Blackmores What Women Want Case Study
Transcription
Blackmores What Women Want Case Study
Case Study What Women Want Fairfax Media - Blackmores CAMPAIGN PERIOD: SEPTEMBER 2012 - APRIL 2013 OBJECTIVES TARGET AUDIENCE STRATEGY Build a stronger connection Health aware individuals The notion of ‘more’ Promote Blackmores’ superior brand quality and heritage, so customers aren’t swayed by cheaper brands. Women who are determined, seek more from their mind and body, and are aware of the positive benefits vitamins and minerals have on their health. To help position Blackmores as a more visionary company, the ‘What Women Want’ campaign was created with the notion that women want more. MORE involvement in relevant environments, MORE engaged conversations, MORE consistency of messaging. Drive brand awareness and consideration of Caltrate Vitamin D. Case Study What Women Want Campaign With Sunday Life’s positioning of BODY. MIND. INSPIRATION, together with Daily Life’s strong online presence, both brands provided the perfect vehicles to promote Blackmores’ visionary positioning. The campaign consisted of three elements: a survey, bespoke content and a reader event. 01 ‘What Women Want’ Survey A special survey was run on Daily Life aiming to discover everything there is to know about women. What makes the modern Australian woman tick? What motivates her? Makes her happy? Sad? How do Australian women feel about the future? balance WHAT DO WOMEN WANT? The survey ran for 4 weeks across desktop and m-site platforms, and attracted 1524 respondents. 1HERSA1 A012 12 THURSDAY, SEPTEMBER 6, 2012 The Sydney Morning Herald smh.com.au WELLBEING The hormone that makes us good or evil In pursuit of unhappiness Satisfaction and growth are preferable to a fleeting happiness, writes Frank Bures. W Paul Zak calls oxytocin the ‘‘moral molecule’’. He tells Oliver Burkeman how hugging, massage and watching soppy movies could make us better people. Fairfax Media - Blackmores T he American academic Paul Zak is renowned among his colleagues for two things he does to people disconcertingly soon after meeting them. The first is hugging: seeing me approach across the library of his club, in midtown Manhattan, he springs to his feet, ignoring my outstretched hand, and enfolds me in his arms. The second is sticking needles in their arms to draw blood. I escape our encounter unpunctured, but plenty of people don’t: Zak’s work, which he refers to as ‘‘vampire studies’’, has involved extracting blood from a bride and groom on their wedding day; from people who have just had massages, or been dancing; from Quakers, before and after their silent worship; and from tribal warriors in Papua New Guinea as they prepare for traditional rituals. That all these people submit so willingly to his needle may have something to do with the fact that he is charm personified. A squarejawed, 50-year-old Californian with good hair, a sunny disposition and a mediafriendly nickname (‘‘Dr Love’’), Zak gives every impression of having been constructed in a laboratory charged with creating the ideal author of a new buzz book – The Moral Molecule. What drives Zak’s hunger for human blood is his interest in the hormone oxytocin, about which he has become one of the world’s most prominent experts. Oxytocin, long known as a female reproductive hormone – it plays a central role in childbirth and breastfeeding – emerges from Zak’s research as something much more allembracing: the ‘‘moral molecule’’ behind all human virtue, trust, affection and love, ‘‘a social glue’’, as he puts it, ‘‘that keeps society together’’. The subtitle of his book, ‘‘The new science of what makes us good or evil’’, gives a sense of the scale of his ambition, which involves nothing less than explaining whole swaths of philosophical and religious questions by reference to a single chemical in the bloodstream. Being treated decently, it turns out, causes people’s oxytocin levels to rise, which in turn prompts them to behave more decently, while experimental subjects given an artificial oxytocin boost – by means of an inhaler – behave more generously and trustingly. And it’s not solely because of its effects on humans that oxytocin is known as ‘‘the cuddle hormone’’: for example, male meadow voles, normally roguishly promiscuous in their interactions with female meadow voles, become passionately monogamous when their oxytocin levels are raised in the lab. The aforementioned wedding took place at a country house in south-west England where Zak set up a temporary research station. He took blood samples, before and after the wedding vows from the bride and groom, close family members and various friends in attendance then flew his spoils – 156 test tubes packed in dry ice – to his laboratory at Claremont University, in southern California. There he discovered the results he had been expecting: the ceremony caused oxytocin to rise in the guests. And it did so in spookily subtle ways: the bride recorded the highest increase, followed by close family members, then less closely involved friends, ‘‘in direct proportion to the likely intensity of emotional engagement in the event’’. (Only the groom bucked the trend: testosterone interferes with oxytocin, and his testosterone was surging.) Mapping the wedding’s oxytocin levels gave rise, in Zak’s vivid phrase, to a human ‘‘solar system’’ with the bride as the sun, the hormone finely calibrated to the emotional warmth each guest felt. ‘‘It was amazing,’’ Zak recalls. ‘‘Just this perfect sense of how oxytocin attunes to the environment.’’ The starting point was a persistent mystery in Zak’s original field, economics: time and again in experiments people behave more generously than traditional economic models predict they should. A classic demonstration of this is known as the Trust Game, in which These findings have striking implications for how we think about morality. Economists tend to pride themselves on being hardheaded realists: morality might be a nice set of ideas about how people ought to behave, this way of thinking goes, but economics is the analysis of how they really behave, motivated not by stirring ethical values but by the desire for personal gain. Perhaps ironically, religions tend to share a similar view: that He recommends a minimum eight hugs a day. Even soppy movies seem to work: he has done the blood tests. Being treated decently, it turns out, causes people’s oxytocin levels to rise. pairs of participants communicate with each other via computer terminals: they never meet and have no idea who the other person is. Person A is given £10 ($15) then invited to send a portion of it electronically to person B. Person A has a motive for doing so: according to the rules, which both players know about, any money A sends to B will triple in value, whereupon B will have the option of sending some of it back as a thank-you. According to conventional notions of rational behaviour, the game should break down before it has begun. Person B, acting selfishly, has no reason to give any money back – and, knowing this, person A shouldn’t send any over in the first place. Yet, in trials of the game, 90 per cent of A-people send money and 95 per cent of B-people send some back. Analysis of the Interactions on Twitter and Facebook seem to lead to oxytocin surges. oxytocin in their bloodstreams reveals what is going on: by sending money to person B, person A is giving a sign of trust – and being on the receiving end of a sign of trust, it emerges, causes oxytocin to increase, motivating more generous behaviour in return. And it is not just receiving money that causes people to feel oxytocin’s warm glow: in other studies Zak has conducted, random windfalls don’t cause nearly so much of it to be released. What counts is being trusted: trust in one person triggers oxytocin in the other, which triggers more trustworthy behaviour, and so on, in a virtuous circle. ‘‘Well, that’s except for the 5 per cent of people who are ‘unconditional non-reciprocators’,’’ says Zak, referring to the consistent minority of people who seem immune to this cycle. ‘‘What we call them in my lab is ‘bastards’.’’ moral conduct doesn’t come naturally but instead needs to be imposed through fear or the promise of reward. Zak himself was raised in a staunch Catholic household: his mother, he likes to say, took him out of Catholic school because it wasn’t strict enough and ‘‘based her childrearing on the assumption that unselfish, moral behaviour was impossible without the ever-present threat of punishment, the more terrifying the better’’. Yet the fact that natural selection has given us oxytocin – a mechanism that allows us to be instinctively trusting and kind – suggests that what most of us think of as ‘‘moral’’ is, in fact, part of how we have evolved to be. ‘‘Human beings are almost the only animals who regularly want to be around strange members of our species,’’ Zak says. ‘‘ It’s fun. But to be able to do that, we have to have something in our heads that says: ‘Oliver is safe, Bob is not safe.’ And that’s oxytocin – this very old, evolutionarily ancient molecule’’ that helps us respond to being trusted with just the right degree of reciprocal trust in response. Zak’s earlier work had established that trust is a crucial precondition for economic prosperity (to conduct transactions, you have to be able to trust others) but also a result of it (once you’re no longer fighting for basic subsistence, you can afford to trust more). Now, he had located the biological mechanism through which this all worked. The Golden Rule – treat others as you’d like to be treated – is, Zak writes, ‘‘a lesson that the body already knows’’. From that follows ... well, everything. ‘‘To me, this is the basis for civilisation: a bunch of strangers living together,’’ he says. ‘‘And, once you have civilisation, you can have specialisation of labour; you can have surplus; you can have university professors, and priests, because now you can afford that, and then you get the advancement of knowledge.’’ This talk of mixing science and morality prompts suspicion in some quarters: just because something is ‘‘natural’’ doesn’t mean it is ‘‘right’’, in an which moral action takes place, that holds out the possibility – a cause of either optimism or alarm, depending on how you look at it – that by manipulating oxytocin we might boost the levels of trust, generosity and ultimately happiness in ourselves and the world at large. It took Zak two years of wrangling with the US Food and Drug Administration and university ethics panels to gain approval to use oxytocin inhalers on experimental subjects. (In the meantime, he got around the restriction by experimenting on himself under the watchful eye of his wife, a neurologist.) But while the red tape was convoluted, the conclusions were not: in exercises such as the Trust Game, oxytocin-loaded participants displayed much greater levels of trust and generosity than those who used inhalers filled with a placebo. All of which would seem to raise some troubling questions: what is to stop car dealers, say, pumping oxytocin into showrooms, or politicians using it when canvassing? (A company called Vero Labs already markets an oxytocin spray it calls ‘‘Liquid Trust’’, aimed both at salespeople and single men on the prowl.) But Zak waves the matter away: it is incredibly hard to get enough oxytocin into the bloodstream, which is why he has to get his subjects to force such large amounts of vapour up their noses; using it covertly would never work. Sure, oxytocin can be stimulated in subtle ways to serve other people’s agendas, ‘‘but they’re already doing that. Why do you think they have cute puppies in toilet paper commercials? To make you feel good.’’ Meanwhile, Zak says, we should all be doing more to boost oxytocin in benign ways. He recommends a minimum of eight hugs a day (pets count, too); massage and even soppy movies seem to work: he has done the blood tests. Interactions on Twitter and Facebook seem to lead to oxytocin surges, offering a powerful retort to the argument that social media is killing real human interaction: in hormonal terms, it appears, the body processes it as an entirely real kind of interaction. Ultimately, one imagines, the oxytocin-savvy citizens of Zak’s of utopia would focus on charity work and community groups, play with their pets and watch romantic comedies. They would be touchy-feely and hug each other all the time – which makes you wonder whether in the modern world the prescriptions of Dr Love might be a bit of a lost cause. Guardian News & Media It helps us respond to being trusted with just the right degree of reciprocal trust. ethical sense, and efforts to derive codes of moral conduct from science rarely end well. Moreover, it is unclear what Zak means when he says oxytocin, or the lack of it, ‘‘makes’’ us good or evil. This is the same problem as with news reports about scientists discovering the part of the brain ‘‘responsible for’’ risk-taking, or greed, or a belief in God: just because you have found the biological underpinnings of some phenomenon, it does not necessarily follow that you have found ‘‘the real cause’’ of it. Still, none of that undermines the most potent aspect of Zak’s work, which is the pragmatic one. If oxytocin is the mechanism through balance At Blackmores we believe in living a balanced life for happiness, health and wellbeing. For tools, tips and personalised advice visit blackmores.com.au or find us on 80 years young and many 1HERSA1 A012 to come hen the Dostoevsky subway station opened in Moscow in 2010, there was concern that the grim scenes from Dostoevsky’s novels, artistically depicted on the grey marble walls (Crime and Punishment’s Raskolnikov about to murder the old woman with an axe, the troubled protagonist of Demons holding a gun to his head), were so depressing that people, overwhelmed by the bleakness, would start throwing themselves onto the tracks. Fortunately, the feared rash of suicides has not materialised. This could be because people do not make those kinds of decisions based on subway art. Or it could be because Russians have a different attitude about happiness than most Westerners. One recent study shows they tend to have darker, more negative thoughts. They also worry less about those feelings and thus experience fewer depressive symptoms than Americans. Russians might brood more, but they don’t dwell that much on their brooding. Americans, on the other hand, brood about their worrying and end up more depressed than the Russians. It is, perhaps, a simple fact of life in the West: we expect to be happy. The right to pursue happiness is part of America’s Declaration of Independence, after all. The feeling has been heightened by the booming field of ‘‘happiness studies’’, which has produced a flow of news stories and books about what will and will not make us happy, about the happiest places to live, and about how to structure our lives so we can be happy almost all the time. Some important findings have emerged. Too many choices lead to dissatisfaction. Chronic pain has a more negative impact than a single accident. We habituate quickly to our acquisitions. A good marriage is worth about $100,000 a year in terms of how happy it makes us. But this headlong rush towards happiness might backfire. Could our constant worrying about why we are not happy be making us more miserable than if we simply accepted some occasional unhappiness as part of life? In viewing unhappiness as a problem to be solved, might we not miss what a little sadness has to offer us? Are we trading long-term satisfaction for feeling good now? Buying our present-day enjoyment at the cost of future meaning? Children are a case in point. Recently, there has been a spate of news stories stating that having children does not increase one’s happiness by any objective measure. Indeed, marital satisfaction declines steeply when kids are born and does not recover until they leave. Having two young children, I can more or less confirm this is true. Yet despite all the headaches, sleep deprivation, stained furniture and general crabbiness, few parents I know regret their decision. One study even found that mothers between 36 to 44 were less likely to be depressed than their childless peers, even if, at a given point on a given day, they might be less happy. Raising kids is not nonstop fun. But eventually (we hope) we’ll be better for it. This jibes with the conclusions of some earlier research. In the 1980s, psychologist Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi found that people did not report their most satisfying experiences as relaxing on the beach, or going to parties, or buying that car they had always wanted. The happy moments came when they were working hard at something, moving toward some goal they had, being challenged and absorbed and focused. A more recent investigation from the Journal of Happiness Studies also found that people who were working hard to accomplish something felt more stress in the moment but were happier in the long term. Suffering is no fun and we usually try to avoid it. But it is also inevitable. Not so long ago, when life was less certain and comfortable, people understood that suffering could be an opportunity to rise to a challenge. They were willing to at least try to extract some meaning from it. When I lived abroad, I went through what’s known as culture shock – a series of mood swings that occur as you learn to function in another culture. As you find ways to cope, you become a new person – someone stronger, more capable, more aware. As you struggle with culture shock, or any of life’s difficulties, something remarkable happens: you grow. For a while, I did not understand this. I could not fathom why I had developed an odd nostalgia for what I remembered as a hard and unhappy time. But that was precisely the time when I stopped being the person I had been and started becoming the person I am. Now, when I recall those unhappy days, I think of them as the best days of my life.The As you find ways to cope, you become a new person. Rotarian Support was provided by ‘What Women Want’ call-outs, promoting survey participation, coupled with Blackmores advertising within Sunday Life throughout the duration of the campaign. Case Study Bespoke content and creative 02 04 Sunday Life Covers and Editorial ‘What Women Want’ Sunday Life - Special Edition Unique Sunday Life covers were designed using Blackmores’ green brand colouring, together with special Editor’s Letters and engaging ‘What I know about..’ editorial pieces. These highlighted personal experiences, views and opinions of the opposite sex, by Ben Folds, Marjorie Bligh, Nick Earls and John Taylor. The results of the ‘What Women Want’ survey were published in a special Sunday Life edition on Nov 18th 2012. The special issue, in partnership with Blackmores, was filled with loads of wellbeing content relevant to the findings of the survey. 03 Fairfax Media - Blackmores Wellbeing Section To continue engagement, a new wellbeing page was created specifically for Blackmores within The Sydney Morning Herald and The Age. Inspired by the ‘What Women Want’ campaign, this regular column discussed relevant topics covering the body, mind and soul. Case Study 06 Reader Event Smashing Results 05 Female’s likelihood to recommend Blackmores Caltrate Vitamin D shifted significantly: +16% pts ‘What Women Want’ Reader Event Impressive Results. Purchase Intention +9% pts A special event was held at Sydney’s The Ivy on Nov 12th 2012, hosted by Jessica Rowe and sponsored by Blackmores, attracting over 100 Sunday Life and Daily Life readers. Brand Favourability +9% pts Brand Recommendation +7% pts Brand Consideration +6% pts An invitation was promoted within Sunday Life and on Daily Life, to encourage readers to attend. Fairfax Media - Blackmores Guest speakers on the panel included; Jacinta Tynan, Jane Caro, Kerri Sackville, Tara Moss, Emily Maguire and Paula Joye, who discussed body and health, sex and relationships, family, career, money and more. “ Exposure to the campaign in The Age, The Sydney Morning Herald and Sunday Life magazine helped Blackmores maintain its top ranking in the competitive set. ” FOR FURTHER INFORMATION PLEASE CONTACT Anna Lenart on (02) 9282 1193