Introduction Mount Elbrus, the highest mountain in Europe, is
Transcription
Introduction Mount Elbrus, the highest mountain in Europe, is
Brain Products Press Release July 2012, Volume 43 IN THE FOCUS On the MOVE with Brain Products – recording EEGs at the rooftop of Europe by Simon Brezovar*, Jurij Dreo* * Laboratory for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Ljubljana, Slovenia Introduction Mount Elbrus, the highest mountain in Europe, is located in the Western Caucasus mountain range. Many Europeans are not familiar with Mt. Elbrus, and the belief that Mt. Blanc is the highest European mountain is still per vasive. But since geographically the whole surface of Elbrus is situated on Caucasus’s north side, Elbrus is rightfully called the rooftop of Europe. With 5642 metres it dwarfs neighbouring mountains Fig. 1: Mount Elbrus by more than 1000 metres. Together with our colleague Iztok Cukjati we decided to organize a research expedition to Mt. Elbrus to study the effects of prolonged high altitude exposure on human physiology, especially on the human brain. High altitude physiology While the fraction of oxygen in the atmosphere remains fairly constant up to the outer troposphere limit (approx. 15000 m), the partial O2 pressure decreases exponentially with altitude. This leads to a reduction of inspired and alveolar oxygen pressure, associated with a decreased oxygen concentration in the blood (hypoxaemia). Hyper ventilation may then occur causing a drop in carbon dioxide levels (hypocapnia) (ViruesOrtega et al., 2006). These physiological changes can lead to acute altitude sickness and in severe cases also to high altitude pulmonar y and/or brain edema. Climbers and travellers who are exposed to high altitude environments report numerous cognitive deficits which can persist for more than a year. Problems with memor y, word recall, attention, decision making, problem solving are reported in literature (ViruesOrtega et al., 2004). Although anecdotal evidence on cognitive changes due to high altitude exposure is common, actual experimental data are quite rare. High altitude and visual attention: out of the lab and into the field The objective of our research expedition was to study the www.brainproducts.com influence of high altitude hypoxia on behavioural and electrophysiological markers of visual attention. Our initial plan was to record participants at three time-points: just before the expedition (baseline), just after the expedition (acute changes) and then one month after the expedition (chronic changes). Even up to three months before the expedition we were still unclear how it would be possible for us to record EEGs on the mountain. While we were vaguely aware of certain commercial portable EEG devices we were sure that our budget could not cover these. We contacted Brain Products to ask for their assistance and due to the interesting nature of our proposed EEG experiment they graciously offered to lend us their newest portable wireless EEG system – the BrainVision MOVE. The long-battery life of the BrainAmps together with the freedom of movement and ease of set-up that the wireless MOVE add-on brings was exactly what we needed. We visited Brain Products HQ near Munich and after a very warm reception we received the necessary equipment. Bottom-up and top-down mechanisms of visual attention Our first measurements took place a few days before the expedition. Subjects had to perform visual tasks that permit discrimination between two systems of visual attention: topdown and bottom-up control (Li et al., 2010). Top-down control regulates the relative signal strength of different information channels based on immediate goals and past experience. Bottom-up control acts automatically to enhance responses to biologically salient stimuli. While bottom-up attention seems reflexive and automatic, top-down attention appears effortful, slow, and context-dependent (e.g. on the number of distractors). Participants were first shown a triangle, and then an array of four triangles, one of which (the target) was identical to the previous sample triangle. They were asked to decide as quickly as possible which side of the screen the target was on, (Figure 2). In the bottom-up (pop-out) condition, the colour and orientation of the three distractors differed from that of the Fig. 2: Visual attention task target. In the top-down (search) condition, however, only the orientation of the distractors differed from that of the target. In addition to the visual attention task we also performed a standard checker-board pattern-reversal visual evoked Brain Products Press Release potential (PRVEP) task as a reference for the effects of hypoxia on earlier stages of visual processing. High altitude EEG recordings: keeping a stiff upper lip It would be ideal to perform the high-altitude recording as high as possible. But since mountain weather is extremely unstable and access to electricity is limited we decided to conduct all measurements at the “Barrel huts camp” located at 3900 m (Figure 3) where we were assured that participants could be seated comfortably and mains power was available. After three days of acclimatization at 3400 m we made our final ascent to the Barrel huts camp. But we found there was no electricity, due to “problems with terrorists”, as we were told. A pylon had reportedly been mined, cutting the power supply Fig. 3: Barrel huts camp to the camp. This meant that we had to curtail our ambitious recording schedule. The limiting factor proved to be one of our older laptops and not the BrainAmps, which have up to 30 hours of runtime with the PowerPack. We therefore decided to record just the PRVEP task on the slopes of Mt. Elbrus. Measurements were performed 3 days after our arrival at the Barrel huts. Subject preparation was of course more difficult than in the lab, where ever ything is to hand and it is not -20°C. But since ever yone was highly motivated there were few problems (Figure 4). The equipment functioned perfectly even in the harsh mountain environment. Moreover, as luck would have it, the laptop batter y lasted until a few seconds after the last subject completed the PRVEP task. Fig. 4: A climber before EEG recording www.brainproducts.com July 2012, Volume 43 Curiosity about the results was naturally great but for the next two days our attention was fully focused on the final ascent. On 2 May at 1 pm we were standing on the rooftop of Europe with an appropriate banner in our hands (Figure 5). Fig. 5: Simon Brezovar on Elbrus Final results All EEG analyses were performed with BrainVision Analyzer 2. First we addressed the question of whether PRVEP latencies and amplitudes were affected by high altitude exposure and whether they returned to their starting values after the expedition. While PRVEP amplitudes were not significantly influenced by the climb, the latencies of some components, especially N1 (N100) and P1 (P100) were increased at high altitude (Figure 6). The N1 component peaked some 10 ms (8 ms – 14 ms) later in comparison to baseline. The P1 peak was prolonged for about 8 ms (6 ms – 12 ms). These differences were consistent across all subjects and highly significant (p < .01) even with our small sample of 6 subjects. In both cases the peak latencies returned to their initial states one week after descent. These findings were independent of Fig. 6: PRVEP results Brain Products Press Release July 2012, Volume 43 electrode position but more pronounced with smaller checkerboard squares (15 arc minutes) than with large ones (1 arc degree) which could indicate that the processing of more complex visual stimuli is affected to a greater degree than the processing of simpler stimuli. Figure 7 (Visual attention task results, topographies show times of individual peak P3 amplitudes, time in ms after display of triangle array is indicated) shows peak topographies of the P3 (P300) ERP for the visual search and pop-out condition for each individual. In four individuals (subjects 1 - 4) the P3 is, somewhat counter-intuitively, increased after the expedition. In subjects 5 and 6 the P3 was reduced after descent. These results are consistent across both tasks. Due to the small sample size it is hard to draw reliable conclusions about the nature of these differential P3 changes under hypoxic conditions but they may point to different coping mechanisms present in each individual. We will be able to expand on these preliminary findings after our next expedition to Muztagh Ata (7546 m) which will take place from 3 - 27 August 2012. Our goal is to connect individual ERP results with other psychological www.brainproducts.com Fig. 7: Visual attention task results or physiological variables to try and improve our understanding of the dynamics of the P3 response to hypoxic conditions.