insights into linguistics
Transcription
insights into linguistics
INSIGHTS INTO LINGUISTICS Class 3 Universidad Laica Vicente Rocafuerte School of Languages Name: _________________________ Phone: _________________________ e-mail: _________________________ Universidad Laica Vicente Rocafuerte English Language School Rebeca Vera, M.Ed. INSIGHTS INTO LINGUISTICS BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Allen, J. P. B. and Corder, S. Pit (1974). Techniques in Applied Linguistics. The Edinburgh Course in Applied Linguistics, Volume 3. UK: Oxford University Press. 2. Fromkin, Victoria, Rodman, Robert & Hyams, Nina (2003). An Introduction to Language. Seventh Edition. Boston, USA: Heinle, Boston 3. Ipsen, Guido (1997). 4. Napoli, Donna Jo (1996). Linguistics. UK: Oxford University Press. 5. Radford, Andrew (1981). Transformational Syntax. A Student’s Guide to Chomsky’s Extended Standard Theory. UK: Cambridge University Press. 6. Radford, Andrew et al. (2005). Linguistics: An Introduction. UK: Cambridge University Press. 7. Richards, Jack and Schmidt, Richard. (2002). Dictionary of Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics. UK: Longman. 8. Thornbury, Scott (1997). About Language. UK: Cambridge University Press, UK. 9. Wallwork, J. F. (1989). Language and Linguistics. An Introduction to the Study of Language. UK: Heinemann Educational Books. Linguistics for Beginners. Germany: University of Kassel. 10. Widdowson, H. G. (1996). Linguistics. UK: Oxford University Press. Journals: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Journal of Linguistics published by Cambridge University Press. (http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=LIN) The Linguistics Journal published by the Linguistics Journal Press. British Virgin Islands. (http://www.linguistics-journal.com/) SKY Journal of Linguistics published by the Linguistic Association of Finland. (http://www.linguistics.fi/skyjol-en.shtml) TESOL Quarterly. USA. Journal of English Linguistics published by SAGE Publications. (http://eng.sagepub.com/content/37/3.toc) Northwest Journal of Linguistics. (http://www.sfu.ca/nwjl/contact.html) Webs: http://www.ling.udel.edu/idsardi/101/ http://www.ling.udel.edu/ling/ http://www.uni-kassel.de/fb8/misc/lfb/html/text/startlfbframeset.html http://www.uni-kassel.de/fb8/misc/lfb/html/navigation/navigate.html http://www.geocities.com/CollegePark/3920/ http://www.mathpsyc.uni-bonn.de/doc/albert/node7.html http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/ASC/COMPET_PERFO.html http://changingminds.org/explanations/critical_theory/concepts/langue_parole.htm http://www.ling.udel.edu/idsardi/101/syll.html http://linguistlist.org/issues/ http://feminafelis.tripod.com/tgl.html Universidad Laica Vicente Rocafuerte de Guayaquil English Language School Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 1 INSIGHTS INTO LINGUISTICS TASK 1. COURSE OBJECTIVE AND EVALUATION PROCEDURES Objectives (a) To clarify objectives and evaluation procedures. (b) To negotiate changes to these if necessary. (c) To give CPs the opportunity to experiment a ‘pyramiding’ activity. Input: (a) Course objectives (b) Evaluation procedures (c) Process language expressions for task Interaction pattern: (a) Pyramiding Tutor’s role: (a) language model (b) Plenary (b) facilitator Activity: Study the course objectives and evaluation procedures (a) individually for 5 minutes, (b) in pairs for 5 minutes, (c) in groups for 10 minutes. Finally discuss these in plenary session with tutor. PROVISIONAL COURSE OBJECTIVES By the end of the course participants should be able to: (a) recognize the diversity of language systems and their fundamental similarities. (b) use the basic terminology of linguistic subfields, including phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax and semantics. (c) use linguistic analysis to investigate the organizing principles of language. (d) solve phonetic, phonology and morphology problems. (e) demonstrate proficiency in constructing syntactic trees. COURSE EVALUATION PROCEDURE The course itself will be evaluated through the following: 1. On-line students’ feedback. 2. Value and achievement of mutually agreed objectives as determined by end-of-course questionnaire. 3. Suggestions for improvement of the course from nominal group technique session at end of course. COURSE PARTICIPANT EVALUATION Below is a suggestion for evaluation the CPs (CP evaluation will be negotiable at the beginning and during the course). An end-of-course evaluation report based on the seven criteria below: 1. Attendance. 2. Level of involvement in class, group work and on-line participation 3. Effort put into out-of-class activities which CPs commit themselves to. 4. Performance on end-of-chapter test. 5. Performance on end-of-term examination. 6. Performance on end-of-course examination. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 2 SESSION DISCUSSION LANGUAGE SUMMARY To all participants: Try to use the following expressions as much as possible when you discuss the various tasks with your tutor or in groups. GENERAL What do we have to do? We have to look for/find (hay que) Where are we? We’re here on line … Which one? This / that one. Where is it? I can’t find it. Oh yeah. Look! It’s here on line … It isn’t here. Here it is. What does it/he/she say? It says here / on line … What does “….” mean, do you think? I think it means … Wait a minute Just a second Hang on a moment Sorry? Pardon? Excuse me? I know. I have it. of course. I’ve got it. I see / understand. I mean (digo) What did you say? I didn’t hear you. I didn’t catch you. OPINIONS What do you think (number one is)? What about you? (y Ud.?) I think it’s … I think so, too. / me, too. Do you agree? Yes, I think that’s right. Well, no. I think that’s wrong. Well, no. I think you’re wrong. Well, … (pues, bueno) I don’t agree, (either). know understand. Me, neither. Have you finished? We’ve finished. It’s already here / We’ve already done it. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 3 GROUP COORDINATOR, ETC. First, … OK Let’s do look at Right Now Shall we letter A. Let’s see now. answer read What about …? question 2? the first one. Why don’t we study the next last other one? What do you have / what have you got? Do we agree? Let’s put write mark underline So, … (entonces) (así) that, then. DOUBT AND CERTAINTY It could be …. (puede ser) I’m not sure. maybe / possibly / perhaps It must be …. (debe ser) has to I’m sure / certain It can’t be. (no puede ser) The only word thing I can think of is … GETTING MORE INFORMATION Could you tell me a bit more about …? Sorry, but I’d like to know some more about … I didn’t quite follow what you said about …. Sorry, that’s not quite what I meant. What I really wanted to know was …. Sorry, I don’t quite understand why …. DISCUSSING Expressing opinions in a more tentative way: I sometimes think that …. Would you agree that …? Well, I’ve heard that …. Do you think it’s right to say that …? Rephrasing your own statements: That’s not quite what I meant… Sorry, let me explain… Let me put it another way … Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 4 Welcome to Linguistics for Beginners The study of linguistics is a vast field. Do not expect to learn everything while going through this course the first time. And do not feel linguistically dwarfish if you find that there are many questions that will remain unanswered at the end of it. This is an introductory course! You will not only find information using this introduction. You can actually study linguistics by filling in the questionnaires that accompany the various chapters. Suggestions for further reading will always be listed alongside the chapters. The contents of this introduction will cover the core knowledge of what is relevant and (together with some ingenuity on your part) should suffice for you to be able to give detailed answers to the questions. In addition, however, you should take your time to look up some of the additional literature. In the bibliography you will also find suggestions for further reading that may help you in the future, when you need more detailed information. Thus, this introduction should still be of use for your studies in the future. Learning Techniques There are many different ways to learn, so we will try to experience as many different ways of learning as possible. Some people are still most comfortable with lectures and examples, so we will have lectures and discussion each week. Other people learn better when discussing problems together in groups, so we will do that too. There are lecture notes. We will be intermixing the "theoretical" material with the "applied" material. This means that the readings will come from more than one chapter or set of notes. At the end of each unit you have to do the worksheet, which will be useful material for tests. If you have a question at any time, please ask it. Other people will benefit too. If you want to ask a question by email, that's great too. My e-mail is: [email protected]. Grading Exam (50%) Tests, project/homework, class and on-line participation and attendance 50% Some tests will include short answer and multiple choice questions. Test questions will include language analysis problems similar to the homework problems. Do the readings before coming to class. Projects are due on the day indicated on the schedule. Homework must be handed in on time, otherwise you will receive a zero (assignments may be handed in early, too. You can send them by mail if you are unable to come to class). Class attendance and participation are essential -- IF you do not show up THEN you will not do well! Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 5 UNIT 1 LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTICS Talking, shouting, whispering, lying, swearing, telling jokes or tales, in short: communication of all sorts by means of articulate sound is something we are so familiar with that we hardly ever come to think about it as something unique. However, no other creature on this planet shows the ability to communicate verbally in the way we do. Take a minute to think about the immense impact spoken and written language has on your everyday life! You could not possibly do without it in situations where you meet other people, like in school, university, or at the breakfast table. The examples are innumerous. In this course, we will take a look at the unique features of human language. As you will see when we proceed, the human curiosity concerning language is no modern phenomenon. Language has been examined by linguists and philosophers for several millennia. Therefore, we can look back on a respectable stock of literature on the topic originating from the times of Ancient Greece until the present day. The result is a compendium of linguistic disciplines that are interwoven with the domains of, among others, philosophy, psychology, neurology, and even computer science: a vast and fascinating network of knowledge. To keep you fascinated (which I hope you are) and to keep you from becoming intimidated (which I hope you are not), we will start right away with the very principles that make human language so special. 1.1. WHAT IS HUMAN LANGUAGE? Language is a highly elaborated signaling system. We call the aspects that are peculiar to it the design features of language. Some of these we find only with the language of human beings, others we have in common with animals. Another aspect of human language is that we express thoughts with words. 1.1.1. Design features of language A principal feature of human language is the duality of patterning. It enables us to use our language in a very economic way for a virtually infinite production of linguistic units. How does this principle work? All human languages have a small, limited set of speech sounds. The limitation derives from the restricted capacity of our vocal apparatus. The speech sounds are referred to as consonants and vowels. Linguistically speaking, the distinctive speech sounds are called phonemes, which are explained in more detail in the chapter on phonology. You cannot use isolated phonemes for communication, because phonemes are by themselves meaningless. But we can assemble and reassemble phonemes into larger linguistic units. These are commonly called “words”. Although our capacity to produce new phonemes is limited, we frequently coin new words. Hence, our capacity to produce vocabulary is unlimited. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 6 Displacement In contrast to other animals, humans have a sense of the past and the future. A gorilla, for example, cannot tell his fellows about his parents, his adventures in the jungle, or his experience of the past. The use of language to talk about things other than “the here and now,” is a characteristic of humans. Displacement is thus our ability to convey a meaning that transcends the immediately perceptible sphere of space and time. Although some animals seem to possess abilities appropriating those of displacement, they lack the freedom to apply this to new contexts. The dance of the honey-bee, for instance, indicates the location of rich deposits of food to other bees. This ability of the bee corresponds to displacement in human language, except for a lack of variation. The bee frequently repeats the same pattern in its dance, whereas humans are able to invent ever new contexts. Open-endedness The ability to say things that have never been said before, including the possibility to express invented things or lies, is also a peculiar feature of human language. Stimulus-freedom is another aspect that distinguishes human language from animal communication. The honey-bee must perform its dance, the woodchuck must cry out in order to warn his fellows when it beholds an eagle. Humans have the ability to say anything they like in any context. This ability is only restricted in certain ceremonial contexts such as church services, etc., where a fixed form is expected to be followed. The possibility to violate this fixed linguistic behaviour is then the source of jokes, such as a bride’s “no”. Arbitrariness Why is a table called “table”? Obviously, the thing never told us its name. And tables do not make a noise similar to the word. The same applies to most of the words of our language. Hence, words and their meanings have no a priori connection. We cannot tell from the sound structure which meaning is behind it. Language is not motivated, as we can also put it. There are, however, exceptions to this rule: language can be iconic, which means that there is a direct correlation between form and meaning. The length of a phrase, for example, could represent a length of time the phrase refers to, like in “a long, long time ago.” Here, the extension serves to visually represent the semantic emphasis. Iconicity in language can be found frequently. This is seen in more detail on semiotics. Another example for non-arbitrariness is onomatopoeia. These are words that seem to resemble sounds. There are many examples for onomatopoetic words, like splash or bang. Some names for animals are also onomatopoetic, for example, “cuckoo”. Still, since animals such as the bird are named differently in different languages, there can be no ultimate motivation for the name. The human vocal tract An elaborated language requires a highly sophisticated speech organ that will enable the speaker to produce the many differentiated sounds. Only humans are endowed with a speech organ of this complexity. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 7 1.2. WHAT IS LINGUISTICS? Linguistics is the scientific study of language. It endeavours to answer the question--what is language and how is represented in the mind? Linguists focus on describing and explaining language and are not concerned with the prescriptive rules of the language (ie., do not split infinitives). Linguists are not required to know many languages and linguists are not interpreters. The underlying goal of the linguist is to try to discover the universals concerning language. That is, what are the common elements of all languages. The linguist then tries to place these elements in a theoretical framework that will describe all languages and also predict what can not occur in a language. Linguistics is a social science that shares common ground with other social sciences such as psychology, anthropology, sociology and archaeology. It also may influence other disciplines such as English, communication studies and computer science. Linguistics for the most part though can be considered a cognitive science. Along with psychology, philosophy and computer science, linguistics is ultimately concerned with how the human brain functions. Linguistics is the scientific inquiry into the human language with all its aspects. All its aspects: these are many. There is a specialized branch for each approach to the examination of language. Until the beginning of the 20th century, scholars were occupied with research on the history of languages and the roots of words in ancient tongues. The famous linguist Ferdinand de Saussure coined this approach the diachronic analysis and moved to the analysis of the system of language, which he assumed to be of greater importance. Saussure stated this in the first decades of this century and thus formed the fundament of modern linguistics. 1.2.1. Diachronic versus synchronic view Diachrony Diachronic linguistics views the historical development of a language. Thus, on the diachronic axis we can go back and forth in time, watching the language with all its features change. Synchrony Synchronic linguistics views a particular state of a language at some given point in time. This could mean Modern English of the present day, or the systematic analysis of the system of Shakespeare’s English. However, no comparisons are made to other states of language or other times. Modern linguistics, following Ferdinand de Saussure, is primarily interested in the synchronic point of view. Saussure postulated the priority of synchrony: no knowledge of the historical development of a language is necessary to examine its present system. He arrived at this radical viewpoint due to his conviction that linguistic research must concentrate on the structure of language. Later, the whole paradigm was hence called structuralism. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 8 1.2.2. The two axes of the synchronic view When we look at the structure of language, we find sentences and words. This is, however, a very rough view. A grammar of a language must be more precise. One axis of the synchronic view is syntagmatic analysis. Here we examine the relationships of all elements of a sentence to one another. We ask ourselves exactly what element appears where and under which condition in a sentence. For example, where do nouns appear? Where are auxiliary verbs applied? All word classes show certain syntagmatic relationships. They can be defined by distribution analysis, a method that classifies elements according to their appearance within the logical order of a sentence. Let's have a look at an example: A + ______ + crosses + the + street Obviously, a noun must appear in the blank space, for example: A woman crosses the street. Of course, nouns and verbs are not all the same. They do not fit into contexts freely. Hence we apply paradigmatic analysis. In our example, the idea of a sandwich crossing the street is impossible. As you can see, the elements of language obviously evince paradigmatic relationships. Elements can be substituted by others of the same paradigmatic class, such as street, lane, road, etc. Articles can also be exchanged. Words that belong to the same paradigmatic class thus belong to the same grammatical class. They also belong to the same lexical field. The following diagram shows the two axes of synchronic analysis: 1.2.3. Langue and parole One such mark was made by Ferdinand de Saussure, the Swiss scholar usually credited with establishing the principles of modern linguistics. In a celebrated series of lectures in the early part of the century, he proposed that linguistics should concern itself with the shared social code, the abstract system, which he called langue, leaving aside the particular actualities of individual utterance, which he called parole. (Widdowson 1996: 21). Langue and parole are more than just 'language and speech' (although this is a useful quick way of remembering them). Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 9 Langue La langue is the whole system of language that precedes and makes speech possible. A sign is a basic unit of langue. Learning a language, we master the system of grammar, spelling, syntax and punctuation. These are all elements of langue. Langue is a system in that it has a large number of elements whereby meaning is created in the arrangements of its elements and the consequent relationships between these arranged elements. Parole Parole is the concrete use of the language, the actual utterances. It is an external manifestation of langue. It is the usage of the system, but not the system. By defining Langue and Parole, Saussure differentiates between the language and how it is used, and therefore enabling these two very different things to be studied as separate entities. As a structuralist, Saussure was interested more in la langue than parole. It was the system by which meaning could be created that was of interest rather than individual instances of its use. 1.2.4. Competence and performance A comparable distinction to that of Saussure, designed to idealize language data, and to define the scope of linguistic enquiry, is made by Noam Chomsky. He distinguishes competence, the knowledge that native speakers have of their language as a system of abstract formal relations, and performance, their actual behaviour. (Widdowson, 1996: 24). A distinction introduced by Chomsky into linguistic theory but of wider application. Competence refers to a speaker's knowledge of his language as manifest in his ability to produce and to understand a theoretically infinite number of sentences most of which he may have never seen or heard before. Performance refers to the specific utterances, including grammatical mistakes and non-linguistic features like hesitations, accompanying the use of language. (http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/ASC/COMPET_PERFO.html) 1.2.5. The various linguistic disciplines: Survey Below are several different disciplines within linguistics. The fields of phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics and language acquisition are considered the core fields of study and a firm knowledge of each is necessary in order to tackle more advanced subjects. Phonetics Phonetics is the study of the production and perception of speech sounds. It is concerned with the sounds of language, how these sounds are articulated and how the hearer perceives them. Phonetics is related to the science of acoustics in that it uses much the same techniques in the analysis of sound that acoustics does. There are three sub-disciplines of phonetics: Articulatory Phonetics: the production of speech sounds. Acoustic Phonetics: the study of the physical production and transmission of speech sounds. Auditory Phonetics: the study of the perception of speech sounds. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 10 Phonology Phonology is the study of the sound patterns of language. It is concerned with how sounds are organized in a language. Phonology examines what occurs to speech sounds when they are combined to form a word and how these speech sounds interact with each other. It endeavors to explain what these phonological processes are in terms of formal rules. Morphology Morphology is the study of word formation and structure. It studies how words are put together from their smaller parts and the rules governing this process. The elements that are combining to form words are called morphemes. A morpheme is the smallest unit of meaning you can have in a language. The word cats, for example, contains the morphemes cat and the plural -s. Syntax Syntax is the study of sentence structure. It attempts to describe what is grammatical in a particular language in term of rules. These rules detail an underlying structure and a transformational process. The underlying structure of English for example would have a subjectverb-object sentence order (John hit the ball). The transformational process would allow an alteration of the word order which could give you something like The ball was hit by John. Semantics Semantics is the study of meaning. It is concerned with describing how we represent the meaning of a word in our mind and how we use this representation in constructing sentences. Semantics is based largely on the study of logic in philosophy. Language Acquisition Language acquisition examines how children learn to speak and how adults learn a second language. Language acquisition is very important because it gives us insight in the underlying processes of language. There are two components which contribute to language acquisition. The innate knowledge of the learner (called Universal Grammar or UG) and the environment. The notion of UG has broad implications. It suggests that all languages operate within the same framework and the understanding of this framework would contribute greatly to the understanding of what language is. Other Disciplines: Sociolinguistics: Sociolinguistics is the study of interrelationships of language and social structure, linguistic variation, and attitudes toward language. Neurolinguistics: Neurolinguistics is the study of the brain and how it functions in the production, perception and acquisition of language. Historical Linguistics: Historical linguistics is the study of language change and the relationships of languages to each other. Anthropological Linguistics: Anthropological linguistics is the study of language and culture and how they interact. Pragmatics: Pragmatics studies meaning in context. Semiotics: Semiotics is the study of signs in communication processes in general. It concerns itself with the analysis of both linguistic and non-linguistic signs as communicative devices and with their systems Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 11 Learning Notes UNIT 1 LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTICS Communication By means of articulate sounds e.g. talking, shouting, whispering, lying, swearing, telling jokes or tales Only human beings show the ability to communicate verbally WHAT IS HUMAN LANGUAGE? It is a highly elaborated signaling system. (Study the concept in the copies) Design features of language 1. Duality of patterning: - It enables us to use our language in a very economic way for a virtually infinite production of linguistic units - All human beings have a small limited set of speech sounds: vowels and consonants. - Phonemes are distinctive speech sounds and are by themselves meaningless. - Words are larger linguistic units. - Limited capacity to produce new phonemes derived from the restricted capacity of our vocal apparatus. - Unlimited capacity to produce vocabulary. 2. Displacement: - It is a sense of past and future. eg. gorilla cannot talk about his adventures in the jungle or experience of the past. - Only humans can talk about things other than “the here and now” - It is our ability to convey a meaning beyond the sphere of space and time. - Animals lack of freedom to apply this to new contexts. eg. the dance of the honey-bee: the bee repeats the same pattern of its dance. 3. Open-endedness: - It is the ability to say things that have never been said before, including the possibility to express invented things or lies is also a peculiar feature of human language. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insight into Linguistics 12 4. Stimulus-freedom: - Humans have the ability to say anything they like in any context. Only restricted in certain ceremonial contexts. e.g. church services, etc. 5. Arbitrariness: - Things never told us its name. Names of things are arbitrary. - Exceptions: iconic language: direct correlation between form and meaning. e.g. long long time ago (length of a phrase) onomatopoeia: words that seem to resemble sounds. eg. splash, bang, names for animals: cuckoo. 6. Human vocal track: - An elaborated language requires a highly sophisticated speech organ that enables the speaker to produce the many differentiated sounds. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 12 Learning Notes WHAT IS LINGUISTICS? - It is the scientific study of language. - Linguists focus on describing and explaining language and are not concerned with the prescriptive rules of the language. - Goal: to try to discover the universals concerning language. Universals are common elements of all languages. Diachronic versus synchronic view Linguistics views are Dyachronic Synchronic It views the historical development of a language. We can go back and forth in time It views a particular state of a language at some given point in time Two axis are Syntagmatic analysis Paradigmatic analysis The relationships of all elements of a sentence to one another. e.g. a noun comes after the article a Elements can be substituted by others of the same paradigmatic class: book, pen Langue and parole: Study from the copies. Competence and performance: Study from the copies. The various linguistic disciplines: Survey: Study from the copies. 1. Phonetics Linguistic Disciplines a. Articulatory Phonetics b. Acoustic Phonetics c. Auditory Phonetics 2. Phonology 3. Morphology 4. Syntax 5. Semantics 6. Language Acquisition a. Sociolinguistics b. Neurolinguistics 7. Other disciplines c. Historical Linguistics d. Anthropological Linguistics e. Pragmatics f. Semiotics Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 13 UNIT 2 LANGUAGE UNIVERSALS Nearly five thousand languages are spoken in the world today. They seem to be quite different, but still, many of them show similar principles, such as word order. For example, in languages such as English, French, and Italian, the words of the clause take the order of first the subject, then the verb, and then the direct object. There even exist basic patterns or principles that are shared by all languages. These patterns are called universals. When the same principles are shared by several languages, we speak of language types. There are several examples for universals. 2.1. SEMANTIC UNIVERSALS There are semantic categories that are shared by all cultures and referred to by all languages - these are called semantic universals. There are many examples of semantic universals. Let's discuss two of them: One semantic universal regards our notion of colour. There exist eleven basic colour terms: black, white, red, green, blue, yellow, brown, purple, pink, orange, and grey. The pattern that all languages universally abide by, is that they do not entertain a notion of a colour term outside of that range. This means, any imaginable color is conceived of as a mixture, shade, or subcategory of one of these eleven basic color terms. As a result, one way of classifying languages is by colour terms. The eleven colour terms are not in usage equally among the languages on Earth. Not all languages have all basic color terms. Some have two, some three, and some four. Others have five, six, or seven, and some have eight to eleven. Those with two colour terms always have black and white, those with three black, white, and red, and those with more have additional basic colour terms according to the order in the list given above. This is a universal pattern. The languages which have the same basic color terms in common belong to the same language type. Hence, we find seven classes of languages according to this scheme. Another semantic universal is the case of pronouns. Think of what it is you do when you talk to someone about yourself. There is always the "I", representing you as the speaker, and the "you", meaning the addressee. You could not possibly do without that, and neither could a speaker of any other language on earth. Again, we find a universal pattern here. Whenever you do not talk about yourself as a person, but as a member of a group, you use the plural "we". English is restricted to these two classes of pronouns: singular and plural, each in the first, second, and third person. All languages that evince this structure are grouped into one language type. There are other languages that make use of even more pronouns. In some languages, it is possible to address two people with a pronoun that specifically indicates not just their being plural, but also their being 'two' people; this is then the dual pronoun. Other examples are languages that have pronouns to refer to the speaker and the addressee together, called inclusive pronouns. Exclusive pronouns refer to the speaker together with people other than the addressee. However, these are not among the European languages. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 14 2.2. PHONOLOGICAL UNIVERSALS Different languages may have very different sets of vowels. If you are familiar with a few foreign languages, you may find it difficult to believe there are universal rules governing the distribution of vowels, but they do exist. Remember our example of basic color terms: A similar pattern could be drawn on the basis of the vowel system. Languages with few vowels always have the same set of vowel types. And if a language has more vowels, it is always the same type of vowel that is added to the set. These vowels may not always sound exactly the same, but they are always created at the same location in our vocal apparatus. 2.3. SYNTACTIC UNIVERSALS Remember the word order of English I mentioned above. Hmhm, you say: that cannot be a universal rule, since you know other sentences from English and possibly from other languages which do not follow this order. You are right, but the order subject, verb, object (SVO) may be defined as the basic order of English sentences. In other languages there are different "basic" orders, such as Japanese (SOV) or Tongan (VSO), a Polynesian language. After an extensive study, one can define two different sets of basic orders that languages follow: First SVO, VSO, SOV and second VOS, OVS, OSV. What is the difference? In the first set the subject precedes the object, in the second set it follows the object. Since the first set is the one which applies to the basic structures of far more languages than the second one does, the universal rule is that there is an overwhelming tendency for the subject of a sentence to precede the direct object among the languages of the world. 2.3. ABSOLUTE UNIVERSALS - universal tendencies; implicational, nonimplicational universals Of course, not all universals can be found in all languages. With so many tongues spoken, it would be hard not to find any exceptions. Most languages have not even been the subject of extensive research as of yet. However, some rules appear without exception in the languages which have been studied so far. We call these absolute universals. If there are minor exceptions to the rule, we speak of universal tendencies or relative universals. In saying this, we take for granted that exceptions may be found in future surveys among languages which have remained unexplored up to the present day. Sometimes a universal holds only if a particular condition of the language structure is fulfilled. These universals are called implicational. Universals which can be stated without a condition are called nonimplicational. In other words, whenever a rule "If ... then ..." is valid, the universal appears in the structure of the respective language. There are thus four types of universals: implicational absolute universals, implicational relative universals, nonimplicational absolute universals, and nonimplicational relative universals. The final determination of which type a universal belongs to is dependent on intensive field research. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 15 WORKSHEET 1 – ON LINGUISTICS AND UNIVERSALS Please check your answers carefully. Name: ___________________________ E-mail: ______________________ Question Name three design Features of language Solution Points 3 Find the core definition for diachronic linguistics in the handout. It is a one-sentence definition. Copy the sentence 1 "The subject is usually followed by the verb, then comes the object." Is this an example for syntagmatic or paradigmatic analysis? paradigmatic syntagmatic 1 a) The human language is the only signaling system for communication purposes we know. True False b) Universals are features that ALL languages have in common. True False True or False? 3 c) Linguistics deals with spoken language only. True False Parallel Saussure’s distinction between “langue” and “parole” and Chomsky’s distinction between “competence” and “performance”. 2 Learning Notes UNIT 2 LANGUAGE UNIVERSALS LANGUAGE UNIVERSALS Semantic Notion of colour Phonological Pronouns Syntactic Absolute Word order - Absolute: rules - Relative: exceptions - Implicational - Nonimplicational Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 16 UNIT 3 THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH I: OLD ENGLISH Looking at a living language, one of the most interesting aspects is language change. All languages, except for the extinct ones, change permanently. Usually we do not notice the change that takes place during our own time because it happens quite slowly. But if we take a look back over a considerable span of time, language change becomes more obvious. Of course there were no textbooks in the beginnings of language, but fortunately linguists have developed certain methods to trace back words even beyond earliest records. Thus we have knowledge not only of the last 1500 years of English. We can even make an assumption about the very roots of the language. English is an Indo-European language. Indo-European was discovered to be the parent language of most European, Anterior-Asian, and Indian languages. As a rule, according to prototypical features of some of these languages, two main branches are defined in the IndoEuropean language tree, namely an eastern branch and a western branch. However, scholars have disputes about where the divisions within the Indo-European language family are to be placed. For example, in examining languages other than the prototypical, it has been found that not all languages can be classified into one of the two main branches, the eastern and western branches, of language families. How do we recover features of languages which are so old that no speakers live to tell us about them? Historical linguistics deduces that an abundant occurrence of features in a daughter language, the presence of which cannot be explained by language universals or by the assumption of them having been borrowed, or adopted, from another tongue, is likely to have been inherited from the parent language. Thus, by inferring from widespread phenomena on a mother tongue from which these phenomena came, linguists trace back languages. In Indo-European languages, for example, obvious correlations can be found. The Latin (L.) and Sanskrit (S.) words for “hundred”, namely L. “centum” and S “satem”, can be traced back to a common root. Since these two languages were considered to be the most prominent examples for the respective branches, the whole branches were named after them. Also, former scholars believed that they should make judgements about the various languages. Sanskrit, Latin, and Greek were commonly believed to be of a higher quality than the modern languages. Often scholars argued that these languages were more "pure" and praised their "perfection" and "clarity". Today we consider such notions to be outdated. There is no room in linguistics for the approval or disapproval of a language. If we look for the origin of a word, we call this the word's etymology (etymon = Greek for "root"). Within the Indo-European family tree and among the centum languages, we find language families like the Germanic, Celtic, or Latin families. Some authors refer to the early Germanic language as "Proto-Germanic". The Germanic language family is again split up in the West-, East-, and North-Germanic groups. While the Scandinavian tongues derived from the North-Germanic language group, Anglo-Frisian and Modern German came from the WestGermanic group. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 17 In the case of English, interaction with other languages was very important during its history, as we will see. Hence, many influences from foreign sources can be found in Modern English, while the family tree does not suggest these interchanges to have occurred. 3.1. LANGUAGES IN BRITAIN BEFORE ENGLISH 3.1.1. Celtic Languages The first culture in England of which we have definite knowledge is the Celtic culture and language. It is assumed that the coming of the Celts to England coincided with the introduction of bronze on the island. There were - and still are - Celtic tongues spoken on the British isles. Celtic Languages in Britain are Welsh, Cornish, Scots Gaelic, Manx, and Irish Gaelic. The main groups of Welsh, Scots and Irish Gaelic still exist, as does Manx, and are even promoted in order to preserve the language community. Cornish, however, became extinct 200 years ago when the last recorded speaker died. Due to the above mentioned promotion, the rest of the Celtic languages have a better chance of surviving. Other Celtic tongues are also still spoken in Brittany (France) and, also on the verge of becoming extinct, are sponsored as well. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 18 Language Welsh (Cymric) Cornish Scots Gaelic Manx Irish Gaelic Area Wales Cornwall Scotland Isle of Man Ireland Status still spoken extinct still spoken still spoken still spoken 3.1.2. Latin Another language in England was Latin. It was spoken extensively for a period of about four centuries before the coming of English. In 55 BC, Julius Caesar decided to invade Britain. Because of the unexpectedly powerful resistance of the Celts, however, a final conquest could not be accomplished until about 100 years later. Almost all of what is now England was then subjected to Roman rule. Naturally, the military conquest of Britain was followed by the romanization of the province, as was the case in other countries and provinces conquered by the Romans, such as Gaul of present day France. The Roman culture and the Latin language were introduced. Note, however, that the Celts, who then inhabited the whole of the British Isles, withstood the Romans in the other parts of the country. Hence, Latin did not spread further north or west of what are roughly the present day English borders. Latin did not replace the Celtic language in Britain. Its use was confined to members of the upper classes such as landowners and the bureaucracy. Nevertheless, vocabulary for items not known to the Celts prior to Romanization infiltrated the language of the, mainly lower class, Celts, to some extent. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 19 Roman Britain 3.2. OLD ENGLISH About the year of 449 an event occurred that profoundly affected the course of history in Britain: the invasion of Britain by certain Germanic tribes. These were the Angles, Saxons, and Jutes who came from regions of Northern Europe where natural disasters and famine, due to overpopulation, had forced them to leave. Since the Roman Empire was under heavy attack at many of its borders at that time, no legions could be spared to defend the British province. The emperor in Rome, therefore, left the British population on their own devices. The British inhabitants, bereft of a military force, subsequently failed to defend themselves and what was once Roman Britain became inhabited by the newcomers. The Celtic population was forced to leave and take refuge in other areas of Britain. The struggle of the Celts against the Anglo-Saxons has been preserved in the myth of the legendary King Arthur who led his people in their resistance. The names "English" and "England" were then drawn from the name of the predominant tribe of the Angles, who had established their most powerful kingdom in the former Roman province. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 20 3.2.1. Features of Old English Old English (OE) was spoken from 449 to 1100 AD. Characteristic features of Old English are that the vocabulary is almost purely Germanic. OE is a period of full inflections: in form of endings to the noun and pronoun, the adjective and the verb. Since the grammar of such languages depends on the synthesis of words and endings, we call them synthetic languages. Nouns It is impossible here to present the inflections of the Old English noun in detail. Their nature may be gathered from two examples of the strong declension and one of the weak: (stone), a masculine a-stem; (gift), a feminine o-stem; and (hunter), a masculine consonant-stem. Verbs There are certain differences between OE verbs and Modern English (ModE) verbs. Verbs are divided into two classes: regular and irregular verbs. Regular verbs all follow the same inflection pattern, while there are irregularities among the second group. The latter consists of strong, weak, and anomalous verbs. Strong verbs are called so because a change of tense is there indicated within the word itself, by a modification of the verb root vowel, such as in sing, sang, sung. In weak verbs, like walk, walked, walked, this change is dependent on being indicated by an additional syllable. OE strong verbs can still be strong verbs in ModE: OE strong verbs may be regularized in ModE: OE weak verbs may be regularized in ModE: Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 21 3.2.2. Scandinavian influence on Old English Invasions and conquests were quite common during the first millennium AD in Britain. From 787 on, the Danes raided the English coasts and the hinterland quite frequently. In 850, they started large-scale invasions. In this period, Ælfred the Great, king of Wessex, gained recognition due to his long but successful struggle against the Danes. In 878 he defeated them and saved his kingdom, although the invaders still remained in the eastern territories. The Danish rule in these countries was also called Danelaw. To cut a long story short - after a lot of battles, defeats and victories, the Danish king Svein became king of England in 1014. The Danish rule lasted until 1042. Their language naturally had some influence on the English tongue. This influence can be seen mainly with the English vocabulary, for example word-borrowings. In Old English, the sound sk, which it had inherited from its Germanic ancestors, had soon been changed to sh. The under the Danish rule introduced Scandinavian words, however, retained their sk sound until today, helping us to identify the Scandinavian word-borrowings in English. This development also produced a range of word pairs - newly introduced Scandinavian words then stood side by side with the already existing altered sh-version, such as skiff-ship; skirt-shirt. The words of these word pairs are thus closely related on a semantic level, but serve to designate different aspects or understanding of the items. Word replacements also occurred. Several of the new foreign words replaced OE ones, as with take-niman; cast-weorpan; cut-ceorfan. In 1066, the Normans invaded England. Through the influence of Norman French, the OE period gradually ended. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 22 WORKSHEET 2 – ON OLD ENGLISH Please check your answers carefully. Name: ___________________________ Question E-mail: ______________________ Solution Points Name an extinct Celtic language that was spoken on the British Isles. 1 Name a Celtic language that is still spoken on the British Isles. 1 What happened to the OE weak verb lufian during its evolution? Your answer should contain: a definition of what a weak verb is, the modern counterpart of "lufian", and a one-sentence explanation of the change that occurred. 3 a) Old English vocabulary was almost entirely Celtic of origin. True False True or False? b) A word with a sk- at the beginning is likely to be of Scandinavian origin. True False 3 c) Latin never replaced the native languages in Britain. True False Mention two characteristic features of Old English. 2 Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 23 Learning Notes UNIT 3 THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH I: OLD ENGLISH LANGUAGES IN BRITAIN BEFORE ENGLISH Celtic Languages are Still spoken Welsh Scots Gaelic Extinct Manx Irish Gaelic Cornish LATIN - Spoken for about four centuries. In 55 BC Julius Caesar invaded Britain. The final conquest took about 100 years. What is now England was subjected to Roman rules. The military conquest of Britain was followed by the Romanization of the province. The Roman Culture and the Latin language were introduced. The Celts withstood (resisted) the Romans in the other part of the country. Latin did not spread further north or west of what are the present day English borders. Latin did not replace the Celtic language Latin was used by upper classes: landowners and the bureaucracy. Latin vocabulary for items not known to the Celts prior to Romanization infiltrated the language of the lower class Celts. HISTORY OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE 1. Old English: 7th century – 1100. 2. Middle English: 1100 – 1450/1500. 3. Modern English: 1500 - now OLD ENGLISH - Period: 449 – 1100 A.D. - In 449 Germanic tribes invaded Britain: Angles, Saxons and Jutes. They came from the regions Northern Europe. - Roman Empire did not defend the British province because it was under heavy attack at many of its borders at that time. - British inhabitants did not have a military force so they could not defend themselves. - The Anglo-Saxons occupied the Roman Britain and the Celts took refuge in other areas of Britain. - The names “English” and “England” were taken from the name of the tribe of the Angles. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 24 Learning Notes 1. Vocabulary purely Germanic 2. OE is a period of full inflections: endings to the Characteristics of Old English noun pronoun adjective verbs 3. These languages are called synthetic languages since the grammar of such languages depends on the synthesis of words and endings. 4. Nouns: masculine and feminine, singular and plural. regular: weak: change: addition of a syllable: walk, walked. 5. Verbs irregular: strong: change: modification of the root vowel: sing, sang, sung Scandinavian influence on Old English - From 787 on, the Danes raided the English coasts and the hinterland quite frequently. In 850 they started large-scale invasions. In 1014 the Danish king Svein became king of England. Danish rule lasted until 1042. - Influence of the Danish language - In English vocabulary: Word-borrowings. The sound sk inherited from its Germanic ancestors was changed to sh - Scandinavian words retained their sk. Example: skirt - This development produced a range of word pairs – newly introduced Scandinavian words stood side by side with the already existing altered sh-version. Examples: skiff-ship; skit-shirt. - Word replacement also occurred: New foreign words replaced OE ones. Examples: take instead of niman; cast instead of weorpan; cut instead of ceorfan. - In 1066 the Normans invaded England and through the influence of Norman French, the OE period gradually ended. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 25 Learning Notes UNIT 3 THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH I: OLD ENGLISH INDO-EUROPEAN Western branch Eastern branch Latin: centum Sanskrit: satem Germanic West AngloFrisian English East Modern German Frisian Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics Celtic North Scandinavian tongues Latin Still spoken Extinct Welsh Scots Gaelic Manx Irish Gaelic Cornish Balto-Slavic IndoIranian UNIT 4 THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH II: MIDDLE ENGLISH 4.1. THE CHANGE FROM OLD ENGLISH TO MIDDLE ENGLISH The Middle English (ME) period lasted from about 1100-1500. Major historical events influenced the language change. In 1066, the Duke of Normandy, the famous William, henceforth called "the Conqueror", sailed across the British Channel. He challenged King Harold of England in the struggle for the English throne. After winning the battle of Hastings where he defeated Harold, William was crowned King of England. A Norman Kingdom was now established. The Anglo-Saxon period was over. The Norman invasion naturally had a profound effect on England’s institutions and its language. The Norman French spoken by the invaders became the language of England's ruling class. The lower classes, while remaining English-speaking, were influenced nevertheless by the new vocabulary. French became the language of the affairs of government, court, the church, the army, and education where the newly adopted French words often substituted their former English counterparts. The linguistic influence of Norman French continued for as long as the Kings ruled both Normandy and England. When King John lost Normandy in the years following 1200, the links to the French-speaking community subsided. English then slowly started to gain more weight as a common tongue within England again. A hundred years later, English was again spoken by representatives of all social classes, this new version of the English language being strikingly different, of course, from the Old English used prior to the Norman invasion. The English spoken at this turn of events is called Middle English. About ten thousand French words had been taken over by English during the Middle English period, and most of them have remained in the language until the present day. Aside from the already mentioned new vocabulary pertaining to the affairs of government, court, the church, the army, and education, many words relating to food and fashion were introduced as well. In some fields an original English terminology did not exist. Therefore, many French terms were borrowed. One example is the names of animals and their meat. Whereas the names of the animals remained the same, their meat was renamed according to the Norman custom. This correlated to the sociological structures: the farmers that raised the animals were predominantly English natives and could afford to keep using their own vocabulary while farming - those serving the meat at the dining room table to the mainly French upper classes had to conform to the French language. ANIMAL sheep cow swine MEAT mutton beef pork Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 27 The English language also has doublets - these are pairs of words that have the same etymology, i.e. the same source, but that differ in meaning because they had been introduced into the English language by two separate languages. The Latin and French influence, for instance, made for many of such word pairs. Latin vocabulary adopted by the Celts directly became a part of English. The same vocabulary was sometimes adopted by the Gauls and introduced to English via Norman French . DOUBLETS Adj. urban urbaine Noun Curtsy courtesy MEANING (area) having qualities of a large settlement (person) having a certain sense for culture female gesture of respect (bending the knees) politeness As far as grammar is concerned, a reduction of inflections began. The grammatical gender disappeared and inflections merged. As the inflections of the Old English disappeared, the word order of Middle English became increasingly fixed. This change made for a great loss of strong verbs. At a time when English was the language mainly of the lower classes and largely removed from educational or literary domains and influence, it was natural that many speakers applied the pattern of inflecting weak verbs to verbs which were historically strong. This linguistic principle of adopting the pattern of a less common form to a more familiar one is called analogy. The exclusive use of the pattern SVO (subject – verb – object; see the chapter on universals) emerged in the twelfth century and has remained part of English ever since. Middle English dialects Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 28 4.2. MODERN ENGLISH The Modern English (ModE) period began in 1500 and lasts until the present day. The complex inflectional system of Old English had been simplified during the ME period. Modern English is therefore called the period of lost inflections. An important phonological change of English vowels took place between 1450 and 1650, when all long vowels changed their quality to a great extent. This development is called the Great English Vowel Shift. For information on phonology, see unit 7. Each long vowel came to be pronounced with a greater elevation of the tongue and closing of the mouth. Those vowels that could be raised were raised and those that could not be raised became diphthongs. Diphthongs are sounds where two vowels are pronounced after another so closely that they become one acoustic phenomenon, like in German "Eule" or "Auto". "Raising" here refers to the position of the tongue in the mouth. This movement is commonly illustrated with the help of the following graphic, which shows where the vowels are produced in the mouth. The top left corner, for example, corresponds to the upper front space in the mouth, where the tongue moves when you pronounce the Some examples can be drawn from the pronunciation of words at the time of Geoffrey Chaucer, one of the most famous authors of ME, and William Shakespeare, whose use of English was already modern. Short vowels were not affected by the Great English Vowel Shift. Thus, ME sak remained ModE sack , ME fish remained ModE fish . This phonological change did not, however, express itself in any alterations of writing conventions. This fact is confusing for many learners of English. The spelling conventions of English vowels had essentially been established by the time of William Caxton, who founded his printing press in 1476. This was some time before the phonological change had progressed very far. Caxton's spelling reflects the pronunciation of the Middle English period and thus does not do justice to Modern English pronunciation. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 29 WORKSHEET 3 – ON MIDDLE ENGLISH Please check your answers carefully. Name: ___________________________ E-mail: ______________________ Question Solution In which areas of social or political life was the influence of Norman French established after 1066? Points 5 What is a doublet? 2 What happened to the English system of vowels during the Great English Vowel Shift? 3 Learning Notes UNIT 4 THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH II: MIDDLE ENGLISH 4.1. THE CHANGE FROM OLD ENGLISH TO MIDDLE ENGLISH - The Middle English (ME) period lasted from about 1100 – 1500. - In 1066 the Duke of Normandy, William the Conqueror, defeated King Harold of England and was crowned King of England. - A Norman Kingdom was established and the Anglo-Saxon period was over. - The Norman French was the language of England’s ruling class, the language of affairs of government, court, the church, the army, and education. - English was spoken by the lower classes, but influenced by the new French vocabulary. - The linguistic influence of Norman French continued for as long as the Kings ruled both Normandy (in France) and England. - After 1200 the King John lost Normandy but the links to the French-speaking community remained. - English started to gain more weight as a common tongue within England again. - One hundred years later, English was again spoken by representative of all social classes. - This new version of English was different from the Old English used before Norman invasion. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 30 - This is called Middle English MIDDLE ENGLISH Vocabulary Grammar French words Doublets - 10,000 French words had been taken over by English - Most of them have Pairs of words that have the same etymology (the same source) but differ in meaning because they had been introduced into the English language by two separate languages remained until present Vocabulary of affairs of: - government - court - the church - the army - education Words for food: - For animals: English, because farmers raised the animals) - For food: French, because they had to serve the meat at the dining room table of the French upper classes. Words for fashion were French Latin vocabulary adopted by the Celts French introduced to English via Norman French urban curtsy urbaine courtesy Reduction of inflections - Gender disappeared - Inflections merged - Word order became increasingly fixed - Use of the pattern SVO emerged in the 12th century - Great loss of strong (irregular) verbs because lower class people applied the inflection of weak (regular) verbs to strong verbs MODERN ENGLISH - The Modern English period began in 1500 and lasts until the present day. MODERN ENGLISH Grammar Period of lost inflections Phonology - Great English Vowel Shift - Long vowels were pronounced with a greater elevation of the tongue - Some vowels became diphthongs QUESTIONS e.g. five /fIf/ became /faIv/ Spelling - Phonological changes did not alter writing conventions - Spelling conventions of English vowels were established Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 31 PROJECT 1 Do a piece of research about this topic: ENGLISH WORDS. MIDDLE ENGLISH WORDS AND MODERN Find 20 words beginning with ...... (A to Z) that originated in the Middle English period and 20 words that originated in the Modern English period. The paper should have: I. II. III. IV. Introduction Content (body) Conclusion Bibliography (references) Check the bibliography page of this course to see the way of presenting it. You should write the reference in every part, that is, where you found the information you are presenting. The paper should have maximum three pages. It must be typed 1½ space, Times Roman 12. Margins: Top: 1.5, Down: 1.6; Left: 3.1; Right: 2.1. Date to submit the paper: Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 32 UNIT 5 COMMUNICATION There is more to communication than just one person speaking and another one listening. Human communication processes are quite complex. We differentiate verbal and nonverbal, oral and written, formal and informal, and intentional and unintentional communication. In addition, there is human-animal communication and human-computer communication. A famous statement says that we are not able to not communicate. In this chapter, we will concentrate on verbal communication between humans in either spoken or written form. Verbal communication involves the use of linguistic symbols that mean something to those who take part in the process. These symbols are spoken words in oral communication and their realization as alphabetical units in written communication. Oral communication refers to messages that are transmitted "out loud" from one person to another. We all participate in this process every day, for example, as speakers or listeners when talking, watching TV, or answering the phone. The most prominent feature of oral communication is that it is not permanent unless it is recorded. Written communication is primarily verbal but involves also other elements due to the variations in writing. In contrast to oral communication, it is not transitory, but permanent. Thus, written messages enable us to keep exact records of language and communication. Living in a purely oral culture would limit our capacity of cultural development enormously. The communication process involves certain elements. Let us have a look at these elements by examining some communication models. 5.1. SAUSSURE’S MODEL OF THE SPEECH CIRCUIT On the one hand, communication is linear in that two persons, A and B, communicate in a way that a message is conveyed from one to the other: A B. On the other hand, the participants in the communication process are both simultaneously active. Person B does not only listen, she or he may answer or at least show some reaction. On the basis of this understanding, Saussure devised a circular communication model, i.e. the model of the speech circuit. It shows the mechanisms of a dialogue: Acoustic signals are sent from a speaker A to a receiver B, who then, in turn, becomes the sender, sending information to A, who becomes the receiver. Saussure outlined two processes within this framework. The first one is phonation. Here the sender formulates mental signs in the mind and then gives acoustic shape to them. The second one, audition, is the opposite process of the receiver transforming the acoustic message into mental signs. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 33 Part of the Saussurean model of the speech circuit consists of his model of the linguistic sign. You now know its most important feature, namely the division into acoustic shape, or acoustic image, and the idea related to the image, the mental concept. Concept and acoustic image are transported in communication. 5.2. SHANNON’S AND MOLES’ COMMUNICATION MODELS In 1949, the American engineer Shannon developed a model which explains what basically happens in communication: The input, or intended message, is sent by a sender via a channel. The message received becomes the output. Input and output may differ substantially as a channel is usually exposed to circumstances that may alter its intended quality of transmission. For instance, the channel of a telephone communication line is usually impaired with noise, which in turn affects the outcome, i.e. output, of the message. Moles appended Shannon's model in 1963, adding a crucial element, the code. The sender and receiver have to have at least a fundamental set of codes in common, in order for them to communicate successfully. For example, two speakers from different countries who do not speak each other's language can only rely on internationally known words, thus making the sought for communication hardly possible. But even speakers of the same language often have problems of the same sort if their 'personal codes' differ greatly. 5.2.1. Elements of the communication process Here are the various components of the communication process in detail. Input. The sender has an intention to communicate with another person. This intention makes up the content of the message. Sender. The sender encodes the message, e.g. the idea of "piece of furniture to sit on" = /tʃeə/. Thus he gives expression to the content. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 34 Channel. The message is sent via a channel, which can be made of a variety of materials. In acoustic communication it consists of air, in written communication of paper or other writing materials. Noise. The channel is subjected to various sources of noise. One example is telephone communication, where numerous secondary sounds are audible. Even a solid channel such as paper can be crushed or stained. Such phenomena are also noise in the communicative sense. Receiver. The receiver decodes the incoming message, or expression. He "translates" it and thus receives the Output. This is the content decoded by the receiver. Code. In the process, the relevance of a code becomes obvious: The codes of the sender and receiver must have at least a certain set in common in order to make communication work. 5.3. BÜHLER’S ORGANON MODEL Plato was the first to discuss an instrumentalist definition of language. According to this definition, language primarily serves the purpose of communication. It is a linguistic tool. From this instrumental approach, Karl Bühler devised a model which described the communicative functions. In his words, language is an "organum for one person's communicating with another about things." Organum is Greek for tool. The three main functions of language Bühler distinguishes in his model are representation, expression, and appeal. Which function applies to which communicative action depends on which relations of the linguistic sign are predominant in a communicative situation. BÜHLER’S ORGANON MODEL How does this model work? Bühler's model describes the communication between a sender and a receiver by including a third party, the objects or states of affairs. A communicative function is then attributed to each act of communication, depending on which of the three parties involved was focused on most heavily. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 35 When the focus is on the sender, we speak of the expressive function of communication. When the focus is on the objects, the function is representative. The third function refers to communication where the focus is on the receiver. This function is called appeal. The circle symbolizes the phenomenon of the sound, that is the actual word spoken. The triangle symbolizes the linguistic sign and shares common space with the circle in some areas, while extending beyond it in other areas. This overlapping portrays the two key features of the relationship between the sign and its physical realization. Abstractive relevance. Where the circle overlaps the triangle, the phenomenon sound contains more acoustic information than the sign does. We are, however, capable of filtering out the relevant information without being hindered by all the additional stuff, e.g. the "ahs" and "ehms" of casual conversation. Apperceptive enlargement. The triangle also covers space beyond the circle. This means that part of the message may be lost, due to either misspellings or omissions on the part of the sender, or because the channel is subjected to noise. In this case, we are still able to fill in the gaps to create a meaningful message. Somehow we gather what got lost. This is what we call apperceptive enlargement. S apperceptive enlargement abstractive relevance 5.4. JAKOBSON’S MODEL OF COMMUNICATIVE FUNCTIONS Jakobson extended Bühler's system of communicative functions. His model reminds us of those lined out at the beginning of this chapter containing all the components of Moles', except for one, namely context. Jakobson stated that a common code is not sufficient for the communicative process. A context is necessary from which the object of communication is drawn. This context resembles Bühler's object correlate. Jakobson allocates a communicative function to each of the components. The emotive function focuses on the addresser and resembles Bühler's expressive function. The addresser's own attitude towards the content of the message is emphazised. Examples are emphatic speech or interjections. The conative function is allocated to the addressee. Bühler called it the appelative function, so it is possible to find both terms in the literature. It is directed towards the addressee. One example is the vocative. The referential function refers to the context. Here we, again, have the function emphasizing that communication is always dealing with something contextual, what Bühler called representative. The phatic function helps to establish contact and refers to the channel of communication. Some of these utterances only serve to maintain contact between two speakers. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 36 The metalinguistic function deals with the code itself. This is the function of language about language. This whole reader is an example of metalanguage. We use it to examine the code. The metalinguistic function is also predominant in questions like "Sorry, what did you say?" where the code is misunderstood and needs correction or clarification. The poetic function is allocated to the message. Messages convey more than just the content. They always contain a creative 'touch' of our own. These additions have no purpose other than to make the message "nicer". Rhetorical figures, pitch or loudness are some aspects of the poetic function. Naturally, several functions may be active simultaneously in utterances. To find out which function predominates requires analysis. Referential Context Emotive Addresser Message Poetic Addressee Conative Code Metalinguistic Contact Phatic Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 37 WORKSHEET 4 – COMMUNICATION Please check your answers carefully. Name: ___________________________ Question E-mail: ______________________ Solution Explain what "noise" means in the context of communication models. Points 2 abstractive relevance: Give examples for the phenomena of apperceptive enlargement and abstractive relevance. 2 apperceptive enlargement: Why is Bühler's communication model called the organon model? 3 a) Communication is only possible if the involved persons speak the same language. True False True or False? b) A person who neither talks nor moves does not communicate. True False Mention Jakobson’s communicative functions. 2 1 Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 38 Learning Notes UNIT 5 COMMUNICATION Verbal Non-verbal Oral Written Formal Informal Intentional Non-intentional Human-animal Human-computer Verbal communication involves use of linguistic symbols which are spoken words alphabetical units oral communication writen communication - Messages transmitted “out loud” from one person to another - It is not permanent unless it is recorded - It is not transitory, but permanent - It enables us to keep exact records of language communication SAUSSURE’S MODEL OF THE SPEECH CIRCUIT - Communication is linear: The message is transmitted from person A to person B and person B does not only listen, but he/she may answer or at least show some reaction. - Acoustic signals are sent from a speaker A to a receiver B, who then, in turn, becomes the sender, sending information to A, who becomes the receiver. - The sender formulates mental signs in the mind and then gives acoustic shape to them. This is phonation. Then the receiver transforms the acoustic message into mental signs. This is audition. Phonation: Audition: Sender: Mental sign: and then gives acoustic shape or acoustic image: telephone Receiver: transforms the acoustic message: telephone into mental sign (concept): See the diagram of the copies Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 39 Learning Notes SHANON’S COMMUNICATION MODEL - Study from the copies. It is quite clear. Study the diagram. The input is the information source or intended message. The sender is the transmitter, where communication originates. The channel is the medium through which the message is carried: speech, memo, mail. The receiver is the person who receives the message, the recipient of the message. The output is the destination or the message received. MOLES’S COMMUNICATION MODEL - Study from the copies. It is quite clear. Study the diagram. The sender and the receiver have to share the same code, that is, the system of signals which can be used to send a message. A natural language is an example of a code. So, if people want to communicate they should speak the same language. If not, they can only rely on international known words, such as, taxi, telephone, TV, thus making communication almost impossible. And even speakers of the same language may have problems when they have different ‘personal codes’. For example, in Spanish, when somebody coming from a poor area says “caleta”, the educated receiver won’t understand that it means “house.” Also, some regions and countries have different codes for certain words. For example: durazno (in Ecuador) and melocotón (in Spain); trousers (in England) and pants (in the United States). Elements of the communication process: - Study all the elements from the handout. They are very clear. BÜHLER’S ORGANON MODEL - Karl Bühler devised a model which described the communicative functions: representation, expression and appeal. - Communication elements: sender, receiver and objects and states of affairs. - When the focus is on the sender, we speak of the expressive function of communication. - When the focus is on the object, the function is representative. - When the focus is on the receiver, the function is appeal. - The circle symbolizes the phenomenon of the sound, that is the actual word spoken. - The triangle symbolizes the linguistic sign (words) and shares common space with the circle in some areas, while extending beyond it in other areas. - Abstractive relevance: Where the circle (sound) overlaps the triangle (words), the phenolmenon sound contains more acoustic information than the sign does. However, we are capable of filtering out (catching) the relevant information without being hindered by additional stuff, e.g. the “ahs”, and “ehms” of casual conversation. S apperceptive enlargement abstractive relevance Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 40 Learning Notes - Apperceptive enlargement: study from the copies. It is clear. JAKOBSON’S MODEL OF COMMUNICATION FUNCTIONS - His model contains all the components of Moles’ plus context. - A common code is not sufficient for the communication process. A context is necessary. The context resembles Bühler’s object correlate. - A message is sent by the addresser to an addressee. For this to occur, the addresser and addressee must use a common code, a physical channel, or contact, and the same frame of reference, or context. (An addresser, using a code, produces a message for the addressee, thanks to a contact, in a context). CONTEXT MESSAGE ADDRESSER ------ ADDRESSEE CONTACT CODE Functions: Study them from the copies. CONTEXT (Referential) Cognitive use of the language ADDRESSER (Emotive) MESSAGE (Poetic ADDRESSEE (Conative) Reflects the speaker’s attitude to the topic of his discourse (Decorative / aesthetic) Centered on the second person CONTACT (Phatic) Channel/Contact between speaker and addressee CODE (Metalinguistic) Focuses on the verbal code Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 41 UNIT 6 PHONETICS Phonetics is a branch of linguistics that studies the material aspects of speech sounds. What is meant by "material aspect"? And what other features exist which characterize sounds? Well, material aspects of sounds are those aspects that make for the physical production, transportation and comprehension of the sound. Another aspect of a sound is its function within a language. The function of sounds is, however, examined by another linguistic discipline, namely phonology. Please take note of the fact though that this represents the European (including the English) categorization of these linguistic disciplines and that Americans follow a different convention. Americans use the term "phonology" to refer to our understanding of both phonetics and phonology. They refer to what we call 'phonetics' also as 'phonetics', but refer to what Europeans call 'phonology' as 'phonemics'. So if you ever come across the American terminology, do not be confused. In any way, in this textbook we will stick to the European terms. The modern alphabet does not suffice to transcribe all sounds on a one-to-one basis. There are many instances though, when we need an internationally comprehensible code for the detailed transcription of sounds, such as in linguistic research, as well as in foreign language teaching. A special alphabet devised by the International Phonetic Association (IPA) is then used. Phonetic characters refer to the actual utterance of a sound. In phonetic writing, the symbols for these sounds are put within brackets, such as: [ ]. 6.1. PHYSICAL ASPECTS OF A SOUND Back to phonetics. We stated above that there are three different physical aspects of a sound. These are the articulatory aspect of the speaker, the acoustic aspect of the channel, and the auditory aspect of the hearer. Articulatory phonetics researches where and how sounds are originated and thus carries out physiological studies of the respiratory tract, trying to locate precisely at which location and in which manner a sound is produced. Acoustic phonetics examines the length, frequency and pitch of sounds. Special instruments are required to measure and analyze the sounds while they travel via the channel. Auditory phonetics studies what happens inside the ear and brain when sounds are finally received. It also interested in our ability to identify and differentiate sounds. 6.2. ARTICULATORY PHONETICS In this textbook, we will concentrate on articulatory phonetics, which also happens to be what modern linguistics has traditionally focused on. Our notion of the typical, classical linguist, zealously studying speech sounds, such as the famous Dr. Higgins from George Bernard Shaw's Pygmalion, is derived from this stage in the discipline. Any speech sound is caused by a stream of air that, originating in our vocal apparatus, escapes our mouth or nose. The various sounds all differ according to the voicing, location and manner of their production. A minor change of any of these three factors may alter a sound significantly. For example, if a stream of air leaves the vocal tract unhindered, the result is a vowel. If the stream of air is obstructed in any way the result is a consonant. Although the procedure is very complex, it is possible to determine the exact voicing, location and manner of articulation of all sounds of a language. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 42 6.2.1. Voicing Try to utter two long consonants, first [z], then [s], continually: "zzzzzzzzsssssssssszzzzzzz". Hold your fingertip to your larynx (Adam's apple) and try to notice what happens. You will feel a vibration. This is caused by a stream of air that is being pressed through a narrow aperture, called glottis, between the vocal cords. It is the pressure of the air on the walls of the glottis that causes the vibration of the cords. We are able to produce two different sets of sounds, which are otherwise identical: voiced and voiceless sounds, by this small change of the glottis. There are many consonants which are differentiated in this way, like [f]-[v], [t]-[d], or [g]-[k]. 6.2.2. Manner of articulation 6.2.2.1 Plosives and continuants Another fundamental distinction of consonants is made between so-called plosives and continuants. Plosives are consonants that are brought about by an explosive release of air from the mouth, e.g. [ ]. They are also called stops, or oral stops. If the air is released through the nose, we call the resulting consonant is a nasal plosive, as in [ ] or [ ], which is also called nasal stop since the mouth is kept closed for the most part. If the air continues to be released after the articulation of the consonant, the sound is a continuant. If we let out air continuously through a space behind the upper teeth, the socalled alveolar ridge, we produce a type of continuant sound called fricative, e.g., [ ]. Affricates are produced by a plosive and a fricative continuant following immediately thereafter, as in [ ], e.g., in the word "chair". Fricatives and affricates with a hissing sound, as [ ] in "zip", or [( ] in "measure" are also called sibilants. Oral stops, i.e. nonnasal plosives, and fricative and affricative continuants all have in common that the air is notlet out through the nose; consonants produced in this way are called obstruents. If air is released also through the nose, these consonants are called sonorants. The sounds [ ] and [ ] are called liquids. 6.2.2.2. Aspiration A further manner of articulating a sound is by either aspirating or not aspirating the sound. Try to pronounce these: [ ]-[ ]. You will notice that while saying the word 'pit', a stream of air evades your mouth, whereas when saying 'spit', your glottis starts to vibrate immediately after the pronunciation of [ ]. Hence, we may call the [ ] aspirated when saying 'pit', while it is unaspirated when saying 'spit'. This distinction results from the glottis remaining open after certain occasions of a sound, namely in the case of aspirated ones. 6.2.3. Place of articulation Consonants are distinguished also according to the location of their production, that is, after the various organs of the vocal tract. Labials are consonants that are articulated by use of the lips. Some of these are created by bringing the lips together, like [ ]. These are called bilabials. Other consonants are brought about by bringing the bottom lip to the upper teeth. These are called labiodentals, e.g. [ ]. Alveolars. These are articulated by raising the tip of the tongue to the alveolar ridge, like [ ]. Velars. If you raise the back of your tongue to the soft velum, velars are produced. An example is [ ]. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 43 Interdentals are the sounds at the beginning of "thin" and "then", in IPA: [ ] and [ ]. In order to articulate these, you have to press the tongue between the teeth. Again you can see that the difference is voicing. Palatals (or Alveopalatals) as in the middle of the word "measure" are produced by the contact of the front part of the tongue with the hard palate just behind the alveolar ridge. With the help of this detailed information we can now refer to every consonant by its location and manner of articulation; [ ], for example, is a voiceless, labiodental fricative. CHART OF ENGLISH CONSONANT PHONEMES Place of articulation Bilabial Manner Labiodental Dental Alveolar ð t d sz of articulation Plosive Fricative fv Affricate Nasal Lateral Approximant 6.3. p b Palatoalveolar (Postalveolar) Palatal Velar Glottal k g h ʃʒ tʃ dʒ m w n l ŋ r j Articulatory phonetics – vowels In contrast to consonants, where voice, manner and place of articulation serve as descriptive categories, vowels are differentiated by their position of the tongue and the lips. These categories are indicative of the different ways in which the two sounds are produced while consonants are produced with the help of many organs, vowels depend only on the position of the tongue and the lips. The positioning the tongue and the lips allow for a great amount of variation, enabling us to voice many more vowels than the familiar five vowels a, e, i, o, u. English has more than a dozen different vowels. Also, in English we find several diphthongs. Vowels can be produced by raising the tongue high, keeping it in the middle, or low in the mouth; also, the tongue can be moved to the front, center or back. The lips can be rounded or nonrounded. If you try to articulate while moving your lips and your tongue around, you will notice how manifold the resulting vowel-like sounds are. A general chart of the vowels of English looks as follows: Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 44 This chart is a rough sketch of the mouth. It is similar to a coordinate system, with the positions high, middle, and low representing one axis, and the positions front, central, and back the other axis. The only position not used in English for the pronunciation of vowels is the high central point. SHORT AND LONG VOWELS FRONT Close i CENTRAL BACK u i: u: υ I Close-mid e o ɔ: e ɜ: ɛ Open-mid ɔ ʌ æ ɑ: ɒ ɑ Open a 6.3.1. English short vowels English has a large number of vowel sounds; the first ones to be examined are short vowels. The symbols for these short vowels are: ɪ kit, bid, hymn, minute e dress, bed, head, many æ trap, bad ɒ lot, odd, wash ʌ strut, mud, love, blood ʊ foot, good, put Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 45 6.3.2. Long vowels Long vowels are the vowels which tend to be longer than the short vowels in similar contexts. It is necessary to say “in similar contexts” because the length of all English vowel sounds varies very much according to the context (such as the type of sound that follows them) and the presence or absence of stress. To remind you that these vowels tend to be long, the symbols consists of one vowel symbol plus a length-mark made of two dots : . iː fleece, sea, machine ɜː nurse, stir, learn, refer ɑː start, father ɔː thought, law, north, war uː goose, two, blue, group 6.3.3. Diphthongs Diphthongs are sounds which consists of a movement or glide from one vowel to another. A vowel which remains constant and does not glide is called a pure vowel, and one of the most common pronunciation mistakes that result in a learner of English having a “foreign” accent is the production of pure vowels where a diphthong should be pronounced. In terms of length, diphthongs are like the long vowels described above. Perhaps the most important thing to remember about all the diphthongs is that the first part is much longer and stronger than the second part. The total number of diphthongs is eight. The easiest way to remember them is in terms of three groups divided as in this diagram: centring ɪə closing closing eɪ face, day, break əʊ goat, show, now eə square, fair, various aɪ price, high, try aʊ mouth, now, how ʊə poor, jury, cure ɔɪ choice, boy near, here, weary Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 46 In American English they add the sound /r/ to these words: /nɪər/ near /skwɛər/ square /pʊər/ poor /hɪər/ here /fɛər/ fair /ˈdʒʊəri/ jury /ˈwɪəri/ weary /ˈvɛəriəs/ various /kyʊər/ cure 6.3.4. Triphthongs A triphthong is a glide from one vowel to another and then to a third, all produced rapidly and without interruption. eɪ + ə = eɪə layer, player aɪ + ə = aɪə liar, fire ɔɪ + ə = ɔɪə loyal, royal əʊ + ə = əʊə lower, mower aʊ + ə = aʊə power, hour In American English they add the sound /r/ to these words: /ˈleɪər/ layer /ˈpleɪər/ player /ˈlaɪər/ liar /faɪər/ fire American and British Vowel Sounds in Phonemic Script EXERCISES 1. Give the correct technical terms for the sounds made in the following ways: a. Both lips coming together. b. The bottom lip and top teeth coming together. c. The tongue touching the upper teeth ridge d. The tongue touching the hard palate. e. The tongue touching the soft palate. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 47 2. Give the correct technical term for the sounds resulting from the following closures: a. Complete closure followed by slow release of air. b. Complete closure of the oral cavity with the air diverted through the nose. c. Partial closure where the air stream is blocked by the tip of the tongue but allowed to escape round the sides of the tongue. d. Incomplete closure. e. Complete closure followed by a sudden release of air. 3. Give the symbol for each of the following sounds and give an example of its use in an English word: a. Voiced bilabial plosive f. High back rounded vowel b. Bilabial nasal g. Voiced alveolar fricative c. Voiceless labiodentals fricative h. Voiced frictionless continuant d. Alveolar lateral i. Voiceless velar plosive e. High front unrounded vowel j. Voiced dental fricative 4. Each of the following sets contains an inappropriate member. Pick it out and say why it is inappropriate. a. m, n, ŋ, b b. b, p, n, t c. b, m, p, s d. l, f, v, s, z e. l, o, e, ɛ f. i, o, ɛ, e g. t, d, l, n, p h. t, d, p, f, s i. b, d, z, r, s j. t, k, ŋ, g 5. Describe the similarities and differences in the sounds represented by the underlined elements in the following sets. (Remember you are dealing with sounds and not letters.) a. leaf leaves f. notion nodding b. assure azure g. rough rub c. sad sat h. feel fall d. seat set i. vine wine e. jut shut j. sing sin Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 48 6. Look at the phonetic transcription and write the words next to them. a. /’brekfəst/ _________________ g. /’nju:zpeɪpə/ ___________________ b. /kəm’pju:tə/ _________________ h. /trəˈdɪʃn/ _______________________ c. /’nɒlɪdʒ/ _________________ i. /ˈdʒɜ:nəlɪst/ ____________________ d. /’hʌŋgri/ _________________ j. /’wepənz/ ______________________ e. /ɜ:ɵ/ ________________ k. /’stætʃu:/ ______________________ f. /ˌmju:zɪkl ˈɪnstrəmənt/ ______________________________________ 7. Read the poem and transcribe the words in phonetic script to complete the lines. I wonder a I wonder why / ðə gra:s ɪz gri:n /, b And why / ðə wɪnd ɪz nevə si:n /. c Who taught / ðə bɜ:dz tə bɪld ə nest /, d And told / ðə tri:z tə teɪk ə rest /? e And when / ðə mu:n ɪz nɒt kwaɪt rɑʊnd /, f Where can / ðə mɪsɪŋ bɪt bi fɑʊnd /? g Who / laɪts ðə stɑ:z /, when they blow out, h And / meɪks ðə laɪtnɪŋ / flash about? i Who paints / ðə reɪnbəʊ ɪn ðə skaɪ /, j And hangs / ðə flʌfi klɑʊdz səʊ haɪ /? Why is it now, do you suppose, That Dad won’t tell me if he knows? 8. Write these words in phonetic symbols. a. word ________________ d. woman ______________ g. half _________ b. weather ________________ e. women ______________ h. island _______ c. sugar f. uncle i. work _______ ________________ ______________ 9. Cross out the word which does not contain the vowel sound on the left. a. /ɪ/ build field fill women b. /e/ leather friend break bread c. /ʌ/ front rough won’t country d. /ɒ/ clock wonder want wash e. /æ/ angry hungry fax salmon f. /i:/ cheese breath meal breathe g. /u:/ spoon wooden zoo souvenir h. /ɔ:/ warm walk store work i. /ɜ:/ world ferry early journalist Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 49 10. Say these words aloud and then transcribe them. Two syllables: a. /ˈmænɪdʒə/ b. /ˈbɪznəs/ c. /fəˈget/ d. /əˈplaɪ/ e. /ˈfɒrən/ f. /ˈkɪdnæpt/ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ g. h. i. j. k. l. /kəˈrɪə/ /rɪˈzaɪn/ /rɪˈtaiəd/ /ˈfæktri/ /dɪˈgri:/ /pəˈli:s/ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ f. /dɪˈrektə/ _____________________ g. /ˈju:nɪfɔ:m/ _____________________ h. /ˈmɜ:dərə/ _____________________ i. /rɪˈdʌndənt/ _____________________ j. /ˌmægəˈzi:n/ ____________________ Three syllables: a. b. c. d. e. /ɪmˈplɔɪə/ /ɪmplɔɪˈi:/ /ˈɪntəvju:/ /mɪljəˈneə/ /ˈpɒlətɪks/ Four syllables: a. /ˌʌnɪmˈplɔɪmənt/ __________________ b. /ˌɪntəvju:ˈi:/ __________________ c. /ˌpɒləˈtɪʃn/ __________________ d. /ˌæplɪˈkeɪʃn/ _____________________ e. /ˌrezɪgˈneɪʃn/ _____________________ f. /ɪnˈtɜ:prɪtə/ ______________________ Ways of pronouncing –ooThe letters –oo- are pronounced in different ways. Examples: soon = /u:/ book = /ʊ/ flood = /ʌ/ Say the following sentences to yourself and write all the –oo- words in the correct columns. a. b. c. d. e. f. g. Have you read the ‘Good Food Guide’ to Britain? The best cooks use a wooden spoon to stir the sauce. Look! There’s a pool of blood on the carpet! If I won the football pools, I’d be flooded with begging letters. We foolishly booked a hotel room without a ensuite bathroom. I took my woolen jumper in case the weather turned cool in the afternoon. He stood on a stool and climbed onto the roof. soon = /u:/ book = /ʊ/ flood = /ʌ/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 50 Ways of pronouncing –ou– The letters –ou– are also pronounced in many different ways. Examples: four = /ɔ:/ group = /u:/ 11. The following groups of four words are all spelt with –ou-. Underline the word with the different pronunciation. a. b. c. d. e. f. g. h. your would accountant drought enough anonymous trouble through court should country ought cough mouse double group neighbour shoulder count bought rough enormous doubt soup pour could fountain thought tough furious country thought 12. Write the words from the box next to the correct diphthong. There are two more words for each diphthong. where know high clear sure enjoy stay now aloud shy noise poor beer phone weigh bear a. /ɪ/ + /ə/ = /ɪə/ near ______________ __________________ b. /e/ + /ə/ = /eə/ c. /e/ + /ɪ/ = /eɪ/ hair ______________ pay ______________ __________________ __________________ go ________________ my ______________ boy _______________ how _______________ tour* ______________ _____________________ __________________ __________________ __________________ __________________ d. e. f. g. h. /ə/ + /ʊ/ /a/ + /ɪ/ /ɔ:/ + /ɪ/ /a/ + /ʊ/ /ʊ/ + /ə/ = /əʊ/ = /aɪ/ = /ɔɪ/ = /aʊ/ = /ʊə/ * Some native speakers do not use the diphthong /ʊə/. They use /ɔ:/ and say /tɔ:/ for tour. However, this is becoming less frequent. 13. Transcribe the words in the sentences in phonetic script. They are all tiphthongs. a. We caught the /pleɪn/ __________ to the (saʊθ/ ___________ of /speɪn/ ___________. b. The /bɔɪ/ ____________ in the red /kəʊt/ _____________ said that he /ɪnˈdʒɔɪd/ ___________________ the journey. c. I’ve /nəʊn/ _____________ Sally for /ˈnɪəlɪ/ __________ /faɪv/ ___________ years. d. She’s /ˈweərɪŋ/____________ a red /rəʊz/ __________ in her /heə/ ______________ e. Fewer people /sməʊk/ ____________ /paɪps/ ___________ these /deɪz/ __________ f. He /laɪks/ _________ to /raɪd/ _________ a big black /ˈməʊtəbaɪk/ _______________. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 51 Learning Notes UNIT 6 PHONETICS Phonetics studies material aspects of speech sound Physical production of the sound Transportation of the sound Comprehension of the sound Americans use ‘Phonology’ to refer to Phonetics and Phonology. And they use ‘Phonemics’ to refer to Phonology. Phonetic Alphabet - Devised by the IPA (International Phonetic Association) - Phonetic characters refer to the actual utterance of a sound - The symbols of the phonetic alphabets are put within brackets Physical aspect of a sound Articulatory phonetics - researches where and how sounds are originated - carries out physiological studies of the respiratory tract: location & manner Speech sound Acoustic phonetics examines: - length - frequency - pitch of sound a stream of air originated in our vocal apparatus Auditory phonetics - studies what happens inside the ear and brain when sounds are received. - Interested in our ability to identify and differentiate sounds escapes our mouth and nose differs voicing voiced voiceless manner of articulation plosives - nasals - non-nasals continuants - fricative - affricate place of articulation - labials: bilabials & labiodental: - alveolars - velars - interdentals - palatals Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 52 Learning Notes Aspiration: aspirating the sound: pit pit not aspirating: spit spit Place of articulation: Labials bilabials: pie, buy, my labiodental: fat, vat Alveolar: nigh, lie, tie, die, sue, zoo Velars: cool, back, bag, bang Interdentals: thigh, thy // /ð / Palatals: you Alveopalatals: measure, leisure, she, Voicing: voiced: /b/ /d/ /g/ /v/ /ð/ /z/ /ʒ/ /dʒ/ / l / /m/ /n/ /w/ /r/ /j/ /ŋ / voiceless: /p/ /t/ /k/ /f/ // /s/ /ʃ/ /h/ / tʃ/ CHART OF ENGLISH CONSONANT PHONEMES Place of articulation Bilabial Manner Labiodental Dental Alveolar ð t d sz of articulation Plosive Fricative p b fv Affricate Nasal Lateral Approximant Palatoalveolar (Postalveolar) Palatal Velar Glottal k g h ʃʒ tʃ dʒ m n l w ŋ r j VOWELS are differentiated by their position of the lips tongue high middle low front centre back rounded nonrounded Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 53 EXERCISES 1. Give the correct technical terms for the sounds made in the following ways: a. Both lips coming together. b. The bottom lip and top teeth coming together. c. The tongue touching the upper teeth ridge d. The tongue touching the hard palate. e. The tongue touching the soft palate. 2. Give the correct technical term for the sounds resulting from the following closures: a. Complete closure followed by slow release of air. b. Complete closure of the oral cavity with the air diverted through the nose. c. Partial closure where the air stream is blocked by the tip of the tongue but allowed to escape round the sides of the tongue. d. Incomplete closure. e. Complete closure followed by a sudden release of air. 3. Give the symbol for each of the following sounds and give an example of its use in an English word: a. Voiced bilabial plosive / / ______________ b. Bilabial nasal / / ______________ c. Voiceless labiodentals fricative / / ______________ d. Alveolar lateral / / ______________ e. High front unrounded vowel / / ______________ f. High back rounded vowel / / ______________ g. Voiced alveolar fricative / / ______________ h. Voiced frictionless continuant / / ______________ i. Voiceless velar plosive / / ______________ j. Voiced dental fricative / / ______________ 4. Each of the following sets contains an inappropriate member. Pick it out and say why it is inappropriate. a. a. m, n, ŋ, b / / _______________________________________ b. b, p, n, t / / _______________________________________ c. b, m, p, s / / _______________________________________ d. l, f, v, s, z / / _______________________________________ e. l, o, e, ɛ / / _______________________________________ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 54 f. i, o, ɛ, e o – / / _______________________________________ g. t, d, l, n, p / / _______________________________________ h. t, d, p, f, s / / _______________________________________ i. b, d, z, r, s / / _______________________________________ j. t, k, ŋ, g / / _______________________________________ 6. Describe the similarities and differences in the sounds represented by the underlined elements in the following sets. (Remember you are dealing with sounds and not letters.) a. leaf leaves / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ f. notion nodding / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ / / _______________________________________ b. assure azure c. sad sat d. seat e. jut set shut g. rough h. feel rub fall i. vine wine j. sing sin Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 55 UNIT 7 PHONOLOGY Phonology deals with the function of sounds within a language. Let us have a close look at these functions. Every language has a precisely defined set of sounds. This set consists of the so-called distinctive sounds. The system of the speech sounds and their usage is defined as duality of patterning: with a given set of components, we may construct an unlimited number of new arrangements of the components. In other words: Our language provides us with a limited set of sounds, but we can arrange these to ever new utterances. 7.1. PHONEMES AND ALLOPHONES By calling a sound distinctive, we refer to its capability of changing the meaning of a word. Naturally, single sounds cannot carry any meaning. "B" or "P", for example, are meaningless utterances. But when several distinct sounds are assembled to a word, each of them suddenly contribute to a meaning. And by exchanging individual distinct sounds, we may change this meaning. We call these sounds phonemes, the smallest distinctive elements of a language. They are easy to discern. Compare two words which differ only by one sound, e.g., "pin" and "bin". By replacing the beginning consonants, the meaning of the word changes. We call such pairs minimal pairs. The test we just performed to locate the phonemes is called Commutation Test. The phonemes thereby discerned are then put within dashes, such as /p/, /b/, for phonological transcription. These are, of course, ideal units of the sound system of a language. They should not be confused with the sounds of actual utterances examined by phonetics. Phonetics tries to differentiate among the sounds with the highest possible degree of accuracy. It does so without regard for the influence a sound may have on the meaning of an utterance. These phonetic units are put in brackets, as you can see in the chapter on phonetics. Not all sounds of a language are necessarily distinctive sounds. Compare the English and American pronunciations of "dance": [ ] versus [ ]. Although there are different sounds in the pair, the meaning does not change. Thus, [ ] and [ ] are not phonemes in this case. We call this phenomenon free variation. The two sounds can be referred to as allophones. These sounds are merely variations in pronunciation of the same phoneme and do not change the meaning of the word. Free variation can be found in various dialects of the same language. In this case, the different pronunciations of words throughout a country do not change the meaning of those words. Another example of sounds which are not phonemes are those which occur in complementary distribution. This means that where one sound of the pair occurs, the other does not. An example for complementary distribution are the aspirated and unaspirated allophones of / /. The initial consonant as in "pill" is aspirated. The consonant after / / in "sprint" is unaspirated. The respective transcriptions would be [ ] and [ ], where [ ] indicates aspiration. Aspirated [ ], as you can see in this example, occurs only at the beginning of words. [ ] and [ ] are only allophones of the same phoneme /p/. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 56 7.2. DISTINCTIVE FEATURES From our studies of phonetics, we know how to describe the features of sounds. The same techniques apply for the description of phonemes. In the pair "fault" and "vault", for example, the difference lies in the voicing of the first phonemes: / / versus / /, the first representing a voiced consonant [+voiced], the second an unvoiced consonant [-voiced]. Voicing is of great importance in the English sound system. Therefore we call it a distinctive feature. Other distinctive features are [±nasal] (for consonants only) or [±consonantal], etc. Every feature has two values, the positive value '+' contrasting the negative value '-'. 7.3. REDUNDANT FEATURES Some features of sounds may be redundant in a language. This means that these sounds have a specific feature, but do not constitute minimal pairs. An example for such a redundant feature is [±nasal] of vowels in English. You have already seen that phonetics treats consonants as nasal or nonnasal only. Still, vowels can be positive nasal [+nasal] or negative nasal [-nasal]. This does not make them into phonemes; they are merely allophones of the same vowel. In the English language the redundant feature of nasality is found to abide to a general rule: Vowels that are [+nasal] are always followed by consonant that are also [+nasal]. Likewise, 'Consonants that are [+nasal] are always preceded by vowels that are also [+nasal]. For example, the vowel in "band" is [+nasal], while in "bar", it is [-nasal]. Still, nasality is a distinctive feature in English consonants. In languages other than English, nasality of vowels may well not be a redundant feature. The general rule stated above says that nasality in English vowels is predictable, because you can tell if a vowel is nasal or not by looking at the consonant following it. 7.4. SUPRASEGMENTAL FEATURES As well as the sounds that occur in speech, a number of other phenomena are of interest to the linguist. The most significant of these, pitch, stress, tone, intonation and timing (tempo), are called suprasegmental features because they accompany speech sounds and are sometimes a feature of the entire utterance rather than of any individual sound. Pitch refers to the normal melodic height of an individual’s speech. In English, higher than usual pitch for a speaker is usually associated with excitement or strain. Human speech may be considered as a succession of syllables, some of which are more strongly stressed than others. Thus, in the word ‘debatable’ it is the second of the four syllables that receives most emphasis. World languages seem to be divided into two types: ‘syllable-timed’ languages like French where syllables are produced at regular intervals of time and where stresses occur randomly; and ‘stress-timed’ languages like English where stresses occur at regular intervals with a random number of syllables occurring between stresses. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 57 In so-called ‘tone languages’, relative pitch can distinguish meanings. In many of the Bantu languages, for example, the word for ‘house’ differs from the word for ‘thread’ in terms of pitch difference only. Tone languages make systematic use of high, low, rising and falling pitch to distinguish meanings. Pitch is not systematically employed in this way in English although, on occasion, different meanings can be carried by changes of pitch or stress as in the sentence: I don’t like him because of his money. If we use a rising pitch and extra stress on ‘because’, the sentence means that I like him but my liking is not related to the fact the he has money. If we do not stress ‘because’ the sentence means that I do not like him and his money is responsible for my dislike. Intonation is associated with pitch in that it involves speech melody over an utterance. The usual intonation for a statement in English involves a slow fall: The tempo (timing) or speed of an utterance is usually associated with a speaker’s frame of mind. Increased speed may suggest excitement; reduced speed may imply boredom, tiredness or a threat. 7.4. RULES OF PHONOLOGY The sequences of phonemes are determined by certain laws that are peculiar to that particular language. The English language, for example, does not allow consonants such as / / or / / to be followed by a consonant similar to it. The phoneme sequence */bgliz/, to give another example, is not permitted in English due to the consonant cluster /bg/ at the beginning. The following rules have been found to apply to the assembly of phoneme sequences in the English language. 7.4.1. Assimilation rules The rule stated above concerning the nasalization of vowels preceding [+nasal] consonants is an assimilation rule. The vowel segment is assimilated to the consonant segment by copying the [±nasal] feature of the latter to the former. You can observe this phenomenon in your own language. There are many instances in which we try to ease articulation and the easiest way of doing this is to simplify articulation by assimilating sounds. The assimilation rule also reflects a phenomenon called coarticulation. According to this principle, features of phonemes spread in anticipation of sounds or perseveration of articulatory processes. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 58 Assimilation: A significance difference in natural connected speech is the way that sounds belonging to one word can cause changes in sounds belonging to neighbouring words. Assuming that we know how the phonemes of a particular word would be realized when the word was pronounced in isolation, when we find a phoneme realized differently as a result of being near some other phoneme belonging to a neighbouring word we call this an instance of assimilation. Example: consonantal sounds at the end of a word are often influenced by the following consonant. Thus / gυd / becomes / gug / under the influence of the following velar consonant. Similarly / wɔn / may become /wəm/ before a following / m /. This is known as assimilation. It does not necessarily always occur, but may do so, and with some speakers is habitual. There are two types of assimilation: Regressive assimilation: If the last consonant of the first word changes to become like the initial consonant of the second word in some way, the assimilation is called regressive (the phoneme that comes first is affected by the one that comes after it). Example: that person / ðæ t pɜ:sņ / light blue /laɪt blu: / becomes / ðæp pɜ:sņ / /laɪp blu: / becomes meat pie /mi:t paɪ / becomes /mi:p paɪ / that case /ðæ t keis/ becomes bright colour /braɪt kʌlə / quite good /kwaɪt gυd / /ðæ k keis/ becomes /braɪk kʌlə / becomes /kwaɪk gυd / An example at the word level: In English the negative prefix appears as im- before words such as possible: impossible. As possible starts with a bilabial sound, the prefix imends in a bilabial sound. Before words like tolerant, however, the prefix is in-: intolerant. As tolerant starts with an alveolar sound, the prefix in- ends in an alveolar sound. As the following sound brings about the change, this process is called regressive assimilation. Progressive assimilation: Example: When the preceding sounds bring about the change. The English plural is either /z/ or /s/ when it occurs after a non-sibilant sound. The voicing feature is taken from the final consonant of the base. books /bυks/ rooms /ru:mz/ The pronunciation of the –ed suffix in English: stopped /stɒpt/ named /neɪmd/ 7.4.2. Feature addition rules Sometimes, features are added to phonemes when they occur in a specific phonetic context. We have already looked at aspirated and unaspirated occurences of stops like / /. At the beginnings of words as in pill, / / is aspirated. The feature of aspiration is hence added because / / is a sound at the beginning of a word. In other phonetic contexts, the feature of aspiration is not added. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 59 7.4.3. Segment-deletion and addition rules Phonological rules of a language may result in the addition or deletion of segments from a phoneme sequence. A good example for this rule is French, where word-final consonants are deleted when a consonant follows. But they are maintained when the following word starts with a vowel or a glide: An example in English: the phoneme / r / cannot occur in syllable-final position in RP, but when a word’s spelling suggests a final / r /, and a word beginning with a vowel follows, the usual pronunciation for RP speakers is to pronounce with / r /. here / hɪǝ / but here are / hɪǝr ǝ / four / fɔ: / but four eggs / fɔ:r egz / 7.4.4. Movement (methathesis) rules In some languages, and in some English dialects, phonemes are rearranged. Consider the example "ask". In some English dialects, it is pronounced [ ]. Historically, "ask" derived from OE "aksian". A metathesis rule changed it in most English dialects to the form commonly known today, whereas in others, the old form is pertained. EXERCISES 1. Supply pairs of English words which can be distinguished by the following sets of consonants. (For example, a minimal pair for /p/ and /b/ could be ‘pear’ and ‘bear’. Remember that you are dealing with sounds and not spellings, so ‘post’ and ‘boast’ would also be a minimal pair). a. /p/ and /b/ b. /t/ and /d/ c. /k/ and /g/ d. /s/ and /z/ e. /ʃ / and /tʃ / f. /n/ and /ŋ/ g. /m/ and /p/ h. /n/ and /d/ i. /r/ and /l/ j. /tʃ/ and /dʒ/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 60 2. Transcribe the following words phonemically, using RP pronunciation. a. ghost f. judging b. among g. splendid c. infiltrate h. underpinned d. farmyard i. thousandths e. chutney j. beautiful 3. What English words are represented by the following transcriptions? Where the phonemic notation could represent more than one word, indicate the alternatives. a. b. c. d. e. /ʃɔ:t/ /’treʒǝ/ /kju:t/ /ju:/ /mi:t/ f. tʃɜ:tʃ/ g. /tʌŋ/ h. /weðǝ/ i. /dʒæz/ j. /’evrɪθɪŋ/ 4. Transcribe the following words in phonemic notation indicating (a) UK and (b) US pronunciation. a. bird b. grass c. new d. castle e. farmyard f. advertisement g. fear h. steward i. half j. better k. advantage l. towards m. I can’t Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 61 Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 62 WORKSHEET 5 – PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY Please check your answers carefully. Name: ___________________________ Question E-mail: ______________________ Solution Points What does the term "manner of articulation" refer to? 2 What is the difference between phonemes and allophones? 3 What is complementary distribution? Explain and give examples. 3 a) Vowels in the English language are not formed at all possible positions in the mouth. True False True or False? b) All sounds that occur in a sound chain are phones. Therefore they are also phonemes. True False 2 http://ipa.typeit.org/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 63 7.4.1. STRESS IN SIMPLE WORDS THE NATURE OF STRESS Stress has been mentioned several times already in this course without any attempt to define what the word means. The nature of stress is simple enough – practically everyone would agree that the first syllable of words like ‘father’, ‘open’, ‘camera’ is stressed, that the middle syllable is stressed in ‘potato’, ‘apartment’, ‘relation’ and that the final syllable is stressed in ‘about’, ‘receive’, ‘perhaps’, and most people feel they have some sort of idea of what the difference is between stressed and unstressed syllables, though they might explain in many different ways. We will mark a stressed syllable in transcription by placing a small vertical line ˈ high up, just before the syllable it relates to; the words quoted above will thus be transcribed as follows: ˈfɑ:ðə ˈəʊpən ˈkæmrə pəˈteɪtəʊ əˈpɑ:tmənt rɪˈleɪʃn ˈəbaʊt rɪˈsi:v pəˈhæps Exercise 1 When you hear the word, write it in phonetic symbols marking the stress mark ˈ before the stressed syllable. enemy subtract collect elephant capital observer carnation profit paradise entertain What are the characteristics of stressed syllables that enable us to identify them? It is important to understand that there are two different ways of approaching this question, one being to consider what the speaker does in producing stressed syllables and the other being to consider what characteristics of sound make a syllable seem to a listener to be stressed. In other words we can study stress from the point of view of production and of perception; the two are obviously closely related, but are not identical. The production of stress is generally believed to depend on the speaker using more muscular energy than is used for unstressed syllables. Measuring muscular effort is difficult, but it seems possible, according to experimental studies, that when we produce stressed syllables, the muscles that we use to expel air from the lungs are more active, producing higher subglottal pressure. It seems possible that similar things happen with muscles in other parts of our speech apparatus. Many experiments have been carried out on the perception of stress, and it is clear that many different sound characteristics are important in making a syllable recognisably stressed. From the perceptual point of view, all stressed syllables have one characteristic in common, and that is called prominence; stressed syllables are recognised as stressed because they are more prominent than unstressed syllables. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 64 What makes a syllable prominent? At least four different factors are important. 1. Most people seem to feel that stressed syllables are louder than unstressed; in other words, loudness is a component of prominence. In a sequence of identical syllables (e.g. ba:ba:ba:ba:), if one syllable is made louder than the others, it will be heard as stressed. However, it is important to realise that it is very difficult for a speaker to make a syllable louder without changing other characteristics of the syllable such as those explained below; if one literally changes only the loudness, the perceptual effect of stress is not very strong. 2. The length of syllables has an important part to play in prominence. If one of the syllables in our “nonsense word” ba:ba:ba:ba: is made longer than the others, there is quite a strong tendency for that syllable to be heard as stressed. 3. Every syllable is said on some pitch; pitch in speech is closely related to the frequency of vibration of the vocal cords and to the musical notion of low- and high-pitched notes. It is essential a perceptual characteristic of speech. If one syllable of our “nonsense word” is said with a pitch that is noticeably different from that of the others, this will have a strong tendency to produce the effect of prominence. For example, if all syllables are said with low pitch except for one said with high pitch, then the high-pitched syllable will be heard as stressed and the others as unstressed. To place some movement of pitch (e.g. rising or falling) on a syllable is even more effective. 4. A syllable will tend to be prominent if it contains a vowel that is different in quality from neighbouring vowels. If we change one of the vowels in our “nonsense word” (e.g. ba:bi:ba:ba:) the “odd” syllable bi: will usually be heard as stressed. This effect is not very powerful nor very important, but there is one particular way in which it is relevant in English: the most frequently encountered vowels in weak syllables are ɪ, ʊ and ə (syllabic consonants are also quite common). We can look on stressed syllables as occurring against a “background” of these weak syllables, so that their prominence is increased by contrast with these background qualities. Prominence, then, is produced by four main factors: a. loudness, b. length, c. pitch, and d. quality. Generally these four factors work together in combination, though syllables may sometimes be made prominent by means of only one or two of them. Experimental work has shown that these factors are not equally important; the strongest effect is produced by pitch, and length is also a powerful factor. Loudness and quality have much less effect. Exercise 2 The following are British place-names. When you hear the number, pronounce them with the stress as marked. You will then hear the correct pronunciation, which you should repeat. 1. ˈʃrəʊzbri 6. ˈbɜ:mɪŋəm 2. pɒlˈperəʊ 7. nͻ:ˈθæmptən 3. æbəˈdi:n 8. dʌnˈdi: 4. wʊlvəˈhæmptən 9. ˈkæntəbri 5. æbəˈrɪstwəθ 10. ˈbeɪzɪŋstəʊk Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 65 LEVELS OF STRESS Up to this point we have talked about stress as though there was a simple distinction between “stressed” and “unstressed” syllables with no intermediate levels; such a treatment would be a two-level analysis of stress. Usually, however, we have to recognise one or more intermediate levels. It should be remembered that in this chapter we are dealing only with stress within the word; this means that we are looking at words as they are said in isolation, which is a rather artificial situation – we do not often say words in isolation, except for a few such as ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘possibly’, ‘please’ and interrogative words such as ‘what’, ‘who’, etc., but looking at words in isolation does help us to see stress placement and stress levels more clearly than studying them in the context of continuous speech. Let us begin by looking at the word ‘around’ /ˈəraʊnd/, where the stress always falls clearly on the last syllable and the first syllable is weak. From the point of view of stress, the most important fact about the way we pronounce this word is that on the second syllable the pitch of the voice does not remain level, but usually falls from a higher to a lower pitch. We might diagram the pitch movement as shown below, where the two parallel lines represent the speaker’s high and low pitch level. ˈəraʊnd ˈəraʊnd _______________ around The prominence that results from this pitch movement, or tone, gives the strongest type of stress; we call this tone, gives the strongest type of stress; we call this tonic strong stress when we wish to refer specifically to this type of stress. It is also called primary stress. In some words, we can observe a type of stress that is weaker than tonic strong stress but stronger than that of the first syllable of ‘around’, for example in the first syllables of the words ‘photographic’ /fəʊtəgræfɪk/, ‘anthropology’ /ænrəpɑləʤi/. The third syllables of ‘activated’ /æktɪveɪtɪd/, ‘enterprising’ /entəpraɪzɪŋ/ are also quite prominent, but most speakers of English would probably not feel that they were as strongly stressed as the first two examples. The stress in the first two examples can be called nontonic strong stress; it is also called secondary stress. It is sometimes represent in transcription with a low mark /ˌ / so that the examples could be transcribed as /fəʊtəˈgræfɪk/, /ˌænrəˈpɑlədʒi/. This convention will only be used where necessary in this course. Levels of stress We have now identified two levels of stress: tonic strong (or primary) and non-tonic strong (or secondary), as well as a third level which could be called unstressed and regarded as being the absence of any recognisable amount of prominence. These are three levels that we will use in describing English stress. However, it is worth noting that unstressed syllables containing ə, ɪ, ʊ or a syllabic consonant will sound less prominent than an unstressed syllable containing some other vowel. For example, the first syllable of ‘poetic’ /pəʊˈetɪk/ is more prominent than the first syllable of ‘pathetic’ /pəˈetɪk/. This could be used as a basis for further division of stress levels, giving us a third and fourth level, but it seems unnecessarily complex to do so. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 66 PLACEMENT OF STRESS WITHIN THE WORD We now come to a question that causes a great deal of difficulty, particularly to foreign learners (who cannot simply dismiss it as an academic question): how can one select the correct syllable or syllables to stress in an English word? As is well known, English is not one of those languages where word stress can be decided simply in relation to the syllables of the words, as can be done in French (where the last syllable is usually stressed), Polish (where the syllable before the last – the penultimate syllable – is stressed) or Czech (where the first syllable is stressed). Many writers have said that English word stressed is so difficult to predict that the best approach is to treat stress placement as a property of the individual word, to be learned when the word itself is learned. Certainly anyone who tries to analyse English stress placement has to recognise that it is a highly complex matter. However, it must also be recognised that in most cases when an English speaker comes across an unfamiliar word, that English speaker can pronounce it with the correct stress (there are exceptions to this, of course); in principle, it should be possible to discover what it is that the English speaker knows and to write this in the form of rules. Recent word based on the theory of generative phonology has claimed that such rules can be stated; the following summary of stress placement in nouns, verbs, and adjectives is based to a large extent on this work, but it is an attempt to present the rules in the simplest possible form. Nevertheless, practically all the rules have exceptions and readers may feel that the rules are so complex that it would be easier to go back to the idea of learning the stress for each word individually. In order to decide on stress placement, it is necessary to make use of some or all of the following information: 1. Whether the word is morphologically simple, or whether it is complex as a result either of containing one or more affixes (that is, prefixes or suffixes) or of being a compound word. 2. The grammatical category to which the word belongs (noun, verb, adjective, etc.). 3. The number of syllables in the word. 4. The phonological structure of those syllables. The rules for complex words are different from those for simple words and these will be dealt later. Obviously, single-syllable words present no problems – if they are pronounced in isolation they are said with tonic strong (i.e. primary) stress. Two-syllable words Verbs: Here the choice is still simple: either the first or the second syllable will be stressed – not both. We will look first at verbs. The basic rule is that if the second syllable of the verb contains a long vowel or diphthong, or if it ends with more than one consonant, that second syllable is stressed. Thus: ‘apply’ /əˈplaɪ/ ‘arrive’ /əˈraɪv/ ’attract’ /əˈtrækt/ ’assist’ /əˈsɪst/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 67 If the final syllable contains a short vowel and one (or no) final consonant, the first syllable is stressed. Thus: ‘enter’ /ˈəntə/ ‘envy’ /ˈenvi/ ’open’ /ˈəʊpən/ ’equal’ /ˈi:kwəl/ A final syllable is also unstressed if it contains /əʊ/ (e.g. ‘follow’ /ˈfɒˈləʊ/, ‘borrow’ /ˈbɒˈrəʊ/. Most two-syllable verbs that seem to be exceptions to the above might be interpreted as being morphologically complex (e.g. ‘permit’ /pəˈmɪt/ = ‘per’+ ‘mit), or we could simply list all such verbs as exceptions. Exercise 3 When you hear the number, pronounce the word with the appropriate stress. You will then hear the correct pronunciation, which you should repeat. Place the stress mark and then write the spelling for these words. 1. dɪsi:v 6. əbʤekt 2. ʃɑ:pən 7. kɒŋkə 3. kəlekt 8. rɪkͻ:d 4. prənaʊns 9. pɒlɪʃ 5. kɒpi 10. dɪpend Adjectives Two-syllable simple adjectives are stressed according to the same rule, giving: ‘lovely’ /ˈlʌvli/ ‘even’ /ˈi:vɪņ/ ‘hollow’ /ˈhɒləʊ/ ’divine’ /dɪˈvaɪn/ ’correct’ /kəˈrekt/ ’alive’ /əˈlaɪv/ As with most stress rules, there are exceptions, for example ‘honest /ˈɒnɪst/, ‘perfect’ /ˈpɜ:fɪkt/ or /ˈpɜ:fekt/, both of which end with two consonants bur are stressed on the first syllable. Exercise 4 When you hear the number, pronounce the word with the appropriate stress. You will then hear the correct pronunciation, which you should repeat. Place the stress mark and then write the spelling for these words. 1. i:zi 6. jeləʊ 2. kəmpli:t 7. ɜ:li 3. meɪʤə 8. səblaɪm 4. ələʊn 9. hevi 5. bɪləʊ 10. əlaɪv Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 68 Nouns Nouns require a different rule: if the second syllable contains a short vowel the stress will usually come on the first syllable. Otherwise it will be on the second syllable. ‘money’ /ˈmʌni/ ‘product’ /ˈprɒdʌkt/ ‘larynx’ /ˈlærɪŋks/ ’estate’ /ɪˈsteɪt/ ’ballon’ /bəˈlu:n/ ’design’ /dɪˈzaɪn/ Other two-syllable words such as adverbs and prepositions seem to behave like verbs and adjectives. Exercise 5 When you hear the number, pronounce the word with the appropriate stress. You will then hear the correct pronunciation, which you should repeat. Place the stress mark and then write the spelling for these words. 1. bɪʃəp 6. ɒfɪs 2. æspekt 7. əreɪ 3. əfeə 8. pətrəʊl 4. kɑ:pɪt 9. dentɪst 5. dɪfi:t 10. ͻ:təm Three-syllable words Here we find a more complicated picture. In verbs, if the last syllable contains a pəˈshort vowel and ends with not more than one consonant, that syllable will be unstressed, and stress will be placed on the preceding (penultimate) syllable. Thus: ‘entertain’ /entəˈteɪn/ ’resurrect’ /rezəˈrekt/ Nouns require a different rule. Here, if the final syllable contains a short vowel or əʊ, it is unstressed; if the syllable preceding this final syllable contains a long vowel or diphthong, or if it ends with more than one consonant, that middle syllable will be stressed. Thus: ‘mimosa’ /mɪˈməʊzə/ ‘potato’ /pəˈteɪtəʊ/ ’disaster’ /dɪˈzɑ:stə/ ’synopsis’ /sɪˈnɒpsɪs/ If the final syllable contains a short vowel and the middle syllable contains a short vowel and ends with not more than one consonant, both final and middle syllables are unstressed and the first syllable is stressed: ‘quantity’ /ˈkwɒntɪti/ ‘cinema’ /ˈsɪnɪmə/ ’emperor’ /ˈemprə/ ’custody’ /ˈkʌstədi/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 69 Most of the above rules show stress tending to go on syllables containing a long vowel or diphthong and/or ending with more than one consonant. However, three-syllable simple nouns are different. If the final syllable is of this type, the stress will usually be placed on the first syllable. The last syllable is usually quite prominent so that in some cases it could be said to have secondary stress. ‘intellect’ /ˈɪntɪlekt/ ‘alkali’ /ˈælkəlaɪ/ or /ˈælklaɪ/ ’marigold’ /ˈmærɪgəʊld/ ’stalactite’ /ˈstæləktaɪt/ Adjectives seem to need the same rule, to produce stress patterns such as: ‘opportune’ /ˈɒpətju:n/ ‘derelict’ /ˈderɪlɪkt/ ’insolent’ /ˈɪnslənt/ ’anthropoid’ /ˈænθrəpͻɪd/ The above rules do not, of course, cover all English words. They apply only to major categories of lexical words (nouns, verbs and adjectives in this chapter), not to grammatical or function words such as articles and prepositions. Exercise 6 When you hear the number, pronounce the word with the appropriate stress. You will then hear the correct pronunciation, which you should repeat. Place the stress mark and then write the spelling for these words. Verbs 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. entəteɪn rezərekt əbændən dɪlɪvə ɪntərʌpt 6. ɪlɪsɪt 7. kɒməndɪə 8. ɪmæʤɪn 9. dɪtɜ:mɪn 10. sepəreɪt Adjectives 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. ɪmpͻ:tņt ɪnͻ:məs derɪlɪkt desɪml æbnͻ:ml 6. ɪnslənt 7. fæntæstɪk 8. negətɪv 9. ækjərət 10. ʌnlaɪkli Nouns 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. fɜ:nɪtʃə dɪzɑ:stə dɪsaɪpl æmbjələns kwɒntɪti 6. kəθi:drəl 7. hɒləkͻ:st 8. trænzɪstə 9. æksɪdņt 10. təmɑ:təʊ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 70 COMPLEX WORD STRESS In the last section the nature of stress was explained and some broad general rules were given for deciding which syllable in a word should receive primary stress. The words that were described were called “simple” words. “Simple” in this context means “not composed of more than one grammatical unit”, so that, for example, the word ‘care’ is simple while ‘careful’ and ‘careless’ (being composed of two grammatical units each) are complex; ‘carefully’ and ‘carelessness’ are also complex, and are composed of three grammatical units each. Unfortunately, it is often difficult to decide on whether a word should be treated as complex or simple. The majority of English words of more than one syllable (polysyllabic words) have come from other languages whose way of constructing words is easily recognisable; for example, we can see how combining ‘mit’ with the prefixes ‘per-‘, ‘sub-‘, ‘com-‘ produced ‘permit’, ‘submit’, ‘commit’, words which have come into English from Latin. Similarly, Greek has given us ‘catalogue, ‘analogue’, ‘dialogue’, ‘monologue’, in which the prefixes ‘ cata-‘, ‘ana-‘, ‘dia-‘, ‘mono-‘ are recognisable. But we cannot automatically treat the separate grammatical units of other languages as separate grammatical units of English. Complex words are of two major types: words made from a basic stem word with the addition of an affix, and compound words, which are made of two (or occasionally more) independent English words (e.g. ‘ice-cream’, ‘armchair’). We will look first at the words made with affixes; these will be called affix words. Affixes are of two sorts in English: prefixes, which come before the stem (e.g. prefix ‘un-‘ + stem ‘pleasant’ ’unpleasant’) and suffixes, which come after the stem (e.g. stem ‘good’ + suffix ‘-ness’ ‘goodness’). Affixes will have one of three possible effects on word stress: 1. The affix itself receives the primary stress (e.g. ‘ semi-‘ + ‘circle’ /ˈsɜ:kl/ ‘semicircle’ /ˈsemɪsɜ:kl/ ‘-ality’ + ‘person’ /ˈpɜ:sņ/ ‘personality’ /pɜ:sņˈælɪti/ ‘personality’ 2. The word is stressed just as if the affix was not there (e.g. ‘pleasant’ /'plezņt/, ‘unpleasant /ʌnˈplezņt/ ‘market’ /ˈmɑ:kɪt/, ‘marketing’ /ˈmɑ:kɪtɪŋ/ 3. The stress remains on the stem, not the affix, but is shifted to a different syllable (e.g. ‘magnet’ /'mægnət/, ‘magnetic’ /mæg'netɪk/ SUFFIXES Suffixes are easier to deal with than prefixes. However, there are so many of these that it will only be possible here to examine a small proportion of them; we will concentrate on those which are common and productive (that is, are applied to a considerable number of stems and could be applied to make new English words). In the case of the others, foreign learners would probably be better advised to learn the stem + affix combination as an individual item. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 71 Suffixes carrying primary stress themselves In the examples given, which seem to be the most common, the primary stress is on the first syllable of the suffix. If the stem consists of more than one syllable there will be a secondary stress on one of the syllables of the stem. This cannot fall on the last syllable of the stem, and is, if necessary, moved to an earlier syllable. For example, in ‘Japan’ /dʒəˈpæn/ the primary stress is on the last syllable, but when we add the stress-carrying suffix ‘-ese’ the primary stress is on the suffix and the secondary stress in placed not on the second syllable but on the first: ‘Japanese’ /ˌdʒæpəˈni:z/. ‘-ain’ (for verbs only) ‘-ee’ ‘-eer’ ‘-ese’ ‘-ette’ ‘-esque’. ‘-ique’ entertain /ˌentəˈteɪn/ refugee /ˌrefjʊˈdʒi:/ mountaineer /ˌmaʊntɪˈnɪə/ Portuguese /ˌpɔ:tʃəˈgi:z/ cigarette /ˌsɪgrˈet/ picturesque /ˌpɪktʃə'resk/ ascertain /ˌæsəˈteɪn/ evacuee /ɪˌvækjuˈi:/ volunteer /ˌvɒlənˈtɪə/ journalese /ˌdʒɜ:nlˈi:z/ launderette /ˌlɔ:ndrˈet/ unique /ˌju:'ni:k/ Suffixes that do not affect stress placement ‘-able’ ‘-age’ ‘-al’ ‘-en’ ‘-ful’ ‘-ing’ ‘-ish’ comfort /'kʌmfət/ anchor /'æŋkə/ refuse (verb) /rɪ'fju:z/ wide /waɪd/ wonder /'wʌndə/ amaze /ə'meɪz/ devil /'devl/ comfortable /'kʌmfətəbl/ anchorage /'æŋkrɪdʒ/ refusal /rɪ'fju:zl/ widen /'waɪdn/ wonderful /'wʌndəfl/ amazing /ə'meɪzɪŋ/ devilish /'devlɪʃ/ (This is the rule for adjectives; verbs with stems of more than one syllable always have the stress on the syllable immediately preceding ‘ish’, e.g. ‘replenish’ /rɪ'plenɪʃ/, ‘demolish’ /dɪ'mɒlɪʃ/. ‘-like’ ‘-less’ ‘-ly’ ‘-ment’ ‘-ness’ ‘-ous’ ‘-fy’ ‘-wise’ ‘-y’ (adjective or noun) bird /bɜ:d/ power /'paʊə/ hurried /hʌrɪd/ punish /'pʌnɪʃ/ yellow /'jeləʊ/ poison /'pɔɪzn/ glory /'glɔ:ri/ other / 'ʌðə/ fun /fʌn/ birdlike /'bɜ:dlaɪk/ powerless /'paʊələs/ hurriedly /’hʌrɪdli/ punishment /'pʌnɪʃmənt/ yellowness /'jeləʊnəs/ poisonous /'pɔɪznəs/ glorify /'glɔ:rɪfaɪ/ otherwise /'ʌðəwaɪz/ funny /'fʌni/ Primary stress on the last syllable of the stem ‘-eous’ ‘-graphy’ ‘-ial ‘-ic’ ‘-ion’ -ious’ ‘-ity’ ‘-ive’ advantage /əd'vɑ:ntɪdʒ/ photo /'fəʊtəʊ/ proverb /'prɒvɜ:b/ climate/'klaɪmɪt/ perfect /'pɜ:fɪkt/ injure /'ɪndʒə/ tranquil /'træŋkwɪl/ reflex /'ri:fleks/ advantageous /ædvən'teɪdʒəs/ photography /fə'tɒgrəfi/ proverbial /prə'vɜ:bɪəl/ climatic /klaɪ'mætɪk/ perfection /pə'fekʃn/ injurious /ɪn'dʒʊəriəs/ tranquillity /træŋ'kwɪlɪti/ reflexive /rɪ'fleksɪv/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 72 The suffixes ‘-ance’. ‘-ant’ and ‘-ary’ When these suffixes are attached to single-syllable stems, the stress is almost always places on the stem. When the stem has two syllables the stress is sometimes on the first, sometimes on the second syllable of the stem. If the final syllable of the stem contains a long vowel or diphthong, or if it ends with more than one consonant, that syllable receives the stress. For example: ‘importance /ɪm'pɔ:tns/; ‘centenary’ /sen'ti:nri/. Otherwise the syllable before the last one receives the stress: ‘consonant’ /'kɒnsnənt/; ‘military’ /'mɪlɪtri/. We will not consider words with stems of more than two syllables. Such words are, from the point of view of this course, too complex and uncommon for it to be worth attempting to write rules. PREFIXES We will only deal briefly with prefixes. Their effect on stress does not have the comparative regularity, independence and predictability of suffixes, and there is no prefix of one or two syllables that always carries primary stress. Consequently, the best treatment seems to be to say that stress in words with prefixes is governed by the same rules as those for words without prefixes. Exercise 7: Stress-carrying suffixes When you hear the number, pronounce the word with stress on the suffix. You will then hear the correct pronunciation which you should repeat. Write the words in phonetic symbols. 1. –ain: entertain 4. –ese: Portuguese 2. –ee: refugee 5. –ette: cigarette 3. –eer: mountaineer 6. –esque: picturesque When you hear the stem word, say he word with the given suffix, putting the stress on that suffix. In these examples, a secondary stress comes on the penultimate syllable of the stem. Write the words in phonetic symbols. employ + -ee absent + -ee engine + eer profit + -eer Sudan + ese Pekin + -ese usher + ette statue + -ette (statuette) Exercise 8. Neutral suffixes When you hear the stem word, add the suffix without changing the stress. Then write the words in phonetic symbols. comfort + -able power + -less anchor + -age hurried + -ly refuse + -al (refusal) punish + -ment wide + -en (widen) yellow + -ness wonder + -ful poison + -ous amaze + -ing (amazing) glory + -fy (glorify) devil + -ish other + -wise bird + -like fun + -y (funny) Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 73 Exercise 9. Stress-moving suffixes When you hear the stem word, say it with the suffix added and put the stress on the last syllable of the stem. advantage + -eous (advantageous) injure + -ious (injurious) photo + -graphy tranquil + -ity (tranquility) proverb + -ial refle + -ive climate + -ic (climatic) embryo + -logy COMPOUND WORDS The main characteristic of the compound words is that it can be analysed into two words, both of which can exist independently as English words. As with many of the distinctions being made in connection with stress, there are areas of uncertainty. For example, it could be argued that ‘photograph’ may be divided into two independent words, ‘photo’ and ‘graph’; yet we usually do not regard it as a compound, but as an affix word. (If someone drew a graph displaying numerical information about photos, this would perhaps be called a ‘photo-graph’ ad the word would be regarded as a compound). Compounds are written in different ways; sometimes they are written as one word, e.g. ‘armchair’, ‘sunflower’, sometimes with the words separated by a hyphen, e.g. ‘gearchange’, ‘fruit-cake’, and sometimes with two words separated by a space, e.g. ‘desk lamp’, ‘battery charger’. In this last case there would, of course be no indication to the foreign learner that the pair of words was to be treated as a compound. There is no clear dividing line between two-word compounds and pairs of words that simply happen to occur together quite frequently. As far as stress is concerned, the question is quite simple. When is primary stress placed on the first word of the compound and when on the second? Both patterns are found. A simple rule can be used, though it is not completely reliable. 1. If the first part of the compound is (in a broad sense) adjectival, the stress goes on the second element, with a secondary stress on the first. For example: ‘loudpeaker’ /ˌlaʊd'spi:kə/ ‘bad-tempered’ /ˌbæd'tempəd/ ‘second-class’ /ˌsekənd'klɑ:s/ ‘three-wheeler’ /ˌθri:'wi:lə/ There are many exceptions to this rule; the majority of these exceptions have in common the fact that they have become more like simple words and less like compounds with the passage of time, and their meaning is no longer easily derived from their constituent parts. For example, ‘greenhouse’ /'gri:nhaʊs/ is derived from ‘green’ and ‘house’, but a greenhouse is not green and is not really a house. Other examples are: ‘gentleman’, ‘bluebell’, ‘background’, ‘broadcast’ – all of these are of the form adjective + noun. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 74 2. If, however, the first element is (in a broad sense) a noun, the stress goes on the first element. For example: ‘typewriter’ /'taɪpraɪtə/ ‘car-ferry’ /'kɑ:feri/ ‘sunrise’ /'sʌnraɪz/ ‘suitcase’ /'sju:tkeɪs/ ‘tea-cup’ /'ti:kʌp/ Exercise 10 When you hear the number, say the item. Then write the words in phonetic symbols. a) First element adjectival, stress on second element. 1. loudspeaker 4. second-class 2. bad-tempered 5. three-wheeler 3. headquarters b) First element nominal, stress on first element. 1. typewriter 4. suitcase 2. car-ferry 5. tea-cup 3. sunrise c) Mixture of type (a) and (b). 1. long-suffering 4. red-blooded 2. gunman 5. gear-box 3. shoelace 6. overweight Variable stress It would be wrong to imagine that the stress pattern is always fixed and unchanging in English words. Stress position may vary for one of two reasons: either as a result of the stress on other words occurring next to the word in question, or because not all speakers of RP agree on the placement of stress in some words. A well-known example is ‘controversy’, which is pronounced by some speakers as /'kɒntrəvɜ:si/ and by others as /kən'trɒvəsi/, it would be quite wrong to say that one version was correct and one incorrect. Other examples of different possibilities are ‘ice-cream’, ‘kilometer’ (either /'kɪləmi:tə/ or /kɪ'lɒmɪtə/ and ‘formidable’ /'fɔ:mɪdəbl/ or /fə'mɪdəbl/ Word-class pairs One aspect of word stress is best treated as a separate issue. There are several dozen pairs of two-syllable words with identical spelling which differ from each other in stress placement, apparently according to word class (noun, verb or adjective). All appear to consist of prefix + stem. We shall treat them as a special type of words and give them the following rule: when a pair of prefix-plus-stem words exists, both members of which are spelt identically, one of which is a verb and the other is either a noun or an adjective, the stress will be placed on the second syllable of the verb but on the first syllable of the noun or adjective. Some common examples are given below: Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 75 (V = verb, A = adjective, N = noun): ‘abstract’ /'æbstrækt/ (A), /æbs'trækt/ (V) ‘conduct’ /'kɒndʌkt/ (N), /kən'dʌkt (V) ‘ contract’ /'kɒntrækt/ (N), /kən'trækt/ (V) ‘contrast’ /'kɒntrɑ:st/ (N), /kən'trɑ:st/ (V) ‘desert’ /'dezət/ (N), /dɪ'zɜ:t/ (V) ‘escort’ /'eskɔ:t/ (N), /ɪs'kɔ:t/ (V) ‘export’ /'ekspɔ:t/ (N), /ɪks'pɔ:t/ (V) ‘import’ /'ɪmpɔ:t/ (N), /ɪm'pɔ:t/ (V) ‘insult’ /'ɪnsʌlt/ (N), /ɪn'sʌlt/ (V) ‘object’ /'ɒbdʒɪkt/ (N), /əb'dʒekt/ (V) ‘perfect’ /'pɜ:fɪkt/ (A), /pə'fekt/ (V) ‘permit’ /'pɜ:mɪt/ (N), /pə'mɪt/ (V) ‘present’ /'preznt/ (N, A), /prɪ'zent/ (V) ‘produce’ /'prɒdju:s/ (N), /prə'dju:s/ (V) ‘protest’ /'prəʊtest/ (N), /prə'test/ (V) ‘rebel’ /'rebl/ (N), /rɪ'bel/ (V) ‘record’ /'rekɔ:d/ (N), /rɪ'kɔ:d/ (V) ‘subject’ /'sʌbdʒɪkt/ (N), /səb'dʒekt/ (V) With verbs we very often have suffixes, such as ‘-ed’, ‘-ing’. eg. ‘conducting.’ and we prefer to have the stressed syllable at the stem, before the suffix. Nouns are very often preceded by a/the, which are unstressed, that is why we prefer to have the stress in the first syllable. e.g. ‘the progress’ /ðə 'prəʊgres/. Exercise 11. Word-class pairs You will hear the number of the item and its word-class. Stress the second syllable if it is a verb; stress the first syllable if it is a noun or adjective. Then write the words in phonetic symbols. 1. abstract (Adjective) 10. object (Noun) 2. conduct (Verb) 11. perfect (Adjective) 3. contract (Noun) 12. permit (Verb) 4. contrast (Verb) 13. present (Adjective) 5. desert (Noun) 14. produce (Verb) 6. escort (Noun) 15. protest (Noun) 7. export (Verb) 16. rebel (Verb) 8. import (Noun) 17. record (Noun) 9. insult (Verb) 18. subject (Noun) Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 76 Learning Notes UNIT 7 Phonology deals with function of sounds within the language distinctive sound: defined set of sounds duality of patterning: system of speech sound and their usage limited set of sounds: but we can arrange these to ever new utterances phonemes: the smallest distinctive element of a language individual distinct sound / distinctive speech sound /pet/ /pɒt/ /pʌt/ /pɪn/ /bɪn/ Commutation: the test to locate phonemes. Put within dashes: /p/ Phonetics: tries to differentiate among the sounds with the highest possible degree of accuracy without regard for the influence of sound on the meaning of an utterance. Allophones: Merely variation in pronunciation of the same phoneme Do not change the meaning of the word. Complementary distribution: Where one sound of the pair occurs, the other does not. /p/ [ [ ] pill (aspirated) ] sprint (unaspirated) Distinctive features [ ] and [ ] are allophones of /p/ voiced /v / voiceless /f / Redundant features These sounds have a specific feature, but do not constitute minimal pairs consonants nasal nonnasal vowels + nasal - negative nasal band bar both are allophone of the same vowel Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 77 Learning Notes Assimilation rules: nasalization of vowels preceding +nasal consonants coarticulation: features of phonemes spread in anticipation of sounds or perseveration of articulatory processes. Rules of Feature addition rules: Features are added to phonemes when they occur occur in a specific phonetic context. Phonology Segment-deletion and addition rules: the car has /ðəkɑ:hæz/ the car is /ðəkɑ:rɪs/ Movement (methathesis) rules: In some English dialects phonemes are rearranged. Methathesis: change in the order of two sounds in a word, eg /flɪm/ for /fɪlm/ film. Methathesis sometimes occurs in the speech of language learners but it may also occur with native speakers. When a metathesized form becomes commonly and regularly used by most native speakers of a language, it may lead to a change in the word. For example, Modern English bird developed by methathesis from Old English brid “young bird”. Assimilation: A significance difference in natural connected speech is the way that sounds belonging to one word can cause changes in sounds belonging to neighbouring words. Assuming that we know how the phonemes of a particular word would be realized when the word was pronounced in isolation, when we find a phoneme realized differently as a result of being near some other phoneme belonging to a neighbouring word we call this an instance of assimilation. Example: consonantal sounds at the end of a word are often influenced by the following consonant. Thus / gυd / becomes / gug / under the influence of the following velar consonant. Similarly / wɔn / may become /wəm/ before a following / m /. This is known as assimilation. It does not necessarily always occur, but may do so, and with some speakers is habitual. There are two types of assimilation: Regressive assimilation: If the last consonant of the first word changes to become like the initial consonant of the second word in some way, the assimilation is called regressive (the phoneme that comes first is affected by the one that comes after it). Example: that person / ðæ t pɜ:sņ / becomes / ðæp pɜ:sņ / light blue /laɪt blu: / becomes /laɪp blu: / meat pie /mi:t paɪ / becomes /mi:p paɪ / that case /ðæ t keis/ becomes /ðæ k keis/ becomes /braɪk kʌlə / bright colour /braɪt kʌlə / quite good /kwaɪt gυd / becomes /kwaɪk gυd / Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 78 Learning Notes Progressive assimilation: If the initial consonant of the second word changes to become like the last consonant of the first word, the assimilation is called progressive. This is rightward assimilation; the phone assimilation to a following phone. (English –s). Example: The English plural is either /z/ or /s/ when it occurs after a non-sibilant sound. The voicing feature is taken from the final consonant of the base. books /bυks/ The pronunciation of the –ed suffix in English: stopped /stɒpt/ "tape" pronounced as "tate" rooms /ru:mz/ named /neɪmd/ Feature addition rules: Adding a whole new segment: pin pʰɪn] random: athlete pronounced /’æ l i: t/ systematic: plural /z/ ->/´z/ after another sibilant (s-type sound) pronounced / ’kl æ sɪz/ classes Segment-deletion rule: dropping a whole segment Examples: random: pronounced /pɪtʃə/ picture slow-like pronounced /sləυlɪ/ a friend of mine systematic: bomb hymn pronounced /əfrendə’maɪn/ pronounced /bɒm / lamb pronounced /læm/ pronounced /hɪm / damn pronounced /dæm/ cupboard pronounced /’kʌpbəd/ Segment-addition rule: the car has /ðəkɑ:hæz/ the car is /ðəkɑ:rɪs/ Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 79 UNIT 8 MORPHOLOGY Whereas phonology studies the smallest distinctive elements of a language, morphology is the study of the smallest meaning units of a language. This smallest meaning unit is also called a morpheme. The overriding rule is that a morpheme must be able to stand alone, i.e. to yield meaning independently of its general context. Let's start out by taking a closer look at the term 'smallest meaning unit' and how it may be understood. We are inclined to think that a 'word' fits the definition of 'smallest meaning unit'. However, not all words are single meaning units. Some words are 'nonsense words' and lack any meaning whatsoever. These are invented words mostly, for example "brillig", or "chorps". Then there are words that are composed of more than one meaning unit. Examples of words which consist of more than one meaning unit are: blue-berry, auto-bus, hence-forth Although a word may be a smallest meaning unit, such as 'blue', it does not necessarily have to be one, as in 'blueberry'. Neither can syllables be regarded as the smallest meaning units. They are, in the first place, phonological units. They consist of a nucleus, which is a vowel, a diphthong, or a vowel-like consonant, and one or several satellites. A satellite is always a consonant. (satellites) + nucleus + (satellites) (satellites) + nucleus + (satellites) (C1, C2,...) + V + (C1, C2,...) (C1, C2,...) + V + (C1, C2,...) M a g n u m Words may consist of several syllables, but not all of them are necessarily meaning units. Example: Mas-sa-chu-setts, po-lice, mo-ther Thus, while a syllable may be a smallest meaning unit, such as the syllable 'end' in the word 'endless', it does not necessarily have to be one. As we can see, a morpheme cannot necessarily be confined to a particular form of a word or a syllable. The study of morphology seeks to, in the first place, determine the precise form (Greek- morph) that an elemental meaning takes. However, it does, as we will see later, remain within the abstract domain, in cases where form either varies or where form is simply nonexistent. Morphemes are also referred to as the minimal linguistic signs; you may also come across the term moneme, which is the French term for morpheme. The above may have given you an understanding of why linguists have been engaged in endless debates over the definition of the word "word". The term remains ambiguous, and we should try as best to avoid it. After all, linguistic terminology is available to describe linguistic units in higher detail. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 80 8.1. TYPES OF MORPHEMES We can discern different types of morphemes when analyzing a text corpus. The process of sorting out which morpheme is which is called distributional analysis. 8.1.1. Grammatical Classification Grammatically speaking, there are two classes of morphemes: * Free morphemes: these may occur on their own, they can be used freely according to the rules of sentence structure, for example "boy, tree, church, go, leave, love." * Bound morphemes: These are an exception to the rule that a morpheme must be able to stand alone; they have no meaning of their own, but add substantial meaning to other morphemes to which they are attached. They are used for various purposes, such as inflection. Example: "-ing, -er" in "working, worker.", where "work" is a free morpheme accompanied by various bound morphemes, namely suffixes. Derivational morphemes can be added to a word to create (derive) a new word. The addition of –ness to ‘happy’, for example, to give ‘happiness’. 8.1.2. Morphological Classification Morphological classification will help you to understand exceptions and different occurrences of morphemes. Homophonous morphemes sound the same, although their morphological function and visual realization may be different, for example the "in" in "inn", and the "in" in "incongruous". Discontinuous morphemes. A morpheme of this type does not appear in one continuous string in a word, but is split up into fragments. For example, the morpheme "ge- and -t" in the German word "ge-reis-t". Still, both parts belong together. It does not exist in present day English. A portmanteau morpheme is the result of a fusion of two other morphemes. For example, the French "au" is a fusion of the former morphemes "à" and "le". Unique or residual morphemes. These are a further exception to the rule that a morpheme must be able to stand alone. Representing bound morphemes, unique or residual morphemes are, by themselves, meaningless and gain meaning only in combination with other morphemes. Examples for this kind of morpheme are the "cran" in "cranberry", or the "mit" in "transmit", "commit", and so on. Zero morphemes. As already mentioned above, there do exist meaningful grammatical features that are not materialized, neither in writing nor in sound waves, in a consistent manner. The plural of a noun, for instance, is not always formed by adding a bound morpheme 's' to the word. Some words are not changed at all when meaning the plural. We usually simply understand which case the speaker meant from the context of the conversation. One such example is the word 'sheep', designating singular as well as plural sheep. The zero morpheme 'plural' would, in this case, designate the abstract concept of the plurality. It then means the plural sheep as opposed to the singular sheep. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 81 8.1.3. Morph, morpheme, and allomorph Note that any language has a register of morphemes, the physical realizations of which are called morphs. While morphemes remain ideal abstract units, the corresponding morphs may show some variation. In the case of the plural morpheme, various realizations are possible. These variations sound and look differently: /z/ in "dogs, beds"; /s/ in "cats"; /iz/ in "garages". All three morphs are different representations of the same morpheme of plurality. Several morphs that belong to the same morpheme are also called allomorphs: variants of one morpheme. In morphological transcription, morphs are commonly put in between braces. The plural morph in "cats" thus becomes {cat}+{s} in morphological transcription. 8.2. MORPHOLOGY AND WORD-FORMATION We have already seen that the term word is highly ambiguous. Now we come to consider it anew. Commonly, various grammatical forms of linguistic units such as "sing", "sang", "sung" are referred to as various words. In all three words of our example, however, we are confronted with various forms of the same unit, namely "sing". We will use a more specific term for these "pure" linguistic units than word. We thus refer to the more abstract units, regardless of their representation as phonological or orthographic units in various inflectional forms, not as words, but lexemes; in linguistic transcription they are distinguished by capitals. Hence the word "cut" can offer three forms of the lexeme CUT: present, past and participle. The sum of the lexemes of a language is called the lexicon. The lexicon of a language is not fixed. New entries may occur, or old ones might become extinct. New entries are called neologisms, if they are created from existing resources of the language. New entries can also be borrowed from other languages. As for the process in the former case, we are able to create new words by applying the morphological rules of our language. Morphological rules apply to both inflection and word formation. 8.2.1. Inflection Inflection is the process of adding an affix to a word or changing it in some other way according to the rules of the grammar of a language. There are two kinds of inflection: The declension of nouns, adjectives, and pronouns, and the conjugation of verbs. There is a set of morphological rules which we apply in order to form the correct tenses or cases. We combine free morphemes with bound morphemes, the latter marking tense, gender, number, case, and so forth. Bound morphemes like {to}, which appears as a marker of the infinitive, are also used in order to form correct sentences. These markers help to create sentences that are correct according to the syntactic rules of a language. We see here that morphology and syntax cannot be regarded as entirely different disciplines. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 82 The inflectional morphemes do not carry any meaning on their own. Examples: Declension: Noun: boy: boys, boy’s, boys’ Adjective: big: bigger Pronouns: I: me, my, mine Conjugation: Verb: work: works, worked, working 8.2.2. Word formation 8.2.2.1. Derivation There are morphemes in English that allow us to change words. These are derivational morphemes: new words are derived in the process. Derivational morphemes are affixes. If affixes are added to the front of a word, we speak of prefixes like in-, a-. If they are attached to the end of the word, they are called suffixes, such as -able, -ish. Derivational morphemes may or may not cause a change of the grammatical class of the word. In any event, the following morphological rules apply to most cases cover the main changes brought about by affixes. We speak of word formation as the productive feature of a language. By changing one element, new classes of words can be produced. NOUN TO ADJECTIVE {boy}+{ish} {virtu}+{ous} {Elizabeth}+{an} {alcohol}+{ic} VERB TO ADJECTIVE NOUN TO NO CHANGE IN NOUN TO ADVERB VERB CLASS {acquitt}+{al} {exact}+{ly} {moral}+{ize} {a}+{moral} {clear}+{ance} {quiet}+{ly} {vaccin}+{ate} {mono}+{theism} {confer}+{ence} {brand}+{ish} {ex}+{wife} {predict}+{ion} {sub}+{minimal} 8.2.2.2. Compounding We may also create new words by combining two free morphemes. The product of such a process is called a compound. While German is notorious for compounding a multitude of units (e.g., Weihnachtsbaumschmuckvertriebsorganisationshandbuchverkäufer), compounds in English usually do not exceed two units. In a compound, the two morphemes always serve two different purposes. The initial morpheme is also called the head of the compound, the following is the body. The head of the compound always determines the body semantically, e.g., {black}+{bird} -> a black bird, whereas the body usually determines the grammatical class of the whole compound. There are various possible combinations of English compounds: -ADJECTIVE ADJECTIVE- {bitter}+{sweet} NOUN{head}+{strong} VERB {carry}+{all} -NOUN {poor}+{house} {rain}+{bow} -{pick}+{pocket} -VERB {high}+{born} {spoon}+{feed} {sleep}+{walk} Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 83 8.2.2.3. Other processes of word-formation Apart from the above mentioned morphological processes, there are also other ways of creating new words: Invention: Xerox, Exxon. Acronymy: NATO, Unicef. Reduplication: zigzag, chitchat. Blending: smog, brunch. Clipping: bus, auto, flu. 8.3. WORD CLASSES AND SENTENCE FUNCTIONS We can divide words into two broad types: content words versus function words. 8.3.1. Content Words. Content words are words which refer to a thing, quality, state, or action and which have meaning (lexical meaning) when the words are used alone. Nouns: John, room, dog, London Verbs: see, retire, laugh Adjectives: fast, deep, yellow, big Adverbs: really, completely, well, much, yesterday Interjections: alas, oh, ugh, phew Speakers can add new Content Words to the language, and so they are also called the "open class" vocabulary, because it is open to additions. 8.3.2. Function Words Function words are words which have little meaning on their own, but which show grammatical relationships in and between sentences (grammatical meaning). Pronouns: I, you, he, which, him Conjunctions: and, but, because Preposition: in, on, at, between, in front of Auxiliaries: do, does, have, be Determiners: the, a, this, my, that In general speakers cannot add new Function Words to the language, and so this is the "closed class" vocabulary. Some difficulties remain, such as with adverbs: one cannot, for example, exchange the adverbs "very" and "quickly" freely in a sentence: *”he ran very quickly” versus “he ran quickly.” To avoid this problem, the subcategory of the intensifiers is used to distinguish adverbs such as 'very', from the common adverbs. The classing of words gives us overall categorical information about those words. Note that words from the same class can take on different functions depending upon their use in a sentence. Nouns, for example, may appear words within the context of a sentence, such as subject, object, etc., this is called functional information. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 84 EXERCISES 1. How many morphemes do you find in each of the following word? Which ones are free and which are bound morphemes? a. sofa f. running b. oranges g. actor c. rainbows h. doctor d. hippopotamus i. revisited e. looked j. potato 2. Identify the morphemes in the following words, indicating where a spelling change may be necessary when one breaks the word down into its constituents. a. equalizers f. predetermination b. inclination g. dangerously c. befriending h. unbelievable d. transportation i. protection e. endearment j. dehumanize 3. Pick out the bound morphemes in the following words and say whether they are derivational morphemes (D) or inflectional morphemes (I). a. inflectional f. respectably b. terrifying g. represents c. hospitalized h. stifles d. regularizes i. previewed e. reviewers j. submariners 4. What are the main allomorphs of each of the following morphemes? Provide examples to illustrate each of the allomorphs. a. –s (as in the verb ‘looks’) b. –ed (as in the verb ‘looked’) c. in- (as in words where the morpheme means ‘not’, for example ‘in + eligible’) d. –er (as in words where the morpheme is used to form nouns from verbs, for example, ‘work + er’ e. the (before ‘apple’ and ‘book’) 5. Segment the following words into free and bound morphemes. Say in each case whether the bound morpheme changes the word class of the free morpheme. a. unbearable. b. moralised c. tranquilisers d. imprudently e. width Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 85 WORKSHEET 6 – MORPHOLOGY Name: _______________________________________ e-mail: _____________________ Please check your answers carefully. Question Solution Points 2 Explain the difference between a syllable and a morpheme. 2 What is an allomorph? Give an example. (You are not required to use phonetic transcription). 3 Is there anything in the definition of the terms 'morph' and 'phone' that could be compared? a) Words always consist of one meaning unit. True False True or False? 3 b) Words always consist of one lexeme. True False c) Lexemes always consist of one meaning unit. True False Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 86 Learning Notes UNIT 8 MORPHOLOGY Morphology studies the smallest distinctive element of a language A word may fit this definition, however - not all words are single meaning units e.g. ‘nonsense words’ - There are words composed of more than one meaning unit. e.g. blue-berry morpheme A syllable cannot be regarded as the smallest meaning unit, they are phonological units. Not all the syllables are meaning units. e.g. po-lice Some syllables can be meaning units. e.g. end-less. 1. Free morpheme: It can be used on its own: work, pen Grammatical 2. Bound morpheme: It is never used alone, but must be used with another morpheme, e.g. as an affix or combining form. e.g. the suffix –ing: writing the suffix –er: worker the s in plurals: boys 1. Homophonous: Morphemes that sound the same, but their morphological function and visual realization may be different. e.g. in in inn and incongruous 2. Discontinuous: It is a morpheme that is interrupted by the insertion of another morphological unit. e.g. “ge- and –t” in the German word “ge-reis-t” TYPES OF MORPHEMES 3. Portmanteau: It is the result of a fusion of two other morphemes. e.g. “al” is the fusion of “a” and “el”. Morphological 4. Unique or residual: Representing bound morphemes, they are, by themselves, meaningless and gain meaning only in combination with other morphemes. e.g. “mit” in “transmit”, “commit”. 5. Zero morpheme: There are meaningful grammatical features that are not materialized, neither in writing nor in sound waves, in a consistent manner. e.g. the plural of some nouns: sheep (sing) – sheep (plural), fish (sing.) – fish (plural). Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 87 Learning Notes MORPH, MORPHEME AND ALLOMORPH: Morpheme: The smallest meaningful unit in a language. Morphemes can have grammatical functions. For example, the –s in boys, which shows that the word is in plural. Morph is the physical realization of the morpheme. For example, the morph of the morpheme –s for plural may show has various realizations. These variations sound and look differently: Morpheme –s morphs: /z/ in “dogs, beds”; /s/ in “cats”; /ɪz/ in “garages” Allophorms are several morphs that belong to the same morpheme: variants of one morpheme. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 88 UNIT 9 SYNTAX Syntax is the study of how words combine to form sentences and the rules which govern the formation of sentences. Syntax is the grammatical arrangement or words in a sentence. It concerns both word order and agreement in the relationship between words. Syntax is primarily concerned with structure of sentences. Examples: The following statements follow normal English word order: The cat sat on the mat. My old brown leather suitcase. The following statements do not follow normal English word order: The cat on the mat sat. My brown leather old suitcase. Use Word order is very important in English, because the language is no longer inflected. That is, individual words do not have endings to show which parts of speech they represent. Changes to conventional syntax are often used to create dramatic, poetic, or comic effect. For instance, poets and song lyricists often change syntactic order to create rhythmic effects. “I’ll sing to him, each spring to him And long for the day when I’ll cling to him, Betwitched, bothered and bewildered am I.” (Cole Porter) A normal sentence in English usually contains at least three elements: subject, verb, and object. Subject The cat John Mary Verb eats likes chose Object the goldfish football the wallpaper Every language has rules of syntax, and to the linguist the essential rules are descriptive. They are the rules which underpin the life of the language and which are extremely slow to change. These are not to be confused with the prescriptive ‘rules’ of traditional grammar (For instance, ‘Never end a sentence with a preposition’). Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 89 An example of a descriptive rule of English syntax is that in the imperative in English, the verb takes the initial position in the sentence, usually directly before the noun which is the object. Put those books on the table. Take the lid off after half an hour. Remove all packaging before heating the soup. Isolate the switch in case of fault. It is important to make a distinction between grammar and syntax, and to realise that syntax is a component of grammar. The term 'grammar' refers to the whole structure of the language including the naming of its parts, its rules of tense, and its sound system. It is a comprehensive term. Syntax only refers to the relationship between the grammatical components of language in use. In other words it is the nature, quality or type of relationship between terms in any given statement which is the province of syntax. 9.1. GRAMATICAL SYSTEM This is where inflection comes in, of course. It can, to begin with, locate the event in time, setting the co-ordinates of past and present by marking the verb for tense. At the same time it marks the verb for aspect, that is to say, it represents the process as taking place either over an open period of time (progressive), or within a closed period of time (perfective), or left unspecified (simple). Tense and aspect are systems which provide the verb with its formal identity as a sentence constituent. So if ‘live’ is to function as a verb, it has to be processed through the system and marked as such. Let us then select present tense and simple aspect. We now have a slightly more focused proposition: artist lives in gothic church. Again, this might be sufficient to signal meaning. It might, for example, figure quite plausibly as a newspaper headline. But notice that our choice of inflection does not only signal tense and aspect, but singular as well, and as such it transfers the signal to the preceding subject. We specify one artist. But this needs to be formally marked as well (even though in certain uses, like headlines and telegrams, convention allows us to waive the requirement in the interests of brevity). Nouns also have systems which provide them with identity conditions. Just as verbs have to be processed through the dual tense/aspect system, so nouns have to be processed through the dual number/definiteness system. Are we talking about a single artist or artists in the plural, the artist or artists we know about, any old artist or an artist or artists unknown? We have to decide, and mark the noun accordingly; and the same, of course, applies to the noun ‘church’. If we then process our sequence of words through the required grammatical systems, we come up with a number of possibilities: The artist lives in a gothic church. An artist lived in a gothic church. The artist was living in a gothic church. Artists live in gothic churches. Artists have lived in gothic church. And so on, and so on. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 90 9.2. CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE Now there are a number of points that are worth noting here. Firstly, the processing of nouns and verbs through their systems has to be co-ordinated. The marking of plurality on the noun, for example, has to match up with a corresponding marking on the verb to which it relates. In other words, they have to fit together as interdependent constituents or a larger structure, that of the sentence. Secondly, this preparation for constituent status may involve only the use of an inflection as in ‘artists’, ‘lives’, and so on. But it may also involve the deployment of separate morphemes, the so-called ‘function words’. This is the case with English, for example, where such function words are required for the marking of definiteness, as in phrases like ‘a/the artist’, ‘is/was living’ and so on. So we can say that the noun phrases (for example, ‘the artist’), and verb phrases (for example, ‘was living’) are constituents of larger structures, but are themselves in turn structured into constituents. Thus, the noun phrase consists of a noun as headword, and markers for number and definiteness. These markers too are interdependent. (Widdowson: 1996: 48-50) What is the structure of a sentence? A natural assumption is that sentences are mere strings of words, with no special structure (this is what we call ‘Hypothesis 1’). This assumption is incorrect, however. We show that sentences have more structure than meets the eye: they are organized intro subgroups of words, which are called ‘constituents’. Constituents have a tree-like structure, which has lead linguists to represent sentences as syntactic trees (this is what we call ‘Hypothesis 2’). The crucial argument in favour of Hypothesis 2 and again Hypothesis 1 stems from certain tests (called ‘constituency tests’), in which a group of words is targeted by a certain operation to yield another sentence. If the operation succeeds, and yields a grammatical sentence, the group of words in question behaves a natural unit, and is likely to be a ‘constituent’. For example in Mary will meet the President the words the President can be moved as a unit to the beginning of the sentence, yielding The President, Mary will meet, which is grammatical. We conclude that the President is a constituent. By contrast, if we attempt to move to the beginning of the sentence the words meet the, the result is ungrammatical: *Meet the Mary will President. This suggests that meet the is not a constituent. With this background in mind, we give an account of structural ambiguities, i.e. cases in which a given string of words can be organized into two distinct trees, with different meanings. 9.3. TREE ANALYSIS Information about the constituents of a sentence is easily seen in the schema known as SYNTACTIC TREES. This diagram provides a visual representation of the Constituent Structure of the sentence. Phrase Structure Rules: NP NP N N: students, assignments, hats, bus … (1) NP N students NP N hat NP DET – N Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 91 DET the, these, a … 2) NP DET these NP N students DET the N hat NP (DET) – N Phrase Structure Rules: PP PP P – NP P: for, with, on … (3) PP P for NP DET the N students (4) I walked out the door. I walked out. PP P – (NP) Phrase Structure Rules: NP, PP (5) (a) the students with a hat (b) the students on the bus with a hat NP (DET) – N – (PP) – (PP) Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insight into Linguistics 95 NP N students PP P on PP NP DET the N bus P with NP DET a N hat Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 92 The Golden Rule of the Tree Structures: Modifiers are always attached within the phrase they modify. Phrase Structures: ADJP (6) a. tall b. surprisingly tall c. very surprisingly tall ADJP (ADVP) – ADJ ADJP ADVP DEG very ADV surprisingly ADJ tall Phrase Structure Rules: NP, ADJP (7) a. the big yellow book b. the very yellow book NP (DET) – (ADJ) – ( ADJ) – N NP DET The ADJP ADJ big ADJP ADJ yellow NP N book DET the ADJP DEG very ADJ yellow N book Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 93 Phrase Structure Rules: VP (8) Ignatious a. left (the room) (quickly) (often) b. (often) (quickly) left VP (ADVP) – (ADVP) – V – (NP) – (ADVP) – (ADVP) (9) Bill often left the room in a huff on Tuesdays. VP (ADVP) – (ADVP) – V (NP) – (ADVP) – (PP) – (PP) VP ADVP A often V left NP DET the PP N room P in PP NP DET a P N on huff NP N Tuesdays PHRASE STRUCTURE RULES (From Radford, Andrew: Transformational Syntax) S NP DET This AUX N boy VP will V speak ADVP PP DEG ADV P very slowly to NP DET N that girl Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 94 The Phrase Structure Rules: Chomsky suggested that structures could be generated by a set of rules which he calls Phrase Structure Rules. Consider for example, the following set of Phrase Structure Rules: (1) S NP – AUX – VP (2) NP D – N (3) VP V – ADVP – PP (4) ADVP DEG – ADV (5) PP P – NP (6) NP D – N S = Sentence NP = Noun Phrase DET = Determiner N = Noun AUX = Auxiliary VP = Verb Phrase V = Verb ADVP = Adverbial Phrase DEG = Degree Adverb ADV = Adverb PP = Prepositional Phrase P = Preposition = ‘can consist of / can be formed from’ = ‘immediately preceding’ The rule can be regarded as specifying ‘You can form a Sentence by taking a Noun Phrase immediately followed by an Auxiliary immediately followed by a Verb Phrase.’ More formally, we can say that the rule will generate the partial tree-structure below: (1) S NP – AUX – VP S NP AUX VP In order to get the lower branches on the left side of the tree, we need a rule like this: (2) NP DET – N S NP DET AUX VP N This reads, Noun Phrase can consist of a Determiner immediately preceding a Noun We can form a Verb Phrase by taking a Verb immediately followed by an Adverbial Phrase immediately followed by a Prepositional Phrase. We can generate: Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 95 (3) VP V – ADVP – PP S NP AUX VP DET N will V Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insight into Linguistics ADVP PP 98 We can form an Adverbial Phrase by taking a Degree Adverb followed by an Adverb; applying this rule to the structure, we expand into: (4) ADVP DEG – ADV S NP DET AUX N VP will V ADVP DEG PP ADV We can form a Prepositional Phrase out of a Preposition immediately followed by a Noun Phrase; applying the rule to the structure will give us: (5) PP P – NP S NP DET This AUX N boy VP will V speak ADVP DEG very ADV slowly PP P to NP DET that N girl Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 96 (6) NP DET – N Rule 6 says that we can form a Noun Phrase by taking a Determiner immediately followed by a Noun; if we apply this rule to expand both the Noun Phrases, we derive the structure: S NP DET This AUX N VP will boy V speak ADVP PP DEG ADV P very slowly to NP DET N that girl Let’s now assume that in addition to the Categorial Rules – that is, rules expanding categories into other categories – the syntactic component of our grammar also contains a Lexicon ( = dictionary), or list of all the words in the language. And let us further assume that the Lexical Entry ( = dictionary entry) for each such Lexical Item ( = word) contains (in addition to other information) a specification of the syntactic category that the word belongs to. For present purposes, we might imagine that our Lexicon contains the following entries: DET this N boy AUX will V speak DEG very ADV slowly P to DET that N girl The final phrase structure rules of the above sentence are: (1) S NP – AUX – VP (2) NP D – N (3) D this (4) N boy (5) AUX will (6) VP V – ADVP – PP (7) V speak (8) ADVP DEG – ADV (9) DEG very (10) ADV slowly (11) PP P – NP (12) P to (13) NP DET – N (14) DET that (15) N girl Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 97 EXERCISE 1 Draw trees for the following phrases: 1. the students with a hat 2. students on the bus with a hat 3. the little dog in the house 4. Mary’s collection of mushrooms 5. that expensive little box of delicious chocolate 6. little black furry cats 7. The Joneses’ new Japanese car 8. Many regions of the United States EXERCISE 2 Draw trees for the following sentences: 1. This girl will speak very carefully to the waiter 2. A boy must speak very sincerely to a girl. 3. That student may signal rather carefully to the waiter. 4. The big cat will chase the little mouse 5. Which car will your father put in the garage? 6. The poor old man must be quite tired 7. The new hotel has a reasonably cheap restaurant. 8. Two young children were seriously injured in the accident. 9. The business meeting with the American executives was very badly organized. 10. The young girl speaks three foreign languages perfectly. Rebeca Vera, M.Ed., Insights into Linguistics 98