developmental dyslexia
Transcription
developmental dyslexia
Links between Action and Perception in Developmental Dyslexia Alice van de Walle de Ghelcke (1), Marie-Anne Schelstraete (1), Martin Gareth Edwards (1), Patrick Quercia (2,3) & Charalambos Papaxanthis (2). (1) Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l’Éducation, Université Catholique de Louvain (U.C.L.), Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. (2) Laboratoire INSERM U 1093 Cognition, Action et Plasticité sensorimotrice, Université de Bourgogne (U.B.), Dijon, France. (3) Département d’Ophtalmologie, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire (C.H.U.), Dijon, France. Participants and method Introduction Developmental dyslexia is a specific, severe and persistent disorder of reading acquisition that appears independent of mental, neurological, visual, hearing or educational deficits (W.H.O., 1993 as cited in Habib, 19971), is frequently observed (Barrouillet et al., 20072) and significantly interferes with school learning and daily living activities (W.H.O., 1993 as cited in Habib, 19971). Despite this term, there remains a poor understanding of the relation between developmental dyslexia and other cognitive and motor disorders which are frequently associated to it (Barrouillet et al., 20072 ; Grammaticos & Klees, 20003 ; Bosse, 20044). In this study, we propose to revise the understanding of developmental dyslexia using a cognitive-motor perspective suggesting that dyslexia can be understood as a proprioceptive dysfunction syndrome (Quercia et al., 20045). Proprioceptive system is involved in the postural regulation but also in the motor control. During the execution of a movement, the C.N.S. uses feedback and feedforward control (Berthoz, 19976) involving the parietal cortex and the cerebellum, which are coincidently involved in the motor control (Decety, 1996 ; Ehrsson et al., 2003 ; Fadiga et al., 2004 ; Jeannerod, 2001 as cited in Skoura et al., 20097) and reading ability (Price, 1998 ; Nicolson et al., 1995 ; Fullbright, 1999 ; Turkeltaub, 2002 as cited in Barrouillet et al., 20072 ; Quercia et al., 20088). These brain areas are recognized as presenting constitutional and functional abnormalities associated to developmental dyslexia (Léonard et al., 1993 ; Paulesu et al. 1996 ; Simos et al., 2000 ; Brown, 2001 ; Nicolson et al., 2001 as cited in Habib, 20029). 18 dyslexics and 18 normo-readers teenagers (mean age = 15.2 ± 1.1 years) were selected from anamnesis, speech therapist and praxis standardized assessments. 1) General motor imagery ability ; MIQ-R (Hall & Martin, 199711) = motor imagery scores for visual and kinesthetic modalities 2) Fitt’s target pointing task (Sirigu et al., 199612) Figure A : Illustration of the pointing movement Figure B : 5 target’s sizes and their corresponding I.D. According to simulation theory, the same feedforward cognitive processes used for planning an action are used for mentally simulating an action (Jeannerod, 200910). We propose that developmental dyslexia may be linked to impairments in the use of feedforward motor control processes, and therefore, participants with developmental dyslexia should present difficulties in the use of action imager simulation. = movement times (real, mental) and kinesthetic imagery scores Results 1) General motor imagery ability ; MIQ-R : dyslexic group < control group for visual (U=64, p=0.002) and kinesthetic (U=66, p=0,002) imagery modalities. 2) Fitt’s target pointing task Discussion and conclusion By revealing significant differences between the two groups, these two tasks highlight a motor imagery deficiency in the dyslexic group. Unlike the control group, it seems that the dyslexic group was unable to cognitively simulate the action, perhaps suggesting a deficit of feedforward action models. To our knowledge, this study is the first investigation concerning the motor imagery ability of dyslexics participants. The results encourages additional investigations concerning the links between motor imagery, action execution and developmental dyslexia. By using new motor imagery tasks, sensory-motor rehabilitation, several techniques of accurate measures and additional controls groups, these next studies will allow to better understand the origin of the observed effects, their relation to reading acquisition and the contribution of this new perspective in clinical practice. References 1. Habib, M. (Ed.). (1997). Dyslexie : le cerveau singulier. Marseille : SOLAL. 2. Barrouillet, P., Billard, C., De Agostini, M., Démonet, J.F., Fayol, M., Gombert, J.E, Habib, M., Le Normand, M.T, Ramus, F., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Valdois, S. (Eds.). (2007). Dyslexie, Dysorthographie, Dyscalculie : bilan des données scientifiques. Paris : Les éditions INSERM. 3. Grammaticos, E., & Klees, M. (2000). Dyslexie où est la différence ? Baisy-Thy (Belgique) : IPEJ asbl. 4. Bosse, M.L. (Ed.)(2004). Activités et adaptations pédagogiques pour la prévention et la prise en compte de la dyslexie à l’école. In S. Valdois, P. Colé & D. David (Eds), Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexies développementales : de la théorie à la pratique orthophonique et pédagogique (pp. 233-258). Marseille : SOLAL. 5. Quercia, P., Robichon, F., & da Silva, O. (2004). Dyslexie de développement et proprioception : approche clinique et thérapeutique. Beaune (France) : Association Graine de Lecteur. 6. Berthoz, A. (Ed.). (1997). Le sens du mouvement. Paris : Odile Jacob sciences. 7. Skoura, X., Vinter, A., & Papaxanthis, C. (2009). Mentally simulated motor actions in children. Developmental neuropsychology, 34(3), 356367. 8. Quercia, P. (Ed.)., Fourage R., Guillarme, L., Marino, A. (Ed.)., Quercia, M. & Saltarelli, S. (2008). Traitement Proprioceptif et Dyslexie. Beaune (France) : AF3dys. 9. Habib, M. (2002). Bases neurologiques des troubles spécifiques d’apprentissage. Réadaptation, 486, 16-28. 10. Jeannerod, M. (Ed.). (2009). Le cerveau volontaire. Paris : Odile Jacob sciences. 11. Hall, C.G., & Martin, K.A. (1997). Questionnaire d’imagerie du mouvement (version française de Laurent, J.) [Manuel & questionnaire]. Ontario : Université du Western. 12. Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science, 273(5281), 1564-1568.