1 A TEST OF THE POLLINATION SYNDROME CONCEPT USING
Transcription
1 A TEST OF THE POLLINATION SYNDROME CONCEPT USING
1 A TEST OF THE POLLINATION SYNDROME CONCEPT USING THE JAMAICAN BLUE MAHOE, HIBISCUS ELATUS By Heidi M. Lovig A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of Humboldt State University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in Biology Committee Membership Dr. Michael R. Mesler, Committee Chair Dr. Mathew D. Johnson, Committee Member Dr. Erik S. Jules, Committee Member Dr. Joseph M. Szewczak, Committee Member Dr. Michael R. Mesler, Graduate Coordinator May 2013 ABSTRACT A TEST OF THE POLLINATION SYNDROME CONCEPT USING THE JAMAICAN BLUE MAHOE, HIBISCUS ELATUS Heidi M. Lovig In this study I tested the ability of the pollination syndrome model to predict the most effective pollinator of a tropical forest tree, Hibiscus elatus (Malvaceae), at three sites in Jamaica, West Indies. The floral characteristics of this species suggested pollination by both bees and bats. Bat syndrome traits include large bowl-shaped flowers borne on stout pedicles that open in the late afternoon or early evening, thick and waxy petals, and copious amounts of dilute nectar secreted at night. Bee-related floral traits include brightly colored petals with nectar guides, diurnal nectar secretion, and a sweet fragrance. This combination of floral syndromes did not accurately predict the most common and effective pollinators of these flowers: bats accounted for almost all pollination while day-flying visitors (honeybees, hummingbirds, flies and wasps) and other night-flying-visitors (moths, flies, and wasps) contributed minimally. This conclusion rests on the much higher visit rates of bats, their much higher likelihood of contacting anthers and stigmas during visits, and the fact that > 99% of pollination occurred at night. The bee syndrome traits of H. elatus are probably vestiges inherited from a bee-pollinated ancestor that have been retained due to lack of fitness trade-offs. H. elatus is likely in a transitional state evolving towards increased specialization for pollination by bats. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to extend my appreciation to my advisor, Dr. Michael Mesler, for all his guidance and enthusiasm throughout this project. Thank you to my committee members, Dr. Mathew Johnson for his assistance in obtaining research permits and logistical support in Jamaica, Dr. Joseph Szewczak for the use of his camera equipment and Dr. Erik Jules for his writing critiques. My grateful thanks to Ted Weller for his valuable instruction on how to mist net for bats. A special thanks to Rose Dana, and Jay, Jayne and Heather Lovig for all their assistance and support as field technicians in Jamaica; without their help this project would have not been possible. And thank you to Tim Carpenter for his support and encouragement throughout this study. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT ……………………………………………………………………ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ……………………………………………………iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ……………………………………………………....iv INTRODUCTION ……………………………………………………………..1 METHODS …………………………………………………………………….4 RESULTS ……………………………………………………………………...10 DISCUSSION ……………………………………………………………….....13 LITERATURE CITED ………………………………………………………...18 TABLES .............................................................................................................25 LIST OF FIGURES …………………………………………………………....30 FIGURES ............................................................................................................31 iv 1 INTRODUCTION The pollination syndrome model is arguably the most familiar and influential idea in the field of pollination biology (Fenster et al. 2004, Ollerton et al. 2009). Although still contentious, there is compelling evidence for the existence of floral trait correlations (syndromes) that transcend taxonomic affinity and predict the most effective pollinators of a given plant species (Stebbins 1970, Armbruster 1988, 2011, Buzato 1994, Fenster et al. 2004, Paw 2009). According to the model, trait correlations emerge as unrelated plants specialize on functionally equivalent pollinators that impose similar selective pressures based on their size, morphology, perceptual ability, and foraging behavior (Stebbins 1970, Faegri & van der Pijl 1979). For example, bat pollinated flowers are large and sturdy, bloom at night, produce musty fragrances and copious dilute nectar — features that make adaptive sense in the context of bat biology (Faegri & van der Pijl 1979, Fleming et al. 2009). Although syndrome traits often successfully predict the most effective pollinator of a given plant (e.g. Armbruster 1988, 2011, Pauw 2009, Arditti et al. 2012, Kishore et al. 2012), cases where specialized flowers are visited by more than one functional group are common (Herrera 1996, Waser et al. 1996, Fleming 2001, Fenster et al. 2004, Grombone-Guaratini et al. 2004, Waser and Ollerton 2006). Such unpredicted generalization does not necessarily weaken the floral syndrome paradigm (Fenster et al. 2004, Ollerton et al. 2009) and often reflects the broad perceptual range and opportunistic foraging of pollinators. For example, in southern Arizona bees avidly visit Agave palmeri, a classically bat-adapted flower, to obtain "left over" nectar that was secreted the 2 night before (Howell & Roth 1981, Slauson 2000). Even though the flowers of this species have no apparent adaptations for bee pollination, bees are nevertheless one of the major pollinators (Slauson 2000). Less common are cases where generalization is actually predicted by floral traits because flowers are adapted to more than one group of pollinator (e.g., Sazima et al. 1994, Buzato et al. 1994, Etcheverry and Alemán 2005, Liu et al. 2006, MarténRodríguez et al. 2009). For example Abutilon rufinerve (Malvaceae) has floral adaptations for bats and hummingbirds, and it is effectively pollinated by both taxa (Buzato et al. 1994). Such dual pollination systems may represent an intermediate stage in the transition from one specialized state to another in response to changes in the composition of pollinator communities (Gottsberger 1986, Buzato et al. 1994). Alternatively, they may represent an adaptive equilibrium where both functional groups exert selective pressure on floral traits (Muchhala 2008). Since switching between pollinator types has been a major feature of angiosperm adaptive radiation (van der Neit & Johnson 2012), studying species with mixed pollination syndromes promises to increase our understanding of the kinds of ecological conditions that favor specialization versus generalization in plant-pollination systems. My study tested the ability of the floral phenotype to predict the most effective pollinator(s) of Hibiscus elatus, a tropical tree whose flowers appear to have traits associated with both bee and bat pollination syndromes (Faegri & van der Pijl 1979, Fryxell 2001, Helversen 2003, Fleming et al. 2009, Lovig unpublished data). Little is 3 known about the pollination biology of this plant, though it has been cited as a bat pollinated species by Fleming et al. (2009). Preliminary observations in Jamaica revealed that its flowers are visited by bats at night, but also by bees and hummingbirds during the day. I asked the following questions: (1) Do the flowers of H. elatus have traits associated with more than one of the classical pollination syndromes? (2) Do the floral traits successfully predict the most effective pollinators of this species? (3) What percentage of pollen deposition occurs at night vs. during the day? 4 METHODS Study organism and study sites— H. elatus is a tree endemic to Jamaica and Cuba (Fryxell 2001). This species is widely distributed in Jamaica from sea level to 1200 m, but does not occur in the immediate costal zone (Adams 1971, 1972, Fryxell 2001). Trees reach 25 m in height, and remain in flower for approximately 3 to 4 months, producing 1 to 40 large bowl-shaped flowers per day (Fryxell 2001, Lovig, unpublished data; Figure 2 A). Flowers are borne on stout pedicles at the tips of branches, and are most abundant in the top 2/3 of the canopy. Traits that suggest pollination by bees include brightly colored petals (yellows and oranges) with prominent red nectar guides at the base, and a sweet fragrance; bat syndrome traits include a stout pedicle, tick waxy petals, large bowl shaped corollas, a large corolla aperture (29 mm, on average), and the fact that stigmas extended well beyond the base of the corolla (~98 mm) (Faegri & van der Pijl 1979, Fryxell 2001, Helversen 2003, Fleming et al. 2009, Lovig unpublished data). Accessible flowers positioned on the tips of branches and the copious amounts of pollen correspond to both syndromes, while the red flowers produced by some trees relates to neither syndrome (Faegri & van der Pijl 1979, Fleming et al. 2009). Buds open in the late afternoon into the night and flowers senesce completely after 48 hours, but are no longer visited after about 24 hours (Lovig, unpublished data). Flowers are bisexual. Anthers are dehisced and stigmas receptive at anthesis. Filaments are fused into a tube that extends from the base of the corolla aperture and rests on the flower's lower petal. The filament tube curves slightly upward and the stigma extends from its tip. Stigmas are receptive both night and 5 day (hydrogen peroxide test: Lovig, unpublished data). Each capsule produces a maximum of about 100 seeds (Lovig, unpublished data). Fieldwork was carried out from December 2010 - January 2011 and from December 2011 - January 2012 at three sites in Jamaica, West Indies. Sites were chosen to represent a range of conditions with the hope of capturing potential variation in pollinator assemblages: (1) Kew Park, a coffee plantation surrounded by patches of forest, Westmoreland Parish (333 m: 18°15' N, 77°56' W), (2) Bunker's Hill, a wet limestone forest, Trelawney Parish, (198 m: 18°22' N, 77°42' W), and (3) Holywell, a recreational reserve surrounded by dense closed-canopy montane forest, in the Blue and John Crow Mountains National Park, St. James Parish, (1067 m: 18°5' N, 76°43' W). Studies were conducted during peak flowering periods on 21 trees (5 at Kew Park, 8 at Bunker's Hill, and 8 at Holywell). All trees at each site producing at least 2 flowers per day within reach of a 15-foot ladder were used. However the majority of trees did not satisfy these conditions, which limited sample sizes. Pollen deposition, day and night— Pollen loads on stigmas were counted to determine how much pollen was deposited during the day versus at night. One or more pairs of mature buds were selected on survey trees each morning. When more than one pair of flowers was selected on a single tree, the pairs were spaced as far apart as possible to minimize non-independence. One bud from each pair was left un-bagged so that flowers were exposed to nocturnal visitation. The second bud was either covered with a thin 6 nylon mesh bag or taped shut (tape was used on windy nights to prevent bag-induced autogamy). The bag or tape was removed at sunrise so that flowers were open to visits during the day. Stigmas were collected at sunrise (night pollination) or within one hour of sunset (day pollination), and the number of pollen grains was counted with a dissection microscope (Figure 2 B). A total of 75 pairs of stigmas was examined across the three sites. Visitor surveys—A combination of visual surveys (day) and video recordings (day and night) was used to study visitor behavior. Videos were recorded using 1 of 4 cameras: (1) ARCHOS mobile video recorder AV 500 series infrared-security camera, (2) Sony HD handheld video recorder, (3) Sony digital HD infrared-video camera recorder HDR-SR11 #1, and (4) Sony digital HD infrared-video camera recorder HDR-SR11 #2. External infrared lights powered by 12volt motorcycle batteries were used to illuminate flowers during nocturnal video recordings. Cameras and infrared lights were mounted on either a modified 12-foot extendable painters-pole, or a 15-foot bamboo ladder. I filmed patches of 1-4 flowers during the day from 0615-1500 and at night from 1815- 0530. Across all sites, I filmed a total of 43 flowers on 9 trees at night for 2.1 hours on average (range 0.1-8.0 h), and 29 flowers on 8 trees during the day for 1.0 hours on average (range 0.2-3.6 h). A total of 91 hours of nocturnal video and 29.2 hours of diurnal video were taken. Diurnal visual surveys were conducted from 0615-1800 on 251 flowers on 18 trees for a total of 61 hours. I watched 1-4 flowers per 10-min visual 7 survey. Videos and visual surveys were analyzed (videos with VLC media player) to identify visitors, the probability of stigma and/or pollen contact during a visit, and to determine visitation rates (visits flower-1 hour-1). Visits by nectar robbers were not included in the above analysis. All visitors could be identified to functional group: bats, hummingbirds, bees, wasps, flies (gnats and other small-bodied flies), hawkmoths and other moths. All bee visits were by Apis mellifera. I mist-netted to capture bats and identify them. One to three mist nets (70/2 2.6 meters tall, 4 shelves, black nylon, 6-8 meters wide) were placed 1/2-1 meter above the ground near the study trees for 2-4 hours between 1900 and 0130 on sampling nights. I obtained pollen samples from captured bats by applying and removing a piece of 3M Scotch transparent tape to the crown of the head, the chest, and the belly (Nathan Muchhala 2007). Tape was mounted on slides and later examined for H. elatus pollen with a compound microscope. Hummingbirds and bats were identified using standard references (Raffaele et al. 2003, Genoways et al. 2005). Flies, wasps, hawkmoths, and other moths were not identified to species. Relative pollinator importance— I calculated importance values to estimate the relative contributions of different visitors to pollen deposition using the equation: PI = V ⋅ Fpollen ⋅ Fstigma 8 where V is the rate of visitation (visits/flower/hour), and Fpollen and Fstigma are the fraction of visits where pollen or stigmas were contacted, respectively (Armbruster 1988, 2011, Martén-Rodríguez et al. 2009). The product of Fpollen ⋅ Fstigma was calculated in lieu of determining the number of grains deposited per visit. The relative importance of each group (WPI) was calculated by adding all PI values together then dividing each group's value by the total. Nectar secretion and flower morphology— Pattern of nectar secretion was estimated for 30 flowers by removing all the nectar three times at approximately 12-hour intervals; sampling periods correspond to nectar produced during the first night (measured at 04:5509:55), the next day (measured at 15:45-20:05), and the second night (measured at 05:5011:15). Flowers were covered 1-3 hours before anthesis with cages and were only uncovered only during sampling occasions to prevent visitors from removing nectar. Logistic considerations made it impossible to sample all flowers at the same time, so night-time samples may include a small amount of day-time secretion. Nectar volume was measured with a 1-mL blunt-tipped graduated syringe by removing all fluid from the base of the nectary; all remaining liquid was removed and measured with 10-μL or 20-μL microcapillary tubes. Nectar sugar concentration was measured with a Bellingham and Stanley pocket refractometer for each sample. 9 Statistical analysis— One-way ANOVAs were used to test for differences in the amount of pollen deposited among sites at night or during the day. I used a paired t-test to determine if there was a difference in the number of pollen deposited on stigmas during the day and at night, pooling all pairs of flowers across sites. Because I sometimes sampled more than one pair of stigmas from the same tree, there is the potential of nonindependence. I used two sample t-tests to determine if there were differences in the amount of nectar secreted over the first night and during the day at both sites, and if there was a difference in the amount of nectar secreted between sites. Visitation rates could not be analyzed statistically because the video and visual survey data were collected in different ways. 10 RESULTS Pollen deposition during the day and night— Although the number of grains deposited at night varied among sites (p < 0.001), in all cases night-time deposition far exceeded day-time deposition (p = 0.02, Table 1). Stigmas exposed during the night received an average of almost 600 grains, which was more than 40 times the number received by stigmas exposed during the day (~14 grains). Moreover, all night-time stigmas received at least some pollen, while a large fraction (20% - 27% depending on site) of day-time stigmas received no pollen (Table 1). With few exceptions, pollen on stigmas consisted entirely of conspecific pollen. Visitor surveys—Flowers were visited both night and day. Nocturnal visitors included bats and moths (sphingids and small-bodied moths). Bats could not be reliably identified from video recordings, but two phyllosdotmids, Monophyllus redmani and Glossophaga soricina, captured in mist nets carried heavy loads of H. elatus pollen (Figure 1, Table 2). Two fruit bats, Artibeus jamaicensis and Ariteus flavescens were also captured, but carried no pollen. Diurnal visitors included Apis mellifera and three species of humming birds: Trochilus polytmus, Mellisuga minima, and Anthracothorax mango. Flies and wasps visited flowers both night and day. There was variation in visit rates between sites, but overall, bats were the most common visitors (0.35 - 0.15 visits flower-1 hour-1, depending on site, Table 3). All other day and night visitors were much less frequent, with few exceptions: hawkmoths visited 11 at 0.14 visits flower-1 hour-1 at Kew Park, and Apis mellifera visited at 0.10 visits flower-1 hour-1 at Holywell (Table 3). Visitors interacted with flowers (Figure 2) differently based on their respective morphologies and feeding behaviors, which affected their likelihood of contacting pollen and stigma. Bats either hovered or alighted on flowers directly on top of anthers and stigmas during visits. This behavior accounted for the bat's high rate of contact with pollen (85.8 %), and stigmas (68.1 %) when they entered flowers (Table 4). All other visitors were much less likely to contact pollen and stigmas (Table 4). In general, Apis mellifera either landed on anthers to collect pollen occasionally crawling over stigmas prior to departing, or landed on corollas, and crawled into flowers to obtain nectar. The foraging behavior of both hawkmoths and moths prevented them from contacting reproductive parts. Hawkmoths hovered far above sexual structures while feeding, and small moths landed on corollas and crawled into flowers to obtain nectar. Hummingbirds robbed nectar by inserting their bill between the calyx and corolla, or perching on the corolla to feed, avoiding reproductive parts. Dipterans did contact pollen and stigmas, but likely did not transfer much pollen due to their relatively small size. Relative pollinator importance— Based on relatively high visit rates and contact probabilities, bats likely accounted for more than 99% of total pollen deposition on stigmas (Table 5). This value matched the pattern of pollination: depending on site, ~96% - 98% of pollen deposition occurred during the night (Table 4). 12 Nectar secretion— There was a significant difference in the amount of nectar secreted over the first night and during the day at both sites; Bunker's Hill (P < 0.001), Kew Park, (P < 0.001). Also, there was significant variation in the amount of nectar produced between sites during the first night (P = 0.039) and during the day (P = 0.012). Nectar production was greatest during the first night (1.84 ml, Figure 3). Much less nectar was produced during the following day (0.29 ml) and second night (0.08 m, Figure 3). Nectar sugar concentration decreased from 13.7 % after the first night, to 9.4 % after the first day, and 2.8% after the second night (Figure 3). 13 DISCUSSION The flowers of H. elatus present an apparent mix of bat and bee pollination syndrome traits. Many of the traits suggest pollination by bats; the large flowers are borne on stout pedicles and open in the late afternoon or early evening, the petals are thick and waxy, and copious amounts dilute nectar are secreted at night. However, the brightly colored petals with nectar guides, and a sweet fragrance, suggest pollination by bees. The fact that flowers remain open and stigmas are receptive both at night and during the day suggest potential pollination by both groups. Based on this mix of traits, the floral syndrome model would predict pollination by bats at night and bees during the day. However, this was not the case. Pollinator importance values and pollen deposition on stigmas show H. elatus has a specialized pollination system that relies almost entirely on glossophagine bats. Since bees provided virtually no fitness contribution to H. elatus, its bee-related traits are likely vestiges inherited from a bee-pollinated ancestor. Ecological conditions in Jamaica may have favored a shift from bee to bat pollination and the evolution of floral adaptations for bats. The relative importance of a given pollinator depends on both the number of visits and the quality of visits (Muchhala 2009). Bats visited flowers often, about once every 1-2 hours, while all other visitors were much less frequent. The quality of bat visits was also much higher. Bats routinley contacted anthers and stigmas during visits. In 14 contrast, even though honeybees, hawkmoths and other moths contacted anthers frequently at some sites, their probability of contacting stigmas was very low. These two components, visit rate and visit quality, combined to indicate that bats were the near exclusive pollinators of H. elatus (relative PI value > .99) in this study. The fact that almost all pollen deposition (~97 %) occurred at night provides confirmation that bats were the primary pollinators. The effectiveness of bats as pollinators can be attributed to their close morphological fit with the flowers. The width of the corolla aperture (26 mm on average) was the primary feature influencing a visitor's probability of contacting anthers and/or stigmas during a visit. The close match between the body size of the bats and the width of the corolla aperture, assured that bats were consistently guided into flowers immediately above exerted staminal column, which maximized the probability of contact with anthers and stigmas. In contrast, the fit between body size and flower width for smaller visitors like bees, flies, and wasps was poor. These visitors were able to maneuver around reproductive structures when entering flowers and seldom contacted anthers and stigmas as they foraged for nectar. Even when pollen-foraging honeybees landed on anthers, they did so well below the position of the stigmas. Although hummingbirds were frequent visitors, they very rarley entered flowers from the front: instead, they typically robbed nectar from between the calyx and corolla, which precluded them from contact with anthers and stigmas. 15 Since native bees do not pollinate H. elatus, and thus are not contemporary agents of natural selection, its apparent bee-related floral tratis require an explanation. The most likely scenario is that these traits were inherited from a bee-pollinated ancestor. Consistent with this view, the closest relative of H. elatus is a widespread coastal shrub, H. tiliaceus (Takayama et al. 2005, 2006b). The pollination bioogy of this species is poorly known, but its flowers are visited by large-bodied carpenter bees (Xylocopa) in the Galapagos Islands and probably elsewhere (McMullen 1989). Several of the floral traits of H. tiliaceus match the apparent bee-traits of H. elatus: brightly colored petals (yellow, orange, and red) with conspicious red nectar guides, diurnal nectar secretion, and a pleasant fragrance (Fryxell 2001, Abe 2006, Elevitch 2006). Thus, I regard the beerelated traits of H. elatus as vestiges of a bee-pollinated ancestor like H. tiliaceus. The bat adaptations of H. elatus flowers appear to have been added to the bee syndrome plan. There is no reason to believe that the pre-existing bee adaptation would have diminished the attractiveness of H. elatus flowers to bats. For example, although Neotropical bat flowers typically produce fetid or musty sulfur-containing fragrance compounds, some bats (including Glossophaga soricina) use spatial memory instead of olfaction to locate blossoms (Voss 1980, von Helversen 1993, 2003, Carter et al. 2010). Likewise, although bat flowers are usually drab, the flowers of many chiropterophilous species are brightly colored (Baker 1961, Johnson & Steiner 2000, Fleming et al. 2009). In the absence of strong adaptive trade offs, the traits adapted for bees have been maintained. 16 Ecological conditions in Jamaica likely favored a shift from bee to bat pollination. Compared to many other oceanic islands, the bee fauna of Jamaica is relatively depauperate (Adams 1972, Raw 1985). Notably, bee species diversity and abundance drop with an increase in elevation. Currently, there are only six large bee species known to pollinate trees on the island, and only three of these are found above 300 meters in places where H. elatus occurs (Raw 1985). As the immediate ancestors of H. elatus adapted to higher elevation inland sites, they likely encountered a bee fauna that was less reliable than it had been on the coast. In contrast bats were probably widespread and relativley abundant inland, as they are today (Genoways 2005). Under these condition, mutations that favored pollination by bats would have been favored (Stebbins 1970, Waser et al. 1996). Bat pollination has evolved in several different angiosperm lineages from ancestors that were pollinated either by bees, birds, or non-flying mammals (Fleming et al. 2009). To date, however, relatively few studies have examined the shift to bat pollination in an explicitly phylogenetic context (Tripp & Manos 2008, van der Neit & Johnson 2012), which hinders our understanding of the origin of bat-related floral adaptations. Assuming that H. tiliaceus is pollinated by bees, my study adds another example of the kinds of trait changes associated with an evolutionary shift from bee to bat pollination. Some of the important modifications that likely evolved to facilitate bat pollination in H. elauts included thicker and waxier petals, thicker pedicel to support visitation by large vertebrates (Glossophaga soricina clings to flowers during visits ( 17 Sazima et al. 1999)), an increase in dilute nectar production at night and an increase in overall flower size: petal length 2-3 times longer in H. elauts (7-12 cm) vs. H. tiliaceus (4-6 cm), and the staminal column extends much further from the corolla in H. elatus (79 cm) vs. H. tiliaceus (2.5-3 cm) (Fryxell 2001). In conclusion, the floral traits of H. elatus did not perfectly predict its pollinators. Although the floral phenotype suggests generalization on bats and bees, pollination was overwhelmingly by bats. I propose that the mix of syndrome traits represents a transitional state in which H. elatus is evolving towards increased floral specialization for pollination by bats. Although maintenance of bee adaptations was likely essential for the initial transition from bee to bat pollination, these adaptations are now vestigial, and we can expect them to decay over time. 18 LITERATURE CITED Abe, T., 2006. Threatened pollination systems in native flora of the Ogasawara (Bonin) Islands. Annals of Botany 98: 317–334. Adams, C.D., 1972. Flowering plants of Jamaica. Robert MacLehose and Company Limited The University Press, Glasgow. Adams, C.D., 1971. The blue mahoe and other bush, an introduction to plant life in Jamaica (1st edition). McGraw-Hill Far Eastern Publishers (S) Ltd. Arditti, J., Elliott, J., Kitching, I.J., and Wasserthal, L.T., 2012. “Good heavens what insect can suck it”– Charles Darwin, Angraecum sesquipedale and Xanthopan morganii praedicta. Botanical Journal of the Linnean Society 169: 403–432. Armbruster, W.S., 1988. Multilevel comparative analysis of the morphology, function, and evolution of Dalechampia blossoms. Ecology 69: 1746–1761. Armbruster, W.S., Gong, Y. and Huang, S., 2011. Are pollination “syndromes” predictive? Asian Dalechampia fit Neotropical models. The American Naturalist 178: 135–143. Baker, H. G., 1961. The adaptation of flowering plants to nocturnal and crepuscular pollinators. The Quarterly Review of Biology 36: 64–73. Buskirk, R., 1985. Zoogeographic patterns and tectonic history of Jamaica and the Northern Caribbean. Journal of Biogeography 12: 445–461. 19 Buzato, S., Sazima, M., and Sazima, I., 1994. Pollination of three species of Abutilon (Malvaceae) intermediate between bat and hummingbird flower syndromes. Flora 189: 327–334. Carter, G.G., Ratcliffe, J.M. and Galef, B.G., 2010. Flower bats (Glossophaga soricina) and fruit bats (Carollia perspicillata) rely on spatial cues over shapes and scents when relocating food. PLoS ONE 5: e10808 Elevitch, C.R., and Thomson L.A.J., 2006. Hibiscus tiliaceus (beach hibiscus) (ver. 1.2.) In C.R. Elevitch (Ed.). Species profiles for Pacific Island agroforestry. Permanent Agriculture Resources (PAR), Hōlualoa, Hawaii. Etcheverry, A.V. and Alemán, C.T., 2005. Reproductive biology of Erythrina falcata (Fabaceae: Papilionoideae). Biotropica 37: 54–63. Faegri, K. and van der Pijl, L., 1979. The Principles of Pollination Ecology (3rd edition). The University of Michigan. Fenster, C.B., Armbruster, S., Wilson, P. Dudash, and M. Thomson, J., 2004. Pollination syndromes and floral specialization. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 35: 375–403. Fleming T.H., 2001. Sonoran Desert columnar cacti and the evolution of generalized pollination systems. Ecological Monographs 71: 511–530. Fleming T.H., Geiselman, C. and Kress, W.J., 2009. The evolution of bat pollination: a phylogenetic perspective. Annals of Botany 104: 1017–1043. 20 Fryxell, P.A., 2001. Talipariti (Malvaceae), a segregate from Hibiscus. Contributions from the University of Michigan Herbarium. Genaro, J., 2008. Origins, composition and distribution of the bees of Cuba (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila). Insecta Mundi 52: 1–16. Genaro, J., and Franz, N., 2008. Bees of Greater Puerto Rico (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila). Insecta Mundi 40: 1–24. Genoways, H.H., Baker, R.J., Bickham, J.W., and Phillips, C.J., 2005. Bats of Jamaica. Special Publications, Museum of Texas Tech University. Gottsberger, G., 1986. Some pollination strategies in Neotropical savannas and forests. Plant Systematics and Evolution 152: 29–45. Grombone-Guaratini, M.T., Solferini, V.N. and Semir, J., 2004. Reproductive biology in a species of Bidens L. (Asteraceae). Scientia Agricola 61: 185–189. Helversen, O. von, 1993. Adaptations of flowers to the pollination by glossophagine bats. In W. Barthlott, C.M. Naumann, K. Schmidt-Loske, and K.L. Schuchmann (Eds.). Animal-plant interactions in tropical environments, pp. 41–59. Zoologisches Forschungs institut und Museum Alexander Koenig, Bonn, Germany. Helversen, O. von, 2003. Object recognition by echolocation: a nectar-feeding bat exploiting the flowers of a rain forest vine. Journal of Comparative Physiology 189: 327–336. 21 Herrera, C.M. 1996. Floral traits and plant adaptation to insect pollinators: a devil's advocate approach. In D.G. Lloyd and S.C.H. Barrett (Eds.). Floral biology: studies on floral evolution in animal-pollinated plants, pp. 65-87. Chapman and Hall, New York, New York, USA Howell, D.J. and Roth, B.S., 1981. Sexual reproduction in agaves: the benefits of bats; the cost of semelparous advertising. Ecology 62: 1–7. Iturralde-Vincent, M., 2001. Geology of the amber-bearing deposits of the Greater Antilles. Caribbean Journal of Science. Johnson, S.D. and Steiner, K.E., 2000. Generalization versus specialization in plant pollination systems. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15: 140–143. Kishore, K., Kalita, H. Rinchen, and D. Lepcha, B., 2012. Evidence of functional specialization and pollination syndrome in Amomum subulatum Roxb. (Zingiberaceae). Current Science (Bangalore) 103: 193–199. Liu, A., Li, D. Wang, H. Kress, and W.J., 2006. Ornithophilous and chiropterophilous pollination in Musa itinerans (Musaceae), a pioneer species in tropical rain forests of Yunnan, Southwestern China. Biotropica 34: 254–260. Martén-Rodríguez, S., Almarales-Castro, A. and Fenster, C.B., 2009. Evaluation of pollination syndromes in Antillean Gesneriaceae: evidence for bat, hummingbird and generalized flowers. Journal of Ecology 97: 348–359. McMullen, C.K., 1989. The Galapagos carpenter bee, just how important is it? Noticias de Galapagos. 22 Muchhala, N., 2009. A generalized pollination system in the tropics: bats, birds and Aphelandra acanthus. Annals of Botany 103: 1481–1487. Muchhala, N., 2007. Adaptive trade-off in floral morphology mediates specialization for flowers pollinated by bats and hummingbirds. The American Naturalist 169: 494-504. Muchhala, N., 2010. Fur versus Feathers: pollen delivery by bats and hummingbirds and consequences for pollen production. The American Naturalist 175: 717–726. Ollerton, J., Alarcón, R., Waser, N.M., Price, M.V., Watts, S., Cranmer, L., Hingston, A., Peter, C.I., and Rotenberry, J., 2009. A global test of the pollination syndrome hypothesis. Annals of Botany 103: 1471 –1480. Pauw, A., 2009. Flies and flowers in Darwin’s race. Evolution 63: 268–79. Raffaele, H., Wiley, J., Garrido, O., Keith, A. and Raffaele, J., 2003. Princeton field guide: birds of the West Indies, Princeton University Press. Raw, A., 1985. The ecology of Jamaican bees (Hymenoptera). Revista Brasileira de Entomologia 29: 1–16. Sazima, M., Buzato, S., and Sazima, I., 1999. Bat-pollinated flower assemblages and bat visitors at two Atlantic Forest sites in Brazil. Annals of Botany 83: 705–712. Sazima, M., Sazima, I., and Buzato, S., 1994. Nectar by day and night: Siphocampylus sulfureus (Lobeliaceae) pollinated by hummingbirds and bats. Plant Systematics and Evolution 191: 237–246. 23 Slauson, L.A., 2000. Pollination biology of two chiropterophilous agaves in Arizona. American Journal of Botany 87: 825–836. Stebbins, G.L., 1970. Adaptive radiation of reproductive characteristics in angiosperms, pollination mechanisms. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 1: 307–326. Takayama, K., Ohi-Toma, T. Kudoh, H., and Kato, H., 2005. Origin and diversification of Hibiscus glaber, species endemic to the oceanic Bonin Islands, revealed by chloroplast DNA polymorphism. Molecular Ecology 14: 1059–1071. Takayama, K., Kajita, T., Murata, J., and Tateishi, Y., 2006a. Isolation and characterization of microsatellites in the sea hibiscus (Hibiscus tiliaceus L., Malvaceae) and related hibiscus species. Molecular Ecology Notes 6: 721–723. Takayama, K., Kajita, T., Murata, J., and Tateishi, Y., 2006b. Phylogeography and genetic structure of Hibiscus tiliaceus, speciation of a pantropical plant with seadrifted seeds. Molecular Ecology 15: 2871–2881. van der Niet, T., and Johnson, S.D., 2006. Phylogenetic evidence for pollinator-driven diversification of angiosperms. Trends in ecology and evolution 27: 353-361. Voss, 1980. Floral biology of Markea neurantha Hemsley Solanaceae, a bat-pollinated epiphyte. American Midland Naturalist 103: 262-268. Waser, N. M., and Ollerton, J., 2006. Plant-pollinator interactions: from specialization to generalization, University of Chicago Press. 24 Waser, N. M.,Chittka, L., Price, M., Williams, N., and Ollerton, J., 1996. Generalization in pollination systems, and why it matters. Ecology 77: 1043–1060. 25 TABLE 1. Mean number and range of conspecific pollen grains (± s.d.) deposited on H. elatus stigmas and the percent of pollen deposited on stigmas at night and during the day. Sites are Kew Park (KP), Bunker’s Hill (BH), and Holywell (HW). Fls is the total number of flowers sampled for pollen day or night at each site. Nocturnal Diurnal % Pollen deposited Site Fls Range Mean (± s.d.) Fls Range Mean (± s.d.) Night Day KP 30 44 – 1348 819.77 374.70 30 0 – 12 17.27 31.54 97.9 % 2.1 % BH 30 26 – 822 461.93 220.31 30 0 – 12 17.33 32.02 96.4 % 3.6 % HW 15 83 – 1180 517.47 344.87 15 0 – 32 7.53 9.88 98.6 % 1.4 % _ 25 26 TABLE 2. Total number Monophyllus redmani (MR) and Glossophaga soricina (GS) sampled for H. elatus pollen (N) at three sites in Jamaica. Sites are Kew Park (KP), Bunker’s Hill (BH) and Holywell (HW). (WP) is the number of individuals with H. elatus pollen. MR GS Site Sampling dates N WP N WP KP 1/4/12 – 1/7/12 8 6 0 0 BH 12/30/11 – 1/2/12 5 5 8 5 11 3 0 0 HW 1/21/12 – 1/28/12 26 27 TABLE 3. Visit rates (visits flower-1 hour-1) to H. elatus based on videos (V) and visual surveys (S) at three sites in Jamaica. Sites are Kew Park (KP), Bunker’s Hill (BH), and Holywell (HW). (h) is the total number of hours surveyed. (Fls) is the total number of flowers surveyed during the night or day at each site. Nocturnal visit rates Site h Fls (V) Bats Hawkmoths KP 31.50 14 0.35 0.14 BH 29.50 21 0.28 HW 30.00 8 0.15 . Other moths Flies Wasps 0.08 - 0.00 0.05 0.06 0.00 - - 0.04 0.05 - Diurnal visits rates Fls (S) Apis 11 104 0.02 0.01 - 30.17 11 102 0.09 0.01 30.88 7 49 0.10 0.01 Site h Fls (V) KP 29.23 BH HW T. polytmus M. minima . A. mango Flies Wasps - 0.01 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 - - 0.03 - 27 28 TABLE 4: Percent of time visitors contacted pollen (%P) and/or stigmas (%S) during visits to H. elatus flowers. (Total) is the total number of visits where pollen and/or stigmas are contacted. Kew Park Nocturnal Visitors Bats Bunker’s Hill Holywell Total 153 %P 89.5 %S 65.4 Total 175 %P 96.6 %S 67.4 Hawkmoths 61 3.3 1.6 33 15.2 0 - Moths 34 32.4 2.9 38 13.2 0 10 10.0 0 Flies - - - 3 0 0 11 9.1 0 Wasps 1 0 100.0 - - - - - - 305 17.4 0 177 29.9 4 0 10 0 0 3.1 - - - 0 - - - 46 15.2 - - Diurnal Visitors Apis mellifera 54 0.09 0.02 T. polytmus 33 0 0 41 2.4 M. minima - - - 30 16.7 A. mango - - - 20 0 42 7.1 7.1 32 15.6 3.1 0 0 30 13.3 0 Flies Wasps 1 Total 35 %P 71.4 - %S 71.4 - 10.9 28 29 TABLE 5. Pollinator Importance (PI) values for visitors to H. elatus flowers are the product of average visit rate (visits flower-1 hour-1) (± s.d.), and the probability of pollen and stigma contact (Fpollen , Fstigma) during a visit. WPI is the relative PI value. Values were pooled across sites. Visitor visit rate (± s.d.) Fpollen Fstigma PI values (x103) WPI Bats 0.26 (0.1) 0.86 0.68 152.0 99.67 Hawkmoths 0.09 (0.07) 0.06 0.01 0.1 0.07 Moths 0.06 (0.02) 0.19 0.01 0.1 0.07 Flies (night) 0.02 (0.03) 0.03 0 0 0 Wasps (night) 0 (0) 0 0.33 0 0 Apis mellifera 0.07 (0.04) 0.16 0.01 0.1 0.07 T. polytmus 0.01 (0) 0.01 0 0 0 M. minima 0.01 (0.01) 0.06 0.01 0 0 A. mango 0.01 (0.01) 0 0 0 0 Flies (day) 0.02 (0.01) 0.13 0.07 0.2 0.13 Wasps (day) 0 (0.01) 0.04 0 0 0 29 30 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Page Monophyllus redmani with light yellow H. elatus pollen on its belly (red arrow) and pollen from a different plant on its chest and head..............27 2 Hibiscus elatus. (A) flower showing wide corolla aperture and position of the stigmas in relation to the anthers. (B) pollen grains on stigma…………..................................................................................… 28 3 Nectar secretion and concentration at Kew Park and Bunker's Hill. Nectar secretion (A) and concentration (B) at Kew Park and Bunker's Hill. Sampling periods correspond to nectar produced during the first night measured at 4:55-9:55 (Morning), the next day measured at 15:45 - 20:05 (Evening), and the second night measured at 5:50-11:15 (Second Morning)...................................................................... 29 31 FIGURE 1: Monophyllus redmani with light yellow H. elatus pollen on its belly (red arrow) and pollen from a different plant on its chest and head 32 A B FIGURE 2: Hibiscus elatus. (A) flower showing wide corolla aperture and position of the stigmas in relation to the anthers. (B) pollen grains on stigma A Kew Park Bunker's Hill 33 B FIGURE 3: Nectar secretion and concentration at Kew Park and Bunker's Hill. Nectar secretion (A) and concentration (B) at Kew Park and Bunker's Hill. Sampling periods correspond to nectar produced during the first night measured at 4:55 9:55 (Morning), the next day measured at 15:45 - 20:05 (Evening), and the second night measured at 5:50-11:15 (Second Morning) 33