verslas - Kūrybiškumo ugdymo centras
Transcription
verslas - Kūrybiškumo ugdymo centras
Online ISSN 2345-0932 TARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS: inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika Online ISSN 2345-0932 INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: Innovations, Psychology, Economics 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) RESEARCH PAPERS Vilnius 2016 Online ISSN 2345-0932 TARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS: inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) MOKSLO DARBAI Vilnius 2016 Vyriausioji redaktorė / Editor-in-Chief Prof. habil. dr. DANGUOLĖ BERESNEVIČIENĖ, Kūrybiškumo ugdymo centras, Lietuva / Center of Creativity Education, Lithuania (edukologija 07 S, psichologija 06 S) Tarptautinės redaktorių kolegijos nariai / International Editorial Board Prof. dr. CARL-MARTIN ALLWOOD, Geteborgo universitetas, Švedija / Gothenburg University, Sweden (psichologija 06 S) Prof. habil. dr. Lietuvos MA akad. ANTANAS BURAČAS, Lietuvos Edukologijos universitetas, Lietuva / Lithuanian University of Education Sciences, Lithuania (vadyba ir administravimas, 03 S, ekonomika 04 S) Prof. habil. dr. PETER FRIEDRICH, Tartu universitetas, Estija / Tartu University, Estonia (ekonomika 04 S) Prof. habil. dr. LEONAS JOVAIŠA, Vilniaus universitetas, Lietuva / Vilnius University, Lithuania (edukologija 07 S, psichologija 06 S) Prof. habil. dr. VLADIMIRS MENSHIKOV, Daugpilio universitetas, Latvija / Daugavpils University, Latvia (sociologija 05 S) Prof. habil. dr. ANITA PIPERE, Daugpilio universitetas, Latvija / Daugavpils University, Latvia (edukologija 07 S, psichologija 06 S) Prof. (HP) dr. JUOZAS RUŢEVIČIUS, Vilniaus universitetas, Lietuva / Vilnius University, Lithuania (vadyba ir administravimas 03 S, ekonomika 04 S) Doc. dr. DALIUS SERAFINAS, Vilniaus universitetas, Lietuva / Vilnius University, Lithuania (vadyba ir administravimas 03 S) Prof. dr. GUNA SVENCE, Rygos Mokytojų ugdymo ir švietimo vadybos akademija, Latvija / Riga Teacher Training and Education Management Academy, Latvia (psichologija 06 S) Doc. dr. WIESLAW URBAN, Balstogės technologijos universitetas, Lenkija / Białystok University of Technology, Poland (vadyba ir administravimas 03 S, ekonomika 04 S) Prof. habil. dr. ALEKSEJ VOROBJOVS, Daugpilio universitetas, Latvija / Daugavpils University, Latvia (edukologija 07 S, psichologija 06 S) Prof. habil. dr. Lietuvos MA akad. POVILAS ZAKAREVIČIUS, Vytauto Didţiojo universitetas, Lietuva / Vytautas Magnus University, Lithuania (vadyba ir administravimas 03 S) Redakcijos adresas / Address: Kūrybiškumo ugdymo centras M. Pretorijaus g. 7–16, LT-06227 Vilnius, Lietuva El. paštas: [email protected] © KŪRYBIŠKUMO UGDYMO CENTRAS, 2016 The journal is abstracted and indexed in the international databases: Business Source Complete (EBSCO); Business Source Corporate Plus (EBSCO), Index Copernicus Journal Master List. Online ISSN 2345-0932 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : i n ova cij os , p sic h olog ija , eko nom ika I nnov ati o ns , P sy c holo gy , Econom ic s 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) Turinys ✻ INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: • Contents Psichologija • Psychology Irina Plotka, Dmitry Igonin, Nina Blumenau. IMPLICIT ATTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS: EFFECT OF CONTEXT (Monograph) ................................................................................................ 7 Slaptieji poţiūrio matavimai: konteksto įtaka (monografija) Introduction. Basic theoretical problems in research of implicit attitudes. The evolution of ideas about the concept of attitude ..................................................................................... 8 A brief history of attitude research .............................................................................................................. 10 Dual - processes models of attitudes ........................................................................................................... 15 Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes .................................................................................................. 23 Context and implicit attitudes ...................................................................................................................... 28 Overview ..................................................................................................................................................... 33 Study 1. Autobiographical events as context ............................................................................................... 34 Method ........................................................................................................................................................ 34 Results. Study 1 ........................................................................................................................................... 40 Discussion and conclusions. Study 1 ........................................................................................................... 48 Study 2. Experimental events as context ..................................................................................................... 50 Method ........................................................................................................................................................ 50 Results. Study2 ............................................................................................................................................ 55 Control groups ............................................................................................................................................. 74 Experimental groups ................................................................................................................................... 83 RRR-100 ...................................................................................................................................................... 86 RRR-200 ...................................................................................................................................................... 87 RRR-300 ...................................................................................................................................................... 87 RRR-400 ...................................................................................................................................................... 87 RRR-500 ...................................................................................................................................................... 88 RRR-600 ...................................................................................................................................................... 88 Discussion and conclusions on Study 2 ....................................................................................................... 93 General conclusions .................................................................................................................................... 95 Acknowledgments ....................................................................................................................................... 97 References ................................................................................................................................................... 98 Appendix A. Tables for results in Study 1 ................................................................................................. 107 Appendix B. Tables for results in Study 1. IAT ........................................................................................ 109 Appendix C. Factors interactions on RT in subliminal evaluative priming task. Study 2. Figures ............ 116 Appendix D. Research of facilitation scores. Study 2 ............................................................................... 123 Appendix E. Research of correlations. Study 2 ......................................................................................... 129 Appendix F. Research of changes of RRR and D. Study 2 ........................................................................ 130 Appendix G. Factor analysis. Study 2 ....................................................................................................... 148 Santrauka................................................................................................................................................... 150 ABOUT THE AUTHORS .................................................................................................................... 151 INFORMATION FOR AUTHORS ...................................................................................................... 152 Psichologija · T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : i n ova cij os , p sic h olog ija , eko nom ika 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12), p. 7–150 ✻ Psychology Online ISSN 2345-0932 INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: I nnov ati o ns , P sy c holo gy , Econom ic s 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12), pp. 7–150 Implicit Attitudes and Measurements: Effect of Context Irina Plotka Baltic International Academy E-mail: [email protected] Dmitry Igonin Baltic International Academy E-mail: [email protected] Nina Blumenau Baltic International Academy E-mail: [email protected] A particular issue of implicit social cognition is the study of the influence of various contextual factors both on implicit measurement procedures, and changes in implicit attitudes (basic associations with the object of attitude). The papers on the study of mechanisms of contextual influences are relatively few in number. The aim of the research is to study the influence of contextual factors on implicit attitudes and their measures on the example of ethnic attitudes. Implementation of this aim has been carried out in two independent studies. In the first study we researched implicit ethnic attitudes of the participants who reported the presence of events related to ethnic attitudes in their recent autobiographical experiences. These events were observed as contextual factors in measuring implicit attitudes by the method of subliminal evaluative (affective) priming (SEP). In the second study the contextual factor was the event generated by watching one of the videos showing conflict or supporting interethnic interactions or neutral events. Participants performed two sessions of implicit measurements of attitudes by using the procedure of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) and SEP. Retest was performed with different delay times. The program of both studies also included the use of self-assessment procedures of ethnic attitudes and a questionnaire for subjective evaluation of emotional significance of contextual events. The study involved 238 participants of both sexes aged 17 to 50 years. The first study involved Latvians and Russians, the second - only Russians. Analysis of SEP effects was conducted in terms of an approach based on the concept of activation spread in a semantic network. As a result, both studies have found a reliable influence of contextual factors on implicit measures of ethnic attitudes, as well as similar changes in reaction time under the influence of contextual factors in the performance of experimental tasks by participants. These changes were associated with the influence of affective valence of a contextual event and the strength of its emotional INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 8 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT impact. In the second study, these changes have also been associated with the time interval between the contextual event and repeated implicit measurement of attitudes. Contextual events had a significant impact on implicit measures of attitudes (facilitation variables, attitudes, single indices of implicit preference (SIIP), D-scores). It was found that SIIP with stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) 300, 500 and 100 ms and 200 ms is closely related to explicit measures of attitudes. The results support the hypothesis that at different times of SOA there is separate cognitive processing of context conditioned and basic associations related to the object of attitude. Processes that define SIIP with SOA 300, 500 and probably 100 and 600 ms are associated with implicit and explicit (“hyperidentity”) ethnic attitude, while processes occurring with SOA 200, 400 ms are related to associations that underlie alternative ethnic attitude (“hypoidentity”). Artificially created contextual events affected the system of correlations between variables - indicators of basic and alternative ethnic attitudes. Contextual impacts allegedly creating new associations with the object of attitude, prone to more rapid inhibition if they correspond to basic attitude and slower – if they correspond to alternative ethnic attitude. The results contribute to better understanding of mechanisms of contextual influences on implicit attitudes and their measures. Key words: Implicit, explicit attitudes, context effect, ethnic attitude, affective priming, IAT, retest delay effect. Introduction. Basic theoretical problems in research of implicit attitudes. The evolution of ideas about the concept of attitude of all, it characterizes a relation of an individual to some object that is based on evaluations of the information about the object. Since the relation to the object reflects emotions, it is not coincidentally that in general psychological definition of attitudes their most important component is, primarily, ref- In the history of social psychology the con- erences to emotions and feelings. For exam- cept of attitude is one of the central. The re- ple, R. Petty and J. Cacioppo define attitude search of attitudes is a matter of great con- as “a general and enduring positive or nega- cern, primarily because of its relation to the tive feeling about some person, object, or behavior. In a broader context, the im- issue.” (Petty & Cacioppo, 1981, р.7). portance of attitude is determined by how it Other definitions of attitude are centered is related to different spheres of an individu- on emphasizing its evaluative nature, i.e., its al‟s social life. formation as a result of evaluation of the ob- There are various definitions of the con- ject. In the broad sense “evaluation refers to cept of attitude, which depend on the theoret- the actions on differentiation of good from ic views of researchers, objectives, instru- bad”, in other words – of favorable from un- ments and context of the study. The essence favorable (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1994; Ea- of attitude is expressed in the fact that, first gly & Chaiken, 1993). Evaluation is one of T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 9 the basic psychological processes by virtue of evaluation of the object” (Fazio, 1995, its global representation in human mental p.247) that points directly to the associations life, and of the role that it plays in determin- as a mechanism of the attitudes formation, ing the meaning of words and different as- and to memory as the system of storage of pects of reality. The close relations of the mental representations. The definitions of evaluation process with the fundamental be- attitude, focused on the underlying types of havioral mechanisms of preference and information and sources of estimates are avoidance are also noted. It is not coinci- common. In this case, such definitions im- dentally that the definitions of attitude are plicitly indicate a structure of attitude. Thus, organically related to the concept of evalua- the attitude is defined “as generalized evalua- tion, since references to evaluation in this tion of the object, which is based on the cog- case may be related to aspects of the emo- nitive, tional valence of the attitude to the object or mation” (Maio & Haddock, 2015, p.18). This be considered in the context of a broader definition assumes three contensive compo- class of different sources of appraisals. Atti- nents of attitude cognitive, affective, behav- tude is also defined as “the categorization of ioral, and relevant information sources for a stimulus object along an evaluative dimen- concrete evaluations, which are then as- sion” (Zanna & Rempel, 1988, p.319). sumed as a basis of generalized evaluation of affective and behavioral infor- Modern cognitive psychologists mean by the attitude object. In brief, modern concep- attitudes, in a broad sense, integration of cog- tualization of attitude is reflected in the fact nitive and emotional evaluations of an object, that the attitude is the overall generalized which can vary in strength (Olson & evaluative information about the object. Atti- Kendrick, 2008). In fact, in this interpretation tudes can be formed to a diverse range of the issue is that the content of the evaluation objects (or stimuli), such as events, abstract is an association between the object of atti- (nationality), personal (self-esteem), social tude and a positive or negative affective va- and psychological (another individual, social lence, which, in appropriate situation, will group), such concepts as behavior (for exam- determine positive, negative or neutral mode ple, eating behavior, deviant behavior) and of individual‟s responding. More narrow others. Evaluative tendency mediates the definitions of attitude in the context of cogni- relation of certain stimuli (objects of attitude) tivism contain direct references to concepts and corresponding responses. The tendency that reflect the theoretical representations of to evaluate is determined by individual expe- the processes, responsible for formation and rience and should have different expressive storage of attitudes. For example, R. Fazio symbols (Bohner, 2001/2004, р.233). defines attitude as “an association in memory In the definition of attitude, besides the between a given object and a given summary object of attitude, a mental process of evalua- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 10 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT tion is distinguished. What is the object of attitude? Basically, attitude objects can be anything that is evaluated along a dimension A brief history of attitude research of favorability. In order to mediate evaluative responses, Attitude research has a long and venerable attitudes should be presented in memory and history that began in the early 20th century be retrieved from it, when we are faced with (Allport, 1935; Bogardus, 1925; Thurstone, the appropriate objects. The process of actu- 1928) and continues today as a central theme alization of attitudes may require some ef- in social psychology (Albarracin, Johnson, & forts and be controlled or occurs spontane- Zanna, 2005; Banaji & Heiphetz, 2010). Ana- ously and automatically. When experiment lyzing the history of the attitude research, participants are asked to evaluate something, scientists, usually, start with reviews relating they can arbitrary construct judgment, using to the issues of origin of the concept of atti- information stored in the memory. tude. Therefore, in the first paragraph of this However, the mere presence of the object monograph the authors paid attention to the of attitude can automatically cause the evalu- different understandings of the concept of ative response without intentional delibera- attitude associated with different theoretical tion or recalling (Bargh, 1997; Bargh, ideas of researchers. In the early 20th centu- Chaiken, Govender, & Pratto, 1992). Auto- ry, social psychologists have raised the ques- matic operation of attitudes can affect judg- tion about the possibility of measuring of ment and behavior, that, seemingly, have no attitudes and of measuring methods. Psy- direct relation to them, and an individual chologists were interested in the measure- himself/herself may not be aware of it. ment of subjective mental attributes, similar (Bohner, 2001/2004, p. 234). Greenwald and to attitudes in the modern sense of the con- Banaji (1995) introduced the term “implicit cept. Development of new methods contribut- attitudes” to refer to such influence. ed to a better understanding of attitudes and Research of attitudes is of great im- of their change. According to reviews pre- portance in terms of studying of function, as sented by different researchers (Maio & Had- they are associated with realization of the dock, 2015; Blair, Dasgupta, & Glaser, psychological needs of a person (Maio & 2015), the first works, related to the measure- Haddock, 2015, p. 13). There are such func- ment procedures were presented by Thur- tions of attitudes as, for example, knowledge stone and Likert (Thurstone, 1928; Thurstone function, utilitarian function, social identity & Chave, 1929; Likert, 1932). For the first function, self-esteem maintenance function, time LaPierre (1934) indicated the relation- and self-monitoring of behavior. ship of attitude of a person and his/her behavior. There are also known other works related T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 11 to attitudes (Guttman, 1944; Osgood, Suci, & Bohner, 2001/2004). Despite some funda- Tannenbaum, 1957). mental difference between the theories of One of the important stages in the under- cognitive consistency (Abelson, et al., 1968), standing of attitude is the issue of its struc- most of them share the assumption that cog- ture. The evidence of this provision became nitive inconsistency causes the unpleasant the emergence of a variety of works that fo- feeling which has powerful influence on the cused attention on the organization and struc- judgments, ture three- (Gawronski, Strack, & Bodenhausen, 2009). component model of attitude includes the Research on cognitive dissonance (Festinger, affective component (emotions (affect) and 1957), for example, has repeatedly shown feelings), cognitive component - beliefs and that people change their attitudes or their knowledge, and behavioral component - ac- behavior in order to reduce the uncomforta- tions and behavior intentions. (Hovland & ble feeling caused by inconsistent cognitions Rosenberg, 1960; Brinol & Petty, 2012). (Harmon-Jones & Mills, 1999; Gawronski et “Although the basic idea of the tripartite al., 2009). Many researches emphasized the model – that attitudes can be based on differ- existence of cognitive-affective consistency ent kinds of information – is accepted today, or affective-evaluative consistency, as well as the notion that attitudes invariably include levels of their expression. It was shown, affective, cognitive and behavioral compo- (Chaiken, nents is not. Rather, attitudes are better char- 1995), that attitudes with high cognitive- acterized as containing one more of these affective consistency or affective-evaluative elements influencing consistency equally easy reproduced and them” (Zanna & Rempel, 1988; Brinol & stable over time, but those which are marked Petty, 2012, р. 287). More widespread under- by low indices of consistency of both types, standing of attitude is that they are object- demonstrates difficulties in actualization and evaluation associations, kept in memory instability in time. The notion of attitude (Fazio, 2007). strength is usually associated with its high of attitude. as Tripartite well as or decisions Pomerantz, and & behavior Giner-Sorolla, Since the early 1950s, cognitive con- affective-evaluative consistency. Many au- sistency has been a topic of continuing inter- thors draw attention to the “strength” of the est in social psychology. The growing inter- evaluative response itself, noting its ex- est to the understanding of cognitive con- tremality and accessibility (fast emergence of sistency was due to the fact that this concept attitude in consciousness) (Fazio, 1995; Boh- provided a better understanding of the organ- ner, 2001/2004). The strength of attitude, ization and change of attitude (Brinol & Pet- associated with its high affective-evaluative ty, 2012). The first researches of cognitive- consistency, is determined by the fact that affective consistency associated with the any encounter with the object of attitude name of Rosenberg (Brinol & Petty, 2012; causes definite and strong feelings, on the I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 12 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT basis of which further the judgments are lined. (Maio & Haddock, 2015). A number made. Further development of the concept of of new theories emerged, such as the theory attitude strength is related to the fact, to what of cognitive compliance, the Ballance theory extent attitudes are persistent over time, re- by Heider (1946), the theory of cognitive sistant to change, likely to influence infor- dissonance by Festinger (1957), and others. mation processing, and predict behavior The Balance theory contributed to the under- (Krosnick & Petty, 1995). Others have at- standing of how attitudes towards different tempted to answer the question, what is the issues and people are linked in the mind of role of attitude strength in their stability or individual (Heider, 1946). The Balance theo- instability and the degree to which attitudes ry suggests that “individuals tend to maintain are stable versus temporary (Wilson, Lind- and sey, & Schooler, 2000). knowledge” (Bohner, 2001/2004, p. 239). At restore a balance between their Researches related to cognitive consisten- this stage of the development of science it cy prevailed, basically, with regard to explic- was important to understand what factors it attitude measures. However, this construct determine human attitudes. has acquired emphasis in connection with the A lot of studies have been devoted to development of implicit measurement (Fazio persuasion of the individual. Normally, per- & Olson, 2003; Gawronski et al., 2009). This suasion means a change of attitude (when application of implicit attitude measures to new facts about the object obtained) as a investigate consistency phenomena was ex- result of processing of the information, often pected to improve our understanding of both in response to additional information about implicit measures (Greenwald, Banaji, Rud- the object of attitude (Chaiken, Wood, & man, Farnham, Nosek, & Mellott, 2002) and Eagly, 1996; Petty & Wegener, 1998). Fur- cognitive consistency in general (Gawronski ther, various theories have arisen, distin- & Strack, 2004; Gawronski et al., 2009). guishing persuasion by the degree of cogni- Later, the new tendencies in considera- tive effort required for processing infor- tion of attitudes as context-dependent and mation (Petty & Cacioppo, 1981). Those changing over time, as opposed to the tradi- theories that considered persuasion as requir- tional understanding of attitude as “stable ing minor cognitive effort, tried to explain knowledge structure” emerged. the mechanisms of change of attitude, using In 40–60s of the twentieth century the the ideas of classical or operant conditioning. growth of a social cognition perspective, Hence, other authors emphasized the im- which was based on understanding of how a portance of mood influence on value judg- person processes the socially significant in- ments regarding the object of attitude. The formation and how attitude affects the infor- feelings and subjective experiences were mation processing and behavior, was out- seen as special cases of heuristic processing. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 13 The role of heuristics is especially great when emerged in the 80s of the twentieth century. the individual is not enough motivated and Attitude researches frequently noted that atti- able to resort to more sophisticated methods tudes are based on cognitive, affective and of information processing (Allport, 1954; behavioral information. At the same time the Bohner, Moskowitz, & Chaiken, 1995; Boh- two-process models began to develop inten- ner, 2001/2004). sively: the Elaboration Likelihood Model The important point was the emergence of (ELM) (Petty & Cacioppo, 1981, 1986) and another approach, emphasizing the important the role of active mental processes (cognitive (Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken, Liberman, & Ea- efforts) involved in changing attitudes. This gly, 1989). The ELM and HSM are both dual situation contributed to the creation of Green- -process models in that they specify two dif- wald‟s theory of Cognitive Response Ap- ferent routes to persuasion. The ELM refers proach to attitude change (Greenwald, 1968; to a central route and a peripheral route: the McGuire, 1981). The essence of which was central route requires an individual to pay that when the message of persuasive nature close attention to the contents of persuasive emerges, people actively relate its content appeal, whereas the peripheral route requires with their already existing knowledge and less thought. attitudes, creating new cognitive responses Heuristic-Systematic Model (HSM) The representatives of this trend considered both the basic provisions of the Cogni- (Bohner, 2001/2004). It should be noted that a new field of re- tive Response Approach theory on the active search, referred to as the socio-cognitive per- “costly” information processing, and other spective, considered not only issues related to theories that allow the possibility of forming the influence of attitudes on the process of persuasion without special effort on the part information processing, but upon behavior as of the subject. ELM model suggests that the well (Maio & Haddock, 2015). formation of persuasion depends on the influ- The problem of how attitudes affect be- ence of the central or peripheral processes of and information processing, and the central pro- Fishbein‟s Theory of Reasoned Action cesses require more cognitive effort than pe- (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). The theory makes ripheral. If a person has little time, his/her it possible to predict a reasonable and rational resources are limited and there is no way to behavior on the basis of attitudes. Later, it carefully analyze the message, then the pe- served as the basis for the motivational model ripheral processes are launched. The proba- as determinant of behavior - MODE by R. bility that the person will process information Fazio (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, in details is related to the motivation and ca- 1995). pabilities of the individual. “The higher the havior was developed in Ajzen A new wave in the study of attitudes motivation and capabilities of the individual, I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 14 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT the more likely it will involve the central cessing (Pierre, Manetti, Kruglanski, & processes Sleeth-Keppler, 2004) denies the basic pro- of information processing” (Bohner, 2001/2004, p. 247). visions of the previous models. The provi- The Heuristic-Systematic Model (HSM) sions of the universal models suggest that in (Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken et al., 1989) has the case of two types of context influence - similarity to the above model in that the both peripheral mental effort depends on the motivation and (systematic), signals are processed proposi- cognitive capacities of the individual. The tionally, i.e., the semantic level of pro- systematic processing (similar to the central cessing is involved. Moreover, both process- processing) is defined as a comprehensive es are top-down cognitive processes and and analytical, where “an individual in order equally influence the measurement results. (heuristic) and central to form an opinion should carefully analyze The aforementioned two-process models all relevant information and synthesize all had a great impact on the development of a the useful information” (Chaiken et al., new understanding of attitudes in the context 1989, p.212). Heuristic processing is deter- of implicit social cognition. Development of mined in broader terms and involves the use implicit social cognition contributed to ad- of heuristics - simple rules of conclusions. vances in computer technology, as well as Although it is assumed that the heuristic the penetration of cognitive psychology in processing is passive and does not require the social psychology. An important deve- any effort to run it, you need a key attribute lopment was the emergence of the classifica- (e.g., pleasant or credible source), which tion of cognitive processes based on various indicates the possibility of its use (if any criteria. The strategy of cognitive infor- heuristics found in subject‟s memory). With mation processing allocates metacognitive, an increase in motivation and processing basic strategic processes (Chaiken, 1987); by capacity growth a systematic analysis of the the level of awareness of cognitive pro- information begins to be carried out, but cessing by the subject - conscious, uncon- heuristics continue to work as well. Both scious (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Dehae- processes together influence the process of ne, 2007); by participation of attention pro- persuasion, just complementing each other cesses (monitoring) - controlled, automatic and leading to some complex effect (Bohner (Bargh, 1994; Posner & Snayder, 1975); by et al., 1995) According to Maio and Had- the contribution of the subject to the learning dock (2007, 2015), “these models have pro- process - descending, ascending (Norman & vided and continue to provide important Rumelhard, 1975; Ochsner, Ray, Robertson, insights into the processes underlying atti- Cooper, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2009); by the tude change” (Maio & Haddock, 2007, degree of autonomy and isolation of cogni- 2015, p. 22). tive Universal model of information pro- processing - controlled, modular (Jackendoff, 1992; Fodor, 1983). Also, a T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 15 new class of attitude measuring procedures cessing (Devine, 1989; Fazio, 1990; Fazio et called implicit measurements emerged (e.g., al., 1995; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2007; based on millisecond timing accuracy for key Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Smith & de- -stroke responses). Implicit measurements Coster, 2000; Strack & Deutsch, 2004; assess attitudes without requiring individu- Chaiken & Trope, 1999). One of them is as- als‟ awareness of their attitude or how it is sociative, based on associations by similarity, being measured (Petty, Fazio, & Brinol, contiguity and space, time, and the other – a 2009). propositional process which operates on the The emergence of new technologies for basis of logical analysis and reasoning attitude measurement over time will provide (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2007; Payne & a new theoretical interpretation of the atti- Gawronski, 2010). tude construct; contribute to the creation of It is a common feeling that both the ex- various directions in implicit social cogni- plicit and implicit attitudes can be seen as the tion, as well as models explaining the nature, result of the learning process. The individual, origin and attitude changes. Interestingly, a when confronted with the objects of the sur- research has revealed that in many domains, rounding world, forms an attitude towards responses on implicit measures of attitude them, based on the experience of subject- are often not consistent with people's self- object interactions on behavioral, cognitive reported ratings of their own attitudes (Petty and/or affective level. The results of this in- et al., 2009). Similarly, implicit measures of teraction are recorded in memory in the form attitude often predict different outcomes of estimates of object properties, which are from self-report measurements (Perugini, various in content and the level of generaliza- Richetin, & Zogmaister, 2010). tion, i.e., they are the product of explicit or implicit learning. Traditional theories of human learning Dual - processes models of attitudes note the dual nature of its mechanisms. In the basis of the first learning mechanism there is automatic and unconscious formation of associative links. The basis of the second In the literature dealing with the problem of mechanism is the formation of conscious and studying attitudes authors are conducting conclusions. Both mechanisms correspond to numerous disputes concerning the under- two independent and competing systems – standing of the concept of attitude and theo- associative and propositional. The first sys- retical models explaining its nature. Basical- tem generates associative knowledge about ly, the most of theoretical models present the world, the second, in the end - the propo- implicit and explicit attitudes as two qualita- sitional. The propositional system has a hig- tively different modes of psychological pro- her order. Associations and propositions as a I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 16 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT form of existence of knowledge differ in the al conceptualization of attitude, which had way of mental representations, as well as the the largest circulation in the initial period of conditions under which they are activated the study of implicit attitudes (Greenwald & and direct behavior (Mitchell, De Houwer, & Banaji, 1995; Smith & DeCoster, 2000; Wil- Lovibond, 2009). These views have contrib- son et al., 2000). As part of the dispositional uted to the theoretical understanding of the approach, there is a streamline which states concept of implicit attitude. that the explicit and implicit attitudes have a One of the first assumptions for distin- same underlying mental representation that is guishing the theories was the extent to which stored in the memory, and the second line they rely on the associative and propositional considers that there are two separate mental approaches in interpreting the results of ex- representations. In terms of the second plicit and implicit measurement of attitudes, streamline both the implicit and explicit atti- as well as in understanding the nature of atti- tudes are separate and do not interact with tudes in general. each other by mental representations of eval- Explicit attitudes have been understood as uations of the same object. a product of primarily propositional, in fact Implicit estimates are a reflection of men- logical processes, and implicit - as a result of tal associations, formed in the early stages of associative. acquisition of the social experience (De Hart, The first processes are deliberate, con- Pelham, & Tennen, 2006; Rudman, 2004). scious, slow, the second - fast, automatic, Upon completion of its formation, these asso- unconscious. If the propositional processes ciations are accepted as unchangeable, stable are based on syllogistic conclusions made over time and resistant to contextual influ- according to the rules, then the associative ences. From this perspective, implicit atti- processes are related to the formation and tudes are automatic evaluations, while, in activation of associations resulting from the contrast, explicit attitudes are associated with effects of spatiotemporal contiguity of events recent and contextual effects and are not au- and perceptual similarity between stimuli. tomatic. In addition to the notion that implicit The associational approach assumes that and explicit attitudes have different mental the attitude is a form of mental representa- representations, as part of dispositional ap- tion, stored in the memory, i.e., represented proach a different understanding has been as a memory trace with a certain structure in suggested: implicit and explicit attitudes have a finished form for subsequent use. Elemen- common representation in memory, but the tary mental representations are, for example, difference between them is specified by the the stored in the memory simple associations level of mental processes which determine between evaluations and the object of atti- the observed behavior, for example, when tude. Such a view corresponds to disposition- direct and indirect measurements of attitudes T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT (Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999; Olson & 17 & Bodenhausen, 2006). Fazio, 2009). The first process is automatic, As an alternative to the associative ap- the second is deliberative. Which of the pro- proach to the understanding of implicit atti- cesses will be dominant is determined by tudes a functional approach was proposed, motivation and the ability to carry out delib- which is the basis of the model of attitudes erate and full evaluation of the object. called The relational elaboration and coher- In turn, the proponents of the proposition- ence (REC) model (Barnes-Holmes, D., al approach have been developing their ideas Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, & Boles, 2010) within the frameworks of structuralistic con- and implicit procedure for their measurement cepts. The main difference between the prop- of IRAP (Implicit Relational Assessment ositional and dispositional (strictly associa- Procedure) (Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes- tional) approach is the refusal of the under- Holmes, Y., Power, Hayden, Milne, & Stew- standing of attitudes as unchangeable and art, 2006). The IRAP procedure is used in a static structures, stored in memory and di- growing number of research and its results rectly determining the responses observed in correspond with the results of alternative the measurements of attitudes. An alternative methods of implicit measurement. The func- idea has been reduced to the notion of atti- tional approach in some respects is similar to tudes as a result of instant associations the propositional, though they are two differ- formed under the influence of cognitive, mo- ent levels of analysis. However, in future we tivational and contextual factors. In this can expect their convergence. sense, the formation of attitude is the result Scheme 1 (Fig.1) presents the classifica- of high-level processing, where the reasoning tion of approaches and the most influential plays an important role. The dominant role of contemporary models of attitudes, developed these associations formed does not change in the framework of these approaches. Loca- the recognition of the existence and stored in tion of model designations from left to right memory relevant object-associative evalua- on the Scheme represents the degree of re- tions and their participation in the formation moteness of their assumptions from exclu- of attitude. sively associational to propositional. Thus, the proponents of the propositional approach recognize that although the implicit The above Scheme shows that the major dual process models include: and explicit attitudes have separate and dis- The Single attitude model – Motivation tinct mental representations, associated with and Opportunity as Determinants (MODE) different processes and manifest themselves model (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams in different situations, the associative and 1995; Fazio, 2007; Olson & Fazio, 2009); the propositional processes interact and must be model of double attitudes (Wilson et al., considered in models of attitudes (Gawronski 2000), the meta-cognitive model (MCM) I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 18 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.1. The theoretical framework of authoritative contemporary models of attitudes and their classification (Petty & Brinol, 2006; Petty, Brinol, & De- & Fazio, 2009). “To the degree that this asso- Marree, 2007), the associative-propositional ciation is sufficiently strong, the evaluation evaluation (APE) model (Gawronski & Bo- associated with the object may be activated denhausen's, 2006a, 2006b, 2007), the con- automatically when encountering that object structivist 2007; (i.e., without intention to evaluate the ob- Schwarz & Bohner, 2001). Subsequently, we ject)” (Gawronski & Sritharan, 2010 p. 217). will focus in more detail on the characteris- This model is mainly focused on the study of tics of each model. association between attitude and behavior. It approach (Schwarz, was one of the first models to integrate the experiences of implicit attitudes measuring, Single Attitude Model based on the reaction time and the measurement of explicit attitudes, based on paper-and The MODE model. The theoretical foun- -pencil measures (Rydell & McConell, dation of the MODE model is in the defini- 2010). According to this approach, explicit tion of attitude as the mental association be- and implicit attitudes have a same underlying tween an object and one‟s summary evalua- mental representation that is stored in the tion of that object (Fazio, 2007; Petty, Brinol, memory, but their differences are determined T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 19 by the level of mental processes that deter- automatically, and explicit attitude, which mine behavior. In that case, if the motivation requires cognitive efforts to retrieve it from and the opportunity to participate in the labor memory (Gawronksi & Sritharan, 2010). -intensive processes is low, then the subse- Similarity to the MODE model is that the quent behavior is under the influence of auto- results of compliance of the direct and indi- matically activated attitude, and if they are rect measurements depend on the level of high, the behavior is caused by “thoughtful cognitive effort and of participants‟ interest attitudes” (Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999; in the results of measurements. The differ- Petty & Brinol, 2010). Direct and indirect ence of the double attitudes model is a differ- measurements can reflect the same results if ent understanding of the origin of explicit in both types of measurements the intellectu- and implicit attitudes. ally intensive processes are involved identi- The Dual Attitude Model assumes the cally. Indirect measurements, such as the presence of two mental representations, procedure of evaluative priming (Fazio et al., which are separate and do not interact with 1995) help to reduce the level of cognitive each other. Within the framework of this effort in solving the task, and thus provide an model, the implicit attitude is associated with alternative to the automatic activation of atti- persistent memory structures, while the ex- tudes. plicit attitude – with a new human experi- Many researchers have suggested that the ence that has not yet entered into the deeper influence of contextual factors on the implicit memory structures, but already relevant and measurement of attitudes is unlikely. Despite used. The discrepancy in the results of meas- this, the others admit the possibility of chang- urements is due to the influence of the deep es in implicit measurements under the influ- (stable) memory structures. Results of the ence of context (Barden, Maddux, Petty, & consistency between direct and indirect Brewer, 2004; Blair, Ma, & Lenton, 2001; measurements depend on the level of cogni- Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001; Wittenbrink, tive efforts and participant‟s interest in meas- Judd, & Park, 2001). urement results (Wilson et al, 2000). The dual attitude model admits that when attitudes are changing, the old implicit attitude Dual Attitude Model still stays in memory, influencing people's judgment and behavior when they are not Dual Attitude Model. According to the Dual able or not motivated to participate in the Attitude Model (Wilson, Lindsay, & Shool- labor-intensive process of extracting their ers, 2000) people often have two attitudes new explicit attitude from the memory. towards the same object stored in the The main achievements of this approach memory: implicit attitude, which is activated are related to the way the attitude is formed I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 20 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT and varies. Same as in the MODE model, the order to extract this label from memory. dual-attitude model states that the associa- The Meta - Cognitive Model primarily tions reflected in implicit measures are high- focuses on the differences between direct ly overlearned and stable (Payne & Gawron- and indirect measurements of attitudes ski, 2010; Gawronksi & Sritharan, 2010). (Petty & Brinol, 2006; Petty et al., 2007). The Dual Attitude Model implies that Both types of attitude can be activated auto- simple context effects are more likely for matically in the event of their equal value, explicit measures. which can lead to a neutral evaluation of indirect measurement results, since simulta- Metacognitive Model (MCM). The MCM neous activation of antagonistic attitudes is a relatively new model, offered by Petty contributes to the formation of implicit am- and Brinol in 2006. Similar to the MODE bivalence (De Liver, Vander Pligt, & Wig- model, it assumes that attitudes constitute boldus, 2007). The effect of conformity be- objects evaluations, kept in memory. But, tween the measurements can be expected in MCM postulates that objects of the attitudes the case where both the old and the new in- can, sometimes, be simultaneously connect- formation regarding the object of attitude ed to the positive and negative valence have roughly the same evaluative meaning, (Rydell & McConnell, 2010; Petty et al., or if the information is contradictory, but 2009). The difference of the Meta-Cognitive subjectively accurate. According to state- Model from the previous models is in the ments of this model, influence of contextual understanding of how the old and newly factors on the implicit measurement is un- formed attitudes function. In the foundation likely. of interaction there are the tags such as “false While agreeing in general with the as- attitude,” in that case, if the old attitude is sumptions of the MODE model with regard rejected by a newly acquired attitude. In to relatively low impact of the context on the case, if the validity of previously formed (so- results of indirect measurements, the meta - called “old”) attitude is contested by the cognitive model considers the possibility of newly formed (or “new”) attitude, the “old” such an effect. In the case of implicit ambiv- one will be labeled as “improper” or alence, in conditions of enhanced processing “wrong” (Petty & Brinol, 2010). But, due to of information about the object, the signals the fact that the old attitude has more devel- of the situation context activate various sub- oped associative network, and a new label, sets of associations stored in memory, and indicating its unsuitability, is still weak, the some of them may have an impact on the effect of the label on the judgment depends implicit measurement indicators (Barden et on the level of motivation and the cognitive al., 2004; Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001; effort that the person is ready to apply in Wittenbrink et al., 2001). T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 21 The model of Associative–Propositional Evaluation (APE). The model of Associa- attitude model (MODE model) and from the tive–Propositional Evaluation (APE) was dual attitude model in the way that it does not developed by Gawronski and Bodenhausen imply the existence of stable evaluations on the basis of the Reflective – Impulsive stored in the memory (Olson & Fazio, 2009). The APE model differs from the single Model by Strack and Deutsch (2004), espe- The development of this model in the cially in order to integrate different theoreti- framework of the constructivist approach cal achievements into a single structure argues that attitudes appear as needed, based (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006, 2007). on feelings and opinions (Schwarz & Bohner, The APE model recognizes the existence of 2001; Wilson & Hodges, 1992; Olson & associative and propositional evaluation pro- Fazio, 2009). In this regard, various contexts cesses, which are reflected in the implicit and provide access to a variety of emotions and explicit measurements of attitudes. The APE knowledge, which are reflected in the chang- model states the existence of previously es of human evaluations. Any conformity in formed (preexisting) associations in the the results of implicit and explicit measure- memory and an instant set of input stimuli ment of attitudes, according to this view, is (Gawronski et al., 2009). because at every moment the same set of Associative and propositional processes building blocks is being recreated, which are interact with each other. Activated associa- reflected in the current evaluation. The re- tions usually serve as the basis for an explicit sults of indirect measurements can be consid- decision, if the information resulting from ered as a result of activation of information these associations was rejected because of its that has not yet tagged as true or false. Re- inconformity with other information instantly sults of direct measurement reflect the results considered. This presumes the impact of as- of activities of propositional processes – sociative processes on propositional process- when all information is verified and defined es. Furthermore, the propositional process of as subjectively true. checking sometimes may activate new asso- In addition, the APE model has a number ciations in the memory, for example, by of implications for the contextual effects. The means of intentional search process (Blair et structure of the association which is activated al., 2001) that determines the effect of the in response to a stimulus depends on the associative processes on propositional pro- overall set of input stimuli that includes con- cesses. These two processes may be per- textual signals and other contextual infor- formed simultaneously or separately, which mation (Barden et al., 2004; Dasgupta & leads to different results in each of the possi- Greenwald, 2001; Wittenbrink et al., 2001). ble combinations (Gawronski & Bodenhasen, Hence, the newly formed associations are 2006a). connected with the signals of context in I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 22 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT which they were formed. Contextual stimuli and explicit attitudes differ in what they ac- are associated with a subset of the associa- cess from memory or demand different re- tions connected with the object of attitude. If sponses based on framing (Schwarz, 1999). the new association does not compete with According to a constructivist perspective, a the existing, the context value is not reflected significant factor in the discrepancy between in the implicit measurements. In the opposite implicit and explicit attitudes is the time fac- case, if new information is rejected, its evalu- tor. Since the explicit methods require more ation becomes dependent on the influence of time, this significantly affects the design context, as it continues to be associated with process, as it provides easy access to more the subset of associations and effects the concepts than in the case of fast implicit results of the implicit measurements (Rydell measurements (Rydell, McConnell, 2010). It & Gawronski, 2009; Gawronski & Sritharan, is obvious that the constructivist approach to 2010). the greater extent supports the importance of contextual stimuli in the process of construc- The Constructivist Perspective Modeling. Under this approach, all attitudes are consid- tion of attitude, prioritizing the impact of ered as immediately constructed evaluations tude activation. contextual stimuli on the processes of atti- of the object reconstituted under the influ- When conducting the explicit measure- ence of a particular situation (Schwarz, 2007; ments of attitudes people may draw addition- Schwarz & Bohner, 2001). In this case, the al information stored in the memory, as op- term attitude does not refer to stable evalua- posed to the implicit measurements of atti- tive information stored in memory but rather tude when it cannot be done. to the process of constructing evaluations based on the information that is available at the time and the demands of the attitude The Systems of Evaluation Model (SEM). The Systems of Evaluation Model (SEM) measure. “In this conceptualization, attitudes (McConnell, Rydell, Strain, & Mackie, 2008; are epiphenomena and are a by-product of Rydell & McConnell, 2006; Rydell, McCo- the & nell, Mackie, & Strain, 2006) suggests that McConnell, 2012 p.299). Therefore, the no- implicit and explicit constructs correspond to tion that people possess attitudes is conceptu- the fundamentally different cognitive sys- ally rejected. tems. Implicit constructs are related to the construction process (Rydell Implicit and explicit attitude measures associative system, and explicit ones are re- converge when the same information is lated to the system, based on rules. This divi- culled from memory to construct an attitude. sion is related to developments in the field of Implicit and explicit attitude measures will memory research, defining two independent differ if the methods used to assess implicit memory systems – systems of fast and slow T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 23 learning (Sloman, 1996; Smith & DeCoster, cessed, using different cognitive mecha- 2000; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Accordingly, nisms, may lead to inconsistent results implicit attitudes are reconstructed according (Rydell & McConnell, 2010). to the principles of association and similarity in the framework of rapid learning system, and explicit attitudes need to add the cognitive processing based on logical principles of information connection, and require addition- Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes al cognitive resources. The model also assumes that implicit attitudes are more de- Applying the instrumentation of cognitive pendent on associative forms of information, psychology to solve the social and psycho- such as, for example, subliminal primes logical problems gave an opportunity to use (Rydell & McConnell, 2006; Rydell et al., the whole battery of new measurement pro- 2006) or associative cues, including race, cedures, known as implicit measures, which weight, physical attractiveness, etc. The mod- belong to the class of unobtrusive measures. el predicts the coincidence of implicit and The number of researches related to the study explicit indicators, if the valence of associa- of various psychometric characteristics of tive information and verbal stimuli related to implicit measurement is increasing every this information coincide (McConnell et al., year (Bar-Anan & Nosek, 2013). Popularity 2008; Rydell & McConnell, 2006; Rydell et of implicit measures is also explained by the al., 2006). The main contribution of this fact that implicit measures do not require model is the claim that the implicit and ex- verbal report of participant about a phenome- plicit attitudes are the “product” of various non under study, for example, self-esteem, types of cognitive processing (style of infor- and attitude towards violence. Indirect mation processing), hence, there are different measures help to obtain information, un- requirements to input information (inputs) distorted by attitudes, influencing the content and different results of its processing of common self-reports. As the main research (outputs). In this case, unlike other models methods different variations of Implicit As- (Fazio, 2007; Petty et al., 2007; Gawronski & sociation Test (IAT) (Greenwald et al., Bodenhausen, 2006; Rydell & McConnell, 1998), or varieties of procedures, based on 2010) that consider the activation of implicit the priming method are used (Fazio et al., and explicit attitudes as stages of the same 1995; Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, cognitive process, the mechanisms of activa- 2005; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997; tion of attitudes are seen as distinct cognitive Payne & Gawronski, 2010). The predictive processes. In this connection, it is assumed validity of the measurements is high enough that even similar types of information pro- (Ferguson, 2007), but modern science, at this I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 24 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT stage, has no unambiguous answer relatively larly, implicit measures of attitude often pre- to the nature of implicit evaluation reactions dict different outcomes from self-report and their relation to the explicit mental mani- measures (Perugini et al., 2010). festations. It is known that attitudes could be Implicit measurements imply are those measured not only by implicit, but also by measurements that are aimed at assessment explicit measurements. It has been estab- of attitude that respondents either do not real- lished that explicit measurements often re- ize or avoid to report (Wittenbrink & veal not true, but socially desirable answers Schwarz, 2007). The terms “implicit” and that participants give only in order not to get “explicit” refers to cognitive constructs and the status of “hostile” (Dovidio & Gaertner, processes, measured by various procedures. 1986). In order to reduce the effect of social The methods, used for these measurements, desirability, the researchers ensured the par- are usually defined as the implicit and explic- ticipants complete anonymity, and special it, however, some authors appeal to use the “lie scales” were introduced in the design of terms “direct” and “indirect” to refer to these self-assessment procedures. The tendency to measurements (De Houwer, Teige-Moci- select socially approved answers by partici- gemba, Spruyt & Moors, 2009). Direct and pant usually does not mean a conscious de- indirect measurements may reflect different ception or desire to report false judgment. experience or its different aspects in relation Often it is associated with the difficulty of to the object of attitude, and indirect meas- participant‟s self-reflection (analysis) of his/ urements do not replace, but complement the her own attitudes, desires and beliefs. results obtained by direct self-reported proce- Usually the factor of social desirability dures (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Wilson et effect is stronger if it concerns social assess- al., 2000; Rudman, 2011). Explicit measure- ments of attitude object. In addition to these ments are direct, controlled, and conscious. two main drawbacks – factor of social desira- They are based on explicit knowledge of bility and insufficiency of introspection - self oneself. Often explicit measurements reveal -assessment procedures have other negative not true but socially desirable answers of sides - inattention, replication errors, the ten- participants. Implicit measurements are indi- dency to anticipate the expectations of re- rect, automatic, and unconscious. Implicit searcher, which in turn, leads to a certain measurements assess automatic (uncons- distortion of judgments (Losel, 1995). cious) evaluative reactions that come to mind Interestingly, the research has revealed spontaneously, with mere presentation of the that in many domains the responses on im- object in relation to which an attitude was plicit measures of attitude are often not con- formed (Petty et al., 2009). sistent with people‟s self-reported ratings of Current implicit measurements are based their own attitudes (Petty et al., 2009). Simi- on measuring the participants‟ reaction time, T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 25 while they perform different tasks and their spreading activation and tendency of activa- attention is focused on the performance of tion of mental constructs, interconnected in these tasks, rather than on the object of atti- long-term memory (Anderson, 1983, Rud- tude (Rudman, 2011). Methodological break- man, 2011). throughs in social psychology are linked with The sequential priming tasks, sometimes priming paradigm and response interference called affective priming tasks, have been tasks as indirect measures of automatic asso- applied by Fazio et al. (1986). Fazio was one ciations. There are various classifications of of the first who applied the affective priming priming paradigms in scientific literature. in research of attitude to the object using a Basically used are: sequential priming, con- latent period of reaction time (Fazio, Chen, ceptual priming. McDonel, & Sherman, 1982; Dovidio & (Klauer, Voss, & Stahl, 2011). Implicit asso- Fazio, 1992). As a result of numerous experi- ciation tests and their derivatives (response ments several important features have been interference tasks as indirect measures of revealed. First, while using the experimental automatic associations) are also often used. It priming procedure, time interval of stimulus is important to distinguish priming as mne- onset asynchrony (SOA) should be in the monic subsystem of implicit memory, prim- range of 300-500 ms, as at longer interval of ing effects, as function implementation of this asynchrony, prime does not effect on the atti- subsystem, as well as the priming procedure, tude of participants of the experiment (Fazio as a form of impact or methodological proce- et al, 1982, Fazio, Powell, & Herr, 1983; dure entailing a more precise or fast solution Powell & Fazio, 1984; Rudman, 2011). priming, and mindset of a task in relation to identical or similar Second, while calculating the results it is impact. A large number of implicit tests are necessary to take into account the speed of based on the priming effect, which refers to the basic response of participants (base line), the effect of the primary stimulus exposure i.e. speed of psychomotor response of the on the processing of the subsequent stimulus. participant to the empty window without Priming effect assumes activation of category stimulus sample (prime). Semantic priming and representation in memory that subse- measurement, based on the priming effect, is quent influence on person‟s perception of the that the combination of the prime stimuli and situation (often ambiguous), without aware- target attributes has a higher strength of asso- ness of activating context (Bargh, 1996; Hig- ciation compared to semantically unassociat- gins, Bargh, & Lombardi, 1985; Fiedler & ed material, and activates “the facilitation Bless, 2004). effect”, expressed in reduction of response According to L. Rudman, the precise time for these combinations. In the semantic priming mechanism is unknown although it priming task paradigm described by Witten- is proved that evaluative priming provokes brink et al. (1997), participants are shown a I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 26 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT word prime at intervals, which are too brief There are several types of commonly for reported awareness (Klauer, Voss & used types of evaluative priming tasks. They Stahl, 2011). The semantic priming proce- include: the lexical decision tasks (LTD) dure allows us to activate the mental repre- (Belinchon, Riviere, & Igoa, 1992), name sentation, of an experimenter‟s interest. The tasks (NT), and the semantic categorization situation in this case arranges in such a way tasks (SCT) (Balota & Chumbley, 1984). that participants do not realize that activated These types of tasks are quite powerful tools mental construct influences their response for research and understanding of the seman- time. tic memory organization. Thus, in the lexical Besides the conceptual priming technique decision tasks the participant is required to the mindset priming is also described. Mind- define, as soon as possible, whether the pre- set priming and sequential priming are as- senting string stimulus is a word or nonword. sumed to have more enduring effects. Mind- In the name task, the participant should name set priming is assumed to have more endur- stimulus, usually aloud as soon as possible. ing effects than sequential priming” (Klauer In turn, the semantic categorization tasks et al., 2011). Their common feature is that require from the participant to make a deci- they assess the impact of external events on sion to which semantic category the stimulus the thoughts, feelings and behavior. belongs. Another significant distinction constitute Limitations of the evaluative priming (of concepts of short-term and long-term or repe- all evaluative priming types) include its high tition priming. The main difference is in the sensitivity to interference in the course of duration of their effect. Short-term priming research; it is reduces internal consistency occurs in response to a single stimulus and reliability of the research compared to presentation, and long-term, repetition prim- explicit measures, and complicates the possi- ing effect is formed as a result of prolonged bility of replication of effects obtained (Fazio exposure of the same stimulus in similar con- & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2006; Rud- ditions. The duration of short-term priming is man, 2011). Till now, there are discussions short, but repetition priming effect retaines about which method of research of reaction for months (Sloman, Hayman, Ohta, Law, & time is more appropriate, either record of Tulving, 1988). base response or control of response to a Researches in which priming method was number of different primes. There is also a applied, revealed that priming effect depends question of choosing the most sensitive on the type of task used in the experiment. mathematical procedure in relation to the For example, Lukas (2000) reported that se- obtained measurements (Ulrich & Miller, mantic priming effect in name task is lower 1994; Whelan, 2008). than in the lexical decision task. The literature analysis shows that the T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 27 nature of priming is ambiguous. Some re- to overcome the existing “positive” automa- searchers consider the priming as a conse- tism. quence of the activation of existing units (or The IAT is a flexible tool that can be ap- nodes) of memory, thus, explaining further plied to assess any representation of a person accelerating of cognitive processing of acti- about himself/herself (Greenwald et al., vated unit (Anderson, 1983). 1998); it is able to measure the processes in Another point of view at the problem of social cognition, which are not covered by priming assumes that priming is a type of common (self-assessment) tests, showing a unconscious or implicit memory, which re- very high predictive (and increment, in rela- fers to the effect of previous impact and is tion to explicit test) validity (De Houwer, expressed in an accelerated identification of 2006; Greenwald, Banaji, Eudman, Farnham, subsequent words or objects in conditions of Nosek, & Mellott, 2002). lower perceptual signals (Tulving & Schacter, 1990). Some versions of the classic Implicit Association Test (IAT) by Greenwald are: Sin- Priming is based on a recent mental repre- gle-Category IAT by Karpinski and Steinman sentations stored in the episodic memory. It (Karpinski, & Steinman, 2006), Single- is believed, that explicit memory is controlled Attribute IAT (Penke, Eichstaedt, & Asen- by the descending processes of cognitive dorpf, 2006), Brief IAT (Sriram & Green- processing, but implicit memory processes to wald, 2009), GO/NO-GO Association Task a large extent depend on the ascending pro- (Nosek & Banaji, 2001), The Extrinsic Effec- cesses of cognitive processing, controlled by tive Simon Task (DeHower, 2003), Approach parameters of the incoming data (Tulving & Avoidance Paradigm (Chen & Bargh, 1999), Schacter, 1990). and Implicit Relational Assessment Proce- Besides priming measurements, implicit association tests gained popularity. dure (IRAP) (Barnes-Holmes et al., 2006). The most comprehensive review of the im- The basis for the development of IAT plicit measurements and problems of their (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) application to various mental constructs is became a principle, according to which the represented in numerous reviews (Bar-Anan often used cognitive associations contribute & Nosek, 2013; Teige-Mocigemba, Klauer & to more relevant and faster cognitive pro- Sherman, 2010). cessing of information, which is linked with While studying the psychometric indices them. The competition of stimuli manifests of most commonly used implicit measure- itself in conflict of automatisms – if for a ments it has been demonstrated that the most person is more common to associate a specif- reliable and correlating with each other re- ic category with positive adjectives, in order sults were obtained using the procedures of to link it with the negative one, it is necessary Implicit Association Test (IAT). Priming I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 28 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT measurement showed relatively low correla- last 15 years have been paid increasing atten- tion with the results of other implicit meas- tion to the study of impact of various contex- urements, however, it is quite a reliable tool tual factors as to implicit measurement pro- to measure the activation of the concepts in cedure, and to change of implicit attitudes. the semantic network (Bar-Anan & Nosek, As distinct from earlier concepts, which were 2013). characterized by an understanding of implicit attitudes both stable over time and resistant to the context (Fazio et al., 1995; Fazio, 2007; Olson, & Fazio, 2009; Payne & Context and implicit attitudes Gawronski, 2010), a new understanding was caused by increasing amount of empirical evidence of the opposite sense (Blair, 2002; In a large sense, the context (from the Latin Fazio & Olson, 2003; Fazio, 2007). contextus – “connection”, “coherence”) usu- The speculation to the problem was ally refers to a set of facts and the circum- fueled by the fact that the distinction be- stances in which an event occurs, any object tween implicit and explicit attitudes was or phenomenon exist. based, in particular, on the recognition of In psychology, it was suggested to distin- their different sensitivity to the influence of guish two main types of contexts, designated the context. It was important for theorists to as context alpha and context beta. As context understand whether the manipulation of con- alpha, Wickens understood the environment text factors does affect explicit attitudes and that is completely indifferent to the central does not affect the implicit ones, i. e. the task, preserving its characteristics and de- matter concerns dissociation. If the former mands the same regardless of the environ- does not raise serious doubts, so the growing ment (Wickens, 1987). Context beta can be amount of experimental evidence convinced defined as things and circumstances, in any of the falsity of the latter assumption. respect, closely associated with the ambigu- Generalization of disproving evidences ous stimulus (and/or task) and directly affect- has led to the realization of the need to focus ing the identification of its actual meaning or not only on the automatic nature of implicit function. Baddeley (1982) has termed these attitudes, but also on consideration for the two and impact on them of the recent and past experi- “interactive” context, respectively, the latter ences, affective experiences and of other was later referred to as integrated context factors (Rudman, 2013). These factors can be (Eich, 1985), or semantic context (Gorfein, attributed to the alpha context, in which the 1987). activation of implicit attitudes and their concepts of “independent” Theorists of implicit attitudes during the measurement occur. For example, recent T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 29 events have affected the implicit attitudes, tives) as attributes in implicit procedures, changing them (Kuhnen et al., 2001; Rud- such as the IAT, it is possible to obtain shifts man, Ashmore & Gary, 2001; Dasgupta & of implicit measures, caused by the influence Asgari, 2004; Plant, Peruche & Butz, 2005; of irrelevant factors. Another example of a Brinol, Horcajo, Becerra, Falces & Sierra, long-term priming effect was obtained in the 2002; Tormala, Brinol & Petty, 2004; Liv- work, in which the influence of preventive ingston, 2002). At the same time, the remote- impact on the prevention of aggressive be- ness of contextual events factor was not suf- havior (Gollwitzer, Banse, Eisenbach & Nau- ficient for explanation of the observed ef- mann, 2007). If after the impact, there were fects, suggesting that not remoteness, but no changes in implicit indices of aggression emotional significance of the events deter- neither in the experimental nor in the control mines changes in the implicit attitudes groups, then, after four months, among the (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Rudman, 2004; participants of the control group (who was Livingston, & not exposed by the impact), in contrast to the Fairchild, 2007; Amodio, Harmon-Jones, & 2002; Rudman, Dohn, experimental group, the growth of implicit Devine, 2003; Cunningham, et al., 2004; indices of aggression was recorded. This fact Ranganath, Smith, & Nosek, 2008; Baccus, indicates to the long-term effects of preven- Baldwin, & Packer, 2004; Olson & Fazio, tive measures, which suppressed possible 2006; Gawronski & LeBel, 2008). natural growth of aggression of participants One of the mechanisms of context-event of the experimental group. influences can be long-term priming, both Contextual impacts on implicit attitudes emotional and semantic. Thus, the context are associated with various sources of infor- influences on corresponding elements of the mation, for example, with such as the autobi- knowledge stored in the memory, are re- ographical tained for a long time, at least measured in involvement (Marini, Rubichi, & Sartori, months. So, Coane and Balota revealed a 2012), and passive observation of events or long-term priming effect with respect to the stories, for example, when viewing videos concepts associated with the seasonal holi- with relevant to implicit attitudes episode, as days, for example, with Christmas. Access to well as reading and imagination of events and the concepts is turned out to be facilitated stories (Foroni & Mayr, 2005; Correll, Park, during the season of festive events (a few Judd, & Wittenbrink, 2007; Marini, Rubichi, months), but in other seasons such facilita- & Sartori, 2011), and participation in persua- tion were not revealed in the cognitive pro- sive campaigns against prejudice. real events, assuming self- cessing of the same concepts (Coane & Balo- The impact of passive viewing of video ta, 2009). Thus, using such primed concepts plots with different content or reading of sto- (for example, affectively significant adjec- ries, relevant to the situation, on the results of I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 30 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT measurements of implicit attitudes, were implicit measurement will be available upon demonstrated in several researches. In this activation of attitude in the current context. case, the experimental design of such re- In assessing the impact of contextual searches often assumes the manipulation of event on implicit measures of attitudes, an variable - degree of identification with the important factor is the correspondence of the shown character or self-involvement of par- valence of information, contained in the ticipants when viewing of plots. event, to the valence of previously formed Thus, viewing of video clips featuring the attitude. It is assumed, that attitude congruent character, smoking a cigarette, increased the information is associated with a larger num- mental association of smoking and smoking ber of different past (pre-experimental) con- intention, and this effect was growing de- texts than incongruent one. Therefore, incon- pending on the degree of identification with gruent information is better stick in memory the main character (Dal Cin, Gibson, Zanna, as units, which occur in fewer contexts, are Shumate, & Fong, 2007). better memorized than those, which were Manipulation of degree of self- included in a larger number of different con- involvement while instructing participants to texts. Another important factor is corre- imagine themselves as characters in episodes spondence of the memory testing context and associated with negative interracial interac- the context of information coding, since the tions led to a shift of implicit racial attitudes, discrepancy of these contexts, incongruent but only in the episodes with counter- information is forgettable to the higher extent stereotypical content (Marini et al., 2012). (Marsh, Meeks, Hicks, Cook, & Clark-Foos, Moreover, this shift has not been revealed in 2006). a week after experimental exposure, what, in Thus, we can assume that contextual the opinion of the authors, indicated to its events with information, incongruent to atti- temporary transitional nature. tude, will be better memorized than events In the research conducted by Lincoln and with congruent information, but only in the colleagues, the influence of different types of case of coincidence of contexts of coding measures on fight against prejudices toward and activation of attitude. As a result, when schizophrenia (Lincoln, Arens, Berger, & measuring implicit attitudes, while there is a Rief, 2008). It turned out that the implicit correspondence of the contexts of coding and negative stereotypes concerning schizophre- activation of attitude, more accessible would nia can be significantly reduced due to cam- be incongruent information. This greater paigns that emphasize either biogenetic or accessibility of incongruent information psychologic causes of schizophrenia. should also affect the obtained implicit Memory processes have an essential role measures. The magnitude of the obtained in what kind of information in the process of effect of better memorization of incongruent T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 31 information, as it can be assumed, should be new information is integrated with the previ- associated with the remoteness of the appro- ously received and how the implicit measures priate contextual event. But in most of the reflect it. conducted researches the constancy in time In one of experiments the impression of any contextual effects at repeated implicit formation paradigm with verbal statements as measurements has not been studied. Usually, evaluative information that was exposed on these effects are understood as stable, un- the discriminate colored background as con- changing over time. As relatively few excep- textual signals was used. tions we can mention the works (Gollwitzer If the provided information about the et al., 2007; Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, object of attitude was homogeneous and con- Hermsen, & Russin, 2000; Olson & Fazio, sistent, the newly formed implicit evaluations 2006; Marini et al., 2012). In addition, con- generalized regardless of context differences textual effects, presumably, causing instanta- (Rydell & Gawronski, 2009, Gawronski & neous shift of implicit measures under the Sritharam, 2010). But if previously received influence of changes in the specific context, information about the object of attitude later leading to the activation of specific associa- came into conflict with the newly obtained, tions, were studied. the implicit evaluations became context- Thus, depending on the priming interpret- dependent, reflecting context contingency, in ing categories (restaurant or health food), which contradictory information was present- acting as a contextual key feature to access ed, with a valence of preliminary information information, differing instantaneous associa- about the object of attitude. tions of fatty food and appetizing or un- The recent research by Rydell & Gawron- healthy eating were activated respectively ski (2009) also gave evidences of contextual (Roefs et al., 2006). independence of initially formed associations But in general, the research of constancy in the case of homogeneity of available infor- of contextual effects over time remains rela- mation about the object of attitude. When the tively little-studied area and can give evi- information, contradictory to the content of dence of their differences in strength, de- these associations appears, erasure of initial pending on the consistency or inconsistency information was not occurred. But changes in of valence of episodic context information implicit measures of attitude became a con- and information of previously formed of atti- text-dependent and could be observed only in tude. the framework of relevant contextual condi- Since the objects of attitude exist in dif- tions in which contradictory information was ferent contextual environments, including received. According to these results, the con- those containing new information about the textualized activation in new associations in object, it is necessary to research how this opposition to previously formed occurs, but I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 32 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT in what exactly, is determined by contingen- them, for example, applying faking instruc- cy of key context features and by features of tions, it is possible to cause a change in im- information while formation of new and old plicit measures (Czellar, 2006; De Houwer, associations (Gawronski & Sritharam, 2010). Beckers, & Moors, 2007; Fiedler & Blümke, But if the implicit attitudes are responsive 2005; Lowery, Hardin, & Sinclair, 2001; to the impacts of the context, the question Steffens, 2004; Verschuere, Prati, & De arises whether these impacts cause changes Houwer, 2009; Degner, 2009; Klauer & in the implicit attitudes (basic associations) Teige-Mocigemba, 2007; Teige-Mocigemba or their effects impact only implicit measure- & Klauer, 2008). ment procedures and are not associated with The second factor is the specifics of the the modification of the corresponding atti- measurement procedure. Since different im- tudes. Search for an answer to this question plicit measurement procedure are designed to turned out to be a difficult problem. The evaluate the same, but may be based on dif- problem lies in the fact that indirect measures ferent mechanisms, the experimental manip- of attitudes themselves do not reflect directly ulations by contextual factors while perform- the changes in the associations, which were ing tasks in alternative ways, theoretically caused by the experimental impacts. Any allow to determine at what level, procedural experimental or basic, a change of associations occurs. measurement procedure is based on the specific mechanisms, involving, Thus, the use of different procedures of when executed, various cognitive processes. implicit measurements of the same attitude These processes determine the effectiveness in conditions of identical context (experi- of performance of the experimental tasks, mental exposure), may result in coincident or through which it is possible only indirectly different results. In case of absence of differ- judge on the content and type of associations, ences in the results of implicit measure- connecting the object and its attitude. There- ments, it can be assumed that the observed fore, at least the part of the changes, induced context effects are, likely, associated with a by the context in implicit measures, can be change of the basic associations, otherwise caused by changes in the processes involved, they are observed at the procedural level. i.e. mechanisms of performance of the exper- In this work we conducted a study of the imental tasks, but not by the changes of the impact of contextual event on the implicit basic associations (Gawronski, Deutsch, measures of ethnic attitudes. LeBel, & Peters, 2008). The work has been conducted under the One of the factors contributing to the assumption of the varied effect of contextual emergence of contextual effects at implicit impact in the case of consistency or incon- measurements, are strategies of performance sistency of information contained in it with of the experimental task. By manipulating the information of previously formed ethnic T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 33 attitude. For this purpose, the research design implicit attitudes by subliminal affective assumed repeated exposure to control the priming. The nature of the contextual factors duration of impact effects. is in the events of real participant‟s life that In addition, to assess the level of changes have occurred in the recent past and involved induced by the context, two different proce- a high degree of personal involvement. Par- dures of implicit measurement of attitudes, ticipants were questioned about the presence the IAT and procedure of subliminal evalua- of relevant contextual events, using a special- tive priming (affective priming procedure) ly designed questionnaire and performed a were applied. The former procedure assumes self-assessment procedure to determine the the formation and evaluation of the direct explicit ethnic attitude. associations between the object of attitude In the second study the nature of the con- and affectively significant words. The latter textual factor has been changed. The contex- one assumes the assessment of latent associa- tual factor was the event formed by the con- tions of this type. Experimental (contextual) tent of a video shown to participants of ex- impact was produced by specially developed perimental groups, related to inter-ethnic videos, each of which contained one type of cooperation and neutral events. The event information: relevant, irrelevant to existing reflected the different situations of positive or attitude, as well as neutral, irrespective to it. negative interethnic interactions. Participants Research aim: is to investigate the influ- in each experimental group viewed the video ence of contextual factors on implicit atti- of positive, or negative, or neutral content. tudes and their measurements. Videos were produced as part of the promotion work by Marija Bambulak (2014). Participants performed two sessions of implicit Overview measurement of attitudes with the procedure of the IAT and subliminal affective priming. Experimental exposure - watching the video - The research aim is implemented in two dif- was held immediately after the first measure- ferent studies. ment session. The repeated measurement The first study aimed to identify differ- session in some groups was carried out im- ences in implicit measurements of ethnic mediately after the first session, in the other - attitudes in participants who reported the in one day, in the third - in a week. Explicit presence of events in their recent autobio- attitudes were measured, using the same self- graphical experiences, which are relevant to assessment procedure as in Study 1. The ef- ethnic attitude and their reaction to these fect of videos on the participants was as- events. The recent autobiographical event sessed with the help of specially designed was seen as a contextual factor in measuring questionnaires. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 34 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Study 1. Autobiographical events as context Research questions 1. Is there a relationship between the autobiographical contextual events related to the attitude and the response time (RT) in the task of subliminal evaluative priming? 118 (male – 45, female – 73), age from 17 to 50 years, Mdn = 26. Two groups “Latvian” – 57, “Russian” – 61. The groups were recruited according to the following criteria: language, culture, origin. The residence in Latvia for at least 5 years. There were no restrictions on the sex and education. Implicit measures 2. Whether the autobiographical context events associated with facilitation effects Modified procedure of unconscious semantic (positive priming) or inhibition (negative priming in a task of affective categorization priming)? (Plotka, Blumenau, Igonin, Simane, & Bam- 3. Under what experimental conditions the bulak, 2013). priming effect is observed? 4. Whether the autobiographical context Priming procedure. Procedure of subliminal events of various emotional valence are asso- evaluative priming in the task of affective ciated with the measure of implicit attitudes? categorization. Prime stimuli: “Russian”, 5. Whether the autobiographical context “Latvian” and empty screen (baseline). Tar- events associated with the index of implicit gets: affective pleasant words (love, joy, hap- preferences? piness, kindness) and unpleasant words (evil, 6. How the affective significance of the autobiographical context events is related to the conformity of the results of implicit and explicit measurements of attitudes? anger, disgust, contempt). Randomly presented intervals of stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA): 100, 200, 300, 400, 500, 600 ms. Participants‟ task was to “Push “Y” key as soon as possible if the positive word appears, and “N” key if the negative appears”. The fixation point (black cross in the Method middle of a white screen) – 1000 ms, Primes Participants 17 ms, Mask (black lattice in the middle of a white screen) coincides with the SOA, Target (not restricted), interval between trials 118 (male – 45, female – 73), age from 17 to (empty screen) 1000 ms. Total 384 trials in 50 years, Mdn = 26. Two groups “Latvian” – each session for each participant. 57, “Russian” – 61. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 35 Proceeding of data. The two most prominent involves how quickly subjects respond to the implicit measures such as, evaluative priming adjectives in the absence of a prime – the and the IAT were used in the Study 1 and in baseline response. Baseline responses are the Study 2. Each employs reaction time used to compute facilitation scores. The term tasks that measure people's attitudes indirect- “facilitation score” is used to encompass both ly. response facilitation and inhibition. Latency (RT) in IAT and subliminal prim- On each SOA for each participant in each ing tasks. The histogram of RT usually group, and for each Target the average values shows the great skewness: the data are of response times were calculated. skewed in a positive direction. Because slow For prime time Baseline basic responses responses are rare, the distribution is not nor- were calculated – average RT0+ for each mal. Most statistical procedures assume that group of positive words (the maximum num- the data are normally distributed. We trans- ber is 16), and RT0- for each group of nega- formed the RT to normalize the distribution. tive words (their number is also from 1 to We used a logarithmic transformation to base 16). e. The distribution of obtained data is more For each prime: Latvian, Russian the av- close to normal distribution. We used logged erage value of RT+ was calculated for each latency (ln(RT)) for all statistical tests. For group of positive words (the maximum num- ease of interpretability, descriptive statistics ber is 8), and average value of RT– was cal- and figures were presented in millisecond culated for each group of negative words (the form. maximum number is 8). Facilitation scores. The extent of implicit connection of certain concepts with positive or negative attitudes can be assessed by using the shifts of reaction time taking into account a baseline. Affective cohesiveness of stimuli From RT0 of basic responses the relevant RT on primes were subtracted R+ = RTo+ – RT+ , R– = RTo– – RT– . (1) Variables R+ and R– are termed assumes the existence of already formed rela- “Facilitation scores” – reactions to positive tion to an ethnic group (ethnic attitude). Fast and negative target words relatively to the response to positive stimuli suggests a posi- rate of response. tive attitude, and the fast reaction to negative Fast reaction to positive words relatively stimuli suggests a negative attitude, and vice to the basic rate R+ > 0 indicates positive versa (Plotka, Igonin, & Blumenau, 2009). attitude. Slow response to the positive words The terms “facilitation” and “inhibition” refer to a speeding up or slowing down of relatively to the basic rate R+ < 0 indicates non-positive attitude. responses relative to some standard. For Fast reaction to negative words relatively Fazio‟s evaluative priming task, that standard to the basic rate indicates negative attitude I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 36 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT R– > 0 – negative attitude. Slow response to lyzed using a within-subjects ANOVA: 2 the negative words relatively to the basic rate (prime: Latvian, Russian) x 2 (target word indicates non-negative attitude R– < 0. valence: good, bad). Evidence of automatic Facilitation scores are applied for reveal- ethnical bias is seen if a prime*target valence ing of priming effect under certain experi- interaction emerges (Rudman, 2011). For mental conditions. For these purposes, the example, in Study 2, at SOA=300 ms in the mean of the variable “facilitation scores” first session both primes facilitate faster with the help of One Sample t-test are com- recognition of positive words, compared with pared with zero. The null hypothesis about negative words. In the second session, the assumption that mean of the variable primes “Russian” do not facilitate faster “facilitation scores” is random non zero is recognition of positive and negative words. tested. In case of deviation of this hypothesis, Primes “Latvian” facilitate faster recognition the presence of priming effect would be re- of negative words and lowest speed of recog- vealed. nition of positive words (Fig.2). Mean facilitation scores can then be ana- FIG.2. Facilitation scores for SOA=300 ms, sessions 1-2. Factors’ Prime and Target interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 37 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Assessment of valence and strength of attitude is positive. implicit attitude. Modified method of shifts. (b) The case of facilitation of cognitive To assess the strength and the valence of processing of positive target words and inhi- implicit attitudes, we introduce a modified bition of negative target words. The resulting variable of shift RR (Plotka & Blumenau, attitude is positive. (c) The case of inhibition of cognitive 2015) which is defined as the difference of processing of both positive and negative tar- facilitation scores: RR = R+ – R– = (RT– –RT+) – (RT0– – RT0+). (2) From (2) it follows that the attitude is positive, if RR > 0, and the attitude is negative, if RR < 0, the attitude is non-positive get words at the lower inhibition of the reaction to the positive words. The resulting attitude is positive. 2. RR = R+ – R– < 0, then R+ < R–. This could be in three cases: and non-negative (middle) if RR = 0. (d) The case of facilitation of cognitive The rationale of the modified method of processing of both positive and negative tar- shifts can be obtained by principle of com- get words at the predominance of facilitating plete induction (Fig.3). effect of the negative words. The resulting 1. RR = R+ – R– > 0, then R+ > R–. This attitude is negative. could be in three cases: (e) The case of inhibition of cognitive (a) The case of facilitation of cognitive processing of positive at the facilitation of processing of both positive and negative tar- processing of negative target words. The re- get words at the predominance of facilitating sulting attitude is negative. effect of the positive words. The resulting RR R d facilitation inhibition (f) The case of inhibition of cognitive e R+ Rg f 0 a b RR>0 h c RR<0 RR=0 FIG.3. Facilitation, inhibition of responses (R+, R-) and valence of attitudes (sign of RR) – all cases I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 38 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT are calculated. processing of both positive and negative target words at the lower inhibiting effect of the The attitude is positive, if negative words. The resulting attitude is neg- . ative. (3) The attitude is negative, if 3. (g-h). If R+ = R–, then RR = R+ – R– = 0. . The attitude is either ambivalent or ambiguous. The effects of facilitation and inhibition (4) The attitude is non-positive and non- of both positive and negative words are iden- negative - (“neutral”), if tical: R+ = R–, RR = 0. . Unlike the conventional method of shifts (RR = RT+ - RT-), the modified method of (5) The comparison of the mean values of RR shifts takes into account the “baseline”. to zero allows us to research and to assess the The variable of shifts RR represents a magnitude of attitude under different experi- continuous variable, which has the time di- mental conditions (SOA primes, the impact mension. The absolute values of this variable of contextual events). characterize the strength of attitude in milli- Single index of implicit preference. The seconds. The sign RR indicates a valence of variable conditionally called as the variable implicit attitude. The attitude is positive if of “shifts of implicit preferences” RRR is the the RR>0, the attitude is negative if RR<0, single index of implicit preference is con- the attitude is non-positive, and non-negative structed using a third contrast (Rudman, (middle), if RR = 0. 2011, p.14, Tab.2.2). For the considered task For practical revealing of strength and the analogous table of contrasts will be the valence of attitudes the quartiles Q1 and Q3 Table 1. TABLE 1. Coding of contrasts for evaluative priming “Latvian”-”Russian” Prime “Latvian” Prime “Latvian” Prime “Russian” Prime “Russian” Negative Positive Negative Positive Contrast 1 -1 1 0 0 Contrast 2 0 0 -1 1 Contrast 3 1 -1 -1 1 Target word Note: numbers 1, 0, -1 are weighting coefficients for the variable of facilitation, which represents the intragroup structure 2х2: Prime (“Latvian”, “Russian”) х Target (Negative, Positive). T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 39 The variable conditionally called as the hilism (EN), Ethnic indifference (EI), Posi- variable of “shifts of implicit preferences” tive ethnic identity (PEI), Ethnoegoism (EE), RRR is the single index of implicit preference Ethnoisolation (EI), Ethnofanaticism (EF). (SIIP) is constructed using a third contrast Ethnonihilism – one of the forms of hy- (Rudman, 2011, p.14, Tab.2.2). For the con- poidentity. Departure from own ethnic group sidered task the analogous table of contrasts and quests for stable socially psychological will be the Table 1: niches, that are not related to ethnic criteria. Contrast 1 is the difference of facilitation Ethnic indifference – blurry ethnic identity scores: that is characterized by indeterminate ethni- RLatvian RLatvian ( RT0Latvian RTLatvian ) cal affiliation, ethnic aspect is not relevant. ( RT0Latvian RTLatvian ) RR Latvian. (6) Contrast 2 is the difference: Positive ethnic identity – positive attitude regarding own nation is correlated with positive attitudes regarding other nations. It creates optimal equilibrium of toleration be- RRussian RRussian ( RT0Russian RTRussian) ( RT0Russian RTRussian) RR Russian. (7) Contrast 3 is the difference between contrasts 2 and 1: tween own ethnic group and other ethnic groups, it is conditions of independence and stable existence of ethnic group, as well as it determines peaceful interaction among cul- RRR RRRussian RRLatvian. (8) tures in polyethnic world. Ethnoegoism – tension and irritation in interaction with dif- Thus, RRR is the difference of biases RR ferent ethnic groups, recognizing the rights of for different primes. It serves as a single in- own nation only to solve problems on the dex preference account of “others”. Ethnoisolation – convic- (Rudman, 2011, p.14). If RRR>0, the ethnic tion about the superiority of own nation, ap- preference towards Russians is revealed. If peal to “purify” national culture, negative RRR<0 on the contrary, the ethnic preference attitude regarding interethnic marriages, xen- towards Latvians is revealed. ophobia. Ethnofanaticism – alacrity to per- of (implicit) automatic The comparison of the mean values of form any actions in the name of ethnic inter- RRR to zero allows us to research the condi- ests that are comprehended somehow or oth- tions of maturity of preferences. er, justification of any sacrifices in the struggle about prosperity of own nation. Accord- Explicit measures ing to Soldatova‟s methodology it is possible “Types of ethnic identity” by Soldatova, G. (1998). This survey contains 30-item, -confidence. The types of identities are dis- 6-point scale (pp. 189-193). Scales: Ethnoni- trism: from the “denial” of ethnic identity to to determine the transformation of ethnic self tributed pursuant to the scale of ethnocen- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 40 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT the national fanaticism. Scales: ethnoegoism, 500, 600 (ms)), “Target” (positive, negative). ethnoisolation, ethnofanaticism – characterize transformation of the ethnic self- Dependent variables confidence pursuant to the type of hyperi- Implicit measured variables. Reaction time dentity (HYPER), whereas scales – ethnoni- RT, facilitation scores, R+ = RTo+ – RT+ , R– hilism and ethnic indifference – are pursuant = RTo– – RT– , the variable of shifts RR = R+ to the type of hypoidentity (HYPO). Ethnical – R– , single index of implicit preference, toleration can be assessed pursuant to the “shifts of implicit preferences” RRR = RRRus- scale of positive ethnic identity, but the level sian of intoleration can be assessed pursuant to all other scales. – RRLatvian. In most cases, the reduction of RT means the average value of RT. Survey “The effect of episodes of dif- Explicit measured variables. “Hypo- ferent emotional valence of on ethnic attitudes”. This is a specially designed ques- identity” tionnaire, which includes a description of scales EN, EI and EE, EIZ, EF, accordingly; recent (relative to the time of the experiment) “Valence of Episodes” (positive, negative), events in Latvia relating to interethnic rela- “Impact of Episodes” (low, medium, high). ty” (HYPO) and “hyperidenti- (HYPER), obtained by summing the tions. Answers to the questionnaire take into account the force of impact, the valence and remoteness of events. The questionnaire has two versions, different in content - for Latvi- Results. Study 1 ans in Latvian and for Russians, in Russian (Tikhomirova, 2013). After the completion of the experiment, all the data that correspond to the wrong task Apparatus performance by the participants (classi- Certified licensed software E-Prime 2.0 Pro- fication of emotionally positive and negative fessional ®. words) were removed from the obtained N = Proceeding of data 45696 data on reaction time (RT): 2528 or 5.5% of observations. From these obtained Descriptive statistics, tests of normality of 43168 data 5% of the data have been re- distribution, boxplots, ANOVA, Repeated moved from the top: i.e. all the data that ex- Measures ANOVA, Factor Analysis, t-tests, ceeded the 95th percentile P95 = 924 ms and correlation analysis were used. the data that is less than 130 ms. Total 4689 data, or 10.3% of observations were re- Independent variables moved. Mean values of RT reaction time for “Group” (RUS, LV); “Prime” (Russian, Lat- each participant with fixed values of the vari- vian, Baseline); “SOA” (100, 200, 300, 400, ables “Group”, “Prime”, “Target”, “SOA” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 41 were calculated for the remaining data. 4248 unconscious emotional priming, when pre- observations were formed, M = 580 ms, SD = sented target verbal stimuli are affectively 70 ms. ambivalent and primed with affectively con- First research question. With the help of nected with them other verbal stimuli previ- ANOVA(2x3x6x2x2x3) the research of fac- ously presented on subliminal level. This tors‟ impact “Group”, “Prime”, “SOA”, phenomenon must be considered in the stud- “Target”, “Valence of Episodes”, “Impact of ies with the use of the above procedure and is Episodes” and their interaction on RT was worth of a special research (Plotka, Igonin, conducted. The main assumptions for ANO- Blumenau, Bambulaka, Ozola, & Simane, VA (normality of distribution and homogeni- 2011; Plotka, et al., 2013). ty of variances) were analysed (Nasledov, 4) “Impact of Episodes” (F(2, 4245) = 2004; Buhl, 2002; Howell, 1999, p. 303). 47.93, p < .001). Multiple comparisons, LSD Post Hoc Test LSD was used. The following for “Impact of Episodes” revealed that RT at factors‟ impacts were revealed. episodes‟ influence of medium strength sig- 1) “Group” (F(1, 4246) = 187.54, p < .001). RT in the group “LV” exceeds the RT in “RUS” statistically significant. 2) “SOA” (F(5, 4242) = 4.24, p = .001). Multiple comparisons (Post Hoc Test LSD) nificant exceeds RT at episodes‟ influence of low and high strength (p < .001) (Fig.4). 5) “Valence of Episodes” (F(1, 3888) = 4.50, p < .05). RT at positive episodes exceeds RT at negative episodes (Fig.5). revealed that RT at SOA=100 ms statistically 6) Factors‟ “Valence of Episodes” and significant exceeds RT at any other SOA (at “Impact of Episodes” interaction‟s impact: 200-300 ms – p < .01, at 400-600 ms – F(1, 3888) = 120.79, p < .001. If impact of p < .001). episodes is low, RT for negative episodes 3). “Target” (F(1, 4246)=23.33, p < .001). exceeds the RT for positive episodes: t(476) The mean of RT at negative target words = -7.53, p < .001. If impact of episodes is exceeds the mean of RT at positive words. medium, RT for positive episodes exceeds This fact resembles the effects observed dur- the RT for negative episodes: t(139) = 8.38, ing some types of explicit tests based on se- p < .001. For negative episodes: RT for medi- mantic information. For example, verification um impact of episodes exceeds the RT for of parity of numbers is faster in the case of low impact episodes (t(1780) = -13.41, presentation of even numbers, rather than odd p < .001), RT for high impact of episodes numbers (see odd effect Bock & Warren, exceeds the RT for low impact episodes 1985); verification of the fact, whether the (t(1928) = -5.13, p < .001), RT for medium presented noun denotes animate or inanimate impact of episodes exceeds the RT for high object is faster if the noun denotes animate impact episodes (t(2530) = 7.66, p < .001). object (Hines, 1990). The findings show that For positive episodes: RT for low impact of such effects are observed also in the task of episodes exceeds the RT for medium impact I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 42 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.4. The means of RT, ms. Factor’s “Impact of Episodes” influence episodes: (t(321) = 6.47, p < .001) (Fig.6). of low intensity episodes: t(1006) = 7) Factors‟ “Impact of Episodes” and -3.53, p < .001. RT for medium intensity epi- “Group” interaction‟s impact: F(2, 3888) = sodes exceeds the RT of high intensity epi- 14.27; p < .001. In LV group RT for medium sodes: t(555) = 2.51, p < .01. In RUS group intensity episodes exceeds the RT of low RT for medium intensity episodes exceeds intensity episodes: t(1263) = -8.11, p < .001. the RT for high intensity episodes exceeds the RT t(1231) = -5.02, p < .001. RT for high intensi- RT of low intensity FIG.5. The means of RT, ms. Factor’s “Valence of Episodes” influence T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) episodes: I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 43 FIG.6. The means of RT, ms. Factors’ “Valence of Episodes*Impact of Episodes” interaction’s impact ty episodes exceeds the RT of low intensity p < .01 (Fig.7). At fixed intensity of episodes episodes: t(1254) = -2.38, p < .05. RT for RT in group LV exceeds RT in group RUS: at medium intensity episodes exceeds the RT of low intensity of episodes: t(1150) = 5.64, high intensity episodes: t(1690) = 2.66, p < .001, at medium intensity of episodes: FIG.7. The means of RT, ms. Factors’ “Impact of Episodes*Group” interaction’s impact I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 44 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT t(1729) = 10.32, p < .001, at high intensity of p < .01. At positive target words RT for me- episodes: t(635) = 6.48, p < .001. dium impact of episodes exceeds the RT of 8) Factors‟ “Impact of Episodes” and low impact of episodes: t(1283) = -8.14, p “Target” interaction‟s impact: F(2, 4032) = < .001. RT for medium impact of episodes 4.17, p < .05. At low and high intensity of exceeds the RT of high impact of episodes: t episodes‟ impact size RT of negative target (1203) = 6.79, p < .001 (Fig.8). words exceeds RT of positive target words. 9) Factors‟ “Valence of Episodes” and Low intensity of episodes: t(1150) = 3.55, “Group” interaction‟s impact: F(1, 3888) = p < .001, high intensity of episodes: t(1192) 7.78, p < .01. In both groups the differences = 4.61, p < .001. At negative target words RT between the means of RT at negative and for medium impact of episodes exceeds the positive episodes‟ are nonsignificant. For RT of low impact of episodes: t(1510) = both negative and positive episodes‟ valences -4.97, p < .001. RT for high impact of epi- RT in LV group exceeds RT in RUS group: t sodes exceeds the RT of low impact of epi- (3778) = 13.46, p < .001 and t(201) = 3.44, p sodes: t(1186) = -2.01, p < .001. RT for me- = 0.001 for negative and positive respective- dium impact of episodes exceeds the RT of ly (Fig.9). high impact of episodes: t(1546) = 2.82, FIG.8. The means of RT, ms. Factors’ “Impact of Episodes*Target” interaction’s impact T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 45 FIG.9. The means of RT, ms. Factors’ “Valence of Episodes*Group” interaction’s impact Thus, the relationship between the autobiographical contextual events related to the The third research question. Priming effect. In the Table A1 (see Appendix A) the attitude and the response time in the task of experimental conditions subliminal priming assessment exists. priming effect is observed are shown. under which the The second research question. With the The fourth research question. With help of ANOVA (6x2x2x3x2x2) each of fac- ANOVA (2 x 2 x 6 x 2 x 3) the research of tors (SOA, Prime, Target, Impact of Epi- factors‟ “Group”, “Prime”, “SOA”, “Valence sodes, Valence of Episodes, Group) impact of Episodes”, “Impact of Episodes” on the on the variable variable RR was conducted. Statistically sig- R , R R nificant impacts: for positive words, for negative words. (9) was verified. Statistically significant was factors‟ “Prime*Target*Impact of “Group” (F(1, 1402) = 7.03, p < .01). In the group “RUS” the mean of RR is positive, in the group “LV” - negative. Epi- “Prime*Valence of episodes*Impact of sodes*Valence of Episodes” interactions‟ Episodes” (F(1, 1296) = 10.00, p<.01) impact: F(1, 2592) = 9.94, p < .01. The mean (Fig.11). M=15.9 (SD=47.02, prime “Russian”, R(-), Thus, the autobiographical context events positive, low, t(53) = 2.36, p < .05) statisti- of various emotional valence associated with cally significant differs from zero (Fig.10). the measure of implicit attitudes. Thus, the autobiographical context events The fifth research question. ANOVA associated with relief effects (positive prim- (2х6х2x3) (Group x SOA x Valence of Epi- ing) or inhibition (negative priming). sodes x Impact of Episodes) revealed statisti- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 46 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.10. Means of R. Factors’ “Prime*Target*Impact of Episodes*Valence of Episodes” interaction cally significant factors‟ interaction “Valence The sixth research question. Conformity of episodes*Impact of Episodes”: F(1, 648) = of results of implicit and explicit measure- 14.59, p < .001 (Fig.12). One sample t-test ments has been verified using Pearson's cor- found that at negative valence and low im- relation coefficients for RRR, HYPER and pact the mean M = 18.5 (SD = 73.0) differs HYPO variables at fixed times of SOA and from zero t(131) = 2.91, p < .01. At positive other experimental conditions. Conformity of valence and medium impact the mean M = measurements was observed with negative 49.0 (SD = 84.6) differs from zero t(17) = correlation coefficients. The results are 2.46, p < .05. It shows the preference of Rus- shown in Table A2 (see Appendix A). sians under these conditions. Thus, the autobiographical context events associated with the index of implicit preferences. In group “RUS” the correspondence is if r(RRR-HYPO)<0 or r(RRR-HYPER)>0. In group “LV” the correspondence is if r(RRR-HYPO)>0 or r(RRR-HYPER)<0. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.11. The means of RR. Factors’ “Prime*Valence of episodes*Impact of Epsodes” interaction FIG.12. The means of RRR. Factors’ “Valence of episodes*Impact of Episodes” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 47 48 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Discussion and conclusions. Study 1 of Russians the RT is shorter. The fastest responses are with the contextual events of small force. The slowest are with contextual The relationship between the autobiograph- events of medium affective intensity. When ical contextual events related to the attitude compared the RT on each level of intensity and the response time in the task of sublimi- of affective contextual events the RT in the nal priming assessment exists. group of Russians is shorter. With contextual events of negative va- While in all cases the response to nega- lence and the average intensity of the emo- tive target words was slower than to posi- tional impact the RT is the longest. The short- tive target words, with the contextual events er RT is observed in contextual events of low of medium intensity it was not observed. intensity. The RT with contextual events of Autobiographical contextual events are negative valence of high intensity is shorter associated with relief effects facilitation than with the events of negative valence of (positive priming) or inhibition (negative medium intensity. With negative contextual priming). The facilitation variable is associ- events of high affective intensity the RT in- ated with contextual events. With positive creases compared to the RT with contextual contextual events of medium affective in- events of negative valence of low intensity tensity the facilitation of cognitive pro- and reduces it in comparison with the expo- cessing of positive target words occurs. In sure of medium affective intensity. other cases, facilitation and inhibition are With contextual events of positive valence of low affective intensity the RT is longer in comparison with the events of medium intensity. manifested identically: the average values do not significantly differ from zero. The most expressed attitudes are observed with the medium affective intensity With contextual events of low affective of positive contextual events. intensity the context events of positive va- The group of Russian gave preference lence slow down the response, and the nega- with low negative episodes and positive tive – accelerate it. With contextual events of medium. medium intensity, on the contrary - positive The autobiographical context events contextual events slow down the response associated with relief effects (positive prim- and the negative events accelerate it. With ing) or inhibition (negative priming). Facili- positive contextual events the strong effects tation variable is associated with the con- were not observed. textual events. At positive contextual events The ratios of the RT with contextual of medium affective intensity facilitation of events of low, medium and high intensity are cognitive processing of positive target identical in both groups, whereas in the group words occurs. In other cases, facilitation T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 49 and inhibition manifest themselves equally: with respect to RT of negative events. Events the mean values were not significantly differ- of average affective strength exerted an in- ent from zero. hibitory effect on RT, while the events with Effect of priming we see in total with subjectively low and high affective strength SОА 200-400 ms Table A1 (see Appendix accelerated the reaction. Variables of priming A). At SOA 100 ms only in group RUS at effect (R), of attitudes (RR) and of indices of prime Russian, negative valence of episodes, implicit preferences (RRR) are also associat- low impact of episodes. At SOA 500 ms only ed with valence and strength of recent emo- in group LV at prime Russian, negative va- tional autobiographical contextual events. lence of episodes, medium impact of epi- This shows that implicit attitudes and their sodes. At SOA 600 ms only in group LV at measures cannot be regarded as independent prime Latvian. of context. The autobiographical context events of In this study, priming effects, both nega- various emotional valence associated with tive and positive, were mainly observed with the measure of implicit attitudes. The most SOA 200-400 ms and in some cases with expressed attitudes are observed with the SOA 500 ms. In general, this result is con- average affective intensity of positive con- sistent with Fazio and others (Fazio et al., textual events. 1982), Powell and Fazio (1984), but with the The autobiographical context events associated with the index of implicit preferences. difference that the effects of priming were found with SOA 200 ms and with some other Results of implicit and explicit measure- times of delay in presentation of the target ments partly overlap. Most of all matches stimulus. This result indicates a more com- were with SOA 500 ms in both groups. The plex picture of priming effect manifestation absolute values of obtained statistically sig- with different SOAs, which is possible due to nificant Pearson‟s correlation coefficients are the effect of contextual autobiographical from .27 to .72. At SOA 300 ms obtained events. two statistically significant correlation coef- The greatest correspondence between the ficients in group of Russians with absolute results of explicit measurements and meas- values .70 and .81. At SOA 100 ms and posi- urements with the priming procedure occurs tive valence of episodes in group LV correla- with SOA 500 ms. This indicates that the tion coefficient is .53. associations activated with SOA 500 ms, The study of reaction time in the task of affective subliminal evaluative probably underlie both implicit and explicit priming measures of attitudes. At the same time, these showed the relationship of RT with affective associations are insignificantly affected by relevant contextual autobiographical events. contextual impacts. Positive events inhibited the reaction time Relationships of implicit and explicit I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 50 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT measures with SOA 500 ms have an interesting feature. Depending on the ethnic group of study participants we found the associations Study 2. Experimental events as context of opposite direction between the variable of Research questions the index of implicit preference and hy- 1. Is there a relationship of the reaction time poidentity. “Pro-Russian” preferences in the and the measure D of implicit attitude in the group of Russian participants are negatively task of Implicit Association Test with the associated with hypoidentity (i.e. reduction in effect of contextual exposure? “pro-Russian” preference), while such prefer- 2. Is there a relationship of the reaction time, ences in the group of Latvian participants priming effect and measures of implicit atti- were positively associated with hypoidentity. tudes in the task of subliminal affective prim- Associations activated with SOA 500 ms and ing with the effect of contextual exposure? underlying the implicit preference have bidirectional association with the preference of the alternative ethnic group. Perhaps the processes causing implicit measures of attitudes with SOA 500 ms are the integrators of ambivalent information with regard to ethnic preferences. By contrast, the processes that determine “pro-Russian” implicit preference with SOA 300 ms are “unidirectional” and characteristic only for the participants of Russian groups. Higher “pro-Russian” preference with SOA 300 ms corresponds to lower “pro-Latvian” explicit preference. And 3. What is the structure of correlations between explicit and implicit measures of attitudes with different SOAs before and after contextual exposure? 4. What are the changes between measures of implicit attitudes with different times of retest delay and values of SOA in the task of subliminal affective priming after contextual exposure? Method vice versa. This result can be interpreted as Participants the fact that with the value of SOA 300 ms 120 (male – 56, female – 64), age 18 – 30, the basic “pro-Russian” attitude is activated Mdn = 19 years. Ethnic group: Russians. as such, without opposing the alternative Criteria for selection: native language, cul- ethnic group. These hypothetical assumptions ture and origin. have been developed in the Study 2. Implicit measures Modified procedure of subliminal evaluative priming in the task of affective categorization (Plotka et al., 2013) see in Study 1. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 51 The Implicit Association Test (Greenwald participants were provided with necessary et al., 1998), modified for experimental re- instructions and training procedures. Partici- search of ethnic attitudes (Tikhomirova, pants were reported that the main purpose of 2013). the research was the study of the influence of emotional states on the perception of words. Explicit measures All participants took part in the first session. Survey “Types of Ethnic Identity” (Soldatova They carried out priming and IAT procedures & Ryzhova, 1998); and filled in the questionnaire “Types of Eth- Survey “Intensity of Emotional Im- pact” (Bambulaka, 2014b). nic Identity.” Duration of the first research session was about 90 minutes. After the first session, the participants were divided into 12 Contextual factors To create emotionally charged episodes in relation to the Latvians and the Russians three videos were created (Bambulaka, 2014a). The first two videos were with actors who demonstrated the inter-ethnic relations. Preview of the first video was to cause negative emotions towards the Latvians. The second video was to cause positive emotions towards the Latvians. The third video was without participation of actors. It contained beautiful relaxing landscapes and aimed to create neutral emotions not related to the Russians and Latvians. Apparatus Certified licensed software E-Prime 2.0 Professional ®. groups: 3 control groups and 9 experimental ones. Nine experimental groups were divided into three parts according to three conditions of the experiment. Groups 1, 2 and 3 watched the video with negative emotional content towards the Latvians. Groups 4, 5, 6 watched the video with positive emotional content towards the Latvians, groups 7, 8, 9 - with neutral emotional content towards the Russians and Latvians. Groups 1, 4, 7 have passed the second research session immediately after viewing the video, groups 2, 5, 8 – in one day, groups 3, 6, 9 – in one week. The second session consisted of performing the priming and IAT procedures and filling in the questionnaire “Intensity of Emotional Impact” – a subjective evaluation of the intensi- Procedure All the participants took part in the research voluntarily. The participant signed the agreement to participate in two sessions of the experiment. In a specially equipped room 15 computers were installed. Before the beginning of research in each of the sessions, the ty of the emotional impact created as a result of viewing videos. It lasted about 40 minutes. For the first control group the second session began in 15 minutes after the first session. The second control group participated in the second session after one day, the third control group – after one week. During I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 52 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT the second session the participants of control appropriate key responses. Instructions were groups performed the experimental priming written in black letters on a white back- and IAT procedures. All the participants took ground and located in the center of the moni- part in the research voluntarily. tor screen. Each sentence began with a new line. Each target word appeared centered on Implicit methods the screen. All target and category words Implicit Association Test. A modified ver- were presented in lowercase letters. Before sion of Two-Category Implicit Association the start of the experiment, on a computer Test (IAT), developed on the basis of IAT monitor a participant was given general in- (Rudman, 2011). The categories of IAT were structions and specific instructions before “Russian”, “Latvian”. The attributes - the each of the blocks (tasks). The task of the words of Schlosberg Scale (Woodworth, participants was the differentiation of pre- Schlosberg, 1955; Schlosberg, 1952) with a sented stimuli. Stimulus word displayed on strong affective meaning (positive or negative) the screen without auditory accompaniment were used. “Unpleasant” attributes: anger, and remained on the screen until the pressing disgust, contempt, evil, hatred. “Pleasant” a key of the participant (Table 2). The RT for attributes: kindness, respect, love, joy, happi- each trail was recorded as the time interval ness. between the onset of stimulus presentation The authors applied the experimental procedure IAT, using seven trial blocks. Performance of the implicit method took an average of 10 to 20 minutes. Participants‟ reaction and pressing the correct key. Words were selected randomly without replacement. The target word remained on the screen until the participants responded. time (RT) was registered. Each stage was pre- To ensure the internal validity of the ex- ceded by a set of instructions concerning the periment the main parameters were un- dimensions of the categorization task and the changed (the time of stimulus presentation, TABLE 2. Study 2: Procedure of Russian-Latvian preference IAT Block Trials Function Left-key response “Q” Right-key response “P” 1 24 Practice Russian Latvian 2 26 Practice Unpleasant words Pleasant words 3 40 Test Unpleasant words + Russian Pleasant words + Latvian 4 40 Test Unpleasant words + Russian Pleasant words + Latvian 5 24 Practice Latvian Russian 6 40 Test Unpleasant words + Latvian Pleasant words + Russian 7 40 Test Unpleasant words + Latvian Pleasant words + Russian T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 53 the intervals between stimuli, number of stim- five scales we can immediately identify two uli – the words, the font, chromatic back- groups of variables: ethnic hypo-identity ground settings). (α = .637) (EN and EI – HYPO) and ethnic As a result of IAT the D-scores (effect size) for implicitly measured hyper-identity (EE, EIZ and EF – HYPER) variable (α = .747) (Bambulaka, 2014b). The factor “Latvian – Russian implicit associations” analysis was also used to define these varia- were calculated (Rudman, 2011): bles. “Factor analysis, as an alternative to a simple summation of the values of the origi- 1 M M3 M7 M4 D 6 2 SD36 SD47 , (10) nal variables, allows us to take into account the real structure of the data and to avoid where Mi is the mean of RT in block “i”, SDik unnecessary loss of information” (Nasledov, – is combined standard deviation for blocks 2004 p.273). We held Factor Analysis with “i” and “k”. two factors (Kaiser criterion), method Maxi- All trials with RT < 300 ms and RT > 4000 ms were deleted. mum likelihood, rotation's method Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. Kaiser-Meyer- The D-statistic is an effect size, based on Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy each person‟s variance in response latencies. equals to .62 (satisfactory adequacy of the If |D|≤0.15 – no effect, if 0.15<|D| ≤0.35 – p, sample), Bartlett's test of Sphericity χ2(10) = if 0.35<|D|<0.60 – medium effect size, if 191.80, p < .001 (data are suitable for factor |D|≥0.60 – large effect size. The positive val- analysis), cumulative percent of total vari- ues of D show the preference of Latvian, the ance explained 64.22%, goodness of fit test negative values – preference of Russian. χ2(4) = 4.16, p = .384, ns (factor model ade- Priming procedure. Procedure of sublim- quately describes the relationships among inal evaluative priming in a task of affective the variables). As a result, it was found that categorization see in Study 1. Factor 1 is described by variables (EIZ, EE, EF) and the second factor is described by Explicit methods variables (EN, EI) (Fig.13). These factors “Types of ethnic identity” by Soldatova, G. can be called the “Hyper-identity” and (1998). “Hypo-identity”. About this survey see in The variables HYPO and HYPER also Study 1. Cronbah‟s alfa was not sufficiently high: were under consideration (see study 1). α = .666. As a result of the reliability analysis Survey “Intensity of emotional im- the scale PEI was removed and Cronbah‟s alfa became equal to α = .766 – acceptable pact” by Bambulaka, Plotka, Igonins (Bambulaka, 2014b). To determine the in- internal consistency (coefficient alpha = .70) tensity of the emotional impact of video the (George & Mallery, 2003). For the remaining authors have designed three questionnaires I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 54 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.13. Factor Plot in Rotated Factor Space to assess the intensity of positive, negative Independent variables and neutral impact. Different formats of “Group” (RUS, LV); “Prime” (Russian, Lat- questions were used - open, closed, hypothet- vian, Baseline); “SOA” (100, 200, 300, 400, ical and alternative questions. The question- 500, 600 (ms)), “Target” (positive, negative), naires include the scales designed to investi- “Valence of event (episode)” (negative, neu- gate the intensity and emotional valence of tral, positive, no impact), “Retest” (at once, experimental exposure. Cronbach‟ Alpha for in one day, in one week). Dependent variables Implicit measured variables. RT, R+, R, neutral survey is .915 (6 items, 41 cases), for positive survey is .916 (10 items, 30 cases), for negative survey .811 (5 items, 30 cases). RR, RRR (see study 1). RRR-100, RRR-200, RRR-300, RRR-400, RRR-500, RRR-600 – Proceeding of data the variable RRR - index of implicit prefer- Descriptive statistics, tests of normality of ences at SOA 100, 200, 300, 400, 500, 600 distribution, boxplots, ANOVA, Repeated ms. Measures ANOVA, Factor Analysis, t-tests, Explicit measured variables. “Ethnic frequency analysis, chi-square test, Fisher‟s hypo-identity” and “ethnic hyper-identity” angular transformation, correlation analysis. obtained from factor analysis, HYPO and HYPER (see study 1); “Intensity of emotional impact” (IEI). T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 55 Results. Study2 sions: no impact: t(9192)=8.38, p < .001; The first research question. The results of IAT neutral: t(11177) = 11.91, p < .001; positive: It was found, that RT in the first session ex- = 10.45, p < .001. ceeds RT in the second session: F(1, 38263) = 526.65, p < .001. t(7322) = 10.14, p < .001; negative: t(8327) 2) “Retest”: session 1 – F(2, 19004) = 38.01, p < .001, session 2 – F(2, 19203) = Research of RT. With the help of ANO- 48.00, p < .001 (Fig.15). As shows Post Hoc VA (4x3x4x2) in each session it was found LSD, in Session 1for differences between at the following factors‟ and factors‟ interac- once – after one day: p = .001; after one day tions impact on RT. – after one week: p < .001. As shows Post 1) “Valence of episodes” in session 1 – Hoc LSD, in Session 2 for differences be- F(2, 19004) = 28.10, p < .001, in session 2 – tween at once – after one day: p = .001; after F(2, 19203) = 11.39, p < .001 (Fig.14). In one day – after one week: p < .001; at once – session 2: as shows Post Hoc LSD for differ- after one week: p = .001. Differences be- ences between negative - neutral: p < .001, tween sessions: at once: t(13770) = 8.09, neutral - positive: ns, no impact – positive, no p < .001; after one day: t(11517) = 14.65, impact and no impact – neutral: ns, positive- p < .001; after one week: t(10578) = 13.00, negative: p < .05. Differences between ses- p < .001. FIG.14. Factor’s “Valence of episodes” impact on RT I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 56 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.15. Factor’s “Retest” impact on RT In the second session RT after one day is minimal. RT after one week is maximal. al impact is maximal. RT if no impact exceeds RT at low impact. 3) “Intensity of emotional impact”: ses- 4) “Valence” and “Retest” interaction: sion 1- F(2, 19004) = 71.51, p < .001, session session 1 – F(4, 19004) = 11.36, p < .001, 2: F(3, 19203) = 61.02, p < .001 (Fig.16). As session 2: F(4, 19203) = 17.46, p < .001 shows Post Hoc LSD, in session 1 for differ- (Fig.17). At negative and at neutral episodes ences between middle – low: ns, middle - the RT at once and RT after one day differ high: p < .001, middle – no impact: ns, high – insignifically. RT after one week exceeds both no impact: p < .001, low – no impact: p < .05. of them (p < .001). At positive emotional va- As shows Post Hoc LSD, in session 2 for lence of episodes all RT in the second session differences between middle – low: ns, low- differ statistically non-significantly. Without high: p < .001, middle - high: p < .001, mid- impact all differences between RT are statisti- dle – no impact: p < .001, high – no impact: p cally significant (p < .001). The largest RT is < .001, low – no impact: p = .001. Differ- at once, then after one day and the smallest ences between sessions – low: t(9662) = 9.12, time after one week. Comparisons of control p < .001; middle: t(7880) = 11.20, p < .001; groups‟ and experimental groups‟ means in high: t(9192) = 8.38, p < .001. In the second sessions 1, 2 see in Table B1 (see Appendix session RT at middle intensity emotional im- B). pact is minimal. RT high intensity of emotion- 5) Factors‟ “Valence” and “Intensity of T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.16. Factor’s “Intensity of emotional impact” impact on RT FIG.17. Means of RT. Factors’ “Valence” and “Retest” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 57 58 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT emotional impact” interaction (Fig.18): largest RT is at high impact episodes. Com- session 1 – F(4, 19004) = 13.30, p < .001, parisons of control groups‟ and experimental session 2 – F(4, 19203) = 3.25, p = .011. The groups‟ means in sessions 1, 2 see in Table FIG.18. Factors’ “Valence” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction FIG.19. Factors’ “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT B2 (see Appendix B). 59 7) Factors‟ “Retest”, “Valence” and 6) Factors‟ “Retest” and “Intensity of “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction emotional impact” interaction. Session 1: (Fig.20). Session 1: F(7, 19004) = 32.18, F(4, 19004) = 32.07, p < .001, session 2: p < .001, Session 2: F(7, 19203) = 15.90, F(4, 19203) = 28.41, p < .001 (Fig.19). Com- p < .001. Comparisons of means for experi- parisons of control groups‟ and experimental mental and control groups see in Tables B4- groups‟ means in sessions 1, 2 see in Table B5 (Appendix B). At medium intensity of B3 (see Appendix B). emotional impact there is minimal RT after FIG.20. Means of RT. Factors’ “Retest”, “Valence” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 60 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT one day and maximal after one week, the sion exceeds the absolute value of D in the same effects negative low impact. At high second session. intensity of negative and neutral emotional 2) “Session”, “Valence” and “Retest” impact there is minimal RT at once and maxi- interaction: session 1 – F(4, 90) = 1.80, p mal after one week. At high intensity of posi- < .05, (Fig.22). At negative valence of the tive emotional impact there is minimal RT emotional impact the minimal D is observed after one week and maximal at once, also the immediately after the first session, the maxi- same if no impact. mal – after one week. At positive valence of Thus, there is a relationship between the the emotional impact the minimal D is ob- newly created associations with the object of served at once after the first session and after attitude and reaction time in the task of the one day after the first session, the maximal – Implicit Association Test. after one week. At neutral valence of the D-scores. Repeated ANOVA within- emotional impact the minimal D is observed subjects factor session (1, 2) and between- after one day and after one week after the subjects episodes first session, the maximal – at once. Without (negative, neutral, positive, no impact), retest impact the minimal D is observed immediate- (at once, after one day, after one week), in- ly after the first session, the maximal – after tensity of emotional impact (low, middle, one day and after one week. The statistical high, no impact) was conducting. The follow- significances of the differences see in Table ing factors‟ and factors‟ interactions impact B8 (Appendix B). factors: valence of on D was found. Relationship between D-scores in the 1) “Session”: F(1, 90) = 11.52, p = .001 (Fig.21). Both D in sessions 1 and 2 are nega- first session and in the second session. Pearson correlation coefficient between D in tive and absolute value of D in the first ses- the first and the second sessions is r(118) = .49, p < .001. The statistically significant relationship between D-scores in the first session and in the second session was revealed. Coincidence IAT measurement results with the results of explicit methods. Correlation analysis was applied. Percentage matching were calculated. The explicit variables “Hyper-identity” and “Hypo-identity” and implicit variable D were used. The positive values of D mean FIG.21. Means of D. Factor’s “Session” preference of Latvians, negative – preference impact of Russians. The negative correlation rela- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 61 FIG.22. Means of D. Factors’ “Valence” and “Retest” interaction tionship between “Hyper-identity” and D Percentage coincidences calculated in the means the coincidence of results. The posi- same way (Tables B6-B7, Appendix B). The tive correlation relationship between “Hypо- maximum match percentage is equal to 40%. identity” and D means the coincidence of The differences between percentages were measurments results. Spearman correlation tested using Fisher‟s Angle Transformation coefficients were calculated. Compliance φ*-test. In sessions 1 and 2 the statistically was investigated according to the scheme: significant differences were not found. (1) for all the participants in the first session; Statistically significant correlation coeffi- (2) for all the participants in the second ses- cients were not found. Percentage coinci- sion; (3) for all members of the control group dences of results were calculated. The maxi- in the first session; (4) for all members of the mum match percentage is equal to 40%. control group in the second session; (5) for Answer to the 1st research question. The all participants in the experimental group in reaction time and the measure D of implicit the first session; (6) for all experimental attitude in the task of the Implicit Association groups of participants in the second session. Test associate with the effect of contextual Statistically significant Spearman correlation exposure. coefficients were not found. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 62 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Discussion and conclusions on the first research question of Study 2 intensity of the emotional event and retest The following consistent patterns for the delay (Fig. 19), it was found that at the me- mean values of the reaction time (in the text dium intensity of emotional impact the short- simply RT) were obtained. est RT is one day after the retest delay and When taking into account the factors of Taking into account only the factor of the longest - after the one week delay. At impact valence we can state that at the neu- low intensity of the emotional impact the tral impact the mean reaction time was the retest time does not effect on the mean reac- shortest (Fig. 14). tion time. At high intensity of the emotional If taking into account the one day retest impact the mean reaction time increases with delay, the mean RT is minimal. If taking into an increase in retest delay time. In the con- account the one week retest delay, the mean trol groups, the mean reaction time decreases value of RT is maximal (Fig. 15). with an increase in retest delay time. When taking into account the valence of Thus, there is a relationship between the episodes and retest delay (Fig. 17) we can newly created associations and the object of ascertain the absence of a statistically signifi- attitude and reaction time in the task of the cant difference between the RT in the groups Implicit Association Test. with negative impact and retest without delay Mean values of the indicators of implicit and with the retest delay of one day. In preference D in both sessions are negative, groups with the negative impact and retest indicating a preference to the Russians (Fig. delay of one week the RT is longer than in 21). In the second session this preference is the same groups, but with the retest delay of weakening, but is still being kept. one day and without delay. A similar pattern At negative valence of the affective con- is observed in the group with the neutral im- textual events and retest without delay the pact. In groups with the positive impact with mean preference is zero. With increasing all retest delays the RT differs statistically retest delay time the preference to the Rus- non-significantly. In the control group with sians increases (Fig. 22). These data may the retest at once the RT is the longest and in indicate a short but quickly transient effect the control group with the one week retest - of negative impact which has increased “pro- the shortest. Latvian” preference. Perhaps this was due to The participants evaluated the intensity of reaction to negative information of mostly the emotional event as average, had the those participants who were initially more shortest RT at retest (Fig. 16). The longest RT “pro-Latvian”. was observed in participants who reported on At positive valence of the affective emo- high emotional intensity of the contextual tional event we observed mean D which did event. not significantly differ from zero at the retest T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 63 without delay and in one day. A week later, negative impact has increased as well as in the preferences to the Russians increase. Unlike control group. In the other groups the average the case of the negative impact, in this case, preference did not differ from zero. At the a more lasting effect of “pro-Latvian” prefer- week retest delay the preference to the Rus- ence was observed. sians increased only in the groups with nega- At neutral valence of the affective emo- tive and positive impacts. The last fact evi- tional event the mean value of D with retest dences of attenuation of associations activa- without delay has the absolute maximum tion due to these contextual impacts. value. Ay one day retest delays the preference decreases and at the one week delay the mean values of D were not significantly different from zero. It is possible that the imple- The second research question. Results of priming in Study 2 mentation of the explicit test by Solodatova Initial number of cases was 96000. 5% cases led to the activation of “pro-Russian” associ- (from both ends of reaction time (RT)) were ations, which quickly attenuated thereafter. removed. After that we removed the cases, In the control groups, the mean value of where was 355≤RT≤ 1033 ms. The number of D without retest delay was not statistically cases was N=89778, grand M=541 ms and significantly different from zero. At retest SD=117 ms. delays of one day the preference to the Rus- Research of RT. The mean of RT in the sians increases. At the one week delay the first session in total exceeds the mean of RT in mean values of D were not significantly dif- the second session F(1, 89424) = 344.74, ferent from zero. In this case, we see a short- p < .001. In the experimental groups mean of term effect of growth in “pro-Latvian” pref- RT in the first session exceeds the mean of RT erences due to the implementation of the in the second session: F(1;65958) = 213.12, explicit test by Soldatova. However, the p < .001. In control groups mean of RT in the emerged “pro-Latvian” associations that did first session exceeds the mean of RT in the not match the basic attitude, preserved for a second longer time, although they had a lower level p < .001. In both sessions the means of RT in of activation. experimental group exceed the means of RT in session: F(1;23464) = 151.25, Before the experimental exposure the control groups: session 1 – F(1;44304) = measure D of the IAT pointed to “pro- 102.72, p < .001, session 2 – F(1;45118) = Russian” preference. In the second measure- 185.12, p < .001. This indicates the effect of ment in all groups except for groups with the learning through the acquisition of experience neutral impact, there was a shift towards “pro obtained in the first session of the experiment, -Latvian” preference observed in the retest but requires a more detailed analysis of the without delay. At the one day delay the “pro- impact effects in the second session. Russian” preference in the group with the Using ANOVA (6x3x2x4x3x4) the study I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 64 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT of influence of the factors SOA, Prime, Tar- 10.57, p < .001. Mean of RT for baseline get, Valence of episode, Retest, Intensity of exceeds the means for Latvian and Russian emotional impact and interactions of them in (p < .001). The difference of means between both sessions was carried out. The statistical primes Latvian and Russian is not statistically significance of differences between the mean significant. In session 2: F(1;45117) = 4.96, values for different factors is established p < .01. For every prime the mean of RT in using the LSD's multiple comparison proce- the first session exceeds the mean of RT in dure. the second session (p < .001). 1) “Taget”. The average response time to 3) Factor‟s “SOA” impacts on RT. In the positive target stimuli is less than to negative session 1: F(5;44300) = 11.86, p < .001. In target 250.63, session 2: F(5;45114) = 13.74, p < .001. The p < .001. This effect was obtained in both slowest responses in both sessions are at 100 control and experimental groups and in the ms (p < .01 in comparison with 200 ms in the both sessions. Also, it is observed both be- first session and p < .001 all other cases). For fore and after the impact. This trend could be every SOA the mean of RT in the first session observed in all the series of authors‟ experi- exceeds the mean of RT in the second session ments, using the priming procedure (Plotka (p < .001) (Fig.23). stimuli: F(1;89424) = Factors “Valence” and “Retest” in the et al., 2013). 2) “Prime”. In session 1: F(2;44303) = first session point to the participants‟ behav- FIG.23. Means of RT in two sessions for factor “SOA” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 65 ior, which in the second session will or will factor‟s “Retest” level the mean of RT in the not undergo the experiment of corresponding first session exceeds the mean of RT in the valence and time of conducting of the second second session (p < .001) (Fig.25). In the session. second session the RT of the week delay is 4) Factor‟s „Valence” impact in the first session: F(3;44302) = 42.98, p < .001, in the second session: F(3;45116) = maximal, RT after one day is minimal. 6) Factor‟s “Intensity of emotional im- 122.01, pact” influence in the second session: p < .001. In every session all means differ F(3;45116) = 88.04, p < .001. At low and statistically significant (p < .01, p < .001). middle levels the means of RT differs non For every factor‟s “Valence” level the mean significantly. Middle and high levels – of RT in the first session exceeds the mean of p < .001. High impact and no impact – RT in the second session (p ≤ .001) (Fig.24). p < .001. In every session all means differ In the second session RT of negative episodes statistically significant (p < .001). For every is maximal, then for positive episodes. The factor‟s “Intensity of emotional impact” level minimal RT is observed without impact. the mean of RT in the first session exceeds 5) Factor‟s “Retest” impact in the first each of the means of RT in the second session session: F(2;44303) = 259.57, p < .001, in the (p < .001) (Fig.26). At high intensity of emo- second tional impact the longest RT is observed. session: F(2;45117) = 513.77, p < .001. In every session all means differ statistically significant (p < .001). For every Also the following factors‟ interactions were found (see Appendix C): FIG.24. Means of RT in two sessions for factor “Valence” I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 66 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.25. Means of RT in two sessions for factor “Retest” FIG.26. Means of RT in two sessions for factor “Valence” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 67 7) “SOA” and “Target”. Session 1: week), intensity of emotional impact (low, F(5;43874) = 0.50, ns. Session 2: F(5;44076) middle, high, no impact). ANOVA was con- = 3.27, p < .01 (Fig.C1); ducting for each SOA and session (results see 8) “Valence” and “Target”. Session 1: F(3;44298) = 4.68, p < .01. Session 2: F(3;45112) = 7.21, p < .001 (Fig.C2); in Table D1, Appendix D). Research of priming-effect. With the help of One-Sample t-test the means of R in 9) “Retest” and “Target”. Session 1: different conditions were compared with ze- F(2;44300) = 7.71, p < .001. Session 2: ro. Statistically significant results shows the F(2;45114) = 4.86, p < .01 (Fig.C3); priming effect. The priming effect is illustrat- 10) “Intensity of emotional impact” and ed in Tables D2-D6 (Appendix D). (Fig.27). “Target”. Session 2: F(3;45112) = 9.36, The priming effect was observed with all the p < .001 (Fig.C4); considered SOAs 100-600 ms under various 11) “Valence” and “Retest”. Session 1: F(6;44294) = 110.44, p < .001. Session 2: F(6;45108) = 161.29, p < .001 (Fig.C5); 12) “Valence” and “Intensity of emotional impact”. Session 2: F(4;45110) = 84.52, p < .001 (Fig.C6); experimental conditions. Research of RR. With the help of ANOVA the impact of following variables and their interactions was found. 1) Factor SOA. Session 1: F(5, 1296) = 2.29, p < .05, session 2: F(5, 1092) = 2.50, 13) “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional p < .05 (Fig.28). At SOA 300 ms the mean of impact”. Session 2: F(6;45108) = 110.69, RR statistically significant differs from zero p < .001 (Fig.C7); (t(239) = 3.65, p < .001). 14) “Target”, “Retest” and “Valence”. 2) Factor SOA in two sessions in experi- Session 1: F(6;44282) = 3.76, p = .001. Ses- mental and control groups. In session 2: sion 2: F(6;45096) = 2.21, p < .05 (Fig.C8); SOA*Group F(5, 1428) = 2.62, p < .05. At 15) “Target”, “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional impact”. Session 2: F(6;45096) = 3.67, p = .001 (Fig.C9); 16) “Valence”, “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional impact”. Session 2: F(7;45091) = 103.80, p < .001 (Fig.C10); Research of facilitation scores. Repeated ANOVA within-subjects factors: prime (Latvian, Russian), target (good words, bad words). Between-subjects factors: valence of episodes (negative, neutral, positive, no impact), retest (at once, after one day, after one SOA 500 ms the mean of RR statistically significant differs from zero (Fig.29). 3) “Valence*SOA”. Session 1 F(10, 1296) = 1.97, p < .05. Session 2 F(10, 1296) = 2.03, p < .05 (Fig.30). 4) “Valence*Retest” in session 2: F(4, 1296) = 3.32, p = .010 (Fig.31). 5) SOA*retest*valency*IEI in session 2: F(35, 1092) = 1.55, p < .05 (Fig.32). 6) SOA*retest*valency in session 2: F(20, 1296) = 2.12, p < .01(Fig.33). Research of RRR. One sample t-test I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 68 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.27. The means of R for each SOA, Valence, Retest showed, that at SOA 100 ms the mean of In the first session the mean of RRR is RRR statistically significant differs from zero near to zero and exceeds the negative mean (t(239) = -2.9, p<.01). In session 1 also at of RT in the second session: F(1, 1194) = SOA=100 ms the mean of RRR statistically 4.31, p < .05 (Fig.34). significant differs from zero (t(119) = -.57, p < .05). At SOA=600 ms RRR in the first session exceeds RRR in the second session (F(1, T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.28. Means of RR. Factor SOA in two sessions FIG.29. Means of RR. Factor SOA in two sessions in experimental and control groups I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 69 70 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.30. Means of RR. Factors’ SOA*valence interaction in two sessions FIG.31. Means of RR. Factors’ retest*valence interaction in the second session T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 71 FIG.32. Means of RR. Factors’ IEI*retest*valence interaction in the second session 238) = 5.03, p < .05). (Fig.35). Repeated ANOVA within-subjects fac- The reaction time, priming effect of tors: SOA, Session. Between-subjects fac- measures of implicit attitudes in the task of tors: valence of episodes, retest, intensity of subliminal affective priming are associated emotional impact founded the factor‟s Ses- with the effect of contextual impact. sion impact (F(1,106) = 4.21, p < .05) I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 72 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Fig.33. Means of RR. Factors’ SOA*retest*valence interaction in the second session Discussion and conclusions on the second research question of Study 2 Factor of the retest delay. The main pattern which is most often manifested in the different experimental conditions is that the SOA factor. The maximum reaction time is maximum reaction time at the presence of the observed without the contextual event with contextual event is exposed at the one week SOA 100 ms. retest delay. It is possible that without the T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 73 Fig.34. Means of RRR. Factor “Session” Fig.35. Means of RRR. Factor “SOA” contextual event the maximum reaction time associations lose the activation faster than the is observed at the retest without delay – there newly created ones which maintain it for is the priming effect of repetition of identical longer. tasks in a small time interval. Contextual The factor of subjective evaluation of events create new associations with the object of attitude and remain their strength intensity of the affective contextual event. The maximal reaction time is observed at longer. The experimental conditions can dis- high subjective evaluation of the intensity of tort this pattern. For example, in the groups the event. With the one week delay and the with positive valence of the contextual event presence of contextual events of negative or with negative target stimuli the maximum neutral valence the maximal reaction time is reaction time has become apparent in the observed with the events of medium affective group with the retest without delay. In the intensity. groups with positive valence of the contextual event with high subjective evaluation of The factor of valence of the affective contextual event. Taking into account only the strength of its impact the maximum reac- the factor of valence of the impact we can tion time is also observed in the retest with- state that at the neutral impact the reaction out delay. time is the shortest, but it exceeds the reac- The minimal reaction time is observed in tion time in the control groups (in the absence many cases for retest in one day, both in the of contextual events). The maximal reaction case of the contextual event, and without it. It time is observed in the experimental groups is possible that at the one day retest the old with positive or negative impacts. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 74 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT With the one week retest delay the maxi- ber of corresponding trends (Fig.36, 37). mal RT is observed in the group with the Analysis of the effects of affective prim- negative impact. With the retest without de- ing was carried out in terms of an approach lay and with the delay of one day the maxi- based on the concept of activation spread in mal RT was observed in the groups with the the semantic network (Collins & Loftus, positive impact. 1975; Collins & Quillian, 1969). The priming effect was observed for all values of SOA in various experimental conditions. Control groups The third research question. Study 2 The Pearson‟s correlation coefficients between implicit variables RRR at different SOA and D in the first and the second sessions and explicit variables HYPO, HYPER, “Hypo-identity” and “Hyper-identity” see Table E1 in Appendix E. Initial data were subjected to the factor analysis by the method of principal component analysis. For the control group we received 5 factors explaining 66.1% of the total variance. For the experimental groups we received 5 factors explaining 47.1% of the total variance. See the rotated component matrix (obtained by VARIMAX rotation) in Tables G1-G2, Appendix G. The role of the explicit method by Soldatova in the retest. The participants in the control and experimental groups after performing the explicit tests of attitudes performed the explicit test according to the method by Soldatova. Performance of this test in itself was an episodic event, relevant to attitudes. In the procedure of subliminal priming the task did not imply access to conscious associations related to the ethnic attitude. The task in the procedure of the IAT also did not require direct evaluation of ethnic preferences, although it implied the association of semantically positive and negative words with the name of an ethnic group. The explicit procedure, as opposed to implicit, required from participants the direct arbitrary evaluation of their attitude towards their own Discussion and conclusion on the third research question of Study 2 and alternative ethnic groups. Answers to the Correlation analysis of the relationship object of ethnic attitude and propositional among magnitudes of index of implicit pref- processing of the information that has been erences (RRR) for different values of SOA extracted from the memory as needed. As a both in the first session, and in the repeated result of this processing the new conclusions (after experimental exposure) showed a num- and associations stored in memory could items of the test by Soldatova required the activation of existing associations with the T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 75 FIG.36. System of correlations among explicit measures of ethnic attitudes and RRR variables for different values of SOA in control groups. Thick lines - statistically significant correlations at the level of p < .05, thin lines - at the level of p < .10. Pro-L1 (HYPO) Pro-L2 (“hypo-identity”) scales, Pro-R1 (HYPER) Pro-R2 (“hyperidentity”) scales according to Soldatova’s technique. RRR-100 – RRR-600 - implicit measures of ethnic attitudes for the corresponding values of SOA and D (IAT) in the first measurement. RRR are indexes of implicit preferences. emerge. Therefore, the participants in the Therefore, we hypothesized that the se- control group were also not devoid of rele- cond measurement in the control groups vant contextual influences caused by work should also have reflected the significant with the explicit test of ethnic attitudes. contextual influences associated with per- The difference between the control and forming the explicit test (the questionnaire of experimental groups thus was only in the fact ethnic preferences) by participants. that the participants of the latter were shown the movies which created new associations Analysis of correlations for control groups. The first trend obtained in the first relevant to ethnic attitude and activated pre- session is defined by the presence of statisti- viously formed associations in addition to cally significant correlations of similar con- those that have emerged when performing tent at SOA 100, 300 and 500 ms with 200 the explicit test. ms lag. So the authors found a negative cor- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 76 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIG.37. System of correlations among explicit measures of ethnic attitudes and RRR variables for different values of SOA in experimental groups. Thick lines - statistically significant correlations at the level of p < .05, thin lines - at the level of p < .10. Pro-L1 (HYPO) Pro-L2 (“hypo-identity”) scales, Pro-R1 (HYPER) Pro-R2 (“hyperidentity”) scales according to Soldatova’s technique. RRR-100 – RRR-600 - implicit measures of ethnic attitudes for the corresponding values of SOA and D2 (IAT) in the second measurement. RRR are indexes of implicit preferences. relation between the RRR value at SOA = strength of the effect of RRR-100 is associat- 100 ms (RRR-100) and measure of attitude D ed with a degree of implicit preference to- IAT. Negative values of D correspond to the wards the participant‟s ethnic group identity pro-Russian attitude, so the higher RRR-100, and reflects activation of the latent uncon- the stronger the strength of pro-Russian atti- scious automatic associations which deter- tude. With this in mind, we can assume that mine the implicit “automatic” ethnic attitude. RRR-100 reflects the effect of the pro- When SOA = 300 ms there were statisti- Russian implicit preference (attitude). The cally significant positive correlations of RRR absence of other significant correlations of -300 (the first measurement) with the scores this measure with others suggests that the on scales HYPER and “Hyper-identity” of T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 77 the explicit test of ethnic preferences by 200 ms, starting from SOA 100 ms, as we Soldatova, also corresponding to “pro- can assume, reflect the effect of strength of Russian” attitude. Higher values of RRR-300 the pro-Russian automatic implicit attitude correspond to higher scores of the Scales of (RRR-100) and the ethnic implicit attitude of Ethnic Identity. Therefore, we can assume hyper-identity (RRR-300). While the magni- that the value of RRR-300 reflects the ethnic tude of RRR-500 may be related to the de- implicit attitude of hyper-identity, which is in gree of activation of negative associations line with the explicit attitudes. towards the alternative ethnic group. In addition, we found a statistically sig- Considering the results of this experiment nificant positive correlation of values of RRR in the context of ideas about the mechanisms -300 and RRR-500 (the first measurement). of spreading the activation in associative This points to the similarities in content of networks related to the effect of priming, we the processes, determining the ethnic prefer- can assume the existence of a kind of a ence at this value of SOA. “wave” to spread the activation, which af- RRR-500 (the first measurement) nega- fects the relevant basic associations of the tively correlates with the value of ethnic pref- existing ethnic attitude. SOA times define erence RRR and the scales HYPO, “Hypo- the time period during which the activation identity” in Soldatova‟s technique, which of different groups of associations connected corresponds to the “pro-Latvian” explicit to the object of attitude may occur. The attitude. Thus, the greater the effect of im- smaller the value of SOA, the stronger the plicit preferences at SOA-500, the less the strength of associations with the object of “pro-Latvian” explicit attitude. It can be as- attitude and they have more automatic na- sumed that high values of RRR-500 are relat- ture. The higher the value of SOA, the less ed to the activation of negative associations, automatic the associations are and they could adjoining the alternative ethnic group. Anoth- be related to the previously occurred proposi- er explanation could be the inhibition of posi- tional processes such as, for example, the tive information on alternative ethnic group. Meta-Cognitive Model involves. Processes With high values of RRR-500 the strength of determining RRR-300, as shown by its asso- “pro-Latvian” associations decreases and ciation with explicit measures of ethnic atti- vice versa. In general, processes which define tude may be based on associations of this the value of RRR-500 may be associated with type. activation of negative associations towards The “wave” begins with early SOA val- the alternative ethnic groups or inhibition of ues and initially affects the associations of the positive. the automatic implicit attitude and then, with All three groups of distinguished statisti- a lag of 200 ms, activates the strongest “pro- cal relationships of RRR at SOA with a lag of Russian” associations of ethnic identity un- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 78 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT derlying the explicit attitude (RRR-300), and, sumed wave of activation spread. The exist- finally negative, not “pro-Latvian” associa- ence of the second wave of associations‟ acti- tions (RRR-500). vation spread is supported by a number of The second trend is less pronounced for emerged correlations RRR-200 and RRR-400 the conditions of first test session, but be- (200 ms lag). In contrast to the first measure- comes more pronounced in the second. In the ment session, in which the preference of RRR first session, with SOA 200 ms we marked -400 did not have statistically significant negative correlation of RRR with evaluation correlations with other variables, in the se- on the scales HYPER and “Hyper-identity” in cond session with this value of SOA we Soldatova‟s technique. This means that the found a positive correlation of RRR-400 with higher RRR-200, the lower the “pro-Russian” the scale HYPO in Soldatova‟s technique. implicit preference and vice versa. The ab- This relationship reflects the activation of sence of other correlations of RRR-200 with “pro-Latvian” relations which are associated other variables (except RRR-600, as will be with the positive explicit “pro-Latvian” atti- discussed below) can be attributed to the ex- tude. The positive correlation between RRR- isting status of “pro-Latvian” associations 200 and RRR-400 is close to significant too, among carriers of mostly “pro-Russian” of which also speaks in favor of affiliation of attitude prior to the experimental exposure. the observed effects of implicit preferences to Arguments in favor of existence of the se- one process. An important corresponding cond wave of associations‟ activation will be evidence was the fact that in the second ses- given in the light of discussion of the effects sion the variable RRR-200 had statistically of the second session. significant relationship with the scale “Hypo- The data of the second session in this case identity” (“pro-Latvian” explicit attitude). combine data of participants in all experi- All these data indicate the relationship be- mental groups, regardless of the time inter- tween RRR with SOA = 200 ms and 400 ms vals between the repeated measurement and with the strength of “pro-Latvian” explicit the first measurement, and affective valence attitude. of showed videos. The indirect evidence of the legitimacy of The second session of measurements was classifying RRR-400 to the effects of the se- made after experimental exposure and acti- cond wave of activation is the presence of the vated the association of both the pre-existing positive correlation between RRR-400 in the ethnic “pro-Russian” attitude and contrary to first and second sessions. An interesting it “pro-Latvian” associations, newly formed point is the lack of statistically significant by the video with positive presentation of the correlations between the preference RRR for alternative ethnic group. This impact should these values of SOA and implicit measures D in theory be reflected in the correlation rela- of the IAT. tionships both the first and the second as- Thus, the associations which are activated T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT within the asynchronyzation band of the se- 79 measured by the IAT. cond “wave” to be associated with the ex- Thus, the experimental exposure and plicit “pro-Latvian” attitude, but not with formation of new “pro-Latvian” associations implicit automatic associations that were related to it have changed the nature of acti- measured by IAT procedures and subliminal vation of automatic “pro-Russian” associa- emotional priming with SOA 100 ms in the tion with SOA = 100. first session. Simultaneously, the values of RRR-300 An analysis of the waves‟ nature changes and RRR-500 negatively correlated with after experimental intervention has revealed each other, i.e. the higher value of RRR with the following. SOA 500 ms corresponded to lower values We suppose that the experimental inter- of RRR with SOA 300 ms and vice versa. vention created new associations, including Thus, with increasing values of RRR-300 contrary to the basic attitude. associated with the processes of the first This should have an impact on the results “wave” of activation spread of pro-Russian” of implicit measurements in the second ses- associations there is a decrease in the value sion. Below we consider some general ef- of RRR-500 that in the first session was asso- fects of this influence. ciated with the activation of not “pro- The common results provided intriguing Latvian” associations. On this basis, we can testifies to the changes of correlations be- assume that in the second session the tween variables associated with each of the strengthening of “pro-Russian” association “waves”, as well as logically consistent links with SOA 300 ms could be accompanied by between the “waves”. a simultaneous increase in the activation of First, during the second measurement we “pro-Latvian” associations with SOA = 500 found a negative statistically significant cor- ms due to the processes decreasing RRR- relation between RRR-100 and scales of ex- 500. This increased activation of “pro- plicit attitude HYPER and “Hyper-identity” Latvian” associations may be due to separate in Soldatova‟s technique (“pro-Russian” influence of experimental exposure to differ- attitude). The higher value of RRR-100 cor- ent components of ethnic attitude. The video responded to a decrease in the “pro-Russian” with “pro-Russian” content could strengthen attitude and vice versa. In contrast, in the the “pro-Russian” associations of the basic first measurement session, there was a nega- implicit attitude (RRR-300), but contrary to tive relationship between the value of RRR- them, the newly formed by the “pro- 100 and measure D of implicit attitude (it Latvian” video fresh and not coinciding with corresponds to the negative values of D), the basic attitude associations increased the obtained by the IAT. Therefore, the higher positive attitude to the Latvians that would value of RRR-100 corresponded to the have resulted in the decrease of RRR-500. stronger “pro-Russian” implicit attitude The correlations of RRR-300 variables in I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 80 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT the first and the second session was negative, tions with SOA 200 ms, 400 ms underlying which could also indicate the decrease in the the explicit “pro-Latvian” attitude is also level of activation of “pro-Russian” associa- apparent. This suggests that the video of tions in the second session compared to the “pro-Latvian” content strengthened the earli- first session under the influence of new con- er positive associations towards the alterna- tradictory associations. tive ethnical group. The system of correlations between RRR A separate question relates to the identi- variables, associated with the first wave of fication of functions of the processes related activation spread of associations in the se- to the effect of activation of associations cond session is characterized by loss of all, with SOA 600 ms. with the exception of RRR-100, statistically Correlations of RRR-600 with other vari- significant correlations of these variables ables could be logically considered in rela- with the scales of explicit attitudes. This fact tion to the phenomena and processes, reflect- may indicate that the experimental exposure ed by the second “wave” (SOA 200 ms, 400 has made a change in the structure of associ- ms, 600 ms). However, this assumption ations connected to the “pro-Russian” atti- turned out to be wrong. The variable RRR- tude, which have been activated with the 600 in the first session correlated positively help of implicit measurement procedures. with the variables of the first “wave,” and Thus, the implicit measures became context- negatively dependent. “wave” of the spread of activation), which with RRR-200 (the second This assumption corresponds to the clearly defines the RRR-600 belonging to the changes in correlations between variables processes related to the activation of “pro- that reflect the processes of the second wave Russian” associations and the prohibition of activation spread of associations related to (inhibition) of not “pro-Russian.” The ab- the “pro-Latvian” attitude. For example, the sence of other links between RRR-600 and value of RRR-200 (the indicator of activation variables reflecting the processes of the se- of “pro-Latvian” associations) was negative- cond wave in the two measurement sessions ly correlated with the value of RRR-500, means that this variable was not directly which also supports the idea that viewing the related to the inhibition or relief of “pro- “pro-Latvian” video could have increased Latvian” associations and affected only the the strength of “pro-Latvian” associations strength of “pro-Russian.” Indirectly, it with SOA 200 ms (the second wave), and could be related to the inhibition of “pro- decreased the strength of “pro-Latvian” as- Latvian” associations, but only in the first sociations with SOA 500 ms (see the data of session and only in connection with the the first “wave” of activation). events of the first wave of activation spread. According to the data of the second ses- Most intriguing was the result of a new sion, the activation of “pro-Latvian” associa- relationship between RRR-600 and RRR-100 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 81 in the second session, which was not ob- lead to a decrease in strength of the explicit served in the first session of the measure- “pro-Russian” of attitude. The latter is simi- ments, and the disappearance of other corre- lar to what happened with the processes that lation links, previously characteristic for determine RRR-100. RRR-600 in the first session. Identified patterns were confirmed in the The chain of correlation links in this case formed a system in which higher values of results of the factor analysis in the control group. RRR-100 corresponded to lower values of the “pro-Russian” explicit attitude and higher values of RRR-600. The reverse is true. Thus, high values of RRR-600 (through the pro- Factor analysis for control groups cesses that reflect the increase in RRR-100) Initial data were subjected to the factor ana- were indirectly associated in the first case lysis by the method of principal component with lower values of the “pro-Russian” im- analysis. We obtained 5 factors explaining plicit attitude (the “pro-Russian” wave of 66.1% of the total variance. See the rotated activation spread of associations, the second component matrix (obtained by VARIMAX session), in the second - with slower values rotation) in Table G1, Appendix G. attitude The first factor includes variables RRR- (through processes, reflected in the increase 300, RRR-500 obtained in both measure- of RRR-500) and the higher values of “pro- ments, as well as the variable RRR-600 Russian” attitude (through processes, reflect- (measured in the first session) and, with a ed in the increase of RRR-300) in the first relatively low load factor, the explicit varia- session (the “pro-Russian” wave of activa- ble of Hyper-identity. Previously, all the tion spread of associations). Finally, in the variables of preference RRR have been at- first session the high values of RRR-600 tributed to the first wave of spread of “pro- could also indirectly (through processes, Russian” associations connected with the reflected in the decrease of RRR-200) corre- object of ethnic attitude. This factor can be spond to higher values of the “pro-Russian” labeled as a “pro-Russian” component of explicit attitude. In all cases, the opposite is ethnic attitude” (Ethnocentrism, ethnic fanat- true. icism). All factor loadings, except for one, of the “pro-Latvian” explicit The experimental exposure has disrupted the observed in the first session logic of cor- have a positive sign. An exception is the variable RRR (2) -300. relation links of RRR-600 with other varia- Associations activated with SOA 300 bles as an indicator of the processes associat- characterize “pro-Russian” preference. In ed with activation of “pro-Russian” associa- this case, in the second measurement (mea- tions. Instead, an RRR-600 turned out to be surement in the second session), this prefer- indirectly related to the processes that can ence is negatively related to the factor. In I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 82 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT general, this can be explained by the effect of This factor is in good agreement with the the contextual impact. High “pro-Russian” idea of the second wave of activation prolif- preference RRR (2) -300 corresponds to low- eration of “pro-Latvian” associations er “pro-Russian” preference with other values This factor can be designated as “loyal of variables RRR related to the factor both in hypoidentity” and may reflect the effects of the first and in the second measurement. The the contextual impact on the process of acti- explicit “pro-Russian” preference was lower vation of basic associations underlying the as well. And vice versa. One explanation for preference to the alternative ethnic group. this interesting fact may be due to the emer- The third factor includes variables RRR- gence of new associations as a result of the 100, RRR (2)-600, RRR (1)-200 of the first contextual event, related to the object of “pro and second measurements with positive fac- -Russian” attitude. Their activation when tor loads and the explicit variable of hyperi- repeated implicit measurement could be a dentity with the negative factor load. Mean- separate process, not yet integrated with ingfully, these variables reflect the effect of those processes that are related to the activa- the contextual impact, which is associated tion of associations of the basic “pro- with the emergence of new associations that Russian” attitude. The absence of such inte- are contrary to the basic “pro-Russian” atti- gration could lead to a negative load in varia- tude. The emergence of these associations ble RRR (2) -300, as the activation of new could inhibit the activation of basic “pro- associations with SOA 300 did not lead to the Russian” associations and activate the basic activation of the old associations which de- “pro-Latvian” associations. termined the values of relevant variables in This factor can be labeled as “disloyal the first measurement and were mostly iden- hypoidentity” and corresponds to the ethnic tical both at implicit, and explicit measure- attitude with which the emergence of nega- ments of ethnic attitude. tive ethnic associations decreases “pro- The second factor includes variables, presumably belonging to the second wave of Russian” preference and increases “proLatvian.” spread of activation of associations. It is as- The fourth factor includes variables RRR sociated with hypoidentity i.e. “pro-Latvian” (1)-100 (with the negative factor load) and attitude. “Pro-Russian” preference RRR (2)- variables D of the first and second measure- 200 and RRR (2)-400 corresponds to hy- ments of the IAT procedure and the variable poidentity and low implicit preference RRR of hypoidentity. The value of the factor is (1)-500 which is associated with “pro- determined by the variables of the implicit Russian” attitude (first factor). The factor attitude D, high values of which correspond describes the loyal attitude towards the alter- to “pro-Latvian” attitude. This also corre- native ethnicity, while maintaining “pro- sponds to the positive factor load of the ex- Russian” implicit attitude. plicit variable of hypoidentity. Thus, the fac- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 83 tor relates to “pro-Latvian” associations severe effects of contextual impacts will be which define implicit and explicit measures reflected in the results obtained. of ethnic attitude and are associated with a Preliminary analysis showed that the decrease in “pro-Russian” preference with number of significant correlation coefficients SOA 100 ms. The factor can be defined as decreased compared to the ones observed in “implicit hypoidentity.” the control group (Fig.37). The fifth factor is of unclear nature and At the same time there is a decrease in associated with variables RRR-400 (the first the number of statistically significant associ- and second measurement), as well as RRR ations between the variables included in the (1)-600. The last variable is included with previously described hypothetical “waves” the positive load into the first factor which of activation of associations. We attribute corresponds to hyperidentity (“pro-Russian” this to the imposition of effects of different attitude). activation processes of associations which The variable RRR (2)-400 was part of the have emerged under the influence of the second factor which reflects the loyal attitude demonstration of positive, neutral and nega- to the alternative ethnic group, while main- tive videos, as well as different patterns of taining a positive attitude towards their own. activation fading in newly formed and previ- The variable RRR (1)-400 was not included in any of the other factors with the factor ously existed associations with the object of attitude at different times of the retest delay. load in excess of the set threshold. It can be The first notable result is the lack of sta- assumed that this factor is associated with tistically significant associations between the the activation of “pro-Russian” associations, variables RRR in the first measurement (First but within the framework of the second wave session). These variables, while being not of spread of associations. It is related to significantly correlated with each other, had those associations which involve the loyal significant correlations with variables in the “pro-Russian” attitude and affect the loyal second measurement (Second session). The attitude towards the alternative ethnic group. latter in a few cases statistically significantly correlated with each other as well. The second result was the emergence of Experimental groups statistically significant correlations between RRR-500 in the first and the second measurement with the variable “strength of the Correlation analysis for experimental groups. The analysis of correlations, given emotional impact” of the contextual event. the data of all experimental groups was per- the questionnaire by which the participants formed. This is due to a number of sampling reported about the strength of the emotional limitations and the assumption that the most feelings, which they have experienced while This variable was measured on the basis of I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 84 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT watching the video shown. This fact clarifies tem of correlations reflects primarily the type the role of the processes that determine RRR of ethnic preferences, in which the higher with SOA 500 ms. The lower RRR-500 implicit “pro-Russian” preference corre- (lower sponds “pro-Russian” preference) corre- to the higher explicit “pro- sponded to a higher strength value of the Latvian” (RRR-100 and RRR-200 in the first emotional impact of the video, and vice ver- measurement). At the same time, the higher sa. Given that the statistically significant cor- is the “pro-Russian” preference RRR-300 relation was also observed between RRR-500 (the second measurement) which positively in the first measurement, i.e., before the ex- correlates to RRR-200. However, the higher perimental exposure, and evaluation of its “pro-Russian” preference with SOA 300 ms emotional strength, conducted after the expo- corresponds to the lower “pro-Russian” im- sure, the processes that determine the RRR- plicit preference RRR-600 and D (second 500 are probably closely associated connect- measurement) and, accordingly, the larger ed with the affective component of attitude. strength of the emotional impact of the even- In other words, these processes involve pro- tual context. Results of the first session of cessing of emotionally significant infor- measurements reflected in the mean for the mation relating to attitude. Thus, in the “pro- sample of experimental groups the existing Russian” participants any exposure, emotion- attitude that reflected both “pro-Russian” and al in valence, caused lower affective reaction “pro-Latvian” preference (the case of reten- than in “pro-Latvian”. The variable of the tion of ethnic identity with positive attitude strength of emotional impact has become to the alternative ethnicity). statistically significantly correlated with RRR The experimental contextual impact with -600 in the second measurement too. This its larger subjective strength could cause the shows that process with this value of SOA processes associated with a decrease in “pro- are also related to the processing of affective Russian” attitude RRR-600 and an increase in information associated with attitude. And in “pro-Russian” attitude (with SOA 300 ms), this case, the higher” pro-Russian” prefer- previously referred to the basic “pro- ence corresponded to lower values of affec- Russian” ethnic attitude. These relationships tive significance of the contextual event. The indicate that the contextual impact manifest- variable RRR-600 was also negatively associ- ed itself with SOA 600. A high value of the ated in the second measurement with the strength of emotional impact of the event measure of the implicit attitude D of the IAT was directly related to the decrease in “pro- in the second measurement (the lower value Russian” attitude, though indirectly it was of D corresponds to the “pro-Russian atti- associated with its increase with SOA 300 tude”). ms. RRR-400 (the second measurement) may Evaluating the resulting correlation pat- correspond to “pro-Latvian” attitude, as indi- tern as a whole, it may be noted that the sys- cated by the positive association of this vari- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 85 able with RRR-100 in the first measurement measures of implicit attitude in the IAT pro- and the data of the control group. In this case, cedure. High value of the factor corresponds the positive relationship of RRR-600 (the to “pro-Latvian” attitude. The factor can be second measurement) with RRR-400 (the first designated as a factor of Latvian identity. measurement) testifies to an increase in “pro- The third factor includes variables of Latvian” preference with the high value of “pro-Russian” preference RRR(1)-400 and the strength of emotional impact of the con- RRR(2)-600 with positive factor loads and textual event. two variables with negative ones (RRR(2)- In the second measurement RRR 400 was 200 and RRR(2)-300). High values of the positively associated with RRR-200 (the first factor were in those participants who had measurement). Since all the relationships of pronounced “pro-Russian” preference with RRR-400 with other variables indicated the SOA 400 ms before the experimental expo- mediated association with explicit “pro- sure, and with 600 ms – after the experi- Latvian” attitude, RRR-200 and RRR-400 can mental exposure. At the same time in the be, as in the case of the control group, at- second measurement they had low “pro- tributed to the second wave of activation of Russian” implicit preference with SOA 200 associations related to the object of attitude. and 300 ms. Such a data pattern can be ex- Factor analysis for experimental groups. As a result of factor analysis five plained by the influence of the context, factors were revealed. Russian” preference with SOA 200-300 ms. which had a negative impact on “pro- The first factor includes variables Perhaps this fact reflects the effect of emer- RRR-100 of both measurements. Its high val- gence, under the influence of the experi- ues correspond to “pro-Russian” preference mental exposure, of newly created associa- with the given value of SOA and lower “pro- tions, contrary to the basic attitude or supple- Russian” preference of RRR(2)-400, as well menting it. as the lower subjective evaluation of the in- The fourth factor includes the variables tensity of emotional impact of the contextual related to “pro-Russian” implicit preference event. The factor reflects the effects of the and explicit attitude, as well as the variable contextual event. It characterizes the stability RRR(2)-400. Signs of factor loads between of the existing expressed “automatic” “pro- variables that in the control group were re- Russian” attitude to affective influences of ferred to the wave of activation of “pro- contextual events in those participants who Russian” associations, in this case, do not were not emotionally affected by the plots of coincide. Therefore, the lower value of the the videos shown and had lower “pro- factor corresponds to higher “pro-Russian” Russian” preference with SOA 400 ms in the hyperidentity and higher implicit preference second measurement. at RRR-500 in the second measurement, but The second factor includes variables D - lower “pro-Russian” preference at RRR(1)- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 86 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 300 and RRR(2)-400. And vice versa. The sociations, contradictory to the basic “pro- factor describes ethnic preferences of those Russian” attitude and corresponding to previ- participants in the experimental groups, who ously formed “pro-Latvian” associations. prior to the experiment had either low implic- The most labile and sensitive to the im- it “pro-Russian” preference at RRR(1)-300, pact of the newly created association, contra- but high scores on the scale of hyperidentity, dictory to the basic attitude, are processes or vice-versa. As a result of the experimental observed with SOA 100 ms and 600 ms, and exposure they had a decrease in “pro- also the processes observed with SOA 300 Russian” preference, found with SOA 500 ms and 500 ms. ms in the second measurement. These processes generally reflect the The fifth factor includes two variables - change of implicit measures in the direction RRR(1)-200 and the explicit variable of HY- of the growing influence of loyal associa- POIDENTITY, both with a positive sign. tions to the alternative ethnic group while This factor can be designated as “loyal hy- retaining the existing basic attitude to the poidentity.” The factor describes the loyal own ethnic group. attitude as to their own, as to the alternative To define the functions and role of the ethnic group. Associations activated with processes determining the RRR preference SOA 200 ms, define both explicit “pro- with SOA 600 ms the further research is Latvian” preference (attitude) and implicit needed. “pro-Russian.” The factor describes this atti- The obtained results provide the neces- tude in the period before the experimental sary preconditions for data analysis with exposure. application of structural modeling proce- Summary. The existence of two separate, dures. but interrelated waves of spread of relevant association activation defining ethnic attitudes is possible. The fourth research question. The changes of RRR and D One wave relates to the activation of as- The means and boxplots of implicit variables sociations of the (positive) attitude object to RRR at different SOA and D in the first and the own ethnic group; its effect is manifested the second sessions and the statistical signifi- with SOA values of 100, 300, 500, and par- cance of differences see in Appendix F tially on 600 ms. (Figures F1-F9 and Tables F1-F15). The second wave is linked with activation of associations that characterizes the attitude towards alternative ethnic group, and its effects are revealed with SOA values of 200 ms RRR-100 and 400 ms. The second wave effects are Groups with the negative impact. Preference more expressed after the creation of new as- in the Russian group (the group with the neg- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 87 ative impact) without delay is statistically Groups with the positive impact. Statisti- more significant (p < .10) than in the similar cally significant differences between groups control group. with different delays of retests were not Groups with the positive impact. Statisti- found, but these differences (p < .05) were cally significant differences between groups between the group with the positive impact with different delays in the retest were not (delay of one day of the retest) and the group found. with the negative impact (delay of one week of the retest). Positive attitude towards the Russians in the first of these two groups is RRR-200 higher than in the second. Groups with the negative impact. There were RRR-300 no differences between the results of repeated measurements without delays and with Groups with the negative impact. Statistical- one day delay. Preference to the Russians ly significant differences between the groups without delays and with one day delay is with different delays of retest were not significantly higher (p < .05 and p < .01, found. respectively) than in the similar group with Groups with the positive impact. In the one week delay of the retest. This means a group with the retest without delays and with shift of preference towards the alternative one day delay the preference RRR is statisti- ethnic group. Preference to the Russians in cally significantly higher (p < .10 in both the experimental group who watched the cases) than in the control group without de- negative video and passed retest in a week lay. was significantly lower than in any of the No other statistically significant differ- control groups (p < .01). Thus, the experi- ences between the groups with different de- mental exposure has statistically significant- lays of the retest were found. ly increased the preference to the Russians during the retest without delay and with one day delay and reduced this preference with the week delay of the retest. Comparing the results of the two sessions revealed a statistically significant difference between the average preference RRR when retest with the delay of one week (U = 16.0, p < .10). In the second session, the average preference of the Russians reduced from 10.5 to 6.5 ms. RRR-400 Groups with the negative impact. Statistically significant differences between the groups with different delays of the retest were not found. However, in the group with the retest delay of one day the preference to the Russians was significantly higher than in the similar control group (p < .01) and in the I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 88 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT control group with the immediate retest is significantly higher than in the control (p < .05). Preference to the Russians in the group with the retest delay of one day group with the negative impact and retest (p < .10) and lower than in the control group delay of one day is higher than in the groups with the retest delay of one week (p < .10). with the positive impact and retest delays of Preference to the Russians in the group one week (p < .05) and without delay with the retest delay of one week is signifi- (p < .01). Preference to the Russians in the cantly lower than in the group with the posi- group with the negative impact and immedi- tive impact and similar delay of the retest ate retest is higher than in the similar group (p < .10). with the positive impact (p < .01). Comparing the results of the two sessions Comparison of the results of the two ses- revealed a statistically significant difference sions of measurements revealed a statistical- between the average preference RRR when ly significant difference between the average retest with the delay of one day (U = 25,0, preference RRR when immediate retest (U = p < .10). In the second session, the average 18,0, p < .05). In the second session, the av- preference to the Russians increased from 7 erage “pro-Russian” preference increased to 13 ms. from 7 to 12 (check the last number) ms. Groups with the positive impact. Statisti- Groups with the positive impact. Statisti- cally significant differences of average RRR- cally significant differences between the 600 ms between the groups with different groups with different delays of the retest delay of retest were not revealed, but this were not found. difference (p < .05) was found between the group with the positive impact and retest in one week and the control group with retest RRR-500 delay of one day: preference to the Russians Statistically significant differences between any groups with different delays of the retest were not found. is higher in the experimental group. Discussion and conclusions on the fourth research question Single index of implicit preference RRR RRR-600 changes at different SOA and times of test delay. The aim of this analysis was to deter- Groups with the negative impact. Statistical- mine the changes in implicit measures of ly significant differences between the groups attitudes depending on the time interval of with different delays of the retest were not the experimental exposure event (viewing found. However, in the group with the im- video by participants of each experimental midiate retest the preference to the Russians group) before the repeated implicit measure- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 89 ment with the delays of at once, one day, one In comparison with the control group, at week. Content of videos varied, activating SOA 100 ms in the experimental group the the old and creating new associations with higher preference to the Russians was found. the object of attitude. The first of them was The processes that occur at this value of the “pro-Russian” (negative), accentuating SOA have been previously assigned to the problems of the Russians in the system of first “wave” of activation of “pro-Russian” inter-ethnic relations, the second of them was associations. At other retest delays the trend neutral, “pro- to reduce the RRR (preference of the Rus- Latvian” (positive). For participants with the sians) was observed, although it was not preference of the Russians the content of the statistically significant. Given the fact that at “pro-Russian” video theoretically had not to other times of delay no statistically signifi- create a large number of new associations cant differences RRR between the partici- and the effect of their activation should fade pants of the experimental and control groups more quickly than for participants viewing were found, it can be assumed that the high- the “pro-Latvian” video. The events of this er values of RRR in the experimental group video could create new associations among without delay of the retest (immediate test) the latter. Presumably, these associations is associated with the activation of the “pro- should be maintained for a longer time under Russian” associations under the influence of the condition of coincidence (similarity) of the negative video. Besides, after viewing contextual cues of information coding with the negative video by participants the prefer- the access cues to these associations when ence RRR-100 at a statistically significant accessing memory. Thus, it was assumed that level negatively correlated with explicit the effect of the experimental exposure measures of the “pro-Russian” basic ethnic would be manifested in the shifts of deferred attitude. Simultaneously, the statistically implicit measures primarily for participants significant relationship with the measure D who viewed the positive video. However, the IAT of the “pro-Russian” implicit attitude, video with negative content can also create observed in the first measurement session new associations among participants with the was lost. It may also indicate the activation preference to the Russians, first of all, among of new associations with the object of atti- those who were not familiar with the facts tude, formed under the influence of the con- presented in the video. Some of them could textual event, which are contrary to the ex- have more positive attitude towards the rep- isting basic ethnic attitude or reflect its new resentatives of the alternative ethnic group. aspects. However, the activation of these Therefore, it was also important to trace the associations proved to be short-lived and shifts of implicit measures at various delays was not observed at other times of retest of retest among the participants who viewed delay. and the third the video with negative content. – With SOA 200 ms statistically signifi- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 90 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT cant differences RRR between the groups of basic attitude must be kept longer if the situ- participants who watched the video with ation of the memory test contains the same negative content without delay of retest and context cues as when encoding information with the delay of one day, were not found. (Gawronski & Sritharan, 2010). But such differences are observed between Group participants who have viewed the these groups and the group with the retest positive video, when retested in one day delay of one week. A marked decrease of showed a statistically insignificant trend RRR was noted in the latter group, and this towards an increase in the “pro-Russian” decrease is also not accidental when com- preferences, but in a week it decreased pared with all control groups. This result again, although not statistically significant. supports the hypothesis that with this value With SOA 300 ms the “pro-Russian” of SOA the new “pro-Latvian” associations preference of participants of the three experi- can be activated, which are stored for a long- mental groups, who have viewed the video er time, whereas the activated “pro-Russian” with negative content, was not significantly associations which were acquired earlier, different. A similar pattern was observed in quickly lose their power. all groups of participants who have viewed Comparing measurements of two consec- the video with positive content. Noteworthy utive sessions is also consistent with this is the fact that in all these groups the prefer- result. With the week delay of the retest a ence of participants was “pro-Russian”. statistically significant decrease in the pref- This can be attributed to the fact that erence to the Russians and, respectively, the participants‟ “pro-Russian” basic implicit “pro-Latvian” preference is observed. This preference that is determined by the process- fact can be attributed to the influence of new es of association activation with this SOA ambivalent associations, established by the has not been changed both with the negative negative video. Its content could cause the and positive contextual event related to the protest and formation of not “pro-Russian” object of attitude. This result does not con- association among those participants of the tradict the assumption that “pro-Russian” experimental group, who do not want to activation of associations which are basic for have confrontational relations with the alter- the attitude was context-independent. native ethnic group and focused on coopera- With SOA 400 ms the pattern has tion and integration with it, without losing changed somewhat. The “pro-Russian” im- their own ethnic identity. The newly created plicit preference was statistically significant- association in this case turned out to be the ly higher in the group watched the negative most accessible at SOA 200 ms and re- video and performed retest in one day com- mained for a long time. pared to the similar group who completed This confirms the assumption that the retest without delay. In the group with de- newly created associations contrary to the layed retest of one day the “pro-Russian” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 91 preference was also higher compare to the of the experimental exposure at 400 ms SOA control groups with retest without delay and has a transitional nature and is stored in the with the delay of one day. delay interval for at least a day, but less than There were also no shifts in preference a week. RRR in all groups of participants who viewed With SOA 500 ms experimental expo- the video with positive content. However, on sure was not associated with any statistically average, in contrast to the situation with SOA significant changes in ethnic preferences in 300 ms, their “pro-Russian” preference de- all experimental groups. Perhaps this is due creased, i.e. the associations contrary to the to the complex role of processes which with basic attitude were activated. SOA 500 ms are associated with activation These data are consistent with the previ- of “pro-Russian” and a tendency to inhibi- ously stated suggestion on the “pro-Latvian” tion of “pro-Latvian” associations (see Fig- “wave” of activation spread of associations ure 36). The results of correlation analysis related to attitude. However, with SOA 400 show that with the value of SOA in the first ms the authors have marked an increase in measurement the value of RRR-500 signifi- the “pro-Russian” preference among the par- cantly negatively correlates with measures of ticipants who watched the negative video and explicit “pro-Latvian” attitudes. Therefore, passed the retest in one day after context ex- we can assume that with SOA 500 ms there posure. Thus, the processes that define pref- is an activation of associations, closely relat- erence with SOA 400 ms are associated with ed to the object of basic attitude. However, “pro-Latvian” preference, but are not limited after experimental contextual exposure, di- to it. The new “pro-Russian” associations rect statistically significant negative associa- activated through viewing the negative video tions RRR-500 with implicit measures of by participants, retain a certain power in a “pro-Latvian” attitude are no longer ob- day after exposure as well. served. Indirectly, on the trend level, they These data are supported by the results of have arisen in connection with the processes comparison of preference measurements in of activation of “pro-Latvian” associations the first and second session. In the second with SOA 200 ms. session the “pro-Russian” preference statisti- With SOA 600 ms the level of “pro- cally significantly increased at the retest Russian” preferences in the groups of partic- without delay and is close to statistically sig- ipants who watched the negative video, are nificant with the delay of one day. A week relatively low and with one week delay they later, the effect of the superiority of the se- become even lower, although in all cases cond session of measurements in these they are statistically insignificant. In the groups almost disappeared. Thus, the activa- group that passed the retest in one day, there tion of the new “pro-Russian” associations was a statistically significant difference of with the object of attitude, created as a result the average “pro-Russian” preference from I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 92 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT the control groups (retest in one day and one ble explanation. The context cues of access week). In the three groups that have viewed to newly formed associations after a week the positive video, the preference was “pro- period of retest delay were not sufficiently Russian” and it statistically significantly ex- complete to access the new information. The ceeded the preference observed in the corre- conditions of retest with one day and one sponding control groups. This shows that the week delays coincide, i.e. contained identical processes of activation of relevant associa- access cues: the exterior of the experimenter, tions at a given value of SOA have been as- equipment and facilities for testing and view- sociated with the activation of the basic “pro- ing the video. With the retest delay of one Russian” attitude. day the memory of the participants could In general, these results support the as- well preserve the content of the video and sumption of attribution of activation process- this information could be activated during es with SOA 500 ms to the first “wave” of the retest period under the influence of the spread of (“pro-Russian”) associations. overall context for the situations of retest and Comparison of preferences in the first showing the video to participants. However, and second sessions also shows a trend to an within a week the content of video, which increase in “pro-Russian” preferences in all created new associations with the object of experimental groups in which the partici- attitude, could be subject to greater forget- pants watched the negative video. This pref- ting than in one day. Therefore, contextual erence is statistically significant only for the cues of access to the stored new associations group with the retest delay of one day. This might be less complete and the new associa- phenomenon can be attributed to the process tions could not be activated to the same de- of forming new associations in the group of gree as with the retest delay of one day. participants who watched the negative video Summary. In general, the data show a and these associations have strengthened the complex and ambiguous pattern of changes existing “pro-Russian” preference which is in explicit measures with different SOAs, most pronounced in one day. Since the dif- part of which can be attributed to experi- ference in the mean between the first and mental exposure. second measurement decreased in the group The processes activated with SOA 100, with retest in one week, it can be assumed 300, 500 and 600 ms, may be associated with that the newly emerged “pro-Russian” asso- the wave of activation of associations with ciations briefly increased the “pro-Russian” the object of the basic ethnic attitude. These preference. But their level of activation to the early associations with the object of attitude end of the week decreased and approached can be activated with the measurement of the values obtained in the first measurement. both implicit and explicit attitudes and are This indicates a transient nature of these as- presumably context independent. The pro- sociations. However, there is another possi- cesses observed in the measurement of atti- T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 93 tudes with SOA 200 ms may also be associ- SOAs need to be further explored. This re- ated with activation of associations with the search opens up a number of prospects for object of basic attitude, but towards the alter- the upcoming study of implicit measures and native ethnic group. Relevant contextual attitudes. In particular, it is necessary to dif- exposure causes activation of processes that ferentiate the processes that define both the affect access to associations, especially with basic and alternative preference. These pro- SOA 400 ms. The obtained data as a whole cesses affect the implicit measures of atti- give rise to the assumption that the processes tudes, obtained by means of procedures of activated with SOA 200 ms and 400 ms, re- evaluative priming. fer to the activation of associations newly It is necessary to research the conformity created under the influence of contextual of alternative implicit measures of attitudes factors. They affect both “pro-Russian” and with those measures which can be obtained “pro-Latvian” attitude. Implicit measures with different values of SOA in the process obtained with these SOAs, are context- of subliminal affective priming in the condi- dependent. tions of relevant contextual influences. Implicit measures with SOA 100 ms and This work will help in prospect to better 600 ms related to activation of “pro-Russian” understand the mechanisms of formation of associations are likely to be context- contextual associations relevant to attitude dependent too. Though, these measures are and to assess the sensitivity of various im- associated with the “pro-Russian” attitude, plicit measures of attitudes towards contex- reflecting the contextual events and new in- tual influences. formation contained in them which confirms the basic attitude. Overall, the data support the assumption that the newly created contextual associa- The work makes it possible to propose a number of ideas for further study of the problem of mental representation of attitudes and contexted cognition. tions contrary to the basic attitude, persist longer. However, this result is more typical with retest delay of one week from the event of the relevant contextual impact with SOA 200 ms, while the new associations correspond- Discussion and conclusions on Study 2 ing to the basic attitude exert their effect with SOA 400 ms and relatively quickly lose their The experimental exposure reflected on the strength. process of association activation that occurs Nevertheless, the mechanisms of pro- with SOA 600 ms and depending on the as- cessing the newly created contextual associa- sessment of the strength of emotional im- tions with the object of attitude with various pact. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 94 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT The large subjective strength of the emo- ethnic group. The relevant contextual expo- tional impact of the contextual event de- sure causes activation of the processes that creased “pro-Russian” preference with SOA affect the access to associations, especially 600 and SOA 400, but was associated with with SOA 400 ms. The obtained data gener- increased “pro-Russian” preference with ally give rise to the assumption that the pro- SOA 300 ms. This indicates context- cesses activated with SOA 200 ms and 400 dependent processes in this range of SOAs. ms, refer to the activation of the associations Pertaining of processes with SOA 200 ms newly created under the influence of the con- and 400 ms to the second wave of spread of textual factor. They affect both the “pro- activation has got additional confirmation. Russian” and “pro-Latvian” attitude. Implicit RRR-100 may reflect both the associations with “pro-Russian” and “pro-Latvian” preference, which is due to valence of existing automatic implicit associations. measures obtained with these SOAs, are subject to contextual influences. The implicit measures with SOA 100 ms and 600 ms associated with the activation of Attention is drawn to the relationship “pro-Russian” associations are likely to be between RRR-100 (the first measurement) the context-sensitive as well. However, these and RRR- 400 and RRR-600 (the second measures are associated with the “pro- measurement). RRR-100 and RRR-600 were Russian” attitude, reflecting the effects of sensitive to contextual impacts (see the re- contextual events and new information con- sults of the control group). tained in them, which confirms the basic In general, the data show the complex attitude. and mixed pattern of changes of implicit Overall, the data support the hypothesis measures with various SOAs, part of which that the newly created contextual associa- can be attributed to the influence of the ex- tions, contrary to the basic attitude persist perimental exposure. longer than its corresponding attitudes. The processes activated with SOA 100, This result was more pronounced at the 300, 500 and 600 ms are possibly related to retest delay of one week from the event of the “wave” of activation of associations with the relevant contextual impact with SOA 200 the object of the basic ethnic attitude. These ms, while the new associations correspond- early associations with the object of attitude ing to the basic attitude exert their effect with can be activated in the measurement of both SOA 400 ms and relatively quickly lose implicit and explicit attitudes and are pre- strength. sumably context independent. The processes However, the mechanisms of processing observed in the measurement of attitudes of newly created contextual associations with with SOA 200 ms may also be associated the object of attitude with various SOAs with the activation of associations with the need to be further explored. This study opens object of basic attitude, but to the alternative up a number of prospects for the upcoming T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 95 study of implicit measures and attitudes. In despite the fact that the subject is not aware particular, it is necessary to differentiate the of this. Whereas the actualization of implicit processes that define both the basic and alter- attitudes is automatic and unconscious in native preference. These processes affect the nature, it is influenced by contextual factors, implicit measures of attitudes, obtained by which is contrary to the existing notion of means of procedures of evaluative priming. sustainability and stability of attitudes. The It is necessary to study the conformity of recent experience, contrary to previously alternative implicit measures of attitudes with formed attitude can have an impact on im- those measures, which can be obtained at plicit measures of attitudes obtained using different values of SOA in the process of procedures such as the IAT, subliminal af- subliminal affective priming in the conditions fective priming and others. In this study we of relevant contextual influences. examined the effects of the recent autobio- This work will help in the perspective to graphical events, as well as artificially gen- better understand the mechanisms of for- erated events on implicit attitudes and their mation of contextual associations relevant to measures. The results were discussed in the attitude and to evaluate the sensitivity of the framework of modern theories of implicit various implicit measures of attitudes to- attitudes. For this purpose, the authors have wards contextual influences. systematized the existing approaches to the This research makes it possible to put a development of modern theories of attitudes. number of ideas for further study of the prob- The main attention was focused both on the lem of mental representation of attitudes and study of the duration of the effects of con- contexted cognition. text, and on the study of correspondence of existing associations with the object of attitude to the newly formed associations that General conclusions have arisen under the influence of contextual events. Following Gawronsky and Striharam (2009), we hypothesized that congruent as- In the tideway of modern concepts the sociation remain unchanged, but their activa- attitude is understood as generalized evalua- tion attenuates faster than the activation of tion data on the object. The process of atti- incongruent associations, making the latter tude actualization may require conations and more sustainable over time, that is, better being conscious, controlled or, on the contra- stored in the memory. We have tested this ry, spontaneous, unconscious, automatic. In idea with the help of procedures of sublimi- the first case the matter involves explicit atti- nal priming and the IAT. Besides that we tudes, in the second – implicit ones. Thus, evaluated the overall context influence on implicit attitudes are evaluating tendencies implicit attitudes and their measures. that may affect the judgment and behavior, Analysis of the effects of affective prim- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 96 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT ing was carried out in terms of an approach these associations as underlying the basic based on the concept of the spread of activa- attitude and manifested both in implicit and tion in the semantic network (Collins & explicit measurements. Another important Loftus, 1975; Collins & Quillian, 1969). finding is separate cognitive processing of The first study was carried out in an at- contextually conditioned and basic associa- tempt to explore the contextual influences tions with the object of attitude at different that affected the research participants in the times of SOA. The research found that the environmental conditions of real life. In the processes that determine the index of implicit second study the contextual influence was preferences with SOA 300, 500 and probably created artificially. In both cases, the authors 100 ms and 600 ms are associated with the found the reliable influence of contextual explicit and implicit ethnic attitude of factors on implicit measures of ethnic atti- “hyperidentity” and the processes occurring tudes. As the first and the second study re- with SOA 200 ms, 400 ms are related to the vealed similar patterns: changes in reaction associations, underlying the alternative ethnic time under the influence of contextual factors attitude (“hypoidentity”). in the performance of experimental tasks by Artificially created contextual events had the participants. These changes were associ- an impact on the system of relationships be- ated with the influence of factors such as tween variables - indicators of basic and al- affective valence of the contextual event, the ternative ethnic attitudes. Contextual effects strength of the emotional impact of the event. creating new associations with the object of In the second study these changes were asso- attitude, prone to more rapid attenuation in ciated with a time interval between the con- the case of their compliance with the basic textual event and repeated measurement. attitude and slower attenuation in the case of Contextual events had a significant impact on correspondence to the alternative ethnic atti- all used implicit measures of attitudes tude. This generally corresponds to the as- (facilitation variable R, variable of attitudes sumptions of Gawronsky and Striharam RR, variable of indices of implicit preference (2009). The revealed patterns require further RRR, D-scores). For the first time it was study, but there is already visible perspective found that the index of implicit preferences, of working on the creation of a new model of as opposed to other measures, is closely re- mental representation of attitude, as well as lated to the explicit measures of attitudes. prospects for the development of new ap- This allowed us to identify the type of associ- proaches to measuring implicit attitudes. ations with the object of attitude and qualify T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 97 Acknowledgments The authors express their gratitude for the collection of empirical data to Marija Bambulaka, Natalia Tikhomirova, Larisa Pushkareva, Ieva Melnupe and Ilva Terauda. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 98 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT References Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcomb, T. M., Rosenberg, M. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1968). Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally. Abelson, R. P. (1995) Attitude extremity. In R. E. Petty, & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 25-41). Manhwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (Eds.). (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Allport, G. W. (1935). C. Murchison, (Ed.), A handbook of social psychology (pp. 798-884). Worcester: Clark University Press. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Devine, P. G. (2003). Individual differences in the activation and control of affective race bias as assessed by startle eyeblink response and selfreport. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 738–753, DOI: 10.1037/00223514.84.4.738 Anderson, J.R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295. Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing implicit self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15, 498–502. Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Domains of recollection. Psychological Review, 89, 708-729. Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). A comparative investigation of seven implicit measures of social cognition. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 668-688. Balota, D.A., & Chumbley, J.I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 340-357. Bambulaka, M. (2014a). The impact of emotional valence of episodic events on the implicit meas- urements of the ethnic attitudes. (Summary of the thesis for obtaining the doctoral degree in Psychology). Daugavpils: Daugavpils University. Retrieved from http://du.lv/wp-content/uploads/2015/12/ Bambulaka_kopsavilkums_saved-1.pdf Bambulaka, M. (2014b). The impact of emotional valence of episodic events on the implicit measurements of the ethnic attitudes. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Daugavpils University, Daugavpils, Latvia. Barden, J., Maddux, W. W., Petty, R. E., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). Contextual moderation of racial bias: The impact of social roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 5–22. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893-912. Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bargh, J. A. (1996).Automaticity in social psychology. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 169-183). New York: Guilford. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1-61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32 (7), 169-177 Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) and the relational elaboration and coherence (REC) model. Psychological Record, 60, 527-542. Banaji, M. R., & Heiphetz, L. (2010). Attitudes. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 353-393). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Belinchón, M., Riviére, A., & Igoa, J. M. (1992). Psicología del lenguaje. Investigación y teoría. Madrid: trotta. Blair, I. V., Ma, J., & Lenton, A. (2001). Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828–841. Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242–261. Blair, I., V., Dasgupta, N., & Glaser, J., (2015). Implicit attitudes. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology: Volume 1: Attitudes and social cognition (pp. 665-691). Washington DC: APA. Bock, J. K., & Warren, R.K. (1985). Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition, 21, 47-67. Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distances. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299–308. Bohner, G., Moskowitz, G., & Chaiken, S. (1995). The interplay of heuristic and systematic processing of social information. European Review of Social Psychology, 6, 33-68. Bohner, G. (2004) Attitudes. In M. Hewstone, & V. Strebe (Eds.), Introduction to social psychology, a European perspective. (G. Y. Lubimova, Trans.). Moscow: Unity. (Original work published 2001). Brinol, P., Horcajo, J., Becerra, A., Falces, C., & Sierra, B., (2002). Cambio de actitudes implícitas. Psicothema, 14, 771-775. Brinol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2012). The history of attitudes and persuasion research. In A. Kruglanski & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of the history of social psychology (pp. 285-320). New York: Psychology Press. Buehl, A., & Zoefel, P. (2002). SPSS 11: Introduction in Modern Data Analysis. 8th ed. Munich, Germany: Addison-Wesley. Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401-423. Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752- 99 766. Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford Press. Chaiken, S., Pomerantz, E. M., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (1995). Structural consistency and attitude strength. In R.E. Petty and J.A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: antecedents and consequences (387-412.) NJ: Erlbaum. Chaiken, S., Wood, W. L., & Eagly, A. H. (1996). Principles of persuasion. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 702-742). NY: Guilford press. Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215-224. Coane, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Priming the holiday spirit: persistent activation due to extra experimental experiences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1124-1128. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–248. Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2007). The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1102–1117. Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–812. Czellar, S. (2006). Self-presentational effects in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Con- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 100 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT sumer Psychology, 16, 92–100. Dal Cin, S., Gibson, B., Zanna, M. P., Shumate, R., & Fong, G. (2007). Smoking in movies, implicit associations with the self, and intentions to smoke. Psychological Science, 18, 559 –563. Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800-814. Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on automatic gender stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 642–658. Degner, J. (2009). On the (un)controllability of affective priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can possibly be prevented. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 327–354. Dehaene, S. (2007). A few steps toward a science of mental life. Mind, Brain and Education, 1, 28-47. DeHart, T., Pelham, B. W., & Tennen, H. (2006). What lies beneath: Parenting style and implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 1–17 De Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 77– 85. De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Moors, A. (2007). Novel attitudes can be faked on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 972–978. De Houwer, J., Teige- Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347-368. De Liver, Y., van der Pligt, J., & Wigboldus, D. (2007). Positive and negative associations underlying ambivalent attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 319–326. Devine, P. G. (1989). Prejudice and stereotypes: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18. Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New technologies for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes. In J. M. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of surveys (pp. 204-237). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich. Eich, J. E. (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(4), 764-770. Fazio, R. H., Chen, J., McDonel, E. C., & Sherman, S. J. (1982). Attitude accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency and the strength of the object-evaluation association. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 339-357. Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Herr, P. M. (1983). Toward a process model of the attitudebehavior relation: Accessing one's attitude upon mere observation of the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 723-735. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE Model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 23 (pp.75–107). New York: Academic Press. Fazio, R.H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. In R.E. Petty & J.A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 247288), Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013– 1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.10. Fazio, R. H., & Towles- Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude- behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual- process theories in social psychology (pp. 97–116). New York: Guilford Press. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297-327. Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25, 603-637. Ferguson, M. J. (2007). The automaticity of evaluation. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 219-264). Psychology Press. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press. Fiedler K., Bless, H. (2004). Attitudes. In M. Houston & V. Shtrebe (Eds.), Introduction to social psychology, a European perspective. (G. Y. Lubimova, Trans.), (pp.231-275) Moscow: Unity. (Original work published 2001). Fiedler, K., & Blumke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the Implicit Association Tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 307–316. Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Foroni, F., & Mayr, U. (2005). The power of a story: New, automatic associations from a single reading of a short scenario. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 139–144. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 61– 89). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2004). On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 535–542. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006a). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006b). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: Conceptual, empirical, and metatheoretical issues. Reply to Albarracin, Hart, and McCulloch (2006), Kruglanski and Dechesne (2006), and Petty and Briсol (2006). Psychological Bulletin, 132, 745–750. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2007). Unraveling the processes underlying evaluation: Attitudes from the perspective of the APE 101 model. Social Cognition, 25, 687–717. Gawronski, B., & LeBel, E. P. (2008). Understanding patterns of attitude change: When implicit measures show change, but explicit measures do not. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1355–1361, doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.005. Gawronski, B., Deutsch, R., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2008). Response interference as a mechanism underlying implicit measures: Some traps and gaps in the assessment of mental associations with experimental paradigms. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 218–225. Gawronski, B., Strack, F., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2009). Attitudes and cognitive consistency: The role of associative and propositional processes. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 85–117). New York: Psychology Press. Gawronski, B., & Sritharan, R. (2010). Formation, change, and contextualization of mental associations: Determinants and principles of variations in implicit measures. In B. Gawronski, & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 216-240). New York, NY: Guilford Press. George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide and reference. 11.0 update (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Gorfein, D. S. (1987). Explaining context effects on short-term memory. In D. S. Gorfein & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Learning and memory: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (pp.153172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gollwitzer, M., Banse, R., Eisenbach, K., & Naumann, A. (2007). Effectiveness of the Vienna social competence training on explicit and implicit aggression: Evidence from an aggressiveness-IAT. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 150–156. Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to persuation, and attitude change. In A.G. Greenwald, T.C. Brock, T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of attitudes. New-York, Academic Press. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 102 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4– 27. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3-25. Guttman, L. (1944). A basis for scaling qualitative data. American Sociological Review, 9, 139– 150. Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (Eds.) (1999). Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC: APA. Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. The Journal of Psychology, 21, 107112. Hewstone M., Stroebe W. (2004). Introduction to social psychology, a European perspective. (G. Y. Lubimova, Trans.). Moscow: Unity. (Original work published 2001) Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. J. (1985). Nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 59–69. Hines, T. (1990). An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits. Memory and Cognition, 18, 40–46. Hovland, C. I., & Rosenberg, M. J. (Eds.). (1960). Attitude organization and change: An analysis of consistency among attitude components. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Howell, D.C. (1999). Fundamental statistics for the behavior sciences. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press. Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32. Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotypic activa- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888. Klauer, K. C., & Teige-Mocigemba, S. (2007). Controllability and resource dependence in automatic evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 648–655. Klauer, K.C., Voss, A., & Stahl, C. (2011). Cognitive methods in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: An overview. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 1-24). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Kuhnen, U., Schiessl, M., Bauer, N., Paulig, N., Pöhlmann, C., & Schmidthals, K. (2001). How robust is the IAT? Measuring and manipulating implicit attitudes of East- and West-Germans. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 135-144. LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, 230–237. Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of Psychology, 22, 1 –55. Livingston, R. W. (2002). The role of perceived negativity in the moderation of African Americans‟ implicit and explicit racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 405–413. Lincoln, T. M., Arens, E., Berger, C., & Rief, W. (2008). Can antistigma campaigns be improved?: A test of the impact of biogenetic vs. psychosocial causal explanations on implicit and explicit attitudes toward schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34, 984–994. Losel, F. (1995). Increasing consensus in the evaluation of offender rehabilitation? Lessons from recent research syntheses. Psychology, Crime & Law, 2, (19-39). Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842-855. Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta- analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 618–630. Maio, G. R. and Haddock, G. (2007). Attitude change. In: Kruglanski, A. & Higgins, E. (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Principles (2nd ed. pp. 565-586). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Maio, G. R., & Haddock, G. (2015). The psychology of attitudes and attitude change. London: Sage Publications. Marini, M., Rubichi, S., Sartori, G. (2011). Implicit racial stereotypes may temporarily shift after reading a story. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conferences of Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1224-1229). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Marini, M., Rubichi, S., & Sartori, G. (2012). The role of self-involvement in shifting IAT effects. Experimental Psychology, 59 (6), 348354. Marsh, R. L., Meeks, J. T., Hicks, J. L., Cook, G. I., & Clark-Foos, A. (2006). Concreteness and item-to-list context associations in the free recall of items differing in context variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1424. McConnell, A. R., Rydell, R. J., Strain, L. M., & Mackie, D. M. (2008). Forming implicit and explicit attitudes toward individuals: Social group association cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 792–807. McGuire, W. J. (1981). The probabilogical model of cognitive structure and attitude change. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp.291-307). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J. & Lovibond P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 32 (2), 183-198. Nasledov, А. D. (2004). Matematicheskie metodi psihologicheskogo issledovanija. Analiz i interpretacija dannih. [Mathematical methods of psychological research. Analysis and interpretation of data]. SPb: Rech. Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Memory and knowledge. In D. A. Norman, D. E. Rumelhart, & the LNR Research Group (Eds.), Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: Freeman. Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The Go/No -Go Association Task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–666. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing 103 automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 421–433. Olson, M. A., & Kendrick, R. V. (2008). Origins of attitudes. In W. Crano & R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and Persuasion. New York: Psychology Press. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes. Insights from the new implicit measures (pp.19–63). New York: Psychology Press. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R., Robertson, E., Cooper, J., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1322-1331. Penke, L., Eichstaedt, J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2006). Single Attribute Implicit Association Tests (SAIAT) for the assessment of unipolar constructs: The case of sociosexuality. Experimental Psychology, 53, 283–291. Perugini, M., Richetin, J., & Zogmaister, C. (2010). Prediction of behavior. In Gawronski B. & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition (pp. 255-278). NY, London: The Gulford press. Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. Payne, B. K., & Gawronski, B. (2010). A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going? In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Issue involvement as a moderator of the effects on attitude of advertising content and context. In I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 104 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT K. B. Monroe & A. Abor (Eds.), Advances in consumer research, 8(1), 20-24. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York: Academic Press. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus mismatching attitude functions: Implications for scrutiny of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 227- 240. Petty, R. E., & Brinol, P. (2006). A metacognitive approach to “implicit” and “explicit” evaluations: Comment on Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2006). Psychological Bulletin, 132, 740–744. Petty, R. E., Brinol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The Meta-Cognitive Model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition, 25, 609-642. Petty, R. E., Brinol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The meta-cognitive model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition, 25, 657–686. Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Brinol, P. (2009). Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures. New York: Psychology Press. Petty, R. E., & Brinol, P. (2010). Attitude structure and change. Implications for implicit measures. In B. Gawronski B., & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition (pp. 335-352). The Gulford press: NY, London. Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sleeth-Kepple, D. (2004). Relevance override: on the reduced impact of “Cues” under highmotivation conditions of persuasion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 251–264. Plant, E. A., Peruche, B. M., & Butz, D. A. (2005). Reducing implicit racial stereotyping: Making race irrelevant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 141–156. Plotka, I., Igonin, D., & Blumenau, N. (2009). The research of the ethnic attitudes by the method of emotional priming. Social Sciences Bulletin, 1(8), 59-74. Plotka, I., Igonin, D., Blumenau, N., Bambulaka, M., Ozola, E, & Simane, L. (2011). Ethnic attitudes measurement with unconscious emotional priming on different SOA. In Proceedings of the International Applied Military Psychology Symposium: IAMPS 2011: Coping with Constrains. http://www.iamps.org/ IAMPS_2011_Paper_Plotka_et_al.pdf Plotka, I., Igonin, D., Blumenau, N., & Simane, L. (2013). The role of relevant recent autobiographical event's emotional valence on implicit measures of ethnic attitudes. Journal of Education, Psychology and Social Sciences, 1(1), 1923. Plotka, I., Igonin, D., Blumenau, N., Simane, L., Lutikova. N., & Morozova, M. (2013). Investigation of attitudes toward gambling with implicit association test and self-reported measures. International Journal of Development and Educational Psychology, 1(2), 597610. Plotka, I., Blumenau, N., Igonin, D., Simane, L., & Bambulak, M. (2013). The impact of relevant recent autobiographical events on implicit correlates of ethnic attitudes. Online abstract of 13th European Congress of Psychology. Stockholm, Sweden. http://www.ecp2013.se/ ECP_2013.pdf Plotka, I., & Blumenau, N. (2015). Implicit methods for studying attitudes: Modern approach to research in social sciences. In Society, Integration, Education. Proceedings of the International Scientific Conference May 22nd-23rd, 2015 (Vol. 1, pp. 303-314). Rezekne: Rezeknes Augstskola. http://journals.ru.lv/index.php/SIE/ issue/view/23. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), 1nformation processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Powell, M. C., Fazio, R. H. (1984). Attitude accessibility as a function of repeated attitudinal expression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 139-148. Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 386–396. Roefs, A., Quaedacjers, L., Werrij, M. Q., T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Wolters, G., Havermans, R., Nederkoorn, C., et al. (2006). The environment influences whether high-fat foods are associated with palatable or with unhealthy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 715–736. Rudman, L. A., Ashmore, R. D., & Gary, M. L. (2001). “Unlearning” automatic biases: The malleability of implicit prejudice and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 856–868. doi:10.1037/00223514.81.5.856. Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 79–82. Rudman, L. A., Dohn, M. C., & Fairchild, K. (2007). Implicit self-esteem compensation: Automatic threat defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 798–813. Rudman, L. A. (2011). Implicit measures for social and personality psychology. London: Sage. Rudman, L. A. (2013). Sources of implicit attitudes. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 13, 79-82. Rydell, R. J., & Gawronski, B. (2009). I like you, I like you not: Understanding the formation of context dependent automatic attitudes. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1118–1152. Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M., & Strain, L. M. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 17, 954–958. Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 995– 1008. Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2010). Consistency and inconsistency in implicit social cognition: The case of implicit and explicit measures of attitudes. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition (pp. 295-310). New York: Guilford. Schlosberg, H. (1952). The description of facial expressions in terms of two dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 229-237. Schwarz, N. (1999). Self- reports: How the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist, 54, 93–105. 105 Schwarz N, & Bohner G. (2001). The construction of attitudes. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes. (pp. 436-457). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social Cognition, 25, 638– 656. Sloman, S. A., Hayman, C. A. G., Ohta, N., Law, J., & Tulving E. (1988). Forgetting in primed fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 223-239. Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22. Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131. Soldatova, G.U. (1998) Psihologija mezhetnicheskoj naprjazhennosti [The psychology of ethnic tensions]. Moskva: Smisl. Spruyt, A., Gast A., & Moors A. (2011). The sequential priming paradigm. In Klauer K.C, Voss A., & Stahl C. (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 48-77). New York, London: The Guilford Press. Sriram, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The brief Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psychology, 56, 283–294. Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51, 165-179. Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). “Automatic” evaluation?: Strategic effects on affective priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1414–1417. Teige-Mocigemba S, Klauer KC, Sherman JW. (2010) A practical guide to implicit association tests and related tasks. In B. Gawronski, & B. K. Payne (Eds.) Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 117-139). New York: Guilford Press. Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be meas- I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 106 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT ured. American Journal of Sociology, 33, 529– 554. doi:10.1086/214483 Thurstone, L. L., & Chave, E. J. (1929). The measurement of attitude. Chicago: University of Chicago press. Tikhomirova, N. (2013). Episodic activation and assessment of ethnic attitude using explicit and implicit measures. (Unpublished master‟s dissertation, supervisor Plotka I). Baltic International Academy, Riga, Latvia. Tormala, Z. L., Brinol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Hidden effects of persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 31, 75-76. Tulving E. & Schacter D.L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247 (4940), 301-306. Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (1994). Effects of truncation of reaction time analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 34-80. Verschuere, B., Prati, V., & De Houwer, J. (2009). Cheating the lie detector: Faking in the autobiographical IAT. Psychological Science, 20, 410–413. Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58, 475482. Wickens, D. D. (1987). The dual meanings of context: Implications for research, theory, and applications. In D. S. Gorfein & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Learning and memory: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (pp.135-152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In A. Tesser & L. Martin (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp. 37-65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262–274. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 815– 827. Wittenbrink, B., & Schwarz, N. (2007). Implicit measures of attitudes. New York: Guilford Press. Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1955). Experimental psychology. London: Methuen. Zanna, M. P. & Rempel, J. K. (1988) Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar-Tal & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315–334). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 107 Appendix A Tables for results in Study 1 TABLE A1. Experimental conditions and priming-effect. Study 1 SOA Group 100 100 RUS 100 100 RUS RUS Prime R Russian R(+) R(-) Russian Russian R(-) R(-) 200 200 200 LV 200 Valence of Episodes Impact of Episodes M SD One Sample t-test 8.8 -9.6 47.2 49.1 t(119)=2.03, p<.05 t(116)=-2.12, p<.05 -11.1 -40 47.1 65.2 t(60)=-1.85, p<.1 t(10)=-2.04, p<.1 R R(+) 3.4 4.5 43.8 42 t(471)=1.68, p<.1 t(233)=1.65, p<.1 negative low R(-) 9.3 43.4 t(113)=2.28, p<.05 Russian R(+) 7 37.1 t(116)=2.06, p<.05 200 LV Latvian R(-) 7.9 34.8 t(56)=1.72, p<.1 200 RUS Latvian R(-) -9.3 41.4 t(60)=-1.75, p<.1 200 RUS Russian R(+) 8.9 34.2 t(60)=2.04, p<.05 200 200 LV RUS Latvian Russian R(-) R(+) 12.5 14.5 36.3 29.9 t(25)=1.75, p<.1 t(23)=2.39, p<.05 R R(+) 5.3 6.1 43.1 40.4 t(471)=2.66, p<.01 t(233)=2.32, p<.05 300 300 negative negative medium high 300 LV R(-) 11.9 46.7 t(113)=2.73, p<.01 300 LV R(+) 7 41.3 t(113)=1.82, p<.1 Latvian R(+) 6.7 42.3 t(116)=1.72, p<.1 13.6 39.1 t(56)=2.64, p<.05 300 300 LV Latvian R(-) 300 LV Latvian R(-) negative low 20.4 33.9 t(10)=1.99, p<.1 300 300 LV LV Latvian Russian R(-) R(+) negative negative medium high 20 15.2 43.9 25.2 t(25)=2.32, p<.05 t(9)=1.90, p<.1 400 400 LV R(-) R(-) 5.1 9.5 43 47.3 t(233)=1.82, p<.1 t(113)=2.15, p<.05 400 Latvian R(-) 9.3 39.2 t(116)=2.56, p<.05 400 LV Latvian R(-) 14.4 43.4 t(56)=2.51, p<.05 400 LV Latvian R(-) negative medium 19.8 49.9 t(25)=2.02, p<.1 400 400 LV RUS Latvian Latvian R(-) R(-) positive negative low low 27.3 13.3 34.1 23.2 t(6)=2.12, p<.1 t(10)=1.90, p<.1 500 LV Russian R(+) negative medium -18 44 t(25)=-2.08, p<.05 600 LV Latvian R(+) -9.8 36 t(56)=-2.06, p<.05 I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 108 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table A2. Correlations between results of implicit and explicit methods. Study 1 SOA, ms Valence Impact Valence*Impact r p N Variables RUS 300 -.205 .113 61 HYPO RRR RUS 500 -.295 <.05 61 HYPO RRR RUS 500 -.274 <.05 58 HYPO RRR RUS 300 low -.701 <.01 14 HYPO RRR RUS 500 medium -.379 <.1 23 HYPO RRR RUS 300 negative*low -.811** <.01 11 HYPO RRR LV 500 negative*medium .362 <.1 26 HYPO RRR LV 600 negative*medium -.326 .104 26 HYPER RRR LV 500 positive*low .718 <.1 7 HYPO RRR LV 100 .526 .118 10 HYPO RRR LV 500 .344 <.1 29 HYPO RRR negative positive medium T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 109 Appendix B Tables for results in Study 1. IAT Table B1 Comparisons of control groups’ and experimental groups’ means in Sessions 1, 2 Experimental group Control group Valence (experimental group) Session 1 Retest 1 11 negative at once 795 485 1842 691 335 1394 p<.001 6 11 positive at once 795 485 1842 763 411 1197 p=.241 7 11 neutral at once 795 485 1842 696 325 2328 p<.001 735 411 1688 778 442 1535 p<.001 735 411 1688 718 421 1393 p=.053 735 411 1688 720 376 1827 p=.459 686 387 1232 809 363 1399 p<.001 686 387 1232 815 458 1219 p<.001 686 387 1232 807 541 1979 p<.001 M SD N M SD N p-value of means' differences Control group after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week Experimental group Session 2 3 12 negative 5 12 positive 2 12 neutral 4 11 negative 8 11 positive 10 11 neutral 1 11 negative at once 744 458 1827 638 278 1401 p<.001 6 11 positive at once 744 458 1827 667 320 1205 p<.001 7 11 neutral at once 744 458 1827 635 249 2332 p<.001 651 300 1710 648 290 1546 p=.976 651 300 1710 682 418 1390 p=.413 651 300 1710 647 308 1797 p=.509 613 240 1236 758 366 1383 p<.001 613 240 1236 665 295 1236 p<.001 613 240 1236 700 356 2013 p<.001 3 12 negative 5 12 positive 2 12 neutral 4 11 negative 8 11 positive 10 11 neutral after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week Note. The multiple comparisons LSD tests were used. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 110 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table B2. Comparisons of control groups’ and experimental groups’ means in Sessions 1, 2 Experimental group Control group M SD N M low negative 718 364 1852 745 438 4762 low neutral 666 294 1710 745 438 4762 low positive 756 389 1361 745 438 4762 middle negative 747 384 1244 745 438 4762 middle neutral 705 349 2008 745 438 4762 middle positive 722 378 932 745 438 4762 high negative 837 415 1232 745 438 4762 high neutral 818 529 2416 745 438 4762 high positive 794 493 1516 745 438 4762 low negative 663 319 1862 677 361 4773 low neutral 628 251 1708 677 361 4773 low positive 659 346 1350 677 361 4773 middle negative 657 313 1236 677 361 4773 middle neutral 629 240 2005 677 361 4773 middle positive 647 329 936 677 361 4773 high negative 729 314 1232 677 361 4773 high neutral 707 376 2429 677 361 4773 high positive 697 369 1545 677 361 4773 no impact Session 2 Valence no impact Session 1 IEI SD t-test N t(4028)=-2.64, p<.01 t(4502)=-8.37, p<.001 t(2433)=0.85, ns t(2167)=0.08, ns t(4687)=-3.98, p<.001 t(1465)=-1.69, ns t(2001)=6.8064, p<.001 t(4130)=5.81, p<.001 t(2325)=3.45, p<.001 t(3821)=-1.52, ns t(4331)=-6.02, p<.001 t(6121)=-1.62, ns t(2170)=-1.93, ns t(5529)=-6.38, p<.001 t(1415)=-2.50, p<.05 t(2149)=5.08, p<.001 t(4715)=3.28, p<.01 t(6315)=1.94, ns Note. The t-tests were used. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 111 Table B3. Comparisons of control groups’ and experimental groups’ means in Sessions 1, 2 Retest Intensity of emotional impact Experimental group Control group M SD N M SD N p-value of means' differences Session 1 Session 2 at once low no impact 689 317 2172 795 485 1842 p<.001 at once middle no impact 698 352 2016 795 485 1842 p<.001 at once high no impact 732 441 927 795 485 1842 p<.001 after one day low no impact 753 395 1673 735 411 1688 p=.012 after one day middle no impact 658 321 774 735 411 1688 p<.001 after one day high no impact 783 443 2112 735 411 1688 p<.001 after one week low no impact 686 335 1078 686 387 1232 p=.435 after one week middle no impact 790 400 1394 686 387 1232 p<.001 after one week high no impact 885 551 2125 686 387 1232 p<.001 at once low no impact 646 292 2171 744 458 1827 p<.001 at once middle no impact 638 287 2024 744 458 1827 p<.001 at once high no impact 676 398 928 744 458 1827 p<.001 after one day low no impact 653 323 1671 651 300 1710 p=.848 after one day middle no impact 562 194 760 651 300 1710 p<.001 after one day high no impact 685 317 2117 651 300 1710 p<.001 after one week low no impact 652 305 1078 613 240 1236 p<.001 after one week middle no impact 688 311 1393 613 240 1236 p<.001 after one week high no impact 748 379 2161 613 240 1236 p<.001 Note. The multiple comparisons LSD tests were used. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 112 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table B4. Comparisons of control groups’ and experimental groups’ means in Session 1 Valence Retest negative negative negative at once at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week negative negative negative negative negative negative Intensity of emotional impact low middle high M SD N M SD N t df p 689 678 759 316 309 477 461 777 156 795 795 795 485 485 485 1842 1842 1842 -5.72 -7.36 -0.90 1072 2221 183 0.001 0.001 ns low 756 426 924 735 411 1688 1.22 1842 ns middle 592 274 156 735 411 1688 -5.96 225 0.001 high 886 491 455 735 411 1688 5.99 636 0.001 low 669 247 467 686 387 1232 -1.04 1308 ns middle 995 478 311 686 387 1232 10.57 418 0.001 high 820 323 621 686 387 1232 7.89 1461 0.001 Experimental group Control group t-test neutral at once low 684 300 1401 795 485 1842 -8.01 3120 0.001 neutral neutral at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week at once at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week middle high 730 685 378 313 617 310 795 795 485 485 1842 1842 -3.40 -5.20 1347 594 0.001 0.001 735 411 1688 neutral neutral neutral neutral neutral neutral positive positive positive positive positive positive positive positive positive low middle 650 304 463 735 411 1688 -4.92 975 0.001 high 743 395 1364 735 411 1688 0.54 2963 ns low 585 246 309 686 387 1232 -5.65 740 0.001 middle 717 347 928 686 387 1232 1.94 2094 ns high 1012 729 742 686 387 1232 11.27 997 0.001 low middle high 713 693 754 384 372 497 310 622 461 795 795 795 485 485 485 1842 1842 1842 -3.31 -5.45 -1.57 491 1385 696 0.001 0.001 ns low 749 353 749 735 411 1688 0.86 1656 ns middle 749 387 155 735 411 1688 0.43 187 ns high 806 541 293 735 411 1688 2.13 353 0.05 low 816 466 302 686 387 1232 4.50 409 0.001 middle 813 377 155 686 387 1232 3.94 197 0.001 high 814 470 762 686 387 1232 6.32 1384 0.001 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 113 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table B5. Comparisons of control groups’ and experimental groups’ means in Session 2 Valence negative negative negative negative negative negative negative negative negative Retest at once at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week Intensity of emotional impact low middle high Experimental group Control group t-test M SD N M SD N t df p 656 629 625 307 264 256 466 779 156 744 744 744 458 458 458 1827 1827 1827 -4.92 -8.02 -5.12 1058 2381 250 0.001 0.001 0.001 low 647 305 933 651 300 1710 -0.25 2641 ns middle 513 192 156 651 300 1710 -8.08 231 0.001 high 697 271 457 651 300 1710 3.15 780 0.01 low 700 353 463 613 240 1236 4.89 629 0.001 middle 802 410 301 613 240 1236 7.69 352 0.001 high 780 346 619 613 240 1236 10.73 925 0.001 neutral at once low 639 256 1400 744 458 1827 -8.28 2975 0.001 neutral neutral at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week at once at once at once after one day after one day after one day after one week after one week after one week middle high 642 605 259 190 621 311 744 744 458 458 1827 1827 -6.84 -9.17 1910 1053 0.001 0.001 651 300 1710 neutral neutral neutral neutral neutral neutral positive positive positive positive positive positive positive positive positive low middle 572 202 448 651 300 1710 -6.58 1021 0.001 high 672 332 1349 651 300 1710 1.84 2744 ns low 582 217 308 613 240 1236 -2.20 511 0.05 middle 647 240 936 613 240 1236 3.31 2170 0.001 high 810 471 769 613 240 1236 10.76 1021 0.001 low middle high 664 646 742 399 336 515 305 624 461 744 744 744 458 458 458 1827 1827 1827 -3.15 -5.68 -0.08 449 1463 656 0.01 0.001 ns low 660 344 738 651 300 1710 0.67 1243 ns middle 584 163 156 651 300 1710 -4.46 264 high 723 309 311 651 300 1710 3.84 2019 ns low 651 289 307 613 240 1236 2.14 417 ns middle 712 404 156 613 240 1236 2.98 169 ns high 660 270 773 613 240 1236 3.99 1497 ns I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 0.05 114 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table B6. Implicit-Explicit correspondence: D(IAT) and Hyper-identity (Percentage) Hyper-I D(IAT) All groups, session 1, N=123 Сount All groups, session 2, N=123 Сount Experimental groups, session 1, N=93 Сount Experimental groups, session 2, N=93 Сount Control groups, session 1, N=30 Сount Control groups, session 2, N=30 Сount % % % % % % low Latvian pleasant than Russian middle no preference high Russian pleasant than Latvian Total 1 15 23 39 0.81 12.20 18.70 31.71 5 15 23 43 4.07 12.20 18.70 34.96 21 3 1 25 22.58 3.23 1.08 26.88 20 12 3 35 12.51 12.90 3.23 37.63 3 4 0 7 10.00 13.33 0.00 23.33 3 3 2 8 10.00 10.00 6.67 26.67 Table B7. Implicit-Explicit correspondence: D(IAT) and Hypo-identity (Percentage) Hyper-I Variables D(IAT) All groups, session 1, N=123 Сount All groups, session 2, N=123 Сount Experimental groups, session 1, N=93 Сount Experimental groups, session 2, N=93 Сount Control groups, session 1, N=30 Сount Control groups, session 2, N=30 Сount % % % % % % low Russian pleasant than Latvian middle no preference high Latvian pleasant than Russian Total 25 12 1 38 20.33 9.76 0.81 30.89 22 14 5 41 17.89 11.38 4.07 33.33 19 7 0 26 20.43 7.53 0.00 27.96 19 10 4 33 20.43 10.75 4.30 35.48 6 5 1 12 20.00 16.67 3.33 40.00 3 4 1 8 10.00 13.33 3.33 26.67 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Table B8. Comparisons of D-scores under different conditions, p-values D, Conditions 1 D, Conditions 2 negative at once negative after one day negative at once negative after one week negative after one day negative after one week positive at once positive after one day positive at once positive after one week positive after one day positive after one week neutral at once neutral after one day neutral at once neutral after one week neutral after one day neutral after one week no impact at once no impact after one day no impact at once no impact after one week no impact after one day no impact after one week negative at once no impact at once negative after one day no impact after one day negative after one week no impact after one week positive at once no impact at once positive after one day no impact after one day positive after one week no impact after one week neutral at once no impact at once neutral after one day no impact after one day neutral after one week no impact after one week I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) p-value .021 .044 .860 .734 .064 .195 .109 .171 .797 .156 .287 .856 .753 .439 .509 .617 .749 .206 .004 .862 .801 115 116 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Appendix C Factors interactions on RT in subliminal evaluative priming task. Study 2. Figures FIGURE C1. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “SOA” and “Target” interaction FIGURE C2. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Valence” and “Target” interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 117 FIGURE C3. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Retest” and “Target” interaction FIGURE C4. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “SOA” and “Target” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 118 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE C5. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Valence” and “Retest” interaction FIGURE C6. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Valence” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 119 FIGURE C3. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Retest” and “Target” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 120 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE C8. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Target”, “Retest” and “Valence” interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 121 FIGURE C9. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Target”, “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 122 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE C10. Means of RT in two sessions for factors’ “Valence”, “Retest” and “Intensity of emotional impact” interaction T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 123 Appendix D Research of facilitation scores. Study 2 TABLE D1. Results of repeated measurements ANOVA for R in study 2 SOA Session 100 1 100 100 Factors F df1 df2 p prime*target 6.4 1 108 <.05 2 IEI 4.97 2 91 <.01 2 valence*retest*IEI 3.33 7 91 <.01 200 2 prime*retest 3.56 2 91 <.05 200 2 target*valence*retest 2.39 4 91 .057 200 2 prime*target*valence*retest 2.42 4 91 0.54 200 2 valence 3.48 2 91 <.05 200 2 retest 3.73 2 91 <.05 200 2 IEI 7.65 2 91 .001 200 2 valence*retest*IEI 2.85 7 91 .01 300 1 target 8.37 1 108 <.001 300 2 prime*target 3.88 1 91 .052 300 2 prime*target*valence*retest*IEI 2.18 7 91 <.05 300 2 target*retest 2.91 2 91 .059 300 2 valence 3.02 2 91 .054 300 2 IEI 4.87 2 91 .010 400 1 prime*target*valence*retest 2.28 6 108 <.05 400 2 prime*target*IEI 3.07 2 91 .051 400 2 prime*retest 6.7 2 91 <.01 400 2 valence*retest 2.44 4 91 .052 500 2 prime 4.62 1 91 <.05 500 2 prime*IEI 3.5 2 91 <.05 500 2 target 5.58 1 91 <.05 500 2 prime*target*valence*retest*IEI 2.05 7 91 .057 500 2 valence 5.95 2 91 <.01 500 2 IEI 5.95 2 91 .056 600 2 prime*valence*retest 2.59 4 91 <.05 600 2 IEI 3.26 2 91 <.05 Note. IEI – intensity of emotional impact. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 124 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE D2. Conditions for priming effect at SOA 100 ms and 200 ms in study 2 df Session 2 t df Sig. (2-tailed) t Sig. (2-tailed) Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact Valence: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive, no-no impact Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week Target: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive Group: e-experimental, c-control SOA Session 1 (no impact) 100 l 1.91 123 .058 100 m -3.04 111 .003 100 no 100 no 100 c 100 c 100 neg neg -0.42 479 .678 -2.18 127 .031 -0.36 127 .716 -2.18 127 .031 -1.71 63 .092 -2.33 63 .023 -0.36 127 .716 -2.18 127 .031 -1.21 239 .229 -1.83 239 .069 100 0 neg -0.20 35 .841 -2.16 35 .038 100 1 pos -2.23 19 .038 -0.55 19 .587 100 0 no -0.55 51 .585 -2.48 51 .017 3.51 111 .001 2.15 127 .033 200 m 200 no 2.66 479 .008 200 neu 0.85 159 .395 3.38 159 .001 200 pos 3.24 83 .002 -0.21 83 .834 200 no -0.05 127 .961 2.15 127 .033 200 0 2.52 179 .013 1.55 179 .123 200 7 0.46 147 .644 3.03 147 .003 200 e neg 1.36 175 .175 2.12 175 .036 200 e pos 3.18 175 .002 1.64 175 .104 200 c neg -0.39 63 .696 3.29 63 .002 200 e 3.14 351 .002 2.65 351 .008 200 c -0.05 127 .961 2.15 127 .033 200 neg 1.02 239 .309 3.31 239 .001 200 pos 2.78 239 .006 1.43 239 .154 2.66 479 .008 3.33 479 .001 200 4.22 959 .000 200 1 neg 2.40 39 .021 -0.45 39 .653 200 0 pos 2.62 31 .013 -0.58 31 .563 200 7 neu 0.27 51 .788 2.80 51 .007 200 Notes. Group: e-experimental, c-control. Target: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive. Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week. Valence: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive, no-no impact. Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 125 TABLE D3. Conditions for priming effect at SOA 300 ms in study 2 df l Session 2 t df Sig. (2-tailed) t Sig. (2-tailed) 300 Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact Valence: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive, no-no impact Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week Target: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive Group: e-experimental, c-control SOA Session 1 (no impact) 2.78 123 .006 5.03 479 .000 -0.68 127 .501 300 neg 2.01 107 .047 -0.26 107 .794 300 neu 4.59 159 .000 2.03 159 .044 pos 300 no 2.21 83 .030 1.96 83 .053 300 0 3.34 179 .001 0.49 179 .627 300 1 3.56 151 .000 -0.91 151 .362 7 300 2.05 147 .042 2.80 147 .006 300 e neg 1.37 175 .173 2.25 175 .026 300 e pos 6.31 175 .000 1.02 175 .311 300 c pos 1.81 63 .075 -0.25 63 .804 300 e 5.32 351 .000 0.89 127 .377 300 c 2.27 351 .024 -0.68 127 .501 6.38 239 .000 0.79 239 .428 300 5.03 479 .000 1.62 479 .105 300 4.66 959 .000 300 300 pos 300 1 neu 4.86 47 .000 -0.40 47 .691 300 1 neg 1.83 39 .075 -0.62 39 .538 300 0 neu 2.88 59 .006 0.05 59 .961 300 7 neu 1.23 51 .224 3.49 51 .001 300 1 no -0.32 43 .747 -1.99 43 .053 Notes. Group: e-experimental, c-control. Target: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive. Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week. Valence: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive, no-no impact. Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 126 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE D4. Conditions for priming effect at SOA 400 ms in study 2 df Session 2 t df Sig. (2-tailed) t Sig. (2-tailed) Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact Valence: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive, no-no impact Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week Target: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive Group: e-experimental, c-control SOA Session 1 (no impact) 400 l -1.74 123 .084 400 h 4.13 115 .000 400 no 3.57 479 .000 0.87 127 .388 400 neg 1.69 107 .093 -0.46 107 .646 400 no 3.27 127 .001 0.87 127 .388 3.28 179 .001 -1.41 179 .160 2.23 147 .027 3.48 147 .001 400 0 400 7 400 e neg 1.86 175 .064 1.30 175 .194 400 c neg 2.08 63 .042 1.92 63 .059 400 c pos 2.53 63 .014 175 .495 400 e 2.39 351 .018 1.39 351 .165 400 c 3.27 127 .001 0.87 127 .388 400 neg 2.64 239 .009 2.06 239 .040 400 pos 2.45 239 .015 0.33 239 .741 400 3.57 479 .000 1.64 479 .102 400 3.67 959 .000 400 0 neg 1.04 35 .307 -2.71 35 .010 400 1 neu 0.33 47 .740 2.41 47 .020 400 7 neg 2.82 31 .008 2.39 31 .023 400 0 pos 2.40 31 .023 0.92 31 .363 400 7 no 1.01 31 .320 1.83 31 .077 400 7 neu 1.28 51 .207 1.79 51 .079 400 0 no 2.91 51 .005 -0.39 51 .700 Notes. Group: e-experimental, c-control. Target: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive. Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week. Valence: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive, no-no impact. Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 127 TABLE D5. Conditions for priming effect at SOA 500 ms in study 2 df l Session 2 t df Sig. (2-tailed) t Sig. (2-tailed) 500 Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact Valence: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive, no-no impact Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week Target: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive Group: e-experimental, c-control SOA Session 1 (no impact) 1.85 123 .066 3.31 479 .001 -0.83 127 .406 500 neg -0.96 107 .340 -1.47 107 .145 500 neu 3.32 159 .001 0.73 159 .464 500 pos 1.69 83 .095 2.68 83 .009 500 no 2.20 127 .030 -0.83 127 .406 0 2.19 179 .030 0.79 179 .429 1 500 no 500 2.68 151 .008 0.61 151 .541 500 e neg 2.55 175 .012 2.28 175 .024 500 c neg 3.19 63 .002 -1.91 63 .061 500 e 2.51 351 .012 0.90 351 .369 500 c 2.20 127 .030 -0.83 127 .406 3.80 239 .000 0.92 239 .359 3.31 479 .001 0.36 479 .716 500 500 neg 500 2.57 959 .010 500 0 neg -0.81 35 .424 -1.97 35 .057 500 1 neu 3.29 47 .002 1.45 47 .153 500 7 neg -2.05 31 .049 -0.79 31 .437 500 0 pos 1.50 31 .143 2.69 31 .011 500 7 neu 1.91 51 .062 -0.07 51 .941 500 0 no 2.24 51 .030 1.19 51 .238 500 Notes. Group: e-experimental, c-control. Target: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive. Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week. Valence: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive, no-no impact. Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 128 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE D6. Conditions for priming effect at SOA 600 ms in study 2 m 600 no df 1.56 479 .121 Session 2 t df Sig. (2-tailed) 600 t Sig. (2-tailed) Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact Valence: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive, no-no impact Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week Target: neg-negative, neuneutral, pos-positive Group: e-experimental, c-control SOA Session 1 (no impact) 3.02 111 .003 1.92 127 .057 600 neu 1.10 159 .271 1.95 159 .053 600 no -0.28 127 .781 1.92 127 .057 600 0 0.17 179 .866 1.83 179 .069 600 7 1.16 147 .247 2.13 147 .035 175 .171 1.77 175 .078 600 e pos 1.38 600 c neg 0.11 63 .910 2.32 63 .024 600 e 1.93 351 .055 1.77 351 .078 600 c -0.28 127 .781 1.92 127 .057 600 neg 0.94 239 .349 1.67 239 .095 600 pos 1.25 239 .211 1.82 239 .070 1.56 479 .121 2.47 479 .014 600 2.85 959 .004 600 1 neg 2.27 39 .029 -1.02 39 .314 600 0 pos 2.87 31 .007 1.66 31 .106 600 7 neu 0.57 51 .573 3.06 51 .004 600 1 no -0.13 43 .894 1.83 43 .074 600 Notes. Group: e-experimental, c-control. Target: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive. Retest: 0-at once, 1-after one day, 7-after one week. Valence: neg-negative, neu-neutral, pos-positive, no-no impact. Intensity of emotional impact: l-low, m-middle, h-high, no-no impact. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 129 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Appendix E Research of correlations. Study 2 TABLE E1. Results of correlations in study 2 Variables Control groups, N=30 -.386* Experimental groups N=88 RRR(1)-100 D(1) RRR(1)-100 RRR(2)-100 .260* .015 RRR(1)-100 RRR(2)-400 -.231* .030 RRR(1)-100 RRR(2)-600 .173 .107 RRR(1)-100 HYPO .252* .018 RRR(1)-100 Hypo-identity .200 .061 RRR(1)-200 RRR(1)-600 -.355 .054 RRR(1)-200 RRR(2)-300 .309 .096 .180 .094 RRR(1)-200 RRR(2)-400 .183 .087 RRR(1)-200 RRR(2)-600 RRR(1)-200 HYPO .197 .066 RRR(1)-200 Hypo-identity .200 .062 RRR(1)-200 HYPER -.345 .062 RRR(1)-200 Hyper-identity -.288 .123 -.193 .072 .317 .035 .088 RRR(1)-200 D(2) RRR(1)-300 RRR(1)-500 .325 .080 RRR(1)-300 RRR(1)-600 .428* .018 RRR(1)-300 RRR(2)-300 -.349 .059 RRR(1)-300 RRR(2)-500 .451* .012 * RRR(1)-300 HYPER .401 .028 RRR(1)-300 Hyper-identity .418* .021 RRR(1)-400 RRR(2)-400 .342 .064 RRR(1)-400 RRR(2)-600 .221* .039 RRR(1)-500 IEI -.213* .046 * .032 RRR(1)-500 RRR(1)-600 .392 RRR(1)-500 RRR(2)-500 .402* .028 * RRR(1)-500 HYPO -.439 .015 RRR(1)-500 Hypo-identity -.339 .067 RRR(1)-600 RRR(1)-200 -.355 .054 RRR(1)-300 * .018 RRR(1)-600 .428 I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 130 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE E1. Results of correlations in study 2 (continue) Variables Control groups, N=30 RRR(2)-100 RRR(2)-600 .446* .013 RRR(2)-100 HYPER -.437* .016 * RRR(2)-100 Hyper-identity -.361 .050 RRR(2)-200 RRR(2)-400 .337 .069 RRR(2)-200 RRR(2)-500 -.318 .087 RRR(2)-200 RRR(2)-600 RRR(2)-200 Hypo-identity .409* .025 RRR(2)-300 RRR(2)-500 -.401* .028 RRR(2)-300 RRR(2)-600 * Experimental groups N=88 -.174 .106 -.190 .076 RRR(2)-400 HYPO .378 .039 RRR(2)-400 Hypo-identity .285 .127 RRR(2)-500 IEI -.207 .052 RRR(2)-600 IEI -.176 .101 RRR(2)-600 D(2) -.178 .097 .185 .085 .486** .000 HYPO D(1) HYPO HYPER * .387 * .035 Hypo-identity D(1) .460 .011 D(1) D(2) .444* .014 Appendix F Research of changes of RRR and D. Study 2 TABLE F1. Changes RRR for negative context events between the levels of retest N2 Mann-Whitney U Sig. after one day 9 10 10.8 9.3 38.0 .568 at once after one day 9 10 11.2 8.9 34.0 .369 1 at once after one day 9 10 9.3 10.7 38.5 .595 2 at once after one day 9 10 9.0 10.9 36.0 .462 Session N1 Mean rank 2 For variable 2 Mean rank 1 For variable 1 Retest Retest 1 at once 100 2 200 200 SOA 100 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 131 TABLE F1. Changes RRR for negative context events between the levels of retest (continue) Retest Retest MannWhitney U For variable 2 Session For variable 1 Sig. 1 at once after one day 9 10 9.6 10.4 41.5 .775 2 at once after one day 9 10 10.9 9.2 37.0 .513 400 1 at once after one day 9 10 7.1 12.6 19.0 .034 400 2 at once after one day 9 10 8.9 11.0 35.5 .438 500 1 at once after one day 9 10 10.4 9.7 41.5 .775 500 2 at once after one day 9 10 9.1 10.8 37.0 .514 600 1 at once after one day 9 10 9.7 10.3 42.0 .806 600 2 at once after one day 9 10 8.6 11.3 32.5 .307 100 1 at once after one week 9 8 9.2 8.8 34.0 .847 100 2 at once after one week 9 8 10.0 7.9 27.0 .386 200 1 at once after one week 9 8 8.4 9.6 31.0 .630 200 2 at once after one week 9 8 10.8 7.0 20.0 .123 300 1 at once after one week 9 8 9.9 8.0 28.0 .441 300 2 at once after one week 9 8 9.2 8.8 34.0 .847 400 1 at once after one week 9 8 6.8 11.5 16.0 .054 400 2 at once after one week 9 8 9.7 8.3 30.0 .564 500 1 at once after one week 9 8 10.2 7.6 25.0 .290 500 2 at once after one week 9 8 9.2 8.8 34.0 .847 600 1 at once after one week 9 8 9.7 8.3 30.0 .563 600 2 at once after one week 9 8 9.7 8.2 29.5 .531 100 1 after one day after one week 10 8 9.0 10.1 35.0 .657 100 2 after one day after one week 10 8 9.2 9.9 37.0 .790 200 1 after one day after one week 10 8 9.6 9.4 39.5 .965 200 2 after one day after one week 10 8 11.9 6.5 16.0 .033 300 1 after one day after one week 10 8 10.6 8.1 29.0 .328 300 2 after one day after one week 10 8 9.0 10.1 36.0 .657 400 1 after one day after one week 10 8 9.7 9.3 38.5 .894 400 2 after one day after one week 10 8 11.1 7.5 24.0 .155 500 1 after one day after one week 10 8 11.0 7.7 25.5 .197 500 2 after one day after one week 10 8 10.4 8.4 31.0 .424 600 1 after one day after one week 10 8 10.3 8.5 32.0 .477 600 2 after one day after one week 10 8 10.9 7.8 26.0 .213 SOA 300 300 N1 N2 Mean rank 1 Mean rank 2 I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 132 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F2. Changes RRR for positive context events between the levels of retest For variable 1 For variable 2 Session Retest Retest 1 after one day at once 5 8 6.4 7.4 17.0 .661 2 after one day at once 5 8 7.4 6.8 18.0 .770 200 1 after one day at once 5 8 6.7 7.2 18.5 .826 200 2 after one day at once 5 8 8.2 6.3 14.0 .379 300 1 after one day at once 5 8 6.2 7.5 16.0 .558 300 2 after one day at once 5 8 7.4 6.8 18.0 .770 400 1 after one day at once 5 8 8.2 6.3 14.0 .380 400 2 after one day at once 5 8 6.6 7.3 18.0 .770 500 1 after one day at once 5 8 6.8 7.1 19.0 .884 500 2 after one day at once 5 8 8.4 6.1 13.0 .306 600 1 after one day at once 5 8 7.7 6.6 16.5 .608 600 2 after one day at once 5 8 6.6 7.3 18.0 .770 100 1 at once after one week 8 8 6.9 10.1 19.0 .172 100 2 at once after one week 8 8 8.0 9.0 28.0 .674 200 1 at once after one week 8 8 8.5 8.5 32.0 1.000 200 2 at once after one week 8 8 7.8 9.3 26.0 .529 300 1 at once after one week 8 8 8.4 8.6 31.0 .916 300 2 at once after one week 8 8 8.9 8.1 29.0 .753 400 1 at once after one week 8 8 8.6 8.4 31.0 .916 400 2 at once after one week 8 8 7.8 9.3 26.0 .528 500 1 at once after one week 8 8 8.3 8.8 30.0 .834 500 2 at once after one week 8 8 7.3 9.8 22.0 .294 600 1 at once after one week 8 8 9.5 7.5 24.0 .401 600 2 at once after one week 8 8 6.9 10.1 19.0 .172 100 1 after one day after one week 5 8 5.0 8.3 10.0 .143 100 2 after one day after one week 5 8 6.8 7.1 19.0 .884 200 1 after one day after one week 5 8 6.6 7.3 18.0 .770 200 2 after one day after one week 5 8 7.6 6.6 17.0 .660 300 1 after one day after one week 5 8 6.9 7.1 19.5 .941 300 2 after one day after one week 5 8 7.7 6.6 16.5 9.80 400 1 after one day after one week 5 8 7.8 6.5 16.0 .558 400 2 after one day after one week 5 8 6.8 7.1 19.0 .883 500 1 after one day after one week 5 8 7.0 7.0 20.0 1.000 500 2 after one day after one week 5 8 8.0 6.4 15.0 .464 600 1 after one day after one week 5 8 8.4 6.1 13.0 .306 600 2 after one day after one week 5 8 5.7 7.8 13.5 .341 SOA 100 100 N1 N2 Mean rank 1 Mean rank 2 MannWhitney U Sig. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 133 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F3. Changes RRR for positive and negative context events between two sessions N1= N2 Mean rank in sesion 1 Mean rank in session 2 MannWhitney U Sig. at once 9 8.8 10.2 34.0 .566 after one day 10 9.7 11.3 42.0 .545 after one week 8 8.8 8.3 30.0 .834 at once 9 9.2 9.8 38.0 .825 negative after one day 10 10.0 11.0 45.0 .705 200 negative after one week 8 10.5 6.5 16.0 .093 300 negative at once 9 8.7 10.3 33.0 .508 300 negative after one day 10 10.6 10.4 49.0 .940 300 negative after one week 8 7.1 9.9 20.5 .227 400 negative at once 9 7.0 12.0 18.0 .047 400 negative after one day 10 8.7 12.4 31.5 .162 400 negative after one week 8 9.1 7.9 27.5 .636 500 negative at once 9 10.3 8.7 33.0 .508 500 negative after one day 10 10.0 11.0 45.0 .705 500 negative after one week 8 8.0 9.0 28.0 .674 600 negative at once 9 8.6 10.4 32.5 .480 600 negative after one day 10 8.0 13.0 25.0 .059 600 negative after one week 8 7.9 9.1 27.0 .599 100 positive after one day 5 5.0 6.0 10.0 .602 100 positive at once 8 8.4 8.6 31.0 .916 100 positive after one week 8 9.0 8.0 28.0 .674 200 positive after one day 5 5.1 5.9 10.5 .674 200 positive at once 8 9.3 7.7 25.5 .495 200 positive after one week 8 8.9 8.1 29.0 .753 300 positive after one day 5 4.2 6.8 6.0 .173 300 positive at once 8 7.5 9.5 24.0 .401 300 positive after one week 8 7.0 10.0 20.0 .208 400 positive after one day 5 6.6 4.4 7.0 .251 400 positive at once 8 10.1 6.9 19.0 .172 400 positive after one week 8 9.3 7.8 26.0 .528 500 positive after one day 5 5.2 5.8 11.0 .754 500 positive at once 8 9.8 7.3 22.0 .294 500 positive after one week 8 9.3 7.7 25.5 .495 600 positive after one day 5 5.4 5.6 12.0 .917 600 positive at once 8 7.4 9.6 23.0 .345 600 positive after one week 8 5.5 11.5 8.0 .012 SOA Valence 100 negative 100 negative 100 negative 200 negative 200 Retest I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 134 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F4. Changes D for negative and positive context events between the levels of retest D(2) Retest Retest 1 at once 2 negative 1 Valence D(1) Ses sion N1 N2 Mean rank 1 Mean rank 2 MannWhitney U Sig. after one day 9 10 10.1 9.9 44.0 .935 at once after one day 9 10 12.1 8.1 26.0 .121 at once after one week 9 8 11.4 7.6 23.5 .133 after one week 9 8 11.9 7.1 18.5 .052 after one day after one week 10 9 11.9 7.9 26.5 .131 2 after one day after one week 10 9 10.2 9.8 43.0 .870 1 after one day after one week 9 8 10.7 7.1 21 .149 2 after one day after one week 9 8 11.5 6.2 13.5 .030 after one day at once 9 8 10.3 7.6 24.5 .267 after one day at once 9 8 9.2 8.8 34 .847 1 at once after one week 8 8 9.1 7.9 27 .599 2 at once after one week 8 8 10.5 6.5 16 .092 1 2 positive at once 1 2 TABLE F5. Changes D for positive and negative context events between two sessions Valence Retest N1= N2 Mean rank 1 Mean rank 2 MannWhitney U Sig. negative at once 9 7.9 11.1 26.0 .200 negative after one day 10 11.0 10.0 45.0 .705 negative after one week 9 8.7 10.3 33.0 .508 positive after one day 9 8.4 10.6 30.5 .377 positive at once 8 7.1 9.9 21.0 .248 positive after one week 8 8.8 8.3 30.0 .833 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 135 TABLE F6. Changes in the means of RRR and D for positive context events between retest at once and after one day df2 f 0.05 100 8 16.0 70.0 5 -7.4 43.9 2.54 7 4 6.09 0.66 11 200 8 7.8 82.9 5 1.6 76.9 1.16 7 4 6.09 0.13 11 300 8 18.1 94.8 5 -8.0 3.89 7 4 6.09 0.57 11 400 8 15.1 73.0 5 36.0 2.58 4 7 4.12 -0.40 11 500 8 -5.9 76.5 5 -6.8 48.0 117. 3 52.2 2.15 7 4 6.09 0.02 11 600 8 2.4 81.4 5 4.6 65.0 1.57 7 4 6.09 -0.05 11 8 22.3 79.6 5 35.8 99.2 1.55 4 7 4.12 -0.27 11 200 8 -16.8 100.0 5 15.8 60.7 2.71 7 4 6.09 -0.65 11 300 8 46.1 72.0 5 50.4 70.5 1.04 7 4 6.09 -0.10 11 400 8 -29.6 65.7 5 -33.8 2.06 4 7 4.12 0.09 11 500 8 -26.4 75.7 5 -15.0 2.86 4 7 4.12 -0.20 11 600 8 33.8 63.7 5 21.2 94.3 128. 1 98.2 2.37 4 7 4.12 0.28 11 8 -0.31 0.35 5 -0.39 0.13 6.83 7 4 6.09 0.46 11 8 -0.13 0.36 5 -0.19 0.18 3.70 7 4 6.09 0.39 11 RRR Mea n2 df1 F N2 Std. Deviation 2 Mean 1 100 2 Std. Deviation 1 N1 Variable Session 1 SO A 1 D 2 t df Note. The critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.20, t0.01=3.11, t0.001=4.44. TABLE F7. Changes in the means of RRR and D for positive context events between retest at once and after one week df1 df2 83.0 8.4 -2.6 15.1 13.3 -44.1 110.4 74.4 43.1 85.6 108.2 84.2 2.49 1.24 4.84 1.38 2.00 1.07 7 7 7 7 7 7 8 8 8 8 8 8 31.1 -3.1 8.3 -10.5 -21.1 62.9 137.6 62.2 110.9 72.9 60.9 37.8 2.99 2.58 2.37 1.23 1.54 2.85 8 8 -0.46 -0.45 0.18 0.21 3.97 2.78 16.0 7.8 18.1 15.1 -5.9 2.4 70.0 82.9 94.8 73.0 76.5 81.4 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 22.3 -16.8 46.1 -29.6 -26.4 33.8 79.6 100.0 72.0 65.7 75.7 63.7 8 8 -0.31 -0.13 0.35 0.36 8 8 8 8 8 8 t df 7 7 7 7 7 7 -1.45 -0.02 0.56 0.00 -0.41 1.12 14 14 14 14 14 14 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 -0.16 -0.33 0.81 -0.55 -0.15 -1.11 14 14 14 14 14 14 7 7 7 7 1.06 2.21 14 14 3.79 N2 f 0.05 Mean 2 D F Mean 1 1 2 RRR SOA 2 100 200 300 400 500 600 Variable Session 1 100 200 300 400 500 600 Std. Deviation 2 Std. Deviation 1 N1 Note. The critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.14, t0.01=2.98, t0.001=4.14. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 136 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F8. Changes in the means of RRR and D for positive context events between retest after one day and after one week Mean 2 Std. Deviation 2 df1 df2 110.4 74.4 43.1 6.33 1.07 1.24 4 7 4 7 4 7 4.12 6.09 4.12 15.1 85.6 1.88 4 7 4.12 0.37 11 13.3 -44.1 31.1 -3.1 8.3 108.2 84.2 137.6 62.2 110.9 4.30 1.68 1.92 1.05 2.48 7 7 7 7 7 4 4 4 4 4 6.09 6.09 6.09 6.09 6.09 -0.38 1.10 0.07 0.54 0.75 11 11 11 11 11 8 -10.5 72.9 1.67 4 7 4.12 -0.50 11 128.1 8 -21.1 60.9 4.42 4 7 4.12 0.12 11 21.2 98.2 8 62.9 37.8 6.75 4 7 4.12 -1.10 11 -0.39 0.13 8 -0.46 0.18 1.72 7 4 6.09 0.76 11 0.18 8 -0.45 0.21 1.33 7 4 6.09 2.21 11 N2 Std. Deviation 1 83.0 8.4 -2.6 Mean 1 N1 F 100 200 300 5 5 5 -7.4 1.6 -8.0 43.9 76.9 48.0 8 8 8 400 5 36.0 117.3 8 500 600 100 200 300 5 5 5 5 5 -6.8 4.6 35.8 15.8 50.4 52.2 65.0 99.2 60.7 70.5 8 8 8 8 8 400 5 -33.8 94.3 500 5 -15.0 600 5 5 5 -0.19 1 RRR 2 Variable Session 1 SOA D 2 f 0.05 t df -1.72 -0.16 -0.21 11 11 11 Note. The critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.20, t0.01=3.11, t0.001=4.44. TABLE F9. Changes in the means of RRR and D for negative context events between retest at once and after one day N2 Mean 2 df1 df2 10 10 10 -1.4 21.2 20.7 61.1 61.6 67.0 1.23 1.63 0.49 8 8 9 9 9 8 3.23 3.23 3.39 0.83 -0.89 -0.65 17 17 17 400 9 -52.4 65.0 10 19.6 70.7 2.58 8 9 3.23 -2.30 17 500 600 100 200 300 400 9 9 9 9 9 9 19.7 -15.4 38.7 1.3 27.9 14.1 64.6 82.9 85.4 79.4 81.7 65.2 10 10 10 10 10 10 22.5 -15.1 2.8 32.1 4.9 58.4 80.5 52.6 51.0 70.5 63.0 85.5 0.64 2.49 1.55 1.27 1.68 2.06 9 8 8 8 8 8 8 9 9 9 9 9 3.39 3.23 3.23 3.23 3.23 3.23 -0.08 -0.01 1.13 -0.90 0.69 -1.26 17 17 17 17 17 17 500 9 -3.9 70.1 10 18.7 93.2 2.86 8 9 3.23 -0.59 17 600 9 10.8 44.7 10 40.0 62.5 2.37 8 9 3.23 -1.16 17 9 9 -0.25 0.00 0.37 0.40 10 10 -0.29 -0.37 0.31 0.45 1.42 0.78 8 8 9 9 3.23 3.23 0.26 1.90 17 17 D Std. Deviation 2 Std. Deviation 1 67.7 78.7 46.9 Mean 1 23.2 -7.4 3.2 N1 1 2 9 9 9 RRR 2 100 200 300 Variable Session 1 f 0.05 SOA F Note. The critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.20, t0.01=2.90, t0.001=3.97. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) t df 137 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F10. Changes in the means of RRR and D for negative context events between retest at once and after one week df1 df2 7 8 8 3.73 3.50 3.50 0.74 -0.71 0.54 15 15 15 8 19.1 73.2 1.38 8 7 3.73 -2.14 15 8 8 8 8 8 -14.5 -33.8 6.4 -67.8 19.3 69.4 79.5 65.1 95.9 55.0 2.00 1.07 2.99 0.68 2.37 8 8 8 8 8 7 7 7 7 7 3.73 3.73 3.73 3.73 3.73 1.05 0.46 0.87 1.62 0.25 15 15 15 15 15 65.2 8 8.5 87.3 1.23 8 7 3.73 0.15 15 -3.9 70.1 8 0.5 97.3 0.52 7 8 3.50 -0.11 15 9 10.8 44.7 8 -0.9 103.7 0.19 7 8 3.50 0.31 15 9 -0.25 0.37 8 -0.50 0.43 0.72 7 8 3.50 1.27 15 9 0.00 0.40 8 -0.34 0.31 1.62 8 7 3.73 1.97 15 23.2 -7.4 3.2 67.7 78.7 46.9 8 8 8 400 9 -52.4 65.0 500 600 100 200 300 9 9 9 9 9 19.7 -15.4 38.7 1.3 27.9 64.6 82.9 85.4 79.4 81.7 400 9 14.1 500 9 600 D 2 Std. Deviation 2 8 7 7 N2 2.49 0.88 0.44 9 9 9 1 Mean 2 Std. Deviation 1 Mean 1 48.1 83.9 70.9 100 200 300 RRR 2 2.0 20.4 -12.3 N1 1 Variable Session SOA F f 0.05 t df Note. The critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.13, t0.01=2.95, t0.001=4.07. TABLE F11. Changes in the means of RRR and D for negative context events between retest after one day and after one week Std. Deviation 1 N2 df1 df2 61.1 61.6 67.0 8 8 8 2.0 20.4 -12.3 48.1 83.9 70.9 6.33 0.54 0.89 9 7 7 7 9 9 4.12 6.09 4.12 -0.13 0.02 1.01 16 16 16 400 10 19.6 70.7 8 19.1 73.2 0.93 7 9 4.12 0.01 16 500 600 100 200 300 400 10 10 10 10 10 10 22.5 -15.1 2.8 32.1 4.9 58.4 80.5 52.6 51.0 70.5 63.0 85.5 8 8 8 8 8 8 -14.5 -33.8 6.4 -67.8 19.3 8.5 69.4 79.5 65.1 95.9 55.0 87.3 4.30 1.68 1.92 1.05 2.48 0.96 9 9 9 9 9 7 7 7 7 7 7 9 6.09 6.09 6.09 6.09 6.09 4.12 1.03 0.60 -0.13 2.55 -0.51 1.22 16 16 16 16 16 16 500 10 18.7 93.2 8 0.5 97.3 0.92 7 9 4.12 0.40 16 600 10 40.0 62.5 8 -0.9 103.7 0.36 7 9 4.12 1.04 16 10 10 -0.29 -0.37 0.31 0.45 8 8 -0.50 -0.34 0.43 0.31 1.72 1.33 9 9 7 7 6.09 6.09 1.18 -0.16 16 16 D Std. Deviation 2 Mean 1 -1.4 21.2 20.7 Mean 2 N1 1 2 10 10 10 RRR 2 100 200 300 Variable Session 1 f 0.05 SOA F Note. TThe critical values for t-distribution are: t0.05=2.12, t0.01=2.92, t0.001=4.01. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) t df 138 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F12. Changes in the means of RRR and D for negative valences of context events and for different retests between the first and the second sessions. Paired t-test negative, at once negative, after one day negative, after one week Sig. (2-tailed) Group N Correlation Sig. .722 9 -0.34 .366 8 .807 9 0.13 .734 -0.68 8 .516 9 -0.39 .303 -1.64 8 .139 9 -0.75 .020 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.70 8 .503 9 -0.12 .765 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -0.95 8 .370 9 0.27 .480 D1 - D2 -2.40 8 .043 9 0.66 .053 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.17 9 .865 10 0.09 .809 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -0.44 9 .668 10 0.31 .377 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 0.69 9 .510 10 0.37 .287 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 -0.95 9 .367 10 -0.36 .308 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.10 9 .925 10 -0.01 .984 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -3.05 9 .014 10 0.52 .124 D1 - D2 0.61 9 .559 10 0.46 .185 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.12 7 .906 8 -0.60 .116 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 1.67 7 .139 8 -0.38 .357 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -0.76 7 .475 8 -0.75 .032 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 0.31 7 .767 8 0.27 .512 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -0.30 7 .775 8 -0.46 .257 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -1.26 7 .247 8 0.71 .050 D1 - D2 -1.41 7 .202 8 0.69 .058 Variables t df RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.37 8 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -0.25 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 139 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F13. Changes in the means of RRR and D for neutral valences of context events and for different retests between the first and the second sessions. Paired t-test negative, at once negative, after one day negative, after one week Sig. (2-tailed) Group N Correlation Sig. .169 15 0.37 .175 14 .821 15 0.41 .128 14 .018 15 0.28 .318 -0.19 14 .852 15 0.02 .955 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.01 14 .996 15 -0.35 .203 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 0.19 14 .849 15 0.08 .765 D1 - D2 -1.04 14 .315 15 0.62 .013 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.83 11 .422 12 0.50 .100 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 1.14 11 .280 12 0.47 .122 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -1.09 11 .300 12 0.00 .993 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 0.31 11 .762 12 0.00 .989 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -1.15 11 .274 12 0.34 .276 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -0.90 11 .390 12 0.04 .909 D1 - D2 -3.14 11 .009 12 0.42 .168 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 0.49 12 .630 13 -0.07 .808 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 1.01 12 .331 13 0.31 .307 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -1.40 12 .187 13 0.11 .726 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 0.57 12 .582 13 0.18 .557 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -1.22 12 .246 13 0.05 .868 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 0.68 12 .508 13 0.46 .117 D1 - D2 -1.41 12 .185 13 0.56 .048 Variables t df RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 1.45 14 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 0.23 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 2.68 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 140 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F14. Changes in the means of RRR and D for positive valences of context events and for different retests between the first and the second sessions. Paired t-test negative, at once negative, after one day negative, after one week Sig. (2-tailed) Group N Correlation Sig. .850 8 0.28 .496 7 .628 8 -0.11 .787 -0.67 7 .526 8 0.01 .987 1.64 7 .145 8 0.38 .348 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.50 7 .631 8 -0.15 .725 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -0.73 7 .491 8 -0.41 .314 D1 - D2 -1.14 7 .291 8 0.12 .777 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -1.10 4 .333 5 0.47 .428 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -0.27 4 .804 5 -0.50 .391 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -1.37 4 .241 5 -0.26 .676 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 0.86 4 .440 5 -0.48 .418 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.13 4 .903 5 -0.05 .936 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -0.23 4 .829 5 -0.95 .011 D1 - D2 -2.08 4 .106 5 0.14 .822 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 1.70 7 .133 8 0.78 .023 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 0.42 7 .685 8 0.38 .357 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -0.26 7 .805 8 -0.02 .964 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 1.46 7 .188 8 0.82 .014 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 0.76 7 .470 8 -0.06 .882 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -2.91 7 .023 8 -0.37 .373 D1 - D2 -0.11 7 .912 8 -0.02 .962 Variables t df RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.20 7 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 0.51 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) 141 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT TABLE F15. Changes in the means of RRR and D for absence of context events and for different retests between the first and the second sessions. Paired t-test negative, at once negative, after one day negative, after one week Sig. (2-tailed) Group N Correlation Sig. .213 12 -0.15 .646 11 .009 12 0.11 .729 11 .530 12 -0.44 .155 2.49 11 .030 12 0.50 .095 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -0.95 11 .362 12 0.37 .234 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 0.62 11 .546 12 -0.27 .391 D1 - D2 -2.25 11 .046 12 0.27 .390 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 -0.39 10 .708 11 0.46 .159 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -0.51 10 .620 11 0.46 .156 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 -2.23 10 .050 11 0.08 .817 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 0.90 10 .391 11 -0.28 .408 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -0.53 10 .605 11 0.25 .463 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -1.00 10 .342 11 -0.09 .799 D1 - D2 -1.78 10 .105 11 0.63 .039 RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 0.18 6 .864 7 -0.71 .076 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -1.50 6 .184 7 0.28 .548 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 0.05 6 .965 7 -0.44 .326 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 -2.21 6 .070 7 0.77 .041 RRR(1)-500 - RRR(2)-500 -2.09 6 .081 7 0.72 .066 RRR(1)-600 - RRR(2)-600 -1.10 6 .313 7 -0.09 .847 D1 - D2 -0.25 6 .811 7 0.17 .716 Variables t df RRR(1)-100 - RRR(2)-100 1.32 11 RRR(1)-200 - RRR(2)-200 -3.14 RRR(1)-300 - RRR(2)-300 0.65 RRR(1)-400 - RRR(2)-400 I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 142 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE F1. Boxplots for D (IAT) in the second session. Faktor “Retest” FIGURE F2. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 100 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 143 FIGURE F3. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 200 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” FIGURE F4. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 300 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 144 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE F5. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 400 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” FIGURE F6. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 500 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 145 FIGURE F7. Boxplots for RRR at SOA 600 ms in the second session. Faktor “Retest” I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 146 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT FIGURE F8. The means of RRR at the different SOA in the second session. Negative emotional valence of context event. Faktor “Retest” T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT 147 FIGURE F9. The means of RRR at the different SOA in the second session. Positive emotional valence of context event. Faktor “Retest” I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 148 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Appendix G Factor analysis. Study 2 TABLE G1. Rotated Component Matrixa. Control groups Component 1 RRR(1)-300 .780 RRR(2)-500 .724 RRR(2)-300 -.706 RRR(1)-500 .535 2 3 .848 RRR(2)-400 .562 .542 RRR(2)-600 .738 RRR(2)-100 .720 RRR(1)-200 .604 .432 -.513 RRR(1)-100 -.498 -.489 D1 .846 D2 .703 Hypo-identity .597 .607 RRR(1)-400 RRR(1)-600 5 -.479 RRR(2)-200 Hyper-identity 4 .861 .501 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 7 iterations. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) .558 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND M EASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT a TABLE G2. Rotated Component Matrix . Experimental groups Component 1 RRR(1)-100 .698 RRR(2)-100 .662 RRR(2)-400 -.457 Intensity of emotional impact -.441 2 3 4 5 .412 RRR(1)-600 D2 .805 D1 .659 RRR(1)-500 RRR(2)-600 .629 RRR(2)-200 -.605 RRR(1)-400 .551 RRR(2)-300 -.472 RRR(2)-500 -.614 RRR(1)-300 .581 Hyper-identity -.551 RRR(1)-200 .755 Hypo-identity .676 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 7 iterations. I NTERNATIONAL BUSINESS : Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) 149 150 I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. I MPLICIT A TTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS : E FFECT OF C ONTEXT Slaptieji požiūrio matavimai: konteksto įtaka Irina Plotka, Dmitry Igonin, Nina Blumenau Santrauka Slaptųjų (netiesioginių) socialinio pažinimo tyrime analizuojama, kaip įvairūs konteksto veiksniai daro įtaką slaptųjų matavimų procedūroms ir kaip kinta slaptieji (pasąmoniniai) požiūriai (bazinės asociacijos su požiūrio objektu). Tyrimų, kuriuose būtų aptariami konteksto veiksnių įtakos mechanizmai, yra labai mažai. Šios tyrimo tikslas – ištirti konteksto veiksnių įtaką slaptiesiems (pasąmoniniams) požiūriams bei jų matavimui, remiantis etninių požiūrių kaitos pavyzdžiu. Šio tikslo buvo siekiama atliekant dvi nepriklausomas studijas. Pirmojoje tyrėme slaptuosius (pasąmoninius) etninius požiūrius tų tiriamųjų, kurie skundėsi, kad jų gyvenime dėl nesenų autobiografinių įvykių atsirado tam tikras požiūris į etnines žmonių grupes. Šie įvykiai buvo stebimi kaip kontekstiniai veiksniai, matuojant slaptuosius požiūrius SEP (nesuvokiamo, pasąmoninio pagrindinio (emocinio) motyvo vertinimo) metodu. Antrojoje studijoje tiriamiesiems pateikėme kontekstinius veiksnius, t. y. įvykius, rodydami vieną iš video, kuriuose buvo rodomi etniniai konfliktai, kituose video buvo rodoma palaikanti, konstruktyvią sąveiką tarp etninių grupių, trečiuose pateikiami neutralūs įvykiai tarp etninių grupių. Tiriamieji dalyvavo dviejose sesijose, kuriose buvo matuojami jų slaptieji (pasąmoniniai) požiūriai, panaudojant slaptųjų asociacijų testą (IAT) ir SEP metodą. Vėliau, su skirtingu atidėliojimo intervalu buvo atliktas retestas. Abiejų studijų programa taip pat apėmė savęs vertinimo procedūras, matuojančias jų etninius požiūrius. Taip pat abiejose studijose tiriamiesiems buvo pateikiamas klausimynas, tiriantis subjektyvų emociškai reikšmingų konteksto įvykių vertinimą. Studijoje dalyvavo 238 abiejų lyčių 17–50 metų amžiaus tiriamieji. Pirmoje studijoje dalyvavo latviai ir rusai, antrojoje – tik rusai. SEP tyrimai buvo atliekami, remiantis semantinio tinklo aktyvinimo išplitimo koncepcija. Tyri- mas abiejose studijose parodė, kad konteksto veiksniai turėjo statistiškai reikšmingą įtaką slaptųjų (pasąmoninių ) požiūrių į etnines grupes formavimuisi. Tą parodė taip pat ir reakcijos laiko pokyčių matavimai, pateikiant įvairius konteksto veiksnius, tiriamiesiems atliekant įvairias eksperimentines užduotis. Šie pokyčiai buvo asocijuoti su emociškai svarbiais konteksto įvykiais bei emocinio smūgio jėga. Antrojoje studijoje šie pokyčiai taip pat buvo asocijuoti su laiko tarpu tarp konteksto įvykio ir pakartotinio slaptojo požiūrio matavimo. Konteksto įvykiai turėjo statistiškai reikšmingos įtakos pasąmoninių požiūrių matavimui. Tą parodė pasirinkimo testas (SIIP), D-skalės. Buvo nustatyta, kad SIIP su atakos stimulu asinchroniškai veikia (SOA) kas 300, 500, 100 ir 200 milisekundžių ir yra artimai susijęs su slaptųjų požiūrių matavimais. Rezultatai patvirtino hipotezę, kad egzistuoja sąveika tarp atskirų konteksto įvykių kognityvinių procesų ir laiko intervalų, kuriais yra pateikiami SOA stimulai. Procesai, kuriuos nustato SIIP su SOA kas 300, 500 ir gal būt kas 100 ir 600 milisekundžių yra asocijuojami su slaptaisiais ir atviraisiais (“hiperidentitetas”) etniniais požiūriais, tuo tarpu procesai, atsirandantys su SOA kas 200, 400 milisekundžių yra asocijuojami su alternatyviuoju etniniu požiūriu (“hipoidentitetas”). Dirbtinai sukurti konteksto įvykiai paveikia tiek bazinio tiek ir alternatyvaus etninio požiūrio tiriamųjų reakcijas. Kontekstinės įtakos tariamai kuria naujas asociacijas su požiūrio objektu, linkusiu į greitesnį susilaikymą, jei jos derinasi su bazine nuomone arba lėtesnį, jei jie derinasi su alternatyviu etniniu požiūriu. Rezultatai prisideda prie geresnio kontekstinių įtakų mechanizmų, susijusio su slaptųjų požiūrių ir jų matavimo supratimo. Raktiniai žodžiai: slaptieji, atvirieji požiūriai, konĮteikta / Submitted: 2016-05-26 Priimta / Accepted: 2016-06-03 T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) About the authors T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : i n ova cij os , p sic h olog ija , eko nom ika ✻ ISSN 2029-5774 INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: I nnov ati o ns , P sy c holo gy , Econom ic s About the authors NINA BLUMENAU – Doctor of Science in Engineering, As.Professor. Research interests: statistical methods for the behavioural sciences, methodology of research in psychology, econometrics, mathematical methods of physics. One of the most important research trends is the problem of social attitudes measurement with implicit methods – experimental procedure of unconscious emotional priming and implicit association test. Baltic International Academy, Department of Psychology. Lomonosova Str.4, Riga, Latvia, LV-1003. E-mail: [email protected] DMITRY IGONIN – Doctor of Psychology, Head of scientific agency Latenta Ltd., Board member of Latvian Professional Psychologists Association (EFPA member). The main fields of scientific activity: cognitive psychology, experi- mental psychology, social psychology (implicit social cognition), applied psychology (organizational psychology and cognitive ergonomics). Baltic International Academy, Department of Psychology. Lomonosova Str.4, Riga, Latvia, LV-1003. E-mail: [email protected]. IRINA PLOTKA – Doctor Psychology, Professor, Director of Professional Master Study Programme „Psychology“, Head of the Department Psychology. The main trends of sientific activity are: experimental psychology, social psychology (implicit social cognition) and its applied branches – ethnic psychology and occupational psychology Baltic International Academy, Department of Psychology. Lomonosova Str.4, Riga, Latvia, LV-1003. E-mail: [email protected] Information for authors T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : i n ova cij os , p sic h olog ija , eko nom ika ✻ ISSN 2029-5774 INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: I nnov ati o ns , P sy c holo gy , Econom ic s Information for authors 1. International Business: Innovations, Psychology, Economics publishes only original scientific research papers and book reviews. results with the published findings of other authors. 2. Submission to the Editorial Office. Please send the electronic Microsoft Word version of your paper (format A4, font Times New Roman, height 12, spacing 1.5, volume half to one printer‟s sheet, i.e. 20 000 to 40 000 characters with spaces, in exceptional cases up to two printer‟ sheets (if agreed with the Editor-in-Chief), e-mail address: [email protected] E d i t o r i a l Of f i c e a d d r e s s: Center of Creativity Education M. Pretorijaus 7-16, LT-06227 Vilnius, Lithuania. Summary in English (or in Lithuanian, if the paper is written in English). 3. The title page must include: 1) a brief and informative title of the paper; 2) author(s) name (s); 3) name of the institution or subsidiary bodies which employ the author(s); 4) author(s) scientific degree, academic name, title, university, department, the number and titles of publications of scholarly papers and books, affiliation with scientific associations, editorial boards, area of scientific interests; 5) e -mail address, phone number, work address, correspondence address, date of the paper submission for publication. 4. Parts of the paper: Abstract, Key words, Introduction, Methods, Results and Discussion, Conclusions, References. Abstract should describe the goal of the study, contain a brief description of methods and the main findings. At the end, three to five key words are added. The abstract is printed before the main text. Introduction presents the idea of the study, methods, aims, objectives or hypotheses. It is desirable that the literature sources quoted in the first section would have a direct relation to the results of the study. Methods describe the study material, its examination procedures and methods applied. Results provide a detailed description of tables, graphs and a statistical data analysis. Discussion offers a comparison of the obtained References. The article must have clearly separated paragraphs. All titles (chapters, sections, tables, figures) are printed in small letters and numbered. 5. Symbols. The paper should be edited, its printed text must be proofread. It is desirable that authors use only standard abbreviations and symbols. Nonstandard abbreviations and symbols should be explained in the text when used for the first time. All measurement results are provided in the international SI system units. 6. Tables and illustrations. A serial number and a short title are written above the tables. All explanations must be in the text or in a short note under the table. Abbreviations and symbols used in the table should be consistent with the main text, images, and drawings. 7. Links. Citation. All cited sources must be reflected in the bibliography. References in the text are written in brackets (only the author‟s surname and the year of publication). If the author is mentioned in the text, only the year is mentioned in brackets after his name. When the same author with the samo publication is referred to in the same paragraph, only his surname is mentioned, and the year is omitted. When citing two authors‟ sources, specify the names of both authors. When the number of authors is 3–5, for the first time all the authors are mentioned, and later only the first author‟s name is written and “et al.” (in the original language) is added. When the number of authors is six or more, both the first and other times please refer to the first author only and add “et al.” (in the original language). If the source author is an organization or there is no author, only the organization name (or the source name) and the year are mentioned. In case of citing various publications of the same author in the same year, letters a, b, c, etc. should be added to the year. T ARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS : inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika. 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) I. Plotka, D. Igonin, N. Blumenau. IMPLICIT ATTITUDES AND MEASUREMENTS: EFFECT OF CONTEXT 153 In case of exact citation of a work, please specify the source page. Teisingumo ministerijos. More information about the source quotes can be found at: http://www.library.uq.edu.au/training/ citation/apa.pdf Pettinger, R. (2002). Global Organizations. Oxford: Capston Publishing. Available from NetLibrary database. 8. List of references in alphabetical order is attached at the end of the paper. Citations are made in accordance with the APA (American Psychological Association) guidelines: http:// www.library.uq.edu.au/training/citation/apa.pdf Please first name sources in Latin and then in non-Latin (e.g., Russian, Bulgarian) scripts. Below you will find examples of the list of references. a) P e r i o d i c a l s Journal paper: Schafer, J. L. & Kang, J. (2008). Average causal effects from nonrandomized studies: A practical guide and simulated example. Psychological Methods, 13, 279–313. Unpublished paper: Kregţdė, S. (in press). Fundamentalios psichologinės atramos švietimo reformai ieškant. Electronic article: Shu-Cheng, Steve, Chi-Friedman, Raymond A. and Yang, Mei-Yu (2009). Are supervisors fair mediators? The effects of personality traits and age difference on expected mediation fairness. Social Behavior and Personality, 37 (1), 59–118. Retrieved from http:// www.swetswise.com/titleBank/ getAtoZList.do?title =187408 b) B o o k s a n d b o o k a r t i c l e s Book: Jovaiša, L. (2008). Profesinio konsultavimo psichologija. Vilnius: Agora. Book whose author is an organization or a group of people: Lietuvos statistikos departamentas (2010). Lietuvos statistikos metraštis. Vilnius: Informacijos–leidybos centras. Book with no author or editor: E-book: c) Conference and symposia proceedings Presentation when the paper is published in the official conference proceedings: Bohrer, S., Zielke, T. and Freiburg, V. (1995). Integrated obstacle detection framework for intelligent cruise control on motorways. Paper presented at IEEE Intelligent Vehicles Symposium. Detroit, MI: Piscataway. Presentation reported at a conference but not published: Bowden, F. J. and Fairley, C. K. (1996, June). Endemic STDs in the Northern Territory: Estimations of effective rates of partner change. Paper presented at the scientific meeting of the Royal Australian College of Physicians, Darwin. 9. Summary in English and in Lithuanian – half the printed page, presented on a separate sheet. It must mention the purpose of the study, a brief description of the methods, key results, with specific numbers and their statistical reliability, and the main findings. Appended at the end are three to five key words. The summary is printed under the main text. 10. Article originality. The authors are responsib-le for the originality of their papers and the accuracy of cited sources. 11. Copyright. Papers presented to International Business: Innovations, Psychology, Economics must be authentic and original works, previously unpublished and not submitted to another publication simultaneously. Authors, when submitting their manuscripts, attest that their work does not violate any existing copyright, and indemnify the rights of this journal from any copyright infringement. To enable the immediate and legal dissemination of a publication, scientific papers and manuscripts become the legal property of the publishers, in the absence of other conditions. Lietuvos Respublika. Konstitucija. (1996). Vilnius: Lietuvos informacijos centras prie We kindly invite to cooperate with our journal. INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS: Innovations, Psychology, Economics. 2016, Vol. 7, No. 2 (12) TARPTAUTINIS VERSLAS: inovacijos, psichologija, ekonomika 2016, t. 7, Nr. 2 (12) MOKSLO DARBAI Vyriausioji redaktorė Danguolė Beresnevičienė Uţ straipsnių turinį atsako autoriai. The authors are responsible for the matter in their papers. 7,08 aut. l., 9,75 sp. l. Išleido Kūrybiškumo ugdymo centras M. Pretorijaus g. 7–16, LT-06227 Vilnius Spausdino UAB “BMK leidykla” J. Jasinskio g. 16, Vilnius