Innovativeness, exploratory behavior, market mavenship, and
Transcription
Innovativeness, exploratory behavior, market mavenship, and
mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 703 Innovativeness, Exploratory Behavior, Market Mavenship, and Opinion Leadership: An Empirical Examination in the Asian Context Ayalla Ruvio and Aviv Shoham University of Haifa, Israel ABSTRACT The market maven construct, developed by Feick and Price (1987), has been used in empirical studies in the USA, South Africa, Germany, Poland, and Hungary. This study extends previous research by being the first to use the general mavenship concept in an Asian country (Israel). Furthermore, the study examines market mavenship and opinion leadership as outcome concepts arising from exploratory behavior or innovativeness tendencies. Additionally, the impact of a three-dimensional exploratory behavior concept is compared to the impact of a unidimensional innovativeness concept on opinion leadership and market mavenship. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used to test the two nomological models based on a sample of 142 adult Israeli consumers. Although both exploratory behavior and innovativeness affect market mavenship and opinion leadership, the impact of the former is stronger. Additionally, the impact of the “new brand trial” facet of exploratory behavior on market maven was comparable to that of innovativeness, whereas its impact on opinion leadership was weaker than the impact of innovativeness. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 24(8): 703–722 (August 2007) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/mar.20180 703 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 704 INTRODUCTION The concepts of opinion leadership and market mavenship have been important to scholars and practitioners mainly because of their central contributing role in describing patterns of adoption of innovations by innovative consumers in the marketplace. Innovative consumers are more likely to be opinion leaders and mavens than less innovative ones. Accordingly, both have been used in studies of innovativeness as behavioral outcomes of innovativeness, which is a personality-like concept. Opinion leaders are consumers who provide information and leadership to others in making their consumption decisions (Childers, 1986). Hence, opinion leadership can be defined as one’s behavioral tendency and ability to influence the purchase decisions of others. Given such a role, opinion leadership has been studied in marketing for many years (Flynn et al., 1996; King & Summers, 1970). The concept of market maven is a later addition to the consumer behavior literature. Feick and Price (1987, p. 85) defined market mavens (p. 85) as “individuals who have information about many kinds of products, places to shop, and other facets of markets, and initiate discussions with consumers and respond to requests from consumers for market information” and argued for market mavenship as expertise on multiple aspects of markets for many products and services. The study of market mavens as separate from opinion leaders is based on three considerations (Feick & Price, 1987). First, mavens might be more useful managerially when information regarding changes in the marketing mix is involved. For example, they might provide others with information about changed prices. In contrast, opinion leaders are important diffusers of information early in the life cycle of a new product or service. Second, opinion leaders tend to be product-specific, whereas mavens have more general market knowledge and can be targeted accordingly. Third, the motives underlying mavenship (obligation to and pleasure in sharing information and the desire to help others) differ from leaders’ motives (Price, Feick, & Guskey, 1995; Walsh, Gwinner, & Swanson, 2004). However, market mavenship and opinion leadership are related. Feick and Price’s domain-specific approach (1987), which is narrower than King and Summers’s broad, marketplace level conceptualization (1970), is followed here. Thus, “The definition of the market maven includes both general marketplace knowledge or expertise and influence . . . comparable to the definition of the opinion leader in that influence derives from knowledge and expertise, but differs in that the expertise in not product specific” (Feick & Price, 1987, p. 85). In other words, mavenship is more general than product- or category-specific opinion leadership. As will be discussed, this study makes several contributions. First, whereas Feick and Price (1987) included innovativeness in their model, this study also included a three-dimensional exploratory behavior concept (Raju, 1980; Stephens, 1991). Because both mavenship and opinion leadership play a role in the diffusion of innovations and of market 704 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 705 information, the use of exploratory behavior could provide a finer-grained assessment of its antecedent role than that accorded by the narrower conceptualization of innovativeness. Second, the impact of this threedimensional conceptualization of exploratory behavior on opinion leadership and market mavenship is compared to the impact of a unidimensional conceptualization of innovativeness on these outcomes. Third, previous studies have been conducted in the USA (e.g., Clark & Goldsmith, 2005; Feick & Price, 1987; Walsh, Gwinner, & Swanson, 2004), India (for industrial mavens; Nataraajan & Madhukar, 1997), South Africa (Abratt, Deon, & Nezer, 1995), Germany (consumer mavenship— Pechtl, 2003; Walsh & Mitchell, 2001; Wiedmann, Walsh, & Mitchell, 2001; e-Mavenship—Walsh, Mitchell, Wiedmann, Frenzel, & Duvenhorst, 2002), China and Korea (in a narrower, price mavenship context; Sternquist, Byun, & Jin, 2004), and Poland (Chelminski & Coulter, 2002; Moore, Kennedy, & Fairhurst, 2003). The present study tested the nomological network of market mavenship in a sample of consumers from a new country—Israel. Israel provides an interesting context for our study because it differs from countries studied in the past on a number of cultural dimensions, an issue revisited in a later section of this paper. Market mavenship and opinion leadership are discussed first, followed by a discussion of their relationships with innovativeness and exploratory behavior. Then, the study used to test these relationships is described and the findings reported. A discussion of the research and managerial implications of the findings concludes the paper. CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND AND RESEARCH HYPOTHESES Market Mavenship As noted, mavenship encompasses general market knowledge and potential influence on other consumers (Cal, 2004; Elliot & Warfield, 1993; Feick, Price, & Higie, 1986; Higie, Feick, & Price, 1987; Lichtenstein & Burton, 1990; Price, Feick, & Higie, 1987; Williams & Slama, 1995). According to Feick and Price (1987), market mavens are experts on many products and couple such expertise with influence. Thus, market mavens are similar to opinion leaders in that their influence is based on knowledge and expertise; yet, they differ in being experts on many products, making mavenship more general than the product- or category-specific opinion leadership. Mavens have been described and profiled in numerous countries and are discussed later (for a recent summary, see Walsh, Gwinner, & Swanson, 2004). Demographics. Research established that they are more likely to be blacks, females, younger, and less educated (Feick & Price, 1987; Higie, Feick, & Price, 1987; Williams & Slama, 1995). However, others failed to MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 705 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 706 find some of these differences (USA—Slama & Williams, 1990; Germany— Wiedmann, Walsh, & Mitchell, 2001) or identified South African and German males as more likely to be opinion leaders (Abratt, Nel, & Nezer, 1995 and Walsh et al., 2002, respectively). Teen mavens were identified as influencers of US family decision making (Belch, Krentler, & Willis-Flurry, 2005). Finally, female market mavens were found to be heavier readers of females’ magazines, direct mail ads (Higie, Feick, & Price, 1987), and grocery ads (Price, Feick, & Guskey-Federouch, 1988). Purchasing Behavior. Mavens are likely to be innovative, have higher media exposure, be, at times, opinion leaders, and perceive themselves as smart buyers (Feick & Price, 1987; Slama, Nataarajan, & Williams, 1992). They use shopping lists and coupons more frequently and budget their shopping to a larger extent than others (Price, Feick, & GuskeyFederouch, 1988). Additionally, they are mavens over a variety of products (Slama & Williams, 1990). Moore, Kennedy, and Fairhurst (2003) compared U.S. and Polish consumers and found them to differ vis-à-vis the role of price mavenship. Finally, Pechtl (2003) reported that mavenship does not affect online grocery shopping adoption for German consumers. Personality. Compared to other consumers, market mavens tend to score higher on extraversion and openness, but not on the other three “big-five” factors (conscientiousness, neuroticism, and agreeableness; Mooradian, 1996). Other personality variables contributing to market mavenship include higher self-esteem, higher conformity tendency, higher need for uniqueness, and higher susceptibility to interpersonal influence (Clark & Goldsmith, 2005). Like Feick and Price’s seminal work (1987), the focus here is on consumer innovativeness and exploratory behavior, which have been central concepts in previous studies on opinion leadership and market mavenship. These concepts will be reviewed subsequently. Opinion Leadership Like leaders in any domain, consumer opinion leaders are individuals to whom others look for information and leadership in making consumption decisions (Childers 1986; Feick & Price, 1987). Opinion leadership refers to individuals’ tendency to influence the purchase decisions of others (King & Summers, 1970). “Opinion leadership happens when individuals try to influence the purchasing behavior of other consumers” (Flynn et al., 1996, p. 138), making it situation-specific. Opinion leaders have an impact on others’ attitudes and actions (Arndt, 1967, 1968; Gatignon & Robertson, 1985; King & Summers, 1970; Rogers, 1983). Importantly, unlike the more general trait of mavenship, some scholars believe that opinion leadership varies by product and that no generalized opinion leadership trait exists (King & Summers, 1970; Langeard, Crousillat, & Weisz, 1977; Myers & Robertson, 1972). However, others 706 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 707 view opinion leaders as category- (i.e., cosmetics) rather than productspecific (i.e., lipsticks; Darden & Reynolds, 1972; Merton, 1957; Myers & Robertson, 1972; Shoham, Rose, & Kahle, 1997). Regardless, even if it is category-specific, it is narrower in scope than mavenship. Thus, the approach used here to be consistent with both approaches is to assess opinion leadership for products on which respondents consider themselves to be leaders (see the later section on study scales). Opinion leaders are technically competent, consume mass media heavily (Rogers, 1983, p. 27; Summers, 1970), and are socially active (Baumgarten, 1975; Venkatraman, 1989). They are conscious of their appearance, selfcentered, and self-confident (Baumgarten, 1975; Summers, 1970), and exhibit public individuism (Chan & Misra, 1990). Feick and Price (1987) argued theoretically and documented empirically that opinion leadership and mavenship are related, but distinct constructs (see also Clark & Goldsmith, 2005). Their argument was based on the narrower scope of the former compared to the latter (see also Coulter, Feick, & Price, 2002). Consequently, both were used in this study as outcomes of consumer innovativeness and exploratory behavior, which are reviewed next. Opinion Leadership, Market Mavenship, Exploratory Behavior, and Innovativeness Consumer innovativeness has been a central construct in studies dealing with the diffusion of innovations, often combined with opinion leadership and market mavenship. Most previous research assigned consumers to one of five adopter classes—innovators, early adopters, early and late majority, and laggards (Goldsmith & Hofacker, 1991; Rogers, 1983). This classification is based on an innovativeness personality trait. Specifically, high-innovativeness consumers tend to be the first or early purchasers of new products in any product category, such as consumer electronics, or domain, such as high-tech products (Assael, 1992: 426–451; Schiffman & Kanuk, 2004: 480–523). Previous research has documented a positive relationship between innovativeness and opinion leadership. Goldsmith and Hofacker (1991) reported that the correlations coefficients between consumer innovativeness and opinion leadership were 0.78 and 0.80 for records and fashion, respectively. Similarly, Flynn et al. (1996) reported a correlation coefficient of 0.65 in their study of clothing fashion products. Similar findings have been obtained for the innovativeness-market mavenship relationship. Although Feick and Price reported weaker significant correlation coefficients (0.14 to 0.34), Goldsmith, Flynn, and Goldsmith (2003) reported almost identical relationships between market mavenship/opinion leadership and innovativeness (0.44 and 0.46, respectively). Exploratory behavior and innovativeness are related but distinct constructs (Baumgartner & Steenkamp, 1996). Raju (1980) viewed MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 707 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 708 exploratory behavior as a tendency, aimed to change environmental stimuli. He recognized seven facets of exploratory consumer behavior, namely, repetitive behavior proneness, innovativeness, risk-taking, exploration through shopping, interpersonal communication, brand switching, and information seeking. However, a later empirical study (Stephens, 1991) identified only three of the seven as valid and reliable, namely, new brand trial propensity (corresponding to brand-switching), informationseeking (identical to the original), and cautiousness (corresponding to risk-taking). New brand trial involves switching brands primarily for change or variety. Information-seeking refers to showing interest in knowing about various product and brands mainly out of curiosity. Finally, cautiousness is a preference for avoiding risks or adventures. Scholars have documented the relationships between components or manifestations of opinion leadership/mavenship and exploratory consumer behavior. For example, adoption of new products and retail stores, which are required for becoming opinion leaders or mavens, includes strong exploratory components (Baumgartner & Steenkamp, 1996; Vankatmaran & Price, 1990). Similarly, curiosity-based acquisition of information (i.e., reading catalogs and discussing purchases with other consumers—manifestations of opinion leadership and mavenship) also contains exploratory tendencies (Price & Ridgway, 1982). Finally, varietyseeking and information-seeking, characteristics of leaders and mavens, are related with exploratory consumer behavior (Baumgartner & Steenkamp, 1996). Importantly, it is not implied here that the [{innovativeness; exploratory behavior}—{opinion leadership; market mavenship}] relationship is causal. Moreover, a cross-sectional study such as the present investigation cannot test such a relationship. Yet, consumer innovativeness and exploratory behavior are seen as behavioral tendencies and opinion leadership and market mavenship as behavioral. Hence, the former are used as explanatory variables. In sum: H1: Consumer innovativeness and exploratory behavior are related positively with market mavenship. H2: Consumer innovativeness and exploratory behavior are related positively with opinion leadership. As was noted, a three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct was used in this research (new brand trial, information-seeking, and cautiousness/risk-taking: Raju, 1980; Stephens, 1991). This contrasts with previous unidimensional approaches to operationalizing innovativeness (Goldsmith & Hofacker, 1991). Goldsmith and Hofacker’s general innovativeness scale (1991) appears to be conceptually and operationally close to the “new brand trial” dimension of exploratory behavior used by Stephens (1991). Thus, this study was designed to answer the following research question (RQ): 708 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 709 Model A New Brand Trial Market Maveship Information Seeking Opinion Leadership Risk-Taking Model B Market Maveship Generalized Innovativeness Opinion Leadership Figure 1. Market Mavenship and Opinion Leadership Nomological Models. RQ: Are the impacts of innovativeness and exploratory behavior comparable? Is the impact of the more specific “new brand trial” facet of exploratory behavior on mavenship and opinion leadership comparable to that of innovativeness? The models designed to test H1–2 and answer RQ include the threedimensional exploratory behavior (Model A) and the generalized consumer innovativeness (Model B) as determinants of market mavenship and opinion leadership. Figure 1 presents the two models. METHOD Sample Data were gathered through a structured questionnaire. Two experienced student teams were instructed thoroughly in research methodology before collecting the data in Northern Israel. The two teams selected a total of MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 709 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 710 300 Israeli consumers in a randomized fashion in shopping and community centers, of whom 142 provided complete questionnaires (44%). Respondents were 44% males and 56% females and were 37 years old, on average. As for education, 26% had up to high school education, another 49% had some college or an undergraduate degree, and 25.4% had postgraduate studies. Consistent with the education distribution, income (coded at three levels: below average, average, and above average) was skewed to the higher level (16%, 32%, and 52%, respectively). The limitations of the sample will be discussed in a later section of the paper. Questionnaire and Scales The questionnaire included previously used and validated scales. All the scales used Likert-type items. First, it included the 6-item market mavenship (Feick & Price, 1987). In support of the scale’s reliability, Feick and Price (1987) reported an alpha of 0.82 and item-to-total correlations between 0.48 and 0.65. The scale was reliable in this research as well (a 5 0.89). Second, it included a modified version of the 6-item scale used by Flynn et al. (1996) to measure opinion leadership. Recall that existing research has viewed opinion leadership as product-specific (King & Summers, 1970; Langeard, Crousillat, & Weisz, 1977; Myers & Robertson, 1972), whereas others have viewed opinion leaders as category-specific (Darden & Reynolds, 1972; Merton, 1957; Myers & Robertson, 1972; Shoham, Rose, & Kahle, 1997). Hence, this study used an operationalization consistent with both approaches and respondents were asked to evaluate their opinion leadership for products they considered themselves to be leaders on. This approach is similar in spirit to the scale as used by Goldsmith (1991) and by Goldsmith, Flynn, and Goldsmith (2003). Consequently, the items were modified by introducing them with “Think of a product you consider yourself an expert on. To what extent do you agree/disagree that . . . ” This was done to enable respondents to refer to a product they see themselves as experts on without having to specify a given product. Flynn et al. (1996) reported that the scale used in their research exhibited acceptable reliability (a 5 0.86). The reliability in our research was also acceptable (a 5 0.81). Third, it included the 24 items used by Stephens (1991; based on Raju, 1980) to operationalize three facets of consumer exploratory behavior. Eight items were used to operationalize each facet of consumer exploratory behavior (new brand trial, information seeking, and cautiousness). New brand trial, information-seeking, and cautiousness had a coefficients of 0.76, 0.73, and 0.75, respectively, in Stephens’s study (1991). The alphas were 0.80, 0.88, and 0.82 in the present study. Finally, a general innovativeness scale (six items) was included (Goldsmith & Hofacker, 1991; Goldsmith, et al., 2003). The scale’s reliability 710 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 711 exceeded 0.80 in the original set of studies. In this study, reliability was acceptable as well (a 5 0.72). Because the original scales were developed in English, one bilingual individual translated them into Hebrew. A second bilingual individual, blind to the originals, back-translated the items into English. Then, a third bilingual individual, in consultation with the other translators, assessed the translation. Inconsistencies were discussed and resolved during this stage. This procedure ensured the cultural and language equivalency of the scales used. FINDINGS Discriminant Validity of Market Mavenship and Opinion Leadership Before testing the measurement models and the research hypotheses, the discriminant validity of the scales for market mavenship and opinion leadership were tested. First, an exploratory factor analysis (with varimax rotation) on the 12 items of the market maven and opinion leadership scales was performed. Three factors were extracted, which explained 69% of the variance. The six market maven items loaded on one factor and the six opinion leadership items were split into two three-item factors corresponding to the positively and negatively phrased items from the original. However, the scree plot and theory suggest two factors. Therefore, another factor analysis was used specifying a two-factor solution. The results of this factor analysis, which explained 59% of the variance, separated the items to two six-item factors corresponding to market mavenship and opinion leadership with loadings # 0.56. The analysis was also rerun using oblique rotation, as opinion leadership and mavenship are related constructs. The results were substantively identical and both are reported in Table 1. Second, the items comprising each scale were averaged to form two scales and a correlation coefficient and a confidence interval were calculated for the two. The correlation was of medium strength (0.49) and the 99% confidence interval (0.31–0.64) did not include the value of 1.00. Thus, the discriminant validity of the two outcome constructs is supported. Additionally, the correlation is similar in magnitude to that reported by Goldsmith, Flynn, and Goldsmith (0.46; 2003). Measurement Models Then, Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used to analyze the data. Following Anderson and Gerbing (1988), a two-step approach was used. Two measurement models (discussed below) were tested first followed by tests of the structural models. The results of these models are described subsequently. MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 711 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 712 Table 1. Market Maven and Opinion Leadership Items—Rotated Factor Solution.1 Market maven Items MM1 MM2 MM3 MM4 MM5 MM6 OL1 OL2 OL3 OL4 OL5 OL6 Opinion leadership Orthogonal rotation Oblique rotation 0.67 0.78 0.82 0.76 0.81 0.80 0.69 0.79 0.82 0.79 0.80 0.83 Orthogonal rotation Oblique rotation 0.73 0.81 0.69 0.58 0.57 0.64 0.75 0.83 0.74 0.54 0.50 0.58 1 Only loading above 0.50 are shown. The measurement models assessed whether the items corresponding to every scale represented the same latent variable. A measurement model with three indicators per latent variable is ideal; however, use of up to five indicators does not complicate the estimation of the model (Bagozzi & Heatherton, 1994). When there are more than five items measuring a given construct (as is the case for the constructs in this study), a parceling procedure can be used (Bagozzi & Heatherton, 1994). Such a procedure randomly combines items into composites. Although reducing random errors and simplifying the model, parceling simultaneously maintains the concept of multiple indicator measurement. Hence, parceling procedures were used for the constructs discussed above. The items for each were randomly parceled into three composites indicators. These indicators entered the measurement model as multiple indicators to estimate their respective latent variables. The measurement models had acceptable fit measures. Specifically, fit measures for Model A included a x2/degrees of freedom ration of 2.36 (x2 5 188.78, df 5 80; p # 0.05), well below the recommended level of 5.0 (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998). Similarly, NFI (0.97), RFI (0.95) and CFI (0.98) were above the recommended 0.90 level, whereas RMSEA was below the recommended 0.10 level (0.09). Fit measures for Model B included a x2/degrees of freedom ration of 2.62 (x2 5 62.85, df 5 24; p # 0.05), below the 5.0 recommended level, NFI (0.98), RFI (0.97), and CFI (0.99) above the recommended 0.90 level, and RMSEA was at the recommended level of 0.10. Thus, the results of the substantive models were examined next. 712 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 713 Hypotheses Testing Notably, the correlation coefficients (see Appendix) provided initial partial support to the three hypotheses. However, H1 and H2 were tested in SEM models with market mavenship and opinion leadership as dependent variables and three facets of consumer exploratory behavior (Model A) or the general innovativeness scale (Model B) as independent variables. The two models were analyzed without and with a correlation between mavenship and leadership. Because the results are substantively identical, only the latter are reported. The results are presented in Table 2. Both models fit the data well. Specifically, with a x2 of 267.40 (df 5 83; p # 0.01), Model A’s x2/df (3.22) was below the 5.0 recommended levels. Model A’s NFI (0.95), RFI (0.93), and CFI (0.97) were above the recommended 0.90 level, whereas RMSEA (0.13) was slightly above the 0.10 recommended level. Similarly, with a x2 of 65.85 (df 5 24; p # 0.015), Model B’s x2/df (2.62) was lower than the 5.0 recommended level. Model B’s NFI (0.98), RFI (0.96), and CFI (0.99) were above the recommended 0.90 level, whereas RMSEA (0.11) was slightly above the 0.10 recommended level. Model A resulted in R2 of 0.36 and 0.24 for market mavenship and opinion leadership, respectively. Model B resulted in R2 of 0.23 and 0.54 for market mavenship and opinion leadership, respectively. Thus, the individual impacts of the three exploratory behavior dimensions (Model A) and the general innovativeness construct (Model B) on mavenship and leadership were examined. Regarding H1, in line with the previous research reviewed earlier, all three facets of consumer exploratory behavior (Model A) and the generalized innovativeness construct (Model B) are related with market mavenship positively and significantly ( p # 0.01). In Model A, new brand trial Table 2. SEM Standardized Results. Market mavenship Dependent variable Independent variable–Model A New brand trial Information-Seeking Risk-Taking R2 x2 (df, p-value); x2/df; NFI; RFI; CFI Independent variable–Model B Generalized innovativeness scale R2 x2 (df, p-value); x2 / df; NFI; RFI; CFI g p# 0.42 0.36 0.22 0.36 0.01 0.01 0.01 g p# 0.46 0.01 0.10 0.29 20.13 0.19 0.24 267.40 (83, p # 0.05; 3.22; 0.95; 0.93; 0.97) 0.48 0.23 Opinion leadership 0.01 0.74 0.01 0.54 62.85 (24, p # 0.05; 2.62; 0.98; 0.96; 0.99) MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 713 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 714 (g 5 0.42), information-seeking (g 5 0.36), and risk-taking (recoded from cautiousness; g 5 0.22) all affected market mavenship positively as expected. The impact of the generalized innovativeness construct on mavenship was also positive (g 5 0.48) and significant (p # 0.01). In sum, H1 was strongly supported. The data provide some support for H2. In Model A, the new brand trial facet of consumer innovativeness was a significant predictor of opinion leadership (g 5 0.46) whereas information seeking (g 5 0.10) and risk taking (recoded from cautiousness; g 5 20.13) were not statistically significant ( p $ 0.10). The impact of the general innovativeness construct (Model B) on opinion leadership was positive (g 5 0.74) and significant ( p # 0.01), as expected. Thus, H2 was partially supported. In sum, the two research hypotheses were mostly substantiated by the data. The scales used to operationalize opinion leadership and market mavenship were related positively but exhibited discriminant validity. Consistent with H1–2, the three facets of consumer exploratory behavior and the general innovativeness construct were related positively with market mavenship. New brand trial (but not information-seeking or risktaking) and the general innovativeness construct were related with opinion leadership. DISCUSSION AND FUTURE RESEARCH Discussion The findings reconfirmed the important role of the market maven construct and reestablished its distinction from the related concept of opinion leadership (Feick & Price, 1987). Each captures a different phenomenon. This is important in that it increases the confidence in using this construct in future research. Hence, scholars should consider including both scales in future research in relevant contexts. However, as will be discussed below, more research is needed in additional countries and cultures. The findings for Model A, which included a three-dimensional conceptualization of consumer exploratory behavior, are discussed first. The findings concerning the relationships of the three exploratory behavior dimensions with market mavenship and opinion leadership are illuminating. Specifically, it appears that for individuals to be opinion leaders, they need to be users of new products/brands, the dimension related to such leadership. In contrast, for individuals to be market mavens, they need to be users of new brands (like opinion leaders), but also seek information and be risk-takers. These differences would have been masked if only innovativeness would have been used. The discussion on Model B, which included an established generalized scale to assess consumer innovativeness, allows further elaboration 714 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 715 on this issue. Recall that a research question was raised about the use of innovativeness and exploratory behavior. RQ asked if a multidimensional exploratory behavior construct would explain more of the variance in market mavenship and opinion leadership than a unidimensional innovation construct. The data provide a mixed set of findings. Specifically, the three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct explained 41% of the variance in mavenship compared to 29% for the innovation construct. However, the innovation construct explained 59% of the variance in opinion leadership compared to 30% for the three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct. Thus, it appears that opinion leadership’s relationship with the innovativeness construct is stronger than with a three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct (new brand trial, information, and cautiousness). In explaining this pattern, recall that consumer opinion leaders are individuals to whom others look for leadership before making consumption decisions (Childers, 1986; Feick & Price, 1987). Opinion leadership is commonly perceived as individuals’ influence over the purchase decisions of others (King & Summers, 1970). Thus, it is exhibited through active attempts to influence the purchasing behavior of others (Flynn, Goldsmith, & Eastman, 1996). The conceptualization of a generalized innovativeness trait is more closely aligned with opinion leadership, compared to the three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct, which includes an information component (which might not be shared with others) and cautiousness (which is personal and internal). In parallel, mavenship includes general market knowledge and potential influence on other consumers (Feick, Price, & Higie, 1986; Higie, Feick, & Price, 1987; Price, Feick, & Higie, 1987; Williams & Slama, 1995). Feick and Price (1987) argued that the concept of market maven includes a general marketplace expertise and influence that is not product specific. Because mavenship is more general that opinion leadership, which tends to be product- or category-specific, the stronger impact of the three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct is not surprising. Additionally, it was argued earlier that the general innovativeness scale appears to be conceptually and operationally close to the “new brand trial” dimension of the three-dimensional exploratory behavior construct. This led us to ask if the impacts of the general innovativeness and the more specific “new brand trial” facet of exploratory behavior on mavenship and opinion leadership would be comparable. The data suggest that these impacts are closer for mavenship (gnew brand trial 5 0.42 and ggeneralized innovativeness 5 0.48), than for opinion leadership (gnew brand trial 5 0.46 and ggeneralized innovativeness 5 0.74). Thus, it appears that innovativeness and exploratory behavior exert similar impacts on mavenship but innovativeness exerts a stronger impact on opinion leadership than the impact of the “new brand trial” component of exploratory behavior. Because the innovation scale is more parsimonious (six items) than the three-dimensional exploratory behavior scale (24 items), the use of either in future research should be based on the goal of such research. Beyond MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 715 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 716 the need to fit the measures to the model used, there is a tradeoff between parsimony (and questionnaire length) and between explained variance of market mavenship. Earlier, the importance of cross-cultural validation of mavenship nomological networks was discussed. As was noted, previous research has been conducted in the USA, South Africa, Germany, Hungary, and Poland, to which, the present Israeli study added a sixth country in a new continent. The evidence for the nomological model is important when cultural differences are accounted for. Hofstede’s work on cultural values (2001) serves to highlight that mavenship’s nomological network appears to be universal, irrespective of the positions countries capture along his five dimensions. Unfortunately, he did not report data on Poland or Hungary and we use the data on Yugoslavia as a proxy (all were communist at the time Hofstede collected his data). Regarding power distance, the USA (40), South Africa (49), and Germany (35) appear to capture low-medium positions on the continuum. Israel (13), in contrast, is a very low powerdistance country and Yugoslavia (76; proxy for Poland and Hungary) is a fairly high power-distance country. Findings substantiating mavenship nomological networks in the face of these differences are important. Hofstede reported (2001) that leadership is more important in low powerdistance cultures. Thus, the role of mavenship (and opinion leadership) should be more pronounced in Israel, and less so in Yugoslavia (proxy for Poland and Hungary), compared to other countries. Ranking these countries on uncertainty avoidance puts Yugoslavia (88, proxy for Poland and Hungary) as the highest, followed by Israel (81), Germany (65), South Africa (49), and the USA (46). Being able to generalize at such diverse levels is important in that Hofstede reported that low uncertaintyavoidance cultures are less resistant to change, making the impact of the “new brand trial” facet of exploratory behavior on mavenship (and opinion leadership) potentially stronger in such countries. As for individualism, the USA is highest (91), followed by Germany (67), South Africa (65), Israel (54), and Yugoslavia (proxy for Poland and Hungary, 27). Here, too, these differences are important in light of Hostede’s findings that low individualism cultures emphasize group decisions and tend to be conservative, potentially reducing the impact on mavenship (and opinion leadership). Finally, South Africa and Germany were the most masculine (66), followed by the USA (62), Israel (47), and Yugoslavia (proxy for Poland and Hungary, 21). Such differences could potentially impact the role of mavenship (and opinion leadership) because low masculinity cultures tend to favor joint decision making, potentially reducing their impacts. In sum, innovativeness/exploratory behavior → mavenship/opinion leadership studies have been conducted in several countries. Finding that these relationships hold in these countries and in Israel, in the face of the many cultural differences across country settings discussed earlier, provides a strong measure of confidence in the generalizability of the nomological models used here. 716 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 717 Future Research Here, future research directions that could incorporate mavenship and opinion leadership are discussed. One promising direction is studying both in the context of power bases. Of the power bases discussed by French and Raven (expert, referent, reward, legitimate, and coercive; 1959), opinion leaders presumably have expert and referent power. Opinion leaders’ expert power derives from unique product- or categoryspecific knowledge and mavens’ expert power should be more general. It would be illuminating to examine if both use expert power and how they use it. Speculatively, both probably use it, but it is based on different information sources and scope. Similarly, although both probably have referent power, it should be more important for leadership than for mavenship. Future research should examine additional personality behavioral characteristics that might impact opinion leading and mavenship. For example, Feick and Price (1987) argued that product involvement in a category is a predominant explanation for opinion leadership, whereas mavenship should be predominantly explained by involvement in the more general marketplace—stores, discounts, coupons, and changes in marketing mix variables. Future research should develop leader- and maven-involvement scales. The impact of a general market involvement scale should be stronger for mavens than for leaders and the impact of a product-specific involvement scale should be stronger for opinion leaders than for mavens. Another direction for future research involves studying the impacts of cultural values on mavenship and opinion leadership. Two possible directions can be followed. First, in way of cross-cultural assessments, future research can address the extent to which people from different cultures are socialized to value individualism and leadership, thus influencing the manner they seek to become leaders/mavens. Hofstede’s work (2001) suggests that some countries are more oriented to individualism (versus collectivism) than other countries. As a country, Israel scored close to the mid-point of the collectivism-individualism continuum. In contrast, many South American countries (e.g., Guatemala, Ecuador, and Panama) are much more collectivistic and many Western countries are much more individualistic (e.g., Australia, and the United Kingdom; Hofstede, 2001) compared to Israel and to other nations where mavenship research has been conducted. Intuitively, consumers in individualistic countries should exhibit stronger opinion leadership and mavenship tendencies, on average, than in collectivistic countries. Thus, simultaneous studies in multiple countries are needed. A second venue is highlighted by two issues discussed earlier. First, Yugoslavia’s scores on the four cultural dimensions were used as a proxy for previous mavenship studies in ex-communist countries (Poland and Hungary). Further research is needed to develop country-level assessments MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 717 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 718 for these dimensions for nations not in Hofstede’s study (2001). Additionally, especially in the face of the proxy used, the four countries where studies have been conducted tend to be low–medium on power distance, medium–high on uncertainty avoidance, medium–high in individualism, and medium on masculinity. Future studies can expand the list of countries to include nations that diverge from this fourdimensional profile. MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS Several managerial implications arise from the findings, which are especially relevant for positioning. When firms need to target opinion leaders early in a product life cycle, they should emphasize the new brand trial facet of exploratory behavior and deemphasize the information seeking and risk taking facets. The relevant scale points out potential creative ideas. For example, “when I eat out, I like to try the most unusual items the restaurant serves, even if I am not sure I would like them” suggests unusualness as a possible advertising theme. Conversely, “when I see a new or different brand on the shelf, I often pick it up just to see what it is like” raises the possibility of “pick it up! Try it on!” advertising theme. In contrast, when there is a need to target the more general market mavens, all three facets of exploratory behavior can be used to advantage, including new brand trial (as earlier), information-seeking and risk-taking. For example, the information seeking item of “I like to shop around and look at displays” suggests a “window shopping” advertising theme. In some cases, recognizing partial overlap of the two groups and an ensuing need to target both, managers should use separate advertising along the lines discussed earlier to reach mavens and opinion leaders effectively. Because separate advertising campaigns might be financially infeasible for some firms, the safest theme to use would be that of new brand trial, which was documented in both models reported here. LIMITATIONS This research has several limitations suggesting caution in using the findings. The findings are based on a convenience sample of Israeli consumers. This raises two generalizability issues. First, would the findings reported here hold for the general Israeli population? Future research is needed to replicate the findings using representative samples of Israelis. Second, would the findings presented here generalize to other countries? Answering this question requires replications of this study with consumers from other nations. As noted earlier, that is especially important in culturally dissimilar countries with extreme scores on individualism or collectivism. 718 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 719 REFERENCES Abratt, R., Deon, N., & Nezer, C. (1995). Role of the market maven in retailing: A general marketplace influencer. Journal of Business and Psychology, 10, 31–56. Anderson J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411–423. Arndt, J. (1967). The role of product-related conversations in the diffusion of a new product. Journal of Marketing Research, 4, 291–295. Arndt, J. (1968). A test of the two-step flow in diffusion of a new product. Journalism Quarterly, 45, 657–665. Assael, H. (1992). Consumer behavior and marketing action, Boston: PWS-Kent. Bagozzi, R. P., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). A general approach to representing multifaceted personality constructs: Application to state self-esteem. Structural Equations Modeling, 1, 35–67. Baumgarten, S. A. (1975). The innovative communicator in the diffusion research. Journal of Marketing Research, 12, 12–18. Baumgartner, H., & Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (1996). Exploratory consumer buying behavior: Conceptualization and measurement. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 13, 121–137. Belch, M. A., Krentler, K. A., & Willis-Flurry, L. A. (2005). Teen internet mavens: Influence in family decision making. Journal of Business Research, 58, 569–575. Cal, Y. R. (2004). The multicultural market maven: A market influencer for a diverse society. In P. Rose (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2004 Conference of the AAA (pp. 201–207). Miami, FL: AAA. Chan, K. K., & Misra, S. (1990). Characteristics of the opinion leader: A new dimension. Journal of Advertising, 19, 53–60. Chelminski, P., & Coulter, P. A. (2002). Examining polish market mavens and their attitudes toward advertising. Journal of East-West Business, 8, 77–89. Childers, T. L. (1986). Assessment of the psychometric properties of an opinion leadership scale. Journal of Marketing Research, 23, 184–188. Clark, R. A., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2005). Market mavens: Psychological influences. Psychology & Marketing, 22, 289–312. Coulter, R. A., Feick, L. F., & Price, L. L. (2002). Changing faces: Cosmetics opinion leadership among women in the new Hungary. European Journal of Marketing, 36, 1287–1308. Darden, W. R., & Reynolds, F. D. (1972). Predicting opinion leadership for men’s apparel fashions. Journal of Marketing Research, 1, 324–328. Elliot, M. T., & Warfield, A. E. (1993). Do market mavens categorize brands differently? In L. McAlister & M. Rothschild (Eds.), Advances in Consumer Research (Vol. 20, pp. 202–208). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Feick, L. F., & Price, L. L. (1987). The market maven: A diffuser of market information. Journal of Marketing, 51, 83–97. Feick, L. F., Price, L. L., & Higie, R. A. (1986). People who use people: The other side of opinion leadership,” In R. J. Lutz (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research (Vol. 13, pp. 301–305). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Flynn, L. R., Goldsmith, R. E., & Eastman, J. K. (1996). Opinion leaders and opinion seekers: Two new measurement scales. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 24, 137–147. MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 719 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 720 French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Gatignon, H., & Robertson, T. S. (1985). A propositional inventory for new diffusion research. Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 849–867. Goldsmith, R. E. (1991). The validity of a scale to measure global innovativeness. Journal of Applied Business Research, 7, 89–97. Goldsmith, R. E., Flynn, L. R., & Goldsmith, E. B. (2003). Innovative consumers and market mavens. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 11, 54–64. Goldsmith, R. E., & Hofacker, C. F. (1991). Measuring consumer innovativeness. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 19, 209–221. Hair, J. F., Jr., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis. Upper River Saddle, NJ: Prentice Hall. Higie, R. A., Feick, L. F., & Price, L. L. (1987). Types and amount of word-of-mouth communications about retailers. Journal of Retailing, 63, 260–278. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s Consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. King, C. W., & Summers, J. O. (1970). Overlap of opinion leadership across consumer product categories. Journal of Marketing Research, 7, 43–50. Langeard, E., Crousillat, M., & Weisz, R. (1977). Exposure to cultural activities and opinion leadership. In H. Keith Hunt (Ed.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 5, pp. 606–610). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research. Lichtenstein, D. R., & Burton, S. (1990). An assessment of the moderating effects of market mavenship and value consciousness on price-quality perception accuracy. In M. E. Goldberg, G. Gorn, & R. W. Pollay (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 17, pp. 53–59). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Merton, R. (1957). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press. Mooradian, T. A. (1996). The five factor model and market mavenship. In K. P. Corfman & J. G. Lynch, Jr., (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 23, pp. 26–33). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Moore, M., Kennedy, K. M., & Fairhurst, A. (2003). Cross-cultural equivalence of price perceptions between US and Polish consumers. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 31, 268–279. Myers, J. H., & Robertson, T. S. (1972). Dimensions of opinion leadership. Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 41–46. Nataraajan, R., & Madhukar, G. A. (1997). A quest for the “industrial maven.” Industrial Marketing Management, 26, 352–362. Pechtl, H. (2003). Adoption of online shopping by German grocery shoppers. The International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, 13, 145–159. Price, L. L., Feick, L. F., & Guskey-Federouch, A. (1988). Couponing behaviors of the market maven: Profile of a super couponer. In M. J. Houston (Ed.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 15, pp. 354–359). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Price, L. L., Feick, L. F., & Guskey, A. (1995). Everyday market helping behavior. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 14, 225–266. Price, L. L., Feick, L. F., & Higie, R. A. (1987). Information sensitive consumers and market information. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 21, 328–341. Price, L. L., & Ridgway, N. M. (1982). Use innovativeness, vicarious exploration and purchase exploration: three facets of consumer varied behavior. In B. J. Walker et al., (Eds.), 1982 educators’ conference proceedings (pp. 56–60). Chicago: American Marketing Association. 720 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 721 Raju, P. S. (1980). Optimum stimulation level: Its relationship to personality, demographics, and exploratory behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 272–282. Rogers, E. M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations. New York: The Free Press. Schiffman, L. G., & Kanuk, L. L. (2004). Consumer behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. Shoham, A., Rose, G. M., & Kahle, L. R. (1997). Opinion leadership and self-concept: A product-type examination. In B. G. Englis & A. Olofsson (Eds.), European advances in consumer research (Vol. 3, pp. 204–210). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Slama, M., Nataraajan, R., & Williams, T. G. (1992). Market mavens and the relationship between smart buying and information provision: An exploratory study. In V. L. Crittenden (Ed.), Developments in marketing science (Vol. 15, pp. 90–93). Chestnut Hill, MA: Academy of Marketing Science. Slama, M., & Williams T. G. (1990). Generalization of the market maven information provision tendency across product categories. In M. E. Goldberg, G. Gorn, & R. W. Pollay (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 17, pp. 48–52). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. Stephens, N. (1991). Cognitive age: A useful concept for advertising? Journal of Advertising, 20, 37–48. Sternquist, B., Byun, S.-E., & Jin, B. (2003). The dimensionality of price perceptions: A cross-cultural comparison of Asian consumers. International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, 14, 83–100. Summers, J. O. (1970). The identity of women’s clothing fashion opinion leaders. Journal of Marketing Research, 7, 178–85. Venkatraman, M. P. (1989). Opinion leaders, adopters, and communication adopters: A role analysis. Psychology & Marketing, 6, 51–68. Venkatraman, M. P., & Price, L. L. (1990). Differentiating between cognitive and sensory innovativeness: Concepts, measurement, and implications. Journal of Business Research, 20, 293–315. Walsh, G., & Mitchell, V.-W. (2001). German market mavens’ decision-making styles. Journal of EuroMarketing, 10, 83–108. Walsh, G., Mitchell, V.-W., Wiedmann, K.-P., Frenzel, T., & Duvenhorst, C. (2002). German emavens on internet music sites. In W. J. Kahoe & J. H. Lindgren (Eds.), Proceedings: Enhancing knowledge development in marketing, AMA 2002 summer educators’ conference (Vol. 13, pp. 435–436). Chicago: AMA. Walsh, G., Gwinner, K. P., & Swanson, S. R. (2004). What makes mavens tick? Exploring the motives of market mavens’ initiation of information diffusion. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 21, 109–122. Wiedmann, K. -P., Walsh, G., & Mitchell, V. -W. (2001). The German manmaven: An agent for diffusing market information. Journal of Marketing Communications, 7, 195–212. Williams, T. G., & Slama, M. E. (1995). Market mavens’ purchase decision evaluative criteria: Implications for brand and store promotion efforts. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 12, 4–21. Note: The order of authors was determined alphabetically. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to: Aviv Shoham, Graduate School of Management, University of Haifa, Haifa 31905, Israel, Tel:+972 4 8249580 Fax: +972 4 8249194, e-mail: ashoham@research. haifa.ac.il. MARKET MAVENSHIP AND OPINION LEADERSHIP Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar 721 mar248_899_20180.qxd 6/13/07 9:17 AM Page 722 APPENDIX Correlation Coefficients. 1. Market Mavenship 2. Opinion Leadership 3. Generalized Innovativeness 4. New Brand Trial 5. Information-Seeking 6. Risk-Taking 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. .49** .37** .39** .50** 2.03 .54** .45** .31** 2.28** .53** .20** 2.35** .46** 2.51** 2.17* * Significant at p , .05 ** Significant at p , .001 722 RUVIO AND SHOHAM Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar