Symbolic Play in Preschool and Primary Settings
Transcription
Symbolic Play in Preschool and Primary Settings
r Research in Review Symbolic Play in Preschool and Primary Settings Patricia Monighan Nourot and Judith L. Van Hoorn A preschool teacher hears from parents that they fear that Andrew won't be ready for kindergarten if his major activity at school is play. Of course play is important,- but what about real learning, like the ABC's and adding and subtracting? Won't he be at a disadvantage? A second grade teacher incorporates "activity time" into each day's schedule. During this time children invent and pursue projects of their own choosing. The principal of the school visits on a day when a group of boys are preparing the advertising copy for their "Model 500" robot that they have built from cardboard boxes and$rt materials over several days' time. The principal questions the value of such playful activity in the second grade curriculum. ,jiiPfe\ a awareness that play is complex and mull ti faceted. One of the major issues teachj ers and researchers struggle with is thy precise nature of the relationship of playj to the concepts and skills valued by oiuj educational system (Greenberg, 1989T .Monighan-Nourot, 1990). By becoming] knowledgeable about the research,on] play, teachers can better meet the needy of the children they servt as well asl justify play to parents, administratorsg and other teachers. Part of the appeal of play is its com33 rom conference ballroom to growing consensus breaks down, howplex and even paradoxical nature. Fori} teachers.' lounges to state departever, when we try to put developmenteachers this paradox is reflected in di-3 ments of education, early childtally appropriate practice into practice, lemmas about supporting play in thej hood educators are agreeing on the and one of the central issues that emerge classroom: Can, we understand it and! importance of developmental ly approas teachers attempt to define the best support it without destroying its veryl priate practice (Bredekamp, 1987). The practice for young children is the role of nature? (Fein, 1985; Sutton-Smith, 1986)j play in the curriculum (Kagan. 1990). We have framed this review with ques-^ Patricia Monighan Nourot', Ph.D., is Many teachers who understand the tions drawn from our own experiencesl Associate Professor and Coordinator of value of play in the early childhood curand from discussions With teachers of] Early Childhood Education, Sonoma State riculum are able to "hold the line" with • young children who want to know (1)1 University, Rohnert Park, California For parents and/or administrators by articuhow to defend the inclusion ol play in thel many years, she was a preschool and lating the reasons why play should be a curriculum, (2) how to understand andl primary teacher. central feature of life in their classrespect the differences seen in the ways] Judith L Van Hoorn, Ph.D., is Asso- rooms. Other teachers, who know in-. different children play, and (3) how-toj ciate Professor of Educational Psychology tuitively that play is important, find that facilitate play in their classrooms. at University of the Pacific Stockton, Cali- they are unable to provide reasons lor fornia. She is a former Head Start teacher, How can teachers defend play] including play in programs for young with a special interest in science children. They may try, olten unin the curriculum? education. successfully, to resist pressures that unDrs. Nourot and Van Hoorn are two of dermine the role of play in their settings. Early childhood educators can help. the coauthors of Looking at children's others understand the relationships be3 Thus early childhood educators struggle play: A bridge between theory and practween children's use of symbols in playj tice, and with colleagues are finishing a to formulate meaningful, convincing arand their use of symbols in the academic! guments for play as it is viewed in the new book entitled Play at the center of the curriculum. Concepts valued in thy larger context of lifelong education. curriculum. What answers can teachers give? Per- . school curriculum such as reading, wntr 7Viur is one of a regular series of Reing, and problem solving are all based on! haps the most frequently quoted cliches search in Review columns: The column in the expectation that children are capa? are "Play is the child's way of learning" this issue was edited by Celia Cenishi, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Curriculum or "Play is the child's work." But what do ' ble of a certain level of abstract, syrrj] bolic thought (AJmy, Monighan,Scales &^ and Teaching at Teachers College, such phrases really mean? Research on Columbia University. Van Hoorn, 1984). children's play increasingly reflects ihe F S0m*^ 40 Young Children • Seplembcr 1991J Play s u p p o r t s s y m b o l i c development In its complex forms play is characterized by the use ol symbols to represent objects, ideas, and situations not present in the immediate time and place. Play also provides occasions lor children lo encounter the perspectives of others and to negotiate important new perspectives on objects, ideas, and leelings (Garvey, 1977; Schwartzman, 1978; Vygolsky, 1976). Research demonstrates relationships between play and the development ol literacy, problem solving, perspective taking, and creativity. Both Piaget (1962) and Vygotsky (1976) identify play as central to the development of a child's ability to use symbols. This characteristic of early Research on children who are popular with peers indicates that children whose thinking is flexible frequently come up with unique alternatives for=4^solving disputes and suggesting compromises. childhood play is the one most thoroughly studied by child development researchers (Rubin, Fein, & Vandenberg, 1983). Teachers see children using symbols as they build a castle with blocks, as well as when they transform roles and situations in their dramatic play, e.g., when Lisa uses a box as a cash register for her store. teachers should include both high-realism and low-realism toys for pretend Play in classrooms in order lo accommodate a full ran^e of developmental fl -stages. ^ loii/i" Children • Si-pli-mlm U*:l) Infant caregivers, parents, and researchers have discovered that beginning at about the age of 18 months the human mind spontaneously engages in symbolic thought, as evidenced by the use of language and pretend play (Fein, 1981; Fenson & Ramsay, 1980; McCune, 1985). These symbolic activities rely on children's abilities to create meaning in their minds and to express that meaning through gesture (driving a pretend car), language and intonation ("OK, honey, it's bedtime," said to a •doll), and objects (sand and rocks used to make a birthday cake.) This ability to .translorm objects or situations through the use ol imagination into meanings that are different Irom the original object or situation lorms the foundation for intellectual development and communication.The use of symbols is not characteristic of all play, but symbolic behavior • does form the basis of many of the intellectual concepts related lo play and is characteristic of the constructive and pretend play of preschool and primarygrade children. Much ol the research on the play of young children has sought to documenl a sequence ol progressive symbolic distancing. As children mature, they are able to use objects that are increasingly discrepant in form and/or function from the objects the child wishes to symbolize (Fein, 1981; Fenson & Ramsay, 1980; McCune, 1985; Sigel. 1987). For example, Pederson, Rock-Green, and Elder (19S.J) report a series ol studies thai demonstrate thai 2- and 3-year-olds have dilliculty using objects that do not resemble the object lo be symbolized, such as using a block lor a hamburger. Researchers in classroom settings h3ve documenied younger children's preference lor replica objects and have indicated that ihis changes lo a preference lor unstructured objects, such as blocks or slicks, as children's capacities lor men- y^^fck /re. .V V V .jfp"»s lal represenlalion mntiiri- (I'ul.iski. 1!)7(); Fein, 1981). The implication kir Icwhcrs is thai younger children may need toy replicasof objects, such as kilchenware or vehides. As children develop, they gradually move toward using objects thai closely resemble their make-believe ideas, such as using a cup lor a phone. Older preschoolers and primary-grade children are likely to preler unstructured props such as blocks, cardboard packing, or marbles, which they may appropriate for many dillerent uses in their make-believe play (Curry & Arnaud, 1984). McLoyd (19S6) recommends thai teachers provide both hiyh-realism and low-realism toys lor prelend play in classrooms in order to .iccommodaie the lull range ol developmental slages. Research on children's role playing also indicates lhat as children's representational concepts develop, they are increasingly able to create make-believe roles and situations without the use ol costumes or props, using more subtle behaviors, such as gesture, language, and intonation, lo mark their transformations into prelend roles (Black, 1989; Matthews, 1977; Wall, Pickert, & Gibson, 1989). Researchers have also studied chil- d r e n ' s creaiion ol -imaginary companions (Manoseviiz, Fling, & Preniice, 1977; Partington &. Grant. 1984). They lound lhat approximately 30% of children between the ages ol three and six create imaginary friends lor themselves, using symbolic concepts to transform objects, roles, and situations into a whotfy^rmaginary or mental realm. Partington and Grant suggest thai imaginary . playmates help children to bridge their solitary fantasy play to socially oriented play by providing an imaginary peer (or peers) whom they can control. As such, play with imaginary companions represents a tool for both symbolic expression and mastery ol social skills. S y m b o l i c p l a y r e l a t e s to the d e v e l o p m e n t of literacy Infant caregivers, parents, and researchers have discovered lhat beginning at about the age of 18 months, the human mind spontaneously engages in symbolic thought, au'denced by the use of language and prelend play. Parents and educators are rightfully concerned about language development and foundations for literacy. Recent research on the development of symbolic thought has linked pretend play to language development (Ervin-Tripp, in press; Fenson, 1984; Genishi & Dyson, • 1984). O t h e r research locuses on school-related literacy and play (Isenberg & Jacob, 19S3; Wolfgang & Sanders, 1981). Children's schooling with regard .to the written symbols ol language and mathematics calls on the ability to per(orm symbolic transformalions. For example, the teacher in the introductory anecdote could explain to Andrew's parents that his ability to understand thai H and K, bat and 14 are combinations of lines that represent sounds, words, and numbers rests in part on his capacity to use a block to represent a truck or a telephone. Likewise, children's capacity to enter the "as if or hypothetical realm of books in which animals talk, such as that created by E. B. White in Charlotte's Web, or children's ability lo create such hypothetical frames ol relerence themselves when telling or writing stories rests on concepts constructed in dramatic play (Bruner. 1986; Roskos. 19S8; Sachs. Goldman. & Charlie, I9S4; WilT liamson & Silvern, J990). Their ability to use explicit language to negotiate multiple roles and to describe hypothetical situations, such as lhal ol the robot design team, housekeeping play, or episodes ol superhero adventures, draws on the same capacities in symbolic " thought as those needed to write a poem Young Children • Si-plciiilier l!WI z-^BX or a slory (Pellegrini, DeSlclano, &. Thompson, 1983). Heath (1983) lound thai children who entered school as sophisticated players enacted a wide range ol themes and events. These children were Irequently successlul in literacy experiences at school. Pellegrini (1980) lound significant relationships between the complexity ol symbolic play and achievement in r e a d i n g and writing lor kindergartners and first graders. ;; Play may contribute to this ability by allowing children to play through their ideas, much the way that adults talk through alternatives to problems they face. Olhers (lsenberg & Jacob, 1985; Jacob. 19S4; Schrader, 1989; Vukelich, 1990) have studied literacy in play, such as when the robot design team prepared the adverlising copy or when children consult a pretend menu or grocery list, write a check, or read a book lo a doll. Others have discussed play in literacy. underscoring the importance ql playful thought processes in literacy activities (Dyson, 1990, 1991; Daiule, 1989). S y m b o l i c p l a y r e l a t e s to thinking and p r o b l e m solving Play is also thought to be relaled lo Ihe construction of logical-maihematical knowledge. Play represents a predominance ol assimilative thinking in which concepts thai children bring lo a situation lake precedence over the need lo imitate or accommodate to a model presented by an adull. Play brings children a wide array pi opportunities to develop categorization concepts, such as selective alteniion lo the relevant Cues of objects, and also allows them lo I'nnsiruct categories at Iheir own pace. I'uiiiiii l'lnlilr.-n • Si-pk-mbcr I'.MH For the past iwu weeks Marie has been working almost daily with a set ol thick crayons, consisting ol eight colors. Today something is new. She chooses a large box ol thin crayons, containing a total ol 40 colors. She picks out all Ihe crayons that have a red color and arranges them separately from crayons ol orange and pink shades. As she colors a piece ol scrap wood with multiple shades ol red.she comments, "This is lor my mom." Lily sils down next lo her. Marie turns, ollering a crayon. "Do you want a redder one?" (Van Hoorn, Nourol, Scales, & Alward, manuscript submilted lor publication). Another quality thought lu be related lo children's play and creativity is the development of divergent thinking, or the ability to enterlain alternative possibilities in a given situation (Christie, 1983; Pepler & Ross, 1981). The flexibility in thinking thai allows one lo solve a probleir^ftgm a fresh perspective or use a tool in a unique way is a component ol effective problem solving (Adams, 1976). Play may contribute to this ability by allowing children to play through their ideas, much the way that adults talk through alternatives to problems they Because the source ol development is face and imagine consequences from from within the child, these categories varying perspectives. This process also become part of the child's mental netleads to the discovery ol new problems works. In this way, as Marie demonor new questions to be asked as children strates, children integrate new experibecome familiar with materials, play, ences into structures ol thinking they and think more deeply about their expehave already developed. Some researchrience (Pepler, 1986). ers have found that more mature play in preschoolers is positively related to soSeveral researchers have studied relaphisticated classification skills (Johntionships among children's play experison. Ersriler, & Lawton, 1982; Rubin & ences and their perlorrnance ol tasks ol Maioni, 1975). divergent thinking and creativity (Dansky, 1980a, b; Johnson, 1976; Pepler & S y m b o l i c p l a y r e l a t e s to Ross, 1981). These tasks included probcreativity a n d i m a g i n a t i o n lems that required children lo think ol a variety of alternative uses for an object Qualities that are sometimes taken for or to list objects that lit a given criterion, granted when reviewing the value of play such as naming all red things. Children for future development are creativity who were involved in play experience and imagination. Much'has been written groups before the tasks were given conconcerning the nature of curriculum ap-. sistently perlormed better than children propriate to prepare children for the who engaged in nonplay activities prior uncertain future ol the 21st century, and ' to the task, although, as Smith, Dagleish, more speculation will surely proliferate and Herzmark (1981) point out, it is not in the next decade. One avenue lo clear whether such gains are the resull achieve this educalional goal is to foster ol the play experiences perl se or the adaptive, flexible, and creative thinking. adults' intervention. ' Singer (1973) and Singer and Singer (1985) have written extensively about Some researchers suggest that structhe contribution ol play to the imaginatured materials may limit playful and tive thinking of children. In the Singers' novel activity (Halliday & McNaughton, view, make-believe play is essential, to 1982; Pepler & Ross, 1981). Teachers the development of the capacity lor inmust be cautious about the cues they give ternal imagery and contributes lo the children about materials when .considerdevelopment ol creativity by opening ing the ways that the children arrange or children to experiences lhat stimulate demonstrate the use ol materials. Play curiosity and Ihe exploration ol alterna- • contexts, loo, affect the relationship ol live situations arid combinations. For play lo creativity. Johnson (1976) and example, the children on the robot team Grilling (1980) lound thai prelend play tried out several methods lor adding with peers facilitated divergent probflexibility lo the robot's arms and delem-solving concepts. In a study ol the cided as a group which script lo tape lor use of slory play with 3-, 4-. and 5-year its speech. The Singers' research also old children, one ol the authors and her emphasizes the psychosocial benefits of colleagues lound thai enactment of dicimaginative play: Children who engage tated stories with peers led to creative in much make-believe are likely lo be responses lor the staging of events, the happier and more flexible when they creation ol slage props, and Ihe repreencounter new situations. sentation ol characters, as groups ol •1.) z*^^. f " ' ' children talked through the dilemmas ol converting dictated stories lo stage productions (Nourot, Henry, & Scales, 1990). Finally, individual dillerences may affect children's performances. Dansky (1980b) lound that those chil' dren who were already pretend players benefited the most from play experiences that called for divergent thinking. Divergent thinking has social applications as well. Research on children who are popular with peers indicates that children who are flexible in their thinking frequently come up with unique alternatives for resolving disputes and suggesting compromises (Hazen & Black, 1984; Rogers & Ross. 1986; Trawick-Smith, 1988). This ability to imagine alternatives has been studied in the domains of perspective taking as well. In Piaget's theory (1962), one important marker of the move Irorn the sensorimotor intelligence of infancy to the more logical operational intelligence ol middle childhood is the ability to decenter Irom one's own perspective and imagine from the physical, cognitive, and emotional viewpoints of others. Studying this aspect of problem solving in classroom settings, some researchers (Cole &. LaVoie, 1985;Connolly & Doyle, 1984; Rubin & Maioni, 1975) lound that children's levels of dramatic play correlated with perspective-taking abilities. Dramatic role playing is thought to contribute to the development ol this ability as children take on imaginary roles and play out make-believe themes consistent with those roles (Johnson, 1990). Playing a waiter in a restaurant. Jerry asks his customer, "Do you want French fries with your hamburger," and then translates the order into Vietnamese lor Bich, the cook. Children also formulate and test hypotheses concerning the nature ol the social world as they play roles, and they communicate implicit rules about those roles and the use ol objects to their peers (Corsaro, 1985; Elgas, Klein, Kantor. &. Fernie, 198S). When one child's version of a role conflicts with that ol another child, the opportunities lo revise and negotiate hypotheses are ample. Four-year-old Mara and three-year-old Juan are playing with a hospital bed. medical props, and two dolls. They agree to have their patients share Lhe toy bed.bul there is only one pillow. Mara lakes a blanket and lolds it several limes, placing il under the head ol tier doll. "Now we both have pillows," she concludes, and the play continues, uninterrupted by disputes (Van Hoorn, Nourot, Scales. & Alward, manuscript s u b m i t t e d lor publication). Researchers studying children's play with other children have speculated that the conflicts and negotiations occurring as children shilt Irom "In play" to "out of play" action lead children to consider the perspectives of others because they want the play to continue (Corsaro, 1985; Doyle &. Connolly, 1989; Garvey, 1977; Reilel & Yeatman, in press). The teacher's role is critical here. Genishi & DiPaolo (1982) report that the teacher's presence and availability influenced the course of preschoolers' arguments. Pel. legrini (1984) reports that less complex •pretend play occurs in the presence of an adult, indicating that adult proximity may discourage children from negotiating their own solutions lo conflict. Sutton-Smi th (1986) and Eisner (1990) remind us that creativity and imagination are more than cognitive constructs such as divergent thinking in problem solving. Imagination is present in a multitude ol contexts of human experience, ranging Irom the objective and verifiable to the subjective and absurd. As teachers,'we must not lose sight of the joyfulness and incongruity that often accompany children's imaginative play, and ol the variety of backgrounds and experiences that children bring to school that influence their play. How can teachers understand and respect the differences they see.in children's play? In linking play lo developmental^ appropriate practice, teachers' understanding ol the role of play in the normal development ol symbolic thinking, problem solving, and creativity is important. Play also provides an understanding of the range ol individual and cultural dillerences in classrooms, another aspect of developmentally appropriate practice. From a psychological perspective, researchers lor Project Zero at Harvard University have studied children over lime as they develop their personal repertoires ol symbolic expression. This is seen in pretend play and related activities such as drawing, music, and language (Woll & Gardner. 1979). The re- searchers describe stylistic dillerences between "pallerners" and "dramatists" in dramatic play, art, and storytelling. Pattemers are concerned with lhe properties and spatial arrangements ol objects, while dramatists are more interested in interpersonal relationships and naxrajjves ol events. Social class and cultural differences in children's play Recent research is just beginning to carefully document the elfects of social class and cultural differences on children's play (Corsaro & Schwarz, in press; Curry, 1971; Genishi & Galvan, 1985; Health, 1983, 1985; Jacob, 1984; . McLo.yd, Ray, & Etter-Lewis, 1985; Schwartzman, 1978; Sutton-Smith & Heath, 1981), although much work was done in the 1970s that laid the foundation for efforts today. Interpretation of these differences has made this area of research particularly controversial. Writing from an anthropological perspective, Schwartzman (1978,1984) suggests that the "deficits" some researchers have described in both culturally and economically diverse populations may really be "differ-^ ences," misinterpreted by researchers who are bound to preconceived ideas • about what play ought to be. Similarly, other writers (Eilerman, 1971; McLoyd, 1982) have pointed out that children who differ culturally or economically Irom the predominantly middle-class researchers .may exhibit imaginative play in ways unfamiliar to the researchers or on a "time table" different from that proposed by Piaget. What have researchers found? Much of the research in the. field was stimulated by Smilansky's finding thai the play of Israeli middle-class children was more complex than that ol the play of Israeli children Irom low socioeconomic levels (Smilansky, 1968). Subsequently, several researchers lound significant differences between the play ol children Irom high and low socioeconomic levels in the United States, Canada, and England (Grilling, 1980; Fein & Stork, 1981; Rubin, Maioni, & Hornung, 1976; Tizard, Philps, & Plewis, 1976a,b), while another did not (Golomb, 1979). The premise behind much ol this research is that children ol poverty have less stimulation and perhaps less parental acceptance ol the value ol play in learning (Tizard el Vnnnn Children • September IHiH In its complex forms, play is characterized by the use of symbols to represent objects, ideas, and situations not present in the immediate time and place. Symbolic play relates to thtitdeoelopment of literacy. al., 1976). This parental attitude is reflected in the amount of private space provided (or play, parental modeling ol make-believe activities, and eflorts ol parents lo help children make sense ol new experiences (Udwin & Shmukler, 1981). McLoyd (19S2) calls lor more carelul consideration ol lhe dillerences between home and school environments, teacher-child interactions, and attention 10 nonverbal aspects ol pretend play. Sutton-Smith & Heath (1981) describe two major styles of imaginative expression seen in play and language thai may illuminate cultural and social class dillerences in play. In the oral style, children's expressions are related to real life events, are coordinated with responses ol others in the group, and may locus on play with sounds and metaphors ol language. In the literate style, children express themselves in ways that more closely resemble most school literacy tasks by focusing on experiences lhat are distant Irom real lile and on individual performances outside the conicxi ol the group. Another variable lhat warrants carelul attention when 'observing lhe play of children Irom diverse backgrounds is setting (Grilling. 19SU). Children who are capable ol engaging in complex play may not do so in certain sellings.such •is Ymiiiu Cluiih.-n • Si-|jii'm!i--i I'.l'.M the traditional classroom. Tizard and colleagues (1976a,b) report that children in England Irom low socioeconomic backgrounds exhibit more complex levels of play outdoors ihan indoors. McLoyd (1982) and Pellegrini (1984) note that the presence ol an adult may make some children war)' of playing in lully expressive ways, while others report adult presence as stimulating to the language and play ol working-class children (Miller & Gan-ey, 19S4; Smilansky, 196S;Sylva, 1984). An implication ol this research is that when teachers assess children's symbolic play as lacking in complexity, they should reassess the environment. Curry (1971) tells of Navajo children who did noi play in the housekeeping corner until the furniture was moved next to the walls lor cleaning one day, and the room thus resembled their familiar context ol a round hogan. T h e play of boys a n d girls differs Generally, research indicates lhat "girl toys." "boy toys," and each sex's themes and topics for play have noi changed much in the last 50 years. Boys are still more likely lo engage in roughand-tumble play, aggressive themes, and play witii vehicles and building materials (DiPielro. 19K1; Fagot & Patterson. 1969). Girls are more likely lo engage in sedentary indoor play, more domestic roles and themes, and goal-orienled construction or craft projects (Johnson & Ershler, 1981; Wall el al., 1989). Carpenter & Huston-Stein (1980) speculate that girls' preferences lor some of these activities^rnay reflect their desire to stay close to the teacher rather than their preference for the activity per se. Since teachers reportedly give more attention lo boys in the classroom (Liss, 1986), perhaps girls' preference lor teacher-led activities represents bids lor attention. Halliday & McNaughton (1982) suggest that the structured nature ol teacherguided tasks may, in lact, limit girls' opportunities to use materials in novel ways and to negotiate conflicts without reliance on adults. Some studies indicate that contemporary children may be more flexible in their gender-stereotyped play, perhaps due to the influence of the feminist m o v e m e n t ( H a l l i d a y & McNaughton, 1972). The majority of studies agree that cross-gender toys and activities are more commonly observed in girls' play than in boys' (Fagot & Leinback, 1983; Liss, 1986), and that stereotypic play increases with age, perhaps because ol the inlluence ol parents or peers (Serbin, Conner, Burchardt, & Citron. 1979; Wall et al., 1989). There may also be more than meets the eye going on in gender-stereotyped play. Abraham and Lieberman (19S5) compared the play of nine girls in situations using baby dolls and Barbie dolls. They lound that the Barbie-doll play suggested more themes than the babydoll play, which was limited in complexity and often degenerated into aggressive play. They speculate lhat playing roles through the teenage Barbie rather than as the baby's mother offers a wider' range of imaginative possibilities in doll play. In research in her own preschool classroom, Monighan-Nourot reported lhat girls olten used Barbies as "laser" weapons and boys nurtured iheir "action figures" (Monighan-Nourot, Scales. Van Hoorn, & Almy, 1987). Just the possession ol a toy considered to be stereotyped does not mean it will be used in stereotypical ways. In fact, Carlson-Paige and Levin (1987) recommend lhat teachers guide children to imagine alternative scenarios to violent or television-based play with their loys. 45 ^ 1 f^- In a unique study ol the play ol a sevenyear-old girl, Kelly-Byrne (1989) calls lor both teachers and researchers to consider how girls and boys are differently socialized and how those experiences affect their play. Black (1989), echoing . this poinL lound that girls were more interested in sustaining interaction in play, while boys were more inlerested in having their ideas accepted by their peers. Paley (1984) cautions teachers not to intervene with loo much vigor in the play of boys and girls and to allow them to find their own way to their understandings of gender. Rough-and-tumble play is a Irequent accompaniment to pretend play and has recently become a topic (or researchers. DiPietro (1981), Pellegrini &. Perimutter (1988), and Humphreys & Smith (1987) maintain that this type of play is a positive socializing experience lor young children, particularly boys, in our society, and is often misinterpreted by teachers as violent play. Rough-andtumble play differs sharply Irom overt aggression. In rough-and-tumble play, children laugh as they tumble in their play and are able to separate Irom one another after the play, unlike real fighting. Rough-and-tumble play is more likely to occur in soft spaces, such as the rug area indoors or the grass outdoors, whereas overt aggression occurs primarily during property disputes. The play fighting of rough-and-tumble play occurs, open-handed rather than with closed fists, and children alternate the roles of aggressor and victim. Negotiating these role exchanges exercises children's perspective-taking abilities (Corsaro. 1985). In understanding individual differences, social class and cultural variations, and gender differences in play, the teacher's keen observational skills are essential. In this way teachers see b e n e a t h the surface of behavioral stereotypes and appreciate the dynamics ol development lor each child. How can teachers support play in preschool and primary classrooms? The teacher's role in setting the stage lor play is highly active and mulli(aceled. Everything the teacher does is an intervention. We can think ol interventions on a conlinuum Irom more in- direct to more direct. For example, preparing the environment and scheduling the day lall at the more indirect end of the continuum, while intervening as a matchmaker, peacekeeper, or coach are more direct interventions (Van.Hoorn, Nourot, Scales, & Alward, manuscript submitted lor publication). Warner (1963) and Wassermann (1990). Children need adequate time lo move Irom the exploration ol objects into play with them and lo construct, elaborate, and refine the products of their imaginations (Reilel, 1984; Reifel & Yeatman, 1991). This is true with play media as diverse as dramatic play, block play, and Teachers can support play in preschool and primary classrooms by preparing the environment and scheduling blocks of time for play, intervening as matchmaker, peacekeeper, or coach. S e t t i n g the s t a g e f o r p l a y Teachers assume multiple roles as they intervene during p-lay. As "artist assistant" (Griifin, 1982), the teacher helps to remove clutter around an ongoing play episode so that the children can maintain their thematic focus. As "peacemaker," the teacher helps children resolve disputes, e.g., by suggesting alternative roles or materials or by interpreting children's motives. As "guardian ol the gale," a sensitive teacher can help a child gain access to play without violating the rights of the players in an ongoing episode. As "matchmaker," the teacher deliberately helps particular children play with one another. In the role ol "parallel player," the teacher uses similar material and plays next to the child, thereby suggesting new variations. As "spectator," the teacher helps children extend their play by commenting on it Irom "outside the play theme," e.g., "Are you using the cash register today?" Teachers can also act as play "participants" by taking active roles in children's play ("Yes, I'm so hungry, I would love a tacol"). As "play tutor" or "coach," teachers' interventions are more direct. In this role, the teacher models and directs the child's play and reinforces the child's elforts lo symbolize and interact. Because this strategy represents a very direct role lor the adult, we believe th3t it must be used sparingly and with great care. Scheduling the day so that children have a lengthy free choice or "breathing out" lime is recommended by Ashlon- play with computers. Tizard and colleagues (1976a,b) caution that too many choices of play activities may distract children, a caution echoed by MonighanNourot and colleagues (1987), who call lor a careful balance of the novel and the familiar in play accessories and activity choices. Distraction may be reduced, however, by provision of well-bounded play areas with clear pathways to con-' nect them. Teachers who provide children with well-protected niches for their play better enable children to concentrate on and sustain their play (Corsaro, 1985;"Ramsey & Reid, 19S8). O r c h e s t r a t i n g c h i l d r e n ' s play Much of the research linking play to aspects ol intellectual and social competence is based on studies in which ' adults coach children in their play. As with studies linking play experiences to cognitive variables such as perspective taking or convergent and divergent . thinking, the role of adult attention versus the role of independent play is a topic ol debate. Christie (19S2), Grilling (1983), Smilansky (1968), Smilansky-& Shelarya (1990). and Woodard (1984) are among those who oiler guidelines for intervening in children's play that respect children's autonomy in their play and allow teachers to facilitate its development. Christie (1985) categorizes these coaching strategies into (1) modeling of make-believe objects and roles, (2) verbal guidance as either a spectator or a participant in children's spontaneous play, (3) thematic fantasy play ^1 tutoring in which teachers structure the enactment of lairy tales, and (4) imaginative play training in which adults engage children in activities designed to enhance their make-believe imagery and expression (See De Mille, 1967; Singer & Singer, 1985 lor specific activities). The above educators concur with Kleiber and Barhelt (1980) that intervening in the least obtrusive manner possible and phasing out ol children's play are guidelines lor using any of the playcoaching strategies. In addition, some research indicates that teachers' intervention in play may reduce gender-stereotyped play. Early research by Fagot and Patterson (1969) suggested that preschool t e a c h e r s tended to create environments more conducive to the quiet and constructive play ol girls, and that these teachers' attitudes and the environment contrasted sharply with the needs ol boys' play. Bianchi and Bakeman (1983) report, that continued teacher encouragement of cross-gender friendships, efforts to diminish stereotyping through stories and modeling, and inclusion of both genders in all school activities can change these patterns of behavior over time. S t r a t e g i e s for a s s e s s i n g p l a y One of the ways that research influences classroom practice is through the establishment ol developmental norms. To return to the point made at the beginning ol this article, play is an essential feature ol developmental^ appropriate curriculum and assessment. Carelul observation ol children's play supports teachers' eflorts to understand both age-related and individual differences in children's development. Research oilers .tools that teachers may find valuable lor keeping track of the development of play in their classrooms and planning lor its enhancement. The most popular scheme lor tracking the development ol play is the system designed by Nmilansky (1968). Drawing on the work tl Piagel (1962), Smilansky traces the omplexiry ol play Irom the early luncional or practice play in infancy through he constructive and dramatic play of ymbol-making in early childhood and mally lo the games with rules which are liaracleristic ol middle childhood. In recent years, several researchers ave combined Smilansky's system with ial ol Parten (1932). Parlen traced the •iimi; Children • September 1991 development ol social play Irom onlooking behavior lo solitary, parallel, and associative play,and finally to cooperative play at its most complex level. Through their work, researchers using Parlen's system combined with Smilansky's have illuminated the role of solitary play and the role of constructive play in development (Rubin, Maioni, & Hornung, 1976; Rubin, 1982). Even more recently, there has been a shilt toward emphasizing teachers' interpretations of play to document and support development and learning (Griilin, 1982; Paley, 1984). In play, teachers are able to see age. appropriate concepts emerge and may use play to assess and support the development of individual children. For example, the second grade teacher who observes the robot design team over several play periods notes that JoAnna has become the electrical engineer who invents the system for lighting the robot's eyes. Brian has taken on the role ol composing on the computer the promotional literature describing the features of the "X-100" model versus the "X-500" model: The "100" model serves food, and the "500" model cleans your house. Anecdotal observations ol these children's negotiations with their peers, photographs of the robot under construction, and copies of the promotional fliers go into the portfolios of the children who created the project. Conclusion Play is complex in its processes, antecedents, and consequences in the lives of children. Researchers continue to struggle lo define play and to measure it (Matthews & Matthews. 1982; Rubin, Fein. & Vandenberg, 1983; Smith, Takhvar, Gore. & Vollstedt, 1986). Teachers and parents debate its role in the curriculum, and children continue to remind us that it is a compelling and essential pari of childhood. -In some interesting research, King (1987) has studied children's definitions of play and their understanding ol its relationship to work. The quality of play most olten mentioned by kindergartners is that it is something that children are Iree to choose lor themselves, rather than having adults make decisions lor them regardless o( how much they may enjoy the task. As children mature, their definition ol play becomes more psycho- logical in nature, emphasizing the factor of enjoyment. Teachers need to carelully consider this child's-eye view in planning for play in their classrooms—even an activity thai may be "fun" Irom the adult's perspective may not elicit a playful mode ol thinking from children if the activity is maqdaasd. In contrast, activities that may appear work-like to adults may be approached by children who are eager to try them from a freely chosen, playful stance. . Even with a growing body of research on play to bolster the conviction that play is essential to the learning of young children, the precise nature of play experiences in learning and development remains a provocative mystery (Fein, 1985). Perhaps it is the mystery itself that intrigues both the researcher and the teacher, lor play appears to exemplify the multilaceted nature of human experience, thought, and feeling. References Abraham, K_, & Lieberman, L (1985). Should Barbie go lo preschool? Young Children, 40 (2) 12-15. Adams, J. (197.6). Conceptual blockbusting: A pleasurable guide to bener problem solving. New York: W. W. Norton. Almy, M., Monighan, P., Scales, B., & Van Hoorn, J. (1964). Recent research on play: The perspective ol lhe teacher. In L. Katz (Ed.). Current topics in early childhood education. Vol. 5, (pp. 1-25). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Ashton-Warner. 5. (1963). Teacher. New York: Simon & Schuster. , Bianchi, B., & Bakeman, R. (19S3). Patterns ol sex typing in a/i open school. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles an d children i play (pp. 219-233 ). New York: Academic Press. Black, B. (1989). Interactive pretense: Social and symbolic skills in preschool play groups. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 35(A), 379-397. Bredekamp, S. (1987). Deuelopmentolly appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving young children from birth through age 8 Washington. DC: National Association lor the Education ol Young Children. Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge. MA: Harvard Universiry Press. Carlson-Paige. N.. & Levin, D. (1987). The war ploy dilemma. New York: Teachers College Press. Carpenter, C.J.. & Huslon-Siein. A. (1980). Activity structure and sex typed behavior in preschool children. Child Development, 51. B62-S72. Christie, J. F. (19S2).Sociodramalic play training. Young Children. 37(4). 25-32.