Behavioral and Characterological Attributional Styles as
Transcription
Behavioral and Characterological Attributional Styles as
The litera on self-b and depr reve two inte pro Fir alt R. resea has prod inco find A c lite rev om th the incon are relat to the meth of asse attr A c stu de or hdid acolleg respec unive He is ighly the first in his fami to go On being asked to name most impo facto in and acti (e.g An & W 19 K & M sition on the Olym divin team In an inter with her Jonath marri lasted 2 y 4 m and 5 d He ears ays on much soul-s in the final mon Afte it was all over he confid to a f that "It must have riend been my faul I'm really not very good at gettin along with wom that "I guess it was really my fault for gettin into that situa We had been drinki and necki a l I s have ot. inhou vited him to my room. Juli stra attr for the da ra al ha m im basic units of all attrib theor The two mos basi ques may also inc her fee of op ab th fu a que Of par int ha be th no p Craig A. Ander and Jody C. Dill, Depa of Psyc Uni and mo de Th tw m w re We thank Lyn Abram Ronn JanoW and the vari son & A 198 An Je & A rn 1 Swe An & B 19 fo re It is cl fr a A. Ander Depar of Psych Univ of Miss .- Psyc:hoIocicaJ American by 1994 Copyript the JounW of Personality and Social Psycholosy 1994. Vol. 66. No.3. S49-SS8 Association, Inc. 0022-3S I4/94i$3.00 Behavioral and Characterological Attributional Styles as Predictors of and Loneliness: Review, Refinement, and Test Sedikides Constantine and Dill, L. Miller, Rowland S. C. Anderson, A. Alice Jody Riger, Depression Craig , JanoW-Bulman's (1979) conceptualizations of self-blame are clear, empirical operationalization is difficult and has resulted in approaches that do not capture the richness of the constructs. Second, past to more accurately represent the self-blame conceptualization revealed that both behavioral and characterological self-blame contribute uniquely to depression and loneliness. Supplementary results regarding circumstantial attributions and regarding attributional styles for success were presented. Empirical issues regarding possible methodological refinements and eWect size, as well as the value of categorical approaches to the study of attributional style were discussed. Pete recently received his PhD in structural engineering (the attributional process). This distinction has proved useful in thinking about the voluminous attribution literature. as well as in dividing up the sequence of processes involved in attributions from this success he replied, "I worked so very hard to do the best that I could in schooL" After years of training and competition, Meredith won a po- 1980). Attributional hometown newspaper she reported, "I have no real secret to success, I was simply born with the necessary athletic ability and have used this gift," This article is mainly concerned with the attributional process, and specifically, with the effects of different types of attributions for good and bad events. Pete's effort attribution for his academic success should produce positive affect as well as high motivation in similar future endeavors. Jonathan's trait attribution for his failed marriage may exacerbate his already negative affective state and could induce self-defeating behaviors in future interpersonal relationships. The other two cases are less clear. Meredith's ability attribution for her diving success may produce generally positive reactions. However, a few well-placed failures in this or related domains, in conjunction with this "ability" view of the task, could lead her to give up too easily. Julie is a college junior. After a party, she and her boyfriend . went to her room, where he physically forced her to have sexual intercourse with him. She explained to the rape crisis counselor These kinds of events and the explanations of them are the plications. Blaming a strategic mistake in judgment might increase her shame and anger in the immediate situation. But, it tions.concern how people go about making a particular attribution for a particular event (the attribution process) and what the effects of a particular attribution are likely to be on a person's emotional, motivational, and behavioral reactions to the event about her ability to predict and control the future. Much research has addressed these types of attributional differ in the types of attributions they consistently make for events in their lives. Such a consistent pattern of attributions across events is known as a person's attributiona/ sty/e. Attributional style differences may playa major role in the development and maintenance of problems in living characterized by nega- versity of Missouri, Columbia; Rowland S. Miller. Department ofPsychology, Sam Houston State University; Alice L. Riger, Department of Psychology, West Virginia University; Constantine Sedikides, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina. Chapel Style Effects tive affect searched problems Hill. are depression and loneliness. (See Ander- reviewers for their many helpful comments on a draft of this article. Correspondence concerning this article should he addressed to Craig lumbia, Columbia, Missouri 65211. Electronic mail may be sent these reviews and more recent research that individual differences in attributional style are associated with problems in living. to [email protected]. 549 !1 MIL RIG DIL SE bec som uns ca ar no co (e m Jan (19 als th tw ty os style can be define as the relati frequ with whic part ity que is fra wi a f or th u c ble cau wil als ha the int fu or mary and how to comb vario dime (An & ima the in va hy si T Deuse 1991, 1993; Ande & R 1991 Carv 198 ige Carve & S 1991) None this appr chei has been produ will contin to yield impo insig Jan (19 ch bl q w the promi of assess attrib at the sam conc how mu do yo bla yo fo w y d . A " o level that peopl use when maki attrib Two inte num of res ha us th sa qu o v wit the dir rat me (se Ta 1) A th q tion see to tap int the co de b J rectly releva studie have been mixe We belie that the emcau The sub ma ve we el su e We hav lab the sec m op c S terms, intern cause The prim disti nctio betw the two Pet et al. als cre an ex ca A c(s o tent, with behav being unsta and char mo rec stu ha us th sa co sc T 550 T ANDERSON, ! Process Assumptions and Measurement Technique ical ones being stable. Stability is not in itself sufficient, though, Although this research primarily concerns attributional processes (Le., the effects of attributions), implicit in any particular attributional style study are assumptions about attribution processes (e.g., how people make attributions). For example, do people think about causes in terms of certain dimensions, such as stability and locus? Or, do people think about causes in terms of categories or types, such as effort and ability? The most popular technique for assessing attributional style has people generate causes for hypothetical events and then rate the causes on key attribution dimensions (controllability, stability, locus, and globality). Recent work (e.g., Anderson, 1991) suggests that such dimensional thinking may be relatively rare, although it is clear that with proper instruction, people can do it acceptably well. The alternative is to develop categorical procedures for assessing attributional style. That is, attributional ular types of attributions are made. The dimensional approach to attributional style research has produced many advances in recent years. There are major issues yet to be resolved, such as which dimensions are truly pri- Impediments to and characterological self-blame Progress (Janoff-Bulman, 1979). However, this approach has not caught on nearly so well despite lated problems may account for this. First, there are definitional problems in creating appropriate attributional style measures of self-blame. Second, the empirical results from the few dipirical anomalies Definitional are the result of the definitional problems. Problems Janoff-Bulman's ical and behavioral (1979) original discussion of characterologself-blame attributions still seems quite apt. Behavioral self.blame is control related. involves attributions to a modifiable source (one's behavior), and is associated with a belief in the future avoidability of a negative outcome. Characterological self.blame is esteem related, involves attributions to a relatively nonmodifiable source (one's character). and is associated with a belief in personal deservingness for past' negative outcomes. (p. 1798) Both are types of self-blame that are. in standard of the att::-ibutor. In blaming one's behavior, an individual is concerned with the fu. ture, particularly the future avoidability of the negative outcome. . . . In blaming himself or herself characterologically, the individ. ual is not concerned with control in the future, but rather with the past, particularly deservingness for past outcomes. (p. 1800) The key elements in this theory can be linked to standard attribution dimensions. These are the locus of the attributed cause (internal for both), the stability of the cause (behavioral are unstable, characterological are stable), and the controllabil. ity of the cause (behavioral are controllable, characterological are uncontrollable). The time-orientation distinction may be linked to the controllability element as well; if the controllabil- Three methods have been used to assess characterological and behavioral attributional styles. Each method has subjects methods differ in how subjects' attributional reactions to the situations are assessed. In the direct rating method, subjects rate how much they blame various factors for each of the described situations. The rated factors include characterological and behavioral blame. The categorical approach to attributional style has also proved fruitful, especially Janoff-Bulman's categories ofbehavioral blame differ in terms of the time orientation Specifically, she wrote the following: dimensional types is in terms of controllability. Behavioral causes are potentially controllable, whereas characterological Ol1esare not. The stability dimension also captures this distinction to some ex- the following (p. 1803): "Given what happened, how much do you blame yourselffor the kind of person you are," with specific information relating to the situation included (e.g., "the kind of person who is in an accident," for her accident scenario). The parallel behavioral blame question was, "Given what happened, similar ones, to assess characterological and behavioral blame Bulman, they do not capture the full richness of those con. structs. The main problem is that the questions only subtly capture the distinction between controllable and uncontrollable those who are intent on getting their extra credit and leaving the lab. Similarly, the time-orientation distinction may be too weakly present to capture subjects' attention. jects generate open-ended attributions for the hypothetical situations. Then, expert raters code the attributions. Peterson, Schwartz, and Seligman (1981) were the first to use this proce- dure. In their study, . Behavioral codes were made when the attribution referred to some action by the subject-either an overt motor behavior or 'internal' behaviors, such as wants, preferences, and intentions. Character. ological attributions referred to personality dispositions of the subject, such as stupidity, immaturity, ability, and so on. (pp. 255-256) BEH AND CHA ST Ander Horow & F 1983 No. I renc No.2 Stoltz & G 1989 alas Tenne H 1987 erzb Carve Ganel & B 1985 ehar Aett, Blank H 1990 olow Major Muell ilde Meyer & T 1986 aylo Note. Metho FC = f choic attrib orce DR = d ratin of blam OC = o irec att co by ex pe ra D BDl = B Depre Inven Zung = Z Selfeck Dep Sca Ow ung c or mo de sc re .ble O article used a m split proce prese rigin poin bise corr edia wer com fro the rep F v a I). As with the direct rating meth this open codi cal and behav const How it too does not seem to futu Or did he tru me tha so ab h c ter, whi is rela sta an un w a f a studie has used this metho (And Horo & F ren pro are mo lik to be se to th di b stance The first two are chara the next two are behav In the other study only three choi were avai havior measu of attrib style Th us, the tech is along aA cspersp harac conti corr with Tw ma pre ha gu re in th ar F IW tha an ano rev fo br pto e on the imila defin prob .- 1 I 551 Table I Studies of Characterological and Behavioral Attributional Style and Depression Characterological Study Stressor Failure Depression N Method Behavioral Success Failure Success -.04 -.27*** -.12* General depression and attributional style studies 121 PetersonetaJ.,1981 84 334 87 Janoff-Bulman, Feather, 1983 1979, No. I 120 248 101 201 FC BDl .32**. FC BDl .41*** OC BDl .72*** OC BDI OC BDl DR BDl DR BDl DR BDI Specific stressor, depression, and attributional 243 Immediate Abortion 3 weeks Abortion 96 Rape 58 *p<.OS. **p<.OI. DR much of the complexity in the characterologi- adequately emphasize the controllability feature, which plays so major a role in Janoff-Bulman's distinction between characterological and behavioral blame. The third method, forced choice, presents several attributional choices with each to-be-imagined situation. Subjects then choose the attribution that best fits them. Only one article (two 1983). In one study, six alternatives were available for each situation: ability, personality, effort, strategy, mood, and circum- ability, strategy, and effort. Ability and personality are internal, uncontrollable, stable causes that tend to reflect a past orientation. Strategy and effort are internal, controllable, unstable causes that reflect a future orientation. Thus, this method has the controllability distinction built into it. However, this method introduces a different problem. In the forced choice method, choosing one cause precludes choosing another. A person who makes many characterological choices necessarily makes few behQvioral ones. The procedure forces fairly substantial negative correlations between the characterological and beuseful in testing the general proposition that attributional and other problems, but it does not testing of which component (characterological. both) is contributing to the correlation. arises -.13 -.11 .10 .29*. -.19 .09 ns -.07 .06 .19* .36*** .31*** style studies BDl" .47** .40* .14* .25* ns ns .46**. Own .28* cally implies behavioral self-blame as well. 1 In the failed marriage example, the characterological attribution "I'm not very good at getting along with women," implies that the attributor behaved in ways that drove his wife away. The key distinction for classifying that attribution as characterological or behavioral is the presumed modifiability of the behavior. In other words, did the attributor expect it to be unstable and controllable in the The dire~ rating questions obscure these issues, rather than highlighting them. The expert raters used in open-ended coding without lem -.44**. ***p<.OOI. method captures depression .00 -.41*** -.10 .15 .19* BDl" DR -.15 .37**. BDl DR 30/31 Rape 1990 DR Frazier, .00 .13 Zung" DR 304 from the fact that characterological allow styles precise knowing Empirical typi- the subject meant, the distinction may Problems characterological behavioral, or self-blame what be hard to apply. The forced choice method reduces this confusion by making the subject choose among attributions that differ in precisely the ways that the theory suggests are important. It is important to note that we are not faulting the various researchers (including ourselves) for this earlier work. Indeed, all the studies and techniques have importantly contributed to the understanding of attributional phenomena. But every study has limitations; the limitations we have highlighted may help explain the inconsistent findings reported in the next section. our attention. blame for failure or bad events should corre- AND MIL RIG DIL SE late positiv with depre and simil prob in livin The ove go wa to pr a c te o t th l h Iioral Table p the resul of all the studi resen we loca (usi self-b were asses (b) depr was asse The tion cla wa an ex re in ef si F t w events correl positi with depre Ther is not a s ing Som los of "m or "s w e in th tr x Tra Fr Di to C that were intend in the origin conc The othe answe to the quest of whet beha blam is rela to but we the ass to ca (c b We gat da on tw di sa u d ver of the var sca Th tw w c of course the same defin prob appl to thos few for all rep ana M re a w succes studie that have been done Thus we cond a s tud To me go of cre un m o t thre cat we use str co ru fo e a eve wa cou as a c on if h it w r The presen study had three goals The first was to asse the second was to provi comp attrib style mea for succes situat The third was abmore expl itTo it ens tha com co ac th tw sa \\ a rela usi aBag T IeN o 55-2 Second, behavioral blame for failure or bad events should correlate negatively with depression and similar problems in living. Characterological and behavioral attributions for success (or positive) events have not received as much theoretical or empirical attention. PsycLit, American Psychological Association, 1990-92) that met the following criteria: (a) Both characterological and behav- top half presents the main studies of interest, namely, those in which general depression and attributional styles were assessed. The bottom half presents studies in which depression as a reaction to a specific stressor was related to characterological and behavioral blame. The studies are organized by their attributional style assessment method. This highlights the inconsistencies in results. Frequency of characterological attributions for failure and bad reversal in this pattern. In contrast, frequency of behavioral attributions for failure events shows marked inconsistency. The inconsistencies, however, are almost. perfectly related to the method of measuring characterological and behavioral blame. The forced choice and the open-ended coding studies show the expected negative relation between frequency of behavioral attributions for failure and level of depression. The direct rating studies all show the opposite relation. . This pattern suggests that the direct rating method may obscure the controllability implications of behavioral attributions assessment methods may make this dimension more salient and thereby may produce the more predictable pattern. This is speculative, however, and the bottom line is that there is no clear. depression and other problems in living. The empirical inconsistencies as well as the definitional problems preclude any strong conclusion. Furthermore, there is too little research on characterological and behavioral attributions for success events to draw any conclusions about their relation to depression. And to further examine these issues. attributions to circumstances for success should positively relate to problems in living (e.g., depression and loneliness). The data are weaker for circumstantial attributions for failure, but it is reasonable to expect negative correlations with problems in living. ries relating characterological self-blame positively and behavioral self-blame negatively to problems in living (depression and loneliness). This overriding goal required us to maximize interpretational clarity, so we devised a conservative attributional style assessment procedure. Note that the cost of interpreta- very different reasons, we expected that the magnitude of the attributional style/problems in livil1g correlations would be considerably lower than those typically found in this area. First, our conservative criteria (described in a later section) guaranteed that many of the attributions generated by subjects would be unclassified because of possible ambiguities. Second, the new attributional style assessment procedure involved translating subjects' dimensional ratings into categorical frequency counts. tion process. Characterological, behavioral, and circumstantial attribu- tional styles were assessed for success and failure events. These categorical attributional style measures were derived with a new assessment method designed to circumvent the definitional and interpretational problems discussed earlier. In our dimensional/categorical translation method, subjects generated openended attributions for a set of hypothetical events. They then rated their own attributions on the key defining dimensions: locus (internality), controllability, and stability. Individual attri- havioral, and circumstantial) on the basis of these ratings. The . frequency of each type was used as the measure of that attributional style and was correlated with depression and loneliness. used to assess the independent contributions of various predic- tors. Overview Goals attributional style categories of behavioral and characterological self-blame in a manner that avoids the definitional and methodological problems found in the direct rating, openended coding, and forced choice methods discussed earlier. The was to assess a third type of attribution that has occasionally been assessed in this domain, external (/ r('lmtstances (Anderson et al., 1983; Peterson et al., 1981). Although there has been less theoretical and empirical attention de\( 1t\:dto this attributional category, the work that does exist suggests that the frequency of butional style category. For instance, an attribution for a failure as internal and stable and uncontrollable. Although this procedure undoubtedly decreased the frequency of each attribution type and was expected to yield relatively low correlations, our 2 For each sample, 12 correlations resulted from the design: 3 attribution types (characterological, behavioral, and circumstantial) x 2 outcomes (success and failure) x 2 criteria (depression and loneliness). propriate, we tested the differences the tests approached significance, two samples were comparable. between indicating each of the 12 pairs of corthat the results from the BEH AND CHA ST (Be & B 197 wa ad Be it ha fe ec it to Sub in bot sam co th R U Sca (Ru Pep & C 19 Th sc is bL otauh proced got us the pure meas we need a l sam arg sam lev eve tim the sam sit ar In th st s the futu but did no me the ca be at sa le T dim def for loc co an gl w t h as in the AS (se An & A 19 p 2 partici for cours credit Stude who were not nativ spea of Englis those who were not Unite State citize and thos who wer older than 26 dropp from the samp Som subj did not final sampl size analy invol depr was 614; for lone ness. the final sampl size was 625.3 of inte We als ass a t ty Th ci hi ty w ties (one large midw unive and two large and one sma sout from the sampl The final samp size was 282 for both the depr ther wer 10 suc and 10 fai ite a s co h f u r and loneli analy Table 2 s the distr of men how and wome in both samp There were appro equa num of men wome Prelim analy yield no sex effec so all reSam 2. Th sam ba ma an de w u t o asse the freq of att ty w t fo e item wer tak fro the AS (a in Sa I) an fr tw (Se Ab Se & v Ba 19 th B o Att Sty Qu (F & T 1 Mend Mock & E 1961 was admi This scal rbau is a cau a n of hy sit ap um on m t h widely used measu of intens of depre and is the mos com sca wit 3athe mi Th th op soc each item can range from O. indic no dt:pr sym . " 553 Table 2 Sample scale scores are much lower: the possible Characteristics Depression Sample women Characteristic men No. . No. . I Loneliness Sample 2 281 155 . 333 127 21 0 63 16 II 6 2 0 No. items in scale Lowest possible Highest possible 90th percentile 75th percentile 50th percentile 25th percentile 10th percentile 13 0 39 9 6 3 I 0 Sample 1 288 337 20 20 80 52 43 34 29 25 Loneliness Sample 2 155 127 20 20 80 55 46 37.5 31 26 goal of fairly testing the basic theoretical propositions could still be met by using a large sample size. That is. in this context, effect size is not an important consideration. although power to detect a weak effect is. In brief, our conservative translation size got us the necessary Sample statistical Sample Three power. sumption that loneliness is it unidimensional construct that varies primarily on experienced intensity or frequency. Each of the 20 items is scored such that a I indicates lack ofloneliness and a 4 indicates a high level ofloneliness. Thus. total scale scores can range from 20 to 80. AttributionaiStyie Sample J. The 20-item "the degree Subjects referred version of the Attributional Style to which the cause can be expected to the extent that the cause was expected to be present to be present at the again in 1 complete all measures at a large midwestern university relevant to this study. Therefore. the Subjects rated the causes on 9-point scales, with 9s representing more controllable, internal, unstable, and global causes. For this study of frequency of specific attribution types, it was necessary to translate these dimension ratings into appropriate attribution types. The characterological type was defined as any attribution that was rated as uncontrollable ( < 5), internal ( > 5), and stable ( < 5). The behavioral type was defined as any attribution that was rated as controllable ( > 5). internal ( > 5), and unstable ( > 5). These were the two main attribution types 2 hundred twenty-one undergraduates at four different universi- western universities) participated for course credit. Subjects with missing data on any of the measures relevant to this study were dropped ( 5). exterdefined as any attribution that was rated as uncontrollable < a nal ( < 5), and unstable ( > 5). Because globality is not theoretically part of these attribution types, it was not used. The number of characterological, behavioral, and circumstantial attributions was counted separately for success and failure items. Because 0 to 10 characterological, within each outcome. ported dimensional Assessment Test (ASAT-III: Anderson et al.. 1988) was used. This scale presents the subject with five hypothetical situations of each of four types: interpersonal success. non interpersonal success, interpersonal failure. and noninterpersonal failure. Because the distinction between interpersonal and noninterpersonal situations is irrelevant to the main thrust of this article, all reported analyses combine across this dimension so that there are 10 success and 10 failure situations. Subjects imagine themselves in the situations. write down the most likely major cause of the specified outcome, and rate that cause on several standard attribution dimensions (stability, locus, controllability. and gIobality). In the present sample. there was one modification to the wording of the stability dimension. In the ASAT-III, stability of a cause refers to Method Six hundred eighty undergraduates correctly range is from 0 to 39. analyses combine behavioral, and circumstantial attributions across sex. First, the original ASAT-III definition of stability was used. Second. Instruments Depression other attributional style scales. the Attributional Style Questionnaire Sample J. The full 2 I-item Beck Depression Inventory (Beck. Ward. measure of depression used in studies of attnnutional style. Each item Scores on describes a specific behavioral manifestation \11'depression. to 3. indicating a severe level ofsymptomatol\)g.v. Total scale scores can thus range from 0 to 63. Sample 2. The 13-item short-form of the Bt:\:k Depression Inventory the scales. the total number of success and failure items was 20 and 18. respectively. Third, the self-generated causes were rated on 5-point 3 Portions dimensional of this data set were analyzed by way of more traditional procedures and reported in Anderson and Riger ( 1991 ). AND MIL RIG DIL SE Relat Freq (% of Tota of Cha Beh uati Th mo fre att w t b ty h As exp the rel fre l In th o wer sm eno tha we sh ex so at Thi cou be a p if ou go w t pr ro a n admin in rando order Testi was done in grou Wit each instru the of items the same for all subj Inwit dep and lon Ou ho w 1 c tasks. On comp tasks subje retur their mat to in this are in ord to ha so po to de th effe und con W t ne u The same basic proce were used with the follo exce varied Secon a b writte debri was adm rief to all sub jects on comp of the quest A m deta deb ore lonelin The two samp were quite com Sam I was slightl less lonely than Samp 2 ( = 3 and 39.0 Ms 6.6 respec This differ was relia 1(91 = 3 p < how allo for .13 a m op vi of th fu a m o items, but a d statis comp of irect the two grou was the two sampl for both depre and lone On the full asp of beh att se ad w a ble numb of mildl and mode depr indi r , 554 3 Attributions by Type of Situation Characterological Failure Success Failure Sample No. attribution items Relative frequency 10 No. 18 10 4.8% 10 3.1% I 10 10 10 10 10 11.1% 20.2% 1.9% 3.1% 6.1% 18 Succss 2 20 18 20 18 20 terological. behavioral. and circumstantial cutoffpoint. attributions 1.1% 0.9% 8.0% 1.8% 13.3% frequency 20 2.0% Relative items Failure Success Failure Success 12.3% Sample attribution Total Circumstantial Behavioral 3.8% and Circumstantial used 3 as the 3.2% Table ioral, and circumstantial for failure attributions for success and failure sit- situations. Procedure Sample 1 attributional The depression. loneliness, and attributional style instruments tributional formed consent was obtained at the start of the testing session. Then the booklets were distributed, with an instruction sheet explaining the the experimenter and indicated whether they wished to receive further information about the study. Those who requested such information were mailed a thorough debriefing. Sample with depression and loneliness. style technique a measurement technique designed that would to maximize allow correlations us to test theoreti- cally important issues in as clean It fashion as possible. The expected attenuation in correlation size is relevant to those issues only in that it forced us to use larger sample sizes than normal tests. 2 it. requested who those was to .~ First, there were four different versions of theattributional style questionnaire, in which item order and attribution dimension order were mailed style correlations were Results Descriptive Summaries Table 2 presents the sample characteristics on depression and .005. Sample I appeared to be slightly more depressed than Sample 2, after adjusting for the different number of depression not appropriate. scale measure The distributional of depression, shapes were comparable the sample contained in an apprecia- The standard definition of mild depression on this scale is a total score of 10 to IS. Fully 29% of the sample displayed scores of 10 or greater. Moderate depression is Jdined as a score of 16-23. Fully 10% had scores of 16 or greater. Table 3 presents the frequency of characterological, behav- Correlational Summaries Predictions The main questions of interest are whether the frequencies of characterological and behavioral attributions correlated with measures of depression and loneliness. Theoretically, the frequency of characterological attributions for failure should correlate positively with depression and loneliness, whereas the frequency of behavioral attributions for failure should correlate negatively with depression and loneliness. Blaming stable uncontrollable aspects of oneself for failure is clearly self-defeating. Blaming unstable controllable aspects of oneself for failure, energize behavioral attempts to overcome initial setbacks. Theoretical predictions for characterological and behavioral attributions in success situations are less clear. The internal and stable aspects of characterological attributions seem adaptive when applied to successes; however, the uncontrollable aspect is generally less positive. Similarly, the internal and controllable to successes, but the unstable aspect is troubling. Circumstantial attributions for successes are obviously maladaptive for both affective and motivational reasons, as one is essentially denying credit and viewing the success as unlikely to be repeatable (i.e., it is uncontrollable) or repeated (i.e.. the I . . BEH AND CHA ST Tw rem qu co (a th ov m seem adapti but the uncon aspe is mala The analys strate we used is quite strai We separa Then we avera the corre corr uswel and in wh wa att sty ca p d sion analys were also used to give a b idea of how well (or ette Mu cor an re W f ex th i tween the attribu frequ meas and depr and lonelin for succe and failur situa As can be seen by = . P < . I ) f 3 0 causes were more depre and more lone than thos who All six attr fre (n of ch jects who attribu succe to chara caus wer .00 I ) c res Th m or w a re l more depres and lonel than those who did not (ps < . 05) dep and the lon Rs (a sa b 2 Do the dif typ of att st co in pen to pre de an lo A the depres correl was signi (p <.Sub 05) 555 Table 4 Correlations Between Attribution Style (Success and Failure) and Loneliness and Depression. Combined Across Samples Behavioral Characterological Criterion Depression Loneliness .p<.05. problem Failure Success Failure Success Failure Success (n (n .178*** .144*** .108** .071* -.107** -.174**. .014 .006 -.072.. -.032 .114..* :119.** = 896) = 907) *.p<.005. *..p<.OOI. cause is unstable). The consequences of circumstance attributions for failure are less clear. The external and unstable aspects Analysis Strategy computed the correlations between the various attribution frequency measures and depression and loneliness for each sample ing a weighted r-to-z transformation poorly) these attribution sion and loneliness. frequency process. Multiple regres- measures predict depres- Findings Zero-ordercorrelations. Table 4 presents the correlations be- these results, the main theoretical predictions were supported. Subjects who attributed imagined failures to characterological did not Circumstantial (ps < .00 I). Similarly, subjects who attributed failures to behavioral causes were less depressed and less lonely than those who did not (ps < .005). Subjects who attributed successes to circumstances were more depressed and lonely than those who did not (ps < .00 I). Overall, these three sets of findings confirm the theoretical work of Janoff-Bulman ( 1979) and rep- of the correlation of attributional style and problems in living when the different measures of attributional style are jointly considered and (b) the inQependent contributions of the different attributional style categories. The forced-choice methodology of previous categorical studies (Anderson et al.. 1983) precludes meaningful use of multiple regression techniques. because the predictors are necessarily correlated at high levels. The present method does not force such high (negative) correlations between types of attributions, so a better idea of how pression and loneliness may be gained by examining multiple regression results. the correlations among the attributional style measures. Interestingly, the strongest correlations demonstrated that people tended to use a particular type of attribution across different outcomes. For example, people who made relatively frequent characterological attributions for success also tended to make characterological attributions for failure (r both the depression sample and the loneliness sample. Frequency of behavioral success and behavioral failure attributions yielded the next highest correlations (rs = .32 and .31.ps < .00 I. for the depression and loneliness samples, respectively). Finally. frequency of circumstantial success and circumstantial failure attributions yielded the third highest set of correlations among attributional style measures (rs = .19, p < .00 I. for both the depression and loneliness samples). licate the forced choice and the open-ended coding findings summarized in Table i. The only empirical anomaly . concerned the finding that sub- Neither depression nor loneliness correlated significantly with characterological attributions for success in Anderson et ai. (1983). This discrepancy may be the result of the very different approaches used to assess attributional tendencies. The present finding also suggests that the negative implications of the uncontrollable aspect of characterological attributions for success outweigh the positive implications of the internal and stable aspects of such attributions. Finally, depression and loneliness were hath negatively correlated with circumstantial attributions 1;1;'t;lilure, although onlywho attributed failures tban those who did not. to circumstanl.'es. were . less depressed success attributions, number of characterological failure attributions, etc.) were entered simultaneously as predictors of depression and loneliness. In both cases, highly significant (ps < in both cases, although still smaller than most correlations reported in the forced choice and open-ended coding studies. The Table 5 presents these results. noted in the literature review, it could be that one type of attribution (characterological) is particularly maladaptive. but that other types (e.g., behavioral) correlate with depression and loneliness only because of methodological quirks of the forced choice method. The results presented in Table 5 rule out this description. The reported t tests examined the unique variance in the criterion associated with each predictor. It can be seen AND MIL RIG DIL SE .Lonel Depre R = F(6. 889) = 9 25, p < . I 00 .68 As exp the rel fre of th at cate we low Th are at lea tw re fo th F that eve a m of att w f id d a it R = .F(6, 900) = 125, p< .I00 0.0 trul dep pe wi ge a m o on ave (an lon pe g re e mo ma att th no (a n poi was nec de as irr to th ca o . inte We kno tha pe fre us di m signifi and uniqu predi depre (ps < .I). Sim 0leas wh rati am sti W a kn l one attr dim Th it sh co a n s Thi rati ana su tw em p In Sam I, 45% of the fai att an 3 o t h 2, the cor fig inc to 67 5 T blame Our new dimen trans met achoice in w that accur captu the locus stabi ay and con metho (Ande et aI., 1983 Spec our met odolog was design to more preci test Jano that ear stu of cat att st A d cess and failure situat The resul conf that (a) all ced For ins in Stu I o A et al (1 2 f (ab and trai In the stu on otncfa h attr in Sam Iapre 2% in Sa 25% m 556 . cess and failure situations contribute uniquely to the prediction of depression and loneliness. 5 Regression Resultsfor Predictor the Combined Partial Characterological failure Characterological success Behavioral failure Behavioralsuccess Circumstances failure slope Samples t'- p< Table Multiple Empirical Issues .138 3.96 .055 -.101 .065 - .089 1.59 -2.91 1.90 - 2.69 .111 3.33 .001 .112 .004 .058 .008 .001 .108 .025 -.177 .075 -.047 .109 3.10 0.74 -5.18 2.20 -1.44 3.30 .002 .463 .001 .028 .151 .001 Circumstances success Low Frequency of Attributional most attributions that most people generate are likely to be depression-neutral. That is, no theoretician in this area expects genic or nondepressogenic Characterological failure Characterological success Behavioralfailure Behavioral success Circumstances failure Circumstances success . For depression, df = 889; for loneliness. Categories pressogenic lonely) attributions. categories. Attribution Indeed. theory it seems unlikely merely asserts that people. Second, the new dimensional-categorical translation procedure produces many attributions that do not fit the conservative definitions of any of the attribution categories. Any attribution that contained at least one dimensional rating at the scale mid- df = 900. that characterological. behavioral, and circumstantial attributions for failure, and circumstantial attributions for success, all ilarly, characterological and behavioral failure attributions and behavioral and circumstantial success attributions all predicted loneliness (ps < .03). These results further confirm the original conceptualization that characterological and behavioral attributional styles independently contribute to problems in living. Discussion Summary Janoff-Bulman's (1979) theoretical insights into characterological and behavioral self-blame have not been consistently matched by the methods used to assess these attributional styles. The direct rating method in particular does not sufficiently highlight the importance of the control aspects of these attribution types. Our literature review confirmed that the varying results of past studies on self-blame and depression are almost perfectly correlated with the methods used to assess self- ended attributions are frequently ambiguous prise that many of the generated attributions in the categorical scheme. with respect to at did not "count" (a) The frequency of attributions with at least one midpoint rating (out of the three dimensions of locus, stability, and controllability) should be fairly high; (b) This frequency should be higher in Sample 2 than in Sample I because the former had fewer non midpoint choices available (5-point rather than 9point rating scales). Our results produced exactly this pattern. success attributions had at least one midpoint rating. In Sample the low frequencies of the attribution types derived from our translation method were neither unexpected nor unreasonable. Small Effect Size was designed to test theoretical derivations from J anoff- Bulman trollability features of her conceptions while avoiding the methodological confounding problems inherent in our earlier forced original predictions that characterological attributions for negative events (failure) would be positively associated with depression and tl)at behavioral attributions for negative events would be negatively associated with depression. In addition, we provided tests of a third category of attributions. circumstances, and tested all three types of attribution frequencies for both suc- three types of attributions correlate with depression and loneliness; (b) each type contributes uniquely to the prediction of depression and loneliness; and (c) attributional styles for both suc- The correlations reported in Table 4 are generally quite small. Indeed, these correlations are considerably smaller than those reported in Anderson et al. (1983). The multiple correlations reported in Table 5 are certainly respectable, but still are somewhat smaller than the forced-choice correlations obtained in cussed earlier, one reason has to do with the methodological differences between the forced-choice procedure and the present rating scale translation procedure. The relative frequencies of attributions meeting the present criteria are much smaller than comparable relative frequencies in the forced-choice proof the failure attributions were to characterological factors BEH AND CHA ST piri and hea atta wa sto ea be th e o asp on red hea att wa so gr re cien was .03 con sm th ef si s i n & M 199 Tw suc co ap to th p il oretica insigh gaine from the trans proc rem relativ (%) freque rema the same This incre in abso presse and nonde peop Seco the num (bu see Sch Ste Ho C & F 1 the correl some but we have a t noti heo sugapp wa unw W b th ap is a e ple typica make categ attrib and trans them We hop tha ou res he re m w sion. In the presen study there was yet an addi tran Abe R. P. (19 A v ex pa W a l is i son & D 1993) does sugge that peop typic euse thin Wa DC Au And C. A. Ho pe th ab ca E A., & A L. H. (1 A m o m dep lon an sh In J. H & G W ( E CA Aca Pre And C. st a e r pecial valuab This may very well invo usin afAnd orc A., & D W E (1 Sc an th re . C. Th pr o c in e cau thin and att sty A a fu t ism per In G. F. Gl & K M ( C E mot soc co (p 94 N Y S A., Ho L. M & F R A 1 r that in other conte are ackno as of im men prac And C. Jen D. A L H (Vr .- , 557 for being defined as characterological. This is a function of the fact that in a forced-choice methodology, each attribution must fit a predetermined attribution category. This is most obvious in Study 2 of Anderson et al. (1983), where all attributions had to be either characterological or behavioral. The present translation methodology does not do this, resulting in many attributions essentially having no impact on the analyses. Thus, the low correlations were expected and do not detract from the importance of the results in any way, because the the- wholly valid. However, the low correlations do suggest that prac- tical use of the translation procedure in applied settings may be limited. Three modifications to our translation method may allow higher correlations to emerge. First, increasing the number of hypothetical situations examined should increase the absolute frequencies of the different types of attributions even if the low lute frequencies should allow better discrimination between earlier. refinements the present study. Other scholars have noted a variety of contextual conditions in which seemingly' small effect sizes are judged as important or impressive (e.g., Abelson, 1985; Prentice First, when one is primarily interested in testing the validity of specific theoretical propositions, effect size is largely irrelevant except for the purposes of estimating the statistical power of various possible sample sizes. Second, when the methodology is known to produce conditions likely to minimize an effect, the discovery of a reliable effect-no matter how small-is impressive. Conclusions de- ber of scale points (say from 9-point to 15-point rating scales) might further increase both the relative and absolute frequencies of relevant attributions. Third, using even-numbered rating scales (e.g, with to points) might further reduce the midpoint ambiguity problem discussed These three methodological ers felt it would be unethical to continue giving the control group placebos. The effect size in terms of a correlation coeffi- are likely to improve gesting an additional limit to the translation procedure. If peo- into dimensions only when forced to do so, then dimensional approaches to the study of attributional style may produce artificially low relations, In essence, there may be something lost in the translation from attribution category to attribution dimen- The categorical approach, best exemplified by Janoff-Bul- man's (1979) theoretical analysis, seems to have been pronounced "dead" prematurely. The vast majority of attribu. tional style studies have used a strictly dimensional approach " for an innovative categorical approach to attributions for smoking cessation). Perhaps the inconsistencies found in early categorical studies led researchers to conclude that the categorical valuable one and that it nicely complements the dimensional patient. References tion, essentially a back translation from the dimensional back to the categorical. Some recent work (Anderson, 1991; Ander- a lot. Psychological Bulletin. 97. 129-133. American Psychogical Association. (1990-92). about attributions in categorical rather than dimensional terms. In the present context, this suggests that additional work on attributional style models of depression and other problems in living may best be done using attributional style measures that focus on attribution types. Such research would nicely complement the dimensional approach that so heavily dominates most current work. Prospective designs using a categorical (type) approach, as well as similar intervention studies, would seem eschoice methodology for assessing attributional psychologists now know that both characterological Attribution: (1990) noted Basic issues and applications (pp. 235-279). San Diego, eryday problems in living: Depression, loneliness, and shyness. Social Cognition. 3. 16-35. style. Because and behav- learned helplessness model of depression. Psychological Inquiry. 2. 14-19. Verlag. style oflonely and depressed Psychology, 45. 127-136. . Rosenthal database. of the typical phenomenal organization of attributions for success and failure. Social Cognition. 9. 295-329. ioral self-blame contribute uniquely to depression and loneliness, future studies can assess both using a forced choice methodology without concern over the confounding problem inherent in this procedure. Alternatively, an open-ended coding method that is explicitly sensitive to all three dimensional features of the key attribution types could be refined and used to test additional categorical attributional style developments. Finally, it is important to remember that what psychologists typically bemoan as "small" effects are often larger than effects importance. :rhe PsycLlT that a major study on as- people. Journal of Personality and utility of the attributional and Social style construct at a moderate level of t t AND MIL RIG DIL Kel H. H., & M J. L. (19 At th an re i Ander C. A.. & R A. L. (1991 A c attri iger ontr Ma B., Mu P.. & H K. (1 A e model of proble in living Dime and situa inte tion and cop wi ab Jo of P a S Me C.B T S. E. Ad to ra J o A., W B. (1992 Attri ein proces in perso In G. Capr & G Heck (Eds Mod . Pet c.: Sch M. & S M E ( 1 e 9 . 324). New York: Harve Whea Beck, A. T., & B R. W. (1972 Scree depr patie eck. in fam Pre D. A., & M D. T. (1 W s ef a i m i m ily practi A r techn Postg apid Med 52. 81-8 Beck. Ward C. H., Mend M., Moc J.. & E J. rba Ros R. (19 Ho are we do in so ps A (1961 An inven for meas depre Arch of Gen Rus D., Pep L. A. & C C. E. (1 T r U u e (Carve Carve C. S. 1 How shoul multi perso 989) cons be Sch T. J., Ste V. J., Ho J. E, C P R & F K. (19 Att aff an ex fo sm So, Ganel R. J., & B V. (198 Dep eha Sel M. E. P., Ab L. Y. Se A v B C C. & S M. F. (1991 Unre issue chei rega the Sto R. E, & G J. P. (19 In at an ty a o Feathe N. T. (1983 Some corre of attrib stye Dep Sw P. D., An K. & B S. (1 A s i a n T.. & T M. (1984 balan mea igge of attri dep A m rev Jo of P S e Aett, G. L.. Blank K. R., & H L. S. (199 olo Ten H., & H S. (19 De se a t h Frazie P. A. (1990 Victim attrib and posttraum Jour Janoff R. (1979 Chara versu beha self -------..- " 4 558- SEDIKIDES specificity. 990. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55. 979- Annual tions in the prediction of depression and loneliness. Social Cognition. Review of Psychology, 31. 457-501. Psychology, 48. 585-599. Personality and Social Psychology, 9. 149-181. personality psychology: Critical reviews and new directions (pp. 295- and depressive symptoms. ogy, 4/. 253-259. sive. Ps.vchological Bulletin. Journal 50. 1226-1234. of Personality and Social Ps.vchol. 112. 160-164. Psychologut.45.775-777. Psychiatr.v, 4. 561-571. tested? Issues illustrated by self-monitoring,attributional hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. and cognitive style: Comparisons between measures. sonalit,v and Social Psycho{og.v, 49. 722-728. meaning and measurement quiry, 2. 21-24. symptoms, self-esteem, Social Psychology butional and Protestant Bulletin. style. Australian of explanatory style, and 56. 577-585. Journal of Per- style. Psychological In- ethic values. Personality and 9. 125-135. Journal Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39. 472-480. cessation treatment. 184. (1979). Depressive ogy, 88. 242-247. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 9. 173- attributional style. Journal of Abnormal depression in college students: The learned helplessness ited. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 316-321. Psychol- model revis- of Psycho log.v, 36. 267-283. Psychology, and complex attributions of blame in self and others. Journal of Social Behavior and Personalit,v, 5. 175-188. of Personality evidence. 50. 974-991. absence of self-protective attributional and Social Psychology, 52. 72-80. biases. Jol/rnalofPersonality and Social Psycho{ogJ: 59. 298-304. blame: Inquiries into depression Social Psychology, 37.1798-1809. and rape. Journal o!'Personality and Received July 24, 1992 Revision received August 17, 1993 Accepted October 4, 1993 .