Lion Relationships in the 21st Century
Transcription
Lion Relationships in the 21st Century
Our Shared Kingdom At Risk: Human – Lion Relationships in the 21st Century A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Yale University in Candidacy for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy by Laly Laing Lichtenfeld Dissertation Directors: Professor Stephen Kellert and Professor Oswald Schmitz Dissertation Committee Member: Professor Alison Richard 2005 © 2005 by Laly Laing Lichtenfeld All rights reserved. Abstract Our Shared Kingdom at Risk: Human – Lion Relationships in the 21st Century Laly Laing Lichtenfeld 2005 Globally, many large carnivore populations are in decline. In most cases, the persistence of these species is linked to their relationships with humans. Traditional conservation approaches tend to focus on the ecological causes of human – carnivore conflicts without fully appreciating the diverse interplay of social and ecological forces driving the outcomes. Even in the case of the African lion, arguably the most well studied large carnivore in the world, little research addresses human attitudes toward lions or the social factors influencing their actions. I focus on a multidisciplinary study of human – lion relationships in the Tarangire ecosystem of northern Tanzania. Specifically, I illustrate the diverse range of ecological and social factors affecting both lions and the key stakeholders encountering them using structured surveys, interviews, and spoor counts as the principal methodologies. Within a single landscape, I demonstrate that a wide variety of attitudes toward lions exist among the local Maasai, professional sport hunting, and photographic tourism communities based on unique assemblies of psychological, political, socio-cultural, economic, and ecological factors. In addition, I evaluate the impact of these groups on the distribution and abundance of lions. From these studies, I determine the relative positive and negative influences of each stakeholder on lions indicating the key variables affecting the long-term conservation of the African lion. Finally, I propose a new, theoretical model of human – lion relationships that emphasizes the abilities of each “culture,” feline and human alike, to affect one another via their respective tolerances of each other. This type of study has wide applications to the global community of humans and large carnivores. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Figures v List of Tables vi Acknowledgements viii I. INTRODUCTION 1 II. A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES TOWARD LIONS AMONG THE PROFESSIONAL SPORT HUNTING INDUSTRY, PHOTOGRAPHIC TOURISM INDUSTRY, AND MAASAI COMMUNITIES IN THE TARANGIRE ECOSYSTEM, TANZANIA 17 III. THE RISK OF LIVING WITH LIONS: THE EFFECTS OF POTENTIAL AND ACTUAL INTERACTIONS WITH LIONS ON THE MAASAI LIVING ADJACENT TO TARANGIRE NATIONAL PARK 73 IV. LIONS LIVING ON THE EDGE: REAL AND PERCEIVED LION ABUNDANCE IN SPORT HUNTING AND VILLAGE LANDS OUTSIDE TARANGIRE NATIONAL PARK, TANZANIA 108 V. CONCLUSION: THE AFRICAN LION ROAMING THROUGH WILDERNESS AND THE HUMAN MIND 133 Bibliography 154 Appendix One: Sample Interview 162 Appendix Two: Questionnaire 166 LIST OF FIGURES 1.1 A SCHEMATIC DIAGRAM OF HUMAN – LION RELATIONSHIPS 12 2.1 THE TARANGIRE ECOSYSTEM 25 2.2 THE LOCATION OF AGRICULTURAL AREAS IN THE TARANGIRE ECOSYSTEM 28 3.1 PREFERENCES TOWARD WILDLIFE AMONG LOCAL COMMUNITY MEMBERS 85 4.1 LOCATION OF THE THREE STUDY SITES (BOXES) IN THE TARANGIRE ECOSYSTEM 112 4.2 THE ROAD NETWORK IN LOIBOR SERRIT 115 5.1 A THEORETICAL MODEL OF HUMAN-LION RELATIONSHIPS 143 LIST OF TABLES 1.1 POTENTIAL KEY ECOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL VARIABLES INFLUENCING THE 13 SURVIVAL OF LION POPULATIONS 2.1 BASIC WILDLIFE VALUES ADAPTED FROM KELLERT (1996) 21 2.2 FACTORS INFLUENCING HUMAN PERCEPTIONS, KNOWLEDGE, AND 21 INTERACTIONS WITH CARNIVORES ADAPTED FROM KELLERT (1992) 2.3 SUMMARY OF INTERVIEWEE CHARACTERISTICS 36 2.4 PERCEIVED THREATS TO LIONS IN THE TARANGIRE ECOSYSTEM ACCORDING 49 TO STAKEHOLDERS 3.1 AVERAGE PLOT SIZE AND HOUSEHOLD LIVESTOCK HOLDINGS PER VILLAGE 84 3.2 RANK OF LARGE CARNIVORES CONSIDERED TO POSE A STRONG THREAT TO RESPONDENTS IN TERMS OF MANIFESTING FEAR OR POTENTIALLY HARMING HUMANS AND LIVESTOCK 88 3.3 FACTOR ANALYSIS OF QUESTIONS RELATING TO RISK PERCEPTIONS OF LIONS 89 3.4 RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK PERCEPTION OF LIONS AND DESIRED FUTURE POPULATION SIZE OF LIONS 89 3.5 RESULTS OF THE LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSES FOR THE SOCIO-ECONOMIC 89 FACTORS THAT INFLUENCE GENERAL ATTITUDES TOWARD LIONS AND RISK PERCEPTIONS 3.6 RESPONDENTS' ESTIMATES OF THE NUMBER OF CATTLE, GOATS, AND SHEEP TAKEN BY CARNIVORES OVER THE LAST TWO YEARS 93 3.7 MEAN NUMBER OF DAYS RESPONDENTS ENCOUNTERED LIONS OVER THE LAST TWO YEARS ACCORDING TO ACTIVITY 95 3.8 RESULTS OF THE LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSES FOR THE RELATIONSHIP 96 BETWEEN SOCIO-ECONOMIC FACTORS AND BENEFITS OF WILDLIFE TOURISM 3.9 RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BENEFITS FROM WILDLIFE TOURISM AND DESIRED FUTURE POPULATION SIZE OF LIONS 97 3.10 WILLINGNESS TO RESTRICT ACTIVITIES FOR LION CONSERVATION 97 Tables vii 4.1 ROAD AND SAMPLING CHARACTERISTICS OF THE THREE STUDY AREAS 116 4.2 THE PERCENTAGE OF CORRECTLY IDENTIFIED LIONS FROM THEIR SPOOR 119 INCLUDING DETERMINATION OF AN INDIVIDUAL’S AGE GROUP AND SEX 4.3 OVERALL AND SEASONAL SPOOR DENSITIES FOR LION, SPOTTED HYENA, 120 AND LEOPARD IN THE THREE STUDY AREAS 4.4 LION POPULATION STRUCTURE BASED ON AGE GROUPS ACCORDING TO 121 INDIVIDUALLY IDENTIFIED LIONS IN THE PARK AND SPOOR DATA FOR THE THREE EXPERIMENTAL AREAS 5.1 POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE FACTORS INFLUENCING HUMAN – LION RELATIONSHIPS ORGANIZED BY KEY ECOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL VARIABLES 140 Acknowledgements I remember being told once that the pursuit of a doctoral degree can often feel like a lonely or solitary journey. Happily, that was not my experience… Throughout this process, I have been supported by a responsive, encouraging, and, challenging doctoral committee who have always pushed me to excel and give just a little bit more. The final product has been immeasurably improved by their suggestions, guidance, and attention to detail along the way. Thank you Steve, Os, and Alison! In support of my research and the People and Predators Project, I would like to thank Yale University, particularly the School of Forestry and Environmental Studies. Swarovski Optik, Safari Club International, and the Jockey Hollow Foundation all contributed to the project. I am also deeply grateful to the many private individuals and family members who have generously supported the project. Many thanks to Susan and Art Babson who responded quickly to a call for help when the Land Rover was ailing! I would like to thank the Tanzanian Wildlife Research Institute, the Wildlife Division, the Tanzanian National Parks Authority, and the Commission for Science and Technology for permission to conduct this study. I am also grateful to the village members of Lolkisale, Loibor Soit, Emboret, Naraquao, Sukuru, Kimotorok, and especially, Loibor Serrit who lent their support and, in many cases, time to the project. Thank you to the professional hunting and photographic tourism interviewees who contributed to this study. I would like to thank and remember Ridge Taylor for our fireside chats and for inviting me to experience sport hunting firsthand. I could not have collected the data without the hard work and skill of my wildlife trackers and research assistants. In particular, I would like to thank Saitoti for opening my eyes to the world of paw prints. Driving a dirt track will never be boring again! And to Michael, for making sure we were all comfortable, well fed, and happy in the bush. To all my friends, both in Tanzania and otherwise, thanks for the encouragement, emails, fireside chats, and more! To Charlie and Lynn, thank you for providing me not only with a very wonderful person in my life (and two great pups!) but for helping me to overcome the pitfalls of bush logistics, car problems, illnesses, and generally all things unforeseen! To Mark and Laurie, your love and support have meant so much to me. Thank you for encouraging my passion and participating in my excitement! To Buddy, you have been my friend, colleague, and partner through both the most difficult and most wonderful days. Sharing this with you has been a special gift. And, most of all, to my mom, who might not have willed that her daughter cavort with lions, but who has embraced my choices, my love of lions and Africa, and our distance with the grace, love, and support that are so much a part of her wonderful, giving spirit. You are always with me and will always be my forever friend! If the lions are lost forever, it would be a great loss to me. Because, first of all, the traditions of the Maasai to celebrate the bravery of lion hunting won’t be seen, and the children won’t see [the lion], they will only be hearing in stories that there was something called a lion. Maasai Elder [The lion], it’s something from nature that is just absolutely incredible. To see this huge animal with the power it has and that majestic appearance, both male and female, you know when you drive around and you look and you see them and they are lying there, you know they don’t need us, really. Professional Sport Hunter Africa represents one of the last wilderness areas in the world and the lion kind of is the monarch of that representation. You know if you have a huge, powerful predator in amidst its own natural environment, how [much] better can that get? Its that feeling of, yeah, we are seeing you know the big picture, the whole sort of wilderness entirety summed up in one animal. Photographic Guide CHAPTER ONE Introduction Chapter One Thirty-five thousand years ago, in what is now known as Chauvet Cave in southeastern France, our ancestors celebrated their relationship to the natural world by painting a wildlife mural on the rocky walls of an old cave bear’s den. It is the most ancient record of rock painting found throughout the world today. And among the red ochre and charcoal figures, including horses, bison, rhinoceroses, bears, mammoths, reindeer and more, were lions. Many, many lions. Indeed, human fascination with large carnivores, and particularly lions, is as enduring as the earliest known artistic account left by our predecessors. Throughout human history, we have shared a landscape with large carnivores that has not only shaped our place within the natural world but has inspired our spiritual and psychological development as we react to the presence of these extremely beautiful, yet frighteningly lethal animals. Their power humbles us, their magnificence inspires us, and their sheer presence forces us to consider our own identity in a world where we are both predator and prey. Having so captivated the human mind, large carnivores have inspired human culture and development, apparent in the pervasive representation of these species in mythology, art, literature, religion, and popular culture. While the lion is perhaps the most widely represented species, large carnivores affect us wherever they may roam. In terms of North American species, few animals have had as much influence as the grizzly bear (Kellert et al. 1996). Native Americans have revered bears, carving their figures in totem Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 3 poles, and worshipping them to the extent that certain tribes view the hunting of bears as taboo (Rockwell 1991). Indeed, many cultures have celebrated the beauty and power of the bear whose physical features and upright stance seem to remind us of ourselves (Shepard and Sanders 1985). The humanization of bears is evident in the many childhood stories that feature them, such as Goldilocks and Winnie the Pooh. Popular culture has embraced the bear, for instance as a guardian of natural landscapes in the image of Smokey Bear, as the clowning, cartoon character Yogi Bear, and, of course, as the warm, comfortable childhood companion, the teddy bear (Kellert et al. 1996). These wholesome, happy impressions are challenged, however, by another vision of the grizzly bear that is suggested by its scientific name, Ursos arctos horribilis. To some people, particularly to those living near them, bears are viewed as deadly killers. Concepts of bears in these cases are more negative given their potential threats to humans and livestock (Kellert 1992). Protection of the bear and associated land-use restrictions can transform them into symbols of government control and subjugation (Kellert 1992; Jope and Shelby 1994). In such cases, the grizzly bear represents the looming, threatening possibility of change and outside intervention in people’s lives (Mattson et al. 1996). In this sense, as we continually redefine our place in the world, the nature of human associations with large carnivores often reflects our changing views toward the environment. For example, perceptions of the wolf have been powerful indicators of our shifting attitudes toward wildlife and nature. Early settlers of the North American continent initially targeted wolves as threats to livestock, settlement, game populations, Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 4 and, more generally, to human progress and vigorously persecuted them (Lopez 1978; Matthiessen 1987; Kellert 1996; Kellert et al. 1996). Wolves were regarded as threatening the advance of human civilization and their eradication a symbol of the human ability to conquer nature. These negative perceptions persisted well into the 1900s. Yet, pioneering studies during the 1980s by Kellert (1985, 1986, 1991) and others (Hook and Robinson 1982; Bath 1989) clearly demonstrated that contemporary attitudes toward wolves were changing. For example, Kellert (1985) illustrated that in the late 1970s and early 1980s attitudes toward wolves were divided, though increasingly positive. Kellert (1991) found more positive attitudes toward wolves among the young, urban, and highly educated sectors. Attitudes also varied by livelihoods; individuals who were economically tied to the local landscape tended to exhibit more negative attitudes (Kellert 1985, 1986, 1991; Bath 1989). Certainly, few individuals have expressed a personal change in the perception of wolves as eloquently as Aldo Leopold, whose 1949 epiphany anticipated the more popular transition to come: In those days we had never heard of passing up a chance to kill a wolf…I thought…that no wolves would mean a hunter’s paradise…Since then I have lived to see state after state extirpate its wolves…I have seen every edible bush and seedling browsed, first to anemic desuetude and then to death…I now suspect that just as a deer herd lives in mortal fear of its wolves, so does a mountain live in mortal fear of its deer…Perhaps this is the hidden meaning in the howl of the Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 5 wolf, long known among mountains, but seldom perceived among men. (Leopold 1949: 137) Interestingly, while wolves and bears have been very prominent figures in defining human – carnivore relationships in North America, the more widely distributed mountain lion has received far less attention in terms of human culture. Kellert et al. (1996) suggest that the generally silent, elusive nature of the mountain lion has been influential in keeping the large cat out of the spotlight despite a consistent record of attacks on humans and livestock that surpasses that of wolves. Similarly, in South America, pumas were less prevalent in people’s minds than jaguars despite their elevated likelihood of attacking humans (Conforti and Azevedo 2003). Individuals appeared to know less about pumas or mountain lions, and therefore, the latter were overshadowed by the more behaviorally and ecologically prominent large carnivores. Certainly, few species, however noticeable they may be, captivate the human mind and imagination as absolutely as the proud, self-reliant figure of an African lion standing boldly in the wide-open grassland of East Africa. Or even lazing about in large, social groups, lions seem to invite our interest while often affecting an air of indifference to our presence. Yet, the eyes of a lion never truly pass over people without perceiving us, at best, as a curiosity or, at worst, as a meal. As an intrepid, even terrifying, yet wholly magnificent species, the African lion has dwelled in the landscape of our minds since the dawn of human evolution. Early in our Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 6 history, the lion was man’s rival, a dominant competitor for food, resources, and space. As local cultures emerged and expanded, however, human relationships to the lion became more diversified. The lion became more than just a physical adversary. By a process of socialization, it became a powerful medium through which various symbolic, spiritual, political, and cultural beliefs were communicated. To fully appreciate the intertwining of our histories, we must first consider an age when man and lion initially walked upon the land together. Lions are believed to have evolved between 1.8 and 3.8 million years ago along with the other big cats: jaguars, snow leopards, tigers, and clouded leopards (O’Brien 1996). The first hominids were thought to evolve around 5 million years ago (Ridley 1993). Originally, they would have shared a landscape with the lion’s predecessors, the sabertooth cats. Paleontological evidence suggests that early hominid – sabertooth relationships were characterized by human avoidance of these species despite a dependence on the scavenged kills of sabertooths for nutritional requirements. While scientific debate exists regarding the degree to which hominids actively confronted the toothy beasts, Marean (1989) hypothesized that as habitats became more and more open, and sabertooths slipped toward extinction, early man was probably forced to become more confrontational when scavenging kills. This competitive relationship may very well have delineated the origins of human – lion relationships. And despite their struggles for resources and space, both lions and humans established themselves as dominant, successful predators and scavengers. Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 7 The magnitude of competition between early man and lions remains unknown. Eaton (1979) suggests, in the best-case scenario, man was co-dominant with the lion. Certainly, more recent accounts of competition, such as the infamous man-eating lions of Tsavo, or the less well known, yet on-going accounts of man-eating lions in southern Tanzania (Baldus 2004) indicate the struggle of some modern human communities to share territories with lions. In Uganda, Treves and Naughton-Treves (1999) revealed that even in the twentieth century people continued to actively scavenge from carnivore kills, risking injury for meat. Attacking a lion with rudimentary weaponry is a dangerous proposition. In the beginning, when weapons were scarcely developed, avoidance of lions was probably a wiser tactic than direct confrontation. Nevertheless, a long history of fierce interactions with lions throughout Africa and other parts of the world developed into a common recognition of the lion as one of the most significant, non-human adversaries. As such, societies rose up to conquer the lion, thereby demonstrating their prowess and strength to the rest of the world. Faced with more technologically advanced, weapon-bearing societies, lion populations dwindled and were pushed back toward the African continent. Warring societies such as the ancient Romans were famous for their gladiatorial battles with lions. Egyptian pharaohs coveted the right to kill lions. And Maasai, Nandi, and other “spear hunting” tribes expressed their strength and skill by hunting lions. In addition to the practical importance of protecting livestock and people from harm, the killing of lions emerged as a social symbol of bravery and domination over nature. Despite its once greater range, it was not Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 8 long before the lion became almost uniquely associated with the African continent (excepting the one remaining population of lions in the Gir Sanctuary of India). With the advent of advanced weaponry and increasing Western appropriation of African lands in the late 1800s and early 1900s, symbolic representations of lions remained bathed in demonstrations of power, although they were dichotomized by political lineage. For Westerners, the killing of a lion indicated both mastery over the natural world, and more subtly, control over the African continent and people who were often at the mercy of hunters to shoot marauding lions. Brian Nicholson, a famous and well-respected hunter, notes the panic created by man-eating lions in southern Tanzania and the desperate call for help he received from the district commissioner: In April 1951, I was walking to a place called Gumbiro, just north of Songea, some 250 miles west of Liwale. Complaints of a lion there were becoming almost hysterical; repeated telegrams from the district commissioner at Songea reached me with every mail tarishi (messenger). According to these telegrams, the lion had taken one hundred people and had evaded all attempts to either hunt it down or trap it, and the district commissioner demanded my personal intervention in this affair. (Nicholson 2001: 167). From this tradition, rooted in colonial intervention in African landscapes, the “noble lion” evoked symbols of bravery, nobility, courage, and majesty. Not surprisingly, the lion was Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 9 taken as the royal emblem of England, perhaps the most successful of the colonial empires. On the contrary, symbolic representations of the lion among African communities often evoked images of murder, violence, and destruction. During the colonial period, this was a means of political symbolism representing local rebellion against foreign powers. For example, in Zambia, the Bisa people distinguished between three types of lions: (1) the ordinary lion (nkalamo) characterized by timidity and minimal interactions with humans, (2) the spiritual lion (mipashi ya chialo) that acted as a guardian and monitored the socio-political landscape, and (3) the imperial lion (nkalamo ya kutuma) that appeared during social crises, attacked pre-determined people, and represented the exploitation of one by another (Marks 1984). During the colonial period, it is not difficult to guess who might have been the imperial lions. Nevertheless, superstitions about man-eating lions went far beyond colonial influences and were rampant throughout Africa. In Tanzania, many people believed the man-eater was actually a reincarnation of a person who was wronged in life and therefore had returned to punish his tormentors. Or, in other cases, families that were persecuted by individuals or communities were believed to possess magical powers enabling them to send lions to kill particular people in acts of revenge (Nicholson 2001). In fact, many of these beliefs remain common in Tanzania. Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 10 Today, the eyes of the lion reflect both the past and the future. Local communities still dwell in that primal and fearsome realm of the lion where loss of human life or livestock remains a likelihood. Yet, while the lion will always engender a degree of fear among most clearheaded, life-respecting individuals who come in contact with the powerful cat, this is tempered by contemporary relationships with lions that emphasize the more positive aspects of human – lion relationships. For example, the lion was recently portrayed in popular culture via Disney’s The Lion King as a familiar and admirable character for children in the cartoon movie and even adults as evidenced by the hit Broadway musical. All over the world, statues of lions can be found guarding the entrances of people’s private compounds suggesting that we are now inviting our former rival into our homes to protect us. And, as world travel has become increasingly fashionable, tourism has promoted the lion as a key species in both the sport hunting and the photographic industries. Certainly, throughout the history of mankind, the African lion has been established as a powerful cultural and social entity immortalized in art, literature, mythology, religion, and the individual histories of those human cultures sharing a common landscape with them. Importantly, in all of these cases, our fascination with lions and the intensity with which we perceive them are intimately tied to the knowledge that the real, wild animal still confronts, threatens, terrifies, and astounds us in an everyday sense. In this way, as both natural and cultural entities, the fates of the lion are irrevocably intertwined. And from this realization, a difficult question arises. If we benefit so greatly from inviting lions into the human sphere, deriving from their lives growth, creativity, and inspiration, Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 11 how can we simultaneously watch them vanish from the natural world we share with them? The lion’s destiny, in an ecological sense, depends on the ability of this large carnivore to meet its physical and nutritional requirements in a world that is increasingly carved up by human communities. How well can the lion, a social creature dependent on large territories and abundant prey, survive in a mosaic of human-altered habitat? The answer to this question depends as much on the ecological resilience and adaptability of lions as it does on the tolerance of those individuals interacting with them. This means we must ensure that the cultural and societal benefits of maintaining wild lions outweighs the costs of conserving one of the world’s most dangerous cats. In this manner, the fate of the African lion is intimately tied to a diverse range of ecological and social factors affecting both lions and the key stakeholders encountering them (Figure 1). In the field of conservation biology, social factors are referred to as the human dimensions of lion management and conservation. Broadly defined, human dimensions refer to the beliefs, attitudes, values, and behaviors people exhibit toward natural resources as well as the socioeconomic, demographic, and organizational characteristics of those individuals (Jacobson 1998). In terms of lions, this includes dynamic interactions of contextually specific socio-cultural, political, economic, and psychological factors influencing the persistence of lions in increasingly humandominated landscapes. Understanding these elements and the ways in which they influence local communities and other individuals interacting with lions is extremely Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 12 Figure 1.1 A Schematic Diagram of Human-Lion Relationships. Ecosystem Human Dimension Political Factors Socio-Cultural Factors + Stakeholders Psychological Factors Lion Population Tolerance for Lions and Prey Species Economic Factors Prey Population important in terms of predicting people’s actions toward the big cats. Nevertheless, Saberwal and Kothari (1996) suggest that a focus on human dimensions is particularly lacking in the developing countries where lions still prowl. With this in mind, the goal of my dissertation is to evaluate the social and ecological factors influencing the future of the wild lion by conducting a case study of human – lion relationships among key stakeholders within the Tarangire ecosystem of northern Tanzania. In the following three chapters, I define and examine the primary variables underlying these social and ecological factors (summarized in Table 1.1) in terms of the degree to which they influence human – lion relationships in the twenty-first century. Specifically, in Chapter Two, I conduct a comparative analysis of attitudes toward lions held by the primary human constituencies interacting with and affecting lions in the Tarangire ecosystem: the professional sport hunting industry, the photographic tourism Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 13 Table 1.1 Potential key ecological and social variables influencing the survival of lion populations. Ecological Population abundance Reproductive ecology Habitat availability/use Prey availability Behavioral ecology Disease Political Economic Property rights Wildlife benefits Land use relations Wildlife costs Resource use/control Incentives Wildlife policies/law Market forces Authority/responsibility Socio-Cultural Wildlife Values Human-lion interactions Traditions/customs Knowledge Psychological Risk Fear Affection Wonder/Awe Curiosity Aesthetic Attraction Respect industry, and the Maasai communities living on the outskirts of Tarangire National Park. Through detailed and comprehensive interviews with key informants, I discover important similarities and differences in the stakeholders’ attitudes toward lions and the primary political, economic, and socio-cultural variables influencing their relationships with the large cats. My emphasis on wildlife values also partially reflects the psychological dimension by emphasizing evaluative (values-based) psychological dispositions. And, I highlight important distinctions in people’s relationships with lions, particularly between the wildlife tourism industries and Maasai communities, that arise as a result of their relative freedom to experience lions on their own terms. Given the unique, ecological reality of the Maasai, in comparison to the rest of the stakeholders, of living with lions, I next explore how daily cohabitation with these dangerous cats affects the Maasai in a physical and psychological sense. Primarily based on a large, structured survey of Maasai villagers, in Chapter Three I compare Maasai perceptions of and interactions with lions that shape their daily awareness of lions and actions toward them. More specifically, I consider the degree to which the risk of living with lions, in terms of both perceived and actual losses of livestock or human lives, affects the Maasai psyche as well as their overall acceptance and tolerance of lions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 14 Therefore, this study focuses on cognitive (knowledge-based) and affective (emotionbased) psychological dispositions. Having considered the major social and ecological factors contributing to human relationships and conflicts with lions in the Tarangire ecosystem, in Chapter Four I shift my focus to an ecological perspective and consider the lion’s ability to negotiate a matrix of multiple land uses resulting from Maasai, photographic, and sport hunting activities in the ecosystem. Using spoor counts as the principal methodology, I estimate lion population density in three study areas inside and outside of Tarangire National Park and question whether popular Maasai perceptions of abundant lion populations are correct. In addition, I determine the primary ecological variables influencing the persistence of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem and the relative effects of professional sport hunting and Maasai villagers on lion survivorship. The concluding chapter focuses on the implications of these studies in terms of the future of human – lion relationships in the twenty-first century. I return to the variables provided in Table 1.1 and emphasize those that emerged as having a strong influence on the future of coexistence between humans and lions. Based on my findings, I provide a theoretical model of human-lion relationships that pushes Figure 1.1 to reflect a deeper understanding of the truly interwoven relationships between humans and lions. And finally, I provide a number of policy suggestions at local, national, and international levels in an attempt to link the improved management and conservation of lions with increased human well-being in landscapes dominated by people and lions alike. Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 15 References Baldus, R. 2004. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man-eating lion killing 35 people. GTZ Wildlife Program, Wildlife Division, Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Bath, A. J. 1989. The public and wolf reintroduction in Yellowstone National Park. Society and Natural Resources. 2: 297-306. Conforti, V. A., and F. C. Cascelli de Azevedo. 2003. Local perceptions of jaguars (Panthera onca) and pumas (Puma concolor) in the Iguacu National Park area, south Brazil. Biological Conservation. 111: 215-221. Eaton, R. L. 1979. Evolution of sociality in the Felidae. Carnivore 2: Supplement 82-89. Harrington, F. and P. Paquet, eds. 1982. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Hook, R. and W. Robinson, 1982. Attitudes of Michigan citizens toward predators. Pages 382-394 in F. Harrington and P. Paquet, eds. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Jacobson, S. K. 1998. Training idiot savants: The lack of human dimensions in conservation biology. Conservation Biology. 12(2): 263-267. Jope, K. and B. Shelby. 1984. Hiker behavior and the outcome of interactions with grizzly bears. Leisure Sciences 6: 257-270. Kellert, S. R. 1985. Public perception of predators, particularly the wolf and the coyote. Biological Conservation. 31: 167-189. Kellert, S. R. 1986. The public and the timber wolf in Minnesota. Transactions of the 51st North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference. Wildlife Management Institute, Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. 1991. Public views of wolf restoration in Michigan. Transactions of the 56th North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference. Wildlife Management Institute, Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. 1992. Public attitudes toward bears and their conservation. Pages 293 – 305 in J. J. Clear, C. Servheen, and L. J. Lyons, eds. Ninth International Bear Conference, Grenoble, France. Kellert, S. R. 1996. The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Island Press, Washington, D. C. Lichtenfeld – Chapter One 16 Kellert, S. R., M. Black, C. R. Rush, and A. J Bath. 1996. Human culture and large carnivore conservation in North America. Conservation Biology 10(4): 977-990. Leopold, A. 1949. A Sand County almanac. Oxford University Press, New York, New York. Lopez, B. 1978. Of wolves and men. Charles Scribner’s and Sons, New York, New York. Marean, C. W. 1989. Sabertooth cats and their relevance for early hominid diet evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 18: 559-582. Marks, S. A. 1984. The imperial lion: Human dimensions of wildlife management in Central Africa. Westview Press, Boulder, Colorado. Matthiessen, P. 1987. Wildlife in America. Viking Press, New York, New York. Mattson, D. J., S. Herrero, R. G. Wright, and C. M. Pease. 1996. Science and management of Rocky Mountain Grizzly Bears. Conservation Biology 10(4): 10131025. Nicholson, B. 2001. The last of old Africa: Big-game hunting in East Africa. Safari Press Inc., Long Beach, California. Nowell and P. Jackson, eds. 1996. Wild cats: Status survey and conservation action plan. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. O’Brien, S. J. 1996. Molecular genetics and phylogenetics of the Felidae. Pages xxiiixxiv in K. Nowell and P. Jackson, eds. Wild cats: Status survey and conservation action plan. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. Ridley, M. 1993. Evolution. Blackwell Science, Inc., Cambridge, Massachusetts. Rockwell, D. 1991. Giving voice to the bear: North American Indian myths, rituals, and images of the bear. Robers Rinehart Publishers, Niwot, Colorado. Saberwal, V. K. and A. Kothari. 1996. The human dimension in Conservation Biology curricula in developing countries. Conservation Biology 10(5): 1328-1331. Shepard, P. and B. Sanders. 1985. The sacred paw: The bear in nature, myth, and literature. Viking Penguin Inc, Toronto, Canada. Treves, A. and L. Naughton-Treves. 1999. Risk and opportunity for humans coexisting with large carnivores. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 275-282. CHAPTER TWO A Comparative Analysis of Attitudes Toward Lions Among the Professional Sport Hunting Industry, Photographic Tourism Industry, and Maasai Communities in the Tarangire ecosystem, Tanzania Chapter Two The relationships of human beings to large carnivores are extremely significant features influencing the survival of these species. Today, few factors affecting the persistence of large carnivores fail to have a human component whether the species in question is a cougar, wolf, jaguar, grizzly bear, black bear, tiger, snow leopard, or lion. Throughout human societies, large carnivores are a constant source of interest, respect, and intrigue as well as of fear, aversion, and dislike. For example, tigers are revered as cultural icons, as one of the pinnacle sport hunting species, as possessing medicinal properties, and yet, they are feared as relentless man-eaters (Weber and Rabinowitz 1996; Quammen 2003). Large carnivores are viewed as symbols of wilderness to some and of destruction to others. They increasingly represent freedom and wildness, although they still compete with people for game species, livestock, and occasionally take human lives. Indeed, they are the only group of species for which either eradication or conservation is a persistent objective of the people interacting with them. As a result, conservation or management programs involving these species are complex and often highly controversial; to be successful, they must consider both the biological and social factors influencing large carnivores. Nevertheless, traditional conservation approaches to conflicts with these species tend to focus on the ecological causes and end results of human – carnivore interactions without fully appreciating the diverse interplay of social and ecological forces driving the outcomes. For example, wolf reintroduction in Michigan in the 1970s failed when all the released animals were killed Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 19 because the attitudes of local residents toward wolves were not considered (Hook and Robinson 1982). Similarly, the relatively unsuccessful experimental release of cougars in Florida focused on panther ecology and behavior rather than on developing institutional and local support for the program (Alvarez 1994). Given this history, policy solutions to carnivore conservation and management problems often fall short of their mark (Clark et al. 2001). Kellert and Clark (1991) provide a more comprehensive model of wildlife policy that suggests that effective wildlife management depends upon recognition of the interplay between science, values, and politics. Therefore, they assert that effective conservation policies depend not only on biophysical or ecological forces but also on a thorough consideration of institutional, regulatory, social, and valuational forces impacting all of the involved human constituencies and their goals. Since goals are based on the individual views of each stakeholder, it is important to evaluate the attitudes of each stakeholder in relation to the conservation problem (Kellert 1996). Applying this model to large carnivores suggests that a consideration of the human dimensions underlying most large carnivore conflicts should be a primary objective of research and conservation programs focused on these species. Nevertheless, even in the case of the African lion, arguably the most well-studied large carnivore in the world, surprisingly little research addresses human attitudes toward lions or the social factors influencing human – lion relationships. This is partly due to the fact that many scientists involved with lions have been trained in the biological rather Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 20 than the social sciences, and therefore, lack the capacity to thoroughly address this issue. Yet, it may also reflect the increased complexity of considering the socioeconomic aspects of large carnivore conservation in developing countries where conservation objectives must be reconciled with the resource dependencies and practical needs of local people (Kellert 1985b). Kellert (1992: 296) defines attitudes toward wildlife as “resulting from four interrelated factors…[including] basic wildlife values, perceptions of a particular species, knowledge and understanding of wildlife, and people – animal interactions.” A typology of nine basic wildlife values was developed by Kellert (1996). Definitions of each value are provided in Table 2.1. In addition to these wildlife values which broadly influence how people view a particular species, perceptions of a species are shaped by factors such as its size, level of intelligence, relationship with humans, and the potential danger or threat it poses to humans. People’s knowledge of a species may also contribute to the formation of their attitudes, though more often by reinforcing them rather than changing them altogether (Kellert 1992). Finally, the nature of human interactions with wildlife will often affect their attitudes toward a species. A complete list of factors influencing human perceptions, knowledge, and interactions with carnivores is provided in Table 2.2. This chapter focuses on attitudes toward lions among the Maasai communities and wildlife tourism industries utilizing land outside the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park. All of these constituencies play important roles in influencing the future of lion conservation and management in this ecosystem, and more broadly, in Tanzania as a Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 21 whole. However, little information exists regarding their attitudes toward lions or the conditions necessary for lions and humans to coexist in a shared landscape. Table 2.1 Basic wildlife values adapted from Kellert (1996). Value Naturalistic Definition Personal pleasure and satisfaction derived from contact with wildlife in natural settings Scientific Emphasizes empirical study and understanding of wildlife Aesthetic Focuses on the physical attraction and appeal of wild animals Utilitarian Practical and material dependence on wild animals and their habitats Humanistic Emotional affinity for wildlife species Dominionistic Emphasis on the mastery and control of wildlife Moralistic Ethical and moral responsibility for conserving and protecting wild animals Negativistic Avoidance of wildlife due to indifference, dislike, or fear. Symbolic Representation of wild animals as a source of human communication and expression Table 2.2 Factors influencing human perceptions, knowledge, and interactions with carnivores adapted from Kellert (1992). Perceptions Size of animal Presumed intelligence Cultural and historic relationship Perceived dangerousness Likelihood of inflicting damage Morphology Mode of locomotion Knowledge Factual understanding Ecological knowledge Awareness of conservation issues Interactions Conservation status Types of conflicts Types of utilization Land use relationships Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 22 Methods Study Area Tarangire National Park is part of an impressive network of protected areas that encompasses approximately 40% of the total land area of Tanzania (926,283 km2) in legally protected status (Earth Trends 2003). This system includes several different types of protected areas including national parks (14; 39,000 km2), game reserves (31; 120,000 km2), game controlled areas (25; 107,000 km2), forest reserves (570; 87,000 km2) and the Ngorongoro Conservation Area (8,300 km2). Additionally, approximately 70,000 km of unprotected land is designated as tourist hunting blocks in open areas (Baldus 2004a). National parks and game reserves in Tanzania receive the highest level of protection. No human settlement is allowed within these areas, and land uses are primarily restricted to wildlife-based tourism. In the game reserves, sport hunting is the chief land use (limited photographic tourism is permitted in some cases, for example, in the Selous Game Reserve). On the other hand, hunting is strictly prohibited in the national parks. The Ngorongoro Conservation Area receives similar protection, however Maasai communities also inhabit the area together with their livestock. Game controlled areas support both professional sport hunting and limited resident and citizen hunting. In addition, permanent settlements, livestock grazing, and farming are permitted in game controlled areas. Open areas generally incorporate local villages as well as a wide variety of land uses including sport hunting, resident and citizen hunting, photographic tourism, livestock grazing and cultivation. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 23 Ownership of wildlife in Tanzania belongs to the government under the auspices of the Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism. All wildlife species are protected under the Wildlife Conservation Act (1974) and may not be killed without the permission of the Director of Wildlife. The only exception to this is made in situations where an individual or his property is threatened by a wild animal. The Wildlife Division, a department of the Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism, governs all wildlife outside of national parks. Within national parks, the Tanzanian National Parks Authority (TANAPA), a parastatal organization, is responsible for wildlife. Despite central control over wildlife, land law in Tanzania gives significant rights regarding the management of village lands to local communities. Based on the Land Act of 1999 and the Village Land Act of 1999, village authorities maintain ownership of village land and the right to manage the land on behalf of the local community. Institutional confusion arises when government-authorized wildlife concessions, particularly in game controlled areas and open areas with hunting blocks, overlap with village lands. In such cases, jurisdictional conflict occurs as village and central government authorities compete over land-use allocations (Nshala 2002). The establishment of the legal right for local communities to manage and utilize wildlife, thereby eliminating this controversy, is one objective of the revised wildlife policy of Tanzania (MNRT 1998). However, local authority over community wildlife management areas has yet to be granted. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 24 In northern Tanzania, the Tarangire ecosystem (12,000 km2) represents a microcosm of Tanzania’s protected area network (Figure 2.1). It includes two national parks, Tarangire National Park (2600 km2) and Lake Manyara National Park (330 km2), the Lolkisale Game Controlled Area, the Simanjiro Game Controlled Areas (West, Naberera, and Kitangare) and Maasai Open Areas (East and West). The two national parks act as core protected areas where wildlife find refuge, particularly in the dry season. In the rainy season, most migratory wildlife such as zebra and wildebeest move out of the parks in search of better grazing, giving the parks a much needed rest. Wildlife migrating out of Tarangire National Park predominantly head east where they encounter a mosaic of hunting and village lands and individuals from the sport hunting and photographic tourism industries as well as local community members. As such, Tarangire National Park and the land to the east represent an area where multiple land uses, several protected and non-protected area designations, dual wildlife authorities, and a variety of private stakeholders are interconnected within a single landscape inhabited by lions. Stakeholders Maasai Communities While archeological evidence indicates that pastoralists have inhabited northern Tanzania for thousands of years, the Maasai are considered to have gained a stronghold in the area during the mid-nineteenth century after driving out the Tatog people (Arhem 1984; Adams and McShane 1996). Historically, their pastoral livelihood, predominantly centered around cattle, depended on a complex range management system that necessitated a semi-permanent lifestyle while moving livestock in search of water and Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 25 Figure 2.1 The Tarangire Ecosystem Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 26 mineral-rich grazing resources (Homewood and Rodgers 1991). During the 1900s, however, a number of these grazing lands were set aside for conservation areas, including the Tarangire Game Reserve in 1957. This disrupted Maasai traditional grazing schemes, for example removing access to important water sources such as the Tarangire River and was partially responsible for changes in their land-use system, eventually leading to the settlement of permanent homesteads (LEAT 1998). Other factors creating land pressures included the post-independence, socialist policies of Julius Nyerere that promoted the consolidation of human communities within registered villages, overall increases in the Maasai population, and the immigration of agriculturists into Maasai areas as a result of pro-agricultural development policies (Muir 1994). The Maasai living outside the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park belong to the Kisongo Maasai, the largest of the twenty sections of Maasai people ranging throughout Kenya and Tanzania. All of the Maasai villages located along the eastern edge of the park are located in Simanjiro district, excepting Lolkisale, which is in Monduli district. Despite increased human immigration into these areas, the villages are characterized by relatively low population density. In Manyara region, which includes Simanjiro district, the population density is estimated at 23 people/km2 (Tanzania Population and Housing Census 2002). However, this estimate includes the entire region and areas of high human habitation close to the urban center of Arusha. Muir (1994) estimated the Simanjiro district population density to be 3.4 people/km2 and the average village population size of Emboret, Loibor Serrit, and Narakauo to be 2400 people/village based on 1988 census data. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 27 Today, the Maasai in these villages practice a combination of semi-nomadic pastoralism, moving livestock from their permanent households to seasonal grazing lands and water sources when necessary, as well as subsistence and small-scale commercial agriculture (Lama 1998; Muir 1994). The size of household cattle herds has declined over the last thirty years due to the combined effects of increasing human populations, reduced grazing land availability, an increase in disease outbreaks among livestock, and the integration of the Maasai into the cash economy, resulting in more animals being sold at market. Muir (1994) estimates between 1984 and 1994 cattle herds in Simanjiro decreased approximately 10.5% from an average of 3.46 cattle per capita to 3.10 mean cattle per capita. Conversely, small stock herds (goats and sheep) have increased suggesting an economic shift as a result of declining cattle herds and/or the greater resilience of these animals under drought conditions. Reduced cattle herds and an increasingly sedentary lifestyle have prompted the Maasai to adopt cultivation as a means of providing some food security while reducing the need to sell cattle to meet their nutritional requirements. A map of agricultural areas in the Tarangire ecosystem is provided in Figure 2.2. Maize and beans are the primary crops grown, usually a single rotation per year, but in years of good rainfall, two crops may be planted in some areas. Today, virtually all Maasai households practice some degree of cultivation with the majority of plots only large enough to meet subsistence needs. For example, Lama (1998) determined that by 1998, in the village of Loibor Soit, 84.4% of the population cultivated for subsistence purposes on plots of less than 12 hectares (average 5.6 hectares) for a total of 345 hectares. Larger, commercially- Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 28 Figure 2.2 The location of agricultural areas in the Tarangire Ecosystem. producing plots (ranging from 12 to 1200 hectares) in Loibor Soit were generally cultivated by a minority of wealthy Maasai, non-Maasai resident farmers such as the Waarusha, and expatriate farmers, representing 3,994 total hectares. It is notable that 5 expatriate farmers and one non-resident farmer acquired approximately 3600 hectares of that total. With the village land of Loibor Soit estimated at 75,680 hectares by Lama (1998), 6% of the total village land appeared under cultivation in 1998. While subsistence level farming appears relatively low in land area, the increase in large scale commercial farms, representing 2.5% of Simanjiro district in 1993 in 72 farms (45,000 hectares) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 29 threatens to place even greater pressure on remaining grazing lands, herding patterns, and access to water (Muir 1994). Sport Hunting Industry Since the early 1900s, sport hunting in Tanzania has attracted wealthy clients in pursuit of wildlife trophies. Over the last century, the hunting industry in Tanzania has developed into a multimillion dollar trade, earning an estimated $27 million in 2001 with $10 million accumulated directly by the Wildlife Division (Baldus 2004b). Currently in Tanzania, 64 wildlife species (not including game birds) are licensed for hunting in over 130 hunting concessions located in game reserves, game controlled areas, and open areas. Approximately 40 hunting companies are registered in Tanzania. While a number of game species are also permitted for hunting by residents and citizens of Tanzania, licenses for trophy species such as lion, leopard, elephant, and zebra are only allocated to the professional sport hunting industry. Hunting concessions or blocks in Tanzania are leased to professional hunting companies for $7500 per year for a period of five years after which the companies must then bid for re-allocation and/or other blocks. Each concession is given an annual quota of animals that may be periodically readjusted by the Wildlife Division. The hunting season is from July through December. Hunting clients are required to be accompanied by a licensed professional hunter associated with a hunting company and a game scout provided by the Wildlife Division. Additionally, a gun-bearer, tracker(s), skinner(s) and a driver may be present, although they are not required. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 30 Lion hunting is an important component of the sport hunting industry. Baldus and Cauldwell (2004) estimate that lion trophy fees are responsible for approximately 9.4% of hunting revenue, despite the fact that lion quotas are lower than most other species. In 2004, lion trophy fees were set at $2000 per animal. This is expected to increase by 25% in 2005. In order to hunt a lion in Tanzania, a client must purchase a 21-day hunting permit. The trophy fee, permit fee ($600), and a conservation fee of $100 per hunter per day must be paid to the Wildlife Division regardless of the length of the actual hunt or whether a lion was eventually shot. While the government sets these fees, individual hunting companies often set their own rates above and beyond these standards based on the quality of their lion trophies (e.g. bigger, fuller manes or dark manes). If a lion is shot, an export permit ($300) must then be obtained to transport the trophy to the client’s country of residence. Hunting of lions in Tanzania is regulated by various rules established to ensure a “fair chase.” For instance, it is prohibited to shoot a lion within 200 meters of a vehicle, 2 km of a national park boundary, or 500 meters of permanent water. In addition, it is not permitted to hunt lions at night (½ hour after sundown until ½ hour before sunrise) or with any type of night-vision equipment or lights. Lions must be shot with a .375 caliber rifle or greater, and the use of poisons, dogs, horses, or any other aids is not allowed. While baiting of lions is officially illegal in Tanzania according to the Wildlife Conservation Act of 1974, it is practiced widely and is sanctioned by the Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism. In general, the professional sport hunter is responsible for choosing acceptable lion trophies for his client and for assisting the client throughout Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 31 the hunt. If a lion is wounded, it is the professional hunter’s responsibility to pursue it despite the inherent danger of the chase. Hunting of immature animals in Tanzania is prohibited as is the hunting of lionesses. Certain, select hunting companies impose additional restrictions on their professional hunters in terms of suitable lion trophies; for example, they may set a minimum allowable age or discourage the hunting of males belonging to a pride. Outside the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park and within the area that is utilized for this study, the Wildlife Division has allocated four blocks for professional sport hunting within the Lolkisale Game Controlled Area, Simanjiro West Game Controlled Area, Simanjiro Kitangare Game Controlled Area, and Maasai West Open Area (the latter three areas and the ecosystem as a whole contain several more hunting blocks, but they are outside the defined study system). Three companies lease these four concession areas (one company owns two blocks) within the study area. Among the four blocks, the total estimated lion quota is 15 lions per year. Photographic Tourism Industry The photographic tourism industry has also grown steadily in Tanzania over the last 100 years. Following massive developments in international air travel after the Second World War, tourism has flourished from an estimated 25 million travelers in 1950 to 450 million world travelers in 1991 (Roe et al. 1997). In Tanzania, as national parks were created and in some cases converted from game reserves (e.g. the Serengeti Game Reserve was gazetted as a national park in 1951), the country became increasingly Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 32 regarded as an exciting destination for individuals interested in viewing wild animals in staggering numbers and dramatic landscapes. Today, the Serengeti National Park, perhaps the most famous protected area in the world, draws tourists from all over the globe to view its impressive herds of herbivores and their predatory counterparts. The most common photographic safari in Tanzania today is based on wildlife viewing experienced in a core circuit of national parks. Parks offer a reasonably good chance of seeing target species like lions and are attractive to tourists who expect to see predators, animals in large numbers, and wildlife relatively close-up. In Tanzania, the most popular tourist circuit involves visiting the Ngorongoro Crater Conservation Area and the Serengeti National Park, usually complemented by overnights in Tarangire National Park, Lake Manyara National Park and/or a trek up Mount Kilimanjaro. Within the parks, visitors must follow strict regulations regarding no off-road driving, adherence to speed limits, game viewing by vehicle only (i.e. no walking safaris, except in speciallypermitted circumstances and, more commonly, in the less congested southern parks), and other rules designed to ensure the safety of both the traveler and the wildlife. Despite the popularity of these park tours, the photographic industry in Tanzania has diversified considerably over the last decade as tourist numbers flourished. For example, visitors to Tarangire National Park increased from 15,716 in 1989-90 to 63,031 in 200203, earning the park an estimated $1,528,215 in entrance fees alone (TANAPA unpublished data cited in Nelson 2004). In response to the rapid growth in the industry during the ‘90s resulting in the occasional over-crowding of some parks at peak periods Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 33 and increasing demand for more active and adventuresome trips, many companies have established tented camps or lodges on the outside of national parks. In these areas, their clients enjoy a wider range of activities, for example walking safaris, night drives, horseback riding, or mountain biking, in an exclusive and often more private setting. In fact, the growth in commercial tourism ventures outside of parks has been so considerable that TANAPA has recently begun to permit limited walking in northern parks (e.g. Oliver’s camp in Tarangire National Park), canoeing on Lake Manyara, and special night drives in Manyara for an added fee, most likely in hopes of reclaiming some of the revenue that has been lost to areas outside the parks. Outside the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park, two photographic lodges (one of which is under construction) and three permanent tented camps have been built based on formal agreements made with the local villages upon whose land the camps have been located (according to the Village Land Act of 1999). In addition, several tourism companies utilize seasonal, non-permanent camps in the area for walking and educational tours. Because some of these camps are also located in the Lolkisale Game Controlled Area, a concession of the central government where photographic safaris are prohibited unless authorized by the Director of Wildlife (MNRT 2000), a considerable degree of institutional confusion surrounds the legality of these camps. As we will see, this confusion regarding land rights has important consequences in terms of people’s relationships to lions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 34 Data Collection The data presented in this chapter focus on explorations of human attitudes toward lions among the professional sport hunting industry, photographic industry, and Maasai communities. Given the inherent complexity of human feelings for and experiences with lions, often resulting in both positive and negative attitudes toward these animals depending upon the nature of individual encounters, I use comprehensive interviews as the data collection tool. A qualitative methodology, interviewing allows for detailed probing of human feelings, attitudes, and opinions on a variety of subjects. In Chapter Three, I then complement this data by evaluating the effects of actual and perceived interactions with lions on the Maasai psyche with a larger, quantitative structured survey. Together, these chapters provide a thorough understanding of the major social and ecological factors contributing to human conflicts with lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. Topics covered in the interview included (1) the importance and meaning of lions to individual stakeholders, (2) general knowledge of lions and lion conservation, (3) views on consumptive and non-consumptive utilization of lions, (4) perceptions of local, village interactions with lions, (5) Tanzanian wildlife policy and lions, and (6) the perceived compatibility of different land use activities and their effects on lions. While the use of the same interview questions between all respondents allows for a comparative treatment of the data, the semi-structured nature of interviewing permits individuals to add their own emphases and to clarify their responses resulting in an in-depth understanding of the subject matter. A sample of the interview questions is provided in Appendix One. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 35 Thirty-three individuals from the sport hunting industry (n=11), photographic tourism industry (n=11), and Maasai communities (n=11) were interviewed between June 2004 and January 2005. On average, interviews were between one and two hours long. All interviews were tape recorded and then transcribed. Maasai interviews were conducted in Swahili, so transcription of these interviews also involved their translation into English. Key informant interviews were conducted with individuals considered to have extensive insight into, experience with, and/or potential influence on lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. Criteria for selection among the sport hunting and photographic tourism industries included active company or individual presence in the ecosystem. In a few cases, general knowledge of the area and extensive insight into lion conservation issues in Tanzania were accepted as reasonable criteria for individuals who had a long history of involvement with the photographic or hunting industries but were not presently active in the ecosystem. This was particularly necessary in the case of the sport hunting industry given the small number of companies located within the study area. The majority of the informants comprised professional hunters or photographic guides and/or company owners. Additionally, the availability of informants was important in the selection process; only individuals living in Arusha were interviewed. As most companies working in the northern sector base themselves out of Arusha, this was considered a reasonable strategy. Among Maasai communities, the village executive officers and village chairmen were targeted as key informants. This was partially due to their status as respected, usually elder, community members. In addition, they were considered knowledgeable Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 36 about local interactions with lions as well as recent wildlife policies impacting local communities. Table 2.3 summarizes characteristics of the key informants as a whole. To assure anonymity, interviews are referred to by number and do not necessarily reflect the order in which they were conducted (SH 1-11: sport hunting industry, PC 1-11: photographic tourism industry, and M 1-11: Maasai communities). Finally, direct quotes are used to illustrate general opinions and do not represent extreme views unless so indicated. Table 2.3 Summary of interviewee characteristics. Maasai Communities Total # of Villages Chairman Village Executive Officer Village/Ward Representative N 6 5 4 2 Sport Hunting Industry Total # of Companies Owner/Prof Hunter Professional Hunter Company Representative N 8 4 4 3 Photographic N Industry Total # of Companies 10 Owner/Guide 7 Guide 3 Company Representative 1 Results Values of Lions Maasai values of lions exhibited strong symbolic, dominionistic, and negativistic tendencies. The lion was simultaneously described as “shujaa,” a brave man, warrior, hero, or champion, and as an “adui,” an enemy, foe, or opponent. All respondents referred to the spearing of a lion by a Maasai murran (warrior) as an event that denotes great respect and pride among the community, portraying the courage of the individuals involved in the hunt who “measure [their] strength against the lion” (M5). M1 notes Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 37 proudly, “It is not every young man that can wait for a lion until he is close enough to hunt him with a spear.” Not only in the killing of a lion does a murran “appear as a brave young man that can save livestock and can also save humans, if and when there are dangerous animals attacking them,” (M1), but he also emerges as an individual capable of defending his community against other humans. In this way, the lion is recast with human dimensions, personified as a worthy human adversary: “We, as Maasai murranis, take the lion as one of us to fight and to cause battles with his fellow murrani” (M6). The hunt of the lion and the celebration if victorious - when both the lion is dead and no people have been injured in the process - are imbued with symbolic meaning. M3 describes a traditional lion hunt: So, [the murranis] were going as a group of 10 or 20, they went to search for him, to search the areas where lions like to hide and areas where they feed….they scare it out, and if the lion runs, he might only run…let’s say 200 meters. So when he reaches 200 meters, that’s where the lion will wait for the people, they have made him angry, so now they, the Maasai community, will circle him, and when they have surrounded him they sing to him. There’s a certain traditional song that they sing to him, and once they have sung to him, the lion looks for a place to run, he looks for a place…where there is a coward. He will know because he is a vitisho (an overwhelming danger, a menace, a terror) that if maybe he goes this way, if this guy Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 38 is startled, he could go through there, and break through. But because of the fahari (pride, glory, magnificence) of the murranis, now, the place that he penetrates, that is where they will spear him, and [the murran] that is before the others, he is the one that will receive sifa (praise, commendation, fame), and the second one. Only the first two get praise. (M3) Following the hunt, the victorious murranis dance and sing in celebration. The two individuals who drove the first and second spears into the lion receive the most glory and are allowed to dance with their weapons in their right hands, and in their left hands, the first murran carries the lion’s tail and has ashes or lime coated on his legs and back, while the second murran holds a front paw. They also skin the head of the lion and dance with it. However, if the lion manages to injure someone, “then it’s the lion that has won, and there is no reason to celebrate” (M4). Lion manes are collected regardless of personal injury and worn proudly at the most significant celebration in the life of a warrior, the Eunoto ceremony. Once a warrior passes beyond into elderhood and considers the prospects of marrying, he will no longer be permitted to wear the headdress as it is only associated with the wild and promiscuous days of warriorhood. While lion hunting acts as a positive means of asserting the dominance and power of the Maasai murran and is an important source of song, dance, and celebration within the community, the sheer presence of lions carries with it a negativistic value for the Maasai who feel both their wealth, in terms of livestock, and their lives to be in endangered by lions. All the respondents indicated a fear of losing livestock or being harmed by a lion, Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 39 and lions were frequently referred to as a destructive, kali (dangerous, cruel, fierce) animal that brings hofu (fear, apprehension). Lions cause anxiety for herdsman, and M1 notes, “during the season that there are lots of lions, I find I have no sleepiness, I do not sleep well at night in order to make sure that my livestock are safe.” Some individuals indicated that this negativism was increasingly countered by an emerging utilitarian value of lions based on wildlife tourism, since “nowadays the lion is seen as having benefits but a long time ago the lion was nothing but an enemy” (M5). However, as will be shown, the actual degree to which local people benefit from lion tourism is unclear. Individuals from the sport hunting industry exhibited a somewhat different variety of values of lions than the Maasai including aesthetic, symbolic, dominionistic, utilitarian, humanistic, and moralistic tendencies. Lions were consistently referred to as beautiful, powerful, majestic creatures – the King of Beasts - that represent the wildness of Africa: The lion has got this, its got this turn on, its got this attraction. You know, it’s a beautiful, it’s a magnificent, it is the King of Beasts. Everybody is attracted by this phenomenal animal. (SH 4) To hear [a lion] in the morning, like when you are walking to a blind in early morning, is the greatest sound there is, for me, I mean that is what Africa is. (SH 2) Africa without lion wouldn’t be Africa. (SH 7) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 40 Part of the importance of that image, for sport hunters, is related to the danger of hunting wild animals that triggers a latent, atavistic fear among individuals who no longer live in constant threat of these species. SH 8 explains that there is a deeper meaning to hunting than perhaps some individuals might expect, in a sense returning us to our roots for “we come from wild places.” He continues: To me, it is important that there are wild places with wild animals and things that are dangerous to us. There is some value to that, to our psychology, to who we are and how we operate, to know that we don’t necessarily have to live with the danger everyday, but to know that there are still wild places out there and that we can experience those…and I think that is what animal hunting is about. It’s touching something that is part of who we are and that doesn’t get used everyday. (SH 8) With respect to lions, SH 9 notes, “I think that is the challenge that brings the tourist hunter. The possibility that you might be eaten.” As such, the lion is considered “the ultimate trophy” (SH 10) in people’s quest to master nature. And similarly, it is an important part of the image of having hunted in Africa, “you know, to classify yourself, I’m an African hunter, you know you have to have a lion” (SH 2). The iconic stature of the lion, appointing it as one of the pinnacle species among trophy hunters, is naturally responsible for the incredible demand for lion hunting. Economically, lions are an essential component in attracting hunting clients to Tanzania. SH 2 comments, “Professionally, we need them. We need to be able to hunt them because Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 41 [the lion] is probably the number one sought after trophy in Tanzania.” Many individuals noted their dependency on the lion for part of their livelihoods and expressed concern over the “importance of being able to hunt in a properly managed environment” (SH 4). Interestingly, most of the professional hunters actually expressed a personal disdain for lion hunting, commenting that they would not themselves covet a lion trophy. SH 11 provided his personal philosophy, “I have never ever shot a lion for myself as a trophy. In all the years I have been hunting…I just refuse to. It’s the least respect I can show to the animals for what I take out of them.” In addition, most individuals noted that the hunting of lions was anti-climatic, expressing sadness over the animal’s death. SH 7 poignantly explains: Personally, when I am hunting a lion, I hope he doesn’t turn up. But you know, I have done it for 40 years now, so for me to hunt a lion, yeah, there’s excitement there. But at the end of the day, when that animal is dead, it looses all its beauty, its charm, unlike a leopard. You know a leopard still seems to retain its beauty, but a lion, it’s like when you catch a marlin, the color fades, you know. And that, to me, is how I feel about lion. (SH 7) Similarly to individuals from the sport hunting industry, respondents from the photographic tourism industry exhibited aesthetic, symbolic, utilitarian, and humanistic values of lion. In addition, they indicated the importance of the scientific and naturalistic values of lions. Again, the lion was portrayed as a beautiful and powerful predator that simultaneously induces fear and wonder among his onlookers: Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 42 People have always looked at large carnivores and been afraid of them, whether it be the wolf, whether it be a lion, whether it be a tiger, or a hyena, or whatever it is, but particularly the large cats, and particularly the social cats like the lions. Everybody is excited because they represent something free and large and something that could be threatening. (PC 8) The people want to see a lion because [he is] a top carnivore. Lions, like I said, they can kill and eat us, and I think it is a good experience for people to help remember that we are part of nature, to feel that raw, you know that feeling of we are actually animals, and a lion is one of those few species that can, that enables that I think. (PC 6) There’s a certain kind of tension about being around big cats, and people really want to get back to that somehow. (PC 7) Lions were viewed as “synonymous with safari; …synonymous with wilderness” (PC 1), and as such, were considered “a phenomenal icon of safaris in Tanzania” (PC 1). Portrayed as being “a mystique of Africa, of being wild, and strong” (PC 2), all respondents noted the importance of the lion to the photographic tourism industry. PC 5 states, “Professionally, [it’s] vital for, I mean it is one of the things that you have to see, definitely, on a safari.” PC 3 elaborates, “Being on safari is not complete and you cannot come to Africa, you cannot come to Tanzania or Africa and say, ‘Oh, I went on safari, and I didn’t see a lion.’ I mean that’s disappointing.” In this respect, lions are directly tied Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 43 to respondents’ livelihoods, and there is “a real direct value and meaning [of lions] in terms of tourism” (PC 6). Finally, evident in almost all photographic interviews was the scientific notion that the lion, as a “top predator” (PC 1), is “like a canary in a goldmine, it’s an indicator species” (PC 6). Photographic respondents referred to the lion’s position at the top of the food chain as an “indication of the health of the wildlife” (PC 10) Along with these sentiments, there pervaded an interest in lion behavior, social structure, and hunting strategies, the observation of which contributed greatly to the individual experience of being in the wild: “…a lot of the theater of the bush comes from lions, and lions doing what they do, killing, hunting, playing, biting, living their lives” (PC 10). Perceptions of Lions There is no doubt that the Maasai identify with and have respect for the lion because “it is an intelligent animal, with strength, with know how, and fierceness” (M4). However, Maasai perceptions of lions varied depending on the likelihood of having aggressive interactions with these animals. Respondents demonstrated a general tolerance for lions that were not in direct conflict with people or livestock. M4 explained, “if lions haven’t caused trouble, then we don’t have anything to do with it…if it hasn’t caused any destruction at all, then you carry on watching your cows, and the lion will carry on hunting its animals, wildlife.” Similarly, M2 relates an important moment in his life with lions: Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 44 I remember the lion, because there was a day when I was traveling at night by motorbike, and I met [lions] on the road, but truthfully, they didn’t harm me or do anything…I stopped because of the hofu (fear, apprehension), but they went off, because it was early morning and misting, so they moved off and we carried on our journey by motorbike. I saw that lions, astonishingly, don’t have any problems with humans. (M2) However, many individuals noted that historically these lions would have been pursued, while contemporarily, government regulations prohibit the killing of animals that haven’t proved a threat to human life or livestock. M7 estimates that the transformation is fairly recent, “maybe going back 10 years, the relationship has changed, because it was that if you encountered a lion, you just hunted it, there was no other reason, but now the hunting of lions is only if it causes damage to livestock...or it tears up someone.” Some individuals were resentful of the government “due to the traditions being lost…because long ago it was anytime, and now due to the parks, events [i.e. lion hunts] are once in a while or not at all” (M3). On the other hand, lions that attack cattle or people are considered “evil-minded” (M5) or to have gone “crazy” (M1). These lions are not tolerated. In such circumstances, negative perceptions of lions arise because “lions are a bad animal” (M8) that harass people and their livestock. This will be discussed further in the section on human – lion interactions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 45 Among the sport hunting and photographic industries, the strength, intelligence, and potential danger posed by lions all contributed to positive perceptions of lions. In fact, contrary to the Maasai, the knowledge that lions possess the capability of wounding or killing a person appears to be an important aspect of individuals’ enjoyment of the species, adding a peculiar thrill otherwise lacking in their daily lives. Nevertheless, some changes in the perceptions of lions were noted to have a historical element to them. While the historical relationship of lions and sport hunters was not considered to have changed in overall meaning, ironically, positive perceptions of lions have been strengthened in recent years by a decline in the overall lion population. Sport hunters noted that in the past, lions were more abundant, and while they have always been coveted as important trophies, they were not as difficult to obtain as today. SH 4 explains, “Right now, lion hunting is, it has more importance than it did. The meaning, I mean the importance of lion hunting right now is, it’s like you are given a key, but only a few keys are given out.” Historically, lion were often shot as vermin on large farms: “Fifty years ago, when I hunted lions, as I say, they were cattle killers on big ranches in Laikipia” (SH 9). This may have lent an additional purpose to the hunt, however it did not alter the sport hunters’ intrinsic reverence of lions as formidable and glamorous trophies. But today, the overall decline in lion numbers means that lion trophies are perceived as increasingly rare and therefore all the more significant. Photographic individuals indicated that their personal perceptions of lions were enhanced depending on the environment in which the animal was spotted. Respondents Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 46 spoke wistfully of the past when the “whole African continent was extremely wild” (PC 3). Today, many of the guides find the lions too habituated in the national parks that were often referred to as controlled environments. PC 5 comments: I think of the experience over the last 50 years, more people now see lions in national parks where they may be sleeping under a tree then do in days gone by. You would have seen more in the marginal areas and closer to towns and stuff like that where they would have been… more alert and would have jumped up and run away. So, I think that people are seeing lions in an almost unreal environment more because of the national parks, so the lions are completely relaxed. (PC 5) Most respondents indicated that they preferred to see lions in a “wilderness experience over the minibus thing” (PC 11) and commented that seeing lions on foot added more excitement and pleasure to the overall experience. On the other hand, clients are perceived to have positive views of the habituation of lions in national parks: I mean I’ve been for 24 years viewing lions here and seeing just how lackadaisical they’ve become or tolerant of tourist vehicles…my original viewing of the lion was very scatty, very piecemeal because back then they weren’t seeing very many people and they were acting in a very standoffish way, where now certainly in the heavier, more densely touristed areas, they are more habituated. So, for me that kind of takes away from the experience, but as somebody coming here as a once in a lifetime Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 47 experience, then I guess they probably like it a lot better. You know, that kind of semi-zoo situation that you have now in the [Ngorongoro] crater floor. (PC 9) Knowledge of Lions and Lion Conservation The Maasai and members of the photographic and sport hunting industries described the basic ecology of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem similarly. Most individuals recognized lions as social animals living in groups or prides. The majority of respondents mentioned the movement of prey species, for example buffalo, wildebeest, and zebra, in and out of Tarangire National Park as an important factor influencing the presence and abundance of lions in the ecosystem. Resident lion prides were acknowledged outside of the park where they fed upon non-migratory species such as antelopes and small mammals, as well as livestock. However, lion abundance was considered to increase during the rainy season when “park prides” followed migratory prey species outside of the park. Lions outside the park were considered to prefer thick bush and korongos (ravines, drainages) to open spaces and to avoid areas of human habitation unless actively pursuing livestock. Some individuals named specific areas that they felt lions favored when outside of the park. Despite similar ecological knowledge, respondents from the sport hunting and particularly the photographic industries expressed a greater degree of factual or scientific knowledge of lions than the Maasai. In this respect, they indicated an awareness of basic ecological and scientific terminology that was lacking, in a formal sense, in the Maasai interviews. Specific terms such as “home range”, “corridor”, “prey availability”, Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 48 “indicator species”, “population dynamics”, and “food chains” were specifically referred to in their interviews. This was not surprising given the greater likelihood of their exposure to this information through higher education or access to scientific literature or lectures. Much of the sport hunters’ knowledge also appeared to be handed down through generations, and while less technical in jargon, reflected a scientific understanding of lions as well. In terms of lion conservation issues, both sport hunting and photographic respondents initially framed their responses by discussing the overall problems affecting lions in Tanzania and, at times, Africa, followed by particular threats to lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. In comparison, the Maasai responded only to local threats to lions. Within the Tarangire ecosystem, the general opinion was that the overshooting of lions by the sport hunting industry was the greatest problem facing the lion population, though sport hunters felt that poisoning of lions by the Maasai was of nearly equal dimension (Table 2.4). Several sport hunters noted that in recent years lions were less likely to feed off of baits, presuming that the cats had learned to stay away from carcasses based on unpleasant encounters with poisoned livestock remains. Others had come across the remains of poisoned lions in the bush. The stakeholders also frequently mentioned loss of habitat as an important factor affecting lion populations. This was often accompanied by recognition of human population increase by industry respondents. In Chapter Four, I test some of these opinions by comparing lion densities in several different land use areas inside and outside of Tarangire National Park. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 49 Table 2.4 Perceived threats to lions in the Tarangire ecosystem according to stakeholders. Rank is indicated in parentheses. For each stakeholder, n = 11. Threat Overshooting of Lions for Sport Land Cultivation/Loss of Habitat Poisoning of Lions Poaching of Lions Loss of Prey Species Human Population Increase Spearing Livestock Raiders Drought/Lack of Water Cultural Killing of Lions by Maasai Disease Maasai 7 (1) 4 (2) 3 (3) 3 (3) 2 (4) 2 (4) 2 (4) - Photographic 11 (1) 7 (2) 5 (4) 2 (5) 7 (2) 7 (2) 6 (3) 2 (5) 2 (5) Sport Hunting 9 (1) 6 (3) 8 (2) 2 (6) 3 (5) 6 (3) 4 (4) 2 (6) 1 (7) Human – Lion Interactions The conservation status of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem has differing degrees of significance to the stakeholders and affected their attitudes in unforeseen ways. Similar to their concepts of conservation issues, Maasai perceptions of lion conservation status were framed in a local context and were based on their opinions of the population status of lions in their regional vicinity as opposed to a Tanzania-wide or continent-wide perspective. While overall, lion numbers were thought to have decreased over time, responses were divided regarding the perceived abundance of lions outside of the park. For instance, M7 felt though the overall lion population had increased, “there are lots of lions throughout this mbuga (steppe, plain) of Tarangire, lots of them, even here at our place [i.e. village] just lots of them, therefore it is not an animal that is close to being lost.” Other individuals indicated that lions were seasonally abundant during the wet season. Despite this lack of consensus on the status of lions outside of the park, the Maasai indicated that if lions were absent from the ecosystem it would be undesirable for cultural and economic reasons. M1 summarizes the general feeling: Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 50 If the lions are lost forever, it would be a great loss for me because first of all the traditions of the Maasai to celebrate the bravery of lion hunting won’t be seen and the children won’t see, they will only be hearing in stories that there was something called a lion. As well as in the aspect of hunting and photography, it will cause a loss as [we] will no longer receive benefits from living with these animals, lions” (M1). While relatively strong support for the persistence of lions was evident, little willingness to be actively involved in lion conservation measures was indicated by the Maasai. Rather, respondents viewed the traditional protection of Maasai grazing lands, utilized by both cattle and wildlife alike, to have positive, though not specifically intended, consequences for lions. Photographic and sport hunting respondents couched their perceptions of lion conservation status in a more global framework indicating an overall reduction in lion numbers, though occasionally noting that Tanzania maintains the largest remaining population of lions in the wild. Nevertheless, the majority of respondents felt lion numbers in the Tarangire ecosystem were also declining. Surprisingly, this local decrease was of questionable significance in terms of how it affected the respondents and their attitudes toward lions. Many respondents from both industries noted that while personally they would be sad to see the lions disappear, their respective industries would be relatively unaffected. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 51 Photographic respondents noted that the absence of lions from Tarangire National Park would naturally dilute the experience of visiting the park and perhaps make it more difficult to sell to clients, however “there are going to be lions somewhere else, so it doesn’t really affect us from a business standpoint that much” (PC 2). Some individuals further explained that Tarangire was sold more on the basis of its elephant population and riverine habitat rather than the promise of seeing lions. Similarly, individuals from the hunting industry noted, providing there were lions in other hunting areas of Tanzania, “there will still be other animals to hunt [in Tarangire], and there [are] always people who want to hunt other species, but if they want cats they will have to go to another area where they are still available” (SH 11). These attitudes express the potential fickleness of industry and suggest that as long as other lion populations exist in Tanzania, there is little incentive for either industry as a whole to actively support lion conservation in the Tarangire ecosystem. On the other hand, considerable variation occurs depending on the size of the business involved, with smaller companies maintaining holdings only in the Tarangire ecosystem necessarily more dependent on the presence of lions. Furthermore, willingness to support lion conservation appears to depend on the individual goals of each company and whether an interest in the long-term welfare of the ecosystem exists as opposed to short-term gains. Additionally, as will be discussed, the ability of these companies to become actively involved in conservation efforts is hindered by current government regulations regarding wildlife. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 52 Attitudes toward direct interactions and conflicts between people and lions also differed between the wildlife tourism industries and the Maasai communities. Among both the photographic and sport hunting industries, interactions with lions were sought after despite awareness of the potential danger involved. In fact, positive attitudes toward lions among these individuals were fostered by the thrill of a frightening encounter with a wild animal, otherwise lacking in their daily lives. On the contrary, for the Maasai, direct human – lion interactions resulted in negative attitudes toward lions associated with the persistent threats lions pose to people and livestock that often result in undesired outcomes. Protecting livestock from lions can be a terrifying experience as M3 relates: We were in the boma (homestead) at night when the lion came. He pounced on and caught a cow, as he was holding the cow, three of us came out, my friends and I. In the boma there was nowhere to hide, a place to climb up, to go, maybe to climb on the huts or to climb a tree. There was only a tiny little tree, that we ran towards, and it ended up that we were dangling [from the tree] and the lion passed close. So, there as the lion passed close, as he passed close, we acted and hit him with a spear. The lion ran and cut into the cows, he went to the other side of the boma, he had gotten one spear so we had wounded him. But, now as it was, we had survived by our underpants, if it wasn’t for that little tree we would have been eaten by lions. (M3) Tolerance of livestock-marauding lions was also related to the manner in which wildlife officials treated depredations. Most Maasai respondents felt that it was the responsibility of the government, particularly TANAPA, to respond to human – lion Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 53 conflicts. However, wildlife authorities were found to be unresponsive: “Even in the past, they never helped. If animals are hurt by a lion, they just get eaten and well…it’s the owner of the cow that has the burden” (M10). Explanations for why the Wildlife Division or TANAPA did not help were related to perceptions that these organizations “don’t want to kill lions” (M9). M9 goes on to express his frustration with the government stating, “Once a lion eats livestock, they act like they haven’t heard, so maybe if you go and get him, then he comes but won’t shoot, he just scares it away. He says nothing at all, they come but do nothing at all.” Nevertheless, most respondents indicated that if they received compensation for animals lost to lions they would no longer need to kill the predator. M4 explains: There you would have a good relationship, even if a cow is eaten, well, it would no longer be a must that the man takes a spear to kill that lion because he knows his cow has been eaten, but he knows that Tarangire [park] will compensate him something small for his livestock. (M4) A third human – lion interaction that affects attitudes toward lions is their utilization for both consumptive sport hunting and non-consumptive photographic tourism. Among the Maasai respondents, limited support existed for sport hunting. This was due to an overwhelming perception that professional sport hunting has contributed to a decrease in lion populations and a general belief that sport hunting and lion conservation were antithetical. A photographic company…“the one that keeps” (M10)… and a hunting Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 54 company…”the one that kills” (M10) were seen to have opposing goals, and Maasai respondents tended to identify themselves more with photographic interests: Now if you compare these photographic companies with the Maasai, you could say that we are the same because the photographic companies are only looking for the benefits of pictures, but the lion still lives and we live with the lions. They destroy our livestock and sometimes we kill them because of the pain of that livestock, but we don’t just kill lions hovio-hovio (haphazardly, indiscriminately) for no reason. (M10) Interestingly, this statement suggests that hunters do not have a legitimate reason for pursuing lions, though their motives for hunting lions, to show their power and mastery over nature, are very similar to the Maasai cultural value of lion hunting as a demonstration of bravery. However, his remark also contains resentful undertones that suggest indirect causes for Maasai antipathy of hunting lions for sport. These appear related more to Maasai – government relations than they do to lions. The lack of authority or power to choose whether to permit hunting on village lands has created a resentment of the government and hunting companies as exemplified by the following statements: Tourist hunters, they hunt lions by permission of the government. They come with their quotas and they pass through the village offices, they tell us that they are going to this particular area to hunt lions. (M7) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 55 The upper government still sends people to hunt animals right here in this village and we have no strength to not allow them. (M10) It’s the hunting companies that are assigned the responsibility. It’s the government that has the responsibility, but if it were the village instead, we would be able to get rid of them. (M1) In a sense, this feeling of powerlessness, of which the presence of hunters provides a constant reminder, compels the Maasai to ally themselves more closely with the photographic community. In addition, Maasai respondents indicated that hunting was poorly regulated and that lion quotas were haphazardly determined. M5 suggests, “Maybe the government should do research to find out how many lions this area has and what amount is appropriate to be hunted each year." Within the sport hunting and photographic industries, lion hunting was viewed, theoretically, as an acceptable consumptive activity that has significant conservation potential. PC 10 comments, “I think a well run sport hunting company operating in a well regulated system can play an important role in maintaining areas for wildlife.” However, limited support existed for lion hunting in Tanzania under the current quota system. In accordance with views expressed by photographic respondents, sport hunters consistently remarked that lion quotas were too high, unscientifically determined, and that, in general, lion hunting was ineffectively regulated by the government. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 56 Individuals who had been hunting in Tanzania for many years viewed the apprenticeship of young professional hunters today as generally easier and more based on image than traditional hunting philosophies that commanded respect for wild animals. SH 11 notes: I feel sorry for young professional hunters who are starting out today…you know there [are] high hopes, big dreams, just the way they dress, the way they behave, walking around flashing photographs everywhere…I tell you if we were seen by our peers, older peers, people we trained under, behaving in such a manner…you were warned, and if you ignored it, you were out. You would just get cut out and you can’t get back in. (SH 11) Respondents also remarked that considerable variability existed regarding the ethics of individual companies and that quite often young, subadult lions were shot by hunters unconcerned with the long-term effects of their actions. Some of these individuals were referred to as “briefcase operators,” temporarily leasing hunting concessions off of established companies, with short-term incentives to recuperate their expenses quickly and therefore to shoot lions indiscriminately. However, considerable concern over the established companies in Tanzania also existed. SH 7 states: Certain companies…are very concerned about the type of lion. Presently, we will not shoot a male that is in a pride…nothing that is immature, no youngsters, but it is a very small group of companies that are concerned, not just lions, but everything else. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 57 But I can tell you its only, out of all the hunting companies, I think there’s 42, and out of the 42, there’s probably about 5 companies that are really concerned about what they do. (SH 7) Most respondents remarked that the market-based economy of sport hunting often made it difficult for companies, particularly small to medium sized ones, to forego the profits of hunting a lion if it was of an inappropriate age, or in extreme cases, sex. In some situations, hunters have also resorted to illegally hunting lions at night using night-vision scopes and lights. In terms of scheduling, while a lion-hunting safari is supposed to be 21 days, some companies book shorter safaris for their clients whilst paying the entire fee 21-day fee. This maximizes the number of lion safaris they can sell in a season and puts additional pressure on the professional hunter to find a suitable trophy lion in a shorter time frame. In this scenario, “you’ve got to shoot the first lion that you see” (SH 2). These problems are compounded by the demands placed on professional hunters by their clients. Noting that the majority of clients are middle class individuals who will only come to hunt lions once in their lives, SH 8 explains: So you’ve got this guy who has saved all his pennies and he is coming and this is his one shot to do it and he wants to do it right, and they see a lion. That guy doesn’t give a shit about whether there [are] lions here tomorrow or the next day. He has focused his whole self on this one safari, and to hell if that’s a four year old lion or a pride male lion. That’s his lion. He’s paid for it, and he is going to take it. And that middle Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 58 band, lower band operator…is in a similar position, his livelihood is depending on this client, because this guy is going to tell his buddies and his buddy’s going to come and hopefully come and hunt with him. (SH 8) Naturally, clients hope to find a lion with a significant mane. Some respondents noted that as a result, older lions were less desirable because their manes often become raggedy and tattered later in life. In contrast, prime adult males typically have fuller, large bodied manes which are highly prized as trophies. Hunters feared that taking these prime males, if currently associated with a pride, could have negative effects on the social behavior of lions and potentially result in an increased incidence of infanticide during pride takeovers. Lion hunting was considered to be so out of control by SH 7 and SH 1 that they suggested a moratorium on lion hunting in Maasailand for a minimum period of 3-5 years. SH 8 also suggested a moratorium but reviewed in a block-by-block manner. In contrast, photographic tourism was considered by all respondents to have the least amount of impact on the environment and lions. A number of individuals noted that the habituation of lions to tourist vehicles could have negative consequences when the animals move out of the park, and the overcrowding of lions by tourist vehicles may impact the lions’ social behavior. However, these were not considered to be serious threats in the overall context of problems facing lions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 59 Various land use relationships are also important in the formation of attitudes toward lions. The ecological requirements of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem, compelling them to range widely throughout a variety of land use types, and the complicated legal environment regarding rights to land and wildlife utilization outside of parks have created an intensely controversial and, at times, hostile environment between the stakeholder communities. The majority of respondents from all stakeholder communities considered photographic tourism and sport hunting to be incompatible when practiced simultaneously in a shared location. From a photographic perspective, PC 1 explains, “you don’t want to really see dead animals when you’re with photographic people.” Individuals from both industries noted that photographic clients often express antipathy towards hunting. In addition, the inherent dangers of hunting in areas utilized by other individuals, the possibility of a hunt being disrupted by photographic clients, and the more secretive and wary nature of lions in hunting areas were all reasons given for a physical or temporal separation of the two activities. Most individuals felt that the industries could share an area by designating time periods for each activity. SH 2 explains the general consensus: You have to decide whose area it actually is. Is it a hunting company’s or photographic’s and have some sort of timetable, you know. Hunting high season and photographic high season are different. They overlap a bit, but they are different, so maybe six months it’s [photographic], six months it’s hunting. (SH 2) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 60 Maasai pastoralism was viewed as less problematic when occurring in areas utilized by sport hunters and photographic tourists. However, several individuals from the hunting and Maasai communities mentioned the potential dangers of hunting (e.g. stray bullets, wounded animals, lions habituated to baits that are then believed to switch to livestock predation in the non-hunting season) in the vicinity of human communities. All respondents noted the overwhelming incompatibility of agriculture with lions, photographic tourism, or sport hunting. Controversies over land and wildlife laws and the as yet unfulfilled promise of increased community control over wildlife as stated in the revised wildlife policy (MNRT 1998) have also created an antagonistic environment indirectly affecting people’s relationship to lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. In this sense, lions have become a symbol in the debate over wildlife management outside of national parks and reserves. While lions are potential “assets that are helpful for building better friendships/relationships between various villages and the national park of Tarangire,” (M4), Maasai respondents indicated a general frustration with the central government’s delay in authorizing community wildlife management: The atmosphere is difficult because every year they say this year we are going to hand over to the communities but when the year comes, they say next year or in the next five years, so it looks like the time to hand over isn’t good. (M1) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 61 The new laws, first of all, haven’t arrived here at all. They haven’t made it to the village. We don’t comprehend them, we don’t know them. They haven’t arrived, so we don’t know them. (M2) This uncertain environment, if continued for too long, can have serious consequences for Maasai – lion relationships by creating a resentment of government that may physically manifest itself in retaliation against lions traversing the boundaries of national parks and village lands. Meanwhile sport hunters and photographic companies appear to maintain an uneasy truce while awaiting the outcome of community wildlife management areas. Sport hunters indicated a resentment of photographic presence outside of Tarangire National Park in designated hunting areas. SH 3 explains: …the hunting companies pay a lot of money for the block. About $7500 and that’s going to increase to $12,000 next year, and according to the [wildlife] act no photographic or anyone is allowed to go into a hunting area to have a camp or anything else…unless they have permission from the director of the ministry, but I don’t know how it is working because now as it is around Tarangire, there are too many [photographic] lodges. (SH 3) Part of this resentment lies in the high fees that the sport hunting companies pay for a block which is then utilized by photographic companies with no additional government Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 62 fee. Photographic companies make agreements and payments to the local communities on a case by case basis that have no set fee structure. However, these agreements are tenuous at best because the villagers have no legal authority to grant wildlife utilization rights (consumptive or non-consumptive) on village lands. Confusion and frustration with the current set of wildlife and land laws is evident among many of the photographic respondents: It is one of these gray areas, where nobody really knows what they can do and the laws don’t, you can find laws that support each side of it, and so, the hunting companies are afraid to be to aggressive and we are willing to just hold out but it is unpleasant. (PC 2) [We need] recognition that we are, you know, that we are legal. Recognition that communities have in there, you know, the final say, that yes, we are allowed to operate. I mean at the moment the way the legislation stands, we are illegal. You know, we are not allowed to conduct photographic tourism in non-national park areas. You know until that day comes, where we are recognized as being a legal entity, then we are going to have to keep a low profile. (PC 9) Under one policy we’re illegal, under another set of legislation we’re legally operating. (PC 1) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 63 Resolution of the contradiction between village rights over land and natural resources and central government control over wildlife utilizing village lands will inevitably alter human-lion relationships in the Tarangire ecosystem. Photographic and sport hunting respondents worried about the ability of the Maasai to control corruption if given control over wildlife and hunting rights. Interestingly, all parties indicated an interest in working collaboratively if and when a new institutional environment for wildlife management materializes with hopes that this would strengthen human-lion relationships. As PC 8 remarks, photographic individuals, sport hunters, and the Maasai all share a common bond when it comes to lions: I think everyone of those groups sees the lion as a special creature. Whether it’s fear, whether it’s awe, whether it’s a photographic moment, whatever it is, we are all excited about lions. We use lions in different ways, and we probably view them in different ways because of that. (PC 8) The challenge is to integrate these different views and uses in a way that is beneficial to all the human communities and, of course, to the lions. Discussion Based on the data presented, some general conclusions regarding attitudes toward lions among the Maasai communities and the professional sport hunting and photographic industries can be made that have implications for lion policy and management. Overall, a number of similarities were found between the photographic and Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 64 sport hunting communities. Overwhelming positive attitudes toward lions and their conservation were evident among participants within both of these industries. Lions were revered from aesthetic, symbolic, humanistic, and utilitarian points of view. In addition, photographic respondents expressed scientific and naturalistic values of lions that were part of the experience of observing lions in their natural, wild state, while sport hunters revealed dominionistic and moralistic values that often underlie hunting encounters with dangerous game. Despite popular misconceptions of hunters among the general public, these individuals often exhibited strong affection and appreciation for large carnivores (Kellert et al. 1996). Positive perceptions of lions were strongly related to the thrill of choosing to be in the presence of a dangerous large carnivore that was otherwise absent from the daily lives of respondents and/or their clients. These perceptions were generally strengthened by an awareness of the decline of lion populations throughout Africa, making the experience of truly wild African lions even more special. On the other hand, the perception of a local population decline in the Tarangire ecosystem was personally disappointing while less significant from a business point of view. All respondents appeared to have a general understanding of the local ecology of lions which was enhanced by knowledge gained from access to scientific information related to lions and conservation. Both the consumptive and non-consumptive utilization of lions further contributed to interest and positive attitudes toward lions, though considerable concern was voiced regarding the management, monitoring, and ethics of sport hunting in Tanzania. Finally, lions were Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 65 viewed as an important symbol in the debate over land management outside of protected areas in Tanzania. Among the Maasai, a more tenuous relationship between humans and lions was evident that teetered between cultural reverence of lions as an appreciated and respected foe and powerful dislike for an animal that can inflict hardships on local communities. In this manner, Maasai values of lions were strongly symbolic, dominionistic, and negativistic. The cultural value of lion hunting was an important aspect of positive sentiments toward lions, though this traditional practice is at odds with national legislation forbidding the killing of non-livestock depredating predators. While this ritual appears to be eroding, there are both social and ecological values to maintaining a respectful wariness among lions for the Maasai, and perhaps, a compromise between law and tradition is needed that both protects Maasai custom and ensures that lion numbers are not significantly reduced by the activity. An emerging utilitarian value of lions also suggested that positive perceptions of lions might increase if individuals received greater benefits from these animals. Nevertheless, current perceptions of lions were mainly negative based on the potential threats lions posed to livestock and human life. Village leaders’ opinions were divided on the current population status of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem, though their general knowledge of lions was similar to that of photographic and sport hunting respondents while lacking a formal, scientific background. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 66 The most prominent negative attitudes toward lions were evident in terms of direct and indirect human – lion interactions. The Maasai indicated a strong frustration with livestock-marauding animals and the Tanzanian government, who though deemed to have responsibility for wildlife, was thought to do little to help compensate or mitigate human – lion conflicts. In addition, the Maasai saw themselves as powerless in terms of land use decisions and unable to regulate the use of lions on village lands. On the outskirts of Nairobi National Park in Kenya, Maasai fed up with government control over wildlife recently killed a large number of lions, exemplifying the significant influence wildlife policies can have on human – lion relationships. While in the Tarangire ecosystem, individuals appeared tired of awaiting changes in the Tanzanian wildlife policy, the situation is not nearly as incendiary. Nevertheless, frustration with national authorities had important negative repercussions regarding Maasai views of the sport hunting industry, since these individuals were often perceived as linked with the government. Finally, despite the more negative attitudes of the Maasai toward lions, they indicated an interest in future collaboration with all stakeholders involved with lions and their conservation, suggesting that a more positive human – lion relationship is still a possibility for the future. Therefore, effective implementation of the revised, more community-friendly, wildlife policy has the potential to significantly alter future attitudes toward Maasai and lion cohabitation. In terms of lion policy and management, these results indicate several important areas requiring further attention. Almost unanimous support from all of the stakeholders was Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 67 given for a scientifically based review of lion hunting quotas in Tanzania. This was particularly noteworthy coming from the professional hunting companies interviewed given the potential short-term costs to them of reducing lion quotas. However, it is unclear whether the majority of hunting companies in Tanzania, who were referred to by the sport hunting respondents themselves as adopting largely unethical practices and harboring shortsighted, economic goals, would support this position. Certainly, the longterm benefits of maintaining a healthy, scientifically monitored lion population in Tanzania should outweigh any economic losses associated with a revised quota system for lions. However, the Tanzanian government will need to be promote this principle before it is generally accepted by most hunting companies. While it is unclear whether the means or institutional support for such an undertaking are currently available, some positive steps are being taken by the Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism regarding lions. As of 2005, a new recommendation against shooting lions under six years of age is being considered. If reasonably enforced, this has the potential to stem what appears to be a fairly common practice of shooting under-aged lions. In addition, hunting respondents also noted that client expectations and the marketbased pressures of the hunting industry often resulted in illegal practices while lion hunting. This suggests that not only are effective reforms the responsibility of the Tanzanian government, but that companies active in Tanzania, and more broadly, the global hunting industry, have a role to play in reforming lion hunting. Education of clients regarding how to judge an acceptable lion trophy, the potentially negative effects of shooting under-aged animals, and, more generally, ethical hunting practices should be Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 68 a priority among international hunting organizations and the media. In fact, it may be useful to develop a certification system for companies that are deemed to hunt lions in a responsible, ethical, and ecologically viable manner. In terms of local village effects on lions and vice-versa, many respondents indicated that Maasai poisoning of lions was prevalent, though village officials were less likely to admit this because, by virtue of their government status, they were responsible for promoting anti-poisoning policies and demonstrating a decline in this activity. On the other hand, the Maasai indicated that livestock predation was an important factor influencing their negative attitudes toward lions. This is commonly found in other parts of the world where carnivores take domestic livestock (Oli et al. 1994; Mech 1995; Kellert et al. 1996) making the understanding of the magnitude of livestock predation and its effect on the human psyche an important policy consideration. This will be addressed in the following chapter on Maasai interactions with lions. Indeed, with regard to human – lion interactions in the Tarangire ecosystem, an important distinction can be made with respect to the stakeholders. Photographic and sport hunting respondents had overwhelmingly positive encounters with lions which were accompanied by the freedom to experience lions on their terms and at times and locations of their choosing. They took pleasure in rekindling an atavistic fear of large, dangerous animals, which in contrast, has never been dormant for the local Maasai population. The degree to which fear of lions is influencing Maasai tolerance of lions is also considered in Chapter Three. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 69 As this study has shown, the Maasai - lion relationship is not straightforward. Neither do they love them, nor do they wholeheartedly hate them. On the contrary, a complicated set of attitudes toward lions arises from cultural, political, and economic forces influencing Maasai-lion relationships. In the future, emerging wildlife policy and law in relation to community rights to wildlife, the nature of Maasai interactions with lions, and the degree to which they maintain a cultural value of lions will influence Maasai attitudes toward lions. Finally, this study has shown that the importance of lions to human communities extends far beyond their ecological or economic significance. As an extremely charismatic species, the lion was viewed as having many emotional, spiritual, and psychological benefits that bring added meaning to human life and help us define our place in the natural world. As such, the heavy print of a lion’s paw in the sand of a dry riverbed has immeasurable worth, and its persistence will forever be a meter of humankind’s kinship with nature. References Adams, J. S. and T. O. McShane. 1992. The myth of wild Africa: Conservation without illusion. University of California Press, Los Angeles, California. Alvarez. K. 1994. The Florida Panther recovery program: An organizational failure of the Endangered Species Act. Pages 205-226 in T. W. Clark, R. P. Reading, and A. L. Clarke, eds. Endangered species recovery: finding the lessons, improving the process. Island Press, Washington, D.C. Arhem, K. 1985. Pastoral man in the Garden of Eden: The Maasai of the Ngorongoro Conservation Area, Tanzania. Uppsala Research Reports in Cultural Anthropology. University of Uppsala, Uppsala. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 70 Baldus, R. D. 2004a. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man – eating lion killing 35 people. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. 41. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Baldus, R. D. 2004b. Tourist hunting and its role in development of wildlife management areas in Tanzania. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. ?. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Baldus, R. D. and A. Cauldwell. 2004. Lion hunting. Pages 16-22 in Baldus, R. D. 2004a. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man – eating lion killing 35 people. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. 41. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Clark, T. W., D. J. Mattson, R. P. Reading, and B. J. Miller. 2001. Interdisciplinary problem solving in carnivore conservation: an introduction. Pages 223-240 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Clark, T. W., R. P. Reading, and A. L. Clarke, eds. 1994. Endangered species recovery: finding the lessons, improving the process. Island Press, Washington, D.C. Earth Trends. 2003. Data online at earthtrends.wri.org. Gittleman, J.L., S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. 2001. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Harrington, F. and P. Paquet, eds. 1982. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Homewood, K. and W. Rodgers. 1991. Maasailand ecology: Pastoralist development and wildlife conservation in Ngorongoro, Tanzania. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Hook, R. and W. Robinson, 1982. Attitudes of Michigan citizens toward predators. Pages 382-394 in F. Harrington and P. Paquet, eds. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Kellert, S. R. 1985b. Social and perceptual factors in endangered species management. Journal of Wildlife Management 49(2): 528-536. Kellert, S. R. 1992. Public attitudes toward bears and their conservation. Pages 293 – 305 in J. J. Clear, C. Servheen, and L. J. Lyons, eds. Ninth International Bear Conference, Grenoble, France. Kellert, S. R. 1996. The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Island Press, Washington, D. C. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 71 Kellert, S. R. and T. W. Clark. 1991. The theory and application of a wildlife policy framework. Pages 17-36 in W. R. Mangun, ed. Public policy issues in wildlife management. Greenwood Press, New York, New York. Kellert, S. R., M. Black, C. R. Rush, and A. J Bath. 1996. Human culture and large carnivore conservation in North America. Conservation Biology 10(4): 977-990. Lama, L. 1998. Conflict and compatability: An inventory and analysis of land use in a Tanzanian wildlife corridor. Ph. D. Dissertation, Binghamton University, State University of New York, New York. LEAT (Lawyer’s Environmental Action Team). 1998. Socio-legal analysis of community-based conservation in Tanzania: Policy, legal, institutional and programmatic issues, considerations, and options. Report prepared for EPIQ, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Mangun, W. R. ed. 1991. Public policy issues in wildlife management. Greenwood Press, New York, New York. Mech, L. D. 1981. The wolf: the ecology and behavior of an endangered species. Doubleday Press, New York, New York. MNRT (Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism). 2000. The wildlife conservation (tourist hunting) regulations. Government Printer, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. MNRT (Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism). 1998. The wildlife policy of Tanzania. MNRT, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Muir, A. 1994. A situational analysis of pastoralism in Simanjiro district, Tanzania. VETAID, Arusha, Tanzania. Nelson, F. 2004. The evolution and impacts of community-based tourism in northern Tanzania. Drylands Issues Paper Series No. 131., International Institute for Environment and Development, London, England. Nshala, R. 2002. Village rights relating to land management, tourism, and tourist hunting. Lawyer’s Environmental Action Team. Available online at www.leat.or.tz/publications. Oli, M. K., I. R. Taylor, and M. E. Rogers. 1994. Snow leopard (Panthera uncia) predation on livestock: An assessment of local perceptions in the Annapurna Conservation Area, Nepal. Biological Conservation 68: 63-68. Quammen, D. 2003. Monster of God: The man-eating predator in the jungles of history and the mind. W.W. Norton and Co., New York, New York. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Two 72 Roe, D., N. Leader-Williams, and B. Dalal-Clayton. 1997. Take only photographs, leave only footprints: the environmental impacts of wildlife tourism. Wildlife Development Series No. 10. International Institute for Environment and Development, London, England. Tanzania Population and Housing Census. 2002. Data online at www.tanzania.go.tz/census. Weber, W. and A. Rabinowitz. 1996. A global perspective on large carnivore conservation. Conservation Biology 10(4): 1046-1054. CHAPTER THREE The Risk of Living with Lions: The Effects of Potential and Actual Interactions with Lions on the Maasai Living Adjacent to Tarangire National Park Chapter Three Globally, human attitudes toward large carnivores are becoming increasingly positive as people recognize these species as both important components of the ecological community and significant sources of human fascination, inspiration, and creativity (Kellert et al. 1996; Chapters One and Two). However, as I demonstrated in Chapter Two, this change is not occurring equivalently within all communities; those individuals living alongside these dangerous, wild animals often maintain negative attitudes toward them. This has important implications for the future of these species given that the coexistence of people and large carnivores is an important and necessary component of their conservation where they range outside of protected areas (Woodroffe 2001). In the Tarangire ecosystem, negative attitudes toward lions among Maasai individuals were strongly influenced by the direct and indirect interactions that resulted from sharing a landscape with the big cats. Adverse feelings were exacerbated by people’s perceptions of governmental failures to mitigate human – lion conflicts and by the inability of local communities to manage wildlife on their lands. In this sense, the Maasai’s negativity toward lions resulted from a complicated coupling of social and ecological forces that inevitably influenced their actions toward lions. While in Chapter Two, I focused on the social dimensions of human – lion relationships, I now turn to how the ecological reality of daily cohabitation with lions affects the Maasai both in a physical and a psychological sense. Specifically, I consider Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 75 how both actual interactions with lions and the sheer possibility of lion encounters, or the perception of risk, influence Maasai – lion conflicts and their tolerance of lions. Risk perception, in terms of wildlife, is related to the uncertainties and threats people associate with particular wildlife species (Bernstein 1996). It incorporates both the perceived possibility of suffering harm or loss as well as the worry that is caused by a particular species (Riley and Decker 2000). Those risks that are not accepted voluntarily or over which humans have little control tend to exacerbate human concern and usually represent low probability events that nevertheless result in elevated risk perceptions (Slovic 1987). Kellert (1985a) hypothesized that risk perception of large carnivores was an important component of people’s tolerance of these animals, yet few studies have tested this. Nevertheless, preliminary studies of cougars in the United States (Riley and Decker 2000) and Asiatic lions in India (Saberwal et al. 1994) showed that elevated risk perceptions negatively influenced local support for the conservation of these species. It is expected that a similar relationships exists regarding the African lion. The objective of this chapter is to provide an analysis of Maasai – lion interactions in the context of real and potential encounters. More specifically, I consider the degree to which risk influences human acceptance and tolerance of lions. In addition, local attitudes toward livestock predation, reported and actual rates of predation, human encounters with lions, and the influence of benefits from wildlife tourism are addressed in terms of their impacts on the local acceptance of lions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 76 Methods The Study Area Though initially protected in 1957 as a Game Reserve, Tarangire National Park (2600 km2) was formally declared in 1970. The park is recognized as an important dry season wildlife refuge (TMCP 2002). During this time, it harbors the second highest concentration of large mammals in northern Tanzania. Large populations of elephant, wildebeest, zebra, and buffalo can be found alongside the Tarangire River that bisects the park from south to north. At the onset of the rains, however, migratory wildlife species tend to move outside of the park into a mosaic of hunting and communal lands (see Figure 2.1). Wildlife habitat outside of Tarangire National Park is increasingly threatened by human population growth and an associated expansion in land cultivation. Of the original 30 migratory routes followed by large herbivores to wet season grazing habitat outside of the park, less than four remain passable today (Kahurananga and Silkiluwasha 1997). The majority of these remaining routes are located outside the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park where animals initially pass through the following village lands: Lolkisale, Loibor Soit, Emboret, Narakauo, Loibor Serrit, Sukuru, and Kimotorok. All of the villages on the eastern boundary of the park are located in Simanjiro district, excepting Lolkisale, which is in Monduli district. While originally recognized as pastoral Maasai villages, an increasing trend in the last decade has been the immigration of agriculturists, predominantly of the Waarusha ethnicity (Muir 1994). The Waarusha Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 77 people are historically affiliated with the Maasai, sharing a similar dialect, however their livelihoods are generally based on agriculture rather than pastoralism. Other individuals in the area include small groups of Ndorobo hunter-gatherers as well as immigrant farm hands and miners of various ethnicities. Pastoralism and small-scale subsistence agriculture are the predominant livelihoods in these villages. Some large-scale farms are found in the proximity of the park. However, these are mostly owned by expatriates and a few, wealthy Waarusha farmers. Cattle, goats, and sheep are the primary livestock raised by villagers. Maize and beans are the main subsistence crops. Local community members depend on the surrounding environments for firewood, grazing, medicinal plants, and other products. In addition, both community members and outsiders (e.g. large scale farm laborers) produce a large amount of charcoal from this area. Recently, an increasing number of village youths have been emigrating to urban centers to find employment. For instance, a nearby rare gems mine attracts a large number of individuals from these villages. These local dependencies on natural resources and the changing social dynamics in the Tarangire ecosystem have important ramifications regarding future land use decisions and changing attitudes toward lion conservation. Along with local communities, several professional hunting and photographic tourism companies use the land outside of the park for wildlife-based tourism. The Game Controlled Areas were originally demarcated primarily for professional sport hunting. Various open areas to the north and south exist where resident hunters are allowed to Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 78 shoot select game species under permit. Several photographic lodges and camps are located between the national park and the villages as well as a few professional hunting camps. In addition, several photographic companies use non-permanent camps in the vicinity. Detailed information regarding the predominant stakeholders (Maasai communities, sport hunting companies, and photographic companies) utilizing the Tarangire ecosystem is provided in Chapter Two. Data Collection The research was conducted between July and December 2002 using a structured survey as the primary data collection tool (see Appendix Two). Three of the six villages (Loibor Serrit, Narakauo, and Lolkisale) bordering the eastern boundary of Tarangire National Park formed the survey population for the study. The survey was conducted with a sample population of 235 homesteads. Elected officials from each village provided a list of the number of homesteads, arranged by subvillage. The homestead or “boma,” which often incorporates several different households, was considered the appropriate level to survey. This was based on the assumption that an encounter with a large carnivore at the level of the “boma” would be likely to affect all of the incorporated households in a similar manner. Therefore, attitudes toward lions within a “boma” would not vary as greatly as those between them. In the villages of Loibor Serrit and Narakauo, this resulted in nearly 100% sampling at the homestead level (n = 67 and n = 83, respectively). Due to the larger overall size of Lolkisale, every other homestead encountered was sampled (n = 85). One adult person (≥ 18 years) in each Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 79 selected homestead was interviewed. On a daily basis, the research assistant was encouraged to try and survey both men and women of different ages; however, the availability of respondents at the homestead also affected the selection of respondents. One research assistant conversant in the local dialects was hired to administer the survey. A local community member who facilitated introductions and reduced general suspicions of the interviewer assisted him in each village. The utilization of Tanzanian research assistants minimized cross-cultural bias and non-sampling error that would have been introduced if the author had been present. The research assistant was asked to try to conduct interviews in a private setting without large gatherings of neighbors or kin so that the respondents would not feel pressured or self-conscious while answering the questions. However, this was not always possible. Typically, the interviewer and his local facilitator either walked or rode bicycles to the homesteads. However, where the distance was great, they were dropped off prior to a group of homesteads in the morning and picked up in the afternoon. In all cases, individuals were told that the study was part of the researcher’s university degree. This was done to ensure that all the respondents had the same understanding of who the researcher was as well as to minimize expectations of aid following the study which might have influenced people to exaggerate their conflicts with carnivores. On average, four surveys were collected per day. Unannounced checks of 5% of the homesteads surveyed in each village were conducted to ensure that the surveys were administered. Randomly selected questions throughout the survey were repeated, and the Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 80 respondent’s answers were crosschecked with those recorded by the interviewer. In all cases, the survey was administered, and the responses were recorded accurately. In addition, during the research period, I frequently met individuals who had participated in the survey. The survey was developed in English and then translated into Swahili and Maa. Each translation was then translated back into English to ensure that the meaning of each question was adequately preserved. The questions were organized into the following broad categories: (1) general wildlife preferences, particularly large carnivores; (2) livestock predation; (3) human interactions with lions; (4) perceptions of lion population trends; and (5) demography (e.g. ethnicity, gender, age, education, and income). The majority of the questions were closed-ended. The survey was pre-tested with the research assistant as well as a sample of local community members from one of the villages. During the pre-testing period, several questions were re-worded for increased comprehension. Each survey took approximately one hour to complete. Observations and unstructured interviews conducted over two years in the study area supplement the data. In addition, a detailed study of the actual rates of livestock predation was conducted in the village of Loibor Serrit for a period of one year. All livestock predation incidents were recorded and photographed by a local research assistant who established a team of informants throughout the village. As soon as an informant became aware of a predation episode, the research assistant was found and brought to the scene where he photographed the livestock remains (if any) and any signs of the predator, such Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 81 as tracks near the carcass. In addition to noting the type and number of livestock taken and the carnivore species responsible for the attack, the assistant collected information regarding the time, location, manner in which the attack took place, and whether or not the livestock owners retaliated against the carnivore. If so, he also recorded the method of retaliation and whether or not it was successful. Statistical Analyses Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 11.5. Initially, response frequencies for the entire sample were calculated based on the pre-determined response categories. In some cases, similar categories (e.g. strongly agree and moderately agree) were combined and presented as one general category (e.g. agree) for further analyses. Most questions followed the Likert scale for response categories (Babbie 1990). The perceived threat lions pose to humans and livestock was measured by three related questions (items) regarding people’s fear of lions and their likelihood of harming people or livestock. These questions were combined to form a risk perception scale, though the potential limitation of a scale based on three questions should be noted. Factor analysis was used to identify underlying variables, or factors, that explain the pattern of correlations within a set of observed variables, in this case the risk perception scale (SPSS 11.5; see Tessler and Warriner 1997; Mehta and Kellert 1998). The scale is considered reliable and valid if it loads highly on a common factor (range 0 to 1; a large value indicates greater factor loading). This scale was later used as the dependent variable Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 82 for logistical regression. Reliability analysis was conducted using Cronbach’s alpha (Cronbach 1951) to determine the internal consistency of the scale based on the average inter-item correlation. The value of Cronbach’s alpha ranges from 0 to 1 with a larger value indicating greater reliability. Bivariate data were analyzed using Pearson’s χ2 tests (cross-tabulations) to determine whether an association existed between two variables. Variables were considered to be dependent upon one another if the significance value (p) was less than 0.05. In such cases, Cramer’s V was used to determine the strength of the association between the variables, ranging from 0 (no association) to 1 (complete association). Logistic regression was used to determine whether demographic variables such as ethnicity, gender, class, or age explained various trends in attitudes toward lions and wildlife tourism activities. Logistic regression analyses are suitable in situations where the dependent variable is dichotomous. Therefore, in each case, the dependent variable was represented by a dummy (indicator) variable. For example, the risk perception scale was dichotomized using the median score of the scale. A response was assigned a code of 1 if it indicated a higher perception of risk and 0 for a lower or non-existent perception of risk. Similarly, respondents who indicated they have received benefits from wildlife tourism were given the code of 1 while those who indicated no benefits received a code of 0. Demographic variables were similarly coded as dummy variables. For example, the respondent was coded as 1 if male and 0 if female; 1 if Maasai and 0 if Waarusha. Class and age categories were coded using the median score: 1 if wealthier (≥ 200,000 TSH per Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 83 year) and 0 if not, 1 if older (≥ 46 years) and 0 if not. Logistic regression analyses were made using the forward stepwise selection of variables that significantly improved the model’s goodness of fit. Significance levels are shown for each variable, while the model χ2 values indicate the significance of the final model. Results Demographic Variables The sample population consisted of one hundred and seventy-seven men (75.3%) and fifty-eight women (24.7%) from the villages of Loibor Serrit, Narakauo, and Lolkisale. The lower percentage of women respondents was attributed to the difficult nature of interviewing women in traditional African societies where men often prevent women from participating. Ninety percent of the sample population were considered long-term residents of their communities having either lived in their respective villages since birth (73%) or been resident there for over 20 years (17%). Of the remaining twenty-four individuals, sixteen were women who had immigrated to a village upon marrying. Respondents ranged in age from 18-75 years; however, the majority of respondents (66%) were between the ages of 36-55. The sample population was relatively evenly split between younger individuals (43%) and older individuals (57%) based on the median age of 46. Seventy-two percent of the respondents belonged to the Maasai ethnic group, while the remainder were of the Waarusha ethnicity (28%). Sixty-six percent of the respondents had no education, while 32% had completed some primary school, and 2% had completed some secondary education. Ninety-nine percent of the sample population cultivated crops on an average plot size of 8.7 hectares (range: 0 – 83 hectares). Similarly, 97% of the population owned livestock with average household livestock Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 84 holdings of 30.8 cattle (range: 0 – 150 cattle), 32.2 goats (range: 0 – 350 goats), and 25.8 sheep (range: 0 – 350). Mean livestock holdings and plot size per village are summarized in Table 3.1. Table 3.1 Average plot size and household livestock holdings per village. Measure Mean Plot Size (hectares) Mean Cattle/Household Mean Goats/Household Mean Sheep/Household Lolkisale 11.4 23 26 23 Narakauo 8.3 37 37 30 Loibor Serrit 5.9 40 36 27 General Wildlife Preferences Overall, respondents’ preferences were evenly distributed between positive and negative perceptions of wildlife. Respondents were asked to indicate whether they liked or disliked twelve different wildlife species (6 herbivores and 6 carnivores): elephant, leopard, wildebeest, impala, lion, wild dog, buffalo, cheetah, zebra, hyena, giraffe, and jackal. Responses for all 12 species were combined in order to analyze general preferences toward wildlife. Fifty-three percent of the sample population either strongly liked or moderately liked these species as a group, while the remainder held negative views of them (47%, n = 2803). However, when the species were analyzed categorically as carnivores or herbivores, preferences were significantly more negative toward the carnivores (χ2 = 261, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.31; Figure 3.1). Among major groups, the Maasai were more likely to dislike carnivores than the Waarusha (χ2 = 40.9, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.13), whereas the Waarusha were evenly divided between like (51%) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 85 100 90 80 Percent (%) 70 60 50 40 30 Attitude 20 Dislike 10 Like 0 Carnivores Herbivores Figure 3.1 Preferences toward wildlife among local community members. Carnivore species include lion, leopard, cheetah, hyena, wild dog, and jackal. Herbivore species include elephant, wildebeest, impala, buffalo, zebra, and giraffe. and dislike (49%). Women were also more likely to dislike carnivores than men (χ2 = 32.4, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.15). Risk Perception of Lions Among the five large carnivores studied, lions consistently ranked first among respondents as the most fearful and potentially harmful predator (Table 3.2). The majority of individuals either strongly feared (65%) or moderately feared (23%) lions. In contrast, 2% feared lions a little, while 10% of respondents did not fear them at all. Similarly, lions were perceived as either very likely (60%), moderately likely (29%) or slightly likely (9%) to harm humans. Only 2% of respondents felt that lions were not at all likely to attack people. In terms of livestock predation, 89% of respondents felt lions were very likely to harm livestock. Only a few individuals felt lions were moderately likely (10%) or slightly likely (1%) to harm livestock, while no one indicated that it was not at all likely. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 86 These three items regarding the potential threats lions present were combined into a single risk perception scale (Table 3.3). Due to differences in phrasing and scale categories, factor analysis was used to identify whether or not the scale was appropriate. The one-factor solution loaded highly on a common factor for all three questions indicating the reliability and validity of the scale (see Mehta and Kellert 1998). The scores of the three items were added to produce an overall score of perceived risk. Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was 0.52. The average score (on a nine-point scale) was 7.75. The average item score (on a three-point scale) was 2.58. The risk perception scale was combined into two categories for further analysis. The scale was used to test the prediction that individuals perceiving a higher level of risk would have lower tolerance for lions as measured by the desire for a decrease in the overall population numbers of lions. This hypothesis was supported. A significantly larger percentage of respondents who perceived a high level of risk associated with lions were more supportive of a reduction in overall lion population size than those who reported a lower level of risk (χ2 = 37.76, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.40; Table 3.4). Despite the high perceived risk associated with lions, the actual number of people who reported being attacked by a lion appeared relatively low. For example, 97% of the population responded a lion had not attacked them in the last two years (n = 235). Only seven individuals reported a lion had attacked them once. Since this response was biased towards those who had survived a lion attack, respondents were also asked to estimate Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 87 how many people from their respective villages lions harmed on an annual basis. Eightynine percent of respondents thought no one would be harmed from their respective villages. Of the 11% who thought one or more individuals would be attacked, all of the respondents were from the village of Loibor Serrit. This suggests that either the real risk of being attacked by lions is low or that people are extremely vigilant in the area. In comparison, a review of lion attacks on humans in Tanzania indicates that human injuries and deaths as a result of lions are far more prevalent in southern Tanzania where recently “not less than 35 people were killed by one or several man-eating lions in an area of 350 km2 … within 20 months” (Baldus 2004: 6). While the reason for this is still unknown, it suggests that in the north either social factors (e.g. increased human vigilance, different livelihood activities/patterns) or ecological factors (e.g. prey availability, habitat type, behavioral ecology) or both are contributing to lower rates of lion attacks on humans. And in the Tarangire ecosystem, while definitive data were not collected, it appears that sexually-transmitted diseases, malaria, tuberculosis, and other diseases are responsible for a far greater proportion of human mortality in the area compared to lions. Nevertheless, it should be noted here that while lion attacks on people do not occur extremely frequently in the Tarangire ecosystem, this does not invalidate the high perception of risk associated with lions. Any memory of a lion attack is likely to be vividly rendered and therefore associated with a belief that the likelihood of being harmed by lions is exceedingly high. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 88 Perceptions that individuals would be harmed by lions in a respondent’s respective village were associated with higher levels of risk (χ2 = 13.28, d.f. = 1, p < 0.001, V = 0.24) as were reported loss of livestock (χ2 = 4.24, d.f. = 1, p < 0.05, V = 0.13) and the lack of exposure to books or magazines about lions (χ2 = 8.52, d.f. = 1, p < 0.01, V = 0.19). Awareness of someone else who had been attacked by a lion (85%) was not associated with an increased perception of one’s own risk of being harmed. In addition, those individuals with exposure to videos about lions (66%) or who had seen lions in the bush (87%) were not more likely to have elevated risk perceptions. Logistical regression revealed that while the Maasai were the most likely to dislike lions (p = 0.001), women and wealthier individuals were likely to have the greatest perception of risk (p < 0.0001 and p < 0.05, respectively; Table 3.5). An individual’s age was not a significant predictor of risk perception. Table 3.2 Rank of large carnivores considered to pose a strong threat to respondents in terms of illiciting fear or potentially harming humans and livestock. See Table 3 for question phrasing. The percent responding to each category is in parentheses (n = 235). Carnivore Lion Leopard Cheetah Spotted Hyena Wild Dog Rank: Strongly Feared 1 (65) 2 (46) 3 (42) 4 (27) 2 (46) Very Likely to Harm People 1 (60) 2 (49) 3 (40) 4 (34) 3 (40) Very Likely to Harm Livestock 1 (88) 1 (88) 3 (65) 2 (70) 3 (65) Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 89 Table 3.3 Factor analysis of questions relating to risk perceptions of lions (n = 235). Based on a 0-3 scale, a high score indicates a greater perception of risk. Respondents who held no opinion (n = 2) were assigned a score of 0 assuming they did not have a strong perception of risk towards lions. Question Factor Loading 0.71 Mean 2.42 Standard Deviation 0.96 2. How likely, if at all, do you think lions are to harm a person in situations where they are not being hunted? Rated in terms of very likely, moderately likely, slightly likely, or not at all likely. 0.77 2.45 0.77 3. How likely, if at all, do you think lions are to harm livestock (cattle, goats, and sheep)? Rated in terms of very likely, moderately likely, slightly likely, or not at all likely. 0.71 2.88 0.35 1. How much, if at all, do you fear lions in situations where the animals are not being hunted? Rated in terms of strongly fear, moderately fear, fear a little, or do not fear. Table 3.4 Relationship between risk perception of lions and desired future population size of lions. Based on responses to the question, “In the next ten years, would you like the numbers of lions to increase greatly, increase a little, decrease a little, decrease greatly, or remain the same in the Tarangire ecosystem?” The initial five categories were collapsed to increase, decrease, and remain the same for the analysis. Perceived Risk High Low N Percent Responding: 'Increase' 25 68 (93) ‘Decrease’ 78 35 (113) 'Remain the Same' 11 18 (29) Table 3.5 Results of the logistic regression analyses for the socio-economic factors (gender = female, tribe = Maasai, class = wealthier, and age = older) that influence general attitudes toward lions and risk perceptions. Significance levels for each variable and chi-square values for the final model are presented. Age was an insignificant predictor for both analyses. Perceptions of Lions General Dislike High Level of Risk Factors Identified: Step I Step II Tribe Gender p = 0.001 p < 0.01 Step III - Gender p< 0.0001 Tribe p< 0.05 Wealth P< 0.05 Final Logistical Model d.f. p χ2 19.89 2 < 0.0001 34.66 3 < 0.0001 Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 90 Lions and Livestock Predation Respondents indicated that lions were accountable for almost 100% of cattle depredation, whereas leopards and hyenas were held responsible for losses of goats and sheep (Table 3.6). The Maasai were significantly more likely to indicate losses of livestock to lions than the Waarusha (χ2 = 40.42, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.42). Maasai cattle herd size (36 animals/household) was greater than that of the Waarusha (21 animals/household). However, herd size was an insignificant predictor of reports of livestock predation as was age, gender, and income of the livestock owner. Instead, reports of cattle predation by lions were related to an individual’s village with residents of Narakauo indicating significantly more cattle losses than the other two villages (χ2 = 11.88, d.f. = 2, p < 0.005, V = 0.23). A similar tendency for higher rates of reported leopard predation in Narakauo was found for goats (χ2 = 7.25, d.f. = 2, p < 0.05, V = 0.18) and sheep (χ2 = 28.24, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.35). In the latter case, accounts of losses of sheep were very low in Loibor Serrit. Reports of hyena predation on goats and sheep also showed a significant spike in Narakauo (χ2 = 24.72, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.32 and χ2 = 43.08, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.43, respectively), and in both circumstances, hyenas appeared to select larger herds (χ2 = 14.42, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.26 and χ2 = 5.46, d.f. = 1, p < 0.05, V = 0.16, respectively). In Narakauo, the percentage of respondents owning cattle (46%), goats (53%), and sheep (46%) was higher than in Loibor Serrit (37%, 40%, and 31%, respectively) and Lolkisale (26%, 29%, and 28% respectively). Combined with the higher than average Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 91 mean livestock holdings per household in Narakauo (see Table 3.1), this suggests that the overall number of livestock in the village of Narakauo is greater than that of Loibor Serrit or Lolkisale. Ecologically, the village of Narakauo is unique in that it contains a large dam with permanent water. This may contribute to the greater herd size. In any case, it seems possible that higher incidences of reported predation in Narakauo are related to the greater abundance of livestock in the area. The majority of respondents considered protecting livestock from lions as both timeconsuming and expensive (91.2% and 91.5%, respectively). However, it was also considered to be an activity that taught young adults bravery and responsibility (97.9%). Respondents were fairly evenly split regarding whether or not people should be compensated for livestock lost to lions. Forty-six percent of respondents thought money should be collected from villagers to pay for livestock lost to lions. Responses were slightly more favorable when asked whether the government should provide compensation (61.3%). Eighty percent of individuals appeared interested in being provided with information regarding how to protect their livestock from lions. Attitudes toward problem lions were definitive. Ninety-six percent of all respondents agreed that lions that predated on livestock should be killed, and 50.7% of the sample population favored the use of poisons. Furthermore, the majority of individuals felt people who kill a lion after it has attacked livestock should not be punished (94.4%). While only 14.9% of respondents admitted they had killed a lion in the last two years, 73.6% of the population knew someone else who had killed a lion. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 92 In addition to the survey data, within the village of Loibor Serrit, a detailed record of all livestock predation events was kept for a period of one year (data are kindly provided by C. Trout). Thirty-seven separate accounts were verified. Hyenas were responsible for the majority of predation incidents (55.3%), followed by lion (28.9%), and leopards (15.8%). In addition to these, three accounts of jackal predation on baby goats were noted. Of the 21 hyena incidents, attacks on goats (n = 9) and cattle (n = 7) more the most prevalent. Four hyena attacks on sheep and one attack on a donkey were also recorded. Leopard attacks occurred only on goats (n = 4) and sheep (n = 2), while lions pursued mainly cattle (n =8) and donkeys (n =3). All incidents resulted in the death of the victim. Hyenas were the only species that killed more than one animal at a time. On six occasions they managed to kill two animals, and one mass killing of ten animals occurred when a boy lost his herd at pasture. In fact, 71.4% of carnivore attacks occurred in the bush when herd animals wandered off and were lost. The remainder of attacks occurred near or in the “boma” (28.6%). Only leopards and lions entered the corrals by either jumping over the thorn fence or by crawling through holes in it, while in the three cases where hyenas attacked livestock at the “boma”, the animals were left on the outside of the enclosure. Despite the prevalence of their kills, retaliation against hyenas was only attempted twice via poison and, in both cases, was deemed unsuccessful. On the other hand, four of the six leopard predation incidents were repaid with endeavors to kill the animals via Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 93 traps (a hole dug in the ground near a carcass or at the base of a tree; a person gets in it with his spear, is covered with light brush, and awaits the predator), snares, poison, guns, and spears. Again, no efforts were successful, though one leopard was caught in a snare and severely mauled a man when it broke free. On the contrary, nine out of the eleven lion depredations resulted in retaliation, and over the period of one year, five lions were killed. In addition to one cub, one adult male, one subadult male, and one adult female were killed (the age and sex of one lion were not recorded; ages are approximations based on the research assistant’s information). Most lions were speared to death, while on one occasion, professional sport hunters shot the marauding lion for the village. Only one unsuccessful attempt to poison a lion was made. And in one case, a man was hospitalized after being attacked by a lion when he speared it from a trap. Table 3.6 Respondents' estimates of the number of cattle, goats, and sheep taken by carnivores over the last two years. Number of respondents (n) out of 235 is indicated in parentheses. SD = Standard Deviation Carnivore Cattle Goats Sheep Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Lion 3.88 (169) 3.88 4.97 (31) 4.80 3.76 (21) 3.02 Leopard - - 5.85 (136) 4.97 4.17 (126) 3.50 Cheetah - - 3.38 (8) 1.69 3.50 (4) 3.32 Hyena 2.25 (4) 1.50 4.11 (133) 3.60 3.36 (95) 2.74 Wild Dog 3 (1) - 3.35 (17) 2.50 3.00 (9) 1.80 Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 94 The Nature of Human – Lion Encounters In the last ten years, most respondents felt that the number of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem had either increased greatly (40%) or a little (36.2%), while only 23.8% felt they had either declined or remained the same. Similarly, the majority of the sample population thought they were either very abundant in the ecosystem (55.3%) or moderately abundant (36.6%). While 54.5% of individuals felt there would be more lions in Tarangire National Park than in an area outside the park of comparable size, over half of the respondents estimated the lion populations in these similarly sized areas to be greater than 1000 animals (51.9% and 53.2%, respectively). These estimates of lion abundance will be tested in Chapter Four. Respondents indicated that the majority of human and lion encounters occurred while grazing livestock, traveling by foot, and while protecting livestock at night (71.9%, 58.3%, and 49.8%, respectively). However, over a two-year period, these encounters appeared to occur relatively infrequently (e.g. for livestock grazing, an average of 2.89 days over two years; Table 3.7). Chi-square tests revealed that men were more likely to encounter lions while livestock grazing (χ2 = 8.60, d.f. = 1, p < 0.01, V = 0.19), traveling by foot (χ2 = 33.32, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.38), or protecting livestock at night (χ2 = 71.16, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.55). Women were more likely to encounter lions while collecting firewood (χ2 = 71.92, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.55) or collecting water/washing clothes (χ2 = 25.35, d.f. = 1, p < 0.0001, V = 0.33), corresponding with the gender-bias of these Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 95 activities. Both men and women were equally likely to encounter lions while farming crops, an activity commonly shared by both groups. The majority of individuals indicated that if they encountered a lion, they would leave it alone (88.1%). Of the remaining 11.9% of respondents who indicated they would actively pursue a lion (i.e. chase, attempt to kill, or incite others to kill it), all were men except for one woman who said she would get others to hunt it. Maasai men were slightly more likely to attempt to kill the lion than the Waarusha men (χ2 = 4.8, d.f. = 1, p < 0.05, V = 0.14). Table 3.7 Mean number of days respondents encountered lions over the last two years according to activity. Number of respondents (n) out of 235 indicated in parentheses. SD = Standard Deviation Activity Livestock Grazing Farming Crops Collecting Firewood Collecting Water/Washing Clothes Traveling by Foot Protecting Livestock at Night Mean 2.89 (169) 2.43 (42) 2.23 (26) 1.63 (24) 1.99 (137) 2.22 (117) SD 2.66 2.46 1.92 0.71 1.58 1.73 The Influence of Wildlife Benefits on Attitudes Toward Lion Conservation The majority of respondents recognized both photographic tourism of lions and the sport hunting of lions as important contributors to the Tanzanian economy (97.9% and 93.2%, respectively). Furthermore, they indicated that both activities have the potential to bring significant economic benefits to local communities (97.1% and 98.3%, respectively). Fewer individuals, however, indicated they had personally benefited from wildlife tourism and professional hunting (14% and 51%, respectively). Logistic Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 96 regression revealed that respondents who reported they received benefits from wildlife tourism were likely to be Waarusha (p< 0.0001) and male (p < 0.01; Table 3.8). Individual benefits from tourism and sport hunting were used to test the prediction that benefits from these activities are associated with elevated tolerances toward lions as measured by a desire for an increase in the overall population numbers of lions. This hypothesis was confirmed. Those individuals who benefited from photographic tourism or professional sport hunting were more likely to support an increase in the lion population over the next ten years (χ2 = 29.20, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.35 and χ2 = 21.50, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001, V = 0.30, respectively; Table 3.9). Nevertheless, respondents appeared relatively unwilling to actively participate in lion conservation by restricting their own activities in areas where lions were found (Table 3.10). While 54.5% and 44.3% of the sample population strongly agreed with limiting the activities of sport hunters and photographic tourists, only 11% of the respondents strongly agreed with limiting the activities of livestock owners or farmers. Table 3.8 Results of the logistic regression analyses for the relationship between socio-economic factors (gender = male, tribe = Waarusha, class = wealthier, and age = older) and benefits of wildlife tourism (n = 231). Significance levels for each variable and chi-square values for the final model are presented. Neither age nor wealth was a significant predictor for the analyses. Benefits Photographic Tourism Sport Hunting Factors Identified: Step I Step II Tribe Gender p< 0.0001 p < 0.01 Tribe p< 0.0001 Gender p< 0.001 Final Logistical Model: d.f. p χ2 32.42 2 < 0.0001 30.15 2 < 0.0001 Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 97 Table 3.9 Relationship between benefits from wildlife tourism and desired future population size of lions. Based on responses to the question, “In the next ten years, would you like the numbers of lions to increase greatly, increase a little, decrease a little, decrease greatly, or remain the same in the Tarangire ecosystem?” The initial five categories were collapsed to increase, decrease, and remain the same for the analysis. Benefits Percent Responding: 'Increase' 'Decrease' 'Remain the Same' Photographic tourism Yes No (n) 27 66 (93) 4 55 (59) 2 81 (83) Sport hunting yes no (n) 64 29 (93) 26 33 (59) 29 54 (83) Table 3.10 Willingness to restrict activities for lion conservation. Measured by responses to the question, "How strongly do you agree or disagree with limiting the activities of the following groups of people in areas where lions are found outside of national parks and game reserves?" Responses are percents (n = 235). Group Strongly Agree 54.5 Moderately Agree 21.3 Moderately Disagree 2.1 Strongly Disagree 15.3 Don’t Know 6.8 Tourists 44.3 30.2 3.0 15.3 7.3 Livestock owners 11.5 20.0 9.8 53.2 5.5 Farmers 11.1 20.0 8.5 54.9 5.5 Sport hunters Discussion Based on the data presented, some general conclusions and recommendations are made regarding potential and actual interactions with lions and their implications for reducing human – lion conflicts. The research presented here has demonstrated the importance of considering the perception of risk as an integral component shaping local attitudes toward and tolerances of lions. People’s perceptions of lions were influenced by their fear of them and by the belief that they are capable of harming humans. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 98 Interestingly, the only documented attacks of large carnivores on humans in Loibor Serrit occurred after individuals provoked them while attempting to kill them in retaliation for loss of livestock. While it is impossible to conclude whether the real risk of lion attacks on humans is low or, on the other hand, whether the Maasai are simply extremely capable of deterring them, the sheer possibility of being attacked by a lion constitutes a serious threat considering the potentially fatal outcome. This was particularly apparent among women whose likelihood of interacting with lions was fairly small, yet they indicated a high level of perceived risk associated with the lion. In any case, the mystique of the lion as a dangerous beast is as important a component shaping local attitudes toward interactions with them as actual incidences of attacks. In fact, due to this persistent fear, programs targeted solely at reducing attacks on humans may not be entirely successful in elevating human tolerance towards lions. The perception of lions as primary livestock killers was also a major component affecting the risk of living with lions. Negative attitudes toward carnivores are common among ranchers and farmers and are generally motivated by a fear of economic loss (Kellert 1985a, Reading and Kellert 1993). Depredation of stock can inflict severe emotional, financial, and political consequences (Mech 1981). In Loibor Serrit, detailed records of actual predation accounts indicated that the vast majority of depredations occurred once herd animals were lost in the bush. This suggests that improvements in livestock husbandry might significantly reduce the number of animals lost to large carnivores. Specifically, many animals were lost when young herd boys fell asleep or lost interest in their livestock, when animals were being moved between “bomas,” when Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 99 pregnant animals disappeared from the herds to give birth, or when sick animals were unable to keep up with the livestock movements. While definitive data were not collected, conversations with key informants indicated that many of these incidents might have been avoided. In terms of the lower predation rates at the “bomas,” it should be acknowledged that Loibor Serrit is situated in a relatively woody environment where a substantial number of thorn bushes are available to build protective fences. In other villages, where the trees and shrubs have been denuded for charcoal burning or other uses, or, in generally more open settings, livestock predation rates at the “boma” may be higher than those in Loibor Serrit as a result of less fortified fences. Not only did accounts of livestock predation elevate people’s perception of risk, but they also motivated negative interactions with the large cats. The majority of lion depredations in Loibor Serrit led to human retaliation, and these attempts were frequently successful. Furthermore, over half of the sample population favored the use of poisons in retaliation against stock-raiding lions. Not only is this a non-selective method for killing stock-raiders, the consequences of poisoning are far greater as many non-implicated species are likely to feed off poisoned carcasses. Poisoning of carnivores was also believed to be a popular local method of dealing with livestock predation by many sport hunters active in the area (Chapter Two). However, while throughout the study period several poisoned tawny eagles were found in the vicinity of Loibor Serrit, none of the known attempts to use poisons appeared successful on large carnivores. Given the illegality of using poisons, it is possible that this method was underreported. However, Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 100 more information is needed to accurately determine the effects of poisoning on the lion population. While the precise number of lions killed annually by Maasai in the Tarangire ecosystem is unknown, survey data presented in this paper indicate that approximately 15% of the sample population killed a lion (irrespective of method used) in the last two years, suggesting at least 17 lion deaths/year among only three Maasai villages. Therefore, rough estimates of the number of lions killed on an annual basis indicate a kill rate of 5.7 lions/village or 39.9 lions/year among the seven villages on the outskirts of the park, although environmental variability may result in gross annual fluctuations. The effects of these kill rates on the lion population will be considered in Chapter Four. This estimate compares reasonably with the five lions that were known to be killed over the period of one year in Loibor Serrit. In one particularly unpleasant account, two lions were speared to death after attacking a man’s donkey, while the individual was hospitalized for critical wounds sustained in the attack. Despite the aggravated relationships between people and lions, little attention is given to the financial, psychological, and social consequences of lion attacks on humans and livestock in the Tarangire ecosystem. However, this research showed that local individuals were interested in learning how to better protect their livestock from lions and other large carnivores. Future educational programs should target this interest with hopes of reducing lion – livestock conflicts. In addition, whereas the village of Loibor Serrit Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 101 indicated a higher likelihood of lion attacks on humans than any other village, it may be useful to conduct a more detailed analysis of this phenomenon. Nevertheless, while reduction of livestock depredation and lion attacks on humans should be considered primary management objectives, these alone may not result in a change in attitudes toward lions. For example, Bath (1987) indicated little change in negative attitudes toward wolves among ranchers in Wyoming after being offered compensation, elimination of problem animals, or confinement of wolves to protected areas. Similarly, Rudnai (1979) observed that human resentment of lions appeared out of proportion to actual livestock depredations outside of Nairobi National Park. She suggested that antipathy towards lions was more a result of historically antagonistic wildlife policies than losses of livestock. Her analysis is supported by recent killing of lions by the Maasai in Nairobi National Park that were motivated by resentment regarding the inequitable distribution of the costs and benefits of conserving wildlife. In the Tarangire ecosystem, data presented in both Chapter Two and this chapter emphasize the extent to which ambiguous and insecure property rights negatively affect Maasai perceptions of lions. In such cases, negative attitudes may be far too complicated and long-standing to be altered by wildlife management programs focused solely on the interactions between carnivores and livestock without considering the human social and psychological consequences of the problem. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 102 Certainly, given the widespread conflicts with lions and the highly negative attitudes toward them, it is unlikely that a change in Maasai-lion relationships will occur without greater local control over lions and significant, individual, realized benefits from the big cats among local community members. While the professional sport hunting and photographic tourism of lions were both seen as important contributors to the Tanzanian economy and as potential sources of income for local communities, relatively fewer individuals perceived actual benefits from these activities. Importantly, those who did were more supportive of lion conservation; however, certain inconsistencies were evident. For example, those benefiting from wildlife tourism (Waarusha and men) were not necessarily those individuals with higher risk perceptions of lions (women and Maasai). While it is acknowledged that past benefits from wildlife have not been targeted for raising tolerances toward lions per se, they were expected to increase participation in wildlife conservation. This research demonstrates the importance of targeting particularly high-risk groups for conservation awareness and the need to make sure all individuals benefit equitably from wildlife tourism. Linking species and habitat protection with community wildlife rights and benefits is one of the goals of the recently revised wildlife policy of Tanzania (MNRT 1998). However, while benefits from tourism may make people more positive toward wildlife conservation, this does not mean that they will actively change their land use practices to accommodate more wildlife. In this study, local individuals appeared strongly opposed to limiting their activities (i.e. livestock grazing and farming) in order to conserve lions regardless of the potential benefits of lion-based tourism. Discussions with community Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 103 members indicated a deep distrust of wildlife management authorities and a fear that conservation activities outside the national park would lead to an eventual loss of access to vital livestock grazing and farming lands. In fact, many individuals supported the clearing of otherwise vacant land as a means of demonstrating local land rights in order to thwart the conservation of wildlife habitat outside of the protected areas. In this sense, the perceived lack of control over wildlife, particularly lions, is viewed more broadly as a loss of control over the environment which is neither psychologically nor socially acceptable. In fact, as their world spirals increasingly out of order, the Maasai may intensify symbolic demonstrations of power by killing more and more lions. While it is beyond the scope of this paper to evaluate the potential efficacy of the Tanzanian wildlife policy, other studies have demonstrated the challenges of facilitating real changes in land use practices in response to economic benefits from wildlife (Kellert et al. 2000). The significance of human perceptions of fear and dislike for carnivores in affecting the long-term persistence of a species has already been demonstrated in North America by the widespread extirpation of large carnivores (e.g. wolves, grizzly bears, coyotes, and mountain lions). This suggests that a potentially similar situation is possible for lions outside of protected areas in Africa. In fact, modern day local attitudes toward lions are not dissimilar to historical relationships between Euro-American settlers and wolves. Much like the lion, wolves were considered a threat to personal safety and were held primarily responsible for financial losses due to cattle depredation despite a multitude of other factors such as disease, weather, and fluctuating market prices (Kellert et al. 1996). Just as wolves were considered a threat to progress and civilization (Young 1946), similar Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 104 views are expressed toward lions as exemplified by this quote from a Tanzanian newspaper, “People living under fear of wild animals, running a daily risk of being killed by lions and other beasts, often tend to believe that they do not live under what can be properly called a Government” (The Guardian 1997). Vulnerability to large carnivores, in this sense, is viewed as backwards and embarrassing to a country increasingly exposed to the global community. While in the United States, a profound change in attitudes toward wolves has occurred in the last century, negative views toward wolves have endured among people living in proximity to them, particularly ranchers and farmers (Tucker and Pletscher 1989; Bath 1987; Kellert et al. 1996). The persistence of negative attitudes toward large carnivores among local communities has important implications for conservation programs in developing countries where significant carnivore populations still exist alongside rapidly growing and expanding, agriculturally based human communities. As such, the research presented here contradicts the more popular notion of the Maasai as ‘custodians of wildlife.’ While, historically, Maasai pastoralism has been considered ecologically compatible with wildlife conservation (Arhem 1985; Diehl 1985; Collet 1987; Homewood and Rodgers 1991), this may have had more to do with the lower human population numbers and land pressures of the past than any concerted efforts toward wildlife conservation. In fact, it is proposed that Maasai attitudes toward large carnivores have not changed considerably over the years. However, the effects of their attitudes and actions toward large carnivores have become more pronounced as increasing land use pressures and human populations have forced lions, people, and cattle Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 105 into closer proximity and therefore more aggravated relationships. Romanticized notions of Maasai – wildlife relationships obscure present-day relationships between these groups and may ultimately undermine the long-term conservation of particular species such as the lion in Maasai-dominated landscapes. Finally, this chapter has demonstrated that attitudes toward large carnivores among local communities in Africa are not necessarily those promoted by conservation organizations, wildlife management bodies, or tourism industries. Conservation programs to protect these animals will not be readily accepted by communities where they interfere with individual livelihoods or where the real or perceived risk of living among these animals outweighs the actual benefits of sharing the landscape with them. Therefore, the success of conservation efforts for large carnivores outside of protected areas will depend as greatly on the social acceptance of these animals by local communities as on the biological variables directly influencing their survival. References Arhem, K. 1985. Pastoral man in the Garden of Eden: The Maasai of the Ngorongoro Conservation Area, Tanzania. Uppsala Research Reports in Cultural Anthropology. University of Uppsala, Uppsala. Babbie, E. 1990. Survey research methods. Wadsworth Publishing Company, Belmont, California. Baldus, R. D. 2004. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man – eating lion killing 35 people. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. 41. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Bath. A. J. 1987. Attitudes of various interest groups in Wyoming toward wolf reintroduction in Yellowstone National Park. MA thesis. University of Wyoming, Laramie, Wyoming. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 106 Bernstein, P. L. 1996. Against the gods: The remarkable story of risk. Wiley, New York, New York. Collet, D. 1987. Pastoralists and wildlife: Image and reality in Kenya Maasailand. Pages 129-148 in D. Anderson and R. Grove, eds. Conservation in Africa. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Cronbach, L. J. 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrica 16: 297-334. Diehl, C. 1985. Wildlife and the Maasai: the story of East African parks. Cultural Survival Quarterly 9(1): 37-40. Homewood, K. and W. Rodgers. 1991. Maasailand ecology: Pastoralist development and wildlife conservation in Ngorongoro, Tanzania. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Kahurananga, J. and F. Silkiluwasha. 1997. The migration of zebra and wildebeest between Tarangire National Park and Simanjiro plains, northern Tanzania, in 1972 and recent trends. African Journal of Ecology 35: 179-185. Kellert, S. R. 1985a. Public perceptions of predators, particularly the wolf and coyote. Biological Conservation 31: 167-198. Kellert, S. R., M. Black, C. R. Rush, and A. J Bath. 1996. Human culture and large carnivore conservation in North America. Conservation Biology 10(4): 977-990. Kellert, S. R., J. N. Mehta, S. A. Ebbin, and L. Lichtenfeld. 2000. Community natural resource management: Promise, rhetoric, and reality. Society and Natural Resources 13: 705-715. Mech, L. D. 1981. The wolf: the ecology and behavior of an endangered species. Doubleday Press, New York, New York. Mehta, J. N. and S. R. Kellert. 1998. Local attitudes toward community-based conservation policy and programmes in Nepal: a case study in the Makalu-Barun Conservation Area. Environmental Conservation 25:320-333. MNRT (Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism). 1998. The wildlife policy of Tanzania. MNRT, Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Muir, A. 1994. A situational analysis of pastoralism in Simanjiro district, Tanzania. VETAID, Waarusha, Tanzania. Reading, R. and S. R. Kellert. 1993 Attitudes toward a proposed reintroduction of black- Lichtenfeld – Chapter Three 107 footed ferrets. Conservation Biology 7: 349-365. Riley, S. J. and D. J. Decker. 2000. Risk perception as a factor in wildlife stakeholder acceptance capacity for cougars in Montana. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 5: 5062. Rudnai, J. 1979. Ecology of lions in Nairobi National Park and the adjoining Kitengela Conservation Unit in Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 17: 85-95. Saberwal, V. K., J. P. Gibbs, R. Chellam, and A. J. T. Johsingh. 1994. Lion-human conflict in the Gir Forest, India. Conservation Biology 8: 501-507. Slovic, P. 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236: 280-285. Tarangire – Manyara Conservation Project. 2002. Final Report. Istituto Oikos and University of Insubria, Italy in Collaboration with TANAPA. Arusha, Tanzania. Tessler, M. and I. Warriner. 1997. Gender, feminism, and attitudes toward international conflict: exploring relationships with survey data from the Middle East. World Politics 49: 250-281. The Guardian (Dar es Salaam). July 23rd, 1997. Tucker, P. and D. Pletscher. 1989. Attitudes of hunters and residents toward wolves in northwestern Montana. Wildlife Society Bulletin 17: 509-514. Woodroffe, R. 2001. Strategies for carnivore conservation: lessons from contemporary extinctions. Pages 61-92 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Young, S. P. 1946. The wolf in American history. Caxton Printers, Cladwell, Idaho. CHAPTER FOUR Lions Living on the Edge: Real and Perceived Lion Abundance in Sport Hunting and Village Lands Outside Tarangire National Park, Tanzania Chapter Four The diminishing global status and distribution of large carnivores is a persistent concern among wildlife managers and conservationists. Recent studies show that most conflicts with these species occur outside or on the boundaries of parks and reserves where they come into contact with human communities (Woodroffe and Ginsberg 1998; Gittleman et al. 2001; Chapters Two and Three). In fact, while the creation of protected areas has extended the tenuous hold large carnivores have on wild existence, these areas have not been able to stem overall species declines (Weber and Rabinowitz 1996). Not surprisingly, outside protected areas, where human – carnivore conflicts are prevalent, local retaliation against these animals is common. For example, communities living adjacent to protected areas in Brazil are responsible for the illegal killing of jaguars that predate on local livestock (Conforti and Azevedo 2003). Subsistence farmers living among snow leopards within the Annapurna Conservation Area in Nepal support the extermination of the species as a solution to livestock predation (Oli et al. 1994). And, as I demonstrated in Chapter Three, the Maasai retaliate against livestock depredating lions in the Tarangire ecosystem with poison, traps, guns, and spears. In the Tarangire ecosystem, local conflicts with lions derive from an interaction of social and ecological factors that affect Maasai attitudes toward lions. In Chapters Two and Three, I showed that a variety of political, economic, valuational, and institutional issues created negative attitudes toward lions. In addition, both the possibility of livestock loss or attacks on humans along with actual lion encounters influenced Maasai tolerance Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 110 of the big cats. An important perception underlying and aggravating these negative attitudes was the pervasive local belief that lions are abundant outside of Tarangire National Park (Chapter Three). Nevertheless, little information is known about the size and status of the lion population in the area. Therefore, the purpose of this chapter is to analyze whether or not the popular notion of abundant lions is grounded in ecological reality by testing the hypothesis that lion densities are high outside of Tarangire National Park. This requires comparing the densities of lions within several land use areas both within and outside of the national park. Specifically, I study the relative, seasonal abundances of lions in a mosaic of protected, village, and professional sport hunting lands using spoor counts as the principal methodology. Conservative population estimates for lions are presented and discussed in terms of local perceptions of lion abundance and the implications for lion conservation in the Tarangire ecosystem. In addition, I evaluate the effects of the Maasai and sport hunters on lion survivorship in terms of their estimated annual kill rates. Methods Study Area I conducted research in three experimental areas within the Tarangire ecosystem of northern Tanzania. The Tarangire ecosystem is approximately 12,000 km2 in size, including Tarangire National Park (2600 km2), Lake Manyara National Park (330 km2), the Lolkisale Game Controlled Area, and extensive village and hunting lands. My research was focused within Tarangire National Park, which is between 3° 67’ and 4° 53’ Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 111 south and 35° 92 and 36° 29 east, and two study sites along the eastern park boundary. The study sites represented three different land uses: the Park (control – wildlife protection), Kikoti (sport hunting), and Loibor Serrit (village/sport hunting; Figure 4.1, boxes A, B, and C, respectively). Tarangire National Park and the adjacent study sites are located within the semi-arid ecological zone (Pratt et al. 1966). The average annual rainfall is 450 – 600 mm with a biannual rainy season. Historically, short rains fall from October to December and long rains from February to May. However, the rains are increasingly erratic and highly variable (Kahurananga and Silkiluwasha 1997). During the study period from February 2003 – May 2004, the short rains failed. Therefore, the data were interpreted for one wet and one dry season. The topography and vegetation of Tarangire National Park and the adjacent study sites consists of rolling hills and valleys characterized by Combretum – Dalbergia and Acacia – Commiphora woodlands, grasslands, and floodplains (Lamprey 1963). The national park is notable for the permanent Tarangire River that bisects the park from south to north. Other water sources include seasonal waterholes, springs, and man-made dams as well as a small, semi- permanent, spring-fed stream in Loibor Serrit. The soil in well-drained areas is dark red, sandy clay loam and in flood plains is typically black cotton (Kahurananga and Silkiluwasha 1997). Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 112 Figure 4.1 Location of three study sites (boxes) in the Tarangire ecosystem. Density Estimates Large carnivore population abundances are often estimated by identifying individuals and groups (Schaller 1972; Smuts et al. 1977; Hofer and East 1995). This derives from long-term research projects focused on the ecology of individual animals. Identification of individuals is not feasible when population estimates are needed in short order for environmental problem-solving over large areas (such as, for example, human-wildlife conflicts). To this end, a number of indirect measures have been developed to estimate relative carnivore population abundances including scent-station surveys, scat surveys, Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 113 track counts, den surveys, and playback or vocalization response surveys (see Gese 2001). I used track counts to evaluate the abundance of large carnivores based on the identification and quantification of fresh spoor left by individual animals on dirt roads. Several studies utilizing track counts for the estimation of cougar, leopard and lion populations have established this technique as both repeatable and objective (Van Dyke et al. 1986; Martin and de Meulenaer 1988; Smallwood and Fitzhugh 1995; Stander 1998; and Funston et al. 2001). In particular, calibrations of leopard and lion spoor counts with direct counts of known individuals have established the validity of this methodology in estimating large carnivore population densities in southern Africa (Stander 1998; Funston et al. 2001). In this study, I use spoor counts broadly as a technique for estimating lion abundance in several study sites. I then calibrate the indirect spoor counts to a direct measure using individual identification of lions within a small area of Tarangire National Park. Indirect Sampling: Spoor Density I adapted to an east African environment methodologies established in southern Africa for large carnivore spoor counts (see Stander et al. 1997; Stander 1998; Funston et al. 2001). In southern Africa, the relatively sandy soil makes the detection of spoor fairly easy. Therefore, the road network is determined primarily based on road availability. In Tanzania, the soil often has a sandy top layer; however, in some areas, the red clay or Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 114 black cotton soil becomes hard packed and unsuitable for tracking. Therefore, in addition to road availability, I considered road suitability in my determination of transects. I conducted spoor counts of lions in three experimental areas: park (150 km2), Kikoti (202 km2), and Loibor Serrit (246 km2). Table 1 provides general characteristics of the roads in each study area. I selected roads in all three study areas that were primarily orientated in a north – south direction. This maximized the likelihood of detecting spoor as I noted most animal tracks and movements were in an east – west direction. Figure 4.2 provides an example of the roads sampled in Loibor Serrit. I enlisted help from traditional Hadzabe hunter-gatherers whose knowledge of and ability to detect wildlife tracks have been revered throughout Tanzania. Based on traditional practices, the Hadzabe hunt wildlife on foot by following tracks until they are close enough to shoot the animal with a bow and arrow. The ability to detect accurately and follow wildlife tracks is essential to their well-being and, in a cultural sense, survival. Today, in recognition of their dependency on wild meat, the Hadzabe are the only tribe in Tanzania that still maintains the right to hunt wildlife without observing the six month hunting season or first having to file for a permit. Therefore, they also have more opportunities than most individuals to keep their tracking skills sharp. I sampled roads for a period of 10 months. In two separate cases (park and Loibor Serrit) data collection was not possible for one month (February and May, respectively). I drove the roads at speeds between 8 and 12 km/h in the early morning with between two Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 115 Figure 4.2 The road network in Loibor Serrit. “B” indicates the beginning of a transect and “E” indicates the end. and four individuals sitting on the front bumpers and sides of the vehicle looking for spoor. I only recorded fresh spoor (i.e. <12 hours old), and therefore, on a given day, I only traveled roads that were not disturbed by other vehicles, livestock, rain, or wind within 12 hours of data collection. In addition, the afternoon before a road was sampled, all old spoor were erased by either dragging dead thorn bushes behind the car, or, in the case of the park, by heavy vehicle traffic. Nevertheless, frequent disturbances, particularly of vehicles, occurred after dark or in the mornings, and I often had to re-drive roads on another day in order to sample them in an undisturbed condition. I recorded data for each road once a month. The road penetration index, defined by Stander (1998) as the relationship between the number and length of roads and the size of the area sampled, indicated a high sampling intensity per study area compared to the Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 116 suggested ratio (6.5 km2: 1 km road, Stander 1998; Table 4.1). I recorded fresh spoor by species, group size, and in the case of lions, individual age group and sex. In all cases, I personally checked the spoor to ensure correct species identification (as we were working in two different languages, Hadzabe and Kiswahili, occasionally the species name was linguistically confused, though the track had been correctly identified; this prevented recording errors that might have resulted from fatigue). In addition, I recorded the time, location, and number of observers. An individual’s spoor was only recorded once per day. Spoor density is the number of individual animals’ spoor per km. Table 4.1 Road and sampling characteristics of the three study areas. Categories Study Area Tarangire N. P. Kikoti Loibor Serrit Sample Area (km2) 150 202 246 No. of Roads 7 9 6 Avg. Road Length (km) 7.8 ± 1.26 9.19 ± 1.3 13.2 ± 1.01 Total Distance of Roads (km) 55 83 79 Road Penetration* 2.6 2.4 3.1 Sampling Effort (n) 9 10 9 2 * Road Penetration is defined as the ratio of x km surface area: 1 km road (see Stander 1998). Direct Sampling: Visual Observation I estimated the density of lions directly for Tarangire National Park in the 150 km2 experimental site based on the individual identification and monitoring of lions. This estimate was independent of spoor counts, although in certain cases, following spoor from a road transect led to individual identification. Upon sighting lions, I photographed each animal with particular attention to the vibrissa-spot pattern that is unique to individual lions (Pennycuick and Rudnai 1970). In addition, ear notches, permanent Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 117 scars, mane size (in males), nose pigmentation, and tooth wear aided in individual recognition. I aged animals according to Smuts et al. (1978) who found body size differences in lions up to three years old. Lions up to two years of age are smaller than adults and are easily recognizable. Females over two years approach their maximum size and are more difficult to age, while males between the ages of two and four years generally have conspicuous, yet incomplete manes. Five-year-old males tend to have full manes, though in some cases, male lions can be maneless, and this was witnessed in the Tarangire ecosystem. Therefore, mane size is not always a reliable predictor of age. Body wear can also aid in estimating the age of lions. Older animals tend to begin to show signs of wear as a result of the dangers of hunting and maintaining pride territories and appear to have an increased number of ear notches and scars, significant tooth wear and discoloration, and, in the case of some older males, tattered manes. Finally, nose pigmentation has been proposed as a reliable means of estimating age based on the percentage of dark coloration in the nose (Whitman et al. 2004). These authors found lions’ noses increasingly darkened with age, reaching 50% blackness at 5 years of age, though variation in the rate of darkening was evident in two separate populations. I used nose pigmentation as an aid in ageing lions along with the aforementioned indicators. However, in some cases, I found cubs with entirely black noses suggesting that nose pigmentation may be expressed differently in the Tarangire population. I classified age groups as the following: juveniles (0 – 2 years), subadults (2 – 4 years), and adults (4+ years). I kept a record of sightings for each individual along with group associations and locality. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 118 Statistical Analyses Means are presented with standard error (X ± SE) or 95% confidence intervals. Differences in several means were analyzed using nonparametric statistics for nonnormally distributed data. The Kruskal-Wallis test indicates that groups are significantly different if the p value is less than 0.05. Tests of two means (e.g. wet and dry season values) utilized the standard two-sample t-test. In all cases, significant differences were measured at p < 0.05. NS indicates non-significant results. Results Reliability and Accuracy of Trackers The Hadzabe trackers were highly capable of identifying species by their spoor. I conducted independent tests of the Hadzabe’s abilities to detect lions from their spoor, including age group and sex, by locating and identifying lion spoor and then following the tracks until the actual lion was found. I then aged and sexed the lion, and the data were compared to the Hadzabe’s determinations (the Hadzabe were not allowed to see the actual animal until after they had identified the track). They correctly identified lions from their spoor in all 16 cases (Table 4.2). Stander et al. (1997) found similar results with San Bushmen in Namibia and provide a graphical analysis of the anatomical variation in spoor between several large carnivore species. The Hadzabe were also capable of correctly identifying the age group and sex of individual lions by their spoor (Table 4.2). Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 119 Table 4.2 The percentage of correctly identified lions from their spoor including determination of an individual’s age group and sex. Lion Species Age Group Number of Cases 16 Correct Identification 16 % Correct 47 44 94 28 97 Sex* 29 * Only adult and sub-adult spoor were sexed. 100 Effects of Locality and Seasonality on Spoor Density The average annual, wet season, and dry season spoor densities for lions, spotted hyenas, and leopards in all three experimental areas are presented in Table 4.3. Significant differences in the annual densities of lions (Kruskal-Wallis: χ2 = 7.32, d.f. = 2, p < .05) and spotted hyenas (Kruskal-Wallis: χ2 = 10.33, d.f. = 2, p < .01) were evident by area. No differences in annual densities between areas were found for leopards (p > 0.05). The median test for lions indicated that park spoor densities were significantly greater than the median value of .08 spoor/km, while spoor densities were significantly lower than the median in Loibor Serrit (χ2 = 11.23, d.f. = 2, p < .005). Spotted hyena spoor densities were also significantly greater than the median value (.42 spoor/km) in the park while lower in Kikoti and Loibor Serrit (χ2 = 8.04, d.f. = 2, p < .05). I also analyzed the data by season. Lion spoor densities (Table 4.3) are significantly greater in the park during the dry season (T = -2.92, p < 0.05) and in Kikoti during the wet season (T = 3.87, p < 0.01). A shortened wet season of February through May in Kikoti results in an even more significant increase in lion spoor density (T = 15.05, p < 0.0001). There is no seasonal difference in lion spoor density for Loibor Serrit unless the Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 120 wet season is extended through July. In this case, lion spoor density is slightly greater in the dry season than the wet season for Loibor Serrit (T = -3.45, p < 0.05). While no annual difference in lion spoor density was found between Kikoti and Loibor Serrit, during peak periods (February - May for Kikoti; August - December for Loibor Serrit) lion spoor density is significantly greater in Kikoti (T = 13.36, p < 0.0001). No seasonal differences among study areas were found for spotted hyenas regardless of the time period analyzed. This was also true for leopards except in the case of Loibor Serrit. Leopard spoor density was slightly greater during the shortened dry season (August – December; T = 2.39, p < 0.05). If one outlier point (no leopard spoor found) is left out of the Kikoti analysis, leopard spoor density is also significantly greater in the dry season (July – December; T = 3.82, p <0.01). Table 4.3 Overall and seasonal spoor densities (# of spoor/km; mean and standard error) for lion, spotted hyena, and leopard in the three study areas. Wet season is from February through June. Dry season is from July through December. Categories Lion Park Kikoti Loibor Serrit Spoor Density Number of Spoor Density Number of Spoor Density Number of n Overall Fresh Spoor Wet Season Fresh Spoor Dry Season Fresh Spoor 9 .21 ± .039 10 .093 ± .022 9 .063 ± .0076 Spotted Hyena Park 9 Kikoti 10 Loibor Serrit 9 .59 ± .044 .39 ± .018 .38 ± .033 100 69 41 .12 ± .038 .15 ± .027 .050 ± .011 25 54 14 .29 ± .040 .038 ± .0074 0.074 ± .0087 75 15 27 276 304 229 .59 ± .053 .39 ± .023 .34 ± .021 119 145 91 .60 ± .072 .40 ± .031 .42 .054 157 159 138 Leopard* Park 9 .030 ± .0091 14 6 8 .028 ± .011 .032 ± .015 10 22 Kikoti 10 .039 ± .0086 32 .052 ± .015 .026 ± .0060 Loibor Serrit 9 .041 ± .0065 26 9 17 .033 ± .0063 .048 ± .010 * If zero density data points are excluded (3 for park and 1 for Kikoti) overall spoor density becomes .045, .043, and .041 for leopard. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 121 Lion Group Size, Composition, and Sex Ratios The average size of foraging groups was 1.92 ± 0.14 (n = 106). There was no difference in foraging group size based on spoor between the three experimental areas (p > 0.05). However, foraging group size was significantly greater for lions visually observed in the park (3.50 ± 0.58, n = 22) in comparison to the spoor data (T = 15.5, p < 0.05). A comparison of age groups based on individual identification of lions and tracking data is presented in Table 4.4. Spoor data in the park revealed a lower percentage of juveniles as compared to those observed visually. If juveniles are excluded from the analysis, the percentages of adults and subadults appear similar between those visually observed in the park and those identified by spoor in the park and Kikoti. Tracking data from Loibor Serrit suggest a higher percentage of subadults and fewer adults. The observed sex ratio in the park (not including juveniles) was 2.4 females: 1 male. This was different from that determined by spoor in the park (1.1 females: 1 male), Kikoti (1.14 females: 1 male), and Loibor Serrit (1.11 females: 1 male). Table 4.4 Lion population structure based on age groups according to individually identified lions in the park and spoor data for the three experimental areas. Parentheses indicate percentages excluding juveniles. Area Park - Visual ID % Adults 47.9 (74.2) n 23 % Subadults 16.7 (25.8) n 8 % Juveniles 35.4 n 17 Park - Spoor 61.0 (72.6) 61 23.0 (27.4) 23 16.0 16 Kikoti 59.4 (68.3) 41 27.5 (31.7) 19 13.0 9 Loibor Serrit 51.2 (55.3) 21 41.5 (44.7) 17 7.3 3 Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 122 Lion Population Density Estimates Based on the individual recognition of lions in the park, a minimum estimate of the peak lion density (when all 31 adults and subadults are present at the height of the dry season) in the 150 km2 study area is 0.21 lions/km2. This is a conservative estimate that does not include juveniles or nomadic individuals. The spoor density in the park for the same month when all lions were found was 0.33 lion spoor/km (juveniles excluded), and the ratio of true density to spoor density is 0.64. Based on this conversion factor, minimum lion population densities (excluding juveniles; annual averages and 95% confidence intervals) in the three study areas are as follows: park (0.12 lions/km2, 0.068 – 0.16 lions/km2), Kikoti (0.052 lions/km2; 0.025 – 0.078 lions/km2), and Loibor Serrit (0.038 lions/km2; 0.028 – 0.049 lions/km2). Discussion Human – wildlife conflict is an extremely significant factor influencing the worldwide fate of large carnivore populations in the twenty-first century. In Chapter Three, I determined that an important aspect underlying negative attitudes toward lions among Maasai communities in the Tarangire ecosystem was the perception that lions are abundant outside of the national park. In this chapter, I test this belief by comparing the real and perceived abundance of lions in several land use areas located inside and outside of Tarangire National Park using spoor counts as the principal methodology. In addition, I consider the effects of the Maasai and sport hunters on lions by estimating the magnitude of annual kill rates. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 123 Maasai individuals correctly determined that lion abundance would be greater within the park as compared to areas outside its boundaries. Average annual lion density in the park is more than two times greater than in Kikoti or Loibor Serrit. However, this contradicts the popular perception that lions were highly abundant outside of the park. Furthermore, while annual lion densities were not significantly different between Kikoti and Loibor Serrit, this obscured important environmental variation. A more appropriate, seasonal analysis based on wet and dry season spoor densities (mean, 95% C.I.) indicated that Kikoti supported peak lion densities (0.093 lions/km2, 0.067 – 0.12 lions/km2) of twice the magnitude of Loibor Serrit (0.048 lions/km2, 0.035 – 0.061 lions/km2). Importantly, the Loibor Serrit lion population was characterized by a greater proportion of subadults than adults. This suggests that lion social dynamics may be a significant factor influencing human – lion conflicts in the Tarangire ecosystem. Subadult lions are typically displaced to poorer quality habitats until they are capable of establishing pride territories (Schaller 1972; Bertram 1973; Rudnai 1979; Hanby and Bygott 1987). Past research has shown that subadult lions are also more likely to be responsible for attacks on livestock and/or humans (Stander 1990; Saberwal et al. 1994). Therefore, Maasai perceptions of high lion densities may be a result of the greater encounter frequency of misbehaving and marauding subadults rather than the overall number of lions in the ecosystem. This suggests that in village lands, where marginal habitat is used by higher incidences of younger, nomadic individuals, human perceptions of the large cats are more strongly influenced by the social dynamic of lions than actual densities. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 124 Therefore, while village centers are capable of supporting lion populations, they do so at depressed levels and altered social structures compared to hunting blocks that do not incorporate local residences and land use activities. Observations made by professional sport hunters utilizing areas further east of the experimental sites led to similar conclusions. The preference of lions for hunting blocks in the absence of local communities indicates the importance of these areas in maintaining critical wildlife habitats outside of national parks and game reserves. This does not imply, however, that sport hunting has no negative effects on the lion population. The following estimates of lion population size in the Tarangire ecosystem facilitate testing of the Maasai belief that the lion population outside of the park tops 1000 animals (in an area of equivalent size to the park; 2400 km2) as well as consideration of the impacts of both Maasai and sport hunting kill rates on lions. However, it should be noted that these are preliminary estimates, and all estimates are exclusive of juveniles. They are applicable only for the period of study and may fluctuate annually according to environmental variability. Significantly, the study was conducted in a year of less than average rainfall. Nevertheless, using the most conservative annual estimate of lion density in the ecosystem (0.038 lions/km2), the minimum lion population size for the Tarangire ecosystem is 456 lions. This is very likely to be an underestimate, since average lion densities for the park and habitats excluding village centers are greater. However, even if the larger, annual density estimate for Kikoti is used, resulting in 624 lions, the lion Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 125 population is still below 1000 animals in an area of 12,000 km2. This indicates that the Maasai have significantly overestimated the abundance of lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. Therefore, it is possible that if a concerted effort is made to educate the Maasai regarding the actual, much lower abundance of lions, it may have important implications for improving Maasai tolerance of lions and reducing aggressive actions toward them. These estimates also have important implications in terms of the effects of Maasai and sport hunting kill rates. In Chapter Three, I conservatively estimated that 39.9 lions were killed per year among the seven Maasai villages outside the park boundary. Attacks on lions were nonselective resulting in the deaths of all age groups and both sexes. Analyzing this kill rate in terms of the abovementioned population estimates suggests that the Maasai-induced, annual lion mortality is between 6.4% and 8.8%. However, in years preceding the warrior’s Eunoto ceremony, a significant spike in male lion mortality may occur in response to an increased demand for lion headdresses. Conversations with local Maasai indicated an interest in reviving this activity in Tarangire where traditional rites have somewhat eroded over time. Because sport hunting is generally selective for adult males (though immature animals are sometimes illegally taken), the permitted quota of 15 lions/year (Chapter Two) is analyzed in terms of the adult male portion of the population only. Given a 1:1 sex ratio and an average proportion of 65.4% adults throughout the three study sites, this results in annual adult male mortality of between 7.4% and 10.1%. High adult male Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 126 mortality was corroborated by observations of very few males over six years of age in the entire study system. While separately, Maasai-induced and sport hunting-induced mortality rates are not exceedingly high (based on average adult survivorship of 70% to 90%; Schaller 1972; Packer et al. 1988; Orford et al. 1988; Whitman et al. 2004), their combined effects, along with loss of habitat and reductions in prey populations, are most likely responsible for the lower lion density in Loibor Serrit. Certainly, all of these factors threaten the long-term persistence of lions in the ecosystem. And, it should be noted that while no data on the reproductive rates of lions throughout the population were available, this information contributes significantly to the determination of human effects on lions. Past research has demonstrated that land to the east of Tarangire National Park plays a crucial role in maintaining large, migratory herbivores during the wet season (TMCP 2002). Data and observations made during the course of this study now indicate this is also true for large carnivores, particularly lions. During the wet season, the abundance of park lions decreases, and park prides expand their home ranges to include areas to the east of the park where they presumably interact with other non-park prides and nomadic individuals (based on visual identification of 11 park lions outside of the park during the wet season). Increases in lion home ranges as a result of fluctuations in prey availability have been well documented (Bertram 1973; Stander 1991; Viljoen 1993). These results have important implications for lion conservation in the Tarangire ecosystem given the increasing trend in habitat conversion for cultivation among resident Maasai communities and immigrant/expatriate communities (Muir 1994; Lama 1998; Chapter Three) as well as the higher mortality rates of lions in village areas. In the absence of successful efforts Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 127 to protect remaining wildlife habitats to the east of the park, it is expected that the lion population in the Tarangire ecosystem will be seriously diminished and increasingly susceptible to the negative, genetic effects of small, isolated populations. Lions are a prominent species whose ecological and behavioral characteristics destine them to come into contact with human communities. Whereas spotted hyenas and, particularly, leopards appeared more robust to differences in the experimental sites, lions suffered in areas of high human habitation. Weaver et al. (1996) demonstrated varying resilience to environmental disturbances among wolves, cougars, wolverines, and grizzly bears according to foraging behavior, dispersal capabilities, and reproductive rates. Crooks (2002) further demonstrated that body size was an important component affecting relative carnivore sensitivities to habitat fragmentation. Utilizing these criteria, the comparatively large lion home ranges (e.g. 42-369 km2; Viljoen 1993), group sizes (e.g. average 5.9 lions/pride; Schaller 1972), prey size requirements (e.g. 90% of total kills in Serengeti were buffalo, wildebeest, zebra, and warthog; Scheel and Packer 1991), and overall body size of lions (e.g. 174.9 kg - males, 119.5 kg - females, average body mass for East African lions; adapted from Smuts et al. 1980) indicates that they may need larger, less disturbed habitats for their survival. However, their relatively high reproductive rate suggests that where habitat is available, lions will be fairly resistant to other human disturbances such as controlled hunting (Whitman et al. 2004; Kikoti). In comparison, the relatively smaller body size of leopards (e.g. 58 kg – males, 37.5 kg – females; Bailey 1993), smaller home ranges (e.g. 37-38 km2 – females in the Serengeti; Cavallo 1993), solitary nature (Bailey 1993), and high adaptability of leopards in terms of Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 128 prey selection and size (Bailey 1993) suggest that they will be more resilient to landscape disturbances. The presence of significant leopard populations in the outskirts of large towns lends testimony to this (Martin and de Meulenaer 1988). Finally, the research presented here demonstrated the utility of spoor counts in providing a successful, relatively low-cost strategy for the simultaneous monitoring of several large carnivore populations. I was able to sample several different habitats that provided important insights into the environmental variation within carnivore populations as well as the effects of seasonality on carnivore distribution. In addition, differences in the foraging group size and sex ratio of lions determined by actual sightings and spoor counts demonstrated the ability of this technique to accurately reflect lion population structure. Certainly, larger groups of lions are easier to spot visually than smaller ones, particularly in the undulating landscape and long grass of the Tarangire ecosystem. Therefore, lion sightings are probably biased toward larger groups. Other studies have demonstrated that pride members spend more time in smaller sub-groups or even alone (Schaller 1972; Packer 1986) suggesting that spoor counts actually present a more accurate representation of lion foraging group sizes than visual observations. In terms of the sex ratio, higher male spoor counts than expected might be a result of the increased likelihood of males to patrol their territories and therefore a greater than average likelihood of crossing roads and being detected by spoor versus visual observations. Furthermore, male spoor counts may be incorporating a greater proportion of nomadic Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 129 males whose active avoidance of pride males may make them less likely to be observed visually. This is supported by Schaller (1972) who found nomadic males walk farther each day than pride animals, and he determined that when nomadic habitats were incorporated in population sampling, the result was an equal sex ratio. In conclusion, I suggest that the use of spoor counts to estimate large carnivore abundance and distribution is an effective, ecologically sensitive method for monitoring these species. This ‘hands-off’ methodology is particularly useful in areas where large carnivores are already threatened by human activities and where interference with them via radio-collaring and other forms of handling would add additional, unnecessary stress as well as fatalities. And, the benefits of this methodology transcend ecological boundaries by incorporating the extensive knowledge of local communities such as the Hadzabe, thereby providing an avenue for the utilization of an ancient, dwindling art form in a modern context. This methodology could be easily adopted for use by wildlife rangers and game scouts supported by governmental, public and private organizations, and companies working in the national parks, game reserves, and communal lands of many African countries. References Bailey, T. N. 1993. The African leopard: a study of the ecology and behavior of a solitary felid. Columbia University Press, New York, New York. Bertram, B. C. R. 1973. Lion population regulation. East African Wildlife Journal 11: 215-225. Cavallo, J. 1993. A study of leopard behavior and ecology in the Seronera Valley, Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 130 Serengeti National Park. Pages 33-43 in Serengeti Wildlife Research Centre Scientific Report 1990-1992, Serengeti, Tanzania. Conforti, V. A., and F. C. Cascelli de Azevedo. 2003. Local perceptions of jaguars (Panthera onca) and pumas (Puma concolor) in the Iguacu National Park area, south Brazil. Biological Conservation. 111: 215-221. Crooks, K. R. 2002. Relative sensitivities of mammalian carnivores to habitat fragmentation. Conservation Biology 16(2): 488-502. Funston, P. J., E. Herrmann, P. Babupi, A. Kruiper, H. Kruiper, H. Jaggers, K. Masule, and K. Kruiper. 2001. Spoor frequency estimates as a method of determining lion and other large mammal densities in the Kgalagadi Transfrontier Park. Unpublished draft provided by main author. Gese, E. M. 2001. Monitoring of terrestrial carnivore populations. Pages 372-396 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Gittleman, J. L., S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne. 2001. Why ‘carnivore conservation’? Pages 1-7 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Hanby, J. P. and J. D. Bygott. 1987. Emigration of subadult lions. Animal Behavior 35: 161-169. Hofer, H. and M. East. 1995. Population dynamics, population size, and the commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyenas. Pages 332-363 in A. R. E. Sinclair and P. Arcese, eds. Serengeti II: Dynamics, management, and conservation of an ecosystem. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Kahurananga, J. and F. Silkiluwasha. 1997. The migration of zebra and wildebeest between Tarangire National Park and Simanjiro plains, northern Tanzania, in 1972 and recent trends. African Journal of Ecology 35: 179-185. Lama, L.1998. Conflicts and compatibility: An inventory and analysis of land use in a Tanzanian wildlife corridor. Ph.D. Dissertation. Binghamton University, New York. Lamprey, H. F. 1963. Ecological separation of the large species in the Tarangire game reserve. Tanganyika. East African Wildlife Journal 1: 67-92. Martin, R. B. and T. de Meulenaer. 1988. Survey of the status of the leopard Panthera pardus in sub-Saharan Africa. CITES, Gland, Switzerland. Muir, A. 1994. A situational analysis of pastoralism in Simanjiro district, Tanzania. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 131 VETAID, Arusha, Tanzania. Oli, M. K., I. R. Taylor, and M. E. Rogers. 1994. Snow leopard (Panthera uncia) predation on livestock: An assessment of local perceptions in the Annapurna Conservation Area, Nepal. Biological Conservation 68: 63-68. Orford, H. J. L., M. R. Perrin, and H. H. Berry. 1988. Contraception, reproduction, and demography of free-ranging Etosha lions (Panthera leo). Journal of Zoology 216: 717-733. Packer, C. 1986. The ecology of sociality in felids. Pages 429-451 in D. I. Rubenstein and R. W. Wrangham, eds. Ecological aspects of social evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Pennycuick, C. J. and J. Rudnai. 1970. A method of identifying individual lions, Panthera leo, with the analysis of the reliability of identification. Journal of Zoology 160: 497-508. Pratt, D. J., P. J. Greenway, and M. D. Gwynne. 1966. A classification of East African rangeland. Journal of Applied Ecology 3: 369-382. Rudnai, J. 1979. Ecology of lions in Nairobi National Park and the adjoining Kitengela Conservation Unit in Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 17: 85-95. Saberwal, V. K., J. P. Gibbs, R. Chellam, and A. J. T. Johnsingh. 1994. Lion-human conflict in the Gir Forest, India. Conservation Biology 8(2): 501-507. Schaller, G. B. 1972. The Serengeti lion: a study of predator-prey relations. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Scheel, D. and C. Packer. 1991. Group hunting behavior of lions: a search for cooperation. Animal Behavior 41: 697-709. Smallwood, K. S. and E. L. Fitzhugh. 1995. A track count for estimating mountain lions Felis concolor california population trends. Biological Conservation 71: 251-259. Smuts, G. L., I. J. Whyte, and T. W. Dearlove. 1977. A mass capture technique for lions. East African Wildlife Journal 15: 81-87. Smuts, G. L., J. L. Anderson, and J. C. Austin. 1978. Age determination of the African lion (Panthera leo). Journal of the Zoological Society of London. 185: 115-146. Smuts, G. L., J. Hanks, and I. J. Whyte. 1980. Comparative growth of wild male and female lions (Panthera leo). Journal of Ecology 190: 365-373. Stander, P. E. 1990. A suggested management strategy for stock-raiding lions in Lichtenfeld – Chapter Four 132 Namibia. South African Journal of Wildlife Research. 20: 37-43. Stander, P. E. 1991. Demography of lions in Etosha National Park, Namibia. Madoqua 18: 1-9. Stander, P. E. 1998. Spoor counts as indices of large carnivore populations: the relationship between spoor frequency, sampling effort, and true density. Journal of Applied Ecology 35; 378-385. Stander, P. E., //. Ghau, D. Tsisaba, //. =oma, and l.l ui. 1997. Tracking and the interpretation of spoor: a scientifically sound method in ecology. Journal of the Zoological Society of London 242: 329-341. Tarangire – Manyara Conservation Project Final Report. 2002. Istituto Oikos and University of Insubria, Italy in Collaboration with TANAPA, Arusha, Tanzania. Van Dyke, F. G., R. H. Brooke, and H. G. Shaw. 1986. Use of road track counts as indices of mountain lion presence. Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 102-109. Viljoen, P. C. 1993. The effects of changes in prey availability on lion predation in a large natural ecosystem in northern Botswana. Pages 193-213 in N. Dunstone and M. L. Gorman, eds. Mammals as predators. Proceedings of the Symposium of the Zoological Society of London, No. 65. Clarendon, Oxford. Weaver, J. L., P. C. Paquet, and L. F. Ruggiero. 1996. Resilience and conservation of large carnivores in the Rocky Mountains. Conservation Biology 10(4): 964-976. Weber, W. and A. Rabinowitz. 1996. A global perspective on large carnivore conservation. Conservation Biology 10(4): 1046-1054. Whitman, K., A. M. Starfield, H. S. Quadling, and C. Packer. 2004. Sustainable trophy hunting of African lions. Nature 428: 175-178. Woodroffe, R. and J. R. Ginsberg. 1998. Edge effects and the extinction of population inside protected areas. Science 280: 2126-2128. CHAPTER FIVE Conclusion: The African Lion Roaming Through Wilderness and the Human Mind Chapter Five In the preceding chapters, I presented research on human – lion relationships in the context of both social and ecological systems. In Chapter One, I introduced the sociocultural, political, economic, psychological, and ecological factors influencing the longterm persistence of lions in human-dominated landscapes. While, quite naturally, all of these factors are influential in affecting human – lion relationships, I predicted that throughout the course of my study, certain key variables would emerge as having greater importance in securing the long-term conservation of the African lion. Not only did these variables come forth in Chapters 2-4, emphasized below, but I also discovered that in order to maintain the truly wild lion, as both an ecological and psychological reality, the preservation of lions in protected areas, separate from human communities, is not enough. Instead, despite the hardships of living among lions, we collectively crave the possibility of prolonging human coexistence with these dangerous cats whose presence has shaped our own relationship with the natural world for as long as we can remember. In Chapter Two, I focused on the complicated set of socio-cultural, political, and economic factors that combine to influence attitudes toward lions among diverse constituencies, including the sport hunting and photographic industries as well as Maasai communities. Within a single landscape, I demonstrated that a wide variety of positive and negative attitudes toward lions existed that influenced individual desires for lion conservation. In particular, the nature of human – lion interactions emerged as a significant variable affecting human perceptions of the large cats. While those individuals from the tourism industries benefited positively from the freedom to experience lions on Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 135 their own terms, Maasai individuals noted the difficulties of daily cohabitation with them. Beyond the physical realities of living with one of the world’s largest cats, they also emphasized the importance of unclear property rights and the lack of authority to control wildlife as highly significant variables influencing their negative attitudes and actions toward lions. In fact, confusion in interpreting the wildlife and land policies and laws of Tanzania contributed to aggravated relationships among all the stakeholders. In Chapter Three, I shifted the analysis from a strongly social perspective to one that considered both the ecological and social dimensions of human interactions with lions from a Maasai point of view. The results of this study demonstrated that Maasai interactions with lions were as strongly influenced by the ecological realities of livestock predation and human – lion encounters as they were by the social and psychological concepts of fear and risk that are stimulated by the notion of living alongside a large and dangerous predator like the lion. And again, Maasai discontent with insecure property rights and their inability to locally manage wildlife were emphasized as having important, negative consequences affecting local tolerance of lions. Then, in Chapter Four, I swung the disciplinary balance in favor of ecology and considered the impact of different human land uses on the distribution and abundance of lions as they navigated an environment wrought with human-drawn boundaries. I determined that habitat availability was an important ecological variable influencing the abundance of lions. Higher lion densities were found in areas of lower human presence that were most likely associated with both greater prey availability and reduced conflicts Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 136 with humans. But interestingly, in village habitats, I also learned that lion behavioral ecology was significantly influencing human – lion relationships. Younger, nomadic individuals typically relegated to marginal habitats and probably lacking important, learned behaviors in terms of wariness of humans were more abundant in village communities. In this sense, human transformation of habitat has unwittingly led to the increased likelihood of local communities interacting with the more typically misbehaving segment of the lion population: the young, marauding, overtly bold, subadult lions who, by virtue of their risky behavior, give lions a bad name. In this final chapter, I now contemplate the linkages between these chapters and, more broadly, human and ecological systems, as they bear on the future of human – lion relationships in the twenty-first century and, inevitably, humankind’s position in the natural world. Specifically, I provide conclusions regarding the positive and negative influences of the Maasai communities as well as of the sport hunting and photographic industries on the lion, and I reconsider the relative significance of the social and ecological factors influencing the lion’s survival. This leads me to return to the original schematic diagram presented in Chapter One (see Figure 1.1) and to revise and expand it in order to more accurately represent a theoretical model of human – lion relationships. Finally, towards the end of the chapter, I move beyond the data to provide a number of policy suggestions at the local, national, and international levels that I believe would make an important contribution towards the better integration of the needs of both people and lions for their mutual benefit. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 137 The Maasai communities in this study were in the unique position among the stakeholders of cohabitating with lions on a daily basis. From an ecological point of view, the long-term, definitive presence of the Maasai and their land use decisions as they react to the pressures of human population growth and expansion as well as an increasing trend towards cultivation will significantly influence the fate of the lion outside of Tarangire National Park. In addition, both the economic costs and psychological effects of living among lions have weighed heavily on their overall attitudes toward lions, often resulting in negative perceptions. Nevertheless, the Maasai maintain a degree of tolerance for lions based on the long-standing, cultural value of lion hunting. Importantly, throughout this scenario, indicating mixed emotions toward lions, ran the recurrent political problem of ambiguous property rights. As a collective group incurring the costs of living among lions, the Maasai appeared to be the last to benefit from them economically and were frustrated over their lack of authority to manage wildlife tourism on village lands as well as the apparent unwillingness of the government to take responsibility for human – lion conflicts. In a sense, while the political solutions to these problems are out of their hands, as they await the government’s decision to implement community rights to manage wildlife, the Maasai are creating their own local solutions by clearing otherwise vacant land, thereby demonstrating their ownership. Unless this problem is resolved and the Maasai begin to receive significant benefits from lions, it is unlikely that they will tolerate their presence much longer, and it is possible that the eradication of lions may become a symbolic demonstration of their discontent. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 138 Individuals from the sport hunting industry expressed much more positive attitudes toward lions based on their values of lions, the importance of lions to the industry’s economy, and the thrill of encountering one of nature’s most dangerous cats. In Chapter Four, I showed that the presence of hunting blocks can maintain important habitat for lions. Yet beyond this and the conservation programs of several concerned and proactive companies, respondents themselves indicated that the majority of hunting companies in Tanzania were more interested in short-term gains than the long-term conservation of lion populations. Indeed, as a country containing a relatively large remaining population of lions, Tanzania affords short-sighted companies the freedom to be relatively unaffected by declining lion populations in certain blocks, simply moving their lion hunts to other areas. Ironically, this mobility – supported by an impression of abundant lion populations in Tanzania – reduces the incentive to invest locally in conservation and may contribute to future declines in lion populations throughout the country. And importantly, despite a great respect for lions and wildlife that is common among hunters, the pressures of market forces – the necessity to provide an expectant client with a lion trophy in a short time frame – encourages the use of illegal practices among some companies (i.e. shooting subadult lions and females, hunting at night) and may discourage the majority of companies from supporting a review of lion hunting quotas. Many of the positive aspects of human – lion relationships demonstrated by the sport hunting respondents, in terms of wildlife values, the thrill of encountering lions, and the iconic stature and importance of the wild feline to the industry, were also shared by individuals from the photographic industry. Among all of the stakeholders, photographic Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 139 companies appeared to have the least amount of negative influence on lions. However, the perception of ‘no impact’ can also have negative consequences if it is then used to justify a lack of responsibility for the wildlife or area from which a company benefits. Similarly to sport hunting companies, photographic respondents mentioned the relative mobility of their industry and noted that the presence of lions in Tarangire was not essential to their economic success. These considerations, as well as jurisdictional confusion regarding the rights of photographic companies to establish business agreements with local communities, can create a lack of incentives for photographic companies to invest heavily in conservation activities outside of national parks. Based on these conclusions, certain social and ecological factors originally presented in Chapter One as potentially influencing lion populations (see Table 1.1) emerge as more significant than others. A revised and contextually specific model is now presented in Table 5.1 that reflects the unique relationship of each of the stakeholders with lions in the Tarangire ecosystem. For example, negative psychological factors appear to predominantly affect those individuals cohabitating with lions, while political variables are important components of all the stakeholders’ relationships with lions. In terms of ecological variables, habitat availability, population abundance, and behavioral ecology appear to be more important in Tarangire than disease or reproductive ecology. Socioculturally, all the stakeholders express positive values toward lions that are nevertheless derived from extremely different types of interactions with them. However, the Maasai also express negative values and are the only group to face the economic costs of lions. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 140 Table 5.1 Positive and negative factors influencing human - lion relationships organized by key ecological and social variables. Variables ECOLOGICAL Maasai Communities Habitat availability/use - increasing cultivation of land - growing human population Population abundance - retaliation against livestockraiding lions - increased hunting of males during ceremonial periods Behavioral ecology - marginal habitat in village areas attracts subadult lions POLITICAL Property rights - insecure property rights/lack of control result in symbolic demonstrations of power Authority/responsibility - frustration with gov response to conflicts decreases tolerance - lack of authority to control wildlife tourism on village lands creates resentment Wildlife policies/law - jurisdictional confusion results in strained relations with all stakeholders and less incentive to conserve ECONOMIC Wildlife benefits - potential revenue from wildlife tourism promotes tolerance Wildlife costs - loss of livestock - human injury or loss of life - time spent protecting livestock from large carnivores SOCIO-CULTURAL Wildlife values - respect for lion as natural foe - negativistic value as a result of hardships induced by lion Human-lion interactions - negative interactions resulting from attacks on humans or livestock decrease tolerance Traditions/customs - cultural reverence for lion hunting encourages tolerance PSYCHOLOGICAL Risk/fear - real and potential risk of human injury or livestock loss reduces tolerance of lions Sport Hunting Companies Population abundance - unscientifically determined quotas lead to over-shooting in certain blocks - shooting of young males that haven't reached reproductive Peak Photographic Companies Behavioral ecology - habituation of lions may endanger animals outside park - overcrowding of lions may impact social behavior Behavioral ecology - removal of adult males in village areas enhances destabilized age structure Land use relations - government demarcated hunting blocks lead to local resentment of sport Hunting - incompatibility of sport hunting and photographic tourism in same time/space Wildlife policies/law - jurisdictional confusion results in strained relations with all stakeholders and less incentive to conserve Incentives/disincentives - lions are essential to Industry - company mobility reduces incentive to locally conserve lions Market forces - pressure to obtain lion trophy can encourage illegal practices Land use relations - incompatibility of sport hunting and photographic tourism in same time/space Wildlife policies/law - jurisdictional confusion results in strained relations with all stakeholders and less incentive to conserve - insecure agreements discourage investment in conservation Incentives/disincentives - lions are essential to industry - company mobility reduces incentive to locally conserve lions - nonconsumptive designation can create perception of lack of impact and therefore less responsibility for wildlife Wildlife values - lion as symbol of wildness - lion as icon of hunting industry Wildlife values - lion as symbol of wildness and ecosystem health - lion as icon of wildlife safaris Human-lion interactions - excitement of dangerous encounter with wild animal - decline in overall lion numbers makes lion hunting a more exclusive/rare experience Human-lion interactions - excitement of dangerous encounter with wild animal - enhanced experience when lions are viewed in exclusive, wild settings Fear - enjoyment of atavistic fear of lions not normally encountered in daily life Fear - enjoyment of atavistic fear of lions not normally encountered in daily life Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 141 While we are still a long way from assuring the long-term survival of lions and many other, similarly challenged large carnivores, this study provides a step forward by highlighting the major factors influencing the future of lion conservation in the Tarangire ecosystem. Although we do not have enough of an understanding of human – lion relationships or, more broadly, human – large carnivore relationships in other contexts, countries, and continents, the application of this type of study in other circumstances is very possible. And, the following theoretical model of human – lion relationships as well as general conclusions are widely applicable to the global community of humans and large carnivores; simply replace “lion” with the large carnivore of choice. The lion has emerged in this study as a species of worldly significance. At a time when the rate of human – induced species extinctions is reaching alarming levels, it is impressive that a large carnivore, like the lion, still manages to find a place in an increasingly human-dominated and altered landscape. From a societal point of view, our tolerance of lions is rooted in our attitudes toward them that appear strongly influenced by the cultural value of these big cats as symbols of power, bravery, and wildness. In the Tarangire ecosystem, even the Maasai, who stand to lose the most, in a daily, economic sense, by the mere presence of lions and hold generally negative attitudes toward them, consider the total loss of lions to be socially undesirable. Therefore, as a cultural icon that moves beyond the boundaries of its own ecological distribution to reach humanity on a global scale, the lion is incredibly well poised to endure in our minds and to engender support for its survival. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 142 But as an ecological reality, the same bold, grand, intrepid, and fearful characteristics that human society reveres in lions are also the traits that inevitably destine lions to come into conflict with local people in a shared landscape. Healthy lion populations cannot be confined to small spaces. In the Tarangire ecosystem, the national park, at its widest point, is only 40 km across – a distance a lion can cover in several nights. The ecological requirements of the lion for large territories and abundant prey compel it to defy humanimposed boundaries on the land, roaming through areas of human habitation in their quest to survive. And the behavioral characteristics of the lions – their bold nature, their guttural roars, and their social lifestyle – mean that their presence will rarely go unnoticed. Even when they cunningly adapt their behaviors, becoming more silent, more nocturnal, more wary, in high pressure areas, the massive paw print of one of the world’s largest cats seems like an arrogant reminder that, yes, a lion was here. Indeed, if the wild lion is to survive in both our minds and our world – that image of power, raw energy, and merciless danger – then his heavy tread cannot be separated from both the habitat and the people who have made him so. In this sense, we must begin to consider a more complex and accurate model of human – lion relationships that truly reflects the connectivity of people and lions throughout their collective histories and landscapes. Revising the schematic diagram provided in Chapter One (see Figure 1.1), I now emphasize the abilities of each “culture,” feline and human alike, to affect each other through their respective tolerance of one another. This results in a theoretical model of human – lion or, more broadly, human – carnivore relationships presented in Figure 5.1. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 143 Figure 5.1 A Theoretical Model of Human – Lion Relationships. Ecosystem Human Dimension Political Factors Socio-Cultural Factors Lion Tolerance for Humans and Livestock Lion Dimension Political Factors Stakeholders Psychological Factors Economic Factors Socio-Cultural Factors Lions Human Tolerance for Lions and Prey Species Psychological Factors Economic Factors Prey Population In this model, people and lions share a landscape or ecosystem and respond to one another in a feedback loop where one “culture” affects another via their perceptions and attitudes (represented by dashed arrows from human/lion dimension) which in turn alter their relative tolerances of one another and inevitably their actions (solid arrow from human/lion tolerance) toward each other. The perceptions and attitudes of both humans and lions are variable and are influenced by the factors incorporated within the human/lion dimension and by the actions of one upon the other. From a socio-biological perspective, the “cultural” dimension of lions is articulated by recognizing that political, socio-cultural, psychological, and economic factors transcend human – wildlife boundaries, acting as lexicons that can also be applied to the big cats. Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 144 For instance, economic factors affecting lions can be described ecologically as the costs and benefits associated with optimal foraging dynamics, optimal group size, and prey size selection. Political factors affecting lions include territorial disputes, the development of male coalitions, and pride dynamics. Socio-culturally, as the only truly social cat, the lion has developed a number of behavioral “customs” or traits including cooperative hunting, shared female rearing of offspring, and infanticide. And finally, as was mentioned in Chapter Four, lions also respond to psychological concepts such as risk and fear as they react to human communities, weighing the costs of lion – lion risk with those of human – lion risk. Just as fear can stimulate bravado in some human communities, hunger in lions can result in more risky and bold behavior. Therefore, from this model, it becomes clear that both humans and lions are capable of changing their attitudes toward one another based on variable characteristics of the ecological and social environment. In order to further verify the theories generated by this study, the next round of research should develop and test hypotheses regarding the circumstances under which people and predator relationships change in a broader, global context. Finally, the long-term coexistence of humans and lions in the wild will depend not only on recognition that the lion’s contribution to humanity – enriching the meaning and experience of human life – merits support for the species continuance, but also, that as recipients of the lion’s endowment, we have a responsibility to take active steps to ensure the conservation of lions in a manner that is both ecologically and socially appropriate. The conservation of lions depends on a public commitment to their preservation on international, national, regional, and perhaps most importantly, local levels. This means Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 145 that we need to acknowledge the discrepancies in our current social systems that permit the global benefit-sharing of lions, in particular as a supreme figure in the extensive photographic and sport hunting spheres, and more generally as a source of human creativity and development, while the costs of maintaining lions are born disproportionately by the world-wide minority of communities still living among them. More specifically, this means that we will have to work to breakdown the political, economic, and legal barriers that prevent local communities, like the Maasai, from sharing in more of the positive aspects of human – lion relationships that are enjoyed by the majority of individuals, businesses, and government entities who, once removed from the daily threats of these bold cats, have the freedom to experience. Policy Recommendations In this final section, I provide some policy recommendations that are supported by the ecological and social data of this study. While perhaps not conventional, I believe these recommendations, if adopted, could make an important contribution to lion conservation in a variety of settings. While organized into local, national, and international categories, all of the recommendations below are considered to be of primary importance, and their ordering should not suggest that some are of secondary significance. In the Tarangire ecosystem: (1) While a variety of different attitudes toward lions are prevalent in the Tarangire ecosystem, all of the stakeholders from the photographic and sport hunting industries, as well as the Maasai communities, indicated an interest in future collaboration Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 146 regarding lion conservation and management. This is particularly noteworthy for individuals from the wildlife tourism industries; despite sharing the lion as an essential species in their businesses, both expressed aggravated relationships with each other resulting from ideological differences as well as direct conflicts over land use. In addition, individuals from both industries indicated that they were not necessarily dependent upon maintaining the long-term presence of the lion in the Tarangire ecosystem, since opportunities to hunt or view lions existed in many other areas of Tanzania. Therefore, this positive step towards collaboration also suggests an emerging willingness to accept greater responsibility for the areas from which they profit. Furthermore, while Maasai communities portrayed relatively positive sentiments toward these industries, resentments regarding the inequitable distribution of benefits derived from lions and rights to wildlife, particularly in terms of the perceived link between the sport hunting industry and the national government, were contributing to negative attitudes toward lions. Therefore, given the positive atmosphere towards greater collaboration, the development of a cooperative forum of individuals – inclusive of the sport hunting and photographic industries, Maasai communities, government entities, and research and/or conservation organizations involved with lions – is recommended to coordinate lion management and conservation within the Tarangire ecosystem. Having established a positive working relationship with all of the various stakeholders, the People and Predators Fund might act as a catalyst for the initial meeting. This forum could be particularly effective in working collectively to resolve the local conflicts that arise from the interactions of these varied constituencies and each other. In addition, this forum should emphasize Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 147 the importance of educating clients, both photographic and sport hunting, to act responsibly in the ecosystem. This group should also address the subsequent policy recommendations for the Tarangire ecosystem. (2) One of the social factors significantly influencing negative attitudes toward lions among the Maasai was their feeling of powerlessness in terms of making land use decisions and regulating tourism activities related to lions on village lands. While the Tanzanian government has made promises of increased community control over wildlife, Maasai individuals appeared frustrated with the slow pace at which this new policy was being implemented. The timely establishment of the legal rights for local communities to manage wildlife on their lands is supported by this study. An increased willingness to conserve lions is linked with the ability of local individuals to derive significant, real benefits from them, but the converse is also possible. As communities grow tired of waiting, they may lose their desire to protect wildlife and act out against it. This is becoming evident in the Tarangire ecosystem where individuals are clearing land for the sole purpose of demonstrating their lawful right to it. Since legal authority will not be granted without government support and implementation, this suggestion is applicable to both the Tarangire ecosystem and Tanzania as a whole. (3) Support was shown among all the stakeholders involved in the Tarangire ecosystem for a governmental, scientific review of the sport hunting industry and quota system as it pertains to lion hunting. Many individuals felt that lion quotas were too high to Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 148 ensure the long-term persistence of particular lion populations in Tanzania. Certainly, the results of this study indicate that in Loibor Serrit, the combined effects of sport hunting and Maasai kill rates resulted in a depleted population with few large, adult males; therefore the temporary suspension of lion hunting in that block is advisable. On the contrary, in Kikoti, the lion population appeared healthier, and if professional hunters adhere to the recent concept of only shooting animals of six years or more, this will most likely be sufficient to regulate the quota in that area. Given these varied results, a block-by-block review of lion quotas would be the most ecologically appropriate course of action, though the financial and institutional capabilities for such an undertaking are probably unavailable. Nevertheless, significant interest was evident among the sport hunting companies interviewed to accept a greater role in monitoring lion populations within their blocks, and it is possible that a cooperative effort between the government, hunting companies, and research organizations could result in a new collaborative model for monitoring lions and other wildlife populations in Tanzania. In addition, the offtake of lions in all blocks should be monitored in an open and objective fashion. Again, this recommendation is applicable to both the Tarangire ecosystem as well as the entire country. (4) Of all the stakeholders, the Maasai displayed the most negative attitudes toward lions. Part of their negativity stemmed from the perceived likelihood of dangerous, human encounters with lions. While the number of lion attacks on humans appeared low in comparison to other areas of Tanzania, even the slightest possibility of an attack is enough to discourage support for lion conservation. Therefore, the development of a Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 149 large carnivore education program that works collaboratively with the Maasai to identify ways in which to further reduce the chance of an attack and that also addresses the more positive aspects of maintaining a lion presence in the ecosystem is recommended. This program should be geared to both men and women, and it should emphasize the cultural significance of the lion that was indicated by many individuals to be an important component affecting their overall tolerance of lions and, therefore, the persistence of the species in the ecosystem (5) While unprovoked attacks on humans occurred fairly infrequently, the predation of lions on livestock was a significant problem in the Tarangire ecosystem that led to economic hardships, human injuries, and retaliation against stock-raiding animals. Given the severity of the problem, on both sides, the continued monitoring of livestock predation in the Tarangire ecosystem is recommended. A livestock predation monitoring program should be implemented in all of the villages, and the development of a provisional compensation program for livestock losses due to large carnivores should be attempted. Given that many depredations occurred when animals were lost in the bush and probably could have been prevented, information regarding ways to avoid livestock predation should also be provided to the local communities. In Tanzania, in addition to numbers two and three above, policy suggestions include: (1) This study and many others have demonstrated that the persistence of large carnivores, like lions, outside of protected areas is heavily dependent on positive Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 150 relationships with the human communities they live among. Where human – carnivore conflicts are prevalent, in particular where large carnivores aggravate people by killing livestock or instill fear within the community, individuals look to the government for support. In Tarangire, the Maasai felt abandoned by the national government, and this perpetuated negative attitudes toward lions. Therefore, it is recommended that a nationwide database of human – carnivore conflicts is created and centralized within the government. Wildlife-based field staff should be trained to respond to carnivore conflicts in a socially appropriate manner that is sensitive to the concerns of the local community and that demonstrates a sincere effort to resolve the problems. In addition, this staff should provide community training and then collaborate with local, village scouts when dealing with problem animals in order to relieve some of the pressure on the government. This would go a long way in improving government and community relationships. Financial support for research programs focused on the resolution of these conflicts should also be sought. (2) In order to meet the needs of an increasing human population in Tanzania – a population that will undoubtedly come into greater conflicts with large carnivores and other wildlife species in the future – the government will require a large body of field staff trained in the human dimensions of wildlife conservation. In addition, local communities will also require this training as they become legally empowered to manage their own wildlife. Nevertheless, human dimensions training is often lacking in developing countries. While Tanzania has a number of field-based training institutes and universities, these institutions should be encouraged to emphasize the Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 151 social sciences and interdisciplinary problem-solving skills in wildlife-based curriculum. In addition, this study has demonstrated spoor counts as a reliable and cost-effective technique for monitoring large carnivores that can be easily adopted by national park rangers, wildlife division game scouts, and village game scouts. Therefore, it is recommended that this technique should also be integrated into field school curriculums. Internationally: (1) This study has demonstrated the worldwide significance of the lion as a species valued for its contribution to human society in terms of our creative, spiritual, and cultural development. While geographically confined in the twenty-first century to Africa and a small piece of India, the lion travels widely in our minds influencing human cultures globally in one way or another. In recognition of this, the lion should be honored and distinguished as the first World Heritage Species. Just as certain landscapes are deemed to have global worth, meriting international efforts to protect them, the wild lion should be similarly valued. This does not suggest protection in the traditional sense, forbidding use, but rather protection in a manner that recognizes the many important human – lion relationships and that celebrates their continuance well into the twenty-second century. (2) Complementing the previous distinction, we need to build international mechanisms that channel increased support, particularly in a financial sense, but also politically, from foreign and local nations that benefit economically and socially from the Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 152 presence of wild lions to those communities overwhelmingly burdened with the costs of living among them. Redistribution of the costs and benefits of maintaining wild lion populations will increase local support for the continued presence of these animals. The development of an international, endowed fund for lions will provide an essential source of finances to meet the costs of implementing the recommendations proposed herein. (3) In terms of sport hunting, research results indicated that while Tanzanian reform of the lion hunting industry is definitely needed, there also exists a responsibility among hunting companies, professional hunters, and their clients to accept and endorse hunting techniques that promote the long-term persistence of lions. In order to reach these individuals and entities on a global scale, international hunting organizations and media should vigorously promote a set of lion hunting standards. This may involve the development of a certification system for companies deemed by a reputable hunting body to be hunting lions in both an ethically and ecologically sound manner. (4) Finally, this study has bridged the gap between the ecological and social scientific disciplines, demonstrating the importance of linking human and ecological systems in the study of modern wildlife conservation issues. While the focus has been lions, this approach can be applied to many other systems, particularly since conflicts between people and wildlife are of global proportions. It is suggested that the proliferation of this type of study which emphasizes the need for a comprehensive understanding of Lichtenfeld – Chapter Five 153 both the ecological and social dynamics contributing to human – wildlife conflicts will be significant in transforming the traditional, disciplinary, ecologicallyoriented approach to resolving wildlife conflicts to one that is multidisciplinary, multidimensional, interactive, and respectful of the human context of conservation. And as a result, our ability to effectively resolve these conflicts and to better protect and preserve the environment and the species we live among will be immeasurably enhanced. The lion in human society represents the noble warrior, the King of all Beasts, a majestic and all-powerful predator that elicits both fear and admiration in us but, above all, gives meaning to our lives by his mere existence. Those who painted the lion onto the rocks of ancient history showed impressive foresight that this animal would continue to persist in our minds as an indelible figure. And in the end, if it must come, it will not be the last image of a lion walking across a landscape of human habitation that is incongruous. The lion does not recognize a boundary between human and ecological systems, only that human beings are a species to be wary of. The incongruity will lie with us who want to see the lion remain wild in all his magnificence yet fail to commit ourselves to accepting the conditions under which this must be so: that lions, indeed, must live among us. In this respect, the solutions to lion conservation lie as much in our understanding of how our human systems interrelate with the ecological world of lions as they do in our understanding of one another. What will ultimately happen to the society we have built, a society that has revered the lion since the dawn of man, if the lion disappears? To this end, it is not only the kingdom of the lion that is at risk, but our own as well. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 154 Bibliography Adams, J. S. and T. O. McShane. 1992. The myth of wild Africa: Conservation without illusion. University of California Press, Los Angeles, California. Alvarez. K. 1994. The Florida Panther recovery program: An organizational failure of the Endangered Species Act. Pages 205-226 in T. W. Clark, R. P. Reading, and A. L. Clarke, eds. Endangered species recovery: finding the lessons, improving the process. Island Press, Washington, D.C. Arhem, K. 1985. Pastoral man in the Garden of Eden: The Maasai of the Ngorongoro Conservation Area, Tanzania. Uppsala Research Reports in Cultural Anthropology. University of Uppsala, Uppsala. Babbie, E. 1990. Survey research methods. Wadsworth Publishing Company, Belmont, California. Bailey, T. N. 1993. The African leopard: a study of the ecology and behavior of a solitary felid. Columbia University Press, New York, New York. Baldus, R. D. 2004a. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man – eating lion killing 35 people. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. 41. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Baldus, R. D. 2004b. Tourist hunting and its role in development of wildlife management areas in Tanzania. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. ?. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Baldus, R. D. and A. Cauldwell. 2004. Lion hunting. Pages 16-22 in Baldus, R. D. 2004a. Lion conservation in Tanzania leads to serious human – lion conflicts with a case study of a man – eating lion killing 35 people. Tanzanian Wildlife Discussion Paper No. 41. GTZ, Wildlife Division, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Bath. A. J. 1987. Attitudes of various interest groups in Wyoming toward wolf reintroduction in Yellowstone National Park. MA thesis. University of Wyoming, Laramie, Wyoming. Bath, A. J. 1989. The public and wolf reintroduction in Yellowstone National Park. Society and Natural Resources. 2: 297-306. Bernstein, P. L. 1996. Against the gods: The remarkable story of risk. Wiley, New York, New York. Bertram, B. C. R. 1973. Lion population regulation. East African Wildlife Journal 11: 215-225. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 155 Cavallo, J. 1993. A study of leopard behavior and ecology in the Seronera Valley, Serengeti National Park. Pages 33-43 in Serengeti Wildlife Research Centre Scientific Report 1990-1992, Serengeti, Tanzania. Clark, T. W., D. J. Mattson, R. P. Reading, and B. J. Miller. 2001. Interdisciplinary problem solving in carnivore conservation: an introduction. Pages 223-240 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Clark, T. W., R. P. Reading, and A. L. Clarke, eds. 1994. Endangered species recovery: finding the lessons, improving the process. Island Press, Washington, D.C. Collet, D. 1987. Pastoralists and wildlife: Image and reality in Kenya Maasailand. Pages 129-148 in D. Anderson and R. Grove, eds. Conservation in Africa. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Conforti, V. A., and F. C. Cascelli de Azevedo. 2003. Local perceptions of jaguars (Panthera onca) and pumas (Puma concolor) in the Iguacu National Park area, south Brazil. Biological Conservation. 111: 215-221. Cronbach, L. J. 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrica 16: 297-334. Crooks, K. R. 2002. Relative sensitivities of mammalian carnivores to habitat fragmentation. Conservation Biology 16(2): 488-502. Diehl, C. 1985. Wildlife and the Maasai: the story of East African parks. Cultural Survival Quarterly 9(1): 37-40. Earth Trends. 2003. Data online at earthtrends.wri.org. Eaton, R. L. 1979. Evolution of sociality in the Felidae. Carnivore 2: Supplement 82-89. Funston, P. J., E. Herrmann, P. Babupi, A. Kruiper, H. Kruiper, H. Jaggers, K. Masule, and K. Kruiper. 2001. Spoor frequency estimates as a method of determining lion and other large mammal densities in the Kgalagadi Transfrontier Park. Unpublished draft provided by main author. Gese, E. M. 2001. Monitoring of terrestrial carnivore populations. Pages 372-396 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Gittleman, J. L., S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne. 2001. Why ‘carnivore conservation’? Pages 1-7 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 156 Gittleman, J.L., S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. 2001. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Hanby, J. P. and J. D. Bygott. 1987. Emigration of subadult lions. Animal Behavior 35: 161-169. Harrington, F. and P. Paquet, eds. 1982. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Hofer, H. and M. East. 1995. Population dynamics, population size, and the commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyenas. Pages 332-363 in A. R. E. Sinclair and P. Arcese, eds. Serengeti II: Dynamics, management, and conservation of an ecosystem. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Homewood, K. and W. Rodgers. 1991. Maasailand ecology: Pastoralist development and wildlife conservation in Ngorongoro, Tanzania. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Hook, R. and W. Robinson, 1982. Attitudes of Michigan citizens toward predators. Pages 382-394 in F. Harrington and P. Paquet, eds. Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishing, New Jersey. Jacobson, S. K. 1998. Training idiot savants: The lack of human dimensions in conservation biology. Conservation Biology. 12(2): 263-267. Jope, K. and B. Shelby. 1984. Hiker behavior and the outcome of interactions with grizzly bears. Leisure Sciences 6: 257-270. Kahurananga, J. and F. Silkiluwasha. 1997. The migration of zebra and wildebeest between Tarangire National Park and Simanjiro plains, northern Tanzania, in 1972 and recent trends. African Journal of Ecology 35: 179-185. Kellert, S. R. 1985a. Public perception of predators, particularly the wolf and the coyote. Biological Conservation. 31: 167-189. Kellert, S. R. 1985b. Social and perceptual factors in endangered species management. Journal of Wildlife Management 49(2): 528-536. Kellert, S. R. 1986. The public and the timber wolf in Minnesota. Transactions of the 51st North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference. Wildlife Management Institute, Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. 1991. Public views of wolf restoration in Michigan. Transactions of the 56th North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference. Wildlife Management Institute, Washington, D. C. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 157 Kellert, S. R. 1992. Public attitudes toward bears and their conservation. Pages 293 – 305 in J. J.Clear, C. Servheen, and L. J. Lyons, eds. Ninth International Bear Conference, Grenoble, France. Kellert, S. R. 1996. The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Island Press, Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. and T. W. Clark. 1991. The theory and application of a wildlife policy framework. Pages 17-36 in W. R. Mangun, ed. Public policy issues in wildlife management. Greenwood Press, New York, New York. Kellert, S. R., M. Black, C. R. Rush, and A. J Bath. 1996. Human culture and large carnivore conservation in North America. Conservation Biology 10(4): 977-990. Kellert, S. R., J. N. Mehta, S. A. Ebbin, and L. Lichtenfeld. 2000. Community natural resource management: Promise, rhetoric, and reality. Society and Natural Resources 13: 705-715. Lama, L. 1998. Conflict and compatability: An inventory and analysis of land use in a Tanzanian wildlife corridor. Ph. D. Dissertation, Binghamton University, State University of New York, New York. LEAT (Lawyer’s Environmental Action Team). 1998. Socio-legal analysis of community-based conservation in Tanzania: Policy, legal, institutional and programmatic issues, considerations, and options. Report prepared for EPIQ, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Leopold, A. 1949. A Sand County almanac. Oxford University Press, New York, New York. Lopez, B. 1978. Of wolves and men. Charles Scribner’s and Sons, New York, New York. Mangun, W. R. ed. 1991. Public policy issues in wildlife management. Greenwood Press, New York, New York. Marean, C. W. 1989. Sabertooth cats and their relevance for early hominid diet evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 18: 559-582. Marks, S. A. 1984. The imperial lion: Human dimensions of wildlife management in Central Africa. Westview Press, Boulder, Colorado. Martin, R. B. and T. de Meulenaer. 1988. Survey of the status of the leopard Panthera pardus in sub-Saharan Africa. CITES, Gland, Switzerland. Matthiessen, P. 1987. Wildlife in America. Viking Press, New York, New York. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 158 Mattson, D. J., S. Herrero, R. G. Wright, and C. M. Pease. 1996. Science and management of Rocky Mountain Grizzly Bears. Conservation Biology 10(4): 10131025. Mech, L. D. 1981. The wolf: the ecology and behavior of an endangered species. Doubleday Press, New York, New York. Mehta, J. N. and S. R. Kellert. 1998. Local attitudes toward community-based conservation policy and programmes in Nepal: a case study in the Makalu-Barun Conservation Area. Environmental Conservation 25:320-333. MNRT (Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism). 2000. The wildlife conservation (tourist hunting) regulations. Government Printer, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. MNRT (Ministry of Natural Resources and Tourism). 1998. The wildlife policy of Tanzania. MNRT, Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania. Muir, A. 1994. A situational analysis of pastoralism in Simanjiro district, Tanzania. VETAID, Arusha, Tanzania. Nelson, F. 2004. The evolution and impacts of community-based tourism in northern Tanzania. Drylands Issues Paper Series No. 131., International Institute for Environment and Development, London, England. Nicholson, B. 2001. The last of old Africa: Big-game hunting in East Africa. Safari Press Inc., Long Beach, California. Nowell and P. Jackson, eds. 1996. Wild cats: Status survey and conservation action plan. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. Nshala, R. 2002. Village rights relating to land management, tourism, and tourist hunting. Lawyer’s Environmental Action Team. Available online at www.leat.or.tz/publications. O’Brien, S. J. 1996. Molecular genetics and phylogenetics of the Felidae. Pages xxiiixxiv in K. Nowell and P. Jackson, eds. Wild cats: Status survey and conservation action plan. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. Oli, M. K., I. R. Taylor, and M. E. Rogers. 1994. Snow leopard (Panthera uncia) predation on livestock: An assessment of local perceptions in the Annapurna Conservation Area, Nepal. Biological Conservation 68: 63-68. Orford, H. J. L., M. R. Perrin, and H. H. Berry. 1988. Contraception, reproduction, and demography of free-ranging Etosha lions (Panthera leo). Journal of Zoology 216: 717-733. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 159 Packer, C. 1986. The ecology of sociality in felids. Pages 429-451 in D. I. Rubenstein and R. W. Wrangham, eds. Ecological aspects of social evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Pennycuick, C. J. and J. Rudnai. 1970. A method of identifying individual lions, Panthera leo, with the analysis of the reliability of identification. Journal of Zoology 160: 497-508. Pratt, D. J., P. J. Greenway, and M. D. Gwynne. 1966. A classification of East African rangeland. Journal of Applied Ecology 3: 369-382. Lamprey, H. F. 1963. Ecological separation of the large species in the Tarangire game reserve. Tanganyika. East African Wildlife Journal 1: 67-92. Quammen, D. 2003. Monster of God: The man-eating predator in the jungles of history and the mind. W.W. Norton and Co., New York, New York. Reading, R. and S. R. Kellert. 1993 Attitudes toward a proposed reintroduction of blackfooted ferrets. Conservation Biology 7: 349-365. Ridley, M. 1993. Evolution. Blackwell Science, Inc., Cambridge, Massachusetts. Riley, S. J. and D. J. Decker. 2000. Risk perception as a factor in wildlife stakeholder acceptance capacity for cougars in Montana. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 5: 5062. Rockwell, D. 1991. Giving voice to the bear: North American Indian myths, rituals, and images of the bear. Robers Rinehart Publishers, Niwot, Colorado. Roe, D., N. Leader-Williams, and B. Dalal-Clayton. 1997. Take only photographs, leave only footprints: the environmental impacts of wildlife tourism. Wildlife Development Series No. 10. International Institute for Environment and Development, London, England. Rudnai, J. 1979. Ecology of lions in Nairobi National Park and the adjoining Kitengela Conservation Unit in Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 17: 85-95. Saberwal, V. K., J. P. Gibbs, R. Chellam, and A. J. T. Johnsingh. 1994. Lion-human conflict in the Gir Forest, India. Conservation Biology 8(2): 501-507. Saberwal, V. K. and A. Kothari. 1996. The human dimension in Conservation Biology curricula in developing countries. Conservation Biology 10(5): 1328-1331. Schaller, G. B. 1972. The Serengeti lion: a study of predator-prey relations. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 160 Scheel, D. and C. Packer. 1991. Group hunting behavior of lions: a search for cooperation. Animal Behavior 41: 697-709. Shepard, P. and B. Sanders. 1985. The sacred paw: The bear in nature, myth, and literature. Viking Penguin Inc, Toronto, Canada. Slovic, P. 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236: 280-285. Smallwood, K. S. and E. L. Fitzhugh. 1995. A track count for estimating mountain lions Felis concolor california population trends. Biological Conservation 71: 251-259. Smuts, G. L., I. J. Whyte, and T. W. Dearlove. 1977. A mass capture technique for lions. East African Wildlife Journal 15: 81-87. Smuts, G. L., J. L. Anderson, and J. C. Austin. 1978. Age determination of the African lion (Panthera leo). Journal of the Zoological Society of London. 185: 115-146. Smuts, G. L., J. Hanks, and I. J. Whyte. 1980. Comparative growth of wild male and female lions (Panthera leo). Journal of Ecology 190: 365-373. Stander, P. E. 1990. A suggested management strategy for stock-raiding lions in Namibia. South African Journal of Wildlife Research. 20: 37-43. Stander, P. E. 1991. Demography of lions in Etosha National Park, Namibia. Madoqua 18: 1-9. Stander, P. E. 1998. Spoor counts as indices of large carnivore populations: the relationship between spoor frequency, sampling effort, and true density. Journal of Applied Ecology 35; 378-385. Stander, P. E., //. Ghau, D. Tsisaba, //. =oma, and l.l ui. 1997. Tracking and the interpretation of spoor: a scientifically sound method in ecology. Journal of the Zoological Society of London 242: 329-341. Tanzania Population and Housing Census. 2002. Data online at www.tanzania.go.tz/census. Tarangire – Manyara Conservation Project. 2002. Final Report. Istituto Oikos and University of Insubria, Italy in Collaboration with TANAPA. Arusha, Tanzania. Tessler, M. and I. Warriner. 1997. Gender, feminism, and attitudes toward international conflict: exploring relationships with survey data from the Middle East. World Politics 49: 250-281. The Guardian (Dar es Salaam). July 23rd, 1997. Lichtenfeld – Bibliography 161 Treves, A. and L. Naughton-Treves. 1999. Risk and opportunity for humans coexisting with large carnivores. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 275-282. Tucker, P. and D. Pletscher. 1989. Attitudes of hunters and residents toward wolves in northwestern Montana. Wildlife Society Bulletin 17: 509-514. Van Dyke, F. G., R. H. Brooke, and H. G. Shaw. 1986. Use of road track counts as indices of mountain lion presence. Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 102-109. Viljoen, P. C. 1993. The effects of changes in prey availability on lion predation in a large natural ecosystem in northern Botswana. Pages 193-213 in N. Dunstone and M. L. Gorman, eds. Mammals as predators. Proceedings of the Symposium of the Zoological Society of London, No. 65. Clarendon, Oxford. Weaver, J. L., P. C. Paquet, and L. F. Ruggiero. 1996. Resilience and conservation of large carnivores in the Rocky Mountains. Conservation Biology 10(4): 964-976. Weber, W. and A. Rabinowitz. 1996. A global perspective on large carnivore conservation. Conservation Biology 10(4): 1046-1054. Whitman, K., A. M. Starfield, H. S. Quadling, and C. Packer. 2004. Sustainable trophy hunting of African lions. Nature 428: 175-178. Woodroffe, R. 2001. Strategies for carnivore conservation: lessons from contemporary extinctions. Pages 61-92 in J. L. Gittleman, S. M. Funk, D. W. Macdonald, and R. K. Wayne, eds. Carnivore Conservation. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. Woodroffe, R. and J. R. Ginsberg. 1998. Edge effects and the extinction of population inside protected areas. Science 280: 2126-2128. Young, S. P. 1946. The wolf in American history. Caxton Printers, Cladwell, Idaho.