1 Gradual Implementation of Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness
Transcription
1 Gradual Implementation of Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness
Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 1 Gradual Implementation of Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness “We know how to solve the literacy gap between ethnic groups, and it won’t cost billions of dollars…” New York Times 10/23/88 Gradual Implementation of Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness Over a Time Period in a Small School Elizabeth Deshotel University of Bridgeport Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 2 Abstract This study compared the grade level equivalency (GLE) using an orthographic scale with a group of 15 mixed ability (and age) students over two years. The second year group was expected to achieve 2 grade levels of growth. While their results were significant and encouraging, they did not all achieve as much as expected due to several controllable factors. Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 3 The Gradual Implementation of Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness In a Small School Over Time My introduction to a language arts method, The Writing and Spelling Road to Reading and Thinking (WSRRT), came in 1987, while my family and I were living in Louisiana and my husband, Clopha, was working as a lobbyist. As First Lady Barbara Bush had chosen literacy as her primary issue, our First Lady in Louisiana, Patty Roemer, also chose literacy to champion. Clopha became very interested in the literacy issue and through his networking, discovered The Writing Road to Reading (WRRT), by Walter and Ramalda Spalding and WSRRT from The Riggs Institute. He was impressed with the reading, spelling, and writing results that he saw in students and was also impressed with the dedication of the teachers who used this method in their classrooms. My husband was instrumental in initiating a statewide three year pilot of this method, which integrates explicit systematic Orton Phonograms into a then-definedwhole-language framework of writing and reading comprehension. Our children attended a school which had much earlier adopted this method exclusively after their teachers were personally trained by Ramalda Spalding. While in pre-kindergarten, our first daughter Hana learned to read, comprehend, spell, and write clearly. I was not trained in elementary education, so at the time I believed that this was the norm for all children. When Hana moved into kindergarten, she was writing legibly, reading, and spelling at a 3rd grade level. Our second daughter and our son achieved the same results in language arts after receiving The Writing Road to Reading instruction at the same school. Through my husband’s involvement with a range of literacy issues, I realized that our children’s experience with language was not the norm. Many parents and teachers struggled to help children learn the most basic skills in reading, spelling, and writing. I participated in a forty hour teacher training course of The Writing Road to Reading and became interested in the method as an educator. In Louisiana, I tutored my own as well as the children of friends. Upon moving to Connecticut in 1998 to work in a high school, and eventually an elementary school, I was able to integrate some Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 4 components of the program for students with cognitive gaps discovered during assessment of their language development. Part-time responsibilities at the elementary school gradually became full-time when in the 2001/2002 academic year, as Principal, I could train all teachers for a less piecemeal implementation schoolwide. During the 2001/2002 school year I taught Level One of this Orton-based Writing and Spelling Road to Reading and Thinking (WSRRT) in a mixed age classroom of 20 students. In the 2002/2003 academic year, 15 of these 20 students were in my Level Two class. This paper is a longitudinal study of these 15 students over these two academic years. The origins of this method and a description of WSRRT itself will be examined. (Similarities with WRRT are not in the scope of this current study.) Dr. Samuel Orton (1879-1948), a professor of neuropsychiatry and neuropathology at the Neurological Institute of Columbia University, brought together neuropsychiatric information and principles of remediation. As early as 1925, he had identified the syndrome of developmental reading disability, separated it from mental defect and brain damage, and offered a physiological explanation with a favorable prognosis. Dr. Orton endeavored to re-teach brain-damaged veterans of war. These patients had suffered physical trauma to the area of their brains connected to language skills. Since part of the brain was missing, the same memory had to be re-established on the other side of the brain. Dr. Orton asked a number of teachers, including Anna Gillingham and Ramalda Spalding to design a teaching program that could re-establish memory on the reverse side of a damaged brain. A multi-sensory method was developed in which letters representing the single sounds of familiar speech (phonemes) were presented, and synthesized into words that carry meaning; the veterans did re-learn to read (McCulloch, 2000). Research by the Medical Center at Georgetown University confirmed Dr. Orton’s theories that young children learning to read used the left temporal region of the brain, while suppressing the right, visual portion (Turkeltaub, Gareau, Flowers, Zeffiro, & Eden, 2003). Ramalda Spalding observed that learning disabled students, using Orton’s methods, were learning to read better than other students. Dr. Orton asked, “What would Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 5 happen if we simply used this same method for all normal primary children”? Mrs. Spalding found that what worked well for serious physiological brain disorders also worked well for normal children. The work of Dr. Orton was adapted for use in schools by Walter and Ramalda Spalding, hence the publishing and classroom usage of The Writing Road to Reading in 1957 (Wm. Morrow & Co., NYC). The Riggs Institute extended this work into a 4-year curriculum series, The Writing and Spelling Road to Reading and Thinking, in 1999. WSRRT integrates and sequences all of the language arts “strands” including correct spelling (it is easier to organize using the written spelling patterns), reading, composition, grammar, syntax, listening, speaking, and legible handwriting. This method teaches an “explicit” phonics; 55 of the 71 phonograms are taught first without pictures or key words, through direct instruction using multiple (sight, sound, voice, writing) pathways for sound to symbol “mapping” of a student’s functioning (listening and speaking) vocabulary to book print. The teacher engages all students in choral responses and Socratic instruction techniques. Once the students master the beginning strokes for the initial eight phonograms for preventing letter reversals, and legible formation of all the sounds of the first 55 phonograms, they start their personal notebook. This expedites building on their basic functional vocabulary to increasingly complex words. Finally, all 71 phonograms are mastered and creative expression through writing completes this neurolinguistic “journey” into good literature and expressive essay composition. The term “explicit” phonics refers, in this case, to the fact that well-intentioned but distracting information (names of letters, pictures, and key words) is not included in the finely sequenced process of teaching the sounds which are represented on paper by a phonogram (phono=sound + gram=written). The term “multi-sensory” calls attention to the fact that the teachers reach optimal results when they are able to access all, and each, of the four main neurological pathways to the brain – seeing, hearing, speaking, and writing. Socratic instruction is the approach wherein students are led by a series of teacher questions and the whole class benefits from the ongoing dialogue and discovery. Direct instruction entails the interaction of teacher to student and not the intermediary of a worksheet or workbook exercise. Phonemic awareness is the recognition of the sounds Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 6 represented by written and printed letters, and combinations of letters. Graphemic awareness is the correct written spelling patterns that make up the full range of words in English speech; students’ speaking and listening vocabulary is being “mapped” to book print. At the time of my introduction to The Writing Road to Reading in 1987, the “Reading Wars” were going full tilt. It was phonics vs. “whole” language. Phonics and phonics instruction were under heavy fire. The federal government compiled reading research in 1985 in a report entitled “Becoming a Nation of Readers”. This report defined “explicit” phonics for the first time and supported its effectiveness (McCulloch, 2000). In 1995, the California Task Force on Reading concluded that an “explicit” skills program that included phonemic awareness, phonics, and decoding skills is needed by the emergent reader. Since that time, a growing number of research articles available through ERIC highlight the relationship between phonemic awareness and the acquisition of reading skills. The Riggs Institute web site provides a valuable source for research materials which support the effective use of “explicit” phonics in a skills-based whole language curriculum. Ehri’s research spanning twenty years was particularly instructive due to its depth of reviewing reading acquisition (Ehri, 1998). The Report from the National Reading Panel states that phonemic awareness training has caused both reading and spelling improvement (Gerber & Klein, 2004). Students with communication needs demonstrate the same positive association between phonemic awareness and reading as those without a disability (Iacono & Cupples, 2004). Since adopting this method in 1998, our school has progressed from having one WSRRT teacher to having all language teachers implementing this system. We strive to improve every year and thus, this research project evolved. The tracking of achievement of my 15 2001/2002 Level One students was examined and found to be below the expected outcome for some. A comparison was made between the achievement of the Level One group and what the curriculum says is possible if the recommendations are followed closely. These same 15 students were in my Level Two group for the 2002/2003 academic year. With an intention to increase adherence to the recommendations, the Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 7 teaching of the Level Two group in 2002/2003 included those items/strategies left out or incompletely utilized. The following hypothesis was proposed: by correcting the previous year’s “mistakes” and partial implementation, every student would achieve a minimum of two years grade level equivalent (GLE) improvement above their starting GLE assessment. The outcome has practical interest to the school, which is seeking to give an optimal learning environment where students (in a mixed age classroom) are comfortable with collaborative learning, self expression through writing, and the enjoyment of the printed word in its many forms. Method Participants A group of 15 mixed ability (and age) students received daily instruction (for 40 minutes) in WSRRT Level One in the 2001/2002 school year and Level Two in the 2002/2003 school year. Racially, the group was equally divided – Oriental, Caucasian, and African American. Table 1 Initial grade level of 15 students in 2001/2002 and reading level Number of Students: Grade in 01/02: Reading Level: 2 nd At or above level 3 rd At or above level 3 rd Below level 1 4 th At or above level 2 4th 1 5 th At or above grade level 4 5 th Below level 2 6th Below level 3 1 1 Table 2 Below level Number of students reading at or above grade level and below grade level Reading Level: Total Number: Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 8 Table 3 At or above level 6 Below level 9 Of below level students, number who were remedial and those ESL Category: Total Number: Remedial 4 ESL 5 Materials The students were assessed initially to determine their grade level reading ability with the Riggs Orthography Scale. This scale indicates mastery of correct spelling patterns in a student’s spoken vocabulary and correlates with their reading ability. Henceforth, tests were administered regularly to determine each student’s growth. The expectation was that students would perform a minimum of two GLE above their starting point by year’s end. Procedure In the summer of 2002, I critiqued myself as a WSRRT teacher. Out of my desire to foster creativity in my students, I had rebelled against the perceived strictness of the WSRRT method. If there was an aspect that I didn’t agree with, then I’d take a shortcut. For example, I cut out one step of the process known as recoding. I had the opportunity to teach a summer spelling camp, and owing to dissatisfaction with my Level One group’s results, I determined to follow the method strictly. Literally, I felt like a brain surgeon “operating,” teaching WSRRT in July 2002. By the end of the four weeks, I had pushed my students and myself to the limit. The results from that mixed age and ability group were impressive. The minimum achieved during that period was one full grade level of growth. The Riggs Orthography scale was administered on a monthly basis to the students in Level One who went on to Level Two, and the results compared. During the second year I worked hard to implement seven important instructional practices. Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 9 Results Table 4a Monthly GLE Results of Level One and Level Two Groups Riggs Orthographic Scale List B List C List D 2001 4th 20th 28th Sep Sep Nov DB HW KM List E 2002 12th Feb List F List G List H 26th Mar 4th Jun 12th Jun 3.7 3.7 3.9 4.3 4.1 3.7 3.7 3.3 4.1 3.4 4.2 HA YC 3.3 2.2 2.9 2.9 3.5 3.3 3.7 3.9 3.7 3.5 3.3 4.3 BA GJ NY 5.1 3.9 3.7 4.7 3 4.7 4.9 3.9 4.3 6.2 3.4 5.4 5.6 3.7 4.5 5.6 3.9 5.8 6 4.3 4.3 3 4.5 6.2 3.9 5.1 3.7 4.7 6.4 5.1 4.7 6 4.2 5.8 4.2 6 BF DB GC HM YY 6 5.8 6.2 3.9 2.9 2.9 3.4 3 2.9 3.3 3.9 3.7 6.4 4.5 4.2 3.3 5.1 VJ NT 3.5 5.4 4.3 5.2 4.2 6.4 4.3 6 4.5 Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 10 Table 4b Riggs Orthographic Scale List A List B List C 2002 4th 13th Sep 8th Oct Nov List D 20th Dec List E 2003 5th Feb List F List G List H List I 25th Mar 28th Apr M ay 29 9th Jun DB HW KM 4.1 3.7 3.9 4.9 4.5 4.3 5.1 4.5 5.1 4.5 4.7 4.2 4.3 3.7 4.5 6.2 4.9 5.2 5.1 4.5 5.1 5.1 5.1 6.2 4.9 5.1 HA YC 3.7 3.5 4.9 4.7 3.9 4.7 3.9 4.9 3.7 4.7 5.2 5.1 5.1 4.2 5.4 5.2 6.2 BA GJ NY 5.1 3.4 5.8 6.8 4.3 7.3 6.4 4.7 6.4 6.4 4.1 5.8 6.6 4.5 7.3 7.7 4.2 7.5 7 3.7 6.8 7.7 5.1 7 6.8 4.3 8 BF DB GC HM YY 5.6 5.1 4.7 3.5 4.9 7.3 5.8 5.2 3.7 6.2 6.8 4.2 5.1 3.4 5.8 6.6 4.7 5.1 3.3 5.8 5.1 4.3 3.9 7 6.4 4.9 4.7 7.5 7.3 6.4 5.2 3.3 5.6 5.6 5.1 4.2 6.6 7 6.2 5.2 4.2 7.3 VJ NT 4.1 6.8 5.4 8.8 4.9 8 4.9 8 5.6 7 4.9 7.5 5.4 7.3 4.9 8.8 7.5 Of the 15 students, 13 had bigger gains in the second year. In the first year the range of growth was -.6 > +2.2 GLE while in year two, the range of growth was +.5 > +2.7 GLE. See appendix for individual analysis of students. Table 5 Amount of GLE growth from first to second year Level One Growth: Level Two Growth: Negative to zero 4 students 0 students .1 to .9 8 students 4 students 1.0 to 1.9 0 students 6 students Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 11 2.0 to 2.9 3 students 5 students The largest, most consistent gains were made by the ESL students. In second place were those who were already reading at or above grade level. These two groups were extremely diligent in completing daily homework assignments. In third place were the remedial students. Of the 4 remedial students, all 4 were negligent in completing homework. (Appendix B) Table 6 Comparing GLE growth of three groups of students ESL: At or above: Remedial: Level I Level II Level I Level II Level I Level II +2.1 +2.7 +.4 +2.1 0 +.9 +2.1 +2.2 -.3 +1.2 +.6 +.5 -.6 +1.1 +.3 +1.2 +.8 +.7 +2.2 +2.4 0 +1.5 +.7 +.8 +.6 +2.0 +.4 +1.4 +.5 +1.7 Although not as great as predicted, the Level two group showed significant growth over Level One. In the ESL students, in the second year, the smallest GLE growth was 1.1 and the highest 2.7. For the students who were at or above grade level, the smallest GLE growth was 1.4 and the largest 2.1. In the remedial group, the smallest growth was .5 and the largest .9. A self-evaluation compared implementation of instructional practices. Table 7 Comparison of Use of Methods of Instruction 2001/2002 2002/2003 First 55 Phonograms; Yes Yes Notebook-Dictation Recoding; No Yes 56-71 Phonograms; Yes Yes Charts Dictation; No 50% Require Choral Response; 50% 75% Methods of Instruction: Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 12 Multi-Sensory Demand; 50% 75% Parent Pull-Out Pages; No No In terms of methodology, the biggest area of difference between the two years is in the area of “recoding.” Decoding and encoding are familiar terms to language teachers, however “recoding” is not. Recoding is reading individual speech sounds, or a series of sounds depicted by one grapheme, from their written printed form (grapheme). Incorporating recoding conscientiously into the daily work was an important factor in the student’s development. Two methodologies that were weak in the first year and then increased in the second year were choral response and requiring the students to use the multi-sensory approach. Discussion Working with the program and seeing growth in the students was very fulfilling. The growth of the ESL students and students who already functioned well in language was above average. However, the growth of our remedial students needed more attention. One factor affecting their outcome, besides the methodologies, was the remedial students’ negligent attitude toward homework. (See appendix.) Use of the Parent Pull Out Pages could help bring more parental involvement with the child’s growth. In addition, development of the Reading Buddies program could be helpful, fostering peer and crossage tutoring. We will continue to work towards 100% implementation of the program as we head for the 2004/2005 school year. Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 13 References Ehri, L. (1998). Research on Learning to Read and Spell: A Personal-Historical Perspective. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 2, 97-114. Gerber, A., & Klein, E. (2004). A Speech-Language Approach to Early Reading Success. Teaching Exceptional Children, 36, 6, 8-15. Iacocno, T., & Cupples, L. (2004). Assessment of Phonemic Awareness and Word Reading Skills of People with Complex Communication Needs. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 2, 437-450. McCulloch, M. The Writing and Spelling Road to Reading and Thinking. Beaverton: K & M Publishing, 2000. Turkeltaub, P., Gareau, L., Flowers, D., Zeffiro, T., & Eden, G. (2003). Development of Neural Mechanisms for Reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 767-773. Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 14 Appendix Detailed Individual Analysis of 15 Students 1. DB - 2nd grade girl in 9/01 - previously instructed in WSRRT - reading at grade level - joined group in 2/02 - growth of .4 in Level 1 - growth of 2.1 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 2. HW - 2nd grade boy in 9/01 - previously instructed in WSRRT - reading above grade level - joined group in 2/02 - regression of .3 in Level 1 - growth of 1.2 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 3. KM - 2nd grade girl in 9/01 - previously instructed in WSRRT - reading above grade level - joined group in 2/02 - growth of .3 in Level 1 - growth of 1.2 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 4. HA - 3rd grade boy in 9/01 - previously instructed in WSRRT - reading at grade level - zero growth in Level 1 - growth of 1.5 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 5. YC - 3rd grade boy in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - ESL student / English not spoken at home - reading 1 grade level below - growth of 2.1 in Level 1 - growth of 2.7 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 6. BA - 4th grade girl in 9/01 - previous WSRRT instruction - reading above grade level - growth of .5 in Level - growth of 1.7 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 7. GJ - 4th grade boy in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - reading 1 grade level below - zero growth in Level 1 Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 15 8. NY - growth of .9 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework - 4th grade girl in 9/01 - previously instructed in WSRRT - ESL student / English not spoken at home - reading 1 grade level below - growth of 2.1 in Level 1 - growth of 2.2 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 9. BF - 5th grade girl in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - reading at grade level - growth of .4 in Level 1 - growth of 1.4 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework 10. DB - 5th grade boy in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - enrolled in school in 2/02 - ESL student / English spoken at home - reading 1 grade level below - regression of .6 in Level 1 - growth of 1.1 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework 11. GC - 5th grade boy in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - reading more than 1 grade level below - growth of .6 in Level 1 - growth of .5 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework 12. HM - 5th grade boy in 9/01 - previous WSRRT instruction - reading more than 1 grade level below - growth of .8 in Level 1 - growth of .7 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework 13. YY - 5th grade girl in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - ESL student / English not spoken at home - reading more than 2 levels below - growth of 2.2 in Level 1 - growth of 2.4 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework 14. VJ - 6th grade boy in 9/01 - no previous WSRRT instruction - reading more than 2 grade levels below - growth of .7 in Level 1 - growth of .8 in Level 2 - negligent in completing homework Graphemic with Phonemic Awareness 16 15. NT - 6th grade girl in 9/01 - previous WSRRT instruction - ESL student / English not spoken at home - reading 1 grade level below - growth of .6 in Level 1 - growth of 2.0 in Level 2 - diligent in completing homework