Theatre, Dance and Performance Training Alba Emoting
Transcription
Theatre, Dance and Performance Training Alba Emoting
This article was downloaded by: [5.70.210.191] On: 27 August 2014, At: 04:22 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Theatre, Dance and Performance Training Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rtdp20 Alba Emoting and emotional melody: surfing the emotional wave in Cachagua, Chile Jessica M. Beck Published online: 21 Sep 2010. To cite this article: Jessica M. Beck (2010) Alba Emoting and emotional melody: surfing the emotional wave in Cachagua, Chile, Theatre, Dance and Performance Training, 1:2, 141-156, DOI: 10.1080/19443927.2010.504998 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19443927.2010.504998 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions Theatre, Dance and Performance Training, Vol. 1(2), 2010, 141–156 Alba Emoting and emotional melody: surfing the emotional wave in Cachagua, Chile Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Jessica M. Beck Alba Emoting is a tool for actors to summon emotion at will through respiratory-facialpostural actions that trigger the physiological components of emotion, developed by neuroscientist Susana Bloch. This article briefly explains the history of Alba Emoting, its development, and recent shifts in practice, as well as a consideration of some criticisms of Alba Emoting. The latter half of the article focuses on my personal investigation of Alba Emoting through practice, primarily centring on a workshop held in Cachagua, Chile in December 2008. The workshop had the specific purpose of using Alba Emoting as the basis of rehearsing excerpts of Federico Garcı́a Lorca’s play The House of Bernarda Alba. Included in this is Bloch’s more recent development of ‘emotional melody’, a technique of scoring a play using the Alba patterns as the basis for rehearsal. Finally, there is a discussion of the potential uses of Alba Emoting and its integration with other techniques, from the perspective of a director. Keywords: Alba Emoting, emotional melody, emotion, breathing, Garcı́a Lorca Breathing and emotion In a dimly lit studio, in the small seaside town of Cachagua, I was lying on a couch curled up in a ball sobbing. Tears were streaming down my cheeks and the muscles in my lower abdomen were shaking. I hadn’t felt this level of sadness so profoundly in a long time. A soft voice instructed me to change my breathing. When I did, my tears cleared immediately, the feeling of despair lifted and I was calm. Then the voice instructed me to change my breathing again, which this time sent me into fits of laughter. Why had I been crying? Or laughing? Nothing had been upsetting, and nothing had struck me as funny. Theatre, Dance and Performance Training ISSN 1944-3927 print/ISSN 1944-3919 online Ó 2010 Taylor & Francis http://www.informaworld.com DOI: 10.1080/19443927.2010.504998 Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 142 J.M. Beck Figure 1 An afternoon in Cachagua, Chile, the home of Susana Bloch. December 2008. Photo by Jessica M. Beck. I was in the studio of Susana Bloch, the Chilean neuroscientist whose life’s work has involved identifying and understanding the physiological aspects of human emotion and developing a tool for actors to be able to summon emotions at will. Later that night I was back in the same studio, this time conducting an interview with Bloch about her research and her passion for theatre. ‘This is Alba Emoting’, she began, ‘A story of a love affair between breathing and emotion.’ (Bloch, personal interview, 21 December 2008) Although the connection between breathing and emotion is nothing new, Bloch and her research team are among the first to explore this relationship in the context of Western science, articulating phenomena that many performers have been intuitively embodying for centuries. In Acting (Re)Considered, Phillip Zarrilli maintains: The description by Bloch and her associates of the psychophysiological process of breath control and muscular contractions basic to inducing each effector pattern is strikingly similar to that of the interior psychophysiological processes of some traditional Asian actors and to Artaud’s ideas. (Zarrilli 1995, p. 98) As this article will be focused on the views of emotion in Western performance, let us refer to Antonin Artaud. In The Theatre and its Double, Artaud (1958, p. 134) is adamant that ‘for every feeling, every mental action, every leap of human emotion there is a corresponding breath which is appropriate to it’. The findings of Bloch and her research team, outlined in the following section, coincide with Artaud’s assertion. Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 143 An overview of Alba Emoting Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 1. The BOS Method, named for its founders Bloch, Orthous, and SantibañezH., is the system of using the activation of the breath, facial expression and postural attitude to physiologically induce a specific emotional effector pattern. Alba Emoting is a psychophysiological method for training actors developed by neuroscientist Susana Bloch, derived from the BOS Method1 originated by scientists Bloch and Guy Santibañez-H. and theatre director Pedro Orthous. The BOS Method later evolved into Alba Emoting, a technique that is intended to serve as a tool for actors to effectively induce the physiological changes that occur with an emotion, and is based on extensive scientific research. The research team, based in Chile in the 1970s, set out to examine the physiological changes that occur during the expression of human emotion by monitoring respiratory movements, heart rate, arterial pressure and changes in muscular tonus in subjects (a combination of patients, drama students and psychology students) who were reliving emotional experiences from their lives under hypnosis (see Santibañez-H. and Bloch 1986). The research suggested ‘the existence of a unique association between particular bodily changes and a corresponding subjective experience’ (Bloch 1991, p. 32), which led Bloch to identify six effector patterns that she considers to be ‘basic emotions’ – anger, tenderness, fear, eroticism (also referred to as sexual love), sadness and joy. Each emotion evidenced a distinct pattern of physiological changes. Bloch and her colleagues noted that while many of the changes that occurred were controlled by involuntary mechanisms, others were not. Those responses that could be controlled voluntarily included breath, facial musculature and postural attitude. The research revealed that if one can learn to activate the voluntary aspects of the pattern together at the same time – breath, facial expression, and postural attitude – one can physiologically activate that emotion. Figure 2 is a brief breakdown of the Alba Emoting effector patterns. (For a more detailed description of the individuals patterns see Bloch et al. 1987, Bloch 2006.) As Elaine Fox (2008, p. 24), a professor of experimental psychology at the University of Essex, asserts in her book Emotion Science, ‘there is no general agreement in emotion science on how emotion should be defined’, nor is there an agreement on ‘basic emotions’, a term Fox encourages us not to take ‘too literally’ (ibid., p. 84). In Bloch’s case, she and her team define emotion as: A complex and dynamic functional state of the entire organism, triggered by an external or internal stimulus, integrated in the central nervous and neuroendocrine systems, involving simultaneously a particular group of effector organs (visceral, humoral, neuromuscular) and a subjective experience. (Bloch and Lemeignan 1992, p. 32) Bloch considers the six patterns to be basic emotions because ‘they correspond to universal invariants of behavior – in a Darwinian sense – and are present in the animal and in the human infant either as innate behaviors or apparent at the very early stages of post-natal development’ (ibid.). The scientists continued their research until the Pinochet revolution in 1973, at which time Bloch left Chile to teach at the Université Pierre et Marie Curie in Paris. From her position in France, Bloch was able to continue developing the BOS Method with theatre director Horacio Muñoz Orellana and actors from Teater Klanen in Denmark (see Bloch et al. 1991, Lemeignan J.M. Beck Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 144 Figure 2 The Alba Emoting effector patterns. et al. 1992). It was during this period of research that Bloch developed her system of teaching the BOS Method in collaboration with Pedro Sandor, which she christened Alba Emoting (partly for ‘alba’ meaning ‘dawn’ in Spanish, and partly in tribute to a production of Garcı́a Lorca’s The House of Bernarda Alba [1999] with Teater Klanen) (Rix 2001, p. 209). After the respiratory-postural-facial configurations of the six emotions were identified, a ‘step-out’ technique was intentionally created so as not to have any characteristics in common with any of the patterns, thus neutralising the six patterns. The step-out technique consists of ‘at least three slow, regular, and deep full breathing cycles followed by a total relaxation of the facial muscles Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 2. According to Plonka ‘the Feldenkrais Method of Somatic Education teaches awareness through movement. [Moshe] Feldenkrais observed that at every moment four things are going on—thinking, feeling (emotion), sensing, and movement’ (Plonka 2007, p. 36). The method is named after its founder Dr. Moshe Feldenkrais (1904–1984). 3. Soma is defined as ‘the term used in this field to identify the living body as experienced from within’ (Joly 2008, pp. 33–34, my translation) and was coined by Thomas Hanna in 1976. Using the term soma rather than ‘body’ is seen in somatic education ‘as an opportunity to resolve the gap or mindbrain conceptual thinking and present an authentic holistic perspective of the person’ (ibid., pp. 33–34). 145 and a change in posture’ (Bloch 1993, p. 128). The aim of Bloch’s system is to teach actors to physiologically trigger emotions, but equally to give them a tool to deactivate the emotion by way of the step-out technique, to prevent what Alba Emoting instructors refer to as an ‘emotional hangover’. Despite various publications, Alba Emoting is still in the process of becoming more widely known and practised. Bloch and her team began publishing articles about their research in scientific journals (see Bloch et al. 1987, Bloch et al. 1991), and eventually in theatre journals (see Bloch 1993, Rix 1993). Bloch began speaking at theatre conferences and it was at one of these lectures that Roxanne Rix, currently a professor at Kutztown University in Pennsylvania, first heard of Alba Emoting. In an article entitled ‘Alba Emoting: A Preliminary Experiment with Emotional Effector Patterns’ (1993), Rix reports on a self-conducted experiment with MFA acting students using only the breathing patterns (rather than the facial and postural components), the results of which convinced her that Alba Emoting was worthy of further investigation. Rix was among the first group of Americans to travel to Chile to work with Bloch and is now the highest level Alba Emoting instructor in the United States. Bloch recently published a book in English, The Alba of Emotions: Managing Emotions through Breathing (2006) and a translation of her newest book Surfeando al Ola Emocional: Reconozca las emociones básicas y comprenda sus emociones mixtas (2008) (Surfing the Emotional Wave: Recognize basic emotions and understand your mixed emotions) will become available next year. Until recently, Alba Emoting workshops have been few and far between. The pioneering instructors in the United States – including Roxanne Rix, Nancy Loitz, Hyrum Conrad, Laura Facciponti and Rocco Dal Vera – took years to acquire the number of hours necessary to become qualified to teach Alba Emoting. Alba Emoting has a certification system based on skill and accumulated hours of training, ranging from Certification Level (CL) one to six (a CL4 is necessary to fully teach Alba Emoting). Currently nine fully certified Alba Emoting instructors teach in the United States. The small number of instructors and lack of training workshops has contributed to the technique’s relative obscurity (outside of South America), but this is beginning to change. In 2007 four Alba Emoting intensive trainings took place outside of Chile – in Puerto Morelos, Mexico, in London, United Kingdom and two in the United States (Asheville, NC and Cincinnati, OH). Laura Facciponti, a CL4 and professor at the University of North Carolina, Asheville, has been conducting training workshops that combine learning Alba Emoting with the Feldenkrais Method.2 The first workshop of this kind was co-run with Lavina Plonka in Mexico in 2007, and subsequently more intensives of this nature have taken place in Montréal, Canada in 2008 and 2009 in conjunction with another Feldenkrais practitioner (and Alba Emoting CL3) Odette Guimond. This pairing of Alba Emoting and the Feldenkrais Method is significant. Yvan Joly (2008, p. 33), an internationally renowned Feldenkrais trainer and psychologist, defines somatic education as ‘the field of practice and knowledge belonging to a variety of methods that are interested in learning an awareness of the living body (the soma3) moving in the environment’ (original emphasis, my translation). After participating in two of the Montréal workshops, Joly considers Alba Emoting to be a form of somatic education. In a testimonial about the workshops Joly states: Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 146 J.M. Beck Figure 3 Participants in an Alba Emoting/Feldenkrais workshop engaging with the ‘joy’ pattern. Montréal, Canada June 2009. Photo by Bernard Dubois. For movement, The Feldenkrais Method promotes ideas like reversibility, awareness, intentionality, degrees of freedom, availability to move in any direction, availability to change the direction without preparation, finding neutral . . . Alba Emoting proposes exactly the same ideas specifically applied to the realm of emotional movements. (Joly, personal communication [email], 15 June 2009) The combination of these two methods is attracting a variety of participants in addition to actors, such as Feldenkrais practitioners, psychologists and ontological coaches. In South America, however, Alba Emoting is a popular tool in itself, being utilised in different fields such as education, family therapy, psychotherapy, management and communication (Bloch, personal communication [email], 5 December 2009). Emotional controversy In The Player’s Passion, Joseph R. Roach traces the complex relationship between science and acting theory throughout history: The nature of the body, its structure, its outer dynamics, and its relationship to the larger world that it inhabits have been the subject of diverse speculation and debate. At the center of this ongoing controversy stands the question of emotion. (Roach 1993, p. 11) 4. Written in 1773, published in 1830. Just as neuroscientists disagree on definitions and approaches to emotion, so do theatre practitioners. Denis Diderot’s (1713–1784) famous Paradoxe sur le Comedien (17734) raises the following question: ‘If the actor were feeling [rather than merely playing] the part, wouldn’t it be virtually impossible for Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 147 Figure 4 Yvan Joly and Johnnie Lyne-Pirkis engaging in the ‘anger’ pattern while Laura Facciponti instructs. Montréal, Canada June 2009. Photo by: Bernard Dubois. him to act the same part twice in a row with the same passion and the same success?’ (Diderot in Gray 2007, p. 250; Gray’s emphasis, Gray’s translation). Diderot concludes it is ‘the absolute lack of sensitivity that makes for the best, truly sublime actors’ (ibid., p. 254). His thesis calls for the corroboration of the much-respected English actor David Garrick (1717– 1779), as Diderot was convinced that his own work was ‘totally and irrefutably exemplified by Garrick’s acting’ (ibid., p. 245). Despite several requests for feedback there is no indication that Garrick ever formally acknowledged his contemporary’s viewpoint. In the introductory chapter of the 1888 survey of actors called Masks or Faces?, William Archer (1856– 1924) complains of Diderot’s use of ‘false logic’ and ‘empty paradoxmongering’ (Archer 1888, p. 4) and dishes out scathing criticism of the philosopher for his lack of practical professional experience. Theatre practitioners throughout the years have interpreted Diderot’s paradox to support their own views. Diderot’s champion was the actor Benoı̂t-Constant Coquelin (1841–1909), a believer in the school of representational acting who regarded the paradox as a ‘literal truth’ (Coquelin 1915, p. 56), to the great annoyance of Konstantin Stanislavsky (1863–1938). Stanislavsky felt that Diderot ‘was not understood’ (Stanislavsky in Whyman 2008, p. 46) and dismissed Coquelin’s style claiming, ‘when it comes to the expression of deep passions, it is either too showy or too superficial’ (Stanislavski 2008, p. 26). Jerzy Grotowski (1933– 1999), although admiring the work of Stanislavsky and emerging from his lineage, insists that Stanislavsky is ‘mistaken’ in his belief that ‘emotions are subject to our will’ (Grotowski 2008, p. 33). Lee Strasberg (1901–1982), while attempting to be ‘respectful’ of Grotowski’s work, defends Stanislavsky’s technique of emotional recall as ‘valuable and useful because of its precision’ rather than relying on ‘some hypothetical ‘‘collective unconscious’’’ (Strasberg 1987, p. 179) and even goes on to suggest that Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 148 J.M. Beck the Laboratory Theatre initiates actors ‘into what amounts to a religious order or cult’ (ibid.). Strasberg’s method is rife with criticism, including Richard Hornby (1992, p. 182) equating Strasberg’s reinterpretation of Stanislavsky’s use of affected memory with ‘the decline of the American theatre’. Playwright and actor David Mamet (1994, p. 12) goes on to condemn all attempts at eliciting emotion through technique, including Stanislavsky’s, as ‘a lot of hogwash’. The disagreements – and in some cases, outright bickering – are endless. Alba Emoting has also been subject to criticism. Elly Konijn (2000, p. 107), a Dutch psychologist, maintains Bloch’s research ‘failed to consider that physiological phenomena measured in actors might be related to things other than the presumed arousal of character related emotions’ which Konijn considers to be essential in her own theory of task-based emotions. Rhonda Blair, an actor and academic researching the relationship between acting and cognitive neuroscience, criticises Konijn’s study for not addressing ‘the psychophysiology of emotions’ or ‘neurocognitive processes out of which feelings and actions arise’ (Blair 2008, p. 49). Paul Ekman, the psychologist who specialises in facial expressions, agrees in general with Bloch’s findings, and his own unpublished research found that ‘when subjects make facial expressions respiration falls into place’ (Ekman in Bloch et al. 1988, p. 202). But he does have disagreements ‘in regard to their [Bloch et al.’s] choice of emotions and the specification of the particular facial expressions which characterize each emotion’ (ibid.). Other common criticisms include objection to the commercial trademark on Alba Emoting, the existence of a self-monitored certification process, and the use of ‘real’ emotions in performance. While Bloch’s research focused on the physiological changes that occur in everyday human emotion, she makes a clear distinction between emotion in life and emotion in performance. Bloch defines acting behaviour as ‘behavior produced at will by an actor in order to transmit gnostic and emotional information to an audience by word, gesture and posture within an artistic framework’ (Bloch et al. 1987, p. 1). Emotion in performance shares similarities with everyday emotion in its physiological components and effects on an observer, but crucial differences exist in the stimulus that triggers them and the accompanying subjective cognitive processes. For these reasons, Bloch (ibid., p. 15) believes that ‘to appear ‘natural’ or ‘true’ on the stage, actors do not need to ‘feel’ the emotion they are playing but must produce the correct effector-expressive output of the emotional behavior’, which is consequently in keeping with Diderot’s conclusion. Blair states: The idea that emotions can exist apart from conscious content might be difficult for actors to grasp at first, but this is fundamental to understanding the current science, which defines emotions as body states, while feelings are consciously registered ‘interpretations’ of these body states. (Blair 2008, p. 47) This clear distinction between ‘feeling’ and ‘emotion’ is potentially useful when working with actors. As a theatre director, my research focuses on exploring different techniques to enable me to begin to specifically address and interrogate the question of emotion in performance. The historical and current disputes about emotion in acting theory prompted my own Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 149 investigation of Alba Emoting. However, while the foundations of Alba Emoting are grounded in concrete scientific research, using the technique in practice (in direct application to a rehearsal process or indeed to a theatrical text) is more subjective and open to interpretation. The following section of the article will detail my personal (and inherently subjective) experiences of learning Alba Emoting. Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Investigating Alba Emoting To use Alba Emoting as a director, one must experience the technique by first learning the patterns. There are three stages that one can expect to go through when learning Alba Emoting; the first phase is considered to be ‘robotic’. In this initial learning period, the subjects are encouraged to explore activating the breath, facial expressions and postural attitude in a non-subtle way, so as to begin to identify, control and feel the voluntary components of the effector patterns. The second stage is ‘induction’ which is the time when all elements of the effector pattern are activated together causing one’s own emotional ‘feeling’. The final stage is ‘integration’, which occurs after a subject has learnt the patterns to a high standard and they can be activated in a very subtle way appropriate for performance (Rix 2001, p. 212). In the first year of learning Alba Emoting, I discovered a few significant uses of the tool that include: 1) awareness, 2) creating a common vocabulary in the rehearsal room, and 3) freeing emotions in everyday life. The first training I attended was a combination Alba Emoting/Feldenkrais workshop in Mexico with Facciponti and Plonka. With 36 hours divided between the two methods, there was only enough time to learn the basic Alba Emoting patterns. This initial workshop enabled me to begin to differentiate between the physiological components of the emotional effector patterns in myself and widened my awareness. I discovered that I was unintentionally mixing in elements of the ‘sadness’ pattern in my everyday life by engaging a furrowed brow. Simply by identifying the tendency with part of the facial expression for ‘sadness’, that habitual expression has cleared, unconsciously, from my everyday life. Further trainings in Cincinnati and London were focused more on the practical applications in theatre. In the Cincinnati workshop, led by Rix, Loitz and Dal Vera, each participant had prepared a monologue, which was filmed at the beginning and again at the end of the 10-day workshop. The first time the monologue was performed as prepared by the actor and the second time the monologue was performed using three specific Alba Emoting patterns. This was not a before/after exercise to show improvement; however, viewing both versions of the monologues, back-toback, illustrated the clarity that the patterns can bring to a performance. One could see clearly that in the previous take, there were unintentional emotion mixes occurring from our personal habitual patterns (such as my furrowed brow). In the second take the emotional effector patterns were perfectly clear. At this point in the training we now had a specific vocabulary referring to the physiological patterns, rather than the subjective abstract concept of an emotion. Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 150 J.M. Beck In the London intensive, led by Facciponti, we used Alba Emoting to rehearse a speech or scene, assigning specific patterns or mixes of patterns to particular lines of text or thought-only to let the work go and notice how rehearsing in that way made a difference, especially in the actor’s clarity and focus. For those of us at Certification Level 2, we began to purposely mix the patterns together. The ‘joy’ pattern causes involuntary muscles in the abdomen to trigger laughter. This was also a difficult pattern for me in the beginning, and though I was eventually able to trigger genuine laughter even if it was nothing like my natural laugh in everyday life. But when asked to try a specific combination of ‘joy’ and ‘sadness’, I immediately recognised my own laugh. In the three years since beginning my training in Alba Emoting, my natural laugh is shifting towards the pure ‘joy’ pattern. I also laugh and cry in everyday life more easily than previously. Perhaps engaging the physiological elements of the pattern correctly has counteracted muscle tensions or other habits that prevent me from fully expressing emotion in life. After attending three intensive training sessions in Alba Emoting, I had a confident grasp on each of the six patterns and could even begin to deliberately mix the patterns. I could see and understand the benefit in having this skill as a performer, but how could I incorporate Alba Emoting into my directing work? The trainings had only provided a little exploration into the possibilities of using Alba Emoting with text for performance. Luckily, Trina Fischer (CL4), who I had met at the Cincinnati training, invited me to participate in a private workshop in Chile in December 2008. Fischer is an actor and director with years of experience applying Alba Emoting to a rehearsal process with her own theatre company Looking for Lilith, now based in Louisville, KY. The purpose of this workshop was to apply Alba Emoting as a rehearsal technique on scenes from Garcı́a Lorca’s The House of Bernarda Alba (1999), using ‘emotional melody’. Emotional melody is the name Bloch (2008, p. 87) gives to ‘the sequence, alternation, and mixture of the different basic emotions, with their various intensities, durations, silences’ found within a dramatic text (my translation). This would be an opportunity to experience using Alba Emoting as part of a rehearsal process and in addition to that, an opportunity to meet and work with Susana Bloch. Emotional melody and The House of Bernarda Alba The Cachagua workshop was led by Trina Fischer, who started working with Bloch more than 10 years ago when she was studying on a Fulbright Scholarship in Santiago. Her husband, Juan Pablo Kalawski, a psychologist also trained in Alba Emoting, accompanied the group and joined in on a few of the sessions. The participants in the workshop included Morgan Rosse, an actor living in New York and a CL2; Alison Vodnoy, a CL1 and recent graduate of the theatre department at the University of Cincinnati; and Angélica Aitken, a Chilean actor who is currently working with Bloch in a group called Alba Nova. When we met Bloch, she spoke more candidly about her experiences as a scientist and academic, including her passion for theatre and fascination with actors and acting. As a young woman Bloch had been offered a place at the Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 151 theatre school in Santiago. She turned it down. Vivien Leigh was one of Bloch’s favourite actors: Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 [Leigh] was so fragile and so beautiful, and so great an actress . . . but so involved in her acting life and her personal life that she became crazy. And I got scared. I said, my god, with the temperament I have, maybe this is too much. I will get so involved. The truth is I got scared. I freaked, and went away and I started studying psychology. (Bloch, personal interview, 18 December 2008) Bloch studied psychology but never lost her passion for theatre, writing a thesis on Social Realism in American Drama analysing Arthur Miller’s Death of a Salesman, and later working on a production of Before Breakfast by Eugene O’Neill while simultaneously studying the behaviour of decorticated rats. Her scientific career took her to the top of her field in the study of pigeon vision (an important research area for cognitive neuroscience), but would ultimately lead her back to theatre through the study of emotion. Out of the six-day workshop, the first two days were devoted to pattern review, while the rest utilised the technique of scoring text with Alba Emoting called emotional melody. Fischer integrated pattern work with her own influences as a director, primarily incorporating Anne Bogart’s Viewpoints and other impulse and awareness exercises. We also began exploring the possibilities of Alba Improvisation, moving freely through different patterns at will in response to the other actors and our impulses. In a group session with Bloch, she brought us right back to the basis of the patterns – the breath. Aligned in a row we were told to stand straight and still, as moais – the statues on Rapa Nui (Easter Island). Bloch instructed us to activate only the breathing component of the patterns, which proved to be difficult, as we had been learning how to integrate all the physiological elements simultaneously. The exercise was very useful, however, and improved our pattern work overall. Bloch used the exercise to remind us of the importance of the breath – without it, she maintains, ‘emotion is not alive’. The remainder of the workshop focused on using Alba Emoting to rehearse the scenes. I was working primarily on a scene between the characters of Poncia and Adela, with fellow participant Angélica Aitken, using an amalgamation of both an English translation and Garcı́a Lorca’s (2007) original Spanish version of the scene. Emotional melody begins with both scene partners ‘scoring’ their scripts. Whether this scoring happens pre-rehearsal or in the midst of rehearsals depends upon the individual director (in this case, we scored the text after our first read-through). Bloch (1993, p. 130) considers the scoring of her emotion-based text analysis to be ‘a form of notation comparable to the Laban system of movement notation’. When notating the emotional melody of the scene, one begins by scoring the scene with the desired emotional effector patterns, combinations of those patterns, and numbers to denote intensity. (Though the patterns themselves are straightforward and based in scientific research, scoring a text with the patterns is ultimately subjective, and open to interpretation by the actors and director working on a particular script.) Levels in Alba Emoting are usually on a scale of 1–5, but they are very personal to the individual on that particular day. Nancy Loitz compares the scale to an accordion placing oneself at the midpoint of the Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 152 J.M. Beck bellows. Depending on the given day, one can stretch to a higher 5, or a more subtle 1. (See Figure 5 for an excerpt of my emotional score.) To begin the process of using emotional melody, Aitken and I scored our scripts and ran our lines, but we did not engage with the patterns until we were working with Fischer and Bloch. We were asked to stand facing outwards and run through our emotional melodies. In effect, the observers could watch the scores before we did. We then ran the scene, without text, simply using our scores. We repeated this a few times and it was in these moments that we could see and adjust the score accordingly. Then we began to run the scene with the text. Though following the same emotional score each time, the scene was always subtly different, as the breath would influence the words uniquely in each moment. Personally, I was sceptical about the use of emotional melody from the beginning. As a director, I work with actors in a more intuitive and organic process (in more than five years of working with professional actors, only one actively used a form of actioning or scoring in rehearsal). But working with emotional melody in practice yielded some fascinating results. The first of which was how rapidly the patterns connected us to the text. We spent four of the workshop days working directly on the scenes and our emotional melodies before performing for a small invited audience. In my initial Figure 5 Emotional Melody score for the character of Adela (excerpt from Lorca’s House of Bernarda Alba). Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 153 scepticism I had presumed that by following the score of the patterns I would be ‘thinking too much’. But, in fact, the experience itself was very freeing, and yet had a structure I could rely upon. Perhaps this was because I was at a point in my Alba Emoting training where I did not have to consciously think about bringing all the elements of the patterns together – the patterns had become integrated into my soma. My participation in and observation of this process led me to the conclusion that using the Alba Emoting patterns allowed for a unified psychophysical connection within myself, and in relation to the text. Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Conclusion: using Alba Emoting as a director Alba Emoting has many different applications in the rehearsal process, and emotional melody is just one example. Emotional melody can be very effective but requires the actors to be trained in Alba Emoting, preferably at a CL2 standard or higher, at a stage where the individual patterns are fully integrated and the actors can consciously mix them. However, if a director is familiar with the patterns, Alba Emoting can still be a very useful tool for developing awareness, creating a common vocabulary, and integration with other techniques. Awareness Being able to observe what is happening to an actor physiologically is invaluable. In 2009 I was asked to consult on a production of Orwell: A Celebration at London’s Trafalgar Studios, working specifically on an excerpt of Orwell’s 1984, the Ministry of Love torture scene. Every time the character of O’Brien raised his hand, the character of Winston would have to wince in intense pain until the torture was over. With my knowledge of Alba Emoting, I was able to suggest to the actor playing Winston different breathing patterns and postural attitudes to assist his portrayal. The actor playing Winston thus gained a physical and reliable tool to achieve the Figure 6 Alan Cox and Ben Porter in Orwell: A Celebration at Trafalgar Studios. London, 2009. Photo by Dawn Cruttenden. 154 J.M. Beck heightened state required of the play, night after night. Similarly, when teaching at drama school I was working with a young actor who had to become very angry in a scene. Every time he attempted the scene he was unintentionally mixing in elements of sadness (when inadvertent mixing of the emotions occurs in Alba Emoting it is called an ‘entanglement’). I simply asked him to release the tension in his brow, and his performance immediately improved. Neither of these actors had any knowledge of Alba Emoting, nor did they need to. Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Vocabulary In a recent practical research project, I was attempting to determine whether using Alba Emoting could help create a common understanding of emotion among a cast. I began by introducing the Alba patterns first for two reasons: 1) the patterns are specific and have a basis in physiology, rather than psychology; and 2) perhaps through them we could create a common vocabulary. In this instance, I used a number/letter system for the Alba Emoting patterns created by Laura Facciponti (see Figure 7). Wanting to separate our subjective notions of emotion from the Alba Emoting effector patterns, Facciponti developed the number/letter system to eliminate the tendency for actors to strive immediately for results. The 1’s are nosebreathing patterns, the 2’s are mouth-breathing patterns, and the 3’s are nose-and-mouth combination patterns. Of course, the actors are aware of what the emotion is, but it does help separate the abstract concept of the emotion from the precise patterns. In my research project, once the actors had the basic awareness of the patterns and could differentiate between the physiological components, Alba Emoting became a very useful vocabulary even without using the patterns directly. I could say to an actor, ‘you have some 3b (sadness) in the speech, is that intentional?’ If it was not, they now had the ability to change it. Figure 7 The number/letter system of pattern organisation developed by Laura Facciponti. Integration with other methods Alba Emoting, while a useful tool, is not a rehearsal process in itself. In the workshop in Cachagua, Trina Fischer was combining Alba Emoting with Viewpoints. Pamela D. Chabora, a CL4 and professor at North Dakota Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 Theatre, Dance and Performance Training 155 State University, writes about integrating Alba Emoting with Lee Strasberg’s method in a chapter included in Method Acting Reconsidered (Krasner 2000). Hyrum Conrad, a CL5 and professor at Brigham Young University, Idaho, has developed what he calls ArcWork, which combines Rasabox exercises with Alba Emoting (Baker 2008, p. 105). Elizabeth Townsend, a recent MFA graduate of Kent State University, writes about using Alba Emoting with the work of Michael Chekhov: ‘Alba Emoting stirs the desired emotion through the combination of breathing patterns, facial expressions, and postures, all of which come into play while using a Psychological Gesture’ (Townsend 2009, p. 35). In my own work I have found Alba Emoting to be effective when using it in conjunction with physical exercises from a Grotowskian tradition as well as the work of South African theatre practitioner Brian Astbury. These examples illustrate the versatility of Alba Emoting as a tool. The wider debate about emotion in acting theory is ongoing and likely to have no resolution. However, I do believe that Alba Emoting is a means of exploring Roach’s (1993, p. 11) ‘question of emotion’ in that it offers a physiological deconstruction of emotional states that can be utilised – by the actor at will – to achieve the embodiment of these emotional states for the purpose of performance. What Alba Emoting cannot do (and nor should it be expected to) is offer a deconstruction of the entirety of the actor’s task in performance (including phenomena such as presence, pre-expressivity, and dual-consciousness). Alba Emoting is also not a substitute for an actor’s ability. Bloch et al. (1987, p. 18) states: ‘by no means does such a method pretend to replace the actor’s intuition, creativity and imagination’.Towards the beginning of Stephen Wangh’s An Acrobat of the Heart (2000, p. 6) he mentions that his book ‘should be about opening new doors, not about closing old ones’. That is how I perceive Alba Emoting. No matter what other methods or techniques one may use, developing the awareness and ability to differentiate between the physiological elements of emotion is an invaluable tool for actors and directors alike. References Archer, W., 1888. Masks or Faces? A Study in the Psychology of Acting. London: Longmans, Green and Co. Artaud, A., 1958. The Theatre and Its Double. New York: Grove Press. Baker, A.K., 2008. Alba Emoting: A Safe, Effective, and Versatile Technique for Generating Emotions in Acting Performance. Thesis (MA). Brigham Young University, Idaho. Blair, R., 2008. The Actor, Image and Action: Acting and Cognitive Neuroscience. London: Routledge. Bloch, S., 2008. Personal Interview, interviewed by Jessica Beck, 21 December 2008. Bloch, S., 2008. Surfeando la Ola Emocional: Reconozca las Emociones Básicas y Comprenda sus Emociones Mixtas. Santiago de Chile: Uqbar Editores. Bloch, S., 2006. The Alba of Emotions: Managing Emotions through Breathing. Santiago: Ediciones Ultramarinos PSE. Bloch, S., 1993. Alba Emoting: A Psychophysiological Technique to Help Actors Create and Control Real Emotions. Theatre Topics, 3 (2), 121–145. Bloch, S. and Lemeignan, M., 1992. Precise Respiratory-posturo-facial Patterns Distinguish Among Human Basic Emotions. Bewegen & Hulpverlening, 1, 31–38. Downloaded by [5.70.210.191] at 04:22 27 August 2014 156 J.M. Beck Bloch, S., Lemeignan, M. and Aguilera-Torres, N., 1991. Specific Respiratory Patterns Distinguish Among Human Basic Emotions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 11 (2), 141–154. Bloch, S., Orthous, P. and Santibañez-H., G., 1988. Commentaries on ‘Effector Patterns of Basic Emotions.’ Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 11, 201–211. Bloch, S., Orthous, P. and Santibañez-H., G., 1987. Effector Patterns of Basic Emotions: A Psychophysical Method for Training Actors. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 10, 1–19. Coquelin, C., 1915. Art and the Actor. Trans. A.L. Alger. New York: Dramatic Museum of Columbia University. Diderot, D., 1883. The Paradox of Acting. Trans. W.H. Pollack. London: Chatto & Windus. Fox, E., 2008. Emotion Science: Cognitive and Neuroscientific Approaches to Understanding Human Emotions. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Garcı́a Lorca, F., 2007. La casa de Bernarda Alba, Yerma, Doña Rosita la soltera. 5th ed. Santiago de Chile: Empresa Editora Zig-Zag. Garcı́a Lorca, F., 1999. The House of Bernarda Alba. Adapted in English by E. Mann. New York: Dramatists Play Service. Gray, J., 2007. Diderot, Garrick, and the Art of Acting. The Age of Johnson, 19, 243–273. Grotowski, J., 2008. ‘Reply to Stanislavski.’ Trans. K. Salata. TDR: The Drama Review, 52 (2) (T198), 31–39. Hornby, R., 1992. The End of Acting: A Radical View. New York: Applause Books. Joly, Y., 2009. [email] (Personal communication, 5 December 2009). Joly, Y., 2008. Educación Somática: Reflexiones Sobre La Práctica De La Conciencia Del Cuerpo En Movimiento. México DF: Plaza y Valdés. Konijn, E., 2000. Acting Emotions: Shaping Emotions Onstage. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press. Krasner, D., ed., 2000. Method Acting Reconsidered: Theory, Practice, Future. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Lemeignan, M., Aguilera-Torres, N. and Bloch, S., 1992. Emotional Effector Patterns of Basic Emotions: Recognition of Expression. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 12 (2), 173–188. Mamet, D., 1994. True and False: Heresy and Common Sense for the Actor. London: Faber & Faber. Plonka, L., 2007. Walking Your Talk: Changing Your Life Through the Magic of Body Language. New York: Jeremy P. Tarcher/Penguin. Rix, R., 1993. Alba Emoting: A Preliminary Experiment with Emotional Effector Patterns. Theatre Topics, XXXI (3), 143–149. Rix, R., 2001. Alba Emoting: A Revolution in Emotion for the Actor. In: I. Watson, ed. Performer Training: Developments Across Cultures. London: Harwood Academic Publishers, 205–219. Roach, J.R., 1993. The Player’s Passion: Studies in the Science of Acting. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Santibañez-H., G. and Bloch, S. 1986. A Qualitative Analysis of Emotional Effector Patterns and their Feedback. Integrative Psychological and Behavioural Science, 21 (3), 108–116. Stanislavski, K., 2008. An Actor’s Work. Trans. J. Benedetti. London: Routledge. Strasberg, L., 1987. A Dream of Passion: The Development of the Method. New York: PLUME, published by Penguin. Townsend, E.A., 2009. The Mind and Body Connection: Alba Emoting and Michael Chekov Technique. Thesis (MFA). Kent State University. Wangh, S., 2000. An Acrobat of the Heart: A Physical Approach to Acting Inspired by the Work of Jerzy Grotowski. New York: Vintage Books. Whyman, R., 2008. The Stanislavsky System of Acting: Legacy and Influence on Modern Performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zarrilli, P.B., ed., 1995. Acting (Re)Considered: A Theoretical and Practical Guide. London: Routledge.