Auditory Systems in Insects - Cold Spring Harbor Monograph Archive
Transcription
Auditory Systems in Insects - Cold Spring Harbor Monograph Archive
07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 155 7 Auditory Systems in Insects Daniel Robert School of Biological Sciences University of Bristol Bristol BS8 1UG, United Kingdom Ronald R. Hoy Department of Neurobiology & Behavior Cornell University Ithaca, New York 14853 E capacity for miniaturization. Virtually all aspects of insect biology convey the sense of successfully uniting form and function in exquisitely small, diverse, and sophisticated motor, sensory, and metabolic systems (Grimaldi and Engel 2005). The ability of insects to fly in an efficient and controlled manner well illustrates how, through evolution by natural selection, they adapted to solve what we consider serious problems of engineering. Insects are little marvels of “evolutionary engineering.” The seemingly boundless ingenuity and creativity of the process of evolutionary adaptation are also reflected in the sensory systems of insects. As we try to make clear in this chapter, hearing in insects is a sophisticated process; understanding its fundamental mechanisms and trying to understand its evolution present many challenges but are likely to be very rewarding. Perhaps because insects are so small, their ears have generally been considered to be simple compared to those of vertebrates. Anatomically, they may be simpler, but their capacity for sound reception and processing turns out to be remarkably elaborate (for reviews, see Fullard and Yack 1993; Hoy 1998; Robert and Göpfert 2002; Robert 2005; Hedwig 2006; Göpfert and Robert 2007). The ears of insects can be as sensitive and acute VOLUTION HAS ENDOWED INSECTS WITH AN EXTRAORDINARY 155 Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 156 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 156 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy as their vertebrate counterparts (Webster et al. 1992; Hoy 1998). Indeed, in some cases their feats of detection surpass the capabilities of vertebrates (Robert and Göpfert 2002). For example, the ultrafast ears of the parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea can distinguish time differences in the arrival of incident sound waves below one microsecond (Mason et al 2001). It is commonly thought that insects cannot evolve large bodies, and this has been a key constraint in the evolution of insect audition, one that has likely fostered innovation. The laws of physical acoustics also apply to tiny insects, of course: For them, the time it takes for a sound wave to pass by is extremely short. Similarly, capturing sound energy with small structures has been thought to be an inefficient process, which challenges the coding capacity of the insect nervous system (Michelsen 1998; Robert and Hoy 1998). Research on insect hearing has shown that insects can hear very well and that to do this, they have unconventional yet highly effective mechanisms (Michelsen 1998; Robert and Göpfert 2002). The general constraints inherent to small body size were recognized by D’Arcy Wentworth Thompson, a pioneer of quantitative and predictive life sciences who advanced the analysis of biological form and function in mathematical and physical terms. In his seminal book, On Growth and Form, first published in 1917, D’Arcy Thompson astutely points out that identical structures differing by size only display vastly different resonant behaviors, in inverse proportion to the square of their length scale. In his words: “Structure apart, mere size is enough to give the lesser birds and beasts a music quite different to our own [...]. A minute insect may utter and receive vibrations of prodigious rapidity; even its little wings may beat hundreds of time a second” (D’Arcy Thompson 1961). Size alone constitutes a severe constraint; for insects only several millimeters in size, sound passes by in a few microseconds or less. As the temporal resolution of insect nervous systems is usually on the scale of the millisecond (sometimes hundreds of microseconds; Rheinländer and Mörchen 1979; Robert et al. 1996; Mason et al. 2001), additional processing is required to resolve such rapid events. As has become increasingly apparent, this processing may rely, not only on better neuronal performance, but also on innovative auditory mechanics (Robert and Göpfert 2002). For insects, life goes fast, and so does the process of sensory perception. In the passage of his works on hearing and size, D’Arcy Thompson commented: “Far more things happen to [the insect] in a second than to us; a thousandth part of second is no longer negligible, and time itself seems to run a different course to ours” (D’Arcy Thompson 1961). Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 157 Auditory Systems in Insects 157 Herein we discuss the capacity of insects to detect sound. The general issues associated with the detection of sound are briefly presented. We discuss directional hearing and active audition, and we consider how active processes facilitate acoustic detection by insects. FORM AND FUNCTION IN INSECT HEARING In insects, hearing is used for tasks such as finding mates and hosts, and avoiding predators (Hoy and Robert 1996; Yager 1999; Hedwig 2006). The literature is plentiful on the structure (Field and Matheson 1998; Yack 2004), neurobiology (Hennig et al. 2004), and molecular genetics of insect hearing (Kernan and Zuker 1995; Jarman 2002; Todi et al. 2004), but our understanding of the mechanical characterization of the hearing process is still sketchy (Robert and Göpfert 2002). To date, no auditory system, insect or other, has been fully characterized in its mechanical details. The gap in knowledge is significant, because hearing involves a chain of events in which mechanical and neural responses work together intimately. This chain of events encompasses the coupling of external sound energy, via a sound receiver—a tympanum or an antenna—to internal mechanosensitive neurons, which respond to the energy input (Göpfert and Robert 2003). We need to have an integrated understanding of the mechanical behavior of the sound receiver and associated vibration-conducting structures, and how these structures interact with the force generating mechanosensory cells. For any animal, hearing is a sophisticated process, relying on the delicate interplay between sound-sensitive morphological structures and mechanically sensitive and mechanically active neurons (insects) or hair cells (vertebrates). Little is known about the mechanical behavior of auditory neurons in insects, in contrast to vertebrate systems, for which both in vivo and in situ studies have generated a more detailed understanding of vibration detection by hair cells, its molecular substrate, and its genetic basis (Kernan and Zucker 1995; Ashmore and Mammano 2001; Manley 2001; Steel and Kros 2001; Fettiplace and Hackney 2006). A DELICATE BUSINESS The process of hearing is delicate because it involves mechanoreception at very low energy levels (Thurm 1982; Bialek 1987; Hudspeth 1997). The threshold of hearing is considered to be near the level of thermal Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 158 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 158 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy noise (4 zeptojoules). Hearing and mechanosensory thresholds are at energy levels about 100 times lower than that of a single green photon (Thurm 1982; Hudspeth 1997). This extreme mechanical sensitivity has made the study of audition experimentally challenging. One key question is how auditory neurons, or hair cells, can be sensitive to mechanical perturbations of such low energy. Neural thresholds for both mechanoreception and hearing in insects were first suggested (Autrum 1948) and then measured (Adam 1972) to be reached with mechanical displacements on the order of a tenth of a nanometer. Recent studies suggest that mechanical perturbations of one nanometer and below are of relevant magnitude (mosquito: Göpfert and Robert 2000; locusts: Windmill et al. 2005). These amplitudes are the levels of mechanical vibration of the external, peripheral auditory structures. The actual amplitude and geometry of the mechanical deflections that insect auditory neurons experience are not yet known, but we need to know them in order to understand cellular mechanics and, eventually, the molecular mechanisms of nanoscale detection. These activation amplitudes and sensory thresholds are expected to vary from one insect species to another, depending on the anatomical construction of their auditory systems and the species-specific requirement for acoustic sensitivity. Although measurement technologies and analytical methods have much improved in the past years, such low levels of vibration remain challenging to measure, especially in intact preparations. Yet, such in situ or in vivo measurements are key to understand fully the mechanisms of hearing and their pathologies, from the physics of the sound receiver to the biophysics of the molecular machinery constitutive of the cellular sensors and actuators. Much uncertainty still remains in our knowledge of the chain of neuromechanical events underlying hearing; outstanding research challenges include the characterization of the flow of vibration through an ear, identifying the molecular machineries sensing and generating vibrations, and, ultimately, understanding the relation between form and function in hearing, at the microscale and nanoscale levels. A synthetic explanation of hearing will encompass an understanding of the systemic, cellular, and molecular organizations and dynamics of the auditory organ (Ashmore and Mammano 2001; Gillespie and Walker 2001; Gillespie et al. 2005). One specific key challenge is the dissection of the neuromechanical machinery that underpins sensitivity and frequency selectivity. What are the dynamic mechanisms that allow a cell, buffeted by thermal noise and metabolic activities such as respiration or locomotion, to be sensitive to mechanical Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 159 Auditory Systems in Insects 159 perturbations that can be about five or six orders of magnitude smaller than itself? STRUCTURAL DIVERSITY OF INSECT EARS Morphologically, the ears of insects come in two basic forms: the tympanal ears, and the flagellar ears. Tympanal ears are defined by a thin, external membrane, a tracheal air sac behind this membrane, and a mechanosensory organ transducing the membrane’s vibrations. The flagellar ears rely on a fundamentally different anatomy. They are antennae or hair shafts that can sway from side to side under the action of an incident sound wave. The mechanosensory organ is at the base of the antenna or hair. One immediately obvious difference between the auditory systems of vertebrates and insects is the location of the sound receivers: Vertebrate ears are invariably located on the head, a legacy of the evolution of hearing in fish (Webster et al. 1992); in insects, tympanal ears can be found anywhere on the body (Fullard and Yack 1993). Tympanal hearing organs have been found on any of the thoracic segments (moths, scarab beetles, mantises, and parasitoid flies), on the prothoracic legs (in bush and field crickets), at the base of the wings (hedylid butterflies, lacewings), on the first abdominal segments (grasshoppers, tiger beetles), or on any (except the last) of the abdominal segments (van Staaden and Römer 1998; for reviews, see Yack and Fullard 1993; Hoy and Robert 1996; Yager 1999; Yack 2004). It is thought that ears may have evolved independently at least 15 times in insects (Hoy and Robert 1996; Yager 1999). Interestingly, tympanal ears have not been found so far in some ecologically important insect orders, such as hymenoptera and odonata. In some insects, tympanal ears can be relatively simple anatomically: For example, those of noctuid moths (Fig. 1A) comprise only two auditory neurons, yet they are capable of sophisticated sound detection, adapting to local circumstances to enhance sensitivity (Windmill et al. 2006). The less well-known flagellar ears rely on an antenna-like or hair structure to capture sound (Fig. 1B–D) (Johnston 1855; Fletcher 1978; Tautz 1979). In ears of this kind, sound-induced vibrations—air movements—are detected by mechanosensory cells situated at the base of the antenna or the hair borne on the cuticle. In Drosophila, for example, the mechanoreceptive organ, Johnston’s organ (Johnston 1855), resides in the pedicel of the antenna (Fig. 1B) (Eberl 1999). In culicid mosquitoes (Fig. 1C,D), Johnston’s organ is situated at the base of the Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 160 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 160 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy Figure 1. The two basic types of ears in insects: The tympanal ears present a thin membrane backed by an air-filled cavity and a mechanoreceptive organ. Nontympanal ears use a hair or an antennal shaft to capture sound energy. (A) Internal view of one of the “simplest” ears known, that of the noctuid moth, comprising only two mechanosensory cells. Arrow points to the sensory cells connecting to the tympanal membrane. Modified from an original scanning electron micrograph (SEM) by H. Ghiradella. (B) The antennal hearing apparatus of Drosophila melanogaster. The pedicel (arrow) is the site of the mechanoreceptive Johnston’s organ, containing 150 – 200 scolopidia (Sivan-Loukianova and Eberl 2005) (Ar) Arista. The red dot indicates the site of the laser interferometric mechanical measurement reported in Fig. 3 (SEM: D. Robert and R. Porter). (C) Close-up of the pedicel (arrow) containing Johnston’s organ in mosquitoes. (SEM: H. Kohler and D. Robert). (D) The auditory antenna of T. brevipalpis. Arrow shows the pedicel. The red dot at the tip of the antenna is the site of laser Doppler vibrometric measurement. (Scanning light micrograph by D. Huber and D. Robert). Bars, 200 µm. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 161 Auditory Systems in Insects 161 antennal shaft and contains some 15,000 scolopidial cells, about as many as there are hair cells in the human cochlea, but in a volume ~100,000 times smaller. Mosquito ears may be the most complex found in insects. Specialization for hearing seems to be extreme in culicidae, toxorhynchinae, and chironomidae Diptera, but actually, more than 95% of insect species are endowed with Johnston’s organ at the base of their antennae (Kristensen 1981). Not all of these species can necessarily hear using their antennae, but certainly, many have the potential to do so. The evolutionary emergence of the sense of hearing in a group that otherwise does not possess this sensory modality is possible, if not expected. It has been previously shown for tympanal ears in tachinid and sarcophagid parasitoid flies (Robert et al. 1992; Robert and Hoy 1998). Antennal hearing, based on the prior presence of Johnston’s organ, may have evolved in insect orders yet to be examined. Interestingly, recent evidence confirms the long-held suspicion (McKeever 1977) that another group of culicid mosquitoes, the Corethrellidae midges, feeding on frogs, phonotactically find their hosts using their antennae as sound detectors (Bernal et al. 2006; Borkent and Belton 2006). Hairs borne on the animal’s body surface that are articulated at the base and equipped with one or several mechanoreceptive neurons can also act as sound or wind receivers (Fletcher 1978; Tautz 1979; Shimozawa and Kanou 1984; Kämper and Kleindienst 1990; Barth 2000, 2004). This type of sound receiver is very widespread among arthropods, so many more species than previously thought, in particular spiders and crustaceans, might have a sense of hearing, of a rather unconventional kind. A remarkable example is that of spiders, which have extremely sensitive mechanosensory hairs all over their bodies (Barth 2004). These hairs can be exquisitely sensitive to small wind inputs (non-harmonic variations of air velocity) and near-field sound (harmonic variations of air velocity), providing information about the presence or approach of prey, mates, or predators. The antennal and hair-like sound receivers are sensitive to the particle velocity component of the propagating sound energy (see Box), rather than the pressure component (Fletcher 1978). The particle velocity component is easier to detect in the vicinity of a sound source, in the so-called near-field. Such antennal and hair receivers have thus been coined nearfield receivers, recognizing their better performance near the source of sound. This is not to say, however, that they cannot work in the far-field of a sound source, nor that they are not sensitive sensory organs, as shown below. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 162 5/11/07 11:59 AM Page 162 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy Acoustical Cues for Directional Hearing and Sound in the Near-field (A) A propagating sound wave can be represented, in the case of a pure tone, by a sinusoidal wave; for instance, representing the variation in pressure in time. This sinusoid is characterized by an amplitude (i.e., peak to peak), a frequency (the inverse of the period T), and a direction of propagation. In air, sound waves propagate at ~343 m/s for normal temperature (20°C) and humidity (20% RH) conditions. Owing to this speed of propagation, the auditory receiver nearer to the sound source, the ipsilateral ear, detects the sound wave earlier than the ear farther from the sound source, the contralateral ear. This interaural time difference (ITD), shown here as the time (or phase) delay between two sinusoid waves (∂t), amounts to ~1.5 10-6 s for an interaural distance (ID) of 5 10-4 m, such as that of the fly Ormia. Passing by and around a solid body, a sound wave also undergoes diffraction if the ratio between the acoustic wavelength and the size of the solid is about 10:1 or smaller. In diffractive conditions, the sound input to the contralateral ear is attenuated, yielding a reduction in the amplitude of the wave (∂A). This is the other cue for directional hearing, the interaural intensity difference, IID. (B) The interaural time difference as a function of the azimuthal angle of incidence. For a sound source reaching the subject at 90° off the midline, the difference in the time of arrival at the ears is 500 µs and 1.5 µs, for humans and flies, respectively. This time decreases as the sine of the angle of incidence. For the fly Ormia, azimuthal angles below ~45° result in submicrosecond ITDs. Such sinusoidal relationship between azimuthal angle and ITD is an approximation; in diffractive condition, the geometry of the auditory organs, as well Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/11/07 11:59 AM Page 163 Auditory Systems in Insects 163 as the size and density of the body part on which they are borne, influences the actual ITD. (C) Attenuation of sound as a function of distance from a flying female mosquito (data from Jackson and Robert 2006). Fitted attenuation function is f(x) = 1/x3. A large proportion of the sound intensity, measured as particle velocity, is lost already some 15 mm away from a flying female. In a sound wave, acoustic energy is embedded in the pressure (p) and the particle velocity (v) such that p = Zv, where Z is the acoustic impedance of the medium (413N•s/m3). Such relation is the acoustic equivalent of Ohm’s law in electricity. The overall intensity (Watt/m2) of sound is I = pv (I = Zv2 = p2/Z). In the close vicinity of a sound source, particle velocity dominates the overall sound intensity, whereas farther away, typically several wavelengths from the source, pressure dominates (Olson 1942). Particle velocity decays as a function of the inverse cube of the distance x from the source (x–3), whereas pressure does so as the inverse square (x–2). As a consequence, particle velocity receivers, such as antennae and hairs, are best suited to detect sounds close to the source. THE HOW AND WHY OF HEARING IN INSECTS Like vertebrates, many insects use their hearing organs to explore and exploit their environment in space and time. Insect ears have to accomplish the same task as vertebrate ears: to detect and encode at least one or a combination of the three fundamental physical parameters of sound (see Box): its frequency, intensity, and direction of propagation (Rayleigh 1876). Although these parameters were identified early Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 164 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 164 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy on, the mechanisms subtending their detection are still not fully understood. First, an animal’s ears must be responsive to behaviorally and ecologically salient signals in the acoustic frequency spectrum. The range of detectable frequencies can greatly vary; for human ears, it is from about 20 Hz to 15 kHz, but for certain Meconematinae neotropical bush crickets, it goes up to 129 kHz (Montealegre-Z et al. 2006). The frequency selectivity of an ear is determined by a combination of physical and physiological mechanisms working in concert. In vertebrate ears, the tuning is partly determined by the passive mechanical response of the cochlea (von Békésy 1960), but the usual exquisite sensitivity and frequency selectivity require supplementary active work by hair cells (Hudspeth 1989, 1997; Dallos 1992; Manley 2001; Robles and Ruggero 2001). The interplay between passive and active mechanisms in insects and their behavioral relevance are just beginning to be unveiled; the current knowledge is presented below in this chapter. Second, an animal’s ears must be sufficiently sensitive to impinging sound waves with vanishingly small energies. High sensitivity is made possible by the active participation of mechanosensitive cells, a process that is thought to counteract viscous damping and/or to modulate the effective stiffness of the sound receiver. The mechanisms boosting the ear’s sensitivity, generally referred to as active hearing, have been the subject of much attention in vertebrates (Hudspeth 1997; Nobili et al 1998; Manley 2001; Robles and Ruggero 2001). In insects, the evidence for active mechanisms in mechanoreception (Moran et al. 1977) and hearing (Kössl and Boyan 1998a,b; Göpfert and Robert 2001, 2003) has been rare, and only recently have remarkable similarities to vertebrates been reported. Third, for most insects, the ability to localize a sound source is behaviorally important. In insects, directional hearing can be achieved in several ways by detecting timing and intensity differences between the auditory receptors (Robert 2005). Vertebrates derive directional information from diffractive effects on the spectral composition of sound (Wightman and Kistler 1997). This has not yet been reported in insects, but the possibility for it exists because, for an insect with a 1-cm body size, frequencies larger than 10 kHz are sufficient to generate diffraction (Olson 1943) and the interaural differences needed for directional hearing (Michelsen 1998; Robert et al. 1999). Sensing the direction of a sound source is particularly challenging for small animals because the cues involved—the time and intensity differences at the two ears—decrease dramatically with decreasing body size (see Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 165 Auditory Systems in Insects 165 Box). What solutions have insects evolved to solve the problem of directional hearing? Insects have been particularly creative, evolutionarily speaking, in generating a rich diversity of auditory morphologies, each with distinct processing mechanisms solving the problem of directional hearing with a small body size (Michelsen 1998; Robert 2005). The apparent evolutionary constraint on insect body size has fostered the development of unique auditory mechanisms, such as acoustically or mechanically coupled receivers. As shown in the following section, these mechanisms allow very small ears to function like large ones. CHALLENGES OF DIRECTIONAL HEARING Detecting the direction of incoming sound waves presents particular challenges for small animals (Michelsen 1998; Robert 2005). We consider two insect species, field crickets and parasitoid flies, which have evolved very distinct biomechanical solutions to the problem of directional hearing (Robert et al. 1996). Field crickets are conspicuous singing insects (Hedwig 2006): To attract females for mating, males produce chirping and trilling calling songs, which the females hear, locate, and home in on. The calling signals of males are essential to their reproductive success, and call conspicuousness, achieved by high sound intensity, enhances mating opportunities (Farris et al. 1997). But the males are acoustic beacons, and are detectable not only by conspecific females, but also by parasitic flies, such as the tachinid Ormia ochracea, which rears its larvae within the body of their obligatory cricket host, Gryllus bimaculatus (Cade 1975). Female crickets and female Ormia flies thus face the same auditory task: that of detecting the mating call with high sensitivity and localizing it precisely (Robert et al. 1992). The mating call of G. bimaculatus is a series of brief (25 ms), tonal amplitude-modulated sound pulses (4.8 – 5 kHz dominant frequency), delivered as continuous trills (repeated at 35–40 Hz) lasting from 0.5 second to several seconds (Cade 1976). Such an acoustic signal is easily detectable by the human ear, yet personal fieldwork experience shows that even for humans, determining its direction in the grassy vegetation is no trivial feat; neither is it for female crickets or parasitoid flies. Biomechanical studies on sound localization in crickets and Ormia flies have revealed critical differences in the mechanisms employed. Crickets localize sounds by using pressure difference acoustic receivers and by comparing binaurally the outputs of each of the acoustically coupled hearing organs (Michelsen 1998). Ormia flies take a different approach, based on mechanical coupling Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 166 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 166 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy Figure 2. SEMs comparing the prosternal anatomy of the tympanate Ormia ochracea (Tachinidae) (A) with that of the atympanate Musca domestica (Muscidae) (B). The prosternal membranes are conspicuously more developed in the tympanate fly and are connected by the presternum, an unpaired sclerite endowed with some rigidity. This structure is responsible for the mechanical coupling between the ears and hence the fly’s capacity for directional hearing. Notably, the presternum is present in both species; in Ormia it extends laterally. The apodemes linking the chordotonal organs to the prosternal membranes attach at the lateral flanges of the presternum (arrows in A and B). Abbreviations: (CvS) cervical sclerites; (N) neck; (PM) prosternal membrane; (Pr) presternum; (Pb) probasisternum; (Cx) prothoracic coxa. Bars, 200 µm. (SEMs courtesy of E. Tuck and R. Porter.) between the tympanal membranes of two adjacent hearing organs (Fig. 2) (Robert 2005). Such intertympanal mechanical coupling has not been reported to date in organisms other than flies. As an evolutionary innovation (Edgecomb et al. 1995), intertympanal coupling is considered to be a consequence of the very small size of the flies compared to the wavelength of the sound they have to detect (Miles et al. 1995; Robert et al. 1996; Robert and Hoy 1998; Robert 2001). In crickets, the first stage of tympanal hearing—bringing a membrane into vibration under the action of sound pressure—is achieved using a sophisticated modified tracheal system to conduct sound across the body. As sound travels into the animal’s respiratory system, the two Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 167 Auditory Systems in Insects 167 sides of each tympanal membrane are exposed to sound pressure. These ears are called pressure-difference receivers, and the tympanal membrane is the first site of processing, where the difference of pressure, the net force across it, is “computed.” This computation is possible because the acoustic pressure field acting on the opposite sides of the tympanal membrane, the external and internal surface areas, generates a harmonic force of opposite sign. These forces can greatly differ in magnitude and phase. If the driving forces have opposite sign but equal amplitude and phase, there is no difference force, so the membrane does not enter harmonic motion. Forces that are out of phase efficiently drive the membrane; the characteristics of tracheal sound transmission are thus crucial in determining the driving force to the auditory system (Michelsen and Rorhseitz 1995; Michelsen 1998; Robert 2005). Pressure-difference receivers are usually bilaterally acoustically coupled, rather than mechanically coupled, and these are most notably found in crickets, bush crickets, and locusts (Miller 1977; Michelsen and Rohrseitz 1995; Michelsen 1998; Schul et al. 1999). A well-balanced pressure-difference auditory receiver system needs to maximize constructive interference on the side of the animal closer to the sound source, and to minimize it on the other, contralateral side. This process is quite likely to be frequency dependent, as transmission through small tracheal tubes affects the velocity of wave propagation and frequency bandwidth (see also Bangert et al. 1998; Schul et al. 1999). In crickets, the tympanal membrane does not directly connect to the chordotonal organ containing the mechanosensory cells. However, the role played by the tympanal membrane in generating the actual mechanical drive to the mechanosensory organ remains unclear. Ormia flies possess a pair of hearing organs that, because the fly is so small, are set close together (Fig. 2): The distance between them is just 0.5 mm, compared to ~15 mm in crickets and ~180 mm in humans. The ears are so close together in Ormia that, even in the best scenario—when the sound comes from one side of the animal, at 90° angle from the midline—the time it takes for a sound wave to pass from one ear to the other is a mere 1.5 x 10–6 seconds (see Box). For humans, the maximal interaural time difference is ~500 x 10–6 seconds, whereas the shortest times that can be resolved binaurally are in the range 4 x 10–6 to 8 x 10–6 seconds, representing a deviation by the sound source of 1–2° from the midline (Middlebrooks and Green 1991; Wightman and Kistler 1993). How short is the smallest resolvable interaural time difference in flies, where the interaural distance is about 360 times smaller than that of humans? Behavioral experiments have shown that Ormia can detect and accurately orient toward a sound source deviating only by 2° in azimuth Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 168 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 168 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy (Mason et al. 2001). Such deviation has been calculated to correspond to time differences between the tympanal membranes of the fly of a mere 50 nanoseconds (this value may not be strictly accurate because the acoustic interaural time differences have not been directly measured). Thus, are flies indeed capable of detection with a temporal resolution that would be considered fast even for electronics? Nature tends to have ways of making things work, not necessarily the same ways used by human engineering (Bleckmann et al. 2004). Finding its own way, the acoustic detection system of the little Ormia fly seems to have taken the problem apart by assigning a different task to each step in the auditory chain of events (Robert et al. 1996). The first and crucial step involves the mechanisms of conversion of acoustic energy into mechanical energy. Sound energy results in the motion of the tympanal membrane, which in turn channels mechanical energy to the sensory neurons. For directional hearing, the essential physical quantities reaching the neurons are the phase (or timing) of the impinging sound wave, and its amplitude. In Ormia, as mentioned above, the tympana are anatomically linked; a flexible cuticular lever connects them across the midline of the animal (Fig. 2A). Most remarkably, when the sound of a singing male cricket impinges on the ears of the fly, this coupled tympanal system undergoes asymmetrical mechanical oscillations. The side nearer to the sound source oscillates with larger amplitude and earlier than the far side. This results in the differential activation of the mechanosensory neurons located in the chordotonal organs linked to each of the membranes (Robert et al. 1996; Robert 2001). Note that, if the two membranes were acting independently from each other, like two separate microphones, the time difference between the two sides would be only 1.45 microseconds, and no amplitude difference would be detectable (Robert et al. 1996). Mechanically, this asymmetrical behavior is generated by the linear combination of two resonant oscillators. In the mode that dominates at the frequency of the cricket song, the two tympana move out of phase and at different amplitudes, displaying motions that are reminiscent of the rocking of a floppy seesaw (Miles et al. 1995; Robert et al. 1996). This system generates, for a sound delivered at 90° to the side of the animal, a mechanical interaural time difference of 50 – 60 microseconds and a mechanical interaural intensity difference of 3–12 decibels. The mechanical time difference at the ears is thus about 40 times longer than the 1.45-microsecond acoustical time difference. There is thus an amplification of the acoustic cues which converts small acoustical cues into much larger mechanical cues (Robert et al. 1996). These mechanical cues are conducted from the tympanal membranes Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 169 Auditory Systems in Insects 169 to the auditory neurons of the chordotonal organ. The exact manner by which tympanal vibrations are transmitted to the chordotonal has not been investigated in detail in flies, or in any other insect. It is possible that further mechanical processing takes place along the connecting apodemes linking tympanal membranes to the mechanosensory neurons. But the accuracy required in the coding of temporal events may still be in the range of microseconds, a challenge for any auditory system (Mason et al. 2001; Robert 2005). Does the neuronal processing in the cricket and fly auditory systems mirror the peripheral mechanisms in exhibiting special adaptations to deal with the consequences of small body size? For female crickets, in the monoaural situation, the intensity of a sound stimulus is encoded in one of two ways. The response latency of the primary afferents decreases with increasing sound level, and the spiking activity of each afferent increases for each sound pulse proportionally to sound intensity (Imaizumi and Pollack 1999; see the authoritative review by Pollack 1998). Interestingly, the coding of intensity information can also be used to regulate the sensitivity of the hearing process. Intracellular recordings of an identified auditory interneuron in a singing cricket have revealed some of the neural mechanisms that contribute to modulating and preserving auditory sensitivity (Poulet and Hedwig 2002). The process involves both the presynaptic inhibition of primary afferents and postsynaptic inhibition of a central interneuron—the well-studied omega neuron. The two inhibitions appear to be phase-locked, or coincidental, with the acoustic input from the song and were later demonstrated to be genuine corollary discharges that originate from the song generation network (Poulet and Hedwig 2006). Collectively, the work on crickets illustrates the sophistication of the peripheral mechanical and neural processing in insect auditory systems and their integration in the behavioral and sensory ecology of the species (Hedwig 2006). In female Ormia flies, the afferent activity shows an additional type of response related to stimulus intensity. Over half of the afferents recorded as single units exclusively encode sound intensity by the conventional quasi-exponential decay relation between latency and increasing intensity. Remarkably, some afferents in the fly auditory organ never discharge more than a single spike per stimulus cycle (Oshinsky and Hoy 2002). Such latency-only coding of sound level in single-spiking afferents has never been reported in the afferent responses of crickets or other insects. Other primary afferents recorded in the same preparation were shown to exhibit spike recruitment and modulation of the tonic discharge as a function of intensity. The single-spiking behavior of the primary Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 170 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 170 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy mechanoreceptive afferents, and its repeatable accuracy, are especially interesting in light of mechanisms for acute sound localization. Not only can some primary afferents spike only once in response to harmonic stimuli some 20 milliseconds in duration or longer, but they do so with remarkable repeatability (Oshinsky and Hoy 2002). Presented with repeating sound stimuli mimicking a cricket song, these primary afferents respond with a consistent delay of 3164 microseconds with, remarkably, a variation of only 95 microseconds (Oshinsky and Hoy 2002). This very small jitter in the temporal response is consistent with the idea that these neurons perform some sparse coding of the temporal information contained in the arrival of the sound wave at the tympanal membranes (Mason et al. 2001). The single-spike encoding of stimulus latency has also been interpreted as a neuronal adaptation to sound localization at the submillisecond timescale (Mason et al. 2001). It is important to recall here that, because of the lack of significant acoustic diffraction in Ormia, no measurable acoustic interaural intensity differences are available (Robert et al. 1996, 1999). The temporal coding observed in the fly’s auditory system is reminiscent of a type of sensory processing called hyperacuity. Under such a scheme, neural coding at submillisecond timescales, as reported to occur in electric fish (Rose and Heiligenberg 1985) and barn owls (Knudsen and Konishi 1979), relies on the convergence of many sensory afferents onto an interneuron that acts as a coincidence detector. The logic is such that only the coherent firing of an ensemble of afferents within a narrow time window—the coincidence—can elicit interneuronal activity. For Ormia, the very small variation in the firing times of the afferent population seems essential to generate the sort of coincidence necessary to the binaural discrimination of acoustic events in the microsecond range. This type of interneuronal processing cascade may explain the hyperacute directionality observed at the behavioral level (Mason et al. 2001). ACTIVE MECHANISMS IN INSECT HEARING Hearing relies on the detection of nanoscale, or even picoscale, vibrations. The inherently weak energies conveyed by propagating sound waves are at the root of the hearing challenge; low-level sounds may often be those that an animal should detect rapidly and reliably. For some insects, detection challenges have been identified that relate to the particularly transient nature of sounds to be detected in the near-field (Jackson and Robert 2006). Selection pressure is likely to have favored the evolution of ears that can detect, and localize, mate, prey, or predators with better Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 171 Auditory Systems in Insects 171 sensitivity, accuracy, rapidity, and reliability, pushing the feat of detection to its physical limits (Bialek 1987). In hearing, these limits involve energy levels so faint as to be comparable to that of thermal noise. One of the keys to fully understanding hearing resides in the identification and characterization of the molecular constituents and their mechanical behavior. Crucially, this needs to be done in the physical and physiological contexts in which mechanosensory cells operate, be that at thermal noise level or just above. The investigation of that process has proved to be a formidable challenge that has attracted sustained scientific attention and resulted in a number of spectacular advances (Dallos 1992; Hudspeth 1997; Gillespie and Walker 2001; Robles and Ruggero 2001; Gillespie et al. 2005). It is now widely accepted that, for both vertebrates and invertebrates, the exquisite sensory sensitivity and frequency selectivity achieved by auditory organs result from an active process (vertebrates: Fettiplace and Hackney 2006; invertebrates: Göpfert and Robert 2007). Using their own metabolic energy, the mechanosensory cells that mediate hearing generate motions that coherently enhance, and sometimes sustain, the minute vibrations they have to detect in the first place (Hudspeth 1989, 1997). This cellular behavior has not yet been observed at the cellular level in an intact auditory system. In vertebrates, both motile and sensory functions are considered to be embodied in a single cell type, the outer hair cell. In insects, whereas active mechanisms have now been shown to contribute to hearing (Göpfert and Robert 2003, 2007; Göpfert et al. 2005), the question of the integration or segregation of function has not been addressed. This question naturally arises because chordotonal (auditory) organs are known across insects to harbor several types of scolopidial mechanoreceptors (Field and Matheson 1998; Eberl 1999; Yack 2004; Boekhoff-Falk 2005). The mechanically sensitive unit is a scolopidium, an assembly of several different cell types accompanied by one or more ciliated axonemal mechanosensory sensory neurons (Yack 2004). It is possible that, in those insects endowed with active mechanisms, different scolopidial or neuronal types fulfil different functions. However, the available evidence in insects seems to point to an integrated sensorymotor function (Göpfert and Robert 2007), possibly much like that found in the outer hair cells of mammals (Fettiplace and Hackney 2006). Active auditory mechanics manifests itself in several characteristic ways in vertebrate hearing. First, sensitivity and tuning are metabolically vulnerable and, thus, depend on the animal’s physiological condition. Second, the active process introduces a nonlinearity in the mechanical Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 172 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 172 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy response to sound. This response is also metabolically vulnerable; for instance, it can be reversibly suppressed by exposure to carbon dioxide. Third, the mechanical response of the auditory organ or part thereof displays, most compellingly, vibrations taking place in the conspicuous absence of external acoustic stimulation. These autonomous vibrations are known as spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in vertebrates and can be readily recorded, when they occasionally take place, as sounds emitted by the ear (Dallos 1992; Hudspeth 1997; Robles and Ruggero 2001). These features have been used as operational criteria to investigate the presence and modalities of active auditory mechanics in insects. To date, only two species have fulfilled this list of criteria: the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster (Göpfert and Robert 2003) and the culicid mosquito species Toxorhynchites brevipalpis (Göpfert and Robert 2001). What are the manifestations of active hearing in insects? In vertebrates, active mechanisms have been shown to contribute to both sensitivity and frequency selectivity of hearing (Hudspeth 1997; Manley 2001). In invertebrates, active mechanisms have been suggested, unveiled, and characterized only recently (Kössl and Boyan 1998a,b; Göpfert and Robert 2001, 2003; Göpfert et al. 2005) and although their function(s) first appears to be similar, the role it plays in the auditory behavior of the animal has not been addressed (Jackson and Robert 2006; Göpfert and Robert 2007). The sensory ecological relevance and the species-specific function of these mechanisms remain to be explored. The presence of these mechanisms in insect auditory systems has opened the door to a broad range of novel questions and renewed experimental opportunities. Some fundamental questions arise. Do the ultimate and proximate functions of active auditory mechanisms differ between insect and vertebrates? Given their presence in animal lineages that diverged 600 million years ago, are the molecular substrates supporting active mechanisms evolutionarily convergent (Boekhoff-Kalk 2005)? Or do they display a deep-rooted legacy of an earlier evolution of cellular motility? Or is it a seamlessly integrated mix between both? Notably, both the hair cells of vertebrates and the scolopidial mechanosensory neurons of insects are of ciliary origin. The phenomenological convergence of active auditory mechanics between insects and vertebrates seen today may well reflect the combination of fundamental properties deeply rooted in the evolution of fluid-coupled mechanosensory systems and, complementarily, the unavoidable biophysical constraints linked to the sensitive, accurate, and robust detection of weak sound energy. Looking into auditory biodiversity may provide the background necessary to resolve these issues. Does the diverse and independent Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 173 Auditory Systems in Insects 173 evolution of insect hearing organs suggest that active auditory mechanisms, should they be found in other insect species, will be commensurately diverse in structure and function? Enticingly, both ultimate and proximate questions are finding an increasing number of detailed answers (Boekhoff-Falk 2005; Göpfert and Robert 2007). Such issues are also being addressed with much success in other sensory systems of insects, most notably, olfaction (Couto et al. 2005; Hallem and Carlson 2006; see Chapter 4). The evidence for active mechanisms in insect hearing all comes from a series of studies on locusts and moths (Coro and Kössl 1998; Kössl and Boyan 1998a,b), mosquitoes (Göpfert and Robert 2001; Jackson and Robert 2006), and flies (Göpfert and Robert 2003; Göpfert et al. 2005; 2006). The studies on the mechanical behavior of antennal hearing of mosquitoes were first to document a series of nonlinear mechanical responses that could be unambiguously interpreted as the result of active processes (Göpfert and Robert 2001). The use of Drosophila as a model system for hearing (Kernan and Zuker 1995; Eberl 1999) instigated the necessary quantum leap; the presence of tell-tale nonlinearities in Drosophila (Göpfert and Robert 2002) meant that active auditory mechanisms could be investigated at the systemic, cellular, molecular, and genetic levels (Göpfert and Robert 2003; Göpfert et al. 2006). Collectively, these studies established an important fact: The origin of the nonlinear mechanical behavior is to be found in the activity of auditory mechanosensory neurons. The molecules involved in insect audition also are starting to be identified (Caldwell and Eberl 2002; Göpfert and Robert 2003; Kim et al. 2003) and for some of these molecules—the mechanotransduction channels—their role in audition is being progressively unveiled (Göpfert and Robert 2003; Göpfert et al. 2006). It is also apparent that several of the key molecular components of the insect auditory (or mechanoreceptive) machinery have homologs in the vertebrate hair cells (Walker et al. 2000; Caldwell and Eberl 2002; Weber et al 2003). Enticingly, in Drosophila, the molecular substrate supporting the mechanical sensitivity to sound is beginning to be unraveled. Recent evidence links the neuron-based generation of motion that enhances mechanical sensitivity to the concerted actions of “transient receptor potential” (TRP) channels (Kim et al. 2003; Göpfert et al. 2006). A recent genetic and mechanical study has shown that the NompC3 gene, which encodes a mechanotransduction channel protein expressed in sensory neurons, plays a role in augmenting the gain of the antennal system, a measure of its sensitivity to sound (Göpfert et al. 2006). Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 174 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 174 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy Remarkably, the external structures of the antennal auditory system of Drosophila, the arista (Fig. 1B), undergo prominent autonomous vibrations, revealing the oscillations of the mechanosensory neurons of Johnston’s organ (Göpfert and Robert 2003). In wild-type flies, such oscillations can reach several tens of nanometers in magnitude, exhibiting a prominent power output for frequencies below about 300 Hz (Fig. 3A). In dead or anesthetized animals, such fluctuations are much reduced (Göpfert and Robert 2003). This vibrational signature of active mechanisms has been shown to be useful in assessing the effects of various mutations on the ear’s mechanics (Göpfert et al. 2005, 2006); a powerful way of analyzing auditory function by genetic dissection (Caldwell and Eberl 2002). In NompC mutants, for example, the fluctuations are much reduced and yield a power spectral density notably lower than that generated by the wild type (Fig. 3B). Remarkably, for mutations involving the TRPV, vanilloid channels, Nan (Nanchung) and Iav (Inactive), antennal fluctuations are observed to be enhanced, exceeding those of the wild type (Fig. 3C) (Göpfert et al. 2006). Nan and Iav mutants exhibit an enhanced Figure 3. Active auditory mechanics in Drosophila; unveiling the role of TRP chan- nels. In the absence of external stimulation, the external structures of the ear, the arista mainly (see Fig. 1B, Ar), undergo significant mechanical fluctuations that can be measured optically by laser interferometry. (A) Time course (top panel) and spectral composition (bottom panel) of the spontaneous mechanical fluctuations in wildtype flies. (B) Absence of such fluctuations in NompC3 mutants. (C) Excess of mechanical fluctuations in Nan mutants. (Modified, with permission, from Göpfert et al. 2006 © Macmillan Publishers Ltd.) Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 175 Auditory Systems in Insects 175 gain and power generation in their autonomous vibrations (Fig. 3) (Göpfert et al. 2006). It was concluded by Göpfert et al. that the mechanical sensitivity of the Drosophila ear is regulated by the combined actions of the distinct NompC and Nan-Iav channels. In the scheme suggested, NompC3 is considered responsible for the amplificatory process, whereas the molecular dyad Nan-Iav is involved in controlling the amount of gain produced (Göpfert et al. 2006). These seemingly antagonistic effects—reduction and enhancement of gain— suggest the presence of a regulatory feedback loop, the output gain of which is controlled by the concerted action of both types of mechanotransduction channels. Knowing the exact physical localization of these channels and their functional positions in the auditory pathway will be important for understanding the molecular anatomy of active auditory mechanisms. Related to this question is that of the relationship between neuronal signaling and neuronal mechanical power generation in these mechanoreceptive neurons, should these functions be operating concomitantly in a single neuron. Such studies, marrying molecular genetics and auditory mechanics, show that it is becoming possible to dissect the chain of events down to its molecular components and to understand, perhaps molecule by molecule, the structure and mode of operation of the entire auditory machinery. Likewise, comparisons with vertebrate systems will be most interesting, contrasting the respective molecular functional constituents and establishing whether or to what extent they share an evolutionary past. In mosquitoes, the mechanical response of the external auditory structures (the flagellum, or shaft, of the antenna) displays the tell-tale signs of active auditory mechanism (Göpfert and Robert 2001). Analysis of the mechanical behavior of the sensory cells, or genetics-based approaches, have hitherto not been possible. The sound-induced mechanical response of the antenna is resonant, resembling that of a simple harmonic oscillator and displaying a maximal response and a phase transition at resonance frequency. This response, however, changes with stimulus amplitude. In effect, the frequency response of the antennal sound receiver gets more sharply tuned as the amplitude of the stimulus is reduced (Göpfert and Robert 2001). This nonlinear effect enhances the frequency selectivity of the antennal oscillator and also affects its sensitivity to sound. Sensitivity has been measured as the gain of the response; that is, the mechanical output normalized by the magnitude of the sound input (Göpfert and Robert 2001). The antennal system becomes mechanically more sensitive to sound as sound intensity decreases. For some sound intensities, the mechanical Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 176 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 176 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy response of the antenna exceeds that predicted by the acoustic input alone, a strong indication that an active process is at work in Johnston’s organ. This effect is physiologically vulnerable; the gain of the mechanical response does not depend on input intensity in dead or anesthetized animals (Göpfert and Robert 2001). This nonlinear behavior was also examined in the Tanzanian mosquito T. brevipalpis for a broad range of varying sound intensities. Because in nature, for mosquitoes as for any other animal, incident sound stimuli are seldom of constant amplitude, antennal mechanics was investigated to gradually changing sound intensities (Fig. 4), an experiment that has rarely been conducted. A gradual, linear increase in sound intensity starting with low-level background noise was presented to the mosquito (at 380 Hz carrier frequency) as the mechanical response of the antenna was measured by laser Doppler vibrometry (Fig. 4A). For low intensities, the response curve shows a response for which stimulus and response are first linearly related (Fig. 4A, zone a). As stimulus intensity continuously increases, a significant departure from linearity can be observed (Fig. 4, zone b), yielding an amount of mechanical displacement exceeding that imposed to the antenna by sound alone. Further up the intensity scale (Fig. 4, zone c), the mechanical response converges back to the linear regime, eventually entering a compressive regime. Such experiments show that the mechanical response is not that of a passive oscillator and that an active mechanism enhancing acoustic sensitivity may explain such response. Interestingly, the frequency selectivity of the antennal receiver (the mechanical oscillator) is also affected by sound intensity (Fig. 4B). The frequency spectrum of the response gain changes with intensity: The low-intensity response displays a relatively broad spectrum (Fig. 4B, curve a), whereas intensities in the middle of the range generate a sharper frequency spectrum with a higher gain (curve b); at yet higher intensities, the response broadens up, and restitutes a diminished gain (curve c). Altogether, such responses indicate that the sensitivity of the antennal system and its capacity to detect a single frequency are enhanced for certain stimulus intensities, in this case at about 400 nm particle displacement. It thus becomes apparent that maximum gain does not take place at very faint sound levels, perhaps suggesting that the active, actuating function of the neurons in Johnston’s organ takes place well above the reported signaling thresholds, which are in the nanometer range for the same species (Göpfert and Robert 2001). The mechanical response of the male antenna to sound simulating a passing female was also measured, not the least because the sound Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 177 Auditory Systems in Insects 177 Figure 4. Mechanical response of the antennal receiver of the mosquito Toxorhyn- chites brevipalpis to a stimulus of linearly increasing intensity. Carrier frequency of the stimulus is 380 Hz. (A) Mechanical response, measured as the displacement of the antennal tip (see Fig. 1D), as a function of sound intensity, expressed as the near-field particle displacement. The straight line indicates response linearity, against which the antennal response can be compared. For sound intensities in the middle and higher ranges (b, c), the response is nonlinear. This indicates that the antenna moves more than predicted by the energy input provided by sound alone. (B) Frequency spectra of the mechanical response at intensity regimes a, b, and c. In the regions of nonlinearity (b, c), the frequency response reveals the sharpening, and the increase in the gain of the response, an enhancement of mechanical sensitivity. (Data courtesy of J. C. Jackson, unpubl.) produced by a female is what the male auditory system has evolved to detect (Clements 1999). The sound of passing females was recorded and analyzed to characterize their likely amplitude profile and time course. The amplitude profile can be approximated by a Lorentzian function, reflecting the transient onset and offset of the sound of a passing female Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 178 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 178 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy mosquito (Jackson and Robert 2006). Playing back sound at 400 Hz carrier frequency mimicking a female, with such amplitude profile, generates a mechanical response that is quite unexpected but highlights the functional complexity of the mosquito auditory system (Fig. 5A). The gain of the antenna’s mechanical response is nonlinear, as expected, but the time evolution reveals that enhanced mechanical sensitivity (gain) occurs on two occasions. As sound intensity increases, antennal gain exhibits a transient peak (Fig. 5, time ~1.2 seconds, sound intensity ~1 millimeter per second), followed by a compression trough coinciding with the time of maximum intensity of stimulation. Remarkably, a large gain peak takes place as stimulus intensity rapidly decreases. The peak of that secondary gain occurs at time ~2.7 seconds in this example, and coincides with sound intensities much smaller (micrometer range) than those that elicited the primary gain. Such gain fluctuations do not occur in mosquitoes that are freshly killed or anesthetized. This result provides first insight into the possible ultimate function of active mechanisms in insect hearing (Jackson and Robert 2006). The prolonged enhancement of gain allows the antenna to oscillate at the frequency of the female flight for a little bit longer. In effect, it extends the hearing range of the male mosquito by several body lengths, a distance which is not negligible in view of the severe attenuation of sound intensity in the near-field. Clearly, behavioral experiments are now needed to test the predicted extension of the male auditory range, and his capacity to detect and orient toward passing females, with and without the help of active auditory mechanisms. A recent behavioral and acoustical study reports on the acoustic interactions between male and female of T. brevipalpis, providing not only a well-needed behavioral context for mosquito hearing, but also a precious insight on what happens when a female enters the detection space of a male (Gibson and Russell 2006). Measuring the acoustic radiation close to tethered flying animals, that study establishes that the flight tones of both males and females are about 420 Hz and that, notably, frequencies can be quite variable, ranging from ~300 Hz to 500 Hz. In males, change in flight frequency is observed to take place in response to stimulation with a 450-Hz synthetic sound (Fig. 5B); the male’s flight frequency departs from 435 Hz to gradually converge to and hover around the stimulus frequency, even outlasting stimulus duration. Remarkably, when male and female tethered mosquitoes are brought together close enough, their flight tones tend to converge, sometimes generating the frequency beats indicative of near-tuning (Fig. 5C). Interestingly, both male and female seem to be capable of altering their flight Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 179 Auditory Systems in Insects 179 Figure 5. Gain fluctuations of antennal mechanics and adaptive flight-tone behavior of T. brevipalpis in response to female-like acoustic stimulation. (A) Time-resolved gain fluctuations of the male antenna in response to an acoustic stimulus mimicking the amplitude and temporal profiles of a passing female. Gain curves show superimposed responses from five stimulus presentations. The control curve (at gain 2) shows the linear gain response of a freshly killed male mosquito. Stimulus curve is the Lorentzian function approximating a female flyby sound profile. (Modified, with permission, from Jackson and Robert 2006 © National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.) (B) Sonographic representation of the flight tone (435 Hz) of a male T. brevipalpis, and its response to the presentation of a 450-Hz tone. The flight frequency of the male increases gradually to nearly match that of the stimulus. (C) Flight tone of a male in solo flight until a flying female is brought within auditory range. After several seconds, male and female flight tone frequencies nearly match. (Modified, with permission, from Gibson and Russell 2006 © Elsevier.) Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 180 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 180 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy tones, sometimes meeting halfway (Gibson and Russell 2006). This “flying in tune” happens within seconds, a timescale similar to that bringing about gain fluctuations in antennal mechanics (Jackson and Robert 2006). Conversely, male–male interactions lead to a divergence in flight tones. Speculatively, this aversive reaction brings the males apart acoustically, possibly avoiding same-sex courtship, but also perhaps constituting a form of competition in the frequency domain of the acoustic space surrounding them. Interestingly, the study by Gibson and Russell also shows that the behavioral frequency specificity of males is higher than that of the mechanical response of the male antenna. The role of active auditory mechanisms in determining the acoustic sensitivity, the frequency selectivity, and the time-resolved dynamics of this intriguing mate-recognition behavior remains to be investigated, using the multiple tools available to insect auditory science. In conclusion, it may be reasonable to suggest that understanding the mechanics and neurobiology of sound reception in diverse insect species continues to unveil the multiplicity of mechanisms by which biological structures can respond to sound and, considering the larger issues of cell signaling and energetics, transduce and/or dissipate flows of acoustic energy that vary greatly in their amplitude and frequency domains. How this is exactly done remains a central challenge. One may also want to advocate, finally, that the great experimental amenability of insects (Kernan and Zuker 1995; Eberl 1999), their unmatched ability to make complex biology happen at the small end of the biometric scale, along with their awe-inspiring diversity in form and function (Grimaldi and Engel 2005), are poised to contribute original insights into the fascinating mechanisms of hearing. REFERENCES Adams W.B. 1972. Mechanical tuning of the acoustic receptor of Prodenia eridania (Cramer) (Noctuidae). J. Exp. Biol. 57: 297–304. Ashmore J.F. and Mammano F. 2001. Can you still see the cochlea for the molecules? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11: 449 – 454. Autrum H. 1948. Ueber Energie- und Zeitgrenzen der Sinnesempfindungen. Naturwissenschaften 35: 361 – 369. Bangert M., Kalmring K., Sickmann T., Stephen R., Jatho M., and Lakes-Harlan R. 1998. Stimulus transmission in the auditory receptor organs of the foreleg of bushcrickets (Tettigoniidae) I. The role of the tympana. Hearing Res. 115: 27–38. Barth F.G. 2000. How to catch the wind: Spider hairs specialized for sensing the movement of air. Naturwissenschaften 87: 51–58. ———. 2004. Spider mechanoreceptors. Curr Opin. Neurobiol. 14: 415–422. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 181 Auditory Systems in Insects 181 Bernal X.E., Rand A.S., and Ryan M.J. 2006. Acoustic preferences and localization performance of blood-sucking flies (Corethrella Coquillett) to túngara frog calls. Behav. Ecol. 17: 709 – 715. Bialek W. 1987. Physical limits to sensation and perception. Annu. Rev. Biophys. Biophys. Chem. 16: 455 – 478. Bleckmann H., Schmitz H., and von der Emde G. 2004. Nature as a model for technical sensors. J. Comp. Physiol. A 190: 971–981. Boekhoff-Falk G. 2005. Hearing in Drosophila: Development of Johnston’s organ and emerging parallels to vertebrate ear development. Dev. Dyn. 232: 550–558. Borkent A. and Belton P. 2006. Attraction of female Uranotaenia lowii (Diptera: Culicidae) to frog calls in Costa Rica. Can. Entomol. 138: 91–94. Cade W.H. 1975. Acoustically orienting parasitoids: Fly phonotaxis to cricket song. Science 190: 1312 – 1313. Caldwell J.C. and Eberl D.F. 2002. Towards a molecular understanding of Drosophila hearing. J. Neurobiol. 53: 172–189. Clements A.N. 1999. The biology of mosquitoes, Vol. 2. Sensory reception and behavior. CABI Publishing, New York. Coro F. and Kössl M. 1998. Distortion-product otoacoustic emissions from the tympanic organ in two noctuid moths. J. Comp. Physiol. A 183: 525–531. Couto A., Alenius M., and Dickson B. 2005. Molecular, anatomical, and functional organization of the Drosophila olfactory system. Curr. Biol. 15: 1535–1547. Dallos P. 1992. The active cochlea. J. Neurosci. 12: 4575–4585. Eberl D.F. 1999. Feeling the vibes: Chordotonal mechanisms in insect hearing. Curr Opin. Neurobiol. 9: 389 – 393. Edgecomb R.S., Robert D., Read M.P., and Hoy R.R. 1995. The tympanal hearing organ of a fly: Phylogenetic analysis of its morphological origins. Cell Tissue Res. 282: 251 – 268. Farris H.E., Forrest T.G., and Hoy R.R. 1997. The effects of calling song spacing and intensity on the attraction of flying crickets. J. Insect Behav. 10: 659–673. Fettiplace R. and Hackney C.M. 2006. The sensory and motor roles of auditory hair cells. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7: 19 –29. Field L.H. and Matheson T. 1998. Chordotonal organs in insects. Adv. Insect Physiol. 27: 1–28. Fletcher N.H. 1978. Acoustical response of hair receptors in insects. J. Comp. Physiol. A 127: 185 – 189. Fullard J.H. and Yack J.E. 1993. The evolutionary biology of insect hearing. Trends Ecol. Evol. 8: 248 – 252. Gibson G. and Russell I. 2006. Flying in tune: Sexual recognition in mosquitoes. Curr. Biol. 16: 1311 – 1316. Gillespie P.G. and Walker R.G. 2001. Molecular basis of mechanosensory transduction. Nature 413: 194– 202. Gillespie P.G., Dumont R.A., and Kachar B. 2005. Have we found the tip link, transduction channel, and gating spring of the hair cell? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15: 389 – 396. Göpfert M.C. and Robert D. 2000. Nanometre-range acoustic sensitivity in male and female mosquitoes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 267: 453–458. ———. 2001. Active auditory mechanics in mosquitoes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 268: 333 – 339. ———. 2002. The mechanical basis of Drosophila audition. J. Exp. Biol. 205: 1199–1208. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 182 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 182 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy ———. 2003. Motion generation by Drosophila mechanosensory neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100: 5514 – 5519. ———. 2007. Active processes in insect hearing. In Active processes and otoacoustic emissions in hearing (ed. G.A. Manley et al.). Springer Science+Business Media, New York. (in press.) Göpfert M.C., Albert J.T., Nadrowski B., and Kamikouchi A. 2006. Specification of auditory sensitivity by Drosophila TRP channels. Nat. Neurosci. 9: 999–1000. Göpfert M.C., Humphris A.D.L., Albert J.T., Robert D., and Hendrich O. 2005. Power gain exhibited by motile mechanosensory neurons in Drosophila ears. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102: 325 – 330. Grimaldi D. and Engel M.S. 2005. Evolution of the insects. Cambridge University Press, New York. Hallem E.A. and Carlson J.R. 2006. Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell 125: 143 – 160. Hedwig B. 2006. Pulses, patterns and paths: Neurobiology of acoustic behaviour in crickets. J. Comp. Physiol. A 192: 677–689. Hennig R.M., Franz A., and Stumpner A. 2004. Processing of auditory information in insects. Microsc. Res. Tech. 63: 351–374. Hoy R.R. 1998. Acute as a bug’s ear: An informal discussion of hearing in insects. In Comparative hearing: Insects (ed. R.R. Hoy et al.), pp. 1–17. Springer Verlag, New York. Hoy R.R. and Robert D. 1996. Tympanal hearing in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 41: 433 – 450. Hudspeth A.J. 1989. How the ear’s works work. Nature 341: 397–404. ———. 1997. Mechanical amplification of stimuli by hair cells. Curr Opin. Neurobiol. 7: 480 – 486. Imaizumi K. and Pollack G.S. 1999. Neural coding of sound frequency by cricket auditory receptors. J. Neurosci. 19: 1508–1516. Jackson J.C. and Robert D. 2006. Nonlinear auditory mechanism enhances female sounds for male mosquitoes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103: 16734–16739. Jarman A.P. 2002. Studies of mechanosensation using the fly. Hum. Mol. Genet. 11: 1215 – 1218. Johnston C. 1855. Auditory apparatus of the Culex mosquito. Q. J. Microsc. Sci. 3: 97–102. Kämper G. and Kleindienst H.-U. 1990. Oscillation of cricket sensory hairs in a low frequency sound field. J. Comp. Physiol. A 167: 193 – 200. Kernan M. and Zuker C. 1995. Genetic approaches to mechanosensory transduction. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5: 443 – 448. Kim J., Chung W.D., Park D.Y., Choi S., Shin D.W., Soh H., Lee H.W., Son W., Yim J., Park C.S., et al. 2003. A TRPV family ion channel required for hearing in Drosophila. Nature 424: 81 – 84. Knudsen E.I. and Konishi M. 1979. Mechanisms of sound localisation in the barn owl (Tyto alba). J. Comp. Physiol. A 133: 13–21. Kössl M. and Boyan G.S. 1998a. Distortion-product otoacoustic emissions from the ear of a grasshopper. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104: 326–335. ———. 1998b. Otoacoustic emissions from a nonvertebrate ear. Naturwissenschaft 85: 124 – 127. Kristensen N.P. 1981. Phylogeny of insect orders. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 26: 135–157. Manley G.A. 2001. Evidence for an active process and a cochlear amplifier in nonmammals. J. Neurophysiol. 86: 541 – 549. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 183 Auditory Systems in Insects 183 Mason A.C., Oshinsky M.L., and Hoy R.R. 2001. Hyperacute directional hearing in a microscale auditory system. Nature 410: 686–690. McKeever S. 1977. Observations of Corethrella feeding on tree frogs (Hyla). Mosquito News 37: 522 – 523. Michelsen A. 1998. Biophysics of sound localization in insects. In Comparative hearing: Insects (ed. R.R. Hoy et al.), pp. 18–62. Springer, New York. Michelsen A. and Rohrseitz K. 1995. Directional sound processing and interaural sound transmission in a small and a large grasshopper. J. Exp. Biol. 198: 1817–1827. Middlebrooks J.C. and Green D.M. 1991. Sound localization by human listeners. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 42: 135 – 159. Miles R.N., Robert D., and Hoy R.R. 1995. Mechanically coupled ears for directional hearing in the parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98: 3059–3070. Miller L.A. 1977. Directional hearing in the locust Schistocerca gregaria Forskål (Acrididae Orthoptera). J. Comp. Physiol. 119: 85–98. Montealegre-Z F., Morris G.K., and Mason A.C. 2006. Generation of extreme ultrasonics in rainforest katydids. J. Exp. Biol. 209: 4923–4937. Moran D.T., Varela F.J., and Rowley III, J.C. 1977. Evidence for an active role of cilia in sensory transduction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 74: 793–797. Nobili R., Mammano F., and Ashmore J. 1998. How well do we understand the cochlea? Trends Neurosci. 21: 159 – 167. Olson H.F. 1943. Elements of acoustical engineering, pp. 19 – 25. Van Nostrand, New York. Oshinsky M.L. and Hoy R.R. 2002. Physiology of the auditory afferents in an acoustic parasitoid fly. J. Neurosci. 22: 7254–7263. Pollack G.S. 1998. Neural processing of acoustic signals. In Comparative hearing: Insects (ed. R.R. Hoy et al.), pp. 139–196. Springer, New York. Poulet J.F. and Hedwig B. 2002. A corollary discharge maintains auditory sensitivity during sound production. Nature 418: 872–876. ———. 2006. The cellular basis of a corollary discharge. Science 311: 518–522. Rayleigh, Lord. 1876. On our perception of the direction of a sound source. Nature May 11th issue, pp. 32–33. Originally published in Proc. Musical Assoc. 1875–1876, pp. 75–84. Rheinlaender J. and Mörchen A. 1979. ‘Time – intensity trading’ in locust auditory interneurons. Nature 281: 672–674. Robert D. 2001. Innovative biomechanics for directional hearing in small flies. Biol. Bull. 200: 190–194. ———. 2005. Directional hearing in insects. In Sound source localization (ed. A.N. Popper and R.R. Fay.), pp. 6 – 35. Springer-Verlag, New York. Robert D. and Göpfert M.C. 2002. Novel schemes for hearing and acoustic orientation in insects. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12: 715–720. Robert D. and Hoy R.R. 1998. The evolutionary innovation of tympanal hearing in Diptera. In Comparative hearing: Insects (ed. R.R. Hoy et al.), pp. 197–227. Springer, New York. Robert D., Amoroso J., and Hoy R.R. 1992. The evolutionary convergence of hearing in a parasitoid fly and its cricket host. Science 258: 1135–1137. Robert D., Miles R.N., and Hoy R.R. 1996. Directional hearing by mechanical coupling in the parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea. J. Comp. Physiol. A 179: 29–44.47. ———. 1999. Tympanal hearing in the sarcophagid parasitoid fly Emblemasoma sp: Biomechanics of directional hearing. J. Exp. Biol. 202: 1865–1876. Robles L. and Ruggero M.A. 2001. Mechanics of the mammalian cochlea. Phys. Rev. 81: 1305 – 1352. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright. 07_Invert_155-184_cs.qxd 184 5/9/07 3:41 PM Page 184 D. Robert and R.R. Hoy Rose G. and Heiligenberg W. 1985. Temporal hyperacuity in electric fish. Nature 318: 178 – 180. Schul J., Holderied M., von Helversen D., and von Helversen O. 1999. Directional hearing in grasshoppers: Neurophysiological testing of a bioacoustic model. J. Exp. Biol. 202: 121 – 133. Shimozawa T. and Kanou M. 1984. Varieties of filiform hairs: Range fractionation by sensory afferents and cercal interneurons of a cricket. J. Comp. Physiol. A 155: 485–493. Sivan-Loukianova E. and Eberl D.F. 2005. Synaptic ultrastructure of Drosophila Johnston’s organ axon terminals as revealed by an enhancer trap. J. Comp. Neurol. A 491: 46–55. Steel K.P. and Kros C.J. 2001. A genetic approach to understanding auditory function. Nat. Genet. 27: 143 – 149. Tautz J. 1979. Reception of particle oscillation in a medium—An unorthodox sensory capacity. Naturwissenschaft 66: 452–461. Thompson D’Arcy W. 1961. On growth and form. Cambridge University Press, United Kingdom. Thurm U. 1982. Biophysik der Mechanoreception. In Biophysik, 2nd. ed. (ed. W. Hoppe et al.), pp. 691–696. Springer, Berlin. Todi S.V., Sharma Y., and Eberl D.F. 2004. Anatomical and molecular design of the Drosophila antenna as a flagellar auditory organ. Micros. Res. Tech. 63: 388–399. van Staaden M.J. and Römer H. 1998. Evolutionary transition from stretch to hearing organs in ancient grasshoppers. Nature 394: 773–776. von Békésy G. 1960. Experiments in hearing. McGraw-Hill, New York. Walker R.G., Willingham A.T., and Zuker C.S. 2000. A Drosophila mechanosensory transduction channel. Science 287: 2229–2234. Weber T., Göpfert M.C., Winter H., Zimmermann U., Kohler H., Meier A., Hendrich O., Rohbock K., Robert D., and Knipper M. 2003. Expression of prestin homologous SLC26 members in auditory organs of non-mammalian vertebrates and insects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100: 7690 – 7695. Webster D.B., Popper A.N., and Fay R.R., eds. 1992. The evolutionary biology of hearing. Springer, New York. Wightman F. and Kistler D.J. 1993. Sound localization: In Human psychophysics (ed. W.A. Yost et al.), pp. 155–192. Springer Verlag, New York. ———. 1997. Monaural sound localization revisited. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101: 1050–1063. Windmill J.F.C., Göpfert M.C., and Robert D. 2005. Tympanal travelling waves in migratory locusts. J. Exp. Biol. 208: 158–168. Windmill J.F.C., Jackson J.C., Tuck E.L., and Robert D. 2006. Keeping up with bats: Dynamic auditory tuning in a moth. Curr. Biol. 16: 2418–2423. Yack J.E. 2004. The structure and function of auditory chordotonal organs in insects. Micros. Res. Tech. 63: 315 – 337. Yack J.E. and Fullard J.H. 1993. What is an insect ear? Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 86: 677 – 682. Yager D.D. 1999. Structure, development, and evolution of insect auditory systems. Micros. Res. Tech. 47: 380 – 400. Invertebrate Neurobiology © 2007 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 978-087969819-5 For conditions see www.cshlpress.com/copyright.