- QMU eTheses Repository
Transcription
- QMU eTheses Repository
A STUDY OF A HOME EXERCISE PROGRAMME FOR COMMUNITY DWELLING PEOPLE WITH LATE-STAGE STROKE GILLIAN BAER A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy QUEEN MARGARET UNIVERSITY 2011 i ABSTRACT BACKGROUND Many people living with chronic stroke are not involved in any form of ongoing rehabilitation, despite having ongoing impairments and limitations in activity and participation. The approach to structuring practice of functional tasks, as part of ongoing rehabilitation, can incorporate diverse techniques. Current texts advocate that physiotherapists construct stroke rehabilitation programmes that incorporate Motor Learning principles, however the evidence to support this is limited. No evidence related to stroke exists as to whether functional tasks should be practised in their entirety (whole practice) or in component parts (part practice). The primary aim of the work reported in this thesis was to investigate the effects of a home exercise programme based on Motor Learning principles of part practice (PP) or whole practice (WP) of selected functional tasks for people at least six months after a stroke. METHODOLOGY A single blind, randomised controlled trial was undertaken, with participants allocated to either a part practice experimental group (PP), a whole practice experimental group (WP) or a control (Con) group. Both experimental groups followed a four week exercise intervention programme of functional tasks based on PP or WP. Outcome measures were undertaken at baseline, at the end of a four week intervention (wk 4), at short-term follow-up (wk 4.5) and at long-term follow-up (wk 16). Outcome measures utilised were the Barthel Index (BI), Motor Assessment Scale (MAS), Timed Up and Go over 2 metres (TUG2m), Step Test, Frenchay Arm Test (FAT), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), Frenchay Activities Index (FAI)and the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS). Differences between the groups at each measurement point were examined using a Kruskal Wallis test. Differences within each group over time were analysed using a Friedman’s Anova, followed up by a Wilcoxon’s Signed Ranks test using a Bonferroni correction where a significant difference was found. RESULTS Sixty four people with late-stage stroke were recruited and provided informed consent. Data were available for analysis for 60 participants (median time since stroke 21 months). No statistically significant differences were found between the three groups at any point for any of the dependent outcome variables. A number of statistically significant within group changes were found in all groups. Most statistically significant changes were demonstrated by PP including on the BI from baseline to wk 4.5; on the MAS from baseline to weeks 4, 4.5 and 16; on the Step Test from baseline to weeks 4, 4.5 and 16 and on the FAT from baseline to week 4. On more global measures the PP group reported statistically significant improvements on the SIS in the domains of strength, mood and mobility from baseline to wk 4; and in the SIS participation domain from baseline to wk 16; as well as a statistically significant within group improvements on FAI from baseline to wk 4. CONCLUSIONS People with late-stage stroke demonstrated capacity for improvements in a number of measures of impairment, activity, participation and mood. The PP group demonstrated improvements, over time, in more of the outcome measures relating to physical ability than either WP or Con groups. Implications for clinical practice and further research are discussed. i ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS It has been a long journey and there are many people to thank for their support along the way. I would like to thank Prof Brian Durward for his support, advice and wisdom throughout the study. His knowledge and willingness to debate were much appreciated. I am also extremely grateful to Prof David Weller for his enthusiasm and his help and generosity in sharing his considerable expertise relating to community based research. My heartfelt thanks also to Prof Marie Donaghy, who stepped in as Director of Studies towards the final stages of this process. Her enthusiasm, support and feedback were highly valued. For statistical advice, my thanks go to Dr Rob Elton from Edinburgh University for his interest in the project and extremely helpful and thought provoking planning meetings. I am also highly appreciative of the advice and assistance provided by Dr Robert Rush from Queen Margaret University. I am indebted to the Chief Scientist Office of the Scottish Executive without whose financial support by means of a Primary Care Research Fund grant, this project would not have been viable. During the planning phase of the project, meetings with many members of the Lothian Primary Care Research Network provided thought-provoking challenges. I am particularly grateful to Dr Brian McKinstry and Dr Lucy McLoughan for their interest and support. My thanks are extended to all the GP practice staff who supported the study and gave of their valuable time. I am also grateful to all the physiotherapists and rehabilitation staff who supported this study and referred potential participants. Particular mention must go to Jane Shiels for her help in arranging access to patients and space in an extremely busy gym to enable the pilot phases of the work. i My thanks go to Richard Wilson for his considerable technical know-how, his assistance with providing some of the equipment for the practice regime and his ability to ensure there was always at least one working activPAL. I am grateful to Sasha Baggaley and Kirsteen Roscoe for their support and organisation when working on the study. Without them and their enthusiasm, recruitment would have been much harder. To all staff in the Department of Physiotherapy, I would like to record my thanks for their unfailing support and good humour. I am extremely grateful to all the people with stroke and their carers who participated in this study, for their interest, their feedback and all the cups of coffee. Lastly for love and support, above and beyond, I must record my deep appreciation to: mum - for always being there dad - for encouraging, amongst other things, a sceptical attitude (I think you would have enjoyed debating the merits of this work) the girls - for your support, encouragement, humour and even childcare to enable me to keep going Finally, a massive thank you to the two most important people - Brendan and Joe you’ve got your mum back. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS 1 INTRODUCTION 1 1.1 Scene setting ………………………………………………………… 1 1.2 Guidelines and Policies for Ongoing Stroke Rehabilitation in the Community …………………………………………………………… 2 1.3 Stroke Services in the Community………………………………….. 3 1.4 The impetus for this study…………………………………………… 4 1.5 Structure of the thesis ………………………………………………. 5 2 STROKE 7 2.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………… 7 2.2 Definitions……………………………………………………………… 7 2.3 Aetiology of Stroke …………………………………………………… 8 2.3.1 Biophysical mechanisms of stroke …………………………………. 10 Epidemiology of Stroke ……………………………………………… 12 2.4 2.4.1 Incidence ………………………………………………………… 13 2.4.2 Prevalence ……………………………………………………... 16 2.5 Stroke Recovery …………………………………………………….. 17 2.5.1 Neurobiological Evidence of Stroke Recovery ………………… 18 2.5.2 Prognostic Indicators of Recovery …………………………… 22 2.6 Functional Recovery Profiles ……………………………………….. 24 2.7 Summary …………………………………………………………….. 26 3 STROKE REHABILITATION 28 3.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………….. 28 3.2 Physiotherapy Intervention in Acute Stroke Management …………. 30 3.3 Acute and Sub-Acute rehabilitation …………………………………… 31 3.4 Evidence to Support Physiotherapy in Late-Stage Stroke Rehabilitation ………………………………………………………….. 38 3.4.1 Studies of Mobility in Late-stage stroke …………………………… 39 3.4.2 Studies of Strengthening in Chronic Stroke ……………………… 44 3.4.3 Studies of mixed strengthening, conditioning and mobility in late-stage stroke ……………………………………………….. 47 iii 3.4.4 Studies of upper limb rehabilitation in late-stage stroke …….. 56 3.5.5 Summary points from studies of later stage stroke rehabilitation 59 3.5 Services available to people with stroke following cessation of formal rehabilitation ………………………………………………….. 3.6 Summary …………………………………………………………….. 69 70 4 MOTOR LEARNING THEORY AND REHABILITATION 71 4.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………….. 71 4.2 Types of Motor Skill to be relearnt …………………………………… 73 4.2.1 Open and Closed Loop Motor Skills ………………………………. 74 4.2.2 Discrete, Serial and Continuous Skills …………………………… 74 4.2.3 Gentile’s Two-Dimensional Taxonomy of Tasks ……………….. 75 Theories of Motor Learning ………………………………………….. 78 4.3.1 Temporal Stages of Motor Learning ……………………………. 78 4.3.2 Structural Theories of Motor Learning …………………………….. 81 4.3 How to Structure Practice …………………………………………… 86 4.4.1 Massed or Distributed Practice …………………………………. 87 4.4.2 Blocked or Random Practice ……………………………………. 90 4.4.3 Variable or Constant Practice …………………………………… . 92 4.4.4 Whole or Part Practice ……………………………………………. 94 4.4.5 Attentional Focus during practice ………………………………… 96 4.4 Feedback ……………………………………………………………… 97 4.5.1 Intrinsic Feedback ………………………………………………… 98 4.5.2 Extrinsic Feedback ………………………………………………….. 98 Neurophysiological Evidence relating to Motor Learning …………… 104 4.5 4.6 4.6.1 The Cerebellum and Motor Learning …………………………….. 105 4.6.2 Structural changes associated with Motor Learning ……………. 109 4.7 Summary ……………………………………………………………… 110 5 RATIONALE FOR STUDY 112 5.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………….. 112 5.2 Research Aims ………………………………………………………… 114 5.2.1 Hypotheses …………………………………………………………….. 115 iv 6 117 6.1 DEVELOPMENT OF THE METHODOLOGY FOR A RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL OF PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE Introduction ……………………………………………………………. 6.2 Development of the Exercise Intervention …………………………. 117 6.2.1 6.2.2 Sample and Recruitment Procedures for the development of the 119 Exercise Intervention …………………………………………………. Selecting and Refining the exercises …………………………………. 119 6.2.3 Pilot Exercise Procedure – in Hospital ………………………………. 120 6.2.4 Analysis of pilot exercises in hospital and impact on final pilot protocol …………………………………………………………………. 122 6.3 117 Determining Screening Tests and Outcome Measures to be used in the final protocol ……………………………………………………….. 124 6.3.1 The Mini Mental State Examination ……………………………… 124 6.3.2 The Functional Reach Test ……………………………………….. 126 6.3.3 Mixed Measures of Impairment and Activity Limitation …………. 128 6.3.3.1 The Rivermead Motor Assessment ……………………………… 128 6.3.3.2 The Motor Assessment Scale …………………………………… 129 6.3.3.3 Piloting the Rivermead Motor Assessment and the Motor Assessment Scale ………………………………………………… 132 6.3.3.4 The Frenchay Arm Test …………………………………………… 132 6.3.4 Global Measures of Activity Limitation ……………………………. 133 6.3.4.1 The Barthel Index ……………………………………………………. 133 6.3.4.2 Piloting the Barthel Index…………………………………………… 135 6.3.4.3 The Frenchay Activites Index ……………………………………… 136 6.3.5 Measuring Aspects of Mobility and Balance ……………………… 138 6.3.5.1 The Timed Up and Go ………………………………………………. 138 6.3.5.2 Familiarisation with timing the Timed Up and Go 139 6.3.5.3 The Step Test ……………………………………………………… 139 Measuring Aspects of Mood 141 The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale …………………… 141 Measuring Quality of Life …………………………………………. 142 6.3.7.1 The Stroke Impact Scale …………………………………………… 143 6.3.7.2 The Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale ………………………. 144 6.3.6 6.3.7 v 6.3.7.3 Comparisions between the Stroke Impact Scale and the Stroke 145 Specific Quality of Life Scale ……………………………………… 6.3.7.4 6.3.8 Piloting the Stroke Impact Scale and the Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale ……………………………………………………….. 146 The final Selection of Outcome Measures ………………………. 147 6.4 Pilot of Exercise Intervention and Outcome Measures in the Community ………………………………………………………………. 148 6.4.1 Methodology of Community pilot of exercise intervention …….. 149 6.4.2 Results of community pilot exercise intervention ………………. 151 6.4.2.1 Number of repetitions for each exercise ………………………… 152 6.4.2.2 Baseline to end of intervention outcome measure data ……….. 155 6.4.3 Summary of key findings from community pilot ………………….. 160 Testing the ActivPAL …………………………………………………… 163 6.5 6.5.1 Methodology for establishing the agreement of activPAL and video data at different walking speeds ……………………………. 166 6.5.2 Results for agreement between activPAL and video data at different walking speeds ………………………………………….. 6.6 6.6.1 167 Establishing Concurrent Validity of a modified Timed Up and Go test over two metres …………………………………………………… 169 Methodology of a pilot study investigating concurrent validity of a modified Timed Up and Go test over two metres ……………. 170 6.6.2 Results of the pilot of the Timed Up and Go ……………………. 171 6.6.3 Summary of findings from Timed Up and Go pilot ……………… 176 6.7 Summary ………………………………………………………………. 177 7.0 METHODOLOGY 179 7.1 Introduction ……………………………………………………………… 179 7.2 Trial Design Overview …………………………………………………. 179 7.3 Subject Populations ……………………………………………………. 181 7.3.1 Inclusion Criteria …………………………………………………… 182 7.3.2. Exclusion criteria …………………………………………………… 182 7.4 Recruitment …………………………………………………………… 182 7.4.1 Initial recruitment strategy ………………………………………… 184 7.4.2 Recruitment Strategy v2 …………………………………………… 188 7.4.3 Recruitment strategy v3 ………………………………………….. 189 vi 7.4.4 Recruitment strategy v4 …………………………………………. 190 7.4.5 Consent …………………………………………………………… 191 7.5 Outcome Measure procedures ……………………………………… 191 7.6 Randomisation ……………………………………………………….. 195 7.7 Procedures ……………………………………………………………. 195 7.7.1 Baseline Outcome Measures visits ……………………………….. 196 7.7.2. Intervention Visit 1 ……………………………………………….. 197 7.7.3 Intervention Visit 2 …………………………………………………. 198 7.7.4 Intervention Visits 3 and 4 ………………………………………… 199 7.8 Data Analysis ………………………………………………………….. 199 8.0 RESULTS 202 8.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………….. 202 8.2. Subject characteristics ………………………………………………… 202 8.3 Drop Outs and Missing Data …………………………………………. 204 8.3.1. Drop Outs ………………………………………………………….. 205 8.3.2 Missing Data ……………………………………………………….. 206 Global Measures of Impairment, Activity and Participation ……….. 207 8.4 8.4.1 The Barthel Index (BI) – descriptive data …………………….... 207 8.4.2 Frenchay Activity Index (FAI) ………………………………….... 211 8.4.3 Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) …………………………………… 215 Measures of Mobility …………………………………………………… 219 8.5.1 Timed Up and GO over 2 metres (TUG2m) ……………………… 219 8.5.2 The Step Test …………………………………………………….. 225 8.5 8.6. 8.6.1 8.7 8.7.1 8.8 Measures of arm and hand function …………………………………. 230 The Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) …………………………………… 230 Measures of Mood …………………………………………………….. 234 The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) …………… Measure of Health Status …………………………………………….. 234 238 8.8.1 The Stroke Impact Scale descriptive data ……………………….. 238 8.8.2 The Stroke Impact Scale – Strength (SIS-str) ……………………. 239 8.8.3 The Stroke Impact Scale – Memory (SIS-mem) ………………… 241 8.8.4 The Stroke Impact Scale – Mood (SIS-mood) ………………….. 243 8.8.5 The Stroke Impact Scale – Communication (SIS-comm) ……… 246 vii 8.8.6 The Stroke Impact Scale – Activities of Daily Living (SIS-ADL) ... 248 8.8.7 The Stroke Impact Scale – Mobility (SIS-mob) …………………. 250 8.8.8 The Stroke Impact Scale – Hand (SIS-hnd) ……………………. 252 8.8.9 The Stroke Impact Scale – Participation (SIS-partic) ……………. 254 8.8.10 The Stroke Impact Scale – Recovery visual analogue scale (SIS-VAS) …………………………………………………………… 257 8.9 Exercise Repetitions ………………………………………………….. 260 8.10 Monitoring Activity ……………………………………………………….. 262 8.11 Summary of Findings …………………………………………………… 265 9.0 DISCUSSION 269 9.1 Introduction …………………………………………………………….. 269 9.2 Sample Characteristics ……………………………………………….. 271 9.3 Discussion of Global Measures of Impairment, Activity, Participation 273 9.3.1 Barthel Index …………………………………………………………. 273 9.3.2 The Motor Assessment Scale ……………………………………… 276 9.3.3 The Frenchay Activity Index ………………………………………. 279 Discussion of Mobility Outcomes …………………………………….. 280 9.4.1 The Timed Up and Go over 2 metres …………………………… 280 9.4.2 Gait Speed …………………………………………………………. 281 9.4.3 Stepping Up ………………………………………………………… 283 9.4.4 Rising to Stand …………………………………………………….. 284 Measures of Arm Function ……………………………………………. 285 The Frenchay Arm Test …………………………………………… 285 Discussion of Measurement of Mood ………………………………… 286 The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale …………………….. 286 Discussion of Health Status …………………………………………… 288 The Stroke Impact Scale Domains ……………………………….. 288 9.4 9.5 9.5.1 9.6 9.6.1 9.7 9.7.1 9.8 The Practice Regime ………………………………………………….. 291 9.9 Sample Measurement of Activity ……………………………………… 294 9.10 Possible explanations for changes in outcomes …………………… 296 9.11 Limitations and Sources of Error …………………………………….. 298 9.11.1 Study Design……………………………………………………….. 299 9.11.2 Recruitment Strategy ……………………………………………… 301 viii 9.11.3 The Sample ………………………………………………………… 302 9.11.4 The Intervention …………………………………………………… 303 9.11.5 Outcome Measurement …………………………………………… 306 9.12 Clinical Implications …………………………………………………… 309 9.13 Future Research ……………………………………………………… 311 9.14 Conclusions …………………………………………………………… 316 REFERENCES ……………………………………………………….. 320 APPENDICES 355 I Pilot information sheet and Pilot Consent form II Pilot exercise sheets …………………………… III Prompts for Barthel Index …………………….. IV Raw data for TUG pilot ……………………….. V Data for pilot Outcome measures: ……………. TUG Motor Assessment Scale The Step Test Frenchay Arm Test Barthel Index VI ActivPAL guide and example data ……………. VII Pilot exercise repetitions ……………………….. VIII Final RCT Information sheet and …………….. Final RCT consent form IX Randomisation list ………………………………. ix X DATA Xa Barthel Index Xb Frenchay Activities Index Xc Motor Assessment Scale Xd Timed Up and Go Xe Step Test Xf Frenchay Arm Test Xg Hospital Anxiety and Depression Xh Stroke Impact Scale Xi Exercise Repetitions Xj ActivPAL data XI x LIST OF FIGURES Figure 2.1 Age specific stroke incidence rates per 100,000 population from three centres in Europe …………………………………… 14 Figure 2.2 Stroke Incidence in Scotland 2000-2009………………………. 16 Figure 3.1 Notional Stroke Rehabilitation matrix ………………………… 29 Figure 4.1 General structural organisation of cerebellum ………………. 106 Figure 4.2 Synaptic organisation of cerebellar circuitry showing excitatory and inhibitory circuits ……………………………. Figure 6.1 Flow diagram of the stages of development ………………. Figure 6.2 Line graph of weekly total Rise to stand repetitions by pilot participants ……………………………………………………… Figure 6.3 Figure 6.4 Figure 6.5 Line graph of weekly total Stepping Up with the unaffected leg repetitions by pilot participants …………………………… Line graph of weekly total of Cuppa time exercise repetitions by pilot participants …………………………………………….. Line graph of weekly total of TipTap exercise repetitions by pilot participants …………………………………………….. 107 118 152 153 154 155 Figure 6.6 Pilot Motor Assessment Scale pre- post intervention scores 156 Figure 6.7 Pilot Timed Up and Go pre- post intervention scores 157 Figure 6.8 Pilot Rise to stand time pre- post intervention scores 158 Figure 6.9 Pilot Step Test data - stepping up with the unaffected leg prepost intervention scores ………………………………………… 159 Figure 6.10 Pilot Frenchay Arm Test data pre- post intervention scores 159 Figure 6.11 Placement of ActivPAL ……………………………………….. 164 Figure 6.12 Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts xi recorded by activPAL and video at 2.5km/h ……………… Figure 6.13 Figure 6.14 Figure 6.15 Figure 6.16 Figure 6.17 Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts recorded by activPAL and video at 5km/h ……………… Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts recorded by activPAL and video at 7.5km/h ……………… Bland and Altman plot of gait velocity for Timed Up and Go performed over 2metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG 3m)… Bland and Altman plot of turn times for Timed Up and Go performed over 2metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG 3m)… 168 168 169 173 174 Bland and Altman plot of the number of steps taken during turning for Timed Up and Go performed over 2metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG 3m)……………………………. 175 Figure 7.1: Map of North East Edinburgh LHCC ……………………….. 185 Figure 7.2 Flow Diagram of Trial Design ………………………………. 194 Figure 8.1 Consort Diagram …………………………………………….. 195 Figure 8.2 Box plot of scores on Barthel Index ………………………. 209 Figure 8.3 Box plot of Frenchay Activity Index scores ………………. 213 Figure 8.4 Box plot of Motor Assessment Scale total scores ……… 216 Figure 8.5a Box plot of Timed Up and Go 2m total time ………………. 221 Figure 8.5b Box plot of Timed Up and Go 2m gait speed ………………. 222 Figure 8.5c Box plot of Rise to Stand time during TUG2m ………………. 222 Figure 8.6a Box plot of Step Test - number of steps onto block with unaffected leg ……………………………….………………. Figure 8.6b Box plot of Step Test - number of steps onto block with affected leg ……………………………….………………. xii 227 228 Figure 8.7 Box plot of Frenchay Arm Test scores ………………………. Figure 8.8a Box plot of Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale - Anxiety subscale ………………………………………. ………………. Figure 8.8b Box plot of Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Depression subscale …………………………… ………………. 232 235 236 Figure 8.9 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - strength domain ………….. 240 Figure 8.10 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - memory domain …………. 242 Figure 8.11 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - mood domain ………….. 244 Figure 8.12 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - communication domain 247 Figure 8.13 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - ADL domain ………….. 249 Figure 8.14 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - mobility domain ………….. 251 Figure 8.15 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - hand domain ………….. 254 Figure 8.16 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - participation domain … 256 Figure 8.17 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale - Recovery VAS………… 259 Figure 8.18 Percentage of time spent in specific position or activity……… 263 Figure 8.19 Number of sit to stand transitions undertaken during a single day…………………………………………………………….… Figure 8.20 Number of steps undertaken during a single day……… xiii 264 264 LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1 Summary of studies of community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke ………………… 62 Table 4.1 Gentile's taxonomy of skills ……………………………………… 77 Table 6.1 Exercises attempted during development of the intervention .. 121 Table 6.2 Screening tests and outcome measures to be used in the definitive trial …………………………………………………….. 148 Table 6.3 Characteristics of Community pilot participants ………………. 149 Table 6.4 Timed Up and Go pilot study - subject characteristics ……….. 171 Table 6.5 Timed Up and go pilot study - summary of gait parameters and components of Timed Up and Go …………………………. 172 Table 6.6 Test - retest relaibility of TUG2m over one week ………… 176 Table 7.1. Schedule of outcome measure visits 193 Table 8.1 Subject characteristics ………………………………………….. 202 Table 8.2 Reasons for drop out following recruitment to the study …….. 204 Table 8.3 Summary of valid and missing data points for all outcome measures at eacn measurement timepoint …………………. 206 Table 8.4 Descriptive data for Barthel index scores …………………… 208 Table 8.5 Descriptive data for Frenchay Activity index scores ……… 212 Table 8.6 Descriptive data for the Motor Assessment Scale total scores 215 Table 8.7 Descriptive data for Timed Up and Go 2m components …… 220 Table 8.8 Medians and interquartile ranges for number of steps on The Step Test ………………………………………………... 226 Table 8.9 Descriptive data for the Frenchay Arm Test ………………….. 231 Table 8.10 Descriptive data for the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale ……………………………………………………………… 235 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - strength domain ………………………………………... 239 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - memory domain ………………………………………... 242 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - mood domain ………………………………………... 244 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - communication domain ………………………………………... 246 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - ADL domain ………………………………………... 248 Table 8.11 Table 8.12 Table 8.13 Table 8.14 Table 8.15 Table 8.16 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale xiv -mobility domain ………………………………………... 251 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - hand domain ………………………………………... 253 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - participation domain ………………………………………... 255 Medians and interquartile ranges for the Stroke Impact Scale - VAS of recovery.. ………………………………………... 258 Table 8.20 Summary of Exercise Repetitions undertaken by participants 261 Table 8.21 Summary of descriptive data of activity undertaken during a single day ……………………………………………………….. 262 Table 8.17 Table 8.18 Table 8.19 xv 1. INTRODUCTION 1.1 Scene setting “The impact of a stroke may continue for as long as the person who has had the stroke lives, which means that services may need to be available for the whole of their life”. (Department of Health, 2007 p38). Every year 15 million people in the world suffer a stroke (Mackay and Mensah 2004). The burden of stroke is considerable, it is reported as the third highest cause of death in the Western world (EUSI 2003) and the leading cause of adult disability (Khaw, 1996: Warlow 1998, Mackay and Mensah, 2004). Stroke is generally considered a disease of older people although it can occur at any age. In Scotland, there has been a reduction in stroke related mortality over the decade 1999 – 2008 (ISD 2009). With an increasingly aging population however, it is likely that the prevalence of people living with chronic stroke related disability will increase (Truelsen et al 2006). Due to the on-going nature of post-stroke disability, the condition of stroke requires extensive resources for management and treatment. It has been reported that stroke accounts for around 4-6% of the annual National Health Service (NHS) budget, which equates to an estimated direct cost of £2.8 billion (National Audit Office, 2005). It has been recognised that while in-patient stroke services have improved markedly over the recent past, services in the community setting to support people with stroke need to be reviewed and improved (Outpatient Services Trialists, 2004; The Stroke Association, 2010). Furthermore, once an individual returns to the community setting there can sometimes be a deterioration in functional ability as the challenges to the individual can be multifaceted and complex compared to the relatively safe and controlled environment within the hospital setting (Outpatient Services Trialists, 2004). In the first four weeks following stroke, approximately 20% of people will die, around 30% will make a full recovery, leaving around 50% of stroke survivors with residual 1 Chapter 1 Introduction significant disability (Langton Hewer et al 1993; Hacke et al, 2000; Hardie et al, 2004). The nature and extent of symptoms following stroke depend on the location and size of the area of brain affected by the stroke event, with the result that people with stroke can present with a vast array of symptoms. The most common sequalae include impairments of motor and sensory functions associated with hemiplegia, cognitive and perceptual disturbance, bladder and bowel dysfunction and disorders of communication and mood. For comprehensive consideration of these issues the reader is referred to relevant texts such as Carr and Shepherd (2003); Stokes (2004), and Barnes et al (2005). The rehabilitation of people with stroke is therefore challenging as any of the symptoms can have an ongoing and extensive impact on broad domains of functioning such as work, leisure and social integration. The management of neurological disability places much reliance on rehabilitation. The therapist has a multitude of therapeutic techniques available for reducing impairment (Ward 2005). Many therapeutic interventions however, are poorly defined and do not necessarily have an explicit theoretical underpinning. In order to advance practice therefore, physiotherapists working in stroke rehabilitation are currently faced by the challenges of defining and undertaking evaluation of complex interventions (Pomeroy and Tallis 2002). 1.2 Guidelines and Policies for Ongoing Stroke Rehabilitation in the Community In the past decade, a number of guidelines and strategies have been developed in the UK in order to improve stroke care. For people with late-stage stroke, recent recommendations from guideline development bodies in England and Scotland identify 2 Chapter 1 Introduction the need for ongoing access to specialist services. The most recent guidelines from the Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Network (SIGN) state: “Stroke patients in the community should have access to specialist therapybased rehabilitation services.” (SIGN 2010, p52). A recent survey of stroke survivors found that 21% felt they required further community based physiotherapy but were not able to access this service from the NHS (Stroke Association, 2010). While the National Stroke Strategy (Department of Health 2007) recommended that people with stroke should be able to access skilled stroke services for “as long as they need it”, resources are not finite. This means that physiotherapists working in stroke rehabilitation need to consider how to provide ongoing practice and rehabilitation to enable people with late-stage stroke to maintain improvements in function. One method of providing ongoing practice that has demonstrated effectiveness is the use of exercise classes (Marigold et al 2005; Mead et al, 2007). Not all people with stroke however are able to access class based activities, therefore physiotherapists also need to consider how to develop effective methods of self practice strategies of functional tasks in other community settings such as the home environment. 1.3 Stroke Services in the Community One of the major aims for the physiotherapist working in stroke rehabilitation is to provide an environment in which the person with stroke can undertake exercise to maximise their potential physical ability and functional independence. It is well documented that better stroke outcomes are associated with more rehabilitation (Langhorne et al, 1996; Kwakkel, 2006). During the in-patient phase of stroke rehabilitation however, it has been well documented that people with stroke spend much of their day alone and inactive, with less than 15% of waking hours being 3 Chapter 1 Introduction engaged in rehabilitation or physical practice of functional activities (Newall et al 1997; Bernhardt et al 2004). It is not clear how active people with stroke are once living in the community. In England and Wales, access to specialist stroke rehabilitation provision for longer term management in the community is available for 55% of people with stroke (Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party, 2010). 62% of people with stroke may however, wait more than two weeks to access these services (The Stroke Association 2010). In 2010 there is recognition of the pressing need to improve stroke services in the community in the United Kingdom (UK) (The Stroke Association 2010). At the time of planning the study reported in this thesis however, the need to provide ongoing management and services for people with late-stage stroke did not appear to be a high priority. 1.4 The impetus for this study Since the 1990’s there has been a growing interest, and growing body of evidence, relating to the effectiveness of “task specific practice” in neurological rehabilitation (for example Dean and Shepherd, 1997; Page, 2003; Sullivan et al, 2007). It is not clear exactly what is meant by task specific practice – but in its’ simplest terms it refers to training specifically for a task. Carr and Shepherd (1998) have stated that when considering whether to structure exercise training as whole or part practice that: “the action should be practised in its entirety, particularly when one part of the action is to a large part dependent on the performance of a preceding part” (Carr and Shepherd 1998, p37). In subsequent work, reference to whole practice is made as analogous to task specific practice (Carr and Shepherd 2003). Conversely, Shumway Cook and 4 Chapter 1 Introduction Woollacott (2001) suggested that part-practice can be an effective way to retrain some tasks. From experience of neurological rehabilitation, physiotherapists often structure practice sessions so that component parts of a movement that are difficult for an individual are practised repetitively but separately to the whole movement. Referring to Motor Learning texts to gain a definitive answer as to whether to structure exercise sessions for people with stroke as whole or part practice, it became apparent that the recommendations to adopt whole practice or part practice was derived from healthy populations, not from people with neurological impairments. The focus of this thesis is on how to structure practice to re-learn functional tasks for community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. The aspiration at the start of this research journey was to add, in a small way, to the body of knowledge in this area. 1.5 Structure of the thesis This thesis reports the findings from a randomised controlled trial (RCT) of a homebased exercise programme for people with late-stage stroke. The exercise programme was based on Motor Learning principles for how to structure task practice. The literature review in the ensuing chapters provides consideration of three areas of literature. Firstly, in chapter two, an overview of the nature of stroke and poststroke recovery mechanisms is presented. An exploration of the process of Stroke Rehabilitation is presented in chapter three. A longitudinal perspective is used to consider the management focus at various time-points after stroke. The main focus of chapter three however is a critical 5 Chapter 1 Introduction consideration of the evidence surrounding physiotherapy rehabilitation strategies for people late after stroke. This review is limited to interventions conducted for community dwelling individuals with late-stage stroke. Furthermore consideration of evidence was restricted to interventions that did not require extensive technical support or expensive equipment, such as Treadmill Training, as this was not felt to be a viable rehabilitation option within the home setting. Chapter four considers literature surrounding Motor Learning and the reacquisition of skill following stroke. As will be seen, the majority of the theory underpinning learning of motor skills was developed by sports scientists, exercise scientists and psychologists. Therapists, psychologists and movement scientists working in neurological rehabilitation have applied the recommendations from the Motor Learning literature to the rehabilitation process and a consideration of the relevance of this has been provided. The rationale for the study and the aims are put forward in chapter five to contextualise the work presented in chapter six. The development of the methodology for the definitive trial is reported in chapter six. This work was exploratory in nature and consisted of pilot work undertaken in an out-patient setting as well as a small pilot of the potential methodology carried out in the community. Findings from the development phase informed the definitive RCT. The methodology for the RCT is reported in chapter seven and the results are presented in chapter eight. The final chapter critically considers the findings from the RCT, relates these findings to previous work, critiques the limitations of the RCT and considers how the current study fits with Motor Learning theory. Finally suggestions for how the work can be taken forward are presented. 6 Chapter 1 Introduction 2. STROKE 2.1 Introduction This chapter provides definitions and an overview of the nature of stroke, providing a summary of epidemiology and aetiology. A more in-depth consideration of stroke recovery and management will be provided with an emphasis on sub-acute and chronic stroke. 2.2 Definitions The World Health Organisation (WHO) defines stroke as: “a clinical syndrome characterised by rapidly developing clinical symptoms and / or signs of focal and at times global … loss of cerebral function with symptoms lasting more than 24 hours or leading to death, with no other apparent cause other than that of vascular origin” (Aho et al 1980). Stroke was traditionally considered as distinct from a threatened stroke or Transient Ischaemic Attack (TIA), with TIA being considered as a cerebral ischaemic event of less than 24 hours duration (Hankey et al 1993). The definition of a TIA has recently been updated as: “a transient episode of neurological dysfunction caused by focal brain, spinal cord, or retinal ischemia, without acute infarction.” (Easton et al, 2009 p 2276). It has been highlighted that TIA should be considered as part of the continuum of stroke, with both conditions being indicative of disturbances to cerebral circulation and increased risk of disability and morbidity (Easton et al, 2009). A further manifestation of cerebrovascular attack, again not recorded as stroke, is Reversible Ischaemic Neurological Deficit (RIND) whereby symptoms resolve completely within 21 days (Warlow et al 2001). 7 Chapter 2 Stroke Stroke is a recovering neurological condition. In the initial hours following the acute event, the severity of symptoms will become apparent. In the aftermath of stroke, recovery will occur to some extent, although around 50% of stroke survivors are left with varying degrees of disability (Langton Hewer et al 1993; Hacke et al, 2000). Stroke is not a homogeneous entity, discrete signs and symptoms are apparent depending on the site and size of lesion and this is discussed further in 2.3. 2.3 Aetiology of Stroke The term “stroke” covers a broad diagnostic range and refers to dysfunction in the blood supply to an area of the brain (Effective Health Care 1992; SIGN 2010). Normal brain function requires an adequate supply of arterial blood to provide nutrition and oxygen as well as an efficient venous system to remove cellular waste products (Siegel and Sapru, 2006). The site and extent of the brain damage caused by a stroke contribute to the subsequent neurological deficits. Depending on the severity of the disruption to the cerebral blood supply, the person with stroke will experience a variety of impairments or loss of brain function, which may result in permanent impairment of function, restricted ability to conduct every day activities, and in severe cases, death. There are a number of different causes of stroke. The most well documented mechanisms of stroke are of either an occlusion within the cranial blood supply (ischaemic stroke – either due to embolus or thrombus) or due to a rupture of blood vessels (haemorrhagic stoke). Ischaemia is reported to account for approximately 80% of Stroke cases (Sudlow & Warlow 1997; Syme et al 2005). The most common cause of ischaemic stroke is occlusion of one of the major cerebral arteries; in 8 Chapter 2 Stroke descending order of frequency these are Middle Cerebral Artery (MCA), Posterior Cerebral Artery (PCA), or Anterior Cerebral Artery (ACA) respectively. The main artery is not always affected, other common sites are their smaller perforating branches to deeper parts of the brain. Brainstem strokes, arising from disease in the vertebral and basilar arteries, are less common. Approximately a further 10% of strokes are reported to be due to Primary Intracerebral Haemorrhage (PICH), 5% due to Subarachnoid Haemorrhage (SAH) and in a further 5% of cases, the cause is uncertain (Sudlow & Warlow 1997). Primary Intracerebral Haemorrhage into the deeper parts of the brain usually occurs in older, hypertensive people. PICH is often associated with a particular type of degeneration, known as lipohyalinosis or fibrohyalinosis, which results in necrotic lesions in the small penetrating arteries of the brain. The arterial walls weaken, are replaced by collagen, the wall thickens and the lumen narrows and it is thought that microaneurysms develop. These may rupture and lead to lacunar infarcts or small deep haemorrhages. The resultant haematoma may spread by splitting planes of white matter to form a substantial mass lesion. Haematomas usually occur in the deeper parts of the brain, often involving the thalamus, lentiform nucleus and external capsule, and less often, the cerebellum and the pons. Sub-Arachnoid Haemorrhage (SAH) involves bleeding into the subarachnoid space, usually arising from rupture of an aneurysm situated at or near the circle of Willis. The most common site is in the region of the anterior communicating artery, with PCA and MCA locations almost as frequent. Congenital factors play some part in the aetiology of berry aneurysms in a younger population, but SAH is not 9 Chapter 2 Stroke predominantly a disease of the young. Hypertension and vascular disease lead to an increase in aneurysm size and subsequent rupture. In a small number of patients, stroke may occur due to general medical disorders which affects either the arteries or the blood going through them e.g. arteritis, the collagen vascular diseases such as systemic lupus erythematosus and polyarteritis nodosa, bacterial endocarditis, mitral valve prolapse and haematological diseases such as thrombocythaemia and sickle cell disease have all been reported as causes of stroke (Baer and Durward 2004). 2.3.1 Biophysical mechanisms of stroke Hademenos and Massoud (1997) have reported the biophysical mechanisms of stroke, stating that there are six distinct processes as identified below: Atherosclerosis Calcified fatty deposits or plaques are laid down circumferentially on the intimal layer of blood vessel walls. As the process advances, blood vessel walls become thicker, irregular, fibrosed and calcified leading to a reduced and more turbulent blood flow. Atherosclerotic processes promote thrombosis, partly due to flow obstruction and partly to high shear stresses on the vessel walls. Atherosclerotic blood vessels are at risk of displacement of small plaques which subsequently may lodge in smaller blood vessels resulting in ischaemia. Atherosclerotic thrombosis accounts for approximately 33% of all stroke cases (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). Embolus 10 Chapter 2 Stroke An embolus is a foreign body such as a blood clot, a bubble of oxygen or a fragment of tissue that circulates through the blood stream until it gets lodged in a blood vessel. Emboli can originate from blood vessels themselves as well as organs such as the heart or lungs. In this condition, small particles of gas or solid matter become “travelling clots” e.g. a collection of platelets dislodged from an atherosclerotic lesion. The embolus circulates until reaching a smaller blood vessel that it blocks with ensuing ischaemia. Emboli are thought to be responsible for approximately 31% of all strokes (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). Thrombus A thrombus is a fibrinous clot formed due to atherosclerosis or blood vessel damage. Blood vessel injury and disruption to the intimal lining of the vessels are thought to be aggravating factors in thrombus formation. Approximately 33% of ischaemic strokes are caused by thrombus (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). Reduced Systemic Pressure In the three mechanisms described above, it is thought that the heart functions under normal systemic pressure. In cases of cardiovascular disease such as atrial fibrillation, cardiac muscle becomes weakened, abnormal heartbeats can occur resulting in reduced systemic pressure and ischaemia (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). Haemorrhage During haemorrhagic stroke events, cerebral blood vessels rupture resulting in blood seeping into surrounding brain tissue and ultimately increasing the pressure on surrounding tissue, reduced vessel diameter and a reduction in blood flow through the vessel. The two main causes of haemorrhage are aneurysm (ballooning of the blood vessel wall) which may rupture to cause a bleed, and arteriovenous malformations (AVM) which is a congenital lesion due to inappropriate capillary 11 Chapter 2 Stroke development, resulting in a tangled mass of weakened blood vessels between arterial and venous systems (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). Vasospasm Cerebral vasospasm occurs in arteries in the subarachnoid space following a subarachnoid haemorrhage (SAH). Vasospasm can last for hours to days, causes reduced blood flow and subsequent cerebral ischaemia. Vasospasm is maximal between five and ten days after SAH, but the damage to surrounding tissue may be minimised with judicious use of vasodilators (Hademenos and Massoud, 1997). The biophysical mechanisms briefly described above may contribute solely or in combination to the cause of stroke. Recent advances in the medical and pharmaceutical treatment of stroke are, in part, based on an increasing understanding of the underlying biophysical mechanisms. 2.4 Epidemiology of Stroke There have been many epidemiological studies in stroke throughout the world, with an increasing number of published studies in the past 25 years as medical interest in the management of stroke gains more attention. Epidemiological studies may access data regarding incidence, prevalence and mortality in a number of ways. The validity of some forms of data provision has been questioned, for example mortality rates may vary regionally depending on disease coding practices (Thorvaldsen et al, 1995). The use of Stroke Registers has improved the validity of data available, improving consensus on disease coding and a more systematic manner of recording stroke events (Thorvaldsen et al, 1995). Criteria for recording 12 Chapter 2 Stroke incidence and mortality include using standard definitions of stroke with at least 80% of cases verified by scan, standard methods for data collection and standard methods for reporting (Feigin and vander Hoorn 2004). These quality criteria should result in improved validity of stroke epidemiological data in the twenty first century. 2.4.1 Incidence Incidence of stroke can be defined as the first-ever cases of stroke occurring in a given population over a defined time period (Warlow et al 2001). There can be difficulties in identifying all patients with stroke as most stroke registers tend to be hospital based (Bamford et al, 1988) and yet not all stroke patients are admitted or referred to hospital. Studies in the United Kingdom towards the end of the 20 th century estimated that hospital admission varied from 55% (Bamford et al, 1988) to 78% (Wolfe et al, 1993). In Scotland, in the 12 months up to end of March 2009, there were around 16,700 hospital admissions for stroke (ISD, 2009). The Oxford Community Stroke Project (OCSP) was the first comprehensive, prospective epidemiological study conducted in the 1980’s (Bamford et al 1988). A population of over 105,000 attending 49 General Practices were meticulously followed by the study team, if stroke was suspected, during a four year period. Stroke was confirmed by computer tomographic scan (CT scan) and clinical assessment. 675 first ever strokes were recorded during this time, equating to an incidence of 200:100,000. The OCSP highlighted an increasing incidence of stroke with increasing age (Bamford et al 1988) and this finding has been replicated by many other studies (for example Sudlow and Warlow, 1997; Wolfe et al, 2000) with 13 Chapter 2 Stroke reports of a relatively low incidence up until the age of around mid 50’s followed by a sharp increase in the 65-74 year age groups and beyond. Age Wolfe, C. D.A. et al. Stroke 2000;31:2074-2079 Figure 2.1 Age specific stroke incidence rates per 100,000 population from three centres in Europe In the Western World, in the latter part of the twentieth century, incidence rates for stroke were generally cited at around 2:1,000 (Bamford 1988). There are variations in worldwide reported incidence of stroke. The MONICA (Monitoring Trends and Determinants in Cardiovascular Disease) study followed populations in 16 European and two Asian populations between 1985 and 1987. 13,597 stroke events were 14 Chapter 2 Stroke registered in a total background population of 2.9 million people aged 35 to 64 years, which is towards the younger end of the stroke population. Age-standardised stroke incidence rates were reported as between 1–2.85:1,000 in men and from 0.5– 1.98: 1,000 in women. The MONICA study also found a higher incidence of stroke in eastern European populations compared to western Europeans (Thorvaldsen et al 1995). More recently, Wolfe et al (2000) gathered incidence figures over a three year period from Dijon, France; Erlangen in Germany and London UK and found that Dijon had the lowest age-standardised incidence rates at 100.4:100,000, followed by London 123.9:100,000 and Erlangen 136.4:100,000. Statistically significant differences in survival rates were also reported with a 35% fatality rate overall, which was more discretely reported as 27% Dijon, 34% Erlangen, and 41% London, (p<0.001). With improved acute medical management of stroke and more systematic gathering of incidence data however, the incidence of stroke is declining. In Oxford, in 2004, the age-specific incidence of stroke had fallen 40%, compared to 1988 data, to an incidence of 0.71: 1,000 (Rothwell et al, 2004). In Scotland, a community based study of incidence rates for the population of the Scottish Borders region between 1998-2000 reported an incidence rate for first ever Cerebral Infarctions of 1.97:1,000 and first ever Cerebral Haemmorhage of 0.24:1,000. (Syme et al, 2005). This was comparable to national data at the time, however incidence rates are declining with 2008-09 reports of an average incidence rate in Scotland of 100,00 (ISD, 2009). just under 169.1: The National Scottish Stroke Audit claims estimates of approximately 15,000 people in Scotland suffer a stroke each year. Hospital care for Stroke patients accounts for 7% of all NHS beds and 5% of the entire NHS budget (ISD, 2010). 15 Chapter 2 Stroke Age-Sex Standardised (European Standard Population) Incidence rate per 100,000 population 300 250 200 150 100 50 0 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 Males 2006 Females 2007 2008 2009 Both Sexes (Information Services Division, 2009) Figure 2.2 Stroke Incidence in Scotland 2000 – 2009. In conclusion, while it is apparent that there is a reduction in the incidence of stroke, the incidence of stroke remains high in older people. The most recent figures in Scotland indicate an incidence rate of 1,761: 100,000 population in people aged over 75 years (ISD, 2009). With stroke being the major cause of disability in adults, the implications of these data, are that it is likely that more people with stroke will be living in the community as the demographic trend of an increasingly aging population is fulfilled. 2.4.2 Prevalence Prevalence refers to the number of people with a defined condition and who are alive within a set population at a certain point in time. It can be argued that 16 Chapter 2 Stroke prevalence data are more important in planning long-term services for stroke than incidence data (Hare et al, 2006), although this argument has been disputed by Warlow et al (2001) who point out that both the co-existing degree of activity limitation and participation restriction as well as pre-morbid conditions impact more realistically on the need for on-going long-term services. Further problems with using stroke prevalence data includes the fact that severely disabled people who die soon after their stroke may not be represented (despite representing a considerable burden to services) and retrospective studies may be confounded by mortality statistics obtained from death certificates that may not cite stroke as a condition or cause of death. One relatively recent study that has been conducted in a robust manner is the Copenhagen study which cites prevalence rates of 3-6:1,000 (Jørgenson et al 1995). To put these data into perspective, approximately 18 – 36 people with stroke would be registered in an average General Practitioner (GP) practice of around 6,000 people. In 1999 - 2000, in Scotland, at the time of planning the study, prevalence data were very similar to those cited by Jørgenson et al (1995) with prevalence rates for all ages cited as 4.8 : 1,000 for men and 4.7 : 1,000 for women (ISD 2000). When considering the older population, prevalence rates rose to 37 : 1,000 for men and 26.7 : 1,000 for women aged over 75. 2.5 Stroke Recovery Outcome following stroke is highly variable and influenced by many factors. For many subjects, there will be some degree of spontaneous recovery. Severity of the initial post-stroke deficit is a key predictor of outcome, however the amount of final 17 Chapter 2 Stroke recovery is not predictable (Chen et al, 2002). Within the first four weeks outcomes are grossly reported as death in approximately 20% of first strokes, full recovery in 30% and residual disability in 40-50% (Langton Hewer, 1993; Warlow, 1998; Hacke et al, 2002). In Scotland, the 30 day mortality rate for hospitalised patients is higher than this and is reported at 28% (Lewsey et al, 2009). Stroke recurrence has recently been reported as declining (Lewsey et al, 2010). In Scotland, in the 15 years preceding 2001 approximately 1 in 10 people hospitalised for stroke were hospitalised for a further stroke incident within five years (Lewsey et al, 2010). With the risk of second strokes being more disabling, this has further implications for rehabilitation services (Rothwell et al, 2004; Lewsey et al, 2010). Over the past 20 years, evidence from basic science and clinical research has started to provide evidence that the brain is capable of recovery following stroke, provided that relevant treatment is applied at an appropriate dosage and at an appropriate timepoint. Functional recovery during the first three to four weeks poststroke is known to occur with resolution of local factors such as oedema, absorption of necrotic matter and the establishment of collateral circulation in the peri-lesional area (Rossini and Forno 2004; Dobkin and Carmichael, 2005). Evidence has also emerged to demonstrate that even long after perceived recovery has plateaued, cortical reorganisation of the human brain may occur with an associated change in motor function (Nudo, 2003; Nudo 2006). 2.5.1 Neurobiological Evidence of Stroke Recovery In the first few days post-stroke, recovery is mainly due to reperfusion of ischaemic penumbra and resolution of oedema (Chen et al, 2002) and restitution of non- 18 Chapter 2 Stroke infarcted penumbra (Kwakkel et al 2004b, Kreisel et al 2007). Emerging evidence from neurobiological research and neuroimaging studies have shown that the human brain is capable of extensive functional and structural plasticity for weeks to months following stroke (Green 2003; Nudo 2003). In addition to plastic changes, functional training as well as compensatory movement strategies may further impact on movement recovery. A summary of the emerging evidence from animal and human studies is presented in this section. Plasticity has been defined as “any enduring change in the cortical properties either morphological or functional” (Donoghue et al 1996; Schallert et al, 2003; Butefisch 2004 (p163). Evidence suggests that the brain is primed for change post-stroke (Teasell & Kalra 2005) and that repeated use of the affected limbs post-stroke is a major influencing factor on potential plastic changes. Evidence from animal studies and brain mapping research indicate that a number of different processes contribute to neuroplasticity, and that these processes occur at different chronological timepoints post-stroke (Schaecter 2004; Kwakkel et al 2004b; Kreisel et al, 2007). Anatomical and tissue repair The unmasking of pre-existing but latent connections, has been suggested as one key mechanism by which cerebral cortex reorganisation is mediated early after stroke (Kwakkel et al 2004b). It has been suggested that the process of unmasking latent synapses occurs within the hyper-acute phase post-stroke – i.e. within minutes. 19 Chapter 2 Stroke A mechanism responsible for synaptic plasticity and cortical reorganisation is the presence of horizontal cortical connections within the motor cortex. The motor cortex has many overlapping motor representations, and within the cortex there is a profound network of horizontal interconnections mainly within discrete representations but also a less dense network between areas (Butefisch 2004). Longer-term changes within the brain have been attributed to processes of axonal regeneration and axonal sprouting as well as synaptogenesis in the peri-infarct area. In rats, it has been demonstrated that peri-infarct axonal sprouting may occur between 3 – 14 days after cortical infarction and that up to 60 days post infarct there are signs of synaptogenesis (Stroemer et al 1995 cited in Nudo 2006). Further, it has been suggested that specific patterns of gene expression early after infarct are responsible for growth promotion associated with sprouting processes and contribute to the repair mechanisms of brain tissue (Nudo 2006). Cortical Reorganisation It is well documented that people with stroke show varying degrees of recovery within days or weeks after the acute event (Bonita and Beaglehole, 1988; Kwakkel et al, 2004b). One of the early explanations for recovery has been that normal brain tissue facilitated a process of reorganisation (Sterr, 2004). This hypothesis has been confirmed on both animals and humans, however it has been found that reorganisation may confer both positive but also negative phenomena. In a study of squirrel monkeys where small lesions were made in part of the distal forelimb representation of the primary motor cortex (M1) and the animals left to recover with no training (Nudo et al, 1996), the intact area reduced in size 20 Chapter 2 Stroke The excitability of the motor cortex has been studied using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) and Positron, Emission Tomography (PET) scanning. Investigation of normal brain activity show activation of the contralateral hemisphere during a motor task. In a study of hand muscle activation in patients within two weeks of stroke, Turton et al (1996) demonstrated a reduced excitability of the ipsilesional motor cortex compared to the contralatesional cortex, however this was shown to improve towards normal patterns as hand motor recovery occurred over a one year period. Liepert et al (2000) used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to investigate the effects of a 12 day programme of Constraint Induced Therapy (CIT) on 13 people with chronic stroke (mean 4.9 years post-event). Immediately after intervention the use of the affected hand in daily activities had significantly improved and the cortical representation of the affected hand muscles was found to be enlarged significantly, accompanied by a shift in the central position of the motor map which implied a recruitment of adjacent brain areas. At six months post intervention, eight subjects were followed up and demonstrated that high activity levels had been retained, although the cortical area representation of the affected cortex had returned to normal size indicating a return to normal balance of motor neurone excitability. Negative cortical reorganisation has been reported in sub-acute stroke patients approximately eight – 16 weeks post-stroke and no longer undertaking rehabilitation. In addition to a reduction in ipsilesional motor cortex excitability, a reduction in the 21 Chapter 2 Stroke cortical representation of affected muscles was also noted, which has been postulated as being related to either neuronal damage or associated with disuse (Traversa et al 1997 in Liepert et al 2000). Feydy et al (2002) undertook a longitudinal study of cortical reorganisation in 14 stroke patients between one and six months port stroke. fMRI was used to monitor cortical reorganisation related to recovery. Two reorganisation patterns were found. Nine patients demonstrated “focusing” whereby initial patterns of ipsilateral and contralateral activation developed towards a normal activation of the contralateral sensorimotor cortex. Five patients showed “persistent recruitment” associated with a lesion of the M1 primary motor cortex, with a perseveration of ipsilateral activation. The studies considered above generally have investigated people within the first year after stroke. The fact that there still exists a potential for recovery, or changes in activity, at months or even years after stroke is less well understood and thought to be mostly due to compensations and behavioural change (Kriesel et al, 2007). Processes related to therapy driven cortical hyperexcitability (Liepert et al, 2000) and motor cortex enlargement in subjects following intense training of limbs, such as that shown following Constraint Induced Therapy (Levy et al, 2001; Wolf et al, 2002) identify a potential for change. 2.5.2 Prognostic indicators of recovery Post-stroke recovery is influenced by various factors such as age, lesion characteristics and pre-morbid status (Duncan et al, 1992b). Interactions between these factors may also play a part in the capacity for recovery. Most evidence relating to physical recovery indicators in stroke arises from medical literature, 22 Chapter 2 Stroke although a recent systematic review (Meijer et al 2003) urges careful interpretation of some findings due to the use of variable methodologies. Age Although stroke can occur at any age, the incidence rises sharply past the age of 65 (Wolfe et al 2000b; ISD 2009). Severity of stroke is not related to age, however age can influence outcomes. With increasing age there is an association with poorer outcomes in basic activities of daily living (ADL) and greater levels of residual disability (Nakayama et al 1994; Kelly Hayes et al 2003). Early recovery of movement: A recent study by Hashimoto and colleagues (2007) used a new five point ordinal rating scale, the Ability of Basic Movement Scale (ABMS) to assess the ability to perform basic movements at the bedside at set times after stroke and relate this to functional ability at discharge. While elements of reliability of the ABMS have been established by the investigatory team, validity is less clearly developed and the 5 activities in the ABMS do not have clear operational definitions, which makes generalised use of this scale problematic. Data on 142 subjects indicated that the “ability to turn over from supine”, “remain sitting” and “sit up” at 10 days post-stroke were “strong” predictors of “functional ability” at discharge.. Sub-acute predictors Within the past 10 years, two systematic reviews of literature have been undertaken to ascertain the evidence-based predictors of functional recovery at six and 12 months post-stroke in a sub-acute stroke population (Kwakkel et al1996; Meijer et al 2003). 23 Chapter 2 Stroke While Kwakkel at al (1996), identified studies up until 1995, Meijer et al (2003) included studies up until 2002, and it is notable that during that additional time period, there has been an increased interest in stroke rehabilitation research. While criticisms have been levelled at the methodology employed in many of the studies, some interesting conclusions can still be drawn. Several studies with internal and statistical validity have identified factors linked with a poorer prognosis. These factors include : the loss of consciousness persisting for 48 hours post-stroke (Wade and Langton Hewer, 1987, Taub et al, 1994); the presence of urinary incontinence at one week post-stroke (Thomessen et al, 1999; Meijer et al, 2003); a lower initial Barthel Index score within the first two weeks post-stroke (Wade and Langton Hewer 1987; Taub et al 1994) and severe limb paresis or paralysis (Taub et al, 1994). Other potential predictors included swallowing problems (Taub et al 1994); apraxia and visuo-spatial problems (Sveen et al 1996) and poor sitting balance (Wade and Langton Hewer 1987). 2.6 Functional Recovery profiles Epidemiological studies of stroke recovery and papers reporting stroke rehabilitation research have repeatedly stated that the most rapid and the majority of physical recovery is seen in the first three months following stroke (Wade and LangtonHewer 1987; Kreisel et al, 2007). It is important to remember that while stroke is not a homogeneous entity and that individual recovery patterns may be diverse, there is still a general regularity to recovery profiles within the first six months irrespective of rehabilitation input (Kwakkel et al 2004b). Most of the early papers describing physical recovery were limited to capturing data within the first three 24 Chapter 2 Stroke months post-stroke (Wade et al, 1985; Wade and Langton Hewer, 1987; Heller et al, 1987) and therefore more is known about that period. Gowland was one of the first physiotherapists to report observations of physical recovery post-stroke in a systematic manner (Gowland 1982). Two hundred and twenty nine people in the sub-acute phase of stroke (median time since onset seven weeks) were included in her work. Measures of physical function were relatively crude, however she was able to report that by the end of the rehabilitation period, while only 5% had functional upper limb recovery, 70% of patients were able to walk independently (Gowland, 1982). Partridge et al (1993) collected data on 348 people with stroke undertaking basic functional activities, relating to bed mobility and gait, over a six week period poststroke. On admission, approximately 29% could move from lying to sitting, 29% could stand up independently and 11% could walk indoors. At six weeks these data had improved to 74%, 73% and 56% respectively. In a large study of 947 people with stroke, data on functional recovery was collected for six months after the initial stroke event (Jørgenson et al, 1995). Data were collected on crude indices of recovery such as the Barthel Index, and on considering the recovery profiles, patients were stratified by initial severity of stroke. The findings in this study demonstrated that for 80% of the stroke population, optimal activities of daily living scores were achieved within eight weeks after stroke onset and that even at 20 weeks the most severely affected patients had achieved their 25 Chapter 2 Stroke best recovery (Jørgenson et al, 1995). However, while claims are made that no more recovery should be expected after the six month post-stroke period, it appears from the study that patients stopped receiving therapy when the team felt that more improvement was ”unlikely”, therefore not capturing any later possible improvement. Partridge et al (1993) reported detailed profiles of basic mobility recovery within the first six weeks of recruitment to their study, however they failed to look at recovery profiles of upper limb function, or to extend the study into the later stages of stroke. The gross measures that have been reported by Gowland (1982) and Jørgenson et al, (1995) may reflect compensatory activity by the unaffected side rather than resolution of motor deficits, or the effects of whether therapy was still being received. These studies do however allow physiotherapists to start to build a picture of functional recovery, which at this point in time requires extension into longitudinal studies to allow a picture of the natural recovery of people with late-stage stroke to be built up. 2.7 Summary This chapter has provided an overview of Stroke epidemiology, aetiology and recovery mechanisms. Stroke is a recovering neurological condition, however the stroke population are heterogeneous and therefore there are variations in the rate and extent of recovery. Incidence rates of stroke are declining, in part this is due to improved healthcare and improved management in the acute phase of stroke. Stroke incidence in older people, however is relatively high and recent prevalence rates in Scotland indicated almost a ten fold increase in stroke prevalence for people aged 75 and over, than compared to stroke prevalence for the adult population as a 26 Chapter 2 Stroke whole (ISD, 2000). With more people expected to survive stroke, with increasing levels of disability, it is clear that a substantial body of evidence is required, so that appropriate interventions are available for stroke survivors, at all stages of the poststroke period. In the following chapters a review of the evidence underpinning Stroke Rehabilitation will be presented, this will have a particular emphasis on rehabilitation in later stage stroke as it pertains to this study. The theory supporting Motor Learning will be presented and a discussion of how this theory is applied to healthy subjects as well as to people with a neurological impairment. Ultimately this review of the literature will contextualise the aims, rationale and hypotheses of the work reported in this thesis. 27 Chapter 2 Stroke 3. STROKE REHABILITATION 3.1 Introduction The process of Stroke Rehabilitation has received considerable interest in the past two decades and there now exists a vast amount of literature in the area. This chapter will commence with a brief overview of what constitutes Stroke Rehabilitation. Following this, the process of Stroke Rehabilitation will be discussed from two perspectives. Firstly, a brief overview of the whole journey of the stroke patient, from stroke onset, is presented in order to put the rehabilitation of people with late-stage stroke into context. This is followed in section 3.4 by a focus on Physiotherapy approaches for Stroke with an emphasis on interventions for people with later-stage stroke and the evidence for Stroke Rehabilitation in the community setting as these two factors are of most relevance for this thesis. Rehabilitation has been defined as: “an active and dynamic process through which a disabled person is helped to acquire knowledge and skills in order to maximise their physical, psychological, and social functioning” (Barnes 2003, p iv4). Stroke rehabilitation covers a substantial period of time and could be considered as the time since stroke until the end of life. Langhorne and Legg (2003) have commented on the false division of ‘early’ or ‘acute’ from ‘later’ rehabilitation when considering the evidence for Stroke Rehabilitation, as the available evidence does not necessarily fall neatly into such precise era. In terms of considering where to focus critique of the volume of papers investigating physiotherapy focused stroke rehabilitation, it was considered useful to consider the continuum of Stroke Rehabilitation as a matrix (see figure 3.1). This matrix indicates “notional stages” of rehabilitation and within this thesis summary evidence will be presented for all 28 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation stages with an in-depth critique on late-stage and community-based interventions. It is accepted that other authors, in considering a similar matrix, may identify different treatment intensities, different stages or apply alternative labels for each stage. Late-stage / Chronic 6 months+ Ongoing rehab 4-6 months post-stroke Sub-acute 1-3 months post-stroke Time since stroke Acute rehabilitation 8 – 28 days post-stroke Acute management 0-7 days post-stroke Daily (7 days) Daily (5 days) Daily (5 days) in-patient in-patient out-patient or treatment rehab community rehab Physiotherapy frequency Figure 3.1. setting and 2 – 3 days per week out-patient / community 1-2 days per month out-patient / community indicative Monitor <1x month No formal treatment rehabilitation Notional Stroke Rehabilitation matrix Indicating representative stroke patient – physiotherapist contact 29 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation It is not simple to summarise Stroke Rehabilitation within a single definition. It has been noted however, that “a clear consensus exists that the purpose of rehabilitation is to limit the impact of stroke related brain damage on daily life by using a mixture of therapeutic and problem solving approaches” (Young and Forster 2007 p86). Principal members of the stroke rehabilitation team, in addition to the medical practitioner, are rehabilitation nurses, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, speech and language therapists and clinical psychologists. In an integrated team, this should allow problems affecting upper and lower limb movements, balance, mobility, activities of daily living (such as bathing and dressing), communication and swallowing problems and disorders of mood and emotions to be addressed (EUSI 2003; Baer and Durward 2004; SIGN 2010;). A key recommendation of recent Stroke rehabilitation guidelines in Scotland states “The core multidisciplinary team should include appropriate levels of nursing, medical, physiotherapy, occupational therapy, speech and language therapy, and social work staff”. (SIGN 2010 p5) In Stroke Rehabilitation, physiotherapists are not only instrumental in the treatment of movement disorders, but also in prevention of secondary complications such as chest infection, soft tissue contracture and pressure sores (Ryerson and Levit 1997; Carr and Shepherd 1998; Edwards 2002; SIGN 2010). An indication of the role of the physiotherapist at various stages in the matrix of the stroke patient journey will be outlined in the following sections. 3.2 Physiotherapy Intervention in Acute Stroke Management during the first seven days post-stroke This section summarises non-pharmacological and non-surgical roles of the physiotherapist in Acute Stroke Rehabilitation. It is recommended that a 30 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation physiotherapy assessment of the stroke patient is undertaken within the first 72 hours post-stroke (RCP 2008). The specific aims of acute management relevant to Physiotherapy include: Positioning of the stroke patient to maintain joint range of motion and muscle length of two joint muscles (Carr and Kenney 1992; Chatterton et al 2001; Carr and Shepherd 2003), to reduce the development of abnormal muscle tone (Bobath 1990, Bhakta 2000; Edwards 2002), and to maintain body alignment (Lynch and Grisogono 1991; Carr and Kenney 1996; Chatterton 2001) In the acute post-stroke days, stroke patients may have reduced ventilatory function which may enhance the likelihood of hypoxaemia and atelectasis (Brott and Reed 1989; EUSI 2003). There is some evidence, from small scale studies, that if upright positioning is tolerated by the patient, this may be beneficial for maintaining respiratory function and preventing some respiratory complications such as infection or atelectasis (Chatterton et al 2000; Rowat et al 2001; Tyson and Nightingale 2004; SIGN 2010). Further, if the patient is able to tolerate mobilisation such as sitting out of bed or taking short assisted walks this also has been shown to have an effect on reducing respiratory complications (Indredravik et al 1999) There is an accumulation of evidence from research undertaken in Europe and Australia to indicate that early mobilisation, commenced within 24 hours of stroke onset and often on the day of admission has a beneficial effect in preventing some of the complications with prolonged immobility and in facilitating an improved physiological outcome, with better control of blood pressure, dehydration, pyrexia and glucose levels (Indredravik et al 1999; Langhorne and Pollock 2002; Bernhardt et al 2008). 31 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation In addition, assessment of and intervention strategies to address physical impairments as well as impairments of sensation, perception and cognition are recommended to be commenced at this point. (Ryerson and Levit 1997; SIGN 2010; RCP 2008). 3.3 Acute and Sub-acute rehabilitation As indicated at the start of this chapter, the “stages” of stroke rehabilitation identified in figure 3.1 are notional, therefore this section will consider the “acute” and “subacute” stages following stroke as a continuum. Many of the interventions will be similar or progressions of initial management strategies and, depending on the clinical picture presented by the patient, changes in the rehabilitation process will be indicated – such as cessation of respiratory management. During the first months post-stroke, physiotherapists will continue with many of the aims from the acute management stage in the first week. There will be long-term aims of maintaining joint range of motion and postural alignment, (Edwards 2002; Carr and Shepherd 2003; Herbert 2005; Bobath 1990) and avoiding soft tissue complications (Edwards 2002; Carr and Shepherd 2003; Ada and Canning 2005). Depending on the treatment philosophy being followed, physiotherapists may place an emphasis on maintaining or promoting normal muscle tone as a pre-cursor to movement re-education (Edwards et al 2002; Bobath 1990) or may encourage the patient to move through synergistic reflex movement patterns with subsequent encouragement of active movements (Brunnstrom 1970). Re-education of movement and functional tasks in relation to daily activity will continue from the acute management stage or will commence in the acute rehabilitation stage. 32 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Similarly, ongoing cognisance of and, where appropriate, rehabilitation of or adaptation of rehabilitation to account for impairments of cognition, memory, sensation, pain or speech may be indicated (Ryerson and Levit 1997; Carr and Shepherd 2003; Edwards 2002; Baer and Durward 2004;). As time since stroke progresses, people who have sustained a mild stroke may no longer be receiving rehabilitation for physical impairments involving the physiotherapist. However, many stroke patients will still require rehabilitation and may receive this either on either an in-patient basis or as an out-patient. In the past 20 years, strong evidence has emerged that people with stroke managed in a dedicated stroke unit tend to have better outcomes not only in reducing mortality, but also in improved functional outcomes than those managed in general medical wards or elderly care wards (for example Langhorne et al 1993; Hankey et al 1999; Seenan et al 2007). While Stroke Units may operate in a variety of ways depending on local requirements, a number of common strategies appear to be associated with successful outcomes of Stroke Unit rehabilitation. These strategies are complex and often inter-related but include early, integrated assessment of medical, nursing and therapy requirements, the instigation of early management strategies such as early mobilisation (Indredavik et al 1999; Langhorne and Pollock 2002). or prevention or treatment of complications such as infection (Indredavik et al 1999; Evans et al 2001; Langhorne and Pollock 2002), planned and co-ordinated multidisciplinary rehabilitation procedures which include early planning for discharge and specialist staff (Jørgensen et al 1995; Indredavik et al 1999; Evans et al 2001; Langhorne and Pollock 2002). 33 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation There is evidence that sub-acute stroke rehabilitation in the home setting can be beneficial (Young and Forster 1991; Gladman et al 1993; Baskett et al 1999). The Bradford Community Stroke trial explored the effects of eight weeks of rehabilitation received either in the home environment or in a day hospital (Young and Forster 1991). 58% of the cohort were less than 12 weeks post-stroke at time of discharge from hospital and 107 of an initial 124 participants were available for follow-up at the end of the intervention. Participants in both groups demonstrated small functional improvements on the Barthel Index and the Motor Club Assessment, but there were no significant differences between the groups except for stair climbing ability and social activity for participants receiving therapy at home (Young and Forster 1991). Similar findings were reported from a trial of 100 people with sub-acute stroke in New Zealand by Baskett et al (1999). In this study, people with stroke sustained at a mean time of five weeks prior to recruitment were randomised to either a control group who received therapist contact in a day hospital environment approximately two or three times a week, or an experimental group who received individualised functional exercises in the home environment. Exercises were recorded in a diary and progressed as deemed relevant by the therapist and the experimental group were visited weekly for up to three months. While both groups showed a trend towards improvement in gait speed and some outcomes such as the Motor Assessment Scale and the Barthel Index, there were no statistically significant differences in functional outcomes between the groups (Baskett et al, 1999). In a large study of 327 people with sub-acute stroke (within 3 months of stroke onset), Gladman et al (1993) found virtually no differences in outcomes between those receiving input from therapists in a new domiciliary rehabilitation service or a traditional hospital out-patients service following discharge from hospital. Despite stratifying for initial hospital admission location (Health Care of the Elderly ward, 34 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Stroke Unit or General Medical ward), there were no significant differences in outcome in Barthel Index scores at three or six months post discharge, and only a significant improvement in extended ADL scores for the Stroke Unit sub-stratum group receiving domicillary rehabilitation for their home and leisure activities Extended ADL scores. While the findings from these three studies suggest that there were no major statistically significant differences in outcomes between groups, the studies demonstrated that it was feasible to deliver on-going sub-acute stroke rehabilitation in the home setting (Young and Forster 1991; Gladman et al 1993; Baskett et al 1999). It was also suggested that rehabilitation provided in the home environment may be preferred by the patient, may be more easily tailored to the needs of an individual and can be more cost effective than on-going rehabilitation delivered in a day hospital setting (Young and Forster 1991). Ongoing Rehabilitation (4 – 6 months post-stroke) Ongoing rehabilitation may take place in the hospital or home environment. Depending on the needs of the patient, the rehabilitation input received may be similar to that outlined in the sub-acute stages, or it may reduce to a level where monitoring or maintenance is the prime focus (SIGN 2010; RCP2008). In Scotland, a recommendation based on a recent Cochrane review of home-based therapy services for people with stroke (Outpatients Service Triallists 2003), states: “Stroke patients in the community should have access to specialist therapybased rehabilitation services” (SIGN 2010, p52). Similarly, in England, specific recommendations are made regarding “the whole stroke pathway” including the provision of “later rehabilitation in the community” 35 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation (RCP 2008, p22). These recommendations indicate that the importance of continuing appropriate stroke rehabilitation, following discharge from hospital, as part of on-going rehabilitation is starting to be recognised. Evidence relating to the effectiveness of physiotherapy for people late after stroke will now be reviewed. 3.4 Evidence to support Physiotherapy in Late-Stage Stroke Rehabilitation The evidence to support the effectiveness of Physiotherapy for people with acute and sub-acute stroke has been emerging over the past two decades. However it was not until the last ten years or so that interest in the potential for rehabilitation for chronic stroke has received sustained attention. A recent Cochrane Review investigated the benefits of “therapy-based” rehabilitation services at more than a year post-stroke (Aziz et al, 2009). It was reported that analysis of the five trials that were included in the review resulted in an indication of a trend towards improvement but that there was insufficient evidence to make definitive conclusions (Aziz et al, 2009). Only two out of the five studies included in the above review were relevant to the work reported in this thesis and are critiqued in this chapter (Green et al, 2002; Wade et al, 1992), the other trials provided mixed Occupational Therapy (OT) and physiotherapy with limited focus on exercise (Mulder et al, 1989; Werner et al, 1996) or provided only OT (Sackley et al 2006). A number of trials that were excluded from the Cochrane Review of therapy-based rehabilitation at more than a year poststroke are included in this chapter (See table 3.1), as the findings have implications for the work reported in this thesis. Recently, Combs et al (2010) claimed that traditional therapy models have been “refocused” to investigate high intensity, task specific, short programmes of training. 36 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation While this high intensity, task specific practice could be considered to incorporate specific approaches aimed at retraining specific areas of the body, such as Constraint Induced Therapy (CIT) (Wolf et al 1989, Taub et al 2000; Wolf et al 2006) and Treadmill Training (TT) (Hesse et al 1994; Moseley et al 2003, Moore et al, 2010), a small number of investigators have studied the potential for change in community dwelling people with chronic stroke receiving therapy at home or in a community setting. A recent Cochrane review considered the effect of Repetitive task training (RTT) for improving functional ability after stroke and included studies of people with stroke along the continuum of stroke rehabilitation and in both home and hospital settings (French et al, 2009). A number of the studies included in that Cochrane review, relevant to the work in this thesis, have been considered in this chapter (e.g. Dean et al, 2000; McLellan and Ada 2004; Salbach et al 2004). Overall, the Cochrane review indicated a “modest” but not a sustained improvement in lower limb function following RTT, an associated positive impact on some aspects of ADL but no improvement in arm function (French et al, 2009). It is beyond the scope of this thesis to consider the novel, highly specific interventions such as CIT or TT as these interventions can only be provided for a limited population and may require intense resource support and are often only feasible in the hospital environment. In part, the limitations are due to the intensive delivery – daily or twice daily, and with CIT often requiring up to six hours daily of intensive task practice (Miltner et al 1999; Smith et al 2000; Sullivan et al 2002; Wolf et al 2006). These high resource demands are inconsistent with interventions aimed at more general and ongoing rehabilitation for people with chronic stroke. Studies included in this section therefore, are limited to considering the evidence supporting 37 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation community-based and low technology approaches to rehabilitation, more details are included in table 3.1. Search Strategy To identify relevant studies to support the work reported in this thesis, a search for English language papers was undertaken. Databases searched were Medline, CINAHL (the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature), the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Web of Science and PEDro (the Physiotherapy Evidence Database). Keywords included in the search strategy were combinations of stroke, cerebrovascular accident, CVA, hemiplegia hemiparesis, late-stage or chronic and combinations of physiotherapy, physical therapy, therapy, rehabilitation, home, home-based, domicillary, community, and combinations of exercise, exercise therapy, training, function, strength, activity, gait, balance, and upper extremity.. Dam et al (1993) undertook one of the earliest studies of stroke rehabilitation that indicated the capacity for positive change over two years for people with relatively severe stroke. The included sample had been discharged from rehabilitation, however the participants entered the study at around three months post- stroke and therefore, would not be regarded as being the chronic phase. The aim was to see if different manifestations or severity of stroke had potential for functional improvement over two years. Fifty one people who had sustained severe strokes, were not yet ambulant and were at least three months post-stroke were recruited. The two year study design was complex, the intervention consisted of providing up to seven bouts of rehabilitation - lasting one to three months, with at least one month rest between each treatment bout. Details of intervention were not clearly reported 38 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation but stated to be between 60 – 120 minutes of physiotherapy, five days a week. Outcomes were taken every three months during the first year and every six months during the second year using the Barthel Index and the gait, motor and total domain scores of the Hemiplegic Stroke Scale (HSS). Interestingly, the HSS was designed with 16 people with acute stroke (Adams et al 1987) and does not appear to have been established with more chronic stroke which may raise questions about the validity of some of the findings. Dam et al (1993) reported that after 12 months, all subjects had improved their neurological status and ability to perform basic ADL, but no further changes were demonstrated in the second year. There was no difference in improvements depending on side of stroke or whether the subjects had suffered an ischaemic or haemorrhagic stroke. While the work by Dam et al (1993) was quite innovative in that it was one of the first to study people with stroke for a prolonged period and demonstrate that recovery was measurable beyond the initial three month period post-stroke. The trial would be difficult to replicate however, as details of the intervention were not clear, the bouts of intervention were lengthy and resource intensive and probably not viable in an environment other than a research situation. The following sections (3.4.1 – 3.4.4.) will consider the evidence relating to community rehabilitation of people with late-stage stroke. On reviewing the literature in this area, the environment in which rehabilitation was conducted varied between the home, community settings such as a gym or community centre, or outpatient facilities. It is therefore difficult to make comparisons between studies based on the effect of environment. Trials in out-patient facilities that required 39 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation considerable amounts of highly technical equipment (such as isokinetic machines) that would not be transferable to the home setting have not been included. Trials that included a circuit of simple exercises that could be performed in the home environment (e.g. rising from a chair, heel raises, reaching exercises) have been included. The constant feature of the participants included in the studies that have been reviewed, is that all participants had been discharged from in-patient rehabilitation and were community dwelling. At the end of this section, summary findings will be identified and an indication of how the definitive trial reported in chapters seven and eight fits with the current literature will be proposed. 3.4.1. Studies of Mobility in Late-stage Stroke The studies included in this section predominantly investigated the effects of physiotherapy on mobility outcomes in late-stage stroke. Wall and Turnbull (1987) reported an early, small scale, non-randomised pre-post intervention study of gait re-education for people at least 18 months post-stroke. Twenty participants were allocated to one of four groups that practiced the same exercises either as two hours of out-patient physiotherapy weekly, two hours of home physiotherapy weekly, a mixed group with one hour each of out-patient and home physiotherapy or a control group. The intervention phase lasted six months. Gait parameters were measured monthly over a 10 month period. No statistically significant differences were found between any of the groups for any measurement point, although isolated points of improved gait speed were apparent within the outpatient group on one occasion and the mixed group on two occasions. The overall conclusion from this study was that the exercise programme was ineffective at 40 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation improving gait, however the small sample size and carefully selected sample may have affected the findings, or the intensity of intervention was insufficient. Wade et al (1992) investigated whether home-based physiotherapy intervention late after stroke was effective in improving mobility for people with chronic stroke. This study was novel in that the intervention consisted primarily of advice, goal setting and unsupervised exercise with an emphasis on self management and problem solving. A randomised cross over design was adopted with subjects either receiving early intervention (EI) following two baseline assessments or late intervention (LI), three months after baselines. A whole battery of outcome measures were recorded at baselines and then at three, six and nine months with a focus on mobility, ADL as well as a measure of manual dexterity to ensure that if changes in stroke impairment were found these were not generalised to upper limb function as well as mobility. On the 10m walk test both groups showed a reduction in walking time immediately after their intervention phase, which was not sustained at follow-up, but there were no statistically significant differences between the groups over time. More clinically relevant was that during the intervention phase seven subjects started walking outside. No significant changes were noted in non-mobility related outcomes. Weaknesses of this study were that the intervention therapist was known to be highly experienced in neurological rehabilitation and it may be that the findings would not be replicated by another individual, also there was a lack of follow up for a number of subjects so findings need to be viewed cautiously. However this was one of the first low intensity, home based rehabilitation studies to show positive outcomes and the potential for functional improvements for people with chronic stroke. 41 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Green et al (2002) undertook a large RCT of “community physiotherapy” in 170 people with chronic stroke and residual mobility problems, this work had been influenced by the work of Wade et al (1992) and aimed to overcome some of the weaknesses such as ensuring the study was adequately powered and intervention was delivered by an established service. Subjects were randomised to either a (maximum) 13 week “community physiotherapy” intervention or were allocated to the control group. All participants were followed up at three, six and nine months after randomisation. A small but significant improvement in Rivermead Mobility Index score was found for the Intervention group at three months, but this did not persist. No significant changes were found for the other measures: Barthel Index, Frenchay Activity Index or Hospital Anxiety and Depression Score. Overall, the effects were small and transitory and may have been attributable to insufficient intensity of treatment or a lack of focus on treatment for non-mobility related problems. McClellan and Ada (2004) undertook a small RCT in Australia investigating the effect of a six week home-based mobility exercise programme for people with stroke who had been discharged from physiotherapy. An innovative feature of their programme was that unlike previous studies that had looked at providing home based physiotherapy for people with chronic stroke, they made the argument that resources are not finite and therefore it was valid to investigate the efficacy of providing limited physiotherapy input (three contacts over six weeks). 26 subjects were randomised to receive either mobility exercises or “sham mobility” upper limb exercises. The authors noted that therapists might have reservations about leaving 42 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation patients to undertake unsupervised exercise in that they may practice incorrectly, be unsafe or there may be compliance issues, however strategies were developed to overcome these potential issues by drawing from a pre-determined list of standardised exercises for each individual, and videotaping correct performance of the exercise (McCLellan and Ada 2004). It was found that on average, participants practised exercises 75% of the time, but that after the intervention had ceased 87% of the subjects continued to practice. By the end of the intervention phase, a statistically significant increase in Functional Reach (which requires loading of the lower limbs and good balance) was found in the intervention group and this was maintained at 14 week follow up. No significant improvements between the groups were found in either walking performance or quality of life. This was a rigorously designed study, which included features that may be relevant to adopt within the UK National Health Service. The lack of statistical significance may be attributed to the fact that the measures were not sensitive enough to show change, that the intervention phase was too short or that the study was underpowered. Similar work investigating an experimental group undertaking a six week task oriented gait training intervention compared to a control group undertaking seated upper extremity work was reported by Salbach et al (2004). Participants with a mean time since stroke of over seven months were recruited over 33 months, which might indicate a problem with engaging interest of potential participants. The exercise programme was designed to strengthen the legs, improve balance and increase walking capacity. While positive effects for the intervention group were found on measures of balance and mobility, these were not found to be statistically significant compared to the control group. 43 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation An interesting small-scale study investigated the effects of a four week dual exercise programme on walking ability in 25 people with a reported mean time since stroke of over four years (Yang et al, 2007). The 13 subjects in the experimental group undertook 12 sessions of gait training with dual task demands of carrying, bouncing, manipulating or kicking balls of various diameters (up to 95cm) while the control group received no intervention. Statistically significant improvements for the experimental group for gait speed, cadence and stride length and time were reported, with walking speeds post intervention cited as within low – normal values of 0.85 – 1.15 m/s, (Yang et al, 2007). While the sample in this study were relatively young (mean age 59 years) and had a relatively high level of recovery with a degree of functional use of the hemiplegic upper extremity, this work continues to add to the body of evidence relating to the potential for improvement late after stroke, although no long-term follow-up was reported. Overall, with the exception of the work by Wall and Turnbull (1982), the studies that have investigated interventions to target mobility function in people with chronic stroke in the community setting, have found small and sometimes, sustained improvements in mobility or mobility related characteristics. 3.4.2. Studies of Strengthening in Chronic Stroke Until recently, many therapists subscribed to the view that resistance training and strengthening was detrimental to people with stroke (Bobath 1990; Davies 1985), however this view has been challenged by a number of authors (Saunders et al 44 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation 2004; Ada et al 2005; Carr and Shepherd 2003). In a systematic review of studies of strengthening interventions for people with both acute and chronic stroke, Ada et al (2005) concluded that strengthening was feasible for people with stroke without a detrimental effect on spasticity, although positive outcomes were more likely early after stroke than in chronic stroke. Further, by pooling data from similar studies they identified that an increase in strength could be accompanied by an improvement in activity levels. An overview of studies designed to investigate strengthening for people with chronic stroke are discussed below. In a small randomised trial of a class-based, four week circuit training for people with chronic stroke ( x 1.3 years), nine participants completed the study, the numbers were therefore low and the experimental group comprised only five people (Dean et al 2000). The experimental intervention comprised a circuit of 10 stations designed to improve lower limb strength and walking ability, while the control participants undertook upper limb tasks. Outcomes were taken at the end of intervention and at two month follow-up. The authors identified statistically significant improvements in a number of measures relating to gait speed and in force generation during standing up for the experimental group, but no changes in upper limb function for either the experimental or control groups. Although this was a small study, an interesting finding was that the positive changes found in the experimental group at the end of the four week programme for gait endurance, gait speed and the number of step ups on the Step Test, were maintained at two month follow-up. This finding indicates that the programme was successful in affecting learning. A small pre- post design study followed six chronic stroke patients all of whom had sustained a stroke at least 12 months prior to the study (Monger et al 2002). The 45 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation subjects in this study received a three week home-based exercise programme where a physiotherapist visited three times weekly and prescribed and progressed task specific training of step ups and sit to stand transitions designed to strengthen lower limb muscles. Subjects were also asked to exercise themselves on the days when they did not receive therapy. This study demonstrated an improvement on the Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) for five out of the six subjects and an improvement in walking speed for all subjects. Additional biomechanical analysis showed that the mean peak vertical ground reaction force during rising to stand occurred closer to the point of thighs off which was a shift towards more normal performance. The findings related to the improvements in sit to stand score on the MAS must be viewed with some caution, not only due to the small sample size but also it is unclear whether the training undertaken by the six subjects consisted of practising the precise criteria that are measured with the MAS. A small study of a community based, low intensity strengthening programme for people with chronic stroke has been reported recently (Cramp et al, 2010). Using an A-B-A design, 18 people with a mean time of seven months post-stroke, were recruited to attend a twice weekly, 60-90 minute exercise programme at a leisure centre for a maximum of 14 weeks. Simple strengthening exercises using everyday equipment such as chairs, steps and theraband formed part of the programme. To be included in analysis, participants had to complete a minimum of 16 sessions out of a possible 28 sessions (Cramp et al, 2010). Key findings from this work were that in addition to improvements in gait velocity, Berg Balance scores and ADL, statistically significant gains in strength in hip extensors, hip abductors, knee extensors and ankle dorsi and plantar- flexors were evident after training. Interestingly it was reported that the positive effect had been gained after eight 46 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation sessions of training and for the majority of measures, no further statistically significant gains were found after a further eight training sessions. While this study demonstrated the feasibility of an exercise programme within a leisure centre setting and positive results in relation to lower limb strength gains, the results need to be viewed cautiously given the small sample size, marked variability in the amount of training undertaken and a relatively recent mean time since stroke meaning that some participants were in a “natural” phase of recovery. Studies that have predominantly investigated strength training in chronic stroke have been limited by low numbers and often a lack of associated task training to maximise any functional gains, which could explain some of the equivocal findings. The work by Monger et al (2002) was relatively resource intensive and the findings would need to be replicated to lend any support to this approach. At this point therefore, while it is clear that strengthening is not detrimental to people with chronic stroke, there is no strong, generalisable evidence that strengthening programmes result in significant positive changes for people with chronic stroke. 3.4.3. Studies of mixed strengthening, conditioning and mobility in latestage stroke A number of studies have investigated the effects of community based rehabilitation programmes which have tended to have a dual focus of improving strength and conditioning or strength and mobility. Texiera –Salmera et al (1999) undertook a small scale, randomised crossover trial recruiting 13 people at least nine months post stroke. Six people received immediate and seven people received a delayed 10 week programme of strengthening and physical conditioning. Participants in this 47 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation study had relatively mild residual impairments in that they had to be able to ambulate for at least 15 minutes, and undertake activity for 45 minutes with rests. Supervised graded exercises included incremental walking, plus stepping or cycling and lower limb strength training. It is not clear if data were normally distributed, however parametric statistics were used throughout which may mean results should be interpreted with caution. Key findings showed some impressive and statistically significant improvements following the 10 week intervention programme. Gait speed improved from 0.77m/s to 0.99m/s (+ 28%), stair climbing rate from 51 to 68 stairs per minute (+37%), an increase in combined lower limb muscle strength in the hemiplegic leg from 192 Nm to 240 Nm (+42%) and a reduction in the Nottingham Health Profile Score from just under 10 to 3 (+78%). There were no significant changes noted in lower limb spasticity. The authors attributed these positive findings, in part, to the combination of strengthening with functional endurance training. Another small scale study recruited 19 people with chronic stroke (mean time 44 or 49 months) into a crossover trial with participants receiving either an immediate or a delayed 10 week low intensity exercise programme (Kim et al, 2004). An individual 10 week programme focusing on posture, gait, movement and ADL function was provided, although precise details of the intervention were not clear. It appears that a physiotherapist visited once a week for up to 60 minutes with participants expected to practice independently, although strategies to ensure this were not reported. Following the intervention phase, a non statistically significant trend towards improvement on the lower extremity component of the STREAM (Stroke Rehabilitation Assessment of Movement) scale was noted. With the lack of detail provided for this study it is not clear whether lack of significance was attributed to 48 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation the lack of power, low intensity intervention, compliance factors or a combination of these issues. Combs et al (2010) reported a series of A-B-A-A case studies on nine people with chronic stroke of more than six months duration, with immediate (within a week) and retention (five months post intervention) testing. The subjects were all fairly high functioning stroke survivors, mobile at “unlimited household walker” level and able to partially elevate the affected arm as well as grasp and release a cloth. Within these case studies, participants followed a highly intensive two week programme of at least three hours a day practice of walking, functional activities and strengthening over five days each week. The results demonstrated only small mean improvements in activity based outcomes, but larger mean improvements in participation measures of the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) domains of perceived recovery, physical ability and participation. Subjects in this study were also classified as high or low functioning depending on their Wolf Motor Function scores and it was found that those subjects classified as low functioning made greatest improvements. In a slightly larger study, Hartman-Maier et al (2007) investigated the effects of an intensive community group rehabilitation programme in 27 people at least 6 months post-stroke and compared post-intervention data with 56 controls. The authors concluded that while the level of disability did not improve in the Intervention group, the programme was beneficial in terms of increasing leisure activity and life satisfaction, although these data should be viewed with caution as detail for the community rehabilitation programme was not provided, so it is unclear what was 49 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation undertaken and some variables (Stroke Impact Scale) were only reported for the Intervention group. In a single blinded RCT, 48 people at least a year post-stroke and discharged from formal rehabilitation were recruited to either an experimental group (n=24) receiving a four week programme (12 sessions) of a simple 30 minute circuit class or a control group with no intervention (Yang et al, 2006). The circuit lasted for 30 minutes at each session and consisted of six work stations using everyday equipment (such as a chair) to allow participants to practice movements to improve lower limb strength. Muscle strength was assessed using a handheld dynamometer and demonstrated statistically significant differences between the groups with increases in all lower extremity groups tested in the intervention group. A similar finding was reported for measures of gait, a step test and the Timed Up and Go test (Yang et al, 2006). Interestingly, the gains in strength were associated with functional improvements. No information was available about long-term effects of the intervention however. Furthermore, there are some areas of uncertainty about aspects of this study, for example while it was noted that exercises were progressed and encouragement was provided, it is not clear whether this was standardised and the experience of the assessor in using the dynamometer was not reported which may affect the reliability of the findings. A similar intensity of intervention of 12 thirty minute circuit sessions over four weeks was conducted in an RCT with 58 people at least six months post-stroke (median 3.9 years) in New Zealand (Mudge et al 2009). The intervention group (n=31) again undertook simple exercises designed to improve gait, balance and strength while 50 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation the control group (n=27) undertook group educational or social sessions. Measures of gait, step count and activity were taken at the end of intervention and at a three month follow-up. Small statistically significant improvements in gait endurance were found at the end of intervention for the intervention group and for gait speed and self reported gait ability at the end of the follow-up period. These findings however were measured in the clinic setting and improvements did not translate to an increase in reported community ambulation or step count. While the work was conducted in a private rehabilitation clinic, the input was such that it could be relocated to a home setting and it was noted that the participants wanted to continue with the exercise circuit following cessation of the trial (Mudge et al, 2009). A non-blind, randomised controlled trial (RCT) investigated the effects of a ten week supervised out-patient exercise programme versus an unsupervised home programme (one week of supervised exercise followed by nine weeks of unsupervised) aimed at improving strength and conditioning in 72 ambulant, chronic stroke survivors (Olney et al 2006). This study found modest improvements in walking speed for both groups but no gains in muscle strength. Interestingly, while no short term physical effects were found in the unsupervised group, at one year follow-up a significant improvement in the Human Activity Profile was demonstrated, which may suggest that undertaking unsupervised exercise had positively impacted on activity levels. Stuart et al (2009) undertook a pragmatic, non-blinded trial of community based exercise with people with chronic stroke. Participants were recruited from three geographical regions in Italy, with those from one region receiving intervention 51 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation (n=49) and those from the two other regions acting as controls and receiving “usual care”. The intervention group received a 13 week programme of three times weekly exercise classes concentrating on strength, balance and walking rehabilitation, with a progressive increase in walking time from six to 15 minutes during the course of the study. Mean change scores were reported. between baseline and six month follow-up and showed that the participants in the intervention group showed statistically significant improvement on all measures of gait, balance and ADL over the six month period, while the control group showed a slight deterioration in many of the measures including gait velocity, Berg Balance Scale, Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) scores and self-rated participation on the SIS. Between groups there was a significant difference, in favour of the intervention group, for many of the mobility and balance outcomes but not ADL (Barthel) or the self-rated domain of mobility on the Stroke Impact Scale. While these changes are noted, the findings must be viewed with caution due to the non-randomised nature of the trial and the fact that the outcome assessor was not blinded to group allocation which may well have confounded findings. Furthermore, some of the changes such as gait velocity, were very small e.g. 0.07m/s gain in gait speed over six months, which clinically may not manifest in any noticeable change in performance. In a rigorously designed RCT the effect of a 12 week programme of exercise or relaxation classes on 66 people with stroke was investigated (Mead et al; 2007). Although the median time since stroke to starting the intervention was six months, some subjects were recruited at less than three months post-stroke, so the sample was a mix of sub-acute and chronic stroke, albeit all were no longer receiving rehabilitation. The intervention consisted of a one hour mixed exercise class, with 52 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation exercises progressed every two to four weeks, or a relaxation class for control subjects as well as a “tea and chat” social component at the end of each class for all subjects. Measures of impairment, activity and participation were taken at baseline, end of intervention and at a four month follow up. Data were transformed to allow parametric analysis by Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) which allowed comparison of performance between groups at end of intervention and follow-up controlling for baseline levels. While both groups improved on a number of measures within their group over time, there was only a difference between the groups for Timed Up and Go, walking velocity and the physical domain of the SF36 at the end of intervention in favour of the exercise group, with only the SF36 score improvement being retained at the four month follow up. While there are a number of strengths to this study including the clearly described interventions and attention to progression as well as accounting for confounders such as social interaction, there were some limitations – most notably that many of the participants may have been considered to be in the “natural” improvement six month window following stroke as well as the high functional level (eg independently ambulant) of the participants. In an investigation of the effects of two different community based exercise programmes, Marigold et al (2005) measured balance, mobility and number of falls in 61 people with chronic stroke. The subjects in this study were functioning at a relatively high level as they not only had to travel to a setting outwith the home, but also had to tolerate activity for an hour. There was however, about 20% drop out with only 48 subjects completing post-intervention assessments and 42 available for one month follow up measures. The study was well controlled with subjects randomised either to receive a 10 week programme of three times weekly one hour 53 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation exercise classes either focussing on stretching and weight shift exercises such as tai chi movements or reaching tasks, or on increasingly demanding balance and mobility tasks such as altering the base of support, increasing the height of obstacles. In addition to a battery of outcome measures, subjects kept a falls diary over a 12 month period from recruitment into the study. It was found that subjects adhered well to the exercise programme with over 92% attendance for both groups. Both groups showed improvements in step reactions times, lower limb muscle activation, timed up and go, reduction in number of falls over 12 months and the Nottingham Health Profile, however only step reaction time, muscle activation and number of falls were significantly improved in the agility compared to the stretching group. The effect of the group, as well as the content of intervention, may well have had an impact on the findings in this study. A number of smaller scale case studies or pre-post design studies have been undertaken which, while they can be perceived as less robust in terms of being able to generalise findings, may show some interesting information that may inform future work. Twenty years ago, Tangeman et al (1990) reported a pre- post trial of rehabilitation aimed at improving balance and function. Forty subjects with chronic stroke recruited to this study underwent four weeks of intensive (two hours daily) rehabilitation aimed at improving function and balance. The detail of the intervention is not fully described but does identify that the rehabilitation consisted of one hour each of Physiotherapy and Occupational Therapy. Disappointingly, given the time at which this work was undertaken, results were measured using non-validated, non-standardised measures of balance, weight shift and activities of daily living. Therefore, while the authors reported that there were significant improvements in weight shift ability, balance and ADL between the start and end of the programme, 54 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation any claims have to be viewed with caution as the measures did not have established reliability or validity. Additionally this work can not be compared rigorously to any other published studies with a similar focus on balance and functional ability in chronic stroke. In a small pre- post design study, six people at least one year post-stroke undertook a three week programme of task specific exercises at home (Monger et al 2002). Participants practised 30 repetitions of rising to stand (RTS) and step ups daily and received nine visits from a therapist. Measures of rise to stand and walking speed showed statistically significant improvements at post testing. The improvement in RTS was impressive with gains of two or more points on the Motor Assessement Scale RTS item for all but one subject. However, the lack of a retention test does not allow comment to be made as to whether the changes were transient or sustained as learnt behaviour over a longer term. While findings from small scale studies can not be generalised to the wider stroke population, these studies can be useful in determining whether further work might be appropriate. Useful information can also be gleaned in relation to trial design, composition of interventions and relevant outcomes to be used for future studies. A body of evidence relating to small gains in selected gait paramenters and muscle strength is becoming apparent following short, four week, interventions of simple mixed programmes of strengthening, balance and gait training for people with chronic stroke. Interestingly, it appears that the duration of intervention of an appropriate input, does not require to be protracted in order to show some positive impact for people with chronic stroke. Many of the studies however, failed to 55 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation consider the long-term effects of the intervention and this is an area that requires to be included in rehabilitation for people with chronic stroke. 3.4.4 Studies of upper limb rehabilitation in late-stage stroke It is estimated that around 80% of people who have a stroke have some degree of upper extremity impairment (Nakayama et al 1994; Parker et al 1986). In comparison to recovery of lower limb function and mobility, functional recovery of the arm is often incomplete or completely absent and this feature will often have a negative impact on the ability to undertake activities of daily living (ADL) (Wade et al 1983; Parker et al 1986; Heller et al 1987, Feys et al 1998). Interventions such as Constraint Induced Therapy (CIT) or Functional Electrical Stimulation (FES) will not be considered in this section, as the focus of this review will continue to be on nontechnological interventions that are suitable for undertaking in the community setting. Some studies of mixed UL and LL interventions have been considered in section 3.3, therefore this section includes only studies focusing on community based UL interventions. A 19 week group exercise programme concentrating on either upper limb (UL) or lower limb (LL) exercises was reported by Pang et al (2006). The group nature of the programme was advocated as a means of increasing accessibility to ongoing rehabilitation. 63 community dwelling people with late stage stroke of more than one year were matched, by gender, prior to randomisation. The study had originally been designed to investigate a lower limb groups exercise programme on cardiovascular fitness (with the UL group presumably acting as control subjects) and 56 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation therefore participants in this study were required to be at a relatively high level of function as they were required to pedal a cycle ergometer at 60rpm. The UL intervention consisted of shoulder exercises using theraband of increasing resistance; hand and arm strengthening, hand and arm mobilising, hand and arm weight bearing and UL functional activities. Measures of functional activity, arm recovery, grip strength and arm use were taken at baseline and at the end of the 19 week intervention. Three of the original participants dropped out of the study – two because of the time commitment and one who found the exercises too fatiguing. Analaysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) showed statistically significant improvements in the UL group on the Wolf Motor Function Test and the UL section of the FMA compared to the LL group. There were no statistically significant differences in grip strength or Motor Activity Log scores. While limited detail of the intervention was provided in the paper, an exercise log book was completed for each patient. This log book may have had a motivating effect on the cohort which may have influenced findings. One of the key weaknesses of the study is that the inclusion criteria were heavily weighted towards fitness levels and lower limb ability (cycle ergometer). If the study had required as stringent UL inclusion criteria, there may have been further improvements, for example 10 of the subjects were classified as “severe” UL paresis, and some participants (number not stated) required electrical stimulation to activate wrist extension which potentially impacted on the ability to grip the dynamometer required for measuring grip strength. Furthermore, no long-term follow up data were given, therefore it is not clear whether any of the changes in performance were sustained. Another large study of 91 people with late-stage stroke was reported by Higgins et al (2006). Participants were randomised to receive arm or mobility training and 57 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation attended eighteen 90 minute treatment sessions over six weeks. The intervention was tailored to the individual, and the UL group used everyday objects while the mobility group concentrated on balance and speed during walking. All participants undertook a supplementary home exercise programme. Results were reported for UL measures and while there were some clinical improvements, with an increase for the number of blocks moved, no statistically significant changes were found on any outcomes despite the intensive and individualised training. It could be argued that the intervention may have been too individualised and that intensive practice on tasks with a high degree of standardisation could be more applicable for an RCT where a proportion of the participants had severe UL impairments. In a study of task specific training undertaken in the participant’s home, Michaelsen et al (2006) recruited 30 people between six months and four years after stroke. Randomisation ensured that equal numbers of participants with mild or moderatesevere UL impairment were allocated to each group. A therapist supervised a programme of reach and grasp, task specific exercises, undertaken for one hour, three times a week over five weeks. Both intervention and control groups undertook the same exercises, with the intervention group being restrained by a “seat-belt” during the task to prevent concurrent trunk movements. Practice followed motor learning principles and included functionally relevant, repetitive practice of reach to grasp movements with target objects of varying size, shape and weight. Functional UL outcomes and kinematic measurements of the trunk and elbow during reaching were taken at baseline, end of intervention and at a one month follow up. It was found that both groups improved their function and demonstrated reduced UL impairment, however this improvement was only statistically significant for the intervention group. There were no significant differences in trunk displacement 58 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation during reaching, the intervention group however showed a significant increase in elbow extension during reaching compared to the control group. Further analysis showed that the mildly impaired control subjects made the least improvements and that for mildly impaired intervention subjects, trunk restraint provided no additional benefits . While this study might be considered peripheral to the community based home exercise interventions that may inform the current study, it did demonstrate the capacity for improvement in UL function in people at least six months after stroke. However, it required considerable therapist contact, and artificially restrained a movement component which detracts from the general uptake of this type of intervention. In a small study of 12 people at least five months post-stroke, Thielman et al (2004) investigated the relative merits of task specific training compared to resistance training of reaching. Participants were classed as “low” or “high” level of arm function depending on upper limb scores on the Motor Assessment Scale. All participants practiced movements to similar targets. Approximately 150 – 180 repetitions of each exercise was undertaken in each exercise session A kinematic analysis demonstrated some modest improvements in upper limb trajectories during reaching, including improvements in participants classified as low level. It is not clear whether improvements were sustained as no follow-up was undertaken. Once again, this small study demonstrates the capacity for improvement late after stroke, even in people with poor function at start of the study. 3.4.5 Summary points from studies of late-stage stroke rehabilitation 59 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Many of the studies reviewed in this chapter indicate the potential for improvements for people with chronic stroke following rehabilitation. However, improvements were often small (e.g. Higgins et al 2006) and sometimes transitory (e.g. Olney et al 2006). Furthermore, in some of the studies changes were not necessarily limited to the intervention group which raised the question of a Hawthorne effect. Many different methodologies were employed and this makes cross study comparison difficult. Sample sizes were highly variable and ranged from six (Monger et al 2002) to 170 (Green et al 2002), with five studies reporting group sizes of <20, which would limit generalisation. The interventions were often not well defined and therefore would make replication difficult. Furthermore, the amount of intervention was highly variable and low intensity interventions may have confounded the possibility of demonstrating positive effects. In table 3.1. the myriad of outcome measures used in studies of community-based interventions for people with late-stage stroke are identified. This makes comparisons between studies difficult. To further compound this issue, there were no standardised outcome measurement schedules. One of the biggest weaknesses was the lack of retention measures. This meant that any changes identified at one time point, may have been either a reflection of permanent change in ability as a result of learning, or it could have been a transient change in performance that would not be sustained. The number of times that outcome measurements were taken also varied, from two in pre-post designs (such as Texeira-Salmela et al 1999), to ten repeated monthly outcome measures (Wall and Turnbull 1987). One of the dangers of many repeated outcome measures is that changes could be due to 60 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation a practice effect on the measure (improving performance on the measure) rather than an improvement in the characteristic of interest. At the time of planning the current study (1999 and 2000) only limited evidence relating to the efficacy of later-stage stroke rehabilitation was available (Wade et al 1992, Dam et al 1993, and Texeira-Salmela et al 1999). Thus the evidence at that time indicated large gaps in the literature and therefore great potential for investigating further the impact of a clearly defined physiotherapy exercise programme for people with chronic stroke. 61 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke EVIDENCE FOR LONG-TERM REHABILITATION Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Time since stroke Intervention Main findings Wall & Turnbull 1987 n = 20 allocated to 4 groups range 45-70 Range 1.5 – 10 years x 65.6 (+ 10.9) x 3.1 yrs (+ 3.9) Wade 1992 Randomised cross over Chronic (>12 months poststroke) n = 40 Indep ambulant Chronic (> 1 yr post-stroke) Early int n =49 Late int n = 45 6 months Ix A - 2 hrs per week OP exs B – 2 hrs week home exs C – 1 hr home /1 hr OP exs D - control 4 week treatment = 2 hrs/day x 4 days /wk (focused on balance and functional activities) No difference between groups on gait parameters Tangeman 1990 Nonrandomised Repeated measures pre-post Pre-post design EI: x 72.3 (+ 9.7) EI: x 53.1 mth (+29.5) 10mwt(s) EI -3.9s; +6.5s; -1.4s LI +6.4s; -3.9s; +2.6s LI: x 72.0 (+ 10.6) LI: x 59.6 mth (+35.3) Both groups received 6 wks physiotherapy (between 1 -6 visits) either after baseline assessments (EI) or after a 3 month delay (LI). Followup at 9 months Complex – up to 7 consecutive “bouts” of daily therapy lasting 1 – 3 months every 3 months, with at least 1 month between “treatment bouts” Both groups: 10 wk programme (3x / wk) of graded strength and physical conditioning I: immed C: delayed 10 wks Dam 1993 Nonrandomised Sub-acute / chronic. Non-ambulant >3 mths post-stroke n=51 x 66.8 (+ 10.1) > 3 months TeixeiraSalmela 1999 Randomised pre- post test trial with delayed intervention for C group Chronic (>9 months poststroke) I: n= 6 C: n=7 I: x 65.9 (+ 10.2) C: x 69.4 (+ 8.9) I: x 9.2 (+ 12.7) C: x 6.4 yrs (+ 6.2) 2 baselines (1 mth apart), end intervention, 3 month follow-up Non-validated measures of weight shift, balance and ADL At 12 months BI - by 65% with two thirds attaining good level of ADL (BI > 70) HSS – total by 25% HSS – gait by 42% HSS – motor by 13% Statistically significant improvements (p>0.007) in gait speed by 28% adjusted activity score 39.2% rate of stair climbing 37.4% NHP of 77.8% affected leg strength 42.3% 62 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Time since stroke Intervention Main findings Dean 2000 Matched pairs randomised design I x : 66.2 (+ 7.7) C x : 62.3 (+ 6.6) I x : 2.3 yrs (+ 0.7) C x : 1.3 yrs (+ 0.9) RCT I x : 71.5 (+ 8.7) C x : 73.5 (+ 8.3) I: >12 mths C: >12 mths I = 1 hr circuit class ( ↑ functional performance and endurance of LE tasks) over 4 weeks C = 1 hr circuit class ( ↑ functional performance and endurance of UE tasks) 3x week over 4 weeks I: max 13 weeks with min 3 contacts by community physiotherapist at home or as OP C: no intervention At end of intervention and two month follow up Experimental group demonstrated Gait speed Gait endurance Force production through affected LL when standing up Step Test Green 2002 Sub-acute / chronic. Convenience sample n=12 >3 months poststroke no longer receiving rehabilitation Chronic >12 mths 170 I: n= 85 C: n=85 Monger 2002 Pre-post design Chronic >12 mths n=6 x 65 (+ 5 x 3.6 yrs (+ 2.9) 3 week task specific training of step-ups and sit-to-stand transitions Kim 2004 Randomised crossover design I 1 x : 61.4 (+ 11.2) I2 x : 62.8 (+ 9.4) I 1 x : 44mth (+ 29.6) I2 x : 49.2 (+ 31.6) 10 weeks of once weekly 60 min home programme of posture, gait, ADL and motor exs McClellan and Ada 2004 RCT to improve mobility; home ex with minimal supervision N = 19 I1 (n=9) = immed home PT I2 (n=10)= delayed home PT I n=13; C n=10 I: x 69 (+ 13) C: x 72 (+ 9) I median 6.5mth (IQR 5.5mth) C median 4.5mth (IQR 3mth) I: 6 weeks home practice of mobility exs taught in hospital environment C: 6 weeks home practice of UL exs No changes in control group Outcomes at baseline, 3, 6 and 9 months RMI Gait speed over 10m BI FAI HADS Pre- post- intervention MAS (standing up) in 5 /6 participants 10mwt: in walking speed in all subjects Grip strength: no change Both groups showed significant in STREAM LE after intervention phase Baseline, end intervention (6 wk), 2 month follow up (14wk) FRT: significantly more in I at both 8 and 14 wks follow up MAS (walking): in both gps, no difference SA-SIP30: no significant changes over time in either group 63 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Salbach 2004 RCT stratified by initial walking speed I: n = 44 C: n = 47 I: Thielman 2004 Matched pairs, prepost-design Chronic ( > 5 mths) N = 12 Marigold 2005 RCT of agility exs vs stretching/ weight shifting RCT Higgins 2006 Michaelsen 2006 Randomised pre-post design Time since stroke Intervention Main findings I: x 7.9 mth (+ 2.9) C: x 7.2 mth (+ 2.4) I: 6 weeks (3x / week) circuit of balance, leg strength and gait exs C: seated arm exs At end of intervention 6mwt - 40m (I) compared to 5m (C) Gait speed 0.14m/s (I)compared to 0.03m/s (C) TUG 1.2 s (I) compared to 1.7s (C) Range 54 – 83 5 – 19 months Smoothing of trajectory of arm kinematics Improved trunk movement during reaching Chronic>12mths post-stroke n=61 I(agility) n= 30 I(stretch) n=31 activity tolerance @ 60 mins I(agility) x : 68.1 (+9.0) I(stretch) x : 67.5 (+ 7.2) I(agility) 4 weeks of 35 mins intervention 3x weekly Task Training vs Strengthen 10 week group exercises, 3x weekly for 1 hour per class Agility: progressively difficult balance tasks Stretch: slow, low impact tai chi and reaching exercises Late-stage N = 91 I(upper limb) I (mobility) Chronic between 6-48 months post I n= 15 C n = 15 I(UL) x : 73 I(mob) x : 71 I(UL) 3x week over 6 weeks 90 minutes each session Individual functional task practice 5 weeks = 1 hour, 3x per week Task specific reach and grasp exercises with (Int) or without (con) trunk restraint in number of blocks moved on Box and block test No significant findings C: x x 71 (+ 12) 73 (+ 8) x : 3.6 yrs (+1.0) I(stretch) x : 3.8 yrs (+ 2.4) x : 7mths I(mob) x : 8 mths x 68.9 (+ 10.3 years) x 16.7 (+ 9.1) mths Outcomes at baseline, end intervention and 1 month retention BBS - no significant difference between groups TUG – trend towards improvement in agility group Step reaction time – significant improvement in agility group No. of falls – reduced by half in the agility group Outcomes at baseline, end intervention and one month follow up. I - TEMPA Functional improvement I - FMA reduced impairment I - Kinematic reduced compensatory trunk movement and improved elbow extension during reach 64 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Time since stroke Intervention Main findings Yang 2006 RCT of strengthening circuit vs no intervention Non-blind RCT: supervised vs nonsupervised ex Single blind RCT UL vs LL (control) training Comparison of I data to an preexisting data set (C) Chronic I: n=24 C: n=24 I x : 56.8 C x : 60 I: x 64.4mths C: x 62.7mths I: Gait speed , 6mwt, TUG I: strength hip, knee and ankle flex and extensors Chronic Sup ex: n=37 Non-sup n=37 Sup x 63.5 (+ 12) NSup ẋ65.8 (+11.6) ẋ4.1yrs (+ 4.4) ẋ3.4yrs (+3.9) Chronic I: n = 27 C: n=56 I x : 61.59 C x : 57.7 I: 35.2mths C: 11.7mths I = 4 weeks (12 sessions) 30 minute circuit of 6 LE strength exs C = no intervention reported Sup= 1.5hrs sessions, 3x weekly over 10 wks NSup = same as S wk1 followed by 9 wks Exs include stretches, aerobic ex and strength Group programme 3x week 1 hr exercise over 19 weeks UL gp – exs for UL, strength and UL functional tasks LL gp exs for mob and bal I= group community rehabilitation 2-4days a week (not stated how long) C = no group rehab RCT with blinded outcome assessor Chronic I: n=13 C: n = 12 I: x 59.5 (+ 11.8) I: Olney 2006 Pang 2006 HartmanMaier (2007) Yang 2007 Chronic UL gp n=30 LL gp n=30 UL LL C: x =64.9 x =66 x 59.2 (+ 12) UL LL x =5.1 x =5.2 x 4.1 (+ 3.1) C: x 4.7 yr (+ 7.4) I: 12 x 30 minutes sessions over 4 weeks of ball manipulation, bouncing and kicking C: no intervention Overall modest physical gains 6minwt: Both S and NS signif at post, 6 mths and 1 year HAP: cont in NSup gp to be signif at 1 yr, non sigat 10 wks but then decline No change in strength over time in either gp PCI: Sig in NSup gp only at 1 yr. Overall UL group – stat significant ↑ in UL functional ability : WMFT FMA FIM – Both groups dependent I worse than C (P=0.004) L-ADL – no significant difference between groups ACS – only reported for I group significant activity pre-post intervention, but C group more active at baseline LSQ – significantly more satisfaction with life as a whole and leisure for I group. SIS – only administered to I group At end intervention statistically significant in, gait speed by 0.3m/s, cadence, stride time and stride length for I group. 65 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Time since stroke Intervention Main findings Mead 2007 RCT of exercise vs. relaxation Sub-acute / Chronic I: n= 32 C: n= 34 I: x 72 (+ 10.4) C: x 71.7 (+ 9.6) I: median 178 days (IQR 86 – 307) C: median 161.5 days (IQR 91.8 – 242.8) 12 weeks group intervention 3x wk in rehabilitation hospital I: ex group including endurance and prog resistance training C: group relaxation Mudge 2009 RCT with blinded outcome assessor Chronic I: n = 31 C n = 27 I: median 76 C: median 71 I : median 3.3 yrs C: median 5.8 yrs I: 12 x 30 minutes circuit over 4 weeks, circuit 15 stations – targeting strength, balance and walking C: social and educational sessions Outcomes at baseline, end of intervention (3 months) and long term retention (7 months) I (3 months): on SF36; STS time; TUG; HADS; bilateral leg extensor strength; FRT and walking economy I (7 months): maintained in leg strength, HADS and STStime. C (3months) walking speed; SF36 (mental health domain), extensor strength of unaffected leg. These were maintained at C (7months) Gait endurance - statistically significant at end intervention for Intervention group, Gait speed statistically significant at 3 month follow up for I group. RMI statistically significant at 3 month follow up for I group. Stuart 2009 Nonrandomised trial Chronic (>9 months poststroke) with mild to mod gait impairment I: n=49 C: n=44 I: x 66.8 (+ 1.4) C: x 70 (+ 1.7) I: x 4.2yrs (+ 0.8) C: x 3.5 yrs (+ I: 3x week 1 hr exercise (walk, strength, balance) for 13 weeks C: “usual care” Nb non-blind outcome assessor 0.5) Overall small physical gains in I group but also some gains for C subjects over study period Gait velocity over 6minwt I: by -0.07m/s; C:↓ by 0.05m/s MI (0-100) I: by 7.4; C: ↓ by -2.1 SPPB (0-12) I: by 1.58; C: ↓ by -0.84 BBS (0-54) : I: by 5.1; C: ↓ -1.5 BI (0-100) : I: by 3.9; C: ↑ by 0.7 I = Various in SIS communication, SIS mobility and SIS participation and for control in same domains 66 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Authors Design Sample Age (yrs) Time since stroke Intervention Main findings Combs (2010) 9 case Studies Ambulant; able to pick up flannel with affected hand x 58.1 (+ 12.5) x 6.5 yrs (+ 5.8yrs) Small mean in activity measures WMF, BBS, TUG, 6minwt with small effect sizes. mean in participation measures SIS and COPM with 5 subjects reporting minimal clinically important differences (10-15 points) on SIS Cramp 2010 Repeated measure A–B–A design N = 18 Sub-acute to chronic (between 3 – 12 months post – stroke) x x A1-B-A2-A3 design (A1: pre; A2: 1 wk post; A3: 5mths post) 3 hrs daily, 5 days per week over 2 weeks, - 7 exercises daily from bank of over 50 individual exs plus 30mins home ex A1 – 4 weeks B - 14 weeks, twice weekly exs (60-90mins); low intensity, no special equipment A2 – 5-10wk follow-up Table 3.1 65 (+ 2) time 7 months Outcomes after 8 interventions (n=17) and after 16 (n=15) in LE strength after 8 and 16 interventions in Gait velocity, after 8 interventions 6mwt after 16 interventions Berg balance after 16 interventions in Nottingham ADL after 16 interventions Summary of studies of community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke (arranged chronologically) 67 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Table 3.1 Summary of Community based exercise programmes for people with late-stage stroke Key for table 3.1 Abbreviation Name of Outcome Measure ABC Scale ACS BI BBS COPM FAI FIM FIM motor FRT HADS HAP HSS Lawton IADL LLFDI LSQ MAS MI NHP PCI RMI SA-SIP30 SF36 SPPB SIS Strength STS time TEMPA TUG WMFT 6mwt 10mwt Activities-Specific Balance Scale Activity Card Sort Barthel Index Berg Balance Scale Canadian Occupational Performance Measure Frenchay Activity Index Functional Independence Measure FIM Motor (for Basic ADL) Functional Reach Test Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Human Activity Profile Hemiplegic Stroke Scale (Graded Neurological Scale) Lawton Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Later Life Function and Disability Instrument Life Satisfaction Questionnaire Motor Assessment Scale Motricity Index Nottingham Health Profile Physiological Cost Index Rivermead mobility Index Stroke Adapted Sickness Impact Profile Short Form 36 Short Physical Performance battery Stroke Impact Scale Muscle Strength Time to stand Up Test of Upper Extremity Performance Timed Up and Go Wolf Motor Function Six min walk 10 metre walk test Possible Score 0-100 0-20 or 0-100 0 - 54 0 - 45 18 - 126 (pos score 13-91) distance 0 - 21 0-23 0-48 0 - 100 1 - 15 0 - 100 0 - 12 0 - 100 Nm Timed 0-39 Timed Timed Timed or gait speed 68 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation 3.5 Services available to people with stroke following cessation of formal Rehabilitation This section considers services available to people with chronic stroke once discharged from hospital rehabilitation services. In large part, this is dependent on geographical location and healthcare funding, however there is no doubt that with increasing healthcare costs and reduction in healthcare budgets and resources, services will be reduced. Guidelines for stroke rehabilitation and ongoing care exist in the United Kingdom (SIGN 2010; RCP 2008). While these guidelines are not directives, they indicate best available evidence-based practice and have been developed, in part, to improve what has been reported as insufficient and poorly coordinated services (Pound et al 1994; Pound et al, 1995; LeWinter and Mikkelsen 1995). Tyson and Turner (2000), conducted an audit to assess the services available to people with stroke, six weeks after hospital discharge, as well as a survey of how these services were viewed by the patients. While most people received a home visit, this was limited to assessment of basic care and more intricate tasks (such as using the telephone) were not tackled, the survey of patients also revealed frustration with promised services that did not materialise (such as respite care, ongoing therapy or meals on wheels). In relation to ongoing therapy, while around half of the sample was referred on for continuing rehabilitation, not all of them received this service. A tension was identified between patients who were dissatisfied at the lack of available therapy and the rehabilitation staff who felt that not only was there a lack of resources to provide ongoing rehabilitation, but also a lack of belief in the benefits of ongoing rehabilitation. 69 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation Green et al (1999) looked more specifically at community physiotherapy provision for people at least a year post-stroke in Bradford, UK. Over a one year period, 83 referrals for physiotherapy were received which represented just 3% of the service referrals for that period and treatments mostly focussed on mobility, balance and “exercise therapy”. The number of treatment interventions was limited with modal contacts being one session. Questions were raised by the authors as to whether the low referral rate reflected genuine need or a reflection of opportunistic referral, however the relatively low workload for community staff was highlighted. 3.6 Summary This chapter has considered evidence relating to physiotherapy and stroke rehabilitation with an emphasis on the evidence to support physiotherapy and latestage stroke. It is clear that there is potential for improvement in functional status for community dwelling people with late-stage stroke, however it is not clear what input will lead to the most effective outcomes. Further research into the long-term effectiveness of physiotherapy in large, well controlled, randomised trials outwith the hospital environment is clearly required. 70 Chapter 3 Stroke Rehabilitation 4. MOTOR LEARNING THEORY AND REHABILITATION 4.1 Introduction In this chapter, consideration will be given to learning of different types of motor skills, theories of motor learning, and how practice sessions can be structured. Much of the formative work in the field of Motor Learning derives from psychologists, sport and exercise scientists, developing practice situations in which a new skill can be best learnt (e.g. factory line assembly tasks) or how to refine an existing skill in order to improve sporting performance. Therefore, much of the evidence derives from work undertaken with subjects with an intact neurological system, in particular young college students. In physiotherapy the findings from the field of motor learning have been applied, with little or no empirical testing to rehabilitation practice in people with neurological impairments. Throughout this chapter, the background from which evidence has been developed will be made clear. Motor Learning focuses on understanding how movement is acquired and how movement is modified (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). This description differentiates Motor Learning from the concept of Motor Control which is concerned with the ability to regulate movement. Clearly Motor Control and Motor Learning are intricately linked as it is not possible to acquire movement without learning how to regulate it. Motor Learning can not be observed but may be inferred from performance. Some key terms require definition from the outset and these terms are identified below. Motor Learning has been defined as: “a set of processes associated with practice or experience leading to relatively permanent changes in the capability for producing skilled action” (Schmidt and Lee 2005 p302; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007 p22) 71 Chapter 4 Motor Learning This definition identifies that important elements to consider in Motor Learning are practice or experience of movement and the resultant ability to generate skilled action. One critical issue when considering Motor Learning is the importance of distinguishing between the potentially transient features of performance of a skill, with the longer lasting or relatively permanent changes associated with learning. Performance is generally considered as undertaking a specific skill, in a specific context at a specific time. As performance improves, then variability of action when undertaking the skill will reduce as performance becomes refined (Magill 2001; Schmidt and Lee 2005). Learning, on the other hand, is considered a relatively permanent change in the ability of an individual to perform a skill (Schmidt and Lee 2005; Shumway Cook and Woolacott 2007). If therefore, a physiotherapist works with a stroke patient to improve their ability to stand up and by the end of the treatment session the patient is able to stand up independently that can be considered a change in performance. If the patient then maintains that ability to consistently stand up independently the next day and the next week, then this consistent performance will be indicative that learning has occurred. In terms of rehabilitation for people with stroke who were previously able to perform skilled actions, the learning of a “lost” or “impaired” motor skill has been described variously as “motor relearning” (Carr and Shepherd 1980) or “recovery of function” or “reacquisition of movement” Hochstenbach and Mulder 1999). (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Studies of motor learning have generally been designed with two phases – an initial practice or “acquisition” phase to ascertain performance and a subsequent retention phase or test. The purpose of taking a retention test is to ensure the diffusion of any performance enhancing factors (such 72 Chapter 4 Motor Learning as feedback) or performance degrading factors (such as fatigue) that may have impacted on the learning situation. A general aim in stroke rehabilitation is to promote as optimal a return to function as possible. For people with stroke therefore, there is a need to practice movement skills in relation to everyday tasks. Stroke patients need to practice both with supervision as in therapy and without supervision which is highly applicable to later stages post-stroke. There is a tension here however, while chapter three identified the emerging literature to identify the potential for people with chronic stroke to make positive improvement in motor functions with some targeted rehabilitation interventions, little cognisance has been given to the optimal practice regimes for relearning motor skills. This chapter therefore aims to summarise current knowledge and understanding of Motor Learning. 4.2 Types of Motor Skill to be relearnt A motor skill has been defined as “an activity or task that has a specific purpose or goal to achieve” (Magill 2007 p 5). In the motor learning literature, the terms “skills”, “motor skills”, “actions” and “movement” can be used interchangeably (Magill 2007). While it has been argued that “movement” can be argued to represent specific “behavioural characteristics” of body parts that are the component parts of a motor skill, (Magill 2007), the terms will be used interchangeably throughout the chapter. Motor skills have been classified in a number of different ways in order to enhance communication about the demands of each task, to structure rehabilitation and to enhance research. These classifications are briefly considered below. 73 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.2.1 Open and Closed Motor Skills When looking at a skill in an environmental context, skills have been classified on a continuum from being performed in a relatively stable and highly predictable environment – a “closed skill” - to those performed in an unstable environment with random factors that may affect performance – an “open skill” (Carr and Shepherd 2003; Magill 2007; Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008). Most motor skills lie somewhere inbetween the two extreme endpoints. It may well be that in stroke rehabilitation, the initial focus of motor skill relearning may be on closed skills such as standing up from a standard height chair in a gym setting with no time pressures and minimal or no environmental distractions with the aim to progress a patient with a high level of recovery to performing the skill in a less predictable, unstable environment such as standing up from a bus seat on a moving bus. 4.2.2 Discrete, Serial and Continuous Skills When learning a new skill, or re-learning skills as in rehabilitation, the trainer or therapist needs to consider the demands of the task. For example a key characteristic might be the ability to generate rapid force production (a long jump take-off) or to coordinate the movement of forward trunk flexion with extensor force production in the lower limbs when rising to stand. The demands of the task, and the ability to structure practice to train for the task are key elements that the physiotherapist needs to take cognisance of. A “discrete skill” is considered one with a distinct start and end point (Schmidt and Lee 2005). A discrete skill may be kicking a ball, standing up or writing one’s name, the end of the movement is clearly defined and not random. While discrete tasks 74 Chapter 4 Motor Learning requiring less cognitive input, may be performed rapidly, some discrete tasks with greater cognitive involvement, such as writing, may take some time to complete. Research into discrete task skill acquisition has often taken the form of button press in response to a stimulus or the learning of a specific pattern of dextrous movements. A “Serial task” is considered as a task that consists of a set of individual movements (Schmidt and Lee 2005). Serial tasks may be considered as a sequence of discrete tasks that are performed in succession, for example brushing ones teeth or getting out of bed. While many elements of a serial task may have a discrete start and end point, it has been argued that the longer time to perform serial tasks and the effortless way in which a skilled performer accomplishes the many different elements of the task enables a separate classification to be recognised (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2005). A “continuous skill” has no clearly defined start and end point, therefore they often tend to be cyclical or fluid (Schmidt and Lee 2005). In a continuous task the movement can continue for a prolonged time and the end point would be considered arbitrary. Continuous tasks are often rhythmical such as walking, or require constant adaptation and tracking such as steering a car. 4.2.3 Gentile’s Two-Dimensional Taxonomy of tasks While the above classifications of tasks and skills provide some description of the complexity of the task, they fail to give a more comprehensive overview of task 75 Chapter 4 Motor Learning demands that physiotherapists may require to take into consideration when undertaking rehabilitation and developing appropriate interventions. In an attempt to overcome this, Gentile (1978) proposed a two-dimensional classification system, specifically for physiotherapists, that took into account both the environmental demands as well as the actions required by the learner in order to complete the task (see table 4.1). The required actions are then further classified depending not only on required movements of the body, but also manipulation of external objects. The environmental demands take into account whether the environment is stationary or mobile and whether the “regulatory conditions” under which the task is performed are static or variable. As can be seen from table 4.1, the complexity and demands of the task increase as one moves through the classification from top left to bottom right. This classification has been widely acknowledged as having wider application in teaching motor skills in a variety of settings (Magill 2007; Schmidt and Wrisberg 2005). It also provides an excellent basis upon which to structure practice in rehabilitation, giving clear guidance as to how to manipulate tasks to increase or decrease task demands. Many of the tasks physiotherapists set for their patients to practice are towards the top/left of the matrix (e.g. less demanding). 76 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Table 4.1. Closed skills Open skills ENVIRONMENTAL CONDITIONS ACTION REQUIREMENTS Stationary Regulatory Conditions and No intertribal variability Stationary Regulatory Conditions and Intertribal variability Moving Regulatory Conditions No intertribal variability Moving Regulatory Conditions and Intertribal variability STABILITY No body transport and No object manipulation Maintain standing balance alone in a room Maintain standing balance – brushing teeth BODY TRANSPORT Body transport with no object manipulation Sit to stand transfer – same chair same supporting surface Maintain standing balance on different floor surfaces (carpet, wood, gravel) Maintain standing balance while unloading crockery from dishwasher Sit to stand transfer from different height chairs, different supporting surfaces Climbing different height stairs with variable weight / shape bags Maintain standing balance on moving escalator (constant speed) Maintain standing balance on escalator, reading a newspaper Walking on an escalator Walking on an escalator carrying a bag of shopping Maintain standing balance on moving bus (variable acceleration) Maintain standing balance on moving bus, carrying bag of shopping Walking on an escalator, negotiating other people Walking on an escalator, negotiating other people, holding a bag of shopping No body transport. Object manipulation Body transport and object manipulation Climbing stairs – holding a handbag Gentile’s taxonomy of skills 77 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.3 Theories of Motor Learning Motor Learning is the study of achieving motor skills as well as the study of modifying these skills. For simplicity, this section discusses conceptual models of Motor Learning that are commonly presented in Motor Learning and Skill Acquisition texts. Both temporal and structural theories exist. 4.3.1 Temporal Stages of Motor Skill Learning The phases within temporal stages of Motor Learning are not meant to be perceived as discrete, but rather blend into one another as the learner moves along a continuum from early learning to later refinement of a motor skill. However artifical stages have been described by various originators of theories in order to allow the observers of Motor Skill learning to identify key stages of learning. 4.3.1.1 The Three Stage model of Fitts and Posner The model proposed by Fitts and Posner in 1967 depicts three proposed main stages of learning a motor skill and is acknowledged as a “classic” theory (Magill 2001). Within this classical model, the initial stage of motor skill learning has been termed the Cognitive Stage of learning. This occurs as the novice tries to understand the nature and complexities of the new task and develops a number of potential movement strategies to enable them to achieve the task. This stage requires the learner to attend to the demands of the task and to focus on cognitively oriented problems such as how much force should be exerted to pick up that glass of juice. A number of strategies may be trialled, performance of the task is often inconsistent and a large number of errors may be demonstrated during task attempts in this initial 78 Chapter 4 Motor Learning stage (Fitts and Posner 1973, Schmidt and Lee 2005; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). In the second stage of motor skill learning, or the Associative stage of learning, the demands of the skill have been understood and set indicators are associated with successful performance of the task. The time in this stage is spent refining the best motor strategies to achieve the task, there is a reduction in the variability of performance and the learner starts to problem solve and identify performance errors such as ‘insufficient force was generated when attempting to grip and lift the glass, that’s why it slipped and spilt’. As the learner moves into the third or Autonomous stage, the skill has been learnt and is conducted with a large degree of automaticity and little conscious thought. In this stage, the learner can undertake repeated skilled movements, problem solve and make any adjustments required as well as dual task, such as lifting the glass of juice and talk to a friend on the phone at the same time. Schmidt and Wrisberg (2008) have labelled the three stages as “trial and error” (Cognitive), “honing in” (Associative) and “free and easy” (Autonomous), which is a simple and clear description of the Three Stage model. As indicated previously, the stages are theoretical, and the execution of the task being learnt gradually becomes more refined as the learner moves towards the autonomous phase. 4.3.1.2 The Two Stage model of Gentile Just a few years after the publication of Fitts and Posners Three Stage model, Antoinette Gentile, a movement science researcher proposed a two stage model of learning that focused on the goal of the learner (Gentile 1978). 79 Chapter 4 Motor Learning In the “Initial” stage, Gentile argued that learners trial a number of movement strategies to become accustomed to the demands of the movement and to achieve the movement goal. In this initial stage, it was proposed that the learner also takes cognisance of certain relevant environmental cues to ensure appropriate organisation of movements, for example in reaching to a box in a cupboard, the learner concentrates on developing appropriate movements of flexion with elevation at the shoulder as well as appropriate extension movements of the wrist and fingers in order to raise the arm to an appropriate height and to prepare the hand to grasp the box. In this early stage, it is argued that the learner also learns to discriminate between environmental cues that do not influence the movement to be performed, for example the decoration on the box has no bearing on the task to be performed, (a “non-regulatory cue”), whereas the weight of the box does (a “regulatory” cue). Gentile proposed that the learner uses trial and error to formulate appropriate movement features that allow successful achievement of the goal. During this initial stage the learner has to actively problem solve when a movement is not fully successful and therefore there is a large cognitive component. As the learner moves towards the second stage of learning in this model, Gentile suggests that there is “a general concept of an effective approach …The action goal is not achieved consistently and [sic] lacks efficiency.” (Gentile 2000 in Carr and Shepherd). In the second stage or “Fixation” or “Diversification” stage, movement skills are refined. The learner develops the capacity to adapt the movement, for example reaching to a smaller box in a higher cupboard. Additionally, this stage is 80 Chapter 4 Motor Learning characterised by the movement being achieved successfully in a more consistent manner and with more economy of effort. The term “fixation” relates to the capability to successfully carry out Closed Skills where basic movement patterns are refined (fixated) to ensure consistency of successful achievement in this stage, for example putting a key in a lock. “Diversification” refers to the ability to adapt the acquired skill in situations where the performance of the skill is not undertaken in stable conditions, these open skills are often described as types of skills required in sporting activities such as having several options for where to pass the ball in hockey with the unpredictable nature of where the opposing team may be. Time to prepare the movement will be extremely limited and so the learner has to be able to anticipate environmental cues and adapt their movement quickly. The models of Fitts and Posner and of Gentile are theoretical and should not be considered as concrete entities. Learning takes place along a continuum, therefore “stages” will blur. Atlhough Gentile proposes a two stage model, there remains elements of cognition, refining the task elements and developing automaticity proficiency. 4.3.2 Structural Theories of Motor Learning A number of theories regarding the processes involved in motor learning have been proposed since the 1970’s. Emerging knowledge has led to revisions to theories and the development of functional imaging techniques is adding substantially to the body of knowledge. A summary of the two most commonly cited theories is presented. 81 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Adams was the first person to propose a theory of motor learning– the Closed Loop Theory (Adams 1971). His theory was based on a hierarchical understanding of movement control (e.g. the work of Sherrington in the early twentieth century) and the body’s response to sensory input. Adams described a closed loop feedback process for motor control, in which during the execution of a movement or action, feedback regarding the success or otherwise of the movement was compared against an existing memory of the movement stored in the nervous system. Adams suggested a memory trace was used to select and initiate a desired movement and that through a period of practice a perceptual trace of the correct movement was constructed and strengthened over time. During activity, it was hypothesised that the correctness of a movement was compared to the perceptual trace, which worked to modulate and correct errors (Adams 1971; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). Adams Closed Loop theory indicated that a person undergoing rehabilitation would be required to undertake repeated and precise practice of specific tasks in order to develop and strengthen the perceptual trace. Errors were deemed to be detrimental to learning as they would promote an incorrect perceptual trace (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). The idea of errorless learning has been shown to be incorrect (see section 4.6), with emerging evidence that variability of tasks improves movement performance (Schmidt and Lee 2005). Further weaknesses in this Closed Loop theory have been identified, for example the theory can not explain how novel movements can be carried out, nor can it explain how movement can be carried out in the absence of sensory feedback (Magill 2007; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). While the Closed Loop theory can now be argued as being 82 Chapter 4 Motor Learning redundant for explaining motor learning in humans, it laid the groundwork against which other theories could be proposed. 4.3.2.1 Schmidts Schema Theory A more robust theory, that still holds an influential place in explaining Motor Learning is the Schema Theory proposed by Richard Schmidt (1975). This theory, based on open loop control, was developed to provide an alternative explanation and overcome some of the weaknesses identified with the Closed Loop Theory (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). Schema Theory emphasises that when learning a new motor programme related to movement, the person learns a general set of rules which are open to adaptation and application to various situations. Important conceptual features of this theory were that of generalised motor programs and schema. Schmidt proposed that a Generalised Motor Program (GMP) contains the rules for a class of movements, rather than a single movement, and allows the production of flexible and skilled actions (Schmidt 1975; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). A GMP has been defined as: “a motor program that defines a pattern of movement rather than a specific movement; this flexibility allows performers to adapt the generalized program to produce variations of the pattern that meets various environmental demands” (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008 p124). Within Schema Theory it is suggested that one GMP controls movements with a set of defined characteristics. Therefore, if in the task of rising to stand, characteristics such as relative sequencing of movement components, movement amplitudes, relative timing and muscle force production remain constant, then whether rising 83 Chapter 4 Motor Learning from a stool or a dining chair, the same GMP will be utilised to control the movement. Alternatively, if there was a change in one of the characteristics then a different GMP would control the movement. Within Schema Theory, it is also suggested that memory plays an important role. Schema have been defined as “a set of rules relating the various outcomes of an individual’s actions … to the parameters that the person sets to produce those outcomes” (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008 p272). It has been proposed that motor memory or recall schema are responsible for selecting an appropriate response and that sensory memory or recognition schema appraises the response. Thus when making repeated attempts at a movement, it is proposed that knowledge of the initial conditions and the specifics of the required movement response to create the recall schema. Memory of the success of the movement and the parameters of the movement requirements (e.g. amount of force) create an association within the nervous system. The recall schema is available prior to the movement being attempted and does not rely on feedback during the movement. With respect to recognition schema, it is suggested this schema is responsible for evaluating prior sensory outcomes with the expected sensory consequences and provide ongoing comparisons with the actual movement. In this way, recognition schema are considered to be responsible for evaluating responses and identifying any anomalies between expected and actual sensory feedback. On completion of the movement, information is fed back into the schema to enable modification of the existing schema. The greater the number of repetitions of the movement results in more data available to refine the rule for that movement, the rule is then retained in the recall schema. 84 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Schema theory would suggest that motor learning is dependent on continual revision and renewal of recognition and recall schema with each attempted movement. With this repeated practice, refinement of the rules of the generalised motor programme occurs, thus reducing error with subsequent movement attempts. Central to Schema Theory is the hypothesis that increasing variability of practice, for example changing parameters such as the chair height to stand up from, or the supporting surface for the feet, will improve learning by creating stronger generalised motor programmes (Schmidt 1975; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Schmidt and Lee 2005) While much has been written in support of Schema Theory, as with all theories, there are problematic issues that have not yet been fully addressed. One question that may be asked is how this theory addresses the issue of how a new movement can be learnt. This conundrum, the “novelty problem” (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008, p123), is sometimes explained in relation to sports, whereby an innovative tennis shot is accounted for by the notion that a “novel” movement relies on following rules for selecting potentially appropriate parameters that have previously been applied in similar movements. While this explanation assists with understanding original movement production it fails to address how a first generalised motor programme or schema can be created in a child that has not experienced movement. 85 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.4 How to structure practice Textbooks on Motor Learning are predominantly aimed at sports and physical activity coaching and derive from work undertaken with young healthy adults (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008; Schmidt and Lee 2005). While the recommendations may be appropriate for people with neurological impairments, this has received scant attention in the neurological field. There is therefore, a challenge for therapists to consider how best to structure rehabilitation sessions to optimise skill acquisition. There is a need to consider the type of task to be practised and balance pragmatic organisational issues when setting up practice sessions to ensure the tasks can be undertaken successfully. Therapists working in rehabilitation will also be concerned to ensure changes in performance relate to learning with long-term retention of any gains. Furthermore, transferability of skill from one environment to another is a critical feature of learning (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Carr and Shepherd 1998). It is recognised that in order to acquire a skill, regular and extensive practice of the skill is required (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Shumway Cook and Wollacott). In the stroke rehabilitation setting, many patients receive only a limited amount of therapy (Bernhardt et al 2004; de Wit et al 2005; Tinson 1989) and opportunities for skill practice may be inadequate. The amount of practice required to regain a skill for people with impairments of motor control is not known, however it has been suggested that thousands of repetitions are required (Bach-y-Rita and Baillet 1987) and that intensive practice is probably required during the initial rehabilitation phase (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Winstein et al 1999). Some authors have recommended that short practice periods interspersed with rest periods similar to circuit training 86 Chapter 4 Motor Learning may be beneficial in later stages or when trying to promote unsupervised practice (Carr and Shepherd 1998). Physiotherapy in stroke rehabilitation is intricately linked with practice of movement. While it is often stated that the more practice that a person undertakes the more learning takes place (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Scmidt and Lee 2005; Marley et al 2000), the number of practice repetitions required to acquire a new skill or relearn a previously skilled movement have not been articulated. There may well be inter-individual variation in the need to practice as each individual will have their own set of experiences on which movements can be learnt or refined. There are several means of structuring practice that need to be considered when developing an optimal practice regime. In terms of stroke rehabilitation, how to structure each limited rehabilitation session is of clear importance, in order to make the best use of time and resources. In addition to the amount and type of practice, cognisance also requires to be taken of the effects of fatigue. 4.4.1 Massed or Distributed Practice Massed practice has been defined as when the amount of practice within an exercise schedule exceeds the amount of rest time between trials, whereas distributed practice is when there is a greater amount of rest time than actual practice time. If the same amount of exercise is to be undertaken, then following a massed practice schedule will achieve the same amount of practice in a shorter time than if it is undertaken in a distributed practice schedule (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Magill 2007). 87 Chapter 4 Motor Learning The vast majority of work in this area has been undertaken with young healthy subjects, with a focus on investigating the practice to rest relationship within practice sessions (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2005). Most authors agree that distributing practice is more beneficial to learning, although potentially detrimental in terms of immediate performance (Lee and Genovese 1988; Magill 2007; Schmidt and Wrisberg 2005; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). An important early study investigated whether it was more beneficial to undertake fewer lengthy practice sessions or a greater number of shorter sessions to improve keyboard skills in postal workers learning to operate a mail sort machine. Total training parameters of 60 hours, with practice occurring over five days each week were set. Participants received either one daily session of one or two hours, or two daily sessions of one or two hours. The group that had the most distributed practice (an hour daily) were found to achieve a target typing speed with least practice (55 hours), all the other groups took over 60 hours to achieve the target and the most intensely massed practice group (two sessions of two hours daily) did not achieve the target. On retention tests at one, three and nine months there was no difference between the three groups that had achieved target speed (Baddely and Longman 1978). In a small study, with more resonance to rehabilitation practice, Shea et al (2000) investigated the learning of a continuous balance task on a balance “stabilometer” with 14 young university students. They found that two sessions of seven trials of a 90 second practice on the stabilometer, conducted over one (massed) or two (distributed) days resulted in better learning for the distributed practice group and that this learning effect was retained at a 24 hour retention test. 88 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Little work in this area has been conducted with people with neurological impairments. Two recent studies investigating a verbal information recall task and a visual route finding ability with populations of people with Multiple Sclerosis (MS) (Goverover et al 2008) or Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) (Goverover et al 2009) have been carried out. In both studies sample sizes were small (n=20 and n=10) and only three acquisition trials were undertaken. Trials were consecutive in the massed practice condition and with five minutes rest in the distributed practice group, with retention tested at 30 minutes (Goverover et al 2009a; Goverover et al 2009b). Dsitributed practice was found to be beneficial for both tasks in the TBI population (Goverover 2009a) but only for the verbal task in the MS population (Goverover 2009b). While inferences were made to support the use of distributed practice in learning functional tasks in these populations, there is, currently, insufficient evidence from empirical studies of physical tasks such as a sit to stand transfer. In relation to stroke, recent authors have deemed Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy (CIMT) as massed practice (Vearrier et al 2004; Marklund and Klässbo 2006; Massie et al 2009; Taub et al 2000). In CIMT, the unaffected limb is constrained for prolonged periods (up to 90% of the waking day) to force movement of the affected limb, which is trained for six to seven hours daily (Taub et al 2000). This intensive intervention, while broadly fitting the description of “massed” practice, includes markedly different characteristics (for example constraint and intense prolonged exercise) to the massed nature of practice within a regular one hour treatment or practice session and therefore findings from CIMT research will not be considered further. 89 Chapter 4 Motor Learning It is unclear why distributed practice is more beneficial for learning, however some explanations have been proposed. Massed practice while improving immediate performance, is more likely to elicit fatigue which may negatively impact on learning (Magill 2007). Alternatively, less cognitive effort may be used with massed practice, as the schedule may induce boredom due to constant repetition, this reduction of cognitive engagement may result in reduced learning. In terms of consolidation of memory, massed practice may not allow sufficient time for the neurobiochemical processes necessary for transformation of the memory trace into a relatively permanent representation (Magill 2007) 4.4.2 Blocked or Random Practice In stroke rehabilitation, the therapist will attempt to improve a number of different skills in a single treatment session, and generally one task will be practised for a number of times before moving on to the next task (e.g. sitting balance then sit-tostand transfer then reaching). Blocked practice is undertaken within a session when task A is practiced repeatedly before task B and so on, whereas random practice occurs when a number of different tasks are practiced non-consecutively and in no particular order within the session. In conditions of blocked practice, performance has been shown to improve during the acquisition or practice phase whereas performance on a retention test which indicates actual learning generally tends to degrade, the reverse has been shown to be true for random practice (Shea and Morgan 1979; Magill and Hall 1990). 90 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Intuitively it may make sense to structure a rehabilitation session to follow a programme of uninterrupted task performance and organisationally it may be easier to structure a practice session with tasks undertaken in blocks. However, the evidence for blocked practice is weak in terms of learning, although some authors have suggested that during the early stage of skill acquisition, blocked practice may enable the learner to gain the fundamentals of the movement to be learnt. There is now a body of evidence derived from laboratory experiments with young normal subjects that supports the superiority of random practice to engender learning. Shea and Morgan (1979) were the first to investigate random or blocked practice. Their sample of college students were required to practice three different patterns of arm movement to knock down barriers as fast as possible. All participants practised 18 repetitions of each movement, but one group did all task A, then B then C, while the other group practised in a random order. While the blocked group performed slightly better by the end of the acquisition phase of six practice sessions, retention tests conducted 10 minutes and 10 days after the acquisition phase showed superior learning for the group who had initially undertaken random practice. These findings have been replicated by other investigators studying relatively simple tasks (e.g. Wilde et al 2005; Wulf and Lee 1993). Few studies investigating random or blocked practice in people post stroke have been conducted. Hanlon (1996) was the first to report in this area. Twenty four participants at least six months post-stroke were randomly assigned to either a “block” or “random” group practising 10 trials daily of a five step upper limb task until task achievement was completed. A separate “control” group received no practice. 91 Chapter 4 Motor Learning All subjects undertook the same retention tests at two and seven days after cessation of the intervention. The random practice group showed significantly better learning of the task than the blocked practice group and control groups on both retention tests. While these findings lend some cautious support to the findings of studies undertaken with young healthy adult participants, the work by Hanlon can be criticised due to the small sample size (n = 24) with only eight subjects in each group. Additionally, there was lack of clarity of randomisation procedures and supplementary non-specific upper limb tasks that were part of the random practice group schedule which might have biased findings due to the extra practice. It was however, the first study undertaken in people with stroke to demonstrate agreement with findings from studies undertaken on young healthy subjects. In a slightly larger study, Pohl et al (2006) investigated the ability of 37 people within 45 days of stroke onset (22 classified as moderate and 15 classified as mild) and 32 age matched controls to acquire a target sequence using a switch box. Participants undertook both random and blocked practice with a total number of 480 repetitions in a single test session. In this study all participants improved their performance as measured by faster reaction time and reduced variability of performance, however a significantly slower time and more variable performance was found for participants deemed as moderate stroke. While this study demonstrated improved performance, no retention test was reported so long term learning could not be assessed. Furthermore, the task (a serial reaction time task) was not analogous to functional daily activities so generalisations can not be made. Despite the weaknesses of the study, the findings are interesting and may indicate that practice may need to be structured differently for people with different severity of stroke. 92 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.4.3 Variable or Constant Practice When practising a task, another feature that needs to be considered is whether to structure the session so that the learner undertakes variable or constant practice. Variable practice allows practice of the same basic movement pattern but with variations of some characteristic, for example when practising sit to stand transfer, altering the seat height or foot base width could be introduced. In constant practice all the task demands would remain static. Variable practice has been shown to be more beneficial to learning than constant practice (Schmidt and Lee 2005), although it can be difficult to schedule in an environment other than a research laboratory or clinic. It has been suggested that variable practice may strengthen the schema resulting in more effective learning of the task rules (Schmidt and Lee 2005). An investigation of the ability to correctly produce a criterion force under conditions of constant or variable practice was undertaken with 24 young healthy adults (Shea and Kohl 1990 experiment 1). The variable practice group undertook a total number of 289 repetitions (17 blocks of 17 trials), of which 85 trials were at the criterion force, whereas the constant practice group undertook 85 trials at the criterion force. Feedback was provided by computerised error information. While no significant difference between the groups was found during acquisition, at a 24 hour retention test, the variable practice group performed with significantly more accuracy (Shea and Kohl 1990 experiment 1). This study allowed the variable practice group a greater intensity of practice than the constant specific practice group and it may be that this greater practice allowed development of more refined schema. In a followup study the same methodology was followed with the addition of another constant specific group that undertook 289 trials at the criterion force (Shea and Kohl 1990 experiment 2). This second experiment showed that at the retention test, the 93 Chapter 4 Motor Learning variable group again performed significantly more accurately than either of the specific constant groups. Furthermore the specific constant group undertaking 289 acquisition trials showed poor ability to reproduce the criterion force with the authors hypothesising that either they had insufficient time to process error feedback during acquisition trials, or that they may have failed to attend to the task requirements due to the repetitive nature of the constant task (Shea and Kohl 1990 experiment 2). 4.4.4 Whole or Part Practice When teaching a new skill or when reacquiring previously skilled movements during rehabilitation, it is common practice to break down a complex movement into component parts or part practice (PP) rather than whole practice (WP) of the task in its entirety. In neurological rehabilitation, PP is often encouraged intuitively by the therapist who may have assessed, for example, that a stroke patient does not have sufficient dynamic balance capability to walk unaided, but may be able to practice weight transfer in standing. While this may make intuitive sense, it has been suggested that stripping away other demands of the task may result in an alteration of the motor programme so that the part task is no longer the same as it is when undertaken in the whole task context (Fontana et al 2009; Schmidt and Lee 2005). Some rehabilitation texts have advocated that movements should be practised in their entirety (Carr and Shepherd 2003), although other authors have recommended different strategies for different types of movement. For complex tasks, where practise of discrete components can be structured easily, it has been suggested that PP is preferable. Whole Practice has been recommended for a discrete task of short duration, or for longer duration, cyclical, continuous tasks such as walking 94 Chapter 4 Motor Learning (Fontana et al 2009; Marley et al 2000). Recommendations for serial tasks however are varied. While Marley et al 2000 recommended serial tasks to be practised under PP conditions, counter-claims have been made in favour of WP of serial movements where parts of the task depend on preceding successful actions (Schmidt and Lee 2005). Hansen et al (2009) found that all 36 of their young healthy adult participants demonstrated improvement on a four component serial aiming task whether undertaking part or whole practice. Practice was structured either as WP or PP of different components with some overlap (eg components 1,2 and 3 or components 2,3 and 4). The improvement in performance after 40 acquisition trials which was maintained on testing learning one day later, was put down to PP participants being able to learn transitions between movement components. It is interesting that learning is said to have occurred after one day as experience with people with stroke is that retention for 24 hours is not necessarily a good indicator that a skill has been relearnt. No studies relating to PP or WP were found for people with stroke. In a small crossover study of 20 elderly participants performing either part practice of a signature task by separating the movement into three components (PP) or performing the whole task in its’ entirety (WP), it was found that during the WP conditions movement times were faster and kinematic variables smoother (Ma and Trombly 2001). These results are similar to findings favouring WP from studies on less functionally complex tasks such as computer games (Fabiani et al 1989;). From the paucity of literature in this area relating to complex functional tasks that are often serial or continuous in nature, it is clear that empirical research in this area is required to add to the body of knowledge contained in texts on Motor Learning. 95 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.4.5 Attentional Focus during practice The ability to attend to tasks and the environment is critical in the relearning of motor skills (Hochstenbach and Mulder 1999). However, many people post-stroke may have impairments in the ability to selectively attend to tasks and the ability to maintain attention, therefore the manner in which a person with stroke is requested to practice skills is important. In providing practice opportunities for learning or relearning a task, the instructor or therapist needs to determine what instructions to provide to facilitate the learner in undertaking the desired movement. In addition to any physical demonstration of the motor skill to be learnt, such as how to grip an object, instruction can either have an internal focus on the body movements required (for example elbow, wrist and hand position) or an external focus on remote features (such as keeping a mug level). A recent review which included studies relating to focus of attention for motor learning has shown that having an external focus of attention for movement outcomes is more beneficial for motor learning than an internal focus concentrating on movement of body parts (Wulf et al 2010). It is known that an internal focus, attending to particular movements of the body, when undertaking an automated motor skill can be detrimental to performance (McNevin et al 2000; Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008). Despite these findings, observational evidence would assert that physiotherapists often instruct people undergoing rehabilitation to move their body in a certain way, inherently utilising an internal attentional focus (“bend your knee”, “lift your toes”). However, this manner of instruction is counter to research that demonstrates having an external focus of attention is more beneficial to motor skill learning than an internal focus (Wulf and Weigelt 1997). 96 Chapter 4 Motor Learning It has even been shown that in some skills, at times, no instruction can be more beneficial than early instruction. Wulf and Weigelt (1997), undertook a study where healthy subjects were either instructed on the desired movement pattern on a ski simulator at the start of the study, or received no instruction. The subjects in the “no instruction” group demonstrated better performance during acquisition and at a retention test. A possible explanation for this is that they had to problem solve for themselves how to improve performance. It is not clear why an external focus of attention is more beneficial to learning a skill than an internal focus, although it has been suggested that consciously trying to control automatic movements may interfere with “normal” motor control processes (McNevin et al 2000). It has also been suggested that focusing on the outcome of a movement rather than the actual movement itself are also pertinent for the focus of feedback (Shea and Wulf 1990). These findings have implications for designing practice to be undertaken without supervision. Optimal strategies may be to provide external cues to focus on – for example moving an object manually and focussing on the object rather than the hand moving it. 4.5 Feedback Feedback is of key importance when learning movement skills in order to gauge success, or otherwise, of performance. Feedback has received considerable attention in Motor Learning research and is important in providing information and motivation for the learner (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Schmidt and 97 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Wrisberg 2008; Schmidt and Lee 2005). Feedback has been described as “information produced from the various sensors as a consequence of moving” (Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008 p 69). This definition however limits consideration of feedback to internal processes and a complimentary definition taking cognisance of external processes could be “augmented error information provided to the performer about goal achievement” (Winstein 1991a p66). 4.5.1 Intrinsic Feedback Intrinsic feedback is feedback that is provided from the learner’s own sensory system as a consequence of movement (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). Intrinsic feedback includes sensations such as touch, pressure and joint position sense of the moving limbs as well as visual and auditory information. 4.5.2 Extrinsic Feedback Extrinsic feedback refers to information provided to the learner externally to supplement intrinsic feedback, this externally derived feedback is also referred to as augmented feedback. In stroke rehabilitation, extrinsic feedback may be supplied by the therapist in a variety of ways using verbal, auditory and material cues. 4.5.2.1 Knowledge of Results Knowledge of Results (KR) is provided to the learner on completion of the movement and supplies information relating to the success of the movement outcome in relation to the goal (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Schmidt & lee 2005). KR is considered extrinsic as it is often provided verbally, however the result of a movement will also generate intrinsic information, therefore KR can be 98 Chapter 4 Motor Learning considered to be augmented by intrinsic feedback. It has been hypothesised that timing feedback to allow active problem solving may be most beneficial for motor learning (Winstein et al 1996). Bilodeau and Bilodeau (1958) were the first to investigate the effect of frequency of KR. In their work 273 airforce personnel undertook 40 trials to learn a lever pull task with either 100%, 33%, 25% or 10% KR. All groups demonstrated improved performance when acquiring the skill, however no retention test to identify learning was undertaken in this early work, therefore it was not clear what the optimum KR schedule would be. Since the early work of Bilodeau and Bilodeau (1958), various studies have been carried out to determine how best to provide KR to learners. In a study of 36 healthy young adults undertaking a investigating barrier knock-down task, feedback was provided either in a known blocked KR (regarding a specific movement parameter) or random KR (Lee and Carnahan 1990). Following 60 acquisition trials (ten blocks of six trials), it was found that while all groups improved their performance, the blocked KR group acquired the skill more quickly than the random KR group. At a retention test (two blocks of six trials with no feedback) undertaken ten minutes after the acquisition phase there was no difference between the groups at the first block, although the random KR group performed better at the second block (Lee and Carnahan 1990). Although this task was a serial mechanistic pattern of movement, unrelated to complex functional tasks, it does provide an indication that delivery of feedback may need to be structured in a similar way to structuring practice. 99 Chapter 4 Motor Learning KR can be provided at various frequencies. KR provided after every movement trial is termed “100% KR”, however it may also be provided by fading the frequency (for example after every second trial - 50% KR). Findings from work in this area generally indicates that less frequent KR may not be beneficial for immediate performance (during acquisition) although it may be beneficial for learning as indicated on retention tests. More recently, Winstein et al (1996) studied a group of 60 young healthy adults ( age 26 years) learning a partial weight bearing task, with 80 repetitions of the task during the acquisition phase and a delayed retention tested at 48 hours. It was found that during acquisition, a group receiving concurrent feedback (during performance) were more accurate (mean accuracy 1.4%) and consistent (mean normalised variable error 1.2%) with the task than groups receiving either 100%KR or 20%KR (mean accuracy 6.4% and 8.2%; mean normalised variance error 5% and 5.3%). During retention however, all groups were less accurate, but the concurrent feedback group performed with the largest error (11.2%). These findings lend credence to the suggestion that while performance may improve transiently with concurrent feedback, this mode of providing information is not beneficial for motor learning. Of the limited studies that have been conducted on people with Stroke, limited support for the findings on studies conducted with young healthy adults has been demonstrated. Winstein et al (1999) investigated 40 people with chronic stroke 100 Chapter 4 Motor Learning (median time since onset 24 months) and 40 age-matched controls 57 years) undertaking a rapid elbow flexion and extension movement by moving a lever. Participants were “pseudo-randomised” to receive either 100% feedback for 99 trials or 67% faded feedback for 99 trials. Feedback was provided on a computer screen. A retention test of 18 trials (two blocks of nine movements) with and without feedback was undertaken one day later. This study showed no difference between feedback groups on accuracy and consistency of performance of the task during either the acquisition or retention phases. There was however a difference in movement pattern between the control group and people with stroke, with the latter group being less accurate and more variable in their performance at all phases. While this study demonstrated the capacity for motor learning for people with stroke, both 100%KR and 67% faded KR were equally as effective. While adding to the body of knowledge in the application of Motor Learning principles to stroke, caution should be exercised due to the highly specific and non-functional nature of the task. 4.5.2.2 Knowledge of Performance Knowledge of Performance (KP) relates to the movement strategy adopted to achieve the task (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007; Schmidt and Lee 2005). KP is often used by physiotherapists to provide information about movement performance for people with motor impairments and altered “normal” movement patterns (for example “… try to relax your shoulders when reaching forwards”). While KP is mainly thought of as being provided verbally, it may also be provided visually with filmed or photographed material. While most of the research on feedback relates to KR, it is often KP that therapists provide as feedback to people with stroke. 101 Chapter 4 Motor Learning The only study found investigating KP with stroke was undertaken with 28 people with chronic stroke (Cirstea and Levin 2007). Participants were randomly allocated to practice pointing movements and either receive 20%KR about the movement precision or faded 26% KP about arm movement patterns. relatively young (KR age 55.7yrs, KP The cohort were age 59.1yrs), but with a mean time since stroke of at least 11 months. The practice schedule was clinically relevant with 10 daily practice sessions for an hour daily over two weeks. In each session, the subjects had to practice 75 pointing movements with the impaired arm to the unimpaired side. The KR group received feedback at the end of the task on every fifth attempt. The faded KP feedback related to shoulder and elbow movements and was provided concurrently on each of the first 25 trials then on every second trial for the next 25 and finally on every fifth trial. While the KR group demonstrated some improvement at the one month retention test, it was the KP group that made greater and statistically significant improvements in shoulder range of movement and improved temporal coordination of shoulder and elbow joints at the end of the intervention and on retention tests at one month. The KP group also demonstrated significant improvement in arm strength and precision movements following intervention. Additional compensatory trunk movements were also found to decrease in both groups. This appeared to be a well controlled and clinically relevant study and seems to indicate that in this sample of people with chronic stroke, KP may be beneficial for learning a general set of movements potentially strengthening a generalised motor programme. 102 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 4.5.2.3 Manual Guidance Manual guidance is a further form of extrinsic feedback that is generally provided concurrently with movement, for example a therapist guiding the forearm and hand to move smoothly within “normal” parameters when reaching forward to a target. A number of authors following a “neuro-developmental” approach to rehabilitation for people with neurological impairments have often advocated manual guidance to facilitate more appropriate sensory feedback, with the assumption that enhancement of normal movement will result in more efficient skill reacquisition (Bobath 1990; Davies 1985; Davies 1990). It might be fair to argue that a number of therapists working in Stroke Rehabilitation in the 21st century would still subscribe to an approach that strongly encourages manual guidance and a philosophy that within stroke rehabilitation errors in movement patterns should not be allowed. Counter arguments, based on emerging neurophysiological and neuropsychological evidence, have pointed out that while guidance may be effective in the very early stages of skill acquisition (Magill 2007; Hochstenbach and Mulder 1999) continued guidance may limit the patient in engaging in the active problem solving required to relearn movement and that retention and long-term carry over of any skills will be inhibited (Majsak 1996; Horak 1991). One therapy related study was found that investigated manual guidance or verbal KR in the learning of a 70% partial weight bearing task (Sidaway et al 2008). Forty young adults were allocated to one of four groups to receive either 100% KR, 33% KR, 100% guidance or 33% guidance during 120 acquisition trials of this task. Retention tests were undertaken after 10 minutes, one day and one week. The 100% guidance group obviously had no error during acquisition and the 100% KR were found to reduce error during this phase. At retention, the KR groups 103 Chapter 4 Motor Learning performed with less error than the guidance groups and the subjects receiving 33% feedback performed with less error. Furthermore, the accuracy of the 100% guidance group performance deteriorated was significantly worse than all other groups across all retention tests. While this study indicates that in healthy young adults consistent guidance was ineffective in learning a motor skill, there still remains a gap in empirical knowledge regarding guidance for people with a neurological impairment. . 4.6 Neurophysiological Evidence relating to Motor Learning The previous sections in this chapter have looked at various characteristics that require organisation within a practice session to allow the learner the optimal chance at acquiring or reacquiring skill. This section will consider the evidence relating to neurophysiological processes involved in motor learning. Over 100 years ago, Sherrington was the first to suggest a simple form of learning by demonstrating that with repeated stimulus of the flexion reflex, habituation was noted to occur (Sherrington 1906 as cited in Burke 2007). Habituation may be short-term with a transient reduction in the excitatory post-synaptic potential (EPSP). It has subsequently been demonstrated that long-term repetitive stimulation results in structural changes, namely a reduction in the number of synaptic connections between the sensory neurones, inter-neurones and motor neurones (Kandel et al 2000). The application of habituation principles in rehabilitation has been demonstrated with the application of Cooksey Cawthorne exercises in cases of dizziness caused by inner ear disorders, whereby repeatedly undertaking 104 Chapter 4 Motor Learning movements that bring about dizziness gradually results in amelioration of the dizziness (Bamiou et al 2000). Sensitisation is another simple form of learning that has been demonstrated and probably involves the same sensory neurones, inter-neurones and motor neurones that are involved in habituation. Sensitisation is a response to potentially damaging stimuli and can be summarised as prolonging the action potential to allow more neurotransmitter to be released at synapses resulting in an increased EPSP. While the same neurones may be involved in habituation and sensitisation, effectiveness of the synaptic connections is enhanced in sensitisation and depressed in habituation (Kandel et al 2000). 4.6.1 The Cerebellum and Motor Learning The cerebellum is critically involved in motor learning. It contains the most neurones in any anatomical subdivision of the brain, however it has comparatively few types of neurone and therefore neuroscientists have studied cerebellar function and understand the cerebellar circuitry relatively well and have hypothesised how the cerebellum is involved in motor learning (Kandel, Schwarz and Jessel 2000; Siegel and Sapru 2006, Bear, Connors and Paradiso 2007). Structurally the cerebellar cortex is organised into three layers (see figure 4.1 and 4.2). The outer molecular layer has relatively few somata, with inhibitory stellate and basket cells distributed between excitatory axons of deeper granule cells which give off long parallel fibres. Dendrites from inhibitory Purkinje cells are also situated 105 Chapter 4 Motor Learning in the outer molecular layer and arise from Purkinje cells in the middle layer. Inhibitory axons from the Purkinje cells extend into the deeper white matter and afford output from the cerebellar cortex. The inner granular layer contains some large Golgi interneurones and numerous granule cells both of which synapse with the main cerebellar afferent input mossy fibres. Figure 4.1. General Structural organisation of cerebellum (from Felten and Shetty 2009, p376) 106 Chapter 4 Motor Learning Figure 4.2. Synaptic organisation of cerebellar circuitry showing excitatory and inhibitory circuits: (from Kandel Schwarz and Jessel 2000 p837) Afferent input to the cerebellum comes from two main sources: excitatory mossy fibres and climbing fibres. Mossy fibres convey sensory information from the brain stem and cerebral cortex and form excitatory synapses with granule cells. The parallel fibres that branch transversely from the granule cells intersect serially with multiple dendrites from the Purkinje cells and it is estimated that each Purkinje cell receives input from up to a million granule cells (Kandel, Schwarz and Jessel 2000). Climbing fibres arise from the Inferior Olivary nucleus of the Medulla Oblongata and 107 Chapter 4 Motor Learning carry information relating to proprioception and vision as well as information from the cerebral cortex. The climbing fibres act as a comparator between expected and actual sensory inputs. These fibres go on to wind around the Purkinje cell bodies and dendrites forming strong connections with up to ten Purkinje neurones (Kandel, Schwarz and Jessel 2000; Seigel and Sapru 2006; Bear, Connors and Pardiso 2007). In the 1970’s two independent researchers – David Marr in England and James Albus in America - proposed that this complex cerebellar circuitry could be involved in motor learning. The underlying principles were that firstly the climbing fibres identified movement error and secondly the climbing fibre induced corrections by reducing the effectiveness of the synapse between parallel fibre and Purkinje cell. This modification was termed Long Term Depression (LTD). With repeated movements, there would be inhibition of error information from the parallel fibres and this would, over time, result in a movement with less error. (Kandel, Schwarz and Jessel 2000; Bear, Connors and Pardiso 2007). It is also suggested that three important neurotransmitter modifications must also occur concurrently for LTD to occur. These mechanisms are a surge of calcium (Ca2+) in the Purkinje cell dendrites; raised sodium (Na+) levels and activation of protein kinase C, these three events result in a decrease in post synaptic membrane AMPA receptor channels which result in a slowing of synaptic transmission. LTD has been established by Ito and colleagues who demonstrated a smaller post synaptic response in Purkinje cells following paired stimulation of the climbing fibres and parallel fibres, compared to the EPSP when only parallel fibres received 108 Chapter 4 Motor Learning stimulation (Bear, Connors and Paradiso 2007). It is not clear whether learning occurs in discrete areas of the cerebellum or how explicit sensory information is involved in learning. While LTD has been demonstrated, the actual role of LTD has not yet been fully proven in motor learning, but it is conceivable that motor learning may arise from modifying synaptic transmission (Seigel and Sapru 2006; Bear, Connors and Paradiso 2007). 4.6.2 Structural changes associated with Motor Learning The process of neuroplasticity has previously been considered in section 2.6. It has been suggested that neuroplasticity can be considered on a scale ranging from “short term changes in the efficiency or strength of synaptic connections to long-term structural changes in the organisation and numbers of connections among neurons” (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). In considering Motor Learning, one would also be expecting a continuum of changes, with evidence of learning represented by persisting structural changes within the CNS. Learning and the memory storage of learning can occur within all parts of the brain (Kandel, Schwarz and Jessel 2000; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). In situations where more complex forms of learning occur with the development of skills and refinement of movement, rather than a response to stimulus, more intricate neural mechanisms are involved. Pascual-Leone et al (1994) undertook a finger movement task with a computer screen and touch pad with four buttons, participants had to press the correct button when a number was flashed onto the computer screen. Blocks of 10 trials were presented and one group followed a 109 Chapter 4 Motor Learning repetitive sequence, whereas the other group followed a random sequence. Reactions times and cortical activity were recorded using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and it was shown that not only did reaction time reduce but also an increase in cortical mapping was demonstrated to the relevant finger muscles. Interestingly, subjects began to recognise the sequence of movement within six to nine blocks of trials, at which point the cortical maps reduced to baseline size. This study demonstrated alterations in motor cortex output and these changes were explained as the subjects having attained explicit learning of the task and were therefore able to predict the next number. The ability to quantify changes in cortical activity, associated with motor performance is an exciting development for clinicians and researchers in this field, and will enable the identification of structural and functional changes in the brain associated with practice. 4.7 Summary This chapter has provided an overview of some of the key issues in the motor learning literature. As indicated, the vast majority of studies were conducted on small samples of young healthy adults and this makes generalisation of findings to a population with CNS impairments problematic. Within the past 15 years or so, a limited number of studies relating to neurological populations have started to provide some applied evidence (e.g. Winstein et al 1999; Hanlon 1996), although research in this area should be considered as in the very early stages. A variety of practice tasks have been studied, however a further criticism of much of the motor learning research to date is that the vast majority relates to simple rapid, discrete tasks such as manipulating a lever (Winstein et al 1999), or producing a 110 Chapter 4 Motor Learning force (Shea and Kohl 1990). Furthermore, the number of repetitions undertaken within practice schedules have tended to be low, often with acquisition of a task claimed with less than 100 trials (e.g. Bilodeau and Bilodeayu 1958; Shea and Morgan 1979; Lee and Carnahan 1990; Winstein et al 1996; Hanlon 1996; Winstein et al 1999 ) with no explanation for the variable number of practice trials adopted. Some influential authors have advocated applying motor learning principles to people with stroke (Carr and Shepherd 1998, Carr and Shepherd 2000), particularly with recommendations for how to structure the practice of functional exercises, however the evidence to support this view is inadequate. There is an urgent need for more work using well designed studies with patient populations to ascertain the appropriateness of applying motor learning principles derived from young healthy adults to the rehabilitation of people with stroke. The need to test Motor Learning theory in a rehabilitation context forms the basis for this study. 111 Chapter 4 Motor Learning 5 5.1 RATIONALE FOR STUDY Introduction The preceding chapters have explored literature relating to Stroke and recovery mechanisms, appraised knowledge relating to Physiotherapy for people with latestage stroke and reviewed evidence to support Motor Learning theory. The rationale for the current study and a brief overview is presented in this chapter. It is well accepted that the greatest and most rapid recovery following stroke occurs within the first three months (Wade et al 1985; Wade and Langton Hewer, 1987). It is also acknowledged that post-stroke recovery does not stop at three months. People with stroke have the capacity for ongoing recovery for months and years (for example Wade et al 1992; Dam et al 1993; Green et al 2002; Ouelette et al, 2004). Despite this knowledge, there is a tension between the desire or guidance to provide on-going rehabilitation (SIGN 2010, RCP 2008) and increasingly limited healthcare resources. There is therefore a need to investigate whether people with late-stage stroke are capable of undertaking exercise at home without therapist supervision. As identified in 4.4, the majority of studies reported in Motor Learning literature have been undertaken with young healthy participants undertaking rapid discrete or serial tasks (for example Shea and Morgan 1979; Wulf and Lee 1993; Hansen et al 2009). Recommendations have been extrapolated from these findings and these recommendations have been applied to structuring stroke rehabilitation practice sessions despite no empirical supporting evidence (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Carr and Shepherd 2003). It has been argued in 4.4 that while a variety of practice paradigms exist, the need to establish appropriate structures within which to undertake exercise practice is still required for people with stroke. 112 Chapter 5 Rationale for the Study Of particular interest in this study was whether to encourage part practice (PP) or whole practice (WP) of functional tasks, this stemmed in part, from clinical experience of structuring practice sessions for people with stroke. In developing the protocol, it was considered prudent to attempt to recruit people with late-stage stroke who had been discharged from formal rehabilitation. There were two main reasons for this decision. Firstly, people who sustained a stroke at least 12 months prior to entering the trial would they be considered to be outwith the stage of natural, or rapid recovery. Secondly, the normal “treatment” for this population would be no treatment and therefore it would be possible to have a control group (Con) who received no intervention – a situation that would be impossible and unethical to justify for people in the earlier stages of recovery. Questions to be addressed in the study included: Does a home physiotherapy programme based on either part- or whole practice strategies result in changes in performance of functional tasks for people with late-stage stroke? Does a home physiotherapy programme based on either part- or whole practice strategies result in changes in parameters of activity limitation, participation and health status for people with late-stage stroke? Are any changes in performance retained after cessation of the intervention phase of the home physiotherapy programme? A secondary question was How much activity is undertaken by people with late-stage stroke in the community? 113 Chapter 5 Rationale for the Study An important consideration in developing the intervention was to select functional tasks that would be meaningful to people with late-stage stroke. As participants would be requested to practice in their own home, without regular therapist supervision, safety was of prime importance. Consideration was also given to ensuring the tasks would be relevant to the vast majority of community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. Each task had to be capable of being practised safely in its entirety, and also to be divided into meaningful and discrete parts to enable physical practice. The study reported in this thesis was developed as a phase II exploratory randomised controlled pilot study to test the feasibility of a targeted but complex intervention for community-dwelling people with late-stage stroke (Campbell et al 2000; Anderson 2008). The process of developing and piloting the interventions are reported in chapter six, the final methodology in chapter seven and the results in chapter eight. 5.2 Research Aims The primary aim of this study was to investigate the effects of a home exercise programme based on Motor Learning principles of part practice (PP) or whole practice (WP) of selected functional tasks for people with late-stage stroke. Recommendations in some physiotherapy texts have advocated one or other 114 Chapter 5 Rationale for the Study approach, but with no empirical evidence (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Carr and Shepherd 2003; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). As part of the study, participants would be requested to document the number of repetitions of each exercise. As participants would be undertaking practice in the home setting, without therapist supervision, it was decided to try and gain some confirmation of activity by the simple use of an activity monitor worn for a single day. The additional benefit of this strategy was that an indication would be provided of the amount of activity undertaken by community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. A secondary aim was, therefore, to explore activity undertaken by community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. 5.2.1 Hypotheses The null hypotheses under investigation are identified below Global measures of impairment, activity and participation HO1 There will be no significant difference in Motor Assessment Scale score between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO2 There will be no significant difference in Barthel Index total score between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO3 There will be no significant difference in Frenchay Activity Index score between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or long-term follow-up HO4 There will be no significant difference in Frenchay Arm Test score between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term followup 115 Chapter 5 Rationale for the Study Measures of mobility HO5 There will be no significant difference in Timed Up and Go time between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO6 There will be no significant difference in gait speed between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO7 There will be no significant difference in Rise to Stand time between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO8 There will be no significant difference in Step Test ability with affected leg between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO9 There will be no significant difference in Step Test ability with unaffected leg between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up Measure of mood and health status HO10 There will be no significant difference in Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) total score between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or short- or long-term follow-up HO11 There will be no significant difference in HADS Anxiety or Depression subscale scores between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or longterm follow-up HO12 There will be no significant difference in Stroke Impact Scale domain scores between Con, PP or WP groups from baseline to end of intervention, or long-term follow-up 116 Chapter 5 Rationale for the Study 6 DEVELOPMENT OF THE METHODOLOGY FOR A RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL OF PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE. 6.1 Introduction This chapter details the chronological journey charting the development of the methodology for the main study. During this development phase, cognisance needed to be taken of the safety and feasibility issues relating to unsupervised practice of functional activities within the home setting as well as the limited personnel and financial resources available to support the study. At times, therefore, a pragmatic approach to problem solving and to developing the methodology was, by necessity, adopted. The main stages of development of the methodology for the main study are reported in this chapter and are detailed in figure 6.1. 6.2 Development of the Exercise Intervention The aim of this exploratory study was to establish exercises that could be practised in either a whole practice (WP) or part practice (PP) format by people living with chronic stroke, as part of the main study. Participants in this phase of the methodology development were also canvassed as to some of the residual poststroke physical problems that were most important to them to improve. 117 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 1a: 1b: Development of Exercise Intervention Determining Screening Tests and Outcome Measures to be used in main study 2: Documentation of Exercise Practice regimes 3: Pilot of Proposed Methodology in the Community 4: Testing the activPAL 5: Establishing psychometric properties of a Modified Outcome Measure 6: Finalising documentation, intervention and study protocol Figure 6.1. Flow diagram of the stages of development of the Methodology for a pilot randomised controlled trial of Physiotherapy for LateStage Stroke 118 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.2.1 Sample and Recruitment Procedures for the development of the Exercise Intervention Four people with stroke were recruited from a database of stroke patients who had previously attended the Physiotherapy Department at Astley Ainslie Hospital Edinburgh, a tertiary centre specialising in post-acute care and in–patient and outpatient rehabilitation. Participants were recruited if they had sustained a stroke, were living at home, had been discharged from formal stroke rehabilitation and were at least four months post-stroke and had no other major co-morbid neurological (such as Multiple Sclerosis), musculoskeletal (such as recent fracture or severe arthritis) or cardiorespiratory (such as unstable angina) conditions that might have precluded their ability to participate. Potential participants that were still attending the Physiotherapy department for monitoring visits were approached by the senior physiotherapist in stroke rehabilitation and provided with verbal and written information about the study (appendix Ia). On return of signed informed consent (appendix Ib), the principal investigator made contact with them and a date was made for the participant to attend the stroke rehabilitation gym. 6.2.2 Selecting and refining the exercises In order to meet the primary aim of the study, the exercises needed to be designed to be performed not only in their entirety (WP) but also in component parts (PP). In addition, the exercise parts for the PP regime needed to make sense to people with stroke, as they would be required to practice independently. All the exercises would need to be undertaken safely in the home environment without supervision. The exercises that were explored in this initial pilot phase were designed be as 119 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology functionally relevant as possible. In considering the actions required for the tasks being piloted, it was clear that the tasks would fall into a closed skill category with elements of body transport or body manipulation (Gentile 1987). Some exercises appeared to lend themselves better to being broken down into component parts than others. The exercises piloted at this stage are listed in table 6.1. 6.2.3 Pilot Exercise Procedure – in Hospital Participants who had agreed to take part in this in-hospital, pilot exercise development phase of the study attended the stroke rehabilitation gym on an agreed date. The principal investigator spent the first five or ten minutes of their visit building a rapport and becoming familiar with the individual’s history of stroke, their experience of rehabilitation and exploring if there were tasks that were still problematic. Following this, exploration of potential exercises and piloting of potential outcome measures was undertaken. Selection and critique of outcome measures is reported in 6.3. Participants initially undertook one or two repetitions of the exercise in its entirety (WP) to ensure understanding of the nature of the task. Following this, exploration of PP structure was undertaken. The in-hospital pilot exercise phase investigated the effects of : The number of components contributing to PP structure of exercises The number of repetitions How feedback could be gained when the exercise was undertaken without supervision The perceived functional relevance to the participant 120 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Exercise P1 Standing up from dining (no arms) chair (height 46cm) With no support in front With zimmer frame in front With chair in front P2 Sitting Down onto chair With no support in front With zimmer frame in front With chair in front P3 Stepping sideways Affected foot crossing over in front Affected foot crossing over behind P4 Stepping up Different height blocks (from 2 – 12cm) With and without support on the unaffected side With and without support in front P5 Stepping down Different height blocks (from 2 – 12cm) With and without support on the unaffected side With and without support in front P6 Touching floor P7 Pinch Grip, transport and release - Thumb and varying other fingers 3cm square block 4cm diameter ball 2cm diameter marble P8 Forearm pronation – supination With various objects – sticks, cones, bottle P9 Making shapes with putty P10 Taking objects out of pot P11 Grasp tennis ball, transport and release P12 Grasp plastic cup from table, transport and release Empty or half full of water Start position varied from close to far Elbow on / off table Transport to mouth / transport to new end position P13 Bridging Arms out to side / across chest P14 ½ Bridging Table 6.1. Exercises attempted intervention during development of exercise 121 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.2.4 Analysis of pilot exercises in-hospital and impact on final pilot protocol Analysis of the pilot exercises in-hospital led to a number of conclusions that subsequently influenced the community pilot. It was found that: For a number of the exercises initially, the number of component parts were too numerous and too confusing for the participants. For example – for exercise P4 – stepping up; seven components had been considered (weight shift, knee bend, heel raise, toe push, knee flexion, foot plant, weight transfer). Reducing the number of components to two or three, facilitated practice of discrete components that made sense to the participants. Structuring variable practice of different components, out of sequence, detracted from the comprehension of the exercise. Blocking the practice to repetitive practice of one component prior to moving to the next component enhanced understanding. Given that participants would practice without supervision, blocked practice of PP or WP was considered to make more intuitive sense and potentially could increase compliance. The number of repetitions was set at 10 per exercise initially, however for some participants, this resulted in fatigue. It was therefore decided to commence the exercises in the community pilot at between six and 10 repetitions. During the four weeks of intervention, it was envisaged that the number of repetitions would be increased, on an individual basis. Exercises P6 (touching floor), P9 (rolling putty), P10 (objects from pot), P13 (bridging) and P14 (½ bridging) were too difficult to structure as component parts that were deemed relevant. These exercises were therefore discarded. Two participants lost balance during exercise P6 (touching floor). Given that safe practice of the exercise regime was paramount, the decision to discard this exercise was vindicated. Similar safety concerns with exercise P1 and P2 122 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology (standing up and sitting down) using the option of a zimmer frame, resulted in discarding the zimmer frame option. For participants with minimal recovery of the upper limb, exercises P7, P8, P11 and P12 were problematic. It was decided to include an element of upper limb weight-bearing with these exercises to allow participants to undertake at least one component of the exercise, to allow smaller amplitude movement and to undertake mental rehearsal of the remaining components. Of the nine remaining exercises that had not been discarded, it was felt that there was overlap between P7(pinch grip), P11 (tennis ball) and P12 (plastic cup) and that not all exercises should be included. P12 was felt to be most functionally relevant and this was therefore retained for the community pilot. Exercise P3 was also discarded, because of some minor safety concerns relating to tripping. Following the in-hospital pilot, six exercises (P1, P2, P4, P5, P8 and P12) that could be practiced as PP or WP were undertaken in a small community pilot. The exercises were feasible to structure so that practice was undertaken in a manner that was meaningful to the pilot participants. Documentation to support the exercises and the practice regime in the community pilot was developed (appendix II). The pilot community intervention and pilot of outcome measures is reported in 6.4. 123 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.3 Determining Screening Tests and Outcome Measures to be used in final protocol This section reports the screening tests and outcome measures that had been considered to be used for the final study. Prior to exploring the utility of the potential outcome measures for this study, a review of related literature was undertaken. Each outcome measure is reported with an associated summary of available evidence relating to psychometric properties. A whole battery of outcome measures were tested for use as the final trial was exploratory in nature and although the aim was to encapsulate different dimensions of impairment, activity and participation some dimensions were tested with more than one outcome measure. 6.3.1 The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) A brief screening test to establish cognitive ability was required to ensure that any person recruited to the study would have the ability to undertake the required tasks, particularly if they did not have a carer to assist with exercises. The MMSE was first published in 1975 as a short test of “the cognitive aspects of mental functions” and is scored between 0 – 30 (Folstein et al, 1975). A cut off score of 23 is generally accepted as indicating cognitive impairment (Anthony et al, 1982; Tombaugh and McIntyre 1992). The MMSE is brief to administer, taking around five to ten minutes (Folstein et al, 1975), does not require any specialist equipment, has standardised instructions, and has been tested for it’s psychometric properties, therefore making it an appealing tool in research when a battery of outcome measures are to be used. This therefore has made it a popular measure in many studies with stroke patients (Pederson et al, 1996b, Anderson et al, 1995). While some criticism has been levelled at the MMSE for being uni-dimensional, Jones and Gallo (2000) argue that inclusion of tasks that test memory, orientation, attention and concentration make the test multidimensional. 124 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology During the development of the MMSE it was tested with 206 people with various conditions including dementia, depression, schizophrenia and in 63 people without impairment. It was found that the MMSE discriminated between people with and without cognitive impairment and that a mean score for normal elderly was 27.6. Additionally the originators found that the MMSE possessed both intra-rater reliability, inter-rater reliability over 24 hours and test-retest reliability over 28 days (Folstein et al, 1975). Many of the studies investigating the psychometric properties of the MMSE have been established in non-stroke populations. In the limited papers establishing psychometric properties of the MMSE with stroke the findings have not been conclusive. Nys et al, (2005) reported no definitive cut-off score for the MMSE in stroke patients, while an earlier study by Blake et al, (2002) recommended a cutoff score of 24 indicating cognitive impairment with good specificity of 88%. Varied scores on the MMSE have been reported with healthy populations. Depending on age, scores based on lower quartile values, normal cut off scores of 29 (age 40 – 49), 28 (age 50 – 79), and 26 (age over 80) have been reported (Bleecker et al, 1988). These data were however, established with a relatively small sample of 194 subjects (Bleecker et al, 1988). In a much larger study of over 18,000 participants, that took age and educational attainment into account, slightly lower values were reported. Total MMSE scores declined with age from a lower quartile score of between 25 - 29 (associated with between eight years school education to degree level education) for 40 – 50 year olds and lower quartile scores of between 22 – 26 for 80 year olds with the same educational experience (Crum et al, 1993). 125 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology The use of the MMSE as a brief screen for cognitive impairment was appropriate for this study as the aim was not to diagnose specificity of cognitive impairments, but to ascertain that participants had sufficient cognitive capacity to participate, for example to follow a three-stage command. The use of the MMSE in this way has therefore made it a popular screening measure in many studies with stroke patients (Anderson et al, 1995; Pedersen et al, 1996b). 6.3.2 The Functional Reach Test (FRT) One of the major aspects of the study was to enable and encourage participants to practise exercises without therapist supervision in their own home. Many people with stroke complain of on-going balance problems (Wade et al, 1992; Green et al, 2002; Tyson et al, 2006) and some of the exercises would be challenging balance and potentially could lead to falls. It was, therefore, important to attempt to screen out potential participants with a higher risk of falls. A simple screening test was required and while most of the literature related to nonstroke specific populations, there were still important parameters that could be applied to this study. A variety of clinical measures have been developed to assess balance and infer falls risk and these include the Berg Balance Scale (Berg et al, 1989) and the Tinetti Performance Oriented Mobility Assessment (Tinetti et al, 1986). While both of these measures have been demonstrated to be valid and reliable, they take around 15 - 20 minutes to perform and therefore, when forming part of an initial assessment battery, this would be too time-consuming. 126 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology An alternative tool is the Functional Reach Test (FRT) (Duncan et al, 1990). The FRT is performed by asking the subject to stand next to a wall, lift one arm straight in front of the body to shoulder level and then stretch as far forward as possible without moving the feet or touching the wall. The forward reach distance is measured from start to end position, usually with a ruler attached to the wall. The ability to reach less than a distance of six inches (15cm) has been shown to be predictive of falls (Duncan et al, 1992a) although this has not been replicated in stroke. This test only takes a minute to perform and has been shown to have interobserver and test retest reliability (Duncan et al, 1990, Duncan et al, 1992a) and concurrent validity (Bernhardt et al, 1998). While a number of criticisms have been levelled at the FRT, including that there is a need to standardise reach strategies (Wernick-Robinson et al, 1999), that the factors contributing to poor balance are not assessed (Perrell et al, 2001), and that sensitivity has not yet been established (Duncan et al, 1992a), the FRT is still a quick, cheap and easily administered screening test for falls risk. An FRT of less than 17.5cm has been shown to identify physical frailty, limited mobility and limitations in ADL (Weiner et al, 1992). It has also been established that a history of two or more falls within a prior six months period can also classify a person as a “recurrent faller” (Duncan et al, 1992a, Shumway Cook et al, 1997). In a study of community-dwelling elderly (over 70 years) men, it was shown that there was significant difference between the FRT of “fallers” <15 cm and the FRT of “nonfallers” >25 cm (Duncan et al, 1992a). As a screening measure for falls risk it was therefore decided to ensure that subjects demonstrated acceptable results for both FRT ability (reach greater than 15cm) and that they did not have a history of more than two or more falls in the preceding six months. 127 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.3.3 Mixed Measures of Impairment and Activity Limitation The initial exploration of the potential functional task exercises, indicated that the intervention was likely to consist of exercises targeting at both an impairment and activity level, therefore it was important to have a measure in the battery of outcome measures that reflected this. Many mixed measures of impairment and activity limitation exist, however some of these are intimately linked to treatment approaches (Fugl-Meyer et al, 1975) which can bias the activities being tested, some take a considerable amount of time (around 60 minutes) to administer, such as the Fugl-Meyer Assessment (Fugl-Meyer et al, 1975), the Chedoke McMaster Stroke Assessment (Gowland et al, 1993) and the Stroke Rehabilitation Assessment of Movement or STREAM, (Daley et al, 1997, Daley et al, 1999) or require a considerable amount of equipment such as the Chedoke McMaster Stroke Assessment (Gowland et al, 1993). It was therefore decided to pilot two brief measures of impairment and activity limitation that required little specialist equipment and were suitable for use in a community setting: the Rivermead Motor Assessment (RMA) and the Motor Assessment Scale (MAS). 6.3.3.1 The Rivermead Motor Assessment (RMA) The Rivermead Motor Assessment was first developed in 1979 by a physiotherapist and clinical psychologist and published as the Rivermead Stroke Assessment (Lincoln and Leadbitter 1979). It was developed using a population of 51 young (under 65) people with stroke to provide a short, valid and reliable assessment of post-stroke physical recovery. A dichotomous yes (1 point), no (0 points) scoring system was developed and the RMA was divided into three sub-sections Gross Function (13 items), Leg and Trunk (10 items) and Arm (15 items). One strength of 128 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology the RMA is that it was reported, initially, as being broadly hierarchical in nature, after three consecutive fails in one section the assessor moves to the next section. While it has been reported as fairly brief to administer, the time can be up to 45 minutes (Lincoln and Leadbitter 1979). The RMA has been reported as having scalability, inter-rater reliability and testretest reliability by the originators in a reasonable sample size of young (under 65) stroke patients (Lincoln and Leadbitter 1979). However scalability in older people with stroke has been challenged, with recommendations that the hierarchy of items may not be appropriate, and attempts at higher level items should be undertaken (Adams et al, 1997; Kurtaiş et al, 2009). Sensitivity to change has been demonstrated by all three sub-sections of the RMA (Kurtaiş et al, 2009). Concurrent validity with the Barthel Index has been reported by Endres et al, (1990), and strong correlations indicative of concurrent validity have also been found with the Trunk Control Test and Motricity Index (Collin and Wade 1990). 6.3.3.2 The Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) The Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) was developed to overcome some of the shortcomings of existing measures of the time, with the aim to measure the progress of stroke patients (Carr et al, 1985). The original MAS consisted of eight everyday motor activities scored on a seven point ordinal scale and a ninth item to measure general muscle tone. Operational definitions were provided for each item to promote consistency in application. 129 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology In the original reporting of the MAS, substantial correlations and high levels of percentage agreements were reported to substantiate the inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the MAS with video data of people with stroke being tested on the MAS items, and samples of twenty physiotherapists and one physiotherapist respectively (Carr et al, 1985). Reliability of general muscle tone however was not assessed by Carr et al, (1985). High correlations for all eight MAS items (r 0.92 – 1.0) but not for tone (r = 0.29) were found in a population of 24 people with chronic stroke (mean time post-stroke 12 months) and using two raters, (Poole and Whitney 1988). Subsequent publications have dropped the general tonus item from the MAS, and while the eight item test is a “modified MAS”, it is consistently described as MAS in the stroke rehabilitation literature and any discussion related to MAS in this thesis will refer to the eight item version. The reliability of MAS has been established in a number of small-scale studies. Loewen and Anderson (1988) video-taped seven people with stroke undertaking MAS and presented these data to 14 physiotherapists, one month apart. The MAS was found to have good to excellent intra-rater reliability and inter-rater reliability (Loewen and Anderson 1988). Various components of validity of the MAS have been established. Content validity of the MAS has not been reported, although the items included in the MAS are said to be reflective of everyday tasks and independent of any treatment philosophy (Carr et al, 1985). The MAS could, however be argued to be linked with the Motor Relearning Programme (MRP) (Carr and Shepherd 1988), as many of the items included on the MAS are addressed within the MRP. Acceptable levels of 130 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology concurrent validity have been established against the Fugl-Meyer Assessment (Poole and Whitney 1988; Malouin et al, 1994) and a newly developed outcome measure - the Mobility Scale for Acute Stroke Patients (Simondsen et al, 2003). In a retrospective audit of 70 stroke patients notes, it was reported that the MAS was responsive to change with no patients regressing from initial admission scores and with mean increase in scores on each item of at least one point (Dean and MacKay 1992). These findings are questioned by English et al, (2006) who found large floor and ceiling effects of the MAS particularly for the three arm items, although the gait item was found to be more sensitive. There has been recent interest regarding the properties of the three upper limb items on the MAS, with these items identified as the UL-MAS subscale (Lannin 2004). Malouin et al, (1994) found the arm items from MAS to have good agreement with arm items from the FMA indicating concurrent validity. The testretest reliability of the three arm items from MAS was found to be good, with scores of Kendal’s T between 0.94 – 1.00 (Loewen and Anderson 1988). Lannin (2004) established that the UL-MAS could be used as a single item score and argued that a single composite UL-MAS score would be useful in research and clinical practice. The suggestion of using the UL-MAS as a valid measure was proposed after data collection on this study had commenced, therefore it was not considered as an independent test of upper limb function. 131 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.3.3.3 Piloting the Rivermead Motor Assessment and the Motor Assessment Scale From the literature available at the time of planning the study, it appeared that both the MAS and the RMA had established psychometric properties, were fairly brief to administer, used little equipment and could be feasible to undertake in the community setting. Both outcome measures were piloted with four people with stroke in the out-patient stroke rehabilitation gym at Astley Ainslie Hospital. While both measures were found to be feasible to use with no requirement for large pieces of specialist equipment, the MAS was completed more rapidly (maximum time 21 minutes), while the RMA took longer, particularly in subjects with upper extremity recovery (maximum time 32 minutes). The operational definitions for MAS were also felt to be clearer and the seven item ordinal scale gave more potential for greater sensitivity in identifying recovery. On a pragmatic level, it was therefore decided to use the MAS for the definitive trial 6.3.3.4 The Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) At the time of developing the study, a simple measure of upper limb activity was sought as the outcome measures already identified did not have comprehensive items relating to arm function, that could be used as independent measures. While simple tests such as the nine hole peg test (Kellor et al, 1971; Mathiowetz et al, 1985) exist, a substantial amount of hand dexterity and UL recovery is required to undertake the test. Having considered the available measures, the FAT (Parker et al, 1986) was considered to be a reasonable measure to pilot as it was concise, administered in under five minutes, and measured five fundamental aspects of arm and hand function. Items are scored on a dichotomous scale of zero or one giving a total possible maximum score of five (Parker et al, 1986; Wade 1989). 132 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Originally, the FAT included seven items (Wade et al, 1983) and was found to be useful for detecting change in upper limb recovery post-stroke. The original measure was reduced to five items by Parker et al, (1986) and appears to have been used in this way ever since. In a study of 187 people at three and six months after stroke, the FAT was found to detect functional change whereas the nine hole peg test (9HPT) did not (Parker et al, 1986). It has been noted however, that the addition of a 9HPT once a person scores fully on the FAT can identify further recovery (Heller et al, 1987). The FAT does have floor and ceiling effects, it does not give an indication of arm strength and is not particularly sensitive (Wade 1989). A person scoring four or five on the FAT however, is likely to have relatively good functional recovery of their hemiplegic arm and for that reason it was decided to use the FAT in this study. 6.3.4 Global Measures of Activity Limitation 6.3.4.1 The Barthel Index (BI) A simple global measure of disability was required and the two main contenders for this were the Barthel Index (BI) and the Functional Independence Measure (FIM). Both measures aim to record actual not potential function (Keith et al, 1987; Collin et al, 1988). Both measures use an ordinal scale, although some proponents argue that the greater number of categories on the FIM (seven) makes it more responsive than the Barthel (between two and four categories). The responsiveness however, has been found to be similar between the FIM and BI in stroke populations (van der Putten et al, 1999; Hsueh et al, 2002). 133 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology The FIM consists of 18 items assessing six functional areas and takes around 45 minutes to administer. Rigorous development procedures for the FIM were undertaken with 114 healthcare professionals from 8 disciplines with a sample of over 100 patients (Keith et al, 1987). The psychometric properties of FIM have been well established with stroke, with FIM possessing face and content validity (Keith et al, 1987), concurrent validity with BI (Hsueh et al, 2002; Kwon et al, 2004) and with the Rankin Scale (Kwon 2004) and good predictive validity (Timbeck et al, 2003). When used with neurological populations FIM demonstrates good intra-rater, interrater and test-retest reliability (Ottenbacher et al, 1996), although stability of the social cognition domain when administered over two separate occasions by two different raters with a population of people with chronic stroke was only fair to adequate (Daving et al, 2001). One of the major disadvantages to using the FIM however, when only one person would be undertaking outcome measures, was the administration time (at least 45 minutes), the costs and the need for training and accreditation to use the FIM. The Barthel Index (BI) was first published in 1958 (Mahoney et al, 1958) and was tested with 144 patients, of whom 68% (n= 99) had neurological conditions. The BI scored from 0 – 100 and originated as a measure of independence, primarily in selfcare (Mahoney et al, 1958; Tennant et al, 1996). In the original paper it was claimed that a person scoring 100 was independent, although this has been shown subsequently not to be the case, with floor and ceiling effects noted (van der Putten 1989, Duncan et al, 1997; Turner-Stokes and Turner-Stokes 1997). A major criticism of the BI is that it primarily focuses on bed mobility and walking ability with 134 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology only crude assessment of upper limb function or more complex tasks of activities of daily living (Kelly Hayes et al 1998). The scoring of 0 – 100 on the original BI was amended to provide a 0 – 20 point scale by Collin et al (1988). Both the 100 point and 20 point BI are in use today, with no distinction made in the title of the test. The 20 point BI was tested for reliability with 25 patients during four different modes of administration (interview of patient, interview of nurse, actual testing by a trained nurse or by an Occupational Therapist) and a close agreement between all four methods was found. It was noted that the middle scores on complex, multidimensional tasks such as feeding and toileting were the most difficult to get agreement on and following this study, the authors expanded the guidelines for administration (Collin et al, 1988). Test-retest reliability of the BI in a small sample (n=22) of people with late-stage stroke has been established (Green et al, 2001). 6.3.4.2 Piloting the Barthel Index The BI has been used extensively in stroke research and despite arguments being made to use the FIM in preference to the BI in the UK (Turner-Stokes et al, 1997), there is no clear standard measure of “disability” (Wade and Collin 1987). When deciding which measure of disability to use, a pragmatic decision was taken to use the BI as it required minimal equiment, had established validity when administered by interview (Wyller et al, 1995) and would be brief to administer within the battery of outcome measures. 135 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology During pilot procedures with four people with stroke, the BI was completed within five minutes. The principal investigator [GB] (who was already familiar with the content) developed standardised prompts to try to ensure as accurate capture of the patients ability level as possible (for example – “you have said you are independent going to the toilet – are you able to manage your clothes? Zips? Do you manage one handed or do you need a little bit of help?”). The reasons for selecting to use the Barthel Index are presented above. In addition, the vast majority of papers relating to research into stroke rehabilitation use either the BI or the FIM, so in order to draw comparisons with previous work it was prudent to use one of these measures. However, the BI does predominantly measure independence in activity in and around the home setting, therefore a more global measure of activity, which incorporated social activities, was also required. 6.3.4.3 The Frenchay Activity Index (FAI) The Frenchay Activity Index (FAI) was developed to use with Stroke Patients as a measure that gave more information regarding lifestyle activities rather than selfcare (Holbrook and Skilbeck 1983). The FAI measures the broad domains of “Domestic Chores”, “Leisure and Work” and “Outdoor Activity” within the past six months. Although it was initially developed for the period of acute care and in-patient rehabilitation, it has been used with later stage stroke patients (Holbrook and Skilbeck 1983; Schuling et al, 1993; Wyller et al, 1996; Green et al, 2001). Maximum score on the FAI is 45, using a four point scoring system from zero to three (Wade et al, 1985) which is a modification from the original one to four scale (Holbrook and Skilbeck 1983). 136 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Factor Analysis has been argued as one manner of determining construct validity of an outcome measure and all items on the FAI have been shown to have a high degree of communality indicating the items are associated with a single concept (Wade et al, 1985). While the ordinal scoring is quite crude, it has been demonstrated that the FAI is able to reflect changes in activity levels pre- and poststroke (Wade et al, 1985; Schuling et al, 2006). A Dutch version of the FAI was tested with 185 people at 26 weeks post-stroke and demonstrated the homogeneity of the scale and a “substantial” relationship with the BI (Pearsons r 0.66) (Schuling et al, 2006). Various aspects of reliability have been established with the FAI. Bland and Altman methods to look at agreement between the FAI completed twice with an interval of seven days showed good levels of agreement with a mean difference of 0.6 and with over half the total scores within two points of each other (Green et al, 2001). A study to investigate whether people with stroke perceived their activity to be the same as the perception of their carer showed good to very good agreement in six items (kappa 0.61 – 1) and moderate agreement in seven items (kappa 0.41- 0.6), with a tendency for carers to score patients lower, particularly on domestic chores, than they score themselves (Wyller et al, 1996). Piloting the FAI with four people with stroke showed that it was administered in around five to ten minutes, was easily understandable with no requirement for standardised prompt questions. 137 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.3.5 Measuring aspects of mobility and balance 6.3.5.1 The Timed Up and Go (TUG) A simple measure of mobility including balance was required and the Timed Up and Go (TUG) seemed a suitable measure for use in the home setting as it is conducted with the patient rising from a chair, walking three metres turning and returning back to sit down in the chair (Podsiadlo and Richardson 1991). It was originally developed with 60 elderly patients (mean age 79.5 years) of whom 23 had sustained a stroke and the time to complete the TUG was between 10 and 240 seconds. Testretest (ICC 0.95, Ng and Hui-Chan 2005) and inter-rater reliability were reported as very good (ICC – 0.99, Podsiadlo and Richardson 1991; ICC – 0.98, Shumway Cook et al, 2000). A good correlation with Berg Balance Scale (r= -0.81), gait speed (r = 0.61) and Barthel Index (r =-0.78) has also been found, indicating aspects of concurrent validity (Podsiadlo and Richardson 1991). A cut-off time of 30 seconds has been reported to discriminate between “fast” and “slow” walkers, as well as the ability to independently walk outside (Podsiadlo and Richardson 1991; Freter and Fruchter 2000). Furthermore, Nikolaus et al, (1996) reported that in a sample of elderly people a mean TUG time of 31.7 seconds was found in people living in a nursing home, compared to a mean TUG time of 20.6 of community dwelling elderly. Given some of the anticipated environmental restrictions of performing outcome measures in a community setting, a development of the TUG to incorporate timing of component parts using a multi-memory stopwatch (Wall et al, 2000) appeared to be an expedient modification of the TUG. This modification was developed over a 10m 138 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology walking course with the lap timer being used to measure discrete events consisting of sit-to-stand time, walking speed, turning time and sitting down time. Recently the reliability and aspects of validity have been established with the stopwatch method of measuring component parts over a three metre walking course (Botolfson et al, 2008). 6.3.5.2 Familiarisation with timing the Timed Up and Go The timing of components of the TUG was piloted with four subjects performing the TUG while being video-recorded on three trials. Real-time recording of the six TUG component parts was undertaken and these data were compared to video data of the same TUG performance two weeks later. Raw data are available in appendix IV. The results showed very good intra-rater reliability between actual and video data with ICC 2,1 all over 0.98. 6.3.5.3 Step Test A dynamic test of balance, including the ability to balance on the hemiplegic leg while moving the other leg, was included as the exercise intervention was likely to include a task to challenge dynamic balance. The Step Test (ST) was developed with a sample of 41 community dwelling older subjects and 41 people with stroke. It was designed to be a simple, clinically relevant test of dynamic single leg stance (Hill et al, 1996). The test consists of stepping one foot on and off a 7.5cm block as many times as possible within 15 seconds (Hill et al, 1996; Bernhardt et al, 1998; Tyson and Connell 2009). Normative values for healthy elderly was mean steps 17.67 + 3.22 (right leg) and 17.37 + 3.03 (left leg). In the stroke population, mean 139 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology steps with the unaffected leg was 6.95 + 4.55, mean steps with the affected leg was 6.39 + 4.53. Overbalancing while performing the ST occurred in 10% of people with stroke, therefore safety measures in the form of stand-by assistance should be adopted (Hill et al, 1996). Furthermore, due to a slight practice effect being noted on the ST, it is recommended that at least one practice session is allowed (Hill et al, 1996, Tyson 2007). Psychometric properties of the ST have been established by the originators and other investigators. Hill et al, (1996) established very good test-retest reliability in healthy elderly (ICC 0.9 – 0.94) and people with stroke (ICC 0.88 – 0.97). Good correlations have also been demonstrated with the FRT, gait velocity and stride length, (Hill et al, 1996, Bernhardt et al, 1998) and the balance item on MAS (Bernhardt et al, 1998) indicating aspects of criterion related (concurrent) validity. In terms of predictive validity of the ST in stroke, each additional step with the affected leg corresponded to a 0.07 m/s – 0.09 m/s increase in gait speed, while an additional step with the unaffected leg corresponded to a 0.07 m/s – 0.08 m/s increase (Mercer et al, 2009a; Mercer et al 2009b). The ST was found to be responsive to change in performance in the first eight weeks following stroke with a significant improvement noted for both ST with the affected and unaffected leg (Bernhardt et al, 1998). One drawback of the ST is the potential for a floor effect in cases where single leg stance is not possible. 140 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.3.6 Measuring Aspects of Mood It is well recognised that undertaking exercise may have a beneficial effect on mood (Lai et al, 2006; Bassey 2000), which may or may not be associated with functional improvements post-stroke. It was, therefore, important to include a simple measure of mood. While a number of measures are available such as the General Health Questionnaire and the Beck Depression Inventory, after consideration of the psychometric properties and pragmatic issues of administration, it was decided to use the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale as it takes under five minutes to complete and is easy to score (Snaith 2003). 6.3.6.1 The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) was originally designed as a tool for self-assessment of mood in non-hospitalised patients such as people attending an out-patient department for a chronic condition but not a psychiatric disorder (Zigmond and Snaith 1983). Although the concepts of “anxiety” and “depression” were not defined in the original paper, the authors stated that they “distinguish[ed] between the concepts of anxiety and depression” and took measures to ensure the original items in each sub-scale derived from appropriate theoretical constructs. The HADS was originally tested with 98 out-patients aged between 16 – 65 years. Each sub-scale consists of seven items scored from zero to three making a total possible score of 21 for each subscale (Zigmond and Snaith 1983). A score of over 11-15 indicates “moderate cases” and scores over 16 identifies “severe cases” (Snaith and Zigmond 1994). Within the stroke population it has been noted that an optimal cut-off score for the total HADS is 11, and a cut-off of 8 for the HADS-D (Aben et al, 2002). 141 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology The HADS has been recommended for use in studies of mood for the general population (Herrmann 1997; Crawford et al, 2001) and for people with stroke (Aben et al, 2002; Bennett et al, 2006). The psychometric properties of HADS have been established, for example it has been found to show good internal consistency (Zigmond and Snaith 1983; Cameron et al, 2008), has construct validity as shown by factor structure analyses (Zigmond and Snaith 1983; Bennett et al, 2006); shows discriminant validity (Cameron et al, 2008) and is responsive to change in symptoms (Bjelland et al, 2002; Cameron et al, 2008). Some weaknesses have also been identified, for example the item on the depression subscale “I feel as if I am slowed down” is problematic for reflecting depression as, for people with stroke, it is also central to the physical condition (Johnston et al, 2000). Overall however, it was felt that the use of HADS to provide an indication of mood was appropriate for this study. 6.3.7 Measuring Quality of Life Quality of Life (QoL) is a complex entity that is represented differently to different individuals. Despite there being no consensus for a definition of QoL, it is accepted that it is multidimensional, encompassing social domains alongside physical and mental domains (Buck et al, 2000). QoL is not a static phenomenon and may change over time (de Haan et al, 1993). A deterioration in QoL has been reported in people with stroke at three and four years after the initial event (Patel et al, 2007; Niemi et al 1988). The most significant declines contributing to a reduced QoL have 142 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology been reported as occurring in domains such as self-care needs, personal relationships, coping with life events, home management and recreation (Saladin, 2000). Quality of Life (QoL) is potentially best assessed on an individual basis using indepth interviews, (de Haan et al, 1993; Tennant 1996), however this methodology was not felt to be appropriate for this study due to time constraints. A number of standardised questionnaires were therefore considered for use, to capture an overview of QoL. Many generic QoL measures have been criticised for not including behaviours or domains more specific to stroke (de Haan et al, 1993; Williams et al, 1999a, Buck et al, 2000), therefore two stroke specific QoL measures were considered for use: the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) (Duncan et al, 1999) and the Stroke Specific Quality of Life (SSQoL) (Williams et al, 1999b). 6.3.7.1 The Stroke Impact Scale The Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) was one of the first stroke specific QoL measure to have been published. It was developed to assess a number of areas considered important for QoL post-stroke. The 64 items on the SIS evaluate the perceived functional effects of motor impairments, as well as the domains of emotion, communication, memory, thinking ability and social role function (Duncan et al, 1999a). The SIS was refined in 2003 to become the SIS v3.0 (Duncan et al, 2003). It has been found to be valid to administer the SIS via telephone, post, or via proxy response (Edwards and O’Connell 2003; Duncan et al, 2002a; Duncan et al, 2002b, Kwon et al 2006). Furthermore, recent recommendations have been made for meaningful minimal detectable changes in each physical domain (Lin et al 2010a). 143 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology These are changes of 4.5, 5.9, 9.2 and 17.8 points on the domains of mobility, ADL, strength and hand function respectively (Lin et al 2010a). Since the development of the SIS, a number of studies have established its psychometric properties. Internal consistency showed very good acceptability with Cronbach from 0.83 to 0.90 (Duncan et al, 1999a). Test–retest reliability over one week was also found to be good in all domains (ICC 0.7 – 0.92) except for emotion which was moderate (ICC 0.57) (Duncan et al, 1999a). Content validity was established through involvement of people with stroke (n=32), care-givers (n=23) and stroke experts (n=9) (Duncan et al, 2001). Concurrent validity has been established against existing measures such as the SF36 and the SIS has been found to cover a broader and more relevant stroke-specific domains than the SF36 (Lai et al, 2003) While there is the potential for floor effects in some domains such as hand function or ceiling effects in the communication domain (Duncan et al, 1999b), overall the SIS has been found to show good levels of responsiveness to change in condition (Lin et al, 2010b). 6.3.7.2 The Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale (SSQoL) The Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale (SSQoL) was developed from interviews with 34 people with stroke within the first six months after stroke. The interviews allowed the most common domains affected by stroke to be identified and typical activities within the domains to be ascertained. Following piloting and refinement 144 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology the definitve SSQoL consisted of 49 items in 12 domains, was scored on a five point Likert scale and, similarly to the SIS, questions related to how responders felt their stroke had affected them within the past week. In a sample of 71 relatively young (mean age 61 years) people at least one month after stroke, the SSQoL was found to be significantly associated with Health Related QOL and to be more sensitive to change whereas the use of the generic Short Form 36 (SF36) showed no association (Williams et al, 1999b). In testing proxy versus patient response to SSQoL, proxies were found to score all domains lower than the patients, with the largest discrepancy in domains of mood, thinking and energy (Williams et al 2006). This trait is commonly seen when gaining proxy responses and the same responder should be involved when undertaking testing on multiple occasions. 6.3.7.3 Comparisons between the Stroke Impact Scale and the Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale. The SIS and the SSQoL have been cited as being the most comprehensive stroke specific QoL measures, providing comprehensive evaluation of domains meaningful to people post-stroke (Salter et al, 2008; Lin et al 2010b). Both the measures have been investigated to determine responsiveness and aspects of criterion validity, but only one recent study has compared the psychometric properties of the two measures (Lin et al, 2010b). 74 people at least six months post-stroke participating in an RCT of UL rehabilitation were assessed on a battery of outcome measures, including the SIS and SSQoL. 145 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology While most SIS responsiveness scores were low to medium for pre- to posttreatment measures, this was better than the responsiveness of the SSQoL where no response was found despite patient self-reports of change. The Standardised Response Mean (SRM) of the SIS =0.5, 95%CI = 0.27 – 0.78, compared to SRM of SSQoL 0.14, 95% CI -0.7 - -0.39. The SIS hand function domain was the only domain on any scale to show medium responsiveness, all other domains were low to moderate. SIS hand function also demonstrated better concurrent validity with good correlations between SIS and criterion measures of FMA and between SIS and the Motor Activity Log, whereas SSQoL UL domain demonstrated fair correlations. A similar pattern was found for concurrent validity with the FAI (Lin et al, 2010b). While these are interesting findings, this information was not available at the time of planning the current study and therefore exploration of the use of both outcome measures was undertaken as part of pilot procedures. 6.3.7.4 Piloting the Stroke Impact Scale and the Stroke Specific Quality of Life Scale At the time of developing the study there were very few papers published supporting either the psychometric properties of the SIS or the SSQoL, although in 2010, it is clear that the SIS has been more rigorously tested. The investigator had felt prior to piloting that the SSQoL probably had broader coverage of domains that would be relevant to people with stroke, but as both the SIS and SSQoL were new measures it seemed appropriate to determine if they were acceptable to potential participants. The SIS and SSQoL were piloted with four people with stroke. It was found that once the scoring system had been explained to the participants, there were no items that appeared confusing. One participant needed further explanation that there was 146 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology no “correct” answer, but that the questionnaires sought to identify how well the person perceived they had recovered from their stroke. Both measures took between 20 – 30 minutes to complete. One item on the SSQoL however required a response to a question related to sexual activity “I have had sex less often than I would like”. This question, or the prospect of answering this type of question caused distress to two of the four pilot subjects (one subject on direct questioning and one subject who had been left to complete the SSQoL without supervision omitted to answer that question and, indeed, the whole page of questions). Due to the potential that offence at, or reluctance to answer a question on SSQoL relating to sexual activity might result in a considerable amount of missing data, a decision was taken to use the SIS in the final study. 6.3.8 The final selection of outcome measures Following review of relevant literature as indicated in the preceding sections the final outcome measures that were selected for use in the final pilot are listed in table 6.2. It was anticipated that administration of the battery of outcome measures would take around 90 minutes to two hours, depending on the ability of the participant. 147 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Test Used as Screening Test The Mini Mental State Examination Functional Reach Test Used in Outcome battery The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale The Motor Assessment Scale The Frenchay Arm Test The Barthel Index The Frenchay Activities Index The Step Test The Timed Up and Go The Stroke Impact Scale. Table 6.2. 6.4 Screening tests and outcome measures to be used in definitive trial Pilot of Exercise Intervention and Outcome Measures in the Community A community pilot of a four week home exercise programme of the six exercises identified following the hospital pilot phase was undertaken to ascertain whether any further refinements to the exercise interventions or the battery of outcome measures would be required. The exercises are detailed in appendix II. There were two main aims of the community pilot phase. The first aim was to explore the feasibility of undertaking the exercises structured as PP or WP in the community setting. The second aim was to ensure the battery of outcome measures were suitable for administration in the community setting and to further familiarise the outcome assessor [GB] with carrying out the outcome measurements. 148 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.4.1 Methodology of Community pilot of exercise intervention Six participants with stroke were recruited to the community pilot phase, via referral from the Senior Physiotherapist. This was therefore a sample of convenience, selected to provide a range of ability and not to compromise the definitive trial. The pilot phase was exploratory in nature, and considered the viability of the practice regime and gained feedback on the utility of the documentation. Subject characteristics are presented in table 6.3. Participant group identifier J D E G A K PP1 WP2 PP3 WP4 PP5 WP6 Age 68 62.3 61.1 58.7 57.8 64 time since stroke (weeks) 56 28 37 50 34 39 side of Dominant hemiplegia hand L R L L L L R R R L R R Table 6.3. Characteristics of Community Pilot participants Community pilot participants were no longer receiving formal rehabilitation, but had all previously undergone stroke rehabilitation at Astley Ainslie Hospital, Edinburgh, a tertiary rehabilitation centre. Inclusion criteria ensured that pilot participants had a diagnosis of stroke as confirmed by CT or MRI scan; were over 18, were > six months post-stroke, were no longer receiving formal rehabilitation and were able to provide informed consent. Potential participants were identified by physiotherapists in the Stroke Rehabilitation team. An initial contact by phone, during which the aim of the pilot was explained, was followed by providing written information relating to 149 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology the community pilot phase, an informed consent form and a return envelope. Once recruited to the community pilot phase a date was made for an initial visit. The Community pilot phase was conducted as a pre-post treatment study. Participants were allocated, alternately, to either a PP or WP exercise group, no control group was included. This format was not analogous to the definitive trial design which would include a control group and would comprise two baselines and repeated follow-ups on cessation of intervention. However, the design adopted did correspond to the exploratory nature of this phase in investigating feasibility of the exercise structure, documentation and utility of outcome measures. For this community pilot phase, the outcome assessor (GB) was not blinded to group allocation. In the definitive trial outcome assessment would be blinded, but there were no resources to support this for the pilot phase. On the initial preintervention visit participants were tested on the battery of screening tests and outcome measures as identified in table 6.2. Field notes were made to refine administration and to ensure standardisation. The exercise interventions were provided in visual and written format in an “Exercise Diary” (appendix II). The specific PP or WP exercises were taught to the participant. Care was taken to ensure that the environment in which exercises would be practiced was safe. This analysis of the environment was taken on an individual basis, as is standard clinical practice. For example if balance was an issue for the stepping up and down exercises, then the practice area might be set up in the 150 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology kitchen next to the work surface, the environment was cleared of hazards (rugs, pet food bowls), a chair was positioned appropriately close to the practice area. In the event that the participant lived alone, every care was taken to ensure that the exercise area (or areas) could either be set up by the participant or could be left in situ. If the participant had a particular problem with an exercise, for example symmetrical weight bearing during rise to stand, then simple, additional prompt notes were included on the Exercise Diary. The target number of repetitions of each exercise was documented in the Exercise Diary and participants were requested to document the actual number undertaken. In addition, participants were requested to document other sustained activity (for example, walk to corner shop and back – 20 minutes). A visit was made to the participants mid way through the four week exercise intervention, to check on progress, ensure the exercises were being practiced correctly, to discuss any issues that had arisen for the participant and to encourage the participant to maintain or increase the number of repetitions for each exercise. At the end of the four week exercise intervention an outcome assessment visit was undertaken and all outcome measures were administered again. 6.4.2 Results of Community pilot of exercise intervention All participants completed the community pilot. Pre – post data were available for all outcome measures for all participants. One participant mislaid his exercise diary between the mid-way and end of intervention visit, therefore data relating to number of repetitions undertaken for each exercise was only available for five participants. 151 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Data are summarised firstly by number of repetitions undertaken for each exercise and the findings for the pre- post outcome measures. Raw data are found in appendix VII. 6.4.2.1 Number of Repetitions for each Exercise Selected graphs are presented in this section. The number of repetitions for the Rise-to-Stand exercise and the Sitting Down exercise were recorded as being identical by all subjects therefore only the rise to stand graph has been presented as figure 6.2. As can be seen two participants increased the number of repetitions each week, while three participants practiced in a relatively constant manner. The maximum number of repetitions was 250 (PP1 in week 3) and minimum 40 (WP4 week 4). 300 participants repetitions 250 PP1 200 WP2 150 PP3 WP4 100 PP5 50 0 STS1 STS2 STS3 STS4 weeks Figure 6.2. Line graph of weekly total of Rise to stand repetitions by pilot participants 152 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology For the Stepping Up and Stepping Down exercises with the unaffected leg (necessitating dynamic balance and weight bearing through the affected leg), a similar pattern of repetitions was undertaken by each participant and therefore a representative graph is presented in figure 6.3. As can be seen PP1 again increased the number of repetitions markedly from week one, while the remaining participants stayed relatively stable. The maximum number of repetitions was 230 (PP1 in week 2) and minimum 40 (WP4 week 4). 250 participants 200 Repetitions PP1 WP2 150 PP3 100 WP4 PP5 50 0 Step Up1 Step Up2 Step Up3 Step Up 4 weeks Figure 6.3. Line graph of weekly total of Stepping Up with the Unaffected Leg repetitions by pilot participants Figure 6.4 indicates the number of repetitions of Cuppa Time exercises with the affected upper limb and figure 6.5 indicates TipTap. Only one participant (PP1) had good recovery in his upper limb and this participant maintained the same number of high repetitions (maximum 230 in week 3) with arm exercises as with lower 153 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology extremity exercises. The remaining participants demonstrated far fewer repetitions of upper limb exercises. WP4 had minimal upper limb recovery and did not practice the exercise, despite being able to undertake forearm weight-bearing. As PP5 had minimal upper limb recovery, this participant (represented by the dashed line in figure 6.4) was encouraged to position the arm as if to undertake the exercise, to practice the weightbearing component and to undertake mental practice of the remaining components 250 participants 200 repetitions PP1 WP2 150 PP3 100 WP4 PP5 50 0 Cuppa1 Cuppa2 Cuppa3 Cuppa4 weeks Figure 6.4. Line graph of weekly total of Cuppa Time exercise repetitions by pilot participants 154 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 250 200 repetitions PP1 WP2 150 PP3 100 WP4 PP5 50 0 Tip1 Tip2 Tip3 Tip4 weeks Figure 6.5. Line graph of weekly total of TipTap exercise repetitions by pilot participants 6.4.2.2 Baseline to end of intervention Outcome Measurement Data Pre and post measures were taken on all six pilot participants for the Motor Assessment Scale, Timed Up and Go, The Step Test, Frenchay Arm Test and the Barthel Index. Data were plotted to allow comparisons to be made pre and post the four week intervention phase. Data are presented in figures 6.6 – 6.10. Motor Assessment Scale Figure 6.6 shows the MAS scores for pilot participants. Two PP (PP1, PP5) and two WP (WP2, WP4) participants made small gains on the MAS, the other two participants demonstrated no change in score. The improvements in all cases were related to sitting balance or rise to stand components and the walking component. Only one participant demonstrated an improvement on the UL items. . From a practical point of view, it became apparent that it was helpful to warn participants of 155 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology the activities to be conducted during testing as one participant was not keen initially to undertake the bed mobility tests. Figure 6.6. Pilot Motor Assessment Scale pre- post-intervention scores Timed Up and Go (TUG) The TUG total time is presented in figure 6.7. The TUG was conducted over three metres. Two PP (PP1, PP3) and two WP (WP2, WP4) participants reduced their total time for the TUG. The other two participants demonstrated a small increase in total time (+0.17 sec PP3 and +0.82 sec WP6) in score. WP4 reduced total TUG time by over 12 seconds, this was possibly related to familiarity with the test procedure and the assessor. An issue relating to having sufficient space to conduct the test became apparent during the pilot phase. This necessitated undertaking a 156 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology further pilot to establish the concurrent validity of a TUG over a shorter distance (reported in section 6.6). Figure 6.7. Pilot Timed Up and Go pre- post-intervention scores Rise to stand The TUG was timed using a multi memory stopwatch with lap timer, this allowed calculation of certain components of the TUG. Rise to stand time was of interest as this was an exercise that the pilot participants had practised during the four weeks intervention. Results for rise to stand times are presented in figure 6.8 Five of the six pilot participants improved their time on the rise to stand component of the TUG. PP1 reduced their time from 3.11 to 2.20 seconds – a reduction in time by a third. Most of the other changes were modest and PP5 demonstrated a slight increase in rise to stand time 157 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Figure 6.8. Pilot Rise to stand time Operationally, it was apparent that it was beneficial to demonstrate the TUG before each testing session, as without doing so some participants waited for a command to walk or turn and this adversely affected their times. The Step Test Data are presented in figure 6.9 for stepping up with the unaffected leg. Undertaking this test required the participant to balance and weight bear through the leg most affected by their stroke. Five of the six pilot participants improved the number of step ups they were able to perform, with WP4 improving by 4 steps, WP6 improving by 3 and WP2 and PP1 improving by two steps. Only PP5 maintained their pre-test score. 158 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Figure 6.9 Pilot Step Test data – stepping up with the unaffected leg. Frenchay Arm Test Data relating to the Frenchay Arm Test are presented in figure 6.10 Figure 6.10 Pilot Frenchay Arm Test data 159 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology As can be seen from the bar chart, only two of the pilot participants were able to gain a score on the Frenchay Arm Test, and only PP1 demonstrated a change. On consideration of the physical status of the pilot participants, it was felt that four out of the six participants with minimal return of UL function was not necessarily representative of the stroke population at large and a decision made to retain the Frenchay Arm Test for the definitive trial. Barthel Index Data were collected for the Barthel Index at pre- post-interventio9n points. All pilot participants Barthel Index scores remained stable. Data are available in appendix VII. 6.4.3 Summary of key findings from Community Pilot The Community pilot phase raised a number of issues that needed to be addressed in the definitive study and these are outlined in this section. The number of repetitions undertaken by the participants was variable and indicated that while a starting level of between six to ten daily repetitions of each exercise was feasible, for some participants there would be a need to commence with a higher number of repetitions. In relation to the number of reported repetitions, the accuracy of reporting could not be verified and in the definitive trial, it was felt that a method for capturing activity over a period of time (to include the exercise practice) might allow a form of verification. Consideration was therefore given to the use of an accelerometer (see 6.5). 160 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Scheduling visits two weeks apart did not allow time for the therapist to identify that the exercises were being undertaken correctly at an early stage. While the two WP participants understood how to structure the practice sessions, there were a number of queries from the PP participants. It was felt that any issues that arose early during the study needed to be addressed within the first few days of the intervention phase, to ensure the practice structure was understood. This issue was highlighted by PP1, PP3 and the researcher. For participants who had minimal upper limb recovery, the two exercises relating to upper limb posed some problems. Given that poor arm function is a common feature post-stroke, it was felt that including exercises to target upper limb activity was relevant. It was decided that for those participants who might demonstrate minimal or no upper limb activity, the affected upper limb would be positioned on the table with the cup or bottle placed in the hand and physical practice would be encouraged. In the situation where physical practice was not possible, the affected upper limb would be placed in the start position, weight bearing encouraged and mental practice with an external focus on movement of the object (cup or bottle) would be followed. The Community pilot raised two main environmental issues. The first of these was the need to be sensitive to the home setting. While there was a need to ensure adequate organisation to enable the exercise practice to occur, this had to be balanced to ensure the home environment did not end up as a mini rehabilitation gym. Creative solutions to storage of the step-up block, particularly for people who 161 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology lived alone in a relatively small dwelling, were required. The other issue that became apparent was linked to being sensitive to the home setting. One pilot participant lived in cramped conditions with numerous ornaments, furniture and other equipment. For this participant, it was not possible to sort out a satisfactory space in which to undertake the Timed Up and Go (TUG), as a clear space of nearly five metres was required to allow chair placement and turning space. It was therefore decided to explore whether undertaking the TUG over a shorter distance would be a valid test in this population (see 6.6). Feedback from pilot participants resulted in a number of changes to the exercise documentation so that it was more user friendly. The terms “affected” and “unaffected” referring to limbs were disliked, two participants recommended changing the documentation to read “bad” and “good”, as this was the terminology used colloquially by participants and carers. A5 size documentation was preferred to A4, as it was less unwieldy. Collating the documentation into a loose leaf, hard covered, A5 ringbinder was suggested to make the whole activity diary more substantial. The use of page dividers with tab extensions was another suggestion that enabled the participants to turn directly to the exercise instructions and exercise record page. The diary exercise record pages had been pre-printed to provide a record for seven days, starting on a Monday. Not all participants commenced on a Monday and found it confusing how to document the final week as five exercise record pages would be required and only four were provided. 162 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Omitting the pre-printed days from the seven day exercise record, solved this problem One participant provided extensive typed feedback, concluding with the statement “The major sense of achievement from doing exercises comes from transferring an exercise activity into my daily life routine. This has given me a great sense of achievement” Modifications to the exercises and documentation that arose from the pilot were applied to the definitive trial. 6.5 Testing the ActivPAL During the development of the exercise practice schedule, consideration was given to how reliable participants might be in reporting the number of exercises they had undertaken (see 6.4.3). Associated with this consideration was a secondary aim of the study, namely to explore activity undertaken by people with late-stage stroke in their home environment. A simple activity monitor that was unobtrusive and easy to operate was required. A number of commercially available devices such as pedometers and accelerometers are available (Rowe 1999; Macko et al, 2002; Tudor-Locke et al, 2002) and consideration was given to using a pedometer for this task. However, while pedometers are simple to use and can quantify cumulative step count within a given period, a pedometer does not provide data relating to other activity. A pragmatic decision was made therefore, to utilise the activPAL™ as step count was only one activity of interest, and the activPAL can measure time spent in different positions: sitting or lying, standing and walking as well as taking a record of the number of rise to stand transitions (Grant et al, 2006; Godfrey et al, 2007). 163 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology The activPAL is a small (35mm x 53mm x 7 mm), lightweight (20 gm) uni-axial accelerometer, worn in the midline, one third of the way up the anterior aspect of the thigh (figure 6.11). It has a sampling rate of 10Hz and operates by detecting alterations in the amplitude of the acceleration signal with the shape of the signal over time. Using proprietary algorithms it then categorises the acceleration signal derived from positional change as time spent in a position, a change from horizontal to vertical (rise to stand (RTS)), or cyclical change from the vertical (stepping). Figure 6.11 Placement of activPAL At the time of planning the study, data was available on the fore-runner to the activPAL (Egerton et al, 2002), but no independent publications were found related to the activPAL. Since 2004, a number of papers have been published that support 164 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology the validity and reliability of the activPAL as a measure of free-living activity (Grant et al, 2006; Ryan et al, 2006; Grant et al, 2008; Dahlgren et al, 2010). Many of these studies have been undertaken with small samples of healthy subjects which could raise queries as to the potential of the activPAL to accurately record movements of rise to stand and walking given some of the movement anomalies displayed by people with stroke. In studies where walking speed has been dictated by a treadmill, speeds have ranged generally from 0.6m/s to 1.4m/s (Maddocks et al, 2010; Grant 2008; Dahlgren 2010), which may be faster than the comfortable walking speed for many people with stroke. Tsavourelou et al, (2009), in a small sample of young healthy subjects, did investigate a range of treadmill speeds, starting at 0.27m/s. and found activPAL to be reliable. Only one small study has been found that used activPAL with stroke (Britton et al, 2008). In that study, the activPAL was accepted as a valid and reliable measurement instrument and used to record the number of RTS transitions which was the focus of the intervention under investigation (Britton et al, 2008). Initial bench testing of the activPAL was undertaken in order to gain familiarity with the equipment. From the bench testing it became apparent that some operational issues could arise and therefore a plain english user guide was created (appendix VI). In summary, there were three main operational issues. Firstly some hardware problems were identified with two of the activPAL units, manifest by “connection error” messages. The hardware problems required the units to be returned to the manufacturer for repair. Secondly, the clock on the activPAL and computer where data download would occur required to be synchronised in-between each download. An issue with failure to synchronise between the activPAL unit and the computer led to problems with identifying when events had occurred. Finally although battery life 165 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology was reported to enable recordings to be made for up to seven days continuously, it was found that after five individual day recordings, a new battery was required to ensure the unit did not fail, as this would result in a lack of data. 6.5.1 Methodology for establishing the agreement of activPAL and video data at different walking speeds With the definitive home-based exercise study ongoing, a further piece of work was undertaken in order to ascertain the accuracy of activPAL at various walking speeds (Baer and O’Loughlin 2007). The aim of this pilot was to establish the agreement between activPAL and gold standard video data when walking on a treadmill at speeds of 0.69m/s, 1.36m/s and 2.08m/s. Twenty young, healthy subjects (four male), with mean age 24.8 years (+ 2.6) were recruited from a population of university students. Participants walked on a treadmill at speeds of 2.5 kilometres per hour (km/h), 5 km/h and 7.5 km/h for 10 minutes at each speed. The activPAL recording was stopped between the walking trial at each speed. Bland and Altman plots were drawn up to explore the levels of agreement between the two methods of measurement (Bland and Altman 1986) of activPAL and video recordings and these are displayed in figures 6.3 – 6.5. The Bland and Altman technique allows the determination of agreement between two clinical measures by plotting the difference between each measure against the mean of the differences (also referred to as “bias”). Plotting the mean difference plus or minus 1.96 standard deviations of the differences (sdiff) allows upper and lower limits of agreement to be determined. There is no fixed value for limits of 166 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology agreement and therefore a judgement has to be made as to whether the spread of data are acceptable (Bland and Altman 1991). 6.5.2 Results for agreement between activPAL and video data at different walking speeds At speeds of 2.5 km/h (figure 6.3) and 5 km/h (figure 6.4), the activPAL showed very good levels of agreement with the gold standard video observation, with the mean bias when walking at speeds of 2.5km/h or 5 km/h, being very close to zero. Mean percentage errors in step count were also very low at -0.42% and -0.15% at speeds of 2.5 km/h and 5 km/h respectively. At the very fast speed of 7.5 km/h (figure 6.5) however, the agreement was poor, the data are markedly dispersed and large errors in step count resulted in mean bias overestimates by 18.2%. On the basis of these data, and given that people with stroke tend to walk at slow to normal speeds of age-matched healthy people (Olney et al, 1994; Olney and Richards 1996; Lamontagne and Fung 2004), it was felt that the activPAL was an appropriate device to gain an indication of step count during the day. 167 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Difference in Step Count (steps) Upper limit of agreement +25 steps 20 0 Mean bias -3.4 steps -20 Lower limit of agreement -32 steps -40 -60 -80 450 550 650 750 850 950 Average Step Count (steps) Figure 6.12. Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts recorded by activPAL and video at 2.5 km/h 20 15 Upper limit of agreement +25 steps Difference step count (steps) 10 5 0 -5 Mean bias +1.6 steps -10 -15 -20 Lower limit of agreement -17 steps -25 -30 -35 950 1000 1050 1100 1150 1200 1250 Average step count (steps) Figure 6.13 Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts recorded by activPAL and video at 5 km/h 168 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 1400 Upper limit of agreement +1066 steps 1200 Difference step count (steps) 1000 800 600 400 Mean bias +250 steps 200 0 -200 Lower limit of agreement -506 steps -400 -600 1200 1300 1400 1500 1600 1700 1800 1900 2000 2100 Average step count (steps) Figure 6.14 6.6 Bland and Altman plot of agreement between step counts recorded by activPAL and video at 7.km/h Establishing Concurrent validity of a modified Timed Up and Go test over two metres During the pilot of the practice exercise regime and testing the feasibility of the battery of outcome measures (section 6.4.), it was found that the Timed Up and Go (TUG) could not be performed easily in the home environment of one pilot participant. In order to undertake the TUG, the participant is required to standup from a chair, walk three metres, turn around and walk back to the chair and sit down. With the turning space required, and the space for the chair a clear testing area of approximately five metres is required. The home environment that was found to be unsuitable did not have a clear testing area of five metres and the participant was not able to be tested outwith the home environment. The 169 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology unsuitability of the environment related to the need to rearrange considerable amounts of furniture and the removal of ornaments and copious personal possessions from the testing area. This was not felt to be satisfactory for the participant. It was decided to retain the TUG within the testing battery, but with modifications, as important information relating to balance and time to stand up was derived from the test. Furthermore, it was possible to derive a measure of gait speed from the three metre walk component of the TUG (Wall et al, 2000). It was therefore decided that modifying the TUG over a shorter distance, might be a viable option in order to allow it to be included within the outcome measure test battery. A pilot study was thus undertaken to investigate the properties of a TUG performed over two metres. The aims of this pilot study were to: 1. establish the concurrent validity of the TUG2m with the TUG 2. investigate the effect of a modified Timed Up and Go over 2 metres (TUG2m) on the gait characteristics of stroke patients 6.6.1 Methodology of a pilot study investigating the Concurrent Validity of the Timed Up and Go over two metres (TUG2m). Non acute stroke patients were recruited from rehabilitation wards at Astley Ainslie Hospital, Edinburgh. All participants gave informed consent to participating in the pilot study, this included consent to having a video taken while undertaking the TUG. Inclusion criteria ensured that participants were independently ambulant with or 170 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology without an aid and were at least 3 months post-stroke. Participants were excluded if they required physical assistance during walking, had comorbid neurological conditions such as Parkinson’s Disease or were unable to understand the nature of the study. Once informed consent had been obtained, brief personal data were gathered from the participant and patient notes. Participants were required to undertake the Timed Up and Go test over both two and three metres. A two minute rest was permitted between the tests. A standard height dining chair, without arms, (seat height 46cm) was used for all participants. An explanation of how to perform the TUG was provided to the participant and this was followed by a demonstration. Participants were randomised, by the toss of a coin, either to undertake the TUG2m followed by the TUG over 3 metres, or vice versa. A video of performance was undertaken and real-time recordings of component parts of the TUG, using a hand-held stop watch were also made (Wall et al, 2000). 6.6.2 Results of the pilot of the Timed Up and Go Fifteen people with sub-acute stroke were recruited to the TUG pilot. Participant characteristics are summarised in table 6.4. TUG pilot - participant characteristics Gender 9 male : 6 female Mean age (years) 67.4 (+ 14.9) time since stroke (weeks) 24 (+ 18.3) Side of hemiplegia 4 right : 11 left Table 6.4. Timed Up and Go pilot study - subject characteristics 171 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology In order to establish whether the TUG2m could be used interchangeably with TUG3m, concurrent validity between the two tests was established by looking at the agreement between the two on a number of key gait parameters. The TUG2m was also examined for stability when the test was undertaken one week apart by five subjects. The characteristics relating to gait parameters and components of the TUG are presented in table 6.5. Gait parameter TUG 2m TUG 3m TUG total time (s) 9.32 (+ 5.35) 13.12 (+ 6.8) 0.27 (+ 0.11) 0.30 (+ 0.17) 5.79 (+ 3.0) 5.84 (+ 2.9) 6.4 (+ 1.9) 6.1 (+ 1.8) Mean (+ standard deviation) Gait velocity (m/s) Mean (+ standard deviation) Turning time (s) Mean (+ standard deviation) Number of component steps during turn Mean (+ standard deviation) Table 6.5. Timed Up and Go pilot study - Summary of gait parameters and components of Timed Up and Go On examining the data for the 15 participants, it was found that, as expected, the TUG2m was completed in 71% of the time (9.32 seconds) taken to perform the TUG over 3m (13.12 seconds). Data were then examined to determine whether gait speed had been affected by the distance walked. On looking at the data, there appeared to be similarity between the gait velocity obtained, in order to determine whether there was agreement between the data, Bland and Altman plots with limits of agreement were calculated (Bland and Altman 1986) and are shown in figure 172 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 6.15. The mean bias on figure 6.15 is very close to zero and overall it can be seen that the two values from TUG2m against TUG3m agree, with 95% of cases falling within two standard deviations of the difference, which equates to + 0.2m/s. This was felt to show an acceptable level of agreement between gait velocity for TUG2m and TUG3m. Difference in gait velocity (m/s) 0.3 0.2 mean + 2SD 0.1 0 mean bias -0.1 -0.2 mean -2SD -0.3 -0.4 0 Figure 6.15 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 mean gait velocity over TUG3m and TUG2m 0.6 Bland and Altman plot of gait velocity for Timed Up and GO performed over 2 metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG3m) On considering the turning times, only the turn when participants crossed the line on the outbound walk in order to commence the return walk was analysed. The reason for this is that various turn strategies were adopted when approaching the chair, some only turning halfway and collapsing onto the chair, others turning a full 180degrees. It was hypothesised that there would be no significant difference between the turn times for TUG2m or TUG3m, as the nature of the task of turning 173 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology had not changed. The turn time of just under 6 seconds was similar in both TUG conditions. A paired t-test showed that there was no significant difference between time taken for turns during the TUG2m and TUG3m, p=0.96. Once again the data Difference turn times TUG2m & TUG3m were plotted to ascertain limits of agreement and these are shown in figure 6.16. 4 3 m ean +2SD 2 1 m ean bias 0 -1 -2 m ean -2SD -3 0 2 4 6 8 10 Mean turn times TUG2m & TUG3m (secs) Figure 6.16. Bland and Altman plot of turn times (in seconds) for Timed Up and Go performed over 2 metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG3m) It was not anticipated that the number of steps taken during the turn component of the TUG would vary depending on the outbound distance walked. As can be seen from table 6.5, the mean number of steps during turning was approximately six and figure 6.17 demonstrates the consistency of performance with all but one subject falling within the limits of agreement. 174 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 12 Difference in steps to turn (no. of steps) 2.5 2 mean + 2SD 1.5 1 0.5 mean bias 0 -0.5 -1 mean - 2SD -1.5 0 2 4 6 8 10 mean steps to turn 3m and 2m TUG Figure 6.17 Bland and Altman plot of the number of steps taken during turning for Timed Up and Go performed over 2 metres (TUG2m) or 3 metres (TUG3m) These data relating to key gait parameters establish that the TUG2m demonstrates concurrent validity with the TUG over three metres and therefore the tests can be used interchangeably for people with stroke. In order to determine stability of the TUG2m, test – retest reliability was calculated using Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC). Five people with stroke were tested one week apart. Intraclass correlation coefficients ICC (2,1) were calculated for three key parameters and these are presented in table 6.6. 175 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology Gait parameter ICC (2,1) reliability Gait velocity (m/s) 0.86 Good Number of steps 0.92 High Turn times (s) 0.75 Fair Table 6.6. Test – retest reliability of TUG2m over one week In order to determine how reliable the ICC data were, the classification according to Youdas et al, (1991) was followed. This system assigns high reliability for an ICC of > 0.91, good reliability for ICC of 0.81 – 0.9, fair reliability for ICC of 0.71 – 0.8 and an ICC of < 0.7 as poor. As can be seen, the gait characteristics of the number of steps taken and gait velocity demonstrated high to good reliability and the turn times with an ICC of 0.75 had fair reliability. These findings support the stability of the TUG2m over time in a small sample of people with stroke. 6.6.3 Summary of Findings from Timed Up and Go pilot This pilot was undertaken to determine whether reducing the walking distance of the TUG to 2m would affect psychometric properties of the measure. As the TUG over 3m has been shown to be a valid and reliable measure with stroke (Podsiadlo and Richardson 1989; Ng and Hui-Chan 2005; Flansbjer et al, 2005), this version was used as a gold standard to look at concurrent validity of a reduced TUG2m. There was a good level of agreement between gait velocity and turning times over both tests, which would support the view that the TUG2m has concurrent validity with the TUG over three metres. The time for TUG2m over a distance 33% shorter than 176 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology TUG was reduced by just under 30% in the TUG2m – and there was no significant difference in turning time. These findings conform to what was anticipated, as it was not envisaged that shortening the distance would change the properties of the TUG. It could be argued that a shortened walking distance of two metres does not allow initial gait acceleration to occur, however this would be the same for all participants. In summary, the TUG2m can be used in place of the TUG over three metres for people with sub-acute or later-stage stroke. 6.7 Summary Extensive pilot work was undertaken in-hospital and within the community setting to develop the methodology to be followed for the definitive trial. Four in-hospital pilot participants and six community pilot participants took part in the development, refinement and piloting of the exercise practice structure for PP and WP of six functional tasks. Documentation was created, piloted and refined in order to support participants in undertaking the practice regime and to provide a tool for documenting practice and indicate key activities. The outcome measures battery that was piloted was acceptable to the participants and, with the exception of TUG, was suitable for use in a community setting. The community pilot demonstrated the need to modify the distance of the TUG. The newly developed TUG2m demonstrated concurrent validity with the TUG and therefore can be used in its place for people with stroke. 177 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology The community pilot phase identified further that a means of verifying exercise practice would be beneficial. Following pilot work, the use of an accelerometer that could identify step count, body position and number of rise to stand transitions was included in the definitive trial. The definitive trial methodology is reported in chapter seven. 178 Chapter 6 Development of Methodology 7.0 7.1 METHODOLOGY Introduction This chapter presents the trial design and experimental methods employed in this study. The development of the Methodology has been reported in chapter 6. The main study reported in this chapter investigated the efficacy of a home-exercise programme for people with late-stage stroke. The design of the home exercises was based either on part-practice (PP) or whole-practice (WP) principles. 7.2 Trial Design Overview The study was conducted on participants with late-stage stroke and investigated the efficacy of a programme of functional home exercises based on either PP principles, or WP principles. This was a prospective, single blind, randomised controlled pilot study with three groups of participants: PP, WP or a control (CON) group. Ethical approval was received from Lothian Regional Ethics Committee (LREC). Participants meeting inclusion criteria (7.3.1.) were randomly allocated to undergo either a four week programme of exercises following part practice (PP), a four week programme of exercises following whole practice (WP) or to receive “normal” intervention (which at this point post-stroke consisted of no formal rehabilitation). An initial letter of invitation and consent form (appendix VIII) to participate in the study was sent to individuals who had been identified as meeting inclusion criteria. If the individual returned the consent form a short telephone screening interview was undertaken by the principal investigator (PI). During the screening interview the PI clearly explained the nature of the study and highlighted that if the potential 179 Chapter 7 Methodology participant was randomised to the control group they would receive no treatment. This strategy was adopted following statistical consultation as it was identified that informing potential participants of the odds of being randomised to a treatment group would help to minimise drop out. A small number of individuals did drop out at this stage (see figure 8.2). Once informed consent was confirmed, participants were visited by the PI to gather the first of two baseline outcome measurements. Once one set of baseline data were collected, details of side of stroke and Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) total score were passed to the clinical research assistant (CRA). The CRA undertook participant group allocation according to an allocation schedule previously drawn up by the statistician. Randomised blocks within four strata, based on side and severity of stroke, were used to allocate participants to groups (Altman 1991). Participants then underwent a second set of baseline measurements at approximately two weeks after the first baseline outcome measurements. Following the two baseline measurement procedures, the four week intervention phase was instigated by the CRA. Further outcome measurements were subsequently taken at four weeks (end of intervention), approximately four weeks and three days (retention / short-term follow-up) and approximately three months (long-term follow-up) after baseline two. The study was designed as a randomised controlled trial (RCT) in order to ensure participants would have an equal chance of receiving exercise or not. Side of stroke affects the presenting impairments (see chapter 2) and therefore it was important to ensure randomisation would allocate participants with cognitive or language impairments approximately equally among the groups. Also it was not clear whether 180 Chapter 7 Methodology starting level of severity could have an impact on potential for improvement and therefore it was felt that this should be accounted for to ensure that no group had a disproportionate number of mild or severely disabled participants. A flow diagram providing an overview of the trial design can be found in Figure 7.2. 7.3 Subject Populations The literature that advocates the application of PP or WP paradigms in stroke rehabilitation does not derive from evidence acquired from people with stroke. At the time of planning the study, available literature did not advocate whether specific abilities were a pre-requisite for people with stroke to exercise using either a PP or WP paradigm. While the literature claims that WP is the preferred exercise approach in people with stroke (Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2002), this has not been objectively demonstrated. No literature was identified that suggested neurological impairments would contra-indicate either PP or WP of functional tasks for individuals with stroke. Given the lack of definitive evidence to support the application of PP or WP exercise paradigms for people with stroke, combined with the well documented evidence of the first three months stroke as being a natural period of the most rapid post-stroke improvement (Langton Hewer et al; 1987), it was decided to recruit people with stroke that could be considered as being at a plateau in terms of recovery. The population that the sample was drawn from was, therefore, individuals with hemiplegia from Lothian region, who had previously received uni- or multidisciplinary stroke rehabilitation but who had been discharged from formal care. 181 Chapter 7 Methodology The following inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied. 7.3.1 i. Inclusion Criteria Age over 18 ii. At least 12 months post-stroke (*this was later modified to 6 months post-stroke see section 7.4.2.) iii. Residual neurological physical deficit due to stroke iv. Discharged from formal Physiotherapy v. Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≥ 22 (Tombaugh and McIntyre 1992; Crum et al 1993) vi. Functional Reach Test ≥15 c.m. (Duncan et al, 1992) vii. Able to understand the nature of the study and give informed consent 7.3.2 i. Exclusion Criteria Age under 18 ii. Pre-existing gross neuropathology – e.g. Multiple Sclerosis, Parkinson’s Disease iii. Co-existing pathology that would prohibit independent exercise – e.g. lower limb fracture iv. Pre-existing disabilities with grossly limited mobility (e.g. lower limb amputation) v. History of two falls within the previous six months 7.4 Recruitment This section reports the original recruitment strategy. A sample size calculation was undertaken based on gait outcomes and a significance level of 0.05. For between groups comparisons for parametric outcomes, a sample size of 33 in each group would have 90% power to detect an effect size of 0.80 using a two group t- 182 Chapter 7 Methodology test with a 0.05 two-sided significance level, and for 80% power, an effect size of 0.70. The non-parametric equivalent, Mann Whitney U tests would be able to detect an effect size of 0.851, for 90% power, and 0.736 for 80% power. For within group comparisons; for parametric outcomes a sample size of 33 will have 90% power to detect an effect size of 0.58 using a paired t-test with a 0.050 two-sided significance level, and for 80% power, an effect size of 0.50. The non-parametric equivalent, Wilcoxon test, would be able to detect an effect size of 0.61, for 90% power, and 0.53 for 80% power This strategy required three separate amendments due to a number of issues which adversely affected recruitment. Consideration of the issues and the steps taken to overcome them are outlined in this section. Ethical approval was gained from Lothian Research Ethics committee (ref number LREC/2001/1/17) for the original recruitment strategy and for all subsequent amendments. At the time that this study was undertaken, there were very few published randomised controlled trials of rehabilitation undertaken with a population of people with chronic stroke and conducted in the primary care setting. Extensive advice was therefore sought in the design of the recruitment strategy, as it has been well documented that failure to recruit to randomised controlled trials results in underpowered studies and a lack of external validity (Altman 1991; Blanton et al, 2006; Lannin and Cusick 2006). Advice was solicited from the General Manager of the North East Edinburgh Local Healthcare Co-operative. (LHCC), the Professor of General Practice – University of Edinburgh, the Research Co-ordinator of the Lothian Primary Care Research Network (LPCRN), the Medical Director of the 183 Chapter 7 Methodology LPCRN, two non research active General Practitioners, the Superintendent Physiotherapist for Domicillary Physiotherapy within Lothian, a research physiotherapist with experience of primary care trials and a medical statistician. During development of the Recruitment strategy the proposed protocol was presented to physiotherapy colleagues and to the LPCRN and minor modifications made based on subsequent discussions. Participants were recruited to the trial from November 2002 to November 2004. 7.4.1 Initial Recruitment Strategy (Recruitment strategy v1) Initially the recruitment plan was to gain as representative a sample population of stroke subjects from as diverse a social, economic and cultural background as possible. It was recognised that not all stroke subjects would necessarily have accessed hospital services and a minority of the stroke population may have remained at home. It was therefore decided that recruitment via General Practitioner (GP) practices would be the optimal strategy for getting as representative sample as possible. Following advice from the LPCRN and the Manager of the LHCC, it was decided to attempt to recruit from a single LHCC within Edinburgh, with recruitment via neighbouring LHCC’s if the target sample was not recruited from a single LHCC. This was also a pragmatic decision given the limited personnel resources available to support the trial (0.6 whole time equivalent Clinical Research Assistant and 0.2 Principal Investigator). North-East Edinburgh LHCC (NEE LHCC) was targeted as the initial recruitment consortium given that the geographical area of the LHCC covered a spectrum of social class A - E. From a 2002 survey, for example, 30% of the responders of NEE 184 Chapter 7 Methodology LHCC had the highest educational qualification as a degree or technical qualification and 20% had standard grade or no qualification; around 22% earnt over £30,000 per annum and around 20% earned £9,000 or less, around 1.8% were unemployed or looking for work. (NEE LHP 2002) Figure 7.1: Map of North East Edinburgh LHCC The initial recruitment strategy was to target people with chronic stroke, chronic was defined as at least 12 months post-stroke onset. A presentation of the study aims and design as well as potential benefits for participants was given to representatives from each GP practice within the NEE LHCC (n = 14). At this presentation, GP practices were invited to take part and identify potential participants for the study. Four practices were not recruited at this point (one because it was the principal 185 Chapter 7 Methodology investigators home GP practice, one because of refurbishment, and two because the practices were already involved in other trials or not interested). This left a total of ten practices from which to recruit. Given that an average GP practice in the UK can be estimated to have approximately 6,000 patients (RCGP 2006) and that prevalence estimates of stroke are around 3 - 6 per 1,000 (Jørgenson et al 1995; Warlaw et al 2001) then even at the lowest estimates it was hypothesised that each practice might yield approximately 18 potential stroke subjects (3 [prevalence] x 6 [practice size in 1,000]. Anticipating a pessimistic 50% uptake to the trial, it was estimated that around nine subjects per practice might be recruited. Following the initial presentation of the study aims to GP practices within NEE LHCC, one practice responded positively immediately and became the first GP practice to recruit potential participants to the trial. The agreed protocol for recruitment was followed. The PI explained the nature of the study to the practice manager, provided a list of inclusion and exclusion criteria and 30 pre-stamped “recruitment envelopes” containing a letter of invitation and explanation of the study (see appendix VIII), a consent form (see appendix VIII), and a postage paid return envelope. the manager was asked to identify individuals with stroke from the practice computer database, following this, a list of names and addresses was generated, the practice manager then entered the name and address onto the prestamped recruitment envelope and posted the envelopes 186 Chapter 7 Methodology in the event of more than 30 potential participants being identified from a single practice, the practice manager was to select every second subject on the list until 30 envelopes were sent out. This first practice and initial recruitment strategy revealed a number of problems that needed to be addressed before the trial could progress. In summary, the practice initially identified over 60 individuals with stroke, however it transpired that a miscoding issue meant that people with stroke were not accurately identified. More worryingly, people who did not have a stroke were identified as potential participants. Therefore individuals who had not sustained a stroke were inappropriately referred to the trial. The next two GP practices were unable to differentiate between resolved stroke symptoms such as Transient Ischaemic Attack (TIA) and full stroke. Therefore letters of invitation were sent to people that had sustained a full stroke as well as people with no residual neurological symptoms following a TIA. This issue was common to all GP records systems using GPAS (General Practitioner Administration System) and therefore the recruitment strategy was modified to try and address these issues. In the event that an individual fitting the inclusion criteria returned an informed consent form, an initial phone-call was made to them by the PI. During this first contact between PI and potential participant, a standard explanation of the study was given. The potential participant was asked verbally whether they understood 187 Chapter 7 Methodology the exclusion criteria and could they confirm they were eligible. The potential participants were also informed that, if recruited, they would not necessarily receive exercises. If the participant was still keen to take part in the trial, then an initial contact visit was arranged for a mutually convenient time. 7.4.2 Recruitment strategy version 2 A refinement of the recruitment strategy was undertaken to address the issues of miscoding and the sensitivity of the GPAS system. Following discussions with the project team, the method remained essentially the same as detailed in 7.4.1, with the exception that the initial meeting with the practice manager was a more extensive meeting. A minor modification ensured that both a practice manager and a GP representative attended a meeting with the PI at subsequent GP practice recruitment visits. In addition, once the list of potential participants had been generated, this was circulated to GPs by the Practice Manager to ascertain that not only did the potential participant definitely have a confirmed diagnosis of stroke, but that the subject also fitted the inclusion and exclusion criteria and still had residual physical deficits. An amended list was then given to the practice manager to address the recruitment envelopes and send out. While this strategy resulted in an approximately further delay of four weeks, the identified subjects were more likely to be suitable to participate in the study. At this point in time, at seven months from the commencement of subject recruitment, it was felt that due to the sluggish recruitment (11 participants recruited in five months), decisive measures had to be implemented in an attempt to bolster recruitment. 188 Chapter 7 Methodology 7.4.3 Recruitment strategy version 3 The original recruitment strategies had been developed with extensive advice from experts within the area with the aim to recruit as representative sample as possible from the population within north-east Edinburgh. The revised recruitment strategy v3 was developed to overcome some of the identified shortfalls in recruitment. A twofold strategy was devised and received ethical approval in April 2003 i. Length of time since stroke in order to be eligible for recruitment was reduced from 12 to six months. The original strategy had been formulated to ensure participants would be stable in terms of physical recovery and unlikely to still be receiving formal physiotherapy. The revisions to recruitment strategy v3 to amend inclusion criteria to include people at least six months post-stroke was envisaged to target individuals who would still be relatively stable in terms of physical ability. Furthermore, the strategy of taking two baseline measures was felt appropriate to identify if there were some fluctuations in performance. ii. In addition to recruiting from GP practices, failure of the GPAS system to appropriately identify suitable individuals would still remain, despite the reduction for eligibility to six months post-stroke. It was therefore decided to extend recruitment to include not only GP practices but also to include physiotherapists. The reason for not using physiotherapists in the original recruitment strategy, was associated with the desire to include participants whether or not they had received physiotherapy as part of their post-stroke rehabilitation. 189 Chapter 7 Methodology With recruitment strategy v3, potential participants were referred from nine discrete physiotherapy services within Lothian (McLeod Street, Royal Victoria Hospital (RVH), Day Hospital at RVH, Western General Hospital, Astley Ainslie Hospital, Liberton Hospital, the Edinburgh Community Rehabilitation Team, Roodlands Hospital, Eastern General Hospital). 7.4.4 Recruitment strategy version 4 The amended recruitment strategy v3 was highly successful in improving recruitment to the study. It was calculated however that target numbers would not be achieved within the timeframe for which the study had received funding and therefore a final amendment to the recruitment strategy was applied for and granted ethical approval in May 2004. The final recruitment strategy v4 involved the placement of a recruitment advertisement in the local free paper which was delivered weekly to households in Edinburgh. The advertisement was placed twice with a gap of two weeks between each insertion. Individuals who had sustained a stroke and were interested in taking part in a physiotherapy research project were asked to contact the PI. At the time of initial contact the following procedure was undertaken: A script was followed, explaining the nature of the study; identifying the exclusion criteria; explaining that if recruited, participants would not necessarily receive exercises and checking that the potential participants were happy for the PI to contact the GP to ensure that the participant met inclusion criteria. 190 Chapter 7 Methodology If the participant was still interested in taking part, the GP was then contacted by letter which explained the nature of study. The GP was requested to confirm, by a set date, that the individual was fit to participate in the study. Once approval had been received from the GP, an initial visit was carried out. This whole procedure took approximately four to six weeks from initial contact to first recruitment visit. 7.4.5 Consent Once potential participants had been identified through the strategies cited above, a “recruitment envelope” was sent out. The “recruitment envelope” comprised an explanation about the study (appendix VIIIa, a consent form (appendix VIIIb) and a stamped address envelope. If no consent form was returned to the PI, no followed up was undertaken, the assumption being that the individual had decided not to participate any further. Participants who did return the consent form were then followed up by phone to arrange an initial visit (as described in 7.4.1). 7.5 Outcome Measures Procedures Data relating to physical, cognitive and perceptual impairments, and functional abilities and a stroke specific measure of quality of life were gathered by the principal investigator who was blinded to group allocation. 191 Chapter 7 Methodology Following extensive pilot procedures standardised, validated outcome measures were used to document ability (see section 6.3). Five outcome measure visits were undertaken. Outcome measurement (OM) data were collected twice during the baseline stage, to determine stability of performance (OM1 and OM2). The two baseline measures were taken approximately two weeks apart. Outcomes were also measured at the end of the four week intervention phase (OM3), and at approximately 72 hours (OM4) and three months (OM5) after cessation of the intervention phase. OM4 was taken to ascertain whether any changes that were apparent at OM3, were retained due to short-term changes in performance. OM5 was taken to determine whether any changes noted at the end of intervention (OM3) persisted which would indicate long-term retention and learning. Not all outcomes were tested at each measurement visit. The MMSE and FRT were solely used for screening purposes and were the first two measures undertaken on the initial visit. Measures of physical ability were taken at all five measurement visits. Measurement of mood, using HADS and recall of activity using FAI, was taken at the first baseline visit, at the end of intervention and at long-term follow-up (OM1, OM3 and OM5). Measurement of self-perceived general status using the SIS was taken at all measurement points with the exception of short-term follow-up. Table 7.1 summarises the outcome measures that were taken at each visit. The activPAL was used to record activity during a single day and to potentially allow triangulation of the exercise session for PP and WP participants. The activPAL was worn for one waking day during week three or week four of the intervention phase of the trial. 192 Chapter 7 Methodology Time point Baseline 1 Baseline 2 End intervention 48-72 hrs post end intervention (short-term follow-up Retention) 3 months post end intervention (Long-term follow-up) Outcome Measure OM1 OM2 OM3 OM4 OM5 Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) Functional Reach Test (FRT) Hospital Anxiety & Depression Scale (HAD) Frenchay Index (FAI) Activity Barthel Index (BI) Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) Timed Up and Go 2m (TUG2m) Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) Step Test Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) Table 7.1. Schedule of outcome measure visits 193 Chapter 7 Methodology Recruitment Eligibility checked / informed consent gained Baseline visits (OM1 and OM2) Randomisation using randomised blocks within four strata of patients Control (CON) Part Practice (PP) Whole Practice (WP) Four week “intervention” period “Post-Intervention” Outcome Measures (OM3) Taken at end of intervention “Retention” Outcome Measures (OM4) Taken at 48 - 72 hours post end of intervention “Long-term follow-up” Outcome Measures (OM5) Taken 3 months post end of intervention Figure 7.2. Flow Diagram of Trial Design 194 Chapter 7 Methodology 7.6 Randomisation Randomisation with minimisation was initially planned for use in this study. Following further discussion with a statistician however, it was decided to use randomised blocks within four strata of patients based on side and severity of stroke, due to the small numbers of patients. Block randomisation with stratification allows approximately equal numbers of participants to be allocated to each group and for each group to consist of participants with a balance of features that could influence outcome (Altman 1991). Severity of stroke was determined by total MAS score with > 32 designated as “mild” and total MAS score < 31 designated “moderate to severe”. Following participant recruitment, and the first recruitment visit, details relating to side of stroke and severity of motor impairment as measured by the Motor Assessment Scale were provided to the Clinical Research Assistant (CRA). The CRA then allocated participants to the Whole Practice (WP), Part Practice (PP) or Control (Con) group following a pre-determined randomisation list (see appendix IX). The PI was blind to group allocation and blind to the content of the randomisation list until the very last outcome measure on the very last participant in the trial had been taken. 7.7 Procedures As described in 7.6, participants were randomised to either a Con, PP or WP group. The Con group received no physical intervention, but were visited by the CRA. Participants in the PP or WP group undertook a four week intervention programme with practice of exercises for functional tasks. Following completion of the two baseline OM visits, the CRA visited all participants on three or four “intervention visits” during the four week intervention period. The 195 Chapter 7 Methodology Con group received the same number of visits as the participants allocated to the intervention arms of the trial in order to counteract the potential therapist-interaction effect. In addition, participants from all groups wore an activPAL for one waking day during week 3, to record activity and to triangulate recorded exercise activity in the diary with activity measured by the activPAL. 7.7.1 Baseline Outcome Measures visits At the first baseline visit (OM1), the principal investigator explained the nature of the study, highlighting that if the participant were suitable to take part in the trial, they would not necessarily receive exercises. If participants were still happy to take part, exclusion criteria were discussed and checked verbally. While assurance had been received previously that participants did meet inclusion criteria from the GP practice or from the referring physiotherapist a fall history may well have been missed. Participants then completed screening tests that required minimum scores in order to fulfil eligibility (Functional Reach Test > 15cm, no history of more than 2 falls in 6 months and a Mini Mental State Exam score of >22). Participants who passed the screening tests were subsequently tested on a battery of outcome measures that included self-report questionnaires (Hospital Anxiety & Depression Scale (HAD), Frenchay Activity Index (FAI), Barthel Index (BI)); impairment and activity ordinal scales (Motor Assessment Scale (MAS), Timed Up and Go 2m (TUG2m), Frenchay Arm Test (FAT), and Brunel Step Test) and a quality of life scale (Stroke Impact Scale (SIS)). 196 Chapter 7 Methodology At the second baseline visit the following outcome measures were repeated Barthel Index, Motor Assessment Scale, Timed Up and Go 2m, Frenchay Arm Test, Brunel Step Test and the Stroke Impact Scale. 7.7.2 Intervention Visit 1 The clinical research assistant followed a standard format during the first visit. Initial conversation reiterated the aim of the study and ascertained that the participant was still prepared to be involved. This procedure, which permitted drop out, had been strongly advised by the statistician, as experience with clinical trials had shown that at the point of group allocation, a number of participants may drop out early in the study, if unhappy with their group allocation. Where participants had a spouse or carer that lived in the same premises, the carer was invited to participate in initial conversations and explanations as it was common practice that the carer assisted the stroke participant filling in the exercise and activity diary or with the exercises. All participants were asked questions about their daily activity and shown how to fill in a simple self-report activity diary. All participants were given information about stroke using a standard Chest Heart and Stroke Scotland (CHSS) booklet “Living With Stroke”. Participants allocated to one of the two exercise groups were taught six exercises to practice. The exercises consisted of practising standing up from a chair, sitting down, stepping onto a step, stepping off a step, pronation and supination holding a bottle and reaching and grasping. All participants allocated to an exercise “arm” of the trial practised the same functional exercises but in different ways. The 197 Chapter 7 Methodology developments of the exercises are detailed in section 6.2 and 6.4. Participants allocated to the control group did not receive any exercise instruction. 7.7.3 Intervention visit 2 Intervention visit 2, occurred at approximately one week after Intervention visit 1. It was not possible to set an exact time-scale as the CRA worked 0.6 whole time equivalent, participants were community dwelling and therefore had a number of other commitments (e.g. voluntary groups, adult education, visits to or from relatives). In order to preserve numbers of participants enrolled into the study, it was necessary to work with an extremely high degree of flexibility. The focus of visit 2 was to ensure that all participants were filling in the diary correctly and to assist completion retrospectively if required. Furthermore, this visit provided an opportunity to discuss and to clarify any questions that participants’ or their carers had about stroke. In addition, participants randomised to the WP or PP groups were checked practising exercises to ensure that they were undertaking the exercises correctly, individualised pointers were given if deemed necessary (for example how to position the arm during “TipTap”). The CRA encouraged participants to increase the number of repetitions of exercises practiced if assessed to be appropriate. Whether exercise repetitions were increased during visit 2, the CRA discussed with all WP and PP participants procedures for increasing exercise repetitions. Participants were reminded at this point that they were able to phone the CRA for advice during the study, if they were unsure of any of the exercises. 198 Chapter 7 Methodology 7.7.4 Intervention visits 3 and 4 The final intervention visit occurred during week 3 or beginning of week 4. The focus of this visit was to attach the activPAL and ensure it was working. Additionally this visit gave the opportunity to ensure that diary completion was on-going and to discuss any further concerns that the participant or carer may have about their stroke condition in general. Frequency of exercise repetitions was again monitored and increasing the number of exercises being practised was encouraged whenever appropriate. If the participants reported having problems with undertaking any of the exercises, there was the opportunity to contact the CRA again and an exceptional visit could be undertaken. 7.8 Data Analysis The study reported in this thesis was a single blind, exploratory randomised controlled trial with repeated measures. The study investigated a home programme of exercises based on part- or whole-practice for people with late-stage stroke. The questions to be were addressed were: Does a home physiotherapy programme based on either part- or whole practice strategies result in changes in performance of functional tasks for people with late-stage stroke? Does a home physiotherapy programme based on either part- or whole practice strategies result in changes in parameters of activity limitation, participation and health status for people with late-stage stroke? Are any changes in performance retained after cessation of the intervention phase of the home physiotherapy programme? and How much activity is undertaken by people with late-stage stroke in the community? 199 Chapter 7 Methodology Data were a mixture of ordinal data (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; the Frenchay Activity Index; the Barthel Index; the Motor Assessment Scale; the Frenchay Arm Test; and the Stroke Impact Scale) and ratio data (Timed Up and Go 2metre Test and the Step Test). Descriptive data and ratio data were presented for activPAL. Initially data were entered into Microsoft Excel 2003 for preliminary analysis of demographic data. Data were subsequently transferred to SPSS 12.0 for Windows (Statistical Package for Social Sciences) and, during later analysis, SPSS 17.0 for Windows was used. Data were plotted using histograms to explore normality of distribution and skewness of data. Shapiro Wilks tests were carried out on all data sets to ascertain whether data were normally distributed (Altman 1991). The majority of data were non-normally distributed data and therefore data will be presented in the Results chapter using median values and inter-quartile ranges (Altman 1991; Hicks 2004). Due to the non-normal distribution, data are graphically presented by means of box and whisker plots and statistical analysis will be undertaken using non-parametric statistical tests (Altman 1991; Field 2009). Where outcome measures of physical impairments and activity limitations had been obtained twice at baseline, initial testing using the Mann Whitney U test was undertaken to ascertain whether there was any difference between the two baseline outcome measurement points. In all cases, there was no significant difference between the two baseline outcome measurement points; a decision was therefore made to consistently make comparisons between the post-intervention tests and baseline two. 200 Chapter 7 Methodology In order to determine if there were any statistically significant differences between the groups at each measurement point, between-group comparisons were undertaken by applying a Kruskal Wallis test to the data. In the event that a statistically significant difference was found between the groups, then post hoc testing using a Mann Whitney U test would be undertaken to determine exactly where the difference was found (Altman 1991; Field 2009). In order to ascertain whether any of the groups demonstrated a statistically significant change over the course of the trial, a within-groups comparison for each outcome measure over time was made using a Friedman’s Anova. In the event that a significant difference was found over time within the group, this was followed up by undertaking post hoc testing using a Wilcoxon test (Altman 1991; Field 2009). Due to the multiple tests that potentially could be carried out with post hoc testing, there is a danger of a type I error occurring (i.e. the null hypothesis being rejected incorrectly). In order to account for this, a Bonferroni correction (p / number of comparisons made) was applied to all post hoc tests. Using the Bonferroni method results in the data having to satisfy an exceptionally small p value, this method has therefore been criticised as being highly restrictive (Field 2009). The positive aspect of using a Bonferroni correction is that if statistical significance is found, then the null hypothesis can be rejected with confidence. The findings from the definitive RCT are presented in chapter eight. 201 Chapter 7 Methodology 8.0 8.1 RESULTS Introduction This chapter reports the participant characteristics, the results for each outcome measured as well as identifying at which points data were missing, the exercise repetitions undertaken by subjects and an indication of the level of activity for a sample of the participants over a single day. Data were plotted initially to determine normality. This process revealed that the majority of data for all outcome measures were positively skewed. As a consequence, non-parametric analyses were conducted throughout (as indicated in 7.8). 8.2 Subject Characteristics Three hundred and fifty letters of invitation were sent to potential stroke subjects. From this initial invitation, 64 people with chronic stroke were recruited for the study, 31 male and 33 female (see Consort diagram figure 8.1). Mean age was 72.9 (+ 9.0) years, with a median of 72.3 years and ranging from 54.3 to 90.8 years. Mean time since stroke was 30.3 months (+ 28.8) with a median time of 21 months and ranging from six months to 13.5 years. 38 subjects had suffered a right Cerebrovascular accident (CVA), 26 suffered a left CVA. n ALL SUBJECTS Control (CON) Part (PP) 64 Age - years x (+ sd dev) Median (min – max) 72.9 (+ 9.0) 72.3 (54.3 – 90.8) 21 73.3 (+9.2), 75.0 (62.2 - 88.1) 23 72.3 (+9.5) 72.3 (54.3 - 86.6) Whole (WP) 20 73.2 (+8.5) 71.4 (59.8 - 90.8). Table 8.1 Subject characteristics Time since stroke Gender Side of - months M:F stroke x (+ sd dev) R:L Median (min – max) 30.3 (+ 28.8) 31 : 33 38 : 26 21 (6 – 162.5) 8 : 13 13 : 8 15 : 8 13 : 10 8 : 12 12 : 8 202 Chapter 8 Results Twenty one participants were randomly allocated to the control group, 23 to the Part Practice group and 20 to the Whole Practice Group. Participants in the Control group had a mean age of 73.3 years (+9.2), with a median of 75.0 years and ranging from 62.2 to 88.1 years. Eight control participants were male, 13 female. Thirteen control participants had suffered a right CVA and 8 suffered a left CVA. Participants in the Part Practice group had a mean age of 72.3 years (+9.5), with a median of 72.3 years and ranging from 54.3 to 86.6 years. Fifteen part practice participants were male, eight female. Thirteen PP participants had suffered a right CVA and 10 suffered a left CVA. Participants in the Whole Practice group had a mean age of 73.2 years (+8.5), with a median of 71.4 years and ranging from 59.8 to 90.8 years. Eight WP participants were male, 12 female. Twelve WP participants had suffered a right CVA and 8 suffered a left CVA. The sample of people with chronic stroke was, therefore, consistent with existing published demographic data. The sample size however was lower than that originally anticipated, despite strategies implemented as outlined in chapter seven to attempt to boost recruitment. 203 Chapter 8 Results 8.3 8.3.1 Drop Outs and Missing Data. Drop Outs Table 8.1 indicates selected sample characteristics from the 64 people with stroke recruited to the initial study. Baseline data were collected on all 64 participants, however four participants dropped out, and their data have not been used to analyse effects of intervention. The data from only 60 participants, therefore, are utilised in subsequent data analysis: Control n=19; Part Practice n=22; Whole Practice n=19. The reasons for drop-out are given in table 8.2 and a Consort diagram of the trial is presented in figure 8.1 Participant ID Group Reason for drop-out 28 Con Admitted to hospital following baseline visits for nonstroke related condition. Health status deteriorated and inappropriate to make further visits. 56 WP Drop out during first visit physiotherapist – no reason given. 60 Con Drop out once allocated to group 64 PP Chest pain during initial intervention visit. Referred back to GP Table 8.2 by intervention Reasons for drop-out following recruitment to the study 204 Chapter 8 Results Referred to study (sent invitation letter to participate) n= 350 Non Responders n = 190 Excluded Inclusion criteria not met n = 84 Refused / not interested n =12 Baseline outcome measures 1 and 2 Recruited n = 64 Randomised with stratification Control Group n = 21 Received intervention n= 19 Drop out before intervention n=2 Drop out n = 0 Drop out n = 0 Part Practice Group Assessed n= 21 Drop out n = 1 Died n = 0 Whole Practice group Assessed n= 18 Drop out n = 0 Died n = 1 Outcome 5 Whole Practice group Assessed n=18 Unable to assess n = 1 [UTA= 1 - hospital admission] Outcome 4 Part Practice Group Assessed n=19 Unable to assess n =3 [UTA=3- ill] long-term follow-up Control Group Assessed n= 19 Drop out n = 0 Died n = 0 Whole Practice group Assessed n= 19 Dropped out n = 0 short-term follow-up Control Group Assessed n=14 Unable to assess n =5 [UTA=2 – ill; 1= refused; 2= away] Drop out n = 0 Part Practice Group Assessed n=21 Unable to assess n= 1 [ill] Dropped out n = 0 Whole Practice Group n=20 Received intervention n= 19 Drop out before intervention n=1 Outcome 3 end intervention Control Group Assessed n=19 Dropped out n = 0 Part Practice Group n = 23 Received intervention n= 22 Drop out before intervention n=1 Figure 8.1. CONSORT DIAGRAM 205 Chapter 8 Results 8.3.2 Missing Data During the study period, there were a small number of occasions when it was not possible to undertake data collection. In order to identify valid and missing data points, these occurrences are summarised in table 8.3. with ensuing discussion of the underlying reason. Valid Missing TIMEPOINT group n Percent n Percent BASELINE control 19* 100.0% 0 .0% part 22 100.0% 0 .0% whole 19 100.0% 0 .0% 95.0% 0 .0% 1 4.5% END INTERVENTION SHORT-TERM FOLLOW-UP LONG-TERM FOLLOW-UP Table 8.3: OF control 19* part 21 95.5% whole 19 100.0% 0 .0% control 14* 70.0% 5 30.0% part 19 86.4% 3 13.6% whole 18 94.7% 1 5.3% control 19* 95.0% 0 .0% part 21 95.5% 1 4.5% whole 18 94.7% 1 5.3% Summary of valid and missing data points for all outcome measures at each measurement timepoint. *=n-1 for Stroke Impact Scale data due to aphasia of participant C46 At baseline there was 100% completion of all outcome measures, with the exception of the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) for one participant (C46) who had dysphasia and tired easily. The lack of SIS data for this one participant was constant at each measurement point. At the end of intervention measurement point, data were missing for one participant (P17). This participant had developed trigeminal 206 Chapter 8 Results neuralgia just prior to the end of the intervention phase and was extremely unwell with severe pain. It was therefore deemed inappropriate to visit. At the short-term follow-up (retention) measurement point data were missing for five participants in the control group, three PP participants and one WP participant. The reasons for the missing data for five control group participants were: illness (n=2 C45 and C58), refused visit (C9), away on short break (C2) and moving accommodation (C63). Missing data for the three PP participants was due to: trigeminal neuralgia (P17) and illness (n=2 P4 and P8). The one WP participant for whom data were missing at the short-term follow-up point had been admitted to hospital for an acute, non-stroke related, condition (W61). At the final, long-term follow-up measurement point, data were missing for one PP subject (P31) who cancelled two appointments and refused a further attempt to organise a visit. The other missing data point was, sadly, due to the death of one WP participant (W21). Given the nature of this community- based RCT, the amount of missing data is relatively low and the reasons for missed outcome measurement visits were not unexpected. 8.4 8.4.1 Global Measures of Impairment, Activity and Participation The Barthel Index (BI) - Descriptive Data (BI) The Barthel Index is an ordinal scale with a possible score between zero and 20 (Mahoney & Barthel, 1965). Numerical median, 25th and 75th percentile and mean and standard deviation data are shown in table 8.4. The median, interquartile range 207 Chapter 8 Results and range of the BI scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figure 8.2. Control Group Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (s.d) End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (s.d) Short-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (s.d) Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (s.d) Significance Testing Within Groups Table 8.4: Part Practice Group Whole Group Practice Significance Test Between groups 18 15 19 17 (2.5) 18 17 19 17.7 (1.7) 17 15 19 16.8 (2.3) NS 18 16 19 17.3 (2.3) 19 17.5 19.5 18.2 (2.0) 18 16 19 17.6 (2.1) NS 18.5 15.75 19 17.5 (2.3) 18 18 19 18.2 (1.8) 18 15.75 19.25 17.7 (2.2) NS NS 18 15 19 17.2 (2.4) 18 17 19.5 18 (1.9) 18 16 19.25 17.6 (2.4) NS p<0.005 p<0.005 Descriptive data for Barthel Index total scores At baseline measure, BI scores showed very little variation with only a difference of one point in the median value for any of the groups. All groups increased their median BI score from baseline to outcome measure taken at the end of intervention. At short term follow-up the median score for the part-practice group had returned to baseline value and stayed at that value at long-term follow-up. The pattern for both the control and the whole practice groups showed the median BI score reduced slightly at both short-term and long-term follow-up, but did not return to baseline 208 Chapter 8 Results values. It may be that the missing data for three subjects in the part-practice group at short-term follow up may have affected these data, however the control group had double the number of missing subjects at this short-term follow up time point and data from the control group did not return to baseline median values. Figure 8.2 Box Plot of scores on Barthel Index 8.4.1.1 Statistical Testing for Barthel Index data Data were analysed to see if there were any differences both within-group over time, and also whether there were any differences between-groups at each time point. 209 Chapter 8 Results 8.4.1.2 Within group comparisons – Barthel Index In order to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time, a Friedman’s Anova was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the three subsequent measurements following the intervention period. The Barthel Index for Con participants did not significantly change during the study period, 2 (3) = 1.67, p=0.64. The part practice participants Barthel Index score did show a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 14.73, p<0.005. The whole practice participants Barthel Index score also demonstrated a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 16.6, p<0.005. Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were used to follow up this data for the part-practice and whole-practice participants. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and so all effects are reported at an adjusted 0.008 level of significance (0.05/6). For the part practice participants it was found that while the Barthel Index score was not significantly greater at the end of intervention compared to baseline, z = 2.5, p= 0.01, r = 0.55; the Barthel Index score was, however, significantly greater at the short-term follow up timepoint compared to baseline, z = 2.67, p=0.008, r = 0.61. For the whole practice participants a similar pattern was demonstrated. The Barthel Index score was significantly greater at the end of intervention compared to baseline, z = 2.95, p= 0.003, r = 0.68; the Barthel Index score was also significantly greater at the short-term follow up timepoint compared to baseline, z = 3.18, p=0.001, r = 0.75. 210 Chapter 8 Results 8.4.1.3 Between group comparisons – Barthel Index To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, part-practice or whole practice participants Barthel Index scores at baseline H (2) = 1.67, p = 0.44; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.37, p = 0.51; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 0.61, p = 0.74; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.84, p = 0.66. 8.4.2 Frenchay Activity Index (FAI) 8.4.2.1 Descriptive Data for Frenchay Activities Index The Frenchay Activity Index is a four point ordinal scale, with sub-components for activities undertaken over the past three or six months and a summed total score. Possible scores range between zero and 30 for the three month sub-component, zero and 15 for the six month component and a total possible score ranging between zero and 45 (Holbrook and Skilbeck 1983). Numerical median, 25th and 75th percentile, mean and standard deviation data are shown in table 8.5. Median, interquartile range and range of the FAI scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figure 8.3. At baseline measure, FAI scores showed slight variation between the groups, but only with a three point difference for the median values between the groups (Con 7, PP 10.5 and WP10). All groups increased their median FAI score from baseline to the end of intervention and this change was maintained at the long-term follow-up. 211 Chapter 8 Results FAI past 6 months Significance Test Between groups FAI total FAI past 3 months con part whole con part whole con part whole BASELINE Median 7 10.5 10 3 3 4 11 14.5 14 th 6 6.75 5 2 1.75 4 8 9.25 9 th 18 17 4 5 5 21 20 25 percentile 75 percentile Means (sd) 11 11 (6.25) (5.43) 15 10.7 (6.1) 3.3 3.4 (1.6) (1.9) 4 (0.9) 14.3 14.3 (7.1) (6.3) NS 20 14.7 (6.5) END INTERVENTION Median th 11 13 12 2 3 4 13 17 19 25 percentile 5 9 5 2 2 4 9 11.5 10 75th percentile 22 17.5 18 4 5 6 25 25.5 23 Means (s.d) 13.2 13 (8.4) (5.8) 11.8 (6.5) 3.5 4.3 (1.6) (2.6) 4.7 (1.4) 16.7 17.2 (9.2) (7.5) NS 16.5 (7.1) LONG-TERM FOLLOW-UP Median 9 12 13.5 4 3 5 14 16 18.5 25th percentile 4 7.5 8.5 3 2 4 8 11.5 10.5 75th percentile 23 16.5 19.25 5 5.5 6 27 23 24 Means (s.d) 12.9 12.4 (8.6) (6.3) 13.4 (5.7) Significance test within groups NS p<0. P<0.0 NS p<0. P<0.0 NS p<0. P<0.0 01 5 01 5 01 5 Table 8.5: 4.1 3.9 (1.6) (2.3) 4.7 (1.2) 17 16.3 (9.6) (7.6) NS 17.3 (7.2) Descriptive Data for Frenchay Activity Index scores 212 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.3 Boxplot of Frenchay Activity Index total scores 8.4.2.2 Within group comparisons Frenchay Activity Index A within-group comparison was undertaken using a Friedman’s ANOVA on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term followup. For both part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants, the Frenchay Activity Index score did show a statistically significant change during the study period, PP: 2 (2) = 8.96, p=0.01; WP: 2 (2) = 6.84, p=0.03. The control participants Frenchay Activity Index total score did not significantly change during the study period, 2 (2) = 3.61, p=0.16. 213 Chapter 8 Results Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were used to follow up this data for the part-practice and whole-practice participants. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and so all effects are reported at a 0.017 level of significance (0.05/3). For the part practice participants it was found that the total FAI score was significantly greater at the end of intervention compared to baseline, z = -3.47, p< 0.005, r = 0.74. For the part practice group, there were no statistically significant differences at any of the other time points. For the whole practice participants the same pattern was demonstrated. The Frenchay Activity Index total score was only significantly greater at the end of intervention compared to baseline, z =-2.4, p<0.05, r = 0.55, but no other statistically significant differences were found at any of the other timepoints. 8.4.2.3 Between group comparisons Frenchay Activity Index To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, part-practice or whole practice participants Frenchay Activity Index scores at baseline H (2) = 0.04, p = 0.98; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.14, p = 0.93; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.11, p = 0.95. 214 Chapter 8 Results 8.4.3 Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) 8.4.3.1 Descriptive Data Motor Assessment Scale The Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) is an ordinal rating scale consisting of eight items. Each item can be assigned a score of between one and six, giving a total possible score of between eight and 48 (Carr and Shepherd 1985). Numerical median, 25th and 75th percentile, mean and standard deviation data are shown in table 8.6. The median, interquartile range and range of the MAS scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figure 8.4. Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Short-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Significance test within groups Control Group Part Practice Group Whole Practice Group Significance test between groups 26 19 35 26.5 (8.9) 27 20.75 34.25 28.1 (7.9) 28 19 35 26.5 (8.5) NS 29 21 39 29.4 (9.0) 32 23.5 40 31.7 (8.4) 35 19 37 29.1 (10.0) NS 27 18.5 34.5 27 (9.4) 32 24 39 31.5 (8.0) 35 19 38.25 29.7 (10.2) NS 31 20 38 29.5 (8.9) 34 22 39 31.9 (8.5) 32.5 19.75 40.25 29.7 (10.3) NS p<0.005 p<0.005 p<0.005 Table 8.6: Descriptive data for the Motor Assessment Scale total scores 215 Chapter 8 Results Motor Assessment Scale total scores at baseline showed very little variation between the groups with only a difference of two points in the median value for any of the groups (Control 26, PP 27, WP 28). All groups increased their median MAS total score from baseline to outcome measure taken at the end of intervention and short-term follow-up, although this change was less for the control group (increase of three points) than the PP or WP group (increase by five and seven points respectively).. At long-term follow-up the median score for the WP had reduced slightly, although not to baseline value, while the control and the PP groups demonstrated a continued small increase in median score. The fact that the control group increased their MAS score was unexpected and will be discussed further in the Discussion chapter (see 9.4.4). Figure 8.4 Boxplot of Motor Assessment Scale total scores 216 Chapter 8 Results 8.4.3.2 Statistical Testing for Motor Assessment Scale data Data were analysed to see if there were any differences both within-group over time, and also whether there were any differences between-groups at each time point for MAS scores 8.4.3.3 Within group comparisons Motor Assessment Scale total scores In order to compare whether there were any changes in MAS scores within each group over time, a Friedman’s Anova was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the three subsequent measurements following the intervention period. Both the intervention (PP and WP) as well as the control participants showed an improvement in MAS total score over time. Control participants MAS score showed a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 17.05, p<0.005; PP participants also demonstrated a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 31.3, p<0.005 and WP participants also demonstrated a statistically significant change on the MAS total score during the study period, 2 (3) = 15.08, p<0.005. Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were used to follow up this data for all groups. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and so all effects are reported at a 0.008 level of significance (0.05/6). For the control group participants, statistically significant differences were found between baseline and the other three outcome measurement points. These significant differences were between baseline and end of intervention z = -3.19, p < 0.005, r = 0.73; baseline and short-term follow-up z =- 2.81, p< 0.005, r = 0.65; and 217 Chapter 8 Results between baseline and long-term follow-up z = -3.29, p< 0.005, r = 0.75. No statistically significant differences were found for MAS total scores for the control group at any other time point comparisons. The same pattern was found for the PP group. Statistically significant differences were found between baseline and end of intervention z = -3.73, p <0.005, r = 0.8; baseline and short-term follow-up z =- 3.63, p<0.005, r = 0.78; and between baseline and long-term follow-up z = -3.45, p< 0.005, r = 0.74. No statistically significant differences were found for MAS total scores for the PP group at any other time point comparisons. A similar pattern was found again for the WP group on MAS total scores. For the WP group however, only one time point demonstrated a statistically significant difference. This was found between baseline and the short-term follow-up point z = -2.77, p < 0.005, r = 0.64. While no statistically significant differences were found for MAS total scores for the WP group at any other time point comparisons, a trend towards significance was demonstrated between the baseline and end of intervention z =- 2.63, p= 0.009; and between baseline and long-term follow-up z = 2.63, p= 0.009. 8.4.3.4 Between group comparisons Motor Assessment Scale total score To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, part-practice or whole practice 218 Chapter 8 Results participants Motor Assessment Scale total scores at baseline H (2) = 0.62, p = 0.74; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.30, p = 0.52; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 1.93, p = 0.38; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.93, p = 0.63. 8.5 Measures of Mobility 8.5.1 The Timed Up and Go over 2 metres (TUG2m) – descriptive data The Timed Up and Go is a validated timed test of walking, balance and transfer ability that was first developed by Podsiadlo and Richardson (1991). As described in chapter 7, the original distance was modified from three to two metres (Timed Up and Go over 2m – TUG2m) and the validity established with people with stroke (Baer et al, 2003). Data relating to total time, gait speed and time to stand up will be presented. Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data are shown in table 8.7. Boxplots showing the distribution of data for the TUG2m for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figures 8.5 a, 8.5.b. and 8.5.c. The TUG2m total time demonstrated very little variation between the groups with no more than five seconds between median values at any of the timepoints. All groups showed a very slight decrease in TUG2m total time as the study progressed, however the stability of the time to perform the items in this measure is a striking feature. A minority of participants in the control group demonstrated extreme values when undertaking the TUG2m (up to four minutes) and the reasons for this will be discussed in chapter 9 (section 9.4.2). 219 Chapter 8 Results TUG2m total time (s) Gait speed m/s Time to stand up (s) control part whole control part whole control part whole BASELINE Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range 32.85 20.68 48.41 27.73 30.83 15.9 55.11 40.79 35.36 22.33 59.41 37.08 0.21 0.11 0.36 0.25 0.21 0.12 0.44 0.32 0.21 0.13 0.26 0.13 2.93 2.11 4.65 2.54 2.6 1.94 4.07 2.13 3.22 1.87 4.67 2.8 29.98 23.52 47.4 23.88 26.23 14.4 51.79 37.39 30.26 22.59 62.29 39.7 0.26 0.12 0.32 0.2 0.24 0.12 0.53 0.41 0.24 0.1 0.39 0.29 2.84 1.96 4.19 2.23 2.65 1.44 4.43 2.99 3.6 1.93 4.76 2.83 31.48 22.78 48.36 25.58 28.63 11.58 49.72 38.14 28.98 21.74 57.68 35.94 0.25 0.15 0.36 0.21 0.32 0.13 0.55 0.42 0.2 0.12 0.35 0.23 2.9 2.08 4.16 2.08 2.42 1.25 3.96 2.71 2.67 1.56 5.4 3.84 27.42 19.86 55.7 35.84 27.94 15.28 46.16 30.88 31.24 20.04 51.68 31.64 0.25 0.1 0.42 0.32 0.23 0.16 0.43 0.27 0.2 0.12 0.38 0.26 3.51 1.86 4.1 2.24 2.23 1.65 4.39 2.74 3.23 1.81 5.76 3.95 END INTERVENTION Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range SHORT-TERM FOLLOW-UP Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range LONG-TERM FOLLOW-UP Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.7: Descriptive Data for Timed Up and Go 2m components The TUG2m gait speed was relatively stable over time, with median gait speed consistently recorded at 0.2 – 0.25 m/s. All groups, including the control group, exhibited a slight increase in gait speed demonstrated at the end of intervention, however the improvement was transient and returned to approximate baseline levels by long-term follow-up. 220 Chapter 8 Results The time to rise to stand (RTS) (the first component of the TUG2m), demonstrated very little variation between the groups median values at any of the timepoints, however there was quite some disparity in the time taken to RTS. All groups contained a minority of participants who exhibited marked difficulty in rising from a chair, as revealed by times in excess of 10 seconds, discussion relating to this characteristic is presented in chapter 9 (section 9.4.2). Figure 8.5.a. Boxplot of Timed Up and Go 2m total time 221 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.5.b. Boxplot of Timed Up and Go 2m – gait speed Figure 8.5.c. Boxplot of Rise to Stand (RTS) time during TUG2m 222 Chapter 8 Results 8.5.1.1 Statistical Testing for Timed Up and Go 2m (TUG2m) data Data were analysed to determine if there were any differences both within-group over time, and also whether there were any differences between-groups at each time point for TUG2m total time; TUG2m gait speed and TUG2m time to stand. 8.5.1.2 Within group comparisons TUG2m total time A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the TUG2m total time data for each group from baseline and at the three subsequent measurement points following the intervention period. Total time stayed relatively stable over time for all groups. There was no statistically significant difference for any of the groups during the study period: Control participants: 2 (3) = 3.77, p=0.29; PP participants: 2 (3) = 6.67, p = 0.08; WP participants: 2 (3) = 5.47, p=0.14. As there were no statistically significant differences, no post hoc tests were undertaken. 8.5.1.3 Between group comparisons TUG2m total time To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point for TUG2m total time, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. No statistically significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP groups TUG2m total time at baseline H (2) = 0.73, p = 0.69; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.73, p = 0.69; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 0.75, p = 0.70; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.75, p = 0.69. 223 Chapter 8 Results 8.5.1.4 Within group comparisons TUG2m gait speed A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the TUG2m gait speed data for each group from baseline and at the three subsequent measurement points following the intervention period. There was no statistically significant difference for any of the groups during the study period: Control participants - 2 (3) = 2.96, p=0.40; PP participants - 2 (3) = 2.44, p = 0.49; WP participants - 2 (3) = 1.59, p=0.66. As there were no statistically significant differences, no post hoc tests were necessary. 8.5.1.5 Between group comparisons TUG2m gait speed To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point for gait speed on the TUG2m, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, PP or WP groups TUG2m gait speed at baseline H (2) = 0.30, p = 0.86; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.57, p = 0.75; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 0.78, p = 0.68; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.60, p = 0.74. 8.5.1.6 Within group comparisons rising to stand A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the TUG2m gait speed data for each group from baseline and at the three subsequent measurement points following the intervention period. There was no statistically significant difference for any of the groups during the study period: control participants - 2 (3) = 0.24, p=0.50; PP participants - 2 (3) = 3.91, p = 0.27; WP participants - 2 (3) = 2.86, p=0.41. As there were no statistically significant differences, no post hoc tests were necessary. 224 Chapter 8 Results 8.5.1.7 Between group comparisons rising to stand (RTS) To identify whether there were any differences between the groups in time taken to rise to stand during the TUG2m, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the data. It was found that there was no statistically significant difference in the RTS time for control, PP or WP groups TUG2m at baseline H (2) = 0.51, p = 0.78; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.99, p = 0.61; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 1.77, p = 0.41; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 1.15, p = 0.56. 8.5.2 The Step Test - Descriptive Data The Step Test is a simple measure of dynamic balance and produces ratio level data of the number of steps onto a block are achieved within a set time period (Hill 1996). Data were collected both for the Step Test with both the impaired and nonimpaired limb. Median, percentile and interquartile range data are presented in table 8.8 and graphical representation of median, interquartile range and range of the Step Test scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figures 8.6.a. and 8.6.b. The Step Test scores at baseline showed a slight difference between median scores. For stepping up with the unaffected foot, the WP group median score was zero compared to median of two for the control and PP groups. During the course of the study, when stepping with the unaffected foot, both the PP and WP groups increased their median Step Test scores from baseline and this persisted to the end of the long-term follow-up. Interestingly, the control group demonstrated a similar pattern, although the changes were smaller. 225 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Short-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range UNAFFECTED LEG AFFECTED LEG stepping up stepping up control Part Whole Control Part Whole 2 0 4 4 2 0 3.5 3.5 0 0 3 3 0 0 2 2 1.5 0 3.25 3.25 0 0 3 3 1 0 5 5 4 0.5 6 5.5 3 0 4 4 2 0 3 3 2 0 5 5 2 0 3 3 2 0 7 7 4 0 7 7 1.5 0 5 5 3 0 4.25 4.25 3 0 5 5 2 0 3 3 2 0 6 6 4 0 8 8 2 0 5 5 2 0 5 5 3 0 4.5 4.5 1 0 3 3 Table 8.8: Descriptive data for number of steps on the Step Test Examining the data for stepping up with the affected leg, a similar pattern of change was found, although the magnitude of improvement was smaller for the PP and WP groups than the control group. The observation that the control group demonstrated positive changes on the Step Test was unexpected and will be discussed further in the Discussion chapter (see section 9.4.3). 226 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.6.a. Boxplot of Step Test - number of steps onto block with unaffected leg 8.5.2.1 Statistical Testing - Step Test data Data were analysed for the Step Test to see if there were any differences both within-group over time, and also whether there were any differences betweengroups at each time point for Step Test scores. The number of steps on the Step Test with affected foot and with the unaffected foot were analysed separately. 227 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.6.b. Boxplot of Step Test - number of steps onto block with affected leg 8.5.2.2 Within group comparisons Step Test scores A comparison of changes in Step Test scores within each group over time was undertaken using, Friedman’s ANOVA. Data for each group were analysed to determine if there were any changes from the baseline to the three subsequent measurements following the intervention period. Step Test performance with the unaffected leg for PP participants demonstrated a statistically significant change during the study period, Friedman’s ANOVA test 2 (3) = 15.52, p<0.005, and while there was a strong trend towards improvement for the WP group, this did not reach statistical significance WP participants 2 (3) = 7.59, p=0.055. The control participants demonstrated no statistically significant changes in ability to step up with the unaffected leg during the study period, 2 (3) = 6.118, p=0.106. 228 Chapter 8 Results Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were used to follow up this data for the PP group. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and so all effects are reported at an adjusted 0.008 level of significance (0.05/6). For the PP group, Wilcoxon signed ranks tests demonstrated that the statistically significant improvements in Step Test performance with the unaffected leg occurred between baseline and the end of intervention, z = -2.84, p<0.005, r= 0.61; between baseline and short-term follow-up z = -2.69, p<0.00, r= 0.57; and between baseline and longterm follow-up z = -2.99, p<0.00, r= 0.64.. Step Test performance with the affected leg. Friedmans ANOVA showed no statistically significant changes for the PP group 2 (3) = 5.64, p=0.13 when performing the step test with the affected leg. The WP group demonstrated a trend towards improvement, however, this did not reach statistical significance 2 (3) = 6.60, p=0.08,. Surprisingly, the control group did demonstrate a statistically significant improvement in performance of the step test with the affected leg over time, 2 (3) = 8.97, p=0.03. Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test with a Bonferroni correction with significance level at 0.008 level were used to determine where the differences occurred. It was found that statistically significant improvements in Step Test performance with the affected leg for the control group occurred only between baseline and the end of intervention, z = -2.74, p=0.006, r= 0.62. The reasons for this finding will be considered in the Discussion (section 9.4.3). 229 Chapter 8 Results 8.5.2.3 Between group comparisons – Step Test To compare whether there were any differences in step up ability between the groups at each time point, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the Step Test data. For stepping up with the unaffected leg, necessitating balance and weightbearing ability on the affected leg, no statistically significant differences were found between the control, part-practice or whole practice participants Step Test scores at baseline H (2) = 1.08, p = 0.58; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 2.19, p = 0.33; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 1.59, p = 0.45; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 2.58, p = 0.28. Stepping up with the affected leg, demonstrated a similar pattern, with no statistically significant differences found between the control, part-practice or whole practice participants Step Test scores at baseline H (2) = 1.20, p = 0.55; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.40, p = 0.82; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 1.67, p = 0.43; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 1.05, p = 0.60. 8.6 8.6.1 Measures of arm and hand function The Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) – descriptive data The Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) is a five item, dichotomous scale of unilateral and bilateral upper limb tasks (Heller et al 1987). Items are either able to be performed (score 1) or not performed (score 0), giving a total possible score between 0 – 5. Numerical median, 25th and 75th percentile, mean and standard deviation data are shown in table 8.9. The median, interquartile range and range of the FAT scores for the Con, PP and WP participants are shown graphically in figure 8.7. 230 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Short-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Mean (sd) Significance test within groups Control Group Part Practice Group Whole Practice Group Significance test between groups 2 0 4 2.1 (2.0) 2 0 4 1.9 (1.7) 0.5 0 4 1.8 (2.1) NS 3 0 4 2.3 (2.0) 3 0 4.5 2.5 (2.1) 2 0 4 1.9 (2.1) NS 1 0 4 1.9 (2.0) 3 0 4 2.3 (2.1) 2 0 4 2.0 (2.1) NS 2 1 4 2.5 (2.0) 3.5 0.25 4 2.6 (1.8) 2 0 4 2.2 (2.1) NS p<0.005 p<0.005 NS Table 8.9: Descriptive data for the Frenchay Arm Test The Frenchay Arm Test scores at baseline showed a slight difference between median scores with the WP group being slightly lower than the mean for the control or PP groups. During the course of the study, both the PP and WP groups increased their median FAT scores from baseline and this persisted to the end of the long-term follow-up. The control group showed more variation with an unexpected increase in the median score of one point at the end of “intervention” but this dropped at short-term follow up. The fact that the control group demonstrated positive changes on the FAT score was unexpected and will be discussed further in chapter 9 (section 9.4.3). 231 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.7 Boxplot of Frenchay Arm Test scores 8.6.1.1 Statistical Testing for Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) data Data were analysed for the FAT to see if there were any differences both withingroup over time, and also whether there were any differences between-groups at each time point for FAT scores 8.6.1.2 Within group comparisons Frenchay Arm Test scores In order to compare whether there were any changes in FAT scores within each group over time, a Friedman’s Anova was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the three subsequent measurements following the intervention period. FAT scores for PP participants demonstrated a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 13.29, p<0.005. There were, 232 Chapter 8 Results however, no statistically significant changes for WP participants 2 (3) = 4.71, p=0.19. Interestingly, the control participants also showed a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (3) = 12.35, p<0.005. Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were used to follow up this data for the PP and the control groups. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and all effects are reported at a 0.008 level of significance (0.05/6). This analysis demonstrated that the statistically significant improvement on FAT scores for PP participants occured between the baseline and end of intervention measures z = 2.72, p=0.006, r=0.58. For the FAT scores of the control group participants, once the Bonferroni correction was applied, there were no statistically significant differences between any of the time point comparisons at the 0.008 level. This may be due to the conservative nature of the Bonferroni method (Altman 1991). There was a trend towards significance between baseline and short-term follow up for the FAT scores of control subjects z=2.0, p = 0.05. Post hoc analysis with Wilcoxon tests were not applied to the WP FAT scores as the initial Friedman’s ANOVA did not demonstrate any statistically significant differences in this group over time. 8.6.1.3 Between group comparisons Frenchay Arm Test score To compare whether there were any differences between the groups at each time point, a Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the FAT data. No statistically significant differences were found between the control, part-practice or whole practice participants FAT scores at baseline H (2) = 0.37, p = 0.83; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.04, p = 0.60; at the short-term follow up period H (2) = 0.45, p = 0.80; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.54, p = 0.77. 233 Chapter 8 Results 8.7 8.7.1 Measures of Mood The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) descriptive data The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) is a simple, 14 item screening tool to identify depression and anxiety (Zigmond and Snaith 1983). HADS uses a four point ordinal scale, to score seven items to screen for depression and seven items to screen for anxiety. Each sub-scale has a total possible score range from zero to 21. Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data are shown in table 8.10. The median, interquartile range and range of the HADS scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown graphically in figures 8.8.a. and 8.8.b. For the HADS-A subscale, all groups demonstrated the same pattern of change. Scores reduced from baseline to the end of intervention and this was followed by a slight increase at long-term follow-up. This pattern of change was also demonstrated by the WP and control groups on the HADS-D subscale, however the PP group showed a very slight increase in HADS-D score over time – this change was less than two points in the median value. 8.7.1.1 Statistical Testing for the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale data Data for both subscales, were analysed to determine if there were any differences both within-group over time, and also whether there were any differences betweengroups at each time point. 234 Chapter 8 Results HADS-A control part HADS-D whole control part whole BASELINE Median 6 6 8 6 4 6 th 4 4 4.25 3 3 3.75 th 75 percentile 7 11.5 10 7 7.75 8.25 Interquartile range 3 7.5 5.75 4 4.75 4.5 25 percentile END INTERVENTION Median 4 5 4 5 5 5 th 2 4 3 3 2.25 3 th 75 percentile 6 9.75 7 7 7.75 6 Interquartile range 4 5.75 4 4 5.5 3 25 percentile LONG-TERM FOLLOW-UP Median 5 6 5.5 5 5.5 6 th 2 5 1.75 3 3.25 3.75 th 75 percentile 6 8.75 7 7 8 7.5 Interquartile range 4 3.75 5.25 4 4.75 3.75 25 percentile Table 8.10: Descriptive data for the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Figure 8.8.a Boxplot of Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Anxiety subscale 235 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.8.b. Boxplot of Hospital Anxiety & Depression Scale – Depression subscale 8.7.1.2 Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) Within group comparisons HADS-A In order to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time, a Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. Participants in the WP group demonstrated a statistically significant reduction in their anxiety level, during the duration of the study, 2 (2) = 8.09, p=0.02. This statistically significant finding was also demonstrated by the control participants, whose HADS-A subscale score demonstrated a statistically significant change during the study period, 2 (2) = 7.86, p=0.02. There was no statistically significant difference for the PP participants, 2 (2) = 2.64, p=0.27. 236 Chapter 8 Results Post hoc tests using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test were undertaken to follow up these findings for HADS-A for the WP and control participants. A Bonferroni correction was applied to the data and so all effects are reported at an adjusted 0.017 level of significance (0.05/3). For the Control group the HADS-A subscale score was statistically significantly reduced between baseline and the end of intervention, z = -2.58, p = 0.010, r = 0.59. No other statistically significant differences were demonstrated at any of the other time point comparisons for the Control group. Although the Friedman’s ANOVA had demonstrated a statistically significant difference on HADS-A for both the WP and control groups over time, on post hoc Wilcoxon tests applying the Bonferroni correction, the significant difference for the WP group could not be confirmed by statistical testing. A trend towards significance was noted for WP comparisons between the HADS-A baseline and end of intervention measures z = -2.27, p=0.02, r = 0.52 and between baseline and longterm follow-up z = -2.26, p=0.02, r = 0.52. HADS-D A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the subscale data for each group on the HADS-D from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time. There was no statistically significant difference on the HADS-D subscale over time for any of the groups, PP participants, 2 (2) = 2.32, p=0.31, WP 2 (2) = 2.8, p=0.25, Control participants, 2 (2) = 2.91, p=0.23. 237 Chapter 8 Results 8.7.1.3 Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)- Between group comparisons The HADS data were analysed for differences between the groups at each timepoint for the subscales of Anxiety and Depression. HADS-A Between Group analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the HADS Anxiety subscale data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, PP or WP participants HADS-A subscale score at baseline H (2) = 1.61, p = 0.49; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.45, p = 0.49; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 3.39, p = 0.18 HADS-D Between Group analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the HADS Depression subscale data. It was found that there was no significant difference between the control, PP or WP participants HADS-A subscale score at baseline H (2) = 1.09, p = 0.58; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.66, p = 0.72; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.29, p = 0.86. 8.8 8.8.1 Measure of Health Status The Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) descriptive data The Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) is a self-report, stroke specific questionnaire that provides a multidimensional measure of the effects of stroke. It consists of 59 items that measure outcomes, from the patient perspective, in eight domains (Duncan et al 2002). The domains covered are: Strength, Hand function, Activities of Daily Living (ADL), Mobility, Communication, Memory and thinking, Emotion and 238 Chapter 8 Results Participation. Domain scores are transformed to give a scale of 0 -100. In addition to the eight domains, there is a vertical visual analogue scale (0 – 100) for respondents to rate self-perceived recovery. Each domain will be reported separately. 8.8.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Strength (SIS-str) Descriptive Data Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SIS-str are shown in table 8.11. The median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-str domain scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown in figure 8.9. Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.11 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 46.88 35.94 56.25 20.31 37.5 29.7 50 20.3 25 25 50 25 43.75 37.5 62.5 25 43.75 37.5 62.5 25 37.5 18.75 56.25 37.5 46.88 31.25 60.94 29.69 43.75 37.5 65.6 28.1 43.75 18.75 59.38 40.63 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – strength domain Clear perceived improvements in strength were reported by WP participants, with an increase in median score of 18 points over the course of the study, while more modest improvements (an increase in median score of five points) were reported by 239 Chapter 8 Results PP participants. The control participants perceived their strength as being relatively stable over the course of the study. Figure 8.9 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale – strength domain 8.8.2.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Strength (SIS-str) Within group Statistical Analysis In order to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time, a Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. The PP group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement for SIS-str domain over time 2 (2) = 9.32, p<0.005. Post hoc analysis using the Wilcoxon 240 Chapter 8 Results signed ranks test with Bonferroni correction at 0.017 was undertaken on the SIS-str domain for the PP group. A statistically significant difference was found between baseline and end of intervention on SIS-str for the PP group z = -3.05, p < 0.005, r = 0.65 and a trend towards significance was found between baseline and long-term follow-up z = -2.35, p = 0.019. The WP group and the control group demonstrated no statistically significant differences in SIS-str domain scores over time as tested by Friedmans ANOVA: WP group 2 (2) = 1.75, p=0.42; control group 2 (2) = 1.82, p=0.4. 8.8.2.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Strength (SIS-str) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-str domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-str domain score at baseline H (2) = 4.96, p = 0.08; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.84, p = 0.40; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.97, p = 0.62. 8.8.3 Stroke Impact Scale – Memory (SIS-mem) Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SISmem are shown in table 8.12. As can be seen, the data for all groups are relatively stable with median scores consistently over 80, which indicates that memory was not perceived as a major issue for this cohort. The median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-mem domain scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown in figure 8.10. 241 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 85.71 56.25 100 43.75 82.14 67.86 93.75 25.89 82.14 64.29 96.43 32.14 82.14 68.75 100 31.25 89.29 70.54 97.32 26.78 92.86 71.43 96.43 25 85.71 72.32 100 27.68 89.29 64.29 100 35.71 78.57 66.07 92.86 25.79 Table 8.12 Median and IQR data for Stroke Impact Scale – memory domain Figure 8.10 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale – memory domain 242 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.3.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Memory (SIS-mem) Within group Statistical Analysis In order to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time, a Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. There were no statistically significant changes on the SIS-mem domain over time for any of the groups: Control 2 (2) = 2.13, p=0.35; PP group 2 (2) = 2.68, p=0.26; WP group 2 (2) = 4.11, p=0.13. 8.8.3.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Memory (SIS-mem) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-mem domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-mem domain score at baseline H (2) = 0.09, p = 0.96; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.12, p = 0.94; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.53, p = 0.77. 8.8.4 Stroke Impact Scale – Mood (SIS-mood) Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SISmood are shown in table 8.13. As can be seen, the SIS-mood data for the control group reduce slightly over the course of the study, the PP group show a slight increase after intervention and the WP group stay relatively stable during the study. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-mood domain scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown in figure 8.11. 243 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 69.44 63.89 79.17 15.28 61.11 54.17 78.47 24.3 69.44 58.33 80.56 21.23 66.67 58.33 86.81 28.48 73.61 64.58 81.25 16.67 69.44 61.11 77.78 16.67 62.5 58.3 73.61 15.28 70.83 54.17 80.56 25.39 66.67 55.56 83.33 27.77 Table 8.13 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – Mood domain Figure 8.11 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale – mood domain 244 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.4.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Mood (SIS-mood) Within group Statistical Analysis In order to compare whether there were any changes within each group over time, a Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline to the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. The PP group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement for SIS-mood domain over time 2 (2) = 11.09, p<0.005. Post hoc analysis using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test with Bonferroni correction at 0.017 was undertaken on the SISmood domain for the PP group. This further analysis revealed a statistically significant difference between baseline and end of intervention on SIS-mood: PP group z = -3.3, p < 0.005, r = 0.7. No significant differences were found at any other timepoint comparisons. The WP group and the control group showed no statistically significant differences in SIS-mood domain scores over time as tested by Friedmans ANOVA: WP group 2 (2) = 1.66, p=0.44; control group 2 (2) = 3.19, p=0.2. 8.8.4.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Mood (SIS-mood) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-mood domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant differences were found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-mood domain score at baseline H (2) = 1.97, p = 0.37; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.48, p = 0.79; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.64, p = 0.73. 245 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.5 Stroke Impact Scale – Communication (SIS-comm) Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SIScomm are shown in table 8.14. With a few exceptions, the SIS-comm data are clustered towards the high score end of the domain. As might be expected in a cohort of people with late-stage stroke, there was minimal variability in communication ability for any group over time. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-comm domain scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants are shown in figure 8.12. Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.14 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 92.86 69.94 100 30.06 91.07 85.71 100 14.29 92.86 85.71 100 14.29 87.5 68.75 100 31.25 94.64 83.93 100 16.07 96.43 92.86 100 7.14 96.43 73.21 100 26.79 92.86 87.5 100 12.5 96.43 85.71 100 14.29 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – communication domain 8.8.5.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Communication (SIS-comm) Within group Statistical Analysis A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up to determine whether there were any changes within each group over time. No statistically significant changes 246 Chapter 8 Results on the SIS-comm domain over time for any of the groups were found: Control 2 (2) = 2.8, p=0.25; PP group 2 (2) = 4.43, p=0.11; WP group 2 (2) = 5.05, p=0.08. Figure 8.12 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale – communication domain 8.8.5.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Communication (SIS-comm) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-comm domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-comm domain score 247 Chapter 8 Results at baseline H (2) = 0.52, p = 0.77; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 5.88, p = 0.06; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.15, p = 0.93. 8.8.6 Stroke Impact Scale – Activities of Daily Living (SIS-ADL) domain Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SISADL are shown in table 8.15. While slight variability in SIS-ADL domain scores was demonstrated by all the groups, essentially scores remained stable over time. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-ADL domain scores for the control (CON), part practice (PP) and whole practice (WP) participants is shown in figure 8.13. Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.15 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 51.25 39.38 68.13 28.75 62.5 47.5 68.13 20.63 55 45 62.5 17.5 50 40.63 62.5 21.87 62.5 57.5 70.63 23.13 62.5 35 65 30 53.75 31.88 77.5 45.62 67.5 58.75 72.5 13.75 52.5 41.25 65 23.75 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – ADL domain 248 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.13 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale – ADL domain 8.8.6.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Activities of Daily Living (SIS-ADL) Within group Statistical Analysis A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline, the end of intervention and long-term follow-up to determine whether there were any changes within each group over time. No statistically significant changes on the SIS-ADL domain over time for any of the groups were found: Control 2 (2) = 1.25, p=0.53; PP group 2 (2) = 0.56, p=0.76; WP group 2 (2) = 3.46, p=0.18. 249 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.6.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Activities of Daily Living (SIS-ADL) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-ADL domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-ADL domain score at baseline H (2) = 2.37, p = 0.31; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 3.15, p = 0.21; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 4.30, p = 0.12. 8.8.7 Stroke Impact Scale – Mobility (SIS-mob) domain Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SISmob are shown in table 8.16. The control group demonstrated stability on scores between baseline and end of intervention period, followed by a slight rise in SISmob domain score. The PP group demonstrated an increase in SIS-mob domain score over the three measurement periods, while the WP group demonstrated an initial slight reduction in SIS-mob score, followed by a rise to just above baseline level at the final follow-up measurement point. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-mob domain scores for the control, PP and WP participants is shown in figure 8.14. 250 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 54.17 38.89 75 36.11 59.72 43.75 72.22 28.47 55.56 36.11 66.67 30.56 54.17 40.97 81.25 40.28 62.5 46.53 81.25 34.72 50 47.22 75 27.78 61.11 47.22 79.86 32.64 66.67 43.06 83.33 40.27 55.56 50 65.28 15.28 Table 8.16 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – mobility domain Figure 8.14 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale –mobility domain 251 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.7.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Mobility Analysis (SIS-mob) Within group Statistical A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. The PP group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement for SIS-mob domain over time 2 (2) = 7.29, p=0.03. Post hoc analysis using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test with Bonferroni correction at 0.017 was undertaken on the SISmob domain for the PP group. A statistically significant difference was found between baseline and end of intervention on SIS-mob for the PP group z = -2.59, p = 0.01, r = 0.55, no other timepoint comparisons showed a statistically significant difference for PP SIS-mob. The WP group and the control group showed no statistically significant differences in SIS-mob domain scores over time as tested by Friedman’s ANOVA: WP group 2 (2) = 0.43, p=0.80; control group 2 (2) = 4.67, p=0.09. 8.8.7.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Mobility (SIS-mob) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-mob domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-mob domain score at baseline H (2) = 0.53, p = 0.77; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 1.45, p = 0.48; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 1.13, p = 0.57. 252 Chapter 8 Results 8.8.8 Stroke Impact Scale – Hand (SIS-hnd) domain Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SIShnd are shown in table 8.17. The baseline medians are comparable between the control and PP group, however the WP group had a lower median baseline inidicating extreme difficulty with hand function. While there was a slight increase in median SIS-hnd score for the control and WP group after the intervention period, this was not sustained at long-term follow-up. Conversely, the PP group SIS-hnd score remained stable at the end of intervention but increased at long-term followup. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-hnd domain scores for the control, PP and WP participants is shown in figure 8.15 Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.17 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 15 0 45 45 12.5 0 40 40 0 0 35 35 30 0 51.25 51.25 12.5 0 56.25 56.25 15 0 40 40 17.5 0 67.5 67.5 20 7.5 60 52.5 10 0 50 50 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – hand domain 253 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.15 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale –hand domain 8.8.8.1 Stroke Impact Scale – Hand (SIS-hnd) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-hnd domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-hnd domain score at baseline H (2) = 0.68, p = 0.71; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.77, p = 0.68; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 1.57, p = 0.46. 8.8.9 Stroke Impact Scale – Participation (SIS-partic) domain Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for the domain of SISmob are shown in table 8.18. All groups demonstrated an increase in SIS-partic 254 Chapter 8 Results domain scores over time. The potential reasons for this finding will be considered in chapter 9. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-mob domain scores for the control, PP and WP participants is shown in figure 8.16. Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.18 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 35.93 17.97 53.13 35.16 39.06 19.53 69.53 50 37.5 28.13 50 21.87 46.88 25 64.06 39.06 46.88 29.69 60.16 30.47 40.63 28.13 71.88 43.75 51.56 26.56 80.47 53.91 68.75 37.5 75 37.5 50 37.5 76.99 39.49 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – Participation domain 255 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.16 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale –Participation domain 8.8.9.1 Stroke Impact Scale Statistical Analysis –Participation (SIS-partic) Within group A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. Both the PP and WP groups demonstrated a statistically significant improvement for SIS-partic domain over time. PP SIS-partic 2 (2) = 17.26, p<0.00; WP SIS-partic 2 (2) = 13.78, p<0.005. Post hoc analysis using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test with Bonferroni correction at 0.017 was undertaken to determine where the differences lay. It was shown that for the PP group, a statistically significant difference was found between baseline and the long-term follow-up point , z = -2.63, p < 0.005, r = 256 Chapter 8 Results 0.56, and between the end of intervention and the long-term follow-up z = -2.88, p <0.005, r = 0.61. For the WP group, the statistically significant difference was shown between baseline and long-term follow-up z = -2.69, p<0.005, r = 0.62. The control group data showed no statistically significant differences in SIS-partic domain scores over time Friedman’s ANOVA 2 (2) = 3.48, p=0.18. 8.8.9.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Participation (SIS-partic) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the SIS-partic domain data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants on SIS-partic domain score at baseline H (2) = 0.48, p = 0.79; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.01, p = 0.99; nor at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.43, p = 0.81. 8.8.10 Stroke Impact Scale – Recovery visual analogue scale (SIS-VAS) Numerical median, percentile and interquartile range data for self – perceived recovery from stroke as measured by vertical visual analogue scale (VAS) on the SIS are shown in table 8.19. All groups demonstrated an increase in perceived recovery VAS over time, with similar spread of data for all groups. A boxplot of median, interquartile range and range of the SIS-mob domain scores for the control, PP and WP participants is shown in figure 8.17. 257 Chapter 8 Results Baseline Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range End of Intervention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Long-term retention Median 25th percentile 75th percentile Interquartile range Table 8.19 Control Group Part Group Practice Whole Practice Group 50 40 63.5 23.5 50 41.75 61.25 19.5 50 40 65 25 56.5 41 71.5 30.5 50.5 43.75 70 26.25 51 40 70 30 56 50 71.75 21.75 60 45 69.5 24.5 61 50.5 74 24.5 Median and Interquartile range data for Stroke Impact Scale – VAS 8.8.10.1 Stroke Impact Scale –Recovery Visual Analogue Scale (SIS-VAS) Within group Statistical Analysis A Friedman’s ANOVA was undertaken on the data for each group from the baseline and at the end of intervention and long-term follow-up. No statistically significant differences were demonstrated for any of the groups over time. Control 2 (2) = 3.40, p = 0.18; PP 2 (2) = 3.32, p = 0.19; WP 2 (2) = 5.61, p = 0.06. 258 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.17 Boxplot for Stroke Impact Scale –Recovery VAS 8.8.10.2 Stroke Impact Scale – Recovery Visual Analogue Scale (SIS-VAS) Between group Statistical Analysis A Kruskal Wallis test was applied to the perceived recovery VAS data to determine if there were any differences between groups at each timepoint. No significant difference was found between the control, PP or WP participants at baseline H (2) = 0.01, p = 0.99; at the end of the intervention period H (2) = 0.23, p = 0.89; or at the long-term follow-up H (2) = 0.86, p = 0.65. 259 Chapter 8 Results 8.9 Exercise Repetitions The exercise diary records were analysed to determine how many repetitions of each exercise were undertaken by PP and WP participants. Participants were requested to practice exercises daily, but reassured that if they were unable to practice on a particular day or days, that would be understandable and a request was made that they state the reason. Reassurance was given that they could miss days and continue to participate in the trial, this was undertaken to limit drop out if a participant had missed a number of consecutive days. On scrutinising the data, there were multiple and random days with no exercises for a variety of reasons such as hospital appointments, family visits, leisure activities, and lack of motivation. In order to allow scrutiny of the data, data have been summed over the four weeks of intervention and total number of repetitions and weekly averages are presented in table 8.20. 260 Chapter 8 Results PART PRACTICE Group Exercise TOTAL Weekly repetitions average Sit to Stand (component parts Exercise 436.19 (+ 117.25) 406 (372 – 500) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) Sit to stand 109.05 (+29.31) 101.5 (93 – 125) Sitting Down (component parts) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) 436.19 (+ 117.25) 406 (372 – 500) 434.77 (+ 134.78) 404 (362 – 492) 109.05 (+29.31) 101.5 (93 – 125) 434.77 (+ 134.78) 404 (362 – 492) 472.71(+ 117.22) 460 (406 = 490) Table 8.20. 463.09 (+ 138.69) 464 (400 - 500) 111.67 (+45.06) 106 (84 – 122.75) 411.74 (+ 139.04) 392 (336 – 475.5) 102.93 (+34.76) 98 (84 – 118.88) Step Up-Down BAD leg lead 108.62 (+33.7) 101 (90.5 – 123) 411.74 (+ 139.04) 392 (336 – 475.5) 102.93 (+34.76) 98 (84 – 118.88) 459.5 (+ 194.9) 442 (387 - 538) 114.88 (+48.72) 110.5 (96.75 – 134.5) 472.67 (+ 191.39) 467 (406 - 543) 118.17 (+47.84) 116.75 (101.5 –135.75) Cuppa Time 118.18 (+29.31) 115 (101.5 – 122.5) TIP TAP (component parts) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) 446.68 (+ 180.25) 424 (336 - 491) 108.62 (+33.7) 101 (90.5 – 123) Cuppa Time (component parts) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) 111.67 (+45.06) 106 (84 – 122.75) Step Up-Down GOOD leg lead Step Up-Down BAD leg lead (component parts) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) 446.68 (+ 180.25) 424 (336 - 491) Sitting Down Step Up-Down GOOD leg lead (component parts) Mean (s.d.) Median (IQR) WHOLE PRACTICE Group TOTAL Weekly repetitions average TIP TAP 115.77 (+34.67) 116 (100 – 125) Summary of Exercise repetitions undertaken by participants 261 Chapter 8 Results 8.10 Monitoring Activity During the four weeks of the intervention phase of the study, participants were requested to wear an ActivPAL™ 1activity monitor – for a single day. Collecting data on amount of activity was a subsidiary element of the study and was undertaken to gain an indicative picture of how community dwelling people with latestage stroke spend their time. Only 45 activPAL records were available for analysis. Summary results are presented in table 8.21 and figures 8.18 – 8.20. PERCENTILES 25th 50th Percentage time in sitting or lying Percentage time in standing Percentage time in walking Number of steps Sit to stand transitions Table 8.21. 75th control part whole control part 71.75 71.00 71.00 3.25 7.50 84.00 79.00 74.50 10.50 12.00 94.50 88.00 89.25 14.50 17.00 whole 6.00 18.50 21.00 control part 1.00 4.50 5.50 8.00 12.50 11.50 whole 4.00 6.50 9.50 control part 181.00 1219.50 1296.00 2465.00 3550.00 4398.50 whole 851.50 1335.50 2123.00 control part 18.00 29.50 29.50 42.00 40.75 60.00 whole 27.50 47.50 79.00 Summary descriptive data of activity undertaken during a single day As can be seen from table 8.21 and figures 8.18 to 8.20, the vast majority of time for all participants, irrespective of group allocation, was spent inactive - either lying down or sitting. While selected data are presented here, the raw data available in appendix 8, revealed that, nine of the 45 participants (20%) spent less than two percent of their waking day in walking. Furthermore, just one of the cohort took 1 PALtechnologies http://www.paltech.plus.com/ 262 Chapter 8 Results more than 10,000 steps during the day and only a further four participants took more than 5,000 steps. These findings relating to indicative pattern of activity will be discussed further in chapter 9 (section 9.9). Figure 8.18 Percentage of time spent in a specific position or activity 263 Chapter 8 Results Figure 8.19. Number of sit to stand transitions undertaken during a single day Figure 8.20. Number of steps taken during a single day 264 Chapter 8 Results 8.11 Summary of Findings This pilot study demonstrated that an RCT of physiotherapy based on different practice paradigms for community-dwelling people with late-stage stroke was feasible. Although recruitment was protracted, a reasonably sized cohort (n=64) were enrolled onto the study and data from 60 participants were available for analysis. A sample of this size in a community-based rehabilitation study in stroke is of a reasonable size, however given the relatively insensitive nature of the outcome measures used, combined with the number of participants in each arm of the trial, any interpretation of key findings presented in the Discussion chapter can not be generalised to the stroke population in general and therefore must be viewed with caution. Key findings are summarised below There were no between group differences at any of the time points, therefore none of the null hypotheses (stated in chapter 5) can be rejected. There were however some within group differences over time, demonstrating capacity for improvement. Impairment was measured by the MAS, the Step Test and the FAT. For MAS which can be considered a mix of impairment and activity items, all three groups demonstrated statistically significant improvements. For both Con and PP participants these improvements were found within each group over time, between baseline and end of intervention and at both follow-up points. For WP participants the within group change was only found between baseline and long-term follow-up. There were however, no differences between the groups at any of the time points, 265 Chapter 8 Results indicating that neither PP or WP or no intervention was more beneficial in gaining improvements as measured by MAS. It was surprising to find the sustained improvements on MAS demonstrated by Con participants and is potentially one of the confounding methodological issues of undertaking a study with multiple measurement points using simple physical outcome measures. Impairment level as measured by the Step Test demonstrated a statistically significant improvement only within the PP group over time when stepping up with the unaffected leg. This test required participants to maintain stability in weightbearing and balance through the affected limb, while moving the unaffected limb on and off a block. When undertaking the Step Test moving the affected limb onto a block only the control participants improved over time. Once again this was a surprising finding and may be partly explained by the multiple measurement issues as well as the motivation to improve demonstrated by the people recruited to this study. Arm impairment levels were measured by the FAT. A statistically significant improvement in FAT was only demonstrated within the PP group, and this improvement was found between the baseline and the end of intervention. This improvement was not sustained however and therefore can only be considered as an indication of improvement in upper limb performance as oppose to improvements in learning. Self perception of hand function as measured by SIS-hnd did not show any significant changes, either between or within any of the groups Activity was measured by self-reported outcomes on the FAI and SIS-partic (participation in usual activities). For both PP and WP participants, there was a statistically significant positive improvement within each group, between baseline measures and the long-term follow-up point for SIS-partic. For FAI there was a 266 Chapter 8 Results statistically significant positive improvement within each group, between baseline measures and the end of intervention. It appears that taking part in this study resulted in participants randomised to one of the intervention arms becoming more active over time, even though this was not a primary aim of the study. In terms of mood, this aspect was measured by HADS-A, HADS-D and SIS-mood. Once again while there were no statistically significant changes between the groups at any of the measurement points, there were some significant changes within groups over time. The Control group significantly reduced their HADS-A score between baseline and the end of intervention period, which can be considered as a transient beneficial effect of participating in this study. Although the median HADSA score increased at the long-term follow-up, it had not increased back to baseline level. No other changes in HADS-A or HADS-D scores were found for any of the other groups at any other time points. For SIS-mood, there was only a statistically significant improvement in mood within the PP participants, between baseline and end of intervention. Once again this needs to be considered a transient change, with no sustained benefits. Finally, there was a perceived improvement on SIS-mob, which is a self-rated domain and combines perception of impairment (e.g. balance) with perceptions of ability (to walk fast or walk several blocks). Once again, it was the PP group that demonstrated a transient improvement in SIS-mob scores between baseline and the end of intervention, but this was not sustained at long-term follow-up. 267 Chapter 8 Results In the Discussion chapter, the findings from the current study will be placed into context with existing knowledge. While, on first glance, it could be argued that there appears to be some indications for adopting PP of functional tasks, most of the changes in outcome appear to indicate transient changes in performance - with the exception of the MAS and the Step Test (leading with the unaffected leg). Confounders such as motivation, the effect of multiple testing and the power of the study will all be considered in relation to the findings presented here. 268 Chapter 8 Results 9. DISCUSSION 9.1. Introduction The theoretical issues being addressed in this thesis derive from Motor Learning theory and relate to how to structure exercise practice when undertaking practice of functional tasks. The primary aim of the exploratory randomised controlled trial (RCT) reported in this thesis was to investigate the effects of undertaking a homebased exercise programme of functional tasks based either on part-practice (PP) or whole practice (WP) in a sample of community dwelling people with late stage stroke. The methodology adopted in the RCT used randomisation with stratification, by side and severity of stroke, to allocate participants to a Control, PP or WP group. All participants received three or four visits from the research assistant during the four week intervention phase. Participants in the PP or WP groups undertook a four week programme of standardised exercises during the intervention phase, while the Con participants received “treatment as normal” which was no intervention. Assessment was undertaken using standardised outcome measures of impairment, activity and participation as well as measures of mood and general health status. Following baseline measures, outcomes were measured at the end of four weeks intervention, at a short term retention follow-up (within 72 hours of end of intervention) and at a long-term follow-up (approximately 3 months following the end of intervention). Data obtained in this study need to be treated cautiously for the following reasons. Firstly the vast majority of data were non-normally distributed, secondly a number of the outcome measures utilise summed ordinal level data and there are inherent limitations associated with this process, in that an increase score in one item and a 269 decrease in another can result in the same total score, thirdly, there were a number of missing data points with 16% (n = 10) missing at the short-term follow up point. In addition, a Bonferroni correction was used when making multiple comparisons on the data in the event that a within-group statistically significant difference was found. Using this type of correction is recognised to be conservative and may result in a Type II error, resulting in false negative decisions regarding the null hypotheses. Furthermore, using a post hoc test with Bonferroni correction can result in a situation where the original comparison identifies a statistically significant difference, which is not identifiable on the post-hoc testing and therefore only trends can be reported (Altman 1991). Conversely, if a statistically significant difference was identified on post hoc testing using a conservative Bonferroni correction, then one can be confident that a true difference does exist. This chapter will discuss the results in relation to relevant literature and current theoretical positions. Study limitations and potential sources of error will be considered and their potential influence on the results will be examined. Finally, implications for clinical practice will be explored and recommendations for future research will be presented. A note of caution however, when considering comparisons of the current study to existing work. It is difficult to compare studies in this area, as although the published literature included in this chapter broadly looked at home or community exercise programmes, with people with chronic or sub-acute stroke and some studies were based on Motor Learning theory, there are many discrepancies in the overall aims of the included studies, the trial designs, sample sizes, and length and composition of the interventions. 270 9.2. Sample Characteristics From the 350 letters of initial invitation to participate in the study, 64 people with stroke were recruited into the study which is a recruitment rate of 18.3%. This recruitment rate was much lower than had originally been anticipated, but is in line with other community based rehabilitation research (Lloyd et al 2010). Overall, the sample of 64 participants that were recruited to the study comprised a much larger sample than many other reported studies investigating physiotherapy for late-stage stroke (such as Texeira-Salmela et al 1999; Dean et al 2000; Monger et al 2002; McLellan and Ada 2004, Michaelsen et al 2006; Combs et al 2009). The characteristics of the cohort were representative of the stroke population at large with median age of 72.3 years (range 54.3 – 90.8), median time since stroke of 21 months (range six months to 13.5 years) and there were almost equal numbers of male (31) and female (33) participants. People who had sustained a right cerebrovascular accident (CVA) were recruited at a ratio of 1.5:1 compared to people with a left CVA (38:26). Despite the characteristic features of the sample, the trial design resulted in a maximum of 22 people in any arm of the trial and therefore interpretation of results still require to be made with caution due to the heterogeneous nature of the stroke population. All attempts were made to ensure that as large a sample as possible would be eligible for inclusion. The key safety exclusion criteria was a history of more than two falls within the past six months, plus a requirement to meet the inclusion criteria of functional reach of > 15 cm, this was to ensure that participants would not be at high risk of falling when undertaking exercise. However as was reported in 8.9, four of the participants did fall during the study period and this will be discussed further in 271 9.4. Potential participants also had to score at least 22 on the Mini Mental State Examination, to ensure there should be adequate cognition and memory to follow the exercise programme if allocated to the PP or WP arm of the trial. The exclusion criteria resulted in rejection of people with co-existing neuropathologies, as this could have influenced the ability to undertake the independent exercises that had been designed for this study, and it may also have affected the learning process. Co-existing disability (such as lower limb amputation) or co-existing concurrent pathology (such as fracture of lower limbs) would also lead to exclusion as the participants would not be able to undertake the exercise programme. Participants with normal, age related pathologies that did allow exercise (such as Osteoarthritis of the hip or knee) were included in the sample. No screening was made for pain, fatigue or depression which are factors commonly associated with stroke (for example Johnson et al 1995; Staub and Bogousslavsky 2001; Glader et al 2002; Hackett et al 2005). It may be that potential participants who had been contacted by letter chose not to respond, finding the prospect of an exercise programme unappealing or worried that it may be too exhausting. Four of the original 64 (6%) participants recruited dropped out, or their data were not considered as part of the analysis (see 8.3.1.). The reasons for drop out were not unexpected. Only one participant dropped out on allocation to the Con group. This single incident supported the strategy of clearly identifying to potential participants, on the initial phone contact and at the first baseline visit, the odds of not receiving exercise (see 7.2). Two participants were withdrawn from the study for medical reasons and one further subject withdrew and requested no further contact. While four drop outs is disappointing, a 6% drop out rate can be considered highly 272 acceptable in a community-based RCT with people with a chronic condition (Freemantle et al 1992). The missing data at key time points (See 8.3.2), particularly at short-term follow-up, may have affected the findings. Given the nature of the research environment and the population under investigation, combined with the limited resources of one parttime clinical research assistant and one part-time blinded outcome assessor, to only have few missing outcome records from a possible 240 was highly satisfactory and provided a reasonably comprehensive dataset for analysis. Data will now be considered in relation to the findings presented in the Results chapter. 9.3. Discussion of Participation Global Measures of Impairment, Activity and 9.3.1. Barthel Index Barthel Index (BI) data was scored 0 – 20 (Collin et al 1988). The median scores showed slight improvement in all groups from baseline, with slight fluctuations in median scores from 17 – 19 points, and changes in median scores of one point in both PP and WP. A median BI score of 18 would indicate that the participants are functioning at a relatively high level (McDowell and Newell 1996) and therefore there may not be much capacity for change in score. The BI was included in the battery of outcome measures however, to allow comparisons to be made with other work in this area, with a recognition of the limitations of the index. 273 Considering the statistical testing, there was no statistically significant difference between the groups at any of the measurement points indicating that the intervention or lack of intervention resulted in no difference in performance on the BI. When examining the change over time within each group, both PP and WP showed a statistically significant improvement. This was found to be present when comparing short-term follow-up to baseline measures and was a somewhat surprising finding. However, on considering the data, it may be that the seven missing PP and WP data points at the short-term follow-up strongly influenced this finding. The Barthel Index is recognised as a standardised and validated measure of disability (Wade 1992, Langhammer et al 2007) and has been used in many studies relating to stroke rehabilitation (such as Wade et al 1992; Dam et al 1993; Green et al 2002; Stuart et al 2009; Langhammer et al 2007). It has however been criticised for being insensitive to change (Wade and Collin 1988; Tennant et al 1996; TurnerStokes and Turner-Stokes 1997) and for demonstrating floor and ceiling effects (Wade 1992; Turner-Stokes and Turner Stokes 1997). Despite these limitations, a change of two points has been proposed as demonstrating a “probably genuine change” (Collin and Wade 1988). The PP and WP groups therefore, while demonstrating a within group statistically significant change over time could not be considered as demonstrating genuine improvement and the significant finding is potentially just an anomaly related to the amount of missing data at the short-term follow-up measurement point. Stability of BI scores have been reported at six months post-stroke, with a tendency to reduce slightly at one year (Langhammer et al, 2007) although these findings were found on a cohort who had received four 274 weeks of intervention at three, six, nine and 12 months post-stroke, so not directly comparable to the present study. The findings from this RCT revealed that this cohort had a higher median score on the BI at all measurement time points (between 17 – 19) and therefore were able to undertake ADL at a more independent level, than the cohort studied by Dam et al (1993) or Duncan et al (1998). Dam et al (1993) recruited participants from three months post-stroke and provided intervention with up to seven periods of treatment for up to two years (Dam et al 1993), while the participants in the study by Duncan et al (1998) were within three months of stroke. The findings from this study show a similar pattern of improvement in people with chronic stroke to that reported by Duncan et al (1998), with her cohort of sub-acute stroke (recruited within eight weeks of stroke onset) showing a two and a half point increase from mean scores of 16.5 rising to 192 over 12 weeks of a home based exercise programme. Slightly greater mean BI score improvements were reported by Dam et al (1993), with changes from 12 to 15 and 15.51 at six, 12 and 24 months after stroke (Dam et al 1993). Although the mean scores reported by Dam et al did not improve to the same point as this study, the amount of improvement ( x 3.5) was greater than the two point increase in median score found in the current cohort. As the participants in the current study had a relatively high median BI score, there would be less room for improvement however. Wade et al (1992), Green et al (2002) and Stuart et al (2009) recruited only people with chronic stroke with a mean time from stroke of over four years, more than one 2 Data transformed from 0-100 BI scoring 275 year and over three years respectively. Interestingly, while the intervention groups in all three studies demonstrated improved primary outcomes related to gait, only statistically significant changes were found in relation to BI scores by Stuart et al (2009). Wade et al (1992) reported a minimal, non significant change in BI ( x change < -0.3), and Green et al (2002) reported both intervention and control participants maintained their median BI score at 18 throughout the study period. Stuart et al (2009) reported a mean change in BI score of 3.9 from the baseline of 79.5 which was statistically significant in their intervention group, but no significant change in the BI score for the control group. These findings, may add support to the notion that the BI may not be the most sensitive measure of ADL to identify meaningful change in rehabilitation outcomes. 9.3.2 The Motor Assessment Scale (MAS) Motor Assessment Scale total scores showed no significant difference at baseline between the groups, with only a two point difference between Con (26), PP (27) and WP (28) groups. At the end of the four weeks intervention, all groups had increased their median scores – Con group by three points, PP by five points and WP by seven points (WP). At the short-term retention follow-up, these changes were maintained by the PP and WP groups, but the Con group returned to one point of baseline. The Motor Learning literature would indicate that this maintenance of improvement demonstrated that the change was due to learning rather than a transient change in performance (Schmidt and Lee 2005, Schmidt and Wrisberg 2008, Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007, Magill 2005). This opinion would appear to be backed up by the long-term retention data which indicated that the PP group had improved their median score by a further two points to a total score of 276 34, the WP group had dropped slightly to a total score of 32.5, but this was still nearly five points above the median baseline of 28. A surprising finding at long-term follow-up was that the Con group had also improved their total score to 31. Statistical analysis demonstrated no statistically significant differences between the groups at any of the outcome measurement points. In contrast, the data for within each group over time confirmed that all three groups showed a statistically significant improvement in MAS total score. This within group improvement was found between baseline and end of intervention and between baseline and longterm follow-up for all three groups, there was also a significant improvement in MAS total score between baseline and short-term follow-up for the Con and PP group. The finding that all three groups significantly improved their MAS total score over the time of the RCT was an interesting finding. Within the exercise programme during the intervention period, one exercise (sit to stand) was practised by PP and WP participants and this activity is directly tested in the MAS (item 4). On looking at the raw data for the MAS changes, it appears that improvements were apparent in a number of other items as well, for example there were changes in walking ability (item 4). It is therefore unlikely that it was just change on one item that resulted in the significant improvement. It may be that the functional nature of the tasks practised in the exercise programme resulted in improvements in body structures or functions that were not formally assessed (for example muscle strength or balance) and that these factors contributed to improved performance on items on the MAS. The reason that the Con group improved their performance on MAS was unexpected. When considering why this improvement might have occurred, 277 examination of follow-up notes for at least five of Con group acknowledge that on allocation to the group their initial feeling was of disappointment, but these five participants also admitted that they found the outcome measure procedure interesting and when they were asked to undertake a test that they couldn’t do (for example item 5, level 5: “Walks 10m with no aid, turns round and picks up a small sandbag from the floor and walks back in 25 seconds”) they then practised that item. While the participants would have been naïve to the time and distance parameters of the test, they remembered walking between landmarks in their house and bending down to pick up an item while being timed. This feature, of practising tasks that were difficult or impossible to achieve, indicates the motivation of the cohort. Additionally it highlights one of the weaknesses of having multiple testing sessions at relatively close intervals. In relation to other studies of late stage stroke that have utilised the MAS as part of a battery of outcome measures, the participants in this study formed a larger cohort and were assessed on the validated eight item MAS, in comparison to the smaller studies that used the MAS, often scoring a single selected item on MAS. In a small pre- post-test design undertaken with six people at least one year poststroke, Monger et al (2002) reported statistically significant improvements on the Sitto-stand (STS) item of the MAS, following a three week programme of 30 repetitions of STS and 30 repetitions of step ups daily. As no retention tests were undertaken this could simply be a transient change in performance rather than a permanent change. McLellan and Ada (2004) did include retention tests, but found no significant differences in the walking item of the MAS following a six week mobility 278 home exercise programme. Similarly, no statistically significant differences were found on the MAS UL subscale in a small study of 12 people at least five months post-stroke, while more sensitive kinematic data did show statistically significant improvements in arm trajectories (Thielman et al 2004). All these studies were smaller and had more focused interventions than the current study, and it could be argued that the nature of the task specific training employed related directly to the specific item of the MAS used for measurement, and these factors restrict any inferences that can be drawn. 9.3.3. The Frenchay Activity Index All groups displayed low baseline scores on the Frenchay Activity Index (FAI) with median baselines of 10, 14.5 and 14 for Con, PP and WP participants respectively. No significant differences between the groups at any of the timepoints were found when the FAI data were examined. There were however, statistically significant improvements within both PP and WP groups over time demonstrated. This improvement was restricted to between baseline and end of intervention with an increase in median scores of five points for the WP group and two and a half points for the PP group. These improvements were not maintained at the long-term followup, although median scores did not return to baseline levels. It may have been that active participation in an exercise programme had encouraged participants to undertake more activity in and around the house such as light housework, local shopping or walking outside, indeed some participants had noted these activities in their exercise diaries. It is not known what level of improvement on the FAI indicates a clinically relevant improvement (Wyller et al 1996), so an improvement of 279 five points potentially may represent a small and transient increase in activity (e.g. from “never” to “less than once a week”). Studies that included the FAI as part of their battery of outcome measures reported similarly low FAI scores at baseline with median scores between 10 - 13 and reported no significant differences between the groups in FAI score at baseline or at three, six or nine month follow-up (Green et al 2002). Similarly, Wade et al (1992) reported mean baseline FAI scores of 11.3 – 13.5 with no significant differences found at any follow-up time points. 9.4 Discussion of Mobility Outcomes Discussion of data pertaining to gait speed, the Timed Up and Go over 2metres (TUG2m), ability to step up onto a block, and ability to rise to stand are presented in this section. 9.4.1 The Timed Up and Go over 2metres In administering the TUG2m, a total time during performance of the test was recorded, using a hand held stop watch with lap-timer function to enable scrutiny of component parts of the test – such as rise-to-stand (see 6.6). There was very little variation between the groups on the total time taken to undertake TUG2m, although all groups improved the total time in comparison to the baseline measure (see 8.5). This stability of performance was noted at all measurement points, with median TUG2m total times of around 30 seconds at the end of intervention measurement point. Some participants recorded extreme times for completion of this test (e.g. C31 > 210 seconds) and in part this was due to difficulty in standing up (see 9.4.4) 280 as well as slow gait speed. No significant differences were noted in TUG2m total time either between the groups at any of the measurement points, or within the groups over time. The findings of no significant change in TUG2m total time contrasts to other studies of people with chronic stroke utilising the original Timed Up and Go over three metres (TUG). Marigold et al (2005) found a significant change (p<0.001) in TUG time, maintained at a retention test, in their study of people with chronic stroke undertaking exercise three times a week over ten weeks aimed at improving balance and mobility. Mead et al (2007) reported a significant improvement (p<0.05) in TUG time for the intervention group ( x 10.5s) compared to the control group ( x 11.5s) following a 12 week intervention, a change that was not maintained at follow-up. A reduction in TUG time ( x 16.9s at baseline, x 16.6s end intervention and x 15.8s at five month retention) was also demonstrated by the nine subjects who undertook an intensive two week home exercise programme reported by Combs et al (2009), with a small effect size (0.04) calculated for the change scores. The three studies considered above, all reported much quicker TUG times (over 3m) than the median times found in this study and possibly indicates that this cohort had more severe impairments, certainly this appears to be reflected in terms of gait speed. 9.4.2. Gait Speed Gait speed was calculated from the outward two metres of the TUG2m. Median time for all groups at baseline was 0.21m/s and this improved slightly at the end of 281 intervention (Con 0.26m/s; PP 0.24m/s; PP 0.24m/s). At follow-up a slight deterioration in median gait speed for Con (0.25m/s) and WP (0.2m/s) at both follow-up points was found, whereas PP demonstrated an improvement (0.32m/s) at short-term, not sustained at long-term follow-up (0.23m/s). It is likely that the six missing data points at short-term follow-up in the PP group inflated the median gait speed. No statistically significant differences in gait speed were found between the groups or within the groups over time. The cohort in this study demonstrated a considerably slower gait speed than most of the published studies in this area (for example Duncan et al 1998; Texeira-Salmela et al 1999; Kim et al 2001; Green et al 2002; Monger et al 2002; Olney et al 2006; Mead et al 2007 and Stuart et al 2009). Only the cohort recruited by Wade et al (1992) demonstrated similar gait speed (0.21m/s – 0.25m/s) over the course of the study. The gait speeds found in the current study are particularly low, in comparison with comfortable healthy elderly gait speeds - reported consistently at above 1m/s (Oberg et al 1993; Bohannon 1997; Steffen et al 2002) and with some reports of comfortable hemiplegic gait speed measured during observation as oppose to part of an intervention study (Kollen et al 2006; Olney et al 1994). Gait speed calculated over two metres can be criticised as being too short a distance to enable sufficient physical space for acceleration of body mass to result in the attainment of comfortable walking speed to be attained. Given some of the environmental restrictions encountered during pilot testing however, it was decided to calculate gait speed in this standardised way. 282 It is recognised that both the activities of rising to stand at the start of the TUG2m and the turning component following the two metre walk may also have negatively impacted on performance. 9.4.3 Stepping Up A step test was undertaken with both the unaffected and affected leg leading the step up. This protocol tested both the ability to have sufficient control to maintain balance on one leg while the other leg moved, and also the ability to flex the hip, knee and ankle of the “step up” leg to clear an obstacle. Once again, there were no significant differences, for stepping up with either leg, between groups at any of the timepoints. There were however statistically significant improvements within the PP group at all 3 measurement points compared to baseline when stepping up with the unaffected leg, which lends some support to using a PP strategy for this activity where there is a requirement for maintaining stability, balance and weight-bearing through the affected leg while undertaking a challenging dynamic task with the unaffected leg. Surprisingly the Con group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement between baseline and the end of intervention, when stepping up with the affected leg. Five of the Con participants acknowledged, following the final follow-up measurements, that they had practised this particular exercise as it had caused them difficulty at baseline testing. It may be that only the “safer” option of balancing on the unaffected leg was undertaken by these participants. While the step they utilised to practice had different dimensions to the testing step, it is possible that repeated practice will have strengthened the requisite motor programme for the action of stepping up and contributed to the improved performance. 283 No other studies investigating the impact of a home exercise programme on people with chronic stroke used the Step Test, so direct comparisons can not be made. Texeira-Salmela et al (1999) and Kim et al (2001) however did include a stair climb speed test as part of their outcome measure battery. While Texeira-Salmela et al found a statistically significant improvement in stair climb with an improvement from baseline to post–intervention of 0.85 stairs/s to 1.13 stairs/s, Kim et al (2001) found no statistically significant differences from mean baseline speeds of 0.65 stairs/s (experimental) and 0.61 stairs/s (control). The biomechanical demands of stair climbing however are different to the Step test, which solely requires foot placement on the step and no weight transference (Hill et al 1996) 9.4.4 Rising to stand Rising to Stand (RTS) was measured as the first component of the TUG2m test and was of interest as this activity had been practised by the PP and WP groups. Median time to stand up at baseline was 2.93s (Con), 2.6s (PP) and 3.22 (WP) and while there were minor changes at end of intervention - 2.84s (Con), 2.65s (PP) and 3.6 (WP); and long term follow up – 3.51s (Con), 2.23s (PP) and 3.23 (WP) no statistically significant differences were found between groups at any of the timepoints, or within groups over time. Only Mead et al (2007) reported RTS times and these were considerably faster than those found in the current study, with mean RTS times ranging between 0.94 – 1.09 seconds. While the times in the current study may have been adversely affected by being recorded as a component of the TUG2m test, median times were considerably 284 slower than mean RTS times for people with stroke that have been reported at around 1.6 to 2s (Baer and Ashburn 1995; Tung et al 2010). Conversely, RTS times reported by Mead et al (2007) were considerably faster than those previously reported. Monger et al (2002) reported item 4 (standing up) of the MAS in isolation from the overall validated MAS. Following a three week home exercise programme five out of a small sample of six participants demonstrated improvement. Two participants were reported to improve by two points to get the top score of six, while a three point improvement to a score of five was demonstrated by three participants. This indicates there is room for improvement in RTS ability in people with chronic stroke, which was a finding in the current study. 9.5. Measure of Arm Function 9.5.1 The Frenchay Arm Test The Frenchay Arm Test (FAT) is scored between zero and five. While the WP group appeared to be less able at baseline (median 0.5 compared to median score 2 for Con and PP), there were no statistically significant differences between the groups at any of the time points. Only PP participants demonstrated a statistically significant improvement over time with a change in score from two to three points between baseline and end of intervention, while the change in median score persisted, it was not found to be statistically significant at any other measurement point. Interestingly there was a trend towards improvement in the WP group (gain to two points at end of intervention and follow-ups) that did not reach significance. 285 While the Con group improved by one point from baseline, once again due to practice of difficult items on the test by a diligent sub-group, the median score fluctuated for this group at follow-up. No other studies of people with chronic stroke reported FAT data. Studies of physiotherapy interventions with people with late-stage stroke reporting grip strength (Pang et al 2006; Langhammer et al 2007) and arm impairment as measured by Fugl Meyer arm (FMA-UL) function (Pang et al 2006; Michaelsen et al 2006) do allow some comparisons. While hemiparetic grip strength has been shown to improve with exercise intervention for people with late-stage stroke, no statistically significant changes were found between intervention and control groups (Pang et al 2006; Langhammer et al 2007). Pang et al (2006) however did demonstrate a statistically significant improvement in arm impairment over time as measured by the FMA-UL, irrespective of the severity of stroke impairment. Similar significant improvements on the FMA-UL were demonstrated by Michaelsen et al (2006). These findings support the finding that people with late-stage stroke have the capacity to improve impairments in arm function, as demonstrated by the PP group. 9.6 Discussion of Measurement of Mood 9.6.1 The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Measures of anxiety and depression were presented as sub-scale scores (HADS-A and HADS-D) in section 8.7. All groups demonstrated a reduction in HADS-A median scores between baseline and end of intervention followed by a slight increase at long-term follow up (Con: 6, 4, 5; PP: 6, 5, 8; WP: 8, 4, 5.5). On 286 examining HADS-D data, while PP median scores showed a very slight increase between baseline, end of intervention and long-term follow up (PP: 4, 5, 5.5) this pattern was not shown by the two other groups (Con: 6, 5, 5; WP: 6, 5, 6). Over time it was found that there was a statistically significant reduction in HADS-A only for the control group between the baseline and end of intervention measure. The reduction on HADS-A by two points in the Con group meant that all Con participants either had no signs of anxiety although a small minority who scored between 8 -10, could be considered mild cases of anxiety (Snaith and Zigmond 1994). Examining the data it appeared that while most participants demonstrated no or mild signs of anxiety and depression, a small minority of respondents scored 1115 on HADS-A or HADS-D, demonstrating moderate cases of anxiety or depression (Snaith and Zigmond 1994). It may be that the Con group gained benefit and reassurance from the contact with the clinical research assistant, and consequently reported a slight reduction in HADS-A scores. The HADS-A data are comparable to findings in similar populations by Green et al (2002) who reported median HADS-A scores at baseline of 7, reducing to 5 for the treatment group and remaining stable for the control group and stabilising at 6 points for both groups at two follow-up assessments points. Wade et al (1992) also report similar HADS-A mean data. Mead et al (2007) report slightly lower mean HADS-A data at end of intervention (Intervention: 3.65; Control 3.99) and at long term followup (Intervention 3.95; Control 4.2). The HADS-D data again are comparable to the work by Green et al (2002) who reported median HADS-D scores of 7 at baseline, reducing to 6 at end of intervention and 7 and 8 at follow-up, with similar scores on mean HADS-D reported by Wade et al (1992). Again Mead et al (2007) reported 287 slightly lower mean HADS-D at end of intervention (Intervention: 4.05; Control 3.51) and at long term follow-up (Intervention 4.21; Control 4.03). None of the studies discussed found a significant difference between the groups for HADS-A or HADS-D (Wade et al 1992; Green et al 2002; and Mead et al 2007). 9.7 Discussion of Health Status 9.7.1 The Stroke Impact Scale domains Each of the Stroke Impact Scale domains are scored out of 100 and are derived from the patient perspective regarding the amount of recovery that has occurred within the specific domain. In this study there were statistically significant improvements within the PP participants in the domains of strength (SIS-str), mood (SIS-mood) and mobility (SIS-mob) from baseline to the end of intervention. Additional significant improvements in participation (SIS-partic) were found for PP and WP participants from baseline to long-term follow-up, and between end of intervention and long-term follow-up PP participants only. 9.7.1.1 Stroke Impact Scale – strength domain A statistically significant 6.25 point perceived improvement in SIS-str from median 37.5 to 43.75 was found for the PP group between baseline and end of intervention. These data are slightly greater than the mean SIS-str of 31.48 in people undergoing community stroke rehabilitation (Hartman-Maeir et al (2007), but this was a one off measurement and no change score was reported. An intensive two week community rehabilitation programme, that included strengthening, reported greater 288 improvements of 10 points from a baseline mean of 59 (Combs et al 2010). As the current study did not specifically target strength training, this might partly explain why a lower SIS-str score was found and other domains improved more. However, it was still an interesting finding that PP participants perceived their strength had improved over the intervention phase of this study. 9.7.1.2 Stroke Impact Scale - mood A statistically significant 12.5 point perceived improvement in SIS-mood from median 61.1 to 73.6 was found for the PP group between baseline and end of intervention, and the score only reduced by 3 points at long term follow-up. This pattern was markedly different to the WP group whose score only varied by three points (69 – 66) and the Con group whose score reduced from 69 to 62.5. It is not clear why this effect was found, although it is well established that exercise has a positive effect on mood (Fox 1999; Eng et al 2003). This positive change on SIS- mood score has not been reported in other studies that have used the SIS to assess outcomes in people with chronic stroke undertaking home or community based exercise programmes (Hartman-Maeir et al 2007; Stuart et al 2009; Combs et al 2010). While direct comparisons can not be made with studies that have used alternative measures for assessing mood, no significant improvements have been found in people with chronic stroke undertaking home or community based exercise programmes with the Short Form 36 (SF36) Mental Health domain (Kim et al 2001; Mead et al 2007), or the stroke-adapted Sickness Impact Profile (SA-SIP30) (McLellan and Ada 2004). 289 9.7.1.3 Stroke Impact Scale – mobility A statistically significant 3 point perceived improvement in SIS-mob from median 59.7 to 62.5 was found for the PP group between baseline and end of intervention, and a further increase to 66.7 at the long-term follow-up. This improving pattern was markedly different to the WP group who reduced their SIS-mob score from 55.5 to 50 at end of intervention, returning to baseline 55.5 at long-term follow-up and the Con group who stayed stable with median scores of 54.15 between baseline and end intervention and an increase to 61 at long-term follow-up. The mobility domain covers topics that include community ambulation, stair negotiation and balance and a possible explanation for the improvement in this study is that the PP group may have felt some carryover from the rise-to-stand or step up exercises to broader mobility performance. The findings in this study compare positively with other studies that have reported SIS-mob. Stuart et al (2009) found no statistically significant improvement in SIS-mob for their intervention group, while the community intervention group studied by Hartman-Maier et al (2007) reported lower mean SISmob scores of 53. 9.7.1.4 Stroke Impact Scale – participation An interesting finding was the long-term statistically significant improvement for SISpartic for both PP and WP between baseline and the final measurement point, as well as between end intervention and long-term follow-up for PP. Almost a 30 point increase in median score was found for PP from baseline to long-term follow-up (39.1 to 68.75), with a 12.5 point increase in median score found in the WP group. These findings are similar to the 13 point improvement in SIS-partic (60 to 73) found by Combs et al (2010) in an intensive two week home exercise programme and the 290 mean 11 point improvement found by Stuart et al (2009) in a 13 week community exercise programme. 9.7.1.5 Other Stroke Impact Scale domains In this study, no further statistically significant findings on other domains in the SIS were reported. Other investigators that have used the SIS to measure outcome of a community or home-based exercise programme for people with chronic stroke have reported similar findings to those found in this study (see 9.7.1.1; 9.7.1.3 and 9.1.7.4.). However, additional significant findings have also been found by other investigators, which did not emerge in this study. In a 13 week programme of community group and home exercises, a significant improvement of 7 points on the SIS-comm domain was found (Stuart et al 2009). It is not clear whether the social component of the community group positively influenced the communication domain. As anticipated, no increase in score on the SIS-comm was found in the current study. Combs et al (2010) reported a mean 11 point improvement in perceived recovery from stroke, as measured on the visual analogue scale, following their two week intensive exercise programme. While the participants in this study reported similar median improvements (10 points PP; 11 points WP), this was not found to be statistically significant. 9.8. The practice regime While the Clinical Research Assistant worked with the participants on the contact visits, to individualise the exercises to some extent (for example to ensure weight bearing on a flat foot), there was no way to check exactly how the exercises were being practised over the four weeks and potentially multiple movement and 291 compensation strategies could have been adopted. It was found that the WP participants understood the exercises to be practised, however there were some queries regarding the comprehension of some PP participants about why there were practising just lifting their bottom off a chair and not the whole act of standing up. Despite ensuring that each exercise had a clear descriptor and a verbal and visual explanation, it may be that the exercises were not practiced in the intended manner. Using video and leaving this with the participant (much like a fitness DVD) could be considered in future studies. An alternative would be having a check list of key features of each exercise and cross-checking these at the end of the intervention phase, however this strategy was not compatible with the resources available for this study. A further consideration, is that it is not known whether there was any transfer of ability from one exercise to another or whether there was interference between the exercises. With the blocked nature of the practice schedule, it is unlikely that interference occurred, however if this did occur, the evidence relating to interference would indicate that over the longer term this would enhance learning (Schmidt and Lee 2005; Schmidt and Wrisberg 2007; Wulf and Lee 1993). Measuring outcome by the use of generalised measures of physical ability and well-being as followed in this study, is considerably different to the majority of Motor Learning studies that have tested discrete task practice by measuring performance on the discrete task itself. The amount of practice that was undertaken by the participants is reported in 8.9. In summary, participants were required to undertake six main exercises and record the number of repetitions in an exercise diary. Weekly averages were calculated from 292 these data. While there were some slight differences in the median number of repetitions undertaken by each group for each exercise, there was no statistically significant difference between the number of repetitions undertaken by the PP and WP groups. For each of the six exercises, participants undertook a median number of repetitions per week of around 100, this resulted in approximately 600 exercise repetitions each week and approximately 2,400 exercise repetitions per PP or WP participant (400 repetitions per exercise) over the four weeks of intervention. Many of the studies of community or home-based exercises for people with latestage stroke have either reported the amount of time spent practising specific exercises, or reported protocols for incremental increases or have not reported the number of repetitions of exercises (Dam et al 1993; Duncan et al 1998; TexeiraSalmela et al 1999; Marigold et al 2005; Pang et al 2006; Olney et al 2006; McLellan and Ada 2004), this makes direct comparisons difficult. Partial details were included in some studies, for example Mead et al (2007) reported increasing the number of repetitions of “pole lifting” from 4 to 15 and the number of sit-to-stand (RTS) repetitions from 4 to 10, however details are not available for other exercises and some elements of their programme were timed. Similar partial information related to strengthening exercises (10 repetitions x 3 sets for five exercises) but not for endurance or balance exercises were reported by Langhammer et al (2007). Two community based studies provided clear details of the repetitions undertaken in their investigations of task specific training. In a five week RCT of upper limb reaching, participants undertook 15 one hour sessions of a therapist-supervised home exercise programme (Michaelsen et al 2006). In each session, 10 minute blocks of exercises (with 2 minutes rest between blocks) were practised, each block was a different exercise with a mean number of repetitions of 51.2 (range 20 – 130). 293 Assuming five blocks were practised each session and three session per week, this means 153 repetitions of five exercises were practised each week with a total of 765 repetitions per week and 3825 repetitions in total (Michaelsen et al 2006). In a three week study of RTS training, the mean number of RTS repetitions was 450 (150 per week), mean number of step-up repetitions 371.8 (124 per week), and mean number of lateral steps 334 (111 per week) (Monger et al 2002). The literature to support practice in Motor Learning would advocate many repetitions and intensive practice of an activity in order for learning to occur (Bach y Rita and Baillet 1987; Carr and Shepherd 1998; Winstein et al 1999). As was identified in section 4.7 however, the number of repetitions has often been low (under 100 in total) in studies investigating the effectiveness of different practice regimes. In the current study, PP and WP participants undertook approximately 400 repetitions of each of six exercises over the four week intervention period. This is similar to the intensity of practice reported by Monger et al (2002) but a lower intensity of practice in comparison to the study reported by Michaelsen et al (2006). All three studies however, have found some elements of statistically significant improvements in physical task performance following the practice protocols. Having considered the findings and undertaken comparisons to published literature, it is not yet possible to state optimal levels of practice intensity for people with chronic stroke. In future work it would be appropriate to prescribe different intensities of practice with identical exercises conducted over varying time periods to more clearly determine how many repetitions would be required for learning to occur. 294 9.9. Sample Measurement of Activity A secondary aim of the study was to explore activity undertaken by community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. Data were available from 45 participants who wore an activPAL™ activity monitor for one waking day. Data were collected for an average time of 11 hours and 48 minutes. On scrutinising the data, it was possible to identify a burst of RTS activity in the activPAL traces from some participants in the PP or WP groups that corresponded with the approximate time of undertaking exercises. Fewer than 20 of the participants had noted their exercise time in the exercise diary however, so it was not possible to confirm the number of RTS transitions for all participants. The median number of RTS transitions differed between the groups. The PP and WP groups undertook more RTS transitions (PP median 42 repetitions, WP median 47repetitions) compared to the Con group (29.5). These data are higher than RTS transitions reported by Jansenn et al (2010) who report mean 16.6 RTS transitions over eight hours at 48 weeks post-stroke, however the activity monitoring system used to collect these data was not described and it is not clear if the system provided valid and reliable data. Britten et al (2008) used an activPAL™ to monitor mean 65.9 RTS transitions in a group of nine sub-acute stroke in-patients undergoing intensive RTS training, compared to mean 18.9 RTS transitions in a control group. The study by Britten took place in a hospital environment, and it is therefore difficult to compare activity recorded in the home and in hospital as the demands on the individual to move will be different, with probably less demands in the hospital setting. 295 The median amount of steps taken during the day was very low for the Con participants - 1,219 steps and PP participants - 2,465 steps, while median step counts for WP participants was 4,389 steps. This is reflected in the differential amounts of time spent in walking and standing, with Con group registering 16% of the day in these positions, PP 20% and WP25%. This means that the rest of the day was spent sitting or lying down. Anecdotal reports of low activity in people post-stroke have been made, however there are very few empirical studies. In a sample of 79 community dwelling people at least six months post-stroke, mean 1,389 steps per day were reported and this was noted to be less than the 3 – 5,000 steps that would be expected in age-matched (mean age 65) sedentary adults (Michael and Macko 2007). The activPAL data, may represent usual activity or, it may possibly provide an overestimation, due to the fact that wearing a monitoring device may have positively impacted on activity levels. There were some technical issues using activPAL, with failure to record, or interrupted recording sessions resulting in the data being discarded. 9.10. Possible explanations for changes in outcomes The results from this study of a home-exercise programme based on either PP or WP principles for people with late-stage stroke have been discussed in the preceding sections. No statistically significant differences were found between the groups at baseline, at the end of intervention or on short-term or long-term followups. Therefore, the intervention received did not appear to have a major influence on outcome at any of measurement points. For some outcomes however, there were significant changes within the groups over time, for the most part these were improvements in relation to baseline. Where these changes occurred, it would 296 indicate that there was change in performance, or in instances when the changes persisted, learning had occurred. This section will consider why the changes might have occurred. One possible reason for the changes is that a process of natural recovery was taking place, the literature relating to motor recovery post-stroke would dispute this (Wade et al 1985; Wade and Langton Hewer 1987). Attempts were made to ensure stability of participant performance, by including two baseline measures of physical outcome measures. Furthermore, including a control group within the study design allowed comparisons to be made with a group receiving no intervention. On the three outcome measures of physical status, where the Control group did show improvements (MAS, Step Test and FAT), this can be partly explained by a subgroup that acknowledged they had practiced tasks that they found difficult in outcome testing. The reduction in Con group HADS-A may possibly be explained by inherent reassurance associated with being involved in a research study and contact with a research physiotherapist. Another explanation pertains to the content of the exercise programmes, the intensity of practice and neuroplastic changes. The content of the exercise programmes aimed to target the functional tasks of RTS, sitting down, stepping onto and off a block, manipulating a cup and pro- and supination of the forearm. The number of repetitions of each exercise being around median values of 400 - 450 repetitions was, according to Motor Learning literature, insufficient for learning to have occurred (Bach y Rita and Baillet 1987; Carr and Shepherd 1998; Winstein et al 1999). A definitive method to demonstrate alterations in brain activity (for 297 example using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging) was beyond the scope of this study, therefore it is only possible to provide speculative explanations. The practice regimes followed in this study, while directive in the exercise to be performed, allowed errors to occur (for example, if the foot did not cover the X on the step-up block), this then enabled the participant to problem solve how to ameliorate the end movement. This may have activated a number of areas in the brain including the pre-motor cortex and supplementary motor area, which have been shown to be involved in retrieval of movements for skilled performance (van Mier et al 1998; Rothwell 2004). By undertaking practice that required repetition and refinement, it is possible that a number of neuroplastic changes occurred such as unmasking of latent synapse or cortical reorganisation. These changes have been suggested both in animal studies and in humans (Ziemann et al 2001; Kleim et al 2004; Bear et al 2007; Rosencratz et al 2007). Within the current study, on some measures, long-term changes were demonstrated. This would indicate that for the activities undertaken in these measures, transient performance enhancements had been superceded by relatively permanent changes in behaviour, or motor learning. However the explanations as to why the changes observed in the current study occurred must remain tentative. It could be that the amount of practice was sufficient to strengthen the motor programme. It would be recommended that future studies investigating parameters of practice in people with chronic stroke need to incorporate neuroimaging techniques to further investigate this area. 9.11. Limitations and Sources of Error A number of limitations and sources of error may have restricted the ability to generalise findings and these are discussed within this section. 298 9.11.1. Study Design This study was an exploratory, single-blind, randomised controlled trial to investigate whether part practice or whole practice of functional tasks would be beneficial in improving functional outcome for community-dwelling people with late-stage stroke. While every attempt was made to design as rigorous a methodology as possible to reduce the number of confounding variables, a number of factors could not be controlled and therefore will have impacted on the methodological quality. A Randomised Controlled Trial of a complex intervention has been cited as being the “gold standard” in research, however an RCT can be problematic to design and execute (Altman 1991; Wade 1999). Undertaking an RCT in the community setting can introduce a further multitude of variables that are difficult to control for (e.g. individual interactions with unique environments). Nevertheless, in an attempt to reduce bias and variability and deliver a standardised intervention which might then start to provide evidence supporting either PP or WP, the strengths of a randomised design were felt to be compelling. The randomisation strategy encompassed stratification, which has been recommended for studies with small samples (Altman 1991; Field 2009). The randomisation list resulted in four strata to ensure that the three groups in the trial were similar for side of stroke and severity of stroke, which are characteristics known to affect outcomes (Kwakkel et al, 1996). It could be that different factors may have been more influential in terms of outcome, for example stroke classification, however it was not possible to gain complete data for all participants and therefore side and severity were deemed to be the most influential factors. An alternative randomisation strategy that had been considered was 299 randomisation with minimisation (Altman 1991). This was discounted however, as a robust system would have been required to update group allocation, taking into account factors of interest, each time a new participant was recruited to the trial. The resources available did not permit this to happen. The number of measurement points used in the study can be both criticised and commended. Two baselines were taken to examine stability of performance on the outcome measures, with baseline testing on two occasions approximately two weeks apart. Undertaking repeated measures so close together may have positively influenced the Con group to remember and practice the tasks that they had identified as being difficult. The strategy of taking repeated assessments however is a robust method of establishing stability (Altman, 1991). Ideally, the two baseline measures should have been undertaken with a four week interval to establish stability over the same time-period as the length of intervention. Given the median time since stroke of 21 months, it is likely that the participants should have been stable (Wade and Langton Hewer 1987). The three measurement points undertaken following the four week intervention were included to evaluate the effects of the four week programme (end of intervention), to determine if short-term learning or change in status had occurred as measured by performance or response retained in the short-term (short-term follow-up – within 72 hours), and to determine if there was indication of persistent learning or change in status over the longer-term (long-term follow-up at three months). While the end of intervention and long-term follow-up outcome visits were relatively easy to organise, there were not only limited resources within which to undertake repeated tests, but the participants or outcome assessor were often not able to co-ordinate a mutually convenient time. This resulted in 12 missing outcome measurement points at short-term follow-up, and 300 therefore any significant changes found at this time-point should be viewed with caution. In any future work, care needs to be taken to ensure that the design is feasible in terms of resources to support outcome measurement and the value of five repeated measures within a sixteen week period should be reconsidered. 9.11.2 Recruitment Strategy The original recruitment strategy was revised on three occasions in order to gain a reasonable sample size. The original intention was based on power calculations from walking recovery data and identified a target sample of 99. Ultimately 64 subjects were recruited and 60 records were available for analysis. This is a reasonable sample size for a trial of community-based stroke rehabilitation. Given the nature of the study, the insensitivity of the majority of the outcome measures and the sample size, it is highly likely that the study was underpowered. There were a number of difficulties associated with recruitment to the study and these have been discussed in section 6.4. The original geographic limitations for Recruitment Strategy v1 were set with cognisance taken of the limited personnel resources to support the study. Limiting the geographical area should have facilitated multiple visits within a short timescale. Widening the geographical area to cover all of Edinburgh assisted with recruitment, but resulted in problems with scheduling, longer inter-participant journey times and therefore reduced the efficiency of the research team and the number of visits that could be made daily. Initial difficulties with identifying people with stroke from GP records systems resulted in extremely slow initial recruitment, by widening the referral pool to include physiotherapists will have resulted in a recruitment bias, in that all the referrals received from a physiotherapist would have undergone relatively recent 301 rehabilitation and possibly were deemed “suitable” for further rehabilitation. The final amendment to the recruitment strategy involved placing an advertisement in the local Edinburgh free paper and this may have resulted in self-selected, more motivated respondents once again adding bias to the sample. 9.11.3. The Sample The sample size of 64 was reasonable and could be argued to be representative of community-dwelling people with late-stage stroke. It is likely however, that the people that responded to letters of invitation were motivated to exercise and therefore possibly more compliant with the programme. Certainly, participants that were recruited through the newspaper advertising were self-selecting and less likely to have depression, fatigue or low levels of motivation. The inclusion and exclusion criteria were designed to ensure as representative a sample from the stroke population as possible. Only one participant with dysphasia was recruited however. Given that dysphasia is estimated to affect around 20 -35% of the stroke population, with around 10% of people with stroke having long-term problems with dysphasia (Engelter et al 2006; Law et al 2009, RCLST 2009), this sub-population were under-represented. On reflection, that one participant was able to be included as their carer was supportive, acted as interpreter when required and was highly organised in completing diary details. In future studies, liaison with a speech and language therapist to ensure relevant presentation of materials and advice regarding more specific inclusion and exclusion criteria would be judicious. 302 9.11.4. The Intervention At the time of planning this exploratory RCT, recent reviews had recommended that interventions in stroke rehabilitation required to be explicitly defined, in order to more clearly plan and conduct evaluation (Pollock et al 2007; Pomeroy and Tallis 2002). Six exercises that were considered to represent fundamental functional tasks and could be structured for practice as a whole movement or as component part movements were developed and piloted for feasibility and acceptability. Clear operational definitions were developed, with written and pictorial instructions for carrying out the exercises included in an individual exercise diary provided for the patient. The clinical research assistant (CRA) explained to the participant and carer, if available, how to practice the exercises. If focus was required on a specific area e.g. equal weight bearing during sitting down, this was noted in the diary as a written reminder. All participants were required to undertake the same exercises and this can be criticised as a “one size fits all approach”, however this was adopted to standardise the intervention received. On reflection, it may have been more relevant to the participants to have developed a catalogue of approximately 15 – 20 exercises targeting different impairment and activity limitations. Each exercise would have required instructions for whole- or part-practice and every exercise would have required piloting in the initial hospital gym and the home environment. Participants would then have been allocated the six most appropriate exercises from an initial assessment and discussion. Not only was there insufficient resources to develop such a catalogue, at the end of the RCT a situation could have arisen whereby all PP group had practiced exercises on the first half of the catalogue and the WP group practiced the second half, which would have been a major confounding variable. 303 In the early pilot work, participants had been asked about tasks they found difficult and so the selection of tasks for the RCT was a combination of consumer involvement and pragmatic decisions about the feasibility of structuring the task into logical parts. The four tasks targeting lower limb and balance were deemed pertinent by all the participants, however the inclusion of the upper limb tasks raised two issues relating to severity of arm impairment. Two participants had very good arm recovery and therefore the upper limb exercises were too easy or perceived irrelevant. This issue had not been fully considered in piloting and it might have been prudent to develop at least two more complex and dextrous tasks (akin to some of the light box or switch tasks reviewed in 4.3 e.g. Pohl et al, 2006). The opposite scenario also occurred whereby participants had no upper limb recovery. In these cases, participants were requested to place the affected arm into position on the table, place the cup or the bottle into the hand and try to undertake mental practice of ten repetitions of the exercise. Written instructions for PP or WP practice, focused on giving an external focus for the exercise (e.g. think about movement of the cup or bottle). This was different to the physical practice which was the focus of the study and introduced another confounding variable. In future work in this area, it would be judicious to ensure a specific minimal amount of upper limb movement as part of inclusion criteria. The exercises undertaken in this RCT were clearly defined as part- or whole practice, however other parameters will have impacted on the practice regime. During the development of the exercises, consideration was given to the evidence supporting all types of practice structure from the Motor Learning literature. From the available evidence, the optimal structure of the practice sessions would have been to draw up a practice list that organised the six exercises into a random pattern 304 with variations given for each exercise – for example altering the chair height or cup to be used. This would have resulted in a random, variable PP or WP regime, and to conform to Motor Learning principles, this regime should have been distributed in nature, so that it was practised at various times throughout the day. A pragmatic approach however was required, especially as such a list would have been quite detailed and, potentially, confusing for the participants. Furthermore, the organisation of equipment to undertake exercise sessions could be challenging for the participants and two pilot participants who had little storage space organised their day around a set exercise time so they could then store the equipment until the next day. The exercise practice regime undertaken in this trial was therefore likely to have been massed, blocked, constant, PP or WP. The four weeks of the intervention was relatively short compared to other, nonintensive, community-based exercise programmes for people with chronic stroke where interventions were often 12 weeks or more (e.g. Dam et al 1993; TexeiraSalmeira et al 1999; Olney et al 2006; Mead et al 2007). It may be that four weeks is insufficient time to show all the potential effects of the intervention, however given the resource limitations it was decided to take a pragmatic approach to this exploratory trial and an intervention that could be delivered and evaluated was used. Home-based exercise programmes that do not require regular therapist supervision are an attractive management strategy in an era of health service restrictions. This strategy may be pertinent for clinical practice, if effective interventions can be developed. As has previously been identified, the amount of practice required for an effective intervention is not yet established. 305 The accuracy of reported exercise repetitions could not be verified and the data reported in section 8.10 may be questioned. In order for neuroplastic changes to occur over the long-term it would be reasonable to argue that thousands of repetitions of an activity need to have taken place. Data from the activity diaries however, indicate that no more than hundreds of repetitions occurred and for some outcomes, notably the MAS and the Step Test there was evidence of learning being retained at both the short- and longterm follow-up. One question that could arise from this finding is whether people with late-stage stroke need to practice activities less for learning to occur. Alternatively, might some of the inherent motor programmes prior to the stroke may take less time to re-establish. 9.11.5. Outcome Measurement The blinded outcome assessor became aware of the group allocation for three participants during outcome measurement sessions. Participants were reminded not to mention whether they had been undertaking exercise or not at the start of each outcome measure visit. One PP participant was revealed to the outcome assessor as being in one of the intervention arms of the trial, by a colleague remarking that she had met the carer and she had mentioned in relation to her husband that “he was delighted with his exercise progress”. Another PP participant left the exercise equipment clearly in view when the outcome assessor visited. The other participant revealed she was in one of the exercise arms of the trial when she stated that she found imagining the exercises “a waste of time”! While strategies had been instigated to prevent this unblinding, improvements could have been made by ensuring the clinical research assistant had removed exercise equipment prior to the outcome assessors visit, or by taking outcome measures at a different site, however these strategies would not have been practical in the context of this trial. 306 A trial with multiple outcome measure sessions was designed in order to ensure stability of baseline performance, to answer the question whether undertaking a home-based exercise programme of functional tasks based either on part-practice (PP) or whole practice (WP) in a sample of community dwelling people with latestage stroke would be beneficial, outcomes were measured at the end of the intervention phase. To determine whether any changes could be attributed to learning rather than performance both a short-term retention and a long-term followup test was undertaken. Potentially however, this resulted in a situation of assessment overload and participants may have learnt to improve performance on the items in the outcome measures, which did appear to be the case for some Control participants. The number of assessments could have been reduced by undertaking a single baseline measure given the chronicity of the sample. The longterm follow-up might have been more usefully placed at six or 12 months from the end of intervention, however this would not have been feasible given the timescale of the study. Including a short-term retention outcome measure point allowed evaluation of short-term learning effects of the intervention and conforms to recommended procedures within Motor Learning research (Schmidt and Lee 2005; Magill 2008; Shumway Cook and Woollacott 2007). Despite the missing data points, this still allowed exploration of whether learning had taken place and it would be appropriate to include a test of short-term retention in subsequent studies. Perhaps one of the major limitations of this exploratory RCT could be regarding the battery of outcome measures that were used. The final battery were chosen following pilot work and were representative of the domains of impairment, activity 307 limitation and participation as outlined in the International Classification of Functioning (WHO 2010) All of the selected outcome measures had been validated for the stroke population (for example Holbrook and Skilbeck 1983; Carr et al 1985; Parker et al 1986; Heller et al 1987; Poole and Whitney 1988; Hill et al 1996; Wyller et al 1996; Aben et al 2002; Duncan et al 2002; Lai et al 2002; Duncan et al Ng and Hui-Chan 2005; Bennett et al 2006) and had been employed in a standardised manner. However, while the interventions were targeted at specific functional tasks, the outcome measures could be criticised as being too insensitive to demonstrate change – for example, the FAT is a dichotomous scale, so to gain a score of 1 on item 1 the participant must “stabilise a ruler while drawing a line with a pencil held in the other hand. To pass the ruler must be held firmly”. Many components of the task might improve over time, but unless all components are passed then a score of 0 is given. Many of the ordinal scales that were used, such as the BI, have also been criticised as potentially being too insensitive to demonstrate change (Tennant et al 1996; Turner-Stokes and Turner-Stokes 1997). Statistically significant changes were demonstrated however, both on the BI and the MAS. Given the amount of improvement in performance required to increase the score on the MAS, it could be argued that meaningful change to the participant had taken place. The time taken to administer the outcome measures was an important consideration in drawing up the final battery. The preliminary visit could last between two to two and a half hours, this was considered to be a maximum time that would be acceptable to the participants. Subsequent outcome measure visits generally took between 45 to 90 minutes (or more on a few occasions). The time per visit depended on the number of tests to be administered, the ability of the participant, 308 limiting factors such as fatigue or pain and the testing environment. It is the nature of exploratory trials to explore the use of potential outcome measure in order to refine the outcome measure battery in definitive trials. This study therefore sought to determine which outcome measures might be most suitable in a definitive trial. At this point however, further pilot work would be required before a definitive recommendation could be made. 9.12. Clinical Implications Consideration of how to structure practice, has been recommended in various texts on stroke rehabilitation (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Carr and Shepherd 2003), however these recommendation do not derive from evidence from people with stroke. It is not inappropriate that recommendations should be made, however the application of Motor Learning theory for people with stroke needs to be made with caution and limitations of recommendations acknowledged. The current study lends further support to the potential for post-stroke recovery a number of years after the initial event (Dam et al 1993; Bach y Rita 2001; Page et al 2004). Following on from this, it can be recommended that services should be provided to people with late-stage stroke to encourage self-practice of functional tasks or specific exercises in their own home setting. This service would not need to be therapist intensive, in fact a rehabilitation assistant under supervision of an appropriately experienced neurological physiotherapist could be the main contact. This suggestion may be counterintuitive to many clinical physiotherapists. Recent guidelines have recognised the need for ongoing rehabilitation in the community with a recommendation that people with stroke should have access to specialist 309 rehabilitation services (SIGN 2010). Encouraging people with stroke to be more responsible for their rehabilitation may, in part, overcome the tension between the guidance to provide a service and the resources required to provide traditional “hands-on” exercises. On developing the study, there was some uncertainty regarding the willingness or ability of people with late-stage stroke to undertake sufficient exercise repetitions in their home environment. The findings from this study indicated that participants did engage with the process, practicing around 100 exercise repetitions a week for each exercise, although maximum repetitions was 250. Given that thousands of repetitions may be required in order for learning to occur (Carr and Shepherd 2003), this schedule may have been inadequate to drive learning. However, therapists should not be cautious about encouraging more repetitions of exercises. Using computer games to set targets and motivate the individual may be one method of encouraging more practice. The findings from this study point towards encouraging PP of functional tasks for people with late stage stroke. However this statement is made cautiously as only six tasks were practiced and this finding may not be replicable with practice of different functional tasks. 310 9.13. Future Research The results from this study demonstrate that it is feasible for people with chronic stroke to undertake structured self-practice of functional tasks within the home environment. While caution must be taken when making any recommendations for clinical practice, the results from this study do allow recommendations for future research to be made. It was identified in chapter four that the majority of writing relating to Motor Learning, skill acquisition and structuring practice relates to young healthy adults, with a focus on learning or refining a sporting skill or practising a rapid, dextrous task. Recommendations relating to how to structure exercise practice sessions for people with stroke have been made, based on these normative data, by a number of eminent authors (Carr and Shepherd 1998; Carr and Shepherd 2003; ShumwayCook and Woollacott 2007). While a number of arguments have been proffered that therapists working in stroke rehabilitation require to understand what components of therapy are actually effective (Ballinger et al, 1999; Pomeroy and Tallis 2002), there is also a related need for therapists to understand the optimal parameters for structuring practice of those effective components. With this in mind, the following two recommendations are made: Recommendation a. Undertake further research into the efficacy of different practice structures for people at all stages of recovery following stroke. Recommendation b. Conduct a study to investigate the effect of varying intensity of repetitions of task practice on task learning for people with stroke. A similar but related study could 311 investigate the effects of undertaking the practice intervention over varying lengths of time. Exploring recommendations a. and b. would allow identification of the optimal intensity for practice schedules, which may vary during different stages of the stroke rehabilitation process. It would also allow investigation of whether self-practice or therapist-assisted practice required to be structured differently. Furthermore, it would allow therapists to determine whether recommendations of practice schedules derived from healthy young adults are applicable to people with stroke. Currently, as identified in section 4.4.1, the majority of work to have investigated structure of exercise practice for people with stroke is in the area of massed practice undertaken in Constraint Induced Therapy (Taub et al 2000; Vearrier et al 2004; Marklund and Klässbo 2006; Massie et al 2009). Limited investigations of random and blocked practice in people with stroke (Hanlon, 1996; Pohl et al, 2006) have also been undertaken. While more work related to the structure of exercise practice in a less intense massed manner and in random practice of functional tasks is still required, there is an urgent need to explore further whether people with stroke should practice complex functional tasks as a whole or in their entirety. In extending the work reported in this thesis the following further research should be undertaken: Recommendation c. Conduct a study of PP and WP of functional tasks for people with stroke, using a catalogue of set exercise instructions for each task. One could potentially look to match pairs with similar levels and patterns of impairment. The difficulties encountered in developing practice strategies in the exploratory phase of the work reported in this thesis could be addressed by developing set 312 exercises. Developing a set catalogue with a greater and more diverse sample of people with stroke may allow for more successful generalisation of the exercises. There was some uncertainty regarding the ability of participants to practice exercises correctly, therefore a further recommendation (d) is presented: Recommendation d. Examine alternative means of providing instruction such as video or audio instruction in a study of home-based functional exercises. Providing video instruction, that is available when a therapist is absent, would allow refinement of appropriate directions. Using video of the participant undertaking the exercise, may assist with comprehension of the exercises and may improve compliance. Recommendations for further research a. – d. are essential to allow physiotherapists to advance evidence-based practice in stroke rehabilitation. In light of diminishing resources in the NHS, knowing optimal methods for practising functional tasks combined with knowing the optimal intensity for self-practice will allow skilled therapists to use their time most efficiently. In the current study, a snapshot of activity was gathered during one waking day. Activity is linked to a healthier lifestyle, may reduce some of the risks associated with recurrent stroke and should therefore be encouraged (Greenlund et al 2002; Lee et al, 2003; Chiuve et al, 2008). In order to explore and understand the activity levels for community-dwelling people with stroke the following recommendation (e) is made. Given the limited mobility of many people with stroke, these data would 313 allow relevant stroke-specific recommendations for activity and it would allow for triangulation of self-reported data with reliable instrumentation Recommendation e. Undertake a larger study comparing self-report and activPAL monitoring of daily activity for community dwelling people with late-stage stroke. Having made recommendations a – e, it is also to gain the “consumer” view on selfpractice and any facilitators or barriers to exercise and activity. Traditionally physiotherapists working in stroke rehabilitation as well as the person with stroke, have considered therapy to be “hands on”. This traditional approach is not necessarily viable for people with chronic stroke, however information requires to be sought in order to determine whether self practice is an acceptable alternative approach to rehabilitation. Understanding what self-practice means from the perspective of the person with stroke will aid therapists when introducing this concept to people with stroke and agreeing and structuring goals to work towards. Furthermore, appropriate written and graphical aids to facilitate practice can best be developed with input from the target population. Recommendation f. Use mixed methods quantitative and qualitative approach to gain a fuller understanding of how acceptable self-practice is to people with stroke and to determine how well they understood the exercise instructions. In chapter 7, numerous problems with recruitment were identified. Although the RCT reported in this thesis can be commended as being of a reasonably sized study into an aspect of stroke rehabilitation, it was under-powered. Additionally, the selected functional outcome measures can be criticised as being too insensitive to 314 identify some changes. The lack of sensitivity of the ordinal scales used, coupled with recruitment difficulties and lack of power lead to the next recommendation. Recommendation g. Refine the battery of outcome measures utilised and include more responsive kinematic or kinetic measures in addition to the rather insensitive measures included in this study. A more expensive recommendation, and one that should only be undertaken if further work does identify optimal ways to structure practice is identified as recommendation h. If specific practice schedules are shown to be beneficial in terms of functional gains, it is important to determine whether the benefits are due to compensatory strategies or whether there is a central alteration in neurological function. Recommendation h. Include neuroimaging as a key outcome to examine both short- and long-term neurological change following the practice intervention. A final recommendation, and of particular relevance in the current economic climate where resources are limited is provided in recommendation i. While questions relating to how to structure practice and the acceptability or promotion of selfpractice may be of interest to some physiotherapists and some people with stroke, it is only a very small part of the overall experience of stroke rehabilitation. Research needs to link in to and inform practice development and this can only be achieved by involving all relevant stakeholders in drawing up programmes of research (Tallon et al 2000; Boote et al 2002). 315 Recommendation i. Undertake qualitative work to investigate the views of policy makers, physiotherapists and service users regarding the requirements for rehabilitation for people with late-stage stroke. 9.14. Conclusions The primary aim of this study was to investigate a home exercise programme, with practice based on PP or WP of functional tasks, for people with late-stage stroke. Sixty four people with late-stage stroke were recruited to the study, drop out was low (n=4), and data were available for analysis of 60 participants. The sample was representative of the general stroke population. Undertaking a randomised controlled trial in the community setting in Edinburgh was challenging, but feasible. Identifying people with stroke from GP record systems proved problematic and the recruitment strategy required four modifications. Recruitment took 12 months longer than anticipated and the cohort recruited was two thirds of the required sample size. The results showed that in terms of a standardised global measure of activity limitation (the Barthel Index), there was no difference between groups at any of the measurement points. Looking longitudinally within each group, both PP and WP demonstrated a one point median improvement between baseline and short term follow-up. This improvement was shown to statistically significant. When considering self reported activity as measured by the Frenchay Activity Index, there was no difference between groups at any of the measurement points. Looking longitudinally within each group, both PP and WP demonstrated increased activity 316 from baseline to end of intervention. This improvement of 2.5 point by PP and 5 points by WP was shown to statistically significant. Results for the Motor Assessment Scale demonstrated no difference between groups at any of the measurement points. Looking longitudinally within each group, an unexpected finding was that all three groups demonstrated improvements over time. Both the Control and PP groups demonstrated statistically significant improvements from baseline to all subsequent measurement points, while the WP group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement between baseline and short term follow-up. The results relating to gait were derived from the Timed up and Go over two metres. No statistically significant difference was found between groups at any of the measurement points for the components of total time, gait speed or rise to stand time. Looking longitudinally within each group, again there were no differences within the groups. Testing step up ability with the affected leg stepping up on the Step Test, once again there was no significant difference between the groups. Looking longitudinally within the groups, only the Control group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement, a change from a median score of zero to two. The data for stepping up with the unaffected leg demonstrated that the PP group made a statistically significant improvement from a median score of two at baseline to four at end of intervention. Testing arm function with the Frenchay Arm Test, once again there was no significant difference between the groups at any time point. Looking longitudinally within the groups, only the PP group demonstrated a statistically significant improvement from baseline to end of intervention. 317 Mood was measured using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Measure. For the Anxiety sub-scale there was no significant difference between the groups at any time point. Looking longitudinally within the groups, once again the unexpected finding was that the Control group demonstrated a statistically significant reduction in anxiety from baseline to end of intervention. For the Depression sub-scale there was no significant difference between the groups at any time point or within the groups over time. The Stroke Impact Scale was used as a general indicator of health status. There was no significant difference between the groups at any time point for any of the domains. Looking longitudinally within the groups, the PP group showed statistically significant improvements on the domains of strength, mood, and mobility from baseline to end of intervention. On the domain of participation, both PP and WP showed a statistically significant improvement between baseline and long-term follow-up, and the PP group demonstrated this improvement between end of intervention and follow up too. In terms of monitoring activity over one day, data were available for 45 participants. Between 75% and 85% of the day was spent in sitting or lying. Only one participant took more than 10,000 steps and only a further four participants took more than 5000 steps. Participants were able to follow the exercise programme, undertake the six exercises and record the number of exercises undertaken. Weekly averages of the exercises were calculated and ranged from 102 for the WP step ups to 118 for PP and WP groups undertaking UL exercises. The amount of repetitions was less than the thousands recommended in Motor Learning literature to effect learning, however 318 as can be seen from this section, there were some positive improvements, some changes were retained at long-term follow-up and this would indicate Motor Learning had occurred. While some positive findings arose from this study, the work should be viewed as embryonic in providing one small fragment of clinically based evidence relating to how to the structure of functional task practice for people with late stage stroke. 319 Reference List Aamodt, G., Kjendahl, A. and Jahnsen, R. 2006. Dimensionality and scalability of the Motor Assessment Scale (MAS). Disability & Rehabilitation, 28 (16), pp.1007-1013. Aben, I., Verhey, F., Lousberg, R., Lodder, J. and Honig, A. 2002. Validity of the Beck Depression Inventory, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, SCL-90, and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale as Screening Instruments for Depression in Stroke Patients. Psychosomatics, 43 (5), pp.386-393. Adams, J.A. 1971. A closed loop theory of motor learning, Journal of Motor Behaviour. 3, pp 111-150 Adams RJ, Meador KJ, Sethi KD, Grotta JC, Thomson DS. 1987. Graded neurologic scale for use in acute hemispheric stroke treatment protocols. Stroke. 18: pp665-669 Adams, S. A., Ashburn, A., Pickering, R. M. and Taylor, D. 1997. The scalability of the Rivermead Motor Assessment in acute stroke patients. Clinical Rehabilitation, 11 (1), pp.42-51. Agrell, B. and Dehlin, O. 2000. Mini mental state examination in geriatric stroke patients. Validity, differences between subgroups of patients, and relationships to somatic and mental variables. Aging (Milan, Italy), 12 (6), pp.439-444. Altman DG. Practical Statistics for Medical Research. 1991. London, Chapman and Hall. Anderson, C. S., Linto, J. and Stewart-Wynne, E. G. 1995. A Population-Based Assessment of the Impact and Burden of Caregiving for Long-term Stroke Survivors. Stroke, 26 (5), pp.843-849. Anderson, R. 2008. New MRC guidance on evaluating complex interventions: clarifying what interventions work by researching how and why they are effective. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 337 (7676), pp.944-945. Anthony, J. C., LeResche, L., Niaz, U., von Korff, M. R. and Folstein, M. F. 1982. Limits of the 'Mini-Mental State' as a screening test for dementia and delirium among hospital patients. Psychological Medicine, 12 (2), pp.397-408. Appelros, P. 2007. Characteristics of the Frenchay Activities Index one year after a stroke: a population-based study. Disability & Rehabilitation, 29 (10), pp.785-790. Aziz NA, Leonardi-Bee J, Phillips MF, Gladman J, Legg LA, Walker M. Therapy-based rehabilitation services for patients living at home more than one year after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2009, Issue 2. Art. No.: CD005952. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD005952.pub2 Bach y Rita, P. 2001. Theoretical and practical considerations in the restoration of function after stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 8 (3), pp.1-15. 320 Reference List Bach-y-Rita, P. and Balliet, R. 1987. Recovery from stroke. In: Duncan PW, Badke MB, eds. Stroke Rehabilitation: The Recovery of Motor Control. Chicago, Year Book Medical Publishers Inc; pp 79-107. Baddeley, A.D. & Longman D. 1978. The influence of length and frequency of training sessions on the rate of learning. Ergonomics. 21: pp 627-635. Baer, G D and Ashburn, A. 1995 Trunk movements in older subjects during sit-to-stand Arch Phys Med Rehab, 76 (9): pp 844 - 849. Baer, G.D. and Durward, B.R. 2003 Establishing the reliability and validity of a two metre timed th up and go (TUG2m) for community dwelling stroke patients. 14 International World Confederation of Physical Therapy Congress Barcelona, Spain RR-PL-1454 Baer G. and O’Loughlin H. 2007. Stepping out: an investigation into the level of agreement between three measures of step count.: Physiotherapy 93(S1):S94 Bamford, J., Sandercock, P., Dennis, M., Warlow, C. and Burn, J. 1991. Classification and natural history of clinically identifiable subtypes of cerebral infarction. The Lancet, 337 (8756), pp.1521-1526. Bamford, J., Sandercock, P., Dennis, M., Warlow, C., Jones, L., McPherson, K., Vessey, M., Fowler, G., Molyneux, A. and Hughes, T. 1988. A prospective study of acute cerebrovascular disease in the community: the Oxfordshire Community Stroke Project 1981-86. 1. Methodology, demography and incident cases of first-ever stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 51 (11), pp.1373-1380. Bamiou, D. E., Davies, R. A., McKee, M. and Luxon, L. M. 2000. Symptoms, disability and handicap in unilateral peripheral vestibular disorders: effects of early presentation and initiation of balance exercises. Scandinavian Audiology, 29 (4), pp.238-244. Barnes, M., Dobkin, B. H. and Bogousslavsky, J. Recovery after stroke. 2005. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Baskett, J.J., Broad, J.B., Reekie, G., Hocking, C. and Green, G. 1999. Shared Responsibility for ongoing rehabilitation: a new approach to home-based therapy after stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation 13 (1): pp 123-133. Bassey, E. J. 2000. The benefits of exercise for the health of older people. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 10 (1), pp.17-31. Bassey, E. J. 2005. The benefits of physical activity for health in older people - An update. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 15 (1), pp.1-8. Bayona, N. A., Bitensky, J., Salter, K. and Teasell, R. 2005. The role of task-specific training in rehabilitation therapies. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 12 (3), pp.58-65. Bear, M., Connors, BW. and Paradiso, MA. Neuroscience Exploring the Brain. 3rd edition. 2007. Philadelphia, Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. 321 Reference List Bennett, H. E., Thomas, S. A., Austen, R., Morris, A. M. S. and Lincoln, N. B. 2006. Validation of screening measures for assessing mood in stroke patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45 (Part 3), pp.367-376. Berg, KO., Wood-Dauphinee, S., Williams, JL. and Maki, B. Measuring balance in the elderly: Validation of an instrument. Physiotherapy Canada, 41[6], 304-311. 1989. Bernhardt, J., Ellis, P., Denisenko, S. and Hill, K. 1998. Changes in balance and locomotion measures during rehabilitation following stroke. Physiotherapy Research International:, 3 (2), pp.109-122. Bernhardt, J., Dewey, H., Thrift, A. and Donnan, G. 2004. Inactive and Alone: Physical Activity Within the First 14 Days of Acute Stroke Unit Care. Stroke, 35 (4), pp.1005-1009 Bernhardt, J., Chitravas, N., Meslo, I., Thrift, A.G. and Indredavik, B. 2008. Not All Stroke Units Are the Same. A Comparison of Physical Activity Patterns in Melbourne, Australia, and Trondheim, Norway. Stroke. 39: pp2059-2065. Bhakta, B. B. 2000. Management of spasticity in stroke. British Medical Bulletin, 56 (2), pp.476485. Bilodeau, E. A. and Bilodeau, I. M. 1958. Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55 (4), pp.379-383. Bilodeau, E. A., Bilodeau, I. M. and Schumsky, D. A. 1959. Some effects of introducing and withdrawing knowledge of results early and late in practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58 (2), pp.142-144. Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T. and Neckelmann, D. 2002. The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: An updated literature review. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research, 52 (2), pp.69-77. Blake, H., McKinney, M., Treece, K., Lee, E. and Lincoln, N. B. 2002. An evaluation of screening measures for cognitive impairment after stroke. Age & Ageing, 31 (6), pp.451-456. Bland, J. M. and Altman, D. G. 1986. Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet, 1 (8476), pp.307-310. Bland, J. M. and Altman, D. G. 2010. Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 47 (8), pp.931-936. Blanton, S., Morris, D. M., Prettyman, M. G., McCulloch, K., Redmond, S., Light, K. E. and Wolf, S. L. 2006. Lessons Learned in Participant Recruitment and Retention: The EXCITE Trial. Physical Therapy, 86 (11), pp.1520-1533. Bleecker, M. L., Bolla-Wilson, K., Kawas, C. and Agnew, J. 1988. Age-specific norms for the Mini-Mental State Exam. Neurology, 38 (10), pp.1565-1568. Bobath, B. Adult Hemiplegia: Evaluation and Treatment. 3rd edition. 1990. Oxford, Heinemann Medical Books. 322 Reference List Bohannon, R. W. 1997. Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20-79 years: reference values and determinants. Age & Ageing, 26 (1), pp.15-19. Bond, J., Gregson, B., Smith, M., Rousseau, N., Lecouturier, J. and Rodgers, H. 1998. Outcomes following acute hospital care for stroke or hip fracture: how useful is an assessment of anxiety or depression for older people? International Journal Of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13 (9), pp.601-610. Bond, M. J., Harris, R. D., Smith, D. S. and Clark, M. S. 1992. An examination of the factor structure of the Frenchay Activities Index. Disability & Rehabilitation, 14 (1), pp.27-29. Bonita, R. and Beaglehole, R. 1988. Recovery of motor function after stroke. Stroke, 19 (12), pp.1497-1500. Botolfsen, P., Helbostad, J. L., Moe-Nilssen, R. and Wall, J. C. 2008. Reliability and concurrent validity of the Expanded Timed Up-and-Go test in older people with impaired mobility. Physiotherapy Research International, 13 (2), pp.94-106. Boyd, L. A. and Winstein, C. J. 2001. Implicit motor-sequence learning in humans following unilateral stroke: the impact of practice and explicit knowledge. Neuroscience Letters, 298 (1), pp.65-69. Boyd, L., Siengsukon, C. F. and Vidoni, E. D. 2006. Middle cerebral artery stroke impairs transfer of learning for complex but not simple motor sequences... Platforms, thematic posters, and posters for CSM 2007. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 30 (4), pp.203-204. Boyd, L. A., Vidoni, E. D. and Wessel, B. D. 2010. Motor learning after stroke: Is skill acquisition a prerequisite for contralesional neuroplastic change? Neuroscience Letters, 482 (1), pp.21-25. Boyd, L. A., Quaney, B. M., Pohl, P. S. and Winstein, C. J. 2007. Learning implicitly: Effects of task and severity after stroke. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 21 (5), pp.444-454. Brauer, S. G., Bew, P. G., Kuys, S. S., Lynch, M. R. and Morrison, G. 2008. Prediction discharge destination after stroke using the motor assessment scale on admission: a prospective, multisite study. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 89 (6), pp.1061-1065. Braun, D. A., Aertsen, A., Wolpert, D. M. and Mehring, C. 2009. Learning optimal adaptation strategies in unpredictable motor tasks. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29 (20), pp.64726478. Britton, E., Harris, N. and Turton, A. 2008. An exploratory randomized controlled trial of assisted practice for improving sit-to-stand in stroke patients in the hospital setting. Clinical Rehabilitation, 22 (5), pp.458-468. Brock, K. A., Goldie, P. A. and Greenwood, K. M. 2002. Evaluating the effectiveness of stroke rehabilitation: choosing a discriminative measure. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 83 (1), pp.92-99. 323 Reference List Brooks, S. P. and Dunnett, S. B. 2009. Lesions of the premotor and supplementary motor areas fail to prevent implicit learning in the operant serial implicit learning task. Brain Research, 1284 pp.116-124. Brunnstrom, S. 1970. Movement therapy in hemiplegia : a neurophysiological approach. London, Harper and Row Buck, D., Jacoby, A., Massey, A. and Ford, G. 2000. Evaluation of Measures Used to Assess Quality of Life After Stroke. Stroke, 31 (8), pp.2004-2010. Butefisch, C. M. 2004. Plasticity in the Human Cerebral Cortex: Lessons from the Normal Brain and from Stroke. The Neuroscientist, 10 (2), pp.163-173. Cameron, I. M., Crawford, J. R., Lawton, K. and Reid, I. C. 2008. Psychometric comparison of PHQ-9 and HADS for measuring depression severity in primary care. The British Journal Of General Practice: The Journal Of The Royal College Of General Practitioners, 58 (546), pp.32-36. Campbell, M., Fitzpatrick, R., Haines, A., Kinmonth, A. L., Sandercock, P., Spiegelhalter, D. and Tyrer, P. 2000. Framework for design and evaluation of complex interventions to improve health. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 321 (7262), pp.694-696. Carr, J.H., and Shepherd, R.B. 1980. Physiotherapy in disorders of the brain. Oxford, Butterworth Heinemann. Carr, J. H. and Shepherd R.B. Neurological Rehabilitation: optimizing motor performance. 1998. Oxford, UK, Butterworth Heinemann. Carr, J. H. and Shepherd R.B. Stroke rehabilitation: guidelines for exercise and training to optimize motor skill. 2003. Edinburgh: UK, Butterworth Heinemann. Carr, J. H., Shepherd, R. B., Nordholm, L. and Lynne, D. 1985. Investigation of a New Motor Assessment Scale for Stroke Patients. Physical Therapy, 65 (2), pp.175-180. Cauraugh, J. H. and Kim, S. B. 2003. Stroke motor recovery: active neuromuscular stimulation and repetitive practice schedules. Journal Of Neurology, Neurosurgery, And Psychiatry, 74 (11), pp.1562-1566. Chatterton, HJ., Pomeroy, V. M. and Gratton, J. 2001. Positioning for stroke patients: a survey of physiotherapists' aims and practices. Disability & Rehabilitation, 23 (10), pp.413-421. Chen, R., Cohen, L. G. and Hallett, M. 2002. Nervous system reorganization following injury. Neuroscience, 111 (4), pp.761-773. Chong, D. K.-H. 1995. Measurement of Instrumental Activities of Daily Living in Stroke. Stroke, 26 (6), pp.1119-1122. Cirstea, M. C. and Levin, M. F. 2007. Improvement of Arm Movement Patterns and Endpoint Control Depends on Type of Feedback During Practice in Stroke Survivors. Neurorehabilitation And Neural Repair, 21 (5), pp.398-411. 324 Reference List Cohen, L. G., Brasil-Neto, J. P., Pascual-Leone, A. and Hallett, M. 1993. Plasticity of cortical motor output organization following deafferentation, cerebral lesions, and skill acquisition. Advances In Neurology, 63 pp.187-200. Collin, C., Wade, D. T., Davies, S. and Horne, V. 1988. The Barthel ADL Index: A reliability study. Disability & Rehabilitation, 10 (2), pp.61-63. Combs, S. A., Kelly, S. P., Barton, R., Ivaska, M. and Nowak, K. 2009. Effects of an intensive, task-specific rehabilitation program for individuals with chronic stroke: A case series. Disability & Rehabilitation, 32 (8), pp.669-678. Cramp, MC., Greenwood, RJ., Gill, M., Lehmann, A., Rothwell, JC., and Scott, OM. 2010. Effectiveness of a community-based low intensity exercise programme for ambulatory stroke survivors. Disabil Rehabil 32 (3): 239 – 247. Crawford, JR., Henry, JD., Crombie, C. and Taylor, E. 2001. Normative data for the HADS from a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 40[3], 429-434. Crum, R. M., Anthony, J. C., Bassett, S. S. and Folstein, M. F. 1993. Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA: The Journal Of The American Medical Association, 269 (18), pp.2386-2391. D'Alisa, S., Baudo, S., Mauro, A. and Miscio, G. 2005. How does stroke restrict participation in long-term post-stroke survivors? Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 112 (3), pp.157-162. Dahlgren, G., Carlsson, D., Moorhead, A., Häger-Ross, C. and McDonough, S. M. 2010. Testretest reliability of step counts with the ActivPAL device in common daily activities. Gait & Posture, 32 (3), pp.386-390. Daley, K., Mayo, N., and Wood-Dauphinee, S. 1999. Reliability of Scores on the Stroke Rehabilitation Assessment of Movement (STREAM) Measure. Physiotherapy Canada. 79 (1), pp 8 – 22. Daley K, Mayo, N., Danys I., Cabot, R. and Wood-Dauphinee, S. 1997. The Stroke Rehabilitation Assessment of Movement (STREAM): refining and validating the content. Physiotherapy Canada 49[4], 269-278. Dancause, N., Ptito, A. and Levin, M. F. 2002. Error correction strategies for motor behavior after unilateral brain damage: short-term motor learning processes. Neuropsychologia, 40 (8), pp.1313-1323. Davies, P. M. Steps to Follow. 1985. New York, Springer-Verlag. Davies, P.M. 1990. Right in the middle : selective trunk activity in the treatment of adult hemiplegia. New York; Springer-Verlag. Daving, Y., Andrén, E., Nordholm, L. and Grimby, G. 2001. Reliability of an interview approach to the Functional Independence Measure. Clinical Rehabilitation, 15 (3), pp.301-310. de Haan, R., Aaronson, N., Limburg, M., Hewer, R. L. and van Crevel, H. 1993. Measuring quality of life in stroke. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 24 (2), pp.320-327. De Wit L; Putman K; Dejaeger E; Baert I; Berman P; Bogaerts K; Brinkmann N; Connell L; Feys H; Jenni W; Kaske C; Lesaffre E; Leys M; Lincoln N; Louckx F; Schuback B; Schupp W; 325 Reference List Smith B; and De Weerdt W. 2005. Use of time by stroke patients: a comparison of four European rehabilitation Centers. Stroke 36 (9): 1977-1983 Dean, C. M. and Mackey, F. M. 2010. Motor Assessment Scale scores as a measure of rehabilitation outcome following stroke. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy 38[1], 31-35. Dean, C. M. and Shepherd, R. B. 1997. Task-Related Training Improves Performance of Seated Reaching Tasks After Stroke : A Randomized Controlled Trial. Stroke, 28 (4), pp.722728. Dean, C. M., Richards, C. L. and Malouin, F. 2000. Task-related circuit training improves performance of locomotor tasks in chronic stroke: a randomized, controlled pilot trial. Archives Of Physical Medicine And Rehabilitation, 81 (4), pp.409-417. Dean, C. M., Richards, C. L. and Malouin, F. 2001. Walking speed over 10 metres overestimates locomotor capacity after stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 15 (4), pp.415-421. Department of Health. 2007. National Stroke Strategy. London; DH Publications. Deprey, S. M. 1999. Random practice to enhance safe compensation of neurological deficits: a case report. Neurology Report, 23 (2), pp.52-56. Ding, Y., Zhou, Y., Lai, Q., Li, J., Park, H. and Diaz, F. G. 2002. Impaired motor activity and motor learning function in rat with middle cerebral artery occlusion. Behavioural Brain Research , 132 (1), pp.29-36. Dobkin, B. H. 2007. Confounders in rehabilitation trials of task-oriented training: lessons from the designs of the EXCITE and SCILT multicenter trials. Neurorehabilitation And Neural Repair, 21 (1), pp.3-13. Dobkin, B. H. and Carmichael, TS. 2005. Principles of Recovery after Stroke. in M. Barnes, B. H. Dobkin, J. Bogousslavsky. Recovery After Stroke. 46-66. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Dorman, P. J., Waddell, F., Slattery, J., Dennis, M. and Sandercock, P. 1997. Is the EuroQol a Valid Measure of Health-Related Quality of Life After Stroke? Stroke, 28 (10), pp.18761882. Doyon, J. and Benali, H. 2005. Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15 (2), pp.161-167. Duncan, P. W., Weiner, D. K., Chandler, J. and Studenski, S. 1990. Functional reach: a new clinical measure of balance. Journal Of Gerontology, 45 (6), p.M192-M197. Duncan, P. W., Studenski, S., Chandler, J. and Prescott, B. 1992a. Functional reach: predictive validity in a sample of elderly male veterans. Journal Of Gerontology, 47 (3), p.M93M98. Duncan, P. W., Goldstein, L. B., Matchar, D., Divine, G. W. and Feussner, J. 1992b. Measurement of motor recovery after stroke. Outcome assessment and sample size requirements. Stroke, 23 (8), pp.1084-1089. 326 Reference List Duncan, P. W., Samsa, G. P., Weinberger, M., Goldstein, L. B., Bonito, A., Witter, D. M., Enarson, C. and Matchar, D. 1997. Health Status of Individuals With Mild Stroke. Stroke, 28 (4), pp.740-745 Duncan, P., Richards, L., Wallace, D., Stoker-Yates, J., Pohl, P., Luchies, C., Ogle, A., and Studenski, S., 1998. A Randomized, Controlled Pilot Study of a Home-Based Exercise Program for Individuals With Mild and Moderate Stroke. Stroke 29; pp 2055 – 2060. Duncan, P. W., Wallace, D., Lai, S. M., Johnson, D., Embretson, S. and Laster, L. J. 1999a. The Stroke Impact Scale Version 2.0: evaluation of reliability, validity, and sensitivity to change. Stroke, 30 (10), pp.2131-2140. Duncan, P. W., Lai, S., van Culin, V., Huang, L., Clausen, D. and Wallace, D. 1999b. Development of a comprehensive assessment toolbox for stroke. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 15 (4), pp.885-915. Duncan, P. W., Lai, S. M., Tyler, D., Perera, S., Reker, D. M. and Studenski, S. 2002. Evaluation of Proxy Responses to the Stroke Impact Scale. Stroke, 33 (11), pp.2593-2599. Duncan, P., Studenski, S., Richards, L., Gollub, S., Lai, S. M., Reker, D., Perera, S., Yates, J., Koch, V., Rigler, S. and Johnson, D. 2003. Randomized clinical trial of therapeutic exercise in subacute stroke. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 34 (9), pp.21732180 Easton, J. D., Saver, J. L., Albers, G. W., Alberts, M. J., Chaturvedi, S., Feldmann, E., Hatsukami, T. S., Higashida, R. T., Johnston, S. C., Kidwell, C. S., Lutsep, H. L., Miller, E. and Sacco, R. L. 2009. Definition and Evaluation of Transient Ischemic Attack: A Scientific Statement for Healthcare Professionals Stroke, 40 (6), pp.2276-2293. Edwards, M.S., 2002. Neurological Physiotherapy: a problem-solving approach. Edinburgh, Churchill Livingstone. Edwards, B. and O'Connell, B. 2003. Internal consistency and validity of the Stroke Impact Scale 2.0 (SIS 2.0) and SIS-16 in an Australian sample. Quality of Life Research, 12 (8), pp.1127-1135. Effective Health Care Series 1992. Stroke Rehabilitation. Leeds: Leeds Evaluation Unit Egerton, T., Maxwell, D., Granat, M., Wellwood, I., Langhorne, P., McAlpine, C. and McDonald, J. Measurement of Upright Activity of Patients in Stroke Units. Physiotherapy 88[3], 176177. Endres, M., Nyáry, I., Bánhidi, M. and Deák, G. 1990. Stroke rehabilitation: a method and evaluation. International Journal Of Rehabilitation Research 13 (3), pp.225-236. Eng, J. J., Chu, K. S., Kim, C. M., Dawson, A. S., Carswell, A. and Hepburn, K. E. 2003. A community-based group exercise program for persons with chronic stroke. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 35 (8), pp.1271-1278. Engelter, S. T., Gostynski, M., Papa, S., Frei, M., Born, C., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Gutzwiller, F. and Lyrer, P. A. 2006. Epidemiology of aphasia attributable to first ischemic stroke: 327 Reference List incidence, severity, fluency, etiology, and thrombolysis. Stroke (00392499), 37 (6), pp.1379-1384. English, C. K., Hillier, S. L., Stiller, K. and Warden-Flood, A. 2006. The sensitivity of three commonly used outcome measures to detect change amongst patients receiving inpatient rehabilitation following stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 20 (1), pp.52-55. English, C. K., Hillier, S. L., Stiller, K. R. and Warden-Flood, A. 2007. Circuit class therapy versus individual physiotherapy sessions during inpatient stroke rehabilitation: a controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 88 (8), pp.955-96 Evans, A., Perez, I. i., Harraf, F., Melbourn, A., Steadman, J., Donaldson, N. and Kalra, L. 2001. Can differences in management processes explain different outcomes between stroke unit and stroke-team care? The Lancet, 358 (9293), pp.1586-1592. Ezekiel, H. J., Lehto, N. K., Marley, T. L., Wishart, L. R. and Lee, T. D. 2001. Application of motor learning principles: the physiotherapy client as a problem-solver. III augmented feedback. Physiotherapy Canada, 53 (1), pp.33-39. Fabiani, M., Buckley, J., Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E. and Logie, R. 1989. The training of complex task performance. Acta Psychologica, 71 (1-3), pp.259-299. Feigin, V. and Vander Hoorn, S. 2004. How to study stroke incidence. The Lancet, 363 (9425), p.1920. Feigin, V. L., Lawes, C. M. M., Bennett, D. A. and Anderson, C. S. 2003. Stroke epidemiology: a review of population-based studies of incidence, prevalence, and case-fatality in the late 20th century. Lancet Neurology, 2 (1), pp.43-53. Fell, D. W. 2004. Progressing Therapeutic Intervention in Patients with Neuromuscular Disorders: A Framework to Assist Clinical Decision Making. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 28 (1). pp 35-46 Felten, DL., and Shetty, AN. 2009. Netter’s Atlas of Neuroscience New York: Elsevier Ferrarello, F., Baccini, M., Rinaldi, L. A., Cavallini, M. C., Mossello, E., Masotti, G., Marchionni, N. and Di Bari, M. Efficacy of physiotherapy interventions late after stroke: a metaanalysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. Feydy, A., Carlier, R., Roby-Brami, A., Bussel, B., Cazalis, F., Pierot, L., Burnod, Y. and Maier, M. A. 2002. Longitudinal Study of Motor Recovery After Stroke: Recruitment and Focusing of Brain Activation. Stroke, 33 (6), pp.1610-1617. Feys, H.M., De Weerdt, W.J., Selz, B.B., Steck, G.A.C., Spichiger, R., Vereeck, L.E., Putman, K.D., and van Hoydonck, G.A. 1998. Effect of a Therapeutic Interventions for the Hemiplegic Upper Limb in the Acute Phase after Stroke: A single-blind Randomised Controleed Multicenter Trial. Stroke 29 (4), pp 785 – 792. Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. 3rd edition. 2009. London, Sage Publications. Fitts, P.M. and Posner, M.I. 1967. Human Performance. Belmont, California, Brooks Cole Publishing. 328 Reference List Flansbjer, U., Holmb+ñck, A. M., Downham, D., Patten, C. and Lexell, J. 2005. Reliability of gait performance tests in men and women with hemiparesis after stroke. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 37 (2), pp.75-82. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. and McHugh, P. R. 1975. "Mini-mental state" : A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12 (3), pp.189-198. Fontana, F. E., Mazzardo, O., Furtado, O., Jr. and Gallagher, J. D. 2009. Whole and part practice: A meta-analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109 (2), pp.517-530. Forrester, L. W., Wheaton, L. A. and Luft, A. R. 2008. Exercise-mediated locomotor recovery and lower-limb neuroplasticity after stroke. Journal Of Rehabilitation Research And Development, 45 (2), pp.205-220. Fox, K. R. 1999. The influence of physical activity on mental well-being. Public Health Nutrition, 2 (3A), pp.411-418. Freemantle, N., Pollock, C., Sheldon, T. A., Mason, J. M., Song, F., Long, A. F. and Ibbotson, S. 1992. Formal rehabilitation after stroke. Quality In Health Care: QHC, 1 (2), pp.134-137. French, B., Leathley, M., Sutton, C., McAdam, J., Thomas, L., Forster, A., Langhorne, P., Price, C., Walker, A. and Watkins, C. 2008. A systematic review of repetitive functional task practice with modelling of resource use, costs and effectiveness. Health Technology Assessment 12[30], 1-117. French B, Thomas LH, Leathley MJ, Sutton CJ, McAdam J, Forster A, Langhorne P, Price CIM, Walker A, Watkins CL. Repetitive task training for improving functional ability after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2009, Issue 4. Art. No.: CD006073. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006073.pub2. French, B., Thomas, L., Leathley, M., Sutton, C., McAdam, J., Forster, A., Langhorne, P., Price, C., Walker, A. and Watkins, C. 2010. Does repetitive task training improve functional activity after stroke? A Cochrane systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 42 (1), pp.9-15. Freter, S. H. and Fruchter, N. 2000. Relationship between Timed Up and Go and gait time in an elderly orthopaedic rehabilitation population. Clinical Rehabilitation, 14 (1), pp.96-101. Fugl-Meyer, A., Jääskö L, Leyman I, Olsson S and Steglind S. 1975. The post-stroke hemiplegic patient. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 7[1], 13-31. Gentile, AM. 1978. A working model of Skill Acquisition with Application to Teaching. Quest 17[1], 3-23. Gentile, AM. 1987. Skill Acquisition: action, movement, and neuromotor processes with Application to Teaching, In Carr, J.H. and Shepherd, R.B. (eds) Movement Science Foundations for Physical Therapy in Rehabilitation. Rockville, Aspen Publishers Gharbawie, O. A. and Whishaw, I. Q. 2006. Parallel stages of learning and recovery of skilled reaching after motor cortex stroke: 'Oppositions' organize normal and compensatory movements. Behavioural Brain Research, 175 (2), pp.249-262. 329 Reference List Gilmore, P. E. and Spaulding, S. J. 2001. Motor control and motor learning: Implications for treatment of individuals post stroke. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics, 20 (1), pp.1-15. Glader, E. L., Stegmayr, B. and Asplund, K. 2002. Poststroke fatigue: a 2-year follow-up study of stroke patients in Sweden. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 33 (5), pp.13271333. Gladman, J., Forster, A. and Young, J. 1995. Hospital- and home-based rehabilitation after discharge from hospital for stroke patients: analysis of two trials. Age & Ageing, 24 (1), pp.49-53 Gladman JR, Lincoln NB, Barer DH. 1993. A randomised controlled trial of domiciliary and hospital-based rehabilitation for stroke patients after discharge from hospital. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 56 (9) :960-6. Godfrey, A., Culhane, K. M. and Lyons, G. M. 2007. Comparison of the performance of the activPAL Professional physical activity logger to a discrete accelerometer-based activity monitor. Medical Engineering & Physics, 29 (8), pp.930-934. Gordon, N. F., Gulanick, M., Costa, F., Fletcher, G., Franklin, B. A., Roth, E. J. and Shephard, T. 2004. Physical Activity and Exercise Recommendations for Stroke Survivors: Circulation, 109 (16), pp.2031-2041. Goverover, Y., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Hillary, F. G., Chiaravalloti, N. and DeLuca, J. 2009a. Application of the spacing effect to improve learning and memory for functional tasks in traumatic brain injury: a pilot study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63 (5), pp.543-548 Goverover, Y., Hillary, F. G., Chiaravalloti, N., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. and DeLuca, J. 2009b. A functional application of the spacing effect to improve learning and memory in persons with multiple sclerosis. Journal Of Clinical And Experimental Neuropsychology, 31 (5), pp.513-522 Gowland, C. 1982. Recovery of motor function following stroke: Profiles and predictors. Physiotherapy Canada 34[2], 77-84. Gowland, C., Stratford, P., Ward, M., Moreland, J., Torresin, W., Van Hullenaar, S., Sanford, J., Barreca, S., Vanspall, B. and Plews, N. 1993. Measuring physical impairment and disability with the Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment. Stroke, 24 (1), pp.58-63. Grant, P. M., Ryan, C. G., Tigbe, W. W. and Granat, M. H. 2006. The validation of a novel activity monitor in the measurement of posture and motion during everyday activities. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40 (12), pp.992-997. Grant, P. M., Ryan, C. G., Tigbe, W. W. and Granat, M. H. 2006. The validation of a novel activity monitor in the measurement of posture and motion during everyday activities. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40 (12), pp.992-997. Grant, PM., Dall, PM., Mitchell, SL and Granat, MH. 2008. Activity-monitor accuracy in measuring step number and cadence in community-dwelling older adults. J Aging Physical Activity 16 (2): 201-214 330 Reference List Green, J. B. 2003. Brain reorganization after stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 10 (3), pp.120. Green, J. and Young, J. 2001. A test-retest reliability study of the Barthel Index, the Rivermead Mobility Index, the Nottingham extended Activities of Daily Living Scale and the Frenchay Activities Index in stroke patients. Disability & Rehabilitation, 23 (15), pp.670676. Green J, Forster A, Boyle S. 2002; Physiotherapy for patients with mobility problems, more than one year after stroke. A randomised controlled trial. Lancet 359:199–203. Green, J., Forster, A. and Young, J. 1999. A survey of community physiotherapy provision after 1 year post-stroke. British Journal of Therapy & Rehabilitation, 6 (5), p.216-221 Green, J., Forster, A., Bogle, S. and Young, J. 2002. Physiotherapy for patients with mobility problems more than 1 year after stroke: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet, 359 (9302), pp.199-203 Green, J., Young, J., Forster, A., Collen, F. and Wade, D. 2004. Combined analysis of two randomized trials of community physiotherapy for patients more than one year post stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18 (3), pp.249-252. Hacke, W., Kaste, M., Skyhoj Olsen, T., Orgogozo, J. M. and Bogousslavsky, J. 2000. European Stroke Initiative (EUSI) Recommendations for Stroke Management The European Stroke Initiative Writing Committee. European Journal of Neurology, 7 (6), pp.607-623. Hackett, M. L., Yapa, C., Parag, V. and Anderson, C. S. 2005. Frequency of depression after stroke: a systematic review of observational studies. Stroke (00392499), 36 (6), pp.1330-1340. Hademenos, G. J. and Massoud, T. F. 1997. Biophysical Mechanisms of Stroke. Stroke, 28 (10), pp.2067-2077 Hale, L., Bennett, D., Bentley, M., Crawshaw, A. and Davis, H. 2003. Stroke rehabilitation -comparing hospital and home-based physiotherapy: the patient's perception. New Zealand Journal of Physiotherapy, 31 (2), pp.84-92. Hanlon, R. E. 1996. Motor learning following unilateral stroke. Archives Of Physical Medicine And Rehabilitation, 77 (8), pp.811-815 Hansen, S., Tremblay, L. and Elliott, D. 2005. Part and whole practice: chunking and online control in the acquisition of a serial motor task. Research Quarterly For Exercise And Sport, 76 (1), pp.60-66. Hardie, K., Hankey, G. J., Jamrozik, K., Broadhurst, R. J. and Anderson, C. 2004. Ten-Year Risk of First Recurrent Stroke and Disability After First-Ever Stroke in the Perth Community Stroke Study. Stroke, 35 (3), pp.731-735. Hare, R., Rogers, H., Lester, H., McManus, R. J. and Mant, J. 2006. What do stroke patients and their carers want from community services? Family Practice, 23 (1), pp.131-136. 331 Reference List Hartman-Maeir, A., Eliad, Y., Kizoni, R., Nahaloni, I., Kelberman, H. and Katz, N. 2007. Evaluation of a long-term community based rehabilitation program for adult stroke survivors. Neurorehabilitation, 22 (4), pp.295-301. Hashimoto, K., Higuchi, K., Nakayama, Y. and Abo, M. 2007. Ability for Basic Movement as an Early Predictor of Functioning Related to Activities of Daily Living in Stroke Patients. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 21 no. 4 353-357 Heller, A., Wade, D. T., Wood, V. A., Sunderland, A., Hewer, R. L. and Ward, E. 1987. Arm function after stroke: measurement and recovery over the first three months. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 50 (6), pp.714-719. Herbert, R. 2005. How muscles respond to stretch. In Refshauge, K., Ada, L., and Ellis, E (eds) Science-based Rehabilitation: theories into practice. Edinburgh, Butterworth Heinemann. Herrmann, C. 1997. International experiences with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-A review of validation data and clinical results. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research, 42 (1), pp.17-41. Hesse, S., Bertelt, C., Schaffrin, A., Malezic, M., and Mauritz, KH. 1994. Restoration of gait in nonambulatory hemiparetic patients by treadmill training with partial body-weight support. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 75(10): pp 1087-1093. Higgins, J., Salbach, N. M., Wood-Dauphinee, S., Richards, C. L., Côté, R. and Mayo, N. E. 2006. The effect of a task-oriented intervention on arm function in people with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 20 (4), pp.296-310. Hill, KD., Bernhardt, J., McGann, AM., Maltese, D. and Berlovits, D. 1996.A new test of Dynamic Standing Balance for Stroke Patients: Reliability, Validity and Comparison with Healthy Elderly. Physiotherapy Canada 48[4], 257-262. Hillary, F. G., Schultheis, M. T., Challis, B. H., Millis, S. R., Carnevale, G. J., Galshi, T. and DeLuca, J. 2003. Spacing of repetitions improves learning and memory after moderate and severe TBI. Journal Of Clinical And Experimental Neuropsychology, 25 (1),pp.49-58 Hochstenbach, J. and Mulder, T. 1999. Neuropsychology and the relearning of motor skills following stroke. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 22 (1), pp.11-19. Holbrook, M.A.R.G. and Skilbeck, C.E.. 1983. An Activities Index for use with Stroke Patients. Age and Ageing, 12 (2), pp.166-170 Holmgren, E., Gosman-Hedstr+Âm, G., Lindstr+Âm, B. and Wester, P. 2010. What is the benefit of a high-intensive exercise program on health-related quality of life and depression after stroke? A randomized controlled trial. Advances in Physiotherapy, 12 (3), pp.125-133. Horak, F. B. Assumptions underlying motor control for rehabilitation. 1991 M. J. Lister. Contemporary Management of Motor Control Problems: Proceedings of the II Step Conference. 11-27.. Alexandria, VA, Foundation for Physical Therapy. Hsueh, I. P., Lin, J. H., Jeng, J. S. and Hsieh, C. L. 2002. Comparison of the psychometric characteristics of the functional independence measure, 5 item Barthel index, and 10 332 Reference List item Barthel index in patients with stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 73 (2), pp.188-190. Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party. National Sentinel Stroke Audit - Organisation of Care 2010. 2010. London, Royal College of Physicians. ISD (Information Services Division). 2000. Scottish Health Statistics 2000. [online].. available from http://www.show.scot.nhs.uk/isd/Scottish_Health_Statistics/SHS2000/home.htm [accessed December 1 2010] ISD (Information Services Division). Stroke [online]. 2009. Available http://www.isdscotland.org/isd/5784.html [accessed November 20 2010]. from ISD and International Services Division. Scottish Stroke Care Audit: 2010 National Report. 2010. Edinburgh, International Services Division. Ivey, F. M., Hafer-Macko, C. E. and Macko, R. F. 2006. Exercise rehabilitation after stroke. Neurorx: The Journal Of The American Society For Experimental Neurotherapeutics, 3 (4), pp.439-450. Jackson, P. L., Doyon, J., Richards, C. L. and Malouin, F. 2004. The Efficacy of Combined Physical and Mental Practice in the Learning of a Foot-Sequence Task after Stroke: A Case Report. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 18 (2), pp.106-111. Janssen, W., Bussmann, J., Selles, R., Koudstaal, P., Ribbers, G. and Stam, H. 2010. Recovery of the Sit-to-Stand Movement After Stroke: A Longitudinal Cohort Study. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 24 (8), pp.763-769. Jette, D. U., Latham, N. K., Smout, R. J., Gassaway, J., Slavin, M. D. and Horn, S. D. 2005. Physical therapy interventions for patients with stroke in inpatient rehabilitation facilities. Physical Therapy, 85 (3), pp.238-248. Johnson, G., Burvill, P. W., Anderson, C. S., Jamrozik, K., Stewart-Wynne, E. G. and Chakera, T. M. 1995. Screening instruments for depression and anxiety following stroke: experience in the Perth community stroke study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 91 (4), pp.252-257 Johnston, M., Pollard, B. and Hennessey, P. 2000. Construct validation of the hospital anxiety and depression scale with clinical populations. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research, 48 (6), pp.579-584 Jones, R. N. and Gallo, J. J. 2000. Dimensions of the Mini-Mental State Examination among community dwelling older adults. Psychological Medicine, 30 (3), pp.605-618. Jorgensen, H. S., Nakayama, H., Raaschou, H. O., Larsen, K., Hubbe, P. and Olsen, T. S. 1995. The Effect of a Stroke Unit: Reductions in Mortality, Discharge Rate to Nursing Home, Length of Hospital Stay, and Cost : A Community-Based Study. Stroke, 26 (7), pp.11781182. 333 Reference List Jourden, F. J., Bandura, A. and Banfield, J. T. 1991. The impact of conceptions of ability on selfregulatory factors and motor skill acquisition. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 13 (3), pp.213-226. Kasai, T. 1988. Is the reduction of reaction time an adequate estimate of the effect of practice? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66 (1), pp.51-56. Keith, R. A., Granger, C. V., Hamilton, B. B. and Sherwin, F. S. 1987. The functional independence measure: a new tool for rehabilitation. Advances In Clinical Rehabilitation, 1 pp.6-18. Kellor, M., Frost, J., Silberberg, N. and Iversen, I 1971.. Hand Strength and Dexterity. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 5, 77-83. Kelly-Hayes, P. M., Robertson, J. T., Broderick, J. P., Duncan, P. W., Hershey, L. A., Roth, E. J., Thies, W. H. and Trombly, C. A. 1998. The American Heart Association Stroke Outcome Classification. Stroke, 29 (6), pp.1274-1280. Kenneth, J. O., Yungwen, H., Carl, V. G. and Roger, C. F. 1996. The reliability of the functional independence measure: A quantitative review. Archives Of Physical Medicine And Rehabilitation 77[12], 1226-1232. Khaw, KT. 1996. Epidemiology of Stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 61, 333-338. Kim, C., Eng, J., MacIntyre, DL. and Dawson, AS. 2001. Effects of isokinetic strength training on walking in persons with stroke: a double-blind controlled pilot study. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 10(6); pp 265-273. Kistler, J. P. and Furie, K. L. 2000. Carotid endarterectomy revisited. The New England Journal Of Medicine, 342 (23), pp.1743-1745. Kleim, J. A., Hogg, T. M., VandenBerg, P. M., Cooper, N. R., Bruneau, R. and Remple, M. 2004. Cortical Synaptogenesis and Motor Map Reorganization Occur during Late, But Not Early, Phase of Motor Skill Learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24 (3), pp.628-633 Kollen, B., Kwakkel, G. and Lindeman, E. 2006. Hemiplegic gait after stroke: is measurement of maximum speed required? Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87 (3), pp.358-363. Krakauer, J. W. 2006. Motor learning: its relevance to stroke recovery and neurorehabilitation. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19 (1), pp.84-90. Kreisel, SH., Hennerici, M. and Bazner H. Pathophysiology of Stroke Rehabilitation: The natural course of clinical recovery, use-dependent plasticity and rehabilitative outcomes. Cerebrovascular Diseases 23[4], 243-255. 2007. Krishnan, R. V. 2006. Relearning toward motor recovery in stroke, spinal cord injury, and cerebral palsy: A cognitive neural systems perspective. International Journal of Neuroscience, 116 (2), pp.127-140. 334 Reference List Kurtais, Y., Küçükdeveci, A., Elhan, A., Yýlmaz, A., Kalli, T., Tur, B. S. and Tennant, A. 2009. Psychometric Properties of the Rivermead Motor Assessment: Its utility in Stroke. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 41[13], 1055-1061. Kwakkel, G. 2006. Impact of intensity of practice after stroke: Issues for consideration. Disability & Rehabilitation, 28 (13-14), pp.823-830. Kwakkel, G., Kollen, B. and Lindeman, E. 2004. Understanding the pattern of functional recovery after stroke: Facts and theories . Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 22[3-5], 281299 Kwakkel, G., van Peppen, R., Wagenaar, R. C., Dauphinee, S. W., Richards, C., Ashburn, A., Miller, K., Lincoln, N., Partridge, C., Wellwood, I. and Langhorne, P. 2004. Effects of augmented exercise therapy time after stroke: a meta-analysis. Stroke (00392499), 35 (11), pp.2529-2536. Kwakkel, G. Wagenaar, RC. Kollen, BJ. And Lankhorst, GJ. 1996. Predicting Disability in Stroke--A Critical Review of the Literature. Age and Ageing, 25 (6), pp.479-489. Kwon, S., Hartzema, A. G., Duncan, P. W. and Min-Lai, S. 2004. Disability Measures in Stroke: Relationship Among the Barthel Index, the Functional Independence Measure, and the Modified Rankin Scale. Stroke, 35 (4), pp.918-923. Kwon, S., Duncan, P., Studenski, S., Perera, S., Lai, S. and Reker, D. 2006. Measuring Stroke Impact with SIS: Construct Validity of SIS Telephone Administration. Quality of Life Research, 15 (3), pp.367-376. Lai, S. M., Studenski, S., Duncan, P. W. and Perera, S. 2002. Persisting Consequences of Stroke Measured by the Stroke Impact Scale. Stroke, 33 (7), pp.1840-1844. Lai, S. M., Perera, S., Duncan, P. W. and Bode, R. 2003. Physical and Social Functioning After Stroke: Comparison of the Stroke Impact Scale and Short Form-36. Stroke, 34 (2), pp.488-493. Lai, S. M., Studenski, S., Richards, L., Perera, S., Reker, D., Rigler, S. and Duncan, P. W. 2006. Therapeutic Exercise and Depressive Symptoms After Stroke. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 54 (2), pp.240-247 Lamb, S. E., Ferrucci, L., Volapto, S., Fried, L. P., Guralnik, J. M. and Gustafson, Y. 2003. Risk Factors for Falling in Home-Dwelling Older Women With Stroke: The Women's Health and Aging Study Stroke, 34 (2), pp.494-501. Lamontagne, A. and Fung, J. 2004. Faster is better: implications for speed-intensive gait training after stroke. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 35 (11), pp.2543-2548. Lang, C. E., MacDonald, J. R. and Gnip, C. 2007. Counting repetitions: an observational study of outpatient therapy for people with hemiparesis post-stroke. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 31 (1), pp.3-10. Langhammer, B. and Stanghelle, J. K. 2006. Co-variation of tests commonly used in stroke rehabilitation. Physiotherapy Research International: 11 (4), pp.228-234. 335 Reference List Langhammer, B., Lindmark, B. and Stanghelle, J. K. 2007. Stroke patients and long-term training: is it worthwhile? A randomized comparison of two different training strategies after rehabilitation. Clinical Rehabilitation, 21 (6), pp.495-510. Langhorne, P. and Legg, L. 2003. Evidence behind Stroke Rehabilitation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 74; iv18-iv21 Langhorne, P., Pollock, A. and in Conjunction with The Stroke Unit Trialists' Collaboration 2002. What are the components of effective stroke unit care? Age and Ageing, 31 (5), pp.365371 Langhorne, P., Coupar, F. and Pollock, A. 2009. Motor recovery after stroke: a systematic review. Lancet Neurology, 8 (8), pp.741-754. Langton Hewer, R. 1993 The Epidemiology of disabling neurological disorders. in Greenwood, R. Barnes, M. and McMillan TM.(eds) Neurological Rehabilitation. 3-12.. London, Churchill Livingstone. Langhorne, P., Wagenaar, R. C. and Partridge, C. 1996 Physiotherapy after stroke: more is better? Physiotherapy Research International 1[2], 75-88. 3. Langhorne, P., Dennis, M. S., Kalra, L., Shepperd, S., Wade, D. T. and Wolfe, C. D. A. 2007. Services for helping acute stroke patients avoid hospital admission. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (4). Langton Hewer, R. 1993. The epidemiology of disabling neurological neurological disorders. In Greenwood, R, Barnes, MP., Mcmillan TM (eds) Neurological rehabilitation. London: Churchill Livingstone: 3-12 Lannin, N. A. 2004. Reliability, validity and factor structure of the upper limb subscale of the Motor Assessment Scale (UL-MAS) in adults following stroke. Disability & Rehabilitation, 26 (2), pp.109-115. Lannin, N. and Cusick, A. 2006. Factors Affecting Patient Recruitment in an Acute Rehabilitation Randomized Controlled Trial. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60 (2), pp.177-181. Law, J., Rush, R., Pringle, A. M., Irving, A. M., Huby, G., Smith, M., Conochie, D., Haworth, C. and Burston, A. 2009. The incidence of cases of aphasia following first stroke referred to speech and language therapy services in Scotland. Aphasiology, 23 (10), pp.1266-1275. Lee, T. D. and Genovese, E. D. 1988. Distribution of practice in motor skill acquisition: learning and performance effects reconsidered. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 58 (4), pp.277-287 Lee, T. D. and Genovese, E. D. 1989. Distribution of practice in motor skill acquisition: differents effects for discrete and continuous tasks. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 60 (1), pp.59-65. Lee, T. D. and Carnahan, H. 1990. When to provide knowledge of results during motor learning: Scheduling effects. Human Performance, 3 (2), pp.87-105. Lee, T. D., Magill, R. A. and Weeks, D. J. 1985. Influence of practice schedule on testing schema theory predictions in adults. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17 (3), pp.283-299. 336 Reference List Lehto, N. K., Marley, T. L., Ezekiel, H. J., Wishart, L. R., Lee, T. D. and Jarus, T. 2001. Application of motor learning principles: the physiotherapy client as a problem-solver. IV. Future directions. Physiotherapy Canada, 53 (2), pp.109-114. Levy, C. E., Nichols, D. S., Schmalbrock, P. M., Keller, P. and Chakeres, D. W. 2001. Functional MRI Evidence of Cortical Reorganization in Upper-Limb Stroke Hemiplegia Treated with Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 80 (1). pp 4-12 Lewinter, M. and Mikkelsen, S. 1995. Patients' experience of rehabilitation after stroke. Disability and Rehabilitation. 17 (1); pp 3 – 9. Lewsey, J. D., Gillies, M., Jhund, P. S., Chalmers, J. W. T., Redpath, A., Briggs, A., Walters, M., Langhorne, P., Capewell, S., McMurray, J. J. V. and MacIntyre, K. 2009. Sex Differences in Incidence, Mortality, and Survival in Individuals With Stroke in Scotland, 1986 to 2005. Stroke, 40 (4), pp.1038-1043. Liepert, J., Bauder, H., Miltner, W. H. R., Taub, E. and Weiller, C. 2000. Treatment-Induced Cortical Reorganization After Stroke in Humans. Stroke, 31 (6), pp.1210-1216. Lin, K., Fu, T., Wu, C., Wang, Y., Liu, J., Hsieh, C. and Lin, S. 2010a. Minimal detectable change and clinically important difference of the Stroke Impact Scale in stroke patients. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 24 (5), pp.486-492. Lin, K. C., Fu, T., Wu, C. Y., Hsieh, Y. W., Chen, C. L. and Lee, P. C. 2010b. Psychometric comparisons of the Stroke Impact Scale 3.0 and Stroke-Specific Quality of Life Scale. Quality of Life Research: An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care & Rehabilitation, 19 (3), pp.435-443. Lin, M., Hwang, H., Hu, M., Wu, H. I., Wang, Y. and Huang, F. 2004. Psychometric comparisons of the timed up and go, one-leg stand, functional reach, and Tinetti balance measures in community-dwelling older people. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 52 (8), pp.1343-1348. Lincoln, N. and Leadbitter, D. Assessment of Motor Function in Stroke Patients. Physiotherapy 65[2], 48-51. 1979. Lincoln, N. B., Gladman, J. R. F., Berman, P., Luther, A. and Challen, K. 1998. Rehabilitation needs of community stroke patients. Disability and Rehabilitation: An International, Multidisciplinary Journal, 20 (12), pp.457-463. Lloyd, G., Dean, C. M. and Ada, L. 2010. Issues in recruiting community-dwelling stroke survivors to clinical trials: the AMBULATE trial. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 31 (4), pp.289-292. Loewen, S. C. and Anderson, B. A. 1988. Reliability of the Modified Motor Assessment Scale and the Barthel Index. Physical Therapy, 68 (7), pp.1077-1081. Loewen, S. C. and Anderson, B. A. 1990. Predictors of stroke outcome using objective measurement scales. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 21 (1), pp.78-81. Lord, S. 2003. Physiotherapy for patients with mobility problems more than 1 year after stroke: a randomised controlled trial. New Zealand Journal of Physiotherapy, 31 (3), p.143. 337 Reference List Lord, S. E., McPherson, K., McNaughton, H. K., Rochester, L. and Weatherall, M. 2004. Community ambulation after stroke: how important and obtainable is it and what measures appear predictive? Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 85 (2), pp.234-239. Ma, H. and Trombly, C. A. 2001. The comparison of motor performance between part and whole tasks in elderly persons. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55 (1), pp.62-67. MacKay, J. and Mensah, G.A. 2004. Atlas of Heart Disease and Stroke. Geneva, World Health Organisation. Mackintosh, S. F., Hill, K. D., Dodd, K. J., Goldie, P. A. and Culham, E. G. 2006. Balance score and a history of falls in hospital predict recurrent falls in the 6 months following stroke rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87 (12), pp.1583-1589. Macko, R. F., Haeuber, E., Shaughnessy, M., Coleman, K. L., Boone, D. A., Smith, G. V. and Silver, K. H. 2002. Microprocessor-based ambulatory activity monitoring in stroke patients. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 34 (3), pp.394-399. Maddocks, M., Petrou, A., Skipper, L. and Wilcock, A. 2010. Validity of three accelerometers during treadmill walking and motor vehicle travel. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 44 (8), pp.606-608. Magill, R.A. 2007. Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Applications. London, McGraw Hill. Magill, R. A. and Hall, K. G. 1990. A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Human Movement Science, 9 (3-5), pp.241-289. Mahoney, F.I. Wood, O.H. and Barthel, D.W. 1958. Rehabilitation of Chronically Ill Patients: The Influence of Complications on the Final Goal. Southern Medical Journal, 51 (5). pp605609. Majsak, M. J. 1996. Application of motor learning principles to the stroke population. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 3 (2), pp.27-59. Malouin, F., Pichard, L., Bonneau, C., Durand, A. and Corriveau, D. 1994. Evaluating motor recovery early after stroke: comparison of the Fugl-Meyer Assessment and the Motor Assessment Scale. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 75 (11), pp.12061212 Margolis, J. F. and Christina, R. W. 1981. A test of Schmidt's schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 52 (4), pp.474-483. Marigold, D. S., Eng, J. J., Dawson, A. S., Inglis, J. T., Harris, J. E. and Gylfadottir, S. 2005. Exercise leads to faster postural reflexes, improved balance and mobility, and fewer falls in older persons with chronic stroke. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 53 (3), pp.416-423. Marley, T. L., Ezekiel, H. J., Lehto, N. K., Wishart, L. R. and Lee, T. D. 2000. Application of motor learning principles: the physiotherapy client as a problem-solver. II. Scheduling practice. Physiotherapy Canada, 52 (4), pp.315-320. 338 Reference List Martelli, M. F., Nicholson, K. and Zasler, N. D. 2008. Skill reacquisition after acquired brain injury: a holistic habit retraining model of neurorehabilitation. Neurorehabilitation, 23 (2), pp.115-126. Massie, C., Malcolm, M. P., Greene, D. and Thaut, M. 2009. The Effects of Constraint-Induced Therapy on Kinematic Outcomes and Compensatory Movement Patterns: An Exploratory Study. Archives Of Physical Medicine And Rehabilitation, 90 (4), pp.571-579 Mathiowetz, V., Weber, K., Kashman, N. and Volland, G. Adult norms for the Nine Hole Peg Test of finger dexterity. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research 5[1], 24-38. 1985. McClellan, R. and Ada, L. 2004. A six-week, resource-efficient mobility program after discharge from rehabilitation improves standing in people affected by stroke: placebo-controlled, randomised trial. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy, 50 (3), pp.163-167 McDowell, I. and Newell, C. Measuring Health. 1996. Oxford, Oxford University Press. McEwen, S. E., Huijbregts, M. P. J., Ryan, J. D. and Polatajko, H. J. 2009. Cognitive strategy use to enhance motor skill acquisition post-stroke: a critical review. Brain Injury, 23 (4), pp.263-277. McNevin, N. H., Wulf, G. and Carlson, C. 2000. Effects of attentional focus, self-control, and dyad training on motor learning: implications for physical rehabilitation. Physical Therapy, 80 (4), pp.373-385 Mead, G. E., Greig, C. A., Cunningham, I., Lewis, S. J., Dinan, S., Saunders, D. H., Fitzsimons, C. and Young, A. 2007. Stroke: a randomized trial of exercise or relaxation. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 55 (6), pp.892-899. Meijer, R., Ihnenfeldt, D. S., de Groot, I. J., van Limbeek, J., Vermeulen, M. and de Haan, R. J. 2003. Prognostic factors for ambulation and activities of daily living in the subacute phase after stroke. A systematic review of the literature. Clinical Rehabilitation, 17 (2), pp.119-129. Mercer, V. S., Freburger, J. K., Chang, S. and Purser, J. L. 2009. Measurement of paretic-lower-extremity loading and weight transfer after stroke. Physical Therapy, 89 (7), pp.653-664 Mercer, V. S., Freburger, J. K., Chang, S. H. and Purser, J. L. 2009. Step Test scores are related to measures of activity and participation in the first 6 months after stroke. Physical Therapy, 89 (10), pp.1061-1071 Michael, K. and Macko, R. F. 2007. Ambulatory activity intensity profiles, fitness, and fatigue in chronic stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 14 (2), pp.5-12. Michaelsen, S. M., Dannenbaum, R. and Levin, M. F. 2006. Task-Specific Training With Trunk Restraint on Arm Recovery in Stroke: Randomized Control Trial. Stroke, 37 (1), pp.186192. Mier, H., Temple, L.W., Perlmutter, J.S., Raichle, M.E. and Petersen, S.E. 1998. Changes in Brain Activity During Motor Learning Measured With PET: Effects of Hand of Performance and Practice. Journal of Neurophysiology. 80 (4); pp 2177-2199 339 Reference List Miller, K. J., Garland, S. J. and Koshland, G. F. 1998. Techniques and efficacy of physiotherapy poststroke. Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 12 (3), pp.473-487. Miltner, W. H. R., Bauder, H., Sommer, M., Dettmers, C. and Taub, E. 1999. Effects of Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy on Patients With Chronic Motor Deficits After Stroke : A Replication. Stroke, 30 (3), pp.586-592. Monger, C., Carr, J. H. and Fowler, V. 2002. Evaluation of a home-based exercise and training programme to improve sit-to-stand in patients with chronic stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 16 (4), pp.361-367. Morioka, S. and Yagi, F. 2003. Effects of perceptual learning exercises on standing balance using a hardness discrimination task in hemiplegic patients following stroke: A randomized controlled pilot trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 17 (6), pp.600-607. Morris, J. H. and Williams, B. 2009. Optimising long-term participation in physical activities after stroke: Exploring new ways of working for physiotherapists. Physiotherapy, 95 (3), pp.227-233. Morrison, V., Pollard, B., Johnston, M. and MacWalter, R. 2005. Anxiety and depression 3 years following stroke: Demographic, clinical, and psychological predictors. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research, 59 (4), pp.209-213. Mudge S, Barber PA, Stott NS. 2009. Circuit-based rehabilitation improves gait endurance but not usual walking activity in chronic stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil; 90:1989-1996. Mulder AHM, De Witte LP, Diederick JPM. 1989. Evaluation of a rehabilitation after-care programme for stroke patients. Journal of Rehabilitation Sciences; 2(4):97–103. Murray, J., Young, J. and Forster, A. 2009. Measuring outcomes in the longer term after a stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 23 (10), pp.918-921. Murray, J., Young, J., Forster, A. and Ashworth, R. 2003. Developing a primary care-based stroke model: the prevalence of longer-term problems experienced by patients and carers. The British Journal Of General Practice: The Journal Of The Royal College Of General Practitioners, 53 (495), pp.803-807. Nakayama H, Jørgensen H.S., Raaschou, H.O., and Olsen, T.S. 1994. Recovery of upper extremity function in stroke patients: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 75(4) pp 394-398 National Audit Office. 2005. Reducing brain damage. Faster access to better stroke care. London, Department of Health. Newell, K. M. 2003. Schema theory (1975): retrospectives and prospectives. Research Quarterly For Exercise And Sport, 74 (4), pp.383-388. Ng, S. S. and Hui-Chan, C. W. 2005. The Timed Up & Go test: its reliability and association with lower-limb impairments and locomotor capacities in people with chronic stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 86 (8), pp.1641-1647. 340 Reference List Ngan-Hing, L. and Stretton, C. 2006. Does rehabilitation improve gait and quality of life for chronic stroke survivors? New Zealand Journal of Physiotherapy, 34 (3), pp.147-152. Niemi, M. L., Laaksonen, R., Kotila, M. and Waltimo, O. 1988. Quality of life 4 years after stroke. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 19 (9), pp.1101-1107. Nikolaus, T., Bach, M., Oster, P. and Schlierf. Prospective value of self-report and performancebased tests of functional status for 18-month outcomes in elderly patients. Aging (Milan, Italy) 8[4], 271-276. 1996. Nudo, R. J. 2003. Functional and structural plasticity in motor cortex: implications for stroke recovery. Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation Clinics of North America, 14 (1S), p.S57S76. Nudo, R. J. 2006. Mechanisms for recovery of motor function following cortical damage. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16 (6), pp.638-644. Nudo, R. J. 2007. The role of skill versus use in the recovery of motor function after stroke. OTJR: Occupation, Participation & Health, 27 pp.24S-32s. Nudo, R. J., Milliken, G. W., Jenkins, W. M. and Merzenich, M. M. 1996. Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 16 (2), pp.785-807. Nys, G. M. S., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., de Kort, P. L. M., Jansen, B. P. W., Kappelle, L. J. and de Haan, E. H. F. 2005. Restrictions of the Mini-Mental State Examination in acute stroke. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20 (5), pp.623-629. O'Connor, R. J., Cano, S. J., Thompson, A. J. and Hobart, J. C. 2004. Exploring rating scale responsiveness: does the total score reflect the sum of its parts? Neurology, 62 (10), pp.1842-1844. Oberg, T., Karsznia, A. and Oberg, K. 1993. Basic gait parameters: normal reference data for normal subjects 10 – 79 years of age. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development. 30 (2); pp 210 – 223 Oliveira, F. T. P. and Goodman, D. 2004. Conscious and effortful or effortless and automatic: A practice/performance paradox in motor learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99 (1), pp.315-324. Olney, S. J. and Richards, C. 1996. Hemiparetic gait following stroke. Part I: Characteristics. Gait & Posture, 4 (2), pp.136-148. Olney, S. J., Griffin, M. P. and McBride, I. D. 1994. Temporal, kinematic, and kinetic variables related to gait speed in subjects with hemiplegia: a regression approach. Physical Therapy, 74 (9), pp.872-885. Olney, S. J., Nymark, J., Brouwer, B., Culham, E., Day, A., Heard, J., Henderson, M. and Parvataneni, K. 2006. A Randomized Controlled Trial of Supervised Versus Unsupervised Exercise Programs for Ambulatory Stroke Survivors. Stroke, 37 (2), pp.476-481. 341 Reference List Orrell, A. J., Eves, F. F. and Masters, R. S. W. 2006. Motor learning of a dynamic balancing task after stroke: implicit implications for stroke rehabilitation. Physical Therapy, 86 (3), pp.369-380. Ouellette, M.M., LeBrasseur, N.K., Bean, J.F., Phillips, E., Stein, J., Frontera, W.R., and Fielding, R.A., 2004; High-Intensity Resistance Training Improves Muscle Strength, Self-Reported Function, and Disability in Long-Term Stroke Survivors Stroke. 35:pp 1404 – 1409. Outpatient Service Trialists 2004. Rehabilitation therapy services for stroke patients living at home: systematic review of randomised trials. Lancet, 363 (9406), pp.352-356. Page, S. J. 2001. Mental practice: a promising restorative technique in stroke rehabilitation. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 8 (3), pp.54-63 Page, S. J. 2003. Intensity versus task-specificity after stroke: how important is intensity? American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 82 (9), pp.730-732. Page, S. J., Gater, D. R. and Rita, P. 2004. Reconsidering the motor recovery plateau in stroke rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 85 (8), pp.1377-1381. Pang, M. Y., Harris, J. E. and Eng, J. J. 2006. A community-based upper-extremity group exercise program improves motor function and performance of functional activities in chronic stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87 (1), pp.1-9. Park, J. H., Wilde, H. and SHEA, C. H. 2004. Part-whole practice of movement sequences. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36 (1), pp.51-61. Parker, V. M., Wade, D. T. and Hewer, R. L. 1986. Loss of arm function after stroke: measurement, frequency, and recovery. Disability & Rehabilitation, 8 (2), pp.69-73. Partridge, C. J., Morris, L. W. and Edwards, M. S. 1993. Recovery from physical disability after stroke: profiles for different levels of starting severity. Clinical Rehabilitation, 7 (3), pp.210-217. Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J. and Hallett, M. 1994. Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science, 263 (5151), pp.12871289. Patel, M. D., McKevitt, C., Lawrence, E., Rudd, A. G. and Wolfe, C. D. A. 2007. Clinical determinants of long-term quality of life after stroke. Age and Ageing, 36 (3), pp.316322. Pedersen, P. M., Jorgensen, H. S., Nakayama, H., Raaschou, H. O. and Olsen, T. S. 1996a. Orientation in the acute and chronic stroke patient: impact on ADL and social activities. The Copenhagen Stroke Study. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 77 (4), pp.336-339 Pedersen, P. M., Jorgensen, H. S., Nakayama, H., Raaschou, H. O. and Olsen, T. S. 1996b. General cognitive function in acute stroke: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Journal of Neurologic Rehabilitation, 10 (3), pp.153-158. 342 Reference List Perell, K. L., Nelson, A., Goldman, R. L., Luther, S. L., Prieto-Lewis, N. and Rubenstein, L. Z. 2001. Fall Risk Assessment Measures. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56 (12), p.M761-M766. Perry, S. B. 2007. Constraints on practice. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 31 (1), pp.10-11. Piercy, M., Carter, J., Mant, J. and Wade, D. T. 2000. Inter-rater reliability of the Frenchay Activities Index in patients with stroke and their carers. Clinical Rehabilitation, 14 (4), pp.433-440. Piron, L., Turolla, A., Agostini, M., Zucconi, C. S., Ventura, L., Tonin, P. and Dam, M. 2010. Motor learning principles for rehabilitation: a pilot randomized controlled study in poststroke patients. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 24 (6), pp.501-508. Platz, T., Denzler, P., Kaden, B. and Mauritz, K. H. 1994. Motor learning after recovery from hemiparesis. Neuropsychologia, 32 (10), pp.1209-1223. Plautz, E. J., Milliken, G. W. and Nudo, R. J. 2000. Effects of Repetitive Motor Training on Movement Representations in Adult Squirrel Monkeys: Role of Use versus Learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 74 (1), pp.27-55. Ploughman, M. 2002. A review of brain neuroplasticity and implications for the physiotherapeutic management of stroke. Physiotherapy Canada, 54 (3), pp.164-176. Podsiadlo, D. and Richardson, S. The timed "Up & Go": a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. Journal of American Geriatric Society 39[2], 142-148. Pohl, P. S., McDowd, J. M., Filion, D., Richards, L. G. and Stiers, W. 2006. Implicit learning of a motor skill after mild and moderate stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 20 (3), pp.246-253. Pollock, A., Baer, G., Langhorne, P. and Pomeroy, V. 2007. Physiotherapy treatment approaches for the recovery of postural control and lower limb function following stroke: a systematic review. Clinical Rehabilitation, 21 (5), pp.395-410. Pollock, A. S., Durward, B. R., Rowe, P. J. and Paul, J. P. 2002. The effect of independent practice of motor tasks by stroke patients: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 16 (5), pp.473-480. Pomeroy, V. M. and Tallis, R. C. 2002. Restoring Movement and Functional Ability after Stroke: Now and the future. Physiotherapy 88[1], 3-17. Poole, J. L. and Whitney, S. L. 1988. Motor Assessment Scale for stroke patients: concurrent validity and interrater reliability... part 1. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 69 (3), pp.195-197. Pound, P., Gompertz, P. and Ebrahim, S. 1994. Patients' satisfaction with stroke services. Clinical Rehabilitation, 8 (1), pp.7-17. Pound, P., Bury, M., Gompertz, P. and Ebrahim, S. 1995. Stroke patients' views on their admission to hospital. BMJ, 311 (6996), pp.18-22. 343 Reference List Refshauge, K., Ada, L. and Ellis, E. 2005. Science-based rehabilitation : theories into practice. Edinburgh, Butterworth Heinemann. Richards, C. L., Malouin, F., Wood-Dauphinee, S., Williams, J. I., Bouchard, J. and Brunet, D. 1993. Task-specific physical therapy for optimization of gait recovery in acute stroke patients. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 74 (6), pp.612-620. Rockwood K, Awalt, E., Carver, D. and MacKnight, C. 2000. Feasibility and measurement properties of the functional reach and thetimed up and go tests in the Canadian study of health and aging. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences , 55 (2), p.M70-M73 Rosenkranz, K., Kacar, A. and Rothwell, J. C. 2007. Differential modulation of motor cortical plasticity and excitability in early and late phases of human motor learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27 (44), pp.12058-12066. Rossini, PM. and Forno, GD. Neuronal post-stroke plasticity. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 22[3-5], 193-2006. 2004. Rothwell, P. M., Coull, A. J., Giles, M. F., Howard, S. C., Silver, L. E., Bull, L. M., Gutnikov, S. A., Edwards, P., Mant, D., Sackley, C. M., Farmer, A., Sandercock, P. A. G., Dennis, M. S., Warlow, C. P., Bamford, J. M. and Anslow, P. 2004. Change in stroke incidence, mortality, case-fatality, severity, and risk factors in Oxfordshire, UK from 1981 to 2004 (Oxford Vascular Study). The Lancet, 363 (9425), pp.1925-1933. Rowe, PJ. 1999. Measurement Systems in Durward, B., Baer G D & Rowe P. 1999. Functional Human Movement: Measurement and Analysis. Butterworth Heinemann, Oxford. Rowat, A., Wardlaw, J., Dennis, M. S. and Warlow, C. P. Patient positioning influences oxygen saturation in the acute phase of stroke. Cerebrovascular Diseases 12[1], 66-72. 2001. Royal College of Physicians. National Clinical Guidelines for Stroke (Third edition). London, Royal College of Physicians. 2008. Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists 2009. RCSLT Resource Manual for Commissioning and Planning Services for SLCN: Dysphagia. London, RCSLT. Ryan, CG., Grant, PM., Tigbe, WW., and Granat, MH. 2006. The validity and reliability of a novel activity monitor as a measure of walking. Brit J Sports Med 40 (9): 779 – 784 Ryan, T., Enderby, P. and Rigby, A. S. 2006. A randomized controlled trial to evaluate intensity of community-based rehabilitation provision following stroke or hip fracture in old age. Clinical Rehabilitation, 20 (2), pp.123-131. Sabari, J. S., Lim, A. L., Velozo, C. A., Lehman, L., Kieran, O. and Lai, J. 2005. Assessing arm and hand function after stroke: a validity test of the hierarchical scoring system used in the motor assessment scale for stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 86 (8), pp.1609-1615. Sackley, C., Wade, DT., Mant, D., Atkinson, JC., Yudkin, P., Cardoso, K. et al 2006. Cluster randomised pilot controlled trial of an occupational therapy intervention for residents with stroke in UK care homes. Stroke 37: 2336 – 2341. 344 Reference List Saladin, L. K. 2000. Measuring quality of life post-stroke. Neurology Report, 24 (4), pp.133-139. Salbach, N. M., Mayo, N. E., Wood-Dauphinee, S., Hanley, J. A., Richards, C. L. and Coté R. 2004. A task-orientated intervention enhances walking distance and speed in the first year post stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18 (5), pp.509519. Salter, K. L., Moses, M. B., Foley, N. C. and Teasell, R. W. 2008. Health-related quality of life after stroke: what are we measuring? International Journal Of Rehabilitation Research 31 (2), pp.111-117 Saunders DH, Greig CA, Young A, Mead GE. 2004. Physical fitness training for stroke patients. Cochrane Database Systematic Reviews. Issue 1:CD003316. Schaechter, J. D. 2004. Motor rehabilitation and brain plasticity after hemiparetic stroke. Progress In Neurobiology, 73 (1), pp.61-72. Schallert, T., Fleming, S. M. and Woodlee, M. T. 2003. Should the injured and intact hemispheres be treated differently during the early phases of physical restorative therapy in experimental stroke or parkinsonism? Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation Clinics of North America, 14 (1S), p.S27-S46. Schmidt, R.A. 1975. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review. 82; pp 225 – 260. Schmidt, R. A. and Lee, T. D. 2005. Motor Control and Learning: a behavioural emphasis. [4th edition]. Champaign, IL: USA, Human Kinetics. Schmidt, R. A. and Wrisberg, C. A. 2008. Motor Learning and Performance: a situation-based learning approach. [4th edition]. Champaign, IL: USA, Human Kinetics. Schreiber, J., Sober, L., Banta, L., Glassbrenner, L., Haman, J., Mistry, N. and Olesinski, K. 2000. Application of motor learning principles with stroke survivors. Occupational Therapy in Health Care, 13 (1), pp.23-44. Schubring-Giese, M., Molina-Luna, K., Hertler, B., Buitrago, M. M., Hanley, D. F. and Luft, A. R. 2007. Speed of motor re-learning after experimental stroke depends on prior skill. Experimental Brain Research, 181 (2), pp.359-365. Schuling, J., de Haan, R., Limburg, M. and Groenier, K. H. 1993. The Frenchay Activities Index. Assessment of functional status in stroke patients. Stroke, 24 (8), pp.1173-1177 Shea, J. B. and Morgan, R. L. 1979. Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5 (2), pp.179-187. Shea, C. H. and Kohl, R. M. 1990. Specificity and variability of practice. Research Quarterly For Exercise And Sport, 61 (2), pp.169-177. Shea, C.H., Lai, Q., Black, C. and Park, J. 2000. Spacing practice sessions across days benefits the learning of motor skills. Human Movement Science. 19 (5); pp 737-760 345 Reference List Shea, C. H., Lai, Q., Wright, D. L., Immink, M. and Black, C. 2001. Consistent and variable practice conditions: Effects on relative and absolute timing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33 (2), pp.139-152 Shea, C. H. and Wulf, G. 2005. Schema Theory: A Critical Appraisal and Reevaluation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37 (2), pp.85-101 Shepherd, R. B. 2001. Exercise and training to optimize functional motor performance in stroke: driving neural reorganization? Neural Plasticity, 8 (1-2), pp.121-129. Sherwood, D. E. and Lee, T. D. 2003. Schema theory: critical review and implications for the role of cognition in a new theory of motor learning. Research Quarterly For Exercise And Sport, 74 (4), pp.376-382 Shumway-Cook, A. and Woollacott, M. 2007. Motor Control. Translating Research Into Practice. 3rd edition. USA: Philadelphia, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Shumway-Cook, A., Baldwin, M., Polissar, N. L. and Gruber, W. 1997. Predicting the Probability for Falls in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Physical Therapy, 77 (8), pp.812-819. Shumway-Cook, A., Brauer, S. and Woollacott, M. 2000. Predicting the Probability for Falls in Community-Dwelling Older Adults Using the Timed Up & Go Test. Physical Therapy, 80 (9), pp.896-903 Sidaway, B., Ahn, S., Boldeau, P., Griffin, S., Noyes, B. and Pelletier, K. 2008. A Comparison of Manual Guidance and Knowledge of Results in the Learning of a Weight-bearing Skill. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 32 (1).pp32-38. Siegel, A. and Sapru, HN. Essential Neuroscience. 2005. Philadelphia, Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. Simondson, J. A., Goldie, P. and Greenwood, K. M. 2003. The Mobility Scale for Acute Stroke Patients: concurrent validity. Clinical Rehabilitation, 17 (5), pp.558-564. Smith, G. V., Forrester, L. W., Silver, K. H. C. and Macko, R. F. 2000. Effects of treadmill training on translational balance perturbation responses in chronic hemiparetic stroke patients. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases, 9 (5), pp.238-245. Smith, J., Forster, A. and Young, J. 2004. A randomized trial to evaluate an education programme for patients and carers after stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18 (7), pp.726736 Smith, P. S., Hembree, J. A. and Thompson, M. E. 2004. Berg Balance Scale and Functional Reach: determining the best clinical tool for individuals post acute stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18 (7), pp.811-818. Snaith, R. P. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. 2003. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 1[1], 29. [online] Available from http://www.hqlo.com/content/1/1/29 Srikanth, V., Thrift, A. G., Fryer, J. L., Saling, M. M., Dewey, H. M., Sturm, J. W. and Donnan, G. A. 2006. The validity of brief screening cognitive instruments in the diagnosis of 346 Reference List cognitive impairment and dementia after first-ever stroke. International Psychogeriatrics / IPA, 18 (2), pp.295-305. Stammers, R. B. 1980. Part and whole practice for a tracking task: effects of task variables and amount of practice. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50 (1), pp.203-210. Staub, F. and Bogousslavsky, J. 2001. Fatigue after stroke: a major but neglected issue. Cerebrovascular Diseases, 12 (2), pp.75-81. Stefan, K., Cohen, L. G., Duque, J., Mazzocchio, R., Celnik, P., Sawaki, L., Ungerleider, L. and Classen, J. 2005. Formation of a motor memory by action observation. The Journal Of Neuroscience: 25 (41), pp.9339-9346. Steffen, T. M., Hacker, T. A. and Mollinger, L. 2002. Age- and Gender-Related Test Performance in Community-Dwelling Elderly People: Six-Minute Walk Test, Berg Balance Scale, Timed Up & Go Test, and Gait Speeds. Physical Therapy, 82 (2), pp.128-137. Sterr, A. 2004. Training-based interventions in motor rehabilitation after stroke: theoretical and clinical considerations. Behavioural Neurology, 15 (3-4), pp.55-63. Stokes, M. Physical Management in Neurological Rehabilitation. 2nd edition. 2004. Edinburgh, Mosby. Stuart, M., Benvenuti, F., Macko, R., Taviani, A., Segenni, L., Mayer, F., Sorkin, J. D., Stanhope, S. J., Macellari, V. and Weinrich, M. 2009. Community-Based Adaptive Physical Activity Program for Chronic Stroke: Feasibility, Safety, and Efficacy of the Empoli Model. Neurorehabilitation And Neural Repair, 23 (7), pp.726-734. Sue-Min, L., Studenski, S., Richards, L., Perera, S., Reker, D., Rigler, S. and Duncan, P. W. 2006. Therapeutic exercise and depressive symptoms after stroke. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 54 (2), pp.240-247. Sullivan, K. J., Knowlton, B. J. and Dobkin, B. H. 2002. Step training with body weight support: Effect of treadmill speed and practice paradigms on poststroke locomotor recovery. Archives Of Physical Medicine And Rehabilitation, 83 (5), pp.683-691. Sullivan, K. J., Brown, D. A., Klassen, T., Mulroy, S., Ge, T., Azen, S. P., Winstein, C. J. and for the Physical Therapy Clinical Research Network (PTClinResNet) 2007. Effects of TaskSpecific Locomotor and Strength Training in Adults Who Were Ambulatory After Stroke: Results of the STEPS Randomized Clinical Trial. Physical Therapy, 87 (12), pp.15801602. Syme, P. D., Byrne, A. W., Chen, R., Devenny, R. and Forbes, J. F. 2005. Community-Based Stroke Incidence in a Scottish Population: The Scottish Borders Stroke Study. Stroke, 36 (9), pp.1837-1843. Tangeman, P. T., Banaitis, D. A. and Williams, A. K. 1990. Rehabilitation of chronic stroke patients: changes in functional performance. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 71 (11), pp.876-880. 347 Reference List Taub, E., Crago, J.E., Burgio, L.D., Groomes, T.E., Cook, E.W., DeLuca, S.C. and Miller, N.E. 1994. An operant approach to rehabilitation medicine: overcoming learned nonuse by shaping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour. 61(2): 281–293. Taub, E., Uswatte, G., van der Lee, J. H., Lankhorst, G. J., Bouter, L. M. and Wagenaar, R. C. 2000. Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy and Massed Practice ò Response. Stroke, 31 (4), pp.983-98c. Taylor, N. F., Dodd, K. J., Shields, N. and Bruder, A. 2007. Therapeutic exercise in physiotherapy practice is beneficial: a summary of systematic reviews 2002-2005. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy, 53 (1), pp.7-16. Teasell, R. W. and Kalra, L. 2004. What's New in Stroke Rehabilitation. Stroke, 35 (2), pp.383385. Tennant, A., Geddes, J. M. L. and Chamberlain, M. A. 1996. The Barthel Index: an ordinal score or interval level measure? Clinical Rehabilitation, 10 (4), pp.301-308. Texeira-Salmela, LF., Olney, S. J. and Nadeau, S. Muscle strengthening and physical conditioning to Reduce Impairment and Disability in Chronic Stroke Survivors. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation 80[10], 1211-1218. 1999. The Stroke Association. 2010. Moving On. A vision for community based physiotherapy after stroke in England. London, The Stroke Association. Thielman, G. T., Dean, C. M. and Gentile, A. M. 2004. Rehabilitation of reaching after stroke: task-related training versus progressive resistive exercise. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 85 (10), pp.1613-1618. Thommessen, B., Bautz-Holter, E. and Laake, K. 1999. Predictors of outcome of rehabilitation of elderly stroke patients in a geriatric ward. Clinical Rehabilitation. 13 (2); pp 123-128 Thorson, A. M., Holmqvist, L. W. s., de Pedro-Cuesta, J. S. and von Koch, L. 2005. A randomized controlled trial of early supported discharge and continued rehabilitation at home after stroke: five-year follow-up of patient outcome. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 36 (2), pp.297-303 Thorvaldsen, P., Asplund, K., Kuulasmaa, K., Rajakangas, A. M. and Schroll, M. 1995. Stroke Incidence, Case Fatality, and Mortality in the WHO MONICA Project. Stroke, 26 (3), pp.361-367. Timbeck, R. J. and Spaulding, S. J. 2004. Ability of the Functional Independence Measure to Predict Rehabilitation Outcomes After Stroke: A Review of the Literature. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics, 22 (1), pp.63-76. Tinetti, ME. 1986. Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly patients. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society 34[2], 119-126. Tinson, D.J. 1989. How stroke patients spend their days: an observational study of the treatment regime offered to patients in hospital with movement disorders following stroke. International Disability Studies. 11(1); pp 45-49. 348 Reference List Tombaugh, T. N. and McIntyre, N. J. 1992. The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive review. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 40 (9), pp.922-935. Tremblay, L., Welsh, T. N. and Elliott, D. 2001. Specificity versus variability: effects of practice conditions on the use of afferent information for manual aiming. Motor Control, 5 (4), pp.347-360 Truelsen, T., Piechowski-J+¦+¦wiak, B., Bonita, R., Mathers, C., Bogousslavsky, J. and Boysen, G. 2006. Stroke incidence and prevalence in Europe: a review of available data. European Journal of Neurology, 13 (6), pp.581-598. Tsavourelou, A., Rowe, P., Babatsikou, F. and Koutis, C. 2009. Validation of the ACTIVPAL in the health promotion context. Health Science Journal, 3 (2), pp.105-114. Tudor-Locke, C., Ainsworth, B. E., Thompson, R. W. and Matthews, C. E. 2002. Comparison of pedometer and accelerometer measures of free-living physical activity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 34 (12), pp.2045-2051. Tung, F., Yang, Y., Lee, C. and Wang, R. 2010. Balance outcomes after additional sit-to-stand training in subjects with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 24 (6), pp.533-542. Turner-Stokes, L. and Turner-Stokes, T. 1997. The use of standardized outcome measures in rehabilitation centres in the UK. Clinical Rehabilitation, 11 (4), pp.306-313. Turton, A., Wroe, S., Trepte, N., Fraser, C. and Lemon, R. N. 1996. Contralateral and ipsilateral EMG responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation during recovery of arm and hand function after stroke. 101[4], 316-328. Tyson, S. F. 2007. Measurement error in functional balance and mobility tests for people with stroke: what are the sources of error and what is the best way to minimize error? Neurorehabilitation And Neural Repair, 21 (1), pp.46-50. Tyson, S. F. and Connell, L. A. 2009. How to measure balance in clinical practice. A systematic review of the psychometrics and clinical utility of measures of balance activity for neurological conditions. Clinical Rehabilitation, 23 (9), pp.824-840. Tyson, S. and Turner, G. 2000. Discharge and follow-up for people with stroke: what happens and why. Clinical Rehabilitation, 14 (4), pp.381-392. Tyson, S. F., Hanley, M., Chillala, J., Selley, A. and Tallis, R. C. 2006. Balance Disability After Stroke. Physical Therapy, 86 (1), pp.30-38. Ungerleider, L. G., Doyon, J. and Karni, A. 2002. Imaging Brain Plasticity during Motor Skill Learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78 (3), pp.553-564. van Baalen, B., Odding, E., van Woensel, M. P. C., van Kessel, M. A., Roebroeck, M. E. and Stam, H. J. 2006. Reliability and sensitivity to change of measurement instruments used in a traumatic brain injury population. Clinical Rehabilitation, 20 (8), pp.686-700. van der Putten, J. J. M. F., Hobart, J. C., Freeman, J. A. and Thompson, A. J. 1999. Measuring change in disability after inpatient rehabilitation: comparison of the responsiveness of 349 Reference List the Barthel Index and the Functional Independence Measure. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 66 (4), pp.480-484. van Dijk, H., Jannink, M. J. A. and Hermens, H. J. 2005. Effect of augmented feedback on motor function of the affected upper extremity in rehabilitation patients: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Journal Of Rehabilitation Medicinee, 37 (4), pp.202-211 van Mier, H., Tempel, L. W., Perlmutter, J. S., Raichle, M. E. and Petersen, S. E. 1998. Changes in brain activity during motor learning measured with PET: Effects of hand of performance and practice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80 (4), pp.2177-2199. Van Rossum, J. H. 1990. Schmidt's schema theory: The empirical base of the variability of practice hypothesis: A critical analysis. Human Movement Science, 9 (3-5), pp.387-435. van Vliet, P. M. and Wulf, G. 2006. Extrinsic feedback for motor learning after stroke: what is the evidence? Disability & Rehabilitation, 28 (13-14), pp.831-840. van Vliet, P. M., Lincoln, N. B. and Foxall, A. 2005. Comparison of Bobath based and movement science based treatment for stroke: a randomised controlled trial. Journal Of Neurology, Neurosurgery, And Psychiatry, 76 (4), pp.503-508. Wade, D. T. 1989. Measuring arm impairment and disability after stroke. Disability & Rehabilitation, 11 (2), pp.89-92. Wade DT. Measurement in Neurological Rehabilitation. 1992. Oxford, Oxford University Press. Wade, D. T. 1999. Randomized controlled trials -- a gold standard? Clinical Rehabilitation, 13 (6), pp.453-455 Wade, D. T. 2003. Community rehabilitation, or rehabilitation in the community? Disability And Rehabilitation, 25 (15), pp.875-881 Wade, D. T. and Collin, C. 1988. The Barthel ADL Index: A standard measure of physical disability? Disability & Rehabilitation, 10 (2), pp.64-67. Wade, D. T. and Hewer, R. L. 1987. Functional abilities after stroke: measurement, natural history and prognosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 50 (2), pp.177182. Wade, D. T., Legh-Smith, J. and Hewer, R. L. 1985. Social activities after stroke: Measurement and natural history using the Frenchay Activities Index. Disability & Rehabilitation, 7 (4), pp.176-181. Wade, D. T., Wood, V. A. and Hewer, R. L. 1985. Recovery after stroke--the first 3 months. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 48 (1), pp.7-13. Wade, D. T., Collen, F. M., Robb, G. F. and Warlow, C. P. 1992. Physiotherapy intervention late after stroke and mobility. British Medical Journal, 304 (6827), pp.609-613. Wade, DT., Langton-Hewer, R., Wood, VA., Skilbeck, CE., and Ismail, HM. 1983 The hemiplegic arm after stroke: measurement and recovery. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 46 (6): 521-524 350 Reference List Wall, J. C. and Turnbull, G. 1987. Evaluation of Out-Patient Physiotherapy and a Home Exercise Program in the Management of Gait Assymetry in Residual Stroke. Journal of Neurologic Rehabilitation 1[3], 115-123. Wall, J. C., Bell, C., Campbell, S. and Davis, J. 2000.The Timed Get-up-and-go Test Revisited: Measurement of the Component Tasks. Journal Of Rehabilitation Research And Development 37[1], 109-114. Wang, R., Chen, H., Chen, C. and Yang, Y. 2005. Efficacy of Bobath versus orthopaedic approach on impairment and function at different motor recovery stages after stroke: a randomized controlled study. Clinical Rehabilitation, 19 (2), pp.155-164. Ward, N. S. 2005. Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 81 (958), pp.510-514. Warlow, C. P. 1998. Epidemiology of stroke. Lancet 352[Suppl III], 1-4. Weiner, D. K., Duncan, P. W., Chandler, J. and Studenski, S. A. 1992. Functional reach: a marker of physical frailty. Journal Of The American Geriatrics Society, 40 (3), pp.203207. Werner, RA., and Kessler, S. 1996. Effectiveness of an effective outpatient rehabilitation program for post acute stroke patients. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1996;75(2):114-20. White, J. H., Alston, M. K., Marquez, J. L., Sweetapple, A. L., Pollack, M. R., Attia, J., Levi, C. R., Sturm, J. and Whyte, S. 2007. Community-dwelling stroke survivors: function is not the whole story with quality of life. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation , 88 (9), pp.1140-1146. Widar, M., Ahlström, G. and Ek, A. 2004. Health-related quality of life in persons with long-term pain after a stroke. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 13 (4), pp.497-505. 4. Wilde, H., Magnuson, C. and Shea, C.H. 2005. Random and Blocked Practice of Movement Sequences: Differential Effects on Response Structure and Movement Speed. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 76 (4); pp. 416-425. 5. Williams, L. S., Weingerger, M., Harris, L. E. and Biller, J. 1999. Measuring quality of life in a way that is meaningful to stroke patients. Neurology, 53 (8), pp.1839-1943. Williams, L. S., Weinberger, M., Harris, L. E., Clark, D. O. and Biller, J. 1999. Development of a stroke-specific quality of life scale. Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 30 (7), pp.1362-1369. Williams, L. S., Bakas, T., Brizendine, E., Plue, L., Tu, W., Hendrie, H. and Kroenke, K. 2006. How valid are family proxy assessments of stroke patients' health-related quality of life? Stroke; A Journal Of Cerebral Circulation, 37 (8), pp.2081-2085 Winstein, C. J. and Knecht, H. G. 1990. Movement science and its relevance to physical therapy. Physical Therapy, 70 (12), pp.759-762. 351 Reference List Winstein, C. J., Pohl, P. S. and Lewthwaite, R. 1994. Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. Research Quarterly For Exercise And Sport, 65 (4), pp.316-323. Winstein, C. J., Pohl, P. S., Cardinale, C., Green, A., Scholtz, L. and Waters, C. S. 1996. Learning a partial-weight-bearing skill: effectiveness of two forms of feedback [corrected] [published erratum appears in PHYS THER 1997 Mar; 77(3): 315]. Physical Therapy, 76 (9), pp.985-993. Winstein, C. J., Merians, A. S. and Sullivan, K. J. 1999. Motor learning after unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia, 37 (8), pp.975-987. Winstein, C. J. and Schmidt, R. A. 1990. Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16 (4), pp.677-691. Winstein, C. J. 1991. Knowledge of Results and Motor Learning Implications for Physical Wishart, L. R., Lee, T. D., Ezekiel, H. J., Marley, T. L. and Lehto, N. K. 2000. Application of motor learning principles: the physiotherapy client as a problem-solver. 1. Concepts. Physiotherapy Canada, 52 (3), pp.229-232. Wolf, S.L., Lecraw, D.E., Barton, L.A. and Jann, B.B. 1989. Forced use of hemiplegic upper extremities to reverse the effect of learned non-use among chronic stroke and headinjured patients. Experimental Neurology. 104 (2): pp 125-132. Wolf, S. L., Blanton, S., Baer, H., Breshears, J. and Butler, A. J. 2002. Repetitive Task Practice: A Critical Review of Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy in Stroke. The Neurologist, 8 (6). Wolf, S. L., Winstein, C. J., Miller, J. P., Taub, E., Uswatte, G., Morris, D., Giuliani, C., Light, K. E., Nichols-Larsen, D. and for the EXCITE Investigators 2006. Effect of ConstraintInduced Movement Therapy on Upper Extremity Function 3 to 9 Months After Stroke. JAMA: The Journal Of The American Medical Association, 296 (17), pp.2095-2104. Wolfe, C. D., Taub, N. A., Woodrow, J., Richardson, E., Warburton, F. G. and Burney, P. G. 1993. Does the incidence, severity, or case fatality of stroke vary in southern England? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 47 (2), pp.139-143. Wolfe, C. D., Tilling, K. and Rudd, A. G. 2000. The effectiveness of community-based rehabilitation for stroke patients who remain at home: a pilot randomized trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 14 (6), pp.563-569. Wolfe, C. D. A., Giroud, M., Kolominsky-Rabas, P., Dundas, R., Lemesle, M., Heuschmann, P. and Rudd, A. 2000. Variations in Stroke Incidence and Survival in 3 Areas of Europe. Stroke, 31 (9), pp.2074-2079. Wolpaw, J. R. 1994. Acquisition and maintenance of the simplest motor skill: Investigation of CNS mechanisms. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 26 (12), pp.1475-1479. Wulf, G. 2007. Self-controlled practice enhances motor learning: implications for physiotherapy. Physiotherapy, 93 (2), pp.96-101. 352 Reference List Wulf, G. and Lee, T. D. 1993. Contextual interference in movements of the same class: Differential effects on program and parameter learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 25 (4), pp.254-263. Wulf, G. and Schmidt, R. A. 1988. Variability in practice: facilitation in retention and transfer through schema formation or context effects? Journal of Motor Behavior, 20 (2), pp.133149. Wulf, G. and Shea, C. H. 2002. Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9 (2), pp.185-211. Wulf, G. and Weigelt, C. 1997. Instructions about Physical principles in learning a complex motor skill: to tell or not to tell. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 68 (4): 362367. Wulf, G., Shea, C. and Lewthwaite, R. 2010. Motor skill learning and performance: a review of influential factors. Medical Education, 44 (1), pp.75-84. Wulf, G., Weigelt, M., Poulter, D. and McNevin, N. 2003. Attentional focus on suprapostural tasks affects balance learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 56A (7), pp.1191-1211 Wyller, T. B., Sveen, U. and Bautz-Holter, E. 1995. The Barthel ADL Index One Year after Stroke: Comparison between Relatives' and Occupational Therapist's Scores. Age and Ageing, 24 (5), pp.398-401. Wyller, T. B., Sveen, U. and Bautz-Holter, E. 1996. The Frenchay Activities Index in stroke patients : agreement between scores by patients and by relatives. Disability & Rehabilitation, 18 (9), pp.454-459. Yang, YR., Wang, RY., Chen, YC., and Kao MJ. 2007 Dual-task exercises improves walking ability in chronic stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 88 (10): 1236 – 1240 Yang YR, Wang RY, Lin KH, 2006. Task-oriented progressive resistance strength training improves muscle strength and functional performance in individuals with stroke. Clin Rehabil ;20:860-870. Youdas, JW., Carey, JR and Garrett, TR. 1991. Reliability of measurements of cervical spine range of motion – comparison of three methods. Phys Ther 71 (2): 98 – 104 Young, J.B. and Forster, A. 1992. The Bradford Community Stroke Trial: results at six months. British Medical Journal (Clin Res Ed) 304 (6834): pp 1085-1089 Young, J. and Forster, A. 2007. Review of stroke rehabilitation. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 334 (7584), pp.86-90. Young, J., Murray, J. and Forster, A. 2003. Review of longer-term problems after disabling stroke. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 13 (1), pp.55-65. Zheng, H., Black, N. D. and Harris, N. D. 2005. Position-sensing technologies for movement analysis in stroke rehabilitation. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing, 43 (4), pp.413-420. 353 Reference List Ziemann, U., Muellbacher, W., Hallett, M. and Cohen, L. G. 2001. Modulation of practicedependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Brain, 124 (Pt 6), pp.1171-1181. Zigmond, A. S. and Snaith, R. P. 1983. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67 (6), pp.361-370. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The HADS: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Windsor 1994: NFER Nelson. 354 Reference List APPENDICES 355 Appendices Appendix I Pilot subject Information INFORMATION SHEET FOR PATIENTS HOME-BASED PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE: A PILOT STUDY Thank you for considering to take part in the above study which will involve helping to develop an exercise programme that Stroke patients can practice at home. The study is being undertaken jointly between Queen Margaret University College, Edinburgh, Edinburgh University and Lothian Primary Care NHS Trust How did I get your name? I asked your treating physiotherapist to enquire whether you might be interested in taking part. If you said you might want to know more about the study she has given you this information sheet. If you might be interested, please read on. Why has your help been requested help? After your stroke you have received treatment from physiotherapists who aimed to help you regain as much movement as possible. I am planning to look at whether a short programme of physiotherapy at least a year after stroke may be of further benefit to patients. Before I can undertake a large study I need to finalise exactly what patients want to practice, the best way to help patients practice exercises at home and the best way to write down exercise instructions. In order to do this I need to ask several stroke patients to try out the exercises and give me some feedback on how easy or hard they are. Where does the study take place? This study will take place in the physiotherapy gym at Astley Ainslie Hospital, and may also involve a visit to your own home if you are agreeable. If I agree to take part – what happens? I will talk to you about what activities you think are important to practice after your stroke. I will then show you some exercises to practice and some written instructions. I would like you to practice for a couple of days and then I will arrange to meet and discuss what you felt about the exercise programme. I will ask you to fill in a couple of questionnaires about your health and well-being, and will also ask you to demonstrate how you do a few daily activities such as getting in and out of a chair, picking up an object, as I want to see how long this takes. I estimate that this should take no more than an hour. How much time will this take? I would want to see you between 2 and 4 times over a 4-week period. Each session will last no more than 2 hours. More information overleaf 356 Appendices Appendix I Pilot subject Information How do I agree to take part? Please sign the attached consent form (or ask your carer to sign it for you) and either return it to your physiotherapist in the attached envelope and your physiotherapist will forward the form to me or else send it back to me directly. Can I refuse to take part? You have every right to refuse – don’t sign the form. If you start the programme and then want to stop, that is fine and this would not affect your normal physiotherapy treatment or any other treatment in any way. What information do I need to give and will it be confidential? I will need to know when you had your stroke. I may ask to video you. You don’t have to agree to this. If you do agree, only myself and another researcher will view the video and the tape will be destroyed at the end of the pilot study (in about 6 months time). All information you provide will be treated as strictly confidential, your name will not be revealed in any published papers related to this study. The researcher who will discuss the study with you and will show you exercises is a qualified physiotherapist working within Edinburgh. Physiotherapy rules of conduct oblige us to treat all patient information with professional confidence. If you have any other questions Please phone Gill Baer - the research physiotherapist, on 0131 317 3356. Alternatively you can contact your treating physiotherapist …………………………………. .. or Katie Wilkie (a senior physiotherapist with Lothian Primary Care NHS Trust) on 0131 537 9163 who is the external and independent advisor to this project. You are not expected to make an immediate decision about taking part. Think about it for a few days. If you do decide to take part in this physiotherapy programme please sign the attached consent form, and return it in attached envelope to your physiotherapist. Thank You so much for your consideration Gillian Baer M.Sc. M.C.S.P. (Study co-ordinator: Home-Based Physiotherapy late after stroke) 357 Appendices Appendix I Pilot consent A PILOT STUDY OF HOME-BASED PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE PATIENT CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE and for VIDEOTAPE RECORDING NAME: D.o.B. ADDRESS: PHONE NO CONSULTANT NAME QMUC STAFF RESPONSIBLE: Gillian Baer I hereby give my consent to participating in the above study. I also give/ do not give* consent to a video being recorded for use in the study. (delete as appropriate) my The purpose of the study has been explained to me and I understand that I can withdraw my consent at any stage. I understand that I will be asked to practice a number of functional tasks and to fill in a couple of questionnaires. I understand that this is to allow the development of a study into exercise for late stage stroke. I am also aware that I will be asked how long it took to practice exercises, fill in questionnaires and whether I felt the exercises were relevant. I am aware that any information I give will be treated in the strictest confidence and I will not be identifiable in any published results. I understand that no personal details (e.g. name, address) will be available in the video and that the video is being produced to allow the researchers to develop appropriate exercise practice for Stroke patients. I am aware that any information viewed will be treated in the strictest confidence, that the video will only be used for research purposes and that the tape will be destroyed on completion of the study (approximately 6 months time). SIGNATURE PATIENT / ADVOCATE DATE (print name and relationship to patient if advocate) 358 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 359 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 360 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 361 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 362 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 363 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 364 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (PP) 365 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 366 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 367 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 368 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 369 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 370 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 371 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 372 Appendices Appendix II Pilot exercises (WP) 373 Appendices Appendix IV TUG raw data age 2m1st side 3metre Patcode 3m Stand 3m-out 3m Turn1 3mback 3m Turn2 3m Sit total 2m Stand 2m-out 2m turn 1 2mback 2m Turn2 2m Sit total velocity 3m -> 3m <-> 2m -> 2m <-> 3metre Pat-code 3m Stand 3m-out 3m Turn1 3mback 3m Turn2 3m Sit 2m Stand 2m-out 2m turn 1 2mback 2m Turn2 2m1st right 2m1st right 2m1st right Time B2 3m1st 2m1st 3m1st 3m1st 2m1st right right right right right right stick D1 D2 E F1 F2 G H1 H2 I 2.93 1.14 1.25 2.15 2.15 1.64 2m1st 3m1st 3m1st right right right left stick K Knostic L M k 2.26 3.03 2.64 1.76 5.54 3.02 12.65 6.21 11.92 4.64 7.68 3.87 16.52 4.12 13.96 4.243 11.25 3.03 11.96 6.36 11.7 4.3 7.58 3.44 21.55 9.86 27.69 11.21 11.06 4.04 14.04 4.54 9.02 4.11 22.98 8.73 19.01 5.04 6.8 5.77 10.44 8.63 6.64 14.82 12.79 10.16 9.69 7.77 7.5 19.4 31.38 7.35 12.01 6.06 30.13 13.96 3.11 2.8 2.38 5.81 1.99 2.88 4.11 3.33 2.72 5.16 3.23 2.99 5.82 6.4 5.35 7.73 2.16 4.83 4.48 1.06 17.25 1.29 2.21 28.63 1.86 1.4 22.46 1.25 1.06 19.53 1.47 2.63 40.67 2.17 1.07 29.39 1.73 1.02 23.34 1.25 1.72 43.44 2.15 1.29 37.763 2.11 1.55 30.35 1.38 3.82 39.25 2.29 2.72 32.75 2.08 1.78 25.93 2.02 2.12 60.51 2.78 2.55 81.92 2.01 3.35 34.52 2.98 2.78 43.88 1.63 1.87 30.95 1.54 2.87 73.23 4.05 3.24 50.3 3.74 5.06 3.03 3.78 2.08 4.21 3.03 4.68 3.44 4.15 2.17 6.87 5.34 6.83 4.35 6.07 3.09 10.25 4.2 10.19 4.61 6.82 2.83 9.35 5.98 7.63 4.3 5.44 2.64 15.8 11.82 21.25 8.44 5.3 11.44 9.07 6.12 5.33 3.78 19.45 8.16 10.87 5.39 4.87 4.11 3.27 7.48 4.02 4.41 7.69 5.25 4.45 9.31 8.4 6.34 6.24 5.2 6.28 15.01 19.52 2.98 7.54 4.03 14.87 10.25 3.54 2.71 2.59 1.86 2.6 3.25 1.91 4.2 4.63 1.94 3.1 3.34 1.82 3.68 4.92 2.88 6.4 6.44 5.91 5.57 1.82 6.86 7.97 2.05 24.97 1.64 25.15 1.54 19.3 1.3 17.47 1.5 13.78 1.54 20.72 2.64 19.28 1.25 15.36 1.93 28.2 1.37 24.16 1.6 18.4 1.91 30.92 1.92 30.57 1.39 20.58 2.11 29.65 4.98 29.11 1.58 20.84 4.91 56.72 3.35 61.01 4.02 32.63 4.47 34.4 2.07 18.57 1.74 55.13 3.96 42.18 0.29 0.31 0.27 0.30 0.36 0.34 0.29 0.29 0.50 0.45 0.42 0.42 0.53 0.52 0.40 0.44 0.59 0.59 0.53 0.57 0.42 0.33 0.48 0.34 0.43 0.44 0.43 0.46 0.54 0.53 0.48 0.47 0.24 0.26 0.29 0.27 0.25 0.29 0.29 0.33 0.39 0.42 0.33 0.38 0.18 0.19 0.20 0.20 0.21 0.22 0.20 0.22 0.27 0.28 0.29 0.30 0.25 0.28 0.21 0.26 0.26 0.31 0.26 0.31 0.40 0.40 0.37 0.34 0.14 0.15 0.13 0.13 0.11 0.10 0.09 0.10 0.27 0.33 0.38 0.48 0.21 0.23 0.22 0.24 0.33 0.40 0.38 0.43 0.13 0.11 0.10 0.12 0.16 0.18 0.18 0.19 A B 2.54 3.99 1.25 1.68 1.39 2.49 1.48 1.76 10.49 2.55 8.35 3.55 6.06 2.74 5.71 2.74 5.11 2.3 7.07 2.55 6.92 3.06 9.11 9.11 7.35 5.83 4.98 11.2 5.88 4.03 1.96 2.06 2.41 2.09 32.66 2.26 2.73 31.76 2.11 0.77 20.13 1.29 1.49 19.51 1.38 7.3 3.96 6.95 4.2 4.72 4.17 5.82 6.71 3.58 2m1st right stick 2m1st right 2m1st right 2m1st right stick J1 2m1st right J2 2m1st right 3m1st right rolator N 3m1st right 3m1st left 3m1st right delta Q 2m1st left 3m1st right O P R S 2.69 3.37 2.78 2.16 3.69 4.57 Steps JC1 10 4 8 5 8 5 8 4 8 4 10 6 9 6 7 5 11 6 10 5 9 4 17 6 15 5 12 4 12 8 13 7 8 4 16 9 14 6 15 8 16 10 8 4 14 7 16 7 10 7 8 8 7 9 9 6 10 9 8 16 15 12 11 12 8 16 14 13 16 8 14 15 3 5 4 3 2 5 5 5 5 5 4 6 4 4 7 7 4 7 6 8 10 4 7 7 6 4 7 5 6 5 5 4 5 4 7 5 6 6 5 5 7 5 7 6 6 4 12 6 11 5 8 4 8 7 9 7 6 4 11 10 9 6 11 8 10 8 6 4 8 7 12 6 7 6 6 5 5 7 6 5 7 6 5 11 10 7 7 8 6 11 10 10 10 6 7 11 3 4 4 4 3 5 5 4 5 6 4 5 5 4 6 6 4 8 6 7 9 4 6 6 374 Appendices - TUG pilot raw data Appendix V PILOT OUTCOME MEASURES raw data PILOT OUTCOME MEASURE DATA TUG time PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 pretime posttime 31.64 28.58 32.38 31.46 36.72 36.89 34.55 31.76 43.44 30.35 21.13 21.96 post1 post2 post3 post4 post5 post6 2.2 8.23 3.1 8.71 4.12 2.22 2.87 10.31 2.98 9.54 3.89 1.87 3.13 12.1 4.65 9.89 3.4 3.72 3.99 8.35 3.55 9.11 4.03 2.73 1.64 11.25 3.03 10.16 2.72 1.55 1.76 5.54 3.43 6.77 3.38 1.08 PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 pre1 pre2 pre3 pre4 pre5 pre6 3.11 8.95 3.2 9.71 4.54 2.13 3.13 10.21 3.57 9.77 3.2 2.5 2.87 12.9 4.2 11.25 3.2 2.3 4.33 10.12 3.45 9.98 4.23 2.44 2.15 16.52 4.12 14.82 4.11 1.72 1.89 5.55 3.12 5.41 2.91 2.25 preRTS postRTS 3.11 2.2 3.13 2.87 2.87 3.13 4.33 3.99 2.15 1.64 1.89 1.76 375 Appendices – pilot outcome measures raw data Appendix V PILOT OUTCOME MEASURES raw data MOTOR ASSESSMENT SCALE PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 PreMA post pre pre pre pre pre pre pre pre S MAS MAS MAS MAS MAS MAS MAS MAS MAS total total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 32 35 6 6 4 2 4 4 4 2 25 25 6 6 4 5 4 0 0 0 25 27 6 6 4 5 4 0 0 0 33 36 6 6 4 2 4 5 4 2 21 23 6 6 4 2 3 0 0 0 22 22 6 6 4 2 4 0 0 0 post PreMAS MAS total total post post post post post post post post MAS1 MAS2 MAS3 MAS4 MAS5 MAS6 MAS7 MAS8 PP1 32 35 PP3 25 25 PP5 25 27 WP2 33 36 WP4 21 23 WP6 22 22 6 6 5 5 4 4 3 2 6 6 4 5 4 0 0 0 6 6 5 6 4 0 0 0 6 6 5 4 4 5 4 2 6 6 3 5 3 0 0 0 6 6 4 2 4 0 0 0 376 Appendices – pilot outcome measures raw data Appendix V PILOT OUTCOME MEASURES raw data STEP TEST preSTEP good PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 2 2 2 3 2 6 postSTEPgood preSTEPbad postSTEPbad 4 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 2 5 3 4 6 3 7 9 6 7 377 Appendices – pilot outcome measures raw data Appendix V PILOT OUTCOME MEASURES raw data BARTHEL INDEX PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 preBarthel postBarthel 19 19 19 19 18 18 19 19 19 19 19 19 FRENCHAY ARM TEST PP1 PP3 PP5 WP2 WP4 WP6 preFAT postFAT 3 4 0 0 0 0 3 3 0 0 0 0 378 Appendices – pilot outcome measures raw data Appendix VI ActivPAL guide ACTIVEPAL QUICK START TIPS: (to be read in conjunction with ActivPAL Idiots guide) 1. Start softwear. 2. Select “communicate with ActivePal from menu. 3. Connect to PC 4. Press on/off button (constant red light) 5. Select “connect” from menu. 6. Press “update”. 7. (Press “reprogram and clear memory”). 8. Disconnect from menu PC. 9. Press on/off button (flashing red light). 10.Tape ActivePal to mid-line of thigh. 11.Press on/off button to switch off. 12.Max 8 sessions recorded. 379 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VI ActivPAL guide ACTIV PAL INTRODUCTION The ActivePal is a small device worn on the leg to measure physical activity. When the leg moves, it generates totals for the periods spent sitting, standing or stepping. It also records the number of transitions from sitting to standing and vice versa. It can record up to 8 sessions and for multiple days (continuous recording in excess of 7 days/110 hours). It should be accurate to within 5%. The stored activity profile is retrieved and processed using a PC. The main uses for the ActivePal are in monitoring patient compliance and response to clinical interventions. THE DEVICE (sensing component) The ActivePal is small, lightweight (20g, including battery) and only 7mm thick. It is worn discreetly on the mid-thigh, either attached to the skin with medical adhesive tape, or to clothing. It can be worn either on the front or the side of the thigh, but tends to be most comfortable for the user when worn on the side of the thigh, midway between hip and knee. The correct orientation of the Pal is indicated by a figure on the front panel – the figure should be upright when the user is upright. The ActivePal can be switched on/off at any time using the ball point of a pen, and the data can be downloaded whenever convenient. A flashing red light will indicate that the device is recording. It requires a battery to operate. It is not waterproof. Adhesive Tape – recommend 3M Medipore tape PC running Microsoft Windows (95, 98, Millennium, 2000, NT, XP), which must have a serial port to allow the ActivePal to communicate with the computer via the interface cable. USING THE ACTIVE PAL SOFTWARE The software package analyses the recorded activity profile and identifies duration and intensity of the activities. The primary outcome measures are duration of stepping, standing and sitting events and cadence. These totals are calculated on a second by second basis allowing the frequency and duration of activities to be analysed. It uses intelligent activity classification algorithms. It offers both quantitative and graphical display options for the activity record. 380 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VI ActivPAL guide BATTERY Recommended: Varta CR2430. The battery should be inserted with the positive terminal surface uppermost, as shown in the polarity diagram on the back of the device. A fresh battery should provide in excess of one week of continuous recording, but performance will vary according to the number and length of recordings, and subject activity. Use the battery removal tool (small blue pin) to remove the battery, by inserting the pin through the aperture on the side of the device. Clearing data and reprogramming the device will greatly reduce the battery lifespan. PROGRAMMING 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Start the ActivePal professional software (START, PROGRAMS, ACTIVEPAL). Select “Communicate with ActivePal” from the file menu or icon. Make sure there is a battery in the ActivePal. Connect the ActivePal to the PC using the interface cable. Press the ON/OFF button on the ActivePal, using a ball point pen. The red light will come on constantly. 6. Select “connect” from the ActivePal menu or icon. 7. When connected, press the “update” button to synchronise the ActivePal with the PC’s clock. 8. If there is unwanted data on the ActivePal, press the “Reprogram and Clear Memory” button (NB, frequent use of this function will reduce the life of the battery). 9. The ActivePal should disconnect automatically after reprogramming– if not, select “disconnect” from the ActivePal menu. 10. Unplug the ActivePal from the interface cable. The red light should go off. RECORDING SESSIONS Before wearing the ActivePal, it must be activated by connecting to the PC in the first instance, and then once it is removed from the PC cabe, by pressing the ON/OFF switch on the front panel, using a ball point pen. A flashing red light indicates that the ActivePal is active. To end the session, press the ON/OFF switch again, so the red light is off. Position the ActivePal on the mid-thigh for optimal comfort and performance of the device – midway between hip and knee. It is best secured to the skin with Medipore tape. 381 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VI ActivPAL guide The ActivePal must be taken off for bathing/showering as it is not waterproof. For these periods remove it and place it on a flat surface, then re-apply it as before. If the ActivePal is removed for longer periods (eg, overnight), it should be switched off at the ON/OFF switch. Note, the ActivePal can hold a maximum of 8 sessions. After 8 sessions have been recorded, a red light will appear constantly and the ActivePal will be unable to record further until the sessions have been deleted. It is not possible to delete one session at a time, only to clear the entire memory, so ensure any data required for further processing has been saved. PROCESSING RECORDINGS 1. Start the software and connect the ActivePal to the PC using the interface cable as before. 2. Press the ON/OFF switch on the ActivePal. The red light will stay on constantly. 3. Select “communicate with ActivePal” and then “connect” from the menu. 4. When connected, all recorded sessions will be displayed in a list. To view the data, click on a session from the list and press the SAVE button. Graphs will be generated for that session, which can be saved to CD-ROM or hard drive (usually too big for disk). Each session will have to be downloaded individually in this way. 5. To view downloaded data, start professional software, and open relevant file. DATA Recordings downloaded from the ActivePal are automatically processed. There is no maximum length for a recording (depends on battery life), but the minimum recording time to be able to process data is 60 seconds. If the recording covers multiple days, only one day can be displayed at a time. Recordings are displayed in two formats: - as a summary hour by hour of the recording period; as totals for the recording period. Each recording can be saved for further analysis or printed. “Print” option prints the selected window. 382 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VI ActivPAL guide “Save” saves the data from the selected window to a .csv file, which can be opened in Microsoft Excel for further analysis. When viewing data that has already been saved to a file, go into ActivePal softwear and open file from there. Summary by Hour Presentation – the recording is presented with an interval resolution of 15 secs. Different colours represent sitting/lying, standing and stepping, and at the end of each hour there is a summary of the number of minutes spent in each activity, number of steps and number of transitions from sitting to standing. Energy expenditure is also given in METs. Totals for Recording Period summary – lists the overall time spent in each activity, alongside number of steps taken and sit/stand transitions. PROTOCOL FOR USE – Getting Started 1. Install ActivePal professional software to your PC. 2. Insert new battery into device. It is advisable to tape the battery into place, and mark with date of insertion/programming (to avoid accidental removal of battery and to give an indication of how long battery has left if in frequent use). 3. Start software (Start, Programs, ActivePal professional). 4. Program the device: Select “communicate with ActivePal” from the file menu; Connect to PC using interface cable; Press on/off switch on device – to get constant red light; Select “connect” from menu; Note the serial port being used to establish the connection; Select “update” to synchronise clocks; Press “reprogram and clear memory”; Unplug ActivePal from interface cable (red light should now go off). 5. Use as required, following details given above. 383 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VI ActivPAL guide TROUBLE SHOOTING 1. Unable to Establish a Connection: - check you have selected the correct serial port, and that the port is not in use by any other device. (into Software, ‘communicate with ActivePal’, go to ‘SETTINGS’ and click on the serial port to which you have connected your interface cable). - Check battery is correctly inserted. - Ensure interface cable properly connected to PC and device. 2. Constant red light when ActivePal not connected to interface cable: - either a system error has occurred, so the ActivePal must be reset by removing and re-inserting the battery and then reprogrammed as before (note, this process uses up the battery life more quickly). This will not affect data stored; - or, the memory is full or the maximum number of recording sessions has been reached. Memory requires to be cleared by erasing recorded data. 3. - No flashing red light when depress on/off switch memory may be full, in which case clear recorded data; system error, in which case remove battery and reprogram; battery expired, in which case replace battery. 384 Appendices – ActivPAL Appendix VII Pilot Exercise Repetitions PILOT REPETITIONS Subject characteristics intials J D E G A K group PP1 WP2 PP3 WP4 PP5 WP6 age 68 62.3 61.1 58.7 57.8 64 time since stroke (WEEKS) 56 28 37 50 34 39 side hemiplegia L R L L L L dom hand R R R L R R Rise to stand exercise subject J D E G A gp PP1 WP2 PP3 WP4 PP5 K WP6 sts1 sts2 sts3 sts4 tot_STS_Down wk_STS_Down 68 185 250 210 713 178.25 150 180 210 210 750 187.5 72 72 72 72 288 72 60 48 42 40 190 47.5 48 48 48 48 192 48 PATIENT LOST DIARY - BUT INCLUDE PRE POST MEASURES Step ups subje ct gp J PP1 D WP2 E PP3 G WP4 A K PP5 WP6 stepupdo wn1 stepupdo wn2 stepupdo wn3 stepupdo wn4 tot_stepup down wk_stepupd own 79 230 230 210 749 187.25 58 60 70 70 258 64.5 72 72 72 72 288 72 52 44 40 40 176 44 48 48 48 48 192 48 PATIENT LOST DIARY - BUT INCLUDE PRE POST MEASURES 385 Appendices – Pilot Exercise repetitions Appendix VII Pilot Exercise Repetitions Arm exercises cuppa 1 subject gp J PP1 D WP2 E PP3 G WP4 A K PP5 WP6 subject gp J PP1 D WP2 E PP3 G WP4 A K PP5 WP6 cuppa 2 cuppa 3 cuppa 4 tot_cuppa wk_cuppa 66 210 230 210 716 96 52 50 70 70 242 60.5 6 12 12 8 38 9.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 10 25 22 25 82 20.5 tiptap1 tiptap2 tiptap3 tiptap4 92 210 210 230 742 185.5 80 100 140 120 440 110 12 12 12 6 42 10.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 10 18 20 22 70 17.5 tot_tiptap wk_tiptap 386 Appendices – Pilot Exercise repetitions Appendix VIII Final Information sheet and consent INFORMATION SHEET FOR PATIENTS HOME-BASED PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE Thank you for considering to take part in the above study which will investigate different management strategies for late-stage stroke. This study is being undertaken jointly between Queen Margaret University College, Edinburgh, Edinburgh University and Lothian Primary Care NHS Trust Your GP practice was asked to send out a letter to all patients on their register who they know to have had a stroke over 12 months ago. Only your GP or the GP practice staff had access to your personal information. If you don’t want to take part in this study simply don’t send the form back. If you might be interested, please read on. Why have we requested your help? After your stroke you may have received treatment from physiotherapists who aimed to help you regain as much movement as possible. This study aims to look at whether a short programme of physiotherapy at least a year after your stroke may be of further benefit to you. We have developed a number of simple procedures and exercises that we think may help improve tasks such as walking or using your arm. What we want to do now is to study the effect of these exercises with a number of patients who had their stroke at least a year ago. Some patients will be asked to do the exercises, others will not. We do however want to look at all participants movement over a 12 month period. Where does the study take place? The study takes place in your own home. We will travel to you. If I agree to take part – what happens? You will receive a number of visits from both a researcher and a physiotherapist over the next12 months. Before starting the study, the researcher will visit and ask you to fill in a couple of questionnaires about your health and well-being. They will also ask how you manage daily activities such as getting in and out of a chair, walking ability and will ask you to demonstrate how you do a few of these tasks. This information will be gathered on 2 visits over about 2 weeks before the exercises start. Each visit will take about an hour. After these visits, a physiotherapist will come and teach you the exercises and check you can do them safely. The physiotherapist will leave an instruction booklet on how to perform the exercises and will visit you regularly to check your progress. The physiotherapist will also leave a diary for you to record daily how many times you practised the exercises. The researcher will visit and re-test you on the tasks at the end of the exercise programme, with 3 further visits just after you finish the programme and at 3 and 12 months later. (This is to see if you improve and also whether any improvement is long-lasting. More information overleaf 387 Appendices Appendix VIII Final Information sheet and consent How long does the programme last? The exercise programme lasts 4 weeks, but as indicated above, the researcher will continue to visit you on 3 occasions over the next 12 months. How do I agree to take part? Please sign or put your mark on the attached consent form and return it to the researcher – Gill Baer – in the attached envelope. Do I have to take part? You have every right to refuse – don’t send this form back. If you start the programme and then want to stop, that is fine and this would not affect your normal physiotherapy treatment or any other treatment in any way. What information do I need to give and will it be confidential? We will need to know when you had your stroke. This information will be obtained from your medical records. No other medical information is required. All information will be treated as strictly confidential, your name will not be revealed in any published papers related to this study. The researcher and the physiotherapist who will treat you are qualified physiotherapists working within Edinburgh. Physiotherapy rules of conduct oblige us to treat all patient information with professional confidence. If you have any other questions Please phone Gill Baer (0131 317 3356). Alternatively you can contact Katie Wilkie (a senior physiotherapist with Lothian Primary Care NHS Trust) on 0131 537 9163 who is the external and independent advisor to this project. We do not expect you to make an immediate decision about taking part. Think about it for a few days. If you do decide to take part in this physiotherapy programme please sign the attached consent form, and return it in attached the pre-paid envelope to: Gill Baer (Study co-ordinator: Home-Based Physiotherapy late after stroke) Department of Physiotherapy Queen Margaret University College Duke St Edinburgh EH6 8HF Thank You so much for your consideration 388 Appendices Appendix VIII Final Information sheet and consent A STUDY OF HOME-BASED PHYSIOTHERAPY FOR LATE-STAGE STROKE PATIENT CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE NAME: D.o.B. ADDRESS: PHONE NO GP NAME ADDRESS QMUC STAFF RESPONSIBLE: Gillian Baer I hereby give my consent to participating in the above study. I understand the purpose of the study and I understand that I can withdraw my consent at any stage. I understand that I will be tested on a number of functional tasks and asked to fill in a couple of questionnaires. I understand that the study may involve a 4 week exercise programme. I am aware that any information given to Health Professionals will be treated in the strictest confidence and I will not be identifiable in any published results. SIGNATURE / MARK of PATIENT DATE 389 Appendices Appendix IX Randomisation list 390 Appendices Appendix IX Randomisation list 391 Appendices Appendix X Data ActivPAL subject 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 gp W C P P W P C P C W C W C W C P P P W C W C P C P P C W P W C C P P W W W W P P C W C C P C %sit_ly %stand %walk no_steps no_STS hrs mins no data no data no data no data no data 90 80 79 8 13 14 2 7 7 211 1326 1704 28 43 45 11 9 10 40 14 19 83 43 71 16 42 19 1 16 10 83 3307 2340 31 65 45 9 8 8 13 5 1 75 46 6 21 41 21 4 13 6 844 3631 1186 87 34 103 13 9 13 13 27 5 88 84 83 7 12 10 5 4 7 3077 874 2638 52 50 30 18 9 10 13 51 26 98 1 1 116 24 13 10 68 88 69 69 96 81 13 4 22 15 3 13 19 8 9 17 1 5 7093 2465 1814 5303 141 2416 32 32 40 63 10 68 7 12 8 9 14 15 54 35 21 49 22 37 81 96 50 79 74 91 71 52 90 77 97 15 3 34 9 18 4 21 28 6 18 2 4 1 16 12 7 5 8 19 4 4 1 1266 76 3629 4824 1350 1335 1472 7275 1105 1253 56 57 17 50 42 126 27 51 102 24 28 16 13 8 13 9 11 12 10 12 11 10 8 36 36 55 9 56 10 17 0 52 14 11 93 88 94 95 6 11 5 3 1 1 1 2 376 79 140 415 21 12 23 8 12 13 14 11 34 32 25 11 no data no data no data no data no data no data no data 392 Appendices Appendix X Data 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 57 58 59 61 62 63 65 subject W P W C W P P P C W W P C P gp 74 19 7 1336 29 11 26 85 91 70 80 82 83 4 3 13 12 9 11 11 6 17 8 9 6 4328 950 10324 3973 5350 1505 22 26 291 31 47 33 14 12 15 14 12 13 44 0 57 30 7 4 61 72 90 77 %sit_ly 25 16 5 12 %stand 14 12 5 11 %walk 4399 3646 1266 5036 no_steps 55 38 29 154 no_STS 13 10 12 12 hrs 43 27 46 44 mins no data no data no data 393 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary FINAL ACTIVITY DIARY (part) 394 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 395 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 396 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 397 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 398 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 399 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 400 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 401 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 402 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 403 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 404 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 405 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 406 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 407 Appendices Appendix XI Final part practice diary 408 Appendices 409