Literacy in America
Transcription
Literacy in America
Cover-image not available Literacy in America Literacy in America An Encyclopedia of History, Theory, and Practice Barbara J. Guzzetti, Editor Donna E. Alvermann and Jerry L. Johns, Editorial Advisors Santa Barbara, California Denver, Colorado Oxford, England Copyright © 2002 by Barbara J. Guzzetti All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior permission in writing from the publishers. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Literacy in America : an encyclopedia of history, theory, and practice / Barbara J. Guzzetti, editor. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 1-57607-358-0 (set : hard : alk. paper) 1. Literacy—United States—Encyclopedias. 2. Reading—United States—Encyclopedias. I. Guzzetti, Barbara J. LC151 .L487 2002 302.2'244'0973—dc21 2002014350 06 05 04 03 02 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an e-book. Visit abc-clio.com for details. ABC-CLIO, Inc. 130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911 Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911 This book is printed on acid-free paper I. Manufactured in the United States of America Contents Preface and Acknowledgments, ix Further Acknowledgments, xi Introduction: The Landscape of Literacy in Seven Portraits, xiii List of Contributors, xxxi Literacy in America, 1 Classroom Writing Assessment, 66 Cloze Procedure, 72 College Literacy and Learning, 73 College Reading and Learning Association, 74 College Reading Association, 75 Comics, 76 Commercial Reading Programs, 77 Community Literacy, 80 Comparative Reading, 82 Comprehension Strategies, 85 Computer-Assisted Instruction, 88 Concept Instruction with Text, 90 Conceptual Change Learning and Texts, 93 Considerate Text, 97 Constructivism, 99 Content-Area Literacy, 101 Context in Literacy, 104 Cooperative Learning, 107 Critical Literacy, 108 Critical Media Literacy, 111 Critical Reading, 113 Criticisms of Reader Response, 115 Critique of the National Reading Panel Report, 118 Volume 1: A–M Ability Grouping, 1 Accountability and Testing, 5 Active Listening, 8 Activity Theory, 10 Adolescent Literacy, 13 Adolescent Literature, 15 Adult Literacy, 19 Adult Literacy Programs, 22 Adult Literacy Testing, 25 American Reading Forum, 29 Artists’ Books, 29 Assessment Interviews, 30 Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers, 31 At-Risk Students, 33 Authentic Assessment, 36 Automaticity and Reading Fluency, 40 Balanced Literacy Instruction, 43 Basal Readers, 45 Bibliotherapy, 48 Bilingual Education, 49 Bilingualism, 52 Biliteracy, 57 Book Clubs, 60 Deaf Students and Literacy, 121 Delayed Readers, 123 Developmental and College Reading, 127 Dialogic Responsiveness, 131 Dialogue Journals, 132 Center for the Expansion of Language and Thinking, 63 Children’s Literature, 64 v Contents Directed Reading Activity and Directed Reading-Thinking Activity, 133 Discourse Analysis, 135 Discursive Theory, 140 Discussion, 143 The Discussion Web, 146 Distance Learning, 147 Diversity, 150 Dynamic Assessment, 154 Dyslexia, 155 Inquiry-Based Instruction, 249 Instant Messaging, 251 Interdisciplinary Approaches to Literacy, 254 International Reading Association, 257 Intertextuality, 258 Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 259 Journal of Literacy Research, 260 Junior Great Books, 260 Kidwatching and Classroom Evaluation, 263 Kinesthetic Methods, 266 KWL and KWL+, 267 Early Literacy, 157 Early Literacy Assessment, 160 Early Literacy Software, 165 Ebonics, 167 Ecological Literacy, 168 Economics of Literacy Development, 170 Effective Schools and Teachers, 173 Elders and Literacy, 175 Electronic Jigsaw, 177 English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment, 178 English Journal, 181 The Even Start Family Literacy Program, 182 Eye Movements, 183 Language Acquisition, 273 Language Arts, 276 Language Arts Instruction, 277 Language Attitudes, 281 Language Experience Approach, 287 Laubach Literacy, 290 Learning Centers, 291 Learning with Texts, 293 Linguistic Approaches to Reading Instruction, 298 Listservs in Literacy, 299 Literacy and Culture, 303 Literacy Autobiography, 308 Literacy Definitions, 310 The Literacy Dictionary, 313 Literacy in Informal Settings, 315 Literacy in Play, 318 Literacy Labs, 323 Literacy Motivation, 326 Literacy Volunteers of America, 330 Literature Circles, 330 Literature-Based Instruction, 334 Family Literacy, 185 Feminist Post-Structuralism, 187 Flexibility, 191 Fluency, 191 Gender and Discussion, 195 Gender and Post-Typographical Text, 199 Gender and Reading, 201 Gender and Writing, 205 Graffiti, 208 Graphic Aids, 212 Graphic Organizers, 213 Group Reading Inventories, 215 Mainstreaming, 339 Media Literacy, 340 Mental Modeling, 344 Metacognition, 345 Middle-School Literacy, 348 Minimum-Competency Testing, 351 Miscue Analysis, 352 Models of the Reading Process, 356 Multicultural Literacy, 364 Multicultural Literature, 368 Multimedia, 374 Multiple Literacies, 376 Multiple Texts, 380 The Handbook of Reading Research, 217 The Head Start Program, 218 Heritage-Language Development, 219 High-Stakes Assessment, 223 History of Reading Instruction, 224 History of the Book, 231 Hypertext, 233 Independent Reading, 239 Individualized Reading, 244 Informal Reading Inventory, 247 vi Contents Reading Clinics, 502 Reading-Comprehension Instruction, 506 Reading-Comprehension Processes, 508 Reading Diagnosis, 515 Reading Excellence Act, 520 Reading Hall of Fame, 521 Reading-Interest Inventories, 522 Reading Is Fundamental, 523 Reading Online, 525 Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 526 Reading Readiness, 526 Reading Recovery, 527 Reading Research and Instruction, 530 Reading Research Quarterly, 530 Reading Specialists, 531 The Reading Teacher, 533 Reading Today, 534 Reading-Writing Relationships, 534 Reciprocal Teaching, 535 Recreational Reading, 539 Redundancy, 541 Refutational Texts, 542 Remediation, 545 Repeated Readings, 548 Research in the Teaching of English, 551 Resistant Reading, 552 Round-Robin Oral Reading, 553 Volume 2: N–Z Narrative and Expository Text, 385 Narrative Text, 388 National Assessment of Educational Progress, 392 National Commission on Reading, 395 National Conference on Research in Language and Literacy, 397 National Council of Teachers of English, 398 National Institute for Literacy, 399 National Reading Conference, 400 National Reading Conference Yearbook, 400 National Reading Panel, 401 The National Right to Read Foundation, 404 Oral Language, 407 Oral Language Development, 411 Oral Reading, 415 Peer and Cross-Age Tutoring, 419 Peer Discussion, 420 Peer Status and Literacy Development, 425 Phonics Instruction, 428 Phonological and Phonemic Awareness, 431 Policy Issues in Testing, 434 The Political Nature of Literacy, 437 Popular Culture, 440 Portfolios, 443 Post-Structuralism and Structuralism, 445 Post-Typographic, 448 Predictable Books, 450 Prior Knowledge and Misconceptions, 453 Prior-Knowledge Assessment, 456 Process Writing, 459 Programmed Instruction, 461 Psycholinguistics, 462 Public Opinion and Literacy, 464 Scaffolded Literacy Instruction, 555 Schema Theory, 556 Schema Theory Criticisms, 558 Secondary-School Reading Programs, 562 Semantic Feature Analysis, 566 Semantic Mapping, 572 Semiotics, 580 Sight Words, 581 Silent Reading, 583 Social Constructivism, 584 Social Justice and Literacies, 589 Social Nature of Literacy, 595 Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, 599 Sociolinguistics and Literacy, 599 Software for Older Readers, 603 Speed Reading, 605 Spelling, 606 Standardized Test Score Decline, 610 Standardized Test Score Interpretation, 613 Standardized Testing, 615 Standards, 617 Story Grammar, 619 Questioning, 469 The RAND Reading Study Group, 475 Rauding Theory, 476 Readability, 480 Read-Alouds, 486 Reader Response, 488 Readers Theatre, 493 The Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 495 Reading Assessment, 495 Reading-Attitude Measures, 501 vii Contents Transmission Instruction, 663 Storytelling, 622 Structural Analysis, 626 Study Guides, 628 Study Skills and Strategies, 631 Subjectivity, 632 Visual Literacy, 665 Vocabulary Instruction, 667 Whole Language and Whole-Language Assessment, 673 Whole Language Umbrella, 677 Word Recognition, 678 Workplace Literacy, 682 Writing across the Curriculum, 686 Writing Assessment, 688 Writing Assessment in Large-Scale Contexts, 693 Teacher Education in Literacy, 639 Teacher Research in Literacy, 643 Television and Reading, 647 Test Preparation, 650 Textbooks, 651 Thematic Organizers, 653 Think-Alouds, 655 Title I, 656 Trade Books, 657 Transactional Theory, 661 Zines, 699 List of Acronyms, 701 Bibliography, 705 Index, 751 About the Editor, 779 viii Preface and Acknowledgments This two-volume encyclopedia provides a comprehensive overview of the study and teaching of literacy in the United States. It addresses the learning and practice of multiple literacies, including reading, writing, listening, and speaking, and such “new literacies” as popular culture, media, and technology. Intended to fill a gap in extant resources on literacy, this work presents the major topics and issues in the field of literacy in language that is as nontechnical as possible. As Alan Purves, late author of the Encyclopedia of English Studies and Language Arts (1994), noted, editing an encyclopedia is a daunting task. The task here was made easier, however, by my collaboration as editor with the two editorial advisers. Together, we generated the initial list of entries by perusing related works, current textbooks and academic texts in the field, and past professional conference programs. We also surveyed subscribers to the National Reading Conference listserv (NRCEMAIL), an electronic discussion group for literacy researchers and professionals, for suggestions on entries. In addition, authors and contributors consulted a web site with a master list of entries and then suggested other entries important to such a reference. The editor, editorial advisers, and contributors collaboratively determined the titles of the entries. We planned for entries of 500 to 4,000 words, with most consisting of about 2,000 words, for a total of approximately 400,000 words in the two volumes. The entries in these volumes represent a range of perspectives, or “frames,” on literacy that explore historical topics along with current trends. Entries represent both the breadth and the depth of varying theories of literacy. If a topic has been the subject of well-known controversy, then it is counterbalanced with an entry representing an opposing view. Each entry begins with a defini- tion of the construct and then elaborates on the issues and research surrounding the topic. In writing the entries, authors were cautioned to refrain from opinions or position statements as much as possible and were asked to document their sources. At the end of most entries, there are references designed to lead the reader to sources cited in the entry and to further reading. The number of citations and references was designed to be proportional to the length of the entry. Due to space constraints, fewer citations and references were permitted than most authors would have preferred. The entries represent five categories. The first is a general category of definitions, process, influences, issues, types, and theories of literacy. This category includes such topics as biliteracy, English as a second language (ESL), heritage-language development, and multicultural concerns. Four other categories include literacy assessment, literacy instruction, literacy resources and organizations, and literacy professional publications and reports. To save space, many general topics include specific terms not listed as entries. For example, the entry on reader-response theory contains explanations of the terms aesthetic and efferent reading. Although the entries can be grouped into five categories according to subject area, they are arranged in alphabetical order in the encyclopedia. Readers may wish to skim the list of entries and the index to find what they are seeking. In addition to the entry itself, most entries are crossreferenced by a “See Also” section at the conclusion. This section refers readers to related entries (along with references) that may be of interest. The entries in these volumes were written for a broad audience. Potential readers include academicians and students in universities and ix Preface and Acknowledgments colleges, literacy specialists, and teachers and school administrators, as well as parents, policymakers, and interested citizens. Hence, contributors were asked to write in terms intelligible both to professionals and to those with little or no prior knowledge of the subject and to define professional jargon whenever possible. ation to Jerry Johns, Distinguished Teaching Professor Emeritus of Northern Illinois University, my first mentor and adviser in literacy education. His guidance and friendship have sustained and enhanced the production of these volumes in both subtle and obvious ways. Third, I wish to thank the past and present staff at ABC-CLIO for their support and direction. It was a pleasure to work with and get to know Marie Ellen Larcada, the acquisitions editor who first interested me in taking on this extensive work and who has continued to provide feedback and support despite leaving her position. I also thank Kevin Downing, former senior acquisitions editor, Vince Burns, former developmental editor, and Melanie Stafford, senior production editor, for their direction and reminders that helped keep the project on schedule. Finally, I offer my gratitude to those who helped with behind-the-scenes tasks. I thank my research assistants, Septimia Filip and Margaret Gamboa, who created and maintained the project’s web site, kept clerical records of contracts and submissions, and contacted authors periodically via e-mail. I also appreciate the assistance of Donald Hutchins, media and technology director for the College of Education at Arizona State University, who worked extensively with me to transfer all entries to CDs. Theirs were no small tasks. My parting thoughts in completing these volumes are of the magnitude and magnificence of the knowledge base we have accumulated in the field of literacy. In these days of political attacks and critiques directed at education in the United States, literacy professionals, parents, and concerned citizens may become discouraged about the state of current affairs. These entries offer a more promising view of literacy in the United States by demonstrating how far we have come as a profession in our understanding of literacy processes, practices, and issues. It is my hope that these volumes will contribute to continual improvement of literacy learning and teaching. Barbara J. Guzzetti Acknowledgments First, as editor of these volumes, I was struck by the depth of knowledge of the contributors, including scholars well known for their research topic, graduate students developing expertise in a particular area, and teachers, consultants, and literacy specialists with working knowledge in their area. I thank these approximately 250 contributors for sharing their unique insights into the myriad aspects of literacy. Without their expertise and dedication to the task, this reference work would not have been possible. I also appreciate their support in various forms—nominations of other authors, illustrations and photographs to accompany their entries, e-mails of moral support, positive feedback and good cheer, and, surprisingly, gifts that related to the project or represented the authors’ geographical regions. In particular, I want to take this occasion to acknowledge my dear friend and colleague Ann Watts Pailliotet, a contributor to the encyclopedia who suddenly and unexpectedly passed away during the production of these volumes. She will be remembered not only for her contributions to the field of technology and literacy but also for her shining spirit. Second, I want to thank my editorial advisers, all well known in the field of literacy as prolific and influential researchers. My thanks go to Donna Alvermann, Distinguished Research Professor at the University of Georgia, a longtime colleague and friend who recommended me as editor for this project. Her advice and support has once again seen me through another longterm effort and has enriched the quality of the product of that effort. I also extend my appreci- x Further Acknowledgments one that focused primarily on reading practices and processes—to its current, more inclusive state, literacy education is in what many contributors to this encyclopedia would call a state of flux—a “becoming” rather than an “is” or a “has been.” It is this dynamic nature of literacy education that makes it such an exciting field in which to work. In compiling these two companion volumes, Barbara Guzzetti has attained what few before her can lay claim to having achieved. Through her knowledge of the field’s breadth and her ability to work with contributors who know its depth, she has assembled a remarkably comprehensive work. Also noteworthy is the fact that Literacy in America represents the thinking and writing of scholars new to the field as well as those who have seen it through several evolutions, if not revolutions. This blending of the socalled new and old is in keeping with the field’s history, present, and future. It is also in keeping with what I would expect that readers of these volumes would appreciate most—the opportunity to become more conversant in the issues and debates that surround literacy education, at least as interpreted by the contributors to this work. Donna E. Alvermann Barbara Guzzetti deserves heartfelt recognition from all those interested in literacy for undertaking such a daunting endeavor. Anyone who has worked with coauthors on a project will quickly understand the added complexities of collaboration. When the collaboration involves the large number of authors who contributed to these volumes, it should be quite easy to recognize the challenges and frustrations that Barbara faced. The completion of such a huge undertaking should give Barbara an immense sense of satisfaction for an outstanding contribution to the profession. I was honored to participate in the project because of its potential significance to the field. Our work is done. Users of the encyclopedia will ultimately determine its value as they read, reflect, and use this tool to enhance their understanding, gain perspective, and pursue the task of promoting higher levels of literacy. Jerry L. Johns President, International Reading Association, 2002–2003 The field of literacy education is expanding on a scale that is sometimes difficult to comprehend. From its inception as a narrowly studied area— xi Introduction: The Landscape of Literacy in Seven Portraits I Perhaps the most daunting challenge in producing this encyclopedia was planning and writing this Introduction. The developmental editor asked me for 10,000 words representing my thoughts on the field of literacy—its development, present status, and future directions. This Introduction was to be designed to be compatible with the mission of the encyclopedia by reflecting on historical aspects of literacy while emphasizing recent developments in the field. I found, however, that I could not bring myself to address only this request. My reasons for this rebellion of sorts stem from my beliefs about what constitutes quality in research. As a feminist qualitative researcher, I believe that it is necessary for researchers to be aware of and explicate their biases and experiences and to explain how those influence the conduct and production of their efforts. I celebrate the shift from an emphasis on cognitive research and experimental designs to ethnographic inquiries conducted from sociocultural frames. I am most interested in issues of social concern— topics, frameworks, and methods that address issues of social justice. My own recent work centers on the new literacies or multiple literacies practiced by those underrepresented in literacy research—adolescents of upper or lower social classes and those marginalized by their own subjectivities, such as their gender or social class. During the production of these volumes, I attempted to keep my own biases and views like these balanced with entries that represent frameworks and positions that I do not share. As a literacy researcher, however, I realize that I bring to this work my personal theoretical frames, my epistemologies and ideologies, as well my own ideas about what constitutes important work— pedagogy and inquiry—in literacy. Therefore, to be the sole author in sketching a retrospective and perspective on the field of literacy would mean presenting only my ethnocentric views. Because I strove for representation of both historical and recent trends in the volumes’ entries, I also chose to obtain a broad representation in this essay, introducing and providing perspectives on those entries. Therefore, in collaboration with my editorial advisers, I invited seven scholars whose work represents diversity in perspective to join me in the Introduction. I charged these researchers with reflecting on historical and recent developments in the field of literacy from their own six frameworks and to identify how the entries in the encyclopedia represented those developments. By doing so, I anticipated giving readers a glimpse into the myriad ways in which multiple perspectives have been and are represented in shaping the field. The first of these scholars, Suzanne Wade, presents an instructional perspective on literacy from a framework of teacher education and scholarship. Her piece reminds us of changes in literacy at the classroom level and of the political nature of literacy instruction. Her perspective is particularly timely as literacy professionals begin to reflect on and address national reports on epistemology and pedagogy in literacy. A second perspective by Yolanda Majors presents a personal and community-based consideration of literacy. Writing from personal memories and reflective insights, she reminds us about the ways literacy learners are influenced by ritualized events outside the classroom. Her stories show how social and cultural expectations can influence literacy development and practice that may be at conflict with or estranged from literacy expectations inside the classroom. xiii Introduction The third view by Allan Neilsen brings an international perspective to literacy. Although this encyclopedia focuses on literacy in the United States, many of its contributors are scholars and practitioners from other nations who face the same issues and research the same topics. Neilsen’s thoughts particularly challenge expanding notions of the literacy landscape in a global sense. His words direct us to refocus constructs of literacy so that literacy implies more than simply substituting the word literacy for the word education. Lorri Neilsen expands on this global theme with a fourth view—a feminist perspective that examines the influences of world events on literacy development and practice. In her essay, Neilsen examines power and access issues that can determine the literate behaviors and skills learners acquire. Her words direct us to those entries that examine underlying principles and consequences of literacy assessment and instruction. A fifth frame on literacy is offered by Michele Knobel and Colin Lankshear. These researchers trace the field historically and provide a sociocultural perspective on literacy theory and instructional practice. Their words remind us of the diverse and sometimes conflicting directions in literacy. Their ideas question the unity of the rubric of a “field” of literacy and cause us to reexamine our ideas about shifting the centrality of literacy development and practice from the classroom to the outside world. Finally, Patricia Alexander presents a view of literacy as a landscape of changes. Her focus traces development of notions of literacy from a cognitive perspective with reading at the core of literacy to a social perspective that encompasses new terrain. Her view identifies the silences in the encyclopedia that represent uncharted territory for future work in literacy. When these views are taken together, it appears that literacy, with its myriad issues and directions, is more of a landscape than a field. It is also a landscape that is ever changing and becoming increasingly more inclusive. Like the world, literacy really is many landscapes or literacies, touched and reformed like the earth itself by the times in which we live. The contributors to these volumes have not only taken us on a journey through these landscapes, they have also assisted in identifying how the territory of literacy may expand and evolve in the future. Together, we invite our readers to join us in extending this journey by perusing the geography represented in the entries of these volumes. We anticipate that your travels will be both engaging and enlightening. Barbara J. Guzzetti II The challenge of discovery lies not in seeking new landscapes but having new eyes. —Marcel Proust When a first edition of a compendium such as the Literacy in America comes out, we might wonder what it tells us about the current state of literacy—in theory, research, practice, and policy. Looking back in time, we might then wonder, how have things changed? And have they really changed? How was literacy defined then—and now? Are the goals, assumptions, and accepted methods of literacy research different, and what paradigms influence how we think about literacy and research? What do we know today about students, both inside and outside of classrooms, that we did not know before? What makes a difference in student success, and who has access to the resources and benefits of literacy? What are accepted practices for literacy instruction, and how have they changed? How have methods and uses of assessment changed? Was literacy as politicized then as it is now? What is the role today of technology? What should it be? How have advances in technology changed what it means to be literate in schools and society? These questions are important because the ways in which they are answered have profound effects on teaching and learning in classrooms. First, these questions and their answers delimit the topics that get attention, the concepts that mark our discourse about literacy and education, and the assumptions that underlie what literacy researchers and educators do. Second, they influence the ways we view students, which literacies in students’ lives count, how we define achievement and success, and how we frame problems in literacy learning (in other words, where blame is placed when students do not succeed). Third, they influence our views of what texts are legitimate, and therefore sanctioned, and our views of what constitutes learning and how it can and should be assessed. xiv Introduction Of course, change has different meanings for different people. A perusal of dictionary definitions illustrates how change can range from superficial differences such as “to put a fresh covering on” (as in “to change a bed”), to greater transformations or transitions from one state, condition, or phase to another (as in “She changed as she matured” or “the changing of the seasons”). Change can also mean substituting, or replacing of one thing for another (as in “change methods, change sides, a change of ownership”). Change is not neutral, but rather quite value laden. For example, it may be viewed in terms of progress and innovation, as with new directions and discoveries. Nevertheless, such a positive view is not always the case. Whereas one person may value an innovation, another may view it as a fad, a loss, or a reactive pendulum swing, which may (or may not) ultimately achieve a balance. Even the idea of balance is not a goal that everyone strives for, out of concern that a favored theory or instructional approach may become diluted. As you read Literacy in America, you will see topics, paradigms, and programs that have both long and short histories. On the topic of classroom instruction, for example, entries range from approaches that are as traditional as the ones we, and generations before us, experienced as schoolchildren (see, for example, Transmission Instruction) to more recent participatory approaches that include Book Clubs, Concept Instruction with Text, Cooperative Learning, Dialogue Journals, Inquiry-Based Instruction, Interdisciplinary Approaches to Literacy, Literature Circles, Peer Discussion, and Process Writing. Another change reflected in this encyclopedia is today’s emphasis on context, which is considered important for both literacy research and practice. A good deal of cognitive literacy research, which focused on students’ learning, took place in laboratories or settings outside the classroom. Results were aggregated in search of generalizable patterns that could presumably be applied across students and classrooms. Although student characteristics such as race, class, sex, ethnicity, and language may have been reported in the methods sections of research studies, these characteristics were largely ignored when results were presented and implications for instruction were drawn. Usually, the only variable of importance was ability level. Results were then used to inform classroom practice, translated into rec- ommendations for instruction applicable to all settings. Literacy research today includes a much greater focus on students as unique individuals, whose identities are shaped by their linguistic and cultural backgrounds; the literacy practices in their homes and communities; and their class, sex, age, talents, future aspirations, and positioning in the social hierarchy of schools and society. These attributes have a profound influence on students’ learning, literacy goals, and practices; the texts they value; the strategies they use to learn and/or cope; their access to literacy; and whether they succeed in school. Case studies of individual learners are not new, of course. There have been many case studies that have served us well as teaching tools for assessing literacy difficulties and planning for instruction (we no longer tend to use words like diagnosis and remediation, which imply a medical model and the search for pathology). Many of the case studies today emphasize students’ strengths as well as their needs and seek to understand their lives and identities outside of school as well as in the classroom. Like many of today’s case studies, entries in this encyclopedia focus on the role of different literacies in family, peer, and community life (see Adolescent Literacy, Adult Literacy, Family Literacy, Literacy in Informal Settings, Literacy in Play, Recreational Reading, and Workplace Literacy). Other entries focus on the effects of labels such as “at risk” and “failing,” asking hard questions such as how students might be viewed differently if schools were different and conceptions of success were expanded. Entries that address issues of diversity in culture and language as well as social justice include: At-Risk Students, Bilingualism, Biliteracy, Diversity, Ebonics, Gender and Discussion, Gender and Post-Typographical Text, Gender and Reading, Gender and Writing, Literacy and Culture, Multicultural Literacy, The Political Nature of Literacy, and Social Justice and Literacies. Even the conception of “literacy” has changed from being viewed as a monolithic entity and has taken on its plural form—literacies—to reflect multiplicities of uses, forms, and subjectivities. Literacy in settings other than school and expanded views of learning also bring with them expanded views of text. Consider, for example, the entries on Graffiti, Popular Culture, Television and Reading, and Zines). The encyclopedia xv Introduction also includes entries that suggest how texts in classrooms have expanded beyond basal readers and textbooks to include Multimedia, Post-Typographic, Refutational Texts, and Trade Books. Other entries emphasize today’s interest in technology, with topics such as Computer-Assisted Instruction, Distance Learning, Early Literacy Software, Hypertext, Instant Messaging, Listservs in Literacy, and Software for Older Readers. These new topics require an expanded repertoire of research approaches and theoretical perspectives, or new eyes, if you will, drawn from many different disciplines. Thus, in addition to cognitive approaches, we find entries in the encyclopedia on Activity Theory, Critical Literacy, Discourse Analysis, Discursive Theory, Feminist Post-Structuralism, Post-Structuralism and Structuralism, Semiotics, Social Constructivism, Sociolinguistics and Literacy, and Transactional Theory. Expanded views of research and theory and the emphasis on individual learners, with their linguistic, cultural, and personal histories, have paralleled changes in instructional research and practice. The earlier concept of research “informing” instruction in a unilateral direction has shifted to one in which research, theory, and practice inform one another in reciprocal ways. This iterative process is reflected in a growing interest in action research, teachers as researchers, and university-school partnerships. Concurrent with these trends are expanded views of knowledge as contextual, constructive, social, divergent, and open to continual reinterpretation. Further, research on instruction draws from other perspectives as well. Consider, for example, the renewed interest in Lev Vygotsky’s work that is one basis of instructional scaffolding. This brings us to the issue of what changes in the field of literacy have most directly affected teaching in classrooms—the topics, policies, curricular and instructional approaches and programs, and assessment practices that impact teachers and students in classrooms and that reflect teachers’ needs and concerns. One of the most profound changes at the school level is inclusive education (for a related topic, see Mainstreaming). Although focusing on individuals with disabilities, advocates of inclusion seek to change the philosophy and structure of schools so that all students, despite differences in language, culture, ethnicity, economic status, gender, and ability, can be educated with their peers in the regular classroom in their neighborhood schools. To many, inclusive education represents a shift from changing individuals (who must become “ready” and earn the right to be in integrated settings) to changing the curriculum and pedagogy to meet students’ needs. Instead of locating literacy problems in students (or their families), many literacy educators emphasize changing the curriculum and instructional practices to meet the needs of students. Curricular and instructional approaches that promote the active, social construction of knowledge, that are interactive, experiential, and inquiry based, and that provide guided instruction have been recommended as ways to include and motivate students who have traditionally been excluded from success in the mainstream. Thus, the encyclopedia includes entries on Biliteracy, Bilingualism, Ebonics, Multicultural Literacy, Multicultural Literature, and various gender issues, all of which represent a shift from the “one-curriculum-fits-all” philosophy of the past. A large repertoire of specific approaches and practices that have been developed for all grade levels and for all facets of literacy are featured in the encyclopedia. Approaches for teaching beginning reading include the entries on Balanced Literacy Instruction, Individualized Reading, Linguistic Approaches to Reading Instruction, Phonics Instruction, and Whole Language and Whole Language Assessment. These approaches include and overlap with topics such as Comprehension Strategies, Fluency, Narrative Text, Oral Reading, Questioning, Read-Alouds, and Silent Reading, to mention only a few of the entries. Specific methods associated for the most part with helping students become motivated, independent learners in the content areas include Graphic Aids, KWL and KWL+, Mental Modeling, Reciprocal Teaching, Semantic Feature Analysis, Semantic Mapping, Structural Analysis, Study Skills and Strategies, Think-Alouds (used for instruction and assessment), and Vocabulary Instruction. Finally, because writing today is considered such an important and integral part of literacy, the encyclopedia includes entries on Dialogue Journals, Process Writing, ReadingWriting Relationships, and Writing across the Curriculum. Literacy has always been political in nature because it is so important in society, communities, and individual lives. How literacy is taught, what xvi Introduction is taught (influenced by curriculum guidelines and standards), who gets the greatest resources, how special services are delivered (inclusion versus pullout), and how literacy and learning are assessed are high-stakes issues. Teachers, parents, administrators, children, and politicians all have different concerns and stakes in accountability, mainly through forms of high-stakes testing that include norm-referenced and criterion-referenced achievement tests, minimum competency testing, National Assessment of Educational Progress, and the English Language Arts Standards. At the same time that high-stakes testing stands at the forefront in federal funding and accountability, more informal authentic assessments are of great importance and value to teachers as part of the instructional assessment cycle. These types of assessment are included in entries such as Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers, Classroom Writing Assessment, Kidwatching and Classroom Evaluation, Miscue Analysis, Portfolios, PriorKnowledge Assessment, Reading-Attitude Measures, and Reading-Interest Inventories. It is intriguing to wonder what literacy educators and researchers will be debating, researching, and teaching when the next encyclopedia of literacy is published. Will we look back on today’s changes as a steady progression or as old relics? What questions will we be asking? What should they be? Ideally, we will be open not only to new discoveries but also to seeing existing landscapes through new eyes, as Marcel Proust would have us do. This will entail open-mindedness, as well as reasoned critiques of past, current, and future views of literacy research, theory, and practice. Suzanne E. Wade contributed to this encyclopedia see these and similar questions as intricately connected, both in complexity and in the various ways of responding to them. By its very nature, however, the practice of literacy leaves to those who wish to think about it the task of unmasking its technology, purposes, and contexts of use. These two volumes provide a range of foundational, comprehensive implements necessary for beginning to do just that. As a comprehensive resource, this encyclopedia enables those seeking an understanding of the various aspects of literacy to draw upon the wisdom and authority of experts from a variety of disciplines. Variety here is key and necessary, since today the dominant view of literacy characterizes it as an area of inquiry in which the parts make up the whole. Scholars cannot adequately address the consequences of the interactions between reader and text, writer and audience, teacher and student without taking into consideration the economic, social, functional, and cultural consequences of the act. None exists separately. Nonetheless, each of the parts making up the whole can be considered separately without losing track of their mutual interdependence. One part in particular—literacy outside the classroom—is addressed here through such entries as Ebonics (African American English), Family Literacy, Graffiti, Instant Messaging, Literacy in Informal Settings, Popular Culture, Workplace Literacy, and other entries within this category. From anthropologists to historians, sociologists to classroom practitioners, disciplined, probing minds interact here to provide the scholarship to interpret a picture of the social and cultural correlates of literacy. This picture is sometimes quite local and personal in both context and purpose for literacy. For instance, as a young girl I stood at the intersection where the politics of voice, power, class, and race meet. And, like many African Americans, I lived and learned in a place where grown folks communed at kitchen tables over cups of Sanka and packs of Pall Mall Golds in the morning hours or on front porches swept clean of dust, rocks, and fallen leaves at dusk. It was here where the adults told, shared, and reinvented stories and meanings about life before an echoing stage of young listeners. We—a peripheral audience of children—watched, listened, learned, and later imitated. As Shirley Brice III The collection of entries in this encyclopedia constitutes a foundational tool kit essential to understanding literacy and its many manifestations, both inside and outside the classroom. Such a resource is essential, as there is growing concern among all levels of educators regarding the meaning of knowing and teaching, and the active role of reading, writing, and speaking in both endeavors. How is meaning created as a reader engages with a text, as a writer recounts personal history for an audience, as a teacher interacts with students during a discussion of metaphor in The Color Purple? Those who have xvii Introduction Heath (1988) suggests, these ritualized events were an occasion for learning in a stream of stimuli, from which we the children would select, practice, and determine the rules of speaking and interacting with words. We learned at an early age how to listen to and perceive these interactions between adults as well as to determine how these interactions related to the world around and beyond. The rules of these verbal dances were not laid out per se. Instead, they were passed down in grown folks’ talk about things in their world. They did not ask or tell us what, how, and why. Rather, they detailed the responses of personalities to events; they praised, they derided, they questioned the reasons for events and compared new items and events to those with which they were familiar (Heath, 1988). They did not simplify their talk about the world for our benefit. They taught, and we learned in the landscape the meanings of their words. Around me stories came to life, and I was called on to create an imagined background for them. These were stories, in artifact and word, of our parents’ childhood at a time when African Americans were “colored” and a loaf of bread cost a dime. People I’d never met would be resurrected and bit by bit pieced together with the tongues of enchanted detail for my mind’s eye. From my mother’s lips, strangers were made familiar and were given the breath of life in words and gestures that shaped, colored, and lent meanings—meanings that played out within a scenery long since decayed. For long afternoons through late evenings we’d sit, listen, and learn. Here, functions of literacy outside the classroom are real and familiar. Unfortunately, however, they are all too often overlooked when we think about literacy, primarily because literacy is often associated with a specific purpose and form of language taught in classrooms. When the purposes of language in the classroom are too distant from those outside students’ lives, resistance and failure are most likely to result. By contrast, when the purposes of literacy in the classroom can be related in familiar socially and culturally situated ways, education is much more successful. Such socially and culturally situated ways of teaching and learning have traditionally been given little consideration within the educational world. Much of this oversight is because there are few data that address such forms of literacy acquisition. In addition, there are also problems of inaccessibility and the lack of alternative points of view and critical reassessments. Such alternative points of view are necessary if we want to gain insight into how people experience learning and teaching beyond school settings. While offering invaluable insight within the field of literacy, the intellectuals who contributed to these volumes have committed themselves to broadening the landscape of education. As testimony to this, we have witnessed an expansion in recent years in the scholarship on literacy across community, classroom, and even workplace, a form of scholarship with univocal origins. Those who were once confined within their own domains of inquiry in anthropology, linguistics, psychology, and literary theory are converging, combining their expertise toward productive multidimensional ends. Through their contributions, these individuals endeavor to provide educators at all levels with a valuable resource, one that helps to dismantle old notions of a one-sizefits-all approach to literacy. In part or in whole, this encyclopedia provides tools useful in understanding the complicated relations among the dimensions of literacy. To the reader belongs the pleasure of investigating them. Yolanda J. Majors IV Museum literacy. Museum literacy? Many of us thought the literacy proliferation of the past decade had peaked with emotional literacy. Nevertheless, even a relatively small sample of the 2,520,000 “literacy” sites, unearthed by a web search engine in just a tenth of a second, suggests that this proliferation is just hitting its stride: visual literacy, numerical literacy, Jewish literacy, financial literacy, consumer literacy, health literacy, food literacy, dance literacy, film literacy, art literacy, wine literacy, sexual literacy, information literacy, environmental literacy, electronic literacy, digital literacy, nutrition literacy, geographic literacy, chemical literacy, biology literacy, library literacy, geoscience literacy, and museum literacy (see Ecological Literacy, Media Literacy, Multicultural Literacy, Multiple Literacies, and Visual Literacy). Perhaps it’s a lack of imagination that’s responsible for the current practice of baptizing such wide-ranging human endeavor and accomplishxviii Introduction ment as “literacy.” For example, when we finally realized that sexual education involves more than technical understanding of reproduction systems, we weren’t quite sure how to name the new incarnation, so we just substituted literacy for education in the old name because it had worked already for computer education, art education, and so forth. Perhaps this apparent proliferation of literacies is the result of shrewd marketing of old wines in new bottles by educational entrepreneurs. The cultural cachet of literacy is undeniably desirable, and what better way to give biology or accounting a face-lift than to have them join the burgeoning family of literacies where, it seems, there’s always room for one more. Asking why there are suddenly so many new players in the literacy landscape isn’t prompted by concerns about professional turf. Nor is it motivated by a desire for a singular circumscription of the notion of literacy. Instead, the question arises from a very basic communication issue: when people use the term literacy, what do they mean? How are they understood? We need to determine what, if any, common conceptual ground these many new notions of literacy share—because if literacy means everything, it means nothing. Historically, in Western culture, literacy has meant the ability to read and write. More specifically, this has meant the ability to read and write words—the dominant sign system in Western culture—and assemblages of words, called texts, that have been configured to explain, persuade, entertain, and so on. Tacit in this notion of literacy is that words and, by extension, texts are the places in which meaning resides. In this scheme, the division of labor is clear. Writing is an act of text production by which an author uses personal knowledge of the world, the context, and linguistic conventions to choose and arrange words skillfully in an attempt to convey intended meanings precisely. Reading then becomes an act of text consumption by which a reader uses personal knowledge of the world, the context, and linguistic conventions in an attempt to discern the author’s meanings accurately. So how do we get from reading and writing word texts to wine literacy or visual literacy? Not easily; because the logocentric (word-centered) worldview that underwrites most of our cultural practices—including curriculum, pedagogy, and assessment in public schools—makes it very, very difficult for us to think about reading or writing without reference to words. Imagine the responses you might get if, for example, you asked people on the street if they could read a shiraz (Australian grape) or write a seascape. Breaking the vicelike grip of logocentrism and expanding our notion of what it means to be literate depend on clarifying the nature of words and their epistemological function in our lives. Rather than seeing words as inherently meaningful we need to understand them as symbolically meaningful, as socially and culturally sanctioned signs that we configure in endless ways to codify our understanding of everyday experience across myriad contexts: the color red represents danger; a thumbs-up gesture represents approval; a downturned mouth represents sadness; the inability to read the printed word represents illiteracy. Of course, depending on the situation, red might represent passion; thumbs-up might represent a request; a downturned mouth might represent a sign of sexual interest. Realistically, given the privileged status of words in mainstream culture, the inability to read the printed word will continue to represent illiterateness in most contexts. Nevertheless, if we accept that the nature and function of words are symbolic and if we recognize that words are only one of many types of symbols or signs by which we make sense of our worlds, it becomes possible to reconceptualize literacy in more inclusive ways. Writing becomes an act of configuring culturally sanctioned signs—word, gesture, sound, color—in one or more medium, for instance, in print, film, paint, clay, fabric. Text becomes a configuration of signs—haiku, mime, symphony, photograph, tapestry. Reading becomes an act of interpreting configurations of culturally sanctioned signs (see Multimedia, and Semiotics). Implicit here is that all textual activity is agenda driven. Writing and reading are done for some reason: to develop theories, pursue relationships, sell products, defend the weak, placate the annoying, console the heartsick—but not just any theory, relationship, product, or weakness. The political innuendo implicit in the notion of agenda reminds us that our textual acts are not naively utilitarian but rather are underwritten by particular assumptions about the nature of reality and knowledge, by ideological beliefs, as well as by social and moral values. There are no neutral or innocent acts of writing or reading; there xix Introduction are no neutral or innocent texts. We write, and read to clarify, preserve, disrupt, advance and resist what we know, believe, and value about our worlds and our places in them (see Critical Literacy, Critical Reading, Resistant Reading, and The Political Nature of Literacy). In this scheme, to write a painting becomes a process of configuring line, shape, hue, tone, texture to achieve some agenda—social commentary, aesthetic experimentation, cultural celebration, personal catharsis. Similarly, to read a painting becomes a process of interpreting a particular configuration of visual signs to satisfy some agenda—learn about technique, experience visceral response, appraise authenticity, psychoanalyze the artist. Mary Pratt’s painting Pomegranates—Open and Closed, for example, understood as an assemblage of signs configured to achieve some agenda, becomes a text in the same sense as Peter Carey’s novel True History of the Kelly Gang. Embodied in this revisionist notion of literacy is a subtle shift in the locus of meaning. If signs, and therefore texts, are not inherently meaningful but rather socially negotiated, the significance or meaning of any assemblage of words, paint, gesture, or sound can also be seen as indeterminate and socially negotiated—brokered in the contexts from which it emerges through the age, experiences, interests, needs, and agendas of those who engage with texts as authors or as readers (see Context in Literacy, Literacy and Culture, Social Constructivism, Social Nature of Literacy, and Subjectivity). Subtle, too, is the possibility that authors and readers can satisfy differing agendas through the same putative texts. With the constraints of intentional fallacy loosened considerably, authors and readers have more leeway in negotiating what they want or need from texts. Thus, although Pratt might have intended Pomegranates—Open and Closed as meditation on common objects, her text can be read plausibly as a comment on human relationships. And although Carey might have intended True History of the Kelly Gang as a recreational adventure tale, it can also be taken up as a chronicle of racism and class struggle in colonial Australia. This is not to say that authors or readers can operate unfettered; communal conventions and practices tend to prevent any descent into interpretive relativism. Finally, this particular revisionist scheme brings new responsibilities. Writers need to become more critically reflexive in order to understand what and whose agendas their texts are intended to serve and whether they condone their own participation in the process. Readers need to monitor critically not only their own textual agendas but also those of the authors with whose texts they engage, asking “What’s going on here?” “For whom?” “What forms of representation are being privileged?” “Why?” (see Critical Literacy, Critical Media Literacy, Critical Reading, and Social Justice and Literacies). So, we return to our communication conundrum: what do we mean when we invoke the notion of literacy? What, if anything, do print literacy, wine literacy, and museum literacy have in common? The simple answer is that they are all semiotic processes; they are all concerned with learning to write and read—to make sense of— our lived and virtual experiences. Obvious? Maybe. But if it is obvious, we don’t act as if it is. In fact, we don’t act as though anything other than words are concerned with meaning-making. This is most blatant in the visual and performing arts, which many seem, at best, as interpretive acts performed with varying degrees of skillfulness and, at worst, as recreational or leisure activities used as a reward or change of pace—as “time off ” from the real curricular work of schools, working with words and numbers. In more subtle ways this epistemological marginalization is true of mathematics and the sciences, too. Rather than seeing numbers, equations, and theorems as ways of exploring and representing lived phenomena, students are groomed for seamless algorithmic performance and prodigious accretion of facts. This hegemony of words continues even at a time when cultural studies, and particularly media studies, have emboldened us to refer confidently to government, race, sex, disease, restaurants, and train compartments as text(ual); and, on a grander scale, to talk blithely about reading the world. These everyday metaphorical invocations of conventional literate activity “work” as conversational glosses because they allude to the primary analogical processes of reading and writing texts for meaning. To move beyond intellectual glibness, we need to use the underlying architecture of this analogy to scaffold our moves from conceptually familiar and comfortable ground to less certain but more promising xx Introduction terrain: to make the familiar strange (e.g., words as signs), and to make the strange familiar again (e.g., painting as writing). The idea that meaning is socially constructed and symbolically mediated through multiple, culturally sanctioned sign systems has circulated among literacy educators and researchers for more than twenty years and has always held great promise for more broadly based conceptualizations of literacy. Yet this idea hasn’t had any sustained or widespread impact on literacy curriculum, pedagogy, or evaluation because we haven’t found ways to make it comprehensible or viable for teachers, parents, administrators, curriculum designers, and assessment specialists. In fairness, the obstacles to new notions of literacy are only partly conceptual; many teachers and teacher educators are working to transform curriculum and pedagogy in literacy education (as reflected in the entry Teacher Education in Literacy). The difficulties are also political and ideological. Conservative sensibilities and collective memories of previous innovations gone wrong have created resistance, among administrative and parent groups in many educational jurisdictions, to any seemingly radical reformulations of literacy and literacy pedagogy. The problems are also partly perceptual. Many teachers feel that they aren’t skilled enough in “alternative” sign systems to offer their students worthwhile experiences in different ways of knowing (“I can’t draw/sing/ dance/use Photoshop, so how could I ever help my students?”). Concerns about products and production values obscure the epistemological possibilities of alternative sign systems and different media. We need to remind ourselves that the overarching pedagogical agenda for new or multiple literacies is not to groom cinematographers, recording engineers, set designers, or other cultural experts; it’s to help us develop broader repertoires for experiencing and representing—knowing—our everyday worlds. In fact, when all is said and done, the most compelling reason for embracing more inclusive notions of literacy is to enable us to know our worlds in many ways. This is particularly urgent for those in our collective care who are “at risk” because they can’t participate fluently in cultures underwritten by conventional notions of literacy. If we think more inclusively about what it means to be literate—to embrace not just the notion but also the practice of multiple literacies—we are more likely to create sanctioned spaces in which all of us can participate meaningfully and joyously in the worlds of ideas and feelings. Allan Neilsen V At this writing, three events coincide. From Signal Hill in Newfoundland, the world marks the one-hundredth anniversary of Guglielmo Marconi’s triumph, the first transatlantic radio transmission, a feat no one believed possible at the time. An international report conducted in 2000 by the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) on the state of literacy across the world releases information that places North American adolescents at the top among their peers on the globe. And, from outside her country, an Afghan woman, Dr. Sima Samar, emerges in the wake of the post-September 11, 2001, conflict to become one of two women appointed to serve in the transitional government of post-Taliban Afghanistan. These are critical, and particular, conjunctions at the beginning of the new century in a time of uncertainty: what are we to make of these events in light of the scope of our work in literacy, education, and research? As the selections in this volume reveal, such seemingly disparate events in our local and larger worlds can be seen as important markers in literacy, particularly as we view our literacies through the many lenses we consider feminist. Just as there is no single perspective on literacy and literacy research, there is no single feminist perspective on what we know, how we know it, and how we presume to learn more to create and support just and democratic environments for teaching and learning. As Rebecca Luce-Kapler’s discussion of gender and writing illustrates so well, our ontologies and epistemologies both shape and are shaped by the cultural, political, social, and psycho-emotional contexts in which we live and work. We cannot step over them in order to speak of our positionings; we are inside them. Similarly, when we imagine more educative and socially just environments for education and research, we must understand that how students become literate—and what their literacy looks like or gives them the power (or not) to do—is shaped by the environments in which they learn. What does an eight-year-old Metis girl in a classxxi Introduction room in Northern Alberta learn about reading, writing, agency, and life as she completes another worksheet on “ing” words, listens to a story about a Southern boy’s adventures, and uses her emergent literacy skills to fashion a form of survival in a classroom where the world she knows is only minimally represented, if at all? In what ways do the literate behaviors she acquires, or into which she is inducted, affect how she sees the world, and who she might become? Here, then, in such a question, we may reach for some common point of consensus—if that is the appropriate word—in feminist perspectives on literacy, learning, and research. As illustrated by the contributions to this volume, the questions feminist researchers ask are myriad: they concern the texts and discourses available or unavailable to students, teachers, and researchers; the educational environments, teaching behaviors, strategies, and practices inherited, perpetuated, applied, and resisted; and the ways and means we consider in a demonstration of what literacy is, how we measure literate behaviors, what values are implicit in these choices and decisions, and further, how we might investigate literacy practices, and who decides. But regardless of the diversity of these perspectives among feminist researchers, we seek—as we presume all educators and investigators in literacy seek—a greater understanding of the mysteries of reading, writing, and learning. Most feminist theorists, researchers, and educators seek to understand and re-vision the political, cultural, and social relations that privilege certain perspectives, voices, approaches, texts, research methods, or sets of discursive practices over another. The term feminist, whether or not it is accompanied by a descriptor such as liberal, post-structuralist, or radical, is necessarily a political word, used with political intent, and effecting political impact. In the last decade of the twentieth century, the field of literacy shifted and broadened to include notions of multiple literacies, issues of social justice, and multiple perspectives on literacy research. Without question, such a shift forced reassessment of what counts as literacy, what counts as research, and what matters in the teaching and learning enterprise. These are unquestioningly political issues, and as we push investigation of them further, we must necessarily tap into larger political and global dimensions of literacy and life. Reading the word, as Paulo Freire reminded us, is reading the world. Writing the word, writing and reading texts and lives: these, too, are literacies of living. And now, early in this new century, with world events forcing our attention to larger global and humanitarian issues, our shifting understandings of literacy continue to broaden and deepen. Many voices from many disciplines have urged such a shift; the field of literacy, especially where feminist and social justice issues are concerned, has been informed by a number of theorists and writers. Maxine Greene has advocated an expansive and diverse approach to literacy as fundamental to democracy, Donna Haraway has influenced our understanding of technologies as gendered, Valerie Walkerdine and Pam Gilbert have reminded literacy educators that class and gender are critical factors regardless of a teacher’s best intentions, and Patricia Hill Collins and Cynthia Dillard, among other African American educators, have brought to the field a long-overdue recognition of the intersection of race and feminist epistemologies. Donna Alvermann has written extensively of feminist post-structuralism and literacies, JoBeth Allen of social justice issues, and Lorri Neilsen of feminist post-structuralist perspectives on research, particularly the role of alternative discourses and genres. What their works have in common has caused a reassessment of reading and writing in our lives: each reminds us we must keep the field open, diverse, and inclusive (see Social Justice and Literacies); that we must refuse fundamentalist and fractured notions of literacy and being; and that we are located in particular settings and positions, even as we attempt to work toward a common good. Several of the entries in these volumes, such as Resistant Reading, and Subjectivity, reflect these growing understandings of how we must balance local and larger visions. And whether the issue is agency, voice, class, culture/ethnicity, gender, or technologies, we recognize that literacy is the seed from which perspectives and power germinate. We recognize that we still have much to learn, and much to do. Less than 100 years ago, reading researchers devised a machine on which a subject-reader rested the chin so that the researcher might document eye movements and thus offer hypotheses about reading speed and comprehension (the entry Eye Movements provides an overview). In the not-sodistant past, researchers measured the circumferxxii Introduction ence of male and female skulls in order to offer hypotheses about intelligence. In the late 1960s, we parceled language into manipulable bits of syntax—t-units and causal connectives, to name a few—and isolated these in our research in the hope that we could determine which units had salience and could thus create, for readers and teachers alike, texts that had a certain logic, linearity, or predictability, regardless of context. At the time, our questions were located in the individual as representative of a norm, and in the text, as representative of accepted cultural norms. As researchers, we did not consider that these norms may have supported a privileged, exclusive, and limited view of literacy. The notion that our entire enterprise was politically shaped—and that it shaped us, politically—was, wittingly or unwittingly, foreign. Politics was World War II or the Cold War; politics was a seat in the House of Representatives or in Parliament. These had nothing to do with teaching, learning, research, or literacy. However, as we see how our perspectives on literacy have changed, educators and researchers are increasingly able to recognize and inform one another on where the universal and the particular meet, exploring how these affect schooling and research. The one-hundredth anniversary of Marconi’s transmission calls to mind the breathtaking growth in technologies over the last century and, among other implications, reminds us of the remarkable ways in which our gendered social practices repeatedly reinforce and replicate themselves in emerging media, in spite of our attempts to resist (see Marion Fey’s entry, Gender and Post-Typographical Text). Such media are multiplying rapidly in the schools and from the outset have pressed us to consider countless questions about literacy, gender, and authority, as Donna Alvermann’s contribution to these volumes illustrates (see Critical Media Literacy). The OECD test results on literacy that rank North American adolescents higher than most (Canadian students at third, U.S. students at fifteenth, among thirty-two countries) do not necessarily prompt everyone to celebrate; rather, the results invite us to consider how we measure and assess literacy growth; whose values are implicit in those measures; which countries are “excelling” at the expense of others; which cultures and forms of literacy are going unnoticed or are being allowed to die, having been overtaken by the weight of other dominant cultural and political forces. Finally, the selection of Sima Samar as interim deputy prime minister and minister responsible for women’s affairs in Afghanistan’s transitional government reminds a feminist researcher in literacy of several issues. First and most obviously, Samar’s tenuous and marginal role reminds us that women, regardless of education, continue to have a voice much more limited than their numbers in the population would suggest (and, although cultural practices and values differ, women in North America do not necessarily enjoy representation in greater degree). Further, her role as the minister responsible for women’s affairs reminds us that we, as a global culture, continue to struggle with issues of inclusion and representation. Do we hope for a time when women’s affairs do not need their own department? In schools, do we study African American poets, or do we study, simply, poets, looking carefully at our curricula to ensure that all voices are represented in ways that reveal our rich cultural landscape? Among many other implications, we might consider how this signal event, the circumstances surrounding it, and our awareness of those events have forever altered our perspective on a number of fronts: global, cultural, social, and personal. The glib phrase education for freedom has deeper and more resonant tones now than ever before. Literacy is our access and our angle on the aspects of our world. Our literacies are how we live inside and respond to our worlds. Although our literate behavior has its own fingerprint, we are ecological and global beings; we read and write in local and larger contexts and in communities that shift, that create us as we help to create them. We can resist these contexts, subvert them, reshape them, challenge them, and draw from them. In our increasingly expansive understanding of literacies, educators and researchers are recognizing that literacy is not only of the head, it is of the body, the senses, the heart. The gendered—some say patriarchal—assumptions that literacy is only of the mind and not written on the body are being questioned. Educators in greater numbers are abandoning miseducative and unsustainable literacy-learning activities and helping students and teachers to consider more holistic, embodied, community and globally directed approaches to text, whether the text is conventional print or a student-created multimedia production (see Media Literacy, and Gender and Discussion). xxiii Introduction Students themselves are becoming authors of their own literacy learning, taking up roles that their teachers once held, demonstrating and stretching new conceptions of literacy in the process. Similarly, classroom teachers look to students and to each other for approaches and inspiration, not to an external authority they may once have relied upon, because they understand that the knowledge they create together creates connection and new possibilities. Such knowledge comes from both boys and girls, from diverse cultural and religious perspectives, from many subject positions, in many voices and in many keys. Educational researchers continue to push our notions of “research literacies” to include participant-shaped qualitative inquiry, narrative approaches, arts-based inquiry, among other research methods and texts. All of these shifts recognize and embrace the complexities we’ve spawned as we have redefined literacy in the last several decades. And all of these shifts are consistent with most conceptions of feminist perspectives on literacy and learning. If it is an equitable world we seek, then our search must, in practice, articulate the local with the global, particular people with universal connections, the word and the image with communities here and beyond. For every electronic text a student beams across the continent, there is a debt to Marconi and an opportunity to examine access and privilege. For every state-mandated literacy test, there arise questions of policy, values, and educating for a market economy. And for every instance of redressing gender imbalance, however minimal, there are millions of young girls across the world now hoping, through their literacy and schooling, to achieve a measure of human potential unlimited by culture or gender. Feminist researchers and educators see the connections among these and look for the possibilities to address them. Lorri Neilsen and aimed at enabling nations and individuals to “progress” economically and socially (see Adult Literacy). At this time within First World countries like the United States, however, official statistics indicated that adult illiteracy was almost nil. Existing adult literacy initiatives in such countries were small-scale, largely voluntary endeavors. In the First World, “literacy” teaching occurred only in marginal spaces of nonformal education work intended to provide a “second chance” for those whose illiteracy was often seen as directly associated with other debilitating or dysfunctional conditions and circumstances—like unemployment, imprisonment, drug abuse, teenage pregnancy, inferior physical and psychic health, and so on. Three contemporaneous factors played key roles in literacy emerging as a new center of gravity within formal education. The first was the rise to prominence of Paulo Freire’s work within the larger context of the radical education movement of the late 1960s and early 1970s. His work with peasant groups in Brazil and Chile exemplified how literacy work could be central to radical approaches to education aimed at building critical social praxis. The second was the dramatic discovery—many called it an invention—of widespread illiteracy among adults in the United States during the early 1970s. This alleged literacy crisis coincided with early awareness of profound structural change in the economy as the country moved toward becoming a postindustrial society. Postindustrialism entailed far-reaching restructuring of the labor market and employment, as well as deep changes in the major organizations and institutions of daily life. Large numbers of people were seen as poorly prepared for these changes. This “literacy crisis” quickly spread to other emerging postindustrial societies. The third factor was the increasing prominence of a sociocultural perspective within linguistics and the social sciences that impacted strongly on conceptual and theoretical understandings of textual practices. Sylvia Scribner and Michael Cole’s seminal work, The Psychology of Literacy (1981), followed by Brian Street’s Literacy in Theory and Practice (1984), provided strong research-based theoretical and conceptual bases from which to critique established approaches to the teaching of reading and writing in schools and an escalating emphasis on “basics” and “functional literacy” fueled by the “literacy crisis.” In this essay we adopt a sociocultural perspective. VI A field of scholarly and pedagogical activity directly known as literacy emerged only comparatively recently in formal education. Before the 1970s, literacy was associated mainly with educationally disadvantaged adults and, in particular, with Third World settings. Beginning in the 1960s, numerous nonformal adult literacy programs were implemented in developing countries xxiv Introduction The emergence of a literacy field within formal education was, of course, superimposed on an already long-established field known as “reading.” Psychology-dominated conceptions of reading became prominent in the 1800s. Language was conceptualized as a fixed system of communication, and reading as a discrete set of mental skills to be mastered in a simple-to-difficult progression. From this perspective, becoming a reader involves first learning the alphabet, then small words, larger words, entire sentences, and so on. Thanks largely to psychological conceptions of what people do with print, reading held dominion over writing in terms of research, theoretical development, and pedagogical focus within formal education for almost 200 years. Educationists in the United States and throughout the West have long regarded literacy as a largely psychological ability—an essentially private possession wired into our heads. Reading (and, secondarily, writing) means mastering decoding and encoding skills, involving cognitive capacities seen as integral to “cracking the alphabetic code,” word formation, phonics, grammar, comprehension, and the like. These skills serve as building blocks for accessing meaning, for communicating, and so on. According to this view, once people have literacy they can use “it” to learn, and to pursue many other benefits, including employment, knowledge, leisure and recreational pursuits, and personal development. With the turn to science that marked the 1950s through the 1970s, psychological approaches to understanding reading and writing became more attuned to individual differences. Educators began promoting the need for individual students to work at their own pace. A new emphasis on higher-order thinking brought related shifts in focus from reading accuracy to reading comprehension. This was also associated with a new emphasis on self-directedness and self-responsibility for learning to read (and, at times, learning to write) that culminated in stand-alone “kits” containing purpose-written texts, purpose-produced filmstrips, flashcards, and the like. Among the best-known artifacts here were the ubiquitous Science Research Associates (SRA) reading kits of the 1960s. Commercial reading schemes based on the principle of having students engage with simple-to-complex sequences of skills proliferated in the 1970s and quickly became “must-have” items in schools. By the mid-1980s, researchers oriented by psychological theories—especially psycholinguistics— had further refined earlier studies of reading development into an exact science that focused particularly on understanding how children successfully encode, decode, and make meaning from printed texts. From the late 1960s on, the dominance of psychology within the field of reading was increasingly challenged by emerging humanistic theories of reading and writing. These confronted psychological theories that mainly attributed reading and writing failure to individuals, rather than taking into account home background, reading materials, teacher approaches to reading and writing instruction, and so forth. The humanistic perspective is characterized by concern with the child’s holistic development. This includes engaging in “authentic” or “naturalistic” learning experiences, attending to the aesthetic dimension of language use (including a new focus on oral language uses), and conceiving knowledge as being (best) built collaboratively rather than individually. It also regards language production as an art rather than as a set of discrete subskills to be mastered sequentially from the simple to the more difficult. From this perspective, reading and writing (and speaking) are referred to collectively as language arts. Theories of natural learning promote the idea that children should learn to read and write in the same way they learn to speak: via full immersion into and engagement with a textual world. This also resulted in a shift from teacher-directed pedagogy to child-centered pedagogies. Humanistic theories also underpinned the development of process approaches to learning to read and write that were influenced directly by analyzing what real (adult) readers and writers do when they read and write. The emphasis on authenticity encouraged a shift away from heavy reliance on lockstep basal reading programs toward using “real” children’s literature in classroom reading programs. Perhaps the best-known teaching approach to emerge from humanistic theories of language use is whole language, with its dual emphasis on social cooperation (e.g., communities of learners) and on individual differences in talents and backgrounds (see Authentic Assessment, Language Arts Instruction, Language Experience Approach, and Whole Language and Whole Language Assessment). xxv Introduction The 1970s and 1980s also witnessed the emergence of sociocultural theories. These became increasingly popular during the 1990s, influencing pre-service and in-service teacher education courses and reading and writing instruction to a notable degree. This theoretical development had begun with the emergence of social cognition theories of language use derived from Lev Vygotsky and Alexsandre Luria (see Constructivism, and Social Constructivism), and the growth of Marxist/critical theories of language practices. This perspective has been influenced directly by anthropology, history, sociology, and philosophy as well and can be credited with bringing the term literacy into common use within education. Sociocultural theories promoted wider—although never predominant— recognition among educationists that “literacy” is inherently ideological, multiple, contested, dynamic, and contingent. Previously, the notion that reading and writing involved a singular, autonomous, neutral technology (alphabetic print) to be mastered through acquisition of cognitive skills and understandings had reigned supreme. From a sociocultural perspective, literacy is a matter of social practice. Literacies are bound up with social, institutional, and cultural relationships and can only be understood when they are situated within their social, cultural, and historical contexts. Moreover, they are always connected to social identities—to being particular kinds of people. Literacies are always embedded in discourses (see Sociolinguistics and Literacy). Texts are integral parts of innumerable everyday lived, spoken, enacted, value- and belief-laden practices that are carried out in specific places and at specific times (Gee, Hull, and Lankshear, 1996). Reading and writing are not the same thing within a youth zine culture, an on-line chat space, a school classroom, a feminist reading group, or different kinds of religious ceremonies. People read and write differently out of different social practices. These different ways with words are part of different ways of being a person and different ways and facets of life. From a sociocultural perspective, it is impossible to separate out from text-mediated social practices the “bits” concerned with reading or writing and to treat them independently of the “nonprint” bits like values and gestures, actions and objects, talk and interaction, tools and spaces (Gee, Hull, and Lankshear, 1996). They are all nonsubtractable parts of integrated wholes. “Literacy bits” do not exist apart from the social practices in which they are embedded and within which they are acquired. If, in some trivial sense, they can be said to exist (e.g., as code), they do not mean anything. Hence, they cannot meaningfully be taught and learned as separate. From one angle, the future could scarcely be brighter for the literacy field. Reduced “welfarism”—requiring individuals to be more selfsufficient—intensified informationalism, the rich rewards attached to high-order symbolic analysis and manipulation in postindustrial economies, and the brave new world of global electronic communications have massively upped the ante for literacies. New literacies are being invented daily on the streets, in workplaces, and in cyberspace. So far as literacy scholars and educators are concerned, there are seemingly boundless new realms to be explored, understood, and taught. From other angles, however, the picture is less rosy. Two aspects seem especially noteworthy. First, the literacy “field” is deeply internally conflicted. From a sociocultural perspective, psychology-based approaches to teaching literacy as largely decontextualized skills offends the principles of efficacious learning. Moreover, it advantages those students for whom skills-based pedagogy confers opportunities to become increasingly fluent, using skills they have already acquired within authentic practices in their homes and communities. From this same standpoint, humanistic approaches privilege those social groups whose home and community-based discourses approximate more closely the comparatively narrow range of textual (book/literature-centered) practices that tend to dominate whole language and process reading and writing pedagogies under classroom conditions. Wholelanguage educators may well see themselves as operating out of a sociocultural perspective. Many sociocultural literacy educators, however, would deny this, on the grounds that the pedagogy does not immerse learners in mature versions of authentic social practices but, rather, often involves highly schoolish caricatures of a narrow and middle/professional class-oriented range of social practices. Meanwhile, official literacy education policies and classroom pedagogical responses incline more and more toward psychological skills-based approaches in pursuit xxvi Introduction of encoding and decoding competence on the part of as many learners as possible. The second aspect concerns the role and place of the classroom within literacy education. Ultimately, the literacy field may be divided on this point. Although the classroom is suited to bookcentered learning and a range of drill and skill procedures and has spawned a repertoire of distinctive school discourses, its capacity to accommodate authentic practices in which diverse everyday literacies are embedded is strictly limited. In addition, many new and emerging literacies associated with social practices mediated by electronic communications and information technologies that are potentially capable of being engaged within classroom-like settings are seen as too risky or otherwise inappropriate for formal education. The internally conflicted “field” of literacy could actually split along a line that divides those educators, theorists, and researchers whose work continues to be predicated on the classroom as the principal and proper site for literacy education from those whose work presumes that literacy education belongs mainly elsewhere. Michele Knobel and Colin Lankshear VII The benefits of a professional encyclopedia are many. It gives the user ready access to the theories, perspectives, and programs valued within a community of practice. Each term is thoughtfully overviewed, carefully explicated, and well contextualized by acknowledged experts who craft their explanations and descriptions for readers whose needs are varied and who may not be immersed within the culture or traditions of that community. And so it is for Literacy in America, the twovolume encyclopedia for the domain of literacy. Still, the editor and contributors for these volumes had loftier goals in mind than merely creating a compilation of literacy theories, perspectives, and programs. Their stated intentions were to emphasize recent orientations, influences, and approaches and, in so doing, reveal trends taking shape in the study and practice of literacy. Although each individual contribution may incorporate a sociohistorical framework, the uninitiated reader of this encyclopedia needs more guidance to discern trends within literacy’s complex realm. Such an accomplishment would be analogous to discerning the landscape of a geographic area by reading descriptions of various cities or landmarks found there. Instead, when individuals desire a broad, integrated look at a physical terrain, they wisely turn to a topographical map that reveals the contours of that landscape. In a similar way, I want to create such a topographical profile based on the configuration and confluence of literacy terminology compiled within these two volumes. This topographical profile should make the trends and transformations referenced in the individual entries of these volumes more transparent to readers. However, rendering a topographical profile of the complex and abstract domain of literacy is not the same as creating a map of a physical terrain. In fact, it takes a massive undertaking like this two-volume encyclopedia with its hundreds of detailed, expertly crafted overviews to concretize the domain, thus allowing such a rendering. Therefore, using this encyclopedia as my guide, I want to take a critical look at the literacy landscape. Specifically, I want to discuss three general dimensions of the literacy landscape suggested by the contents of this informative encyclopedia and to compare those features to past chartings. First, I begin with an examination of the boundaries and regions of the literacy domain. I do so for the purpose of answering this critical question: What constitutes the field of literacy? In other words, how does this current assemblage demark literacy from other domains of research and practice? Such an exploration may also reveal how experts today and in the past have conceptualized this realm. Second, I look deeper inside the domain of literacy to identify regions of increasing activity and rapid expansion. Finally, I undertake a similar inspection to locate areas that appear fallow or seriously underdeveloped. Before proceeding, I must emphasize that areas of relative activity or inactivity are those of the domain of literacy and should not be perceived as shortcomings of this encyclopedia. In fact, the form of mapping I present here is only possible because of the richness of this encyclopedic work, which allows me to pinpoint these contours in literacy’s landscape. Several features of the overall landscape of literacy, as represented in these volumes, are immediately apparent. First, literacy is a vast and diverse domain that encompasses a range of linguistic, sociocultural, pedagogical, and communicative theories, processes, and approaches, from eye-movement research to discursive the- xxvii Introduction ory and from reader’s theater to reading clinics (see entries on these topics). Despite this scope and diversity, there are a few shared elements that distinguish literacy from surrounding domains. Fundamentally, literacy is concerned with language and the forces that influence its nature, acquisition, and use. Second, much of the territory now called “literacy” was once largely subsumed under the realm of “language arts.” Yet this transformation from language arts to literacy is far more than titular. It signifies a dramatic reconceptualization of the landscape. Specifically, under that older moniker, language arts, this territory fundamentally consisted of four rather discrete regions— reading, writing, speaking, and listening. There was little cross-fertilization of ideas from region to region, little in the way of shared features, and limited concern with the social, cultural, and political undercurrents that bind them together. Today, the landscape of literacy is noticeably more fluid and overlapping, particularly as it relates to the regions of reading and writing. As in the past, however, reading seemingly dominates the landscape, although writing is a realm of increasing significance and far more intertwined with reading. Speaking and listening, which have historically been areas of modest activity, have been reframed and increasingly absorbed into the realms of reading and writing. Nowhere is this transformation more evident than in dissolution of the old realm of speech. That is, we find that the formal area of speech has literally disappeared from the landscape. In its place, we find numerous entries in these volumes related to oral communication and verbal interaction linked to both reading (see Discussion, Oral Language, and Peer Discussion) and writing (see Dialogue Journals, and The Discussion Web). With only one entry to its credit (see Active Listening), the old realm of listening has experienced a similar dissolution and absorption. Further, it is evident that the boundaries of literacy have widened in recent years, spreading into new and uncharted territories. These new regions are often represented in these volumes by entries dealing with alternative literacies (see Critical Media Literacy, and Ecological Literacy). These entries also address the sociocultural forces shaping literacy processes in classrooms and the broader society (see Gender and Discussion, and Feminist Post-Structuralism). Because mappings are routinely influenced by social, cultural, and political factors of the time, they are typically biased renderings. The same can be said for the depiction of literacy based on this two-volume encyclopedia. Within this compilation, the aforementioned emergent regions have been somewhat exaggerated in scope relative to the entire domain of literacy. This enhancement is particularly apparent from the vantage point of everyday educational practice, where more traditional aspects of literacy (see Phonics Instruction, Predictable Books, and Questioning) still hold sway. However, according to the editor of this work, this enhancement was intentionally undertaken to highlight recent lines of inquiry within literacy for audiences that may be more familiar and absorbed with traditional dimensions of the domain. As with the concepts related to oral communication and verbal interactions, the clustering of theories, perspectives, and programs in these volumes is indicative of increasing activity or rapid expansion upon the theoretical, practical, and political landscape of literacy. In many ways, those regions of development reflect the changing society in which we live and in which literacy operates. For example, among the widespread theoretical transformations that have changed the global landscape of education, and thus literacy, is the apparent shift from strongly individualistic to social conceptions of knowledge and knowing. In essence, the nature of learning and development in past decades was described in terms of theories or models that dealt almost exclusively with cognition or changes in an individual’s mind. The goal was to improve the quantity and quality of one’s existing or prior knowledge or to enhance the efficiency with which one’s knowledge could be activated and applied. Today, by comparison, there is much more concern with the social nature of learning and development. This change is manifest in the growing interest in the influence of group membership on literacy achievement and practice (see Gender and Reading, and Social Nature of Literacy). It is also visible in the way that those groups construct shared knowledge through cooperation and collaboration (see Constructivism, Social Constructivism, and Social Nature of Literacy). Those transformations from individual to collective perspectives are reflected both explicitly and subtly in the literacy encyclopedia entries. For example, the encyclopedia includes an ex- xxviii Introduction plicit point-counterpoint discussion of schema theory. Schema theory was a theoretical model of individualistic knowledge structures that dominated the literacy domain for much of the 1970s and early 1980s. The premises and richness of that theory are considered herein (see Prior Knowledge and Misconceptions, Schema Theory, and Story Grammar), along with the limitations or shortcomings of that theory as seen from a sociocontextual vantage point. In addition, there are many subtle markers of the shift to more socially constructed, socially shared models of knowledge and knowing. The discussions of constructivism and context in literacy, for instance, address this theoretical shift within the literacy community, as do entries dealing with social orientations (see Gender and Writing, Graffiti, and Resistant Reading) and socially shared practices (see Book Clubs, The Discussion Web, and Scaffolded Literacy Instruction). Moreover, beyond social membership or orientation, there are clusters of entries in this encyclopedia that mirror the growing presence of computer-based technologies. Had I sketched literacy’s landscape in the 1970s, I would have seen scant evidence of technology—perhaps a few references to television or the media. In today’s mapping, however, the outcropping of computer-based terminology is distinctive and casts a large shadow over the domain of literacy. That shadow extends to pedagogical practice (see Computer-Assisted Instruction, Distance Learning, and Electronic Jigsaw) and student assessment (see Dynamic Assessment). It also embraces the basic conceptualization of text (see Multiple Texts) and modes of communication (see Instant Messaging, and Reading Online). Despite its visible presence, computer-based technology’s overall impact on the nature, acquisition, and practice of literacy remains ill-determined. That is largely attributable to the fact that these technological changes are occurring at such a rapid pace that the literacy community has been unable to extensively and thoroughly track their effects. It will be interesting to return to that dimension of the literacy landscape in the future to see whether the community has developed a richer, more sophisticated understanding of the impact of technology on the prevailing climate and culture of the domain. The areas of rapid expansion are also suggestive of powerful political and pedagogical under- currents transforming the broader educational environment to which literacy belongs. The time and attention devoted to the topic of assessment is a monument to those powerful forces. Assessment has always been a component of the literacy landscape. In decades past, however, the conversations about assessment would have dealt primarily with individual diagnosis. Vestiges of those conversations still dot the current landscape (see Cloze Procedure, Informal Reading Inventory, Reading Diagnosis, and Reading-Interest Inventories). Alongside those more individual and more traditional assessment landmarks are new and imposing structures. These new monoliths represent efforts to broaden our assessment repertoire (see Authentic Assessment, and Portfolios). They are also markers of a dramatic shift away from assessment as a means of improving literacy instruction for individual students to a mechanism whereby the government and the public hold districts, schools, administrators, and teachers accountable (see Accountability and Testing, High-Stakes Assessment, National Assessment of Educational Progress, and Public Opinion and Literacy). Although there is much debate and speculation within the literacy community regarding these new assessment thrusts, we have yet to ascertain the long-term benefits or the potential risks they pose. There is no question that literacy is a domain of high activity and interest within the wider educational sphere. Yet there are still regions that remain undercultivated or suffer from neglect, as is to be expected in any complex domain. Two of these fallow areas can be identified by the lack of attention they garner within the extensive compilation in this encyclopedia. The first deals with the relative inattention given to successful or highly competent readers versus those perceived to be at risk or in need. The second considers the discontinuity between key ages and stages of literacy. Historically, the literacy domain, as with the broader educational community, has been deeply concerned with those children, youth, and even adults who have not experienced success or shared in the benefits and pleasures that linguistic competence affords. This focus is both understandable and warranted. The continuing interest in student assessment and the array of programs and interventions aimed at improving students’ performance provide ample evidence xxix Introduction of this orientation (see Bilingual Education, The Even Start Family Literacy Program, The Head Start Program, Reading Recovery, and Title I). Yet logically, there would seem to be as many individuals with the potential to achieve competence in the domain of literacy as those who struggle or have identified needs. If the literacy community is committed to fostering individuals who not only possess minimum competencies but who also find pleasure and fulfillment in literacy acts throughout their lifetime, then it would be wise to reevaluate its consideration of this overlooked region of the landscape. Similarly, there are clusters of activity within the domain related to individuals at differing points in their literacy development. There are those young children just becoming acquainted with this awesome domain of literacy and all the power and potential it presents (see Early Literacy, and Early Literacy Assessment). Further, the experts in these volumes discuss students who have entered a new phase of life and schooling, and for whom the lure and value of literacy has changed for better and for worse (see Adolescent Literacy, and Middle-School Literacy). In addition, there are references within this encyclopedia to even more cognitively and physically mature populations—populations still struggling with the demands that a highly literate society poses (see Adult Literacy Testing, and Developmental and College Reading). Surprisingly, as in times past, when the regions of literacy—reading, writing, speaking, and listening—existed as separate, discrete areas of research and practice, I see limited attempts to integrate these ages and stages of literacy development. That is to say, what is conspicuously absent in the domain of literacy, as marked by this compilation, is any deep understanding of or reflection about the processes and conditions that allow for progression across these regions of growth and change. What confluence of factors and forces urge young children forward from a region of emerging literacy, where they are only becoming acclimated to this immense terrain, to the place where they are willing and able to delve more competently into the resources of the domain? How do their maturing abilities and evolving needs take root and expand into adulthood, ensuring that literacy remains fertile and productive ground throughout their life? These are clearly questions that members of the literacy community are ill-prepared to answer. Further, this void in literacy’s landscape will persist until the community learns to draw on the vast resources of surrounding domains, such as development, motivation, and educational psychology, that can enlighten and inform our research and practice. Such cross-domain fertilization can only enhance the existing landscape and interject new vitality into the literacy domain. As this brief charting reveals, the domain of literacy is not only extensive and rich with resources but also alive with activity. By surveying the informative and well-chosen entries in this encyclopedia, as I have done here, areas of emergent development or undercultivation become more readily apparent. Indeed, this survey reveals the conscious efforts to integrate across regions within the landscape. The emerging areas of new exploration and the concern with literacy’s place within the broader educational, social, cultural, and political contexts are advances to be celebrated. Nevertheless, the literacy community cannot overlook the fact that there are still segments of this domain that it has failed to cultivate or has allowed to wither from neglect. As a community committed to the practice and processes of literacy for all individuals across the lifespan, literacy educators, administrators, and researchers must take this occasion, the landmark publication of Literacy in America, as an opportunity for self-reflection and reassessment. Only then can this community prepare for the next decade of literacy’s research and practice and ensure the continued health and prosperity of this critical domain. Patricia A. Alexander References Gee, J., G. Hull, and C. Lankshear. 1996. The New Work Order: Behind the Language of the New Capitalism. Boulder: Westview Press. Heath, S. B. 1988. Protean Shapes in Literacy Events: Ever-Shifting Oral and Literate Traditions. In E. R. Kintgen, B. M. Moll, and M. Rose, eds., Perspectives on Literacy, pp. 348–370. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Scribner, S., and M. Cole. 1981. The Psychology of Literacy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Street, B. 1984. Literacy in Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. xxx Contributors Ira E. Aaron, Emeritus University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Patricia L. Anders University of Arizona Tucson, Arizona Richard Beach University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Martha A. Adler University of Michigan–Ann Arbor Ann Arbor, Michigan Nicki L. Anzelmo-Skelton Southeastern Louisiana University Hammond, Louisiana Thomas W. Bean University of Nevada–Las Vegas Las Vegas, Nevada Peter Afflerbach University of Maryland– College Park College Park, Maryland Bonnie B. Armbruster University of Illinois– Urbana-Champaign Urbana-Champaign, Illinois Patricia A. Alexander University of Maryland– College Park College Park, Maryland Gwynne Ellen Ash University of Delaware Newark, Delaware JoBeth Allen University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Richard L. Allington University of Florida Gainesville, Florida Janice F. Almasi State University of New York–Buffalo Buffalo, New York Marino C. Alvarez Tennessee State University Nashville, Tennessee Donna E. Alvermann University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Deborah J. Augsburger Lewis University Romeoville, Illinois R. Scott Baldwin Edinboro University of Pennsylvania Edinboro, Pennsylvania Diane Barone University of Nevada–Reno Reno, Nevada Rebecca Barr National-Louis University Evanston, Illinois James F. Baumann University of Georgia Athens, Georgia xxxi E. Jo Ann Belk University of Memphis Memphis, Tennessee Beth Berghoff Indiana University/Purdue University Indianapolis, Indiana Roberta L. Berglund Independent Consultant Oak Brook, Illinois Camille L. Z. Blachowicz National-Louis University Evanston, Illinois Randy Bomer University of Texas Austin, Texas Connie A. Bridge University of Illinois–Chicago Chicago, Illinois Kathryn Brimmer Oakland University Rochester, Michigan Contributors Lorelei R. Brush American Institutes for Research Washington, D.C. Robert C. Calfee University of California– Riverside Riverside, California Tracy Carman Literacy Volunteers of America Syracuse, New York Ronald P. Carver University of Missouri–Kansas City Kansas City, Missouri Marrietta Castle, Emerita Western Illinois University Rock Island, Illinois Earl H. Cheek Jr. Louisiana State University Baton Rouge, Louisiana Meredith Rogers Cherland University of Regina Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada James Christie Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Michelle Commeyras University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Henry T. Dunbar Reading Is Fundamental Washington, D.C. Kathleen E. Cox University of Maryland– College Park College Park, Maryland Pamela J. Dunston Clemson University Clemson, South Carolina Jane L. Davidson, Emerita Northern Illinois University DeKalb, Illinois Jonathan Eakle University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Alan Davis University of Colorado–Denver Denver, Colorado Jacqueline Edmondson The Pennsylvania State University University Park, Pennsylvania Deborah J. Davis Louisiana State University Baton Rouge, Louisiana Laurie Elish-Piper Northern Illinois University DeKalb, Illinois Susan Deese-Roberts University of New Mexico Albuquerque, New Mexico John Elkins University of Queensland Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Martha Dillner University of Houston– Clear Lake Houston, Texas Deborah R. Dillon University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Cynthia B. Elliott Southeastern Louisiana State University Hammond, Louisiana Billie J. Enz Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Caroline T. Clark Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio Carol N. Dixon University of California–Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, California Karen S. Evans Marquette University Milwaukee, Wisconsin Kathy Cochran Furman University Greenville, South Carolina Janice A. Dole University of Utah Salt Lake City, Utah Bettina Fabos University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa Jill E. Cole Wesley College Dover, Delaware Mark Dressman University of Illinois– Urbana-Champaign Urbana-Champaign, Illinois Mark Faust University of Georgia Athens, Georgia John P. Comings Harvard University Cambridge, Massachusetts Ann M. Duffy University of North Carolina–Greensboro Greensboro, North Carolina xxxii Marion Harris Fey State University College of New York–Geneseo Geneseo, New York Contributors Anna Figueira Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Margaret Finders Washington University St. Louis, Missouri Peter J. Fisher National-Louis University Evanston, Illinois Jill Fitzgerald University of North Carolina–Chapel Hill Chapel Hill, North Carolina Amy Seely Flint Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Rona F. Flippo Fitchburg State College Cambridge, Massachusetts James Flood San Diego State University San Diego, California Linda Flower Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Barbara J. Fox North Carolina State University Raleigh, North Carolina Edward Fry, Emeritus Rutgers University Rutgers, New Jersey Lee Galda University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Margaret Gamboa Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Linda B. Gambrell Clemson University Clemson, South Carolina Georgia Earnest García University of Illinois– Urbana-Champaign Urbana-Champaign, Illinois Maryl Gearhart University of California– Berkeley Berkeley, California James Paul Gee University of Wisconsin– Madison Madison, Wisconsin MariAnne George Oakland University Rochester, Michigan Shawn M. Glynn University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Kenneth S. Goodman, Emeritus University of Arizona Tucson, Arizona Yetta M. Goodman University of Arizona Tucson, Arizona Judith L. Green University of California– Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, California Ruth D. Handel Montclair State University Upper Montclair, New Jersey Violet J. Harris University of Illinois– Urbana-Champaign Urbana-Champaign, Illinois Jerome C. Harste Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Douglas K. Hartman University of Pittsburgh Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Kathy N. Headley Clemson University Clemson, South Carolina Harold L. Herber, Emeritus Syracuse University Syracuse, New York Alison H. Heron University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Elfrieda H. Hiebert University of Michigan–Ann Arbor Ann Arbor, Michigan Kathy Highfield Oakland University Rochester, Michigan John Guthrie University of Maryland– College Park College Park, Maryland Richard E. Hodges University of Puget Sound Tacoma, Washington Barbara J. Guzzetti Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Kerry A. Hoffman Purdue University West Lafayette, Indiana Margaret Carmody Hagood College of Charleston Charleston, South Carolina Taylor Holt Rutgers University Rutgers, New Jersey Diane Hamm Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan Stephen E. Hornstein St. Cloud State University St. Cloud, Minnesota xxxiii Contributors George G. Hruby University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Michele Knobel Central Queensland University Queensland, Australia Cynthia Lewis University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa Sarah Hudelson Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Bethel H. Kogut Laubach Literacy International Syracuse, New York Mitzi Lewison Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Cynthia R. Hynd University of Illinois–Chicago Chicago, Illinois Kimi Kondo-Brown University of Hawaii–Manoa Honolulu, Hawaii Jimmy D. Lindsey Southern University Baton Rouge, Louisiana Gay Ivey University of Maryland– College Park College Park, Maryland Susan P. Kornuta East Baton Rouge Parish Schools Baton Rouge, Louisiana Laura R. Lipsett The Ohio Center for Essential School Reform Reynoldsburg, Ohio Bob W. Jerrolds, Emeritus North Georgia College and State University Oahlonega, Georgia Stephen Krashen University of Southern California Los Angeles, California Rebecca Luce-Kapler Queens University Kingston, Ontario, Canada Jerry L. Johns, Emeritus Northern Illinois University DeKalb, Illinois Linda D. Labbo University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Denise Johnson College of William and Mary Williamsburg, Virginia Missy Laine University of Cincinnati Cincinnati, Ohio Mary Johnston Oakland University Rochester, Michigan Colin Lankshear University of Ballarat Ballarat, Australia Michael L. Kamil Stanford University Palo Alto, California Diane Lapp San Diego State University San Diego, California Wendy C. Kasten Kent State University Kent, Ohio Christine Leland Indiana University/Purdue University Indianapolis, Indiana Francis E. Kazemek St. Cloud State University St. Cloud, Minnesota Laurie Kingsley University of Missouri– Columbia Columbia, Missouri Noma LeMoine Los Angeles Unified School District Los Angeles, California Susan Davis Lenski Illinois State University Normal, Illinois xxxiv Allan Luke University of Queensland Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Carmen Luke University of Queensland Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Jeff MacSwan Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Yolanda Majors University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Anthony V. Manzo California State University–Fullerton Fullerton, California Ula Manzo University of California– Fullerton Fullerton, California Peggy VanLeirsburg Marciniec University of Wisconsin– Superior Superior, Wisconsin Contributors Howard Margolis Queens College of the City University of New York Queens, New York Katherine Maria College of New Rochelle New Rochelle, New York Larry Mikulecky Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Mary Jane Mitchell North Carolina State University Raleigh, North Carolina Prisca Martens Towson University Towson, Maryland Elizabeth Birr Moje University of Michigan– Ann Arbor Ann Arbor, Michigan Mona W. Matthews Georgia State University Atlanta, Georgia Karla J. Möller Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Sandra McCormick, Emerita Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio Charles Monaghan Independent Scholar Brooklyn, New York Kathleen M. McCoy Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona E. Jennifer Monaghan Brooklyn College of the City University of New York Brooklyn, New York Ann McGill-Franzen University of Florida Gainesville, Florida Michael C. McKenna Georgia Southern University Savannah, Georgia Virginia R. Monseau Youngstown State University Youngstown, Ohio David W. Moore Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Maureen McLaughlin East Stroudsberg University of Pennsylvania East Stroudsberg, Pennsylvania Sharon Arthur Moore Peoria Unified School District Glendale, Arizona Susan McMahon National-Louis University Evanston, Illinois Gretchen Morrison Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Jeff McQuillan California State University–Fullerton Fullerton, California Timothy G. Morrison Brigham Young University Provo, Utah John Micklos Jr. Reading Today Newark, Delaware Lesley Mandel Morrow Rutgers University Rutgers, New Jersey Jacqueline Y. Munyer University of Maryland College Park, Maryland xxxv Donna M. Murphy Edinboro University of Pennsylvania Edinboro, Pennsylvania K. Denise Muth University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Allan Neilsen Mount Saint Vincent University Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada Lorri Neilsen Mount Saint Vincent University Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada Susan Neuman University of Michigan–Ann Arbor Ann Arbor, Michigan Sherrie L. Nist University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Stephen P. Norris University of Alberta Calgary, Alberta, Canada David G. O’Brien University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Donna M. Ogle National-Louis University Evanston, Illinois Penny Oldfather University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Michael F. Opitz University of Northern Colorado Greeley, Colorado Nancy Padak Kent State University Kent, Ohio Contributors †Ann Watts Pailliotet Whitman College Walla Walla, Washington Gaoyin Qian Lehman College Bronx, New York Annemarie Sullivan Palincsar University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan Taffy E. Raphael University of Illinois–Chicago Chicago, Illinois Jeanne R. Paratore Boston University Boston, Massachusetts Timothy Rasinski Kent State University Kent, Ohio Lavada Jacumin Parmer University of Mobile Mobile, Alabama John E. Readence University of Nevada–Las Vegas Las Vegas, Nevada P. Elizabeth Pate University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Lynn Reddy National Institute for Literacy Washington, D.C. P. David Pearson University of California– Berkeley Berkeley, California David Reinking University of Georgia Athens, Georgia M. Christina Pennington Clemson University Clemson, South Carolina Kay Pentzien Oakland University Rochester, Michigan Stephen Phelps Buffalo State College Buffalo, New York Linda M. Phillips University of Alberta Calgary, Alberta, Canada Gay Su Pinnell Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio Cheryl Pocius Oakland University Rochester, Michigan Lynne Hebert Remson Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona D. Ray Reutzel Southern Utah University Cedar City, Utah Carole S. Rhodes Adelphi University Garden City, New York Victoria Gentry Ridgeway Clemson University Clemson, South Carolina Pat Rigg American Language and Literacy Tucson, Arizona Victoria J. Risko Peabody College of Vanderbilt University Nashville, Tennessee Victoria Purcell-Gates Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan xxxvi Richard Robinson University of Missouri– Columbia Columbia, Missouri Theresa Rogers University of British Columbia Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Kellie Rolstad Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Jonathan Rose Drew University Madison, New Jersey Nancy L. Roser University of Texas–Austin Austin, Texas Kathleen Roskos John Carroll University University Heights, Ohio Martha Rapp Ruddell Sonoma State University Rohnert Park, California William H. Rupley Texas A&M University College Station, Texas Leslie S. Rush University of Wyoming Laramie, Wyoming Martha H. Rusnak Lewis University Romeoville, Illinois Terry Salinger American Institute for Research Washington, D.C. S. Jay Samuels University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Contributors Amy C. Sass Rutgers University Rutgers, New Jersey Norman A. Stahl Northern Illinois University DeKalb, Illinois Diane L. Schallert University of Texas–Austin Austin, Texas Steven A. Stahl University of Illinois Champaign-Urbana, Illinois Barbara R. Schirmer Miami University Oxford, Ohio Patricia Lambert Stock Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan Judith A. Scott University of California– anta Cruz Santa Cruz, California Jeanne Swafford Mississippi State University Starkville, Mississippi Timothy Shanahan University of Illinois–Chicago Chicago, Illinois Kathy G. Short University of Arizona Tucson, Arizona Michele L. Simpson University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Yvonne Siu-Runyan University of Northern Colorado Greeley, Colorado Peter Smagorinsky University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Karen Smith Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona M. Cecil Smith Northern Illinois University DeKalb, Illinois Michael W. Smith Rutgers University Rutgers, New Jersey William Earl Smith Ohio University Athens, Ohio Susan Swan Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Janet P. Swartz Abt Associates Cambridge, Massachusetts Anne P. Sweet U.S. Department of Education Washington, D.C. Joy Sweet Right to Read Washington, D.C. Barbara M. Taylor University of Minnesota Minneapolis, Minnesota Shane Templeton University of Nevada–Reno Reno, Nevada Robert J. Tierney University of British Columbia Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Cheri Foster Triplett Virginia Tech University Blacksburg, Virginia Brenda Turnbull Policy Studies Associates Washington, D.C. xxxvii Norman J. Unrau California State University– Los Angeles Los Angeles, California Richard T. Vacca Kent State University Kent, Ohio Sam Vagenas University of Phoenix Phoenix, Arizona Sheila W. Valencia University of Washington Seattle, Washington Johan W. van der Jagt Southeastern Louisiana University Hammond, Louisiana Suzanne E. Wade University of Utah Salt Lake City, Utah Kenneth J. Weiss Nazareth College of Rochester Rochester, New York Mary Ann Wham University of Wisconsin–Whitewater Whitewater, Wisconsin Terrence G. Wiley Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona Arlette Ingram Willis University of Illinois–Chicago Chicago, Illinois Peter Winograd University of New Mexico Albuquerque, New Mexico Linda S. Wold Purdue University Calumet Hammond, Indiana Contributors Carolyn F. Woods Harrison County Schools Gulfport, Mississippi Crystal Marie Wooten Phoenix Parks, Recreation, and Library Program Phoenix, Arizona xxxviii A Ability Grouping formed within classes also became more common during the 1920s and 1930s. Researchers in the 1930s asked whether the practice of ability grouping really led to the creation of a group of students who were similar in their knowledge. Upon investigation, they found that student performance in one area (e.g., reading) was not highly related to that in another (e.g., math) and that classes formed to be high ability and low ability in a particular area were, in fact, highly overlapping in that same area when measured by a different test. A second question pursued by researchers asked whether students grouped by ability learned more than comparable students in more heterogeneous groups. The literature includes two major waves of reviews on achievement outcomes, one in the 1930s and the other in the 1960s. Reviewers of both periods emphasize the equivocal and inconsistent results from study to study and criticize the quality of the quasi-experimental studies. Yet, basing their studies mainly on the same evidence, they draw quite different conclusions. Whereas early reviewers tended to conclude that ability grouping benefited “slow” students, many in the later group of reviewers discerned a tendency for ability-grouped high achievers to learn more than comparable students in heterogeneous groups but for abilitygrouped low achievers to do less well than those in heterogeneous groups. Concern about whether and how students should be organized for classroom instruction stimulated theoretical and ideological debate. One assumption underlying ability grouping and tracking strategies is that the content of instruction needs to be matched to the prior knowledge of students for them to realize optimal learning. Yet if ability groups were not as Ability grouping refers to the division of students into groups on the basis of their intelligence or achievement. Within-class grouping occurs mainly in elementary-school classes and mainly for reading instruction. For within-class grouping, group membership can be modified over time since all groups are taught by a single teacher and different groups can be formed for other content areas. Further, in those subject areas in which the class is instructed as a unit, children have the opportunity to interact with classmates who vary in ability. Between-class grouping in elementary schools or tracking in high schools refers to the assignment of students to classes on the basis of achievement in an effort to increase the homogeneity among students within classes. In these classes, students interact only with other students of roughly similar achievement. Historical Literature on Ability Grouping The impulse to regularize education in the United States goes back at least to the time of Horace Mann in the 1830s. Ability grouping into classes was first documented somewhat later in 1862 in the St. Louis schools. Although it is more difficult to date the emergence of ability grouping within classrooms, one of the earliest references, found in the Story Hour Readers Manual (American Book Company, 1913) suggests separate groups for those who are “slow and need more assistance” and those who “progress rapidly.” Schools at the turn of the century expanded rapidly as new waves of immigration occurred. Educators responded with a variety of school reforms, including various grouping schemes identified with their respective cities (Detroit; Joplin, Missouri; Denver; Gary, Indiana; Winnetka, Illinois; Dalton, Georgia). Reading groups 1 Ability Grouping Students in this picture are grouped by ability (Elizabeth Crews) thing in different schools or even in the same school. The educational experiences of students vary markedly across tracks or groups, depending on the level and quality of the curricular materials used, the capabilities of teachers, and the aggregate characteristics of students from class to class. Reviews of this literature focus mainly on the results from case studies and surveys (Barr and Dreeben, 1991; Oakes, Gamoran, and Page, 1992). Concern with equality spawned several lines of inquiry, some going beyond narrow ideological concerns and leading to conceptualizations of ability grouping as part of the social systems of schools and classrooms. Some of the more interesting research of the 1970s and 1980s stems from concern with the mechanisms, such as curriculum differentiation and instructional quality, through which social background might influence education and life chances. Rebecca Barr and Robert Dreeben (1991), for example, argue that the rate of curricular presentation is responsive to average group ability, which in turn influences students’ learning. homogeneous as generally assumed and if results concerning learning were equivocal, could the practice be justified? Questions focused not only on whether the practice of ability grouping was educationally sound but also on whether it violated the principle of equal educational opportunity. Were some children being discriminated against by being placed in low-level reading groups or in low-ability classes? The Moses v. Washington Parish School Board (1971) decision, for example, was based on the argument that the practice was not acceptable since educational research does not justify its use. Research on Ability Grouping in the 1970s and 1980s Research in the 1970s and 1980s reflects trends in educational research toward examining the processes through descriptive research methods. During this period, the research on ability grouping became more broadly conceptualized, focusing on the instruction that ability groups receive. It has become clear that membership in an ability group or track does not mean the same 2 Ability Grouping terest in such issues in the 1990s. A number of sociological studies have focused on tracking in the middle grades and high school (see, for example, Dauber, Alexander, and Entwisle, 1996). Relatively little recent research has focused on ability grouping within elementary-school classes, however. Three influences have combined to shape the practice of ability grouping in elementary-school reading instruction. First, as mentioned previously, constructivist theories of learning and views on “whole language” led in the 1980s and 1990s to experimentation with total-class instruction and more individualized and flexible forms of grouping. Second, immigration policies in the previous decades resulted in an increase of students from other nations in American schools. Third, initiatives to include students with special needs in regular classes are encouraged (mainstreaming, inclusion). Given this set of conditions, what might their impact be on ability grouping? Shifts away from grouping on the basis of ability for instruction in elementary schools have been striking. Sharon Kletzien (1996) examined the reading programs in 300 schools, nationally recognized by the Blue Ribbon School Recognition programs of the U.S. Department of Education. From the subset with clearly described grouping patterns, the dominant pattern of ability groups within heterogeneous classes decreased from 92 percent in 1985–1986 to 52 percent in 1991–1992. Increases showed during that same period for the use of heterogeneous and flexible groups in heterogeneous classes (6 percent to 36 percent). However, because these were “Blue Ribbon” schools, the shift would likely be somewhat less in schools more generally. Yet even for award winning programs, a decline from 92 percent to 52 percent in a well-established practice such as ability grouping is remarkable. An extensive case-study literature also documents the shift from ability-grouped reading instruction to total-class instruction and to more flexible forms of classroom grouping. Many reports describe approaches used by teachers to form heterogeneous and flexible groups (e.g., Radencich and McKay, 1995). Some case studies focus on the implementation of literature circles and reading workshops; others describe a combination of alternative formats. Similarly, analyses of frequently used basal-series manuals reveal that suggestions are made for instruction in student-led groups, for students working in pairs and individually, as well as in teacher-led small groups. In addition to studies of grouping practices, more interpretive forms of research focus on teachers’ and students’ perceptions of ability grouping and more flexible forms of grouping. Sally Watson Moody, Sharon Vaughn, and Jeanne Shay Schumm (1997), for example, interviewed twenty-nine third-grade teachers about grouping decisions and practices. The teachers reported feeling constrained by administrative demands for them to provide total-class reading instruction; most would have preferred using mixed-ability grouping arrangements. In a similar study involving teachers from grades three to five, most reported combining whole language with basal instruction. The most common grouping formats for reading instruction tended to be whole class, followed by small groups, and then pairs. For each of these formats, teachers tended to favor mixed-ability over same-ability membership. A somewhat different perspective on ability grouping was elicited from students about their work in mixed-ability groups. Third-grade students who were interviewed expressed concerns about the noise and other distractions in small groups and the difficulty in getting help from teachers. Lower-achieving students were concerned about being teased and embarrassed by peers; higher achievers focused on the slow pace and limited challenge of the work. Increases in immigration concurrent with increases in the number of special learners in regular classrooms have led to highly diverse classes. Unlike the first few decades of the twentieth century, when increased student diversity led to the adoption of ability grouping, the responses of educators are currently more mixed. Moreover, increased class diversity has occurred at a time when teachers are in the process of implementing total-class instruction and more flexible forms of classroom organization. This coincidence of forces has spawned a body of research, much of it conducted by researchers from the field of special education, to assess the effectiveness of alternative ways of organizing students for instruction. The implicit argument in this research is not that ability group represents a viable solution but that other forms of 4 Ability Grouping Concerning the structure of groups, the assertion is repeatedly made that ability groups, once established, are highly stable. Observational evidence, however, suggests some mobility between ability groups within classes, in the range of 20 to 35 percent in grades one to three and from 10 to 25 percent in grades four to seven. It also shows that teachers change the number and composition of groups over the course of the school year. Teachers show different patterns of group changes; more effective teachers tend to move more students up than down. There has been relatively little documentation of the stability of ability-group membership from year to year, but there appears to be a high degree of stability in group membership during the primary years, when ability grouping is employed. Along somewhat different lines, other researchers consider how individualized versus more traditional forms of teaching influence classroom authority, friendship among students, the attribution of status, and collaboration among teachers. They argue that traditional forms of instruction (restricted curricular tasks, ability grouping, and comparative grading) serve as the occasion for attributing low status to low achievers, thereby depressing their interaction and learning. Explanations based on case studies of classrooms and schools tend to focus on the quality of instructional interaction, the perceptions and attitudes of participants, and their interpretations of events. Rather than positing models in which events and activities are examined for their relationships, these researchers describe the intentions of participants and the constellations of factors that characterize the instructional experiences of low- and high-achieving groups and classes. Case studies in elementary and high schools examine the academic task characteristics of low and high groups. With respect to the curriculum, low-group members typically cover less material, complete simpler assignments, and perform more drill and skill work than students in higher-achieving groups. Although content differences have been emphasized, these differences may be of minor significance in relation to the similarities in curriculum that characterize U.S. elementary-school instruction: Most children in a grade in the same school use the same reading textbooks and related materials, and although groups may proceed at somewhat different paces through the materials, the curricular tasks are essentially the same. Case studies also reveal the nature of the interaction during instruction. In elementary schools, low reading group members tend to experience a greater number of intrusions and less time on task than do students in higher-achieving groups. Lower-group members read orally more often, focus on smaller units of print, have decoding rather than meaning emphasized, are asked more questions that require recall of information rather than reasoning, receive different prompts from teachers, and are provided more structure through the provision of advanced organizers for lessons than their higher-group peers. Although some instructional researchers also claim that low-group members receive less instructional time, others have not found differential time allocations. Findings are similar in middle- and highschool classes. Interpretive studies at the secondary level link the instructional treatment of students to teacher assumptions about the potential learning of their students. Because they are perceived as being unwilling or incapable of completing academic work, they are given simplified tasks and learn correspondingly little. Similarly, it is typically concluded that low-achieving students are being treated unfairly and that their instruction is causing them to achieve poorly. It is assumed that low-group members would learn more if they participated in more challenging instruction. But before it can be concluded that the same instruction is appropriate, low groups and classes need to receive and respond positively to the same kind of instruction that their higherachieving peers receive. Only a limited number of reviewers have explored the results from survey studies and casestudy descriptions in combination. Adam Gamoran and Mark Berends (1987), for example, draw from both types of studies to explore how ability grouping works. They not only explore the mechanism intervening between group structure and learning but also provide rich descriptions of classroom events. Recent Research on Ability Grouping Researchers have shown intense interest in ability grouping during most of the twentieth century, but they have shown especially selective in3 Accountability and Testing Moody, Sally W., Sharon Vaughn, and Jeanne S. Schumm. 1997. “Instructional Grouping for Reading: Teachers’ Views.” Remedial and Special Education 18 (6):347–356. Oakes, Jeannie, Adam Gamoran, and Reba N. Page. 1992. “Curriculum Differentiation: Opportunities, Outcomes, and Meanings.” In P. W. Jackson, ed., Handbook of Research on Curriculum, pp. 570–608. New York: Macmillan. Radencich, Marguerite C., and Lyn J. McKay, eds. 1995. Flexible Grouping for Literacy in the Elementary Grades. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. grouping are more effective than total-class instruction. In one of the most comprehensive reviews of this literature, Batya Elbaum and colleagues (1999) employed the method of metaanalysis to statistically integrate the results from research that met specified criteria for inclusion. Using this approach, they summarized the findings from studies conducted between 1975 and 1995. The majority of studies (sixteen out of twenty) involved regular as well as special-education students (learning disability [LD] and behavioral disorder [BD]) working in pairs; the remaining studies involved students working in small groups or combined grouping formats. The results from this meta-analysis indicated that students working in pairs learned more than comparable students receiving total-class instruction. Some research on ability groups and tracking, then, continued into the 1990s, but this research focused mainly on ability grouping and tracking at the middle- and high-school level. In elementary schools, by contrast, there was little examination of the nature or consequences of ability grouping. Research focused instead on alternatives to total-class instruction such as learning in pairs and small groups and their effect on learning, particularly for groups of special learners. Rebecca Barr Accountability and Testing Accountability, according to Webster’s Third New International Dictionary, means being responsible, being answerable. Educators have always been responsible for students’ success in school (however it may be defined), and students have always been responsible for their learning. In this sense, accountability in education is not new. The more pragmatic definition, however, and certainly the one that is common today, connotes serious consequences—assigning praise or blame, rewards or punishments—to the people and institutions responsible (see High-Stakes Assessment). Although many believe that such high stakes mark a new twist on accountability, high stakes can be traced back to the 1860s when inspectors in England and Wales toured the country examining children’s reading, arithmetic, and learning in other subjects and then used the results to allocate teachers’ salaries. More recent history locates accountability in the original Title I of the Elementary and Secondary Education Act (ESEA) (1965), which demanded that evaluation and accountability be tied to federal funds for the education of children in poverty (see Title I). This created an enormous need for normreferenced tests, and it cemented the link between testing and the accountability component of Title I; no other type of measure could have systematically provided information on the progress and achievement of thousands of students across the country. Later, in the 1970s and 1980s, the minimum competency testing (MCT) movement further strengthened the link between testing and accountability, and it reified high-stakes testing. During this time, students were required to pass a test of basic, minimallevel skills in order to graduate from high school. Along with the high-stakes concept, MCT also See Also Context in Literacy; Cooperative Learning; Individualized Reading; Peer Discussion References Barr, Rebecca, and Robert Dreeben. 1991. “Grouping Students for Reading Instruction.” In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 2, pp. 885–910. New York: Longman. Dauber, Susan L., Karl L. Alexander, and Doris R. Entwisle. 1996. “Tracking and Transitions through the Middle Grades: Channeling Educational Trajectories.” Sociology of Education 69 (4):290–307. Elbaum, Batya, Sharon Vaughn, Marie Hughes, and Sally W. Moody. 1999. “Grouping Practices and Reading Outcomes for Students with Disabilities.” Exceptional Children 63 (3):399–415. Gamoran, Adam, and Mark Berends. 1987. “The Effects of Stratification in Secondary Schools: Synthesis of Survey and Ethnographic Research.” Review of Educational Research 57 (4):415–435. Kletzien, Sharon B. 1996. “Reading Programs in Nationally Recognized Elementary Schools.” Reading Research and Instruction 35 (3):260–274. 5 Accountability and Testing introduced what is now a well-accepted view of testing—that testing can serve as both a measure and a lever of reform. Not only could tests provide information about students’ mastery of basic skills, but teachers and students could also use these tests to identify the content of instruction. The 1980s and early 1990s saw an expansion of the use of standardized tests for accountability, accompanied by concerns about their validity and negative effects on teaching and learning. creased as well. For example, by 2008, students in twenty-four states will have to pass a state exam to graduate from high school, and by 2003, seven states will required children to pass state tests to be promoted in certain grades (“A Better Balance,” 2001). At the teacher level, test results are being used in some states to reward teachers monetarily for good student performance, and they are influencing the retention and recruitment of teachers. And at the school level, test results are increasingly being used for state governance of education. Schools are being accredited based on scores, and some states have even given their education departments the power to close, take over, or overhaul chronically low-performing schools. Some research suggests that in the current environment, there may be no “lowstakes” accountability, even when states try to minimize the consequences. Teachers in states with public reporting but low stakes seem to experience as much pressure as those teaching in states with high-stakes tests. States have responded to the accountability call in a wide variety of ways. For example, they have set different cut-off scores for acceptable performance, included different students in the testing program, used different measures for assessing performance (i.e., performance assessments, multiple-choice tests, classroom work), tested at different grades, and provided different levels of rewards, sanctions, and supports. Less apparent are the different ways that states have developed to report student progress and to rank schools. The most common method is to report absolute performance, or current status, often in terms of the average score for students at the tested grade levels or the percent of students who meet or exceed preestablished standards. Florida, for example, uses this method to grade schools from A to F, based on students’ performance on the Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test. Many states that report current status also include a measure of improvement, sometimes referred to as value-added. Some states compare scores for students at the same grade level over two years (i.e., fourth grade in 2000 compared with fourth grade in 2001); others compare school performance of groups of students across years (fourth grade in 2000, fifth grade in 2001). Some states track the performance of individual students across years, and still others measure the change in percent of students who fall in the The Current Context The current situation with regard to testing and accountability, based in the standards movement of the 1990s, grows out of this long history (see Standards). Standards-based reform was conceptualized to address some of the problems of the past: overemphasis on basic, low-level skills; narrowing of the curriculum to fit the test; inflated test scores; test-like instructional methods; and differential expectations for students. Rather than having a test set the content of instruction, educators at state and local levels have defined demanding content standards (what students should know and be able to do) and performance standards (how good is good enough) that apply to all students, including those served by Title I. The indicator of success is students’ performance rather than district compliance with state regulations or other “inputs” such as course offerings, personnel qualifications, and the like. In addition, the standards movement calls for several other reforms: (1) assessments that are aligned with these standards, (2) student performance that is standards-based rather than norm-referenced, (3) development of new performance-based assessments, and (4) attention to providing high-quality opportunities for students to learn. Although these changes mark a dramatic shift in education, accountability remains the linchpin in the system, and state tests remain the primary source of data for accountability. In fact, as of 2001, forty-eight states publicly reported test scores, forty-five of them reported school-level data, and thirty-one set cutoff scores for student performance. Thirtythree states used, or planned to use, test scores to hold schools accountable, and fourteen of them relied solely on test scores; no other data were used (“A Better Balance,” 2001). In the current accountability environment, stakes for schools, students, and teachers have in6 Accountability and Testing lowest performing group. Finally, states also vary in the degree to which they include indicators other than test performance in the accountability system (e.g., dropout rate, mobility, socioeconomic status), but these indicators rarely carry enough weight to offset the test scores. With such variability in accountability systems, it is extremely difficult to evaluate the effectiveness of these accountability systems or of the standardsbased reform effort. Accountability models that include different factors and different ways of evaluating progress yield dramatically different results and lead to substantively different conclusions. high-stakes accountability has replicated some of the concerns associated with earlier testing efforts (e.g., narrowing of the curriculum, inflated test scores, extended test preparation), in spite of the fact that content standards are supposed to drive instruction rather than a test. Furthermore, although many new tests tend to focus on higher-level understanding and include a combination of open-ended and multiple-choice items, there are concerns about the measures themselves (i.e., alignment with standards, reliability, validity). Second, many are concerned that policymakers underestimate how long it takes to implement new content standards and substantive changes in curriculum and instruction. In an effort to motivate change and set high expectations for all students, policymakers have often set performance standards on state tests that are unrealistic for students and teachers to reach in a short time. When the emphasis is on quick results, the tendency is to focus too much on boosting test scores and less on instigating deep and meaningful changes in teaching and learning. Some measurement experts have suggested that test scores might be better used as trailing indicators rather than as leading indicators of change. In other words, test scores are more likely to show change after schools have ample opportunity to put into place new instructional strategies and curriculum; that is, after opportunity to learn, for both teachers and students, has been assured. A third caution is related to the first two: schools, teachers, and students must be provided with needed support if they are going to be held accountable for improvement. That support may be in the form of professional development for teachers and administrators, additional resources for struggling students, higher pay for educators, materials and texts for instruction, and the like. Evidence is clear that simply implementing new assessments or accountability requirements will not, by itself, produce lasting and meaningful change. Yet recent research suggests professional development opportunities are inadequate to meet teachers’ needs. In 2001, forty-two states provided funds for professional development, but the funds didn’t necessarily go to all the schools and districts in a state, and the discrepancies across states were dramatic (e.g., less than $100 per teacher each year in Utah, Concerns and Issues High-stakes testing and accountability are implemented by policymakers with the intention of improving education. Many believe that such accountability in a standards-based system can create incentives for educators to focus on important content outcomes, help policymakers allocate resources to schools in need, and inspire effort on the part of teachers, administrators, and students. Nevertheless, concerns about highstakes accountability are well documented in practitioner journals, scholarly journals and reports, and position statements by professional organizations (e.g., American Educational Research Association; International Reading Association). The issues are too complex and too numerous to present in this entry (for a review, see Fuhrman, 1999; Linn, 2000, 2001). I highlight three of the more pressing issues to give a sense of the difficult terrain. First, there are concerns about overreliance on test scores as the primary accountability indicator. Most educators and measurement experts caution against using any single test to make high-stakes decisions, especially about individual students, yet the evidence is clear that state test scores continue to dominate accountability efforts. One reason is that even though there is unequivocal support for using multiple measures, just what these other measures might be or how they would work is unclear. Furthermore, tests are relatively inexpensive, can be externally mandated, are quickly implemented, and yield results that are visible and accessible to the public. Against such forces, expanding accountability indicators will be difficult. This current overreliance on a single test for 7 Active Listening Active Listening $3,500 per teacher each year in Alaska). Similarly, although fifteen states required that students receive additional help if they failed to pass the test to earn a high-school diploma, only nine paid for such assistance (“A Better Balance,” 2001). Active listening is the ability to listen constructively to make meaning from the utterances of others. It requires listening with interest and acceptance and has an empathetic component— being aware of the speaker’s feelings and being able to allow yourself to appreciate the speaker’s point of reference (Studer, 1993–1994). The listener’s attitude is critical in active listening and includes the following: (1) desire to understand what the speaker is saying, (2) desire to be helpful to the speaker, (3) desire to accept the speaker’s feeling regardless of his or her disclaimer of your own feelings, (4) knowledge that feelings can be transitory, not permanent, and (5) ability to perceive the speaker as having an identity separate from your own. Active listening is guided by the following rules: noticing attitudes and feelings conveyed in the message; telling speakers what you heard them say; using words in retelling that are different from the speaker’s without changing the meaning of the utterance; avoiding sending messages that are evaluative or questioning (unless clarification is needed); and paraphrasing the utterance to show your understanding of what was said. In active listening, there are four basic techniques that are used to help the speaking-listening process. First, active listeners encourage speakers in order to convey interest and keep the speaker talking. They do this by using noncommittal words or phrases with a positive tone, for example, “I see,” “Uh-huh,” “That’s interesting.” Second, active listeners restate the speaker’s utterance to show that they are listening and that they understand the facts of the utterance. They do this by restating the basic ideas and facts that they heard and say, for example, “If I understand your comments correctly, your idea is. . . .” Third, active listeners demonstrate that they understand the speaker’s feelings; they do this by restating the speaker’s basic feelings, for example, by responding, “You feel that . . . ” or “You were disturbed by . . . .” Finally, active listeners summarize by pulling important facts and ideas together. They do this by restating and summarizing both ideas and feelings that the speaker has expressed. Examples are “These seem to be the key ideas you mentioned” or “If I understand you, you feel this way about the situation.” Cognitive active listening The Future Accountability and testing are guaranteed to be in our educational future. In fact, indications are that the role of both is increasing. In the face of such efforts, there is a pressing need to address existing concerns and issues. Kenneth Sirotnik and Kathy Kimball (1999) suggest that we need standards for standards-based accountability systems, and indeed, many knowledgeable and thoughtful people have offered strategies for dealing with the challenges. As might be expected, recommendations focus on many facets of the problem, including establishing a more comprehension accountability system; improving tests and models for evaluating progress; providing more resources for students, teachers, and schools; and supporting and elevating the role of classroom-based assessment. These recommendations are likely to focus the attention of educators, researchers, and policymakers in the coming years as accountability and testing continue to occupy a prominent place on the reform agenda. Sheila W. Valencia See Also High-Stakes Assessment; Reading Assessment; Standards; Title I; Writing Assessment References “A Better Balance: Standards, Tests, and the Tools to Succeed: Quality Counts.” 2001. Education Week 20:17. Fuhrman, Susan H. 1999. “The New Accountability: Consortium for Policy Research in Education Policy Brief.” Available: http://www.cpre.org. Linn, Robert L. 2000. “Assessments and Accountability.” Educational Researcher 29 (2):4–16. ———. 2001. The Design and Evaluation of Educational Assessment and Accountability Systems. CSE Technical Report 539. Los Angeles: National Center for Research on Evaluation, Standards, and Student Testing. Sirotnik, Kenneth A., and Kathy Kimball. 1999. “Standards for Standards-Based Accountability Systems.” Phi Delta Kappan 81 (3):209–214. 8 Children listen actively to taped messages (Elizabeth Crews) Activity Theory requires the listener to construct meaning through synthesizing, analyzing, interpreting, and evaluating the messages of speakers. It is an active process that demands that the listener construct meaning through inferencing and questioning for clarification. James Flood and Diane Lapp negotiate the values of the societies in which they grow. An example of how studying culture can inform educational practice comes from an ethnography conducted by Shirley Brice Heath (1983). Heath studied a set of small communities in the Carolina piedmont region, including a small African American neighborhood (Trackton), a small white Christian fundamentalist community (Roadville), and the middleclass community with which they shared a school district. She found that the three communities had distinctly different orientations to reading that affected their prospects for success in school. In Trackton, families encouraged high levels of sociability. Children were encouraged to go outside and play and otherwise interact with others in their community. A highly literate person was one who could perform verbally. In contrast, solitary and isolated activities such as quiet reading were viewed as antisocial and were thus discouraged. This community, then, enculturated its members with a belief that literacy is interactive and performative. In Roadville, reading was taught through Bible study. The Bible was treated as a revered text that revealed an absolute, literal truth that was not to be questioned. Children in Roadville, then, were deeply enculturated with a belief that written texts have an invariant meaning that is not open to question. The middle-class families fostered an orientation to reading that more closely matched that of the school. Quiet, solitary reading was encouraged, and discussions of texts allowed for interpretation. The consequences of these three different cultural orientations to reading were that the children from middle-class families had greater success in school. Heath’s study illustrates that understanding the development of whole cultures can help reveal how individuals within those cultures learn how to think over time. Activity theorists argue that it is particularly important to understand cultural differences when students perform differently in school and to rethink educational practice to allow for more equitable access to school success. Changing the context of education can provide people with different developmental trajectories with opportunities to use their culturally learned knowledge in their formal learning. References Studer, Jeannie R. 1993–1994. “Listen So That Parents Will Speak.” Childhood Education (Winter):74–76. Activity Theory According to activity theory, people’s thought patterns originate in the cultural life that surrounds them. In other words, people are born into cultures that have particular values, goals, ways of thinking and acting, and other factors that contribute to a cohesive and orderly society. By participating in the social practices of their cultures, people adopt the ways of thinking that are consistent with life within those cultures. Their thinking thus takes place in relationship with other people, both those who are immediately present and those who have helped to build the culture that they grow into. Activity theory is generally attributed to Aleksei Leont’ev (1981), a student of Lev Vygotsky’s. Vygotsky’s (1978, 1987) sociocultural psychology is thus central to an activity theory perspective. There are three important themes that are central to understanding human development from Vygotsky’s perspective: an emphasis on human development, an assumption that human consciousness has social origins, and a belief that mental processes are mediated by tools and signs. An Emphasis on Human Development Activity theorists are concerned with both the development of whole cultures and the development of individuals within those cultures. Understanding how a culture develops is central to understanding how societies structure life to shape how people within them learn to think and view the world. Activity theorists thus focus on development at several levels, including culture as a whole, the many subcultures that exist within them, the overlapping cultures that affect development in an increasingly global world, and individuals as they appropriate, resist, and 10 Activity Theory An Assumption That Human Consciousness Has Social Origins As Heath’s study shows, cognition has a cultural basis. People learn how to think by taking part in the life around them. In particular, they learn how to think by listening to and participating in the talk that surrounds them. This facet of activity theory helps to explain how people come to hold particular worldviews, and more specifically, how worldviews vary across different cultures. A clear example would be that of a child who grew up in Nazi Germany in the late 1930s. This child would have been surrounded by discourse and artifacts that offered genocide as a legitimate societal goal. Through participation in this culture, the child would have accepted this view and would have been considered a good citizen within the confines of this society for pursuing this goal, even while it was regarded by members of other cultures as heinous and criminal. An individual’s goal of perpetuating Aryan supremacy originates in the overriding goal of Nazi culture and is adopted by members of that society. Belief in Aryan supremacy might be taught directly by the society’s elders, or it might be more subtly suggested through the hidden ways in which the environment promotes development toward these goals. Social life in a Nazi culture is permeated with these beliefs, thus making Aryan supremacy appear to be a natural, unquestionable fact of life. From an educational standpoint, activity theorists attempt to reveal the norms—particularly those that appear to be natural, unquestionable facts of life—that prevail in educational settings and consider their consequences. The norms of the white middle class are so well ingrained in school culture that they appear to be the natural way for school to be conducted. Anyone who comes to school with a different cultural orientation to literacy, behavior, or other routines is judged as deficient, even if that person might be viewed as highly competent in another setting. Entering an environment, such as school, that recognizes and affirms only one way of solving problems creates disadvantages for those not enculturated to those modes of action. Luis Moll and James Greenberg (1990), for instance, were concerned with the poor performance of Hispanic students in U.S. schools. In particular, Moll was troubled that some educational psychologists argued that Hispanic stu- dents were cognitively deficient because of their scores on standardized tests and their performance on school-based tasks. To provide a different perspective on the cognitive competence of Mexican American students, he conducted a community ethnography that documented extensively the cognitive tasks accomplished in everyday life. He found that families, many living on ranches, were skilled at repairing and maintaining sophisticated machinery, planning and operating budgets, extracting medicine from insects, and executing myriad other complex tasks in their home life. He also found that in the Mexican American community, most tasks were carried out in groups. People shared funds of knowledge so that goals could be reached collaboratively. These same students, however skilled at home, would perform poorly in school. Although the Mexican American community valued and encouraged collaboration, collaboration was discouraged and often punished in school. In contrast, schools operated according to traditional U.S. values and emphasized individualism. Mexican American students were thus mismatched with the middle-class values by which the school operated. Furthermore, rather than solving problems in situ, students were evaluated in school according to their ability to solve problems in abstract situations. Moll argued that instead of being incompetent at problem solving, these students were ill-matched with the school’s way of presenting problems to be solved. Moll’s research, like Heath’s, illustrates the ways frameworks for thinking exist first in culture. These frameworks are then adopted by people as they participate in cultural practices over time. From an educational standpoint, it is important to understand which value systems dominate school practice and to look at school failure as potentially a result of mismatches in cultural norms and expectations. A Belief That Mental Processes Are Mediated by Tools and Signs The notion that cognition is mediated by tools and signs refers to the ways people think by means of psychological tools. Many activity theorists focus on speech, believed to be the “tool of tools.” Others have argued that students have a “tool kit” that includes many ways of thinking that schools typically do not recognize. Those who study speech often focus on speech 11 Activity Theory genres, that is, the ways in which speech is orchestrated in whole systems of vocabulary, syntax, tone, and other factors. In an everyday example, a person might speak to a baby using soft tones, a simple vocabulary, repetition, and terms of endearment. This same person would probably switch to a different set of language codes while preparing a legal document. Each of these situations requires knowledge of the appropriate speech genre for successful communication. The speech genre of classrooms is typically associated with the discourse of the white middle class. One characteristic of this speech is its politeness, particularly its indirectness. When teachers want students to do something, rather than ordering them to do so, they suggest that they might do so; for example, they say, “Is this where your scissors belong?” Students from other cultures might find this baffling, expecting instead something along these lines: “Put those scissors on that shelf.” Such students are likely to be confused by the prevailing speech genre of school, and they become defined in school terms as uncooperative because they do not respond appropriately to the indirect imperative. Literacy researchers have found speech genres to be a rich source of study. Carol Lee (1993), for instance, has studied the African American speech genre of signifying—a form of verbal jousting—and finds it central to understanding important works of African American fiction, such as The Color Purple and Their Eyes Were Watching God. She developed instructional strategies to enable urban African American students to study their own language processes and identify their features, and then use that knowledge to inform their reading of sophisticated fiction. She argued that African American students use rich and imaginative figurative speech in their daily lives that is rarely applied to their school learning. Lee’s research illustrates one way that teachers can alter the kinds of speech genres allowed in their classrooms to enable a broader array of students to have access to success. Other researchers have focused on how to broaden not just the speech genres of classrooms but the variety of tools that students can use in their thinking. Peter Smagorinsky (1995) and his colleagues, for instance, have looked at ways teachers can open up students’ cultural tool kits to allow for drawing, music, drama, dance, and other media as means of interpreting literature. These studies have found that when students are given their choice of interpretive media, they typically engage in the same kinds of cognitive processes they use when speaking or writing. Students relate literary characters to personal situations, go through an extensive process of composition (plan, compose, revise, edit, share), work collaboratively, and come to new realizations as they compose. These studies suggest that students’ thinking during literacy events may be enabled by using a variety of literacy tools. Furthermore, a greater range of students can have access to interpretive success than is possible when only written evaluation is provided. Contribution of Activity Theory to Literacy Research Activity theory serves as a useful framework for understanding how and why things happen in particular situations. It serves as a particularly valuable lens for studying issues of cultural diversity, especially when the institution of school must provide an arena in which learners from diverse backgrounds share facilities, spaces, texts, and experiences. When the assumptions that structure life in school are so ingrained and invisible that they appear natural, immovable, and unquestionable, then students enculturated to different ways of interacting and learning appear to be ill-adapted and are often assumed to be cognitively deficient. For educators interested in issues of equity for diverse populations, activity theory provides a perspective with great explanatory power. Furthermore, it suggests that changing the setting of schooling ought to be a key strategy in addressing educational inequity. This direction runs in sharp contrast to efforts to make students from diverse backgrounds more middle class in an effort to improve their school performance. Peter Smagorinsky References Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lee, Carol D. 1993. Signifying as a Scaffold for Literary Interpretation: The Pedagogical Implications of an African-American Discourse Genre. Research Report No. 26. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Leont’ev, A. N. 1981. Problems of the Development of Mind. Moscow: Progress Publishers. 12 Adolescent Literacy cohort will enter the adult world of the new millennium and be expected to read and write more than ever before in human history. Members of this generation will need advanced levels of literacy in order to perform their jobs, manage their households, carry out their roles as citizens, and conduct their lives. These adolescents will need advanced levels of literacy to deal with the plethora of information they will encounter everywhere they turn. Literacy will be needed for them to fuel their imaginations to mold the world of the future. In a complex world, their ability to read and write will be essential (Moore et al., 1999). Such sentiment validates the belief that literacy development beyond the early grades deserves attention. Students continue the process of learning to read, and some even continue to struggle with the basic processes of reading and writing, beyond the elementary years. Adolescents require support in the decoding, comprehension, and studying of the various texts they encounter in school and in their daily lives. There is still much to be learned about the practices unique to the different texts, disciplines, and unique situations of adolescence. The need for additional specialized literacy practices is further complicated by the demands of a changing and increasingly more technological world. Allan Luke and John Elkins (1998) have explained that in today’s world, adolescence and adulthood involve the constructing of communities and identities in relation to changing textual and media contexts. Becoming an adult also involves finding a way of moving forward in an increasingly explosive and uncertain job market, in a society that is by nature risky, where texts of any type are used to make, define, and position individuals at every point and in virtually every walk of life—in the mall, in school, on-line, and even face-to-face. In addition, the need to reexamine our focus on the literacy learning of adolescents seems clear, particularly if we look at how our current conceptualization of literacy in the middle and secondary schools, in secondary reading and content-area reading, has limited our thinking. Secondary reading, as it is generally understood, carries with it notions of reading in a lab setting. In this type of setting, students who have not learned to read are closeted and work individually with grade-leveled sets of materials supposedly designed to elevate them to the appropriate Moll, Luis C., and James B. Greenberg. 1990. “Creating Zones of Possibilities: Combining Social Contexts for Instruction.” In L. C. Moll, ed., Vygotsky and Education: Instructional Implications and Applications of Sociohistorical Psychology, pp. 319–348. New York: Cambridge University Press. Smagorinsky, Peter. 1995. “Constructing Meaning in the Disciplines: Reconceptualizing Writing across the Curriculum as Composing across the Curriculum.” American Journal of Education 103:160–184. Vygotsky, Lev S. 1978. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Edited by M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, and E. Souberman. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ———. 1987. Collected Works. Vol. 1. Edited by R. Rieber and A. Carton. Translated by N. Minick. New York: Plenum. Adolescent Literacy Adolescent literacy refers to the effort to reconceptualize the literacy of adolescents by going beyond the school- and textbook-based definitions of literacy (e.g., secondary reading, content-area reading) and acknowledging that there are multiple literacies and multiple texts—texts that transcend the traditional adult-sanctioned conceptions and definitions of a text (Alvermann et al., 1998; Moje et al., 2000). The adolescent literacy movement acknowledges that the expanded definition of text now includes but is not limited to CD-ROMs, popular music, television, the Internet, films, and magazines. The cues that adolescents get from these texts and these literacies play a significant role in the development of their emerging individual and social identities. Further, adolescent literacy advocates that schools must provide the time and space for students to explore these multiple texts and the new literacies that are concomitant with them. Enough researchers and practitioners in the field of literacy have now supported the importance of adolescent literacy for it to be considered a hot topic for reading research and practice for the new millennium (Cassidy and Cassidy, 2000–2001). Cognizant of the importance of adolescent literacy and the serious attention it deserved, the International Reading Association established the Commission on Adolescent Literacy. The commission called for renewed attention to the literacy needs of adolescents by stating that this 13 Adolescent Literacy Adolescent girl demonstrates the many literacies in her life with the posters on her wall (Barbara Guzzetti) focus on adolescents was created as a means of moving away from the constraints imposed by current concepts of literacy to a broader and more generative view. To provide additional impetus for this more generative view of literacy, the Commission on Adolescent Literacy pointed out in 1999 that adolescents have not been given the same support as beginning-level readers in elementary schools. Although state and federal dollars allocated to literacy programs for younger readers had increased and federal funding for research was focused in the early grades with the Reading Excellence Act during the mid-1990s and federal funding for the Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement, the commission emphasized that monies for adolescent readers had actually decreased and research support was negligible. Thus, the commission advocated new programmatic efforts to develop adolescents’ literacy growth that revolved around the following seven principles: First, students need access to a wide variety of reading material that they both grade level in reading so they can then be successful with their subject-matter materials. Such reading brings with it vestiges of remedial reading, which limits its usefulness, given the full range of adolescents’ reading needs. Content reading or content literacy, by contrast, carries with it an association that every teacher should be a teacher of reading. This notion makes sense to content-reading specialists, but not to subject-matter teachers. In fact, the saying that every teacher should be a teacher of reading has probably done more to influence teachers to object to reading instruction in their content areas than anything else, since they are trained to be teachers of content, not teachers of reading. Traditional definitions of content-area reading are usually confined to the in-school reading and writing of subject-matter materials, thus making reading instruction one-dimensional. As a consequence, with both secondary reading and content-area reading, instructional methods and materials might not match the literacy needs of individual adolescents. Thus, the 14 Adolescent Literature want to and are able to read. Second, students need instruction that builds both the skill and desire to read increasingly complex materials. Third, students need assessment that shows them their strengths as well as their needs and that guides their teachers to design instruction that will help them grow as readers. Fourth, students need expert teachers who model and provide explicit instruction in reading comprehension and study strategies across the curriculum. Fifth, students need reading specialists who will assist individual students having difficulty learning how to read. Sixth, students need teachers who understand the complexities of adolescent readers, respect their individual differences, and respond to their individuality. Finally, students need homes, communities, and a nation that will support their efforts to achieve advanced levels of literacy and provide the support necessary for them to succeed (Moore et al., 1999). One study that demonstrated this more generative view of literacy for adolescents was conducted by Thomas Bean, Shannon Bean, and Kristen Bean (1999). In this article, Bean and his two adolescent daughters described the multiple texts and multiple literacies the young women used over a two-week period. Not only did they use their various content-area textbooks, but they also used computers, conventional phones, cell phones, pagers, the Internet, electronic mail, art, music, drama, film, video games, and an assortment of digital aids, such as calculators and palm pilots. This study illustrated that these two adolescents wanted to participate in literacy practices that were suited to the way they viewed their daily lives, and they wanted to be viewed as competent literacy users who already possessed the knowledge, skills, and plans necessary for the world of the future. Thus, they moved far beyond the narrow confines of how literacy is currently conceptualized in our schools. The study also illustrates the notion that for adolescents to be literate in today’s society, they must become sociotechnically literate and skilled at multitasking —epitomizing the broader and more generative view advocated by adolescent literacy in which the role of the adolescent in the teaching and learning of literacy is central and highlighted. This more generative view of adolescent literacy can also be promoted in our middle and secondary classrooms. Elizabeth Moje and her colleagues (2000) have recommended that teachers consider the following. First, watch and listen to adolescents in a variety of contexts; pay attention to what they can do and think about ways to bring that skill into the classroom. Second, use interdisciplinary project-based teaching strategies that engage students in group-based inquiry about questions or problems of interest to them in their daily lives. Finally, draw from the texts that adolescents value and offer them alternate text representations that contrast with the traditional school texts they typically encounter. All of these suggestions require a deep respect for adolescents and the creation of a challenging, responsive literacy curriculum that puts adolescents first. John E. Readence See Also Content-Area Literacy; Middle-School Literacy; Popular Culture; Secondary-School Reading Programs References Alvermann, Donna E., Kathleen A. Hinchman, David W. Moore, Stephen F. Phelps, and Diane R. Waff, eds. 1998. Reconceptualizing the Literacies in Adolescents’ Lives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bean, Thomas W., Shannon K. Bean, and Kristen F. Bean. 1999. “Intergenerational Conversations and Two Adolescents’ Multiple Literacies: Implications for Redefining Content Area Literacy.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 42 (March): 438–448. Cassidy, Jack, and Drew Cassidy. 2000–2001. “What’s Hot, What’s Not for 2001.” Reading Today 18 (3) (December/January):1, 18. Luke, Allan, and John Elkins. 1998. “Reinventing Literacy in ‘New Times.’” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 42 (September):4–7. Moje, Elizabeth Birr, Josephine Peyton Young, John E. Readence, and David W. Moore. 2000. “Reinventing Adolescent Literacy for New Times: Perennial and Millennial Issues.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 43 (February): 400–410. Moore, David W., Thomas W. Bean, Deanna Birdyshaw, and James A. Rycik. 1999. “Adolescent Literacy: A Position Statement.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 43 (September): 97–112. Adolescent Literature The term adolescent literature has been used over the years to describe literature for adolescents, junior teen novels, juvenile fiction, and most commonly as young adult literature, due to the 15 Adolescent Literature pejorative nature of the terms juvenile and adolescence (Bushman and Haas, 2001). Definitions of adolescent literature also vary. Some leaders in the field suggest it is literature that features adolescents as its major characters. Others say the determining criterion is the reader: if the reader is an adolescent, then the literature is adolescent literature (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). However, most academic scholars rely on the authors’ intentions and believe it is literature written by authors who envision adolescents as their primary audience, writing for youths between the ages of eleven and eighteen. college campuses. Such a course is often required for certification to teach English-language arts or reading at middle and secondary levels. Middle and secondary teachers who once would not have considered using adolescent novels now regularly assign works by authors such as Robert Cormier, S. E. Hinton, Lois Lowry, and Walter Dean Myers (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). Scholarship has been productive as well: master’s theses and doctoral dissertations, critical studies of authors and themes, journal articles, textbooks, conference presentations, and workshops represent significant forms of literary research. Yet adolescent literature continues to have a historical stigma. Many teachers still consider it literature for younger or struggling readers, not literature worthy of critical study. It is often viewed as most appropriate for upper-elementary or middle-school reading (Monseau, 1996; Monseau and Salvner, 2000; Stover, 1996). In addition, the trend in the last fifteen years or so has been for publishers to market to younger and younger audiences (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). High-school curricula continue to reflect the “canon” of literary works that rarely impart adolescent experiences. Most teachers want students to learn to love literature but may not consider that this might never happen if students are required to read literature they do not understand and fail to have enjoyable, let alone critical, engagements. Such curricular choices may be slowly changing, perhaps due to students who simply refuse to read or cannot read the “classics.” Reading requirements may also change with growing teacher appreciation of the increased diversity and sophistication of this literature, along with the publication of books and the addition of college courses for teachers that reflect new curricula and practices. With the rise of reader response (see Reader Response) and other new literary theories, teachers are finding that students are more apt to engage with adolescent literature, which encourages them to have more meaningful experiences (Soter, 1999). Historical Perspectives Contemporary adolescent literature has existed in various forms for more than fifty years. In the 1950s, fewer than sixty books published yearly focused on adolescents as their primary audience, whereas by the late 1960s, adolescent literature became known as the most rapidly expanding literary genre. During the 1970s, the majority of larger publishers did not have separate divisions devoted to adolescent literature, but by the 1990s, most did. From the mid-1970s to mid-1980s, financial and critical bases changed for many authors who were now earning a living exclusively by writing adolescent literature (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). This popularity was aided by television and movie industries that sought stories that would appeal to a youth-oriented society, helped by a publishing industry that discovered teenagers were willing to spend money on paperbacks. Most libraries today have separate young adult sections. Bookstores ranging from the large chains to the independently owned to those in cyberspace (e.g., Amazon.com) have extensive young adult literature sections, complete with book reviews and recommendations for readers. Criticisms The field of adolescent literature is ever-changing, just as young adults themselves are, and it reflects the society in which adolescents matriculate. This literature has undergone a continuous cycle of criticism, first for being formulaic, and more recently, for “too real-to-life situations” (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). It is a field that continues to gain respectability and commercial viability. Undergraduate and graduate courses in young adult literature are prominent on most Style and Characteristic Features Adolescent literature is diverse in terms of style and content. This genre includes novels, short stories, poetry, drama, and nonfiction, as well as stories of adventure and accomplishment, romances, mysteries, tales of the supernatural, folklore, humor, fantasy, science fiction, history, 16 Adolescent Literature autobiography, series books, and informative nonfiction. The late 1960s gave rise to “new realism,” or “problem novels,” meaning coming-ofage stories about rites of passages from childhood to adulthood. Protagonists are written into difficult situations and settings, use language the way adolescents talk (e.g., profanity, slang, dialects), and exhibit adolescent attitudes (e.g., questioning of authority, alienation from adults, and so on) (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001). Problem novels demonstrate the philosophy that adolescents are more likely to be happy and successful when they have realistic expectations garnered from both good and bad in society. With realistic expectations, they are able to make better life decisions. Real-life problems include relationships, violence, war, race issues, AIDS, mental illness, pregnancy, death, and moral dilemmas. Proponents state that such books help adolescents to understand their own and other people’s feelings and behaviors. Sometimes referred to as bibliotherapy (see Bibliotherapy), the belief is that adolescents can benefit psychologically from reading and talking about problems of fictional characters. Discovering other people have problems provides adolescents with some comfort and opens avenues for communication. Many critics, however, are concerned with the “bleak” trend in adolescent literature that deals with such serious issues. Yet adolescent literature in this way is much like the classics, depicting adolescents in predicaments, often in contemporary settings. Of course, not all books are bleak. Many include humorous and lighthearted experiences. Typically, adolescent literature offers hope to readers and may encourage teens to read for efferent and aesthetic reasons when they otherwise may be uninspired to read. in the past twenty years, the National Council of Teachers of English (NCTE) has published numerous rationales for commonly challenged books that are available upon request. Awards and Honors Numerous awards and honors are presented annually to adolescent literature. These include the Newbery Medal sponsored by the American Library Association (ALA), awarded to the author deemed to have made the most distinguished contribution to literature published the preceding year. The Newbery Medal awards both gold and silver honors. The Coretta Scott King Award is presented annually by the ALA, given to a black author and black illustrator whose works encourage and promote world unity and peace and serve as inspiration to youths. The Scott O’Dell Historical Fiction Award is presented to a work of historical fiction set in the New World, written by a U.S. citizen and published in the United States. The ALA’s Best Picks list is published annually, and various other professional associations and journals also publish recommended books. Many authors are multiple honorees of various awards over multiple decades, including Walter Dean Myers, who was almost a solo voice in the 1980s when he began publishing about the experiences of African American youths (primarily males) and the dignity of their life experiences, told in realistic settings with authentic dialects. Authors Today’s writers include women and men from diverse socioeconomic and cultural backgrounds from all over the world. Many authors reflect themselves in their work, using autobiographical experiences to help them connect with their adolescent audience. Many writers come to this genre from other occupations. For example, Gary Paulsen lived in a variety of locations and held a variety of jobs before becoming a writer. Paulsen includes his own journeys in his adventure and survival stories about teens struggling to survive nature’s extremes. Many of these authors are prolific and have published vastly different books, strong proof of the literary excellence of their work. Numerous authors are favorites of adolescent readers and their teachers and librarians. Some who are repeatedly recognized include the aforementioned, Censorship Censorship has been an issue with adolescent literature since the late 1960s. With the popularity of new realism or problem novels, adolescent literature was deemed by some as no longer “safe.” Censors come from liberal and conservative ends of the political spectrum. Books have been criticized for profane language, antisocial behavior, teen pregnancy, sex, biases on social issues, and religious or irreligious perspectives. The irony is that many censors assume that the classics have no objectionable words, actions, or ideas. In answer to the growing number of challenged books 17 Adolescent Literature as well as Richard Peck, Katherine Paterson, M. E. Kerr, Gary Soto, Francesca Lia Block, Angela Johnson, Paul Fleischman, Paul Janeczko, and Mel Glenn. Donald R. Gallo has anthologized multiple collections of diverse short stories written by a variety of authors. Paul Janeczko, both poet and anthologist, has been praised for his collections of poetry centered around connecting themes. Secondary English teacher and poet Mel Glenn’s collections include stories told in poem format and poems that present biographical sketches of wide-ranging teen perspectives and experiences. 1996); selecting and teaching adolescent literature (Bushman and Haas, 2001; Monseau and Salvner, 2000; Reed, 1994); reading with multicultural perspectives (Brown and Stephens, 1998); including adolescent literature in the middle-school curricula (Stover, 1996); using adolescent literature as a complement to the traditional secondary canon (Herz and Gallo, 1996; Kaywell, 1993); and using new literary theories when reading adolescent literature (Moore, 1997; Soter, 1999). The Twayne United States Young Adult Authors series is written by a variety of scholars in the field and presents critical biographies of established authors. Laura R. Lipsett Professional Associations and Publications There are various professional associations and publications that promote and review adolescent literature. The NCTE, the International Reading Association (IRA), and the ALA feature presentations at their annual conferences and frequently ask authors to speak. Two national professional organizations grew dramatically during the 1980s and are still growing in popularity: the Assembly on Literature for Adolescents (ALAN) of NCTE and the Special Interest Group on Adolescent Literature (SIGNAL) of the IRA. ALAN publishes the ALAN Review, devoted solely to promoting adolescent literature. This journal offers articles written from a variety of perspectives along with interviews with authors, middle- and secondary-school teachers, librarians, publishers, and university professors. IRA publishes the Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, featuring articles on reading interests, literature, and reviews of new books. The ALA publishes Booklist, which contains reviews to guide librarians’ purchases. The School Library Journal and Voice of Youth Advocates (VOYA) provide best-book lists and reviews. NCTE’s English Journal prints a column titled “Young Adult Literature,” featuring issues and trends as well as reviews. NCTE’s Voices from the Middle, aimed at middle-school teachers’ interests, publishes “Clip and File: Reviews of Books for Middle-Level Readers,” written by student readers in the same format as the ALAN Review’s “Clip and File: YA Books.” See Also Adolescent Literacy; Bibliotherapy References Brown, Jean E., and Elaine C. Stephens. 1998. United in Diversity: Using Multicultural Young Adult Literature in the Classroom. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Bushman, John H., and Kay Parks Haas. 2001. Using Young Adult Literature in the English Classroom. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/PrenticeHall. Herz, Sarah K., and Donald R. Gallo. 1996. From Hinton to Hamlet: Building Bridges between Young Adult Literature and the Classics. Westport, CT: Greenwood. Kaywell, Joan F. 1993. Young Adult Literature as a Complement to the Classics. Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon. Monseau, Virginia R. 1996. Responding to Young Adult Literature. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Monseau, Virginia R., and Gary M. Salvner, eds. 2000. Reading Their World: The Young Adult Novel in the Classroom. 2nd ed. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Moore, John Noell. 1997. Interpreting Young Adult Literature: Literary Theory in the Secondary Classroom. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Nilsen, Alleen Pace, and Kenneth L. Donelson. 2001. Literature for Today’s Young Adults. 6th ed. New York: Longman. Reed, Arthea J. S. 1994. Reading Adolescents: The Young Adult Book and the School. New York: Merrill. Soter, Anna O. 1999. Young Adult Literature and the New Literary Theories. New York: Teachers College Press. Stover, Lois Thomas. 1996. Young Adult Literature: The Heart of the Middle School Curriculum. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Pedagogical and Reference Texts Several scholars have published books, some in revised editions, on the following subjects: field and genre (Nilsen and Donelson, 2001); responding to adolescent literature (Monseau, 18 Adult Literacy Adult Literacy Adult Literacy Learners Adult literacy learners are a diverse group in terms of their racial, ethnic, language, age, and even socioeconomic status. A large number of low-literate adults are over the age of sixty— products of a schooling system that was very different from the current educational system. In addition, many low-literate adults are also classified as low income. It is not possible to determine a causal relationship between literacy and income, but a strong correlation does exist. A growing number of low-literate adults are also English-language learners (ELL) who may or may not be literate in their native language. In addition to these demographic features, it is important to note that adult literacy learners also face affective issues in the process of becoming literate. First, many adult literacy learners have had negative experiences with schooling and must therefore overcome these feelings in order to become engaged in adult literacy education. In addition, many low-literate adults are faced with a number of stressful situations that result in little available time or energy to pursue literacy education. Some common challenges include working multiple jobs to “make ends meet,” single parenting, financial problems, housing problems, domestic violence, isolation, transportation problems, and child-care problems. The challenges faced by adult literacy learners often affect their engagement with adult literacy programs, persistence in programs, and educational progress in programs. Adult literacy is broadly defined as an adult’s ability to read, write, listen, and speak in order to accomplish daily events in society, in the family, and on the job. Traditional definitions of adult literacy focus on functional aspects of literacy and the acquisition and use of specific reading and writing skills. More recent definitions of adult literacy may include references to computing, solving problems, viewing, and visually representing. Other definitions describe adult literacy as a political and transformative process that is embedded in social, cultural, and power contexts. Adult literacy can also be defined in relation to the educational programs designed to teach adult learners, such as adult basic education, workplace literacy, family literacy, and English as a Second Language (ESL) programs. Definitions of adult literacy have changed significantly during recent years. Although in the early 1900s literacy was often defined as the ability to sign one’s name, expectations have increased steadily through the years in terms of what it means to be literate. In 1958, the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) defined an adult as literate if he or she could to read and write a simple statement about daily life. The definition of functional literacy was first used as a result of the 1940 U.S. Census, which defined an adult as literate if he or she had completed at least five years of school and was able to pass an examination written at the fourth-grade level. In 1978, UNESCO defined literacy as the ability to use reading, writing, and calculation in activities necessary for effective participation in one’s group and community. The 1991 National Literacy Act defined literacy as an adult’s ability to read, write, speak English, compute, and solve problems at the level necessary to accomplish goals, function at work, and develop to one’s potential. Building on this definition, in 1992, the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) defined literacy in three areas: prose literacy (finding and using information from connected texts, including newspapers, stories, and poems); document literacy (understanding and using information from charts, tables, graphs, maps, and so on); and quantitative literacy (using information in prose and document texts to complete mathematical operations). Adult Basic Education Adult Basic Education (ABE) programs focus on helping adults learn basic skills and prepare to earn the General Equivalency Diploma (GED). Most programs begin with adults who are reading at approximately the fourth-grade level (see Adult Literacy Programs). Adults who are reading at lower levels may be taught by volunteer tutors or in adult education programs. ABE programs are generally federally funded and are administered by the individual states. In the 1990s, it was estimated that 3 million adults were enrolled in ABE programs across the United States. Although some workplace literacy programs are criticized for not helping workers become literate, their real problem is that they focus on job-specific skills that are not easily transferred to other contexts. 19 Adult Literacy criticism of some family literacy programs is that they promote deficit views of low-literate and diverse families rather than building on the unique strengths and attributes of each family. English as a Second Language Adult Literacy With the increasing number of English-language learners (ELL), English as a Second Language (ESL) programs are being offered for adults in conjunction with ABE, workplace, family literacy, and other educational programs (see Adult Literacy Programs, The Even Start Family Literacy Program, and Family Literacy). These ESL adult literacy programs tend to retain many of the characteristics of the program in which they are housed (e.g., ABE, workplace literacy, family literacy). Most ESL adult literacy classes are divided into beginning, intermediate, and advanced levels. Beginning classes tend to focus on oral English proficiency and beginning reading/writing instruction. As students move into intermediate and advanced classes, the focus shifts to include more emphasis on reading/writing proficiency in English. Legislation, Policy, and Adult Literacy During the civil rights era in the 1960s and the Right to Read Campaign in the 1970s (see The National Right to Read Foundation), the public began to hear about the need to view literacy as a right for all. In addition, it was perceived that lack of literacy was a national shame. In 1981, a number of professional organizations and other groups formed the National Coalition for Literacy to work toward increasing literacy in the United States. This group supported a media blitz about adult literacy that increased public awareness of adult literacy issues and challenges. In 1982, the Subcommittee on Postsecondary Education of the House Committee on Education and Labor convened hearings on adult literacy. The Adult Literacy Initiative (ALI) was passed in 1983 to provide limited funding to private volunteer groups interested in supporting adult literacy. In 1985, the National Adult Literacy Project (NALP) commissioned a study of adult literacy programs to determine effective instructional approaches for adult literacy learners. The results of the 1992 National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) increased concern about adult literacy issues, as the numbers indicated that many adults in the United States scored in the Woman reading a book on the University of Minnesota campus (Skjold Photographs) Family Literacy The term family literacy was coined by Denny Taylor (1983) in her book Family Literacy (see Family Literacy). Another key work that laid the groundwork for the development of family literacy was Shirley Brice Heath’s (1983) book Ways with Words, which described the varied uses, approaches to, and conceptions of literacy within three different communities. In the 1980s, family literacy programs began to appear as a way to assist low-literate parents and their young children develop literacy skills. Such programs typically have four key components: adult literacy, early childhood education, parent education, and parent and child together-time. Support and funding for family literacy programs have been provided by the U.S. federal government through Even Start. In addition, much funding for family literacy has been provided by the National Center for Family Literacy, the Barbara Bush Foundation for Family Literacy, and a number of private businesses and foundations. A common 20 Adult Literacy lowest levels of the assessment (see Adult Literacy Testing). All of these events resulted in increased awareness, concern, and priority for adult literacy in the United States. With the advent of the “Republican revolution” in the mid-1990s, federal legislation began to emphasize welfare reform in relation to adult literacy education. With the passage of the Personal Responsibility and Work Opportunity Reconciliation Act (PRWORA) in 1996, the nation’s welfare laws were reformed, and the Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (TANF) system was created to provide block grants to states. States were expected to move welfare recipients into work within a two-year period. PRWORA was then altered as a result of the Department of Labor’s 1997 Welfare-to-Work Program. This legislation provides money to states to prepare and support welfare recipients for the workforce. The Workforce Investment Act (WIA) of 1998 includes, as Title II, the Adult Education and Family Literacy Act, which replaced the Adult Education Act and the National Literacy Act as the basis for federal funding and involvement in adult literacy programs. Individual state policies may vary, but these pieces of legislation have had a significant impact on adult literacy. The overall result of the welfare reform legislation has been to put added pressure on adult literacy programs to provide education and training that will prepare all students to secure employment within a relatively short time frame. As a result, many adult literacy programs and educators find themselves struggling to reconcile competing viewpoints about adult literacy education: namely, how can beliefs about adult literacy education that focus on social action, change, and empowerment be pursued in light of the recent trend of emphasizing economic growth over other outcomes of adult literacy education? ucation to female homesteaders in isolated areas of Canada. In 1911, Cora Stewart, a superintendent of schools in Kentucky started “Moonlight schools” for adults to attend in the evening after work. She wrote special materials for the schools and used volunteers to provide the instruction. The curriculum focused on basic language, history, civics, agriculture, rural life, and sanitation. As a result of World War I, the U.S. military discovered that thousands of soldiers could not read well enough to follow printed directions necessary for their jobs. The military played a key role in raising awareness of adult literacy problems, as well as in establishing functional literacy training programs to teach job-oriented literacy skills to military personnel. Although the U.S. military has been involved in adult literacy education for a number of years, its work has not been extended to the civilian population. Frank Laubach is best known for the worldwide initiative of “Each One Teach One” that he began in 1929. This approach to adult literacy was built on the belief that literate adults have a responsibility to help other adults become literate. As an educator, sociologist, and minister, Laubach sought to teach adults to read life skills materials and religious texts. His approach to reading instruction was based on phonics, using key words for vowel and consonant sounds. He developed literacy programs in over sixty countries and developed literacy charts and primers in over 150 languages. His approach resulted in the development of the Laubach Way to Reading series. In 1969, Laubach Literacy Action (LLA) was organized in the United States and Canada. In the 1990s, more than 80,000 volunteers and 100,000 learners were involved in Laubach Literacy Action programs. Paulo Freire, a radical social transformationist, worked with Brazilian peasants to demonstrate the political, sociocultural, and transformative aspects of literacy. He espoused a liberatory approach to literacy education in his Pedagogy of the Oppressed (1970), rather than the traditional model of education that he described as the “banking concept,” wherein the teacher dispenses knowledge and the learner is passively filled. His work established the political nature of literacy, as well as challenging traditional models of education commonly used with adult literacy learners (see The Political Nature of Literacy). Early Influences on Adult Literacy The roots of adult literacy reach back several hundred years. In the late 1800s and early 1900s, Alfred Fitzpatrick initiated a crusade in Canada to take education to the people. He developed the concept of the laborer-teacher who assisted his fellow workers with reading, writing, math, and citizenship. He founded the Reading Tent Association, later known as Frontier College. He also supported female “outriders” to provide ed21 Adult Literacy Programs Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Hull, Glynda, ed. 1999. Changing Work, Changing Workers: Critical Perspectives on Language, Literacy, and Skills. Albany: State University of New York Press. Martin, Larry G., and James C. Fisher, eds. 1999. The Welfare-to-Word Challenge for Adult Literacy Educators. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Taylor, Denny. 1983. Family Literacy. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Issues and Challenges in the Field of Adult Literacy A number of critical issues and challenges face the field of adult literacy. One ongoing debate focuses on the need for adult literacy educators to obtain specific credentials. One side of the argument stresses the need for adult literacy educators to be well prepared to teach adults, whereas the other side advocates the current trend in adult literacy wherein volunteers and individuals who are interested in working with adults are able to do so. Another issue is the heavy reliance on grants and other “soft monies” to fund adult literacy programs. As a result of this funding approach, many adult literacy programs are developed and implemented, only to find they must dissolve when the grant funding ceases. In addition, the high percentage of low-literate adult learners with undiagnosed learning disabilities (LD) is a challenge for adult literacy educators who may not have any preparation for working with students with LD. Another related issue is the high cost of diagnosing learning disabilities for adult learners; to have modifications on the GED, however, they must have documentation of a disability. Questions about how to pay for the diagnosis and where the responsibility lies remain unsolved. Another challenge is the rapid growth in demand for ESL adult literacy programs that often outpaces the development of programs to serve these populations. Another ongoing challenge is the need for additional research about how adults acquire literacy and what the effective approaches to adult literacy instruction are. For much of its history, adult literacy has relied on research and instructional approaches used with school-age populations. With increasing knowledge about adult development and learning, research conducted with adult populations is needed to answer critical questions about how to educate diverse adult literacy learners effectively. Laurie Elish-Piper Adult Literacy Programs Adult literacy programs are instructional programs that help adults, both immigrants and those who have not been successful in the K–12 system, to improve their ability to read, write, and speak English; to compute and solve problems at levels of proficiency necessary to function on the job and in society; to achieve their goals; and to develop their knowledge and potential. The 1992 National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) estimated the population of adults who have very low literacy skills (NALS Level 1) at between 21 and 23 percent of all Americans over the age of sixteen (see Adult Literacy). The NALS estimated that an additional 25 percent of the adult population had literacy skills that were low in relation to the demands of contemporary society and its economy (NALS Level 2). More than half of the people who scored in NALS Level 1 do not speak English, are over age sixty-five, or have cognitive or physical handicaps. A large proportion of the remaining NALS Level 1 adults probably have learning disabilities or grew up in environments that did not support the acquisition of literacy skills. People who scored in NALS Level 2 are more likely to be native-born adults who did not do well in school and who have not been in jobs that required them to use literacy skills. The adult literacy programs that provide educational services to this population are funded by federal, state, and local government agencies and by private funding sources. Approximately 4 million adults participate in these programs each year, but half of these students stay in their program for less than thirty hours. Total expenditures for these services from all sources is estimated to be $1.2 billion annually. See Also Adult Literacy Programs; Adult Literacy Testing; Family Literacy; Laubach Literacy References Askov, Eunice N. 2000. Handbook of Adult and Continuing Education. Edited by Arthur L. Wilson and Elisabeth Hayes. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Freire, Paulo. 1970. Pedagogy of the Oppressed. New York: Seabury Press. 22 Adult Literacy Programs History of Adult Literacy Programs Adult literacy programs have a long history in the United States. Colonial newspapers published advertisements for private tutors who taught adults to read. The first commitment of government funds took place during the Revolutionary War, when General George Washington directed chaplains to teach soldiers to read and write at Valley Forge. The U.S. Army and Navy continued to teach literacy to soldiers and sailors during the nineteenth century, but government support did not expand until the twentieth century. In the early part of the twentieth century, services were targeted at the large influx of immigrants who did not speak English. Military testing during World War I found that a large number of native-born Americans were illiterate or had low literacy skills. Several state and national efforts supported adult literacy programs during the 1920s, and then in the 1930s, the Civilian Conservation Corps (CCC) and the Works Progress Administration (WPA) provided adult literacy programs as a way to educate adults and employ educated unemployed workers as teachers. After World War II, military testing again identified adult illiteracy and low literacy as a significant problem. The Eisenhower administration was concerned about this issue and established the National Commission on Adult Literacy to look for a solution through a government program, but no program was established until Congress passed Lyndon Johnson’s Economic Opportunity Act in 1964. This act included, as Title IIB, the Adult Basic Education program. In 1966, federal funding for adult literacy programs was moved to the Office of Education and then to the Department of Education. By the middle of the twentieth century, adult literacy programs began serving three distinct populations, and these services have continued to the present day. Adult English for Speakers of Other Languages (ESOL) programs help immigrants learn to speak, read, and write English. Adult Basic Education (ABE) programs help adults improve their literacy and math skills. Adult Secondary Education (ASE) programs help adults acquire a high-school credential, usually by passing the General Equivalency Diploma (GED) test. All three programs include the improvement of reading, writing, and math skills. In 1999, federal funding for adult literacy programs became part of the Workforce Investment Adult immigrants in an English as a Second Language (ESL) class (Michael Siluk) Act (WIA) as Title II, the Adult Education and Family Literacy Act. The new law consolidated over fifty employment, training, and literacy programs into three block grants to states to be used for adult education and family literacy, for disadvantaged youth, and for adult employment and training services. Although WIA is focused on preparing people for employment, it also supports services geared toward adults pursuing their education for other purposes, such as citizenship, parenting, or their own personal improvement. At the state level, WIA funds may be administered by an education agency or an employment and training agency. Each state is required to match WIA funds, but a handful of states provide funds that are five to ten times as great as the federal share. Approximately 30 percent of funding comes from federal sources and 60 percent from state sources. The remaining 10 percent comes from local governments, corporations, foundations, individuals, and local institutions (libraries, for example). Structure of Adult Literacy Program Services Program formats vary widely. Some programs follow a classroom format, some use one-on-one tutoring, and some provide one-on-one tutoring to several adults working individually within a class. In recent years, computer technology and electronic media have been integrated into instruction and used for self-study. Large programs offer classes at different skill levels, whereas smaller programs often form classes of students who are at different levels. Most students are involved in 23 Adult Literacy Programs instruction for three to five hours a week, though some attend for only a few hours a month and others for up to twenty hours per week. Some programs run in closed cycles of a few months to a year, whereas others have ongoing classes with an open-entry admission policy. Instruction takes place in a variety of venues, including community centers, workplaces, libraries, prisons, community colleges, churches, homeless shelters, and schools. Several specialty programs that focus on the needs of specific populations have developed out of an expressed need by students and through legislative mandates. Family literacy programs support learning among several generations in the same family with the goal of helping both adults and children improve their reading skills. Workplace literacy programs take place at a student’s place of employment, and the content of instruction is drawn from work tasks. Corrections education takes place in prisons and jails and is focused on helping inmates improve their employability so that they will be less likely to return to prison. Transitional education programs help adults who pass the GED test to gain the academic literacy skills needed to be successful in post-secondary education and training programs. Student proficiency levels are usually assessed with one of several standardized tests upon entry into a program. This information is used to assign students to a class, but it is also used as a baseline assessment that is then matched later with a second test score for program accountability purposes. The most commonly used tests are the Test of Adult Basic Education (TABE), the Adult Basic Learning Examination (ABLE), and the Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment System (CASAS). All three are norm-referenced tests developed to measure the reading comprehension of adults. Adult literacy program students are either recent immigrants or adults who, as children, failed to acquire strong literacy skills in school. Although no national study has examined the skills of adult literacy program students, a study of several hundred ABE students found that almost 85 percent had weak print skills, low reading fluency, and limited oral vocabularies and background knowledge. ESOL students who are literate in their own languages stay in programs longer than other groups of students and appear to make progress. High-school dropouts with ninth-grade literacy skills or higher stay in ASE programs for a short period of time before taking the GED test. GED students are usually successful. ESOL students who are not literate in their own languages and ABE students drop out early and usually make slow progress. Most program participants are between the ages of eighteen and sixty-five. An analysis of 1990 census data and the 1992 NALS data estimates that among this age group, approximately 7 million adults do not speak English well, 25 million do not have a high-school diploma, and 35 million speak English and have a high-school diploma but have NALS Level 1 or 2 skills. These are unduplicated counts. This latter group rarely participates in adult literacy programs. Evaluations and Reform of Adult Literacy Programs Evaluations of adult literacy programs have identified a number of weaknesses, including low retention rates, lack of full-time staff, inadequate teacher preparation and support, and little research available to inform program design. These weaknesses are the result of low funding levels. Although the national average funding is approximately $300 per participant each year, the range is probably between $100 and $1,500. With this level of funding, adult literacy programs find it difficult to serve students who need well-trained teachers and long-term services. In 2000, a reform movement began with the publication of From the Margins to the Mainstream: An Action Agenda for Literacy. This document has set the following goal: by 2010, a system of high-quality adult literacy, language, and lifelong learning service that will help adults in every community make measurable gains toward achieving their goals as family members, workers, citizens, and lifelong learners. To meet this goal, the document sets three priorities. The first is to increase resources by both changing existing policies to support higher quality programs and expanding federal, state, and private funding. The second is to increase access by providing: better outreach to potential students, support service to make it possible for them to study, and more convenient ways to learn, including the use of technology. The third is to focus on improving the quality of instruction by supporting programs to develop goals and standards that reflect the concerns of all stakeholders and that address 24 Adult Literacy Testing Adult Literacy Testing systematic program quality issues, provide better-trained staff, and expand research and development efforts. Several national organizations and agencies support the implementation of adult literacy programs and are contributing to the reform agenda. The U.S. Department of Education’s Division of Adult Education and Literacy (DAEL) administers state block grants under Title II of WIA, manages the WIA accountability system, the National Reporting System (NRS), and supports several research and development efforts. The National Institute for Literacy (NIFL), which is funded through the Department of Education but overseen by a board that is appointed by the president, promotes improvement of services through research and development, communications, and consensus-building activities. The National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy pursues research and disseminates research findings. The National Coalition for Literacy is a consortium of institutions interested in adult literacy that advocates policies that improve and expand services. Laubach Literacy and Literacy Volunteers of America support volunteer and community-based literacy programs. The National Center for Family Literacy promotes family literacy and provides funding, training, and technical assistance to programs. These institutions and the many adult literacy programs they support constitute a third education system that fills the gaps left by the K–12 and post-secondary education systems. John P. Comings Adult literacy testing refers to the assessment of adults’ reading, writing, and arithmetic skills through the uses of different kinds of measures. Adult literacy testing is conducted for two broad and largely unrelated purposes. First, it is used to measure the literacy skills of adults enrolled in adult education programs, such as Adult Basic Education (ABE), English as a Second Language (ESL), and General Equivalency Diploma (GED), or high-school equivalency programs. Standardized tests are frequently used to determine adults’ reading, writing, and math skills prior to entry into these programs and to measure gains in their literacy skills following instruction. The second purpose pertains to recent federally mandated efforts to determine the literacy skills of adults in the United States—and by extension, adults in comparable nations. The tests that are used for these purposes consist of two types: standardized tests (which are norm referenced) and competency-based tests (which are criterion referenced). Standardized tests are widely used in adult literacy programs because state and federal agencies overseeing adult education mandate their use for reporting student achievement. These tests assess reading and listening comprehension, oral responses to visual and verbal prompts, knowledge of the mechanics of writing (e.g., grammar, spelling, punctuation), and math. Competency-based tests are growing in popularity because they are presumed to better meet students’ needs. They are relatively easy to administer and score but more difficult to interpret. Assessing adults’ literacy skills is a vexing problem in basic education, for several reasons. First, there is little consensus on the definition of literacy (Venezky, Wagner, and Ciliberti, 1990). How adult education programs define literacy determines the kinds of instruction offered and the tests used to measure literacy skills. There is little uniformity in curricula or instructional methods across the spectrum of literacy programs. Second, many of the measures used to assess adults’ literacy skills have questionable validity, as several have been normed on populations that are different from the population of adults having low literacy skills (i.e., school-age children and youth). Third, standardized measures of adult literacy are often administered by instructors with little knowledge about appropriate test adminis- See Also Adult Literacy; Adult Literacy Testing References Comings, John, Barbara Garner, and Cristine Smith, eds. 2000. Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Vol. 1. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. ———. 2001. Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Vol. 2. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. ———. 2002. Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Vol. 3. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kirsch, Irwin S., Ann Jungeblut, Lynn Jenkins, and Andrew Kolstad. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. National Institute for Literacy. 2000. From the Margins to the Mainstream: An Action Agenda for Literacy. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. 25 Adult Literacy Testing tration procedures. Frequently, test-giving procedures are violated (e.g., too much time is given), thereby invalidating the tests. Fourth, the test results are often misinterpreted. It is largely assumed that adults who participate in literacy programs are illiterate, that is, lacking in literacy ability to such an extent that they cannot read, write, or do math. However, it is more accurate to say that many, if not most, of the adults in these programs have low literacy skills. That is, they have some reading, writing, and arithmetic skills, but these are not sufficient for them to function well at home, in their communities, and at work. Some adults have learning disabilities that affect their ability to adequately read and write. Other adults may be competent readers or writers but have poor math skills. Participants in ESL programs may be literate in their native language, but not in English. Or they may be low-literate in their native language and unable to read and write in English. 1994), however, each state sets its own benchmark for passing the tests. Although reading is not assessed, much reading is required and individuals need to have high-school-level reading skills to be successful on the GED tests. Most test-takers participate in a GED preparation program prior to taking the tests, but participation is generally not required, and many people prepare for the GED tests on their own. Standardized Literacy Skills Tests There are a number of commercially available tests that have been adopted by adult literacy programs around the United States. The most commonly used tests are the Adult Basic Learning Examination (ABLE), Basic English Skills Tests (BEST), Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment System (CASAS), English as a Second Language Oral Assessment (ESLOA), Reading Evaluation Adult Diagnosis (READ), and Test of Adult Basic Education (TABE). Standardized reading-achievement tests from batteries such as the Wide-Range Achievement Test are also used. The tests that are most frequently used in adult literacy programs, the TABE and the CASAS, will be briefly described. Tests of Adult Literacy Adult literacy programs are usually required by individual states to adopt specific tests for program accountability and data-reporting purposes. The U.S. government also requires that states receiving federal funds for adult education report student achievement data for basic education programs. The assessment of students’ literacy skills before and after literacy instruction is often secondary to program accountability. There is no single test used to assess the literacy skills of adults in basic education programs, with the exception of high-school equivalency programs. Here, the GED tests are used. The TABE This is a norm-referenced test that only recently has been normed on adults rather than children. The TABE is designed to measure reading, mathematics, language, and spelling skills. Both English- and Spanish-language versions are available. The TABE has two forms (versions 5/6 and 7/8) covering grade levels from 0.0 to 12.9. A locator test can be used to help literacy instructors with placement of students into appropriate programs by matching learners’ test performance to level of instruction. There is some evidence, however, that few literacy programs actually use the locator test to appropriately place students or to plan instruction (Inman and Trott, 1999). A limitation of the TABE is its focus on determining grade-level reading ability rather than identifying learner competencies. High-School Equivalency Examination The GED tests are sponsored by the American Council on Education and are generally administered by community colleges and university testing centers to adults who lack a high-school diploma. The GED tests are norm referenced. They require adults to demonstrate competence in five formal categories that represent a typical high-school curriculum: Interpreting Literature and the Arts, Mathematics, Science, Social Studies, and Writing Skills. The test questions are multiple choice, except for Writing Skills, in which examinees write essays. Passing scores are set so that 70 percent of graduating high-school seniors would obtain passing scores (Lowe, The CASAS The Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment System of California has developed learner competencies, assessment tools, and training manuals for adult basic education programs. A major revision of the CASAS was completed in 1994. 26 Adult Literacy Testing The CASAS focuses on a large set of adult competencies that closely match those identified by the U.S. Department of Labor as the skills necessary for work (Secretary’s Commission on Achieving Necessary Skills, 1991). Adult competencies in real-life contexts are assessed by the CASAS. These competencies go well beyond traditional literacy skills and include: basic communication (e.g., writing a letter), consumer economics (e.g., using catalogs to order consumer goods), community resources (e.g., using maps), health (e.g., filling out medical-history forms), employment (e.g., interpreting wage and benefits forms), government and law (e.g., interpreting a ballot), computation (e.g., arithmetic and measurement skills), learning to learn (e.g., using thinking skills), and domestic skills (e.g., home care). The CASAS is used in California and several other states by federally and state-funded adult literacy programs. States using CASAS also require the TABE for reporting adults’ progress. Adult Literacy Testing for National Assessments As noted earlier, the second purpose of adult literacy testing is to determine the literacy skills of the populations of adults in the United States and Canada, and to a somewhat lesser extent, in other Western developed nations, such as Germany and the United Kingdom. There have been a number of efforts devoted to assessing adults’ literacy skills in the United States over the past three decades. The most recent efforts occurred in 1985 and 1992. Another national assessment is planned for 2002. The assessment method used was developed by the Educational Testing Service (ETS). ETS defined three purportedly distinct forms of literacy skill: prose, document, and quantitative (PDQ) literacy. Each type was measured by a separate scale, an approach thought to better reflect the multifaceted nature of literacy than a single scale. The PDQ assessment results in a profile of literacy skills. The advantage of this approach is that it eliminates the arbitrary use of a cut point or single standard to distinguish “illiterate” from “literate” adults. The three skill domains encompass the predominate types of text materials and literacy tasks that “typical” adults encounter in everyday life. Prose literacy is the ability to understand and use information from texts that include editorials, news stories, poems, and fiction. Examples of prose tasks include finding information in a newspaper article or inferring a theme from a poem. Document literacy is the ability to locate and use information contained in materials such as job applications, payroll forms, transportation schedules, maps, tables, and graphs (Kirsch and Jungeblut, 1985). Examples include locating an intersection on a street map and entering personal information on a job application form. Quantitative literacy means being able to apply arithmetic operations by using numbers embedded in printed materials (Kirsch and Jungeblut, 1985). Examples include balancing a checkbook and summing purchases on a catalog order form. Item response theory (IRT) is used to estimate adults’ literacy abilities from their responses to a small number of tasks that are administered. IRT is a statistical method for scaling individual test items for difficulty in such a way that a given test item has a known probability of being correctly completed by an individual performing at a given Limits of Standardized Tests Standardized adult literacy tests are not useful for diagnosing adult learners’ skill deficits, particularly at the lowest literacy levels (Burt and Keenan, 1995). Tests such as the TABE, for example, determine grade-level reading ability rather than identifying learner competencies. These tests are also not achievement tests in the sense of determining what students have learned in their literacy classes (Lytle and Wolfe, 1989). The information that the tests provide is generally not useful to students themselves as the scores don’t translate into tangible learner goals or indicate what adults need to learn. Because practitioners in adult literacy programs are often not well trained in standardized test administration, their uses of these tests may invalidate the results (Business Council for Effective Literacy, 1990). Greater efforts need to be devoted to training adult literacy educators in the proper administration and interpretation of standardized assessments. Despite the limitations of standardized measures, it is certain that they will continue to be used in adult literacy programs. The data derived from such measures are assumed to be objective, and the tests are relatively easy to administer and can be given quickly to large groups of students. The results can be readily reported in a manner familiar to most people (e.g., mean scores or grade levels). 27 Adult Literacy Testing level of proficiency. An 80-percent probability of correct response was the criterion used by ETS. The difficulty level of each literacy task was then placed along a scale. The resulting performance of groups of test-takers was also plotted along the same scale. Thus, scoring of the PDQ measures was based on the difficulty of each scale item, the probability of correct responding, and respondent and task characteristics. ETS adopted a 500-point scale, with five “levels” of literacy ability, for the purposes of reporting the results of adult literacy assessments. Level 1 (0–225) is the lowest level. Adults who perform at this level are able to locate single pieces of information in brief texts and can accomplish relatively simple arithmetic operations. In contrast, adults at Level 5 (376–500) are able to make high-level inferences, using their background knowledge, when reading densely packed and complex texts and are able to perform multiple quantitative operations sequentially. There is some evidence that rather than assessing three dimensions of literacy ability, the PDQ approach actually taps into a single, general literacy ability. Stephen Reder (1998) has shown high intercorrelations among the three scales. The PDQ approach was first used for the Young Adult Literacy Survey (Kirsch and Jungeblut, 1985). This survey was conducted in 1985 to assess the literacy skills of a nationally representative sample of American adults age twentyone to twenty-five. The 1992 National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) was the first nationwide assessment of adults’ literacy abilities in the United States. More than 26,000 adults, age sixteen and older, participated in the NALS, completing both the PDQ tests and an extensive background interview that gathered data on demographic characteristics, language, labor-force participation, education background, civic participation, and literacy practices. The NALS results showed that about one-half of the adult population (estimated at 90 million adults) scored at the two lowest proficiency levels on the PDQ tests. Less than 5 percent scored at the highest level. These findings led to greater public attention to the problems of adult literacy in the United States. The National Center for Education Statistics plans to conduct its National Assessment of Adult Literacy in 2002 to evaluate the “state” of adult literacy at the beginning of the twenty-first century. Data from this study will be compared to the NALS in order to examine trends in adult literacy over a ten-year period and determine whether the United States has been successful in improving adult literacy. M. Cecil Smith See Also Adult Literacy; Adult Literacy Programs References Burt, Miriam, and Fran Keenan. 1995. Adult ESL Learner Assessment: Purposes and Tools. Washington, DC: National Clearinghouse for ESL Literacy Education. ERIC Document Reproduction Service, ED 386 962. Business Council for Effective Literacy. 1990. “Standardized Tests: Their Use and Misuse.” BCEL Newsletter for the Business Community 22 (January):6–9. Inman, Patricia, and Charles E. Trott. 1999. Pre-GED Assessment and Certification in Illinois: Final Report and Recommendations. De Kalb: Center for Governmental Studies, Northern Illinois University. Kirsch, Irwin S., and Ann Jungeblut. 1985. Literacy: Profiles of America’s Young Adults. Final Report. Washington, DC: National Assessment of Educational Progress, U.S. Department of Education. Kirsch, Irwin S., Ann Jungeblut, Lynne Jenkins, and Andrew Kolstad. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education. Lowe, Jean H. 1994. “Assessment of Adult Learners: Purpose and Context.” Mosaic: Research Notes on Literacy 4:2. Lytle, Susan L., and M. Wolfe. 1989. Adult Literacy Education: Program Evaluation and Learner Assessment. Columbus, OH: Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and Vocational Education. ERIC Document Reproduction Service, ED 315 665. Reder, Stephen. 1998. Dimensionality and Construct Validity of the NALS Assessment. In M. C. Smith, ed., Literacy for the Twenty-First Century: Research, Policy, Practices, and the National Adult Literacy Survey, pp. 37–57. Westport, CT: Praeger. Secretary’s Commission on Achieving Necessary Skills. 1991. What Work Requires of Schools: A SCANS Report for America 2000. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Labor. Venezky, Richard L., Daniel A. Wagner, and Barrie S. Ciliberti, eds. 1990. Toward Defining Literacy. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. 28 Artists’ Books American Reading Forum tack. In its mentoring program and in other sessions, graduate students are provided an opportunity to meet with leaders of their field without being cast into the role of sycophants. Small stipends are made available by members of the organization to help defray conference expenses for some of the organization’s most promising graduate-student members. Over the years, the organization has refined its goals to include the following: to provide a true forum for reading education where new research can be generated, research in progress can be refined, completed research can be reported, and reported research can be evaluated; to provide for the translation of reading research, theory, and philosophical deliberations into sound practice, but with no research, discussion, or contemplation to be discarded because its implementation is not immediately apparent; and to insure that in the field of reading, no idea is too bold or new to be given a hearing and none too old to be given consideration. More information can be found on the ARF web site: www.fd.appstate. edu/arfonline. Bob W. Jerrolds The American Reading Forum (ARF) is composed of a small group of scholars who seek to submit reading research and instructional practices to intense empirical scrutiny. The ARF was organized in 1980 and held its first annual conference in Sarasota, Florida, in December of that year under its original name, the American Reading Conference. The formation of the group grew out of concern that the papers presented at reading conferences and in reading journals were not subject to sufficient review and discussion. As reading programs expanded rapidly in the late twentieth century, an examination of those programs revealed too many instances in which individuals had six or seven minutes to present their research or scholarly papers. Since these conferences usually ran from two and a half to four days with several hundred presenters scheduled, there was little to no scholarly exchange regarding the presentations. The charter members of the organization were chiefly leaders in the reading fields, joined by some of their graduate students. A high percentage of the charter members had been presidents and members of the boards of other reading organizations. From its founding, the ARF has restricted its membership to 100–125 people. Its three-day conferences are always held in Florida at or near the end of the first semester of the school year. The programs continue to be limited to about forty papers, thus allowing time for reaction and discussion. One of several innovative aspects of the organization is that the papers are not submitted for consideration for publication in the organization’s yearbook until several weeks after the presentation at the annual conference. Thus, the authors can profit from criticism and suggestions received at the conference. Members of the audience are invited to submit reaction papers for possible publication in conjunction with the papers that are accepted for publication. The yearbook is peer reviewed and is indexed with major reference sources. The ARF has always sought to encourage and support graduate students who show the promise of becoming outstanding researchers and leaders in the reading field. The organization has tried to provide a forum in which researchers in training can present their research and ideas about research, receiving helpful criticisms and suggestions without being subjected to savage at- References Jerrolds, Bob W. 1990. “History of the American Reading Forum.” Yearbook of the American Reading Forum 10:193–198. Artists’ Books An artist’s book is a work of art in a book or booklike form that incorporates or exploits visual, tactile, structural, or textual elements. This sort of book can vary from mass-produced copies to limited editions with hand-set printing to one-of-a-kind books with painted embellishments. Artists’ books can assume many forms, ranging in style from the artist’s taking preexisting books and altering them to books made from nontraditional materials such as metal, beads, scraps of unusual paper, and natural objects like sticks or leaves. Some books are made to conceal all within; the pages may be glued, nailed, or fastened together in some manner so that the book may never be opened. Other books have smaller books hidden within so that there are several books to open. Some artists’ books are handbound with special bindings, whereas others have pages that are loosely held together or are not bound together at all. 29 Assessment Interviews Assessment Interviews Assessment interviews are oral questionnaires. They are conversations between a teacher and a student that are guided by predetermined questions and goals but allow for open-ended queries and spontaneous discussion when appropriate. Although assessment traditionally includes written tests, individual student work, portfolios, and grades, assessment interviewing provides an additional dimension. Emelie Lowrey Parker and her colleagues (1995) explain that when oral interviews are used, educators can gain insight into a child’s perspective and gather important information at a personal level. The child is then an active part of the assessment process. Assessment interviews can show that an educator values students’ voices, and this simple fact can motivate students to continue in their literacy development. The goal of assessment is to determine students’ instructional needs. Assessment should permeate the literacy curriculum daily so appropriate curriculum can be created to meet those needs. Terry Salinger (1998) lists three types of assessment: standardized, observation, and interviews. Although all three should be used to gain a full picture of a student’s abilities, interviews can supply an educator with students’ own views of their preferences, attitudes, strengths, and weaknesses. Assessment interviews show not just a product but a reflection about that product as well. An example of an assessment interview is included in the Motivation to Read Profile (MRP) developed by Linda Gambrell and her colleagues (1996). The MRP has two parts: a written reading survey and a conversational interview. The three sections of the conversational interview consist of factors related to narrative text, informational material, and reading motivation and habits. These educators realized that although the written survey was an important part of the profile, the conversational aspect of the assessment made it even more valuable. The purpose of the conversational interview is to gain insight into what motivates a student to read, to reveal interests that may inform the curriculum, and to elicit authentic knowledge about a student’s reading experiences and abilities. The assessment can follow a predetermined script, but tangents are anticipated and encouraged in order to provide a depth of understanding between teacher Crystal Wooten, author of artist books, displaying one of her books (Barbara Guzzetti) As more and more people became interested in handcrafted books in recent decades, the form of the book led artists to a new nontraditional format for expression. The book format has offered artists countless avenues for expression. Each artist’s book varies greatly. Hence, the definitions of an artist’s book are numerous, and somewhat controversial. Some of the controversy arises from the idea that a book has to behave like book; otherwise, it becomes sculpture. Also, artists’ books do not always conform to all aspects of a traditional book. Often, artists’ books are dangerously close to crossing the border between book and sculpture. When viewing such books, one important aspect to take into consideration is the artist’s intent. The intent of the artist makes an artist’s book different from other books because it was conceived and executed from the beginning as a work of art and as an artist’s book. Crystal Marie Wooten See Also Children’s Literature References LaPlantz, Shereen. 1995. Cover to Cover. Asheville, NC: Lark Books. 30 Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers and student. Specific questions from the MRP ask students to tell about interesting things they have read or learned, how they found out about these materials, and what was important about what they read. The questions probe what the students have read recently and what they think they need to learn to become a better reader. Although these questions were developed to apply to a majority of students and educators, teachers should always feel free to adjust, expand, or revise the questioning to fit the students’ needs or the teacher’s concerns. Thomas Gunning (1996) also suggested some interview questions that would be appropriate to assess reading, adapted from Mary Jett-Simpson (1990). These questions ask students to reflect upon how they get ready to read, what they do when reading is difficult, and how they understand what they have read. Questions similar to these could be constructed for other areas of literacy learning such as writing, listening, speaking, and spelling. Sharon Martens Galley (1996) recommends video- or audiotaping some interviews in order to show evidence of a student’s literacy progress. These tapes can then be revisited for further reflection or self-assessment purposes. Conducting assessment interviews takes patience and expertise. The interviews need to be informal and friendly, but also productive and efficient (Salinger, 1998). Interviews do not need to take place very often or be held for too long. Two or three questions asked monthly may work best for young children, whereas longer interviews held twice yearly may suit older students. The keys to successful assessment interviewing are well-chosen, thought-provoking questions and a probing, caring interviewer. The advantages of assessment interviews are clear. Although students may be able to participate successfully in other forms of assessment, the interview allows the educator to extend and adapt the questions to obtain a wider range of information, especially from the student’s perspective. For the student who is not proficient at more traditional types of assessment, the interview format may be even more crucial. The educator can conduct an interview to determine strengths, weaknesses, and curriculum needs when other forms of assessment do not produce the necessary results. However, weaknesses in the assessment interview are present as well. The usefulness of the interview depends on the will- ingness and ability of the students to discuss their literacy habits and beliefs (Gunning, 1996). Sometimes, information received from an interview may need to be verified by other assessment methods. A combination of assessment techniques—standardized, observational, written, and oral—may provide the best representation of a student’s literacy progress. Jill E. Cole See Also Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers; Classroom Writing Assessment; Reading Assessment; Writing Assessment; Writing Assessment in Large-Scale Contexts References Galley, Sharon Martens. 1996. “Talking Their Walk: Interviewing Fifth Graders about Their Literacy Journeys.” Language Arts 73:249–254. Gambrell, Linda, Barbara Martin Palmer, Rose Marie Codling, and Susan Anders Mazzoni. 1996. “Assessing Motivation to Read.” Reading Teacher 49:518–533. Gunning, Thomas G. 1996. Creating Reading Instruction for All Children. 2nd ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Jett-Simpson, Mary, ed. 1990. Toward an Ecological Assessment of Reading Progress. Schofield: Wisconsin State Reading Association. Parker, Emelie Lowrey, Regla Armengol, Leigh Baxley Brooke, Kelly Redmond Carper, Sharon Cronin, Anne Cooper Denman, Patricia Irwin, Jennifer McGunnigle, Tess Pardini, and Nancy P. Kurtz. 1995. “Teachers’ Choices in Classroom Assessment.” Reading Teacher 48:622–624. Salinger, Terry. 1998. “How Do We Assess Young Children’s Literacy Learning?” In Susan B. Neuman and Kathleen A. Roskos, eds., Children Achieving: Best Practices in Early Literacy, pp. 223–249. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers Interviewing can be defined as having a conversation with a definite purpose. Once the purpose for the interview is established, questions are generated to assist the interviewer in obtaining the desired information. Interviews of parents and teachers yield information that can be used to study a variety of attributes related to reading or writing, such as attitudes, perceptions, and feelings about the causes of a particular problem 31 Assessment Interviews for Parents and Teachers Teacher discussing a student with the student’s parents (Laura Dwight) tinent information about the children’s health and family history, and information about their children’s previous school experiences. To begin such an interview, parents might be asked a question such as, “Tell me about what your child reads at home.” If the child is struggling with reading, the parents might be asked to tell about their child’s reading or writing problem areas. Simply asking parents to talk about their children can be an excellent way to build credibility with them, garnering their support and assistance with the activities that will be performed by their children both in and out of school. Other sample questions are: “How do you think your child learned to read?” “How is reading used in your family?” “How is writing used in your family?” and “Do you visit the library?” Parents’ cultural backgrounds will most likely influence the interview. It is necessary to know how authority figures are viewed within the parents’ culture in order to ensure that appropriate conversational rules are used. There are some guidelines that can be used to enhance the chances of a successful interview. These include meeting in a private setting, avoiding educational area. Current research has revealed that interviews can provide reliable and valid information that cannot be obtained from other assessment measures. Further, because they can be very informal, much like having a conversation, interviews can be an excellent way to begin a more comprehensive assessment of children. At other times, the interview can be made more structured through the development and use of an interview guide. In these instances, a tape recorder is often used so that responses can be further analyzed. When thinking about using interviews to assess reading or writing behaviors, several different individuals need to be considered. Parents and teachers are valuable information sources that can shed light on how children approach and think about literacy-related events. Interviewing Parents One of the goals of interviewing parents is to obtain their assistance in understanding their children. Parents can yield information about the reading habits of the family such as the parents’ view of reading, parent-child relationships, parents’ attitudes about their children’s reading, per32 At-Risk Students jargon, listening actively, keeping note taking to a minimum, asking open-ended questions to get parents to talk, and assuring parents that the information they provide will be kept confidential. that as valuable as it can be, information from previous teachers is just that. When using an interview to access their perceptions of students, we have to guard against absorbing any preconceived ideas about individual children. For this reason, interviews with teachers are best used after the new teacher has had some time to get to know the children. Michael F. Opitz Interviewing Teachers Teachers who have worked with students are able to provide additional information about students that cannot be gained from other sources. For example, an interview with a teacher can reveal the teacher’s perception of learners and how the children have adjusted to the school environment. Identifying a teacher’s perception of learners is necessary because this information can then be used in selecting teaching strategies for specific lessons that will make students’ success more likely. Discovering teachers’ perceptions of the children’s ability to adjust to the school environment and expectations can reveal specific behaviors that may be interfering with learning. Once identified, these behaviors can be replaced with those that facilitate academic growth. How children’s reading and writing was assessed and diagnosed as well as the specific type of reading instruction that was given to children can also be revealed during an interview. Information such as this can often be a contributing factor to how children perform as they read and write. For example, if children appear to rely on using visual cues at the expense of using meaning, the interview could very well reveal that the children were taught with a phonics approach. As with interviews with parents, these teacher interviews should have a definite purpose, and specific questions must be established to reveal the information that is sought. Questions such as “How would you describe this child’s reading?” “What did you use to determine what the children needed to learn?” “What kinds of reading materials did you use?” “How do you make decisions about what children need from day to day?” “Did you have any concerns about any of the children that are now in my classroom?” are but a few of the questions that can be asked of teachers. Asking these questions in sincere, genuine ways makes for the best possible interview. The teacher being interviewed senses that there is a sincere desire to know rather than an attempt to obtain information to pass judgment on what has occurred in the past. Information from these interviews needs to be held in confidence. We also need to keep in mind See Also Assessment Interviews At-Risk Students At-risk is a term that entered the educational vernacular as a result of the widely read and often quoted study A Nation at Risk: The Imperative for Educational Reform (National Commission on Excellence in Education, 1983). Central to this study’s findings was the conclusion that thencurrent educational policy and practice, specifically with regard to reading instruction, caused substantial numbers of U.S. children to be at-risk for failure in learning to read and, ultimately, for being successful in school. In the nearly two decades since its initial use, at-risk has acquired many diverse meanings and has become a common expression used by educators, policymakers, the media, and the public alike to describe children and adolescents who exhibit one or more of a broad range of social, familial, economic, linguistic, cultural, and educational conditions associated with school failure. Thus, the term in its popular usage has evolved from its initial focus on educational policies and practices that place learners at-risk to a description of the learners themselves; during the 1980s and 1990s, it very nearly replaced the labels traditionally used to refer to the student experiencing reading problems—the “remedial reader,” “disabled reader,” “delayed reader,” and the like (see Delayed Readers). Along the way, at-risk has achieved common use with little or no agreement as to its exact meaning. Essentially, those who use it have their own definition in mind: Some assume a rather specific definition limited to an urban, innercity, poor and marginalized student population, whereas others use a broad-based definition of the term to refer to any student whose academic success is in jeopardy or who may be in danger of 33 At-Risk Students dropping out of school, whether the origin of the perceived risk is sociocultural or educational. Dorothy Strickland and Leslie Mandel Morrow (2000) identify factors that place young children at risk for failure in learning to read: (1) children with a history of preschool language impairment or delay, (2) children with limited proficiency in English or whose home English dialect is different from the dialect of instruction, (3) children whose parents had difficulty learning to read, (4) children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder, (5) children who lack motivation to learn to read or have few experiences with purposeful, pleasurable reading, (6) children from poor neighborhoods whose families lack sufficient resources for adequate housing, health care, and nutrition, and (7) children who attend schools in which classroom practices are deemed ineffective, regardless of community socioeconomic status. Richard Vacca and Nancy Padak (1990) give a definition of at-risk, older students: (1) students who are alienated from a system that has failed them, who never learned to read and rarely attempt to do so, (2) students who learned to read but whose participation in school is marginal, who can read but only do so under duress, and (3) students who demonstrate characteristics of “learned helplessness,” who feel they do not have the resources for overcoming failure and are further limited by low self-image and negative attitudes. learning to read when provided regular classroom reading instruction” (Klenk and Kibby, 2000) and these difficulties frequently follow them throughout their academic careers and sometimes throughout their lives. Interventions The question then becomes: How do we meet the needs of this small but significant portion of schoolchildren who struggle with literacy and are at-risk for school failure? The predominant approach throughout the twentieth century, which continues today in practice, was a medical model in which a specialist diagnosed the student’s reading problem and provided remediation. For the most part, Title I remedial reading programs in public schools, which were intended to serve at-risk students (see Title I), were and still are based on this medical deficit model in which the goal is to find out which of the skills taught in regular classroom reading instruction are missing for each student and then apply teaching to correct for those missing skills (Ruddell, 2001). To accommodate for this medical model, since their inception Title I remedial reading programs have predominantly been “pullout” programs in which, at the elementary level, children leave their regular classrooms at specified times and go to the remedial-reading class to work individually or in small groups with a reading specialist for the purpose of filling in or catching up on reading skills that were missed or not learned in regular instruction. For middlelevel and high-school students, Title I instruction is substituted for one elective class during a semester or year and may occur in small-group or individualized instruction in a resource- or remedial-room setting. Although it is difficult to assess Title I pullout programs with any accuracy because of the many nuanced differences in Title I remedial programs across the country, the general finding of studies conducted over the past twenty years is that the instruction these programs offer, for many complex and varied reasons, appears to assist students toward higher reading achievement but rarely promotes them to a level comparable to their mainstream peers (Klenk and Kibby, 2000). It is important to point out, however, that for many individual children, Title I remedial-reading instruction has provided much-needed and very successful support toward academic success. Criticisms of the Label Of concern to some educators is that we may be labeling too many students as “at-risk” by including students who exhibit any difference from the mainstream or who have talents not traditionally acknowledged in schools (Ruddell, 2001) and that our use of such labels as “at-risk” (or “disabled” or “remedial”) lead to the implicit conclusion that something is wrong with the students themselves (Klenk and Kibby, 2000). As a result, many educators seriously question the inclusion of linguistically and ethnically diverse students as a group into the category of at-risk and prefer terms for students who are experiencing difficulty with literacy that describe a situation rather than label the learner: “readers in trouble,” “struggling readers,” “delayed readers,” and the like. But whatever label or term is used to identify the situation, the fact is that “a small but significant portion of otherwise normal American children encounter major difficulties in 34 At-Risk Students Another approach for assisting at-risk learners that gained widespread acceptance in the 1990s was “early intervention,” in which the goal was to identify at-risk students—students scoring below the twentieth percentile on reading tests— very early in the learning-to-read stage of first grade and provide systematic, intensive one-onone instruction to prevent reading problems. Marie Clay’s Reading Recovery program (1979) was the primary model for early intervention programs and was used or adapted in many schools throughout the United States (see Reading Recovery). As with the Title I programs, Reading Recovery was predominantly a pullout program that provided systematic skill instruction and practice, writing experience, silent and oral-reading practice, and reading for pleasure. ing problems in learning to read and write are not missing out on large blocks of regular classroom instruction. Title I and regular classroom teachers are now frequently joined by America Reads volunteers who work one-on-one to tutor primary-grade children identified as at-risk in in-school and after-school programs. No research is currently available to evaluate the usefulness of the volunteer programs. Currently, instruction for at-risk children and older students remains centered in Title I and early intervention programs, and as can be imagined, these programs vary widely according to specific school and district characteristics and needs. A recent trend by policymakers and politicians is to address the many complex of issues associated with risk of school failure by setting higher academic standards, administering more and more high-stakes tests (see HighStakes Assessment), and retaining students in grade until they pass the tests. Schools responding to such policy and legislative mandates are attempting to provide various forms of Saturday-school, after-school (called “Cool School” in some districts), and summer-school programs that offer alternative experiences and instruction for students at-risk rather than simple repetition of grades that students have already failed (Posnick-Goodwin, 2001). “Pre-classes” or “halfclasses,” in which students do not repeat a grade but are placed in a class between two grade levels (e.g., pre-seventh grade between sixth and seventh grades), are considered to be intervention rather than retention or remedial instruction. “Pre-kindergarten” classes are for children who have not yet started school and who exhibit characteristics or meet criteria deemed to place them at-risk. There is still little or no evidence to determine the usefulness of these forms of intervention. Previous experience with mandatory minimumcompetence testing in the 1970s cautions that “intervention” based on high-stakes testing may cause dropout among older at-risk students to increase significantly. Thirty-five years of experience with Title I remedial programs cautions us further about the effectiveness of remedial programs in isolation from regular classroom instruction. The overwhelming evidence about the negative effect of school retention must give us pause in instituting wholesale retention programs for students who do not pass high-stakes tests. Recent Intervention Approaches Toward the end of the 1990s, Title I regulations changed to allow greater freedom for schools and districts to develop remedial-reading programs. Concurrently, there was a shift in philosophy regarding the usefulness of pullout programs and the role of the reading specialist. Specifically, educators began to question the value of removing students from their regular classroom instruction for the purpose of providing intensive remedial instruction. The concern was that by so doing, educators systematically deprived them of the very important developmental instruction that composes the content of the regular classroom, thus compounding students’ difficulty in acquiring age- and grade-appropriate literacy skills. This, in turn, called into question the medical model of diagnosis and treatment of reading problems, as well as the role of the reading specialist to provide stand-alone instruction outside of the regular classroom literacy curriculum. These influences produced new Title I remedial programs in which the reading specialist works alongside the classroom teacher in the regular class to provide services for children identified as at-risk. In such programs, remedial instruction is contextualized by and is a part of regular literacy instruction; the remedial teacher may teach non–Title I and Title I students together, giving special attention to the Title I children in a small group and designing instruction collaboratively with the classroom teacher to coincide with regular instruction. Of special benefit in this model of Title I instruction is that children experienc35 Authentic Assessment Shearer, Brenda A., Martha Rapp Ruddell, and MaryEllen Vogt. 2001. “Successful Middle School Reading Intervention: Negotiated Strategies and Individual Choice.” In Timothy Shanahan and Flora V. Rodriguez-Brown, eds., National Reading Conference Yearbook 50, pp. 558–571. Chicago: National Reading Conference. Strickland, Dorothy S., and Leslie M. Morrow. 2000. Beginning Reading and Writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Vacca, Richard, and Nancy D. Padak. 1990. “Who’s at Risk in Reading?” Journal of Reading 33 (7):486–488. Alternatives to Remediation and Intervention An alternative to the remedial and intervention approaches above is typified by the accelerated school movement led by Henry Levin (1993) in which the approach to at-risk learners is not to slow down their progress but rather to accelerate it, that is, to enrich instruction as one would for gifted students. Such an approach would result in instruction that (1) treats at-risk students as able, eager learners capable of learning from each other as well as from the teacher, (2) builds on students’ strengths instead of focusing on their weaknesses, (3) connects explicitly with students’ prior knowledge, previous experience, and the own lived worlds, and (4) involves many experiences in which students have the opportunity to reflect on their own literacy learning and development (Ruddell, 2000). Limited evidence (Shearer, Ruddell, and Vogt, 2001) suggests that such instruction is, indeed, beneficial for at-risk learners and, further, that their literacy accomplishment can, even in middle grades, place them at a level comparable with their mainstream peers. Martha Rapp Ruddell Authentic Assessment Two important features characterize authentic assessment. The first is the proximity of authentic assessment to valued classroom literacy routines. What a student does in the assessment situation is clearly related to the learning and achievement that are goals of schooling. Authentic assessment emanates from classroom practice, and it is conducted during regular activities of the classroom. As authentic assessment is embedded in classroom materials, routines, and instruction, the inferences made from the authentic assessment data may be more directly connected to curriculum, instruction, and learning. A second meaning for the term authentic assessment focuses on the relationship of the assessment to learning and performances inside and outside the classroom. In this sense, authentic assessment involves students’ performance of a variety of tasks in social contexts that anticipate the use of reading in the lives of students as related to personal fulfillment, creativity, and work. Combining these two related but distinct ideas describes authentic assessment in which students engage in representative and possibly complex tasks that are connected with regular and valued literacy activities inside and beyond the classroom. Authentic assessment may describe the diverse ways in which students grow and develop in the literacy curriculum. Although the two meanings are related, they reflect the broad and different manner in which authentic assessment may be conceptualized. They also hint at the confusion that may arise from assigning a single meaning to the term. For example, if a student spends classroom reading time practicing multiple-choice test items, then a multiple-choice test may be an authentic assessment, since it reflects See Also Delayed Readers; High-Stakes Assessment; Remediation; Title I References Clay, Marie M. 1979. The Early Detection of Reading Difficulties. Auckland, NZ: Heinemann. Klenk, Laura, and Michael W. Kibby. 2000. “Re-Mediating Reading Difficulties: Appraising the Past, Reconciling the Present, Constructing the Future.” In Michael L. Kamil, Peter B. Mosenthal, P. David Pearson, and Rebecca Barr, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 3, pp. 667–690. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Levin, Henry M. 1993. “Prologue.” In Wendy S. Hopfenberg et al., The Accelerated Schools Resource Guide, pp. xi–xvi. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. National Commission on Excellence in Education. 1983. A Nation at Risk: The Imperative for Educational Reform. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Posnick-Goodwin, Sherry. 2001. “Intervention and Retention: Schools Get Tough on Students Who Don’t Make the Grade.” California Educator 5 (7):6–11. Ruddell, Martha R. 2000. “Just a Closer Look: The Current Political Climate of Literacy Education in the USA.” WSRA Journal 43 (2):1–10. ———. 2001. Teaching Content Reading and Writing. 3rd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons. 36 Authentic Assessment classroom practice and instructional goals. This is an altogether different notion of authentic assessment—one in which students use reading strategies in a complex reading performance assessment that relates to their learning of reading strategies and content-domain knowledge. The authentic assessment of students’ literacy development may be conducted with various assessment materials and procedures. Following from the two definitions given previously, authentic assessment may include running records and reading inventories (see Informal Reading Inventory). These are conducted as students read, with teachers observing and recording the detail of students’ accomplishments and miscues. From this, teachers use on-line information about students’ oral reading and silent reading to develop an account of the processes students use to decode words and construct meaning. Other authentic assessment, including performance assessments, portfolios, interviews, and observation forms, allows for the examination of students’ literacy processes and products at varying degrees of complexity and in the social context of the classroom. In addition, teacher questioning and students’ answers and retellings are also authentic when they help determine the nature of students’ understanding. Such an array of authentic assessments can help describe students’ growth and achievement as they relate to the complex act of reading. Acting with the knowledge gained from reading, students may be involved in performance assessments. Sharing of rubrics in performance assessment helps demonstrate the relationship of instruction and assessment: the means by which students will be evaluated serve as a map for how to perform well. In this sense, performance assessment rubrics may serve as a guide to both instruction and assessment. Literacy portfolios may be authentic assessments, serving as repositories for students’ work and contexts for students’ reflection and self-assessment. Adept use of portfolios also helps students and teachers conduct authentic assessment of complex and long-term projects, such as writing in relation to reading. The ability to document and organize the progression of work within the portfolio contributes to the unique contributions possible with portfolios. The popularity of authentic assessment is fueled, in part, by the preponderance of large-scale multiple-choice tests and their limited ability to describe students’ complex literacy growth (Valencia, Hiebert, and Afflerbach, 1994). Such tests are criticized because they provide little or no formative assessment information and no immediately useful information, and they do not reflect the breadth and depth of learning that occur in a rich literacy curriculum. If complex learning is specified in standards documents (see Standards) and curriculum guides, the measures used to assess it must be capable of reflecting and accurately measuring this complexity. Authentic assessment materials and procedures reflect the complexity of the literacy growth they are intended to describe. Authentic assessment is often teacher- and student-centered, which may allow for the immediate and effective use of assessment information in the classroom. Authentic assessment may serve a formative purpose, as teachers use the information to shape instruction and provide feedback for students. Some consider authentic assessment to be an optimal means of measuring and describing the progress of students, teachers, and schools. It is also a means of revealing epistemologies, theories of what teaching is, how children learn, and what they might learn. For example, an authentic assessment that requires students to read two texts and then write an original account of how the texts are similar and how they differ does not mesh with the view of the reader as a passive recipient of information from text. The characteristics of particular assessment materials and procedures are not the sole determinants of whether the assessment is authentic. Rather, it is how these materials and procedures connect with the curriculum, how information from the assessment is used, and what the assessment demands of students that signifies an assessment as authentic. Reading assessments must be considered not as isolated instruments and procedures but as parts of the complex world of schooling (American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National Education Association, 1997). The promise of authentic assessment is realized, in part, when it is developed and selected according to complex criteria. Historically, assessment is judged by the psychometric standards of validity and reliability. It is imperative to augment this important information through the identifica37 Authentic Assessment tion of the potential benefits and demands of any authentic assessment. Diane Leipzig and Peter Afflerbach (2000) developed the CURRV (consequences, usefulness, roles and responsibilities, reliability, and validity of assessment) framework for examining the suitability of assessment for measuring and describing students’ literacy growth. Applied to authentic assessment, the framework can be used to anticipate and examine the consequences of assessment, its usefulness, and the roles and responsibilities it creates. This information complements the important aspects of the reliability and validity of the assessment. questions such as: How appropriate is my lesson? Should I change the learning goals and targets? How are individual students doing in their small groups? and How can I accommodate each learner in this diverse classroom? Similarly, students may receive feedback from authentic assessment that provides details on the ways they are succeeding or on the ways they need to improve on a particular literacy strategy or skill. Authentic assessment can provide detailed information on students’ performance at complex tasks. For example, the innovative curriculum that asks students to perform tasks that reflect the complexity of work inside and outside the classroom must be accompanied by sophisticated assessment that is capable of describing that complexity. Quality, detailed information that guides instruction, that informs students and their families, that demonstrates good learning and teacher ability to conduct such assessment is useful in building goodwill throughout the school community. The Consequences of Assessment The potential consequences of a successful authentic assessment program include effective use of school time and resources and teachers’ and students’ participation in the culture of assessment. Authentic assessment may take place during the course of regular classroom literacy activities. Thus, the need to take valuable classroom time from instruction and learning to prepare students to take an assessment is lessened. A related consequence is that time and money may be saved because authentic assessment information is generated in situ. A further consequence is that students may not have to learn complex routines of transforming their procedural and content knowledge for a specific testing scenario, because authentic assessment is conducted as teaching and learning occur. Enhanced motivation may be a further consequence: students who learn to become invested in reading assessment may be more motivated to learn. Reading assessment that is clearly aligned with instruction and conducted within instruction may provide teachers and students with the opportunity to better understand and use assessment. Students may develop independence in the important task of self-assessment, which is central to success in literacy endeavors. Roles and Responsibilities As described by Scott Paris and his colleagues (1992), authentic assessment is the result of a complex process of determining learning goals, relating assessment materials and procedures to those goals, and developing the expertise to develop, administer, and interpret authentic literacy assessment results. Each of these key areas demands that clear and specific roles and responsibilities be determined, assigned, and fostered. Teachers must become assessment experts (Johnston, 1997) and must develop the ability to effectively collect, interpret, and use assessment information. This is especially the case in an era of high-stakes assessments: advocates of authentic assessment must demonstrate that alternatives to multiple-choice machine-scored tests meet psychometric standards of validity and reliability (see High-Stakes Assessment). To help teachers meet the challenge presented by such new roles and responsibilities, school districts must provide the extensive professional development that is needed—authentic assessment done well is a challenging task. Students must adopt a more active approach to assessment, moving from the passive state that is often fostered by assessments that are removed considerably in time and space from literacy learning events. In addition, the pertinent characteristics of authentic Usefulness of Authentic Assessment Authentic assessment proves useful as it provides detailed information related to current instruction and complex learning tasks and accomplishments. It is characterized by its close proximity to instruction and learning and by the immediacy with which it can provide formative feedback in the midst of teaching and learning. This information can help teachers frame and answer 38 Authentic Assessment assessment should be supported by the communication of results to all stakeholders, including students, teachers, parents and families, administrators, board members, legislators, the business community, and other interested parties. This information may include the specific nature of a new assessment, the benefits it offers in relation to an existing assessment, and examples of the detail and usefulness of the information that the new assessment yields. expertise in developing, conducting, and interpreting authentic assessment information is crucial. As standardized tests are framed by aspirations of the equivalent treatment of each student who is taking a test, it is imperative that teachers develop the ability to assess authentically in a reliable manner. That is, they must build a set of predictable and fair assessment routines to conduct authentic assessment. Further, teachers must strive to develop reliable means for analyzing the authentic assessment information they collect. As authentic assessments often involve examination of students’ written and spoken work, it is imperative that teachers regularly reflect on how bias for or against a particular language, performance, or student may be present and held in check. The complexity of tasks and performances that students undertake in authentic assessment create a corresponding complexity for teachers gathering and interpreting this assessment information. Peter Afflerbach Validity of Authentic Assessment Authentic literacy assessment must meet rigorous standards of validity (Messick, 1994). Construct validity, or the accurate relation of an authentic assessment to the construct of reading, is made possible because authentic assessment can more fully represent the learning and growth that result from effective reading instruction. Ecological validity may be guaranteed when authentic assessment is done well because assessment takes place around instruction and learning and does not involve a knowledge-transformation task. It does not seek to describe reading achievement with means that are not reflective of typical classroom literacy acts, and there is little or no perturbation of the learning environment in the service of the assessment. Similarly, content validity, or the relation of the authentic assessment to the curriculum and instruction, may be enhanced as the assessment comes from instruction and learning. Concurrent validity of authentic assessment may pose a challenge because it hinges on the degree to which an authentic literacy assessment relates to other measures of literacy. Authentic assessment may be used because there is strong agreement that existing high-stakes assessments are not sensitive to the breadth and depth of student learning. In such instances, high positive correlations between the results of two assessments that focus on literacy but differ substantially in how assessment is conceptualized and measured should not be expected. See Also High-Stakes Assessment; Informal Reading Inventory; Kidwatching and Classroom Evaluation; Portfolios; Standards References American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National Education Association. 1997. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National Education Association. Johnston, Peter. 1997. Knowing Literacy: Constructive Literacy Assessment. Portland, ME: Stenhouse. Leipzig, Diane, and Peter Afflerbach. 2000. “Determining the Suitability of Assessments: Using the CURRV Framework.” In L. Baker, M. Dreher, and J. Guthrie, eds., Engaging Young Readers: Promoting Achievement and Independence, pp. 159–187. New York: Guilford Press. Messick, Sam. 1994. “The Interplay of Evidence and Consequences in the Validation of Performance Assessments.” Educational Researcher 23 (2):13–23. Paris, Scott, Robert Calfee, Nicola Filby, Elfrieda Hiebert, P. David Pearson, Sheila Valencia, and Kenneth Wolf. 1992. “A Framework for Authentic Literacy Assessment.” Reading Teacher 46:88–98. Valencia, Sheila, Elfrieda Hiebert, and Peter Afflerbach. 1994. Authentic Assessment: Practices and Possibilities. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Reliability of Authentic Assessment The determination that authentic assessment is reliable remains a final goal of determining the suitability of authentic assessment. There must be confidence that the authentic assessment materials and procedures that are used with twenty or twenty-five different students within a single classroom are fair and consistent. Here, teacher 39 Automaticity and Reading Fluency Automaticity and Reading Fluency Gay Sue Pinnell and her colleagues in 1995) on a national sample of fourth graders (see National Assessment of Educational Progress). In this study, the National Assessment of Educational Progress found that 44 percent of the fourth graders were not fluent readers. There were high correlations between fluency and comprehension. Fourth graders who were not fluent at word recognition tended to have difficulty with comprehension. As the body of knowledge about automaticity and fluency has increased, there is growing recognition of its importance. Although the terms automaticity and fluency are often used interchangeably, there is a subtle difference in how these two terms are used. Automaticity is the more general term; it refers to a wide variety of highly skilled behaviors and thought processes that can be executed with little conscious effort or attention as the result of long periods of practice. Reading fluency, by contrast, although similar to the definition of automaticity, has its use limited to skilled reading performance. Fluency refers to the ability to comprehend a text with little conscious effort or attention as the result of long periods of practice. Fluent readers can read orally from a text they have never seen before with expression, accuracy, speed, and comprehension. Although most teachers recognize the importance of reading fluency, unfortunately, little is done specifically to help students reach this goal. D. Ray Reutzel and Paul Hollingsworth (1991) found that instruction in reading fluency has been a neglected part of reading instruction, despite the fact that many reading authorities consider it to be important. When David LaBerge and Jay Samuels first wrote their article in 1974 on automatic information processing in reading, they focused only on word recognition, but in the last twenty years, the concept of fluency has enlarged so that it now includes comprehension processes as well word recognition. Reading educators use the term reading fluency in referring to a highly developed level of skill attainment that may range from ease of decoding printed material to complicated inferential operations used in constructing meaning from the printed material on the page. For example, when fluent readers read a text such as the following, they make numerous automatic inferences: “The man was puffing away absentmindedly while walking through the woods. Many animals and trees were destroyed in the ensuing fire.” In reading this text, the reader automatically infers that the man was puffing on a lit cigarette, that he dropped it on flammable material on the ground, and thus caused a fire. Stages of Reading Development In the development and assessment of automatic complex skills such as reading, it is useful to know what stages the beginning student passes through on the road to fluency. Knowledge of the characteristics of each of the stages can be useful to teachers in their diagnosis and remediation of reading difficulties. The three stages from beginning to fluent reading are: nonaccurate, accurate but nonfluent, and accurate and fluent. During the nonaccurate beginning stage of reading skill, when students are asked to read a new text orally, reading speed is slow, with many word-recognition errors, and expression and comprehension are poor. With considerable instruction and practice, the student enters the accurate but nonfluent stage. When accurate but nonfluent students are asked to read a new text orally, wordrecognition accuracy is high, speed of reading is still slow, and oral-reading expression and comprehension are poor. Again, after considerable practice reading, the student enters the third stage, which may be termed the accurate and fluent stage. When fluent readers are asked to read orally from a new text, their word recognition, speed, expression, and comprehension are good. Property Lists for Determining Automaticity There are still other ways to determine when a student is automatic, or fluent, in reading. Researchers have put together property lists that can be used to distinguish automatic from nonautomatic processes. The reason for the property lists is that some nonautomatic processes can masquerade as automatic. Property lists help to determine if the execution of a complex skill was done automatically or not. For example, a student may be able to read a text orally with high word-recognition accuracy and Importance of Reading Fluency The importance of reading fluency is underscored by a study done by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (as reported by 40 Automaticity and Reading Fluency speed, and a teacher might mistakenly assume the student is fluent when in fact the student is not. The reason for good word recognition coupled with poor comprehension is that if the decoding task consumes considerable amounts of the student’s cognitive resources, comprehension suffers. Two additional properties must be exhibited before identifying the student as a fluent reader. The oral reading should have good expression and the student should be able to recall some of the major points in the reading passage. Appropriate expression is important because it is an indicator that the student comprehends the passage while decoding it. Lack of expression is not the sign of an uncooperative student, but of a student who has difficulty understanding a text while decoding. If a student can engage in two complex tasks at the same time, then at least one of them is automatic. In the case of reading, where decoding and comprehension are complex tasks, if the student can perform both simultaneously, as is the case in oral reading with comprehension, then at least one of the tasks is automatic. We can assume that it is the decoding that is automatic. Additional properties of complex skills learned to the automatic level include the ability to perform the skill with minimal attention, without conscious awareness, and without interfering with other processes that are occurring at the same time. The last characteristic is critical, especially as it pertains to reading. As stated earlier, the decoding task should not interfere with the comprehension task. If it does, then the student is not yet at the fluent reading stage. To this property list, we can add additional characteristics, such as the complex skill that comes about as the result of long periods of practice; once it is activated, the automatic skill is difficult to suppress. For example, we know that when we are driving an automobile, we should focus attention on the traffic and we should try to suppress our desire to read the words on a billboard. Nevertheless, when fluent readers see words on a billboard, they have difficulty suppressing their desire to read them. istics of automaticity and fluency may be considered to be on a continuum rather than as bipolar traits. To illustrate the importance of considering automaticity and fluency on a continuum rather than as bipolar traits, consider reading speed. Reading speed for the beginning reader is characteristically slow. With practice, however, the speed of reading increases, but the shift to fast reading is not abrupt. Instead, it is gradual. Data gathered on reading speed for students over time reveal a gradual, continuous improvement in speed in which only the beginning and end points can be identified as “slow” or “fast.” Reading speed, like other aspects of fluency, such as accuracy of word recognition or ability to read a text orally with expression, show gradual and incremental improvement resulting from practice. Word Recognition Several researchers, such as Robert Calfee and Dale C. Piontkowski (1981) and Keith Stanovich (1985), reported that development of automaticity in word recognition is associated with improved comprehension. To understand why automatic word-recognition skills can influence comprehension, three basic components of the reading process must be considered. First, in order to read a text, attention is needed to perform the decoding and comprehension processes. Unfortunately, the amount of attention that is available for performing these tasks is limited. Second, the words in the text must be decoded, or recognized. Third, the reader must comprehend the text. For the beginning reader, the decoding act is so difficult that all of the available attention is used up just for that one task, leaving an insufficient amount of attention for the comprehension process. To comprehend the text, the beginning reader resorts to the strategy of switching attention back and forth from decoding to comprehension, but this strategy places a considerable load on memory. Thus, by switching attention back and forth, the beginning reader manages to understand the text, but only with great effort. As the result of extended time spent in reading practice, the decoding task becomes automatic. When the decoding becomes automatic, the word-recognition task can be done with minimal attention, leaving the reader free to focus attention on the task of comprehension. At this stage of reading development, when both the decoding and comprehension tasks are per- Caveats Having presented this property list as a binary dichotomous state—that is, either one is automatic or not—there is a caveat worth stating. A strong argument can be made that the character41 Automaticity and Reading Fluency formed simultaneously, we can say the reader is fluent. One of the reasons that fluency is so important as a reading goal is that automatic word recognition allows the reader to concentrate on constructing meaning from the text. Being a fluent or automatic reader should not be thought of as a stage in which all text can be processed with ease. At times, the reader may encounter texts where uncommon words, such as epistrophe, anfractuous, and contralesional may appear. If this occurs, the reader is usually not automatic at recognizing these words, and then effortful decoding strategies will have to be used. The highly skilled automatic reader has the option of either recognizing words in an effortless, automatic manner or in a style that uses high levels of attention and effort. The beginning reader, however, does not have these options available. Virtually all word recognition engaged in by beginning readers uses large amounts of attention and requires considerable effort, thus impeding comprehension. points made in the passage that the student is reading. What makes this simple procedure such a good diagnostic tool for reading fluency is that the procedure requires the student to decode and comprehend it at the same time. Those students who are not automatic at word recognition will find their ability to comprehend while reading orally to be severely compromised. Diagnosing Reading Stages An important aspect of the teacher’s knowledge of reading instruction should be the ability to assess the stage the reader has reached. A simple procedure for diagnosing a student’s reading stage involves selecting a text of one to two pages in length from the books that the student uses at school and having the student read the text orally to the teacher. The text must be one that the student has not read before. Instructions to the student are to read the text orally, and when the reading is completed, the student must recall as much of the text as possible. As the student reads orally, the teacher keeps a record of word-recognition errors, expression, and reading speed. If the student makes more than 10-percent wordrecognition errors, if the reading speed is less than eighty-five words a minute, or if there is lack of oral-reading expression, there is a high probability the student is not automatic at word recognition. Of critical diagnostic importance is the student’s ability to capture some of the See Also Comprehension Strategies; National Assessment of Educational Progress; Word Recognition References Calfee, Robert C., and Dale C. Piontkowski. 1981. “The Reading Diary: Acquisition of Decoding.” Reading Research Quarterly 16:346–373. LaBerge, David, and S. Jay Samuels. 1974. “Toward a Theory of Automatic Information Processing in Reading.” Cognitive Psychology 6 (2):293–323. Pinnell, Gay Sue, Jean J. Pikulski, Karen K. Wixson, John R. Campbell, Phil B. Gough, and Adrian S. Beatty. 1995. Listening to Children Read Aloud. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, U.S. Department of Education. Reutzel, D. Ray, and Paul M. Hollingsworth. 1991. “Reading Comprehension Skills: Testing the Distinctiveness Hypothesis.” Reading Research and Instruction 30:32–46. Samuels, S. Jay. 1979. “The Method of Repeated Reading.” Reading Teacher 32:403–408. Stanovich, Keith. 1985. “Concepts in Developmental Theories of Reading Skill.” Developmental Review 10:72–100. Instructing for Reading Fluency If the diagnostic test of reading fluency indicates that the student is not automatic at the decoding task, one of the best methods for helping beginning readers become fluent is to use the method of repeated reading (Samuels, 1979). There are numerous experimental studies showing the efficacy of this method. The other way to help students become fluent readers is to have them spend considerable time reading enjoyable books and stories that are at their recreational level of ability. S. Jay Samuels 42 B Balanced Literacy Instruction effective than any single one. H. Alan Robinson (1977) noted that during the 1920s and 1930s, reading-skill sequences and activities predominated but were often accompanied by supplemental materials that contained “realistic” reading passages. He further reported that in the 1940s and 1950s, skill charts broke word identification and comprehension down into teachable parts, but basal-reader selections were often integrated into the other language arts. Although heavy reliance on basal materials and skill instruction continued into the 1960s, recommendations for balanced or eclectic literacy instruction were not uncommon. For example, following their research on elementary reading instruction, Mary Austin and Coleman Morrison (1963) concluded that multiple approaches should be utilized rather than a single method. A balanced view also appears to represent more contemporary teachers. A mid-1990s survey revealed that 89 percent of elementaryschool teachers identified with a “balanced approach” and 76 percent concurred that they adhered to an “eclectic” perspective toward reading instruction (Baumann et al., 2000). Balanced literacy instruction involves a combination of instruction in literacy skills and strategies and immersion in literature and literary experiences. The instructional component typically includes teacher-initiated or guided lessons in reading and writing strategies in the context of authentic literature and composition tasks. The immersion component includes daily reading and writing activities such as teacher read-alouds, self-selected independent reading, written composition, oral expression, and literature response (Ivey, Baumann, and Jarrard, 2000). Although current conceptions of balanced literacy vary in focus and emphasis, most involve some combination of teaching reading and writing strategies and holistic literacy experiences, with teacher decisionmaking a central tenet. The future of balanced literacy depends upon teachers’ abilities to orchestrate the multiple instruction and immersion structures required to implement an effective balanced literacy program. It also requires the support of administrators who will provide teachers with the freedom to initiate balances that address students’ unique literacy instructional needs. Current Conceptions Recent research and writings on balanced literacy instruction have been generated by researchers and writers such as James Baumann, Susan BlairLarsen, James Cunningham, Ann Duffy, Laurie Elish-Piper, Jill Fitzgerald, Gay Ivey, Jerry Johns, Ellen McIntyre, P. David Pearson, Michael Pressley, Taffy Raphael, D. Ray Reutzel, Dixie Lee Spiegel, Ruth Wharton-McDonald, and Kathryn Williams, among others. Their ideas and work ground the following description. Writers such as Jill Fitzgerald and Michael Pressley have noted that balanced literacy is not Historical Precedent Use of the terms balanced reading or balanced literacy is a recent phenomenon, but there is historical precedent for combining skill instruction and literary experiences. Although there has never before been a balanced era in American reading instruction, early in the twentieth century several prominent figures in literacy education advocated a version of a “balanced,”“eclectic,”“combination,” or “composite” reading program. For example, William S. Gray (1925) argued that a combination of reading methods would be more 43 Balanced Literacy Instruction a monolithic concept but instead comes in multiple manifestations. Dixie Lee Spiegel’s (1998) characterization, however, has captured many of the common features of balanced literacy. She has argued that balanced literacy is research based and flexible, grounded on the view of teachers as thoughtful informed decisionmakers, and built upon a comprehensive view of literacy. This comprehensive view includes word identification, meaning construction through efferent and aesthetic stances, expressive written composition, and the development of an appreciation and lifelong pursuit of literacy for self-fulfillment and learning. Balanced literacy is not without its critics, however. Frank Smith and Constance Weaver, for example, have warned of the danger of providing children with an eclectic mix of methods and materials devoid of focus or philosophy. Further, David Pearson and Taffy Raphael (1999) have cautioned that balanced programs may oversimplify the complexities of literacy instruction; instead, they have argued, teachers must exercise multiple balances of contextual and curricular factors in order to provide effective literacy instruction. Spiegel (1998) has made clear that teacher decisionmaking is central to a balanced literacy approach, which helps teachers become reflective when determining how to accommodate students’ individual needs through the reading curriculum and instruction. ing the school day. These teachers reported that what they learned from working with one student at a time helped them to reconsider the needs of all students in their classes. Similarly, Randy Bonner (1999) has described some of the subtleties of teaching reading that teachers learned as they tutored individual struggling readers, developed case studies of their students, and exchanged their experiences with peer teachers in a study group. We view professional development opportunities such as these as crucial to the future of balanced literacy instruction. Ann M. Duffy, Gay Ivey, and James F. Baumann See Also Reading-Comprehension Instruction References Austin, Mary C., and Coleman Morrison. 1963. The First R: The Harvard Report on Reading in Elementary Schools. New York: Macmillan. Baumann, James F., James F. Hoffman, Jennifer S. Moon, and Ann M. Duffy-Hester. 2000. “The First R Yesterday and Today: U.S. Elementary Reading Instruction Practices Reported by Teachers and Administrators.” Reading Research Quarterly 35:338–377. Bonner, Randy. 1999. “Conferring with Struggling Readers: The Test of Our Craft Knowledge, Courage, and Hope.” New Advocate 12 (1):21–38. Broaddus, Karen, and Janet W. Bloodgood. 1999. “We’re Supposed to Already Know How to Teach Reading: Teacher Change to Support Struggling Readers.” Reading Research Quarterly 34:426–451. Duffy, Gerald G., and James V. Hoffman. 1999. “In Pursuit of an Illusion: The Flawed Search for a Perfect Method.” Reading Teacher 53:10–16. Gray, William S. 1925. Summary of Investigations Relating to Reading. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ivey, Gay, James F. Baumann, and Dora Jarrard. 2000. “Exploring Literacy Balance: Iterations in a Second-Grade and a Sixth-Grade Classroom.” Reading Research and Instruction 39 (4):291–310. Pearson, P. David, and Taffy E. Raphael. 1999. “Toward an Ecologically Balanced Literacy Curriculum.” In Linda B. Gambrell, Lesley Mandel Morrow, Susan B. Neuman, and Michael Pressley, eds., Best Practices in Literacy Instruction, pp. 22–33. New York: Guilford Press. Robinson, H. Alan. 1977. “Reading Instruction and Research: In Historical Perspective.” In H. Alan Robinson, ed., Reading and Writing Instruction in the United States: Historical Trends, pp. 44–58. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Spiegel, Dixie Lee. 1998. “Silver Bullets, Babies, and Bath Water: Literature Response Groups in a The Future Consistent with current conceptions about the complexity of balanced literacy, Gerald Duffy and James Hoffman (1999) recently argued that the teacher, not the particular method or combination of methods, is the key to effective reading instruction. The future of balanced literacy depends on teachers cultivating expertise in teaching individual children and then applying that one-on-one mentality when teaching an entire class. Cultivating this expertise will undoubtedly be a complicated process of professional development, particularly when juxtaposed against the more common, single-session, methods-oriented workshops; however, there are examples that hold much promise. For example, Karen Broaddus and Janet Bloodgood (1999) have documented the changes in the philosophy and practice of experienced teachers who were given the opportunity to tutor struggling readers dur44 Basal Readers Balanced Literacy Program.” Reading Teacher 52 (2):114–124. Bible and its teachings was the earliest impetus for reading instruction. Nevertheless, the notion of universal literacy is an American creation linked to the establishment of free public-school education. In the late eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries, the work of German educators influenced the thinking, materials, and methods used for instructing American children (Hoffman, 2001). In the earliest American method of teaching reading, the student learned the names of the individual letters, then repeating syllables using those letters. This “alphabet method,” going back to Greek and Roman reading instruction methodology, assumed that learning the names of letters by rote would lead to reading words. Letter names were considered to be the basic unit. Reading materials for the youngest students often consisted of several pages of alphabet letters and syllables to recite. It was considered an educational breakthrough when reading instruction began to use the “word method” as a way to teach reading rather than beginning with individual letter names (Betts, 1957). In the word method, famously promoted by Horace Mann in the first half of the nineteenth century, students were taught whole words first. It was assumed that words, rather than letters, were the basic unit. Because little was known at the time about helping students learn whole words and how to apply the words taught in isolation to larger pieces of text or to transfer the skills to other words, students struggled with this method. Even though the word method received a lot of attention in the professional papers of the time, it never supplanted the alphabet method in the mid-nineteenth century for teaching beginning reading and had no effect at all on how reading was taught to older students. Later on, reading would blend the word method with the phonetic method for a more successful approach. Using an entirely different perspective, a sixteenth-century (or nineteenth, depending upon sources) German educator named Valentin Ickelsaomer experimented with teaching young children to read using a “phonetic method” in the early 1500s (Matthews, 1966). This approach viewed the sounds of letters as the basic unit and recognized that letters represented multiple sounds in different words. It was assumed that teaching beginning readers those sounds would lead to earlier reading success for more children. Basal Readers The most common method of introducing and practicing reading in the United States is still to use basal readers, a series of graded passages in book sets that students read, sometimes moving through their grade-level text as a class and sometimes moving through the text in ability groups that are paced differently. Although the overall structure looks very much like that of three-quarters of a century ago, the reading selections and strategies included today have morphed into a different genre—basal anthologies with selections from children’s trade books (literature) and magazines used to teach reading. The majority of current basals still use an analytic (learning sound-symbol relationships in context and through spelling patterns) rather than a synthetic (learning sound-symbol relationships through systematic, explicit instruction of individual phonemes) approach to word learning. The morphing continues, with impetus from “the reading wars” (public debates of the synthetic versus analytic phonics proponents) of the late twentieth and early twenty-first centuries, both with the addition of strong phonics strands in the basal anthologies and with newer basals using reading texts that recycle linguistic approaches from the past. The Beginnings of Basal Readers “See Spot. See Spot run. Run, Spot, run!” Many who read these words will experience a flash of recognition from first grade. Others will only recognize the words as the butt of jokes on greeting cards or in comedy sketches. Your reaction probably depends upon when you learned to read. Indeed, until Sally, Dick, and Jane “died” in the early 1970s, roughly twenty-five years after their introduction on the American reading scene, an overwhelming number of American primary-grade children learned to read with Scott, Foresman basal readers. But how did children learn to read before Sally, Dick, and Jane? The history of reading instruction in this country is a fascinating amalgam of sociology, psychology, and the American fascination with innovation and expediency. Teaching people to read so they could access the 45 Basal Readers In America’s earliest years, reading was viewed as a highly utilitarian activity. The main purpose for teaching reading was to encourage access to biblical teachings. Nationalism dominated in the early days of the United States, and reading materials such as Noah Webster’s eighteenth-century An American Spelling Book (the blue-back speller) were used to promote patriotism and moral behavior that would lead to an ethically strong country (Smith, 1965). Indeed, it was a breakthrough to introduce the concept of using connected text whose purpose was as much to influence the moral well-being of the reader as to teach reading skills rather than syllables for early reading. Pleasure reading, however, was not a goal of schools until much later. Although the word method was not the predominant way of teaching reading, it exerted some influence by leading to sentence reading followed by story reading. With stories as reading materials in the first set of “graded readers,” reading texts became increasingly more difficult as the reader progressed through them. Children might continue with the same reader for more than one school year as they worked their way through the reading selections. As a result, several different ages of students could be reading from the same book. As the materials were very difficult for beginning readers, with very little repetition of vocabulary, it took considerable time for students to learn the material. Learning material meant memorizing it and reciting the memorized text verbatim to the teacher. Only when students had “learned the lesson” could they progress to the next selection. Meaning was not checked. The assumption was that if students could repeat the words, they knew the meaning of the text, an assumption today’s educators know to be false. Although there were other graded reading materials, McGuffey’s Eclectic Readers dominated the American scene for nearly 100 years, into the twentieth century. ing, the Europeans, particularly the Germans, produced a great deal of professional literature. American educators of the time read this literature and sought changes in American schools and materials that would reflect the advances of such European educators as Johann Pestalozzi. Graded readers—the early basals (1840s to 1860s)—were produced partly to fill the recognized need for materials that became gradually more difficult, but they were mainly introduced to accommodate readers’ needs in the new system of organizing schools in the cities into grade-level groupings (Smith, 1965). America set out to educate its entire populace with public funds. This challenge brought a wide range of reading abilities into the school system. The materials needed to reflect the range. Evolution also occurred as companies producing basal readers proliferated and competed for market share. The influence on how teachers instructed students began to change with the advent in 1839 of the first “normal school” preparing teachers. The early basals typically included letter naming and syllable production, along with spelling, for the youngest readers. The syllables became words, which then evolved into sentence-level reading. Those sentences moved into a loose story from a collection of sentences. Through the reading of the words in sentences, students were taught to decode the sounds of English. Readers for older students included not only stories but lessons in grammar and spelling. Spelling words was the major decoding strategy employed. The stories in the earliest reading materials were very didactic. Basals remained largely the same until the early twentieth century, when stories were made more appealing to children with the introduction of continuing characters whose daily lives played out in basal selections. Contemporary Basal-Reading Materials Forty years ago, arguments similar to today’s, though not as intensely followed by the press, were expressed about analytic versus synthetic approaches to reading instruction. Nila Banton Smith’s American Reading Instruction (1965) pushed that argument back to the nineteenth century. Teaching the youngest students to read has garnered the majority of attention in the field almost since reading instruction became a separate subject. Proponents of analytic approaches thought Changes to Basal Readers The change from teaching reading via hornbooks (a primer in which the alphabet and religious material were affixed to a wooden frame underneath a transparent sheet of horn) to the blue-back speller to McGuffey’s readers was in response to several forces. Even though America generated scant professional material about teaching read46 Basal Readers the whole process of learning to read ought to be slowed down so that children learned more reallife skills and explored firsthand academic subjects such as science. Slowing down the process involved teaching letter names and sounds as needed by the texts selected rather than apart from the texts. Analytic approaches allowed more focus on developing appreciation for reading and for comprehending text, features missing from early instruction with synthetic approaches. Proponents of synthetic reading showed that with systematic and explicit instruction, children learned letter names and sounds more quickly than with analytic approaches. They also argued that the stilted, unnatural language of the earlier era’s basal readers for young children did not allow for comprehension anyway, so why not just get to the core of the language—letter names and sounds—and be done with it. This would allow children to explore more difficult, and more interesting, texts earlier than was possible with analytic reading. An early emphasis on comprehension was misplaced, advocates believed. Also, learning to read ought to be inherently interesting, so there was no need to stimulate interest artificially through text. Over the past 300 years, there is evidence of both analytic and synthetic approaches having ascendance in the materials provided to students. The “reading wars” of the late twentieth century, however, polarized and politicized the discussion more than in any other era, led by Rudolph Flesch’s 1955 best-seller, Why Johnny Can’t Read—and What You Can Do about It. The battles are largely waged around beginning reading issues (and not in the teaching of reading to students who are fluent) and largely concern word-identification issues rather than the teaching of comprehension. The last fifty years has seen the ebb and flow of linguistic approaches to reading. Basal readers from the 1930s, 1940s, 1950s, and into the 1960s show a stronger influence from a controlled vocabulary and the analytic approach than from synthetic phonics. Workbooks and work sheets dominated the era, allowing additional practice with high-frequency vocabulary and, largely, analytic phonics skills. During these decades, more attention was paid to developing attitudes and interests around reading than ever before. Beginning in the late 1960s, reading materials show the influence of psycholinguistics. With the advent of the application of psycholinguistics, vocabulary control was largely lost as language in readers became more natural. At the same time, companies produced supplemental linguistic readers or phonics programs and workbooks that focused on spelling patterns or isolated phonics instruction (Aukerman, 1984). Some of these linguistically influenced materials were altered or provided augmented alphabet systems that used color or alternative alphabets to regularize the irregularities in English orthography. Although the reading materials of the 1970s and 1980s were more interesting than recounts of bland events in the daily lives of Alice and Jerry or Sally, Dick, and Jane, they were still largely stories and still written by the publishing company as reading instructional materials. It was not until the 1980s that companies began the shift toward using actual selections from children’s literature, and the 1990s saw wholesale inclusion of informative texts into the basals. During the 1980s and 1990s, publishers began to call their materials basal anthologies, reflecting the new variety in both genre and sources. These materials included reading and writing workshop structures, flexible reading groups, and reading and writing across the curriculum. Workbooks and work sheets were downplayed as authentic reading, and writing dominated. In the 1990s, publishing companies also began providing crosswalks between their materials and the emerging state standards. The 1990s and 2000s also show effects from the reading wars. Although synthetic phonics instruction never truly disappeared from classrooms except in pockets across the country, the campaign to make explicit, systematic, sequential phonics part of nearly every reading program is underway. Publishers are resorting again to creating texts for young readers. These decodable texts reinforce and practice the synthetic phonics skills that are taught in the resurgence of workbooks and work sheets. Publishers, responding to the demands of lucrative adoption states like California and Texas, find it difficult to appease the forces controlling the purchase of materials. Sharon Arthur Moore See Also History of Reading Instruction; Linguistic Approaches to Reading Instruction; Phonics Instruction; Phonological and Phonemic Awareness 47 Bibliotherapy References Aukerman, Robert C. 1984. Approaches to Beginning Reading. 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Betts, Emmett Albert. 1957. Foundations of Reading Instruction. New York: American Book Company. Flesch, Rudolph. 1955; 1986 reissue. Why Johnny Can’t Read. New York: HarperCollins. Hoffman, James V. December 2001. “Words (on Words in Leveled Texts for Beginning Readers).” Paper presented at the National Reading Conference, San Antonio, TX. Matthews, Mitford M. 1966. Teaching to Read, Historically Considered. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Smith, Nila Banton. 1965. American Reading Instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. tellectually, attain better understanding of their own motivation, or achieve awareness of something applicable to their own life. Classroom implementation of bibliotherapy consists of a series of sequenced steps. First, the teacher selects a book, taking into consideration the needs of students. Second, the students read the book—and this can be completed either through read-alouds or totally through independent silent reading. Third, students are given time for reflection. In the last step, the students and the teacher discuss the book within a group, which may range from small to large. Heidi McCarty and Lynn Chalmers (1997) suggest that the discussion stage of bibliotherapy in the classroom should be highly structured, following a specific sequence that is usually led by the teacher. The steps are as follows: (1) the teacher asks students to retell the plot, highlighting the feelings, characters, and situations that occurred, (2) teacher asks the students probing questions about their feelings and their identification with characters and events in the story, (3) students transfer the situation from the book to real-life situations that will lead them to explore the effects of certain feelings and behaviors, and (4) students draw conclusions and generalizations from events depicted in the book. Bibliotherapy has been found to be effective in many studies across disciplines. For specific information on the results, see the literature on bibliotherapy and the education of children (Nelms, 1993); special education (McCarty and Chalmers, 1997); counseling (Myers, 1998); and teacher education (Morawski, 1997). Diane Lapp and James Flood Bibliotherapy Bibliotherapy is an instructional process in which readers are guided through reading texts to help them grow in self-awareness (Harris and Hodges, 1995). Bibliotherapy has been described by many educators and counselors as a phenomenon that permits children and adults to see how others confront and solve problems similar to their own. More broadly, bibliotherapy is a tool in which students read books that deal with different situations that parallel situations in their own lives, for example, coping with divorce, losing of a loved one, feeling rejected, or feeling isolated. By reading these works, children and adults are able to see how others encounter anxiety and frustration and how they apply their insight in real-life situations. With the help of a supportive person like a teacher or parent, children and adults may gain insight into alternative solutions to their problems and thereby alleviate their emotional and mental pressure. Bibliotherapy has the potential not only to solve problems but also to prevent them. Bibliotherapy usually consists of three stages of self-development: (1) identification, (2) catharsis, and (3) insight. In the identification stage, students affiliate some real or fictional character with themselves or with associates. In the catharsis stage, students emote in response to compassionate writers who describe progress they have made in their own painful struggle to know themselves. In the insight stage, students become more open to solving their problems in- See Also Adolescent Literature; Children’s Literature; Multicultural Literature References Harris, Theodore, and Richard Hodges, eds. 1995. The Literacy Dictionary: The Vocabulary of Reading and Writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. McCarty, Heidi, and Lynn Chalmers. 1997. “Bibliotherapy Intervention and Prevention.” Teaching Exceptional Children 29 (6):12–13, 16–17. Morawski, C. 1997. “A Role for Bibliotherapy in Teacher Education.” Reading Horizons 37 (3):243–259. Myers, J. 1998. “Bibliotherapy and DCT: Co-Constructing the Therapeutic Metaphor.” 48 Bilingual Education Children reading books in English and Spanish (Elizabeth Crews) For discussion of the second goal, see the entry Heritage-Language Development. Confusion about English-language development in bilingual children is understandable. How can they acquire English, their second language, while being taught in their first language? Bilingual education can help English-language development in two ways. First, when we give a child quality education in the primary language, we give the child a form of knowledge that makes English input more comprehensible. A child who understands history—thanks to history instruction in the first language—will have a better chance of understanding history taught in English than a child without this background knowledge; and more comprehensible English input means more acquisition of English. Second, there is strong evidence that literacy transfers across languages, that building literacy in the primary language is a shortcut to English literacy. The argument is straightforward: If we learn to read by understanding the messages on Journal of Counseling and Development 76:243–250. Nelms, Ben. 1993. “Teachers and Teaching in Novels, Biographies, Film, and Song.” English Journal 82:96–98. Bilingual Education It is helpful to distinguish two goals of bilingual education. The first is the development of “academic English” and school success, and the second is the development of the child’s first language (the “heritage” language). The development of academic language needs to be distinguished from the development of conversational language; academic language means the ability to understand demanding texts, write compositions and reports, and do story problems. The development of academic language takes considerably longer than the development of conversational language (Cummins, 1989). The focus here is on a crucial aspect of goal one: the development of academic English. 49 Bilingual Education the page (Smith, 1994), it is easier to learn to read if we understand the language. And of course, once we can read, we can read—the ability transfers to other languages. The empirical support for the claim that literacy transfers across languages comes from studies showing that both the reading process and the reading development process are similar in different languages. Studies also show that correlations between literacy development in the first language and the second language are positive, when length of residence in the host country is controlled. All the above is true even when the writing systems of the two languages are very different (Krashen, 1996). Effective bilingual programs thus have three characteristics. First, they provide background knowledge through subject-matter teaching in the first language. This should be done to the point that subsequent subject-matter instruction in English is comprehensible. Second, they provide literacy in the first language, which transfers to the second language. Third, they provide quality instruction in the second language, beginning with second-language classes on the very first day. In properly organized programs, subject matter is taught in the second language as soon as it can be made comprehensible. cation. Second-language acquirers who do well in English academic language development and do well in school in the United States have frequently had a solid education in their primary language before coming to the United States. Such children have had “de facto” bilingual education, subject-matter and literacy development in the primary language, and in some cases extensive formal instruction in English as a second language. In addition to case histories (Krashen, 1996), studies also report positive correlations between years of education in the home country and English proficiency among immigrants (e.g., Chiswick, 1991; Espenshade and Fu, 1997). Financial Success without Bilingual Education A popular argument against bilingual education is the fact that many immigrants have succeeded economically without it. This is largely the case for those who arrived in the United States in the first part of the twentieth century. It is established, however, that immigrants did not do well in school during this time. But in fact, very few native speakers of English did well in school in those days: In 1910, only 13.5 percent of the total population had graduated from high school; today that figure is around 83 percent. If immigrants did so poorly in school, how did they succeed economically? In the first part of the twentieth century, education was not a prerequisite to economic success. It is now. Years ago, there was reasonably well-paid work in manufacturing and agriculture that did not require a high school diploma or college degree. Today, nearly all work that leads to a decent living requires education: U.S. government figures show that the earnings of those who are not high-school graduates are below poverty level, on average (Krashen, 1999). The Evidence for Bilingual Education: Program Evaluations In all published studies in which the above three conditions are met, bilingual education has been a winner. Children in properly organized bilingual programs acquire at least as much of the second language as children in comparison programs and typically acquire more (e.g., Mortensen, 1984; Appel, 1984), in some cases doing as well as native speakers of the second language on reading tests. Several critics have claimed that in some evaluations, “English immersion” programs were found to be superior to bilingual education. In each case, however, programs labeled “immersion” were really bilingual education, with a substantial part of the day’s work taught in the primary language. In other studies, little or no description of “bilingual education” is provided, sample sizes are small, and program durations are short (Krashen, 1996). Public Opinion Surveys reveal considerable agreement with the principles underlying bilingual education. In a series of studies, Fay Shin (reviewed in Krashen, 1996) reported that most minority parents, along with teachers and administrators, agreed that “developing literacy through the first language facilitates literacy development in English” and that “learning subject matter through the first language helps make subject matter study in English more comprehensible.” In addition, two- Effect of Previous Education The framework presented here helps to explain cases of apparent success without bilingual edu50 Bilingual Education thirds of respondents to polls conducted by the Los Angeles Times (October 15, 1997) and Dallas Morning News (May 28, 1998) agreed that some use of the child’s first language in school was desirable: Only one-third preferred “English only.” If this is so, why did anti-bilingual education measures pass in California in 1998 (Proposition 227) and Arizona in 2000 (Proposition 203)? Research confirms that the public was not fully aware of what these initiatives contained. In one study, 57 percent of voters said they would support California’s Proposition 227 when presented with the version printed on the ballot. When given details of the actual initiative (dismantling successful programs, limiting special help in English to only one year, allowing teachers to be sued if they violated the new policy), only 15 percent said they would support it (Krashen, 1999). In addition, according to the Los Angeles Times, two-thirds of those who supported Proposition 227 did so because “English is so important” (Los Angeles Times, April 13, 1998). They were unaware of the fact that English-language development is an important goal of bilingual education and that properly organized bilingual programs succeed in teaching English. there is no evidence that Proposition 227 succeeded. Kenji Hakuta (2000) concluded that (1) districts that kept bilingual education improved, and (2) districts that never offered bilingual education improved. A major problem is that nearly all the media focus has been on one district in California—Oceanside. After Proposition 227 passed in 1998, the Oceanside district dropped bilingual education and enthusiastically embraced English immersion, and test scores increased. Hakuta (2000) has shown, however, that gains for Oceanside’s English learners were similar to gains made in many California schools that retained bilingual education. In addition, the bilingual program that Oceanside dropped was a poor one. In an article in the September 2, 2000, Washington Post, Oceanside superintendent Ken Noonan stated that Oceanside’s “bilingual” program was actually taught only in Spanish for four years or longer. It was therefore not a bilingual program but a monolingual Spanish program. As noted above, properly organized bilingual programs introduce English the first day, and teach subject matter in English as soon as it can be made comprehensible. The San Diego Union Tribune (October 5, 2000) confirmed suspicions that Oceanside’s pre-Proposition 227 efforts were dismal, pointing out that before it went into effect, one bilingual school had a severe shortage of books. Has Proposition 227 Worked? It has been claimed that the increase in test scores in California is evidence that Proposition 227 worked, that dismantling bilingual education was a success. It is true that test scores increased throughout California since Proposition 227 passed but there is no evidence linking this increase to dropping bilingual education. As Proposition 227 went into effect during 1998, new tests (the SAT9) were introduced in California. When new tests are introduced, test scores typically increase, which is why tests need to be recalibrated every few years. Typical test-score inflation is about 1.5 to 2.0 points per year. This increase accounts for about half of the gains seen in reading scores for second and third graders in the SAT9 reading test in California since 1998 and for all of the increase in grades four through seven, and it suggests that SAT9 reading scores have actually declined slightly in grades eight through eleven. Test scores are affected by a number of factors that have nothing to do with student learning, such as testing only selected students and coaching on test-taking strategies. Even if the use of SAT9 scores were legitimate, Improving Bilingual Education Bilingual education has done well, but it can do much better. The biggest problem for students in these programs is the absence of books, in both the first and the second language. It is now established that reading for meaning, especially free voluntary reading, is a major source of our literacy competence and that those with more access to books read more (see Recreational Reading). Free voluntary reading can help all components of bilingual education: It is a source of comprehensible input in English and a means for developing knowledge and literacy in the first language, as well as a way of continuing first-language development. Many children with limited English proficiency have little access to books in any language. The average Hispanic family in the United States with limited-English-proficient children has about twenty-six books in the home, about onesixth the U.S. average (Ramirez et al., 1991). 51 Bilingualism Literacy: Latino Children and Free Reading Resources in Schools.” Bilingual Research Journal 18 (1–2):67–82. Ramirez, J. David, Sandra Yuen, Dena Ramey, and David Pasta. 1991. Final Report: Longitudinal Study of an English Immersion Strategy and an Early-Exit Transitional Bilingual Education Program for Language-Minority Children. Vol. 1. San Mateo, CA: Aguirre International. Smith, Frank. 1994. Understanding Reading. 5th ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. School is not helping to solve this problem. Sandra Pucci (1994) investigated school libraries in schools with strong bilingual programs in Southern California and found that books in Spanish were very scarce. Those that were available, though often of high quality, were usually short and for younger children. The access problem is also present with respect to books in English. Children from low-income families have little access to books in school libraries, public libraries, and in their communities (see Recreational Reading). Enriching the print environment is not the only recommendation that can be made in discussing improvement of bilingual education, but it is an excellent place to begin. If it is true that learning to read in the primary language is in fact beneficial, then children need something to read. Stephen Krashen Bilingualism Bilingualism refers to the ability, in an individual or a society, to speak two languages. Bilingualism can develop simultaneously, as when two languages are acquired in infancy (simultaneous bilingualism), or sequentially (sequential bilingualism), as in the case of second-language acquisition (SLA). The field of bilingualism generally concerns simultaneous bilingualism, with SLA or sequential bilingualism largely regarded as a separate field (see Language Acquisition). See Also Heritage-Language Development; Recreational Reading References Appel, René. 1984. Immigrant Children Learning Dutch. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris. Burnham-Massey, Laurie, and Marilyn Pina. 1990. “Effects of Reading Instruction on English Academic Achievement of LEP Children.” Reading Improvement 27:129–132. Chiswick, Barry. 1991. “Speaking, Reading, and Earnings among Low-Skilled Immigrants.” Journal of Labor Economics 9:149–170. Cummins, Jim. 1989. Empowering Minority Students. Ontario, CA: California Association for Bilingual Education. Espenshade, Thomas, and Haishan Fu. 1997. “An Analysis of English-Language Proficiency among U.S. Immigrants.” American Sociological Review 62:288–305. Hakuta, Kenji. 2000. “Points on SAT-9 Performance and Proposition 227.” Available: http://www.s tanford.edu/~hakuta/SAT9/SAT9_2000/bullets. htm. Krashen, Stephen. 1996. Under Attack: The Case against Bilingual Education. Culver City, CA: Language Education Associates. ———. 1999. Condemned without a Trial: Bogus Arguments against Bilingual Education. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Mortensen, Eileen. 1984. “Reading Achievement of Native Spanish-Speaking Elementary Students in Bilingual vs. Monolingual Programs.” Bilingual Review 11 (3):31–36. Pucci, Sandra. 1994. “Supporting Spanish Language Separate versus Unified Systems Researchers in bilingualism have long been interested in whether children who grow up with two languages initially develop a unified linguistic system that later separates or whether these children begin with two separate linguistic systems from the earliest stages of acquisition. Earlier research on bilingual language acquisition proposed that children initially use both linguistic systems in an undifferentiated manner and that a gradual process of separation begins with the lexicon (vocabulary), then moves on to morphology (rules of word formation), and finally to syntax (rules of word order). However, critics of this perspective charged that on close analysis, there is no compelling evidence for the presence of an undifferentiated language system in early bilinguals. Indeed, nearly all of the evidence appears to have relied on the observation that infant bilinguals frequently do not have “translation equivalents” for items in their lexicon, an observation true of many adult bilinguals as well. More recently, research has focused on grammatical (rather than lexical) aspects of bilingual language development, and it has been found that the two systems appear to be differentiated from the earliest stages. For instance, Jürgen Meisel (1990) found that French and German 52 Bilingualism Bilingual Cantonese-English teacher quizzing students (Elizabeth Crews) some milk”), with the sentence beginning in English, switching to Spanish, then moving back to English again. Codeswitching within a single sentence like this is called intrasentential codeswitching; codeswitching between sentences is termed intersentential codeswitching. In the past, educators and others often assumed that codeswitching was indicative of a language disability of some kind. It was often alleged that bilinguals used codeswitching as a “coping strategy” for incomplete mastery of both languages. However, recent research into the social and linguistic characteristics of codeswitching strongly suggest otherwise. John Gumperz, whom many credit with inventing the term codeswitching, discovered six major functions of conversational codeswitching: (1) quotation, (2) addressee specification, (3) interjection, (4) reiteration, (5) message qualification, and (6) personification versus objectification. Gumperz (1982) analyzed codeswitching as a discourse strategy and found that participants in his study were able to use it effectively to convey meaning and build group identity. In an analysis of a bilingual community in Great bilingual infants differentiated syntax and morphology as soon as functional categories emerged, an indication that the two systems had separated at the syntactic level. Satomi Mishina (1998) studied tense marking, negation, and question formation in two Japanese-English infant bilinguals and found that for these children the two systems were properly differentiated throughout their linguistic development and that both learners followed the developmental stages individually associated with the two languages in monolingual children. Although simultaneous bilinguals appear to have separate grammatical systems for their two languages from the earliest stages of development, they often mix their languages in a variety of ways—codeswitching, borrowing, and calques among them. Codeswitching Codeswitching is a speech style in which fluent bilinguals move in and out of two (or conceivably more) languages. For instance, a SpanishEnglish bilingual might say, “This morning mi hermano y yo fuimos a comprar some milk” (“This morning my brother and I went to buy 53 Bilingualism Britain, Li Wei, Lesley Milroy, and Pong-Sin Ching (1992) have more recently developed a social network theory of bilingual codeswitching. In addition to the social aspects of bilingual codeswitching, much attention has been given to the study of its linguistic structure. Like monolingual language, bilingual codeswitching is highly structured and rule governed. Specifically, language mixture appears to be constrained by the interaction of subtle grammatical principles in bilingual speech. For instance, simultaneous bilinguals commonly codeswitch between subjects and verbs, as in “Mis amigos finished first” (“My friends finished first”) but would judge codeswitches between a subject pronoun and a verb (like “Ellos finished first,” “They finished first”) to be ill-formed or ungrammatical. (“Ungrammatical” here means that simultaneous native bilinguals presented with this sentence have a negative psychological reaction similar to the reaction English speakers have to sentences like “Martin built a barn red,” in contrast to our positive reaction to “Martin painted a barn red”; our subconscious knowledge of grammar tells us that the first sentence is structurally flawed but the second is fine.) A number of linguists have formulated theories about the underlying structure of codeswitching. Shana Poplack’s equivalence constraint and free morpheme constraint are among the most widely known. Poplack’s equivalence constraint postulates that codeswitches will tend to occur where word orders are similar in the two languages. For instance, in English, object pronouns follow the verb, whereas in Spanish they precede the verb. Thus, although bilingual codeswitchers regard “I saw la muchacha” (“I saw the girl”) as well-formed, “Yo her ví” or “Yo ví her” (“I saw her”) is judged to be ill-formed. In Poplack’s view, the difference in psychological judgment is explained by the equivalence constraint: The first example (“I saw la muchacha”) is well-formed because the English and Spanish word orders are the same at the junction of the switch, but the second example (“Yo ví her”) is ill-formed because English and Spanish word orders differ in this instance. Poplack’s free morpheme constraint posits that codeswitches cannot occur between a free morpheme (a word that can stand alone, like walk or eat in English) and a bound morpheme (a meaningful part of a word that cannot stand alone, like -ed in the En- glish word walked or -ó in the Spanish habló, “He spoke”). This constraint is intended to explain the ungrammaticality of examples like “He eat-ó” (“He ate”), where a Spanish bound morpheme (-ó, past tense marker) is attached to an English free morpheme (“eat”). Although Poplack’s work, carried out in the early 1980s, is illustrative and perhaps most accessible to nonspecialists, considerable work has been done on the grammatical structure of codeswitching since her initial studies. For instance, emphasizing that ungrammaticality in codeswitching results from the interaction of the mixed grammars, not from any codeswitchingspecific rules. Jeff MacSwan (1999) proposed a theory of codeswitching that applies recent work in syntactic theory (Chomsky, 1995) to the data of language mixture. In addition, Pieter Muysken’s (2000) recent work divides codeswitching into three different types (insertion, alternation, and congruent lexicalization) and analyzes these in terms of contrasting grammatical properties of the languages involved as well as other factors. (See Romaine [1995], MacSwan [1999], and Muysken [2000] for detailed discussion of research on codeswitching.) Contrary to early impressions, the linguistic study of bilingual codeswitching has revealed that simultaneous bilinguals, just like monolinguals, are sensitive to extremely subtle requirements of their linguistic systems and use their languages creatively to satisfy a variety of social purposes and to achieve a sense of identity as part of a bilingual or multilingual community. This program of research has shown that codeswitching may not be used as an indicator of language disability. Borrowings and Calques Words are often borrowed from one language by another in situations of language contact. Similarly, language contact often results in calquing, the use of expressions that appear to use the grammar of one language but the vocabulary of another. Although borrowing is common in contact situations, the degree to which speakers are aware of the non-native character of borrowed words may differ with each borrowed item. For instance, a monolingual English speaker might use the term pork without the slightest awareness that it was borrowed from French during the Norman conquest. In contrast, a 54 Bilingualism speaker might use the expression tour de force fully aware that the expression is of French origin. In this latter case, the English speaker may have some grasp of the grammatical structure of the phrase without having full knowledge of French grammar. An English speaker who encounters the French word genre may also have difficulty pronouncing the word (because the first sound of the word introduces a sequence that English phonology does not readily permit). Borrowing should be carefully distinguished from codeswitching. Borrowed words are usually marked by what has been called “morphological nativization.” For instance, Nahuatl, an indigenous language in Mexico that has borrowed heavily from Spanish, marks Spanish verbs incorporated into the language with the thematic suffixes -oa (transitive), -(i)hui (intransitive), and -lia (applicative). Thus, a Nahuatl speaker might say “Costarihui in neca trabajo” (“That work is costly”), where the Nahuatl intransitive suffix has been affixed to the Spanish word costar (“cost”). So marked, these Spanish words have been morphologically nativized and can no longer be regarded as Spanish words, except in an etymological sense (just as the English word “pork” is etymologically French, but an English speaker who uses it is not necessarily a FrenchEnglish bilingual). Another indication that a word has been borrowed is “phonological nativization.” Here, the “loan item” (the word borrowed from another language) is pronounced using the sound system of the language into which it has been borrowed, as when English speakers pronounce the Spanish-origin word “taco” with aspiration following the “t” and a [w]-off glide following the “o,” among other features characteristic of English phonology. Most speakers of English who use the word “taco,” then, use it as an English word and could not be said to be bilingual simply because they incorporated this word into their vocabulary. Such speakers are borrowing words from other languages, but they are not codeswitching. Codeswitching involves the use of more than one language in a single sentence or block of discourse and can only be done by bilinguals. It is also possible to borrow only pragmatic or morphosyntactic properties while using the phonetic material of the native language; this is the case of calques, also called loan translations. These are special instances of borrowing in which the phonetic properties of words from one language are used in combination with pragmatic or morphological properties of words from another. For instance, Nahuatl speakers in the towns around the Malinche volcano use Nimayana (“I am hungry”); however, while conducting research on Nahuatl in Mexico, Jane and Kenneth Hill observed a Spanish-Nahuatl bilingual who used the expression Nicpia apiztli, which literally means “I have hunger,” apparently modeled after the Spanish equivalent Tengo hambre (“I have hunger”). Thus, although Nicpia apiztli is well-formed from the point of view of grammar (just as English “I have hunger” is wellformed), we might think of this loan translation as pragmatically, or perhaps stylistically, disfavored. (See Kenji Hakuta’s [1986] excellent overview of research on bilingualism for further examples and discussion.) Language contact is a complex and fascinating topic that leads to instances of borrowing, calquing, and codeswitching. A unified account of calquing and codeswitching might view “loan translation” as a kind of codeswitching at the level of abstract grammatical features. Borrowing, by contrast, takes lexical items of one language and accommodates them to the linguistic requirements of another. (Words that are borrowed “on the spot” are referred to as “nonce borrowings.”) Crucially, codeswitching and calquing involve the interaction of two (or more) distinct linguistic systems, whereas borrowing takes specific lexical items (their phonetic and semantic features, primarily) from another language for use in a single linguistic system. Bilingual Language Proficiency and the Education of Linguistic Minorities In a famous and oft-quoted definition of bilingualism, Einar Haugen said bilingualism began at the point where the speaker of one language could produce complete meaningful utterances in another language. Although others have insisted that “true bilinguals” are equally capable of discussing any topic in either language (“ambilingualism”), a more common view is that because bilinguals typically use their languages in different domains of interaction, they should be expected to develop nonoverlapping vocabularies. Indeed, Joshua Fishman has argued that this “diglossia,” or use of separate languages in distinct domains, has the effect of preserving bilin55 Bilingualism gualism in communities where social and political forces may discourage it. (See Romaine [1995] for a detailed review of research on societal bilingualism and language preservation.) School is a domain of language use, so it is expected that children will develop school vocabulary in whatever language or languages happen to be used at school. Research has shown that language-minority children, who speak a nonmajority language that is generally stigmatized in the larger community, benefit from instruction in their native language at school (August and Hakuta, 1998). Bilingual instruction allows these children to develop school-related vocabulary in their native language while learning English. More important, however, it allows them to keep up academically with other children because they are able to understand instruction during the years it takes to master English. Some researchers concerned with the education of linguistic minorities have characterized school-related language as a special stage of linguistic development that evidences “complex syntax” and “expanded vocabulary.” Nevertheless, although there certainly are vocabulary, speech styles, and other aspects of language that are peculiar to the school environment, there is no empirical or theoretical justification for the claim that these forms constitute a stage of greater linguistic sophistication. The language of school is peculiar, like the language of farms or the language of fast-food restaurants, but the presumption that it is more sophisticated derives from social and political values, not empirically grounded linguistic analysis. Nonetheless, the view is widespread and has been extremely influential in the scholarly literature on bilingual education. Jim Cummins (1979), for instance, proposed the Threshold Hypothesis, in which he hypothesized that negative cognitive and academic effects result from low levels of competence in both languages. Following Scandinavian researchers, Cummins referred to this presumed “low ability” in both languages as semilingualism but later changed the term to limited bilingualism. Although the Threshold Hypothesis has been widely publicized, evidence presented in support of the associated idea of semilingualism has not been persuasive. Christina Bratt Paulston (1983), for instance, reviewed numerous Scandinavian studies that sought linguistic evidence for the ex- istence of semilingualism in Sweden and found no empirical evidence to support that such a thing exists. Jeff MacSwan (2000) reviewed reputed evidence from studies of language variation, linguistic structure, school performances, and language loss and concluded that it was all either spurious or irrelevant to the basic proposal. A concept related to semilingualism in Cummins’s framework is the distinction between Basic Interpersonal Communication Skills (BICS) and Cognitive Academic Language Proficiency (CALP). Again, critics have been uncomfortable with equating the language of school, and hence the language of the educated classes, with language that is said to be inherently more complex and richer and that places greater demands on cognitive resources. (See Cummins [2000] for detailed discussion of his views on language proficiency.) Although much has been learned, the study of bilingualism is still very much in its infancy. Nevertheless, the field appears to be growing and to be attracting great interest throughout the world. The social and linguistic analysis of bilingual speech is an exciting field with implications for linguistics, psychology, education, and a host of other areas of inquiry. Jeff MacSwan See Also Biliteracy; Heritage-Language Development; Language Acquisition References August, Diane, and Kenji Hakuta, eds. 1998. Educating Language-Minority Children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge: MIT Press. Cummins, James. 1979. “Linguistic Interdependence and the Educational Development of Bilingual Children.” Review of Educational Research 49:221–251. ———. 2000. Language, Power, and Pedagogy: Bilingual Children in the Crossfire. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Gumperz, John. 1982. Discourse Strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hakuta, Kenji. 1986. Mirror of Language: The Debate on Bilingualism. New York: Basic Books. MacSwan, Jeff. 1999. A Minimalist Approach to Intrasentential Codeswitching. New York: Garland. ———. 2000. “The Threshold Hypothesis, Semilingualism, and Other Contributions to a Deficit View of Linguistic Minorities.” Hispanic Journal of Behavior Sciences 20 (1):3–45. 56 Biliteracy proficiency in a foreign language for advancement to candidacy to advanced degrees, and well-educated biliterate individuals are often held in esteem for their facility in languages other than English. Members of language minorities who achieve functional literacy in English are often held in lower esteem despite their biliterate abilities. Although only a fraction of the world’s estimated 5,000–6,000 languages are used as mediums of school and societal literacy, many societies routinely use more than one language for governmental, educational, and social purposes. Canada is officially bilingual and uses French and English as languages of literacy. India has two national official languages and fifteen regional languages that coexist with them. Even in the English–dominant United Kingdom, Wales has a dual-language policy that encourages the use of Welsh along with English and now actively promotes Welsh bilingualism and biliteracy (Baker and Jones, 2000). Meisel, Jürgen. 1990. “Grammatical Development in the Simultaneous Acquisition of Two First Languages.” In J. M. Meisel, ed., Two First Languages: Early Grammatical Development in Bilingual Children. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris. Mishina, Satomi. 1998. “Language Separation in Early Bilingual Development: A Longitudinal Study of Japanese/English Bilingual Children.” Ph.D. diss., University of California, Los Angeles. Muysken, Pieter. 2000. Bilingual Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Paulston, Christina Bratt. 1983. Swedish Research and Debate about Bilingualism. Stockholm: National Swedish Board of Education. Romaine, Suzanne. 1995. Bilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell. Wei, Li, Lesley Milroy, and Pong-Sin Ching. 1992. “A Two-Step Sociolinguistic Analysis of CodeSwitching and Language Choice.” International Journal of Applied Linguistics 2 (1):63–86. Biliteracy Functions of Biliteracy Biliteracy serves a number of functions, including developing the ability to read sacred texts in classical languages, which are no longer used for other purposes. Muslims learn classical Arabic to read the Koran, and orthodox Jews learn Hebrew to read the Torah. Christian religious scholars must learn Latin, classical Greek, and Aramaic and Hindu Sanskrit. Biliteracy in English and the native language is often an educational goal for language-minority students and their parents. Language minorities refers to language speakers in a society who have a smaller population or less power than the dominant-language group. Biliteracy is necessary in order to have access to employment and to participate in the social, political, and economic life of the dominant society as well as in local communities. Many communities in the United States are bilingual and biliterate. In the United States, native-language newspapers serve immigrant and other language-minority communities, allowing native-language literacy to exist alongside English literacy. Foreign-language newspapers provide a means for first-generation immigrants to use their stronger language of literacy as they acquire English literacy. Native-language literacy also allows them to maintain connections with their countries of origin and Biliteracy refers to the ability to use two or more languages of literacy. There is no consensus on a definition of literacy. The definition used here entails the ability of people to use reading and writing to meet their pragmatic needs and achieve their goals. Most societies, even those like the United States in which one language is the dominant medium of communication, are multilingual and multiliterate. Although biliteracy and bilingualism are related, they are not identical because a person may be orally bilingual but not literate or may be orally bilingual and literate in only one language. Perspectives on Biliteracy Biliteracy may be considered from the point of view of individuals, communities, or societies. Literacy in more than one language has both pragmatic and status value. International trade, globalization, and the need to access and exchange knowledge across languages make bilingualism and biliteracy valuable assets. Historically, in early modern Europe and the early years of the American Republic, biliteracy was an expectation for those of the educated elite. People were not considered fully literate if they had not learned to read Latin or Greek, even if they could read and write in their vernacular language. Today, some university majors still require reading 57 Biliteracy Chinese volunteer working with recent immigrant (Elizabeth Crews) use literacy. Years of schooling are problematic because merely attending school does not ensure that a person will become truly literate. Self-assessments are also unreliable because people may either inflate or depreciate their actual abilities. Despite their limitations, surrogate and self-assessment data often are the only easily accessible data. The 1992 NALS provides some useful biliteracy data that were derived from all three types of measures. Fifteen states participated, including California and Texas, which have large Hispanic populations. Although the methodological design of the direct-measurement portion of the NALS is somewhat controversial, the schooling and self-assessment data provide an interesting profile of the extent of biliteracy among adults. Biliteracy is most prevalent among language minorities. Reynaldo Macías (1990) noted that there are three patterns of literacy among language minorities: (1) native-language literacy, (2) second-language literacy, typically in English, which denotes no native-language literacy, and follow news within their communities that is not covered in English-language newspapers. Reliance on non-English presses is not new. In 1910, there were 540 German-language newspapers in the United States. Spanish, Chinese, and Vietnamese newspapers serve similar functions in biliterate communities today. Assessing and Measuring Societal Biliteracy It is difficult, though not impossible, to assess and measure biliteracy in the U.S. population. There are three types of literacy measures: direct measures, such as the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS), which directly test individuals; surrogate measures, which are based on years of schooling (equating six or eight years of schooling with literacy); and self-assessments, which derive from surveys such as the U.S. Census, wherein individuals are asked to judge how well they read and write. Direct measures are always preferable but are costly and may lack ecological validity; that is, they may not accurately simulate real-world literacy tasks or how people actually 58 Biliteracy (3) biliteracy, typically in one’s native language and in English. Nonliteracy, meaning no literacy in any language, is also a possibility. According to the 1992 NALS findings based on self-reported data, approximately 7 percent of the adult population in the United States was biliterate. Among whites, the biliteracy rate was only 3 percent. Only 2 percent of African Americans were biliterate. Biliteracy rates were much higher for Hispanics, 35 percent of whom were biliterate, compared to 33 percent who were literate only in English and 27 percent only in Spanish. Biliteracy was most prevalent among Asians and Pacific Islanders, among whom nearly half (47 percent) were biliterate. Higher biliteracy rates among Hispanics and Asians are not surprising, given that immigration rates are higher for these groups (Greenberg et al., 2001). NALS data indicated that biliterates tended to have higher levels of education than monoliterates. Among biliterates, 48 percent acquired some post-secondary education, compared to only 43 percent for those literate only in English. Most biliterates do not have balanced abilities in two or more languages, because their language experiences and contexts for learning are usually not parallel across languages. NALS data are important because most national literacy estimations focus solely on English. Their failure to acknowledge literacy among those who are literate in languages other than English inflates the magnitude of a perceived “literacy crisis.” The extent to which language minorities become biliterate in English and their native language is contingent on several factors. For those born in the United States, biliteracy is largely determined by whether they have access to a quality bilingual education program or, later in high school or college, to a quality foreign- or nativelanguage program that develops literacy in their home language. For immigrants, the development of biliteracy in their native language and English is dependent on whether they have had access to quality education in their native language and whether they have had literacy instruction in English as a foreign language prior to coming to the United States. In many countries, English is now being taught in primary schools; however, the quality of instruction varies greatly. More Hispanic immigrants come from Mexico than any other country. Many Mexican immigrants have not had formal education beyond the primary grades, although literacy rates and education levels are rising. Basic Spanish literacy rates for the years 1995–1999 are reported as 92 percent for Mexican males and 87 percent for females; however, only about 64 percent of the population reached secondary school (Dutcher, 2001). English literacy is often a requirement for job training. Undereducation makes it difficult for many Mexican and Central American adult immigrants to compete for better-paying jobs, and many are unable to participate in programs that would give them skills for job mobility. As a result, some adult-education policymakers are now advocating training programs that build from Spanish literacy to allow marginalized immigrants to gain job skills while they develop oral English and English literacy. Promoting Biliteracy For those who value languages, biliteracy presents both challenges and opportunities. Today, the majority of the world’s estimated 5,000–6,000 languages are endangered. In the United States, many Native American languages are likewise being threatened even as their speakers have acquired English and English literacy. Although writing systems have been developed for many of these languages, thus ensuring that they are recorded before they completely disappear, more proactive measures are needed to ensure their survival. Historically, Cherokee provides an example of a language that was saved through the development of a writing system and the promotion of the language in schools. By the mid-nineteenth century, biliteracy in Cherokee and English was common. Unfortunately, the imposition of English-only policies in the latter part of the nineteenth century led to a decline in both Cherokee and English literacy. For the past several decades, federal bilingual education policy has sanctioned transitional bilingual education but has stopped well short of endorsing maintenance bilingual education. In transitional approaches, only initial literacy is developed in the minority language to allow students to keep pace academically while they make the transition to English literacy. Transitional models usually wean students away from nativelanguage literacy within three to five years and are not effective in promoting biliteracy. Among educational models, maintenance 59 Book Clubs programs are more effective than transitional programs in promoting the retention of native language while developing English literacy for language minorities. For monolingual Englishspeaking students, immersion programs, which begin with instruction in foreign languages and gradually introduce English literacy, have proven effective. So-called dual immersion, or two-way bilingual programs, have proven effective when English-speaking language-majority and language-minority children are brought together in the same program. These programs divide the use and development of literacy in two languages. There is a need to ensure that special consideration is given to language-minority students, because some evidence suggests that these programs advantage English speakers more than language-minority students (Valdés, 1997). Foreign-language instruction provides another path for attaining biliteracy. Unfortunately, in the United States, opportunities for foreignlanguage instruction are usually delayed until middle or high school. Goals of foreign-language instruction do not always promote the goal of biliteracy. Consequently, many who study foreign languages fail to acquire more than a very rudimentary knowledge of them. Recently, national attention has been drawn to developing students’ heritage languages. Heritage-language learners are those who grow up in a home where a non-English language is used. They may either have a passive understanding of the language or be partially bilingual, and they may participate in a variety of program types (Peyton, Ranard, and McGinnis, 2001). Heritage-language literacy is now being promoted in a number of languages. Mandarin and Japanese are frequently taught in Asian American immigrant communities, and a number of universities offer courses in literacy for native speakers of Spanish, Chinese, and Cambodian, just to name a few. The promoting of heritage-language literacy offers a promising means for increasing the number of biliterate people in the United States. Despite these efforts, given the dominance of English as a national and international language, many in the United States lack the motivation to learn other languages and acquire literacy in them. Nevertheless, the development of literacy in languages other than English can have useful benefits for the monolingual, monoliterate, English-speaking population in trade, cross-cultural understanding, diplomacy, and national security. In an age of increasing global interdependence, it is to be hoped that more people will come to appreciate the value of acquiring literacy in other languages and that more policymakers will support educational programs that promote this. Terrence G. Wiley See Also Adult Literacy; Adult Literacy Testing; Bilingual Education; Bilingualism; Diversity; English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment; Heritage-Language Development; Language Acquisition; Language Attitudes; Literacy Definitions; Policy Issues in Testing; Social Justice and Literacies; Social Nature of Literacy; Sociolinguistics and Literacy References Baker, Colin, and Sylvia P. Jones. 2000. Encyclopedia of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Dutcher, Nadine. 2001. Expanding Educational Opportunities in Diverse Societies. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. Greenberg, Elizabeth, Reynaldo F. Macías, David Rhodes, and Tsze Chan. 2001. English Literacy and Language Minorities in the United States. National Center for Education Statistics. NCES 2001–464. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education Research and Improvement. Macías, Reynaldo F. 1990. “Definitions of Literacy: A Response.” In Robert L. Venezky, David A. Wagner, and B. S. Ciliberti, eds., Toward Defining Literacy, pp. 17–23. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Peyton, Joy K., Donald A. Ranard, and Scott McGinnis. 2001. Heritage Languages in America: Preserving a National Resource. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. Valdés, Guadalupe. 1997. “Dual-Language Immersion Programs: A Cautionary Note concerning the Education of Language Minority Students.” Harvard Educational Review 67 (3):391–429. Book Clubs Since 1989, ongoing collaboration between university-based researchers and classroom-based practitioners has resulted in the evolution of the original Book Club model. Initially, the program was constructed to teach reading comprehension and critical thinking to elementary children. The design revolved around promoting support and instruction for student-led discussions related to the literature students were reading. Although 60 Book Clubs this was the focal point, the program included suggestions for varied contexts for reading; multiple types of responses, both oral and written; and instruction to foster independent reading and higher-order thinking. Thus, the design included five components: (1) reading, (2) writing, (3) instruction, (4) small-group discussions (book clubs), and (5) total-class discussions (community share). Although all components were essential, the exact order and amount of time devoted varied, depending on student needs and the instructional focus. Further, teachers’ experiences with Book Club often led to their merging instruction with community share so that instruction in the form of brief, mini-lessons could be provided on content relevant to the curriculum or student needs. Even though Book Club is evolving, some elements are key to its basic foundation. One fundamental concept is that students need the time and opportunity to talk among themselves about topics related to books, for several reasons: (1) talk helps develop thinking, (2) engagement is predicated on interest and students are more interested in topics they select, and (3) students’ talk enables teachers to assess their learning. Since most classes are too large for all students to have occasion to express their developing thought, small groups provide the context for this. Further, this frees teachers to monitor students’ thinking and concept development because they can circulate among the groups, noting both individual student growth and class-level development. Instruction can then be better focused on students’ needs, either individual, group, or total-class. Although the small, student-led discussions are pivotal to the program, they are not the sole component. Teachers provide instruction on multiple ways of responding to texts so that students can foster varied ways of comprehending texts. Further, teachers stress not only personal responses but also reactions that enable learners to engage in critical thought. That is, students respond in ways that help them compare and contrast texts or parts of texts (such as characters, plots, and settings), evaluate what they have read, and synthesize ideas across texts. Students respond both orally and in writing and in both rough draft and polished formats. Because this is an integrated literacy program, reading is essential. The key need is for authentic texts (that is, texts written to communicate meaning and not to teach reading) that students find engaging but that also promote independence. Further, instruction emphasizes this movement toward independence with multiple types of texts and begins with learners’ needs. The specifics of all the components have been elaborated in The Book Club Connection: Literacy Learning and Classroom Talk. Therefore, this entry will focus next on growth of the Book Club model by describing how two researchers have taken the concepts developed initially and continue to work with them in different contexts, exploring other questions. Susan McMahon and her colleagues proceeded by examining how the Book Club format can support student learning and talk through integrated language-arts and social-studies curricula. From 1995 to 2000, in the Language Arts and Social Studies Integration Project (LASSI), researchers collaborated with teachers to investigate student responses related to both fictionnarrative and nonfiction-expository texts that are connected by an integrated theme. Using the original components as a framework to develop integrated units, the project expanded it to include additional concepts related to social-studies learning. That is, Book Club focused on developing reading independence, particularly fostering comprehension and higher-order thinking. Although these concepts are related to learning other content areas, researchers on this project found that teachers needed additional support in understanding how to incorporate concepts, skills, and strategies essential to particular content areas. Therefore, research on this elaboration of Book Club has led to an expansion that supports teachers’ efforts to plan, implement, and assess student learning during integrated instructional units. In addition to expanding the original framework, LASSI also investigated how students’ discussions can support their learning of social studies content by examining the differences between student commentary associated with nonfiction-expository texts versus fiction-narrative texts. Students across several grade levels and classrooms clearly talk differently about nonfiction texts, often accepting ideas without challenge. This has led to developing instructional approaches that foster more critical stances among students reading nonfiction. 61 Book Clubs autobiographical book clubs, teachers experience and discuss narrative texts in order to understand their lives and those of their students. Book Club began as an integrated literacy program that responded to students’ need to discuss among themselves the books they were reading. As a literacy program, it included reading, writing, speaking, and listening. The original model proved successful in helping teachers and students explore multiple ways of understanding the books they were reading, and it was so successful that the model has served as a basis for continued research and staff development. Susan McMahon Taking an alternative approach to further exploration of the original Book Club model, Taffy Raphael and her colleagues developed Book Club Plus in 1997. The goal for this project was to support diverse classrooms of learners as they engage in age-appropriate texts. With increasing diversity in American classrooms, students enter the discourse with varied prior experiences, cultural lenses, and reading abilities. Book Club Plus has taken on exploration of this topic in order to identify ways of helping teachers provide suitable instruction for these students. The premises for this project are: (1) learning takes time, (2) learning involves multiple interactions with texts, (3) skills and strategies are learned in communities of practice, and (4) narrative is pivotal in understanding and sharing this with others. Book Club Plus also includes another component—professional development. This aspect of the project encourages teachers to participate in learning contexts that help them focus on culture and identity. Through graduate course work and See Also Gender and Reading References McMahon, Susan I., Taffy E. Raphael, and Virginia Goatley, eds. 1997. The Book Club Connection: Literacy Learning and Classroom Talk. New York: Teachers College Press. 62 C Center for the Expansion of Language and Thinking support groups in Canada and the United States. A coalition of these groups and individuals led to the organization of the Whole Language Umbrella (available: http://www.ncte.org/wlu/), now an affiliate of NCTE. The organization is committed to teachers’ sharing successful practices and supporting each other as they develop classroom curricula focused on equity and social justice. CELT members (teacher educators, language researchers, teachers and administrators in public and private K–12 schools, authors and book publishers, members and staff of professional organizations) individually and in combination offer consultation, workshops, and long-range professional development. Topics include whole-language pedagogy in language and sign systems across the curriculum, miscue analysis, inquiry curriculum, democratic classrooms, multicultural, multilingual, and multidialectical issues, children’s and adolescent literature, the evaluation of reading and writing programs, and other current trends and issues. A recent collaborative project by CELT members culminated in a publication on teacher education: Whole Language Voices in Teacher Education, published by Stenhouse (available: http://www.stenhouse.com) (Whitmore, Goodman, and Center for the Expansion of Language and Thinking, 1996). For additional information, write to: CELT, 100 Heritage Road, Bloomington, IN 47408, or visit CELT’s web site (available: http://www.coe.missouri.edu/~celt/). Yetta M. Goodman The Center for the Expansion of Language and Thinking (CELT), founded in 1972, is an international nonprofit educational corporation grounded in the principles of education for democracy with the emphasis on language learning and inquiry. It is dedicated to improving education through a dynamic curriculum based on theoretical understandings about the social nature of language, thinking, learning, and teaching. CELT was initiated in the late 1960s and early 1970s, when Kenneth S. Goodman, then professor at Wayne State University, was engaged in research on readers’ patterns of miscues during the oral reading of whole texts. While miscue researchers (scholars who focus on deviations in oral reading from the text) were studying the reading of students in Detroit, they formed a study group to further their understanding of language and its relationship to reading and writing processes and instruction. As they moved away from Detroit to other academic institutions and school districts, they recognized the need for a professional organization; hence, CELT was born. CELT, an invitational organization, promotes continuing professional development by holding rejuvenation conferences for its members. Members meet and present their work at conferences of the International Reading Association (IRA), the National Council of Teachers of English (NCTE), and the Whole Language Umbrella. They also organize professional development opportunities for teachers, teacher educators, and researchers focusing on how the use of language is applied in classroom settings. CELT members have been active in the conceptualization of whole-language pedagogy and have been integral to the development of teacher See Also Whole Language and Whole Language Assessment References Whitmore, Kathryn F., Yetta M. Goodman, and Center for the Expansion of Language and Thinking. 1996. Whole Language Voices in Teacher Education. York, ME: Stenhouse Publishers. 63 Children’s Literature Children’s Literature trator of the best American picture book published the preceding year. The award committee considers excellence in the art itself and in how that art interprets the text (Peltola, 2000). Children’s literature is the corpus of books written expressly for children, from birth through age twelve. Adolescent literature, sometimes subsumed under the category children’s literature, is written for an audience between the ages of twelve and eighteen. Publications in both children’s and adolescent literature are often referred to as “trade books,” meaning books written for a general audience (as opposed to textbooks). As in books for adults, these trade books span many topics, themes, and audiences within their range. These books may be fiction or nonfiction and are structured as narratives, poetry, exposition, or descriptive texts. Trade books for children may be picture books, illustrated books, or full-length texts. They span the genres of folklore, fantasy, science fiction, contemporary realistic fiction, historical fiction, biography, and nonfiction (Galda and Cullinan, 2002). Poetry and Folklore Poetry, a literary form in which experiences and ideas are brought to our attention in a unique fashion through precise, distilled language, is another genre of children’s literature. From the earliest cradle songs to the complexities of metaphoric comments on life in haiku, poetry for children is available in single-volume picture books, subject-specific anthologies, author-specific anthologies, and general anthologies that are structured in various ways. Poetry is a thriving genre, and the National Council of Teachers of English sponsors an award given every three years to an outstanding poet for children in honor of the body of that poet’s work. Folklore, stories, and songs that began in the oral traditions of various cultures compose another extremely popular genre of children’s literature. Folklore consists of folktales, fairy tales, legends, myths, fables, folk songs, nursery rhymes, and religious tales. In a picture-book format, folklore is often beautifully illustrated by artists who use these time-honored stories to express their artistic interpretations. Folklore is also presented in anthologies that may be organized around tale types, such as trickster tales or lore from a particular culture. The oral nature of folklore makes it excellent material for storytelling and drama. The Newbery Medal is given annually by the American Library Association to the author of the most distinguished contribution to literature for children that was published the preceding year. Although this award can and has been given to writers of picture books, poets, and biographers, it is most often awarded to writers of fantasy, science fiction, contemporary realistic fiction, or historical fiction. Picture Books Unique to children’s literature is the picture book, in which the illustrations are of equal or greater importance than the text in conveying meaning. This genre, based on format rather than content, contains books that range from fiction to nonfiction and from fantasy to realism, addressing historical to contemporary subjects. A majority of picture books are for younger readers, with some geared toward the older child or adolescent reader. Picture books for babies are often printed on cardboard stock, have only a few pages, and are dominated by the illustrations rather than the text. Some of these books tell a story or present a concept important to young children. Others try to involve young children by asking them to participate in certain actions or gestures. Although many picture books are intended to be read aloud to children, some are created to be read by children themselves. These generally contain fewer words, carefully selected, and rely heavily on the illustrations to convey meaning. Some are “wordless,” using the illustrations to convey a story or information. In picture books, authors and illustrators tell stories, create poems and songs, and present information that interests children. All genres of children’s literature can be found in picture-book format. The most prestigious award for picture books is the Caldecott Award, given annually by the American Library Association (ALA) to the illus- Fantasy and Science Fiction In fantasy, things that could not happen, characters who could not exist, and places that could not exist in the real world are made believable through precise description, engaging characters, logical plot, and a compelling theme. This genre is presented in picture-book format for young children. In this form, both the illustra64 Children’s Literature tions and the text create a believable fantasy world. Many books for preschool children are fantasy in that they have animal characters behaving as a real child would. In books for older readers, fantasy writers explore conflicts between good and evil, asking eternal questions about life. Animal fantasy, in which animals think, talk, and act, is popular with many readers. So, too, are fantasies in which a miniature world is created to explore issues of our real world. Some fantasies are the classic quest tale in which a hero leaves home to seek something—an object, a fate, a future—and returns home changed, and usually wiser. Other fantasies are literary lore, using traditional folktales and motifs as the basis for fully elaborated stories. Many fantasies use fantasy devices such as time slips and magic. Science fiction is an extrapolation from scientific principles, a logical extension of scientific possibilities. It explores this question: “If this scientific premise or promise is possible, then what might the world look like in the future?” Some science fiction is geared toward younger readers, presenting a story in which some type of space travel or alien adventure is a main plot element. The majority of the books in this genre, however, are for older readers. They pose engaging questions about ecology, survival, mind control, and social conditions. Grandmother reading a children’s book to her granddaughter (Skjold Photographs) who created our history, demonstrating how people’s lives are influenced by their historical period. Although most of the historical fiction available today is written by contemporary authors who have deliberately set their stories in the past, some books, often called historical realism, were actually written as contemporary realism in the past and have become historical by virtue of the passage of time. Contemporary and Historical Fiction Fiction that is both contemporary and realistic is very popular with children and adolescents, and this genre is presented in many guises. Although fantasy creates alternative worlds and science fiction introduces possible worlds, contemporary realistic fiction attempts to mirror the actual world as we know it today. In realistic fiction we find animal stories, adventure stories, mysteries, sports stories, humorous stories, romances, stories about relationships with others and about growing up. Contemporary realistic fiction includes many popular series books that focus on one character or group of characters across several books. Historical fiction, by contrast, seeks to recreate a world that existed in the past. The setting for historical fiction can range from prehistory to almost yesterday, and these renditions are usually presented in great detail in order to make the contents both believable and interesting. Books in this genre often trace the lives of the people Biography, Autobiography, and Memoir Biographies tell the story of the life or part of the life of a particular person, usually paying special attention to that person’s achievements. Autobiography recreates the story of the author’s own life. Biographies can be chronological, recounting events in the order in which they actually occurred; or episodic, highlighting a certain period of a person’s life; or interpretive, in which events are selected and arranged to create a certain effect, a particular understanding of the subject’s life. Although most biographies available for 65 Classroom Writing Assessment children focus on a single individual, there are some excellent collective biographies available in which authors present the lives of several people who share a commonality with others in the same volume. Biographies range from almost entirely fictional to authentic biographical nonfiction based entirely on documented fact. Memoirs have become increasingly available in trade books for children and young adults. A memoir focuses on particular experiences in life that were especially significant. True memoir is entirely nonfiction, but many writers have blurred the boundaries of memoir with fictional accounts of real-life experiences. ment of visual literacy. Nonfiction trade books offer students the opportunity to explore subject matter deeply and widely. In the second half of the twentieth century, children’s literature became big business, expanding to fill the needs of parents and teachers. As the corpus of trade books grew, children’s literature began to develop multicultural variety. Although still a literature of the mainstream, there are an increasing number of outstanding books that present the experiences and histories of diverse cultures, present cultural practices, and explore culture itself (Yokota, 2001). As the United States becomes a more international community, its literature for children will also reflect global diversity. Lee Galda Nonfiction Nonfiction is the largest genre of children’s literature. Trade books in this genre consider topics that range across the full spectrum of information available to adults, but the author shapes the coverage of those topics to reach a particular audience. Nonfiction today contains excellent, vivid writing, artistically beautiful illustrations that are also appropriate for the subject matter, and an array of organizational aids that enable readers to find information efficiently. Recently, special awards have been established to honor those who write nonfiction. The first such award is the Orbis Pictus Award, sponsored by the National Council of Teachers of English. The award is based on excellence in four areas: accuracy, organization, design, and style. See Also Adolescent Literature; Bibliotherapy; Book Clubs; Critical Literacy; Discussion; Family Literacy; Gender and Reading; Independent Reading; Literature Circles; Literature-Based Instruction; Multicultural Literature; Read-Alouds; Reader Response; Trade Books References Galda, Lee, and Bernice Cullinan. 2002. Literature and the Child. 3rd ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Peltola, Bette J. 2000. “Newbery and Caldecott Awards: Authorization and Terms.” In Association for Library Service to Children, ed., The Newbery and Caldecott Awards: A Guide to the Medal and Honor Books, pp. 1–9. Chicago: American Library Association. Yokota, Junko, ed. 2001. Kaleidoscope: A Multicultural Booklist for Grades K–8. 3rd ed. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Children’s Literature Then and Now Literature written expressly for children has been available for about 400 years. The earliest forms of children’s literature were designed to instruct and inform, especially regarding proper behavior. The publication of the fairy tales collected by Charles Perrault in 1697 marked the beginning of a new purpose of children’s books—to delight. Over the years this has continued to be a central function of literature for children. Today’s children have available a vast array of choices in literature, including many genres and subgenres. Trade books support the teaching of virtually any school subject, be it composition, reading, science, social studies, mathematics, art, or music. Poetry and fiction are often used as material for exploration of themes; picture books are often resources for art education and play an extremely important role in the develop- Classroom Writing Assessment Classroom writing assessment is a topic of importance to educators concerned with the development of young writers. From the etymology of the word assessment, we learn that someone who assesses has the role of assisting in a process of judgment. If we extend that definition, we can view writing assessment in its social context, as an event in which someone helps writers judge their own writing. The purpose here is to consider how classrooms can provide these assessment opportunities to young writers of diverse cultural and linguistic backgrounds (see Authentic Assessment). 66 Classroom Writing Assessment What Is Being Assessed? Written Language and Its Development Educators’ views of writing as rhetoric and technical skill have been replaced with views of writing as composing meaning (International Reading Association and National Council of Teachers of English, 1994). Writing is a process of establishing a purpose for written communication, which draws on resources, models, and invention to accomplish that purpose. When composing text, writers may use or adapt other writers’ genres and styles, and thus a writer’s previous practice with established rhetoric has value in that sense. Indeed, humans have invented distinctive genres of writing to accomplish recurring purposes: Expository genres are for description or argumentation; narrative, poetic, and theatrical genres explore human experience. But we need to recognize the cultural functions of genres as well as their continual evolution, and we must resist prescribing their value. At times, reproduction of an established genre fits a writer’s purpose. But at other times, a writer feels the need to build upon, play with, or abandon the inventions of the past, and those departures are crucial to the empowerment of students whose literacies differ from cultures in power. It is the writer’s communicative purpose that determines choices of content, text structure, formality, language, and mechanics. The implication for writing assessment is that assessors must consider the author’s intended purpose for the writing. tegrate or omit steps, or recycle through phases multiple times. But no matter how writers work, to accomplish their goals, they must coordinate strands of reasoning and make choices among many aspects of language and the complex conventions of the print medium. As writers develop ideas and compose, they solicit either real or imagined readers to respond to their emerging text. Indeed, writing and reading can be viewed as transactions between those who compose text and those who construct meaning from text (Rosenblatt, 1969); writers consider their readers, and readers raise issues for writers (see Transactional Theory, and Reading-Writing Relationships). If we define assessment as responses from a helpful reader and ensuing dialogues between writer and reader, we see that assessment plays a critical role in the writing process. Writing Development and Writing Instruction Long before children can make interpretable marks on paper, they compose texts that respond to and reflect their cultural and linguistic experience. Children tell tales, invent songs, make lists, compose messages, and create images with language. As young children develop conceptions of the conventions of written language, they invent marks or graphics to represent their communicative acts or ask someone to transcribe for them. At the same time, they develop understandings of written genres as they listen to others read and begin to read themselves. Once children can write in a more conventional sense and have opportunities to accomplish goals with their writing, they are positioned to develop understandings of the structure and function of different genres, including those they invent themselves (Hall, 1997). Initially, they may interweave multiple forms of representation into their composing, including graphics on the page, as well as talk and action that do not make it to the page. From this point on, writing development is a lifelong process. Developing writers construct understandings of rhetorical possibilities from what they read and hear, and from readers’ responses to their texts. Although young writers benefit from composing text modeled on culturally valued genres, the goal of writing instruction and writing assessment is neither rhetorical skill nor mastery of writing mechanics. Skills are essential resources Writing Process Composing meaning requires writers to orchestrate a complex set of activities (see Process Writing). The writing process is commonly described as a series of phases. Writers begin with an idea, consult resources, and brainstorm possible approaches to implementation. As their purpose solidifies, they construct a working outline in graphic or text form. They then draft the piece, consulting their resources and outline as they write, perhaps revising the outline and resources as they work. Before writers revise the draft, they ask for feedback and then reread the piece, imagining how readers might interpret or misinterpret their text. They revise, then polish the piece, editing mechanics and readying the piece for publication. This characterization of writing as a sequence of phases is simplistic; writers often in67 Classroom Writing Assessment for students, but mastering them does not assure that students will become writers who can compose text for a variety of purposes and a range of audiences. Students need to master genres of writing that have currency in a range of cultural contexts and need to take creative risks at the same time, fashioning text in new ways for new purposes. Young writers need to envision the possibility of creating new meanings, even new genres. In classrooms where students establish their own goals for their writing, the function of classroom assessment is not to wield the mighty red pen but to provide a readership able to make meaning of developing writers’ work and contribute helpful, formative perspectives. There are several features of the scenario that highlight the limitations of testing for assessment of writing. Although writers can draw from outside resources and reader feedback as they compose, students take only a test. Multiple-choice item formats provide only an indirect measure of students’ competence with composing. The test answers are predetermined, unlike the surprises we expect and value from each new piece of writing. Summary scores provide little information to guide improvement in a student’s writing or a teacher’s methods of writing instruction. In the minds of many, a traditional test cannot provide a measure of the complex craft of writing, nor can it provide writers with useful information about the quality of their work. Assessment and Testing: Situating Classroom Writing Assessment in the Debate Writing assessment in the classroom both builds upon and departs from assessment practices used in other contexts. This section situates classroom assessment within the debate between assessment and testing (Wiggins, 1998). Although the contrast simplifies the complexities, it is a springboard for discussion of the special role of classroom assessment (Winograd, Martinez, and Noll, 1999) (see Accountability and Testing, High-Stakes Assessment, and Policy Issues in Testing). Direct Writing Assessment Direct writing assessment is evaluation of students’ competence from a writing sample. The term direct captures concerns about indirect assessment of writing inferred from students’ performance on multiple-choice test items, as just described. In a typical direct assessment, students are given a writing prompt and a time limit, and trained raters assign scores capturing the overall quality of the samples. Direct writing assessment addresses central criticisms of traditional testing in that students compose, and assessors make judgments of their completed texts. It places authority in the scorers, who are almost always classroom teachers specially trained for the task. However, concerns have been raised about the authenticity and value of this method of assessment as well (Wiggins, 1998; Winograd, Martinez, and Noll, 1999). The prompt is imposed, and students have limited resources and limited opportunity to revise. Scorers must use a prescribed rubric in prescribed ways to achieve adequate reliability. When direct assessment is used for sorting and ranking students, those purposes inevitably press for efficiency and technical quality, and the result may be a writing experience that is not educative for either students or teachers (Wiggins, 1998) and is likely to be particularly problematic for students writing in their second language or students from cultures where collaboration is the norm. Critics worry that students and teachers may view these brief, solitary assessments and the accompanying scoring procedures as models for good writing and good classroom assessment. Testing Testing evokes a formal occasion of evaluation and a specific kind of evaluation activity. Consider the following scenario. Students bend over their desks, responding to items written by outside testing experts. The test is designed to measure students’ achievement in a subject area, and the scores are used for summative reporting or for admissions requirements. Students are asked to demonstrate knowledge of what they have been taught; the format of the items is multiple choice, leaving students no opportunity to choose how to demonstrate what they know. Students have little understanding of the scoring criteria and later receive summary scores containing limited information about their performance. This image of testing captures how testing can feel external to the student and the teacher, and scenarios like this one are often invoked to set off the advantages of classroom assessments deeply integrated with ongoing processes of teaching and learning. 68 Classroom Writing Assessment Rubrics Although there are controversies about direct assessment as a model for classroom assessment, the rubrics used for scoring writing samples have been widely adapted for classroom use. Rubrics are scales for evaluating the relative quality of a piece of writing or a collection of writing, and they focus the assessors’ attention on content. Red-ink corrections of spelling, grammar, and punctuation are replaced with judgments on the effectiveness of the writing. The content and structure of writing rubrics vary considerably, and space permits only illustrations of the variations. A rubric might be structured as holistic, containing one scale for overall quality, or as analytic, with several dimensions such as content, organization, style, and mechanics. The content of a rubric may be generic and designed to capture the qualities of good writing, or it may be specific to particular genres or even particular assignments. Rubrics were developed to support objective evaluation of writing samples, but several decades of research have demonstrated that scorers’ judgments may be biased and fail to consider the writer’s intent or the full complexity of the written piece (Maylath, 1997). Classroom teachers who appropriate rubrics for classroom use need to be aware of these risks. For example, assessors may be influenced by aspects of language use such as vocabulary, dialect, grammatical correctness, and complexity—aspects that may not be crucial to the effectiveness of the writing; they may be influenced by surface features of the text, such as length, mechanical accuracy, or neatness. These biases are technical challenges when rubrics are used for large-scale testing programs. Fortunately, in the classroom, teachers’ awareness of these possible biases can become entry points for assessment conversations. nities for learning and improvement. In culturally and linguistically diverse classrooms, teachers can use rubrics and other tools to guide assessment conversations but should be prepared to be surprised by the texts students compose. The classroom may be best positioned to realize this vision of educative writing assessment (Wiggins, 1998). Methods of Assessing Writing in the Classroom This section provides an overview of assessment methods used in the classroom. Although the section is organized as a list, these practices should be conceptualized as a coherent assessment system (Spandel and Stiggins, 1997). Developing writers benefit from multiple opportunities for analysis of their writing, and their teachers benefit from multiple sources of evidence of student learning (see Kidwatching and Classroom Evaluation). Written Comments Pen in hand, a reader responds to text, making comments about what feelings and insights the text evokes, and what seems missing or misleading. In the classroom, a teacher who is committed to the benefits of reader response writes comments as a reader, not as a grader, and engages students in responding to one another’s writing in much the same way. The students and the teacher comment at various points in the writing process, expanding the reader’s role to include suggestions for strengthening the writing. Since the writer may not agree with a reader’s perspective, written comments should be entry points for assessment conversations that allow both writer and reader opportunities to clarify their interpretations. Writers benefit from comments that consolidate what the writing accomplishes and what it does not, along with suggestions for strengthening the piece. Teachers often speak of the importance of providing both commendations and recommendations, linking both to criteria for good writing that have been discussed and illustrated repeatedly in class discussions (Wolf and Gearhart, 1994). Lessons Learned from the Debate Assessments are designed for particular purposes, and any given method affords certain insights about students’ writing but limits others. An important theme in the recent testing-assessment debate is that writing assessment tasks and the criteria used to judge students’ writing should reflect what teachers want writers to be able to do. Assessments should inspire young writers to write with purpose and invention, and the assessments themselves should be opportu- Assessment Criteria: The Special Role of Writing Rubrics Students need to develop understandings of the techniques that make writing effective, and as69 Classroom Writing Assessment sessment criteria provide a technical language that captures the qualities of effective writing. Criteria are a framework for class discussions of writing samples and a guide for student writers as they compose. To help students evaluate their progress, rubrics can represent the characteristics of developing writing along performance continua, with higher levels more closely fitting the criteria for effective writing. Consider two examples of rubrics designed for classroom assessment, the first a generic rubric, the second a genre-specific rubric for narrative writing. that to my sister,’ Lou cried, moving to shield Tasha with her body.”). Rubrics should be resources for student reflection and analysis, not prescriptions for formulaic texts. Rubrics can inform and act as a scaffold for a young writer’s work, but they can also constrain and limit the composing process. Completion criteria—for instance, that a piece must have five paragraphs or that each paragraph must begin with a topic sentence—may not fit a particular genre of writing or a particular writer’s intention. Generic rubrics obscure genre differences and may discourage a young writer’s choice and innovation. Vague rubrics provide little guidance, for example, when they define levels in quantitative and comparative terms, perhaps requiring “some supporting detail” or “few transition words.” Students and teachers are left to puzzle out where and why these features are needed in any given piece of writing. However a rubric is constructed, it should not stand alone. Teachers can use rubrics to guide students in analysis of published texts and their own writing, so criteria become part of the interpretive process in the classroom. To enhance students’ understandings of the rubric and their perceptions of ownership, teachers can engage students in the very construction of the rubric itself (Ainsworth and Christinson, 1998). A Generic Rubric Vicki Spandel and Richard Stiggins (1997) developed a model of analytical writing assessment that contains six traits: ideas, organization, voice, word choice, sentence fluency, and conventions. Each trait is defined using a technical language to capture the components of the construct, and teachers are encouraged to integrate this language in their writing instruction. To support judgments of these writing traits, the authors developed performance continua that can be organized as a rubric. For example, for organization, writing at the beginning level has no real lead or conclusion, the sequence is confusing, and pacing is too fast or too slow. At higher levels, a piece demonstrates progressively more effective introductions and conclusions, sequencing, pacing, and appropriate use of transitions. Portfolio Assessment A writing portfolio contains a body of writing, and portfolio assessment is the process of judging the work (see Portfolios). There are many models of portfolios and portfolio assessment (Calfee and Perfumo, 1996). A portfolio can range from a simple compilation of writing to an organized presentation of a writer’s work and its evolution; it can span work from one unit of study, an entire course, or several grade levels. Evaluation of the portfolio (often structured as a rubric) can focus on the range of work, the quality of work, the development of the work, or all of these; it can be reported as a rubric score or as written comments. The portfolio may be assembled and evaluated for the purpose of summative assessment or may be integrated in ongoing work in the classroom as students review their portfolio on a regular basis and reflect on their growth. Students prepare a typical portfolio as follows. The writer selects work samples to provide evi- A Genre-Specific Rubric Shelby Wolf and Maryl Gearhart (1994) developed a rubric for the assessment of narrative writing containing five dimensions: theme, character, setting, plot, and communication. The rubric constructs are drawn from literary analysis, and teachers are provided with instructional resources to guide class discussions on literature and students’ narrative writing. Each dimension of the rubric contains six levels that reflect what is known about pathways to the development of narrative writing. For example, the theme dimension at the second level is represented in a child’s text as a series of simple statements (“I like my Mom. I like my Dad.”) or in the coherence of the action itself (“He blew up the plane. Pow!”). At the fifth level, the child’s writing displays beginning use of secondary themes, and the main theme is increasingly revealed through discovery rather than delivery (“‘You can’t do 70 Classroom Writing Assessment dence of growing competence along a number of dimensions. For example, to demonstrate competence with the writing process, the student selects artifacts from all phases of a writing assignment, including brainstorming, outlining, drafting, revising, editing, and publishing. To demonstrate writing in a range of genres, the student writer selects a poem, a personal narrative, a persuasive letter, and a science report; to convey growth during the academic year, the selections would reflect early and later pieces or samples that show growth within a one- to twomonth poetry unit. Student writers make these choices, organize the samples, and then write a letter of introduction to the reader, contextualizing the collection in an autobiographical account of their development as a writer. They prepare a table of contents to guide the reader, bind the portfolio, and decorate the cover to produce a polished presentation of their accomplishments. Portfolio assessment is more than the process of collecting and evaluating the portfolio. Portfolios support reflection when students review their work and confer with teachers or parents to discuss their growth and set goals for further work. Conferences are contexts for student writers and their readers to negotiate their understandings of texts face-to-face. Each participant comes away with a clearer understanding of what the writer has accomplished, what needs to be worked on, and what resources and strategies will enable the student to improve. Self-Assessment Effective writers try to anticipate the possible ways that readers may interpret their writing, particularly when writing for a focused purpose. A persuasive letter must do more, for example, than express the writer’s frustration; it must convince critical readers that the issue is worthy of their consideration. In classrooms where writing is viewed as a transaction between the writer and reader, teachers encourage students to read their own writing as readers. Students may find a quiet corner where they can read their piece aloud, or they may assess their draft using the class rubric. Students may reflect on their learning and growth in their journal or in their autobiographical portfolio letter of introduction. The goal is to help students internalize the kinds of assessment dialogues between writers and readers that occur every day in the classroom (Wiggins, 1998). Maryl Gearhart Conferences Developing writers benefit from many and varied opportunities for someone to help them evaluate the strengths and weaknesses of their writing. Conferences range in formality, purpose, and participation. A teacher might rove through a classroom as students write, conducting miniconferences with students informally about the progress of a piece. Or the teacher may arrange lengthier and more formal times to confer with each student about writing progress, and those events may focus on the development of a particular piece of writing or a student’s portfolio. A peer may read a student’s writing and respond in person or in writing to strengths and weaknesses; the process benefits both students, because it illustrates ways that someone’s writing may be interpreted in unexpected ways. In student-led conferences, students present a prepared portfolio to their parents, showcasing their best writing and their progress and setting goals for further improvement (Davies et al., 1992). These conferences provide teachers with valuable opportunities to see how students’ writing reflects their parents’ cultural and linguistic practices. See Also Authentic Assessment; Kidwatching and Classroom Evaluation; Portfolios; Process Writing; Writing across the Curriculum References Ainsworth, Larry, and Jan Christinson. 1998. Student Generated Rubrics: An Assessment Model to Help All Students Succeed. Orangeburg, NY: Dale Seymour. Calfee, Robert, and Pam Perfumo, eds. 1996. Writing Portfolios in the Classroom: Policy and Practice, Promise and Peril. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Davies, Anne, Caren Cameron, Colleen Politano, and Kathleen Gregory. 1992. Together Is Better: Collaborative Assessment, Evaluation, and Reporting. Winnipeg, MB, Canada: Peguis. Hall, Nigel. 1997. “Young Children as Authors.” In Vic Edwards and David Corson, eds., Encyclopedia of Language and Education. Vol. 2, Literacy, pp. 69–76. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. International Reading Association and National Council of Teachers of English. 1994. Standards for the Assessment of Reading and Writing. 71 Cloze Procedure should be typed using blank lines to represent the omitted words. All blank lines should be the same length. Depending on the teacher’s purpose, a dash representing each letter of the omitted word can be used. However, this will yield a significantly higher score than using blank lines of equal length. Before they attempt to fill in the missing words, students should read the entire cloze passage. After filling in the missing words, students should reread the passage. A lengthy period of time is usually needed for students to complete the cloze passage. Younger students and less successful readers will find the cloze a difficult and frustrating task. These students may not be able to finish an entire cloze passage. In scoring the cloze, the words that are supplied by the student are compared with the original words in the original text. Only exact matches are considered correct answers. Synonyms should only be counted as correct answers if the cloze is being used as a teaching strategy for improving students’ use of context clues and comprehension of individual sentences. When teachers use the cloze to assess text readability, they employ the following criteria to evaluate the likelihood of students’ success when reading the text. Students scoring above 50 percent can read the text independently (without teacher assistance). Students scoring 34–50 percent correct are within the instructional level (the student can be successful with teacher assistance) but may need enrichment activities in conjunction with the text to ensure success. Students scoring below 34 percent will be frustrated by the difficulty of the text. If the cloze procedure indicates students are at the frustration level (text is too difficult), an easier text should be used. There are no criteria for determining functional reading levels when synonyms are accepted. There are several modifications to the traditional cloze procedure. The Maze is sometimes used as an introduction to a traditional cloze procedure. It differs from the cloze in that it provides three answer choices for each deleted word. Answer choices usually follow a pattern: the correct word, a word signifying the same part of speech, a word signifying a different part of speech. Some Maze procedures provide students with a word bank rather than placing words beneath blanks. ESL students may find this type of cloze procedure easier. Criteria for evaluating students’ per- Newark, DE: International Reading Association and Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Maylath, Bruce. 1997. “Assessors’ Language Awareness in the Evaluation of Academic Writing.” In Leo van Lier and David Corson, eds., Encyclopedia of Language and Education. Vol. 6, Knowledge about Language, pp. 195–204. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Rosenblatt, Louise. 1969. “Toward a Transactional Theory of Reading.” Journal of Reading Behavior 1 (1):31–51. Spandel, Vicki, and Richard J. Stiggins. 1997. Creating Writers: Linking Writing Assessment and Instruction. 2nd ed. New York: Longman. Wiggins, Grant. 1998. Educative Assessment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Winograd, Peter, Rebecca Blum Martinez, and Elizabeth Noll. 1999. “Alternative Assessments of Learning and Literacy: A U.S. Perspective.” In Daniel A. Wagner, Richard L. Venezky, and Brian V. Street, eds., Literacy: An International Handbook, pp. 203–209. Boulder: Westview Press. Wolf, Shelby A., and Maryl Gearhart. 1994. “Writing What You Read: A Framework for Narrative Assessment.” Language Arts 71:425–445. Cloze Procedure The cloze procedure is an informal assessment that requires the reader to supply words that have been systematically deleted from a passage (Harris and Sipay, 1990). The cloze can be used for several purposes—as an assessment of a student’s ability to construct meaning of individual sentences using context clues, syntax, and other information from a specific text (Tierney and Readence, 2000), as a measure of a text’s readability level, or for placement of students for instruction. The cloze procedure can be used with readers of all ages, as well as with English as a second language (ESL) students. To create a cloze, a teacher chooses a passage from the middle of a text that is typical of the material students would be expected to read. The passage should be 250–350 words in length. The passage should be one that students have not read or studied previously. No words are deleted from the first and last sentences of the passage. Beginning with the second sentence of the passage, the teacher deletes every nth word (usually every fifth word). A minimum of fifty deletions is recommended for high reliability. Generally, proper nouns are not omitted. The passage 72 College Literacy and Learning Reading to Every Child. 3rd ed. New York: Macmillan. Nurss, Joanne R., and Ruth A. Hough. 1992. Reading and the ESL Student: In What Research Has to Say About Reading Instruction. 2nd ed. Edited by S. Jay Samuels and Alan E. Farstrup. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Tierney, Robert J., and John E. Readence. 2000. Reading Strategies and Practices: A Compendium. 5th ed. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. formance on the Maze are more stringent than the cloze due to students’ ability to recognize rather than supply correct answers. Students scoring 60–70 percent are within their instructional level when reading the text. Above 70 percent, students can read the material independently, but below 60 percent, students become frustrated (Harris and Sipay, 1990). Teachers should analyze why students made errors and design instruction to meet their needs (Lapp and Flood, 1992). The Opin is another modification of the cloze procedure. This assessment requires the use of only one or two sentences with one or two important words deleted. Students read the sentences, fill in the blanks, and explain their word choices. ESL students’ explanations for answers allow the teacher to determine whether the student has learned the rules of language. As with the Maze, the Opin does not provide specific information about grade-level reading ability (Lapp and Flood, 1992). Teachers focus on the students’ word choices to evaluate comprehension. The cloze procedure has several advantages. First, it is easier and quicker to construct, administer, score, and interpret than some other informal reading assessments. Second, it can be group administered. Third, it provides a measure of students’ ability to use semantic (word and sentence meaning) and syntactic (word-order relationships) cues (Nurss and Hough, 1992). However, like most reading assessments, the cloze procedure has limitations. First, students’ prior knowledge of the topic and their ability to use language will influence their performance on the cloze (Harris and Sipay, 1990). Second, the cloze provides only limited diagnostic information. Little can be determined from the cloze concerning students’ decoding strategies or comprehension. Although some teachers believe that cloze activities help students focus on meaning rather than pronunciation of words, there is little research to support the use of cloze as a means for improving comprehension. Pamela J. Dunston and M. Christina Pennington College Literacy and Learning College Literacy and Learning (CLL) is a special interest group (SIG) of the International Reading Association (IRA), as revised May 2, 1973, and serves the same international membership the IRA encompasses. The CLL’s purpose is to provide for an exchange of ideas and techniques concerning remedial and developmental reading and study-skills programs indigenous to twoand four-year colleges and universities in the areas of methods, diagnosis of students’ reading, and evaluation of effectiveness of programs, textbooks, and teaching materials. The mission of CLL is to propose and encourage the adoption of certain specific qualifications for educators in college reading and study programs and to act as a resource body to aid colleges and universities in implementing or improving reading and study programs for their students. CLL’s conference and business meeting is held during the IRA annual conference, which is scheduled during the final days of April and into the beginning of May. For CLL to maintain a SIG status with IRA, a minimum of 100 CLL members who are also IRA members must be present. In order to extend CLL’s exchange of ideas and techniques, educators and researchers in the fields of college developmental literacy and study skills are encouraged to submit articles to the following publications: Journal of College Literacy and Learning, Innovative Learning Strategies (ILS), and NewsNotes. The Journal of College Literacy and Learning is an annual, refereed publication related to college and post-secondary reading and writing improvement. Four copies of fifteen- to twenty-page manuscripts are submitted to the journal’s editor. Copies of the Journal of College Literacy and Learning are distributed at the annual CLL meeting. Innovative Learning Strategies is a biennial, refereed yearbook. Authors are encouraged to submit four See Also Informal Reading Inventory References Harris, Albert J., and Edward R. Sipay. 1990. How to Increase Reading Ability: A Guide to Developmental and Remedial Methods. 9th ed. New York: Longman. Lapp, Diane, and James Flood. 1992. Teaching 73 College Reading and Learning Association given biannually in the following categories: Outstanding Dissertation, Outstanding Service, Outstanding Writing, and Dedication and Commitment to the Field of College Reading and Study Skills. Nominations are submitted to the awards chair for committee review. Plaques are distributed to the winners at the CLL conference business meeting. Contact information for the organization is available through the listserv and on the web site (available: http://www.ucollege. uc.edu/cll). Missy Laine College Reading and Learning Association The College Reading and Learning Association (CRLA) was founded in 1966 and is the oldest learning-assistance association in the United States. CRLA is an organization of student-centered professionals (faculty, staff, and administrators) active in the fields of reading, learning assistance, developmental education, and tutorial services at the college/adult level. The goals of CRLA are to provide media for dialogue among professionals, to cooperate and coordinate with related professional organizations, to increase the tools available to improve student learning, to act to ensure an environment where effective learning can take place, and to provide information and consultants to bodies enacting legislation directly related to college reading, learning assistance, developmental education, and tutorial services. Participation in CRLA activities promotes the sharing of ideas and concerns through networking with those in the field. Membership in the organization is international, with most members in the United States and Canada. CRLA holds an annual fall conference, which provides opportunities for networking and professional development through participation in preconference institutes, concurrent sessions, round tables, and special interest group (SIG) activities. These conferences bring in keynote speakers who are leaders in the related fields. The conferences provide opportunities to interact with experts in the field during structured events such as Lunch with a Mentor and preconference institutes with featured speakers and during lessstructured social events such as the Newcomers Reception and the nightly hospitality suite. College Literacy and Learning aids colleges and universities in setting up reading programs for their students (Planetworks) copies of their articles pertinent to college-level reading programs and strategies that enhance students’ academic success and to include: a description of program models, a discussion of successful instructional strategies/materials, and a research report, if available. This journal may be purchased at the CLL annual conference or by contacting the ILS editor. NewsNotes is the College Literacy and Learning newsletter, which includes information on contacting administrative officers and committee chairs, announcements, important events notices, CLL conference information, and short articles on ideas for the college classroom that cover such topics as strategies, activities, and assistance for struggling students. Presenters from the CLL conference are encouraged to submit a brief summary of their conference presentation. NewsNotes is mailed to the membership biannually. To submit information, contact the NewsNotes chair. College Literacy and Learning Awards are 74 College Reading Association for the group was college developmental reading and study skills; its original members were located in the Northeast and Mid-Atlantic states. Since then, however, CRA’s scope has expanded to include literacy professionals interested in one or more of its four divisions. Membership has expanded as well; most of CRA’s approximately 500 individual members and 800 institutional members are located in North America. CRA achieves its purposes in three major ways. First, the organization sponsors an annual conference lasting two and a half days, usually Thursday through Sunday in late October or early November. The conference agenda always includes featured sessions led by respected literacy-education scholars, along with symposia, workshops, and many individual sessions. CRA also sponsors several scholarly publications. Its quarterly journal, Reading Research and Instruction, is found in many college libraries. (In 1970–1984, the journal was called Reading World. Prior to 1970, its title was Journal of the Reading Specialist.) Conference Proceedings were published in 1961–1970. In 1989, CRA decided to begin publishing papers from its conferences again; an annual Yearbook has been published since that time. The organization has sponsored occasional monographs over the years as well. (Yearbooks and many of the monographs are available to nonmembers via the ERIC Document Reproduction Service.) Finally, CRA’s awards program helps to achieve its overall purposes. The organization currently gives annual awards for outstanding dissertation, outstanding master’s research, lifetime achievement, research and scholarship, and service to the organization. Any student may submit work for the dissertation or master’s research awards, which are adjudicated by the Research Committee. CRA members nominate people for the other awards, a process overseen by the Awards Committee. CRA’s web site provides general information about the organization, a list of current officers, information about conferences, and more. Resources also include Literacy Cases On-Line, available at http://literacy.okstate.edu. Nancy Padak CRLA annually publishes three newsletters and two editions of the Journal of College Reading and Learning (JCRL). CRLA has also published The Tutor Training Handbook and, in conjunction with H and H Publishing, Starting a Learning Center Monograph. CRLA established the International Tutor Certification Program in 1989 to provide professional standards for tutor training as well as a coherent tutor-training curriculum. Over 400 programs have been certified at up to three levels, resulting in increased tutor motivation, rewards for tutor accomplishments, and visibility for the certified program. More recently, CRLA established the International Mentoring Certification Program. Both programs provide a framework for those institutions beginning or revamping tutoring or mentoring programs. CRLA encourages its members to form state or regional associations; some groups elect officers and are formally organized as chapters, whereas other groups are led by CRLA-appointed directors. More information about the College Reading and Learning Association, including contacts for state and regional associations, can be accessed via the web site (available: http://www.crla.net). Susan Deese-Roberts College Reading Association The College Reading Association (CRA) is a professional, educational, not-for-profit organization devoted to the purpose of fostering and promoting directly or indirectly the growth and development of the teaching and learning processes related to reading at all levels (CRA, Constitution and Bylaws, 1993). CRA sponsors four divisions: Adult Learning, Clinical Education, College Education, and Teacher Education. Members may be affiliated with one, several, or no divisions. CRA began in 1958 when a group of college reading professors met at Temple University in Philadelphia to explore the possibility of forming a professional organization for colleagues in the Northeast and Mid-Atlantic states. The professors decided to survey colleagues, and when they learned that others shared their interest, they organized CRA’s first conference, drew up a constitution for the fledgling organization, and sought not-for-profit status, which was granted in 1963 (Alexander and Strode, 1999). The original focus References Alexander, J. Estill, and Susan L. Strode. 1999. History of the College Reading Association, 1958–1998. Carrollton, GA: College Reading Association. 75 Comics College Reading Association. 1993. Constitution and Bylaws. Carrollton, GA: College Reading Association. Comics Two types of comics are addressed in the literature in literacy—cartoon or comic strips, such as those found in daily newspapers, and comic books, presenting action or adventure stories in cartoon format. Both comic strips and comic books have been recommended as alternative texts to motivate and instruct students. Comics and cartoons have been used as instructional materials to foster language skills, provide values clarification, and promote critical thinking. Comic books have also been recommended as a way to teach students the techniques found in narrative, such as Spider-Man episodes that can be analyzed to study foreshadowing, dramatic fiction narration, flashback, irony, symbolism, metaphor, and allusion (Palumbo, 1979). John Elliott (1985) pointed out that comics are a powerful medium as they combine the verbal and the pictorial. Students can be taught how the comic conveys its message, thereby enhancing their understanding of the techniques of other media. Research on the benefits of using comic books to enhance vocabulary and comprehension has shown mixed results. John Guthrie (1978) found that good readers made similar gains on a standardized reading test when they read either books or comics, but that poor readers did not. Richard Campbell (1977) tested the effects of using high-interest comic books with fourth graders and found some gains in the students’ vocabulary and comprehension compared to those who did not read comic books. Comic books and their readers have also been studied as a recreational or informal literacy practice. Jeffrey Brown (1997) identified preadolescent and adolescent boys as the primary readers of comic books. Brown interviewed boys at comic book stores, shopping malls, and comic book conventions. He found that young males read comic books for social reasons, for a sense of community with the characters and the narrative world or with other comic book readers. Boys read comic books to gain prestige among other boys, to feel a sense of kinship with comic book artists and their characters, to emulate Comics are among the types of literature that can motivate children to read (Michael Siluk) moral codes of conduct, and to find comfort in a world of family turmoil. Boys had intense emotional involvement with the characters they followed and used these characters as role models for forming their gender identity. Comics have been criticized for their sex role stereotyping of both males and females. Females appear less frequently than males in comics and often remain within the home in stereotypical roles (Brabant and Mooney, 1986). Comic book superheroes present stereotypical representations of society’s notions of what a man should be—powerful, tough, independent, resourceful, and dashing. Hence, teachers may wish to teach students who read comics to read them critically, and deconstruct their gendered messages. Barbara J. Guzzetti See Also Critical Literacy; Gender and Reading; Popular Culture References Brabant, Sarah, and Linda Mooney. 1986. “Sex Role Stereotyping in the Sunday Comics: Ten Years Later.” Sex Roles 14 (3):412–418. Brown, Jeffrey. 1997. New Heroes: Gender, Race, Fans, and Comic Book Superheroes. Ph.D. diss., 76 Commercial Reading Programs University of Toronto, Canada. Diss., Abstracts International 59 (06) 1818. (AAT NQ27882). Campbell, Richard W. 1977. Using Comic Books as an Alternative Supplement to the Basal Reading Program at Albert Sidney Johnston Elementary School. ERIC Document Reproduction Service, ED 141 797. Elliott, John. 1985. The Study of, and through, Comic Books in the Language Classroom. ERIC Document Reproduction Service, ED 263 747. Guthrie, John. 1978. “Research Views: Comics.” The Reading Teacher 32 (3):376–378. Palumbo, Donald. October 1979. “The Use of Comics as an Approach to Introducing the Techniques and Terms of Narrative to Novice Readers.” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Popular Culture Association in the South, Louisville, Kentucky. letter names and each accompanying sound as well as groupings of letters into syllables. In addition, there were often alphabet verses, with each one illustrating a specific letter. Sentences from the Bible were also frequently included as an aid to teach a variety of moral principles. During the latter part of the nineteenth century, a large number of graded commercial reading series were developed and published. Of these, easily the most famous and most influential was the McGuffey reader (Westerhoff, 1978). These distinctive reading books were among the first commercial reading materials to be both grade-level specific and organized according to increasing difficulty of the material. The contents were generally oriented toward moralistic themes and provided information that was intended to inspire patriotic feelings in students. The McGuffey readers are also noted for their first extended use of various types of the emerging genre of American literature. The early part of the twentieth century saw increased interest in the teaching of reading as well as the beginning of the professional training of classroom teachers in reading. The literacy work by such individuals as Edmund Huey, Arthur Gates, and William S. Gray began to be reflected in the commercial reading materials of this period. Most notably, these changes included a new emphasis on the integration of reading with various other school subjects, the need to teach silent versus oral reading, and the adjustment of reading instruction to meet individual differences in students. Following World War I, a number of factors strongly influenced the development of commercial reading materials. Of particular note was the development of a variety of standardized reading tests that could measure in a very direct manner the relative effectiveness of various types of reading materials. To a large degree, the results of these tests affected both the format and the content of many commercial reading programs. Whereas authors of classroom reading materials had previously felt little constraint over how they organized and developed their products, they now began to see test results as a clear determiner of their products. In addition, the work of educational philosophers such as John Dewey began to shape the format and content of many commercial reading materials. Emphasis shifted from the use of a series of readers toward a wider Commercial Reading Programs Commercial reading programs can be defined as those published literacy materials that are specifically designed for the teaching of reading. Traditionally, commercial reading programs have included a wide range of components, such as student readers, workbooks, and supplemental instructional aids, that is, arts and craft activities, library books, and various types of technology aids. The intended use of these commercial reading programs varies, ranging from a complete reading curriculum to a supplemental part of the total reading program. Controversial issues associated with commercial reading programs have involved their content, their use in the classroom, and their cost. Historical Background of Commercial Reading Materials The use of commercial reading materials has had a long and storied history in reading education. Almost since the earliest formal teaching of reading, there have been some types of commercially developed reading books available (Smith, 1934). In the latter part of the seventeenth and into the eighteenth century, commercial reading materials were already evident in the public schools. These early commercial reading materials were almost always based on religious or moral themes. The dominant reading textbook of this period was the New England Primer, which, in a variety of editions and formats, was eventually published in more than twenty-five editions. The lesson format consisted of learning individual 77 Commercial Reading Programs Advice books were a kind of commercial reading material common in the nineteenth century (John Frost, Easy Exercises, 1839, p. 21) basic readers, or basals, these commercial reading materials often centered on events that occurred in the lives of typical middle-class children and their families. Of particular importance during this time was the introduction and development of teacher’s manuals that frequently contained detailed instructions on the teaching of reading. This period also saw a significant increase in the supplemental materials developed for the reading teacher. Along with the basic reader, supplements frequently included workbooks, skill exercises, library materials, and various arts and crafts activities. This trend of increasing diversity in supplemental reading materials continues in today’s commercial reading programs. If a common theme could be identified in current reading programs, it would be the emphasis on the use of a wide variety of both traditional and contemporary literature. Typically, these commercial reading materials have been influenced by the latest developments in technology, such as the personal computer and the Internet. Controversial issues related to current variety of reading materials. These included library books, magazines, and other types of popular literature. Commercial reading materials were selected according to the individual interests of students, and thus a wide range of reading resources was found in the typical classroom reading program. Problems with the use of commercial reading materials in this manner were quickly evident. Of particular concern was the lack of available reading materials, especially those related to a single topic or individual student interest. In addition, teachers were concerned about the adequate provision of needed reading skills and related opportunities for meaningful assessment procedures. There was also the added burden of higher cost associated with this approach. Although most schools gradually moved back to a basic reading series for primary instruction, this concept was to reemerge in a variety of forms, most notably in the “individualized” approach to reading. The period surrounding World War II saw the emergence of reading programs most typified by the “Dick and Jane” readers. Also referred to as 78 Commercial Reading Programs commercial reading materials involve the appropriate use of ethnic literature, whether to use complete or altered text material, and the basic role of these textbooks in an effective classroom literacy program. teacher’s guide, the result is a fast-paced teaching model that doesn’t take into account whether students have actually learned the information. Teachers are assumed to have taught well if they have taught the lesson, and students are assumed to have learned if they have responded with the predetermined answers (position taken by the National Council of Teachers of English, 1989, cited in Issues and Trends). Other content issues include concerns regarding whether images and information about women and minorities are presented accurately. Many of the basal anthologies used in classrooms contain literature that is not equally affirming for all social groups and in fact tends to represent traditional white middle- and upper-class families. It has also been suggested that publishers’ attempts to be more multiculturally inclusive have resulted in new updated illustrations that give the illusion of ethnic representation while maintaining a story line that is culturally biased toward middle-class white America’s values and ideas. Adding to the questions regarding content of commercial reading programs is concern about whether the typical excerpts found in basals are actually faithful to the intended meaning and integrity of the original unabridged versions. More recently, the emergence of several computer-based commercial reading programs has given rise to more question and controversy. Many educators feel that these programs, which are usually based on an extrinsic reward system, are not giving students a solid foundation for lifelong reading. Frequently, these programs provide motivational push by rewarding students as they move up levels and accumulate points by reading as many books as possible. Many educators feel that students are reading just carefully enough to answer the basic knowledge and content questions that typically accompany each book and are not reading with the depth that is required for higher-order thinking skills such as analysis and interpretation. Other controversial issues regarding computer-based reading programs include cost to schools and book availability. Students are required to choose literature that is supported by the program because it comes with an accompanying computer-based assessment tool. Other books go unread, and decisions to purchase new books are based on whether they are supported by the program rather than on qualities of good literature. Controversial Issues Related to Commercial Reading Materials Controversial issues continue to surround the use of commercial reading programs, among them questions about their content, their use in the classroom, ethics in publishing, and high costs to schools. The basal reader, one of the most commonly used types of commercial reading programs, has long been at the center of the controversy. More recently, the rise of computerbased commercial reading programs has only added to the controversy over effective use of commercial reading materials in the classroom. Some believe that the basic role of the teacher as an instructional leader is being seriously challenged today by various forms of technology, especially the personal computer. Recent advances such as the development of books on computers and sophisticated search techniques related to the Internet have only added to this current controversy over the primary role of commercial reading materials in an effective classroom reading program. Much of the controversy has been caused by the content and sequence of lessons included in many of the teacher guides that are designed to accompany basal-reading anthologies. Suggested instructional plans tend to dramatically limit the volume of actual in-school reading, despite evidence that extensive time spent reading and writing is what nourishes the development of literacy. Basal-reading programs tend to limit student reading time in order to allow more time for extension activities that include workbook pages and skill sheets. A six-week unit might focus on just one single book. It is argued that basal-reading anthologies just don’t contain enough reading material to develop high levels of reading proficiency in children (Allington, 2001). It has also been suggested that the sequencing of skills in a basal-reading series exists not because this is how children learn to read but simply because of the logistics of developing a series of lessons that can be taught in sequence. Many educators feel that when teachers attempt to complete each lesson as presented in the 79 Community Literacy Community Literacy More controversy stems from the fact that the basal industry is big business, producing sales of more than $400 million annually (Shannon and Goodman, 1994). This fact raises ethical issues regarding whose interests are being met. In the hopes of meeting the needs of many different schools with many different philosophies, commercial reading programs tend to include an eclectic assortment of components. Although this inclusivity attempts to meet the needs of differing beliefs and pedagogies, it is not necessarily based on sound research or theoretical underpinnings. Because they are eager to make a difference in reading education, parents, teachers, administrators, and politicians tend to purchase and implement commercial reading programs long before the ideas contained therein have been sufficiently tested or validated (Mosenthal and Kamil, 1996). Richard Robinson and Laurie Kingsley Community literacy refers to adult literacy programs in which members of the community work with literacy mentors (who are usually university based) to resolve personal economic, social, or political problems through written and spoken methods. Such problem solving (focused on, for example, issues of youth and respect or problems of urban unemployment) may result in several different kinds of texts, including performative scripts, poetry and narratives, and highly polished brochures geared toward wide audiences. Distinguished from the discourses of advocacy and selfexpression, community literacy is designed to link personal and public action with probing reflection. In creating a “local public” around community questions, it continues a tradition of democratic discourse (Hauser, 1999). The practice of community literacy, as described by Wayne Peck, Linda Flower, and Lorraine Higgins (1995), is also a hybrid discourse (a discourse that uses several dialects or multimedia to serve its purpose). Community literacy can be defined by four critical features: It is focused on intercultural dialogue, motivated by a vision of social change, supported by a strategic view of writing, and shaped by a process of inquiry. Community literacy is a view to literate action that stems from Deweyan pragmatism, Freirian calls for justice, and a rhetorical, social-cognitive research-based form of problem solving; it is also a form of inquiry that focuses on the consequences of intercultural, hybrid literate acts. Differing from functional literacy (the ability to read and write), which asks participants to gain expertise in the dominant discourse to survive in the world at large, community literacy focuses on literate acts (booklets, letters, performances, web pages, and so on) that solve problems within a specific community context. Although knowledge of the dominant discourse can be helpful in this goal, it is not the primary focus. Differing from expressivist literacy, which focuses on the free communication of feelings and perceptions, community literacy is an inquiry-driven problem-solving process that asks participants to make tough rhetorical choices in the face of complex and diverse audiences. Expressing feelings and opinions is a necessary part of this process, but it leads to negotiation and revision of ideas and texts to effect change. Community literacy asks participants to go beyond their given knowledge by collaborating with oth- See Also Basal Readers References Allington, Richard C. 2001. What Really Matters for Struggling Readers: Designing Research-Based Programs. New York: Addison-Wesley Educational Publishers. Mosenthal, Peter B., and Michael L. Kamil. 1996. “Epilogue: Understanding Progress in Reading Research.” In Rebecca Barr, Michael L. Kamil, Peter B. Mosenthal, and P. David Pearson, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 2, pp. 1013–1046. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. National Council of Teachers of English, Commission on Reading. 1989. “Basal Readers and the State of American Reading Instruction: A Call for Action.” Reprinted in 2000, in Richard D. Robinson, Michael C. McKenna, and Judy M. Wedman, eds., Issues and Trends in Literacy Education, pp. 157–159. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Shannon, Patrick, and Kenneth Goodman, eds. 1994. Basal Readers: A Second Look. Katonah, NY: Richard C. Owen Publishers. Smith, Nila Banton. 1934. American Reading Instruction. New York: Silver Burdett. Westerhoff, John H. 1978. McGuffey and His Readers: Piety, Morality, and Education in Nineteenth-Century America. Nashville: Abingdon Press. 80 Community Literacy ers whose (intercultural) expertise is different from theirs. Therefore, community literacy is an intercultural, situated form of problem solving that draws on multiple kinds of nontraditional expertise stemming from inner-city teens, single mothers, and struggling workers (among others). An example of community literacy in action is Pittsburgh’s Community Literacy Center. The Community Literacy Center (CLC) began as a collaboration between Carnegie Mellon University and Pittsburgh’s Community House (a historic settlement house on the North Side—an urban, largely African American community). Plagued by persistent problems of racism, unemployment (resulting in part from the collapse of the steel industry), poor schooling, police brutality, and gang violence, the North Side was a good choice for performing situated, intercultural inquiries that used literate acts to enact social change. At the CLC, mentors from Carnegie Mellon University and local community-based organizations worked with urban teenagers from local high schools to address problems that have negatively impacted the youths’ lives. Past issues have included the police-enforced curfew, the school-suspension policy, gangs and respect, and youth unemployment. Teens and mentors collaborate (often across large cultural gaps) to create hybrid rhetorical texts (texts that use their own home discourse and language but that also use Standard English to reach a larger audience) with the goal of effecting positive change. The culmination of this process is twofold: first, a publication that is geared both toward those living in the problem and those who have the power to make changes with the problem; and second, an exercise called community conversation in which teens perform their texts to a diverse audience to open a problem-solving dialogue as a way to receive feedback on their work and to move the problem-solving process past the walls of the CLC. Community literacy is an intercultural process. The diverse participants in these literate acts must learn to cross boundaries of experience, geography, gender, age, discourse, ethnicity, and class (among other things). The intercultural nature of community literacy is more than a byproduct of its participant base: widely varied experience and hybrid discourse are seen as a vital part of the practice and process of problem analysis and solution. The participants’ goals are more likely to be reached when everyone’s diverse expertise is considered in the process of creating their finished texts. Difference, then, becomes part of the knowledge from which to build rhetorically persuasive texts that have a good chance of affecting the problems they address. When a mentor and teen work together, this difference can have a generative affect. For example, teens writing about an experience with gang violence may just wish to tell their story, but the response of the college mentor who fails to get it pushes such writers to deal with interpretations they didn’t anticipate. For example, the mentor could ask: “It seems as if you are saying gangs are OK. Is that the job you want your text to do?” The teen could then respond with a different interpretation: “I don’t want people to think that gangs are good but that kids join gangs for important reasons like respect and protection.” This process includes further levels of expertise at the community conversation, where government officials, parents, community workers, academics, and adults from the community react to the teens’ texts. Community literacy is also a strategic process. Drawing from the theory and research of socialcognitive rhetoric, community literacy helps writers see their literate acts as a process of goals, plans, arguments, construction, and negotiation. As a rhetorical process, community literacy strives to create texts that are sensitive to audience, context, persuasiveness, and the real possibility of consequences. As a social-cognitive process, community literacy focuses on the agency of individual writers who must negotiate among competing claims, goals, evidence, plans, and experiences. To help participants navigate such stormy waters, rhetoric brings strong strategies to the table. For example, mentors and teens learn to “rival”—to take a rival hypothesis stance to problems, seeking diverse interpretations that situate their ideas and literate acts in the complex contexts of social problems. When teens come to the CLC with their necessarily bounded experience (which also holds true for the mentors), rival hypothesis thinking can add depth to their argument. For example, when thinking about the police-enforced curfew issue, mentors and teens hypothesized about how diverse people would see the situation. How did the police interpret the curfew? Teachers? Parents? Ministers? Local government? Consideration of rivals both widened 81 Comparative Reading the experience base of the CLC participants and helped the teens to create stronger arguments that addressed the viewpoints and experience of multiple stakeholders in their final text. Community literacy also focuses on agentdriven writing. Teens and mentors work to create rhetorical artifacts that are then used as persuasive documents geared toward changing problematic situations. Moreover, this writing both affects and is affected by the understandings of the teens and the mentors. Writing then becomes a task on which to focus activity, a process that engenders learning for teens and mentors, and an important consequence of the process itself. Community literacy doesn’t end at open discussions or with personal writing that never aims at a wider audience. Instead, the texts the teens and mentors produce are a type of rhetorical praxis. For example, the written recommendations about school-suspension policies actually affected the suspension policy for the school district in question. Moreover, for teens to have the tangible rhetorical product in their hands is evidence of their own growing expertise in using rhetorical strategies to solve problems in their own lives, as well as in their communities. Community literacy is also an inquiry. It is in part a reflective and active process that closely analyzes the problematic situation with focus on what it will take to enable more positive consequences. Inquiry is also a process of understanding—of seeing open questions, realizing that social problems are far more complex than our partial representations grasp, that it is difficult to engage people in a hybrid discourse and even more difficult to build understandings that cross those differences. Part of the inquiry that community literacy supports is not about what to do, but about how someone else sees the world—and about what a teen, mentor, or adult can come to understand through the processes of writing, collaboration, and dialogue. In an inquiry, diverse literacies are not seen as “bad” or “good” (for example, dominant discourse is not necessarily bad and oppressive, nor is home dialect always good and liberating). Rather, these diverse discourses are seen as vital tools to be used in the teens’ and mentors’ collaboration to meet given goals. These inquiries are also situated in local contexts, and their data are the lived experience of the participants. At the same time, inquiries need diverse expertise, and collaboration becomes both a necessity and a process of learning for all participants. Community literacy is, in the end, about public deliberation, social change, and praxis. Sharing the ethical and philosophical imperatives of William James’s and John Dewey’s pragmatism, Cornel West’s prophetic pragmatism, and Paulo Freire’s radical critical literacy, community literacy is geared toward real consequences and multidimensional understandings in a complex world. In this sense, community literacy has grown from the imperative question of how to learn across differences of culture, experience, and values. It is a type of observation-based theory building: given the data, given what we know, what is the best way to go forward? Susan Swan and Linda Flower See Also Constructivism; Critical Literacy; Diversity; InquiryBased Instruction; Multicultural Literacy; Scaffolded Literacy Instruction; Social Justice and Literacies References Deans, Thomas. 2000. Writing Partnerships: ServiceLearning in Composition. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Flower, Linda. 1997. Problem-Solving Strategies for Writing in Classroom and Community. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Flower, Linda, Eleanor Long, and Lorraine Higgins. 2000. Learning to Rival: A Literate Practice for Intercultural Inquiry. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hauser, Gerard A. 1999. Vernacular Voices: The Rhetoric of Publics and Public Spheres. Columbia: University of South Carolina Press. Peck, Wayne, Linda Flower, and Lorraine Higgins. 1995. “Community Literacy.” College Composition and Communication 46 (2):199–222. Comparative Reading Comparative reading is the scholarly field in which researchers and theorists undertake the comparison of reading and related variables across national and cultural groups. John Downing may be given credit for coining the term comparative reading in 1969, although William S. Gray’s earlier work for the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) might be identified as the seminal, although not the first, comparative-reading re82 Comparative Reading search in the modern era of literacy pedagogy. As we begin the twenty-first century, the field of comparative reading will focus initially on two large multinational comparative-reading achievement studies: the Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS) undertaken by the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA) and the Programme for International Student Achievement (PISA) as sponsored by the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). Yet given past responses by politicians, journalists, and even educators to such large-scale projects, literacy educators need to be fully aware of the acceptable uses of comparative-reading research as well as potential abuses of such research. Comparative reading has its roots in the birth of the field of comparative education. In the 1800s, with the development of national education systems, educators started to examine systems and curricula in other countries. Such a utilitarian endeavor was designed to inform and improve local programs and instruction through the importation and integration of the best pedagogical practices and organizational structures from prestigious countries such as Switzerland and Prussia. With the coming of the 1900s, education was viewed as a touchstone for a nation; hence, comparative education became a method by which scholars could study the social, economic, and political dynamics of a country as well as the commonalties and differences between national and cultural groups. Through the period of World War II, comparative education continued to focus on the nation-state. The field of comparative reading traces its modern history back to the issuance of The Teaching of Reading by UNESCO in 1949. This International Bureau of Education survey asked forty-five national ministries of education to respond to questions about the teaching of reading. Not surprisingly from today’s perspective, the descriptive responses to the sixteen openended questions seemed to suggest that the definitions of reading and associated variables were often viewed from different theoretical, cultural, and practical perspectives from nation to nation. Cross-national standards for comparative activities did not yet exist. It was with William S. Gray’s 1953 pamphlets, “Preliminary Survey on Methods of Teaching Reading and Writing” (parts 1 and 2), and his pioneering 1956 text, The Teaching of Reading and Writing, based on his cross-national interviews, content analyses, and eye-movement research that a scholarly foundation for the field evolved. In addition, these publications for UNESCO were designed to help literacy leaders, particularly those in developing nations, to design programs and curriculum for the delivery of effective reading instruction. Six years later in 1962, Arthur Foshay and colleagues reported on the UNESCO Institute of Education project, in which the assessment of reading achievement of thirteen-year-olds from twelve nations was one component of what might be identified as the first carefully designed comparative study. In 1966, representatives from around the world came to Paris to attend the first International Reading Association (IRA)–sponsored World Congress on Reading. Then in 1973, the IEA released its ten-nation comparative literature study report by Alan Purves, Arthur Foshay, and G. Hanson, along with its comparative reading-comprehension study of fifteen countries, reported by Robert Thorndike. Also in this watershed year, Downing edited Comparative Reading: Cross-National Studies of Behavior and Processes in Reading and Writing, a book that was to become the first comprehensive text in the field of comparative reading. This critical and empirical analysis of reading around the world presented both theoretical and methodological underpinnings for the field of comparative reading, along with case studies of reading instruction in thirteen countries. With the high visibility provided by the IEA investigations, the ongoing success of the World Congresses on Reading and the accompanying proceedings, and Downing’s resolute support for the field in chapters and journal articles, comparative reading began to take on the nature of an academic specialty. During the 1980s, the growth of the field can be observed through the review of the evergrowing number of investigations and case reports covered in Eve Malmquist’s 1982 annotated bibliography for the IRA, John Hladczuk and William Eller’s book-length bibliography on comparative reading in 1987, and in another work by John Hladczuk, William Eller, and Sharon Hladczuk in 1989 on worldwide literacy and illiteracy, as well as annotations in the Annual Summary of Investigations relating to reading. Also in the 1980s, scholars such as Eve 83 Comparative Reading Malmquist and Hans Grundin, as well as John Downing, presented models for comparativereading research and theory. The 1990s saw the release of several monographs and texts on a new cross-national study by the IEA (Elley, 1994) with the participation of thirty-two school systems from around the world. In addition, the OECD sponsored the International Assessment of Adult Literacy in twenty countries or regions, which for the first time provided reliable cross-national data on adult literacy. Finally, the in the spirit of Downing’s earlier work, case studies continued, such as Margaret Harris and Giyoo Hatano’s (1999) edited work covering cross-linguistic factors in learning to read and write in nine countries and John Hladczuk and William Eller’s (1992) portrayal of instructional systems in twenty-six nations. Now, at the beginning of the twenty-first century, two major cross-national studies of “reading literacy” are underway: the Programme for International Student Achievement (PISA), under the auspices of the OECD in 2000, and the Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS), under the sponsorship of the IEA in 2001. PISA will focus on fifteen-year-olds who are about to make the transition from the world of school to the world of work. PIRLS will focus on fourth graders (nine- and ten-year-olds) who are moving from the learning-to-read stage to the reading-to-learn stage. Although there are differences in the design of each study and the developmental levels of the individuals being tested, there are similarities in assessment procedures, content of the assessment devices, and perhaps most important, the belief that reading is an interactive, constructive process. Technical reports will be issued on a regular basis, and information on the progress of these two endeavors can be found by visiting the education section or the links in the OECD’s web site (www.oecd.org). With a growing body of cross-national comparative studies, national case studies, and other reports, it becomes ever more important that the consumers of such literature understand that there are both positive uses and common problems associated with the academic and pedagogical writings on the topic. Norman Stahl, Bonnie Higginson, and James King have drawn upon writings in the field to describe both in depth. Their recommendations for appropriate uses of comparative reading include: describing various approaches to reading instruction, understanding both similarities and differences in national and educational cultures, remedying misperceptions and developing alternative conceptions of a national or cultural group, making decisions based on “parallel phenomena” observed in other nations, developing broad-based generalizations of literacy pedagogy across national boundaries, and training future teachers and reading professionals. These writers have also warned against misuses of comparative-reading data, including: the inappropriate use of case building for or against a local system based on comparative research, the misinterpretation or misrepresentation of crossnational or cross-cultural findings, the treatment of the results of a nation’s literacy assessment as an “Olympic” event in worldwide pedagogical games, and the practice of either ethnocentrism or overidentification with another educational system or culture. Downing saw the role of comparative reading as employing cross-cultural research and comparative study to expand both our theoretical and practical knowledge of the processes of literacy behavior. With this perspective in mind and with the careful study of comparative-reading research, literacy educators may come to understand that any well-defined theory of reading should be formulated, as appropriate, on constructs that cross traditional national, linguistic, gender, or cultural boundaries. Further, so as to promote a more fluid pedagogical worldview in the profession, both the theory and research of comparative reading should be an important cornerstone of both the formal graduate and undergraduate preparation of future teachers and literacy specialists and the in-service or certification renewal programs for practicing teachers. Norman A. Stahl See Also Reading Assessment; Writing Assessment References Downing, John, ed. 1973. Comparative Reading: Cross-National Studies of Behavior and Processes in Reading and Writing. New York: Macmillan. Reissued 1979. Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfilms International. Elley, Warwick B. 1994. The IEA Study of Reading Literacy: Achievement and Instruction in ThirtyTwo School Systems. Cambridge: Pergamon. Gray, William S. 1956. The Teaching of Reading and Writing. Paris: UNESCO. 84 Comprehension Strategies Traditional Comprehension Skills and the New Comprehension Strategies Twenty years ago, comprehension was taught as a sequence of separate skills identified in the basal-reading programs that dominated American reading instruction. Scott Paris and his colleagues (1991) identified skills as automatic procedures that readers used without being aware of them. Comprehension skills were traditionally “taught” by having students complete workbook pages in which they chose “the main idea” of a paragraph from one of four alternatives or reorganized sentences in the correct sequence of a paragraph they had just read. It was expected or assumed that through repeated practice, students would learn these skills and apply them to the new texts they read. There was no assumption of flexibility in the use of the skills for different texts, tasks, and purposes. There was no assumption that readers thought about what they were doing or reflected on whether the skill was the appropriate one to use. Nevertheless, a classic study by Dolores Durkin (1978–1979) demonstrated that students were not taught the skills effectively. Durkin showed that by following the directions in the basal-reading programs, teachers mostly “tested” the skills rather than “taught” them. In other words, students were directed to “find the main idea” and to “create a summary of a story,” but there was no help or assistance for students who could not complete these activities on their own. Further, even if students did complete the activities appropriately, it was often through unconscious awareness or luck rather than through conscious and deliberate planning and implementation. There was nothing intentional in either the teacher’s instructions or the students’ behaviors. Thus, Durkin convinced a generation of reading researchers that many students were unlikely to learn comprehension skills well enough to apply them to their daily reading. Durkin concluded that the “mentioning” rather than teaching of skills was a major problem in comprehension instruction in American schools. Durkin’s research presaged work on strategy instruction, in which students are explicitly taught how to use a number of strategies to improve their comprehension of text. Unlike skills, strategies are defined as “conscious, intentional procedures under the control of readers.” Research conducted over the past twenty years sug- Harris, Margaret, and Giyoo Hatano, eds. 1999. Learning to Read and Write: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hladczuk, John, and William Eller. 1987. Comparative Reading: An International Bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. ———. 1992. International Handbook of Reading Education. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Hladczuk, John, William Eller, and Sharon Hladczuk. 1989. Literacy/Illiteracy in the World: A Bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Malmquist, Eve. 1982. Handbook on Comparative Reading: An Annotated Bibliography. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Stahl, Norman A., Bonnie C. Higginson, and James R. King. 1993. “Appropriate Use of Comparative Literacy Research in the 1990s.” Journal of Reading 37 (2):104–113. Comprehension Strategies Comprehension strategies are procedures that active readers use to improve their comprehension of text (Report of the National Reading Panel, 2000). For example, readers may make predictions about an upcoming text, they may ask themselves questions about what they are reading, or they may summarize a text they just read. Each of these procedures or activities is considered to be a comprehension strategy that readers use to help them deepen their comprehension. Most researchers refer to strategies as conscious processes under the direct control of readers. They are deliberate, goal directed, and open to inspection in the sense that readers are aware of what they are doing and why they are doing it. Over time, however, and with practice, comprehension strategies can become automatic procedures that readers use without conscious planning. A number of strategies have been researched thoroughly and have been shown to improve comprehension. These include strategies such as identifying existing prior knowledge, visualizing, inferring, summarizing, synthesizing, predicting, determining importance, generating questions, monitoring comprehension, and repairing comprehension breakdowns. The value of these strategies is that they are useful for developing instructional procedures and help readers become independent of the teacher (Report of the National Reading Panel, 2000). 85 Comprehension Strategies gests that readers benefit from being taught the conscious and intentional use of strategies. Strategies are always goal directed. They are applied thoughtfully and with consideration of better comprehension. In addition, strategies emphasize reasoning, problem solving, and critical-thinking abilities. Strategies are also flexible and adaptable. Readers must learn when and where to apply a particular strategy. If that strategy does not work, readers try another. Finally, strategies imply metacognitive awareness. Effective strategy users are aware of the strategies they are using, and they reflect on their reading and their level of understanding. Still another contribution to developing notions of comprehension strategies comes from a body of research on metacognitive strategies. Linda Baker and Ann Brown (1984) summarized research that demonstrated convincingly that skilled readers are aware of whether they understand what they are reading. Further, skilled readers know what to do when they do not understand. They have developed a number of strategies that they effectively employ when comprehension breaks down for them. They use these strategies flexibly, and they adapt them when needed for different tasks and purposes. Less-skilled readers, by contrast, are not aware of whether they understand or not, and they do not have a set of metacognitive strategies to use when comprehension breaks down. However, an important finding from this body of work is that less-skilled readers can be taught to be metacognitive when they read. When they are taught, their comprehension improves significantly. Genesis of Comprehension Strategies The term comprehension strategies comes from an amalgamation of related bodies of research on cognitive and metacognitive strategies. The foundational body of theory and research for both cognitive and metacognitive strategies is drawn from cognitive psychology. This theory and research emphasizes the active, as opposed to the passive, nature of the comprehension process. Beginning in the 1970s, researchers concluded that comprehension was not a passive process in which readers simply “take in” the exact meaning in a text. Cognitive theory and research suggested that readers construct mental representations of what they read. These representations result from an interaction between readers’ prior knowledge and the new knowledge derived from the text. The resulting mental representations are not exact replications of the author’s intended message but are instead representations based on the range of meanings included in the text. Another important body of research informing comprehension strategies is work conducted in the 1980s on cognitive strategies. This work was designed to identify strategies that improve attention, memory, and learning. Claire Weinstein and Richard Mayer (1987) identified several cognitive or learning strategies that improve learning. These strategies determine how much is learned and how well what is learned is organized in memory. These strategies range from basic rehearsal strategies such as repetition to complex elaboration strategies such as summarizing. Thus, there is a clear overlap between comprehension strategies and cognitive or learning strategies identified by researchers. Comprehension Strategy Instruction According to the National Reading Panel (2000), an important finding of reading research is that comprehension strategies can be effectively taught. When teachers successfully teach comprehension strategies, students’ comprehension improves. This is especially true for less-skilled readers. The seminal work on comprehension strategy instruction was a series of studies conducted and summarized by Ann Palincsar and Ann Brown (1984). These researchers developed an instructional program consisting of the teaching of a set of four strategies: predicting, summarizing, asking questions, and clarifying hard parts of text. They taught these strategies directly to lessskilled middle-school students. As part of the instruction, teachers modeled the use of the strategies and provided students with abundant help and support as they learned the strategies. Students worked in peer teaching groups and practiced the use of the strategies until they could use them independently. The Report of the National Reading Panel (2000) reviewed several additional studies using these reciprocal teaching strategies. Evidence from their integrative review suggested that students who learned reciprocal teaching strategies were able to transfer their use of the techniques to other texts they read. In general, the research showed that teaching comprehen86 Comprehension Strategies sion strategies improved students’ comprehension of text. Other studies also tested the effectiveness of other kinds of comprehension strategy instruction. Gerald Duffy and his colleagues (1987) taught teachers to explicitly discuss the mental processes and cognitive strategies involved in comprehension. Specifically, they explicitly taught students what strategy they were learning, why they were learning it, why it was important, and how and when they could use it as they read. They found that this type of direct explanation of the reasoning and problem-solving nature of strategic reading improved students’ awareness of their strategy use and their comprehension. Another approach to comprehension strategies is identified as transactional strategy instruction (Pressley and Woloshyn, 1995). This instruction is similar to the direct explanation of comprehension strategies used by Duffy and his colleagues in that the reasoning and problemsolving nature of strategies are discussed. In transactional strategy instruction, however, strategies are learned not through the direct explanation of the teacher but through the dialogue that goes on between the teacher and students and among the students themselves. Thus, transactional strategy instruction is collaborative; learning about the strategies primarily takes place through the interactive transaction among students in the classroom. Despite the differences in the instructional programs using comprehension strategies, some common elements of comprehension strategy instruction can be gleaned. The method occurs when teachers: (1) model or directly explain the strategies, (2) provide students with guided practice in the use of the strategies through teacherstudent and student-student discussions, (3) provide students with independent practice in the use of the strategies, and (4) discuss the flexible and adaptable use of the strategies for different purposes. sion strategies to describe not only what readers do to help them comprehend texts but also what teachers do to help students comprehend texts. Thus, there is more than one use of the term comprehension strategies in the literature and in educational settings. Usually, comprehension strategies refer to specific strategies such as predicting, summarizing, visualizing, monitoring comprehension, and so forth. These strategies are under the direct control of readers. Readers learn how to apply the strategies to their reading of a text. However, sometimes authors use the term comprehension strategies to refer to specific activities or procedures that teachers use to help their students improve their comprehension of particular texts. Some examples of such strategies include the group-constructed KWL (Know, Want to Know, and Learned), story maps, graphic organizers, and cooperative learning. These are more like comprehension aids and instructional activities than strategies in the cognitive sense. The teacher completes the activity directly with students. As a result of completing the strategy or activity, students understand a particular text better. What is the difference between the two kinds of comprehension strategies? Reader or learner strategies are taught so that the students themselves control the strategies. Students learn how to use the strategies and apply them to different texts they read. There is an assumption that students can transfer the strategies from one text to another. By contrast, teacher strategies are activities that teachers complete with their students. Most often, there is no assumption or expectation that students will learn how to use these strategies on their own when they are reading without the teacher’s help. Recently, a proliferation of books has come on the market to help teachers teach reading comprehension to their K–12 students. These books often include both reader and teacher strategies under the term comprehension strategies. However, there is a difference in who controls the use of the strategy. The advantage of reader or learner strategies is that the reader or learner exercises the control, as opposed to teacher strategies controlled by the teacher. So long as teachers understand the purposes and uses of the two kinds of strategies, they can design appropriate comprehension strategy instruction for their students. Janice A. Dole Differences between Comprehension Strategies and Teacher Strategies in the Teacher-Practice Literature Comprehension strategies have been defined in much of the cognitive literature as strategies readers use to actively process text to improve comprehension. The educational literature is also replete with the use of the term comprehen87 Computer-Assisted Instruction grams or on-line resources that are designed to teach skills to students (CAI as teacher), to tutor students as they practice skills (CAI as tutor), or to serve as a resource tool (CAI as tool) as students engage in literate acts. See Also KWL and KWL+; Metacognition; National Reading Panel; Reading-Comprehension Processes References Baker, Linda, and Ann L. Brown. 1984. “Metacognitive Skills and Reading.” In P. David Pearson, ed., Handbook of Reading Research, pp. 353–394. New York: Longman. Duffy, Gerald G., Laura Roehler, Eva Sivan, Gary Rackliffe, Cassandra Book, Michael S. Meloth, Linda G. Vavurs, Roy Wesselman, Joyce Putnam, and Dina Bassiri. 1987. “Effects of Explaining the Reasoning Associated with Using Reading Strategies.” Reading Research Quarterly 22:347–368. Durkin, Dolores. 1978–1979. “What Classroom Observations Reveal about Reading Comprehension.” Reading Research Quarterly 14:518–544. National Reading Panel. 2000. Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching Children to Read. Report of the Subgroups. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Palincsar, Ann S., and Ann L. Brown. 1984. “Reciprocal Teaching of Comprehension-Fostering and Comprehension-Monitoring Activities.” Cognition and Instruction 2:117–175. Paris, Scott G., Barbara A. Wasik, and Julianne C. Turner. 1991. “The Development of Strategic Readers.” In Rebecca Barr, P. David Pearson, Michael Kamil, and Peter Mosenthal, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, pp. 609–640. New York: Longman. Pressley, Michael, and Vera Woloshyn. 1995. Cognitive Strategy Instruction That Really Improves Children’s Academic Performance. Cambridge: Brookline Books. Weinstein, Claire F., and Richard F. Mayer. 1987. “The Teaching of Learning Strategies.” In Merlin C. Wittrock, ed., Handbook of Research on Teaching, pp. 315–327. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. CAI as Teacher Some early and current versions of CAI teaching applications are based on behaviorist learning theories that rely on direct instruction and students’ mastery of a scope and sequence of performance-based learning objectives. Students are expected to work their way independently through progressively difficult levels of skills mastery until they have successfully completed and mastered all of the skills included in a course of study. This approach is based on the assumption that students will learn basic literacy skills and strategies in an on-screen instructional environment that provides them with initial instruction followed by a learning stimulus (learning task) and a performance response (feedback on correctness). Early versions of CAI teaching drill-and-practice applications were primarily print based and resembled a series of successively more difficult worksheets on the screen. Indeed, all aspects of the programs, from instruction to practice and assessment, were print based. Programs were also designed to branch or recycle students through screens that presented various levels of worksheets in response to their scores on learning tasks. For example, if a student demonstrated a below-mastery score (less than 80 percent) on a Level 5 phonics practice worksheet designed to teach initial consonant blends, the program would automatically re-present an instructional sequence or cycle of remedial lessons on initial consonants at Level 4. Programs were designed to provide additional practice and assessment until students demonstrated mastery at a targeted level. Educators could periodically check students’ progress as reported in printouts of whole-class or individual-student scores. CAI drill-and-practice teaching programs are primarily designed to serve as stand-alone programs that do not intersect with classroom instruction. Many programs are housed in a computer lab that students are scheduled to visit on a weekly or monthly basis. Therefore, the lessons are more likely to be aligned with a preprogrammed scope and sequence or with state Computer-Assisted Instruction Computer-assisted instruction (CAI) is based upon the notion that computer applications can be used in ways that support K–12 students’ learning. Educators first began to consider the role of computers and computer-assisted instruction with the advent of portable and affordable microcomputers such as the Apple II during the 1970s (Alessi and Trollip, 1991). The activities included in CAI programs have changed over the years to reflect the prevalent learning theories of different educational eras. The majority of literacy-related CAI applications involve pro88 Computer-Assisted Instruction Students using a computer in the library (Elizabeth Crews) about the /at/ family”). Special-needs students are frequently able to use computer-tutoring programs to help in their academic development. Features of elementary-grade programs are likely to include various types and levels of tutoring support in key literacy areas such as decoding, automatic word recognition, spelling, vocabulary acquisition, and comprehension (see Early Literacy Software). learning standards than with daily classroom literacy lessons. CAI as Tutor CAI tutoring applications are based on constructivist and sociocognitive learning theories that rely on features of the program to provide support for student learning (see Constructivism). The purpose of tutoring-oriented CAI applications is to furnish students with additional practice with literacy skills that have been previously taught by the teacher in the classroom. For example, many tutoring applications for primarygrade children rely on a game-playing format that involves an animated cartoon figure that appears on-screen. The figure provides information, offers prompts, or reminds students about how they may navigate through the program. Feedback on students’ choices as they play literacy-related games is offered through special multimedia effects such as music, animations, or verbal comments (e.g., “That’s wrong. Think about the first sound you hear” or “Try again, but think CAI as Tool CAI serves as an effective tool when it is used to link students with interactive resources that are available on demand. When viewed from a sociocognitive perspective, CAI applications are designed to scaffold student learning. For example, intermediate-level students may utilize CAI tool supports that involve a variety of resources such as supplemental text frames or help menus that pop up on screen. Lynn Anderson-Inman and Mark Horney (1998) examined a multimedia version of a science textbook that offered hearing-impaired students with various re89 Concept Instruction with Text References Alessi, Stephen M., and Stanley R. Trollip. 1991. Computer-Based Instruction: Methods and Development. 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Anderson-Inman, Lynn, and Mark Horney. April 1998. “Profiles of Hypertext Readers: Case Studies from the ElectroText Project.” Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the American Educational Research Association, Atlanta, GA. sources that were designed to support their comprehension. Students clicked on buttons that linked technical or concept-rich words within on-screen text passages to supplemental screen animations (e.g., the word orbit is linked to an animation of a planet orbiting around the sun), definitions, additional print-based examples, or an American Sign Language video translation of text passages. Text reader utility programs allow students who are trying to read difficult materials to download pages of novels or textbooks onto a computer screen and have the passages read aloud on demand. Students thinking about a book may enrich their perception by participating in e-mail or on-line literature discussion groups. Students’ writing skills are improved when they use resource tool features of word processing programs to compose essays. Teachers who have adequate resources and training in using computers in the classroom can bring their enthusiasm to class and create opportunities for CAI in everyday instruction for students of various literacy abilities. CAI tools are designed to complement and enrich the classroom literacy curriculum for below-average, average, and above-average students. Successful implementation is likely to depend upon the classroom teacher to integrate CAI into the overall literacy program. Successful teachers may model (demonstrate how to use the program), mentor (support students’ initial encounters with the program), manage (make sure that all students have access to the program), and monitor (make sure that students are all benefiting from the program) students’ use of applications. Computer technologies are continually becoming more sophisticated, so it is inevitable that the role of computer-assisted instruction will continue to evolve. On-screen learning environments are likely to become more complex, interactive, and responsive to students’ demonstrated literacy learning strengths and needs. CAI is also likely to demand increasingly complex learning theories, carefully crafted design interface, and robust connections to literacy instructional programs within classrooms and in extended online learning communities. Linda D. Labbo and Denise Johnson Concept Instruction with Text Concept instruction with text refers to three central aspects of instruction that foster in-depth conceptual learning of expository text (informational text). Concept instruction with text is defined as providing extensive opportunities for students to interact with multilayered knowledge, to transform meaning by manipulating information, and to experience optimal challenge during reading. Conceptual learning from text occurs when students have formed a mental representation consisting of four elements. Those elements include basic propositions about the domain (facts), relations among the propositions (facts), concepts or generalizations that broadly relate propositions to each other, and a network of concepts. Students with conceptual knowledge can use this network flexibly to solve problems or to serve as an analogy for new learning. This flexible network and all its parts constitute an explanatory understanding of the domain (a network of interrelated concepts that serve as a critical component in a discipline of knowledge). For example, when reading an expository text about an ecological science theme, it is important for students to distinguish among various features of an animal (e.g., the beak or the foot of a bird [propositional level]). It is equally important for students to understand how the beak and the foot are related (both aid in feeding [relational knowledge]) and how a concept such as feeding (concept level) relates to other ecological principles, such as defense, predation, or reproduction (network of concepts). To acquire a domain of richly elaborated knowledge, students must encounter and interact with all these levels of knowledge. In concept instruction with text, teachers rely on texts that contain all levels of knowledge (e.g., propositional, relational, and conceptual levels). Students read, discuss, and write about such texts See Also Constructivism; Software for Older Readers 90 Concept Instruction with Text in a setting in which this material is relevant and useful. Some examples of this sort of context might be: a thematic unit, a project, or a set of student-generated learning goals that establish a role for the text as an information source. In content areas such as science and history, learning from text is accelerated by “hands-on” experiences or vivid activities, such as historical enactments. These experiences benefit concept learning by providing concrete referents for the basic propositions (they see the bird’s beak up close) and by creating opportunity for spontaneous questioning (“Why are some beaks so curved?”). Such interactions foster the process of building multitiered knowledge. Concept instruction with text is designed to increase this kind of knowledge by giving students opportunities to manipulate information and transform their knowledge. There is evidence that when students create new representations of text, such as concept mapping, constructing projects, building models, or drawing graphical representations, they rely on deep structural knowledge of a domain. Thus, they build knowledge structures to represent domain principles. For example, through drawing, readers can induce and generate a new and coherent representation of text because it directs their attention to specific concepts in the text passage. Finally, concept instruction with text enables students to experience optimal challenge during reading. Optimal challenge in reading refers to the alignment of reading skill with appropriately difficult text. Motivation theorists posit that when challenges are slightly ahead of skills, a tension is created that stimulates concentration and effort (see Literacy Motivation). Optimal challenge is a function of prior knowledge and competence as well as task difficulty (see Prior Knowledge and Misconceptions). For instance, when students of high competence and prior knowledge are confronted with a difficult task or text, they are likely to experience optimal difficulty. However, when students of low competence and prior knowledge face a difficult task, they will likely report frustration. Optimally challenging reading events will be those that are at a student’s intermediate level of difficulty. That is, they are neither too easy nor too hard. Optimally challenging reading activities heighten conceptual learning from text because students learn to be devoted to deep thinking and concerted atten- tion. When students can meet increasingly difficult goals, they see concrete evidence of their growth. This evidence, in turn, increases their perceptions of competence and willingness to persist in the face of difficulty. Further, optimal challenge supports the positive relationship between competence and task value. If students feel competent in completing a challenging reading task, they will also be interested in reading and find reading to be important and useful. Supporting Competence for Learning from Text Concept instruction can be utilized in multiple domains, including social studies/history or science. For instance, the multiple tiers of conceptual knowledge in history include political themes such as conflict (and diverse viewpoints about those themes); evidence of the conflict in the form of protests or rebellion; particular events such as the Boston Massacre; and features of the events, such as persons or their actions. Exposing students to variegated texts with these tiers, expecting students to transform the information, and identifying optimally challenging tasks to perform with these texts is concept instruction with text in history. For example, Bruce VanSledright and Christine Kelly (1998) examined the reading practices of fifth graders who were studying the Boston Massacre. The students, who were asked whether they thought it was important to use more than one book when studying a colony, realized that multiple texts would have different information, which would be important in understanding the Boston Massacre. Evaluating the credibility, bias, and political persuasion of the historical writer is a prominent strategy in history reading. The students’ interest in text was related to their motivation to understand the historical time period and its conflicts. Multiple texts increase the likelihood that texts will be optimally challenging and that students will cognitively engage in building a critical understanding. Several programs have also used conceptual instruction in science with beneficial results on reading engagement and conceptual learning from text. John Guthrie and his colleagues (1998) implemented a classroom intervention called concept-oriented reading instruction (CORI) to emphasize conceptual instruction in reading and science. CORI teachers were trained 91 Concept Instruction with Text to provide multilayered instruction, knowledgetransformation activities, and optimal challenge during an integrated reading/science unit. CORI teachers used conceptual themes to organize central disciplinary principles in a multilayered fashion. The conceptual theme was accessible to all students and allowed for an ebb and flow between the facts and principles of the domain. Using the theme “birds around the world,” teachers helped their students to embrace nine ecological principles (such as defense and predation). CORI teachers enabled students to search through multiple trade books to integrate information about the theme. After reading, students often summarized, made graphic organizers (student construction of a spatial representation of text-based knowledge, such as a concept map or Venn diagram) (see Graphic Organizers), drew and labeled illustrations of the text information, and created models and artifacts based on their new understandings gleaned from the multiple texts. In terms of optimal challenge, CORI teachers used a wide array of interesting texts to accommodate a range of ability levels in order to ensure students worked at the edge of their competency. In a typical CORI classroom, students conduct science activities within a conceptual study theme. In the midst of a conceptual theme on aquatic life, a science activity might be to visit a freshwater habitat for students to collect pond water and specimens. Students would then ask personal questions about the animals and plants they observed. Next, they would search through multiple texts to find the answers to their questions, and they would choose from an abundance of books ranging in level of difficulty. For example, students may have begun with an easy text when the topic was new and knowledge relatively fragmented. As students gain knowledge, they become increasingly able to read and gain information from more challenging texts. Students use multiple knowledge-transformation activities to learn from the text. This includes concept mapping, illustrating and labeling text ideas, or conducting experiments based on text information. Finally, students present a display of their knowledge to classmates. This, too, is accomplished using a variety of knowledge-transformation activities, ranging from poster presentations to the creation of artifacts. In several quantitative studies of CORI, John Guthrie and his associates have documented the benefits of concept instruction on conceptual learning from text, reading strategy use, and reading motivation (Guthrie and Cox, 2001). Among other classroom intervention programs that have also emphasized concept instruction with text, Marlene Scardamalia and her colleagues (1994) implemented a classroom intervention called Computer Supported Intentional Learning Environments (CSILE). CSILE classrooms contained networked computers connected to a communal database. During a typical day in a CSILE classroom, students researched topics using the computers for thirty minutes per day. They browsed through experts’ and classmates’ notes and information, attached notes and graphics found in databases, and recorded information found through other avenues. Students simultaneously used multiple text sources to gather information (see Multiple Texts). Personal inquiries were posted in the database to which other students responded; thus, an ongoing communication among students provided the impetus for knowledge growth. A series of studies reported the effects of CSILE on students’ ability to construct knowledge from multiple texts and other sources. For instance, CSILE students exhibited their ability to represent knowledge in multiple forms, including graphics, and to better comprehend expository text (see Narrative and Expository Text). One study analyzed students’ cognitive actions in order to examine whether student usage of the computer system resulted in differential conceptual learning from text (Oshima, Scardamalia, and Bereiter, 1996). Indeed, students who treated information flow from computer to self as a unidirectional exchange learned relatively few principles and higher-order relations. In contrast, students who sought to construct meaning in a bidirectional interchange of textual information with other students and sources gained higher levels of knowledge. These students questioned and rebutted information and acted as co-creators of the knowledge. In addition, high conceptual learners took notes that were coordinated with the principles of the domain, whereas low conceptual learners wrote many fragmented notes. Ann Brown (1997) designed a curriculum called Fostering Communities of Learners (FCL), to be used in second- to fifth-grade sci92 Conceptual Change Learning and Texts ence classrooms. Her general philosophy was that students develop their knowledge through dialogue in a social learning community. Students were expected to research some subset of a topic and produce an artifact based on the content. One study compared three groups of students with regard to conceptual learning outcomes. One group received instruction characterized by a jigsaw approach, in which teams of students studying various subthemes of a topic shared their subset of expertise with classmates so that all students could integrate subtheme information with the overall conceptual theme and learn the subthemes of a conceptual unit. During various phases of learning, students were involved in three participant structures: composing on the computer, conducting research using multiple texts, and interacting with the teacher. In these structures, jigsaw groups worked simultaneously on the subtopics of a conceptual theme. Students gathered and presented findings to each other and engaged in asking questions of peers and clarifying concepts. A summary of the FCL studies showed that students gained deep-level understandings about the scientific topics of study as expressed in problem solving by analogy tasks. Some educators advocate teaching for text recall in content areas. They place a high premium on recall of important facts in domains such as history or science. The subject matter is thought to compose a fixed structure of many facts and concepts that are to be learned and remembered. Amount of factual recall is viewed as a good test of amount of understanding. This view of knowledge acquisition is thought to be encouraged with a read-and-reproduce model. Although one goal of concept instruction with text is to assure propositional recall, an equally important goal is to impart principled understanding. In contrast to instruction that emphasizes accuracy and static “possession” of text information, knowledge in a concept instruction classroom is viewed as multilayered and dynamic. Taken together, the CORI, CSILE, and FCL studies show the powerful effects of a conceptual emphasis in instruction. In each of these programs, students were given multiple opportunities to create relations between the facts and principles of the conceptual domain, to experience optimal challenge, and to manipulate information in order to transform meaning. It has been shown that concept instruction helps stu- dents understand that there are multiple, often rival, viewpoints within a domain of knowledge. Students should learn to create their personal understandings based on text and to reconcile discrepancies among diverse texts and their own knowledge. Thus, searching for information in multiple trade books or original documents, being presented with diverse viewpoints, and manipulating incoming information into a variety of forms is instrumental. Kathleen E. Cox and John Guthrie See Also Graphic Organizers; Literacy Motivation; Multiple Texts; Narrative and Expository Text; Prior Knowledge and Misconceptions References Brown, Ann. 1997. “Transforming Schools into Communities of Thinking and Learning about Serious Matters.” American Psychologist 52 (3):399–413. Guthrie, John, and Kathleen Cox. 2001. “Classroom Conditions for Motivation and Engagement in Reading.” Educational Psychology Review 13 (3):283–302. Guthrie, John, P. Van Meter, G. Hancock, S. Alao, E. Anderson, and A. McCann. 1998. “Does Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction Increase Strategy Use and Conceptual Learning from Text?” Journal of Educational Psychology 90 (2):261–278. Oshima, J., Marlene Scardamalia, and Carl Bereiter. 1996. “Collaborative Learning Processes Associated with High and Low Conceptual Progress.” Instructional Science 24:125–155. Scardamalia, Marlene, Carl Bereiter, and Mary Lamon. 1994. “The CSILE Project: Trying to Bridge the Classroom into World 3.” In Kate McGilly, ed., Classroom Lessons: Integrating Cognitive Theory and Classroom Practice, pp. 201–228. Cambridge: MIT Press. VanSledright, Bruce, and Christine Kelly. 1998. “Reading American History: The Influence of Using Multiple Sources on Six Fifth Graders.” Elementary School Journal 98:239–265. Conceptual Change Learning and Texts Conceptual change learning (CCL) involves students in perceiving the inconsistency between their preconceptions and a new conception to be learned. To learn the new conception, students may need to reorganize or replace their incom93 Conceptual Change Learning and Texts plete knowledge structure. Conceptual change text refers to a type of text that is designed to help students to see the conflict between their preconceptions and the new conception by providing scientific explanations of a natural phenomenon. Such text includes the refutational text and text that is integrated in computer simulations to facilitate conceptual change. Despite the variations in forms, conceptual change text explicitly contrasts or challenges intuitive understandings of natural phenomena with scientifically accepted theories. Conceptual change text is designed in accordance with the four necessary conditions (i.e., dissatisfaction, intelligibility, plausibility, and fruitfulness) specified in a theoretical model of conceptual change (Posner et al., 1982). George Posner and his colleagues argued that conceptual change is based on the conditions that students should feel dissatisfied with their preconceptions, that they should understand the new scientific conception, that the new conception should solve problems that old concepts cannot solve, and that students should have an opportunity to apply the newly learned conception to a different situation. tional goals such as self-efficacy and levels of interest, are related to the depth of text processing in conceptual change learning. Conceptual Change Text Research during the 1980s During the 1980s, researchers in reading education examined effects for a review of different texts on conceptual change (see Guzzetti et al., 1993, for a review). Research on conceptual change texts established that refutational text, in general, has a positive effect on eradicating students’ misconceptions. Specifically, its effectiveness is shown when it is combined with strategies like demonstration and the Discussion Web, which directly challenge students’ misconceptions. Two variations of refutational text are: refutational considerate expository text and considerate soft expository text. Refutational considerate expository text enables students to gather appropriate information with minimal effort, whereas considerate soft expository text is a hybrid text that combines narrative with expository structures. Both refutational considerate expository text and considerate soft expository text have also been found to be effective in facilitating students’ conceptual change. As Guzzetti and her colleagues (1993) pointed out, researchers in reading education have been more interested than researchers in science education in exploring alternative types of text and text-based strategies in their misconception studies. That is why the studies investigating the effects of different types of text were conducted by researchers in reading education. Conceptual Change Learning Research Conceptual change learning has been a focus of research in reading and science education since the 1980s. The theory that undergirds conceptual change learning was originally developed using Jean Piaget’s cognitive model and schema theory. Researchers since the early 1990s have begun to acknowledge the important role played by students’ motivational goals, their epistemological beliefs, and various social factors in conceptual change learning. Using a Vygotskian perspective, some researchers have stressed that conceptual change learning is socially constructed. Conceptual change learning in science involves both personal and social processes rather than a purely cognitive process that can be stimulated simply by challenging students’ commonsense conceptions through discrepant events. Students’ commonsense conceptions should be examined in different social contexts. Other researchers have emphasized the importance of students’ epistemological beliefs and motivational goals in their levels of cognitive engagement in classroom academic tasks, particularly in their willingness to persist at these tasks. Epistemological beliefs, together with motiva- Conceptual Change Text Research during the 1990s Since the 1990s, new focuses have emerged in research on conceptual change texts. Researchers have started to examine text structures that are designed for conceptual change and that investigate students’ responses to the different kinds of texts used in the process of conceptual change learning. In addition, a few researchers have begun to use computer simulations that integrate texts to help students to make conceptual change. A large body of research on conceptual change learning (Chambers and Andre, 1997; Hynd, Alvermann, and Qian, 1997; see also Guzzetti et al., 1993, for a review) has documented the effec94 Conceptual Change Learning and Texts tiveness of refutational text. Researchers have become interested in examining students’ responses to various kinds of refutational texts (e.g., expository versus narrative refutational texts) when they are engaged in conceptual change learning (Guzzetti et al., 1997); researchers are also evaluating the quality of texts in science textbooks on the complex concepts that require conceptual change (Shiland, 1997). Barbara Guzzetti and her research team (1997) involved high-school students taking science courses in discussing their preferences for and reactions to the different kinds of text structures, also asking them to comment on which texts were more credible or helpful in learning the science concept and to recommend how physics texts be written to be most helpful and effective. In conceptual change learning, students preferred refutational text in general and refutational expository text in particular. Students liked refutational text because that sort of text was easier to understand, enabled them to see why their naive conceptions were at variance with the scientific conceptions, and helped them to effectively learn the scientific concept. Students reported that they did not use science textbooks much to learn science concepts. In contrast, they learned the science concepts through experiments and labs or hands-on activities and from other sources by studying their notes and using rote memory. Students with different kinds of prior knowledge about the concept to be learned responded differently to the refutational text. Students who demonstrated a certain amount of prior knowledge but were not familiar with the terms and concepts found that refutational text was helpful in providing them with new concepts and terminology, allowing them to better understand explanations of counterintuitive ideas. However, students who had no prior knowledge of related concepts found that supplementing refutational text with teacher discussion was particularly helpful. Students also offered their insight into revising textbooks. They recommended that the refutational section in the text be highlighted to alert students to the important information and avoid confusion. Students complained that they were confused by ambiguous statements in the text. Therefore, they suggested that refutational text should directly and unambiguously refute the wrong, unscientific conceptions. Students’ criticism of expository text commonly found in textbooks included using extraneous, unfamiliar concepts, citing complex examples rather than simple ones, and describing in imprecise language. Thomas Shiland (1997) applied the theoretical model of conceptual change offered by George Posner and his colleagues (1982) to evaluate the quality of textbook materials. By operationalizing the four conditions (i.e., dissatisfaction, intelligibility, plausibility, and fruitfulness), Shiland examined six modern secondary chemistry textbooks on quantum mechanics. He found that the texts did not provide a sufficient basis for students to accept the quantum mechanical model over the Bohr model because none of the conditions of dissatisfaction, intelligibility, plausibility, and fruitfulness were met. The limitations of the Bohr model were not adequately discussed in the texts. The quantum model was not intelligibly presented. The texts did not clearly show how the quantum mechanical model can correct the shortcomings of the Bohr model. Students did not have ample opportunity to apply the quantum model in problem-solving situations. Since the 1990s, several studies (Biemans and Simons, 1995, 1996; Carlsen and Andre, 1992) have focused on the role of the computer simulation in conceptual change learning. Some researchers (Carlsen and Andre, 1992) were interested in combining a computer simulation with refutational text, while others (Biemans and Simons, 1995, 1996) examined the effects of a computer simulation integrated with text on conceptual change learning. These researchers believed that a computer simulation allowed students to actively test out their preconceptions and that an active test was more effective for students than reading refutational text. Although the computer simulation helped college students overcome misconceptions about electric circuits and acquire a more developmentally advanced model of series circuits, its combined effect with refutational text and its expected advantage over refutational text was not found (Carlsen and Andre, 1992). The nonsignificant result might be related to the technical difficulties in using the simulation. Based on the four conditions of conceptual change, Karen Sheila Ali (cited in Biemans and Simons 1995, 1996) developed a computer simulation model called CONTACT strategy 95 Conceptual Change Learning and Texts (CONTinuousACTivation) to facilitate conceptual change. Ali’s CONTACT strategy consists of five steps that include searching for preconceptions, comparing and contrasting preconceptions with the new information, formulating new conceptions, applying new conceptions, and evaluating the new conceptions. The activity of “studying the text” is integrated after searching for one’s own preconceptions or after evaluating the new conceptions. Students are required to compare and contrast their preconceptions with the scientific information after they study the text. Feedback is given to ensure that students learn the correct conceptions. Finally, students have to evaluate the new conception by comparing it with their answer to the practice question. Students have to state whether both answers are in concordance with each other or not. If not, the students have to study the most important part of the text again. This procedure has to be followed until students learn the correct conception. Biemans and Simons (1996) have revised the CONTACT strategy in order to increase the efficiency and flexibility of the strategy and solve the problem of selective attention. Two major adaptations are related to the text used in the strategy. One adaptation concerns the use of visual presentation of the concept. When students are engaged in searching for their own preconceptions, formulating new conceptions, or applying the new conceptions, they can ask for a corresponding picture. Both text information and visual presentation are used to optimize the students’ opportunities to activate their conceptions. The other adaptation concerns highlighting the most important information in the text. When students are comparing and contrasting the preconditions with the new information and evaluating the new conceptions, crucial concepts are accentuated in different colors to focus the students’ attention on essential elements in the text. Additional adaptations have been made to spread students’ attention by stressing the comprehension of the whole text in the training process, accentuating all the important information from the text, optimizing scrolling options so that students can search for information on other text screens, and reducing procedural steps so that students can pay more attention to the text itself. The studies by Biemans and Simons (1995, 1996) involved fifth- and sixth- grade students in conceptual change learning. They found that the original and revised CONTACT strategies had different effects on conceptual change learning among students who had different degrees of familiarity with the subject matter. Research on text with computer simulations has demonstrated that integration of text into computer simulations yields some encouraging results in conceptual change learning as compared to the use of refutational text as a separate strategy or discrete part of the computer simulation. Refutational text as an integral part of the computer simulation appears to have some advantages over the traditional use of refutational text. Researchers could easily accentuate the important information, optimize the scrolling options that help students search for information in other text screens, and present in multiple ways the scientific conception in both textual and visual modes. Gaoyin Qian See Also Narrative and Expository Text; Prior Knowledge and Misconceptions; Prior-Knowledge Assessment; Refutational Texts References Alvermann, Donna E., and Cynthia R. Hynd. December 1989. “The Influence of Discussion and Text on the Learning of Counterintuitive Science Concepts.” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Reading Conference, Austin, TX. Biemans, Harm J. A., and P. Robert-Jan Simons. 1995. “How to Use Preconceptions? The Contact Strategy Dismantled.” European Journal of Psychology of Education 10:243–259. ———. 1996. “Contact 2: A Computer-Assisted Instructional Strategy for Promoting Conceptual Change.” Instructional Science 10:57–176. Carlsen, David D., and Thomas Andre. 1992. “Use of a Microcomputer Simulation and Conceptual Change Text to Overcome Student Preconceptions about Electric Circuits.” Journal of ComputerBased Instruction 19:105–109. Chambers, Sharon K., and Thomas Andre. 1997. “Gender, Prior Knowledge, Interest, and Experience in Electricity and Conceptual Change Text Manipulations in Learning about Direct Current.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34:107–123. Guzzetti, Barbara J., Tonja E. Snyder, Gene V. Glass, and Warren S. Gamas. 1993. “Promoting Conceptual Change in Science: A Comparative Meta-Analysis of Instructional Interventions from 96 Considerate Text learning. Two features of text that help readers make such connections are coherence and audience appropriateness. Reading Education and Science Education.” Reading Research Quarterly 28:117–159. Guzzetti, Barbara J., Wayne O. Williams, Stephanie A. Skeels, and Shwu Ming Wu. 1997. “Influence of Text Structure on Learning Counterintuitive Physics Concepts.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34:701–719. Hynd, Cynthia, Donna Alvermann, and Gaoyin Qian. 1997. “Preservice Elementary School Teachers’ Conceptual Change about Projectile Motion: Refutation Text.” Science Education 81:1–27. Pintrich, Paul R., Ronald W. Marx, and Robert A. Boyle. 1993. “Beyond Cold Conceptual Change: The Role of Motivational Belief and Classroom Contextual Factors in the Process of Conceptual Change.” Review of Educational Research 63:167–199. Posner, George J., Kenneth A. Strike, Peter W. Hewson, and William A. Gertzog. 1982. “Accommodation of a Scientific Conception: Toward a Theory of Conceptual Change.” Science Education 66:211–227. Shiland, Thomas W. 1997. “Quantum Mechanics and Conceptual Change in High School Chemistry Textbooks.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34:535–545. Coherence The more coherent the text is, the more the reader will be able to make internal connections and construct a coherent cognitive model of the information in the text. Texts cohere both globally and locally. Global coherence refers to text characteristics that facilitate the integration of high-level, important ideas across the entire text. Local coherence refers to several kinds of links or ties that connect ideas together within and between sentences. Global coherence is achieved by the overall structure or organization of the text. Structure refers to the system of arrangement of ideas in a text and the nature of the relationships connecting the ideas. A few basic text structures found in informational text include simple listing, compare and contrast, temporal sequence, cause and effect, and problem and solution. Research has shown that the better organized the text, the greater the learning. Also, the reader’s ability to identify and use the organization of the text enhances learning from reading. Therefore, a considerate text has a clear, easily identifiable organization. Clear text organization can be accomplished in several ways, for example, through the use of headings and subheadings, introductions and summaries, topic sentences, and signal words and phrases that announce the text structure. In considerate text, headings and subheadings provide information about upcoming content and how it is organized. For example, “Problems of the New Government” suggests a problem and solution structure, whereas “Distinguishing Poisonous from Nonpoisonous Snakes” indicates a compare and contrast structure. In considerate text, introductions and summaries provide clear, succinct information about the content and organization of an extended text segment. For example, a chapter introduction may announce a cause and effect structure by beginning: “This chapter describes how glaciers form and how they change the shape of the land . . .” Topic sentences can provide similar information for a single paragraph. The paragraph beginning with the topic sentence “There were four presidential candidates in the election of Considerate Text Considerate, or friendly, text is readable, understandable, and memorable. The concept was developed in the early 1980s (Armbruster, 1984; Kantor, Anderson, and Armbruster, 1983) in response to the perception that some informational texts used in classrooms, especially content-area textbooks, were too difficult for students to read, understand, and remember. Considerate text is characterized by features such as coherence and audience appropriateness, which were suggested by cognitive theory and research to facilitate learning from reading. The notion of considerateness thus goes beyond readability, as measured by readability formulas. The concept of considerate text can be helpful in evaluating, revising, and writing informational text. According to cognitive theory, readers form a representation of what is read by making internal connections (connections among information within the text) as well as external connections (connections between text information and the reader’s prior knowledge and experience). Texts that help readers make internal and external connections enhance comprehension and 97 Considerate Text 1824” tells the reader to expect the paragraph to contain a simple listing of the four candidates, presumably including some descriptive information about each. Certain words and phrases also signal, or announce, the structure of considerate text. Most text structures have related signal words and phrases. For example, “because,” “therefore,” “as a result,” and “consequently” signify a cause and effect structure, whereas “similarly,”“both . . . and,” “in contrast,” and “on the other hand” denote a compare and contrast structure. A text is considerate if these signal words and phrases are used explicitly rather than left for the reader to infer. In addition to global coherence, considerate texts also have local coherence. Local coherence is achieved by means of several kinds of cohesive ties that help carry meaning across phrase, clause, and sentence boundaries. Examples of common cohesive ties include pronoun reference, or the use of a pronoun to refer to a previously mentioned noun (e.g., “The photocopy machine is down now. It will be repaired tomorrow.”); substitution, or the replacement of a word or words for a previously mentioned noun phrase, verb phrase, or clause (e.g., “The starlet, her male companion, and a cadre of reporters entered the hall. The party proceeded to the ballroom.”); and conjunctions or connectives (e.g., “Insect sounds are used to warn of danger or to woo a mate.”). A rather large body of research has established the importance of cohesive ties in understanding and remembering text. In addition to explicit and unambiguous pronoun references, substitutions, and conjunctions, considerate text also contains a clear, logical flow of information and transition statements that help the reader move easily from topic to topic. In short, considerate text helps readers make internal connections among textual ideas, which is necessary for forming a coherent representation of the text’s meaning. A considerate text describes and explains information adequately for the target audience. A text is considerate if the author has explained the material using content, vocabulary, and language structure at a level appropriate to the reader’s background knowledge. If the text contains too much explanation, the reader may become bored; if too little, the reader will not understand. Unfortunately, some content-area textbooks present topics in such a superficial manner that readers often fail to grasp the concept or follow the explanation. Or the textbooks overwhelm the reader with too much new information, such as too many new vocabulary words. If the information load is too great, readers can become frustrated and abandon the task of trying to make sense of the text. In short, there must be an adequate match between what the reader knows before reading and what the author chooses to write down. Considerate texts also engage the reader. Readers must be actively engaged in the reading process to comprehend and learn. Of course, other factors besides the text influence the reader’s engagement, such as the reader’s interest in the topic and the situation in which the text is read (for example, whether it is self-selected and read for pleasure or assigned by a teacher). Aspects of the text, however, can affect the reader’s attention to and engagement in reading. Two aspects that have been investigated in research are interestingness and voice. Interesting features of text include inherently interesting topics, novelty, unexpectedness, character identification, lively anecdotes, fast action, and concrete and vivid detail. Research has shown that such intuitively appealing features of a text will not necessarily promote comprehension (e.g., Graves et al., 1991; Garner et al., 1991). For example, readers may recall more interesting details rather than less interesting important information. A related aspect of text that might affect engagement is voice. Voice refers to the qualities of text that help it speak to the reader or form a relationship between the author and the reader. For example, authors may add voice to text by using language that resembles oral language, by making events seem more real or immediate, or by describing the emotional reactions of the people in the text. Research has shown that comprehension can be enhanced by text that exhibits Audience Appropriateness In addition to making internal connections, readers must also make external connections between the information in the text and their background knowledge and experience. Considerate text is appropriate to the needs of the reading audience in that it provides adequate explanation and elaboration of information and engages the reader. 98 Constructivism Textbooks.” Remedial and Special Education 9:47–52. Beck, Isabel L., Margaret G. McKeown, and Jo Worthy. 1995. “Giving a Text Voice Can Improve Students’ Understanding.” Reading Research Quarterly 30 (2):220–238. Garner, Ruth, Patricia A. Alexander, Mark G. Gillingham, Jonna M. Kulikowich, and Rachel Brown. 1991. “Interest and Learning from Text.” American Educational Research Journal 28 (3):643–659. Graves, Michael F., Maureen C. Prenn, Jason Earle, Marty Thompson, Vivian Johnson, and Wayne H. Slater. 1991. “Commentary: Improving Instructional Text: Some Lessons Learned.” Reading Research Quarterly 26 (2):110–122. Kantor, Robbie N., Thomas H. Anderson, and Bonnie B. Armbruster. 1983. “How Are Children’s Textbooks Inconsiderate? Or, of Flyswatters and Alfa.” Journal of Curriculum Studies 15 (1):61–72. both coherence and voice; voice without coherence does not seem to enhance comprehension (Beck, McKeown, and Worthy, 1995). In sum, considerate text is appropriate to the reading audience for which it is intended in that it provides adequate explanation and elaboration of information and is able to engage the reader, perhaps by providing interest or personal connection to the reader. Beyond Readability Although readability is part of the concept of considerate text, the concept of considerateness goes beyond the traditional notion of the “readability” or difficulty of a text. Readability is typically measured by readability formulas, which are mathematical methods of estimating the grade level for which a text is suited. Readability formulas typically involve measures of word familiarity or difficulty and sentence length. Although the familiarity or difficulty of words and the length of sentences do contribute to the readability of a text, they are not the only, or even the most important, factors that affect readability. Constructivism The term constructivism refers to a philosophical stance that emphasizes the generative, dynamic nature of communication and of other intellectual and social processes. Nancy Spivey Nelson (1997) defines constructivism as a theoretical metaphor that likens meaning-making to acts of construction, or acts of building, in which humans are the constructors, meaning or knowledge is their construction, and prior knowledge and experience are the constructive materials that are used. With respect to literacy, constructivists maintain that meaning is not located in texts and is not “taken” from texts by readers. Instead, meanings for texts are generated by readers in response to textual cues provided by writers, who had themselves engaged in the generative process of meaning construction that is known as composing. Since reading and writing are both active, creative processes, they have similarities, and one is not the inverse of the other. Scholarly work taking a constructivist perspective began to flourish in the late 1970s, although many of the principal tenets were articulated earlier during the nineteenth century by such philosophers as Immanuel Kant, Giambattista Vico, and Friedrich Hegel. The empirical and theoretical work from the twentieth century, which continues on into the twenty-first century, has tended to cut across disciplinary boundaries, coming from education, psychology, artificial in- Usefulness of the Concept of Considerate Text The concept of considerate text can be helpful to educators, authors, and editors. Educators may wish to consider the features of considerate text when evaluating instructional materials. For example, textbook adoption committees may evaluate the relative considerateness of the textbooks they are considering (Armbruster and Anderson, 1988). Teachers may use the concept of considerate text to shed light on some of the reading comprehension problems of their students or to plan instruction that helps students read and understand inconsiderate text. Authors may use the concept to help them write considerate text, and editors may benefit from the notion in revising text. Bonnie B. Armbruster See Also Readability; Textbooks References Armbruster, Bonnie B. 1984. “The Problem of ‘Inconsiderate Text.’” In Gerald G. Duffy, Laura R. Roehler, and Jana Mason, eds., Comprehension Instruction: Perspectives and Suggestions, pp. 202–217. New York: Longman. Armbruster, Bonnie B., and Thomas H. Anderson. 1988. “On Selecting ‘Considerate’ Content Area 99 Constructivism telligence, linguistics, sociology, anthropology, and English studies. There are various forms of constructivism, and they can be differentiated, in part, by the notion of agency that they present: whether the constructive agent is portrayed as an individual, as a small group or pair of people, or as a large society or community. The various forms of constructivism to be discussed here, regardless of the nature of the constructive agent, are those that have had the most significant impact on literacy research and education. A constructivist orientation has also guided research into cognitive aspects of composition. The focus for composition, as for reading, has been on operations of organizing, selecting, and connecting. For years, educators and scholars in composition had attended to the nature of the written product, but constructivists brought new attention to the mental product that is created when writers compose. In considering the nature of the mental product—meaning—they pointed out interesting and recursive changes that transpire over the course of composing. In accordance with these perspectives, writing instruction began to give greater attention to the process of composition. Besides this large body of work dealing with comprehension and composition, constructivist scholarship has dealt with the nature of cognition. Some of this work has been based on Piagetian cognitive-developmental constructivism, which focuses on, among other things, people’s ability to take the perspective of others. Other research has been grounded in George Kelly’s (1955) personal construct theory, which addresses the mental categories through which individuals perceive experiences and other people. Cognitive Constructivism In cognitive constructivism, the individual is the primary agent for construction of knowledge. As the constructive agent, a person brings his or her own background, knowledge, and experience to the act of composing or comprehending and draws on them in organizing, selecting, and connecting mental material cued by the text. This constructivist perspective is shaped largely by the works of Frederic Bartlett, a British psychologist who published Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology in 1932. He examined the meaning-making processes of individuals through a series of studies that were to influence constructivist research in future decades. The best-known study in this collection is his work with the story “The War of the Ghosts,” in which Bartlett analyzed the recall processes of readers and theorized about their meaning-making processes. Four decades after Bartlett’s book was published, researchers began to follow Bartlett’s lead and appropriated aspects of his approach: considering people’s prior knowledge (of the topic, of the discourse pattern, of the genre, and so on) and analyzing the kinds of selections and additions they make when integrating their textual understandings with their own worldviews or putting them to their own uses. From the late 1970s into the 1990s, numerous constructivist studies focused on the following topics: knowledge frameworks (e.g., schemas, scripts); text organization, particularly stories but also expository patterns; and the types of inferences, or additions, that people make when they read. Literacy educators took these new understandings about reading and developed strategies for enhancing and monitoring comprehension through active reading processes that drew from students’ prior experiences. Social Constructivism In recent years there has been a growing interest in the social aspects of meaning-making (Bruffee, 1984), in which groups of people serve as collaborative constructive agents. Some groups, which tend to be relatively small, are composed of people who interact directly with one another, such as students in a classroom, members of a book discussion group, or participants in an online listserver. The group engages in shared literary practices, such as discussing a text or authoring a story, and, in doing so, constructs knowledge that is shared—knowledge that can be said to be socially constructed. When working together, group members follow spoken or unspoken discourse conventions. Much of the work taking this perspective on literate practices is grounded in the Vygotskian view that intrapsychological development derives from interpsychological interactions with others. These new perspectives on learning and comprehension altered literacy instruction, as the nature of classrooms became more collaborative than didactic. However, larger discourse communities or even societies may also be seen as constructive 100 Content-Area Literacy agents, collectively building knowledge. Social constructivism taking this more macro perspective is sometimes labeled constructionism. Communication in large collectives, whose members do not necessarily come in direct contact or even know one another, often takes place through written discourse. An example would be a disciplinary discourse community, communicating largely through its journals and books and sharing its knowledge through those forums (Porter, 1986). Potential scholarly contributions go through a review, or gatekeeping, process and are judged socially in accordance with community norms before they are considered “knowledge.” Even in large communities, individual members still identify with one another in terms of shared values, practices, and language, and individuals are connected through common goals and interests. Deborah J. Davis texts, represent sets of potential meanings for students who study a subject. To be literate in a given subject area, students must use reading, writing, and speech to construct meaning as they engage in text-related learning. The ability to use literacy to learn, however, varies from subject area to subject area. Content-area literacy is always situational. A variety of learner-related, text-related, and classroom-related factors influence content literacy in a given subject area. Some of these situational factors include the learner’s prior knowledge of, attitude toward, and interest in the subject; the learner’s purpose for reading and writing; the language and conceptual difficulty of the instructional material; the way ideas are organized in text; the assumptions authors make about their audience of readers; and the beliefs and attitudes teachers have about the use of texts in their instructional routines. Cognitive and metacognitive principles undergird many of the content-area literacy practices evident in classrooms today. Cognitive processes permit students to think with texts. Metacognitive processes, in a similar vein, allow students to be aware of their own cognitive processes as they read and to engage in self-monitoring activity during meaning construction. Teachers who hold constructivist beliefs recognize that students learn with text, not necessarily from texts. The expression “learning from text” suggests that the flow of meaning is from text to reader. Learning with text, however, implies that a transaction takes place between the reader and the text rather than the transmission of knowledge from text to reader. Within a constructivist framework, students have much to contribute to their own learning as they negotiate meaning and socially construct knowledge through learning situations that require reading, writing, and discussion. Content-area literacy requires that learning be strategic. Strategic readers and writers are not only knowledgeable about their own reading and writing processes but also in control of reading and writing activities. Strategic learners develop and use a repertoire of strategies to make sense of text during reading and writing. They know what, how, when, and why it is important to monitor what they are reading and writing and to regulate their use of comprehension and composing strategies. As a result, content-area literacy practices depend on the teacher’s ability to See Also Social Constructivism References Bartlett, Frederic C. 1932. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bruffee, Kenneth A. 1984. “Collaborative Learning and the Conversation of Mankind. College English 46:635–662. Kelly, George A. 1955. The Psychology of Personal Constructs. New York: Norton. Nelson, Nancy Spivey. 1997. The Constructivist Metaphor: Reading, Writing, and the Making of Meaning. San Diego: Academic Press. Porter, James E. 1986. “Intertextuality and the Discourse Community.” Rhetoric Review 5:34–47. Content-Area Literacy Content-area literacy reflects the ability to use reading, writing, and discussion to learn in a given subject area. It is often defined as the level of reading, writing, and discussion skill that learners need in an academic subject to comprehend and respond to ideas in texts used for instructional purposes. In today’s content-area classrooms, instructional materials are typically fixed in typesetter’s ink on printed pages or appear on a computer screen in an electronic environment. More often than not, students encounter these texts as reading assignments made from textbooks, the Internet, CD-ROMs, trade books, magazines, newspapers, reference materials, and the like. Instructional materials, like all 101 Content-Area Literacy scaffold instruction. Scaffolded instruction allows content-area teachers to provide the level of support that students need to develop and use reading and writing strategies to learn subject matter. of content-area reading practices. The irony behind their resistance toward reading is that content-area teachers genuinely value the role that reading plays in learning but fail to attend to reading in their own practices. The RICA movement has experienced a major paradigmatic shift since Gray’s time. From the early 1900s through the 1960s, the predominant paradigm was skills based. Early on in the RICA movement, reading researchers were as interested in the identification of reading and study skills associated with each of the content areas as they were in the effects of various instructional variables on the acquisition of reading/study skills and learning in content areas. As a result, a popular practice in the 1920s and 1930s was to list specific reading and study skills unique to a given content area. Some researchers compared students’ performance on general tests of reading with performance on content-specific achievement tests. Typically, they found that the ability to read generally is related to the ability to read in a given content area, but not perfectly. As a result, they concluded that there are skills common to different subject areas, but some of these skills hold special relationships to achievement in each of the subject areas. A recurring issue related to the skills paradigm—locus of instruction—dominated the research on content-area reading (Moore, Readence, and Rickelman, 1983). Locus of instruction refers to the configuration of instructional variables that affect reading and subject-matter learning: for example, the appropriate teacher to deliver reading skills (reading teacher or content teacher); the appropriate location in which instruction takes place (reading classroom or content classroom); and the appropriate instructional material (general reading materials or subject-matter textbooks). As a result, reading researchers studied the effects of teaching reading and study skills from two fundamentally different instructional approaches: (1) the “direct” instructional approach, in which the teaching of reading and study skills is separate from the content classroom, is based on the assumption of transfer to content areas, and (2) the “functional” instructional approach, in which the teaching of reading is embedded within the context of content learning, using content course materials. The functional approach is based on the assumption that reading A Historical Perspective Content-area literacy is of critical importance in the academic lives of students because it helps to shape the learning strategies by which they construct knowledge and think critically about texts. Like most instructional concepts, the term content-area literacy has evolved over time. It has its historical roots in two distinct but related educational movements: reading in content areas (RICA) and writing across the curriculum (WAC). However, it is only in the past two decades that content-area literacy has been used consistently as a descriptor to characterize students’ ability to use reading and writing to learn in academic contexts. Since the beginning of the twentieth century, when researchers and scholars first became interested in the relationship between reading and learning, interest focused almost exclusively on reading in content areas, with very little attention focused on the role that writing plays in learning. William S. Gray, preeminent among first-generation reading educators, is often credited with having forged the beginnings of the RICA movement. Today, RICA is a well-established area of inquiry and study in the literacy field—so much so that one would hardly think of it as a movement. Yet at the onset of the twentieth century, there was little concerted activity within the educational community to champion the relationship between reading and learning in content areas. Beginning in the early 1900s, however, Gray and others argued that effective teaching must provide for the improvement and refinement of the reading, attitudes, habits, and skills that are needed in all school activities involving reading. He is often associated with what has become an ill-fated cliché in education, “Every teacher is a teacher of reading.” The saying did not go over well with teachers in Gray’s time and continues to be misunderstood, if not rejected, by today’s contentarea teachers. Often cultural and school organizational forces work against the use of contentarea reading practices and exert enormous influence on the way content-area teachers view their roles, think about instruction, and resist the use 102 Content-Area Literacy is best facilitated by content-area teachers in authentic-learning situations. The shift away from a skills paradigm to a cognitive learning paradigm occurred in the 1970s and 1980s. Reading researchers focused their energy on better understanding the role of cognitive and metacognitive processes in reading and on validating learning strategies grounded in cognitive and metacognitive principles. As a result, numerous reading research studies related to prior knowledge, text structure, metacognition, and strategic learning have had a major influence on today’s content-area reading practices. Early in the shift away from a skills paradigm, Harold Herber (1970) wrote the first textbook exclusively devoted to reading in content areas. His seminal work resulted in a renaissance within the RICA movement. Herber developed an instructional model based on a functional approach to reading in content areas. He argued that skills taught in reading classes are applicable to content materials, but students must adapt the skills to meet the peculiarities of each subject they study. For nearly two decades, Herber and his research associates sought to refine and validate promising instructional strategies and procedures designed to guide reading and learning, some of which form the basis for content-area literacy practice today. Since the publication of Herber’s textbook, numerous other books have been written on content-area reading practices, all of which reflect a cognitive learning perspective. The proliferation of content-area reading textbooks in the 1980s and 1990s has extended the RICA movement into the twenty-first century. But more important, these books recognized the powerful bonds between reading and writing. The term content-area literacy came into play in the mid1980s and 1990s as literacy scholars sought to better understand and explain how reading and writing relate to learning. Moreover, they drew substantively on writing-process research and the WAC movement to explore the role that writing plays in subject-matter learning. Because so much of what students write about is tied to what they are reading, writing across the curriculum is more likely to result in students reading text material on a regular basis rather than delaying reading until it is time to take a test on the material. When students write about what they have read, both the writing task and the dif- ficulty of the text material contribute to the kind of learning that takes place. Writing tasks, in which students connect personal experiences, thoughts, and opinions, often produce better results than study questions on various measures of student performance such as time on task and the recall of information from text. The WAC movement emerged from groundbreaking writing theory and research conducted in the 1970s in England by James Britton and his research team. The researchers were mainly concerned with understanding the types of writing used by adolescents in various school-related situations. They discovered that writing to learn centered around the distinction between expressive and transactional functions of language. A transactional writing function is academic and formal in nature. It serves to inform, persuade, and instruct. Transactional discourse is not the discourse of students’ everyday use of language, which is more expressive and informal. Transactional functions of writing dominated all school subjects and were most evident in learning tasks where students were required to report and record information being studied (Britton et al., 1975). Today, the WAC movement has made its presence felt in middle and high schools as well as on college campuses. Leading advocates of writing across the curriculum encourage teachers in all disciplines to use writing to improve student learning. They argue that students can use writing to interact personally with ideas and information without the pressure of producing polished, finished products. Teachers assign writing not to produce excellent pieces of writing but to get students to explore ways of making sense of text material. The WAC movement underscores the importance of the expressive functions of writing, which are best suited for exploration and discovery. When school-related writing practices encourage informal, everyday language to express thoughts, feelings, and opinions, students are more likely to think about and explore new ideas encountered in learning situations. The expressive function is often missing in students’ writing in content-area classrooms, especially in situations where teachers have not been exposed to the theory and practice of writing as an instrument of learning, reflection, and discovery. Richard T. Vacca 103 Context in Literacy See Also Constructivism; Metacognition; Multiple Texts; Study Skills and Strategies; Transmission Instruction; Writing across the Curriculum References Britton, James, Tony Burgess, Nancy Martin, Alex McLeod, and Harold Rosen. 1975. The Development of Writing Abilities (11–18). London: Macmillan Education. Herber, Harold. 1970. Teaching Reading in the Content Areas. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall. Moore, David, John Readence, and Robert Rickelman. 1983. “An Historical Exploration of Content Area Reading Instruction.” Reading Research Quarterly 18:419–438. text, the linguistic context, or the interpersonal context). Sometimes the term was used in multiple ways within a single article. The researchers also found that in some areas of study, the term was theoretically defined in initial research studies (e.g., the linguistic context) but in later studies was assumed by other authors to be understood, and thus left undefined. This assumption that a particular term is commonly understood is at the center of the problem. Alessandro Duranti and Charles Goodwin (1992) provide insights into why context has become so problematic and why it is important to understand what counts as context for particular researchers, readers, and speakers. They present a view of context as crucial to both qualitative and quantitative studies and as having a shifting set of definitions as new perspectives and research traditions have been developed. In the fields focusing on language study, context has become viewed as a product of language use (an interactionally accomplished phenomena) rather than as functioning as a set of constraints on linguistic performance or predefined sets of forms and contents. Duranti and Goodwin view the concept of context as key to understanding the relationships among language, culture, and social organization, as well as in the study of how language is structured. They argue that currently, with the broad range of paradigms concerned with the study of language in context, a single definition may not be possible or even desirable. Rather, what is necessary is the understanding of how context is used, what it means within a particular situation or study, and how its definition influences what can be known. If we adapt Duranti and Goodwin’s argument to the study of context in literacy, and the converse, literacy in context, the definition of the term context must be seen as tied closely to theoretical positions on what counts as literacy and the units of analysis resulting from different theoretical stances to the study of literacy. These relationships vary from traditional views of context as given to views of context as the product of interactions. Context in Literacy What counts as context in literacy is one of the key issues facing educators and researchers alike. This was made visible by two comparative studies of the way the term context is viewed across research programs both within literacy studies and across fields concerned with language in use. Lesley Rex, Judith Green and Carol Dixon (1998) conducted a review of all uses of the term context in literacy studies published in major literacy research journals—Reading Research Quarterly and Research in the Teaching of English (1989–1993) and Journal of Literacy Research (1996). These years were reviewed to provide a profile of publications across editorial teams and to insure that work across theoretically different periods of time was covered. The 1996 journal review was conducted to make certain that the patterns in the earlier review were present at the time of publication and in the journal in which the publications would occur. Rex and her colleagues found little consistency in how the term context was used, few attempts to operationally or theoretically define it, and little to no overlap in citations in articles. These inconsistencies were due to a range of factors such as differences in the object of study (e.g., the society, a classroom, people interacting, a printed text, and linguistic features of a text), in theoretical perspectives (e.g., linguistic, sociocultural, behaviorist, feminist, and structuralist), and in the view of the phenomena studied (e.g., literacy, reading, writing, and in some recent cases, speaking). Often the term context was preceded by another term (e.g., the political context, the school con- Traditional or Predefined Views of Context The traditional view of literacy often equates to a single reader with a single text, defining context as the words around the particular word or bit of text that readers draw on to make meaning from 104 Context in Literacy the text. The text is viewed as the container and boundary in which the meaning is held. The units of analysis for this definition of context are the printed words in a sentence, a page, a paragraph, or in some instances, larger segments of the text. Underlying this view of context are a set of assumptions about what readers use to make meaning and what meanings it is possible to make. From this perspective, meaning is in the words in the text, both in individual words and in strings of words. A brief review of language arts and reading textbooks shows that contextual surround, the words around a particular word or bit of text, commonly defines the meaning of context in such textbooks. Nevertheless, if we reconsider the findings reported by Rex, Green, and Dixon (1998), context has also been equated with setting, often treated as a variable (i.e., social context is defined in terms of socioeconomic status) or a physical place (e.g., home context, classroom context, or community context). From this perspective, context is a source of influence, but what constitutes context is represented by surrogates. Often context is reduced to a single aspect and is assumed to remain the same in all ways it is used. For example, even when home context is a variable, differences in the amount of space available to a child at home to read or do his or her work, the number of people living in the space, the relationships of the child to those in the home or other space, and other aspects of the home situation may not be considered. A similar argument can be made for terms such as linguistically or culturally diverse classrooms, bilingual speakers, biliterate readers, gifted and talented students, special-needs students, as well as traditional classrooms or regular education students. The variation within each category and the complexity of populations and places makes the use of these descriptors as definitions of context or even the population problematic. ity) as contexts for each other, and of contexts as a product of interactions between and among people or between a person and the object (artifact) with which she or he is interacting (see Constructivism, and Social Constructivism). For example, Fredrick Erickson and Jeffrey Shultz (1981) argue that context is not given in the setting (e.g., the dinner table) but in what people are doing with each other. From this perspective, people read the contextualization cues (e.g., pitch, stress, intonation, gesture, eye gaze), kinesics (movements or gestures of participants), and proxemics (the distance between participants), as well as objects that accompany the lexical items (i.e., words) in a speech event. They then select among the possible ways of reading the activity (speech) those that are socially appropriate to what is being proposed and negotiated and then take action in relationship to those interpretations. Mikhail Bakhtin (1986) argues that speakers and writers speak/write with an implicated other in mind, whether physically present or not, and hearers/readers hear/read with the speaker/writer in mind. Thus, the context of speaking/writing is a dialogic event with context represented in the choices of lexical items and speech genres between and among hearers/readers. Research from this perspective examines how people in interaction socially organize and accomplish the work of interacting by examining the moment-by-moment accomplishment of context. What counts as context is signaled in participants’ discourse and actions, what they hold each other accountable to and for, what they orient to, and how they take up and respond to what is occurring (see Discourse Analysis, and Discursive Theory). Context, from this perspective, is not static or given but is constantly being constructed and reconstructed by participants in a given event. By extension, what counts as literacy and literacy in context is also produced by participants of the event (see Discourse Analysis). Context as Socially Accomplished The conceptualization of context as socially constructed or accomplished is grounded in work across a number of disciplines, including anthropology, pragmatics, sociohistorical psychology, sociolinguistics, and sociology (see Sociolinguistics and Literacy). Across these traditions is the understanding of the social construction of everyday life, of people and their actions (activ- Intertextuality as Interacting Contexts In the 1980s and 1990s, the importance of the concept of intertextuality (linking two texts within a single context) for literacy studies has been recognized (see Intertextuality). Intertextuality has been examined in two principal ways. For some, it is present in the citations across articles or other texts, linking one text to another, 105 Context in Literacy not merely one author to another. In this approach, intertextuality has been used as an analytic tool to explore how texts are informed by and related to others across time, and in some instances, across languages. This perspective is the more traditional way in which intertextuality, seen as interacting text, has been examined. The second way that intertextuality has been conceptualized draws on the work on social construction and the understanding that words are not mere lexical items but represent concepts and bring with them the historical context in which the concepts were used. David Bloome and Ann Egan-Robertson (1993) argue that intertextuality is not reflected in the mere juxtaposition of texts but in the actions of people in everyday life. They provide five criteria that reveal the intertextual nature of everyday life. For them, intertextuality is (1) proposed, (2) recognized, (3) acknowledged, (4) interactionally accomplished, and (5) socially significant to participants in an event, whether that event includes multiple people or a single reader with a text. That is, participants in events, in the choices of lexical items, the discourse practices they use, the actions they take, and the speech genres they use with each other signal what counts as an intertextual link across contexts. This view of intertextuality provides an expanded view of text. It goes beyond the view of text as a written document to include people as texts for each other, historicity of texts, the world as text, and other dimensions of discourse and actions, among others. From this perspective, context is both developing in the moment and brought to the moment by participants. One way to view these types of intertextual links is through the concepts of vertical and horizontal intertextuality. Horizontal intertextuality refers to the developing context and links between and among texts in the moment. Vertical intertextuality refers to the historical linking of texts over time. These distinctions provide a way of viewing context as not merely what is present in the here and now; multiple contexts are always present in any interactions between and among members of a social group. working across theoretical perspectives or cultural groups. Joanne Golden (1988) asked: If a text exists without a reader, is there any meaning? In reviewing structuralist, phenomenological, and rhetorical perspectives on reader-response theory, she explored how these theories conceptualized the relationships among real readers, ideal readers, and authors. From this comparative study, she argued that theories view meaning as in the text, as in the head of the reader, or as between the text and the reader, and that each view brings a different set of relationships among author(s), reader(s), and text(s). Her comparative analysis suggested that to understand context in literacy, it is also important to understand how the theoretical position of the researcher inscribes a particular set of relationships from the outset, thus implicating particular views of context and its influence in what counts as reading, author, reader, and text. Another important conceptualization of the relationship between context and literacy comes from sociocultural studies of literacy within and across local, national, and international contexts. From a sociocultural perspective, a distinction is made between traditional views of literacy and more grounded or socially oriented views of literacy. Traditional conceptualizations of literacy view it as a set of skills or processes that a person develops within his or her own head. In contrast to this view is the conceptualization of literacy as a sociocultural phenomenon. From this perspective, people learn and develop literacy practices within a community. The practices are constructed by members as they interact across time and events, and from these moments of participation, individual members acquire, develop, and construct the practices that define what counts as literacy within a classroom, a local community, a nation, and across national boundaries. The potential impact of these two conceptualizations can be seen in the ways in which different views of literacy reflect perspectives in the research field when we need to understand what counts as literacy when literacy is counted. From this perspective, when researchers want to count instances of literacy for national and cross-national studies, it is crucial to identify clear units of analysis, the boundaries of those units, and the perspectives guiding the research. Without criteria for understanding the differences, what Context in Literacy as Culturally Constituted These views of context and intertextuality suggest that it is important to understand how context in literacy has been viewed by researchers 106 Cooperative Learning Cooperative Learning counts as context in literacy will be invisible, the aggregation of knowledge across studies will be constrained, and policy implications of work will be difficult to ascertain. These differing views of context show that without an understanding of what each researcher means by context or what each group reading the research understands context to be, the influence or role of context in literacy will remain invisible. Any research program has a language that influences what questions are appropriate to ask, what methods are used, and what can be known. Each of the conceptions of context in literacy presented previously can be understood as one possible representation, not the representation. Viewed in this way, the importance of understanding and making explicit what counts as context in literacy studies becomes crucial to understanding the policy and practice implications of literacy research. Judith L. Green and Carol N. Dixon Cooperative learning is a process to actively engage students in their own learning through structured group participation. Cooperative groups can be formed in pairs but are more commonly arranged in small groups. Students interact with each other, exchange information and ideas, and are held accountable for their own learning. Perhaps the most well-known advocate of cooperative learning is Robert Slavin (1983). Slavin conducted a two-year study of cooperative learning in reading and language arts instruction. Students were engaged in reading and process-writing activities related to stories they read in heterogeneous learning teams (Stevens and Slavin, 1995). These students were compared in achievement to students who had not experienced cooperative learning. Those elementary students who were exposed to cooperative learning as well as the other strategies demonstrated higher achievement in reading vocabulary, comprehension, and language expression than those who were not. Cooperative learning has also been used as an instructional approach in content areas such as mathematics, social studies, and science. It is an approach that is complementary to inquiry learning in subject areas. Students in inquiry form their own questions and in cooperative groups often use multiple texts to find the answers to their queries. Research on cooperative learning in subject areas has had mixed results. For example, some researchers in literacy, like Nancy Marshall (1991) and Cynthia Hynd and her colleagues (1994) found that when students were placed into cooperative learning groups for science instruction, those who were the most powerful or influential speakers persuaded others to accept their nonscientific conceptions. In other words, students in cooperative learning groups convinced each other of their misconceptions. Hence, cooperative learning needs to be carefully structured and monitored with teacher guidance. Barbara J. Guzzetti See Also Constructivism; Discourse Analysis; Discursive Theory; Intertextuality; Social Constructivism; Sociolinguistics and Literacy References Bakhtin, Mikhail. 1986. Speech Genres and Other Late Essays. Austin: University of Texas Press. Bloome, David, and Ann Egan-Robertson. 1993. “The Social Construction of Intertextuality in Classroom Reading and Writing Lessons.” Reading Research Quarterly 28 (4):304–334. Duranti, Alessandro, and Charles Goodwin. 1992. Rethinking Context. New York: Cambridge University Press. Erickson, Fredrick, and Jeffrey Shultz. 1981. “When Is a Context? Some Issues and Methods in the Analysis of Social Competence.” In Judith Green and Cynthia Wallat, eds., Ethnography and Language in Educational Settings, pp. 147–150. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Golden, Joanne. 1988. “The Construction of a Literary Text in a Story Reading Lesson.” In Judith Green and Judy Harker, eds., Multiple Perspective Analyses of Classroom Discourse, pp. 71–106. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Rex, Lesley, Judith Green, and Carol Dixon. 1998. “What Counts When Context Counts: The Uncommon ‘Common’ Language of Literacy Research.” Journal of Literacy Research 30 (3):405–433. See Also Inquiry-Based Instruction; Multiple Texts References Hynd, Cynthia, Melinda McNish, Gaoyin Quian, Mark Keith, and Katherine Lay. 1994. Learning Counter-Intuitive Science Concepts: Effects of Text 107 Critical Literacy Groups of students studying together (Michael Siluk) also critically read a work of art, a movie, a popular toy, or a television news broadcast. Critical literacy focuses on the issue of power and how people use language (or art, or math, and so on) to question and confront injustices both locally and in the larger society. Critical-literacy practices teach people how to gain a greater understanding of the ways social and cultural forces shape their choices and their lives. These practices are rooted in the belief that although democratic principles are regularly voiced in cultural discourse, they are often not enacted in daily life. Critical literacy offers a way to analyze, understand, and confront the social practices that afford power to certain groups and deny it to others. Critical linguists argue that power is exercised through language and that language study reveals how power supports or disrupts dominant systems of meaning (Fairclough, 1989). People who adopt a critical-literacy perspective question the everyday world, challenge the legitimacy of socially constructed power relationships, interrogate the relationship between language and power, and consider actions that and Educational Environment. Research report no. 16. Athens, GA: National Reading Research Center. Marshall, Nancy. December 1991. “The Effects of Social Pressure and Personal Belief on Overcoming Science Misconceptions.” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Reading Conference, Miami, FL. Slavin, Robert E. 1983. Cooperative Learning. New York: Longman. Stevens, Robert, and Robert Slavin. 1995. “The Cooperative Elementary School Effects on Students’ Achievement, Attitudes, and Social Relations.” American Educational Research Journal 32 (2):321–351. Critical Literacy Critical literacy is an approach to literacy that focuses on creating readers who are aware that texts position people in certain ways and serve some interests but not others. Critical literacy goes beyond narrow definitions of text, using a variety of sign systems as lenses to examine how power and privilege operate in the world. Just as people can read a piece of text critically, they can 108 Critical Literacy can be taken to promote social justice. They analyze popular culture and media and build an appreciation of multiple realities and viewpoints. Influenced by the work of Paulo Freire (1972), critical theorists and literacy educators have advocated these practices over the past thirty years. Although critical-literacy practices are slowly gaining acceptance by classroom teachers, many still consider them to be out of the mainstream and radical. Critical literacies are rooted in principles of democracy and justice, questioning and analysis, resistance and action—all uncommon in the traditional pedagogy that defines a teacher as a transmitter of knowledge. In addition, these practices are substantively different from what are commonly referred to as critical-thinking approaches. Although critical-thinking practices have focused more on logic and comprehension, critical literacies have focused on identifying social practices that keep dominant ways of knowing in place. Five dimensions of critical literacy can be discussed separately, but they are actually interdependent. These include: interrogating the everyday world, questioning power relationships, appreciating multiple realities and viewpoints, analyzing popular culture and media, and taking action to promote social justice. grammar, and cultural discourses work in terms of agency, passivity, and power. Another aspect of interrogating the everyday world is for individuals and groups to examine how cultural and historical influences have shaped all aspects of life, including the experience of schooling. Using education as an example, it is possible to open up pedagogy and curricular content for critique by asking why some groups benefit from current forms of education more than others. Critical educators encourage investigation of a wide range of commonly held assumptions like beliefs that boys are better at sports (or math) than girls, students who live in poverty don’t have many cultural resources to bring to school, and competitive sports build character. In an environment where teachers focus on expanding critical literacies, students are also encouraged to interrogate classroom and environmental texts by asking questions about authors’ intentions and what they want readers to believe (Luke and Freebody, 1997). As a way of interrogating the commonplace, Paulo Freire calls for a problem-posing rather than a problem-solving curriculum where classroom engagements are grounded in the lives and interests of students. He urges educators to present information to students that is directly related to questions raised in the classroom community and not in a prescribed curriculum. In this model, teachers and children negotiate curriculum, allowing space for real-life issues and popular culture to become topics of study. Students play a major role in planning, gathering resources, and assessing learning. The goal is for teachers to become partners with students in meaningful inquiry. Interrogating the Everyday World Routines, habits, beliefs, and theories about how the world works and what it takes to be successful guide all aspects of people’s lives. These factors impact the social groups people join, how they spend their time, and the careers they pursue. Without a critical perspective, these assumptions are seen as sensible and innocent, often just the way things are, and not in need of examination. A critical stance requires a step outside of one’s usual modes of perception and comprehension using new frames to understand experience. Patrick Shannon (1995) argues for the development of a language of critique. This language can then be used to disrupt what is considered to be normal by asking new questions, seeing everyday issues through new lenses, demystifying naturalized views of the world, and visualizing how things might be different. One way a language of critique can be developed is through the study of language itself. This includes analyzing how language is socially situated, how it shapes identity, and how words, Questioning Power Relationships Advocates of critical literacy suggest that although teaching is a non-neutral form of social practice, it often takes place with no conscious awareness of the sociopolitical systems and power relationships that are part of every teaching episode. Studying how language works can be a productive tool for deconstructing and reconstructing the relationships between language and power. Taking a critical-literacy perspective requires an analysis of how language is used to maintain domination, how nondominant groups can gain access to dominant forms of language without devaluing their own language and 109 Critical Literacy culture, how diverse forms of language can be used as cultural resources, and how social action can change existing discourses (Janks, 2000). As a result of researching and analyzing language and power, educators with critical perspectives challenge the legitimacy of unequal power relationships, question existing hierarchies, and examine social structures that keep power in the hands of a few. They interrogate privilege and status, not just in lives of others but in their own lives as well. They investigate oppression—especially forms of oppression that appear to be natural or part of the status quo. This means that their students study a wide scope of power relationships ranging from issues of why some children are marginalized on the playground to why some groups of people are marginalized in the larger society. Through these investigations, participants gain an understanding of the complexities surrounding power relationships and begin to imagine how things might be different. Students and teachers explore the use of resistance, dialogue, and public debate as tools to engage in the politics of daily life. points of others. They provide examples of the self as multilayered, fragmented, or fluid. Tensions arise as the same event is interpreted or understood in different ways by different characters. The issues in these books are often messy, complex, and not easily resolved. Advocates of critical literacy examine competing narratives that describe social and political “realities” and construct counternarratives that challenge the dominant discourses. In paying attention to multiple perspectives and realities, they seek to make difference visible—creating a curriculum that honors and highlights difference rather than one that strives for consensus and conformity. Analyzing Popular Culture and Media People in today’s world are bombarded with powerful images from television, radio, computer games, the Internet, and various forms of print media. Taking a critical stance involves an examination of how social norms are communicated through the various arenas of popular culture and how identities are shaped by these experiences. Critical-literacy practices lead to an examination of how individuals and groups are positioned and constructed by popular culture and media. Cultural icons like Pokémon and Barbie are studied in terms of the messages they convey about what is or should be valued. This leads to an analysis of how the media and consumer culture are shaping our collective perceptions, responses, and actions. Appreciating Multiple Realities and Viewpoints When people adopt a critical stance, they attempt to stand in the shoes of others in order to understand experience and text not only from personal experience but also from the viewpoints of others. Individuals with a critical perspective consider various views concurrently as they seek to gain a richer and more complete understanding of the issue at hand. This often means juxtaposing multiple and contradictory textual accounts of an event (Luke and Freebody, 1997). Readers interrogate texts in terms of which voices are heard and which are missing and consider how a story would be different if it was told from a different perspective, for instance, from that of the slave rather than the slave owner. In selecting materials for classroom use, educators who are attempting to develop a criticalliteracy curriculum seek out texts that give voice to those who have been silenced or marginalized—the migrant farmworker, the unemployed father, the ridiculed child, the genocide victim (Harste et al., 2000). They also seek out multiview books where the story is told using a variety of voices. These books show how identities are constructed socially, emanating from the view- Taking Action to Promote Social Justice Taking a critical stance means using language and other sign systems to get things done in the world. This is exemplified by Paulo Freire’s call for people to become actors in the world rather than spectators. He also stresses the importance of praxis—reflection and action that transforms the world. This sense of agency can be strengthened by reading books and viewing accounts of the struggles that occur when people take action to right injustices. Individuals compose their own narratives, counternarratives, letters, essays, reports, poems, commercials, posters, plays, and web pages to promote social change. They participate in discussions that focus on issues of oppression, fairness, and transformation. They use a variety of literacies to conduct surveys and gather data to explain, expose, and find solutions for real-world problems. They use the arts to ex110 Critical Media Literacy press critical understandings and to get messages of justice and democracy out into the world. Instead of being positioned as helpless victims, people use critical literacy to rewrite their identities as social activists who challenge the status quo and demand changes. They use cultural resources and critical literacies to develop powerful voices and speak out collectively against injustice. Mitzi Lewison and Christine Leland the perceived evils of popular media are often criticized for their heavy-handed tactics, including censoring, boycotting, or blaming the media for society’s ills. Such approaches are not what is meant by critical media literacy. More typically, critical media literacy is seen as simply being concerned with helping students develop an awareness of the power of media messages so that informed, or empowering, decisions can be made about their use. This awareness is taught in various ways. Some literacy educators, especially those working from a media-studies perspective (e.g., Semali and Pailliotet, 1999), advocate identifying the various ideological positions that different media texts afford readers, viewers, and listeners. For this group of educators, knowing the ways in which individuals are marked by race, class, and gender is central to pursuing critical media literacy. Generally speaking, although educators working from a media-studies perspective acknowledge that the process of teaching critical media literacy will vary according to the text under consideration and the context in which an individual finds herself or himself when attempting to make meaning of that text, they adhere to the belief that it is important to help students identify the biased and stereotyped messages conveyed through mass-media production. A major pedagogical objective from a media-studies perspective is to assist readers (viewers, listeners) in becoming adept at resisting any attempt by the media to manipulate their worldviews. The potential for such manipulation is seen as a threat to personal freedom, and ultimately to society at large. Other literacy educators, and in particular those who view critical media literacy from a cultural-studies perspective, are concerned not so much with countering the media’s so-called threatening and manipulative hold on audiences as they are with striking a balance between pleasure and critique. For this group of educators (e.g., Luke, 1997; Morgan, 1997), allowing individuals little or no freedom to explore their pleasures in constructing meaning from media texts is tantamount to missing opportunities for developing within those same individuals a healthy skepticism of the textual messages. Instead of forcing students to critique the very texts they find pleasurable, educators who take a cultural-studies approach to critical media literacy look for ways to guide readers, viewers, and lis- See Also Critical Media Literacy References Fairclough, Norman. 1989. Language and Power. New York: Longman. Freire, Paulo. 1970. Pedagogy of the Oppressed. New York: Herder and Herder. Harste, Jerome C., Amy Breau, Christine Leland, Mitzi Lewison, Anne Ociepka, and Vivian Vasquez. 2000. “Supporting Critical Conversations in Classrooms.” In Kathryn Mitchell Pierce, ed., Adventuring with Books: A Booklist for Pre-K— Grade 6, 12th ed., pp. 506–554. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Janks, Hilary. 2000. “Domination, Access, Diversity, and Design: A Synthesis for Critical Literacy Education.” Educational Review 52 (2):175–186. Luke, Allan, and Peter Freebody. 1997. “Shaping the Social Practices of Reading.” In Sandy Muspratt, Allan Luke, and Peter Freebody, eds., Constructing Critical Literacies, pp. 185–225. Creskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Shannon, Patrick. 1995. Text, Lies, and Videotape. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Critical Media Literacy Theoretically speaking, critical media literacy may be defined broadly in one of two ways: (1) it is emancipatory, or empowering, in that it seeks to free people from coercive practices, and (2) it recognizes that knowledge constitutes power. Educators who teach critical media literacy within an emancipatory frame typically focus on creating communities of active readers, viewers, and listeners capable of identifying the various ideological positions that print and nonprint texts offer them. They also focus on teaching people how to make informed decisions about which ideological position they will accept or take up, which they will resist, and which they will attempt to modify. A few extreme emancipatory approaches to instruction that attempt to free students from 111 Critical Media Literacy cupy within it is only half the story. The other half is understanding how the meanings that audiences make of media texts are negotiated in relation to people’s different social circumstances and positioning (e.g., adult, child, teenager, male, female, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic class) as well as the cultural contexts in which such texts are consumed (Luke, 1997). As Storey (1996) explained, texts are never inscribed with meaning that is guaranteed once and for all to reflect the intentions of production; instead, such meaning is negotiated by audiences (readers) and expressed differently within specific contexts and at specific moments in time. Researchers in the 1960s and 1970s, influenced as they were by Marxist theory of a society divided along race, class, and gender lines, focused primarily on how print and nonprint textual images reflected certain ideological stances believed to be oppressive to some groups of people (e.g., women and blacks) while maintaining the status quo, or even furthering the goals, of other more powerful groups. By the 1980s, researchers began to shift their attention from analyzing the texts themselves to studying the audiences for those texts. To understand audience consumption of popular media texts (e.g., rap lyrics, films, video games, celebrity magazines), it is necessary to analyze the historical and social conditions in which such texts are constituted. From a cultural-studies perspective on critical media literacy, audiences are not passive. They do not merely reflect the images, language, and sounds of commercially produced media texts; rather, they actively engage in producing their own meanings that then become part of the historical and social conditions in which future media texts are constituted and consumed (Alvermann and Hagood, 2000). It is this interactivity, coupled with the fact that the same text may evoke different meanings from people thought to share common cultural understandings, that can make analyzing audience consumption such an unpredictable process. Just as studying audiences’ responses to a particular media text is fraught with uncertainties, so too is the teaching of critical media literacy. To reduce such teaching to a focus on audience consumption without also considering the coercive forces at work in media production is thought to have potentially serious consequences. For example, it could blind both teachers and students Students learn how to read and listen critically by the time they are adolescents (Elizabeth Crews) teners through a self-reflective process aimed at teaching them to question their own pleasures within their own set of circumstances and with texts of their own choosing. In today’s media culture, texts are often hybrids of the images, language, and sounds they incorporate. They are commonly associated with television, video, multimedia, hypermedia, the Internet, and other forms of new communication technologies such as instant messaging and e-mailing. Less commonly thought of as media texts are the symbolically rich structures through which people make meaning when they engage in music, film, dance, drama, art, and other nonprint forms of communication. Becoming literate in a world that increasingly mingles print and nonprint texts is part and parcel of living in the twenty-first century. It is also a function of learning how to identify coercive power arrangements within the media establishment and what strategies are available for resisting them. But understanding from a media-studies perspective how the intentions of producers of various media texts may construct people’s knowledge of their everyday world and the social, economic, and political positions they oc112 Critical Reading to how the media works on and through them as subjects of a textual authority that is linked to larger economies of power and privilege (McLaren and Lankshear, 1993). Another potential danger in focusing on audience consumption at the expense of media production is that by privileging meaning that is constructed at the point of reception, teachers could run the risk of inscribing even further the stereotyped thinking and biases that students bring to their understanding of popular media texts. This tension between a cultural-studies perspective (with its emphasis on audience consumption) and a media-studies perspective (with its concern for what texts do to audiences, that is, how they produce meanings that readers take up) is reflected in much of the debate surrounding modernist pedagogical discourse. From the latter perspective, teachers and students are viewed as inhabiting stable membership categories that permit little or no movement from one category to the other. Moreover, individuals within those categories are perceived as possessing fairly stable characteristics. Thus, from a media-studies perspective, teachers are perceived as knowledgeable (the knowers) and students as learners (the naive ones). However, from a cultural-studies perspective, it is quite possible to imagine situations in which some border crossing between the categorical teacher-student “divide” might occur. For example, it is conceivable that students are not as naive as some teachers might imagine. In fact, to attribute a certain naïveté to them—to suggest that they do not recognize the coercive nature of the media that teachers are trying to make them aware of (in an empowering sense)—is to suggest that power is something teachers own, a piece of property of sorts, that can be given away or withheld at will. According to Australian educator Wendy Morgan (1997), this view of empowerment is problematic in that it is arrogant and can lead to selfdefeating pedagogical practices. Working from the perspective that binary oppositions, such as those between domination (power) and subordination (powerlessness), are suspect, Morgan argues for classroom practices that abandon the search for villains or heroes in media texts, for oppressors or emancipators, and for the general labeling of oppositional categories such as “us” and “them.” Doing away with such overtly sim- plistic binaries, Morgan argues, would give teachers and students alike the opportunity to explore how people act provisionally—sometimes this way, sometimes that way—at particular times, given particular circumstances. It would also give rise to challenging pedagogically the notion that power is a possession to be distributed at will (as, for example, by media producers) and substitute in its place the idea that power is socially negotiated between audiences and producers of various media texts. Donna E. Alvermann See Also Critical Literacy; Media Literacy; Visual Literacy References Alvermann, Donna E., and Margaret C. Hagood. 2000. “Critical Media Literacy: Research, Theory, and Practice in ‘New Times.’” Journal of Educational Research 3:193–205. Luke, Carmen. 1997. “Media Literacy and Cultural Studies.” In Sandy Muspratt, Allan Luke, and Peter Freebody, eds., Constructing Critical Literacies: Teaching and Learning Textual Practice, pp. 19–49. Creskill, NJ: Hampton Press. McLaren, Peter L., and Colin Lankshear. 1993. “Critical Literacy and the Postmodern Turn.” In Colin Lankshear and Peter L. McLaren, eds., Critical Literacy: Politics, Praxis, and the Postmodern, pp. 379–419. Albany: State University of New York Press. Morgan, Wendy. 1997. Critical Literacy in the Classroom: The Art of the Possible. New York: Routledge. Semali, Ladislaus, and Ann Watts Pailliotet, eds. 1999. Intermediality: The Teachers’ Handbook of Critical Media Literacy. Boulder: Westview Press. Storey, John. 1996. Cultural Studies and the Study of Popular Culture. Athens: University of Georgia Press. Critical Reading Emmet Betts has been credited for introducing the term critical reading. Betts viewed critical reading as the process of making judgments in reading by evaluating the relevancy and adequacy of what is read in terms of some norms or standards. A valuable resource for anyone interested in critical reading from a historical perspective is the edited volume Critical Reading (King, Ellinger, and Wolf, 1967), a compendium of conceptual, empirical, and instructional literature from a wide variety of sources. The editors 113 Critical Reading of that volume defined critical reading as an analytical and evaluative process that requires the reader to make rational judgments about both the content and style of writing based upon valid criteria. In the literature circa 1960, critical reading is often referred to as the application of critical thinking to reading printed texts. It has also been proposed that critical reading is a prerequisite for, as well as an end product of, critical thinking. In everyday usage, critical thinking is reasonable reflective thinking focused on deciding what to believe or do (Ennis, 1987). Taking into account and weighing evidence to decide what to believe is critical thinking, whereas defending unexamined beliefs is rationalizing. Critical thinking is based on Western philosophical thinkers such as Immanuel Kant, who heralded the transcendental power of reasoning over superstitious thinking. Interest in critical thinking and critical reading has historically represented a search for a normative theory of correct reasoning. Although never explicitly stated in the literature of the 1960s, 1970s, and 1980s, critical thinking/reading was viewed as a way of superseding ideology, politics, and power relations through a logical, rational analysis of text. What remained uncontested was that critical thinking itself is an ideological construct with historical roots in ancient Greece. The critical thinker was assumed to be an autonomous individual able to impartially reflect, critique, and discover what to believe. One of the educational challenges with critical reading has been find a way to test it. Janice Dole (1977) conducted a multitrait-multimethod analysis to examine the validity of both the construct of critical reading and several instruments designed to measure critical reading. Specifically, she wanted to determine if critical reading, the ability to interpret information, could be differentiated from literal reading, the ability to derive information explicitly stated. An empirically distinguishable difference was not found between critical and literal reading. Dole concluded that further examination was needed on the operationalization of critical reading to examine its relation to other dimensions of reading comprehension. Measuring critical reading remains an unsolved educational issue. Helen Crossen (1948) published one of the earliest empirical studies of critical reading. Crossen’s study provides an interesting backdrop for considering the trajectory of the term critical reading from then to now. Crossen set out to determine what relationship, if any, exists between a reader’s attitude toward a topic and the ability to read critically material about that topic. Her ninth-grade subjects read about the Negro (sic) people and the German people (World War II was in progress). The results gave rise to two questions. Why did an unfavorable attitude affect the pupils’ reading about the Negro (sic), but not about the Germans? And why did pupils who held unfavorable rather than favorable attitudes obtain lower scores on the critical reading test? Crossen’s findings of yesteryear are poignant for those who are critiquing contemporary teacherpreparation curricula for paying little or no attention to race relations, ethics, and ideology. Today, there is an educational divide that must be considered with regard to critical reading. There are those who seek all-encompassing normative, generalizable theories of reasoning, reading, and critical reading. However, others assume that literacy practices operate within a sociopolitical context—a context defined and legitimated by those who have the power and authority to do so. From a sociopolitical perspective, critical readers are engaged in detecting and handling ideological dimensions of literacy and the role of literacy in enactments and productions of power (Lankshear et al., 1997). One finds among the multidisciplinary studies of literacy challenges to the presumption that learning to read and write invariably contributes to social progress, to personal improvement and mobility, perhaps to better health, almost certainly to cognitive development. Critics reject the autonomous or instrumentalist approach in which learning reading and writing is divorced from a critical analysis of the political and social order. More than anyone else, the Brazilian educator Freire is credited with drawing attention to literacy as the practice of power. Freire and others write about a critical reading of the world and propose that a semiliterate person is one who can identify words but is unable to read the world. Reading the world for Freire and those who embrace his project of emancipatory political praxis through critical literacy involves understanding the politics of oppression in totalitarian regimes and in liberal democracies. In 1961, Francis Chase offered a related vision of critical reading when he faulted teaching 114 Criticisms of Reader Response Measure Critical Reading Ability. Ph.D. diss., University of Colorado. Dissertation Abstracts International, AAT 7808892. Ennis, Robert H. 1987. “A Taxonomy of Critical Thinking Dispositions and Abilities.” In Joan B. Baron and Robert J. Sternberg, eds., Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Practice, pp. 9–26. New York: W. H. Freeman. King, Martha L., Bernice D. Ellinger, and Willavene Wolf, eds. 1967. Critical Reading. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott Company. Lankshear, Colin, James P. Gee, Michele Knobel, and Chris Searle. 1997. Changing Literacies. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press. methods for treating reading primarily as a process of discerning rather quickly what the author had written. Chase promoted the idea that teaching reading must also involve developing the capacity to understand ourselves in the world in which we live (King, Ellinger, and Wolf, 1967). There can be little doubt that the term critical remains educationally viable, but some recognize it as a “contested concept” (Lankshear et al., 1997). Educating for critical thinking/reading is claimed by various discourse communities in education, each one adhering to a set of values—to different ideologies. When encountering the term critical reading, we must consider the author’s ideological perspective. In retrospect, it seems clear that Crossen’s ninth-grade readers of the 1940s could not engage in a critical reading text about Negroes or Germans without first examining the ideologies of race and ethnicity that they ascribed to, explicitly or implicitly. A sociopolitical perspective on critical reading explicitly acknowledges this. In the 1960s, the focus was more or less on how to read a particular printed text using established criteria based on principles of logic. Critical reading, like other literacy practices, was under the influence of Frederick Taylor’s scientific management, wherein education was about acquiring skills and content that could be measured for competency. That treats critical reading as technocratic rationality. It leads to critical reading in a narrow sense. Today, the imperative is to critically read the self and the world, which in turn makes possible a critical reading of the word—the text. A broader or wider sense of critical reading has developed since the days of Helen Crossen’s study. To read critically in the wider sense is to respond to a particular text as an embodiment of a larger discursive logic (Lankshear et al., 1997). Today, when one comes across the term critical reading, it is best to consider the term in light of what can be discerned about the literacy ideology of the person using the term. Michelle Commeyras Criticisms of Reader Response Reader response refers to a number of different critical theories and practices that share an emphasis on the role of the reader and the act of reading in the interpretation of literary texts (Beach, 1993) (see Reader Response). Reader response is both a school of literary theory and a type of classroom practice in the teaching of literature. Criticisms of reader-response theory critique the idea that literary experience exists only in the mind of the reader and has little to do with the literary text or the social worlds that surround it. Criticisms of reader-response teaching practice critique the idea that only the student’s responses to a literary work matter and suggest that teachers do more than ask young people to look within themselves for meaning. Teachers, the critics argue, should expand the focus to include political, social, and cultural factors that shape the act of reading. At this point in history, most new and recently established language arts and literature curricula are grounded in reader-response theory and espouse reader-response teaching methods. Criticisms of reader-response theory and practice can help to keep teachers and others aware of the need to balance respect for the reader’s response through literature instruction that also does justice to the artistry of the author’s text and to the influence of the social world. See Also Critical Literacy; Resistant Reading References Crossen, Helen J. 1948. “Effects of the Attitudes of the Reader upon Critical Reading Ability.” Journal of Educational Research 42:289–298. Dole, Janice A. 1977. A Validation of the Construct of Critical Reading and of Three Tests Designed to Criticisms of Reader-Response Theory Literary theory, the foundation for our literature teaching practices, is always a dialogue, a constant exchange of ideas that look at literature first from one perspective and then from another. In considering the reading process, literary theorists are most often interested in one or 115 Criticisms of Reader Response more of its aspects—the reader, the author, the text, the social world. Although reader-response theory does not represent one clearly unified theoretical perspective, all reader-response theories focus on the mind of the reader during the act of reading. Most critiques of reader-response theory respond to one of two schools of thought—phenomenology and reception theory—each of which has influenced literature teaching and curriculum development over the last thirty years. factors that shape the reading “self ” and the reader’s mind. Criticisms of phenomenology and of reception theory have flourished within the turbulent postmodern intellectual milieu in which we are now living, in which no theory is to be considered balanced, and in which no meanings can ever be fixed. Assuming that language can never be stable and that history must be considered in every intellectual endeavor, postmodern (or post-structuralist) theorists of identity formation show us how readers’ identities are constructed. Phenomenology and Philosophical Critiques Phenomenology, a philosophy articulated by the German philosopher Husserl (1970), reduces reality to the contents of human consciousness alone. It proposes the human mind as the source and origin of all meaning. It provides an ideal philosophical foundation for reader-response practices that consider only what happens in the mind of the reader during the act of reading. Critics of phenomenology (for example, Gadamer, 1982) ask how the consciousness of the human subject came into being in the first place. They assert that human consciousness is produced by social conditions, even as it in turn produces them. They see phenomenology as essentialist, antihistorical discourse because it proposes a consistent, unchanging human self that stands alone outside of history. Critics of phenomenology argue that the experience of human consciousness is exactly that: human experience. They point out that all experience involves language and that language is inherently social, created by and operating within human society. Pure consciousness, and the unified reading subject, do not exist. Political Theory and Ideological Critiques Another branch of criticism of reader-response theory is rooted in political theory. Terry Eagleton (1996) has said that the political has to do with how human beings organize their social life together and the power relations that this involves. He argues that the history of modern literary theory is part of the political and ideological history of our time. No one can find a vantage point outside the social world where it is possible to read and interpret literature. “The reader” is a social entity with political and ideological (as well as aesthetic) dimensions. Literary theory can be used for political purposes. Several schools of literary theory develop this idea (Marxist, feminist, and postcolonial are among them). Unlike reader-response theory, each begins with certain ideas about the nature of power relations. Marxist literary theory concerns itself with class struggle, feminist theory with power and gender in literature and reading, and postcolonial theory with the ways in which literary constructions of the cultural Other work to preserve relations of dominance. These theories represent quite different ways of looking at the world, at reading, and at literature. Political literary theorists, who are concerned with educating youth to change the world for the better through the study of literature, critique reader-response theory for failing to take account of power in politics and the social world. They argue that reader-response theory itself is not apolitical. When it assumes a unified, rational, essential self and ignores the impact of the social world upon the human mind, it promotes a conservative view of the world as fixed, unchanging, and unchangeable. This is the critique from the Left. There have also been ideological critiques of Reception Theory and Psychological Critiques Reader-response theorist Wolfgang Iser (1978) described meaning as the result of an interaction between reader and text, as an effect to be experienced, not a message to be found. The “reception theorists” also examine the reader’s role in literature, seeing text as potential, as something to be realized in the mind of the reader. Critics of reception theory see it as a liberal humanist ideology. Grounded in phenomenology, it presupposes the unity of the reading subject and fails to place the reader in history. It ignores the social 116 Criticisms of Reader Response reader-response theory and practice from the political Right. Those who understand education as a form of transmission, in which the knowledge of the teacher is deposited into the heads of students, do not accept the idea that students are capable of producing knowledge for themselves. If “knowledge” of authors, literary historical periods, and literary devices is what must be transmitted from teacher to student, and if this knowledge is a shared “cultural literacy” that unites the nation (see the work of critic E. D. Hirsch), then reader-response teaching practices are an abnegation of scholarly authority and educational standards. Or if many students in a class express impatience with Hamlet and his failure to act, the teacher can choose to explore with them the psychology of masculinity with which they have grown up. The teacher can help them find evidence in film, advertising, and music videos of contemporary cultural discourses that mark violence and action as expectations for masculinity. Students can be helped to understand that they have come to think and feel as they do through interaction with what they read, view, and hear. Students should have the opportunity to learn something about the forces that have shaped their attitudes and their minds. A second approach to criticizing reader-response teaching practices asks teachers to help students become aware of the cultural and historical forces that surround the politics of reading. The work of Louise Rosenblatt (1978) pointed out that the meaning of a response varies according to differences in specific social contexts, which are in turn embedded in certain historical and cultural contexts. Teachers should encourage young readers to think about political categories like race, gender, and social class in order to raise questions about how these social positions exert a powerful influence over the reading and writing of literature. Teachers ought to discuss with the students the literary canon, and how and why the works they read in school have been selected for them. When young men read works written from the point of view of a woman, when Euro-American students read works written from the point of view of an AfroAmerican, when gay and lesbian students read works written from the point of view of a heterosexual person, what happens and how do they respond? How have their identities been constructed in interaction with cultural beliefs about these social categories? Students need to consider explicitly the nature of the resistance they may feel in encountering other points of view. Power relations are involved and should not be ignored. Many alternatives to strictly response-based teaching practices are now being proposed, all of them theoretically grounded in other views of what literature is and what happens when we read. Some alternatives are text centered, but not in any narrow way. They rely on postmodern assumptions about language and history and remain sensitive to the world beyond the text. Criticisms of Reader-Response Practice Critics of reader-response-based practice say that it is too individualistic and personal, that it fails to make explicit the many ways in which the social world shapes reading and the identity of the reader (Cherland, 2000). It teaches young people to look only within themselves for meaning. Teaching literature is more than arranging for a dualistic interaction between reader and text, and more than accepting student responses. Yes, teaching practice should respect what may be happening in the mind of the reader. But there is much more that teachers and students ought to be aware of. There are two important ways to criticize reader-response teaching practices. One takes a psychological approach, seeking to correct the assumption that the young reader has an essential, unified, rational “self ” that responds to literature in a cultural vacuum. It suggests that readers need to understand themselves as people who construct their own identities in interaction with the cultural discourses that surround them. A second category takes a cultural/historical approach, seeking to help young readers understand not themselves, but rather the cultural and historical forces that surround the politics of the act of reading. Psychological critics of reader-response teaching practice ask teachers to make young readers aware of the cultural discourses that have helped to shape their minds and their responses. If, for example, a young reader reports feeling anger with and disgust for Celie upon reading about incest on the first page of The Color Purple by Alice Walker, the teacher can provide information about incest taboos in this and other cultures, and the culture’s tendency to blame the victim. 117 Critique of the National Reading Panel Report Subject English, pp. 104–116. Toronto: Irwin Publishing. Eagleton, Terry. 1996. Literary Theory: An Introduction. 2nd ed. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1982. Truth and Method. New York: Crossroads. Harper, Helen. 1998. “Dangerous Desires: Feminist Literary Criticism in a High School Writing Class.” Theory Into Practice 37:220–228. Husserl, Edmund. 1970. The Crisis of European Sciences and Transcendental Phenomenology: An Introduction to Phenomenological Philosophy. Translated by David Carr. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Iser, Wolfgang. 1978. The Act of Reading: A Theory of Aesthetic Response. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Leggo, Carl. 1998. “Open(ing) Texts: Deconstructing and Responding to Poetry.” Theory Into Practice 37:186–192. Moore, John Noell. 1998. “Street Signs: Semiotics, Romeo and Juliet, and Young Adult Literature.” Theory Into Practice 37:211–219. Rosenblatt, Louise. 1978. The Reader, the Text and the Poem: The Transactional Theory of the Literary Work. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Singh, Michael Garbutcheon, and James Greenlaw. 1998. “Postcolonial Theory in the Literature Classroom: Contrapuntal Readings.” Theory Into Practice 37:193–200. Other alternatives are centered not on the text but on power relationships and the politics of literature study. Contemporary text-centered approaches to literature teaching include deconstruction (Leggo, 1998), feminist demonstration readings (Harper, 1998), and semiotic analysis (Moore, 1998). Each begins with the student’s responses but then attempts to link them to the psychology and the structures of the literary text. Moore, for example, shows students how to find the signs and codes in the text that have called forth their responses and then explores with students similar systems of signs in other texts and in wider cultural patterns of meaning. Other alternatives to response-based teaching practices are oriented to the study of power relations in the social world. Feminist approaches may suggest that students read several works by female authors who are not often included in the traditional curriculum. Marxist approaches may suggest that students remain aware of the role of money and economic power in, say, To Kill a Mockingbird, as poor people are silenced in court and denied in school. Postcolonial approaches may suggest “contrapuntal readings” (Michael Singh and James Greenlaw, 1998, first suggested in the work of literary critic Edward Said) in which two works that deal with the same time and place in history are read together, one representing the point of view of the dominant culture and one representing the point of view of the Other. In working with alternatives to reader-response instructional practices, the teacher’s struggle will be to continue to engage students with literature on a personal level while remaining committed to readings that are sensitive to the culture. Literature teaching must recognize that reading is not just a psychological or an aesthetic practice. It is also a social practice that has political consequences. Meredith Rogers Cherland Critique of the National Reading Panel Report Although reports of national commissions on reading research are not new, these efforts have increased over the past decade with two national reports in short succession: Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Snow, Burns, and Griffin, 1998) and Teaching Children to Read (National Reading Panel, 2000). The speed with which these national endeavors are following one another reflects changes in the literacy demands of the digital age. At the same time, economic, linguistic, and cultural diversity has increased in American schools. The rapid succession of national reports demonstrates efforts to understand these changes. The charge of the National Reading Panel (NRP) was to assess the status of research-based knowledge, including the effectiveness of various approaches to teaching children to read. For top- See Also Reader Response References Beach, Richard. 1993. A Teacher’s Introduction to Reader-Response Theories. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Cherland, Meredith. 2000. “Teaching Beyond Reader Response: Reading the Culture to Know the Self.” In Barrie R. C. Barrell and Roberta F. Hammett, eds., Advocating Change: Contemporary Issues in 118 Critique of the National Reading Panel Report ics such as alphabetics and fluency, their answers are comprehensive enough to be useful in the arenas of practice. For the topics of comprehension, teacher education, and computer technology, the panel repeats the educational researchers’ mantra that “more research is needed.” This critique considers topics that were not considered by the NRP and does not revisit the conclusions of the panel. Specifically, the following topics are considered: the definition of scientific research, exclusion of topics raised in the panel’s regional hearings, and the need to mark a clear course for future research. go hand in hand with qualitative analyses that describe the problems that effective teachers encounter and solve as they implement methods with particular groups of children. Exclusion of Particular Topics Alan Farstrup (2000), executive director of the International Reading Association, has cautioned against criticizing the report for neglecting certain topics. After all, choices need to be made in selecting literatures for any review. The panel failed, however, to explain why it focused on some topics and ignored others. By listing the selected topics right after the list of prominent topics in the regional hearings, the panel makes the exclusion of particular topics obvious, particularly the use of literature-based approaches to reading instruction, the role of community and family in students’ reading development, and reading development of students learning English as a second language. The panel’s failure to attend to literaturebased instruction means that the practices that dominate American classrooms were left unaddressed. The large reading textbook programs that are used in the majority of American classrooms continue to be literature based, despite mandates for phonetically decodable texts from America’s two largest states that adopt textbooks centrally. Programs have addressed the mandate by Texas and California for phonetically decodable texts through ancillary components and modifications in the first and second of the five anthologies that form the first-grade program. The findings of the panel regarding the length of effective phonemic awareness treatments and the integration of letter and phoneme manipulation can do much to direct the proliferation of policies and materials in ways that can support children’s literacy levels and teachers’ interest and skill in providing appropriate instruction. Without examining the larger context in which instruction of alphabetics and fluency occurs—the massive textbook programs that continue to dominate American schools—the connection of these findings to practice remains uncertain. By not considering whether and how the texts of literature-based programs promote fluency, the findings regarding fluency are unlikely to make a dent in the reading speed and comprehension of the sizable proportion of an American age cohort that reads slowly and infrequently. Definition of Scientific Research The panel identified experimental studies as the highest standard of evidence. Since few experimental studies were located in the research, studies of a quasi-experimental design were included in the meta-analyses (statistical integration of research) that provided the basis for the panel’s findings on topics of phonemic awareness, phonics, and fluency. Differences in findings related to the rigor of studies, including the use of random assignment, were not found to be critical in any of the meta-analyses. Throughout the report, however, reading research is evaluated on the presence of experimental studies and, based on this criterion, is found wanting. Such a characterization is unfortunate for the teachers and teacher educators who use this research and also relays an incorrect perception about the role of current reading research in educational reform to policymakers and the public. A substantial body of scientific research that uses designs other than experimental and quasiexperimental designs is available to guide practice. Further, the panel fails to discuss the reasons many classroom studies do not use random assignment or have the characteristics of those conducted in laboratory settings. For example, a critical component in high-poverty schools with high student achievement is teachers’ commitment to particular reading programs (Adler and Fisher, 2001). Such a conclusion was obtained through qualitative investigation—interviews and observations. Not only would such a finding not be uncovered in an experiment, but it raises questions about the random assignment of teachers to methods. Quantitative analyses are useful in designing and modifying instructional materials and methods. These analyses need to 119 Critique of the National Reading Panel Report The panel also failed to explain why the topic of community and family programs was not reviewed. Wide-scale federal efforts such as Title I and the Reading Excellence Act have tied family literacy efforts to school programs. Extending children’s literacy into home contexts and involving families and community organizations in literacy makes sense. How school-based educators attain the expertise to accomplish these goals requires substantiation. The panel’s reason for omitting this topic may well have been that the research base is inadequate. As with teacher education and computer technology, that conclusion itself would have been useful. The reason given by the panel for eliminating reading by students learning English from their agenda was a recently initiated research program by the federal government on the topic. A new research program by federal agencies and attention to a topic in a national report are two very different issues. Overlap of scholars who focus on reading and who focus on second-language learning has been limited. For American teachers, the children who require their expertise most in reading instruction are often children who are learning to speak English. It is all well and good to recommend that children learn to read in their native languages, as Catherine Snow and her colleagues (1998) urge, but the exigencies of many instructional situations require teachers to deal with new or multiple-language groups. By relegating the literacy learning of English-language learners beyond the purview of reading educators, the panel has contributed to maintaining the status quo, in which this population is likely to remain challenged in attaining literacy standards. Reading Study Group (RRSG) began its deliberations as the NRP was finalizing its report. The RRSG refers to Report of the NRP and Preventing Reading Difficulties in its introduction. The RRSG chose to focus on proficient reading, particularly on comprehension and knowledge acquisition through reading. In making this choice, the RRSG built on the NRP conclusions regarding comprehension and vocabulary. Connections between the findings of the previous reports on alphabetics and fluency and the research agenda proposed by the RRSG are less explicit. The Report of the NRP includes a number of critical findings on alphabetics and fluency— findings at a level of specificity for practice that have not previously been reported by a national panel. In order for these findings to make their way into teachers’ practices and into the texts that most children read daily, a follow-up panel to examine the critical topics left unexplored by the NRP would be required. By recognizing needs in particular areas rather than ignoring them, the panel could have fostered a broader view of research that would support teachers and children in America’s classrooms at the beginning of the twenty-first century. These classrooms demand a view of literacy for diversity. Elfrieda H. Hiebert and Martha A. Adler See Also National Reading Panel; The RAND Reading Study Group References Adler, Martha A., and Charles W. Fisher. 2001. “Early Reading Programs in High Poverty Schools: A Case Study of Beating the Odds.” Reading Teacher 54 (6):616–619. Farstrup, Alan E. 2000. “What the Panel Really Said—and Didn’t Say.” Reading Today 18(1):8. National Reading Panel. 2000. Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Snow, Catherine E., M. Susan Burns, and Peg Griffin, eds. 1998. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. The Research Agenda Neither handbooks nor reviews of particular topics in journals provide cross-literature integration or weigh the needs or importance of one form of research over another. The identification of a research agenda should be the role of a national panel, and in this regard the NRP comes up short. Had the panel advised Congress that evaluations of research to inform practice should be extended to other topics, it would have rendered a service to the profession. The Rand 120 D deaf community). The importance of deafness to literacy development lies in the child’s ability to acquire enough knowledge of language to understand its written form. Deaf Students and Literacy Deafness in children is defined as a hearing loss severe enough to adversely affect a child’s spoken communication and educational performance. For medical purposes, deafness is typically categorized as a severe or profound hearing loss. For educational purposes, language acquisition and educational performance, rather than degree of hearing loss, are the factors used in identifying deafness. The extent to which hearing loss influences reading and writing development depends on the type and cause of the loss, the degree of loss, and the child’s ability to learn language via visual and auditory modalities. Although hearing loss is medically classified as slight, mild, moderate, moderate-severe, severe, and profound, in reality no two individuals have the same pattern of hearing even within these categories. The ability to functionally use hearing for learning and using language differs from individual to individual. Deafness is a low-incidence disability. It is estimated that 0.10 percent of children are severely and profoundly deaf. Approximately 25 percent of students who are deaf have one additional disability, and 9 percent have two or more other disabilities (National Center for Education Statistics, 2001). Most individuals with a hearing loss are able to benefit from some form of amplification, although some deaf students prefer not to use hearing aids. Many deaf children have received cochlear implants, which are surgically implanted electronic devices that directly stimulate the auditory nerve fibers in the cochlea. Children with hearing aids or cochlear implants tend to learn and use spoken English (or the language of the child’s country or region). Children who do not rely on amplification tend to learn and use sign language (or the language of the Literacy Achievement Children who are deaf begin to develop as readers and writers from the point in early childhood when they become aware of print in their environment and the uses of print by significant individuals in their lives, just as hearing children do. Preschool deaf children have been found to demonstrate developmentally appropriate knowledge and understanding of written language and uses of literacy even when language acquisition is delayed in comparison to hearing children (Williams and McLean, 1997). However, when deaf children become engaged in formal reading and writing instruction in school, their literacy development typically lags behind their hearing peers. The average reading level of deaf students who graduate from high school is fourth grade (Gallaudet Research Institute, 2001). Achievement of the average deaf student provides no basis for predicting the achievement of any single child. Many children who are deaf achieve at reading levels commensurate with hearing children, and many deaf adults read proficiently. Deaf children have the same cognitive ability to learn language as children with hearing, and they have the cognitive ability to become proficient readers and writers. Instructional Practices Instructional challenges center around the deaf child’s language ability, prior knowledge and experiences, vocabulary, and word recognition. To understand written material, the deaf student 121 Deaf Students and Literacy Deaf student in class (Associated Press/The New Mexican) the child’s particular knowledge of the many topics included in a given text passage. The child’s vocabulary knowledge is also connected to background knowledge. Literacy instruction that incorporates vocabulary instruction recognizes that the deaf child often does not have the same breadth and depth of vocabulary as hearing children. Knowledge of genre depends on the child’s reading experience. The young deaf child tends to have less experience with storybook reading by parents because of language delays, and the deaf child often reads less than hearing peers because of the challenges inherent in reading. Thus, literacy instruction must also involve building the child’s experience with genre. It is an open question whether deaf readers can effectively use letter-sound relationships for identifying words in print. Certainly, those children with profound hearing losses who do not benefit from amplification may not develop phonemic awareness, which is the awareness of the sounds within spoken words. The reader who must understand the sentence structures, text cohesion devices, and figurative language that the author uses. The deaf student needs background knowledge of the text topic, experience with the genre, and familiarity with the vocabulary. The deaf student must also be able to recognize words in print. The deaf child’s access to spoken English is limited, regardless of whether the child uses amplification, has a cochlear implant, or signs. Thus, understanding the grammatical structures and meanings of written English is a daunting task for most deaf children. Literacy instruction must always take into account the difficulties inherent in reading and writing a language that is not native to the child. Hearing loss not only directly affects language development but also limits the individual’s ability to take in incidental information, such as through overhearing conversations and listening to the television while carrying out another activity. Literacy instruction, therefore, must also take into account 122 Delayed Readers cannot distinguish the distinct sounds made by letters and letter combinations is not likely to use letter-sound relationships for word recognition. Thus, the word-recognition strategy of phonic analysis should not be emphasized with deaf students. Literacy instruction for the deaf reader should be aimed at the word-recognition strategies of structural cues, word analogies, context, and automaticity. Promising research into improving the literacy instruction of deaf students has involved cognitive strategies. This research is based on the assumption that systematic instruction in vocabulary and word recognition must be balanced with instruction in applying comprehension strategies consciously during reading. Deaf students must be taught to monitor the successful use of word-recognition skills and application of background knowledge and vocabulary. Among the cognitive strategies investigated, it has been found that mental imagery, self-questioning, summarizing, predicting, and think-aloud approaches enhance comprehension and stimulate higher-level thinking in deaf readers (Strassman, 1997). Barbara R. Schirmer age learners—even if at a slower rate (e.g., Ehri and McCormick, 1998). Current perspective holds that reading behaviors of poor readers are not unexplained abnormalities but instead reflect reading responses that would be typical for average students at an earlier stage of learning. Associated with this view is the conclusion that with appropriate interventions, it is possible for these learners ultimately to attain reading success. The approach to eliminating struggles with literacy has centered on two key factors: searching for the cause(s) of reading difficulties (with the related hope of finding preventive measures) and instituting various instructional interventions and programs. Past Contributions to Our Understanding From the 1800s through the 1970s, a number of hypotheses specifying conditions believed to be causes of low reading achievement proved to be faulty. Among those conditions were congenital word blindness, lack of cerebral dominance, inefficient eye movements, common eye defects such as nearsightedness, and inappropriate diet. Mild emotional disorders were suggested as a casual factor during this time period; the theory of “multiple causation” was also proposed. There were two aspects to this hypothesis, both of which are still believed valid: first, that more than one cause may lead to reading delays and that the cause may differ from person to person, and second, that multiple causes may at times affect a single individual. References Gallaudet Research Institute. 2001. Available: http://gri.gallaudet.edu/literacyindex.html. National Center for Education Statistics. 2001. Available: http://www.nces.ed.gov. Strassman, Barbara K. 1997. “Metacognition and Reading in Children Who Are Deaf.” Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:140–149. Williams, Cheri L., and Mari M. McLean. 1997. “Young Deaf Children’s Response to Picture Book Reading in a Preschool Setting.” Research in the Teaching of English 31:337–366. Current Views During the 1980s and 1990s, one crucial step forward in understanding causal factors took place. Research converged from a number of countries indicating that low phonemic awareness—characterized by reduced skill in recognizing separate sounds in spoken language—is a major factor distinguishing delayed readers from average achievers. These findings led to today’s robust trend to institute training programs to increase phonemic awareness, with generally positive results reported from investigations of this instruction. However, why some individuals have low phonemic awareness remains unclear, with both “nature” and “nurture” arguments still under consideration. A second currently prevalent theory, advanced by Keith Stanovich (1986), is labeled “Matthew Delayed Readers Delayed readers are individuals who, despite average intelligence and adequate instruction, exhibit significant difficulties in learning to read. They have also been referred to as disabled readers or remedial readers—and, in some disciplines and some countries, dyslexics. In the United States, the designation delayed reader is gaining favor as result of current theory and research indicating that poor readers, no matter the severity of their difficulties, progress through the same phases of reading development as aver123 Delayed Readers effects,” a term that refers to the statement in the biblical book Matthew about the rich getting richer and the poor getting poorer. The Matthew-effects hypothesis blames the tenacity of reading delays on a cycle of unfortunate situations: for example, low phonemic awareness leads to difficulty in word learning, which in turn leads to fewer words learned than is the norm; fewer words learned results in less text read than the amount read by average students. Less text read means less practice with words and thus less automatic response to text; less automatic response means slow reading, which means—once again—less text covered, which in itself means less practice with words, and so on. In addition, since words are learned from context as well as from explicit instruction, less text read also means that the avenue for reading development is considerably narrowed for poor readers. Further, less automatic response can be deleterious to comprehension. Currently, this tangled sequence is cited to suggest implications for programming, for example, in support of early intervention. (For more details on causation, see McCormick [1999].) kinesthetic approach to word learning in which words were traced and written as well as practiced through sight recognition and phonics applications. Also during this decade, the first U.S. textbook on methods for remedial-reading programs was authored by Clarence T. Gray. In the 1930s, Marion Monroe popularized a combination phonics-kinesthetic method for delayed readers, and in response to one causal theory of the day, machines were employed to measure and train eye movements. The mid-twentieth century saw universities instituting programs to train reading specialists (especially after U.S. government–subsidized Title I reading programs were established in the nation’s low-income schools); allied to this trend, many states began offering certification for reading teachers. There was interest in teaching to a student’s “strongest modality” and in perceptual/motor training (both notions were eventually discounted by research). The learning disability (LD) field was given impetus by passage of the Education for All Handicapped Children Act; this increased the number of LD classes in schools, with the majority of referred students sent to these classes because of reading delays. Diagnostic-prescriptive teaching, characterized by detailed use of assessment information to plan every facet of instruction, enjoyed some popularity. In tandem with this trend, there was proliferation of criterion-referenced tests in which all aspects of reading responses were quantified, but the Reading Miscue Inventory was also widely advocated to allow qualitative judgments about delayed readers’ reading behaviors. Interventions and Programs: Past Trends In past decades, interventions for delayed readers have in part followed instructional trends for average readers. In addition to submitting to the same pendulum swings between phonics programs and whole-word programs, interventions for low-achieving readers have incorporated the language-experience approach, that is, instruction based on psycholinguistic principles, linguistic approaches, and computer-based instruction. Moreover, most assessment procedures, formal and informal, have been similar to those used with students in general. The reading disabilities field also has a history of its own. The early part of the twentieth century ushered in many “firsts.” In 1916, the Elementary School Journal published the first article on disabled readers, the article featuring a discussion on use of test results to plan remedial work. At about the same time, the first program for nonreaders was described, with recommendations for stories structured to highlight phonics understandings (an early call for decodable text), plus dramatization of words to aid recall. The first reading clinic was begun at UCLA in the 1920s, where Grace Fernald introduced a Present Trends and Issues The two final decades of the twentieth century witnessed a number of changes. Some of these are still ongoing as instructional interventions for delayed readers. Others are on the wane as the twenty-first century begins. During those last decades, the whole-language movement, noted for deemphasizing certain types of traditional skills instruction in favor of text immersion, exercised an impact on remedial instruction. Generally, however, this impact was somewhat less than that seen in regular classroom programs. Currently, in both settings, that approach to literacy has been replaced to a substantial degree with “balanced reading instruction,” which aims 124 Delayed Readers to attend equally to direct teaching of skills and provision of ample practice in reading authentic texts. struction of the nation’s children be based on research, not opinion. In the studies examined, positive effects are found for the use of synthetic phonics with delayed readers (as opposed to lessstructured approaches) and phonemic awareness instruction. In addition, these reports spotlighted the value of repeated oral reading of the same text, the teaching of a variety of comprehension strategies, and the provision of multiple exposures to words to deepen understandings of meanings. However, for delayed readers, negative results were reported for the use of silent reading alone to develop strategies, the incidental teaching of phonics, and phonics approaches that avoid explicitly highlighting sounds in isolation. In some cases, these findings confirm the conventional wisdom about how to instruct delayed readers; in other cases, they are stimulating new views. Specific Instructional Techniques The emphasis on comprehension research in the 1980s, which shaped rich strategies for students who have difficulties understanding text meanings, has given way to revived interest in word recognition/word identification processes, partly the result of an influential book published in the 1990s, Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print (Adams, 1990). This book comprehensively reviewed research on factors leading to word-recognition growth for average readers and for delayed readers and, among other contributions, confirmed the positive role of phonics as part of a larger word-learning program. Research evaluated in this book popularized instruction on word identification through analogies and further fueled the enthusiasm for phonemic awareness training. Other programmatic changes resulted from interest in linkages between reading and writing in the 1980s, which led to inclusion of writing and spelling as part of word-learning instruction in remedial programs of the 1990s, a trend that continues. Also generating interest at present is a powerful line of research specifying naturally occurring phases of word learning (Ehri, 1994). These studies, originally detailed to examine behaviors of average students, have excited attention among educators of delayed readers because they show promise for addressing the most prevalent problem of these learners—word recognition/word identification difficulties. These data hold potential for preventing misunderstanding of low achievers’ reading responses and can assist teachers in framing appropriate, effective instruction for delayed learners at each phase. Recent attention has also been given to research reviews sponsored by U.S. government agencies such as the National Research Council and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Report of the National Reading Panel, 2000; Snow, Burns, and Griffin, 1998). These provide support for specific instructional procedures relevant to general reading achievement, but they also include much of interest for delayed readers. The commissions preparing these reviews were formed in response to a currently prevalent issue: concern that in- Organized Programmatic Efforts Title I programs and LD classes remain the largest providers of special reading instruction in the United States. Several other distinctive initiatives for at-risk readers begun during the late twentieth century such as Success for All (a wellknown schoolwide prevention and remedial effort) and Reading Recovery (an early intervention program) also continue in schools as we move into the early years of a new century. Despite documented effectiveness of one-to-one instruction (Wasik and Slavin, 1993), concern about costs and budgets in some quarters has led to development of several other projects that have shown successes in delivering instruction in small-group settings to readers at-risk for failure, for example, the Cunningham Blocks Program and Early Intervention in Reading. In addition, the America Reads program has captured interest but has also stirred issues. This widespread U.S. Office of Education endeavor to enlist college-age work-study students as tutors of poor readers has been applauded by many, but it has also generated controversy about placing the instruction of delayed readers in the hands of minimally trained individuals. Some evidence has been presented, however, that young at-risk readers are able to make gains under such guidance, if they participate fully throughout the program (Fitzgerald, 2001). Although not in evidence nationwide, there is a promising trend toward the growing closeness 125 Delayed Readers of the reading-disabilities and the learningdisabilities fields. Over time, many comparisons have been made between those individuals who have been labeled “reading delayed” (or “reading disabled”) and those labeled “learning disabled”—when the learning disability is reading related. Consistently, research and practice have failed to discern any differences in either the instructional needs of these individuals or in the causes of their difficulties. As a result, in many parts of the United States, programming for students receiving learning-disabilities services is increasingly undifferentiated from that provided in remedial-reading classes. An issue confronting both the reading-disabilities field and the learning-disabilities field, however, is neglect of those individuals with the most severe delays. Delayed readers are not all cut from the same cloth; some have mild delays, whereas others experience moderate or severe impediments to learning. Students with mild reading difficulties are most often instructed by regular classroom teachers, whereas those with moderate and severe delays more frequently receive services from specially trained reading teachers or LD teachers. In some circumstances, where available, instruction for those with moderate or severe delays may occur in university-based reading clinics. Although the most serious cases are included in such special programs, their needs are often different from the majority of students enrolled in these classes. Unfortunately, only minimal research attention has been given to amelioration of severe reading difficulties, only infrequently are teachers trained to provide instruction known to address the learning problems of these individuals, and only occasionally are their issues confronted in journal articles or conference programs—and this is the case in both the readingdisabilities and the learning-disabilities fields. A second immediate concern is failure to harness the burgeoning promise of technology to upgrade instruction of the nation’s delayed readers. Creative uses of the tools of this science are in everyday evidence in regular classrooms, but programs formulated specifically to resolve the problems of poor readers are still surprisingly limited. Drill and practice have been made more palatable, word processing activities link writing with reading, and speech capabilities have been fiddled with, but basically, the best we can say is that these programs are better than they used to be. New ideas and “out-of-the-box” thinking are called for—as well as teacher training for the most efficacious use of technology. Another current issue relates to dissemination of research findings. Often, existing solutions to instructional problems are slow to reach those educators best situated to provide direct assistance to delayed readers, that is, teachers in the schools. The trail of dissemination from university researcher to classroom teacher, reading teacher, or LD teacher can be murky, convoluted, prolonged, or simply curtailed. Information may be poorly communicated or, if well presented, communicated through outlets infrequently accessed by teachers. A matter to be confronted is the illogicality of research funding most often being granted for producing new research, whereas few funds—or influential delivery methods—are to be had for distributing helpful data that are already available but not widely known. Lack of attention to research findings may also be one of the answers to another contemporary question: why are major nationwide efforts to help poor readers—Title I programs and learning-disability programs—only modestly successful? Admittedly, student-achievement data for these programs have been conflicting at times, but even so, major successes have not been in great evidence. It is likely that many students are more capable readers than they would have been without the programs, but there are also many who, despite years of enrollment, remain well below the average for their classes. When data are available to explicate answers for students with learning problems, again the question is: how do we make this information available to teachers on the firing lines? A related subject is the question of the best type of education for teachers of delayed readers. It is a truism that methods don’t teach, materials don’t teach, and classroom organizations don’t teach—teachers teach. The quality of a program is only as good as the quality of the teaching in evidence. It may be more than coincidental that states where budgets limit certification of highly trained reading teachers often concomitantly see reading scores fall. Clinical education of reading teachers at the university level is regaining popularity and evidencing success (Evensen and Mosenthal, 1999). In addition, intensive training, as in the Reading Recovery model, has been advocated. As can be seen from the five preced126 Developmental and College Reading ing issues, many concerns of the time relating to educating delayed readers are in some way related to educating their teachers. Sandra McCormick the reading tasks in which college students engage differ from those required in either elementary- or secondary-school settings, approaches to text reading also differ. That is, college students have learned how to read; now they must read to learn. As such, they must use generative and active reading strategies that embody cognitive, metacognitive, and self-regulatory processes, selecting such strategies based on the task, the text, and students’ own characteristics as learners. Students enrolled in post-secondary institutions have needed assistance in developing stronger reading and studying practices almost since the inception of these techniques. As early as 1927, researchers and university presidents bemoaned the fact that students had great difficulty in adjusting to the academic demands of college, particularly tasks that involved reading and studying. Subsequently, post-secondary institutions designed courses or entire programs to improve college students’ abilities to learn from text. In the United States, the growth of programs to promote improved reading at the college level has been strongly tied to particular landmark events, mostly through federal legislation. Events such as the G.I. Bill of Rights in 1944, the National Defense Act of 1958, the civil rights movement, and the Higher Education Act of 1968 all spurred greater enrollment in post-secondary institutions, thus causing a trickle-down effect of programs for college students with less than adequate reading skills. Although legislation dictated student population, two early movements—behaviorism in the 1940s and 1950s and humanism in the 1960s— influenced the types of programs and instruction available for college students, as well as the types of studies that were conducted. Research efforts focused on program effectiveness and materials for aiding students’ reading rate, comprehension, and study skills. Researchers such as George Spache and Alton Raygor led the way with college reading studies that were presented at the Southwest Reading Conference for Colleges and Universities, a precursor to the National Reading Conference. In the 1960s, humanism also became evident in practice and pedagogy, although behaviorism continued to influence research. Two overriding conclusions can be drawn from the history of college reading. First, whether the need for assistance was because students lacked basic skills or because the transfer See Also National Reading Panel; Reading Clinics; Reading Recovery; Remediation; Title I References Adams, Marilyn J. 1990. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge: MIT Press. Ehri, Linnea Carlson. 1994. “Development of the Ability to Read Words: Update.” In Robert B. Ruddell, Martha Rapp Ruddell, and Harry Singer, eds., Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading, 4th ed., pp. 323–358. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Ehri, Linnea Carlson, and Sandra McCormick. 1998. “Phases of Word Learning: Implications for Instruction with Delayed Readers and Disabled Readers.” Reading and Writing Quarterly 14 (2):135–164. Evensen, Dorothy H., and Peter B. Mosenthal, eds. 1999. Advances in Reading/Language Research. Vol. 6, Reconsidering the Role of the Reading Clinic in a New Age of Literacy. Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Fitzgerald, Jill. 2001. “Can Minimally Trained College Student Volunteers Help Young At-Risk Children to Read Better?” Reading Research Quarterly 36 (1):28–46. McCormick, Sandra. 1999. Instructing Students Who Have Literacy Problems. Columbus, OH: PrenticeHall. National Reading Panel. 2000. Teaching Children to Read. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Snow, Catherine, M. Susan Burns, and Peg Griffin. 1998. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Stanovich, Keith E. 1986. “Matthew Effects on Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy.” Reading Research Quarterly 26:360–407. Wasik, Barbara A., and Robert E. Slavin. 1993. “Preventing Early Reading Failure with One-toOne Tutoring: A Review of Five Programs.” Reading Research Quarterly 28:179–200. Developmental and College Reading Developmental/college reading is broad term that refers to the academic literacy skills necessary to be successful in post-secondary settings. Because 127 Developmental and College Reading Japanese exchange student studying with host (Skjold Photographs) learning proposes that four factors interact to maximize the characteristics of learners, including their beliefs, motivation, and background; the characteristics of the task; the characteristics of the texts; and the strategies selected. According to Jenkins, active learners consider the nature of the material to be learned and examine the criterial task, determining the products (i.e., recognition or recall) and levels of thinking embodied in that task. Jenkins’s model and similar models developed by John Bransford and Ann Brown and her colleagues, and later by John Thomas and William Rohwer, enabled researchers studying college readers to examine the variety of contextual variables that can influence how the reading process is viewed. These investigations altered the way research on learning from text was conducted. Especially influential, particularly in light of the flood of strategy research that took place in the 1980s and early 1990s, was Merlin Wittrock’s theory of generative processing. According to Wittrock (1974, 1986), generative strategies are behaviors that students choose to employ in order to influence their reading comprehension. from high school to college was academically traumatic for students, the solutions to solve these problems were similar. Second, much of the early research in college reading simply described effective reading or study-skills courses. However, with the development of reading and learning models, researchers began to base their investigations on stronger theoretical grounds. This theory draws on the early contributions of John Bransford, Ann Brown, James Jenkins, Michael Pressley, John Thomas, William Rohwer, David Rumelhart, and Merlin Wittrock. Theoretical Assumptions Since the late 1970s, both interactive and generative theoretical models have driven research focusing on reading to learn. Interactive models, though diverse, share the common assumptions that there are a number of variables that interact to impact students’ learning from text and that context is crucial to understanding text. Such models focus on the importance of the role the reader plays in text understanding, and they were crucial in advancing the study of college learners. James Jenkins’s (1979) interactive model of 128 Developmental and College Reading Thus, generative strategies require students to be active and effortful in their reading rather than passive. That is, students are generating meaning from their reading assignments by seeking links between the text and what they already know, by assimilating knowledge into their existing schemata or constructing new schemata to incorporate the new information, and by employing strategies that embody a variety of cognitive and metacognitive processes. From these theoretical models of reading and learning, several important generalizations can be drawn. First, the models imply that there are no generic best strategies or methods of reading to learn. Rather, strategies must be generative in nature and are considered appropriate when they match the demands of the texts and tasks and the beliefs and background knowledge of the learner. Second, reading to learn involves more than a knowledge of the possible strategies. Students must understand the what, when, how, and why of strategies and apply them consciously to their own tasks and texts. Third, these models suggest that there is a core of essential cognitive and metacognitive processes that cut across domains and must be embedded in the strategies selected. The cognitive processes include selecting and summarizing, organizing, and elaborating; the metacognitive processes include monitoring, evaluating, and planning. Influential researchers who studied specific strategies that embody these processes include Donald Dansereau (and numerous colleagues), Alison King, Marjorie Lipson, Richard Mayer, Sherrie Nist, Michael Pressley, Michele Simpson, and Claire Ellen Weinstein. Pressley and his colleagues, college students tend to have difficulty selecting or isolating key information and summarizing that information using their own words. Some of the most widely used strategies for selecting key ideas are text-marking strategies such as underlining and highlighting. Although the research on underlining and highlighting is extensive, the findings are very inconsistent, given the wide array of materials and methodologies used in the various studies. Moreover, highlighting and underlining do not meet Wittrock’s definition of generative strategies because they do not require students to transform and organize ideas as they do if they are summarizing the information using their own words. Researchers such as King, and Dansereau and his colleagues, suggest that when students are taught the steps involved in summarizing, their reading comprehension and ability to monitor improve. Summarization as a reading strategy has taken a variety of forms. Nist and Simpson have investigated one such form, textbook annotation, a cognitive and metacognitive strategy that involves students in writing brief summaries in the margins of their texts and in organizing key ideas. Once students have written their annotations, they read what they have written and ask themselves these questions: Do my annotations make sense? Do they coincide with what I already know? What ideas still confuse me? How will I mark these confusing ideas so I can refer back to them? These questions help the students monitor and evaluate their comprehension, using metacognitive processes they typically overlook in their reading and studying. Annotation studies have typically found that students can be trained to annotate narrative and expository text and that annotation has an impact on their test performance and summary-writing abilities. However, as a generative reading strategy, textbook annotation does have some drawbacks. Most notably, students perceive annotation as costly because they view it as time intensive. Selecting and Summarizing When students select, they are making decisions about which text information they should target for further study, usually through some form of generative text marking such as text annotation. Being able to select key information from the text that matches the task set forth by the instructor is central to this process. Summarizing is the reader’s ability to put information in his or her own words. It involves transforming text into a written form that is precise, succinct, and sensible. Both selecting and summarizing have a large influence on how much and what text information is learned. As noted by Nist and Simpson, as well as by Organizing Once text information is selected for further study, students must organize it into a form that makes sense to them so that it can be more easily retained. Some texts are considerate and well organized. Other texts, however, require students to transform the important material in a more 129 Developmental and College Reading efficient and effective way. Organizing strategies encourage students to select key ideas and subordinate ideas, to form links across those ideas, and then to choose a way to visually represent those relationships in an abbreviated spatial format. Although there are many spatial formats that help students to organize what they have read, most of these are teacher-provided (e.g., graphic organizers) rather than student-generated strategies. The one exception, however, is concept mapping. When students create concept maps, they are attempting to represent complex interrelationships between and across ideas. Maps sometimes look like flow charts, depicting a hierarchy or linear relationship, or they can look like charts, representing complex interrelationships among ideas. Studies on mapping by Dansereau and his colleagues as well those by Lipson have found that at-risk college learners performed better on recall and recognition tasks when they received explicit instruction on the use of mapping. Mapping appears to be especially effective in situations where students must read complex expository materials, such as in the sciences, and then demonstrate their understanding on measures requiring higher levels of thinking. As such, mapping seems to benefit those students who have extensive background knowledge about the topics they are reading. by generating “why” questions and then answering those questions. Because the teacher is responsible for inserting the elaborative questions into the student’s reading, this strategy does not fully satisfy Wittrock’s definition of a generative strategy. However, elaborative questions can be the stimulus for students learning the types of questions they should be asking themselves as they read. Several important findings have emerged from these studies. Most important for college students, it appears that the quality of the generated elaboration does not have an impact on students’ understanding when the targeted topic is one about which they have some prior knowledge. Simpson and her co-researchers engaged students in another type of elaborative strategy. Elaborative verbal rehearsals involve students in rehearsing aloud the important ideas that emerge from their reading. An effective elaborative rehearsal consists of relating ideas across text and to prior knowledge, incorporating personal reactions or opinions about ideas, including appropriate text examples, and creating new examples or applications. Simpson and her colleagues found in their training study that college students who used elaborative verbal rehearsals performed significantly better on immediate and delayed recognition and recall measures than their counterparts who used rote-level verbal rehearsals (i.e., repeating key ideas and details). One of the drawbacks to this elaboration strategy is that students must have a good understanding of the content before it can readily be used. Elaborating In elaborating, students personalize the information to be learned from text. They use their existing knowledge to add information that is not explicitly stated in the text as a way of making the text easier to learn. As Simpson and her colleagues suggest, they may create analogies or examples, draw inferences, generate images or word associations, or explain the relationships among two or more concepts. Pressley and his collaborators, however, suggest that many college students do not spontaneously elaborate as they read their textbook assignments. There are two different strategies that address the task of elaboration, a challenging but very necessary reading process: elaborative interrogation and elaborative verbal rehearsals. Pressley and his colleagues have conducted numerous studies on elaborative interrogation, which involves students in making connections between ideas they have read and their prior knowledge Monitoring and Evaluating Readers engage in monitoring in order to evaluate their level of understanding and the appropriateness of the strategies they use to learn from text. According to Weinstein and Mayer, when students monitor their comprehension, they establish goals and consciously assess the degree to which those goals are being met. If necessary, they modify the strategies they are using to meet the goals. Monitoring is perhaps the most difficult process to observe and describe. Although many college students are not metacognitively aware as they read, studies have shown that they can be trained to monitor their text understanding by using a variety of techniques and generative reading strategies. The most common approach taken by researchers is 130 Dialogic Responsiveness Dialogic Responsiveness to teach students how to self-test and create meaningful questions before they read, while they read, or after they read (e.g., King, 1992). By monitoring in this manner, students identify gaps or errors in their reading comprehension, thus improving their reading comprehension as well. To teach students to create task-appropriate and meaningful questions that elicit higher levels of thinking, researchers have typically provided students with generic question stems that encourage them to analyze, explain, compare, contrast, and create new applications (e.g., “What is an example of . . .?”). Other research studies have capitalized on the power of collaborative learning, in which students work cooperatively in dyads or small groups, asking each other questions and answering them in a reciprocal manner. Such findings suggest that answering the questions is as important as asking the questions because students are encouraged to clarify their understanding of concepts when they are explaining an answer to another student or to themselves. Sherrie L. Nist and Michele L. Simpson A synthesis of the multiple interpretations of Lev Vygotsky’s theories in literacy research suggests that teachers can be responsive to a student’s complex literacy needs through supportive dialogue. This phenomenon of support is referred to as dialogic responsiveness. Social constructivist theories, research, and practice revolve around the common assumption that human thought is shaped in communicative activities. Although they share this common assumption, social constructivist literacy researchers have focused on varied and often competing aspects of literacy learning because of competing interpretations of Vygotsky, including sociolinguistic, sociocognitive, sociocultural, socioemotional, and sociomotivational interpretations. For example, sociolinguistic interpretations take into account Vygotsky’s (1978) idea that human learning is mediated by our communicative symbol systems (i.e., oral and written language). Thus, sociolinguistic literacy research has focused on the natural language acquisition of children as they communicate with others using both oral and written language. Teachers and parents are encouraged to be responsive to the literate acts of young children as they use scribbles to write a narrative or use storybook language to read a story. Sociolinguistic interpretations suggest that teachers can have collaborative conceptual conversations with young children as they read and write to encourage their continuing language acquisition and that teachers can encourage peer interaction as another means of verbal support for young literacy learners. Sociocognitive interpretations in literacy learning focus on Vygotsky’s (1978) theory of the zone of proximal development (ZPD). Countering Jean Piaget’s idea of a set developmental level, Vygotsky’s ZPD presented the idea that a child’s developmental potential is even greater with the assistance of an adult. Sociocognitive literacy research has described how the instructional dialogue, between a child and an adult, influences the child’s cognitive development. Verbal scaffolding (a term used to describe the supportive dialogue provided by the adult/teacher) is encouraged in both code and comprehension aspects of meaning-making from text. This requires a teacher’s or tutor’s constant attentiveness to a reader’s developing literacy concepts and supported instruction that See Also Metacognition; Study Skills and Strategies References Bransford, John D. 1979. Human Cognition. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Brown, Ann L., Joseph C. Campione, and Jeanne D. Day. 1981. “Learning to Learn: On Training Students to Learn from Text.” Educational Researcher 10:14–21. Jenkins, James J. 1979. “Four Points to Remember: A Tetrahedral Model of Memory Experiments.” In Fergus Craik and Laird Cermak, eds., Levels of Processing in Human Memory, pp. 429–446. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. King, Allison. 1992. “Comparison of Self-Questioning, Summarizing, and Note-Taking Review as Strategies for Learning from Lectures.” American Educational Research Journal 29:303–323. Rumelhart, David. 1977. “Toward an Interactive Model of Reading.” In S. Dornic, ed., Attention and Performance VI. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thomas, John W., and William Rohwer. “Academic Studying: The Role of Learning Strategies.” Educational Researcher 21:19–41. Wittrock, Merlin C. 1974. “Learning as a Generative Process.” Educational Psychologist 11:87–95. ———. 1987. “Students’ Thought Processes.” In M. C. Wittrock, ed., Handbook of Research on Teaching, pp. 297–314. New York: Macmillan. 131 Dialogue Journals is slightly ahead of the reader’s instructional level. Although Vygotsky (1978), who is often viewed as the father of social constructivism, argued that motivation and emotion are integral aspects of learning and criticized a history of research that focused solely on cognition, social constructivists have only recently begun to investigate both the motivational and emotional aspects of literacy learning. Socioemotional and sociomotivational traditions suggest that personal conversations between teachers and students can create an emotionally safe verbal environment in which students are more apt to take risks. When students do not feel motivated in literacy contexts, teachers are encouraged to express empathy and genuine interest in their feelings. These types of personal conversations also provide teachers with the opportunity to get to know students well enough to help them make personal connections to literacy. Sociocultural interpretations of Vygotsky emphasize the complexities of thought and language development in the communicative experiences of culture. There are two Vygotskian ideas that have influenced sociocultural literacy research, including the notion that there is a reciprocal relationship (each influences the other) between everyday concepts and schooled concepts in cognitive development and the notion that the development of higher mental processes is mediated by symbols and tools (Moll, 1990). Thus, sociocultural literacy research has focused on the everyday language and literacy practices (symbols and tools) of particular cultural groups and how these everyday practices are valued and utilized in the institution of school. Sociocultural interpretations reveal that being responsive to young literacy learners requires that teachers know and value students as unique individuals, including valuing the students’ own literacy values and literacy practices. Through child-relevant instructional conversations and supported interaction, teachers can help students make connections with their own experiences, construct new cognitive knowledge, and develop literacy skills in the unfamiliar discourse taught in schools. A synthesis of these multiple interpretations of Vygotsky suggests that educators can be responsive to the complex components of learning to read, including language acquisition, cognition, culture, emotion, and motivation— through active participation in dialogue. A synthesis of social constructivist literacy research suggests that teachers can meet the complex needs of literacy learners through “dialogic responsiveness.” Cheri Foster Triplett See Also Scaffolded Literacy Instruction; Social Constructivism References Moll, Louis. 1990. Vygotsky and Education: Instructional Implications and Applications of Sociohistorical Psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Tharp, Roland, and Ronald Gallimore. 1998. Rousing Minds to Life: Teaching, Learning, and Schooling in Social Context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Vygotsky, Lev. 1978. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Dialogue Journals Dialogue journals are a journal form in which an ongoing written conversation takes place between two (or more) persons on a regular basis. In school situations, dialogue journals normally occur between the teacher and a student or between two students. Outside the school, dialogue journals are often kept by parents and their children. Teachers (and parents) who become partners with their students (children) in the ongoing and recursive reading and writing encourage literacy development and share in a more personal side of their children that may not otherwise be possible (Rasinski and Allen, 1988). The primary purpose of the dialogue journal is communication and relationship building between the journalists. Jana Staton (1984) noted that dialogue journals provide a vehicle for focused and continuing conversations in which two participants bring about new understandings, new ideas, and new meaning. Karen Bromley (1993) has noted that dialogue journals in schools serve a variety of instructional purposes. They individualize the learning experience, provide accurate and authentic models of writing for students to emulate and authentic audiences for student writing, develop writing fluency through authentic application, provide a nonthreatening and open context for 132 Directed Reading Activity and Directed Reading-Thinking Activity writing, validate self-expression and build motivation and self-confidence for writing, nurture interpersonal connections, and integrate reading, writing, and thinking naturally. Dialogue journals may be particularly useful in the literacy development of students of limited English proficiency (Peyton and Reed, 1990) and students with significant learning difficulties (Gaustad and Messenheimer-Young, 1991). Timothy Rasinski and JoBeth Allen (1988) also noted that dialogue journals provide teachers and parents with a vehicle for conversing with children in a much more personal and intimate manner than would normally be seen in oral conversation. Educators have identified several principles that are key to successful dialogue journals. An authentic and intrinsic purpose for the keeping of a journal between journalists—to get to know one another better, to overcome problems in the classroom, and so on—is critical to the success of a dialogue journal partnership. Recognition that the journalists are equal partners in the dialogue journal is essential. Dialogue journalists must allow for freedom of topic; either partner may freely choose a topic for discussion in the journal. Written communications between journalists should be frequent and continuous over an extended period of time. Journals are private communications between the journalists, and because of the personal nature of many of the communications, journalists should allow for reflection on and rereading of journal entries. The process of keeping a dialogue journal involves one journalist making and dating a journal entry and then physically passing the journal to the partner. After the second journalist records his or her entry, the journal is returned to the first journalist. Journal entries can include observations, questions, statements of problems and concerns, and responses to entries made by the journalist’s partner. Conversational topics can range from student performance in specific academic areas to the general classroom climate to personal concerns, at school or home, experienced by the student. Keeping a dialogue journal, especially when a teacher keeps dialogue journals with several students, can be time consuming. Bromley (1993) recommends that teachers initially keep dialogue journals with only a few students and read and respond to a limited number of journals each day. More students can be added by the teacher as comfort and routine in keeping and responding to students are developed. Timothy Rasinski References Bromley, Karen. 1993. Journaling: Engagements in Reading, Writing, and Thinking. New York: Scholastic. Gaustad, Martha Gonter, and Trinka MessenheimerYoung. 1991. “Dialogue Journals for Students with Learning Disabilities.” Teaching Exceptional Children 23 (3):28–32. Peyton, Joy Kreeft, and Leslee Reed. 1990. Dialogue Journal Writing with Non-Native English Speakers: A Handbook for Teachers. Alexandria, VA: TESOL. Rasinski, Timothy V., and JoBeth Allen. 1988. “Parent-Child Dialogue Journals: Family Learning.” Teaching and Learning: The Journal of Natural Learning 2:3–13. Staton, Jana. 1984. “The Power of Responding in Dialogue Journals.” In Toby Fulwiler, ed., The Journal Book, pp. 47–63. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. ———. 1985. “Using Dialogue Journals for Developing Thinking, Reading, and Writing with Hearing-Impaired Students.” Volta Review 87:127–153. Directed Reading Activity and Directed Reading-Thinking Activity Directed Reading Activity (DRA) and Directed Reading-Thinking Activity (DR-TA) are two teaching strategies used to guide students as they read text. DRA is one of the oldest and most widely used frameworks for reading instruction. DRA is designed to assist teachers in providing systematic group-reading instruction, in guiding and engaging students in reading texts, and in providing students with direct instruction in word recognition and comprehension (Tierney and Readence, 2000). DRA is commonly associated with basal-reading instruction in the elementary grades but can be used with students at all grade levels and with a variety of texts. DRA is a three-step teaching process that involves prereading, during-reading, and postreading activities. In the prereading step, students’ prior knowledge of the topic or concept is activated, or background knowledge developed, and vocabulary words are introduced. Vocabulary is presented both orally and visually in the context of sentences. Students’ purposes for reading are usually established by the teacher during pre133 Directed Reading Activity and Directed Reading-Thinking Activity reading. Based on students’ needs and abilities, silent reading of the text is conducted in one of two ways: First, students read the text in its entirety and then orally respond to literal, inferential, and interpretive questions posed by the teacher during postreading discussion; or, second, the teacher divides the text into three or four sections and then guides students as they silently read through one section at a time. After a section of the text has been read, the teacher orally poses questions and engages students in discussion before continuing on to the next section. Once each section has been read and discussed, the teacher poses summative questions related to the entire selection, thereby integrating each part of the text into a more comprehensible whole. The postreading comprehension check and discussion may be followed by oral rereading or extension activities that connect the text with writing, the arts, or other curricular areas such as math, science, and social studies. Direct instruction in comprehension and word recognition is provided during the postreading step. Robert Tierney and John Readence (2000) caution teachers to engage in ongoing evaluation of DRA’s effectiveness in meeting students’ needs. Instruction should be meaningful to students, related to the reading task, and explained and modeled by the teacher. Teachers should be aware that DRA is teacher centered and places students in passive reading roles. Unlike DRA, Directed Reading-Thinking Activity engages students in active reading and thinking and places the teacher in the role of facilitator. DR-TA is an instructional approach that requires students to predict, set purposes for reading, and actively seek evidence in the text to support predictions (Stauffer, 1969). DR-TA is designed to help students establish purposes for reading, to generalize, analyze, induce, assimilate, and integrate information, to read critically and reflectively, and to engage in higher levels of cognitive reasoning (Widomski, 1983). DR-TA is a two-part teaching strategy. In the first phase, the teacher guides and directs students’ thinking as they read the text. During the second phase, the teacher provides instruction in identified areas of need, enrichment, and extension. Teachers prepare to use DR-TA by selecting a text that students will be interested in reading. Based on the teacher’s familiarity with the text, four to six stopping points are selected for prediction and discussion. The teacher formulates open-ended questions to accompany each stopping point. The questions should encourage students to predict what the upcoming reading will be about, what will happen, or what will be learned. Teachers encourage students to provide reasoning for predictions by asking, “Why do you think that?” Stopping points are usually after the title, after the first few paragraphs, at points of high interest, action, or possible confusion, and just before the end. At each stopping point, students predict what will happen or what will be learned based on information from the text and prior knowledge, confirm or adjust predictions based on new information learned from reading, and provide proof or support of predictions using the text or prior knowledge. The process of predicting, confirming, and refuting predictions helps students connect prior knowledge with information from the text. The DR-TA lesson begins with the teacher reading the title of the text aloud and asking students to predict what the story or lesson will be about. Teachers should note that students’ predictions are listed on the board or overhead at each stopping point. Students then read the first few paragraphs silently. At the first stopping point, the teacher asks students to confirm or adjust their predictions. Following discussion at the second stopping point, the teacher asks students to formulate predictions about the next segment of text. This discussion-prediction pattern is followed throughout the reading of the text. At the final stopping point, students develop predictions as they decide upon the selection’s ending. Finally, students relate how evidence within the text influenced their decisions. The heart of DR-TA lies in students’ formulation of responses to the questions “What do you think will happen next?” and “Why do you think so?” The first question elicits prediction, and the second requires explanation and reasoning. The second phase of DR-TA involves instruction based on student needs that were identified in phase one. For example, the teacher may offer vocabulary instruction if students had difficulty understanding specific terms. A variety of enrichment and extension activities may be presented at this point, but activity selection should be based on the teacher’s observation of students’ needs. 134 Discourse Analysis sentence, it is better to define discourse as language in use. She views language in use as discourse used to mean and do something in realworld contexts that are read and interpreted by hearers/readers. Regardless of the definition used, a review of studies purporting to use discourse analysis in education shows confusion about what counts as discourse analysis. Some educational researchers reduce discourse to talk, and they record talk merely to identify repeated terms or behaviors or to code the talk using predefined categories, ignoring the disciplinary roots of traditions that gave rise to discourse analysis. Often such studies decontextualize discourse from its context, equating one instance of talk as equivalent to other instances of the same term, phrase, sentence, or connected sequences of utterances. Most of these approaches are grounded in behavioral theories, not theories of language in use, pragmatics, semiotics, or linguistics, and with the exception of recent work from a social behaviorist perspective, they ignore the context of use and the connected nature of talk. For example, in preset category systems, the lexical item OK is often coded as praise, ignoring contextual differences in what is intended by the use of the term or what is accomplished through its use. When the context of use is considered, the term OK can have a variety of meanings depending on how it is spoken, when it is used, what it refers to, and how members respond to its use. It can be praise (“OK! That was great!”), a way of holding your turn at speaking (“Mmmm, okaaaayyy, ummm, it was Bobbie who did that, wasn’t it?”), or a way of signaling a change in direction of the activity being undertaken (“OK, let’s go on now.”), among other uses. (For discussions of different systems for observing interaction see Evertson and Green, 1986; and for context, see Duranti and Goodwin, 1992.) In contrast, approaches grounded in work on language in use and in social construction of everyday life in and through discourse take as a given that the meaning of each use can only be understood in the context of occurrence, by considering what precedes it and what follows. Just how much text preceding or following the bit of discourse being examined is necessary to establish the context of use differs again by theoretical perspective, research tradition, analysts’ questions and purpose(s), and types of data available. The major differences between DRA and DR-TA are the instructional point for vocabulary, the emphasis placed on students’ active engagement in the reading-thinking process, and the teacher’s role. DRA is teacher centered and depends on teacher-formulated purposes for reading and questions for comprehension building. DR-TA engages students in active reading and formulation of predictions and places the teacher in the role of facilitator. When using DRA, vocabulary terms are introduced prior to reading, whereas vocabulary instruction does not occur until after reading when DR-TA is used. Pamela J. Dunston and Kathy N. Headley See Also Critical Reading References Stauffer, Russell G. 1969. Reading Maturity as a Cognitive Process. New York: Harper and Row. Tierney, Robert J., and John E. Readence. 2000. Reading Strategies and Practices: A Compendium. 5th ed. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Widomski, Cheryl L. 1983. “Building Foundations for Reading Comprehension.” Reading World 22:306–313. Discourse Analysis Any discussion of discourse analysis must start by defining the term discourse and understanding how analysis works into the definition. To understand the contribution of discourse analysis to the study of literacy in schools, other institutions, and communities, it is necessary to distinguish discourse analysis from other approaches to the study of talk and interaction in social groups and educational settings. Examination of approaches to discourse analysis across disciplines provides a picture of diverse theoretical languages used and variation in the objects of study, purposes, and methodological tools and processes, resulting in differences in understandings and in what can be known through each approach. Adam Jaworski and Nikalos Coupland (1999) provide multiple definitions, arguing that most approaches to discourse analysis are based on the understanding that, at a minimum, discourse refers to connected talk or written text above (longer than) the level of the sentence. These perspectives vary on other dimensions of language in use and context. Deborah Cameron (2001) argues that rather than using the definition connected text greater than a 135 Discourse Analysis To illustrate these differences and to examine how variations in approach contribute to the knowledge available through different traditions of discourse analysis, five of the most widely used approaches in education will now be discussed: conversation analysis, critical discourse analysis, ethnographically framed approaches, pragmatics, and sociolinguistics. We will present these approaches, beginning with those focusing more specifically on language use (form and function) and then present those that focus more specifically on what is accomplished in and through language in use, realizing that these distinctions are for heuristic purposes. In actuality, these distinctions can be, and often are, blurred within individual studies. Central to these traditions is a view of discourse as socially constructed in and through actions and interactions of members of a social group. Discourse, therefore, emanates from a group and not solely from an individual, although individuals can and do contribute to the discourse among participants, make choices about which discourse practices and processes to use, and through discourse define what counts as socially appropriate actions. Differences among these perspectives are related to the level of context considered (macro society or local moments, or combinations thereof) and whether they focus on oral or written discourse or the relationship between the two. These approaches also differ in their views of historicity (the relationship of particular moments of use to previous ones) and intertextuality (the relationship of present contexts to previous or future actions, times, places, or events) (see Intertextuality). formance, and institutional language demands. These studies examined differences among systems of language and their uses in schools and in local communities, suggesting a difference model rather than the then dominant deficit model, which viewed dialect speakers as deficient in language when entering school. These sociolinguistic approaches led to the construction of new understandings of differences in language use and the need to examine closely academic genres and language demands of the classroom. Such studies demonstrated ways in which language variation is ordinary and occurs across events, actors, groups, and purposes. Sociolinguistic studies led to the need to understand language that resources students (teachers and others) bring and how these resources are, or are not, supported in classrooms. One approach, interactional sociolinguistics, expanded the object of study and, concurrently, the ways in which data are collected, segments of talk are selected, and the units of analysis are chosen. This approach requires use of analytic units that involve the ways members construct extended stretches of interaction, patterns of interaction, demands for participating, and responses to what is said and done. This latter aspect entails examination of chains of action rather than individual instances of language use. Authors associated with sociolinguistics include: Joshua Fishman, Dell Hymes, William Labov, Peter Trudgill, and Walt Wolfram; interactional sociolinguists include Michelle Foster, Erving Goffman, John Gumperz, Monica Heller, Sarah Michaels, Cathleen O’Connor, Celia Roberts, Deborah Schiffren, Michael Stubbs, and Deborah Tannen. Traditions of Discourse Analysis Pragmatics Discourse approaches associated with pragmatics have their roots in philosophy of language and focus on how members use language to mean and do things in the world. Although the pragmatics approach shares some goals with sociolinguistic approaches, pragmatics has in the past focused on what speakers do and how they convey meaning, by examining units such as speech acts and pragmatic and discourse principles (e.g., conversational cooperation and felicity of contributions to topic). Historically, this work has drawn heavily on the philosophical work of John Austin, John Searle, and H. P. Grice. For ex- Sociolinguistics Sociolinguistic approaches to discourse analysis were some of the earliest used to study language in use in educational settings (see Sociolinguistics and Literacy). These studies examined such issues as: language use and its relationship to race, gender, ethnicity, and class; how such variations in use influenced assessment of the ability of students; students’ communicative competence and performance; and how schools meet the needs of linguistically different speakers. Sociolinguistic studies examine differences in language as a system, grammatical use, speech per136 Discourse Analysis ample, work drawing on Austin tends to analyze three interrelated aspects of speech acts. It examines locution (actual words used), illocutionary force (how it was meant to be heard), and perlocution (the effect) of speech acts used by a speaker to accomplish a particular intention (e.g., to state something, command someone to do something, and request something). This work has importance when issues of form, intention, and effect on hearers are examined. Central to this work is the distinction between syntax and extralinguistic or prosodic features of language in use that signal to others how something is meant to be heard (e.g., pitch, stress, intonation, and pause). Analysis of speech acts (utterances) often entails marking of prosodic features of talk as a basis for identifying illocutionary force and for interpreting meaning, both intended and read by the other. Using the example of OK, a pragmatic analysis would view a rising pitch in American English to signal a question or a request for confirmation; whereas, oh-kay, with even stress on each syllable and a higher pitch than the preceding talk, could be heard as praise. Because of its focus on language in use, pragmatics is often seen as overlapping some approaches to sociolinguistics. tion to identify mechanisms that members of a social group use to construct the social organization of actions. Analysis has examined mechanisms such as turn taking, use of names, and ways members orient to and hold each other accountable to what is occurring. For example, the IRE sequence (Initiate-Respond-Evaluate) often found in traditional classrooms is viewed as constituting a key mechanism, the product of which is a particular type of schooling as experienced by students. One turn in a sequence is viewed as placing a demand for a response on the next, and through the reflexive actions of participants, the constituent elements of the interaction are identified. Patterns of interaction (e.g., I-R-E sequences) are examined to understand how the social organization of actions is structured, in and through the interactions among members in the moment. Conversation analysts and ethnomethodologists ask the question “What counts as ———?” (e.g., literacy, text, gendered actions, or opening of a conversation) to construct locally situated understandings of the mechanisms used and the social organization of actions constructed through their use. Activities are products of interactions among members. Analysts seek to construct an answer to this question from a member’s perspective by observing closely the sequence of actions among participants in a particular setting. This approach entails a situated perspective, and claims are grounded in the social accomplishment of actions. Analysts look for isolated, clearly bounded sequences and then treat these sequences as units of analysis (e.g., a sequence of instructions; the sequence used to close a lesson; and 911 calls). Conversation Analysis Conversation analysis (CA) also focuses on language in use. However, the goal and methodological practices of CA differ. Conversation analysis has its sociological roots in the work of Harold Garfinkel. It is related to work in ethnomethodology and includes work by Carolyn Baker, Aaron Cicourel, James Heap, John Heritage, Gene Lerner, Hugh Mehan, Doug McBeth, Alex McHoul, Emmanuel Schegloff, and Don Zimmerman. CA constructs its arguments from data, and analysts are enjoined from using evidence or theories from outside of the talk-in-interaction. Among others, issues of context, activity, and gender are viewed as constituted in and through the talk-in-interaction; they do not draw on information, beliefs, or theory external to these interactions. The analyst looks for observable evidence and must be able to point to a particular utterance (or place) when making a claim about something in the interaction. The analytic focus of CA is not conversation or discourse, but talk-in-interaction. Analysts examine the sequential work of people in interac- Critical Linguistics and Critical Discourse Analysis Critical linguistics and critical discourse analysis (CDA) studies language in its relationship to power and ideology. Power is viewed in two ways: in terms of asymmetries that exist between participants in discourse events and in terms of unequal capacity to control the production of texts and how they are distributed and consumed. Texts are both those constructed in a local discursive event (oral or written, or both) and those created beyond that event (e.g., media, technological, graphic, and others). CDA is both a theoretical perspective and a methodological 137 Discourse Analysis approach used to examine power-ideology relationships in particular sociocultural contexts; like CA, CDA entails a situated perspective. What differs is the level of situation under examination (both the sequential production and larger units of text) and what lens is brought to the analysis. For example, Norman Fairclough (1993) argues that a bit of discourse is simultaneously a text, a discourse practice, and a social practice through which a larger text or discourse activity or event is being constructed. Local discourse choices of speakers/writers are drawn from discourses within the broader sociocultural contexts. A range of properties of discourse practices and texts is regarded as potentially ideological, including features of vocabulary, metaphors, genres, grammatical conventions, style, and discourse strategies (e.g., turn taking, politeness conventions, and topic appropriateness). Choices that writers or speakers make in constructing texts begin to shape and then are shaped by the connected text(s) being constructed. Through this process, the writer/speaker/group inscribes an ideological position within the local sociocultural context. In some approaches to CDA, the issue of consciousness of decisions is of concern and has led to ways of addressing the issue of naturalization of language. Roz Ivanic (1998) used CDA to develop approaches that show students (and others) how the linguistic choices they make in constructing texts are not natural but are shaped by other texts (intertextuality) or discourse within their sociocultural ecology. Choices among discourses (and discourse features) also inscribe identities of the speaker/writer that are then available to others to read and interpret. CDA examines discourse choices and issues about who has access to these choices, for what purposes, and in what ways. Authors associated with critical linguistics and critical discourse analysis include Frances Christie, James Gee, Michael Halliday, Gunter Kress, Jay Lemke, Jim Martin, Teun Van Dijk, and Theo Van Leeuwen. settings), with sociolinguistic and other forms of discourse analysis. The oldest of these is ethnography of communication, sometimes referred to as the ethnography of speaking. This approach examines how patterns of language use, in particular communities, or sustaining groups, occur and what the consequences of their use are for speakers; how language use marks one as a member of a particular speech group; and how members of one speech community act, interpret, evaluate, and respond to speakers of other languages or dialects. These approaches to discourse analysis require overtime examination of who can speak in what ways, when, where, under what conditions, and for what purposes. Analysts also explore the outcomes of these ways of speaking for a particular speaker in relation to the event in which the interaction occurred (see Muriel Saville-Troike, 1989, for a comprehensive discussion of this approach; other authors include Elinor Ochs, Patricia Duff, Candy Goodwin, John Gumperz, Dell Hymes, Marilyn Martin-Jones, and Bambi Schieffelin). Studies of the ethnography of communication have examined differences in speech styles among groups, language socialization processes, institutionally framed expectations for language use, miscommunication among speakers of the same language or different languages, and contrasts between language of home, school, and community, as well as between first- and secondlanguage learners. In educational settings, studies drawing on the ethnography of communication have examined, among other topics, interethnic communication and the relationships between language and identity and between language and perceived ability. More recently, a second area of focus has developed that brings a broader focus on language and literacy as socially accomplished. This approach seeks to answer questions about what counts as language use and literacy within and across local events, groups, and settings. Such study views language as primary to the conduct of everyday life, with literacy and discourse practices as the outcomes of the interactions among members of the group. Discourse analysis, from this perspective, views discourse as both a process and a product of local interactions and sees it as intertextually tied to past and future events constituting human activity. Language is both a resource for communication and an outcome of communica- Ethnographically Framed Discourse Analysis In the past four decades, traditions have developed that bring together ethnographic studies of the social construction of everyday life within and across sustaining groups, sometimes called speech communities (e.g., families, peer groups, ethnic groups, classrooms, and other institution 138 Discourse Analysis tion across time and events. Prior uses of language (and literacy) are material resources that members draw on to construct new interactions, communicate with others, and read and interpret what is occurring in the present event under construction. Authors associated with this perspective include David Bloome, David Barton, Carol Dixon, Michele Foster, Judith Green, Shirley Brice Heath, Judith Kalman, Greg Kelly, Elsie Rockwell, Brian Street, and Gordon Wells. Two traditions have emerged as new theoretical and methodological directions within this group: new literacy studies (David Barton, Mary Hamilton, Brian Street, and colleagues in the United Kingdom) and interactional ethnography (Theresa Crawford, Maria Lucia Castanheira, Carol Dixon and Judith Green [2001], and their colleagues in the United States). words but the related actions (nonverbal), contextual cues, and historical ties (intertextual referents). These directions provide new challenges for authors. Given theory-method relationships, it is imperative that the logic of discourse analysis used be included in published works as part of the evidence trail. The richness of this approach brings both challenges and resources to the study of literacy and language in educational settings. Judith L. Green and Carol N. Dixon See Also Feminist Post-Structuralism; Intertextuality; PostStructuralism and Structuralism; Sociolinguistics and Literacy References Cameron, Deborah. 2001. Working with Spoken Discourse. London: Sage. Crawford, Theresa, Maria Lucia Castanheira, Carol Dixon, and Judith Green. 2001. “What Counts as Literacy: An International Ethnographic Perspective.” In Joy Cunning and Claire Wyatt Smith, eds., Literacy and the Curriculum: Success in Senior Secondary Schooling, pp. 32–43. Camberwell, Victoria: The Australian Council for Educational Research. Duranti, Alessandro, and Charles Goodwin. 1992. Rethinking Context. New York: Cambridge University Press. Evertson, Carolyn, and Judith Green. 1986. “Observation as Inquiry and Method.” In Meryl Wittrock, ed., The Handbook for Research on Teaching, 3rd ed., pp. 162–213. New York: Macmillan. Fairclough, Norman. 1993. “Discourse and Text: Linguistic and Intertextual Analysis within Discourse Analysis.” Discourse and Society 3 (2):193–218. Ivanic, Roz. 1998. Writing and Identity: The Discoursal Construction of Identity in Academic Writing. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Jaworski, A., and N. Coupland. 1999. The Discourse Reader. New York: Routledge. Ochs, Elinor. 1979. “Transcription as Theory.” In Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schefflin, eds., Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic. Saville-Troika, Muriel. 1989. The Ethnography of Communication: An Introduction. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell. Implications for Analyzing and Reporting Data The five approaches described above represent some of those available. Others include feminist and post-structural approaches and theories as well as cognitive, symbolic interactionist, and narrative analysis approaches (see Feminist PostStructuralism, and Post-Structuralism and Structuralism). These approaches differ in theoretical orientations and in the ways they lead to construction of transcripts, analysis practices, and the conclusions or claims that can be drawn from the data. Recently, work by sociocultural/ sociohistorical theorists (e.g., Lev Vygotsky and Mikhail Bakhtin and their intellectual descendants) has raised questions about relationships between speakers and hearers and about the roles and relationships among participants across time and events. These frameworks have led researchers to explore the relationships between text and talk, speech genres and other forms of literate practices, identity formations and their relationships to discourse practices, among other subjects. These new perspectives confirm Elinor Ochs’s (1979) argument that a transcript inscribes the analyst’s theory of the relationships between and among actors (speakers) and that the linear transcription inscribes assumed hierarchical/power relationships rather than representing actual relationships. Many new approaches have explored ways of representing the interactions among speakers in ways that represent not only the 139 Discursive Theory Discursive Theory ernment offices, or social groups with certain sorts of genres, such as baseball cards, comic books, chess, politics, novels, or movies. A person has to be socialized into a practice to learn to read texts of type X in way Y, a practice other people have already mastered. Since this is so, we can turn literacy on its head and refer specifically to the social institutions or social groups that have these practices, rather than to the practices themselves. When we do this, we realize that the practices of such social groups are never just literacy practices. They also involve ways of talking, interacting, thinking, valuing, and believing. Researchers who take a sociocultural approach to literacy believe that literacy in and of itself, abstracted from historical conditions and social practices, has no definitive set of predictable effects, for example, causing people to be more intelligent, more analytical, or more modern (Graff, 1987). Rather, literacy has different effects in the context of different historically varying social and cultural practices. For example, schoolbased writing and reading leads to different effects than reading and writing as they are embedded in various religious practices around the world. Of course, there are multiple and different school-based practices and multiple and different religious practices, each with multiple effects. A Sociocultural Perspective on Literacy Research that views literacy in its social, cultural, economic, and historical contexts often uses the term discourse as a higher-order concept within which literacy is situated. Before delineating the range of meanings the concept of discourse has come to have in this research, it is important to contrast traditional views of literacy with viewpoints that take a sociocultural and sociohistorical perspective. Traditionally, literacy has been viewed as the psychologically defined ability to read and write, often with an emphasis on basic reading skills (e.g., decoding and literal comprehension). A sociocultural perspective (Gee, 1996) starts with the assumption that reading always has to do with being able to read something. This something will always be a text of a certain type. Different types of texts (e.g., newspapers, comic books, law books, poems) call for different types of background knowledge and require different skills in order to be read meaningfully. In turn, no one would say anyone could read a given text if he or she did not know what the text meant. But there are many different levels of meaning that can be given to or taken from any text, many different ways in which any text can be read. A person can read a friend’s letter as a mere report, an indication of her state of mind, a prognosis of her future actions; a person can read a novel as a typification of its period and place, as vicarious experience, as art, as a guide to living, and so on and so forth. Given this perspective of reading as reading a certain type of text in a certain way, we can ask: How does a reader acquire the ability to read a certain type of text in a certain way? Here proponents of a sociocultural approach to literacy argue that a person acquires a way of reading a certain type of text by being apprenticed as a member of a social practice wherein people not only read texts of this type in this way, but also talk about such texts in certain ways, hold certain attitudes and values about them, and socially interact over them in certain ways. Thus, a person does not learn to read texts of type X in way Y unless he or she has had experience in settings where texts of type X are read in way Y. These settings are various sorts of social institutions, like churches, banks, schools, gov- Discourses In an influential work, the linguist Norman Fairclough (1995) has developed a critical sociocultural approach to language, along the lines detailed above, in which the notion of discourse plays a prominent role. Fairclough views both oral and written language as modes of action in terms of how people act upon the world and upon each other. He uses the term “discourse,” when it is used (in phrases such as “the discourse of neoliberal economics” or “radical feminist discourses of sexuality versus patriarchal discourses of sexuality”) as a term for distinctive ways of using language to construe the material and social world from a particular perspective. Fairclough goes on to define the notion of an order of discourse, by which he means the set of discursive practices associated with a particular social domain or institution as well as the boundaries and relationships between them (Fairclough, 1995). For example, an academic draws on a variety of ways of using oral and writ140 Discursive Theory ten language: lectures, discussions, research publications, committee reports, and so forth. Traditionally, businesspeople have drawn on a different order of discourse containing different sorts of oral and written text types and ways with words. However, it is common today to see some elements of the business order of discourse get imported into the academic one, as colleges and universities operate more like entrepreneurial enterprises in the so-called new economy. In his use of the terms discourse and order of discourse, Fairclough was influenced by Michel Foucault’s widely known work (1981). Foucault was interested in the ways in which, at particular historical periods, alignments and relationships among particular types of texts, practices, and institutions in a society set limits to what is “sayable” and “thinkable.” He was also interested in how particular and historically distinctive ways of talking, thinking, and seeing the world spread across various linked, but independent, institutions. For example, Foucault studied how, in the eighteenth century, institutions such as prisons, hospitals, and schools began to replace the use of physical force to discipline people with forms of discipline based on constant observation (surveillance) and professional or specialist knowledge about human development and deviance. The prisoner, the patient, and the student became “clients” of “professionals.” The linguist James Paul Gee (1996) has also appealed to the notion of discourse, though he uses the term in a somewhat different way. He distinguishes between “Discourse” with a “big D” and “discourse” with a “little d.” By discourse with a little d, he means any instance of language-in-use to communicate. Discourse with a “big D” is a way in which people get recognized (and recognize themselves and others) as distinctive “kinds of people” through engaging in distinctive and partially repeatable social practices, whether they are members of a Los Angeles street gang, lawyers, or biologists of a certain sort, mental patients of a certain type, or members of a particular first-grade classroom. A Discourse is composed of (and integrates) distinctive ways speaking, acting, interacting, thinking, believing, dressing, valuing, as well as using various sorts of tools, and technologies at the right times and places, so as to get recognized as a particular socially distinctive “kind of person” doing a particular socially distinctive kind of action. For Fairclough and Gee, literacy is always a particular way of using written language within a specific (and historically changing) order of discourse or “Discourse” connected to specific cultures or institutions. Since orders of discourse or Discourses always involve people’s socially situated identities and are always integrally connected to values and beliefs about what counts as “acceptable,” “appropriate,” or “normal” kinds of people, texts, language, and meanings, they are inextricably “political” in the sense that viewpoints about the distribution of social goods (such as status, possessions, credentials, or social worthiness) are always at stake. Communication across Discourses The work of Ron and Suzzane Scollon (1981), in their now classic study of communication between Athabaskans (a group of aboriginals found in the United States and Canada) and AngloAmericans and Canadians, exemplifies these perspectives in a powerful way. The Scollons believe that patterns of language-in-use (“discourse patterns” in the “little d” sense of discourse) in different cultures reflect particular “reality sets” or worldviews adopted by these cultures. Such patterns are among the strongest expressions of personal and cultural identity. The Scollons argue that changes in a person’s discourse patterns—for example, in acquiring a new form of literacy— may involve changes in identity. For example, Athabaskans differ from many mainstream Canadian and American English speakers in how they engage in communication. They have a high degree of respect for the individuality of others and carefully guard their own individuality. Thus, they prefer to avoid conversation except when the point of view of all participants is well known. On the other hand, English speakers feel that the main way to get to know other people’s points of view is through conversation with them. Furthermore, for Athabaskans, people in subordinate positions do not display their talents; rather they observe the person in the superordinate position. For instance, adults as either parents or teachers are supposed to display abilities and qualities for the child to learn. However, in mainstream U.S. society, children are supposed to show off their abilities for teachers and other adults. Anglo-Canadian and American schools have adopted a model of literacy based on the values 141 Discursive Theory of essayist prose style that is problematic for many Athabaskans. The essay took on this role in history (in fact, in the seventeenth century) and the role of the essay is changing in current times under pressure from new media and technology. In essayist prose, the important relationships to be signaled are those between sentence and sentence, not those between speakers, nor those between sentence and speaker. For a reader this requires a constant monitoring of grammatical and lexical information. With the heightened emphasis on truth value rather than social or rhetorical conditions, comes the necessity to be explicit about logical implications. A further significant aspect of essayist prose style is the fictionalization of both the audience and the author. The reader of an essayist text is not an ordinary human being, but an idealization, a rational mind posited by the rational body of knowledge of which the essay is a part. By the same token, the author is a fiction, since the process of writing and editing essayist texts leads to an effacement of individual and idiosyncratic identity. For the Athabaskan, writing in this essayist mode can constitute a crisis in ethnic identity. To produce an essay would require the Athabaskan to produce a major display, which would be appropriate only if the Athabaskan was in a position of dominance in relation to the audience. But the audience, and the author, are fictionalized in essayist prose and the text becomes decontextualized. This means that a contextualized, social relationship of dominance is obscured. Where the relationship of the communicants is unknown, the Athabaskan prefers silence. The paradox of prose for the Athabaskan then is that if it is communication between known author and audience it is contextualized and compatible with Athabaskan values, but not good essayist prose. To the extent that it becomes decontextualized and thus good essayist prose, Athabaskans are less likely to seek to communicate through this method. The Athabaskan set of discourse patterns are to a large extent mutually exclusive of the discourse patterns of essayist prose. The Scollons describe how Athabaskans use words, actions, interactions, values, and beliefs to enact and get recognized as a certain type of Native American or aboriginal people. This is a Discourse in Gee’s sense. Of course, many Athabaskans can enact and recognize other socially situated identities; that is, engage in other Discourses. On the other hand, schools want students to use words, actions, interactions, values, and beliefs to enact and get recognized as “educated” or “literate” people. This also is a Discourse (one that has, like all Discourses, changed through time). The Scollons point out that at the level of socially situated identities these two Discourses can conflict with each other. Furthermore, the Scollons argue that the Discourse of school-based literacy is connected to an order of discourse, in Fairclough’s sense, that is a set of different, but related practices with print, for example, essays, reports, stories, literature, and so forth. However, in this order of discourse one form—the essay—stands out as the paradigm instance of the values behind schoolbased literacy (which is why the Scollons refer to school-based literacy as “essayist literacy”). Instead of the terms “discourse” or “Discourse,” other theorists have used the term culture. However, culture has a great many different meanings. The terms discourse and order of discourse for Foucault and Fairclough are meant to single out the ways in which texts and institutions set limits to meaning at particular times and places, often across various cultural groups in a society. The term “Discourse” for Gee is meant to single out the ways in which people at all different levels integrate words, actions, interactions, values, beliefs, and the use of objects, tools, and technologies to enact and recognize multiple, changing, sometimes conflicting, socially situated identities, some of which are “culture like” and others of which are not (e.g., a member of the Green Party may be affiliated with people across the world via the Internet without sharing much culture with these other people). James Paul Gee References Fairclough, Norman. 1995. Critical Discourse Analysis. London: Longman. Foucault, Michel. 1981. History of Sexuality, Vol. 1. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. Gee, James P. 1996. Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in Discourses. 2d ed. London: Taylor and Francis. Graff, Harvey J. 1987. The Legacies of Literacy: Continuities and Contradictions in Western Culture and Society. Bloomington: University of Indiana Press. 142 Discussion Scollon, Ronald, and Suzzane W. Scollon. 1981. Narrative, Literacy, and Face in Interethnic Communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. the role that social interaction with others plays in shaping thought. In addition, Mikhail Bakhtin’s dialogic principle shows that as students share ideas within a discussion, these ideas are not only combined but are transformed to provide new understandings that are greater than the sum of the individual contributions. Thus, discussion can effectively act as a tool for critical thinking, provide students with the chance to socially construct meaning, and allow teachers to observe, analyze, and shape students’ learning in meaningful ways. In addition to aiding students in the development of thought, discussion serves as a motivator for students’ learning. This motivational aspect occurs, in part, because discussions allow students to contribute their own ideas and beliefs, interact more substantively with peers, and receive instant feedback about these same contributions. Actively participating in the learning process and receiving quality responses from peers and teachers are important conditions that play a key role in learning. Although other conditions are equally important, the ability to participate in a discussion as a learning activity and to receive feedback that is constructive but nonthreatening is essential. As noted, effective discussions are planned in advance, yet they are flexible. This dynamic balance between planning and flexibility ensures that discussions are used thoughtfully as tools for learning and that they are efficient and focused. This balance ensures that teachers adapt to individual students’ needs and to the direction that learning processes dictate during discussion sessions. Planned discussions require that teachers identify instructional purposes for the discussion, analyze texts or activities to determine what content is amenable to discussion, select strategies that match the goal and content of the material or problem to be discussed, organize how students will work together in small groups or as a whole class while still allowing some student choice in these matters, work with students to learn appropriate discussion interactions patterns, and design evaluation tools that can be used before discussions commence, during the activity, and afterward. These evaluation tools should include ways for teachers, individual students, and peers to assess not only the process of learning during discussion but also what was learned and by whom. Discussion Teachers have long used discussions as an instructional tool to promote students’ active processing of ideas to improve learning. Although discussion serves a number of purposes, the most compelling reason for its use is the direct connection between discussion and thought. Traditionally, teachers have used discussion as a way to impart information and to assess what students have learned from the presentation, previously read texts, or activities students engaged in within larger classroom lessons. For example, what many teachers refer to or enact as a discussion is often a teacher-directed conversation that follows the Initiate-Respond-Evaluate format (IRE). In such cases, teachers typically initiate a question or topic, students respond, and teachers evaluate such responses. Students are not given the opportunity to socially construct meaning as they might in authentic conversations or dialogues, nor do they learn to analyze and solve problems or offer evidence for their responses (Alvermann, O’Brien, and Dillon, 1990). In contrast, effective discussions are characterized as planned activities in which students contribute at least half of the talk, interact with other peers rather than solely with the teacher, and present multiple views with exchanges longer than the two- to three-word responses typical of the IRE format. Wilbert McKeachie (1986) noted that effective discussions allow students to critically analyze their own and other group members’ logic, rationale, and evidence associated with particular stances toward a topic. In this process, students learn to use the ideas and products of group interactions as they formulate their own viewpoints and support for such stances. Moreover, because discussions are generated and supported through language, students learn to use language to generate thought—a crucial component of learning. This concept is directly supported by Lev Vygotsky’s stance, which characterizes discussions as opportunities to not only use language to foster thought but to see what students are thinking and how they process ideas and to understand 143 Discussion Students and teacher involved in a discussion (Elizabeth Crews) points of view or increase awareness of a perspective, or to actively engage students in identifying a problem and the processes and actions required to solve it. To initiate a discussion, it is important that both teachers and students be adequately prepared. A requisite knowledge base from which to begin might include readings, activities, experiences, and a variety of other sources other than the teacher. Students need to draw upon these resources; how well students process these preliminary sources impacts directly upon the quality of discussions. Hence, teachers need to see discussions as part of larger instructional planning, including what occurs prior to and after the session proper. Further, the appropriate selection of strategies to guide discussions is crucial. Teachers may select graphic aids that present central ideas or questions that students may use to prepare for discussion sessions. These same aids may be used to amend or expand ideas during interactions or after the discussion. Structures such as these enable open discussions and Teachers have struggled to plan effective discussions, primarily due to a lack of understanding of the components and characteristics that make discussions work. Claude Goldenberg (1992–1993) asserted that there are no explicit steps that teachers should follow to ensure successful discussions because student learning is less clearly defined and organized during these events. Nevertheless, instructionally beneficial discussions do have common characteristics, including teachers who: pose questions that do not readily have answers, are responsive to student contributions and seek to keep the conversation connected, design and maintain challenging yet nonthreatening environments, and promote broad student participation, for example, by allowing students to select when they contribute to the interactions. The achievement of goals and the success of a discussion are attributable to differences in discussion formats. For example, the goal of the discussion may be to clarify ideas, to grapple with a particular issue, to allow students to see multiple 144 Discussion contributions, focusing students’ efforts and ensuring that ideas presented are supported with relevant evidence instead of generalizations or emotional arguments. James Dillon (1984) proposed alternatives to the teacher’s asking questions, which often lead to IRE sessions. Instead of this, teachers are encouraged to use the following techniques: make a declarative or factual statement to start a discussion, make a reflective statement related to a previous comment, describe a student’s state of mind or the reasons that student might have for contributing particular statements to the conversation, seek student elaboration on a statement made by another peer, encourage students to ask questions generally or ask questions of one another, and maintain a deliberate silence to encourage reflection. Teachers have also used the devil’s advocate strategy to help generate discussions and explore various perspectives on an issue or point of view. The strategies outlined are ones that promote active learning and student talk; they also promote comprehension of texts. McKeachie (1986) asserted that when teachers select the format needed for a discussion, their choice also directly affects the type of discussion that ensues and who controls it. For example, teachers are often unwilling to relinquish control of the discussion to allow students to socially construct meaning. Rather, control is maintained through the selection of content, the pace of the interactions, and teacher selection of which students can contribute and which cannot. Thus, goals that are solely teacher-centered reflect discussion activities that are teacher-centered as well. Likewise, student-centered goals reflect activities that allow students to control the content and flow of discussion. Note that teacher-centered discussions are usually ineffective because teachers are not using discussion as a vehicle for social construction of meaning. Clearly, the actions of teachers within a discussion are critical. Teachers are required to step out of their traditional roles, in which they often dispense information and evaluate learning, and assume a collaborative role with students. For example, in the revised role, teachers do not control the content and flow of discussions but rather facilitate actions, refocus the conversation, and possibly clarify issues when required. Often, teachers determine that they need to take a more active role with difficult texts or when communi- cation between peers breaks down. During these moments, teachers scaffold students’ responses, guiding them to richer contributions and making connections that were not readily apparent to the students. Also, teachers who successfully conduct classroom discussions are able to build on students’ prior knowledge and make efforts to draw all students into the discussion, helping to curb incidents in which a few students monopolize the discussion. Key to involving all students as effective contributors during discussion activities is the teacher’s role in helping students learn effective discussion-interaction patterns. Students do not naturally learn appropriate discussion interactions. Rather, they require support from teachers who can help them analyze the course of the discussion when roles are taken up by particular group members in useful or in less effective ways, also finding ways to include all students. Teachers can also support student learning by modeling effective discussion actions or strategies; demonstrating how to monitor interactions to determine who is seizing power, silencing others, or monopolizing interactions; and helping students reflect on which students are not actively engaged in conversations and why. The interactions between the teacher and students and between peers are crucial to whether discussions promote learning. Assessment is key to ensuring that discussions are used to enhance student learning and that students have positive experiences during interactions. What has historically made discussion sessions challenging is the teacher’s ability to develop ways to assess learning, to take these assessments and use them in future planning, and to use the findings to show what students have achieved. In planning discussions, teachers and students must consider all possible outcomes and contributions prior to the discussion, as well as appropriate follow-up activities that will help assess the effectiveness of the discussion and students’ learning. Teachers will want to consider both individual and group assessments; equally important are self-assessments and peer assessments. All assessments should focus on content elements (Gambrell and Almasi, 1996) as well as on the processes that facilitated learning. Evaluating the actions or strategies used by students during discussions allows teachers to scaffold students’ learning about the purposes of discussions and the actions that allow them to work ef145 The Discussion Web fectively and efficiently. In addition, Karen Evans (2001) noted that content-element assessments could be generated by thinking about the literacy skills and strategies that students need to develop and creating grids of indicators that document these learnings. The grids can be used in a routine manner, with students being regularly observed during discussions and the evidence of their learning recorded, accompanied by noting areas for growth. Similarly, teachers can develop tools for students to use in order to assess their own learning, the processes they use, and their peers’ contributions. Considerable coaching will be required to help students move beyond surface-level analyses of their own and their peers’ contributions toward focusing on substantive interactions and products. Effective discussions are possible when teachers consider essential components that must be addressed, including careful planning, opportunities for constructive and ample student talk, and ongoing assessment of the interactions and contributions of all participants. When the conditions are optimal, discussion sessions offer students the experience of using language to generate thought and to create new ideas. An additional benefit to using discussions is that students learn in a way that they find meaningful and motivating. Deborah R. Dillon and Kerry A. Hoffman REASONS NO YES CENTRAL QUESTION CONCLUSION Figure 1. Discussion Web The Discussion Web The Discussion Web is a graphic aid embedded in a discussion format that encourages students to look at opposing viewpoints before determining their own conclusions. It gives all students the ability to form opinions and share them with a peer. Too often, classroom discussions involve the teacher and only a few willing participants. The Discussion Web, which can be used across grade levels and content areas, enables students to work initially in pairs as they work out their shared response to a question posed by the teacher. The Discussion Web was created by Donna Alvermann, who combined the WebOutline created by James Duthie (1986) and the think-pairshare technique by Jay McTighe and Frank Lyman (1988). The Discussion Web is formatted as a dialogue that teaches students to efficiently discuss topics presented in texts. A central question posed to the students appears in a box in the middle of a handout page; the students write their opinions, representing one side or the other of the issue, on the lines provided on either side of the box. After oral discussion in small groups, they record their final conclusion. There are several steps to a Discussion Web. First, students think individually about the ideas they want to contribute to the discussion and then discuss these ideas with a partner and reach consensus on their opinions. Next, the partners pair up with a different set of partners to work toward a consensus by eliminating any inconsistencies and contradictions in their own thinking. Then, the two sets of partners, working as a group of four, come to a decision and join with another group of four. Finally, the group of eight decides which ideas a spokesperson from the group will share with the entire class in the whole-group discussion that follows. See Also The Discussion Web; Gender and Discussion; Graphic Aids References Alvermann, Donna E., David G. O’Brien, and Deborah R. Dillon. 1990. “What Teachers Do When They Say They’re Having Discussions of Content Area Reading Assignments: A Qualitative Analysis.” Reading Research Quarterly 25:296–322. Dillon, James T. 1984. “Research on Questioning and Discussion.” Educational Leadership 42:50–56. Evans, Karen S. 2001. Literature Discussion Groups in the Intermediate Grades: Dilemmas and Possibilities. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Gambrell, Linda B., and Janice F. Almasi, eds. 1996. Lively Discussions! Fostering Engaged Reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Goldenberg, Claude. 1992–1993. “Instructional Conversations: Promoting Comprehension through Discussion.” Reading Teacher 46:316–326. McKeachie, Wilbert J. 1986. Teaching Tips: A Guidebook for the Beginning College Teacher. 8th ed. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. 146 Distance Learning By talking with partners and pairs of partners prior to engaging in whole-class discussions, students have multiple opportunities to interact (Alvermann, 1991). The Discussion Web allows students to defend their point of view with details from the text that support their opinions. At first, many students may need guidance from the teacher, along with the structure of the Discussion Web. Later, students may be able to devise their own topics and manage their own discussions. The Discussion Web allows those who are typically less prone to talk in whole-class discussions, particularly females (who generally aren’t called on enough in a teacher-led discussion), to develop their opinions and feel confident discussing them in small groups and then sharing within the larger context of the classroom. The Discussion Web offers a symmetrical discussion for both males and females; it gives students confidence by requiring all of them to base their opinions on evidence provided in the text. The Discussion Web incorporates all four of the language arts (reading, writing, speaking, and listening). It can function as a postreading strategy or as a prereading or prewriting device. It allows students to work alone, in pairs, in small groups, and then as part of a whole-class interaction. Adaptations can be made to use the Discussion Web in math with word problems, in science with experiments, and in social studies for debating viewpoints. Its flexibility for use across a wide variety of other subjects and grade levels makes it a usable tool for any curriculum. Gretchen Morrison do not have to be physically present at the same location as the instructor. Thus, learning may take place at one or more remote sites. The two basic modes of instructional delivery are asynchronous and synchronous. Synchronous refers to instruction in which all participants must be active at the same time. Synchronous delivery systems are often referred to as “real-time” delivery systems. Asynchronous learning does not require the simultaneous participation of the instructor or the participants. In asynchronous learning, students can choose the time during which they wish to receive instruction. Asynchronous learning is often referred to as “anytime, anywhere” learning. Although a key aspect of distance-learning environments is that students do not have to be physically present, there are additional distance-education classes that are conducted in tandem with face-to-face instruction. In this manner, the distance learning takes place as an adjunct to traditional class instructional modes. This additional instructional time can be either asynchronous or synchronous. A key element of the use of all such technologies is the ability to enhance communication between a teacher and learners. Delivery Systems The original forms of distance-learning delivery systems were correspondence courses in which students were given tasks to perform; their work was then evaluated by an instructor and returned to them. The instructional vehicle was via either print text or television. In either case, the mode of communication and learning was one-way, that is, student to teacher with no interaction from others. Distance learning today tends to be more interactive, and the path of communication is most often between a teacher and several students as well as among students. Current delivery systems are built on several technological options. There are four basic forms of such technologies: video-based, audio-based, databased, and computer-based. Each has a different degree of interactivity, depending on the manner in which it is delivered. All can be conducted either in a synchronous or asynchronous forum. Video-based delivery of instruction can be conducted through broadcast television, closedcircuit transmissions, and videotape. It often involves only one-way transmission. When video- See Also Discussion References Alvermann, Donna. 1991. “The Discussion Web: A Graphic Aid for Learning across the Curriculum.” Reading Teacher 45:92–98. Duthie, James. 1986. “The Web: A Powerful Tool for the Teaching and Evaluation of the Expository Essay.” The History and Social Science Teacher 21:232–236. McTighe, Jay, and Frank Lyman. 1988. “Cueing Thinking in the Classroom: The Promise of Theory-Embedded Tools.” Educational Leadership 45 (7):18–24. Distance Learning Distance learning involves students participating in an instructional delivery system in which they 147 Distance Learning based instruction takes place in a synchronous environment, if multiple-mode communication is desired, it is necessary to have audio hookups that allow students to communicate with each other and with the instructor. When instruction is conducted in an asynchronous environment, interaction can take other forms, such as communication via e-mail or listservs. Recently, many schools have developed means for two-way interactive video hookups by using cameras, microphones, and monitors. This two-way interactive learning necessitates the connection via satellite dishes, cable connectivity, or fiber optic linkages to connect multiple sites. Many feel that this very closely replicates face-to-face instruction. Audio-based delivery systems are easier to transmit since they are less costly and more easily reach a wide audience. Little special equipment is necessary for the transmission of information to the student. The modes of delivery center on either radio broadcasts or audiocassettes. Audio communication is also being advanced via the Internet, with radio broadcast capabilities that extend well beyond the traditional radio broadcast frequencies. Radio broadcasts mandate synchronous learning, whereas audiocassettes can be used in asynchronous learning contexts. Radio is a one-way communication medium in which only the instructor communicates with the learner. Increasingly, the use of audio teleconferencing is used to counteract the one-way communication dilemma. In audio teleconferencing, individuals or groups at multiple sites can interact. In a literacy program, for example, teachers can use this technology to arrange for students to “meet” an author of a book they are reading with little more technology necessary than using a conference-call option. Databased technologies are designed to teach or manage specific aspects of instruction. Computer-assisted instruction (CAI) is the term used when a hardware system or software program helps deliver instructional content. It helps the teacher teach and helps the student learn. Applications for literacy classes are many. Word processing is an example of a basic, fundamental mode of CAI practice in a literacy program. It is vital for students to read, write, and develop good communication skills. Writing with a computer facilitates text modification and is often found to encourage written communication that might not otherwise take place. Databases, a form of CAI, are designed to help the user generate files that may contain hundreds of pieces of information, store it on a disk, and arrange and sort it. Students can use databases to catalog books they’ve read and cross-reference them by genre, theme, author, or many other categories. Software programs that help students draw or chart are valuable additions to a literacy program, as are presentation packages that help foster outlining skills, information processing, and overall communication ease. Kenneth Hinze (1989) and Edward Tufte (1990) have investigated the use of graphics as an aid in conveying meaning and enhancing communication and have found them to be beneficial. Computer-managed instruction (CMI) is a means for instructors to manage the instruction delivered to and worked on in their classes. CMI can be useful to traditional face-to-face instructors and also to those using distance-education technologies. Computer-based technologies can provide instruction and also be an adjunct to audio and video telecommunication. With additional hardware and software such as modems, a microphone, speakers, and an individual video hookup, they can provide desktop video conferencing. Computer-mediated communication (CMC) refers to using the computer to mediate communication between and among instructor and students. E-mail, computer conferencing, and Internet-based communication make up the most common CMCs. Computer conferencing is a text-only asynchronous technology that permits conferencing between two or more individuals. The information is maintained on a web site, and like conversation, messages are shown in threads. Students can read what others have written and either respond or not. Computer conferencing is similar to a “chat” room (chat rooms are like telephones, except that words are written rather than spoken), but the communication does not occur in “real” time. Additional modes of interclass communication can involve listservs or newsgroups. A listserv is a mailing list in which topic-specific information is sent to the participants. This information is sent to all who subscribe to the listserv, and these participants can respond (referred to as “posting”) to all on the list or to individuals on the listserv. Newsgroups are also topic specific, but unlike listservs, a user does not have to subscribe to the newsgroup and it is open to all who wish to access the group. 148 Distance Learning Implications of Distance Learning for the Student and the Instructor There is a limited body of research on the effectiveness of distance learning. Thus far, research suggests that the most successful distance learners are those who voluntarily seek further education, have post-secondary education goals with expectations for higher grades, and are highly motivated and self-disciplined. When distance education is combined with face-to-face instruction, students seem to benefit more. Several key themes pervade the current body of research regarding distance learning from a student perspective, including: the need for interaction among students, the need for student-teacher interaction, and the role of this pedagogy on student learning. These and other studies also point out that good distance-learning practices follow traditional teaching practices, and the conditions that influence effective instruction may be common across different environments and populations (Russell, 1992). The implementation of distance education in both K–12 schools and in higher education can be a very costly and controversial endeavor. Audio telecommunications involve minimal implementation with low costs, but more advanced modes of delivery such as two-way interactive communication are much more costly. Regardless of the technology, distance education involves a great deal of planning on the part of the instructor and often requires almost three times the planning time of traditional face-to-face instructional delivery. The planning time decreases with repetition of courses. The curriculum often needs to be redesigned, and special activities need to be created in order to facilitate active participation by the learners. Teachers need to be taught about the use of this technology and have to learn how to use it in the most effective way. Teachers must be able to assist those learners who encounter problems with the learning context and with the technological aspects of the learning environment. Interactive web sites and distance-learning environments can provide powerful additions to classes. It has been reported that less than 10 percent of teachers are actively engaged in the use of these resources, and those who do use them tend to be very experienced with computers in general. Particularly in literacy, the use of distancelearning environments can be beneficial. To access information via distance, particularly if the mode of instruction is web-based, participants must be able to communicate effectively. Thus, the importance of reading and writing is paramount to access the content or participate in discussions in a chat room or in a threaded discussion. The rationale and need for expertise in reading and writing thus becomes more meaningful and relevant, especially to reluctant readers. Trade-book publishers offer abundant materials that can be integrated into distance-learning environments. Numerous authors have web pages with interactive components that can then be incorporated in a distance-learning environment by a teacher. In this manner, teachers “share” expertise with others, and students benefit from the knowledge that is shared by other instructors. Educators can enhance their regular teaching with the inclusion of some of the elements of distance learning. Students can find e-pals (electronic pen pals) to learn about other schools, countries, or cultures. Teacher-education students in college classes can be paired up with students in elementary or high-school classes to form e-pal connections with benefits to both constituencies. Students studying a particular country might interview other students from that country via e-mail and thereby learn much more about the mores and culture. Scientific experiments can be conducted at a distance, with students sharing and comparing data. As with all educational endeavors, the focus of distance learning should be on the effect on the learner. Meeting the instructional goals of the program and the instructional needs of the learner should be foremost. Good pedagogy is vital whether students are engaged in face-toface instruction or are learning at a distance. In both contexts, the principles of effective teaching are critical, but with distance-learning situations, instructors need to be even more organized, thoughtful, methodical, and mindful of students beforehand. Since feedback from learners is not as immediate as in face-to-face instruction, modifications in the instructional plan and in the tasks given to learners or the simulations presented may be made, but they will often occur at a slower pace. Educators, then, must be more proactive than reactive. The convergence of technology and education benefits and empowers learners in new and extraordinary ways. Educators and students are increasingly viewing learn149 Diversity ing at a distance as a viable means of instructional delivery. More and more demands are being placed on both learners and educators, and the lightning speed of access to information and communication via technology is having a positive effect on literacy education. Although distance learning is currently in its infancy, the potential and power of this mode of instruction is burgeoning. Carole S. Rhodes is considered the cultural and linguistic mainstream in the United States will continue to increase. The educational and literacy needs of culturally and linguistically diverse children in these groups may go unaddressed as they are encouraged to assimilate into mainstream discourse. It is imperative for educators to understand the connection between culture and literacy learning. Sonia Nieto (1999) has argued that the concept of culture was created by societies to account for the influence of a broad array of factors. She also noted that as a concept, culture is multidimensional and learned. Some literacy educators have been challenged to include the cultural, linguistic, and literacy experiences of children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds in the curriculum, instruction, and assessment. Understanding how to make meaning is at the heart of literacy. How people learn to make sense of their world, or learn to make meaning, is affected by the culture and language into which they were born. Literacy theories and strategies that are effective with mainstream groups will not magically work as well with culturally and linguistically diverse children. Literacy theories, methods, materials, and assessment techniques must be informed by the research of cultural and linguistic insiders whose area of expertise has identified culturally responsive and respectful approaches to literacy education. Moreover, to improve the academic achievement of children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds, educators must acknowledge the disparities of the past, the discursive practices of the present, and the need to affirm and value individual and group cultural and linguistic differences. The diversity of students in today’s literacy classrooms underscores the importance of developing curricula, teaching strategies, and policies to help all students succeed in school. Efforts to welcome and understand all students—and to treat their cultural and linguistic backgrounds as equally valid and important—are paramount in literacy classrooms. Effective literacy instruction builds upon the cultural and linguistic backgrounds, ways of making meaning, and prior knowledge that each child brings into the classroom. Such instruction acknowledges the indivisibility of culture in language and literacy learning. Understanding and respecting the array See Also Computer-Assisted Instruction; Critical Media Literacy; Early Literacy Software; Electronic Jigsaw; Hypertext; Instant Messaging; Software for Older Readers References Hinze, Kenneth. 1989. “PC Datagraphics and Mapping.” Social Science Computer Review 7(1):72–75. Rhodes, Carole S. 1998. “Multiple Perceptions and Perspectives: Faculty/Students Responses to Distance Learning.” Proceedings from the Ninth International Conference of the Society for Information Technology and Teacher Education. Norfolk, VA: Association for the Advancement of Computing in Education. Russell, Thomas. 1992. “Television’s Indelible Impact on Distance Education: What We Should Have Learned from Comparative Research.” Research in Distance Education (4) 4:2–4. Tufte, Edward. 1990. Envisioning Information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press. Diversity Diversity refers to differences among groups as well as individuals. In the late twentieth century, many educators used the term to acknowledge ethnic/racial, linguistic, and economic differences. The term diversity has also been used in reference to differences of gender, age, religion, intellect, and sexual orientation. Schools and school curricula have not always welcomed, affirmed, or valued diversity as equally valid, important, or desirable as mainstream experiences. The literacy needs of children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds are becoming increasingly important in the new millennium as more children of color enter schools. The rapidly shifting demographics of schoolage children and projections for the future both suggest that the enrollment of children who are culturally and linguistically different from what 150 Diversity Classroom of racially diverse students (Elizabeth Crews) nicity or racial backgrounds of students in public elementary and secondary schools as follows: 63.5 percent are European Americans (non-Hispanic), 17 percent are African Americans (nonHispanic), 14.4 percent are Latino/a Americans, 3.9 percent are Asian Americans or Pacific Islanders, and 1.2 percent are American Indian or Alaska Natives. Based on 2000 U.S. Census data, it has been estimated that school-age children will be compsed of the following racial/ethnic groups: 49.6 million Whites, 10.9 million African Americans, 12.3 Latino/Latina Americans, 2.5 million Asian Americans, and 840,000 Native Americans. Within each ethnic/racial group there are commonalties of language and culture as well as distinct dialectal, religious, class, and geographical differences. Georgia Garcia and Arlette Ingram Willis (2001) have observed that there are many differences between and within each racial category identified by the U.S. Census Bureau. In addition, Ogbu (1987) has argued that the type of entry into the country, whether voluntary or involuntary, and a group’s educa- of different cultures and languages represented in classrooms helps educators adopt strategies for teaching literacy that encourage and support student achievement. Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Learners The U.S. Census Bureau has recognized five major racial groups and asks respondents to selfidentify as: White, Black or African American, American Indian and Alaska Native, Asian, or Native Hawaiian and Other Pacific Islander. People who identify as being of Hispanic or Latino/a origin may be from any race that is part of the Spanish culture. Respondents who wish to selfidentify as being of mixed race, or multiracial, are given the option to claim “some other race,” with sixty-three possible choices. Children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds currently compose more than 36 percent of the total elementary and secondary U.S. school population. The social constructs of race and class are used by the National Center for Educational Statistics (2000) to estimate the eth151 Diversity tional history prior to entry are important elements to keep in mind. In the twentieth century, the largest group of people of color in the United States was African American, descendants from Africa, the West Indies, and Haiti. Currently, there are increasing numbers of Latinos and Latinas in schools. These students trace their cultural or linguistic backgrounds to Mexico, Puerto Rico, Cuba, and a variety of Caribbean and Central and South American countries. In addition, there has been an increase in Asian Pacific Islander groups, mostly Hawaiians but also including Pacific Islanders, Samoans, and Guamanians; Southeast Asians, from Vietnam, Thailand, Cambodia, Laos, Burma, and the Philippines; and East Asians, from China, Japan, and Korea. Many recent immigrants from all groups differ significantly from former immigrant groups in their levels of education, literacy, social class, religion, and geographical homeland. They may also have differences in communication patterns, expectations, and sense of time (Haung, 1993). Children from 280 different Native American nations are enrolled in public, private, and tribal schools. Students in these schools may differ in terms of their language, traditions, economics, and social interactions. Finally, the 2000 census created opportunities for individuals to acknowledge more than one racial category. Not surprisingly, millions of Americans elected to self-identify as biracial (with each parent being from a different racial background) or multiracial (with generations of mixed-race forebears). Educating children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds and from mixed-race backgrounds requires understanding the distinction between cultural group identity and individual identity. John Berry (1986) has encouraged us to remember that group and individual acculturation may differ significantly. Therefore, it is imperative to acknowledge and address difference, while not generalizing notions of difference to each individual. requisite for school success. In a similar manner, Nieto (1999) has written that students’ language and culture may often interfere with schooling because they do not conform to the expectations of schools. Several literacy researchers have maintained that there are specific teacher knowledge bases that are necessary to acquire before instructing children in culturally and linguistically diverse classrooms. A teacher’s culture, language, social interests, goals, cognition, and values—especially if different from the students’—could conceivably create a barrier to understanding what is best for children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds (Orange and Horwitz, 1999). This barrier—what Rosalinda Barrera (1992) has identified as a “cultural gap”—affects how teachers respond to children of different cultures. Teachers can break through this barrier by reflecting on their self-knowledge and by learning to acknowledge and respect the languages, literacies, literatures, and cultural ways of knowing of their students. When teachers become aware of their own cultural backgrounds and values, they have an opportunity to recognize and address biases, those preconceived notions that make it difficult for them to accept, understand, and effectively teach their students. It is also important for literacy teachers to acknowledge and challenge ideologies and policies that privilege one cultural way of knowing, language, or literature over others. There are numerous studies that have documented examples of the relationship between culture and literacy development and examples of best practice for a variety of cultural groups: Native Hawaiian communities, families, and children; African American and European American children and their families; inner-city African American families; and many Native American nations. Cultural and linguistic differences may include the forms of questions that children are asked as well as expectations about when and how to respond to questions about the uses of reading and writing or the styles of oral narration. There may be differences in language use and patterns of interactions between adults and children that differ between diverse cultural groups and the mainstream. There may also be culturally specific rules or expectations with regard to behavior and interpersonal interactions along gender lines. Literacy Teachers in Diverse Classrooms Multicultural proponent Geneva Gay has observed that schooling has been narrowly defined and drawn from one cultural way of knowing to the near exclusion of all others, forcing some culturally and linguistically diverse children to relinquish their cultural ways of knowing as a pre152 Diversity Understanding each child’s culture will influence the way teachers frame literacy instruction and create and use assessment. Literacy instruction should explicitly build upon the cultural knowledge, ways of making meaning, and prior knowledge that children bring with them to the classroom. In this manner, children feel that their culture, language, and literature are important and valued at school. ing. Similarly, Eileen Craviotto and Ana Heras (1999) have identified characteristics of culturally relevant classrooms: (1) using families as resources, (2) reading multicultural literature, (3) regarding students as active learners, (4) emphasizing classroom dialogue, (5) providing opportunities for exploration, and (6) using multiple languages in the classroom. The authors stress that these strategies can enhance students’ literacy learning. The literacy needs of children from diverse backgrounds cannot be divorced from the importance of addressing the linguistic, emotional, and psychological needs that accompany literacy learning. Researchers have observed that children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds, as well as immigrant children, often seek to develop relationships of caring and respect with classroom teachers. The long-term cumulative effects of cultural insensitivity may be hard to measure using traditional forms. The literacy curriculum can be an important venue to help students understand the relationships among culture, power, and literacy. There is no one best way to teach all students; instead, a variety of instructional strategies should be incorporated. Diversity training should be a continuous process requiring a long-term commitment because building cultural knowledge and sensibilities is a lifelong proposition. According to feminist Adrienne Rich (1979), students and teachers should enter into an agreement whereby they will all do their best to respect, educate, and learn from each another. Arlette Ingram Willis Culturally Informed Teaching Literacy approaches that appear to support the language and literacy of children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds must be coupled with teacher knowledge and commitment. If not, the result is a “tourist approach” that focuses on celebrating holidays and festivals, glorifying heroes or exceptional people, and adding culturally sensitive and appropriate literature. These shallow approaches assume that the inclusion of multicultural materials is all that is needed to address diversity in school settings. Multicultural materials and activities alone are insufficient for social change. In order to address issues of cultural and linguistic difference, substantive changes must be made to the curriculum and instruction. Literacy studies have shown that students are more academically successful when they feel welcomed, valued, and challenged by material that builds upon their prior knowledge, experiences, and interests. Dorothy Strickland (1998) identified several characteristics of relevant literacy instruction for culturally and linguistically diverse students: (1) variability exists within and across linguistic and cultural communities, (2) a student’s conceptual framework and background experiences are critical to literacy learning, (3) both learning and teaching are enhanced when context is acknowledged and used, (4) the use of language for real communication enhances learning, (5) the use of materials and experiences to which student can relate helps establish and expand their conceptual framework, and (6) a focus on high-level thinking and problem solving is critical for all children. She also emphasized the existing variability across students’ home communities, the construction of meaning from different perspectives, the acknowledgment of context in literacy learning, the use of language for real communication, the use of relevant literacy materials, and a focus on high-level thinking and problem solv- See Also Ebonics; Multicultural Literature References Barrera, Rosalinda. 1992. “The Cultural Gap in Literature-Based Literacy Instruction.” Education and Urban Society 24 (2):227–243. Berry, John. 1986. “Multiculturalism and Psychology in Plural Societies.” In Lars Ekstrand, ed., Ethnic Minorities and Immigrants in a Cross-Cultural Perspective, pp. 35–51. Berwyn, NY: Swets North America. Craviotto, Eileen, and Ana Heras. 1999. “Cultures of the Fourth-Grade Bilingual Classroom.” Primary Voices 7 (3):25–35. Garcia, Georgia, and Arlette Willis. 2001. “Frameworks for Understanding Multicultural Literacies.” In Peter Mosenthal and Patricia Schmidt, eds., Reconceptualizing Literacy in the 153 Dynamic Assessment teaching and learning that chronicles students’ development. Age of Pluralism and Multiculturalism. Vol. 9, Advances in Reading/Language Research, pp. 3–32. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Gay, Geneva. 1994. A Synthesis of Scholarship in Multicultural Education. Urban Monograph Series. Oak Brook, IL: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory. Available: http://www.ncrel.org/sdrs/areas/issues/educatrs/ leadrshp/le0gay.htm. Haung, Gary. 1993. “Beyond Culture: Communicating with Asian-American Children and Families.” ERIC Digest (on-line). Available: http://www.ed.gov/databases/ERIC_Digests/ed366 673.html. National Center for Educational Statistics. 2000. Digest of Educational Statistics: 1999. NCES 2000–031. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement. Nieto, Sonia. 1999. The Light in Their Eyes: Creating Multicultural Learning Communities. New York: Teachers College Press. Ogbu, John. 1987. “Variability in Minority Responses to Schooling: Non-Immigrants vs. Immigrants.” In G. Spindler and L. Spindler, eds., Interpretive Ethnography of Education, pp. 225–280. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Orange, Carolyn, and Rosalind Horowitz. 1999. “An Academic Standoff: Literacy Task Preferences of African American and Mexican American Male Adolescents versus Teacher-Expected Preferences.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 43 (1):28–39. Rich, Adrienne. 1979. “Claiming an Education.” In On Live Secrets and Silence: Selected Prose, 1966–1978. New York: W. W. Norton. Strickland, Dorothy. 1998. “Principles of Instruction.” In Michael Opitz, ed., Literacy Instruction for Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Students: A Collection of Articles and Commentaries, pp. 50–52. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Theoretical Framework The dynamic approach to assessment reflects constructivist theory and offers a more authentic expression of current cognitive-developmental theory than traditional standardized procedures (Lidz, 1995). Constructivists espouse that students create meaning by connecting what they know and have experienced with what they are learning. They construct meaning through these connections when educators pose relevant problems, encourage student inquiry, structure learning activities around primary concepts, value students’ points of view, and assess student learning in the context of the teaching. Because of the continuous, ongoing nature of dynamic assessment, it is viewed not as a separate event but rather as a natural, purposeful component of the educational process. Students are assessed naturally within the context of lessons and activities. Teachers then analyze student performance to inform future practice (Brooks and Brooks, 1993). From a theoretical perspective, Lev Vygotsky’s thinking embodies the essence of dynamic assessment: elements of social transaction and discourse, contextual embeddedness, and creation of a zone of proximal development (Lidz, 1995). The social nature of the learning process affords students and teachers frequent, inherent opportunities to negotiate meaning in multiple contexts. Contextual embeddedness encourages participants to view assessment as an integral part of the teaching and learning process. The zone of proximal development offers a context for scaffolded learning experiences at students’ instructional levels. Vygotsky’s work suggests that assessment practices that focus entirely on the child’s unaided performance fail to tap important information that can be identified by analyzing the child’s dynamic performance in the zone of proximal development, in which the child responds with adults or more experienced learners. Opportunities to assess in such scaffolded contexts, in which the student responds with adults or more experienced peers, reveal both the learner’s fully matured and emerging abilities. Such emerging abilities may not be evident without the scaffolds provided by more competent sources (Minnick, 1987). Dynamic Assessment Dynamic assessment is a more authentic alternative than the traditional practice of focusing on products to assess student knowledge. It is characterized as dynamic because it is interactive, ongoing, and focused on process. It is distinctive because it evinces the responsiveness of the learner, offering not a snapshot but an ongoing view of student performance. It demonstrates that assessment is not an educational add-on but a natural, continuous component of 154 Dyslexia Dyslexia Role in Portfolio Assessment Dynamic assessment is characteristic of the types of measures often compiled to show growth over time in portfolio assessment. Such assessment is goal based, authentic, multifaceted, and reflective, a continuous process that chronicles development. It is the ongoing nature of dynamic assessment that demonstrates its value in documenting the learning process, as opposed to using a onetime, summative assessment to document a learning product. A completed story or research report, written using the various steps in the process, is an appropriate summative or product assessment to support such a goal. In contrast, dynamic assessments are formative in nature and delineate the process in which the student engaged to create the product. Evidence of such ongoing assessment might include the student’s prewriting activity, first draft, peer-review comments, teacher observations, and revisions. These dynamic assessments also offer both the students and the teacher more insightful views of learning. For example, knowing how well they performed in various stages of the process facilitates future goal setting for students and gives direction to the teacher’s future planning. Robert Tierney (1998) values the alignment of dynamic assessment with classroom practices. He suggests that assessments should emerge from classrooms rather than be imposed upon them. The formative nature of dynamic assessment contributes to this alignment because the measures used are often more informal (see Portfolios). Maureen McLaughlin Dyslexia, the most common type of learning disability, refers to an inability to acquire functional reading skills despite the presence of normal intelligence, exposure to adequate educational opportunities, and motivation. Dyslexia affects an estimated 5 to 10 percent of the school-age population (Feifer, 1998). Dyslexia is a complex, multifaceted syndrome characterized as a learning disability that impairs the ability to read words. A prototypical person with dyslexia could be described as having an underlying neuropsychological deficit in basic reading skills that has led to secondary problems with reading comprehension, written expression, and math computation (Padget, 1998). Dyslexia has been studied since the beginning of the twentieth century and was referred to as word blindness or congenital word blindness. Although over seventy terms have been used synonymously for the condition—for example, developmental reading disability or specific reading disability—dyslexia is the historically preferred term. A variety of neurobiological mechanisms related to dyslexia, including analysis of specific brain regions, hemispheric shifting, and genetic predisposition, have been proposed as the cause of the condition. Subtle anatomical and functional deviations in the brain correlate with specific types of reading disorders. Analysis of specific brain regions associated with dyslexia have focused on neural systems serving language primarily in the perisylvian cortex in the left hemisphere. Findings based on electrophysiological studies, regional cerebral blood-flow profiles, positron emission tomography (PET) studies, and postmortem examinations have shown deviations in the cortex and underlying temporal lobe dysfunction, as well as anatomical lesions in people with dyslexia. Hemispheric shifting, based on the specific strategy used by readers to decode words, appears to be at the forefront of most research involving cerebral processing and dyslexia (Feifer, 1998). Clinical studies have led to the emergence of two distinct subtypes of dyslexia: surface dyslexia and phonological dyslexia. Surface dyslexia is characterized by reading totally by sound, that is, an overreliance on a phonological route in which words can be decoded sequentially using grapheme to phoneme conversions, but words are not recognized on the basis of See Also Authentic Assessment; Portfolios; Scaffolded Literacy Instruction References Brooks, J. G., and M. G. Brooks. 1993. In Search of Understanding: The Case for Constructivist Classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Lidz, Carol S. 1995. “Dynamic Assessment and the Legacy of L. S. Vygotsky.” School Psychology International 16:143–153. Minnick, Norris. 1987. “Implications of Vygotsky’s Theories for Dynamic Assessment.” In C. S. Lidz, ed., Dynamic Assessment: An Interactional Approach for Evaluating Learning Potential, pp. 116–140. New York: Guilford. Tierney, Robert J. 1998. “Literacy Assessment Reform: Shifting Beliefs, Principled Possibilities, and Emerging Practices.” Reading Teacher 51:374–390. 155 Dyslexia Dyslexic student and teacher (Associated Press/The Tennessean) cent of children who have a parent with reading disability have the same difficulty. When a child is identified as dyslexic, there is a 40-percent chance that one or more siblings will also be dyslexic (Lyon, 1998). Kathleen McCoy meaning or by accessing the semantic lexicon. People with surface-level dyslexia have poorly developed sight vocabulary. In contrast, people with phonological dyslexia have an almost total inability to apply grapheme to phoneme rules. They produce errors for derivational paralexias (e.g., running for run) and read content words better than function words (e.g., nouns better than adjectives) (Newby, Recht, and Caldwell, 1993). Oral reading indicates that they can comprehend the general meaning of a word but cannot pronounce it correctly (e.g., girl is pronounced she, and money as dollar). Surface-level dyslexia may result from an overreliance on the left hemisphere, thus suppressing the right hemisphere to detect perceptual features of the text. Conversely, phonological dyslexia may be an overreliance on the right hemisphere, thus suppressing the phonetic strategies of the left hemisphere. Investigations leading to identification of subtypes and the causes of dyslexia remain one of the most significant and persistent problems in the field of reading for researchers and practitioners. Genetic evidence for some types of reading disabilities with deficits in phonemic awareness is emerging. One of the most important risk factors is family history. An estimated 23 to 65 per- See Also Delayed Readers References Feifer, Steven G. April 1998. “Neurological Features of Dyslexia.” Paper presented at the Thirtieth Annual National Convention of the National Association of School Psychologists, Orlando, FL. ED 421 810. Lyon, Reid G. 1998. “The NICHD Research Program in Reading Development, Reading Disorders and Reading Instruction: A Summary of Research Findings. Keys to Successful Learning: A National Summit on Research in Learning Disabilities.” New York: National Center for Learning Disabilities. ED 430 366. Newby, Robert F., Donna R. Recht, and JoAnne Caldwell. 1993. “Validation of a Clinical Method for the Diagnosis of Two Subtypes of Dyslexia.” Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 11 (1):72–83. Padget, S. Yancey. 1998. “Lessons from Research on Dyslexia: Implications for a Classification System for Learning Disabilities.” Learning Disabilities Quarterly 21 (2):167–178. 156 E read to frequently; others, only occasionally. Still others have received some direct instruction from their caregivers rather than only informal assistance. Similarly, some children express great interest in literacy, learning about letters and words on their own, in contrast to other children who are more content with building blocks and play. This suggests that some children already exhibit some of the skills associated with literacy achievement when they enter the primary grades but that others will need more intensive instruction. A central goal during the preschool years is to enhance children’s exposure to and concepts about print (International Reading Association/National Association for the Education of Young Children, 1998). In the course of being read to by their parents or caregivers, children learn to distinguish many print features. They learn that print (rather than pictures) carries the meaning of the story, that the strings of letters between spaces are words and in print correspond to an oral version, and that reading in English progresses from left to right and from top to bottom. Children also learn about print from the labels, signs, and logos they see around them. Highly visible print labels on objects, signs, and bulletin boards in classrooms demonstrate the practical uses of written language. These everyday experiences expose children to print in a variety of forms and to its functions in many different contexts. A fundamental insight developed in children’s early years is the alphabetic principle, the understanding that there is a systematic relationship between letters and sounds. Through instruction, children learn that the alphabet comprises a limited set of letters and that these letters stand for the sounds that make up spoken words. Children learn about the shapes of letters by distin- Early Literacy Early literacy is defined as the beginning forms of early reading and writing that become increasingly conventionalized over the early years between infancy and age eight with high-quality instruction. Today, unlike in previous decades, it is acknowledged that even in the first few months of life, children begin to experiment with language. In the midst of gaining facility with oral language and through interactions with others, children acquire the insight that specific kinds of marks on pages—print—can also represent meaning. At first, children use the physical and visual cues surrounding print to determine what something says. But as they develop an understanding of the match between speech and print, they begin to process letters, translate them into sounds, and connect this information with a known meaning. These understandings represent the roots of early literacy development. For many children, the beginnings of literacy appear in activities such as pretend play, drawing, conversations about books with their closest relatives, their family. Parents serve as models, provide rich experiences, and offer help and encouragement to their young children. They engage them in day-to-day activities where they see, use, and experience the purposes of print and its use in daily living. Literacy development is said to begin in these relationships, and it becomes extended and elaborated through quality instruction in the preschool, kindergarten, and primary years. There is considerable variation in patterns of early literacy development. Some children come to the primary grades having encountered a wide range of home-based literacy experiences, whereas others do not. Some children have been 157 Early Literacy guishing one character from another according to spatial features. Teachers often help children to differentiate letters visually and involve them in comparing letter shapes. Alphabet books and alphabet puzzles in which children can see and compare letters may be a key to efficient and easy learning. By kindergarten, children can discern these letter shapes with increasing ease and fluency. Children’s proficiency in letter naming is a well-established predictor of their end-of-year achievement, because it mediates the ability to remember sounds. At the same time, children begin to learn about the sounds of language through exposure to games, nursery rhymes, and word games. Research by Morag MacLean and her colleagues (1987) indicates that knowledge of nursery rhymes specifically relates to the more abstract phonological knowledge later on. According to the National Reading Panel report (Neuman, 2000), children’s ability in phonemic awareness has been shown to strongly relate to later reading achievement. Phonemic awareness refers to a child’s understanding and conscious awareness that speech is composed of identifiable units, such as spoken words, syllables, and sounds. Children develop a great deal of knowledge of the alphabetic system through their beginning attempts at writing. A classic study by Charles Read in 1971 found that even without formal spelling instruction, preschoolers use their tacit knowledge of phonological relations to spell words. Phonic spelling refers to beginners’ use of the symbols they associate with the sounds they hear in the words that they wish to write. For example, a child may initially write b or bk for the word book, to be followed by more conventionalized forms later on. Although children’s phonic spelling may not comply with correct spellings of words in the beginning, the process allows them to think actively about letter-sound relations. As children engage in writing, they are learning to segment the words they wish to spell into constituent sounds. During these early years, children’s vocabulary develops at a rapid pace. Vocabulary increases through listening to stories. Children, therefore, need to be exposed to vocabulary from a large variety of book genres, including informational texts as well as narratives. In addition, it is widely recognized that some explanation of vocabulary prior to listening to a story is related significantly to children’s learning of new words. For example, David Dickinson and Miriam Smith (1994) found that asking predictive and analytic questions before and after the readings produced positive effects on vocabulary learning and comprehension. Repeated readings appear to further reinforce the language of the text and familiarize children with the way different genres are structured. Understanding the forms of informational and narrative texts seems to distinguish those children who have been read to well from those who have not. In one study, for example, Christine Pappas (1991) found that after multiple exposures to a story (three readings), children’s retelling became increasingly rich, integrating their knowledge about the world, the language of the book, and the message of the author. Thus, considering the benefits for vocabulary development and comprehension, the case is strong for interactive storybook reading. Increasing the volume of children’s playful, stimulating experiences with good books is associated with accelerated growth in reading competence. Best Practices in Early Literacy Quality programs in early literacy build on a set of research-based principles about how young children learn and develop (Neuman and Roskos, 1998). They emphasize the importance of integrated learning, motivation, teaching skills in content-rich settings, and high levels of teacher guidance. These principles form the foundation for classroom organization and management, instructional decisionmaking, and the selection of learning experiences to promote children’s capabilities. Children’s Learning Benefits through Integrated Instruction Effective teachers use integrated learning to organize large amounts of content into meaningful concepts. Since integration is more efficient than teaching subjects in isolation, integrated learning provides more time and opportunity for repeated practice with familiar concepts. Further, children are likely to learn and apply skills, increasing the likelihood of their interest and motivation. Skillful teachers recognize that thematic instruction must have coherence and depth. Cafeteria-style approaches that teach a little of this 158 Early Literacy and a little of that give only spotty attention to content and make only limited connections between subjects. Instead, effective teachers specify what is to be learned in each subject area in order to ensure that young children gain sufficient knowledge and mastery of skills. dependent and make possible the “art and science” of effective teaching. Play Supports Children’s Learning Effective teachers recognize that the exploration and manipulation of objects, make-believe play, and games make important contributions to children’s development. In play, children express and represent their ideas, learn to interact with others, and practice newly acquired skills and knowledge. Teachers provide conditions to affect what children will choose to play and which materials will influence how they play. They construct learning and playful environments that involve children in literacy and other symbolic activities. At times, teachers take on roles and actively engage children in language activities that are first imitated, then expanded upon, and later integrated into children’s developing language repertoire. These teachers seek to enhance language and play while leaving children in control of it. Learning Requires That Children’s Minds (Not Just Their Bodies) Be Active From the very beginning, education in a democracy must allow children not only to acquire knowledge but also to make reasoned decisions and choices. Effective teaching actively engages children in mastering both content and learning processes, helping children connect new learning to what they already know and can do. Effective teachers strike a balance between structure and choice in their instructional planning. Sometimes, teachers present a concept or a skill that is planned and directed to ensure that knowledge is thoroughly understood, not superficially absorbed. At other times, they recognize that children need to explore, manipulate, and use ideas, working in centers of their choosing that have been carefully prepared with teacher guidance. Both are necessary for young children’s learning and development. Developing Competence Enhances Motivation and Self-Esteem Rather than directly teaching “self-esteem,” effective teachers recognize that learning experiences and practices that help children to become skillful at doing many things are far more effective than those designed to be highly motivating or “cute.” Children thrive in classrooms where they develop real friendships and are in the company of teachers who combine nurturance and support with high, but realistic, standards and expectations. Self-esteem grows when children are challenged and begin to develop a history of achievement through reasonable effort. These instructional principles that engage children in learning skills in contentrich contexts, play, integrated across subject domains with high levels of teacher support and guidance, provide opportunities for all children to achieve while ensuring that individual children will receive the extra support they need to progress. As children’s capabilities develop in these early years and they become more fluent readers, instruction will turn from a central focus on helping children learn to read and write to helping children read and write to learn. Increasingly, the emphasis will be on engaging children to become independent and productive in their reading, helping to extend their reasoning and comprehension abilities in learning about their world. Susan Neuman High Levels of Teacher Interaction Optimize Children’s Learning Teachers are greatly influential in helping children to reach their potential. They assist and guide children’s learning, involving them in experiences that are slightly more difficult than what they can master on their own. Scaffolding is an especially descriptive term for understanding how teachers enable children to move toward higher levels of learning, with the level and amount of assistance gradually decreasing as the children become able to perform tasks independently. Teachers encourage children to express their ideas through language and raise questions that enable them to develop more complex ideas and concepts. Effective teachers work on the edge of children’s current competence, providing learning experiences that are challenging but achievable. These teachers use a wide range of teaching strategies. Modeling and demonstrating provide standards of practice; explicit instruction, questioning, and ongoing feedback help to challenge and expand children’s ideas and skills. All of these strategies are inter159 Early Literacy Assessment settings. The effects of parenting and environmental events were largely ignored. The belief was that children were not “ready” to read until they were six years old or so. Assuming average physical development, a child would then be more capable of visual demands such as matching letters and determining directionality and of auditory demands like hearing the small differences between sounds. Reading, some believed, consisted of a bottom-up hierarchy in which a child had to learn letters and sounds before being challenged by the printed word, only then moving on to text and eventually to meaning. Thus, assessments have reflected that ideal and measured reading readiness, a hierarchy of skills including visual and auditory discrimination, letter recognition, color recognition, and the ability to compare same and different shapes. Significant research since the 1970s reflects a paradigm shift. Literacy skills, both reading and writing, are now seen to be intertwined, beginning with infants’ hearing and reproducing phonemes, or sounds, that represent speech. The speech/language match to print is an amazing development that occurs as parents read to their children on a regular basis and as children are exposed to print in their environment. Toddlers can recognize the written and pictorial symbols that represent McDonald’s, Pizza Hut, and Target. In our society, rich with print and language, children are exposed to a variety of literacy experiences before beginning formal schooling. This exposure to the form and function of print, it is currently believed, forms the basis for literacy learning. Emergent literacy, then, reflects the paradigm on which the content of many current assessments of early literacy are based. The International Reading Association cautions against the use of high-stakes assessments for young children. First of all, a formal testing situation may put undue pressure on a child, resulting in inadequate and invalid results. Literacy skills and abilities develop at a differential rate. Many children find it very hard to sit still and focus on one task for a long period of time. In short, one must use the testing results with an eye toward the assessment situation; although the results may be helpful, the information may lack some amount of validity. It is also true that testing offers a picture of one student on one day in one situation. Results may vary accordingly on a different day and in a different setting. See Also Literacy in Play; Phonological and Phonemic Awareness References Dickinson, David, and Miriam Smith. 1994. “LongTerm Effects of Preschool Teachers’ Book Readings on Low-Income Children’s Vocabulary and Story Comprehension.” Reading Research Quarterly 29:104–122. International Reading Association and National Association for the Education of Young Children. 1998. “Learning to Read and Write: Developmentally Appropriate Practices for Young Children.” Reading Teacher 52:193–216. McLean, Morag, Peter Bryant, and Lynette Bradley. 1987. “Rhymes, Nursery Rhymes, and Reading in Early Childhood.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33(3):255–281. Neuman, Susan B. 2000. Report of the National Reading Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Neuman, Susan B., and Kathleen Roskos, eds. 1998. Children Achieving: Best Practices in Early Literacy. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Pappas, Christine. 1991. “Young Children’s Strategies in Learning the ‘Book Language’ of Information Books.” Discourse Processes 14:203–225. Read, Charles. 1971. “Pre-School Children’s Knowledge of English Phonology.” Harvard Educational Review 41:1–34. Early Literacy Assessment The purpose of educational assessments, including those that measure the progress of emergent and beginning readers, is to gather data. Areas of both strength and need can be determined in order to make effective instructional decisions based on these data. Through both standardized and informal assessments, educators can determine a child’s growth toward independent reading and writing. The assessment of emergent literacy generally refers to preschool and kindergarten students who are exploring and experimenting with reading and writing. Beginning readers are often in late kindergarten through first or early second grade. Therefore, the age measured by early assessments spans a continuum from approximately age three through seven. A paradigm shift in the 1970s and 1980s reflects the assessments that have widely been used by educators. Prior to this time, learning to read was viewed as a process fixed in formal school 160 Early Literacy Assessment Using both standardized and informal assessments, educators can determine a variety of factors that represent a child’s growth toward independent reading and writing. When selecting early literacy assessment, educators must be purposeful: What do I need to discern? Which instrument can satisfy that aim? (five to eighteen hours of instruction), are taught in small groups, and are combined with letter instruction. Exposure to and success with various aspects of phonemic awareness is one predictor of success in learning to read. Recognizing the letters of the alphabet and the sounds they represent is another predictor of success in learning to read. The alphabetic principle also means that children recognize that patterns of letters make up words. The accuracy with which children can name letters is important, but equally important is the automaticity with which children can read letters. A child who can recognize letters with speed and confidence will have an easier time learning about letter sounds and word spellings. Another important and assessable developmental aspect of emergent literacy is the child’s concepts about print. Marie Clay (1993) explains that over a period of time, children gradually learn a great deal about the printed word. Children observe how adults handle books as they read. Environmental print is also a source of opportunity for incidental print learning. Teachers need to understand what children already know about print and what they have yet to sort out about our printed language. Concepts about print include understanding that print carries meaning and knowing the difference between the front, back, top, and bottom of a book, where to begin reading, in which direction to read (left to right, top to bottom), and that letters make up a word and that words make up sentences. Children need to realize that pictures enrich and accompany text. More advanced concepts might include knowing the difference between capital and lower-case letters, realizing that sentences end with different marks, knowing that capital letters begin a sentence, and understanding what quotation marks mean. In addition, some children might be able to recognize and write some basic sight words as well as their given and family names. Aspects of Early Literacy Development Learning to read and to write begins when young children hear and mimic language. They are curious and begin to experiment with sounds and with communication. Parents, caregivers, and family members are the child’s first teachers. Children repeat the sounds they hear, gradually approximating acceptable, understandable speech. Literacy begins at birth and develops at differing rates; it begins with communication and language. The early language play that children engage in, such as rhyming and repetition, becomes an important basis for the development of reading. This is phonological awareness, the understanding that our language is made up of sounds. Knowing that these sound segments, or phonemes, are used to build words develops along with language and sound play. This is known as phonemic awareness. Phonemic awareness is the notion that words are made up of a specific sequence of sounds, or phonemes. Some level of awareness of phonemes is necessary for children to use as a basis for learning to read print (Adams et al., 1990). Phonemic awareness does not develop spontaneously in all children; it eludes those from about one-fourth of all middle-class homes and substantially more from backgrounds that are less literacy rich. Because of this, phonemic awareness needs to be encouraged and, often, directly taught. Activities that encourage the development of phonemic awareness include: rhyming, letter-sound matching, segmentation, blending, deletion of sounds, and manipulation of sounds. This list is not sequential, and success at all tasks is not a prerequisite of reading. The National Reading Panel (2000) examined fiftytwo articles on literacy and determined that phonemic awareness instruction has a significant, positive effect on reading and spelling, and is most effective when one or two different activities are followed, not more. Programs are most effective when they are not too long or too short Aspects of Beginning Reading Children grow in reading and writing at their own pace; the process is developmental in nature. Some come to formal schooling with more experiences and more knowledge about print than others. In general, most of this proficiency is attributed to experiences at home, preschool, and kindergarten. In first and second grade, children 161 Early Literacy Assessment expand and enrich their literacy concepts. Their oral language has developed exponentially as they acquire words for more complex topics and ideas. Children also develop their sight word recognition, usually with words that are familiar and in their speaking vocabularies. In addition, as they become more familiar with letters and sounds, children further develop word-recognition strategies (using sounds, context, and so on). Phonics, the relationship between sounds and symbols, is a tool that students can use to enable them to decode words. When basic rules and principles of letter-sound relationships are understood and applied to unknown words, reading and understanding are facilitated. Teachers need to realize which of these orthographic rules are known and, further, which need to be taught. An understanding of phonics can supply students with an additional, valuable tool with which they can unlock words and enhance text meaning. When they can recognize enough words to read connected text, students move toward reading text with a narrative or expository thread. Comprehending text is an important part of beginning reading. As students develop into proficient readers, getting meaning from text read both orally and silently is the ultimate goal. Comprehension is the essence of reading. Therefore, teachers need to assess whether a student understands what is read. and Representing Phonemes with Letters. Students use a booklet and pencil. Administration of all sections takes about half an hour (Adams et al., 1990). Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) The following four tasks, each with several forms, were designed to be a brief screening device to determine which children might be slow to develop early literacy skills and which might benefit from early intervention. These timed, oneminute probes are individually administered and can also be used repeatedly in different forms to measure the effectiveness of specific interventions. Letter Naming Fluency (kindergarten through early first grade) measures how many randomly ordered upper- and lower-case letters a child can name in one minute (forty to sixty is considered proficient). Phonemic Segmentation Fluency (kindergarten through first grade) measures a child’s ability to segment orally presented words into phonemes. Accurately producing thirty-five to forty-five phonemes per minute is considered adequate. Nonsense Word Fluency (first grade) consists of a nonsense word–reading task to determine whether a child applies letter-sound correspondence to decode words (thirty to forty letter sounds is considered adequate). Onset Recognition (kindergarten) determines how many stimulus sounds a child can associate with a named picture in one minute (Curriculum-Based Network, 1999). Assessment Instruments The following instruments can be used by educators who understand their use and can effectively apply and interpret the resultant data. They represent a selection of published materials available to educators. Additional instruments, such as informal-reading inventories, attitude and interest surveys, or state- and district-constructed assessments might also aid in gaining information about the progress of emergent and beginning readers. Early Literacy Assessments This collection offers a wide range of assessments for measuring early literacy abilities. Several short, appealing activities are designed to gain a profile of an emergent reader. Educators need to choose which assessments might help them gain the most information about a child. Included are: Interviews about Reading Attitude and Interest, Retelling a Story, Literacy Knowledge (concepts about print), Wordless Picture Reading, Auditory Discrimination, Rhyme Detection, Alphabet Knowledge, Phoneme Segmentation, Writing, Developmental Spelling, Consonant Phonic Elements (initial and final), Decoding, Caption Reading, Basic Sight Word Knowledge, and Passage Reading (Johns, Lenski, and Elish-Piper, 1999). The Assessment Test The Assessment Test is a part of Phonemic Awareness in Young Children, which is a complete, concise curriculum for the instruction of phonemic awareness in kindergarten and firstgrade classrooms. Components of this group-administered test include Detecting Rhymes, Counting Syllables, Matching Initial Sounds, Counting Phonemes, Comparing Word Lengths, 162 Early Literacy Assessment Early Reading Screening Instrument (ERSI) The ERSI is a series of tasks that focus on beginning readers’ print-related ability. It was designed to work as a screening of late kindergarten and early first-grade students to predict students who would benefit from early reading intervention programs. The individually administered tasks include the following: Alphabet Knowledge measures recognition of upper- and lower-case letters in a variant sequence; production of letters is also assessed. Concept of Word in Text measures a beginning reader’s ability to match spoken words to written words; given memory support and a short rehearsal, a student must finger point to text and read a sentence. Concept of Word in Text measures a student’s ability to match spoken words to text; a student must locate a key word in text that has just been read. Phoneme Awareness is measured in this battery by a twelve-word spelling task; students are encouraged to use their letters and sounds to produce a written stimulus word. Developmental spelling may produce a mixture of correct and incorrect phonemes; scoring word by word, the total number of correct phonemes and patterns or errors provides predictive and diagnostic information (Morris, 1999). Concepts about Print (CAP) are tested, including identifying the front of the book and understanding that print carries meaning, that clusters of letters are called words, that there are both upper- and lower-case letters, that spaces have meaning, and that ending punctuation gives meaning. Two little books, which also act as forms of CAP, are titled Follow Me, Moon and No Shoes (Clay, 1993). Word Test is based on the idea that beginning readers need a base of sight words that are the most frequent. The Ready to Read series is used during the first year of schooling in Auckland, New Zealand. Writing Sample rates writing samples taken from beginning writers. Clay has urged teachers to rate samples relative to language level, message quality, and directional principles. Writing Vocabulary measures how many words a child can write in ten minutes. Prompts are allowed. Scoring is based on correct spelling, directionality, series of words, and correct use of capitals. Hearing Sounds in Words requires a child to write a sentence that is dictated. The child writes the sounds that are heard. Scoring is directed toward phonemic awareness (Clay, 1993). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III The PPVT-III is an individually administered, norm-referenced test designed to measure the receptive vocabulary in students from age two and a half through adult. Subjects are asked to determine which of four pictures on an easel depicts the meaning of the stimulus word. There is no writing required on the part of the subject, and this test usually takes ten to fifteen minutes to administer. The authors determine the PPVTIII as predictive of verbal ability and, thus, of language. Success with language is frequently linked with success in reading. Scores are reported as a variety of derived forms: standard scores, percentiles, stanines (ranks), normal curve equivalents (NCEs), and age equivalents (Dunn and Dunn, 1998). Metropolitan Reading Instructional Tests This survey battery is designed for students in kindergarten through ninth grade. It is suitable for group use and is divided into six levels. The paper-and-pencil items that measure early reading skills include visual discrimination, letter recognition, auditory discrimination, sight vocabulary, consonants, and vowels (Farr, Prescott, Balow, and Hogan, 1986). An Observation Survey of Early Literacy Achievement This survey is a collection of standardized observational tasks for teachers to use to measure and monitor literacy progress for students at beginning first-grade level. Clay has suggested that these tasks be used in concert to gain a complete picture of a developing reader rather than separating one or two tasks to use as screening instruments. The survey tasks include: Running Record, which scores oral reading behavior to determine any pattern of miscues and to observe problem solving on the part of the child. Letter Identification includes upper and lower case. Screening Children for Related Early Educational Needs (SCREEN) SCREEN is an individually administered battery of tests designed to assess academic ability in children from three to seven years of age. SCREEN may be used to assess children who may exhibit risk factors for learning. The parts of this battery include: Test of Early Language Development 163 Early Literacy Assessment (TELD), which contains eighteen items that measure syntax, semantics, and the ability to use language. Test of Early Written Language (TEWL) assesses the mechanics of writing and also the expression of ideas. There are sixteen items. Test of Early Reading Ability (TERA) consists of eighteen items that measure concepts about print and retelling. Cloze items are also included. Test of Early Mathematics Ability (TEMA) measures rote counting, the use of a number line, basic number facts, and place value. There are eighteen items (Hresko et al., 1988). Yopp-Singer Test of Phonemic Segmentation This is an individually administered screening survey to determine a child’s ability to break a word into its separate phonemes. There are twenty-two items that are administered to kindergarten and first graders. There are no alternate forms or specific, rigorous norms. The author suggests that students who score eleven or better are likely to be phonemically aware. Students who cannot orally segment sounds from a given word may have difficulty learning to read and spell (Yopp, 1995). Peggy VanLeirsburg Marciniec Specific Level Assessment of Awareness of Print and Sound This is an informal measure of fifteen tasks that are related to letter-sound correspondence and print awareness. It also measures a child’s knowledge of letter sounds and the application of this knowledge to decode nonsense words. Specific Level Assessment of Awareness of Print and Sound is designed to use with kindergarten or first graders and also with students whose alphabet and print experiences may be lacking. There are no established scoring conventions, but the authors advise that students who are successful early readers are generally accurate with these basic skills (Howell, Fox, and Morehead, 1993). See Also Phonics Instruction; Phonological and Phonemic Awareness References Adams, Marilyn J., Barbara R. Foorman, Ingvar Lundberg, and Terri Beeler. 1990. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge: MIT Press. Clay, Marie. 1993. An Observation Survey of Early Literacy Achievement. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Curriculum-Based Network. 1999. “Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS).” Eugene: School Psychology Program, University of Oregon. Dunn, Lloyd M., and Leota M. Dunn. 1998. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance. Farr, Roger C., George Prescott, Irving H. Balow, and Thomas Hogan. 1986. Metropolitan Reading Instructional Tests. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Howell, Kenneth W., Sheila L. Fox, and Mada K. Morehead. 1993. Specific Level Assessment of Awareness of Print and Sound. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing. Hresko, Wayne, Donald Reid, Donald Hammill, Herbert Ginsburg, and Arthur J. Baroody. 1988. Screening Children for Related Early Educational Needs. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Johns, Jerry, Susan Lenski, and Laurie Elish-Piper. 1999. Early Literacy Assessments. Dubuque, IA: Kendall Hunt. Morris, Darrell. 1999. “Early Reading Screening Instrument (ERSI).” Best Practices in SpeechLanguage Pathology 2:43–51. National Reading Panel. 2000. Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching Children to Read. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Pub. no. 00-4769. Test of Phonological Awareness (TOPA) TOPA is a norm-referenced test of young children’s awareness of the phonological structure of words. There are two versions, one for kindergarten and one for early elementary. Each contains two subtests of ten items. Initial sounds must be matched in the first subtest. Children listen for the initial sound that is different in the second part (Torgesen and Bryant, 1994). Word Reading Efficiency and Nonword Efficiency This test is for students in kindergarten through twelfth grade. It is individually administered and diagnostic in nature. This screening provides a measure of the ability of students to apply phonic skills to increasing complex nonwords. They are asked to apply their knowledge of letter-sound relationships in pronouncing unfamiliar, nonwords without the aid of context clues. Analysis of errors can provide an indicator of strengths and needs in decoding ability (Torgesen and Wagner, 1998). 164 Early Literacy Software Child using a computer for learning (Laura Dwight) Foundations of Literacy Software Software that effectively supports young children’s acquisition of foundational literacy concepts, such as phonemic awareness, letter recognition, sound-symbol relationships, ABC order, and concept of word (Teale and Sulzby, 1989) are designed to go beyond the repetitive drilland-practice format of many of the lower-level programs that have dominated the marketplace. Effective software fosters children’s initial and ongoing acquisition of skills through playful engagement within gamelike scenarios that offer multiple occasions to explore literacy concepts in highly predictable and responsive screen environments. For example, in programs such as Blue’s Clues ABC Time Activities, children can playfully explore ABC order by following a letter maze in a sandbox, associate sound and symbol relationships by matching soapbubble pictures to sounds of initial consonants, and go on a vocabulary word safari while interacting with popular television characters. Spelling games, like Spelling Blaster, and typing games such as Read, Write, and Type may also help young children associate keyboard letters with sounds. Torgesen, Joseph, and Brian Bryant. 1994. Test of Phonological Awareness. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Torgesen, Joseph, and Richard Wagner. 1998. Word Reading Efficiency and Nonword Efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Yopp, Hallie Kay. 1995. Yopp-Singer Test of Phonemic Segmentation. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Early Literacy Software Computers and literacy-related software have become commonplace features in early childhood classrooms within the last decade (Becker, 1993). In 1999, hundreds of software programs involving reading were available commercially. Early childhood experts consider only a fraction of the software programs targeted for young children to be developmentally appropriate (Haugland and Shade, 1994). Developmentally appropriate early literacy software programs are designed to effectively support young children as they (1) acquire foundational literacy skills, (2) develop reading abilities, and (3) develop writing abilities. 165 Early Literacy Software Reading Software Software that effectively fosters young children’s development of reading abilities offers various levels of support for word recognition and comprehension of narrative stories, reference materials, and informational text. CD-ROM Talking Books are interactive, digital versions of stories that employ multimedia features such as animation, music, sound effects, highlighted text, and modeled fluent reading. For example, Dr. Seuss’s Green Eggs and Ham allows children to use the mouse to access words that are pronounced, passages that are reread, illustrations that are reanimated, and special effects that produce visual or auditory responses. The interactive features focus on word recognition, reading fluency, and multimedia support for story comprehension when animations are integrally related to the story (Miller, Blackstock, and Miller, 1994). Digital encyclopedias, such as Microsoft Encarta Multimedia Encyclopedia, and digital dictionaries, such as American Heritage Children’s Dictionary, provide convenient references with embedded links to auxiliary materials or additional information. Reference software can often be updated via the Internet. Support for content literacy (see Content-Area Literacy) or thematic studies may occur as children walk through virtual museums like The Louvre Museum for Kids, encounter biological concepts such as 3-D Body Adventure, or simulate excursions through space and time as in Madeline: European Adventures or Reading Galaxy. development of abilities to create multimedia documents that contain associated links. Highlighted text or portions of screens may open additional screen windows to video and audio streams, geographical maps, or related textual supplementary information. Educators of young children who are interested in learning more about selecting developmentally appropriate software can locate various reviews of early childhood literacy software online (see Tech Learning, available: http://www. techlearning.com/review.html). Linda D. Labbo and Jonathan Eakle See Also Content-Area Literacy; Hypertext; Software for Older Readers References Becker, Howard. 1993. “Decision Making about Computer Acquisition and Use in American Schools.” Computers and Education 20:341–352. Haugland, Susan, and Daniel Shade. 1994. “Software Evaluation for Young Children.” In June Wright and Daniel Shade, eds., Young Children: Active Learners in a Technological Age. Washington, DC: NAEYC Press. Miller, Larry, James Blackstock, and R. Miller. 1994. “An Exploratory Study into the Use of CD-ROM Storybooks.” Computers in Education 22:187–204. Teale, William, and Elizabeth Sulzby. 1989. “Emergent Literacy: New Perspectives on Young Children’s Reading and Writing Development.” In Dorothy Strickland and Lesley Morro, eds., Emerging Literacy: Young Children Learn to Read and Write, pp. 1–15. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Selected Examples of Foundational Literacy Acquisition Software: Baily’s Book House, Edmark, Redmond, VA. Blue’s Clues ABC Time Activities, Humongous Entertainment, Bothell, WA. Jump Start Kindergarten, Knowledge Adventure, Torrance, CA. Selected Examples of Reading Development Software Titles: Dr. Seuss’s ABC, Broderbund/The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Dr. Seuss’s Green Eggs and Ham, Broderbund/The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Just Grandma and Me, Broderbund/The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Stellaluna, Broderbund/The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Selected Examples of Writing Development Software Titles: Writing Software Software that effectively fosters young children’s development of writing abilities supports composing and shaping ideas on screen through various symbol systems. When composing, children may wish to draw pictures for prewriting or as an expression of early communicative symbol making (Teale and Sulzby, 1989). KidPix Studio Deluxe, a multimedia software graphics and paint program, allows beginning readers/ writers to initially experiment with graphics, digital photos, and special visual/audio effects. With the help of an adult, word-processing programs such as Microsoft Word can support children’s writing processes through formatting pages, editing invented spelling, and printing final drafts for publishing. Simple hypertext programs, such as Hypercard, support children’s 166 Ebonics KidpixStudio Deluxe (Broderbund/Mattell), The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Read, Write, and Type, The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Storybook Weaver Deluxe (MECC), The Learning Company, Novato, CA. Ultimate Writing and Creativity Center, The Learning Company, Novato, CA. where the term Ebonics was coined have suggested that the term was meant to repudiate the view that the language of enslaved Africans is based in English or resulted from a European-invented pidgin English. Notwithstanding these differences, adherents of all three perspectives agree that Ebonics is governed by a system of linguistic rules (grammatical, syntactical, morphological, pragmatic, and semantic) and paralinguistic (nonverbal) features. These unique features define the communicative competence of the descendants of enslaved Africans in diaspora, including the Caribbean and the United States, and incorporate the idiomatic expressions, social communication behaviors, and cultural mores of African people compelled to adapt to the conditions of enslavement throughout the world. African American Ebonics borrows the bulk of its vocabulary from English but preserves some of the grammar, phonology, and pragmatic structures of West African (Niger-Congo) languages. Ebonics Ebonics, a term coined by a group of African American scholars in 1973, combines two words—ebony, meaning black, and phonics, which refers to the science or study of sounds. Thus, literally, Ebonics refers to the science or study of black sounds. But how the term is defined operationally depends on the theoretical perspective the person defining it holds about the origin and historical development of this language. Is Ebonics an offshoot of English? Does Ebonics derive from a simplified pidgin language employed by participants in the Atlantic slave trade? Or does Ebonics have its origin in the languages of West Africa? Some scholars (English-origin theorists) maintain that Ebonics, referred to as black dialect, is no different from the white dialects of English and suggest that the underlying grammar of this black dialect is English. The differences between black dialects and white dialects are said to have resulted from enslaved Africans’ exposure to poor English models or to their impaired attempts to approximate good English models, or both. Another group of scholars (Creolists) view Ebonics as a system of communication used by enslaved Africans in America, which evolved from the simplified languages (pidgins) developed on the West Coast of Africa and in the Caribbean. Most Creolists equate Ebonics with such terms as Black English, Vernacular Black English, and African American Vernacular English. A third theoretical perspective holds that Ebonics is not an English dialect and that, therefore, terms such as black dialect, Black English, or African American Vernacular English are inappropriate. This African-origin theory posits that Ebonics is a new language rooted in African (especially Niger-Congo) languages. Indeed, Africologists present at the 1973 conference The Public View of Ebonics The public debate around Ebonics focuses mainly on whether this system of communication, which is used by nearly all African Americans at least some of the time (Smitherman, 1977), is a rule-based language or simply an amalgamation of errors resulting from the feeble attempts of African people to learn English. This debate has paid little attention to the extensive empirical linguistic research, yet American society has a very high level of tolerance for this misinformation. Decades ago, linguists established that African American Ebonics is systematic and rule governed like all natural languages and that speakers of this language should be viewed the same as speakers of any other language. Unfortunately, this linguistic knowledge base has penetrated neither public opinion nor the views of many educators, whose attitudes about how black children speak are a critical determinant of how black children fare in school. Ebonics and Learning Although the public debate centers on how Ebonics is viewed, the more critical pedagogical question concerns the impact it has on learning in American educational institutions. Perennially, the academic achievement of African Amer167 Ecological Literacy ican students has been disconcerting. The extensive use of Ebonics, which differs significantly from the language of school (Academic English), is widely recognized as a barrier to learning in African American students. This barrier is formed in part by the negative attitudes of educators toward Ebonics and in part by the structural differences between Ebonics and Standard American or Academic English. Teachers’ low opinions and misunderstandings about Ebonics contribute to the failure of many African American students in America’s public schools (Baugh, 1999). Because teacher attitudes and beliefs are critical variables influencing achievement in students, one of the greatest challenges facing teacher development institutions is the need to change negative attitudes toward Ebonics by building teachers’ knowledge of the linguistic research on Ebonics. How teachers respond to language difference in African American students has a significant impact on classroom instructional practice and student achievement. Although changes in educator attitudes are important, they represent only part of the puzzle. African American Ebonics is structurally different from Standard American English in significant ways. It differs phonologically in how sounds are formed and used to construct words, grammatically in how words and sentences are formed to carry meaning, and pragmatically in how language is used in social contexts. Therefore, speakers of Ebonics who possess limited access to interactive models of Standard American English need structured, research-based interventions that promote acquisition of Standard English. The shared vocabulary of Ebonics and Standard American English veils the complex linguistic differences (grammatical, phonological, and pragmatic) between the two language systems, thus masking the difficulty speakers of Ebonics have with Standard English acquisition. Reading and oral language are highly correlated, and how the two connect has significant implications for the process of becoming literate. Many problems related to language and literacy acquisition in speakers of Ebonics result from a lack of familiarity with the linguistic constraints of Standard American and Academic English. Speakers of Ebonics have difficulty distinguishing Standard English phonemes, which results in problems with sound-spelling relationships, loss of confidence in the alphabet, and differentiation of homophones, causing linguistically based reading and writing problems (Berdan, 1978). As the twenty-first century brings increased demands for language use that exceeds basic proficiency, how the language acquisition needs of students for whom Ebonics is native are addressed in primary- and secondary-learning institutions is critical to these students’ ability to access post-secondary educational opportunities and career options. We have an obligation to our future generations to move beyond uninformed public debates about Ebonics to acknowledgment of the three decades of linguistic research on the topic and the application of that knowledge to efficacy in the education of those who speak Ebonics. Noma LeMoine References Baugh, John. 1999. “Considerations in Preparing Teachers for Linguistic Diversity.” In Carolyn Adger, Donna Christian, and Orlando Taylor, eds., Making the Connections: Language and Academic Achievement among African American Students, pp. 97–114. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics and Delta Systems. Berdan, Robert. 1978. “Dialect Fair Reading Instruction for Speakers of Black English.” Paper prepared for the Sociolinguistics of Reading Session, Sociolinguistics Research Program, Ninth World Congress of Sociology, Uppsala, Sweden, National Institute of Education, Department of Health, Education and Welfare. Smitherman, Geneva. 1977. Talkin’ and Testifyin’: The Language of Black America. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Ecological Literacy Ecological literacy refers to the embeddedness of our human lives in cultural and physical contexts and the ways in which we interpret those contexts. Ecological literacy has been thought of in several different ways. The first of these, instead of limiting literacy to the skills required to read and write, uses ecology as a metaphor to demonstrate that literacy is a social endeavor (Barton, 1994). When we view literacy as deeply embedded in a social context, we are taking an ecological approach to literacy. We accept the understanding that the environment or culture in which literacy is practiced shapes the many forms of literacy and that literacy, in turn, shapes 168 Ecological Literacy Third grade students putting on a skit about conservation (Elizabeth Crews) pact of our way of life on the environment, we must also recognize a third conceptualization of ecological literacy—the development of our ability to read or interpret the world around us. One ecologist, David Orr (1992), has argued that ecological literacy should be taught in schools, but that this form of education should begin with developing students’ abilities to observe and interpret nature. He argued that without a sense of wonder and delight in the world of nature that surrounds us, no amount of learning about ecology will prevent us from destroying that world. What is needed is a sense of kinship with the world. Because we often do not recognize the embeddedness of our society in nature, we have done much damage to our planet. This sense of kinship is difficult to obtain, because education has been a primarily indoor activity, with fewer and fewer opportunities for direct experience with nature. Along with this sense of kinship with nature, we must learn, or perhaps relearn, how to understand and interpret our natural environment. the environment or culture in which it is practiced. A second conceptualization of ecological literacy frames it as the capacity to understand the connections between humans and their environment. Ecologists, concerned that education should include an understanding of the relationships between humans and the earth, have presented basic concepts that should be central to ecological literacy. Students should understand, for example, how humanity has changed the earth’s ecosystems, that certain relationships exist between organisms and their environment, and that the nature of ecosystems is changeable. Ecologists also advocate activism, mainstreaming environmental courses throughout school curricula and raising funds for environmental activities on campus as ways to promote ecological literacy. Along with the recognition that literacy is deeply embedded in a social context and that it is important for us as humans to recognize the im169 Economics of Literacy Development To explore ecological literacy more fully, in this sense of reading our natural environment, we must see literacy occupying a broader space than has traditionally been made for it, as an ability to read nonprint texts, including nature itself. This widening of the arena of literacy is supported by work done by the New London Group, a group of researchers and theorists that originally met in New London, New Hampshire, in September 1994. Drawn together from a variety of disciplines by their concern for transformations occurring in the personal, public, and working lives of people today, this group developed a pedagogy of multiliteracies that is broader than language alone and that allows for variation in different cultures and contexts (New London Group, 2000). Highlighting the increased diversity through globalization of English and the increasingly multimodal presence of communications technologies, the multiliteracies argument provides a flexible and openended structure for both analysis and pedagogy. The schools of Manitoba, Canada, have recognized the need to broaden the definition of literacy. In its 2000 Grade Three English-Language Arts curriculum, this district has added “the land” to possible sources that students may read in learning to manage ideas and information. In addition, students are encouraged to systematically observe and record information from their environment. The International Reading Association and the National Council of Teachers of English have also included reading nonprint text in their 2000 Standards for the English-Language Arts. A conceptualization of literacy that includes both print text and the world around us has great potential to deepen our understanding of literacy and to help us reestablish connections among ourselves and our environment. In a reciprocal and ecological literacy, we choose as our texts the world around us and open ourselves to being aware of and reading our environment. also interpretation of topographical maps, sign systems designed for navigation along the trail, weather, the gestures and movements of animals and humans, geographical formations, and their own bodies. These complex and multimodal literacies both fit into and expand upon the multiliteracies described by the New London Group (2000). Continuing to explore these multiple forms of literacy can help us develop a deeper, more complex understanding of literacy, with applications not only for our relationship with the environment but also for our educational practice of literacy in schools. Leslie S. Rush See Also Literacy in Informal Settings References Barton, David. 1994. Literacy: An Introduction to the Ecology of Written Language. Oxford: Blackwell. New London Group. 2000. “A Pedagogy of Multiliteracies: Designing Social Futures.” In Bill Cope and Mary Kalantzis, eds., Multiliteracies: Literacy Learning and the Design of Social Futures, pp. 9–37. New York: Routledge. Orr, David. 1992. Ecological Literacy: Education and the Transition to a Postmodern World. Albany: State University of New York Press. Rush, Leslie. 2002. “Multiliteracies of Appalachian Trail Thru-Hikers.” Ph.D. diss., University of Georgia. Economics of Literacy Development Economics of literacy development refers to three distinct but connected concepts: (1) the relationship between literacy proficiency and personal income, (2) the relationship between government support for literacy resources (schools, libraries) and community literacy levels, and (3) the relationship between the “price” of literacy resources and the amount that people read. Most people are used to thinking of the first of these relationships. Literacy development, it is commonly assumed, affects an individual’s economic future, that is, the ability to secure a good job and high wages. According to this line of reasoning, the literacy development of a given population will affect the economic health of its state or country. Although this is no doubt true to a large extent, it is the other two areas of intersection between economics and literacy—the promotion of Research in Ecological Literacy Unfortunately, little research has been done in the area of ecological literacy. In an effort to begin rethinking literacy in its embeddedness in culture and in nature, my dissertation (Rush, 2002) examines the ecological literacy practices of the Appalachian Trail thru-hiker community. For thru-hikers who backpack from Georgia to Maine, literacy involves not only written text but 170 Economics of Literacy Development literacy through economic policy and the analysis of reading behavior as an economic transaction—that are of greater interest to education practitioners and researchers. One of these topics has already generated a considerable amount of research, but the other has seldom been formally addressed in the scientific literature. school libraries), and community (bookstores, public libraries). Studies of home environment and reading frequency have consistently found that children who have plentiful access to books and other reading materials at home read more frequently. Such access is usually (but not always) associated with other parental behaviors that encourage literacy development, such as modeling reading, taking trips to the library and bookstore, and reading aloud to the child. More important, there is a small but growing body of research to demonstrate that experimentally increasing access to reading materials in the home can, in and of itself, lead to greater reading frequency. This type of intervention is, of course, somewhat rare due to the expense involved in physically supplying books to each child to take home, but the positive results so far suggest a causal relationship between the ready availability of a physical resource (books) and the use of that resource (reading) (McQuillan, 1998). Although the home provides a child’s first exposure to literacy, various national and international surveys have found that most schoolchildren report the school and public library as being the primary source of reading material (Krashen, 1993). This is true particularly of children from low-income communities, where families lack the financial means to purchase a large quantity of books. Not surprisingly, several studies have confirmed that as in the case of the home, increasing how much access children have to reading materials in their school and classroom libraries increases the amount of reading students engage in, as well as increasing their reading proficiency (McQuillan, 1998). This effect is also observed at the community level, with public libraries. Children and adults who live closer to the public library, for example, tend to use the library more than those who live farther away. This effect has remained strong even after the widespread adoption of the automobile. Similarly, other library-science researchers have found that the higher the quality of the public library (all other things being equal), the more people will use that library. Since the quality of classroom, school, and public libraries results directly from the resources provided to these institutions, the effect of a government’s economic policy on literacy development now takes a clearer form: Governments that spend more money on the “tools” of How Economic Policy Promotes (and Retards) Literacy Development The connection between economic policy and literacy development can be understood best by first thinking about the possible variables that affect growth in reading and writing proficiency. It is accepted by most researchers from both a “code” or skills-emphasis orientation, and by those with more “meaning” or literature-based approaches, that the more children read, the better they read. Although the debates over literacy instruction tend to be heated and contentious, almost all parties agree that getting students to read voluminously is the goal of good literacy instruction. This general relationship—that in effect, “practice makes perfect”— has been found to hold for a wide range of physical and cognitive skills. The best review of this relationship between volume of reading (sometimes referred to as “print exposure”) and literacy development can be found in Stephen Krashen’s appropriately titled book, The Power of Reading (1993). Krashen discusses correlational and experimental studies that have found that increased reading leads to higher levels of both reading and writing proficiency among children of all ages. If the amount of reading one does explains a good deal of the variation in how well one reads, how can we explain the differences in the volume of reading that children and adults do? There is no single answer to this question, but there is growing evidence that the way governments allocate funds to schools and public libraries affects whether schools and communities have high or low levels of literacy. The number of printed materials (books, magazines, comics, newspapers, and so on) in a reader’s environment influences reading frequency, which in turn influences reading proficiency. Just as you can’t learn to be a world-class cyclist without a bicycle, you cannot be a highly proficient reader without print. Access to reading material can come from three places: home, school (classroom and 171 Economics of Literacy Development literacy via library collections have a substantive impact on literacy development in a community. This leads to another important issue: How are such resources currently allocated? from local sources, such as local property taxes. As a result, low-income communities, with a weaker tax base, have much poorer public-library services than high-income communities. It is interesting to note that although many states in the United States now have “finance equalization” to ensure that poorer school districts receive an adequate amount of per-pupil funding, no such programs exist for public libraries. Further, some recent research suggests that it is precisely during the summer months—when the public library is the only free source of reading materials—that the gap between high- and lowlevel readers widens the most. Two caveats are in order to put these results in their proper context. First, income is not itself the key variable in predicting literacy achievement in these print-access studies. Income tends to serve as a rough proxy for print access, but the two are not identical. The amount of print access in the home, school, and community has a distinct and strong effect on literacy development independent of income, and this can be separated from general-funding levels, teacher-pupil ratios, and other educational resources. Unfortunately, children in low-income environments generally have poorer access in all three domains than their high-income peers. Second, it should be noted that physical access to reading materials alone is not thought in most cases to be sufficient for literacy growth. It is, however, considered a necessary condition without which no other form of assistance or influence (modeling, adult intervention) can succeed. Disparities in Print Access Due to Economic Policies Several recent local, national, and cross-national studies (McQuillan, 1998; Neuman and Celano, 2001) have documented that in all three domains (home, school, community), there are grave, and sometimes dramatic, disparities in literary resources between high- and low-income communities. Children from high-income families, for example, have been found to have up to 100 times more reading materials than their low-income peers. Schools in high-income communities similarly have been documented to have book-per-pupil ratios several times greater than schools in low-income communities. These disparities are particularly acute for language-minority students in the United States. Researchers have found that schools that serve a large number of speakers of languages other than English have few print resources in non-English languages, even when these schools have bilingual or dual-language programs. Such schools are, in effect, doubly hit by print deprivation, since they also tend to be located in poor communities, which themselves have a limited number of books and other reading materials in English. Public libraries also vary widely in terms of the number of books per capita available on their shelves. In one study, high-income communities were found to have three times more books than low-income communities in the same state. These differences were reflected by several indicators of library quality, among them the number of librarians available to provide services to the public. Susan Neuman and Donna Celano (2001) found that these disparities exist even within a single community, with middle-income areas of a city having better-quality library services than low-income neighborhoods. Most of these disparities are rooted in economic policy decisions. Governments and policymakers decide how to allocate scarce public resources and in what form. In the United States, some states have chosen to give generous support to their public-school libraries with tax dollars, whereas others have not. At the community level, public libraries tend to get most of their funding Literacy Development as Economic Activity Literacy development can both affect and be affected by economic policy on a local, state, and national level. There is a third sense in which we can consider the relationship between economics and literacy, one that to date has not been formally examined by reading and writing scholars but is quite common in analogous fields of leisure and recreation studies. That is to view the act of reading (leisure reading, in particular) as consumption of a product (reading materials), which has an attendant “price” determined by the forces of supply and demand. For more than two decades, researchers in leisure studies have studied the supply-and-demand functions of public recreation centers, complete with formulas to relate the distance and quality of parks to 172 Effective Schools and Teachers their amount of use by publics with different demographic characteristics. These studies have discovered that these “free” public resources do not, in fact, cost the same to all members of the community and that their use responds to typical market forces. Concerning recreation centers, for example, travel time is a price that people must pay because they forgo alternative uses of their time (including income-producing activity in the case of adults) and purchase the actual means of transportation (gas, bus fare) to the park site. More readily available parks decrease the cost of their use, so that those who live in a community with lots of parks and recreation centers pay a lower price. Similarly, the quality of recreation areas determines the value gained from accessing them. A high-quality recreation area (e.g., one with more lakes, tennis courts, and the like) has a lower cost to the user than a lowquality area does, since people gets “more for their money” by spending the time and energy to visit the high-quality park. These same factors of travel time and facility quality may be used to understand how public services related to literacy, such as school and public libraries, are or are not used. As noted above, the less time it takes to travel to the library, the more people are likely to use it, regardless of their socioeconomic level. Quality considerations similar to those found in public recreation centers come into play as well. Libraries of higher quality (more books, more librarians), greater ease of access (open more hours), and better services have higher circulations, again controlling for socioeconomic differences. Knowing these factors, then, it is possible to lower the effective price of library use to readers. In this way, government policy could be adjusted to increase the supply of print materials in areas where reading frequency and literacy levels are low, thus boosting the literacy proficiency of the community. This is precisely what developing nations have done in recent years, with the advent of “book flood” programs to provide more reading resources to less-literate populations in order to lower their “cost” of reading. All this would require considerably more sophisticated analysis than has been carried out thus far. Nevertheless, given the demonstrated importance of what we may call the “macroeconomic” forces discussed here on literacy develop- ment, similar “microeconomic” analysis should prove a useful area of study for both literacy researchers and practitioners. Jeff McQuillan See Also The Political Nature of Literacy References Krashen, Stephen D. 1993. The Power of Reading: Insights from the Research. Englewood, CO: Libraries Unlimited. McQuillan, Jeff. 1998. The Literacy Crisis: False Claims, Real Solutions. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Neuman, Susan, and Donna Celano. 2001. “Access to Print in Low-Income and Middle-Income Communities: An Ecological Study of Four Neighborhoods.” Reading Research Quarterly 36 (1):8–26. Effective Schools and Teachers Summarizing research on effective schools as related to reading achievement from the 1970s and 1980s, James Hoffman (1991) discussed eight characteristics: (1) a clear school mission, (2) effective instructional leadership and effective instructional practices, (3) high expectations for pupil learning, (4) a positive school climate, (5) continuous curriculum improvement, (6) maximizing of instructional time, (7) regular monitoring of pupil progress, and (8) positive homeschool relationships. In recent years, a considerable number of studies on effective schools are again appearing, undoubtedly due to widespread national concerns about the reading achievement levels of our students. Barbara Taylor, Michael Pressley, and David Pearson (2002) summarized strikingly similar findings from five large-scale research studies published in 1997–2000 on highpoverty elementary schools with high reading scores. Similar findings were also reported by Anthony Bryk and his colleagues (1998). Three studies pointed to the importance of building strong leadership. Effective principals redirected people’s time and energy, worked to develop a collective sense of responsibility among the staff for school improvement, fostered teacher leadership, and provided opportunities for collaboration and professional development. The importance of strong staff collaboration in the delivery of reading instruction was stressed in 173 Effective Schools and Teachers four of the studies. Teachers in effective schools talked and worked together within and across grades to best meet students’ needs. Four of the studies reported ongoing professional development tied to research-based reading practices as an important factor in effective schools, especially when teachers learned together within their building to improve instruction. In a related vein, research on effective school reform and teacher professional development has also stressed the importance of teachers’ learning and changing together over an extended period of time as they reflect on their practice and implement new teaching strategies. Four of the studies on effective schools reported that teachers in these schools regularly shared assessment data on students’ reading performance to make instructional decisions. Teachers also worked across grade levels to align benchmarks or standards with instruction and required assessments. All five studies reported that effective schools made a concerted effort to involve parents as partners. Schools first concentrated on securing the confidence of parents and then worked with parents on ways they could support their students’ literacy development. A great deal is also known about effective teachers of reading in the elementary grades. From the research on teaching from the 1960s and 1970s, we learned that the more effective teachers concentrated on academics, had high pupil engagement, and provided direct instruction. Effective direct instruction focused on clear learning goals, questioning as a way of monitoring students’ understanding of content covered, and regular feedback to students about their academic progress. Beginning with the research of Gerry Duffy and Laura Roehler and their colleagues (1987), attention shifted to the role of cognitive processes used by excellent teachers in the delivery of reading instruction. The more effective teachers engaged in modeling and explanation of reading strategies students could use to decode and understand texts. Michael Knapp (1995) also found that effective teachers engaged their students in higher-level thinking more than lower-level skills. Michael Pressley and his colleagues (2001) reported that effective primarygrade teachers exemplified balanced literacy instruction, in that they taught skills but also got their students engaged in a great deal of actual reading and writing. These teachers also taught students to use strategies in reading and writing and fostered their independence as learners. Taylor and her colleagues (2000) found that accomplished primary-grade teachers engaged in more small-group than whole-group reading instruction, maintained high levels of pupil engagement, preferred coaching to telling in their interactions with students when they were teaching reading, and engaged students in more higherlevel thinking during literacy instruction than other teachers. Barbara M. Taylor See Also Balanced Literacy Instruction References Bryk, Anthony S., Penny Bender Sebring, David Kerbow, Sharon Rollow, and John Q. Easton. 1998. Charting Chicago School Reform: Democratic Localism as a Lever for Change. Boulder: Westview. Duffy, Gerald G., Laura R. Roehler, Eva Sivan, Gary Rackliffe, Cassandra Book, Michael Meloth, Linda Vavrus, Roy Wesselman, Joyce Putnam, and Dina Bassiri. 1987. “Effects of Explaining the Reasoning Associated with Using Reading Strategies.” Reading Research Quarterly 20:347–368. Hoffman, James V. 1991. “Teacher and School Effects in Learning to Read.” In Rebecca Barr, Michael L. Kamil, Peter B. Mosenthal, and P. David Pearson, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 2, pp. 911–950. New York: Longman. Knapp, Michael S. 1995. Teaching for Meaning in High-Poverty Classrooms. New York: Teachers College Press. Pressley, Michael, Ruth Wharton-McDonald, Richard Allington, Cathy C. Block, Leslie Morrow, Diane Tracey, Kim Baker, Gregory Brooks, John Cronin, Eileen Nelson, and Debra Woo. 2001. “A Study of Effective First-Grade Literacy Instruction.” Scientific Studies of Reading 5 (10):35–58. Taylor, Barbara M., Michael Pressley, and P. David Pearson. 2002. Research-Supported Characteristics of Teachers and Schools That Promote Reading Achievement. In Barbara M. Taylor and P. David Pearson, eds., Teaching Reading: Effective Schools, Accomplished Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Taylor, Barbara M., P. David Pearson, Kathleen Clark, and Sharon Walpole. 2000. “Effective Schools and Accomplished Teachers: Lessons about Primary Grade Reading Instruction in Low-Income Schools.” Elementary School Journal 101 (2):121–166. Wharton-MacDonald, Ruth, Michael Pressley, and 174 Elders and Literacy A senior citizen reads her magazine (Elizabeth Crews) Jennifer M. Hampston. 1998. “Literacy Instruction in Nine First Grade Classrooms: Teacher Characteristics and Student Achievement.” Elementary School Journal 99:101–128. into ages sixty-five to seventy-four, seventy-five to eighty-four, and eighty-five and older. The recent profile from the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2001) stated that in 1999, there were 34.5 million people over sixty-five years old. That’s approximately 13 percent of the total population. These distinctions among particular cohorts within the overarching category of elders are vital. The number of elders over sixty-five will increase to 70 million, or 20 percent of the total population, by 2030, clearly reflecting the impact of the baby boom generation. Between 1970 and 1999, the rate of high-school completion among those over sixty-five rose from 28 percent to 68 percent; however, this increase also varied dramatically by race and ethnicity. Finally, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services profile noted that in 1999, 15 percent of those over sixty-five had a college degree. Thus, the levels of educational attainment and, most likely, the corresponding varieties of Elders and Literacy The functions and uses of literacy among the elderly in the United States are, at best, sketchily known. This should be a cause for concern in light of the fact that the number of elders is increasing rapidly. What we do know about elders and their literacy practices comes from a limited number of research studies and reports from practitioners in the field. Who Are the Elderly? Defining “the elderly” is complex. The American Association of Retired Persons (AARP) recruits individuals at forty-nine years of age. Gerontologists often use such terms as “young old,” “very old,” and “old old.” Cohorts are often divided 175 Elders and Literacy literacy practices differ among those born immediately prior to World War II and those born during the first decades of the twentieth century. These differences will become even greater in the future when those born after World War II reach sixty-five. All of these differences are also compounded by race/ethnicity, socioeconomic class, and elders’ native language. who might be classified as limited in their literacy abilities according to some list of basic skills or set of criteria (for example, the NALS) might indeed read and write little but may still be successful in all other areas of their lives. Similarly, elders who are considered highly literate might see little need to read and write and thus seldom do so. Finally, the reading habits of the elderly are as diverse as they are in any other group of adults. Interest surveys show that the elderly like to read newspapers, magazines, mysteries, and religious and inspirational texts because they are brief and informative (Smith, 1993). What Do We Know about Elders’ Literacy? The National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) conducted by the Educational Testing Service (Kirsch et al., 1993) employed a literacy assessment and background interview with a sample of 26,000 adults who were sixteen and older. Subsequently, researchers have broken out the data on those adults sixty and older and have found that with a variety of texts, for example, newspapers, books, and magazines, younger and middle-aged adults typically demonstrated greater reading proficiency than older adults. Such national surveys are by their very nature limited, since they attempt to measure literacy according to some general set of criteria and ignore the particular social and cultural contexts in which literacy is actually used by individuals. Likewise, smaller studies of the elderly are often severely limited by their narrow focus, for example, by semantic memory and its effects on spelling, the impact of noise when identifying lists of words, syntactic processing of texts, and so forth. Typically, these data are gathered in experimental, “laboratory” situations and not through in situ practices. Case-study research typically involved small groups of elders in specific contexts and profiles. For example research by Gail Weinstein-Shr (1995), allows us to make some tentative observations, if not generalizations, about elders and literacy. Those elders who are active readers use literacy as a social as well as a personal process; that is, reading helps them connect to others through sharing and discussion. Likewise, elders often write to maintain connections through correspondence, to explore and preserve their pasts through memoirs, autobiographies, and “life reviews,” and to counter the effects of illness and loneliness. Like younger adults, elders’ perceptions and uses of literacy vary according to their socioeconomic status, racial/ethnic background, level of education, and particular cultural context. Those Future Directions Although the information about elders’ literacy perceptions, needs, and uses is limited, it still provides clues to the necessary approach to literacy development for the elderly. It appears from what we do know that such programs must be based on the actual realities of the lives of the elderly in specific situations, on the social and connective power of reading and writing, and on the fact that they tend to prefer informal learning situations. Continuing literacy development for the elderly must take into consideration all of the complex differences described above. The AARP web site, for example, which offers free on-line classes on such topics as “Writing for Children” and “Shakespearean Drama” cannot be used as a single template. The sixty-five-year-old professional who is financially secure in 2010 will need, and will want, different literacy experiences than the sixty-five-year-old high-school dropout who, of necessity, is still in the workforce. The sixty-five-year-old who speaks English as a second language (ESL) or who speaks little English at all will need and want another type of literacy experience. Regardless of elders’ socioeconomic, educational, or linguistic status in 2001, 2010, or 2030, the graying of America demands that policy planners and program developers pay serious attention to the continuous literacy development of the elderly. Such development of literacy range and power needs to be based on a broad and comprehensive understanding of literacy. It should center around a life-task(s) orientation that views literacy as a tool for gaining self-understanding, self-expression, and empowerment 176 Electronic Jigsaw and not upon a narrow-training (in the pejorative sense) perspective. Francis E. Kazemek subtopic in a unit that is being studied. Each cooperative learning group takes one piece or part of the unit, researches it using electronic as well as traditional resources, and prepares a multimedia presentation to teach the rest of the class. Students use storyboards to organize their multimedia presentations as they write and record their own scripts. The multimedia, content-rich pieces are put together by each group, which shares its information piece of the puzzle with the class. The total puzzle is viewed as complete when all of the subtopic information is combined into a whole. Popular electronic presentation programs, such as Hyperstudio or Microsoft PowerPoint, may be used by students to prepare the presentations. Students use digital cameras, music selections, photographs, imported graphics from disk files, and the Internet to enhance the multimedia presentations. Viewing the electronic presentations in their entirety is an effective way to culminate a unit of study. The Electronic Jigsaw learning strategy is particularly useful in exploring various units of study in social studies or science. This strategy can be thought of as a reading-method framework that has certain procedural steps and options for completing the strategy. The procedural steps and options include: See Also Adult Literacy; Literacy Autobiography References Kirsch, Irwin, Ann Jungeblut, L. Jenkins, and A. Kolstad. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Princeton: Educational Testing Service. Smith, M. Cecil. 1993. “The Reading Abilities and Practices of Older Adults.” Educational Gerontology 19:417–432. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 2001. A Profile of Older Americans: 2000. Washington, DC: Administration on Aging. Weinstein-Shr, Gail. 1995. Literacy and Older Adults in the United States. Philadelphia: National Center on Adult Literacy. Electronic Jigsaw Electronic Jigsaw (eJigsaw) is a cooperative learning strategy that integrates technology with content-area literacy. The purpose of the Electronic Jigsaw is for students to learn from each other through cooperative learning groups engaged in peer teaching while using various technology resources in their research and presentation of information. Using the eJigsaw strategy requires that students apply previously learned research skills (i.e., information retrieval, summarizing information), which gives their work an authentic purpose. Additionally, the use of technology makes the research and the final presentation meaningful and motivating for students. Using technology as a learning-enhancement tool with the Jigsaw was first described by Robert Slavin (1986) as Jigsaw II. In this variation, all students read the entire assignment, but each student becomes an expert about a specific topic within the assignment. The Jigsaw, a cooperative learning strategy, was originally developed as a means of promoting positive race relations in public-school classrooms, but it was also found to have positive effects on students’ learning. The purpose of the Jigsaw is for learners to learn from each other. The process of using the Electronic Jigsaw in the classroom builds on the notion that students will become experts about a particular part or 1. Identify the subtopics to be studied 2. Conduct research in cooperative groups 3. Organize information learned about the subtopic 4. Prepare an electronic presentation program 5. Share research presentations The National Educational Technology Standards for Students (International Society for Technology Education, 2000) state that the most effective learning environments blend both traditional and new approaches to facilitate learning of content while addressing individual needs. Teaching that way infuses technology into the curriculum and demonstrates the nature of using technology as a tool for learning. The Electronic Jigsaw incorporates various technologies in such a way that students can access information as well as present their findings for others. Cynthia B. Elliott and Susan P. Kornuta 177 English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment Eligibility Decisions One of the first purposes for the English-literacy evaluation of English-language learners is to determine which students are eligible for bilingual education or ESL services. According to federal law, all school districts must meet the educational needs of English-language learners (Crawford, 1995). It is not sufficient to provide them with merely the same instruction as native-English-speaking students. When parents first enroll a child in a school district, they are almost always asked whether the child speaks a language other than English. If the parents answer yes, then the district usually requires an evaluation of the child’s English-language proficiency. Although the construct of English-language proficiency includes speaking, listening, reading, and writing, the focus of the evaluation tends to vary according to the age of the student and the district or state policy. For example, the evaluation of preschool children’s English proficiency often emphasizes oral proficiency (listening and speaking) and does not include emergent literacy, even though this is an important aspect of their academic development. Older children’s eligibility tends to be based on an evaluation of their English listening and speaking performance, with some attention, often minor, given to their reading and writing performance. The types of assessments used to estimate students’ English-language proficiency vary. At the preschool level, districts often develop their own assessments, such as a home language survey along with an interview or observation protocol that can be administered by bilingual or ESL personnel or the children’s classroom teachers. At other grade levels, commercial measures tend to be used. These measures range from rating sheets that teachers complete based on interviews or classroom observations to individual assessments completed by the students. In the latter type of assessment, students are usually asked to respond to tape-recorded instructions by pointing out the correct answer and by audio-recording their oral responses. Generally, commercial language proficiency measures are designed for and normed on English-language learners. Although these measures may include reading and writing, they tend to emphasize students’ pronunciation, grammar, oral vocabulary, and ability to communicate in English in a variety of contexts. Because students need more than oral English proficiency to par- See Also Cooperative Learning References Godoy, Al, producer, and Cynthia B. Elliott, director. 1999. Literacy and Learning in Content Area Reading: Electronic Jigsaw, Literacy, and Social Studies. Videotape. Available from Louisiana Public Broadcasting. International Society for Technology Education. 2000. National Educational Technology Standards for Students—Connecting Curriculum and Technology. Eugene, OR: International Society for Technology in Education (ISTE) NETS Project. Slavin, Robert E. 1986. “A Cooperative Learning Approach to Content Areas: Jigsaw Teaching.” In D. Lapp, J. Flood, and N. Farnan, eds., Content Area Reading and Learning: Instructional Strategies, pp. 330–345. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment The English-literacy evaluation and assessment of English-language learners—students who are acquiring English as a second language (ESL)— serve important educational purposes, even though there are serious biases involved in the process. The term evaluation refers to judgments based on the interpretation of student performance data, whereas assessment refers to the design, collection, analysis, and report of student data. The English literacy performance of English-language learners is evaluated for at least four purposes: (1) to identify those students who are eligible for bilingual education or ESL services, (2) to determine students’ placement in educational programs, (3) to monitor student progress and inform classroom instruction, and (4) to meet accountability requirements. When the students’ second-language status and bilingualism are not taken into account in the design, administration, scoring, and reporting of the assessments, then the accuracy and usefulness of the data, as well as the evaluation, are compromised. A serious problem with formal Englishliteracy assessments (standardized tests, standards-based assessments, and commercial assessments) is that they do not differentiate between the student’s knowledge of English and English-literacy performance. 178 English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment ticipate successfully in English-speaking classrooms, second-language experts warn that students’ eligibility for bilingual education or ESL instruction should also be based on their English reading and writing performance. ing strategies acquired in one language to a second language. Therefore, it is important to know how well English-language learners can read and write in their native language. Lastly, it is difficult to determine whether English-language learners have a language or cognitive delay without first estimating their language and cognitive development in their native language. Most researchers and educators recommend that multiple assessments be used to monitor student performance in the programs and to decide when English-language learners should receive special education or Title I services or be placed in an all-English classroom. Multiple assessments usually involve (1) an English-language proficiency measure normed on Englishlanguage learners, (2) a standardized academic test (especially a reading and writing test) in English normed on native-English speakers, (3) classroom or performance-based assessments that reveal how individual students approach, interpret, and complete literacy tasks in English, and, when possible, (4) assessments in the native language, such as a standardized reading test and classroom assessment. Many districts require that English-language learners perform at a certain percentile on a standardized reading test in English before they are reclassified and moved into an all-English setting. Because the educational goal is for English-language learners to perform at grade level in English, the ideal score should be the fiftieth percentile. However, it is not unusual for school districts to accept lower percentile scores. Program Placement Another purpose for the English-literacy evaluation of English-language learners is to decide on their placement in bilingual, ESL, or all-English settings and to determine whether they are eligible for placement in special education or Title I programs. The use of assessments to monitor students’ performance in the programs and to decide when students should be released from one program and transferred to another also fits within this purpose. Researchers and secondlanguage educators warn school personnel not to place or release students solely based on their oral English performance (Cummins, 1981). Sometimes, English-language learners appear to be fluent English speakers but do not have the requisite English-literacy development to perform at grade level in an all-English classroom. At other times, educators mistakenly interpret the accented English of English-language learners as meaning that they are not literate in their native language or English. They erroneously assume that the students have learning delays and need special-education services. In evaluating the English-literacy development of English-language learners for placement purposes, it is important to take into account what is known about their literacy development and academic success (August and Hakuta, 1997; García, 2000). For example, English-language learners often develop their oral skills in English at a much faster rate than their academic skills. To place them in all-English settings solely based on their oral English proficiency ignores the English-literacy development that they need to tackle academic work in English. Similarly, whenever possible, the English-literacy evaluation of English-language learners should be complemented with a native-language literacy evaluation. A number of researchers have reported that students’ reading performance in their native language is a much stronger predictor of their English reading performance than their oral English performance. Considerable evidence has also indicated that bilingual students are capable of transferring knowledge and read- Monitoring Student Progress and Informing Classroom Instruction A third purpose for the English evaluation of English-language learners’ literacy performance is to monitor student progress and to inform classroom instruction. Classroom assessments or performance-based assessments usually provide this type of data. These assessments are similar in that the classroom teacher is the observer and recorder, many of the same types of tasks occur in both types of assessments, and the tasks are usually integrated into classroom instruction and reflect the school curriculum (García and Pearson, 1994). For example, it is not unusual for both types of assessments to include running records, story retellings, think-alouds, writing portfolios, reading logs, ongoing teacher obser179 English as a Second Language (ESL) Literacy Evaluation and Assessment vations, and student self-evaluation. However, performance-based assessments are often developed outside of the individual classroom, involve the completion of specific tasks, and have someone other than the classroom teacher evaluating student performance. When classroom assessments and performance-based assessments are conducted throughout the school year, not just at the beginning or end of the school year, and are tied to the school curriculum, then they not only inform instruction but also provide useful data for monitoring student progress. An advantage that classroom assessments and performance-based assessments have over formal assessments is that they have the potential to reveal what English-language learners can and cannot do on authentic literacy tasks (García and Pearson, 1994). Standardized tests rarely indicate why students perform poorly, nor do they highlight what needs to be done to help students improve their literacy performance. Also, they are usually not tied to the school curriculum or to district or state standards (levels of expectation for student performance). Because standardized tests are only written in one language, they do not reveal how bilingual students are developing their literacy skills in two languages. In contrast, through classroom assessment, a teacher can document when individual students have difficulty with specific vocabulary items, what type of instruction works best for them, and when they are able to comprehend and use the vocabulary items appropriately. If the teacher understands the native language or is able to make use of native-language peers, aides, or parents, then it can also be determined whether the students already know the vocabulary concept and just need to know the English label or whether they need to learn the concept. The appropriate use and interpretation of classroom and performance-based assessments with English-language learners require considerable expertise in second-language acquisition. For example, both classroom teachers and the developers and scorers of performance assessments need to understand that English-language learners often reveal more comprehension of English text when they are allowed to respond (orally or in writing) in their native language. purposes. Assessments are used for accountability purposes when student performance data are compared to determine student progress and teacher, school, district, and state performance. These assessments are characterized as high stakes when they are used to determine how much funding is provided to states, school districts, or schools; which districts, schools, or teachers are sanctioned or rewarded; or when students are to be promoted, retained, or allowed to graduate. Advocates of the accountability reform movement claim that holding students, teachers, schools, districts, and states accountable for high expectations will result in improved instruction and student performance. It is for these reasons that some educators want Englishlanguage learners to participate in English accountability assessments even when they are aware of the assessment limitations. Most of the national and state assessments developed for accountability purposes are based on standards that are designed and normed for native-English-speaking students. As a result, critics warn that the measures are not accurate indicators of English-language learners’ literacy development. At a minimum, they recommend that the scores for English-language learners should be disaggregated and reported separately. Sometimes, English-language learners who are still receiving ESL and bilingual-education services are required to participate in accountability assessments. School districts concerned about the potential low scores of English-language learners often illegally exclude them from participating in the assessments. Bilingual-education teachers, who are forced to participate in the assessments and who are worried about their students’ English performance, have been known to decrease the amount of literacy instruction that they provide in the native language and overemphasize English-literacy instruction, violating the very purpose of bilingual education. To offset district concerns about the potentially low performance of English-language learners on accountability measures, some of the state and national assessments exclude students who have not completed a specific number of years in bilingual-education or ESL programs. In other cases, they allow English-language learners to participate with accommodations. For example, English-language learners may be given more time to complete the assessment, the test admin- Accountability Requirements English-language learners’ literacy development in English is also evaluated for accountability 180 English Journal istrator may read the instructions or questions aloud, or students may be allowed to use dictionaries. A few states, such as Illinois, have developed and normed their own accountability measures in English for English-language learners. knowledge and strategy use is typically not included in the English-literacy assessment of English-language learners. Georgia Earnest García See Also Biliteracy References August, Diane, and Hakuta Kenji, eds. 1997. Improving Schooling for Language-Minority Children: A Research Agenda. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Crawford, James. 1995. Bilingual Education: History, Politics, Theory, and Practice. 3rd ed. Los Angeles: Bilingual Education Services. Cummins, Jim. 1981. “The Role of Primary Language Development in Promoting Educational Success for Language Minority Students.” In California State Department of Education, ed., Schooling and Language Minority Students: A Theoretical Framework, pp. 3–49. Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dissemination and Assessment Center, California State University–Los Angeles. García, Georgia E. 2000. “Bilingual Children’s Reading.” In Michael Kamil, Peter Mosenthal, P. David Pearson, and Rebecca Barr, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 3, pp. 813–834. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. García, Georgia E., and P. David Pearson. 1994. “Assessment and Diversity.” Review of Research in Education 20:337–391. Jiménez, Robert T., Georgia E. García, and P. David Pearson. 1996. “The Reading Strategies of Bilingual Latina/o Students Who Are Successful English Readers: Opportunities and Obstacles.” Reading Research Quarterly 31 (l):90–112. Bias Issues There are a number of factors that adversely affect the literacy assessment of English-language learners and make it difficult to accurately estimate their English-literacy development (García and Pearson, 1994). For example, on formal reading measures, as compared to native-English speakers, English-language learners face more unknown English vocabulary in the test instructions, passages, and test items. They are adversely affected when key vocabulary items in the passages are paraphrased in the test questions. They often have to read passages for which they do not have the appropriate background knowledge and are rarely given the opportunity to read passages for which they do have the appropriate background knowledge. Bilingual readers, compared to monolingual readers, often need more time to process text in the second language. Because reading assessments in English are typically normed on native-English speakers, it is not uncommon for bilingual students to need more time to complete the assessment than the amount allowed. The appropriateness of using literacy assessments in English that are developed for nativeEnglish-speaking students with English-language learners is questionable, given that the assessments do not reflect what is known about second-language reading or the experiences of successful second-language readers. For example, it is not unusual for English-language learners to demonstrate much higher levels of reading comprehension in English when they are given the test questions in their native language or are allowed to write their answers to semiconstructed or open-ended questions in the native language. Researchers also report that bilingual students who are successful English readers have a unitary view of reading across their two languages, use similar high-level metacognitive and cognitive reading strategies while reading in their two languages, and transfer knowledge acquired in one language to the other (Jiménez, García, and Pearson, 1996). They judiciously employ bilingual strategies, such as codeswitching, accessing cognates, and translating. This type of English Journal English Journal is a publication of the National Council of Teachers of English (NCTE) geared primarily toward secondary-school and middleschool teachers of English and language arts. Teacher educators also subscribe to the journal. Articles span a variety of topics of interest to this audience, such as discussion of pertinent professional issues, arguments for a particular point of view about the teaching of English and language arts, or new ideas and descriptions of innovative classroom practice. Because secondary- and middle-school teachers are the primary audience, English Journal does not accept reports of quantitative research or articles on literary criticism. Most issues of the journal are designed around a theme related to the teaching of writ181 The Even Start Family Literacy Program that time, the program was administered by the U.S. Department of Education, which awarded funding to seventy-six local school districts. Currently, there are more than 800 Even Start projects across the country, with an average of fifty families per project. The majority are collaborative efforts between a local school district and a community-based organization, administered by state Even Start coordinators. Through state-run grant competitions, local projects receive federal Even Start funds, with increasing local match requirements over the first eight years of the project. There are also set-aside funds administered by the federal government for migrant families, Indian tribes, and tribal organizations, along with statewide family literacy initiatives. Even Start legislation specifies that projects must build on existing community resources to develop an integrated model of intensive early childhood education, adult education, and parenting education. In addition, projects must serve the families most in need of these services, provide opportunities for parents and children to be involved in activities together, offer some home-based services, and make supports available, such as transportation, to enable families to participate. However, the specific program design is determined by local project staff. For example, projects decide on the schedule of activities and whether services will be supported by Even Start funds or through collaboration with other local agencies. The majority of projects offer a center-based early childhood program. Programs for infants and toddlers are generally staffed by Even Start, with preschool programs more often provided through collaboration with community agencies. Services for school-age children include homework assistance, family events, and summer enrichment activities. Across projects, there is a range of adult-education services offered, including preparation for the GED, instruction in basic reading and math skills, and English as a second language. Parenting education includes group discussions as well as parent-child activities either at home or in the early childhood classroom. To date, there have been two national evaluations of Even Start (St. Pierre et al., 1995; Tao, Gamse, and Tarr, 1998). Both reported significant gains in adults’ literacy skills and children’s general cognitive skills. However, except for a randomized study conducted in five projects, ing, literature, and language, but articles on a variety of topics are considered. With a subscription base of approximately 45,000, English Journal reaches teachers and teacher educators internationally. The journal is published bimonthly—September through July— and each issue contains approximately ten to twelve articles. In addition, there are six regular columns: English in the News, Learning with Technology, Poetry, Professional Links (reviews of professional books and web sites), Talk about Books (reviews of fiction and nonfiction), and Young Adult Literature. English Journal also includes several features: Cross-Conversations, Insights for Interns, Speaking My Mind, and Teacher to Teacher. Reviews of books and classroom materials are handled by the appropriate column editors and are not accepted from unsolicited manuscripts. Poetry is submitted directly to the poetry column editor. Calls for thematic issues and for ongoing features are announced in the front section of each issue, detailing topics, listing deadlines, and noting whether inquiries are welcome. The journal web site carries this information as well (available: http://www.cc.ysu. edu/tej). Virginia R. Monseau See Also National Council of Teachers of English The Even Start Family Literacy Program The Even Start Family Literacy Program uses an integrated approach to early childhood education, parenting education, and adult education for families with young children. The overarching goal is to help break the cycle of intergenerational poverty and low literacy among families with limited educational experiences. Specific goals are: to help parents improve their literacy or basic educational skills, to help parents become full partners in educating their children, and to help children reach their full potential as learners. To be eligible for the program, families must have a child under eight years old and a parent who is either eligible for adult-education services under the Adult Education Act or still within the state’s compulsory school-attendance range. The Even Start Family Literacy Program began in 1989 as part of the revised Elementary and Secondary Education Act of 1965 (ESEA). At 182 Eye Movements a smaller and somewhat more portable camera— and the Reading Eye and Reading Eye II cameras). The seminal literature attributed to these researchers continues to be cited. Prior to the 1920s, one of the overriding issues was the study of fixations and saccades. This stop-and-go process piqued researchers’ interest because of the primary question: does it affect the reader’s ability to understand text? Or from a fluency perspective, do fewer fixations along a line of print indicate that a person is a good reader, and do more fixations suggest reading difficulty? Since the 1920s, eye-movement researchers have attempted to explain the impact of ocular pursuit (smooth and linear movement), fixations, saccades, and regressions (returning to viewed elements) on the reading process. According to Eleanor Gibson and Harry Levin (1975), basic findings in recent years have indicated that eye movement is stable by the fourth grade (frequency and duration of fixations and decreases in regressions). Eye movement involves fixations 94 percent of the time and saccades 6 percent of the time—fixations of 240 to 250 milliseconds—which are satisfactory. Eye movement is responsive to the difficulty of the materials and comprehension and is different for oral and silent reading (e.g., fewer fixations and shorter regressions during silent reading). One of the more interesting statements made by Gibson and Levin was that our general understanding of the relationship between eye movement and reading was provided by early researchers using primitive devices, and their discoveries continue to remain “trustworthy.” It has also been reported (Daneman, 1991) that a preponderance of published data suggest that irregular eye movements are symptomatic of reading problems rather than causal. Earl H. Cheek Jr. and Jimmy D. Lindsey there were no control groups to gauge whether these gains can actually be attributed to participation in the program. Janet P. Swartz See Also The Head Start Program References St. Pierre, Robert, Beth Gamse, Judith Alamprese, Tracy Rimdzius, and Fumiyo Tao. 1998. The Even Start Family Literacy Program: Evidence from the Past and a Look to the Future. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Planning, and Evaluation Service. Available: http://www.ed.gov/pubs/EvenStart. St. Pierre, Robert, Janet P. Swartz, Beth Gamse, Stephen Murray, Dennis Deck, and Phil Nickel. 1995. National Evaluation of the Even Start Family Literacy Program: Final Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Planning, and Evaluation Service. Tao, Fumiyo, Beth Gamse, and Hope Tarr. 1998. National Evaluation of the Even Start Family Literacy Program: 1994–97 Final Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Planning, and Evaluation Service. Eye Movements Perhaps one of the more intriguing areas of reading inquiry during the past 100 years has been that of eye movements. As research on the reading process began, there was an immediate interest in the way the eye moved across the printed page. Investigators wanted to know whether perception occurred as the eyes were moving across the printed page, and if not, why? Another issue they sought to address was the regularity of eye movements, specifically the occurrence of fixations and saccades—movement between fixations (Venezky, 1984). Early eye-movement researchers included Emile Javal (a pioneer who first reported this phenomenon in 1878), Raymond Dodge (who devised the corneal reflection method for determining eye movements), W. F. Dearborn (who was interested in word structure and adult pronunciations), and Edmund Huey (who was known for the synthesis of eye-movement findings in his 1908 textbook). They were followed by Guy Buswell and Miles Tinker (who developed and used cameras to examine eye movements) and Earl Taylor and Tinker’s son Stanford Tinker (who developed and used the Opthalmograph— References Daneman, Meredyth. 1991. “Individual Differences in Reading Skills.” In Rebecca Barr, Michael L. Kamil, Peter B. Mosenthal, and P. David Pearson, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, vol. 2, p. 517. New York: Longman. Gibson, Eleanor J., and Harry Levin. 1975. The Psychology of Reading. Cambridge: MIT Press. Venezky, Richard L. 1984. “The History of Reading Research.” In P. David Pearson, Rebecca Barr, Michael L. Kamil, and Peter B. Mosenthal, eds., Handbook of Reading Research, pp. 7–10. New York: Longman. 183 F tended to meet two goals—to provide parents with instruction that will both advance their own literacy skills and abilities and help them support their children’s success in learning how to read and write. Programs differ in the ways they deliver these services. Some offer direct instruction to the parent only, with the intention of affecting the child’s literacy learning through parental actions. Others provide direct instruction in literacy to parent and child in separate settings and also involve parents and children in site-based, joint literacy events and activities. Some programs also require that parents participate in a parenting component, apparently on the basis of an as yet undocumented assumption that parents who lack literacy proficiency also lack understanding of effective parenting practices. The program model that may be most widely practiced in the United States was put forth by Sharon Darling and Andrew Hayes (1988–1989) at the National Center for Family Literacy and has become the basis for the largest federally funded family literacy initiative, the Even Start program. This model includes four components: parent literacy education, child literacy education, parent and child activity time, and parenting education. In recent years, family literacy programs have proliferated in schools and communities across the United States and, at the same time, have become the focus of vigorous debate. Many view them as the answer to a host of problems associated with society in general, and school failure in particular. For example, Darling, founder and president of the National Center for Family Literacy, described family literacy as one of the most important initiatives in the effort to reform welfare and suggested that family literacy programs have the potential to strengthen family Family Literacy The role that parents play in their children’s education has long been a focus of study by educators and policymakers, particularly in relation to efforts to understand high rates of failure among some groups of children. Evidence documenting the relationship between children’s early reading success and parents’ own reading behaviors has led many educators to seek educational interventions that address the family unit rather than the child alone. Thomas Sticht was among the earliest to refer to such programs as “intergenerational literacy programs,” and subsequently others have referred to them variously as “two-generation programs” and, most recently, as “family literacy” programs. Of particular interest has been the relationship between children’s school success and two parent-related factors: parental education and home literacy practices. The importance of the first factor, parental education, is underscored by results from the National Assessment of Educational Progress, which, over several administrations, has repeatedly shown that children who have higher rates of performance on reading achievement tests also have parents with higher levels of education. The importance of the second factor, home literacy practices, became a particular focus in 1966 with the publication of Dolores Durkin’s oft-cited study on children who read early and in the years since has received substantial support from numerous other investigations. Evidence consistently supports a relationship that links children’s early success in reading with parents’ own reading and interest in books, parent-child storybook reading, and parents’ general interactions with their children around print. Family literacy programs are generally in185 Family Literacy Parents read the Bible to their children (Skjold Photographs) comes of family literacy intervention programs. They also disagree strongly in their perceptions of the literate lives of families that are often targeted as participants in such programs. For example, Darling and Hayes (1988–1989) described the daily lives of such families as essentially devoid of any literate activity and, as a consequence, as unlikely to provide children with sufficient opportunity to acquire basic knowledge about literacy and language. Illiterate parents, they said, lack the resources to support their children’s school success, and as a result, an intergenerational cycle of illiteracy ensues. In contrast, researchers such as Shirley Brice Heath (1983), Luis Moll and James Greenberg (1990), Denny Taylor and Catherine DorseyGaines (1988), and William Teale (1986) assert that nearly all families embed some forms of literacy and language events within their daily routines. Nevertheless, these events are often different from those that teachers expect or are familiar with, and consequently, they go unnoticed. Proponents of this point of view support their claim with evidence from studies across different cultural, linguistic, and economic groups. values and functioning and advance families toward self-sufficiency. This point of view enjoys substantial political and legislative support. As a result, family literacy interventions are now singled out as priorities in many federally and statefunded reading programs for early childhood, elementary, and adult education. Others, however, strongly disagree with the claim that education will provide a shield against poverty, low employment, and other societal problems. Among the most vocal on this side of the debate, Denny Taylor (1997) has relied on a six-year ethnographic study of families living in poverty, along with numerous anecdotal accounts collected from parents, teachers, and researchers, to argue that high unemployment and poverty are the result of inequalities within society that prevent individuals from achieving economic advancement despite personal motivation or educational attainment. Those sharing this point of view also point to census data that indicate that race and gender correlate more highly with unemployment and poverty than does education. Those on each side of the debate differ not only in what they believe to be the likely out186 Feminist Post-Structuralism They conclude that children fail not because they are language and literacy deprived, but because they are language and literacy different. As such, they enter the schoolhouse doors without knowledge of language patterns and literacy events that are valued and privileged in most classrooms. Vivian Gadsden (1994) summarized the disagreement and dissension that characterizes the work in family literacy as emerging from two seriously conflicting premises: one that perceives the family’s lack of school-like literacy as a barrier to learning, the other that views the home literacy practices that are already present—however different from school-based literacy—as a bridge to new learnings. Rather than choosing sides in the debate, however, Gadsden argued that both premises may be useful—that educators might adopt a reciprocal approach predicated on an understanding that teachers need to instruct parents in school-based literacy and also seek to learn about and integrate parents’ existing knowledge and resources into the school curriculum. Several programs that adhere to a reciprocal approach to family literacy have been studied, and results indicate some potential benefits. Among their combined findings are improved English proficiency for parents, improvements in children’s knowledge of letter names and print awareness, more frequent visits by parents to school, greater numbers of literacy materials at home, increased confidence on the part of parents in helping with their children’s homework, increased interaction between parents and their children’s teachers, and increased understanding among parents of how they are able to support their children’s literacy learning. Family literacy interventions based on a practice of reciprocity in learning between parents and teachers may hold promise for increasing the access to educational opportunity for linguistically and culturally different children and adults. Nevertheless, in recent critical reviews of related research, researchers have cautioned that existing studies are largely characterized by diverse populations, small sample sizes, and nonexperimental designs and, as such, must be considered to be exploratory rather than conclusive in understanding effective practice in family literacy. Thus, as research and practice in family literacy moves forward, it must heed and acknowledge the diversity in the populations that are often served by family literacy interventions and the implications of such diversity for research and teaching, while also recognizing the need for methodological rigor that will enable educators to learn which interventions are likely to serve those populations most effectively. Jeanne R. Paratore See Also Community Literacy; The Even Start Family Literacy Program; National Assessment of Educational Progress References Darling, Sharon, and Andrew Hayes. 1988–1989. Family Literacy Project: Final Project Report. Louisville, KY: National Center for Family Literacy. Durkin, Dolores. 1966. Children Who Read Early. New York: Teachers College Press. Gadsden, Vivian L. 1994. Understanding Family Literacy: Conceptual Issues Facing the Field. Philadephia: University of Pennsylvania National Center for Adult Literacy. Heath, Shirley B. 1983. Ways with Words. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Moll, Luis, and James B. Greenberg. 1990. “Creating Zones of Possibilities: Combining Social Contexts for Instruction.” In L. C. Moll, ed., Vygotsky in Education, pp. 319–349. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sticht, Thomas G., and Barbara McDonald. 1989. “Making the Nation Smarter: The Intergenerational Transfer of Literacy.” San Diego: Institute for Adult Literacy. Taylor, Denny. 1997. Many Families, Many Literacies. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Taylor, Denny, and Catherine Dorsey-Gaines. 1988. Growing Up Literate: Learning from Inner-City Families. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Teale, William H. 1986. “Home Background and Young Children’s Literacy Development.” In W. H. Teale and E. Sulzby, eds., Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading, pp. 173–206. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Feminist Post-Structuralism Post-structuralism is commonly associated with the work of French theorist Michel Foucault. It is both a social theory and a methodology. Like all theories, it has many interpretations and derivations, one of which is feminist post-structuralism. It has its intellectual roots partly in “French” feminism (Julia Kristeva, Luce Irigaray, Hélène 187 Feminist Post-Structuralism Cixous) and partly in North American and British feminist interpretations of Foucault (Michele Barrett, Lois McNay, Caroline Ramazanoglu, Jana Sawicki). Feminist post-structuralists focus on deconstruction of the patriarchal canon, and locate “the feminine” at its theoretical and methodological center. Post-structuralism is a critique of modernism, and its historical marker, the term post, locates it after and in response to structuralism. Poststructuralism arose as an intellectual movement in the late 1960s, partly in response to disillusionment with Marxism, partly as an alternative to then dominant structuralist paradigms in the social sciences (e.g., Jean Piaget, Lawrence Kohlberg, Noam Chomsky, Claude Lévi-Strauss, de Saussure). Post-structuralism challenges Enlightenment rationality and scientific reason, the autonomous transcendental individual, the veracity and transparency of language, the truth of knowledge, and the telos of progressive accounts of history. Feminist post-structuralism, in turn, appropriated post-structuralism’s analytic tools to critique structuralist accounts of gender differences and the masculinist epistemological foundations of all social theory, including poststructuralism itself. Its task has been to deconstruct the masters’ narratives and the historical entrenchment of patriarchy that has buried women’s voices, subjectivity, and opportunities. Foucault advocated that genealogy, as both a methodology and a critique of history, be replaced by multiple and overlapping histories. Genealogy studies the material conditions of discourse, its location in fields of power, and its tactics of producing “truths,” knowledge, and subjectivities. A genealogical study of the discursive formation of the social subject can also reveal its “Other”: the silenced, marginalized, “abnormal Other” of the normative subject. Feminist post-structuralists argue that modernist history of “great men” and events has silenced the local histories, voices, and memories of women and people of color. This has resulted in a rewriting of histories and a stunning exposé of the politics of knowledge at work in two dominant modernist discourses—patriarchy and colonialism—both renowned for their subjugating tactics of masquerading as truth, of silencing and “othering” women and all groups of nonWestern racial, ethnic, or cultural origins. Knowledge Foucault argued that knowledge is a social construct with a history and context and is infused with politics. Knowledge is closely related to his concept of discourse, commonly understood as socially legitimated knowledge and practices that systematically construct and formalize objects in the world. Thus, knowledge—or any discourse— cannot be conceptualized independently of its institutionalization in concrete material practices and power relations. Curricular knowledge, for example, is unthinkable outside its schoolbased institutionalization. Knowledge and practice co-constitute and validate each other. Knowledge-as-discourse is a formalized system of theories, concepts, and statements about objects in the world that derive their “truth” value from rules and procedures embodied in institutional practices. Hence, textbook knowledge, legitimated by a massive schooling, bureaucratic, and legal apparatus, has achieved the status of a formalized truth that students, teachers, and parents abide by. Knowledge is the product of historically contingent organizing principles that name and classify things, giving them value and meaning in relation to other things located on various knowledge grids (education, psychology, law, history). The production of knowledge is not an autonomous process without human agency History Post-structuralism rejects the modernist teleological concept of history that sees each epoch as the dialectical outcome of previous eras and as the rational march of human history toward an ideal state. Foucault, instead, argued for “the end of history” as we know it: a break with grand historical narratives and totalizing conceptions of history. Post-structuralists claim that history is messy and uneven, without fixed points of origin, consisting of unpredictable convergences, discontinuities, and ruptures. Modern concepts of mass literacy, for example, emerged in the early sixteenth century as the printing press coincided with the rise of Protestantism and its injunction that every individual should have personal access to the word of scripture. The mass production of the same text enabled by the press, coupled with an emergent ideology of individual readership, produced the need for universal literacy and, hence, for early versions of universal and mass schooling. 188 Feminist Post-Structuralism but is embedded in social and power relations that are infused with contradictions and conflict over meaning and authority. The “culture wars” among educators is a case in point, whether in Japan, the United States, or Australia: Whose history, whose perspective? Was it settlement or invasion? For post-structuralists, there is nothing neutral, fixed, or essential about either truth or knowledge. Instead, they are historical sociocultural constructs, always political and always regulatory of the objects about which knowledge speaks and of which truth claims are made. state) power relations that shape teachers’ work, social conduct, and workplace relations. Teachers also become part of the educational knowledge industry through their participation in, say, curriculum development, as progressive or regressive change agents in their schools, or as they climb the state’s department hierarchy to more senior positions. Their location within the larger network of the educational enterprise means that they gain access over time to different kinds of power and knowledge, becoming subject themselves to different levels and kinds of power. Similarly, the production of knowledge from the student “body”—student collectives in a class, school, district, state, nation—derives from student testing over which teachers (and the state) preside. The massive archive produced from the regime of the examination is organized and interpreted through prevailing educational discourses and becomes knowledge that can legitimate extant or create new concepts, theory, policy, and subject categories (e.g., students atrisk, with special needs, gifted; attention deficit disorder [ADD] or English as a second language [ESL] students). Theory, or the “truth” about the educable subject, generates new pedagogical practices—some punitive, others beneficent— that claim to cater to (and normalize) those categories of difference. These scenarios illustrate not only how knowledge and power combine to control, subject, and normalize the individual but also how they can create productive spaces and practices (inclusive, special, compensatory, or accelerated education; gender equity policy; antiracist pedagogy). Power Contrary to the “repressive hypothesis” of power dominating modernist social theory, Foucault conceptualized power as productive, fluid, multifaceted, and diffuse rather than as fixed in one place, “owned” and exercised by one person or group in only negative and punitive ways. Power is intimately connected to knowledge; it is relational and historically contingent. All social relations are constituted by power dynamics (parent-child; judge-jury-accused), and relations of power derive their legitimacy and force from the discourses that regulate and govern individual subjects. People exercise power as much as they are subjected by it. For instance, a teacher has power over students in the way questions are posed and to whom they are directed; how curricular knowledge is interpreted and distributed; how students are seated, tested, graded; how educational capital and credentials are distributed. At the same time, a teacher is subject to the layers of institutional power and control of the school and district administration, the discourse of policy and curriculum, teacher testing, and legal responsibilities. Educational discourse—the entire ensemble of theory, practices, rules, laws, policies—is the knowledge regime that sustains the many subjectivities and subject positions of all people variously engaged in schooling in different relations of power (student, teacher, parent, learning support or administrative staff, principal). Power and knowledge operate in a co-constitutive circuit. To illustrate: teachers’ certified knowledge (the teaching credential) legitimates their authority to teach and exercise power over large numbers of students, but it also subjects them to local (school) and external (district, Language Knowledge “speaks” through language, although ideas, meanings, and classificatory taxonomies are also expressed in iconic imagery and spatial organization—whether the textbook illustration, graph, or chart, or the school or classroom configurations. Language, for all variants of poststructuralism, is the analytic key: a window to the discursive construction of knowledge, truth, power, social organization, and the social subject. Post-structuralism critiques the modernist/humanist conception of language that assumes that language is a transparent window to the real, that meaning is fixed in the linguistic signifier, and that the rational, self-conscious, and self-knowing subject has autonomous con189 Feminist Post-Structuralism trol and choice over “authentic” self-expression through language. Feminist post-structuralists argue that the gendered politics of language preclude authentic voice and self-expression. They have shown how women, girls, and concepts of the feminine have been subsumed by the generic “he” or “man,” how what used to be called “malestream” thought is embedded in educational policy, curriculum, and theory—both “conservative” and “radical” (Luke and Gore, 1992). Others have deconstructed the language of theoretical discourses that produce gender differences (Henriques et al., 1984); gender constructions in school yard play (Walkerdine, 1990), curriculum (Gilbert, 1996), and teacher talk (Baker and Freebody, 1996); the role of gender discourses in the language(s) of popular culture that are appropriated by young people in the process of identity formation (Alverman, Moon, and Hagood, 1999; Christian-Smith, 1993). Language gives meaning to social reality, including the way social subjects make themselves, others, and the world intelligible. Post-structuralists thus argue that language—signs organized in discourse—provides discursive subject positions and subjectivities through which we live our lives and make sense of the world. Feminist post-structuralists have long argued that a globally dominant, although culturally inflected, masculinist epistemology historically has limited women’s subject positions, muted their subjectivity, denied them an education, a voice, a speaking/writing position. Women have thus been unable to speak or write themselves into history or any speakable narrative. Feminist post-structuralists have sought to reclaim language and speaking positions for women, although their “speech” bears the residue of the language and genres of the father. hand, and one’s subjectivity or subjective sense of “self,” on the other. Everyday life consists of multiple and competing discourses and meanings alongside dominant discourses that, in turn, create multiple subject positions—some marginalized, others hegemonic. The subject negotiates, lives, and acts through a range of subject positions at every moment: acquiescent to normative and dominant subjectivities one moment, resisting or reshaping other “given” positions at another moment, attempting to unify disparate positions at yet another. Although discourses normalize, regulate, and position people into power/knowledge fields that govern their subject positions and actions, it is in the gaps and overlaps of discourses that change and resistance are enacted. Discourses are social products, themselves subject to the shifting historical winds of political interests embodied in social subjects and institutions. As such, the very social and historical nature of power, of socially constructed knowledge and socially embedded subjects, makes all discourses highly unstable and contingent, keeping them in continual flux. Yet access to any number of historically dominant or emergent discourses can form the analytic lenses that we turn on dominant discourses to transform them. For post-structuralists, a composite of discourses shapes the practical templates for everyday life that, in turn, constitute the multifaceted parameters by which social agents act in the world. Carmen Luke See Also Post-Structuralism and Structuralism; Subjectivity References Alverman, Donna, J. S. Moon, and Margaret Hagood. 1999. Popular Culture in the Classroom: Teaching and Researching Critical Media Literacy. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Baker, Carolyn, and Peter Freebody. 1996. “Categories and Sense-Making in the Talk and Texts of Schooling.” In Geoff Bull and Michele Anstey, eds., The Literacy Lexicon, pp. 145–162. Sydney: Prentice-Hall. Christian-Smith, Linda. 1993. Texts of Desire: Essays on Fiction, Femininities, and Schooling. London: Falmer Press. Gilbert, Pam. 1996. “Gender Talk and Silence: Speaking and Listening as Social Practice.” In Geoff Bull and Michele Anstey, eds., The Literacy Lexicon, pp. 163–176. Sydney: Prentice-Hall. Henriques, Julian, Wendy Hollway, Cathy Urwin, Subjects, Subjectivity, Subject Positions The subject is a social construct that varies across time, cultural and institutional locations, and across and within discourses. Subjectivity refers to the internal private sense of oneself (although largely derived from prevailing discourses one has access to) including private reflection, memories, the unsayable of dreams, affect, emotion, and other extra-linguistic experiences and thoughts. Subject positions are taken up, contested, or negotiated in between prescribed positions offered in discourse, on the one 190 Fluency Couze Venn, and Valerie Walkerdine. 1984. Changing the Subject. New York: Methuen. Luke, Carmen, and Jennifer Gore. 1992. Feminisms and Critical Pedagogy. New York: Routledge. Walkerdine, Valerie. 1990. Schoolgirl Fictions. London: Verso. needs (Allington, 2000), and there is a growing body of research that addresses the benefits of flexible instruction. For example, Janet Lerner (2000) reported that teachers could manipulate variables such as time, grouping, and methodology to accommodate differences among students and to help students achieve success without watering down the content. The practice of “manipulating instructional variables” provides teachers with the mechanism to meet literacy objectives and support students’ learning. Researchers (Allington, 2001) have reported that educators have actually changed their beliefs regarding flexibility and how best to teach students. Additionally, flexibility in programming and developing technologies provide “students” of all ages the opportunity for lifelong learning that enhances literacy and work abilities. Jimmy D. Lindsey, Carolyn F. Woods, and Nicki L. Anzelmo-Skelton Flexibility Flexibility refers to the decisions readers make during the reading process to adjust their reading rate to promote fluency or comprehension. (It can also refer to the judgments professionals make during literacy instruction to meet students’ individual needs.) An important component of the reading process is flexibility, or the rate at which reading takes place. Flexibility in reading rate is the ability to read varying materials at different rates and for different purposes. In general, reading rate is determined by the purpose for reading (e.g., leisure, guided), in conjunction with the nature of the materials being read (e.g., organization, concept density), and there is a correlation between reading rate and level of comprehension (i.e., increased reading rate can promote higher understanding). Conversely, as reported by Timothy Rasinski and Nancy Padak (1998), problems in flexibility or reading rate can also negatively affect fluency and comprehension. For example, many students are unable to regulate their reading rate to correspond to purpose or characteristics of printed material without direct instruction. Nevertheless, there are several interventions that may be helpful in teaching readers to vary and ultimately increase reading rates, including repeating readings (Lerner, 2000), teaching phrasing, and using practice materials just below a reader’s instructional level. Flexibility in literacy instruction is a delivery method intended to meet the individual needs of all students (e.g., matching instructional attributes with learner characteristics). This system is in sharp contrast to finding and implementing one method to teach all children, particularly for those students whose needs differ at different times in their lives and respond to varying curricula and strategies (Lerner, 2000). In an effort to afford all students the opportunity to access the curriculum and become literate, teachers must constantly organize and reorganize instruction to ensure flexible events to meet individual See Also Automaticity and Reading Fluency References Allington, Richard L. 2001. What Really Matters for Struggling Readers: Designing Research-Based Programs. Reading, MA: Longman. Lerner, Janet W. 2000. Learning Disabilities: Theories, Diagnosis, and Teaching Strategies. 8th ed. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Rasinski, Timothy V., and Nancy D. Padak. 1998. “How Elementary Students Referred for Compensatory Reading Instruction Perform on School-Based Measures of Word Recognition, Fluency, and Comprehension.” Reading Psychology 19 (2):185–216. Fluency Fluency in reading refers to the ability to read in a smooth, expressive, accurate, and meaningful manner with appropriate phrasing, at an appropriate rate, and with good comprehension. Although reading fluency in most commonly thought of in terms of oral reading, it is believed that the manifestations and outcomes of fluency are present in silent reading as well. According to the National Reading Panel (Rasinski, 2000), teachers need to understand that word-recognition accuracy is not the end point of reading instruction. Reading fluency is a level of expertise beyond word-recognition accuracy. Moreover, reading comprehension is aided 191 Fluency by fluency. Proficient readers read words accurately, rapidly, and efficiently, devoting their finite cognitive resources to the task of comprehending the text. One factor commonly associated with reading fluency is automaticity in word recognition or decoding. Automatic or attention-free decoding is a major contributor to fluency in reading. As readers’ word recognition achieves a level of automaticity—the point where words can be decoded with minimal cognitive effort—readers are more able to direct their cognitive resources to the higher-level task of text comprehension. In this sense, then, reading fluency can be defined as efficiency in decoding. Peter Schreiber (1980) offered an alternative explanation for reading fluency. According to Schreiber, reading fluency develops as the reader achieves syntactic control of the text. That is, the fluent reader is able to chunk the text into syntactically appropriate and meaningful phrases. Developing the ability to phrase text can be a difficult task for many readers, as written text does not have reliable intrasentential-phrase boundary markers. Commas and other forms of punctuation do not provide the same reliable cues to intrasentential phrasing that are found in the prosodic features that speakers use to embed their speech and that mark phrase boundaries for listeners. As readers’ fluency improves through greater awareness and control of the meaningful syntactic structures in written text, comprehension also improves. sured by reading rate, however, that effectively distinguished the more-effective from the lesseffective readers. Readers who were most fluent and read with the highest levels of comprehension read at nearly three times the reading rate of the least-effective group of readers. In its integrative review of research on reading fluency, the National Reading Panel (Rasinski, 2000) reported that instruction in and development of reading fluency, particularly through repeated oral readings of text with feedback and guidance, does lead to significant gains in overall student achievement—for good readers as well as for those who struggle in reading. Nevertheless, despite the growing recognition that fluency plays an important role in reading, reading fluency is often a neglected part of reading instruction. An examination of current materials designed for reading instruction reveals that reading fluency continues to be largely ignored as an important instructional factor for teachers and students. Nurturing Fluent Reading through Instruction There are several principles of instruction that should guide instruction in fluency. Learning readers need to develop an understanding of the nature of fluent reading and its importance in the overall reading process. This can be fostered through teachers’ modeling of fluent reading for students and by their drawing students’ attention to various aspects of fluent reading. Fluency is developed by practice or repeated readings of connected written discourse. S. Jay Samuels (1979) and others have found that the repeated readings of individual texts by a reader generally resulted in improved comprehension and overall performance on passages that not been previously encountered by the reader. Support provided by a fluent oral rendition of the text, through choral, partner (paired or shared reading), or recorded reading, while being read simultaneously by the student, also led to improved reading fluency. Since disfluency in reading is often marked by reading in a word-byword manner, fluent reading can also be fostered through an instructional focus on the phrased nature of text to be read and explicitly marking the phrase boundaries within texts. Finally, during instruction, fluency can be promoted through the use of written texts that are well Importance of Fluency Inasmuch as reading fluency has been associated with efficiency in text processing and comprehension, it should play a significant role in proficient reading and should be a key element in effective reading instruction. In a large-scale study of reading fluency among fourth-grade students, Gay Sue Pinnell and her colleagues (1995) found that oral reading fluency, as measured by a descriptive fluency rubric, was significantly associated with silent-reading comprehension and that nearly 50 percent of all fourth graders had not yet achieved even a minimally acceptable level of reading fluency. The study also noted that fluent and less-fluent readers, as well as effective and less-effective comprehenders, demonstrated fairly proficient levels of word-recognition accuracy. It was word-recognition efficiency, as mea192 Fluency within the reader’s ability to read easily and expressively. Material that is too difficult or unfamiliar will, by its very nature, be a source of disfluency while reading. Thus, fluency can be nurtured by providing students with texts that are relatively easy to read. Brief, predictable texts, with clear phrase boundaries, such as rhyming and rhythmical poetry, are excellent choices for developing reading fluency. Although the above guidelines or principles of fluency instruction will nurture fluent reading, instructional routines that are based upon and integrate more than one of the guidelines will result in even more effective fluency instruction for students. In one study of an integrated fluency instruction model, for example, a successful fluency development lesson combined the modeling of fluent reading of short easy texts, repeated reading with formative feedback, and support through repeated choral readings, with the program implemented regularly over the course of a school year. This model had a significant effect on the overall reading development of at-risk second-grade readers. Thus, it seems reasonable to conclude that fluency is an important and valid component of successful reading. Fluency is associated with reading comprehension and overall reading proficiency, and it can be measured easily and reliably. Moreover, instruction to foster fluency is possible, and such instruction can lead to generalized improvements in comprehension as well as in word recognition, textual phrasing, and overall fluency. It is essential for developing readers to acquire the automatic-word and phraseprocessing abilities that are key components of fluent reading, because the development of these processing abilities permit the reader to devote his or her limited cognitive capacity to the primary task of reading—comprehension. Timothy Rasinski See Also Automaticity and Reading Fluency; ReadingComprehension Processes; Word Recognition References Allington, Richard. 1983. “Fluency: The Neglected Goal of the Reading Program.” Reading Teacher 36:556–561. Hasbrouck, Jan E., and Gerald Tindal. 1992. “Curriculum-Based Oral Reading Fluency Norms for Students in Grades 2 through 5.” Teaching Exceptional Children 24 (3):41–45. Pinnell, Gay S., John J. Pikulski, Karen K. Wixson, Jay R. Campbell, Phillip B. Gough, and Alexandra S. Beatty. 1995. Listening to Children Read Aloud. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, U.S. Department of Education. Rasinski, Timothy. 2000. Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Samuels, S. Jay. 1979. “The Method of Repeated Readings.” Reading Teacher 32:403–408. Schreiber, Peter. 1980. “On the Acquisition of Reading Fluency.” Journal of Reading Behavior 12:177–186. Zutell, Jerry, and Timothy Rasinski. 1991. “Training Teachers to Attend to Their Students’ Oral Reading Fluency.” Theory Into Practice 30:211–217. Measuring Reading Fluency Reading fluency is most easily measured through reading rate. Rate data obtained from oneminute oral-reading probes (curriculum-based measurement) and measured in words read correctly per minute (wcpm) provide a simple and consistent way for teachers to assess reading fluency that combines rate and accuracy. Jan Hasbrouck and Gerald Tindal (1992) reported midyear average reading rates of 78 wcpm for second grade, 93 wcpm for third grade, 112 wcpm for fourth grade, and 118 wcpm for fifth grade. Jerry Zutell and Timothy Rasinski (1991) also noted that fluency can be assessed more holistically through careful listening to students’ oral reading and then rating the readings on a descriptive scale. In his dissertation, Rasinski found that such a rating scale was a significant predictor of third- and fifth-grade students’ comprehension and overall reading achievement. A similar finding on fourth graders was reported by Gay Sue Pinnell and her colleagues (1995). 193 G boys would directly state their ideas and were more likely to argue their position with comments such as, “That’s wrong because . . . .” Moreover, the boys’ perception of the girls’ language style was that it indicated a lack of knowledge on the girls’ part and implied that girls did not understand physics as well as boys. The type of talk used in discussions may also be influenced by gender. In her study of the different types of talk used by girls and boys in literature discussions, Meredith Cherland (1992) found that girls tend to use a discourse of feeling, but boys tend to use a discourse of action. According to Cherland, a discourse of feeling focuses on the emotion in the text, deals with human relationships, values caring, and looks at the plot in terms of how it helps the reader understand character development. In contrast, a discourse of action is concerned with logic, values reason and believability, and seeks meaning in the plot and action. Cherland called these differences gendered talk and hypothesized that a discourse of action is more likely to be valued in a male-dominated society. Another way of addressing how gender influences discussion is to look at how girls and boys use their talk to achieve different ends. In her study of an eighth-grade language arts class, Heather Blair (2000) found that boys talked in order to establish status in their group. The boys frequently discredited and belittled others and made overt sexual references, including homophobic insults. Through such discourse patterns, the boys obtained and reinforced their power and privilege in the class. In contrast, the girls used their discussions to build their relationships, to identify with those they saw as their friends, and to separate themselves from those not viewed as friends. Gender and Discussion According to research on students’ speech patterns in classrooms, gender plays a role in determining how students engage in discussion. Much of this research focuses on the differences between boys’ and girls’ discourse patterns. More recent work, however, has begun to advocate moving beyond such dichotomous forms of conceptualizing how gender influences classroom talk. Differences between Girls’ and Boys’ Discussion Patterns Some common distinctions associated with gender are: girls have the discussion floor less than boys, talk less when they do have the floor, often have their ideas disregarded, and are interrupted more than boys; girls are less likely to answer questions and are more likely to belittle or doubt their own ideas. For example, in her study of science discussions in a first-grade classroom, Karen Gallas (1995) found a small group of powerful boys who dominated the discussions and actively worked to prevent other students from breaking into the conversation. In their study of seventh- and eighth-grade language arts classes, Donna Alvermann and her colleagues (1997) found that girls often engaged in what they termed “sorry talk. In such talk, girls qualified or apologized for their contributions to classroom discussions and consequently, diluted the power of what they had to say.” Another way girls dilute the authority of their contributions is to pose their ideas as questions rather than statements. In their study of discussions in high-school physics classes, Barbara Guzzetti and Wayne Williams (1996) found that girls tended to phrase their ideas in ways such as, “Have you thought about this. . . .” In contrast, 195 Gender and Discussion Girls and boys in a discussion group (Elizabeth Crews) may choose to be silent during discussions—not because they have nothing to contribute but rather as a means of resistance to the male-dominated discourse patterns established. The girls in Guzzetti and Williams’s study (1996) often declined to engage in discussions, particularly debates where ideas were being refuted. When asked about their participation patterns, the girls responded that they felt ineffectual in relaying their ideas and perceived their opinions to be dismissed by the boys. Moreover, in later discussions with the students, the boys’ statements indicated that they were proud of the ways in which they dominated and silenced the girls in their class. Another example of girls choosing to be silent due to male dominance occurred in Karen Evans and her colleagues’ (1998) study of fifth-grade literature discussion groups. In one discussion group, the boys used their talk as a means of establishing power. They controlled who talked and what could be talked about and frequently made demeaning, hurtful remarks to the girls in Similar results were found in Guzzetti and Williams’s (1996) high-school study. In their small-group discussions, girls appeared to engage in a collaborative discourse style characterized by more interaction, more willingness to consider others’ opinions, and an emphasis on consensus building. In contrast, the boys engaged in an independent discourse style in which they were more aggressive and competitive in their talk and less likely to negotiate shared meanings. These same patterns have also been found in adult discussions. In her study of gendered discourse practices in a graduate-level course, Margaret Gritsavage (1997) found that women were more likely than men to validate others’ comments, whereas men were more likely to interrupt women (not the other men), compete for the discussion floor, and evaluate others’ comments. Interpreting Silence in Discussion One possible consequence of such gendered discourse patterns is that students, particularly girls, 196 Gender and Discussion the group. One girl member tried to initiate book-related discussions; however, her attempts were unilaterally defeated by the boys, who preferred to engage in insulting conversation directed toward the other female member. After two days of discussion, this girl also began to be a target of the boys’ insults. Rather than continue her attempts to engage in book-related discussions and risk being insulted even further, she became a virtual silent member of the group while the boys continued to belittle the other female member. Consequently, this research reveals how gendered discourse can result in silencing students, particularly girls, and illustrates how some students may have compelling reasons for not speaking. der discourse patterns by assuming the conversational floor and keeping it for a substantial amount of time, dominating parts of the discussion, and forcefully voicing her opinions. However, Heather also exhibited gender-typical discourse through the use of comments that revealed her need to maintain a relationship with others in her group and a desire to have her group members enjoy the story. Bronwyn Davies (1993) also found girls and boys engaging in ways that contested gendertypical forms of discourse. In her study, a group of sixth-grade students were discussing a traditional fairy tale (i.e., one that reinforces gender stereotypes) and a feminist fairy tale (i.e., a version of a traditional fairy tale that contradicts gender stereotypes). In this discussion, a boy named James engaged in numerous gender-typical discourse patterns such as interrupting, talking over people, and disrupting what others were saying. However, James was also the group member most willing to interpret the feminist fairy tale in feminist ways. For example, he connected heroism with a female character, suggested that the female protagonist could be ugly, and argued that beauty does not necessarily equate with goodness. In his group, however, James’s contributions were consistently ignored and unaccepted, particularly by the girls in his group. The girls’ efforts to reject his contributions illustrate how they engaged in dominating discourse practices that led to James’s withdrawing from the conversation. Students such as Heather and James help reveal the limits of conceptualizing gender as discrete categories of boy and girl. Students seldom fit neatly into one of these categories, and their contributions to discussions rarely reveal only one type of gender-typical discourse pattern. Moving beyond Dichotomous Gender Differences Recently, researchers like Donna Alvermann and David Moore have begun to argue that research on gender and discussion needs to move away from conceptualizing gender in dichotomous, essentialized ways. Such a notion of gender separates females and males into discrete categories and emphasizes the differences between them. Furthermore, these researchers suggest that this notion of gender runs the risk of perpetuating gender stereotypes and limits our ability to see the complexity with which gender influences discussion. This research calls attention to the multiple subjectivities and cultural influences (i.e., age, gender, race, economic status) students bring with them to discussions and the fluid nature with which students move among the various identities and discourses available to them. In other words, all boys do not always engage in a discourse of action, dominate the discussion, and use their talk as a means of establishing power, just as all girls do not always engage in a discourse of feeling, relinquish the speaking floor, and use their talk as a means of establishing relationships. Rather, boys and girls move among the possible discourse patterns available and display behavior consistent across gender roles. This was evident in David Moore’s (1997) study of discussions in a twelfth-grade Advanced English Placement class. In this class, students exhibited behavior and used discourse patterns that both accepted and contested traditional gender expectations. For example, in one discussion, a girl named Heather contested typical gen- Influence of Context on Students’ Gendered Discussion Patterns Recent research has also begun to explore how the discussion context influences which multiple subjectivities and gendered discourse patterns are taken up by students. Subjectivities such as gender, ethnicity, ability, and status, along with classroom culture and norms, all have the potential to influence how gender interacts with students’ discussion patterns. Moreover, these various factors all interact with each other. Consequently, it may be impractical to single out 197 Gender and Discussion and separate the influence of gender from these other factors. Research has suggested that gender is a powerful factor operating in the discussion context. The impact of being in a heterogeneous- or homogeneous-gender group was investigated by Karen Evans (2002) in her study of fifth-grade literature discussion groups. When in heterogeneous-gender groups, girls were likely to either exhibit male discourse patterns (i.e., dominate discussion, control who could talk) or remain silent. Both options created problems in many heterogeneous-gender groups. When used by females, the male discourse patterns were viewed by others as aggressive. The girls’ silence was often a form of resistance to the perceived male discourse patterns being used by the boys in their group. The boys, however, interpreted the girls’ silence as noncompliance with the assigned task. When in all-girl groups, girls were much more likely to actively facilitate discussion among their female group members, using their talk to maintain relationships and identify with friends. Furthermore, homogeneous groups (both all-boy and all-girl) were more successful overall than heterogeneous groups, and students consistently stated that they preferred to work in samegender discussion groups. Guzzetti and Williams (1996) also found differences between homogeneous- and heterogeneous-gender groups at the high-school level. When in heterogeneous lab groups, males were the ones engaged in manipulating the equipment, giving directions, and making verbal inferences about their observations. Females were often limited to setting up the equipment and recording data that the boys generated through actually conducting the experiment. Even in groups where there was only one male, that male was still most likely to give orders, assume agreement by the females in the group, and talk to demonstrate or show the girls how to proceed. In contrast, when in a homogeneous-gender lab group, the females became much more active in their participation. They set up the equipment, manipulated the experiment, identified errors and resolved them, made observations, recorded data, and negotiated meanings. This research suggests that the discussion context influences, at least partially, which of the various gendered discourses students choose to use. Homogeneous-gender groups appear to have a particular impact on how females choose to participate. In same-gender groups, females are more likely to become active participants and engage in a wider range of verbal interactions. These findings should not be interpreted to mean that teachers should only use same-gender groups, but rather, they reveal the importance of broadening educators’ notions of acceptable forms of discourse, valuing different types of discourse, and breaking down commonly held stereotypes regarding gendered discourse that traditionally tend to privilege discourse patterns most frequently associated with males. Interventions Researchers have begun to explore ways of interrupting gendered discursive practices. One possible method is to place students in homogeneous-gender groups. Homogenous-gender groups appear to influence particularly the ways girls choose to participate in small-group settings (Evans, 2002; Guzzetti and Williams, 1996). Gallas (1995) also tried various ways of interrupting the discourse patterns established by the small group of dominating boys in her classroom. She established what she termed talk protocols, which included such procedures as limiting the number of comments the boys could make, monitoring the amount of time they talked, and having the speaker select the next person to talk. She also tried giving the boys other jobs to perform during the discussion (i.e., watching for people who are trying to get into the discussion) and separated the talkers and nontalkers into different groups. Methods like these represent possible ways teachers might begin to interrupt gendered discourse practices. As Alvermann and her colleagues (1997) caution, however, teachers are often unaware of how they themselves contribute to gendered discourse practices, which makes it difficult for them to change such patterns. Recognizing that gender does influence how students choose to participate in discussions is a first and necessary step toward helping students and teachers examine and interrupt the hidden and often taken-for-granted manner in which gender impacts discursive practices. Karen S. Evans See Also Discussion; Gender and Post-Typographical Text; Gender and Reading; Gender and Writing; Subjectivity 198 Gender and Post-Typographical Text Acknowledging that the fundamental changes accompanying electronic text have social, political, and cultural implications, teachers and researchers seek to understand the implications of these changes for all populations and at all levels of schooling. Most researchers who study this question view gender as a social construction, that is, as learned through interactions with others. The studies of gender and post-typographical text have focused on a range of students from elementary through college levels. Many of these studies are identified in a large project in which a group of researchers (Guzzetti et al., 2002) systematically reviewed the literature on gender and literacies. References Alvermann, Donna E., Michelle Commeyras, Josephine P. Young, Sally Randall, and David Hinson. 1997. “Interrupting Gendered Discursive Practices in Classroom Talk about Texts: Easy to Think About, Difficult to Do.” Journal of Literacy Research 29 (1):73–104. Blaire, Heather A. 2000. “Genderlects: Girl Talk and Boy Talk in a Middle-Years Classroom. Language Arts 77 (4):315–323. Cherland, Meredith R. 1992. “Gendered Readings: Cultural Restraints upon Response to Literature.” New Advocate 5 (3):187–198. Davies, Bronwyn. 1993. “Beyond Dualism and Towards Multiple Subjectivities.” In Linda K. Christian-Smith, ed., Texts of Desire: Essays on Fiction, Femininity, and Schooling, pp. 145–173. London: Falmer Press. Evans, Karen S. 2002. “Fifth-Grade Students’ Perceptions of How They Experience Literature Discussion Groups.” Reading Research Quarterly 37(1):46–69. Evans, Karen S., Donna Alvermann, and Patricia L. Anders. 1998. “Literature Discussion Groups: An Examination of Gender Roles.” Reading Research and Instruction 37 (2):107–122. Gallas, Karen. 1995. Talking Their Way into Science: Hearing Children’s Questions and Theories, Responding with Curricula. New York: Teachers College Press. Gritsavage, Margaret. December 1997. “Gendered Discourse in Classroom Conversations about Gender, Culture, and Literacy.” Paper presented at the National Reading Conference, Phoenix, AZ. Guzzetti, Barbara J., and Wayne O. Williams. 1996. “Changing the Pattern of Gendered Discussion: Lessons from Science Classrooms.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy 40 (1):38–47. Moore, David W. 1997. “Some Complexities of Gendered Talk about Texts.” Journal of Literacy Research 29 (4):507–530. Gender and Post-Typographical Text in the College Environment Early studies focusing on gender and post-typographical text took place in college writing classes in which students shared writing and responses to writing through on-line “conferences,” both synchronous and asynchronous. Authors of some of these studies (Selfe, 1992) cautioned against gender bias but also observed the promise of more democratic, equitable discussions through post-typographical text (Cooper and Selfe, 1990). Basic to the positive findings were certain qualities of communicating through post-typographical text: the time provided for considering one’s thoughts, a focus on the language of the communication rather than on the participant’s appearance or nonverbal gestures, and the possibility of building an on-line community as a result of these features. Such unique literacy forums provided through asynchronous e-mail, listservs, and computer conferencing offered participants appealing freedom. For example, in several distance-learning writing classes taught entirely through electronic communication, adult female students were empowered to voice their ideas and thus affirm and in several cases change some of their perspectives (Fey, 1994). Initially, the students read feminist and multicultural readings and then responded to them through post-typographical text. They then wrote essays that were peer edited through the computer medium. Although the positive claims of the benefits of post-typographical text for communication at the college level continue to emerge, questions continue to be raised about whether on-line Gender and Post-Typographical Text The topic of gender and post-typographical (electronic) text has emerged in the field of literacy because electronic text is created and used by both women and men alike. Since computer technology has traditionally been considered a male domain, it is important to ensure that interactions with this text are fair for all users. Gender stereotypes associated with attitudes toward computer use and reports of male dominance have stimulated this concern. 199 Gender and Post-Typographical Text classroom discussions are empowering for all women students. As with face-to-face classroom experiences, using post-typographical text for a classroom medium is complex. At the college level, gender considerations in the use of electronic communication in a mixed-gender class appear to depend on an array of factors, for example, the curriculum, the unique characteristics of participants, and the teacher’s pedagogical approach. terms of how they used computers and the topics they chose to write about on-line. She also discovered gender bias on the part of the teacher as she assisted students in using the computers. John Pryor (1995), an elementary teacher in the United Kingdom, also aimed to discover ways to promote gender equity in the use of computers. Concerned that even when teachers are sensitive to gender issues they are not always effective in bringing about gender equity, Pryor engaged in a two-year study in which he worked in a fourth-grade classroom once a week to understand gender issues as they applied to groups of students working with computers. He observed that boys were more likely than girls to engage in conflict because they were less able than girls to distinguish between debating and quarreling. When emphasis was placed on the process of group work rather than the results, the boys’ language became less harsh and competitive. Mixed-ability grouping also supported Pryor’s goal of improving collaboration and promoting gender equity. Cynthia Lewis and Bettina Fabos (2000) turned to instant messaging to examine social practices when post-typographical text is used in an out-of-class environment. Instant messaging enables participants to communicate to one another in real time, that is, simultaneously. The researchers focused on the extracurricular home use of instant messaging so that teachers might also understand students’ out-of-school literacy practices. The two middle-school girls in their study reported that instant messaging eased their communication with classmates and at times enabled them to hide their own identities by transforming their language patterns and tone. Through instant messaging, the girls were able to negotiate social relationships and enhance their social standing at school. Even though students had an opportunity to speak up, however, masculine conventions determined their stance. Consequently, Lewis and Fabos doubted that instant messaging provides a completely genderneutral environment. Gender and Post-Typographical Text in the School Environment The role gender plays in the use of post-typographical text in the schools is also a concern. Although research focused on this subject is limited, researchers and teachers are beginning to understand the interplay of gender and posttypographical text in the schools. In some instances, instead of promoting gender equity, electronic communication appears to reproduce the hierarchy still present in contemporary culture. To understand how and when gender attitudes toward computers are instilled, Julie Nicholson and her colleagues (1998) observed groups of first-grade students as they worked in same-gender and mixed-gender groups to compose stories on the computer without the direction of a teacher. The young male students destroyed female partners’ confidence through constant critical remarks, threats, and strong directives. Although girls supported and encouraged collaboration with one another, boys created competition by comparing stories and pointing out differences in a competitive manner. Researchers concluded that young girls’ confidence in using computers waned when they were antagonized and criticized by boys during their computer sessions. In a feminist teacher-research project describing third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade girls’ interactions with post-typographical text, Alice Christie (1995) reported inconsistent results in her efforts to support students in gender equitable uses of the computer. She aimed to disrupt the stereotypical attitudes toward gender and technology. Christie noted that girls more than boys most commonly used post-typographical text to build relationships and share feelings of concern, but she also found that young girls used computer texts to defy gender stereotypes (as did boys) in Gender and Post-Typographical Text in School-College Collaborations With the ease of communicating through the Internet and the increasing availability of Internet connections in schools and colleges, teachers are taking advantage of the possibilities of extending 200 Gender and Reading References Christie, Alice. 1995. “No Chips on Their Shoulders: Girls, Boys, and Telecommunications.” Ph.D. diss., Arizona State University West, Phoenix. Cooper, Marilyn M., and Cynthia L. Selfe. 1990. “Computer Conferences and Learning: Authority, Resistance, and Internally Persuasive Discourse.” College English 52:847–869. Fey, Marion H. 1994. “Finding Voice through Computer Communication: A New Venue for Collaboration.” Journal of Advanced Composition 14 (1):221–237. ———. 1997. “Literate Behavior in a Cross-Age Computer-Mediated Discussion: A Question of Empowerment.” In Charles K. Kinzer, Kathleen A. Hinchman, and Donald J. Leu, eds., Inquiries in Literacy Theory and Practice: Forty-Sixth Yearbook of the National Reading Conference, pp. 507–518. Chicago: National Reading Conference. ———. 1998. “Critical Literacy in School-College Collaboration through Computer Networking: A Feminist Research Project.” Journal of Literacy Research 30:85–117. Guzzetti, Barbara, Josephine Young, Margaret Gritsavage, Laurie Fyfe, and Marie Hardenbrook. 2002. Reading, Writing and Talking Gender in Literacy Learning. Newark, DE: The International Reading Association/The National Reading Conference Literacy Series. Lewis, Cynthia, and Bettina Fabos. 2000. “But Will It Work in the Heartland? A Response and Illustration.” Journal of Adolescent Literacy 43 (5):462–469. Nicholson, Julie, Adrienne Gelpi, Shannon Young, and Elizabeth Sulzby. 1998. “Influences of Gender and Open-Ended Software on First Graders’ Collaborative Composing Activities on Computers.” Journal of Computing in Childhood Education 9 (1):3–42. Pryor, John. 1995. “Gender Issues in Group Work—A Case Study Involving Computers.” British Educational Research Journal 21 (30):277–288. Selfe, Cynthia L. 1992. “Preparing English Teachers for the Virtual Age: The Case for Technology Critics.” In Gail E. Hawisher and Paul LeBlanc, eds., Re-Imagining Computers and Composition: Teaching and Research in the Virtual Age, pp. 24–42. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook. classroom communication to populations beyond the school through electronic communication. This use of post-typographical text also supports the increased focus in schools and colleges on authentic learning and service learning. As preservice teachers link to school students in many areas of the country, they benefit from working with actual students and in many cases are exposed to populations of students different from those they would observe during field visits to local schools. These connections also perform a type of service learning since each population helps the other to meet its learning goals. At this point, most of these connections are through asynchronous electronic communication, although links through real-time, synchronous communication are beginning to be explored as well. As with other uses of post-typographical text, school-college collaborations are being evaluated with respect to gender issues. Two research studies on school-college electronic collaborations conducted by teachers (Fey, 1997, 1998) evaluated the communications with respect to gender and, particularly, language use. Both collaborations used the asynchronous communication of listservs to link college students in a small town with high-school students in urban and suburban schools. The topics for discussion adopted a critical literacy stance that focused on issues of power and assisted students in challenging existing structures of inequality and oppression. In the 1997 collaboration, discussions about ethical issues led to agonistic, hierarchical language by some males. Although a number of students reported positive experiences, some males inhibited the expression of voice for several female participants, whose participation in the conversation was thereby limited. In the 1998 collaboration, females persisted in communicating despite differences or conflict in discussions. The contexts for the two studies differed in a number of respects, for example, the number of participants, the students’ understanding of gender issues, and the time period of involvement of the students. A closer look at features such as these may lead to a better understanding of ways to assure positive learning experiences with posttypographical text. Marion Harris Fey Gender and Reading Until as late as the 1980s, discussions about reading and gender were limited for the most part to calling attention to the gender of a protagonist of any given story. With a deepening understanding of societal influences, scholars have focused at- See Also Gender and Discussion; Gender and Writing; Instant Messaging; Post-Typographic 201 Gender and Reading dered. Girls tend to select romance and fiction; boys tend to read adventure and nonfiction. Boys read superhero comic books whereas girls read series novels and teen magazines, marketed especially for girls. Magazine selection as well as production is gendered as well. Magazines that target girls are targeted for the teenage girl. Magazines for boys are most often aimed at an adult readership (Moss, 1995). Texts, ranging from fairy tales to adult novels, have been criticized because they seem to constrain the roles available to boys and girls and serve to reinscribe them into patriarchy. Like the fairy tales, popular book series are written in such a way as to construct models of masculinity and femininity that reinforce gender stereotypes, with males in active, powerful roles and females relegated to roles in which they find their place in the society through romance and rescuers. Cultural Constructions of Gender and Reading Before examining gender and reading in more depth, let’s clarify each term. More than simply the abilities to decode and comprehend, reading is now understood as a set of social practices constructed by families, schools, and other institutions. Understanding reading as a social and cultural construction focuses attention to the ways in which certain practices are privileged or ignored in particular contexts. Reading carries with it significant cultural capital that governs social actions and social consequences. As concerns the term gender, scholars now argue for a more cultural definition, situating it within broader societal forces, as has been done with the term reading. Beyond biology, scholars now accept a more constructed view of gender. This shifts the focus from biological processes to the social processes that enable and constrain beliefs, practices, and even desires. Meredith Cherland (1994), for example, explains that gender has to do not so much with what people are (male or female) but with what they do (gender-appropriate ways of acting in the world). Gender-appropriate actions involve a multifaceted set of culturally constructed actions. Reading is one of the culturally constructed actions that makes up the complex system of societal influences. Because gender is culturally constructed and reading is a social practice, gender is always present in reading (Cherland, 1994). Scholars such as Teenage girls reading magazines (Laura Dwight) tention on gender to examine the social, political, and developmental nature of reading. Likewise, attentiveness to reading makes gender distinctions apparent. Reading is one means through which girls and boys learn to construct and reconstruct their desires and gender roles, as well as their awareness of social positions and power dynamics (Christian-Smith, 1993). In other words, reading is one means by which young people come to know their place in the world, and that place comes already equipped with gender scripts that dictate what is appropriate to wear, to do, to say, and even to read as a gendered being in a society. From a very early age, children have clear ideas about demarcations along gender lines. They can tell you as early as the primary grades that certain books are “boys’ books” and that others are clearly “girls’ books.” By the early elementary grades, children may tease and chastise others for making gender-inappropriate reading choices. In school as well as out of school, reading choices are distinctly gen202 Gender and Reading Cherland, Linda Christian-Smith, Pam Gilbert, and others describe the complex ways in which entering a story world allows readers to come to know what counts as being a woman or a man in the world. Through stories, children learn the range of ways to be masculine and feminine in a culture. Inversely, readers enter a story world with a repertoire of already gendered social practices that influence their interpretations. Scholars have begun to ask about the contents of texts and, equally important, about how texts are being read. Gender is never the sole variable that determines how texts get read. Race, class, gender, and many other influences come into play in establishing how reading is enacted. That said, it is important to repeat that gender is always inescapably present in reading. another lens to examine reading and gender. Talk about texts provides a means to perform gender roles. Children enter the classroom with a repertoire of already gendered language practices that constitute their resources for interpreting and participating in classroom discussions. Joseph Tobin (2000), for example, has noted that he observed third- and fourth-grade boys and girls using a discussion of violence in a film to display gender. The boys showcased their knowledge of the physical world, something that tends to be considered masculine; the girls made statements about their empathy for the victims. A classroom is always an arena to perform gender roles, and girls and boys stand to gain or lose status based on their gender competence. When a teacher asks children to select texts, she must realize that she is asking her students to perform gender roles. Girls may read “boys’ books,” but boys rarely accept an invitation to read what they consider to be a “girl book.” Why? Gemma Moss (1995) suggests that we are asking the powerless to take on more powerful strategies, and inversely, we are asking the more powerful to appropriate less powerful strategies. Such invitations are not equal. How boys and girls read and talk about particular texts center on reading as it intertwines with a gendered sense of self. How home, school, and societal influences shape reading practices have also been the focus of studies of how children learn to read. For example, Judith Solsken (1993) conducted a threeyear study of early literacy learning. Her study of children as they made the transition from entering kindergarten through completion of second grade documents that in even progressive schools and supportive homes, the dimensions of gender identity may perpetuate traditional gender roles and actually account for important links that influence reading. Such accounts of learning to read showcase the complexities that both boys and girls face due to gendered divisions of labor in their homes and in school. Through examinations of the ways in which larger home, school, and societal influences shape reading practices, we see how gender and reading impinge upon one another. Reading is one means by which children come to understand the range of ways to be masculine and feminine in a culture. Equally important, gender as a set of social practices shapes the ways in which children engage with texts. Reading Gender Scholars have approached reading gender in a variety of ways. In an attempt to understand how gender and reading impinge upon each other, scholars have focused on the larger home, school, and societal influences that shape reading practices, examining content analyses of texts, readers reading texts, and how children learn to read. In a content analysis, researchers look at story lines, descriptions, and pictures in an attempt to understand the construction of the feminine and the masculine in a textual world. Some scholars challenge the notions that texts hold fixed meanings, and thus, it is not the content alone that should be examined. Assumptions that texts are potentially powerful in their effect on readers rest upon a notion of vulnerable readers (Moss, 1995). What matters most about the texts is not the content alone but the ways in which readers use that content. Studies of how readers read text focus not simply on the content of the reading materials but on how readers interact with those textual worlds. In a study of romance readers, for example, Janice Radway (1984) argued that adult women constructed reading as a “declaration of their independence,” citing a private pleasure and an escape from their daily responsibilities. In a study of sixth-grade girls, Cherland (1994) maintained that reading may serve as “combative” (as an escape from being good) and “compensatory” (as a tool to feel more powerful). In addition to gaining understanding of how children read, how they talk about reading provides 203 Gender and Reading Curriculum and School Performance Curricular materials and discussion patterns have been criticized for gender bias. A predominance of gender bias in textbooks and other curricular materials is well documented. The structures of classrooms and selections of texts dictate how children construct and experience gender (Cherland, 1994). Gender expectations shape how children construct and experience reading. Although studies show that boys may receive more attention in some classes, the teaching of reading is often absent in such studies. Since girls tend to outperform in reading, there is little attention to the language arts, which according to a cross-national study found that the perception of literacy as feminine shapes the ways schools teach reading (American Association of University Women, 1998), which may further the gender gap in reading competencies. Views that females are “naturally” more inclined to be better readers and that expectations that boys will lag behind seem to be just good common sense. Students themselves hold these gendered views of reading, with distinctions between male and female competencies in reading becoming more intensified after the fourth grade and becoming deeply entrenched by the last years of high school (American Association of University Women, 1998). Notions of good reading performance may be based for the most part on middle-class, white cultural views, and according to Signithia Fordham (1993), the denial of diversity of gender constructions in schools forces some social groups to silence. Inattentiveness to diversity of gender roles in the school context serves to marginalize some readers, especially those who do not share the racial, ethnic, socioeconomic, or cultural expectations of the school. who have brought together scholars from content areas as diverse as mathematics, science, physical education, technology, and literacy, call for a pedagogy that examines privilege and power. They suggested that across content disciplines, the curriculum at its core should pay attention to the processes that produce patterns of participation and achievement. Moss has suggested the need for an “ethnography of reading” pedagogy that stresses the ways in which diverse social and cultural practices shape how texts get read. In other words, these scholars have championed the need in schools for boys and girls to examine what is taken as “natural” in any content and to investigate the politics and privileges embedded in their reading, whatever it might be—fiction, nonfiction, textbooks, or the larger culture. Margaret Finders See Also Gender and Discussion; Gender and PostTypographical Text; Gender and Writing References American Association of University Women. 1998. Gender Gaps: Where Schools Still Fail Our Children. Washington, DC: AAUW Educational Foundation. Cherland, Meredith. 1994. Private Practices: Girls Reading Fiction and Constructing Identity. London: Taylor and Francis. Christian-Smith, Linda. 1993. “Constituting and Reconstituting Desire: Fiction, Fantasy and Femininity.” In Linda Christian-Smith, ed., Texts of Desire: Essays on Fiction, Femininity, and Schooling, pp. 1–8. London: Falmer Press. Fordham, Signithia. 1993. “Those Loud Black Girls: (Black) Women, Silence, and Gender ‘Passing’ in the Academy.” Anthropology and Education Quarterly 24:3–32. Gaskell, Jane, and John Willinsky. 1995. Gender In/Forms Curriculum: From Enrichment to Transformation. New York: Teachers College Press. Gilbert, Pam. 1994. “And They Lived Happily Ever After: Cultural Storylines and the Construction of Gender.” In A. H. Dyson and C. Genishi, eds., The Need for Story. Urbana, IL: NCTE. Moss, Gemma. 1995. “Rewriting Reading.” In Janet Holland and Maud Blair, eds., with Sue Sheldon. Debates and Issues in Feminist Research and Pedagogy, pp. 157–168. Buckingham, UK: Open University. Radway, Janice. 1984. Reading the Romance: Women, Patriarchy, and Popular Fiction. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina. Solsken, Judith. 1993. Literacy, Gender, and Work in Families and in School. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Initiatives and Pedagogical Approaches Recent initiatives have focused on pedagogical approaches that call for examinations of biases in texts, interaction patterns, and the larger culture. Scholars are calling for a pedagogy that builds from an awareness of the influence of both language and culture upon students’ lives. Cherland (1994), for example, called for a critical pedagogy that explores vested interests in texts and examines the ways in which individuals are positioned to read and respond in certain ways. Likewise, Jane Gaskell and John Willinsky (1995), 204 Gender and Writing Tobin, Joseph. 2000. Good Guys Don’t Wear Hats: Children Talk about the Media. New York: Teachers College Press. two terms interchangeably. Trying to make distinctions between biological difference and processes of socialization suggests that one can clearly distinguish between naturally determined and culturally determined characteristics of men and women. As Judith Butler, Michel Foucault, and others have pointed out, however, conceptions of sex are as constructed as gender, making any distinctions between the two difficult to determine. The complex understandings of genetics and hormones that are emerging, for example, point to the difficulty of distinguishing innate or biological influences from those that are culturally and socially constructed. Much of the gender research and theory has been guided and influenced by feminist movements and postcolonial and queer theory, all of which critique power structures, question and distinguish differences, and shift thinking away from hierarchical models. Gender and Writing Gender is a variable in the historical, social, and cultural contexts that influence and are influenced by the creation of texts and writing processes. Gender differs from sex in that the former distinguishes the cultural categories of masculine and feminine, whereas the latter distinguishes biological difference. Because writing gives humans the capability to reflect and analyze language and is closely connected to self-understanding, the role of gender in the development of subjectivity through writing is an important consideration. Four main perspectives on the relationship between writing and gender are: (1) organic and embodied, (2) socially constructed, (3) poststructural, and (4) complex. The organic and embodied perspective derives from the development of a two-sex model and considers physical and psychosocial differences between women and men and how these differences influence writing. The socially constructed perspective focuses on cultural, historical, and social influences and how men and women are socialized differently, affecting the structure of their texts and their writing processes. The post-structural perspective considers the discursive and shifting nature of gender and subjectivity and its relationship to writing as a discursive practice. The complex perspective draws on recent ecological theories of complexity to understand gender and writing as coevolving within human and nonhuman systems. Writing Writing is a powerful technology for thinking and learning that commits the word to space and enlarges the potential of language, including its heuristic power. Writing creates conceptions of reality, influences individuals’ understanding of themselves, and traces their relations to places and other people. Writing also serves to integrate and interpret experience, creating a subjunctive or liminal space for real, imagined, and possible lives of those who write and read. Writing makes possible such understandings because it enables human beings to reflect on ideas; that is, consciousness of words creates a distinction between the words themselves and the ideas they express. Such capabilities mean that humans can use written language as an object of reflection and analysis. Because the practice of writing is so strongly connected to self-understanding and the development of subjectivity, the influences of gender are important when considering such a process. Gender Gender, like genre, arises from the Latin root genus, meaning birth or kind. Traditionally, gender was a grammatical classification in languages for masculine, feminine, or neuter nouns, whereas biological classifications used the term “sex.” When anthropologists and sociologists began to use gender to distinguish cultural categories from biological ones, gender became one of the descriptors for differences among humans that helped develop understandings of people’s identities. As its usage expanded, individuals sometimes equated gender with sex and used the Writing as Organic and Embodied Before the two-sex model of understanding difference, people considered male and female bodies as fundamentally the same with only the visibility of their genitalia contrasting. Once society used differing sexual characteristics to maintain power structures, the male body became the dominant and privileged form. Society consid205 Gender and Writing ered women to be lacking the attributes of the male; in other words, female identity was based on who women were not in comparison to men, rather than who they were. French philosopher Luce Irigaray explained that women are defined as complements of men and do not exist in their own right in the symbolic order. She argued for the creation of an ethics of sexual difference that defines women in their own terms and not in relation to men. The conception of the two differing sexes evolved from a focus on physiology to encompass the differences in psychology, with women again defined as lacking. The work of Nancy Chodorow, among others, challenged that conception. Chodorow argued that gender differences were created relationally. Because women are usually the primary caretakers of children, she noted, males, in order to establish their gender identity, must differentiate themselves from their mothers, whereas girls establish their gender identity through relating to and identifying with the mother. The work of Carol Gilligan also identified psychological distinctions between males and females in their decisionmaking processes, with males found to be hierarchical and focused on authority and females attending more to contextual factors. Mary Belenky and her collaborators (1986) highlighted other gender differences in their analysis of the interview transcripts of 135 women. From these data, they identified five distinct ways of women’s knowing. The five distinct ways of women’s knowing include silence—a place of not knowing where a woman feels she has no voice or power—and received knowing, in which the woman trusts the knowledge of others whom she sees as more powerful and knowledgeable and from whom she can learn. Subjective knowing describes personal and private knowledge based on intuition and feeling. Procedural knowing includes processes and techniques for acquiring, validating, and evaluating knowledge. Constructed knowing understands truth as contextual and knowledge as tentative rather than absolute, where the knower constructs the known. With the focus of gender differences being biological and psychological, a feminist aesthetic of writing developed—that is, an erotics of writing that comes from the self, that tends to be sensual and connected to the world and the body and that is seen to be potentially revolutionary in ques- tioning the existing structures of literary canons and the understanding of what it means to be female. Writers such as Adrienne Rich, Audre Lorde, and Susan Griffin have developed this feminist aesthetic through their writing. In her theoretical explorations of writing, Hélène Cixous (1991) explored connections among the female body, sexual expression, and writing, called l’écriture féminine. Cixous draws on the semiotic associated with the female body such as its fluidity and open boundaries, shaping her writing in response and creating a women’s language. Establishing a feminist aesthetic of writing based on specific characteristics of biology and psychology is difficult because of the global nature of the categories. Viewing writing from this perspective erases many of the differences within genders, such as cultural, historical, and social contexts, and suggests that writing strategies are without context. Some research that focuses on variables describing specific gender differences in writing points to the relational metaphors in women’s writing and the individuation choices in men’s writing and suggests that such evidence supports innate differences between males and females in their writing abilities. The work of many women writers, however, also highlights the strengths of this perspective in acknowledging that writing is embodied, that who we are shapes and is shaped by the texts we create and read, and that traces of our histories reside in language. Writing as Socially Constructed The role of cultural, historical, and social influences on gender and writing is an important characteristic of this perspective. As Mikhail Bakhtin noted, language is understood not as transparent or apolitical but as a site where discursive struggles occur between those who desire power through determining meaning and those who would interrupt that power for a plurality of meaning. In the 1970s, Robin Lakoff published her work Language and Woman’s Place, which pointed to the difficulty of representing women’s experience in a language that bears the marks of its male-defined history and that discriminates against women. Elaine Showalter (1989) proposed a different approach to literary criticism, which she called gynocriticism, examining how texts reveal ideological inscriptions of gender. These and other feminist theories, as well as the 206 Gender and Writing work of gender theorists, furthered the understanding of gender as part of a network of cultural and social relations. In understanding language as colored by its contexts, this perspective sees writing as a process that mediates cultural knowledge with textuality. Writers draw from these ideological and discursive systems while at the same time the discourses define the choices available to them. If women construct narratives about interactions of connection and men write about separation and achievement, they are reflecting the perceptual frameworks that have shaped them. When writers are conscious of such choices, they can use writing as a way to resist or subvert gender definitions or other cultural expectations. The work of Sandra Gilbert and Susan Gubar (1979), considered one of the turning points in the study of women’s writing, reveals how gender operates textually. Many genres, such as poetry and drama, erected barriers against women at a time when authorship was considered a male role. Men wrote social discourse; women expressed a self through confessional pieces or disguised their work with a masculine pseudonym. As Gilbert and Gubar pointed out, however, women writers were able to create submerged or hidden meanings even when writing within the constraints of the day. Some genres of writing continue to be privileged over others, but all genres are affected by gendered social regulation. For instance, some research that examines the gender differences in writing relates expository, objective, and linear prose with male writing. As writers become more skilled, researchers discover that it is more difficult to distinguish between “male” and “female” writing. Some studies suggest that capable writers can recognize and use various genre styles; others question whether women simply learn to write in the privileged academic discourse, that is, using expository and linear style. One must also consider the context in which the writing is read. Many readers assume they can identify pieces written by males or females, revealing that society continues to make assumptions about the clearly delineated presence of gender in texts. Susan Friedman described strategies that women use in their writing to question and subvert genres and canons and to claim the public space of texts. For example, they choose typical plot structures to write their own ideologies, such as rewriting fairy tales, or they reconfigure narrative patterns to structure their writing in ways meaningful to their experience. Alternatively, women weave together different genres such as oral and written conventions or the lyrical and the narrative. New relational patterns are created that can disrupt or unify seemingly contradictory or fragmented discourses. In seeing writing as socially constructed, questions continue about what is female writing and what is male. In the search for answers, research about writing and gender can mire researchers in syntactic and lexical levels of analysis rather than asking broader questions of discourse structure and use. Because of the complex nature of language, it is difficult to understand and trace the relation between gender and writing, so the temptation to single out one cause or one variable is common. At the same time, understanding the social construction of texts and gender encourages writers to push the boundaries, to rewrite the structures of genre, and to consider the interplay among race, class, sexual orientation, and gender through writing. Writing as Post-Structural Text There is a close relationship between the recognition of the social construction of texts and post-structuralism. Both theories recognize the complexity of cultural, historical, and social variables that influence gender and writing; poststructuralism, however, focuses more closely on how discourses create individuals’ subjectivity. Chris Weedon (1987) has defined subjectivity as a woman’s way of understanding herself and her relation to the world, including her conscious and unconscious thoughts and emotions. Because of its discursive character, subjectivity is constantly open to change, always shifting away from the modernist conception of a fixed and stable self at the center. Gender, as a discourse related to subjectivity, is understood not as a masculine-feminine binary but as a broader continuum of possibilities. From a post-structural perspective, writing is the learned social discursive practice of a gendered subject, open to negotiation and change. Going beyond familiar discursive patterns is difficult because they are recognizable, connecting with cultural mores to seem almost natural and invisible. At the same time, writing becomes a mode of knowing that can continually interro207 Graffiti gate its own methods and processes. For example, deconstruction, which questions the possibility of all-encompassing systems or discourses, can challenge the construction of a text, revealing the elisions and gaps. Post-structuralism points out the permeable nature of boundaries and, as with gender, understands that all writing contains elements of many genres and texts, creating an intertextual character. Frigga Haug has argued that because gendered patriarchal discourses have constructed women’s subjectivity, the importance of writing for women is crucial. Through such a process, they can create a history by retrieving from the dominant culture a new image of themselves, enabling them to construct alternative interpretations. To disrupt such expectations and the patriarchal structures of language and texts, some women turn to avant-garde writing, with its fragmented nature and subversion of pattern. Many are selfreflexive, deliberately drawing attention to the process of writing and the structures of the text, taking apart the inherited fabric of form, and melting the boundaries between genres. Weedon has suggested that is equally important for men to use post-structural techniques to deconstruct masculinity and its role in patriarchal power. Although fragmentation and self-reflexivity are strategies that critique existing literary frameworks, they are also becoming features of popular culture forms, such as music videos, and can stimulate consumption and maintain the status quo. Further, although such writing has opened up possibilities for women to disrupt some of the patriarchal structures that confined their writing, for some women, the decentering of the self discursively is of little value if their sense of self is still uncertain. The interests of individual writers, too, may be lost in deconstructionist criticism that continues to support white, middle-class patriarchy. Moreover, writers must realize that discursive practices are embedded in material power relations that also need transformation. Nevertheless, post-structural writing opens up greater possibilities for creating subjectivities and offers a way for writers to be oppositional within the ideologies and conceptual frameworks of patriarchy. tionship not easily defined and part of a web of relations that stretch beyond interpersonal interactions, cultural norms, and historical processes to nonhuman and subhuman systems. Laurie Finke (1992) sees complexity as a poetics that is cultural and indeterminate, drawing on the creative energy of chaos theory to highlight how order marginalizes, excludes, and neutralizes. As a literacy and complexity researcher, Finke, along with Katherine Hayles and others, explores the complex systems of which writing and gender are a part, emphasizing the importance of examining competing discourses and engaging in debates without resorting to essentialism, binary division, or uncritical assimilation. Understanding writing from such a perspective acknowledges that writing and gender together do not create a totalizing system that renders differences and contradictions invisible but rather are part of an ever-emerging pattern of cultural productions that feeds back into society, maintaining and refashioning it. Rebecca Luce-Kapler See Also Post-Structuralism and Structuralism; Subjectivity References Belenky, Mary Field, Blythe McVicker, Nancy Clinchy, Rule Goldberger, and Jill Mattuck Tarule. 1986. Women’s Ways of Knowing: The Development of Self, Voice, and Mind. New York: Basic Books. Cixous, Hélène. 1991. Coming to Writing and Other Essays. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Finke, Laurie A. 1992. Feminist Theory, Women’s Writing. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press. Gilbert, Sandra, and Susan Gubar. 1979. Madwoman in the Attic: The Woman Writer and the Nineteenth-Century Literary Imagination. New Haven: Yale University Press. Showalter, Elaine. 1989. Speaking of Gender. New York: Routledge. Weedon, Chris. 1987. Feminist Practice and PostStructuralist Theory. Cambridge and Oxford: Blackwell. Graffiti At a most basic level, graffiti can be defined simply as unsanctioned writing on public spaces. In a more complex rendering, graffiti can be considered to be systemically encoded expressions of identity, attempts to make meaning, or statements of resistance to dominant power struc- Writing and Complexity Complexity is a newly emerging focus that understands gender and writing as a complex rela208 Graffiti Example of graffiti (Elizabeth Birr Moje) graffiti writing quite similar to the kind found on walls in a modern urban city to examples in ancient Rome and Pompeii, and he argues that prehistoric cave writing is considered by some to be a form of graffiti. Although practiced worldwide and throughout history, the codes associated with graffiti are determined by both geographic and social space and by the set or identity of the graffiti writers. In addition, graffiti can be distinguished from more conventional writing practices by its fluidity. The codes and rules of graffiti writing are standardized by particular groups, but the standards are always changing. Because much of graffiti writing is connected to acts of resistance and underground practice, the codes change of necessity, as a way of maintaining secrecy and of confusing authorities who might seek to challenge the graffiti writers. The codes also change, however, as new ideas and expressions are exchanged. Sociologists and anthropologists have for years studied graffiti writing as a social practice embedded in gangs and tagging crews, but rarely have these scholars studied graffiti as an act of tures. In either definition, graffiti must be considered an act of literacy, one that integrates alphabetic and iconic representational forms in systematic ways. Ralph Cintron (1997) argued that graffiti writers appropriate mainstream symbols and recontextualize them into new meanings that allow the writers to enter an otherwise closed discourse. Elizabeth Moje (2000) has suggested that for a group of young people in one community, graffiti writing (and other gang-connected practices) provided a way for them “to be part of the story” in a place where their ethnic, classed, and religious stories were not heard or valued. Graffiti writing, from these perspectives, is usually a practice of marginalized individuals. Moreover, graffiti writing is not merely a resistant or deviant act, it is a literate practice used to claim a space and a voice in dominant society and to express oneself or one’s group identity to others who share that identity. Often thought of as a contemporary problem, graffiti is a historically significant, worldwide literacy practice. Matthew Hunt (1996) has traced 209 Graffiti literacy. Literacy research and theory has tended to focus on the reading and writing of conventional formulations of alphabetic print, mainly the print that one finds in published texts. Some exceptions include the work of Jill Aguilar, Ralph Cintron, Dwight Conquergood, Matthew Hunt, and Elizabeth Moje. In addition, Miriam Camitta, Amy Shuman, and Debbie Smith have studied the implications of graffiti writing and other “vernacular” literacy practices for classroom teaching (Camitta, 1993). by the number “187” written at the top of the list of gangs. The symbol “187” is a play on the standard police code for homicide and indicates a call to action that the members of the gangs listed in the graffito should be executed. The use of the Roman numeral XV indicates that the East Side King Crips claim the 15th Street territory. Second, the street names of the gangstas who make up the King Crips are written around the letters KC (e.g., Lil Blue, Budda, and so on). With this writing, each of the members of the 15th Street King Crips claims membership and affiliation with the gang. Budda, Lil Blue, Oreo, and the others are homeboys—dedicated to loyalty to each other and to the gang at all costs, even if such loyalty requires them to harm a non-gangaffiliated friend, a rival gang member who may be a friend in different circumstances, or another homeboy who has been disloyal to the gang. The “c” written under each street name or moniker indicates the writer’s ownership or copyright of that name and implies ownership of the larger public space as well. It is in relation to this claiming of the 15th Street space that a second point can be made about the meaning embedded in this particular graffito. Although the members of the surrounding community may not be able to read the code written by the 15th Street King Crips, they nonetheless receive a message about the claiming of space. To the people who live in this relatively affluent neighborhood, this unsanctioned writing is an encroachment on their territory. Despite the fact that the community members probably could not read a symbol like “187” to know that it calls for the death of rival gangstas, the discovery of such graffiti was met with shock and alarm by community members, and the wall was quickly buffed to erase the reminder of this encroachment on their territory (and to prevent would-be customers from avoiding the store). In contrast to gang graffiti, graffiti written or “thrown up” (Hunt, 1996) by taggers are considered artistic, although similarly unsanctioned, writings in public spaces. Tags are not used to claim territory but to advertise the individual artist or, at times, an entire tagging crew (a group of tagging artists). For nongangstas or nontaggers, tagger graffiti would probably be indistinguishable from gang graffiti. Taggers and gangstas, however, can easily distinguish types of graffiti and are respectful of the different types. What Counts as Graffiti? What, exactly, are graffiti? As in any language or code, general graffiti codes cannot be described for graffiti writers across the world but must be analyzed in particular groups. In addition, graffiti writing is typically connected to a number of other representational and communicative practices, including particular dress codes, body proxemics, and conventional reading and writing practices (Cintron, 1997; Conquergood, 1990; Hunt, 1996; Moje, 2000); thus, any description of the print alone provides only a partial description of the literate practice involved in graffiti writing. In broad terms, however, graffiti are stylized, unsanctioned writings in publicly visible spaces such as fences, billboards, sidewalks, or buildings (Aguilar, 2000; Hunt, 1996). This definition is not in itself fully explanatory, however. For example, “Car Wash— $2.00/car” might be scrawled on a building in chalk, but the scrawl would not necessarily be considered graffiti. The reading of something as graffiti, then, depends on what the reader counts not only as sanctioned or unsanctioned but also as worthwhile. For many people, graffiti are associated with street gangs. What makes something gang graffiti, however, is whether it is used to send messages about territory to other gangs. For example, the graffito (singular of graffiti) shown here represents gang writing: The graffito shown in the photograph was written on the wall of a “public” space, a store in a city neighborhood shopping area. The graffito serves dual purposes: First, it tells a number of rival gangs that the East Side King Crips claim this space or territory. In fact, the other gangs are negated by being “crossed out” (note the Xs drawn over the names of several gangs—QVO, BMG, SAS, POG, 21st Street, and M1F—on the left side of the wall) and are explicitly threatened 210 Graffiti One of the adolescents studied by Elizabeth Moje (2000) made the distinction between tagging and graffiti best in an essay he wrote about distinctions between tagger and gang graffiti: “Taggers write for a statement and for artwork and a sign of respect. Graffiti is to mark where you come from like gangs and what they claim as their turf.” In general, taggers and gangstas assert that tagging is “art” and is not necessarily related to gang activity, although taggers—who are usually members of tagging “crews”—are at times hired by gangs to produce gang-related graffiti. According to the youth interviewed in Moje’s (2000) study, gangstas will usually not “cross out” a tag unless it encroaches on their territory; taggers will not cross out a gang graffito, because crossing out by taggers is done as a form of evaluation of the art and taggers recognize that gang graffiti are not written as art. In addition, such cross-outs might be read as challenges to the gang. Hunt (1996) described graffiti created by the best taggers (known as piecers, from the abbreviation of masterpiece) as colorful, appearing to be three-dimensional, and highly visible or “in the heavens.” Elizabeth Moje (2000) observed several examples of young people practicing tags in notebooks that they carried in school. For example, one tag observed was the name of a tagging crew, “Midtown,” written in elaborate lettering, with a clawed fist holding a bomb around the letter “o.” As a tag, this graffito could be considered somewhat unusual in that the clawed hand holding a lit bomb seems to threaten violence, and in that the graffito seems to claim territory—aspects that tend to be associated with gang graffiti rather than with tagging crews. Although generally nonviolent in nature, tags quite commonly communicate resistance or claims for identity. In this case, the tagger seems to want to advertise the Midtown crew as people with something to say. However, it is possible that despite the bubble writing, which would seem to indicate a tag rather than a gang graffito, the writer is actually using tag writing to display a gang name. Given the context of this tag—part of a school project on tagging and gang graffiti—it is possible that the writer (an identified gang member) mixed linguistic conventions to mask his membership in an actual gang. In part because of the school setting for which it was written, then, the tag sends complicated messages about identity, membership, and voice. Although it is risky to engage in tagging practices in school, it is by no means unusual for gang-connected adolescents (and wannabes, that is, people who, for reasons of safety or popularity, want to be associated with gangs or tagging crews) to use school time to draw tags on paper. In addition to the tags that taggers might write on highly visible spaces, many serious taggers keep tagging notebooks or sketchbooks in which they practice their tags in pencil or pen before throwing them up on a wall, sign, or fence (Hunt, 1996). It was common in one middle school (Moje, 2000) to see students with oversized sheets of bond paper, calligraphy pens, and charcoal pencils used for practicing their tags. The Midtown tag, although prepared specifically for a classroom writing project, is an example of a typical notebook tag. These notebooks—and the accompanying writing/drawing equipment— served both as practice sites and as emblems of tagger and gangsta identity. Because taggers tag as a way of making themselves known, these notebooks announce the ability to tag, at least on paper. They serve, in this sense, as an advertisement. Significance of Graffiti in Literacy and Educational Research and Practice A number of studies show that young people may turn to graffiti writing, and other unsanctioned literacy and language practices, as a way of writing themselves into the world (Aguilar, 2000; Cintron, 1997; Hunt, 1996; Moje, 2000). Comments from a number of young graffiti writers speak to the importance of understanding how these unsanctioned literacy practices, although typically thought to be negative and perhaps worthless, may serve as tools for transforming thought and experience in the lives of marginalized youth. Despite a recent emphasis in educational theory on conceptualizing literacy as a tool for changing thought and experience, however, when adults typically speak of the graffiti writing, they rarely talk about such a literacy form as a tool. Instead, graffiti writing is often referred to in terms of deviance or resistance. If literacy educators want to claim that literacy is a tool for transforming thought and experience, then literacy theorists and researchers need to extend that theoretical claim to all literacy practices by asking what a literacy practice like graffiti writing does for adolescents. As educators, it is important to acknowledge graffiti as a timeless 211 Graphic Aids type), nor do they change how such text is displayed on some surface (e.g., including texts as sidebars to the main text or varying the width of margins or background color). Nonetheless, many graphic aids include alphabetic texts in the form of captions or labels. Further, graphic displays not explicitly aimed at providing information cannot be categorized as graphic aids. For example, pictures or graphic designs aimed primarily at enhancing attention or interest (e.g., a picture of someone driving on the first page of a driving manual) or included mainly for aesthetic reasons (e.g., a cover design that is engaging and pleasing to the eye) are not considered to be graphic aids. Nonetheless, the visual elements of a graphic aid itself might be analyzed in terms of their overall effect on attention or interest. Finally, graphic aids do not include icons frequently used in place of alphabetic texts. The research pertaining to graphic aids has focused on determining their role in the comprehension of texts and on how they might be designed or manipulated to increase learning and understanding. A variety of theoretical perspectives has been used to guide this research, ranging from principles of textual design associated with the field of instructional technology to cognitive theories of textual processing associated with the field of educational psychology. Beginning in the mid-1980s, interest increased in graphic aids used in conjunction with computerbased texts. Computer-based texts have changed the role and function of graphic aids, compared to the conventional printed texts with which they were previously associated. For example, in computer-based texts, graphic displays can be animated and displayed contingently, depending upon a reader’s actions during reading. Further, computer-based texts may naturally subordinate alphabetic texts to graphic information (Bolter, 1991; Lanham, 1993), thus suggesting that graphic “aids” is a misnomer. Graphic aids have received relatively little attention within instructional programs. Most attention has been focused on acquiring literal information from graphic representations (e.g., “reading” a table), although there have long been calls for a more interpretive stance on graphic aids (Summers, 1965). One instructional strategy referred to as the graphics information lesson (GIL) has been proposed as a means for encouraging such an interpretive stance while literacy practice and to ask why young people in today’s society are using graffiti, how the use of graffiti relates to the learning of academic literacies, and how graffiti writing might shape the larger life possibilities of these young people. Elizabeth Birr Moje References Aguilar, Jill A. April, 2000. “Chicano Street Signs: Graffiti as Public Literacy Practice.” Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Conference, New Orleans, LA. Camitta, Miriam. 1993. “Vernacular Writing: Varieties of Literacy among Philadelphia High School Students.” In B. V. Street, ed., Cross-Cultural Approaches to Literacy, pp. 228–246. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Cintron, Ralph. 1997. Angels’ Town: Chero Ways, Gang Life, and Rhetorics of the Everyday. Boston: Beacon Press. Conquergood, Dwight. 1990. The Heart Broken in Half (video). Chicago: Siegel Productions. Hunt, Matthew. 1996. “The Sociolinguistics of Tagging and Chicano Gang Graffiti.” Ph.D. diss., University of Southern California, Los Angeles. Moje, Elizabeth B. 2000. “To Be Part of the Story: The Literacy Practices of Gangsta Adolescents.” Teachers College Record 102:652–690. Graphic Aids Graphic aids is a term that has been used in research and instructional practice to identify various nonalphabetic representations inserted purposefully in expository texts for the sake of enhancing comprehension and learning. Specifically, the purpose of graphic aids is to supplement and extend information presented in a particular alphabetic text, thereby assisting the reader to understand and learn while reading that text independently. Graphic aids include maps, charts, tables, figures, drawings, photographs, diagrams, pictures, illustrations, and other primarily nonalphabetic representations, typically displayed near the alphabetic texts to which they are related. They are generally associated with conventional printed textbooks employed in a variety of instructional contexts, but they may also be found in other informational materials such as brochures, manuals, and directions for assembling items or for carrying out a task requiring physical manipulation. Graphic aids are not visual variations of an alphabetic text (e.g., the use of italics or boldface 212 Graphic Organizers Advance Organizer Structured Overview Graphic Organizer Pictures Photos Cartoons Maps Diagrams Venn Diagrams Flow Charts Matrices Concept Maps Story Maps Character Maps Figure 1 Graphic organizers visually represent relations among elements of a concept and, as such, present a large amount of information in few words. They can be constructed by teachers or by students and are useful before, during, and after reading. In addition, graphic organizers are useful tools for planning instruction, teaching, learning, prewriting, and assessment. Theoretical underpinnings of graphic organizers include David Ausubel’s concept of advance organizers (a short piece of text written at a higher abstract level than the information it serves to introduce), Richard Anderson’s conceptualization of schema theory (schema theory emphasizes the importance of prior knowledge in learning), and Lev Vygotsky’s notion of semiotic mediation (the idea that culturally derived signs/words can mediate learning). In the late 1960s and early 1970s, researchers at Syracuse University conducted a number of experiments on the use of vocabulary words as advance organizers. Richard Earle (1969) termed the resulting diagrams structured overviews. A structured overview was essentially a diagram composed of essential vocabulary terms arranged to convey the relationships that existed among the terms. As teachers adapted these structured overviews, they began using other enhancing content-area instruction (Reinking, 1986). David Reinking References Bolter, Jay David. 1991. Writing Space: The Computer, Hypertext, and the History of Writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lanham, Richard A. 1993. The Electronic Word: Democracy, Technology, and the Arts. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Reinking, David. 1986. “Integrating Graphic Aids into Content Area Instruction: The Graphic Information Lesson.” Journal of Reading 30:146–151. Summers, Edward G. 1965. “Utilizing Visual Aids in Reading Materials for Effective Learning.” In H. L. Herber, ed., Developing Study Skills in Secondary Schools, pp. 97–155. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Graphic Organizers Graphic organizers are visual representations of concepts or information. The term applies to a variety of forms, including cartoons, pictures, diagrams, structured overviews, Venn diagrams, semantic maps, character maps, and story maps. 213 Graphic Organizers EXPOSITORY TEXT Cause/ Effect Fishbone Map Descriptive Web NARRATIVE TEXT Sequential Story Web Comparison Matrix Timeline Story Map Venn Diagram Network Tree Series of Events Chain Cycle Figure 2 visuals, including pictures, charts, Venn diagrams, flow charts, cartoons, and semantic maps in their teaching. Many of these visual aids were unlike the structured overviews that had been developed by the Syracuse researchers and the more general term graphic organizer began to be applied to a family of visuals. Figure 1 is a graphic organizer that represents the evolution of the graphic organizer. Constructed by teachers, graphic organizers are useful in planning instruction, vocabulary exercises, and assessments. Teacher-constructed graphic organizers may be used to introduce technical vocabulary for students whose insufficient prior knowledge needs elaboration. Use of diagrams, charts, and other forms of graphic organizers can scaffold student learning by providing a skeletal structure of the information, facilitating students’ ability to organize information meaningfully. A teacher-created graphic organizer may be used to guide reading when students are asked to annotate the graphic organizer or to complete one left partially blank. More-able students can be asked to create their own graphic organizer as they read. Less-able students may need more scaffolding. When a graphic organizer provides the structure of the information in advance, students can more easily differentiate important from unimportant information and can see how important elements of a concept are related. This in turn improves students’ comprehension. Having students construct graphic organizers to represent their learning provides a vehicle for reflection after reading. Although no two graphic organizers will be identical, the learners should be able to justify the relations and hierarchies depicted in them. An effective strategy is to have pairs or small groups of students create graphic organizers and explain them to the class. For assessment purposes, Cathleen Rafferty and Linda Fleschner (1993) developed guidelines to evaluate student-constructed graphic organizers. Teachers have a wide variety of visuals to choose from when creating graphic organizers. Effective graphic organizers are created with the knowledge structure or text structure, or both, in mind. Figure 2 summarizes the relationship between the type of graphic organizer and the text structure. Victoria Gentry Ridgeway and Kathy Cochran See Also Scaffolded Literacy Instruction; Semantic Mapping; Thematic Organizers References Bromley, Karen, Linda Irwin-De Vitis, and Marcia Modlo. 1995. Graphic Organizers: Visual Strategies for Active Learning. New York: Scholastic. Earle, Richard A. 1969. “Use of the Structured Overview in Mathematics Classes.” In Harold L. Herber and Peter L. Sanders, eds., Research in Reading in the Content Areas: First Year Report, pp. 49–58. Syracuse, NY: Reading and Language Arts Center, Syracuse University. Rafferty, Cathleen D., and Linda K. Fleschner. 1993. “Concept Mapping: A Viable Alternative to Objective and Essay Exams.” Reading Research and Instruction 32 (3):25–34. 214 Group Reading Inventories Group Reading Inventories presented in the passage. Following the question, the targeted skill should be identified. For example, “What is the incubation period of the Emperor penguin’s egg?” The second type of GRI consists of several graded passages taken from a text with questions for each passage. Students’ functional reading levels, that is, independent, instructional, and frustration levels, can be determined using this type of GRI. Three assessment sessions are required to complete the graded-passages GRI. To create this type of GRI, several graded reading passages and questions are developed following the same procedures used to create a single-passage GRI. Approximately ten questions reflecting literal, inferential, and critical reading/thinking should accompany each passage. At the first assessment session, all students receive the same passage and are instructed to read it silently. Questions are distributed to students once reading is completed. Students correctly answering 70 percent or more of the questions (high group) receive a second passage that is two reading levels higher than the first passage. Students scoring below 70 percent correct (low group) receive a second passage that is two reading levels below the first passage. Once again, students read the passages silently and receive and respond to related questions. Within the higher scoring group of students, the teacher determines those who scored 70 percent or more correct and provides them with a third passage that is one reading level higher than the second passage. Students in the group that scored below 70 percent correct are given a third passage that is one reading level lower than the second passage. Within the lower scoring group, the teacher follows the same procedure using the same criteria: students scoring above 70 percent correct receive a passage one reading level higher than the second passage; students scoring below 70 percent correct receive a passage one reading level lower. The final assessment session, then, requires the teacher to administer appropriate reading passages and questions at four different reading levels (Henk and Helfeldt, 1985). Students’ reading ability levels can be determined from either the single-passage or multiple-passage GRI. Although researchers’ criteria for determining students’ independent, instructional, and frustration reading levels varies, students scoring 80 percent or more correct are gen- The group reading inventory (GRI) is a two-part, informal, teacher-made assessment patterned after the informal reading inventory. The GRI is designed to assess fourth-grade through collegeage students’ ability to use and comprehend expository text (McWilliams and Rakes, 1980). Part one of the assessment is designed to gather information about how well students can use illustrations, references, and other components of content-area texts. Part two assesses students’ abilities to respond to literal and inferential comprehension questions following silent reading. Unlike informal reading inventories that are administered to one student at a time, the GRI is designed for group administration. This assessment is used to help teachers determine whether students’ comprehension levels match the readability of the content-area text. Group reading inventories can take one of two forms—either a single passage with questions or several graded passages with related questions for each passage. The single-passage GRI format is administered in a single assessment session and provides general information about students’ reading levels and the readability of the text. However, the single-passage GRI does not provide a complete picture of students’ reading abilities. A single-passage GRI is constructed by selecting a text passage of approximately 500 words. The passage should be one that students have not read before and it should cover a complete concept. The teacher then develops the two-part GRI by writing a total of twelve to fifteen questions based on the passage and the text from which the passage was taken. The first part of the GRI requires students to use their textbook to answer the first six to eight questions. These questions focus on students’ knowledge of textbook aids like the table of contents, glossary, index, and graphic aids. A question, for example, may ask students the page number on which a particular chapter begins. The second part of the GRI targets students’ comprehension of information from the selected passage. For part two, students silently read the passage in the text. The remaining six to eight questions on the passage are distributed once all students have read the material. Responses are written without referring to the text. Questions for this part of the GRI reflect literal and inferential thinking and target main ideas, vocabulary, and important details 215 Group Reading Inventories graded reading passages for the JIRI are derived from high-interest adolescent literature. The passages are read silently, and responses to comprehension questions are written without referring to the passage. Comprehension questions are designed to measure vocabulary, details, main ideas, cause and effect, and inference (Cagney, 1983). Because the purpose of the JIRI is to match students’ reading levels with high-interest adolescent literature, teachers cannot use the assessment results to make decisions about whether students will be able to read contentarea texts successfully. Pamela J. Dunston and M. Christina Pennington erally considered to be reading at the independent level (level at which readers can read comfortably with little or no assistance). Students scoring 65–80 percent correct are reading within their instructional level (level at which readers can read with assistance from a teacher or knowledgeable other), and students scoring below 65 percent correct are reading at the frustration level (level at which readers experience stress and discomfort even when assisted). Students falling within the frustration level will need extensive teacher support and supplemental instruction in order to be successful. Analysis of students’ responses to comprehension questions associated with either type of GRI provides the teacher with information concerning specific comprehension weaknesses. Based on the types of questions most frequently missed by students, the teacher can plan instruction to address students’ needs. A commercially produced GRI designed to measure students’ reading levels with narrative text is the Johnston Informal Reading Inventory (JIRI). The JIRI is a three-part assessment that provides information about students’ knowledge of antonyms, synonyms, and reading levels. The See Also Informal Reading Inventory References Cagney, Margaret. 1983. “Johnston Informal Reading Inventory.” Journal of Reading 26:530–532. Henk, William A., and Joan P. Helfeldt. 1985. “The Group Reading Inventory in the Social Studies Classroom.” Social Education 49:224–227. McWilliams, Lana, and Thomas A. Rakes. 1980. “Assessing Reading Skills in Science.” Science and Children 18:21–22. 216 H The Handbook of Reading Research For example, work in neurobiology is included in Volume III, despite the fact that a relatively small volume of research on it has occurred. The topics across all volumes reflect a broadening of the scope of problems being studied by reading researchers. The reviews in The HRR are interpretive and evidence-driven. Experts in the field are selected to produce each review. The reviews combine historical analyses of research, syntheses of current research, and conclusions about future directions for research. Authors put their reviews in context, relating them to earlier reviews and making them relevant for future—and past— research. Volume III includes two new types of reviews, methodological and geographic. Ten reviews of research methodologies are included to highlight the increasing importance of methodology in the conduct and interpretation of reading research. Because of the increasing emphasis on international issues in reading research, five reviews relating to specific geographic regions were also included. The ten essays on methodology have been reprinted in a separate volume (Kamil et al., 2002). All authors and editors of Volume III have contributed their honoraria and royalties to a research fund administered by the National Reading Conference to promote research in international literacy issues. The HRR is a primary source of information about the current state of knowledge about reading research. It provides background on almost all of the currently important topics in reading and serves as an entry point for more detailed research. Michael L. Kamil The Handbook of Reading Research (HRR) is a multivolume collection of reviews of reading research compiled by experts in each of the represented specialties. There have been three volumes published, in 1984, 1991, and 2000, respectively. A fourth volume is in production. The three published volumes contain a total of 107 articles authored or coauthored by 189 researchers, for nearly 3,000 pages in all. Although each volume is only slightly different in length, over the course of their publication the number of articles almost doubled from Volume 1 to Volume III. The number of authors and coauthors has more than doubled, from forty in Volume I to eighty-seven in Volume III. These increases seem to be representative of the increasing complexity and collaborative style of reading research. The amount of literature on reading research is enormous, consisting of more than 100,000 published journal articles. To make sense of this wealth of information, systematic reviews of this literature are required. Each of the three handbook volumes represents an attempt to characterize the state of knowledge in reading research at a given point in time, realizing that not all topics can be reviewed in each volume. Topics were chosen for each volume because they were representative of published literature to date. For Volumes II and III, the topics were chosen to highlight those areas of reading research that had accumulated substantial new material since the prior review. Topics range from early and emergent literacy to adult and workplace literacy. They include research in historical issues, basic processes, applications, and policy. Volume IV will follow a similar plan. Emerging trends are also highlighted. References Barr, Rebecca, Michael Kamil, Peter Mosenthal, and P. David Pearson, eds. 1991. Handbook of 217 The Head Start Program Reading Research, Volume II. New York: Longman. Reprinted in 1996, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Kamil, M., P. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, and R. Barr, eds. 2000. Handbook of Reading Research, Volume III. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ———. 2002. Methods of Literacy Research: The Methodology Chapters from the Handbook of Reading Research Volume III. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Mosenthal, P., P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, and M. Kamil, eds. 2002. Methods of Literacy Research: The Methodology Chapters from the Handbook of Reading Research Volume IV. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pearson, P. D., R. Barr, M. Kamil, and P. Mosenthal, eds. 1984. Handbook of Reading Research. New York: Longman. Reprinted in 1984, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. The Head Start Program Head Start is a comprehensive services program for low-income children from birth to age five and their families. Its goal is to foster the healthy development of the whole child, which includes social-emotional, cognitive, language, and physical development. To achieve these goals, program services are individualized and include education; medical, dental, and mental health; and nutrition. A basic element of program philosophy is the idea that parents are the primary educators, nurturers, and advocates of their children. Consequently, Head Start has a strong commitment to partnering with parents to enhance the child’s development. Head Start was initiated in 1965 as a part of the federal government’s War on Poverty. It started as a summer program serving 561,000 children and costing $96.4 million and grew slowly until the 1990s. In that decade, the program expanded in cost from $1.5 billion to nearly $5 billion. In fiscal year 2000, it served 857,664 children and was funded at $5,266,211,000. Since its inception, Head Start has touched the lives of 19,397,000 children (Head Start Bureau, 2001). To guide the implementation of Head Start services, this nationally operated program has instituted the Head Start Program Performance Standards. The objective of the education activities is to provide each child with a safe, nurturing, stimulating, enjoyable, and secure environment to help that child gain the skills and confidence necessary to succeed in Head Start, in school, and Children participating in a Head Start program (Laura Dwight) in life (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Administration on Children, Youth and Families, 1996). The Head Start curriculum definition states that teachers must set goals for children’s development and learning, outline the experiences children need to accomplish these goals, specify the behaviors of adults that will allow these goals to be met, and supply the materials and equipment that support the goals. In the areas of cognitive and language development, these standards require a program that shows a balance between child-initiated and teacher-directed activities, uses a variety of strategies and approaches, encourages children’s self-expression and interaction with one another and with adults, and supports literacy and numeracy activities. Head Start staff must assess the child at the beginning of the year, at some time during the year, and at the end of the year and must use these data to plan the program, individualize the program for the child, and judge the achieved outcomes. Since 1965, when Head Start began, several national studies have assessed its success. A 218 Heritage-Language Development Start Program Quality and Outcomes. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Administration on Children, Youth and Families. meta-analysis of these studies (Hubbell, 1983) reported that Head Start children showed shortterm gains on intelligence measures and performed better than non–Head Start children from similar socioeconomic groups. Some studies showed that these gains were maintained into elementary school, but some did not. In general, Head Start graduates performed better on teacher ratings, retention in grade, and assignment to special education. The current results of an ongoing study, the Family and Child Experiences Survey (FACES) (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Administration on Children, Youth and Families, 2000) define more clearly the cognitive skills of Head Start children as they make their transition to kindergarten. For example, Head Start children could tell their name and age, identify ten basic colors by name, count four objects, show the front cover of a storybook and open it to start reading, and answer simple factual questions about a story read to them. Head Start children showed significant growth in their vocabularies over the Head Start year. In the year before starting kindergarten, about 24 percent scored close to or above the national mean in the fall, whereas 34 percent did so in the spring, showing a 40percent increase in those scoring at or above the mean. Nevertheless, a typical Head Start child, in the spring before kindergarten, could not tell his or her address, identify most letters of the alphabet, or understand that a reader goes from left to right and top to bottom when reading English text. Finally, Head Start graduates’ improvement in kindergarten exceeded the growth of typical kindergartners, suggesting that the program does prepare children in many ways to succeed in school. Lorelei R. Brush Heritage-Language Development Heritage languages are the home or ancestral languages of certain immigrant or indigenous groups of people. A “heritage language” may also be called a “first language,” “native language,” or “mother tongue” when the language is indeed the person’s first-learned language. A “heritage language” may also be referred to as a “minority language,” “ethnic language,” or “ancestral language” when the target language is associated with a person’s heritage but is not necessarily the individual’s first-learned language or the language used in the home. Thus, target language groups in heritage-language development studies vary considerably in terms of their historical and cultural backgrounds and their levels of heritage-language proficiency (Cummins, 1983). Heritage-language development studies have typically been conducted within the contexts of minority education, bilingualism (see Bilingualism), and language maintenance, language shift, or language revitalization. Heritage-Language Development: Academic and Personal Benefits A growing number of minority-education studies have argued for the importance and merits of active school involvement in the development of heritage languages. These arguments are based on empirical evidence that language-minority students who preserve their own language and culture as well as their distinct ethnic identity have strong pride in their heritage, succeed in mainstream school and society, and have satisfying communication with their family members. For example, extensive empirical research on heritage-language development conducted in Canada suggests that the development of heritage-language children’s first language enhances their learning of the dominant nonheritage language and their overall academic achievement (Cummins, 1991). According to Cummins’s “interdependence hypothesis,” one’s academic literacy skills in L1 (first language) and L2 (second language) are interrelated because they derive from the same underlying cognitive proficiency, See Also The Even Start Family Literacy Program References Head Start Bureau. 2001. 2001 Head Start Fact Sheet. Washington, DC: Head Start Bureau. Hubbell, Ruth. 1983. A Review of Head Start Research Since 1970. Washington, DC: CSR. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Administration on Children, Youth and Families. 1996. “Head Start Program Performance Standards.” 45 CFR 1304, Federal Register 61, no. 215 (November 5):57186–57227. ———. 2000. FACES Findings: New Research on Head 219 Heritage-Language Development Class of Vietnamese children learning to read Vietnamese (Elizabeth Crews) guage policy: a subtractive policy of language assimilatio