5 August 2014 - Amazon Web Services
Transcription
5 August 2014 - Amazon Web Services
Autism research news and opinion 29 July 2014 - 5 August 2014 For the latest news, visit SFARI.org on the Web Suicidal thoughts alarmingly common in people with autism Sarah DeWeerdt 31 July 2014 Julia Yellow As a teenager, Bianca Marshack often flew into rages over seemingly minor problems — as when her mother, Kathy, didn’t bring her favorite chicken dinner home from the grocery store. Her anger would quickly spiral out of control, and she would threaten to kill herself. “I would try to just hold her, to calm her down and say, ‘I’m here, I’m here for you,’” recalls Kathy Marshack, a Portland, Oregon-area psychologist. Bianca had been diagnosed at age 13 with a high-functioning form of autism called Asperger syndrome, and as she got older her moods could be explosive. “Sometimes she would say, ‘If you would just kill me, then we would both not have to suffer anymore,’” Kathy remembers. Bianca’s behavior reflects the striking paradox of emotional turmoil in autism, an aspect of the disorder that has received attention only in the past few years. Often, people with the disorder can seem emotionless, with a flat affect and little interest in talking about feelings — their own or anyone else’s. But they may also have outbursts in which they make dramatic, shocking threats to end their lives. Experienced clinicians have long had a sense that people with autism are at increased risk of ‘suicidality,’ which encompasses thoughts, plans and attempts to kill oneself. Their suspicions have been borne out in recent years: Several large studies of adolescents and adults with autism reveal that bleak moods and suicidal despair are alarmingly common, particularly among those on the milder end of the spectrum with so-called high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. At the same time, this suicidality may be difficult to recognize, because people with autism don’t talk about their emotions in typical ways — for example, they may report feeling suicidal without describing themselves as depressed. In the most recent study, published in June in The Lancet Psychiatry, two-thirds of a group of adults diagnosed with Asperger syndrome said they had thought about committing suicide at some point, and 35 percent had made specific plans or actually made an attempt1. “These are individuals who have been struggling all their lives to fit in,” says study leader Simon Baron-Cohen, professor of developmental psychopathology at the University of Cambridge in the U.K. “Along the way, they have really been suffering.” In fact, researchers say, some of the cognitive patterns seen in people with autism, such as the tendency to perseverate or get stuck on a particular line of thought, may make these individuals particularly vulnerable to suicidality. With this new evidence, they are starting to look for ways to identify suicidality and prevent it in this population. “This is a community in distress,” says Katherine Gotham, assistant professor of psychiatry at Vanderbilt University in Nashville, Tennessee, who studies depression in autism. “This is something we need to know more about and do something about, and the faster the better.” Hidden epidemic: For a long time, suicide was largely ignored by autism researchers. “When I got started taking care of people with autism, there was this belief that it’s not possible for them to have depression,” says Janet Lainhart, professor of psychiatry at the University of Wisconsin in Madison. People with autism were thought to have little experience of emotion, let alone of suicidal despair. Through the 1990s and 2000s, even as knowledge and awareness of autism rapidly accelerated, few rigorous studies investigated the topic2. One of the earliest, in 2007, involved just a few people but offered a strong suggestion that persistent, serious thoughts of suicide are common. In that study, ten adolescents with Asperger syndrome answered a detailed questionnaire about how frequently they had had various suicidal thoughts over the previous month. Half scored in a range that indicates high risk for suicide3. Although Asperger syndrome is no longer an official diagnosis in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, the term is still in use in the U.K. and elsewhere, and many individuals still identify with it. In the past couple of years, larger studies have confirmed that suicidality is common among young people with autism. Last year, researchers reported that among 791 children with autism younger than age 16, 14 percent had either talked about or attempted suicide, compared with just 0.5 percent of children without autism4. Another study of 102 children aged 7 to 16 with anxiety and high-functioning autism found that 11 percent had suicidal thoughts and behaviors5. Teenagers are already at a higher risk of suicide because of the emotional and social turmoil of adolescence. Having autism intensifies these difficulties, says Oren Shtayermman, associate professor in the School of Health Professions at the New York Institute of Technology, who conducted the 2007 study. “As they become adolescents, they become more and more ostracized from their peer groups,” he says. “They become more and more isolated from society.” Age may not offer any relief from this sense of isolation. An unpublished analysis of medical records of more than 2,000 California adults with autism found that 1.8 percent of these individuals attempted suicide between 2008 and 2012, compared with 0.3 percent of controls. Rehan Siddiqui became severely depressed in early 2001, during his second semester of college. “I had no friends, really. I went to class, I attended lectures, but had no one to hang out with.” During that period, he sometimes thought about ending his life, but was too deeply depressed to take any action. “I said I wish I would die, I wish I would die in a car crash, stuff like that,” he recalls. When he was diagnosed with Asperger syndrome nearly two years later, it came as a great relief, he says: “Finally there was a reason for why I’m so different.” “These are individuals who have been struggling all their lives to fit in. Along the way, they have really been suffering.” Some researchers say people like Siddiqui may be even more vulnerable to suicidality than those diagnosed as children, having spent years to decades without an explanation for their struggles — and without access to help. The 374 participants in Baron-Cohen’s, study, for example, were diagnosed at an average age of 31. They were nine times more likely than people in the general population to experience suicidal thoughts. The adults with autism in the California study were more than five times more likely than controls to attempt suicide. “I think this is the tip of the iceberg in terms of the bigger picture of suicidal ideation,” says Lisa Croen, director of the Autism Research Program at Kaiser Permanente, a nonprofit health care system based in California. For many people with the disorder, the longing for social and romantic relationships, independence and meaningful work is intense, but there are few programs to help them fulfill these basic human desires. Siddiqui says his hope is “to live a nice, independent life.” He lives with a roommate and is excited that he recently got a car, because it will help him stay more socially engaged. But bouts of depression — winter is especially difficult — have sometimes prevented him from working. “I’ve had my ups and downs,” he says. “Ideally, I would love to have a job where I can support myself without any government support.” Depression’s new face: Among the general population, most people who become suicidal are depressed. That’s true among people with autism too4. And because people with autism have high rates of depression, it’s not surprising that they are frequently suicidal. Still, traditional screens for depression may miss the emotional experience of people with autism. In Baron-Cohen’s study, suicidality was far more common than depression as it is usually diagnosed. Although 66 percent of the participants reported having suicidal thoughts, less than half as many reported feelings of depression. That’s not because they don't have those feelings, however. “They may not be able to access or have the vocabulary to describe their emotional state,” says Baron-Cohen. This condition, called alexithymia, is common in people with autism. Yet if clinicians listen closely, they may hear clues — albeit not the usual ones. People with autism may be unlikely to describe themselves as depressed, but “they will explain their emotional pain in idiosyncratic ways,” says Lainhart. One clinician recalls an individual with autism who was so deeply sad and hopeless that he described himself as “now darkness.” Another said she constantly thought that the time had come for her leave this planet, in search of another galaxy where she would fit in better and find a friend. The way people with autism think may itself make them more vulnerable to suicidality. For example, they often don’t think to reach out to others when they are upset: Asking for help is, after all, a social skill. They also tend to have rigid, inflexible thinking, so once suicide enters their mind, it may stay there. Gotham and her colleagues have found that patterns of repetitive thought contribute to depression in people with autism. “I think that there could be something similar going on with suicidality,” she says. People with autism often struggle to imagine the thoughts and feelings of others, including their own future selves. As a result, they may have trouble believing they will ever feel better. They can also easily become overwhelmed by the small but complex problems of everyday life and respond with extreme thoughts or statements. In the study of children with anxiety and high-functioning autism, researchers found that the children sometimes made suicidal threats for attention or to escape from an unpleasant situation. “Often it was a reaction to some limit being set or placed on the child,” says study leader Eric Storch, professor of pediatrics, psychology and psychiatry at the University of South Florida in Tampa. For example, a parent might announce that it’s time to stop playing video games, and a child might throw a tantrum, lose control and say that he is going to kill himself. Regardless of the intent, parents and clinicians should take threats of suicide from people with autism seriously. “It’s their best attempt to raise a red flag saying that they need help,” says Lainhart. Storch and his colleagues are developing a program for suicidal teens with autism based on a similar one for those with Tourette syndrome. The next step for Baron-Cohen’s group is simple but unusual: The researchers plan to ask people from the Asperger syndrome clinic what would help them. “We just have to be a little bit cautious not to jump to solutions that are off-the-shelf,” Baron-Cohen says. For example, telephone crisis lines are often recommended for suicidally depressed people — but because of their social deficits, people with autism may be unlikely to call. Since her daughter’s diagnosis, Kathy Marshack has oriented her psychotherapy practice to help families who have a child or parent on the autism spectrum. “At the time, it was frightening to me that Bianca was suicidal,” Marshack says. But looking back, she says, her daughter probably lacked the empathy to recognize the effect her pleas about wanting to die would have on her mother. “She was in such pain,” Marshack says. “She was trying to say, ‘I feel helpless.’” News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1: Cassidy S. et al. The Lancet Psychiatry 1, 142-147 (2014) Abstract 2: Hannon G. and E.P. Taylor Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 1197-1204 (2013) PubMed 3: Shtayermman O. Issues Compr. Pediatr. Nurs. 30, 87-107 (2007) PubMed 4: Mayes S.D. et al. Res. Autism Spect. Disord. 7, 109-119 (2013) Abstract 5: Storch E.A. et al. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2450-2459 (2013) PubMed Mutant protein causes reversible autism-like behaviors Alla Katsnelson 4 August 2014 Mice that begin expressing a mutant version of a protein called neurexin at 2 weeks of age develop autism-like behaviors that researchers can erase weeks or months later. The report, published 24 July in Cell Reports, suggests that it may be possible to treat autism symptoms even in adulthood1. In particular, the animals’ social behavior, such as self-grooming and interaction with other mice, returned to normal. Julia Yellow “I think this is good news for the autism field because it shows that reversion even in old animals can be possible,” says lead researcher Francisco G. Scholl, professor of medical physiology and biophysics at the University of Seville in Spain. The neurexin family of proteins helps neurons send signals across a synapse — the point at which two neurons connect. From their position on one side of the synapse, they bind to another family of proteins called neuroligins on the other side to facilitate the release of chemical messengers2. Mutations in neurexins and other synaptic proteins have been linked to autism. Several studies over the past few years have successfully reversed symptoms in mouse models of autism and related conditions such as Rett syndrome, tuberous sclerosis and fragile X syndrome. Last year, for example, researchers administered gene therapy to correct behavioral and other deficits in adult mice modeling Rett syndrome, and another study used a drug for sleeping sickness to reverse social and other deficits in a mouse model of severe autism. “It does give people hope that some aspects of autism can be improved in adulthood,” says Gavin Rumbaugh, associate professor of neuroscience at the Scripps Research Institute in Jupiter, Florida, who was not involved in the study. Research so far suggests that social deficits may be especially amenable to reversal in adulthood. Rumbaugh says this may be because the mutations associated with social behavior disrupt the functioning of individual neurons, rather than affecting overall brain structure or circuits. Reversal of fate: In the new study, Scholl and his colleagues engineered mice that develop normally for the first two weeks of life and then express a mutant fragment of one type of neurexin — called neurexin1β — in certain neurons of the forebrain. This mutant fragment blocks the normal protein from binding to its partner neuroligin, and prevents the movement of chemical messengers between neurons. Weeks later, the mice develop behavioral abnormalities: Compared with controls, they groom themselves compulsively, display repetitive behaviors, and show less interest in other mice and in social odors such as urine, and more in nonsocial odors such as vanilla and orange blossoms. Neurons in the somatosensory cortex — a region with an especially high amount of mutant neurexin — send fewer messages than normal. Neurexin is expressed in the mouse throughout development and into adulthood. Scholl’s team chose to block its function at 2 weeks of age to show that this is enough to trigger autism-like behaviors. They turned off the expression of the mutant protein at 2 to 4 months of age in one group of mice and at 9 to 11 months in another. Within two weeks of the treatment, the mice show no signs of their previous symptoms and behave like controls do. “This clearly shows that neurexin dysfunction in postnatal neurons is sufficient to trigger autism symptoms and that correct neurexin function is needed throughout life,” Scholl says. It would be interesting to know whether the same reversal would be possible if the mutant protein were introduced earlier — for example, around the first week, when brain development is at its peak, notes Catherine Fernandes, lecturer in social genetic and developmental psychiatry at Kings College London, who was not involved in the study. “[Scholl’s team is] almost implying that if there was a prenatal effect, it would have been reversed, but of course they haven’t done that,” she says. Gone awry: Neurons with a mutant neurexin form healthy synapses, but information doesn't travel across them as it should. Another question left unaddressed is whether turning the mutant protein off in adulthood also restores normal signaling, says Lucas PozzoMiller, professor of neurobiology at the University of Alabama at Birmingham, who was not involved in the study. “They didn’t go back and check to see if the synapses got better after they stopped the production of mutant proteins, like the behaviors did,” he says. Perhaps a bigger issue is that the link between the behavioral symptoms in mice and those of people with autism is unclear. Many genes affect social behavior, but not all those genes are necessarily involved in autism. The same is true at the circuit level, notes Rumbaugh. “We don’t understand how similar the circuitry and the function that drives behavior in the mouse brain is to that in the human brain,” he says. “Can we reasonably say that those are the circuits that are disrupted in an autistic child’s brain?” News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1. Rabaneda L.G. et al. Cell Reports 8, 338-346 (2014) PubMed 2. Südhof T.C. Nature 455,903-911 (2008) PubMed Induced stem cells retain traces of their former state Marissa Fessenden 30 July 2014 Reprogrammed stem cells carry remnants of their previous cell states in the form of chemical cues that alter gene expression, reports a paper published 10 July in Nature1. An alternative method that creates so-called nuclear transfer embryonic stem cells produces fewer errors. Working with stem cells offers tantalizing opportunities to study the genetic basis of complex diseases such as autism. Stem cells that are pluripotent, meaning that they can develop into any cell type, can come from several different sources. Fresh start: Stem cells created through nuclear transfer — a technique that replaces the nucleus of an egg cell with a nucleus from an adult cell — carry fewer reprogramming errors. Embryonic stem cells are harvested directly from an embryo created through in vitro fertilization. Induced pluripotent stem cells, or iPS cells, are adult cells reprogrammed to an undeveloped state through chemical manipulation. Nuclear transfer embryonic stem cells (NT ES cells) are created by replacing the nucleus of an egg cell with the nucleus of an adult cell. NT ES and iPS cells both retain the unique genetic signature of the adult cell donor, so they can be used to study diseases such as autism and to test therapies. iPS cells also skirt the ethical challenges of using embryos. Some earlier research indicated that iPS cells sometimes carry large deletions or duplications of DNA called copy number variations. It was unclear whether those variations are introduced during the reprogramming process or represent an incomplete reversion to an undeveloped cell. The new study directly compared NT ES cells and iPS cells with embryonic cells with the same genetic background, produced through in vitro fertilization. The three cell types all carry small numbers of copy number variants. However, when the researchers examined chemical tags on the DNA — epigenetic marks — they found that iPS cells retain tags harking back to their previous state. These altered epigenetic patterns also affect how the marked genes are transcribed into RNA. The differences indicate that iPS cells are incompletely reprogrammed, the researchers suggest, whereas the chemical environment of the egg, even in vitro, reprograms NT ES cells more faithfully. Further work with NT ES cells may reveal what kind of chemical cues the egg relies on and whether scientists could use them to create better iPS cells. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1. Ma H. et al. Nature 511, 177-183 (2014) PubMed Remembering Paul Patterson (1944-2014) Sarkis Mazmanian 29 July 2014 Paul H. Patterson, a transformative figure in autism research, died on 25 June. He was 70. We deeply mourn the loss of this true innovator and iconoclast, whose discoveries remain ahead of their time. As the Anne P. and Benjamin F. Biaggini Professor of Biological Sciences at the California Institute of Technology (Caltech), Paul influenced the thinking and research of dozens of scientists. His groundbreaking discoveries advanced novel paradigms in neuroscience and immunology, and introduced concepts that will continue to be developed by researchers worldwide. Paul had a dynamic career that led him to study the immune system’s effect on animal behavior, a concept that is still very much emerging. He also helped lead some of the first studies linking environmental factors to neurologic development and function. Paul Patterson As research in these areas grows, Paul’s discoveries will be even more appreciated. His research was preclinical, but it was heavily influenced by the epidemiologic and clinical studies of others. He used animal models of behavioral and neurodegenerative disorders to understand fundamental disease mechanisms, and his work was poised to test therapies for clinical use. Paul spent much time of his time engaged in community outreach, from his constant appearances to meet with families of children with autism, to his avid blogging and his recent book, Infectious Behavior. He was naturally inspired to help others, which is likely why he told many of us, shortly after his diagnosis of glioblastoma multiforme, that he wished he had just three more years. He and I were in the early planning stages of a clinical trial for a possible probiotic therapy to address gastrointestinal issues in autism. I should have reminded him that he has already helped so many people: He enriched the careers and lives of many trainees and colleagues. Interrelated fields: Paul completed his Ph.D. with William Lennarz at Johns Hopkins University in 1970, continued his training at Harvard Medical School as a postdoctoral fellow, and eventually became a faculty member there in the very first department of neurobiology established in the U.S. In this unique environment, he pioneered the primary culture of peripheral neurons and used this to discover that developing sympathetic neurons can switch their neurotransmitter phenotype from noradrenergic to cholinergic in response to environmental factors. This was a fundamental discovery in neuroscience, as it violated the ‘one neuron, one transmitter’ concept. It showed that neurotransmitter identity is not genetically determined and immutable, but subject to regulation by the environment. Paul's quest to purify and characterize the factor that controls this switch culminated in 1989, five years after his move to Caltech, with the purification and microsequencing of the ‘cholinergic differentiation factor.’ The sequence of this factor revealed, astonishingly, that it was identical to leukemia inhibitory factor, a cytokine previously identified based on its immunological function. This discovery, along with his early adoption of monoclonal antibodies as a tool to query the nervous system, marked the beginning of Paul's transformation into a ‘neuroimmunologist’ and the start of his thinking that factors outside the nervous system affect neurologic function. Paul continued his work on the effects of cytokines on the developing and diseased nervous system, deploying antibodies both as tools and therapeutic candidates. In the early 2000s, these lines of research led him to become increasingly interested in the interplay between the biology of inflammation and its impact on the developing brain and behavior. Emboldened by his unique perspective, Paul expanded on the link between the immune system and behavior by establishing a mouse model of autism, based on epidemiologic studies showing that infection during pregnancy increases the child’s risk of the disorder. He showed that stimulation of the immune system in pregnant mice results in pups with altered behaviors consistent with the three core deficits of autism, and characterized the immune pathways that promoted these outcomes. His discovery served to increase awareness of environmental influences on neurodevelopmental conditions; studies by other investigators corroborated his observations in people. Gastrointestinal disturbances have also been linked to children with autism. In one of his most recent studies, Paul demonstrated that the gut microbiome, the diverse collection of intestinal bacteria, regulates behaviors in mouse models of autism, and that probiotic treatment leads to improvements in behavioral deficits. These studies provide the hope that microbial therapies may be able to alleviate neurodevelopmental disorders with strong environmental influences, perhaps through balancing of the immune system. Paul was a true scientist, and unwaveringly applied the scientific approach to everything in life, forever asking, “What is the evidence for that?” He was also a meticulous cook who perfected exotic dishes to satisfy his eclectic tastes. His essence as a scientist was on display in the lab announcement in which Paul described his illness to his trainees and colleagues. He said he was going to beat the illness through a clinical trial that, ironically, used modulation of the patient’s immune system as the therapeutic modality. Paul is survived by his loving wife Carolyn, son Paul Clair and the countless people whose lives he touched — including the children with autism who may someday benefit from his pioneering discoveries. Sarkis Mazmanian is the Louis & Nelly Soux Professor of Microbiology at the California Institute of Technology in Pasadena. Editor’s note: We, the journalists at SFARI.org, deeply miss Paul Patterson. He was persistent and passionate in his belief that the immune system is involved in autism, but whether a paper supported his theory or refuted it, he always made time to comment on the work. He was an avid reader of the site, and never shied away from telling us what he thought. We fondly remember his sense of humor, too: In November, at the Society for Neuroscience conference, he was visibly ill, but joked that his surgical scar was the result of a scuffle with a Cell editor. “You should see the other guy,” he said, guffawing. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. Tools for autism screening must vary with language, culture Kate Yandell 1 August 2014 At its core, autism is the same disorder worldwide. And ideally, it should be possible to identify it consistently and accurately everywhere. But most screening methods for the disorder were developed in the U.K. and U.S., and linguistic and cultural differences can affect their performance elsewhere. These differences can make a real difference to diagnosis: “Yes” answers that predict an autism diagnosis in the original screen may not do it so well in an adaptation. For instance, when parents in the U.S. report that their children are not interested in their peers, it correlates strongly with autism. In Japan, where expectations from children are different, the same question does poorly at predicting autism. This may be because Japanese parents interpret their children’s reticence as shyness or modesty, qualities valued in their culture, and don't consider it unusual behavior. This is one of the many cultural differences reported in a review of autism screens published 9 July in Cultural contrast: Parents in Japan may view Autism. The review analyzed 21 studies published certain autism traits as a sign that their child is since 2004 on the adaptation of autism screening modest or reserved, qualities prized in Japanese culture. questionnaires for other cultures. These questionnaires are given to children’s caregivers, clinicians or teachers to identify children who might benefit from further evaluation. In all, the review covers nine tests’ performance in 20 countries (counting Taiwan and China as separate countries) and 10 languages. Of the 21 studies, 12 made various types of modifications to the screening questionnaires. Sometimes, they simply noted that caregivers around the world do not universally understand specific toys, nor do they necessarily have names for certain games. For instance, there is no single word for peekaboo in Mexico, so researchers translating a U.S.-developed screening questionnaire for toddlers had to first explain the game before asking parents if their children enjoy playing it. Other changes relate to how people in different cultures categorize information. While adapting a test for China, for instance, researchers found that on questions to which U.S. caregivers were okay with giving “yes” and “no” answers, their Chinese counterparts were more comfortable selecting from answers such as “never,” “seldom,” “usually” and “often.” The review generally assessed whether the 21 adaptations followed appropriate translation and validation guidelines. For instance, guidelines suggest that two independent people or teams translate the questionnaire, and then another person back-translate the questionnaire into the original language to see if any meaning was lost. Researchers should also assess how participants react to the translated questionnaire. None of the 21 adaptations followed all of the guidelines perfectly, and most of them missed multiple steps. However, it is unclear whether some of the adaptations followed guidelines but just didn’t say so in the reports. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation.