Reading between the lines - Kenniscentrum Begrijpend Lezen
Transcription
Reading between the lines - Kenniscentrum Begrijpend Lezen
10() DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES J. c. & Goswami, U. (20()5). Reading and skilled reading across l<UII'."'''I'.L BIll/etill, 131, :\-29. A. M., & Stone, G. 0. Statistical of the bidi Behavior Researrh The DRC model of visual word aloud: An pxt(:'nsion to German. l~eading j:C"MMMM between the Lines ;11 C~(:«llifil'c Proccsscs ill Rcadill,i!, C-;olflprc/tcllsioll PJld \'an del] Brock, P,ll),lyiotcl Kcndcou. M~lry Jlllt' dlld White, Sarah (:arison Van den Broek, P. W., White, M. J., Kendeou, P., & Carlson, S. (2009). Reading between the lines: Developmental and individual differences in cognitive processes in reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, C. Schatschneider, & C. Phythian-Sence (Eds.), Beyond decoding: The behavioral and biological foundations of reading comprehension (pp. 107-123). New York: Guilford Press. is ,1n c'ssential of Ii difficultic's read in?; and under to them tc'nd to suffer from 111 tor Education 2002; Neltional is formalc'duc'tltion difficult for tht'se individuals, but the to rdll'ct, shelre ,mel n~,lS(ln effectively are hindered in of tl1l'ir live'S. Thus, it is csscntial that Wl' um:iersl,md lhl' processes that hId to successful reading com prehension and thl' ways in which these processes can fail. Such understanding can h,lVl' far-reaching implica tions for educational practice, specifically with respect to assessment, diagnosis, and intervention for both good and struggling readers. In this chapter, we discuss the cognitive processes underlying read and the development of these processes. The chap ter consists of three m<1jor sections. The first section provides a general overview of processes in reading comprehension drawing on 108 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES recent cognitive theories. The second section summarizes the develop_ ment of reading comprehension and difficulties experienced by strug readers in the context of findings from two studies. In the third and final section, we discuss theoretical and implica tions derived fwm the results of this research. COGNITIVE PROCESSES IN READING COMPREHENSION Extensive research h,1s l'xamined the cngni ti\'l~ processes of reading comprehension CernsbcKher, 1990; (;ral'sser, & Trabasso, Kintsch & van Diik. 1978; Trabasso & Vim den Broek, 1985; van Piast,l, & 2006; Whitehurst & Loni gall, 20(6). J\ common theme that has emerged is thilt to is l'ssl'lltial that a )"(';)(i('r is ,1bl(' to trans latl' written code intoll1(',mingflll ]'lIlguage ullits imel to cornbine these Ull i ts into a coherell t Ine'n ta I rt'PrL'S(,1l ta tion of the text. This tion call be ,lCcesscd by the rt',lder for d iffl'rent purposes after compldl'd: to recall inforll1,ltioll from till' tl'xt, to answer qUl'stions, to the knowkdgl' obtailll'd from the t('xl, to drmv p<ll'dllc'ls to other tL'xts, ,1nd so 011. Thus, it is tlw qu,llilv of the reddl'r's findl rt'pr('senta tion of a text th'lt ddermil1l'S thl' 1IItim'ltl' sllccess of his or lwr compre IWl1sioIl of th,l! tnt. SIOIl through ,1 tl'xl lions once read ,llld offlinc represcntiltions "rp caus'lily rl'Lltl'd: The proccsses that unfold during mOl11l'nt-by-monwnt H'iltiing comprchension provide the basis for the construclilln of the olflil1(' t('xl rl'Dresent,ltion in the re'ldcr's mind. If the online pnKl'SSt'S so docs the final tion. automatic, and relatively whereas others are slow, relatively effortfuL The mix of automatic and processes differs among individual readers and, indeed, for a particular reader acrOSS reading situations as a function of fatigue, text difficulty, or reading purpose). The process of interconnecting and integrating informdtion is con strained, on the one hand, by the limitations of human attentional capac ity and short-term memory and, on the other hand, by the stalldards of that a reader attempts to maintain in a particular reading situ ation (e.g., as a function of motivation or goal for reading, the nature of the reading task; see \'an den Broek, Risden, & vall de Veldc, 19R9). Readers cem use a wide range of standards to mainh1in coherence, but two of standards, callsal coherence and refl'renti,ll coherence, ,lr(' prevalent in virtually all reading situa tions dnd an' p<lrticuiarly important for the construction of coherent (e.g., Craessl'r & Cbrk,19R5; Kintsch,19RR; O'Brien & Myers,19R7; Tmbasso & Sperry, 19R5). Consider, for l'x<ll1lpll', the spn tence pa i r: John dropped the banana peel on the floor. Mary fell on her back. Most readers infer that till' infonnCltion in tlWS(' two sentences is calls often \sithout fl>'lli/ing that (1 causnl infe]"('ncl' h,lS been m,lde. "IlK' fact that John dropped till' ban,m<1 pl'l'1 on tht> floor is not sufficient to l'xpl<lin why Milry fell unless the readl'r inft'rs that b,ll1,llld pecls <lrc slippery, ,1nd vvlwn a person may f,ll1 (\,,111 den i3roek, Illl)()). Caus,l coherence, is ,1tt,lil1l'd v.. hen there is sufficient inform'ltion by tile text or in the n,l tll I"e of the b<lckground knowledge of tlll' rl'ildef to ('\·ents. add ition, rderential colll'rellce occurs when the re,lcil'r connects objects, charactt'rs, and other entities across sentences in tIll' tl'Xt. Con sider, for (,X,ll11 pic, the sentence p,lir: The COlllprehension Processes during Reading (Online) The online construction oi d cohercnt mental of the text involves a complex set of processes that involve connecting and inte the text iniormation that the wader currently is reading information that occurred earlier in the text as well as with infonna tion from background knowle-dge. Some of these proc('sses are quick, 09 Developrnel1t gave the waiter $10. He returned to her the change. this pi.1ir of sentences, most readers establish rderential coherence inferring that "he" in the second sentence rders to the "waiter" in the sentence and that "her" refl'rs to the "lady," again usually without eVen becoming aware of making such inferences. Interestinglv. readers tend to infer a causal rela tion, based on their Waiters (and restaurantst people paying waiters. and so on. 110 Thus, as the reader progresses through a text, he or she attempts to maintain coherence by gathering information from the currently read sentence while integrating this information with previously read text and background knowledge. The reader does this by attempting to identify causal, referential, and other relations. Any such attempt, needs to take place within rather severe limitations of a per son's working memory or attentionallimitations; a reader is unable to simply accumulate every piece of information provided or activated the text but instead is subject to a continuous w,1xing and waning of available information. A useful way to scape in which individual pieces of information in their levels of activation over time (for a review, see the model by van den l3roek, Risden, Fletcher, & Thurlow, 1996; see also Goldman & Varma, 1(95). Only pieces of information that are activated C,ln be connected by H1l' comprehl'nd('r. As a result, if to-be-connected pieces of inform,ltiOll Me r('ildilv ,w,lililblt' at the same time (e.g., if they are presl'nted close in tinll' or are in-ailablc close in the text), comprt'lwl1siol1 is an effortless, automatic pnlCl'ss. If to-be connected pieCl's of information are not nc',ldily avclibbie. the execution of strategic pron'ssl's is fl'quirl'd, resulting in !11(ll"l' dfortfuI reading or even failure to compreI-wnd altogeHwr. Thl'rl' ,1re consideri1ble individ ual and developn1L'ntal diffvrel1ces in the dfectiVl'IH'SS ,mel dficiency of thl' various pmCt'SSl'S th"t result in the Iandscapl' of activ,ltions (reflect ing diffl'rencl's in, e.g., background knowledge', working memory, expe rience, motivtltion, knowledge of 11 Reading Development DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES ~ b 0 ~ I ~\12 \€ o I (~) FIGURE 5.1. Clllsal network of MOO/IS For (1Ccurate comprehension to occur, it is neCl'ssary that one's Tl1(:'n tal reprt'Sent,ltioll reflects such ill1l'twork representation. This represen tation includes both qutlJltitati'lll' ,md qualitalive as~1l'cts of the represen tation. Tht'refofl" assessment of comprehension ne(:~ds to involv(:' both measurements of childn'n's memory for (1 text should not only provide ,1n index of how IIllltli they rCCi111 (the quantita tive ,lspect) but alsotu/1111 is rL'caill'd (the LJllc1litatiw aspect): Ideas that The Mental Representation of Texts (Offline) A 111emory represent,ltion of the h'xt <1rld rl'lev,mt background knowl emerges from the dyn,lmically fluctu,lting I,mdscape of dctivation, processes, and strategies. This representation is coherent to tlw extent that it forms a llei,POrk of relations (Craesser & Clark, 1985; O'Brien & Myers, 1987; Trabasso & v,m den Brot'k, 1985). An L'xampk of a causal network is presented in Figure 5.1. The numbered nodes in this network refer to unique idl'<1s from the beginning of a narrative story, listed in 5.2. The story is about Lenore, a young princess vvho becomes sick. In the network in Figure the arrows between the nodes repre sent the causal connections between the corresponding text units. For example, Lenore's illness (unit/node 3) causes the King to become con cerned about his daughter (unit/node 10), which, in turn, causes the King to ask if his daughter needs anything (unit/node 13). 1 2 3, 4 5, 6, T 8, 9, 10. 11, 12, 13, Once upon a time, there lived a Princess named Lenore who was ten years old. One day, Lenore became ill from eating too many chocolate-raspberry tarts, The royal physician was called to come to make Lenore feel better. He took her temperalure, felt her pulse, and made her stick out her tongue. Knowing that the King was concerned, the physician brought him in after he was finished with the examination. When the King saw his daughter lying there, he asked if there was anything she wanted. FIGURE 5.2. task. [rum Moon", written text used for narrative com 12 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES Read1l1g I )evelopment have many connections playa more central role in the causal network structure than do ideas with few connections. Indeed, skilled compre henders recall highly connected ideas more often than they recall ideas with few connections Graesser & Clark, 1985; Trabasso, Secco, & van den Broek, 1984; & Sperry, 1985; van den Broek & 1993). For example, for the story in Figures 5.1 and skilled readers are more likely to recall that Lenore became ill (unit/node 3, with six connections) than to recall that the princess was 10 years old node 2, with one connection). The qua of reca II is more indicative of good comprehension than the quantity, for adults as well as children (e.g., Casteel, 1993; Goldman & Varnhapt'n 1986; Trabasso et aI., 1984). DEVELOPMENT OF READING COMPREHENSION For skilled adult rl'aclers, the construction of a col)('r('I11, meaningful most of the tinw is r<ltlll'r errortil'ss for Storch & tiOllS of CI1IIL1ren's memorv tor nMratin's show th,1t chil d fl'l1, too, ,1 rl' s('nsi tive to the (\lllSal and rdcren ti,]1 structu re of till' events encoullter. Fven children as young as -t ycars tend to remember idl'as with many meaningful cOlllWCtiOI1S rnon.' often th'll1 ideas with few connections. Howl'ver, tic ,lge d iffprt'IKcs a Iso enwrge. For instance, older children are more sl'nsitivc to the c,ms,]I structure of a story than are younger children (,mel adults Me l'\l'n more sensitive). In addition, younger Child1'l'n tend to iorus on reCtllling causal relations thnt occur within a particular episode of ,] story, whereas older children arc better ahle to idt:'ntify CtHlsdl relations tl1,lt span across multiple sodes and events in a story. Identifying ca usa I reId tions ,Kross episodes of a story leads to a broader, more complex understanding of abstract components, such as a story's overall theme. dren also tend to emphaSize relations the concrete events in a storv. whereas older childr~n are able to notice rela tions involving 13 more abstract components, such as underlying intentions and l.wals of characters (van den Broek, 1997; Williams, 1 Early Contributions to the Development of Reading Comprehension The fact that even preschool children engage in extensive coherence building raises the following questions: (1) How do code-related and oral comprehension skills develop? (2) How do these two sets of skills combinp--or not combine-to support the development of reading comprehension skills? To add ress these questions, we tested two groups of preschool and kindergMten children (at the ages of 4 and 6 years, (1nd monitored them as they transitioned from prere(lders testing them again after 2 years (at the ages of 6 tinw, oral cornprehl'l1sion skills-the <lbility to extract Ilw,11ling about events ,1I1d f,]ets and identify sem,mtic relations between thost' ('vt'nts and facts and code-rel'lted litt'racy skills-till' to tr,1I1s],1te writtell svmhols into nw,1l1ingful words-were' assl'ssl'd. At t'<1C11 ,1gt', video ,md audio n,ur<ltivps were used to assc'ss oral comprehension skills, in addition to receptive vocaDul<1rY ,1S mea sured bv the l't',]bodv Picture Vocabui<1ry Test-Ill (Dunn & Dunn, For till' K-Yl'iu-old children, ,1 v.'ritten l1<1rrative W,lS included to ,lssess reading comprehension. All n,uf<ltivl'S Wl'rl' ,1gC appropriat(' on Fk'sch-Kincaid scorc's) ,md were ,1nalyzl'd in accordann' with the describc'd in till' previous section to ddl'rmine their causal structuH'S. Outconw variables for till' cOlln,rehcllsion measures were th" total ,1l11OLlnts of [('call of idl'<] units that w(,re een tra I to the G1US,1] network structure' (i.l'., with many cOnIlL'ctions), fl'presenting a combill,ltion of quantitatin' ,111<1 l]l.wliti1tivl' aspects of the child's rep resc'ntatioll of each narr,ltive. Code-related skills werl' examined m("lSlll"l'S of phonemic <lW<1reness ,me! lettt'r ,111J word idcntification (Storch & Whitehurst Oral comprehension and code-related skills emerged as unique and intcrreh1Lcd factors that contribute to overall reading comprehen sion. With regard to the development of these skills, oral comprehen sion skills at one age uniquely ural comprehension skills 2 years IcHer (i.e., from age 4 to age 6 and from age 6 to age 8). Similarly, a child's code-related skills at one age predicted that child's code-related skills 2 years later. These two unique sets of skills were also interre particularly dt the youngest age. For preschool children (age 4t r' 114 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES oral comprehension skills strongly predicted code-rdated skills. As chil dren developed, the predictive relation became weaker, suggesting that these two types of skills gradually separate as a child develops. With regard to later reading skills, oral comprehension and code related skills in early elementary school each independently predicted comprehension (i.e., at age 8). Thus, oral comprehension and code-reb ted skills jointly contribute to reading comprehension skills in a child's early elementary school years. In summary, well before children learn to read, oral comprehen_ sion and code-related skills arc present as separate interrelated sets of skills. Each of these sets of skills follows its own developmental tra jectory, with tht' skills at one age predicting the same skills 2 years later. When the child eventll<lllv reads texts for comprt'hension, in the early each of these two sets of skills independently contribute's to reading SUCCl'SS. tht' e'mnh,lsis in preschool education has been on ,lS a means to improve later reading comprl'hensiol1. OUf findings support that pl'actin', but they that oral compreht'l1sion, in addition to code-rl'lated litt>racy skills, also should be emphasized during ('<Hly instruction if lwnsion skills <lfe to dl'vclop by tIll' tillle i1 child is in school. This is particularly il1lport<lnt bvcausl' dftt'r these years, texts increasingly bt'conw thl' ml'ans for conveying content and, IwnCt', ,1 lack of skills th,lt Me prel'l'quisitc to comprt'iwnsion likely results in (l CilSCildl' of i)C,ldl'm ic sllch ilS infl'H'IKt' generation and comprehension monitoring, devel oped in IIw eMly grades beconw more and morl' important for aca demic SLiccess in tIll' hig/wr grades. Ypt as naliol1dl rt'jJOl'ts continue to brgt' numlwrs of students in 1,1ter ell'llwntary school and in school compwhend what they read in school or at skills h,w(' been mastered. Til readers in tlw latl'r grades, it is important to kinds oi difficultip<; thdt tl1l'Y ('xperience. In doing so, it is consider the processes fl'ad ing, beca use tha t is w hen the major ityof Later Contributions to the Development of Reading Comprehension We investigated how struggling and proficient readers differ as integrate and connect infonnation during reading by observing data Reading 115 from eye movements and think-aloud protocols. These methodologies were chosen because they provided both quantitative and qualitative aspects of online comprehension processes, parallel to our analysis of comprehension processes in the resulting representation (offline) from our study of young readers. average, and proficient readers from fourth-, seventh-, ninth-grade classes participated in this study. Levels of reading comprehension ability were assessed by means of the Maze Test of Cur riculum Based Measurement (CBM-Maze; Deno, 1985; Espin & Foegen, Students in each grade werl' divided into equal subgroups based on the distribution of Maze scores; those in the lower 20'X, of the distri bution were identified as struggling readers and those between 40 and 6D'},,, and over 8m;) of the distribution as average and proficient rcad erSt fespectivt'ly. The validity of the identified subgroups was tested comparing their scores on the standardized Cates-M,lcCinitie Read Comprehension Tl'st (Mi)CCinitie, MacCinitie, Maria, & Dreyer, 2000). Comprehension scores of the subgroups ilS assessed the Cates MacCinitie test diffHed significcllltly, and the diffl'rences were consis tent with the CBM-Maze test results. 1\11 students read a variety of (age-appropri<lte) texts in both eye ,. ,llld think-aloud tasks. The eye-tracking nwthodology reveals the number, duration, and type of eye movements that occur when reading a tl'xt, thus capturing quantitative and qualitative aspects of online tomprel1l'nsion proC'l'sses. Hence', it allows (lilt' to ddermim' how 111,lny fix,ltions and how much tinw a readt'r dt'votes to a partiClI lar ilrl'il in the tl'xt as well dS whether the rl'ader returned to that area of the text ,liter hi1\ing continued rt>ading looking back at prior text). The think-aloud methodology reveals the inft'rential and nOllinferential students as thev proceed through a text, thus capturing sinn prOCl'sses in a manner complementMY to that by eye of processes reve'lled during think-aloud procedures range from readers repeating text in their own words (i.e., paraphrasing) to Specific predictions about an event that will occur later in the text inference). Tlw eye-tracking results were remarkably consistent across the three grades. At each grade level, struggling readers proceeded the texts \vith the same number of fixations as average and proficient readers, but fixation times were longer for struggling readers than for more proficient peers. In addition, struggling, average, and pro ficient readers looked back to the previous text with equal ~ 116 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES but struggling readers were less systematic than more proficient read_ ers with to ,uhat information they reread. For example, strug_ gling readers returned to reread parts of the text as often as did the other readers, but instead of reading specific, informative segments, as readers did, the struggling readers reread entire sections of the text-and often uninformative sections at returning to their starting ._,--, The cognitive processes identified in the think-aloud Protocols revealed two distinct subgroups of struggling readers, each display_ a unique p<lttern of comprehension processes. Each subgroup pro duced less-than-optim,l! mental representations of the texts they as evidenced by poor memory for the texts. With regMci to the processes during reading, one subgroup of inclined than the other to engage in the enet's, inferences t h,l t d r,l\V on they did so as eVl'f, unlike the morl' readers frl'qllcnt!y produced the context of till' text. The second subgroup of stmggling n'clliers was morl' inclined to del1end on the text. Unlikl' the other groUD of strug in fOfm,) tion fL'w of strug activitil's nlostlv !o tlK' textual or repeilting the ll'xt and n'.ldefs did not exhibit cogni tivl' proCl'SSl~S th'lt were elramatic<llly diffel'l'l1t from ,1H'ragl' (lnd re,)dprs. In struggling H.-<)ders pnKl'l'cll'd through the tl'xl in simi lar fashion ,lS more in ferL'l1 tia I p ron'ssl'S t h,lt mation into <l cohel'l'nt ineffectual and time-cOnSlll1ling m,lllller in which struggling readers madl' their W<ly through till' text th'lt the more l1roficient readers Long, Oppy, & and profiCient readers differed in the strategies used dur with struggling readefs overemphasizing eith('r generat elaborative inferences or paraphr,)Sing the tl'Xt. Thus, struggling readers appear to comprisl' at least two distinct subgroups, with one subgroup going bel/om! the text to try and establish a coherent represen tation (but doing so ineffectively) and another staying Il'itliill the text and establish a coherent representation, thereby limiting their to recognize important relations. These distinct subgroups of 117 struggling readers can perhaps be described as explorers and plodders, respectively, in analogy to subgroups of dyslexic readers in the con text of word processing (Olson, Kliegl, LJavidson, & Foltz, 1985). Other subgroups may exist as well. Unfortunately, neither of these patterns of processes help struggling readers achieve success in their effort to comprehend what they are reading. DISCUSSION The described rPsp,lrch results contribute to Ollr ullderstcH1ding of the devdopml'nt of reading comprehension ,llld to eiforts to prevent, d nose, ,md femedv rl'cKling cOnl[)rl'ilensioll oroblems across the school years. Theoretical Implications <mel l]u<llily of cognitive proCl'SSl'S that occur dur till' propertil's of the tt'xtU<l! ,Kross different ,1gl' groups and dif our uncll'fstanding of reading comprehen sion. TIll' results 01 the iirst study n'portl'd here indicate th,)t, in till' prl'school \'(',HS, two distinct cluStlTS of skills rl'll'v,1llt to "lter re,lel ing written cOLil' (ktlL'r, vvmels) into concepts <mel 1l1l',1I1ingful lcmgwlge and those' imol\-ing the idl'lltific,)tion of llll'aningful felatiolls and the construction of COIll'rL'llCl' ,llllong thl' fdCtS, l'vents, and other COl1\'l'vl'd by the I,mgu()ge. These clusters show n'm,lrkable children lll<lturc ,md, illlport,mtly, m,lke strong, indepl'ndl'llt contribu tions to rL,,)ding colllprl'Ill'llsion by the time children begin to read. It th<l t both sets of "ki lis ,lre llecessa ry for rem'! illg com prl'hen sion. for many childn'll the necl'ss,)ry skills have Me re,leiy 10 read texts for many others struggle', even into higher elementary cllld The results of the second study indicate that ers tend to generate inaccurate and incomplete representations reading. When we considered their online processes, it appeared that the struggling readers did not differ from more proficient readers in the types of cognitive processes in which they engaged per se but that they differ with respect to the effectiveness and efficiency of those pro r 118 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES cesses. Moreover, the results suggest that struggling readers comprise different subgroups. In our study, two such subgroups were identified consistently for each of the three grades that were investigated and 9th). One subgroup of struggling readers drew extensively on background knowledge to infer connections between text units and between text units and their prior knowledge; unfortunately, the acti vated background knowledge frequently was irrelevant or incorrect in the context of the text. The second subgroup of struggling readers tended to limit their processing to the text itself, frequently rereading or otherwise reprocessing text units, without bringing in relevant back ground knowledge. I t is imoortant to nole tha t these subgroups were indistinguish to their performance once reading was For example, both subgroups differed from more proficient [(:'aders in terms of memory for the texts to the same degree. it is only \A,lhen online processes during reading Wt'n' c(lllsiderl'd thilt the subgroups emt'rged. In this study, two subgroups were idt'ntified. Future rt'st'dfch Illay iden additional subgroups or bl' ablp to make' filwr gr,lim'd distinctions within the subgroups th,lt we idl'tltifil'd. Practical hllpHcations th,lt t,vo distinct clusters of skills-associated with trans rl'spl'cti\'clv-come children's n',ld sion and, moreover, that thest' clusters of skills <llrcady ,Hl' the preschool years has imDliciltions for ,11 though tIlt' dret1 with the as phonemic awareness and word dl'codine:, is e:clll'raliv re'cognized, comprehen sion skills in instruction as well. These skills C,1n be introduced practiced in l1onrl'ading contexts sllch as telt'\'ision vit'wing or listen Viln den Rroek, I ,orch, & Thurlow, 1996) and should be aimed at children's <1bilitv to crc,lte coherence. This includes aware ness of coherence as well as specific strategies for constructing coher ence, Causal coherencp and referential coherence are good starting points because they tend to be amply present in the narrative materials to which preschool children typically are exposed and because these of coherence also are central to other types of information, includ the expository texts used in later school years. It is imnnrt:mt to TIll' find Reading Development 9 note mat, although these skills can be introduced as strategies, the goal should be that they eventually become internalized and automatized so that they can be applied without consuming excessive attentional resources (Laberge & Samuels, 1974). Second, early identification of potential problems in comprehen sion ability may open the door for interventions that ensure that chil dren hilve the comprehension skills necessary for reading comprehen sion once they enter elementary school. To identify potential problems and to assess the effectiveness of interventions, proper comprehension assessment tools need to be developed. This leads to a third implication of our results: The goal of compre hension assessment is to determine whether a reader is able to gener all' a coherent and accurate representation of the presented informa tion. Such assessment should include both the qll<lIltity and quality of a student's representation, focusing on the constructed (causal) network (KendeOLl, Vim den Broek, White, & Lynch, 2007; van den Brm'k et aI., Most tmdition,ll assessnwnts me limited to mCilsuring quantity (e.g., the amount of information a child recalls). At least as imporhmt is the quality of the representation. ln the current studies, we present one eXil111pk' of assessment of quality: g<1uging the extent to which a child's reGll1 is sensitive to the centr<llity of information the l'xtent to which he or she rt'members highly connected information). Other wilys could t'<lsily be developed: for example, asking coherence build ,lin1l'd at different levels of inference making and using techniques to scale the questions-and the skills gtlllge'-lln qU<llittltivcly different dimensions. TIlt' results rq';Mding struggling readers highlight a fourth implica tion for educational practice: Dit1gnosis of reading problems and inter Vl'ntion in struggling readers bent'fit from considemtion of the actu,11 procl'sst's in 1I,;hicb tlwse readers engage as they proceed through a text. Struggling readers may appear similar in terms of the product of their diffl'r when their cognitive processes are consid ered. Moreover, it is the online processes that provide the foundation for successful-and failed-coherence' building, so interventions and use ful di<lgnosis are mosllikely to be effecIi Vt' when applied to the reader's activities during reading and when they are based on an understandof which processes are being executed properly and which are not Indeed, many strategy interventions in the literature address potential causes of reading difficulties by methods that implicitly are assumed to improve the effectiveness of processing, although such processing is not dirpctlv assessed (e.g., Dole, Duffy, Roehler, & Pearson, 1991; Fuchs, ,. 121 Development 20 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES ;'''/-'.ir(1 Fuchs, Mathes, & Simmons, 1997; PaHncsar & !3rown, 1984; Pressley, that explicit 2000; Yuill & Oakhill, 1988). Our results on processing, both in attempts to understand the origin of (reading) comprehension problems and the development of interventions, is essentiaL This, in turn, has a further implication for assessment, besides the quantity-quality distinction, namely that assessment tools should include process measures as well. In conclusion, successful reading comprehension depends on development of different skills, ranging from skills particular to trans lating the textual code into meaningful concepts to skills particular to combining units 01" information into a coherent whole. All of these skills begin their developmentill trajectory well before the school years. Indeed, the processes imd skills in preschool children described here themselves likely have precursors, at evpn earlier ;lgl'S van den 1997). Although in education comprehension it is as much ,) pme!':;:;. Educa in diagnOSis and intervention tiona I process('s th,)t h',ld re,lders to by explicitly Sllccess or failure. edilion Espin, C. A., & A. (1996). Validity of general outcome measures for .. ,',_._ secondary students' performance on content-area tasks. Excep 62, 497~514. D., Fuchs, L. S., i\1athes, P. C .. & Simmons, D. C. Making classrooms more r<>c<'nn~i ,')4, 174-206. Peer-assisted to diversity. Amcr- Hills dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Goldm,m, S. R., & V<1rma, S. (1995). CAPping the modl,1 of discourse comprehension. In C. A. VVeaver, S. Mannes, & C. R. Fletcher (I'-tis.), Disc,lIlrs£' COlllprcl/l'lI~ioll: £~sl1ys ill 11011(11' of Wolter Kintscll (pp. M,lhwah, NJ: Erlb,)UI11. GoldmJIl, S. R., & Varnilagcll, C. K. (19f\b). Ml·mory [pr embedded and seq lIen storv structures. of i....l clIlOfll ,"',! 1111/>.'11(/</(',25, 4ll141f\. 1'. II., & Tuntl1er, W. E. (1 91l6). I Graessl'r, A. c., & C\,lrk, rdllmlioJl, 7,6-1 D. L. r Slrticlul'CS J ......... A.. v,.c· "I' jllln/il'it kllOiul- Norwood. NI: i\bll'x. Graessl'r, A. C, Sin~l'r, M., & Irdbasso, T. (19<)4). C(lilstruclin~ infercllces dur ing l1aIT,ltiV(' COfllprl'lwllsion. /<'l'l'icw./O{, :'>71-:;9::;. Kendl'oll, 1'., \,,1/1 ciVil BroL'K, P.. WhitL', M ..L & l.ynch, ]. (2ll07). Comprehen sion ill preschool and ('<HI)' l'il'nll'lltMy children: Skill dl'VL'\opml'llt and stl'<ltl'g\ intl'l'\l'lltiolls. III D. S. McN<)mar,l '//I('oril':;, illtcn'l'IIliolls, olld ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This n'sl'<lrch w,l.~ supportL·d J)l.u:tn, L. M., & Dunn, L. rv1. (1997). Peabody Picture vocabulary <PPVT-W). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services. gr'lIlh from Ihl' Cenll'r for ,lnd tilt' DlT,)[lllwnt of EduC,ltioll,lll's\'cilol(lgv (both from the Cl'nrn for Ihe IrllprO\·,'nwnt of E.lr!l· Rl\ld /\chil'\'t'fl1l'!lI, ']!lel a Rl'"Liing SCdll'-up grdllt (CID/\ 1l.f.3(),)) from till' U.s. Institute nf EducMioll Sci !',lUi \'(111 dl'n Brock rl\'('in'(\ supporl l\l'sl',)rci1, Unil'L'r;.;itv of Mll1l1L'sot,1. l'<llhlViot,1 Kl'ndl'ou received through" McCi11 Univl'rsit\ Rl'S(',Hciwr lund. A CO/lstruc Erlhalltn. discourse Kintsch. W. (1 t)HK). Tlw lISl' pf 1{1'1,i('1l', 95, \6:1- H12. model. )971'1). Tow,)fd ,) model of text Kintsch, \N., & \',)11 \)ijk, T. A. I~('l'i{,ii), Wi, :'>63-3'1'L inf(lrrnation (1'174). TOWdrd ,1 theory of /1,2Y3323. d i ffNl' nccs i 11 I ht' ti nil' l.cllrIl- TIll' REFERENCES C<lin, K., Oakhill, L & I'. E. (200.f). Childn'J1's C oncurrl'nt pred iclioll componcnt skills.lolll"llili of FdllCiltimlil! Clsted, M. A. (1993). Effects of inference ,jren's inferential \'l'rb,l! tlbilily, and on chil- . Deno, S. l.. (191'15). CurriClllum-based l11l'dSlIrellwn t: The C/lildrl'll, 52, 219-2::12. C. C., Roehler, L. R., & Pearson, P. D. (]991). Mm'ing from the old to the new: Research on readinl! cl)mprehension instruction. Ikuic1i' of EduCIlliolloj /(.,·s(,I1I'(II, 61, 2,\9-26-+. compre Tlworic5, lwnsion. illfl,},l'elltitlll~. 47-71). New York: Erlbaum. IIlId !"'"" 22 DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACHES Reading Oakhill, j., Cain, K, & Bryant, P E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Lal1f;uagc and Cogni tive Processes, 18,443--468. E. J., & Myers, J. L (1987). The role of caus,11 connections in the of text. Memory t"'.r Cogniliol1, 15,419--427. Olson, R K, Kliegl, R, Davidson, B. L & Foltz, G. (19Wi). Individual and deveI nnm,'nbl differences in disability. In G. E. MacKinnon & 1. G. Rmdil1S rc~earclt: Adl'(lllc('s ill IlIcory and pmclice (Vol. 4, pp, 1-(4). New York: Academic Press. Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L (19H4). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension: activities. Ill1d In~ll'IIctioll, 1,117-175. narrative comprehension in young 38,36-76. str,ltegies instruction. In J. Osborn & F. all: IS';lle,.; ill icadlillg tll/d (pp. 1 New York: Cuilford Press. M. (2000). Whilt should comprehension instruction bl' of! In M. I.. Kamil, 1'. B. MOSl'nthal, 1'. D. l'ear'iOll, & R. B,lrr n',warcJl (Vol. 3, pp. 545-586). 1'v1.1hw<1h, NJ: Frlbaum. C. J. (2002). Ordll,lIlgu,lge ,111<1 code-rel,ltl'd precur sors to rmding: Rvidt'no' from d longitutiin,11 structllr,ll I11ndel. Deuclop !J:';l/clwlogy, 38, Sn4-947. Trab,lsso, T., Secco, T., & V<lll den nroek, 1'. (1l/84). CaLls<11 cohesion and story coherl'TKl'. In II. M,1l1dL N. L Stein, & T. Tr<1b,l;iso (Etls.), Lel/millx lind (Pt" 8,1-111). M,lll\v,lh, NJ: [rlb,lL1f11. L L. (1985). Cw,;al rel,1tclil1L'ss ,md import-mel' of sh)ry ],J., 595-611. Tr,lbnsso, T, & V,1tl dl'll Bfllek, I'. (1985). elL1S,ll and tlw I'l'presentation of l1arr,lti\'l'l'Vl'nts. 2-1,612-(,30. Vim dl'n !3n)l'k, I'. (1l)l)O). infl'rl'ncl' n1,1kcr: 1l)\V,lrds a process model of i nfC'I'l'lll'l' In ()_ 1\. n,llota, C. B. Flores d' Arcais, & K. 423 445). Hillsd'llc, NJ: Erlb,lUll1. van den Bnwk, I'. (1994) ,llld nW!110I'V of n,lfr,1ti Vl' texts. In M. A. Cl'rnsbilchcr (Ed.), Hlllldh,Jok 539-588). Lnndon: AC,1dl'lllic ('ress. v(ln den Bnwk, I'. Disc(lVl'ring the Cl'nwnt of the lInhersp: The develop nwnt of ('vent from childhood to ,ldulthood. In I' \'<1n den Brock, P SpillL' in van den aL van den Broek, P, & of causal 23 recogni relations in memory for narrative texts: Evidence from tion. Discollrsl' Processes, 16,75-98. van den Broek, E, Lorch, E. P, & Thurlow, K (1996). Children's and adults' memory for television stories: The role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchicallevcL Child Developlllcl1t, 67, 301 ()-3028. vanden Brock, P, Risden, K., Fletcher, CR., & Thurlow, R (1996). A "landscape" vicw of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In B. K. Britton & A. C Graesser Mode!:; lexl (pp. 165--187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. van den Broek, 1'., Risden, K., & HlIsebye-Hartmann, E. The role of readers' standards for Cl)herence in the generation of inferences during R. F. Lorch & E. J. O'Brien SOl/rees of CO!ICrCI1CC ill rL'm/iIlS (pp. 353-373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlballl1l. van de Ve1de, R. G. (1989). Man, verbal text, inferencing, and coherencc_ In W. Hevdrich, F Nl'ub<1ul'r, J. S. ('etofi, & E. Sozer (Eds.), COllllcxill/lIl1Ll cO/ICn'l;o': /\l/tlll/sis of/c.-a alld di;;COllr~l' (pp. 174-217). New York: Walter ell' Wagncr, R. K., Piast,l, S. B., & J. K. Tr,LXler & M. A. Gerllsbill'her pp.llll 11·12). New Yllrk: Flscvil'L Whitehurst, C. L & Llll1ig,1I1, C. l (1998). Child i111d en1l'rgl'n t Child Dn'clol'lIIt'1/1, 69, 841\-872. of studpilts with ilnd without dis,lbiliti<'s: Id('ntifi(".ltillll of Ildrrative themes ,1l1d tic text rl'pre sell t,l tions. /011 /'/I1l1 [)f SIJ('citll Edl/[IIlio/l, 25, 135- 154. Effl'cts of infL'rl'ncl' aW<1feness on poor 2,,1345. comprelll'ns!(>n. ZW,l,1Il, K A., & 1{,lpP, D. N. Discourse comprehension. In M. J, 'lhLX In & M. A. Ccrnsb,lclwr lalldbook 0( (2nd ed., pp_ New York: EIsC'\'il'r.