/food choice question cluster of DMQ 80 Table 9. Factors influencing reasoning in Regulation of food or tobacco question cluster of DMQ 81 Table 10. Reasoning scores for SSI and BIO groups 82 Table 11. Perspectives scores for SSI and BIO groups 83 Table 12. Codes for VOSI Question 1 90 Table 13. Codes for VOSI Question 2 91 Table 14. Codes for VOSI Question 3a 92 Table 15. Codes for VOSI Question 3b 92 Table 16. Codes for VOSI Question 3c 93 Table 17. Codes for VOSI Question 4a 94 Table 18. Codes for VOSI Question 4b 94 Table 19. Codes for VOSI Question 4c 95 x Table 20. Codes for VOSI Question 5a 96 Table 21. Codes for VOSI Question 5b 97 Table 22. Codes for VOSI Question 6a 98 Table 23. Codes for VOSI Question 6b 99 Table 24. Codes for VOSI Question 7 100 Table 25. Codes for VOSI Question 8 101 Table 26. BCI scores for SSI and BIO groups 107 Figure 1. Convergence model of triangulation design for dissertation 76, 118 xi CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Rationale Preparing students to take informed positions on complex problems through critical evaluation is considered a primary goal of university education (Association of American Colleges and Universities, 2007; King & Kitchener, 1994; Baxter Magolda, 1999) and an important aspect of scientific literacy (Sadler & Zeidler, 2009; Roberts, 2007). In approaching contemporary problems, the ability to understand and position oneself in interdisciplinary issues is essential (Boix Mansilla & Duraising, 2007). This is especially true where the study of biology meets contemporary global problems. For example, to understand the nature of disease, it is essential to examine psychological and socioeconomic aspects as well as biology and pathology. Even when such socioscientific issues have been addressed in the classroom, however, studies have found that instruction has focused on subject matter knowledge, giving little attention to the importance of informed decision making (Lederman, 2003). In recent literature, the pedagogical framework of socioscientific issues (SSI), which integrates science concepts and their social significance, involving students in a community of dialogue, discussion, and debate, has been established as effective for students to develop their reasoning with the types of issues described, as well as content knowledge (Sadler, 2009; Zeidler, Sadler, Applebaum, & Callahan, 2009). The national initiative, Science for All Americans, calls for science education that promotes decision making for citizenship (Rutherford and Ahlgren, 1991). Consistent with this goal, a recent report of the Association of American Colleges and Universities (2007) asserts 1 The LEAP [Liberal Education and America’s Promise] National Leadership Council recommends, in sum, an education that intentionally fosters, across multiple fields of study, wide-ranging knowledge of science, cultures, and society; high-level intellectual and practical skills; an active commitment to personal and social responsibility; and the demonstrated ability to apply learning to complex problems and challenges (p. 4). The goals of the SSI movement overlap significantly with those of university educators to help students learn to integrate disciplines to responsibly approach real problems. Much of the literature on SSI has concentrated on decision making for citizenship, however, fluency in discussing socioscientific issues and integrating perspectives from different disciplinary fields will be especially important for those entering fields that will tackle problems like climate change and epidemics (Thompson Klein, 1990). The Institute of Medicine (IOM) has recognized a need for goals consistent with those of SSI in the training of physicians. The IOM calls for integration of the social sciences in medical curricula, considering half of the mortality in the US can be attributed to social factors, including diet, drug abuse, and lifestyle habits (2004). According to the IOM, “To make measurable improvements in the health of Americans, physicians must be equipped with the knowledge and skills from the behavioral and social sciences needed to recognize, understand, and effectively respond to patients as individuals, not just to their symptoms” (p. 16). Extending these goals to undergraduate science majors who will enter medical school and health careers, more research is needed into effective learning contexts for developing socioscientific reasoning and integration of different perspectives into decision making. In addition to the gap in the literature describing SSI as preparation for science careers, most studies of SSI contexts have focused on K-12 students, but few have been 2 conducted with adults or college students (Bell & Lederman, 2003). Since many researchers have documented important changes in cognitive and ethical development, as well as reasoning approaches (Baxter Magolda, 1999; King & Kitchener, 1994, Perry, 1970), outcomes of SSI in college students are likely to differ from those of younger students. Also, very few studies have investigated SSI interventions spanning more than a unit or semester (Zeidler et al., 2009). This study addresses these gaps in the literature. Motivation for Research My interest in this research developed from my experiences as a student and instructor in college level biology courses. Reflecting on the very structured labs I taught and the courses I took as an undergraduate, I questioned the effectiveness of the predominant lecture-based approach in traditional biology courses. I began to read about student centered and active learning approaches such as case-based and collaborative learning, as well as SSI, which incorporates both of these approaches. As a student in science education, I became familiar with research studies documenting the success and conditions for these teaching strategies, observed courses employing them, and incorporated them into my own teaching. These experiences convinced me that students gain knowledge that is more meaningful to them and more transferrable to new situations when they have opportunities to discuss and explain concepts to peers and apply concepts to real problems. Through pre-dissertation research and in-depth discussion with a mentor who is an expert in biology education and physiology professor, I explored the outcomes and conditions necessary for effective case-based and team-based biology instruction. This experience impressed me because students increased their content knowledge by working 3 together and anecdotally reported deeper and longer-lasting understanding of the content as compared to their other biology courses, but expressed extreme frustration with team work. I was convinced that situated and collaborative learning were highly effective in teaching science content, but they were also difficult to employ successfully since they deviated from students’ expectations and comfort levels. In addition, the biology professor I worked with introduced me to literature on cognitive and ethical development in college students. Through reading and discussing the work of Perry (1979) and Baxter Magolda (1994), I was convinced that college students exhibit consistent patterns in their understanding of knowledge and approaches to learning as they develop in a college environment. Understanding these patterns can help instructors to grasp how their students are thinking and begin instruction from what their students know. Understanding these patterns helps instructors scaffold their students. It also helps instructors articulate desired outcomes for students, such as understanding knowledge as a product of human inquiry rather than mandated facts to memorize. I became interested in the Human Biology program, since the program director (also the biology professor who had mentored me) and faculty had carefully designed the program to enact the strategies I have discussed and reflect a theoretical position based on developmental research. The program used an interdisciplinary approach to help students learn to reason and take positions on controversial issues with both scientific and social implications. I saw the goals of the program and the pedagogical strategies employed as consistent with SSI, and felt that description of Human Biology core courses could contribute to the SSI literature by illustrating how SSI was taught and how students 4 responded within a college level learning environment. Based on my experience and prior knowledge, I approached this study with a personal bias toward the Human Biology program. Since the program goals, pedagogical strategies, and curricula were consistent with effective practices from the literature, I expected Human Biology students to have better learning gains and epistemological development than students in a traditional biology major. However, I recognized the complexity of the learning environment where many approaches are integrated and imperfectly employed. Key aspects of the Human Biology program, including collaboration, situated learning, and reflective practice have shown various levels of effectiveness in prior research considering that outcomes are affected by countless variables. Although I expected better outcomes for students in Human Biology, I approached the learning environment hoping to uncover specific aspects of both majors that helped or hindered learning through interviews, open-ended questionnaires, and observation. Considering my prior expectations that the pedagogy of the Human Biology program would result in better outcomes, I attempted to limit my own bias through blind analysis of questionnaires, semi-structured interview protocols, and consultation with other researchers on both instruments and collected data. I recognize, however, that my desire to find evidence supporting the SSI-based approach in Human Biology is a limitation of the study. Research Questions My dissertation investigates the effects of a four-year interdisciplinary undergraduate Human Biology program focused on the progressive development of evidence-based reasoning skills and reflective judgment to form personal commitments 5 on real interdisciplinary issues. Although the program incorporates many theoretical constructs and pedagogical strategies, I will focus on the SSI context embodied in the program. As Human Biology students typically chose the program as an alternative to the traditional biology major and are similar in achievement and future career paths, my dissertation compares Human Biology and biology majors. I first address how Human Biology majors compared with traditional biology majors in reasoning and experiences with SSI. I then address how the context of the program affected students’ understanding of scientific inquiry. Scientific inquiry is explicitly discussed and practiced throughout the Human Biology program as a means of providing evidence-based conclusions about scientific phenomena, and is a central disciplinary process in science. Thirdly I address how levels and perceptions of biology content knowledge differ between groups, exploring the criticism that Human Biology majors may compromise basic science knowledge in their focus on interdisciplinary, socioscientific issues. Finally, I compare general perceptions of biology and Human Biology majors, including perceptions of outcomes and perceptions of the learning environment. Research questions include: Socioscientific Issues (1) Do Human Biology majors reason with SSI differently from traditional biology majors? (2) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their experiences with SSI differ? Understanding of Scientific Inquiry (3) Do Human Biology and biology majors understand scientific inquiry differently? 6 Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge (4) Do Human Biology and biology majors differ in their general biology content knowledge? (5) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their content knowledge differ? General Perceptions of Majors (6) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ general perceptions of their majors differ? 7 CHAPTER TWO: BACKGROUND FOR STUDY Theoretical Framework In my dissertation research, I view student development through a theoretical lens informed by situated cognition and the complementary developmental theories of King and Kitchener (1994), Perry (1999), and Baxter Magolda (1992, 1999). Situated Cognition My theoretical lens is informed by situated cognition, which posits that knowledge is connected to the context in which it is learned (Brown et al., 1989). As a tool is understood through its use, students make sense of a new concept in the context of its application and discipline. Domain-specific learning promotes a knowledge structure that allows knowledge to be accessed for relevant problems and not remain “inert” (Bransford et al., 1986). Effective learning environments offer students opportunities to work with and apply concepts in contexts authentic to their use. They also remain authentic to common practices in the fields, such as collaboration. For example, physicians, nurses, and other health care professionals work in teams, and scientists collaborate among and between research teams. Through this theoretical lens, pedagogy that emphasizes interpersonal interaction and is contextualized in realistic problems should promote development of concepts, skills, and disciplinary knowledge. Developmental Constructs in College Students Reflective Judgment Based on their longitudinal study with college students, King and Kitchener (1994) assert that reflective thinking is needed in uncertain or controversial situations. To manage situations where information is incomplete, people need to continually evaluate 8 tentative solutions in terms of usefulness and plausibility, incorporating new data and different arguments. The Reflective Judgment Model describes a pattern of development of “epistemic cognition:” “As individuals develop, they become better able to evaluate knowledge claims and to explain and defend their points of view on controversial issues” (King & Kitchener, 1994, p. 13). As developmental theorists, King and Kitchener (1994) identified patterns or stages of development through which individuals progress. The first three stages are prereflective, where knowledge is viewed as absolute, concrete, and directly observable, though sometimes unavailable or temporarily uncertain. Pre-reflective thinkers see direct correspondence between their beliefs and truth or assertions of authority and truth. Alternate positions are not perceived and there is no conflict because “right” answers clearly exist. The middle two developmental stages are quasi-reflective. Knowledge is viewed as either uncertain or contextual, and knowledge claims tend to be idiosyncratic, for example only evidence supporting a belief may be considered. Quasi-reflective thinkers may see beliefs as tied to context, where alternative beliefs with different contexts are viewed as equally true. Quasi-reflective thinkers may be hindered by complexity in forming conclusions (King & Kitchener, 1994). The last two stages are the reflective stages. Reflective thinkers recognize knowledge as an outcome of inquiry and originating from different sources. Knowledge claims are evaluated based on evidence. Ideas are considered across contexts and different perspectives are perceived and incorporated into reasoning. Criteria such as 9 weight of evidence, and need for and usefulness of a solution are applied in reasoning (King & Kitchener, 1994). Perry’s Scheme of Intellectual and Ethical Development Through interview data from a fifteen year study with Harvard undergraduates, Perry developed a model for the ways students view their experiences throughout their college years. Students tend to enter college from the perspective of simple dualism, where knowledge is seen as dualistic (right versus wrong, good versus bad), and right answers are held by authorities. In complex dualism, uncertainty and differing opinions are acknowledged, but seen as results of poorly informed authorities or undiscovered answers. In relativism, students accept differing positions as personal opinions and view values and knowledge as contextual. In the most mature positions, termed commitment in relativism, students recognize multiple views, but develop and act upon a commitment to a particular view (Perry, 1999). Insights from Perry on how students view the nature of knowledge shed light on how students perceive experiences with interdisciplinary and SSI-based learning environments. Epistemological Reflection Model Baxter Magolda (1992) developed the Epistemological Reflection model through a longitudinal study with eighty students through their college years and eight years postcollege. Her model relates to those of Perry and King and Kitchener, but also incorporates gender-related patterns. She used open-ended interviews to probe participants on their views of knowledge, learning, and influences of different aspects of their educational experience. She identified four reasoning patterns. 10 First, absolute knowers, typically in the first two years of college, viewed knowledge as right or wrong with no uncertainty. The instructor’s role is to authoritatively convey knowledge and the learner’s role is to acquire that knowledge. Female absolute knowers were more likely to exhibit a receiving pattern where knowledge was gained by passively listening and taking notes, and male absolute knowers were more likely to use a mastery pattern, actively seeking interaction with instructors and peers. In the second pattern, transitional knowing, seen throughout the college years, individuals perceived uncertainty in some areas. For example, certainty remained in chemistry, but uncertainty was understood in studying AIDS. In this stage, participants moved from a focus on acquiring knowledge toward understanding, and required more opportunities to explore ideas with others. Female transitional knowers tended to use an interpersonal approach, desiring to hear perspectives of peers, express their own ideas on uncertain issues with encouragement of instructors, and use personal judgment to resolve uncertainty. Male transitional knowers tended to use an impersonal approach, where they valued debate, fairness of evaluation, and resolving uncertainty through logic and background research. In the third pattern, independent knowing, seen toward the end and after college, students perceived uncertainty and viewed ideas of peers as equally valid to those of authority. Women more commonly espoused the interindividual pattern, where they valued both listening to others’ views and developing their own, and used others’ views to develop their own. Men more commonly espoused the individual pattern, where they focused on their own ideas, but attempted to listen to others. 11 The fourth pattern, contextual knowing, emerged in students toward the end of college or after college. Baxter Magolda says, “Contextual knowers looked at all aspects of a situation or issue, sought out expert advice in that particular context, and integrated their own and others’ views in deciding what to think.” (1999, p. 50). These individuals integrated relational and impersonal, or individualized ways of knowing. They relied on evidence from different sources to form their own positions. Conception of Human Biology Program through Theoretical Framework Through my theoretical lens, pedagogy that emphasizes interpersonal interaction and is contextualized in realistic problems should promote development of concepts and skills, and disciplinary knowledge. In the Human Biology program, case studies situate learning in realistic problems, promoting development of knowledge less likely to remain “inert.” The collaborative nature of the program promotes cognitive development through interaction and explanation, and situates learning in a realistic context, since biological inquiry is generally a collaborative endeavor. Through participation in inquiry, students should gain an understanding of the inquiry process. Aspects of Reflective Judgment (King & Kitchener, 1994) have been recognized as important epistemic competencies for reasoning in SSI, such as recognizing problems as complex and inquiry-based, considering multiple perspectives, and using evidence to make decisions (Zeidler, Sadler, Applebaum, & Callahan, 2009). Complementary to this framework, I view SSI, as enacted in the Human Biology program, as consistent with the goals of interdisciplinary learning, to help students apply disciplinary lenses to understand and take positions on complex problems (Thompson Klein, 1990; Boix Mansilla, 2000). 12 The developmental frameworks discussed serve as theoretical foundations for my study in understanding the epistemological assumptions behind student perceptions. My goal is not to apply developmental stages to participants or document changes in their developmental stages, but to approach my research with the understanding that the ability to perceive complexity or uncertainty in situations, understand inquiry-based rather than authority-based sources of knowledge, base personal positions on evidence, and consider multiple perspectives develops over time. The classroom environment, including roles of instructors and peers is important, and reasoning with such ill-structured problems may facilitate these developmental processes (Baxter Magolda, 1992; Baxter Magolda, 1999; Zeidler, Sadler, Applebaum, & Callahan, 2009). Literature Review This study examines Human Biology students’ understanding of socioscientific issues, understanding of inquiry, and biology conceptual knowledge. The study examines how key pedagogical aspects of the program including integration of disciplines, position taking on socioscientific issues, collaboration, and contextualization of learning in authentic activities relate to those outcomes and perceptions of those outcomes. In this literature review, I discuss theory and research on these aspects of the program. Socioscientific Issues Socioscientific issues (SSI) are often centered in problems that may be informed by concepts, theories, and methods from multiple disciplines. Many challenges individuals and nations must face currently and in the near future, such as dilemmas brought about by medical advances or environmental issues related to a growing human population, may be addressed from perspectives from both biology and the social 13 sciences (Sadler, 2004). Sadler (2004) defines SSI as having “central roles of both social and scientific factors.” Many proponents for including SSI in science curricula argue that they promote development of students into citizens able to apply scientific knowledge and “habits of mind” to decisions. (Sadler). The American Association for the Advancement of Science (1990) and the National Research Council (1996) consider ability to negotiate SSI an important part of scientific literacy. Sadler & Zeidler (2009) explain how SSI is consistent with Roberts’ (2007) Vision II of scientific literacy, which “derives its meaning from the character of situations with a scientific component, situations that students are likely to encounter as citizens” (p. 730). Similarly, they view SSI as consistent with the PISA definition of scientific literacy, including “scientific knowledge and use of that knowledge to identify questions, to acquire new knowledge, to explain scientific phenomena, and to draw evidence-based conclusions about sciencerelated issues,” science as an inquiry-based human endeavor of seeking knowledge, awareness of the roles of science and technology in defining our physical, social and cultural environments, and willingness to approach issues of science and technology reflectively (Sadler & Zeidler, 2009). The SSI movement builds upon the Science, Technology, and Society (STS) movement, which emerged in the early 1980s. STS sought to help students understand how the areas of science technology and social issues are interdependent, but according to Sadler (2004), the SSI movement focuses on “empowering” students to make informed decisions on science issues that impact and will impact their lives. Going beyond the boundaries of STS, SSI take into consideration the ethical value of decisions and moral development of students (Zeidler, Sadler, Simmons, & Howe, 2005). In addition, SSI has 14 been more fully developed as a form of pedagogy. Zeidler et al. (2005) provide a conceptual model of SSI where students’ cognitive and moral development is encouraged through focus on nature of science issues, classroom discourse issues, cultural issues, and case-based issues. Research on Reasoning and Argumentation in SSI. Classroom incorporation of SSI involves informal reasoning, where students develop and evaluate their own positions about complex situations (Kuhn, 1993). Informal reasoning generally takes place when problems are ill-structured, have no clearcut answers, or involve controversy and relevant information is not readily available. Informal reasoning includes consideration of “causes and consequences, pros and cons, and positions and alternatives” (Sadler, 2004). Research has revealed that individuals generally do not exhibit quality argumentation in response to socioscientific issues. Students often fail to justify their claims adequately or acknowledge opposing viewpoints, however several SSI interventions have shown gains in argumentation. Zohar and Nemet (2002) found that intervention groups of 9th grade students involved in an SSI-based genetics unit had significant pre to post gains on a genetics-based argumentation test, while students in the traditionally-taught comparison group did not show significant gains. Dori, Tal, and Tsaushu (2003) also found that students improved in argumentation after an SSI intervention, but lower achieving students had greater gains. After an SSI unit including debate, a field trip, research, and presentation of research, Pedretti (1999) found that a class of fifth and sixth grade students involved in a unit on mining that took place in the classroom and a science center were more likely to consider multiple perspectives and 15 consider the ethical dimensions of problems. In a study with high school students engaged in an SSI unit on malaria, Tal and Hochberg (2003) found that students improved from pre to post tests on a test of argumentation. They assessed several dimensions of argumentation basing their rubric on that of Hogan, Nastasi, & Pressley (2000), including generativity of assertions, elaboration of ideas, number of justifications, explanations, logical coherence, and synthesis of counter ideas. Students improved on all subscales except synthesis. Analysis of student portfolios also showed that students improved in their reasoning over the course of the unit and deepened their reflections. Tal and Hochberg note that this environment enhances reasoning by creating the need to continually present new evidence and re-evaluate thinking. Several researchers examined student argumentation in small or whole-class groups after SSI interventions. Albe (2008) found that SSI contexts were effective contexts for “collaborative argumentation” where students challenged their peers to justify positions, explain their point of view, and consider other perspectives. Zohar and Nemet (2002) also reported gains in the quality of argumentation in group discussions. Tal and Kedmi (2006) found that an SSI intervention for non-science majors at the high school level improved group argumentation, including number of justifications, incorporation of scientific knowledge, incorporation of different aspects (for example, environmental or economic), and synthesis of counter arguments. They found that students improved significantly in all areas except synthesis. Sadler (2004) argues that productive interventions should facilitate students making personal connections with the issues and stress the importance of justifying claims and exploring conflicting points of view. He suggests that students need plenty of 16 practice with these processes, and opportunities to reflect on examples of effective arguments. Teachers also must provide effective support in argumentation with SSI. Finding little effect on argumentation in SSI-based classes team-taught by science and humanities teachers, Harris and Ratcliffe (2005) concluded that a great deal of scaffolding and support were needed in SSI classrooms. Tal & Kedmi (2006) also noted that student gains in argumentation could have been more pronounced with greater teacher support. They stressed the importance of teacher modeling and instruction on argumentation, which was underrepresented in the classroom they studied. Teachers and researchers discussed the value of team teaching in modeling argumentation, creating a community of practice, and supporting teachers learning to teach in SSI environments. Research on the Role of Conceptual Knowledge in SSI Although the general literature on informal reasoning suggests that conceptual understanding is unrelated to argumentation and informal reasoning (Sadler, 2004, Perkins, Farady, & Bushey, 1991), studies on informal reasoning with SSI conclude that conceptual knowledge is important to informal reasoning. Fleming (1986) and Tytler, Duggan, and Gott (2001) found that informal reasoning was limited when participants lacked relevant conceptual knowledge. Hogan (2002) and Zeidler and Shafer (1984) found that conceptual knowledge enhanced informal reasoning in SSI. Few studies of socioscientific issues took place in college settings, however Zeidler and Schafer (1984) conducted their study with environmental science undergraduate majors and non-science majors. Participants were assessed on content knowledge, affect, and moral reasoning on issues related to the environment. Although the instrument in this study targeted moral reasoning, rather than informal reasoning, 17 moral reasoning is widely accepted as an essential component of informal reasoning (Sadler, 2004). The environmental science majors scored higher on the content knowledge test as expected. While they were not significantly different from the nonmajors in their positive attitudes toward the environment and the general moral reasoning measure, they scored higher in the measure of moral reasoning contextualized in environmental issues. This study also supports the positive relationship between content knowledge and informal reasoning in SSI. Aside from relation of conceptual knowledge to reasoning, SSI contexts have shown to be effective, or not detrimental to gaining content knowledge. Dori, Tal, and Tsaushu, (2003) found that learning in SSI case studies resulted in significant gains in content knowledge, especially for lower-ability students; while Yager, Lim, and Yager (2006) found that SSI and traditional groups did not differ significantly in their content learning. Zohar and Nemet (2003) found that intervention groups of 9th grade students involved in an SSI-based genetics unit performed significantly better on a genetics knowledge test. Also, Zeidler, Sadler, Simmons, and Howes, (2005) found that SSI students performed better on assessments of anatomy and physiology concept knowledge than the traditionally taught comparison group. Barker and Millar (1996) found that in their study of 400 English secondary students at 36 schools, students in an SSI-based curriculum, the Salters Advanced Chemistry (SAC) course, had no significant differences from students in traditional chemistry courses on tests of chemistry concepts. Although Barber (2001) found that students in the SAC curriculum performed worse than those in traditional courses on a 18 standardized chemistry test, these results support Barber’s assertion that the standardized test may have been geared toward a traditional curriculum. Reflective Judgment in SSI King & Kitchener’s (1994) Reflective Judgment model provides a useful framework for considering student development in SSI. Since SSI creates contexts where students are faced with the kinds of problematic situations that require reflective thinking, practice and support in socioscientific reasoning should promote development in reflective judgment. Zeidler, Sadler, Applebaum, and Callahan (2009) found that students in two secondary level anatomy and physiology classes where an SSI curriculum was followed showed significant development in reflective judgment between the beginning and end of the year, while students in a non-SSI comparison group did not show change according to qualitative and quantitative results from the Prototypic Reflective Judgment Interview (King & Kitchener, 1994). These results indicate that a sustained SSI approach can positively affect students’ development of reflective judgment. Research on the Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry in SSI Although more research is needed in this area, SSI appear to provide effective contexts for development of understanding of the nature of science. Khishfe and Lederman (2006) compared two ninth grade classes, one in which NOS teaching was imbedded in an SSI context on global warming and one in which NOS teaching was decontextualized. NOS was explicitly addressed in both classes. Pre and post questionnaires and interviews revealed that both groups improved NOS conceptions, but the study did not provide evidence that an SSI environment provided advantages for students’ development of informed NOS understandings. 19 Walker and Zeidler (2007) also investigated students’ NOS understandings in an SSI context. They studied two mixed level high school classes working on a unit on genetically modified foods in the WISE (Web-based Inquiry Science Environment: Bell & Linn, 2000) computer-based learning environment which scaffolds students’ science reasoning. NOS ideas and assessment were embedded throughout the activities. Walker and Zeidler analyzed these assessments, data from the Nature of Scientific Knowledge Scale, and student interviews using the Views on the Nature of Science Questionnaire (Lederman, Abd-El- Khalick, Bell, & Schwartz, 2001). They found that students did develop in their NOS understandings, especially in the tentative and creative/subjective NOS. However, students did not effectively integrate NOS concepts into arguments in a debate at the end of the unit. While NOS describes science as an entire domain, scientific inquiry has been differentiated as “what scientists do” (Schwartz, Lederman, & Lederman, 2008). Key aspects of the nature of scientific inquiry (NOSI) include investigations being guided by questions, use of different methods in scientific investigations dependent on the question, different reasons for scientific investigations, evidence-based justification of knowledge, recognition of anomalous data, differences between data and evidence, and standards of practice and peer review in scientific communities. Although NOS understanding has been studied in relation to SSI, I am unaware of published studies that explicitly address how understanding of scientific inquiry may be facilitated in SSI environments. Sadler (2009) states, “The one clear conclusion that can be drawn from this section [on SSI and NOS] is that there has been more rhetoric regarding the potential for SSI-related interventions to promote student understandings of nature of science than 20 empirical evidence.” More studies are needed to establish how teaching about inquiry and NOS can be imbedded within SSI environments and how students are able to articulate and apply these understandings. In summary, reasoning with SSI should move students toward taking informed positions on controversial issues that span scientific and social domains. Research suggests that supporting students’ understanding of relevant science concepts and practice with socioscientific reasoning can help students learn to develop effective arguments in similar contexts. SSI does not appear to hinder conceptual learning, but does appear to encourage development of reflective judgment. SSI may also be effective contexts to help students understand the nature of science and nature or scientific inquiry. In Human Biology, reasoning with socioscientific issues is structured throughout the program. These activities should help students learn to advocate for particular positions and make evidence-based arguments. Interdisciplinary Learning Conceptions and Origins of Interdisciplinarity As with SSI in general, interdisciplinarity was a key aspect of the Human Biology program, helping students develop a reflective approach to framing problems in Human Biology. Surprisingly, to my knowledge, research and theory on interdisciplinarity have not been connected in depth to SSI. This may be related to the fact that there is great deal of confusion about interdisciplinarity. First, there is no agreed-upon definition. Some consider interdisciplinary work a strictly educational domain, while others see interdisciplinarity at work in research, government, and professions. Some view interdisciplinary work as seeking to unify knowledge, while others see it as innovative, 21 seeking to develop new knowledge. Also the concept may be seen as merging of ideas among the physical and life sciences, or as using disciplinary concepts to bridge the hard sciences and the social sciences. Interdisciplinarity may also be seen as using knowledge from an academic discipline to approach problems in professions (Thompson Klein, 1990). Second, people are generally unfamiliar with interdisciplinary scholarly work. Interdisciplinary professional groups are fairly new and some proponents are reluctant to pursue interdisciplinary movements due to the narrow-mindedness that has come with professionalization in other fields. Thompson Klein also cites a “general disinclination to place individual activities within a larger conceptual framework or wider body of knowledge” (1990, p. 13). The third reason Thompson Klein cites for the confusion about interdisciplinarity is that it has no unified discourse. Interdisciplinary spans many different literatures. The written and spoken “texts” that are generated are not read by a common audience (1990, p. 13). Interdisciplinarity often originates as a result of specialization in particular disciplines. Relationships among disciplines may emerge as in-depth study or particular problems approach the boundaries of these disciplines. Interdisciplinary fields are often problem-centered, drawing from different disciplines to inform different aspects of a problem. For example, environmental psychology addresses problems that have both psychological and environmental components. Interdisciplinarity is also called upon in problems that are complex or have insufficient resources providing insight into those situations (Thompson Klein, 1990). 22 People use several metaphors to understand interdisciplinarity, such as geopolitics, the machine, and the organism. The geopolitical metaphor is commonly applied because, in the words of Robert L. Scott, there is a “distinctly political face to the circumstances in which interdisciplinary efforts must thrive or not.” Klein says, “If the disciplines have become ‘warring fortresses between which envoys are sent, and occasional temporary alliances are formed,’ then calls for ‘truth and synthesis’ are inevitably charged with political overtones” (1990, p. 78). Disciplines maintain an “orthodox” position of authority with perspectives viewed as doctrine with which no one should experiment. The machine or computer metaphor has been used calling for “interfacing” of methods and concepts to address particular problems. The metaphor of the organism employs the idea of symbiosis of the disciplines and a fertility that gives rise to new disciplines. Conversely, the organism metaphor is used to characterize specialization as a pathology threatening the viability of a discipline, where interdisciplinarity is a symptom of the disease (Klein 1990, p. 81). These metaphors demonstrate both the political and controversial nature of interdisciplinarity. Roles of the Disciplines Thompson Klein (1990) defines a discipline as the “tools, methods, procedures, exempla, concepts, and theories that account coherently for a set of objects or subjects” Disciplines change over time in response to external and internal demands. Members of a disciplinary community develop a “world view” specific to that discipline and establish and hold work to criteria for excellence (p. 104-105). These are generally considered positive aspects of disciplines, but may be seen as excluding productive ideas or ignoring some aspects of problems (Thompson Klein, 1990). 23 Two types of interdisciplinary positions have developed. The nondisciplinary perspective tends to treat the disciplines with disdain, viewing them as dangerously authoritarian. For example, critical interdisciplinarity seeks to revise disciplinary theories. The disciplinary perspective considers grounding in disciplinary work critical to interdisciplinary work. Proponents of this perspective see a need for a “disciplinary home,” Disciplinary theories and methods from cognate disciplines are seen as “tools” to solve problems. The disciplines are “the base for integration, and the substance for metacritical reflection” (Thompson Klein, 1993, p. 106). Interdisciplinary fields are thought of as “borrowing” from established disciplines. Reasons for borrowing include: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. To help structure a relatively unstructured domain; To simplify a domain; To complete a domain; To explain a domain; To enable a domain to get a complete picture of its own framework; To allow for experimentation where the domain does not permit it (Kinneavy, 1980, p. 144; Thompson Klein, 1990, p. 85). Thompson Klein argues that the practice of “borrowing” from disciplines is often problematic. She says, “Resorting to ‘an alien expertise’ to solve an immediate problem is often evidence of a ‘quick-fix mentality’ rather than a long-term, integrated solution” (1990, p. 88). Common problems of borrowing from other disciplines include 1) misunderstanding of borrowed concepts, 2) using ideas and methods out of context, 3) use of borrowed concepts “out of favor” in the context in which they originate, 4) “illusions of certainty” about concepts used cautiously in the original discipline, 5) relying too heavily on one perspective, and 6) dismissing contradictory evidence or explanations. (Thompson Klein). Thompson Klein argues that at least a basic 24 understanding of the disciplines from which theories, concepts, or methods are borrowed is essential in interdisciplinary work. She calls this the “burden of comprehension” (a term she borrowed from Janice M. Lauer). Interdisciplinary Education The goals and pitfalls of interdisciplinary education are similar to those of interdisciplinarity in general, as discussed above. Goals of interdisciplinary education include innovation, knowledge integration, student choice in inquiry, deductive reasoning, synthesis of knowledge, and reasoning by analogy (Kavalovski, 1979; Newell & Green, 1982/1998; Boix Mansilla & Duraising, 2007). Boix Mansilla and Duraising (2007) define interdisciplinary understanding as “the capacity to integrate knowledge and modes of thinking in two or more disciplines or established areas of expertise to produce a cognitive advancement—such as explaining a phenomenon, solving a problem, or creating a product—in ways that would have been impossible or unlikely through single disciplinary means.” Approaches to interdisciplinary higher education vary greatly. Postmodern critiques have argued that disciplinary boundaries promote and protect unfair power structures, and interdisciplinary fields, such as gender and culture studies emerged from this perspective. Other interdisciplinary fields have developed maintaining the centrality of a particular discipline, but borrowing from other disciplines to approach problems too broad for traditional disciplinary approaches (Grossman, Wineburg, & Beers 2000). Boix Mansilla, Miller, and Gardner (2000) argue that secondary education should primarily seek to ground students in disciplines, but help students to bring disciplinary lenses together to understand and solve specific problems. They hold that disciplines are 25 important for raising important questions, providing a time-tested conceptual framework, and defining standards of excellence. This perspective is representative of the Human Biology community and compatible with the goals of SSI. Assessment of Interdisciplinary Learning In response to the claim that university faculty are not equipped to evaluate interdisciplinary programs or to help students understand complex, interdisciplinary issues (Schilling, 2001), Boix Mansilla and Duraising (2007) developed an assessment framework, identifying criteria for quality interdisciplinary work and goals for interdisciplinary education. They interviewed faculty members at well-known interdisciplinary programs and characterized these individuals’ analyses of student work, such as exams, integrative papers, and capstone presentations. Data included 69 interviews of faculty and students, focusing on teaching and learning, especially assessment, 10 classroom observations, 40 pieces of student work, and official documents from each program. The framework includes three areas of focus: disciplinary grounding, “advancement through integration,” and critical awareness. Quality interdisciplinary student work is grounded in “disciplinary theories, findings, examples, methods, validation criteria, genres, and forms of communication.” These aspects of disciplines employed should be appropriate to the problem and accurately applied. Important disciplinary perspectives should be represented. Quality student work also integrates different disciplinary ideas to advance understanding. Integrative devices include conceptual frameworks, graphs, explanations, models, solutions to problems, or metaphors. Finally, quality student work shows evidence of reflection and a clear sense 26 of purpose, “that is, framing problems in ways that invite interdisciplinary approaches and exhibiting awareness of distinct disciplinary contributions, how the overall integration ‘works,’ and the limitations of the integration.” Professors expected that students not amass information, but use information in new situations. They should balance perspectives in response to the purpose of the work. Students should be reflective about the advantages and limitations of interdisciplinary work. In summary, interdisciplinarity is a misunderstood and contentious concept among scholars. In the Human Biology program, grounding in different disciplines offers new tools to approach problems. The inquiry process, an important disciplinary aspect of science, is central to learning in Human Biology. An interdisciplinary perspective should offer students an opportunity to reflect on the purposes, advantages, and limitations of scientific inquiry. The Human Biology program also embodies the problem-centered nature of interdisciplinary education. Finally the program exhibits the criteria for excellence in interdisciplinary work as discussed by Mansilla and Duraising (2007). Coursework is grounded in the disciplines involved, students are expected to advance their understanding through “advancement through integration” of disciplinary ideas, and they are expected to reflect deeply on their interdisciplinary work. Collaborative Learning Collaborative work is an essential component of the learning environment in Human Biology and is an essential part of teaching through SSI. A need to teach science in the collaborative context of doing science has been emphasized in recent years. Scientists and engineers need to work in collaborative contexts, students should have opportunities to work together in the science classroom (Springer, Stanne, & Donovan, 27 1999). Collaborative work is a central aspect of the culture of the science disciplines, however, as Brown, Collins, & Deguid (1989) claim, classroom contexts often “deny students the chance to engage the relevant domain culture, because that culture is not in evidence.” A great deal of research has investigated the value of collaborative learning, yielding conflicting results. A meta-analysis of group learning in college level Science, Math, Engineering, and Technology (SMET) courses by Springer, Stanne, and Donovan (1999) found that students involved in small group learning attained greater achievement than students who received instruction without collaborative or cooperative grouping. Students who worked in small groups also showed higher levels of persistence through SMET courses and more favorable attitudes toward their classes. In his meta-analysis of cooperative learning, Bossert (1988) found that cooperative learning can improve students’ memory skills, retention of knowledge, and problem solving. However, a review of literature by Davidson (1985) found that only one third of the studies examined showed better learning outcomes with collaborative as opposed to independent learning. Cohen (1994) suggests that the aspects of collaborative learning that lead to success only occur in certain situations. Some researchers have conducted studies to isolate specific aspects of the structuring of collaborative learning environments that influence its effectiveness. Although these studies shed light on particular aspects of collaborative learning and problem solving, they were performed outside of the normal classroom context and used tasks that were peripheral to the curricula. They can inform design of classroom collaborative experiences, but a descriptive picture of the context in which collaborative 28 learning takes place is needed. The Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education (CBASSE, 1999) recommends researchers of all educational fields come together to investigate collaborative learning and specific problems that must be overcome to make it effective (CBASSE, 1999, p. 280). Cohen (1994) argues that researchers need to conduct observational studies that examine how student interaction relates to outcome variables and allow them to make inferences about the aspects of collaborative learning that lead to success. For these reasons, I have chosen to include descriptive studies of collaborative learning environments which provide evidence for its effectiveness and detail how it may be successfully implemented. Important Processes in Collaborative Learning Some classroom studies have highlighted the important processes in collaborative learning that lead to effective learning outcomes. From her review of literature, Cohen (1994) argues that elaborated discussion is central to conceptual learning. Cohen says, “For conceptual learning, effective interaction should be more of a mutual exchange process in which ideas, hypotheses, strategies, and speculations are shared.” Cohen argues that the strongest predictor of achievement is giving elaborated explanations. In their study, Okada and Simon (1997) addressed the process of collaborative discovery learning and the role of discussion in collaboration. They questioned whether pairs of students would perform better than individuals on a computer simulated genetics discovery task in science, how pairs’ and individuals’ discovery processes would differ, and what variables would impact performance in discovery tasks. Okada and Simon found that pairs were more successful than individuals due to their participation in more explanatory experiences, such as considering multiple hypotheses, discussing alternative 29 ideas, or considering justification. The implication of the research is that explanatory activities help students organize information for theory-building. Giglers and de Jong (2005) investigated how prior knowledge affects learning in a collaborative discovery learning environment. The authors expanded Klahr and Dunbar’s (1988) Scientific Discovery as Dual Search (SDDS) model to their own extended SDDS model, which explains the discovery learning process between two individuals, and used the model to interpret their results. Giglers and de Jong found that students with higher levels of definitional knowledge were more likely to discuss interpretation of results in problem-solving sessions, and that discussion of hypothesis generation and experimentation was increased when dyads had different levels of prior knowledge, although extremely different levels may have hindered success. They found a negative correlation between number of technical remarks and the definitional knowledge test, and a positive correlation between the definitional knowledge test and remarks related to data interpretation. They found positive correlations between the difference in generic knowledge test scores and hypothesis generation remarks as well as experimental design and execution remarks. Explaining results in terms of Vygotsky’s (1978) theory of the zone of proximal development, they conclude that heterogeneous teams were more successful and that in a collaborative learning setting, there should be a more capable other. They also asserted that it is important for students in a group to be aware of their own and each others’ beliefs. This has implications for the design of the learning environment to help make these beliefs explicit. Hogan (1999) addresses a need for instructional methods tailored to help students in collaborative reasoning and making sense of data in applied problems. She describes 30 the problem established in the literature that students tend to reason poorly, concentrate on surface features, and fail to communicate equally. The purpose was to create a collaborative inquiry intervention in which regulation of student discourse was addressed explicitly in the curriculum. Hogan assessed competencies for knowledge co-construction after the “Thinking Aloud Together” intervention, which explicitly stressed metacognitive, regulatory and strategic skills. She assumed that for complex, open-ended problems, rules or heuristics were not useful, but students needed to be aware of and able to regulate their knowledge building processes. Four classes of eighth graders received the intervention of a 12-week unit focusing on metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive regulation, thinking practices, and reflection, and four control classes did not. Lessons in the intervention group involved metacognitive knowledge, metacogntive regulation, and thinking practices, and included a reflection phase. Hogan found that intervention students gained in metacognitive knowledge and ability to articulate collaborative reasoning processes. Intervention students did not, however, show enhanced collaborative reasoning skills. Also, students with learner-asexplorer perspective had greater learning gains. Hogan suggests that the results may be related to a lack of integration of reflection into the activities. She concludes that we need to better understand metacognitive processes at the group level. In response to a need for theories on how convergence of meaning is achieved, Roschelle (1992) created and supported a process model for collaborative conceptual change. Roschelle found that conceptual change among two female high school students did take place in science problem solving sessions and concluded that the data provide support for his proposed process model of convergent conceptual change. The model 31 includes the construction of the situation at an intermediate level of abstraction, the use of metaphors in reference to the situation, cycles of “displaying, confirming, and repairing situated actions,” and progressively requiring higher standards of evidence. Team-based Learning Team-based learning is a specific form of collaborative learning used in the Human Biology program. Where other literature on collaborative learning describes contexts in which duration of collaboration varies, team-based learning uses long term student grouping. Although team-based learning is similar to problem based learning, the small amount of literature on team-based learning reveals two primary differences in structure and purpose of the pedagogy. First in team-based learning, students are asked to apply information they have studied previously, while in problem based learning students acquire information in the process of problem solving. Secondly, team-based learning relies predominantly on the team structure to keep groups functioning effectively while problem-based learning employs a tutor to facilitate group sessions (Fink, 2002). An important assumption in team-based learning is that established teams will perform at a higher level than their highest-achieving team member. Michaelson, Watson, and Black (1989) investigated the differences between individual and group decision making “in a situation that was as representative as possible of everyday work situations” in the classroom. Previous studies had concluded that the achievement of the most capable member of the group represented the upper limit of the group. Michaelson et al. (1989) explain that previous studies may have failed to find higher achievement of the group than most-capable member because groups studied had little experience working together and tasks failed to resemble authentic tasks that teams might encounter. In their 32 study of 222 project learning teams from 25 organizational courses over 5 years, the majority of class time was spent in group problem-solving. Students worked in the same groups throughout the course, logging a minimum of 32 hours in group problem-solving and data was collected from a series of six exams taken individually and as teams, a normal part of the curriculum to which students were well accustomed. Michaelson et al. found that in this context, teams outperformed the most successful individual 97% of the time. While the mean score for individuals was 74.2% and the mean score for the highest individual in the team was 82.6%, the average group score was 89.9%. In summary, research supports the use of collaborative learning environments to promote science conceptual knowledge and scientific reasoning. Explanatory experiences, including consideration of multiple hypotheses, and justification of ideas are productive processes for theory-building should be encouraged (Okada and Simon 1997). Roschelle (1992) sheds light on important processes that take place in collaborative science learning, including construction of the situation at an intermediate level of abstraction, use of metaphors in reference to the situation, cycles of “displaying, confirming, and repairing situated actions,” and progressively requiring higher standards of evidence. Factors internal to students also affect the outcomes of collaborative learning. Students’ prior knowledge affects their levels of interpretation of results, hypothesis generation, and experimentation, suggesting groups should be structured for diversity of prior knowledge (Giglers and de Jong, 2005). Finally, the perspective of the student affects learning outcomes in collaborative environments, where a learner-asexplorer perspective has been shown to relate to greater learning gains in a collaborative learning environment (Hogan, 1999). The collaborative aspect of the Human Biology 33 program, carried out through group discussion and teamwork should have provided opportunities for productive cognitive processes in conceptual learning as well as teamwork skills. Contextualized Learning In the Human Biology program, learning was designed to be contextualized in real world problems that are often interdisciplinary in nature and collaboratively approached. Work stemming from cognitive science stresses the importance of context to learning (Brown, Collins, & Deguid, 1989). As we understand the concept of a tool through its use, not a decontextualized examination of it, students need to experience new concepts in the context of their discipline and application. Brown et al. (1989) argue, “by ignoring the situated nature of cognition, education defeats its own goal of providing usable, robust knowledge.” The context of scientific work, from lab research to environmental science to healthcare, is collaborative. According to Brown et al. learning is a process of enculturation. As in trade apprenticeships, learners participate in a community with common goals. Brown et al. propose cognitive apprenticeship for authentic contextual learning in the classroom. Students and teachers work together on tasks authentic to the discipline of study to understand key concepts. The role of the teacher changes from the dispenser of knowledge to a coach who helps students move through a process of gaining experience and competence with concepts and skills. The teacher models concepts and skills for students, allows them to practice with appropriate scaffolding, then fades scaffolding as students demonstrate mastery. Research on cognitive structures supports the importance of context and domainspecific knowledge (Bransford, Sherwood, Vye, & Reiser, 1986). Adams et al. (1985) 34 showed that learning activities that gave students experience with problems and showed them how information was useful in solving those problems helped them access the knowledge they need to solve a particular problem. Domain-specific learning promotes a knowledge structure that allows knowledge to be accessed for relevant problems, not remain inert. Anchored Instruction The approach of anchored instruction, where learning is embedded in a rich, realistic context, is an example of situated cognition theory put into action (Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt, CTGV, 1990). This approach is intended to avoid students’ acquisition of “inert” knowledge, knowledge acquired in a classroom context that is generally unusable in real-life settings and may be accessed only in response to specific cues, such as exam questions (Brown & Palinscar, 1989). Instead learning is “anchored” in realistic “macrocontexts” similar to those in which the knowledge will be useful. The video contexts are complex enough to encompass problem-solving from multiple perspectives. Students are responsible to choose relevant data from the macrocontext and generate problems that need solving from the story. In a study with the Jasper Woodbury Adventures, a video series designed to teach math concepts through anchored instruction, fourth and fifth grade students were found to improve in problem generation. Also lower achievers had a higher level of participation (CTGV, 1990). This suggests that anchored instruction can create a zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978) where students are supported by peers as they participate in activities and gain competence. This ideas of a “macrocontext” is encompassed by problem-based or casebased learning, approaches used in SSI and embraced in the Human Biology program. 35 Problem-based Learning in Medicine The problem-based learning (PBL) model was initially developed in medical education by Barrows and colleagues (Savery & Duffy, 1995). Research in medical education had found that medical students retained little of the information presented in lectures by the time they reached their clinical training, and that they did not use the information in practice. The lecture method was criticized for allowing students to be passive and failing to teach skills of critical thinking and problem solving. Also, medical content is taught as isolated “facts” from the perspective of scientists rather than clinicians (Williams, 1992). Williams argues that if biological mechanisms are only understood independently, medical students will have trouble dealing with the variation resulting from interaction of different mechanisms in patients. Also, real patients present with problems with roots in psychology rather than biology, and decisions and abilities to seek treatment depend on many social and economic factors. Medical education should prepare students to deal with these issues of uncertainty. Savery and Duffy (1995) describe the constructivist principles on which PBL is based: anchoring activities into a broader problem, helping students to take ownership for the problem, engaging students in an authentic task, matching the complexity of the task to that of the environment in which knowledge will be used, allowing students to take ownership of their problem-solving process, supporting and challenging students’ thinking, encouraging social negotiation and testing of ideas, and supporting reflection on content and process. In PBL, social negotiation of meaning and metacognitive skills are essential, since PBL teams must monitor their understanding and progress (Savery & Duffy, 1995). 36 Hmelo-Silver (2007) explains that PBL presents students with complex problems, but provides scaffolding that makes tasks “accessible, manageable, and within students’ zone of proximal development.” Collaborative groups allow complexity to be reduced through pooling ideas and experiences and sharing work load. Tutors help groups to manage interactions and engage in productive reflections, and white boards help groups to organize and keep track of information. Also the sequencing of PBL is intended to provide scaffolding by increasing the level of detail as students gain competency. PBL emphasizes students understanding the reasoning behind a process as well as the process itself (Hmelo-Silver, 2007). In studies, PBL has been found to increase motivation, self-directed learning, and the opportunity to integrate science into real problems (Hmelo & Evensen, 2000). Reviews of literature by Albanese and Mitchell (1993) and Vernon and Blake (1993) reported that PBL students scored higher on clinical assessments than traditionally trained medical students. Dochy, Segers, Van den Bossche, and Gijbels, (2003) found that PBL students scored higher than traditional students in tests of knowledge application. According to another analysis of literature by Norman & Schmidt (1992), although PBL may initially result in decreased learning as compared to traditional teaching methods, it increases retention of knowledge. The analysis also suggests that PBL could facilitate transfer to other problems and help students integrate science concepts into a clinical context. Also, PBL increases students’ intrinsic interest in content and improves self-directed learning skills (Norman & Schmidt, 1992). Some authors have found PBL ineffective for some aspects of student learning. In their metaanalysis, Albanese and Mitchell (1993) found that PBL resulted in lower scores 37 on basic science exams, and students spent more time studying. Colliver (2000) found no statistical differences between PBL and traditionally taught students on standardized or course exams for first and second year medical students. Vernon & Blake (1993) found a decrease in basic science scores of PBL students and Dochy et al. (2003) found no difference in tests of declarative knowledge. In their review of literature, Norman & Schmidt (1992) reported no difference in problem solving skills between PBL and traditionally taught students. Hmelo-Silver (2007) argues that most literature comparing PBL and traditional curricula is “reactive,” where data are presented in such a way to support a particular position toward it. Also PBL students generally opt to participate in that track, so students are not randomly assigned (Hmelo-Silver, 2007). Although PBL students may perform lower on basic science exams, PBL has positive effects on processes important for practice, such as tests of clinical knowledge, transfer of knowledge to new problems, and hypothesis-driven reasoning. Problem-based and Project-based Learning Sherwood, Petrosino, Lin, and the Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt (1998) discuss problem-based macrocontexts, an example of an anchored instruction learning environment. Design principles for this learning environment include a narrative format, “generative design of stories,” data embedded within the context, complex problems, use of video, connections across curricula, and paired episodes to promote transfer of concepts. Problem-based macrocontexts allow students to undergo an iterative process of exploring, assessing and revising their understanding. The use of video helps students to recognize relevant information. Sherwood et al. (1998) base their design 38 principles on concepts of cognitive science: collaborative inquiry, real world problems, incorporation of technology, and modeling a research community in the classroom. In a preliminary study of the problem-based macrocontext, Scientists in Action, two groups of students studied the same content by reading a book chapter. The intervention group then watched the Stones River Mystery video, which was relevant to that content, while the control group watched a video developed to situate different content. Sherwood et al. (1998) found that the intervention group performed at a higher level compared to the control group on measures of motivation, problem-solving and noticing relevant information for problem-solving. However to effectively use this model, teachers must develop a student-centered style of teaching and face such challenges as allowing students to come to their own solutions, even when on wrong pathways, to know when to intervene versus allowing students to struggle, and integrating materials into the curriculum. Barron et al. (1998) also encourage the combination of problem-based and project-based learning. They describe their version of problem-based learning as “the use of authentic but simulated problems that students and teachers can explore collaboratively.” They describe project-based learning as taking place in everyday environments and having tangible outcomes, whereas simulated environments are considered problem-based. Barron et al. identify four principles of design for problembased and project-based learning: developing “learning-appropriate goals that lead to deep understanding,” preceding project-based assignments with problem-based ones and incorporating scaffolds like embedded teaching and contrasting cases, providing opportunities for revision and reflection, and social organization to promote students’ 39 sense of agency. They suggest that using problem-based and project-based learning together allows students to develop shared knowledge and skills through work on a problem, then apply that knowledge to a project. Work on the project further develops skills and deepens understanding of concepts. Barron et al. (1998) describe a problem-to-project intervention called SMART. The students are first involved in a problem anchored in a video-based story, which requires them to develop a blue-print. Students receive formative assessment on their blueprints and undergo a process of revision using media and peer resources. Students use the knowledge and skills developed while working on the problem to collaboratively design a playhouse taking certain constraints into consideration. Students undergo a process of revision throughout the project, and at the completion, present the project to their peers. All members of the class participate in evaluation of the project. Barron et al. assert that the presentation is important to help students reflect on “what it means to explain one's thinking and how to convince someone of the accuracy of a plan as well as issues such as what makes a presentation engaging.” Evaluating five classes of fifth graders on their blue-print designs of a chair, their achievement on a standards-based geometry test, and collaborative playhouse designs, Barron et al. found that all levels of math students improved substantially in their abilities to apply, understand, and present geometric concepts. Also, they found that students took advantage of the opportunity to revise their ideas. Case-based Learning Case-based learning has been used for many years in business and legal education. Like cognitive apprenticeship and anchored instruction, this approach 40 contextualizes concepts and provides scaffolding for learning. It provides authentic activities for learning, which Williams (1992) defines as “coherent, meaningful, and purposeful,” and representative of the activities members of a culture actually participate in. Like anchored instruction, the contexts for case-based learning are complex. The case method is often used for ill-structured problems, which lack “a consistent underlying theory that can act as a structure for organizing knowledge” (Williams). The case method in law does not emphasize collaborative learning, but is social in nature in that it involves group discussion and a public forum for Socratic questioning. Kolodner, Hmelo, and Narayanan (1996) stress the cognitive factors of case-based reasoning, like “access to old experiences (cases), and use of old experiences in reasoning” in design of effective learning environments. They explain that case-based reasoning is based on making inferences from specific and “cohesive knowledge structures” which link specific aspects of situations. Learning in CBR takes place by interpreting and committing to memory new experiences, re-interpreting previous experiences so they may be more easily indexed, and making generalizations from multiple experiences. Williams (1992) discusses case-based learning and problem-based learning in terms of several criteria. The learning experience should begin with a problem, the teacher should model expert problem solving, and students must be given opportunities to solve problems on their own with scaffolding and immediate feedback from the teacher. Instruction should stress metacognitive strategies and frequent formative assessment by both teachers and students. Problems should be authentic, complex, and target multiple skills. Formats, such as story or video should help to make the problem manageable and 41 problems should be sequenced to meet students’ needs as they progress through the course. Modification and Integration of PBL and Case-based Reasoning Williams (1992) suggests that theories from cognitive apprenticeship and anchored instruction may improve the effectiveness of problem-based learning and the case method. She argues that expert models should be available to students so they may understand the thinking processes involved in solving contextual problems. Also, cases should target a wide range of skills. Efforts should be made to reduce complexity of cases by using story or video formats. This could facilitate novices’ learning of domain specific vocabulary. Cases should be sequenced according to difficulty and diversity to reduce student frustration and facilitate their progress. Also, attention should be paid to helping students pick out important information. Presenting the same concept in multiple contexts like hypothetical situations may help students in this goal. These methods should engage students in “learning to learn.” Giving students opportunities to actually apply knowledge, as in project-based learning is also important as well. Lastly more frequent opportunities for assessment would improve the effectiveness of these instructional models (Williams). Kolodner, Hmelo, and Narayanan (1996) recommend combining features of casebased reasoning and PBL. The two methods are complementary because PBL provides students with cases they may use to reason in future problems and CBR encourages students to reflect on the lessons that come out of each experience and predict how those lessons may apply to future situations. CBR also stresses the importance of feedback to identify flaws in thinking and missing knowledge. When using knowledge from an old 42 experience fails in a new experience, the learner reevaluates the old situation. In developing problems, Kolodner et al. (1996) argue that failures are important, but should be handled gently, so “students understand their role in learning.” Working with others can also enhance CBR by increasing the number of available cases from which to reason. While PBL suggests indexing cases to develop learning issues for problem solving, CBR suggests indexing cases to predict effects of problem solutions. Looking forward to other situations in which knowledge from the current case may be useful, as well as looking backward to search memory for similar experiences may promote transfer (Kolodner et al.). Kolodner et al. suggest incorporating tools, like case libraries which help students compare cases, determine what is important to take from cases, and test their ideas. They also suggest that when tutors are not available, scaffolding may be provided in a computer environment. A computer environment may also facilitate reflection to enhance transfer. In summary, anchored instruction, problem-based learning, project-based learning, and case-based reasoning provide models for learning science in the context of authentic problems. Focus on domain knowledge should help students to develop knowledge structures that help them access relevant information in problem solving. In addition, instruction drawing from these models should encourage students to engage in reflection to make knowledge explicit and elaborated, monitor and assess their learning process, and develop strategies for more effective learning. Relation to Study The goals and pedagogy of the interdisciplinary Human Biology program were grounded in research from many related fields in science education and learning sciences. 43 The core courses integrated different disciplinary perspectives and facilitated student reasoning in the context of socioscientific issues, encouraging development of crossdisciplinary thinking and evidence-based decision making. A collaborative, team-based environment was used to facilitate both learning of content and development of reasoning processes. Learning in context of problems and case studies was emphasized to help students develop knowledge they can apply to future situations. These research-based features of the Human Biology program contrast the traditional lecture-based and content-driven college biology learning environment. This dissertation investigates how participation in this learning environment affects student outcomes and perceptions of learning experiences. 44 CHAPTER 3: METHOD Context of Study The study took place in a large research-oriented university. Participants were recruited from the Human Biology program and the traditional biology major. Since I focus on the SSI-based context of the Human Biology program, I will refer to this group as SSI and the biology group as BIO. These majors differed where, in addition to required and elective courses, all SSI majors took yearly core courses and seminars and maintained a four-year reflective portfolio. BIO majors created a cohesive curriculum of required and elective courses, but were not involved in longitudinal projects or yearly core courses. In the SSI major, case-based socioscientific reasoning was deliberately structured into all core courses, with a developmental focus on moving students from exploring different perspectives to position-taking and advocating for evidence-based positions. For the SSI group, I will provide an in-depth description of the class context and activities used to specifically teach SSI. I will then describe the comparison (BIO) group in terms of curriculum and general teaching methods. SSI Group The published mission of the Human Biology program was “to integrate the biological and social sciences with the humanities and the arts in the study of human beings and the human condition” (Human Biology, 2007). Students enrolled in the program planned to enter life science graduate programs or professional programs, such as medicine, nursing, dentistry, physical therapy, law, or journalism, or pursue careers in teaching, the life science industry, or public policy. The program included selected courses from four concentration areas: human environment and ecology, human origins 45 and survival, human health and disease, and human reproduction and sexuality, as well as a series of interdisciplinary core courses taken each year. The foundation of the program was established in yearly core courses. These interdisciplinary courses connected primary biological concepts with related social and ethical issues and explicitly addressed epistemological concepts in biology including uncertainty, tentativeness, and the centrality of evidence to knowledge in biology (Human Biology, 2007). The interdisciplinary nature of the program responded to recent trends, including converging fields of disciplinary knowledge, professional requirements, and the need to solve problems that are both social and intellectual in nature (Thompson Klein, 1990). Core courses were team taught by an expert from the sciences and another discipline, and the specific course topic depended on the expertise of the instructors. Key themes running through these courses included scientific literacy through position-taking on socioscientific issues, collaboration, contextualized learning, and reflection. Scientific literacy includes “informed decision-making, the ability to analyze, synthesize and evaluate information, dealing sensibly with moral reasoning and ethical issues, and understanding connections inherent in socioscientific issues” (Zeidler, 2001). In the Human Biology core classes, scientific literacy was conceptualized to connect the inquiry process with social context, historical context, and ethical context (Human Biology, 2007). These efforts toward scientific literacy echo the goals of Socioscientific Issues (SSI). Understanding of SSI in Human Biology was developed through discussion, peer evaluation, and service learning. Contextualizing learning in case studies promotes ethical development through consideration of how power and authority influence scientific endeavors (Zeidler and Keefer, 2003). 46 Student collaboration was a central component of the SSI program (Human Biology, 2007). Students worked in teams for entire semesters on case studies, course projects, and even exams. This aspect of the program is based on evidence for the effectiveness of collaborative learning and scholarly work on best practices in team-based learning (Fink, 2002, Schlegel & Pace, 2004). Contextualizing learning in scientific problems through case studies and service learning projects was also an essential aspect of the program. Like the similar instructional strategies of problem-based and case-based learning, problems addressed in core courses are complex, target multiple skills, and resemble the activities in which members of a culture actually participate. Within the case context, the instructors provide models of expert problem solving (Williams, 1992). The team teaching approach allows students to see integration of different perspectives to solve problems. Reflection and documentation of content learning, collaborative processes, and personal experiences are scaffolded through class discussions and a progressive electronic portfolio which individuals update throughout the entire program. Technologybased reflective scaffolds have been found effective to help students articulate ideas and develop and reflect on explanations (Land & Zembal-Saul, 2003). In the following section, I will provide context for the teaching of SSI in the two core courses I observed: the 200 level and 400 level courses. 200 Level Core Course The syllabus describes the primary objective of the course as follows: This course introduces the social and ethical dimensions of human biological experience and the construction of scientific knowledge through in-depth consideration of human death and disease…we will use a collaborative, case-based approach to explore the operationalization of 47 scientific concepts, the logic of scientific inquiry, and the effective communication of evidence, interpretation, and claims. Learning goals included constructing models integrating different fields of science, gathering, evaluating, and applying scientific data to understand patterns of disease and death, evaluating different perspectives on death and disease and arguing a chosen position using scientific evidence, and developing a portfolio in areas of inquiry of personal interest. The structure of the course was team-based. Teams of 5-6 students collaborated in and out of class, and the majority of class time involved teams working together on particular tasks, generally case studies, followed by whole class debriefing. Since the course depended on active student involvement, participation and peer evaluation made up 20% of the course grade. Other activities included short lectures by instructors, guest lectures, short individual assessment activities, team presentations, and exams. In class, teams were seated around individual tables to encourage team interaction. Each table was equipped with at least one laptop to research topics and create documents. The atmosphere of the classroom was energetic and informal, with the majority of interaction within teams. Like most of the core courses, the class was team-taught by an expert in the biological sciencese (in this course a neuroscientist) and an expert from the social sciences or humanities (in this course a sociologist with specialization in epidemiology). Generally, one professor presented new information and when cases were discussed, each professor modeled his or her disciplinary perspective and the importance of integrating those perspectives. They were honest about the limitations of their knowledge and participated in information searches when difficult questions arose. 48 The course was structured into three modules: death and dying, infectious disease, and HIV and AIDS. These modules focused on understanding operational definitions of death and disease for different contexts, physiological and microbiological understanding of these topics, understanding of these topics at multiple scales, and use of scientific information in arguing positions. I will describe activities from the third module in depth to demonstrate how the course was designed to encourage effective reasoning with SSI, teach science content in context, help students evaluate differing perspectives and develop and argue their own positions. The third module spanned approximately four weeks. The stated goal of the module was, “We use in-depth analysis of HIV/AIDS to investigate how complex pathogens, politics, and ideologies contribute to infectious disease epidemics locally and globally.” Specific learning goals included biology content, such as differentiating between viruses and retroviruses, analysis of public epidemiological data, and argumentation regarding political and ideological controversies around the disease. The largest part of the module included discussion of a controversy ignored by most scientists and the media over whether the HIV virus actually causes AIDS, which was presented in two papers from the journal, Science (Duesberg, 1988 and Blattner, 1998). Before class, students read the position papers and completed a reflective writing assignment asking them to answer why they do or do not believe HIV causes AIDS, what issues were important from the readings, and five areas they needed to learn more about to resolve the problem. Team members were instructed to share their responses, pool knowledge and research some answers to questions they had identified. They were also asked to develop a consensus list of key issues from the readings. To better understand Duesberg’s 49 argument against HIV as the causative agent in AIDS, each team was assigned one or two of Duesberg’s ideas to investigate and explain in depth for the next class. Students used additional sources to develop short presentations on such topics as accepted postulates of virology, normal characteristics of retroviruses, and normal presentation of disease after viral infection. Students’ final assignment for “The Duesberg Phenomenon” was to present an argument supporting or refuting Duesberg’s argument. One professor explained the assignment, emphasizing that students should focus on making good arguments. Students’ handout read, “Your goal in your investigation is to evaluate Duesberg’s position and that of his critics with reference to current scientific knowledge. As you evaluate their positions, consider what we have learned about evaluating scientific evidence: for which side is the evidence strongest? What additional information is needed to fully evaluate the competing positions?” Students were told they were expected to consult relevant sources other than those provided to the class, finding information that was current and reliable. Each team was expected to define both Duesberg’s and Blattner’s positions clearly and use evidence to make an argument for their positions. Students made 5-7 minute presentations, answered questions posed by their peers and professors, and turned in a summary of their argument. In the second part of the module, students considered government responses to the AIDS epidemic, using the contexts of Brazil and South Africa. First the class was introduced to Brazil’s position rejecting funds from the United States, which were set aside for AIDS programs on the condition that the country make a declaration condemning prostitution. The students were asked to consider, “Is it appropriate for the 50 U.S. government to place ideological constraints on funding for global health initiatives?” Teams were split in half by “yes” and “no” positions, and were instructed to develop “logical evidence based arguments” for assigned positions. Each side was given about 30 minutes to research their positions, and then they made their arguments to their team members. As a class they presented preliminary points for “yes” and “no” positions. The teams then developed consensus positions and arguments supporting their positions incorporating background information on the history of US funding for AIDS programs and of AIDS in Brazil. In a handout they were told, “The most effective arguments will take into account the points made by the opposition.” For their final projects, teams were assigned a paper comparing and contrasting how AIDS is experienced in Brazil and South Africa, and presenting a “multiscalar model of AIDS.” Teams were expected to integrate aspects of the biology of AIDS with epidemiological data, prevention strategies, treatments, and factors influencing treatment of individual patients in each country. Teams were required to provide a visual representation of the model with a complete description. 400 Level Core Course This core course titled “Complex Problems of Humanity” was considered the capstone for the Human Biology program and was geared toward student advocacy. Unlike the other three core courses, it was not team-taught, but lead by the director of the program. The course was primarily collaborative and project-based and students took responsibility for the direction of the course. It involved service learning components, working with other organizations to participate in the National Global Warming TeachIn, and work with the local Parks and Recreation Department to assess and research local 51 water quality. This course focused not only on understanding socioscientific issues and arguing positions based on evidence, but challenged students to organize and act based on these positions. The course description in the syllabus read as follows: In this course students will focus on significant problems at the interface of science and society, such as global warming, water contamination and scarcity, fossil fuel exploitation, insufficient global healthcare, and inefficient use of dwindling land resources. Students will advocate for change so as to persuade policy makers and community leaders to support change using innovative approaches that reflect the foundations of science. Learning goals included engaging scholars in an endeavor to educate the community about the complexity of problems and the need for different perspectives in finding solutions, understanding of different dimensions of problems from local to global, understanding advocacy through individual reflection and team work, and learning how to confront challenges as “an engaged citizen with an evidence-based approach to advocacy.” Three full modules were completed in the course. The first module was intended to educate the public and focused on global warming. The second module, intended to engage students in the community, included a service learning project with local Parks and Recreation and a state river water quality program. In this module, students researched the definition of a water shed, reflected on films and completed case studies on ecological and political concerns involving water, and connected this knowledge to local water issues through collecting water samples and conducting original research projects. The final project included group poster presentations of this research open to the university and community. The third module included a personal reflection on advocacy through an audio-recorded “This I Believe” statement. To illustrate how goals of SSI were enacted in this course I will discuss the first module in-depth. 52 The four week global warming module began with general education on the subject through independent study and discussion of various readings including Gore’s (2006) An Inconvenient Truth. The majority of class time was spent on the collaborative project of planning and holding an on-campus teach-in incorporating the goals of the National Teach-In (http://www.nationalteachin.org). Students were expected to review the goals of the National Teach-In and review the scientific data and human and ecological implications of global warming independently. In class, students were introduced to two graduate students from biology and political science who organized the first teach-in at the university the previous year. These graduate students described the outcomes of the previous teach-in as well as specific challenges and suggestions for the next one. The majority of class time was spent on organization of the event. The professor led a team-building activity where students grouped themselves according to their styles of working in teams. They expressed the strengths and weaknesses of their team work styles and what others needed to know about them to work effectively. Some described themselves as detail-oriented, others as big picture-oriented, some as most concerned about getting things done, and others with understanding different points of view. This activity prepared students to approach a difficult and complex task with understanding and strategies for incorporating different approaches. After having opportunities to brainstorm ideas for the teach-in with input from the graduate students, students were asked to condense and report their goals for the teach-in. These included clarifying myths about global warming, getting good attendance, motivating participants, educating participants on how they could help, providing 53 information on local resources for energy conservation, becoming more fluent with ideas and vocabulary related to global warming, and making impacts like reducing carbon footprints and improving health. Students shared ideas for local groups and professors who might prepare presentations or exhibits for the teach-in. They were also given opportunities to ask questions of the class. They shared what they knew about contacting politicians, how human health can be related to global warming, and what kinds of visual aids might be useful for conveying the environmental impact of global warming. For the remainder of the module, students worked in groups to reserve a room in the student union, book speakers and representatives of community resources to meet goals previously specified, manage funding resources and ask businesses for donations, and decide on schedule and room layout issues. The teach-in lasted a full day and was well attended by students, professors, and passers-by. Several representatives of local conservation resources were available for consultation, professors gave presentations on relevant research, and brochures and student-developed educational resources were available. At the end of the module, students were asked to reflect upon the experience, including conceptual learning as well as personal and team-building experiences. They prepared a list of effective strategies and suggestions for future teach-ins. Overall in this module, students studied the scientific and social implications of global warming, developed informed positions about what could and should be done about the issue, and advocated for their cause through a collaborative effort combining local and national resources. This activity not only helped them reason in a socioscientific context, but to put their knowledge and personal 54 positions into action. This required reaching consensus among many different individual perspectives to create and carry out specific goals. Biology Comparison Group BIO majors were recruited as a reasonable comparison group to SSI majors, considering they take many of the same courses and pursue similar career and professional paths. Although their coursework is very similar to SSI majors and they take many of the same courses, they were chosen as a comparison group because they did not participate in the series of core courses, Human Biology community events, or portfolios which were designed to develop socioscientific reasoning and promote reflection on interdisciplinary, biology-related issues. Since there were no correlates to Human Biology core courses and individual programs of study varied greatly, I will describe the basic curriculum and major goals. The Biology major offered Bachelor of Arts (B.A.) and Bachelor of Science (B.S.) degrees. The B.A. required three more foreign language courses and six more courses in arts, humanities, cultural studies, and social or historical sciences, four fewer courses in chemistry, physics, and math, and two fewer upper level biology courses. The same core courses in introductory biology, molecular biology, and evolution were required for both degrees. Many of the courses had associated labs. Based on my limited observation of biology courses (molecular biology), participation as a lab instructor (histology), and student interviews, I understood that biology courses were primarily lecture-based. In lecture sessions, professors usually used slideshow presentations and students took notes. Students were given opportunities to ask questions, and many large courses scheduled discussion sessions with graduate instructors, where students worked 55 on problem sets or brought questions on course material. Lab courses offered students opportunities to learn techniques and verify concepts taught in class, and in some instances were inquiry-based where students independently investigated their own research questions. Although we could not verify that all biology courses did not have SSI components, no biology course descriptions reviewed included SSI, and none of the 16 students interviewed reported having been involved in significant in-depth discussion of SSI in biology courses. Methodology Worldview I approached my research from a worldview incorporating elements of pragmatism and constructivism (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007). This is consistent with Creswell’s and Plano Clark’s worldview stance 2, which states, “Researchers can use multiple paradigms or worldviews in their mixed methods study” (2007, p. 27). From a pragmatist perspective, I approached the study asking “what works,” specifically, what are the significant differences in outcomes and experiences in students who choose an alternative program as opposed to those who choose a traditional major? Creswell and Plano Clark explain that in a pragmatist approach the researcher takes multiple stances, some of which are biased and some which are unbiased. Combination of qualitative and quantitative research methods is appropriate to this worldview (p. 24). Where a pragmatist approach may combine elements of post-positivism and constructivism, within this approach I lean toward a constructivist worldview. Within the question of “what works,” I recognize multiple realities. Although I hope to reveal trends, I also seek to bring out different perspectives of participants. The majority of the study 56 takes an inductive approach, beginning with students’ understandings and looking for themes or trends. Appropriately, the majority of my study is qualitative in nature, using semi-structured interviews and open-ended surveys where codes emerge from student responses (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007, p. 24) Research Design The design for this mixed methods study is consistent with the triangulation design, in which qualitative and quantitative methods are combined “to obtain different but complementary data on the same topic” (Morse, 1991, p. 122, Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007 p. 62). Combination of qualitative and quantitative data helps to validate or problematize findings from individual sources. Although in part of my study I transform qualitative questionnaire data into quantitative results, (consistent with the Data Transformation Model), the Convergence Model best describes my study. In this model, both qualitative and quantitative data are collected and results are converged. To interpret results, outcomes of each component are compared and contrasted to validate or further explain a phenomenon (p. 64-65). I consider my design to be concurrent, since data was primarily collected in one phase (p. 81). Interviews and observations were conducted in the same phase as the questionnaires, although questionnaires of only interviewed students were reviewed prior to interviews to allow me to check validity of questionnaires and probe any unclear aspects of questionnaire responses. Participants Participants included students at the mid-point and end of their college careers. These groups were chosen because the Human Biology senior class was very small (19 students), and both 200 level and 400 level core classes were in session. 200 level 57 participants included 30 SSI students (77% of the class) and a matching sample of 30 BIO students, and 400 level included 15 SSI (79% of the class) and 20 BIO students. SSI students were recruited from core classes, and BIO majors were recruited from biology classes at the 200 and 400 levels. The only criteria for participation in the study were major and level of progression in the major, as determined by the levels of recruitment courses. I recognize the convenience sample was not ideal, but reasonable to make data collection feasible. Recruitment courses included two sections of 200 level molecular biology (approximately 200 students each) and four lab sections of 400 level Human Tissue Biology (approximately 120 students total). SSI students were recruited first, and recruitment of BIO students continued until nearly equal sample sizes were reached. Payment of twenty dollars served as an incentive to participate in surveys, and those who were willing to participate in interviews were treated to coffee during the interview. For the SSI group, 200 level students included 23 females and 7 males and 400 level included 14 females and 1 male. For BIO, 200 level students included 18 females and 12 males, and 400 level included 10 females and 10 males. Self-reported ethnicities for 200 level SSI stuents included 21 White, 4 African American, 1 Multi-racial, 1 Hispanic, 1 South Asian, 1 Greek, and 1 not identified. For 400 level SSI students, 11 reported themselves as White, 1 as African American, 1 as Hispanic, and 2 as Multiracial. For 200 level BIO, 28 reported themselves as White, 1 as Asian, and 1 as Middle Eastern. For 400 level BIO students, 12 reported themselves as White, 2 as African American, 4 as Asian, and 1 as Middle-Eastern, and 1 was unidentified. Overall, SSI and BIO participants reported similar professional goals and grade point averages (with BIO students approximately .2 points higher on a 4 point scale; see 58 Table 1). SSI and BIO students were nearly equal in students planning to go to medical school and graduate school or research. Small but comparable numbers of students from each program planned to study law, public health or social work, and nursing. A greater number of BIO students planned to enter other graduate level health professions, like dentistry, optometry, and physical or occupational therapy. Responses to why students chose their majors indicate that the biology major was a close fit to the requirements for these professional programs. SSI students were slightly more likely to plan to enter the workforce or to obtain a business degree. Minor choices reveal some differences in focus of study between SSI and BIO participants. Minors are reported by adding together all minors listed by students in each group. Some students had multiple minors and some had no minor. Multiple minors in one category for one student were counted as one minor (one instance). Double majors were few, but were included as minors because they illustrate additional expertise. BIO students were more likely to have multiple minors than SSI students (43% vs. 17% respectively in 200 level classes and 55% vs. 40% in 400 level classes). This could be due to the interdisciplinary nature of the Human Biology program. Since focus areas allow students to explore areas outside of biology, a minor may not be viewed as necessary to illustrate expertise. SSI participants were more likely to minor in psychology or business. These students may be more interested in behavioral aspects of humans. More SSI participants minored in biology, exercise science, or nutrition, by taking enough additional biology courses to meet the qualifications. It is important to note that the biology minor was only an option for SSI students. BIO students were much more likely to minor in chemistry. Informal discussions with students suggest this is because 59 the requirements for the biology major include a great deal of chemistry. BIO students were also much more likely to minor in areas of the humanities, such as foreign language, literature, creative writing, music, or visual arts. The reason for this is unclear, although a more focused curriculum may offer more flexibility to pursue an additional focus area. Groups were similar in social science minors and public health. Table 1 Demographic information for SSI and BIO participants. SSI 2 n=30 Career Path Medicine/PA Nursing Other grad health profession Work/other Graduate School/Research MBA Public Health/Social work Undecided/unknown Law Minor Psychology Other social science Human Sexuality Humanities Business/Manage-ment Public Health Biology/Exercise Science/Nutrition Chemistry Information Technology none Average GPA Lab Experience (n/n reported) Teaching Experience (n/n reported) BIO2 n=30 SSI4 n=15 BIO4 n=20 SSI Total n=45 BIO Total n=50 13(43%) 3(10%) 4(13%) 13(43%) 2(7%) 9(30%) 5(33%) 1(7%) 4(27%) 6 (30%) 0 8(40%) 18 (40%) 4(9%) 8(18%) 19(38%) 2(4%) 17(34%) 2(7%) 3(10%) 0 4(13%) 2(13%) 1(7%) 0 2(10%) 4(9%) 4(9%) 0 6(12%) 2(7%) 0 0 0 0 2(13%) 0 1(5%) 2(4%) 2(4%) 0 1(2%) 2(7%) 1(3%) 1(3%) 1(3%) 0 0 2(10%) 1(5%) 2(4%) 1(2%) 3(6%) 2(4%) 9(30%) 4(13%) 2(7%) 7(23%) 5(17%) 0 3(10%) 5(17%) 4(13%) 0 13(43%) 0 1(3%) 2(7%) 6(40%) 3(20%) 1(7%) 1(7%) 0 1(7%) 3(20%) 5(25%) 6(30%) 0 4(20%) 2(10%) 1(5%) 0 15(33%) 7(16%) 3(7%) 8(18%) 5(11%) 1(2%) 6(13%) 10(20%) 10(20%) 0 17(34%) 2(4%) 2(4%) 2(4%) 1(3%) 0 5(17%) 3.20 3/27 (11%) 4/27 (15%) (30%) 1(3%) 6(20%) 3.41 7/30 (23%) 4/30 (13%) 4(27%) 0 2(13%) 3.32 3/13 (23%) 6/13 (46%) 13(65%) 1(5%) 1(5%) 3.41 6/18 (33%) 8/18 (44%) 5(11%) 0 7(16%) 3.26 6/40 (15%) 10/40 (25%) 22(44%) 2(4%) 7(14%) 3.41 13/48 (27%) 12/48 (25%) SSI and BIO participants both reported that one fourth of the groups had experience teaching, including undergraduate teaching assistantships, teaching programs 60 for children, and health-related training programs. BIO students were about twice as likely to have participated in a research group (BIO: 27%, SSI 15%). Although the Human Biology program worked to encourage undergraduate research, surveys suggested biology majors may have participated more to be competitive for professional programs. General Procedures of Data Collection Recruitment Participants were recruited from 200 level and 400 level classes and lab sessions. Since all SSI students at the 200 and 400 levels were currently enrolled in core courses, recruitment was conducted at the end of these class sessions. For BIO students, I consulted with the director of Human Biology who was also a biology professor to choose comparable classes to recruit biology majors. BIO participants were recruited from two 200 level molecular biology lecture classes and four lab sections of a 400 level human tissue biology class. This course was chosen because of its human focus. In each class, I presented an overhead slide explaining the purpose of the study, the compensation of 20 dollars for participating in the 75 minute questionnaire, and the available times to complete the questionnaire in a computer lab on campus. At the end of class I provided sign-up sheets for students to choose a time to complete the questionnaire. One to two days before scheduled sessions I sent email reminders to participants. Collection of Questionnaires I oversaw each questionnaire session in computer labs on campus. First students read and signed a consent form to participate in the study. They then completed a written demographic information page (see Appendix C), which asked students to submit an identification code, as well as demographic data, major, minor, career plans, and 61 activities. While completing these sheets, students were emailed links to the Biology Concept Inventory and a Survey Monkey link, which included the questionnaires on SSI (DMQ) and scientific inquiry (VOSI; see Appendix D). Students identified themselves in the Survey Monkey questionnaire with the identification code entered on their demographic sheets. Students were allowed as much time as they needed, but in general spent one hour to 75 minutes. General Interview Procedure Demographic sheets of students who checked a box on their consent forms indicating that they were willing to participate in an optional interview were separated by year and major. I interviewed a subset of sixteen participants (four from each group by year and major). SSI participants were chosen to best represent the population of the small program by sex and ethnicity, then to provide a variety of grade point averages and course backgrounds. BIO participants were chosen to match SSI participants as closely as possible by these criteria. No male students from the 200 level BIO group were available for an interview, so only female students were interviewed. Those willing to be interviewed made arrangements to meet me in a neutral environment like a coffee shop. They were treated to coffee as compensation. The interviews followed a semistructured interview protocol (see Appendix F), but interviews varied depending on the interests or concerns of the participants. In addition to interview notes, interviews were audiotaped and transcribed, with the exception of one interview (200 level BIO participant), for which audio data could not be recovered. For this interview, extensive interview notes were used for analysis. 62 Course Observations and Professor Interviews Observations and professor interviews served as secondary data sources. To provide context for the core courses, I attended more than half of class sessions for both 200 and 400 level Human Biology core courses. Field notes were taken describing the activities and atmosphere of classes attended, and all professors for these courses were interviewed to further establish goals for SSI and perceptions of student progress. One interview with both 200 level professors was audiotaped and transcribed. The director of the program who taught the 400 level class consulted with me often and participated in advising on the dissertation. In addition, copies of syllabi, assignments, and handouts used in these classes were collected. Data Collection for Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI Decision Making Questionnaire (DMQ) Participants took a modified version of the Decision Making Questionnaire (Bell & Lederman, 2003) near the end of the spring semester. The questionnaire was developed by Bell based on various resources and validated through review by an expert panel of four science educators and two scientists, then final modifications were made. Science and technology issues were chosen to represent real controversial issues in which citizens may need to consider and interpret a great deal of evidence to make decisions. Although this questionnaire was originally used several years ago, I selected it because it was designed to measure socioscientific reasoning in adults. Also, the scenarios and the science behind them were likely to be familiar to a general audience of science students, so little time was needed to provide participants with background information. The original questionnaire included scenarios of science and technology- 63 related controversial issues, including fetal tissue implantation, climate change, the relationship between diet and cancer, and smoking. To reduce the time required of participants, and because of the highly emotional nature of the topic, I chose to omit the first scenario on fetal tissue implantation. Scenarios were followed by questions that asked students to take yes or no positions and explain the factors influencing their decisions. SSI Portion of Interviews In addition to the DMQ questionnaire, Bell & Lederman (2003) conducted follow-up interviews with participants, where they asked probing questions to validate questionnaire responses and evaluate reasoning strategies. I used or adapted these questions to further probe reasoning in each of the three scenarios (see Appendix F), and asked clarifying questions about student responses on the DMQ. Follow-up questions specifically asked how participants made decisions in response to opposing arguments, which were still debated at the time of the interview. Data Analysis for Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI Comparison of Decisions The modified DMQ was analyzed blindly to groups. “Yes,” “no,” and “undecided” decisions for each question were totaled for the four groups and compared by percentages of students choosing each decision. Differences in decisions between SSI and BIO students were tested for significance using Fisher’s Exact tests for 200 level, 400 level, and total groups. Since undecided decisions were few, they were omitted from the analysis. 64 Comparison of Factors in Decision-Making Based on the entire set of unidentified DMQ responses, categories of factors considered in decision-making for each question were established through several rounds of development and revision. As similar themes in these factors emerged, category codes were developed, refined, and used to re-code questionnaire responses. After I refined and reduced the codes, a second researcher confirmed these codes or suggested adjustments to the coding scheme, based on her analysis of an approximate 20% sample of questionnaires (20 questionnaires). Finally, I reviewed all questionnaire responses and adjusted coding to accommodate those small adjustments. Since clusters of questions in the DMQ received similar responses in factors considered, questions were grouped into emergent clusters that differed slightly from grouping of questions by scenario. For each cluster, a list of “reasoning categories” was developed and refined from the codes designated for the respective questions. For each questionnaire, reasoning categories represented at least once in each cluster were determined. Coded questionnaires were then identified by year and major, and the number of students citing each reasoning category were compared for groups by calculating percentages. Development of Reasoning Rubric To assess reasoning with SSI, a scale was adapted from Zohar’s and Nemet’s (2002) argument analysis, and Tal and Hochberg’s (2000) Reasoning Complexity Rubric originally developed from Hogan, Nastasi, & Pressley, (2000). The simplified scale included number and explanation of justifications (see Tables 2 and 3). Like both cited analyses, the rubric rated responses on number of justifications supporting their decisions 65 as well as whether students explained an underlying reason or mechanism for their justifications (Tal & Hochberg, 2000; Zohar & Nemet, 2002). No points were awarded when no reason was cited or the reason was nonsensical in the context of the question, one point was awarded for one unelaborated or unexplained justification, two points were given for two or more unelaborated justifications, three points were given for one elaborated justification, four points were given for multiple justifications with one elaboration, and five points were given for multiple elaborated justifications. Table 2 Rubric for reasoning and perspectives applied to DMQ Reasoning Score (R) 0- 1- 2- 3- 4- 5- No justification/nonsensica l in context of question One justification of decision: mechanism unelaborated Two or more justifications of decision: mechanisms unelaborated One justification of decision: mechanism explained with examples Two or more justifications of decision: one mechanism explained Two or more justifications of decision: multiple mechanisms explained Perspectives Score (P) Example: Ban Smoking? 0- No evidence of multiple perspectives 1- Recognizes other perspectives exist, but does not elaborate them. 2- Elaborates on different perspectives, but offers no logical conceptual resolution. 3- Considers different perspectives in depth and reaches a clear, complex resolution No…tobacco companies are right in saying that smoking is a free choice of the consumer. However, it is not the free choice of the nonsmoker receiving passive cigarette smoke. So, though cigarette smoking should not be illegal, there should, however, be legislation passed that confines cigarette smoking only to smokers. R: 4; Two reasons support decision: free choice and reasonable alternative (explained) P: 3; Resolution incorporates perspectives of smokers, tobacco companies, and nonsmokers 66 Table 3 Examples of scoring for reasoning scale. Score Participant Response Explanation for scoring 1 No, that would have a negative effect on the economy One unelaborated justification. 2 Yes, I do not like cigarette smoke in general, and children seem to be starting smoking earlier and earlier--it is very sad. Two separate unelaborated justifications (personal dislike, children smoking) 3 I would be willing to pay increased taxes to provide funding for research on alternative energy resources because in the end by being more efficient and less dependent on foreign oil I will save money. One justification is explained. 4 Yes, I believe that more money should be given to this research and IMPLEMENTATION. It is already understood how solar and wind work, but they must be implemented! We must be a leader in the fight on global warming, demonstrating that this issue is at the forefront of our concerns. Two justifications given (focus on implementation and example for world). Implementation is elaborated, but why the US needs to lead is not. 5 No [do not make smoking illegal]. With as much information about the risks of smoking available today, people should be responsible enough to educate themselves and make their own decisions about smoking. I do however think that smoking in public areas should be illegal because then you are exposing others to danger. Two justifications are given for the position (keep smoking legal, but not in public areas). Each justification is explained. 67 Table 4 Examples of scoring for perspectives scale Score Participant Response Explanation for scoring 0 I would be ok with this [tax increases to fund alternative energy] because these techniques are much better for our environment. Only one perspective is evident 1 Yes, people who choose not to smoke should not be put in danger by those who choose to smoke. There should be a special place designated for smokers because they chose to live an unhealthy lifestyle. Recognizes the position of “those who choose to smoke,” but does not fairly consider this point of view 2 It should because people that smoke are putting themselves at risk for many types of cancer and diseases. This in turn increases the cost of healthcare that will have to be provided because of their smoking. However, it also is someone's decision or not to smoke, and you can't really pass legislation against it because they have the liberty to do it, even though it is extremely harmful. Expresses health value of legislation, but also recognizes the individual’s control over health. However, no resolution is reached. 3 On a personal level I believe they should [set limits on emissions], though I do see reasons for not doing so. The united states competes in the global market with nearly every country on earth, and if one country uses methods that are cheaper, though cause more pollutants and greenhouse gasses, they are more likely to perform better economically due to their ability to produce the product at less of a cost for the present (though long term these often will cost us more). If a legally-binding limit can be reached, I do think the united states however, as one of the more powerful nations, has somewhat of an obligation to do its best in complying with whatever may be better for the future of mankind. Explains and offers examples for reasons for supporting and nonsupporting positions. A resolution is reached where the influence of the US is seen as an adequate reason to commit despite the drawbacks. Since multiple perspectives were commonly cited in questionnaires, though not explicitly elicited by the DMQ, a perspectives score was adapted from the “synthesis” component of the Reasoning Complexity Rubric (Tal & Hochberg, 2000). Since 68 inclusion of multiple perspectives was generally not in-depth due to the short nature of responses, a simplified 3-point scale was used. No points were given if the participant discussed only one perspective, one point was awarded when another perspective was recognized, two points were given when another perspective was elaborated, but not resolved with the perspective guiding the decision, and three points were given when multiple perspectives were elaborated and incorporated into a resolution consistent with the decision (see Tables 2 and 4). The scoring rubric was reviewed for validity by the advising faculty member and the researcher completing secondary coding. I analyzed the whole DMQ data set and the second coder independently analyzed an approximate 20% sample (20 questionnaires). Analysis was conducted blind to groups. Inter-rater reliabilities based on a 20% sample were 78% for reasoning and 85% for perspectives. Discrepancies were resolved to reach 100% agreement and the remainder of the sample was then revised for consistency. Average reasoning scores were computed for each student, and independent-samples ttests were conducted to compare SSI and BIO group totals. Data for average perspectives scores were skewed toward the lower end of the scale, so I used a Mann-Whitney test for non-parametric data for this scale. Interview Analysis Qualitative questionnaire data and interview transcripts were triangulated to enhance validity of interpretations. Analysis of DMQ follow-up questions from Bell and Lederman (2003) was guided by themes developed from King and Kitchener’s Reflective Judgment Model (1994), with insights from Sadler, Barab, and Scott (2007), including view of knowledge, recognition of complexity, consideration of perspectives, and use of 69 evidence (see Table 5). Student responses to the three follow-up questions were assessed using the criteria described in Table 5. SSI and BIO participants were then compared across groups. Responses to general questions about experiences with SSI were compared within groups for emergent themes, then compared across groups. Table 5 Assessment themes for stages of reflective judgment Theme Pre-reflective Quasi-reflective View of knowledge Complexity Absolute, authority driven Not perceived Uncertain, contextual Tentative and inquiry-based Other perspectives Evidence Unrecognized Perceived, frustrated with ambiguity Contextual nature complicates evaluation Used idiosyncratically in reasoning Understood, criteria applied for evaluation Considered across contexts Not considered; truth is directly observable Reflective Evaluated by criteria, applied in context Data Collection for Understanding of Scientific Inquiry Views of Scientific Inquiry (VOSI) Questionnaire The second section of the questionnaire included seven questions from the “Views of Scientific Inquiry” (VOSI) questionnaire (Schwartz, Lederman, & Lederman, 2008), which measures students’ understanding of the inquiry process. I chose to use questions from versions used with pre-service and in-service teachers and high school students. I considered these versions, rather than the scientist version (VOSI-Sci) most appropriate for college science majors, since most do not have experience with science outside of the classroom context. In addition, one question from the Views of the Nature of Science questionnaire (VNOS, Lederman, Abd-El-Khalick, Bell, & Schwartz, 2002), was included to probe students’ understanding of theory and tentativeness in science. The VOSI was developed to investigate participants’ understanding in the 70 following concepts: 1) scientific investigations are guided by questions, 2) scientists use multiple methods, 3) there are multiple purposes driving investigations, 4) scientists must justify scientific knowledge, 5) scientists must recognize and manage anomalous data, 6) data and evidence are different, and 7) scientists work in communities of practice. Due to time constraints, I included only seven questions from VOSI (see Appendix D). The focus concepts often overlapped between questions, so these were coded in groups to elucidate students’ understandings of these concepts. I chose this open-ended instrument to assess student responses about the nature of scientific inquiry. The inquiry process is central to the disciplines of biology as well as the social sciences. This questionnaire asks students to reflect on the meaning of inquiry and what scientists do as individuals and as a community. A reflective approach to content and process is a key component of the Human Biology program, and as Boix Mansilla and Duraising (2007) assert, reflection on the roles and integration of disciplines is a criterion for quality interdisciplinary work. Thompson Klein (2007) explains that integration of disciplines allows “metacritical reflection.” This questionnaire was used to probe whether SSI students have come to view scientific inquiry differently from those in a traditional program. Inquiry Portion of Interview The majority of this segment of the interview involved asking students to explain or clarify their responses to questions on the modified VOSI. Participants were asked to explain their understanding of scientific inquiry and discuss how they developed that understanding. Questions varied depending on students’ questionnaires, but most 71 participants were asked to explain their conceptions of experiments, data/evidence, and theories. Data Analysis for Understanding of Scientific Inquiry VOSI Analysis Qualitative analysis questions began with initial coding of responses with targeted concepts of the survey in mind. These included “processes of inquiry,” “meaning of experiment,” “purpose of inquiry,” “definition or existence of scientific method,” “difference between data and evidence,” “subjectivity” and “identification of anomaly” (modified from start codes in Schwartz, 2004). The final question borrowed from the VNOS (Lederman, Abd-El-Khalick, Bell, & Schwartz, 2002) assessed tentativeness of science and definition of theory. Questions from all blinded questionnaires were coded in groups that corresponded to focus concepts of the questions (see Appendix G) Emergent codes were compiled throughout the initial analysis and after the first round, these codes were reduced and revised. The questionnaires were then cyclically revised and recoded until consistent. A graduate student familiar with the VOSI was consulted to review the final coding scheme and suggestions were incorporated. After coding was complete, questionnaires were divided into groups and codes were tallied for each group. Percentages for each code were computed, since group size differed, and these were compared across groups. Analysis of Inquiry Portion of Interview This portion of the interview was used primarily to ascertain the accuracy of my interpretations of VOSI responses. I compared my initial interpretations of the VOSI to student responses to probing and clarification questions and found answers to be 72 consistent. I also coded the inquiry portion of the text according to themes from interview questions, including conceptions of inquiry, experiment, data and evidence, and theory. I compared general codes among students within groups and across groups. Data Collection for Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge Biology Concept Inventory (BCI) Basic biology content knowledge was assessed using the Biology Concept Inventory (BCI, Klymkowsky & Garvin-Doxas, 2008). The BCI was developed and researched to measure content knowledge of central biology concepts and reveal misconceptions. Distractor questions were based on common misconceptions and written in students’ words from interviews conducted in the development of the BCI to reveal students’ levels of understanding (D’Avanzo, 2008). The BCI was developed based on responses of more than 500 college students to open-ended questions about biological concepts. The major source of misconceptions revealed in the development of this tool was students’ belief that random processes are very inefficient, while biological processes are efficient, having some sort of “driver,” such as natural selection or concentration gradients. Students did not understand the connection between constantly occurring random processes and complex behaviors that emerge (Klymkowsky & Garvin-Doxas, 2008). I chose the BCI to measure how students understood the major concepts in the discipline of biology while targeting common misconceptions. Quality interdisciplinary work demonstrates grounding in the disciplines employed (Boix Mansilla and Duraising, 2007), so students in both the Human Biology and biology majors should demonstrate high levels of basic content knowledge. Since the concept inventory focuses on major 73 concepts in biology rather than detailed processes or vocabulary from biology subdisciplines, it should fairly assess students who have chosen different biology electives and those who have not yet taken upper-level classes. The analysis should shed light on whether Human Biology majors develop different levels of basic biology content knowledge, since they invest a great deal of time considering social aspects of scientific issues. Perceptions of Content Knowledge Portion of Interview As a part of the interview protocol for the 16 students interviewed, participants were asked, “How would you describe your level of biology content knowledge after completing (or at this point in) your major?” Students were then asked to respond to three open-ended questions used in the development of the BCI to check whether student misconceptions were consistent with those represented in the literature on the concept inventory. Data Analysis of Levels and Perceptions of Content Knowledge BCI Analysis Due to a communication error concerning the BCI software, a large portion of BCI data could not be retrieved. Printed copies of the BCI were requested from students, and fairly equivalent samples were obtained for each subgroup (15/30 from 200 level BIO, 13/30 from 200 level SSI, 11/20 from 400 level BIO, and 13/15 from 400 level SSI). Due to small sample size, a paired t-test was conducted to compare the means of the total scores on the BCI between the Human Biology students and the biology students. Analysis of Content Portion of Interview 74 Student responses to BCI validation questions were checked for consistency with the assertions of the BCI developers, finding similar misconceptions in student responses from both groups. All text concerning student perceptions of their content knowledge was reviewed for emergent themes first in individual transcripts, then comparing within subgroups and majors. Finally themes from SSI and BIO majors were compared to reveal similarities and differences in perceptions. Data Collection for General Perceptions of Major Interview Data Collection Within the semistructured interviews, students from both groups were asked to talk about their perceptions of classroom learning environments in their majors. They were asked about teaching strategies they found helpful and unhelpful, as well as how they viewed levels of community and interaction with professors. They were directed to speak primarily of courses within their major, but they were free to discuss experiences in any of their courses. Participants were also asked if they had experienced non-traditional teaching strategies, which deviated from a typical lecture format. Finally, students were asked to talk about personal outcomes of their majors and what experiences, inside or outside of normal major requirements were most significant in their development. Analysis of General Major Perceptions Text from the general perceptions segments of the interviews were coded independently for major ideas and compared within groups for emergent themes. These themes, supported with quotes and examples were compared and contrasted between SSI and BIO groups. 75 Summary of Methods In summary, I compare SSI and BIO groups in terms of socioscientific reasoning, understanding of scientific inquiry, and levels and perceptions of content knowledge. In addition I identify themes in students’ general perceptions of their majors. I use a mixed methods convergence model of triangulation design (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007) comparing and contrasting various data sources including both qualitative and quantitative data to develop interpretations (see Figure 1). Figure 1. Convergence model of triangulation design for dissertation Note. Adapted from Creswell and Plano-Clark (2007). 76 CHAPTER 4: RESULTS Socioscientific Reasoning and Perceptions of SSI Comparison of Decisions On eleven situational position-taking questions on the modified DMQ, SSI and BIO groups were very similar in their responses (see Table 6). Decisions were compared between groups for 200 and 400 level classes and total SSI and BIO students. According to Fisher’s Exact probability tests of “yes” and “no” responses for SSI and BIO groups at each level, the only significant difference was for question 8 in the 400 level class (SSI 60% yes, BIO 90% yes, p=.027). This question asked whether students exercised on a regular basis, and differed from other questions in that it tested behavior rather than reasoning. Although results were not significant, larger differences between groups were seen for question 1, where BIO students were more likely to support legally binding limits on carbon emissions (more pronounced at 200 level, 63% SSI vs. 87% BIO). Also, BIO participants were more likely to answer “yes” to question 7, reporting that they were likely to incorporate research into their decisions about what they eat (more pronounced at 400-level, 33% SSI vs. 50% BIO). Finally, 400-level SSI participants were more likely to answer “no,” (80% SSI vs. 50% BIO) to question 10, asking whether students thought cigarette smoking should be made illegal. 77 Table 6 Percentages of SSI and BIO students by decision for questions on the modified DMQ Question numbers Climate change and policy 1 2 3 4 Health research/food choice 6 7 8 Regulation of food or tobacco 9 10 11 12 Total SSI 200/400 Yes SSI BIO Total 200/400 BIO Total SSI 200/400 No SSI BIO Total 200/400 BIO Total 63/87 33/47 70/60 63/73 71 38 67 67 87/85 47/40 83/80 77/60 86 44 82 70 30/13 63/53 20/33 33/27 24 60 24 31 10/15 53/60 17/20 23/30 12 56 18 26 73/80 50/33 80/60 76 44 73 80/85 57/50 70/90 82 54 78 23/7 43/60 17/40 18 49 24 17/15 40/40 20/5 16 40 14 70/47 40/20 83/100 90/93 62 33 90 91 65 60/60 30/50 87/85 83/90 60 38 86 86 70 30/53 57/80 17/0 7/7 38 64 11 7 32 40/40 67/50 13/15 17/10 40 60 14 14 28 Comparison of Factors Influencing Reasoning Through multiple rounds of coding, distinct categories were developed for factors students indicated as influencing their reasoning in each of three clusters of questions. These factors could be positively influencing their decision, negatively influencing it, or simply brought up for consideration. In this comparison, the quality of reasoning is not considered, but simply the types of factors mentioned. Questions from the DMQ were clustered by similarity in responses. Ten categories were developed for the climate change and policy cluster (questions 1-4), 9 categories were developed for the diet and health research/food choice cluster (questions 5-9), and 11 categories were developed for the regulation of food or tobacco cluster (questions 9-12). 78 For the climate change and policy cluster, factor categories included any reference to the environment, evidence related to global warming, the need to keep individuals or nations accountable for their role in climate change, the political influence the US has in the world, influence of decisions on public perception of the US, personal values or responsibility, political views, practical outcomes of decisions, suggestions of alternative options to those suggested in the DMQ, or economic aspects of decisions. For the diet and health research/food choice cluster, factor categories included food and exercise choices as part of general lifestyle, preferences unrelated to research, long-term effects like disease prevention, limitations on time and resources, influence of personal experiences (such as witnessing family member’s disease), knowledge of research, unawareness of research, distrust of research, and ambivalence toward health knowledge or research. For the regulation of food or tobacco cluster, factor categories included health, scientific evidence, need or desire to remove the problem through regulation, health as personal responsibility, moral or social concerns, personal preferences, smoking/unhealthy foods being appropriate in some situations, the need to be consistent (as comparing cigarettes with alcohol), economic concerns, practical concerns, and alternatives to regulation. In general, SSI and BIO participants cited similar factors as influencing their reasoning (see Tables 7-9). Total SSI and BIO groups did not vary by more than 15% on the frequency of citing individual factors within question clusters. They differed by less than 10% on the majority of factors (24/30). However, SSI students cited more factors as influencing their decisions, considering the percentage of students citing particular factors is higher for the majority of categories (29/30). 79 Table 7 Factors influencing reasoning in Climate change and policy question cluster of DMQ Factors Environment Evidence Accountability Political influence Public perception Personal values Political views Practical outcomes Other options Economic %BIO2 (n=30) %SSI2 (n=30) SSI2BIO2 (%) %BIO4 (n=20) %SSI4 (n=15) SSI4BIO4 (%) %Total BIO %Total SSI SSI-BIO Total 83.3 66.7 -16.7 50 33.3 16.7 70.0 55.6 -14.4 33.3 43.3 10 35 46.7 11.7 34.0 44.4 10.5 23.3 43.3 20 40 26.7 13.3 30.0 37.8 7.8 33.3 13.3 -20 35 46.7 11.7 34.0 24.4 -9.5 6.7 23.3 16.7 25 26.7 1.7 14.0 24.4 10.4 56.7 70 13.3 75 66.7 8.3 64.0 68.9 4.9 16.7 16.7 0 15 26.7 11.6 16.0 20.0 4.0 63.3 70 6.7 70 66.7 3.3 66.0 68.9 2.9 46.7 63.3 16.7 50 60 10 48.0 62.2 14.2 90 80 -10 80 80 0 86.0 80.0 -6.0 Table 8 Factors influencing reasoning in Diet and health research/food choice question cluster of DMQ Factors Lifestyle choice Personal preferences Limitations Long-term effect Personal experience Research Unaware of research Distrust research Ambivalence % BIO2 (n=30) %SSI2 (n=30) SSI2BIO2 (%) % BIO4 (n=20) %SSI4 (n=15) SSI4-BIO4 (%) 83.3 90 6.7 100 93.3 -6.7 16.7 30 13.3 35 46.7 11.7 10 30 13.3 16.7 3.3 -13.3 20 25 26.7 33.3 6.7 8.3 16.7 20 3.3 5 20 15 40 13.3 33.3 10 -6.7 -3.3 20 5 26.7 0 6.7 -5 10 16.7 6.7 15 6.7 -8.3 3.3 6.7 0 13.3 13.33 3.3 80 %Total BIO %Total SSI SSIBIO Total 90.0 91.1 1.1 24.0 14.0 35.6 17.8 11.5 3.8 28.0 22.2 -5.8 12.0 32.0 20.0 31.1 8.0 -0.9 10.0 6.7 -3.3 12.0 2.0 13.4 8.9 1.4 6.9 Table 9 Factors influencing reasoning in Regulation of food or tobacco question cluster of DMQ % BIO2 (n=30) Factors Health Evidence Remove problem Personal responsibility Moral/Social Personal Preferences Appropriate sometimes Consistency Economic Practical concerns Other options %SSI2 (n=30) SSI2-BIO2 (%) % BIO4 (n=20) %SSI4 (n=15) SSI4BIO4 (%) %Total BIO %Total SSI 36.7 6.7 40 20 3.3 13.3 35 20 26.7 20 -8.3 0 36.0 12.0 35.6 20.0 SSIBIO Total -0.5 8.0 63.3 53.3 -10 35 73.3 38.3 52.0 60.0 8.0 70 56.7 -13.3 40 73.3 33.3 58.0 62.2 4.2 76.7 6.7 90 13.3 13.3 6.7 100 10 86.7 20 -13.3 10 86.0 88.9 2.9 8.0 15.5 7.5 0 13.3 13.3 25 13.3 -11.7 6.7 23.3 26.7 13.3 20 -10 10 10 6.7 20 -3.3 10 10.0 8.0 18.0 13.3 20.0 15.5 3.3 12.0 -2.4 33.3 23.3 -10 55 60 5 42.0 35.5 -6.4 50 40 -10 35 66.7 31.7 44.0 48.9 4.9 Differences of more than 10% between total SSI and BIO groups were found for four reason categories. In the global warming question cluster, SSI students were more likely to discuss alternatives to legislation (62% vs. 48%), refer to public perception of U.S. (24% vs. 10%), and refer to evidence (44% vs. 34%), but were less likely to include environmental factors in their decisions (56% vs. 70%). In the diet and health research/food choice cluster, SSI participants were more likely to report that they make food and exercise choices according to personal preferences or tastes (35% vs. 24%). In the regulation of food and tobacco cluster, SSI students were more likely to discuss consistency with other laws as influencing their reasoning (20% vs. 8%). Although the difference for total groups was smaller, 400 level SSI students were much more likely than BIO participants to cite availability of other options, like banning smoking in public places, and the importance of personal responsibility as reasons why smoking should not be banned (67% SSI vs. 35% BIO). They were also more likely to 81 discuss legislation of unhealthy foods or tobacco as a way to remove the problem (73% vs. 35%). Comparison of Reasoning Using an independent samples t-test, mean reasoning scores were found to be significantly higher for SSI students (M=3.46, SD=.63) than for BIO students (M=3.19, SD=.68), t(93)= -1.98, p=.05; (see Table 10). For both SSI and BIO groups, reasoning scores were higher for 400 level groups by about .2 points on the 5-point scale. Table 10 Reasoning scores for SSI and BIO groups 200 level Bio SSI (n=30) (n=30) Reasoning Mean 3.10 3.40 Reasoning St. Dev. .66 .61 *p< .05 400 level Bio SSI (n=20) (n=15) 3.33 3.58 .70 .67 Bio (n=50) 3.19 .68 Total SSI (n=45) 3.46 .63 Sig. .050* For perspectives scores, although the SSI mean (M=1.24, SD=.71) was slightly higher than the BIO mean (M=1.04, SD=.57) no significant difference was found between groups according to a Mann-Whitney test for non-parametric data (See Table 11). Still, it is worth noting that although the number of DMQ items in which participant responses included any reference to multiple perspectives was nearly equal between groups (SSI mean= 3.9; BIO mean= 3.8), of these responses, SSI students scored a higher percentage of scores of 3 (55% vs. 47%), and lower percentage of scores of 1 (34% vs. 43%). 82 Table 11 Perspectives scores for SSI and BIO groups 200 level Perspectives Mean Perspectives St. Dev *p<.05 Bio (n=30) .93 .54 SSI (n=30) 1.10 .67 400 level Bio (n=20) 1.06 .59 SSI (n=15) 1.26 .73 Total Bio (n=50) 1.04 .57 SSI Sig. (n=45) 1.24 .154 .71 Comparison of Reasoning in DMQ Follow-up Questions In interviews and surveys, the majority of both SSI and BIO students’ responses to scenarios were consistent with King and Kitchener’s (1994) reflective stages. Students from both groups viewed knowledge as uncertain, tentative and inquiry-based, except two SSI and two BIO students who suggested there was a single “best” answer. All students perceived complexity of problems with only one BIO student hindered by it. All students considered alternate perspectives across contexts, except one BIO student who had difficulty resolving them. The majority of students in both groups sought resolutions by recognizing similarities in different perspectives, for example, using their biological knowledge to combine theories of carbon emissions and deforestation in contributing to climate change. All students except one BIO student discussed applying criteria to logically evaluate evidence. For example, most students said that to persuade them make dietary changes, claims must have accumulated a great deal of long-standing evidence. 83 Perceptions of SSI in Majors Views of SSI Students All SSI students interviewed mentioned that they highly valued incorporation of social aspects of biology-related problems in Human Biology. Gary described the major as follows: …the premise of our written assignments were to not only include the biology or the physiology or the science aspect of what is going on in these problems [controversial issues], but also to discuss the social implications, whether it’s a good or a bad thing, what views are held, if we accept the view, what would that change about society, things like that. And so it’s almost like the core of the major is actually to focus on these things. Not simply what is the science, but how does it affect people and why is that important? Sarah contrasted Human Biology courses with traditional biology courses: Because a regular biology class would be like, oh, this is what a retrovirus does, like AIDS. Well, now in this class, we’re learning all about AIDS, what goes into it, how it affects the body more in-depth, how it affects the social aspects, where in another class, we’re not really going to talk about, like how people get treatment and how they (inaudible) funds or anything, and that’s what we talked about in this class. So I feel like what we get is more specific issues, but then within those issues it covers the whole range of the issue. All SSI students except one 400 level and one 200 level student noted that the ability to consider multiple perspectives is important in science and health fields. Laura cited a recent newspaper commentary arguing that academics had become too specialized. “He said, you don’t have anyone in the world who understands the water issue from every perspective. You have economists who understand it from that perspective, you have politicians who understand it from that perspective, you have people who are thirsty who understand it from that perspective. He said, but you don’t have anyone who can put it all together.” Laura argued that to solve eminent social and 84 ecological problems like water conservation, people need to be familiar with different disciplinary perspectives. Kelly also referred to science careers, explaining that Human Biology has helped her decide to pursue a degree in public health as well as medical training. She said, And I think that’s what Human Biology makes me realize is that, when a patient comes through in the clinic, there are other things going on that you don’t see, that you have to think about, like can the patient afford to be in there, are they going to take their medications, there are so many other things on the different levels that you have to consider, that you don’t just consider as an MD. Sarah related the importance of incorporating multiple perspectives to her current volunteer work in a cancer center and a women’s shelter. “And it just reinforces how I want to help people, all the issues that surround, the social issues that we talked about in class, I experience that now.” Despite the value she placed on considering multiple perspectives, one student voiced concern that this training would not be valued in professional school or the workplace. Laura said, “I’m worried that I’m going to encounter people who are not interdisciplinary and people who are very rigid scientists and they aren’t going to be able to appreciate the sociology.” She felt that the program’s focus on integrating disciplinary perspectives was not yet common or accepted in professional programs SSI students plan to enter. All SSI students also said that they valued exposure to new ideas or controversies. Several students mentioned that Human Biology core courses illuminated controversy in issues they had never seen as problematic. For example, Laura said, “I didn’t know that there was going to be a water crisis until we studied it in [the 400 level core course].” Shawna said, “I guess it kind of made me realize how controversial some things are that 85 I’ve already had pretty opinionated stances on.” She said she became more willing to reevaluate her positions in light of new information and perspectives. Kelly said, Some of [the issues], I think, you didn’t even know were controversial. Like, I mean, to certain things, like the AIDS case we just studied, so it’s like you know, I never knew that anybody believed that HIV [doesn’t cause] AIDS, I never knew that. So I think that’s one of the biggest advantages of Human Biology is that they raise awareness about the controversy that does exist, and how different perspectives like on the same issue, get completely different results. Or what people believe is completely different. Views of BIO Students When asked how well prepared they believed they were to make decisions on science issues with social implications, all BIO students said that they had little experience with SSI in traditional science classes they had taken. Polly said, In terms of just the biology classes I don’t think that they have prepared me very well to have a stand because teachers usually, if they do bring up controversial topics…right now I’m thinking about stem cell research, um so I’m really interested in that, but the bulk of knowledge I do have about that is outside of the classroom. And I took cell biology where they like touched on it, but I mean not into the amount of detail that one would need to have a stand. In addition, all BIO participants felt that their biology courses focused primarily on mechanisms. They felt it was left to the student to connect social aspects or alternative perspectives. Ellen said, “I think they’ve kind of created those opportunities with the genetics and the molecular biologies to, um, understand the mechanisms behind this, but I think it’s left upon the individual to like search for those answers [to social problems] in other disciplines or in the same disciplines in other fields...” Tracy said that such issues had been briefly addressed in her biology classes, but she hadn’t had opportunities to explore them in depth. The professor gave “just the overall result, what we learned from 86 it and move on.” When asked if she had had opportunities to integrate perspectives on socioscientific issues in her courses, Carrie said, “I would probably say no. I mean, they pretty much all focus in their own area. They never really connect to each other, explicitly. I’ve never really thought about it that much either.” Although all BIO students were interested in considering the social implications of biological issues, one student felt that students would resist in-depth discussion of SSI in biology courses. Kevin said, On one degree I can see why it would be really good for them to teach us that…but honestly I can see how they make it optional. Just because, even for biologists, we have a lot of people who have hard core believes that have been raised since childhood… And so I can see how a university would try to distance themselves from offering something that would just cause a flare up or something. It’s not quite the 70s anymore but you still watch out. Although they had few experiences discussing SSI in their biology courses, all BIO participants said they had taken classes that explored social aspects of scientific issues to some degree. These courses included anthropology and human genetics, medical sociology, psychology, and religion and evolution. Approaches to studying issues in these courses varied. Some students (2 of 4 who discussed this) felt those courses focused on social aspects and expected knowledge on or generalized the biology content. Carrie said of her medical sociology course, I think that most of the students in that class were like premed or predental or whatnot, so I think that they probably assumed that you have some biological background, but, we never really got into anything really science, it was more like issues that, like people who were going into the sciences, particularly medical careers and how those related to sociological aspects of how we live. Some students (2 of 4 who discussed the issue) said that their courses focused on both biological concepts and social implications. Tracy said, “Yes, it [anthropology and 87 human genetics] was about 60/40 [science/social issues]. [The professor] provided a book that was optional, like a genetics dictionary, and it is a 2-day a week class, so the first day she would have a lecture about the topic, so we wouldn’t be in the dark. And the next day we would discuss.” Despite having little SSI integrated into biology courses, half of BIO participants at both levels reported feeling more able to make well-formed arguments with SSI. These students integrated their knowledge from science and social science courses to consider particular issues in depth. Kevin explained that the emphasis on supporting ideas with evidence in the biology major helped him to reason more effectively with SSI. “Honestly I think my major helped me argue this stuff a lot. Which is good, it gave me a way to [use] my evidence and support my theories with it, in all honesty, supported from a logical view, whether than being too emotional about one of my ideas.” Tracy attributed her ability to argue for a position in SSI to her anthropology courses. She said, I thought at the beginning when you said that a lot of students are tripped up by the first questions [from DMQ] was interesting because I know for a fact that if I didn’t have a lot of my anthropological classes, I would get tripped up on those. But since I’ve applied them, and have to explain them over and over again, it’s like I have a deeper understanding, whether they are correct or not, of being able to sit with someone and explain the effect to them. And then, I love the smoking example, because it’s definitely something I’ve thought about and I just, I was very excited to write about what I thought. Overall, BIO participants expressed concern about SSI and found ways to apply their biological knowledge to such problems. 88 Understanding of Inquiry Modified Views of Scientific Inquiry Questionnaire Emergent codes from the modified VOSI questionnaires were reduced and tallied by groups (see coding scheme in Appendix G) Percentages of participants citing codes for each question are included in Tables 12-25. A code was included in the tables only if at least 5% of the total SSI or BIO groups cited that code. Processes and Purposes of Inquiry. Question 1 asked, “What types of activities do scientists do to learn about the natural world? Be specific about how they go about their work” (see Table 12). SSI students were more likely to indicate that scientists could participate in either natural sciences or social sciences (22% vs. 8%), refer to the scientific method in their responses (36% vs. 20%), and indicate that inquiry begins with a question (47% vs. 36%). BIO participants were more likely to discuss a purely experimental view of science (38% vs. 22%) and discuss process skills of science, such as hypothesizing and collecting and analyzing data (80% vs. 69%). 89 Table 12 Codes for VOSI Question 1 Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 DIFF METHODS 56.67 40.00 16.67 40.00 45.00 -5.00 51.11 42.00 9.11 SCIENTIFIC METHOD 26.67 20.00 6.67 53.33 20.00 33.33 35.56 20.00 15.56 DIFF SCIENCES 20.00 6.67 13.33 26.67 10.00 16.67 22.22 8.00 14.22 QUES 36.67 43.33 -6.67 66.67 25.00 41.67 46.67 36.00 10.67 CONTROL 13.33 10.00 3.33 13.33 10.00 3.33 13.33 10.00 3.33 EXPERIMENTAL 20.00 33.33 -13.33 26.67 45.00 -18.33 22.22 38.00 -15.78 PROCESSES 66.67 73.33 -6.67 73.33 90.00 -16.67 68.89 80.00 -11.11 GENERAL INQUIRY 13.33 13.33 0.00 0.00 5.00 -5.00 8.89 10.00 -1.11 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO Note. Question 1 asked, “What types of activities do scientists do to learn about the natural world? Be specific about how they go about their work.” Question 2 asked, “How do scientists decide what and how to investigate?” (see Table 13). Responses were coded as internal, external, or practical concerns. Groups were fairly consistent in the types of concerns they cited, although the BIO group was slightly more likely to cite only internal concerns (18% vs. 11%) and the SSI group was slightly more likely to cite all three types of concerns (29% vs. 22%). The SSI group was also more likely to cite more than one type of concern (69% vs. 60%). 90 Table 13. Codes for VOSI Question 2 Code %SSI2 %BIO2 INTERNAL 10.00 13.33 EXTERNAL 10.00 INT/EXT %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 -3.33 13.33 25.00 -11.67 11.11 18.00 -6.89 13.33 -3.33 0.00 5.00 -5.00 6.67 10.00 -3.33 36.67 36.67 0.00 26.67 15.00 11.67 33.33 28.00 5.33 6.67 16.67 -10.00 6.67 5.00 1.67 6.67 12.00 -5.33 ONE TYPE 23.33 26.67 13.33 26.67 10.00 0.00 40.00 13.33 35.00 30.00 5.00 -16.67 28.89 22.22 22.00 28.00 6.89 -5.78 MORE THAN ONE TYPE 66.67 63.33 3.33 73.33 55.00 18.33 68.89 60.00 8.89 INT/PRACT INT/EXT/ PRACT %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO Note. Question 2 asked, “How do scientists decide what and how to investigate? Describe all the factors you think influence the work of scientists. Be as specific as possible.” Meaning of Experiment Question 3 asked participants to “Write a definition of a scientific experiment” (see Tables 14-16). BIO participants were more likely than SSI participants to include testing of a hypothesis (74% vs. 60%) and ensuring validity or precision of experiments (18% vs. 7%). When asked, “Give an example from something you have done or heard about in science that illustrates your definition of a scientific experiment,” BIO participants were more likely to discuss an experiment conducted in a research lab or by a research team (28% vs. 11%). They were also slightly more likely to give an explanation why the example they described was an experiment that was consistent with their definition of experiment. (82% vs 76%). 91 Table 14 Codes for VOSI Question 3a Code %SSI 2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO REPLICABLE 13.33 13.33 0.00 26.67 10.00 16.67 17.78 12.00 5.78 SCIENTIFIC METHOD 16.67 16.67 0.00 20.00 0.00 20.00 17.78 10.00 7.78 TESTS HYPOTHESIS 60.00 70.00 -10.00 60.00 80.00 -20.00 60.00 74.00 -14.00 6.67 20.00 -13.33 6.67 15.00 -8.33 6.67 18.00 -11.33 CONTROLLED 30.00 26.67 3.33 26.67 20.00 6.67 28.89 24.00 4.89 GENERAL INQUIRY 26.67 20.00 6.67 13.33 15.00 -1.67 22.22 18.00 4.22 VALID/ PRECISE Note. Question 3a asked, “Write a definition of a scientific experiment. A scientific experiment is…” Table 15 Codes for VOSI Question 3b Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO RESEARCH GROUP 10.00 30.00 -20.00 13.33 25.00 -11.67 11.11 28.00 -16.89 WHOLE PROJECT 20.00 13.33 6.67 20.00 20.00 0.00 20.00 16.00 4.00 CLASS 46.67 46.67 0.00 46.67 35.00 11.67 46.67 42.00 4.67 SOCIAL SCIENCE 10.00 3.33 6.67 6.67 10.00 -3.33 8.89 6.00 2.89 Note. Question 3b asked, “Give an example from something you have done or heard about in science that illustrates your definition of a scientific experiment. 92 Table 16 Codes for VOSI Question 3c Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO CONSISTENT 80.00 76.67 3.33 66.67 90.00 -23.33 75.56 82.00 -6.44 INCONSIST -ENT 16.67 16.67 0.00 33.33 10.00 23.33 22.22 14.00 8.22 Note. Question 3c asked, “Explain why you consider your example to be a scientific experiment.” Definition or Existence of the Scientific Method When asked, “Is there one scientific method or set of steps that all investigations must follow to be considered science?” SSI and BIO groups varied by only 3% in their responses, where 69% of SSI students and 72% of BIO students responded “yes” (see Tables 17-19). When asked to describe the steps of the method, SSI students were more likely to include asking a question (49% vs 22%) and making observations (27% vs 8%). BIO students were more likely to include hypothesizing (68% vs 23%) and analysis (40% vs. 29%) in their responses. When those who answered that there was not one scientific method were asked to “describe two investigations that follow different methods” and “explain how the methods differ and how they can still be considered scientific,” 400 level SSI students were more likely to explain that social sciences may not invoke the scientific method, but are still scientific (11% vs. 4%). 93 Table 17 Codes for VOSI Question 4a Code %SSI2 %BIO2 YES 70.00 76.67 %SSI2%BIO2 -6.67 NO 30.00 23.33 6.67 %SSI4 %BIO4 66.67 65.00 %SSI4%BIO4 1.67 33.33 35.00 -1.67 %SSI %BIO 68.89 72.00 %SSI%BIO -3.11 31.11 28.00 3.11 Note. Question 4a asked, “What do you think? Is there one scientific method or set of steps that all investigations must follow to be considered science?” Table 18 Codes for VOSI Question 4b QUESTION OBSERVAT -ION HYPOTHES IS ANALYSIS COMMUNI CATE REVISE/ REPEAT DATA BG RESEARCH CONCLUSIONS MAY VARY %SSI2 %BIO2 50.00 33.33 %SSI2%BIO2 16.67 30.00 10.00 60.00 %SSI4 %BIO4 46.67 5.00 %SSI4%BIO4 41.67 20.00 20.00 5.00 70.00 -10.00 40.00 26.67 40.00 -13.33 0.00 13.33 13.33 70.00 20.00 %SSI %BIO 48.89 22.00 %SSI%BIO 26.89 15.00 26.67 8.00 18.67 65.00 -25.00 53.33 68.00 -14.67 33.33 40.00 -6.67 28.89 40.00 -11.11 -13.33 6.67 0.00 6.67 2.22 8.00 -5.78 23.33 70.00 20.00 -10.00 0.00 0.00 20.00 60.00 13.33 10.00 60.00 5.00 10.00 0.00 8.33 15.56 66.67 17.78 18.00 66.00 14.00 -2.44 0.67 3.78 60.00 56.67 3.33 46.67 55.00 -8.33 55.56 56.00 -0.44 6.67 3.33 3.33 0.00 10.00 -10.00 4.44 6.00 -1.56 Note. Question 4b asked, “If you think there is one scientific method, what are the steps of this method?” 94 Table 19. Codes for VOSI Question 4c Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 6.67 6.67 0.00 20.00 0.00 20.00 11.11 4.00 7.11 CONTROL 16.67 0.00 16.67 6.67 25.00 -18.33 13.33 10.00 3.33 EXPLORE/ CONFIRM 3.33 10.00 -6.67 13.33 5.00 8.33 6.67 8.00 -1.33 DIFFERENT METHODS 13.33 10.00 3.33 6.67 5.00 1.67 11.11 8.00 3.11 INVESTIGATE 6.67 6.67 0.00 6.67 15.00 -8.33 6.67 10.00 -3.33 SYSTEMAT IC 6.67 0.00 6.67 6.67 5.00 1.67 6.67 2.00 4.67 DIFFERENT SCIENCES %SSI4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO Note. Question 4c asked, “If you think that scientific investigations can follow more than one method, describe two investigations that follow different methods. Explain how the methods differ and how they can still be considered scientific.” Impact of Researcher on Science and Subjectivity. When asked, “If several scientists, working independently, ask the same question and follow the same procedures to collect data, will they necessarily come to the same conclusions?” SSI students were only slightly more likely (93% vs. 86%) to say “no” than BIO participants (see Table 20). They were more likely to cite error as a reason for differences (47% vs. 30%), but BIO participants were more likely to cite different interpretations of data (40% vs. 31%). When asked, “Does your response to (a) change if the scientists are working together?” BIO participants were only slightly more likely to respond that their answer would not change (62% vs. 58%, see Table 21). SSI students more commonly responded that scientists would still be using different procedures (18% vs. 4%), while BIO students were more likely to cite error (16% vs. 7%) or different interpretations of scientists (28% vs. 20%). 95 Table 20 Codes for VOSI Question 5a Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO NO 96.67 86.67 10.00 86.67 85.00 1.67 93.33 86.00 7.33 ERROR 50.00 26.67 23.33 40.00 35.00 5.00 46.67 30.00 16.67 DIFFEENT DATA 40.00 56.67 -16.67 53.33 30.00 23.33 44.44 46.00 -1.56 DIFFERENT METHODS 23.33 6.67 16.67 6.67 35.00 -28.33 17.78 18.00 -0.22 DIFFERENT INTERPRET ATIONS 33.33 46.67 -13.33 26.67 30.00 -3.33 31.11 40.00 -8.89 CAN COME CLOSE 10.00 3.33 6.67 6.67 0.00 6.67 8.89 2.00 6.89 YES 3.33 3.33 0.00 0.00 5.00 -5.00 2.22 4.00 -1.78 MAYBE 0.00 6.67 -6.67 13.33 10.00 3.33 4.44 8.00 -3.56 SAME PROC/ DATA 0.00 3.33 -3.33 20.00 5.00 15.00 6.67 4.00 2.67 0.00 6.67 -6.67 0.00 10.00 -10.00 0.00 8.00 -8.00 DIFFERENT INTERP Note. Question 5a asked, “If you think there is one scientific method, what are the steps of this method? If several scientists, working independently, ask the same question and follow the same procedures to collect data, will they necessarily come to the same conclusions? Explain why or why not.” 96 Table 21. Codes for VOSI Question 5b Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO CHANGE 36.67 30.00 6.67 20.00 15.00 5.00 31.11 24.00 7.11 SAME DATA 10.00 10.00 0.00 20.00 5.00 15.00 13.33 8.00 5.33 CONSENSUS 23.33 20.00 3.33 13.33 5.00 8.33 20.00 14.00 6.00 REDUCE ERROR 10.00 3.33 6.67 0.00 10.00 -10.00 6.67 6.00 0.67 NO CHANGE 53.33 60.00 -6.67 66.67 65.00 1.67 57.78 62.00 -4.22 DIFF. METHODS 16.67 0.00 16.67 20.00 10.00 10.00 17.78 4.00 13.78 DIFF. INTERP 20.00 33.33 -13.33 20.00 20.00 0.00 20.00 28.00 -8.00 DIFF. DATA 10.00 3.33 6.67 13.33 15.00 -1.67 11.11 8.00 3.11 ERROR 3.33 16.67 -13.33 13.33 15.00 -1.67 6.67 16.00 -9.33 MORE LIKELY SAME 6.67 13.33 -6.67 6.67 15.00 -8.33 6.67 14.00 -7.33 MAYBE CHANGE 10.00 10.00 0.00 13.33 20.00 -6.67 11.11 14.00 -2.89 SAME PROC/ DATA 10.00 3.33 6.67 13.33 10.00 3.33 11.11 6.00 5.11 CONSENSUS 10.00 6.67 3.33 13.33 5.00 8.33 11.11 6.00 5.11 6.67 3.33 3.33 6.67 10.00 -3.33 6.67 6.00 0.67 DIFFERENT INTERP. Note. Question 5b asked, “Does your response to (a) change if the scientists are working together? Explain.” Difference Between Data and Evidence. When asked, “What does the word “data” mean in science?” BIO participants were more likely to define it as information collected in inquiry (90% vs. 73%), whereas SSI participants often defined it as “results” (22% vs. 8%, see Table 22). When asked, “Is ‘data’ the same or different from ‘evidence’?” both groups responded “different” (89% SSI and 86% BIO, see Table 23). BIO students were somewhat more likely to respond 97 that evidence is less precise or exact than data (12% vs. 4%), but many reported that it was more certain or definitive than data (10% BIO vs. 4% SSI). SSI participants were more likely to report that data can be evidence, but not explain how they are different (11% vs. 2%) and to say that evidence is data that has been analyzed or interpreted (9% vs. 2%). Table 22 Codes for VOSI Question 6a Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO INFO COLLECTED 76.67 86.67 -10.00 66.67 95.00 -28.33 73.33 90.00 -16.67 RESULTS 23.33 10.00 13.33 20.00 5.00 15.00 22.22 8.00 14.22 NUMERIC 10.00 10.00 0.00 0.00 5.00 -5.00 6.67 8.00 -1.33 QUANT/ QUAL 10.00 13.33 -3.33 26.67 10.00 16.67 15.56 12.00 3.56 Note. Question 6a asked, “What does the word “data” mean in science?” 98 %SSI%BIO Table 23 Codes for VOSI Question 6b Code %SSI 2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 DIFFER EVIDENCE SUPPORTS/ EVIDENCE IS MORE CERTAIN EVIDENCE IS LESS PRECISE 86.67 86.67 0.00 93.33 85.00 8.33 88.89 86.00 2.89 56.67 46.67 10.00 33.33 65.00 -31.67 48.89 54.00 -5.11 3.33 13.33 -10.00 6.67 5.00 1.67 4.44 10.00 -5.56 6.67 6.67 0.00 0.00 20.00 -20.00 4.44 12.00 -7.56 3.33 3.33 0.00 13.33 5.00 8.33 6.67 4.00 2.67 6.67 6.67 0.00 6.67 5.00 1.67 6.67 6.00 0.67 10.00 3.33 6.67 13.33 0.00 13.33 11.11 2.00 9.11 6.67 13.33 0.00 13.33 6.67 0.00 13.33 6.67 5.00 10.00 8.33 -3.33 8.89 11.11 2.00 12.00 6.89 -0.89 10.00 13.33 -3.33 0.00 10.00 -10.00 6.67 12.00 -5.33 EV. IS GENERALIZATION DATA IS NUMBERS DATA CAN BE EVIDENCE EVIDENCE IS ANALYZED SAME SUPPORT %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO Note. Question 6b asked, “Is “data” the same or different from “evidence”? Explain.” Methods of Data Analysis When asked, “What is ‘data analysis’?” and “What is involved in doing data analysis?” both groups most commonly cited interpretation or making meaning of data (89% SSI and 96% BIO, see Tables 24). SSI participants were much more likely to include visualizing data through graphs or charts (29% vs. 14%) and checking validity or accuracy of data collection (22% vs. 6%). They also included using statistics more (40% vs. 30%) and compiling and reviewing data (16% vs. 6%). Both groups showed marked differences between 200 and 400 levels in inclusion of statistical tests (SSI increasing 50% and BIO increasing 42%) and finding patterns in data (SSI increasing 17% and BIO increasing 20%). 99 Table 24 Codes for VOSI question 7 Code %SSI2 %BIO2 VISUALIZE 26.67 13.33 %SSI2%BIO2 13.33 CHECK VALIDITY/ ERROR 30.00 6.67 INTERPRET 86.67 COMPARE %SSI4 %BIO4 33.33 15.00 %SSI4%BIO4 18.33 23.33 6.67 5.00 96.67 -10.00 93.33 10.00 16.67 -6.67 STATISTICS 23.33 13.33 ORGANIZE 16.67 REVIEW/ COMPILE DATA %SSI %BIO 28.89 14.00 %SSI%BIO 14.89 1.67 22.22 6.00 16.22 95.00 -1.67 88.89 96.00 -7.11 26.67 5.00 21.67 15.56 12.00 3.56 10.00 73.33 55.00 18.33 40.00 30.00 10.00 20.00 -3.33 0.00 0.00 0.00 11.11 12.00 -0.89 23.33 10.00 13.33 0.00 0.00 0.00 15.56 6.00 9.56 REDUCE/TR ANSFORM 10.00 6.67 3.33 0.00 0.00 0.00 6.67 4.00 2.67 PATTERNS 23.33 20.00 3.33 40.00 40.00 0.00 28.89 28.00 0.89 Note. Question 7 asked, “(a) What is “data analysis”? and (b) What is involved in doing data analysis?” Tentativeness of Theory and Purpose of Theory. The final question asked, “After scientists have developed a theory, does the theory ever change? If you believe that theories do change, explain why theories are still important to the scientific community.” The majority of both groups responded “yes” (98% SSI and 96% BIO, see Table 25). Both groups commonly reported that discovery or availability of new data contributed to the tentativeness of theory (49% SSI and 46% BIO), but BIO majors were more likely to discuss new technology as a catalyst for change (16% vs. 7%). BIO majors were also more likely to discuss a falsification view of changing theories, explaining that theories are put forth by scientists to be retested until they are either disproven or withstand adequate attempts at falsification (26% vs. 8%). 100 Surprisingly, the BIO group was slightly more likely to cite new interpretations of existing data (8% vs. 2%). Both groups commonly explained that theories were important for generation of ideas for future inquiry (47% SSI and 42% BIO), but SSI students were more likely to report that theories are needed for lively discussion within scientific communities (16% vs. 8%). BIO majors were slightly more likely to explain that theories are important to provide a record of scientific thinking over time (8% vs. 2%). Table 25 Codes for questionnaire Question 8 Code %SSI2 %BIO2 %SSI2%BIO2 %SSI4 %BIO4 %SSI4%BIO4 %SSI %BIO %SSI%BIO 96.67 96.67 0.00 100.00 95.00 5.00 97.78 96.00 1.78 43.33 36.67 6.67 60.00 60.00 0.00 48.89 46.00 2.89 6.67 13.33 -6.67 6.67 20.00 -13.33 6.67 16.00 -9.33 0.00 10.00 -10.00 6.67 5.00 1.67 2.22 8.00 -5.78 10.00 6.67 3.33 6.67 0.00 6.67 8.89 4.00 4.89 26.67 23.33 3.33 40.00 35.00 5.00 31.11 28.00 3.11 3.33 10.00 -6.67 0.00 0.00 0.00 2.22 6.00 -3.78 16.67 6.67 10.00 6.67 15.00 -8.33 13.33 10.00 3.33 10.00 30.00 -20.00 33.33 20.00 13.33 17.78 26.00 -8.22 46.67 46.67 0.00 46.67 35.00 11.67 46.67 42.00 4.67 16.67 6.67 10.00 13.33 10.00 3.33 15.56 8.00 7.56 3.33 6.67 -3.33 0.00 10.00 -10.00 2.22 8.00 -5.78 YES NEW DATA (NEW TECH) NEW INTERPRET ATIONS SCIENCE CHANGES BEST CURRENT EXPLANATION THEORY LESS TRUE THAN LAW FINAL TRUTH EXISTS FALSIFICAT -ION VIEW IDEAS FOR NEW INQUIRY PROMOTES DISCUSSION HISTORICAL RECORD Note. Question 8 asked, “After scientists have developed a theory, does the theory ever change? If you believe that theories do change, explain why theories are still important to the scientific community. Defend your answer with examples.” 101 Inquiry Portion of Interviews Definition of Inquiry SSI group. For the SSI group, all the 400 level students and three of the 200 level students defined inquiry as questioning and trying to understand or explain phenomena. One 200 level student said inquiry was recognizing and solving a problem. Three 400 level students mentioned there were different ways of approaching inquiry, and one 200 level student said scientists needed to look at problems at different levels. Kelly said, Like if you have a good hypothesis, you’re going to look at everything from life style to the biology. You can’t just straight look at it, like the blood pressure…it’s kind of like if you really want to get to the bottom of the problem, you have to look at it from the lense of a biologist as well as a sociologist…So yes, when you get to the testing, or the experimental part of it, it should be everything from the actual, the way you think of it as a laboratory experiment, to observing a person in their environment, like their life style. BIO group. Similarly, three of the 400 level BIO students and two of the 200 level BIO students said inquiry was seeking to answer a question. One 400 level and one 200 level student said inquiry was solving a problem, and one 200 level student defined inquiry by the processes of collecting and interpreting data. Experiences Leading to Inquiry Understanding SSI group. All of the 400 level SSI students mentioned an upper level physiology class taught through the biology department by the director of Human Biology as having a significant role in establishing their understanding of inquiry. Three 400 level and two 200 level SSI students referred to the Human Biology program in general as significant, while one at each level cited research lab experience, one at each level cited everyday life experiences, one 400 level student cited reading scientific literature, and one 200 level student cited classes in general. In support of Human Biology, Gabrielle said, 102 I think the majority of where I learn this is Human Biology, just because, and it’s not all biology based, like you have to look at different problems thru the sociological perspective and so, but you still learn the scientific method, you come up with a question, and you kind of try to figure out what’s working or not and also I think it helps that we did a lot of research especially in upper physiology, we did a lot of research and lab reports that really prepared me for that. Gary said, “I think actually, this is one thing I’m happy about, the Human Biology program for…just the idea that, things like inquiry aren’t set to just one set of standards. There isn’t just a lab procedure that someone gives to you, that you perform and you write a lab report on. We’re asked to do our own investigation, things like that.” BIO group. For the biology students, three 400 level and two 200 level students said they gained their understanding of inquiry through classes (one of which included social science classes), one 400 level and three 400 level students cited work in research labs, two 400 level and one 200 level student cited K-12 science classes, two 400 level students cited life or college in general (science is an intuitive or human process), and one student cited interaction with scientists, influence of family members or listening to the media. Two BIO students related their experience in research labs very differently. Chloe felt her lab experienced reinforced her previous learning on the scientific method. She said, I feel like kind of even in high school you learn about the scientific method, stating your hypothesis, and all your variables and stuff like that. You feel like you actually understand it and [the research lab] does help to actually be in that environment. See it there on paper. Or try to accomplish this. We’re going to do this experiment to figure it out. We’ll do this data analysis to see the results. Tracy felt that her class experiences taught her a different scientific method than she experienced in the research lab. 103 Honestly, my only experience with anything to do with the scientific method has to be in the labs I’ve taken and that would be chemistry labs and those very, very much follow that guideline. They have the purpose that you want to find, what you’re going to use, the fact that you need to have a positive or a negative control, everything is along with what my high school education has been, and in the classes where there aren’t any labs, sure the professor will bring up and experiment but we don’t go into the minute details of how it was conducted, we just get the results and what it means to us. Tracy felt that her science classes presented a prescriptive scientific method, offered “cookbook” style lab experiences, and presented outcomes, not actual processes of scientific research. Definition of Experiment SSI group. For SSI students, two of the 400 level and all of the 200 level students defined “experiment” the same as “inquiry,” involving questioning and carrying out various processes to reach conclusions. Only two 400 level students discussed manipulating or controlling variables. For the 200 level SSI students, one understood inquiry as the procedural part of the scientific method, and one was unable to define experiment. Of three SSI students from each level asked to discuss the scientific method, two 400 level and one 200 level students mentioned varied, “fluid,” or “circular” enactment of steps. One 400 level and two 200 level students said there were different methods of inquiry included in the scientific method. One said there is no scientific method, arguing that clinical observation doesn’t fit the steps, but is still scientific. BIO group. For the BIO group, two students from each level defined experiment similar to inquiry in general, as a way to find answers or test a hypothesis. One 400 level and two 200 level BIO students said that an experiment involved manipulating or controlling variables (one mentioned inclusion of social science research), and two 200 104 level students said experiments could include naturalistic or observational studies. One 200 level student said experiments attempt to disprove something, while inquiry attempts to answer a question. Two students at each level discussed the scientific method, where one at each level described it as a general process of hypothesis testing and one at each level mentioned varied orders of steps. Understanding of Data and Evidence SSI group. For SSI students, all 400 level and three 200 level students understood that evidence differed from data in that it is used to “make a case “ for an assertion, support or refute an assertion, or is interpreted from collected data. Dana said, “Data is just what it is, it’s the calculations. And evidence is when you use data to try to persuade someone to see a certain point of view, or to argue a point. Data can be used as evidence to advocate for a certain drug or something that reduces the heart rate, or whatnot.” Three 400 level students were confused about whether data could be quantitative or qualitative, and were inclined to see data as numbers. One SSI student from each level said that evidence was more concrete or more “backed up,” showing confusion about the nature of evidence. BIO group. The BIO students had similar responses, where three from each level understood that evidence supports a conclusion (although one defined data and evidence as the same). One 400 level student said data is numbers, and one student from each level thought evidence was more tangible, like evidence from a crime scene. Definition of Theory SSI group. All SSI students recognized theory as supported with evidence, but most had misconceptions about the nature of scientific theory. All of the 400 level SSI 105 students and 2/3 of 200 level students who discussed theory viewed theory as less evidence-backed than law or fact. Ben said, “I guess it’s like an opinion on why something happens that hasn’t been proven yet. I would probably say it’s based on the evidence that you collected at the best, like an explanation for something at the point, but you can’t claim it’s 100% true…or accurate.” Shawna had a more informed understanding of theory, saying “I guess it’s just basically a statement that you can make once you have substantial evidence and research that backs up the claim. Generally I guess accepted by the scientific community.” BIO group. The BIO students also recognized that theories are supported with evidence (all students), but varied greatly in their more specific definitions. Of the 400 level BIO students, one said a theory is the same as a law, a supported hypothesis that has not yet disproven, one said it is a hypothesis no one has tried to dispute yet, and one said it is a currently accepted idea. For the 200 level students, one said theory is the best explanation but less substantiated than law, one said theory is a supported idea that scientists try to disprove, one student described theories as “ideas of how things happen,” and guidelines leading to new ideas, and one said theories are ideas that become permanent with sufficient evidence. Overall, interviewed students saw theories as evidence backed, although more SSI students referred to a hierarchy in which theory was less backed than laws or facts. Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge Biology Concept Inventory BCI scores from a limited sample of 26 Human Biology majors and 26 biology majors showed little difference between the two groups (see Table 26.) At each level and 106 in total, Human Biology and biology means varied by less than one question on the concept inventory (SSI total M= 13.66, SD=3.29 and BIO M= 14.0, SD=3.25). Table 26 BCI scores for SSI and BIO groups Mean score (out of 30 points) Standard Deviation *p<.05 200 level Bio SSI (n=15) (n=13) 13.20 12.23 400 level Bio SSI (n=11) (n=13) 14.77 14.64 Bio (n=26) 13.81 Total SSI (n=26) 13.50 3.00 3.19 3.25 3.29 2.98 3.53 Sig. .736 Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge Portion of Interview All SSI students interviewed said they had a good understanding of the general concepts of biology except one senior level student. However, two students mentioned that their overall biology content knowledge was only average. Gary explained that although his “big picture” knowledge was strong, he felt that more detailed knowledge valued more highly, especially in standardized tests like the medical exam, the MCAT. All SSI students also discussed how they were stronger in their areas of interest in biology. These areas included anatomy and physiology, cellular mechanisms, and epidemiology, subjects relevant to their future careers in medicine or health fields. SSI Group Three SSI students gauged their content knowledge according to their preparation for the MCAT. Two students were confident relying on their premedical classes outside of Human Biology to establish this content knowledge, while one senior student expressed concern that too much time had passed since he took his premedical courses to feel confident on the exam. 107 Three students responded that content knowledge was lacking in Human Biology and they would like to see more detailed concepts incorporated into core courses. Gary noted, “Of course, now I’m a senior, so I’m getting ready to take the MCATS and the aptitude tests and part of that section is biology and I haven’t started to study and I am somewhat apprehensive about the fact that I’m going to have to put more work into it than I probably should as a Human Biology major necessarily.” Still, two of these students recognized that with different student interests in Human Biology from medicine to biology research to social science focuses, it would be difficult to accommodate all students with an in-depth focus on biology concepts. Kelly said, “I guess it’s because I’m really interested in the biological processes as well. So I would like to see more of a balance of that, but, I don’t know how you avoid boring the people who already know all of it, it’s hard because we’re from such different backgrounds.” Shawna noted that Human Biology core courses could spur interest in areas of biology and offer opportunities to independently gain content knowledge, although this work was not required. She noted, “I guess a lot of times certain resources are provided as kind of a gateway to look into that aspect of it more, but the way in which we’re tested in class, it doesn’t require that you actually have a thorough understanding of the science.” Ben noted that he gained most of his content knowledge through independent study, “like reading the chapters and the assigned work,” in both biology and Human Biology courses. He said, " Like with the Human Biology, I learned more because I feel obligated to my group to make sure I know all of it,” stressing that resources for learning biology content were provided and incentives to learn were emphasized through the team learning structure. 108 Two of the 400 level SSI students commented that taking fewer upper level biology courses was appropriate, since detailed content knowledge was not the sole focus of their major. Dana said, “I think, like compared to the regular biology degree, I have a different understanding of biology. I mean, I’ve taken like the actual biology courses like [lists biology courses], but I feel like with Human Biology, you have to look at a broader scale, so it’s more like an epidemiological perspective, the thing is, so I probably haven’t focused as much on the cell, things like that.” They recognized that participation in the program allowed them to choose how many upper level biology courses they would take. BIO Group BIO students generally gave shorter responses when asked about their perceptions of their biology content knowledge. All students reported feeling comfortable with the foundational concepts in biology. All except for two students reported feeling less comfortable with more detailed concepts. Half of BIO students at each level mentioned that they understood the biology curriculum as providing the “big picture” in early classes and providing “details” in upper-level courses. Ellen explained, “I think more so how they do it here is that they give you an overview and then through, like molecular biology, genetics, what have you, microbiology, um, you can see the pieces that make the whole.” Two senior students noted that they felt more comfortable in areas where they had taken more classes. Kevin felt strong in genetics and evolution because these concepts were repeated throughout the curriculum and Carrie felt strong in evolution because she had elected to take additional courses on evolution. Kevin felt that his strength in biology content related directly to his experience in a research lab. He said, “I worked in a 109 molecular biology lab for the year before I took half these classes where I was actually running the tests and running the labs, which I really believe the labs are the best for me… Applying the process is what actually helped me.” For Kevin, lab experienced reinforced his development of content knowledge. Student Perceptions of Majors Both SSI and BIO participants were interviewed to understand their perceptions of four three general areas of their majors: personal outcomes, perceptions SSI in their majors, and perceptions of the learning environment in their courses. Perceptions of Personal Outcomes SSI Group All SSI participants said they felt more able to consider multiple perspectives as a result of participating in the program. Shawna explained, “I think it’s been good to see both sides of things. I guess I’ve kind of gotten a little less opinionated in the sense that I’m a little bit more open to other reasoning on that I never saw as logical before but I guess I understand them better now.” Kelly elaborated on the pedagogical approach that helped her develop this competency. …AIDS, tuberculosis, everything, you looked at it all from like, from the very miniscule bacteria to the entire city. Every level. So, I mean, I think that’s one of the essential principals of Human Biology, is that you look. You look at different viewpoints, like when you have a problem, not a problem but anything that you have to bring into perspectives to really get a feel or understanding of what’s going on. All SSI students except one 200 level student reported feeling more able to discuss controversial issues or take a position on such issues. Kelly said, I’d say the overall goal is just making us question information we’ve been given and evaluate our own things that are important to us, and to try to really get out what you believe in. It’s helped me grow, having confidence 110 in doubting something. Like I guess before I kind of never even thought to doubt stuff, like one professor told me is true, like what I learn in school is true, and I’ve never been encouraged to doubt something. Sarah said, “I just think, I have a broader outlook on things now when they’re presented to me, instead of just like, oh this is what happens like this, when I can be like, oh well, this [is an] issue that surrounds it and you have to think about what if this happened, and contrast it.” Through discussion of SSI issues in core courses, students learned to question information presented, and to explore different aspects of the issue. Two 400 level SSI participants noted that they improved in making evidencebased arguments or were more willing to research issues. Gabrielle said, “I think I’m more open and more, I’m willing to do more research to find, for me to pick a side, to support my beliefs a little bit more, whereas before, I don’t agree with this, but I wouldn’t really know why I wouldn’t agree, or take the time to see the opposing side, but no, my way is the right way, kind of thing.” Dana said, “I’m able to formulate good arguments and research other primary literature, like we’ve had to do that so much, that’s definitely a big thing that I’m glad I’ve learned how to do for my future. BIO Group Three out of four BIO participants at each level considered discovering passions in biology or biology-related professions as important outcomes of their majors. Ella appreciated being exposed to “new and upcoming ideas.” Ellen said she gained a better understanding of science and its “utility for the world.” Polly said [the biology major] “has made me love science even more. Learning about it made me want to pursue my goals, I guess.” 111 More than half of BIO participants (3/4 400 level and 2/4 200 level) said they developed diligence or responsibility in their major. Kevin said, “It’s honestly, the main thing it’s taught me to do is how to establish goals and meet them myself.” Polly said, “I started out with not a very good idea of how to study for science classes, especially in college it’s very different from in high school, so it took me a while to get used to it, but once I did, I started liking the courses more and more. I tend to like things more if I know them very well. So I’ve just developed in how best I learn.” Two out of four of the 400 level BIO participants said they learned about the challenges of scientific research or graduate school. Natalie said, “I think I’m more realistic now about how hard it will be to do what I want to do… I mean, I’m still optimistic, I’m not saying that someone who wants to do something shouldn’t, I’m just more realistic about what I need to do and how it has to be done.” Ellen said, “This major has helped me to see that things take time, and um, it has allowed me to see that you’re not always going to have a direct result, sometimes, I don’t like this answer, but more often you’re gonna fail than you succeed, and that’s in experiments as well, so and the importance of diligence, so that’s what this has helped me to see.” She noted that her experience in a research lab as well as in her classes helped her see that scientists must learn to accept failure and frustration as part of their daily experience. After graduation, she intended to gain more experience in a research lab to prepare her for the psychological rigors of medical school. She said, “You need to be able to take it mentally—deal with the stress.” 112 Perceptions of the Learning Environment SSI Group All SSI students interviewed valued the team-teaching approach. They appreciated having experts from different disciplines model the integration of perspectives to approach an issue. They also valued the accessibility of knowledgeable teachers to help them apply complicated content knowledge. Dana said, “It’s interesting how they have usually two teachers teaching together and like how, they had a neuroscientist with a sociologist teaching together to just give you a lot of different perspectives on disease… It’s very integrated and they try to give you as much different backgrounds as possible.” Laura felt that integration of perspectives varied among her core courses. She felt some professors were too attached to one disciplinary perspective when dealing with issues, and this was an ineffective model for problem solving with socioscientific issues. The majority of SSI students (3/4 at both levels) said that they valued collaborative work in their major. Laura said, “So they [core classes] are very team based. And the rationale behind it has to do with the fact that when you get out into the real world, 9 times out of 10, most jobs you’re going to be working in a team... In some ways it’s very effective and in other ways it’s really annoying. But I guess that’s more how real life is anyway” Sarah said, “Now I know how to depend on other people, and I know how to provide to the team as well, or provide to just anybody who’s depending on me.” Although they valued teamwork in the major, many of the SSI students interviewed said they found the team work challenging (3/4 200 level and 2/4 400 level). They cited 113 different expectations and levels of engagement among team members as sources of frustration. BIO Group Although they noted that biology classes tended to be lecture-driven, all BIO participants said that they valued opportunities for independent research or participation in a lab. They said those experiences helped them to see how science is really done. Chloe noted that participation in a research lab helped her see how as a scientists, you use “your own underlying process,” as opposed to a strict classroom definition of the scientific method, where “they teach you rigid steps.” Ellen felt that her experience in a research lab opened her eyes to new opportunities for discovery in biology she wouldn’t have known from classes alone. She said, “Others may feel there’s one track to go through biology—they’ve only seen one side of biology.” Polly also felt her experience in her major was dramatically enhanced by participation in a research lab. When asked how her experience would have been different if she had “just had the courses,” Polly said, “Oh, I think it would be really different. Yeah, I wouldn’t know the material as well probably. I wouldn’t enjoy it as much because I wouldn’t understand really as well as I do, yeah, so that would be unfortunate I’d say.” Other students felt that experiences in both research labs and course laboratories reinforced their conceptual learning in their biology classes. Kevin said, “I know certain people who do learn better from the books, but I believe more so people learn better from the labs… You have to understand it if you do an experiment rather than just regurgitate information you’ve heard about it.” Kevin also felt that the few inquiry-based course labs he took helped him understand how scientists pose questions. He said, “…it actually 114 helped me to understand how to ask a good question, because there are so many questions you have to mull over and it seems easy when you read about somebody else’s, but it’s not nearly as easy as everybody makes it sound.” Some BIO students also noted that research lab experience and course labs helped them understand the breadth and big-picture goals of science. Natalie said, “I think that the labs have helped me to have a glimpse of what kind of fields of biology are out there. Like [histology class], I love it because I want to go into that field, pathology, or histology.” Tracy noted, “It’s interesting [working in a lab] because you see what all this work goes into. You see why you have to have the basic understanding.” As well as laboratory experience, all BIO students said they valued opportunities for discussion or debate. BIO participants described discussion sessions associated with courses as opportunities to review concepts and work through problem sets in a nonthreatening environment with a graduate or advanced undergraduate student. Natalie said, [I learn best in a] “discussion type thing, when the professors, they’re not down some stairs all the way at the bottom of the lecture hall.” Carrie said, “I think [discussion sessions are] definitely helpful, because you can see stuff hands on that’s just not being like being lectured to you in class, like you can do that kind of stuff. I mean, discussion helps a less threatening environment.” Polly noted, “I liked having to discuss something, once again if you’re made to discuss something, it kind of furthers your understanding of it.” Ellen mentioned that she also valued opportunities to discuss controversial issues in her biology and social science courses. She said, “I welcome debates, but I think, I kinda like when your perspectives are challenged because it allows you to prove what you know, why you know it, and why it should matter.” 115 When discussing the level of community in their majors, all except one 400 level BIO students said community isn’t facilitated, but develops, especially with upperclassmen. They explained that the large size of the major makes developing a sense of community difficult. Polly said, “I wouldn’t say it’s like collaborating is discouraged, but it’s just not facilitated too much.” Kevin noted that the common practice of grading on a curve in biology classes seemed to discourage some students from working with other students. However, he said that many students worked together in spite of this. He said, “Students studied together to help through difficult classes. It [grading on a curve] should have discouraged us but in the end, being my friends in biology, it really incurred us to work hard core together, get together, make schedules, because it would make us do the work.” BIO participants also noted that a stronger sense of community developed in upper-level students as class size decreased. Two discussed a particular social event for senior biology majors that helped them feel more connected. Although not all participants were specifically asked about interaction with professors, the majority of them said that it was available when students sought it (2/4 at 400 level and all 200 level). When asked, “Do you have an opportunity to get to know the professors?” Carrie said, “I would say that you have to make an effort to do that, it’s not easy to do that. I especially think it’s hard when you’re doing well in the class, you don’t really have any mood to go into office hours, so, you know.” Explaining that going to office hours was the primary way to get to know their professors, BIO students said that generally professors clearly wanted to help students. 116 CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION Review of Study In summary, my dissertation looks for similarities and differences between students in an SSI-based Human Biology major and students in a more traditional biology major. I compare the two groups in terms of socioscientific reasoning, understanding of scientific inquiry, and levels and perceptions of content knowledge. In addition I identify themes in students’ general perceptions of their majors. The research questions divided by topical sections of the study are as follows: Socioscientific Issues (1) Do Human Biology majors reason with SSI differently from traditional biology majors? (2) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their experiences with SSI differ? Understanding of Scientific Inquiry (3) Do Human Biology and biology majors understand scientific inquiry differently? Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge (4) Do Human Biology and biology majors differ in their general biology content knowledge? (5) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their content knowledge differ? General Perceptions of Majors (6) How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ general perceptions of their majors differ? 117 I use a mixed methods convergence model of triangulation design (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007) comparing and contrasting various data sources including both qualitative and quantitative data to develop interpretations (see Figure 1). Figure 1. Convergence model of triangulation design for dissertation Note. Adapted from Creswell and Plano-Clark (2007). In this chapter, I will discuss results from each section of the dissertation, including socioscientific reasoning, understanding of inquiry, levels and perceptions of content knowledge, and general major perceptions. I will then discuss limitations of the study, major findings and their implications, and future directions for research. Socioscientific Issues Socioscientific Reasoning The results of this study suggest that an SSI-focused interdisciplinary major in Human Biology provides some benefits in reasoning and consideration of multiple perspectives in complex problems over a traditional biology major. Although decisions 118 and the categories of factors considered in decision-making were similar for SSI and BIO group, the SSI group showed higher levels of socioscientific reasoning. Consistent with Bell’s and Lederman’s (2003) study with professors having different NOS views, few differences were found in frequencies of decisions between the two groups. It is unsurprising that positions differ little between similar groups of scientifically literate pre-professionals. The only significant difference in decisions was based on whether students exercised regularly. Although this question tested whether students based their behavior on scientific knowledge, it did not ask them to take a position on a controversial issue. SSI students were less likely to exercise regularly and they commonly reported time constraints as the reason for this behavior. Though few, some differences in reasons for decisions were found. BIO participants were more likely to support legally binding limits on carbon emissions (more pronounced at 200 level). One reason for this may relate to the fact that SSI participants were more likely to suggest alternatives to legislation, like incentives (62% vs. 48%) in this scenario. Perhaps having extensive experience with argumentation helped SSI students to think creatively about alternatives to the suggested response. SSI participants were also less likely to include environmental factors in their decisions (56% vs. 70% BIO), so perhaps BIO students were more attuned to environmental concerns, whereas SSI participants were more likely to focus on social concerns, or to more seriously consider both socioeconomic and environmental aspects of the problem. Overall, for the global warming cluster, SSI students included more reasons as influencing their decision, so perhaps SSI students viewed the problem as more complex then the BIO groups. Participants often noted that people would be affected differently by legislation, so 119 different approaches would be needed to assure fairness. In this cluster, SSI participants were also more likely to refer to public perception of U.S. (24% vs. 10%). This difference may relate to their focus on the social aspect of problems and consideration of different perspectives. Many participants discussed how improving global perceptions of the U.S. could facilitate cooperation in solving problems like global warming. Finally, SSI participants were more likely to refer to evidence (44% vs. 34%) in their reasons for making decisions in this cluster. This difference could relate to the explicit focus on evidence-based arguments in their core courses. 400 level SSI participants were also more likely to answer “no,” (80% SSI vs. 50% BIO) when asked whether they thought cigarette smoking should be made illegal. Like in the global warming cluster, this difference could be explained considering that 400 level SSI students were much more likely to cite availability of other options, like banning smoking in public places, and the importance of personal responsibility as reasons why smoking should not be banned (67% SSI vs. 35% BIO). Finally, BIO majors were more likely to report that they make food and exercise choices according to personal preferences or tastes (35% vs. 24%). This appears consistent with SSI students’ lower participation in regular exercise. The reasons for this difference are unclear. Although it seems SSI participants were aware of the benefits of exercise and healthy diets, they were less likely to apply this knowledge to their own lives. Although most decisions and reasons behind decisions were similar, we expected that reasoning processes would be more developed in SSI students. Theoretically, work in socioscientific issues, which SSI students reported as having consistently and BIO 120 students as having infrequently, allowed students to develop reasoning processes that could be applied to other issues. My analysis found that SSI students at both levels had reasoning scores .25-.3 points higher on a 5-point scale. Both groups showed higher reasoning scores for 400 level than 200 level, although the difference between classes for the BIO group was slightly higher (.23 for BIO vs. .18 for SSI). Higher scores at the 400 level would be expected regardless of instruction, since higher levels of reflective judgment, which relates to reasoning in SSI, would be expected with development and experience in a college environment (King & Kitchener, 1994). Still, I expected a greater difference between levels for SSI students as they have consistently been exposed to socioscientific issues. Studies that compared SSI and nonSSI groups in pre-post improvement for more short-term interventions found greater improvement in reasoning for SSI students (Tal & Hochberg, 2003; Zohar & Nemet, 2002; Dori et al., 2003). Also, Zeidler et al. (2009) found that students exposed to an SSI curriculum for one school year improved in reflective judgment, while the non-SSI group did not improve. Differences between 200 level and 400 level students cannot be considered “improvement” as in pre-post studies, but with groups of students that were originally very similar, these differences could be interpreted to show how extended exposure to SSI (between the second and fourth year) affected gains over time in reasoning with SSI. My findings may be partly explained by the small sample size of the 400 level classes and the newness of the Human Biology major. The 400 level SSI class experienced the first implementation of the SSI curriculum in each year of their major. When the 200 level group matriculated, program goals were more established, faculty were more experienced, and the curriculum had been adapted. Also considering the 121 higher attrition rate in the BIO program, it is likely that for both majors the 200 level groups were comparatively different from the 400 level groups. Although pre-test data were not available, the consistent higher scores of the SSI groups suggest that participation in the Human Biology program related to more sophisticated socioscientific reasoning. SSI students were more likely to use multiple justifications to support their positions and to better explain those justifications. This likely relates to the focus on socioscientific reasoning in core courses. Students were routinely challenged to make evidence-based arguments, using as much relevant and credible data as possible. Their arguments were assessed in position papers and critiqued by their peers and professors in debates. It should be noted that average scores for both groups were fairly high (SSI: 3.46 and BIO: 3.19 on a 5-point scale). On average, students from both groups were likely to include and elaborate on at least one justification of their positions. Few studies of socioscientific reasoning have been conducted with college students, who should exhibit much higher levels of intellectual development and reflective judgment than middle school or high school students. Perhaps higher developmental levels, especially considering the high achievement levels of my sample, may relate to the fairly small difference in socioscientific reasoning. Also, perhaps a greater difference between groups could have been found with a more sensitive instrument and coding scheme. Consideration of Multiple Perspectives For perspectives scores, no significant difference was found between groups according to a Mann-Whitney test for non-parametric data. Still, it is worth noting that although the number of DMQ items in which participant responses included multiple 122 perspectives was nearly equal between groups (BIO mean: 3.8; SSI: 3.9), of these responses, SSI students scored a higher percentage of scores of 3 (55% vs. 47%), and lower percentage of scores of 1 (34% vs. 43%). Although on average, BIO and SSI students mentioned other perspectives an almost equal number of times in the questionnaire, SSI students were more likely to consider other perspectives in depth and reach a logical conclusion. Often in responses given a perspectives score of one, an alternative perspective was referenced, but given no context. In responses scored 3 (more frequent for SSI), alternate perspectives were considered in depth and the participant was able to reach a resolution incorporating different perspectives. This result may be related to the focus in Human Biology on fully considering different perspectives before committing to a position. Incorporation of other perspectives was considered an important part of a good argument, and students were assessed on how well this was done. In the sophomore level core course, students were explicitly told, “The most effective arguments will take into account the points made by the opposition.” Consistent with this result, SSI students also consistently noted in interviews that they were more likely to consider other perspectives than before entering the Human Biology major. They reflected on the importance of being aware of other ideas and hearing from all sides. Few qualitative differences were seen considering the general principles of reflective judgment between SSI and BIO interviewees in their responses to DMQ follow-up questions. This may be explained by the fact that all students in this study were high achieving pre-professionals who had reached high levels of development. However, when probing reasoning processes on scenarios, SSI students tended to provide examples 123 from their core courses when they saw relevance. For example, two of the SSI students interviewed compared a hypothetical argument that there is no clear causal mechanism between smoking and cancer to the scientific debate over whether HIV causes AIDS discussed in their core course. SSI students also mentioned many examples of cases from their core courses when asked about their experiences with SSI or aspects of their major that they valued. Perhaps extended experience with SSI, as provided in the Human Biology major, provides a repertoire of cases students can access in relevant situations (Bransford et al., 1986). Understanding of Scientific Inquiry Views of Different Disciplines and Perspectives in Science Consistent with in-class and interview assertions of Human Biology professors, responses on the modified VOSI suggest that SSI students are more likely than BIO students to view social science research as scientific inquiry. They more commonly recognized that methods in social science, such as interviewing or using questionnaires may not fit the scientific method, but are still scientific. In Human Biology core courses, students read and discussed social science research articles, and professors stressed that researchers from both social science and biological perspectives were scientists. In interviews, some students mentioned inquiry projects they conducted in core courses, which studied human behavior. These experiences as well as the discourse in Human Biology classes likely contributed to this difference in understanding of inquiry. Although they were more likely to mention social science research as part of science, BIO students were somewhat more likely to indicate that scientists working independently or collaboratively might reach different conclusions due to different 124 interpretations based on different backgrounds or perspectives of scientists. This is surprising in light of interviews where SSI students were likely to discuss consideration of multiple perspectives when thinking about scientific problems as a major program outcome. Perhaps SSI students were more inclined to view “different perspectives” as different disciplinary perspectives, rather than different ideas informing scientists working in the same discipline on the same research. Although few students from each group (8% BIO and 2% SSI) made the connection, BIO students were also slightly more likely to cite new interpretations of existing data as a catalyst for theory change. This is surprising considering that students discussed different interpretations of data in core courses, such as the “Duesberg Phenomenon” with AIDS, and commonly referred to this example in interviews. SSI students were more likely to report that theories are important to promote discussion of the scientific community. Perhaps SSI students understood that different interpretations led to debate and discussion, but did not connect them to theory change. It is important to note that the majority of both groups did not include subjectivity in their reasoning for different conclusions between different scientists. SSI students were more likely to cite error as a reason for differences in independent researchers’ conclusions (47% vs. 30%), and note that scientists do not use identical procedures (18% vs. 4%). Both groups most commonly referred to different data resulting from uncontrollable factors as contributing to differences in conclusions. An understanding that people come from different perspectives, does not necessarily mean students will recognize how different perspectives influence conclusions from specific data. 125 Views of the Scientific Method and Experiment SSI majors were more likely than BIO majors to use the words, “scientific method” when explaining how scientists investigate (36% vs. 20%). This is unsurprising, considering that the term, “scientific method” was included in the discourse of core courses. However, in core course discourse, the definition of scientific method was broadly conceived to incorporate different aspects of scientific research, not presented as rigid steps. The sociologist professor of the 200 level course said they were “with the first year courses, pretty focused on a very traditional scientific method of approach and that we’ve not abandoned the thinking, but they learn about other kinds of data that are used to test hypothesis, generate hypothesis, and so on, in the second year of course.” Later in the program, the scientific method was broadened to include other forms of science. The sociologist professor said, “And then the contexts of, say reading about cholera, they learn that Snow did things that weren’t experimental, and that he did observational work, and followed sort of a scientific method in his approach to thinking thru the problem but it wasn’t bench science, it isn’t what I think students necessarily come into college thinking that science is.” The biologist professor of the 200 level course agreed, In second year we really introduce the fact that you can’t apply those methods strictly to everything. That there are other methods that you utilize to make predictions and assess data…I think one of the things that we try to do is emphasize that science is science. I mean, the word science doesn’t apply to only bench scientists. People who do social science are scientists. The scientific method can apply. You just have to use different techniques, and you have to analyze the data differently, but it’s still science. And it’s still valid. Later in the interview, the 200 level professors continue their discussion of how they present science and the scientific method: 126 Sociologist: There’s always as least one moment in each semester where someone says something to me, well you know, Dr.[neuroscientist] is a scientist, and you’re the sociologist, and I would say no, we’re both scientists, we just do different kinds of science. Neuroscientist – And I think that, I don’t think that we necessarily explicitly set out to, on a mission, to fix that misconception but it is something that, it’s a recurring issue and it’s something that we usually spend the entire semester addressing over and over because when the kids leave our classroom then they end up in the world where people make this distinction again, and so we’re continually battling that and trying to help them understand that science is this interdisciplinary concept and there are a number of different ways that you address these scientific questions. Or questions in general. I mean science is just a way to address the questions. When asked, “Is there one scientific method or set of steps that all investigations must follow to be considered science?” SSI and BIO groups were similar in their responses (69% of SSI students and 72% of BIO students responded “yes”). However, students who responded “yes” differed between groups, where SSI students were more likely to include asking a question (49% vs. 22%) and making observations (27% vs. 8%). This is consistent with the fact that SSI students were more likely to report that science begins with a question. The scientific method has promoted a common misconception that inquiry begins with a hypothesis (Schwartz, Lederman, & Lederman, 2008). SSI students appear to grasp the idea that an inclusive scientific method incorporates and begins with a problem or a question. Also, incorporation of observations by SSI students suggests a broad conception of a scientific method, where knowledge develops through other means than manipulating variables. BIO students were more likely to include hypothesizing (68% vs. 23%) and analysis (40% vs. 29%) in their responses, both traditional parts of the scientific method. In their discussions of what scientists do, BIO participants were more likely to discuss a purely experimental view of science (38% vs. 22%) and discuss specific process 127 skills of science, such as hypothesizing and collecting and analyzing data (80% vs. 69%). Schwartz, Lederman, & Lederman (2008) assert that viewing inquiry as controlled experiments is a common misconception among students. In their definitions of “experiment,” BIO participants were more likely than SSI participants to include testing of a hypothesis (74% vs. 60%) and ensuring validity or precision of experiments (18% vs. 7%). These differences may relate to the discourse in Human Biology core courses, where science and the scientific method are inclusive of multiple forms of inquiry, but maintain key processes in science, including “develop scientific questions, design and conduct investigations, analyze and interpret data” (from a program revision document, July 2009). When asked to define “experiment,” most participants from both groups did not explicitly distinguish an experiment from inquiry in general. Few students from both groups (29% SSI and 24% BIO) mentioned that variables are isolated or controlled in an experiment. These questionnaire results were consistent with student interviews. When asked to give an example of an experiment, BIO participants were more likely to discuss a study conducted in a research lab or by a research team (28% vs. 11%), although the majority of students from both groups discussed personal experiences from class activities. They were also slightly more likely to give an explanation why the example they described was consistent with their definition of experiment (82% vs.76%). Perhaps more exposure to experimental science (consistent with the finding that BIO students were more likely to participate in research labs), not only affected BIO participants to see inquiry as more experimental, but to develop coherence between examples of what scientists do and their definitions of what scientists do. 128 Data and Evidence Both groups saw data as different from evidence (89% SSI and 86% BIO), and about half of each group said that evidence was different in that it is used to support an argument (49% SSI and 54% BIO). More SSI students mentioned a similar idea, that evidence is analyzed or interpreted data (9% vs. 2%). A greater percentage of interviewed students had similar conceptions, but responses were also similar between groups. These findings suggest that a majority of both groups grasp the primary difference between data and evidence. On the questionnaire, BIO students were somewhat more likely to respond that evidence is less precise or exact than data (12% vs. 4%), suggesting a common misconception that data and evidence are hierarchical in their truth value. SSI students more commonly reported two vague descriptions of data and evidence. They much more commonly referred to data as “results” (22% vs. 8%), which does not differentiate between raw data and analyzed data. They were also more likely to report that data can be evidence, but not explain how they are different (11% vs. 2%). Professors from the 200 level course agreed that their students struggled with presenting evidence-based arguments, and felt more scaffolding was needed. Neuroscientist: That’s been a real challenge for us is to help them understand what evidence is. That’s something that we really struggled with and work on this task this semester. Sociologist: Yea. I think it will stick with some people and not with others. We know we won’t get 100% but there might be people who need us to give them explicit instruction about how you do that. When asked, “What is ‘data analysis’?” and “What is involved in doing data analysis?” both groups accurately responded interpretation or making meaning of data (89% SSI and 96% BIO). However, SSI participants were much more likely to include visualizing data through graphs or charts (29% vs. 14%) and checking validity or 129 accuracy of data collection (22% vs. 6%). They also included using statistics more (40% vs. 30%) and compiling and reviewing data (16% vs. 6%). It is possible that SSI students described more specific processes because of opportunities to carry out full inquiry projects in their core courses. Most SSI students supported this in interviews, saying that both Human Biology courses and an elective course in physiology helped them establish an understanding of inquiry through experience. BIO participants may have differed more in their experiences with inquiry. More BIO participants participated in research labs, but interview participants discussed having had more “cookbook” laboratory experiences than projects where they developed questions, collected and analyzed data, and drew their own conclusions. Interviewees reported that an introductory level lab for BIO majors included a culminating project where students independently conducted the full range of inquiry with questions they had generated. Still, it is possible that SSI students were more familiar with processes conducted at all stages of inquiry. It is also interesting that 400 level students from both groups were much more likely to include statistical tests (SSI increasing 50% and BIO increasing 42%) and finding patterns in data (SSI increasing 17% and BIO increasing 20%). This finding suggests that students become more familiar with specific methods in data analysis with more experience. Tentativeness of Theory and Purpose of Theory The majority of both groups reported that theories are tentative (98% SSI and 96% BIO). Both groups commonly reported that discovery or availability of new data contributed to theory change (49% SSI and 96% BIO), but BIO majors were more likely to discuss new technology as a catalyst for change (16% vs. 7%). Perhaps BIO majors 130 were more attuned to advances in molecular and gene technology, which have recently revolutionized research in biology. BIO students may have spent more time in class or other settings with different biology professors, who may have been inclined to discuss changes in research that result from new technology. BIO majors were also more likely to discuss a falsification view of changing theories, explaining that theories are put forth by scientists to be retested until they are either disproven or withstand adequate attempts at falsification (26% vs. 8%). Chalmers (1999) explains that this position is problematic in that theories are difficult to falsify once and for all. Falsifying evidence may be found to be inaccurate or misinterpreted. In addition, SSI students were more likely to report that theories are needed for lively discussion within scientific communities (16% vs. 8%). Perhaps explicit discussion of conflicting arguments and editorials in core courses such as the “Duesberg” debate allowed them to recognize a lively discourse that changed thinking about scientific controversies. Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge Both groups scored within one question of each other on the Biology Concept Inventory, a finding that was consistent at both 200 and 400 levels. This suggests that incorporating a focus on socioscientific issues did not detract from development of basic content knowledge. Although differences were not seen in performance, in interviews, SSI majors mostly felt confident in their basic content knowledge, but were less confident in detailed content knowledge. Some mentioned that by participating in core courses and courses from the social sciences and humanities, they were unable to take as many upper level biology classes as they would if they had majored in biology. Some felt this was 131 reasonable, but others felt the major should have incorporated more biology content. BIO majors interviewed were similarly confident in their basic biology knowledge, but felt their knowledge of specific biology content depended on their interests and upper level courses they had chosen. It is unclear how Human Biology core courses contributed to students’ biology content knowledge. The concepts included in the BCI were covered in introductory level biology classes that both SSI and BIO majors were likely to have taken. Both groups scored fairly low (a finding consistent with Klymkowsky, Furtak, Cooper, Garvan-Doxas, & Gonzalas, submitted), indicating the persistence of many common misconceptions. This was apparent in students’ answers to BCI validation questions in interviews. Biology content addressed in Human Biology core courses depended on the topics covered, and often covered specific topics in human anatomy and physiology, so major misconceptions in evolutionary biology and genetics may not have been addressed. Some SSI students recognized opportunities for deeper research into science concepts in core courses, although not all students took advantage of these opportunities. Core courses provided resources for deeper content learning and opportunities to research and reflect upon biology concepts of interest. Core course professors indicated that they considered application of biology concepts a crucial part of the course. The neuroscientist professor said, We shoot for a balance because we really do want to provide some content and although content isn’t the goal of the Human Biology program or any of the core courses, per say, we’re not trying to provide their content. What we’re really looking to do is to integrate, or enhance their ability to integrate what they are learning across their courses. But to do that and to help the students understand that science is sort of a content based process as well. We really use those exams to try to balance those things. 132 The professor gave the example of viruses as content knowledge that was presented in class and incorporated into exams because it was a crucial part of understanding problems related to disease. Although students may have learned new biology content effectively and applied understandings gained in biology courses, they may not have had additional opportunities, as compared to biology students, to improve the basic biology content knowledge tested in the BCI. Perceptions of Majors Perceptions of Personal Outcomes When asked what they valued most about their experience in their majors, SSI participants consistently cited having the opportunity to explore controversial interdisciplinary issues in depth. They felt they had improved their abilities to discuss and take positions on these issues, and they noted that this competency was essential for future careers in science or health care. They also felt that the major offered them opportunities to understand controversies they were not familiar with and situations they never before saw as problematic. When asked what the major student outcomes instructors worked toward, the neuroscientist instructor of the 200 level core course said, “the major outcomes that we’re looking for mirror the outcomes of the program as a whole, that we’re looking for the students to be able to think more critically, more flexibly, about the problems that we talk about in the course, and what makes our course specific is the context that we frame those problems within.” Clearly, SSI participants at both levels grasped this central program goal. BIO participants also valued many aspects of their major, although none of the students voluntarily brought up reasoning with socioscientific issues as an important 133 outcome. They felt their major helped them learn to be more diligent and responsible, which would serve them in future research and professional schools. Many discussed rigors of the biology major, which sometimes discouraged them, but often made them feel more prepared for future challenges. Most BIO students appreciated opportunities to learn about new ideas and discover new passions for science. Surprisingly, all students interviewed had taken classes that have explored socioscientific issues to some degree. This may be partially due to a convenience sample of highly motivated biology majors; all interview participants had experience working in a research lab, compared to 27% BIO students sampled. Still, these experiences showed that exploring socioscientific issues outside of Human Biology core courses is possible, and many students pursue this interest. Although participants did not feel that their biology courses offered opportunities to learn about or discuss socioscientific issues in depth, many felt these courses helped them understand biological mechanisms more thoroughly, which enhanced their understanding of socioscientific issues discussed in other classes. One 200 level BIO participant explained that she was so disappointed in the lack of opportunities to discuss SSI that she planned to change her major to anthropology while maintaining a pre-medical curriculum. Tracy said, You know when I started with a biology major, I was excited for what you just said, to be able to apply my biological understanding in topics that were relevant, that was on the news. And that’s the problem I fell into was that the classes that I was taking, understandably, were the basics of everything, that was my problem, it was like, when am I going to get to the things that I can truly apply in my every day world? That’s not to say, as I learn right now about the many steps of transcription and in translation, it’s beautiful and I like to learn about it, but it would be cool to watch the news and how they talk about global warming and the ice caps melting and exactly be able to say, you know, why is that, why is it that CO2 levels are contributing to the atmospheric thickening, where as I feel 134 like I almost get that information from the media, from watching An Inconvenient Truth. Kevin, while acknowledging the value of incorporating SSI into biology classes, felt that avoiding controversy in biology courses was preferable. Clearly, biology courses address a great deal of content, and some may argue that SSI should be explored through other venues, like independent projects or other courses. The Human Biology core courses may be considered a consistent venue for reasoning with SSI. The neuroscientist professor of the 200 level core course explained that the Human Biology core courses were designed specifically for this purpose, while biology courses should supply more indepth biology content. Addressing biology content, he said, “That’s what you’re going to get if you want to learn cell biology, that’s why IU has a great resources, people who know a lot about cell biology, you go over there and you take those courses, if it makes sense for your curriculum.” Still, professors in Human Biology recognized a resistance from students who felt their core courses should supply more content. The 200 level neuroscientist professor argued that content should be taught, but framed differently from a traditional biology class. He said, And you know, sort of the idea is that the content that we want them to walk away with is content that will allow them to apply their newly gained knowledge of bacteria to a variety of situations. Which I think is different from the way they approach it in the biology department, and having sat through some of those courses, I know that one of the things is you memorize this, this is this specific bacteria and this is that specific virus and you memorize what they look like. And we don’t so much care about what they look like, we care about how is this disease similar to that one, and why would you treat something with antibiotics and one infection with antibiotics and another one you wouldn’t. Why is that? Those are the kinds of questions that we want the students to be able to understand because they see things on the news about this infection that’s occurring and a lot of lay people say, oh take antibiotics. That doesn’t work! 135 This illuminates important questions for future research: “How should content and socioscientific reasoning be balanced in college biology courses?” and “Should SSI be provided in supplemental courses rather than integrating it into biology courses?” This also raises the question whether socioscientific reasoning should be a mandatory or optional part of undergraduate science training. Perceptions of the Learning Environment It is important to not that the learning environment played an important role in students’ perceptions of their majors and personal outcomes. Both SSI and BIO groups valued opportunities for discussion and application of concepts. BIO participants were more likely to cite class discussion sessions and laboratories, and participation in research labs as critical experiences for their learning. SSI participants more commonly cited case studies as important opportunities to apply concepts. Throughout the interviews, SSI participants cited many examples of case studies from core courses, and often related them to other issues. This may have implications for transfer of knowledge and skills to new problems. Another important aspect of the learning environment involved a sense of community. SSI participants valued a sense of family among students and professors. Part of this they attributed to a small program, and part they attributed to the team-based nature of core courses. They felt this aspect prepared them for collaborative work environments and helped them develop confidence in their inter-personal skills and public speaking skills. BIO participants did not feel that their major was structured to support community, primarily because of its size. Interaction with professors was not structured, but could be fruitful when students pursued that interaction. BIO participants 136 noted that the overall major and class size decreased in the junior and senior years, which allowed for a desired sense of community to develop more easily. This raises the question of how SSI learning environments need to be structured. Sadler (2009) explains that participation in communities of practice (Lave, 1991) is central to SSI. Students must come to understand the culture of the community, including its rules and practices, and they adopt and project particular identities within those communities. Can learning environments that purposefully nurture communities of practice in SSI-contexts be structured for large classes, or is a small program size important to this aspect? If effective communities of practice could be established through an SSI framework in large majors like biology, could this help students feel more supported and increase persistence in science majors? Limitations of the Study As a broad, exploratory study of a four-year program, this study had several limitations. The conclusions are limited by unavailability of questionnaire pre data for participants. Questionnaire differences between groups, though further explored and supported through interviews, cannot necessarily be contributed to their experiences in their majors. These differences may have been present initially in the samples. Secondly, the two majors differ in diversity of students, since Human Biology is more likely to attract students interested in the human divisions of the discipline. Collection of demographic data helped to reveal similarities and differences of students in the two majors. Possible curricula and focus areas are also more diverse for biology students, so they are more likely to diverge in their coursework and experiences related to biology. 137 A third limitation is the degree to which I may assume biology majors experience “traditional” biology teaching. Interviews and anecdotal experience from occasional visits to biology classes helped to ascertain that biology students primarily experience lectures and “cookbook” labs, but I could not verify this through consistent observation due to time constraints and the vast possibilities of classes to attend. BIO students attested to my primary assumption that they did not experience yearly interdisciplinary, SSI-based core courses. Still, most BIO students interviewed reported experiencing some negotiation of socioscientific issues, and some took elective courses that specifically addressed SSI. It is unclear how much these experiences contributed to BIO students’ reasoning with socioscientific issues. Another limitation is that students who participated in interviews were not representative of the total samples from each group. Interview participants were not compensated for their time, so the majority of students originally selected were unable or unwilling to participate. Clearly BIO participants included a disproportional number of students with research lab experience, so it is unclear whether other factors may have been disproportionately represented. Also, students willing to participate in interviews may have been more inclined to promote their majors or speak to program goals. In addition, I was more familiar to SSI students having been present in their core course, and to some 400 level BIO students, having taught them in histology lab sections. This may have influenced them to be more invested in my project. Interview participants were treated as individual cases, but repetition of ideas from participants from each group was considered convincing evidence for conclusions. 138 The instruments used also presented some limitations. For example, misunderstandings in the implementation of the BCI resulted in loss of a significant amount of data. Also, the DMQ questions resulted in fairly short responses that may not have sufficiently captured students’ reasoning. Accordingly, the rubrics applied measured very simple aspects of socioscientific reasoning, including number and explanation of justifications and discussion of multiple perspectives. Longer and more in-depth scenarios and questions may have produced data appropriate to a more sensitive rubric for socioscientific isues. Considering the research-based foundations of practices in Human Biology and consistency of observed teaching and classroom activities with these principles, larger differences favoring the SSI group were expected in socioscientific reasoning and views of scientific inquiry. Since the program was only in its fourth year, teaching strategies may have varied among core courses and from year to year, and aspects of program goals were likely implemented inconsistently. Finally, my presence in Human Biology classes as a researcher and position as an instructor in the histology labs may have limited my findings. Students in Human Biology became familiar with me through my frequent class observations and participation in Human Biology events. They knew I had worked with the program director and was supportive of the program. These students may have been more willing to participate in the study, since it could potentially lead to advancement and improvement of the program. Also, having interacted frequently with the students while in the classroom, they may have wanted to help me with my dissertations. Similarly, biology students from my 400 level histology course may have been more willing to 139 participate, having known me as an instructor. Students who knew me before the study may have invested more thought and effort in the questionnaires which may have affected the results. Major Findings and Implications Socioscientific Issues Major findings in response to research question 1, “Do Human Biology majors reason with SSI differently from traditional biology majors?” are that SSI students showed enhanced socioscientific reasoning according to a simple scale, and that they appear to be more likely to incorporate different perspectives into their decision making. Perhaps more in-depth instruments, similar to position papers and debates held in core courses would reveal greater differences. In response to research question 2, “How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their experiences with SSI differ?” interviews showed that SSI students “bought in” to the goal of recognizing different perspectives in decision making and increased in their perceptions of controversy or problematic situations. In addition, SSI students appeared to use case studies from core courses to reason with new similar problems. These findings together suggest that classroom experience with socioscientific reasoning through case studies, with a focus on integrating different perspectives, impacts students in their awareness of and approach to real-life problems as well as their ability to reason effectively with such problems. Understanding of Scientific Inquiry In response to research question 3, “Do Human Biology and biology majors understand scientific inquiry differently?” SSI and BIO majors do show some 140 differences. SSI students are somewhat more likely to include social science inquiry in their definition of scientific inquiry, presumably because they have been exposed to scientists and research from different disciplines. They were able to discuss specific processes of scientific inquiry, attributing their understanding to Human Biology core or elective classes. However, SSI students, like the BIO students, still appeared to be confused about the nature of evidence, although they were slightly more likely to refer to using evidence in their decision making on the DMQ global warming scenario. Human Biology professors expressed that their students had difficulty understanding the concept of backing up arguments with evidence, although they explicitly addressed it in class. Perhaps more scaffolding in evidence-based reasoning with explicit discussion of the meaning of evidence is needed. Also, this confusion raises the question of how students conceive of evidence to back up scientific claims versus evidence to back up decisions on socioscientific issues. These different applications nuance the meaning of evidence and may contribute to confusion. Levels and Perceptions of Biology Content Knowledge In response to research question 4, “Do Human Biology and biology majors differ in their general biology content knowledge?” findings from a very limited sample suggest that SSI and BIO majors do not differ in their basic biology content knowledge. In response to question 5, “How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ perceptions of their content knowledge differ?” Human Biology students may feel that they are receiving a less rigorous biology content learning experience as compared to biology majors. Professors have attempted to clarify that core courses are not centered in content, but socioscientific reasoning. This raises the question, “Where is the most 141 effective place in a college environment to teach SSI?” Are optional interdisciplinary courses like Human Biology core courses that attract a small population of students preferable to required science courses integrated with SSI? General Perceptions of Majors In response to Question 6, “How do Human Biology and traditional biology majors’ general perceptions of their majors differ?” interviews suggested that all students valued their majors and were satisfied with personal outcomes. SSI students consistently viewed integrating different perspectives and social aspects of scientific problems as significant program outcomes. SSI students also valued aspects of the learning environment designed to support ethical, cognitive, and epistemological development, including an inclusive, collaborative environment and supportive relationships with faculty. SSI students felt a greater sense of community as compared to BIO students. These findings support the idea that careful structuring of the learning environment provides essential components of learning through SSI. Future Directions This exploratory work leads to new research on outcomes of SSI learning environments for college science majors, for which little research has been published. One future direction is to investigate how SSI learning environments affect students’ thinking about science careers. For example, I would like to investigate whether SSI students are more likely to perceive complexity of problems faced by science professionals and if they recognize a need to incorporate different perspectives. For example, how might exposure to SSI influence a premedical student to think about addressing different aspects of patients that influence their health, such as availability of 142 healthy food, limitations on their ability to travel to clinics or pharmacies, or psychological states that may influence behavior? This study would incorporate background study and interviews with science professionals to better understand the situations in which interdisciplinary collaboration and understanding of different perspectives are needed. Pre and post tests including scenarios faced by science professionals as well as interviews would assess changes in students’ thinking about these issues. Another direction for future research is a more in-depth look at how students’ understanding of inquiry is facilitated in an SSI-based course. This would involve a case study of an SSI-based course to reveal opportunities afforded by the curriculum to discuss and experience scientific inquiry. I would further document how an SSI-based environment may help students develop understandings of inquiry that include fields outside the boundaries of what is traditionally considered “science.” I would also further consider how students come to understand evidence in scientific inquiry. For example, do they see similarities between using evidence to support their positions on issues and scientists supporting their conclusions with evidence from their investigations? Do students understand the relationship between data and evidence? Such a study would involve careful consideration of the curriculum and intended methods of teaching inquiry, continual debriefing with the teacher, and observation as well as pre and post instruments and interviews to gauge student thinking about inquiry. Conclusion Overall, this study contributes to our understanding of how an SSI-focused, fouryear program can help students reason and make informed decisions on real and complex 143 problems. With the assumption supported that traditional university biology teaching provides few opportunities for critical evaluation of interdisciplinary problems currently called for (AACU, 2007), it provides an example of effective SSI teaching, with modeling of reasoning from social science and biological disciplinary perspectives. The study provides a springboard for new studies investigating SSI in interdisciplinary college contexts. 144 REFERENCES Adams, L. T., Perfetto, G. A., Yearwood, A., Kasserman, J., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1985). Facilitating Access. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt University. Albanese, M.A. and Mitchell, S. (1993). Problem-based learning: a review of literature on its outcomes and implementation issues. Academic Medicine, 68, 52-81. Albe, V. (2008). When scientific knowledge, daily life experience, epistemological and social considerations intersect: Students’ argumentation in group discussion on a socio-scientific issue. Research in Science Education, 38, 67–90. American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1990). Science for all Americans. New York: Oxford University Press. Association of American Colleges and Universities. (2007). College Learning for the New Global Century. Washington, D.C.: AACU. Barber, M. (2001). A comparison of NEAB and Salters A-level Chemistry: Students views and achievements. York, UK: University of York. Barker, V., & Millar, R. (1996). Differences between Salters’ and traditional A-level chemistry students’ understanding of basic chemical ideas. York, UK: University of York. Barron, B., Schwartz, D., Vye, N., Moore, A., Petrosino, A., Zech, L., Bransford, J. & Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1998). Doing with understanding: Lessons from research on problem- and project-based learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 7, 271-312. Baxter Magolda, M. (1992) Knowing and reasoning in college: Gender-related patterns in students’ intellectual development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Baxter Magolda, M. (1999). Creating Contexts for Learning and Self-Authorship Constructive-Developmental Pedagogy. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University Press. Bell, R. & Lederman, N. 2003. Understandings of the nature of science and decision making on science and technology based issues. Science Education, 87, 352– 377. Bell, P., & Linn, M. C. (2000). Scientific arguments as learning artifacts: Designing for learning from the web with KIE. International Journal of Science Education, 22(8), 797-817. Blattner, W., Gallo, R.C., & Temin, H.N. (1988). HIV casuses AIDS. Science, 241, 515516. 145 Boix Mansilla, V. & Duraising, E. (2007). Targeted assessment of students’ interdisciplinary work: An empirically grounded framework. The Journal of Higher Education,78, 215-237. Boix Mansilla, V., Miller, W.C., Gardner, H. (2000). On disciplinary lenses and disciplinary work. In S. Wineburg & P. Grossman (Eds.), Interdisciplinary curriculum: Challenges to implementation. (pp. 1-16). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Bossert, S.T. (1988). Cooperative activities in the classroom. Review of Research in Education, 15, 225-250. Bransford, J., Sherwood, R., Vye, N., & Reiser, J. (1986). Teaching thinking and problem solving: Suggestions from research. American Psychologist, 41, 10781089. Brown, A. & Palincsar, A. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowledge, Learning and Instruction (pp. 307-336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Brown, J.S., Allan Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning, Educational Researcher, 18, 32 - 42. Chalmers, A.F. (1999). What is this thing called science? Australia: University of Queensland Press. Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt (CTGV). (1990). Anchored instruction and its relationship to situated cognition. Educational Researcher, 19, 2-10. Colliver, J.A. (2000). Effectiveness of problem-based learning curricula: research and theory. Academic Medicine, 75, 259-66. Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education (CBASSE). (1999). How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School. Washington D.C.: National Academy Press. Committee on Behavioral and Social Sciences in Medical School Curriculum, Institute of Medicine of the National Academies. (2004). Improving medical education: Enhancing the behavioral and social science content of medical school curricula. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press. Cohen, E. (1994). Restructuring the classroom: Conditions for productive small groups. Review of Educational Research, 64, 1-35. Creswell, J. (2009). Research Design: Qualitative, Quantitative, and Mixed Methods Approaches. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications. D’Avanzo, C. (2008). Biology Concept Inventories: Overview, Status, and Next Steps. BioScience, 58, 1079-1085. 146 Davidson, N. (1985). Small group learning and teaching in mathematics: A selective review of the research. In R. Slavin, S. Sharan, S. Kagan, R. Hertz-Lazarowitz, G. Webb, & R. Schmuck (Eds.), Learning to cooperate, cooperating to learn. New York: Plenum. Dochy, F., Segers, M., Van den Bossche, P., & Gijbels, D. (2003). Effects of problembased learning: A meta-analysis. Learn. Instr. 13: 533–568. Dori, Y.J., Tal, R., & Tsaushu, M. (2003). Teaching biotechnology through case studies: Can we improve higher-order thinking skills of non-science majors? Science Education, 87, 767–793. Duesberg, P. (1988). HIV is not the cause of AIDS. Science, 241, 514-517. Fink, L. (2002). Beyond small groups: Harnessing the extraordinary power of learning teams. In L. K. Michaelsen, A. B. Knight, and L. D. Fink (Eds.), Team-Based Learning: a Transformative Use of Small Groups (pp.3-26). Westport, CT: Praeger Press. Fleming, R. (1986a). Adolescent reasoning in socio-scientific issues. Part I: Social cognition. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 23, 677–687. Gijlers, H. & de Jong, T. (2005). The relation between prior knowledge and students’ collaborative discovery learning processes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42,264-282. Gore, A. (2006). An Inconvenient truth The planetary emergency of global warming and what we can do about it. New York, NY: Rodale. Grossman, P., Wineburg, S., Beers, S. (2000). Introduction: When theory meets practice in the world of school. In S. Wineburg & P. Grossman (Eds.), Interdisciplinary curriculum: Challenges to implementation. (pp. 1-16). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Harris, R., & Ratcliffe, M. (2005). Socio-scientific issues and the quality of exploratory talk--what can be learned from schools involved in a ‘collapsed day’ project? Curriculum Journal, 16, 439–453. Hmelo-Silver, C., Duncan, R., & Chinn, C. (2007). Scaffolding and achievement in problem-based inquiry learning: a response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark. (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42, 99-107. Hmelo, C.E., & Evensen, D.H. (2000). Problem-based learning: Gaining insights on learning interactions through multiple methods of inquiry. In C. E. Hmelo, & D. H. Evensen (Eds.), Problem-based learning: A research perspective on learning interactions (pp. 1-19). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hogan, K. (1999). Thinking aloud together: A test of an intervention to foster students’ collaborative scientific reasoning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36, 1085-1109. 147 Hogan, K. (2002). Small groups’ ecological reasoning while making an environmental management decision. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 341–368. Hogan, K., Nastasi, B.K., & Pressley, M. (2000). Discourse patterns and collaborative scientific reasoning in peer and teacher-guided discussions. Cognition und Instructional, I7 (4), 379-432. Human Biology Program, Indiana University (2007). Welcome to Human Biology at Indiana University. Retrieved December October 1, 2008, from http://www.indiana.edu/~humbio. Kavalovsky,V. C. (1979). Interdisciplinary education and humanistic aspiration. In J. J. Kockelmans (Ed.), Interdisciplinarity and higher education(pp. 224–43). University Park, PA:Pennsylvania State University Press. Khishfe, R., & Lederman, N.G. (2006). Teaching nature of science within a controversial topic: Integrated versus non-integrated. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43, 395–318. King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (Eds.). (1994). Developing reflective judgment: Understanding and promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San Francisco: Jossey-Boss. Kinneavy, J. (1980). A theory of discourse. New York, NY: Norton. Klahr, D., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 12, 1-55. Klymkowsky, M. W. & Garvin-Doxas, K. (2008). Recognizing student misconceptions through Ed’s Tools and the Biology Concept Inventory. PLoS Biology, 6, 14-17. Kolodner, J., Hmelo, C., Narayanan, N. (1996). Problem-based learning meets case-based reasoning. Proceedings of the 1996 international conference on Learning sciences, 188-195. Kuhn, D. (1993). Science as argument: Implications for teaching and learning scientific thinking. Science Education, 77, 319–337. Land, S. & Zembal-Saul, C. (2003). Scaffolding reflection and articulation of scientific explanations in a data-rich, project-based learning environment: An investigation of progress portfolio. Educational Technology Research and Development, 51, 65-84. Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lederman, N. (2003). Introduction. In D.L. Zeidler (Ed.),The Role of Moral Reasoning on Socioscientific Issues and Discourse in Science Education (pp. 1-4). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers. 148 Lederman, N.G., Abd-El-Khalick, F., Bell, R.L., & Schwartz, R.S. (2002). Views of nature of science questionnaire: Toward valid and meaningful assessment of learners’ conceptions of nature of science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 497–521. Michaelson, L., Watson, W., Black, R. (1989). A realistic test of individual versus group consensus decision making. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 834-839. Morse, J.M. (1991). Approaches to qualitative-quantitative methodological triangulation. Nursing Research, 40, 120-123. National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Newell,W., & Green,W. J. (1982/1998). Defining and teaching interdisciplinary studies. In W. Newell (Ed.) Interdisciplinarity: Essays from the literature. (pp. 23–30). New York: College Board Publications. Norman, G. & Schmidt, H. (1992). The psychological basis of problem-based learning: a review of the evidence. Academic Medicine. 67, 557-65. Okada, T. & Simon, H. A. (1997). Collaborative discovery in a scientific domain. Cognitive Science, 21, 109-146. Pedretti, E. (1999). Decision making and STS education: Exploring scientific knowledge and social responsibility in schools and science centers through an issues-based approach. School Science and Mathematics, 99, 174–181. Perkins, D.N., Farady, M., & Bushey, B. (1991). Everyday reasoning and the roots of intelligence. In J.F. Voss, D.N. Perkins, & J.W. Segal (Eds.), Informal reasoning and education (pp. 83–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Perry, W. G. (1979). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years: A scheme. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Roberts, D.A. (2007). Scientific literacy / science literacy. In S.K. Abell, & N.G. Lederman (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education (pp. 729 – 780). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Roschelle, J. (1992). Learning by collaborating: Convergent conceptual change. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2, 235-276. Rutherford, F. and Ahlgren, A. (1991). Science for All Americans. Retrieved October 30, 2008, from http://www.project2061.org/publications/sfaa/online/intro.htm. Sadler, T.D. (2004). Informal reasoning regarding socio-scientific issues: A critical review of research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41, 513–536. Sadler, T. D. (2009). Situated learning in science education: socio-scientific issues as contexts for practice, Studies in Science Education, 45, 1-42. 149 Sadler, T., Barab, S., Scott, B. (2007). What do students gain by engaging in socioscientific inquiry? Research in Science Education, 37, 371-391. Sadler, T. D. & Zeidler, D. L. (2009). Scientific literacy, PISA, and socioscientific discourse: Assessment for progressive aims of science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 909-921. Savery, J. R., and Duffy, T. M. (1995). Problem based learning: An instructional model and its constructivist framework. Educational Technology, 35, 31-38. Schilling, K. L. (2001). Interdisciplinary assessment for interdisciplinary programs. In B. L. Smith & J. McCann (Eds.), Reinventing ourselves: Interdisciplinary education, collaborative learning and experimentation in higher education (pp. 344–54). Bolton, MA: Anker. Schlegel, W. M. & Pace, D. (2004). Using collaborative learning to decode the disciplines in physiology and history. In: J. Mittendorf & D. Pace (Eds.), Decoding the disciplines: How do we more effectively bring students into the thinking we do in our disciplines? Editors: Joan Mittendorf and David Pace. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Schwartz, Lederman N.G., & Lederman R.S. (2008, March). An Instrument to assess the views of scientific inquiry: the VOSI questionnaire. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, St. Louis, MO. Schwartz, R. S., Lederman, N. G., & Thompson, R. (2001, March). Grade nine students’ views of nature of science and scientific inquiry: The effects of an inquiry enthusiast’s approach to teaching science as inquiry. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Baltimore, MD. Sherwood, R., Petrosino, A., Lin, X.D., and the Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1998). Problem based macro contexts in science instruction: Design issues and applications. In B.J. Fraser & K. Tobin (Eds.), International handbook of science education (pp. 349-362). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer. Springer, L., Stanne, M., Donovan, S. (1999). Effects of small-group learning on undergraduates in science, mathematics, engineering, and technology: A meta analysis. Review of Educational Research, 69, 21-51. Tal, R., & Hochberg, N. (2003). Assessing high order thinking of students participating in the ‘WISE’ project in Israel. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 29, 69–89. Tal, T., & Kedmi, Y. (2006). Teaching socio-scientific issues: Classroom culture and students’ performances. Cultural Studies in Science, 1, 615–644. Thompson Klein, J. (1990). Interdisciplinarity: History, Theory, and Practice. Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press. 150 Tytler, R., Duggan, S., & Gott, R. (2001). Dimensions of evidence, the public understanding of science and science education. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 815–832. Vernon, D.T. & Blake, R.L. (1993). Does problem-based learning work? A meta-analysis of evaluative research. Academic Medicine, 68, 550-63. Walker, K.A., & Zeidler, D.L. (2007). Promoting discourse about socio-scientific issues through scaffolded inquiry. International Journal of Science Education, 29,1387–1410. Willaims, S. (1992). Putting case-based instruction into context: examples from legal and medical education. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2, 367-427. Yager, S.O., Lim, G., & Yager, R. (2006). The advantages of an STS approach over a typical textbook dominated approach in middle school science. School Science and Mathematics, 106, 248–260. Zeidler, D. (2001). Participating in program development: Standard F. In D. Siebert & W. McIntosh (Eds.), College pathways to the science education standards (p. 18-22). Arlington, VA: National ScienceTeachers Press. Zeidler, D. & Keefer, M. (2006). The role of moral reasoning and the status of socioscientific issues in science education: Philosophical, psychological, and pedagogical considerations. In D.L. Zeidler (Ed.),The Role of Moral Reasoning on Socioscientific Issues and Discourse in Science Education (pp. 7-38). Norwell, MA:Kluwer Academic Publishers. Zeidler, D., Sadler, T., Applebaum, S., Callahan, B. (2009). Advancing reflective judgment through socioscientific issues. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 74-101. Zeidler, D.L., Sadler, T.D., Simmons, M.L., & Howe, E.V. (2005). Beyond STS: A research-based framework for socioscientific issues education. Science Education, 89. Zeidler, D.L. & Schafer, L.E. (1984). Identifying mediating factors of moral reasoning in science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 21, 1-15. Zohar, A. & Nemet, F. (2002). Fostering students’ knowledge and argumentation skills through dilemmas in human genetics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 35-62. 151 Appendix A Informed Consent Statement IRB Study #09-13719 INDIANA UNIVERSITY BLOOMINGTON INFORMED CONSENT STATEMENT Student Development in Biology (Student) You are invited to participate in a research study of student development of conceptual and disciplinary knowledge in four-year biology programs. You were selected as a possible subject because you are a participant in Human Biology or Biology degree programs. We ask that you read this form and ask any questions you may have before agreeing to be in the study. The study is being conducted by Jennifer Eastwood, Science Education, Indiana University, Bloomington. This is an unfunded dissertation study. STUDY PURPOSE The purpose of this study is to understand students’ perceptions of their experiences in their degree programs, and their understandings of biology content, scientific inquiry, and social issues related to biology. NUMBER OF PEOPLE TAKING PART IN THE STUDY: If you agree to participate, you will be one of 168 subjects who will be participating in this research. PROCEDURES FOR THE STUDY: If you agree to be in the study, you will do the following things: • • • Complete one survey, which will take between 1 1/2 and 2 hours to complete. The survey includes a section on biology content knowledge, understanding of scientific inquiry, and reasoning on social and scientific issues. Its purpose is to test content knowledge and collect student views on inquiry and social/scientific issues. Allow the researcher access to assignments you have completed in Human Biology coursework, which have been submitted electronically to Oncourse or your e-portfolio. You may be asked to participate in one or two audiotaped interviews, ranging between 30-45 minutes. Please check a box below and place your initials next to the box stating whether or not you would be willing to participate in the interview portion: ______ (intials) Yes, please contact me about participating in audiotaped interviews. I understand that if I do not want to participate later that I may chose not to. ______ (initials) No, please do not contact me about participating in audiotaped interviews. RISKS OF TAKING PART IN THE STUDY: While on the study, the risks or discomforts are: feeling uncomfortable in an interview or answering questions on the surveys, and a small risk of losing confidentiality. Great care will be taken to ensure that confidentiality is protected, and any data connected to your name and/or voice will only be seen by the researcher. No names or identities will be reported in publications or presentations of this research. BENEFITS OF TAKING PART IN THE STUDY: The benefits to participation that are reasonable to expect are having the opportunity to assess your own learning through surveys, and reflecting on your experiences in interviews. ALTERNATIVES TO TAKING PART IN THE STUDY: 152 Instead of being in the study, you have these options: You may choose not to participate in surveys or interviews. All student work collected will be normal classroom assignments, so you may choose not to allow the researcher access to your work. CONFIDENTIALITY Efforts will be made to keep your personal information confidential. We cannot guarantee absolute confidentiality. Your personal information may be disclosed if required by law. Your identity will be held in confidence in reports in which the study may be published. All voice recordings will be destroyed within 10 years. Organizations that may inspect and/or copy your research records for quality assurance and data analysis include groups such as the study investigator and his/her research associates, the IUB Institutional Review Board or its designees, the study sponsor, and (as allowed by law) state or federal agencies, specifically the Office for Human Research Protections (OHRP) etc., who may need to access your research records. PAYMENT You will receive compensation of $20 for taking part in this study. CONTACTS FOR QUESTIONS OR PROBLEMS For questions about the study or a research-related injury, contact the researcher, Jennifer Eastwood at [email protected]. For questions about your rights as a research participant or to discuss problems, complaints or concerns about a research study, or to obtain information, or offer input, contact the IUB Human Subjects office, 530 E Kirkwood Ave, Carmichael Center, L03, Bloomington IN 47408, 812-855-3067 or by email at [email protected] VOLUNTARY NATURE OF STUDY Taking part in this study is voluntary. You may choose not to take part or may leave the study at any time. Leaving the study will not result in any penalty or loss of benefits to which you are entitled. Your decision whether or not to participate in this study will not affect your current or future relations with the investigator(s). SUBJECT’S CONSENT In consideration of all of the above, I give my consent to participate in this research study. I will be given a copy of this informed consent document to keep for my records. I agree to take part in this study. Subject’s Printed Name: Date: Subject’s Signature: (must be dated by the subject) Printed Name of Person Obtaining Consent: Date: Signature of Person Obtaining Consent: V. 12/2008 1 153 Appendix B Overhead Slide for Participant Recruitment !"##$%&'()*+,&-./+)0+,&-.$*&+1$'%*"*2+"*+3*.$%2%'.-'&$+4")5)2/+ •!6+'7+"*&$%$#&$.+"*+8)9+&8$+5$'%*"*2+$*:"%)*7$*&+'*.+;%)2%'7+#&%-<&-%$+'=$<&+ #&-.$*&+5$'%*"*2+'*.+;$%<$;()*#+)0+>")5)2/+<)*&$*&?+-*.$%#&'*."*2+)0+"*@-"%/?+ '*.+%$'#)*"*2+9"&8+#)<"'5+'*.+#<"$*(A<+"##-$#B+ •!C)-%+;'%(<";'()*+"*<5-.$#D+ •!E+#-%:$/+0)<-#"*2+)*+>")5)2/+<)*&$*&?+#<"$*(A<+"*@-"%/?+'*.+ #)<")#<"$*(A<+"##-$#+ •!E>)-&+F+G+8)-%#+ •!H'I$*+"*+4'55'*(*$+<)7;-&$%+5'>#+ •!JKL+<)7;$*#'()*++ •!H"7$#+':'"5'>5$D++ M)*?+M'%<8+KND+O;7+)%+PDNL+;7+ Q$.?+M'%<8+KO?+ODNL+;7+ H8-%?+M'%<8+KP?+PDFO+;7+ R%"?+M'%<8+KS?+KDNL+;7+ •!E+0$9+#&-.$*&#+9"55+>$+'#I$.+&)+;'%(<";'&$+"*+'+#8)%&+"*&$%:"$9+ R$$5+0%$$+&)+<)*&'<&+7$?+T$**"0$%+U'#&9)).?+V:'*.-#$W"*."'*'B$.-+ 154 Appendix C Demographic Sheet Name:__________________________ ID Number (entered on survey)_______________ 1. What is your major? 2. What is your minor? 3. What is your gender? 4. What is your race/ethnicity? 5. Do you have a focus area? (pre-medical, ecology, molecular biology, etc.)? 6. How many college-level biology classes have you taken (including this semester)? Science classes? Social science classes? 7. What do you plan to do after graduation (graduate school, medical school, etc.)? 8. What is your GPA? Major GPA? 9. Why did you choose your major? 155 10. What extra-curricular (outside of course requirements) activities are you involved in? For each, how long have you been involved? 11. Have you worked in a research lab? _________ If yes, how long were you involved? What is the focus of that lab? What is/was your reason for being involved in the lab? 12 Have you been involved in teaching in your undergraduate career? If so, which classes? Describe your role. 156 Appendix D Open-ended Questionnaire ID number___________________ Questionnaire: Scientific Inquiry, Biology Content, and Social and Scientific Issues PART I: VIEWS ON SCIENTIFIC INQUIRY Adapted from Schwartz et al. (2008) The following questions are asking for your views related to science and scientific investigations. There are no right or wrong answers. Please answer each of the following questions. You can use all the space provided to answer a question and continue on the back of the pages if necessary. 1. What types of activities do scientists do to learn about the natural world? Be specific about how they go about their work. 2. What scientists choose to study and how they learn about the natural world may be influenced by a variety of factors. How do scientists decide what and how to investigate? Describe all the factors you think influence the work of scientists. Be as specific as possible. 3. (a) Write a definition of a scientific experiment? A scientific experiment is…… (b) Give an example from something you have done or heard about in science that illustrates your definition of a scientific experiment. (c) Explain why you consider your example to be a scientific experiment. 4. Some people have claimed that all scientific investigations must follow the same general set of steps or method to be considered science. Others have claimed there are different general methods that scientific investigations can follow. 157 (a) What do you think? Is there one scientific method or set of steps that all investigations must follow to be considered science? Highlight one answer: • Yes, there is one scientific method (set of steps) to science. • No, there is more than one scientific method to science. • If you answered “yes,” go to (b) below. If you answered “no,” go to (c) below. (b) If you think there is one scientific method, what are the steps of this method? (c) If you think that scientific investigations can follow more than one method, describe two investigations that follow different methods. Explain how the methods differ and how they can still be considered scientific. 5. (a) If several scientists, working independently, ask the same question and follow the same procedures to collect data, will they necessarily come to the same conclusions? Explain why or why not. (b) Does your response to (a) change if the scientists are working together? Explain. 6. (a) What does the word “data” mean in science? (b) Is “data” the same or different from “evidence” ? Explain. 7. (a) What is “data analysis” ? (b) What is involved in doing data analysis? 158 8. After scientists have developed a theory, does the theory ever change? If you believe that theories do change, explain why theories are important to the scientific community. Defend your answer with examples. PART II: DECISION MAKING QUESTIONNAIRE (DMQ) Adapted from Bell and Lederman (2003) Instructions Answer the following questions using as much space as you need. Please note that there are no “right” or “wrong” answers to these questions. I am simply interested in your views on a number of issues about science. Scenario I Today, global climate change is a major environmental issue facing the United States and the international community. According to one side, the prospect of human-induced global warming is a near certainty, and failure to address the problem will have catastrophic ecological consequences. According to the other side, global warming is a hypothesis lacking scientific validation, and reducing greenhouse gas emissions will have serious negative economic consequences. In 1992, the United States, along with roughly 150 other nations, signed the United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change (FCCC) at the Earth Summit in Rio de Janeiro. The FCCC was ratified by the US Senate in 1992 and has now been ratified by a total of 166 nations. The ultimate objective of this treaty is to “achieve stabilization of greenhouse gas concentrations in the atmosphere at a level thatwould prevent dangerous anthropogenic interference with the climate system.” In line with this objective, the most industrialized nations, including the United States, agreed to the voluntarily aim of returning their greenhouse gas emissions back to 1990 levels by the year 2000. However, the United States and most other industrialized nations are not on course to meet this target. In fact, emissions in the United States are projected to be 13% higher in the year 2000 than they were in 1990. Because these voluntary targets have proven inadequate in curbing emissions growth, there is now widespread agreement that legally-binding measures are necessary. The upcoming climate conference in Kyoto, Japan, is based on the premise that the participating nations should agree, for the first time, upon a legally-binding limit on emissions. 159 Please answer questions 1-4 based on Scenario I. Questions 1. Should the United States agree to legally-binding limits on greenhouse gas emissions? Why or why not? 2. Should the United States impose special taxes on carbon dioxide emission to encourage energy conservation, even if this increased monthly electricity and heating bills by $25 per month? Why or why not? 3. Would you be willing to pay increased taxes in order to provide funding for research on alternative energy resources, such as solar power and fusion reactors? Why or why not? 4. Should the United States reduce automobile emissions by setting higher gas mileage standards, even if this increased the average cost of a new car by $500? Why or why not? Scenario II Researchers are just beginning to unravel the role of diet and nutrition in the development of cancer, or carcinogenesis. It is clear that carcinogenesis is a slow process, often taking 10–30 years. Diet may play an important role during the initiation of cancer whereby certain foods may serve to increase detoxifying enzymes that help stop the initial stimulation and growth of the cancer cells. At the same time, other nutrients and foods such as fat may serve as promoters for already initiated cancer cells. Scientists have estimated that diet is responsible for 20–40% of all cancers, perhaps as high as 70%. Diets rich in fruits, vegetables, and fiber have consistently been shown to have a beneficial effect on cancer. On the other hand, heavy consumption of red meats, saturated fats, and salty foods have been linked to a variety of cancers. Other lifestyle factors related to nutrition also appear to be associated with cancer. Obesity has been linked to a variety of cancers, including endometrial, breast, colon, and ovarian. Alcohol consumption has been linked to cancers of the digestive tract and liver. Conversely, several studies have supported the beneficial aspects of physical activity, which may reduce the risk of several types of cancer, including colon, breast, and prostate. Questions 1. How would you rate your overall awareness of the impact of diet and related factors on the development of cancer? 160 2. Has your awareness of the benefits of physical activity and a diet rich in fruits and vegetables impacted how you conduct your life? If not, why not? If so, in what way(s)? 3. Do you ever base decisions about what to eat on your understandings of current research into diet and cancer? If not, why not. If so, in what ways? 4. Do you regularly exercise? Why or why not? 5. Would you support increased legislation on foods associated with cancer, including removing high risk foods from the market? Scenario III Many researchers believe that smoking accounts for a large proportion of all cancers and as much as 30% of all cancer deaths. Cigarette smoking has specifically been implicated as the cause of cancer of the lung, oral cavity, larynx, esophagus, bladder, kidney, and pancreas. Additionally, the risk of developing cancer is greater for people who smoke more and who start smoking at a younger age. Furthermore, researchers believe that smoking may be the cause of 25–30% of all heart disease. Exposure to passive tobacco smoke is very likely a significant cause of cancer in nonsmokers. Some scientists believe that the increased risk could be as high as 50%. It has been estimated that thousands of people die each year due to exposure to passive cigarette smoke. Recently, nicotine in cigarette tobacco has been identified as a drug whose addictiveness exceeds that of opium and heroine. In addition to this, documents have come to light that indicate that some tobacco companies have used a variety of methods to increase the amount and potency of nicotine in cigarette tobacco. Finally, it has been shown that many people begin smoking as teenagers, and once started, have a very difficult time quitting. In contrast to these claims, tobacco companies have consistently asserted that while tobacco may be associated with increased risk for various cancers and heart disease, it has never been proven to cause these diseases. Furthermore, to smoke or not is a free choice that should be up to the consumer, not government agencies. Questions 1. Given the reported dangers of cigarette smoke and its addictiveness, should legislation be passed that would make cigarette smoking illegal? Why or why not? 2. Would you support legislation that makes it more difficult for minors to obtain cigarettes and/or penalizes tobacco companies who target minors in their advertising? Why or why not? 3. Do the alleged dangers of passive cigarette smoke justify banning smoking in public places such as restaurants and bars? Why or why not? 161 Appendix E Biology Concept Inventory ©bioliteracy.net Used in this dissertation with permission of developer Accessed from https://edstools.colorado.edu/input/i-multi.php?inv=bci&cond=0 Question: 1 Many types of house plants droop when they have not been watered and quickly "straighten up" after watering. The reason that they change shape after watering is because ... Water reacts with, and stiffens, their cell walls. Water is used to generate energy that moves the plant. Water changes the concentration of salts within the plant. Water enters and expands their cells. Question: 2 In which way are plants and animals different in how they obtain energy? Animals use ATP; plants do not. Plants capture energy from sunlight; animals capture chemical energy. Plants store energy in sugar molecules; animals do not. Animals can synthesize sugars from simpler molecules; plants cannot. Question: 3 In which way are plants and animals different in how they use energy? Animals use energy to break down molecules; plants cannot. Animals use energy to move; plants cannot. Plants use energy directly, animals must transform it. 162 Question: 4 How can a catastrophic global event influence evolutionary change? Undesirable versions of the gene are removed. New genes are generated. Only some species may survive the event. There are short term effects that disappear over time. Question: 5 There exists a population in which there are three distinct versions of the gene A (a1, a2, and a3). Originally, each version was present in equal numbers of individuals. Which version of the gene an individual carries has no measurable effect on its reproductive success. As you follow the population over a number of generations, you find that the frequency of a1 and a3 drop to 0%. What is the most likely explanation? There was an increased rate of mutation in organisms that carry either a1 or a3. Mutations have occured that changed a1 and a3 into a2. Individuals carrying a1 or a3 were removed by natural selection. Random variations led to a failure to produce individuals carying a1 or a3. Question: 6 Natural selection produces evolutionary change by ... changing the frequency of various versions of genes. reducing the number of new mutations. producing genes needed for new environments. reducing the effects of detrimental versions of genes. Question: 7 If two parents display distinct forms of a trait and all their offspring (of which there are hundreds) display the same new form of the trait, you would be justified in concluding that ... both parents were heterozygous for the gene that controls the trait. both parents were homozygous for the gene that controls the trait. one parent was heterozygous, the other was homozygous for the gene that controls the trait. a recombination event has occurred in one or both parents. 163 Question: 8 You are doing experiments to test whether a specific type of acupuncture works. This type of acupuncture holds that specific needle insertion points influence specific parts of the body. As part of your experimental design, you randomize your treatments so that some people get acupuncture needles inserted into the "correct" sites and others into "incorrect" sites. What is the point of inserting needles into incorrect places? It serves as a negative control. It serves as a positive control. It controls for whether the person can feel the needle. It controls for whether needles are necessary. Question: 9 As part of your experiments on the scientific validity of this particular type of acupuncture, it would be important to ... test only people who believe in acupuncture. test only people without opinions, pro or con, about acupuncture. have the study performed by researchers who believe in this form of acupuncture. determine whether placing needles in different places produces different results. Question: 10 What makes DNA a good place to store information? The hydrogen bonds that hold it together are very stable and difficult to break. The bases always bind to their correct partner. The sequence of bases does not greatly influence the structure of the molecule. The overall shape of the molecule reflects the information stored in it. Question: 11 What is it about nucleic acids that makes copying genetic information straightforward? Hydrogen bonds are easily broken. The binding of bases to one another is specific. The sequence of bases encodes information. The shape of the molecule is determined by the information it contains. 164 Question: 12 It is often the case that a structure (such as a functional eye) is lost during the course of evolution. This is because ... It is no longer actively used. Mutations accumulate that disrupt its function. It interferes with other traits and functions. The cost of maintaining it is not justified by the benefits it brings. Question: 13 When we want to know whether a specific molecule will pass through a biological membrane, we need to consider ... The specific types of lipids present in the membrane. The degree to which the molecule is water soluble. Whether the molecule is actively repelled by the lipid layer. Whether the molecule is harmful to the cell. Question: 14 How might a mutation be creative? It could not be; all naturally occuring mutations are destructive. If the mutation inactivated a gene that was harmful. If the mutation altered the gene product's activity. If the mutation had no effect on the activity of the gene product. Question: 15 An allele exists that is harmful when either homozygous or heterozygous. Over the course of a few generations the frequency of this allele increases. Which is a possible explanation? The allele ... is located close to a favorable allele of another gene. has benefits that cannot be measured in terms of reproductive fitness. is resistant to change by mutation. encodes an essential protein. 165 Question: 16 In a diploid organism, what do we mean when we say that a trait is dominant? It is stronger than a recessive form of the trait. It is due to more, or a more active gene product than is the recessive trait. The trait associated with the allele is present whenever the allele is present. The allele associated with the trait inactivates the products of recessive alleles. Question: 17 How does a molecule bind to its correct partner and avoid "incorrect" interactions? The two molecules send signals to each other. The molecules have sensors that check for incorrect bindings. Correct binding results in lower energy than incorrect binding. Correctly bound molecules fit perfectly, like puzzle pieces. Question: 18 Once two molecules bind to one another, how could they come back apart again? A chemical reaction must change the structure of one of the molecules. Collisions with other molecules could knock them apart. The complex will need to be degraded. They would need to bind to yet another molecule. Question: 19 Why is double-stranded DNA not a good catalyst? It is stable and does not bind to other molecules. It isn't very flexible and can't fold into different shapes. It easily binds to other molecules. It is located in the nucleus. 166 Question: 20 Lipids can form structures like micelles and bilayers because of ... their inability to bond with water molecules. their inability to interact with other molecules. their ability to bind specifically to other lipid molecules. the ability of parts of lipid molecules to interact strongly with water. Question: 21 A mutation leads to a dominant trait; what can you conclude about the mutation's effect? It results in an overactive gene product. It results in a normal gene product that accumulates to higher levels than normal. It results in a gene product with a new function. It depends upon the nature of the gene product and the mutation. Question: 22 How similar is your genetic information to that of your parents? For each gene, one of your alleles is from one parent and the other is from the other parent. You have a set of genes similar to those your parents inherited from their parents. You contain the same genetic information as each of your parents, just half as much. Depending on how much crossing over happens, you could have a lot of one parent's genetic information and little of the other parent's genetic information. Question: 23 An individual, "A", displays two distinct traits. A single, but different gene controls each trait. You examine A's offspring, of which there are hundreds, and find that most display either the same two traits displayed by A, or neither trait. There are, however, rare offspring that display one or the other trait, but not both. The genes controlling the two traits are located on different chromosomes. The genes controlling the two traits are located close together on a single chromosome. The genes controlling the two traits are located at opposite ends of the same chromosome. 167 Question: 24 A mutation leads to a recessive trait; what can you conclude about the mutation's effect? It results in a non-functional gene product. It results in a normal gene product that accumulates to lower levels than normal. It results in a gene product with a new function. It depends upon the nature of the gene product and the mutation. Question: 25 Imagine an ADP molecule inside a bacterial cell. Which best describes how it would manage to "find" an ATP synthase so that it could become an ATP molecule? It would follow the hydrogen ion flow. The ATP synthase would grab it. Its electronegativity would attract it to the ATP synthase. It would actively be pumped to the right area. Random movements would bring it to the ATP synthase. Question: 26 You follow the frequency of a particular version of a gene in a population of asexual organisms. Over time, you find that this version of the gene disappears from the population. Its disappearance is presumably due to ... genetic drift. its effects on reproductive success. its mutation. the randomness of survival. Question: 27 Consider a diploid organism that is homozygous for a particular gene. How might the deletion of this gene from one of the two chromosomes produce a phenotype? If the gene encodes a multifunctional protein. If one copy of the gene did not produce enough gene product. If the deleted allele were dominant. If the gene encoded a transcription factor. 168 Question: 28 Gene A and gene B are located on the same chromosome. Consider the following cross: AB/ab X ab/ab. Under what conditions would you expect to find 25% of the individuals with an Ab genotype. It cannot happen because the A and B genes are linked. It will always occur, because of independent assortment. It will occur only when the genes are far away from one another. It will occur only when the genes are close enough for recombination to occur between them. Question: 29 Sexual reproduction leads to genetic drift because ... there is randomness associated with finding a mate. not all alleles are passed from parent to offspring. it is associated with an increase in mutation rate. it produces new combinations of alleles. Question: 30 How is genetic drift like molecular diffusion? Both are the result of directed movements. Both involve passing through a barrier. Both involve random events without regard to ultimate outcome. They are not alike. Genetic drift is random; diffusion typically has a direction 169 Appendix F Semistructured Interview Protocol for Human Biology and Biology Majors Perceptions and Knowledge of Content Learning • How would you describe your level of biology content knowledge after completing your program? • Validate BCI o Can you describe your understanding of how evolution works? Always selecting adv. Traits? o What makes DNA a good place to store information? o What is diffusion and why does it occur? Perceptions and Knowledge of Inquiry • From your experience, how would you define inquiry in biology? • What experiences helped you develop that understanding? • Validate VOSI o What is an experiment? o Define data. Is it the same as evidence? What’s the difference? o What is a theory? Other VOSI items Perceptions and Knowledge of Socioscientific Issues • How well do you feel your program has prepared you to understand and make decisions on controversial issues related to biology (issues that have social impact)? Can you give some examples? • Validate DMQ o From scenario 1: Recent science reports have argued that the phenomenon of global warming may be due primarily to landscape transformations from forest and grassland to concrete roads and buildings, rather than carbon emissions. In view of this conflicting evidence, how could you make decisions about regulating carbon emissions? o From scenario 2: How do you make dietary decisions when nutritionists have repeatedly altered their recommendations, as in the case of the inclusion of Omega-3 fatty acid supplements in the diet? 170 o From scenario 3: How would you make decisions considering some scientists’ assertions that the links between tobacco and cancer have never been proven? Perceptions of Program • Why did you choose to be a student in your program? • How would you describe the primary teaching strategies used in your program? • Of these aspects, what was most helpful to your learning? • What did you find unhelpful or difficult? • Have you had any classes that use non-traditional approaches? • Have you had any classes that have helped you to explore different perspectives toward biology issues with social implications? • How would you describe the level of community in your program? • Were there any opportunities that were available or that you took advantage of, that enhanced your experience as a Human Biology/biology major? Future Plans, Impact of Program • What are your plans after graduation? • Would you say your program has helped you meet your goals? How? • How would you describe your development in your program? How have you changed over the past 4 years as a result of being a biology/HUBI major? 171 Appendix G Coding Scheme for the Modified VOSI Question 1. What types of activities do scientists do to learn about the natural world? Be specific about how they go about their work. Addresses Processes of inquiry 2. What scientists choose to study and how they learn about the natural world may be influenced by a variety of factors. How do scientists decide what and how to investigate? Describe all the factors you think influence the work of scientists. Be as specific as possible. Addresses Purpose of inquiry 3. (a) Write a definition of a scientific experiment. A scientific experiment is…… Addresses Meaning of experiment Response Codes Definition QUESTION Begin with/center investigation in question DIFF METHODS Different environments require different methods; field vs. wild DIFF SCIENCES Different fields of inquiry (social sciences vs. biological/physical require different approaches) CONTROL Some investigations are controlled, others involve observation of uncontrolled natural process EXPERIMENTAL Purely experimental conception; controlled environment SCI METHOD Explicitly states that scientists use “scientific method” GENERAL INQUIRY searching for information about natural world SCIENCE PROCESS SKILLS at least one: hypothesis, data collection, analysis, etc. SOCIAL SCIENCE PROCESSES INTERNAL at least one: interview, survey, etc. Reasons internal to scientist-interest, personal/family reasons, prior experiences, influences on personal background Reasons external to scientist other than practical concerns-current trends, interest of society, current influences of other people EXTERNAL Practical concerns or limitations-funding, resources, time constraints PRACTICAL CONTROL Control and manipulate variables REPLICABLE Must be replicable GENERAL INQUIRY Searching for answers/new knowledge SCI METHOD- States that the scientific method must be followed TESTS HYPOTHESIS Hypothesis is tested VALIDITY/ Has validity, accuracy, regulation, or 172 ACCURACY extensive planning CAUSE-EFFECT WHOLE PROJECT Looks for causal relationships Uses example of whole project or scientific endeavor (ex/ cancer research, discover gene, etc.) 3. (b) Give an example from something you have done or heard about in science that illustrates your definition of a scientific experiment. CLASS Describe class experiment RESEARCH GROUP Describe controlled lab/field experiment SOC SCI Describe social science project Addresses Meaning of experiment 3. (c) Explain why you consider your example to be a scientific experiment. PROCEDURE Only procedural part of a project, ex/ gel electrophoresis with 3a CONSISTENT INCONSISTENT Addresses Meaning of experiment 4. (a) Is there one scientific method or set of steps that all investigations must follow to be considered science? If you answered “yes,” answer (b) below. If you answered “no,” answer (c) below. Addresses Definition/existence of scientific method 4. (b) If you think there is one scientific method, what are the steps of this method? Addresses Definition of scientific method YES NO QUESTION includes questions OBSERVE includes observation BG includes background research HYPOTHESIS includes hypothesizing DATA includes data collection/methods ANALYSIS includes analysis CONCLUSIONS includes development of conclusion/theory REVISE/REPEAT revise hypothesis and continue research COMMUNICATE presenting or publishing GENERAL INQUIRY Scientific method viewed as general information-gathering FOLLOWS STEPS Certain steps must be followed 173 4. (c) If you think that scientific investigations can follow more than one method, describe two investigations that follow different methods. Explain how the methods differ and how they can still be considered scientific. Addresses Explanation of multiple methods SOME VARIATION Scientists vary steps or order of steps DIFF METHODS Methods vary depending on project CONTROL Controlled environment vs. “wild” (ability or desire to control) EXPLORATORY/ CONFIRMATORY Interventionist or observational DIFF METHODS Different science protocols, materials for different science subject matter DIFF SCIENCES Natural and physical sciences vary from social sciences DIFF ORDER OF METHODS Scientists may do steps in different orders INVESTIGATE Scientific if answers are being sought NAT WORLD Deal with natural world SYSTEMATIC Rigorous or regulated RELIABLE YES Others should see same results SAME PROCEDURES Scientists will use the same procedures; fully replicable NO 5. (a) If several scientists, working independently, ask the same question and follow the same procedures to collect data, will they necessarily come to the same conclusions? Explain why or why not. Addresses Impact of researcher on science Identification/resolution of anomaly Subjectivity ERROR There will still be differences in error or accuracy DIFF DATA Uncontrollable factors or different environments lead to different data DIFF METHODS Scientists will still use different procedures DIFF INTERPRETATIONS Scientists interpret results differently or have different perspectives CAN COME CLOSE Scientists should have similar results MAYBE Elements of both yes and no answers CHANGE SAME DATA If working together data will be the same 174 5. (b) Does your response to (a) change if the scientists are working together? Explain. Addresses Identification/resolution of anomaly subjectivity SAME PROCEDURES Procedures will be the same REACH CONSENSUS Scientists will challenge and resolve differences REDUCE ERROR Working together reduces differences resulting from error NO DIFF INTERPRETATIONS Scientists still interpret results differently. They may disagree or have personal biases. DIFF DATA Uncontrollable factors still result in different data. DIFF METHODS MORE LIKELY Scientists will still use different methods MAYBE CHANGE SAME DATA/METHODS DIFF INTERPRETATIONS INFO COLLECTED 6. (a) What does the word “data” mean in science? Addresses Difference between data and evidence Information collected in experiments or observations RESULTS Uses only the term, “results” NUMBERS Data must be quantitative QUANT/QUAL Data can be quantitative or qualitative SUPPORTS IDEA Data supports a hypothesis or idea ANALYSES SAME Data has been analyzed BOTH SUPPORT Both support an idea or answer a question DIFF 6. (b) Is “data” the same or different from “evidence” ? Explain. Addresses Difference between data and evidence EVIDENCE SUPPORTS Evidence supports or proves an idea, or explains or interprets a situation EVIDENCE IS MORE CERTAIN Evidence is more definite, certain, conclusive, or factual EVIDENCE IS LESS PRECISE Evidence is less “correct,” “scientific,” exact, or is biased EVIDENCE IS GENERALIZA-TION Evidence is a generalization, abstraction, compilation, or analysis of data 175 DATA IS NUMBERS Data is always quantitative DATA CAN BE EVIDENCE State data can be evidence, but not how they are different EVIDENCE CAN CHANGE Evidence is less permanent NO EVIDENCE IN SCIENCE There can be no such thing as evidence in science because there is no proof in science. EVIDENCE FROM OTHER SOURCES Evidence can come from other sources than the current inquiry EV IS COMPARED Evidence is compared to other evidence All data is useful, evidence may be discarded EVIDENCE MAY BE DISCARDED NO THEORY IS LAW Theory and law are the same-both do not change. YES NEW DATA (NEW TECH) NEW INTERPRETATIONS 8. After scientists have developed a theory, does the theory ever change? If you believe that theories do change, explain why theories are still important to the scientific community. Defend your answer with examples. Addresses Tentativeness of theory Purpose of theory Availability or discovery of new data leads to change (New technology leads to change) Theories change through new interpretations of existing data SCIENCE CHANGES General statement without elaboration SCIENTISTS REVISE OWN THEORIES Changing of theories is an individual process—no community involved BEST WORKING EXPLANATION It is the best explanation for our current understanding of a phenomenon THEORY LESS “TRUE” THAN LAW Theory is lower in hierarchy of confidence MAY REACH FINAL STATE Theories can eventually become laws or be proved true. CUMULATIVE VIEW Theories are built upon only to become better or more complex FALSIFICATION VIEW Theories are put forth to be retested or falsified IDEAS FOR NEW Theories provide ideas for future inquiry or 176 INQUIRY basis for revision of understanding Theories promote discussion of ideas in scientific communities ENCOURAGE DISCUSSION Theories provide record of how thinking has changed over time PROVIDE HISTORICAL RECORD 177 Appendix H Methods Matrix 178 179 VITA Jennifer Lynne Eastwood July, 2010 University Address: Science Education Program Curriculum and Instruction W. W. Wright Education Building Indiana University 201 N. Rose Avenue Bloomington, Indiana 47405 Home Address: 2473 S. Woolery Mill Drive Bloomington, Indiana 4740tr3 (812) 272-9079 [email protected] EDUCATION 2005-present Ph.D., Science Education (expected completion date May, 2010), Curriculum and Instruction Department, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana. • Advisor and Doctoral Committee Chair: Dr. Robert D. Sherwood • Dissertation title: The Effects of an Interdisciplinary Undergraduate Human Biology Program on Socioscientific Reasoning, Content Learning, and Understanding of Inquiry. • Minor: Anatomy and Cell Biology 2002-2005 Master of Sciences, Anatomy and Cell Biology, Medical Sciences Program, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana. • Thesis title: “Proteomic based identification of epidermal markers for the nipple.” • Advisor, Dr. John Foley 1997-2001 Bachelor of Arts, Biology, Truman State University, Kirksville, MO. • Minor, French TEACHING 2009-present Associate Instructor, A464, Human Tissue Biology, Indiana University Bloomington, IN. Completed one lab section, currently teaching two sections: Introduce histological slides and staining techniques, conduct microscope based labs, and develop practical exams. 2009-2010 Mentor to Pre-service Teachers, Saturday Science, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN. Provide support and feedback in the development and teaching of six-week informal science courses for K-2 students from the community. 2009 Associate Instructor, A215, Basic Human Anatomy, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN. • Taught two sections: Conducted lab, review, and exam sessions in undergraduate gross anatomy and histology. • Provided introductions and one-on-one assistance to students in understanding form/function relationships, and conducted small group demonstrations with human donors. 2007-2009 Associate Instructor, Q200, Introduction to Scientific Inquiry, Indiana University Bloomington, IN. • Taught five sections of a lab-based introductory science course for pre-service elementary teachers focusing on application of scientific inquiry and the nature of science. • Collaborated with instructors to develop inquiry-based activities and assessments. 2005 Associate Instructor, P312, Learning: Theory into Practice, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN. Developed and taught two sections of a course on Educational Psychology and Learning Sciences theory for pre-service secondary teachers. 2003-2006 Associate Instructor, A215, Basic Human Anatomy • Description above (2009); 13 sections • Conducted and presented educational research to improve A215 lab. 2001-2002 Secondary Biology Teacher, Crawford ISD, Crawford, Texas. • Taught 7th grade Life Science and 10th grade Biology. • Served as Crawford ISD yearbook co-sponsor. 2000 Teaching Assistant, General Biology, Truman State University, Kirksville, MO. Provided assistance in grading, course organization, and assisted students in lab activities. 2000 Teaching Assistant, Comparative Anatomy, Truman State University, Kirksville, MO. Assisted students in animal dissections and developing lab practical exams. RESEARCH ACADEMIC APPOINTMENTS 2008-2009 Graduate Educational Consultant, Indiana University Bloomington Libraries, Bloomington, IN. • Provided support with assessment development, project evaluation, and Human Subjects compliance for project on integration of ACRL Information Literacy Competency Standards for Higher Education into interdisciplinary courses including Biology and Gender Studies. • Project Title: “Information Fluency for the Disciplines,” 2007 SoTL Leadership Grant. 2008 Research Assistant, Science Education, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN. Assisted faculty on grant applications, data collection, data analysis, and manuscript writing for various projects. PUBLICATIONS Akerson, V., Buzzeli, C., Eastwood, J. (2010). The Relationship Between Preservice Early Childhood Teachers’ Cultural Values and their Perceptions of Scientists’ Cultural Values. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 21, 205-214 Buck, G., Cook, K., Quigley, C., Eastwood, J., & Lucas, Y. (2009). Four profiles of urban, low SES, African-American girls' attitudes toward science: A sequential explanatory mixed-methods study. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 3, 386-410. Eastwood, J. (2007). Standardizing Quality in a Large Undergraduate Anatomy Lab. HAPS-Educator, Summer Edition. Eastwood, J. Offutt, C. Menon, K. Keel, M. Hrncirova, P. Novotny, M.V., Arnold, R., Foley, J. (2007). Identification of markers for nipple epidermis: changes in expression during pregnancy and lactation. Differentiation. 75, 75-83. MANUSCRIPTS IN PREPARATION OR REVIEW Eastwood, J. L., & Schlegel, W. M. Electronic Reflection by Student Teams Facilitates and Provides Evidence of Team-Based Learning Process. Advances in Physiology Education. In preparation. Akerson, V., Buzelli, C., & Eastwood, J. ‘Strangers in a strange land’: Bridging the gap between preservice early childhood teachers’ cultural values and their perceptions of values held by scientists. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. In Review. Akerson, V.A., Donnely, L., Riggs, M., & Eastwood, J. Supporting preservice elementary teachers’ nature of science instruction through a community of learners. Journal of Science Teacher Education. In Review. CONFERENCE PROCEEDINGS (PUBLISHED) Buck, G. A., Cook, K., Quigley, C., Eastwood, J., Lucas, Y. (2009). Exploring how urban AfricanAmerican girls position themselves in science learning: A sequential explanatory mixed-methods study. Paper Presented at National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Garden Grove, CA. Akerson, V.A., Donnely, L., Riggs, M., & Eastwood, J. (2009). Supporting Preservice Elementary Teachers’ Nature of Science Instruction Through a Community of Learners. Paper Presented at National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Garden Grove, CA. Akerson, V.A., Donnely, L., Riggs, M., & Eastwood, J. (2009). Supporting Preservice Elementary Teachers’ Nature of Science Instruction Through a Community of Learners. Paper presented at American Educational Research Association (AERA), San Diego, CA. PRESENTATIONS AT ACADEMIC MEETINGS Eastwood, J., Cook, K., Sherwood, R., & Schlegel, W. (2010). “Not Simply What’s the Science, but How Does It Affect People, and Why Is That Important?” Effects of an Interdisciplinary Human Biology Program Focused on Socioscientific Reasoning. Research presentation, National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Philadelphia, PA. Akerson, V., Buzelli, C., & Eastwood, J. (2010). ‘Strangers in a Strange Land’: Bridging the Gap between Preservice Early Childhood Teachers’ Cultural Values and their Perceptions of Values Held by Scientists. Reasearch presentation, National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Philadelphia, PA. Eastwood, J. L., & Schlegel, W.M., (2009). Electronic Reflection by Student Teams Facilitates and Provides Evidence of Team-Based Learning Process. Research presentation, International Society for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning (ISSOTL), Bloomington, IN. Eastwood, J. L. (2009). Reflecting Student Learning in a Team-based and Case-based Physiology Class: the Developmental and Representational Roles of Reflective Activities. Research presentation, Indiana University Science Education Research Symposium, Bloomington, IN. Buck, G.A., Cook, K.L., Quigley, C.F., & Eastwood J. L. (2009) Exploring how urban African-American girls position themselves in science learning: A sequential explanatory mixed-methods study. Research presentation, National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Garden Grove, CA. Akerson, V., Buzelli, C., & Eastwood, J. (2009). The Relationship between Preservice Early Childhood Teachers’ Cultural Values and the Cultural Values they Believe that Scientists Hold. Research presentation, American Educational Research Association (AERA), San Diego, CA. Akerson, V.A., Donnely, L., Riggs, M., & Eastwood, J. (2009). Supporting Preservice Elementary Teachers’ Nature of Science Instruction Through a Community of Learners. Research presentation, National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Garden Grove, CA. Akerson, V.A., Donnely, L., Riggs, M., & Eastwood, J. (2009). Supporting Preservice Elementary Teachers’ Nature of Science Instruction Through a Community of Learners. Research presentation, American Educational Research Association (AERA), San Diego, CA. Buck, G., Amirshokoohi, A., Beeman-Cadwallader, N., Caylor, B., Eastwood, J., Nargund, V., Schmelz, R., Sher, M. (2007). Making Inquiry into Chemistry Discernible to Pre-Service Teachers. Workshop presented at Annual Meeting of the School Science and Mathematics Association, Indianapolis, IN. Eastwood, J. & Schlegel, W. M. (2006). Creating a Model for Pedagogies of Uncertainty. Research presentation, International Society for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning (ISSOTL), Washington D.C. French Doubleday, A. & Eastwood, J. (2006). Standardizing Quality in a Large Undergraduate Anatomy Lab. Workshop presented at the Annual Meeting of the Human Anatomy and Physiology Society (HAPS), Austin TX. Eastwood, J., Schlegel, W., Duffy, T. (2006). Perceptions of Equitable Contribution and Team Performance in a Case-Based Human Physiology Course. Indiana University Scholarship of Teaching and Learning Spring Poster Session, Bloomington, IN. Eastwood, J., Hrncirova, P., Arnold, R., Foley, J.G. (2005). Proteomic Based Identification of Epidermal Markers for the Murine Nipple. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting for the Society for Investigative Dermatology, St. Louis, MO. Eastwood J. & French Doubleday, A. (2005). Tips from the Trenches: an Associate Instructor’s Perspective on Teaching Human Anatomy Lab. Workshop presented at the Annual Meeting for the Human Anatomy and Physiology Society (HAPS), St. Louis, MO. HONORS AND SCHOLARSHIPS Medical Sciences Outstanding Associate Instructor Award, Indiana University, 2010 First Year Graduate Student Fellowship, Medical Sciences, Indiana University, 2002. Cum Laude, Truman State University, 2001. Presidential Scholarship, Truman State University, 1997. Combined Ability Scholarship, Truman State University, 1997. GRANT WRITING AND AWARDS Indiana University Graduate School Grant in Aid of Doctoral Research. Title: The Impacts of an Interdisciplinary Undergraduate Human Biology Program on Socioscientific Reasoning, Content Learning, and Understanding of Inquiry. February, 2010. $1000: Funded. Indiana University Graduate and Professional Student Organization Travel Award. Title: The Impacts of an Interdisciplinary Undergraduate Human Biology Program on Socioscientific Reasoning, Content Learning, and Understanding of Inquiry. February, 2010. $250: Funded. Spencer Foundation Dissertation Fellowship. Title: Can an Interdisciplinary Program Promote Students’ Development of Content Knowledge, Inquiry Understanding, and Ethical Reasoning? A Mixed-Methods Study of an Undergraduate Human Biology Program. 2009-2010. $25,000: Unfunded. NSF Discovery Research K-12. Title: Indiana University DR-K12 Resource Network Proposal. P.I.s: Robert Sherwood, Catherine Brown, Robert Goldstone, & Jonathan Plucker. January 2008. $4,997,821: Unfunded. Role: assisted in research and preparation of grant. CURRENT MEMBERSHIPS IN PROFESSIONAL ORGANIZATIONS National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) International Society for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning (ISSOTL) Human Anatomy and Physiology Society (HAPS) PROFESSIONAL SERVICE 2009 Conference Reviewer, National Association of Research in Science Teaching. 2009 Conference Reviewer, International Society for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning 2005-2009 Editorial Committee, Human Anatomy and Physiology Society Edited quarterly publications and review educational research submissions. 2006 Conference Reviewer, National Association of Research in Science Teaching. 2006 International Conference of the Learning Sciences Session Coordinator • Collaborated with conference directors in program planning and session organization. • Served as a liaison and session host for presenters. 2005-2006 Learning Sciences Student Representative. • Voiced student concerns at faculty meetings. • Facilitated communication between faculty and students. 2001 Student Director for Regional Science Olympiad Competition, Kirksville, MO. • Worked with professors and secondary teachers to organize and facilitate the competition for middle and high school students. • Developed and conducted a Life Science event in the competition. • Co-sponsored an event at the 2001 Missouri state-wide competition. COMMUNITY SERVICE 2008-present Global Women’s Gathering Co-organize weekly meetings for international women to help them practice English, learn about the community, and develop relationships. 2007 English Teaching, Tbilisi, Georgia, 1/07-8/07. • Taught children’s English courses at Tbilisi Evangelical Baptist Church and a local after-school program. • Provided advanced lessons and English conversation practice for adults. PROFESSIONAL REFERENCES Dr. Robert Sherwood, Professor of Science Education Ph.D. Advisor W.W. Wright Education Building Room 2070 Indiana University Bloomington, IN 47405 Phone : (812) 856-8154 Email: [email protected] Dr. Valarie Akerson, Associate Professor of Science Education Ph.D. committee member and research mentor Indiana University Dept. of Curriculum and Instruction 201 North Rose Avenue Bloomington, IN 47405 812-856-8140 Email: [email protected] Dr. Whitney Schlegel, Associate Professor of Biology Ph.D. committee member and research mentor Jordan Hall 300 Indiana University Bloomington, IN 47405 Phone: (812) 855-7116 Email: [email protected] Dr. Valerie Dean O’Loughlin, Associate Professor of Anatomy, Director of Undergraduate Human Anatomy Mentor in teaching Jordan Hall 105 Indiana University Bloomington, IN 47405 Phone: (812)855-7723 Email: [email protected]