Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre Fotografía de
Transcription
Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre Fotografía de
Teaching English through literature Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre Fotografía de Laura Pulido, Gimnasio Campestre 55 REVISI oN DE TEMA ó 56 TEACHING ENGLISH THROUGH LITERATURE: JOINT BOOK READING IN PRESCHOOL Marina Meladze Head of the English Department at Gimnasio Campestre Author’s e-mail:[email protected] Recieved: May 24th, 2012 Approved: June 24th, 2012 SUMMARY RESUMEN Nowadays there are several methods to teach English. Initially, all of them have the same goal: to make the learning effective and enjoyable. One of the most difficult issues to deal with in this process is the way to intertwine language arts, meaning the theoretical part, and the context, which deals with the communicative approach. The gap between to know about the language and to be able to use it is very ample. In order to narrow this gap, the method of teaching English through literature has proved to be very efficient. In this paper, the main methodologies of teaching English through literature in preschool - joint book reading – are presented and their achievements and limitations in order to be implemented at Gimnasio Campestre are discussed. Hoy en día existen varios métodos para enseñar inglés. Inicialmente, todos ellos tienen el mismo objetivo: hacer que el aprendizaje sea eficaz y agradable. Uno de los temas más difíciles de tratar en este proceso es la manera de entrelazar las artes del lenguaje, es decir, la parte teórica y el contexto, que se ocupa del enfoque comunicativo. La brecha entre el saber sobre el lenguaje y la capacidad para utilizarlo es muy amplia. Con el fin de reducir esta brecha, el método de enseñanza de inglés a través de la literatura ha demostrado ser muy eficaz. En este documento se presentan las principales metodologías de enseñanza de inglés a través de la literatura en preescolar - joint book reading - y se discuten sus logros y limitaciones a fin de ser implementadas en el Gimnasio Campestre. Key words: Literature, joint book reading, receptive learning and expressive learning, inferential skills, context clues. El Astrolabio Palabras Clave: literatura, lec- tura compartida, aprendizaje receptivo, aprendizaje expresi- vo, habilidades inferenciales, significado por contextos. 57 THEORETICAL BACKGROUND During the process of any type of learning and, especially, a foreign language learning process, one of the most important goals to achieve is to use what is learnt in practice. Because of this reason, the main concern of the language teachers is not only to teach the language itself, but to develop the students´ abilities of using the language for a variety of communicative purposes. There is an enormous gap between teaching about the language and communicating in the language. The biggest problem today is that many teachers believe teaching about the language will make the students capable of being able to use it spontaneously for communicative purposes. In order to narrow this gap between the theory and its application, the language classes and its content have to resemble the real social context. This awareness of the social context can be achieved using the world of literature which portrays the society with all its positive deeds and negative outcomes in a varied, authentic and natural language. Using literature as an imagination basis, the learner might Photo by Laura Pulido, Gimnasio Campestre encounter his world outside the classroom and make language acquisition more use-focused instead of form-focused. The term literature for the pre-school kids (Pre-k to 1st grades), because of their obvious skill development process and age limitation factors, can be defined as joint “story – reading” (aloud) and “storytelling” more than reading independently and comprehending what is read. Due to this fact, these two activities will be considered the foundation to build up and develop different language skills. Some authors have considered the book reading and storytelling situations as the major activity in which labelling and vocabulary development occurs in young children. For example Ninio (1989), studying the vocabulary acquisition, found that comprehension, production and imitation represent alternative forms of rehearsal, with imitation more likely to occur for less-learned but already comprehended topics. In a study with 3 to 4 year old children, Phillips and McNaughton (1990) found that during the story reading time both, the child and the adult were focused on the narrative and were trying to construct meaning (words and events) through questions and statements. During these moments children also made inferences about written information developing their thinking skills. In their study with kindergarten students, Eller, Pappas and Brown (1988) determined that the vocabulary and grammar rules were acquired when the books were read three times. Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre 58 Elley (1989) focused on the vocabulary acquisition steps through exposure to repeated story reading and storytelling. Two experiments with 5 and 6 year students provided the evidence that reading stories aloud to children was a significant source of vocabulary acquisition because new learning was relatively permanent and the students who scored low at the beginning of the study gained, at least, as many words as the students who had scored higher. Reading aloud and storytelling have got social-cognitive dimension, that cannot be overlooked. Aspects of the language are very successfully acquired by modelling procedures in an interactive and abstract processes relying on perception, representation, and production. This abstract comprehension of the language structure lets the students extract syntactic rules and generate almost infinite variety of new sentences they have never learned before. MAIN ASPECTS OF LANGUAGE LEARNING A) RECEPTIVE AND EXPRESSIVE LEARNING Researches about the relation between storybook reading and children language development are addressing two important aspects: 1) First of all, studies have been conducted to determine whether young children learn new words incidentally when they listen to the stories they are read or told about. Correlational studies have generally found that storybook reading is positively El Astrolabio associated with vocabulary development (Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994; Bus, van IJzendoorn & Pelligrini, 1995). 2) Second, many studies go beyond this limited conditions and research about whether children learn more when adults read to them in interactive manner. The assumption that interactive behaviors used by adults serve a didactic function and foster language acquisition has been supported by experimental evidence (e.g. Pemberton & Watkins, 1987; Eller, Pappas & Brown, 1988; Elley, 1989; Leung & Pikulski, 1990). According to these research results, two types of learning have to be taken into account when analyzing the main aspects of language learning: receptive and expressive languages. According to the Language Communication Theory, Receptive language is the comprehension of language - listening and understanding what is communicated. Expressive language refers to being able to produce speech and communicate a message. B) VOCABULARY Definition: There are many reasons for providing students with a deep instruction to build and widen vocabulary, but none is more important than the contribution vocabulary knowledge offers to language learning itself. New words are like bricks to construct the whole language development. 59 It´s very interesting to notice that just one single instructional model in vocabulary teaching is not enough for optimal vocabulary learning. Therefore effective instruction must use several methods to help students acquire new vocabulary and increase their knowledge about new words. Effective instruction includes opportunities for both, incidental word learning and intentional word teaching, and has to be directed to both, receptive and expressive language. (U.S.A. National Reading Panel, 2000). Incidental Word Learning Through Oral Language: Most of the word learning occurs incidentally when the student has experiences and approaches to the language. However, although this type of learning is called ‘incidental’, the process often reflects conscious choices the teachers make to talk about determined and organized/previously planned topics to expand the students’ vocabulary about a determined topic. Once the students start pre-school, the teacher talk they hear all day long, offers them great opportunities to get familiarized with the oral language that promotes the vocabulary growth. However, as shown in the last research mentioned, in most cases the vocabulary chosen by the teacher was limited to daily routine words and concrete experiences (“here and now”). To counteract these frequently reported patterns, an intervention called Paved for Success (phonological awareness and vocabulary enhancement) with classes of preschool children was implemented (Scgwanenflugel et al, 2001). The examination of this study determined that children in the classroom in which the teachers consistently involved children in interactive teacher-child talk and storybook reading ended up with larger vocabularies than did children who served as controls. Incidental Word Learning Trough Teacher Read – Aloud: Due to the fact children´s books often encompass extensive and descriptive language, reading them aloud to students can be an excellent way to focus their attention on words. So, It was not surprising at all that reading aloud children´s books was found to increase the students´ vocabulary, especially from pre-school through the elementary grades (Stahl, Richeck and Vandevier, 1991). However, we have to consider that reading aloud by itself is not enough to either shape vocabulary or to upsurge comprehension. To understand a story, students must relate their background knowledge to the ideas presented in the story. So, the real value of reading aloud activity for vocabulary growth lies not in the reading alone, but in the teacher-student talk that accompanies the reading because it endorses students’ information about new or rare words. Intentional, Explicit Information Beck et al. (2002) proposed that teachers should place major consideration on words’ usefulness and frequency of use. To help in this effort, these researchers categorized all the words present in a language words into three groups: Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre 60 •Group One consists of words the students are likely to know. For example: table, baby, or happy. •Group Two contains words that are “likely to appear frequently in a wide variety of texts.” (2002, p. 16), but whose meanings students do not know. For example: build or observe. •Group Three is setting the words that rarely appear in texts. (It is limited to one or two occurrences), and because they are often specific to particular content, students can use the context of texts to establish their meaning. For example: measure or differentiate. In the research already mentioned, Beck suggested that for instructional purposes, teachers should ignore Group One and Group Three words and concentrate exclusively on Group Two words. Their argument is that most students already know Group One words and that Group Three words should be taught at point of contact, or as they occur in reading. Group Two words, however, appear often in student texts, so they are the words that can add most to students’ language development. Teaching Specific Words Several researchers suggests different methods for teaching specific words related to specific texts as well as specific sets of words related to particular topics. For the purpose of this study, Graves (2000) will be taken into account. According to his studies the following are the types of words that have to be emphasized on and used to guarantee the efficient language development: El Astrolabio •Words that are synonyms for words that students already know. • Words that students know at some level, but that have multiple meanings. • Words that represent concepts that may be new to students. •Words that can be taught in context. Teaching Unknown Words: Synonyms Linking significant selection words to accustomed synonyms before students read can be an efficient and minimally disruptive way to help them get the most from reading. Teachers can provide this instruction economically by emphasizing on the target words and providing quick definitions that use synonyms students are likely to know. When teaching the language that contains similar words, cognates, the synonyms that sound alike in native language, could be used as well, especially with little children or beginners. For this purpose the word “sufficient” as the synonym of “enough” or the “valiant” as the synonym for “brave” could be used for the Spanish speaking students when learning English. Teachers also can use synonyms as part of point-of-contact teaching for particular words as they read aloud and not only before the reading. Teaching Multiple-Meaning Words Semantic maps can be operative resources to enlarge students’ information of words, with which they are already familiar, but which have multiple meaning. A semantic map is a graphic organizer that is prepared about a word that represents an important concept. On the 61 Photo by Laura Pulido, Gimnasio Campestre map, related words are gathered around the target word according to criteria that teachers or students choose. These criteria might include such structures as similar or dissimilar attributes, connotative or denotative meanings, or even shared linguistic components. Identifying and Using Context Clues Teaching Words for New and Complex Concepts C) LISTENING COMPREHENSION AND THE LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT One technique that is recommended to follow when teaching words for new words or concepts is asking students to identify critical attributes associated with a word (Frayer, Frederick, & Klausmeier, 1969). Teachers lead students in a discussion where they compare and contrast essential features and examples of a word or concept. Students can identify characteristics and examples for a concept after the discussion. This activity can be supported with a visual representation. Context clues are clues to the meaning of a word that are contained in the text and illustrations that surround it. Context clues can include definitions, examples, and restatements, as well as charts, pictures, and type features. How Might Book Reading Promote Language Acquisition Even More Efficiently? In this section of the work, the features of book reading interactions and how those interactions might be influencing language acquisition will be considered. Many researchers have analyzed conversations around book reading in order to identify the features or aspects of those conversations that effectively support children’s language development (e.g., Ninio, 1983; Ninio &Bruner, 1978; Snow &Ninio, 1986). Another topic analyzed Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre 62 will be the one about how each reading strategy might specifically support children’s language development. Non-Immediate Talk Non-immediate talk refers to the talk produced during the teacher-student interaction which goes beyond the information contained in text or illustrations to make predictions, connections to the child’s past experiences, other books, or the real world, to draw inferences, analyze information, or discuss the meaning of words and offer explanations. One important component of non-immediate talk is the discussion or explanation of vocabulary—for example, clarification on the word meaning. Non-immediate talk offers occasions for children to understand and use the more refined vocabulary required for expressing eva- Photo by Laura Pulido, Gimnasio Campestre El Astrolabio luative reactions to the book, discussing characters’ internal states, making predictions concerning the next episode, and so on. These types of talk inevitably introduce relatively complex language development. In classroom situations where teachers read to groups of children, it may appear to be more difficult to ask the openended questions characteristic of nonimmediate talk. It also seems possible that the benefit of non-immediate talk would be lost without the opportunity for individual verbal participation. However, in the classrooms of 4-year-olds in the School Study of Language and Literacy, the amount of non-immediate talk used by the teachers while reading to the group was strongly associated with the receptive vocabulary scores of our target children and the short sentence construction. (Beals et al.,1994). Dickinson and Smith (1994) identified the importance of a particular type of non-immediate talk during group book readings in the preschool classroom. It was, without any doubt, child-involved analytical talk, referring to interpretation, prediction, and vocabulary statements made by both the teacher and students. Four-year-olds, who were exposed to a high proportion of childinvolved analytic talk during group book reading in preschool, had higher kindergarten vocabulary scores even when controlling for total amount of book-related talk. Child-involved analytic talk, like non-immediate talk during book reading and like dialogic book reading talk, can be presumed to promote language development by presenting words in a rich semantic context and by promoting children’s use of novel lexical. 63 Dialogic Reading Whitehurst and his colleagues (Zeverbergen & Whitehurst, 2009) structured a method of reading aloud to young children in order to enrich their language development process called Dialogic Reading. Dialogic reading is based on three theoretical principles: encouraging the child to become an active learner during book reading (e.g., asking the child questions), providing feedback that employs more sophisticated language (e.g., expansions), and finally, challenging the child’s knowledge and skills by rising the conversation to a level just above their ability (e.g., asking about characteristics of an object for which they already know the label). In experimental studies, the preschoolaged children of the teachers who were trained to use dialogic reading techniques did, in fact, score higher on measures of expressive language than the children of untrained teachers (Whitehurst et al., 1988). Another interesting test to prove the positive effects of the dialogic reading Photo by Laura Pulido, Gimnasio Campestre was realized by Lim (1999). She worked with Korean-immigrant families whose children were losing their spoken Korean as the family language gradually changed to English. Lim taught dialogic reading techniques to the teachers and parents of these families in Korean, and provided Korean books for them to read with their children. The children in the experimental group showed significant improvements in their oral productive and receptive Korean skills, even though just the book reading sessions constituted their major source of exposure. The length of time that the teachers and parents spent during the Korean book reading was directly related to the improvements made by the children. The main reason of the fact dialogic reading can be related to the language improvement is that it is based on several language development principles. It provides richer semantic contexts for new words, it tends to last longer than just reading—thus giving children longer exposure to the book vocabulary, and promotes children’s use of new lexical items. Comprehender-Style Book Readings Teachers could be differentiate according to the styles they employ when reading to young children; there are teachers called “describers, ” who focus on description (similar to immediate talk, which may involve simply labeling or describing pictures), and there are the ones called “comprehenders, ” who focus on story meaning (similar to non-immediate talk, which may include inferences and interpretation of the story). These different styles are associated with children’s language skills. Children using the “comprehender” style had higher vocabulary Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre 64 scores than children whose teachers favored the “descriptor” style focusing on labels; both styles and child vocabulary differences remained consistent over the course of the longitudinal study (Haden, Reese, & Fivush, 1996). Reese and Cox (1999) researched about the effect of book reading style on preschoolers’ emergent language skills including vocabulary. After analyzing the results, they could determine three styles: describer, comprehender, and performance-oriented (in which the teacher discussed the story after the reading). Their results indicated that children’s initial language skills were necessarily to be taken into account in determining the effect of reading style on language growth. In general terms, children with lower initial language level showed more growth on the posttest if they were in the describer group, but the kids who had higher initial scores seemed to benefit most from the performance-oriented style of reading. Younger children, the ones who have to be more focused on acquiring nouns, benefit from the more noun-oriented describer style, however, linguistically more advanced children benefit from the discussion associated with a comprehension focus, during which they are exposed to more sophisticated language and have opportunities to use new words themselves. Final Considerations After analyzing the main theoretical approaches to the topic of this paper and evaluating the results of relevant studies on the theme, the following considerations could be stated: El Astrolabio The main steps to be followed when teaching English through literature in preschool are based on the internationally accepted language skills and can be used to develop the four competences Gimnasio Campestre has as its cornerstones: Information Management, Critical Thinking, Creativity and Metacognition. There are some limitations, though, in terms of current researches because there are not many studies done specifically for the Spanish speaking pre-school students language acquisition process when it is based on joint book reading. It would be the main objective of the second part of this work to design a study to be applied to the Gimnasio Campestre pre-school students taking into account the theoretical bases structured in this work and the schools pedagogical model and, based on this study, make the necessary changes to the method being applied nowadays. REFERENCES Anderson, R. C., & Shifrin, Z. (1980). The meaning of words in context. In R. J. Spiro, B. C., Bruce & W. F Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbawm Associates. Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G. (1982). The effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, Volume 74 Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Omanson, R. C. (1987). The effects and uses of diverse vocabulary instructional techniques. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisitions. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbawm Associates. Beals, D., DeTemple, J M, & Dickinson, D K (1994). Talking and Listening that support Early Literacy Development of Children from Low-Income Familes in Bridges to Literacy: Children, Families and Schools David K Dickinson (ed.) Oxford. Blackwell. 65 Bus, A.G., Ijzendoorn van, M.H., & Pellegrini, A.D. Joint Book Reading Makes Success in Learning to Read: A Meta-Analysis on Intergenerational Transmission of Literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65(1),1-21 Dickinson, D. K., & Smith, M. W. (1994). Long-term effects of preschool teachers’ book readings on low-income children’s vocabulary and story comprehension. In Reading Research Quarterly, 29 (2), 104-122. Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P.O. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school, and literacy achievement at age five. In S. Barnett (Chair), The social prerequisites of literacy development. Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL. Eller, R., Pappas, C.. (1998) Lexical Development of Kindergarteners: Learning from written context. Journal of Literary Research. Volume XX No.1 Elley W. (1989) Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research Quarterly Spring 1989. XXIV/2 Frayer, D. A. Frederick, W. C. & Klausmeier, H. J. (1969). A Schema for Testing the Level of Cognitive Mastery. Madison, WI: Wisconsin Center for Education Research. Graves, M. F. (2000). Raising Readers and Writers. The Reading Teacher Journal (39) 920-930 Lieberman, J. E. (1967). The effects of direct instruction in vocabulary concepts on reading achievement. In ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 010 985. Ninio, A. (1989). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Development Psychology, (19), de http://micro5.mscc.huji. ac.il/~msninio/Joint-bookreading1983.pdf. Pappas, C. C., & Brown, E. (1987). Young children learning story discourse: Three case studies. Elementary School Journal, (87). Journal of Research in Childhood Education, (10), 37 – 48. Scarborough , H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, (14), 245 – 302. Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E., Kuhn, M. R., (2011). Aligning Theory and Assessment of Reading Fluency: Automaticity. Prosody and Definitions of Fluency. Reading Research Quarterly,(45), 230–251. Stahl, Richeck and Vandevier, (1991).The Cornerstones of Reading Comprehension: Teaching for Vocabulary and Text Understanding. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72-110. Tizard, B., Hughes, M., Carmichael H., and Pinkerton, G. (1980) Four year olds talking to mothers and teachers. In Monograph Supplement no.2, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Language and Language Disorders in Childhood, L.A.Herzov. Vivas Eleonora (2001) Effects of story reading on language. Language Learning. Research in Language Studies Journal. Volume 46, (2). Wells, G. (1985a). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance & A Hildyard. (Eds) Literacy, language and learning: The consequences of reading and writing. (229-253) Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Wells, G. (1985b). Language development in the preschool years. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Wells, G. (1986). The meaning makers: Children learning language and using language to learn. Cambridge, UK: Press Syndicate Cambridge University. Zevenbergen, A.A. & Whitehurst, G.J. (2003). Dialogic reading: A shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In A. Van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E.B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and teachers (177-200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Copyright 2003 by Lawrence Erlbawm Associates, Inc. Adapted by permission. Pemberton & Watkins. (1987). The importance of recasts in a child’s early linguistic environment. Reading Research Quarterly, (25) U.S.A. National Reading Panel 2000. Reese and Cox (1999) Quality of adult book reading affects children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, (35), 20-28. Snow, C. E., Tabors, P. O., Nicholson, P., & Kurland, B. (1994). SHELL: Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first grade children. Investigación y Ciencia del Gimnasio Campestre