UNIVERSITÄT DER BUNDESWEHR MÜNCHEN Fakultät für
Transcription
UNIVERSITÄT DER BUNDESWEHR MÜNCHEN Fakultät für
UNIVERSITÄT DER BUNDESWEHR MÜNCHEN Fakultät für Elektrotechnik und Informationstechnik Basic Timing Concepts for the Execution of Multiple Motor Tasks: Coordination of periodic tapping with discrete tasks Cong Khac Dung Vollständiger Abdruck der von der Fakultät für Elektrotechnik und Informationstechnik der Universität der Bundeswehr München zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Ingenieurswissenschaften (Dr.-Ing.) genehmigten Dissertation. Vorsitzender des Promotionsausschusses: Univ.-Prof. Dr.-Ing. Gerhard Bauch 1. Berichter: Univ.-Prof. Dr.techn. Christian Kargel 2. Berichter: Prof. Dr. –Ing.habil. Werner Wolf Die Dissertation wurde am 06.12.2011 bei der Universität der Bundeswehr München eingereicht und durch die Fakultät für Elektronik und Informationstechnik am 29.02.2012 angenommen. Die mündliche Prüfung fand am 20.08.2012 statt. 1 Abstract................................................................................................................................... 6 Kurzfassung ............................................................................................................................ 8 1 Introduction ..................................................................................................................... 11 1.1 Multi-tasking: sharing a single execution unit or coordinating multiple units .................... 11 1.2 Motivation for this dissertation ........................................................................................ 12 1.3 Motor Coordination – Task Scheduling and Timing......................................................... 13 1.3.1 Single-Task (ST) Condition ............................................................................................ 13 1.3.2 Dual-Task (DT) condition .............................................................................................. 17 2 Literature review on finger tapping ................................................................................ 23 2.1 Overview ........................................................................................................................ 23 2.1.1 Literature on Clinical research ........................................................................................ 23 2.1.2 Psychological and physiological research........................................................................ 23 2.2 Literatur on clinical research ........................................................................................... 26 2.3 Literature on psychological and physiological research .................................................... 27 2.3.1 ST condition .................................................................................................................. 27 2.3.2 DT condition .................................................................................................................. 41 2.4 Summary ........................................................................................................................ 52 3 Literature review on eye blinks ....................................................................................... 55 3.1 Fluctuation of blink number during an interval (Greene 1986) ......................................... 56 3.2 Patterns of Blink Rate in Normal Subjects (Bentivoglio et al. 1997)................................. 57 3.3 Stochastic models for spontaneous blink(Hoshino 1996).................................................. 58 3.4 Model for audiomotor integration (Bangert et al. 2006) ................................................... 59 3.5 Effect of mental task on eye blink rate (Karson et al. 1981) ............................................. 63 3.6 Mapping cortical areas with functional MRI (Tsubota el al. 1999) ................................... 63 3.7 The neural representation of temporal information (Ivry & Spencer 2004) ....................... 68 3.8 How brain activity correlates with temporal complexity (Lewis et al. 2004) ..................... 73 3.9 The role of supplementary motor area in moving preparation (Jenskin et al. 2000) ........... 74 3.10 Investigation of the daily pattern of eye-blink rate (Barbato et al. 2000) ........................... 77 3.11 A brain Stem Reflex in Eye Blink (Evinger 1995) ........................................................... 79 3.12 The Blink Recovery Process in Patients with bell’s Palsy (VanderWerf et al. 2007) ......... 82 3.13 Summary ........................................................................................................................ 88 4 Literature review on saccades and timing processes ...................................................... 90 4.1 Visual saccade and memory processes (Claeys et al. 1999) .............................................. 90 4.2 Saccade with concurrent auditory task (Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983)...................... 92 4.3 Saccadic eye movement and manual control system (Megaw & Armstrong (1973)........... 94 4.4 Cognitive load on saccadic eye movements (Stuyven et al. 2000) .................................... 98 4.5 Human head-eye coordination during tapping task (Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001) 101 2 4.6 Distributed neural systems underlying the timekeeping (Rao et al. 1997) ....................... 103 4.7 Gaze effects on movement activation patterns (Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) .......... 107 4.8 Models for saccade generation circuitry (Girard & Berthoz 2005) .................................. 110 4.8.1 Reticular formation saccadic burst generators ............................................................... 110 4.8.2 Superior colliculus ....................................................................................................... 111 4.8.3 Cerebellum .................................................................................................................. 111 4.8.4 Basal Ganglia ............................................................................................................... 111 4.8.5 Premotor cortical areas ................................................................................................. 111 4.9 Gaze and Hand Position Effects on Brain Activation (Bedard & Sanes 2009) ................. 112 4.10 Coordination of ocular and hand motor systems (Bekkering et al. 1994) ........................ 112 4.11 Theoordination of saccadic and manual movements (Binsted & Elliot 1999).................. 113 4.12 Eye Position Effects on neuronal Activity (Boussaoud, Jouffrais, & Bremmer 1998)...... 115 4.13 The main function of the cerebellum and basal ganglia (Dreher & Grafman 2002) ........ 116 4.14 Central Bottleneck of Information processing with fMRI (Dux et al. 2006) .................... 117 4.15 The organization of eye and limb movements during reaching (Fisk & Goodable 1985) . 118 4.16 Modality pairing effects on dual-task (Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006) ................................ 120 4.17 Summary ...................................................................................................................... 121 5 Motor coordination framework: which gaps are addressed by this study ................... 123 5.1 The form of continuous movement trajectories .............................................................. 123 5.2 The movement order ..................................................................................................... 123 5.3 The effects of feedback ................................................................................................. 123 5.4 The effects of amplitude (force) .................................................................................... 124 5.5 The effects of multiple effectors .................................................................................... 124 5.6 The effects of attention ................................................................................................. 124 5.7 Single-task conditions ................................................................................................... 125 5.7.1 Basic considerations ..................................................................................................... 125 5.7.2 The effects of feedback ................................................................................................ 126 5.7.3 The effects of amplitude (force) .................................................................................... 126 5.7.4 The effects of multiple effectors ................................................................................... 127 5.7.5 The effects of attention ................................................................................................. 127 5.8 Dual-task conditions ..................................................................................................... 127 5.8.1 Basic considerations ..................................................................................................... 127 5.8.2 The effects of feedback ................................................................................................ 128 5.8.3 The effects of amplitude (force) .................................................................................... 129 5.8.4 The effects of attention ................................................................................................. 129 5.8.5 Eye-Hand movement coordination without common spatial target ................................. 129 5.8.6 Multiple effectors ......................................................................................................... 130 6 Methods – Experimental Concepts, Recorded signals .................................................. 132 3 6.1 Materials and method.................................................................................................... 132 6.1.1 Subjects and experimental setup ................................................................................... 132 6.1.2 Experimental setup ....................................................................................................... 132 6.2 Procedures and experimental paradigms ........................................................................ 136 6.2.1 The Synchronization-Continuation paradigm ................................................................ 136 6.2.2 Single-task conditions .................................................................................................. 137 6.2.3 Dual-task conditions..................................................................................................... 140 6.3 Signal analysis .............................................................................................................. 141 6.3.1 Event detection ............................................................................................................ 142 6.3.2 The time course analysis of signals ............................................................................... 142 6.4 Phase Resetting Curve (PRC) ........................................................................................ 147 6.4.1 General idea ................................................................................................................. 147 6.4.2 Phase Resetting Curve construction .............................................................................. 147 6.5 Periodic-discrete process Interaction Categories ............................................................ 149 6.5.1 Classification based on discrete events.......................................................................... 149 6.5.2 Classification based on continuous trajectories of fingers .............................................. 151 6.6 Statistical data analysis ................................................................................................. 156 7 Results............................................................................................................................ 157 7.1 Single-Task (ST) condition ........................................................................................... 157 7.1.1 Periodic tapping ST ...................................................................................................... 157 7.1.2 Periodic tapping and spontaneous eye blinks ................................................................. 162 7.2 Dual-Task (DT) condition ............................................................................................. 169 7.2.1 Basic interaction patterns and their PRCs ...................................................................... 170 7.2.2 Dominant tapping behaviour and discrete-tap timing characteristics in normal tapping .. 174 7.2.3 Time course analysis of the tapping process .................................................................. 179 7.2.4 Effects of physiological parameters .............................................................................. 188 7.3 Hand-Foot condition ..................................................................................................... 192 7.4 OM-DT condition ......................................................................................................... 198 8 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 202 8.1 ST condition ................................................................................................................. 202 8.1.1 Timing task .................................................................................................................. 202 8.1.2 Timing task and spontaneous blinking .......................................................................... 202 8.2 DT condition ................................................................................................................ 204 8.2.1 BM .............................................................................................................................. 204 8.2.2 OM-SM condition ........................................................................................................ 209 8.2.3 Multiple effectors ......................................................................................................... 211 9 Summary and outlook ................................................................................................... 213 9.1 Summary ...................................................................................................................... 213 4 9.2 Outlook ........................................................................................................................ 215 9.2.1 Effect of force, attention and external feedback ............................................................. 215 9.2.2 Mental task instead of motor task ................................................................................. 216 9.2.3 Checking memory limit of time interval........................................................................ 216 9.2.4 Audiomotor overlearned in musical trained people........................................................ 216 References ........................................................................................................................... 218 10 Appendix........................................................................................................................ 239 10.1 Classification of the interactions based on discrete events .............................................. 239 10.2 Software ....................................................................................................................... 242 10.3 Showdata ...................................................................................................................... 244 10.3.1 Variable .................................................................................................................. 244 10.3.2 Menu ...................................................................................................................... 246 10.3.3 Control buttons........................................................................................................ 254 10.4 AnalyzeTapResults ....................................................................................................... 254 10.4.1 Variable .................................................................................................................. 254 10.4.2 Menu: ..................................................................................................................... 257 10.5 Interaction classification in DT-experiment ................................................................... 270 5 Abstract Making fast decision such as football scoring or musical rhythm modulation requires timing accuracy. Wing & Kristofferson (1973b) reported in the negative correlation between successive intertap intervals of repetitive discrete motor responses based on the assumed two independent processes (timekeeper and motor delay). Helmut & Ivry (1996) reported the better performance of the repetitive tapping task when individuals tapped with two hands in comparison to single-handed tapping. A pilot experiment replicated this topic but with repetitive mental task and different peripheral motor implementations. Numerous daily activities require timing and performing more than one task simultaneously. Multi-tasking necessitates motor coordination. A challenging behavioral requirement especially in multitasking is to maintain both spatial and temporal accuracy of all motor actions given in response to an emergency, where possible resource bottlenecks may become apparent. Laboratory investigations on this topic often use dual-task experiments, e.g. bimanual tapping (BM, i.e. hitting a key or a surface by a finger tip) with different instructions for the right and left hand, respectively. A conventional experimental setup for tapping data measurement consists only of the ground contact sensors like micro switches for the motor action observation; the evaluation of the discrete events provided by these switches is quite simple, but also the amount of obtained information is limited. A novel experimental design for tapping experiments with high-resolution recording of the complete time course of the continuous finger movements was approached and the required evaluation procedures for the biomechanical and EMG data is described. The latter are based on sophisticated maximum-likelihood-techniques, which again is an example of progress in research through advanced bio-signal processing. The finger tapping task as designed by Stevens (1886) was used. The experimental paradigm consists of synchronization and continuation phase. Tapping included normal tapping, contact-free tapping, and isometric tapping for both single-task (ST) and dual-task (DT) conditions. Furthermore voice tapping and mental tapping in combination with normal tapping in ST condition were approached. ST covers the control experiment as reference and experiments studying timing of periodic movement. Finger positions and ground contact forces was recorded. The DT was performed on different limbs. The coordination of periodic right hand tapping with single stimulus evoked discrete left hand taps was investigated to check for task interactions and a possible relationship between “phase resetting” (tapping literature, e.g. J. Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki, 1979) and “phase entrainment” (tremor literature, e.g. R.J. Elble, Higgins, & Hughes 1994). In ST only the results of voice tapping consistently confirmed the proposed model of Wing & Kristofferson (1973). The correlation biased to zero or even positive in isometric tapping and sometime in contact-free and normal tapping. The bimanual advantage in repetitive tapping performance was observed in isometric and in combined mental-normal tapping whereas disadvantage was observed in normal tapping and sometimes in contact-free tapping. The results proved that the different motor implementations leading to different motor delay and different feedback in closed-loop control contributed differentially to the correlation function in successive intertap intervals. The integration of central commands already occurs at high level in brain in case of combined mental/normal tapping. Additional to correction process in timing based on feedback a second correction process based on asynchrony between both fingers exists and caused the absence of bimanual advantage sometime in contact-free tapping and more effective in normal tapping. Four different types of coordination schemes were observed in DT tapping behavior: Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI), Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR), Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH) and Discrete 6 Tap Entrainment (DTE); MTI and PTR correspond to the phase resetting effect as described earlier in tapping for the coordination of periodic tapping with single discrete taps. (DTE) reflected the impact of the periodic tapping on the discrete tap and PTH of the discrete tap on the periodic tapping both leading to a synchronized execution of the two concurrent tapping tasks are the observed novel aspects. All subjects showed a dominant tapping behavior but also other non-dominant forms of the four reported coordination schemes in some trials. Even MTI presents marginal interaction, continuous trajectory revealed hidden mutual interaction such as mutual modification of tap duration and slope duration, tap embedding, varied force or amplitude, tap delay and tap cancelling although rhythm is stable. The results reflect possible constraints of the sensorimotor system in handling two competing tasks. From the point of view of the theory of oscillations the dominant effect of the discrete movement is considered as a quasi-elastic force attracting the periodic movement as a system to the position of equilibriums and proportional to the displacement of system from these equilibriums or repelling it away from equilibriums. These two equilibriums are reflected in PRC (Phase Resetting Curve). Against this repulsive force damping force and restoring force are provided when oscillation grows too large or becomes too small respectively. This point of view was verified by parameters such as specific instruction on task, required force, multiple effectors, Feedback in closed-loop control, normal vs. contact-free movement. To determine whether interaction is specific for hand-hand interaction only, extending of limb homology (upper and lower) and limb laterality was applied for discrete response, i. e foot and handfoot combinations was done. In all conditions the same tapping behaviors were observed. The results proved that the mechanism responsible for the observed interaction effect is not effector specific. The location of the shared motor control for upper and lower limbs within the higher brain levels is suggested. The interaction of periodic self-paced finger tapping with concurrently executed saccades also was addressed. Because the both movements share some known common neural control pathways. Resource bottlenecks may become apparent in maintaining both spatial and temporal accuracy of concurrent motor actions. Instead of the discrete left hand response, the participants now executed a single saccadic eye movement to a fixed visual target in parallel to continuous periodic tapping of the dominant hand. We expected these reactive saccades to act as a strong perturbation event to the continuous tapping, but the experimental data did not reveal a considerable interference in this specific oculo-manual (OM) DT experiment. I.e. sharing neural pathways reported in many experiments for eye and hand movements does not always cause DT costs. Not only the mutual cross-talk between voluntary movements but also between spontaneous eye blinks and continuous, self-paced unimanual and bimanual tapping was studied. Both types of motor activities were analyzed with regard to their time-structure in synchronization-continuation tapping tasks which involved different task instructions, namely "standard" finger tapping, "strong" tapping requiring more forceful finger movements, and "impulse-like" tapping where upwarddownward finger movements had to be very fast. In a further control condition, tapping was omitted altogether. The manual tapping revealed a prominent entrainment on the blink behavior. Bimanual tapping was more effective than unimanual tapping, “strong” and “impulse-like” tapping showed the largest effects. Conversely, no significant effects of the eye blinks on the timing of periodic tapping across all experiments were found. The functional control structures of finger and eye blinking movement might contains some intersections. 7 Keywords: Spontaneous Endogenous Blinks, Unimanual and Bimanual Finger Tapping, Index Finger Tapping, Saccade, Dual Task, Interference, motor system, biosignal processing, motor interaction, interlimb coordination, reaction time. Kurzfassung Schnelle bzw. spontane Entscheidungen zu treffen wie der Torschuss im Fußball oder wie die plötzliche Rhythmusveränderung während einer musikalischen Ausführung erfordern eine hohe zeitliche Genauigkeit bei der Ausführung. Unter der Annahme von zwei unabhängigen Prozessen (Zeitgeber und motorische Verzögerung) berichteten Wing & Kristofferson (1973) eine negative Korrelation zwischen sukzessiven Intervallen bei repetitiven diskreten motorischen Aktionen, was die Interpretation einer permanenten Korrektur des zeitlichen Ablaufs zulässt. Helmut & Ivry (1996) berichteten weiterhin, dass eine zweihändige Ausführung im Tapping-Experiment mit einer geringeren Varianz der Folgeintervalle ausgeführt wird als eine als einhändige. Dieses Thema wurde in einem Pilotexperiment in dieser Arbeit wieder aufgegriffen, wobei ein sog. mentales Tapping und unterschiedliche Implementierungen der motorischen Aktion (z.B. Fuß) eingeschlossen wurden. Auch zahlreiche tägliche Aktivitäten erfordern Koordination des zeitlichen Ablaufs und die gleichzeitige Durchführung von mehr als einer Aufgabe, also das sog. Multitasking. Motorische Koordination ist offensichtlich notwendig für Multitasking. Eine spezielle Herausforderung beim Multitasking ist, die räumliche und zeitliche Genauigkeit der motorischen Bewegungskoordination im Falle einer Notfallsituation einzuhalten; dabei werden oft Engpässe bei der Verfügbarkeit notwendiger Ressourcen erkennbar. Viele Laboruntersuchungen verwenden „dual-task“ Experimente, z.B. bimanuelles Tapping (d.h. Aufschlagen eines Fingers auf eine Oberfläche oder einer Taste) mit unterschiedlichen Instruktionen für den linken und den rechten Finger. Der konventionelle experimentelle Aufbau für die Datenaufnahme von FingertappingExperimenten besteht nur aus einem digitalen Berührungssensor wie z.B. einem Mikroschalter, der die motorische Aktivität registriert. Die Evaluierung dieser vom Mikroschalter erzeugten diskreten Ereignisse ist einfach, aber die gewonnen Informationen sind beschränkt. In dieser Arbeit wird ein neues experimentelles Design mit hochauflösender Aufnahme der kontinuierlichen Fingerbewegung eingesetzt und die dafür benötigte Prozedur zur Evaluierung von biomechanischen Daten beschrieben. Diese Evaluierung basiert auf einer aufwendigen „Maximum-Likelihood“ Technik, und ist ein Beispiel für den Fortschritt in der Biosignalverarbeitung. Grundsätzlich wurde das experimentelle Design des Tappings von Stevens (1886) verwendet. Das experimentelle Paradigma besteht aus der Synchronisation- und der Fortführung-Phase. Die experimentellen Bedingungen beinhalten normales, kontaktfreies und isometrisches Fingertapping für „single-task“ (ST) und „dual-task“ (DT) Aufgaben. Außerdem wurden sie auf sprachliches und mentales Tapping in Kombination mit normalem Fingertapping für ST erweitert. ST deckt die Kontrollversuche als Referenzen für DT als auch unabhängige Experimente zur Untersuchung der Zeitsteuerung ab. DT wurde mit verschiedenen Extremitäten (Finger, Fuß) ausgeführt. Taskinteraktion und die Relation zwischen „Phase Resetting“ (Yamanishi, M. Kawato, & R. Suzuki, 1979) und „Phase Entrainment“ (tremor literature, e.g. R.J. Elble, C. Higgins & L. Hughes, 1994) wurden durch die Koordination zwischen dominanten Finger und nicht-dominanten Finger untersucht, wobei dem dominanten Finger das periodische Tapping und dem nicht-dominanten die diskrete durch Stimulus ausgelöste Tap-Bewegung zugewiesen sind. Bei den ST Experimenten bestätigten nur die Daten mit sprachlichem Tapping konsistent das Kristofferson & Wing Modell (1973b). Die Korrelationsfunktion der sukzessiven Intervalle hat sich zu 8 Null oder sogar in den positiven Bereich bei den isometrischen und manchmal auch bei den kontaktfreien und normalen Konditionen geneigt. Der bekannte bimanuelle Vorteil beim repetitiven Fingertapping-Experiment wurde bei der isometrischen Bedingung und in der „normal-mental“ Kombination gefunden. Dagegen war beim normalen Tapping und teilweise auch im kontaktfreien Tapping dieser bimanuelle Vorteil nicht zu sehen. Das Resultat lässt darauf schließen, dass die unterschiedlichen motorischen Implementierungen des Tappings zu unterschiedlichen motorischen Verzögerungen führen sowie auch unterschiedliche sensorische Informationen (z.B. taktile Empfindung an der Fingerkuppe) über die ausgeführte motorische Aktion liefern, was im geschlossenen sensomotorischen Regelkreis sich auswirkt und zu unterschiedlichen Korrelationsfunktionen der sukzessiven Tapping-Intervalle beitragen kann. Eine Integration auf der höchsten Ebene der motorischen Kontrolle lässt sich aus Ergebnissen der „normal-mental“ Kombination zu vermuten. Die schlechtere Leistung in normalen und kontakt-freien Ausführungen kann dahingehend interpretiert werden, dass bei dem Kontroll- bzw. Korrekturprozess des Tappings zusätzlich zu der zwischen afferentem Feedback und interner (also mentaler) Ablaufprädiktion der Tapping-Bewegungen ermittelten Differenz eine weitere afferente Korrekturkomponente existiert, die auf dem Synchronisationsfehler zwischen beiden Fingeranschlägen basiert. Dieser Prozess scheint den Genauigkeitsvorteil des bimanuellen Tappings manchmal beim kontaktfreien und meist beim normalen Tapping zu beeinträchtigen. Aus DT-Daten haben sich vier verschiedene Typen des Koordinationsverhaltens, nämlich „marginale Tapping-Interaktion“ (MTI), „Retardierung des periodischen Tappings“ (PTR), „Beschleunigung des periodischen Tappings“ (PTH) und „Synchronisation des diskreten Taps“ (DTE) klassifizieren lassen. MTI und PTR entsprechen dem „Phase Resetting“ Effekt, der schon früher für die Koordination von periodischen und diskreten Fingerbewegungen in Tapping-Experimenten beschrieben wurde. DTE reflektiert die Einwirkung von periodischer Bewegung auf die diskrete Bewegung, während PTH die umgekehrte Wirkrichtung darstellt. Diese gegenseitigen Einflüsse, die zur Bewegungssynchronisation der zwei konkurrierenden Tapping-Bewegungen führen, ist ein neuer beobachteter Aspekt. Die periodischen Fingeranschläge sind oft früher als die diskreten aufgetreten, wenn die beiden zur gleichzeitigen Ausführung geplant sind. Dies lässt sich dadurch erklären, dass die periodische Muskelaktivierung durch eine Verkopplung mit der diskreten Muskelaktivierung unbewusst vergrößert wird, was zur schnelleren Abwärtsbewegung führt. Alle Versuchspersonen zeigten ein dominantes Verhalten, aber auch die anderen nicht-dominanten der vier oben angegebenen Interaktionstypen traten manchmal auf. Obwohl nur eine geringe Störung der TappingIntervalle beim MIT-Verhalten gezeigt wurde, ist dort im kontinuierlichen Bewegungsverlauf eine gegenseitige Interaktion wie die Modifikation der Dauer der Tapping-Bewegung zu sehen. Obwohl das periodische Tapping-Timing stabil war, wurden Phänomene wie erhöhte auf den Kraftsensor ausgeübte Kraft der periodischen Tapping-Bewegungen bei Synchronisation, Einbetten vom diskreten Tapping-Bewegungen in die periodischen Tapping-Bewegungen, Verzögerung der diskreten Tapping-Bewegungen, und Rücknahme von periodischen Tapping-Bewegungen gefunden. Diese Befunde lassen auf eine mögliche Beschränkung des sensomotorischen Systems bei der Behandlung zweier konkurrierender Aufgaben schließen. Im Blickwinkel der Oszillationstheorie kann der dominante Effekt der diskreten Tapping-Bewegung als die quasi-elastische Kraft betrachtet werden, die die periodische Tapping-Bewegung zum oder weg vom Gleichgewichtszustand anzieht bzw. abstößt. Dieser Effekt ist von der Auslenkung des Systems von den beiden Gleichgewichtszuständen abhängig. Diese zwei Gleichgewichtszustände sind in PRC (Phase Resetting Curve) reflektiert. Der Anziehungskraft bzw. Abstoßungskraft der diskreten Tapping-Bewegungen wirken eine Rückstellkraft bzw. Dämpfungskraft den periodischen Tapping-Bewegungen entgegen, wenn die Amplituden der 9 Oszillation zu viel wachsen beziehungsweise zu klein werden. Diese Ansicht ist durch Modifikation der Parameter wie z.B. spezifische Instruktionen für die diskrete Tapping-Bewegung, Kraftanforderung, und Modifikation der sensorischen Information (Feedback) verifiziert. Um zu prüfen, ob die gefundenen Interaktionen nur für das manuelle Tapping spezifisch sind, wurden auch Hand-Fuß-Kombinationen für die diskreten Tapping-Bewegungen getestet; damit konnten auch Aspekte wie „homologe versus nicht-homologe (d.h. obere vs. untere) Gliedmaßen“ und „seitenspezifisch (links vs. rechts)“ betrachtet werden. In allen Konditionen wurden die gleiche Koordinationsverhalten gefunden. Der Befund hat gezeigt, dass die für die beobachteten Interaktionen verantwortlichen Mechanismen nicht Gliedmaßen spezifisch sind. Eine für die Steuerung der oberen und unteren Körpergliedmaßen verantwortliche Gehirnstruktur ist als Ort für diese motorische Interaktion zu vermuten. In diesem Zusammenhang wurde auch die Koordination zwischen repetitiven manuellen TappingBewegungen und schnellen Augenbewegungen (Sakkaden) in DT Experimenten untersucht. Da die beiden Bewegungen bekannterweise einen gemeinsamen neuronalen Kontrollpfad teilen, würden Engpässe bei den Ressourcen sich auf die räumliche und zeitliche Genauigkeit der konkurrierenden motorischen Aktionen auswirken. Statt des diskreten Fingertaps haben die Versuchspersonen eine reaktive Sakkade zwischen zwei horizontal fixierten visuellen Zielen während des mit der dominanten Hand ausgeführten repetitiven Tappings durchgeführt. Die Erwartung, dass auch die Sakkade eine starke Störung auf das periodische Tapping haben wird, wurde durch die experimentellen Daten widerlegt, da keine Interferenz gefunden wurde. D. h. gemeinsam benutzte Gehirnstrukturen verursachen nicht immer die „dual-task“ Kosten. Darüber hinaus wurden nicht nur das Übersprechen bei willkürlichen Bewegungen, sondern auch zwischen dem spontanen Lidschlag der Augen und unimanuellen sowie bimanuellen periodischen Fingertapping untersucht. Die Rhythmusstrukturen dieser beiden Typen der motorischen Aktionen wurden bei verschiedenen Anforderungen an Kraft und Bewegungsart des periodischen Fingertappings im Tapping-Experiment analysiert. Außer dem normalen Tapping wurden auch impulsartiges Tapping (schnelle Aufwärtsbewegung und Abwärtsbewegung ohne Anforderung an Kraft) und starkes Tapping (große Aufschlagkraft) untersucht. Auch der spontane Lidschlag ohne Tapping wurde als Kontrollversuch getestet. Das Fingertapping hat dabei ein auffälliges Entrainment auf den Lidschlag gezeigt, wobei die bimanuelle Bewegungssynchronisation mehr Effekt als die unimanuelle hat. Impulsartiges und starkes Tapping haben die größten Auswirkungen. Dagegen wurden keine signifikanten Effekte des Lidschlags auf das Fingertapping in den Experimenten gefunden. Eine funktionelle Überlappung der neuronalen motorischen Kontrollstrukturen, die sowohl für die rhythmische Fingerbewegung als auch für den spontanen Lidschlag verantwortlich sind, können daher postuliert werden. 10 1 Introduction 1.1 Multi-tasking: sharing a single execution unit or coordinating multiple units In computer science, multi-tasking and parallel processing is a well-known research topic. Nowadays, most of operating systems (like UNIX, WINDOWS, etc.) allow to safely and efficiently run several independent tasks at the same time without any noticeable delays, thus the user can work on different programs at the same time. But these programs are not handled exactly at the same time by the computer – in fact, the user programs as sub-processes were interleaved under the regime of the operating system and had access to the central processing unit (CPU) in successive turns until the processes are finished. . Realizing this virtual multi-tasking by rapidly switching between different processes provides the illusion of parallel executions. Priority schemes and response analysis were performed to optimize this CPU sharing. Truly parallel processing, however, is possible if more processor cores are integrated in one CPU (the actual trend in PC technology), or several CPUs are plugged into one computer (workstation technology), or the task load is distributed over multiple tightly connected computers (distributed processing), - all these advanced solutions allow the simultaneous use of more than one processor core or CPU to execute multiple computational threads. New ideas in the computer science could be motivated if the knowledge about how these issues are done in the human brain would have been available. In basic research, The Dutch physicist Christian Huygens (1629-1695) discovered the synchronisation of two pendulum clocks when they are mounted near of each other on the same support. His fortuitous observation that the clocks attained synchronisation after some time initiated an entire sub-branch of mathematics: the theory of coupled oscillators. (The term “synchronisation” delineates the tendency of two repetitive (periodic) processes to progress in a certain phase relationship.) Two centuries later, this phenomenon of synchronisation was investigated systematically, mainly by engineers, physicists and mathematicians. Synchronisation has been studied in very different systems such as electronic devices (Ramírez-Ávila et al. 2003; Guisset, Deneubourg, & Ramírez-Ávila 2002; Fortuna, Frasca, & Rizzo 2003), chemical systems (Wang, Kiss, & Hudson 2000; Kiss, Zhai, & Hudson 2002a, b; Shabunin et al. 2003), biological systems (Bonabeau, Theraulaz, & Deneubourg 1998; Delgado & Sole 2000; Glass 2001), and ecological systems (Weatherhead & Yezerinac 1998; Blasius & Stone 2000). In living subjects, cyclic processes are governing important behavioural processes like e.g. walking, and therefore, were/are addressed by large body of research. A specific focus guides studies investigating the mirror neuron system by using fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) to examine if certain voxels in the brain are shared between action observation and execution (Gazzola & Keysers 2009). Not only cycles, waves, and frequencies have been the targets of the physical scientists, engineers and mathematicians, but also the variety of biological rhythms and their cooperation in a single body or in a group of individuals were investigated, and the findings have impressed the biologists. Mutual synchronization occurs in many populations of biological oscillators. Synchronisation in living organism can be observed in, e.g., the bees’ respiration (Moritz & Southwick 1992), human female menstrual cycles (McClintock 1971), the clapping of humans in theatres (Maródi, D’Ovidio, & Vicsek 2002; Neda, Nikitin, & Vicsek 2003), and the flashing among fireflies (Strogatz & Stewart 1993; Moiseff & Copeland 2000). In most of the theoretical work of mutual synchronization, the smooth (global) interactions were studied. The episodic or pulse-like interactions are a specific case such as coordination in finger tapping (a tap is a hit of a surface by a 11 finger tip); they can be observed, when a discrete movement is executed while performing periodic movements concurrently. Finger tapping is a motor process and has been widely used in many motor control investigations. The Finger Tapping Test (FTT), originally developed as part of the Halstead Reitan Battery (HRB 1) of neuropsychological tests, is a simple measure of motor speed and motor control timing and is used in neuropsychology as a sensitive test for brain damage such as laterality of neuropsychological functions (e.g. Chaves et al. 1983; Friedman, Polson, & Dafoe 1988), and performance at cognitive levels (Dodrill 1978). The goal of the finger tapping experiments also was to study a certain definite control variable by analysing the structure of discrete time events (like the movement onsets, the maximum value of speed, etc.) during the tapping process. Perceptual timing related to tapping was examined (Aschersleben & Prinz 1995; Wing and Kristofferson 1973b; Swinnen 2002; Repp 2001). An oscillatory neural network controlling the coordinated finger movements was assumed (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1980). The resetting of the phase of circadian clocks by light was recognized by Bünning in the 1970s. Johnson (1999) listed a number of circadian researchers in the 1950s developing these ideas further and began to map the daily patterns of light responsiveness. Phase-Resetting-Curves (PRC) and Phase-Transition-Curves (PTC) – as described in Sect.2.1.2.3 and 2.3.2.1 later - were proposed as useful descriptions to present the phase changes of a circadian rhythm as a function of the circadian phase subjected to stimuli. Stimuli can be light pulse, temperature pulses, or pulses of drugs or chemicals. PRC plots the phase shift versus old phase (initial phase) whereas PTC plots a “new phase” versus “initial phase”. The phase resetting experiment was applied to investigate the coordination of discrete and rhythmic movements (Yoshino et al. 2002, De Rugy & Sternad 2003). 1.2 Motivation for this dissertation Human life consists of many periodic processes, most of them running in the background. Neural networks in the brain and spinal cord control rhythmic behaviours such as breathing, running and chewing. And other motor activities can be performed concurrently, like speaking, reading, or grasping. Moreover, many of human everyday motor activities require interlimb coordination: using a mobile phone during walking, driving a car, playing tennis, performing piano; thus timing of several movements in parallel is demanded. A fascinating example of such multi-tasking is the one-man band - this musical tradition demonstrates the amazing capability of humans to execute several parallel actions with reliable spatio-temporal accuracy, based on a high degree of motor coordination between different effectors. The coordinative process is so naturally governed by the central nervous system that many of our daily multi-tasking activities seem to be effortless and easy. However, while musicians are trained to perform more than one task, normal individuals dealing with some dual-tasking or multi-tasking are usually troubled. Can a perfect efficiency, i.e. without any loss of speed and accuracy compared to its performance in isolation, be expected, when at least two independent tasks are executed simultaneously? This should be possible only if the multiple tasks do not share any capacity-limited information-processing system. So, how can this multi-task processing be resolved in the brain? And is that anything like currently done in operating systems of computer operating systems? Did computer scientists ‘reinvent’ the wheel, or can they learn something from multiple task processing strategies the brain employs? Is there a “central bottleneck” with the first come first served basis or round-robin scheduling? Or are both serial processing and parallel processing ongoing in the brain? 1 http://www.minddisorders.com/Flu-Inv/Halstead-Reitan-Battery.html 12 Do dual-task (DT) interference and management overhead exist due to a “central executive” that manages the whole thing? 1.3 Motor Coordination – Task Scheduling and Timing 1.3.1 Single-Task (ST) Condition The single-task condition is the simplest case for motor control, since all resources of the system are available for the currently active process without restrictions. Therefore, this case usually serves as the reference for DT experiments, since comparison between results from ST and DT reveals the load introduced by DT. At the first glance, it looks like that the ST case is the normal case in everyday human behaviour. But analysing this behaviour in more detail will reveal that most of the single actions are performed on the background of some other motor activity; i.e. the DT condition is the very usual one. Within this work, the term ST also includes all those cases, where a background action is active but at an unconscious level. In the following, different kinds of those ST conditions are addressed. 1.3.1.1 Coordination of voluntary limb movement on the background of involuntary (tremor) movement Figure 1-1: Cognitive control of single-task on one hand involving internal timing for fast stimulus-evoked reaction and prominent tremor-at-rest (Staude et al. 1995). (Muscle images taken from Thomas et al. 1990) Tremors are understood to be generated by internal oscillators and feedback loops, respectively, thus they represent an unconscious movement. Mutual interference between tremors and voluntary movements on the same limb are reported: discrete movements superimposed upon a periodic rhythmic movement such as tremor are supposed to be affected by phase entrainment (e.g. Elble, Higgins, & Hughes, 1994; Staude et al. 1995); the so-called “entrainment effect” describes the observation that the discrete reaction (when planed in the movement plane of the tremor) is “waiting” until the tremor moves the limb in the direction of the required discrete movement. This behaviour can be interpreted within the framework of the minimum energy model of Bernstein (1967). Fig. 1-1 shows a functional scheme for this case. . Because this tremor condition represents a pathological condition, it was not included in this study. Instead, the basic DT cases with a voluntary periodic background movement (described below) will regard this condition. 13 1.3.1.2 Timing in periodic finger tapping Note: The literature is using the term “periodic” for a continuously executed tapping, even if the term does not really fit to the process: due to the stochastic variation of the intertap-intervals (i.e. the time between two taps), this tapping is not really periodic but repetitive. Nevertheless, this work uses “periodic” to be compatible with literature . Figure 1-2: A) Two mechanisms for representing temporal information (Ivry 1996): Clock-counter models postulate a pacemaker that produces output to a counter. Longer intervals are represented by an increased number of pacemaker output pulses that accumulate in the counter. Interval-based models assume that different intervals are represented by distinct counters, each corresponding to a specific duration. B) Schematic of two process mechanism for timing of repetitive discrete motor response (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b). (Taken and modified from Ivry 1996 and from Wing & Kristofferson 1973b) For periodic movements by a single limb, the existence of an internal timing system which handles the temporal information is suggested. It has been hypothesized that the cerebellum and the basal ganglia operate as specialized modules for timing (Ivry 1993, 1996). Models of human tapping to a periodic external auditory source (pace) have been developed in synchronization experiments requiring performers to tap synchronously with this reference stream and in the absence of this stream after a synchronisation phase with pacing. The basic functional concept is shown in Fig. 1-2 (Ivry 1996). The most basic model in information processing theory is the Wing and Kristofferson model (1973b); it consists of the timekeeper and the motor implementation (response) (Fig. 1-2B), and both are subject to random fluctuations. The important role of sensory information in temporal control was considered (Drewing & Aschersleben 2003). A nonlinear model was applied for error correction in psychological processes (Pressing 1998). The variable of interest is the timing difference between the tap (response) and the nearest reference point in the reference stream (trigger). 14 Figure 1-3: A) Multi timer model. Each timer is generated for each hand (Ivry & Richardson 2002). B) central timing model of synchronous two-handed rhythm production (Vorberg & Hambuch 1984). (Modified from Ivry & Richardson 2002 and from Vorberg & Hambuch 1984) Controversial hypotheses Dual output tapping like bimanual tapping is mainly considered in this work, because it addresses motor coordination, and coordination means more than one motor action. For this case, a coupling of effector-specific timing structures (Helmuth & Ivry 1996 (Fig. 1-3A)) is contrasted with the proposal of one integrated timing structure that controls both movements (Vorberg & Hambuch 1984 (Fig. 1-3B); cf., Schmidt 1980). From experimental evidence in bimanual tapping, Ivry (Ivry 1996; Ivry & Richardson 2002) favored the hypothesis that separate timing mechanisms become functionally coupled when timing of multiple effectors is centrally controlled (Fig. 1-3A). Figure 1-4: A) The dynamics is decomposed into end-effector x and neural units . The latter are bilaterally coupled via the function I and force (F) the end-effectors x. The state of x, in turn, is mapped to the -level via the feedback function G (Peper, Beek, & Daffertshofer 2000). B) Hypothetical timer intervals (Tn) and motor delays (Ln, Rn) and their relation to the observable inter-response intervals of the left-hand (I n ) and righthand (Jn) response sequences. A rhythm consisting of three notes (1, 2, 3) is assumed to be produced repeatedly (Vorberg & Hambuch, 1984). (Modified from Peper, Beek, & Daffertshofer 2000 and from Vorberg & Hambuch, 1984) Peper, Beek, & Daffertshofer (2000) tested the influence of amplitude on timing pattern stability (as predicted by this hypothesis) in a bimanual 1:1 frequency coordination task without imposing any amplitude constraints and proposed a dynamical model for rhythmic interlimb coordination composed of the neural and effector level, respectively (Fig. 1-4A). Vorberg & Hanbuch (1984) proposed that the same central timing commands control both motor subsystems for bimanual tapping. An important feature of their model is the symmetry in the dependence of the left-hand and the right–hand Interresponse interval (IRI) sequences. These dependence relations are described in 15 terms of covariance; i.e. the covariance between any pair of left-hand IRIs equals that between the corresponding common timer intervals, but is independent of the properties of the motor delays (Fig. 1-4B). Dissertation objective Temporal assimilations and interferences during desynchronized bimanual movements (Deutsch 1983; Jagacinski et al. 1988) and bimanual advantage (Helmuth & Ivry 1996) were reported. Experimental support for model applied to error correction is good but the variable of interest yet is based on the timing difference between the tap and the nearest reference point in the reference stream. This study also addresses the timing difference between the both fingers’ taps in the case of simple bimanual synchronous tapping. The assumption of a common timing system which controls both hands might be violated (Vorberg & Hambuch 1984) when a performer tries to compensate for the asynchrony between the hands by triggering the early hand only after some delay. The crux of the Wing and Kristofferson model (1973b) is that the presence of two independent fluctuation sources (timekeeper and motor implementation) causes the autocorrelations between -0.5 and 0 for adjacent intervals of lag one depending on the relative contribution between two source variances. Modification of timer variance and motor delay might be obtained by different experimental conditions such as contact-free, isometric and voice tapping (Fig. 1-2B). The mental task such as counting together with unimanual tapping although does not require a second motor command. If the tapping performance is changed, the question whether the common timer is improved by more attention due to the mental task or a second trigger command for counting is integrated with motor command for tapping has to be considered. 1.3.1.3 Timing in spontaneous blinking and finger tapping Figure 1-5: Cognitive control of single-task on one hand involving internal timing for rhythmic movement and spontaneous eye blinks. Next item in the framework of motor coordination is the case where two independent motor actions are performed, with one of them being unconsciously timed; a good example is the eye blinks which occur at a spontaneous rate. Usually, individuals do not pay attention to eye blinks, since their execution does not require much planning or even not any cognitive control (Fig. 1-5). There is a distributed brain network active in blinking (Frederico et al. 1998; Mazziotta et al. 1998; Tsubota et al. 1999). When a human performs a task requiring visual vigilance, the spontaneous blinking rate is 16 reduced (Bauer et al. 1985). Therefore, the study of blink rate is broadly estimated. Also, the oculomotor system interacts constantly with blinking. Mutual interaction takes place between blinking and eye movements. Smooth movements of eye suppress blinking whereas blinks tend to follow saccade eye movements and always accompany combined rapid head and eye movements (Evinger et al. 1984). Various external factors influenced blink behaviour (Ponder & Kennedy 1927). Psychological and perceptual factor (fatigue, attention, stress, cognitive or emotional states) dramatically change the blink rate. Therefore, completion for an arithmetic solution as a cognitive task can be marked by spontaneous blniks (e.g., Evinger 1995). It was also found that blinking often accompanies other tasks with some regularity, for instance, at “physical gaps” or at “punctuationmarks” during reading, or at the onset of redirecting the gaze when sequentially looking at multiple objects (Arthur 1945). Eye blinks can also be related to selective attention, and they can serve as an indicator to disclose deception as well (Fukuda 2001). Dissertation objective This study addresses the central spontaneous blink control process which paces the blinking and investigates blink behaviour during voluntary repetitive finger tapping to examine issues of motor and perceptual timing as well as other effects such as task concurrency, laterality of neuropsychological functions, etc. Various control mechanisms of tapping were proposed like, e.g., a single central pacemaker, which successively provides relevant intervals and triggers motor commands each time an interval has elapsed (Wing 2002; Ivry & Spencer 2004). 1.3.2 Dual-Task (DT) condition As mentioned before, the ST situation is not dominant in human acting behavior rather dual-task (DT) situations are more common. Certainly, conducting two independent tasks concurrently means sharing the resources, which implicitly leads to DT interference. The onset times of the two stimuli evoking task execution can be equal (simultaneous start) and different (asynchronous start). Both situations have been widely studied in terms of ideomotor compatibility (i.e. one of the two motor actions is an involuntary one like tremor in patients) (Staude et al. 1995) and of the psychological refractory effect (i.e. there is a dead time after finishing an action before the next can be started) (Greenwald 2003; cf. Lien, Proctor, & Allen 2002). As well, DT interference was investigated in different tandems of tasks (Pashler 1984; Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman 1993; Brass et al. 2000). A broad range of sensorimotor and/or mental tasks ranging from simple task combinations to complicated task pairs have been used and DT costs, i.e. signs of decreased performance were evaluated. In simpler cases (e.g. Welford 1952), latencies and mutual dependencies of two tasks were often analysed as a function of a varied SOA (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony), mostly in speeded choice reaction experiments (e.g. Pashler 1984; Pashler & Johnston 1989). Interference in DT should be minor, if both tasks are guided by exogenous timing trains, since a timing deviation become directly obvious by the perceived asynchrony between stimulus and response. A more sensitive method is to bind one task with an internal cue and observe its timing changes due to the exogenously triggered execution of the other task (open loop condition). 17 Controversial hypotheses Favouring the serial organization of sensorimotor transformation stages of perception, cognition and action within a single-channel (Welford 1952; 1967), the so-called perception-cognition-actionloop (Gottlieb 2007), the central bottleneck model expressing a limited capacity at the central response selection stage (Pashler 1984; Pashler & Johnson 1989) was accounted for DT costs, whereas other research results which point at the limitations of central resources emphasized the necessity of the strategic allocation and sharing of these resources in DT paradigms (Logan & Gordon 2001; Navon & Miller 2002). (For a more extended review of such models, see Byrne & Anderson 2001, Hazeltine et al. 2006). These dominant models are based on generic nature of central processing being independent of stimuli and response modalities. Therefore, it was referred to as the generic central bottleneck model (Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006). On the other side, acknowledging parallel flow of the central (cognitive) processing, the Executive Process Interactive Control - the EPIC model of multiple task performance (Meyer & Kieras 1997) - has associated all bottlenecks with peripheral processes or with explicit scheduling decisions. Irrespective the vast amount of data accumulated and the extensive discussions, the question on serial versus parallel cognitive processes involved in DTs is still being debated staying without consensus achieved (Hazeltine et al. 2006). 1.3.2.1 Coordination of two voluntary movements at a single upper limb with different spatial targets Figure 1-6: Cognitive control in dual tasking at a single joint. The concept shows two different internal timing units for fast discrete and slow rhythmic hand movements, respectively. Execution of two different motor tasks at the same elbow joint revealed the interdependence of time characteristics. Subjects performed fast, discrete elbow flexion movements and simultaneously produced rhythmical oscillations about initial and final visual targets embedded on a horizontal surface (Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1994) (Fig. 1-6). When movements of the left and right index fingers varied in spatial and motor congruence, dominated spatial incongruence on performance was clear (Obhi & Goodale 2005). To approach spatial congruence and motor congruence pairs of letters were presented on computer screen cueing the required movement directions and by altering hand orientation, respectively 18 1.3.2.2 Coordination of two voluntary movements at different upper limbs with common spatial target Figure 1-7: Cognitive control of dual tasking but now executed by different limbs. Like in Fig. 1-6, again the concept assumes two different internal timings for fast discrete and slow rhythmic movements respectively. The issue of motor coordination in dual-task (DT) and multi-task (MT) situations early attracted research attention in motor control (Bernstein 1967; Greenwald 1972; Klapp 1979), and much work was done since then to investigate the interlimb coordination and its timing (Fig. 1-7) (e.g. Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki, 1980; Kelso 1984; Latash et al. 2002; Semjen & Summers, 2002; Wei, Wertman, & Sternad 2003, …). One specific instance is the existence of bimanual interference when two manual tasks are conducted simultaneously (e.g. Swinnen & Wenderoth 2004). Dissertation objective The following fact is of particular interest for the present study which investigates interaction effects within motor MT: due to the open loop condition, a self-paced tapping is highly sensitive to interferences with other ongoing processes; e.g., the timing accuracy of periodic tapping with the dominant hand suffers from the concurrent execution of another (discrete) motor task with the nondominant hand (Yoshino et al. 2002). The interesting aspects such as the perturbation of the continuous process and the anti-phase coordination between hands with respect to the agonist and antagonist muscle activity, or, more generally, the phase locking phenomenon (i.e. periodic movements of two limbs show a strict temporal relationship) are still obscured; how endpoint trajectories are shaped due to these action elements during the joint action time. Analysis based on MEG waveform and PRCs maybe not reveal all facts and mechanisms if the finally observable trajectories are dismissed as it was done by all the “switch” studies which evaluates the times of switch closures (or openings) as the only characteristic measures for the limb movement (i.e. discretisation to a single bit). These investigations require more complete biomechanical and physiological observations like the continuous recording of, e.g., position of the finger (Semjen & Summers 2002), in order to detail the interaction of the fast discrete movements with the repetitive tapping. This will allow for combining the dynamical system approach with the information processing aspects of movement timing. But the analysis of these complicated and somehow noisy signals requires highly sophisticated biosignal processing for their evaluation. 19 Thus, other objectives of this dissertation are to extend the “switch”-study of Yoshino et al. (2002) appropriately and to clarify in more detail the strict temporal relationship and the mutual interaction between both movements. The trajectory formations would explain the kind of optimality principles used by the CNS (Central Nerve System) to satisfy the constraint of periodic stability with respect to the endogenous timing cue. Therefore, inspecting e.g. the finger tip position data in normal tapping leads to an extended analysis of the tapping movements which reflect the motor process in more detail. In turn, this supports efforts to establish a model for a common abstraction of the DT interference. 1.3.2.3 Coordination of voluntary movements on the upper limbs with different spatial variables In everyday behaviour, there are many cases when the hands perform discrete and/or repetitive movements while the eyes are directed elsewhere. The nature of the coordination between eyes and hands has been studied for visually guided manual actions (for review, see Jeannerod 1988; Binsted & Elliot 1999; Crawford et al. 2003), with the general finding that ocular and manual reaction times mostly co-vary. When directed to the same target, both movements tend to start almost simultaneously (Fisk & Goodale 1985); also, temporal characteristics of saccades are influenced by arm kinetics (Snyder 2000; Lunenburger, Kutz, & Hoffmann 2000; Snyder et al. 2002; van Donkelaar, Siu, & Walterschied. 2004). Moreover, Fox et al. (1985) revealed that the execution of saccades and finger movements activates overlapping cortical areas: both of them recruit the supplementary motor area and the cerebellum. Neural activity in the (saccade related) superior colliculus and in the (limb movement related) posterior parietal cortex is modulated by limb (Stuphorn, Hoffmann, & Miller 1999) and eye positions (Snyder 2000). In an imaging study, Bedard, Thangavel, & Sanes (2008) observed modulation of (visually-cued) finger tapping related brain activity for different static gaze directions. This and more recent results (Bedard & Sanes 2009) further extend findings on brain areas with combinatorial effects of static gaze direction and finger movements. Figure 1-8: Cognitive control in a DT situation involving two different internal timings for fast horizontal discrete eye and slow vertical rhythmic hand movements, respectively. The concept shows two different internal timing units for the discrete eye movement and the rhythmic hand movements, respectively. 20 Dissertation objective The above mentioned interaction of two movements related to the two tasks in the DT situation can be effector-specific (i.e. more peripheral) or task-specific (more central); this ambiguity cannot be decided by DT experiments employing two hands (i.e. two homological elements). Therefore, an alternative DT paradigm combining such seemingly unrelated movements as manual (periodic tapping) and eye movements (goal-directed saccade) is of specific interest in this context. If using a saccade scheme with a primary stimulus evoked saccade starting from the fixation point and directed to a target together with a second self-paced saccade back to the fixation point, this couple of saccades form a period running in concurrence with periodic tapping. The DT required cognitive control involving two different internal timings (Fig. 1-8). 1.3.2.4 Coordination of voluntary movements on the upper and lower limbs with common spatial variables Figure 1-9: Cognitive control of dual-task on hand and foot involving two different internal timings for fast discrete and slow rhythmic movements. There is a huge number of investigations concerning bimanual tapping, however, little is known about foot tapping or combined hand-foot tapping (Fig. 1-9); hand-foot coordination seems to be quite different from bimanual coordination: e.g., it is shown that in a hand-foot coupled motion the foot’s influence on the hand is greater than that of the hand on the foot (Chipman 2004). 21 Figure 1-10: literality of limb control. Different coordination patterns between limbs on the same side and between different sides due to the delay are assumed. Dissertation objective From the structural point of view, the interesting point is the different kind of possible interactions; there are 2 tasks and 4 limbs, which results in 49 possible effector combinations. A main aspect is the laterality of limb control shown in Fig. 1-10, which let assume a different coordination (interaction) pattern between limbs on the same side and between crossed limb pairs due to the delay. From behavioral point of view, the purposes of the hands and of the feet are quite different: to grasp or manipulate things and to move the hands together with the body to another place, respectively. Therefore, it cannot be assumed that an interaction scheme found in the hands by DT experiments can be replicated in the feet. In particular, homology and laterality of the observed limbs are important aspects. Maybe, interaction between hand and foot both on the same side is different from the cross situation when each of the two tasks in DT is exclusively dedicated to one side, because for the cross situation the pathways via the Corpus Callosum are additionally engaged. Also, the different peripheral delays due to the different length of the spinal pathways can influence the interaction scheme. Thus, combined hand and foot responses are investigated in the same way as in Section 1.3.2.2 to check whether previously demonstrated bimanual DT interaction can be replicated in the case of hand and foot responses. 22 2 Literature review on finger tapping Note: This literature review reflects related work of other authors. To achieve a compressed but clear description of this work, often original phrases were taken from the original papers without specifically labeling them, because mostly they are optimal with respect to information density. 2.1 Overview As already mentioned, finger Tapping (FT) represents a very simple task but FT behavior reflects the whole sensorimotor chain, thus it allows checking the integrity of the participating mechanisms. FT studies comprised different forms of movement (contact vs. contact-free tapping) and different stimulation (auditory vs. visual), and different coordination forms (in-phase vs. anti-phase). This chapter should give a rough overview about the different approaches and aspects before entering into details in Sect. 2.2 and 2.3. 2.1.1 Literature on Clinical research Obviously, the Finger Tapping Test (FTT) should reflect brain disorders, since it engages the whole Perception-Cognition-Action (PCA) loop; if any mechanism shows pathological behavior, it should be reproduced in the FTT up to a certain extent. Thus, the functioning of the central sulcus where the motor strip is most important is reflected directly in the FTT (Mitrushina, Boone & D'Elia 1999). Tests of motor performance can be used as reliable indicators of the integrity of brain functions by which performance of both motor level (speed, coordination) and cognitive level (alertness, attention) are examined (Dodrill 1978). The impacts of both neurological and psychological pathology on motor speed have been shown by numerous studies. Shaw et al. (1987) investigated the effects of lithium carbonate in bipolar patients. Heaton, Nelson & Thompson (1985) assessed neuropsychological functioning in patients who had relapsing-remitting and chronic progressive multiple sclerosis. FTT was impaired by Alzheimer’s disease (Wefel, Hoyt & Massama 1999), schizophrenia (Flashman et al. 1996) and traumatic brain injury (Gelmacher & Hill 1997). 2.1.2 Psychological and physiological research Information-processing theory analyzing discrete events and dynamic systems theory concerning continuous movement are two main theoretical approaches. Periodic tapping encompasses continuous movement and discrete events generated on a hard surface required more explicit temporal control than do continuous movement without contact (Delignières, Lemoine, & Torre 2004; Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry 2002) and may involve different brain circuits (Spencer, Ivry, & Zelaznik 2005; Spencer et al. 2003). Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer (2004) observed that paced finger movements are more asymmetric than unpaced ones. 2.1.2.1 Sensorimotor synchronization Coordination of perception and action was involved during temporal coordination of a motor rhythm with an external rhythm in music (Repp 2005). The notion sensorimotor synchronization (SMS) was approached for this referential behavior. Pollok et al. (2005) investigated brain area associated with a unimanual auditory paced finger tapping task and demonstrated a neural oscillatory network. SMS apparently requires attention and intention (Repp 2005). Without error correction, the variability inherent in any periodic motor activity would accumulate in progress. In a study of sensorimotor synchronization, Repp (2000) instructed participants to tap their finger in 23 synchrony with an excerpt of piano music. He employed a perturbation method called “pulse change” to lengthen or to shorten a single interval. He showed that not only stimulus changes that are explicitly detected lead to error correction in synchronized tapping, i.e. pulse changes in stimulus timing that were well below the explicit detection threshold led to effective adjustments in the timing of the motor response. Repp (2001) investigated the finger tap synchronization with an auditory sequence by which a small sudden tempo change in the sequence was approached. He concluded that a rapid internal phase correction and a slow internal period correction of the tapping period occurred. Period correction due to tempo changes of stimulation was found to be strongly dependent on intention, attention, and awareness (Repp & Keller 2004). Repp (2006) reported the distractor effects of an auditory sequence on the timing of self-paced finger tapping. Schmidt RC & O’Brien B (1997) reported from investigations of unintended between-person coordination that dynamical organizing principles are involved in natural interpersonal synchronization. Richardson, Marsh, & Schmidt (2005) studied the visual and verbal interaction of coactors. The results showed that verbal interaction alone was not sufficient for unintentional coordination to occur. Peters (1985) claimed that attention is an important factor in the interaction between superordinate and subordinate control mechanisms in a variety of bimanual tapping tasks. Miyake (2002) reported anticipatory timing control with attention and without attention. Most SMS studies found anticipation tendency or negative mean asynchrony (NMA) that taps tend to precede sequence tones. Different nerve transmission times from the finger to the brain and from the ear to the brain were suggested (Paillard 1949; Fraisse 1980). Aschersleben (2002; Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001) proposed that consciously perceived synchrony is achieved by accumulating evidence at different rates from different sensory channels. Enhanced auditory feedback on taps decreased the NMA (Aschersleben & Prinz 1995, 1997) whereas reduced tactile feedback through anesthesia increased NMA (Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001). 2.1.2.2 Timing in repetitive tapping Schulze (1978), Keele et al. (1989) interested in the nature of human timing mechanism for perceptual judgments about short temporal intervals. Schulz (1978) is in favor of an internal timekeeper which is synchronized with the presented pattern. Keele et al. (1989) supports the interval-based judgments (i.e. the timer records the intervals produced by finger hits) rather than the beat-based judgments (i.e. the stimuli (tones) establish internal beats which continue and serve as reference points for the perception of subsequent events). Wing & Kristofferson (1973a, b; Wing 1980) proposed a two-tier model that partitions the ITI variance into two independent central timekeeper and peripheral motor implementation. This model predicts a negative correlation of successive ITIs. The variability of this timekeeper increased with the metronome period (Semjen, Schulze, & Vorberg 2000). Helmut & Ivry (1996) reported a reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. The model provides the independence of motor variance on the period (Wing & Kristofferson 1973a). Different control signals to each effectors’s movement In a repetitive bimanual tapping task are averaged and provide timing advantage (Drewing et al. 2004). Various feedback components (tactile kinaesthetic, and auditory) are linearly integrated to form one central representation (Mates & Aschersleben 2000). Rhythmic tapping performance of patients with focal lesions in the cerebellum was impaired when tapping with an effector ipsilateral to the lesion in comparison with contralateral to the lesion (Ivry, Keele, & Diener 1988). Callosotomy patients exhibited strong temporal coupling in the bimanual condition and within-hand temporal variability was reduced in the bimanual condition compared to the unimanual conditions. Ivry RB & Hazeltine 24 (1999) suggested that motor commands from the two hemispheres are integrated subcortically. Motivated by the previous results (Helmuth & Ivry 1996; Ivry & Hazeltine 1999; Ivry & Richardson 2002) proposed the multiple timer model assuming that separate signals are generated for each effector. The intended anti-phase coordination in human hand movements became unstable and changed to in-phase coordination as the tempo of the pacing sequence is increased (Haken, Kelso, & Bunz 1985), Haken developed a theoretical model using concepts central to the interdisciplinary field of synergetics and nonlinear oscillator theory. Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer (2002) outlined a more elaborate system of coupled oscillators comprising additional two coupled limit cycle oscillators at the neural level, which are coupled to the oscillator representing the end-effectors. Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1980) reported that in-phase and anti-phase coordination are the preferred ones to which other phase relationships tend to revert. 2.1.2.3 Phase Response Curve (PRC) and Phase Transition Curve (PTC) Hastings & Sweeney (1958) presented the first form of phase response curve plotting the relationship between the intensity of a single 3 hour light perturbation and the number of hours by which the phase is shifted. Burchard (1958) and DeCoursey (1960) reported PRCs of rodents in their Ph.D. theses. Pittendrigh & Bruce (1959) published PRCs of fruit flies, and DeCoursey (1959) of flying squirrels. Winfree (1980) defined two types of PRCs (type 0 and type 1) depending on the strength of the stimulus influence: Type 1 shows a small phase shift whereas Type 0 a large one. The average slope of the PTC curve is referred by these indexes. Yoshino et al. (2002) applied the phase resetting experiment to investigate the disturbance of the series of left finger taps in response to impulsive auditory cues on the right periodic tapping movement. The results showed type-0 and type-1 reset in Winfree’s definition. De Rugy & Sternad (2003) investigated the combination of discrete and rhythmic movements in a single-joint (elbow) rotation. The initiation of discrete movement was performed either in a reaction time (i.e. stimulus enforced) or in a self-pace fashion. The results showed that its synchronization with the rhythmic task was more pronounced in the self-initiated discrete movement than in reaction time fashion. Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1980) psychophysically studied the properties of the human finger tapping using PTCs based on its average slope; they identified type-0 and type-1 assuming that an oscillatory neural network controls the finger. 2.1.2.4 Human motor control and timing (Thomas et al. 1990) Neurons and other specialized cells are components of nervous system. The peripheral nervous system and the central nervous system are roughly separated.The peripheral nervous system is composed of sensory neurons and the neuronal pathways. The later is the transfer medium to the central nervous system. Spinal cord (with the neurons located there) and the brain make up the central nervous system. Neurons generate and conduct impulse between and within the two systems. 25 Figure 2-1: a) the circuit carries impulses produced by a reflex action: receptor cell, sensory neuron, interneuron at spinal level, and motor neuron (according to Thomas et al 1990). b) Convergence of long and short latency reflexes at spinal level is believed to ensure necessary feedback during ongoing movements. (Left image taken from Thomas et al. 1990) Neurons are organized in circuits and networks. The reflex arc is the simplest one designed to administer unconscious automatic actions. These automatic actions are determined in nature to keep in a protective way the body in homeostasis. For instance, in the knee jerk, a stimulus is detected by a receptor cell detects a stimulus and synapses with a sensory neuron. The sensory neuron transfers the impulses to the spinal cord and therefore is synapsed with an interneuron. A motor neuron which is synapsed with the interneuron carries the impulse out to a motor unit in a muscle (Fig. 2-1a) and a contraction occurs. The spinal rhythm generators are controlled by several parts of the brain. A sequence of neuronal circuits comprised in the spinal cord mediates basic movement rhythms from the command center. Afferent signals from the peripheral receptors can modify the rhythmic movement providing information about how the movements are proceeding. The neural basis of internal timing mechanisms is often investigated by exploiting the ability of humans to accurately maintain temporal information. The required interval developed during pacing is represented in an internal mechanism on which timing is assumed to be based on (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b; Fig. 21b). 2.2 Literatur on clinical research The Halstead-Reitan battery is a fixed set of eight tests for evaluating a wide range of nervous system and brain function2. The tests consists of the Finger Tapping Test (FTT) and other tests such as the Category Test, the Speech Perception Test, the Tactual Performance Test, and the Seashore Rhythm Test. The Klove Grooved Pegboard, the Reitan Aphasia Screening Test, the sub-battery of perceptual tests, the various Wechsler Intelligence Scales, the Trail Making Test, and other test are additional included. A procedure described as an “extended Halstead-Reitan battery” has recently appeared that includes the original tests plus several additional ones. Participants have to tap as 2 http://www.minddisorders.com/Flu-Inv/Halstead-Reitan-Battery.html 26 quickly as possible with their extended index finger and hand palm down on a lever. They have to keep hand and arm stationary. The lever is attached to a counting device. Five trials are approached and each lasts ten seconds. There is a short pause between trials and 2-min rests after every third trial. Motor speed was measured and the damage of particular areas of the brain is determined. Four groups of 25 subjects consisting of control person, right-hemisphere damage, lefthemisphere damage, and bilateral damage were studied under the Tapping and Tactual Performance tests (Dodrill 1978). Measures of performance included those of each hand taken separately as well as their sum. The relative performances of each hand on each task were simultaneously considered. The results revealed high level of statistical significant difference between the control and braindamaged groups. Shaw et al. (1987) assessed weekly over a 5-week period 22 outpatients with affective disorders in remission. Placebo was given and lithium was reinstated during this 5-week period. They used the finger tapping test and the Buschke selective reminding protocol for motor speed and memory assessment respectively. To ensure remitted status mood assessment was approached by clinical interview, a subjective state questionnaire, the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression, the Longitudinal Rating of Manic States Scale. Atomic spectrophotometry was used to assess plasma lithium from blood samples. They reported that lithium had significant detrimental effect on memory and motor speed. Geldmacher & Hill (1997) studied 20 patients after severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) and 21 healthy controls. They reported that TBI subjects were slower on finger tapping. Qualitative and quantitative factors contributed to impaired visuospatial ability following TBI were concluded. Wefel, Hoyt, & Massama (1999) reported that Patients with Alzheimer's Disease (AD) reporting depressive symptomatology (AD-Dep) exhibited an unexpected pattern of greater right hand advantage on the Finger Tapping Test after investigation of the differences in cognitive functioning between 37 AD-Dep and a control group of 98 nondepressed participants with AD. Flashman et al. (1996) examined the relationship between soft signs and neuropsychological performance in patients with schizophrenia and reported that patients with neurological soft signs demonstrated significantly poorer performance on finger tapping and other test such as the Purdue Pegboard task and part B of the Trail Making Test. They concluded that soft signs are a manifestation of a localizable behavioral deficit of the systems involving in motor speed, coordination, and sequencing and are not indicative of global cognitive impairment. The specific deficit in motor abilities is consistent with the types of neurological soft signs that are minor (‘soft’) neurological abnormalities in sensory and motor performance identified by clinical examination and suggests involvement of frontal/subcortical circuitry in schizophrenia. 2.3 Literature on psychological and physiological research 2.3.1 ST condition 2.3.1.1 Timekeeper and motor delay (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b) Wing & Kristofferson (1973b) studied the correlation of successive intertap intervals in a simple periodic tapping task. They used a combination of paced and unpaced tapping of a telegraph key and instructed participants to depress the key periodically to close the contact in synchrony with the sequence of 10-msec-duration 2000Hz auditory pulses and continue tapping for further 32 responses. The pulses were separated by intervals of 180 through 350 msec. The last 30 intervals of the unpaced phase were analysed. Estimates of the correlation lag 1 between adjacent intervals were based on the ratio of the covariance lag 0 and lag 1 of these intervals. The results showed that the correlation between successive intervals of lag one lied within the range (0, -1/2) for all subjects. 27 Figure 2-2: Schematic of two-process mechanism for the timing of repetitive discrete motor responses according to Wing and Kristofferson model (1973b). (Modified from Wing and Kristofferson model 1973b) They proposed a two-stage timekeeper model of timing in repetitive motor behavior. The model comprises of a central timekeeper process on the controlling level and the peripheral implementation system on the executive level. These two processes are independent of each other. The central timer emits a motor command for the finger to tap to the peripheral implementation system and the motor response is generated. The motor variance causes the delay observation of the tap. The second source of variation in Timekeeper is also accounted in the absence of external pacing. The intercommand interval C n separate the motor commands in time interval C n. The time duration between the beginning of the implementation of the first tap and the beginning of the implementation of the second tap defines the timing interval I of the response. The intercommand interval fluctuates around its mean. The time duration between the central timer commands to the observed tap defines the motor delay D (Fig. 2-2. Cn and D as uncorrelated stochastic processes are n n in the following relation: In= Cn+ (Dn+1- Dn) (1) And statistically an longer (than the mean intertap interval) intertap interval is usually followed by an shorter ( than the mean intertap interval) one and vice versa and vice versa.Cn are Dn are assume to be independent and identical distributed with a normal distribution N(µ,θ) and N(ɳ,ϕ) respectively. The uncorrelation between Cn and Dn leads to Var(In) = θ+2ϕ and Cov(In, In+1) = -ϕ. Depending on the relative contribution of Cn and Dn autocorrelations between -0.5 and 0 for successive intertap intervals and autocorrelation of 0 for lags higher than 1 are implied. 2.3.1.2 Timing in repetitive tapping 2.3.1.2.1 Discrete timing Schulz (1978) wanted to find out by which mechanism human discriminated a regular sequence of beats from an irregular one. 28 Figure 2-3: Three different types of displacement that have to be detected. (Taken from Schulz 1978) Figure 2-4: Ordinal predictions of the three theories with respect to the three different types of displacement. (Taken from Schulz 1978) Figure 2-5: Detectability of displacement in different conditions for five subjects. The data in one row are from one subject. (Taken from Schulz 1978) Five participants heard a random sequence of standard and comparison patterns and then had to decide whether the sequence was regular or not. Two tones of different frequency (1600 and 122 Hz) were presented in alternation. The standard stimuli were regularly spaced with an interval of 300 ms and three types of displacements (Fig. 2-3) were introduced for testing the three theories: successive interval discrimination (SID), comparison with an internal rhythm (CIR), and comparison with internal interval (CII). The SID would be the comparison of the durations of successive intervals, 29 the CIR is the comparison of the actual input with the actively generated rhythmic sequence and the CII is the comparison of an internal reference interval with the incoming intervals. The idea is that the SID incorporates a mechanism that is only sensitive to local displacement in a rhythmic pattern whereas the other two are more sensitive to global changes of a pattern. The arguments were based on the probability of detecting a difference of the two temporal intervals of different length in different displacement conditions. The order of the detection probabilities was predicted. Fig. 2-4 shows the ordinal predictions of the three theories in the three types of displacement. Figure 2-6: Sensitivity index calculated from combined data. Three beats until the beginning of the displacement. The size of the bars corresponds to one unit of the standard deviation. (Taken from Schulz 1978) Figure 2-7: As Fig. 2-6 but under the condition of five beats until displacement. (Taken from Schulz 1978) Participants were trained to direct their attention to the critical changes by a clearly perceptible delay of 30 ms before the main experiment. Fig. 2-5 shows the values of discrimination index d’ of signal detectability theory of all subjects from the hit and false alarm rates. Fig. 2-6, 2-7 shows the results of analysis using the deviations of Gourevich & Galanter (1967). The results showed a 30 consistency with the theory that the subject establishes an internal time keeper that is synchronized with the input and against which the input is evaluated. However the theory that the subject builds up a referent interval with which the intervals in the stimulus are compared has not been ruled out for some subjects. Keele et al. (1989) was concerned with possible mechanism underlying the perception and production of short intervals. Two experiments were conducted. Slightly increments and decrements (10 ms, 15 ms or 25 ms) were used to the base-time interval (300 ms). One control condition for constant intertone intervals and three conditions for interval change were approached. The difficulty of task is predicted to be differentially presented in these conditions according to different hypotheses. For instance for the hypothesis that a memory trace representing the perceived average of the first two standard intervals is stored and used to compare to the subsequent intervals. This hypothesis differs from the number of comparison intervals in different conditions. Let represent the sequence as t t t t t t when the intertone intervals were equal. In condition 1 the sequence was represented as t t t + t + t + t +, in condition 2 t t t + t t t and in condition 3 t t t + t – t t. In experiment 1 17 subjects listened to the total of 270 series of seven tones. The subjects judges whether the intervals between the tones were all equal. The ANOVA showed significant difference between all conditions. In experiment 2 the first four successive tones were separated by base-time interval. After a pause of either 540 ms or 660 ms four more tones were presented. 10 Subjects should judges whether or not all the intervals in the second part of a series were equal in length to those in the first part. In condition 1 the following three intervals were all longer than the standard by 25 ms. In condition 2 the first interval after the pause was incremented. In condition 3 the first following the pause was incremented by 25 ms and the second was decremented. They found in both experiments that judgments the easiest when all intervals following the standards were incremented. The condition 2 was easier than the condition 3. The distinction between beat theory and the memory-interval theory rides on the difference in the outcome of condition 2 and condition 3. Both experiments in this study are consistent in suggesting that judgements of temporal equivalence are based not on synchrony of events with internal beats, but on a memory for interval durations. If a time interval is recycled from end to beginning, then in essence it can mimic a beat mechanism. If a neural loop from cortex to cerebellum and back to cortex is responsible for timing function (Ivry & Keele 1989), then internal beats might continually be produced cycling through the loop. 2.3.1.2.2 Discrete and continuous timing In a synchronization-continuation task Delignières, Lemoine, & Torre (2004) instructed participants to synchronize their taps in the tapping task and synchronize the left reversal point of the oscillation (maximal pronation) of a joystick with the 10 signals of a metronome. The frequency of the metronome was 1.25Hz. After 10 signals participants continued the task regularly following the initial tempo. The continuation phase was pursued up to the recording of 700 successive time intervals. They applied spectral analysis on series of produced time intervals and the doublelogarithmic plot of average power spectra. 31 Figure 2-8: Averaged power spectra (in log-log coordinates), for the tapping task (left) and the oscillatory task (right). (Taken from Delignières et al. 2004) They found that the results in tapping were consistent with a discrete, event based timing model. For the tapping task the slope of the average power spectra in the low frequency was close to -1, and confirmed the hypothesis of Gilden, Thornton, & Mallon (1995) concerning the presence of 1/f noise in the series of intervals produced by the internal clock. Fig. 2-8 shows the average power spectra. The positive slopes in the high frequency region confirmed the presence of a differenced noise in the series as postulated by Wing-Kristofferson model. In the oscillation condition the negative slopes in high frequency region suggesting a single white noise error term in the series. Thus, the spectra suggested a continuous dynamic mechanism based on the regulation of effector stiffness. 2.3.1.2.3 Implicit and explicit timing (Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry 2002) Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry (2002) used tapping, circle drawing and duration discrimination tasks in four experiments to explore the hypothesis that temporal processes may be represented and controlled explicitly or implicitly. Twenty five participants performed the repetitive tapping task and drawing task in experiment 1. In tapping task they tapped the index finger of dominant hand on the table coincident with the high-pitch tone of the alternating high/low-pitch tone (interstimulus interval=500 ms). In drawing task they were required to complete one circle during the interval between the low and high-pitch tone and then to pause during subsequent 500 ms interval between the two tones. Participants were guided with four circles positioned at 0 o , 90 o , 180 o and 270 o along the imagined circumference of the target circle. The guide at 90 o served as the location of the on-time marker. Instructions emphasized that the main goal was to be on time. Both tasks were performed with the continuation procedure routinely used for studying timing. The results showed a significant correlation between temporal variability on tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks in contrast with the findings of Robertson et al. (1999), i.e. the fundamental difference between drawing circles in a continuous or intermittent manner existed. They proposed that participants used an internal timing process due to the inserted pause to directly control the required interval between movement circles and to orchestrate movement initiation. The pause made the task similar to tapping by transforming the cycles from continuous to discrete, i.e. explicit timing control of timing is used under such discrete conditions. The interonset times in the circle task provide the requisite representation for specific events. A continuous circle drawing condition was included in experiment 2 to compare the patterns of correlations among the three tasks. They expected that a positive correlation between the measures 32 of temporal variability on the tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks and conversely a low correlation between tapping and continuous circle drawing would be obtained. A second change in experiment 2 is the reduced cycle duration to 800 ms. Twenty five subjects took part in this experiment. Again, temporal variability in the tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks was significantly correlated whereas the obtained positive correlations between continuous circle drawing and tapping tasks and between continuous circle drawing and intermittent circle task were not significant. Total variability on intermittent drawing task was more highly correlated with the pause phase than with the movement phase. Pause phase during this task was correlated with tapping variability whereas movement phase was not. In general, the correlations for the spatial and temporal measures were positive but low. Together, the results is consistent with the hypothesis that explicit temporal control is related to timing the onset of each cycle, and it is the pause phase in which such control is most required and factors contributing to temporal and spatial variability are to some extent dissociable. The relationship between the tapping and auditory duration discrimination task was addressed in experiment 3. The finding that performance on an auditory duration discrimination task was correlated with timing precision in tapping (Keele et al. 1985) was replicated. The positive correlation between all three tasks (tapping, intermittent circle drawing, and duration discrimination) was expected. Thirty five subjects were tested in this experiment. Results: significant correlation on timing variability between the tapping task and both the intermittent circle drawing task and the duration discrimination task was found. Correlation on performance between continuous circle drawing and tapping task was lower and not significant. No correlation was found between the continuous drawing task and the discrimination task whereas the correlation for total variability on the intermittent drawing task and the discrimination task was high. The correlation between the duration discrimination and temporal variability of the pause phase of discrete drawing was significant. In summary the tapping, intermittent drawing, and duration discrimination tasks draw on a common timing process, i.e. an explicit representation of temporal information is required for these three tasks. Tapping was tested at 1000 ms, 800 ms, and 400 ms. The cycle duration of the intermittent task was either 1,000 ms or 800 ms and the continuous circle drawing task was performed with target intervals ranging from 400 to 800 ms in Experiment 1-3. The correlation pattern varied as a function of the intervals was addressed in experiment 4. Thirty participants were tested with two versions of the duration discrimination tasks (400 ms and 800 ms). The tapping task with an 800-ms target interval and an intermittent drawing task with an overall duration of 800 ms, partitioned into a 400-ms pause and a 400-ms movement phase were used. The results showed that 800-ms tapping were more strongly correlated with the 800-ms version of the perception task than the 400-ms version. The reverse pattern was found for the correlations between duration discrimination and the pause phase of the intermittent circle drawing task. As expected the spatial measures were not related to duration discrimination performance. 2.3.1.3 Neural network underlying finger movements ( Pollok et al. 2005) Pollok et al. (2005) examined cerebromuscular and cerebrocerebral coupling in a unimanual auditory paced finger tapping task for 5 min. Continuous brain activity was recorded with a 122channel whole-head neuromagnetometer and surface EMG of the first dorsal interosseus was measured. Ten participants performed alternating brisk finger flexions and extensions of their right and left index finger in synchronization with a regular auditory pacing signal. Interstimulus interval 33 was 800 ms and ingrained in white noise. As control condition, neuromagnetic activity at rest was measured for 5 min. The results showed that task execution was served by an oscillatory network which consists of cerebellum, primary sensorimotor cortex, primary auditory cortex ipsilateral to the taping hand, , lateral as well as mesial premotor areas, the posterior parietal cortex and thalamus contralateral. An asymmetric motor control in right-handers was indicated by a clear activity in the primary sensorimotor cortex ipsilateral to the right hand. Significant Cerebrocerebral coupling was observed at 8–12 Hz and also the significant execution 8–12 Hz oscillations in a large scale network of simple motor tasks. 2.3.1.4 Movement trajectories in timing control ( Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer 2004) Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer (2004) addressed the assistance of movement trajectories on timing accuracy in an experiment involving synchronization or syncopation with an external auditory metronome. The degree of asymmetry in the flexion and extension movement times was positive correlated. The hard ground contact might cause these substantial differences. Huys et al. (2008) demonstrated the requirement of a time keeper during discrete movements but not during fast rhythmic movements. To accomplish varying behavioral functions such as speed constraints different timing control mechanisms presumably are employed via differential recruitment of neural subsystems. 2.3.1.5 The effects of attention Repp & Keller (2004) investigated the adaptation to tempo changes for their hypothesis that sensorimotor synchronization rests on phase correction and period correction. A two-process correction model was applied to obtain separate parameter estimates for these two correction processes. Phase correction was largely automatic and period correction required conscious awareness and attention. They asked peoples to tap their finger in synchrony with an auditory sequence. Ten participants tap with right index finger on an electronic percussion held on their lap. Participant started tapping with the third tone of a sequence. There were single task and dual task condition for two cases (adaptive and nonadaptive). The first session of the adaptive single-task condition contains three tasks: 1) synchronization and adaption to tempo change, 2) keeping tapping to the last sequence after it was ceased, 3) reporting the change by pressing one of three keys labelled with “slowed down”, “no change”, and “speed up”, respectively. Participants were informed about the change location in a sequence but were urged not to guess but to report their perceptual experience as truth as possible. Except for the added mental arithmetic task session 2 was identical. A random series of eight digits (1 or 2) were added to this task. The appearance of each digit was synchronized with the sequence tones. Subjects had to remember the calculated sum and should focus on this task but without sacrificing accuracy in tapping. In session 3 and 4 (single-task and dualtask, respectively) for non-adaptive case participants were asked not to adapt to any tempo change. The assessment of awareness about tempo changes through perceptual judgments was performed. Various attentions was realized by single-task and dual-task conditions. Instruction was given for adaptation or not to the tempo change. The result showed that their intention was manipulated through instruction. Their attentional resources were varied by the mental task. A linear shift with perturbation magnitude of the next tap occurred by a small phase perturbation in an otherwise isochronous sequence and is called phase 34 correction. The adjustment of period based on perceived discrepancies between its duration and the tone interonset interval (IOI) in the sequence is called period correction and the adjustment based on asynchrony between tap and tone is called phase correction. Period correction was strongly dependent on all three variables (intention, attention, awareness), whereas phase correction only on intention. Repp (2006) investigated the distraction of an auditory sequence on the timing of self-pace finger tapping. Two interleaved isochronous sequences of digital piano tones T and D were provided. The T sequence had a fixed tempo and a fixed pitch that was always lower than that of the D sequence. The T had an IOI (inter-onset interval) of 500 ms and D of either 450 or 550 ms. The D sequence started inphase with the 7th T tone. Eight musical trained participants tapped in the first experiment with the third T tone with the index finger of the preferred hand and should ignore the D sequence. The results showed that the tap timing was modulated by the D sequence. A synchronizationcontinuation paradigm and two conditions (with or without auditory feedback) were used in the second experiment. Different tempi for D sequences were approached during continuation tapping. Participants first synchronized with the T sequence and continued their self-paced tapping after the D sequence was stopped. Similar equipment and procedure similar to those in Experiment 1 were used. Tapping variability and of tempo deviation were increased. The negative correlation between successive inter-tap intervals was eliminated and tapping period approached the period of D sequence when they were closed to each other. These effects were independent of auditory feedback. Schmidt RC & O’Brien B (1997) studied the coordination between unintended persons by analysis the cross-spectral of the movements of ten pairs of participants. They performed a simple pendulum oscillation with comfortable rate in the same length and different length condition. Visual information about each other’s movement in the first trial was available but not in the second trial and no goal to coordinate for participants. The results showed an interpersonal synchrony and particularly a higher coherence and a more dense distribution of relative phase angles near 0 o and 180 o and a greater tendency to entrain in the second trial half. Richardson, Marsch, & Schmidt (2005) wanted to discover how the rhythmic limb movements of coactors unintentionally are constraint by the visual and verbal action in two experiments. The first experiment investigated the effect of conversational interaction on unintentional synchrony. Thirty six participants were required to verbally identify as many differences as they could between two cartoon pictures (puzzle task) and to swing a pendulum with their own self-selected tempo at the same time. Three conditions were conducted: visual (looking at the picture positioned on each other’s pendulum), visual-verbal (as in verbal + conversing for puzzle task), and verbal (conversing and face away). Higher coordination degree for visually coupled conditions was revealed by cross spectral analysis. Verbal interaction was not found and did not enhance in combination with visual action. The absence of influence of verbal action still even existed in the second experiment by longer trials and different swinging times at begin. Peters (1985) conducted two experiments for his postulation that a superordinate control mechanism initiates action in subordinate control mechanisms, which in turn set the movement trajectories in the two hands. The first experiment consists of three part experiment (1a, 1b, 1c). 20 left-handers and 12 right-handers were involved in 1a, 44 left-handers and 28 right-handers in 1b, 28 left-handers and 28 right-handers in 1c. Participants tapped on telegraph keys in 1a and on levers 35 mounted on microswitches in 1b and 1c. All subjects began by starting with single-hand performances, three 10-sec trials each for each hand, alternating hands from trial to trial and alternating the beginning hand from subject to subject. There are two task conditions: fast (F)/slow (S) and 2:1 Rhythm for dual-task condition. Tapping as quickly as possible with one hand and keeping a slow and regular beat of own choice with other hand were the combination for the first task condition. There were four dual tasks: RS/LF; RF/LS; LS/RF; LF/RS. Performing two taps with one hand against one with other hand with self-selected pace were the combination (L2:R1; R2:L1) for the second task condition. Slow hand beginning and fast hand joining or fast hand beginning and slow hand joining is the combination hand/task which is counterbalanced to reduced order effects. In experiment 2 five musically sophisticated subjects were required to tap out the second of rhythms with one hand while tapping slowly and regularly (beat) or as fast as possible with other hand. Hand/task combination are RR/LB; RR/LF; LR/RB; LR/RF; LR; RR; LB; RB; LF; RF (RR/LH = right rhythm/left beat, RR/LF = right rhythm/left fast, LR = single left rhythm, LB = single left beat, LF = single left fast). Both left- and right-handers performed the slow/fast dual task better when they commenced the task with the fast rather than with the slow hand. For the right-handers performance asymmetry was found, the right fast hand was superior in fast/slow task. In rhythm task for the right-handers the fast right hand was significantly less variable and greater variability in the left hand was found. Focus of attention on the nonpreferred hand was the interpretation for the performance asymmetries in right-hands such that the right rhythm/left fast task was inferior to that of the left rhythm /right fast. Movement intent is translated as immediately into action (superordinate) for the right hand and hence the right hand has to vie for attention under right rhythm/left fast condition. Miyake (2002) investigated the anticipation mechanism of timing control in sensorimotor synchronization by using synchronization finger tapping between tap onset and periodic tone onset. Four participants were instructed to adjust tap onsets to 110 stimulus onsets as precisely as possible by pressing a button. Seven different ISI (interstimulus interval) (450, 1200, 2400, 3600, 4800, 6000, and 7200 ms) were used. To clarify the relation between temporal integration (maximum capacity 3 s window) and the emergence of consciousness selective attention was approached. Reading short composition was used to control selective attention. Mates (1994a, b) reported that isochronous sequences with ISIs up to 1800 ms resulted in anticipatory responses and ISIs longer than 2400 ms in reactive responses. Miyake (2002) reported that with attention and without attention stable anticipatory response was observed when ISI were between 450 ms and 1200 ms. With attention the alternation between anticipatory and reactive responses was typically in the sequences with ISIs between 2400 ms and 4800 ms and a stable reactive response with ISIs beyond 6000 ms. However anticipatory responses disappeared in the sequences with ISIs beyond 2400 ms and stable reactive response became dominant. The goal of this study is to show that sensorimotor synchronization composed of two different dynamics (dualanticipation mechanism). One necessitates selective attention and it has long-range temporal interaction, the other does not depend on the attention and its interaction range is short. 2.3.1.6 The effects of sensory information To study the prediction of the temporal structure of events, Aschersleben & Prinz (1995) instructed subjects to synchronize their finger taps with an isochronous sequence of signals (e. g clicks). They confirmed the effect that the tap precedes the clicks described already more than 100 years ago (Dunlap 1910; Johnson 1898). Error between sensory feedback representation of the tap 36 and their central representation is suggested to be anticipated, i.e. in order to be perceived as being in synchrony the establishment of the synchrony between tap and pacing signal has to be present at the central representation level and the anticipated action effect determines the timing of an action. Negative asynchrony arises due to differences in peripheral and central processing times, i.e. differences in the nerve conduction time between click and tap and their corresponding central representations (Fraisse 1980; Paillard 1949). She continued to examine the "negative asynchrony". The subjects were instructed to use different effectors for tapping (foot vs. hand) in experiment 1 and 2. Extrinsic auditory feedback was added to the intrinsic tactile/kinaesthetic feedback in experiment 2. They also controlled whether the results observed were due to purely sensory factors within the auditory modality in experiment 3. They argued that the tactile/kinaesthetic feedback representing the tap and the auditory guiding signal represents the clicks are differently coded. These two central codes are different in code generation due to different processing times and hence the asynchrony would be different.. In the first experiment 14 participants tapped with two effectors (index finger or big toe) simultaneously in synchrony with 26 pace signals in two sessions. The first session contained three subconditions horizontal (two hands or two feet), vertical (right hand and right foot or left hand and left foot), and diagonal (left hand and right foot or right hand and left foot) and two tasks in each subcondition differed in the pair of effectors involved. In the second session only one of the four effectors was performed and hence the single condition contained four tasks. As expected, a negative asynchrony was observed throughout. Foot showed larger absolute value of the asynchrony. No effect of the effectors’ body side (left vs. right) or of the number of limbs involved (single vs. coupled) and no difference in the size of the asynchrony between the three coupling conditions were observed. 16 new participants tapped with the right index finger in the first session and with the right big toe in the second one for the second experiment. Each session was subdivided into two blocks and auditory feedback was provided in the second block. Asynchrony was observed as in experiment 1 and depended on effector. Asynchrony was smaller when additional auditory feedback was presented. In the experiment 3 subjects had to judge pairs of tones and were asked whether the signals had been simultaneous or not. The SOA (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony) between the two signals, the 400Hz tone and the 2000-Hz tone, was varied in 20 steps ranging from -127 ms to +127 ms (±127, ±95, ±71, ±53, ±35, ±23, ±15, ±10, ±5, ±2). As in experiment 2, the interval between the 400-Hz signals was 800 ms. The results showed that subjects were able to perceive very precisely whether or not the stimuli used in experiment 2 appear simultaneously. There is no tendency to prefer one over the other sequence of tones. Based on their results they suggested that tap-click synchronization took place at the central level where theses two sensory codes were superimposed in time. To confirm again this model the impact of peripheral nerve block and the effect of feedback delay were examined (Aschersleben & Prinz 1997) (Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001). The effect of feedback delay was examined in two experiments. Four delays (0. 30. 50, and 70 ms) were applied in ascending order for the first experiment. In the first experiment 10 women and 10 men started to tap within the first 3 of the sequence of the pacing signals separated by 800 ms and then tapped along with the signal as precisely as possible. In the second experiment 8 women and 4 men participated. The procedure was identical for both experiments. The set of four balanced orders was used for the four delay conditions in the second experiment to prevent subjects from aware of 37 the delays. Both experiments revealed the same results. She found a linear relationship between the size of delay and the asynchrony between the tap and click. The impact of peripheral nerve block was realized by the elimination of tactile feedback (complete anaesthesia of the moving index finger). As the first task nine participants were required to tap as fast as possible with the index finger on a metal plate. Standard tapping, isometric tapping, and contact-free tapping were performed for other three tasks. These three tasks required subjects to tap in synchrony with an isochronous click sequence (400 Hz, interstimulus interval 800 ms). The control conditions without peripheral nerve block were performed in the first session and tapping tasks were tested under conditions with peripheral nerve block in the second session. The results showed that timing was affected by anesthesia. Asynchrony was increased in standard and isometric tapping under conditions with nerve block. Both amplitudes and forces (standard vs. isometric tapping) showed no significant difference in the asynchronies. Semjen, Schulze, & Vorberg (2000) extended and tested the Wing & Kristofferson model proposed by Voberg & Wing (1994). They sketched a two-level model to synchronic tapping with a linear feedback mechanism that corrects for the phase errors. A fixed proportion of the last synchronization errors are subtracted from the timekeeper intervals. The next-to-the-last synchronization errors are also taken into account (second-order correction). Twelve right-handed subjects tapped periodically in synchrony with the clicks of the metronome and then without the metronome. Different rates were used (period = 200, 240, 280, 320, 400, 480, 560, or 640 ms). The results showed that IRIs (Interresponse intervals) tended to be shorter for the long interval range. The amount of anticipation increased with target duration. Means and lag 0 to 3 serial auto-covariances of 30 interresponse times (IRI) or asynchronies per series were calculated using the estimators given by Vorberg & Wing (1996).These statistics were then averaged over the 16 trials for the combination (tempo * task (synchronization vs. continuation) * subject). They concluded that as the prescribed interval increased the first-order correction was more focused whereas the contribution of second order correction was decreased. 2.3.1.7 Bimanual advantage in tapping (Helmut & Ivry 1996) The reduced within-hand variability in studies of intertap intervals was reported when participants tap with both hands, as opposed to single-handed tapping (Helmut & Ivry 1996; Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002, Drewing et al. 2004; Drewing & Aschersleben 2003). Helmut & Ivry (1996) addressed performance on unimanual and bimanual tapping tasks in normal individuals by comparing the within-hand variability and found that temporal variability was consistently reduced during bimanual movements. Participants typed the “ENTER” key on a standard keyboard in synchronization to a series of 12 50-ms tones separated by 400 ms and continued to maintain 32 target intervals. Three conditions were conducted: right hand only, left hand only, and both hands at once. Furthermore, nonhomologous actions and Between-Participants actions were performed. Flexion and extension of the index finger for one limb and for other limb the forearm flexion and extension again were involved in nonhomologous actions. Participants were tested in pairs for Between-Participants actions. There were three conditions. Two conditions involved unimanual tapping. For one condition, the participant on the right tapped alone. For the second unimanual condition, the participant on the left tapped alone. The participant who was not tapping simply rested. In the third condition, the two participants were asked to tap with the right index finger as in the unimanual condition and they were instructed to watch the movements of their own and their partner’s finger. 38 The bimanual reduction was also found with nonhomologous movements involving the finger and forearm. The bimanual advantage was found only when two movements were produced by a single individual. They proposed separate timers for each hand whose outputs are then averaged. 2.3.1.7.1 The effects of central commands The tapping performance of a person deafferented below the neck was compared with those of age-matched controls (Drewing et al 2004). The deafferentation was due to a complete large sensory fiber peripheral neuronopathy having an acute onset at age 19. The deafferented person lost all cutaneous tactile and kinaesthetic sensibility below the neck. The participants tapped with their index fingers either with the dominant hand or with both hands simultaneously on metal sensory plates fixed to a wooden board. The same two-phase paradigm was approached (12 pace signals, 45 taps in synchronization phase). The results from controls still confirm bimanual advantage. The deafferented person showed an even more pronounced bimanual advantage than controls. They supposed the hypothesis that the integration of different central control signals relating to each effector’s movements provides profits for bimanual timing. Ivry, Keele, & Diener (1988) tested seven patients with focal lesions in the cerebellum on a repetitive tapping task. Participants pressed with the impaired and unimpaired index fingers on a microswitch mounted on a wooden block to a series of 12 tones presented at regular intervals of 550 ms and continued 31 taps at the same rate when the tone ended. They found that all patients have more variable tapping with their impaired hand and suggested a dissociation of separable neural systems responsible for rhythmic movements. Figure 2-9: Multiple Timer Model. Timer1 and Timer 2 are generated for each hand during bimanual tapping and assumed to be normally distributed. Independent signals are combined at the output gate and issued to both hands simultaneously. (Taken from Ivry & Richardson 2002) To account for the improved performance during multieffector tapping, Ivry & Richardson (2002) proposed the multiple timer models rested on three assumptions. First separate signals are generated for each effector with its independent temporal representations of the desired target interval. These signals correspond to the clock signals in the Wing-Kristofferson model. Second there exists a central gating process that provides the link between central control commands and the motor periphery (Vorberg & Wing 1996) through which the two timing signals are averaged (Fig. 2-9). Third the gating process not only initiates the responses of each effector but also triggers the next timing cycle ensuring the temporal coupling despite the fact that they are associated with independent timing elements. 39 2.3.1.7.2 The effects of sensory information In this model sensory information of motor consequences are fed back to the level of timekeepers in order to compensate for mismatches between the actual and the desired moments of the execution of motor responses (Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer 2000). Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben (2002) tested the hypothesis that enhancement of sensory reafferences might support the bimanual advantage. Additional to replication of the bimanual advantage in tapping with two fingers of the same hand compared with single finger tapping they reduced tactile-kinaesthetic reafferences stemming from the additional hand during bimanual tapping and eliminated asynchrony by means of a firm mechanical coupling of the index fingers of both hands. Reduction of reafferences was realized by three conditions: the left hand rested, tapped contact-free or on a solid surface. The right hand always tapped on the solid surface. The experimental protocol consists of synchronization phase and continuation phase. 12 pacing signal were provided in synchronization phase and participants had to maintain the target ITI for further 45 taps. The results demonstrated that the bimanual advantage decreased when tactile reafferences from the left hand taps were omitted and the bimanual advantage replicated for the condition of mechanical coupling. Drewing & Aschersleben (2003) observed the within-hand variability of intertap intervals (experiment 1) and examined the influence of additional sensory reafferences by adding and removing auditory feedback to each tap (experiment 2). Participant synchronized their tapping with their index finger(s) in synchrony with 12 pacing tones separated by 400 ms and continued to maintain tapping with this target interval without the tones for 45 taps. Left hand only, right hand only, and both hand were tapped synchronously. Tones were presented monaurally to the ear ipsilateral to the tap in experiment 2. Figure 2-10: Raw sketch of a reformulation of wing-Kristofferson model. The timer system provides the time points for the goals of the action instead of those for triggering motor commands. The motor system plans trajectories in accordance with the prescribed action goals. Action goals are communicated in terms of sensory reafferences. An intertap interval Ij results from a timer interval Cj and the previous and the following errors of the motor system, Mj-1 and Mj. (Taken from Drewing & Aschersleben 2003). They found a reduction of timer variance when auditory feedback was added and the bimanual advantage decreased when this feedback was removed. They assumed that the increase in sensory reafferences in bimanual tapping is at least partly benefited. A reformulation of the WingKristofferson model (Fig. 2-10) was proposed wherein action goals were provided by the timer in terms of sensory reafferences. 40 2.3.1.8 Error correction In the context of synchronisation in tapping of a single hand with an external stimulus (Mates 1994a, b; Repp 2005) took in to account some correction errors. Thaut, Miller & Schauer (1998) reported that a small step change of ISI (interstimulus interval) leaded to a rapid adaptation of the tapping period but slow adaptation of the relative phase of the taps whereas a larger step change leaded to initial period overshoot followed by rapid adaptation of both period and phase, To replicate the results Repp (2001) instructed eight participants in one experiment to depress a white key with the index finger of the preferred hand in synchrony with the sequences each of 22 tones. The base IOI (inter onset interval) was 500 ms, each sequence contained a single step change in one of possible positions ranging from 7th to the 16th tone in the sequence. All IOIs were longer or shorter than the baseline IOI by a fixed amount. Participants were required to press one of three keys to report whether there was a deceleration, acceleration or no change in tempo. Repp (2001) separated in two error-corrective-mechanisms phase correction and period correction which derived from two threshold hypotheses. To explain the results Repp (2001) used the dual-process model of internal correction (Mates 1994b) with additional assumption that period correction depends on conscious awareness of tempo change whereas phase correction does not. The phase error correction in synchronization is limited by the temporal order discrimination threshold which constrains perception of the synchronization error. The period error correction is limited by an interval (or tempo) discrimination threshold that constrains the perception of changes in stimulus interval duration (or tempo) and/or a mismatch between timekeeper and stimulus periods. To strengthen this assumption there are three differences in the second experiment and detection responses were required. 1) Participants were required to continue tapping for a while after each sequence of tones was ceased. 2) The sequences terminated at various distances after a step change. 3) Isochronous sequences with IOI durations corresponding to those following step changes were included. On the whole he reported the success of the study. 2.3.2 DT condition Discrete movements superimposed upon a periodic rhythmic movement are supposed to be affected by phase entrainment (e.g. Elble, Higgins, & Hughes, 1994; Staude et al. 1995); i.e. the timing of the discrete movement shows some dependence on the simultaneous execution of the periodic movement. Beside the onset of the go-stimulus the onset of the single discrete motor response is also affecteded by the ongoing periodic movement in the background. The onset probability of the discrete response was modulated across the period of the rhythmic movement. The initiation of the discrete movement is more likely when the periodic movement is in the same direction with it and less probable in opposite direction. When subjects made fast, discrete elbow flexion movements about initial and final visual targets and simultaneously produced rhythmical oscillations only about the initial or the final target, an interdependence of time characteristics of these two motor tasks was clearly observable (Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1994). The starting at the same moment of the discrete agonist burst and the rhythmical burst caused the most likely onset time and resulted in a smooth conjugation. The initiation of the discrete movement reset the phase of the rhythmical movements. An experimental paradigm requiring the execution of a discrete movement whilst performing a periodic movement was reported by Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1979), Yoshino et al. (2002), Heuer & Klein (2005): A periodic tapping on one hand and a discrete response as reaction on other hand were performed in a bimanual-task where the first one was in response to an external trigger 41 event. Observed data were analyzed under the concept of phase resetting according to limit cycle oscillators (Winfree 1980) with the issue how is the impact of the execution of the discrete movement on the rhythm of the periodic movement. Weak perturbations (Type 1) showed small phase shifts of the ongoing rhythmic tapping movement and strong perturbations (Type 0) causes large phase shifts similar to a restart of the current cycle were identified. Some subjects were able to continue the self-paced tapping rhythm nearly without any phase shift whereas other subjects typically showed the restart of their periodic tapping process by the discrete response and their periodic tapping continues regularly as before the perturbation. Yoshino et al (2002) tried to clarify the control mechanism of the internal neural clock assumed to exist (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b; Ivry 1996). They continued the work of Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1980) using multichannel magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure the neural activity during tapping. Based on the temporal structure of MEG waveforms in response to the perturbation and the PRCs of the brain activity they proposed a hypothetical block diagram of neural system that controls periodic right index finger tapping and concurrent discrete left index finger tapping. Figure 2-11: Definition of parameters and the evaluation method of a Phase Transition Curve (PTC). (Modified from Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki1979) Figure 2-12: Two types of PTC. (a) Type The i-th cophase θi denotes the elapsed time of the i-th reference event from the end of perturbation normalized by τ (Fig. 2-11)1; (b) Type 0. The abscissa is the old phase before perturbation and the ordinate shows the new phase. (Modified from Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979) 42 2.3.2.1 Interaction in tapping (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979) Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1979) studied the functional interaction between the neural oscillator which is assumed to control finger tapping and the neural networks which control the same tasks. Participants were instructed to perform a response task to the visual signal in combination with the periodic tapping task. A disturbance of the periodic finger tapping caused by this response task was studied by PTC which is explained by Fig. 2-11 and Fig.2-12. An arbitrary reference event such as maximum of a physical quantity X projected by the internal state of the biological oscillator is defined. Phase Ф is defined as t/τ where τ is the oscillation period and t is the actual time. Perturbation of duration T is applied to the free running oscillator from the phase Ф – T/ τ to Ф. The phase Ф when perturbation ends is called as the “old phase”. The ith delay бi and the ith new phase Ф’i are defined as бi = Ф + θi - i (mod1), (2) ’ Ф i = Ф - бi(mod1) Where the ith cophase θi denotes the elapsed time of the ith reference event from the end of perturbation normalized by τ (Fig. 2-11). The ith delay бi indicates how much the maximum of X are delayed (б i>0) or advanced (бi<0) by perturbation. бi and Фi’ have their limit б and Ф’ after a long period because the period returns to its stable state. Ф’ = Ф - implies the new phase transited from the old phase by perturbation. If б i is dependent on the actual phase Ф, then бi = f(Ф). Thus, бi(Ф) and Ф’(Ф) are called PRC and PTC, respectively. Because both Ф and Ф’ are cyclic (i.e. mod 1), the PTC Ф’ (Ф) is biperiodic and Ф’ can change only by an integral number while Ф changes from 0 to 1. The obtained PTCs are limited to the two types. The one is the curve with an average slope of 1 (Type 1) and the other of zero (Type 0) (Fig. 2-12). Table 2.1: 10 experimental conditions for one session in the experiment 2. (Taken from Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979) In the experiments of Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1979), tapping was performed on a key in synchrony with the pacing signal and subjects had to learn the tapping interval to reproduce it 30 times without the pace signal after pace cessation. There were two experiments and three tasks (voicing the “s” sound, pushing the key, discrimination of figures) for the response task. Four participants had to perform three tasks in the first experiment and nine subjects had only the first task in the second experiment. The pacing signal with a 1000 ms interval was used for the first experiment. Parameters such as force pushing the key, tapping intervals and the choice of tapping 43 hand were varied in the second experiment. For the second experiment, a tapping interval was chosen out of 1000, 700, 400, and 200 ms with a 20 g motor load. A motor load was chosen out of 200 and 800 g for the 1000 and 400 ms tapping intervals. Table 2.1 shows the conditions for one session. The PTC from the first experiment showed that interaction with key pushing was the largest and with pattern recognition was the smallest. The second experiment showed that subject’s experience, learning had effect on PTC. PTC tends to a Type 0 shape for short tapping intervals. Motor load and alternation of tapping hand did not affect PTC. Although the perturbation caused the phase shifts, the stability of the following tapping interval evoked the hypothesis that a neural network produce stable periodic outputs controlling the finger and depend upon the strength of the interaction with other network controlling key-pushing task. Figure 2-13: The block diagram of the neural network which controls the finger tapping and the network which controls the key-pushing response. Thin arrows indicate information flows within a neural network. Bold arrows indicate information flows between both networks. Experimental design is bimanually. (Modified from Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979) Fig. 2-13 illustrates subsystems and information flows between them. They suggested that the interaction between the tapping network and the key-pushing network is rather central as interaction 2. Interaction 3 does not exist because the motor load had no effect. Functional separation between the motor center of tapping and the motor center of the response has been achieved for experienced subjects; i.e. the interaction 2 can be decreased step by step by learning. 2.3.2.2 Coupled oscillators Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1980) continued to investigate the assumption that for coordinated finger movements the finger tapping by the left hand is controlled by one oscillatory neural network and the finger tapping by the right hand by another oscillatory neural network. Subjects were requested to tap their right hand or left hand on the key with one of 10 various phase differences in synchrony with two pacing signals (1000 ms interval) with a constant phase difference presented for the left and right hand, respectively, and learn these intervals. The phase differences were chosen out of 10 steps such as 0, 100, 200 … 900 ms. After training, the pace signals were presented 10 times only, and subjects were asked to continue the tapping without pacing signals. The analysis was based on systematic error and standard deviation of phase differences. Phase transition curves were measured from previous tapping experiment and used to analyze the dynamical behavior of the system model (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979). 44 The results showed the performance of both hands finger tapping is better at the phase difference 0.0 and 0.5 than that at other phase differences. They propose two coupled neural oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping and reported that prediction by the model is in good agreement with the results of the experiments. Another approach in the dynamical system approach evolving in the field of movement science emphasizes on pattern formation and self-organization from synergetic, the interdisciplinary approach to complex behaviour in physical, chemical and biological systems formulated by Haken (Haken 1987, Haken et al. 1996). The qualitative changes in behaviour of the systems that are composed of many individual parts mutually interacting in a nonlinear fashion are focused by synergetic. For these qualitative changes of interest and hence for the pattern over which it is defined, the collective variables or so-called order parameters can be mathematical identified. The slower continuous change of variables, the so-called control parameters, associated with the functional components of the system can lead to abrupt changes in the order parameters and affect the stability properties of them. Points at which the rate of change is zero are called fixed points. With appropriately chosen dynamical variables characterizing the state of the dynamical system, the initial values uniquely specify the future evolution of the system in form of differential equations. Once the fixed points have been reached, the state of the system no longer changes. The negative or positive slope of the rate of change as it passes through the fixed point indicates that this is a fixedpoint attractor or a repeller respectively. In a study of bimanual rhythmic movements (Kelso 1984) subjects were instructed to cycle the hands at the wrist in the horizontal plane in an asymmetrical mode, i.e. one in which flexion (extension) of one wrist was accompanied by extension (flexion) of the other. Subjects grasped a handle with each hand. Potentiometers were mounted over the respective axis of motion. The instruction to increase rate of cycling either in response to a verbal cue at 15-s intervals or by a metronome with an interpulse interval that could be adjusted in 100 ms increments every 15 s. The frequency of the metronome ranged from 1 to 5Hz. Resistive load was also applied in another experiment by clamping the vertical rods leading to the potentiometers. At a critical frequency, the antiphase pattern was abruptly abandoned and changed to the inphase pattern, i.e. homologous muscle groups were simultaneously activated. Only the in-phase pattern could be stably performed when the frequency of the metronome was continue to be increased. The hands move freely or are subject to resistive loading did not affect this critical frequency. Haken, Kelso, & Bunz (1985) identified the relative phase φ between the oscillating fingers as order parameter due to its abrupt change and the frequency of the metronome as the control parameter and adopted basic ideas from synergetic. The motion of the hands is assumed more or less harmonic and takes the form X1 = r1cos(ωt +φ1), (3) X2 = r2cos(ωt + φ2), (4) Where ω is the basic frequency of the hand movement, r 1 , r 2 are amplitudes, φ1, φ2 time dependent quantities. φ = φ2- φ1 is then the relative phase. A potential function V was specified such that the differentiation (apostrophe expression) with respect to time of φ is φ' = ӘV/Әφ, (5) The dynamic has to be 2 -periodic, the assumed symmetry between the both fingers leads to the same transformation for φ = - φ The system is in equilibrium when the time-derivative of φ is zero, i.e. the (local) minimum of V(φ) is a stable state or an attractor point, a (local) maximum of V(φ) 45 is an unstable state or a repeller point. Generally, when V’(φ)!= 0 is in an unstable state, the system will be attracted towards (local) minimum. The following form of V(φ) was chosen to explain Kelso’s experiment results: V(φ) = - a cosφ - b cos2φ (6) Figure 2-14: The potential V for varying values of b/a given in each diagram in the upper right corner. The black ball represents the stability of the antiphase coordination. (Modified from Haken, Kelso, & Bunz 1985) Fig. 2-14 shows the shape of the potential layout changes as a function of b/a. As can be seen the gradual change of the ratio b/a results in loss of stability of the antiphase coordination where φ = followed by a sudden transition of the system (black ball) to the stable state φ = 0. b/a functions as the control parameter and hence describes the change in movement frequency. Haken, Kelso, & Bunz (1985) derived the following complex system composed of the mechanical motions of the hand generated with respect to the restoring and damping forces in f 1 and f 2 and the coupling I 12 and I 21 between them. X1’’ + f1 ( x1, x1’) = I12 ( x1, x1’, x2, x2’), X2’’+ f2 ( x2, x2’) = I21 ( x2, x2’, x1, x1’), (7) (8) 2 Iij = ( xi’- xj’)( A + B(xi- xj) ), i = 1, 2; j = 1, 2, (9) The parameters a, b in equations (6) are related to the parameters A, B in equation (9) and to the real amplitudes of the component oscillators. The so-called damping terms contained in f 1 and f 2 account for the injection of energy into the system or for the loss of energy. The component oscillators will be self-sustaining and their long-term behaviour will be periodically stable with a specific combination of these damping terms. For example with a Rayleigh term (of form βx ’3), the amplitude of the oscillations decreases with frequency, and, with a Van de Pol term (of form γ x 2 x’) the peak velocity increases with increasing frequency. Aramaki et al. (2006) replicated the spontaneous transition from less stable antiphase patterns to the more stable inphase pattern in bimanual finger tapping but conducted event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging to depict the region of the brain in which cross talk occurs. They found the interaction between the signals controlling each hand were prominent during the phase transition. 46 2.3.2.3 Phase resetting on the simple limit-cycle oscillator (Yoshino et al. (2002; Winfree 1980 and other) To understand how the rhythm of human periodic finger tapping is controlled by the existingassumed internal neural clock, Yoshino et al. (2002) instructed subjects to tap the left finger in response to impulsive auditory cues. Periodic tapping was performed on the right hand. The impulsive auditory signal as cues was presented randomly within the tapping cycle of the periodic right hand at various phases. The paced and unpaced paradigm was used. Eight participants were instructed to synchronize their right index finger tapping to 30 auditory pulses separated by 600 ms and continue tapping at this rate after pace cessation. In response to an impulsive cue given randomly (6.5 to 10 s) at various phases within the tapping cycle, participants executed single left finger taps as rapidly as possible. They measured simultaneously the tapping movement and the corresponding muscle activities with electromyography, responses of the neural activities with magnetoencephalography (MEG). For the right index finger tapping response and for the left sensorimotor cortex MEG response PRCs were established. Figure 2-15: Left: PRC of MEG in response to single left index finger tap stimulations recorded from the left sensorimotor cortex for a type-1 subject (subject 1). Magnitude of MEG response was gray-level-coded as in the left panel of figure. Right: PRC of corresponding right index finger tapping. The abscissa is stimulation phase Фstim; the ordinate is time (in s) with respect to right index finger tap just prior to left index finger tap. The left index finger tap stimulation was applied along the line connecting (Фstim, time) = (0, 0) and (Фstim, time) = (1, N) indicated by dashed line. Control tapping period was N ~ 0.6 s. Occurrences of right index finger taps is plotted by the points, and their mean onset time within each set (1 ~ 10 divided according to the Фstim) is indicated by an asterisk with an SD bar. Dotted lines above the dashed left tap stimulation line represent occurrences of right index finger taps in the control case. (Taken from Yoshino et al. 2002) 47 Figure 2-16: Left: PRC of MEG in response to single left index finger taps recorded from left sensorimotor cortex for a type-0 subject (subject 7). Right: PRC of corresponding right index finger tapping. For details, see Fig. 2-15 legend. (Taken from Yoshino et al. 2002) Figure 2-17: Left: PRC of MEG in response to single left index finger taps recorded from left sensorimotor cortex (subject 6). Right: PRC of corresponding right index finger tapping. For details, see Fig. 2-15 legend. This subject showed a resetting pattern with little phase shift for Фstim < 0:5 and an obvious type-0 reset for Фstim > 0:5; the latter led to the type-0 classification. (Taken from Yoshino et al. 2002) 48 Figure 2-18: Phase portraits of the limit-cycle oscillator model (Eq. 10). The phase of the state point on limit cycle γ is defined as Ф = θ/2π [0, 1], where θ is counterclockwise angle from the positive X-axis. Impulsive stimulation with intensity A translates a state point horizontally from C to D. Since limit cycle γ is stable, the perturbed state point returns to γ. Thus, the phase is reset from Фstim to Фstim - Δ. a low stimulus intensity A = 0.5 and type-1 reset; b large stimulus intensity A = 1.5 and type-0 reset. The actual state point corresponding to the stimulus onset is denoted as the stimulus phase Фstim. (Taken from Yoshino et al. 2002) MEG data in response to stimulations were divided into ten sets according to Фstim. Set 1 consists of data for Фstim Є [0, 0.1], set 2 for Фstim Є [0.1, 0.2]... set 10 for Фstim Є [0,9, 1.0], Each set includes about 60 tap responses. Type-0 reset according to Winfree’s definition was shown in four out of eight subjects, and the others showed type-1 reset (Fig. 2-15, 2-16, 2-17). They speculated a neural pathway by which the left index finger tap system affects the periodic right index finger and results in the phase reset. The following simple limit-cycle oscillator model in polar coordinates (r, θ) was proposed r . = Kr(1 – r2), (10) ’ θ =ω Where ω>0 represents angular velocity and K is a positive constant. A unit circle of the stable limit cycle γ is formed at the origin O (Fig. 2-18) with natural period N = 2π/ω. Any trajectory deviated from origin O moves asymptotically to γ as t -> ∞. The system’s state is defined by phase Ф = θ/2π Є [0, 1] and the reference event of model occurs at Ф = 0, when the state point crosses the positive Xaxis. A state point on γ will be pushed horizontally away (C->D) by the intensity (A) of the stimulus from γ to γ’. The perturbed trajectory will return back to γ. The perturbed state point D behaves identically to the point C’ on γ where γ intersects the line OD. The phase reset (delay) Δ can be written as a function of Фstim as Δ (Фstim) = Фstim - (1/2π)tan-1 (sin2π Фstim /(A + cos2πФstim)) (11) Winfree (1980) started with the observation that most systems whose state vary in only one way and have the same last state and first state can fall into either category of ring device that its rate of advance through its cycle is conditioned by an external influence (intensity parameter I). The instantaneous rate of change of phase is jointly determined by its instantaneous state (phase Ф) and by the external influence parameter I. Ф' = V(Ф, I); 49 ’ Figure 2-19: (a) the angular velocity (vertically) as a function of phase (horizontally), using model Ф = 1 + cos2πФ, with I = 0. The circle (left side) depicts by the length of the curved arrow the angular velocity at each phase. (b) As in (a) but I = 1/2. (c) As in (a) but I = 1.5 past the bifurcation at I = 1. An attractor-repellor pair has opened up from phase Ф = 1/2, inverting the angular velocity within that arc. Note that Winfree’s concept uses a clockwise progressing of the system phase, whereas in Yoshino et al (2002) the usual behaviour is counterclockwise progression. (Modified from Winfree 1980) In standard environment with I =I0, the cycle is calibrated to define Ф’ = 1 (Fig. 2-19a). In any other environment I I0, the angular velocity of the ring device generally varies throughout its cycle (Fig. 219b). Thus, the ring device runs faster or slower, depending on its current phase (Ф(t)) as long as exposed to I I0. With sufficiently large I, V may even become negative during part of the cycle (Fig. 2-19c), then the phase “sticks” at the attracting stagnation point Фa but it will not start the next cycle if there is no exogenous support to pass the repelling stagnation point Фr, e. g. by changing back I to I0 for a while. Investigating the collective behaviour of limit-cycle oscillators, Winfree discovered that collective synchronization is a threshold phenomenon. By exceeding the critical coupling strength, some oscillator transit to a common frequency (Ariaratnam & Strogatz 2000). Many other authors refined the model with applications. Kuramoto’s model (1984) displays locked, partially locked, or incoherent states, depending on chosen parameters. Ariaratnam & Strogatz (2000) discovered novel hybrid states corresponding to various mixtures of locking, incoherence, and oscillator death (a cessation of oscillation caused by excessively strong coupling). 2.3.2.4 Effect of periodic movement on discrete movement Staude, Cong-Khac, & Wolf (2006) conducted experiments with subjects performing rapid finger abduction movements during a secondary rhythmic adduction-adduction movement of the same finger. Movements were either voluntarily produced by the subject or passively imposed by a torque motor. The results confirmed the ability of one movement to constrain or even impede the execution of the other due to gating process. Wachter et al. (2008) employed a dual-task condition as used by Yoshino et al. (2002) and reported that one of four tapping behavior DTE (discrete tap entrainment) is a directed effect of the periodic on the discrete process. 2.3.2.5 The effects of force (Loseby, Piek, & Barrett 2001) Loseby, Piek, & Barrett (2001) investigated force requirement as variable which interact with control variable (frequency) to produce a combined influence on stability of antiphase bimanual finger tapping. Beginning with the right finger 10 right handed participants tapped their finger in an 50 alternating pattern. They should increase force on the right finger in response to a visual stimulus and should maintain the required tapping speed. Three tapping rates were used. The phase relation was affected by an increase of force at higher tapping rates (200 ms, 400 ms) but not at slower tapping rate (600 ms). The results support the suggestion that force combined with the rate of tapping to shift the critical point at which antiphase tapping becomes unstable. Force as a control variable should be investigated. Figure 2-20: The dynamics is decomposed into end-effectors X and neural units ξ. The latter are bilaterally coupled via the function I and force (F) the end-effectors X. The state of X, in turn, is mapped to the ξ-level via the feedback function G. (Modified from Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer 2002) 2.3.2.6 The effects of sensory information (Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer 2002) Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer (2002) tried to remedy the shortcomings of HKB-model (mentioned in 2.2.1.3), namely the amplitude-frequency relation and the unclear correlation structure between successive intervals, by proposing a more encompassing model. In the model there are two coupled oscillators at the neural level. These oscillators are in turn coupled to a linearly damped oscillator representing the corresponding end-effector (Fig. 2-20). The strengths of the original model in describing the stability-related aspects of interlimb coordination were preserved while the effector level has an effect on the neural level through a feedback function, and fluctuating forces are included to account for phenomena such as critical fluctuations of the correlations between successive intervals. ξj" + ω2j ξj – Nj(ξj, ξj‘) = Gj(Xj) + Ijk(ξj, ξj‘, ξj, ξj") {+ Гj(ξ)(t)} (12) ’’ 2 vXj + Ωj Xj + µjX’j = Fj(ξj) ( ) ’ {+ Гj X (t)} The indices j = 1, 2 and k = 1, 2 distinguish between the two coupled neural oscillators (left and right), Ij is the coupling, Xj are the limb oscillations and ξj the neural oscillations; Fj(ξj) is driving force of ξj on Xj; Gj (Xj) is feedback function and Гj(ξ)(t) fluctuating forces. The nonlinearities N generate selfsustained limit cycles for ξj. 2.3.2.7 The effects of attention (De Rugy & Sternad 2003) De Rugy & Sternad (2003) performed a DT study with the focus on the effect of instruction. A vertical wooden handle was affixed to the end of the forearm support and six subjects had to grasp with their arm. They were instructed to oscillate (2Hz) their arm between two visible targets in synchrony with an auditory metronome for 5s, one full cycle per beat. They had to continue oscillating for between 2 and 5 s at the same frequency after metronome cessation. 51 Figure 2-21: Representative trial segments as an illustration of the three movements conditions. (Taken from Rugy & Sternad 2003) Three movement conditions in 4 different target arrangements were approached (Fig. 2-21): (1) a shift in the midpoint of the oscillation (MID), (2) a change in the amplitude of the oscillation (AMP), (3) both a shift in the midpoint and a change of the amplitude of the oscillation (MID+AMP). These tree task conditions were performed under two timing instructions. The first, termed ‘‘reaction time’’ (RT) emphasized both the reaction time and the speed of the change, i.e. subjects had to react as fast as possible but to maintain the required target hit. In the second instruction, termed “self-paced” (SP) subjects could react whenever they felt most comfortable after the trigger signal but were required to perform the change as fast as possible. The results showed the similar tendency of synchronization between discrete and rhythmic movements in all three tasks and instruction conditions but the synchronization was most pronounced in the self-paced discrete movement. 2.4 Summary Tapping performance was evaluated as reliable indicators of the integrity of brain functions in clinical research. The Tapping Test discriminated between the control and brain-damaged groups at high level of statistical significance (Dodrill 1978). Motor performance was declined when lithium was introduced (Shaw et al. 1987). Patients with traumatic brain injury are slower on finger tapping (Geldmacher & Hill 1997). Patients with Alzheimer's disease showed a greater right hand advantage on FTT (Wefel, Hoyt, & Massama 1999). Patients with neurological soft signs demonstrated significantly poorer motor speed and motor coordination in FTT (Flashman et al. 1996). Information-processing theory and dynamic systems theory are approached in psychological and physiological research. Periodic tapping encompasses continuous movement with and without discrete events may involve different brain circuits (Delignières, Lemoine, & Torre 2004; Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry 2002; Spencer, Ivry, & Zelaznik 2005; Spencer et al. 2003; Huys et al. 2008). The lateral regions of the cerebellum are critical for an accurate internal timing function (Ivry, Keele, & Diener (1988). For the production of an isochronous sequence of taps in the continuation phase after the pacing stimulus is ceased, a time keeper generates motor commands in an interval C n without any feedback or correction mechanism (Wing & Kristofferson 1973a, b, Wing 1980). The observable taps are produced after a certain motor delay Dn (Fig.2-10). Both Cn and Dn are subject to random fluctuations and assumed to be independent from each other. This open-loop model predicts autocorrelations between -0.5 and 0 for successive intertap intervals, depending on the relative contribution of the component variances. The model provides the indepence of Dn on the period (Wing & Kristofferson 1973a). The regularity of C n is not based upon the comparison of successive ones just as Schulze (1978) favored the internal timekeeper from experiments on the discrimination of temporal intervals. Keele et al. (1989) supported the interval theory that the internal timer records 52 the intervals and this stored interval is reproduced and used in comparison. Sensory input below the conscious detection threshold is still of use in controlling the timing of motor control (Repp 2000). An auditory distractor sequence attracts the rhythmic movement regardless of whether or not the taps generated auditory feedback (Repp 2006). Several experiments showed the importance role of sensory feedback in timing control (Aschersleben & Prinz 1995, 1997; Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001). Negative asynchrony let explained by different nerve transmission times of the tap onsets and the audio signals (Paillard 1949; Fraisse 1980). Synchrony of movements with sequence of events is established at the level of central representations (Aschersleben 2002). Tactile, kinaesthetic, and auditory feedback are linearly integrated to form one central representation (Mates & Aschersleben 2000). The internal timekeeper augmented by a firstorder feedback accounts well for the stochastic aspects of synchronisation performance (Semjen, Schulze, & Vorberg 2000). If the sensory information is fed back to the level of the timekeeper, then not only a compensation for the mismatches between the previous and the actual desired motor responses for the individual finger but also between the two fingers in bimanual condition. Several studies reported the bimanual advantage (Helmut & Ivry 1996; Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002; Drewing & Aschersleben 2003; Drewing et al. 2004) in bimanual tapping in comparison to unimanual tapping. On the one side, this advantage profits from the integration of different central control signals related to each effector (Drewing et al. 2004; Ivry & Keller 1989; Ivry & Richardson 2002; Ivry & Hazeltine 1999). The sensory information of the other movements (Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002; Aschersleben & Prinz, 1995) and auditory feedback (Drewing & Aschersleben 2003) also contribute to the advantage. Mental tapping such as counting without voice together with normal tapping on one hand do not require motor command for the second effector. Can C n be improved by this additional mental task? The dynamical principles are even involved in natural interpersonal synchrony (Schmidt & O’Brien 1997; Richardson, Marsh, & Schmidt 2005). Are these dynamical principles also involved in individual without physical integration of different central control signals? The discrete feature of timing behavior, the motor delay and the mismatches between two hands are modified in isometric condition and in contact-free condition. The second source of variance (Dn) is then modified or excluded in isometric tapping. The first source of variance (Cn) is improved by sensory reafferences in tapping with contact and particularly in voice tapping but impaired in isometric tapping and contact-free condition. A system of coupled oscillators is outlined, which comprises two coupled limit cycle oscillators at the neural level coupled with the ones representing the end-effectors (Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer 2002). The initiation of voluntary movement was time-locked to the tremor cycle in patients with moderate to severe essential tremor (Elble, Higgins, & Hughes 1994; Staude et al. 1995). One movement constrains the execution of the other (Staude, Cong-Khac, & Wolf 2006, Wachter et al. 2008). The discrete movement affects the rhythm of the periodic movement (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979; Yoshino et al. 2002; Heuer & Klein 2005; Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1994). Winfree (1980) approached the viewpoint of “phase resetting” according to definition of general limit cycle oscillators. Phase Resetting Curve and Phase Response Curve provide crucial insights in many researches (Burchard 1958; DeCoursey 1959; DeCoursey 1959; Pittendrigh & Bruce 1959). Phase shifts of the ongoing rhythmic tapping movement were found. Haken emphasized timing structures and self-organization mechanisms as described in synergetic (Haken 1987; Haken et al. 1996). The phase of the luminescence was shifted by a manipulation of the dark and light periods (Hastings & Sweeney 1958). Only two stable phase states (inphase and antiphase) between the hands are present (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1980) and one attractor state migrates to the other at a critical cycling frequency (Kelso 1984; Aramaki et al. 2006). The performance of bimanual tapping is better 53 at the phase difference 0.0 and 0.5 than that at other phase differences (Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1980). Movement force and rate interact to influence the outcome of the tapping pattern and force as a control parameter is needed to investigate in further research (Loesby, Piek, & Barrett2001). High velocity movements towards the target provide perceptual information relevant to accuracy in synchronization (Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer 2004). The postulated event-based timing control could be restricted to a limited conditions characterized by the discrete events (Delignières, Lemoine, & Torre 2004) and a continuous, dynamic timing mechanism is suggested from the results of an oscillatory motion of the hand. Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry (2002) also supported the hypothesis that there is an important distinction between the control processes associated with timing tasks involving discrete and continuous events. This distinction is clearly presented in normal tapping in comparison to isometric tapping and contact-free tapping. In ISI between 2400 and 4800 ms attention is needed for anticipatory responses but in ISIs up to 1200 ms attention is not needed (Mates 1994a, b, Miyake 2002). Only intentional period correction seems to require attention, whereas phase correction has an automatic component that cannot be suppressed (Repp & Keller 2004). Phase correction does not depend on awareness of tempo change (Repp 2001). Subconscious mechanisms of action regulation (phase correction) and conscious processes (period correction) are involved in perceptual judgment and action planning (Repp 2005). Continuous, dynamic timing mechanism, event-based timing control, and trajectory contribution together with the role of sensory feedback in timing control, the two stable phase states between the hands might still exist but may be not expected to be 0 and 0.5. An asymmetric motor control in right-handers is suggested due to additional oscillatory activity in the primary sensorimotor cortex ipsilateral to the tapping hand (Pollok et al. 2005). The performance in dual task suffers when attention was focused on the nonpreferred hand (Peters 1985). A specific focus on the constraints between the two movement elements varied the synchronization (De Rugy & Sternad 2003). The role exchange between two hands or the required focus on specific hand would also change the two stable states. 54 3 Literature review on eye blinks Note: This literature review reflects related work of other authors. To achieve a compressed but clear description of this work, often original phrases were taken from the original papers without specially labeling them, because mostly they are optimal with respect to information density. The physiological basis of blinking is simple: two antagonistic muscles, the levator palpebrae superioris (LPS) and orbicularis oculi (OO) muscles, participate in eyelid movements during blinking (Evinger, Manning, & Sibony 1991; Esteban, Traba, & Prieto 2004); turning off the otherwise tonically active LPS together with bursting OO activity causes a rapid lowering of the upper eyelid. The opposite process with OO silence and LPS activity elevates the eyelid back to the upper position (Esteban & Salinero 1979; Evinger 1995). (Tsubota et al. 1999). A distributed network built up by the primary motor cortex, the visual cortex, the cingulate motor cortex, the posterior parietal cortex, the dorsolateral prefontal cortex, the central thalamus and the cerebellum, which participate in spontaneous as well as in voluntary and reflexive blinking, is active . Protection against corneal drying mainly is guaranteed by the functional role of spontaneous blinking (Evinger et al. 2002) which is avoided by an appropriate tear film distribution over its surface (Evinger 1995; VanderWerf et al. 2007). Spontaneous blinking rates reported so far differ: e.g., from 12/min by King & Michels (1957) up to 24/min by Collins et al. (1989). All these reported blink rates, which are much higher than required to keep the cornea moist, indicates the involvement of various processes different from motor control in the blink mechanism, Moisture is dispersed evenly across the surface of the eyeball and the eye surface of any debris is cleared by the movement of the upper lid over the surface of the eye 3. An eye blink conditioning test that can identify alcohol-exposed children who do not have distinctive fatal alcohol syndrome features has been developed (Sandra W. Jacobson, of Wayne State University School of Medicine (http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2008-02/ace-ebm012808.php). Pavlov I (1849 –1936, he was a physiologist, psychologist, and physician) demonstrated a form of associative learning procedure which involves presentations of a neutral stimulus along with a stimulus of some significance. The neutral stimulus does not result in an overt behavioral response and is called “conditioned stimulus” whereas the significant stimulus necessarily evokes an innate, often reflexive, response and is called “unconditioned stimulus”. Eye blink conditioning (EBC) as a form of classical conditioning has been used broadly for studying neural structures and the underlying memory and learning mechanisms. These mentioned factors mostly reflect cognitive (cortical) aspects, i.e., the central stage of blinking control. Doughty (2001) supported the idea of blinks being controlled by a central pacemaker residing in the basal ganglia. Providing further support for this view, Freudenthaler et al. (2003) observed various blink patterns during video display terminal usage and presumed that at least the frequency of homogenous blink patterns can be based on an endogenous pacemaker, whereas heterogeneous patterns could also originate from a central pacemaker but being modulated by internal as well as external factors. More solid ground to the central or endogenous control explanation came from dopamine hypothesis of blink control assuming that spontaneous blinking rate is a neurobiological measure of dopaminergic activity (Dreisbach et al. 2005; Taylor et al. 1999; cf., van der Post et al. 2004). This supposed link between dopaminergic activity and spontaneous blinking was confirmed in studies of different psychiatric and neurologic patients who suffered from 3 http://www.ehow.com/how-does_5245342_human-eye-blink_.html 55 consequences of the altered dopaminergic activity such as in Parkinson disease, schizophrenia, depression, etc. (Karson 1983; Stevens & Livermore 1978; MacLean et al. 1985). Besides these central factors in spontaneous blink control, peripheral factors exist as well: such as damage of ocular surface (Tsubota et al. 1996), ocular anaesthesia (Collins et al. 1989; Nakamori et al. 1997; Naase, Doughty, & Button 2005), presentation of sensory stimuli to eye surface (Nakamori et al. 1997) and pharmacological substance effects (Dudinski, Finnin, & Reed 1983). Thus, a controversy about dominance of central vs. peripheral factors in blink control evolved, which stimulated a hypothesis on a basic central control being potentially also modulated by the peripheral factors. If exogenous blink stimuli are eliminated (e.g., as a result of anesthesia or in constant environmental conditions), then the central control should become dominant. Furthermore, if a central blink generator with a stationary pace rate is responsible then the generator can determine IBIs, which, however, can show some random fluctuations (Ponder & Kennedy 1927; Naase, Doughty, & Button 2005). 3.1 Fluctuation of blink number during an interval (Greene 1986) Greene (1986) recorded blinking rate of nine subjects (males, ages 18-22) over a 30-s time frame during five different intervals each. During the observation period, the subject was engaged in a question-and-answer type discussion with the interviewer in the format of a political poll. Figure 3-1: A comparison of theory and experiment for nine subjects. Sampling time window is 30 s intervals. Average blink rate for the group is 13.16 blinks per interval. Theoretical curve is the Poisson density with mean of 13. Standard deviation of the theoretical density is ±3.61. Average interblink delay time is 2.28 + 1.35 s. A continuous curve has been faired through the discrete density function for display purposes. The chi-squared goodness of fit test yields significance at the 0.97 level for 15 data classes (13 degrees of freedom) and at the 0.94 significance for 12 classes (10 degrees of freedom). (Taken from Green 1986) The blinking rate distribution was taken during active conversation (Fig. 3-1). Greene found that the number of blinks per time interval and the delay time between blinks can fluctuate considerably about the mean values and suggested that it indicates the need for the Poisson and exponential distributions to describe the phenomenon. He assumed that the blinks are independent and the total number of blinks over a given time period can be modeled using the Poisson probability density 56 function. He used the chi-squared test to compare the Gaussian and Poisson statistics with the experimentally obtained, and confirmed the highly significant agreement. 3.2 Patterns of Blink Rate in Normal Subjects (Bentivoglio et al. 1997) Bentivoglio et al. (1997) measured the normal blink rate (BR) variations in relation to behavioral tasks of 150 healthy volunteers (70 males and 80 females; aged 35.9 f 17.9 years, range 5-87 years). Figure 3-2: Relative frequency of blink rate values at rest (A), during conversation (B), and during reading (C) are fitted with a log-normal distribution. (Taken from Bentivoglio et al. 1997) 57 Figure 3-3: Blink rate values in seven defined age groups during the three behavioural tasks considered. Between-task differences are significant in each age group with the exception of conversation vs. rest at ages 514, 3544, and >65. (Taken from Bentivoglio et al. 1997) Three videotape segments were recorded for 2 min 30 s in the following order: a) free conversation b) reading aloud a passage that required mental and visual concentration c) quiet rest with eye open. ANOVA and student’s tests were used for analysis. The data measured in the three conditions (rest, reading, conversation) had similar distribution curves. Fig. 3-2 shows the best fit for these distributions represented by a log-normal curve. Fig. 3-3 shows blink rate values in seven defined age groups. They found that 67% of the blink rate pattern conversation>rest>reading, 22.7% of rest>conversation>reading and 8.0% conversation>reading>rest. The data measured in the three experimental conditions had similar distribution curves. It was concluded that a log-normal curve was the best curve fit for the data (Fig. 3-2), representing 70% of the measured values at rest and during conversation and 80% of those measured while reading, and the remaining values being best fitted with a normal curve. The idea that blink rate is modulated primarily by central mechanisms and cognitive tasks and that local ocular conditions are of limited relevance is supported. 3.3 Stochastic models for spontaneous blink(Hoshino 1996) Hoshino (1996) proposed one-dimensional stochastic diffusion models for spontaneous eye blink and analyzed the blink burst during low vigilance. The models presumed the interblink interval distribution to be first-passage-time probability densities of the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck process. 58 + Figure 3-4: Interblink histograms and Ornstein-Uhlenbeck-first-passage-time densities (Taken from Hoshino 1996) To estimate the parameters, he optically measured the behavior of upper eyelids with infrared LED and CdS when the vigilance of the 6 subjects was high and low, and transformed into point series of interblink intervals by peak-picking technique. Fig. 3-4 shows two examples of interblink histograms fitted with the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck first passage-time probability distribution function. The upper diagram represents the data during high vigilance and the lower during low vigilance. It is assumed that the spontaneous interblink intervals are generated in accordance with a renewal process formed by the first passage times Ts of a potential X of a virtual blink generator to a threshold potential S. The blink potential is a one-dimensional diffusion process, the value of which is reset to the resting potential X0 at the moment corresponding to the time of the previous response generation. A good agreement with the model of the interblink distribution was confirmed. 3.4 Model for audiomotor integration (Bangert et al. 2006) Bangert et al. (2006) performed an eye blink conditioning procedure to study the coupling of the auditory and motor domains in non-musicians (NM) and professional pianists (PP). In the conditioning procedure, a short airpuff against the cornea is used as the unconditioned stimulus (US), and the eye blink represents the unconditioned reaction; a tone preceding the airpuff is used as the conditioned stimulus (CS), and eye blink to the tone serves as the conditioned reaction. The fundamental frequencies of the five different tones (c', d', e', f', g') were 1046.5 Hz, 1174.7 Hz, 1318.5 Hz, 1396.9 Hz, and 1568.0 Hz. The CS-US latencies were varied (200 ms, 400 ms, 800 ms, and 1000 ms). Pianists acquired an implicit knowledge of the organization of key-pitch associations on a piano keyboard and may be considered as a conditioned reflex in itself with the tone as the unconditioned stimulus. . As the unconditioned reaction the sensation of the tone, as the conditioned stimulus the visual and tactile features of the keyboard, as the conditioned reaction the mental image of the tone were considered. As a model for overlearned audiomotor integration served 17 pianists, 14 non-musicians were instructed to respond to auditory stimuli (piano tones) during the training session. Subjects performed keystrokes on a silent piano during a subsequent testing session. About 70 random stimuli out of five tones used during auditory condition were presented in the training session (session 0) to evaluate the eye blink baseline. In session of auditory conditioning (session 1) 100 presentations of the randomized tones (ISI (Interstimulus Interval): 4000 1500 ms) were used and a visual distractor stimulus with an ISI of 1300 ms was delivered. German words for red, yellow, blue, white were colored by corresponding color and presented randomly 59 (ISI=1300 ms) in a randomized sequence. Subjects had to count every colored word during a session, but, depending on the background of the screen (dark grey or black), to attend either to the actual color, or the semantic content of the words, respectively. This attention shift was prompted every 60 seconds. Session 2 was silent tapping requiring subjects to press the piano key without sound about every 4-5 s. Figure 3-5: Example of group-averaged eye blink signal for the Auditory Conditioning session (left) and the Silent Tapping session (right). Example of a group-averaged time series of normalized event-related eye blink signal for the Auditory Conditioning session (left) and the Silent Tapping session (right)(n = 9). Non-musicians (NM) are depicted in the upper and professional pianists (PP) in the lower panel. The red curve is the response to the target tone and the yellow curve is the response to nontarget tones. t = 0 (dashed line) refers to tone (CS) onset or keystroke, respectively. The dotted line marks the onset of the airpuff (US, at t = 400 ms), but note that only during the Auditory Conditioning the US was present. Peaks with a distance of more than standard deviation from baseline (SD curves not shown) have been labelled T (twitches) and B (blinks), followed by a number indicating the latency from event onset (e.g. "1" = 100 ms), or the relative latency from US onset (subscript US for unified nomenclature despite varying CS-US latencies). NB: (1) In the average, n(nontargets) = 4*n(target) applies. (2) In the PP Silent tapping condition, peaks received the labels T0 and Tus0 because the majority of the individual spikes coincided with the reference time (compare Fig. 3-6), although the peak of the averaged time series appears at 100 ms offset. (Taken from Bangert et al. 2006) 60 Figure 3-6: Peri-Stimulus Time Histograms. PSTHs of eye blinks (light green and light blue) and eyelid twitches (dark green and dark blue) during the three experimental sessions (n = 5). Baseline session 0: left column; Auditory Conditioning session 1: centre column; Silent Tapping session 2: right column. The histogram bins were 40 ms wide; bars are stacked. The only condition with the US (airpuff) actually present have been highlighted in green (Please note that the y-axis [events per bin] in these green panels have been scaled down by a factor of 10 for display reasons, as the aversive stimulus generates a highly time-locked response in 100% of the presentations, thus creating much higher event counts in the respective time bin). The nontarget presentations (± 1, ± 2, ± 3, ± 4) have not been collapsed to one histogram, but have been ordered in four different rows in the graph with respect to their perceptual 'distance' to the target, i.e. frequency distance in the auditory session, and spatial distance on the piano keyboard in the motor session, respectively. The category "± 1" designates the neighbouring key on the keyboard (to the left and right, respectively). The maximum distance to the target within the 5-tone-space is ± 4. Non-musicians (NM) are depicted in the upper 5 rows and professional pianists (PP) in the lower five rows. t = 0 (dotted line) refers to tone (CS) onset or keystroke, respectively. The dashed line marks the onset of the airpuff (US, at t = 200 ms). Note that only during the Auditory Conditioning and only for the target tone the US was present (Green panels). Please note the presence of the two twitch-related peaks during session 2 in the Pianist group. (Taken from Bangert et al. 2006) 61 Figure 3-7: Overall Excitability (ratio of the number of trials containing a positive response to the total number of trials). Excitability of eye blink events in the two experimental conditions. Red: NM group; Yellow: PP group. (Taken from Bangert et al. 2006) Figure 3-8: Sensitivity. Eye blink Sensitivity d' for the two groups in sessions 1 and 2. d' was high in the conditioning session due to the presence of the US. In session 2, d' drops to a small value indicate no specificity for the key related to the target tone. In any part of the experiment, no sensitivity difference between the groups is observed. NB: The graph shows eye blink sensitivity only. Twitches, however, display an equally low d' in both sessions 1 and 2. Red: NM group; Yellow: PP group. Inset: Correlation of d' with the US-CS delay in session 1. A positive correlation is present in the non-musician group (upper panel, r = 0.8, p < 0.05) but not in the musician group (lower panel, r = 0.4, p = n.s.). In session 2 (not shown), no positive correlation is found in either group. (Taken from Bangert et al. 2006) The results of the subject group with CS-US interval 400 ms are shown in Fig. 3-5 as an example and entire dataset of the 200 ms group in Fig. 3-6 (the panels for the other CS-US delays are similar). Ratio of the number of trials with positive response to the total number of trials was calculated and shown in Fig. 3-7. The hit rates and the alarm rate are shown in Fig. 3-8. The silent tapping session revealed a higher total likelihood of blinking in the pianist group, i.e. after classical conditioning to a sensory stimulus involuntary reflex responses are elicited by a voluntary motor action, which through long-term training is arbitrarily associated with the conditioning stimulus. 62 3.5 Effect of mental task on eye blink rate (Karson et al. 1981) Karson et al. (1981) studied how the spontaneous eye blink rate in 36 normal subjects are affected by several mental tasks. Blink rates were measured in the following order: 1) casual conversation with the examiner 2) silence 3) silence with simultaneous gum chewing 4) proverb interpretation 5) memorization of paragraph 6) blink suppression 7) speeded blinking, and 8) conversation with the examiner. 41 subjects were asked to read a handed card listing the proverb and 4 lettered choices silently but say the letter of the correct interpretation. For the remainder of the proverb task, the examiner read 5 proverbs aloud and asked the subject for an oral interpretation of each. The examiner read a paragraph aloud, and the subject recounted it verbatim as best he could. Subjects were asked to stop blinking as long as possible for suppression and to blink as fast as possible for speeding. Figure 3-9: Mean blink rate for each task. (Taken from Karson et al. 1981) The mean blink rates measured during various tasks are given in Fig. 3-9. One-way ANOVA was used for the overall comparison of the means. The results showed that tasks requiring speech and listening to a paragraph to be memorized are associated with an increased whereas Reading with a reduced blink rate. 3.6 Mapping cortical areas with functional MRI (Tsubota el al. 1999) To examine the complicated process associated with vision-related functions Tsubota el al. (1999) used Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) mapping cortical areas that control eye blink to detect changes caused by focal variations in blood oxygenation. 63 Figure 3-10: A conventional T1-weighted sagittal head image showing the orientation of the imaging plane and the planned number of fMRI images (16 in the present case). Each image/slice was 7 mm thick and the slice identification number is used to identify the corresponding axial fMRI image shown in Fig. 3-11. (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) Fig. 3-10 shows the conventional T1-weighted imaging of the head. Eight volunteers took part in their study. Two of them were dry eye patients. (Eyes closed)- (blink or blink inhibition) as a two step sequence in three cycles was contained in the experimental scheme. Participants lay supine on the scanner bed and were asked to keep the eyes closed for 1 min (control phase) followed by blinking for another minute (experimental phase), then the ‘same eye closed 1 min and blink 1 min’ repeated once or more times. The investigator dictated the blink rate to the subjects. In inhibition phase participants were asked not to blink or at most one blink within the 1-min interval if the inhibition could not be sustained. 64 Figure 3-11: A full series of fMRI images showing brain areas activated by normal blinking in the same volunteer as in Fig. 3-10 (baseline : eyes closed). This series starts at top left (i.e., image No. 1), ending at bottom right (i.e., image No. 16). The colorcoded P-map scale is shown on the right margin (in 10−n). Both the anterior portion of the visual cortex (images Nos 5 and 6) and the orbitofrontal cortex (images Nos 15 and 16) showed activation. (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) 65 Figure 3-12: Brain activation as a function of eye blinking of the same volunteer in Fig. 3-10 (baseline: eyes closed and experimental: normal blinking). Top left image and bottom graph: orbitofrontal area activation (redyellow areas in image and in graph); and top right and bottom graph: anterior visual cortex activation (redblue areas at bottom of image and in the graph). The periodicity corresponding to the control and experimental phases can be seen clearly in the orbitofrontal cortex although less obvious in the visual cortex. (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) Figure 3-13: Activation of orbitofrontal areas by normal blinking (right image and in graph) and blinking inhibition (left image and in graph), both of which were compared with the baseline control of eyes closed. (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) 66 Table 3.1: Brain activation as a result of blinking inhibition with no light perception. (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) Figure 3-14: fMRI images of the anterior portion of the visual cortex in a dry eye volunteer (Table 3.1, ID No. 8). (Top): with (left) and without (right) blinking inhibition. Yellow rectangles are cursors showing corresponding areas of interest in the visual cortex of the two images. (Bottom): After topical anaesthesia: with (left) and without (right) blinking inhibition. Notice the visual cortex activation (left images: the hyperintense regions) during blink inhibition (see also table 3.1). (Taken from Tsubota et al. 1999) The three separate areas of activation (left, right, and central) contained in the orbitofrontal cortex are shown in Fig. 3-11. An example of orbitofrontal and anterior visual cortex activation of the same volunteer is shown in Fig. 3-12. The activity of central area is reduced while the activity of the bilateral orbitofrontal areas is increased. Visual cortex activation was much larger with voluntary blink inhibition (Fig. 3-14). Areas in the orbitofrontal cortex and in some cases, the primary visual cortex and the visual cortex including the anterior portion of the visual cortex were activated by normal blinking in all subjects. The visual vortex was strongly activated in dry eye patients when blink was inhibited. Especially the blink rate appeared to be controlled in the orbitofrontal cortex. The results strongly indicate that the orbitofrontal cortex is the primary site of blink control especially the blink rate. 67 3.7 The neural representation of temporal information (Ivry & Spencer 2004) Ivry & Spencer (2004) summarized recent investigation of temporal processing in a review. They concluded that cerebellum is engaged during tasks requiring the precise representation of temporal information such as sequence learning, rhythmic tapping, duration discrimination, phoneme perception, and attentional anticipation. Figure 3-15: Hypothesized gating operation of the basal ganglia as part of a decision making process. (a) Potentiated cortical representations provide input to the basal ganglia. The output from the basal ganglia reflects selected representations that have reached threshold. (From Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun (2002), art work by F Forney.) (b) The functional consequences of this gating process will depend on input–output circuitry (Alexander & Crutcher 1990). For example, the motor loop will trigger overt movements, whereas the prefrontal loop involves the updating of working memory. (Taken from Ivry & Spencer 2004) Figure 3-16: Gating of activated representations through threshold adjustment. The green line represents the activation signal that serves as an input to the basal ganglia. Drop-lines indicate time of gating for a particular threshold setting (“DAant”,”Normal”. “DAag”) (a) Dopamine agonists lower the threshold, leading to the gating operation being invoked with less activation. Dopamine antagonists raise the threshold. This mechanism can be applied to understand the effects of dopamine depletion in Parkinson’s disease (PD) or the effects of dopamine-based reinforcement. For the latter, reinforcement signals serve to lower thresholds, leading to increased probability of an input reaching threshold in the future. (b) Tendency of PD patients to speed up during unpaced finger tapping could result from short-term modulation of elevated thresholds. After each output, the system resets and a new activation signal accrues for the next response. The gaps indicate that the input to the gating mechanism might not be immediate, but builds up near the target time, reflecting activation in upstream systems that determine onset time (e.g. cerebellum). Assuming that variation in the activation function is random, gating will tend to occur earlier as the threshold is reduced over cycles. (Modified from Ivry & Spencer 2004) Imaging studies provided sufficient arguments and lesion studies stronger test. Increased temporal variability is associated with lesions of the cerebellum. The recruiting of subregions within the cerebellar cortex for timing was assumed. The basal ganglia operate as a threshold mechanism and are an integral part of decision processes (Fig. 3-15). In the basal ganglia gated activations reaching threshold are implemented. Threshold settings are modulated by dopamine which inputs to the striatum (Fig. 3-16). The likelihood of reinforced actions to be implemented is increased by the 68 lowered threshold even if the input patterns are unchanged. Dopamine agonists would lower thresholds and dopamine antagonists would raise it. Figure 3-17: Mean produced interval durations for group data as a function of ordinal position in each condition averaged across the two slightly different duration ratios (integer and non-integer), and synchronise and continue phases. Square and triangle symbols show the target intervals for the two rhythm conditions with slightly differing ratios ((a) 321 and 858 ms (in ratio 1:2.7) or (b) 282 and 936 ms (1:3.3)). Diamond symbols indicate the target interval for the isochronous condition. Error bars indicate the within group standard deviation for producing each interval. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) 69 Figure 3-18: Measures for accuracy for group data averaged across integer and non-integer, for both synchronize and continue phases of the task. Error bars indicate the within group standard error of the mean for each condition. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) Figure 3-19: The mean coefficient of variation (CV) for group data averaged across integer and non-integer, for both synchronize and continue phases of the task. Error bars indicate the within group standard error of the mean for each condition. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) 70 Figure 3-20: Functional activity in response to parametric modelling of initiate (red) and synchronise (blue) phases with areas of overlap shown in green. Data was thresholded at P < 0.01. The slices shown were taken at sagittal: −5, 10, 26, 34 mm; axial: 40, 50, 60, 70 mm; coronal: −33, −10, 7, 37 mm. The figure is in radiological convention such that the L side corresponds to R and vice versa, the white dividing lines show the location of the anterior commissure in some views, letters refer to specific structures: (A) SMA; (B) preSMA; (C) dPMC; (D) DLPFC/dPMC; (E)DLPFC. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) 71 Table 3.2: MNI coordinates for the highest local maxima of BOLD activity found in each functional area associated with the synchronize > continue (A); continue> synchronize (B) contrasts. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) 72 Table 3.2 (continued) Columns show the coordinates in millimetres from the anterior commissure, the z-score value of each local max, laterality, and an anatomical description of the point’s location on the SPM canonical brain. SFG: superior frontal gyrus, SFS: superior frontal sulcus, MFG: middle frontal gyrus, MFS: middle frontal sulcus, IFG: inferior frontal gyrus, IFS: inferior frontal sulcus, STG: superior temporal gyrus, TTG: transverse temporal gyrus, IIPCS: inferior portion of inferior precentral sulcus, SSPCS: superior portion of superior precentral sulcus, ISPCS: inferior portion of superior precentral sulcus, VVPCS ventral portion of ventral precentral sulcus, SOG: superior occipital gyrus, MOG: middle occipital gyrus, IOG: inferior occipital gyrus. (Taken from Lewis et al. 2004) 3.8 How brain activity correlates with temporal complexity (Lewis et al. 2004) Lewis et al. (2004) searched for activity correlating with temporal complexity using fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging). Images were thresholded using clusters determined by Zscore and cluster significance. Ten participants were involved in a production of temporal rhythms by tapping with the right index finger on a force sensor. The task was divided into three phases: (1) movement selection and initiation for an overlearned tapping, (2) synchronization of finger tapping with an external auditory cue, and (3) continued tapping in absence of the auditory pacer. They initially tapped in time with a sequence of auditory cues to identify it and then 18 s for synchronization. They had to maintain their tapping accurately for 18 s after the cues were ceased. The stimulus set comprised one isochronous pattern of repeating 500 ms and three set of multiinterval rhythms. In two further control conditions participants were required to maintain fixation and to press a button in response to tones heard at two unpredictable intervals. Fig. 3-17 shows the mean intervals in each rhythm produced by each subject during fMRI session. Mean performance of target intervals was significantly better for isochronous sequences than for the three rhythmic conditions (Fig. 3-18). The coefficient of variation (CV) for each interval was lower for isochronous sequences than for the three rhythmic conditions (Fig. 3-19). Fig. 3-20 shows the clusters of significant fMRI activity rendered onto the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) canonical brain. The results showed the greater activation of bilateral Supplementary Motor Cortex (SMA) and basal ganglia in continuation tapping than in synchronization tapping (Fig. 3-20 and table 3.2). Temporal complexity task revealed activity in bilateral supplementary and pre-supplementary motor cortex (SMA and preSMA) during initiation phase, rostral dorsal premotor cortex (PMC), basal ganglia, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), among other areas. Right primary motor cortex, right DLPFC, ventral PMC and more caudal dorsal, and bilateral SMA showed correlated activity during synchronization phase but not during continuation phase. They suggested that during initiation phase selection of timing parameters is the reason for the preSMA and rostral dorsal PMC activities while temporal error monitoring or correction for centromedial prefrontal cortex during both initiation and synchronization phase. Further adjustment of the timing control processes related to its continued production in absence of external cues might be not needed after timed movement sequence has been overlearned. This resulted in the significantly absence of activity during synchronization phase. 73 3.9 The role of supplementary motor area in moving preparation (Jenskin et al. 2000) Using PTE (Positron Emission Tomography) scanning Jenkins et al. (2000) tested the postulation about the responsibility of preparation preceding self-initiated and predictably cued movements for equivalent levels of SMA (Supplementary Motor Area) activation in these two conditions. The measure values of the distribution of radioactivity following the intravenous injection of the positron-emitting tracer H2O was used as an index of relative regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). Statistical parametric mapping (SPM) was used to perform statistical analysis of rCBF images. Z-score and cluster significance were approached to determine clusters for thresholding the images. Six raised briskly the finger above a force touch sensor to made self-initiated right index finger extensions and then returned the finger to the sensor in the first condition. An audible tone followed the break of the contact. The extension movements were performed at random intervals between 2 and 7 s. Participants made the same movements of the generated intervals from the first condition which were replayed in response to the tones. In the third condition subjects made no movements after attending the same generated tones. Figure 3-21: SPM projections of the sites of significantly increased rCBF for the comparison of self-initiated movements with rest (threshold at P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons). The activated areas are shown projected onto single sagittal, coronal and transverse planes conforming to the stereotactic atlas of Talairach & Tournoux (1988). For the purposes of illustration, the equivalent SPMs from the related study of Johanshahi et al. (1995) (B) are shown alongside the data from the current study (A), demonstrating a very similar pattern of activation in both. VPC = vertical line through the posterior commissure. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) 74 Table 3.3: Significant rCBF increases during self-initiated movement compared with rest. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) Figure 3-22: SPM projections of the sites of significantly increased rCBF for the comparison of unpredictably triggered movements with rest (threshold at P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons). The activated areas are shown projected onto single sagittal, coronal and transverse planes conforming to the stereotactic atlas of Talairach & Tournoux (1988). For the purposes of illustration, the equivalent SPMs from the related study of Johanshahi et al. (1995) (B) are shown alongside the data from the current study (A), illustrating that while predictable triggering results in extensive activation of SMA and adjacent anterior cingulate cortex, unpredictable triggering results in little activation of mesial frontal cortex while activation of contralateral primary sensorimotor cortex and striatum is preserved. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) 75 Figure 3-23: SPM projections of the sites of significantly increased rCBF for the comparison of self-initiated movements with triggered movements (threshold at P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons). The activated areas are shown projected onto single sagittal, coronal and transverse planes conforming to the stereotactic atlas of Talairach & Tournoux (1988). For the purposes of illustration, the equivalent SPMs from the related study of Johanshahi et al. (1995) (B) are shown alongside the data from the current study (A), showing that when triggered movements are predictable, significant differences are limited to the DLPFC (in this case only in the right hemisphere), but when they are unpredictable, differences are also found in mesial frontal and parietal cortex. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) Figure 3-24: SPM projections of the sites of significantly increased rCBF for the triggered movements compared with self-initiated movements (threshold at P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons). The activated areas are shown projected onto single sagittal, coronal and transverse planes conforming to the stereotactic atlas of Talairach & Tournoux (1988). (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) Table 3.4: significant rCBF increases during externally triggered movement compared with rest. (Taken from Jenkins) 76 Table 3.5: significant rCBF increases during self-initiated movement compared with externally triggered movement. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) Foci of significant change in rCBF for comparison indicated in the table title. The coordinates in a standard stereotactic space (Talairach & Tournoux 1988) are given in mm in x, y, and z for the maximally significant pixel in each area, where x is the lateral displacement from the midline (- for left hemisphere); y is the anteroposterior displacement relative to the anterior commisure (AC) (- posterior (PC) to this); and z is the vertical position relative to the AC – PC line (- if below this). The vertical extent of each area activated (in mm relative to the AC – PC line) is tabulated, and the level of significance is given by the Z score (where Z is the standard deviation of the standard normal distribution). The mean percentage rCBF increase compared with the rest is given for each area, measured at the pixel of maximal significance. L = left hemisphere; R = right hemisphere; CMAr = rostral cingulated premotor area; CMAd = dorsal cingulated motor area. Table 3.6: significant rCBF increases during externally triggered movement compared with self-initiated movement. (Taken from Jenkins et al. 2000) The axial extent of the areas activated, the peak Z scores and the percentage increases in rCBF are given in table 3.3 for comparison of self-initiated versus rest, in table 3.4 for triggered versus rest, in table 3.5 for self-initiated versus triggered, and in table 3.6 for triggered versus self-initiated. The SPM projections of this comparison are shown in Fig. 3-21 for comparison of self- initiated versus rest, in Fig. 3-22 for triggered versus rest, in Fig. 3-23 for self-initiated versus triggered, and in Fig. 324 for triggered versus self-initiated. The results showed that contralateral primary sensorimotor cortex, caudal SMA and contralateral putamen were activated in unpredictably cued movements compared with the rest condition. Rostral SMA, adjacent anterior cingulated cortex and bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) additionally were activated in self-initiated movements compared with cued movements. The primary role of rostral SMA in movement preparation and of caudal SMA as motor executive area are suggested. DLPFC was activated only when decisions were required about the timing of movements during the self-initiated task 3.10 Investigation of the daily pattern of eye-blink rate (Barbato et al. 2000) Barbato et al. (2000) assess blink rates of 24 healthy subjects at different times of the day. Subjects were asked to sit silently in front of a blank, neutral wall. They recorded vertical and 77 horizontal electro-oculograms (EOGs) on a polygraph. Subjects also performed a blink-suppression test (BST) trying to avoid blinking for the longest time possible. Table 3.7: Relationship between eye-blink rate (BR).and blink-suppression times (BST). BR: mean number of blinks during two consecutive minutes. BST: time interval from the end of eye blink to the first eye movement occurring during a blink suppression task. Blink suppression time was not available in the record of one of the subjects. Rho: Significant negative correlations between eye-blink rates and ability to suppress eye blinks; P: significant level (ANOVA). (Taken from Barbato et al. 2000) Figure 3-25: Diurnal average profiles of blink rate blink suppression time, slow eye movements and Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. (Taken from Barbato et al. 2000) One-way ANOVA was used to assess the changes across time of blink parameters and sleepiness. Blink rate increased significantly across the day (Fig. 3-25). No significant changes in eye-blink suppression time and slow eye movements were found. A decreasing trend at the evening recording time point was recognizable for eye-blink suppression. Significant negative correlations between eyeblink and ability to suppress eye blinks were found only at two time points (13.30 and 20.30 h) of four time points examined (10.00, 17.00) (table 3.7). The finding suggested an increase of central dopamine activity in the late evening. The counteracting of the rising sleep drive against the dopamine-mediated activation is the possible hypothesis reflected in the “forbidden zone for sleep”. In the evaluation of neuropsychological and biological parameters, and in the choice of the drug treatment regimen the role of diurnal variation of dopamine function should be taken into account. 78 3.11 A brain Stem Reflex in Eye Blink (Evinger 1995) Additional to transient lid movements by blinking, lid movements accompany eye movements evolved so that the lid-closing muscle governs blinks and the lid-opening muscle determines lid position. To avoid damage to the eye reflex blinks lowers the upper eye lid. Reflex blinks also occur in response to drying to maintain tear film continuity on the cornea by spreading tears. Primates produce spontaneous blinks in the absence of sensory stimuli. This blink rate is much higher than that required to keep the cornea moist. Figure 3-26: Movements of upper eyelid and their kinematics in humans. A: reflex blink evoked by electrical stimulation of supraorbital branch of trigeminal nerve (▲, SO stim). Two bursts of orbicularis oculi electromyogram (OOemg, RI and R2) activity rapidly lower upper eyelid, which rises slowly after end of OOemg activity. Pos: position of upper eyelid; Vel: velocity of upper eyelid. B: family of upward and downward lid saccades that accompany upward and downward saccadic eye movements. Unlike the case with blinks, up and down maximum lid saccade velocities are nearly equal. C: maximum velocity the lid achieves as a function of amplitude of lid movement for lid lowering during a blink (blink down) and with lid saccades (saccade down) and lid raising during blinks (blink up) and with lid saccades (saccade up). (Taken from Barbato et al. 2000) Blink amplitude and blink rate are changing dramatically with emotional state and cognitive tasks. A task which requires vigilance reduces spontaneous blinking. Downward lid movements accompany downward eye movements. Blinks exhibit a very rapid downward movement followed by a slower up phase (Fig. 3-26A). For both upward and downward lid movements lid saccades reached in contrast nearly equal velocities (Fig. 3-26B). For both down and up lid saccades and down and up phases of a blink the relationship between the amplitude of the lid movement and the maximum velocity is unique (Fig 3-26C). For blink up phases and upward lid saccades the best-fitting linear relationship between maximum velocity and amplitude are virtually identical 79 Figure 3-27: Interactions of passive downward forces and active contractions of lid-closing orbicularis oculi (OO) and lid-raising levator palpebrae (LP) muscles in producing blinks and lid saccades. A: muscle and ligament stretching that occurs when going from eyelid closed state (closed) to eyelid open state (open). B: lid position (pos) and rectified EMG activity of levator palpebrae superioris and orbicularis oculi muscles with an up lid saccade, a down lid saccade, and a blink. APO: aponeurosis of LP; PL: palpebral ligaments; STL: superior transverse ligament. (Taken from Evinger 1995) Force producing movement of the upper eyelid The movements of the upper eyelid can be explained by the interaction of the following four forces: 1) the spring-like orbicularis oculi (OO) muscle wrapping around the upper and lower eyelids like a purse string generates a downward force, 2) the levator palpebrae superioris (LP) muscle originating at the back of the bony orbit exerts an upward force and inserts on the lower margin of the upper eyelid, 3) stretching tendons and ligaments connected to the LP produces a passive downward force, and 4) a smooth muscle bridging the belly of the LP and its tendon produces an upward force (Fig 327A). Energy state is lowest when eyelids are closed (Fig. 3-27A). Several structures having springlike properties are stretched by the contraction of LP when opening eyelids. The more LP contraction the greater the stretching and the larger the passive downward forces created. A higher level of tonic activity accords a burst of activity is produced by the LP (Fig. 3-27B).The lid is moved quickly to a more elevated position by this burst and is hold in place against the increased downward forces by the new tonic activity. Both the OO and LP participate in blinking (Fig. 3-27B). The 4.force acts on the upper eyelid and produces very small effects on the blink or lid saccades. 80 Figure 3-28: Orbicularis oculi EMG (OOemg) activity evoked by light flashes of different intensities and durations in an alert albino rabbit. A: while stimulus duration was held constant at 25 ms, lights of 3 different 3 intensities were flashed in the eye. B: while intensity was held constant at 5 X 1 O /ft-L, lights of 3 different durations were flashed in the rabbit’s eye. Each trace is mean of 5 rectified OOemg responses. (Taken from Evinger 1995) Figure 3-29: Self-Inhibition in human blink reflex. A: presenting 2 identical electrical stimuli to supraorbital branch of trigeminal nerve (▲, SO Stim) with an inter-stimulus interval of 500 ms significantly reduces magnitude of blink (lid pos) and orbicularis oculi EMG (OOemg) activity in response to 2nd stimulus relative to 1 st. B: magnitude of 2nd, test R2 component of the OOemg activity divided by magnitude of the 1 st, condition R2 activity, as a function of time between condition and test stimuli for 3 normal human subjects. Even at 1 s, OOemg activity remains reduced. Each point is mean of at least 5 trials. C: a patient with hemifacial spasm who has lost much of his blink self-inhibition so that a voluntary blink initiates a spasm of blinks that holds lid closed. Lid Pos: position of upper eyelid; Lid Vel: velocity of upper eyelid. (Taken from Evinger 1995) 81 Blink reflex circuit in the brain To create reflex blinks at least two parallel long and short circuits are involved. They exhibit different physiological characteristics and produce distinct components of OO motoneuron activity. The initial burst of OO motoneuron activity is generated by the short-latency circuit (Fig. 3-26A R1, Fig. 3-28 initial response). A specific magnitude of OO motoneuron is produced by a stimulus. Even though the well occurring of the entire response after completion of the stimulus, with duration constant causes the increasing of stimulus amplitude larger responses from the short-latency circuit (Fig. 3-28A). Even though the longer lasting of the stimulus than the short-latency circuit response while leaving its intensity constant the increasing of stimulus duration produces no amplitude change of the initial response. (Fig 3-28B). A larger response at a significantly longer latency in contrast is produced by the long-latency circuit than the short one (Fig. 3-26A R2, Fig. 3-28, later response). The response magnitude generated by the long-latency circuit is increased by lengthened stimulus duration with hold constant amplitude but the change of the response of the short one is not significant (Fig. 3-28B). Ensuing reflex blinks are suppressed (Fig. 3-29A) in a “refractory period” which is initiated by blink evoked by wind rushes. The suppression process gradually recovers over a 1-to-2s period (Fig. 3-29B). The self-inhibition after each blink is the reason that a saccade of blinks is not initiated. A spasm of lid closure can e simply produced by a blink when the nervous system loses or reduces this self-inhibition. 3.12 The Blink Recovery Process in Patients with bell’s Palsy (VanderWerf et al. 2007) VanderWerf et al. (2007) examined whether the adaptive changes and normal kinematical values of eyelid movement during blinking of patients with Bell’s palsy will be reached after recovery. Figure 3-30: Simultaneously recorded OO-EMG activities, upper eyelid and eye movements during blinking in one subject. (A) Superposition of six successive traces of voluntary blinks and their OO-EMG activities. (B) Superposition of six successive traces of palsied corneal airpuff-induced blinks and their OO-EMG activities. Lines 1 and 2: OO-EMG activities in the palsied and nonpalsied eyelid, respectively; lines 3 and 5: vertical displacement; lines 4 and 6: the horizontal eyelid displacement at palsied and nonpalsied sides; lines 7 and 9: represent the vertical eye displacement at palsied and nonpalsied sides; and lines 8 and 10: represent the horizontal eye displacement at palsied and nonpalsied sides. Note the abnormal vertical displacement (lines 7 and 9) and the large horizontal displacement (lines 8 and 10) of the eyes during voluntary blinking. The bar in front of lines 1 and 2 corresponds with a 200 µV OO-EMG signal. Vertical bar in front of lines 3 to 10 corresponds with a 50° rotation; duration bar: 100 ms. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 82 Table 3.8: OO-EMG and eyelid kinematics at four moments during recovery of voluntary and reflex blinking. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 83 Figure 3-31: Profiles of six superimposed successive traces of simultaneous recorded eyelid and eye movement during voluntary and reflex blinking measured from the same patient as in Fig. 3-30. (A) Eyelid movement during voluntary blinking recorded at the onset of the affliction. (B, C) Eyelid and eye movement during voluntary blinking recorded at 30 and 72 weeks, respectively. (D) Eyelid movement after a corneal airpuff on the palsied side recorded at the onset of the affliction. (E, F) Eyelid and eye movement after a corneal airpuff on the palsied side recorded at 30 at 72 weeks, respectively. Eyelid movement motility remained impaired in both types of blinking throughout the study. Both eyes move in the same abnormal direction during voluntary blinking, whereas in reflex blinking the direction of eye movement is normal after 72 weeks; however, the amplitude on the palsied side remains smaller. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 84 Table 3.9: Differences in OO-EMG and eyelid kinematics between nonpalsied and palsied sides. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 85 Table 3.10: Eye Movement Parameters after 30 and 72 Weeks of Recovery. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 86 Figure 3-32: Schematic representations of OO-EMG and eyelid kinematics during recovery. Shown are eyelid movement start time (A), duration of the up phase (B), maximum amplitude (C), maximum velocity (D), and time maximum velocity (E), along with the amplitude (F), the summed amplitudes (G), and the start time (H) of the OO-EMG over the recovery time. Light blue: voluntary blinking, red: corneal air-puff–induced blinking on the nonpalsied side; green: corneal air-puff–induced blinking on the palsied side; purple: acoustic-click–induced blinking. Thick lines: values on the palsied side; thin lines: values on the nonpalsied side. In (A), (E), and (H), values from voluntary blinking (light blue) are absent, as an exact start time of a “trigger” cannot be determined for this type of blinking. (Taken from VanderWerf et al. 2007) 87 Between all types of blinks a significant difference in eyelid kinematics is shown in Table 3.8. The OO-EMG activity in the palsied eyelid was absent at the onset of the affliction. Both eye and eyelid movements were disturbed. Within the first 18 weeks there was no recovery of eyelid movement on the palsied side. A difference of eyelid motility (Fig. 3-30A, 3-30B, 3-30C, 3-30F) between eyelids was found at the end of study. During voluntary blinking eye movements were still impaired (Fig. 3-31C). The movement start times of both eyelids showed a clear time delay in the first 18 weeks (Fig. 332A, table 3.8). The start time of the nonpalsied eyelid was slightly shorted until 1 year. The start times of both eyes were not significantly different. In voluntary and air-puff conditions the prolongation of the downphase duration of the palsied side was found. Except for acoustic condition the upphase duration of the nonpalsied eyelid was always longer than that of the palsied one (Fig. 3-32B, table 3.8). On the palsied side the maximum amplitude and velocity significantly increased at least 84 weeks (Fig. 3-32C, 3-32D, table 3.8). The largest increase followed by a small continuous increase for the air-puff condition on the palsied side. The time of maximum amplitude and velocity on the palsied side shortened after 18 weeks until the end of the study (Fig. 3-32E, table 3.8). Voluntary blinks revealed the largest difference between the downphase duration of palsied and nonpalsied eyelids at the end of study (table 3.9). The OO-EMG of the palsied eyelid was continuously decreased in reflex blinks. In reflex blinking, after 1 year OOEMG of the palsied eyelid was reset and showed values similar to the OO-EMG of the nonpalsied eyelid (Fig. 3-32F). Independent of the stimulation side the decrease of the integrated OO-EMG of the nonpalsied eyelid was found until 18 weeks. Throughout the study the sum of the integrated OOEMG of both eyelids was almost constant (Fig. 3-32G). Until the end of study the start time of the OO-EMG was measurable in reflex blinks (Fig. 3-32H, table 3.8). In all type of blinking, the maximum amplitude of eye movements on the palsied side always remained two times smaller than that on the nonpalsied side (table 3.10). Throughout the study the velocities, the maximum amplitudes and the eyelid movement remained smaller on the palsied side. During the study the impairment of eye movements in voluntary condition is consistent and motility of eyelid is reduced. These indicate the altering of the controlling higher brain structures caused by the affliction 3.13 Summary The distribution of the temporal relationship between one stochastic process and the constant periodic one can be mathematically figured out. If the interblink intervals are described by a stochastic process (Greene 1986; Hoshino 1996; Bentivoglio et al. 1997) then it is interesting to compare the observed phases describing the temporal relationship between tapping and blinking with those observed for blinking without tapping. In musicians a tight coupling of motor domains and the auditory even on sub-cogniive processing levels was suggested (Bangert et al. 2006). If there is also a tight coupling of the rhythm in music cognition, then the tapping would elicit an eye blink response more often than in non-musicians. During silence the blink rate was significantly lower than during speech (Karson et al. 1981). How is the concurrency between the effects of speech and other factors and the effects of tapping on blinking? The significance of visual cortex activation may be associated with attention and arousal (Tsubota et al. 1999). The prePMC and rostral dorsal PMC activities correlated with temporal complexity are absent during overlearned tapping (continuation phase) (Lewis et al. 2004). Tapping with required timing involves attention and arousal. The basal ganglia are an integral part of decision processes, operating as a threshold mechanism and dopamine inputs to the striatum modulate threshold settings (Ivry & Spencer 2004). A rising sleep drive 88 counteracting dopamine mediated activation may be reflected in the “forbidden zone for sleep” (Barbato et al. 2000). Central dopaminergic activity indicated by dopamine gene polymorphisms and spontaneous eye blink rate plays an important role in the modulation of this balance (Dreisbach et al. 2005). Correlated activity was observed in bilateral SMA, more caudal dorsal and ventral PMC, right DLPFC and right primary motor cortex during synchronization of finger tapping with an external auditory cue but not during continuation tapping. Increased activation of rostral supplementary motor area and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was found for endogenous blinks; similarly to hand motor actions (Jenkins et al. 2000). I.e. there is a shared brain network for blinking and tapping and it can create appropriate conditions for their coordination. Higher brain structure modifies eyelid and eye movement control during blinking (VanderWerf et al. 2007). Spontaneous blinks can be good markers of completion of cognitive tasks such as a solution of arithmetic problems (e.g. Evinger 1995). The representation of time for speech generation might be derived from an endogenous timing process (or a pacemaker) linked to some type of counting device (Ivry & Richardson 2002). If counting process during timing can be considered as such cognitive task then spontaneous blinks can be good markers of its completion. 89 4 Literature review on saccades and timing processes Note: This literature review reflects related work of other authors. To achieve a compressed but clear description of this work, often original phrases were taken from the original papers without specifically labeling them, because mostly they are optimal with respect to information density. Visible parts of environment are registered by saccade and a cognitive map is built up. Saccades are investigated in several ways such as towards a stimulus, anti-saccade, towards a remembered point, etc. Pathophysiologic saccades are identified by deviation from a healthy condition such as attention-deficit, hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and Nystagmus4. Saccades are used in this study as another motor output to investigate motor coordination in dual-task. 4.1 Visual saccade and memory processes (Claeys et al. 1999) Claeys et al. (1999) studied parallel processes containing visual and memory information processing. 30 healthy persons (13 males and 17 females, age from 18 to 83 years) were subjected to six tasks: (1) the prosaccade, (2) the no-saccade, and (3) the antisaccade task, each in two conditions, either (a) with or (b) without the random tap task. In the prosaccade task, the participants were required to move eyes towards the cue as quickly as possible; in the antisaccade task, the saccade had to be executed mirror-like in the opposite direction of the stimulus. In the no-saccade task, subjects had to continue to fixate the center of the screen in spite of the appearance of a lateral stimulus. In the random tap task, a tap had to be performed randomly at an unpredictable rhythm on the zero key of the numeric keyboard by the dominant hand, at a rate of approximately one tap per second. The aim of this “random time interval generation” (RIG) is to examine it as a secondary task that interferes with executive function in dual-task studies, while keeping the load on other processing systems to a minimum (Vandierendonck, De Vooght, & Van der Goten 1998). (RIG) task requires that the intervals produced do not form a systematic and repetitive series. 4 http://saccadic.askdefine.com/ 90 Figure 4-1: Distribution of the number of errors made in the prosaccade (PS), the no-saccade (NS) and the antisaccade task (AS) without and with tapping (median, 10-90 th percentiles). (Taken from Claeys et al. 1999) Figure 4-2: Correlation between the difference in latency time and the difference in number of errors with and without tapping in the antisaccade task (6 conditions*30 trials*30 stimuli; r = –0.43; n = 30; 95% confidence interval -0.68 to –0.08; p =0.02). Because of the normal distribution of the latency times, the significance of differences was assessed by the Student’s t-test. (Taken from Claeys et al. 1999) 91 Table 4.1: Mean latency in msec and standard deviation in the prosaccade and the antisaccade task. (Taken from Claeys et al. 1999) The hypothesis is that an increased load to the central executive would decrease the ability to inhibit prepotent saccades (prosaccade), where the random tap task is believed to be a pure central executive task, i.e. this lead to a decreased performance in the antisaccade task, if the antisaccade task would also be under central executive control because it is non-automatic activity. By concurrent tapping a significant increasing of latency times was shown both in the prosaccade and antisaccde task (table 4.1). The saccadic latencies in the antisaccade task both with and without tapping were significantly longer than the latencies in the prosaccade task with and without tapping, respectively. The number of saccadic errors was not significantly increased in all of the three saccade tasks by the random tap task (Fig. 4-1). In the antisaccade task, subjects made significantly more errors compared to the no-saccade and the prosaccade task both with and without tapping (Fig. 4-2). Claeys et al (1999) concluded that the prosaccade task is brought under willed control of the central executive because subjects were instructed to look as quickly as possible in the direction of the stimulus. The antisaccade task is a more difficult task to perform than the no-saccade task. A more sensitive parameter seems the latency to be than the number of errors. 4.2 Saccade with concurrent auditory task (Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983) Divided attention and an opponent-process visual processing model were supported by Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber (1983). Figure 4-3: Flow diagram for sequence of primary visual tracking task and concurrent auditory task. (Taken from Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983) 92 Figure 4-4: Representative raw data of a subject following jumpwise vertical target, frequency .5 Hz, concurrent task difficulty 1.2. During task, subject misses approximately 4% of target movements. (Spikes are eye blinks). (Taken from Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983) Figure 4-5: Representative raw data of a subject following jumpwise vertical target, frequency 1.0 Hz, concurrent task difficulty 1.2. During task, subject misses approximately 56% of target movements. (Spikes are eye blinks). (Taken from Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983) Figure 4-6: Mean saccadic eye movement magnitude plotted as a percent of target magnitude, concurrent task difficulty, and task periods. (Taken from Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983) 93 Ten adult males either separately tracked the jumpwise target or identified dots and dashes that they listened to on a headset by a thumb switch. A 10-s visual tracking practice period followed by 1 10-s auditory task dot-dash identification practice period was presented to participants (Fig. 4-3). Subjects were instructed to visually track the jumpwise target only for the first 30-s period of actual trial after practice. The concurrent task period then begun with the same visual target and the added dot-dash task. At the end of the task period, the auditory task ceased, and the target continued its motion for an additional 30 s visual tracking period. The concurrent task was presented at two difficulties, DF .6 and DF 1.2. For the DF 1.2, there were approximately 36 tones per the 30-sec task period (36/30 s = 1.2, SD = 2.1); for the DF .6, there were approximately 18 tones per the 30-sec task period (18/30 s = .6, SD = .93). For both difficulty levels, the ratio was 1.4 dots to 1 dash. Results showed both an elimination of discrete saccades and a shortening of eye movement paths due to the concurrent auditory task (Fig. 4-4, 4-5, 4-6) and most important that there were joint effects of the concurrent auditory task difficulty and the visual tracking task difficulty (frequency). Dependent effects of the visual tracking task and the concurrent auditory task were confirmed. A divided attention and an opponent-process visual processing model were supported. 4.3 Saccadic eye movement and manual control system (Megaw & Armstrong (1973) Megaw, & Armstrong (1973) tried to determine whether latency in a saccadic eye movement was dependent on certain probabilistic properties of a random step input task, and to establish whether any interaction existed between the eye movement and manual control system when performed simultaneously. Six participants performed a discrete tracking task under conditions of separate and simultaneous saccadic eye tracking and manual tracking in experiment 1. The target for step input could arrive at one of 5 horizontal positions as a vertical line on a display oscilloscope. The interval between each step input varied randomly among 1.5, 1.7, 1.9, and 2.1 sec. The subjects gripped with their right hand a free-moving handle to control a follower line. Each subject was tested on 3 conditions: (1) only tracking the input with their eyes alone (2) position the follower line within the current target area by operating a control handle (motor only), (3) control the follower line to travel between adjacent target positions. The input characteristics of the third condition were the same as for the eye-only condition and the travelling of the follower line were the same as in the motor-only condition. Four other participants were tested on 3 different types of sequences involving only eye tracking in experiment 2 to exclude the transfer effect to the eyes-only condition because all subject had experienced all the 3 conditions. The significant dependence of eye latencies on direction uncertainty might have reflected central facility by the motor system during simultaneous performance. Each experimental run included blocks of trials, each block containing the possible target steps in each direction. They attempted to fit the set of observations by a simple linear model RTijkl= M + αAj+ βIi + Dk+ Bl + Eijkl, where M = the mean effect; I = direction information (I i= -log 2Pi bits, where Pi = direction probability); A = angle subtended by the target; D = direction of response; B = the effect of blocks, and E = the residual error. 94 Figure 4-7: Recording of simultaneous motor (displacement of the control lever and its acceleration) and eye movements to a probable target step where angle of target presentation (A) = 20°. The recording delays of 22 ms, to the eye and acceleration traces have been removed. Over the whole experiment the corrected acceleration RT (RTa) preceded the displacement RT (RTd) by an average of 39 ms, due to the relative sensitivity of the accelerometer as a response device. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Figure 4-8: Reaction time as a function of angle of target presentation (A) where direction information (I) = 0, based on mean estimates of α. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) 95 Figure 4-9: Reaction time as a function of direction information (I) where angle of target presentation (A) =0, based on mean estimates of β. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Figure 4-10: Examples of directional and anticipatory errors by the eye system. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Figure 4-11: Stick diagram of the relationship between eye movement and initial acceleration phases of the motor response during simultaneous performance. (Direction information [I] = .68 bits and angle of target presentation [A] = 25°). (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) 96 Table 4.2: ten equiprobable target steps for experiment 2. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Table 4.3: estimates of standard errors (in milliseconds, df = 57) for each subject and for the four main sets (RTe-Eo, RTe-Em, ) of reaction time data for experiment 1 based on the combined 2- and 3-factor interactions (direction, blocks, and target steps represent the main factor) excluding the Target Steps * Direction term.. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Table 4.4: Estimates of response measurements at the four target angles obtained from the eyes-motor condition of experiment 1. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) Table 4.5: Means (in milliseconds), estimates of α (in milliseconds per degree) and β (in milliseconds per bit), and standard errors for the four subjects and two conditions of experiment 2. (Taken from Megaw, & Armstrong 1973) 97 The accelerometer has a relative sensitivity and hence the corrected acceleration RT (RTa) preceded the displacement RT (RTd) by an average of 39 msec. Four sets of data were analyzed, RTe (relative latencies of the eye) from the eyes-only and eyes-motor conditions (RTe-Eo, RTe-Em) and RTa (corrected acceleration RT) from the motor-only and eyes-motor conditions (RTa-Mo, RTa-Em). Table 4.3 gives the estimates of the standard errors for each subject and for the 4 sets of data based on the combined 1- and 3-factor interactions (direction*block*target step) excluding the Target Steps * Direction term. Eye movement duration (Emt) and angle of eye movement (Ea), maximum positive acceleration (Amax), time from the end of RTa to Amax (Am), time from Amax to zero acceleration or maximum velocity (Ao), and the difference RTa – RTe (RTdiff) are summarized in table 4.4. Table 4.5 gives the results including estimates of α, β, and the standard errors. Fig. 4-7 illustrates a typical response for a probable target step during the eyes-motor condition. Fig. 4-8 illustrates the relationship between RT and angle of target presentation when (direction information) I = 0 based on the estimates of α averaged over the 6 subjects, Fig. 4-9 the relationship between RT and direction information based on the estimates of β when A = 0, and Fig. 4-11 the relationship between eye movements and the initial acceleration phases of the motor responses based on the averaged responses of the 6 subjects. Fig. 4-10 shows such a response where the eye nearly came to rest at 10o and 30o before finally reaching the 40o target position. The result showed that both saccadic and motor latencies were dependent on direction probability. The increase in mean saccadic latency observed during simultaneous performance was not significant, i.e. the two systems are substantial independent in information processing. In experiment 2 four subjects who had not experienced manual tracking was tested on 3 different types of sequences in eyes-only condition. Additional to the 5-position condition used in the first experiment two other sequences were developed which used only 3 target positions, restraining choice of response to the 2 remaining equiprobable target positions and reducing the number of possible target steps in each direction from 10 to 3. The result confirmed the dependence of saccadic latencies on direction information and further indicated that they were independent of extent uncertainty in the manner shown by Megaw (1972) for motor latencies. 4.4 Cognitive load on saccadic eye movements (Stuyven et al. 2000) Stuyven et al. (2000) studied the Random time Interval Generation (RIG) task on saccade latencies and errors to test the hypothesis that antisaccades require controlled processing due to the prepotent response in prosaccades that needs to be inhibited. The first possibility is that a central component would involve the cognitive control over actions to be performed such as the inhibition of a reflexive saccade, the start of a saccade, the creation of a random series of time intervals, the second possibility is that motor interference would occur when eye movement and finger movement are to be performed at the same time. To address these possibilities, a first experiment compared saccade performance within prosaccade and the antisaccade task, executed alone and in combination with the RIG task and fixed tapping task. The fixed tapping task was added to exclude possible motor component interference explanations. Saccade task was a between-subjects and load (control, fixed, RIG) a within-subjects variable in the 2 saccade (prosaccade or antisaccade) *2 order of presentation (fixed-control-RIG and controlRIG-fixed)*3 load design. A white square appeared as the fixation point. To the left or to the right of the fixation point another square of the same size appeared 70 mm indicating the direction in which the saccade had to be made. Subjects were instructed to tap either an unpredictable sequence for 98 the RIG task, or at a fixed rate of one per second for the fixed tapping task on the computer key. Possible errors are: (1) saccades in the wrong direction; (2) unperformed saccades; (3) reflexive saccades in the antisaccade task. Figure 4-12: Latencies in ms for prosaccades (PS) and antisaccades (AS) as a function of secondary task conditions in experiment 1. (Taken from Stuyven et al. 2000) The condition order had no effect on the latencies or on the number of error. No significant interactions between saccade task and cognitive load and no significant difference between RIG and fixed tapping were found. On the mean latencies, the main effect of saccade task was significant. Prosaccades were faster than antisaccades. Load also showed significant effect. Task*load interaction was significant. Fig. 4-12 clearly shows that this difference was smaller but significant in the prosaccade than in the antisaccade task. The fixed tapping and the RIG task had comparable effects on the eye movement latencies. The effect of load was significant in both prosaccade and antisaccade task. Table 4.6: Mean number of errors per condition in experiment 1. (Taken from Stuyven et al. 2000) Table 4.6 shows the average number of errors in the prosaccade and antisaccade task under the different secondary task conditions. More errors occurred in antisaccades than in prosaccades. The effect of load and the task*load interaction were significant. The interaction of saccade task with the contrast between fixed tapping and control was not reliable. The larger impairment of antisaccade performance and the required executive control for accuracy in fixed tapping would lead to possible cognitive interpretation. To test this hypothesis, experiment 2 compared performance under strict instructions and more lenient instructions for fixed tapping task on both prosaccades and antisaccades under single- and dual-task conditions. In the lenient instructions were told to hit the key about 1 hit per second but it was stressed that small deviations were not important (against a rate of 1 hit per second in strict instruction). 99 Table 4.7: Mean number of errors per condition in Experiment 2. (Taken from Stuyven et al. 2000) On the mean latencies, the load effect and of instruction load was significant for antisaccades but not for prosaccades. The effect of fixed tapping under strict instructions was significant but not under lenient ones. Table 4.7 presents the mean number of errors for the pro- and antisaccade task under different instructions and under load and noload conditions. None of the effects were significant. A possible hypothesis is that the prosaccade execution also needs control rather than they are automatic although they are faster. Experiment 3 was designed to show mere controlled execution, without inhibition, is enough to obtain interference effects. Exogenous prosaccade (triggered by stimulus) and a saccade task which can only performed in a controlled manner (without peripheral stimulus) but an inhibition of prepotent saccades is not needed are two used tasks. The larger peripheral stimuli and the smaller fixation point were used to increase the exogenous triggering of the prosaccades. For the endogenous saccade task, this cross fixation point was replaced by an arrow pointing to the left or right, indicating the direction of saccade to be made. Two very small squares remained visible throughout the endogenous saccade to get comparable amplitude. Figure 4-13: Latencies in ms for prosaccades (PS) and endogenous saccades (ES) as a function of load in Experiment 3. (Taken from Stuyven et al. 2000) Fig. 4-13 summarizes the main results. The endogenous saccades were significantly slower than the prosaccades. The main effect of RIG load was significant however was much larger for the endogenous saccades. Separately analyses revealed the significant effect of load on both saccade tasks. 100 4.5 Human head-eye coordination during tapping task (Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001) The general principles underlying the way in which the head and eyes cooperate in the performance of manual tasks is still far from understanding (Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001). They examined the ‘natural’ temporal coordination of head and eye of four subjects who tapped a sequence of targets. This sequence was arranged in 3D on a worktable in front of them. The angular separation of targets was random. Two criteria for a coordinated head/eye movements, were examined: (1) the head and eye moved in the same direction; and (2) the horizontal components of both the head and eye were larger than 10°. Figure 4-14: Proportion (%) of coordinated head/eye movements in which the head led the eye, the head and eye moved simultaneously and the eye led the head. The performance of each of the 4 subjects is shown separately. (Taken from Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001) 101 Figure 4-15: (A) Distribution (%) of coordinated head/eye movements (7 ms bins) in which the head led the eye, the head and eye moved simultaneously, and the eye led the head. Latency difference was calculated as headonset minus eye-onset, the convention introduced by Guitton & Volle (1987). The data were pooled across subjects because individual differences were small (see Fig. 4-14). (B) Distribution (%) of coordinated head/eye movements near (±20 ms) the temporal resolution limit, viz. 2 ms. This distribution shows when the head led the eye, the head and eye moved simultaneously, and the eye led the head with respect to the smallest temporal interval that could be measured. (Taken from Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001) 102 Figure 4-16: (A) Mean right gaze-shift-size (°) and standard deviations when gaze-shifts went to the right and the head led the eye, the head and eye moved simultaneously, and the eye led the head. The performance of each subject is shown separately. (B) Mean left gaze-shift-size and standard deviations when gaze-shifts went to the left. See Fig. 4-14 for the color code of each subject. (Taken from Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001) Head and eye movements were considered to begin simultaneously if their onset occurred within 8 ms of each other. The results showed that that the head tended to start moving before the eyes 48 % of the time. Both the head and eye started to move ‘simultaneously’ 37% of the time. The eye started to move before the head only 15% of the time. Fig. 4-14 summarizes the differences among the three groups of proportions of coordinated head/eye movements which are all significant. Fig. 415A shows the distribution, Fig. 4-16A the distribution of gaze-shift sizes, Fig. 4-16B the distribution of individual subject’s gaze-shift directions of the three types of coordinated head/eye movements: the eye leading, eye and head starting simultaneously and head leading. Fig. 4-15B plots the proportion of the data that fell near (±20ms) their temporal resolution limit (~2 ms). The finding that the eye was least likely to lead in this tapping task is inconsistent with most prior work on human head/eye coordination. More observations under natural conditions are needed before to understand why when and how human beings coordinate head and eyes. 4.6 Distributed neural systems underlying the timekeeping (Rao et al. 1997) Rao et al. (1997) used the whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging to get more knowledge of the neural systems underlying timekeeping operations of human in a simple tapping task. A series of four consecutive activation conditions consisting of two experimental (S (synchronization), C (continuation)) and two control tasks (L (listen), D (discrimination)), which were preceded and followed by a rest period, was approached. Thirteen participants pressed a key by the right index finger in time with a series of tones separated by 300 or 600 ms in the S phase and they had to maintain the same tapping rate in the C phase. Participants attended the same pacing tone but were instructed not to tap in the L phase. In the D phase, they listened the same series of tone 103 pairs but with a transition in pitch and were instructed to press the key whenever a transition occurred. Figure 4-17: Mean (±SEM) inter-response interval (A) and total variability (B) as a function of pacing interval (300 or 600 ms) and condition (Synchronization, Continuation). Total variability is expressed as SD. (Taken from Rao et al. 1997) Table 4.8: Activation foci as a function of task and pacing interval. (Taken from Rao et al. 1997) 104 Figure 4-18: Areas demonstrating significantly increased MR signal intensity changes for each of the four conditions (S, synchronization; C, continuation; L, listening; D, discrimination) and pacing tone intervals (300 or 600 msec) relative to rest. Functional activity (shown in color) is overlaid onto averaged axial anatomic scans (right side of brain is on reader’s right). SMC, Sensorimotor cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; put., putamen; thal., thalamus; cer., cerebellum. z indicates the number of millimeters above (+) or below (-) the anterior–posterior commissure line. (Taken from Rao et al. 1997) 105 Figure 4-19: A) Areas of increased MR signal intensity for the synchronization (S) and continuation (C) conditions at two pacing tone intervals. The two sagittal slices are located 48 and 57 mm right of the interhemispheric fissure. STG activation is observed in both conditions, despite the absence of a tone stimulus in the C condition. IFG activation is observed only in the C condition. B) Areas of increased MR signal intensity for the continuation (C) and discrimination (D) conditions at two pacing tone intervals. The sagittal slice is located 3 mm left of the interhemispheric fissure. The horizontal green line indicates the intersection of the anterior and posterior commissures (z = 0); the perpendicular vertical line crosses through the anterior commissure (VAC line; y = 0). The functional activity for the C condition is located primarily within the SMA proper (located posterior to the VAC line), whereas activity for the D condition is located largely within the preSMA region (anterior to the VAC line). (Taken from Rao et al. 1997) Fig. 4-17 displays the reaction time from the S and C conditions of the tapping task. Table 4.8 shows the centers, volumes, and peak intensities of the activation foci, as well as the number of subjects demonstrating significantly activated tissue within each foci. Within the left sensorimotor cortex, the right cerebellum, and the right superior temporal gyrus the same activation was found in both S and C conditions (Fig. 4-18, Fig.4-19). The caudal supplementary motor area (SMA), the left putamen, and the left ventrolateral thalamus, the right inferior frontal gyrus were activated in the C condition. The D condition activated the rostral caudal supplementary motor area. No activation was observed in the left superior temporal gyrus (STG) for the L condition at the slower stimulus rate (600 ms interval) and the magnitude of activation within the STG produced less activation than conditions requiring a sensory discrimination. They suggested that three interrelated neural systems were involved. Putamen, ventrolateral thalamus, supplementary motor areas were involved in explicit timing in C condition, dorsal dentate nucleus and sensorimotor cortex in sensorimotor processing both in C and S conditions, and inferior frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus mediated auditory sensory memory for discrimination task in D condition. 106 4.7 Gaze effects on movement activation patterns (Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes (1999) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to investigate the modification of gaze direction on the pattern of finger movement activation. With the supinated right arm at the right body side laid eleven subjects supine. They either lay still or tapped with the right hand on a tip of the thumb at a rate of two per second for 30 sec. In both conditions they fixed gaze in one of three directions: leftward 10-15 o , central, or rightward 10-15 o without rotating the head. Figure 4-20: Cerebral cortical regions assessed and exemplar activation patterns. A, B, Color-coded illustration of the cortical regions assessed for functional MR activation. MI in red, PMA in green, SMA in purple, SPL in orange, and IPL in yellow. See Materials and Methods for additional details of sulcal and gyral landmarks defining each region. VAC, Vertical plane through the anterior commissure perpendicular to a line between the anterior and posterior commissures; VPC, vertical plane through the posterior commissure perpendicular to a line between the anterior and posterior commissures. C, Functional MR labelling in two exemplars, slice obtained from a single participant (slice planes indicated on whole brain volumes at right). The least activation occurred for leftward gaze, whereas that for both central and rightward gaze exceeded that for leftward gaze. The images with overlain label depict mostly portions of the left, contralateral hemisphere (L, left; R, right). Red arrowhead indicates fundus of central sulcus (indicated by green lines). (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) 107 Figure 4-21: Functional activation. The number of activated voxels in each analysed brain region for each direction of gaze. All areas exhibited the east amount of activation for leftward gaze. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) Figure 4-22: Spatial distribution for gaze-independent and gaze-dependent voxels. Exemplar activation patterns from two participants (one slice each in left and right panels), illustrating the intermixing of gaze-independent and gaze-dependent voxels across brain regions. Voxels indicated in white correspond to gaze-independent, and those in black correspond to gaze-dependent. White triangle indicates the interparietal sulcus; white triangle with a black outline indicates SMAc; black triangle with white outline indicates MIc. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) 108 Figure 4-23: Gaze-related MR signal intensity. The percentage increase in movement-related (vs no-movement) in MR signal intensity is illustrated for MIc (A) and SMAc (B) for each of the gaze directions and for the two classes of activated voxels; gaze-dependent and gaze-independent. No differences in MR signal were observed for the gaze-independent voxels in either MIc or SMAc. By contrast, MR signal obtained from labelled voxels in MIc and SMAc in increased for the gaze-dependent voxels when participants looked in a sector of visual space that yielded more active voxels. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) Table 4.9: Activation occurrence. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) Table 4.10: Gaze direction exhibiting the greatest activation. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) 109 Table 4.11: Areal congruence of the gaze direction effect. (Taken from Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999) The general location of these cerebral cortical regions is illustrated in Fig. 4-20A. Fig. 4-20B depicts a single participant’s brain defined on two horizontal slices. One participant exhibited typical functional MR labeling occurring in frontal and parietal cortex during finger movement and the obtained distribution of functional activation is illustrated in Fig. 4-20C. For each cortical area the labeled voxel counts are shown in Fig. 4-21. Two participants whose data indicated no clear anatomic segregation of voxels with gaze-dependent activation from those with gaze-independent activation in SMAc (contralateral Supplementary Motor Area) MIc (contralateral Motor Cortex), PMAc (contralateral Premotor Cortex), or the parietal lobe areas examined are shown in Fig. 4-22. For all subjects when participants directed gaze towards at least one location for labeled voxels are yielded during finger movement in the left and to a less extent in the right hemisphere (table 4.9). During either rightward or central gaze the greatest amount of MR labels for MIc, MIi (ipsilateral Motor Cortex), SMAc, and SPLc (contralateral superior parietal lobule) are exhibited (table 4.10). The results showed that MRI signals from the hemisphere contralateral to the moving hand revealed activation in primary motor cortex, lateral and medial premotor cortex, and a wide extend of the lateral superior and inferior parietal lobules. In summary, statistical significance was found in 9 of 10 of the paired comparisons between contralateral cortical areas (table 4.11). Hand moving aligned with gaze increased the activation of the related area compared to when gaze was directed away from the moving hand (Fig. 4-23). They suggested that a large-scale of cortical networks related to finger actions exists, in which the skeletomotor processing in the cerebral cortex is consistently modified by gaze direction. 4.8 Models for saccade generation circuitry (Girard & Berthoz 2005) The computational models for the five main brain regions (Cerebellum, premotor cortical areas (PMA), reticular formation saccadic burst generators (SBG), basal ganglia (BG), and superior colliculus (SC)) was reviewed during saccade generation (Girard & Berthoz 2005). 4.8.1 Reticular formation saccadic burst generators Activations generated by SBG are transmitted to horizontal and vertical ocular motoneurons. Motoneurons have burst-tonic discharge pattern caused proportional amplitude of saccade. The tonic neurons of two neural integrators (Moschovakis, Scudder, & Highstein 1996) providing these tonic activities are located in the interstitial nucleus of Cajal (vertical integrator) and nucleus 110 prepositus hypoglossi (horizontal integrator), in interaction with the vestibular nuclei. The burst of activity are composed of a set of neuron classes having specific patterns of activity. 4.8.2 Superior colliculus SC is a multilayered structure. The superficial layers receive direct retinal inputs. Their rostral parts respond to visual stimuli close to the fovea and peripheral parts activate more caudal sites. The activity of neurons of this layer follows a bell-shape tuning function centered on a preferred position. The deeper layers called visuomotor exhibit small visual bursts before the motor bursts and usually are not sufficient to generate a saccade. 4.8.3 Cerebellum The cerebellum plays an important role in the integration of sensory perception, coordination and motor control. There are many neural pathways linking the cerebellum with the cerebral motor cortex which sends information to the muscles causing them to move and the spinocerebellar tract which provides proprioceptive feedback on the position of the body in space. Using the constant feedback the cerebellum integrates these pathway to fine-tune motor activity. The motoneuron activity during saccades is significantly influenced by projections from the cerebellum. Hypermetria, alteration of the main sequence and an increased variability of the amplitude are induced by the damage or inactivation of the cerebellar afferent areas (Optican & Robinson 1980). Numerous experimental results found that the lobules VIc and VII of the vermis are the areas of the cerebellar cortex involved in saccade generation. Ipsilateral and topographically organized projections are drawn towards a subpart of the caudal fastigial nuclei (a cerebellar central nuclei) called FOR (Fastigial Oculomotor Area). FOR affects the saccade generation circuitry at the level of the saccade generator. 4.8.4 Basal Ganglia BG is a set of interconnected subcortial nuclei involved in large cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical loops which consist of motor, oculomotor, prefrontal (dorsolateral prefrontal and lateral orbitofrontal) and limbic loops. These loops have similar internal connectivity but interact with different cortical areas and brainstem nuclei. In saccade generation the oculomotor loop interacts with the frontal eye fields (FEF) and the parietal posterior cortex (PPC) and projects to the SC, gating the loci of activation on the collicular map. The prefrontal loop involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) enable the learning and restitution of sequences of saccades as it is involved in working memory processes. 4.8.5 Premotor cortical areas Many cortex areas are more or less implicated in the saccadic premotor activity. The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and lateral intraparietal area (LIP) modulate the stream of the cortical visual processing by attentional processes for determination of location. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) allows temporal organization of saccades and even saccade inhibition. The presupplementary eye fields (preSEF) projects to the supplementary eye fields (SEF) which can execute saccades by sending saccade orders to the frontal eye fields (FEF). The FEF operates the final target selection stage by interacting with the basal ganglia and sends the corresponding motor command to 111 the SC and SBG. Finally, FEF, SEF and DLPFC receive projections from the anterior cingulate cortex which provides them with motivational modulation. 4.9 Gaze and Hand Position Effects on Brain Activation (Bedard & Sanes 2009) Bedard & Sanes (2009) investigated the integration of the gaze and hand signals to generate movements. They hypothesized that the involvement of parietal, frontal and subcortical regions exhibit modulation of movements related activation as a function of gaze and hand positions. 15 healthy adults (aged 19-34 yr; 8 females, 7 males, all right-handed) were recruited in 6 runs of scanning using functional MRI. During each run, participants has to fix gaze at one of three visual targets (left, center, right of their body and only one annulus was visible) presented randomly. By turning of the annulus center from white to black with frequency of 3Hz participants tapped thrice with their right thumb for each gaze position. The right arm was fully extended and half-pronated beside participant’s right side or the arm crossed the body midline in midflexion so that the right hand became aligned with the left shoulder. A two-way ANOVA (2 hand positions * 2 gaze positions) with repeated measures and the t-test revealed RT were significantly slower when gazing right with the hand on the left. Several areas (sensory motor cortex, supramarginal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, superior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate, and left cerebellum) that exhibited activation related to a mixture of these hand and gaze positions. Activation driven only by gaze orientation were found in regions with the left insula, left cuneus, left midcingulate gyrus, left putamen, and right tempo-occipital. The main effect of gaze position was significantly revealed by the ANOVA in clusters exhibiting finger movement-related activation (contralateral primary motor cortex, supplementary motor area, cerebellum, anterior cingulated gyrus and right supramarginal gyrus). Clusters with hand position effects were also found in the cerebellum bilaterally. They suggested that processing is superimposed specific to goal-directed movements based on the baseline conditions of this study and a multiplicity of frames of references is used for movements. 4.10 Coordination of ocular and hand motor systems (Bekkering et al. 1994) Bekkering et al. (1994) measured reaction time (RT) latencies of saccade eye and hand motor responses using dual-task methodology to investigate whether the both movements share processes. When RT latencies in the dual-task conditions do not change the independent mediating of both responses is assumed. In contrast, the two movements share processes when the RT latencies differ from each other. In experiment 1 they required 12 right-hand students to move (eyes alone, hand alone, or eyes and hand concurrently (dual)) as quickly as possible to a large target stimulus appearing randomly either to the left or right of a central fixation point. An ANOVA yielded a significant effect for task condition and a paired two-tailed t-test indicated that RT of the eyes was significantly longer in the duals-task condition than in the single-task condition. To discriminate between the two interpretations that the consequence of being required to produce any two responses, or the sharing of specific processes in the context of pointing to a target they manipulated the nature of the manual response in experiment 2. Instead of a goal-directed hand movement to the target stimulus, subjects had to make a button-press response with either the index (for the left target) or the middle finger (for the right target) of the right hand. They argued that if the interference effect disappears, support is found for a specific interference effect caused by 112 a sharing of processes associated with the control of coordinated aimed eye and hand movements. The results showed that RT latencies of the eyes were significantly shorter than the RT latencies of the finger. The most important result of experiment 2 was the absence of a significant interaction effect between type of movement (eye vs. finger) and task condition (single vs. dual). The specific interference interpretation caused by a sharing of processes associated with the control of the coordinated aimed eye and hand movements is supported, i.e. the overhead costs due to coordinating any two responses are excluded. 4.11 Theoordination of saccadic and manual movements (Binsted & Elliot 1999) Binsted & Elliott (1999) conducted two experiments to identify the type of information that mediates any eye-hand coordination. They used a point-light array to generate Müller-Lyer configuration target endpoints (in-Müller, out-Müller, ‘X’) for 30 cm aiming movements. The endpoint was a 37 LED ‘X’ generated by the intersection of perpendicular lines. The “in” and “out” Müller-lyer figures were established by illumination of ipsilateral arms of “X” figure. Vision of the hand was removed by employing liquid crystal occlusion goggles. Manual movements were measured by an IRED (infrared–emitting diode) attached to the participant’s right index finger. Eye movements were measured by an Applied Science Laboratories, series 4000-SU HMO, and headmounted eye-tracking system. The level of dependence of the manual system on selected channels of ocular/visual information was examined in experiment 1. Eleven students were asked to move as quickly as possible from the home position to the centre point of the presented figure. The home position and endpoint figure were positioned 30 cm apart. The home position consisted of a 2-cm-diameter button. The retinal and extraretinal information were manipulated by various vision of the limb and target, eye position, and the concurrent of eye movement. In full information condition (FULL) concurrent eye and hand movements presumably were made using all available information normally presented for eye-hand coordination. In the NOEFF no concurrent eye efference5 was available for modulating hand movements. The no-vision conditions (NOVIS) allowed neither concurrent eye movement nor vision of the hand in motion. Vision was returned following the completion of each trial. Correlations were calculated between: (1) end location of the primary eye and hand movements, (2) end location of the eye of primary saccade and at final position of the hand, (3) eye reaction time and hand reaction time, (4) total time of the eye to reach the end of the primary saccade with total time of hand to peak velocity, and (5) time to complete the primary saccade with hand time to peak velocity (reaction times removed). Single sample t-test was used to obtain significant effects. On eye movements a significant effect of endpoint configuration was found for the primary saccade and the final location of the eye after corrective6 saccades. In hand movements the main effect for vision revealed participants moved significantly less distance in the NOVIS trials than in either FULL or NOEFF situations. Hand movements variability increased significantly when vision of the hand was removed. In eye-hand coordination the transformed z-scores for eye-hand coordination revealed a significant relation between eye and hand reaction time. However, when reaction was removed, this 5 The terms ‘efference’ and ‘efferent information’ are intended to include efferent copy, corollary discharge, and movement intention/programing information. 6 Inaccuracies in the initial impulse visually detected during the final portion of movement are corrected 113 correlation approached zero. Neither the correlation between the displacements of the primary movements of the eye and hand nor those for the final displacements reached significant level. To completely examine the illusory implications for efferent theory and eye-hand coordination they reproduced the earlier findings by replicating specific procedural practices used by those researchers (Elliot & Lee 1995; Gentilucci et al. 1996) who enabled subjects the free viewing of the illusion endpoint prior to and during the planning phases of a response. In experiment 2 participants were explicitly asked to place the end of the pointer over the centre point of the LED. They believed that this instructional adaptation decreased the goal tolerance to successfully reach the target. Hand initiation can turn off the LED display by an additional switch attached to the home position (TAR <-> NOTAR). All forms of extraretinal feedback and feedforward were available as in experiment 1 for the FULL condition. However, these information sources were biased when the illusory endpoints were present. In biased proprioception conditions (BIAS) due to illusory endpoint concurrent efference was removed, eye position was incorrect on selected trials. In non-biased proprioception (UNBIAS) condition, concurrent efference was removed and the eye attained a veridical position prior to each trial. Again all levels of endpoint configuration were significantly different from each other on eye movements. The results also reflected that the hand movements were far slower than eye movements, the eye failed to overcome its predictable undershooting tendency prior to hand movement completion. The undershooting was increased when the eye was to maintain a fixation without a visual target. The interaction between endpoint and eye position at hand completion resulted largely from the illusory bias in the FULL conditions as compared to the UNBIAS trials. However, the BIAS condition failed to produce a biased position. On hand movements the main effect for vision of target and eye position was elicited with increasing of reaction time as a result of simultaneous eye-hand movement and removal of target vision. In movement time large values were again found for the eye-hand condition (FULL). On the final hand displacement endpoint configuration generated main effect and interacted with both vision of target and eye position. The variability in final figure displacement revealed an effect only for the vision of the target. There was again an interaction between vision of target and endpoint configuration. Peak velocity of the aiming movements and peak hand deceleration increased with the removal of vision of the target. Peak deceleration was also affected endpoint configuration and specifically greatest by aiming at the in-Müller configuration. On eye-hand coordination the correlations involving the location of the eye and that of the hand at completion and at the end of the primary movement were significant in NOTAR-FULL trials compared to zero. Inconsistent with these no-vision effects and correlations, however, there was a small, although significant, eye-hand relation in BIAS-TAR trials. Together the Müller-lyer illusion effectively biased saccade eye movements but did generate biases in manual movements only when the target lights were extinguished at movement initiation. The corrective eye movements could not overcome the biasing information. The hypothesis that the biasing in primary eye movement leads to the biasing in hand movement is not supported. Common information shared by the hand and eye or planning and control of manual system based on efference from initial saccade are weakened. No spatial relations with respect to bias for both the eye and hand developed. If the hand is more dependent on extraretinal information when the target is extinguished, the degree of oculo-manual coordination should increase. Temporal relation between both movements was not found.. Rapid manual aiming benefits clearly from the availability of vision of both the limb and target 114 4.12 Eye Position Effects on neuronal Activity (Boussaoud, Jouffrais, & Bremmer 1998) Based on the observation that visual inputs to the brain are mapped in a retinocentric reference frame, whereas the motor system plans movements in a body-centered frame. Boussaoud, Jouffrais, & Bremmer (1998) concerned about whether the premotor areas receive visual information from the parietal cortex and studied dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) neurons in two monkeys while they performed a conditional visuomotor task and maintained fixation at different gaze angles. Two rhesus monkeys were trained to perform a conditional visuomotor task with the left hand to receive liquid reward and their head was firmly fixed 32 cm in front of a computer screen. A panel of three metal touch pads was located at the bottom of the screen. The central pad was aligned on the monkey’s body axis and the other two 12 cm to the left and the right. A white square appeared at one of nine locations forming a grid served as a precue (PC), which directs the monkey’ attention to a given location. A second colored square of the same size presented at the previously cued location after a variable delay (the instructed delay period or set-related or preparatory activity) served as a motor instructional conditional cue (MIC), which guided the monkey’s motor response. A red MIC instructed a movement to the left touch pad and a green to the right one independently of their spatial location. Monkey 1 was allowed to move its eye after the go signal (offset of cue) whereas monkey 2 was trained to fixate throughout the trial period up to the end of movement. A trial begins after the monkeys put its hand on the central pad. A video monitor provided visual stimuli. Four major epochs were measured for the analysis of neuronal activity. Precue activity: 100 ms after the presentation of the precue; post-MIC: 100 ms after the onset of MIC; set-related activity: just before the offset of MIC (go signal); and movement-related activity: after the go signal. The ANOVA of the response time (RT) showed no significant effect of gaze angle but affected by both the location of MIC cues and the direction of limb movement. The data of the precue period and the delay period did not show EMG activity. Eye movements showed comparable response times with those of limb movements. In relation to gaze angle no significant variations of EMG activity was found during the instructed delay period. To-the-left Movement revealed significant stronger phasic, movement-related activity than to-the-right movement. This phasic and movement-related activity also was varied with gaze angle. The lumbar paravertebral muscle was differentially active with gaze angle. A minority of PMd cells discharge in relation to visual cue when they simply direct spatial attention with no instructional meaning. When MIC is green a phasic, signal-related activity lasts for ~400 ms after the cue’s onset. The vast majority of PMd cells discharge preferentially following motor instruction cues than after the precue. Irrespective of the task period analyzed the discharge rate of a large proportion of cells are significantly affected by gaze angle. The retinal effect, i.e. activity variations related to target location in retinocentric coordinates, was significant in a modest number of cells depending on the task epoch (the highest proportion during the earliest epoch after MIC onset). The proportions were relatively low for set-related and movement related. The direction sensitivity was observed in all three task periods but relatively stronger during the set and movement periods than in the signal period. Gaze effect was highly significant and typically large. Signal-related activity was nearly twice higher when the monkey fixated in the preferred gaze direction and showed significant variations with gaze angle for leftward movement. Set-related activity is nearly absent when movement is to the left and dramatically varies with gaze angle. The weakest neuronal discharge is observed for gaze when gaze and limb movement are coincided. The discharge rate is significantly higher during preparation of a leftward movement when gaze is deviated to the right. Analysis of PMd movement-related cells showed significant activity variations with gaze angle and 115 the clear effect of limb movement-direction on the cell’s discharge rate. A regression analysis showed that the neuron activity varied linearly with eye position. This provide evidence that eye position signal modulate the neuronal activity beyond sensory areas. Arm movement and gaze direction at least are the two direction parameters and limb movement directions are coded in a head-centered reference frame. 4.13 The main function of the cerebellum and basal ganglia (Dreher & Grafman 2002) The challenge of the primary function in motor control of the cerebellum and the basal ganglia is the evidence that they also are activated during the performance of cognitive and attention tasks (Dreher & Grafman 2002). They tried to identify their specific roles by using magnetic resonance imaging. They tested three hypotheses using a task-switching experiment with a 2*2 factorial design varying timing (random relative to fix) and task order (unpredictable relative to predictable). Switching attentional set is mediated and error signals are provided regarding stimuli or rewards by the cerebellum (first hypothesis). The third hypothesis is that they operate as in internal timing providing the precise representation of temporal representation across various tasks. Eight healthy subjects responded to visually presented letters by pressing response buttons with their right or left hand in 8 conditions. Each condition is cued by a distinct visual instruction. 2 conditions is used for the baseline (Task A, vowel-consonant discrimination; Task B, case discrimination), 4 task switching conditions obtained by crossing the task order and timing factors, and 2 conditions was used for control (Union task, A or B). If the letter was a vowel subjects had to press the right button and if it was a consonant they had to press the left button in the “vowelconsonant” condition. If the letter was a vowel subjects had to press the right button and if the letter was a consonant in the “vowel-consonant” condition the left. If the letter was in upper case subjects had to press the right button and if the letter was in lower case the left in the “case discrimination”. In the “switching” conditions, subjects had to perform the vowel-consonant condition if the letter was red and the case discrimination condition if the letter was green. There were four switch conditions: “fixed predictable”, “random predictable”, “fixed unpredictable”, and “random unpredictable”. Fix indicates timing between stimuli. In the unpredictable condition a switch was pseudo-randomized. In the predictable a switch occurred between tasks every three stimuli. In the union condition as control condition if the letter was a vowel or was in upper case subjects and if both were true subjects had to press the right button and the left button otherwise. In the first control condition stimuli appeared in constant distance and in the second one was pseudorandomized. Task order (predictable vs. unpredictable) and timing (fixed vs. random) are the factor in ANOVA for analysis of RT and percentage of errors. A significant response time cost was found in all task-switching conditions. The unpredictable order of the tasks worsened the performance. No main effect of timing was found. RTs were significantly slower in the task-switching conditions averaged together than in the control condition. None of the brain area (caudate nucleus, thalamus and cerebellum) was significantly activated in the comparison between control and task-switching condition by examining the voxels. The main effect of task-order unpredictability activated the anterior putamen bilaterally. No significant activation was found in the cerebellum. The main effect of timing irregularity was to activate the right posterior cerebellar hemispheres and the dentate nucleus. In the basal ganglia no significant activation was found. Interactions between timing and task order factors showed in brain activated the right head of the caudate nucleus and the posterior putamen bilaterally. No interactions were found in the 116 cerebellum. The task-switching condition with unpredictable task order and random timing specially activated the substantia nigra. The caudate activation correlated positively with activation of DLPFC (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and the cerebellum by unpredictable task order. In addition when timing was random, the caudate activation correlated with the pro-SMA and the IPS (intra-parietal sulcus). The cerebellum activation positively correlated with the DLPFC when timing was random and with the pre-SMA activation when both timing and task order were unpredictable. All together their results supported distinctive roles of cerebellum and basal ganglia. Timing irregularity primarily induced the activation of the cerebellum while task order unpredictability induced the activation of the anterior striatum independent of reward delivery. Switching attention was not specific in the cerebellum and basal ganglia. 4.14 Central Bottleneck of Information processing with fMRI (Dux et al. 2006) Dux et al. (2006) used fMRI (time-resolved functional magnetic resonance imaging) to study the bottleneck occurring at the central, amodal stage of information processing when two response selections have to be concurrently executed. Dual-task (experiment 1), single-task (experiment 2), and response selection load (experiment 3) were approached. In experiment 1, subjects had to select the appropriate manual response to a complex auditory stimulus in one task (AM task) and the appropriate vocal response to a visual stimulus in other task (VV task). The first task is called task 1 and the second task 2.The stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the two tasks was either short or long. Eight visual and audio stimuli required distinct vocal response and distinct key press response respectively. Concurrently processing two sensorimotor tasks presenting intrinsic limitations in the procedures would ensure the dual-task costs. Two localizer tasks (each task involved an eight alternative force choice (AFC)) were included to isolate the regions which have been hypothesized to be involved in response selection. Subjects performed separate ordered blocks of trials of single AM and VV tasks, dual-tasks, and fixation blocks (passively view a fixation square) so that both localizer runs each block type preceded and followed one another an equal number of times. Significant dual-task interference was expected at the short SOA because RT to task 1 was generally shorter than the long SOA. In addition long RT and prolonged psychological refractory periods (PRPs) were expected due to the high number of response alternatives. A peak latency difference at the long but not at the short SOA was revealed in the left premotor cortex. The data demonstrated robust dual-task interference but this interference is largely revealed by a postponement of task 2 as predicted by the central bottleneck model and other capacity-limited models of the PRP. The first model predicts that response selection for task 2 is postponed until response for task 1 is completed. The data showed a strong correlation between two tasks at shot SOA supports this prediction. The data also supported the independence of response selection of task 2 on the response selection of task 1 because the mean RT to task 1 is shorter than the long SOA and a marginal correlation between two task RTs is shown. The Slow-Fast RT latency difference of the activation pattern in the left pLPFC (posterior lateral prefrontal cortex) was larger at the short than at the long SOA. The time between the onset of stimulus 1 and the response to task 2 (S1R2) is strongly correlated with task 1 RT at the short SOA but not at the long SOA. At the long SOA no significant latency difference was found but a peak difference at the short SOA between short and long S1R2 RTs a. Thus, the role of pLPFC in 1 central bottleneck of information processing was evidently revealed in task 1 RT and S1R2 RT. 117 To avoid the vocal artifacts they scanned subjects during single AM task trials in experiment 2. They again observed a peak latency difference between Slow and fast RTs but no difference in onset latency. The comparison of the time courses in pLPFC for the dual-task and single-task conditions provides further evidence for its key function as neural substrate of the central bottleneck. The peak latency in the left pLPFC was greater under dual-task conditions than single-task conditions. Manipulation of response selection load would provide the involvement evidence of pLPFC. Subjects performing single AM tasks were required choosing between either two or six response alternatives with the 2FAC and 6FAC (alternative force choice) trials separately blocked in experiment 3. RTs were shorter in the 2AFC condition than in the 6AFC condition. The left pLPFC’ activity was stronger in the 6AFC condition than in the 2AFC condition and the difference between these conditions arose at trial onset. Taken together a key role for pLPFC was again consistent. SMFC (superior medial frontal cortex) also exhibited a peak latency difference between Slow and Fast RTs, but no onset latency difference in the single-task condition. Dual-task conditions showed greater peak latency than single-task conditions. In the response selection load experiment SMFC showed greater activity in the 6AFC condition than the 2AFC condition. The inconsistent pattern of activity observed across experiments in the cerebellum, the right IFG (inferior frontal gyrus), and the IPS (intra-parietal sulcus) makes it difficult to ascribe to them any specific role in dual-task limitations. Dual-task limitation possibly is revealed in the inability of pLPFC and of SMFC to process both tasks at once. 4.15 The organization of eye and limb movements during reaching (Fisk & Goodable 1985) Fisk & Goodable (1985) examined the spatial and temporal organization of unrestricted limb movements directed to small targets in two experiments. In experiment 1, subjects had to point their index finger quickly and accurately immediately following illumination of the target (visual field of target presentation: left or right, brief or persistent target duration) to the position on the screen (10o or 20o eccentricity) after fixation at the central light and a ready command. Subjects were instructed to look to the targets (eye movement condition) as well as point to them or maintain fixation on the central light while pointing (no eye movement condition). The results showed that subjects initiated a reach within 500 ms after target illumination. The ratio of the minimum vector distance between the initial and final positions of the finger to the actual distance travelled was not significant affected by variations in the experimental conditions. The correction movement in vertical dimension was largely responsible for the deviation of the trajectory from a straight line path. The acceleration phase constituted approximately the first third of the total movement duration, while the longer deceleration included a very low velocity movement just before the finger contact on the screen. The affection of the target laterality with respect to the hand used to reach on the latency and kinematics of the limb was clear. The trend toward shorter latencies for reaches with the right hand opposed to the left hand. The interaction of the factors (visual field, target eccentricity) was found significant for movement latency, maximum velocity, mean velocity, and duration. The increase in latency with increased target eccentricity was much greater for contralaterally opposed to ipsilaterally directed reaches. The more eccentric targets the greater maximum velocity, mean velocity during ipsilateral reach. The duration of contralateral reaches increased dramatically with increased target eccentricity. The variation in the illumination only affected the movement duration. The duration of target illumination, the laterality, and eccentricity had significant effect on pointing accuracy. The absolute vertical error was smaller when 118 subjects looked to the target than they did not. Variable vertical error was reduced for the eyemovement compared to no-eye-movement trials. Both absolute lateral and absolute vertical error showed significant interaction between eye movement condition and the target duration. A similar trend was also noted for the variable vector error (the standard deviations of the signed lateral and vertical error scores and the standard deviation of the unsigned vector error scores). Thus, to ensure an optimal level of pointing accuracy neither the eye movements nor the persistent target were sufficient The accuracy of final horizontal eye position was affected by all of the same factors in accuracy of the limb movement. A significant correlation for the end point of movement by the eyes and the finger was found. A trend toward an effect of the duration of target illumination on the strength of this correlation was indicated. There were similarities in the effects of the experimental conditions on the latencies of eye and hand (ipsilateral target versus contralateral target). Right hand had shorter response latencies than left hand. Eye movement latencies lower for right-handed movements than for left-handed movements. Figure 4-24: The four reaching conditions and two initial fixation points for a subject performing reaches with the left hand. The filled triangles represent the position of fixation at the initiation of a trial while the open ones represent the position of the target. All target positions shown are at 20 o eccentricity from the body axis. Initial fixation varies between 0o (central) and 30o (eccentric). The position of target with regard to the visual field and body space frames of reference are illustrated for each reaching condition. For the central fixation trials these positions corresponded, but for the eccentric fixation trials this correspondence was eliminated. The eccentricity of the targets and fixation points in these diagrams are exaggerated for the sake of clarity. (Taken from Fisk & Goodable 1985) In experiment 2, location of the initial fixation point was systematically varied to disembed the effects of the visual hemifields from those of body-relative hemispace (Fig. 4-24). Significant effects of target laterality and eccentricity were evident in the body space analyses. Taken together the most consistent differences between reaches directed across the body axis to targets presented in the contralateral visual field and reaches directed at ipsilateral targets were observed. The findings suggested that the hemispherical organized neural systems are involved in the programming of visually guided limb movements. A close relationship between movement latency for both motor systems was found. An integration of both eye and hand movements was involved. The laterality of the target position relative to the body axis modified the kinematics of reaching movements. 119 4.16 Modality pairing effects on dual-task (Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006) The Generic central bottleneck predicts three discrete processing stages: a prebottleneck stage (Stage A), a bottleneck stage (Stage B), and a postbottleneck (Stage C). Both Stage A and Stage C but not Stage B can proceed in parallel with any other processing stage. Figure 4-25: Panel a: three stages of processing for Tasks 1 and 2 at a long stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA, when the SOA is long, the bottleneck stages (Stage B) for the two tasks are required at nonoverlapping times, so each task proceeds without interruption. Panel b: three stages of processing for Tasks 1 and 2 at a short SOA. When the SOA is short, the bottleneck stage (Stage B) for Task 2 must wait for the bottleneck stage for Task 1 to be completed, so Task 2 is slowed. (Taken from Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006) When the SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) is long, the bottleneck stages are not overlapped, and RTs (reaction time) for both tasks are determined only by the sum of durations of three component stages (Fig. 4-25). Stimulus categorization is implied in the stage A and response execution in the stage C. Although, the modality pairings should not qualitatively change the way the bottleneck mechanism operates, they might alter the duration of its stages. Hazeltine & Ruthruff (2006) used the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure to examine the effects of input/output modality pairings on dualtask performance to evaluate these predictions. Table 4.12 Four task combinations used in the experiment (taken from Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006). The first two letters indicate the composition of Task 1, (AM: Auditory stimulus, Manual response; AV: Auditory stimulus, Vocal response; VM: Visual stimulus, Manual response; VV: Visual stimulus, Vocal response). The second two letters indicate the composition of Task 2, using the same abbreviations as for Task 1 Four groups of 96 participants differed in terms of the S (stimulus)-R (response) associations for the two tasks (table 4.12). The AVVM (auditory-vocal/visual-manual) group responded by saying the words “one” and “two” to the tone (task 1) and by pressing the “H” and “J” keys to the symbols “#” 120 and “%” (task 2), respectively. The VMAV group performed the same two tasks except that the visualmanual task was task 1 and the auditory-vocal task was task 2. The AMVV group responded to the tones by pressing the “H” and “J” keys (Task 1) and to the “#” and “%” symbols by saying the words “one” and “two” (Task 2), respectively. Both the VVAM group and the AMVV group have same tasks, except the exchange task number 1 for the visual-vocal task and the auditory-manual task. Three different trial types according to the SOA duration (short, intermediate, and long) were provided. Auditory-vocal task with a visual-manual task is referred to as the ‘‘standard’’ modality pairings and auditory-manual task with a visual-vocal task to as nonstandard ones. RTs from the intermediate SOA were divided into 140 s bins. On Task1 RT (RT1) there were no significant main effects but statistically reliable interactions between stimulus*response, SOA*stimulus, and SOA*stimulus*response. Standard modality pairing had an advantage compared to nonstandard pairing. There were main effects of SOA reflecting the PRP effect, and response reflecting advantage for vocal response but not stimulus in Task 2. There was no advantage for the standard modality pairings at the long SOA although an overall advantage for auditory stimuli and an overall advantage for manual responses existed. Task 2 RT (RT2) at the short SOA showed much greater variation across groups. This variation was due to a reliable stimulus*response interaction. The PRP effects were larger for the AMVV and VVAM groups than for the AVVM and VMAV groups at the short SOA compared to the long SOA. On average, the nonstandard groups showed more dual-task interferences than the standard groups but RT2s at the long SOA were similar across the four groups. Furthermore the combinations of stimuli and of responses were identical for all four groups. The nonstandard groups produced much larger PRP-RT1 differences than the standard groups. Thus, the dual-task costs on Task 2 were still significantly larger for the nonstandard groups. On the proportions of correct responses on task 1 there were significant main effects of SOA but not stimulus or response. There were also reliable stimulus*SOA interactions and stimulus*response interactions. On task 2 a significant main effect of SOA but not stimulus or response. The stimulus*response and SOA*response were reliable. No three-way interaction for Task 2 and stimulus*response interaction was robust at both SOA. On task 1 the RTs did not vary with SOA but on Task 2 the correlation coefficients were reliable negative for all groups for the first two SOA bins, and reliable negative for VMAV, AMVV, and VVAM groups for the thirst SOA bin. Consistent with the GCB model, the slopes along the mean RTs tended to decrease as the SOA increased. The two nonstandard groups showed steeply slopes at the short SOA than the standard groups. Taken together, modality pairings had large effects on dual-task reaction times. Because the task demands were the same across the groups, the modality pairing effect cannot attributed to the difficulty of stimulus classification or response execution. Beside the postponement of central operations, the findings suggested that dual-task interference also arises from a slowing of central operations whose magnitude is sensitive to the input/output modality pairings. 4.17 Summary Natural experiments have to be made to understand how eyes and hand are coordinated in everyday tasks (Herst, Epelboim, & Steinman 2001). Reticular formation saccadic burst generators, superior colliculus, cerebellum, basal ganglia and premotor cortical areas are the five main brain regions involved in saccade generation (Girard & Berthoz 2005). In monkeys, gaze interactions occur in several arm movement related areas (e.g., 121 Boussaoud, Jouffrais, & Bremmer 1998) in addition to the visual cortical areas. Three interrelated neural systems (one that is involved in explicit timing (putamen, ventrolateral thalamus, supplementary motor area (SMA)), one that mediates auditory sensory memory (inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), superior temporal gyrus (STG)), and another that is involved in sensorimotor processing (dorsal dentate nucleus, sensorimotor cortex)) are suggested to be responsible for the internal generation of precisely timed movements (Rao et al. 1997). Large-scale cortical networks are related to finger actions and gaze direction signals modified skeletomotor processing in the cerebral cortex (Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999). Several areas that exhibited activation related to a mixture of hand and gaze positions were found: the sensorimotor cortex, supramarginal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, superior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate, and left cerebellum. On one side, the central bottle neck of information processing is suggested in a neural network of frontal lobe area (Dux et al., 2006). Under visual control reaching toward a target implies a common integration of eye and hand movements (Fisk & Goodable 1985). Divided attention model predicts decrements on the visual performance to be offset by an increase in concurrent task performance and opponent-process model predicts a shortening of saccade movement paths. Both models are supported when subjects simultaneously visually tracked a jumpwise moving target and identified randomly generated auditory dots and dashes (Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber 1983). On the other side, an effect of central executive load was not expected in prosaccade task because it is an automatic activity (Claeys et al. 1999). Interaction on prosaccades could arise from a controlled execution of these saccades (Stuyven et al. 2000). Information processing for the eye tracking and manual tracking substantially was independent. On the other hand both system share common input processes (Megaw & Armstrong, 1973). The ocular and manual motor systems are not operating independently when initiating saccadic eye and goal-directed hand movements but saccadic eye movements and button-press responses could be initiated without delay (Bekkering et al. 1999). Although in trials with concurrent eye movement and elimination of retinal target information on limb movement initiation only manual bias in response to illusory targets occurred no temporal relation between eye and hand movements was found (Binsted & Elliott 1999). Beside the postponement of the central operation due to central bottle-neck, the effects of input/output modality pairing slowed down its performance in dual-task (Hazeltine & Ruthruff 2006). With timing irregularity the cerebellum is primarily activated while with task order unpredictability the anterior striatum is activated. These support two forms of readjustment presenting the distinctive roles (Dreher & Grafman 2002). For the gaze only regions within the left insula, left cuneus, left midcingulate gyrus, left putamen, and right tempo-occipital and for the hand cluster in the cerebellum were found (Bedard & Sanes 2009). The results indicate that at least two signals are integrated in these areas for performing visual-motor actions. The approached oculo-manual tasks will not focused on the visually guided eye–hand coordination in space. Common spatial variables for both movements are avoided and the saccade target is fixed to prevent any spatial uncertainty. An exogenously triggered reflexive oculo-manual task together with an endogenously timed repetitive manual task would reduce probability of temporal coupling between tasks in oculo-manual dual-task. Will concurrent operation of these two very different effectors engaged in two independent tasks cause interference? 122 5 Motor coordination framework: which gaps are addressed by this study For execution of voluntary movements is there a transformation of the activity in the central nervous system of the spatial variable (direction) and mechanical variable (amplitude and velocity) of the limb, as neural representation, into signals that activate the muscles moving the limb? Would the interdependence between multi movements reflected in neural representation and also in signals that activate the muscles be sufficient to give the answer? 5.1 The form of continuous movement trajectories Classical evaluation of experimental tapping data simply determines the intertap interval sequence (i.e. only the times of contact onsets of the finger tip to the ground plate are considered). Both Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1979) and Yoshino et al. (2002) used phase resetting experiments to investigates the dominant periodic tapping disturbed by the non-dominant finger tap in response to impulsive stimulus, Although they started from the dynamic system perspective, i.e. continuous movement primarily is concerned, only information processing perspective was taken, i.e. discrete events, which equals an abstraction of the continuous position signal, generated by the finger tap was analyzed. Recording the finger positions as analogue signals is approached to improve the classical evaluation. The motion of the fingers would be less harmonic in tapping on hard surface and did not take the standard cosines form (Haken, Kelso, & Bunz 1985). The degree of Asymmetry in the flexion and extension movement times would increase (Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer 2004). Semjen & Summers (2002) recorded Sample movement trajectories in the production of bimanual rhythmic movements with a 1:2 frequency ratio. They showed that although both hands often moved together, the slower hand alternated in full-amplitude and reduced-amplitude movements. 5.2 The movement order A prominent theme of motor control research is that complex movements are produced by combining elements from fundamental classes of primitive movements (Hogan & Sternad 2007). One compartmentalization in the research literature is the separation between rhythmic and discrete movements (Wing & Kristofferson 1973a, b; Schulz 1978; Delignières, Lemoine, & Torre 2004; Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry 2002). Each repetition is separated by a salient event in tapping on the hard surface with hits are performed in a rhythmic fashion (Hogan & Sternad 2007). This periodic tapping should be considered as a sequence of individual steps or continuous rhythmic movements (Hogan & Sternad 2007)? We employed contact-free tapping to partly mask this salient event and isometric tapping where the fingers were fixed. Isometric tapping would substantially mask the execution level of motor and eliminate the discrete properties of sensory feedback. The recurrent periodic behaviour at the cognitive level in the case of this reduced degree-of-freedom would be pronounced. 5.3 The effects of feedback Perceived synchrony seems to depend on all available forms of sensory evidence (Aschersleben 2002). Central representation of the tap is formed by various feedback components (Mates & Aschersleben 2000). More explicit temporal control is required when continuous movements encompass a series of discrete contact (Delignieres, Lemoine, & Torre 2004; Zelaznik, Spencer, & Ivry 123 2002). Rapid internal phase correction and a slow internal period correction of the tapping period occurred (Repp 2001). In timing control if the actual tap is timed from the preceding tap then the nature of the perceptual information on which period correction is based has to be considered. Period correction is based on perception of discrepancies between the internal timekeeper period and the sequence IOI duration (Mates 1994a, b). According to pattern formation and selforganization from synergetic the periodic timing in tapping as an oscillator will be self-sustaining and their long-term behaviour will be periodically stable with a specific combination of the damping terms and restoring terms (Haken, Kelso, & Bunz 1985). These terms have to be more complex in the presence of discrete features in comparison with pure continuous movements. 5.4 The effects of amplitude (force) Force fields generated by spinal cord compartments serve as primitives have been suggested (Bizzi, Mussa-Ivaldi, & Giszter 1991; Giszter, Mussa-Ivaldi, & Bizzi 1993; Hart & Giszter 2004). Mirror movement is a robust feature of the mature motor system which is reported in many studies (Carson 20059. This tendency not only occurred in association with specific disorders of the CNS but is also observed frequently in normally developing children (Carson 2005). Motor irradiation, an increase in the excitability of the (opposite) homologous motor pathways is visible when unimanual movements are performed. Strong tapping is used in both ST and DT experiments (Carson 2005). The increased force clearly leads to the increased accumulating evidence from different sensory channels (Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001; Aschersleben 2002). 5.5 The effects of multiple effectors Involuntary contractions emerged during intended unilateral engagement of the opposite limb (Carson 2005). Simultaneous activation of homologous muscles during bimanual coordination produces a more stable pattern than alternative activation (egocentric constraint; Swinnen et al. 1997). The transition-related activation that is distinct from motor execution activation not only might be because our preference for symmetry arises from a preference for perceptual symmetry (Mechsner et al. 2001; Salter et al. 2004; Welsh, Almeida, & Lee 2005) but also from a strong constraint derived from the interhemispheric anatomical coupling (Aramaki et al. 2006). The effects of multiple effectors clearly leads to following effects: the increase of the integration degree of different central control signals related to each effector’s movement (Drewing et al. 2004; Ivry, Keele, & Diener 1988; Ivry RB & Hazeltine 1999; Helmuth & Ivry 1996); the increase of oscillatory coupling degree (Beek, Peper, & Daffertshofer 2002; Haken, Kelso, & Bunz 1985); and again the increased accumulating evidence from different sensory channels (Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001; Aschersleben 2002). 5.6 The effects of attention With attention we can perfectly tap at one tempo while listening to an auditory sequence of beats at a different tempo (Repp 2006). Period correction is sensitive to the attentional requirements of the task and period correction is, at least in part, a higher level cognitive function (Repp & Keller 2004). Phase correction and period correction in response to perturbation are based on the fluctuations of attentional energy entrained by a metronome sequence (Barnes & Jones 2000). Thus rhythm perception is viewed as a form of covert synchronization and attentional dynamics are closely linked to the motor system, with motor imagery or simulation of the sensory consequences of 124 rhythmic action accompanying the burst of attentional energy (Barnes & Jones 2000). Additional mental task or apply of multi effectors would require more attentional energy but the performance could be rewarded or impacted. Focus on one movement would increase its performance but would decrease the performance of other. 5.7 Single-task conditions 5.7.1 Basic considerations 5.7.1.1 Dynamical concept for control of periodic finger tapping An oscillator is any system in which every state is constrained to recur at regular intervals. In finger tapping we refer to the rest position of the finger as the “equilibrium position”. The displacement of the fingertip during tapping equals the distance from the equilibrium position. We can perform periodic finger tapping without any external pacemaker, which has lead to the hypothesis of an “internal clock” in the nervous system generating the periodic rhythm (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b; Ivry 1996). The periodic dynamics of the internal clock for the periodic finger tapping control, its stability was demonstrated by the study of Scholz, Kelso, & Schöner (1987), can be modeled by a so-called limit-cycle oscillator. The limit-cycle oscillators incorporate a dissipative mechanism to damp oscillation that grow too large and a source of energy to pump up those that become too small, i.e. as a result of this idealization, at the notion of conservative systems we assume that the sum of potential and kinetic energy remains constant (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). For example as simplified systems with restricted questions the energy of the RC circuit consisted of the energy of the electric field in the capacitor (q /2C), and the energy of the RL circuit consisted of the energy of the magnetic field in the self-induction coil (=Li /2). The energy also remains unvaried in the jump because the charge q of the capacitor in the first case and the current i in the second do not vary (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). The state of the motion of periodic process through phase space presented in its coordinates is described in mathematic. The study of the dynamics of oscillatory systems had used at first the phase plane. The motion builds a particular path in the phase space and after some perturbation return to their accustomed rhythm if it is hit out (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). The orthogonal Cartesian coordinates provides the independent variable x and its derivation y in the concept of the phase plane and on this x, y plane the motion of a harmonic oscillator is studied. A point on the x, y plane representing the state of the periodic process have the corresponding values of the coordinate x and velocity y. There is one and only one state of the system which corresponds to each point on the x, y plane. Totality of all possible states of the periodic process is represented. The so-called "representative" point and its velocity is called the phase velocity. The representative point follows on a path called a phase path. Note that this path is not the motion trajectory and the phase velocity is not the motion velocity. The so-called integral curve is for example determined by the equation dy/dx = -ω02 * x/y. A curve which over the whole time of its motion (from t = - ∞ to t = + ∞) described by the representative point will be called a complete phase path. Singular points designate the points where the direction of the tangent becomes indeterminate. Separatrices define the isolated finite sections of phase curve passing through singular points (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). 125 5.7.1.2 Information processing concept for timing mechanism The judgments of temporal equivalence are based on a memory for prescribed interval duration (Keele et al. 1989) that we learn during synchronization phase. The perceptual centre of the perceptual or motor event we use as the reference point for synchronization. This perceptual centre (Morton, Marcus, & Frankish 1976) is determined not only by stimulus length or stimulus intensity in the perceptual events but also by factors such as the intensity of somatosensory feedback in the motor events. We continue the external beat after the initial events and then use that movement as a base for judgment and a timed interval is recycled from end to beginning, i.e. the central timer generates at these specific times a motor command for the finger to tap to the peripheral implementation system, which in turn generates the motor response (Wing & Kristofferson 1973b). The central timer as the temporal pacemaker produces regular output pulses at this constant base frequency (Treisman 1963; Treisman et al. 1990). 5.7.2 The effects of feedback Bimanual advantage, Performance during multi-effector tapping benefit from integration of various feedbacks and of several central command signals. Because the neuronal pathways that connect sensory neuron to the central nervous system are different, the most benefit of feedback is clearly in voice tapping. The performance of timekeeper clearly is improved. But breath keeping during voice tapping would impact the task performance. The discrete feature of tactile feedback is mostly masked in contact-free tapping. Thus the degree of asymmetry in the flexion and extension movement reduced and the movement trajectory would be more proportional. The Performance of timekeeper is expected to be suffered. The movement amplitude without constraint would lead to increase of motor variance. Because both performances (timekeeper and motor implementation) are suffered in voice tapping and contact-free tapping, the correlation of successive intervals is difficult to predict. The discrete feature of tactile feedback also is partly masked in isometric tapping but the peripheral motor implementation is excluded in isometric tapping. Thus a prediction of zero of the correlation lag-1 between adjacent intervals is expected. A tight coupling of sensorimotor processes in trained musicians extends to preattentively mediated reflexes (Bangert et al. 2006). This evidence leads to the question whether a tight coupling of the required rhythm of the tapping and motor domains in musicians exists (Bangert et al. 2006). Musicians have a stronger cognitive representation of rhythm than non-musicians. Would this mental representation of rhythm have an effect on blinking? 5.7.3 The effects of amplitude (force) Separate timing signals are generated for each effector and then are integrated to produce coordinated motor commands (Ivry, Keele, & Diener 1988). Increased finger force intensifies not only internal (kinaesthetic/tactile) feedback but also central command signals. It also requires more attentional resource and energy resource for motor implementation. Both the variance resources of time keeper and motor implementation together with the role of feedback come into play. The task performance clearly will benefit from the increase of available sensory information but whether it would also benefit from the more needed energy for motor implementation due to its variance source. A higher required force would increase neural activity on the shared brain network for blinking and tapping and a stronger coupling would be expected to entrain the spontaneous blinking. 126 5.7.4 The effects of multiple effectors Analogously not only the more sensory information but also the more central command signals are available and more attentional resource and energy resource for motor implementation are required. Additional to the cognitive level and the implementation level together with feedback, the integration of various components, come into play. Separate signals are generated for each effector (Ivry, Richardson, & Helmuth 2002). Various feedback components (tactile, kinesthetic) are linearly integrated to form one central representation (Mates & Aschersleben 2000). Motor commands from the two hemispheres are integrated subcortically (Ivry RB & Hazeltine 1999). The task performance would benefit from the subtle error correction that is well below the explicit detection threshold led to effective adjustments in the timing of the motor response (Repp 2000). But the asynchrony between different effectors also leads to this subtle correction. The question is whether this correction process will improve or impact the timing control. The integration of various command signals and the enhanced discrete properties of sensory feedback supporting the rhythmic task create more appropriate conditions for the hand motor system to entrain the concurrent spontaneous blinking. 5.7.5 The effects of attention Timing control apparently requires attention. Phase correction was largely automatic and period correction required conscious awareness and attention (Repp & Keller 2004). The attentional resources can be varied by introducing the mental task. If additional mental tapping changes the tapping performance then attention supports the timekeeper or there is a command signal for this mental tapping that the better performance benefits form the integration of the two command signals. Contra to or pro to the common central timer would be partly reflected. Focus of attention on the nonpreferred hand impacts the dual-task performance (Peters 1985) and would also impacts the bimanual advantage. More focus on the required task would modulate the spontaneous blink rate. 5.8 Dual-task conditions 5.8.1 Basic considerations The stationary motion can be first of all states of equilibrium in which accelerations reduce to zero, i.e. disturbance has no effect on the periodic movement. In the vicinity of the states of equilibrium force disturbance either bring back the periodic movement to the state of equilibrium or remove it still farther away. In the first case we shall have stable states of equilibrium and in the second unstable ones (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). The phenomenon of phase depending resetting of the behaviour of the periodic movement would not only reveal the states of equilibrium but also determine their stability with respect to small variations of the coordinates and velocities. Stable states of equilibrium are in this case a necessary condition that the system might be found in the vicinity after undergoing the disturbance duration. Pulse coupling is common in biology. Fireflies interact only when they see the sudden flash of another and shift their rhythm accordingly (Strogatz & Stewart 1993). Phase shift of periodic rhythm by the sudden impulsive force is the paradigm of pulse coupled oscillator system. Force will attract the periodic movement to the position of equilibrium or repels it away from a position of equilibrium and both are proportional to the displacement of system (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). Thus we will peek into the magical world of coupled nonlinear oscillators. 127 5.8.1.1 The effects of discrete movement on periodic movement Due to oscillator coupling the impulsive discrete movement subjected to a limit-cycle oscillator moves the state point away from the limit-cycle, but it returns to the limit-cycle asymptotically and therefore causes the phase shift (Yoshino et al. 2002). The degree of this phase shift, demonstrated by the phase-resetting-curve, would reveal much information about the underlying dynamical system. If there is no interaction (phase shift = 0) the tapping behaviour complying with Type 1 Phase Reset and if the phase shift is outright Type 0 Phase Reset has to be presented. Modification of the bottom-up effect such as somatosensory feedback and top-down effect such as force, attention, multiple effectors would lead to different results. The physiological parameters (force, attention, feedback, etc.) would determine the shifting degree so that a mixed form of phase resetting is possible. As the parameter is varied, the integral curves or PRC curves will vary. If we assume that the potential energy is an analytic function of the parameter, then these variations will occur continuously (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1966). The general form of the curves will undergo quantitative variations only, and only for certain special so-called "bifurcation" values of the parameter shall we have qualitative variations of the character of the curves. The bifurcation values of the parameter will be, in this case, the values of the parameter for which a variation of the character of the singular points and separatrices occurs. Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin (1966) gave the following definition which is not connected with the conservativeness of the system: a value of the parameter λ = λ0, will be called by us ordinary if such a finite ε (ε > 0) exists that for all λ satisfying the condition | λ - λ0| < ε we have the same topologic structure in the mapping-out of the phase plane by the integral curves. The other values of the parameters for which this condition is not satisfied will be called bifurcation or branch values. This existence of these bifurcation or branch values would clearly be reflected in our position signal. 5.8.1.2 The effect of periodic movement on discrete movement In most reports, only one-sided-affect of periodic finger on discrete finger was studied. Staude, Cong-Khac, & Wolf (2006) reported the mutual interaction between two different movements on one finger. The movement trajectory during interaction and the reaction time of the discrete movement would reflect the effect of the periodic movement on it. If this effect is found then the question is whether this gating also exists in the same dual-task for movement coordination but of different fingers. 5.8.2 The effects of feedback The effectiveness of the conscious process involved in perceptual judgment and action planning mentioned in 5.3 has to be taken into account in the case that if the clearly disturbance of the periodic movement by the discrete movement is easily perceivable. Furthermore the dynamic system approach does not include the role of perception in coordination and does not address other possible stable phase differences. If the central representation of the tap through its sensory feedback is used as a base for timing judgment then there is ample evidence for the importance of sensory effects on the timing of movements and are the physiological parameters and dynamical parameters (the restoring force and damping force) determine the shifting degree of the state point on limit-cycle. 128 5.8.3 The effects of amplitude (force) Phase-shifting stimuli are posited to change the state variables in all research topics using PRCs and their strength determines the new state. In fact, in every organism for which type 0 resetting has been found experimentally, a stimulus of reduced strength has been shown to lead to type 1 (Kronauer, Jewett, & Czeisler 1993; Peterson 1980; Taylor et al 1982; Gander & Lewis 1983). Force combined with the rate of tapping can shift the value of critical point at which antiphase tapping becomes unstable (Loesby, Piek, & Barrett 2001). Intensified force requires attentional energy and increases the command signal. Focus on nonpreferred hand damages the dual-task performance (Peter 1985). Increasing force of the discrete movement acting as a force on the state point on limit-cycle would increase the degree of the phase shift. The more required attentional energy together with the enhancement of sensory information by increasing force on the periodic movement would support the timing control but as mentioned above whether the more needed energy resource for motor implementation would also have effect on the motor variance source. 5.8.4 The effects of attention The hypothesis that physiological constraint which is responsible for the interaction is affected by varying the instruction was proposed (De Rugy & Sternad 2003). The moment that is physiologically optimal in combination of the discrete movement with the ongoing oscillation is the synchronization. The intention to initiate the discrete movement as fast as possible revealed that the concurrent effect of focused central control signal relating to discrete movement acting on the rhythmic movement against its free landing choice due to time-stressed emerges (De Rugy & Sternad 2003). Dual-task performance suffered when attention was focused on the non-preferred hand during bimanual dual-tasks (Peters 1985). Focus on one movement would entrain the other movement, i.e. focus on periodic movement would improve the timing function and focus on discrete movement would impact. 5.8.5 Eye-Hand movement coordination without common spatial target Since possible perceptual and movement production bottlenecks (De Jong 1993) as well as cross-talk (Navon & Miller 1987) may be avoided in a way that the stimuli (visual and auditory) as well as the required responses (saccade and finger tapping, respectively) for the two tasks engage different modalities. Eye movements towards a lateral visual target (prosaccades) are performed automatically (Roberts, Hager, & Heron 1994). The significantly higher rate of gain of information of the eye system over the motor system suggested that certain features of the information processing occurred independently (Megaw & Armstrong 1973). Saccadic eye movements and button-press responses could be initiated without delay (Bekkering et al. 1994). It is also consequent to ask what happens when these two effectors (eyes and hands), now not sharing a target, are acting independently but concurrently. Only few studies have explored eye-hand interaction in different DT situations (e.g., Bekkering et al. 1994; Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman 1993; Claeys et al. 1999; Stuyven et al. 2000), and their results are ambiguous. In DT paradigms with two discrete tasks (manual reaction and saccade, e.g., Bekkering et al. 1994), oculo-manual (OM) interference was found in case of targets being unpredictable in time and space but not in case of some degree of predictability. DT paradigms with combination of periodic and discrete tasks were also used (e.g., Claeys et al. 1999; Stuyven et al. 2000) and showed some general small increase of saccade latency due to the secondary (tapping) task, but, unfortunately, the tapping behaviour was not analyzed. This demonstrates that the issue of interference between eye and hand movements has remained 129 controversial. On the one hand, since both effectors share some common brain structures, OM interaction effects in DT would not be surprising. On the other hand, this sharing of common networks must not necessarily be effective in all DT conditions but can be redundant (i.e., not basically necessary for the DT execution) allowing some independence of the two tasks. Therefore, our study addresses the question of whether the strong DT interaction found in BM-DT condition is also be present in an equivalent OM-DT condition. 5.8.6 Multiple effectors How are the cortical areas involved in these motor processes? Within the primary motor cortex, there is no overlapping or anatomical linking of hand and foot representations (Huntley & Jones 1991); however, there are common upstream to the primary motor cortex from secondary motor hand and foot areas (Murthy & Fetz 1996). Hand and foot motor representations considerably overlap in the secondary motor areas (the dorsal and ventral premotor cortices, the supplementary motor area, and the cingulate cortex) (Fink et al. 1997), where coordination-related interactions between hand and foot movements should appear (Byblow et al. 2007). Baldissera et al. (2002) showed that excitatory changes occur in the hand motor area during voluntary cyclic movements of the foot even if the hand is resting during the experiment. Raethjen et al. (2008) showed that the cortical motor areas are involved in the generation of voluntary rhythmic foot movements in a similar way as they are during rhythmic voluntary hand movements (Pollok et al. 2005). When a hand and a foot move together, a strong interaction takes place between their cortical areas (Liepert, Terborg, & Weiller 1999). In summary, neurophysiological data supports the following two alternate hypotheses: concurrent hand-foot responses in a DT situation can be performed either in an integrated or — alternatively — in an isolated way. Thus, analysis of previous reports does not allow clear conclusions of interaction and coordination between periodic and discrete tapping movements in DTs executed by the combined effectors such as the upper and lower limbs — neither from physiological nor from the observational point of view. Synchronization emerges cooperatively in communities of oscillators. If a few oscillators happen to synchronize, their combined signal exerting a stronger effect on the others, i.e. the additional oscillators into the synchronized nucleus amplify the integrated signal (Strogatz & Stewart 1993). Timing is better as more sensory information becomes available (Drewing & Aschersleben 2003), the timer variance for both hands decreased systematically with increasing sensory of the extra (left) hand. The bimanual advantage decreased for both hands when auditory feedback for the taps with the extra hand was omitted. Person suffering from a complete and permanent loss of tactile and kinaesthetic afferents and consequently having no peripherally originating feedback of movement were involved. Drewing et al. (2004) compared their results with those –matched controls. This person showed an even more pronounced bimanual advantage than controls. Hence the bimanual advantage is not only due to actual sensory feedback but also profits in opposite direction from the averaging of different central control signals that relate to each effector’s movements. This kind of integration was also illustrated in the hand-foot overlap area in secondary motor areas (Fink et al. 1997) and the originated upstream of primary motor cortex (Byblow et al. 2007) by their coordination-related interactions. Thus on the one side the more amount of sensory information in bimanual single-task support timing but on the other side the more intensity of central control signals. According to the functional cerebral distance model (Kinsbourne & Hicks 1978) and the confirmed less stable ipsilateral hand–foot coordination patterns for anti-phase 130 coordination a difference may be in number of anti-phase coordination between ipsilateral and contralateral hand–foot coordination patterns has to be present. 131 6 Methods – Experimental Concepts, Recorded signals There are numerous examples of temporal synchronization and rhythmicity in group-living organisms (Greenfield & Roizen 1993; Snedden & Greenfield 1998) and human (McClintock 1971). Also, there are theoretical systems of coupled oscillators and corresponding experimental observations on the behaviour of interacting oscillators in physics. Thus, asking for analogy in biological systems motivated the present study: Can the phenomenon of phase depending resetting also be observed in the human motor system? The answer must be given by experiments: the focus is directed to the dual-task (DT) coordination of a simple discrete motor response with a periodic action, which requires observation of the neuromuscular system in conjunction with the perceptual system during DT execution. Three issues are mainly addressed: (i) the stationarity of the periodic movement when disturbed by the discrete reaction, (ii) the reaction times measured by the discrete responses during the concurrent execution of a periodic movement, and (iii) the time relationship between both motor actions. Besides the timing parameter, the spatial information about the movement trajectories is an interesting parameter. Experimental work mostly reports about one-sided-effects; i.e. both motor components of a DT experiment (the periodic movement and the discrete movement) were performed by the same limb. E.g., Staude, Dengler & Wolf (2002) conducted experiments with subjects performing reactive rapid index finger abduction movements during a secondary internally paced rhythmic (tremor-like) adduction-abduction movement of the same finger. The results confirmed the ability of one movement to constrain or even impede the execution of the other. Due to the physical binding of both in the same effector, it remained unclear where this interaction takes place within the sensorimotor chain. Also, this type of experiment is difficult for the subjects to perform and requires high concentration. Another type of motor control experiment is tapping (i.e. up-down movement) introduced by Stevens (1886) and mostly performed by the index fingers; it is much easier to do, and tasks can be distributed between different effectors (both hands and both feet). In addition, the neural pathways and control of these effectors are well known, which allows a physiological interpretation of observed data. Thus, tapping was selected as the basic experimental concept for this study. 6.1 Materials and method 6.1.1 Subjects and experimental setup Twenty two right-handed young healthy subjects (ages 21 - 50, mean 27) took part in the different experimental conditions (described later). . Their informed consent according to the international Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research (2002) were given but the aim of the study was not. All subjects did not have any disease or any history or signs of disease or neurological disorder and had normal vision. 6.1.2 Experimental setup The classical tapping setup is very simple, which leads to a widespread use of tapping in experimental psychology: pacing is performed by some digitally controlled discrete visual or auditory stimuli, and the finger taps activate micro switches when hitting the ground surface. Thus, only recording of a few digital signals is required, and evaluation of these time series simply needs to detect the state change of binary signals by some threshold procedure. Finally, the intervals between state changes in signals are determined and statistically analyzed (Wing, Kristofferson, 1973b). 132 Clearly, this basic setup can be improved significantly by recording the finger positions as analogue signals, because they comprise much more detailed information about the kinetic process of the finger movements, if sophisticated signal evaluation tools far beyond simple threshold are used. Figure 6-1: Experimental situation. Actual experiment investigates bimanual normal tapping together with saccadic eye movement recording. Figure 6-2: Two types of experiments: A) normal tapping with moving index finger; B) isometric tapping without movement of the limb but force development only. Figure 6-3: A) Schematic illustration of the experimental setup. B) Sensors and stimulators. The tapping of the index fingers is recorded by the force transducers (embedded in the table top) responding to the hits, and by vertical laser position sensors fixed above the finger. The pacing signal during the synchronization phase and the go signal during the continuation phase are generated by the loudspeaker and/or the LED-flash in the middle. 133 Figure 6-4: EMG recording from the finger flexors and extensors. Left plot indicates the electrode positions (arrows), the right picture shows the active electrode used. Figure 6-5: Foot pedals for including feet as tapping effectors. Left: 3 pedals are located such that participants can operate them comfortably. The left and right pedal are for tapping responses, the pedal between is for requesting a break in the experimental procedure. Right: The return spring was adjusted such that the counteracting force equals the gravity and the passive elasticity forces of the soleus muscle; therefore, the foot remains in the upper resting position without muscle activity. The general experimental situation is scheme of our advanced experimental setup is shown in Fig. 6-1: Participants were seated with elbows comfortably semi-flexed on a height-adjustable chair at the table in a well illuminated room (luminance about 45 Lux), their forearms and palms rested on a table (ulnar side down), which allowed effortless bimanual tapping with the index fingers. In the normal tapping condition, tapping was performed by moving the finger tip up and down with the upper position being the resting position (Fig. 6-2A). In contact-free tapping ("tapping in the air"), elbows and hand palm were rested on rubber foam tailored such that the index finger cannot reach the table surface during the tapping. In isometric tapping, the subject’s index fingers were fixed to the force sensors by orthopedic finger splints. Thus, the subjects were not able to lift their index fingers but only they could develop directional up and down forces (Fig. 6-2B). A schematic overview of the tapping experiment is given in Fig. 6-3. It shows the sensors for recording the physical quantities together with stimulus sources: Two force transducers embedded in the tabletop record the tap hits (ground contact) of the index finger tip of the left and the right hand, respectively. Also, there are two laser sensors to measure the vertical position of the index fingers, and, according to Fig. 6-4, active EMG electrodes MYO 110 (Liberting Technologies Inc.) are applied to register the EMG from the extensors and flexors located in the forearms. For the single discrete tap either acoustic or visual stimulus, for the periodic tap a sequence of beeps were used. For the ocular-manual (OM) experiment, the setup was supplemented by a chin-rest and two light-bars each consisting of 4 bright, vertically aligned LEDs (bar size: 0.8 degree of width, 3.6 degree of height, 38 degrees distance between bars); they were fixed on a white screen and served as saccade target (right bar) and as the fixation mark (left bar). The white screen was placed at the distance of 70 cm at eye level in front of the participant. For eye movement registration, the IRIS infra-red light eye movement system (model 6500, SKALAR Medical b.v.) mounted on a helmet was 134 employed. The device provides an optimal resolution of 2 minutes of arc within 90 degrees horizontally and 35 degrees vertically, and it yields a bandwidth of 100 Hz (-3 dB). In experiments without eye movement recording, eye blinks were monitored by electrooculographic techniques (EOG). A single-channel high-gain differential input electrooculogram amplifier module (EOG100B, Biopac Inc., USA) was used together with two Ag-AgCl electrodes placed above and below the right eye referenced to linked mastoids (as used e.g., by Skotte et al. 2007) for tracking the eye blinks. For including tapping by feet, two foot-switches were added to the setup (Fig. 6-5). They were fixed on the floor for recording right and left foot taps. Foot was placed on the switch in a resting position, and, in response to the go signal, it was pressed downwards while the force and position changes developed by the foot was recorded. For the force signal recording, a force transducer was inserted between lower pedal surface and the reset spring, and for the position signal recording, the angle of the pedal with respect to horizontal level was indicated by a potentiometer fixed to the pivot of the pedal. Of course, these two signals contain almost the same information and work in parallel. Figure 6-6: Screen dump of Diadem configuration. The box on the right shows the configured input channels. Finally, a microphone was installed for voice recording to allow "verbal tapping" by respectively saying the same "word" like … tac tac tac tac …. with sequence timing according to the pacing during the synchronization phase. Loudspeakers provided the auditory pacing signals (50 ms tone duration, 1000 Hz) with an ISI of 600 ms (in some cases, other interval durations were used). Participants were also exposed to the auditory go-signal (50 ms, 2000 Hz) (right bar) which triggered the discrete tap (or saccade). A Pentium IV computer equipped with two input-output cards (PCI-MIO-16-4, National Instrument Inc) controlled the experiments and recorded all relevant signals at a sampling frequency of 1 kHz using DIADEM 9.1 software; Fig. 6-6 depicts a configuration scheme of the experimental control. 135 6.2 Procedures and experimental paradigms Tapping can be conducted in two basic kinds: (i) moving limb or (ii) fixed limb, both with periodic antagonistic activation of the muscles serving the joint. When the limb is moving ("normal tapping"), the position is recorded over time and the force sensor is recording the touch-down forces of the "landing" limb; when the limb is fixed ("isometric tapping"), the force developed by the muscles is recorded as a function of time. When "tapping in the air" is performed, no forces are recorded but the finger positions only. As already emphasized, the interaction in dual-task was focused by this study mainly. Thus, the experimental work also concentrates on DT experiments. However, tapping in DT is based on the undisturbed tapping behaviour which can be observed in single-task condition. Therefore, some ST aspects were roughly investigated in addition to obtain a reference platform for interpretation of the DT tapping behaviour. These ST experiments fit more to pilot study character, but they demonstrate the versatility of the experimental design of this work and the promising perspectives for using it in future research activities. The following sections will describe all the different ST and DT paradigms. 6.2.1 The Synchronization-Continuation paradigm Figure 6-7: Time course of the tapping paradigm in dual-task condition. The basic element is a trial comprising a synchronization phase and continuation phase. The latter is divided in 12 segments. All pacing occurs mostly with an interstimulus interval ISI = 600 ms. The basic experimental concept of this work is the synchronization/continuation tapping task as introduced by Stevens (1886). The basic structure is depicted in Fig. 6-7. Beside the reaction time single-task, the experimental session consisted of 24 trials, each trial was about 75 s, 22s or 9s and starts with introducing the tapping pace by five or twelve pacing audio signals in the leading “synchronization phase” which allows the participant to adjust his/her tapping to the pace frequency; in principle, this represents a closed loop condition. Then, the participants proceed with a self-paced tapping in the so-called “continuation phase” at the same rate as introduced by the external pacer before. 136 Figure 6-8: The concept of the feedback loop control the dynamic behaviour of the performance. The sensed value is compared to the desired value to create the error for the controller. Other internal interferences arise from other cortical processes like execution of cognitive tasks. The salient point of this method is that this continuation tapping is performed in open loop condition, which is very sensitive to any interference with other ongoing processes and sensorimotor inputs (Fig. 6-8). Figure 6-9: Time course of the tapping paradigm in single-task condition. The basic element is a trial comprising a synchronization phase and continuation phase. The latter is divided in 12 segments each ending with a resynchronization phase where a triad of paces were presented additionally (shown by a group of 3 vertical arrows). This paradigm was the same for unimanual and bimanual tapping. For some statistical analysis, larger deviations of the actual tapping frequency from the original pace signal frequency are obstructive. Therefore, the original concept of Stevens (1886) was extended: the continuation phase of each trial was additionally divided into 12 (bi-partite) segments; the first part of each segment (with duration randomly selected between 6 s and 8 s) involved pure self-paced tapping, whereas in the second resynchronization part (Fig. 6-9), an additional triad of pacing signals helped to stabilize the tapping rate in self-pacing. The onset of the first pace within the triad was synchronized with the actual tap, whereas the next two paces occurred with the standard ISI as used in the synchronization phase (Fig. 6-9). 6.2.2 Single-task conditions Basically, all tasks used as combined set in DT conditions should be investigated in isolated form, too, and performed in ST conditions. There are two categories: 137 periodic tapping and its control discrete tap execution in reaction time condition The group of the discrete tap was generalized to discrete motor responses like saccadic eye movements and eye blinks, whereas the periodic tapping was extended to "voice tapping" with vocalising "… tac … tac … tac …". 6.2.2.1 Reaction Time RT These experiments allowed recording of undisturbed discrete responses. A single discrete tapping experiment in which the observers performed 72 stimulus-induced discrete taps in the absence of any periodic movement under normal and isometric conditions, respectively, was the basic scheme. To preserve the bimanual tapping scenario of the DT experiment, subjects were required to respond to the acoustic stimulus by a simultaneous discrete tap of both the left and the right index finger, but only the left hand signal was used to determine the reaction time (RT). The observers were required to response with a single saccade to the onset of the visual gosignal, the saccade latency was measured. The same spatio-temporal parameters as in the oculomanual dual-task were used for this simple saccadic task. 6.2.2.2 Periodic Tapping ST In order to compare of the timing of the periodic tapping in isolated and dual-task conditions, a single manual experiment in which participants tapped without a concurrent discrete response task under normal and isometric conditions. Figure 6-10: (A) Timing of the "blink experiment". Again, the trial comprised a synchronization phase and continuation phase, the latter with resynchronization for stabilization of the tap frequency. The first pace of the resynchronization triad was synchronized with the actual periodic tap. (B) Basically, the scheme represents a ST concept because the blink is assumed to occur independently. The temporal relationship of the blink with respect to the reference tap is defined by the interval tperturbation and the phase = tperturbation / ISI. For replicating Drewing & Aschersleben’s (2003) results in simple bimanual tapping, the same design of synchronization/continuation procedure was used under three conditions: left hand only, right hand only and both hands synchronously. To obtain modification of tactile feedback normal, 138 contact-free, isometric and voice tapping were employed. The session consisted of three parts, each containing blocks of nine trials of each experimental condition. The trial consisted of 12 taps for synchronization phase and about 40 taps (15 taps) for finger tapping (for voice tapping) in continuation phase. The order of blocks was counterbalanced within and between participants by a latin square design (Bailey 1996). A Latin square is an n × n table filled with n different symbols in such a way that each symbol occurs exactly once in each row and exactly once in each column. The order was not changed in each part. Five participants were tested in these experiments. In all conditions, trials containing at least one rather long or short intertap interval (outliers) or with a linear trend of the duration of the intertap intervals were immediately repeated (the mean intertap interval ± four standard deviations determined online in the actual trial served as tolerance range for the outlier, and linear trends were defined by a significant (p < .05) correlation between the length of an intertap interval and its sequence order within the trial showing a range of [1 . . 40] and [1 .. 15] in finger tapping and voice tapping, respectively. The used ISI was 400 ms, in hand-foot experiments also ISI = 600 ms was used but also other ISI values were tested in pilot experiments. To check the spontaneous blinking during the periodic tapping, the following experiments were conducted with the ISI of 550 ms (Fig. 6-10) Experiment 1: standard tapping. Participants could tap as they felt comfortable, without any special instruction about finger movement and contact force. Experiment 2: strong tapping. Participants have got instructions to tap stronger, i.e., with force more than in case of standard tapping but not exaggerating (to avoid fatigue). Experiment 3: impulse-like tapping. The finger taps had to be as short as possible. A specific instruction was given: the upward and downward movements of the finger tip had to be moved as fast as possible with the duration of the ground contact as short as possible. No tap force restrictions were applied. Each experiment was split into two parts: the first 12 trials were dedicated to unimanual tapping with the index finger of the dominant hand only, followed by 12 trials of bimanual tapping with both index fingers simultaneously. Two participants performed in the inverted order but no order effect was found. In unimanual condition, the non-dominant (inactive) index finger rested on the force sensor surface. Seven participants took part in all three experimental conditions and a control condition. 6.2.2.3 Periodic limb tapping and mental (cognitive) tapping ST As in 6.6.6.6 the same periodic tapping ST was performed concurrently with a simple periodic mental task to replicate experiments of Drewing & Aschersleben’s (2003). Different peripheral motor implementations were realized by different experiment conditions (normal, contact-free, isometric). Unimanual tapping and bimanual tapping were approached. The correlation function between adjacent periodic intertap intervals was analyzed. The multiple-limb advantage was concerned. As a mental task, counting from 1 to 4 (4-grouping condition) and from 1 to 8 (8-grouping condition) was used. Pilot experiments showed in the 4-grouping condition that counting " .. 1 .. 2 .. 3 .. 4 .. 1 .. 2 .. 3 .. 4 .. 1 .." can be substituted a simple rhythm like " .. tac .. tac .. tac.. tac.. tac.. tac .. tac .. tac.. tac.." emphasizing the forth item in sequence. The imaginary (silent) counting was performed in mother language without any additional motor activation but rather as a pure mental tapping. 139 6.2.3 Dual-task conditions Figure 6-11: Dual-task condition. During the periodic tapping, the participants are required to perform discrete taps in response to go signals (stimuli) during the continuation phase. The temporal relationship with respect to the reference tap is defined by the interval tperturbation and the phase = tperturbation / ISI. In DT experiments, participants had to concurrently perform a discrete motor task by the nondominant finger, by the feet or by the eyes, and a rhythmic motor task by the index finger of the dominant hand, or both tasks by the dominant hand (Fig. 6-11). Within the continuation phase of each trial, go-signals for discrete responses were interspersed at randomized interval of 2-5 s (Fig. 611); the participants had to respond to these stimuli by the execution of one or several discrete motor reactions (e.g., a tap or a saccade). There were 12 go-signals with corresponding 12 discrete responses within each trial. Thus, a total of 288 discrete responses were usually obtained within a session. The ISI of 600 ms was used (according to Yoshino et al. 2002) as it was reported to be convenient for participants, but also other ISI values were used in some pilot experiments. In all DT conditions, participants were asked to react as quickly as possible by either a single tap, a combination of hand/foot taps or a saccade, in response to a randomly presented audio stimulus, whereas they should maintain the periodic movement as regular as possible. This basic paradigm was used in eight specific experimental schemes: 1. Experiment 1: normal tapping with tactile feedback of finger tip landing. Participants could tap as they felt comfortable, without any special instruction for the requested tapping movement. The goal of this condition was to study the interaction between the discrete event (e.g. finger beat) as disturbance and the periodic tapping events. 2. Experiment 2: isometric tapping without movement of the fingers. The mechanical constraints of the executive limbs are not further effective, thus their consideration by the executive brain level is not demanded. But it reduced the sensory availability of pronounced discrete events as well as the kinesthetic feedback which both are used for explicit timing control. Thus, an exaggerated instability of periodic movement was expected. 3. Experiment 3: contact-free tapping ("tapping in the air"). As a mixture of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, tactile feedback from the finger tip is avoided by removing of discrete sensory events such as landing of the finger tip, but maintaining the mechanical movement and demanding its brain control. Again, a reduced stability of periodic movement was expected 4. Experiment 4: strong tapping. The finger taps should be very pronounced. Peak contact force aim was individually adjusted to participants (20 percent of the maximum voluntary force). 140 The concurrent effect is an increased central control related to each effector’s movements, and, in addition, an increased sensory information inflow (kinesthetic/tactile) which needed to be processed. 5. Experiment 5: normal periodic tapping by the dominant hand competing with discrete reactions including combinations of hand/foot reaction (multi effector responses): RH-LF - participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with left foot. RH-RF - participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with right foot. RH-LH-LF - participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with left hand and left foot together. RH-LH-RF - participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with left hand and right foot together. RH-RF-LF- participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with right and left feet together. RH-LH-RF-LF – participants performed periodic tapping with right hand and executed discrete responses with left hand and both feet together. The increase of the motor control load through the demanded integration of multiple discrete central commands caused an augmented load to central levels, as well as lateralization effects had to be considered. 6. Experiment 6: as Experiment 1 but with more attention to one of the two movements, which lead to a prioritization of a task. Subjects were instructed either to react as fast as possible or to maintain the rhythmic movement as constant as possible. 7. Experiment 7: as Experiment 1 but both tasks were performed on the dominant hand. 8. Experiment 8: as Experiment 1 but as the discrete movement the participants had to perform a goal directed saccade. 6.3 Signal analysis Even if some registered data were online processed as required for the closed loop control of the experiments (e.g. synchronization of the resynchronization pacing to the ongoing tapping), most data evaluation was performed offline by Matlab scripts running on PC. Data evaluation concentrates on two aspects: The conventional timing analysis with extraction of the onset and offset times of motor acts (e.g. ground contacts of the finger), and The time course analysis of the analogue signals like position, force, and EMG activity, all of them imaging the continuous motor control process of the motor activities. The first task (1) is obligatory required by the aim of the study: DT execution means task switching, which is assessed by analyzing the timing structure of the motor events. And tapping itself is a sequence of motor acts (down-movement, up-movement, pause in resting position), which is characterized by their timing. Thus, detection of these motor events must be performed to obtain a segmentation of the continuous experimental recordings according to the different sensorimotor processes. The second task (2) mirrors the advanced study character of this work, also reflecting the engineering thinking of causality that the continuous time course of the system outputs signal for a specific input indicating the system's function. Therefore, time course analysis leading to a classification of the transients between task states refines the pure time events analysis. Within the 141 framework of this study, mainly the change dynamics (e.g. movement gradients, slew rates of slopes, signal discontinuities, etc.) were investigated in the analogue signals like position and force signals. All automatic evaluation results were visually checked before further statistical treatment to avoid misinterpretations of results due to evaluation errors. During this interactive (time-consuming) control, evaluation errors were corrected if possible, or they led to discard the respective single response (less than 5% of the responses). 6.3.1 Event detection The simplest method for event detection is the threshold-based event detection performed on the sensor data; an event can be captured when the observed signal exceeds this predefined threshold. As a more sophisticated approach, the maximum likelihood (ML) method computes the likelihood of a change for each time instant, and the most likely one is taken as the estimate of change onset time. Using simple amplitude threshold methods, particularly weak and abnormal signal profiles may lead to high inaccuracy as well as systematic errors. For detection the change onset of weak and highly variable signal profiles such as EMG-signals, a method bases on statistical signal processing including a priori knowledge on the generator process of the monitored signal was described by Staude (2001). In many applications, the change is rather smooth but not an abrupt one. The linear regression method would miss a local change. For the non-abrupt changes, the rise time of the change as an additional parameter was considered in a so-called 'step, ramp-step and ramp profile model' (Hofer, Staude, & Wolf 2004). The detection procedure used for the evaluation of the force data in normal tapping by this work is based on conventional simple threshold analysis, because there is a binary structure in the signal: (1) signal is at baseline level as long as the finger does not contact the sensor surface and (2) signal shows some substantial component during the impulse-like contact. Therefore, the event detection is simply achieved by using an adaptive threshold criterion (more details in 6.3.2.1). Complicated signals such as finger position, isometric force, eye-blink, saccades, and foot tapping, however, reflect analogue processes for which event detection requests more sophisticated analysis. Therefore, a more complex event detection scheme based on template matching (Hofer, Staude, & Wolf 2005) was applied. 6.3.2 The time course analysis of signals Classical evaluation of experimental tapping data simply determines the intertap interval sequence (i.e. only the times of contact onsets of the finger tip to the ground plate are further considered), which equals an abstraction of the continuous position signal by an event time sequence. Then, this sequence was statistically analyzed. Therefore, inspecting the finger tip position data in normal tapping, leads to an extended analysis of the tapping movements which reflect the motor process in more detail. 142 Figure 6-12: Force signal time course and events in normal tapping. Note that force (in N) increase is shown downwards. a) Force response of a single tap out of the periodic sequence; the contact duration L is split into the landing phase Lp and the resting phase W2. The event markers M1, M2, and M3 are determined through using two sliding windows W1 and W2 for a moving average approach; these windows are structured into two parts x and y. For details, see text. b) Example of automatic event detection result which is interactively controlled subsequently. 6.3.2.1 Simply Structured Signals A typical force response in normal tapping is shown in Fig. 6-12. A single tap magnified in Fig. 612a looks consists of an initial large force stroke component resulting from the shock-like touchdown of the finger mass on the inelastic sensor surface (inertia energy); it is followed by a period during which the finger tip "rests" on the force sensor and the spring-like behaviour of the compressed tissue becomes effective (second-order process). Both components are individually pronounced; the higher the velocity of the finger downward movement (i.e. stronger muscle activation), the higher is the amplitude of the first peak. It determines also the amplitude of the second component but together with tissue and mass parameters. Since there is no specific instruction on tap force and 143 speed given in normal tapping, the overall shape of the force tap response can vary. The events to detect are: (1) onset M1 indicating finger tip landing; (2) maximum amplitude of the second component M2; and (3) offset M3 when the finger tip leaves the surface. The used algorithm for event detection is similar to the approach of Hodges & Bui (1996). For detection of the force onset M1, a sliding window W1 is shifted sample by sample along the sequence (Fig. 6-12a). The mean value m of the first x samples of W1 is taken as the baseline level of unloaded force sensor and a threshold h = + k* is defined, where k is a constant factor and is the standard deviation estimated from the initial x samples. As soon as the first signal value in the second interval y of W1 exceeds this threshold h, detection of an onset is assumed. Analogously, the offset point M3 is determined, but now the signal is scanned in the reverse direction, starting at half a pacing period (i.e. ISI/2) after M1 detection with the condition that the mean value m of the second x samples of W3 approaches the mean value m of the first x samples of W1, otherwise an arbitrary event notated by a question mark for this offset is set for visual checking. Finally, the time of the maximum M2 is determined by estimating the duration of the initial landing period Lp and fitting a parabola to the signal within window W2 (length L–Lp, ending at M3) to reduce noise effects. The vertex of the parabola indicates the location M2 of the force maximum during this second contact period. The algorithm is then restarted for detection of the remaining events. After automatic event detection was performed throughout all 288 segments, results were visually controlled (Fig. 6-12b). In the case of an error, the markers can be corrected interactively. This also yields feedback on the performance of the algorithm and the algorithm can be adapted according to specific signal conditions. This option is particularly necessary for patient data, for which central motor disorders such as tremors can dramatically deteriorate the correct detection performance. Figure 6-13: Position signals (panel a) and corresponding force signals (panel b) in normal tapping. The discrete tap causes shortening of the corresponding intertap interval of the periodic task (marked) but does not affect the overall shape of the individual movement, thus a simple detection algorithm would be sufficient. 144 Figure 6-14: Position and force signals in normal tapping like in Fig. 6-13. But the shape of the periodic tap is affected by the discrete tap, which prohibits simple threshold procedures to be effective. Note that the partial tap during which the index finger does not reach the force sensor surface remains hidden, if simple ground contact recording is used. Figure 6-15: The Ramp-Step-Approach for event detection. a) Model function for a ramp-step-like change which is used for change detection. b) Estimated movement pattern (dashed line) for the position signal shown in Fig. 6-14a. The three ramp-step segments I, II, III consider the partial tap perfectly. 145 6.3.2.2 Complicated signals Data recordings from finger position, isometric finger tapping, eye blink, saccades, and foot tapping signals are more complicated, because they reflect the total motor activity of the periodic and discrete movement Fig. 6-13 shows position and force signals in normal tapping comprising four periodic right hand taps with a discrete left hand tap between. Basically, the position signals also look simply structured, even if there is some shortening of the up-phase of the corresponding periodic tap by the discrete tap. But inspecting an equivalent situation depicted in Fig. 6-14a, a more complicated signal shape can be recognized obviously due to some severe interaction between both taps. Therefore, simple analysis with threshold decision will not correctly work in this case, and a more sophisticated approach like a maximum-likelihood based algorithm (Hofer, Staude, & Wolf 2005) must be applied. Interestingly to note that such partial taps shown also occur when subjects were required to tap with both hands but with a 1:2 frequency ratio (Semjen & Summers 2002). This more sophisticated algorithm is a sequential method which uses a ramp-step model for the change to be detected. This model of the signal transient shown in Fig. 6-15a is a piece-wise linear function composed of three concatenated segments describing a single change; the parameters are the ramp duration τ and the ramp amplitude h as well as the times a, b defining the interval [a, b] and k indicating the ramp onset. The detection process starts at a predetermined time (e.g. the beginning of a trial) with a fixed window length (b-a) and optimizes the parameters τ, h, and k such that the resulting model fits the real signal in the interval [a, b] best. If this fails, the interval is shifted forward (for details, see Hofer, Staude, & Wolf 2005). The length of the interval [a, b] is crucial: it must show a minimum length to avoid local optima of the fitting due to some occasional noise structure, and it must be limited to meet the dynamics of the change sequence. The same template is used for modeling flexion h<0 and extension movements h>0. Using this algorithm, it was possible to successfully resolve all the interaction events as shown in Fig. 6-15b. 6.3.2.3 Surface myoelectric signals The method for the determination of muscle activation changes from surface myoelectric signals suggested in (Staude, Kafka, & Wolf 2000) is used for the analysis of the EMG recordings. Transitions between muscle activation patterns are detected as characteristic changes in the variance of the prewhitened surface electromyogram (SEMG) signal. Figure 6-16: Determination of muscle activation intervals from a SEMG recording. The identified muscle activation levels are indicated by letters a, b, c. The bold line depicts the square root of the estimated variance pattern. 146 The algorithm comprises three major processing steps. In an initial step, the raw SEMG is processed by an adaptive whitening filter in order to remove information from the signal which is not relevant for detection. The whitened signal is then investigated by a decision rule based upon the approximated generalized likelihood-ratio (AGLR) test which signals possible changes in the variance pattern. It produces a sequence of change times indicating transitions between stationary epochs of approximately constant variance. In order to identify different levels of muscle activation, a post processor performs an unsupervised classification by grouping the detected segments according to their variances. The variance of each newly detected segment is compared to the already identified clusters by multiple application of the F-test. If the variance of the current segment significantly differs from the already identified clusters, a new cluster is created. Otherwise, the segment is merged with the previous segment. The classification procedure thus creates a sequence of segments of constant muscle activation together with the variances of their corresponding activation levels, as illustrated in Fig. 6-16. Using this information, duration and magnitude of muscle activation patterns associated with the tapping process investigated can be simply derived. 6.4 Phase Resetting Curve (PRC) 6.4.1 General idea The PRC reflects the principle and degree of interaction between the regular periodic taps and the discrete taps. It shows the timing of the periodic tapping as a function of the phase within the periodic tap cycle at which the discrete tap is initiated. If the periodic tapping is almost independent of the discrete perturbation events, the PRC will show horizontal dot lines with the dots uniformly distributed over the x-axis. Any deviation from such a pattern indicates some interaction between the two motor tasks. Figure 6-17: Construction of a phase resetting curve (PRC). The left diagram shows a part of a PRC magnified, while the right diagram elucidates the selection of the data points. The shaded area on the right will be then included to the PRC (indicated by the arrow) on the left. 6.4.2 Phase Resetting Curve construction As shown in Fig. 6-10 and 6-11 as well as in Fig. 6-17, each segment contains one discrete (single) tap (saccade) in response to a go signal (right bar), in ST one single eye blink. Within segments, the following events were defined according to Yoshino et al. (2002): 147 The non-dominant hand (foot) tap (saccade) following the go signal or the eye blink is considered to be the discrete response (perturbation event) (internally) triggered by the go signal (eye blink control). The reaction time (RT) in dual-task condition is defined as the interval between go signal onset and the corresponding response tap (saccade). The right hand tap of the periodic tapping occurring just before the single discrete nondominant hand tap or the blink represents the reference tap (reference event) which defines the time origin (i.e. t = 0) within each segment. The onset time tperturbation of the perturbation event (discrete tap, saccade, blink) relatively to the reference tap is normalized by the interstimulus interval (ISI) of pacing signal and introduced as perturbation phase Φ = tperturbation/ ISI. Φ describes the locus of the discrete movement (eye blink) with respect to the cycle of the periodic tapping, with Φ = 0 (as well as Φ = 1) indicating simultaneous (in-phase) execution of taps (eye blink) and Φ = 0.5 indicating that the discrete movement (eye blink) is executed in the midst of the periodic cycle (anti-phase execution). But it should be noted that the definition of Φ does not consider either the variation of the intertap intervals nor some gradual frequency deviation in periodic tapping, since it refers to the constant value of ISI = 600 ms (550ms in eye blink experiments). Thus, Φ values > 1 will be obtained in cases when tperturbation (as defined in Fig. 6-10, 6.11) will exceed ISI, which implicitly requires the actual periodic tap interval to exceed ISI, too. These cases are very rare since subjects tend to fasten the tapping in the continuation phase, so those segments were simply discarded (< 1 %). But nevertheless, this fixed normalization reference value ISI provides some objective comparison between subjects. For the single-discrete-task data, SRT was determined as the interval between go signal onset and the corresponding discrete response tap of the non-dominant hand (foot, saccade). To present the phase changes of the periodic rhythm as a function of the periodic phase that the discrete tap (saccade) or eye blink is given, the so-called phase resetting curves (PRC) in the form used by Yoshino et al. (2002) was applied; these curves are derived from the force or position sensor signals. The PRC shows the intertap intervals (in s) of all segments on the ordinate (dimension: time t) as a function of the phase Φ plotted on abscissa. The construction of a PRC is demonstrated by Fig. 617: (1) Each symbol (dot, circle, cross, triangle) in the diagram represents one of the periodic (right index finger) taps. (2) Each symbol belongs to a group of six subsequent periodic taps aligned vertically like a column as shown in Fig. 6-17 by the shaded area on the right; for each discrete tap (saccade) or eye blink, such a group of six periodic taps is picked out from the sequence. (3) To determine these six symbols of one group, the discrete tap (saccade) or eye blink is located in the segment; the corresponding reference tap is determined as the last periodic tap before the discrete tap (saccade) or eye blink (see Fig. 6-10, 6-11). The reference tap together with the two periodic taps preceding it and the three periodic taps following it constitute the tap group for one segment. (4) This group of six symbols will be added to the PRC such that its reference tap is vertically aligned to t = 0 (ordinate); the horizontal position of the tap group on the abscissa is determined by the phase of the perturbation Φ, given by the onset of the single discrete tap (saccade) or eye blink within this segment. This procedure is performed for all 288 segments of a session in DT condition and for all number of eye blinks, resulting in a superposition of all the 288 (number of eye blinks) sixsymbol groups in a single graph. (5) The inclined dashed line above the abscissa (t = 0) represents the occurrences of the single discrete taps (saccade) or eye blinks (perturbation events). 148 The spreading of the PRC-lines in the vertical direction reproduces the tap-to-tap variation of the observed inter-tap interval - ITIs,. Since all reference tap onsets are aligned to time t=0 within the frame of each single response, the first PRC-lines above and below the abscissa represent single ITIs (and the variability of 1 interval), whereas the second PRC-lines above and below reflect the sums of 2 ITIs and, therefore, their larger scatter reflects the double variation span with respect to t = 0. 6.5 Periodic-discrete process Interaction Categories A main objective of the work was directed to possible interaction between the two tasks which are concurrently executed within the DT-framework. Interaction is visualized by the PRC configurations on the basis on the raw data, but can be classified to specific types which were derived experimentally. Classification can be simply performed on the basis of the event times, but also by analyzing the movement parameters of the taps. 6.5.1 Classification based on discrete events The strong interaction occurs in DT OM-DT experiments either as directed effect from one process to the other, or both processes interact mutually. Both forms of interaction between the periodic and the discrete tapping process were specified according to the timing structure of the taps given by: the actual interval of the periodic tapping, during which the discrete tap occurs, the reaction time (RT) of the discrete tap, and the temporal relation between the periodic tap and the discrete tap. 149 Figure 6-18: Phase resetting curve construction and basic interpretation. Construction: (A) ideally, if tapping is not affected by the discrete taps and if discrete taps occur completely independent of the tapping process, the taps before and after the reference tap would form horizontal lines. (B) If the reactive event fully resets the tapping oscillator, the next periodic tap would occur with a delay of one full ISI after the discrete tap, thus these taps build the first inclined post-reference-tap dot-line (parallel to the inclined dashed line). (C) For small values of Φ, the nearly horizontal dot ‘line’ composed of the right hand taps next after the perturbation event may indicate a MTI behaviour; but beyond this range of Φ, the next periodic tap for t > 0 occurs simultaneously with the discrete left hand tap, which can be interpreted as a skip to the final state of the actual periodic tapping process or hastening of the cycling. (D) Any systematic deviation of a uniform (horizontal) distribution of the dots over phase indicates the reverse effect of (B): the periodic tapping process controls the discrete tap timing. Panel D shows an example where the discrete tap onsets are synchronized with the undisturbed periodic tapping; therefore, dot locations are restricted to particular phases (here: 0 < Ф < 0:2). The interaction patterns were structured by visual inspection, which results in an abstract definition of typical interaction categories. In a second step, formal criteria for the parameters which allow assignment of the actual interaction pattern to a specific category were determined by cluster analysis. These criteria are specified in the Appendix. (Note that this pattern recognition approach is purely formal and does not include any physiological aspect.) The defined interaction categories are (Fig. 6-18): 1) Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI) Segments show non visible or only very weak signs of interactions in the evaluation of force signals. 2) Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR) Segments show a distinct prolongation of the actual intertap interval of the periodic tapping This type is further sub-structured into categories: 150 - Tap Delaying (TD): The process of the periodic movement is shortly paused by the discrete tap, - Tap Cancelling (TC): The execution of the ongoing periodic movement is stopped on the fly, simultaneously with the execution of the discrete tap. In both cases, the reaction time of the discrete tap is normal. 3) Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH) Premature execution of the next scheduled periodic tap, i.e. the actual intertap interval enveloping the discrete tap is shorter than usual. 4) Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE) Entrainment of the discrete tap by the periodic tap, i.e. the execution of the discrete tap is delayed to be in-phase with the next periodic tap. In addition to the directed interaction categories PTR, PTH (dominant effects on periodic task) and DTE (dominant effect on discrete task), often a combination of cases PTR and PTH, respectively, with DTE reflecting a mutual interaction may occur. This type is called 5) Mixed Tapping Strategies (MTS) Segments show elements of two categories definitively. Detailed quantitative criteria used for assignment of the segments to these categories are given in the Appendix. Certainly, this formal separation of the classes is strict, but not really always unique like “black and white” but “grey” with regard to subjective interpretation by the experimenters; nevertheless, it allows replication of results and some basic statistical evaluation. It should be considered that the MTI set will also contain a bias of those segments where the interaction leads to timing patterns looking just normal and unaffected. 6.5.2 Classification based on continuous trajectories of fingers This approach is based on the a priori knowledge that rhythmic motor actions executed by different limbs tend to be synchronized, either in-phase or anti-phase. Along this line, 7 different exclusive conditions were defined as slope interaction schemes: down-slope synchronization (DS) up-slope synchronization (US) 2-sided slope synchronization (TS) 2-sided anti-phase slope synchronization (AP2S) 1-sided anti-phase slope synchronization (AP1S) slope asynchrony in overlapping taps (SA) tap asynchrony (TA) Criteria for this nomenclature will be presented below and are mutually exclusive; but note that they are heuristically motivated, because the physiological background for these synchronizationconditions is not yet clear and demands for further basic investigations. But obviously, even if the central go commands for both motor actions (discrete tap and periodic tap execution) are simultaneously issued to the peripheral plant, variations in the neural processes and influences of non-stationary biomechanical components will lead to different delays and slope profiles. Therefore, criteria used in this study will mainly refer to a minimum overlapping period of both actions (i.e. overlap in time of the periodic right hand tap and the discrete non-dominant hand tap must last a specified duration at least); because the overlapping periods of the individual as well as of the pooled data clearly show a non-uniform distribution, i. e an attraction evidently exists. The minimum duration of this overlapping period used as a criterion for synchrony is determined by the 50% of the minimum slope period of the two finger trajectories. This criterion is applied for all patterns. The start and the end of the overlapping is determined by the last begin and the first end of the slope 151 period out of two slope periods. For abbreviation the left one-sided coordination or the right onesided coordination (as the number) is used when the interaction is determined on the reference tap (1) or on the next tap after reference tap (2) respectively. 1) Down-slope synchronization (DS) Down-slope synchronization will be detected if the overlapping duration of both the down-slope period of discrete tap and that of the periodic tap exceeds the criterion. Figure 6-19: Down-slope synchronization (DS) of the periodic tap (blue) and the discrete tap (green) as a response to the Go command. a) DS case in which the slope of the periodic tap precedes that of the single tap (DS1), b) DS case with reversed slope sequence (DS2) c) Zoomed DS period (type DS1). The horizontal continuous line segments (red) indicate the estimation of the actual tap duration, the dotted oblique lines (red) shows the estimated linear regression lines of the movement trajectories as achieved by the ramp-step-model. DS is rather rarely. A sample of DS is shown by Fig. 6-19a, b. Clearly, both down-slopes (upward direction in the graph) overlap significantly and elucidated by zoomed interval of concern (6.19c). This also applies for the subsequent up-slopes, which will be discussed later. As already mentioned in the protocol description above, the temporal relation between the down-slope of the discrete tap response and the down-slope of the actual periodic tap determines the definition of the so-called “reference tap” which is the last periodic tap before the discrete tap response. Since the criterion for slope synchronization requires only some but not full overlapping of both slopes, the sequence of both down-slope onsets is irrelevant for the DS-classification. Nevertheless, the sequence is coded by a trailing “1” in case the synchronized periodic tap precedes the discrete tap and a “2” in case the synchronized periodic tap succeeds to the discrete tap (i.e. DS1, DS2). The same rules are applied for the other synchronization conditions. 2) Up-slope synchronization (US) In principle, the decision process is symmetric to the DS condition. 152 Figure 6-20: Up-slope synchronization (US). For details, see Fig. 6-19. a) US1, b) US2, c) Zoomed up-slope synchronization period (type US1) Fig. 6-20a, c depicts an example for US; the up-slope (downward direction in the graph) of both the discrete tap (green line) and the concurrent periodic tap (blue curve) are vastly overlapping (6.20b). 3) 2-sided slope synchronization (TS) This total synchronization of the discrete tap with the concurrent periodic tap requires both the DS criterion and the US criterion to be met. Figure 6-21: a) Down-slopes and up-slopes of both the periodic tapping (thin curve) and the discrete tap (thick curve) are vastly overlapping. b) Zoomed synchronization period elucidating the overlap period. 153 It should be noted that 2-sided slope synchronization always requires that the corresponding tap durations are of the same size (Fig. 6-21). 4) 2-sided anti-phase slope synchronization (AP2S) The slope synchronization can happen cross-like, i.e. in anti-phase-relationship. Then, the overlapping of the up-slope period of the discrete tap with the down-slope period of the concurrent periodic tap (or vice versa) fulfilled the criterion. Figure 6-22: 2-sided anti-phase synchronization. a) The downward onset of the discrete tap and the upward onset of the reference tap started at the same time (or vice versa) on the next half period. b) Zoomed synchronization period elucidating an overlap period. The term AP2S1 is used when the up-slope of the periodic tapping synchronize with the downslope of the discrete tap, whereas AP2S2 indicates the reverse sequence (Fig. 6-22). 5) 1-sided anti-phase slope synchronization (AP1S1|AP1S2) The cross-like slope synchronization can be one-sided. Figure 6-23: 1-sided anti-phase synchronization: a) AP1S1 The upward slope of the reference tap is coordinated with the downward slope of the discrete tap while the discrete upward slope was performed during the pause of the reference tap, b) AP1S2: same as in a, but with reversed signs of the slopes, c) Zoomed synchronization period elucidates the overlap period. The other movement slope of the discrete response takes place during the pause of the periodic finger (Fig. 6-23). 154 6) Slope asynchrony in overlapping taps (SA) In this remaining case, some overlapping of the taps exists but the criteria for slope synchronization was not fulfilled. Figure 6-24: slope asynchrony (SA) in overlapping taps. The number again indicates the sequence of slopes as in DS and US: a) SA1, b) SA2. Fig. 6-24 shows the overlapping of both tap duration but not the overlapping of the slope duration. 7) Tap asynchrony (TA) The total asynchrony is another case of tap asynchrony, referring to the condition that the discrete tap occurs during the pause of the periodic tapping in the top of the up-position or between the start of the upslope and the end of the downslope of the periodic tap. Figure 6-25: Tap asynchrony. The discrete tap occurs in the resting phase of the periodic tapping. Fig. 6-25a shows that the discrete tap is embedded during the pause of the periodic tap. Sometimes, a small mirror tap in the right hand can be observed (Fig. 6-25b). 155 6.6 Statistical data analysis One(multiple)-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were applied to determine whether one (several) given factor(s) (grouping variables), such as experimental conditions (contact-free, isometric, strong tap, …) , coordination (unimanual, dual), task (single, dual), etc. have a significant effect on the mean of certain behavior across any of the groups under study. The ANOVA returns F statistic, p-value, the sums of squares (SS), degrees of freedom (df), and mean squares (SS/df). Any pvalue near zero casts doubt on the associated null hypothesis, which assumes that the effects of factors are absent. However, if more than two groups are to be analyzed the one (multiple)-way ANOVA does not specifically indicate which pair of groups exhibits statistical differences. To determine which specific pair/pairs are differentially expressed Post Hoc tests multi-compare was applied in this specific situation. To test for uniform distribution, the 2I-test (Sachs, 1984) or post-hoc mean comparisons using Bonferroni-corrected t-tests (Sachs, 1984) were used. For instance, the eye blink phases were divided among k equidistant classes, the 2I-test variable was calculated and compared to the percentage point for significance level alpha from chi square distribution; the nullhypothesis of the test is that eye blink phases of k classes would occur with equal frequency in the case that the test variable is smaller than the percentage point. 156 7 Results The obtained experimental data are basically presented using the same structure as for the description of the experiments in Chapter 6. First, Chapter 7.1 reports the results of ST condition. Specifically, the results of timing control in single periodic tapping are presented in 7.1.1. The mean intertap intervals (ITI) and their standard deviation are reported for in all experiment conditions. The correlations from lag zero to lag 5 of ITIs are presented. ANOVA and the ‘post ad hoc test’ were applied to compare the performance with factor “coordination” (unimanual (UM) vs. bimanual (BM), mental tapping vs. normal tapping) and effectors (left hand, right hand, voice). The entrainments of spontaneous eye blinks by periodic tapping under different experimental condition are described in Chapter 7.1.2. Subsequently, Chapter 7.2, 7.3, and 7.4 report the results of DT condition in BM, OM, and handfoot combinations. Chapter 7.2.1 presents the basic interaction patterns illustrated by PRCs and the time courses. Chapter 7.2.2 investigates reaction times, as well as distributions of discrete tap occurrences over phase together with PRCs. Chapter 7.2.3 analyzes the continuous time course of the tapping process by inspecting the shapes of the finger trajectories reflecting changes of the timing control during interaction. Chapter 7.2.4 reports the effects of the experimental condition on the tapping behaviors. Also, the BM-DT condition is extended to combinations of hand-foot tapping which are reported in Chapter 7.3. The final Chapter 7.4 addresses the experiments with saccadic eye movements as discrete responses; results of oculomanual (OM)-DT experiments are compared to BM-DT experiments. All the results of the control experiments as references are reported in the corresponding chapters. As stated in Chapter 6.2.2, the reference values obtained in ST (and also notask) control experiments serve for the comparison between them and the corresponding values in DT-task (or ST, respectively) experiments. The ST control experiments for DT consist either of stimulus-induced discrete taps (hand, foot), and saccades, respectively, in the absence of any periodic movement under normal and/or isometric conditions, or of periodic tapping in the absence of any discrete task. 7.1 Single-Task (ST) condition 7.1.1 Periodic tapping ST As mentioned in Chapter 6, the periodic tapping had to meet specific inter-tap-interval (ITI) criteria for statistical requirements (Mean intertap interval of the actual trial ± four standard deviations are the boundaries for the mean of the intervals in continuation phase. Because of the stationary assumption of the Wing-Kristofferson-model (1973 a, b) about the expected interval length, linear trend was defined by significant (p<0.05) correlation between the length of an interval and its position (Drewing & Aschersleben 2003)). Therefore, about 30% of four subjects’ trials had to be repeated due to some ITI outlier. The repetitions were only performed to that extend that subjects could endure. The result of every trial was considered as one observation of an experimental condition for calculation of mean, standard deviation and ANOVA analysis. 157 Figure 7-1: One-way ANOVA and multiple comparison procedure were applied to every subject’s (P1, P2, P3, and P4) tapping in periodic ST experiment. Each group mean is graphically illustrated by a symbol and an interval around the symbol. The intervals of the means are disjoint or overlap indicating significantly difference or not significantly difference respectively. The test compared the variability between the different factors. A) left finger with factor “coordination” (UM, BM): all experiments (47-54 trials) reveal a significant effect of BM advantage in isometric tapping, but not all in normal and contact-free tapping. Three of four subjects show BM disadvantage in normal tapping and two of four in contact-free tapping. B) Right finger with factor “coordination” (UM, BM): the results reveal BM advantage in isometric tapping. Three of four subjects show BM disadvantage in contact-free tapping and two of four in normal tapping. C) UM tapping and voice tapping experiment, (73-81 trials) with factor “effector” (left hand, right hand, and voice): all subjects show the same pattern that voice tapping has largest variance and - except for P4 - right hand is better than left hand. Notation for the factor: LE: left hand, RI: right hand, VO: voice. D) (21-27 trials) with factor “experiment” (normal tapping, normal + mental tapping), one subject performed the 4-group and two subjects the 8-group condition with factor “experiment”: all subjects show the same pattern that normal tapping together with mental tapping improve the tapping performance mostly significantly. Notation for factor: N: normal, N+M: normal+mental. 158 Table 7.1: Mean (standard deviation) of ITIs of left hand, right hand and voice in periodic ST experiments, all values given in ms. Columns 3 and 4 show the left and right hand in UM and columns 5 and 6 in BM condition, respectively. The mean (standard deviation) of each trial was considered as an observation from an experimental condition (contact-free ("tapping in the air"), isometric, normal). Generally there is no significant difference. Note that the used ISI was 400 ms. Subjects Manual P1 Normal Isometric contact-free Normal Isometric contact-free Normal Isometric contact-free Normal Isometric contact-free P2 P3 P4 Unimanual (UM) left hand right hand 387(12) 393(16) 389(23) 381(15) 382(8) 373(9) 380(10) 377(9) 384(12) 374(12) 375(10) 378(9) 401(6) 389(7) 390(12) 374(8) 386(5) 383(6) 400(12) 404(10) 412(18) 429(18) 386(11) 386(12) Bimanual (BM) left hand right hand 384(8) 384(8) 385(10) 385(10) 375(10) 376(10) 379(12) 379(12) 382(11) 382(11) 376(8) 375(8) 408(8) 409(8) 386(11) 386(11) 387(6) 388(6) 404(11) 403(9) 408(5) 408(6) 385(8) 384(8) Voice 408(13) 384(10) 393(6) 378(5) Table 7.1 shows mean and standard deviation (in parentheses) of ITI of the left hand, right hand and voice. Although the antagonist muscle was not activated in isometric tapping, a strong EMG signal of extensor muscle was found in this condition, too. Generally, no significant difference between the different effectors was revealed. One-way ANOVA (47-54 trials, as stated that the repetitions were only performed to such an extent that subjects could endure, the guideline number was 54 trials (three phases*nine trials per phase) of two conditions (UM vs. BM) with factors “coordination” (UM, BM) and the post hoc test were applied to every subject’s tapping sequence. In Fig. 7-1A and 7-1B, the tests show the comparison of variability of the left and right hand between UM and BM condition. All subjects show a BM advantage in isometric tapping experiments, but not in all of the other two experimental conditions. Two of four subjects show rather a disadvantage on the left hand in contact-free, three of four in normal tapping. The right hand data showed the similar results. The same One-way ANOVA (73-81 trials) with factor effector (left hand, right hand, and voice) and the post hoc test were applied to every subject’s UM tapping and voice tapping experiment (Fig. 7-1C). Almost all subjects show the same pattern that voice tapping has largest variance and, except for P4, right hand is better than left hand. In normal tapping combined with mental tapping, the 8-group-condition caused a clear positive linear trend of the duration of ITI. Participants were alerted by the experimenter to raise their slower tapping rate. Hence the criterion for repeating an experiment due to linear trend was not applied in the comparison between normal tapping with and without mental tapping because one participant’ BM tapping data still showed a significant linear trend in 9 of 34 trials in normal tapping (without mental tapping) against 5 of 35 in normal tapping with mental tapping (counting) on the left finger and 12 of 34 against 7 of 35 on the right finger, only. The results show that normal tapping together with mental tapping all improved the performance (Fig. 7-1D). 159 Figure 7-2: The autocovariance of the ITI in manual tapping A) left hand in UM tapping of all subjects. For lag 1, only P1 shows negative correlation (i.e. direct compensation of a too short ITI by a subsequent longer ITI, and v.v.) in three experimental conditions, P2 only in normal tapping. Three other subjects show rather some higher trend to positive correlation (i.e. smooth shifts of the tapping frequency) in isometric tapping. B) right hand in UM tapping of all subjects. For lag 1, P1 shows negative correlation in normal tapping and isometric tapping, P4 in normal tapping but with reduced degree compared to the left hand. Again the three other subjects show a higher trend to positive correlation in isometric tapping than in left hand. C) left hand in BM tapping of all subjects. For lag 1, P1 again shows negative correlation in all experiment condition, P2 and P3 in normal tapping and contact-free tapping and P4 in normal tapping. Again, P2, P3, P4 show a higher trend to positive correlation in isometric tapping than in left hand. Generally, the left hand show stronger negative correlation compared to UM condition. D) right hand in BM tapping of all subjects. For lag 1 P1 shows negative correlation in normal and isometric tapping, P2 and P3 in normal tapping and contact-free tapping and P4 in normal and isometric tapping. P2, P3 show a higher trend to positive correlation in isometric tapping than in left hand. Generally, the right hand also show stronger negative correlation compared to UM condition. As a further aspect, the binding between subsequent ITI was investigated by correlation analysis of the ITI series. A high correlation would indicate an overall integral control of the tapping frequency generator. Fig. 7-2A, B, C, and D show the ITI correlation functions from lag 1 (direct online control) 160 to lag 5 (more global control) for the left hand and right hand in UM and right hand in BM condition, respectively. Figure 7-3: The autocovariance of the ITI in manual, voice and mental tapping. A) The voice intervals pooled for all show a clear negative correlation at lag 1. B) The intervals of 3 subject’s data in normal tapping combined with mental tapping. For lag 1, minority of graphs shows a negative correlation at lag 1, in general there is no specific trend. C) One-way ANOVA (21-27 trials) with factor experimental condition (normal tapping, normal + mental tapping), and the post hoc test were applied to three subjects’ data. One subject performed 4-group and two 8-group.The tests compared the mean of correlation lag 1 of successive ITIs between two experiments. Almost all data show some bias to positive correlation in normal tapping with mental tapping compared to normal tapping alone. Notation for factor: N: normal tapping, N+M: normal+mental tapping. 161 In summary, correlation analysis of ITI (Fig. 7-2A, B, C, and D) shows stronger negative correlation for both hands in BM condition than in UM condition at lag 1. Voice tapping clearly revealed a negative correlation (Fig. 7-2A). Three subjects showed higher trend to positive correlation in isometric tapping. Generally, mental tapping biases the correlation lag 1 to positive direction (Fig. 7.3B compared to 7-2A, B, C, and D). One-way ANOVA (about 27 trials) with factor experiment (normal tapping, normal + mental tapping) and the post hoc test compared the mean of correlation lag 1 of successive ITIs between two experiments and revealed a bias to positive correlation in normal tapping with mental tapping (Fig. 7-3C). Table 7.2 (A, B): Mean contact forces (Fmean in N) and mean contact durations (D mean in ms) of the right hand finger tap in UM (A) and BM (B) tapping conditions. Values are shown for all participants (P1 … P7) executing normal tapping (Exp1), strong tapping (Exp2), and impulse-like tapping (Exp3). A Unimanual Exp. 1 Exp. 2 F mean D mean F mean D mean F mean D mean P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 1.50 1.85 1.18 1.95 1.05 1.23 0.57 115 140 177 119 217 183 116 6.10 4.16 7.59 2.55 2.43 7.83 5.48 174 154 237 131 231 239 154 0.75 1.57 0.80 1.24 0.98 0.82 1.24 46 65 78 77 29 48 61 B Bimanual Exp. 1 Exp. 2 F mean D mean F mean D mean F mean D mean 1.25 1.60 0.72 2.06 0.78 0.69 0.63 110 117 160 111 127 155 121 7.30 3.60 5.73 2.68 4.40 5.84 6.34 205 137 241 132 227 244 154 0.60 1.52 0.69 1.81 1.01 0.69 0.79 34 62 77 79 23 42 57 P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 Exp. 3 Exp. 3 7.1.2 Periodic tapping and spontaneous eye blinks The analysis of ST data investigates the question, whether – according to the idea of a possible common central motor timing - spontaneous eye blinks and periodic tapping reveal a common root; this should be more obvious in those tapping tasks which require more attention than normal tapping (Exp.1 in Table 7.2) due to specific instructions like "strong"-tapping (Exp.2) and impulse-like tapping (Exp.3). Table 7.2 shows the mean contact forces (in N) and the mean contact durations (in ms) generated by the participants in these three tapping experiments. Capability of all subjects in performing adequately the required tasks is apparent for both UM and BM tapping experiments: the mean contact forces were largest in strong tapping whereas mean contact durations were shortest in 162 impulse-like tapping. The longest contact durations in strong tapping show that in general, the required larger peak forces are combined with longer contact durations. 163 Figure 7-4: PRCs for the BM tapping in ST experiments when the eye blink events are taken as discrete response (disturbance of the periodic tapping process). The row headers indicate participant number and Experiments 1 (normal tapping), 2 (strong tapping) and 3 (impulse-like tapping), respectively. The upper three dot-lines of all PRCs stay horizontal indicating that the tapping process is not significantly disturbed by the preceding spontaneous blinks. However, in particular in impulse-like tapping, the dot symbols are not uniformly distributed over phase, but show a higher density at small phase values. Presence of such preferred phases indicates that the onsets of the eye blinks are entrained to some extent by the periodic taps. Scaling of abscissa is normalized phase, scaling of ordinate is s. 164 Figure 7-5: Phase histograms of eye blinks in BM tapping. Data of tapping experiments and of the reference experiment are depicted for all participants. All histograms of tapping experiments show a clear shaping of the distributions compared to the approximate uniform distribution of the corresponding reference experiment. Note that the scatter of the peaks of the phase histograms indicates individual internal delays. 165 If the eye blink events are formally regarded as discrete motor responses (i.e. disturbance of the periodic tapping process), PRCs can be constructed to reveal possible interactions between the blink timing process and the tapping process. The PRCs of all participants for the three BM tapping ST experiments are displayed in Fig. 7-4; all diagrams show that the six dot-lines are basically horizontal indicating that the tapping process is not significantly disturbed by the actual spontaneous blink. However, there is a trend that the dot symbols show a higher density at small phase values. These preferred phases reveal the entrainment of the onsets of the eye blinks by the periodic taps. The PRCs of the UM tapping experiments showed a similar horizontal orientation but less pronounced concentration of dots at the preferred small phases. Differences between UM and BM conditions are elaborated in more detail below. The phases of the eye blink events (reflecting the blink timing control) can be depicted in histograms; independence of the blink timing and the tapping process would predict a uniform distribution. In Fig. 7-5 (column (2), (3), (4)), these phase histograms for the ST experiments are shown and they reveal preferred phases of the blink events to occur during the tapping cycle: all histograms of the BM ST-experiments show peaky distributions (although with different degrees of modulation), which clearly differ from a uniform distribution as expected for independent processes. The different locations of the peaks in Fig. 7-5 might indicate some individual internal delays (typical for each participant). Also, the strength of interference varied between individuals: P1, P4, and P5 showed a very strong effect of tapping upon spontaneous blinking in all experiments. P7 had a less pronounced shaping of phase distributions for standard (uninstructed) tapping, possibly due to very light surface contact forces (Table 7.2). 166 Figure 7-6: PRC (A) and phase histogram (normalized by the total number of blinks) (B) of the reference experiment of participant P1. The phase distribution (B) represents a possible template of a uniform distribution which is scattered due to the limited observation period of the scattered ITI. Also, the horizontal dot-lines in the PRC (A) are consistent with the expected behaviour in the case of independent processes. Note that the scattered ITI causes the spreading of dots and decreasing density at phases Ф~1, too (see text for details). The first column in Fig. 7-5 shows phase histograms taken as reference, even if no real reference experiment can be performed since blinking is an unconscious process. On the other hand, no ideal statistics (which leads to the assumption of a uniform distribution of phases) can be expected due to the limited observation period and the limited stationarity of the biological processes. Therefore an estimation of a "real" reference histogram was achieved by an artificial “experiment”. For this purpose, the phase analysis was performed with data from different experimental sessions thus independence was given by principle: the blink event time series was combined with the tapping time series of another experiment of the same subject (participant P1). The PRC in Fig. 7-6A proves that the eye blink events do not entrain the periodic tapping in this case (also expected by principle). The resulting phase histogram (normalized by the total number of blinks) shown in Fig. 7-6B is taken as the reference for P1; it shows a basically uniform structure even if some structure due to the variability of the measured ITIs can be recognized. Thus, both diagrams are consistent with the theoretical expectation for independent processes. 167 Figure 7-7: Phase histograms of eye blinks during unimanual (A) and bimanual (B) tapping. Individual data of participant P1 are depicted; the column headers (normal, strong, and impulse-like) indicate Experiments 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Ordinate scaling shows the frequency of occurrence in all panels. All histograms show a clear shaping of the distributions, being more prominent in bimanual tapping. Table 7.3: 2I-test assertion of the null hypothesis of "uniform phase distribution", shown for all participants and all tapping experiments. Larger values of the 2I-test statistic indicate stronger deviations from the uniform distribution whereas the shaded values mark non-significant differences (p=0.05). The test indicated significant deviations for the majority of the tapping experiments. Consistently, the expected uniform distribution was confirmed for the reference experiments in all subjects. P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 Ref. 21.54 12.87 18.06 17.83 10.85 15.17 14.27 Unimanual Exp. 1 Exp. 2 36.55 29.64 29.45 38.16 20.09 32.15 40.59 38.34 96.78 115.64 14.51 29.16 17.70 39.01 Bimanual Exp. 3 Exp. 1 Exp. 2 63.20 69.34 95.05 135.15 98.97 85.77 168.53 44.19 32.66 50.00 84.20 126.83 143.67 218.70 329.14 32.71 35.28 34.29 47.34 15.73 54.22 Exp. 3 101.78 168.51 172.75 94.49 328.74 81.10 44.55 Generally, the phase preference of blink events was more prominent in the BM tapping experiments than in the UM tapping experiments. Fig. 7-7 demonstrates this difference between UM (Fig. 7-7A) and BM (Fig. 7-7B) tapping for participant P1what is also typical for the other participants P2-P7. The 2I-test (Sachs, 1984) was applied for a quantitative analysis of deviations from the uniform distribution of the phase. The test was performed separately for each experiment and each individual. The test asserts the null hypothesis that the distributions are uniform; the decision is taken on the significant level p<0.05. In order to prohibit false positive decisions due to scattered ITI, the test was confined to phase values 0<Ф<0.8. The 2I-test values for all subjects are presented in Table 7.3. Larger values indicate larger deviations from the uniform distribution. Shaded cells mark 2I-test values which did not reach significance while non-shaded cells indicate significant deviation from a uniform distribution. The test expectedly proves the uniform distribution for the reference data (blinking only) consistently showing small 2I-test values below the significance limit. By contrast, for 38 of 42 phase distributions obtained in the ST experiments with tapping, the 2I-test indicated significant deviations from the uniform distribution. While the four distributions for which the null hypothesis was not rejected were all obtained in the standard (uninstructed) tapping situation 168 (Experiment 1), both the strong tapping (Experiment 2) and impulse-like tapping (Experiment 3) data exhibited the non-uniform distributions in all participants. Moreover, with the exception of subject P7, who generally showed a less pronounced shaping of phase distributions in uninstructed tapping (cf., Fig. 7-5), 2I-test values were consistently larger in BM tapping compared to UM tapping experiments. Thus, the concurrently active motor process stronger entrained spontaneous blinking in case of the tapping task either (i) intensified by instruction, or (ii) performed in the BM condition. Finally, it should be noted that the modification of Steven's (1886) original tapping paradigm by introducing the re-synchronizing pace triads did not affect the basic result, namely that the tapping entrains blinking; this was checked in pilot experiments without resynchronization. However, the additional pacing signals significantly reduced the ITI variation and prevented frequency drifts in the self-paced tapping such as drifts in the continuation phase would hamper the clarity of results since phase is dependent on ITIs. Further, the mean values the three ITIs before, including and after the blink were 522.37 ms (SD ± 34.00), 525.59 ms (SD ± 34.50) and 521.36 ms (SD ± 35.50), respectively; thus, it is a clear indication that the blink events (occurring after the reference event) do not influence the timing of the tapping. 7.2 Dual-Task (DT) condition The first part 7.2.1 of this chapter will report on the overall tapping behaviour which is comprised by the classical so-called ‘phase resetting curves’ (PRC) as originally used by Yoshino et al. (2002); interaction between the periodic tapping process and the discrete response are described for 7 representative subjects performing normal and isometric tapping. The PRC information is supplemented with the statistical analysis of these subjects’ individual tapping behaviour by Section 7.2.2; also, its effect on discrete-tap timing characteristics which took part in normal and isometric conditions is addressed. Chapter 7.2.3 focuses on interactions between the periodic and discrete tapping processes as observed during the transition period, when the finger moves from the resting position and hit position (contacting surface) (and v.v.) in BM-DT conditions. The analysis used the ANOVA and the complying multiple-comparison test with significance level of p<0.001. The effects of experimental condition (contact-free, isometric, strong tapping) on this overall tapping behaviour were reported in Chapter 7.2.4. 169 Figure 7-8: Typical phase resetting curves for the different tapping behaviours observed in the BM-DT experiments. a) Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI): the straight horizontal formation of the dots in the PRC indicates almost no perturbation of the periodic tapping of the right hand due to the concurrent discrete tap of the left hand. b) Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR): the inclined formation of the dots (somehow in parallel with the dashed line) after the perturbation indicates a resetting of the periodic tapping process to the starting state. c) Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH): similar to the PRC for PTR, the dot ‘lines’ after the perturbation due to the discrete tap show an inclined orientation, but there is an additional branch starting around phase Φ = 0.26. Dots on this line represent periodic right hand taps simultaneously executed with the discrete left hand tap. d) Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE) combined with Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH): The density of dots is essentially raised in the phase range from 0 to 0.4, which indicates a preference for concurrent execution of the discrete left hand tap with the periodic right hand tap. Since intervals of the periodic tapping (ITI) are almost stable within this phase range, the execution of the discrete tap is delayed until the next periodic right hand tap in DTE. For larger phase values, additionally a PTH like in Fig.7-8c can be observed. The inclined line indicates the times of the discrete taps. Symbols: black dots - periodic taps preceding the discrete tap; circles – first periodic tap after discrete tap; crosses – second periodic tap after discrete tap; triangles - third periodic tap after discrete tap. 7.2.1 Basic interaction patterns and their PRCs Because the PRC ordinate represents ongoing time and all reference tap onsets are aligned to time t=0 within the frame of each single response (see Fig. 6-11), the first PRC-lines above and below the abscissa (e.g. Fig. 7-8) represent single ITIs (and their variability), whereas the second PRC-lines above and below reflect the sums of two ITIs and, therefore, their larger scatter reflects the double variation span with respect to t=0. Data analysis of the BM-DT experiments by PRCs results in four typical interaction patterns: (i) Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI), (ii) Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR), (iii) Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH), and (iiii) Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE). They are demonstrated by the respective PRCs in Fig. 7-8. Even this nomenclature is a new result of this work (prepublication of this work by Wachter et al. 2008), there is a correspondence to 170 Figure 7-9: Tap Delay (TD) in normal tapping (a) and in isometric tapping (b). The execution of the periodic right hand tap is paused during the execution of the discrete left hand tap. This behaviour for the discrete tap occurrences (the last two taps) is highlighted, whereas the first discrete tap seems not to cause any change in the periodic tapping. This figure demonstrates pausing taking place in the up-position of the periodic tapping, but it happens in down position as well. Ordinate scales are arbitrary. Abscissa: time in seconds. Figure 7-10: Tap Cancelling (TC) in normal tapping (a) and in isometric tapping (b). When the execution of the discrete tap starts, the continuation of the periodic tapping is stopped and the finger moves back to the upposition. the previous state of the art established with the terms “Type 0 Phase Resetting” and “Type 1 Phase Resetting” by Yoshino et al. (2002). MTI can be interpreted as Type 1 Phase Resetting, while PTR and 171 PTH correspond to Type 0 Phase Resetting. DTE which describes the directed effect of the periodic tapping on the discrete tap is a novel aspect forwarded by this study, because Yoshino et al. (2002) only investigated the directed effect of the discrete tap on the periodic tapping. DTE may occur in combination with each of the other periodic tapping patterns MTI, PTR and PTH. 7.2.1.1 Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI) Fig. 7-8a shows PRCs typical for MTI; the occurrence of the discrete left hand tap is indicated by the inclined dashed line from the origin to the point (abscissa: Φ = 1, ordinate: time = N (= 0.6s). From the straight horizontal orientation of the upper dot ‘lines’ it follows that the timing of the periodic taps is almost but not completely independent of the perturbing discrete tap. This independence was decreased in other condition different from normal tapping. 7.2.1.2 Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR) PTR is dominant in PRCs like in Fig. 7-8b. The occurrence of the perturbation (i.e. the discrete tap by the left hand) is somewhere (uniformly distributed) on the inclined dashed line after zero time; since the cycling of the subsequent periodic tapping restarts at the perturbation event, the next following periodic tap of the right hand occurs always after some “pausing” interval following the perturbation, independently of Φ, which results in the inclined dot ‘lines’ for positive times (upper half of the diagram). A retardation of the periodic right hand tapping is basically characterized by some pausing of the periodic tapping process during the execution of the discrete left hand tap. Such a pausing mainly occurs at two prominent states within the periodic tapping process and causes a distinct prolongation of the actual ITI. PTR will be described by the following two different cases: (i) Tap Delaying (TD): The periodic tapping is interrupted by the discrete tap. The subject is pausing the periodic process either at the up-position (around phase 0.5 (Fig. 6-25a) or at the down-position (around phase 0, 1 (Fig. 6-20a)) when starting the execution of the discrete tap. The pausing in the first case causes a lengthening of the periodic tap duration. The periodic process continues later, after the discrete movement will be finished. Subjects apply this strategy in all conditions (Fig. 7-9a shows normal tapping, Fig. 7-9b isometric tapping). (ii) Tap Cancelling (TC): The second kind of PTR is a more severe intervention in the periodic right hand tapping process by the discrete left hand tap event (Fig. 6-25b: the subject stops the on-going execution of the periodic movement on the fly at any position before ground contact, when the discrete movement is launched, and the finger is moved down or up (phase 0.5; note that this upward movement is against gravity thus needs muscle activation) together with it. Therefore, no ground contact is observed by the contact force sensors, even if there is a clear tapping activity in the position signal (Fig. 7-10a) and a strong buckling of the force amplitude in isometric tapping (Fig. 710b). It should be mentioned that distinction of these two cases appears ambiguous since the borderline between them is blurred. Especially in isometric tapping, the profiles of the time course look similar, because the decision is taken on the buckling amplitude only (e.g. Fig. 7-10a right and Fig. 7-10b left). Certainly, additional features for distinction can be derived from an extended data set in order to assure the two different interaction patterns TD and TC, but nevertheless this is an option, because TD and TC are subgroups of the well defined interaction pattern PTR. 172 Note that no difference between Tap Delay (TD) and Tap Cancelling (TC) is visible in the PRCs, because they are based on the timing structure only and TD is not the only cause for this behaviour in the range of small phases. Figure 7-11: Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH) in normal tapping (a) and in isometric tapping (b). A premature right hand tap possibly renders its synchronized execution with the discrete left hand tap. Figure 7-12: Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE) in normal tapping (a) and in isometric tapping (b). Delaying the discrete left hand tap possibly renders its synchronized execution in-phase with the next (periodic) right hand tap. The plots demonstrate this behaviour by the delayed execution of the left tap with respect to the go signal (vertical bar). 173 7.2.1.3 Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH) Dominant PTH is demonstrated in Fig. 7-8c und the time course is depicted in Fig. 7-11. For small values of Φ, the nearly horizontal dot ‘line’ composed of the right hand taps next after the perturbation event may indicate a MTI-behaviour in almost all subjects showing this pattern in normal condition. But beyond this range of Φ, the next periodic tap for t > 0 occurs simultaneously with the discrete left hand tap, which can be interpreted as a skip to the final state of the actual periodic tapping process or hastening of the cycling. The transient between both states seems to be limited by the minimum ITI which the individual subject can achieve, maybe due to biomechanical limitations. The further periodic tapping continues with standard ITIs after this first right hand tap after/with the perturbation event. This principle shows up either in all conditions (Fig. 7-11a, b show normal and isometric tapping). 7.2.1.4 Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE) As will be shown in 7.2.3.4, the more expressed shortening of the last preceding periods of the left one-sided coordination (i.e. the interaction is determined on the reference tap (6.5.2)) compared to the right one-sided coordination (i.e. the interaction is determined on the next tap after the reference tap (6.5.2)) makes the definition of reference tap difficult and hides the behaviour DTE (more details in 7.2.3.4 and Fig. 7-28). DTE behaviour also manifests itself in the PRC as a distinct aggregation of dots at small phase Φ (Fig. 7-8d). Small values of Φ mean that the execution of the discrete tap is somehow synchronized with the periodic tap, which usually is achieved by delaying the discrete tap until the simultaneous execution (Fig. 7-12). Thus, subjects with DTE dominance execute both taps in-phase within a preferred range of phase Φ from 0 to 0.4, so the dot density in this range is much higher than elsewhere. Note that the overall shape of the PRC in Fig. 7-8d shows PTH behaviour like in Fig. 7-8c, too, which is another form of an in-phase execution of both taps. But in addition to the dominant directed effect of the discrete task on the periodic task (PTH, Fig. 7-8c), a clear entrainment effect of the periodic task on the discrete task can be observed in Fig. 7-8d and Fig. 7-12. This effect indicates a mutual interaction between both. A distinct but less pronounced DTE component can also be recognized for the MTI subject in Fig. 7-8a. 7.2.2 Dominant tapping behaviour and discrete-tap timing characteristics in normal tapping In order to investigate the relationship between PRCs, individual tapping behaviour, and reaction times in more detail, the distributions of discrete tap occurrences over phase Φ and average reaction times in ST and DT experiments were determined for each of the 7 subjects in normal and isometric conditions. Subjects were grouped according to their dominant tapping behaviour and multiple comparisons of means were conducted to investigate possible effects of interaction patterns on onset phase and reaction time. 174 Figure 7-13: Histograms of onset phase Φ of the discrete taps. Data of the PRCs shown in Fig. 7-8 are shown in histogram form, also indicating the shape parameters ‘average onset phase Φ av’ of Φ (more details in 7.2.2.2). Note that even if diagrams show dominant interaction patterns, they all include the total of all 288 responses of an experiment, i.e. the responses of the other interaction patterns, too. Data for all subjects are summarized in numerical form by table 7.5. Table 7.4 (A, B): Proportion of interaction patterns in BM tapping for all subjects depicted for normal tapping (A) and isometric tapping (B). Values indicate the rate of occurrence in percent. Dominant tapping behaviour was determined by the most frequent interaction pattern out of PTH, PTR, DTE, and MTS (see definition of interaction pattern in Section 6.5.1 and Appendix). DTO (discrete tap omitted) represents an error class indicating that execution of the discrete tap failed. If none of the patterns exceeded a threshold of 25%, the dominant tapping behaviour was considered MTI. A Tapping behaviour [%] in normal tapping Subject PTR MTI PTH TD TC DTE MTS Subj. 1 58,33 8,34 20,84 6,25 2,08 3,47 Subj. 2 50,00 8,33 9,03 2,08 13,19 17,02 Subj. 3 62,15 9,72 3,82 2,08 12,85 9,03 Subj. 4 34,03 51,04 9,72 1,04 1,39 2,43 Subj. 5 53,13 0,69 28,82 4,17 2,77 10,07 Subj. 6 79,51 3,82 3,13 0,35 7,29 5,21 Subj. 7 45,59 29,86 9,38 1,04 5,90 7,98 175 Dominant behaviour DTO 0,69 0,35 0,35 0,35 0,35 0,69 0,35 PTR MTI MTI PTH PTR MTI PTH B Tapping behaviour [%] in isometric tapping Subject PTR Dominant behaviour MTI PTH TD DTE MTS DTO TC Subj. 1 Subj. 2 Subj. 3 Subj. 4 Subj. 5 Subj. 6 Subj. 7 52,08 28,82 73,61 35,07 12,15 70,83 35,07 6,94 2,78 2,08 35,42 1,39 4,86 4,86 19,10 32,99 2,08 6,94 64,23 4,17 28,12 10,07 11,11 1,39 6,25 10,42 0,69 9,38 4,17 6,60 11,46 2,43 0,35 8,68 3,13 7,29 17,01 9,03 12,85 11,11 10,42 18,40 0,35 0,69 0,35 1,04 0,35 0,35 1,04 PTR PTR MTI PTH PTR MTI PTR 7.2.2.1 Interaction patterns and dominant tapping behaviour The percentages of interaction patterns for 7 subjects are presented in table 7.4. Generally, all interaction patterns defined in Section 6.5.1 were observed in all subjects. A preference for one of dominant tapping behaviours was expressed by a dominant occurrence frequency of this interaction pattern. The subjects’ dominant tapping behaviour was determined by the criteria that it exceeded a threshold of 25% out of PTH, PTR, DTE and MTS. If none of the patterns was found, the dominant tapping behaviour was considered MTI. 7.2.2.2 Discrete taps: Distributions of their phase Φ and reaction times If the execution of the discrete taps is independent of the periodic tapping, the distribution of the onset phase Φ (within this cycle of periodic tapping) of the discrete taps is expected to be uniform because the imperative stimulus triggering the discrete tap was presented independently of the cycle of the periodic tapping. Any deviation from the uniform distribution indicates some interaction of the discrete and the periodic motor task. Fig. 7-13 depicts distributions of onset phase Φ of the discrete taps of the same data in Fig. 7-8. The relative phase-independent effect of the periodic task on the discrete task from Subject 5’s data is presented by the phase distribution somehow near to a uniform one (Fig. 7-13b). By contrast, the strict preference of small phase values in Fig. 7-13d indicates a strong DTE component, i.e. the execution of the discrete tap happens concurrently with the execution of the (pacing) next periodic tap for Subject 7. Also, a tendency to in-phase and (less expressed) anti-phase synchronisation of the discrete tap can be recognized in Fig. 7-13a. 176 Table 7.5 (A, B): Average onset phase Φav, skewness S of onset phase distribution, and average reaction time RTav of DT and SRTav of ST conditions for all subjects and experiments. A Subject Subj. 1 Subj. 2 Subj. 3 Subj. 4 Subj. 5 Subj. 6 Subj. 7 B Subject Subj. 1 Subj. 2 Subj. 3 Subj. 4 Subj. 5 Subj. 6 Subj. 7 normal tapping Φav S RTav SRTav 0.41 0.15 227 197 0.41 0.10 395 191 0.42 0.19 331 211 0.44 0.10 240 240 0.51 -0.08 258 195 0.40 0.29 281 219 0.27 0.85 322 213 isometric tapping Φav S RTav SRTav 0.37 0.54 182 161 0.43 0.15 282 196 0.48 0.14 227 230 0.27 0.90 197 174 0.50 0.25 199 168 0.35 0.60 210 219 0.40 0.28 250 217 dominant behaviour PTR MTI MTI PTH PTR MTI PTH dominant behaviour PTR PTR MTI PTH PTR MTI PTR The 2I-test (Sachs, 1984) revealed significant deviations from the uniform distribution for all subjects and experiments (p < 0.01) except for the normal tapping data of Subject 5 (Fig. 7-13b). The m3 average onset phase Φav and the skewness S = 3 of the phase distribution would make the m2 2 statement about task interaction and were determined for each DT trial, where m3 is the sample third central moment and m 2 is the sample variance. A perfectly symmetric distribution corresponds to shape parameters Φav = 0.5 and S = 0. Phase values deviating from Φav = 0.5 and a positive or negative skew S indicate an asymmetric distribution and, consequently, task interaction. These shape parameters Φav and S are summarized for all subjects and experiments in table 7.5. Moreover, average reaction times RTav of the DT experiments and SRTav of the ST reference experiments are shown (table 7.5). A 2-way ANOVA (24 repetitions with 12 responses per subject and experiment, 7 x 24 x 2 = 336 samples) with factors subject (1…7) and experiment (normal, isometric) revealed a significant main effect of subject on all variables (Φav: F(6, 322) = 22.70,S: F(6, 322) = 7.37, RTav: F(6, 322) = 76.62, all p < 0.0001). Moreover, there was a significant main effect of experiment on RTav (F(1, 322) = 318.77, p < 0.0001) and skew S (F(1, 322) = 9.17, p < 0.01) but not on onset phase Φav (F(1, 322) = 0.61). Post-hoc mean comparisons using Bonferroni-corrected t-tests (Sachs, 1984) revealed that both in normal and in isometric tapping six out of the seven phase distributions were roughly uniform. Average values of Φav = 0.43 and a slightly positive skew S = 0.23 of these subjects’ data is consistent with a small peak at Φav < 0.4 in the corresponding histograms and the increased dot density in corresponding PRCs (e.g. Fig. 7-8a, Fig. 7-13a). This indicates that the majority of discrete taps was initiated independently of the phase of the periodic movement in these subjects, even if there was some tendency to in-phase and anti-phase tapping of the right and left hand. A significantly more 177 pronounced asymmetry and a strict preference of in-phase tapping were found for Subject 7 in normal tapping and for Subject 4 in isometric tapping. The shape parameters of these two subjects changed between experiments (p < 0.001) while differences in phase distributions of the other subjects did not reach significance. Interestingly, Subject 4 showed a close to uniform distribution (Φav = 0.44, S = 0.10) in normal tapping but changed to a more asymmetric distribution (Φ av = 0.27, S = 0.90) in isometric tapping. By contrast, Subject 7 showed a positive skew (Φ av = 0.27, S = 0.85) in normal tapping (c.f. Fig. 7-13d) but a roughly uniform distribution (Φav = 0.40, S = 0.27) in isometric tapping. This decreasing asymmetry was accompanied by a change in dominant tapping behaviour from PTH to PTR (table 7.4). By contrast, despite of the significant change in shape parameters, Subject 4 showed dominant PTH both in normal and isometric tapping, while Subject 2 switched from MTI to PTR behaviour without remarkable changes in shape parameters. Thus, an alteration of dominant tapping behaviour was not necessarily accompanied by a corresponding change in shape parameters and vice versa. For the statistical analysis of reaction times, ST data SRTav were grouped in 24 repetitions with 3 responses per subject and experiment. Together with the DT reaction times RTav, they were analyzed by a 3-way ANOVA (Sachs, 1984) with factors subject (1…7), experiment (normal, isometric), and task (single, dual) which revealed significant main effects of all factors (subject: F(6, 644) = 74.73, experiment: F(1, 644) = 330.32, task: F(1, 644) = 526.87, all p < 0.0001) on RT. Post-hoc mean comparisons revealed that, generally, RT was shorter in isometric tapping than in normal tapping. This may reflect the fact that in isometric tapping the first detectable change in the force signal almost directly images the changes in muscle contraction while in normal tapping the onset in the force signal indicates the time of first ground contact of the finger tip. Depending upon the subject's individual initial (‘resting’) position of the finger above ground level, the latter occurs with some delay which is reflected by the prolonged average RT. Figure 7-14: Multiple comparison of average reaction times of single-task (empty circles) and dual-task (filled circles) experiments for normal (a, b) and isometric (c, d) tapping. Horizontal bars represent the tolerance intervals. Disjoint intervals indicate that mean values are significantly different (p < 0.05). Average reaction times RTav and SRTav for all subjects are depicted in Fig. 7-14. Generally, RTav of the DT experiments were longer than SRTav of the ST reference experiments (normal: 293 ms > 209 ms, isometric: 221 ms > 195 ms, p < 0.001) indicating a clear effect of task complexity on RT. As can 178 be seen from Fig. 7-14a, in normal tapping the prolongation of RT was more pronounced for the subjects with dominant MTI behaviour compared to the subjects with PTR or PTH. Despite his dominant PTH behaviour, Subject 7 shows a larger delay similar to that of the MTI subjects which is consistent with the asymmetric phase distribution indicating a strong tendency to in-phase tapping and a distinct DTE component for this subject. The effect of directed interaction on RT is even more obvious in Fig. 7-14b where subjects were grouped according to their dominant behaviour. Interaction patterns PTR and PTH which are assumed to predominately affect the periodic task only show a small delay of 46 ms and 55 ms, respectively, while dominant MTI behaviour is associated with a significantly larger prolongation of 128 ms, which indicates some (hidden) mutual interaction of both tasks. Interaction effects were less pronounced in isometric tapping (Figs. 7.14c and 7.14d). Generally, prolongation of RT due to the additional periodic task (26 ms) was smaller than in normal tapping (84 ms). Moreover, this prolongation is mainly caused by Subject 2 who reported a general difficulty to coordinate the two motor tasks; maybe, this problem is reflected by the fact that this subject had the longest RT in DT both in normal and isometric tapping. For four subjects, there were no significant differences between SRT (ST) and RT (DT). It should be noted that, despite the obvious statistical significance of the differences in RT shown in Figs. 7.14b and 7.14d, conclusions on the causal relationship between dominant tapping behaviour and response latency have to be drawn with care. While the estimates of Φ av, S, RTav, SRTav are based upon a sufficiently large number of samples (24 repetitions with 12 responses per subject and experiment), the classification according to the dominant tapping behaviour is based on the comparably small number of seven subjects being assigned to the three classes PTR, PTH and MTI. Therefore, latencies associated with a particular tapping behaviour may be biased by the individual RT of the corresponding subject(s). Nevertheless, the general consistency of the results obtained by heuristic analysis of tapping behaviour and the results obtained by statistical analysis of the "objective" variables Φav, S, RTav, SRTav indicate a close relationship between dominant tapping behaviour and discrete-tap timing characteristics. Note that even if each diagram in Fig. 7-8 presents a dominant tapping behaviour, it always comprises all data of an experiment, i.e. all occurrences of the other interaction patterns, too. Moreover, since the four typical appearances of PRCs and corresponding phase distributions for normal and isometric tapping look very similar, only data of normal tapping are presented. 7.2.3 Time course analysis of the tapping process Analysis of tapping behavior based on the continuous trajectories of fingers is another possibility to assess the motor coordination problem. The nomenclature for the shape conditions was described in 6.5.2 and should be shortly repeated; 7 different exclusive conditions were defined as slope interaction schemes: 1. down-slope synchronization (DS) 2. up-slope synchronization (US) 3. 2-sided slope synchronization (TS) 4. 2-sided antiphase slope synchronization (AP2S) 5. 1-sided antiphase slope synchronization (AP1S) 6. slope asynchrony in overlapping taps (SA) 7. tap asynchrony (TA) If the interaction is determined on the reference tap, a “1” follows the letters (e.g. DS1), and on the next tap after the reference tap, a “2” is written (e.g. DS2). 179 Phase Figure 7-15: Phase distribution of pooled data: a) the left one-sided coordination (TS1; DS1; US1; SA1; AP1S1) cover small phases b) the rest (TA; AP2S) cover the middle phases c) the right one-sided coordination (TS2; DS2; US2; AP1S2, SA2) cover the large phases. Phase Figure 7-16: pooled phases separated in two classes of fast reactions (upper diagram) and slower reaction times (slower diagram). The strict preferred left one-sided coordination has almost fast reaction time whereas the right one-sided coordination has the delayed reaction for the next preferred coordination. Fig. 7-15 shows the phase distribution pooled for all subjects for the left one-sided coordination (TS1, DS1, SA1, US1, AP1S1), the right one-sided coordination (TS2, DS2, SA2, US2, AP1S2), and the rest (TA, AP2S) and Fig. 7-16 all together separated by fast and slow RT in two classes. The hidden mutual interaction of both tasks is now more pronounced. On one side, the effect of the discrete tap on the periodic tapping is clearly indicated by preferred phases (Fig. 7-15a, c). On the other side, the effect of the periodic tapping on the discrete tap resulting in a faster RT is mainly appearing for small phases (Fig. 7-16, upper panel), and delayed reactions mainly happen with phases between 0.3 and 0.8 (Fig. 7-16, lower panel). Data were pooled to obtain large amount of data for every coordination pattern. Inspecting the finger tip position data in normal tapping and the force data in isometric hand (foot) tapping revealed the phase relationships between the discrete left hand tap and the periodic right hand taps more clearly, and the coordinated movement behaviour can be specified. Generally, all trajectory coordination patterns can be observed in all subjects but with different probability of occurrence depending on which interaction pattern is dominant. Based on the sided interaction of 180 discrete taps with two neighbouring periodic taps, the coordination patterns are grouped in four classes. The first most frequent class is the left one-sided coordination (DS1, SA1, US1, and AP1S1), the second one is the right one-sided coordination (DS2, SA2, US2, AP1S2). These two most frequently classes reveal the synchronization even without any detailed analysis,. The other two less frequently occurring conditions are two-sided cross-like coordination (AP2S) as well as the total asynchrony (TA). 7.2.3.1 Tap duration adjustment The tap durations of the affected periodic taps and the other neighbouring taps and the discrete taps were mutually compared for the different coordination patterns. Figure 7-17: Distribution of the discrete tap durations and the four neighbouring periodic tap durations. PTD: periodic tap duration; DTD: discrete tap duration; index: 0 the reference tap, -1 the last periodic tap preceding reference tap, +1, +2 the 2 succeeding periodic taps. The shorter discrete tap durations are accompanied by shorter reference tap durations (left diagram for (TS1, DS1)) and shorter durations of the periodic taps succeeding the reference taps (right diagram for (TS2, DS2)), respectively. 181 Figure 7-18: ANOVA was used to compare the means of the observations in the tap duration of the four neighbouring taps (two preceding ptd 2 , ptd 1 , one succeeding ptd 1 and the reference tap td 0 ) and the corresponding discrete taps (dtd). The test asserted that the affected periodic tap duration (reference taps in (A (TS1), upper panel) and taps succeeding reference taps in (B (TS2), lower panel) have means significantly different from the others tap durations. Figure 7-19: Distribution of the discrete tap durations and the four neighbouring periodic tap durations, same as in Fig.7-17 but now for the conditions US1 and US2. PTD: periodic tap duration; DTD: discrete tap duration. The reference tap durations were prolonged (left diagram) for embedding of the discrete taps whereas the discrete taps were prolonged for embedding of the periodic taps succeeding the reference taps. The embedded taps were slightly shortened in both cases (PTD 0 in left diagram and PTD 1 in right diagram). 182 Figure 7-20: Distribution of the discrete tap durations of TA. A well-formed distribution (~ normal distribution) indicates the preferred combination of both tap duration to obtain the required timing of periodic task. Generally, the short discrete tap durations due to their impulse-like characteristic equalized the affected periodic ones in many subjects. This gives evidence for synchronisation. This effect is more pronounced in (TS1, TS2) (Figs. 6.21 7.17) and also in part of (SA1, SA2) when the both fingers are near enough from each other. The ANOVA revealed that the affected periodic taps have mean durations significantly different from those of neighbouring taps but within them do not (Fig. 7-18). For the embedding of the discrete tap into the periodic tap for a common upward movement, prolongation (TD) of the enclosing tap duration and shortening of the enclosed tap duration is the used strategy in (US1, US2) (Fig. 6-20, 7-19). The quick inserting of the discrete tap during the resting phase (up-position) of the periodic finger to obtain stable timing reflected their well-form distribution and their variability in other cases (Fig. 6-25b, 7-20). This corresponds to the most preference of simple related frequency ratio 2:1 in the performance of polyrhythms (Summers 2002). 7.2.3.2 Slope duration adjustment The comparison of the periodic slope durations between the affected taps and other neighbouring taps and the comparison of discrete slope durations between different coordination patterns were also performed. Figure 7-21: Distribution of the discrete downslope durations and those of the four neighbouring periodic downslope durations. PD: periodic downslope; DD: discrete downslope; index: 0 the reference downslope, -1 the last periodic downslope preceding reference downslope, +1, +2 are the corresponding for the succeeding periodic downslopes. The fast discrete downslope durations shortened the reference downslope durations (left diagram for (TS1, DS1)) and the periodic downslope durations succeeding the reference taps (right diagram for (TS2, DS2)). 183 Figure 7-22: Distribution of discrete upslope durations. The longer periodic upslope durations prolonged the discrete upslope durations when they were overlapping (upper diagram) compared to cases when they do not (lower diagram). Figure 7-23: Distribution of periodic upslope durations in one-sided cross-like coordination. The shorter discrete downslope durations accelerated the coordinated reference upslope durations (AP1S1, upper panel) compared to the non-coordinated case (AP1S2, lower panel). The faster discrete downslopes shortened the synchronized periodic downslopes in (TS1, DS1, TS2, DS2) (Figs. 6-19, 6-21, 7-21), whereas the faster discrete upslopes were prolonged by the slower periodic upslopes in (TS1, US1, TS2, US2) (Figs. 6.20 a,b, 6-21, 7-22). Again, this trend was also found in part of (SA1, SA2). Upward movements often ended together although they started at different time or at different amplitudes. In one-sided cross-like coordination the upslope duration of the reference taps were shortened by the fast discrete downslopes (Fig. 6-23a, 7-23). 7.2.3.3 Trajectories adjustment In two-sided cross-like coordination (AP2S) the attraction of the faster downslope duration of one finger on the slower upslope duration of the other one leaded to a more harmonic form of both trajectories (Fig. 6-22). A speedup of the periodic tapping in PTH was presented by a stronger deflection (as an ahead abort of its process) and entrained by the discrete one (Fig. 6-20b). The 184 insertion of the discrete tap into the periodic tap was often combined with the shortening of the discrete tap duration and lengthening of the periodic one (Fig. 6-20). Figure 7-24: Distribution of affected ITIs (middle Diagram) and two neighbouring ITIs (upper and lower Diagrams) in TA. The affected ITIs were strong retarded or even reset. Figure 7-25: Distribution of affected ITIs in (SA1, US1) separated in small phases and large phases. The larger the phases are the farer the discrete taps from the reference tap and the longer are the periods. 185 Figure 7-26: Distribution of affected ITIs in (AP1S1) separated in small phases and large phases. The larger these phases are the farer the discrete taps from the reference tap and the more instable are the periods. Figure 7-27: Distribution of affected ITIs (2nd Diagram) and two neighbouring ITIs (1st and 3rd Diagrams) in twosided, cross-like coordination. The affected ITIs were contracted on both sides. 186 Figure 7-28: Distribution of ITIs preceding the discrete taps separated in two groups of coordination patterns. Diagram a) and b) contain ITIs separated in two groups of coordination patterns (TS1, DS1, AS1, US1) and (TS2, DS2, AS2, US2), respectively. Diagram c and d contain the first group separated in two classes of lower reaction times (<mean (RT) – standard deviation (RT)) and of higher reaction times (>mean (RT) + standard deviation (RT)), respectively. The left one-sided coordination (TS1, DS1, AS1, and US1) had more shortened ITIs than the right one but these ITIs with longer RT were shorter than ones with shorter RT. 7.2.3.4 Periodic timing adjustment The strong PTR caused by TD or TC and almost found in TA is shown in Fig. 7-24. In normal tapping, although the range of small phases enclosing most (TS1, DS1, SA1, US1, and AP1S1) cases shows that timing is relatively stable, a weak PTR is hidden in (SA1, US1). The subsequent cycling was started at the perturbation events. This is indirectly shown in the distribution of affected periods over phases separated in small and large phases (Fig. 7-25). The affected periods were longer when the discrete taps are farer from the reference tap. The instability of AP1S1 is shown in Fig. 7-26. In two-sided cross-like coordination (AP2S) the affected periods were often shortened (Fig. 7-27) due to the attraction of the faster down periods on the slower up periods as mentioned above. Depending on reaction time, the right one-sided coordination, where the both movements are in the same direction, could result in PTH or a mix of PTH with DTE behaviour. In PTH, the launching of the discrete tap reset the phase of the periodic tapping process and causes a premature execution of the next periodic tap. Instead of PTH of the right hand tap to maintain the prescribed period stable, 187 the execution of the discrete tap is postponed until the next possible stable coordination emerges, which leads to DTE. A trade-off between reaction time and timing requirement would yield in the mixed behaviour (DTE with PTH). The more shortening of the last preceding periods of the left onesided coordination compared to the right one indicates the challenge of the definition of reference tap and hides the behaviour DTE (Fig. 7-28). 7.2.3.5 Force adjustment The overall difference in forces of five periodic taps around discrete taps was estimated by ANOVA and the complying multiple comparison test. Force of the periodic finger was increased when it was performed together with the discrete finger on the hard surface (ANOVA, F(1, 288)=7.78, P<0.0001). 7.2.4 Effects of physiological parameters ANOVA and the complying multiple-comparison test were used to determine the overall difference in amplitudes of five periodic taps around discrete taps in the contact-free condition of 5 subjects’ data. Movement amplitude of the synchronized periodic finger was often decreased in comparison with other periodic amplitudes around (ANOVA, F(1, 288)=7.78, P<0.02). Visual inspection of position signals shows a marked asymmetry in normal tapping compared to the contact-free-tapping particularly during the continuation phase, i.e.. the flexion or downward phase of the movement has a much steeper slope than the extension or upward phase in normal tapping while they are more harmonic in contact-free condition. Thus, the biomechanical differences between flexion and extension are not significantly attributable to the difference between the two phases of downward and upward movement in a given cycle. The following results also are apparent from position signals and traces in PRCs without any complicated analysis. The TD and TC were increased in isometric and particularly TC in contact-free condition. This stopping clearly shows up as a strong buckling of the amplitude. The tap delay and cancelling yielded in an incline to higher degree of PTR, i.e.. it trends to Type 0 Phase Reset even in the first half of the periodic tap interval. It is opposite in strong tapping that within the second half of the periodic tap interval TC was reduced and the premature execution of the next periodic tap together with launching of the discrete tap was increased. Even in strong PTR behaviour where the inclined formation of the dots are in parallel with the dashed line of the discrete tap after the perturbation in normal tapping, These dots are now falling down on the dashed line The reduction of TC was combined with DTE and PTH and a clearer synchronization was presented by an increased density of the dots around small and large phases in PRC. The MTI-behaviour hence degraded in isometric and contact-free conditions. 188 Phase Figure 7-29: Phase distribution of 5 subjects in the attention condition (focus on both tasks (left column), focus on periodic task (middle column), and focus on discrete task (right column)). 189 Time (ms) Figure 7-30: disturbed ITI distribution of same 5 subjects in the attention condition (focus on both tasks (left column), focus on periodic task (middle column), and focus on discrete task (right column)). 190 Time (ms) Figure 7-31: Reaction time distribution of the same 5 subjects in the attention condition (focus on both tasks (left column), focus on periodic task (middle column), and focus on discrete task (right column)). The phase distributions in the attention condition performed under one of the two timing instructions ‘‘focus on periodic’’ and ‘‘focus on reaction’’ show the 2-modal form with higher clustered distribution in the first condition (Fig. 7-29). The I2-tests established that almost all distributions were significantly different from a uniform distribution (α < 0.01), however the test variables of the first condition were higher as well as the constraint on the discrete movement onset was visibly more pronounced. ANOVA (24 repetitions with 12 responses per experiment, 24 x 2 = 48 variance samples) with factor instruction (focus on periodic, focus on reaction) was performed on the 191 variation of the ITIs around disturbance for every subject. The ANOVA showed a significant main effect of instruction (α < 0.05). The variability of the first succeeding ITI was higher when the subjects with strong PTR paid attention on periodic movement than when they paid attention on discrete reaction. The affected periods of the other subjects were more shortened causing the mean value highly lower than the pacing frequency when they focused on reaction whereas the mean value approached the pacing frequency when they focused on periodic movement (Fig. 7-30). All subject approximately reached the same mean reaction time when they focused on the task (Fig. 7-31), The comparison between normal tapping and strong tapping showed higher clustered phase distribution of the 2-modal form, the larger 2I-test variables for uniform distribution of phases as well as the constraint on the discrete movement onset was visibly more pronounced and the reduced variability of the succeeding ITIs in strong tapping, the affected period of subjects with strong PTR behaviour were shortened such that a change to PTH is obvious. 7.3 Hand-Foot condition Analogously to the LH condition, the participants of the response conditions LF, RF, LH-LF, LH-RF, RF-LF, and LH-RF-LF reproduced each of the different types of coordination patterns and also the dominant behaviour while the other types also appeared in some trials and furthermore they can be observed with discrete foot responses, too. Table 6: The Proportion of interaction patterns MTI, PTR, PTH, DTE, and MTS (in %) in different response conditions for all participants. The amount of discrete taps (segments) considered is between 276 and 288 for each effector combination (variation is due to discarded erroneous segments). The dominant tapping pattern values are highlighted. LH P MTI 1 33.2 2 69.7 3 48.5 4 36.9 5 27.8 6 90.2 LH-RF P MTI 1 43.4 2 54.9 3 25.5 4 37.6 5 30.0 6 90.6 PTR 18.2 18.5 18.8 55.8 29.9 0.3 PTR 17.4 11.3 31.2 15.4 39.2 7.7 PTH 41.3 11.1 30.3 0.0 40.3 8.7 PTH 28.8 32.3 39.6 31.1 29.0 1.2 DTE 2.1 0.7 1.7 0.3 0.7 0.4 DTE 2.8 1.0 0.4 3.1 0.4 0.2 MTS 5.2 0.0 0.7 7.0 1.3 0.4 RF MTI 38.0 38.0 36.8 52.1 38.3 96.9 MTS 7.6 0.5 3.3 12.8 1.4 0.3 LH-LF MTI PTR 41.0 17.8 40.3 33.7 34.7 45.0 44.5 43.9 30.3 34.4 88.5 11.3 PTR 10.5 20.6 42.0 14.2 24.0 1.4 PTH 20.9 39.7 19.1 27.1 34.5 1.0 PTH 29.5 25.5 18.4 9.3 31.7 0.0 192 DTE 8.0 0.0 0.7 2.8 0.7 0.7 DTE 3.8 0.5 0.5 0.2 1.8 0.0 MTS 22.6 1.7 1.4 3.8 2.5 0.0 LF MTI 42.0 63.2 45.6 47.6 24.6 97.9 PTR 10.3 18.7 39.3 43.7 38.9 0.3 PTH 9.2 18.1 14.7 1.4 34.5 0.4 DTE 15.2 0.0 0.0 0.7 0.3 1.4 MTS 23.3 0.0 0.4 6.6 1.7 0.0 MTS 7.9 0.0 1.4 2.1 2.8 0.2 RF-LF MTI PTR 41.0 25.3 42.7 20.2 46.0 46.8 36.8 31.3 31.2 31.0 91.0 7.4 PTH 18.4 35.3 4.3 20.1 30.3 0.9 DTE 4.0 0.9 0.6 2.1 0.7 0.5 MTS 11.3 0.6 2.2 9.7 6.8 0.2 LH-RF-LF P MTI PTR 1 41.1 13.8 2 44.7 17.6 3 33.9 33.3 4 41.0 16.2 5 28.5 34.8 6 85.2 14.2 PTH 31.3 37.2 27.8 38.8 25.5 0.6 DTE 2.0 0.0 0.8 0.7 4.4 0.0 MTS 11.8 0.5 4.0 3.2 6.8 0.0 Table 6 shows the percentage of each of the interaction types for all participants and all conditions. Figure 7-32: Phase resetting curves (PRC) for single response conditions (see list in text): (A) PRC for LH (in LH condition), (B) PRC for LF (in LF condition), and (C) PRC for RF (in RF condition), all for Participant 3. In all cases, the dot lines above abscissa (i.e., taps after the reference tap) are clearly inclined, revealing a strong interaction between the periodic tapping and the discrete motor responses. 193 2.5 A1 LH A2 LF 2.0 1.5 Time (s) 1.0 0.5 0 - 0.5 - 1.0 - 1.5 2.5 B1 LH B2 RF C1 LF C2 RF 2.0 1.5 T ime (s) 1.0 0.5 0 - 0.5 - 1.0 - 1.5 2.5 2.0 T ime (s) 1.5 1.0 0.5 0 - 0.5 - 1.0 - 1.5 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 Phase 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 Phase Figure 7-33: Phase resetting curves (PRC) for double response conditions (see list in text): (A1) PRC for LH (in LH-LF condition), (A2) PRC for LF (in LH-LF condition), (B1) PRC for LH (in LH-RF condition), (B2) PRC for RF (in LH-RF condition), (C1) PRC for LF (in LF-RF condition), and (C2) PRC for RF (in LF-RF condition), all for Participant 3. Again in all cases, the dot lines above abscissa (i.e., taps after the reference tap) are clearly inclined, revealing the same strong interaction effect for double discrete responses like observed with single responses shown in Fig. 7-32. 194 Figure 7-34: Phase resetting curves (PRC) for triple response condition (see list in text): (A) PRC for LH (in LH-LFRF condition), (B) PRC for LF (in LH-LF-RF condition), and (C) PRC for RF (in LH-LF-RF condition), all for Participant 3. Also for triple discrete responses, the same strong interaction effects like for single and dual discrete responses are observed. It is apparently that the increased proportion of MTI responses originates from the effect that many responses with phase values Φ around 0 and around 0.9 are classified as MTI because of the "regular" periodic ITI. It should be considered that the numerical criteria for interaction pattern classification are not absolutely tight but they yield classification rates of about 90% compared to the classification of an expert (i.e., 100% level); nevertheless, this effectiveness is sufficient for a general grouping. The PRCs plotted for each participant in each experimental condition indicate PTR and PTH as dominant interaction patterns for Participants 1 – 5. None of the participants have shown DTE as a dominant behavior, but Participant 6 turned out to belong to the rare group of absolute MTI dominant people. In Fig. 7-32, 7-33, and 7-34, the PRCs of Participant 3 are displayed, who is representative of PTR/PTH-dominant Participants 1 - 5 (i.e., in total, 12 PRCs are plotted for each condition; in case of combined responses, PRCs are presented separately for each participating effector (Fig. 7-33 and 7-34). The PRCs of Participant 6 (with MTI dominance) are not informative since they are monotonically constructed of a horizontal dot lines as a result of the lacking interaction between the periodic tapping and discrete responses in the DT conditions. 195 Table 7: table shows correlation coefficients between limb RTs in different effectors’ combinations. Data are obtained from DT experiments with triple discrete responses. Participant 1 2 3 4 5 6 Mean LH /LF 0.88 0.62 0.88 0.90 0.90 0.77 0.83 LH /RF 0.86 0.79 0.82 0.88 0.85 0.80 0.83 LF /RF 0.96 0.85 0.91 0.96 0.91 0.88 0.91 For underlining the coupling effects of the effectors within the discrete responses, the correlation coefficients between the participating limbs were calculated for the triple response RTs in DT (Table 7). Evidently, correlation coefficients are high for all participants and in all considered pairs, ranging from 0.62 to 0.96. The pooled means showed values above 0.8 for all effector combinations, with a maximum of 0.91 for the LF-RF couple. These high correlation coefficients are also reflected in the corresponding scatter diagrams (Fig. 7-36). The three subplots A, B, and C again show the pooled data of all participants; clearly, all distributions scatter around the diagonal (representing a correlation coefficient of 1.0) with the LF-RF case showing the tightest clustering. Left Hand Left Foot Right Foot 180 200 220 240 260 Mean RT (ms) 280 300 320 Figure 7-35: Mean RTs together with their confidence intervals for the left hand (LH), left foot (LF) and right foot (RF) with task condition as parameter (dots: ST; squares: DT). The relationship between the three limbs is the same in ST and DT, but RTs for DT are longer reflecting the DT costs. Different widths of confidence intervals are due to the different sample sizes of ST and DT conditions. All PRCs in Fig. 7-32, 7-33, and 7-34 visualize that discrete hand and foot responses basically share the same timing structure. Fig. 7-32 depicts PRCs for the single discrete responses (either LH, RF, or LF) of Participant 3. The BM DT experiment (Fig. 7-32A) revealed more scattered periodic tapping after the perturbation (at t = 0) as compared to the DT experiments employing foot responses (Fig. 732B and 7-32C); however, the general interaction pattern does not depend on the effectors. Fig. 7-33 presents the data with the paired discrete responses; interestingly to note that the phase value at which the transition from PTR to PTH behavior occurs is reduced in case when a contralateral response of LH and RF is required (0.5 → 0.3). This is also true for left hand behavior in the triple response conditions (Fig. 7-34A). Statistical analysis of RTs was performed for all participants and for all conditions, but results of discrete triple responses in both DT and ST conditions were most informative. Three-way ANOVA of RT with the factors participant, task and limb revealed highly significant main effect of all factors on RT: participant, F(5, 5487) = 98.97; task, F(1, 5487) = 44.85; limb, F(2, 5487) = 445.07, all p’s < 0.001. 196 ANOVA also revealed significant interaction effects in pairs participant – limb (F(10, 5487) = 32.41) and participant – task (F(5, 5487) = 76.64) (all p’s < 0.001). However, there were no significant interaction effects between limb and task, F(2, 5487) = 0.01. Post hoc mean comparisons (Fig. 7-35) revealed that mean RT for DT is significantly longer than mean RT for ST, indicating an effect of the DT complexity on RT. RTs for the left hand were significantly shorter than RTs for either foot on average. RTs for the right foot were slightly shorter than RTs for the left foot, but this difference was not statistically significant. Even though individual RT behavior showed some variations among the participants (e.g., slower and faster RT compared to the standard) all the results fit to the RT scheme depicted in Fig. 7-35. This figure also indicates the tight relationship between the RTs of the different effectors: their mutual relations within the ST group and the DT group are almost the same showing about 25 ms longer RTs for the feet. RT differences between ST and DT did not significantly vary across the limbs: for LH = 56.0 ms (confidence interval [43.5 – 68.5 ms], for LF= 56.7 ms [44.7 – 68.6 ms], and for RF= 56.0 ms [44.6 – 67.5 ms]). Analogous to the strong condition the trend to Type 0 Phase Reset in the first half of the periodic tap interval was also found particularly with the combination of left foot and left hand but within the second half of the periodic tap interval TC was reduced and the premature execution of the next periodic tap together with launching of the discrete tap was increased. This result was pronounced in subjects with strong PTR behaviour. In many cases a higher density of the dots around small and large phases in PRC and the MTI-behaviour is obviously degraded. 197 Figure 7-36: RT scatter diagrams for all limb combinations (A: LH-LF, B: LH-RF, C: LF-RF) in DT triple discrete responses. The high correlation shown by the narrow clustering of dots around the diagonal indicates a common motor command to all limbs, even if the timing of the command itself can be affected by DT costs. In A and B, the slight leftward shift of the dot clusters corresponds to the shorter neural propagation delay to the hand muscles compared to the delay of the foot muscles. 7.4 OM-DT condition Although the eye and hand movements share some common brain structure, the issue of their interference has remained controversial. The PRCs clearly indicate an interaction between hands and between hand and foot. The sharing of common networks must not necessarily be effective in all DT conditions but can be redundant in this condition. 198 a. Oculo-manual dual-task 2.0 Time (s) 1.0 Reference taps 0 -1.0 -2.0 b. Bimanual dual-task 2.0 Time (s) 1.0 Reference taps 0 -1.0 -2.0 0 0.2 0.4 Phase 0.6 0.8 1 Figure 7-37: Typical examples of phase resetting curves (PRCs): Results of participant N5 obtained in the oculomanual and BM DT experiments (“a” and “b”, respectively) are presented. Inclined solid line shows the onsets of discrete events. Note that different symbols (square, circle, and triangle) in the PRC-lines above the abscissa indicate the first, second, and third taps executed after the reference tap, respectively. The occurrence of the reference taps is at t=0. More details of PRC construction can be seen in the Section 6.4.1. (a) The horizontal orientation of all PRC-lines indicates that saccade execution does not disturb the periodic tapping. (a) The changed orientation of the PRC-lines above the abscissa (representing post-perturbation taps) in comparison to the lines below the abscissa (comprised of pre-perturbation taps) clearly indicates an interaction between the two motor tasks. The results of these DT experiments were evaluated based on the phase resetting curves. Fig. 737 shows typical examples from the oculo-manual (Fig. 7-37A) and BM DT (Fig. 7-37B) experiments, both performed by participant N5. The major finding of these experiments drawn from PRC profiles is that the periodic tapping timing is nearly unaffected by execution of the saccade as the discrete event. In Fig. 7-37A, the three successive taps executed after each perturbation event above the abscissa are approximately parallel to the abscissa as well as parallel to the two preceding taps executed before the reference tap independent of the phase ϕ of the perturbation event. This type of behavior is termed weak interaction, which corresponds to ‘type 1 phase resetting behavior’ according to Yoshino et al. (2002). (Note that the usage of the terms “type 0 reset” and “type 1 reset” in Yoshino et al., 2002, is not entirely compatible with the original definition of Winfree, 1980.) The inclined line above the abscissa arising from the origin (t=0, ϕ=0) of the graphs in Fig. 7-37 represents the locus of the onset times of the discrete responses. This line represents no measured data but is setup by definition: the perturbation onset phase value depicted on the abscissa is directly proportional to tperturbation (shown on ordinate) according to Φ = tperturbation/ N. as defined in Section 6.4.2. 199 a. (N3) 2.0 Time (s) 1.5 1.0 0.5 a1 a2 b1 b2 b. (N13) Time (s) 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 c. (N11) Time (s) 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 c1 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 c2 1.2 0 Phase 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 Phase Figure 7-38: Results of DT experiments selected to visualize inter-individual variability: Phase resetting curves (PRCs) of three typical participants (N3, N11, and N13) obtained in the BM and the oculo-manual experiments show the timing of first three periodic taps executed after the reference tap as function of the discrete response onset phase. Inclined solid line represents the onsets of discrete events. Different symbols (square, circle, and triangle) indicate the first, second, and third taps executed after the reference tap, respectively. Note that the occurrence of the reference taps is at t=0. Results plotted in (a) show an example representative for the group of nine participants with strong BM and weak oculo-manual interaction (shown in a1 and a2 plots, respectively). In (b), again the PRC-lines are tilted with respect to the abscissa in the BM conditions (b1), whereas in the oculo-manual task (b2) they are horizontal, which indicates only a slight interaction in the oculomanual experiments. In the BM DT (b1) there is a noticeable scatter and a down-jump of the PRC-lines which tend to incline at the end, indicating periodic tap hastening; this tapping behavior was predominantly shown by two participants. It corresponds again to a strong interaction in BM DT conditions. Another representative participant in c shows a rare case (only two in our results) where PRC-lines of both plots have approximately the same horizontal orientation (i.e., no significant interaction in BM (c1) as well as in oculo-manual (c2) tasks). In striking contrast to this oculo-manual performance, the periodic tapping in the BM DT (7.35B) shows a distortion of performance by the left hand tap: the three PRC-lines above the abscissa (which represents t=0) appear to be inclined, being parallel to the inclined line (i.e., discrete tap onset time) in Fig. 7-37A. This indicates that the periodic tap following the perturbation event occurs 1 cycle after the perturbation event. For larger values of ϕ, in several participants the next periodic tap is premature and occurs together with the discrete response indicated by a down-step in the course of the respective PRC-line (e.g., at ϕ≈0.6 in Fig. 7-37B as well as at ϕ≈0.2 in Fig. 7-38B1). This strong interaction between the two tasks in the DT scheme has been termed as “type 0 reset” behavior by Yoshino et al. (2002). Our findings are in perfect agreement with study of Yoshino et al. (2002) who also found strong interaction for BM DT tapping. Fig. 7-38 presents the three PRC types of the experimental oculo-manual and of the control BM DT being observed in the 13 participants. The PRCs are reduced to the first three taps executed after the reference tap (i.e., to the three PRC-lines above the abscissa), for graphical purposes. 9 of the 13 200 participants build a homogenous group being represented by Fig. 7-38A; they exhibited strong interaction in BM finger tapping (left graph) and weak interaction in oculo-manual DT conditions (right graph). Two other participants behaved in the same way, but they showed the hastening effect in addition (i.e., if the interval between the reference tap and the discrete tap exceeds some minimum (“refractory”) period, the next periodic tap occurs simultaneously with the discrete tap (see Fig. 7-38B). Fig. 7-38C gives an example of the rare case of weak interaction in BM DT (Fig. 738Cc1), too, observed in two participants (such tapping behavior was discussed in the context of well-trained musicians by e.g., Franek et al. (1991) and Watson (2006)). However, this finding still follows the line of very weak interaction in oculo-manual DT as observed in the other participants. Thus, in summary, we conclude that saccade discrete task does not strongly modulate the periodic manual tapping in oculo-manual DT situation, which is in high contrast to a manual discrete response in BM DT finger tapping experiments (Yoshino et al., 2002). The performance ITIs of the periodic tapping in a ST as control was checked for task coupling within a DT situation. The mean ITI values were 564.8 ms (±46.8 ms SD) in the right hand tapping task, 532.8 ms (51.6 ms SD) in the BM DT, and 552.2 ms (±45.8 ms SD) in the oculomanual DT. ANOVA did not reveal a significant difference between the ITIs (p>0.05) of single manual and oculomanual DT conditions, i.e. the periodic tapping in the oculo-manual DT experiment is not strongly modulated. Finally, the timing of the saccadic reactions was analyzed. Again, reference values for saccade latencies were collected in ocular experiments where the periodic tapping was omitted. The mean values obtained in the ocular ST and the oculo-manual DT experiments were 229.9 ms (±60.6 ms SD) and 258 ms (±74.5 ms SD), respectively. Saccade latencies in both experiments were independent of the task conditions, and no systematic interaction was identified across subjects. 201 8 Discussion 8.1 ST condition 8.1.1 Timing task Our results in isometric tapping replicated and confirmed the bimanual advantage which was presented for each hand (Fig. 7-1 A,B). Our results did not show the improvement of timing during bimanual tapping without elimination of asynchrony by means of a firm mechanical coupling of the index fingers of both hands as in Drewing’s experiments (Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002) in normal tapping (Fig. 7.1 A,B). This bimanual advantage was often absent in contact-free tapping or even the disadvantage was found in normal tapping (Fig. 7-1 A,B). The bimanual performance did not benefit from the asynchronous reafferences as compared to using only sensory reafferences of one hand. These reafferences are enhanced by tactile-kinaesthetic feedback. The benefit of reafferences and the integration of central commands in timing control would improve the timekeeper. In our experiments, participants tapped on the hard surface of the force sensor case and in Drewing’s experiment on a moveable spring-controlled key. The moving swing of the finger in our experiment might yield more freedom for movement development than in Drewing’s experiments, and, hence, it contained larger variance resource of mechanical implementation and larger asynchronous error. The results suggest that the conscious decision processes of temporal judgment involved two correction processes based on sensory reafferences and asynchronous errors. It is possible to speculate that the first correction process is the source for the bimanual advantage, whereas the second correction process as a function of asynchrony size is the source for bimanual disadvantage and has a contribution to the correlation function between successive ITIs. The smaller the timer variance, the larger the variance of motor delay will bias the correlation lag 1 between successive ITIs to -0.5 (Wing & Kristofferson (1973b)). The first correction process would reduce timer variance but mechanical variance would increase peripheral motor variance. Without mechanical implementations (i.e. without peripheral motor variance) the trend to the zero correlation has to be present. These suggestions were confirmed in isometric tapping compared to normal and contactfree tapping (Fig. 7-1 A, B). The most benefit of reafferences for the timekeeper would be in the case of voice tapping (Fig. 7-1C) although breath keeping is needed for the motor process and peripheral motor delay might be the largest one. Taken together, the bias of correlation lag 1 to -0.5 had to be present in voice, normal and contact-free tapping. It is not sure to suggest that the peripheral motor variance in isometric tapping is absent because the EMG showed large amplitude of antagonist. The bimanual advantage again was found not only in multiple effectors but also in mental tapping in combination with normal tapping (Fig. 7-1 D). If the memory capacity of 20 taps (Yamada 1996) and temporal memory for interval duration (Steven et al. 1988) are used for timing task in ST then the clock-counter mechanism according to which successive intervals are random is rejected. The slower trend and positive correlation of successive intervals in mental grouping condition confirmed this contradiction. 8.1.2 Timing task and spontaneous blinking The results of eye blink behavior during distinct rhythmic finger tapping tasks (standard, strong, and impulse-like tapping) compared to the reference data without tapping revealed that spontaneous eye blink (SB) behavior is affected by self-paced finger tapping (Fig. 7-5), whereas the tapping behavior seems to be unaffected by the eye blinks (Fig. 7-4). It is possible to suggest that the blink behavior reflects not only psychological and perceptual factors (such as attention, stress, 202 fatigue, etc.) but also concurrently active simple motor processes. Theoretically, Poisson distribution of IBI histograms of SB observed over longer periods would leads to uniform distributions in phase histograms as shown in Fig. 7-5 (column 1). However, for the overwhelming majority of tapping data, the 2I-Test rejected the null hypothesis (uniform distribution over phase) (Fig. 7-5 (column 2, 3, 4)). Hence blinking in these conditions cannot be considered as being purely spontaneous but is rather dependent on the tapping process, too. The relationship between blink behavior and other centrally controlled monotonous motor actions like gait and respiration has not been so far systematically investigated (e.g. Wilson, Fullenkamp, & Davis 1994). Speech increased blink rate during verbal tasks (tasks (Schuri & von Cramon 1981; Von Cramon & Schuri 1980). This report is in line with the results of the present study showing a 30 percent average increase of blink rate during tapping compared to the reference experiment without performing a cyclic motor task. However, cognitive functions for word selection and motor control for pronunciation are required. Representation of time for speech generation might be derived from an endogenous timing process or pacemaker linked to some type of counting device (Ivry & Richardson 2002). Von Cramon & Schuri (1980) reported that counting loudly up to 100 increased blinking significantly, whereas reciting the alphabet had no significant effects on blinking. A coupling or decoupling of the time-structured simple motor tasks like in this study from blinking, depending on the task requirements, can be suggested. Additional to logistically demand for synchronous activation of both hands (Fig. 7-7), the strong and the impulse-like bimanual tapping physically require higher force and rate of movement (Fig. 7-5 (column 3,4)). Thus, stronger triggering of motor commands as well as higher attentive load are expected and, indeed, resulted in a stronger coupling (phase synchronization caused by phase entrainment) than uninstructed unimanual tapping (Fig. 7-5 (column 2)). Supporting the aforementioned, evidence was found that instructed force production requires cognitive resources (Zijdewind et al. 2006) as well. Further framework such as the existence of a central bottleneck, resonant properties of eyelid motor system and its widely distributed brain network can create appropriate conditions for the hand motor system to synchronize the onset of its motor events with onsets of concurrent spontaneous movements (blinks). On the one hand, the suggestion is that motor commands for bimanual tapping coming from two hemispheres are integrated for control of the coordinated behavior (Ivry & Richardson 2002), and the eyelid movements are centrally originated and controlled, but influenced via certain “secondary paths” (Ponder & Kennedy 1927). On the other hand, the neural pathways responsible for tapping may cross-talk to these “secondary paths” leading to entrainment of blinks. A major role in these processes can be assigned also to dopaminergic regulation of motor actions (e.g. Dreisbach et al. 2005). Thus, the internal representations of motor commands indicate their "strength" not only through changes of the blink rate but also through specific timing. The entraining effect of strong tapping and impulse-like tapping on blinking due to probably more pronounced motor commands is equally obvious as its increased form observed in bimanual tapping (compared to unimanual tapping) (Fig. 7-7). 203 8.2 DT condition 8.2.1 BM In this study, the introduction of a discrete movement might be considered as an external perturbation on the ongoing rhythmical movement. On the other hand, the ongoing movement affected some characteristics of the evolving discrete movement. Thereby, the interaction of rhythmical and discrete movements may occur at the central control and/or peripheral levels. We determine the synchronization reflecting the stable phase locking and the unstable one; the specific effect of discrete movement on periodic one was directed to the underlying control mechanism leading to the typical PRCs (Fig. 7-8). The mutual interaction was reflected in continuous trajectories (6.5.2, Fig. 7-11, Fig. 7-12). We evaluated not only the data of the ground contact times, the force and trajectory developing (isometric tapping, foot tapping, finger movement) process, but also the physiological parameters realized by different experimental conditions (7.2.4). Synchrony is the most familiar mode of organization for coupled oscillators in secular interactions (Blasius & Stone 2000, Bonabeau, Theraulaz, & Deneubourg.1998, Delgado & Sole 2000, Glass 2001 ...). Transient phase shift subjected to impulsive force is synchrony in episode interactions. The behaviour of communities of oscillators depends on the strength of coupling among them (Strogatz & Stewart 1993). If their interactions are weak, the oscillators will be unable to reach synchrony, whereas if the interactions are strong, the oscillators will overcome their individual differences. Transient phase shift is the episode effect leading to synchrony in secular trend during interaction. The data showed in fact no perfect dissociation between trajectories. With a view on continuous movement trajectories, the coordination constraints are reflected in the fact that the position, velocity, and acceleration of both fingers are to some extent interdependent even if timing is stable (Fig. 6-19b, 6-21a, 6-25b). The time course of one finger affected the time course of the other one. These restrictions indicate limitations in central processing resources or occurrence of neural crosstalk between sensory and motor structures controlling each finger i.e. control signals producing different movement patterns interact at a central level and modify each other. Basically, the obtained experimental results in normal tapping are consistent with the results of previous investigations on the coordination of discrete movements and periodic movements (e.g., Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979; Yoshino et al. 2002). Since the basic experimental paradigm was cloned to that of the study of Yoshino et al. (2002), not surprisingly ‘Periodic Tap Retardation’ (PTR) behaviour (Type 0) (Fig. 7-8b) and ‘Marginal Tapping Interaction’ (MTI) behaviour (Type 1) (Fig. 7-8a) like in this target study could be observed. Type 0 Phase Reset during the second half of the periodic tap interval can be explained by the stronger coupling factor between two signals of the same direction (exhibitory coupling) and Type 1 Phase Reset within the first half by weaker coupling factor between two signals of the opposite direction (inhibitory coupling) and a correction process based on discrete feedback. 8.2.1.1 Effect of discrete movement on periodic movement Heuer & Klein (2005) discussed a coupling between concurrent movements generally as cross talk mechanism which appears if the movement (i.e. motor activity) is already "on the way" when the other movement will be launched. Correspondingly, the ongoing periodic tapping movement which is continuously "on the way" is subject to be affected by the upcoming discrete tap most probably. The suppression in Tap Delay (Fig. 6-20a, 6-25a) and the replacement in Tap Cancelling (Fig. 6-25b) of rhythmical activity by the discrete movement suggest that the oscillation probably inclined and declined respectively owing to the elastic and damping properties of muscles and inertial mechanics. 204 The frequency of oscillation was not only usually greater after than before the discrete movement (Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1994) in single limb condition but also in interlimb coordination as in our study. Levin et al. (1992) and Feldman (1993) explained in terms of the strong coactivation command accompanying fast discrete movements, but it might be again the episode effect leading to synchronization just as the hypothesis of the existence of a transitional frequency suggested for interlimb coordination during rhythmical movements (Kelso, Southard, & Goodman 1979). Figure 8.1: Limit-cycle model. F: external force. Ft: tangential component of F; Fr: centripetal component of F 8.2.1.1.1 Stable and unstable states of equilibrium In the vicinity of the stable states of equilibrium (phase range (0-0.3, 0.75-1), remember the cyclic repetition!), the state point on the limit-cycle (Fig. 8-1) remained in the vicinity of a stable state (attractor) after undergoing the disturbance duration (Ft ~ 0, Fr ~ F) because the effective force (Ft) is null or ignorable. In the vicinity of the unstable states (repeller) of equilibrium (0.3-0.6), the state point was repelled away (Ft ~ F, Fr ~ 0) forwards or backwards to the stable states because the effective force is maximal. The limit cycle interpretation afforded by the present experimental findings can be used to address the critical phase problem in mechanistic terms, further resolving the PRC similarity puzzle. The stable states of equilibrium form the attraction regions and the unstable ones the repelling region. The various mixtures of locking were reflected in trajectory coordination, where the rate of change is zero because the current state is located at stable fixed point. The attraction reflected in non-uniformly distribution of the overlapping of the (down-) upslope as well as of the cross-like period slope when the homogeneous and the non-homogeneous muscles are activated together, the occasional insertion of the fast discrete in the slow periodic finger’s path and then moving up together. The tap duration of one finger inclined to equalize the other one, the two affected periodic taps were contracted in two-sided cross-like coordination, the well-formed distribution of the discrete tap duration by intertwining in two periodic taps. The constraints of temporal relationship associated with attraction are presented under discrete events by the dense distribution or under continuous movement trajectories by the higher number of sided coordination (TS1, US1, SA1, TS2, DS2, SA2, and US2) around stable states. The repelling reflected in the rest phase range for instance in PTH and PTR and presented in compact distribution or low number of TA and AP2S coordination. PTH is described in position data more appropriate and reflects the stronger force of stimulus on the state point pushing it quickly on the way to attraction region (Figure 6.20b). This pushing force was intensified in multiple effectors and in strong condition through the absence of TC 205 in PTR, which usually were observed, i.e. the oscillation was set to death. The critical phase is not a property of the oscillator alone: it is a function as well of the mode of action of the perturbing pulse. The rate of advance or retardation through the cycle of the periodic movement is conditioned jointly by an external influence of discrete central command signal and the actual state (current phase). The ring device runs faster or slower, depending on its current phase as long as exposed to this external influence. The rate of change is not zero when the current state is located anywhere between two fixed points. The oscillator could be set to death in tap delay (TD) or was set to death (PTH). A cessation of oscillation would happen if the current state is directed away from the attraction stagnation point and towards the repelling stagnation point and a hastening if the current state is directed away from the repelling stagnation point and towards the attracting stagnation point. If the external influence is not large enough to cause a complete resetting a transient resetting leading to a mixture behaviour would be present. In normal tapping condition, the chance to affect the periodic tapping movement causing PTH is higher, particularly in strong and multiple effectors condition, than in isometric and contact-free tapping whereas in isometric and contact-free tapping the chance causing PTR is higher; tapping with movement follows the rules of a second order mechanical system comprising a memory due to inertia and feedback-based regulation approach, whereas a simple proportional mechanical system and lacked discrete feedback are reflected both in isometric tapping and in contact-free tapping. A specific definition of the concepts about the generation of control variables (CV) was used (Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1997; Asatryan & Feldman 1965; Feldman & Levin 1993; cf. Stein 1982; Berkinblit, Feldman, & Fukson 1986; Gottlieb, Corcos, & Agarwal 1989; Latash 1993). The control level issues CVs, whereas the peripheral level responds to the CVs as well as to other variables via proprioceptive feedback and mechanical interaction of the joint with the load. Thus, a pronounced resetting at representation level which did not reach the execution level (TC) was observed more often in contact-free and isometric tapping. Moreover the non-harmonic movement due to asymmetry between the flexion and extension phases suggests limitations on autonomous limit cycle oscillators as models of timed repetitive movements because they are inherently symmetric. 8.2.1.1.2 The dissipative mechanism and a source of energy of limit-cycle oscillators Central signals control the muscles in a predicted manner to compensate for interaction torques, motion about one joint leads to load arising at other joint (Gribble & Ostry 1999). To compensate for elastic loads on objects, movement induced inertial, and viscous subject adjust control signals to finger muscles in a predictive manner (Flanagan & Wing 1997) during rapid arm movements with hand-held loads in a study of grip force adjustments. Just as in the case of multi-joint motion, control signals must be coordinated appropriately to muscles to stabilize the periodic movement, when awaiting interaction with other mechanical components/systems. In the dissipative mechanism the damping and restoring force are used to pump up oscillation that becomes too small and to damp those that grow too large, respectively (Andronov, Vitt, & Khaikin 1996). The restoring and the damping forces are applied to bring the system back to the given state. Some subjects showed PRCs which start like MTI in a certain phase range up to 0.4 in normal condition. This would be explained by the restoring force generated with the support of the discrete events of periodic finger (reference tap) used as a resuming point for timing of the affected period. Without this support, different degrees of resetting were observed just as in contact-free and in isometric condition. In contact-free and isometric conditions, time judgment could not be well performed because the discrete event of the reference tap was absent or was blurred by constraints 206 of biomechanical movement. The restoring force also was exhibited in repelling and in attraction region. The trajectory deflecting in TC reflected the restoring force because the downwards movement indicates the oscillation become too small and the subsequent upwards movement is against gravitation. The amplitudes of the periodic movement when it moved together with the discrete one were smaller in contact-free condition whereas larger force in normal and isometric tapping. This would indicate that the damping force is needed to avoid the large growing of the oscillation which was observed when it is limited by the hard surface because the amplitude of downward movement in normal tapping is not needed to be controlled but in contact-free and isometric tapping. 8.2.1.2 Effect of periodic movement on discrete movement The ongoing high-frequency oscillations constrained the time of initiation of the evolving motor task (Adamovich, Levin, & Feldman 1997). Goodman & Kelso (1983) reported that the probability of the initiation of a discrete elbow movement depended on the phase of physiological tremor in normal subjects. This constraint emerged even when the both motor tasks are performed by different limbs. A novel aspect in tapping is the phase entrainment (Staude, Dengler, & Wolf 2002) effect (DTE). The DTE behaviour belongs to the most preferred “intrinsic” coordination facilitating stable cooperation. Obviously, DTE is a directed effect of the periodic on the discrete process which introduces a delay in the discrete tap reaction and thus an increased RT. DTE and its combination with PTH were not yet discussed in tapping studies. It becomes not directly apparent in PRCs since the onset time of the discrete tap is coded in the inclined dashed line but the corresponding RT (betraying DTE) is hidden; the increased density of data points in the range of small phase values (Ф < 0.3) and the more shortening of the immediately preceding interval with corresponding higher reaction time indicate the entrainment. It is most obvious from Figure 7.28d revealed the onset phase of the discrete tap not within the affected cycle of the periodic tapping. 8.2.1.3 Mutual interaction 8.2.1.3.1 Phase entrainment and coupled periodic processes Coupling of two processes can occur in one direction or in mutual interaction of processes by which one task predominantly affects the other. Tapping in-phase and anti-phase, respectively, with anti-phase being less stable and thus less frequent (Kelso 1984; Haken, Kelso, & Bunz, 1985) are the preferred tapping behaviours and represent synchronized actions, which seem to be easier for the system than arbitrary asynchrony. Speeding up the periodic tap (PTH) and delaying the discrete tap (DTE) are the combined strategy to establish the in-phase and anti-phase coordination. The detection of both, PTH and DTE tapping behaviour, is confined to cases where the actual scheduled phase relationship of both taps is not compatible, and, hence, they cannot be detected by principle in cases where phases match occasionally. Further, the mutual interaction was presented in mixed forms of DTE combined with PTH and PTR, respectively; i.e. at the same time when the launching of the discrete tap is delayed, the periodic tap process is advanced or it is paused in order to achieve a synchronized timing. This may explain why none of the subjects tested showed pure dominant DTE behaviour (Table 7.4). The hidden mutual interaction may be reflected in subjects classified as dominant MTI who consistently showed prolonged dual-task RT compared to PTR and PTH (Figure 7.14, which may reflect such situations in these subjects. By the way, the analysis of continuous trajectories revealed the directed effect of the periodic on the discrete process in the modification of their slope periods and tap durations. Some small mirror activity was observed in the slow rhythm 207 hand during the pauses when subjects had to produce a bimanual tapping rhythm with a 1:2 frequency ratio, when the other hand performed the additional tap to obtain the double tapping rate (Semjen & Summers 2002). All these reports point to the fact that entrainment of two motor task represents a basic mechanism in motor coordination, independent from the temporal characteristic (periodic, discrete) of the motor task. Thus, future research will concern the development of a model connecting the “phase entrainment” scenery with the “coupled oscillator” and “tapping” sceneries. 8.2.1.4 Coordination strategies Another issue is whether the kind of coordination is selected at random, or is there a principle? Although every individual shows a dominant tapping behaviour, all interaction patterns can be observed. Obviously, the unspecific instruction ‘to accurately execute the periodic tapping as well as to react as quickly as possible to the go signal’ contains a leaky element, as the paradigm of this study follows that of several other tapping studies (e.g. Yoshino et al. 2002). The ambiguous instruction forced the subject to give preference to one of the two competing motor tasks in case they cannot be served at the same time by the sensorimotor system. But it should be emphasized that even if this "instruction" argument sounds reasonable, it represents a hypothesis and is verified by the instruction specific data In the case of unspecific instruction, all subjects were not able to execute the two concurrent tasks totally independently and somehow managed the competing tasks by using either MTI, PTR, PTH, DTE or mixed forms of strategies (MTS). The assumption that the motor control system can usually handle only one task at a time and it will give dominance to one of the two tasks in the dual-task condition (Greenwald 1972; Klapp 1979) is considered. This assumption is verified by change of the conventional management of the competing tasks, i.e. it could be overcharged by physiological parameters. Temprado et al. (2002) and Temprado et al. (1999) reported that the subjects are not able to execute dual-task conditions with shared attention. The motor control system takes a decision for managing spontaneous coordination in favour of one of the tasks. Moreover, Ivry & Richardson (2002) suggested a multiple timer model assuming that timed bimanual movements are controlled by separate but mutually coupled timers. Future experimental work will clarify whether the hastening (PTH) and retardation (PTR) effects can be explained by those intervalbased gating and resetting processes as suggested by the multiple timer model (Ivry & Richardson 2002), or whether they are better modeled by a system of continuously coupled clocks as described in the dynamical systems literature (Winfree 1980; Schoener 2002, Ariaratnam & Strogatz 2001). 8.2.1.5 Information processing theory versus dynamic system theory In the information processing perspective, the notion of time keeping is inherently connected with abstract mental representations. Schaal et al. (2004), using fMRI, reported contralateral activity in several non-primary motor areas and in the cerebellum during discrete wrist movements that was absent during their rhythmic counterparts. Timing as a property originated from a disturbed neural network (Rao et al. 1997; Jantzen et al. 2007). The neural basis underlying timing still need to be elucidated. The perceptual centre of a perceptual or motor event seem to be used as the reference point for synchronization. Beside the factors such as stimulus length or stimulus intensity in the perceptual events the intensity of somatosensory feedback of the motor events also has an effect on this perceptual centre. Rhythmic timing reflecting stochastic and dynamical properties in terms of discrete timing were considered in Information processing perspective. The view of timing and coordination in dynamical system approach is the properties arising from (self-organized) pattern formation processes. Winfree (1967) proposed that self-entraining communities of this sort possibly 208 exist within individual metazoan animals and plants. On this basis of the diurnal coordination of their physiological process was observed. The dynamical system approach concerns the special phenomena arising from the smooth rhythmical interaction of whole populations of periodic processes or the episode interaction between a discrete and a periodic process . There may also be a level of ‘‘strategic’’ components for timing. Stable periodic timing but with interdependence such as TC with acceleration and non retardation, discrete tap entrainment could indicate that the closedloop control strategy allowing a certain amount of "sloppiness" in timing control may have evolved to take account of the inherent time delays of feedback loops. A full stochastic-dynamical system accounting for a complete spatiotemporal pattern will encompass the one accounting for a discrete timing. 8.2.2 OM-SM condition Dual-task costs can simply emerge from coordination of the two ongoing tasks even if they share neither perceptual nor motor resources. Further overlapping neural processing resources for the control of ballistic eye and hand movements are known. Hence, we expected strong DT interference effects in our OM-DT paradigm in a similar way as it was reported for BM-DT. However, with the same experimental concept, the results disproved this assumption. In comparison to the BM-DT, the DT costs of tapping observed in the OM-DT experiment turned out to be really weak, if existing at all – in general, a simple goal-directed saccade to a fixed target did not disturbed significantly the finger tapping (e.g., Fig. 7-37A) whereas the BM-DT experiment replicated the already known remarkably strong interference (exemplified in Fig. 7-37B). The weak interference in OM-DTis not in contradiction to the vast number of studies showing eye-hand-movement interference but rather confirms the response-selection bottleneck model. For the internal timekeeper controlling the periodic tapping a cognitive stage is not required. According to Dux et al. (2006) the posterior lateral prefrontal cortex and, possibly, the superior medial frontal cortex are the suitable candidates for an amodal bottleneck of information processing but they seem not to participate in a tapping task (Rao et al. 1997). Our results also support the hypothesis of Wei, Wertman, & Sternad (2003) that the discrete and rhythmic actions belong to two different control regimes. 8.2.2.1 Neural substrates of eye-hand movements - related studies In continuous tapping, active involvement of the bilateral supplementary motor areas, basal ganglia (Rao et al. 1997; Lewis et al. 2004) and cerebellum (Dreher & Grafman 2002; Ivry & Spencer 2004) was found as well as activity in the left putamen, the left ventrolateral thalamus, and the caudal supplementary motor area (Rao et al. 1997; Jenkins et al. 2000). Further, a distributed network of areas is involved in many studies concerning saccades, e.g. the reticular formation, superior colliculus, frontal eye fields, posterior parietal cortex, cerebellum, premotor cortex and basal ganglia (e.g., reviewed in Girard & Bertolz 2005). Gaze direction signals consistently modified skeletomotor processing in the cerebral cortex (Baker et al. 1999). In monkeys, gaze interactions occur in several arm movement related areas (e.g., Boussaoud et al. 1998) in addition to the visual cortical areas. In humans, Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes (1999) measured neural pattern of finger tapping related activation in conditions where participants (with the head fixed) maintained gaze either on the tapping hand or away from it; brain images were taken while participants performed sequential finger movements of the right hand (each of the finger tips was successively touched by the thumb). In comparison to the condition when gaze was directed away from the tapping hand the more aligning of gaze with the tapping hand increased activation in a wide extent of the lateral 209 superior and inferior parietal lobules of the hemisphere contralateral to the moving hand, in the primary motor cortex and in lateral and medial premotor cortices. Gaze direction as a salient variable for the neural systems controlling hand motor actions likely modify activation patterns dynamically and instantaneously (Baker, Donoghue, & Sanes 1999). These results provide a solid ground for eyehand interaction. From this point of view, a strong influence of the saccades on tapping should be expected but was not found in the OM-DT condition of our study. The nearly non-interaction as observed in our OM-DT situation indicates either that the same brain resources are not required by both tasks or the load is too small for bottleneck effects to become apparent. Schaal et al. (2004) reported that rhythmic movements activated a small number of unilateral primary motor areas whereas discrete movements activated additional contralateral non-primary motor areas showing strong bilateral cerebral and cerebellar activity. These results mirror findings of Lewis & Miall (2003) on task timing related brain activity and support our results. Thus, the discrete task characterized by the need for cognitive control recruits prefrontal and parietal areas, while the rhythmic task accomplished via autonomous control is mostly based on primary motor circuits not demanding the frontal areas: e.g., the dorsolateral prefrontal areas associated with “higher-level” cognitive functions (Rao et al. 1997) were not active during tapping. Such a concept can explain the non-significant OM-DT costs in our experiments. 8.2.2.2 Behavioural observations in combined eye-hand movement studies Indications of OM-DT interferences but in discrete-discrete DT situations (e.g., Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman 1993; Baedeker & Wolf 1987; Bekkering et al. 1994) were reported in a large amount of psychophysical studies on combined eye-hand movements. E.g., in studies concerning visually guided pointing, two tasks were combined by the shared target, which may elicit task grouping (preprogramming). Saccadic DT studies should employ a concurrent “task that is logically independent of the eye movement” (Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman 1993) to avoid task grouping. On the other hand, Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman (1993) explained the results that certain types of saccades (e.g., to a colored spot) do not show marked DT interference with the duality of the saccade control system (involving the superior colliculus and the frontal eye fields). They assumed that, contrary to the frontal eye field signals, collicular signals are not affected in the sensorimotor DT task. So far, fewer studies have been devoted to discrete-rhythmic DT behavior involving eye movements. Rhythmic step-jumping saccades in DT with a go/no-go type competing manual task exhibited some interference in studies of Malmstrom, Reed, & Weber (1983), but the degree is difficult to judge as saccadic latencies were not analyzed. Claeys et al. (1999) as well Stuyven et al. (2000) explored perceptual and memory processes related to antisaccades and prosaccades in terms of a different load imposed on the central executive. They combined random finger tapping as a secondary task and saccade execution in a DT paradigm. As a result, prosaccades as well as antisaccades showed some small general latency increase due to the secondary (tapping) task - a tendency also observed in our study, especially in non-experienced participants. Unfortunately, these reports did not include an analysis of the tapping behavior, thus no direct comparison with our findings is possible. The interference of eye and hand movements has to be presented in either the concurrent timing or the shared spatial target information, i.e. the resulting trajectories will reflect the interference. Sailer et al. (2000) used different eye movement tasks in combined eye and hand movement experiments to study temporal aspects of motor coordination. They concluded that i) both motor systems rely on common information to initiate movement, and ii) temporal coupling is stronger for 210 intentional tasks than for reflexive tasks. These findings relate to our study in two ways: First, an important point to be emphasized is that, in our study, the OM tasks were planned to be as “temporal” as possible realized by the focus was on concurrent operation of two very different effectors engaged in two independent tasks without common spatial variables and with fixed points for the saccade. Second, an exogenously triggered reflexive OM task together with an endogenously timed repetitive manual task reduces probability of temporal coupling between tasks in OM-DT according to Sailer et al. (2000). In the real-life conditions, there are similar situations of undisturbed eye-hand performance in which the eyes monitor the visual scenery while the hand is involved in a pure temporal task. A pianist successfully performs rhythmical strokes on the keyboard while reading the notes using visually controlled scanning saccades, and further examples can found in aviation, driving, etc. Our “weak/no-interaction”-finding fits well to the results of Sailer et al. (2000) and emphasizes their statement that “eye-hand coordination differs with the task employed” and that “dual-task interference is content-dependent” (Hazeltine, Ruthruff, & Remington 2006). 8.2.3 Multiple effectors The DT condition for hand-foot coordination comprised the discrete responses of the upper and lower limbs to a single acoustic go signal in parallel to a periodic right index finger tapping; seven discrete response types were analyzed: single (experiments LH, LF, and RF), double (experiments LHLF, LH-RF, LF-RF), and triple responses (experiment LH-LF-RF). The interaction and coordination patterns reported for bimanual DT conditions (Yoshino et al. 2002) are also observed in case of the foot responses. The interaction patterns also contralaterally or ipsilaterally occur in these heterogeneous discrete hand-foot couples. From the essential result showing up in the PRCs, it is possible to suggest that a unique motor control for the upper and lower limbs exists in the neural mechanisms located within those higher level brain areas. It may refer to motor coordination strategies that motor control in these DT conditions is not effector specific but rather task-based, the system decides according to demands of the pending tasks independent of effectors involved in the execution of these tasks, which is supported by the results of the ANOVA on RTs. The distinct neural mechanisms which are responsible for discrete-rhythmic DT coordination involving distinct effectors are not yet quite clear although DT interaction devoted to discretediscrete task tandems attracted huge attention (Greenwald 1972; Klapp 1979; Staude, Dengler, & Wolf 2002; Sternad, Dean, & Schaal 2000; Swinnen et al. 1997) and periodic-discrete task tandems are also gaining special attention (Swinnen & Wenderoth 2004; Sharikadze et al. 2009; Wachter et al. 2004; Yoshino et al. 2002). The two different classes of movement — rhythmic and discrete movement — executed in parallel or in sequence are essential aspects of multi-tasking which requires a high degree of motor coordination between effectors. Studies using fMRI showed that different cortical areas are involved in the rhythmic and discrete movement control (Schaal & Sternad 2001; Schaal et al. 2004). Also, psychophysical observations led to the conclusion that, in bimanual task, rhythmic and discrete movements are two different classes of behaviors, and their coupling occurs at a higher level of the central nervous system (Ronsse, Sternad D, & Lefevre 2009; Sternad, Dean, & Schaal 2000; Wei, Wertman, & Sternad 2003). The supplementary motor area (SMA) plays an equally significant role in preparation of single and repetitive voluntary movements (Shibasaki et al. 1993). SMA is involved in the organization and control of coordination between the homologous as well as the non-homologous limb segments (Debaere et al. 2003). A similar assumption can be drawn from the direct interaction of hand and foot movements in our DT tapping according to the known neural structure outlined in the introduction. 211 The coordination of the upper and lower limbs is required during typical rhythmic motor tasks such as walking, running, or swimming. How are different limb movements coordinated, and how do they interact with each other? The arm musculature still rhythmically contracts even when the upper limbs are restrained (Ballesteros, Buchtal, & Rosenfalck, 1965). On the other side muscular activity is established in the movement of the arms during walking and it is not simple passive pendulum movement (Jackson 1983; Jackson, Joseph, & Wyard, 1983a, b). Keele et al. (1985) and Keele, Ivry, & Pokorny (1987) showed in tapping experiments that different effector systems could share the same timing mechanisms for rhythmic movement generation. In their studies, participants who were consistent in finger tapping were more consistent in foot tapping, too. Although during gait and other natural activities the existence of flexible, task-dependent neuronal coupling between the leg and arm movements was suggested based on much evidence the specific interaction between legs and arms remains unclear (Dietz, Fouad, & Bastiaanse, 2001; Wannier et al. 2001). We found that the RT for left hand about 25 ms shorter than RTs for either foot, on average (Figure 7.33). This can be explained by the longer length of the reflex pathways of the foot than of the hand. Propagation delay of neural impulses towards the hand muscles is significantly shorter than towards the foot muscles and accordingly, RT for hand responses should be smaller in comparison to foot responses. Single response correlation analysis between limbs (Table 7, Figure 7.34) shows that all limbs are timed similarly, even in the DT condition, where the timing of discrete response command is subject of influence of the DT interaction. Further, ANOVA on RT has shown no effects between effector and task condition. These two results may indicate that the DT interaction occurs at a central level and the command for the discrete response is effector-unspecific. In addition, the subsequent response selection for the required hand and foot action is driven from symmetric pathways. This is in good agreement with the assumptions that there is at least one central pattern generator (CPG) for each limb, that these CPGs are located in the spinal cord level, and that strengths of coupling between the CPGs of homologous limbs (of hands or of feet) are larger than between the CPGs of non-homologous limbs (hand-foot) (Zehr & Duysens 2004). The individual differences in task performance would explain the significant interaction effects for the pairs of participant and limb, and of participant and task. Although each participant performed the task and set up the sequence structure of acting limbs in simultaneous responses in ‘own way’, all of these behaviors still fit to a global conventional DT behavior. 212 9 Summary and outlook 9.1 Summary The improvement of the classical setup which now includes force and position recording as a substitution of the previous simple ground contact recording is reasonable and discloses some internal processes like the phase locking phenomenon and timing control reflected by interactive changes of trajectories. High velocity downward movements to obtain accuracy in synchronization (Balasubramaniam, Wing, & Daffertshofer (2004)) are reflected in all tapping experiments (normal, contact-free, and isometric). Not only one movement constrains or even impedes the execution of the other in dual-task executed by a single limb but also when execution is distributed on two different limbs like two fingers. The abstraction of the continuous position signal by an event time sequence showed a stable global timing of periodic tapping but the continuous signal revealed the mutual dependence in details. Including of partial taps and transient event-related changes in the signal profile of single taps do not simplify the modelling aspects but can lead to a more profound insight of motor timing mechanisms. The new techniques with highly accurate position recording by laser equipment will now allow to simply extend the experimental program to big proximal muscles like biceps and also to other kinds of tapping like tooth tapping (Nagasawa et al. 1993) in order to again assess the classical problem: to which level of the neural information processing hierarchy these timing processes of motor control can be assigned. The importance role of sensory feedback in timing control was demonstrated by several authors (Repp 2000, 2006; Aschersleben & Prinz 1995, 1997; Aschersleben, Gehrke, & Prinz 2001), The bimanual advantage (Helmut & Ivry 1996; Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002; Drewing & Aschersleben 2003; Drewing et al. 2004) profits from the integration of different central control signals and the amount of sensory information (Drewing et al. 2004; Drewing, Hennings, & Aschersleben 2002; Ivry & Keller 1989; Ivry & Richardson 2002; Aschersleben & Prinz 1995). This advantage was replicated in isometric tapping. The assumption of a common timing system that might be violated (Vorberg & Hambuch 1984) when a performer tries to compensate for the asynchrony between the hands by triggering the early hand only after some delay. The bimanual advantage was suffered in normal and contact-free tapping by this correction process. Not only has the integration of different central control signals that are related to each effector but also additional mental tapping contributed to the bimanual advantage. The smaller variance of motor delay which will bias the correlation lag 1 between successive ITIs to 0 (Wing & Kristofferson (1973b)) was confirmed in isometric tapping in comparison with normal tapping and contact-free tapping. The dominance of motor variance over the reafferent sensory feedback in voice tapping resulted in clearly biasing of the correlation lag 1 to -0.5. Successive intervals according to clock-counter mechanism are random. This hypothesis is rejected by the gradually slower speed and positive correlation of successive intervals in mental tapping. Spontaneous blinking as a concurrently active motor process is entrained by the finger tapping. Opposite effect was not clear, i.e. the tapping behavior seems to be unaffected by the eye blinks. Stronger tapping triggering stronger motor command as well as higher attentive loads and the integration of motor commands for bimanual tapping are expected and indeed resulted in a stronger entrainment. The understanding of the synchronization process allowed us to formulate a model based on the concept of limit-cycle such as the attracting and the repelling area and the interaction behaviours. Furthermore, to complete the formulation of the model, the physiological parameters can be determined by different experimental conditions. The resulting model is a differential equation with 213 two parameters specifying the intensity and the duration of disturbance and it is easily solved for the simplest case in which some simplifying hypotheses are assumed. An impulsive force with constant direction acts on a state point running with constant velocity on the limit cycle. Under these conditions the system exhibits the two singular states, one stable is the attraction area and the other unstable is the repelling one. The initial conditions would determine the achievement of one of these states. It is not easy to know exactly the initial conditions because we can only measure the observable events. Obviously, the in-phase synchronization is most probable and the preferred state by which the best performance can be obtained because it is stable. The model considerations of limit cycle synchronisation (Wei, Wertman, & Sternad 2003) can be extended by now including the trajectory information in timed repetitive movements in the context of weak and strong perturbations. This concept was mainly inspired by Winfree (1980) and based on simple discrete event observation (e.g., contact timing). The introduction of a discrete movement might be considered as a perturbation on the ongoing rhythmical movement. The particular aspects are the nonlinear shortening or lengthening of the periodic process due to the external discrete movement. Both shortening and lengthening of the periodic process running on the background subjected to a perturbation of discrete movement would operate on the acquisition of robust synchronization. Influence and sensitivity of physiological parameters are phase-dependent. The effects of discrete tap on periodic one carries the state point to a new point in phase space.The resetting of the state point to a new phase depends on the phase and magnitude of the perturbing event represented by the phase position and size of the vector in model. Weak inputs (small vector) perturb the system less and small Type 1 phase shifts will be observed. Strng inputs (large vector) carried the system across the unstable equilibrium point and very large Type 0 phase shifts will occur as a consequence. Based on the simple Winfree model and on the experimental observations of the impulsive effects of the discrete movement on the periodic one, principally a perturbation produces a phase delay when the both movements are on the opposite direction and produce a phase advance when both movements are in the same direction. The obtained experimental results in normal tapping are consistent with the results of previous investigations on the coordination of discrete movements and periodic movements (e.g., Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki 1979; Yoshino et al. 2002). Beside ‘Periodic Tap Retardation’ (PTR) behavior (Type 0) and ‘Marginal Tapping Interaction’ (MTI) behavior (Type 1), “Periodic Tap Hastening” (PTH) and “Discrete Tap Entrainment” (DTE) could be observed. The speed of periodic movement was usually greater after than before the discrete movement. Phase range (0.3-0.6) was very often the range in which the state point was repelled away. The attraction was found in synchronization of both fingers and also in the cross-like coordination. Based on the discrete events, this attraction was reflected in dense distribution of phase, and based on continuous trajectories this attraction was reflected by the overlapping period of down-upward movements as well as the equalization of both the slope durations and the tap durations. Furthermore, the embedding of discrete tap into the two surrounding periodic tap with well-formed distribution of their tap duration to obtain stable timing by the simplest related frequency ratio 2:1 also was found. The PRC indicates excitatory coupling presented in PTH or inhibitory coupling presented in PTR behaviour. PTR and PTH reflect the repelling effect pushing the state point to the stable state. This repelling effect was clearer when force or multi-effectors were required for discrete movement. The strength of phase-shifting inputs to the limit-cycle also is reduced when discrete events used as feedback in the closed-loop control for the restoring force. The trajectory deflecting in TC of PTR might reflect the restoring force, whereas the reduced periodic amplitude in contact-free condition by synchronization might reflect the damping force. More PTH but fewer PTR in normal tapping was 214 found in comparison with isometric and contact-free tapping. Tapping with movement comprises a memory due to inertia and feedback-based regulation approach. A simple proportional mechanical system without regulation based on discrete feedback approximately is reflected both in isometric tapping and in contact-free tapping. The pooled data even showed PTR in phase range up to 0.4, some subjects showed PRCs which start like MTI in this small phase range in normal tapping but then like PTH. This MTI can be explained by the support of the discrete events of reference tap used as a resuming point for timing of the affected period for the first case because this MTI was absent in contact-free and isometric tapping. For PTH, a more comfortable stronger force is the reason. TD+PTR might represent pronounced resetting at representation level which does not reach the execution level. On the other hand, the ongoing movement affected some characteristics of the discrete movement. The time course of one finger affected the time course of the other one. The discrete tap reaction was delayed (DTE) to obtain preferred coordination. The ambiguous instruction forced the subject to give preference to one of the two competing motor tasks and hence all interaction patterns and mix forms can be observed. Eye-hand coordination generally does not always cause DT costs. Sharing neural pathways might reduce redundancy and/or resources could be spared. In the modern high-end computers, only actually required resources are activated and those which can provoke interference are spared. One can speculate that the neural system uses similar scaling methods, too. The chance for strong interference during execution of two simple independent OM tasks will be low, as our results demonstrate for this specific condition if only more complicated tasks require more complex and cross-functional modules. It should be noted that this finding supplements but does not oppose the reports showing eye-hand-interactions in complex dual-tasking (e.g. Pashler, Carrier, & Hoffman 1993). The investigation of hand-foot combination imposingly showed the similar interaction pattern as in case of a periodic right index finger tapping and single discrete responses executed by the other hand. The specification of this DT behavior was even extended by demonstrating that this interaction pattern is basically independent of which upper or lower limb is selected for the discrete response. This fact holds also for our (musically trained) participant showing almost no interaction between the rhythmic activity and the discrete responses (MTI behavior): this behavior is not changed when using the foot instead of hand for the discrete response. However, these rare cases of participants (who were discussed by Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki (1980)) apparently demonstrate that the DT interaction mechanism can be outperformed by other factors like intensively trained motor sequences as discussed for musicians – thus the window for future research is still open. 9.2 Outlook 9.2.1 Effect of force, attention and external feedback There are reports about the influence of sensory reafferences on continuation tapping such as feedback tones for taps decreased intertap interval variability as compared to tapping without tones (Barratt et al. 1981; Kolers & Brewster 1985). Aschersleben et al. (2002) confirmed that the temporal control of repetitive movements is based on sensory information i.e. timing becomes more precise the more sensory information becomes available. In a series of finger-tapping tasks consisted of nine combinations of pace and force strong interactions between the two factors pace and force under high pace conditions were found although motor timing was independent of force control in controls of weak forces and slow pace (Inui et al. 1998). 215 The difference between the “reaction time” and the “self-paced” instruction is worthy to study with clear emphasize on one task and selection of subject behaviour (strong PTR and strong MTI) under further force and feedback variations. Force requirement on one hand Extern feedback (audio, visual) on periodic taps Only Contact-free periodic tapping 9.2.2 Mental task instead of motor task Information processing capacity is limited for any individual. If parallel execution of the two tasks require more than the total capacity their performance on either or both deteriorates because performing every task requires a given portion of capacity (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott 2000; Neumann 1984; Wickens 1989). Research for studying attention and posture control has used dualtask paradigms in which the primary task postural control and a secondary task were performed together. The extent of the declination of the performance on either task declined indicated the interference between the processes controlling the two task, and thus the extent to which the two tasks shared attentional resources (Kerr, Condon, & McDonald 1985). Yardley et al. (1999) studied the postural stability with competing demands for attentional resources by articulation.36 normal subjects were engaged for studying the postural sway. Subjects had to repeat a number aloud (articulation), count backwards multiples of seven aloud (articulation and attention), count backwards silently (attention), and neither articulation nor attention was require. A significant increase in sway was reported in articulation condition, whereas no effect of attention was observed. Demand for attentional resource such as silently counting has no effect in postural sway maybe because it presents a periodic process. It is interesting to study a mental response such as imagination of a discrete movement. 9.2.3 Checking memory limit of time interval The conclusion that judgements of temporal equivalence are based not only on synchrony of events with internal beats, but on a memory for interval durations (Keele et al. 1989) needs still further support. Yamada & Yonera (2001) estimated the temporal control mechanism of tapping with rhythmic patterns. Subjects who were at intermediate levels of musical performance made equalled interval tapping in several tempos (180, 370, 800ms/tap). The results of this study show that the memory is able to govern 20 taps to control of equalled interval tapping. The 20-taps-memory capacity, the judgements of temporal equivalence are based on a memory for prescribed interval duration that we learn during synchronization phase and the motor event of the reference tap we use as the reference point for temporal judgement are the factors leading to the question whether the 20-taps-memory capacity can still be maintained in this bimanual dual-task at larger periods such as 800ms, 1000ms, 1200ms. 9.2.4 Audiomotor overlearned in musical trained people A tight connection between action and perception of the sensory feedback in Musicians was suggested (Bangert et al., 2006). Even when the physical presence of the sensory feed back is experimentally suppressed a mental representation is still generated. It is interesting to check 216 whether the stronger cognitive representation of rhythm in musicians also has a strong entrainment on eyeblink. 217 References Adamovich SV, Levin MF, & Feldman AG (1994). Merging different motor patterns: Coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res, 99(2):325-337. (FA0041) Adamovich SV, Levin MF, & Feldman AG (1997). Central modifications of reflex parameters may underlie the fastest arm movements. J Neurophysiol, 77(3):1460-9. Alexander GE & Crutcher MD (1990). Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci, 13:266-271. Andronov AA, Vitt AA, & Khaikin SE (1996). Theory of Oscillators. Pergamon press, 3rd edition. Aramaki Y, Honda M, Okada T, & Sadato N (2006). Neural correlates of the spontaneous phase transition during bimanual coordination. Cereb cortex, 16:1338-1348. Ariaratnam JT & Strogatz SH (2001). Phase Diagram for the Winfree Model of Coupled Nonlinear Oscillators. Phys Rev Lett, 86(19): 4278-4281. Arthur H (1945). The origin and purposes of blinking. Br J Ophthalmol, 29(9):445-467. Asatryan DG & Feldman AG (1965). Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. 1. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the joint on execution of a postural task. Biophysics, 10:925-935. Aschersleben G & Prinz W (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(3):305-317. (FA0117) Aschersleben G & Prinz W (1997). Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. J Motor Behav, 29:35–46. (VA001) Aschersleben G, Gehrke J, & Prinz W (2001). Tapping with peripheral nerve block: A role for tactile feedback in the timing of movements. Exp Brain Res, 136:331-339. (FA0094) Aschersleben G (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain Cognition, 48:66-79. (FA0081) Aschersleben G, Stenneken P, Cole J, &Prinz W (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action:Attention and performance, XIX:227–244. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Aschersleben G, Gehrke J, & Prinz W (2004). A psychophysical approach to action timing. In C Kaernbach, E Schröger, & H Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition, 117-136. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 218 Baker JT, Donoghue JP, & Sanes JN (1999). Gaze direction and motor cortical activation. J Neurosci, 19:10044-10052. (FB0257) Baedeker C & Wolf W (1987). Influence of saccades on manual reactions - a reaction time and VEP study. Vision Res, 27:609-619. (FB0258) Bailey RA (1996). Orthogonal partitions for designed experiments. Designs, Codes and Cryptography, 8:45–77. Balasubramaniam R, Wing AM, & Daffertshofer A (2004). Keeping with the beat: movement trajectories contribute to movement timing. Exp Brain Res, 159(1):129-34. (VB0101) Baldissera FG, Borroni P, Cavallari P, & Cerri G. (2002). Excitability changes in human corticospinal projections to forearm muscles during voluntary movement of ipsilateral foot. The Journal of Physiology, 539:903-911. Bangert M, Jürgens U, Häusler U, & Altenmüller E (2006). Classical conditioned responses to absent tones. BMC Neuroscience, 7(1), 60. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-7-60. Barbato G, Ficca G, Muscettola G, Fichele M, Beatrice M, & Rinaldi F (2000). Diurnal variation in spontaneous eye-blink rate. Psychiatry Res, 93(2):145-151. Barnes R & Jones MR (2000). Expectancy, Attention, and Time. Cognitive Psychol, 41(3): 254–311. (NB0003) Barratt ES, Patton J, Olsson NG, & Zuker G (1981). Impulsivity and paced tapping. JMotor Behav, 13(4):286–300. Bauer LO, Strock BD, Goldstein R, Stern JA, & Walrath LC (1985). Auditory discrimination and the eyeblink. Psychophysiology, 22(6):636-41. Bedard P, Thangavel A, & Sanes JN (2008). Gaze influences finger movement-related and visualrelated activation across the human brain. Exp Brain Res, 188:63–75. (FB0259) Bedard P & Sanes JN (2009). Gaze and hand position effects on finger-movement-related human brain activation. J Neurophysiol, 101:834-842. Beek PJ, Peper CE, & Daffertshofer A (2000). Timekeepers versus nonlinear oscillators: how the approaches differ. In P. Desain & L. Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm Perception and Production (pp. 9–33). Beek PJ, Peper CE, & Daffertshofer A (2002). Modeling Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination: Beyond the Haken–Kelso–Bunz Model. Brain and Cognition, 48(1):149–165. (FB0216) Bekkering H, Adam JJ, Kingma H, Huson A., & Whiting HTA (1994). Reaction time latencies of eye and hand movements in single- and dual-task conditions. Exp Brain Res, 97:471–476. (FB0260) 219 Bentivoglio AR, Bressman SB. Cassetta E, Creta D, Tonalin P, & Albanese A (1997). Analysis of Blink Rate Patterns in Normal Subjects. Mov Disord, 12(6):1028-1034 Berkinblit MB, Feldman AG, & Fukson OI (1986). Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci, 9(4):585-638. Bernstein NA (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, Oxford. Binsted G & Elliott D (1999). Ocular perturbations and retinal/ extraretinal information: the coordination of saccadic and manual movements. Exp Brain Res, 127:193–206. (FB0262) Bizzi E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, & Giszter S (1991). Computations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspective. Science, 253(5017):287–291. Blasius B & Stone L (2000). Chaos and phase synchronization in ecological systems. Int J Bif, & Chaos, 10(10):2361–2380. Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G, & Deneubourg JL. (1998). The synchronization of recruitmentbased activities in ants. Biosystems, 45(3):195–211. Boussaoud D, Jouffrais C, & Bremmer F (1998). Eye position effects on the neuronal activity of dorsal premotor cortex in the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol, 80:1132–1150. (FB0261) Brass M, Bekkering H, Wohlschlaeger A, & Prinz W (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn, 44:124143. (CB0015) Burchard JE (1958). Resetting a biological clock. Ph.D. thesis, Princeton University, New Jersey. Bünning E (1970). Potato cellars, trains, and dreams: discovering the biological clock. In: Sweeney BM. Rhythmic phenomena in plants. San Diego, California: Academic, pp.10-12. Byblow WD, Coxon JP, Stinear CM, Fleming MK, Williams G, Müller JFM & Ziemann U (2007). Functional Connectivity between Secondary and Primary Motor Areas Underlying Hand–Foot Coordination. J Neurophysiol, 98: 414–422. Byrne MD & Anderson JR (2001). Serial modules in parallel: the psychological refractory period and perfect time-sharing. Psychol Rev, 108(4):847-848. (VB0014) Carson RG (2005). Neural pathways mediating bilateral interactions between the upper limbs. Brain Res Rev, 49(3):641-662. (FC0172) Chaves EL, Trautt GM, Brandon A, & Steyaert J (1983). Effects of test anxiety and sex of subject on neuropsychological test performance: Finger tapping, trail making, digit span, and digit symbol test. Percept Mot Skills, 56(3):923–929. 220 Chipman (2004). Nonlinearities in hand and foot coordination. Theses. Claeys K, Crevits L, Stuyven E, Van Der Goten K, Depuydt C,& Vandierendonck A (1999). Parallel visual and memory processes. Doc Ophthalmol, 95, 349–358. (NC0001) Collins M, Seeto R, Campbell L, & Ross M (1989). Blinking and corneal sensitivity. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh), 67(5):525-531. Crawford JD, Henriques DY, Medendorp WP, & Khan AZ (2003). Ocular kinematics and eye–hand coordination. Strabismus, 11:33–47. (PDF) DeCoursey PJ (1959). Daily activity rhythms in the flying squirrel, Glaucomys volans, Ph.D. thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison. DeCoursey PJ (1960). Daily light sensitivity rhythm in a rodent. Science, 131(3392):33-35. Debaere F, Wenderoth N, Sunaert S, Van Hecke P, & Swinnen SP (2003). Internal vs. external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback. NeuroImage, 19(3):764-776. De Jong R 1993. Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 19:965–980. Delgado J & Sole RV (2000). Self-synchronization and task fulfilment in ant colonies. J Theor Biol, 205(3):433–441. Delignières D, Lemoine L, & Torre K (2004). Time intervals production in tapping and oscillatory motion. Hum Mov Sci, 23(2):87-103. (FD0195) De Rugy A & Sternad D (2003). Interaction between discrete and rhythmic movements: reaction time and phase of discrete movement initiation during oscillatory movements. Brain Res, 994:160–174. (FD0196) Deutsch D (1983). The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel. Perception & Psychophysics, 34(4):331–337. (VD0021) Dietz V, Fouad K, & Bastiaanse CM (2001). Neuronal coordination of arm and leg movements during human locomotion. European Journal of Neuroscience, 14:1906-1914. Dodrill CB (1978). The hand dynamometer as a neuropsychological measure. J Consult Clin Psychol, 46(6):1432-1435. Doughty MJ (2001). In consideration of three types of spontaneous eyeblink activity in normal humans-during reading and video display terminal use, in primary gaze and while in conversation. Optom Vis Sci, 78(10):712-725. (FD0197) 221 Dreher JC & Grafman J (2002). The roles of the cerebellum and basal ganglia in timing and error prediction. Eur J Neurosci, 16:1609–1619. (ND0002) Dreisbach G, Müller J, Goschke T, Strobel A, Schulze K, Lesch KP, & Brocke B (2005). Dopamine and cognitive control: the influence of spontaneous eyeblink rate and dopamine gene polymorphisms on perseveration and distractibility. Behav Neurosci, 119(2):483-490. Drewing K, Hennings M, & Aschersleben G (2002). The contribution of tactile reafference to temporal regularity during bimanual finger tapping. Psychol Res, 66:60–70. (FD0155) Drewing K & Aschersleben G (2003). Reduced timing variability during bimanual coupling: A role for sensory information. J Exp Psychol, 56(2):329-50. (FD0154) Drewing K, Stenneken P, Cole J, Prinz W, & Aschersleben G (2004). Timing of bimanual movements and deafferentation: implications for the role of sensory movement effects. Exp Brain Res, 158(1):5057. (FD0199) Dudinski O, Finnin BC, & Reed BL (1983). Acceptability of thickened eye drops to human subjects. Curr Ther Res, 33(2):322-337. Dunlap K (1910). Reactions to rhythmic stimuli, with attempt to synchronize. Psycholl Rev, 17:399– 416. (VD0047) Dux PE, Ivanoff J, Asplund CL, & Marois R (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron, 52:1109–1120. (ND003) Megaw ED & Armstrong W (1973). Individual and simultaneous tracking of a step input by the horizontal saccadic eye movement and manual control systems. J Exp Psychol, 100:18-28. (FE0063) Elble RJ, Higgins C, & Hughes L (1994). Essential tremor entrains rapid voluntary movements. Exp Neurol, 126,138-143. (FE0019) Elliott D & Lee TD (1995). The role of information on manual aiming bias. Psychol Res, 58:2–9. Esteban A & Salinero E (1979). Reciprocal reflex activity in ocular muscles. Implications in spontaneous blinking and Bell’s phenomenon. Eur Neurol, 18(3):157-65. Esteban, A., Traba, A.,, & Prieto, J. (2004). Eyelid movements in health and disease. The supranuclear impairment of the palpebral motility. Neurophysiol Clin, 34(1):3-15. Evinger C, Shaw MD, Peck CK, Manning KA, & Baker R (1984). Blinking and associated eye movements in humans, guinea pigs, and rabbits. J Neurophysiol, 52(2):323-339. (FE0064) Evinger C, Manning, KA, & Sibony PA (1991). Eyelid movement mechanisms and normal data. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 32(2):387-400. 222 Evinger C (1995). A brainstem reflex in the blink of an eye. News Physiol Sci, 10(4):147-153. Evinger C, Bao JB., Powers AS, Kassem, IS, Schicatano, EJ, Henriquez, VM, & Peshori, K.R. (2002). Dry eye, blinking, and blepharospasm. Mov Disorders, 17(suppl 2):75–78. Feldman AG & Levin MF (1993). Control variables and related concepts in motor control. Concepts Neurosci, 4:25-51. Feldman AG (1993). The coactivation command for antagonist muscles involving Ib interneurons in mammalian motor control systems: an electrophysiologically testable model. Neurosci Lett, 155:167170. Ballesteros ML, Buchthal F, & Rosenfalck P (1965). The pattern of muscular activity during the arm swing of natural walking. Acta Physiol Scand, 63:296–310. Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, Pietrzyk U, & Passingham RE (1997). Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex. J Neurophysiol, 77:2164–2174. Fisk JD & Goodale MA (1985). The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space. Exp Brain Res, 60:159–178. (FF0155) Flanagan JR & Wing AM (1997). The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. J Neurosci, 17(4):1519–1528. Flashman LA, Flaum M, Gupta S, & Andreason NC (1996). Soft signs and neuropsychological performance in schizophrenia. Am Jl of Psychiatry, 153(4):526-532. Fortuna L, Frasca M, & Rizzo A (2003). Experimental pulse synchronisation of two chaotic circuits. Chaos, Solit, & Fract, 17(2-3):355–361. Fox PT, Fox JM, Raichle ME, & Burde RM (1985). The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary saccades, a positron emission tomographic study. J Neurophysiol, 54:348–369. (WF0050) Fraisse P (1980). Les synchronisations sensori-motrices aux rythmes (The sensorimotor synchronization of rhythms). In Anticipation et comportement (ed. Requin J), 233–257. Pris, France : Centre National. Franek M, Mates J, Radil T, Beck K, & Poeppel E (1991). Finger tapping in musicians and nonmusicians. Int J Psychophysiol, 11:277–279. Frederico F, Simone IL, Lucifero V, Defazio G, De Salvia R, Mezzapesa DM, Petruzzellis M, Tortorella C, & Livrea P (1998). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in primary blepharospasm. Neurology, 51(3): 892–895. 223 Freudenthaler N, Heuf H, Kadner G, & Schlote T (2003). Characteristic of spontaneous eyeblink activity during video display terminal use in healthy volunteers. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol, 241(11): 914-920. Friedman A, Polson MC, & Dafoe CG (1988). Dividing attention between the hands and the head: Performance trade-offs between rapid finger tapping and verbal memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hum Percept Perform, 14: 60–68. Fukuda K (2001). Eye blinks: new indices for the detection of deception. Int J Psychphysiol, 40(3): 239245. Gander PH & Lewis RD (1983). Phase-resetting action of light on the circadian activity rhythm of Rattus exulans. Am J Physiol, 245(1): R10-R17. Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, & Mangun GR (2002). Cognitive neuroscience: the biology of the mind. 2nd Edition. New York, NY: W.W. Norton and Company. Gazzola V & Keysers C (2009). The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject´Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data. Cereb Cortex, New York NY, 19(6):1239-1255. (FG0176) Geldmacher DS & Hills EC (1997). Effect of stimulus number, target-to distractor ratio, and motor speed on visual spatial search quality following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 11(1):59-66. Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E, Saetti MC, & Toni I (1996). Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia, 34(5):369–376. Gilden DL, Thornton T, & Mallon MW (1995). 1/f noise in human cognition. Science, 267(5205):18371839. (NG0002) Girard B & Berthoz A (2005). From brainstem to cortex: computational models of saccade generation circuitry. Prog Neurobiol, 77:215–251. (FG0177) Giszter SF, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, & Bizzi E (1993). Convergent force fields organized in the frog’s spinal cord. J Neurosci, 13(2):467–491. Glass L (2001). Synchronization and rhythmic processes in biology. Nature, 410:277–284. Goodman D & Kelso JAS (1983). Exploring the functional significance of physiological tremor: a biospectroscopic approach. Exp Brain Res, 49(3):419-31. (FG0043) Gottlieb GL, Corcos DM, & Agarwal GC (1989). Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom. Behav Brain Sci, 12:189 210. Gottliebe J (2007). From thought to action: the parietal cortex as a bridge between perception, action, and cognition. Neuron, 53(1):9-16. 224 Gourevitch V & Galanter E (1967). A significance test for one parameter isosensitivity functions. Psychometrika, 32(1):25-33. Greene PR (1986). Gaussian and Poisson blink statistics: a preliminary study. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 33(3):359-361. (WG0078) Greenfield MD & Roizen I (1993). Katydid synchronous chorusing is an evolutionarily stable outcome of female choice. Nature, 364(6438):618–620. Greenwald AG (1972). On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. J Exp Psychol, 94(1): 52–57. (VG0028) Greenwald AG (2003). On Doing Two Things at Once: III. Confirmation of Perfect Timesharing. When Simultaneous Tasks Are Ideomotor Compatible. J Exp Psychol, 29(5):859–868. (FG0178) Gribble PL & Ostry DJ (1999). Compensation for Interaction Torques During Single- and Multijoint Limb Movement. J Neurophysiol, 82(5):2310–2326. (FG0149) Guisset JL, Deneubourg JL, & Ramírez-Ávila GM (2002). The phase information associated to synchronized electronic fireflies. arXiv.nlin.AO/0206036. (PDF) Guitton D & Volle M (1987). Gaze control in humans: eye head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. J Neurophysiol, 58(3):427–459. (FG0179) Haken H, Kelso JAS, & Bunz H (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern, 51:347-356. (FH0033) Haken H (1987). Advanced Synergetics. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. Haken H, Peper CE, Beek PJ, & Daffertshofer A (1996). A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping. Physica D, 90(1-2):179–196. Hastings JW & Sweeney BM (1958). A persistent diurnal rhythm of luminescence in gonyaulax polyedra. Biol Bull, 115:440-458. Hart CB & Giszter SF (2004). Modular premotor drives and unit bursts as primitives for frog motor behaviors. J Neurosci, 24(22):5269–5282. (FH0202) Hazeltine E & Ruthruff E (2006). Modality pairing effects and the response selection bottleneck. Psychol Res, 70:504–513. Hazeltine E, Ruthruff E, & Remington RW (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognit Psychol, 52:291345. (FH0208) 225 Heaton RK, Nelson LM, Thompson DS, Burks JS, & Franklin GM (1985). Neuropsychological findings in relapsing-remitting and chronic progressive multiple sclerosis. J Consult Clin Psychol 53(1):103-110. Helmuth LL & Ivry RB (1996). When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 22(2):278–293. (FH0204) Herst AN, Epelboim J, & Steinman RM (2001). Temporal coordination of the human head and eye during a natural sequential tapping task. Vision Res, 41(25-26): 3307–3319. (FH0205) Heuer H & Klein W (2005). Intermanual interaction in discrete and periodic bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes. Exp Brain Res, 167:220–237. (FH0206) Hodges PW & Bui BH (1996). A comparison of computer-based methods for the determination of onset of muscle contraction using electromyography. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 101(6):511–9 Hofer H, Staude G, & Wolf W (2004). Change-point estimation by template matching – some basic aspects. Informatik, Biometrie and Epidemiologie in Med. u. Biol, 35. Hofer H, Staude G, & Wolf W (2005). Sequential segmentation of time series containing gradual changes in mean: Fitting multiple ramp-step templates. ESGCO Proc, 92:95. Hogan N & Sternad D (2007). On rhythmic and discrete movements: Reflections, definitions and implications for motor control. Exp Brain Res, 181:13–30. Hoshino K (1996). One-dimensional stochastic models for spontaneous and burst blink. Robot and Human Communication, 5th IEEE International Workshop, 11-14:229-231. (PDF) Huntley GW & Jones EG (1991). Relationship of intrinsic connections to forelimb movement representations in monkey motor cortex: A correlative anatomic and physiological study. J Neurophysiol, 66(2):390–413. Huys R, Studenka BE, Rheaume NL, Zelaznik HN, & Jirsa VK (2008). Distinct Timing Mechanisms Produce Discrete and Continuous Movements. PLoS Comput Biol, 4(4): e1000061. (PDF) Inui N, Ichihara T, Minami T, & Matsui A (1998). Interactions: timing and force control of fingertapping sequences. Percept Mot Skills, 86:1395-401. Ivry RB, Keele SW, & Diener HC (1988). Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Exp Brain Res, 73(1):167–180. (FI0031) Ivry RI & Keele SW (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1-2):134150. Ivry R & Hazeltine E (1999). Subcortical locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient. Hum Mov Sci, 18(2-3):345-375. (FI0032) 226 Ivry R (1993). Cerebellar involvement in the explicit representation of temporal information. Ann NY Acad Sci, 682:214-30. (FI0034) Ivry RB (1996). The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(6):851-857. (FI0033) Ivry RB & Richardson TD (2002). Temporal control and coordination: the multiple timer model. Brain Cogn, 48(1):117-132. (FI0021) Ivry RB, Richardson TC, & Helmuth LL (2002). Improved temporal stability in multieffector movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hum Percept Perform, 28: 72–92. Ivry RB & Spencer RM (2004). The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 14(2): 225-232. (VI0009) Jackson KM (1983). Why the upper limbs move during human walking. J theor biol, 105:311–315. (FJ0090) Jackson K M, Joseph J, & Wyard SJ (1983a). The upper limbs during human walking. Part I: Sagittal movement. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 23:425–434. Jackson KM, Joseph J, & Wyard SJ (1983b). The upper limbs during human walking. Part2: Function. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 23:435–446. Jagacinski RJ, Marshburn E, Klapp ST, & Jones MR (1988). Tests of parallel versus integrated structure in polyrhythmic tapping. Jl Mot Beh, 20(4):416–442. Jantzen KJ, Oullier O, Marshall M, Steinberg FL, & Kelso JAS (2007). A parametric fMRI investigation of context effects in sensorimotor timing and coordination. Neuropsychologica, 45(4): 673–684. Jeannerod M (1988). The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal-Directed Movements. Clarendon Press, Oxford, UK. Jenkins IH, Jahanshahi M, Jueptner M, Passingham RE, & Brooks DJ (2000). Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. Brain, 123:1216–1228. (FJ0089) Johanshahi M, Jenskin IH, Browa RG, Marsden CD, Passingham RE, & Brooks DJ (1995). Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements: I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson’s disease subjects. Brain, 118:913-33. Johnson WS (1898). Researches in practice and habit. Studies from the Yale Psychology Laboratory, 6:51–105. Johnson CH (1999). Forty years of prcs–what have we learned? Chronobiol Int, 16(6):711–743. 227 Karson CN, Berman KF, Donnelly EF, Mendelson WB, Kleinman JE, & Wyatt RJ (1981). Speaking, Thinking and Blinking. Psychiatry Res, 5(3):243-6. (FK0233) Karson CN (1983). Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic system. Brain, 106(3): 643-653. Keele SW, Pokorny RA, Corcos DM, & Ivry R (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychol, 60(2-3):173–191. Keele SW, Ivry R, & Pokorny R (1987). Force control and its relation to timing. J Motor Behav, 19:96114. Keele SW, Nicoletti R, Ivry RI, & Pakoray RA (1989). Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or interval-based judgements? Psychol Res, 50:251-256. (FK0236) Kelso JAS, Southard DL, & Goodman D (1979). On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science, 203:1029-1031. (FK0163) Kelso JAS (1984). Phase transitions and critical behaviour in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol, 15:R1000–R1004. (FK0235) Kerr B, Condon SM, & McDonald LA (1985). Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 11(5):617–22. King DC & Michels KM (1957). Muscular tension and the human blink rate. J Exp Psychol, 53(2):113116. Kinsbourne M & Hicks RE (1978). Functional cerebral space: a model for overflow, transfer and interference effects in human performance: a tutorial review. In: Attention and Performance VII (ed. J. Requin), 345–362. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA. Kiss IZ, Zhai Y, & Hudson JL (2002a). Collective dynamics of chaotic chemical oscillators and the law of large numbers. Phys Rev Lett, 88(23):238301. Kiss IZ, Zhai Y, & Hudson JL. (2002b). Emerging coherence in a population of chemical oscillators. Science, 296(5573):1676–1678. Klapp ST (1979). Doing two things at once: the role of temporal compatibility. Mem Cogn, 7:375–381. (VK0018) Kolers PA & Brewster JM (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 11(2):150–167. Kronauer RE, Jewett ME, & Czeisler CA (1993). Commentary: the human circadian response to light strong and weak resetting. J Biol Rhythms, 8:351–360. (PDF) Kuramoto Y (1984). Chemical Oscillations, Waves, and Turbulence. Springer, Berlin. 228 Latash ML (1993). Control of human movement. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL. Latash ML, Scholz JP, Danion F, & Schöner G (2002). Finger coordination during discrete and oscillatory force production tasks. Exp Brain Res, 146: 419-432. (FL0148) Levin MF, Feldman AG, Milner TE, & Lamarre Y (1992). Reciprocal and coactivation commands for fast wrist movements. Exp Brain Res, 89(3):669-677. (FL0120) Lewis PA & Miall RC (2003). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurements: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 13:250–255. (NL0005) Lewis PA, Wing AM, Pope PA, Praamstra P, & Miall RC (2004). Brain activity correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initialization, synchronisation and continuation phases of paced finger tapping. Neuropsychologia, 42:1301–1312. Logan GD & Gordon RD (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychol Rev, 108:393-434. Lien MC, Proctor RW, & Allen PA (2002). Ideomotor compatibility in the psychological refractory period effect: 29 years of oversimplification. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 28(2):396-409. (VL0044) Liepert J, Terborg C, & Weiller C (1999). Motor plasticity induced by synchronized thumb and foot movements. Exp Brain Res, 125(4):435–439. Lunenburger L, Kutz DF, & Hoffmann KP (2000). Influence of arm movements on saccades in humans. Eur J Neurosci, 12:4107–4116. MacLean WEJr, Lewis MH, Bryson-Brockmann WA, Ellis DN, Arendt RE, & Baumeister AA (1985). Blink rate and stereotyped s: Evidence for dopamine involvement? Biol Psychiatry, 20(12):1321-1325. Malmstrom FV, Reed LE, & Weber RJ (1983). Saccadic eye movements during a concurrent auditory task. Bull Psychonomic Soc, 21:31–34. (WM0005) Maródi M, D’Ovidio F, & Vicsek T (2002). Synchronization of oscillators with long range interaction: Phase transition and anomalous finite size effects. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter, 66:011109. Yamada M (1996). Temporal Control Mechanism in Equaled Interval Tapping. Appl Hum Sci, 15(3):105-110. Mates J & Aschersleben G (2000). Sensorimotor synchronization: the impact of temporally displaced auditory feedback. Acta Psychol, 104(1):29-44. Mazziotta JC, Hutchinson M, Fife TD, & Woods R (1998). Advanced neuroimaging methods in the study of movement disorders: dystonia and blepharospasm. Adv Neurol, 78:153-160. 229 Mates J (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. I. Timing and error corrections. Biol Cybern, 70(5):463–473. (FM0094) Mates J (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. II. Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations. Biol Cybern, 70(5):475–484. (FM0094). McClintock MK (1971). Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature, 229(5282):244–245. Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knoblich G, & Prinz W (2001). Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature, 414:69-73. (FM0225) Megaw ED (1972). Direction and extent uncertainty in step-input tracking. J Motor Behav, 4(3):171186. Meyer DE & Kieras DE (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multipletask performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychol Rev, 104(1):3-65. Mitrushina MN, Boone KB, & D'Elia LF (1999). Handbook of Normative Data for Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. Miyake Y (2002). Two modes of application in sensori-motor synchronization. SICE 5:2690-2695. (PDF) Moiseff A & Copeland J (2000). A new type of synchronized flashing in a north American firefly. J Insect Behav, 13(4):597–612. Moritz RF & Southwick EE (1992). Bees as Superorganisms. Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. New York. Morton J, Marcus SM, & Frankish C (1976). Perceptual centers (P-centers). Psychol Rev, 83:405-408. Moschovakis AK, Scudder CA, & Highstein SM (1996). The microscopic anatomy and physiology of the mammalian saccadic system. Prog Neurobiol, 50(2-3):133:254. Murthy VN & Fetz EE (1996). Oscillatory activity in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys: Synchronization of local field potentials and relation to behavior. J Neurophysiol, 76:3949–3967. Naase T, Doughty MJ, & Button NF (2005). An assessment of the pattern of spontaneous eyeblink activity under the influence of topical ocular anaesthesia. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol, 243(4):306-312. (WN0022). Nagasawa T, Yuasa Y, Tsuga K, & Tsuru H (1993). Reproducibility of tooth tapping rhythm in young and elderly dentate subjects. J Oral Rehabil, 20(4):441-7. Nakamori K, Odawara M, Nakajima T, Mizutani T, & Tsubota K (1997). Blinking is controlled primarily by ocular surface conditions. Am J Ophthalmol, 124(1):24-30. 230 Navon D & Miller J (1987). The role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 13. (PDF) Navon D & Miller J (2002). Queuing or sharing. A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychol, 44:193-251. Neda Z, Nikitin A, & Vicsek T (2003). Synchronization of two-mode stochastic oscillators: a new model for rhythmic applause and much more. Physica A, 321(1-2):238–247. Neumann O (1984). Automatic processing: a review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In: Prinz W, Sanders AF, editors. Cognition and Motor Processes. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 255–93. Obhi SS & Goodale AM (2005). Bimanual Interference in Rapid Discrete Movements Is Task Specific and Occurs at Multiple Levels of Processing. J Neurophysiol, 94(3):1861–1868. Optican LM & Robinson DA (1980). Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system, J Neurophysiol. 44(6):1058–1076. (VO0008) Paillard J (1949). Quelques données psychophysiologiques relatives au déclenchement de la commande motrice [Some psychophysiological data relating to the triggering of motor commands]. L'Année Psychologique, 48:28–47. Pashler H (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 10(3):358-377. (VP0053) Pashler H & Johnston JC (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Q J Exp Psychol, A 41:19-45. Pashler H, Carrier M, & Hoffman J (1993). Saccadic eye movements and dual-task interference. Q J Exp Psychol, A 46:51-82 Loseby PN, Piek JP, & Barrett NC (2001). The influence of speed and force on bimanual finger tapping patterns. Hum Mov Sci, 20(4-5):531-547. (FL0150) Peper CE, Beek PJ, & Daffertshofer A (2000). Considerations regarding a comprehensive model of (poly)rhythmic movements. In P. Desain & L.Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm perception and production, 35– 49. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger. Peters M (1985). Constraints in the performance of bimanual tasks and their expression in unskilled and skilled subjects. Q J Exp Psychol, 37(2):171–196. Peterson EL (1980). Phase-resetting a mosquito circadian oscillator. J Comp Physiol, 138(3):201-211. (PDF) 231 Pittenrigh CS & Bruce VG (1959). Daily rhythms as coupled oscillator systems and their relation to thermoperiodism and photoperiodism. In: withrow RB, editor, Photoperiodism and related phenomena in plants and animals. Washington, DC: Am Assoc Adv Sci, 475-505. Pollok B, Gross J, Müller K, Aschersleben G, & Schnitzler A (2005). The cerebral oscillatory network associated with auditory paced finger movements. Neuroimage, 24(3):646-655. (FP0163) Ponder E & Kennedy WP (1927). On the act of blinking. Q J Exp Physiol, 18(2):89-110. (FP0164) Pressing J (1998). Referential behaviour theory: A framework for multiple perspectives on motor control. In Piek J (Ed.), Motor control and human skill: A multidisciplinary perspective, 357_384. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Ramírez-Ávila, GM, Guisset JL, & Deneubourg JL (2003). Synchronous behavior in small populations of light-controlled oscillators. In Stoop, R., editor, 11th International IEEE Workshop on Nonlinear Dynamics of Electronic Systems, pages 201–204, Scuol/Schuls, Switzerland. Rao SM, Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Bobholz JA, Cox RW, & Binder JR (1997). Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. J Neurosci, 17:5528–5535. (FR0165) Raethjen J, Govindan RB, Binder S, Zeuner KE, Deuschl G, & Stolze H (2008). Cortical representation of rhythmic foot movements. Brain Res, (1236)79-84. (FR0160) Repp BH (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psycho Res, 63:106-128. (VR0041) Repp BH (2001). Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Hum Mov sci, 20(3):277-312. (VR0045) Repp BH & Keller PE (2004). Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization: Effects of intention, attention, and awareness. Q J Exp Psychol, 57A(3):499–521. (FR0166) Repp BH (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon Bull Rev, 12(6):969–992. (PDF) Repp BH (2006). Does an auditory distractor sequence affect self-paced tapping? Acta Psychol, 121(1):81–107. (FR0167) Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, & Schmidt RC (2005). Effects of Visual and Verbal Interaction on Unintentional Interpersonal Coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 31(1):62-79. (PDF) Roberts RJ, Hager LD, & Heron C (1994). Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. J Exp Psychol, 123(4):374–393. 232 Robertson SD, Zelaznik HN, Lantero D, Bojczyk KG, Spencer RM, Doffin JG, & Schneidt T (1999). Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: Evidence against a single timing process for motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 25(5):1316–1330. Ronsse R, Sternad D, & Lefevre P (2009). A computational model for rhythmic and discrete movements in uni- and bimanual coordination. Neural Comput, 21(5):1335–1370. (VR0053) Sachs L (1984). Applied statistics. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer. (buch avalaible) Sailer U, Eggert T, Ditterich J, & Straube A (2000). Spatial and temporal aspects of eye–hand coordination across different tasks. Exp Brain Res, 134:163–173. Salter JE, Wishart LR, Lee TD, & Simon D (2004). Perceptual and motor contributions to bimanual coordination. Neurosci Lett, 363(2):102-107. (FS0376) Schaal S & Sternad D (2001). Origins and Violations of the 2/3 Power Law in Rhythmic 3D Arm Movements. Brain Res, 136, 60-72. (FS0377) Schaal S, Sternad D, Osu R, & Kawato M (2004). Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete. Nature Neurosci, 7:1136–1143. (FS0378) Schmidt RA (1980). On the theoretical status of time in motor-program representations. In Stelmach GE & Requin J (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (145–165). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Schmidt RC & O'Brien B (1997). Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecol Psychol, 9(3):189-206. (FS0379) Scholz JP, Kelso JAS, & Schöner G (1987). Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical slowing down and switching time. Phys Lett, 123(8):390-394. (PDF) Schoener G (2002). Timing, clocks, and dynamical systems. Brain Cognition, 48(1):31–51. Schulze HH (1978). The dectectability of local and global displacements in regular rhythmic patterns. Psychol Res, 40:173-181. Semjen A, Schulze HH, & Vorberg D (2000). Timing precision in continuation and synchronization tapping. Psychol Res, 63(2):137-147. (FS0303) Semjen A & Summers JJ. (2002). Timing goals in bimanual coordination. Q J Exp Psychol A, 55(1):15571. (VS0089) Shabunin A, Astakhov V, Demidov V, Provata A, Baras F, Nicolis G, & Anishchenko V (2003). Modeling chemical reactions by forced limit-cycle oscillator: synchronization phenomena and transition to chaos. Chaos, Solit, & Fract, 15(2):395–405. Sharikatze M, Dung CK, Staude G, Deubel H, & Werner W (2009). Dual-tasking: Is manual tapping independent of concurrently executed saccades? Brain Res, 1283:41-49. 233 Shaw ED, Stokes PE, Mann JJ & Manevitz A (1987). Effects of lithium carbonate on the memory and motor speed of bipolar patients. J Abnorm Psychol, 96 (1):64-69. Shibasaki H, Sadato N, Lyshkow H, Yonekura Y, Honda M, Nagamine T, Suwazono S, Magata Y, Ikeda A, Miyazaki M, Fukuyama H, Asato R, & Konishi J (1993). Both primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area play an important role in complex finger movement. Brain, 116(6):138798. (FS0380). Shumway-Cook A & Woollacott M (2000). Attentional demands and postural control: the effect of sensory context. J Gerontol, 55(1):M10–6. Schuri U, & von Cramon D (1981). Heart rate and blink rate responses during mental arithmetic with and without continuous verbalization of results. Psychophysiology, 18:650–653. Skotte JH, Nojgaard JK, Jorgensen LV, Christensen KB., Sjogaard G (2007). Eye blink frequency during different computer tasks quantified by electrooculography. Eur J AppL Physiol, 99(2):113-119. Snedden WA & Greenfield MD (1998). Females prefer leading males: relative call timing and sexual selection in katydid choruses. An Behav, 56(5):1091–1098. Snyder LH (2000). Coordinate transformations for eye and arm movements in the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 10(6):747–754. Snyder LH, Calton JL, Dickinson AR, & Lawrence BM (2002). Eye–hand coordination: saccades are faster when accompanied by a coordinated arm movement. J Neurophysiol, 87:2279–2286. Spencer RM, Zelaznick HN, Diedrichsen J, & Ivry RB (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300(5624):1437-1439. Spencer RMC, Ivry RB, & Zelaznik HN (2005). Role of the cerebellum in movements: Control of timing or movement transitions? Exp Brain Res, 161(3):383-396. (VS0110) Staude G, Wolf W, Ott M, Oertel WH, & Dengler R (1995). Tremor as a Factor in Prolonged Reaction Times of Parkinsonian Patients. Movement Disord, 10:153–162. (FS0129) Staude G, Kafka V, & Wolf W (2000). Determination of premotor silent periods from surface myoelectric signals. Biomed Tech Suppl 2, 45:228-232. Staude G (2001). Precise onset detection of human motor responses using a whitening filter and the loglikelihood ratio test. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. (BME), 48(11):1292–1305. (QS0037) Staude G, Dengler R, & Wolf W (2002). The discontinuous nature of motor execution II. Merging discrete and rhythmic movements in a single-joint system: the phase entrainment effect. Biol cybern, 86(6):427-443. (FS0381) 234 Staude G, Cong-Khac D, & Wolf W (2006). Modeling the coordination of concurrent single-joint movements. Proc. Biomed. Technik, Zürich, Sept.-6-9, 2 pages. (BS0090) Stein RB (1982). What muscle variable(s) does the nervous system control in limb movements? Behav Brain Sci, 5:535 577. Sternad D, Dean WJ, & Schaal S (2000). Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements, Hum Mov Sci, 19(4):627–664. Stevens LT (1886). On the time-sense. Mind, 11:393-404. (PDF) Stevens JR & Livermore A Jr (1978). Eye blinking and rapid eye movement: pulsed photic stimulation of the brain. Exp Neurol, 60(3):541-556. Strogatz SH & Stewart I (1993). Coupled oscillators and biological synchronization. Sci Am, 269(6):102–109. (FS0382) Stuphorn V, Hoffmann KP, & Miller LE (1999). Correlation of primate superior colliculus and reticular formation discharge with proximal limb muscle activity. J Neurophysiol, 81(4):1978–1982. Stuyven E, Van Der Goten K, Vandierendonck A, Clayes K, & Crevits L (2000). The effect of cognitive load on saccadic eye movements. Acta Psychol, 104(1):69–85. (WS0138) Summers J (2002). Practice and Training in Bimanual Coordination Tasks: Strategies and Constraints. Brain Cognition, 48(1):166–178. Swinnen SP, Jardin K, Meulenbroek R, Dounskaia N, & Hofkens-Van Den Brandt M (1997). Egocentric and Allocentric Constraints in the Expression of Patterns of Interlimb Coordination. J Cognitive Neurosci, 9(3):348-377. Swinnen S & Wenderoth N (2004). Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill. Trends Cogn Sci, 8(1):18–25. Swinnen SP (2002). Intermanual coordination: from behavioural principles to neural-network interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci, 3(5):350–361 Talairach J & Tournoux P (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme. Taylor W, Krasnow R, Dunlap JC, Broda H, & Hastings JW (1982). Critical Pulses of Anisomycin Drive the Circadian Oscillator in Gonyaulax Towards Its Singularity. J Comp Physiol, 148(1):11-25. (PDF) Taylor JR, Elsworth JD, Lawrence MS, Sladek JR, Jr Roth RH., & Redmond DE Jr (1999). Spontaneous blink rates correlate with dopamine levels in the caudate nucleus of MPTP-treated monkeys. Exp Neurol, 158:214–220. 235 Temprado JJ, Zanone PG, Monno A, & Laurent M (1999). Attentional load associated with performing and stabilizing perferred bimanuals patterns. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 25(6):1579-1594. Temprado JJ, Monno A, Zanone PG, & Kelso JAS (2002). Attentional demands reflect learning-induced alterations of bimanual coordination dynamics. Eur J Neurosci, 16(7):1-6. Thomas C, Mennel HD, Gebert G, & Bewermeyer H (1990). Nervensystem. Solcher u. R. Heene. ISBN 3-7945-1290-1. (Buch available) Treisman M (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychol Monogr, 77(13):1-31. Treisman M, Faulkner A, Naish PLN, & Brogan D (1990). The internal clock: evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristics frequency. Perception, 19(6):705-743. Tsubota K, Hata S, Okusawa Y, Egami F, Ohtsuki T, & Nakamori K (1996). Quantitative videographic analysis of blinking in normal subjects and patients with dry eye. Arch Ophthalmol, 114(6), 715-720. Tsubota K, Kwong KK, Lee TY, Nakamura J, & Cheng HM (1999). Functional MRI of brain activation by eye blinking. Exp Eye Res, 69(1):1-7. (PDF) Van der Post J, de Waal PP, de Kam ML, Cohen AF, & van Gerven JM A (2004). No evidence of the usefulness of eye blinking as a marker for central dopaminergic activity. J Psychopharmacol, 18(1):109–114. VanderWerf F, Reits D, Smit AE, & Metselaar M (2007). Blink recovery in patients with Bell’s Palsy: A neurophysiological and behavioral longitudinal study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 48(1):203-213. (FV0051) Vandierendonck A, Vooght GDe, Van der Goten K (1998). Does random time interval generation interfere with working memory executive functions? Eur J of Cognitive Psychol, 10(4): 413–442. (NV0001) Van Donkelaar P, Siu KC, & Walterschied J (2004). Saccadic output is influenced by limb kinetics during eye–hand coordination. J Motor Behav, 36:245–252. (WD0094) Von Cramon D & Schuri U (1980). Blink frequency and speed motor activity. Neuropsychologia, 18(45):603-606. Vorberg D & Hambuch R (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 423:390-406. (VV0021) Vorberg D & Wing A (1994). Modelle für Variabilität und Abhängigkeit bei der zeitlichen Steuerung. In Heuer H & Keele SW (Eds.), Psychomotorik: Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, 3:223-320, Göttingen: Hogrefe. 236 Vorberg D & Wing A (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In Heuer H & Keele SW (Eds), Handbook of perception and action, Motor Skills, 2:181-262. London: Academic Press. Wachter C, Gerisch H, Nave PRW, Staude G, & Wolf W (2004). Synchronization of periodic movements with external events and interlimb coordination: phase entrainment or phase resetting? Measur Sci Rev, 4:20–28. Wachter C, Cong DK, Staude G, & Wolf W. (2008). Coordination of a discrete response with periodic finger tapping: additional experimental aspects for a subtle mechanism. J Motor Behav, 40(5):417432. Wannier T, Bastiaanse C, Colombo G. & Dietz V (2001). Arm to leg coordination in human during walking, creeping and swimming. Exp Brain Res, 14:375-379. Wang W, Kiss IZ, & Hudson JL (2000). Experiments on arrays of globally coupled chaotic electrochemical oscillators: Synchronization and clustering. Chaos, 10(1):248–256. Watson AHD (2006). What can studying musicians tell us about motor control of the hand? J Anat, 208:527–542. Weatherhead PJ & Yezerinac SM (1998). Breeding synchrony and extra-pair mating in birds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 43(3):217–219. Wefel JS, Hoyt BD, & Massama PJ (1999). Neuropsychological functioning in depressed versus nondepressed participants with Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol, 13(3):249257 Wei K, Wertman G, & Sternad D (2003). Interactions between rhythmic and discrete components in a bimanual task, Motor Control, 72(2):134–154. Welford AT (1952). The “Psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance A review and a theory. Br J Psychol, 43:2-19. Welford AT (1967). Single channel operation in the brain. Acta Psychol, 27:5-22. Welsh TN, Almeida QJ, & Lee TD (2005). The effect of postural stability and spatial orientation of the upper limbs on interlimb coordination. Exp Brain Res, 161(3):265-275. Wickens CD (1989). Attention and skilled performance. In: Holding DH, editor. Human Skills. NY: John Wiley, & Sons, 71–105. Wilson GF, Fullenkamp P, & Davis I (1994). Evoked potential, cardiac, blink, and respiration measures of pilot workload in air-to-ground missions. Aviat Space Environ Med, 65(2):100-5. Winfree AT (1967). Biological Rhythms and the Behavior of Populations of Coupled Oscillators. J Theoret Biol, 16(1):15-42. (Fw0162) 237 Winfree AT (1980). The Geometry of Biological Time. Springer Verlag, New York. Wing AM & Kristofferson AB (1973a). Response delays and timing of discrete motor responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 14:5-12. Wing AM & Kristofferson AB (1973b). The timing of interresponse intervals. Perception and Psychophysics, 13:455-460. (FW0142) Wing AM (1980). The long and short of timing in response sequences. In GE Stelmach & J Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior, 1:469-486. Amsterdam: North-Holland. (FW0144) Wing AM (2002). Voluntary timing and brain function: an information processing approach. Brain Cogn, 48(1):7-30. Yamada M & Yonera S (2001). Temporal control mechanism for tapping of rhythmic patterns constructed by two kinds of time values. Acoustical Science and Technology, 22(3):245-252. (PDF) Yamanishi J, Kawato M, & Suzuki R (1979). Studies on human finger tapping neural networks by phase transition curves. Biol Cybern, 33(4):119-208. (FY0035) Yamanishi J, Kawato M, & Suzuki R (1980). Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping in both hands. Biol Cybern, 37:219–225. (FY0036) Yardley L, Gardner M, Leadbetter A, & Lavie N (1999). Effect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural control. Neuroreport, 10(2):215-9. Yoshino K, Takagi K, Nomura T, Sato S, & Tonoike M (2002). MEG Responses during rhythmic finger tapping in humans to phasic stimulation and their interpretation based on neural mechanisms. Biol Cybern, 86:483-496. (FY0042) Zehr EP & Duysens J (2004). Regulation of arm and leg movement during human locomotion. Neuroscientist, 10(4): 347-361. Zelaznik HN, Spencer RM, & Ivry RB (2002). Dissociation of Explicit and Implicit Timing in Repetitive Tapping and Drawing Movements. J Expl Psycho: Hum Percept Perform, 28(3):575–588. (FZ0041) Zijdewind I, van Duinen H, Zielman R, & Lorist MM (2006). Interaction between force production and cognitive performance in humans. Clin Neurophysiol, 117(3):660-7. 238 10 Appendix 10.1 Classification of the interactions based on discrete events The basic idea of this classification is to categorize the tapping behavior of the subjects into the following typical interaction patterns (for description of patterns, see Section 6.5.1): 1) Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI), 2) Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR), - Tap Delaying (TD), - Tap Cancelling (TC), 3) Periodic Tap Hastening (PTH), 4) Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE), 5) Mixed Tapping Strategies (MTS). The additionally introduced error class “Discrete Tap Omitted” (DTO) contains all segments in which the discrete tap was not executed. For the statistical evaluation in Section 7.2, these groups are defined by quantitative estimation resulting in a motor response scheme for the periodic taps and the discrete taps. Such a simple response scheme simply is derived from statistical observations. Actually, decision for a tap behavior category is taken according to rules which are based on the length of the disturbed interval between the reference tap and the next following periodic tap which encloses the discrete tap as well as on the deviation of the dual-task discrete taps RT from SRT reference value determined in single-task experiments. These rules were applied for the automatic classification of the segments of real data recorded in the experiments. Determination of the SRT references First, the individual mean reaction time SRTav and the corresponding standard deviation σsrt are determined from the left hand taps responses of these reaction time sessions; the accompanying right hand responses are not further considered. The lower bound SRTmin and the upper bound SRTmax was set to: SRTmin = SRTav – σsrt fsrt SRTmax = SRTav + σsrt fsrt The factor fsrt was empirically determined but equal for all subjects. Determination of the periodic tapping interval references The mean periodic tapping interval Nav and the corresponding standard deviation σN were derived from the data of the normal tapping experiment. In order to get representative values (undisturbed intervals), not only the intertap interval preceding the reference tap but also the two further preceding intervals was considered. The mean Nav and the standard deviation σN were determined for each experiment of each subject. The lower and upper bound Nmin and Nmax, respectively, for the tapping period between reference tap and first periodic tap after the reference tap was set to: Nmin = Nav – σN fN Nmax = Nav + σN fN . The factor fN was empirically determined but equal for all subjects and experiments. 239 Definitions of the interaction patterns The dual-task data of every valid trial is divided into 12 segments when it contains one discrete single tap. 5 periodic taps around the discrete tap are taken for each segment and shown on the ordinate of PRC. (1) Marginal Tapping Interaction (MTI) A sequence is assigned to this category, if the segment does not show any signs of the following interactions - RT of the discrete tap lies within the range limited by the lower bound SRTmin and the upper bound SRTmax, and - the duration of the periodic tapping interval lies within range limited by the upper bound N max and the lower bound Nmin. (2) Periodic Tap Retardation (PTR) a) Tap Delaying (TD) A sequence is assigned to this category - if RT of the discrete tap is within the lower bound SRTmin and the upper bound SRTmax, and - if the duration of the periodic tapping interval enclosing this discrete tap exceeds the upper bound Nmax. b) Tap Cancelling (TC) A sequence is assigned to this category - If RT of the discrete tap is within the lower bound SRTmin and the upper bound SRTmax, and - If the execution of the ongoing periodic movement is stopped on the fly simultaneously with the execution of the discrete tap; in normal tapping no ground contact is observed and in isometric tapping the amplitude of the tap is about one third of the usual amplitude, and - If the duration of the periodic tapping interval enclosing this discrete tap exceeds the upper bound Nmax. (3) (PTH) A sequence is assigned to this category - If RT of the discrete tap, which simultaneously is executed together with the first periodic tap after the reference tap, is within the lower bound SRTmin and the upper bound SRTmax, and - If the duration of the periodic tapping interval after the reference tap is shorter than the lower tapping period bound Nmin. (4) Discrete Tap Entrainment (DTE) A sequence is assigned to this category - If the duration of the periodic tapping interval after the reference tap is within the lower bound N min and the upper bound Nmax of the tapping period, and - If the discrete tap is executed simultaneously together with the periodic tap, and - If its RT exceeds the upper bound SRTmax. It should be noted that phase entrainment of the discrete tap can be combined with every value of RT: with very short RT and normal RT as they also occur in single-task reaction time experiments, but in addition with unusual long RT which are most probably due to the entrainment. The criteria used here only select trials with those distinctly prolonged RT, i.e. their rate is certainly underestimated. 240 (5) Mixed Tapping Strategies (MTS) A sequence is assigned to this category - If the RT of the discrete tap exceeds the upper bound SRTmax and - If the duration of the periodic tapping interval enclosing this discrete tap exceeds the upper bound Nmax or - If the duration of the periodic tapping interval enclosing this discrete tap is shorter than the lower bound Nmin of the tapping period or - If the execution of the ongoing periodic movement is stopped on the fly simultaneously with the execution of the discrete tap; in normal tapping no ground contact is observed and in isometric tapping the amplitude of the tap is about one third of the usual amplitude. (6) Discrete Tap Omitted (DTO) A sequence is assigned to this error class, if the discrete tap is missing, independent from the periodic tapping interval duration. Determination of reference values (change bias) Subjects showed a preference for a dominant tapping behavior reflected by a dominant occurrence of the corresponding interaction pattern. But a specific interaction pattern can simply occur occasionally due to the asynchrony of the two tapping processes, since the timing criteria for a specific category (as listed above) can be matched just by chance and not due to any specific interaction. Therefore, a lower bound reference value which indicates this chance bias of occurrence was determined for each subject and experiment; so any value significantly different from this chance bias will indicate some interaction of both tapping processes. Chance bias values were estimated by simulating the motor event sequence: (i) the periodic tap intervals were generated at random with a mean interval and standard deviation matching those of the subject data, (ii) the go signal sequence was randomized between 3000 and 5000 ms, and (iii) the reaction times again were generated at random with the mean and standard deviation matching those of the subject data. Thus, the periodic events and the discrete event were totally independent in this simulation. The generated event sequence for both (simulated) motor tasks was input to the same simple motor response scheme as described above which was used to assign the recorded segments to the interaction pattern categories. The resulting bias values are depicted in Fig. 10.1 as “expectation values”. Again to emphasize that neither this event simulation nor the motor response scheme does incorporate any complex physiological and psychological background, but is rather mechanistically derived from statistical observations only. Note that calculation of these values is restricted to categories MTI, DTE, TD and PTH by principle, because this formal categorization described above does not include position signal evaluation but only event times. 241 Figure 10-1: Distribution of interaction patterns in bimanual tapping and chance bias (expectation) for Subj. 4. Left from the dashed vertical line, groups of 3 bars show the proportion of each interaction category for the three cases: (i) normal tapping (left bar), (ii) isometric tapping (middle bar), (iii) chance bias (no-interaction prediction, right bar). Right from the vertical dashed line, only experimental data are given, since change bias estimates cannot be obtained by principle. Comparison between the experimental results and chance bias is illustrated by Fig. 10.1 which shows three bars for each category on the left side of the diagrams: (i) normal tapping, (ii) isometric tapping, and (iii) chance bias (no-interaction prediction) for Subject 4. Clearly, the chance bias for MTI (left group) being near to 100 % is dominant, because all segments not being assigned to any specific interaction type are designated to the MTI group; i.e. the criteria for the different categories are very specific and their timing structures will almost never appear by chance. 10.2 Software Experimental control schemes were realized using mainly control system DIAdem (National Instrument Inc.) and additional MATLAB for short computation during pause between experimental trials. DIAdem offers interactively inspecting data during experiment and a wide range of mathematical routines for analyzing data. DIAdem uses a built-in script language which contains normal programming constructs such as loops and case statements. Body limb movements are separately recorded for each in several channels and saved in a binary data file. Meta-file containing information such as name of subject, used condition file, date, comment, channel names, binary file names, etc is generated. This meta-file is used in MATLAB-program for reading binary data and offline-analysis and is saved in Header as program variable (more details below). MATLAB is a high-level language and interactive environment for performing computationally intensive tasks. MATLAB is used for off-line analysis. There are two main MATLAB modules Showdata and AnalyzeTapResults. The main function of Showdata is to display experiment data and to provide interactive user interface. The graphic user interface is realized by event listener with callback function and dialog windows. Action is evoked by mouse click on menu, undermenu, button, label, and graphical objects such as marker (more deltails later). Example for the menu DIAdem file: hfile = uimenu(hfig, 'Label', 'DIAdemFile'); With the callback function for the undermenu open: uimenu(hfile, 'Label', 'Open file', 'Accelerator', 'D', 'Callback', callback); Where callback = [s1, usermode, s2, s3]; and 242 usermode = 'UserData.mode = ''open file''; '; s1 = 'UserData = get(gcf, ''UserData'');' s2 = 'set(gcf, ''UserData'', UserData);' s3 = 'uiresume;' The built-in MATLAB variable UserData is set by the callback function. This variable is queried for the actual action (open file (more details later)) and control is given back to graphical user interface (uiresume). The recorded channels can be selected in multiple modes. The characteristic events of signal changes such as onset, offset, and maximum are detected by two algorithms. For simple signal such as force signal linear regression method and simple threshold algorithm, and for complex signal such as finger position signal, sophisticated ramp-step model are approached. These events are presented by vertical or tangential red lines and are named marker as program variable. These detected markers can be manually set, deleted, changed and saved. The data structure of marker is a tree-structured data type called Result and is saved in a MATLAB-format file, which is used for analysis in AnalyseTapResults. Additional information are added to this marker structure and used to display the classification of interaction in DT situation. Saving, loading, and modifying of data structure in any order is possible. Offline analysis is mainly performed in AnalyzeTapResults based on the result file generated in Showdata. The experimental condition (eye-hand tapping, hand-hand-hand, hand-foot tapping, etc.) and task condition (dual-task, single-task) branch off the analysis process into different functions such as ProcessPeriodicRightHand, ProcessPeriodicRightHandAndRightFoot, ProcessEyeHand, ProcessRT, etc. In these functions timing of characteristic events selected by user on interactive dialog window are read from the result file and checked for consistency. The user is notified about the missing events. These missed events have to be checked and corrected in Showdata. The correct timing of events is saved in a MATLAB-format for later use by loading it if the same timing structure is needed for other analysis. Timing structure can be changed for instance by modified or new added marker. The analyzed result also is saved in a MATLAB format files called AnalysedData. For plotting this analyzed result also can be loaded if new analysis is not needed for the new plotting actions. Different specific plot functions and these functions are realized by undermenu. To start both programs, simply type the program name in MATLAB command line and press the return button. Both programs run in an endless loop waiting for an action on the menu and can be exit by the menu exit or by the default close operation of MATLAB. The MATLAB switch instruction branch off to the corresponding action based on program variable UserData.mode(=usermode) set by callback function. Both modules use common custom dialog AutoDetectUIInput(detectAnalyse) and default MATLAB dialog uigetdir(path, 'Please select directory') for getting the user input parameters. The input parameter detectAnalyse has following elements for three dialog arts text dialog, list box dialog and radio button dialog: % the header name for the text box field defaultValueHeader = 'default'; customValueHeader = 'custom values'; % the input parameter names for the text dialog defaultNames = {'Bin Step'; 'XLim2'; 'YLim2'; 'Phase from'; 'Phase to'}; % the default input parameter values for the text dialog defaultValues = {'30', '500', '60', '0.35', '0.45'}; % the input parameter names for the list box dialog namesToSelect = {'Reaction limb', 'Periodic limb', 'signal event'}; % the input parameter values for the list box dialog valuesToSelect = [{'left hand'; 'right hand'; 'left foot'; 'right foot'; 'go'}, ... {'left hand'; 'right hand'; 'left foot'; 'right foot'; ''}, ... {'force'; 'position'; 'agonist'; 'antagonist'; ''}]; 243 % the header name for the radio button dialog boxHeaders = {'Select channels'}; % the input parameter names for the radio button dialog boxNames = {'channelName1'; 'channelName2'; 'channelName3'}; % the input parameter values for the radio button dialog boxValue = {'0'; '0'; '0'}; Example for getting input values: qqAnalyse = AutoDetectUIInput(detectAnalyse); binStep = str2num(char(qqAnalyse.customValues(1))); xlimit2 = str2num(char(qqAnalyse.customValues(2))); ylimit2 = str2num(char(qqAnalyse.customValues(3))); phaseFrom = str2num(char(qqAnalyse.customValues(4))); phaseTo = str2num(char(qqAnalyse.customValues(5))); reactionLimb = deblank(char(detectUiParam.valuesToSelect(qq.selectedValues(1), 1))); periodicLimb = deblank(char(detectUiParam.valuesToSelect(qq.selectedValues(2), 2))); signalEvent = deblank(char(detectUiParam.valuesToSelect(qq.selectedValues(3), 3))); if qqAnalyse.boxValues(1, 1)=='1'; 10.3 Showdata 10.3.1 Variable 10.3.1.1 Global variable Global variable are considered as static variable and accessible in all functions Header: contains meta information about the experiment data in following fields 2.1 element 2.3 Filename 2.5 FullFilename 2.7 FilePath 2.9 NotANumber 2.11 Name 2.13 Comment 2.15 Author 2.17 Date 2.19 Time 2.21 TimeFormat 2.23 Channel 2.25 NChannels 2.27 NDataChan 2.29 FileType 2.31 NumberMarkers 2.33 Version 2.35 DataSize 2.37 DataFormat 2.39 ChannelDataPos 2.2 meaning 2.4 experiment data file name 2.6 path and file name 2.8 path 2.10 minimum invalid value (exceed data type float) 2.12 participant name 2.14 additional information about experiment 2.16 experimenter 2.18 performing date of experiment 2.20 performing time of experiment 2.22 'dd.mm.yyyy hh:nn:ss.ffff' 2.24 vector of channels 2.26 number of channels 2.28 index of data channel (not time channel) 2.30 2.32 2.34 'DIAdem9' 2.36 size of recorded data 2.38 data format 2.40 unused 244 Example to access meta data: Header.Channel(i).Length: %data length of the channel number i; Header.Channel(i).SampleTime %sample time (1ms) Header.Channel(i).Name %channel name for i = 1:Header.NChannels %loop through all channel Result: contains data structure of characteristic events of all signals which were detected by custom algorithms 2.41 Element 2.43 Status 2.45 NDataChan 2.47 Channel 2.49 Channel structure 2.42 Meaning 2.44 status of Header 2.46 number of channels 2.48 channel list 2.50 Meaning 2.53 Channel name 2.56 Trial number 2.59 Marker list 2.52 Name 2.55 Trial 2.58 Marker 2.61 Marker structure 2.62 Meaning 2.65 YLocation 2.69 XLocation 2.73 Name 2.77 Magnitude 2.81 RiseTime 2.85 Character 2.63 2.66 y-coordinate of marker 2.67 2.70 time of marker 2.71 2.74 marker name 2.75 2.78 y-coordinate of 2.79 tangent 2.82 x-coordinate of tangent 2.86 interaction class name 2.51 2.54 2.57 2.60 2.64 2.68 2.72 2.76 2.80 2.83 2.84 2.87 2.88 Example accessing marker: for cidx = 1:length(Result.Channel), % for every channel ms = []; % initial markers for this channel if isfield(Result.Channel(cidx), 'trial'), trialTo = length(Result.Channel(cidx).trial); % number of trial if trialTo>0 for tidx = 1:trialTo, % for every trial if ~isempty(Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker) midx = 1; % initial marker index % as long as the marker index is valid while midx <= length(Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker) m = Rsult.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker(midx); % check if the marker name is valid if isContainedString({'s', 'm', 'e', '1', '2'}, m.Name) % trial number for this marker m.trial = tidx; array end midx = midx + 1; end end end end end 245 ms = [ms m]; % append to marker end UserData: default global MATLAB variable specifying data that associate with actual graphic object (window) and usable in other m files. 2.89 Element 2.91 Warntxt 2.93 Mode 2.95 showCharacteristicsOfInteraction 2.97 trial 2.99 datstart 2.101 plot.dattime 2.103 plot.datsize 2.105 plot.showchan 2.107 plot.zoomstart 2.109 plot.zoomsize 2.111 markerstatus 2.113 selectedChannelsToShowMarkers 2.115 markerNames 2.117 StartSignal 2.119 PaceSignal 2.121 GoSignal 2.90 Meaning 2.92 hint for setting marker operation 2.94 execution mode generated by action on menu. 2.96 indication whether interaction classification should be displayed 2.98 actual trial in Showdata 2.100 start time of actual segment in Showdata 2.102 maximum time vector of all channels 2.104 segment time range 2.106 selected channels for displayed 2.108 start time of zoom if it is activated. 2.110 zoom size 2.112 marker status 2.114 selected channels for marker display 2.116 marker name vector 2.118 control signal trial 2.120 control signal pace 2.122 control signal stimulus For example accessing data in function PlotAcqData: if ~isempty(UserData.plot.zoomstart) & (UserData.plot.zoomsize ~= 0) 10.3.1.2 Local variable actualReactionIndex, actualPauseIndex are current index of actual discrete movement and pause signal currently displayed on the screen providing jump on the desired go and pause signal number. 10.3.2 Menu Menu and undermenu are created in MATLAB by the function uicontrol. Default menu from MATLAB are file, edit, view, insert, tools, desktop window help. Other menus are custom menus. 246 Figure 10-2: dialog for selecting the experiment data file 247 Figure 10-3: dialog shows the list of all experiment data files 10.3.2.1 DIAdemfile: Menu for experiment data file. 10.3.2.1.1 Unternenu open file: Click on menu open file, a dialog created by calling [ffiles, fpath] = ListFilesForSelect; for selecting the experiment data file (Fig. 10.2). The function ListFilesForSelect uses the default MATLAB function uigetdir which return the directory path and all file names in the selected directory. This list of all experiment data files is shown in the next dialog window (Fig. 10.3) by calling the default MATLABfunction listdlg. Figure 10-4: dialog for selecting of channels 248 Figure 10-5: the result file is loaded and the markers are displayed in Showdata Fig. 10.4 shows dialog window by calling AutoDetectUIInput(detectAnalyse) for selecting of channels. The input parameter detectAnalyse consists of only radio button dialog. The channel names are read from the variable Header. The variable Header is created from the meta-file created by DIAdem for global usage. All the channel data of control signals (go command, pacing) are read and saved in variable UserData. The result file is loaded and the markers are displayed (Fig. 10.5). Displaying signals and characteristic events is implemented in function PlotAcqData. It calls the function GetDiaData to read all the binary data of selected signals with the help of variable Header. save result file: save result file containing markers exit: exit Showdata 249 10.3.2.2 Options 10.3.2.2.1 Untermenu Select channels: select the channels to be displayed (Fig. 10.4). Unit display in Newton can be selected. Figure 10-6: dialog for selecting the maximum time duration to be displayed. Select segment size: select the time duration to be displayed on the screen (Fig. 10.6) Header of content file: print the structure of Header containing information about experiment. Reverse signals: display the reversed signal Figure 10-7: shows the numbered onset of the registered extern clock Show extern takt: show the registered extern clock which onset is numbered (Fig. 10.7) 10.3.2.3 Characterize 10.3.2.3.1 Unternenu Show characteristics of interactions: display the classified interaction in DT tapping Hide characteristics of interactions: hide the classified interaction in DT tapping Print number of interaction characteristics: print the number of classified interaction in DT tapping 250 10.3.2.4 Event marker 10.3.2.4.1 Untermenu Figure 10-8: dialog for selecting the new individual marker name Change marker name: change individual marker name Activate on the menu and click on the marker leads to a dialog window for selecting the new marker name (Fig. 10.8): Delete marker within area: delete all the markers within the area masked by the left mouse. Press and drag a rectangle inclosing the markers to be deleted. Set marker: single setting new marker A cross line appear on the mouse cursor for setting the new marker. The dialog for selecting the marker name appears. Set marker per slope: set the tangent to the down (up) ward movement Press and drag from the start point to the end point of the tangent on the slope then release the mouse. Change marker: replace the individual marker. Analog to the menu set marker. 251 Figure 10-9: dialog for deleting all markers within the time range given by user Delete marker: single deleting marker Delete marker from to: delete all markers within the time range given by user (Fig. 10.9) 2.123 Parameter 2.125 From 2.127 To 2.129 YLoc 2.131 DeltaY 2.133 Slope 2.124 Meaning 2.126 start time 2.128 end time 2.130 y-coordinaten of markers 2.132 tolerant values for both direction of YLoc. 2.134 down(up)-slope or all (none). Change marker name from to: replace all marker names by the new name within time range given by user Dialog window is analog to menu “delete marker from to” 252 Show only markers: display the marker whose names are selected by user Show slope: display the tangent to the down (up) ward movement Get time duration: display time duration masked by the left mouse 10.3.2.5 Detection 10.3.2.5.1 Untermenu Figure 10-10: dialog for selecting the detection algorithm Auto detection: let the characteristic events in signal to be detected. User has to select the algorithm (Fig. 10.10) Figure 10-11: dialog shows the parameter for the simple detection algorithm Dialog for simple threshold algorithm (Fig. 10.11): 2.135 Parameter 2.137 Minimum length of an event 2.139 Distance of next peak 2.141 Noise in force channels 2.143 Distance of next peak 2.145 Minimum length of an event 2.136 Meaning 2.138 minimum event duration 2.140 minimum distance from signal onset to the real force peak. 2.142 tolerance in ms from the baseline 2.144 distance from signal onset to the force peak 2.146 minimum event duration 253 Figure 10-12: dialog shows the parameter for the ramp-step detection algorithm Dialog for ramp-step algorithm (Fig. 10.12) 10.3.3 Control buttons GoNr: jump to the go number given in the input text field Prev. go: jump to the previous go number Next go: jump to the next go number Prev. pause: jump to the previous pause number Next pause: jump to the next pause number Go to: jump to the time point given in the input text field Next miss.: jump to the next location where marker is missed which is checked and saved in AnalyzeTapResults 10.4 AnalyzeTapResults 10.4.1 Variable 10.4.1.1 Global variable resultFileName: full result file name used for other m files as title for diagram. AnalysedData: analysed data structure 254 All analyzed information is stored in the variable AnalysedData. AnalyzedData is structured into four layers. AnalysedData is the root. The second layer is combined by the periodic and discrete limb in order. The third indicates the signal event (force, position, etc.). For example AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.referenceOnsets is the onset timing vector of the reference tap of force signal in the case that the periodic movement is the right finger and the discrete limb is the left finger. For continuous analysis (slope analysis) the signal event is replaced by the classification because up to now it is implemented for the dual-task experiment with periodic right hand and discrete left hand. ds1Us1 (ds2Us2): 2-sided down (up) slope synchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) ds1 (ds2): 1-sided downslope synchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) sa1 (sa2): 1-sided slope asynchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) ap1 (ap2): 1-sided slope antiphase synchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) us1 (us2): 1-sided upslope synchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) ap: 2-sided slope antiphase synchronization at the reference tap (next tap of reference tap) ta: tap asynchronization % the periodic limb is the right, the discrete limb is the left finger and signal is the force channel. AnalysedData. rightHandleftHand.force. Following results are saved: % force onset of periodic tap after reference tap where the discrete limb is the left hand referenceOnsets % the trial number vector where the discrete limb is the right foot trialNrs % time duration from the discrete right tap to the reference tap timeElaps % phase defined for the discrete tap normalized by constant ITI phases % compliment phase defined for discrete tap normalized by constant ITI complPhases % dynamic phase defined for the discrete tap normalized by mean of 3 preceding ITIs dynamicPhases % compliment phase defined for discrete tap normalized by mean of 3 preceeding complDynamicPhases ITIs % reaction time vector RTs % time duration between the 5 periodic taps around the reference tap thirdITIsPrecedingDiscrete secondITIsPrecedingDiscrete lastITIsPrecedingDiscrete ITIsClosingDiscrete firstITIsSucceedingDiscrete secondITIsSucceedingDiscrete % Analog time duration between the 8 periodic taps around the go command and phase are defined analogously for the go signal 255 % The corresponding go timing goSignals % Intertap intervals separated into the non-disturbed and the disturbed one for the periodic limb. The onset, trial numbers are also saved. AnalysedData rightHandleftHand.force.unaffectedITIs AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.unaffectedOnsets AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.unaffectedTrialNrs % And the IPT for the heart messurement (down: downslope onset) AnalysedData.puls.down.unaffectedIPIs = unaffectedITIs; AnalysedData.puls.down.unaffectedOnsets = unaffectedOnsets; AnalysedData.puls.down.unaffectedTrialNrs = unaffectedTrialNrs; For the slope analysis: % the coefficient estimates of the discrete upward movement reactionUpEstimates % onset and offset of the discrete upward movement reactionUpslopeOnsetsOffsets % the coefficient estimates of the discrete downward movement reactionDownEstimates % onset and offset of the discrete downward movement reactionDownslopeOnsetsOffsets % the coefficient estimates (slope gradient and y-intercept) of the 3th, 2sd, 1st periodic upward movement preceeding (succeeding) the reference thirdPrecedingUpEstimates secondPrecedingUpEstimates lastPrecedingUpEstimates firstSucceedingUpEstimates secondSucceedingUpEstimates % analog for the downward movement thirdPrecedingDownEstimates secondPrecedingDownEstimates lastPrecedingDownEstimates firstSucceedingDownEstimates secondSucceedingDownEstimates % onset and offset of the 3th, 2sd, 1st periodic upward movement preceeding (succeeding) the reference thirdPrecedingUpslopeOnsetsOffsets secondPrecedingUpslopeOnsetsOffsets lastPrecedingUpslopeOnsetsOffsets firstSucceedingUpslopeOnsetsOffsets secondSucceedingUpslopeOnsetsOffsets thirdPrecedingDownslopeOnsetsOffsets secondPrecedingDownslopeOnsetsOffsets lastPrecedingDownslopeOnsetsOffsets firstSucceedingDownslopeOnsetsOffsets secondSucceedingDownslopeOnsetsOffsets 256 % time duration between the succeeding (preceding) periodic taps and the reference tap intervalsAroundDisturb % phase, dynamic phase and corresponding complement phase (1-phase) defined for the discrete tap phases complPhases % reaction time vector for the force and position signal forceRT posRT % indexes Of reference tap where its downward movement offset still before the go indexesOfRefTapBeforeGo % timing of 6 periodic taps around the discrete tap periodicForceOnsetsOffsets % timing of the discrete tap discreteForceOnsetsOffsets % amplitudes of 6 periodic taps around the discrete tap periodicForceAmplitude % amplitudes of the discrete tap discreteForceAmplitude % phase of go as disturbance but the reference tap is defined based on discrete tap goAsReferencePhases % time duration from the reference tap to the go as disturbance timeElaps % time duration from the go to the next tap of reference tap timeLeft % phase, dynamic phase and corresponding complement phase (1-phase) defined for the go and the go timing phases complPhases goSignals 10.4.2 Menu: Default menu from MATLAB: file, edit, view, insert, tools, help. 10.4.2.1 Resultfile: 10.4.2.1.1 Unternenu Open file: open experimental data file for analysis All interactions over dialog window are analog to the menu DIAdem file in Showdata. Input parameters are the experimental condition and periodic limb. detectAnalyse.namesToSelect = {'Experiment', 'Periodic limbs', ‘event to analize’}; detectAnalyse.valuesToSelect = [{'hand-foot'; 'ST ITI'; 257 'ST reaction'; 'saccade-hand'; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''}, ... {'left hand'; 'right hand'; 'left hand und right hand'; ... 'left foot'; 'right foot'; 'left foot and right foot'; ... 'right hand und right foot'; 'right hand und left foot'; ... 'left hand und left foot'; 'left hand und right foot'}, … {'discrete event'; 'slope'; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''}]; If the old analyzed data should be used then AnalysedData or AnalysedITI is loaded and plotting is available. Otherwise the global variable Header and Result are loaded and depending on the selected experimental condition, task condition and event to analyze one of different function calls is invoked: 1. 2. 3. 4. ProcessRT: single-task discrete reaction experiment ProcessEyeHand: occulo-hand dual-task experiment ProcessPeriodicRightHandSlope: analysis based on the tangent to the down(up)ward movement. ProcessPeriodicRightHand: hand-hand or hand-foot dual-task experiment by which right hand is periodic 5. ProcessRightHandAndRightFoot: hand-hand or hand-foot dual-task experiment by which right hand and right foot are periodic 6. … The running processes of all the modules ProcessX in principal are similar. Control signal and markers of all channels are at first read from binary file and result file respectively in GetTimingVectors() and assigned to local variables for analysis. The timing of the control signal (go-command, pacing signal) are read only once from the binary data file described in global variable Header and signal onsets are extracted. This control timing is stored in a MATLAB format file “experimentFileName_controlTime.mat” for later loading controlTimeFile = [resultFile(1:end-7) '-controlTime.mat']; if ~exist(controlTimeFile, 'file') for i=1:Header.NChannels, if (strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, 'TimeGoSignal')) file = fullfile(Header.FilePath, Header.Channel(i).Filename); FID2 = fopen(file, 'r'); if FID2 == -1, % no file opened. error([' ### PorceeITI: datafile ', 10, ... ' ', file, 10, ... ' not found!', 10]); else % file is successfully opened - get data of old data set if old_data_set == 0; % control varaible aready indicates new data set error(' ### ProcessITI detects old and new data set indicator concurrently (goSignal)'); end old_data_set= 1; % set control of data set version to old data set fseek(FID2, (Header.Channel(i).StartIndex - 1 ) * 8, -1); aLength = Header.Channel(i).Length; goSignal = fread(FID2, aLength, 'float64', 8); goSignal = goSignal*1000; %software clock times are in s - adapt them to ms fclose(FID2); end % Pacing signals to adapt periodic tapping 258 elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, 'TimePaceSignal')||strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, 'TimeTapSignal') if old_data_set == 0; % control varaible aready indicates new data set error(' ### ProcessITI detects old and new data set indicator concurrently (TimePaceSignal)'); end old_data_set= 1; % set control of data set version to old data set % presumably, here the same clock problem is given as before with go signals !!! Must be corrected as well! paceSignal = GetDiaTrigger(i, 1, Header.Channel(i).Length)*1000; % now the new data sets are addressed % elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000GoSignal') if old_data_set == 1; % control varaible aready indicates old data set error(' ### ProcessITI detects new and old data set indicator concurrently (1000GoSignal)'); end if ~isempty(goSignal) error(' ### ProcessITI detects new and old data set indicator concurrently (1000GoSignal-2)'); end old_data_set= 0; % set control of data set version to old data set % load data and determine timing onsets [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); goSignal = (find(diff(amplitude)>2.5) + 1); % '+1' because 'diff' shows the time of last sample being 0 if ~isempty (goSignal) % goSignal = [goSignal(1); goSignal(find(diff(goSignal)>1500)+1)]; else error(' ### ProcessITI detected new data set but no Go-signals! (1000GoSignal-3)'); end % % procedure for the subsequent channels are similar than for the % '1000GoSignal' elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000PaceSignal') [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); paceSignal = (find(diff(amplitude)>2.5) + 1); paceSignal = [paceSignal(1); paceSignal(find(diff(paceSignal)>500)+1)]; elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000RhythReproIn') [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); startSignal = (find(diff(amplitude)>0.5) + 1); startSignal = [startSignal(1); startSignal(find(diff(startSignal)>30000)+1)]; elseif (strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000StartSignal')) 259 [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); startSignal = (find(diff(amplitude)>2.5) + 1); startSignal = [startSignal(1); startSignal(find(diff(startSignal)>500)+1)]; % specific part for start signal decoding (see description above) if ~isempty(find(diff(startSignal)<20000)) pauseSignal = startSignal(2:end); endSignal = startSignal(2:2:end); tmp % temporary vector tmp = startSignal(1:2:end); % copy times where trial started startSignal = tmp; if length(endSignal) ~= length(startSignal); % check for same length error(' ### ProcessITI detects diffent length in endSignal and startSignal'); end end elseif (strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, 'TimePauseResume')) pauseSignal = GetDiaTrigger(i, 1, Header.Channel(i).Length)*1000; elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000LDoldtFB') % Synch-signal from Matlab-PC to diadem-PC landoldtAmplitude [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); landoldtFBSignal = (find(diff(amplitude)>0.5) + 1); landoldtFBSignal = [landoldtFBSignal(1); landoldtFBSignal(find(diff(landoldtFBSignal)>1500)+1)]; REACT = 1; STOP = 2; NOTHING = 3; for i = 1:length(goSignal) ampl = max(amplitude(landoldtFBSignal(i)-100:landoldtFBSignal(i)+100)); if (ampl>0.5) && (ampl<1.5) landoldtAmplitude(i) = REACT; elseif (ampl>1.5) && (ampl<2.5) landoldtAmplitude(i) = STOP; elseif (ampl>2.5) && (ampl<3.5) landoldtAmplitude(i) = NOTHING; else error(['Unexpected landoldt amplitude ' num2str(ampl) ' at landoldt number ' num2str(i)]); end end elseif strcmp(Header.Channel(i).Name, '1000Landoldt') [amplitude, xnew] = GetDiaData(i, 0, Header.Channel(i).Length); goSignal = find(amplitude<5); goSignal = [goSignal(1); goSignal(find(diff(goSignal)>1500)+1)]; end end 260 % End of channel search loop in case when control file does not exist %saving data structure for control signal if exist('goSignal', 'var') control.goSignal = goSignal; end if exist('startSignal', 'var') control.startSignal = startSignal; end if exist('paceSignal') control.paceSignal = paceSignal; end if exist('pauseSignal') control.pauseSignal = pauseSignal; end disp([controlTimeFile ' saved!!']); save(controlTimeFile, 'control'); % control file exists, thus read it else load(controlTimeFile); disp(['Loading ' controlTimeFile]); end The GetTimingVectors() return the variable timing containing all event timing. timing.saccade1Onsets; timing.saccade1StartMagnitudes; timing.returnSaccade1Magnitudes; timing.saccade1Offsets; timing.returnSaccade1Onsets; timing.returnSaccade1Offsets; timing.blinkDownOnsets; timing.blinkDownOffsets; timing.blinkUpOnsets; timing.blinkUpOffsets; timing.leftForceOnsets; timing.leftForceMaxs; timing.leftForceOffsets; timing.leftPosDownOnsets; timing.leftPosDownOffsets; timing.leftPosUpOnsets; timing.leftPosUpOffsets; timing.agonistLeftOnsets; timing.agonistLeftOffsets; timing.antagonistLeftOnsets; timing.antagonistLeftOffsets; timing.rightForceOnsets; timing.rightForceMaxs; timing.rightForceOffsets; timing.rightPosDownOnsets; timing.rightPosDownOffsets; timing.rightPosUpOnsets; timing.rightPosUpOffsets; timing.agonistRightOnsets; timing.agonistRightOffsets; timing.antagonistRightOnsets; timing.antagonistRightOffsets; timing.leftFootForceDownOnsets; timing.leftFootForceDownOffsets; timing.leftFootForceUpOnsets; timing.leftFootForceUpOffsets; 261 timing.leftFootPosDownOnsets; timing.leftFootPosDownOffsets; timing.leftFootPosUpOnsets; timing.leftFootPosUpOffsets; timing.leftTibialisOnsets; timing.leftTibialisOffsets; timing.leftSoleusOnsets; timing.leftSoleusOffsets; timing.rightFootForceDownOnsets; timing.rightFootForceDownOffsets; timing.rightFootForceUpOnsets; timing.rightFootForceUpOffsets; timing.rightFootPosDownOnsets; timing.rightFootPosDownOffsets; timing.rightFootPosUpOnsets; timing.rightFootPosUpOffsets; timing.rightTibialisOnsets; timing.rightTibialisOffsets; timing.rightSoleusOnsets; timing.rightSoleusOffsets; timing.pulsDownOnsets; timing.pulsUpOnsets; A query dialog for timing vector (whether it should be generated in case of marker change or the existing one should be used): timingFile = [resultFile(1:end-11) '-timing.mat']; alt = YesNoQuestion(' ', 'New timing?', 'yes', 'no'); if strcmp(alt, 'yes') isGetting = 1; else if exist(timingFile, 'file') isGetting = false; else Notify('Timging vector does not exist!! quit'); return; end end Missing markers of all characteristic events from the result file are checked if isGetting % start reading the marker from result file for cidx = 1:length(Result.Channel), ms if isfield(Result.Channel(cidx), 'trial'), trialTo = length(Result.Channel(cidx).trial); if trialTo>0 hwaitb = WaitBarN(trialTo, 0, ['Getting markers of ' Result.Channel(cidx).Name '. Please wait']); for tidx = 1:trialTo, WaitBarN(hwaitb, tidx); if ~isempty(Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker), midx = 1; while midx <= length(Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker), 262 m = Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker(midx); if isempty(m.XLocation)|isempty(m.Name) Result.Channel(cidx).trial(tidx).Marker(midx) midx = midx - 1; Result.Status = 'changed'; elseif isContainedString({'s', 'm', 'e', '1', '2'}, m.Name) m.trial = tidx; ms = [ms m]; end midx = midx + 1; end end end delete(hwaitb); end end if length(ms)>0 % temporary timing times = [ms(1, :).XLocation]; % sort the markers according to timing [times indexes] = sort(times); ms = ms(indexes); % initialize the number of markers for every channel startNr = 0; maxNr = 0; endNr = 0; % missing marker timing will be saved in file MissingMarkers for use in showdata faultTime % excluding indexes of timing during pauses indexes ok = 1; if pauseLen>0 % excluding timing during pauses times = [ms.XLocation]; for i=1:2:pauseLen if i==1 idxs = find(times<pauseSignal(i)); else if i<(pauseLen-1) idxs = find( (times>(pauseSignal(i-1)))&(times<pauseSignal(i)) ); else idxs = find( (times>(pauseSignal(i+1)))|... ((times>(pauseSignal(i-1)))&(times<pauseSignal(i))) ); end end indexes = [indexes idxs]; end end if ~isempty(indexes) ms = ms(indexes); end numberOfTimePoints = size(ms, 2); % asign timing to local variables according to the channel % and check missing markers. Save the missing markers in % file MissingMarkers.txt, notify user to correct them in % showdata and return if any marker is missing if strcmp(Result.Channel(cidx).Name, '1000ForceLeft') 263 for i = 1:numberOfTimePoints if strcmp(ms(i).Name(1), '1')|strcmp(ms(i).Name(1), 's') leftForceOnsets = [leftForceOnsets ms(i).XLocation]; startNr = startNr + 1; elseif strcmp(ms(i).Name(1), '2')|strcmp(ms(i).Name(1), 'e') leftForceOffsets = [leftForceOffsets ms(i).XLocation]; endNr = endNr + 1; end if (startNr-endNr)>1 faultTime = [faultTime ms(i).XLocation]; disp(['Missing right force end marker at time: ' num2str(ms(i).XLocation) ' ms']); ok = 0; endNr = endNr + 1; elseif (endNr-startNr)>0 faultTime = [faultTime ms(i).XLocation]; disp(['Missing right force start marker at time: ' num2str(ms(i).XLocation) ' ms']); ok = 0; startNr = startNr + 1; end end if ~ok MissingMarkers; return; end ……. The missing timing is saved in file “MissingMarkers.txt” used for correction in Showdata. function MissingMarkers; % dialog for selecting the location for the file MissingMarkers.txt detectUiParam.prompText = 'File path and file name for missing marks'; detectUiParam.defaultValueHeader = 'default'; detectUiParam.customValueHeader = 'custom values'; detectUiParam.defaultNames = {'Path'}; detectUiParam.defaultValues = {'C:\BioCybCodes\MissingMarkers.txt'}; qq = AutoDetectUIInput(detectUiParam); if ischar(qq) return; end file = char(qq.customValues(1)); FID = fopen(file, 'w'); for i=1:length(faultTime) fprintf(FID,'%s\n', num2str(faultTime(i))); end fclose(FID); return; end If there is any missed marker of any channel the program (function MissingMarkers()) will notify the user and exit. Correction has to be done in Showdata. Open the experiment file in Showdata and click 264 on the button MissingMarker. The next missed one is present on a click. The click without moving on the screen indicates the last missed marker is reached. After successful reading of correct markers, the next valid index of the discrete tap is searched for every go command (for j = 1:goLen). If several discrete taps are found, only the first after the go command is selected and the user is notified. if j < goLen, leftHandForceReactionIdxs = find((leftForceOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (leftForceOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); leftHandPosReactionIdxs = find((leftPosDownOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (leftPosDownOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); leftAgonistReactionIdxs = find((agonistLeftOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (agonistLeftOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); leftAntagonistReactionIdxs = find((antagonistLeftOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (antagonistLeftOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); leftFootForceReactionIdxs = find((leftFootForceDownOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (leftFootForceDownOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); leftFootPosReactionIdxs = find((leftFootPosDownOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (leftFootPosDownOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); rightFootForceReactionIdxs = find((rightFootForceDownOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (rightFootForceDownOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); rightFootPosReactionIdxs = find((rightFootPosDownOnsets > goSignal(j)) & (rightFootPosDownOnsets < goSignal(j + 1))); else leftHandForceReactionIdxs = find(leftForceOnsets> goSignal(j)); leftHandPosReactionIdxs = find(leftPosDownOnsets> goSignal(j)); leftAgonistReactionIdxs = find(agonistLeftOnsets> goSignal(j)); leftAntagonistReactionIdxs = find(antagonistLeftOnsets> goSignal(j)); leftFootPosReactionIdxs = find(leftFootPosDownOnsets> goSignal(j)); leftFootForceReactionIdxs = find(leftFootForceDownOnsets> goSignal(j)); rightFootForceReactionIdxs = find(rightFootForceDownOnsets> goSignal(j)); rightFootPosReactionIdxs = find(rightFootPosDownOnsets> goSignal(j)); end 265 And the actual index of the periodic tap corresponding to the actual discrete tap is calculated by: if ~exist('numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce', 'var') numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce = length(find(rightForceOnsets <= leftForceOnsets(leftHandForceReactionIdx))); periodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForceIdx = numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce; else numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce = length(find(rightForceOnsets(periodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForc eIdx+1:end) < leftForceOnsets(leftHandForceReactionIdx))); periodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForceIdx = periodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForceIdx + numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce; end The variables numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForce, numberPeriodicForcePrecedingLeftFootForce, etc. are the corresponding processed number of periodic tap which serve skipping the already processed periodic tap in the next searching. periodicForcePrecedingLeftHandForceIdx, periodicForcePrecedingLeftFootForceIdx, etc. are the current processing indexes for the periodic tap before discrete left hand and discrete left foot etc. respectively. Five periodic taps around this discrete taps are then extracted. The reference tap is defined as the last periodic tap succeeding discrete tap or go command (as stimulus). The interaction results such as trial numbers, phases, distances between reference tap to these five taps, etc. are calculated in function GetInteractionResults with the following input parameter: 2.147 Parameter 2.149 Path 2.151 periodicOnsets 2.153 discreteOnset 2.155 goSignal 2.157 periode 2.159 trialNr 2.148 Meaning 2.150 the saving path 2.152 5 periodic taps around the reference tap 2.154 discrete event timing 2.156 the actual go timing 2.158 standard period 2.160 the actual trial number ITIsAround = zeros(3,1); for k = [3 2 1] ITIsAround(k,1) = periodicOnsets(4-(k-1)) - periodicOnsets(4-k); end N = mean(ITIsAround); if (strcmp(path, 'rightHandLeftHand.force')) AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.goSignals = [AnalysedData.rrightHandLeftHand.force.goSignals goSignal]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.referenceOnsets = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.referenceOnsets periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.trialNrs = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.trialNrs trialNr]; 266 AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.RTs = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.RTs discreteOnset - goSignal]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.thirdITIsPrecedingDiscrete = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.thirdITIsPrecedingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(1) – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.secondITIsPrecedingDiscrete = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.secondITIsPrecedingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(2) – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.lastITIsPrecedingDiscrete = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.lastITIsPrecedingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(3) – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.ITIsClosingDiscrete = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.ITIsClosingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(5) – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.firstITIsSucceedingDiscrete = [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.firstITIsSucceedingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(6) – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.secondITIsSucceedingDiscrete [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.secondITIsSucceedingDiscrete (periodicOnsets(7) – periodicOnsets(4)]; = AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.timeElaps = … [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.timeElaps discreteOnset – periodicOnsets(4)]; AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.timeLeft = … [AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.timeLeft periodicOnsets(5) - discreteOnset]; AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.phases = [AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.phases AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.timeElaps(end)/periode]; AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.complPhases = [AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.complPhases AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.timeLeft(end)/periode]; AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.dynamicPhases = [AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.dynamicPhases AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.timeElaps(end)/N]; AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.complDynamicPhases = [AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.complDynamicPhases AnalysedData.rightHandLeftHand.force.timeLeft(end)/N]; ITI that happened outside the pacing signal and not contained the discrete taps are recorded as undisturbed ITI (AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.unaffectedITIs). The two outer pacing events are extracted for every grouped pacing signal. 267 paceOuterSignal firstPaceOfGroupFound = 0; for i=1:length(paceSignal)-1 if (paceSignal(i+1)-paceSignal(i))<(periode+100) if firstPaceOfGroupFound==0 paceOuterSignal = [paceOuterSignal paceSignal(i)]; firstPaceOfGroupFound = 1; end else firstPaceOfGroupFound = 0; paceOuterSignal = [paceOuterSignal paceSignal(i)]; end end paceOuterSignal = [paceOuterSignal paceSignal(end)]; The excluded periodic interval considered as disturbed for several discrete taps in multiple limb condition is the maximum interval inclosing all theses discrete taps. The process runs through every periodic tap. For the first pacing it checks the current ITI whether the first and second tap are before this pacing. If they are before then the current ITI and trial number are saved. for j=1:rightForceLen-1 if (paceIdx==1) trial = find(startSignal<=paceOuterSignal(paceIdx)); trialNr = trial(end); if (rightForceOnsets(j)<paceOuterSignal(paceIdx) && rightForceOnsets(j+1)<paceOuterSignal(paceIdx)) AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.unaffectedITIs(j) = rightForceOnsets(j+1) - rightForceOnsets(j); AnalysedData.rightHandleftHand.force.unaffectedTrialNrs(j) = trialNr; Otherwise it increases the pacing index and goes to the next periodic tap else paceIdx = paceIdx + 1; continue; end For the next pacing the process checks whether the two taps of current ITI are between the last pacing of the previous trial and the first pacing of the next trial or after the last pacing. else % for the next pacing if (paceIdx<paceLen) if (periodicEvents(j)<=paceOuterSignal(paceIdx) || periodicEvents(j+1)<paceOuterSignal(paceIdx) ) % go to next periodic tap if it is still before the previous pacing continue; elseif (periodicEvents(j)>=paceOuterSignal(paceIdx+1) periodicEvents(j+1)>=paceOuterSignal(paceIdx+1) ) % increase the pacing index if the second tap of % current ITI exceeds the next pacing paceIdx = paceIdx + 2; continue; end elseif (periodicEvents(j)<=paceOuterSignal(end) || periodicEvents(j+1)<=paceOuterSignal(end)) % go to next periodic tap if it is still before the previous pacing 268 || continue; end end If the two periodic taps still lies between the two pacing then they will be checked for their inclosing in the maximum interval which contains all the possible discrete taps. If none of discrete tap was found then the actual ITI is saved. In the case that three discrete movements exist if (sizeOfDiscreteEvents2>0 && sizeOfDiscreteEvents3>0 % if 3 discrete movements exist rightLimit = discreteEvents1(discreteIdx1); rightLimit = min(rightLimit, discreteEvents2(discreteIdx2)); if (periodicEvents(j)<rightLimit && periodicEvents(j+1)<rightLimit) % if the current ITI is still before all discrete movements then the current ITI and trial number are saved unaffectedITIs(j) = periodicEvents(j+1) - periodicEvents(j); unaffectedTrialNrs(j) = trialNr; else Otherwise the index of the relevant discrete is increased. % otherwise if (periodicEvents(j)>=discreteEvents1(discreteIdx1) || periodicEvents(j+1)>=discreteEvents1(discreteIdx1)) % if any periodic tap of the current ITI exceeds the current first discrete tap then increase this index if discreteIdx1<sizeOfDiscreteEvents1 discreteIdx1 = discreteIdx1 + 1; end end if (periodicEvents(j)>=discreteEvents2(discreteIdx2) || periodicEvents(j+1)>=discreteEvents2(discreteIdx2)) % if any periodic tap of the current ITI exceeds the current second discrete then increase the index if discreteIdx2<sizeOfDiscreteEvents2 discreteIdx2 = discreteIdx2 + 1; end end The analyzed results are saved in a MATLAB format file. exit: exit program 10.4.2.2 Plot 10.4.2.2.1 Untermenu ITI Histogram: plot intertap intervals histogram Phase resetting curve: plot phase resetting curve in DT experiment Reaction time in simple experiment: plot reaction time in experiment where only discrete movements of the same limbs were required. Reaction time in combination experiment: plot reaction time in experiment where discrete movements in different limbs in any combination were required. 269 ITI before and after reference tap: plot histogram of 5 intertap intervals around the discrete reaction. Phase: plot phase histogram. PeriodicTapDurationHist: plot the histogram of five periodic tap durations and the discrete tap duration. DiscreteTapDurationHist: plot the histogram of discrete tap duration. The plot can be listing (one subplot per class) or combined (divided into synchronization and asynchronization). Go phase: plot phase histogram of go signal. PlotTapDuration: plot tap (slope) duration of discrete tap and five surrounding periodic taps. The first interaction is displayed and the left mouse click leads to the next interaction. From the piece-wise linear function composed of three concatenated segments that models a single change of ramp-step-model we obtain the change onset and the change offset. The down (up)-slope is the tangent starting from the change onset to the change offset. Slope duration is defined as time duration from its onset to its offset. Duration of the taps is determined by the horizontal distance between its downslope and upslope at certain y-coordinate. This y-coordinate is chosen for example in the case of synchronization the upslope offset of the discrete tap with the assumption that it is the end of the driving force because the upslope is usually higher than the rest position of the finger before downward movement. This assumption is justified by visual inspection. In the case of asynchronization the upslope offset of the reference tap is selected. As a mean for visualization the function b = robustfit(X,Y) is used. It uses robust linear regression to fit Y as a function of the columns of X, and returns the vector b of coefficient estimates. b(1)+b(2)*X is the slope, i. e b(1) is the y-intercept and b(2) is the slope gradient. This algorithm gives lower weight to points that do not fit well. The results are less sensitive to outliers in the data as compared with ordinary least squares regression. 10.5 Interaction classification in DT-experiment The classification of interaction in DT experiment is performed in function CharakterizeInteraction. The result is stored in variable AnalysedData with following elements which also are used for visualization: % standard deviation, mean and sigma factor given by user of the 3 preceding ITIs AnalysedData.interaction.std3PrecedITI; AnalysedData.interaction.mean3PrecedITI; AnalysedData.interaction.itiSigmaFactor; % interactions by which the reactions were before the go commands AnalysedData.interaction.predictedInterval; % times of these predicted reactions AnalysedData.interaction.predictedTimePoint; % coressponding reaction times AnalysedData.interaction.predictedRT; % interactions by which the reactions were delayed for synchronization AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentInterval; % times of these reactions AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentTimePoint; % coressponding reaction times AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentRT % periodic tapw were hastened for synchronization 270 AnalysedData.interaction.hasteningInterval AnalysedData.interaction.hasteningTimePoint AnalysedData.interaction.hasteningRT % periodic tap was interrupt and resumed after reaction AnalysedData.interaction.resetPauseInterval; AnalysedData.interaction.resetPauseTimePoint; AnalysedData.interaction.resetPauseRT; % reaction was delayed and periodic process was resumed AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentResetInterval; AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentResetTimePoint; AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentResetRT; % reaction was delayed and periodic tap was hastened AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentHasteningInterval; AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentHasteningTimePoint; AnalysedData.interaction.entrainmentHasteningRT; % no sign of interaction or weak interaction AnalysedData.interaction.stableInterval; AnalysedData.interaction.stableTimePoint; AnalysedData.interaction.stableRT; % the corresponding number of classes AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPhaseEntrainment; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPhaseHastening; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPhaseResetPause; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfInterruption; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPhaseEntrainmentReset; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPhaseEntrainmentHastening; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfStableCoordination; AnalysedData.interaction.numberOfPredictedStimulus; Following are the input parameters: detectUiParam.defaultValueHeader = 'default'; detectUiParam.customValueHeader = 'custom values'; detectUiParam.defaultNames = {'ITISigma', 'RTSigma', 'Interrupt percentage'}; detectUiParam.defaultValues = {'3', '5', '0.3'}; detectUiParam.namesToSelect = {'Subject', 'Reaction limb'}; detectUiParam.valuesToSelect = [{'AM'; 'BJ'; 'CB'; 'CKD'; 'DS'; 'JS';'KhP'; 'RN'; 'WW'}, ... {'left hand'; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''; ''}]; % getting input values qq = AutoDetectUIInput(detectUiParam); if ischar(qq) return; end % sigma factor for ITI itiSigmaFactor = str2num(char(qq.customValues(1))); % sigma factor for RT rtSigmaFactor = str2num(char(qq.customValues(2))); % percentage for interruption of finger position interruptPercentage = str2num(char(qq.customValues(3))); subject = deblank(char(detectUiParam.valuesToSelect(qq.selectedValues(1), 1))); reactionLimb = deblank(char(detectUiParam.valuesToSelect(qq.selectedValues(2), 2))); And all experiment data file names of every subject wherein the first is the single-task reaction experiment. files = {... 271 '261208AMReactionTime', ... '230209AMBimanual_rep', ... '050109AMRhandLhandRfoot', ... '030109AMRhandLhandLfoot', ... '050109AMRhandLhandLfootRfoot', ... }; Reaction time and timing are read from the analyzed data for calculating the mean, deviation values and the upper and under bound respectively. i. e the experiment data has to be analyzed at first. All the markers of the selected reaction limb are read from the result file of the first data file. if fileIdx==1 if length(file)>0 if ~isempty(findstr(reactionLimb, 'left hand')) RT = AnalysedData.leftHand.force.RTs; elseif ~isempty(findstr(reactionLimb, 'left foot')) RT = AnalysedData.leftFoot.force.RTs; elseif ~isempty(findstr(reactionLimb, 'right foot')) RT = AnalysedData.rightFoot.force.RTs; end rtMean = mean(RT); rtStd = std(RT); underRTBound = rtMean - rtSigmaFactor*rtStd; upperRTBound = rtMean + rtSigmaFactor*rtStd; end Every interaction is checked for the next discrete tap. if isfield(Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx), 'trial'), faultTime for tidx = 1:length(Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial), trialNr = trialNr + 1; if ~isempty(Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker), times = [Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker.XLocation]; [times indexes] = sort(times); Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker = Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker(indexes); % extract the reaction timing for midx = 1:length(Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker), mname = Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx).Marker(midx). Name(1); if mname=='b'|mname=='m'|mname=='1' mtime = Result.Channel(forceLeftIdx).trial(tidx). Marker(midx).XLocation; % check the consistency between analyzed timing and timing reading from result file if abs(leftHandMaxsPoint(rtId)-mtime)>1 disp(['Mismatch between reaction time point and max marker at time: ' num2str(mtime), 10, 'reaction time point: ' num2str(leftHandMaxsPoint(rtId)) ' from file ' file]); 272 continue; else ……… If RT is larger than the upper bound and the corresponding intertap interval is normal then the interaction is classified as entrainment. if (interactionRT(rtId)>upperRTBound) if(intervalsContainReactionTaps(rtId)<=upperITIBound)&... (intervalsContainReactionTaps(rtId)>=underITIBound) ……………. See Wachter et al. (2008) for more details about this process. Name list of ST-Experiments replicating Aschersleben’s Experiments P1: Cornelia Budach P2: Cong Thi Phuong Thuy P3: Thach Thanh Thao P4: Wolfgang Weber 273