ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW

Transcription

ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW
IMAGINATION, COGNITION AND PERSONALITY, Vol. 30(1) 5-56, 2010-2011
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND
CONCEPTUAL REVIEW
JOHN C. WILLIAMS
VA Healthcare System, Long Beach
STEVEN JAY LYNN
State University of New York at Binghamton
ABSTRACT
Recognition of the value of acceptance of the self, others, and the flux
of human experience, has philosophical and religious roots that date back
thousands of years. The past two decades have witnessed a swell of interest in
acceptance, as evidenced by an increase in acceptance-based therapeutic
interventions, and a growing appreciation of the paradoxical nature of
acceptance and personal change and the role of experiential avoidance
in psychopathology. In this article we review historical and contemporary
descriptions and definitions of acceptance, measures of acceptance, the
relation between acceptance and change, and the role of acceptance in
psychopathology and psychotherapy. Our central objective is to delineate
a rich conceptual scheme that encompasses the diverse ways in which
acceptance has been explicated in classical and contemporary writings,
and to highlight the need for further validation of this useful and
popular construct.
The notion that acceptance—of oneself, other people, circumstances, and the
world at large—can be a force for personal change has deep roots in Eastern
and Western culture. The benefits of acceptance are described in religious
texts (e.g., Buddhist Sutras, Bhagavad Gita, New Testament, Tao Te Ching, Yoga
5
Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc.
doi: 10.2190/IC.30.1.c
http://baywood.com
6 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Aphorisms), philosophical tracts (e.g., Aurelius, Epictetus, Keirkegaard,
Krishnamurti, Lao Tzu, Watts), and literary works (e.g., Austen, Nabokov,
Pirsig, Shakespeare, Steinbeck, Tolstoy) across diverse eras and cultures.
In the psychological literature, acceptance has intrigued scholars for the
better part of a century. Although a diverse group of theorists have contributed
to understanding the salutary effects of acceptance (e.g., Ellis, Freud, Horney,
James, Skinner), typically humanistic and existential theorists, particularly
Carl Rogers, are credited with elaborating acceptance as a mechanism of selfactualization and therapeutic change. More specifically, early quasi-experimental
work (e.g., Hall, 1918; Young, 1924), theory development (Rogers, 1944b;
Wilson, 1942), and measurement (e.g., Berger, 1952; Fey, 1954; Phillips, 1951;
Sheerer, 1949; Stock, 1949) led to the validation of two specific constructs:
self-acceptance and acceptance of others, which centered on developing an
awareness of both the positive and negative aspects of the self and others
while maintaining an attitude of positive regard. Whereas these two constructs
have persisted in the literature, over the past two decades a new wave of interest
in acceptance has emerged, most notably in what has been described as the
“third wave” of behavioral and cognitive behavioral approaches and selfregulation techniques (e.g., Hayes, 2004b; Hayes, Jacobson, Follette, &
Dougher, 1994). Recent empirical research generally has focused on the efficacy
of acceptance-based psychotherapeutic treatments. Less attention has been paid
to the validity of the construct itself, even as contemporary theorists appear to
have broadened the scope of the construct to embrace more classical notions of
acceptance and mindfulness, another construct with deep roots in philosophy
and religion.
During the initial resurgence of interest in acceptance, Wulfert (1994) observed
that there was no universal definition of acceptance. A variety of definitions can
add to the richness of meaning and nuance of acceptance, but if a construct
becomes overbroad, multiply defined, with overlapping components, it can lead to
poor specificity—that is, it can come to mean everything and nothing. Although
a number of researchers have attempted to define, operationalize, and measure
acceptance, there has been no reasonably comprehensive and systematic framework for understanding the broader, perhaps multifaceted, construct of acceptance
that is presented in the extant literature.
Our purpose is to review research and theory pertaining to the construct of
acceptance in order to provide, if not an empirically-based organizational framework—as such an effort would be premature—at least a digest of issues relevant
to acceptance construct validity (as even this has not yet been attempted),
and to suggest directions for further research. We will proceed by first addressing
definitional issues, the variable usage of the term acceptance in psychology, and
basic denotations. We then turn to the historical aspects of acceptance, its validation in the 20th century, and finally to its current status, before we conclude
with recommendations for future research.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
7
DEFINING ACCEPTANCE
The term acceptance has a number of definitions. The etymological root of
accept is the Latin acceptare, the Old French accepter, and finally the Middle
English accept used by Chaucer and Wyclif in the 14th century (Onions,
1966). To accept is “to receive willingly or with approval,” to take toward
(i.e., ac + capere) rather than cast away (p. 7). Four modern definitions (Brown,
1993; Merriam-Webster, 1993; Weiner & Simpson, 1991) bear directly on the
present discussion:
1. taking or receiving something, as a pleasure, a satisfaction of a claim, or a
duty;
2. favorable reception, regard, or approval;
3. assenting to or believing; and
4. acceptableness.
These four definitions are found in a number of branches of psychology.
For example, one branch of social psychology deals with individuals’ acceptance
of group norms in the form of conformity, compliance, identification, and internalization (e.g., Brown, 1936; Cialdini & Goldstein; 2004; Hollander, 1960; Kelman,
1958; Menzel, 1957; Sherif, 1936). In another branch, social acceptance refers
to the effects of being or not being accepted by others, such as parental acceptance
of children (e.g., Burchinal et al., 1957), peers’ acceptance of adolescents (e.g.,
Kuhlen & Lee, 1943), or society’s acceptance of the disabled (e.g., Ladieu et al.,
1948), or mentally ill (e.g., Roman & Floyd, 1981).
In clinical psychological science, the treatment compliance literature employs
the term acceptance to describe the extent to which a mental health patient
is willing to comply with medical, psychiatric, or psychological interventions
(e.g., Baile & Engel, 1978; Demyttenaere, 1997; Hunter, 1942; Imber et al., 1956;
Scheel et al., 2004). Other uses of the term vary broadly, from specific disciplines,
such as design acceptance (e.g., Madni, 1988), job acceptance (Marozas &
May, 1980), and technology acceptance (e.g., Colvin & Goh, 2005), to broader
scientific issues relevant to all disciplines such as the “acceptance” of theories
(e.g., Chow, 1992), models (e.g., Lynn & Fite, 1998), techniques (e.g., Hudson
et al., 1998), and measures (e.g., Dahlstrom, 1992). Whereas all of these forms of
acceptance conform in one way or another to the definitions given above, they
may be distinct research areas or broad principles with little or no bearing on
the type of acceptance under consideration here. As such, they may be considered
beyond the boundaries of the construct discussed in this review.
Experiential Acceptance
A title/keyword search of “acceptance” in PsychINFO returns over 28,000
citations, a substantial proportion of which are not relevant to the present discussion. This may stem from the fact that the form of acceptance under consideration
8 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
lacks a modifying adjective (e.g., self-acceptance, social acceptance, technology
acceptance) to distinguish it from other more general uses. For the purposes of this
discussion, we prefer the term experiential acceptance, recently used by BlockLerner and others (Block-Lerner, Salters-Pedneault, & Tull, 2005; Cardaciotto,
2005; Roemer & Orsillo, 2007). In addition to distinguishing it from other forms
of acceptance, this term also captures an essential aspect of the construct that is
not necessarily shared by other forms of acceptance; namely, that the primary
object of acceptance is experience itself (rather than any particular stimulus).
This adjective is also used in the term experiential avoidance (Chapman, Gratz,
& Brown, 2006; Hayes, Strosahl, Wilson, Bisset, Pistorello, Toarmino, et al.,
2004a; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996; Tull & Roemer, 2003),
which is a key component of contemporary theoretical and empirical contributions
to the construct of acceptance. At the outset, it bears emphasis that acceptance of
a stimulus is not the same as condoning it or deeming it in any way acceptable: to
experientially accept a stimulus means to acknowledge that a stimulus is present,
rather than apply any evaluative designation.
ANCIENT RELIGIOUS AND PHILOSOPHICAL
PRECURSORS
A variety of ancient philosophical and religious sources consider acceptance
to be integral to personal and social development. Spanning millennia as well as
continents, they also emphasize different aspects of acceptance, or define it in
different ways. A brief discussion of these aspects of acceptance may help to
contextualize the current conceptualizations, as these basic principles appear
with some regularity in modern and contemporary treatments of psychological
acceptance. The main concepts that comprise acceptance as a philosophical and
religious principle can be described in terms of nonattachment, nonavoidance,
nonjudgment, tolerance, and willingness.
Nonattachment
Twenty-five hundred years ago, the Buddha attributed human suffering to
the tendency to cling to thoughts, people, sensory objects, and habitual ways
of acting and feeling in the world. The Buddha’s observations echoed earlier
Hindu, Vedic, and Yoga philosophies, which enjoined practitioners to withdraw
the mind from evanescent sense objects in order to refocus it on the one true
object, the divine. Often embodied in formal meditation practices, nonattachment
was taught as a path to mental discipline, internal quiescence, and ultimately to
enlightenment. In principle, because all possessions and people are created and
destroyed, are born and die, any attempt to hold onto things or people is pointless
and leads inevitably to suffering. To practice nonattachment is to accept that
the objects of experience wax and wane, and that to allow them to come and go
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
9
naturally is preferable to any attempt to control or retain them. In Taoist thought,
the sage accepts the world, and the ebb and flow of things, without holding on.
Interestingly, Seng-Tsan, The Third Zen Patriarchs observed that this includes
everything, and that “even to be attached to the idea of enlightenment is to go
astray” (Kornfield, 1993, p. 149).
Nonavoidance
Like nonattachment, nonavoidance is a valued activity in Eastern and Western
contemplative traditions. Marcus Aurelius (trans. 1965) wrote that passing one’s
days in pursuit or avoidance leads to a disturbed mind and an unhappy existence.
Nonavoidance and nonattachment can be seen as complementary, depending on
whether or not one is fleeing toward or from a given stimulus. In Eastern classical
literature, in fact, attachment tends to be the central construct, and avoidance is
regarded as a particular form of attachment. An avoidance of insects, for example,
could reflect an attachment to a real or imagined insect-free environment. Like
nonattachment, nonavoidance can be defined as refraining from a pointless and
maladaptive behavior, running away when no physical threat is present. This,
combined with the maladaptive “running toward” of attachment, results in a
definition of acceptance as the capacity to remain available to present experience,
without attempting to terminate the painful or prolong the pleasant. Though
both nonattachment and nonavoidance may require effort, both can be defined
therefore as a nondoing. As Lao Tzu put it, “The Master does his job and then
stops” (trans. 1988, p. 30).
Nonjudgment
Nonjudgment is a theme in both the Eastern and Western canon. Matthew (7:1)
wrote “judge not that ye may not be judged,” Hamlet described the kingdom of
Denmark as a prison, reasoning “nothing is either good or bad, but thinking
makes it so” (William Shakespeare, 1989, p. 250), and Lao Tzu succinctly
captured the spirit of nonjudgment when he observed that the “mark of a moderate
man is freedom from his own ideas” (trans. 1988, p.59). Nonjudgment is central
to modern conceptualizations of acceptance: it is a conscious abstention from
the categorization of experience as good or bad, or right or wrong. Aurelius
advised to refrain from internally embellishing our sense impressions with
internal commentary. Nonjudgment involves describing stimuli rather than adding
evaluations of them. Nonjudgment is a central component of mindfulness meditation, and can be found in the Zen concept of fushizen-fushiaku, literally “not
thinking good, not thinking bad,” which represents a transcendence of distinguishing phenomena as good and bad (Fischer-Schreiber, Ehrhard & Diener,
1991, p. 74). As Seng-Tsan, the Third Zen Patriarch observed, “the burdensome
practice of judging brings annoyance and weariness” (Kornfield, 1993, p. 150).
10 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Tolerance
Acceptance requires that a person tolerate experiences as they are given;
otherwise one simply pursues pleasure, flees pain, and becomes more caught up
in judging experiences than in having them. The Stoics (e.g., Marcus Aurelius,
trans. 1965; Epictetus, trans. 1950; Seneca, trans. 1920) valued the ability
to tolerate or willingly withstand experiences because it increases self-control,
detachment from emotion, and indifference to pleasure and pain. Epictetus suggested selecting the worst seat at a social gathering, such as one without shade,
in order to develop an indifference to the sun and heat. Similarly, an avoidance
of snakes might diminish if one were able to tolerate progressive contact, similar
to modern behavioral exposure methods. Because acceptance involves making
contact with reality just as it is in the moment, it is necessary, lest one lapse
into avoidance, to be able to remain present and aware even when the stimuli
available are less than desirable Accordingly, tolerance can be considered an
ability to remain present and experience whatever is occurring in the moment.
Note, however, that this ability is not equivalent to resignation or helplessness in
the face of the aversive. It is a choice, which does not preclude concurrent efforts
to change what is occurring. Tolerance can be built up or acquired through willing
exposure to a wide variety of experiences, or to specific troublesome stimuli. In
this sense, tolerance is something that can be practiced as well as acquired.
Willingness
If one wishes to escape from one experience or lose oneself in another, this is
often possible. Acceptance, therefore, is a choice. Epictetus (1950) wrote that
when one is invited to entertainment, one should take what one finds. That is,
one should choose to participate in a event, even if it is not exactly to one’s taste.
For the Stoics, in fact, exercise of the will was paramount. Acceptance involves
a free choice. That said, willingness may well involve surrender or compliance,
so long as it is not coerced. Marcus Aurelius (1965) wrote, for example, that our
freedom is never compromised when we follow the direction of someone who
puts us right. The benefits that can accrue from allowing another person to
guide one’s behavior are integral to a host of spiritual traditions. In Eastern
meditative practices, for example, one accepts the instructions of a master and
agrees neither to add nor subtract from his or her instructions. Similarly, Blaise
Pascal (trans. 1966) accepts the counsel of his spiritual director because he
considers the directives to be essential to his spiritual development. Whether
directed by another, or simply determined by what is present, acceptance includes
the willingness to have an experience.
Limitations
It is difficult to say which of these ancient concepts should be considered central
components of contemporary acceptance and which may be better accounted for
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
11
by other constructs or are separate constructs in their own right. Additionally,
ancient acceptance includes elements that may be considered more radical, paradoxical, and profound than contemporary psychological science has conceptualized. For example, Zen philosophy describes the world as perfect as it is, without
need of change, and practitioners may expect to have this realization as they
progress. Moreover, continuous, unbroken states of acceptance are described in
the classical literature of the East and are associated with states of enlightenment.
Modern psychological literature, in contrast, usually describes the effects of
acceptance in terms of behavioral adaptivity, self-actualization, or well-being.
In the classical Western literature, acceptance can be seen in the extreme nonattachment of religious celibacy and the renunciation of possessions, and most
profoundly in the lives of saints and martyrs who have accepted, even welcomed,
gruesome experiences, such as torture and violent death. The modern psychological literature has not dealt with such extreme forms of acceptance, although
there may be some overlap in the form of radical acceptance (e.g., Brach,
2003; Linehan, 1994), which is sometimes used to describe a Zen-like state of
acceptance, usually periodic, consisting of a thorough willingness to experience
whatever occurs in the moment.
Ancient forms of acceptance also include elements of paradox (e.g., the Taoist
notion that the greatest form of help is to give up helping, and that as the sage
bends he is straightened) which may or may not be compatible with contemporary
conceptualizations of acceptance. Pantanjali, the 2nd century author of the Yoga
Sutras, the original manual of yoga, wrote that “yoga is the control of thought
waves in the mind” (Prabhavananda & Isherwood, 1981, p. 15), and that one
goal of yoga practice over an extended period of time is the cessation of thought
altogether. At first glance, these statements might seem at odds with modern
principles of acceptance, given that thought control is sometimes maladaptive
(e.g., thought suppression and rebound effects; Wegner, Schneider, Carter, &
White, 1987), and thoughts are generally considered to be mental events to
be accepted rather than eliminated. In the case of yoga philosophy, however,
thoughts are controlled by allowing them to come and go without absorption:
they are released the moment they arise, usually by redirecting the attention
to a predetermined object, such as the breath. In fact, the ability to accept and
experience thought content without reservation may be considered a pre-requisite
for this practice. These points may be as yet more esoteric than discussion in
terms of the empirical findings may allow. Nevertheless, we are left with a rather
interesting compound paradox: that we are to accept internal events in order to
be rid of them, and we control them by letting go.
In conclusion, acceptance to the ancients was a notion integrated into the
fabric of religion, philosophy, folk tradition, and daily life. Although the benefits
of acceptance were empirical, in the sense that they were wrought from collective
experience, they were not, of course, subjected to a modern scientific method.
Moreover, coming from diverse cultural and linguistic traditions, the various
12 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
historical contributions are difficult to assemble into a single ancient construct.
That said, researchers and theorists in psychology have defined and operationalized acceptance in part by drawing on these ancient conceptualizations. Therefore, they are useful to examine as we seek to describe acceptance as it is
conceptualized today.
ACCEPTANCE IN THE 20TH CENTURY
Turning now to a brief history of acceptance in 20th century psychology,
we leave a gap of nearly 2,000 years. One reason is that acceptance theorists tend
to cite sources from the classical rather than medieval or modern philosophy.
Additionally, a review of acceptance across all of Eastern and Western philosophy
is beyond the scope of this article. To our knowledge, such a review has not been
attempted, even though a number of authors do discuss aspects of the philosophical history of acceptance in their treatments of the construct (in particular,
see Dryden & Still, 2006; Ellis, 2000).
In a similar vein, acceptance can be found across a spectrum of psychological
theories. Moreover, psychological treatments almost always rely on the willingness of the patient to participate in the therapy, and, perhaps to a lesser extent,
on the patient’s awareness and acknowledgment of the problem under treatment.
Our goal, therefore, is not to catalog the construct of acceptance in every corner
of psychology. Rather, we trace the development of the construct in the psychological literature, noting in particular some of the conceptualizations that have
taken hold over the 20th century, with special attention to the discipline of clinical
psychology and experiential acceptance.
1910s
Although acceptance was not his focus, Freud (1910/1965) could have been
writing about nonattachment when he wrote that “every single hysteric and
neurotic . . . not only do they remember painful experiences of the remote past, but
they still cling to them emotionally; they cannot get free of the past and for its
sake they neglect what is real and immediate” (p. 17). Freud (e.g., 1913/1957a,
1914/1957b) also contributed to the notion that acceptance of others was related
to self-acceptance, and that avoidance can be maladaptive.
In a similar vein, Hall (1918) indicated that nonavoidance of aversive stimuli
had potential benefits; for example, confronting rather than avoiding the stimuli
of death during wartime aided a soldier or citizen in accepting the possibility of
his or her own death. As a result, he or she became “very familiar with the thought
that he may be the next victim and so accepts the fact with growing equanimity”
(p. 373). He further suggested that acceptance of death lifted the spirits and
improved performance in life-threatening situations. Early in the century, then,
there was an indication that acceptance in these forms (nonattachment and
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
13
nonavoidance) was potentially beneficial and that the opposite could lead to
neurotic symptoms and distress.
1920s and 1930s
In the 1920s and 1930s, the term acceptance sometimes was used to describe a
way of behaving or relating to stimuli that emphasized openness toward reality
rather than defensiveness, negation, or denial (Ferenczi, 1926; Penrose, 1927).
Whereas a lack of acceptance was associated with intrapsychic conflict and
pathology, acceptance of oneself and one’s situation contributed to positive
outcomes in mental health, such as adjustment after a divorce (Waller, 1930),
treatment for alcoholism (Grant, 1929), or personality disturbance (Tidd, 1937).
As such, self-acceptance was discussed as a therapeutic goal in the context of
psychodynamic psychotherapy (e.g., Rank, 1945; Taft, 1933). Even in these early
years, some noted the potential difficulties associated with assessing acceptance
in individuals. Peck (1929), for example, raised the concern that a patient could
appear more defensive while actually growing in acceptance, which can be
contrasted with later investigations of the role of social desirability in the measurement of acceptance (Crowne & Stephens, 1961); in particular, that high selfreported self-acceptance in fact may reflect defensiveness or lack of insight
(Zuckerman & Monashkin, 1957). Early acceptance theorists also began to posit
a positive correlation between self-acceptance and acceptance of others (e.g.,
Adler, 1926; Fromm, 1939; Horney, 1937).
1940s
In the 1940s, acceptance figured in the study and treatment of military personnel
during and after World War II. Wilson (1942), for example, wrote that admitting
and accepting the experience of fear associated with air raids was a protective
factor against stress disorders among civilian populations. Rogers (1944a) and
his colleagues (Rogers & Wallen, 1946) noted a host of adjustment problems
faced by returning soldiers, including hostility, vocational uncertainty, marital
and family problems, and disabilities due to injuries. Among the major treatment recommendations was to facilitate the patient’s acceptance of the daunting
feelings engendered by the war and also by the transition back to civilian life.
Beyond military applications, Rogers (1940, 1943, 1944b, 1947) and others
(e.g., Snyder, 1947) began to place the development of self-acceptance in a
position of primary importance in the therapeutic process. Rogers wrote that a
client should “admit his real self with its childish patterns, its aggressive feelings,
and its ambivalences, as well as its mature impulses, and rationalized exterior”
(Rogers, 1940, p. 162). For Rogers, self-acceptance was “the foundation of
insight” (Rogers, 1940, p. 163). Moreover, the “self” in self-acceptance included
the acknowledgment and experiencing of the entirety of internal experience, not
simply an esteemed self-concept. Beyond the self, acceptance extended to external
14 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
stimuli, such as other people (i.e., acceptance of others), which he described
as a subjective process not dependent on the pleasantness or unpleasantness of
the environment, but rather on one’s attitude toward it. Like others before him,
Rogers also hypothesized that acceptance of self and of others should be correlated positively; that is, a person with high self-acceptance should be more
likely to be accepting of others (Rogers, 1951). Finally, Rogers theorized that
the development of self-acceptance and acceptance of others leads to a more
realistic, and perhaps paradoxically more positive, view of the self and others.
That is, although the term self-acceptance is sometimes used synonymously
with self-esteem or positive self-regard, Rogers emphasized frank knowledgment
of the self as it is.
1950s
In the 1950s, the self-acceptance and acceptance of others constructs were
subject to further theory development, empirical investigation, and scale development. A host of theorists were cited during this period, including Adler, Ellis,
Horney, Fromm, Perls, Rogers, and Sullivan (e.g., Berger, 1952; Fey, 1954; see
also Dryden & Still, 2006). Several lines of research were initiated, driven by
hypotheses about: a) positive correlations between self-acceptance, acceptance of
others, and acceptance by others; b) negative correlations between self-acceptance
and psychopathology; and c) the relation between acceptance and other constructs.
These investigations used a variety of self-report and informant questionnaires
and checklists, often developed for the purpose of testing specific hypotheses.
Several of these scales became standard measures of self-acceptance and
acceptance of others, such as the scales constructed by Berger (1952), Fey
(1954), and Phillips (1951).
The findings from empirical investigations supported predictions about acceptance, with a few notable exceptions. First, self-acceptance and acceptance of
others were found to be positively correlated (e.g., Fey, 1954; Kennedy, 1958;
Omwake, 1954; Sheerer, 1949; Trent, 1957; Williams, 1962), with correlations
in the range of .30–.60 across most studies. In contrast, however, acceptance by
others (that is, ratings of a participant by others, as opposed to ratings of perceived acceptance by the participant) was not found to be correlated with either
self-acceptance or acceptance of others (Omwake, 1954; Williams, 1962).
Second, group comparisons found high self-accepters to be significantly lower
on measures of psychopathology (Berger, 1955; Bills, 1953; Zuckerman &
Monashkin, 1957; Zuckerman & Oltean, 1959). Third, a number expected associations with other constructs emerged. For example, self-acceptance was determined to be associated with childhood adjustment (Taylor & Combs, 1952),
marital happiness (Eastman, 1958), and frustration tolerance (Doleys &
Kregarman, 1959). Some unexpected associations were found, such as a finding
that self-accepting underachievers grossly overestimated their grades (Mitchell,
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
15
1959); and some expected associations were not found, such as therapists higher
in self-acceptance and acceptance of others were not rated as better therapists
by supervisors (Streitfeld, 1959).
1960s
In the 1960s, investigators continued to examine the relationships between
self-acceptance, acceptance of others, psychopathology, and other constructs.
Empirical investigations continued to find that self-acceptance and acceptance
of others were correlated, and that neither was correlated with acceptance by
others (Rubin, 1967; Suinn, 1961; Suinn & Hill, 1964; Williams, 1962). Some of
these studies broadened the validity of this relationship beyond self-report and
laboratory studies to experimental interactions with specific groups, such as the
mentally ill (Holzberg, Gewirtz, & Ebner, 1964), to specific others, such as a
child’s parents (Medinnus & Curtis, 1963), and to naturalistic studies on college
campuses (Graham & Barr, 1967).
A number of predicted associations with additional constructs also were
examined. Several researchers found that acceptance and anxiety were negatively related (Ohnmacht & Muro, 1967; Pilisuk, 1963). Interestingly, Suinn
and Hill (1964) also found that that anxiety disrupted the relationship between
self-acceptance and acceptance of others: increased anxiety was associated with
greater decreases in self-acceptance than in acceptance of others. Berger (1961,
1963) found that willingness to accept limitations was associated with college
achievement, but the relationship seemed to hold for liberal arts students but not
engineering students. Other investigations failed to find some expected relationships, and in the process the nomological network (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955)
of acceptance became more specific. Among these relationships, self-acceptance:
a) did not predict performance under stress (Goldfarb, 1961); b) was not affected
by experimentally induced success and failure experiences (Solway & Fehr,
1969); and c) was not related to hypnotizibility (Vingoe, 1967). Also during
this period, a number of investigators began to question the validity of the selfacceptance construct and the instruments used to measure it.
1970s
In the 1970s, the positive correlation between self-acceptance and acceptance
of others continued to garner empirical support (e.g., Jucha, Rendecka, & Zuraw,
1979; Kawagishi, 1972). In addition, low self-acceptance continued to be associated with psychopathology (e.g., Guidano, Liotti, & Pancheri, 1971) and was
also found to be associated with an external rather than internal locus of control
(Chandler, 1976; Lombardo & Berzonsky, 1975). Notably, Shepard (1979) contributed a substantial multitrait-multimethod construct validation study examining
self-acceptance, acceptance of others, and self-description. He found that selfacceptance had moderate construct validity, was correlated as expected with
16 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
acceptance of others, had limited discriminant validity with respect to selfdescription, and was a sensible model for “the evaluative component of the
self-concept construct” (p. 139). Also of note, researchers began to describe
specific stimulus domains of acceptance beyond the general categories of self
and other. One prominent example was acceptance of physical disability. First
measured by Linkowski (1971), acceptance of disability subsequently became a
domain of study in the physical disability literature (e.g., Boone, Roessler, &
Cooper, 1978; Evers, Kraaimaat, van Lankveld, Jongen, Jacobs, & Bijlsma,
2001; Groomes & Leahy, 2002; Kravetz, Faust, & David, 2000; Osuji, 1985;
Starr & Heiserman, 1977).
1980s
By the 1980s, acceptance, particularly self-acceptance, had been established
as a useful variable in mental health theory and research, and newer lines of
research began to focus on applied issues, such as self-acceptance techniques
in various mental health disciplines (Baisden, Lindstrom, & Hector, 1982;
Brandel, 1982; Kornblum & Anderson, 1982; Rudnick, 1982), and with specific
populations (Kus, 1988; Leavy & Adams, 1986; Tenzer, 1989). Construct validation also continued apace, with further examination of self-acceptance and other
constructs (e.g., Epstein & Feist, 1988; Long, 1986). The self-other correlation
also continued to be examined, with more specific hypotheses, such as the finding that self-accepting men were more accepting of women in less traditional
professional roles (Grube, Kleinhesselink, & Kearney, 1982). In a somewhat
different vein, novel acceptance treatments began to emerge, such as Morita
therapy, distinguished by less emphasis on accepting “the self” and more on
accepting specific internal experiences (e.g., anxiety) as “natural objects,” for the
purpose of increasing adaptive behavior (Ishiyama, 1983, p. 172; Ishiyama, 1987).
1990s
The 1990s were notable for the concurrence of established lines of acceptance
research and the emergence of new models of acceptance theory and treatment,
most notably what we have termed experiential acceptance. The self-acceptance
literature continued along established lines. The validity of the self-other correlation continued to be studied (e.g., Hurley, 1989, 1991, 1993; Hurley,
Feintuch, & Mandell, 1991; Hurley & Rosenberg, 1990), as did the relationship
of self-acceptance to a variety of variables, such as: psychopathology (Richter,
Richter, Eisemann, & Seering, 1995), treatment outcomes (Lemberg, 1993;
Weissman & Appleton, 1995), behavioral effects (Pufal-Struzik, 1998), height
and intelligence (Rienzi, Scrams, & Uhles, 1992), culture and occupation
(Long, 1991; Long, & Martinez, 1994), disability (Fukunishi, Koyama, &
Tombimatsu, 1995; Smart & Smart, 1991), and quality of life (Rogers, 1995).
Ryff (1995) included self-acceptance as one of six facets of well-being in her
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
17
widely used well-being scales (see Measurement of Acceptance section below).
Meanwhile, Ellis (1996), reviewing his 50 years of group and individual psychotherapy, argued that because self-esteem can be maladaptive at both high and
low levels, unconditional self-acceptance is a more useful construct. Still, selfesteem and self-acceptance have not yet been convincingly disambiguated.
Self-acceptance research also surged in Japanese psychology journals, with
scale development efforts (Itatsu, 1994, 1995; Sawazaki, 1993) and construct
validation with variables derived from Western acceptance research (Ito, 1991;
Makino & Tagami, 1998; Ueda, 1996). Generally, these investigations suggested some degree of cross-cultural applicability of the principles of acceptance,
although this has not been examined directly. More recently, self-acceptance
research has continued to refine and validate the construct (e.g., Durm & Glaze,
2001a), examine its history (e.g., Dryden & Still, 2006), and study its relationship
to other variables, such as psychopathology (e.g., Scott, 2007), psychological
health (e.g., Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001), self-esteem (e.g., MacInnes, 2006),
mindfulness (Carson & Langer, 2006), and acceptance of others (e.g., Durm &
Glaze, 2001b).
A new branch of acceptance research began in the 1990s, experiential acceptance, driven in part by a convergence of mindfulness-based meditation, cognitive-behavioral interventions, and the notion that acceptance and change could be
complementary therapeutic strategies. Although the body of literature that has
sprung from the initial convergence is growing rapidly and includes many
researchers, some noteworthy pioneers were Hayes (Hayes & Wilson, 1994),
Kabat-Zinn (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, Massion, Kristeller, & Peterson, 1992), and
Linehan (e.g., Linehan, Armstrong, Suarez, & Allmon, 1991), and others (e.g.,
Cordova, Dougher, Jacobson, Koerner, Marlatt; Hayes et al., 1994) who developed theoretical models and treatment paradigms based in part on the principles of experiential acceptance. The following section reviews some important
elements of these new developments.
CONTEMPORARY EXPERIENTIAL ACCEPTANCE:
THEORY AND RESEARCH
A variety of acceptance-based treatments have been developed and investigated for use with a range of disorders, including anxiety (e.g., Roemer & Orsillo,
2007), depression (e.g., Kanter, Baruch, & Gaynor, 2006), psychosis (e.g., Pankey
& Hayes, 2003), personality disorder (e.g., Gratz & Gunderson, 2006; Linehan
et al., 1991), chronic pain (e.g., McCracken & Eccleston, 2006), depressive
relapse (Teasdale, Segal, Williams, Ridgeway, Soulsby, & Lau, 2000), substance
abuse (Marlatt, 2002), trauma (Follette, 1994), couple discord (e.g., Jacobson,
Christensen, Prince, Cordova, & Eldridge, 2000), and smoking cessation (e.g.,
Gifford, Kohlenberg, Hayes, Antonuccio, Piasecki, Rasmussen-Hall, et al., 2004).
In addition, a host of empirical and theoretical papers have been put forth on the
18 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
relationship between acceptance and other constructs (e.g., Levy & Ebbeck,
2005; Lundh, 2004) on the integration of acceptance techniques into other
therapies (e.g., Callaghan, Gregg, Marx, Kohlenberg, & Gifford, 2004; Lynn,
Das, Hallquist, & Williams, 2006), and on the efficacy of acceptance-based
interventions (e.g., Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006; Hayes, Wilson,
Gifford, Bissett, Piasecki, Batten, et al., 2004b; Lau & McMain, 2005; Longmore
& Worrell, 2007; Roemer & Orsillo, 2002; Telford, Kralik, & Koch, 2006). In
the following review, we discuss a number of conceptual and empirical components of a nascent theory of experiential acceptance: the benefits of acceptance;
the development of acceptance-based treatments; the role of nonacceptance in
psychopathology; the constructs of thought suppression, experiential avoidance,
and emotional nonacceptance; and the paradox of acceptance and change.
Benefits of Acceptance
Ellis and Robb (1994) consider “unconditional self-acceptance as crucial to
solid emotional and behavioral health” (p. 91). Linehan (1994) believes that
moment-by-moment acceptance of the self and others is “an important part of any
self-management, interpersonal effectiveness, or emotion regulation program”
(p. 75). Dryden (1987, 1998) includes self-acceptance, high frustration tolerance,
and acceptance of uncertainty among criteria for psychological health. Other
theorists and researchers (Hayes et al., 1994; also Bond & Bunce, 2003; Greenberg
& Safran, 1987) have included among the benefits of acceptance:
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
expanded range of available experiences;
increased potential for productive action;
increased compassion and reduced blaming of others;
increased compliance, serenity, and reasonableness;
decreased distressing negative emotions, and
positive therapeutic outcomes.
Meta-analyses (e.g., Baer, 2003) and qualitative research reviews (e.g., Shapiro
& Walsh, 2003) have documented the promise of acceptance and mindfulness
techniques across many indices of psychological functioning.
Empirically, the benefits of acceptance are often described in terms of decreased
symptoms associated with acceptance-based treatment. In addition to the studies
noted above, there are a number of excellent conceptual and empirical reviews
that discuss acceptance-based treatments (e.g., Baer, 2006; Baer & Huss, 2008;
Hayes, Follette, & Linehan, 2004c; Hayes et al., 2006; Orsillo & Roemer, 2005).
In addition to symptom reduction, the benefits of acceptance in the context of
mental illness can also be conceptualized in terms of other treatment goals that
may occur whether or not symptom reduction takes place. These include modified
beliefs about symptoms, decreased distress about symptoms, and willingness
to experience symptoms while expanding adaptive behavioral repertoires. For
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
19
example, in recent research with acceptance-based interventions and patients
with psychosis, Bach and Hayes (2002) found that patients in an Acceptance
and Commitment Therapy (ACT) group showed significantly higher reporting
and lower believability of psychotic symptoms (e.g., auditory hallucinations)
compared to a treatment-as-usual (TAU) group. Most important, the rehospitalization rate in the ACT group was half that of the TAU group at 4-month
follow-up. That is, the acceptance treatment was associated with reduced rehospitalization but not decreased symptoms (although the authors noted the possibility that the frequency of reported symptoms could have been due to a decrease
in symptoms combined with a greater degree of willingness to report them).
Gaudiano and Herbert (2006) replicated and extended the study, finding (in
addition to equivalent symptom reporting, decreased believability, and reduced
rehospitalization in the ACT condition) that the change in believability was
associated with change in distress after controlling for change in frequency in the
ACT group only. In a similar vein, Teasdale et al. (2002) found that increased
metacognitive awareness (i.e., the extent to which aversive thoughts and feelings
are experienced as mental events rather than the self) mediated the reduced
relapse rates of formerly depressed patients in both mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy and traditional cognitive-behavioral therapy.
These findings represent key concepts in acceptance-based treatment: when
stimuli (in this case, symptoms) are experienced with willingness and nonjudgment, without avoidance or struggle, in the service of valued behavioral goals,
the result is a decrease in distress and maladaptive behavior. These concepts are
explicated fully in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (e.g., Hayes, Strosahl,
& Wilson, 1999), the preeminent acceptance-based treatment.
Treatment
For half a century, acceptance has been included in humanistic-experiential
therapies (e.g., Greenberg, 1994; Rogers, 1961) and Rational Emotive Therapy
(Ellis, 1961; Ellis & Grieger, 1986; Ellis & Robb, 1994), generally in the form
of self-acceptance. More recently, experiential acceptance has been incorporated
into a number of cognitive-behavioral therapies, including: Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Hayes & Strosahl, 2004; Hayes et al., 1999); Dialectical
Behavior Therapy (Linehan,1993a, 1993b); Integrative Behavioral Couple Therapy
(Jacobson & Christensen, 1996; Jacobson et al., 2000); Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002; Teasdale et al., 2000); and
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (Kabat-Zinn, 1984, 1990).
Acceptance-based interventions have been developed to treat specific disorders, and syndromes, including genderalized anxiety disorder (Borkovec,
Alcaine, & Behar, 2004), eating disorders (Kristeller & Hallett, 1999; Wilson,
1996), substance abuse (Hayes et al., 2004b), trichotilomania (Twohig & Woods,
2004), nicotine dependence (Gifford et al., 2004), and chronic pain (Dahl, Wilson,
20 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
& Nilsson, 2004; Kabat-Zinn, 1982; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth & Burney, 1985;
McCracken & Eccleston, 2006). Recently, a large-scale study of severely
depressed people found that behavioral activation, an approach that stresses
accepting depressed feelings while actively engaging in social and occupational activities, proved to be superior to a purely cognitive treatment of
depression, and equal in effectiveness to pharmacological treatment (Dimidjian,
Hollon, Dobson, Schmaling, Kohlenberg, Addis, et al., 2006; Dobson, Hollon,
Dimidjian, Schmaling, Kohlenberg, Gallop, et al., 2008). Acceptance-based
approaches have also been successfully integrated into alcohol relapse prevention programs (e.g., Marlatt, 2002), and theory suggests that they may have
value in combination with hypnotic procedures (Lynn et al., 2006), in enhancing
athletic performance (Gardner & Moore, 2004), and in suicide prevention
(Williams & Swales, 2004). Nevertheless, it is not yet clear which aspects of
these programs (e.g., exposure, enhanced expectancy, acceptance) are implicated
in therapeutic changes.
Acceptance and Psychopathology
Conceptually, a corollary of the beneficial aspects of acceptance is that nonacceptance is associated with psychopathology. Although nonacceptance can
be a problematic term because it lacks specificity, in that it may refer to a host of
constructs that could be considered opposite to acceptance, such as denial, escape,
prejudice, avoidance, and noncompliance, it is sometimes used to describe low
levels of acceptance across a variety of behavioral and experiential domains.
Ellis and Robb (1994), for example, have noted that people experience anxiety
and depression when they do not accept themselves, others, and frustrating
circumstances. Similarly, low experiential acceptance has been conceptualized
as a contributing to or maintaining psychopathology (e.g., Hayes et al., 2004d;
Strosahl, Hayes, Wilson, & Gifford, 2004). This contention finds descriptive
support in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV;
American Psychiatric Association, 1994), insofar as avoidance, a form of
nonacceptance, is specified among the diagnostic criteria of mental disorders,
including avoidant personality disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, phobia,
posttraumatic stress disorder, and substance dependence. Analogously, attachment—a dimension of nonacceptance defined as a maladaptive attention to or
pursuit of certain stimuli over others—is described indirectly in the DSM-IV in
the form of preoccupations of one sort or another, as in body dysmorphic disorder;
hypochondriasis; eating disorders; pathological gambling; schizophrenia,
paranoid type; and narcissistic, obsessive-compulsive, and paranoid personality
disorders. From a behavioral perspective, Skinner (1972) suggested that the
“time and energy consumed in the avoidance of punishment” could be freed up
for “more reinforcing activities” (p. 76).
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
21
Empirically, research areas deal with nonacceptance constructs implicated
in the development and maintenance of psychopathology. These include the
well researched thought suppression (e.g., Wegner et al., 1987) and experiential
avoidance (e.g., Hayes et al., 1996), as well as the more recent emotional nonacceptance (e.g., Gratz, Bornovalova, Delany-Brumsey, Nick, & Lejuez, 2007;
Tull & Roemer, 2007). These constructs, particularly the first two, have been
reviewed elsewhere, so we will discuss them briefly.
Thought Suppression
Prefigured by Freud’s (1953) attention to thoughts driven from consciousness,
as well as early research that instructions to suppress thoughts causes them to
return (e.g., Antrobus & Singer, 1964; Langfeld, 1910), Wegner and colleagues
(e.g., 1987) elaborated the paradoxical effects of thought suppression in the
production and maintenance of psychological disorders. In particular, obsessivecompulsive disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and depression can be characterized in part by distressing, intrusive thoughts and frequent suppression attempts (Beevers, Wenzlaff, Hayes, & Scott, 1999; Purdon, 1999; Wegner,
1989; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). Suppression appears to increase the frequency
of the unwanted thoughts as well as intensify the emotional distress (Purdon,
1999). Wenzlaff and Wegner (2000) describe thought suppression as “a popular
form of mental control . . . that it can be counterproductive, helping assure the
very state of mind one had hoped to avoid” (p. 59). Although there are some
inconsistencies in the literature (e.g., Purdon, 1999), many studies have documented the paradoxical effects of thought suppression.
Experiential Avoidance
Chawla and Ostafin (2007) describe experiential avoidance as “unwillingness
to remain in contact with private experiences such as painful thoughts and
emotions” (p. 871). Karekla, Forsythe, and Kelly (2004) claim that experiential
avoidance is a “core psychological diathesies underlying the development
and maintenance of several forms of psychopathology . . . and human suffering
in general” (pp. 725-726). Blackledge and Hayes (2001) describe experiential
avoidance as a focus in most systems of therapy. Freud’s (e.g., 1914/1957b)
psychodynamic theory, for example, emphasizes the repression of painful
or threatening material into the unconscious. In general, avoiding negative
affect has been described in a variety of theories as a contributor to psychopathology (e.g., Foa, Steketee, & Young, 1984; Hayes et al., 1999; Kelly, 1955;
Mowrer, 1947).
Experiential avoidance can be conceptualized as comprising a number of
related constructs, such as thought suppression (Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000),
emotional suppression (Gross & Levenson, 1993), avoidance coping (Penley,
Tomaka, & Wiebe, 2002), reappraisal (Lazarus, 1991), and self-deception (Paulhus,
22 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
1988). Thus, experiential avoidance may be a candidate for an organizing construct, though Chawla and Ostafin (2007) caution that it lacks theoretical integration and incremental validity relative to other constructs. Like thought suppression, avoidance is not only maladaptive, but also invokes ironic or paradoxical
processes (e.g., Hayes et al., 1999). For example, not only does avoidance
predispose or contribute to a variety of clinical syndromes, including depression
(see MacLeod, Bjork, & Bjork, 2003; Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, 1995)
and anxiety (Amir, Coles, Brigidi, & Foa, 2001), but the process of inhibiting
thoughts, feelings, memories, and other internal events increases the probability
that those very events will recur (Hayes & Wilson, 2003; Polivy & Herman,
1987; Strauss, Doyle, & Kreipe, 1994; Wegner et al., 1987). Both thought
suppression and experiential avoidance thus appear to be associated with a
rebound of unwanted content into consciousness.
Emotional Nonacceptance
Emotional nonacceptance has been defined as “the evaluation of emotions as
bad or wrong,” leading to a “subsequent development of secondary emotional
responses (e.g., fear or shame)” which may “motivate attempts to avoid emotions”
(Gratz et al., 2007, p. 257). Although limited, a small body of research supports
emotional nonacceptance as a factor in emotion regulation difficulties (Tull
& Roemer, 2007) and anxiety disorder-related pathology (see Hayes et al., 2006;
Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, 2005; Salters-Pedneault, Tull, & Roemer,
2004). Tull and Roemer (2007) suggest that “attempting to extinguish the emotion
altogether or responding to an emotional experience with fear, shame, or some
other negative emotion (secondary emotional responding or nonacceptance) is
associated with worse outcomes” (p. 379). Some studies have demonstrated that
the suppression of emotional expression or experience may result in increased
physiological arousal (Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006; Gross
& Levenson, 1993, 1997). Prior research has shown that when emotions are
suppressed or concealed, memory (Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Westphal, &
Coifman, 2004; Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003; Richards & Gross, 1999) and
problem solving (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998) are compromised, and stress and anxiety escalate, as indexed by physiological (Richards et al.,
2003, Study 2) and subjective (Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004) markers.
Acceptance and the Paradox of Change
Acceptance is sometimes misunderstood to be antithetical to change. The
widely quoted “Serenity Prayer” of Alcoholics Anonymous (1967), for example,
can be misconstrued to mean that only what cannot be changed should be
accepted. Because acceptance is not tantamount to resignation to the status quo,
and efforts to change the status quo are often highly desirable (e.g., Cordova
& Kohlenberg, 1994; Dougher & Hackbert, 1994), acceptance and change can
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
23
co-occur in at least two ways. First, if acceptance involves acknowledging the
unvarnished facts of a situation rather than passivity or resignation, acceptance
can occur throughout the change process. Ellis and Robb (1994) note that when
one chooses change in response to a given set of circumstances, one must accept
the antecedent circumstances as they are, then accept the process of change itself,
and finally accept the results of the change effort. Second, acceptance itself can
spur change transformation. Hayes (1994) contends that “when one gives up on
trying to be different, one becomes . . . immediately different in a very profound
way” (p. 20), and Greenberg (1994) holds that “the more people accept themselves in their full complexity, the more they change” (p. 55). Acceptance and
change strategies have been combined in recent therapies based on mindfulness and acceptance principles, such as ACT, DBT, IBCT, MBSR, MBCT, and
others. It may be that the “paradox of acceptance and change” is due in part to a
misunderstanding of acceptance as well as the dialectic of acceptance and change
as complementary processes.
Acceptance and Mindfulness
An important consideration when discussing the construct of experiential
acceptance is its relation to mindful awareness or mindfulness. Bishop, Lau,
Shapiro, Carlson, Anderson, Carmody, et al. (2004) recently proposed an operational definition of mindfulness that focuses on two components: sustained attention to present experience, and an attitude of openness, curiosity, and acceptance.
Noting the use of the term acceptance in the definition of mindfulness, it often
happens that the two constructs are discussed in tandem (e.g., “mindfulness
and acceptance” research, treatments, or special interest groups), both in broad
theoretical principles as well as specific interventions (e.g., Baer & Huss, 2008;
Hayes et al., 2004d; Herbert & Cardaciotto, 2005; Hofmann & Asmundson,
2008; Roemer & Orsillo, 2002; Walser & Westrup, 2007). The major mindfulness interventions include an acceptance component (e.g., MBCT, MBSR), and
acceptance interventions (e.g., ACT, DBT) often include informal mindfulnessbased awareness or formal mindfulness meditation training as treatment components. Finally, a number of mindfulness assessments include sets of items
and factored dimensions, meant to assess acceptance.
Some theorists have proposed that mindfulness is a component of acceptance
(e.g., of radical acceptance; Brach, 2003), whereas others consider acceptance
to be a facet of mindfulness (e.g., Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004; Cardaciotto, 2005;
Kabat-Zinn, 1994). At present there appears to be more support for the latter
view, but this may reflect differential interest in the investigation of the structure
of the two constructs. Whereas the structure of mindfulness is under investigation
(e.g., Adele & Feldman, 2004; Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney,
2006; Lau, Bishop, Segal, Buis, Anderson, Carlson, et al., 2006), the structural
validation of experiential acceptance is in the early stages. The two constructs also
24 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
may be distinct but overlapping. Another difficulty is that mindfulness can refer to
a theoretical construct, a psychological process, and a set of practices (Germer,
2005), which may increase the complexity of differentiating it from acceptance.
ADDITIONAL FACETS OF EXPERIENTIAL
ACCEPTANCE
In this section we describe hypothesized facets of experiential acceptance
evident in both contemporary and ancient literature which remain as yet largely
speculative due to a lack of empirical scrutiny. Whenever possible, we have
chosen to call these facets by the names given to them in the psychological
literature. Where this is not possible or expedient, we have attempted to present
each facet in a way that is reasonably coherent and faithful to a sometimes diffuse
literature. We approach this task as descriptive rather than definitive. Our hope is
that these considerations will contribute to the development of testable hypotheses
to validate experiential acceptance more thoroughly.
Awareness
McCurry and Schmidt (1994) and others (e.g., Kohlenberg, 1994; Linehan,
1994; Marlatt, 1994) have observed that “awareness is basic to the acceptance
process” (p. 242); in order to accept a stimulus, one first must be aware of it.
Experiential acceptance may involve an ability to observe any internal or external
stimulus as it is occurring, in contrast to avoidance (Hayes, 1994), suppression
(Wegner, 1989), or denial (Linehan, 1994), or it may simply mean remaining
“fully present” with whatever stimuli are available (Gifford, 1994, p. 220).
Awareness is an area in which mindfulness and acceptance may overlap, in
that mindfulness implies directed attention (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Mindfulness has
been described in fact as an “aware non-attached state of mental acceptance”
(Marlatt, 1994), and may also overlap with experiential acceptance because
it requires attention without evaluation, judgment, avoidance, or attachment
toward any stimulus.
Nonattachment
The most basic definition of nonattachment in experiential acceptance may
be “letting go” (e.g., Beattie, 1990), which means to release one’s attachment
to internal or external stimuli (Koerner, Jacobson, & Christensen, 1994). Some
classes of stimuli that have been discussed in terms of attachment and experiential acceptance include the effects of drugs or alcohol (Marlatt, 1994), one’s
personal goals (Ellis & Robb, 1994), or the results of one’s efforts (Martell,
Addis, & Dimidjian, 2004; Suzuki, 1973). Nonattachment can also be described
in terms of constructs such as “decentering” (Segal et al., 2002, p. 41),
“metacognitive awareness” (Teasdale et al., 2002), “metacognitive experiencing”
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
25
(Wells, 2000), and “self as context” (Strosahl et al., 2004, p. 44). Nonattachment
in these constructs involves the simultaneous observation of internal or external
events as they are experienced. For example, if one has the thought “I am bad,”
a nonattached perspective would include an observation such as “I am having
a thought that I am bad,” as opposed to an attached perspective, unable to distinguish the thought from reality.
Another aspect of nonattachment is behavioral—one may accept the experience of impulses to act (e.g., to pursue or avoid stimuli) without acting on
them, sometimes called nondoing (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). Nondoing can be used
to distinguish nonattachment and nonavoidance, in terms of the function of a
behavior. For an alcoholic, acceptance of an urge to drink may involve not walking
into a bar. For a socially phobic person, however, acceptance of an impulse to
avoid socializing may involve walking into the same bar. The behaviors are
topographically opposite, but functionally similar: both represent nonattachment
to private urges. Another way of considering the relationship between attachment
and avoidance is that avoidance tends to be negatively reinforced through the
removal of an aversive stimulus, whereas attachment tends to be positively
reinforced through the addition or maintenance of an appetitive stimulus.
Nonjudgment
Nonjudgment is central to modern conceptualizations of acceptance (e.g.,
Cordova & Kohlenberg, 1994, Germer, 2005; Hayes, 2004a; Hayes & Wilson,
2003; Peterson, 1994) as well as mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006; Dimidjian &
Linehan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Kohlenberg, 1994; Linehan, 1993a). Nonjudgment is a conscious abstention from categorizing experience as good or bad
(Cordova & Kohlenberg, 1994), or right or wrong (Hayes, 1994; Linehan, 1994),
such as “anxiety is bad.” Nonjudgment is included in a number of cognitivebehavioral therapies that employ mindfulness and acceptance (e.g., ACT, DBT,
MBCT), as well as older therapies such as Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy
(REBT; David, Lynn, & Ellis, 2009; Ellis & Dryden, 1997). According to REBT,
beliefs mediate the emotional consequence of an activating event, and to that
extent judgments determine experience. Highly negative evaluation of internal
or external events has been characterized as “awfulizing” and catastrophizing,
which may potentiate anxiety and depression (Beck, Emery, & Greenberg,
1996; Ellis & Robb, 1994), and the expectation of negative events or their
continuing without relief can have serious negative consequences (see Abramson,
Alloy, & Hogan, 1997; Kirsch & Lynn, 1995). Nonjudgment in experiential
acceptance involves actively processing stimuli according to their descriptive
or concrete properties rather than evaluating them.
Radical Acceptance
Some theorists use the term radical acceptance to describe a Zen-like state of
acceptance, consisting of a thorough willingness to experience whatever is taking
26 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
place in the moment (e.g., Brach, 2003; Linehan, 1994). Radical acceptance
may be the positive extreme of experiential acceptance, in which, according to
Linehan (1994), one experiences “total allowance now,” and a “constant accepting
in each successive moment” (p. 80). As noted above, in the classical literature
acceptance is associated with wisdom and enlightenment, which can be profound and paradoxical. In a similar vein, some modern authors (e.g., Robbins,
Schmidt, & Linehan, 2004) have noted paradoxical effects of radical acceptance.
Fruzzetti and Iverson (2004) hold that by radically accepting a stimulus one has
experienced as negative, the valence of the stimulus may be reversed so that
it not only loses its aversive quality but acquires appetitive properties. It is
possible to imagine, for example, a person who is phobic of snakes subsequently
coming to own and enjoy them.
Tolerance
Whether tolerance should be considered an aspect of acceptance has been the
subject of some debate. For example, Fruzzetti and Iverson (2004) describe
acceptance as inclusive of “simple tolerance” (p. 177), but Hayes and colleagues
(2004a) assert that acceptance should not be equated with tolerance or resignation.
Some authors, such as Linehan (1994) and McCurry (1994), take issue with the
word tolerance itself, based on its association with stoical austerity, though Ellis
and Robb (1994) note, in fact, that elements of stoic philosophy were key
ingredients in the development of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy. Tolerance
need not connote helpless resignation, as its etymological roots can be found in a
variety of verbs such as endure, permit, allow, indulge, bear, and carry (Onions,
1966, p. 929).
These roots contribute to three modern denotations of tolerance (Weiner &
Simpson, 1991, p. 2075) . First, as an ability to endure pain or hardship, acceptance
may include an ability to tolerate emotional distress (e.g., Afari, 1994; Cordova
& Kohlenberg, 1994; Fruzzetti & Iverson, 2004; Linehan, 1994; Wulfert, 1994),
especially when such tolerance takes the place of maladaptive behaviors or in
the service of one’s values (Hayes & Strosahl, 2004). Tolerance can represent an
ability to endure the moment, even if the moment is unpleasant and one intends to
improve it. Second, as a disposition to be patient with the opinions or practices of
others, including a freedom from bigotry or undue severity in judging others,
tolerance is consistent with acceptance of others (e.g., Ellis & Robb, 1994; Fruzzetti
& Iverson, 2004; Gandhi, 1999; Greenberg, 1994; Linehan, 1994). Third, tolerance
is consistent with behavioral principles, such as habituation, an acquired ability to
experience a stimulus based on repeated exposure, and exposure therapy (e.g.,
McAllister & McAllister, 1995; O’Donohue & Krasner, 1995), in which repeated
contact with a stimulus results in a decrease in arousal and an expansion of the
behavioral repertoire. Tolerance then may be built up toward specific stimuli,
such as one’s own emotions or the situations that engender them. Altogether, these
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
27
definitions are consistent with theorists who characterize experiential acceptance
as an ability to experience stimuli without resorting to avoidance or escape.
Willingness
Willingness is represented in the acceptance literature as a disposition to
undertake an action or consent to an experience. Germer (2005) defined acceptance as a willingness is to “let things be just as they are the moment we become
aware of them, accepting pleasurable and painful experiences as they arise”
(p. 7). For Wulfert (1994) acceptance is the willingness “to face difficult situations and tolerate whatever feelings come up” (p. 215). Marlatt (1994) describes
the willingness to “surf” urges rather than abuse substances: clients are instructed
to “let go” and “allow the urge to rise and fall on its own” (p. 181). Willingness,
then, is often associated with enacting a new, adaptive, behavior while tolerating
the discomfort associated with the change. In ACT (Hayes et al., 1999), for
example, substantial attention is given to developing new behavioral repertoires,
and taking action in accordance with valued goals. More generally, a patient
who follows a mental health professional’s instructions practices willingness
in the sense of treatment adherence.
Domains of Acceptance
Stimulus domains define the objects of acceptance, or what is accepted when
acceptance takes place. The objects of experiential acceptance may comprise
whatever stimuli are present, though historically the acceptance literature has
specified two primary domains: “self-acceptance” (e.g., Berger, 1952; Ellis &
Dryden, 1997; Maslow, 1954; Rogers, 1961) and “acceptance of others” (e.g.,
Ellis & Robb, 1994; Fey, 1954, 1955; Greenberg, 1994; Phillips, 1951). As
discussed above, the self and other domains have been substantially validated
over the years and also have been shown to be correlated (e.g., Berger, 1952;
Omwake, 1954; Suinn & Geiger, 1965; Trent, 1957). They also point to two, more
general, categories which together also would include all of the relevant stimulus
domains pertaining to experiential acceptance. These can be termed internal and
external, with internal referring to private experience, or stimuli occurring within
the self, and external referring to all stimuli occurring outside the self.
Internal stimuli have been conceptualized as comprising at least three types
of stimuli:
1. thoughts (e.g., Hayes, 1994; Kornfield, 1993);
2. feelings (e.g., Ellis & Robb, 1994; Hayes, 1994; Martell et al., 2004;
Rogers, 1961); and
3. sensations (e.g., Hayes, 1994; Kornfield, 1993; Marlatt, 1994; McCracken,
1998), as well as complex events stemming from the interaction of these
three basic domains.
28 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
External stimuli associated with acceptance have been defined mainly with
respect to two types of stimuli: other people and situations. Together, internal and
external acceptance would appear to cover most domains of experience. Within
the domains of internal and external, however, there are more possible classes of
stimuli beyond the five sub-domains listed here (e.g., pain is a specific stimulus
domain associated with unique treatment paradigms and measurement instruments within the acceptance literature).
Measurement of Acceptance
Classic Self-Acceptance Scales of the 1950s
Scales designed to measure acceptance have developed over the years along
with emerging conceptualizations of the construct. Early measures of acceptance
were developed in the 1950s based on contemporary conceptualizations of selfacceptance and acceptance of others, drawing on theorists such as Adler, Ellis,
Horney, Fromm, Perls, Rogers, and Sullivan. In general, acceptance in these
measures was operationalized as a positive attitude toward the self and others.
Although at least eight acceptance measures were developed in the 1950s (see
Crowne & Stephens, 1961), only four were used widely: Berger’s (1952)
Expressed Acceptance of Self and Others Scale, Bills’ (1958; Bills, Vance, &
McLean, 1951) Index of Adjustment and Values, Fey’s (1954) Acceptance of
Self and Others Scale, and Phillips’ (1951) Self-Others Questionnaire, each
of which accumulated some degree of reliability and validity data (Fey, 1955;
Omwake, 1954; Sheerer, 1949). These measures were used in studies supportive
of a number of hypotheses about acceptance, including the prediction that selfacceptance and acceptance of others were positively correlated, that acceptance
was negatively correlated with psychopathology, and that measures of acceptance
tended to converge on a single construct.
Within a decade of the development of these measures, however, a number of
investigators began to question the validity of the self-acceptance construct and
the instruments used to measure it, on conceptual and methodological grounds.
The major criticisms were that the construct was poorly defined, varying substantially across researchers and theorists, and that the instruments were conceptually unsound, poorly constructed, and confounded with social desirability
(e.g., Block & Thomas, 1955; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964; Crowne & Stephens,
1961; Kinkler & Myers; 1963; Stone, 1964). Although only a few acceptance
scales were used much beyond the studies in which they were developed—the
rest being constructed for a specific research purpose and never again cited—
the critics of acceptance measures tended to review them together. Some scales,
however, received relatively more validation over time than others.
Berger’s (1952) scale has received the widest use and most thorough validation. Composed of 64 items (36 self-acceptance and 28 acceptance of others),
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
29
self-acceptance and acceptance of others were operationalized on 16 behavioral
dimensions, largely based on Sheerer’s (1949) “acceptance and respect” for
oneself and others (p. 170). The scale was initially validated on a combined
school, community, prison, and clinical sample of 315 subjects. The scale showed
good reliability (.75–.89 for self-acceptance; .78–.88 for other acceptance) and
initial validity based on expected correlations between the self and other item
sets (.36–.69 across samples) and expected correlations with free writing tasks
rated for degree of acceptance (.78–.90). Early uses of the scale included testing
the relationship between self and other acceptance, and between acceptance and
psychopathology (Berger, 1952, 1955).
Because the Berger scale has the largest body of research of any scale of that
era, it is a useful reference for a host of established construct relations. In
addition, it includes both self and other scales, which may be used alone or in
combination. The main weakness of the Berger scale concerns the extent to
which it measures acceptance, as opposed to social desirability or, for that matter,
self-esteem (e.g., Antill & Cunningham, 1979; Eagly & Whitehead, 1972; Neff,
Kirkpatrick, & Rude, 2007). Nevertheless, the Berger scale may be considered
the measure of choice among the original acceptance scales from the 1950s.
Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire (USAQ)
The USAQ (Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001) was designed to operationalize
self-acceptance consistent with the principles of Rational Emotive Behavior
Therapy (REBT; Ellis, 1973; Ellis & Dryden, 1997). It was constructed to
differentiate self-acceptance from self-esteem, although it has shown a substantial
correlation with self-esteem, and the nature of the relationship between the
two constructs remains under investigation (e.g., Chamberlain & Haaga 2001;
MacInnes, 2006; Thompson & Waltz, 2008). Relatedly, the USAQ is the only
scale that specifically addresses unconditional self-acceptance as distinct from
positive self-regard. The USAQ is also the only recent self-acceptance scale
constructed as a stand-alone measure, rather than as a component of another
construct, such as well-being. Despite these strengths, the major drawback of the
USAQ is that it is a relatively new scale with a good reliability profile but
limited construct validity data.
Self-Acceptance Subscales of Multitrait Measures
Personal Orientation Inventory (POI; Knapp, 1976; Shostrom, 1964, 1966)—
The POI is a 120-item inventory of psychological well-being, mental health, and
self-actualization with substantial evidence of validity (Fox, Knapp, & Michael,
1968; Hattie, 1986; Shostrom, 1964). The POI includes twelve scales, three of
which are relevant to acceptance: self-acceptance, self-regard, and acceptance
of aggression. A strength of this scale is its substantial reliability and validity,
supported by a host of studies over the last 50 years. Another apparent strength is
30 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
the inclusion of multiple acceptance-related scales, and the intentional differentiation between self-acceptance and self-regard. That said, although the selfregard and self-acceptance scales generally correlate in the .20 range (Shostrom,
1973), Shepard (1979) was unable to distinguish the two scales at the item
level, and other studies have suggested that fewer dimensions overall may be
more parsimonious (e.g., Klavetter & Mogar, 1967). Further, although the selfacceptance and self-regard scales were used in the major construct validation of
the self-acceptance construct to date (Shepard, 1979), few studies have reported
the reliability and validity of the self-acceptance subscale alone.
California Psychological Inventory (CPI; Gough, 1957, 1987)—The CPI is a
434-item assessment of normal personality (Gough, 1957; Gough & Bradley,
1996). A 260-item shorter version is also available. The CPI has a research
base of approximately 2,000 citations, and is based on a normative sample of
6,000 men and women (see Gough & Bradley, 1996). The CPI includes 29
subscales. The Self-Acceptance subscale measures “sense of personal worth,
self-acceptance, and capacity for independent thinking and action; being secure
with and sure of oneself” (Megargee, 1972, p. 67). The CPI also includes a
Tolerance subscale, which measures “permissive, accepting, and nonjudgmental
social beliefs and attitudes” (p. 91). Like the POI, a major strength of the CPI is
its established reliability and validity. Also like the POI, however, the body of
research supportive of the CPI relates to the scale as a whole rather than individual
subscales. Although the CPI has been used in a number of acceptance studies
(e.g., Ganter, 1962; Greene, Baucom, & Macon, 1980), few studies have reported
reliability and validity data for the self-acceptance subscale, with the exception
of Vingoe (1968). Additionally, factor analytic studies generally have not found
structural evidence of a self-acceptance factor (Crites, Bechtoldt, Goodstein,
& Heilbrun, 1961; Mitchell & Pierce-Jones, 1960; Nichols & Schnell, 1963;
Pumroy, 1962; Springob & Struening, 1964).
Scales of Psychological Well-Being (SPW; Ryff, 1995)—The SPW is an 84-item
scale of psychological well-being composed of six 14-item scales constructed to
measure the dimensions of autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth,
positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. The reliability
and validity of the SPW were demonstrated in the original development studies
and follow-up appraisals (e.g., Ryff, 1989, 1995; Ryff & Keyes, 1995; Ryff
& Singer, 2006). Self-acceptance is included among those dimensions that
“encompass a breadth of wellness that includes positive evaluations of oneself
and one’s past life” (Ryff, 1995, p. 720). The Self-Acceptance scale is operationalized as follows. A high scorer “possesses a positive attitude toward the self;
acknowledges and accepts multiple aspects of self, including good and bad
qualities; feels positive about past life” (p. 727). Measures of happiness show
modest to strong associations with the Self-Acceptance scale (Ryff, 1995).
Based on the validation of its six-factor model, the SPW may be considered a
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
31
primary measure of psychological well-being. The major strength of this scale
is its substantial reliability, validity, and corpus of construct relations established.
Its major weakness is the limited reliability of the Self-Acceptance scale (a = .46).
Experiential Acceptance Scales
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ)
A variety of new instruments have been developed that operationalize experiential acceptance. The AAQ (Hayes et al., 2004a) was developed to measure
acceptance, operationalized primarily as experiential avoidance. It was validated
initially on 10 samples collected during separate investigations of experiential
avoidance. A 32-item version of the scale was developed using an item pool based
on the theory of experiential avoidance employed in ACT, in which experiential
avoidance was defined as “the phenomenon that occurs when a person is unwilling
to remain in contact with particular private experiences . . . and takes steps to
alter the form or frequency of these experiences and the contexts that occasion
them, even when these forms of avoidance cause behavioral harm” (p. 554). The
AAQ has a substantial research base, having been used in at least 70 studies
investigating the role of experiential acceptance in clinical disorders, such as
anxiety (e.g., Tull & Roemer, 2007), depression, (e.g., Cribb, Moulds, & Carter,
2006), and personality (e.g., Gratz, Rosenthal, Tull, Lejuez, & Gunderson, 2006),
as well as general psychological vulnerability (e.g., Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth,
& Steger, 2006), well-being (e.g., Kashdan & Breen, 2007), job performance
(e.g., Bond & Flaxman, 2006), and pain tolerance (Feldner, Hekmat, Zvolensky,
Vowles, Secrist, & Leen-Feldner, 2006). The AAQ has also been instrumental
in the development of other scales (e.g., Baer et al., 2004; Gratz & Roemer,
2004; McCracken, Vowles, & Eccleston, 2004).
A number of versions of the scale are in use at present, with 9, 10, and 16 items,
as well as the initial 49-item pool, sometimes used in factor analytic studies
(R. Baer, personal communication, 2006). The AAQ is generally considered
to be composed of one general factor of experiential avoidance, or conversely
experiential acceptance with reverse coding (e.g., Hayes et al., 2004a). That
said, the AAQ appears to operationalize, in adition to nonavoidance of aversive
stimuli, facets reflecting willingness to experience unpleasant emotions and a
capacity for productive action in the face of those stimuli and emotions.
The AAQ may be considered the current gold standard measure of experiential
acceptance. Nevertheless, the factor structure has not yet been resolved, due in
part to the fact that there are a number of versions of the AAQ in use. Additionally,
the internal consistency has not always been optimal. These issues, however,
may be resolved when the revised AAQ (AAQ-R) is published. Although the
factor structure may include dimensions associated with willingness and action,
the AAQ was constructed to measure experiential avoidance, and may therefore
measure only one dimension of acceptance.
32 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Other Avoidance Scales
In addition to the AAQ, there are a number of other measures that have
been used to measure and validate various types of avoidance, including: the
Cognitive-Behavioral Avoidance Scale (CBAS; Ottenbreit & Dobson, 2004),
which measures avoidance across several dimensions, including cognitive versus
behavioral avoidance, active versus passive avoidance, and social versus nonsocial avoidance; the White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI; Wegner &
Zanakos, 1994), which measures the tendency to suppress unwanted thoughts;
the Thought Control Questionnaire (TCQ; Wells & Davies, 1994), which
measures the specific strategies used to control unpleasant or unwanted thoughts;
and the Escape-Avoidance subscale of the Ways of Coping Questionnaire (WCQ;
Folkman & Lazarus, 1988), which measures coping characterized by escape
and avoidance, such as wishing a situation would go away, or by eating, drinking,
smoking, or using drugs to feel better. The CBAS, WBSI, TCQ, and WCQ
represent some of the core instruments that have been used to operationalize
and validate the construct of avoidance, and have also shown moderate correlations with the AAQ.
Experiential Acceptance Subscales in
Multidimensional Measures
Similar to self-acceptance, experiential acceptance also has been included
as a factor in multidimensional measures. In some cases, acceptance skills are
conceptualized as components of larger skill sets. The Difficulties in Emotion
Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) includes an Emotional Nonacceptance (EN) factor of items reflecting “a tendency to have negative secondary
emotional responses to one’s negative emotions, or nonaccepting reactions to
one’s distress” (p. 47). The DERS operationalizes emotion regulation as involving
awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions, and the inclusion of the
EN factor is based on the theory that a lack of emotional acceptance is maladaptive
and associated with greater difficulties in emotion regulation. In a similar vein, the
COPE scales (COPE; Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989), a general measure of
coping, include an Acceptance factor composed of items assessing the degree to
which a person can: a) accept the existence of stressors, and b) accept the limits of
his or her own coping strategies. The COPE operationalizes acceptance as the
opposite of denial and assesses coping in part as the ability to attempt to deal
directly with stressors even when one’s coping skills are inadequate. A shorter version
of the COPE, the Brief COPE (BC; Carver, 1997) retains acceptance items.
Domain-Specific Acceptance Scales
A number of scales assess acceptance within specific stimulus domains, or item
content categories including acceptance of: athletic performance (Sport-Specific
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
33
Self-Acceptance Scale; SSS; Waite, Gansneder, & Rotella, 1990), death
(Death Acceptance Scale; DAS; Ray & Najman, 1974), disability (Acceptance of
Disability Scale; ADS; Linkowski, 1971), illness (Acceptance of Illness Scale;
AIS; Felton, Revenson, & Hinrichsen, 1984), and romantic partners (Frequency
and Acceptability of Partner Behavior Inventory; FAPBI; Doss & Christensen,
2006). The way acceptance is operationalized is not necessarily uniform across
these measures, although a number of domain-specific scales have been closely
patterned on the AAQ. These include scales assessing acceptance of auditory
hallucinations (Voices Acceptance and Action Scale; VAAS; Shawyer, Ratcliff,
Mackinnon, Farhall, Hayes, & Copolov, 2007) and chronic pain (Chronic Pain
Acceptance Questionnaire; CPAQ; McCracken et al., 2004), and scales currently
in development, including acceptance of diabetes, epilepsy, smoking, and tinnitus
(S. C. Hayes, personal communication, August 2, 2005).
Most acceptance scales are in fact specific to only one or two domains and
dimensions. The AAQ, for example employs thoughts and feelings as stimulus
domains and operationalizes acceptance as nonavoidance. In contrast, the classic
self-acceptance measures employ domains of self and other and operationalize
acceptance along the dimension of positive regard. If acceptance is a multidimensional construct, no extant scale can be considered a truly general measure
of acceptance necessary to establish further structural validity.
Measurement of Related Constructs
Measures of Mindfulness
In the past decade, a number of measures have been developed to assess mindful
awareness. These measures differ somewhat in their operationalization and use.
For example, the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; Baer et al.,
2004) is based on mindfulness skills taught in Dialectical Behavior Therapy
(DBT; Linehan, 1994) and was designed to measure mindfulness in the
general population. In contrast, the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI;
Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmüller, Kleinknecht, & Schmidt, 2006) is based on
Vipassana principles and was designed to measures changes in mindfulness
in meditators over the course an ongoing practice. Similarly, the Toronto
Mindfulness Scale (TMS; Lau et al., 2006) was designed as a state measure
of mindfulness, for use immediately following a meditation session. The
Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale–Revised (CMS-R; Feldman, Hayes,
Kumar, Greeson, & Laurenceau, 2007), based on contemporary mindfulness
theory, is a multi-dimensional scale that appears to measure changes in
mindfulness over the course of psychotherapy. In contrast, the Mindful Awareness
and Attention Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) and Mindfulness Questionnaire (MQ; Chadwick, Hember, Mead, Lilley, & Dagnan, 2005; see Baer et al.,
2006) assess single dimensions of mindfulness, respectively, present attention and
awareness, and a mindful approach to distressing thoughts and images. Other
34 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
scales have been validated initially, such as Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale
(PHLMS; Cardaciotto, 2005; Cardaciotto & Herbert, 2005), a two-factor scale
measuring awareness and acceptance. One noteworthy scale has resulted
from a factor-analytic study of five mindfulness scales (CAMS, FMI, KIMS,
MAAS, and MQ) by Baer and colleagues (2006). This Five-Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire (FFMQ) has provided evidence for the structural validity of
mindfulness as measured by the KIMS scale: items from the five measures
appear to map onto the KIMS four-factor structure (Observe, Describe, Act
With Awareness, Accept Without Judgment) with the addition of one additional
factor (Nonreactivity). Although it will require further validation, the FFMQ
appears to be a useful instrument for construct validation of mindfulness and
acceptance.
The psychometric properties and validity of these measures have been
described in detail elsewhere (see Baer et al., 2006; Block-Lerner et al., 2005),
including their correlations with acceptance scales (e.g., the KIMS accept dimension has shown a –.26* correlation with the AAQ; Baer et al., 2004). However,
most of these scales operationalize mindfulness in ways that overlap with
acceptance. For example, the FMI used acceptance-like facets to construct items
reflecting nonjudgment toward the self and others, and openness to positive and
negative sensations. Other scales include distinct acceptance dimensions with
subsequent validation by factor analysis, such as the CAMS (Acceptance of
Internal Experiences factor), the KIMS and FFMQ (Accept without Judgment
factor), the PHLMS (Acceptance factor), and the TMS (Observation factor,
composed of curiosity, acceptance, and openness to experience). In contrast,
the MAAS was constructed to assess only attention to and awareness of what is
occurring in the present, and not “attributes such as acceptance, trust, empathy,
gratitude, or the various others that have been associated with mindfulness”
(Brown & Ryan, 2003, p. 824). As a result, MAAS items contain no obvious
acceptance content. It may be the case that the MAAS may be considered the
purest measure of mindfulness with respect to acceptance content, although it
should be noted that the MAAS was significantly correlated with the KIMS
accept without judgment factor at .30* (Baer et al., 2004).
Scales Measuring Hypothesized Acceptance Dimensions
We consider here a number of measures that may operationalize some of the
remaining dimensions of experiential acceptance in the literature. One scale that
may assess nonattachment is the Experiences Questionnaire (EQ; Fresco, Moore,
van Dulmen, Segal, Ma, Teasdale, et al., 2007), which measures decentering;
specifically, “the ability to observe one’s thoughts and feelings as temporary,
objective events in the mind, as opposed to reflections of the self that are
necessarily true” (p. 234). Another is the Nonattachment Scale (NAS; Hoffman,
2007), a measure of Buddhist nonattachment across four domains: attachment,
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
35
wisdom, morality, and mindfulness. Both the EQ and NAS are new scales, but
with further validation each may be a useful addition to the complement of
measures available to validate the structure of experiential acceptance.
Distress tolerance is a common construct in the affect dysregulation literature,
and the Distress Tolerance Scale (DTS; Simons & Gaher, 2005) assesses “the
capacity to experience and withstand negative psychological states” (p. 83).
The DTS fits a single factor model in the initial validation study, though it was
developed to address four domains: tolerance of emotional distress, subjective
appraisal of distress, absorption in negative emotion, and attempts at regulation.
The DTS may also be a useful measure in the validation of acceptance.
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Our survey has revealed that the term acceptance has been established across
a wide range of disciplines, and that even within clinical science it has been
conceptualized broadly. Self-acceptance has been considered a component of
psychological health since it was first described a century ago, and this contention
has found support in subsequent empirical investigation. For example, higher
self-acceptance is associated with lower levels of psychopathology, validated with
host of studies using multiple methods measuring a spectrum of psychopathology.
Additionally, self-acceptance correlates positively with measures of adjustment,
well-being, satisfying interpersonal relationships, affect regulation, and other
correlates of mental health. Another well established relation is the positive
correlation between self-acceptance and acceptance of others (note, however, that
being self-accepting does not correlate with being accepted by others). One area of
further study includes examining the divergence of self-acceptance definitions:
some conceptualizations and measurement instruments are closer to
nonjudgmental self-awareness, others to unconditional positive self-regard, and
still others to self-esteem.
Experiential acceptance appears to be negatively related to psychopathology,
and constructs integral to the development and maintenance of psychopathology
(e.g., thought suppression, experiential avoidance, and emotional nonacceptance)
can be defined more generally as nonacceptance. In addition, experiential
acceptance appears to be positively related to emotional regulation, frustration tolerance, well-being, and other indicators of mental health. Accordingly,
acceptance-based interventions have been efficacious in treating a wide range of
problems (e.g., mental disorders, marital discord, pain syndromes), and specific
interventions have been developed for a growing number of conditions and
populations. Treatment packages, such as ACT and DBT, have a substantial
base of efficacy research and often serve as the model upon which the more
targeted interventions are based. Nevertheless, additional trials are necessary
to evaluate the role of exposure as well as placebo and nonspecific effects in
these treatments.
36 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Our review has raised important issues related to the construct validity of
experiential acceptance, which may be of interest as areas for future research.
The first issue relates to content validity. Classical, modern, and contemporary
conceptualizations vary in the inclusion, exclusion, and emphasis of the hypothesized facets we have described (e.g., nonavoidance, nonjudgment, selfacceptance), and acceptance measures differ in the way the construct is operationalized. Relatedly, the structural validity of acceptance also remains to be
investigated. Indeed, no single measure captures acceptance as broadly as it has
been explicated in the literature.
The second issue relates to convergent validity. It is not clear, for example,
whether contemporary conceptualizations of acceptance are distinct from earlier
notions of self and other acceptance. Although they appear to diverge theoretically
(e.g., positive self-regard vs. nonavoidance), this has not been investigated.
Relatedly, there are a growing number of new acceptance measures; however,
the extent to which they converge on a single construct or show incremental
validity with respect to existing measures remains to be determined.
The third question relates to discriminant validity. It is difficult to disambiguate
experiential acceptance from other constructs, particularly mindfulness, although
the structural validity of mindfulness has received the most attention (e.g., Baer,
Smith, & Allen, 2004; Baer, et al., 2006). Thus it is difficult to say, for example,
which facets of acceptance reviewed belong to which construct, or if there may
be a unifying construct present. Moreover, although mindfulness and acceptance
can be cultivated, there may be significant innate differences among people
that vary in terms of temperament and attentional style. The genesis of individual
differences, including why some people are more tolerant of painful emotions
than others, remains largely unexplored. More refined and inclusive measures
of acceptance will help investigators to address the nature, extent, and determinants of individual differences, and assist researchers in addressing the
following questions related to psychotherapy and personal change: Do some
dimensions of acceptance account for more robust changes than other dimensions across diverse psychotherapies? Are some aspects of acceptance (e.g.,
self-acceptance) non-specific ingredients of effective psychotherapy, or do some
dimensions of acceptance produce more specific or perdurable treatment effects?
And, finally, how can we strive for goals while maintaining a present-centered,
accepting mindset?
We have much to learn about the flip-side of acceptance—nonacceptance; in
particular, its relationship with psychopathology through escape from experiences, and the resulting negative effects through ironic processes. An important
question is whether the causal arrow points in the other direction, such that
psychopathology engenders a lack of acceptance of self and others, attempts to
suppress distressing emotions, and a failure to attend to moment-to-moment
experience. In all likelihood, there is a recursive relation between psychopathology and acceptance, although this has yet to be established. The question of the
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
37
relation between nonacceptance and measures of general psychopathology, trait
neuroticism, and depression are also in need of empirical attention.
The paradoxical effects of acceptance are not well understood. Perhaps the
very idea that we can tolerate emotionally painful experiences bolsters hope,
expectations for change and positive outcomes, independent of the effects of
habituation of fears or dysphoria by way of exposure. When we appraise cognitions as merely “thoughts,” we may well curtail the tendency to catastrophize
or distort thinking, and thereby experience relief from anxiety and depression.
Enhanced awareness of maladaptive thought patterns and attachments to
habitual ways of thinking may afford awareness of early indicators of maladaptive responses (Baer, 2003; Linehan, 1993a, 1993b; Marlatt, 1994) and tendencies
to relapse from disorders such as depression (Teasdale et al., 1995), thereby
establishing an “early warning system” (Lynn et al., 2006). Clearly, there is much
room for theory development and models that link different dimensions with
measurable psychological and health-related outcomes, and with well-established
psychological principles (e.g., exposure) of behavior change.
In closing, although there have been substantial advances in the development
of acceptance as a clinical intervention, much work remains to be done with
respect to the construct. The current status of the acceptance literature in fact
suggests that the time is not yet ripe for an integrative model of acceptance. The
acceptance-based treatments that can be subsumed under cognitive-behavioral
theory are quite new, and the terms used to describe acceptance (e.g., nonavoidance, willingness, etc.) have been used rather loosely. Even ACT, which is
arguably the most fully developed acceptance treatment, has only recently begun
to describe how acceptance and mindfulness relate to one another (possibly,
the former is a component process of the latter; Baer et al., 2006). Acceptance
researchers do not appear yet to have gathered enough evidence to support one
factor solution over another. In short, it may be too soon to determine the extent
to which all of the various conceptualizations (e.g., ACT, DBT, MBSR, etc.)
overlap or converge on a single construct.
That said, the systematic study of acceptance can contribute to our understanding of different psychotherapies, the genesis and maintenance of psychological
disorders, and the mechanisms and processes that facilitate and impede personal
change. From its early roots in Buddhist, Hindu, and Taoist writings, to more
contemporary scholarly works from a literary, philosophical, and scientific perspective, acceptance is an idea whose time has come.
REFERENCES
Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Hogan, M. E. (1997). Cognitive/personality subtypes of
depression: Theories in search of disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research. Special
Issue: Cognitive/Personality Subtypes of Depression, 21(3), 247-265.
38 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Adele, M. H., & Feldman, G. (2004). Clarifying the construct of mindfulness in the context
of emotion regulation and the process of change in therapy. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 11(3), 255-262.
Adler, A. (1926). The neurotic constitution. New York: Dodd, Mead.
Afari, N. (1994). The nature of acceptance in Rational-Emotive Therapy. In S. C. Hayes,
N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change:
Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Alcoholics Anonymous World Services. (1967). As Bill sees it. New York: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Amir, N., Coles, M. E., Brigidi, B., & Foa, E. B. (2001). The effect of practice on recall
of emotional information in individuals with generalized social phobia. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 110, 76-82.
Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1979). Self-esteem as a function of masculinity in
both sexes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(4), 783-785.
Antrobus, J. S., & Singer, J. L. (1964). Eye movements accompanying daydreaming, visual
imagery, and thought suppression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69,
244-252.
Bach, P., & Hayes, S. C. (2002). The use of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy to
prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: A randomized controlled trial.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(5), 1129-1139.
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and
empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125-143.
Baer, R. A. (Ed.). (2006). Mindfulness-based treatment approaches: Clinician’s guide to
evidence base and applications. San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Baer, R. A., & Huss, D. B. (2008). Mindfulness- and acceptance-based therapy. Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report:
The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11, 191-206.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using selfreport assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27-45.
Baile, W. F., & Engel, B. T. (1978). A behavioral strategy for promoting treatment compliance following myocardial infarction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 40(5), 413-419.
Baisden, H. E., Lindstrom, V. J., & Hector, J. V. (1982). Self-hate, self-esteem, and
self-acceptance. Rational Living, 17(2), 3-12.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion:
Is the self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
1252-1265.
Beattie, M. (1990). The language of letting go. Center City, MN: Hazelden.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (Eds.). (1996). Cognitive therapy for evaluation anxieties. In C. G. Lindemann (Ed.), Handbook of the treatment of the anxiety
disorders (2nd ed., pp. 235-260). Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson.
Beevers, C. G., Wenzlaff, R. M., Hayes, A. M., & Scott, W. D. (1999). Depression and the
ironic effects of thought suppression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6(2),
133-148.
Berger, E. M. (1952). The relation between expressed acceptance of self and expressed
acceptance of others. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 47, 778-782.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
39
Berger, E. M. (1955). Relationships among acceptance of self, acceptance of others,
and MMPI scores. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 2(4), 279-284.
Berger, E. M. (1961). Willingness to accept limitations and college achievement. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 8, 140-146.
Berger, E. M. (1963). Willingness to accept limitations and college achievement: A
replication. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 10(2), 176-178.
Bills, R. E. (1953). Rorschach characteristics of persons scoring high and low in acceptance
of self. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 17(1), 36-38.
Bills, R. E. (1958). Manual for the Index of Adjustment and Values. Auburn, AL: Alabama
Polytechnic Institute.
Bills, R. E., Vance, E. L., & McLean, O. S. (1951). An Index of Adjustment and Values.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 15, 257-261.
Bishop, S., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N., Carmody, J., et al. (2004).
Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 11, 230-241.
Blackledge, J. T., & Hayes, S. C. (2001). Emotion regulation in acceptance and commitment therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Special Issue: Treating Emotion
Regulation Problems in Psychotherapy, 57(2), 243-255.
Blaise Pascal (1966). Pensees. New York: Penguin Group.
Block, J., & Thomas, H. (1955). Is satisfaction with self a measure of adjustment? Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 254-259.
Block-Lerner, J., Salters-Pedneault, K., & Tull, M. T. (2005). Assessing mindfulness
and experiential acceptance: Attempts to capture inherently elusive phenomena.
New York: Springer Science.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The
importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional
expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15, 482-487.
Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2003). The role of acceptance and job control in mental
health, job satisfaction, and work performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,
1057-1067.
Bond, F. W., & Flaxman, P. E. (2006). The ability of psychological flexibility and job
control to predict learning, job performance, and mental health. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 26(1-2), 113-130.
Boone, S. E., Roessler, R. T., & Cooper, P. G. (1978). Hope and manifest anxiety:
Motivational dynamics of acceptance of disability. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
25(6), 551-556.
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.),
Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 78-108). New
York: Guilford Press.
Brach, T. (2003). Radical acceptance: Embracing your life with the heart of a Buddha.
New York: Random House.
Brandel, I. W. (1982). Puzzling your career: A self-responsibility, self-acceptance
approach to career planning. Personnel & Guidance Journal, 61(4), 225-228.
Brown, J. F. (1936). Psychology and the social order. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Brown, L. (Ed.). (1993). The Oxford shorter English dictionary. New York: Oxford
University Press.
40 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and
its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84(4), 822-848.
Burchinal, L. G., Hawes, G. R., & Gardner, B. (1957). The relationship between parental
acceptance and adjustment of children. Child Development, 28, 65-77.
Callaghan, G. M., Gregg, J. A., Marx, B. P., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Gifford, E. (2004).
FACT: The utility of an integration of functional analytic psychotherapy and
acceptance and commitment therapy to alleviate human suffering. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41(3), 195-207.
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Effects
of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety
and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1251-1263.
Cardaciotto, L. (2005). Assessing mindfulness: The development of a bi-dimensional
measure of awareness and acceptance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Drexel
University.
Cardaciotto, L., & Herbert, J. D. (2005, November). The development of a bi-dimensional
measure of mindfulness: The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS). Paper presented at the 39th Annual Convention of the Association for Behavioral and
Cognitive Therapies,Washington, DC.
Carson, S. H., & Langer, E. J. (2006). Mindfulness and self-acceptance. Journal of
Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 24(1), 29-43.
Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Consider
the brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 92-100.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies:
A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
56(2), 267-283.
Chadwick, P., Hember, M., Mead, S., Lilley, B., & Dagnan, D. (2005). Responding
mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: Reliability and validity of the
Mindfulness Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript.
Chamberlain, J. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (2001). Unconditional self-acceptance and psychological health. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 19(3),
163-176.
Chandler, T. A. (1976). A note on the relationship of internality-externality, selfacceptance, and self-ideal discrepancies. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary
and Applied, 94(1), 145-146.
Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate
self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
44(3), 371-394.
Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional
approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
63(9), 871-890.
Chow, S. L. (1992). Acceptance of a theory: Justification or rhetoric? Journal for the
Theory of Social Behaviour, 22(4), 447-474.
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity.
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591-621.
Colvin, C. A., & Goh, A. (2005). Validation of the technology acceptance model for
police. Journal of Criminal Justice, 33(1), 89-95.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
41
Cordova, J. V. (2001). Acceptance in behavior therapy: Understanding the process of
change. The Behavior Analyst, 24(2), 213-226.
Cordova, J. V., & Kohlenberg, R. J. (1994). Acceptance and the therapeutic relationship. In
S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and
change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Cribb, G., Moulds, M. L., & Carter, S. (2006). Rumination and experiential avoidance
in depression. Behaviour Change, 23(3), 165-176.
Crites, J. O., Bechtoldt, H. P., Goodstein, L. D., & Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. (1961). A factor
analysis of the California Psychological Inventory. Journal of Applied Psychology,
45(6), 408-414.
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281-302.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence. New York: Wiley.
Crowne, D. P., & Stephens, M. W. (1961). Self-acceptance and self-evaluative behavior:
A critique of methodology. Psychological Bulletin, 58, 104-121.
Dahl, J., Wilson, K., & Nilsson, A. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy and
the treatment of persons at risk for long-term disability resulting from stress and
pain symptoms: A preliminary randomized trial. Behavior Therapy, 35, 785-802.
Dahlstrom, W. G. (1992). The growth in acceptance of the MMPI. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 23(5), 345-348.
David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Ellis, A. (Eds.). (2009). Rational and irrational beliefs: Theory,
research, and clinical practice. New York: Oxford Press.
Demyttenaere, K. (1997). Compliance during treatment with antidepressants. Journal
of Affective Disorders, 43(1), 27-39.
Dimidjian, S., Hollon, S. D., Dobson, K. S., Schmaling, K. B., Kohlenberg, R. J., Addis,
M. E., et al. (2006). Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy,
and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with major depression.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(4), 658-670.
Dimidjian, S., & Linehan, M. M. (2003). Defining an agenda for future research on
the clinical application of mindfulness practice. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 10(2), 166-171.
Dobson, K. S., Hollon, S. D., Dimidjian, S., Schmaling, K. B., Kohlenberg, R. J., Gallop,
R. J., et al. (2008). Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy,
and antidepressant medication in the prevention of relapse and recurrence in major
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(3), 468-477.
Doleys, E. J., & Kregarman, J. (1959). Construct validity of the Chicago Q-sort: Frustration
tolerance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, 177-179.
Doss, B. D., & Christensen, A. (2006). Acceptance in romantic relationships: The frequency and acceptability of partner behavior inventory. Psychological Assessment,
18(3), 289-302.
Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (1994). A behavior-analytic account of depression and
a case report using acceptance-based procedures. Behavior Analyst, 17(2), 321-334.
Dryden, W. (1987). Counselling individuals: The rational-emotive approach. London:
Taylor & Francis.
Dryden, W. (1998). Developing self-acceptance: A brief, educational, small group
approach. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
42 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Dryden, W., & Still, A. (2006). Historical aspects of mindfulness and self-acceptance in
psychotherapy. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 24(1), 3-28.
Durm, M. W., & Glaze, P. E. (2001a). Relation of self-acceptance and acceptance of
others. Psychological Reports, 88(2), 410.
Durm, M. W., & Glaze, P. E. (2001b). Construct validity for self-acceptance and fear
of negative evaluation. Psychological Reports, 89(2), 386.
Eagly, A. H., & Whitehead, G. I. (1972). Effect of choice on receptivity to favorable and
unfavorable evaluation of oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
22(2), 223-230.
Eastman, D. (1958). Self acceptance and marital happiness. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 22(2), 95-99.
Ellis, A. (1961). A rational approach to premarital counseling. Psychological Reports, 8,
333-338.
Ellis, A. (1973). Humanistic psychotherapy: The rational-emotive approach. New York:
Julian.
Ellis, A. (1996). How I learned to help clients feel better and get better. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 33(1), 149-151.
Ellis, A. (2000). Can rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT) be effectively used
with people who have devout beliefs in god and religion? Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 31(1), 29-33.
Ellis, A., & Grieger, R. M. (Eds.). (1986). Handbook of rational-emotive therapy. New
York: Springer.
Ellis, A., & Dryden, W. (1997). The practice of rational emotive behavior therapy. New
York: Springer.
Ellis, A., & Robb, H. (1994). Acceptance in rational-emotive therapy. In S. C. Hayes, N. S.
Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content
and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Epictetus (1950). Discourses & fragments. London: Everyman.
Epstein, S., & Feist, G. J. (1988). Relation between self- and other-acceptance and its
moderation by identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2),
309-315.
Evers, A. W. M., Kraaimaat, F. W., van Lankveld, W., Jongen, P. J. H., Jacobs, J. W. G.,
& Bijlsma, J. W. J. (2001). Beyond unfavorable thinking: The illness cognition
questionnaire for chronic diseases. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
69(6), 1026-1036.
Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J. (2007). Mindfulness
and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the cognitive
and affective mindfulness scale-revised (CMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 29(3), 177-190.
Feldner, M. T., Hekmat, H., Zvolensky, M. J., Vowles, K. E., Secrist, Z., & Leen-Feldner,
E. W. (2006). The role of experiential avoidance in acute pain tolerance: A laboratory
test. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37(2), 146-158.
Felton, B. J., Revenson, T. A., & Hinrichsen, G. A. (1984). Stress and coping in the
explanation of psychological adjustment among chronically ill adults. Social Science
& Medicine, 18(10), 889-898.
Ferenczi, S. (1926). The problem of acceptance of unpleasant ideas: Advances in knowledge of the sense of reality. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 7, 312-323.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
43
Fey, W. F. (1954). Acceptance of self and others, and its relation to therapy-readiness.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 10, 269-271.
Fey, W. F. (1955). Acceptance by others and its relation to acceptance of self and others:
A revaluation. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50(2), 274-276.
Fischer-Schreiber, I., Ehrhard, F., & Diener, M. S. (1991). The Shambhala dictionary
of Buddhism and Zen. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Young, M. C. (1984). Agoraphobia: Phenomenological aspects,
associated characteristics, and theoretical considerations. Clinical Psychology Review,
4, 431-457.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus. R. S. (1988). Ways of coping questionnaire manual. Redwood
City, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Follette, V. M. (1994). Survivors of child sexual abuse: Treatment using a contextual
analysis. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.),
Acceptance and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 255-268).
Reno, NV: Context Press.
Fox, J., Knapp, R. R., & Michael, W. B. (1968). Assessment of self-actualization of
psychiatric patients: Validity of the Personal Orientation Inventory. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 28, 565-569.
Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., van Dulmen, M. H. M., Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Teasdale,
J. D., et al. (2007). Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire:
Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 234-246.
Freud, S. (1953). The interpretation of dreams. London: Hogarth.
Freud, S. (1957a). Remembering, repeating and working-through (Further Recommendations on the Technique of Psycho-Analysis II). In J. Strachey & S. Freud (Eds.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud: Vol. 14
(1911-1913): The case of Schreber, papers on technique and other works (pp. 145156). London: Hogarth.
Freud, S. (1957b). On narcissism: An introduction. In J. Strachey & S. Freud (Eds.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud: Vol. 14
(1914): On the history of the psycho-analytic movement, papers on metapsychology,
and other works (pp. 67-104). London: Hogarth.
Freud, S. (1965). Five lectures on psycho-analysis. (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York:
Norton. (Original work published 1910.)
Fromm, E. (1939). Selfishness and self-love. Psychiatry: Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 2, 507-523.
Fruzzetti, A. E., & Iverson, K. M. (2004). Mindfulness, acceptance, validation, and
“individual” psychopathology in couples. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, & M. M.
Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance (pp. 66-95). New York: Guilford.
Fukunishi, I., Koyama, I., & Tombimatsu, H. (1995). Psychological acceptance and
alexithymia in spinal cord injury patients. Psychological Reports, 76(2), 475-481.
Ganter, R. L. (1962). Some relationships between a measure of self-acceptance and
indications of the acceptance of others in a nursing education program. American
Doctoral Dissertations, 23(5), 1605.
Gandhi, M. K. (1999). Vows and observances. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Hills.
Gardner, F. L., & Moore, Z. E. (2004). A mindfulness-acceptance-commitment-based
approach to athletic performance enhancement: Theoretical considerations. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 707-724.
44 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Gaudiano, B. A., & Herbert, J. D. (2006). Acute treatment of inpatients with psychotic
symptoms using acceptance and commitment therapy: Pilot results. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 44(3), 415-437.
Germer, C. K. (2005). Mindfulness. In C. K. Germer, R. D. Siegel, & P. R. Fulton (Eds.),
Mindfulness and psychotherapy (pp. 3-27) New York: Guilford.
Gifford, E. V. (1994). Setting a course for behavior change: The verbal context of
acceptance. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.),
Acceptance and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 218-222). Reno,
NV: Context Press.
Gifford, E. V., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., Antonuccio, D. G., Piasecki, M., Rasmussen-Hall, M. L. et al. (2004). Acceptance-based treatment for smoking cessation. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 669-706.
Goldfarb, A. (1961). Performance under stress in relation to intellectual control and
self-acceptance. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 25(1), 7-12.
Gough, H. G. (1957). California psychological inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Gough, H. G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory: Administrator’s guide. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gough, H. G., & Bradley, P. (1996). California Psychological Inventory manual (3rd
ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Graham, J. R., & Barr, K. G. (1967). Q-sort study of the relationship between students’
self-acceptance and acceptance of their college. Psychological Reports, 21(3), 779-780.
Grant, J. M. (1929). Treatment of alcoholics. Family, 10, 138-143.
Gratz, K. L., Bornovalova, M. A., Delany-Brumsey, A., Nick, B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2007).
A laboratory-based study of the relationship between childhood abuse and experiential
avoidance among inner-city substance users: The role of emotional nonacceptance.
Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 256-268.
Gratz, K. L., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). Preliminary data on acceptance-based emotion
regulation group intervention for deliberate self-harm among women with borderline
personality disorder. Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 25-35.
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation
and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 26(1), 41-54.
Gratz, K. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Tull, M. T., Lejuez, C. W., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006).
An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 850-855.
Greenberg, L. (1994). Acceptance and change: Acceptance in experiential therapy. In S. C.
Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and
change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Greenberg, L., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford.
Greene, R. L., Baucom, D. H., & Macon, R. S. (1980). Students’ acceptance of high
and low generalized personality interpretations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36,
166-170.
Groomes, D. A. G., & Leahy, M. J. (2002). The relationships among the stress appraisal
process, coping disposition, and level of acceptance of disability. Rehabilitation
Counseling Bulletin, 46(1), 14-23.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
45
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, selfreport, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64,
970-986.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting
negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95-103.
Grube, J. W., Kleinhesselink, R. R., & Kearney, K. A. (1982). Male self-acceptance and
attractive toward women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 107-112.
Guidano, V. F., Liotti, G., & Pancheri, P. (1971). Semantic differential analysis of
self-concept, self-acceptance and parental figures in neurotic and normal subjects.
Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 32(4), 298-311.
Hall, G. S. (1918). Morale in war and after. Psychological Bulletin, 15(11), 361-426.
Hayes, S. C. (1994). Content, context, and the types of psychological acceptance. In
S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance
and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context
Press.
Hayes, S. C. (2004a). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and the new behavior
therapies: Mindfulness, acceptance, and relationship. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, &
M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance (pp. 1-29). New York: Guilford.
Hayes, S. C. (2004b). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, relational frame theory,
and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35(4),
639-665.
Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. M. (Eds.). (2004c). Mindfulness and
acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Jacobson, N. S., Follette, V. M., & Dougher, M. J. (Eds.). (1994). Acceptance
and change: Content and context in psychotherapy. Reno, NV: Context Press.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance
and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 44(1), 1-25.
Hayes, S. C., & Strosahl, K. D. (Eds.). (2004). A practical guide to Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy. New York: Springer.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Binting, K., Twohig, M., & Wilson, K. (2004d). What is
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. In S. C. Hayes & K. D. Strosahl (Eds.),
A practical guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (pp. 3-29). New York:
Springer.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment
therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bisset, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al.
(2004a). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model.
The Psychological Record, 54, 553-578.
Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1994). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Altering
the verbal support for experiential avoidance. Behavior Analyst, 17(2), 289-303.
Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (2003). Mindfulness: Method and process. Clinical Psychology:
Science & Practice, 10(2), 161-165.
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Bissett, R., Piasecki, M., Batten, S. V., et al.
(2004b). A preliminary trial of twelve-step facilitation and acceptance and commitment therapy with polysubstance-abusing methadone-maintained opiate addicts.
Behavior Therapy, 35, 667-688.
46 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 1152-1168.
Herbert, J. D., & Cardaciotto, L. (2005). An acceptance and mindfulness-based perspective
on social anxiety disorder. New York: Springer Science.
Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Acceptance and mindfulness-based
therapy: New wave or old hat? Clinical Psychology Review, 28(1), 1-16.
Hoffman, K. T. (2007). A questionnaire development and preliminary construct validation
study of the Buddhist concept of nonattachment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Hollander, E. P. (1960). Competence and conformity in the acceptance of influence.
The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61(3), 365-369.
Holzberg, J. D., Gewirtz, H., & Ebner, E. (1964). Changes in moral judgment and
self-acceptance in college students as a function of companionship with hospitalized
mental patients. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28(4), 299-303.
Horney, K. (1937). The neurotic personality of our time. New York: Norton.
Hudson, J. I., Chase, E. A., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (1998). Eye movement desensitization and
reprocessing in eating disorders: Caution against premature acceptance. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 23(1), 1-5.
Hunter, E. D. (1942). Subjective difficulties incident to the acceptance of psychoanalysis.
Psychiatry: Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 5, 495-498.
Hurley, J. R. (1989). Self-acceptance and other-acceptance scales for small groups.
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 115(4), 483-503.
Hurley, J. R. (1991). Self-acceptance, acceptance of others, and SYMLOG: Equivalent
measures of the two central interpersonal dimensions? Journal of Clinical Psychology,
47(4), 576-582.
Hurley, J. R. (1993). Accepting self, accepting others, and rating peers generously.
Psychological Reports, 73(3, Pt. 1), 768-770.
Hurley, J. R., Feintuch, B., & Mandell, M. J. (1991). Novice leaders’ first three groups:
Change and consistency in acceptance of self and others. Journal of Social Psychology,
131(2), 233-245.
Hurley, J. R., & Rosenberg, D. B. (1990). Group members’ gains in acceptance of self
and others associated with leader’s behavior. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology
Monographs, 116(4), 413-434.
Imber, S. D., Frank, J. D., Gliedman, L. H., Nash, E. H., & Stone, A. R. (1956).
Suggestibility, social class and the acceptance of psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 12, 341-344.
Ishiyama, F. I. (1983). A case of severe test anxiety treated in morita therapy: Acceptance
of anxiety and not fighting it. Canadian Counsellor, 17(4), 172-174.
Ishiyama, F. I. (1987). Use of morita therapy in shyness counseling in the west: Promoting
clients’ self-acceptance and action taking. Journal of Counseling & Development,
65(10), 547-551.
Itatsu, H. (1994). Basic research on the relation between self-acceptance and interpersonal
attitude. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 42(1), 86-94.
Itatsu, H. (1995). The relationship of self-acceptance to life attitude and hopelessness.
Japanese Journal of Counseling Science, 28(1), 37-46.
Ito, M. (1991). The construction of a self-acceptance scale and developmental
changes in adolescence: The developmental process of self-acceptance from the
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
47
viewpoint of two dimensions. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2(2),
70-77.
Jacobson, N. S., & Christensen, A. (1996). Acceptance and change in couple therapy.
New York: Norton.
Jacobson, N. S., Christensen, A., Prince, S. E., Cordova, J., & Eldridge, K. (2000).
Integrative behavioral couple therapy: An acceptance-based, promising new treatment for couple discord. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(2),
351-355.
Jucha, Z., Rendecka, A., & Zuraw, J. (1979). Self-acceptance and its relation to acceptance
of others. Psychologia Wychowawcza, 22(1), 14-38.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain
patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations
and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33-47.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind
to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Dell.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday
life. New York: Hyperion.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and
future. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice, 10(2), 144-156.
Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., & Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use of mindfulness
meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8,
163-190.
Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller, J., & Peterson, L. G. (1992). Effectiveness of a
meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149(7), 936-943.
Kanter, J. W., Baruch, D. E., & Gaynor, S. T. (2006). Acceptance and commitment
therapy and behavioral activation for the treatment of depression: Description and
comparison. Behavior Analyst, 29(2), 161-185.
Karkela, M., Forsythe, J. P., & Kelly, M. M. (2004). Emotional avoidance and
panicogenic responding to a biological challenge procedure. Behavior Therapy, 35,
725-746.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Experiential avoidance
as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion
regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1301-1320.
Kashdan, T. B., & Breen, W. E. (2007). Materialism and diminished well-being: Experiential avoidance as a mediating mechanism. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology,
26(5), 521-539.
Kawagishi, H. (1972). A study of self-acceptance and acceptance of others with accent
on measures of acceptance. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 20(3),
170-178.
Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.
Kelman, H. (1958). Compliance, identification, and internalization: three processes of
attitude change. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1, 51-60.
Kennedy, P. M. (1958). Acceptance of self and acceptance of others as interdependent
variables in interpersonal relations. Oxford, England: Catholic University Press.
Kinkler, R. C., & Myers, R. A. (1963). Some concomitants of self-ideal discrepancy
measures of self-acceptance. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 10(1), 83-86.
48 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Kipling, R. (1994). The collected poems of Rudyard Kipling. Hertfordshire, England:
Wordsworth.
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1995). The altered state of hypnosis: Changes in the theoretical
landscape. American Psychologist, 50, 846-858.
Klavetter, R. E., & Mogar, R. E. (1967). Stability and internal consistency of a measure of
self-actualization. Psychological Reports, 21(2), 422-424.
Knapp, R. R. (1976). Handbook for the Personal Orientation Inventory. San Diego, CA:
Edits Publishers.
Koerner, K., Jacobson, N. S., & Christensen, A. (1994). Emotional acceptance in Integrative Behavioral Couple Therapy. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, &
M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content and context in psychotherapy
(pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Kohlenberg, B. S. (1994). The experiential acquisition of acceptance: Clinical supervision
as a laboratory. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.),
Acceptance and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV:
Context Press.
Kornblum, H., & Anderson, B. (1982). “Acceptance” reassessed: A point of view.
Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 12(3), 171-178.
Kornfield, J. (1993). Teachings of the Buddha. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Kravetz, S., Faust, M., & David, M. (2000). Accepting the mental illness label, perceived
control over the illness, and quality of life. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 23(4),
323-332.
Kristeller, J. L., & Hallett, C. B. (1999). An exploratory study of a meditation-based
intervention for binge eating disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 4(3), 357-363.
Kuhlen, R. G., & Lee, B. J. (1943). Personality characteristics and social acceptability
in adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 34(6), 321-340.
Kus, R. J. (1988). Alcoholism and non-acceptance of gay self: The critical link. Journal
of Homosexuality, 15(1-2), 25-41.
Ladieu, G., Adler, D. L., & Dembo, T. (1948). Studies in adjustment to visible injuries:
Social acceptance of the injured. Journal of Social Issues, 4(4), 55-61.
Langfeld, H. S. (1910). Suppression with negative instruction. Psychological Bulletin, 7,
200-208.
Lao Tzu. (1988). Tao Te Ching (S. Mitchell, Trans.). New York: Harper & Row.
Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., et al. (2006).
The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 62(12), 1445-1467.
Lau, M. A., & McMain, S. F. (2005). Integrating mindfulness meditation with cognitive
and behavioural therapies: The challenge of combining acceptance- and change-based
strategies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/La Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie,
50(13), 863-869.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Leavy, R. L., & Adams, E. M. (1986). Feminism as a correlate of self-esteem, selfacceptance, and social support among lesbians. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
10(4), 321-326.
Lemberg, R. (1993). How I practice: A therapeutic teaching model to foster selfacceptance in the eating disorder patient. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment
& Prevention, 1(2), 171-174.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
49
Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). Effects of acceptance
versus suppression of emotion on subjective and physiological response to carbon
dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 35, 747-766.
Levy, S. S., & Ebbeck, V. (2005). The exercise and self-esteem model in adult women: The
inclusion of physical acceptance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6(5), 571-584.
Linehan, M. M. (1994). Acceptance and change: The central dialectic in psychotherapy.
In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance
and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context
Press.
Linehan, M. M. (1993a). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford.
Linehan, M. M. (1993b). Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford.
Linehan, M. M., Armstrong, H. E., Suarez, A., & Allmon, D. (1991). Cognitivebehavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 48(12), 1060-1064.
Linkowski, D. C. (1971). A scale to measure acceptance of disability. Rehabilitation
Counseling Bulletin, 14(4), 236-244.
Lombardo, J. P., & Berzonsky, M. D. (1975). Locus of reinforcement control, self-image
disparity, and self-acceptance: A replication. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 127(1),
147-148.
Long, V. O. (1986). Relationship of masculinity to self-esteem and self-acceptance in
female professionals, college students, clients, and victims of domestic violence.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(3), 323-327.
Long, V. O. (1991). Masculinity, femininity, and women scientists’ self-esteem and selfacceptance. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 125(3), 263-270.
Long, V. O., & Martinez, E. A. (1994). Masculinity, femininity, and Hispanic professional
women’s self-esteem and self-acceptance. Journal of Counseling & Development,
73(2), 183-186.
Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cognitive
behavior therapy? Clinical Psychology Review, 27(2), 173-187.
Lundh, L. (2004). Perfectionism and acceptance. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive
Behavior Therapy, 22(4), 255-269.
Lynn, S. J., Das, L. S., Hallquist, M. N., & Williams, J. C. (2006). Mindfulness, acceptance,
and hypnosis: Cognitive and clinical perspectives. International Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis, 54 (2), 143-166.
Lynn, S. J., & Fite, R. (1998). Will the false memory debate increase acceptance of the
sociocognitive model of hypnosis? Contemporary Hypnosis, 15(3), 171-174.
MacInnes, D. L. (2006). Self-esteem and self-acceptance: An examination into their
relationship and their effect on psychological health. Journal of Psychiatric and
Mental Health Nursing, 13(5), 483-489.
MacLeod, M. D., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2003). The role of retrieval-induced
forgetting in the construction and distortion of memories. In B. Kokinov & W. Hirst
(Eds.), Constructive memory NBU Series in Cognitive Science. Sophia, Bulgaria:
New Bulgarian University.
Madni, A. M. (1988). The role of human factors in expert systems design and acceptance.
Human Factors Special Issue: Expert Systems, 30(4), 395-414.
50 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Makino, Y., & Tagami, F. (1998). Subjective well-being and self acceptance. Japanese
Journal of Psychology, 69(2), 143-148.
Marcus Aurelius. (trans. 1965). Meditations. London: Dent.
Marlatt, G. A. (1994). Addiction, mindfulness, and acceptance. In S. C. Hayes, N. S.
Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content
and context in psychotherapy (pp. 175-197). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Marlatt, G. A. (2002). Buddhist philosophy and the treatment of addictive behavior.
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 9, 44-49.
Marozas, D. S., & May, D. C. (1980). Factors which motivate job acceptance among
teachers of severely and profoundly mentally retarded children. Education & Training
of the Mentally Retarded, 15(4), 293-297.
Martell, C., Addis, M., & Dimidjian, S. (2004). Finding the action in behavioral activation:
The search for empirically supported interventions and mechanisms of change. In
S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, & M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance
(pp. 152-167). New York: Guilford.
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. Oxford, England: Harpers.
McAllister, W. R., & McAllister, D. E. (1995). Two-factor fear theory: Implications
for understanding anxiety-based clinical phenomena. In W. O’Donohue & L. Krasner
(Eds.), Theories of behavior therapy: Exploring behavior change. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
McCracken, L. M. (1998). Learning to live with pain: Acceptance of pain predicts
adjustment in persons with chronic pain. Pain, 74(1), 21-27.
McCracken, L. M., & Eccleston, C. (2006). A comparison of the relative utility of coping
and acceptance-based measures in a sample of chronic pain sufferers. European
Journal of Pain, 10, 23-29.
McCracken, L. M., Vowles, K. E., & Eccleston, C. (2004). Acceptance of chronic pain:
Component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain, 107(1-2), 159-166.
McCurry, C., & Schmidt, A. (1994). Acceptance and the therapeutic relationship. In
S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance
and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context
Press.
Medinnus, G. R., & Curtis, F. J. (1963). The relation between maternal self-acceptance
and child acceptance. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 27, 542-544.
Megargee, E. I. (1972). The California psychological inventory handbook. San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary
evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1281-1310.
Menzel, H. (1957). Public and private conformity under different conditions of
acceptance in the group. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55(3),
398-402.
Merriam-Webster. (1993). Collegiate dictionary (10th ed.). Springfield, MA: Author
Mitchell, J. V. (1959). Goal-setting behavior as a function of self-acceptance, overand underachievement, and related personality variables. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 50(3), 93-104.
Mitchell, J. V., & Pierce-Jones, J. (1960). A factor analysis of Gough’s California psychological inventory. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24(5), 453-456.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
51
Mowrer, O. H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of “conditioning”
and “problem solving.” Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102-148.
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and adaptive
psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 139-154.
Nichols, R. C., & Schnell, R. R. (1963). Factor scales for the California psychological
inventory. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 27(3), 228-235.
O’Donohue, W., & Krasner, L. (1995). Theories of behavior therapy: Exploring behavior
change. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Ohnmacht, F. W., & Muro, J. J. (1967). Self-acceptance: Some anxiety and cognitive style
relationships. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 67(2), 235-239.
Omwake, K. T. (1954). The relation between acceptance of self and acceptance of
others shown by three personality inventories. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 18,
443-446.
Onions, C. T. (Ed.). (1966). The Oxford dictionary of English etymology. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Orsillo, S. M., & Roemer, L. (Eds.). (2005). Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches
to anxiety: Conceptualization and treatment. New York: Springer.
Osuji, O. N. (1985). Personality factors in acceptance of loss among the physically
disabled. Psychological Record, 35(1), 23-28.
Ottenbreit, N. D., & Dobson, K. S. (2004). Avoidance and depression: The construction
of the Cognitive-Behavioral Avoidance Scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42,
293-313.
Pankey, J., & Hayes, S. C. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy for psychosis.
International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 3(2), 311-328.
Paulhus, D. L. (1988). Self-deception: Where do we stand? In J. S. Lockard & D. L. Paulhus
(Eds.), Self-deception: An adaptive mechanism? (pp. 252-255). Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Peck, M. W. (1929). Negative transference in psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Review, 16,
162-180.
Penley, J. A., Tomaka, J., & Wiebe, J. S. (2002). The association of coping to physical
and psychological health outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 25, 551-603.
Penrose, L. S. (1927). Some psychoanalytical notes on negation. International Journal
of Psycho-Analysis, 8, 47-52.
Peterson, R. F. (1994). Acceptance, experience, and choice. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson,
V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content and context
in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Phillips, E. L. (1951). Attitudes toward the self and others: A brief questionnaire report.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 15, 79-81.
Pilisuk, M. (1963). Anxiety, self-acceptance, and open-mindedness. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 19(4), 387-391.
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1987). Diagnosis and treatment of normal eating. Journal
of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. Special Issue: Eating Disorders, 55, 635-644.
Prabhavananda, S., & Isherwood, C. (1981). How to know God: The yoga aphorisms of
Pantanjali. Hollywood, CA: Vedanta.
Pufal-Struzik, I. (1998). Self-acceptance and behaviour control in creatively gifted
young people. High Ability Studies, 9(2), 197-205.
52 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Pumroy, D. K. (1962). Relationship between the social desirability scale and the California
psychological inventory. Psychological Reports, 10, 795-796.
Purdon, C. (1999). Thought suppression and psychopathology. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 37(11), 1029-1054.
Rank, O. (1945). Will therapy, and truth and reality. New York: Knopf.
Ray, J. J., & Najman, J. (1974). Death anxiety and death acceptance: A preliminary
approach. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 5(4), 311-315.
Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in romantic
relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feelings. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 20, 599-620.
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of emotion suppression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,
1033-1044.
Richter, J., Richter, G., Eisemann, M., & Seering, B. (1995). Depression, perceived
parental rearing and self-acceptance. European Psychiatry, 10(6), 290-296.
Rienzi, B. M., Scrams, D. J., & Uhles, P. (1992). GPA and height are related to selfacceptance scores of female college students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74(2), 354.
Robbins, C. J., Schmidt, H., & Linehan, M. M. (2004). Dialectical Behavior Therapy:
Synthesizing radical acceptance with skillful means. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette,
& M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance (pp. 66-95). New York:
Guilford.
Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2002). Expanding our conceptualization of and treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: Integrating mindfulness/acceptance-based
approaches with existing cognitive-behavioral models. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 9(1), 54-68.
Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2007). An open trial of an acceptance-based behavior
therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 38(1), 72-85.
Rogers, C. R. (1940). The processes of therapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 4(5),
161-164.
Rogers, C. R. (1943). Therapy in guidance clinics. The Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 38(2), 284-289.
Rogers, C. R. (1944a). Psychological adjustments of discharged service personnel. Psychological Bulletin, 41, 689-696.
Rogers, C. R. (1944b). The development of insight in a counseling relationship. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 8(6), 331-341.
Rogers, C. R. (1947). Some observations on the organization of personality. American
Psychologist, 2(9), 358-368.
Rogers, C. R. (1951). A research program in client-centered therapy. Research Publications of the Association for Research in Nervous & Mental Disease, 31, 106-113.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Rogers, C. R. (1995). What understanding and acceptance mean to me. Journal of
Humanistic Psychology, 35(4), 7-22.
Rogers, C. R., & Wallen, J. L. (1946). Counseling with returned servicemen. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Roman, P. M., & Floyd, H. H. (1981). Social acceptance of psychiatric illness and
psychiatric treatment. Social Psychiatry, 16(1), 21-29.
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
53
Rudnick, S. (1982). Communicating acceptance to the psychotic patient. The American
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 42(3), 265-269.
Rubin, I. M. (1967). Increased self-acceptance: A means of reducing prejudice. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 5(2), 233-238.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning
of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
1069-1081.
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 99-104.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 719-727.
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. H. (2006). Best news yet on the six-factor model of well-being.
Social Science Research, 35(4), 1103-1119.
Salters-Pedneault, K., Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2004). The role of avoidance of
emotional material in the anxiety disorders. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 11,
95-114.
Sawazaki, T. (1993). A study of self-acceptance: I. Examination of reliability and
validity of self-acceptance scale in adolescence. Japanese Journal of Counseling
Science, 26(1), 29-37.
Scheel, M. J., Hanson, W. E., & Razzhavaikina, T. I. (2004). The process of recommending homework in psychotherapy: A review of therapist delivery methods, client
acceptability, and factors that affect compliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice, Training, 41(1), 38-55.
Scott, J. (2007). The effect of perfectionism and unconditional self-acceptance on depression. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 25(1), 35-64.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, S., & Teasdale, J. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy
for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Guilford.
Seneca. (1920). Epistles (J. Basore, Trans.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Shapiro, S. L., & Walsh, R. (2003). An analysis of recent meditation research and
suggestions for future directions. Humanistic Psychologist. Special Issue: Transpersonal Psychology, 31, 86-114.
Shawyer, F., Ratcliff, K., Mackinnon, A., Farhall, J., Hayes, S. C., & Copolov, D.
(2007). The voice acceptance and action scale (VAAS): Pilot data. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 63(6), 593-606.
Sheerer, E. T. (1949). An analysis of the relationship between acceptance of and respect
for self and acceptance of and respect for others in ten counseling cases. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 13, 169-175.
Shepard, L. A. (1979). Self-acceptance: The evaluative component of the self-concept
construct. American Educational Research Journal, 16(2), 139-160.
Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York: HarperCollins.
Shostrom, E. (1964). An inventory for the measurement of self-actualization. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 24, 207-218.
Shostrom, E. (1966). Personal orientation inventory manual. San Diego, CA: Educational
and Industrial Testing Service.
Shostrom, E. L. (1973). Comment on a test review: The personal orientation inventory.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 20(5), 479-481.
54 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Simons, J. S., & Gaher, R. M. (2005). The Distress Tolerance Scale: Development and
validation of a self-report measure. Motivation and Emotion, 29(2), 83-102.
Skinner, B. F. (1972). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.
Smart, J. F., & Smart, D. W. (1991). Acceptance of disability and the Mexican American
culture. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 34(4), 357-367.
Snyder, W. U. (1947). The present status of psychotherapeutic counseling. Psychological
Bulletin, 44(4), 297-386.
Solway, K. S., & Fehr, R. C. (1969). Situational variables and self-acceptance. Psychological Reports, 25(1), 253-254.
Springob, H. K., & Struening, E. L. (1964). A factor analysis of the California psychological inventory on a high school population. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
11(2), 173-179.
Starr, P., & Heiserman, K. (1977). Acceptance of disability by teenagers with oral-facial
clefts. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 20(3), 198-201.
Stock, D. (1949). An investigation into the interrelations between the self concept and
feelings directed toward other persons and groups. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
13, 176-180.
Stone, L. A. (1964). A factor analysis of the Berger willingness to accept limitations
scale—A brief. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 11(3), 285.
Strauss, J., Doyle, A. E., & Kreipe, R. E. (1994). The paradoxical effect of diet commercials
on reinhibition of dietary restraint. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 441-444.
Streitfeld, J. W. (1959). Expressed acceptance of self and others by psychotherapists.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23(5), 435-441.
Strosahl, K. D., Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., & Gifford, E. V. (2004). An ACT primer: Core
therapy processes, intervention strategies, and therapist competencies. In S. C. Hayes
& K. D. Strosahl (Eds.), A practical guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
(pp. 31-58). New York: Springer.
Suinn, R. M. (1961). The relationship between self-acceptance and acceptance of others:
A learning theory analysis. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(1),
37-42.
Suinn, R. M., & Geiger, J. (1965). Stress and the stability of self- and other-attitudes.
Journal of General Psychology, 73(1), 177-180.
Suinn, R. M., & Hill, H. (1964). Influence of anxiety on the relationship between self-acceptance and acceptance of others. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28(2), 116-119.
Suzuki, S. (1973). Zen mind, beginner’s mind. New York: John Weatherhill.
Taft, J. (1933). The dynamics of therapy in a controlled relationship. New York:
Macmillan.
Taylor, C., & Combs, A. W. (1952). Self-acceptance and adjustment. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 16(2), 89-91.
Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002).
Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275-287.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy
prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training
help? Behaviour Research & Therapy, 33(1), 25-39.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M.,
& Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by
ACCEPTANCE: AN HISTORICAL AND CONCEPTUAL REVIEW /
55
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology,
68(4), 615-623.
Telford, K., Kralik, D., & Koch, T. (2006). Acceptance and denial: Implications for
people adapting to chronic illness: Literature review. Journal of Advanced Nursing,
55(4), 457-464.
Tenzer, S. (1989). Fat acceptance therapy (F.A.T.): A non-dieting group approach to
physical wellness, insight and self-acceptance. Women & Therapy. Special Issue:
Fat Oppression and Psychotherapy: A Feminist Perspective, 8(3), 39-47.
Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. A. (2008). Mindfulness, self-esteem, and unconditional
self-acceptance. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 26(2),
119-126.
Tidd, C. W. (1937). Increasing reality acceptance by a schizoid personality during
analysis. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 1, 176-183.
Trent, R. D. (1957). The relation between expressed self-acceptance and expressed
attitudes toward Negroes and whites among Negro children. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 91, 25-31.
Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2003). Alternative explanations of emotional numbing
of posttraumatic stress disorder: An examination of hyperarousal and experiential avoidance. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25(3),
147-154.
Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2007). Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the
experience of uncued panic attacks: Evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional
nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 378-391.
Twohig, M., P., & Woods, D. W. (2004). A preliminary investigation of acceptance and
commitment therapy and habit reversal as a treatment for trichotillomania. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 803-821.
Ueda, T. (1996). Self-acceptance as adaptively resigning the self to low self-evaluation.
Japanese Journal of Psychology, 67(4), 327-332.
Vingoe, F. J. (1967). Self-awareness, self-acceptance, and hypnotizability. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 72(5, Pt. 1), 454-456.
Vingoe, F. J. (1968). Note on the validity of the California psychological inventory.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32(6), 725-727.
Waite, B. T., Gansneder, B., & Rotella, R. J. (1990). A sport-specific measure of selfacceptance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 12(3), 264-279.
Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmüller, V., Kleinknecht, N., & Schmidt, S. (2006).
Measuring mindfulness—The Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Personality
and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1543-1555.
Waller, W. (1930). The old love and the new: Divorce and readjustment. Oxford, England:
Liveright.
Walser, R. D., & Westrup, D. (2007). Acceptance & commitment therapy for the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder and trauma-related problems: A practitioner’s
guide to using mindfulness and acceptance strategies. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger
Publications.
Wegner, D. M. (1989). White bears and other unwanted thoughts: Suppression, obsession,
and the psychology of mental control. New York: Guilford.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects
of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13.
56 / WILLIAMS AND LYNN
Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 616-640.
Weiner, E. S. C., & Simpson, J. A. (Eds.). (1991). The compact Oxford English dictionary
(2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Weissman, J., & Appleton, C. (1995). The therapeutic aspects of acceptance. Perspectives
in Psychiatric Care, 31(1), 19-23.
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of
Psychology, 31, 39-91.
Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacognition. New York: Wiley.
Wells, A., & Davies, M. I. (1994). The Thought Control Questionnaire: A measure of
individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 32, 871-878.
William Shakespeare. (1989). Hamlet. London: Routledge.
Williams, J. E. (1962). Acceptance by others and its relationship to acceptance of self
and others: A repeat of Fey’s study. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
65(6), 438-442.
Williams, J. M. G., & Swales, M. (2004). The use of mindfulness-based approaches for
suicidal patients. Archives of Suicide Research, 8, 315-329.
Wilson, G. T. (1996). Acceptance and change in the treatment of eating disorders and
obesity. Behavior Therapy, 27(3), 417-439.
Wilson, H. (1942). Mental reaction to air-raids. Lancet, 242, 284-287.
Wulfert, E. (1994). Acceptance in the treatment of alcoholism. In S. C. Hayes, N. S.
Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content
and context in psychotherapy (pp. 13-32). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Young, P. T. (1924). The phenomenological point of view. Psychological Review, 31,
288-296.
Zuckerman, M., & Monashkin, I. (1957). Self-acceptance and psychopathology. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 21, 145-148.
Zuckerman, M., & Oltean, M. (1959). Some relationships between maternal attitude
factors and authoritarianism, personality needs, psychopathology, and selfacceptance. Child Development, 30, 27-36.
Direct reprint requests to:
John C. Williams, Ph.D.
VA Healthcare System (06/116B)
Long Beach, CA 90822
e-mail: [email protected]