Investigation of the Availability Heuristic in the Estimation of the
Transcription
Investigation of the Availability Heuristic in the Estimation of the
Investigation of the Availability Heuristic in the Estimation of the Frequency of Names IB HL Psychology IA Sample 4 Table of Contents Abstract Introduc tion Method Results Discussion Work Cited Appendix i - Standardized Instructions Appendix il - Informed Letter of Consent Appendix ill - Debriefi ng Notes Appendix iv - List 1: 20 More Famous Names and 17 Less famous Names Appendix v - List 2: 17 More Famous Names and 20 Less Famous Names Appendix vi - Raw Data Appendix vil - Mann Whitney Test Calculations Abstract According to Tversky & Kahneman (1973), av ailability heuristic is a circu which people estimate the frequency of an event, or the amount of somet class, based on how easily an example can be brought to mind. The aim of experiment was to test if the availability heuristic has an effect on the partici ability to estimate the frequency of famous or not so famous names written on It was hypothesized that the availability heuristic will have an affect on the participants' ability to estimate the frequency of famous or not so famous nam on lists given. The design was independent measures design and the partit (N=12) were chosen using the opportunity sampling method. Group 1 ha( 20 More Famous names in and 17 Less Famous names read aloud to them 2 had a list of 17 More Famous names in and 20 Less Famous names read them. The independent variable of this study was the number of famous ve famous names read out from lists to the participants. The dependent variable study was the list that was estimated as having a larger amount of names on it or not so famous. A Mann Whitney test was used to determine the significance of the result results showed that the information was not signifi cant because our reall support the hypothesis. Therefore the null hypothesis was retained and we conclusion that availability heuristic does not have an effect on one's ability t( the frequency of More 'Famous or Less Famous names that were read aloud frt Word Count: 280 A. Introduction Availability heuristic is a human cognitive bias when people predict the frequ, event, or a quantity within a population, based on how easily an example can I about. It can cause one to overestimate probabilities of events that are associat memorable or vivid amounts. Due to these memorable events being overstate( coverage in the media, the bias is intensified in society. Two examples of this probability of plane crashes and the kidnapping of children, both of which are but due to the amount of media coverage on incidents relating to them, big ma the people overestimate their probability and therefore behave consequently. Av ailability heuristic was first shown in a study conducted by Tversky & Kahl 1973 they researched J u d g m availability ent investigate bias in judgments about lists of names. In their experin presented 4 -u n d elistsr to the participants. Two of the lists had 19 famous women an famous u n cmen e on r them, t a and the other two lists had 19 famous men and 20 less h -wi omen. n t With y : the use of a between-groups design, which describes the statistic comparison h e u of two r or i more different groups of subjects that are focused on difi experiences s t i orctreatments, the first group was asked to recall as many names a! and a the second n group was asked to estimate what was more frequent, famous n less d famous names. The results gave a two-fold implication. To begin with, the that b the famous i a names were most easily recalled compared to the less famous Additionally, s e despite s the fact that the less famous names were more frequent, t, majority of the participants estimated that the famous names were called more " This i shows that a key factor that emerged from this study is that although the a heuristic n serves as an effective strategy in many situations, leading to accurate, they o can also lead to 'systematic errors', especially when judging frequency. r Another study on availability heuristic was a study by Schwarz et al (1991), in they had participants describe examples of behavior, either very assertive or ve unassertive, behaviors in which they acted out in different role-playing scenari( participants were also asked to describe either 6 or 12 examples of assertive or unassertive behavior. Afterward the participants were asked to rate their own assertiveness. The results showed that the participants rated themselves as bein assertive after describing 6, instead of 12, examples for an assertive behavior c( and rated themselves as being less assertive after describing 6, instead of 12, ex unassertive behavior. The study showed the implications of recalled content we qualifi ed by the ease with the personal content that could be brought to mind. Chapman & Chapman (1967) conducted a study, in which they described a bias judgment of the frequency in which two events co-occur. Their study showed tl: occurrence of a paired distinctive stimuli resulted in an overestimation of the fr( of certain pairings. In order to test this theory, judges were presented with infori that concerned several hypothetical mental patients. The data for each patient cc a clinical d i 111 drawing. The participants clearly overestimated the frequency of co-occurren( connections, such as suspiciousness and peculiar eyes. This effect was labelec correlation". The strength of the associative link between the two events conic the basis for one's judgment of how frequently the two events co-occur. Whei association is strong, one is more likely to conclude that the events have been frequently. Therefore, strong associates will be estimated as having occurred t frequently. Aim: The aim of this experiment is to test if the availability heuristic has an at participants' ability to estimate the frequency of famous or not so famous nam on lists given. Hypothesis: Av ailability heuristic will have an affect on the participants' abilli estimate the frequency of famous or not so famous names written on lists give Null Hypothesis: Av ailability heuristic will not have an affect on the participai to estimate the frequency of famous or not so famous names written on lists gi B. Method: Design The type of design we have chosen for our experiment was an independent me, design. We had to conduct the experiment on two different groups, both under conditions. The advantage of using an independent measures design is that it keep the validity of the results high, even though we have a small number of gi Controls that we have undertaken to avoid confounding variables are making s do not explain what we are looking for before the experiment is started, readinl names from the list instead of giving it to the participants, and conducting the e on two separate groups to avoid the expectancy effect. The independent variabl experiment is the number of famous versus not so famous names read out from participants, and the dependent variable is the list that was estimated as having amount of names on it, famous or not so famous. Ethical guidelines were met b the participants a letter of consent before the experiment and by debriefing ther rules and instructions of the experiment. C. Method: Participants In our experiment we had two groups of 12 students; all were between the ages 18. In group I, which we read the list of 20 More Famous names and 17 Less F names (see app. iv) to, there were three males and nine females, nine of the stuc group I were non-native English speakers, and in group 2, which we read the li! More Famous names and 20 Less Famous names (see app.v) to, there were six six females, nine of the students were non-native English speakers. Our particip made up of multicultural, multilingual students that all came from an internatioi We used opportunity sampling because the sample was already given to us base when we were avniinhie Thp d v n t n a would be made up of a lot of the same kinds of people, for example, all the pe sample were students between the ages of 16 — 18. In order to decide which gi which list, we fl ipped a coin and it was decided that group I would have list I consisted of 20 More Famous names and 17 Less Famous names read to them 2 would have list 2, which consisted of 17 More Famous names and 20 Less names, read to them. Materials - 2 5 pieces of paper - 2 5 pens 25 copies of the letter of consent for the participants to sign (appet - L i s t 1 (see appendix iv) - L i s t 2 (appendix v) D. Method: Procedure Kahneman & Tversky (1973) 1) D iv id e participants into two groups of 12 assigning the participants intc and 2. 2) A s k participants to sign consent forms (see appendix ii) 3) Dis tribute paper and pen to participants. 4) Read aloud list 1 to group I (see appendix iv) 5) Read aloud list to 2 to group 2 (see appendix v) 6) A s k group 1 to estimate which category has more names in it, more fan less famous, and write estimation in figures on their paper. The particip have 1 minute to do so. 7) A s k group 2 to estimate which category has more names in it, more fan] less famous, and write estimation in figures on their paper. The particip have 1 minute to do so. 8) Collec t papers from participants. 9) D ebr ief participants by explaining them the purpose of the experiment,i them for participating, and let them know that once the results are calcu w ill be available for their viewing. (see app. iii) E. Results: Descriptive Table 1. The Means and Standard Deviations ' Mean Standard Deviation Group 1 More Famous Less Famous Names Names 19.58 14.25 8.59 6.92 Group 2 More Famous Le Names 16.83 4.75 Figure 1. w o n ttumabom K y v e t Wotv NoovivoA q•ovi op t i r o , i • The Level of data used was interval (see app. vi for raw data). The mean gives average number, and standard deviation shows you how far away it is from the Table 2 shows that the mean of group 1 More Famous names was 19.58, the m( group 2 More Famous names was 16.83; the mean of group 1 Less Famous nan 14.25, and the mean of group 2 Less Famous names was 16.30. The standard d( group 1 More Famous names was 8.59, the standard deviation of group 2 More names was 6.92, the standard deviation of group I Less Famous names was 4.7. standard deviation of group 2 Less Famous names was 3.60. The size of the sta deviation for More Famous names in group I is large compared to the mean, ov size of the mean. Although most of the numbers range from 10 — 25 there are nt less as 8 and as large 40, which can throw off the data because they are outliers. of the standard deviation for Less Famous names in group I is large compared t mean, it is half the size of the mean, this is because there were numbers ranging 25, many of which were outliers. F. Results: Inferential The type of test used to carry out the calculations was the Mann Whitney Tes vii for Mann Whitney calculations). We used this test because we had an inde samples design. Because of the size of the sample and the lack of a standard d of data, the level of data was reduced to ordinal. The U value of 63.5 does not critical value of 0.05. Consequently, the results are not significant and may be chance. The null hypothesis is retained. Therefore, availability heuristic will n affect on the participants' ability to estimate the frequency of famous or not s( names written on lists given. F. Discussion The results show that availability heuristic does not have an effect on the esti' the frequency of names read from a list. Although the mean in group I shove majority of the participants guessed that the frequency of the names read fron -More Famous Names was higher then the list of names read from the list of L Names, the mean of group 2 showed that the majority of the participants gues there was a similar amount of names read from both lists. Therefore, with the Mann Whitney Test, we were able to fi nd out that our information was not sig In the study conducted by Tversky & Kahneman (1973), their results were sig.] whereas ours were not. Tversky 8z.• Kahneman were looking at how a class whc instances are easily retrieved will seem larger than a class of equal frequency v occurrences are not as easily retrievable. Our experiment did not confirm this t Tversky & Kahneman also factored gender into their study, this is something ti out. In order to get more in-depth information, if this study was to be repeated, should be factored in. In Chapman & Chapman's (1967) study, they asked juc4 associate a mental patient with a drawing, whereas in our study we were asklN participants to associate names heard with familiarity. Chapman & Chapman f( there was a clear overestimation of frequency, whereas our results showed othe Schwarz et al (1991) showed that implications of recalled content were qualifie familiarity of the content, our results showed that even though one of the lists v familiar, it didn't help the participants estimate the amount of names in the lists A confounding variable in our experiment was language. Many of the participa of 12 in both groups, were non-native speakers, this is because an independent ! design was used and therefore there was participant variability. The language 13 be a problem when trying to understand the names read aloud and the instructio culture some of the participants might have been more or less familiar with the the lists. In the future it would be wise to make sure that the sample was only ol culture. Based on the results of this experiment, availability heuristic does not have a one's ability to estimate the frequency of More Famous or Less Famous narr read aloud from lists. Wor k Cited: Anissimov, Michael. "What is Av ailability Bias?" wisegeek. N.p., 9 Sept. 20 - Web. 1 Nov. 2010. <http://www.wis egeek .c om/ , what isChapman & Chapman, L. J. (1967). Illusory Correlation in Observational R a v a of i Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 151-155. Journal labil i t y - S. (1997). The Av ailability Heuristic: Effects of Fame and Gende, McKelvie, bias Estimated Frequency of Male and Female Names, journal of Social Psycho!, . 137(1), 63-78. Retrieved from Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collect database. h t m > . Appendix i. Standardized Instructions Welcome. As you were previously informed, you are taking part in a study. F to the names that will be read aloud by the researchers, and follow the instruct to you. Appendix L e t t e r of Consent , amw illin g t o takepar ti n this experiment. am informed and aware of the instructions involved, and the experiment's ai investigate the availability heuristic when judging a list of names given to a gt multicultural, multilingual participants depending on the familiarity of the nail I am aware that I have the ability to withdraw my results at any point in time d experiment. I am aware that by participating in this study I will endure no physical or ment to myself or others. I am aware that I will be debriefed after the experiment's procedure is carried ( results have been recorded. Gender: Are you a native English speaker? Y e s / No Participant's signature: Date of experiment: Appendix D e b r i e fi n g Notes We would like to thank you for participating in our experiment. We had you number of names from a list containing famous people names and a list conta well-known people's names. We wanted you to do this as we were examining heuristics and wanted to determine if you estimated that there were more nam of famous people names than the list of less well known peoples names. We fo there was not a concrete correlation in this study. Thus, we have had to accept hypothesis of the same frequency of number of words will be estimated for mo names and less famous names by the multicultural, multilingual group of parti attending a international school, regardless of the familiarity of the names and many times they are mentioned. Thank you. Appendix iv.. List 1 ()I'M More Famous Names and 17 Less Famous Arames 20 famous names 1. Brad Pitt 1 2. Megan Fox 1 1 3. Orlando Bloom 1 4. Kate Hudson 5. Tom Cruise 2 1 1 Penelope Cruz . Jennifer Garner . George Clooney 5 . Angelina Jolie . Miley Cyrus 6 1 8. Mariah Carey . 4 1 7. Britney Spears Ben Affl eck 3 1 6. Nicole Kidman . 7 8 W i l l Smith . Jennifer Aniston 9. Tom Hanks 1 9 . Will Farell 10. Lil Wayne 2 0 . Vince Vaughn 17 less famous 1. C hr is Pine 10. Ashely Benson 2. Benjamin Bratt 11. Kelly Rutherford 3. E l l e Fanning 12. Donald Faison 4. Matthew Morriosn 5. C hr is Colfer 13. Kelly Rowan 14. Peter Gallagher 6. D o u g Savant 15. Lee Norris 7. K y l e MacLachian 16. Moira Kelly 8. L i s a Edelstein 17. Carlos Bernard 9. J u lie Benz Appendix v. - Lis t 2 20 famous names o f 1 Brad 7 Pitt L M o 2. Megan Fox r e F Orlando a 3. Bloom m o 4. Kate Hudson u s 5.NTom Cruise a 6. Nicole Kidman m 7.e Britney Spears s 8.aMariah Carey 9.nTom Hanks d 2 17 0 less famous L 1. Chris Pine e s 2. Benjamin Bratt s 3. E l l e Fanning F a 4. Mat t hew Morriosn m5. C h r is Colfer o u 6. D o u g Savant s 7. K y l e MacLachlan N a 8. L i s a Edelstein m9. J u lie Benz e s 10. Ashely Benson 10. Lil Wayne 11. Ben Aftleck 12. Penelope Cruz 13. Jennifer Garner 14. George Clooney 15. Angelina Jolie 16. Miley Cyrus 17. Will Smith 11. Kelly Rutherford 12. Donald Faison 13. Kelly Rowan 14. Peter Gallagher 15. Lee Norris 16. Moira Kelly 17. Carlos Bernard 18. Elisha Cuthbert 19. David Duchovny 20. Evan Handler Appendix vi: Table 1: Raw Data for Group 1 and 2 Frequency Estimates of Names in Group 1 More Famous 13 25 15 20 40 16 30 15 18 15 20 Group 2 Less Famous 8 25 15 15 5 15 20 9 20 17 20 2 More Famous 15 15 21 23 9 18 20 20 15 13 10 23 Less 15 18 17 19 11 21 20 20 10 13 15 17 Appendix vii: 1 Sample A: Group 1 More Famous names Sample B: Group 2 More Famous names. Ranks for 1c ount 1 Sample A 1 Sample B 1 R a w Data fo r 1Sample A j Sample B 4.5 IS 2 22 8.5 25 3 8.5 19 2 1 4 IS 16.5 203 20 40 2 3 Critical Values of U fo r rq--12. ntoLevel of Significance for a Directional Tes t .05 I . 0 2 5 1 . 0 1 Non-Directional Test 24 2 6 12 U.S 16 18 7 23 16.5 30 8 20 •5 16.5 Ls 20 1l ower limit 1 5 13 1upper limit 9 133 115 18 10 8.5 4.5 15 163 3 -— 20.5 0. 20 12 1 9 1 0 23 1. 0 5 42 102 3 7 1 0 7 . 0 2 I 31 1 1 3