Redes neuronales y mapas cerebrales durante el aprendizaje del
Transcription
Redes neuronales y mapas cerebrales durante el aprendizaje del
Redes neuronales y mapas cerebrales durante el aprendizaje del lenguaje y la música Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells Catalan Institution for Research and Advanced Studies (ICREA) Cognition and Brain Plasticity Unit Department of Basic Psychology IDIBELL – Institut de Investigació Biomedica de Bellvitge Campus Hospital de Bellvitge University of Barcelona www.brainvitge.org Cognition and Brain Plastivity Unit www.brainvitge.org Meltzoff, Kuhl, Movellan, Terrence & Sejnowski, Science 2009 “Suppose and adult and a child human arrive on Mars and discover that there are Martians who seem to speak a language to one another. If the adult and child human stay on Mars for several years and try to learn Martian, what do you think will be the outcome?” L. Gleitman and E. Newport, 1995 Invitations to Cognitive Science, Language From Gupta et al., 2004 Why do we want to study language learning? Language Learning streams Bilingualism Language Learning mechanisms - Dorsal audio-motor - Ventral meaning integration General Learning mechanisms - Episodic lexical interface - Reward-motivation circuits Individual Differences: Brain Plasticity Res. Framework: i. Human Simulation paradigm ii. Comparisons with other learning domains iii. The interface between cognitive control, reward-motivation and lang. learning iv. Indiv.Differences in Lang. Learning v. Infants and Patients: Intervention & re-learning strategies Translational research: Re-learning and neurorehab. Selective or strategic use of one-mechanism Brain mechanisms involved in “on-line Language” Learning (first evidences) a1. Speech segmentation problem Cunillera et al., NI 2009 Lopez-Barroso et al., Cer. C. 2011; PNAS 2013 De Diego-Balaguer et al., Plos One, 2007 a2. Learning the meaning of new-words Mestres-Misse et al., Cer. C. 2007 Mestres-Misse et al., JOCN 2008, 2009 NI 2010 Rodriguez-Fornells et al., PTRSL 2010 Speech segmentation and word recognition problem How do infants segment words from continous speech? Can you see silences signaling the onset of each word? Saffran et al., 1996: a statistical learning mechanism might be used for language acquisition in infants 2 minutes of exposition to nonsense syllabes (8 months): golabopadotibidakutupirogolabopadotigolabobidakutupiro.... Transition P syllables within a word (go-la) = 1 Transition P syllables in between words (bo-pa)= 0.33 words: golabo padoti bidaku tupiro mean = 7.9 seg. non-words: dakupa labopa kutupi pirogo mean = 8.8 seg. But, which is the output of the statistical learning mechanism in infants segmenting speech? (Saffran et al., 2001) Familiarization phase (Language A: pabiku tibudo golatu daropi ) English frame I like my We saw the You play with What a nice pabiku tibudo tudaro pigola Nonsense frame Zy fike my Gee baw fuh Foo dray miff Vut luh kife pabiku tibudo tudaro pigola Design Language conditions: pirutabagolitokudapirutabagoligukibobagolitokudapirutagukibo.... Random conditions: pigukukidatagotolibobarubakutorupigubotakidaligolitokubabogo…. 1st block 2nd block 3rd block 4th block Behavioral test Language A 2 min. 2 min. 2 min. 2 min. Baseline A piruta Language B Baseline B bobago Audio-motor interface in speech segmentation Language conditions: pirutabagolitokudapirutabagoligukibo Random conditions: pigukukidatagotolibobarubakutorupi McNealy et al., JN 2006 Cunillera et al., Neuroimage 2009 Dorsal audio-motor interface involved in Speech segmentation Superior Longitudinal fasciulus (SLF) Efference copy of the “articulated possible new word”, and fine-tuning the template-matching algorithm Mapping the encoded sequence of sounds into a sequence of articulatory gestures (for keeping the segmented word active through rehearsal – Phonological WM) Output: ordered sequence of auditory representations feedforwaded to the PMC Template-matching algorithm = detection of coincidences btw the stored representations and “phonological templates” derived from previous exposures (Warren et al., 2005) Rodriguez-Fornells et al., 2009 Phil. Trans R.S. B-Biol. Sci. Rauschecker & Scott, 2009; Hickok & Poeppel, 2003, 2007; Scott & Wise 2004 Functional connectivity of the dorsal audio-motor interface Identification of three different dorsal learning networks involved in word learning using Independent Component Analysis (ICA) Two different studies fMRI (3T), same paradigm: 1st cohort: 27 Spanish participants 2nd cohort:16 German participants Lopez-Barroso, Ripolles, et al. (2015 Neuroimage) Auditory-motor coupling hypothesis in MUSIC LEARNING actions to sounds sounds to actions Dorsal audio-motor coupling induced by MUSIC LEARNING in chronic stroke hemiparetic patients Rodriguez-Fornells et al., 2012 A. N.Y. A. Sciences Under review. Nature Sc. Reports Word learning (speech segmentation) Rodriguez-Fornells et al., 2009 Phil. Trans R.S. B-Biol. Sci. NEW questions Language Learning Bilingualism Learning streams - Dorsal audio-motor - Ventral meaning integration - Episodic lexical interface Individual Differences: Brain Plasticity Translational research: Re-learning and neurorehab. Selective or strategic use of one-mechanism Individual Differences in Speech Segmentation and Arcuate Fasciculus tractography (Diffusion Tensor Imaging DTI) Language conditions: pirutabagolitokudapirutabagoligukibo Lopez-Barroso, Catani, et al.. (PNAS 2013) Individual Differences in Speech Segmentation and Arcuate Fasciculus tractography (Diffusion Tensor Imaging DTI) Lopez-Barroso, Catani, et al.. (PNAS 2013) > The AF and in relation to auditory-motor integration, might represent a key step for language evolution. > Comparative studies indicate that apes and monkeys have homologs for Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas in terms of grey matter connected by a dorsal and a ventral pathway (Rilling et al., 2011). However, the trajectory of the AF is different between species. Middle and inferior temporal gyri terminations from the inferior frontal gyrus are more prominent in humans than in macaques and chimpanzees (Rilling et al., 2008). > These findings suggest that the ability to integrate auditorymotor information confers some advantages in the manipulation of the representation of acoustic stimuli, thus allowing its storage in the long-term memory. Rilling et al., 2012 Rilling et al., 2008 Ontogenetically, the AF develops slower than other associative pathways, including the ventral pathway (Perani et al., 2011). The AF’s terminations connecting with Broca’s area show progressive development during childhood, still under development at the age of 7 y (Brauer 2011). However, the connections with premotor cortex, those responsible to auditory-motor integration, can be tracked in newborns (Brauer et al., 2013, Perani et al., 2011), suggesting a role in early language acquisition. Brauer et al., 2013 Brain language streams involved in “on-line Language” Learning a1. Speech segmentation problem a2. Learning the meaning of new-words Cunillera et al., NI 2009 Lopez-Barroso et al., Cer. C. 2011 De Diego-Balaguer et al., Plos One, 2007 Mestres-Misse et al., Cer. C. 2007 Mestres-Misse et al., JOCN 2008, 2009 NI 2010 Rodriguez-Fornells et al., PTRSL 2010 Word-learning task (Mestres-Missé et al., Cer. Cor. 2007; J. Cog Neuropsco. 2008) Non-word meaning M+ Mario always forgets where he leaves the rankey It was expensive the repair of the rankey I punctured again the wheel of the rankey Non-word no-meaning MI have bought the tickets for the garty On the construction-site you must wear a garty Everyday I buy two loaves of fresh garty Real word R My brother Peter is buying a bit of salt The rice we made needs a bit more salt. My doctor advised me not to eat with salt. a. Cloze probability of the sentences (N = 160) 1ª frase: 6,11% (SD= 10,38) 2ª frase: 28,97% (SD= 18,97) 3ª frase: 76,05% (SD= 17,75) b. Meaning extraction after reading the sentences (N = 15): 91.12 % (10.73) c. Meaning extraction in the ERP experiment app. 84% (N = 24) (Mestres-Missé et al., Cer. Cor. 2007) Mestres-Missé et al., Cerebral Cor. 2007 See also recent studies using this task: Batterink & Neville, 2011; Borovsky, Elman & Kutas, 2012 Implications of the Meaning Integration interface in the contextual learning task Non-word meaning - M+ Non-word no-meaning - MReal word - R Mestres-Misse et al., JoCN 2008 Mestres-Misse et al., JoCN 2008 Question? i. Reward – motivated Language Learning. fMRI-DTI combined study Language Learning streams Language Learning mechanisms - Dorsal audio-motor (development) - Ventral meaning integration General Learning mechanisms - Episodic lexical interface - Reward-motivation circuits Individual Differences Is there any pleasure associated to language learning experiences? Ripolles et al., Current Biology, 2014 a. Speech provides critical emotional value to children promoting infant-parent bounding (hearing and adult voice provides comfort and pleasure experiences in early stages of development) (Kuhl et al., 2005). b. Infant-directed prosodically enriched speech communication of affect –arousal modulation). (attract infant’s attention and c. In stressful situations children experience increased oxytocin release upon hearing their mother’s voice (Seltzer 2010; DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). This hormone release promotes affiliative behaviors and is closely related to emotion and reward processing (Strathearn et al., 2009). d. Infants pleasure depends on communication. This could explain the internal “drive to learn new words” (intrinsically motivated). Importance of social interaction in Language Learning (Bruner, 1983; Vigotsky, 1962) e. Social gating perspective in Language acquisition (Bloom, Tomasello, Kuhl, Goldstein). Infant language is gated by the motivational properties (attention and arousal) inherent in social interactions (Kuhl, 2007). a. At EVOLUTIONARY LEVELS, recent cortical language and communication systems might have relayed on existing subcortical (emotion-reward) mechanisms (Syal and Finlay, 2011; Panksepp, 1998), reinforcing human motivation to learn a new language (inner drive). b. The initial development of a ‘‘protolanguage’’ in human ancestors was probably crucial for sharing information-knowledge and emotions, improving success on reward-seeking behaviors, bonding social groups, and increasing the chances of group survival in competitive environments. c. This protolanguage might have been naturally selected and reinforced by interlinking it with ancient brain mechanisms involved in hedonic reward processing d. Finally, adult individuals that are language learning “addicts” (language learning seekers). Rodriguez-Fornells et al., 2010 Phil. Trans R.S. B-Biol. Sci. Dopamine and Reward-Motivated Learning Lisman & Grace, The hippocampal-VTA loop controlling the entry of information in Long Term Memory. Neuron 2005 Evidences of dopaminergic modulation of word learning (Knecht et al., An. Neurol 2004) Neural correlates of listening to highly rewarding music Salimpoor & Zatorre NN 2012, Science 2014 Sescouse et al., NI2013 Ventral striatum (NAc) Ventral striatum (NAc) Camara et al., JN 2010 Ripolles et al., Current Biology 2014 Sescouse et al., NI2013 Ripolles et al., CB 2014 Ripolles et al., CB 2014 CONCLUSIONS on motivation-reward and Language learning: Thus, consistent with the songbird’s instinct to learn to sing, human beings also display an urge to acquire language (Fitch, 2010), and both adaptive behaviors might be driven by similar, phylogenetically older, reward-related circuits. Songbirds possess area X, a striatal nucleus analog to the human basal ganglia, which is crucial for song learning in both young and adult birds (Margoliash & Nusbaum, 2009). In addition, area X receives midbrain dopaminergic projections (Mooney, 2009) and shows increased FoxP2 (a gene associated in humans with language and speech) expression during periods of learning (Scharff & Haesler, 2005). Reduced cortical and reward subcortical connectivity in autistic children (which might difficult the capacity of them to experience speech as a rewarding episode) and influencing the correct development of their communication (Abrams et al., PNAS 2013). Doupe et al., TICS 2005 CONCLUSIONS on motivation-reward and Language learning: Thus, consistent with the songbird’s instinct to learn to sing, human beings also display an urge to acquire language (Fitch, 2010), and both adaptive behaviors might be driven by similar, phylogenetically older, reward-related circuits. Songbirds possess area X, a striatal nucleus analog to the human basal ganglia, which is crucial for song learning in both young and adult birds (Margoliash & Nusbaum, 2009). In addition, area X receives midbrain dopaminergic projections (Mooney, 2009) and shows increased FoxP2 (a gene associated in humans with language and speech) expression during periods of learning (Scharff & Haesler, 2005). Reduced cortical and reward subcortical connectivity in autistic children (which might difficult the capacity of them to experience speech as a rewarding episode) and influencing the correct development of their communication (Abrams et al., PNAS 2013). Previous studies have shown that insensitivity to the human voice may underlie the hallmark communication deficits of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Kanner, 1943). The social motivation theory of ASD suggest that deficits in representing the reward value of social stimuli, including voice-speech, impedes children with ASD from actively engaging with these stimuli and consequently impairs social skill development (Dawson et al., 2002) The findings suggest that weak connectivity between speech and reward centers impede children with ASD from experiencing speech as pleasurable, thus influencing how social communication skills develop, according to the authors Abrams et al., 2013 CONCLUSIONS on motivation-reward and Language learning: This hypothesis favors current perspectives, which emphasize that language was an evolutionary innovation built on different preexisting cognitive capabilities, probably ‘‘hijacking’’ old evolutionary solutions as reward-reinforcement mechanisms. Language learning could then rely on the interaction between general-domain cognitive abilities (e.g., theory of mind, associative learning, analogical processing, or joint attention) and more-specific linguistic ones (Bloom 1992). Syal and Finlay, 2011 Future questions: i. Comparison of infant neurophysiological mechanisms in place for Language Learning with adult mechanisms. ii. If infants isolated and “learn” the word, how is it going to be represented and structured in their Mental Lexicon we need longitudinal studies iii. Memory systems (the interplay between episodic and semantic memory) iv. Individual differences in the plasticity of the different language learning streams (dorsal and ventral). Role of metaplasticity in these language streams (or how initial learning could constrain future learning). v. Plan strategic rehabilitation programs based on preserved grey and white matter regions in aphasic patients and based on the function of the different language learning streams (stroke-aphasia project) vi. On-going studies on infant perinatal stroke (MRI – fine grained language learning evaluation) vii. Follow-up the study of pleasure and reward in Language Learning in at risk infants (ASD) Ramon y Cajal, 1904, “Texture of the Nervous system of man and the vertebrates”. “Todo el mundo sabe que la habilidad de un pianista para tocar su instrumento y su adaptación a esta profesión, requiere de muchos años de gimnasia mental y muscular. Pero para poder comprender este fenómeno tan importante, es necesario aceptar que, además de el refuerzo que se produzca en las vías cerebrales pre-existentes, nuevas vías deben crearse mediante la ramificación o crecimiento progresivo de los terminales dendríticos y los procesos axonales.” Supported by: Ramon y Cajal research program from the MCYT, Spain Spanish Government, I+D programs BSO2002, SEJ2005 Catalan Government (SGR research groups, NECOM group) www.brainvitge.org Volkswagenstifung (2005-2007) European coordinated program Fundació La Marato (programa Neurociencies, 2007) Icrea Junior and Senior programs • Clement Francois, Pablo Ripolles & Claudia Peñazola • Diana Lopez-Barroso, Toni Cunillera, Anna Mestres-Misse, Ruth de Diego Balaguer • Dept. Neuropsychology and Klinik für Neurologie II. Otto von Guericke University. Magdeburg: T.F. Münte, M. Rotte, & C. Tempelmann & Toemme Noesselt • Department of Psychology, University of Turku: M. Laine • Temple University: Nadine Martin