1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations
Transcription
1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations
J. J. Universität Paderborn Fakultät für Kulturwissenschaften Institut für Medienwissenschaften Diplomarbeit in englischer Sprache: A critical Exploration of Web Usability Guidelines and Design Principles with respect to the special Needs of Elders Vorgelegt am 17. Juli 2006 von Kai Alexander Lohr∗ bei Prof. Dr. rer. nat. Gerd Szwillus ∗ Breslauer Straße 5, D-45739 Oer-Erkenschwick, Telefon: (0 23 68) 69 38 87; Matrikelnummer: 6108400 Eidesstattliche Erklärung Ich erkläre hiermit an Eides Statt, dass ich die vorliegende Arbeit selbständig und ohne Benutzung anderer als der angegebenen Hilfsmittel angefertigt habe; die aus fremden Quellen direkt oder indirekt übernommenen Gedanken sind ausnahmslos als solche kenntlich gemacht. Die Arbeit wurde bisher weder im Inland noch im Ausland in gleicher oder ähnlicher Form einer anderen Prüfungsbehörde vorgelegt und auch noch nicht veröffentlicht. (Kai Alexander Lohr) Ort, Datum ii Contents 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations 1.1 Age-related Terminology . . . . . . . . . . 1.1.1 Normal Ageing . . . . . . . . . . . 1.1.2 Elders and Seniors . . . . . . . . . 1.2 On Guidelines and Principles . . . . . . . 1.3 Aspects Addressed in this Work . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 5 6 . . . . . . . . . . 7 7 12 13 13 14 16 16 16 18 20 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind 3.1 Sensory Processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.1.1 Skin Senses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.1.2 Hearing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.1.3 Vision . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.2 Cognitive Processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.2.1 Reaction Times and Rate of Information Processing 3.2.2 Attention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 23 23 25 26 30 31 33 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? 2.1 The Digital Divide and how Seniors use the Internet . . 2.2 Elements of Innovation Diffusion Theory . . . . . . . . 2.2.1 Pre-Adoption Stages: Knowledge and Persuasion 2.2.2 Adopter Characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.3 Adoptee Characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.4 Other Theories of Technology Acceptance . . . 2.3 Possible Age Slope Explanations . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.3.1 Adoptee Perspective . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.3.2 Adopter Perspective . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4 Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Contents 3.3 3.4 3.2.3 Memory Development and Implications for 3.2.4 Notes on Intelligence and Problem Solving Mobility and Fine Motor Skills . . . . . . . . . . Summary and Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . Learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 Web Usability Recommendations 4.1 Facilitate Perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.1.1 Text Formatting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.1.2 Colours and Graphics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.1.3 Audio- and Multi-Media Content . . . . . . . . 4.2 Facilitate Handling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2.1 Scrolling versus Paging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2.2 ›Seek-and-Hide‹ Menus . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2.3 Click Targets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2.4 Input Elements and Keyboard Use . . . . . . . 4.3 Reduce the Likelihood of Distraction . . . . . . . . . . 4.3.1 Advertisements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.3.2 Ornamentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.3.3 Disruptive Navigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.4 Keep Users in Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.4.1 External Applications, Downloads, and Plug-Ins 4.4.2 Pop-Ups and New Browser Windows . . . . . . 4.5 Reduce Complexity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.5.1 Text and Language . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.5.2 Site Structure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.5.3 Page Layout . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.5.4 Help and Contact . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.6 Provide a Sound Base for Navigation Decisions . . . . . 4.6.1 Navigation Paradigms and Tools . . . . . . . . 4.6.2 Link Labelling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.7 Examples from the Web . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.7.1 News: spiegel.de . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.7.2 On-line Pharmacy: docmorris.de . . . . . . . . . iv . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 37 40 41 44 44 45 46 47 48 48 50 51 52 53 53 55 55 56 56 57 58 58 60 62 63 64 64 68 70 70 72 Contents 4.7.3 4.7.4 Travel: bad-sassendorf.de . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Public Authority: kreis-recklinghausen.de . . . . . . 73 74 5 Final Remarks 5.1 Conflicts and Inspirations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2 Ageing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.3 Technological Innovation as a Social Process . . . . . . . . 77 77 79 79 Appendix 80 v List of Figures 2.1 2.2 Internet usage for 2004 in selected European Union countries by age. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Use of selected Internet services in Germany by age-groups 4.1 Economy section on the Web site of Spiegel Online . 4.2 Home page of the DocMorris Internet pharmacy . . . 4.3 Third-level page from the Web site of Bad Sassendorf 4.4 Home page of the German district Recklinghausen . . vi . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 11 71 72 74 75 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations In many western countries, a generational shift is currently in progress. The large generations born directly after the Second World War are approaching retirement and a continuous decline in birthrates has caused the traditional age pyramid of population to morph into a rhombus-like shape. The portion of the populous commonly referred to as ›the elderly‹ or ›older adults‹ has been growing in size, relative to the rest, and is likely to continue to do so (see, e. g.: destatis 2003)1 . Various businesses begin to acknowledge this shift by marketing products and services that also address the requirements of the new consumer group. Originally available and of interest only to scientists, the World Wide Web, hereafter referred to as the ›Web‹, is now being used by millions of people around the world. The Internet and the Web as its user interface increasingly permeate every-day life. Media and communication technology are becoming an integral part of products and services in many fields of application. They also act as business and distribution channels and in these capacities, their use can result in considerable cost savings. The increasing reliance on communication technology in transactions of various kinds mandates that the technology itself as well as the necessary skills and knowledge to use it be available to all. Otherwise, individuals without access to these prerequisites would be excluded from social, economic, and eventually even from political participation. Practises exist to ensure that technological appliances are safe and easy to use for as many individuals as possible. The Internet and the Web are no exception. The tensions between Internet and the Web on one side and the phenomenon of ageing on the other mark the overall area of interest for this work. 1 Statistisches Bundesamt, Wiesbaden; henceforth referred to as destatis. 1 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations 1.1 Age-related Terminology To establish a clear understanding of the concept of ageing, a few notes on terminology are inevitable. This is especially true as quite different interpretations of the term old exist. The following considerations lead up to the definition of two terms, ›seniors‹ and ›elders‹, which shall find use as a means to address the intended “target group”. Expressions of age “borderlines” or ranges with arbitrarily drawn age intervals, typically in the guise of terms like ›Generation-n+‹, frequently appear in common discourse. The group of the 14–49 year-olds is a suitable example. As far as media planning and advertising are concerned, it is the most important consumer group (see: Bieri et al. 2006, p. 19). Retirement by law currently occurs at age 65 in Germany, but only recently, the government agreed to raise the retirement age for younger generations gradually to 67. Backes & Clemens (2003, p. 23) hold that actual retirement age, averaging around age 60 in Germany, traditionally is one of the most formal age borderlines, but different social systems may impose others, some of them uncannily early. The age of de-facto retirement may be such a case, depending on the line of work. As far as the modern day labour market in Germany is concerned, employees aged 40 years and over, depending on their education, might well be considered old and quite unlikely to find employment again, should they loose their jobs (see: Gourmelon 1994, p. 6). For research purposes, categorisations based on chronological age are a common occurrence. Spirduso (1995, p. 7) gives an insightful summarising comment regarding categories of “oldness” : »Reviewing several research studies will show that subjects ranging in age from 35 to 100 have been labelled ‘old’.« She goes on to say that there are no established standards for age categories. Solely utilising chronological age as a determinant, her proposal yields four different categories: Young-old, Old, Old-old, and Oldest old (see also Table 1.1). Backes & Clemens (2003, p. 26) similarly see the need to subdivide what was formerly the single phase of old age, but choose to do so based on functional abilities: Those still able to provide or care for someone other 2 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations Table 1.1: Chronological age categories (abbrev. from Spirduso 1995, p. 8) Age (years) 25–44 45–64 65–74 75–84 85–99 100+ Term Adult Middle-aged Adult Young-old Old Old-old Oldest-old than themselves and to generally function normally make up the group of the young old, those self-sufficient but unable to provide or care for another person are the old, and finally those, who require assistance in day-to-day living are the old-old. Note that these categories not necessarily conform to the age ranges given in Table 1.1, although they bear similar names. 1.1.1 Normal Ageing In order to obtain a way to fitfully restrict the “target group” in this examination, it is imperative to establish a concept of normal ageing effects in contrast to pathologically induced effects. For this purpose, Spirduso (1995, pp. 6–7) offers a useful set of terms: primary ageing encompasses such processes as occur naturally and are independent of environmental factors (e. g. injuries sustained, man-made health hazards endured, or other forms of abuse) and diseases, whereas secondary ageing includes the interactions produced between the aforementioned external factors and primary ageing. Smoking and obesity are good examples of environmental factors. Both are commonly considered risk factors for stroke (disease), which in turn might hasten the natural deterioration of certain physical and cognitive abilities. Directly defining normal ageing as primary ageing is immensely impractical, however, as some amount of secondary ageing cannot be avoided. Some extend of disease, injury, or abuse has to be incorporated into normal ageing. Goldstein & Ritter (1997, p. 528) employ a differentiation between impairments and disabilities to describe different manifestations of 3 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations reduced hearing. The spirit of their interpretation can serve in this broader context as well: by this definition of normal ageing, a normally aged individual may experience impairments, that is to say hindrances or nuisances, but no disabilities, which are understood as conditions prohibiting complete self-reliance. This is not meant as a means to discriminate against persons with disabilities. The rather pointed wording is necessary to enable a clear differentiation between usability enhancements benefiting older Internet users and special requirements involving non-standard computer interaction methods, which are beyond the scope of this document. 1.1.2 Elders and Seniors Simply stating a floor and possibly a ceiling age to define the actual subject group of this examination would be the wrong approach. Different systems within a single body may, depending on the individual secondary ageing history, age at a different rate: »An individual can be 75 years old chronologically but have a 65-year-old heart and a 85 year-old gastrointestinalsystem« (Zarit & Zarit 1987, p. 22). Similar differences show in studies that evaluate the performance of older subjects: result plots show a broader deviation. This definition instead approaches from the functional perspective: ›elders‹, or the adjective ›elder‹ henceforth describes persons (1) not suffering from a disability that prevents them from interacting with a computer in the “normal” way by using a display, a mouse or mouse-like device, and a keyboard, and who (2) experience some age-related deficiencies as part of normal ageing, such as those discussed in chapter 3. Thereby, chronological age is removed as a dominating facet of the definition and the only relation to it is that impairments of any kind tend to show after a certain chronological age. The material reviewed in chapter 2 on Internet usage varies in fineness of the applied age intervals and also in which age-group is the oldest. As Backes & Clemens point out, retirement usually marks the transition to a phase of life that is completely different from the phases preceeding it. For example, it no longer revolves mainly around work or the upbringing of chil- 4 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations dren and may allow the pursuit of other interests. Owing to achievements in health care, it is likely to go on for quite some time before life moves on to the terminal phase with death at the end. Thus, the term ›seniors‹ or the adjective ›senior‹ describes elder individuals who have retired. 1.2 On Guidelines and Principles The title of this document includes the terms ›usability guidelines‹ and ›design principles‹. In a definition made by Shneiderman & Plaisant (2005, pp. 61/66), each of these terms describes distinctly different concepts: guidelines have evolved as best practice or advice on specific problems encountered in a field of interface design, with their scope limited to this particular field and often with little or no relevance outside its original domain; design principles, on the other hand, are more general in scope and are applicable to multiple domains, but only after context-specific interpretation. Shneiderman & Plaisant (2005, pp. 74–75) give as examples the »8 golden rules of interface design«, but Nielsen’s (1993, pp. 115f) 10 usability heuristics also fit the above definition of principles; indeed, there is considerable overlap between these two examples. So sharp a distinction is not always possible. Indeed, the two terms mentioned above occasionally even find synonymous use. The material reviewed in the course of this work was mostly labelled as guidelines, yet in many cases the wording is sufficiently vague so as to be of little direct use without interpretation in every individual case of application. Some guidelines come to resemble design principles in the sense of the above definition. Consider as example Kurniawan & Zaphiris (2005, p. 131): »Screen Layout, navigation and terminology used should be simple, clear and consistent«. The question arises precisely what simple and clear means in each of the mentioned contexts and also with regard to elders. Consistency is a design principle on its very own (see: Nielsen 1993, p. 132; Shneiderman & Plaisant 2005, p. 75), applicable to several different aspects of user interfaces (e. g. action sequences, colouration, terminology). Some guidelines, or parts thereof, cross over from or to other media or fields of interface design 5 1 Introduction and Preliminary Considerations to address rather specific problems and thereby demonstrate flexibility in the other extreme. 1.3 Aspects Addressed in this Work Until recently, the typical Internet user was young, male, well educated, and probably quite successful in socio-economic respects. Considered next to the introductory comments on the changes in the age structure of many western societies, this raises interesting questions with respect to the influence of age on the likelihood of technology use. The examination in chapter 2 investigates these age influences in the case of Internet usage and attempts to provide possible explanations for some of them. It establishes at least that scepticism against new technology is caused not exclusively by increasing chronological age. It also provides helpful background information on Internet usage in different age groups. The effects of ageing on the human body and mind, leastways those effects relevant for computer use without special equipment, are the central topic in chapter 3. The discussion therein presents information on the agerelated changes in physical and mental human properties. Some of the changes, particularly those in vision, are relatively well documented and readily observable. Others, particularly those of a higher cognitive nature, are more obscure and less conclusive, owing to the fact that they tend to be harder to assess. Nevertheless, knowledge about these developments forms the basis for the consideration of special age-related Web design needs and in some cases, it can point to possible strategies to address these needs. Giving condensed advice on Web design and usability is the central objective of chapter 4. The recommendations made therein base on observations made in chapter 2 and chapter 3 and on an exploration of ageingrelated and general Web usability materials. Comments and extensions supplement the findings. These elements then aggregate into six basic and relatively specific recommendations, which allow a comparatively direct application to Web design issues. In their wording, they attempt to reflect both ageing- and Web-specific problem fields. 6 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? This chapter explores what some may regard as a contradiction: Senior individuals and their relationship to the Internet. In a first step, this chapter will assess the existence and nature of an age-related Digital Divide. It will also explore usage preferences of senior Internet users. It will then present elements of the Innovation Diffusion Theory (abbreviated: IDT; Rogers 2003) as a backdrop for the discussion of possible explanations for the age-related digital divide in the final section. 2.1 The Digital Divide and how Seniors use the Internet According to Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia (2006b, § 1)1 , an American technology adviser to the Clinton administration coined the term »Digital Divide« to denote ». . . the socio-economic difference between communities in their access to computers and the Internet«, which has subsequently been employed to name differences both between countries and social groups within countries. The focus, initially on infrastructure issues such as access to cheap and reliable connections, moved towards differences in individual abilities and usage competence (see: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia 2006b, § 3). The term »“gray [!] gap”« appears in a report from Fox (2001, p. 2) to describe disproportionally low Internet usage among US citizens above a certain age, compared to other age-groups. To assess the nature of such a usage gap, it is necessary to review more recent material on Internet usage with regard to the United States and the European Union on a global level and with particular attention to the situation in Germany. 1 In cited electronic documents, the structure apparent from headings or paragraphs will be used for reference, should other means like link anchors (designated by ), pagination, or numbered headings be unavailable. m 7 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? 100 90 80 % of population 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 16 to 24 Germany 25 to 34 35 to 44 45 to 54 55 to 64 Age group Italy 65 to 74 16 to 74 Sweden Source: Demunter (2005a, Table 2 on p. 3) Note: Only the points represent actual data. The lines are given for slope illustration only. Figure 2.1: Internet usage for 2004 in selected European Union countries by age. For the United States, illustrations (see: Fox 2001, p. 4; Fox 2005, p. 1) show a “descending stair” across the age axis and a pronounced drop of Internet usage between the 50–64 and 65-and-older age-groups. This general appearance seems to be equally present in the European theatre (see Figure 2.1): Internet usage decreases gradually along the age axis and a pronounced drop occurs between the 45–54 and 55–64 age-group. The aforementioned figure shows data for only three EU nations; still it exemplifies that even among those countries that were members of the European Union before its eastward enlargement, remarkable differences in Internet usage continue to exist. It is also not too surprising to see that Figure 2.1 shows a much more benign development for Sweden. The survey 8 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? data for other Nordic countries was similar in this respect, and, in terms of average usage, Sweden, Denmark, and Finland rank best in that order (see: Demunter 2005a, p. 3). Also note the particularly sharp drop-off for Germany in the 55–64 age-group, which is much larger than in the aforementioned Nordic countries. When referring to the apparent decrementing effect that age has on the likelihood of Internet usage, it is convenient to name the continuous manifestation the ›age slope‹ and the sharp drop occurring between two age-groups the ›age gap‹. Across nations and generations, there are also considerable differences in how individuals use the Internet. For the sake of scope, this section will discuss the issue of Internet usage preferences for the United States and Germany only. Apart from e-mail, which is the Internet service unquestionably used most frequently by senior users (see: Fox & Madden 2005, p. 1), Fox identify some key applications that enjoy popularity of roughly similar proportions across the age-groups: retrieving health-related, religious, or financial information, playing on-line games, utilising stock trading facilities, and making an Internet-based phone call (see: Fox 2004, p. 10). Regarding other on-line transactions such as buying products, making travel arrangements, or on-line banking, Fox (2004, p. 8) observes that US seniors show a considerable reluctance to conduct them on-line, while using the Internet for researching them prior to actually performing them off-line does not face so large a constraint. For most other research activities, the report lists usage figures that are below those of the younger age-group, but it identifies one topic that is of more interest to its senior age-group of 65 years and older: genealogy and family research (see: Fox 2004, p. 10). With regard to more recent figures from Fox & Madden (2005, p. 3), the notion of lower usage figures for older Internet participants generally preserves— at least for those seniors behind the age gap at 70 years and older (see: Fox 2005, p. 2). The difference may in some cases, however, reflect more structural changes: seniors are less likely to conduct school research online, for they themselves do not have to attend formal educational facilities any more. A similar rationale applies to using the Internet to search for a job, which can be expected to be less important for pensioners. Still, 9 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? there are numerous services used by 50 % or more of the senior Internet users and Fox & Madden’s (2005, p. 3) figures show that to the group labelled as »mature« (aged 60–69), usage rates in activities such as product research, news reading, banking, making travel arrangements, and even purchasing goods and services do not stray too far from the average. Interestingly, finding health-related information on the Web appears to be especially important to quinquagenarian Internet users and figures for the older sub-groups are “only” around the average. Concerning more recently popularised applications such as instant messaging, downloading games, videos, or music, or participating in on-line auctions, findings continue to suggest that senior Internet users are markedly below the average in these usage categories. Fox (2001, p. 10), however, relates this effect to users’ on-line experience. With higher-aged users less likely to be Internet users, owing to the overall age slope, the number of experienced users in older age-groups is probably lower than in younger age-groups and thus may contribute to this manifestation. The available data from Eurostat2 and the German destatis is unfortunately less detailed in terms of Internet activities. The examination of the relevant material for Germany yields e-mail and the rather generalised activity of ›searching for information on products and services‹ as the most important Internet applications. The data from Eurostat shows e-mail to be more important for the youngest (16–24) and oldest (65–74) age-groups (see: Figure 2.2), whereas in the more granular destatis data, e-mail and information searching were equally important for the oldest group of 55 years and older (see: destatis 2006, pp. 31–32). Aside from these differences, the cause of which are most likely the different age categories in the survey data, usage patterns for these two activities do not differ too much across the age-groups and are reasonably close to the average. Searching for on-line services and information related to travel, in contrast, does show a difference in frequency of use in Germany, as Demunter (2005b, Table 3, p. 3) witnesses: senior users are more likely to search for travel2 Statistical Office of the European Communities, Luxembourg; henceforth referred to as Eurostat. 10 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? 90 % of Internet users 80 70 60 50 40 30 16 to 24 25 to 34 35 to 44 45 to 54 55 to 64 Age group 65 to 74 16 to 74 Send/receive e-mail Seek product/service information Use/buy travel services* Seek information from public authorities Use financial services Order goods/services** Source: aggregated from Eurostat (2006) data for 2004, except where indicated by * (data for 2004 from Demunter 2005b, Table 3 on p. 3) and by ** (data for 2005 from Eurostat 2006). Note: Only the points represent actual data. The lines are given for slope illustration only. Figure 2.2: Use of selected Internet services in Germany by age-groups related information and services than are other age-groups. The material from destatis (2006, p. 31) also supports this conclusion. Another notable use case is financial services, which here too encompasses on-line banking and share purchasing. In most EU member states, only very few Internet banking users belong to the youngest age-group, many members of which probably are not yet part of the labour market and thus unlikely to necessitate it (see: Demunter 2005a, p. 5). Particularly notable is, however, that the usage of these financial services in Germany reaches levels above the average for the oldest age-group (see: Figure 2.2). Material from destatis (2006, p. 35) also offers insight on health-related Internet research, albeit 11 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? at a lower age resolution: health-related topics have been researched most by German Internet users in the group of 25–55 years of age (at 45 %), with the oldest group of 55 years and older ranking second at 39 %. This mirrors very generally the findings from Fox & Madden (2005) mentioned earlier for the situation in the US. Similarly, the aforementioned reluctance to conduct purchases of goods and services on-line (see: Fox 2005, p. 8) reappears in the Eurostat data as a steep age slope (illustrated in Figure 2.2) and is also visible in the data from destatis (2006, p. 36). Regarding the activity of downloading or playing music or games, the Eurostat survey yields an age slope that in some EU countries features a rise for the oldest age groups (see: Demunter 2005b, p. 4). For Germany, however, the slope is continuous (see also Figure 2.2) and thus marks this activity as primarily appealing to younger Germans. Conclusively, the existence of an age slope and an age gap, visible both when examining Internet usage as a whole and many specific Internet activities, becomes apparent. 2.2 Elements of Innovation Diffusion Theory In IDT, two primary dimensions are the ›adoptee‹ (the innovation being discussed) and its adopter(s) (human society, groups or individuals). In both cases, the characteristics presented here were developed from an expost perspective. In the case of the adoptee, they can be harnessed to predict the rate of adoption (see: Rogers 2003, p. 221), in the case of the adopter, they provide a framework of generalisations regarding an adopter’s innovativeness (see: Rogers 2003, p. 282). Furthermore, especially the first two stages of the Innovation-Decision process, which precede the actual decision to adopt or reject an innovation, are of particular interest, as it is in these stages that the characteristics mentioned above are of fundamental importance (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 170/174–175). The following sections first explain the pre-adoption stages, then the relevant adopter and adoptee characteristics; a final section provides the rationale for favouring the IDT paradigm over other possibly relevant theories in this discussion. 12 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? 2.2.1 Pre-Adoption Stages: Knowledge and Persuasion Within the first stage of Knowledge, with which the decision process starts, Rogers (2003, pp. 172–173) differentiates between three types of knowledge: »awareness-knowledge« literally denotes awareness that an innovation exists, »how-to knowledge« deals with the proper operation of the innovation, and »principles-knowledge« deals with the “inner workings” and scientific principles. Awareness is obviously the starting point. It motivates further information seeking on the part of the individual, which may also occur during the Persuasion stage (see below) (see: Rogers 2003, p. 173). Especially for complex innovations, Rogers (2003, p. 173) reasons that more how-to knowledge needs to be acquired in order to make later adoption and continued use more likely; similarly, principles-knowledge can be of assistance, but is not necessarily a requirement. Based on Knowledge, ». . . the individual forms a favourable or unfavourable Attitude toward the innovation« at the Persuasion stage, where most individuals look towards their »near peers« for confirmation, even though other sources are available. (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 174–175). In contrast to the Knowledge stage, which involves mainly cognition based on facts, the ». . . main type of thinking at the persuasion stage is affective (or feeling)« (Rogers 2003, p. 175). 2.2.2 Adopter Characteristics Rogers (2003, pp. 279–282) forms categories of adopters based on the ». . . time at which an individual adopts an innovation . . .«: Innovators, Early Adopters, Early Majority, Late Majority, and Laggards, presented in decreasing order of »innovativeness«. He goes on to characterise the correlation of an adopter’s innovativeness with socio-economic characteristics, personality variables, and communication behaviour. The reader is kindly referred to the original for an in-depth description; yet some striking key elements useful for the general discussion in this paper shall follow. Note that individuals can, and usually do exhibit a different amount of “innovativeness”, regarding different adoptees (see: de Jager 2005). 13 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? Looking back on previous diffusion research, Rogers (2003, p. 288) emphasises that ». . . many diffusion studies . . . show no relationship [with age] . . .«, whereas some conclude that higher age correlates with earlier adoption and others in turn claim that lower age indicates probable earlier adoption; he reasons that this does not clearly support any expression of influence with regard to age as a primary characteristic. Rather, socioeconomic factors, a high level of education, social status (e. g. occupational prestige), and economic status, positively relate to innovativeness (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 288–289). For personality traits, those who adopt earlier typically exhibit traits such as high intelligence and empathy, whereas dogmatic thinking, fatalism and adversity towards change may characterise late adopters (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 289–290). Regarding communication behaviour, Rogers (2003, pp. 290–291) holds that high innovativeness probably correlates with a high amount of social interconnection, travel across social system boundaries, access to mass media, and a tendency to more actively search for new innovations, and various other factors. 2.2.3 Adoptee Characteristics Rogers (2003, p. 220) coins five »perceived attributes« for innovations that determine the rate of its adoption: Relative Advantage, Compatibility, Complexity, Trialability, and Observability, each of which the following paragraphs will introduce. The idea that sceptical individuals may view an innovation in a different light than its supporters is implicated, as already related in subsection 2.2.1. The Relative Advantage of an innovation constitutes improvements or benefits, often relative to earlier ideas. The type of improvement or benefit mainly depends on the type of innovation in question, but may be on economic grounds (e. g. increased profits, productivity, cost-benefitrelation, improved functionality) or increase an individual’s social status (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 229–231). According to Rogers (2003, p. 233), Relative Advantage, may well be ». . . one of the strongest predictors for the rate of adoption of an innovation«. Considerations on infrastructure mat- 14 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? ters constitute the Compatibility attribute. It refers to past experiences, for example with similar ideas or innovations, the prospective user’s perceived needs, past innovations that are in use and must support or must be supported by the new innovation, but also to social values and beliefs (see: Rogers 2003, p. 240). Problems in this field may arise from technical issues (consider e. g. a new type of PDA or mobile phone: failure to make provisions for PC and specifically Microsoft Outlook synchronisation would likely hamper acceptability), but might well involve such a simple thing as a product name3 (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 250–251). Another important aspect in this context is what Rogers (2003, p. 254) calls the »empty vessel fallacy«: already present knowledge can make the diffusion of an innovation either easier or more difficult, and ignoring persistent knowledge means to deny the importance of compatibility. Another key variable is Complexity. This obviously denotes a relationship between the perceived or actual complexity, ease of use, or ease of understanding, and how well the innovation will diffuse (see: Rogers 2003, p. 257). This characteristic appears to be of exceptional importance for innovations involving computers and related technology, for in the past it has shown to be a particularly strong hindrance to adoption (see: Rogers 2003, pp. 257–258). In general precedence, however, Rogers (2003, p. 258) ranks Complexity after Relative Advantage and Compatibility in that order. The two remaining factors both govern aspects of contact with the innovation: Trialability encompasses the possibility of »trying out« (Rogers 2003, p. 258) the innovation prior to an individuals own decision to adopt. Observability refers to how easily the innovation can be “seen in action” or the results of its use become visible in public or private environments and thus reach out to individuals not directly in contact with the innovation or its owner (see: Rogers 2003, p. 258). Both attributes areconsidered to be positively related to quicker adoption; regarding Trialability, however, Rogers (2003, p. 258) remarks that it is probably more important to earlier adopters, as they lack precedents of use of the innovation in their vicinity. 3 which can have a derogatory or even taboo meaning in other languages 15 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? 2.2.4 Other Theories of Technology Acceptance The author is aware that IDT is not the only model available for innovation or technology acceptance discussions. Venkatesh et al. (2003), for example, propose a unified model that encompasses elements of several theories used in research on the acceptance of information systems, among them IDT. Their Unified Theory of the Acceptance and Usage of Technology (UTAUT) identifies age and gender as moderators of the influence of two primary factors largely corresponding to the IDT adoptee characteristics of Relative Advantage and Complexity: the impact of the UTAUT performance expectancy (advantage) construct is less important to women and to older individuals, whereas the effort expectancy (complexity) construct is actually of more importance women and older individuals (see: Venkatesh et al. 2003, pp. 449–450). The UTAUT model, however, addresses technology acceptance at organisational level, whereas the framework put forth by Rogers is especially suitable for the more general discussion featured later in this chapter, because it allows a more detailed appraisal of the adopters. 2.3 Possible Age Slope Explanations The following passages will attempt to categorise plausible explanations for the age gap and slope observed in section 2.1 by moving along the characteristics IDT as outlined in section 2.2. 2.3.1 Adoptee Perspective For a variety of reasons, Internet use may provide a considerable relative advantage. With increasing age, individuals are more likely to have or develop health problems or chronic illnesses—despite an overall lengthened lifespan (see chapter 3). The Internet can (and does, see section 2.1) serve as a source of information on health-related topics. It may help to maintain as independent a lifestyle as possible, easing the effects of restricted mobility (see section 3.3) by enabling users to order goods and services and have them delivered to the door, or conduct dealings with various institutions 16 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? (public, financial, or other) from home. The Internet can also provide information, services, and opportunities regarding hobbies and various leisure activities, with travel being of particular interest (see section 2.1). As another example, e-mail and on-line communities provide opportunities to maintain or develop social relationships and contacts. Numerous advantages of Internet use are identifiable, relative to continued non-use; some of them may even be of increased benefit specifically to seniors. With regard to complexity, computers and Internet access, despite efforts to the contrary, still require a considerable amount of non-trivial computer knowledge or third-party help during set-up and configuration. Knowledge acquired during pre-retirement careers may be out of date, as the development of hardware and software proceeded at a remarkably high pace. Additionally, Web site design is traditionally diverse and there are very loose standards or conventions at best; thus, strategies successfully applied on one Web site may fail on another, increasing the overall complexity. Furthermore, non-informational content, be it additional animation, sound, video, or, the purest form, advertising, is often part of the page display. Advertisements in particular are not always4 clearly distinguishable from the informational content and again increase overall complexity. In conclusion, a considerable learning effort may be necessary to master the use of one or, depending on prior experience, even two media elements: the computer and the Internet. Computers and the Internet integrate increasingly well with other media and communication equipment of recent date, for example mobile phones, PDAs, fax machines, cameras, television, video, and music playing devices. Some of these, most notably mp3 players, actually developed as an extension to existing or expanding practices or technological innovations in the computer and Internet sphere. Interfacing with older equipment such as analogue audio or video devices likely requires more in-depth knowledge or assistance. Internet access solutions that utilise a television set as display may be an exciting opportunity, because they side-step the need for computer knowledge. Indigenous knowledge or prior experience—acquired from 4 depending on the layout 17 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? earlier forms of information technology, or during the completed phases of Internet diffusion—may be non-existent (no contact with computers or Internet at work or prior to retirement, as appropriate), second-hand (e. g. from other media), not much help (contact prior to wide-spread Internet use), or even conflicting (e. g. fear of job loss due to IT-based rationalisation). Therefore, interactions with computers can be assumed to appear radically different from established behaviour and thus likely to be perceived as incompatible. Even though computers and Internet access have become more portable, they largely take place in stationary settings. Consequently, observability and trialability are limited to such settings, many of which have an educational or occupational characteristic and are thus observable to restricted groups. Leisure use would most likely take place in private; even public use is usually limited to certain localities, such as Internet cafes. Still, to the actively interested individual, access to and information and training on the new technologies are available from community colleges, public libraries, and private organisations. More easily and above all passively available indirect observability of computer and Internet use and also of its consequences is available through various amounts of media coverage. 2.3.2 Adopter Perspective Especially the statements made in the previous passages on observability, trialability, and compatibility have already hinted at a social and thus communicative aspect. With increasing age, the number of an individual’s social connections may be subject to some decline, because after retirement, employment and thus participation in the associated social system largely cease and the individual’s peer group, subject to the effects of illness and death, is mostly in a similar situation (see, e. g.: Mollenkopf 1998, p. 129; Backes & Clemens 2003, p. 207). Most seniors are also not part of the formal education system and thus lack contact to these social systems as well: relatively few seniors actually engage in educational activities or even academic studies (see: Backes & Clemens 2003, p. 214), although (renewed) participation in these systems is a possibility. Mollenkopf (1998, p. 129) 18 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? also notes that relatives are likely to live independently. Contact with them would therefore be less regular. Particularly disadvantaged, regarding exposure to computers and Internet use, are seniors that were already retired before these technologies were in widespread use in employment contexts (see: Bischoff 2001, p. 13). In some settings, there are protective measures in effect for older individuals still in employment, which allow these employees to decline the introduction of new information technology into their personal work (e. g. Stadtverwaltung Aachen 1994, § 8(5), p. 5). There is the possibility that similar agreements exist, at least in other German communal civil service departments, and that such agreements may shield qualifying employees from experiencing the new technological developments themselves. Retirement also brings about changes in media usage. For example, Lehr (2003, p. 322) observes that in many cases, time freed due to retirement is spent, to a considerable extend, on increased media consume—mostly focussed on “traditional” media such as television, newspapers, radio, magazines. Gauthier & Smeeding (2000, p. 21) also conclude an increase in the amount of ». . . time spent on passive leisure activities . . .«5 with age. Earlier works (see, e. g.: Becker et al. 1991, pp. 72f; Brünner 1997, pp. 146f) revealed a considerable discrepancy between the pensioners’ desire to lead an active life by, for example, extending hobbies and leisure activities like travel, and the factual implementation of such desires, although they also acknowledge that such patterns are expected to change somewhat with the retirement of following generations in the near future. Other patterns of personal behaviour that can hamper the diffusion of computers and Internet use are likely to be present among the older subgroups of the population. Traditional role models of and stereotypes on both gender and age may discourage the adoption of this new (young) and technical (complex) innovation, especially in older women. The widening of gender differences in German Internet usage, as noted by Greiner et al. (2006, pp. 50–51), is probably a manifestation of this effect: Gender differ5 The authors’ definition of »passive leisure« is media consume and relaxing (Gauthier & Smeeding 2000, p. 35) 19 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? ences are apparent in age-groups of over 25 years and henceforth tend to be more pronounced, particularly for the age-group of 55 years and older. As mentioned earlier in subsection 2.2.4, Venkatesh et al. (2003, pp. 449– 450) recognise corresponding influences of age and gender in their UTAUT model. Seniors are also reported to be particularly sceptical about their own learning abilities; this may likewise act to discourage them from engaging in pre-adoptive training and research for fear of “proving their incompetence” (see: Stadelhofer 2000, p. 188) and consecutively from adopting the innovation. Similar effects are known to appear in cognitive ageing research: Schneider et al. (1994, pp. 113–114) have reported that performance of older participants markedly improves when researchers take care to minimise the “fear of failure”. All these factors may combine to discourage participating more actively in the use of new technologies. To overcome them, Stadelhofer (2000, p. 189) argues in favour of separate and gradual tutorials among fellow individuals (especially for women) and notes an often enthusiastic usage pattern as the participants grow more confident (and competent) as well as an enrichment in interpersonal communication with younger relatives and other individuals. Finally, regarding interactions with levels of education, Stadelhofer (2000, p. 187) has found that encouraging Internet training and use among older individuals who show interest in technology, further education, or both is much easier than reaching out to seniors without such interests. It appears to be well established that older generations have a lower percentage of individuals with high-level formal education (see, e. g.: Isserstedt et al. 2004, pp. 103–104). The level of formal education would also have some effect on other variables such as socio-economic status and thus on available monetary resources. 2.4 Conclusion Despite considerable benefits and an increased (mostly passive) consumption of mass media, which should actually promote awareness and how-to knowledge, a majority (75 %) of senior non-users say that they do not re- 20 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? quire Internet access and 34 % say they have insufficient knowledge6 (see: destatis 2006, p. 7). Both statements can support an observation by Gehrke (2001a, p. 29) on “encouragement” campaigns and advertisements that fail to properly address the older target groups. Combined with less contact to the social systems that are primarily involved with computer and Internet deployment, restricted Trialability and Observability, and the fact that the own peer group is mostly in a similar situation, this is likely to result in substantial knowledge deficits regarding both usage and the numerous benefits. Without the necessary knowledge, it would be difficult for every individual to reach a positive attitude. Furthermore, socially induced and also traditional personal behaviour patterns and stereotypes put seniors and especially senior women, who outnumber men in the higher age-groups, in a position that may make them reluctant to actively seek engagement. A relatively high amount of complexity, however, makes extended amounts of training and information acquisition essential. Finally, formal education is quite capable of generating an Internet usage slope on its own: observe that students’ use of the Internet has reached soaring levels across the EU; a high level of education as a positive influence can be considered to be readily established (see, e. g.: Greiner et al. 2006, p. 51; Demunter 2005a, pp. 3/4). This appraisal is far from complete, off course, and at a rather macroscopic level. Nevertheless, it manages to highlight that there is more behind age-slope and age-gap than a generalised stereotype of age-related scepticism towards technological developments. With regard to usability and design recommendations, the findings from this chapter allow for four main conclusions. Senior Internet users may have less experience overall with both computers and the Internet, which leads to the first assumption that seniors using the Internet are more likely to be novice users. Secondly, there is evidence to suggest that a socially or culturally induced lack of confidence in their own abilities may, in combination with their limited experience and knowledge, cause senior individuals to behave very cautiously. The base of the third assumption is that seniors not 6 Item named ambiguously; could refer to knowledge about the Internet in general or to a lack of skills. 21 2 Seniors and the Web: Programmed Conflict? living in English-speaking countries are less likely to be familiar with the English language, which is the source of many technical and jargon terms. Therefore, computer and Internet jargon may form an additional language barrier, not exclusively for senior users , but for them in particular. As a final note, statistics (see, e. g.: destatis 2006, p. 8) show that senior Internet users also are less likely to be using broadband connections, despite the fact that broadband access is becoming increasingly commonplace in many western countries. The bandwidth available to senior Internet users is therefore still likely to be subject to the limits of modem or ISDN connections. This carries additional weight because statistics also show that the vast majority of senior users, due to retirement, connects to the Internet from home and no longer has access to facilities at work. 22 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind It is not possible to give a full and thorough examination of how ageing affects the human body and mind within the scope of this document. Nevertheless, some illustration regarding those physical and cognitive abilities important for accessing the Web are necessary in order to evaluate guidelines with regard to elders. The main subdivision in this chapter is between sensory processing and cognitive processing, which, although the distinction is not necessarily made like this in cognitive literature, here serves the purpose of organising the discussion of ageing effects on relevant human abilities. High levels of physical performance, although they have proven useful in predicting some aspects of cognitive functioning and health as well, are not of particular importance for using computer systems. Therefore, a single section shall briefly discuss mobility and fine motor skills only. 3.1 Sensory Processing Those sensual modalities relevant to computer use shall be discussed in sequence of increasing importance, beginning with a more brief section on skin senses. Audio signals in various forms and applications have not yet become a standard feature of Web sites, but their importance is evident through secondary and audio-visual content. The focus of the discussion will be on vision. 3.1.1 Skin Senses Sensual modalities involving the skin are of peripheral importance only because they do not feature primarily on the Web itself. The idea of con- 23 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind veying haptic information in addition to video and audio signals to the computer user is much more readily embraced in the computer gaming context, where technologies such as ForceFeedback find use to enhance the gaming experience, most often in the form of vibration or resistance. Logitech, a well-known manufacturer of computer mice, joysticks, and other input devices, did indeed try to introduce some form of haptic feedback mouse several years ago, but discontinued this venture, as it turned out to be unsuccessful on the market (see: König 2006). The devices apparently used vibration to create a haptic impression or hints at clickability of whatever was under the pointer (e. g. window borders, buttons). Skin senses are more significant for evaluating ergonomics or the suitability for elders of computer input equipment such as keyboards and mice, which is beyond the scope of this document. Nevertheless, it is worth noticing that their sensitivity is also subject to change with age (see: Zarit & Zarit 1987, p. 26). Skin senses include three subtypes of mechanoreception (related to skin deformation: touch, pressure, vibration), temperature, and pain; most of these experience some form of age-related decline (see: Plattig 1984, pp. 421/423; Verrillo & Verrillo 1985, p. 16). Verrillo & Verrillo (1985, pp. 16–22) offer a more detailed discussion especially with regard to vibrosensitivity, which they find to decline steadily with age for most vibration frequencies except 25 Hz. A combination of reasons can account for this development, most notably decreasing numbers of receptor cells, reduced conductivity of the central nervous system (which serves as transporting channel for the gross portion of each of the skin senses), reduced circulation both in the periphery and the brain, skin growth and ensuing insulation of nerve ends, and influences of skin and body core temperature (see: Plattig 1984, pp. 423/425; Verrillo & Verrillo 1985, pp. 17/19). There are findings indicating that multi-modal feedback with a haptic component might be beneficial to older computer users (see: Emery et al. 2002, p. 51). This result is, however, rendered academic unless such input devices become broadly available. 24 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind 3.1.2 Hearing A decline in hearing ability is part of the general stereotype of ageing, and at times receives illustration through everyday experience: an elderly CRT computer-display in the office of an older employee emits a highpitched sound that leaves the occupant of the room perfectly untroubled, but severely annoys younger colleagues as soon as they enter. Age-related changes are widely recognised as being responsible for this, as the following discussion shall show. As already mentioned in the introduction of this chapter, the relevance of this process is supplemented, given the fact that computers usually incorporate sound-producing gear today, but it weighs less heavy than the changes in vision. Still, audio content may be present on the Web, for example as a supplemental material, as part of video content, or as auxiliary (Web site texts read out through software). The term presbycusis encompasses progressive age-related hearing loss. Authors generally attribute this condition to hair cell damage in the inner ear (see, e. g.: Bayles & Kaszniak 1987, p. 151; Goldstein & Ritter 1997, p. 530), or consider mechanical damage in lieu of or combination with impairments on cellular level, emphasising that purely age-related changes are not so easily identifiable (see: Verrillo & Verrillo 1985, pp. 10–11), in contrast to the case of presbyopia (see subsection 3.1.3). A gradual diminishment in the ability to hear especially high frequency sounds—as portrayed in the introductory example—is the result most common in the aged populace (see, e. g.: Bayles & Kaszniak 1987, p. 153; Krauss Whitbourne 1996, p. 26). This decline progressively occurs throughout adulthood and an increasing difficulty in pitch discrimination accompanies it (see, e. g.: Verrillo & Verrillo 1985, pp. 11–12). Some authors relate that presbycusis affects men more severely than women, possibly due to more frequent or longer noise exposure, workrelated or other (see, e. g.: Krauss Whitbourne 1996, p. 26; Goldstein & Ritter 1997, p. 530). This would suggest an additional environmental component with influence on the development of hearing abilities. Goldstein & Ritter (1997, p. 531) furthermore venture that age alone may not necessarily induce progressive hearing loss, as members of pre-industrial societies 25 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind without contact to loud environments apparently fail to display such development. Although presbycusis may be more or less noticeable, depending on the individual extend of the hearing loss, its impact on every-day functioning remains relatively benign unless it impairs speech intellegibility. Sound frequencies between 0.4 and 3 kHz typically make up human speech, which roughly corresponds to the “sweet spot” of human hearing between 2 and 4 kHz (see: Goldstein & Ritter 1997, p. 353). As Fozard (1990, pp. 160– 161) reports, reception of these speech-relevant frequencies are also subject to age-related decline, but more slowly than high frequencies, at least until the age of 60 years; after that, the decline progresses more quickly. Kausler (1991, p. 102) also points out that in addition to changes of a receptional nature, central processes probably play a role in causing age differences in speech understanding. Aside from this processing aspect and focussed on the overall development of hearing abilities as Verrillo & Verrillo (1985, p. 13) illustrate it, the notion of lower frequencies being less severely affected prompts to infer that men, on account of usually having deeper voices, may be easier to understand. Although intelligibility of normal speech in unaltered condition and a calm environment appears to remain relatively unharmed until around age 70, both Fozard (1990, p. 161) and Kausler (1991, pp. 100–102) conclude that it is most adversely affected by increased speed of presentation, interruption, and background noise. With regard to the development of language understanding performance under non-optimal conditions, the illustration by Kausler (1991, p. 101) implicates that age-related decline may start much earlier than age 60, depending on the specific presenting condition. Schneider & PichoraFuller (2000, p. 161) point out that even clinically normal hearing abilities do not necessarily imply similar performance of elders in real-world settings with background noise and other impeding factors present. 3.1.3 Vision It is no exaggeration to call vision the most important sensual modality. Computers, though striving to become increasingly multi-medial, still 26 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind rely mainly on seeing. Most information on the Web is presented in the form of text, images, or a combination thereof. The visual apparatus and the nervous system associated with it are definitely suspect to age-related change. There is, off course, also a wide range of relevant diseases which usually have a tremendous impact on visual abilities, are more prevalent with increased age, and remain largely untreatable in cases that involve the nervous system (see, e. g.: Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 168; Schieber 2006, pp. 133–134). They can, however, count as disabilities rather than impairments (see: subsection 1.1.1) and are thus not part of this examination. With increasing age, the human eye looses its ability to accommodate for near vision, due to structural changes within the eye scientifically termed presbyopia. The advent of presbyopia is perhaps one of the most notorious foreshadows of “old age” and is directly associated with primary ageing (see: Plattig 1984, p. 425). The lens is made of epithelium (as are, e. g. fingernails), and continuously produces fresh cells throughout life; yet it is confined physically, because it is situated in the eye, and therefore unable to grow outwards (see, e. g.: Schaie & Willis 1991, p. 442). As a consequence, cells accumulate and the lens becomes harder and less flexible with age. Some authors (see, e. g.: Schaie & Willis 1991, p. 442; Goldstein & Ritter 1997, pp. 512–513) hold that the ciliary body, responsible for forming the lens into shape for near vision, looses strength as it ages and thereby further diminishes the ability of the eye to accommodate, but Schieber (2006, p. 131) attributes presbyopia to lens rigidity alone. As a result of presbyopia, the focal point for near vision moves further and further away. According to Goldstein & Ritter (1997, pp. 512–513), the ability of the eye to accommodate declines steadily throughout adulthood, with the focal point moving from 10 centimetres at 20 years of age to 22 centimetres at 40 years, then drops off sharply until age 60, where the focal point is around 100 centimetres. Individual manifestations may vary, but it is generally agreed that somewhere in the fourth decade, this effect becomes discomforting, as the focal point moves out beyond the 50 centimetre border Goldstein & Ritter (1997, p. 513) name as utmost comfortable reading distance and 27 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind requires readers to hold out newspapers or books “at arm’s length” in order to focus on the text. By the age of 60, the eye has completely lost the ability to accommodate (see, e. g.: Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 169; Schieber 2006, p. 131). The amount of light that reaches the retina of the aged eye decreases by roughly two thirds (see, e. g.: Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 170; Schieber 2006, p. 130). Light rays reaching the eye must pass the pupil, which with age decreases in aperture. The scientific term for this phenomenon is senile miosis and may actually describe ». . . a dynamic process acting to optimize [!] retinal contrast and overall visual performance« (Schieber 2006, p. 131). Furthermore, normally translucent components of the eye, namely the cornea, the lens, and the vitreous, become slightly opaque with age, and the lens also develops a yellow tint; these changes further decrease the amount of light admitted. Especially the increased optical density of the lens increases intra-ocular scatter, which makes the aged eye more sensitive to glare (Schaie & Willis 1991, pp. 443–444) and also increases the blur of the retinal image beyond correction (see: Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 171). Another contributor to reduced retinal illumination is ptosis: increased elasticity often causes the eye lids to droop into the light path (see: Schieber 2006, pp. 132–133). A decline in retinal photo receptors is another commonly reported cause for the decreased sensitivity to light. According to Schieber (2006, p. 132), the number of rods, the receptor cell type responsible for monochrome vision, reduces dramatically with age. A most interesting ramification, with regard to the overall topic of this essay, is an age-related decrease in contrast sensitivity, the ability to discern alternating black and white patterns. Fozard (1990, pp. 155–156) establishes that both decreased amounts of light reaching the retina as well as changes in the retina itself and the nervous system beyond are responsible for declines in this field. Schneider & Pichora-Fuller (2000, p. 172) and Kausler (1991, pp. 91–92) come to the same conclusion and the latter adds information on the rating of different levels of brightness: ». . . a much larger increase in light intensity was needed for elderly subjects than for 28 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind young subjects to produce the same increase in perceived brightness.« A contrast perceivable for young adults may be invisible to elders. For the cones as the cell type responsible for colour vision, the extend of the age-related decline still subject to some debate. Schieber (2006, p. 132) concludes that, given the ascertained decline of rods with age, a decline of the number of cones also seems likely, but in the past has proven to be difficult to measure. Apart from this development at cellular level, the light absorption of the aged lens causes difficulties in colour vision, as it specifically reduces the amount of short wavelength light that reach the retina. Difficulties in colour discrimination are the likely result of this. Although Verrillo & Verrillo (1985, p. 8) relate a steady decline from age 20 and onwards with noticeable changes around age 70, Fozard (1990, pp. 152–153), summarises more conservative conclusions with little change in colour vision except for test subjects in their 80s and criticises that the observed effects could well be limited to the testing situation. Kausler (1991, p. 95) remarks that accuracy in colour discrimination has dropped by up to 25 % at age 70 and 50 % at age 90, adding that this may be of relevance primarily, when fine shades have to be distinguished. Schneider & Pichora-Fuller (2000, p. 170) as well as Schieber (2006, p. 145) characterise the aged colour vision as gradually approaching that of (anomalous) tritanopia. An interesting observation in this respect is that changes in colour perception may be responsible for a preference of bright colours and highly contrastive combinations among the elder population (see: Brünner 1997, p. 45). Age-related changes in contrast sensitivity, colour vision, and retinal luminance all contribute to a loss in overall acuity. Even with corrective glasses, these factors still ». . . lead to a more blurred, lower contrast and less bright retinal image« (Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 173). The development of corrected visual acuity itself has been depicted in overly pessimistic terms in past research; more recent studies have yielded a better (and also more accurate) prognosis: ». . . relatively unimpaired levels of visual acuity can be maintained “for most persons” . . . until 88 years of age« (Schieber 2006, p. 138). 29 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind The aforementioned changes on a more receptional level would be expected to influence also higher-order visual functioning. Indeed, age-related reductions in the field of view are commonly reported (see, e. g.: Fozard 1990, p. 153; Kausler 1991, p. 92). Within the field of view, targets can be recognised even if they do not appear at the immediate point of focus. Schneider & Pichora-Fuller (2000, p. 174) assume a reduction of more than 20 % by age 70 and relate that, full corrected acuity notwithstanding, ». . . an older adult’s useful visual field can be only one third that of a younger adult . . .«, depending on the visual scene in question; the authors name presence of distractors, target/distractor similarity, and dual-task settings as likely influences. This establishes a link to research on attention (see subsection 3.2.2). Regarding the onset of the decline in the field of view, Schaie & Willis (1991, p. 444) concludes that it remains steady until age 55, then diminishes—with pronounced differences usually present by age 75. Schaie & Willis (1991, p. 444) as well as Kausler (1991, p. 92) stress that a strongly diminished field of view can have a profound impact on everyday life; the latter also remarks that it may be one of the factors responsible for the increased amount of accidents in traffic elders experience. A decline is also observed in temporal resolution, which is in essence the lowest frequency of a flickering stimulus at which it is perceived as continuous; elders require a lower frequency to detect flickering (see, e. g.: Schneider & Pichora-Fuller 2000, p. 174). According to Schneider & Pichora-Fuller (2000, p. 175), elders have a higher threshold in detecting uniform movement against a background and are less adept at tracking fast-moving targets. 3.2 Cognitive Processing Numerous ways exist to conceptualise cognitive functioning and to explain the ageing effects. Borderlines between the constructs as well as the influences they exert on the assessment of one another are not always clearly determinable. The discussion of cognitive ageing will begin with the commonly reported reduction of timed performance, observe changes in the 30 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind two very influential concepts of attention and memory, and end with notes on intelligence and problem solving behaviour. As shall be evident, there is no consent as to what is the uniform cause of age differences in cognitive performance, or whether or not there is a single cause or more than one. 3.2.1 Reaction Times and Rate of Information Processing Reaction times have been monitored in ageing research to asses a wide variety of cognitive functions and consistently produced higher latency in elder participants as result (see, e. g.: Schaie & Willis 1991, p. 453; Hartley 2006, p. 184). Related observations include more physical manifestations as well, such as different and slower movement patterns (see, e. g.: Spirduso 1995, p. 185); as task complexity increases, the age differences in response time generally become more pronounced (see, e. g.: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 292). The cause of slowed reaction was held to be the construct of central information processing, which was investigated both as a symptom of and a possible universal explanation for age-related declines in cognitive functioning (see, e. g.: Hartley 2006, p. 183). There have been observations challenging the appropriateness of this very convenient perspective of relating all age deficiencies solely to reduced processing speed: experiment tasks have exhibited systematically different slowing patterns, which have led researchers to conclude that several underlying processes of cognitive functioning are subject to different amounts of age-related slowing (see, e. g.: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 293), although which processes these are and to what extend they are affected, is still subject to research. Furthermore, the pureness of response time as a measure has been called into question, because laboratory tests, while still simplified, compared to the real world, have to be sufficiently complex to produce age differences and are thus likely to involve several age-sensitive factors at once, thereby combining their effects (see: Hartley 2006, p. 200). The slowing phenomenon has been attributed to age-related change of the central nervous system: actual neural decline, reduced nerve conductivity, reduced blood perfusion, and reduced levels neurotransmitters. Cerella 31 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind (1990, pp. 202–203) illustrated that a neural network could be employed to model the information processing ability of the central nervous system and how the increased response latency could be explained as broken connections between neurons, which require detours around broken links and thus slow the response. Even though an actual decline of the neurons themselves is in doubt and the contribution of nerve conductivity reductions to reduced response times have been considered marginal (see: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 293), the level of neurotransmitters, such as dopamine, have been reported as declining with age (see, e. g.: Schaie & Willis 1991, p. 452; Spirduso 1995, p. 268). A pathologically sharp decline of dopamine is one of the causes of Parkinson’s disease and makes Parkinson’s patients exhibit exaggerated slowness and difficulties in motor movements, among other symptoms (see, e. g.: Spirduso 1995, p. 268; NINDS 2006, What_is). Thus, a mild, natural decline of dopamine would suggest non-pathological manifestations of similar symptoms with age. There have also been studies indicating that physical fitness, like in other fields of research on ageing, has beneficial influence on response speed, to the extend that physically active elders out-perform inactive younger participants in timed tasks (see, e. g.: Spirduso 1995, pp. 272–273; Hartley 2006, p. 186). Since physical exercise increases blood perfusion, it could thereby exert a positive influence on the brain and its functioning. Similarly, practice sessions have been reported to improve the performance of elders, but they cannot eliminate age differences completely (see, e. g.: Hartley 2006, p. 186). On a behavioural level, a different trade-off between speed and accuracy in elder participants, brought about by social “pressure” to show competence and thus to make as few errors as possible, or by a reduced ability to optimally adjust the personal speed/accuracy tradeoff based on error feedback, was also discussed as possible cause (see e. g.: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 294). According to Hartley (2006, pp. 200–201), reduced response times among elders, although indubitably present, are no longer considered as a universal cause of age-related cognitive decline. Rather, research now investigates the interrelations between cognitive constructs, possibly present common m 32 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind decline causes, and ageing, and considers processing speed ». . . as one of several possible markers for the physiological intactness of the organism or . . . as one of many cognitive processes« (Hartley 2006, p. 201). 3.2.2 Attention In a functional perspective, attention is frequently discussed in terms of selective, divided, and sustained attention (see, e. g.: Kausler 1991, p. 134; Matthews et al. 2000, p. 294). Tasks as well as theories may, however, not honour such distinctions (see: McDowd & Shaw 2000, pp. 221–222). Age-related deficits in selective attention, the ability to ignore irrelevant information, or rather to discriminate relevant from irrelevant information (see, e. g.: Bayles & Kaszniak 1987, p. 255), have been a consistent result of cognitive ageing research, even though certain modifiers do apply. In visual search tasks, which require the location of a target stimulus such as a letter within an array of distractor items (numbers, other letters, symbols), it generally takes elders longer than younger participants to detect targets; the performance of elder participants reduces particularly sharp for targets very similar to non-targets (see: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 295; McDowd & Shaw 2000, p. 228). On the other hand, singularly different targets (targets that are discriminable by a single characteristic) may even allow elders to perform at equivalent or only slightly lower levels than younger individuals (see, e. g.: Plude 1990, p. 481; McDowd & Shaw 2000, p. 228). There is also a line of research suggesting that elders perform better in visual search tasks when a cue to the spatial location of the target is given (see, e. g.: D’Aloisio & Klein 1990, p. 461). In attention assessment tasks that used text reading comprehension, elders performed worse, relative to younger participants, when the distracting material was semantically related to the target material, particularly when ambiguity resulted from the interference; measures to make target and non-target text more discriminable, however, induced better performance in elder participants in this discipline as well (see: McDowd & Shaw 2000, pp. 255–256). Diminished performance for elders in the field of coordinating multiple tasks (divided attention) are likewise well documented, although McDowd 33 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind & Shaw (2000, p. 236) note that a discussion revolves around how age differences could be just as well explicable through differences in the demands of the individual tasks employed in the coordination tests, rather than age differences in the ability to divide attention. In conclusion, they list difficulty (e. g. processing or memory demands), novelty, and similarity in stimulus or response as possible task-specific influences and also express that ». . . aerobic fitness . . . and extended training . . . may be important and manipulable factors . . . in attentional performance« (see: McDowd & Shaw 2000, pp. 241–242). Task complexity as the main determiner of age deficits in divided attention tasks is identified by Matthews et al. (2000, p. 296), but these authors also acknowledge that it may be not the multipletask aspect per se that is age sensitive, but other processes, depending on the specific task in question. McDowd & Shaw (2000, pp. 250–251) further comment on switching attention from one task to another and conclude that for simple tasks, equivalence exists or can be established after training, but as the tasks’ memory demands increase, age-related deficiencies in the ability to switch attention become apparent; physical fitness may again positively influence abilities here. With regard to sustained attention, the continuous observation of stimuli over extended periods, the picture is more diverse. McDowd & Shaw (2000, pp. 252/254) conclude that studies suggests an ». . . age equivalence in vigilance task performance . . .«, but acknowledge that more demanding tasks do result in age differences. Matthews et al. (2000, p. 298) on the other hand quote a number of studies that produced age differences mostly in the amount of stimuli detected (or false positives produced), but not necessarily in the rate of the characteristic performance decline, which takes place over the time of task duration. This interpretation is in line with earlier works on this topic (see, e. g.: McDowd & Birren 1990, p. 225; Kausler 1991, pp. 186–187). All reviewed authors, however, sustain the notion that the complexity of the vigilance task is related to the magnitude of the age deficits and more complex tasks yield larger age-related deficits. Again, a high level of physical fitness is a possible factor in better vigilance task performance (see: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 298). 34 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind 3.2.3 Memory Development and Implications for Learning In cognitive research, several types or views of memory have been distinguished. The distinction between stores is based on how long a memory is retained: sensory, short-term and long-term memory. Over time, the conceptualisation of memory has been refined to a functional perspective to further account for experimental observations (see: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 61). It appears that within long-term memory, a distinction between declarative memory (sometimes referred to as explicit) and non-declarative memory (occasionally named implicit or procedural) can be considered to be well established on behavioural as well as neurological grounds (see: Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 210). In this view, declarative memory is concious (the individual is aware that memories exist and can usually describe them verbally), whereas non-declarative memories are subconcious; examples are skills or classical conditioning (see, e. g.: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 62; Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 210). Age-related deficits in non-declarative memory functions have been reported as being mild, non-significant, or non-existent (see, e. g.: Zacks et al. 2000, pp. 306/311; Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 214), although these statements most likely refer to retention and continued use of already learned items (see below). This discussion will nevertheless leave non-declarative memory aside and instead focus on those memory types with pronounced age-related decline. An important distinction within the construct of declarative memory is between episodic memory, which covers events, and semantic memory, which holds factual knowledge (see: Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 213). Ageing research, according to Hoyer & Verhaegen (2006, p. 213), has resulted in a stable consensus regarding deficits of older individuals in episodic memory tasks. These tasks require the recollection of specific contextual information about the time, place, or situation in which the individual formed the memory in question, as opposed to the construct of familiarity, which only ». . . involves a feeling of “recognition” . . .« (see: Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 212). Semantic memory, although generally associated with the same brain region, exhibits a different pattern of decline; recollection and familiarity probably draw on different cranial resources. Because it reflects a 35 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind person’s knowledge accumulated over time, semantic memory performance actually increases initially. Hoyer & Verhaegen (2006, pp. 214–215) report the age of 60 as the point whereafter age-differences become pronounced for both episodic and semantic memory. Still, elders perform better in tasks that do not require the recall of contextual information, such as where the memory or knowledge originated. Zacks et al. (2000, p. 325) say that difficulties for elders have been shown to involve ». . . color [!], case, or font, . . . [or the] temporal sequence [of target items] . . .« and also more fundamental details such as the media type of item presentation, for example whether photographs, videos, or speech was used, or the sex of the speaker. The construct of working memory accounts for ». . . tasks that require the individual to simultaneously store and actively transform information« in what was previously referred to as short-term memory (see: Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 217). It is in line with traditional store models and evolved to account for these »processing issues« that they failed to address (see: Matthews et al. 2000, p. 63). Age differences have been consistently reported for simple memory-span tasks as well as more complex tasks involving multiple task processing, as Hoyer & Verhaegen (2006, p. 217) report. Deficits in working memory have profound impact on cognitive ability, because many forms of cognitive processing involve working memory somehow; among others, relations with fluid intelligence factors (see subsection 3.2.4 and language have been established (see: Zacks et al. 2000, p. 217; Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, p. 302). Apart from more general explanations such as decline of low-level sensory functions and generalised slowing, memory decline has also been attributed to deficits in cognitive control functions, which handle interference, multiple task coordination and attention switching, among others (see: Zacks et al. 2000, pp.218–221; Hoyer & Verhaegen 2006, pp. 302–305). This apparently links the topic of memory closely to research on attention. Either way, it has been shown that memory deficits are amendable to training: application of suitable mnemo techniques can at least make age differences smaller, although the efficacy of such measures reduces with further ageing 36 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind and likely depends on continued training and application in everyday life (see, e. g.: Philipp & Kliegl 2000, pp. 99–100). Changes in the functioning or effectiveness of memory has consequences for learning. Zacks et al. (2000, pp. 314–315) point out how proper structuring of the learning material (in this case, the example is a word list) and reference to already present knowledge positively affect memory performance in both younger individuals and elders. The acquisition of more complex skills or abilities, even if they largely feature under the heading of implicit (non-declarative) learning, do show age differences to the disadvantage of elders, which Hoyer & Touron (2003, pp. 34/36) attribute largely to deficits in lower-level perception and generalised slowing of information processing. Expert knowledge, if relevant to the task or material to be learned, does result in considerable performance benefit for both elders and younger test groups, but task- or skill-specific experience does not necessarily lead to a performance advantage for older experts; at least, it appears to be able to compensate for some age-related reductions of general cognitive ability (see, e. g.: Lehr 2003, pp. 101–102; Hoyer & Touron 2003, pp. 36–37). Among other influences, Lehr (2003, p. 94) also points out presentation speed and complexity (lower for well-structured content) as well as motivational and environmental factors, the like of which (Stadelhofer 2000) have already been discussed in the context of subsection 2.3.2. 3.2.4 Notes on Intelligence and Problem Solving Intelligence itself is not an independent cognitive construct, but rather a conglomerate thereof. With Bayles & Kaszniak (1987, p. 232), ». . . it is difficult to find a human cognitive process that has not been included, at one time or another, within the definition of intelligence.« Thus, intelligence studies may serve to indicate an individual’s proficiency in problem solving and cognitive ability, but this has to be done with caution, for in past decades, the observed age differences in cross-sectional intelligence surveys were one of the pillars of the deficit view of ageing, which concluded a steady decline of intelligence with age (see, e. g.: Brünner 1997, pp. 24–25; Lehr 2003, p. 76): cohort effects from factors like nutrition or education 37 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind level were too easily attributed to the ageing process and promoted an exaggeratedly negative view of the cognitive ability of elders. In intelligence studies, the differentiation between crystallised and fluid intelligence is of renown and each is assessed by different subsets of intelligence test batteries (see, e. g.: Lehr 2003, p. 78). The former are more dependent on education and experience and thus tend to show no decline or even an increase in scores, whereas the latter tend to exhibit a moderate decline with age (see, e. g.: Kausler 1991, pp. 723–724). Fluid intelligence represents indigenous abilities and includes measures like inductive reasoning and spatial ability; crystalised intelligence includes acquired knowledge, verbal, and numerical ability (see, e. g.: Bayles & Kaszniak 1987, p. 234; Lehr 2003, p. 78). The different results when comparing cross-sectional and longitudinal data is striking even when the methodological problems of longitudinal studies like drop-outs or positive selection are taken into account. Schaie concludes from his long-running study series that the intelligence decline in adulthood is much more benign than previously thought and also greatly varies inter-individually: there are strong correlations with socioeconomic status and longitudinal studies tend to show no decline or only moderate decline, especially among individuals for whom the continued application of academic skills is a necessity (see: Schaie 2005, p. 421). They furthermore conclude that most individuals may experience a decline in ability past age 60, but ». . . it is not until the 80s are reached that the average older adult will fall below the middle range of performance for young adults« (see: Schaie 2005, p. 419). Evaluations of everyday problem solving abilities face difficulties regarding comparison and generalisability of findings because of differences in task and scoring approaches and a confinement of eligible age groups (see: Marsiske & Margrett 2006, pp. 319–320). In this field, Marsiske & Margrett (2006, p. 320) remark, studies have been largely of cross-sectional design and thus are subject to the caveat of cohort confounding (see above). Study results of problem-solving and decision-making, it appears, have a relationship to intelligence measures and the development of their results over time 38 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind is quite similar to those of intelligence studies, at least cross-sectionally (see: Marsiske & Margrett 2006, p. 328). Based on a three-stage model of problem-solving, where an internal representation of the problem is formed, a strategy for solution is developed and finally applied to solve the problem, Kausler (1991, pp. 656–657) concludes that age differences may stem from a tendency to pay undue attention to irrelevant details in the first stage, from difficulties in generating an effective solution strategy (heuristic) and increased likelihood of continuing the use of an ineffectual heuristic rather than abandon it in favour for a new one in the second stage, and increased caution and scepticism against the quality of the solution at the third stage. Age differences further increase as tasks induce increased memory loads (see: Kausler 1991, p. 657 and also the passage on working memory in subsection 3.2.3). In Decision-making, there is a tendency in elders to narrow the focus and consider less options. Studies have shown elders to base decisions on less information (i. e. they ask fewer questions, take fewer aspects into consideration than do younger participants), but these tendencies may be a result of reduced information processing capacities or even a strategy employed to actively counter these deficiencies and to avoid an “information overload” or too high memory demands (see: Marsiske & Margrett 2006, pp. 324/326). Deficits in holding on to previous but non-optimal heuristics have also been observed in younger individuals for tasks that are of little practical interest to them; thus, the age differences in problem-solving and decision-making, at least to some extend, may be task specific (see: Marsiske & Margrett 2006, p. 326). As has been mentioned, older participants also appear to have less confidence in their problem solutions. There are similar observations in decisions-making research (see: Marsiske & Margrett 2006, p. 326), and this may be seen in analogy to motivational aspects of engaging in new learning efforts (see subsection 2.3.2). 39 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind 3.3 Mobility and Fine Motor Skills Mobility in the aged is most basically characterised by reduced speed of operation. Maintaining balance is of key importance and is achieved through a complex interplay of sensory and neuromuscular systems, normally requiring little or no concious effort; each of these systems, however, changes with age and balance becomes gradually more difficult to attain or reattain after balance loss, either naturally during the movement process or through environmental upsets (see: Spirduso 1995, p. 178). Pathological influences and consecutively falling incidents remain the most important source of mobility limitations. Healthy elder individuals usually are able to move normally, but when loss of balance and falling, made more likely through disease (see: Spirduso 1995, p. 175), become a very real risk of day-to-day locomotion, individuals may chose to restrict their activity. Falling incidents, which usually carry severe health consequences, can start a “vicious circle” that continuously worsens the mobility situation by increasing both fear and likelihood of falling (see: Spirduso 1995, pp. 168/177–178). The often observed decrement in walking speed probably reflects the necessity for more economic energy usage and the increased difficulty of maintaining balance; it evolves as a measure to compensate for age-related declines in the systems involved in locomotion (see: Spirduso 1995, p. 171). Intricate movements requiring fine motor control are usually formed and executed in the same way across age groups, but a slowing with age and also subtle differences in movement patterns are reported (see: Spirduso 1995, p. 219), as are reductions in the ability to make finely tuned and precise manual movements. For aiming tasks, Spirduso (1995, p. 220) concludes that antagonist muscles may ». . . not be activated as quickly as is necessary to stop the movement on the target«; she also attributes the ». . . jerky, uncoordinated movement sometimes seen in older adults« to this reduced ability to finely control muscular force application. Reduced inhibitory control, already mentioned earlier in the context of processing speed and in the guise of slow decline in neurotransmitter levels (see subsection 3.2.1), may also be responsible less accurate movement control. As with response 40 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind to external stimuli, Spirduso (1995, pp. 241/242) holds that elders usually favour accuracy over speed in intricate movements. 3.4 Summary and Conclusions The discussion in the previous sections of this chapter helped to identify certain problem fields. In some cases the reviewed material also points directly to possible approaches for strategies to externally compensate for ageing effects. The upcoming discussion of Web usability recommendations will refer to these results. In the fields of sensory processing and fine motor skills, the findings are relatively close to the actual functioning and therefore allow a fairly direct application. For the domain of hearing, problems ensue mainly from the reduced ability to understand speech, from increased susceptibility to aural distractions like background noise or bad signal quality, and from too fast presentation speed. These must be taken into account in the production of spoken text or audio-visual content. Compensating strategies could be using a deep (male) voices for recorded speech, a slow and sequential rather than parallel presentation of stimuli (e. g. music, speech, sound effects), and a high signal quality. Of particular interest and saliency are the changes in vision. Reduced overall acuity is well documented as a ramification of ageing. Along with impaired colour vision, increased susceptibility to glare, and reduced contrast sensitivity, the visual deficits call for a special effort to make optical presentations highly visible. Intensity differences for both colour and contrast combinations have to be sufficiently pronounced in order to be easily detectable. Compensation strategies mainly involve avoiding certain colour combinations and very small content. Elders should also be spared from having to detect low contrasts and fine hues. Finally, reductions in the ability to finely control intricate movements can be seen as a result of declines in fine motor control and skin sensitivity. These developments may impede the very precise handling of pointing and input devices. 41 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind The material reviewed on attention has identified task complexity as an important determinant of performance´, especially for elders. Closely related are findings on the coordination and simultaneous execution of multiple tasks, where the performance of elders declines particularly sharply. Furthermore, distractions in visual search tasks have a stronger impact on elders. In the case of visual searching, singular identification characteristics for targets and location cuing could form a basis for a viable strategy to compensate age-induced deficiencies. The age-related decline in memory capacity is a factor possibly affecting several cognitive domains, but the direct consequence is that less information can be held directly available. Another problem field in the context of memory is recollection, the ability to recall contextual information, which has been shown to decline. Closely related to memory, complex learning material is another source of difficulty for elders. Favouring recognition over recollection may in some cases be a possibility to overcome memory problems. For learning materials, a good structure and references to existing knowledge have been identified as facilitating factors. The finding that elders pay undue attention to irrelevant detail in problem solving draws up a parallel to the results from the domain of attention. Other observations in this field are a lack of confidence in self-generated problem solutions and decisions, which parallels findings from chapter 2 on a lack of confidence many senior individuals have in their abilities. Furthermore, elders may exhibit a reduced likelihood of abandoning a previously successful solution strategy that fails or is no longer optimal and a tendency to consider less options. The numerous problem fields identified in this chapter can easily paint a rather bleak picture of the abilities of elder individuals. It is a noteworthy conclusion of ageing research, though, that increasing age also brings with it increasing diversity. There may be individuals to whom several of these age-related reductions apply, but very few will exhibit all of them. Furthermore, the performance declines associated with normal ageing usually are without serious consequences for everyday life in many fields, especially in the cognitive domain. Many individuals can be expected to retain key 42 3 Selected Ageing Effects on the Human Body and Mind abilities into their seventies or eighties and possibly further. Generalisations aimed at the whole of the elder populace are quite difficult to make, especially with regard to the cognitive processing performance. 43 4 Web Usability Recommendations The following sections will present guidelines and findings on Web design and usability that address special requirements elder users may have and also includes relevant material that is not directly age-specific. Several different approaches were considered to organise these usability recommendations. For example, Beier & Vaughan (2003) propose a framework for use with cross-product software design guidelines, which focuses strongly on the properties of the “product”. Instead, six sections aggregate the most important recommendations, comments, and extensions and the section titles come quite close to the initially presented definition of design princples. In some cases like section 4.5, the titles strongly resemble the examples cited in section 1.2. Nevertheless, most of them allude to the specific conditions of the Web and mirror the requirements of elder users in an attempt to bridge between these two aspects. A final section presents four short exemplary cases made on Web sites of recent date and reasonable interest to the group of elder users. 4.1 Facilitate Perception As mentioned earlier, the Web is still mainly a visual medium. Most of the reviewed sources mirror this state of affairs and place a focus on visual aspects. The following passages first deal with the readable presentation of text on computer displays, then cover the issues regarding illustrative graphics and colour. Finally, they comment on audio- and multi-media contents, albeit briefly. 44 4 Web Usability Recommendations 4.1.1 Text Formatting To address the problem field of acuity, numerous sources recommend the use of large, easy-to-read type for texts published on the Web and intended to be read by elders. Contrary to expectations, Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, p. 33) found that text size did not significantly affect the performance of elder users in their usability study—an outcome they attribute to the increased amount of space the larger text requires and the ensuing necessity to scroll. Nevertheless, elder-specific guideline sets typically recommend using a sans-serif type, sized to match 12–14 point on paper. Bernard et al. (2001) report that in their comparison of reading speed and user preference for sans-serif versus serif type of 12 and 14 point size, the sans-serif was the preferred typeface in all sizes and read faster at 12 points, whereas the serif type was read faster in 14 point size but never preferred. In contrast to these findings, Lynch & Horton (2004b, § 4), generally advise the use of serif typefaces adapted for screen use (in this case Times New Roman or Georgia) for body text on Web pages, especially such pages as are expected to be printed rather than read on the screen. With recent Web technology, it should be no trouble to specify an alternative typeface in a version optimised for printing. Some publications (see, e. g.: Holt 2000, p. 3; Bollwage 2005, p. 62) specifically denounce the use of italicised type on screens, because the usually low display resolution (in most cases 72–96 dpi) produces a stair effect on the slanted lines and makes the text hard to read. For emphasise, boldface is recommended instead. On paragraph level, the elder-specific guidelines generally call for ragged left justification to be used instead of full or centre justification, advise that line length be kept short (to 35–50 characters), and that line spacing be increased. These recommendations do not directly address any of the previously identified problem fields, but by minimizing the necessary eye movements and lessening the danger of loosing the line, they facilitate reading (see, e. g.: Bollwage 2005, p. 52). The Web imposes limits, however, on the control of typographic appearance. Line breaking is particularly troublesome to control, as fluctuations caused by screen resolution, the operating system and browser software, and the preferences set therein make the out- 45 4 Web Usability Recommendations come very difficult to predict. Griffiths (2004, § 1.4) demonstrates how the use of relative measures in size definitions can remedy many design issues while at the same time improving usability by supporting browser-side text resizing. Recommendations on text background address problems in contrast vision and glare. To maximise the contrast, the obvious choice is black text on a white background without patterns of any kind. Using pure white as background, however, can cause problems on its own. This touches the problem field of glare: Bollwage (2005, p. 62) points out that because a computer screen basically resembles a lamp, small text, text in a typeface with thin strokes, or text in a typeface with pronounced differences in stroke strength is particularly tiresome to read on a screen; he advises less bright shades (e. g. beige) for use as text background and a sans-serif typeface with sufficiently strong strokes. Similarly, Ellis & Kurniawan (2000, p. 270) reports a light yellow background for text presentation as the final result in his participatory design study. Using actual colours as text backgrounds, even if they are off-white as suggested above, touches the problem field of colour vision. Shades of green, blue, or violet should therefore not be the colours used to tint the background of text on the Web if one of these colours appears in the foreground. For illustration, consider Holt’s (2000, p. 3) example of blue-coloured links embedded in green text or picture such links in front of a greenish background; such a combination could seriously impede usability. 4.1.2 Colours and Graphics Elder-specific guideline sets ask designers to generally avoid the critical colour combinations in the blue-green (see, e. g.: Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131) or blue-green-yellow (see, e. g.: NIA & NLM 2002, p. 6) ranges, thereby addressing the problem field of colour vision. Becker (2004a, p. 20) says that in general, combinations of colours that are spectrally similar cause difficulty in distinguishing between them and combinations of highly saturated colours in close proximity are very tiring to the eye because they require it to refocus continuously. Lynch & Horton (2004a) state that 46 4 Web Usability Recommendations because ». . . most Web users have computers and monitors set to “thousands” or “millions” of colors . . .«, adhering to a Web-save colour palette of only 216 colours is no longer necessary to ensure proper page display in all settings. Nevertheless, a conservative use of colour on Web pages, as it is advertised by many guideline collections (see, e. g.: Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131) may actually improve its power as a highlighting measure. Where a change of colour finds use to encode some form of feedback on user actions, this change needs to be sufficiently intensive, so as to be easily determinable. This is most relevant for the display of click targets and will be discussed in detail elsewhere (see subsection 4.2.3). The material reviewed in the course of this work typically advises against graphics except for illustrative purposes in direct relation to the textual content. In keeping with established accessibility standards, graphics should be equipped with alternative texts or even longer descriptions, as appropriate. Additionally, any informational content encoded in colours must be accessible in an alternative form. This consideration is obviously not limited to making pages accessible to elder individuals, but is covered in accessibility requirements addressing the different forms of colour vision impairment. Various software tools (see, e. g.: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia 2006a, External_Links for a listing) are available to access the visibility of Web pages under various conditions of colour vision and could prove a valuable tool in identifying any colour-related usability issues. m 4.1.3 Audio- and Multi-Media Content The US National Institutes of Health provide a distinct senior health Web site (NIA & NLM 2006) augmented with synthetic speech output; it reads the labels of links and buttons when the mouse pointer hovers over them and content text upon a click on the passage. Apart from such far-reaching efforts, the importance of audio or audio-visual content as informational content is very definitely second to text and graphics. Only Holt (2000, p. 7) comments on the elder-specific aspects of audio content and her suggestions are in line with those already pointed out in section 3.4: presenting sounds »one at a time« and avoiding background noise or distortions in 47 4 Web Usability Recommendations speech presentations. For multi-medial content, suggestions by Bieri et al. (2006, p. 21) on the shaping of audio- and audio-visual advertising material apply: rapid sequences, cuts, and parallel presentation of information are unsuitable for elders. These recommendations draw upon findings on attention (parallel processing deficits) and information processing as well. 4.2 Facilitate Handling The title of this section is very nearly figurative language, because the following passages relate mainly to the problem field of intricate movements, although the section on click targets has a strong visual component as well. Mouse interactions will be the main point of interest, but typing input on a computer keyboard may also present some serious difficulties, especially to those elders new to the world of computers. 4.2.1 Scrolling versus Paging The question whether scrolling down long pages or clicking through a sequence of several short ones is the best solution to present longer passages of content has in the past enticed vigorous controversy. The position of the fold, where the bottom end of the screen cuts off a page that is too long to display completely in the browser window, is not easily determinable because it varies with such parameters as display resolution, window size, typeface, type size, and other system-specific settings on the users’ computers. Depending on the length of the page, this can hide a considerable portion of the content or functionality of the page. Therefore, particularly Nielsen has been known to argue strongly in favour of chunking single long pages into several smaller ones and to avoid the necessity of scrolling vertically (see, e. g.: Nielsen 1996, Nielsen 1997, Nielsen 2000) as much as possible. Even though Nielsen (2000, pp. 112/115) acknowledges that scrolling is a problem especially for pages intended for navigation and not so severe for pages displaying lengthy text content, the idea that users generally are unlikely to scroll downward still holds considerable sway (see, e. g.: Becker 2004a, p. 394). Summarising various contributions made on 48 4 Web Usability Recommendations this topic, Wirth (2004, pp. 161–163) points out that there are affirmative arguments for both paradigms (scrolling as well as paging) and the best choice very likely depends on the audience, purpose, and content of the individual Web site. Obviously, the negative or positive effects of scrolling versus paging can be overcome in various ways. For example, a site could provide several different views of the same page (e. g. a paginated and a monolithic variant) and leave the choice to the users—a favourable approach, according to Wirth (2004, p. 165), though it is important to note that it also increases the number of items displayed on the page and thus the complexity of the interface. Consider as a possible alternative the hybrid solution proposed by Gustafson (2004). The reviewed guidelines and research materials generally advise against the use of scrolling (see, e. g.: Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131) for several reasons. Gappa & Nordbrock (2004, pp. 83/84) re-emphasise the “users-do-not-scroll” argument because they observed that the elder users in their study did indeed often miss information presented below the fold. Another compelling reason is the requirement of fine motor movement for controlling the scroll operation. Even a scrolling wheel or a comparable device, which by now is a standard feature of computer mice and allows the scrolling with a single finger movement, has been identified as a potential usability issue with elders (see: Aula 2005, p. 77). Depending on the browser software, the Up-, Down-, Page-Up-, and Page-Down-keys may allow to control the scrolling, but they require the users to move from the mouse to the keyboard as primary interaction device. Without a wheel mouse or using the keyboard, the users can either click the scroll bar buttons to scroll down by line, or click and hold the slider and freely move it upor downwards. Hawthorne (2003, p. 41) emphatically criticises that the above scroll bar elements present tiny targets1 that are by default coloured in shades of grey and exhibit a default behaviour that may constitute a source of error and, consequently, frustration to some elder users. Note the reference to the problem fields of colour and contrast vision. Unfortunately, the appearance of the scroll bars depends on the settings of the operating 1 the slider becomes smaller when more content is below the fold 49 4 Web Usability Recommendations system and is not reliably customisable through the use of Web technology. Hawthorne (2003, p. 41) furthermore argues that the movement of the text in the window, which is the most visible feedback of scroll bar use, may actually cause confusion in elder users who could loose track of their position within the page. Because scroll bars appear at the very edge of the screen in the users’ visual periphery, they might easily be overlooked (see e. g.: Hawthorne 2003, p. 41). This further strengthens the argument against scrolling Web pages because a scroll bar alone is a barely visible hint for further content below the fold. Also, other page elements like horizontal rules or borders might prematurely suggest that there is no more relevant content (see: Spool et al. 1999, p. 78). A possible remedy could be dedicated scrolling buttons (see, e. g.: Badre 2002, p. 102) to facilitate interaction for elder users. They are larger targets than the standard-issue scroll bar elements and could also provide additional hints on content hidden beneath the fold. 4.2.2 ›Seek-and-Hide‹ Menus For similar reasons already mentioned in the context of scrolling and scroll bars, many sources (see, e. g.: NIA & NLM 2002, p. 9; Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131) advise against the use, or at least against the extensive use of menu-like contraptions for Web navigation. This refers to page elements mimicking the functionality of menu bars as typically featured in computer software where they roll out a list of functions upon a mouse click. On the Web, such constructions typically display navigation options when the pointer is above the visible menu element. Like their software counterparts, they can present a multi-level hierarchy. The labelling of such constructions as ›Seek-and-Hide‹ menus alludes to their behaviour: users have to seek out the menu options to see what they contain; the item lists hide themselves again as soon as the pointer moves away. Indeed, even in the context of computer applications, the suitability of such menus for elder users has been called into question (see, e. g.: Ellis & Kurniawan 2000, p. 273; Hawthorne 2003, p. 40). The main criticism against menus on the Web, which can be deployed both vertically and 50 4 Web Usability Recommendations horizontally, is again the fine mouse movement necessary to successfully control them and to select the desired option—especially where multiple levels of selection, small type for the labels, or both of these factors are involved. Elder users may be unable to hold the mouse steady enough for the sub-menus to display continuously and due to unsteady movement, hitting targets may be much more difficult. The standard-issue drop-down fields provided by HTML, which display only a single element of a list and upon a single mouse click roll out the alternative options, have faced criticism on the ground of hiding all but one option in the closed position and offering only a limited amount of control on the formatting and grouping of the entries, when employed as a navigation device. 4.2.3 Click Targets To increase the target size and to give more elaborate feedback on target acquisition were the primary motives to use stylised graphic buttons (dimensions: 120 × 22 pixels) labelled exclusively with text for the main navigation in Ellis & Kurniawan’s (2000, p. 271) iterative design experiment. The motives in this case soundly reflect the findings on stimulus intensity, which detail that in order to detect a difference, elders require larger changes. Elder users are likely to miss very subtle optical hints and difficulties with intricate movements call for a distinctive hint of target acquisition. Thanks to advances in Web technology, the initially mentioned solution can by now be considered obsolete: Appearance changes even for normal text links can be controlled very elaborately and effects such as indentation, contrast reversal, typeface and size, colour changes, or within lists possibly even graphical indicators can be added to make one element stand out from a group of similar elements or from its immediate background and surroundings. Of course, the arrangement has to maintain readability in the changed appearance as well and using more than one of the aforementioned options is advisable for increased effectiveness. Other observations made by Ellis & Kurniawan (2000, p. 271) teach a very interesting lesson about white space around click targets: with their click on a target link, which was part of an unordered list of insufficiently 51 4 Web Usability Recommendations spaced items, the elder users not only failed to hit the link they wanted to follow, they accidentally hit an adjacent link and thus were taken to a page they did not expect. Depending on the label and description of the link, such a mistake may even go unnoticed and thereby cause considerable confusion and frustration. The individual links should appear with a sufficient amount of white space around them to avoid this problem. List bullets and carefully chosen, meaningful icons can add to the usability of links, for in their appearance, they often come to resemble buttons. To further facilitate the navigation for elder users and to reduce the likelihood of error and confusion, Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, p. 36) recommend that bullets and icons be incorporated into adjacent links, where applicable; their participants frequently attempted to click on such elements as well as other non-links like headings. Because even allegedly obvious iconography like a reading glass for “Search” or a house for “Home page” may not convey the intended message, Wirth (2004, p. 97) as well as many guideline sets strongly discourage omitting a textual component and employing icons as links on their own. Without a label, the icon element is bound to loose size and with that visual saliency; a combination of icon and textual label has the additional benefit of familiarising the users with the meaning of the icon and it may thus facilitate recognition on different Web sites (knowledge transfer). 4.2.4 Input Elements and Keyboard Use Aula (2005, pp. 75–76) reports on various observed difficulties elder users had with the text-editing elements of a search interface: laborious text typing, lost input due to typing while the focus was not in the text input element, frequent typing errors, difficulties in correcting typing errors, in positioning the caret, and in deleting old queries. All of these observations directly relate to the problem field of intricate movement and are relevant for designing input forms for other purposes than search. To remedy these problems, Aula (2005, p. 76) asks for a distinct »clear search box« button to be provided as well as larger type size and enhanced visual or audio feedback indicating the presence or absence of the input 52 4 Web Usability Recommendations focus in text input elements. A larger typeface here addresses not only visual acuity deficits: if the single characters are larger, it is easier to position the caret between them. With present-day Web technologies at least, the typeface and size in text input elements can be modified normally and the elements should also scale accordingly to accommodate the intended number of characters. Adding an auditory cue for “unfocussed” typing may be an issue best addressed at the level of the browser software, although Web-based solutions are probably possible, too. Furthermore, it may be advisable to break larger forms into logical units and present them sequentially. Because typing may be a tedious task for elder users, especially if they are unfamiliar with keyboards, typed text should not be lost when the user switches between screens in such sequences. 4.3 Reduce the Likelihood of Distraction Advertisements are doubtlessly distractions of the purest form and many Web sites that offer free services use them for financial reasons. Advertisements embedded into pages alongside normal elements such as navigation or content as well as graphic ornaments are, essentially, distractions in a display that has to be visually searched. They can also have considerable impact on the amount of time needed to load a page. In a broader interpretation of the term, distractions occur also in the course of normal Web site navigation. The following passages will assess findings on different forms of distractions users may have to face on the Web and also point out possible ways to make Web browsing with distractions easier for elder users, who are particularly susceptible to their negative influence. 4.3.1 Advertisements Although animation can have illustrative applications, it is most commonly used on the Web to embellish advertisements. Wirth (2004, pp. 130–131) underscores the negative effect non-illustrative moving items can have on users, quite regardless of the recipient’s age; he argues that persistent movement will disrupt any individual’s concentration, because it has the high- 53 4 Web Usability Recommendations est priority for the allocation of attention. The fact that parallel tasks present elder users with increasing difficulty emphasises the ensuing difficulty even more. Therefore, elder users should benefit excessively from sites that choose to abstain from using animated advertisements altogether. If possible, static or solely text-based ads are to be preferred, if their display is necessary for financial reasons. Web advertisements are likely to be more effective when they are displayed interspersed with content at irregular locations or in formatting akin to navigation elements. They are also, however, much harder to distinguish from relevant page content. Accessing the relevance of a visual stimulus has been shown to take elder users longer. Thus, the above-mentioned practise inconveniences elder users more severely than younger users. In fact, this is the inverse of the possible compensation strategy already mentioned in section 3.4. Most authors specifically advise to consistently place advertisements in the page margin only, as their appearance in the periphery can make their presence less invasive (see, e. g.: Badre 2002, p. 92), and argue in favour of clear and consistent formatting and placement of links and navigation elements. Placing the advertisements of a Web site consistently in a certain area can act as a location cue in that the actual site navigation and content will appear elsewhere on the screen (e. g. everywhere but at the left page margin). Observations made in the study by Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, p. 34) suggest that not including a right-margin advertisement space in the minimal dimensions of a page (the minimum screen resolution or window size at which the page displays without horizontal scroll bars), can allow the users to see the page without any distracting advertisements, for the test users in the study were often not aware of the additional content in windows with horizontal scroll bars. However, horizontal scroll bars are usually considered a major design flaw and the benefits of such practise obviously depend too much on the configuration of the users’ computers to constitute a reliable strategy to reduce the distracting effects of advertisements. A very interesting alternative to traditional Web advertisements could be to offer access to the more elaborate services of a Web site after the user has 54 4 Web Usability Recommendations watched a short commercial. How the commercial presents the message to the audience could be varied according to the clients’ needs and preferences as well as the bandwidth and multi-media capacities of the user’s browser software. An example for this approach can be seen at the Merriam-Webster Online Web site (Merriam-Webster Inc. 2006), which offers free access to its normally restricted services after a flash presentation or a timed slide show of static graphics. Afterwards, the screen does not contain any advertising. Presenting advertisements in this manner allows users to stay in control of their actions. They can also deal with advertisements and the target Web site sequentially, thereby ridding themselves of the otherwise necessary parallel processing effort for Web pages containing ads. 4.3.2 Ornamentation Many of the reviewed sources on elder-specific Web design encourage modesty in the decorative application of colour, graphics or multi-media effects. The over-enthusiastic use of colour or graphic measures to visually highlight important information threatens the effectiveness of such measures and increases the complexity of the display, thus making visual searches more difficult. As already discussed in the context of advertisements, animation has considerable potential for drawing attention and should therefore not be present, except if it is informative in nature and directly relating to the other content of the page. Even then, in the spirit of the recommendation made in section 4.4, they should not start without the users’ informed consent. As already hinted in the introduction of this section, the likely limitations on the available bandwidth further discourage the excessive use of ornamental elements of any kind. 4.3.3 Disruptive Navigation Some navigation options have been identified as particularly confusing especially for elder users. Page-internal links that move downward in a longer document, possibly by several screen-fulls, may cause the user to loose track of their position because the page header and regular navigation controls 55 4 Web Usability Recommendations can scroll out of view (see, e. g.: Ellis & Kurniawan 2000, p. 269). For similar reasons, targeting page-internal link targets directly from a different page, so that following the link displays the link target directly as scrolled to the corresponding position with the header and navigation controls possibly out of view can be expected to have a similarly disorienting effect on elder users. This is also an additional argument to favour pagination into a series of smaller chunks over longer documents with a list of sub-headings as page-internal targets (see subsection 4.2.1). A slightly different source of confusion are links referring to the same page in which they appear (such as a working “Home Page”-link on the home page itself); they may cause users to look for changes that actually did not happen (see, e. g.: Chadwick-Dias et al. 2003, p. 34). 4.4 Keep Users in Control The issues discussed in this section deal with actions carried out by the browser or system software without being explicitly initiated by the users. Such actions can take the users out of the context of their immediate task or require them to deal with a largely unrelated situation. In some respects, this is reminiscent of the proverbial dog that takes its owner for a walk. The findings especially from chapter 2 strongly suggest that a feeling of being in control is particularly helpful for elder users. Unexpected developments and intrusions should be expected to undermine the users’ confidence in their ability to successfully execute their intended actions. The following passages address commonly encountered aspects of Web sites that constitute possible threats to the users’ feeling of control. 4.4.1 External Applications, Downloads, and Plug-Ins Information in various formats not directly displayable in the browser software is often part of Web sites. Such content requires additional software to display properly, either within a page, within the browser window, or by being opened in a new window of external applications, such as a PDF viewer or player software for sounds, music, or video. Any shortcomings on 56 4 Web Usability Recommendations the user’s side in terms of missing plug-ins or software or even the unexpected opening of such external applications may confront novice and elder users with severe difficulties and cause confusion—especially if unexpected additional dialogues, warnings, or error messages ensue (see: Aula 2005, p. 75). Also doubtlessly confusing are any automatically initiated download processes or redirects to pages from which to download and install the missing software pieces. Such behaviour confronts the users with the results of actions they did not approve and thus takes the initiative out of their hands. A gentle notice should be supplied for any failed attempts to display such content within a page, stating what was supposed to be displayed, why it is not there, and giving very brief instructions on how to fix the problem. In these circumstances, it might be helpful to point out a swift way of returning to the page with the embedded content (e. g. to make a bookmark), because the user would very likely want to see it again with the additional content enabled. If content requiring external applications is made available via links, the label and additional descriptions should at least notify the users that this may open in a different application or result in a download. 4.4.2 Pop-Ups and New Browser Windows Opening pop-up windows or new browser windows makes using the Web unnecessarily difficult for elder users, for they may miss the opening of a new window altogether and then find themselves wondering why the cannot get back to the previous page or where the browser controls have gone (see, e. g.: Chadwick-Dias et al. 2003, p. 37). Some authors report that occasionally their test users did not notice a transition to a different Web site. Hence, there are guidelines asking for a warning to be incorporated into the link, when this is the case. Opening a fresh browser window to emphasise the following of links to different Web sites is rather too subtle a hint and doing so breaks the correct functioning of the “Back”-button, which is basically an undo-mechanism, relatively easy to understand and quite effective. The management of several application windows has been 57 4 Web Usability Recommendations shown to be a source of difficulty especially for novice elder users (see, e. g.: Ellis & Kurniawan 2000, p. 271), but the “Back”-button very probably is one of the prime strategies for this user-group to get back to a known page on a Web site. With respect to the findings on strategy shifts, the recommendation clearly is not to open new windows and pop-ups unless there really is absolutely no other way for a Web-based application to work properly. In such cases, the links resulting in the new windows or pop-ups should be labelled accordingly. For example, opening a pop-up window simply to display a printer-friendly variant of the current page is probably not necessary. Equipping the printable page with a single link to go exactly one step backwards does not impede its “printability”. 4.5 Reduce Complexity As summarised in section 3.4 complexity has been identified as a very important influence factor for many aspects of cognitive performance. Admittedly, the title of this section is rather imprecise. This merely reflects the fact that there are also many sources of complexity. Good structure as well as references to present knowledge have been identified as possible strategies to reduce complexity. The discussion in this section will first focus on text and language. Afterwards, it moves on to Web site structure and considers complexity with regard to the layout of individual Web pages. A final passage will comment on the use of site-specific help and contact features. 4.5.1 Text and Language With regard to text length on Web pages, Krug (2006, p. 45) coins the very provocative sentence: »Get rid of half the words on each page, then get rid of half what’s left.« Although he does not make any assumptions about the targeted Web audience, the general thrust of his statement appears to have particular validity for elder Web users. In the first of their consecutive usability studies, Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, p. 34) observed a tendency in their elder participants to read most, sometimes all of the text presented 58 4 Web Usability Recommendations on the screen, especially when they were asked to fill in forms. Even though the authors did not report whether or not a similar behaviour occurred in their follow-up study with a modified design and also did not identify a specific influence factor responsible for this behaviour, their observation encourages concise writing, nevertheless. The complexity of text content is a concern investigated by Becker (2004b, pp. 400–401) in a study of Web-based health information resources; she suspects that highly complex texts2 may prove a barrier to older adults, owing to both an age-related decline in text comprehension and the lower level of formal education. Note that these assumptions provide an interesting counterpoint to the not elder-specific work of Spool et al. (1999, pp. 70–71), who found that user success was associated with sites that contain highly complex texts3 . The purpose of the page in question, for example navigation versus a detailed presentation of content, would of course tremendously influence how concise or complex the text can or should be. In this respect, a guideline calling for simplicity in the sense of the assessment metrics and small amounts of text on pages at the same time may constitute a contradiction in itself. Detailed recommendations regarding composition style should transfer from other domains such as technical writing to the writing for Web content in general. The reviewed age- and Web-specific material mostly emphasises the need for text clarity. As Bieri et al. (2006, p. 27) conclude for advertisements and promotional material aimed at elders, messages and information of textual nature presented to elder users on the Web should be phrased unambiguously, without the need to make inferences or anticipate the meaning of the message, and should draw on knowledge the user already has. Extending these suggestions, the NIA & NLM (2002, p. 7) checklist instructs writers to use positive statements and active voice constructions; both measures work to make sentences easier to understand and are well-established guidelines for technical writers. 2 3 rated as complex in automatically measured reading complexity metrics complexity again rated on automatically calculated metrics 59 4 Web Usability Recommendations Without any doubt, however, the use of jargon and unexplained technical terms that elders as well as Internet novices in general are likely to be unaware of, can be a source of confusion and frustration (see, e. g.: Chadwick-Dias et al. 2003, p. 34). Without reference to age-specific issues and addressing the situation for German Web users, Wirth (2004, pp. 58– 59) argues that many words and expressions commonly used on the Web leave startling percentages of users without any idea as to what they mean; he presents the word »Sitemap [!]« (unknown to 45 %) as an example for a term describing something of considerable relevance and potential benefit. Another example given by Wirth (2004, p. 58) relates to many users’ unawareness of the meaning of the expression »Skip Intro« (unknown to 39 %), which is notoriously used to label a link that bypasses the showing of an animated introductory trailer and leads to the actual home page of a Web site. Similar observations on the use of computer and Internet terminology occurred to Aula (2005, p. 74), who notes that even for messages presented in the users’ native language, ». . . the meaning . . . was not understandable . . .«. These illustrations clearly show how seemingly unobtrusive terms can become potential pitfalls for novice or elder users. If the use of such terminology is unavoidable, the Web site must provide easily understandable explanations, possibly by means of a glossary. 4.5.2 Site Structure Most Web sites can be expected to present their content in some form of hierarchical structure which might also feature internal cross-references via links that are not part of the main navigation. With reference to elders, recommendations exist which call for no more than two hierarchical levels on a Web site (see: Gappa & Nordbrock 2004, pp. 83/84) or plainly state »Do not use a deep hierarchy . . .« (Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131). The background appears to be the increasing anxiety of the elder users of »getting lost« within the structure of the Web site or loosing track of the navigation steps already made (Gappa & Nordbrock 2004, p. 83). Suggestions to keep the structure of Web sites shallow, however, are not limited to elders; consider Rosenfeld & Morville (2002, pp. 67–69), who recommend 60 4 Web Usability Recommendations to generally favour a shallow and broad over a deep and narrow hierarchy and advise to use two or three levels at most. These recommendations base on research by Larson & Czerwinski (1998, p. 30), who found that in relatively broad but shallow hierarchies (16 and 32 nodes, 2 levels), the search performance of the participants was better than in a narrow but deeper hierarchy (8 options, 3 levels)—results these authors attribute to the fact that making decisions about navigation options took more time than a visual search of the display. Interestingly, Larson & Czerwinski (1998, p. 30) found no correlation between short term memory and search performance in the narrow-deep hierarchy assessments, as opposed to their other settings, where both memory and visual search ability were relevant influences. Based on their own experimemts, Gappa & Nordbrock (2004, p. 84) also recommend limiting the main navigation options on a page to six which, they acknowledge, can come into conflict with their other recommendation of avoiding hierarchies with more than two levels. Conflicting, as these notions may be, they soundly reflect that age-related deficits may be present in both visual searching ability and memory. Limiting the organisation of a Web site in this manner is not always possible; larger Web sites often require more hierarchy levels to organise their content and problems regarding navigation also manifest in the development process as well, as can be concluded from Krug’s (2006, p. 70) remarks that material delivered for usability evaluation often contains site charts showing a four-tier hierarchy and design samples for the home page and navigation and content pages for the first two levels. These comments nicely commemorate the problem that occurs after the first two levels of a hierarchy have been incorporated into the Web site navigation: a Web page has only two natural directions along which category listings might be displayed easily; these are from left to right and from top to bottom.4 Afterwards, some form of aggregation is necessary to accommodate all navigation options, for example in a “Windows-Explorer”-like tree navigation, and showing too many sub-nodes may easily result in cluttered displays. For such deeper hierar4 Note that for non-western Web sites, other conventions such as right-to-left may apply. 61 4 Web Usability Recommendations chies, Bernard (2003) recommends a concave hierarchy: a larger number of options at the top and bottom level of the structure and fewer choices in between. The organisational dilemma described in the initial paragraph remains largely unsolvable except for small Web sites with limited amounts of content, where such considerations are of less immediate relevance. The observations made with regard to elder-specific usability issues, however, highlight the importance of both devising a carefully organised structure and presenting it in a way that prevents the users from being overwhelmed by choices and getting lost. 4.5.3 Page Layout A very well-established design fundamental is using various visual cues available to construct a meaningful visual hierarchy that can effectively guide the users’ attention to the information or functionality they are looking for. In keeping with research findings on attention and location cuing, guidelines for elder-friendly Web design strongly emphasise the importance of layout simplicity and spatial consistency: the consistent placement of interface elements on all pages, throughout the Web site. The wording usually refers directly to navigational elements, but a clear and above all predictable visual alignment of page elements in general certainly improves the usability of the site especially for elder or novice users, because it facilitates the transfer of acquired knowledge (see, e. g.: Nayak et al. 2006, p. 8). This remark has not only site-internal scope, but applies also to knowledge acquired on the Web in general. Thus, honouring wellestablished Web conventions regarding the placement of page elements will allow all users to continue using well-learned strategies. The use of elaborate content management systems for most larger Web sites usually ensures that at least navigation elements appear in a consistent form and placement. Using a page layout on a home page that is significantly different from the rest of the Web site (see, e. g.: Krug 2006, pp. 107–109), for example by displaying a horizontal navigation bar for the main site categories on the home page and a vertical navigation bar on the left margin every- 62 4 Web Usability Recommendations where else, may not be advisable, as this layout change can cause confusion, especially if the labelling is inconsistent as well. For reasons already discussed in subsection 4.2.1 on problems with Web pages that require scrolling, placing any important or potentially helpful page elements (e. g. utilities to resize the text, to print, to access the search or contact information) below the fold or even at the bottom of the page should be considered a remarkably bad idea. The place for such elements is most certainly within the header where they are prominently visible at all but the tiniest window sizes and also directly available. 4.5.4 Help and Contact Some of the reviewed guideline sets (see: Holt 2000, p. 5; Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131) recommend dedicated help pages and a collection of frequently asked questions5 to cover problems with the Web site itself as well as with the provided content, products, or services. The idea of providing instructions or even a manual on how to use an individual Web site may to some appear as somewhat contrary to usage strategies commonly applied on the Web (see e. g.: Krug 2006, p. 26). Nevertheless, such practice may help to reduce the user’s uncertainty, according to Singh & Pereira (2005, pp. 100–101) who bring up such site-specific tutorials in the context of cultural and national differences in Web usage. A possible form for such endeavours is characterised by Rosenfeld & Morville (2002, p. 126) as ». . . guided tours, tutorials, and micro-portals focussed around a specific audience, topic, or task.« Providing information on how to contact the entity behind a site is in the best interest of the owner and also, to some extent, a legal requirement. Elder users, as has been discussed in chapter 2, might be quite willing to look for information on-line, but some may prefer to conduct transactions “with real people” outside the Internet and would probably appreciate a prominently visibly opportunity of contacting a real person or doing enquiries in a way they prefer (e. g. by e-mail, telephone, fax, or mail). 5 typically labelled just with the acronym FAQ 63 4 Web Usability Recommendations 4.6 Provide a Sound Base for Navigation Decisions On Web sites, successful navigation is the key to reaching the desired information. Therefore, usable navigation elements are of paramount importance for user satisfaction (see: Nayak et al. 2006, p. 8). Metaphors from the real world have frequently found use in computer and information technology to tap into the existing knowledge of users and thereby to facilitate the understanding of the technology. Metaphors might be employed to mimic the behaviour of real world items or to organise information. For example, the quite literal human resource office metaphor implemented in the experiment described by Gourmelon (1994, pp. 187–189) manages to effectively hide the complexity of the underlying computer system and taps the domain-specific knowledge of the intended user group. To maintain a universally accessible Web site, however, only general knowledge can be considered a reliable resource for suitable interface metaphors. Still, some elements known from established media, most notably from printed media like books, magazines, or newspapers can offer useful inspirations and also might be helpful in overcoming some of the naming difficulties uncovered previously. This can be particularly helpful for several navigation tools relating to a Web site in general, which have been identified as valuable aids to elder users by facilitating navigation and information finding. The first subsection discusses how such tools and different navigation paradigms can address the requirements of elder users. The second subsection then reviews findings on the smallest possible navigation element, the individual link, and especially its labelling. 4.6.1 Navigation Paradigms and Tools Numerous paradigms exist to present the main navigation options of a Web site. One of the most successful, if well designed, is the tab set6 ; many sites use the tab metaphor to present their main information categories, often in combination with colour coding (see: Krug 2006, p. 79). Most elder-specific guidelines reviewed here do not specifically endorse a distinct way to im6 as known from filing cabinets 64 4 Web Usability Recommendations plement the navigation, but they give rationales against some, which have been discussed in subsection 4.2.2. Kurniawan & Zaphiris (2005, p. 131), however, include a guideline specifically asking for links to appear in a bullet list. Present-day Web technologies can customise the appearance of such lists and their items to a remarkable degree, and the result manages to simultaneously mark-up the list content correctly (important with regard to accessibility for non-visual Web browsing devices) and display it in a visually appealing manner, for example in the form of a tab set or a vertical navigation bar. For the vertical displays, it is advisable to have the entries preceded by some form of bullet or indicator to make them identifiable as distinct entries even when they have been wrapped across more than one line. Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, pp. 34/36) also emphasise the importance of clearly indicating the presently selected item in the navigation system. In line with the statements in subsection 4.2.3, more than one cue should be used for this. For deeply hierarchical Web sites, the use of a Breadcrumb-device, which displays links to the superior pages, is also very advisable to provide an additional cue about the present position on the Web site. For functions that require user input, Dickinson et al. (2005, p. 627) have found in the course of their development project that even long linear navigation paths with specific instructions displayed on each step page are a feasible solution for elder users. They acknowledge, however, that such an approach is suitable only for specific actions that can be broken down into a predictable sequence of smaller steps. When doing so, however, accompanying each step with instructions detailing the purpose of the sequence, the steps already done, the purpose of the current step, and the remaining steps in the sequence would help elder users to keep track of their activity. A very good example for this type of navigation is the order processing in an electronic shop, where the customer reviews the list of items in the shopping cart, provides information about delivery and payment, and finally proceeds to acknowledge the order placement. The NIA & NLM (2002, p. 10) checklist explicitly advocates the incorporation of “Previous page”- and “Next page”-buttons, which obviously make sense 65 4 Web Usability Recommendations within a sequence of pages only7 . Defining such a sequence for an entire Web site is seldom feasible, but at the lowest levels in information hierarchies with three or more tiers, where the individual pages probably are closely related, it could be an alternative to including yet another subset of links in the main navigation system. Nayak et al. (2006, p. 8) recommend to always provide a prominently visible link to the home page of a Web site. Apart from any such functionality already incorporated into a logo, a dedicated link would offer an escape route to “lost” users with an option of returning to a known page and would usefully complement the functionality of the “Back”-button. This is especially true for situations in which going one step backwards is not a viable solution, for example when the result of a Web search led directly to a lower-tier page of a Web site. One of the most important navigation tools, especially on larger Web sites, is a site-wide search facility. The participants in Gappa & Nordbrock’s (2004, p. 83) study often turned to the search box, some after they had not obtained the requested information by other means, some directly as the primary solution strategy—a finding which emphasises the importance of well-designed search facilities. Elder users may encounter unique problems when dealing with search engines or site-wide search functions, some of which were already discussed in subsection 4.2.4. Aula (2005, p. 77) notices structural difficulties such as understanding the scope of a search function (e. g. search this site versus search the Web) or dealing with the result display. Gappa & Nordbrock (2004, p. 84) also make detailed suggestions regarding the query handling: forgive spelling mistakes, accept declined nouns or conjugated verbs and suggest synonyms based on previous queries. Supplying a history of previously entered queries could also be a way to improve the handling of search facilities for elder users. According to Gappa & Nordbrock (2004, p. 84), especially the elder users in their study tried to transfer their knowledge of printed media and employ according search strategies like browsing from an index. This directly 7 with the possible exception of “Previous page”, which could also simply go one step back, just like the “Back”-button of the browser software. 66 4 Web Usability Recommendations points towards two auxiliary structures borrowed from printed media that might form a valuable alternative or complement to traditional search facilities: the Table of Contents and the Index. According to Rosenfeld & Morville (2002, pp. 121–122), the term Table of Contents was the original label used for site maps. Although some site maps do indeed deliver a map-like representation of their Web sites, the most common form is simply a structured listing of links to the main and subordinate sections. Site maps receive praise for providing a good overview of the contents of a Web site and direct access to information; they are usually named as very valuable navigation aids especially for elder users (see, e. g.: NIA & NLM 2002, p. 10; Kurniawan & Zaphiris 2005, p. 131). Depending on the nature and purpose of the Web site, a contrivance modelled after indices found in printed media (see, e. g.: Rosenfeld & Morville 2002, pp. 123–125) could also facilitate direct access to information through sorted keywords from various domains, (e. g. author, subject, location, or other categories as suitable), thereby bypassing the hierarchical structure. An Index represents a relatively simple and strictly linear search space and should thus be intuitively usable for elder users. As Rosenfeld & Morville (2002, p. 123) state, an index alone might be sufficient for smaller Web sites, whereas larger structures would likely require site maps and search functions additionally. Finally, a noticeably different marking for visited links to make them distinguishable from unvisited links provides a valuable memory cue for elder users and the reviewed guideline literature frequently advises such marking. It is possible to maintain several different colour schemes for links, either to make them more readable within the overall page design, or to convey additional information about the links nature. An example is the application of different colours to links within the displayed page, to other pages within the same Web site, or to different Web sites altogether. There are no established customs for such coloured markings, however, and thus any attempt to introduce one would probably require a legend to explain the meaning. Not all links, however, need to incorporate the “visited”feedback; for purely functional elements such links to access a site-wide functionality (e. g. the search, shopping cart, changing the type size) or 67 4 Web Usability Recommendations links continuing or backtracking a pre-defined sequence of steps, graphic buttons or icons with accompanying text may even be more efficient. 4.6.2 Link Labelling Displaying links with a sufficiently descriptive label that allows users to foresee what kind of content it leads to is not necessarily a requirement for elder users only. Spool et al. (1999, p. 33) determined two essential success criteria for links in their study: a link must allow users to ». . . predict where . . . [it] will lead« and to ». . . differentiate . . . [it] from other, nearby links.« Adding a short textual description for links that are otherwise unclearly labelled, proved to be a valuable hint on which users could base their navigation decisions (see: Spool et al. 1999, pp. 35–36). Based on experience with their model for the computational evaluation of Web site architecture, Miller & Remington (2004, p. 216) argue that the quality of the link labels, the degree of their ambiguity, very likely exerts a greater influence on the search performance in multi-tiered Web site hierarchies than the number of levels. With respect to the findings on problem-solving and decisionmaking, it is reasonable to assume that the low quality of links in terms of the criteria mentioned above will be particularly hindering for elder users. Present-day Web technologies offer various ways of displaying additional information not only for links, but for many other objects on Web pages by using the title-attribute. As a result of supplying such a title, a tool tip appears when the pointer hovers above the titled object. Hawthorne (2003, pp. 41–42) raises a number of valid concerns against the standard tool tip behaviour, such as unreadably minuscule type, unexpected and thus disruptive appearance, or obscuring the view of the controls below the hint. Hawthorne introduced a number of customisations on the level of the operating system to remedy these effects, but for Web pages, such solutions are hardly feasible. The accepted solution in Ellis & Kurniawan’s (2000, p. 271) experiment was based on hidden layers containing further descriptive text and made visible when the pointer touched the navigation buttons. The most easily available solution to this problem, however, would be a longer link label or a permanently visible descriptive sentence. 68 4 Web Usability Recommendations Apart from the global requirement of consistent terminology, there are some recommendations on the wording of link labels. Drawing on observations made in the course of their studies, Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, pp. 36/33) advise the use of »Action Word Links«, which presumably caused their participants to overcome the »Cautious Clicking«-behaviour exhibited in the baseline study. Essentially, this recommendation corresponds to the points made initially regarding descriptiveness, for the authors say that ». . . older adults . . . are much more likely to click links that explicitly tell them what will happen when they click« (Chadwick-Dias et al. 2003, p. 36). Pre-study evaluation results prompted Dickinson et al. (2005, p. 626) to reconsider the labelling of the buttons and links in their application: »Terms like “my home page” were removed because of uncertainty about who “my” referred to.«8 Instead, addressing the user directly in the third person appears to be a sufficiently clear alternative. Using the actual heading of a Web page as the label of the link leading to it and presenting this heading so that it makes the page easily identifiable is another example of a guideline well established aside from any agerelated considerations (see, e. g.: Badre 2002, p. 136). Doing so promotes consistency, as both the labels and the headings can be generated from the same source; this practice also provides an immediate control mechanism for the users, as to where they are and whether they followed the correct link. Note, however, that the page heading is not automatically the page title, which appears in the title bar of the browser window and will be used as the caption for any bookmarks made from a page. Including both the name of the Web site and the main heading of a page in its title would therefore provide a valuable memory cue to facilitate recognition in the browser bookmark list. Particularly on Web sites catering to audiences whose native language is not English, it is important to use understandable labelling for all navigation and function elements. For the “metaphorical” tools introduced in the previous subsection (site map and index), reverting their labels to the 8 Consider also, however, the clarity of the term “home page”, especially for novice users or those without any command of the English language. 69 4 Web Usability Recommendations local equivalent of Table of Contents and Index should greatly promote easy understanding and improve the usability by giving all users a clear idea of what to expect. Appropriate labels on German Web sites would be Inhaltsverzeichnis and Stichwortverzeichnis. It is at least doubtable that elder users would notice the potential helpfulness of these tools for navigating a Web site if the were to retain their Web-specific names. 4.7 Examples from the Web In this section, comments on a number of Web pages will serve as illustrations for some of the recommendations made in this chapter. They were chosen from Web sites likely to be within the sphere of interest of elder Web users, according to the findings in section 2.1. The comments are certainly not exhaustive, but they do show that a number of usability issues can be uncovered through application of the principles developed in this chapter. They also show that the problems identified in the course of this work do have their manifestations on the Web despite advances in technology and efforts to make information universally accessible. The screen shots given within the text are monochrome only and some may be too long to fit on a single page at a reasonable size. Appendix B. reprints all screen shots in colour and also supplies the full-length images, scaled to height. 4.7.1 News: spiegel.de The Web site depicted in Figure 4.1 uses a static layout optimised in width for a display resolution of 800 × 600 pixels. At that display setting, the vertical banner advertisement would not be visible. The main point of criticism in this example bases on the recommendations made in section 4.3 under the heading of ›Reduce the Likelihood of Distraction‹. Consider, the text-based advertisements in right column, directly adjacent to the column with the main articles. They bear a striking similarity to other elements with actual content in that column because they employ the very same formatting for their links. The top-most ad at least could easily be mistaken for a normal article, despite the different background colours for these ads 70 4 Web Usability Recommendations http://www.spiegel.de/wirtschaft/ Screenshot at 1024 × 786 display resolution. Retrieved 11 July 2006, 23:30 UTC+1 with Microsoft Internet Explorer 6. Figure 4.1: The economy section on the Web site of Spiegel Online. The black box indicates the approximate view on a 800 × 600 display, the line indicates the approximate position of the fold at 1024 × 786. See Appendix B.1. for a colour plate. (not the same for all, though) and the designation Anzeige (advertisement) in the top-right corner, which are the only markings applied to this kind of advertisement. These text-based ads as well as small flash-based ones occasionally appear within the text of articles, too. With respect to the recommendations from section 4.2 and section 4.5, the remarkable length of the page also inspire negative comment (see Appendix B.1. for the fulllength version). 71 4 Web Usability Recommendations http://www.docmorris.de/ Screenshot at 800 × 600 display resolution. Retrieved 11 July 2006, 17:36 UTC+1 with Microsoft Internet Explorer 6. Figure 4.2: The home page of the DocMorris Internet pharmacy. The black lines indicate the apprpximate position of the fold at resolutions of 800 × 600 and 1024 × 786 pixels. See Appendix B.2. for a colour plate. 4.7.2 On-line Pharmacy: docmorris.de The home page displayed in Figure 4.2 belongs to an Internet pharmacy based in Holland. At a first glance, it appears to be reasonably well suited for elder users: the controls are generously sized and the main intentions users are likely to have are prominently addressed and labelled with verbs. Thus, the site does relatively well, especially with regard to ›Facilitating Perception‹ (section 4.1) and ›Provide a Sound Base for Navigation Decisions‹ (section 4.6). Still, there are some minor points to be made for 72 4 Web Usability Recommendations each. The text resizing option resides well below the fold at a display resolution of 800 × 600 and also at the more generous 1024 × 786 pixels. The telephone number for direct contact to service staff is below the fold as well and the home page itself does not appear in the tab set used as the main navigation. There is a home page link, commendably labelled in plain German (Startseite), but it is rather small and does not convey the information that this is, in fact, the home page. Likewise labelled in German but also rather too small is the link to the site map in the top-left corner (Übersicht). As explained earlier in subsection 4.6.1, the German word for Table of Contents may be a better label to describe the functionality because Übersicht (overview) is rather vague. Unclear terminology appears also in the text below the search function (the acronym PZN ) and in the tab link to DocMorris’ on-line shop. In keeping with the notion on action word links, it could be labelled Einkaufen (go shopping). 4.7.3 Travel: bad-sassendorf.de Tourism based especially on elaborate health and spa facilities is a main charactaristic of the German town Bad Sassendorf. A page from the lowest level of the Web site hierarchy appears in Figure 4.3. In this case, the main concerns are about the ›Facilitate Handling‹ (section 4.2) and ›Reduce Complexity‹ (section 4.5) recommendations, but some comments also concern ›Facilitate Perception‹ (section 4.1). Although it is not directly evident from the screen shot, the Web site employs colour coding for its main sections. All the surfaces shown in shades of yellow (see Appendix B.3.) change to shades of green, blue, violet, or other colours, depending on the selection made in the menu bar on the right. As a result, the colour change is very well noticeable, but it always affects all the navigation tabs and thus is not connected to the specific sections in the menu bar at all. The type size used throughout the site is also very small and not amendable to browser-side resizing9 . There is no possibility to change the type size on the site itself. Furthermore, the menu bar resides at the right, instead of the 9 It does not respond to resizing in Microsoft Internet Explorer 6. Other browser software may use a different resizing technique. 73 4 Web Usability Recommendations http://www.bad-sassendorf.de/generator.aspx/templateId= renderPage/id=3576.html Screenshot at 800 × 600 display resolution. Retrieved 11 July 2006, 15:17 UTC+1 with Microsoft Internet Explorer 6. Figure 4.3: A third-level page from the Web site of Bad Sassendorf. The black line indicates the approximate position of the fold at a resolution of 800 × 600 pixels. See Appendix B.3. for a colour plate. well-established left side, offers no feedback on target capture other than the change of the pointer, and the colour change indicating the presently selected menu item is very subtle indeed, especially for the less saturated colours employed in the design. The breadcrumb navigation, which could provide valuable feedback about the current position on the site, resides below the fold as do the search function and the print button. The icons at the top offer helpful navigation options in a prominently visible position and are all equipped with text labels. These labels, however, contain Internet terminology: Home and Sitemap). 4.7.4 Public Authority: kreis-recklinghausen.de The home page of the German district Recklinghausen (Figure 4.4) offers access to information, forms, and services provided by the local authori- 74 4 Web Usability Recommendations http://www.kreis-recklinghausen.de/ Screenshot at 1024 × 786 display resolution. Retrieved 11 July 2006, 16:43 UTC+1 with Mozilla Firefox 1.5. Figure 4.4: The home page of the German district Recklinghausen. The black line indicates the position of the fold at a display resolution of 1024× 786 pixels. See Appendix B.4. for a colour plate. ties. It uses frames to display a vertical set of always present links at the left border and a header at the top. The most important grievances on this site once more are covered under the heading of ›Facilitate Handling‹ (section 4.2), with references also to ›Facilitate Perception‹ (section 4.1), ›Keep the Users in Control‹ (section 4.4), and ›Provide a Sound Base for Navigation Decisions‹ (section 4.6). The leftmost frame offers no scroll bar even if its contents disappear below the fold due to window size or screen resolution. On the author’s machine, the four links at the bottom were inaccessible at a resolution of 800×600 pixels. This is particularly vexing because among the endangered links are the ones titled Home and Email. The ›Seek-and-Hide‹ menu bar 75 4 Web Usability Recommendations at the top of the main content frame will follow any scroll movements and it thus remains accessible at all times. It displays up to three levels of the Web site hierarchy. The item lists of the menu do not display their labels in a colour combination that offers very high contrast, for they appear in blue text on a blueish background. Some of the links in the menu leave the Web site without any prior notice and also open a fresh browser window in the process. A help option does appear at the utmost right in the menu bar and it reveals, among other items, links to a collection of frequently asked questions and to a site map, which could be put to much better use, were they permanently visible and, in the case of the site map, equipped with more descriptive labels. The navigation system on this Web site does not give any feedback about the current position in the structure. The links in the centre all open new browser windows and many, but not all of them have a warning incorporated into their label, which in itself usually conveys no information whatsoever about the target but relies solely on the preceding unlinked text. Many of the links not only open new windows but also lead to documents in the PDF format, possibly launching another application. Finally, the button to trigger the search function is rather small and would likely present a challenging target to many elder users. 76 5 Final Remarks This work used a tripartite approach to explore the context of elders, ageing, and Web usability. The first part was aimed at the sentiment of technology scepticism among seniors and identified possible causes on the grounds of mainly social influences on the process of technology adoption. The second part then assembled information about human ageing and its consequences for normal Human–Computer interaction. These considerations provided the basis for the formulation of six basic recommendations as a result of an exploration of existing Web usability guidelines and relevant research. The following passages will revisit each of the main topics in reversed order of appearance. The recommendations made in chapter 4 aggregate what appear to be the most important guidelines to address difficulties that especially elder users are likely to experience. They can probably help to uncover usability issues of special relevance to this user group, but the examples employed for illustrative purposes do not, off course, constitute a full-scale evaluation. 5.1 Conflicts and Inspirations The recommendations are not without conflict. Type size, for example, is a field of tension: A larger base type size often means longer pages. The need for scrolling, as has been noted by Chadwick-Dias et al. (2003, p. 36), may offset any positive effects of a larger type size. Indeed, the need for scrolling in itself constitutes a handling problem and the recommendations made in section 4.2 advise to avoid it if there is a sane alternative. A case of outright contradiction is the structural dilemma described in subsection 4.5.2: limiting the number of options visible and at the same time the number of hierarchical levels in the Web site structure. 77 5 Final Remarks Even more salient is the impact of the various strategies to counter agerelated difficulties that have found their way into the recommendations. They typically address problems that are common to many, if not all Web sites and in some cases, a site-per-site implementation may even duplicate already present browser-side functionality. Even when elder users were aware of potentially beneficial browser-side functionality like type size controls or colour settings, they have been observed to be somewhat reluctant to use them, even if such features might make using the Web easier (see: Ellis & Kurniawan 2000, p. 273). Offering the programme functionality in the form of simple tool bars with enlarged buttons, each containing an icon and a label, is suitable for a limited amount of functionality, but as Hawthorne (2003, p. 40) concludes ». . . it would not scale up to a more complex application«, the configuration of which is beyond many novice users anyway. Individually providing the functionality on Web sites cannot be the final solution to this problem because it would mean additional options displayed on the Web pages, less space for the intended content, and ultimately increasing complexity, which should actually be reduced, as has been captured in section 4.5. These conflicts may, however, serve as inspirations for future improvements, especially of the browser software. The most obvious starting point would be offering the most important and useful features as labelled tool bar buttons by default. Suitable examples are a site- or time-specific history, the page-internal search, and especially text resizing and colour preference options. This would allow the removal of redundant features and controls from both browsers and Web pages and could help to simplify the interface finally presented to the Web users. Furthermore, enhancing the “internal” communication of Web site and browser software in order to rid the users of tedious but necessary tasks and thereby promoting usability for all users should be an interesting line of future development. Automatic language selection is, perhaps, a start, but there is potential for further integration, because many aspects of Web browsing do not concern only a single Web site. 78 5 Final Remarks 5.2 Ageing The assessment of age-related change of mind and body revealed numerous problem fields and the recommendations in chapter 4 try to address them. It is important to remember, however, that ageing is not a disease, and it does not necessarily bring debilitating change with it. A uniform finding of ageing research, at least in the cognitive branch, is the increasing diversity of the results, and many factors have been shown to influence the extend of age-related decline, most notably physical health and fitness. Possibly, societal changes will also effect the manifestation of the ageing process. Still, some changes are a very likely natural accompaniment of this ageing process and also, as of now, irreversible. The most notable examples are the changes in visual perception (in acuity, contrast and colour vision) and particularly the loss of the ability to accommodate for near vision. Product design in general, not only Web design, will have to address these and other requirements in design in order to avoid excluding a growing number of human beings. 5.3 Technological Innovation as a Social Process From the examination of the “Senior-Individual–Internet” relation, the central finding, perhaps, is that there is a multitude of factors responsible for an individual’s interest in and attitude towards technology. Many of these factors originate in human society and thus are subject to change over time. In the near future, so much is certain, the age gap in Internet usage as described in chapter 2 will vanish, but others will not (yet). For Internet usage, other divides exist and may continue to do so for some time along the gender or education borders. Ultimately, they may simply be a question of socio-economic status before using the Internet comes as naturally as using a telephone. Some of the factors identified as possible causes for the age gap, for example the lack of ›ability confidence‹, which is probably a by-product of the general societal attitude towards ageing and the aged, may take a longer time to change. 79 Appendix A. Logitech e-mail correspondence A.1. Inquiry Text I understand that some time ago, Logitech offered computer mouse with force feedback feature. I also heard about mice that used or were to use various patterns of vibration in order to give some impression of the haptic quality of whatever was under the pointer (e.g. window borders, panels, and similar), branded as "iFeel". As apparently none of these are presently in your range of products, I concluded that your company must have discounted the approach of force feedback mice for some reason. If this assumption is correct, I would like to know, why these products were discontinued. A.2. Answer Betreff: IFeel mouse [Incident: 060320-001233] Von: "Logitech EMEA Support" <[email protected]> Datum: Mon, 20 Mar 2006 19:11:08 +0100 (CET) An: [email protected] 80 Appendix Recently you requested personal assistance from our on-line support center. Below is a summary of your request and our response. If this issue is not resolved to your satisfaction, you may reopen it within the next 14 days. Thank you for allowing us to be of service to you. To update this question by email, please reply to this message. Because your reply will be processed automatically, you MUST enter your reply in the space below. Text entered into any other part of this message will be discarded. [===> Please enter your reply below this line <===] [===> Please enter your reply above this line <===] Subject IFeel mouse Discussion Thread Response (Percy König) 20/03/2006 07.11 PM Sehr geehrter Herr Lohr! Vielen Dank für Ihre Anfrage. Wie ich Ihrer E-Mail entnehme, haben Sie Fragen zur den iFeel-Mäusen. Logitech untersucht immer in neue Formen der Eingabegeräte und deshalb hatten wir in der Vergangenheit auch Mäuse, die 81 Appendix ein Feedback über Vibration zum Benutzer wiedergaben. Diese Technik hat aber nicht Erfolg am Markt gehabt, sodass wir diese Art von Eingabegeräten nicht weiter entwickelt haben. Dies war eine Kooperation mit der Firma Immersion. Unter deren Website http://www.immersion.com können Sie viele Informationen zu diesem Thema finden. Viele Fragen werden in unseren FAQs http://logitech-de-emea.custhelp.com beantwortet. Ich helfe auch gerne per E-Mail weiter. Mit freundlichen Grüßen Percy König Logitech Europe SA European Customer Service Question Reference No060320-001233 Product Level 1: Logitech Product Level 2: Pointing Devices Product Level 3: iFeel Mouse Date Created: 20/03/2006 02.22 PM Last Updated: 20/03/2006 07.11 PM Status: Solved Modellnummer: Teilenummer: Betriebssystem: Software Version: Computer Marke: Ihr Händler: 82 Appendix B. Screenshots B.1. spiegel.de 83 Appendix 84 Appendix B.2. docmorris.de 85 Appendix B.3. bad-sassendorf.de 86 Appendix B.4. kreis-recklinghausen.de 87 Appendix 88 Bibliography Aula, A. (2005), ‘User study on older adults’ use of the Web and search engines’, Universal Access in the Information Society 4, 67–81. Backes, G. M. & Clemens, W. (2003), Lebensphase Alter: eine Einführung in die sozialwissenschaftliche Alternsforschung, Grundlagentexte Soziologie, Juventa Verlag, Weinheim. Badre, A. N. (2002), Shaping Web Usability: Interaction Design in Context, Addison-Wesley, Boston, San Francisco, New York. Bayles, K. & Kaszniak, A. W. (1987), Communication and Cognition in Normal Aging and Dementia, Taylor & Francis, London. Becker, H., Eichenseer, C., Becker, U. et al. (1991), Die Älteren: zur Lebenssituation der 55- bis 70jährigen; eine Studie der Institute Infratest Sozialforschung, Sinus und Horst Becker, Reihe Praktische Demokratie, Verlag J. H. W. Dietz Nachf., Bonn. Becker, S. A. (2004a), ‘E-Government Visual Accessibility for Older Adult Users’, Social Science Computer Review 22(1), 11–23. Becker, S. A. (2004b), ‘A Study of Web Usability for Older Adults Seeking Online Health Resources’, ACM Transactions on Computer–Human Interaction 11(4), 387–406. Beier, B. & Vaughan, M. W. (2003), The Bull’s-Eye: A Framework for Web Application User Interface Design Guidelines, in ‘CHI ’03: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems’, ACM Press, New York, pp. 489–496. 89 Bibliography Bernard, M. L. (2003), ‘Optimal Web Design: How can I make my website’s structure more navigable?’, Software Usability Research Laboratory; Department of Psychology; Wichita State University, Wichita. Retrieved 14:26 UTC+1 26 April 2006 from http://psychology.wichita.edu/optimalweb/structure.htm. Bernard, M., Liao, C. & Mills, M. (2001), ‘Determining the Best Online Font for Older Adults’, Software Usability Research Laboratory; Department of Psychology; Wichita State University, Wichita. Retrieved 18:46 UTC+1 26 April 2006 from http: //psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/3W/fontSR.htm. Bieri, R., Florack, A. & Scarabis, M. (2006), ‘Der Zuschnitt von Werbung auf die Zielgruppe älterer Menschen’, Zeitschrift für Medienpsychologie 18(1), 19–30. Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W., eds (1990), Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, Vol. 2 of The Handbooks of Aging, 3 edn, Academic Press, San Diego. Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W., eds (2006), Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, Vol. 2 of The Handbooks of Aging, 6 edn, Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego. Bischoff, S. (2001), „Ältere Menschen – Why offline?“, in Gehrke (2001b), pp. 13–23. Bollwage, M. (2005), Typografie kompakt: Der richtige Umgang mit Schrift am Computer, 2nd edn, Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York. Brünner, B. O. (1997), Die Zielgruppe Senioren: eine interdisziplinäre Analyse der älteren Konsumenten, Lang, Frankfurt am Main. Also: PhD thesis, Helmut-Schmidt-Universität (Bundeswehr), Hamburg. Cerella, J. (1990), Aging and Information-Processing rate, in Birren & Schaie (1990), pp. 201–221. 90 Bibliography Chadwick-Dias, A., McNulty, M. & Tullis, T. (2003), Web usability and age: how design changes can improve performance, in ‘ACM Conference on Universal Usability: Proceedings of the 2003 conference on Universal Usability in Vancouver, BC, Canada’, ACM press, New York, pp. 30–37. Craik, F. I. M. & Salthouse, T. A., eds (2000), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 2nd edn, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah NJ, London. D’Aloisio, A. & Klein, R. M. (1990), Aging and the Development in Visual Attention, in Enns (1990), pp. 447–466. de Jager, P. (2005), ‘The Danger of the “Early Adopter” Myth’, Retrieved 13:56 UTC+1, 14 April 2006 from http://www.technobility.com/docs/article032.htm. Demunter, C. (2005a), The digital divide in Europe, number 38/2005 in ‘Statistics in Focus’, Statistical Office of the European Communities (Eurostat), Luxembourg. Available on-line from http://epp.eurostat.cec.eu.int/cache/ITY_OFFPUB/KS-NP-05-038/ EN/KS-NP-05-038-EN.PDF. Demunter, C. (2005b), Internet activities in the European Union, number 40/2005 in ‘Statistics in Focus’, Statistical Office of the European Communities (Eurostat), Luxembourg. Available on-line from http://epp.eurostat.cec.eu.int/cache/ITY_OFFPUB/KS-NP-05-040/ EN/KS-NP-05-040-EN.PDF. destatis (2003), Im Jahr 2050 wird jeder Dritte in Deutschland 60 Jahre oder älter sein, Statistisches Bundesamt (destatis), Wiesbaden. Press release, 6 June 2003. Available on-line from http://www.destatis.de/presse/deutsch/pm2003/p2300022.htm. destatis (2006), Informationstechnologie in Unternehmen und Haushalten 2005: Tabellenanhang zur Pressebroschüre, Statistisches Bundesamt 91 Bibliography (destatis), Wiesbaden. Appendage to Greiner et al. (2006). Available on-line from http://www.destatis.de/download/d/veroe/ Tabellenanhang_Haushalte_IKT2005.pdf. Dickinson, A., Newell, A. F., Smith, M. J. et al. (2005), ‘Introducing the Internet to the over-60s: Developing an email system for older novice computer users’, Interacting with Computers 17, 621–642. Ellis, D. R. & Kurniawan, S. H. (2000), ‘Increasing the Usability of Online Information for Older Users: A Case Study in Participatory Design’, International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction 12(2), 263–276. Emery, V. K., Edwards, P. J., Jacko, J. A. et al. (2002), Toward achieving universal usability for older adults through multimodal feedback, in ‘ACM Conference on Universal Usability: Proceedings of the 2003 conference on Universal Usability in Vancouver, BC, Canada’, ACM press, New York, pp. 46–53. Enns, J. T., ed. (1990), The Development of Attention: Research and Theory, North-Holland, Amsterdam, New York, Oxford. Eurostat (2006), ‘Eurostat - data navigation tree’, Statistical Office of the European Communities (Eurostat), Luxembourg. Retrieved 21:11 UTC+1, 14 April 2006 from http://epp.eurostat.cec.eu.int/portal/page?_pageid=1996, 45323734&_dad=portal&_schema=PORTAL&screen=welcomeref&open= /infosoc/policy&language=en&product=EU_MASTER_information_ society&root=EU_MASTER_information_society&scrollto=0. Fox, S. (2001), ‘Wired seniors: A fervent few, inspired by family ties’, PEW Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC. Retrieved 14:21 UTC+1, 13 December 2005 from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Wired_Seniors_Report.pdf. Fox, S. (2004), ‘Older Americans and the Internet: Just 22 % go online, but their enthusiasm for email and search may inspire their peers to 92 Bibliography take the leap’, PEW Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC. Retrieved 15:26 UTC+1, 15 January 2005 from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Seniors_Online_2004.pdf. Fox, S. (2005), ‘Digital Divisions’, PEW Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC. Retrieved 20:33 UTC+1, 16 May 2006 from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Digital_Divisions_ Oct_5_2005.pdf. Fox, S. & Madden, M. (2005), ‘Generations online’, PEW Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC. Retrieved 20:17 UTC+1, 16 May 2006 from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Generations_Memo.pdf. Fozard, J. L. (1990), Vision and Hearing in Aging, in Birren & Schaie (1990), pp. 150–170. Gappa, H. & Nordbrock, G. (2004), ‘Applying Web accessibility to Internet portals’, Universal Access in the Information Society 3, 80–87. Gauthier, A. H. & Smeeding, T. M. (2000), ‘Time use at older ages: Cross-national Differences’, Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), Paris. Retrieved 18:53 UTC+1, 19 May 2006 from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/20/24/2431647.pdf. Gehrke, B. (2001a), Offline im Onlinezeitalter, in Ältere Menschen Neue Medien – Why offline? (Gehrke 2001b), pp. 23–65. Gehrke, B., ed. (2001b), Ältere Menschen Neue Medien – Why offline?, Europäisches Zentrum für Medienkompetenz GmbH, Marl. Goldstein, E. B. & Ritter, M. (1997), Wahrnehmungspsychologie, Spektrum Akademischer Verlag, Heidelberg. Gourmelon, A. (1994), Richtlinien für die gedächtnisfreundliche und altersgerechte Gestaltung von Dialogschnittstellen, PhD thesis, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität, Erlangen-Nürnberg. 93 Bibliography Greiner, U., Hauschild, W., Kahle, I. et al. (2006), Informationstechnologie in Unternehmen und Haushalten 2005, Statistisches Bundesamt (destatis), Wiesbaden. Available on-line from http://www.destatis.de/download/d/veroe/ Pressebroschuere_IKT2005.pdf. Griffiths, P. (2004), ‘Elastic Design’, A List Apart. Retrieved 17:05 UTC+1, 26 April 2006 from http://www.alistapart.com/articles/elastic/. Gustafson, A. (2004), ‘Let Them Eat Cake’, A List Apart. Retrieved 13:55 UTC+1, 25 April 2006 from http://www.alistapart.com/articles/eatcake/. Hartley, A. (2006), Changing Role of the Speed of Processing Construct in the Cognitive Psychology of Human Aging, in Birren & Schaie (2006), pp. 183–207. Hawthorne, D. (2003), How universal is good design for older users?, in ‘ACM Conference on Universal Usability: Proceedings of the 2003 conference on Universal Usability in Vancouver, BC, Canada’, ACM press, New York, pp. 38–45. Holt, B. (2000), Creating Senior-Friendly Web Sites, number 4/2000 in ‘Issue Brief’, Center for Medicare Education, Washington, DC. Available on-line from http://www.medicareed.org/PublicationFiles/V1N4.pdf. Hoyer, W. J. & Touron, D. R. (2003), Learning in Adulthood, in J. Demick & C. Andreoletti, eds, ‘Handbook of Adult Development’, The Plenum Series in Adult Development and Aging, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York, Boston, Dordrecht, pp. 23–41. Hoyer, W. J. & Verhaegen, P. (2006), Memory Aging, in Birren & Schaie (2006), pp. 209–232. 94 Bibliography Isserstedt, W., Middendorff, E., Weber, S. et al. (2004), Die wirtschaftliche und soziale Lage der Studierenden in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland 2003, 17. Sozialerhebung des Deutschen Studentenwerks, Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF), Bonn, Berlin. Available on-line from http://www.bmbf.de/pub/040714_soz17hauptbericht.pdf. Kausler, D. H. (1991), Experimental Psychology, Cognition, and Human Aging, 2nd edn, Springer, New York. König, P. (2006), ‘IFeel mouse [Incident: 060320-001233]’, e-mail correspondence with Logitech EMEA support, 20 March 2006; see Appendix A. for transcript. Krauss Whitbourne, S. (1996), Psychological Perspectives in the Normal Aging Process, in L. L. Carstensen, B. A. Edelstein & L. Dornbrand, eds, ‘The Practical Handbook of Clinical Gerontology’, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks Calif, pp. 3–35. Krug, S. (2006), Don’t Make Me Think! A Common Sense Approach To Web Usability, New Riders Publishing, Berkeley. Kurniawan, S. & Zaphiris, P. (2005), Research-Derived Web Design Guidelines for Older People, in ‘Assets ’05: Proceedings of the 7th international ACM SIGACCESS conference on Computers and accessibility’, ACM Press, New York, pp. 129–135. Larson, K. & Czerwinski, M. (1998), Web Page Design: Implications of Memory, Structure and Scent for Information Retrieval, in ‘Proceedings of CHI’98, Human Factor in Computing Systems (Los Angeles, 21–23 April 1998)’, ACM press, New York, pp. 25–32. Lehr, U. M. (2003), Psychologie des Alterns, 10th edn, Quelle & Meyer Verlag, Wiebelsheim. 95 Bibliography Lynch, P. & Horton, S. (2004a), ‘GRAPHICS: “Browser-safe” colors’, Web Style Guide. Retrieved 19:41 UTC+1 22 April 2006 from http://www.webstyleguide.com/graphics/safe.html. Lynch, P. & Horton, S. (2004b), ‘TYPOGRAPHY: Typefaces’, Web Style Guide. Retrieved 17:45 UTC+1 26 April 2006 from http://www.webstyleguide.com/type/face.html. Marsiske, M. & Margrett, J. A. (2006), Everyday Problem Solving and Decision Making, in Birren & Schaie (2006), pp. 315–342. Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J. et al. (2000), Human performance: cognition, stress, and individual differences, Psychology Press, Hove. McDowd, J. M. & Birren, J. E. (1990), Aging and Attentional Processes, in Birren & Schaie (1990), pp. 222–233. McDowd, J. M. & Shaw, R. J. (2000), Attention and Aging: A Functional Perspective, in Craik & Salthouse (2000), pp. 221–292. Merriam-Webster Inc. (2006), ‘Merriam-Webster Online’, Merriam-Webster Inc., Springfield, MA. Retrieved 09:22 UTC+1, 5 May 2006 from http://www.merriam-webster.com/. Miller, C. S. & Remington, R. W. (2004), ‘Modeling Information Navigation: Implications for Information Architecture’, Human–Computer Interaction 19, 225–271. Mollenkopf, H. (1998), Soziale Integration und Teilhabe alter Menschen durch Technik, in T. Erkert & J. Salomon, eds, ‘Seniorinnen und Senioren in der Wissensgesellschaft’, Kleine, Bielefeld, pp. 125–134. Nayak, L., Priest, L. & Stuart-Hamilton, I. (2006), ‘Website design attributes for retrieving health information by older adults: an application of architectural criteria’, Universal Access in the Information Society. Springer “Online First” article. DOI 96 Bibliography 10.1007/s10209-006-0029-9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10209-006-0029-9. NIA & NLM (2002), Making Your Website Senior Friendly: A Checklist, National Institute of Aging (NIA) and National Library of Medicine (NLM), Bethesda, MD. Available on-line from http://www.nlm.nih.gov/pubs/checklist.pdf. NIA & NLM (2006), ‘NIHSeniorHealth Health Information for Older Adults’, National Institute of Aging (NIA) and National Library of Medicine (NLM), Bethesda, MD. Retrieved 17:20 UTC+1, 17 January 2006 from http://nihseniorhealth.gov/. Nielsen, J. (1993), Usability Engineering, Morgan Kaufmann, San Diego, San Francisco, New York. Nielsen, J. (1996), ‘Original Top Ten Mistakes in Web Design’, Jakob Nielsen’s AlertBox. Retrieved 17:39 UTC+1, 5 May2006 from http://www.useit.com/alertbox/9605a.html. Nielsen, J. (1997), ‘Changes in Web Usability Since 1994’, Jakob Nielsen’s AlertBox. Retrieved 17:47 UTC+1, 5 May2006 from http://www.useit.com/alertbox/9712a.html. Nielsen, J. (2000), Designing Web Usability: The Practice of Simplicity, New Riders Publishing, Indianapolis. NINDS (2006), ‘NINDS Parkinson’s Disease Information Page’, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), Bethesda, MD. Retrieved 12:54 UTC+1, 15 May 2006 from http://www.ninds. nih.gov/disorders/parkinsons_disease/parkinsons_disease.htm. Philipp, D. & Kliegl, R. (2000), Gedächtnistraining, in H.-W. Wahl & C. Tesch-Römer, eds, ‘Angewandte Gerontologie in Schlüsselbegriffen’, Verlag W. Kohlhammer, Stuttgart, Berlin, Köln, pp. 97–101. 97 Bibliography Plattig, K.-H. (1984), Sinnesorgane, in W. D. Oswald, W. M. Hermann, S. Kanowski et al., eds, ‘Gerontologie - Medizinische, psychologische und sozialwissenschaftliche Grundbegriffe’, Kohlhammer, Stuttgart, pp. 421–429. Plude, D. J. (1990), Aging, Feature Integration, and Visual Selective Attention, in Enns (1990), pp. 467–487. Rogers, E. M. (2003), Diffusion of Innovations, 5th edn, Free Press, New York, London, Toronto. Rosenfeld, L. & Morville, P. (2002), Information Architecture for the World Wide Web, 2nd edn, O’Reilly. Schaie, K. W. (2005), Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study, Oxford University Press, Oxford, New York. Schaie, K. W. & Willis, S. L. (1991), Adult Development and Aging, HarperCollins Publishers, New York. Schieber, F. (2006), Vision and Aging, in Birren & Schaie (2006), pp. 129–161. Schneider, B. A. & Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000), Implications of Perceptual Deterioration for Cognitive Aging Research, in Craik & Salthouse (2000), pp. 155–219. Schneider, K., Götzl, H. & Henrich, S. (1994), Motivation und Gedächtnis im Alter, in W. Kuhn, T. Büttner, W. Heinemann et al., eds, ‘Altern, Gehirn und Persönlichkeit’, Verlag Hans Huber, Bern, Göttingen, Toronto, pp. 109–118. Shneiderman, B. & Plaisant, C. (2005), Designing the User Interface: Strategies for Effective Human–Computer Interaction, 4th edn, Pearson Education/Addison Wesley, Boston, San Francisco, New York. 98 Bibliography Singh, N. & Pereira, A. (2005), The Culturally Customized Web Site: Customizing Web Sites for the Global Marketplace, Elsevier Butterworth–Heinemann, Burlington, Oxford. Spirduso, W. W. (1995), Physical Dimensions of Aging, Human Kinetics, Champaign, Windsor, Leeds. Spool, J. M., Scanlon, T., Schroeder, W. et al. (1999), Web Site Usability: A Designer’s Guide, Morgan Kaufmann, San Francisco, San Diego, New York. Stadelhofer, C. (2000), ‘Möglichkeiten und Chancen der Internetnutzung durch ältere’, Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie 33(3), 186–194. Stadtverwaltung Aachen (1994), ‘Dienstvereinbarung über den Einsatz technikunterstützter Informationsverarbeitung bei der Stadtverwaltung Aachen’, Stadtverwaltung Aachen. Retrieved 31 May 2006 from http://www.aachen.de/de/stadt_buerger/pdfs_ stadtbuerger/pdf_personalrat/vereinbarungen/tuiv.pdf. Venkatesh, V., Morris, M. G., Davis, G. B. et al. (2003), ‘User acceptance of information technology: Toward a unified view’, MIS Quarterly 27(3), 425–478. Verrillo, R. T. & Verrillo, V. (1985), Sensory and Perceptual Performance, in N. Charness, ed., ‘Aging and Human Performance’, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, New York, Brisbane, pp. 1–46. Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia (2006a), ‘Color blindness’, Wikipedia – The Free Encyclopedia. Retrieved 15:08 UTC+1. 27 June 2006 from http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Color_ blindness&oldid=60766235. Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia (2006b), ‘Digital divide’, Wikipedia – The Free Encyclopedia. Retrieved 20:13 UTC+1. 16 May 2006 from 99 Bibliography http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Digital_ divide&oldid=53166723. Wirth, T. (2004), Missing Links: Über gutes Webdesign, 2nd edn, Hanser Fachbuchverlag, München. Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L. & Li, K. Z. H. (2000), Human Memory, in Craik & Salthouse (2000), pp. 293–357. Zarit, J. M. & Zarit, S. H. (1987), Molar Aging: The Physiology and Psychology of Normal Aging, in L. L. Carstensen & B. A. Edelstein, eds, ‘Handbook of clinical gerontology’, Vol. 146 of Pergamon general psychology series, Pergamon Press, New York, pp. 18–31. 100