Understanding the transgenerational cycle of parenting : the role of
Transcription
Understanding the transgenerational cycle of parenting : the role of
The University of Toledo The University of Toledo Digital Repository Theses and Dissertations 2006 Understanding the transgenerational cycle of parenting : the role of past parenting experiences and emotional functioning Tracie Pasold The University of Toledo Follow this and additional works at: http://utdr.utoledo.edu/theses-dissertations Recommended Citation Pasold, Tracie, "Understanding the transgenerational cycle of parenting : the role of past parenting experiences and emotional functioning" (2006). Theses and Dissertations. Paper 1374. This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by The University of Toledo Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of The University of Toledo Digital Repository. For more information, please see the repository's About page. A Dissertation Entitled Understanding the Transgenerational Cycle of Parenting: The Role of Past Parenting Experiences and Emotional Functioning by Tracie Lee Pasold, M.A. Submitted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for The Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology ______________________________ Advisor: Jeanne B. Funk, Ph.D. ______________________________ Kathleen F. Baird, Ph.D. ______________________________ Robert Elliott, Ph.D. _____________________________ Alice Skeens, Ph.D. ______________________________ Mojisola F. Tiamiyu, Ph.D. ______________________________ Graduate School The University of Toledo December 2006 An Abstract of Understanding the Transgenerational Cycle of Parenting: The Role of Past Parenting Experiences and Emotional Functioning Tracie L. Pasold, M.A. Submitted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology University of Toledo December 2006 The present study examined relationships among past parenting experiences, maltreatment, symptoms of PTSD, delinquency and psychopathy traits, and the role of these variables in the intergenerational transmission of parenting styles. It was hypothesized that past parenting experiences, maltreatment, and symptoms of PTSD would predict callous-unemotional psychopathy (CU), delinquency, and anticipated future parenting. Participants were 326 volunteers from public high schools and a Juvenile Detention Center (JDC). All completed the Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API), Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS), Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory ii (YPI); measures of symptoms of PTSD and social desirability. Public school participants reported more father’s and mother’s warmth, less father’s and mother’s rejection, and more father’s overprotection. JDC participants reported more maltreatment, more PTSD, and higher CU. CU was predicted by lower father’s and mother’s positive parenting among public school females. Maltreatment experiences predicted more arrests among public school males and females. Fewer symptoms of PTSD predicted more arrests among public school females. More mother’s positive parenting predicted fewer arrests in public school females. Results of the current study support research regarding the intergenerational transmission of parenting. JDC participants reported significantly higher anticipated rejection and overprotection in future parenting. Among public school females, higher levels of callous-unemotional psychopathy significantly predicted lower anticipated warmth and higher anticipated rejection in future parenting. Among public school males, more maltreatment experiences predicted less anticipated warmth in future parenting. Among public school males, more PTSD predicted more anticipated rejection and more anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Among JDC participants, more maltreatment predicted less anticipated overprotection in future parenting, and more PTSD predicted more anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Higher levels of callous-unemotional psychopathy also predicted more anticipated overprotection in this sample. These results demonstrate the critical importance of inclusion of positive parenting in prevention and intervention strategies. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This research project and resulting document mark the completion of my graduate career. Many individuals have played a crucial role in my success over the course of these five years. I must express sincere gratitude to all of my committee members, Dr. Jeanne Funk; Dr. Robert Elliott; Dr. Mojisola Tiamiyu; Dr. Alice Skeens; and Dr. Kathleen Baird for their support of me on this project from its very infancy and their stellar contributions to its development, execution, and final product. The individual contributions made by every member of this team, not only to this project, but to my graduate education and training overall, to me, have been selfless “gifts” that I will be forever grateful for. The quality of mentorship offered me has done, and will continue to do, much to support the achievement of my dream to become the best psychologist that I can be. Thank you to all of you! I must extend an extra special thank you to Dr. Jeanne Funk for going above and beyond to be supportive of me in the achievement of my goals and aspirations. She has served as my mentor throughout my graduate education and training and has been tireless in her efforts to guide and encourage me and to foster my ability to achieve my fullest potential. Thank you, Jeanne! Lastly, but certainly not least, I extend gratitude to my family and friends for their support and encouragement throughout this long journey. My mother and father, in particular, have always passionately persevered in their love and support of me throughout my life and, for this, I am most grateful. It pleases me very much to make them proud in return. iv Table of Contents Abstract. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .ii Acknowledgements. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv Table of Contents. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v List of Tables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii List of Appendices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xiv I. Chapter One: Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 II. Chapter Two: Review of the Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 The Impact of Parenting Across the Lifespan. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .4 Parental Rejection and Psychological Adjustment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 Parental Behaviors and Psychological Adjustment: Other Negative Parenting Practices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Parenting Behaviors and the Social Self and Self-Concept. . . . . . . . . . .20 Parenting Behaviors and Hostility and Aggression. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .25 Parenting Behaviors and More Severe Psychiatric Problems. . . . . . . . .28 Intergenerational Transmission of Parenting Behaviors. . . . . . . . . . . . .32 Child Maltreatment and Its Outcomes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 Developmental Perspectives on Child Maltreatment Outcomes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Domestic Violence as a Form of Maltreatment . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Child Maltreatment and Long-Term Adjustment .. . . . . . . . . . 40 Child Maltreatment and Social Functioning. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Child Maltreatment and Revictimization. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .46 v Child Maltreatment and Other Psychological Sequelae. . . . . . 47 Intergenerational Transmission of Maltreatment. . . . . . . . . . . .61 Juvenile Delinquency. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .64 Maltreatment and Juvenile Delinquency. . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . .64 Other Familial Correlates of Delinquency. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .69 Juvenile Psychopathy. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Statement of the Problem. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .77 Study Hypotheses .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 III. Chapter Three: Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .81 Participants. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81 Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Anticipated Future Parenting Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .91 Child Maltreatment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96 Post Traumatic Stress Symptomatology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 Social Desirability. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .98 Procedure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .99 IV. Chapter Four: Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .102 Data Analyses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 Preliminary Analyses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .102 Regression Analyses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 Parenting, Maltreatment, and PTSD as Predictors of Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 vi Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Delinquency . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Warmth In Future Parenting . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting . . . . . . . . . . 152 Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Overprotection In Future Parenting. . . . . . . 162 V. Chapter Five: Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176 Parenting Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176 Sample Comparisons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179 Importance of Parenting, Maltreatment, and PTSD for CallousUnemotional psychopathy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .183 Importance of Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy for Delinquency . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 184 Importance of Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy for Anticipated Future Parenting . . . 186 Study Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 192 Clinical Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194 VI. References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 198 VII. Appendices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 214 vii List of Tables Table 1 Summary of Outcomes Found to be Associated with Experiences of Specific Parenting Behaviors. . . . . . . . . . . 30 Table 2 Summary of Outcomes Found to be Associated with Specific Maltreatment Experiences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .58 Table 3 Gender, Age, Grade, and Ethnicity Distributions for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Table 4 Distributions of Contact with Law Enforcement, Number of Arrests, and Number of Juvenile Detention Center Placements for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . .84 Table 5 Distributions of Frequency of Relocations to Different Homes for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . 86 Table 6 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the API for Public School and JDC Participants . . 103 Table 7 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the PPPS for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .105 Table 8 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Measure of PTSD Symptoms for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .108 Table 9 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .109 Table 10 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Measure of Maltreatment Experiences for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 Table 11 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Measure of Social Desirability for Public School and JDC Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .111 Table 12 Means and Standard Deviations for the API Scales Of Anticipated Warmth, Anticipated Rejection, and Anticipated Overprotection by Sample and Gender. . . . . .112 viii Table 13 Total Maltreatment Experiences, Total CallousUnemotional Psychopathy, Total PTSD Symptoms, and Total Social Desirability Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and JDC Participants by Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .115 Table 14 PPPS Father’s Warmth, Rejection, and Overprotection Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and JDC Participants by Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .117 Table 15 PPPS Mother’s Warmth, Rejection, and Overprotection Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and JDC Participants by Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .118 Table 16 Total Father’s and Mother’s Positive Parenting Means And Standard Deviations for Public School and JDC Participants by Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .121 Table 17 Bivariate Correlations Between All Study Variables . . . . 123 Table 18 Partial Correlations Between All Study Variables . . . . . . 124 Table 19 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Females . . . .126 Table 20 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . 127 Table 21 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .129 Table 22 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Females . . . 130 ix Table 23 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . 131 Table 24 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .133 Table 25 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .135 Table 26 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 Table 27 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .138 Table 28 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .140 Table 29 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .141 Table 30 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .143 x Table 31 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 Table 32 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .147 Table 33 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 Table 34 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .150 Table 35 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .151 Table 36 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for The JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153 Table 37 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .155 xi Table 38 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156 Table 39 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .158 Table 40 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160 Table 41 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .161 Table 42 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for The JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 163 Table 43 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females . . . . . .165 Table 44 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . 166 xii Table 45 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .168 Table 46 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females . . . . . .170 Table 47 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males . . . . . . . 171 Table 48 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the JDC Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .173 Table 49 Summary of Relationships Found Between Predictor And Outcome Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .175 xiii List of Appendices Demographic Form . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A Juvenile Court Judge Consent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . B Public School Administration Consent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . C Public School Participant Parent Consent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . D Juvenile Detention Center Participant Consent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . E Public School Student Participant Consent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . F Anticipated Future Parenting Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .G Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of the API Using All Questionnaires. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .H Factor Loadings of the Items Within the API Separately by Public School and JDC Samples. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .J Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of all Items Within the PPPS Using All Questionnaire Data. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . K Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of Father Items Within the PPPS using All Questionnaire Data. . . . . . . . . . .L Means, Standard Deviations and Factor Loadings of Mother Items Within the PPPS using All Questionnaire Data . . . . . . . . . .M Factor Loadings of All Items of the PPPS using Public School Questionnaires Only . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .N Factor Loadings of All Items of the PPPS using JDC Questionnaires Only. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . O xiv Maltreatment Questions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . P Youth Psychopathy Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Q PTSD Questions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .R Social Desirability Measure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .S xv Chapter One Introduction Parenting experiences, particularly warmth, rejection, overprotection, and discipline, have an important impact on personality formation and development (Rohner, 1986). Among the wide-range of internalizing and externalizing problems associated with these parenting behaviors, strong associations exist between oppositional defiant, conduct, antisocial, and delinquent behaviors and the experience of low warmth (Caspi et al., 2004; Palmer & Hollin, 1996), high rejection (Palmer & Hollin, 1997; Ruchkin et al., 2001; Ruchkin, Eisemann, & Hagglof, 1998), both high and low overprotection (Pittman & Chase-Lansdale, 2001; Rey & Plapp, 1990), and harsh discipline (Holmes & Robbins, 1987; Muris et al., 1996). Low parental involvement, low positive reinforcement, low monitoring and supervision, and inconsistent and harsh discipline have also been associated with psychopathy traits (Wootton et al., 1997) and these traits, in turn, lead to antisocial and delinquent behavior (Gretton, Hare, & Catchpole, 2004; Salekin et al., 2004; Vincent et al., 2003). As with the parenting experiences noted previously, maltreatment experiences, particularly sexual abuse, physical abuse, neglect, and emotional abuse, are also associated with extensive internalizing and externalizing problems (Manly et al., 2001). Among these are the associations between maltreatment and PTSD (DeBellis, 2001; 1 Kendall-Tackett, 2002) and delinquency (Jaffee et al., 2004; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Mabanglo, 2002). Because parenting experiences are known to have a significant impact on emotions and behavior, the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors has been a topic of interest to psychologists. The transfer of parenting styles and behaviors has been indicated to occur through modeling, learning, and the development of personality traits that have an impact on future parenting (Simons et al., 1992; Whitbeck et al., 1992). As with other parenting behaviors, maltreatment experiences are also modeled and learned and may lead to maladjusted adults with emotion dysregulation and negative cognitions. These personality characteristics not only lead to antisocial and delinquent behaviors that extend from childhood to adulthood (Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004; Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001), but also increase the transmission of maltreatment across generations (Cunningham, 2003; DeBellis, 2001; Ruchkin et al., 2002). Symptoms of PTSD that are associated with maltreatment also play an important role in predicting transmission of maltreatment through victimizing of others or revictimizing of the self as part of the compulsion to reenact the trauma (Green, 1998). The goal of the current study was to expand upon existing research as well as to explore new areas about the impact of parenting. Specific parenting experiences of warmth, rejection, overprotection, and discipline, as well as maltreatment experiences of sexual abuse, physical abuse, neglect, and emotional abuse were variables of interest. These experiences and their association with PTSD symptoms and specific psychopathy traits were examined. Additionally, the role of parenting experiences, PTSD symptoms, 2 and psychopathy in the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors were studied. 3 Chapter Two Review of the Literature The Impact of Parenting Across the Lifespan It is well-established that the family, parental behavior in particular, plays an important role in the social and psychological adjustment of children and adolescents, extending into adulthood. For both children and adolescents, there are specific aspects of parental behavior that appear to be critical: appropriate discipline; clear supervision; and warmth and nurturance in the interactions between the parent and the child (Palmer & Hollin, 1996). Parental patterns that are lax, erratic, and strict in discipline have been associated with antisocial children and juvenile delinquents and other behavioral and psychological problems that will extend into adult life (Palmer & Hollin, 1996). A brief overview of the following literature review on the role of past parenting experiences in overall psychological adjustment and well-being is provided at the end of this section (Table 1). Parental Rejection and Psychological Adjustment. Numerous studies have implicated specific parental behaviors as being causal factors in psychological adjustment and psychopathology, beginning in early childhood and continuing into adulthood. R. P. Rohner (1986) developed a theory of parental acceptance-rejection based on numerous studies published over the past half century. According to his theory, parental acceptance or rejection of a child has a significant impact on the child’s personality formation and 4 development. Rohner’s theory focuses on specific personality dispositions that seem to characterize rejected children, adolescents, and adults. These include dependence or defensive independence, depending on the extent of rejection; emotional unresponsiveness; hostility, aggression, passive aggression, or problems managing hostility and aggression; negative self-esteem; negative self-adequacy; negative worldview; and emotional instability. Other research suggests that the experience of parental rejection has a negative impact on a child’s feelings of security and self-esteem and also leads to feelings of being unloved, unwanted, and unaccepted. For example, Peretti, Clark, and Johnson (1985) studied 123 (69 female) third grade students and found that the rejected children demonstrated behaviors indicating negative emotional adjustment. The goal behind these behaviors seemed to be to get the attention of parents or others in an attempt to gain acceptance and to replace lost feelings of security. The children were given a questionnaire that dealt with parental rejection. An interview was conducted with teachers to determine the frequency of negative attention-seeking behaviors in the classroom. Boys who experienced parental rejection were more boisterous and disruptive in their behavior (more noisy, crude, and engaged in making gross remarks), frequently disrupted the classroom with talking, and displayed more restlessness (making noises, banging books, tapping the desktop, or leaving their desks). Use of profanity towards teachers and classmates and throwing things were also noted as attention-seeking behaviors among the boys. Among females, whispering was the most common attentionseeking behavior. Excessive quarrelsomeness, stubbornness, and temper tantrums 5 (outbursts of anger, resentment, irritability, and impatience) without provocation were also reported to be girls’ ways of gaining attention (Peretti, Clark, & Johnson). Ruchkin, Koposov, Eisemann, and Hagglof (2001) found parental rejection to be a predictor of antisocial behavior. Their 193 (85 females) delinquent participants were obtained from juvenile detention centers and ranged in age from 14 to 19 years. The measures administered were the Retrospective Childhood Problems, Antisocial Behavior Checklist, Temperament and Character Inventory, and the EMBU (Swedish acronym for Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran – “own memories of parental rearing”). Their results suggest that those who reported childhood experiences of neglecting and rejecting parental behaviors were at higher risk for developing conduct problems early on, placing them at risk of more long-term antisocial behavior (Ruchkin, Koposov, Eisemann, & Hagglof). Similarly, other research on perceived parental rejection and overprotection has found these specific behaviors to be risk factors for the development of conduct and oppositional disorders and delinquency. Rey and Plapp (1990) sampled normal (N = 62, 23 females), Conduct Disordered (N = 62, 29 females), and Oppositional Defiant Disordered (N = 49, 23 females) adolescents aged 12 to 16 years of age. All participants completed the Parental Bonding Instrument. There were significant differences between the normal control and clinical groups in perceived parental overprotection and indifference and rejection. Specifically, the Conduct Disorder and Oppositional Defiant groups perceived their parents to be significantly more overprotective and less caring than the normal control group. A study by Palmer and Hollin (1999) compared 97 convicted male offenders (age range from 13 to 21 years) to 273 non-offenders (157 females, age range from 12 and 27 years). Paternal rejection, which was measured by the 6 shortened version of the EMBU, most strongly discriminated between the two groups. The male offenders perceived their fathers as more rejecting, which the researchers noted suggests an association between high levels of rejection and high levels of self-reported delinquency (Palmer & Hollin). Palmer and Hollin (1996) further explored the relationship between parental rejection and lack of warmth and self-reported delinquency (Palmer & Hollin, 1996) and the development of moral reasoning (Palmer & Hollin, 1997). Sixty-four undergraduate students (42 females) aged 18 to 27 years completed the Sociomoral Reflection MeasureShort Form, the EMBU, and a measure of self-reported delinquency. Results indicated, overall, that a warm and non-rejecting parenting style may aid in moral development (as measured through the participant’s reasoning in regards to the importance of five norms: contract and truth, affiliation, life, property and law, and legal justice). High selfreported delinquency levels were associated with parental rejection and lack of warmth. The researchers suggested that parental rejection inhibits positive attachment between the parent and child, impacting how the child is influenced by the parents’ and society’s values and beliefs. In children, they suggest that poor attachment leads to low control or influence over the child’s choices, which results in maladjustment and engaging in delinquent behaviors (Palmer & Hollin, 1996; Palmer & Hollin, 1997). Further investigation of this relationship by Simons and colleagues (1989) indicated that, for the most part, parental rejection leads to adolescent deviance rather than deviance leading to parental rejection (Simons, Robertson, & Downs). Simons and colleagues’ study included 244 participants (approximate even split between males and females) between the ages of 13 and 17. Information was obtained from each participant initially and again 7 one year later. Measures included a scale of parental rejection developed by the Behavior Research and Evaluation Corporation, the short form of the Family Environment Scale, and a self-report inventory of delinquent behavior. Zemore and Rinholm (1989) found an association between depression-proneness in sons and perceptions of fathers as cold and rejecting. Depression-proneness in daughters was found to be associated with mothers who were perceived as intrusive and controlling. These relationships were independent of the participants’ affective state at the time of the study and provide support for the hypothesis that the experience of parental rejection and control may lead to the development of a depression-prone personality. Participants in this study consisted of 95 (54 females) university students ranging in age from 17 to 21 years. Measures utilized were the Parental Bonding Instrument, Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale, Depression Adjective Checklist, Beck Depression Inventory (short form), Depression-Proneness Rating Scales, and the Fear Survey Schedule. Perceived parental rejection has been found to play a crucial role in the attitudes towards psychoactive substance-related behaviors among youth. Teichman and Kefir (2000) sampled 195 ninth, tenth, and eleventh graders (87 females). Measures completed included the Questionnaire to Assess Personal and Social Attitudes to Hashish, Marijuana, and Other Substances; the Behavioral Intentions Questionnaire; Schaefer’s Children’s Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory; Ascribed Parental Attitudes Towards Substances; and Ascribed Parental Use of Psychoactive Substances. Results indicated that perceived parental rejection and non-acceptance had a significant impact on children’s attitudes and intent to use psychoactive substances. Specifically, participants 8 who perceived their parents as more rejecting and less accepting reported more positive attitudes towards and intent to use psychoactive substances. Ruchkin, Eisemann, and Hagglof’s (1998) research adds further support for a significant relationship between specific parenting behaviors, particularly rejecting parenting, and psychological problems. Participants in their study consisted of 133 delinquent volunteers from juvenile corrections facilities ranging in age from 15 to 18 years and 108 school children ranging in age from 14 to 18 years. Participants were administered the EMBU, the Achenbach Youth Self-Report, and the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Results indicated parental rejection to be significantly related to problem behaviors and psychological symptoms including inattention, anxiety/depression, delinquent behavior, internalizing problems, somatic complaints, and social problems. Specifically, parental behaviors relating to rejection, including physical punishment, rejection as an individual, hostility, lack of respect for participant’s point of view, and criticizing of participant’s inadequacies in public were found to significantly relate to the presence of psychopathology in both delinquents and controls. Ruchkin and colleagues (1999) found a significant relationship between perceptions of parental rejection and resulting avoidant coping styles (Ruchkin, Eisemann, & Hagglof). Their sample consisted of 178 male delinquent adolescents ranging in age from 15 to 18 years and a group of 91 adolescent male controls ranging in age from 14 to 18 years. Participants were given the EMBU, the Temperament and Character Inventory, the Coping Scale for Children and Youth, and the Social Desirability Scale. 9 To summarize, strong support exists for a relationship between experiences of specific parenting behaviors that are more rejecting and psychological maladjustment. Specifically, the experience of parental rejection has been found to be related to lower self-esteem and feelings of being unloved, unwanted, and unaccepted. The experience of parental rejection has also been implicated in externalizing disorders such as oppositional and conduct problems as well as antisocial behavior; and internalizing problems such as anxiety and depression. In addition to parental rejection, other parental behaviors have been found to play an important role in overall psychological adjustment. Parental Behaviors and Psychological Adjustment: Other Negative Parenting Practices. In addition to experiences of parental rejection, other negative parenting practices have been found to be related to the development of psychological problems in childhood that extend into adulthood. For example, dysfunctional levels of emotional involvement by parents were found to relate to problems in children’s emotional expression (Lumley et al., 1996). In addition, maternal over-involvement (Kerver, van Son, & de Groot, 1992), inconsistent and harsh discipline by parents (Holmes & Robins, 1987) and parental rejection (Vrasti, Eisemann, & Bucur, 1993) are other specific aspects of parental behavior that have been found to have a negative impact on psychological functioning. Kerver and colleagues (1992) conducted a one-year prospective study in which 107 adult participants (53 females, aged 19 years and up) completed the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and the Parental Bonding Instrument initially, and again one year later. Non-depressed participants who reported maternal over-involvement had an 8.5 fold increased risk of becoming depressed one year later. 10 Holmes and Robins (1987) found that unfair, inconsistent, and harsh parental discipline predicted both alcohol and depressive disorders even after controlling for the influence of parental psychiatric history, age of participant, and childhood behavior problems. Adult participants (aged 18 years and older) were interviewed three times over the course of two years. The study design consisted of three groups: there were 73 participants in the depression group (diagnosis of major depressive episode ever in lifetime and never met criteria for another diagnosis with the exception of phobia or panic); 145 participants in the alcoholic group (met criteria for diagnosis of alcohol abuse/dependence ever in lifetime and never met criteria for another diagnosis); and 384 participants in the control group (never met criteria for a diagnosis and never had any alcohol or depressive symptoms). The Home Environment Interview was utilized, which examined the participant’s experience of home life from the age of 6 through 12 years. Unfair, inconsistent, and harsh parental discipline was significantly more pronounced among participants diagnosed with alcohol and depressive disorders compared to controls. Magnus and colleagues (1999) found the particular parental behaviors of warmth, support, and acceptance to be significantly and independently related to child adjustment under stressful conditions, school adjustment, and social problem-solving. Their research focused on 261 second through sixth grade children and their parents. Measures utilized were the Teacher-Child Rating Scale, the Child Rating Scale, the Group Social Problem Solving measure, and the Realistic Control Measure. Parents were interviewed in regards to parental attitudes, involvement, and discipline. The researchers concluded that those 11 who experienced more warmth, support, and acceptance had better adjustment under stress, in school, and in social problem-solving. Perceived lack of maternal warmth, harsh discipline by parents, and poor paternal care have been associated with suicidal ideation in adolescents and young adults (Wagner & Cohen, 1994; Tousignant, Bastien, & Hamel, 1993). For example, Wagner and Cohen (1994) had 178 participants (pairs of siblings consisting of study children: 84 females; siblings: 94 females) ranging in age from 11 to 21 years complete subscales from the Child Report of Parental Behavior. In addition, participants completed the Power Assertive Punishment Scale and another scale measuring maternal discipline practices. Finally, participants were interviewed using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Low maternal warmth and harsh discipline were found to significantly relate to suicidal ideation. Tousignant, Bastien, and Hamel sampled 2,327 high school students from the 9th 10th and 11th grades and 701 young adults aged 18 to 24 years. Their participants completed the Parental Bonding Instrument in addition to answering questions regarding suicidal behavior. Perceived poor parental care was associated with suicidal behavior in the high school group. Among the young adults, only paternal care was found to be associated with suicidal behavior. Ross, Clayer, and Campbell (1983) found suicidal ideation among adult participants, which was measured by a studydesigned scale of suicidal ideation, to be associated with the specific parental behaviors of favoring siblings, guilt engendering, unstimulating, rejecting, and unaffectionate. Their sample consisted of 85 university students with a mean age of 18.6, 72 medical and surgical patients with a mean age of 20.1, and 125 state employees with a mean age of 39.1. Participants also completed the EMBU and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire 12 (EPQ). Having a mother who was intolerant and abusive and a father who was depriving and punitive were associated with suicidal ideation. Pittman and Chase-Lansdale (2001) conducted research with 302 female participants between the ages of 15 and 18. The current emotional functioning of the mothers of the participants was measured by the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; parenting was measured by the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment and the Parental Strictness/Supervision Scale; delinquent behavior was measured by the National Longitudinal Study of Youth and items from the Youth Deviance Scale; adolescent psychological functioning was measured by the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale and the Cognitive Distress Scale; academic success was obtained through a question pertaining to grades achieved; work orientation was measured through the work orientation subscale of the Psychosocial Maturity Index; and sexual experience was measured through specific questions surrounding sexual experiences. Results indicated that those who reported both more minor and major delinquent behaviors and clinical levels of depressive symptoms perceived maternal behaviors that were more disengaged (low warmth, supervision, and monitoring). Additionally, the perception of mothers as disengaged was associated with lower grades and a higher likelihood to be sexually active and having had sex at a younger age. Girls who perceived their mothers as disengaged showed the worst adjustment overall (Pittman & Chase-Lansdale). Maternal expressed emotion was found to strongly predict children’s antisocial behavior problems (Caspi et al., 2004). Existing data from the Environmental Risk Longitudinal Twin Study consisted of 1,116 five year old twin pairs (56% monozygotic, 13 44% dizygotic, 49% male). Raters coded positive, negative, and warmth comments made by mother about child during an interview and the CBCL was completed by the mother and a teacher. Mother’s emotional attitudes (displaying warmth and having a positive attitude) towards their children were found to be significantly associated with children’s antisocial behavior problems. Negativity, negative comments, lack of warmth, and lack of positive attitude towards the child resulted in antisocial behavior problems (Caspi et al). A relationship between perceived parenting styles and resulting psychological distress and offending characteristics of 122 incarcerated young male offenders ranging in age from 15 to 22 years was found by Chambers, Power, Loucks, and Swanson (2001). The Parental Bonding Instrument, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Beck’s Hopelessness Scale, and Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale were utilized. Juvenile offenders perceived their parents as being lower in affection and either very low or very high in control when compared to non-offending adolescents. A relationship was also found between maternal care and how young offenders viewed their past offending. In particular, those with high-caring mothers viewed their most serious crime as being more dangerous and harmful compared to offenders with low-caring mothers. The researchers concluded that sons of more caring mothers were more aware of the impact of offending actions. Young offenders whose mothers were highly controlling were more likely to view their offending behavior as being under their own control. The researchers suggested that young offenders with mothers who are more caring and controlling may find it easier to stop engaging in delinquent behavior in the future because they may tend to feel more in control in addition to being more aware of the impact of their offending. 14 Symptoms of psychological distress (including anxiety and depression) were found to be strongly predicted by high parental control and low care by one or both parents. Low parental care in the group of offenders was found to be significantly linked to higher levels of anxiety, depression, hopelessness, pessimism, and low self-esteem in comparison to controls. Paternal control was significantly linked to the age of first arrest (a more controlling father resulted in a lower age of the offender when first arrested). Offenders who viewed their fathers as high in care also viewed their father’s views as more important. Those who perceived high care and high control from their mothers were more likely to see their mother’s views as important. Research conducted by Muris, Bogels, Meesters, van der Kamp, and van Oosten (1996) adds support to other research that has identified relationships between specific parenting behaviors and resulting conduct problems in children. Their participants were 64 children (33 girls) ranging in age from 8 to 18 years who were referred to a community mental health center. All of the mothers and 42 fathers also participated in the study. The children completed the EMBU-C (child version) and the Fear Survey Schedule for Children. The parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist and the parent version of the EMBU. A higher frequency of more negative rearing practices (more hostile, rejecting, overprotecting, and punitive), along with a low frequency of positive rearing behaviors, was associated with externalizing and disruptive behaviors in the children. More perceived parental warmth has been found to be associated with fewer reported psychological symptoms by adolescents (Xia & Qian, 2001). Among their sample of 127 adolescents (age range was 16 to 22 years, 32 females), those who recalled 15 more parental behaviors of punishment, over-involvement, overprotection, rejection and denial reported more psychological symptoms including somatization, obsessioncompulsion, depression, hostility, paranoid ideation, phobic anxiety, psychoticism and lower index scores on general mental health status. Instruments utilized were the EMBU and the SCL-90. A recent study by Leung, McBride-Chang, and Lai (2004) explored the relations among maternal parenting style, academic competence, and life satisfaction in 346 7th grade Chinese students (220 female, 126 male). The Parental Image Differential (completed by both child and mother), the Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale, and Academic Self-Concept measure were completed. Maternal concern was found to significantly predict overall life satisfaction. Maternal restrictiveness was significantly negatively related to academic competence. Academic competence, in turn, significantly predicted overall life satisfaction (Leung, McBride-Chang, & Lai). Kim and colleagues (2003) explored parenting behaviors and the occurrence and co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and conduct problems among 897 (54% female) African American children and their primary caregivers. Parenting behaviors and clinical symptoms were assessed when the child was 10 and then again at age 12 using version four of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. The categories of harshinconsistent parenting, nurturant-involved parenting, warmth, and hostility were measured through study-devised questions asked of the child and parent. The children with higher levels of conduct problems reported significantly higher levels of harshinconsistent and hostile parenting and significantly lower levels of warm, nurturantinvolved parenting. Lower levels of warm, nurturant-involved parenting and higher 16 levels of harsh-inconsistent and hostile parenting were significantly related to the child’s depressive symptoms. Those with co-occurring problems (both conduct problems and depressive symptoms) reported receiving the highest levels of hostility, lowest levels of warmth, significantly less nurturant-involved parenting, and significantly more inconsistent and harsh parenting. Those whose conduct problems and depressive symptoms increased between the first and second assessment periods reported a decrease in nurturant-involved parenting and warmth, along with a significant increase in harshinconsistent parenting and hostility (Kim et al). Moris, Meesters, and van den Berg (2003) explored internalizing and externalizing problems, self-reported attachment style, and perceived parental rearing among 742 (397 males, 345 females) adolescents ages 12 to 18 years (mean age 14). The Attachment questionnaire for Children, the Child Version of the EMBU, and the Youth Self-Report were completed. Parental emotional warmth was found to be negatively associated with various symptoms of psychopathology. Parental rejection and overprotection were found to be positively associated with internalizing (anxiousdepressed, somatic complaints) and externalizing (aggression, delinquent behavior) problems. Those who were securely attached perceived their parents as more emotionally warm and less rejecting and overprotective compared to those who were insecurely attached. Those who reported low levels of emotional warmth and high levels of rejection and overprotection reported significantly more symptoms overall (Moris, Meesters, & van den Berg). Wolfradt, Hempel, and Miles (2003) examined perceived parenting styles, depersonalization, anxiety, and coping behavior among 276 (150 females) high school 17 students aged 14 to 17 (mean age 15). The Zurich Brief Questionnaire for the Assessment of Parental Behaviors, Questionnaire of Experiences of Dissociation, StateTrait Anxiety Inventory, and a coping questionnaire were completed. Results indicated high levels of depersonalization to be strongly associated with high levels of parental pressure and control. Trait anxiety was found to be positively associated with parental pressure and control and negatively associated with parental warmth. Parental warmth was significantly associated with more active coping strategies. An authoritarian style (high control and pressure, low warmth) of parenting was associated with high levels of depersonalization, anxiety, and passive coping and low levels of active coping. Authoritative (high control and warmth, moderate pressure) and permissive (high warmth, low control and pressure) styles of parenting were both associated with high levels of active coping and low levels of depersonalization. A permissive style of parenting was associated with low levels of anxiety and an authoritative style of parenting was associated with moderate levels of anxiety. An indifferent (low control and warmth, moderate pressure) style of parenting was associated with low levels of active coping and moderate levels of depersonalization and anxiety (Wolfradt, Hempel, & Miles). Kim-Cohen and colleagues (2004) examined environmental processes involved in children’s resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation using data from the Environmental Risk Longitudinal Twin Study. Participants included 1,116 (49% male) five year old twin pairs (56% monozygotic, 44% dizygotic). The CBCL teacher and parent forms were completed. Observations of the child in new situations, social interactions, talkativeness, self-confidence, and self-reliance were also done. Higher 18 levels of maternal emotional warmth were significantly associated with children’s behavioral resilience in the context of SES deprivation. The researchers also concluded that parental warmth and caring may serve to promote good behavioral regulation and less antisocial behavior in children (Kim-Cohen et al., 2004). Shaw and colleagues (2004) examined emotional support from parents early in life, aging, and health among 2,905 (56% female) participants aged 25 to 74 years (mean age 44). A study-devised comprehensive questionnaire included questions pertaining to depressive symptoms, chronic conditions, emotional support, negative interaction, personal control beliefs, and self-esteem. Current mental and physical health was found to be associated with psychosocial conditions, parental support in particular, in childhood. Early parental support was found to be significantly associated with health across much of the adult life span. Those who reported the experience of abundant support from parents during childhood were significantly more likely to have relatively good health throughout adulthood. Those who had inadequate parental support in childhood were significantly more likely to have poorer health as adults. Personal control, self-esteem, and family relationships (support and negative interaction) accounted for most of the relationship between early parental support and adult depressive symptoms. Personal control, self-esteem, and negative interaction accounted for a large portion of the relationship between parental support and adult physical health (Shaw et al., 2004). To summarize, a significant amount of support exists for relationships between specific negative parenting behaviors and psychological adjustment. Specifically, inconsistent and harsh discipline as well as over-involvement by parents have been 19 implicated in later substance abuse and depression. Parenting behaviors that entail lack of warmth, poor care, harsh discipline, rejection, lack of affection, and engendering of guilt have been implicated in suicidal thoughts and behaviors as well as both internalizing and externalizing problems. Parental behaviors that are more warm, supportive, and accepting have been implicated in the fostering of moral development, better adjustment under stress and in social problem solving. Consistent support has also been found for important relationships between specific parenting behaviors and self-concept and the social self. Parenting Behaviors and the Social Self and Self-Concept. The family is the first place where people learn social behavior and where they develop and solidify ideas about their social selves based on interactions with primary caretakers. Research has identified important relationships between specific past parenting behaviors and the social self. The specific parental behaviors of overprotection, rejection, and lack of emotional warmth have been associated with social anxiety and phobia (Bogels, van Oosten, Muris, & Smulders, 2001). Participants in a study by Bogels and colleagues consisted of 190 (99 girls) children between the ages of 8 and 18 years and their parents. The clinical group consisted of 64 children (31 girls) who were referred for diagnosis and treatment at a community mental health center. The 126 members (68 girls) of the control group were recruited from elementary and secondary schools. The children completed the Social Anxiety Scale, the EMBU-C, and the Parent Attitudes Towards Child Rearing Scale (simplified for use with the children). The parents completed the parent version of the EMBU, the Parent Attitudes Towards Child Rearing Scale, and the Fear Questionnaire. Results indicated that children’s experience of parental overprotection was weakly 20 associated with social anxiety in childhood. In addition, socially anxious children reported more parental rejection and less emotional warmth than controls. Parish and McCluskey (1992) found a relationship between self-concept and perceived levels of parental warmth and hostility. Participants (123 college students ranging in age from 18 to 34 years) were asked to rate on a seven-point scale level of 1) restrictiveness vs. permissiveness and 2) warmth vs. hostility of each of their parents. In addition, they evaluated themselves and both of their parents on the Personal Attribute Inventory. Similarly, results of a longitudinal investigation by Koestner and colleagues (1991) suggest a significant relationship between early experiences of parenting behaviors that were more rejecting and restrictive and the development of a self-critical personality style. These researchers studied 156 (83 girls) participants who were five years old at original participation in the study. Age five measures consisted of a standardized interview on parenting dimensions that gathered information from the participant’s mother pertaining to her attitudes, feelings, and behaviors related to childrearing. Global indices of parental restrictiveness and parental rejection were created through manipulation of the scores from each of the dimensions related to these constructs, based on factor analyses of the coded data from the interviews. Additionally, ratings of the child’s temperament were obtained from the mothers’ descriptions of their child’s behavior prior to age five. Follow-up was conducted when the participants were age 12, and again at age 31. At the age 12 follow-up, participants were administered the Self-Criticism Scale, the Self-Concept Inventory, a composite measure of self-criticism which was created by combining the two previous scales, and an antisocial aggressive attitudes scale. At age 31 follow-up, the participants were administered the Adjective 21 Checklist, a derivation of an Adjective Checklist Self-Criticism scale, and a questionnaire on aggressive actions and inhibited aggressive impulses. A positive relationship between parental restrictiveness and rejection and self-criticism in adulthood was found. Maternal restrictiveness and rejection were most important in predicting self-criticism in females at age 12. For males, paternal restrictiveness was the only significant predictor of selfcriticism at age 12. Overall, males’ experience of fathers who were more restrictive and rejecting significantly predicted later self-criticism. For females, the experience of restricting and rejecting mothers was significantly related to later self-criticism. Similarly, Kitahara (1987) found a significant relationship between 71 adult participants’ perceptions of the experience of parental rejection in childhood as measured by the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire and negative personality assessment of the self as an adult as measured by the Personality Assessment Questionnaire. Cheng and Furnham (2004) explored perceived parental rearing style, self-esteem and self-criticism as predictors of happiness using 356 (97 females) participants aged 15 to 25 (mean age 17). The Parental Bonding Instrument, Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale, Self-Criticism Questionnaire, and Oxford Happiness Inventory were completed. Maternal factors were found to be direct correlates of happiness. Specifically, maternal care significantly predicted happiness both directly and indirectly. Maternal warmth was positively associated with self-esteem. Parental discouragement of behavioral freedom was significantly associated with self evaluation, self-criticism, happiness, and depression. Parental denial of psychological autonomy was significantly associated with self-esteem and self-criticism (Cheng & Furnham). 22 A study of 30 parent-child dyads by Alessandri and Lewis (1993) investigated the effects of negative evaluations and lack of positive evaluations by parents. These researchers concluded through observation of interactions between parent and child (three-year-olds) during performance tasks, that experiences of negative feedback and lack of positive feedback were associated with the child’s expression of shame. A longitudinal study by Allaman, Joyce, and Crandell (1972) identified a relationship between rejecting parental behaviors and resulting preoccupation with evaluative statements of others. In the first study of their two-study design, their sample consisted of 95 children between the ages of 6 and 12 years. Home visits took place semiannually and Parent Behavior Ratings (PBR) were made immediately after each visit. The children completed the Children’s Social Desirability Scale on three different occasions with a three-year interval between each administration. The second study consisted of 65 participants between the ages of 18 and 26 years. The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and the Parent Behavior Inventory were completed by each participant. The researchers concluded from the results of both studies that harsh parental child-rearing behaviors, specifically hostility, criticism, restrictiveness, punitiveness, and coercion, result in the individual’s placing high importance on social evaluation and a lower expectation of being socially accepted. Arrindell, Emmelkamp, Monsma, and Brilman (1983) found that adults who were socially phobic tended to perceive both of their parents as rejecting, overprotective, and lacking in emotional warmth. Their participants consisted of 40 agoraphobics, 29 social phobics and 21 height phobics as well as a control group of 277 non-patient normal volunteers. All participants completed the EMBU. In comparison to controls, social 23 phobics perceived both parents as low on emotional warmth or care and high on rejection and overprotection. Agoraphobics perceived both parents as low on emotional warmth and only their mothers as high on rejection. Height phobics perceived both their parents as low on emotional warmth and high on rejection and overprotection. Vitaro and colleagues (2000) concluded from their research that positive parental attachment reduces the influence of deviant friends. Participants consisted of 567 boys who were part of a longitudinal study beginning during kindergarten. Teachers completed the Social Behavior Questionnaire when the participants were age 6 and again at age 10. At 11 and 12 years of age, the Pupil Evaluation Inventory was used to obtain peer assessments. Attitudes towards delinquency were obtained through a six-item questionnaire created by the researchers that the participants completed when they were 11 and 12 years old. Additionally at ages 11 and 12, participants completed a studydesigned questionnaire pertaining to family experiences (parental monitoring, emotional attachment to parents). At ages 13 and 14, participants completed items from the SelfReported Delinquency Questionnaire. The researchers concluded that, initially, parental monitoring aids in the prevention of association with deviant friends. Once an association has been made, the child’s affective bond with the parents provides a buffer against the influence of those deviant peers. Parental rejection was significantly related to delinquency or deviance even after controlling for the effects of other family variables of control, organization, religiosity, and conflict (Vitaro, Brendgen & Tremblay, 2000). Kim and Rohner (2003) explored perceived parental acceptance and emotional empathy among 725 (408 females) undergraduate students aged 19 to 21 (mean age 19). The Parental Acceptance Rejection Questionnaire and the Emotional Empathy Test were 24 used. Results revealed significant positive correlations between parental (both maternal and paternal) acceptance and emotional empathy. Maternal acceptance by itself was significantly associated with daughters’ but not sons’ empathy while paternal acceptance by itself was significantly associated with sons’ but not daughters’ empathy (Kim & Rohner, 2003). In summary, the relationship between specific parenting behaviors and social functioning is well-established. Specifically, overprotection, rejection, and low warmth in parenting have been implicated in social anxiety and phobia, a tendency to be preoccupied with evaluations of others, and a lower expectation of being socially accepted. Rejecting and restricting parenting has been found to be related to a more selfcritical personality style. More warmth and less hostility have been found to foster a more positive self-concept. Previous research has also explored relationships between specific parenting behaviors and other areas of functioning, like hostility and aggression and more severe psychiatric symptoms. Parenting Behaviors and Hostility and Aggression. Meesters, Muris, and Esselink (1995) identified an association between hostility and perceived parental behavior. Participants consisted of 291 students (age range 17 to 34 years) in schools of Higher Vocational Education (physiotherapy, ergotherapy (increasing comfort in the environment), speech therapy, and teacher training college) who completed the EMBU and the Cook and Medley Hostility scale. Participants who scored high in hostility reported experiencing more parental rejection and overprotection and less emotional warmth than those who scored lower in hostility. 25 Different types of parenting behavior have been found to moderate the relationship between aggressive behavior in early adolescence and later aggressive episodes (Brendgen et al., 2001). Brendgen and colleagues’ longitudinal study tested 525 male participants at age 13 and again at age 16 or 17. Teachers rated proactive and reactive aggressive behaviors of the participants at age 13. When the participants reached the age of 16 or 17, they were asked to complete a measure focusing on delinquencyrelated physical violence and physical violence against a dating partner (the Physical Aggression subscale of the Conflict Tactics Scale). Parental supervision was assessed at ages 13, 14 and 15. Participants completed the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) at age 15 to measure parental warmth and caregiving behavior. Those who scored high in reactive aggression (affective, defensive, angry outbursts in response to actual or perceived provocations or threats) initially, but who also reported experiencing parents who were warm and caring, were found to be less likely to display later violence against a dating partner. Low to moderate warmth and caregiving was found to play a crucial role in boys’ early reactive aggression which, in turn, predicted later dating violence. Reactively aggressive dispositions appear to be moderated by the experience of parental warmth and caregiving which results in less physical violence against a dating partner later in life. The researchers concluded that high parental warmth and caregiving fostered the development of more positive expectations and ideas about close relationships, and this reduced the risk of aggressive and violent behavior towards a dating partner (Brendgen et al). In an investigation of levels of hostility in 67 adult male myocardial infarction patients and 139 healthy controls (Meesters & Muris, 1996), specific perceived parental 26 rearing behaviors were found to be related to levels of hostility in the adult patients as well as in healthy adult male controls. Measures utilized were the EMBU, the CookMedley Hostility Scale, and the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory. Specifically, perceived parental rejection and lack of emotional warmth were found to be associated with high levels of hostility. Parental rejection appeared to be particularly related to more expressive characteristics of hostility (assault, verbal hostility, indirect hostility, irritability), while lack of emotional warmth was found to be strongly associated with experiential characteristics of hostility (resentment and suspicion). These findings support the crucial role that particular perceived parental rearing behaviors have in the development of psychological characteristics, specifically hostility, that may increase risk for coronary disease (Meesters & Muris). A more recent study by Chang and colleagues (2003) explored harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression using 325 children (45% female) ages 3 to 6 (mean age 4). The Parental Acceptance Rejection Questionnaire (completed by both parents), Emotion Regulation Checklist (completed by mother), and a teacher’s rating of the child’s aggression were used. Both mother’s and father’s harsh parenting were found to be strongly associated with both the child’s emotion regulation and aggression. The child’s emotion regulation was found to be significantly correlated with his or her aggression (Chang et al.). To summarize, strong support exists for an important relationship between parenting behaviors, specifically, rejection, overprotection, lack of emotional warmth, and low care, and increased levels of hostility and aggression. Research has also 27 explored relationships between specific parenting behaviors and psychiatric symptomatology. Parenting Behaviors and More Severe Psychiatric Problems. Winefield and colleagues (1989) found that adult participants who were rated by psychiatrists as showing evidence of the existence of a psychiatric disorder reported less supportive parental relationships compared to those without any evidence of a psychiatric disorder. Individuals who reported experiencing parents who were more supportive, non-rejecting and not over-involved also reported less depression, less social isolation, and more overall life satisfaction. Their sample consisted of 101 adult participants (55 women) who completed the s-EMBU (a shortened form of the EMBU) in addition to scales on work satisfaction, self-esteem, depressive affect, social isolation, and total life satisfaction. Benjaminsen and colleagues (1984) examined the relationship between parental rearing behaviors and features of adult personality. They sampled 200 adults ranging in age from 18 to 72 years (114 females). The participants completed the EMBU, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, and Lazare-Klerman-Armor Trait Scale. The researchers reported that adult female participants who reported that their fathers were guiltengendering scored higher on the Neuroticism scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ). Higher scores on the Psychoticism scale of the EPQ were associated with the experience of rejecting fathers and shaming mothers. Psychopathology of female participants was related to perceptions of specific parental rearing behaviors such as guilt-engendering, shaming, and being punitive and overinvolved (Benjaminsen et al). 28 Research examining associations between parental behaviors and schizophrenia found that parents of schizophrenic patients displayed less warmth compared to parents of controls (McCreadie, Williamson, Athawes, Connolly, & Tilak-Singh, 1994). McCreadie and colleagues (1994) concluded that perceived parental behaviors of rejection and overprotection may increase the severity of current symptoms of adult schizophrenic patients. Participants consisted of 50 adults (18 females) diagnosed with schizophrenia. The EMBU was completed in addition to an interview using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale and an interview using the Social Deficit Syndrome Scale. Mothers of the participants were interviewed through the use of the Scale for Assessment of Premorbid Schizoid and Schizotypal Traits and Premorbid Social Adjustment Scale. The results of the research summarized in this literature review highlight the significant impact of parenting that occurs across the lifespan. In particular, parental warmth, rejection, and overprotection have a significant role in psychological adjustment and functioning across the lifespan. Specifically, parenting styles that are less warm, more rejecting, and overprotective are associated with negative psychological functioning and adjustment in childhood that extends into adulthood. The importance of positive parenting behaviors is further supported by research that suggests that parenting styles persist across generations. 29 Negativity, negative comments, lack of warmth, and lack of positive attitude towards the child Lower in affection and either very low or very high in control Harsh parenting Maternal warmth and care Unfair, inconsistent, and harsh discipline Maternal overinvolvement Caspi et al., 2004 Kim & Rohner, 2003 Kim et al., 2003 Chang et al., 2003 Cheng & Furnham, 2004 Holmes & Robins, 1987 Kerver et al., 1992 Chambers et al., 2001 Brendgen et al., 2001 Bogels et al., 2001 Benjaminsen et al., 1984 30 Parental acceptance Higher levels of harsh-inconsistent and hostile parenting and lower levels of warm, nurturant-involved parenting Negative feedback and lack of positive feedback Overprotection, rejection, and lack of emotional warmth Guilt-engendering, shaming, and being punitive and over-involved Overprotection, rejection, and lack of emotional warmth Low warmth and caregiving Alessandri & Lewis, 1993 Arrindell et al., 1983 Parenting Behaviors Researcher(s) Summary of Outcomes Found to be Associated with Experiences of Specific Parenting Behaviors Table 1 Emotional empathy Conduct problems, depressive symptoms Poor emotion regulation, high aggression Happiness, high self-esteem Alcohol and depressive disorders Depression Anxiety, depression, delinquency High in reactive aggression (affective, defensive, angry outbursts in response to actual or perceived provocations or threats) Antisocial behavior problems High neuroticism, psychoticism, psychopathology Social anxiety and phobia Social anxiety and phobia Expression of shame Outcomes Rejection and overprotection More parental rejection and overprotection and less emotional warmth Low warmth, high rejection and overprotection More hostile, rejecting, overprotecting, and punitive Strict Discipline, low warmth, rejection Low parental warmth and high hostility Rejection McCreadie et al., 1994 Meesters & Muris, 1996 Meesters, Muris, & Esselink, 1995 Moris, Meesters, & van den Berg, 2003 Muris et al., 1996 31 Negative self-concept Negative attention-seeking behaviors (boisterous, disruptive), temper outbursts, anger, resentment, irritability, impatience Dysfunctional levels of emotional involvement warmth, support, and acceptance Lumley et al., 1996 Magnus et al., 1999 Palmer & Hollin, 1996 Palmer & Hollin, 1997 Palmer & Hollin, 1999 Parish & McCluskey, 1992 Peretti, Clark, & Johnson, 1985 Insecure attachment, anxious-depressed, somatic complaints, aggression, delinquent behavior Externalizing and disruptive behaviors, conduct problems Poor attachment, low moral development, Antisocial behaviors, delinquency Maternal concern, restrictiveness Leung, McBride-Chang, & Lai, 2004 Low over-all life satisfaction, low academic competence Problems with emotional expression Better adjustment under stress, in school, and in social problem-solving Increased severity of current symptoms of adult schizophrenic patients High hostility (assault, verbal hostility, indirect hostility, irritability, resentment, suspicion) Rejection and restrictiveness Kitahara, 1987 Koestner et al.,1991 Behavioral resilience, good behavioral regulation, and less antisocial behavior Self-critical personality style Higher levels of emotional warmth and care Kim-Cohen et al., 2004 Inter-Generational Transmission of Parenting Behaviors Intergenerational transmission of parenting has been defined as a process that entails an earlier generation psychologically influencing the parenting attitudes and behaviors of the next generation (Van IJzendoorn, 1992). Past research has examined and found strong evidence supporting the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors (Simons, Bearman, Conger, & Chao, 1992; Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Wu, 1991; Whitbeck et al., 1992). For example, Simons and colleagues (1992) conducted a study in which they obtained data from approximately 200 two-parent families that included a 7th grade child and at least one sibling who was within four years of age of the 7th grade child. Participants completed the Supportive Parenting Questionnaire, Harsh Discipline Scale, Hostile Parenting Scale, Discipline Beliefs Scale, and the Impact of Parenting Scale. Additionally, mothers and fathers reported on the supportive parenting and harsh parenting of their parents. They concluded that parents communicate their parenting beliefs directly through their parenting practices. Their results also indicated that parents who supported corporal punishment tended to utilize a more harsh style of discipline and this more harsh style of discipline was found to be related to adolescent children subscribing to corporal punishment as an effective means of discipline. Additionally, parents who viewed parenting as having an important impact on child development were significantly more likely to be more warm and supportive in their parenting and, in turn, 32 their children also reported beliefs in the importance of positive parenting on child development (Simons, Bearman, Conger, & Chao). Research by Whitbeck and colleagues (1992) explored intergenerational aspects of depressed mood and perceived parental rejection. They collected survey and observational data from a sample of 451 intact families with at least one seventh grade child and a sibling whose age was within four years of the seventh grade child’s age. Interviewers visited each family for two hours on two occasions. At the first visit, a set of questionnaires was completed which measured individual characteristics and family processes. On the second visit (within two weeks of the first), the family was videotaped engaging in structured interaction tasks. Trained observers rated the videotaped interactions on specific aspects of family interaction and individual characteristics. Generation one rejection was measured by the parents completing Elliots’ parental rejection scale which consists of five-items inquiring into specific rejecting parental behaviors (e.g., My mother/father really trusted me, My mother/father found fault with me even when I didn’t deserve it, My mother/father really cared for me). Both parents (generation 2) completed the SCL-90, Elliots’ self-report parental rejection scale, reported on their spouse’s depressed affect, and an observer reported on observed depressed affect of each parent. The children (generation 3) completed the depression subscale of the SCL-90, Elliots’ parental rejection scale, parents reported on the child’s depressed affect, and a researcher reported observations of the children’s and parents’ depressed affect. The researchers concluded that perceived rejection by parents increases the probability of depressed affect in adults. Depressed affect, in turn, increases the probability of parental behaviors that are rejecting. Specifically, parental behaviors that 33 are rejecting increased the probability of depressed affect among offspring, which increased the likelihood that these offspring became rejecting parents. These results indicate that parental rearing behaviors may be transmitted across generations through the development of personality traits that have an impact on parenting, as well as through modeling of parental behaviors (Whitbeck et al.). A more recent review of the literature that focused on intergenerational studies of parenting and the transfer of risk from parent to child was performed by Serbin and Karp (2003). Based on their review, Serbin and Karp suggest three main pathways involved in the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors. First, problematic parenting may be the result of a combination of modeling by the individual’s own parents and the extension of a pattern of aggressive and antisocial behavior typically observed in early childhood. Parents with a history of childhood aggression have been consistently found to have continuing social, behavioral, and health difficulties, as do their offspring. Second, childhood aggression and antisocial behavior appear to significantly contribute to problematic parenting and negative outcomes for offspring through the identified pathway from childhood behavior patterns to adolescent risk-taking behavior, low education and occupational attainment, maternal smoking, substance abuse, and poverty, as well as other problematic conditions. Third, parental involvement, warmth, and nurturance have important protective effects on later family formation and parenting (Serbin & Karp). Child Maltreatment and Its Outcomes Intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors is a particularly critical issue in relationship to child maltreatment. Child maltreatment typically includes 34 experiences of sexual abuse, physical abuse, physical neglect, emotional abuse, and/or emotional neglect. Based on their review of the past decade of literature, Kaplan and colleagues (1999) devised definitions for each type of maltreatment. Specifically, sexual abuse includes intrusion/penetration, molestation with genital contact, or other unspecified acts that do not necessarily involve genital contact. Physical abuse includes the experience of injury or risk of injury as a result of having been hit with a hand or other object or having been punched, kicked, shaken, thrown, burned, stabbed, or choked by a parent or parent substitute. The term physical neglect references harm or endangerment that is the result of refusal or delay of health care, abandonment, expulsion, and inadequate nutrition, clothing, hygiene, and supervision. Emotional abuse includes verbal or emotional assault/abuse, harsh nonphysical punishments, close confinement, or threats of other maltreatment. Emotional neglect includes failure to provide adequate nurturance/affection and emotional support, permitting drug/alcohol abuse, permitting other maladaptive behavior, refusing/delaying psychological care, or permitting exposure to domestic violence (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna) The adverse and lifelong impact of maltreatment on mental health has been welldocumented. Maltreatment experiences in childhood lead to overwhelming levels of stress that have been associated with alterations of biological stress systems and with adverse influences on brain development (DeBellis, 2001). Adults with child maltreatment histories are more likely to manifest multiple health risk behaviors and serious medical illnesses as well as higher psychiatric and medical utilization rates (DeBellis, 2001). Ecological, social, biological, and psychological conditions that have been found to be associated with maltreatment may result in the inability to successfully 35 complete major and critical stages of development, which may inevitably have a profound impact on functioning across the life span (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001). A brief overview of the following literature review on child maltreatment and its outcomes is provided at the end of the section (Table 2). Developmental Perspectives on Child Maltreatment Outcomes. A developmental psychopathology approach has been utilized in a growing number of studies of child maltreatment (Manly et al., 2001). From the very beginning of life children are faced with central developmental tasks and the successful completion of these tasks prepares the child for subsequent developmental issues and each developmental task retains significance throughout the life span. Environments where maltreatment exists are extreme deviations from normal environments and these negative environments have a detrimental impact on children’s capacities to adaptively negotiate and successfully complete important developmental tasks (Manley et al.). Manly and colleagues (2001) examined the contributions of developmental timing and subtype on the adjustment of maltreated children using 492 maltreated (physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional maltreatment, physical neglect) and 322 non-maltreated comparison children aged 5-11 (mean age of 7) who attended a day camp. Of the total sample, 38% were female. The CBCL-Teacher Form, a behavior checklist to assess aggressive, withdrawn and cooperative behavior in peer interactions, and another studydevised questionnaire which assessed personality, social, and cognitive functioning were completed by camp counselors. Maltreated children were found to exhibit significantly higher levels of both internalizing and externalizing symptoms compared to nonmaltreated children. Specifically, maltreated children were found to be more 36 aggressive, withdrawn, and less cooperative than nonmaltreated children. Maltreated children were also found to be more frequently rated by their peers as more aggressive, more disruptive, and less cooperative than nonmaltreated children. Lower ego resiliency and higher ego undercontrol were more common among maltreated children compared to nonmaltreated children. Early and chronic maltreatment was found to more negatively impact the maltreated child’s capacities to competently and successfully complete developmental challenges. Additionally, the severity of later maltreatment was also found to contribute to an array of negative outcomes. The researchers determined that the negative impact of maltreatment experiences is likely to persist into adulthood (Manly et al., 2001). The long-term and negative consequences of maltreatment experiences have been well-documented (Edwards et al., 2003; Glaser, 2000; Higgins & McCabe, 2003; Kendall-Tackett, 2002). A review of the literature by Glaser (2000) revealed strong support for an association between childhood maltreatment and social, emotional, behavioral, and cognitive maladaptation as well as psychopathology. Additionally, independent associations between sexual, physical, and emotional abuse and adult mental health problems have been consistently supported (Glaser). Domestic Violence as a Form of Maltreatment. Exposure to domestic violence has been more recently classified as a form of maltreatment (Holden, 2003). An extensive review of the literature on children’s exposure to domestic violence performed by Holden revealed that children are aware of much of the domestic violence that occurs in the home. Holden’s review found that exposure to domestic violence leads to symptoms of Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) as well as other internalizing and 37 externalizing problems. Children exposed to domestic violence are considered to be maltreated because they are living in an environment that is psychologically abusive. Children are terrorized by marital violence, which is defined as “caregiver behavior that threatens or is likely to physically hurt, kill, abandon, or place the child or child’s loved ones or objects in recognizably dangerous situations” (Holden, p. 156). Holden’s literature review revealed that exposure to a parent being verbally or physically assaulted leads to physiological arousal, emotional distress, and trauma symptoms for the child that are related to experiences of fear regarding safety as well as generalized fear and hopelessness. These abusive adult relationships also model violence as a way of handling interpersonal interactions. Specifically, physical abuse or witnessing of domestic violence serve as learning models for children who learn that violence is both a normal part of a close relationship and the appropriate way to respond to stress (Cunningham, 2003). The review by Holden revealed that, in domestic violence situations, children experience comments that degrade, belittle, criticize, and ridicule. Additionally, these children may also experience lack of appropriate emotional responsiveness from caregivers (ignoring of the child’s attempts and needs to interact and showing no positive emotion to the child) and isolation (confining or placing unreasonable limits on the child or on contact with others). Neglect of mental health, medical, or educational needs has also consistently been found to be common in families where domestic violence occurs (Holden). There is also an increased likelihood or high rate of overlap between domestic violence and physical child abuse as well as some risk for sexual abuse. Witnessing of domestic violence has consistently been found to have a detrimental impact on the functioning of the exposed child (Mabanglo, 2002; Maughan & 38 Cicchetti, 2002). An extensive review of the literature by Mabanglo revealed strong support for a link between exposure to domestic violence and the development of a wide range of symptomatology and behavioral maladjustment, specifically higher rates of internalizing and externalizing problems, non-compliance, aggression, anxiety, depression, attention deficits, and PTSD. Maughan and Cicchetti (2002) investigated the impact of domestic violence on children’s emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment using a sample of 88 (46.6% male) maltreated and 51 (54.9% male) nonmaltreated children in mother-child dyads. The children’s ages ranged 4 to 6 years with a mean age of five. The children were exposed to three scripted simulations of affective interactions (friendly, angry, and reconciliatory) between their mothers and a research assistant and interviewed after each regarding their emotional responses as well as rated by observers during the interactions. The Conflict Tactics Scale and CBCL were completed by the mothers. A strong relationship was found between domestic violence and parent-child aggression in that mothers of maltreated children reported significantly higher incidents of inter-adult verbal aggression and physical violence in the home than did mothers of the nonmaltreated children. Physical abuse and neglect independently predicted significant elevations in children’s delinquent, withdrawn, and social problem behaviors. Physically abused children compared to nonmaltreated children had significantly higher maternal reports of aggression and increased levels of anxious and depressed symptoms. Overall, physically abused children were significantly more symptomatic across each of the five behavior categories compared to the nonmaltreated children, followed by children in the neglected group and the two maltreatment groups (neglect and physical abuse). 39 Dysregulated emotional patterns (undercontrolled/ambivalent or overcontrolled/unresponsive) were displayed by 80% of the maltreated children compared to 37% of the nonmaltreated children. The researchers suggest that, though adaptive in chaotic and threatening environments, these emotional styles may lead to increased vulnerability to other developmental risks associated with social functioning, such as poor peer relations and socioemotional difficulties (Maughan & Cicchetti). Child Maltreatment and Long-Term Adjustment. Kendall-Tackett (2002) performed an extensive literature review that focused on the long-term effects of childhood abuse. Based on this review, the researcher suggested a model using a health psychology framework based on identified behavioral, social, cognitive, and emotional pathways. Results of this literature review suggest that abuse during childhood puts the individual at risk for problems in specific areas of life-functioning, including high rates of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder, the tendency to participate in harmful activities, a likelihood of having difficulties in relationships, and having negative beliefs and attitudes towards self and others. These problems are strongly interrelated, and their presence increases the likelihood of health problems (Kendall-Tackett). Higgins and McCabe (2003) explored the relationship between maltreatment and adjustment through two studies. The first study utilized a community sample of 48 biological parents who were asked to report on the adjustment of their children. Fifty children (22 female) who ranged in age from 5 to 12 years (mean age of 8.6 years) were described. The Comprehensive Child Maltreatment Scale for Parents was used to measure sexual abuse, physical abuse, psychological maltreatment, neglect and witnessing family violence. The Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scales II 40 (FACES II), the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), and the Child Sexual Behavior Inventory were also utilized. The results of the first study revealed that having one episode of maltreatment increased the chance of additional episodes. The overall experience of maltreatment was found to be negatively related to each of the measures of adjustment. In comparing the impact of each individual form of maltreatment, similar contributions to poor levels of adjustment were found. Psychological maltreatment was found to have the strongest association with maladjustment, followed by witnessing family violence. Specific family background variables of poor family adaptability, low family cohesion, sexual punitiveness, and divorce were found to be strong predictors of all types of maltreatment. The second study conducted by Higgins and McCabe (2003) consisted of a community sample of 138 adults (119 female) who reported on maltreatment experiences (sexual, physical, psychological, neglect, witnessing family violence), family functioning during childhood, and current psychological adjustment. The Comprehensive Child Maltreatment Scale for Adults, FACES II, Trauma Symptom Checklist (TSC-40), and the Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale were completed. The correlation between psychological maltreatment and the other types of maltreatment was the strongest, suggesting that psychological maltreatment often occurs concurrently with and may actually be a component of other types of maltreatment. Low family adaptability and sexual punitiveness were among the strongest predictors of maltreatment. Both experiences of maltreatment and negative family environment were found to make significant contributions to adjustment. Neglect made a unique contribution to low self-esteem. The five types of maltreatment all predicted adults’ reports of trauma symptoms and self- 41 depreciation. Family background variables also predicted trauma symptoms. Lack of positive relationships with others and lack of purpose in life were significantly predicted by the five maltreatment scales. To summarize results across both studies, child maltreatment was found to be negatively associated with the three measures of adjustment in childhood. In addition, child maltreatment was also associated with both adjustment problems and aspects of growth and development in adulthood (Higgins & McCabe). Recently, Edwards and colleagues (2003) investigated the relationship between maltreatment and mental health using data from 8,667 questionnaires returned (54.2% female) from community respondents recruited from physician’s offices who were undergoing physical examinations. Ages ranged from 19 to 97 with the average age being 55. A 36-item Short Form Health Survey was utilized to assess current mental health status. A 162-item Family Health Questionnaire was also administered that inquired into childhood abuse (sexual, physical, witness domestic violence, emotional abuse) and exposure to family dysfunction as well as current health behaviors and conditions. Results revealed significant relationships between all forms of maltreatment and compromised mental health. Mental health was found to worsen as the number of abuse types increased, with the experience of emotional abuse accentuating this effect. Specifically, mental health worsened as the intensity of emotional abuse increased. The experience of multiple forms of maltreatment was common, with 34% of the abused sample reporting at least two types of maltreatment. Emotional abuse was found to play a particularly important role in adult mental health as an independent predictor and also served to heighten the effect of other types of abuse experienced (Edwards et al.). 42 Negative outcomes for maltreated children not only stem from the severe negative impact that these experiences have on overall functioning, but also seem related to other aspects of the overall environment of the maltreated child. Lau and Weisz (2003) examined the implications for presenting problems, treatment attrition, and long-term outcomes for maltreated children using 343 (123 females) youth, 157 (ages 7-17, mean age 11) who were maltreated (physical, sexual, neglect, emotional) and 186 who were not, as determined by child protective services records using the Maltreatment Classification System. Other measures utilized include the CBCL and the Brief Symptoms Inventory which were completed by parents, and Coddington’s Life Events Scale for Children (LES) was completed by the children. Maltreatment was found to be associated with poorer treatment persistence (attend less, more likely to drop out early) which was found to be due to characteristics of the maltreating parents (fear of being reported, lack of concern for child, stressful and disorganized family environment, parents’ negative feelings about the credibility or effectiveness of treatment). Poorer long-term outcome was found among maltreated children who demonstrated the same level of problem severity when reassessed two years after initial completion of measures (Lau & Weisz). Child Maltreatment and Social Functioning. The ability to create and maintain relationships is affected by past abuse (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; KendallTackett, 2002; Mabanglo, 2002; Manly et al., 2001). Kendall-Tackett’s review of the past decade of literature revealed relationship dysfunction to be a “pandemic” outcome of childhood sexual abuse as well as other forms of child maltreatment. Kendall-Tackett concluded that adult survivors of child maltreatment appear to adopt one of two distinct 43 problematic interpersonal styles. The first, which is referred to as avoidant, entails low interdependency, low self disclosure, low warmth, few interpersonal ties and few friends, and decreased likelihood of marriage. The second, which is referred to as intrusive, entails extreme needs for closeness, excessive self disclosure, and the tendency to be excessively warm and overly demanding and controlling in relationships. Interpersonally, abused/maltreated children may become either aggressive and domineering or may become overly submissive to appease others in an attempt to protect themselves from what they anticipate to be pervasive social threat (Shields & Cicchetti, 2001). Shields and Cicchetti (2001) explored the relationship between maltreatment and bullying and victimization using 169 maltreated (60 females) and 98 nonmaltreated comparison children (35 females) who ranged in age from 8 to 12 years (mean age 8). The Mount Hope Family Center Bully-Victim Questionnaire, the Emotion Regulation QScale, Emotion Regulation Checklist, Child Behavior Checklist-Teachers Form, peer ratings, and Minnesota Behavior Ratings-Agency and Dependency were used. Maltreated (sexual and physical abuse in particular) children were significantly more likely to bully their peers and to be more at risk for victimization compared to nonmaltreated children. Maltreated (physical and sexual abuse in particular) children were also found to be more emotionally dysregulated (more anxious and hyperaroused, angry, unempathic) and disruptive which placed them at increased risk for bullying and victimization. The researchers concluded that the maladaptive social functioning of the maltreated child may be the result of the development of maladaptive cognitive models of relationships as dangerous and malevolent (Shields & Cicchetti). 44 Kaplan and colleagues (1999) concluded from their review of the past decade of literature that the insecure, disorganized patterns of attachment that have been found among those with a history of maltreatment may lead to peer rejection. Their review also revealed consistent findings to support that physically abused and/or neglected kids tend to be more disliked and less popular and to display less intimacy, more conflict, more aggression, and more negative affect towards close friends. Physically neglected children may also display difficulty in understanding appropriate affective responses to interpersonal situations and limited social problem solving skills (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna). Salzinger and colleagues (2001) investigated the effect of abuse on social and emotional functioning using 100 (35 females) 9-12 (mean age 10) year old physically abused children and 100 nonabused comparison children. Social preference, positive reciprocity, peer rejection, and negative reciprocity were measured and the CBCL was completed by parent and teacher. Results revealed abuse to be positively associated with poor outcomes and problem behaviors and negatively associated with positive social expectations. Additionally, a negative association was found between abuse and positive social status outcomes and prosocial behaviors. Children’s social expectations and behavior were found to be mediated by the effect of abuse on positive and negative peer social status and on positive and negative reciprocity. Abused children were more likely than comparison children to behave aggressively and less likely to engage in prosocial behavior. Their tendency toward negative social expectations and antisocial behavior as well as lack of prosocial behavior was found to negatively impact their social status and reciprocity. Salzinger and colleagues concluded that adults in abusive families model 45 externalizing or aggressive behavior both by how they treat the child as well as how they behave towards other adults. Negative social interactions with caregivers at home lead to avoidance and withdrawn behavior in an abused child. Abuse was found to be associated with a pessimistic social outlook that was reflected in children’s reported negative social expectations for friends. These negative social expectations were found to be associated with children’s internalizing problems. Abused children were found to be not as wellliked by their peers as were nonabused children, their friendships were less likely to be reciprocated, and they were more likely to be rejected by classmate peers, even by children whom the abused children considered to be friends. Ultimately, the resulting lack of social and emotional support from both parents and peers places abused children at risk for problematic behaviors over the life-course (Salzinger et al.). Child Maltreatment and Revictimization. The potential for revictimization has been explored as another important outcome related to maltreatment. Kendall-Tackett’s (2002) review of the past decade of literature on child maltreatment revealed five types of experiences during childhood to be strong predictors of later victimization: physical neglect by a caretaker; emotional withdrawal by a caretaker; a caretaker’s failure to provide needed protection; sexual abuse by a noncaretaker; and any type of sexual abuse. Kaplan and colleagues (1999) concluded from their review of the past decade of literature that insecure disorganized patterns of attachment that have consistently been found among those with histories of maltreatment appear to be related to engaging in relationships that are marked by revictimization or the victimizing of others. Revictimization has been postulated to impact health through injury and potential death. The resulting chronic stress has a tendency to lead to self-medicating behaviors of 46 participating in harmful activities such as substance abuse, eating disorders, and smoking. Chronic stress also can lead to an elevation in blood levels of triglycerides, free fatty acids, cholesterol, glucose, and insulin which have been found to be related to cardiovascular disease and diabetes (Kendall-Tackett). Kendall-Tackett (2002) identified a specific internal mental framework among children who have been abused that negatively influences how they interpret stressful life events, the motives and actions of others, beliefs about how much power they have in situations, and how much they can do to help themselves. Specifically, maltreated children develop an internal working model where they tend to view the world as a dangerous place and to overestimate danger and adversity in their environment. In adulthood, this internal working model tends to lead to the tendency to underestimate their sense of self-efficacy in coping with both real and perceived danger as well as to chronic perceptions of helplessness, powerlessness, and danger. These chronically negative, fearful, or mistrusting thoughts can contribute to emotional distress and increase the risk of depression and predictably lead to a shortened life span (KendallTackett). Child Maltreatment Other Psychological Sequelae. A review by Mabanglo (2002) revealed consistent support for a significant relationship between violence exposure, specifically physical abuse, sexual abuse, and exposure to domestic and community violence, and a wide variety of physiological, emotional, and behavioral problems that are associated with long-term effects. Some of the more prominent problems that have been consistently found to be strongly related to exposure to violence, particularly physical abuse, sexual abuse, domestic, and community violence, include 47 depression, phobias, ADHD, sleep disorders, somatization disorder, disorders of attachment, oppositional and conduct disorders, PTSD, dissociative reactions, eating disturbances, and substance abuse (Mabanglo). PTSD rates among sexually abused children have been consistently supported to be in the 40 to 50 percent range and between 60 and 100 percent among adults with histories of childhood sexual abuse. Symptoms of PTSD have been reported to be more severe among children and adults who also experienced the commonly co-occurring physical abuse (Rodriguez, Vande Kemp, & Foy, 1998). Duration, severity, threats of violence, coercion, and perpetrator identity are variables of the sexual abuse experience that have been found to account for a significant portion of the variance in the level of PTSD symptoms, with these variables independently predicting PTSD diagnosis (Mabanglo, 2002; Rodriguez, Vande Kemp, & Foy, 1998). PTSD and depression have been documented as among the most commonly occurring symptoms of maltreatment including physical, sexual, and psychological abuse and physical neglect (Ackerman et al., 1998; Kendall-Tackett, 2002). Compared to individuals without an abuse history, those with an abuse history have been found to have a fourfold greater lifetime risk of developing major depression, as well as being more vulnerable to developing PTSD when exposed to life-stressors in adulthood (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Kendall-Tackett). Other anxiety disorders, conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and ADHD, have been consistently found to occur at rates several times higher than those found among individuals without a history of maltreatment (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999). Emotional maltreatment, in particular, has been implicated to have a stronger relationship than other forms of maltreatment to impaired long term 48 psychological functioning, specifically internalizing and externalizing behaviors, social impairment, low self esteem, suicidal behavior, psychiatric diagnoses, and hospitalizations (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999). Children with maltreatment-related PTSD may be one of the most seriously affected groups of maltreated children. DeBellis (2001) reviewed the past decade of literature on the impact of child maltreatment and concluded that maltreatment-related PTSD may be viewed as a complex, environmentally induced, developmental disorder that places victims of childhood sexual or physical abuse and neglect at increased risk for developing a lifetime history of PTSD and PTSD symptoms. The review found that maltreatment-related trauma may supersede any genetic, social, or psychological resilience factors, thus increasing the risk for PTSD and the impairments associated with PTSD in the majority of abuse and neglect victims (DeBellis). DeBellis determined that lifetime PTSD prevalence rates for interpersonal trauma in all age groups were greater than for non-interpersonal traumas, ranging from 30 to 50% of individuals studied. Additionally, rates of PTSD in maltreated children have been found to be similar to those among children and adults traumatized by war and homicide. DeBellis’s review revealed that even if childhood trauma does not result in PTSD in childhood, it increases the risk for PTSD in adulthood. Ackerman and colleagues (1998) evaluated 204 children (131 female) aged 7 to 13 years who had been sexually abused (100 female), physically abused (10 female), and both sexually and physically abused (21 female). Both child and parent (caregiver) forms of the revised Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (DICA) were administered. The CBCL was completed by caregivers and classroom teachers. 49 Caregivers were interviewed regarding information related to the nature, severity, and duration of abuse, characteristics of perpetrator, and socioeconomic status of the family. Overall, results revealed anxiety and behavior disorders to be more common than mood disorders among all three groups studied. Phobic disorder was found to be the most frequent diagnosis among all three groups, followed by PTSD, separation anxiety, and ODD. Their results indicated a high prevalence of PTSD among all three groups with PTSD likely to be comorbid with one or more other disorders, particularly separation anxiety, major depression, and dysthymia. Earlier age of onset of abuse was found to be a factor in PTSD and other psychiatric disturbances (Ackerman et al.). Safren and colleagues (2002) investigated the relationship between the history of child abuse and panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder among 149 adults with a mean age of 37, 75 (49 women, 26 men) of whom had been diagnosed with panic disorder, 46 (22 women, 24 men) with social phobia, and 28 (14 women, 14 men) with generalized anxiety disorder. Diagnoses were made using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV; the Clinical Global Impressions Scale was used to rate anxiety severity; and a semistructured interview was used to obtain information regarding physical and sexual abuse as well as other childhood history (e.g., divorce, parental death). Results indicated that those with more than one of the three anxiety disorders tended to have increased rates of childhood physical or sexual abuse compared to those with just one diagnosis. Specifically, 31 percent of those with anxiety disorder comorbidity and 18 percent of those with no anxiety disorder comorbidity had histories of childhood physical or sexual abuse. Additionally, seventeen percent of the participants were found to have comorbid major depressive disorder (MDD). Those with 50 the comorbid MDD were found to be significantly more likely to report a history of childhood physical or sexual abuse compared to those without. With and without controlling for comorbid anxiety disorders or comorbid depression, those with panic disorder had significantly higher rates of past childhood physical or sexual abuse than those with social phobia. Rates of generalized anxiety disorder did not differ significantly from either of the other two diagnoses (Safren et al.). Children with maltreatment-related PTSD demonstrate deficits within the domains of attention and abstract reasoning/executive functioning. Specifically, children with PTSD have been found to be more susceptible to distraction, to demonstrate greater impulsivity, and to make more errors on tasks of sustained attention, as well as weaker ability in the areas of hypothesis testing, problem solving, and semantic organization (Beers & DeBellis, 2002). Ruchkin and colleagues (2002) examined violence exposure, posttraumatic stress, and personality in 300 male adolescents ages 14 to 19 (mean age of 16) who were inmates at a juvenile detention center. The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children – Present and Lifetime Version, Child Posttraumatic Stress Reaction Index, the Youth Self Report, Survey of Exposure to Community Violence, and The Temperament and Character Inventory were utilized. Comorbid diagnoses among the delinquents with PTSD compared to those without PTSD included Major Depression, Separation Anxiety, Generalized Anxiety, ADHD, Conduct Disorder, Alcohol Abuse, and Drug Abuse. A high rate of violence exposure-related PTSD was observed in that most of the participants reported some posttraumatic stress and one quarter of the sample met full criteria for PTSD. Differences in the levels of 51 comorbid psychopathology were also found in those with a full PTSD diagnosis compared to those with a partial PTSD diagnosis. Postraumatic stress was accompanied by a wide range of psychopathology/internalizing problems with highest rates among those who met full criteria for PTSD. The main difference that emerged between the partial and no PTSD groups was the higher rates of major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder among the partial PTSD group. Those with full PTSD were found to present with a wide range of, and generally more than three, comorbid diagnoses and differed from the partial and no PTSD groups in most diagnoses and self-reported problems. Specifically, differences in separation anxiety, past ADHD, and substance abuse existed which may suggest that those children with prior existing psychiatric disorders/emotional susceptibility may be at increased risk for PTSD. A relationship was also found between posttraumatic stress and behavior inhibition, particularly high harm avoidance and low self-directedness, which lead to higher rates of psychopathology and internalizing problems and also lead to the tendency to be more sensitive to stress as a result of poor coping styles and being more fearful, nervous, passive, and low in energy. Higher rates of exposure to violence were related to higher problem scores, greater posttraumatic stress, and other types of psychopathology. The degree of violence exposure was found to be related to both externalizing and internalizing symptoms and the correlations between symptoms and victimization were more significant for direct experience compared to witnessing. Higher rates of PTSD were associated with higher rates of other types of psychopathology (Ruchkin et al., 2002). Muller and colleagues (2000) explored the relationship between exposure to family and community violence and psychopathology among 65 adolescents (40 females) 52 who ranged in age from 13 to 17 years (mean age of 14) and had been admitted to a psychiatric hospital. DSM-IV diagnoses were obtained from chart review, the Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors, My Exposure to Violence, Checklist of Children’s Distress Symptoms: Self-Report Version, and the Child Behavior Checklist – Youth Self Report were used. Participants who had been exposed to or witnessed higher levels of violence (family and community) reported significantly greater psychopathology compared to those with lower levels of exposure or witnessing of violence. Social support was found to act as a buffer against maladaptive effects of experiences of family violence but not community violence. Across all analyses, exposure to family violence (both witnessing and being a victim) was found to be more consistently associated with greater measures of psychopathology (internalizing, externalizing, PTSD symptomatology, and PTSD diagnosis) when social support was low than when it was high. A possible explanation for these finding proposed by the researchers was that exposure to family violence may have a different impact on development, which may allow social support to act as a buffer to the effects of such exposure compared to that of community violence. Specifically, social support serves to improve interpersonal functioning by promoting a more positive overall sense of self (Muller et al.). McLean and Gallop (2003) explored the implications of childhood sexual abuse for adult Borderline Personality Disorder and PTSD using 65 women who had earlyonset (approximately 4 years of age) sexual abuse (38 women) and late-onset (approximately 15 years of age) sexual abuse (27 women). The Revised Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines, the Structured Interview for Disorders of Extreme Stress, and the Traumatic Antecedents Questionnaire were utilized. The diagnoses of both 53 Borderline Personality Disorder and PTSD were both found to be significantly higher in participants reporting early-onset sexual abuse suggesting that sexual abuse may be a significant predictor of meeting criteria for both diagnoses. Nearly all of the women (36 of the 38 participants) with a history of childhood sexual abuse met the diagnostic criteria for both Borderline Personality Disorder and PTSD (McLean & Gallop, 2003). Addiction is another potential consequence of child maltreatment (DeBellis, 2001; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Kendall-Tackett, 2002; Libby et al., 2004; Mabanglo, 2002). Kendall-Tackett’s extensive literature review indicates that individuals with a history of victimization, particularly those who experienced four or more adverse childhood events, were more likely to consider themselves to be alcoholics, to have used illegal drugs, and to have injected drugs. This association has been attributed to the dysregulation of biological stress systems and self-medication for chronic PTSD symptoms (DeBellis, 2001). In addition to alcohol and other drugs, significant and consistent support has been found for an association between a history of childhood maltreatment, particularly sexual and physical abuse, and cigarette smoking (Kaplan et al.; Kendall-Tackett). Problematic eating behaviors and obesity have also consistently been found to be more common among adult survivors of childhood maltreatment, with the relationship particularly strong for those who experienced sexual abuse (Kendall-Tackett, 2002; Mabanglo, 2002). A history of combined physical and sexual abuse has been associated with a fourfold risk of reporting suicidal thoughts, more than a sixfold risk of reporting a suicide plan, a twelvefold risk of reporting a noninjurious suicide attempt, and a fortysevenfold 54 risk of reporting an injurious suicide attempt (Bensley et al., 1999; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Kendall-Tackett, 2002; Mabanglo, 2002). Physical abuse alone has been associated with doubled risk of reporting suicidal thoughts, threefold risk of reporting suicide plan, fivefold risk of reporting noninjurious suicide attempt, and more than elevenfold risk of reporting an injurious suicide attempt (Bensley et al.). Johnson and colleagues (2002) explored the relationships between childhood adversities, interpersonal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts. Their sample was obtained from existing longitudinal data and consisted of 659 families with children between ages 1-11. The Disorganizing Poverty Interview (death of parent, disabling parental injury or illness, living in unsafe neighborhood, low maternal age, low parental education, parental separation or divorce, peer aggression, low family income, school violence, experience of a crime, single parenthood) was utilized. Harsh parenting punishment, low maternal education aspirations for the child, maternal possessiveness, maternal verbal abuse, and childhood physical and sexual abuse were associated with increased risk for suicide attempts during late adolescence or early adulthood even after controlling for all of the covariates. Maladaptive parenting or abuse during childhood or early adolescence was found to be significantly associated with substantial interpersonal difficulties during middle adolescence. Additionally, interpersonal difficulties were determined to account for 51% of the association between maladaptive parenting or abuse during childhood or early adolescence and suicide attempts during late adolescence or early adulthood. Johnson and colleagues concluded that children who experience maladaptive parenting or child abuse may lack social skills that are essential for the development and maintenance of adaptive relationships with peers and adults. Without 55 these important social skills, the tendency to become socially isolated or to relate to others in an aggressive or aversive manner may likely be the result. The resulting social isolation may contribute to feelings of despair, hopelessness, and suicidal behavior (Johnson et al.). High risk sexual behavior has been identified as the most highly documented form of harmful behavior in maltreatment survivors, particularly sexual abuse survivors. In particular, earlier onset of consensual sexual activity and higher rates of teen pregnancy, multiple sexual partners, unprotected intercourse, STDs, and higher rates of abortion have been consistently identified as more likely among maltreated individuals (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Kendall-Tackett, 2002; Mabanglo, 2002). Sleep problems have consistently been found to be strongly related to maltreatment, particularly physical and sexual abuse and exposure to domestic violence (Kendall-Tackett, 2002; Mabanglo, 2002). Kendall-Tackett’s literature review revealed strong support for a relationship between a history of childhood abuse and trouble sleeping. This review reiterated the importance of sleep to health and the higher incidence of infections, illnesses, accidents, and more severe and chronic depression among those who do not get adequate sleep. Delayed growth and altered brain development may be possible correlates of maltreatment (Glaser, 2000; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999). Glaser’s review of the literature found consistent support for the existence of crucial and prominent brain differences among maltreated children compared to those who were not maltreated. Specifically, children with maltreatment-related PTSD have been found to have smaller cerebral volumes; smaller total midsagittal area of the corpus callosum; and larger total 56 lateral ventricles and cortical and prefrontal cortical CSF volumes (Glaser). Glaser concluded that changes in brain size are likely to be strongly related to traumatic early childhood experiences, and therefore possibly related to increased catecholamine concentrations and raised cortisol levels. Maltreatment has been indicated to be strongly related to abnormal neuoroendocrine functioning, specifically chronic hyperactivity of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Chronic hyperactivity of the HPA may bring about neuronal loss in the hippocampus, inhibit the process of neurogenesis, slow down the development of myelination, and lead to abnormalities in synaptic pruning, which all contribute to impairments in affective and cognitive functioning (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001). Additionally, the increased average daily levels of cortisol typically found among maltreated children are indicative of hypercortisolism, which increases the risk for developing neurobiological anomalies (Cicchetti & Rogosch). Impairments in cognitive abilities and poor academic achievement have been consistently documented to exist among those with histories of maltreatment (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Mabanglo, 2002). Specifically, a negative correlation between verbal IQ and the severity of abuse as well as the existence of a lower IQ and reading ability have been identified among maltreated children (DeBellis, 2001). Cognitive deficits have implications for aggressive and conduct-disordered behavior as well as other negative outcomes (Kaplan et al.). 57 Physical, sexual abuse Physical, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional abuse Sexual, physical, witness domestic violence, emotional abuse Physical, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional abuse Family violence and maltreatment Bensley et al., 1999 Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001 Edwards et al., 2003 Goldstein et al., 2003 58 Abuse Benda & Corwyn, 2002 Glaser, 2000 Physical, sexual abuse Ackerman et al., 1998 Prominent brain differences - smaller cerebral volumes; smaller total midsagittal area of the corpus callosum; and larger total lateral ventricles and cortical and prefrontal cortical CSF volumes, increased catecholamine concentrations and raised cortisol levels Lying, stealing, arrests, and hyperactivity PTSD, depression, phobic disorder, separation anxiety, oppositional defiant disorder, dysthymia, comorbidity Violent behavior, alienation, attachment problems Suicidal thoughts, suicidal plan, noninjurious and injurious suicide attempts, antisocial behavior Chronic hyperactivity of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis which leads to neuronal loss in the hippocampus, inhibited neurogenesis, slowed myelination, and abnormalities in synaptic pruning, resulting in impairments in affective and cognitive functioning Compromised over-all mental health Researcher(s) Maltreatment Experiences Outcomes _____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Summary of Outcomes Found to be Associated with Specific Maltreatment Experiences Table 2 Witness domestic violence Exposure to domestic violence Physical maltreatment Emotional, physical, sexual abuse Physical and sexual abuse, neglect, psychological abuse Physical abuse, neglect, sexual abuse Neglect Physical, sexual, neglect, emotional Witness domestic violence, physical abuse, sexual abuse Physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional maltreatment, physical neglect Holden, 2003 Hunter et al., 2004 Jaffee et al., 2004 Johnson et al., 2002 Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999 Kendall-Tackett, 2002 Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004 Lau & Weisz, 2003 Mabanglo, 2002 Manly et al., 2001 59 Sexual abuse, physical abuse, psychological maltreatment, neglect and witnessing family violence Higgins & McCabe, 2003 Poor levels of adjustment across all scales of the CBCL, low self-esteem, trauma symptoms, self-depreciation, lack of positive relationships, lack of purpose in life PTSD, externalizing Aggression, delinquency Antisocial behaviors Increased risk for suicidal ideation and attempts, lack of social skills, More disliked and less popular, display less intimacy, more conflict, more aggression, delinquency, more negative affect, and limited social problem solving skills, substance abuse, major depression, PTSD, low self esteem, suicidal behavior, psychiatric diagnoses, and hospitalizations Disorganized pattern of attachment, revictimization, victimization of others, substance abuse Aggression and delinquency, impulsivity and irritability, hypervigilance, paranoia, affiliations with deviant peers Poorer treatment persistence, poorer long-term outcome Non-compliance, aggression, anxiety, depression, attention deficits, PTSD, phobias, ADHD, sleep disorders, somatization disorder, disorders of attachment, oppositional and conduct disorders, suicidal ideation, dissociative reactions, eating disturbances, and substance abuse Aggressive, withdrawn, less cooperative, lower ego resiliency, and higher ego undercontrol Physical, sexual abuse Physical abuse Sexual, physical abuse Physical, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional abuse Safren et al., 2002 Salzinger et al., 2001 Shields & Cicchetti, 2001 Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001 60 Sexual abuse Exposure to violence Witness domestic violence, physical abuse, neglect Sexual abuse Physical abuse, neglect Exposure to family and community violence (witness and victim) Rodriguez, Vande Kemp, & Foy, 1998 Ruchkin et al., 2002 McLean & Gallop, 2003 McMahon & Clay-Warner, 2002 Muller et al., 2000 Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002 Delinquent, withdrawn, and social problem behaviors, aggression, anxious and depressed Borderline personality disorder, PTSD Criminal behavior, delinquency Greater levels of psychopathology (internalizing, externalizing, PTSD symptomatology, and PTSD diagnosis), negative sense of self, impaired social/interpersonal functioning PTSD PTSD, Major Depression, Separation Anxiety, Generalized Anxiety, ADHD, Conduct Disorder, Alcohol Abuse, and Drug Abuse Panic disorder, social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, comorbidity Aggression, negative social expectations, antisocial behaviors, lack of prosocial behavior, withdrawn behavior, internalizing problems, more likely to be rejected by peers More likely to bully peers, more at risk for victimization, more anxious and hyperaroused, angry, unempathic, disruptive, impaired social functioning Shoplifting, frequent lying, fraud, pickpocketing, minor aggression, physical fighting, violence, shoplifting, property damage, moderately serious and serious delinquency, truancy, running away, staying out late, stubbornness, defiance, authority avoidance Intergenerational Transmission of Maltreatment. As with other parenting behaviors, those behaviors that are considered maltreatment may also be transmitted across generations. Parental modeling of violence leads to the child’s learning of violence as a normal and appropriate way of behaving as well as inadequate learning of nonviolent parenting skills. This behavioral repertoire may be carried through into adulthood and the next generation of parenting (Cunningham, 2003). Cunningham examined the joint contribution of experiencing and witnessing violence during childhood on subsequent child abuse by those now in the parent role using a sample of 2,889 adults obtained from the Second National Family Violence Survey. The Conflict Tactics Scale, as well as other questions pertaining to direct violent experiences and witnessing of domestic violence were administered. Those who either directly experienced (were hit) or witnessed parental violence or both were found to have higher than average and similar child abuse rates. Only those with neither form of family violence had lower than average rates of parental abuse of children. Participants who were only hit as a teen were less likely to abuse their own children than those who only witnessed domestic violence and than those who reported both forms of family violence. Witnessing domestic violence was found to increase levels of child abuse, regardless of the experience of being hit. Those who were only hit as a teen were found to be more than twice as likely to abuse their own children as those who experienced no family violence. Ultimately, the experience of either form of violence was found to significantly increase the odds of child abuse in the next generation. Cunningham concluded that those who experience family violence during childhood may become maladjusted adults 61 characterized by dysregulated feelings and negative cognitions; this increases the likelihood they will abuse their own children. Based on a review of the past decade of literature, DeBellis (2001) devised a developmental traumatology model for the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment. Adverse parenting skills are believed to result from the impact of childhood traumatic stress on brain development and the development of mental illness (DeBellis). This pathway has been implicated to lead to the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment (DeBellis). A history of child maltreatment has been consistently found to be associated with poor parenting skills and a higher risk of intergenerational transmission of substance abuse, negative and problematic parent-child interactions, domestic violence, and child maltreatment (DeBellis). Green (1998) concluded from an extensive literature review that research has demonstrated a strong association between dissociation, PTSD, pathological alterations in the limbic system, and short-term memory deficits. Specifically, it has been indicated that the tendency to become fixated on traumatic experiences results in aspects of the trauma being reenacted by the maltreated child through play as well as in relationships with others through either victimizing of others or by recreating their own victimization. Regarding alterations in the limbic system, Green reported that researchers have suggested that PTSD-related damage to hippocampal neurons may be the result of stressinduced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis imbalance in which glucocorticoids are hyper-secreted. Overstimulation of the stress response systems produces impulsivity, hyperactivity, and lower threshold for violence. Additionally, Green’s literature review revealed dissociation to be common among both adults and children with maltreatment 62 histories. Dissociation’s role in the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment is related to the resulting impaired memory of one’s own childhood maltreatment experiences. Learning from the past has not occurred which leads to the inability to empathize with their child’s feelings about victimization. In addition to dissociation, other primitive defenses such as avoidance and distancing behavior, identification with the aggressor, hypervigilance, denial, projection, ambivalent attachment to mother, and splitting prevent victims from integrating their traumatic memories and emotions into awareness, which leads to the likelihood of reenacting the abuse trauma with others. The internalizing and maintenance of these defenses throughout childhood and adulthood results in the carrying through of pathological and abusive parenting into the next generation (Green). Locke and Newcomb (2004) explored child maltreatment, parent alcohol and drug related problems, polydrug problems, and intergenerational transmission of parenting practices using 318 parents (237 females) with an average age 35. The Children of Alcoholics Screening Test, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, Parental Acceptance Rejection Questionnaire, and polydrug problem questions were completed. A history of child maltreatment and parental drug problems were both found to have an adverse impact on parenting practices in the next generation. Child maltreatment had a direct and negative influence on parenting in the next generation. The experience of emotional neglect as a child had a negative influence on parenting even more than the overall influence of child maltreatment. Parental alcohol or drug related problems predicted polydrug problems, which in turn predicted poor parenting (Locke & Newcomb). 63 Juvenile Delinquency Maltreatment and Juvenile Delinquency. Suboptimal parenting has been implicated in the development of childhood delinquency. Aggressive and delinquent behaviors in both adolescence and adulthood have been consistently found to be among the most frequent correlates of physical abuse (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999) and neglect (Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004). A history of physical abuse and neglect increases the risk for aggression and criminal behavior by increasing levels of impulsivity and irritability, hypervigilance, paranoia, affiliations with deviant peers, as well as inhibiting the recognition of pain in both self and others (Kaplan et al.; Knutson et al.). Traumatic and violent experiences have been found to be related to specific personality characteristics and emotional functioning that lead to antisocial activity which, in turn, leads to increased exposure to further violence. This is a negative, self-perpetuating cycle (Ruchkin et al., 2002). Stouthamer-Loeber and colleagues explored the relationship between maltreatment and disruptive and delinquent behavior among 250 delinquent and 250 nondelinquent boys with an average age of 13. Data was obtained from a protective services agency pertaining to maltreatment, the Child Behavior Checklist, Diagnostic Schedule for Children, Self-Report Delinquency interview, and the Youth Self Report were used. A higher prevalence of disruptive or delinquent behavior was found among the maltreated compared to the nonmaltreated participants. Results indicated that those who experienced maltreatment had an earlier age of onset for minor covert behavior (shoplifting, frequent lying) and for moderately serious delinquency (fraud, pickpocketing) compared to controls. Maltreated boys were more likely to be involved in 64 the steps in the overt (minor aggression, physical fighting, violence) and the covert (shoplifting, frequent lying, property damage, moderately serious and serious delinquency) pathways and more likely to reach the most severe step in the authority conflict (truancy, running away, staying out late) and overt pathways (violence behaviors of rape, attack, strongarm). Maltreatment was associated with each of the steps in the authority conflict pathway (stubbornness, defiance, authority avoidance). Compared to controls, maltreated boys had four times higher risk of displaying authority avoidance behaviors, such as truancy, staying out late at night, and running away from home. More than half of the maltreated boys had at least one petition to juvenile court compared to approximately 38 percent of the controls. Maltreated boys were more likely to have a petition for both nonindex violence offenses and nonindex property offenses. Specifically, the risk of court contact was about twice as high for the maltreated boys. Nearly seven percent of the maltreated boys were homicide offenders compared to three percent of the controls. Protective agency contact tended to precede or co-occur with overt and covert problem behavior (Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001). Knutson and colleagues (2004) tested a theoretical model of social disadvantage and neglectful parenting as precursors to the development of antisocial and aggressive behavior in childhood. Their sample consisted of 310 first and 361 fifth grade children (51% girls). Information pertaining to the constructs of social disadvantage, antisocial behavior and aggression, denial of care neglect, supervisory neglect, and punitive discipline were obtained through interview questions, direct observations, and parent and teacher completion of the CBCL initially and again five years later. In both groups, denial of care neglect was significantly related to both antisocial behavior at baseline and 65 change in antisocial behavior five years later. Supervisory neglect was found to be significantly related to antisocial outcome for the fifth grade group only. Strong support was also evident for neglect and punitive discipline as critical factors in determining the consequences of social disadvantage in the development of antisocial behavior. The contribution of neglect to current and future antisocial behavior was significantly more than that of social disadvantage and punitive discipline. Deficient parenting overall was found to be a strong predictor of future antisocial behavior (Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004). Benda and Corwyn (2002) explored the relationship between abuse and violent behavior among 1,031 adolescents ages 13-18. A 150-item questionnaire was administered in the participants’ classrooms that included questions about violent behavior, race, family structure, father’s education, attachment to mother and father, moral beliefs, religiosity, self-esteem, physical abuse, sexual abuse, frustration, alienation, suicide, peer association, differential rewards, and modeling. The researchers concluded that an important and significant relationship exists between abuse in childhood and violence among younger adolescents, suggesting that childhood abuse acts as a key explanatory factor for violent behavior in both younger and older adolescents. The researchers suggest that the weak attachment that likely accompanies abuse may lead to frustration and alienation which may in turn lead to violence directed at both the self and others (Benda & Corwyn). Bensley and colleagues (1999) examined health risk behaviors that may be associated with a history of physical abuse, sexual abuse, and combined physical and sexual abuse. Participants in the 8th, 10th, and 12th grades were recruited from 44 66 Washington State public schools. A total of 4,790 adolescents (2518 female) completed questionnaires that addressed physical fitness, unintentional and intentional injury behaviors, alcohol, tobacco, and other drug use, HIV education, abuse history, antisocial behavior, and suicidal ideation and behavior. Results revealed an abuse history to be strongly associated with antisocial behavior. The association was found to be stronger for combined physical and sexual abuse than for physical or sexual abuse alone and stronger for more severe antisocial behavior than for less severe. Specifically, children with a combined history of physical and sexual abuse had more than threefold risk of one antisocial behavior and more than sevenfold risk of two or more antisocial behaviors. Children who reported only physical abuse had double the risk of reporting one antisocial behavior and fourfold risk of reporting two or more antisocial behaviors. Gender differences emerged such that girls were found to be more likely to report suicidal thoughts and noninjurious suicide attempts while boys were more likely to report antisocial behavior (Bensley et al.). Physical maltreatment is an important environmental risk variable that has been causally linked to children’s antisocial behavior because it increases an individual’s risk of engaging in violent, antisocial behavior in adolescence and adulthood. Jaffee and colleagues (2004) investigated the relationship between maltreatment and antisocial behaviors. Their longitudinal twin study design consisted of 1,116 twin pairs and their families (51% female) who were interviewed at the age of five and then again at the age of seven. Physical maltreatment was determined through a standardized clinical interview of mothers. Mothers also completed the Young Adult Behavior Checklist. A diagnostic interview of the children was performed. Physical maltreatment was found to 67 predict future antisocial outcome with the amount of physical abuse experienced related to the amount and severity of subsequent antisocial behaviors. The effects of physical maltreatment remained significant after controlling for the parents’ history of antisocial behavior as well as after controlling for any genetic transmission of antisocial behavior. Approximately half of the intergenerational transmission of antisocial behavior was found to be environmentally mediated. Parents with a history of antisocial behavior were found to be more likely to maltreat their children. These parents also had children who engaged in high levels of antisocial behavior. Physical maltreatment was found to be significantly related to children’s antisocial behavior, even after controlling for parents’ history of antisocial behavior. The researchers concluded from their longitudinal data that physical maltreatment is likely not heritable, therefore, it is not likely that genetic features play a key role in the intergenerational transmission of aggressive behavior and child maltreatment (Jaffee et al.). When maltreatment occurs, the child is frequently removed from the care of the abusing parent or caretaker. Recent research investigating the role of social service placement and family disorganization in future criminality of abused children found a significant relationship between placement and criminal behavior (McMahon & ClayWarner, 2002). Utilizing existing juvenile and adult court records of 749 individuals who had been physically abused or neglected before the age of 12, McMahon and ClayWarner found that placement may act as a contributing factor to criminality for abused or neglected children who have experienced a recent family separation (divorce, marital separation, death in family). Specifically, abused or neglected children who experienced a family separation and were removed from their homes were almost twice as likely to 68 have an adult arrest in comparison to abused or neglected children who experienced a family separation and were not removed from their homes. The child’s potential support system of siblings, friends, neighbors, or teachers is lost on removal, thus increasing the risk for criminality (McMahon & Clay-Warner). Other Familial Correlates of Delinquency. Relationships between other familial experiences and delinquent behaviors have also been identified. Goldstein and colleagues (2003) explored comorbid symptom patterns in 232 female juvenile offenders ages 12 to 18 years. The Massachusetts Youth Screening Inventory (MAYSI), Youth Self Report (YSR), and Million Adolescent Clinical Inventory (MACI) were completed. Results revealed family discord to be significantly related to externalizing symptoms, specifically lying, stealing, arrests, and hyperactivity. Sohn (2003) explored characteristics of 196 young people in correctional institutions (100 males) and a comparative sample of 727 (369 males) community students who had never been convicted. Ages ranged from 12 to 15 (mean age 14). The CBCL and Malaise Inventory (which is a measure of melancholic symptomatology) were used. Offending boys and girls were found to be significantly more likely to be from nontraditional families (living with single parent or other guardian such as grandparents, relatives, or others) compared to non-offenders (50% and 4% respectively). Offenders reported significantly higher rates of internalizing problems (withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed) and social adjustment problems were almost twice as high among offenders compared to non-offenders. Significantly more externalizing behaviors, symptoms of malaise, and lower educational attainment were also found among offenders. Perceived parenting style predicted internalizing/externalizing/social 69 problems and malaise. Poor parenting was reported among 68% of the offenders and significant levels of emotional and behavioral problems were associated with perceived poor parenting. Hunter and colleagues (2004) explored developmental pathways and risk factors in sexual aggression and delinquency in 157 adolescent males ages 12 to 18 (mean age 15). Hostility Toward Women, Adversarial Sexual Beliefs, Rape Myths Acceptance, Acceptance of Interpersonal Violence, Mating Effort Scale, Negative/Positive Masculinity/femininity, Social Self-Esteem Inventory, Youth Self-Reports: Anxious/Depressed, Social Problems, Withdrawn, Delinquent Behavior, and Aggressive Behavior scales were used. Nonsexual aggression and delinquency were found to be significantly and positively associated with Egotistical-Antagonistic Masculinity, Psychosocial Deficits, Exposure to Abuse of Females, and Exposure to Male-Modeled Antisocial Behavior. Childhood exposure to violence against women and male-modeled antisocial behavior significantly increased the risk of nonsexual aggression and delinquency. The researchers concluded that their results suggest that young males may be vulnerable to internalizing the values and imitating the behavior of socially deviant peers and adults. Lack of parental involvement may also lead to aggression and dominance. Perrone and colleagues (2004) examined parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency among 13,536 adolescents from grades 7-12. The Delinquency Scale was used. Additional questions were devised pertaining to self control, including impulsivity, a preference for simple tasks, favoring of physical over mental activities, selfcenteredness, and temper. Parental efficacy questions pertained to attachment to child 70 and effectiveness in recognizing and responding to problematic behavior. Level of parental efficacy was found to be a significant predictor of youth’s levels of self-control. Self-control and parental efficacy were both found to be significantly associated with delinquency. Bao, Haas, and Pi (2004) explored life-strain, negative emotions, and delinquency among 615 (310 male) adolescents ages 13 to 18 (mean age 15) attending public school in grades 8 to 11. Exposure to negative stimuli was assessed with indicators of negative relations with peers, parents, and teachers. Negative emotions were assessed with indicators of anger, resentment, anxiety, and depression. Delinquency/deviance was measured with scales for violent offenses, property offenses, and school-related deviance. Control variables included demographic traits and variables of social control (attachment to peers, parents, and school) and social learning theories (affiliation with deviant peers). Negative relations with parents, teachers, and peers were independently and positively associated with violent offenses, property offenses, school deviance, anger, resentment, anxiety, and depression. Negative emotions were significantly and positively associated with all delinquent or deviant outcomes with anger being the most important mediator. Adolescents from economically poorer homes were more likely to report violent offenses. Attachments to parents and school were both significantly and negatively associated with school deviance. Juvenile Psychopathy Psychopathy is one personality characteristic that has been studied in relation to juvenile delinquency. Psychopathy is viewed as a personality disorder that is characterized by early onset and long-term social and interpersonal dysfunction (Hare, 71 1998). Psychopathic individuals have been described as grandiose, manipulative, forceful, and cold-hearted. Shallow and labile emotions; the inability to form longlasting bonds to people, principles, and goals; and lack of empathy, guilt, and remorse are other prominent characteristics associated with the psychopathic individual. Individuals with psychopathy also tend to be impulsive, sensation seeking, and irresponsible. Violation of social and legal norms leads to the disproportionate involvement in crime and in particular violent crime among psychopathic individuals (Hare, 1998). Cruise and colleagues (2003) explored the juvenile justice perspective of adolescent psychopathy among 424 juvenile justice professionals (juvenile detention officers and juvenile probation officers) who completed a survey on conceptualization of antisocial behavior of adolescents. The resulting factor structure of psychopathy for males included criminal versatility/irresponsible behavior, serious violent/conduct problems (deliberate use of force, physical cruelty to people and animals, destruction of property), lack of empathy/conning and manipulative use of others, anger/impulsivity/absence of behavioral controls, and family problems/nonviolent delinquency. Factors for females included irresponsible behavior/nonviolent delinquency, serious violent/conduct problems, lack of empathy/callous use of others, anger/poor behavioral controls, and family problems. Salekin and colleagues (2004) examined relations between psychopathy and disruptive behavior disorders in a sample of 130 (92 males) young offenders ages 9 to 18 (mean age 14). The PCL-YV, SRP-II, APSD, APS were completed. Conduct Disorder, ADHD, ODD, Adjustment disorder, and substance abuse correlated highly with the psychopathy scales. Specifically, substance abuse, anger, aggression, and interpersonal 72 problems correlated positively with psychopathy. Additionally, psychopathy was found to be positively correlated with violent and nonviolent offenses, number of physical fights youth engaged in per year, drug use, history of family arrest, and association with deviant peers. Rates of psychopathy in the youth sampled were found to be similar to those in adult samples. Dixon, Howie, and Starling (2004) examined psychopathology in 100 female juvenile offenders ages 13 to 19 years old (mean age 16) and a comparison group of 100 females. The schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age children – Present and Lifetime Version, and Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scale II were used. Offenders reported significantly more psychopathology compared to nonoffenders, specifically, conduct disorder, substance abuse, alcohol abuse, depression, PTSD, psychoses, separation anxiety disorder, ADHD, and suicide attempts. All but one of the offenders had at least one diagnosis and the average number of diagnoses per offender was 4. Ninety-three percent of the offenders had more than one diagnosis and 78% of these had three or more diagnoses. In comparison, 41% of the non-offenders had one or more diagnoses (24% had one diagnosis only, 17% of these had two or more diagnoses). Offender status was found to be most significantly associated with the number of psychological diagnoses, with the presence of two or more diagnoses presenting the highest risk. Offenders were found to have had significantly more types of traumatic experiences compared to non-offenders. The most common traumatic experiences among offenders included witnessing violent crime, being confronted with traumatic news, witnessing domestic violence, sexual abuse, and physical abuse. In comparison, non-offenders most commonly reported traumatic experiences that included 73 being confronted with traumatic news and witnessing a violent crime. Regarding family environment, significantly more of the non-offenders were living in intact families compared to offenders who were found to be more likely to have been homeless, living independently, in a residential or foster care setting, or with extended family. Gretton, Hare, and Catchpole (2004) explored psychopathy and offending from adolescence to adulthood by following the criminal outcomes of 157 adolescent boys ages 12 to 18 over a period of ten years. The Psychopathy Check List – Youth Version (PCL:YV) was used to examine psychopathy among the offenders. Results revealed psychopathy to be a strong predictor of offending behavior over the 10-year period. Predictive validity of the PCL:YV was particularly strong for violent offending with higher PCL:YV scores being associated with both an increased likelihood of nonviolent and violent offending. Those with higher levels of psychopathic features were found to continue to engage in high rates of both violent and nonviolent offending into adulthood. The higher potential for long-term violence distinguished psychopathic offenders from other adolescent offenders. The strong relationship between psychopathy and violent recidivism remained after controlling for number of previous violent and nonviolent offenses, age of first offense, and conduct disorder symptoms which is suggestive of psychopathy as an independent and potent risk factor for violent and nonviolent delinquent behaviors. Vincent and colleagues (2003) explored subtypes of adolescent offenders, specifically affective traits and antisocial behavior patterns among 259 young male offenders ages 12 to 19 (mean age 16) who were sentenced to maximum and minimum security settings. The PCL:YV was completed. Results indicated that those who scored 74 higher in psychopathy initiated criminality and conduct significantly earlier, were among the most serious offenders, and engaged in more frequent and severe offending patterns. Recidivism among those with higher psychopathy scores was significantly more prominent and more probable. Specifically, 90% of the high psychopathy group committed at least one offense approximately four times sooner than those in the low psychopathy group, with half being charged with at least one violent reoffense (twice that of those in the low psychopathy group). Frick and colleagues (2003) explored the four-year stability of psychopathic traits in 1,136 (53% female) non-referred youth with a mean age of 10. Ninety-eight (47% female) of these children participated in the longitudinal portion (4 yearly assessments) of the study. The Antisocial Process Screening Devise (APSD), Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Computerized Reward Dominance Task, Children’s Sensation Seeking Scale, Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children – fourth edition, Self Report of Delinquency, and the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire were completed. Results revealed stability of psychopathic traits across four years. Children with higher levels of conduct problem behavior tended to have more stable levels of psychopathic traits. Psychopathic traits were found to be more stable than other problems of adjustment in children and to be comparable in stability to personality traits assessed in adults. Socioeconomic status and quality of parenting were found to be the two most consistent predictors of the stability of these traits across the four years. The researchers suggested parenting to play an important role in development of guilt and empathy and other aspects of conscience that have been implicated as components of psychopathy. 75 Wootton and colleagues (1997) explored ineffective Parenting and childhood conduct problems and the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Their sample included 136 (30% female) children between the ages of 6 and 13 (mean age of 8) and 30 comparative children. The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children was used in identifying Oppositional Defiant Disorder and Conduct Disorder. The Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (DBD), Psychopathy screening devise (PSD), and Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (APQ) were also used. Ineffective parenting (low parental involvement, low positive reinforcement, low monitoring and supervision, inconsistent discipline, use of corporal punishment) was associated with conduct problems only in children without significant levels of callous (lack of empathy, manipulativeness) and unemotional (lack of guilt, emotional constrictedness) traits. Children high on these traits exhibited a significant number of conduct problems. Watson and colleagues (1992) examined psychopathy traits, specifically narcissism, and perceptions of past parenting experiences among 324 undergraduates (199 female) with an average age of 19. Participants completed the Goal Instability and Superiority Scales, Narcissistic Personality Inventory, the Parental Authority Questionnaire, and Depression and Anxiety Scales. Perceived parental authoritativeness was associated with less narcissistic maladjustment, parental permissiveness was associated with immature grandiosity, and parental authoritarianism was associated with inadequate idealization. This review of the literature exemplifies the important implications that the specific variables and processes discussed have for negative outcomes, demonstrating the 76 importance of further examination of these variables to add support and to expand upon current knowledge. Statement of the Problem Though the specific variables and associations described above have been wellresearched and documented, the goal of the current study was to further explore relationships that have not yet been studied. Specifically, the current study explored relationships between parenting experiences (warmth, rejection, overprotection, and discipline) and psychopathy, specifically the Callous-Unemotional trait. Additionally, previous research has not examined associations between maltreatment experiences, PTSD, and psychopathy. Processes of intergenerational transmission of parenting practices were also further explored in relation to the variables of warmth, rejection, overprotection, discipline, maltreatment, PTSD symptomatology, and the CallousUnemotional psychopathy trait. These variables and expected associations were informed by studying both a delinquent group and a comparison group of community adolescents. Study Hypotheses 1. Positive parenting, maltreatment and PTSD were expected to predict callousunemotional psychopathy. a. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, fathers’ and mothers’ positive parenting were expected to be negatively related to callous-unemotional psychopathy. 77 b. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, maltreatment was expected to be positively related to callous-unemotional psychopathy. c. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, PTSD was expected to be positively related to callous-unemotional psychopathy. 2. Positive parenting, maltreatment, PTSD, and callous-unemotional psychopathy were expected to predict delinquency. a. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, fathers’ and mothers’ positive parenting were expected to be negatively related to number of reported arrests. b. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, maltreatment was expected to be positively related to number of reported arrests. c. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, symptoms of PTSD were expected to be negatively related to number of reported arrests. d. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, callous-unemotional psychopathy was expected to be positively related to number of reported arrests. 3. Positive parenting, maltreatment, PTSD, and callous-unemotional psychopathy were expected to predict anticipated warmth in future parenting. 78 a. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, fathers’ and mothers’ positive parenting were expected to be positively related to anticipated warmth in future parenting. b. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, maltreatment was expected to be negatively related to anticipated warmth in future parenting. c. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, symptoms of PTSD were expected to be negatively related to anticipated warmth in future parenting. d. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, callous-unemotional psychopathy was expected to be negatively related to anticipated warmth in future parenting. 4. Positive parenting, maltreatment, PTSD, and callous-unemotional psychopathy were expected to predict anticipated rejection in future parenting. a. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, fathers’ and mothers’ positive parenting were expected to be negatively related to anticipated rejection in future parenting. b. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, maltreatment was expected to be positively related to anticipated rejection in future parenting. c. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, symptoms of PTSD were expected to be positively related to anticipated rejection in future parenting. 79 d. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, callous-unemotional psychopathy was expected to be positively related to anticipated rejection in future parenting. 5. Positive parenting, maltreatment, PTSD, and callous-unemotional psychopathy were expected to predict anticipated overprotection in future parenting. a. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, fathers’ and mothers’ positive parenting were expected to be negatively related to anticipated overprotection in future parenting. b. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, maltreatment was expected to be negatively related to anticipated overprotection in future parenting. c. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, symptoms of PTSD were expected to be positively related to anticipated overprotection in future parenting. d. Among both males and females in the public school and JDC samples, callous-unemotional psychopathy was expected to be negatively related to anticipated overprotection in future parenting. 80 Chapter Three Method Participants The total sample consisted of 326 volunteers, 223 of which were recruited from three Midwestern suburban metropolitan area public high schools and 103 of which were recruited from a Midwestern metropolitan Juvenile Detention Center. Public High School Participants. The public high school sample consisted of 110 female (51.2%) and 105 male volunteer adolescents who were recruited from three Midwestern suburban community public high schools (grades 9 through 12). Eight participants did not report their gender. Age ranged from 14 to 19 years with a mean age of 16.18 (SD = 1.23). The majority of the sample was age 15 (25.1%), 16 (26.9%), and 17 (20.6%) with the remaining sample consisting of 7.2 percent age 14 and 16.6 percent age 18, and .9 percent age 19 years of age. Six participants did not report their age. Distribution of the sample across grades was as follows: 9th (20.6%), 10th (22.9%), 11th (25.1%), and 12th (25.6%). Thirteen participants did not report their grade. The racial backgrounds of the sample were primarily European American (87.4%) with the remaining sample consisting of African American (.4%), Hispanic American (5.4%), Asian American (1.3%), Biracial (.9%), and 81 Other (1.3%). Seven participants did not report their ethnicity. Gender, age, grade, and ethnicity distributions are presented in Table 3. Participants reported on whether they had ever been in trouble with the law and/or arrested. The majority of the sample (70.9%) reported never having been in trouble with the law. Six participants did not report on whether they had ever been in trouble with the law. Of those who reported having been in trouble with the law (26.5%), 37.3 percent had never actually been arrested, 25.4 percent had been arrested once, 15.3 percent had been arrested twice, 10.2 percent had been arrested three times, and 11.9 percent had been arrested four or more times with 20 arrests being the highest reported. The average number of arrests was 1.86 (SD = 3.45). Distributions of contact with law enforcement and number of arrests are presented in Table 4. Participants also reported on whether they had ever been detained in a juvenile detention center. Of those who had been involved with the law, 71.2 percent had never been detained in a juvenile detention center, 11.9 percent had been detained once, 10.2 percent had been detained twice, and 6.8 percent had been detained three or more times with 15 detention center placements being the highest number reported. The average number of times detained in a juvenile detention center was .90 (SD = 2.47). The distribution of detention center placements is presented in Table 4. 82 Table 3 Gender, Age, Grade, and Ethnicity Distributions for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants Public School ______________ JDC ______________ Gender Male 105 89 Female 110 12 13 0 2 14 16 11 15 56 35 16 60 24 17 46 28 18 37 0 19 2 0 8 0 9 9 46 26 10 51 24 11 56 16 12 57 8 195 27 African American 1 52 Hispanic American 12 10 Asian American 3 0 Biracial 2 10 Other 3 1 Age Grade Ethnicity European American 83 Table 4 Distributions of Contact with Law Enforcement, Number of Arrests, and Number of Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Placements for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants Public School ______________ JDC ______________ Law Enforcement Contact No 158 0 Yes 59 98 Number of Arrests 0 22 1 15 9 2 9 10 3 6 8 4 4 17 5 0 15 6+ 3 34 Number of JDC Placements 0 42 1 7 17 2 6 14 3 1 10 4 0 14 5 0 11 6+ 3 27 84 Participants were also asked about frequency of relocations to different homes over the course of their life to date. It should be noted that this demographic question was not specific enough to examine inherent differences likely to exist between the public school sample and the JDC sample regarding relocations to different homes. For example, relocations among the public school sample were likely to include the entire family versus JDC participants’ relocations to different living situations (i.e., foster homes, grandparent or other family member’s homes).Of the total sample, 24.2 percent had never moved, 23.8 percent reported moving once, 10.3 percent had moved twice, 11.7 percent had moved three times, 12.1 percent had moved four times, and 15 percent had moved five or more times. Six participants did not report on the number of relocations. The average number of moves overall was 2.35 (SD = 2.41). The distribution of relocations to different homes is displayed in Table 5. Juvenile Detention Center Participants. The total Juvenile Detention Center sample consisted of 89 male (86.4%) and 12 female volunteer adolescents who were recruited from and currently detained in a local juvenile detention center. Two participants did not report their gender. Age ranged from 13 to 17 years with a mean age of 15.65 (SD = 1.07). The majority of the sample was 15 (34%), 16 (23.3%), and 17 (27.2%), with the remaining sample consisting of 1.9 percent age 13 and 10.7 percent age 14. Three participants did not report their age. Distribution of the sample across grades was as follows: 6th (1%), 7th (1.9%), 8th (8.7%), 9th (25.2%), 10th (23.3%), 11th (15.5%), and 12th (7.8%). Seventeen participants did not report their grade. The racial backgrounds of the sample were primarily African American (50.5%), with the remaining sample consisting of European American (26.2%), Hispanic 85 American (9.7%), Biracial (9.7%), and Other (1%). Three participants did not report their ethnicity. Gender, age, grade, and ethnicity distributions are presented in Table 3. Table 5 Distributions of Frequency of Relocations to Different homes for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants Public School ______________ JDC _______________ Frequency of Relocations 0 54 10 1 53 5 2 23 8 3 26 19 4 27 13 5 13 10 6 7 2 7 7 3 8 3 5 9 0 0 10+ 4 17 Participants were also asked to report the extent of their involvement with law or the juvenile justice system. Frequency of arrests ranged from one (8.7%) to 25 (1.0%) with the average number of arrests being 6.09 (SD = 5.07). The majority of the sample reported having been arrested five or fewer times (63.7 percent), 25.6 percent had been 86 arrested between six and ten times, and 10.7 percent had been arrested between 11 to 25 times. Nine participants did not report number of arrests. The number of reported juvenile detention center placements ranged from one to 20 with the average number of placements being 4.70 (SD = 3.97). The majority of the sample (71.3%) reported five or fewer separate juvenile detention center placements, with the remaining sample reporting six to 20 detention center placements. Nine participants did not report the number of detention center placements. Distributions of contact with law enforcement, number of arrests, and detention center placements are presented in Table 4. Participants were also asked about frequency of relocations to different homes over the course of their life to date. It should be noted that this demographic question was not specific enough to examine inherent differences likely to exist between the public school sample and the JDC sample regarding relocations to different homes. For example, relocations among the public school sample were likely to include the entire family versus JDC participants’ relocations to different living situations (i.e., foster homes, grandparent or other family member’s homes). The range of frequency of moves was from zero to 50. Of the total sample, 9.7 percent had never moved, 4.9 percent reported moving once, 7.8 percent had moved twice, 18.4 percent had moved three times, 12.6 percent had moved four times, and 40.2 percent had moved five or more times. The average number of moves overall was 6.25 (SD = 7.69). Eleven participants did not report number of relocations. The distribution of relocations to different homes is presented in Table 5. 87 Measures Participants completed a demographic information form (Appendix A) describing their gender; age; ethnicity; grade level; whether he or she had ever been in trouble with the law and, if yes, the number of arrests and placements in a juvenile detention center; frequency of relocations/moves; and parent or guardian’s education level and employment. They also completed five questionnaires measuring anticipated future parenting style, PTSD symptoms, Callous/unemotional psychopathy trait, maltreatment experiences, and past parenting experiences. Anticipated Future Parenting. The Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API; Appendix G) was used to measure thoughts about anticipated style of future parenting. The API was developed for use in prior research (Pasold, 2003). Initially, the questions that comprised the API were based on the questions on the s-EMBU (Swedish acronym for Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran – “own memories of parental rearing”), short version (Arrindell et al., 1999). Initial reliability analysis of the first version of the API as a whole suggested that reliability was not very strong (approximately .50). Further reliability analysis of the three anticipated scales of warmth, rejection, and overprotection revealed an acceptable warmth dimension (Cronbach’s alpha approximately .70), but the remaining two targeted dimensions of rejection and overprotection were not as reliable (approximately .40 for each of these scales). Additionally, the measure did not include other aspects of parenting behavior that have been implicated as being important, specifically discipline and reward. Data from several focus group discussions was subsequently obtained and used to revise questions within the API. The revised version was piloted through administration of the measure to 83 volunteering undergraduates 88 who were recruited from Introduction to Psychology discussion sections. Reliability analysis was performed on the data. Initial analysis of all items revealed an alpha of .60, which was improved to .65 upon removal of items 5 and 26. Reliability analysis of the individual scales revealed acceptable alphas. Specifically, the initial alpha for the Warmth scale was .38, which was improved to .47 with removal of item 23. For the Overprotection scale, the initial alpha was .50, which was improved to .59 with removal of item 6. For the Rejection scale, the initial alpha was .29, which was improved to .47 with removal of item 25. After deletion of item 5 in the initial reliability analysis of the total items, four items remain on the Discipline scale. The initial alpha for these four items is .40, which was improved to .53 with the deletion of item 22. Two additional items were added to this scale and include, If my child does something wrong, I will ground him/her; and If my child does something wrong, I will throw away something that he/she really likes to punish him/her. On the current version of the API, participants are asked to rate the degree that each statement applies to them using a three-point Likert-type scale (1 – No, 2 – Maybe, 3 – Yes). The 31-question measure contains the following five scales: Warmth (seven items); Overprotection (eight items); Rejection (nine items); Discipline (six items); and Encourage/Discourage (one item). Higher scores on the scales of warmth, rejection, and overprotection mean more displays of warmth, rejection, overprotection. Higher scores on the encourage/discourage scale represent high encouraging behaviors while lower scores on this scale mean more discouraging behaviors. Higher scores on the discipline scale represent more harsh (physical) punishment behaviors. For purposes of the current 89 study, the discipline and encourage/discourage scales were not utilized as these variables were beyond the scope of the current study. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the API using the data from the 325 completed questionnaires from the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 38% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 17%, the second explaining 11%, and the third explaining 10% of the total variance. The first factor, Warmth, is reflective of displays of interest in the child’s life and verbal and nonverbal affection and love. The second factor, Rejection, contains items describing behaviors that demonstrate non-acceptance of the child’s individual characteristics and abilities. The third factor, overprotection, is reflective of behaviors that are representative of over-involvement and restriction. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix H. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the API for all items within the three scales using the data from the 223 questionnaires completed by the public school participants in the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated threefactor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 38% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 21%, the second explaining 11%, and the third explaining 6% of the total variance. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix I. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the API for all items within the three scales using the data from the 102 questionnaires completed by the Juvenile Detention Center participants in the current study. An attempt to force the 90 anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 41% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 23%, the second explaining 11%, and the third explaining 7% of the total variance. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix I. Reliability analyses using the data from the 223 questionnaires completed by public school participants from the current study revealed an overall scale reliability for the 31 items of the API to be .80, which could not be improved by eliminating any of the present items. Separate reliability analyses for each of the three individual factors revealed a Cronbach alpha of .80 for Anticipated Warmth, .59 for Anticipated Overprotection, and .64 for Anticipated Rejection. Reliability could not be improved for any of the three scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Reliability analyses were performed on the API using the data from the 102 questionnaires completed by Juvenile Detention Center participants from the current study. The overall reliability for the 31 items of the API was .78, which could not be improved by eliminating any of the present items. Separate reliability analyses for each of the three individual factors revealed a Cronbach alpha of .80 for Anticipated Warmth, .56 for Anticipated Overprotection, and .61 for Anticipated Rejection. Reliability could not be improved for any of the scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Past Parenting Experiences. The Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS; Appendix J) was used to measure past parenting experiences. The 31 questions within the PPPS were adapted from the Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API) described above. Two questions were added to the beginning of the PPPS asking the participant to 91 indicate who they will be referring to when they answer the questions about their mother figure (question 1) and father figure (question 2), making the PPPS a total of 33 items. Participants are asked to rate the degree that each individual statement applies to them using a Three-point Likert-type scale (1 – No, 2 – Maybe, 3 – Yes). Each question provides choices for the participant to indicate the degree to which each behavior applies to each parent (mother and father figures) separately. The measure contains five scales that mirror the API scales, including the following: Warmth (seven items); Overprotection (eight items); Rejection (nine items); Discipline (six items); and Encourage/Discourage (one item). Higher scores on the scales of warmth, rejection, and overprotection mean more displays of warmth, rejection, overprotection. Higher scores on the encourage/discourage scale represent high encouraging behaviors, while lower scores on this scale mean more discouraging behaviors. Higher scores on the discipline scale represent more harsh (physical) punishment behaviors. For purposes of the current study, the discipline and encourage/discourage scales were not utilized as these variables were beyond the scope of the current study. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the PPPS for all items within the three scales using the data from the 325 completed questionnaires from the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 47% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 19%, the second explaining 16%, and the third explaining 12% of the total variance. The first factor, Warmth, is reflective of displays of interest in the child’s life and verbal and nonverbal affection and love. The second factor, Rejection, contains items describing 92 behaviors that demonstrate non-acceptance of the child’s individual characteristics and abilities. The third factor, overprotection, is reflective of behaviors that are representative of over-involvement and restriction. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix K. Factor analyses were also performed to examine the scale structure of the PPPS for father and mother items separately using the data from the 325 completed questionnaires from the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that for father items, together, the three factors explained 59% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 35%, the second explaining 15%, and the third explaining 9% of the total variance. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix L. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that for mother items, together, the three factors explained 54% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 29%, the second explaining 14%, and the third explaining 11% of the total variance. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix M. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the PPPS for all items within the three scales using the data from the 218 questionnaires completed by the public school participants from the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 45% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 26%, the second explaining 11%, and the third explaining 8% of the total variance. Due to the results of the factor analyses of the PPPS, the three 93 scales of warmth, overprotection, and rejection were combined into one positive parenting variable for use in data analyses for the current study. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix N. Factor analyses were performed to examine the scale structure of the PPPS for all items within the three scales using the data from the 100 questionnaires completed by the Juvenile Detention Center participants from the current study. An attempt to force the anticipated three-factor solution using the Principal Components method with Varimax rotation revealed that, together, the three factors explained 52% of the variance with the first factor solution explaining 29%, the second explaining 13%, and the third explaining 10% of the total variance. Due to the results of the factor analyses of the PPPS, the three scales of warmth, overprotection, and rejection were combined into one positive parenting variable for use in data analyses for the current study. Factor loadings are presented in Appendix O. Reliability analyses using the data from the 218 questionnaires completed by public school participants in the current study revealed an overall scale reliability for the 31 items of the PPPS to be .91, which could not be improved by eliminating any of the present items. Separate reliability analyses for each of the three individual factors revealed a Cronbach alpha of .91 for Warmth, .76 for Overprotection, and .88 for Rejection. Reliability could not be improved for any of the three scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Reliability analyses were performed separately on each of the scales for Father and Mother. Father’s Rejection was .83, Mother’s Rejection was .84; Father’s Overprotection was .72, Mother’s Overprotection was .61; Father’s 94 Warmth was .90, Mother’s warmth was .89. Reliability could not be improved for any of the scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Using the data from the 100 questionnaires completed by Juvenile Detention Center participants in the current study, reliability analyses revealed overall scale reliability for the 31 items of the PPPS to be .91, which could not be improved by eliminating any of the present items. Separate reliability analyses for each of the three individual factors revealed a Cronbach alpha of .92 for Warmth, .83 for Overprotection, and .82 for Rejection. Reliability could not be improved for any of the scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Reliability analyses were performed separately on each of the scales for Father and Mother. Father’s Rejection was .68, Mother’s Rejection was .78; Father’s Overprotection was .85, Mother’s Overprotection was .73; Father’s Warmth was .94, Mother’s warmth was .91. Reliability could not be improved for any of the scales by removing any of the items present within each scale. Child Maltreatment. A questionnaire pertaining to experiences of maltreatment (Appendix P), specifically sexual abuse, physical abuse, physical neglect, and emotional abuse, was developed based on recent research on maltreatment (Bernstein et al., 2003; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999). Each targeted maltreatment experience is measured by four questions that were adapted from the Child Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bernstein et al., 2003), a screening measure for maltreatment histories in both clinical and nonreferred groups. Participants were asked to rate the degree that each individual statement applies to them using a four-point Likert-type scale (1 – never true, 2 – rarely true, 3 – sometimes true, 4 – often true). Examples of questions related to sexual abuse include, Someone tried to make me touch them or they tried to touch me in a 95 sexual way; Someone made threats that they would hurt me or tell lies about me if I did not do sexual things with them. Examples of physical abuse questions include, I was punished with a belt, board, or other object; I was hit or beaten badly enough that it was noticed by someone like a babysitter, teacher, or doctor. Examples of questions pertaining to physical neglect include, I have felt like there was no one to take care of me and protect me; I was not taken to the doctor when I was sick or needed a checkup. Questions related to emotional abuse include, I have been called mean names by people in my family; I have been scared of being hurt by someone in my family. The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI; Andershed et al.,2002; Appendix Q). The YPI is a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in youth aged 12 to 18. This measure was chosen for use in the current study because it is appropriate for use with a community as well as a delinquent sample while other available measures were designed for use only with delinquent populations. Andershed and colleagues specifically designed the YPI to overcome the problems inherent in measuring traits associated with psychopathy through use of selfreport. Items were formulated to indirectly, rather than transparently, measure the various traits. Specifically, items were developed to appear positive or admirable to individuals with psychopathic traits but not to others. From an initial pool of six to ten items, the best five items were selected to comprise each subscale based on reliability analyses. The final version of the YPI consists of 50 items, ten subscales with five items each. The subscales include Dishonest Charm (glibness and superficial charm), Grandiosity (grandiosity/egocentricity), Lying (frequent lying and lying with ease), Manipulation (conning and manipulative traits), Callousness (callousness and lack of 96 empathy), Unemotionality (shallow affect/poverty in affective reactions), Remorselessness (lack of adequate feelings of guilt and remorse), Impulsivity, Thrillseeking (need for stimulation and excitement, proneness to boredom), and Irresponsibility. Participants are asked to rate the degree that each individual statement applies to them using a four-point Likert-type scale (1 – does not apply at all, 2 – does not apply well, 3 – applies fairly well, 4 – applies very well). Reliability and validity of the YPI were examined using a large community sample of 1,024 (553 female) 16-year-olds (Andershed et al., 2002). Questions pertaining to conduct problems (property offenses, serious property offenses, violent offenses, serious violent offenses, vandalism, use of illegal drugs, and caught by police), early behavioral problems (what participants had done before starting seventh grade), poor behavioral control (aggressive behavior), and problems with hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention were asked using a self-report format. Results indicated acceptably high alpha reliabilities ranging from .66 to .82 for each of the subscales of the YPI. Principal axis factor analyses with Oblique Promax rotation revealed a distinctive three-factor structure. Confirmatory factor analyses were used to test the three-factor model. The YPI subscales of Dishonest Charm, Lying, Grandiosity, and Manipulation made up the Grandiose-manipulative dimension; Callousness, Unemotionality, and Remorselessness comprised the Callous-unemotional dimension; and Impulsivity, Irresponsibility, and Thrill-seeking made up the Impulsive-irresponsible dimension. Confirmatory factor analyses fit indices suggested a good fit to the data for both boys and girls. 97 Validity of the YPI was examined through correlational analyses between the personality dimensions measured by the YPI and conduct problems. Significant correlations existed between conduct problems and the YPI total and the three dimensions for both boys and girls. Early behavioral problems, poor behavioral control, and hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention were also found to be strongly correlated with the YPI total and dimensions. Additional correlational analyses using teachers’ reports of problem behaviors revealed the YPI total score to be highly positively correlated with teacher-rated problem behaviors, and negatively correlated with school achievement. PTSD Symptomatology. Questions related to symptoms of PTSD were based on Pynoos and colleagues’ (1998) UCLA PTSD Index for DSM-IV (Child Version, Revision 1). A total of 20 questions were selected to measure symptoms of PTSD that include re-experiencing (five questions), increased arousal (five questions), and avoidance behavior (eight questions). Two questions inquire into specific feelings about traumatic events (Appendix R). Participants are asked to rate the degree that each individual statement applies to them using a four-point Likert-type scale (1 – never true, 2 – rarely true, 3 – sometimes true, 4 – often true). PTSD severity is obtained by calculating a total score. Social Desirability. Questions to measure socially desirable responding were adapted from the Crandall Social Desirability Test for Children, Short Form (CSDTC-SF; Carifio, 1994). The original version of the CSDTC was developed to measure the tendency of children and adolescents to give socially desirable answers rather than answers reflecting the individual’s true opinion. This 48-item measure was reduced to 98 twelve items using factor analytic techniques to enhance its usefulness in research and program evaluation (Carifio). Higher scores indicate stronger tendencies to respond in a socially desirable manner. For use in the current study, the measure was entitled About Me (Appendix S). The short form was significantly related to middle school students’ tendency to agree with myths about alcohol and drinking (F (1, 254) = 6.1; p < .01; Carifio). Procedure Participants were recruited from public schools in the suburban Northwest Ohio area and the Lucas County Juvenile Detention Center. Public school administrators were contacted and information about the current study was presented. Permission was granted by the principal of three public high schools. The researcher visited each school and verbally presented information about the study to students and distributed parental consent forms within study halls. Students who brought back signed parental consents on the day of the researcher’s follow-up visit or were age 18 or older were provided information and gave written informed consent immediately prior to their participation in the study. At the Detention Center, the presiding administrative judge was presented with information about the current study and his written consent to recruit participants from within the detention center was obtained. This judge retains temporary guardianship of the adolescents while they are detained in the juvenile detention center and has the right to grant consent for participation in research studies. Adolescents within the detention center were provided information about the study and informed of their right to participate or decline participation. Volunteering detention center participants provided 99 written consent prior to their participation in the study. Each participant was also provided the contact number of an advocate that they could use to discuss any concerns about the study. All participants were informed of the risks and benefits of the study and that they would suffer no consequences if they should decide not to participate or to discontinue participation. Upon obtaining written consent, participants completed a packet of measures that included the five self-report measures previously described. The five measures were administered in the following order, Perceptions of Future Parenting (API); Symptoms of Trauma Questionnaire (PTSD); Youth Psychopathy Inventory – Callous/Unemotional scale; Traumatic Events Questionnaire (maltreatment); and Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS). This order was established in an effort to obtain perceptions of anticipated future parenting styles that have not been directly affected by completion of the perceptions of past parenting measure (PPPS). Six additional brief self-report measures that were associated with a separate study were also included in the packet. These additional questionnaires measured exposure to real life violence, media violence, general psychopathology, hostility, aggression, and empathy. The measures were administered within the classroom setting (study halls) for the public school participants. Participants in the Juvenile Detention Center were taken from their holding area in groups of up to six to an available office space within the detention center Psychology Department to complete the measures. Total completion time for all measures was approximately 45 to 50 minutes for public school participants and 60 to 90 minutes for detention center participants. The experimenter was present throughout administration of the questionnaires to answer any questions. No substantive questions 100 were asked by public school participants. Juvenile Detention Center participants sometimes asked questions related to the content of the question, specifically clarification on what the question was asking. A few of the Juvenile Detention Center participants asked to have the entire packet of items read to them due to their reading level and inability to read and/or understand the items. All participants were offered a piece of candy for their participation. Public school participants were offered the opportunity to enter a drawing for a twenty-five dollar gift certificate to a local mall. 101 Chapter Four Results Data Analyses Due to the notable demographic differences between the public school and Juvenile Detention Center samples, analyses were performed separately by group. Preliminary Analyses. Means and standard deviations were calculated and are presented separately by public school and Juvenile Detention Center participant groups for items within the API in Table 6, the PPPS in Table 7, PTSD symptoms in Table 8, YPI in Table 9, Maltreatment experiences in Table 10, and About Me (social desirability) in Table 11. Anticipated warmth, rejection, and overprotection scores for each participant were obtained by summing the relevant items from the API for each of these three scales. For the total public school sample, the average score for anticipated warmth was 20.18 (SD = 1.68), for anticipated rejection 11.41 (SD = 2.11), and for anticipated overprotection 15.02 (SD = 2.60). Gender differences were significant for anticipated rejection (t(215) = 3.801, p < .01, d = -.52) with males scoring higher and anticipated warmth (t(215) = 2.808, p < .01, d = .39) with females scoring higher. Gender differences were not significant for anticipated overprotection (t(215) = .710, p > .05, d = -.10). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 12. 102 Table 6 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the API for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants ITEM Public School Warmth Scale 1. I will spend one-on-one time with my children 8. I will attend my child(ren)’s sporting events and other extracurricular activities 13. I will express affection and caring towards my child(ren) through words and gestures 16. I will use verbal statements to praise and encourage my child 18. I will listen to my child(ren) 23. I will show interest in what is going on in my child’s life 28. I will make sure my child(ren) feels like he/she can come to me for anything Overprotection Scale 2. I will require my child(ren) to check in regularly when out 6. I will try to instill fear in my child(ren) so that he/she will be afraid to do things 10. I will insist on meeting and approving all of my child(ren)’s friends 11. It will be hard for me to let my child(ren) become independent 14. My child(ren) will be expected to tell me who they are going to be with, where they are going, what they are planning to do, and why they want to do it before being allowed to go out 17. My child(ren) will not be allowed to do things that other children are allowed to do because I will be afraid that something will happen to them 21. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she can’t handle things on his/her own 24. I am likely to be overly worried that something might happen to my child(ren) Rejection Scale 3. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she is not good enough 7. I will compare my child to siblings or other children 103 M SD JDC M SD 2.88 .37 2.96 .24 2.91 .31 2.89 .37 2.83 .41 2.64 .59 2.87 2.91 .36 .30 2.79 2.89 .49 .37 2.89 .36 2.85 .41 2.89 .38 2.89 .37 2.59 .56 2.80 .45 1.37 .62 1.60 .75 2.07 .69 2.47 .64 1.80 .71 1.92 .77 2.45 .65 2.49 .64 1.48 .56 1.95 .69 1.25 .55 1.47 .66 2.02 .72 2.23 .77 1.13 .43 1.15 .52 1.16 .42 1.31 .61 9. I will probably favor one child over another 15. I will try to understand and support my child(ren)’s choices (reverse scored) 19. I will show interest in what my child(ren) is interested in (reverse scored) 20. I will probably expect too much from my children 25. I will make decisions for my child 29. No matter how well my child does at something, I will tell them they could have done better 31. I will listen to and try to understand my child(ren) (reverse scored) Discipline Scale 5. If my child(ren) does something wrong I will ground him/her 12. I will spank my child when he/she does something wrong 22. When my child does something wrong, I will discuss what was done wrong and why it was wrong with him/her 26. If my child does something wrong, I will throw away something that he/she really likes to punish him/her 27. I will give my child added chores to do as a means of punishment 30. I will use a belt, paddle, or other object to punish my child Note. Scores range from 1 to 3 for each item 104 1.15 .41 1.17 .47 1.06 .23 1.18 .45 1.18 .40 1.14 .42 1.70 1.64 .70 .61 1.76 2.05 .72 .67 1.35 .62 1.86 .80 1.05 .25 1.15 .50 2.26 .59 2.44 .59 1.55 .67 1.88 .65 2.81 .45 2.75 .50 1.14 .40 1.34 .57 2.18 .67 2.25 .67 1.13 .42 1.57 .72 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. my parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent ITEM .80 .68 .74 .76 .78 .76 .63 .77 .60 2.26 2.52 2.31 2.28 2.24 2.27 1.35 2.15 1.31 105 .79 2.16 1.48 1.39 2.41 2.51 2.44 2.49 2.61 2.47 2.52 2.35 2.43 .74 2.18 .70 .64 .67 .68 .74 .68 .63 .68 .69 .77 .69 Mother M SD Father M SD Public School 1.35 1.45 1.74 1.88 1.84 1.93 1.96 1.89 1.82 1.64 .66 .72 .80 .88 .91 .91 .91 .89 .86 .79 .80 Father SD 1.78 M JDC 1.52 1.76 2.21 2.43 2.39 2.39 2.49 2.39 2.35 2.05 2.31 .74 .85 .79 .74 .80 .79 .70 .75 .79 .83 .72 Mother M SD Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the PPPS for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants Table 7 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen t to me 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) 22. My parents expect too much from me 27. My parents try to make decisions for me 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) .77 .67 .67 .72 .67 .62 .66 .76 .70 .72 .69 .65 .74 2.21 1.53 1.45 1.78 1.47 1.36 1.39 1.73 1.86 1.74 1.67 1.49 1.70 106 1.50 1.54 1.89 1.77 1.83 1.51 1.41 1.38 1.45 2.05 1.57 1.64 2.44 .65 .72 .69 .71 .68 .69 .66 .66 .67 .72 .72 .67 .67 2.02 1.64 2.30 1.60 1.65 2.10 1.39 1.42 1.65 1.73 1.47 1.60 1.79 .90 .80 .79 .72 .74 .89 .72 .71 .83 .80 .70 .77 .83 1.59 1.84 2.02 1.82 1.98 1.63 1.58 1.57 1.49 2.16 1.69 1.89 2.28 .79 .78 .73 .78 .75 .76 .79 .79 .77 .81 .77 .79 .69 Note. Scores range from 1 to 3 for each item Discipline Scale 7. To punish me when I do something wrong, my parents ground me 14. I get hit by my parents when I do something wrong 24. When I do something wrong, my parents explain to me what was done wrong and why it was wrong 28. When I do something wrong, my parents throw away something of mine that I really like to punish me 29. When I do something wrong, my parents make me to extra jobs at home 32. My parents hit me with a belt, paddle, or other object to punish me when I do something wrong .75 .43 .77 .45 .74 .49 1.74 1.18 2.21 1.16 1.64 1.20 107 1.14 1.74 1.18 2.34 1.18 1.85 .41 .73 .49 .75 .47 .74 1.42 1.58 1.33 1.78 1.37 1.86 .71 .75 .62 .82 .65 .83 1.59 2.03 1.49 2.22 1.67 2.14 .76 .77 .73 .76 .75 .74 Table 8 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Measure of PTSD Symptoms for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants ITEM Public School M 1. I watch out for danger or things that I am afraid of 2. I get very upset, afraid, or sad when something reminds me of a bad thing that happened to me 3. Thoughts, pictures, or sounds of a bad thing that happened to me come into my mind when I don’t want them to and they upset me 4. I feel grouchy, angry or mad 5. I have dreams about a bad thing that happened to me or other bad dreams 6. I feel like I am back when the bad thing happened and living through it again 7. I feel like being by myself and not being with my friends 8. I feel alone inside and not close to other people 9. I try not to talk about, think about, or have feelings about a bad thing that happened to me 10. I have trouble feeling happy or loved 11. I have trouble feeling sad or angry 12. I feel jumpy and easily started, like when I hear a loud noise or when something surprises me 13. I have trouble getting to sleep or I wake up a lot during the night 14. I think that part of a bad thing that happened to me is my fault 15. I have trouble remembering parts of a bad thing that happened to me 16. I have trouble concentrating or paying attention 17. I try to stay away from people, places, or things that make me remember a bad thing that happened 18. When something reminds me of a bad thing that happened, I have strong feelings in my body, like my heart beating fast, my head aches, or my stomach aches 19. I think that I will not live a long life 20. I am afraid that a bad thing that happened to me will happen again Note. Scores range from 1 to 4 for each item 108 SD JDC M SD 2.79 .86 2.97 .88 2.44 1.01 2.76 1.00 2.13 2.48 .98 .81 2.27 2.62 1.07 .82 1.95 .93 2.17 1.09 1.68 .88 1.99 .99 2.06 2.10 .92 1.04 2.13 2.21 .88 .99 2.48 1.96 1.96 1.06 .99 .89 2.58 2.02 1.85 1.08 1.03 .95 2.26 .99 2.37 1.06 2.48 1.08 2.76 1.09 2.30 1.00 2.47 1.02 1.96 2.57 .90 1.05 1.92 2.50 1.00 1.07 2.15 1.04 2.46 1.05 2.22 2.07 1.02 1.04 2.27 2.16 1.12 1.07 2.09 1.03 2.37 1.10 Table 9 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants ITEM Public School 1. I usually feel calm when other people are scared 2. I have the ability not to feel guilt and regret about things that I think other people would feel guilty about 3. I think that crying is a sign of weakness, even if no one sees you 4. When other people have problems, it is often their own fault, therefore, one should not help them 5. I seldom regret things I do, even if other people feel that they are wrong 6. It’s important to me not to hurt other people’s feelings 7. To be nervous and worried is a sign of weakness 8. When someone finds out about something that I’ve done wrong, I feel more angry than guilty 9. I often become sad or moved by watching sad things on TV or film 10. What scares others usually doesn’t scare me 11. I don’t understand how people can be touched enough to cry by looking at things on TV or a movie 12. To feel guilt and remorseful about things you have done that have hurt other people is a sign of weakness 13. I don’t let my feelings affect me as much as other people’s feelings seem to affect them 14. To feel guilt and regret when you have done something wrong is a waste of time 15. I usually become sad when I see other people crying or being sad Note. Scores range from 1 to 4 for each item 109 JDC M SD M SD 2.76 .80 2.59 1.02 2.23 .91 2.30 1.06 2.11 1.02 2.13 1.19 1.57 .70 1.96 1.01 2.22 .98 2.36 1.04 1.73 1.92 .92 .89 2.25 2.00 1.03 .97 2.21 1.01 2.53 .99 2.35 2.49 1.07 .86 2.81 2.56 1.04 1.03 2.02 1.06 2.36 1.12 1.76 .88 1.99 1.02 2.36 .99 2.34 1.09 1.98 .94 2.25 1.03 2.39 .94 2.67 1.04 Table 10 Means and Standard Deviations of Individual Items Within the Measure of Maltreatment Experiences for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants ITEM Public School 1. I didn’t have enough to eat 2. I have been called mean names by people in my family 3. I have been scared of being hurt by someone in my family 4. I have felt like there was no one to take care of me and protect me 5. I was punished with a belt, board, or other object 6. Someone tried to make me do or watch sexual things 7. Someone in my family hit me so hard that it left marks or bruises 8. Someone tried to make me touch them or they tried to touch me in a sexual way 9. I felt that someone in my family hated me 10. My parents were too drunk or high to take care of the family 11. I had sex with someone a lot older than me (more than five years older) 12. Someone in my family said things that were hurtful or insulting to me 13. I was hit or beaten badly enough that it was noticed by someone like a babysitter, teacher or doctor 14. Someone made threats that they would hurt me or tell lies about me if I did not do sexual things with them 15. Someone in my family hit or beat me badly enough that I had to see a doctor 16. I was not taken to the doctor when I was sick or needed a checkup Note. Scores range from 1 to 4 for each item 110 JDC M 1.89 SD .94 M 1.96 SD 1.01 2.18 .99 2.37 1.06 1.67 .96 1.82 1.06 1.63 1.60 .95 .94 1.87 2.36 1.13 1.07 1.29 .76 1.53 .99 1.46 .83 2.01 1.12 1.30 1.79 .78 1.02 1.56 2.09 1.02 1.18 1.21 .58 1.51 .93 1.21 .67 2.30 1.26 2.03 1.01 2.29 1.13 1.18 .61 1.50 .93 1.15 .60 1.39 .84 1.09 .43 1.39 .86 1.43 .82 1.71 .99 Table 11 Means and Standard Deviations Within the Measure of Social Desirability for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants ITEM Public School M 1. M SD 1.22 .42 1.25 .43 2. Sometimes I do not feel like doing what my teachers want me to do 1.20 .40 1.22 .41 3. Sometimes I have felt like throwing things or breaking them 1.37 .48 1.25 .43 I never talk back to my parent or guardian 1.80 .40 1.68 .47 When I make a mistake, I always admit that I am wrong 1.59 .49 1.59 .49 6. I sometimes feel like making fun of other people 1.35 .48 1.29 .46 I always wash my hands before every meal 1.66 .48 1.49 .50 Sometimes I wish I could just hang out instead of going to school 1.10 .31 1.29 .46 I have never been tempted to break a law or rule 1.75 .43 1.80 .40 10. Sometimes I dislike helping my parent or guardian even though I know they need my help around the house 1.17 .38 1.37 .49 11. Sometimes I say things just to impress my friends 1.58 .49 1.52 .50 12. I never should when I feel angry 1.81 Note. Scores range from 1 to 2 for each item. .39 1.80 .40 4. 5. 7. 8. 9. I am always respectful to older people SD JDC 111 Table 12 API scales of Anticipated Warmth, Anticipated Rejection, and Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants by Gender Public School Parenting Behavior M SD JDC M SD ________________________________________________________________________ Anticipated Warmth* Males 19.93 1.71 20.02 1.64 Females 20.54 1.44 19.09 3.48 Males 11.89 2.27 12.60 2.37 Females 10.85 1.72 14.18 3.49 Males 15.12 2.75 16.96 2.59 Females 14.87 2.43 16.73 3.29 Anticipated Rejection* Anticipated Overprotection* *Note. Possible range of scores: Anticipated Warmth 7 to 21; Anticipated Overprotection 8 to 24; Anticipated Rejection 9 to 27 112 For the JDC sample, the average score for anticipated warmth was 19.92 (SD = 1.91), for anticipated rejection 12.76 (SD = 2.52), and for anticipated overprotection 16.93 (SD = 2.64). Gender differences were significant for anticipated rejection (t(100) = -1.98, p < .05, d = .63) with males scoring higher. Gender differences were not significant for anticipated warmth (t(100) = 1.52, p > .05, d = -.49) or anticipated overprotection (t(100) = .267, p > .05, d = -.09). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 12. Independent t-tests were performed to compare the means for each of the scales of the API for the two groups. Significantly higher anticipated rejection was reported by the Juvenile Detention Center participants (M = 12.76, SD = 2.52) than public school participants (M = 11.41, SD = 2.11), t(325) = 5.049, p < .01, d = .60. Higher anticipated overprotection was also reported by the Juvenile Detention Center participants (M = 16.93, SD = 2.64) than the public school participants (M = 15.02, SD = 2.60), t(325) = 6.109, p < .01, d = .73. A significant difference did not exist between the two groups for anticipated warmth (t(325) = -1.252, p > .05, d = -.15). Total maltreatment experiences for each participant were obtained by summing all items from the maltreatment measure. For the total public school sample, the average total maltreatment score was 24.13 (SD = 8.30). Gender differences were not significant (t(212) = -1.77, p > .05, d = .24 ). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. For the JDC sample, the average total maltreatment score was 29.64 (S = 10.53). Gender differences were not significant (t(99) = -1.73, p > .05, d = .58). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. Independent t-test revealed that the 113 Juvenile Detention Center sample (M = 29.64, SD = 10.53) reported significantly more maltreatment experiences than the public school sample (M = 24.13, SD = 8.30), t(320) = 5.056, p < .01, d = .61. Total PTSD symptoms for each participant were obtained by summing all items from the measure of PTSD symptoms. For the total public school sample, the average total PTSD symptoms score was 44.13 (SD = 10.72). Gender differences were significant (t(215) = -3.03, p < .05, d = .41) with females scoring higher. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. For the JDC sample, the average total PTSD symptoms score was 46.86 (SD = 11.81). Gender differences were not significant (t(100) = -1.21, p > .05, d = .39). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. The Juvenile Detention Center participants reported significantly higher PTSD symptoms on average than the public school sample, t(323) = 2.065, p < .05, d = .25. Total callous-unemotional trait for each participant was obtained by summing all relevant items from the YPI. For the total public school sample, the average total callous-unemotional score was 21.72 (SD = 5.46). Gender differences were significant (t(213) = 6.72, p < .01, d = -.92) with males scoring higher. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. For the JDC sample, the average total callousunemotional score was 23.67 (SD = 4.89). Gender differences were not significant (t(99) = 1.15, p > .05, d = -.38). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. The Juvenile Detention Center participants reported more callous-unemotional psychopathy trait on average than the public school participants, t(319) = 3.074, p < .01, d = .37. 114 Table 13 Total Maltreatment Experiences, Total Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy, Total PTSD Symptoms, and Social Desirability Total Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants by Gender Public School Variable M SD JDC M SD ________________________________________________________________________ Maltreatment* Males 22.93 7.46 28.94 9.78 Females 24.89 8.59 34.90 14.72 Males 23.96 4.99 23.88 5.03 Females 19.37 4.99 22.00 3.30 Males 42.05 10.98 46.37 11.69 Females 46.39 10.07 50.91 12.24 Males 3.81 2.46 4.08 2.46 Females 3.97 2.34 5.41 2.78 Callous-Unemotional* PTSD Symptoms* Social Desirability* *Note. Possible range of scores: Maltreatment 16 to 64; Callous-Unemotional 10 to 40; PTSD Symptoms 20 to 80; Social Desirability 0 to 12. 115 A social desirability total was obtained by summing all items from the About Me measure after recoding of specific items. For the total public school sample, the average total social desirability score was 3.93 (SD = 2.40). Gender differences were not significant (t(212) = .47, p > .05, d = .07). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. For the JDC sample, the average total social desirability score was 4.32 (SD = 2.56). Gender differences were not significant (t(97) = 1.74, p > .05, d = .54). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 13. A significant difference did not exist between samples on the social desirability total (t(319) = 1.31, p > .05, d = .16). Perceived warmth, rejection, and overprotection scores for mothers and fathers for each participant were obtained by summing the relevant items from the PPPS for each of these three scales. The average score for father’s warmth for the total public school sample was 15.96 (SD = 4.19), for father’s rejection 14.41 (SD = 4.01), and for father’s overprotection 14.06 (SD = 3.24). Gender differences were not significant for fathers’ warmth (t(211) = .61, p > .05, d = -.08), rejection (t(211) = -1.60, p > .05, d = .22), or overprotection (t(211) = .03, p > .05, d = -.00). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 14. The average score for mother’s warmth for the total public school sample was 17.31 (SD = 3.80), for mother’s rejection 14.26 (SD = 4.07), and for mother’s overprotection 15.50 (SD = 2.83). Gender differences were not significant for mother’s warmth (t(211) = -.68, p > .05, d = .10), rejection (t(211) = -1.34, p > .05, d = .19), or overprotection (t(211) = .38, p > .05, d = -.05). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 15. For the total JDC sample, the average score for father’s warmth was 12.86 (SD = 5.16), for father’s rejection 15.77 (SD = 3.78) and for father’s overprotection 13.01 (SD = 116 4.31). Gender differences were significant for father’s warmth (t(98) = 2.39, p < .05, d = -.80) with males scoring higher and father’s rejection (t(98) = -3.18, p < .01, d = 1.06) with females scoring higher. Gender differences were not significant for father’s overprotection (t(98) = 1.09, p > .05, d = -.37). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 14. Table 14 PPPS Father’s Warmth, Rejection, and Overprotection Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants by Gender Public School Parenting Behavior M SD JDC M SD ________________________________________________________________________ Father’s Warmth* Males 16.17 3.82 13.25 5.24 Females 15.82 4.50 9.20 3.36 Males 13.96 3.46 15.27 3.46 Females 14.84 4.45 19.00 3.94 Males 14.05 2.96 13.05 4.33 Females 14.04 3.49 11.50 3.21 Father’s Rejection* Father’s Overprotection* *Note. Possible range of scores: Father’s Warmth 7 to 21; Father’s Overprotection 8 to 24; Father’s Rejection 9 to 27 117 Table 15 PPPS Mother’s Warmth, Rejection, and Overprotection Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants by Gender Public School Parenting Behavior M SD JDC M SD ________________________________________________________________________ Mother’s Warmth* Males 17.18 3.51 16.49 4.34 Females 17.54 3.99 14.90 4.07 Males 13.84 3.45 15.04 3.95 Females 14.59 4.53 18.70 4.16 Males 15.58 2.81 15.76 3.53 Females 15.44 2.85 16.30 4.06 Mother’s Rejection* Mother’s Overprotection* *Note. Possible range of scores: Mother’s Warmth 7 to 21; Mother’s Overprotection 8 to 24; Mother’s Rejection 9 to 27 118 The average score for mother’s warmth for the JDC sample was 16.35 (SD = 4.31), for mother’s rejection 15.53 (SD = 4.20) and for mother’s overprotection 15.94 (SD = 3.63). Gender differences were not significant for mother’s warmth (t(99) = 1.11, p > .05, d = -.37) or mothers’ overprotection (t(99) = -.45, p > .05, d = .15). Gender differences were significant for mother’s rejection (t(99) = -2.76, p < .05, d = .92) with females scoring higher. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 15. Independent t-test comparisons of the means of the two samples for each of the scales of the PPPS revealed some significant differences. Public school participants reported experiencing significantly more father’s warmth, t(316) = -5.676, p < .01, d = .69), and father’s overprotection, t(316) = -2.397, p < .05, d = -.29, and less father’s rejection, t(316) = 2.851, p = .01, d = .35, than the Juvenile Detention Center participants. Regarding perceptions of mother’s past parenting behaviors, public school participants reported experiencing significantly more mother’s warmth, t(317) = -2.021, p < .05, d = .24, and less mother’s rejection, t(317) = 2.574, p = .01, d = .31, than Juvenile Detention Center participants. Differences between groups were not significant for mother’s overprotection, t(317) = 1.191, p > .05, d = .14. For the purposes of analysis, all items within the PPPS were combined to reflect one positive parenting variable for mother behaviors and one positive parenting variable for father behaviors. Negative items were recoded and relevant items were totaled separately for father items and for mother items, producing a total father positive parenting score and a total mother positive parenting score, with a higher score reflecting the experience of more positive parenting behaviors. For the public school sample, the 119 average total father’s positive parenting score was 55.49 (SD = 7.82) and the average total mother’s positive parenting score was 55.56 (SD = 7.86) Gender differences were not significant for father’s positive parenting (t(211) = 1.15, p < .05, d = -.16) or for mother’s positive parenting (t(211) = .23, p < .05, d = -.03). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 16. For the JDC sample, the average total father’s positive parenting score was 52.08 (SD = 7.14) and the average total mother’s positive parenting score was 52.87 (SD = 7.50). Gender differences were significant for father’s positive parenting (t(98) = 2.78, p < .05, d = -.93) and for mother’s positive parenting (t(99) = 2.44, p < .05, d = -.81) with males scoring higher. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 16. The public school sample reported more father’s positive parenting (t(316) = -3.709, p < .01, d = -.45) and more mother’s positive parenting (t(317) = -2.878, p < .01, d = -.35) than Juvenile Detention Center participants. Bivariate correlations between primary study variables are presented in Table 17. As anticipated, significant correlations existed at the p < .05 level between anticipated warmth in future parenting and past parenting experiences of father’s rejection and overprotection. Anticipated warmth was negatively correlated with father’s rejection and positively correlated with father’s overprotection. Significant correlations existed at the p < .01 level between anticipated warmth in future parenting and the callous-unemotional trait, total maltreatment experiences, and past parenting experiences of father’s warmth, mother’s rejection and mother’s warmth. Anticipated warmth was negatively correlated with the callous-unemotional trait, total maltreatment experiences, and mother’s rejection. Anticipated warmth was positively correlated with both father’s warmth and mother’s warmth. 120 Table 16 Total Father’s and Mother’s Positive Parenting Means and Standard Deviations for Public School and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants by Gender Public School M SD JDC M SD ________________________________________________________________________ Father’s Positive Parenting* Males 56.17 6.67 52.93 6.81 Females 54.94 8.60 46.70 5.70 Males 55.76 6.90 53.69 7.08 Females 55.51 8.63 47.90 7.45 Mother’s Positive Parenting* *Note. Possible range of scores: 24 to 72 A significant positive correlation existed at the p < .05 level between anticipated rejection in future parenting and past parenting experience of father’s rejection. Significant correlations existed at the p < .01 level between anticipated rejection in future parenting and total symptoms of posttraumatic stress, the callous-unemotional trait, total maltreatment experiences, and past parenting experiences of father’s warmth, mother’s 121 rejection, and mother’s overprotection. Anticipated rejection was negatively correlated with past parenting experience of father’s warmth and positively correlated with total symptoms of posttraumatic stress, the callous-unemotional trait, total maltreatment experiences, and past parenting experiences of mothers’ rejection and mother’s overprotection. Anticipated overprotection in future parenting was positively correlated at the p < .01 level with total symptoms of posttraumatic stress and past parenting experience of mother’s overprotection. Partial correlations were performed to examine relationships between study variables after controlling for the effects of social desirability and gender. Significant correlations continued to exist between study variables after removing the effects of both gender and social desirability. Results are presented in Table 18. 122 .16** .22** -.12* .39** -.00 -.08 -.03 -.05 -.06 .03 -.06 3. AntProtect 4. AntReject 5. PTSD 6. CalUnemo 7. Maltreat 8. F Reject 9. F Protect 10. F Warm 11. M Reject 12. M Protect 13. M Warm .25** .04 -.17** .25** .12* -.11* -.25** -.25** -.07 -.51** .14* .07 .23** .02 -.05 .06 .06 .09 .09 .20** .28** -.07 .18** .17** -.21** -.04 .14* .22** .30** .20** -.20** .16** .31** -.26** .03 .33** .49** .02 -.21** .05 .15** -.22** -.06 .08 .18** -.44** -.02 .43** -.40** -.09 .39** -.38** .04 .52** -.67* .00 .13* .38** .00 .48** .51** .09 -.39** -.67** .14* .29** *p < .05, **p < .01 123 14. Social Desire -.04 .07 .16** .03 -.16** -.13* -.12* -.13* .05 .17** -.10 .05 .16** _____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ -.12* 2. AntWarm 1. Gender Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 _____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Bivariate Correlations Between all Study Variables Table 17 -.43** -.04 -.17* -.21** -.11* .10 .21** -.15** .06 .23** 3. AntReject 4. PTSD 5. CalUnemo 6. Maltreat 7. F Reject 8. F Protect 9. F Warm 10. M Reject 11. M Protect 12. M Warm .07 .22** .05 -.06 .06 .10 .12* .05 .28** .30** -.04 .18** .18** -.18** -.03 .17** .19** .21** .22** -.18** .18** .30** -.23** .03 .31** .47** .02 -.18** .05 .17** -.17** -.04 .09 .14* -.43** -.01 .44** -.38** -.08 .40** -.36** .02 .48** -.67** -.04 .15** .38** -.03 .51** .48** .10 -.37** -.69** .11 .29** *p < .05, **p < .01 124 ______________________________________________________________________________________________________________ .10 2. AntProtect 1. AntWarm Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 _______________________________________________________________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Partial Correlations Between All Study Variables Table 18 Regression Analyses The Enter method was utilized for all regression analyses. For the public school sample, all regressions were performed separately by gender due to initial regression analyses indicating significant gender differences. Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms as Predictors of Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine predictors of callousunemotional psychopathy. Independent variables were father’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, and PTSD symptoms. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Father’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered. For the public school sample, for females, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in callous-unemotional psychopathy (F [1, 106] = 5.00, p < .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for another 8% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 105] = 9.51, p < .01). Maltreatment and PTSD explained another 1% of the variance above the other two variables and the change in R was not significant. Father’s positive parenting was a significant predictor of callousunemotional psychopathy with less experiences of father’s positive parenting related to higher scores on callous-unemotional psychopathy among females. Results of the regression are presented in Table 19. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in callous-unemotional psychopathy (F [1, 99] = 2.45, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained less than one percent of the variance above the first variable and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 1% of the variance above the first two variables 125 and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of callous-unemotional psychopathy among males. Results of the regression are presented in Table 20. Table 19 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 -.45 .20 -.21* -.33 .20 -.15 -.17 .05 -.29** Social Desirability -.31 .21 -.15 Father’s Positive Parenting -.14 .07 -.25* Maltreatment .06 .07 .11 -.02 .06 -.04 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .08 Step 3 PTSD .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 126 Table 20 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .02 -.32 .21 -.16 -.31 .21 -.15 -.02 .08 -.03 Social Desirability -.27 .22 -.13 Father’s Positive Parenting -.00 .08 -.01 Maltreatment .05 .08 .09 -.00 .05 -.01 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 127 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, Step 1 of the regression also included gender. Gender and social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in callousunemotional psychopathy (F [2, 93] = 1.10, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained an additional less than one percent of the variance over the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of callous-unemotional psychopathy among the Juvenile Detention Center participants. Results from the regression are presented in Table 21. The same regression analyses were repeated using the mother’s positive parenting variable entered in Step 2 in place of the father’s positive parenting variable. Independent variables were mother’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, and PTSD symptoms. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Mother’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered. Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in callous-unemotional psychopathy (F [1, 106] = 5.00, p < .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional 8% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 105] = 9.83, p < .01). Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 1% of the variance above the other two variables and the change in R was not significant. Mother’s positive parenting was found to significantly predict callousunemotional psychopathy with less experiences of mother’s positive parenting related to higher scores on callous-unemotional psychopathy among females. Results of the regression are presented in Table 22. 128 Table 21 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender 1.23 1.61 .08 -.21 .19 -.12 .95 1.71 .06 -.24 .20 -.13 .03 .08 .06 Gender 1.14 1.72 .07 Social Desirability -.26 .20 -.14 Father’s Positive Parenting .02 .08 .04 Maltreatment .06 .05 .14 -.04 .05 -.12 Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Gender Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .02 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 95 129 Table 22 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 -.45 .20 -.21* -.30 .20 -.14 -.17 .05 -.30** Social Desirability -.27 .21 -.13 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.15 .06 -.25* Maltreatment .07 .07 .13 -.02 .06 -.05 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .08 Step 3 PTSD .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 Among the males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in callous-unemotional psychopathy (F [1, 99] = 2.45, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained another 2% of the variance and the change in R 130 was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained less than one percent additional variance above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to significantly predict callous-unemotional psychopathy among males. Results of the regression are presented in Table 23. Table 23 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .02 -.32 .21 -.16 -.24 .21 -.12 -.11 .08 -.15 Social Desirability -.21 .22 -.10 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.10 .08 -.14 .04 .08 .07 -.01 .05 -.04 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 131 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, Step 1 of the regression included gender. Gender and social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in callousunemotional psychopathy (F [2,94] = 1.03, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 1% of the variance over the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 1% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of callous-unemotional psychopathy among Juvenile Detention Center participants. Results from the regression are presented in Table 24. Positive Parenting, Maltreatment, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy as predictors of Delinquency Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine predictors of delinquency. Independent variables were positive past parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms, and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Father’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callousunemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 132 Table 24 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, and PTSD Symptoms Predicting Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender 1.35 1.62 .09 -.19 .19 -.10 Gender 1.65 1.67 .11 Social Desirability -.19 .20 -.10 -.05 .07 -.08 Gender 1.62 1.68 .11 Social Desirability -.22 .20 -.12 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.04 .08 -.08 Maltreatment .04 .06 .09 -.04 .05 -.12 Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Mother’s Positive Parenting .01 Step 3 PTSD .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 95 133 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in delinquency (F [1, 106] = 2.92, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 22% of the variance above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 103] = 15.25, p < .01). Callousunemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment significantly predicted delinquency with more reported maltreatment experiences related to higher reported arrests. PTSD was also found to significantly predict delinquency with fewer reported PTSD symptoms related to higher reported arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 25. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in delinquency (F [1, 99] = 4.68, p < .05). Father’s positive parenting explained less than one percent additional variance over the first variable and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 31% of the variance over the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 22.68, p < .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for less than one percent additional variance in delinquency and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment was found to significantly predict delinquency with more reported maltreatment experiences related to higher reported arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 26. 134 Table 25 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 -.13 .07 -.16 -.10 .07 -.14 -.02 .02 -.13 Social Desirability -.07 .07 -.10 Father’s Positive Parenting .01 .02 .10 Maltreatment .12 .02 .61** -.04 .02 -.26** -.07 .07 -.10 Father’s Positive Parenting .02 .02 .10 Maltreatment .12 .02 .61** -.04 .02 -.26** .00 .03 .01 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 PTSD .22 Step 4 Social Desirability PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 135 Table 26 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 -.19 .09 -.21* -.19 .09 -.21* -.00 .03 -.01 Social Desirability -.07 .08 -.08 Father’s Positive Parenting .04 .03 .13 Maltreatment .19 .03 .64** -.03 .02 -.16 -.06 .08 -.08 Father’s Positive Parenting .04 .03 .13 Maltreatment .18 .03 .64** -.03 .02 -.16 .01 .04 .05 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .31 Step 4 Social Desirability PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 136 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for 7% of the variance in delinquency (F [2, 88] = 3.16, p = .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for less than one percent of the variance over the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 1% of the variance above the other three variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. Social desirability was a significant predictor of delinquency with higher social desirability scores related to lower reported number of arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 27. The same regression analyses were performed with the mother’s positive parenting variable entered in Step 2 in place of the father’s positive parenting variable. Independent variables were mother’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Mother’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callous-unemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 137 Table 27 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender .45 1.78 .03 Social Desirability .07 -.50 .21 Gender .09 1.88 Social Desirability -.53 .21 -.27* .04 .08 .07 Gender .38 1.91 .02 Social Desirability -.53 .22 -.27* Father’s Positive Parenting .06 .08 .09 Maltreatment .04 .06 .10 -.00 .05 -.01 .38 1.93 .02 -.53 .22 -.27* Father’s Positive Parenting .06 .09 .09 Maltreatment .04 .06 .10 -.00 .05 -.01 .00 .11 .00 -.25* Step 2 Father’s Positive Parenting .00 .01 Step 3 PTSD .01 Step 4 Gender Social Desirability PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 91 138 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in delinquency (F [1,106] = 2.92, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 4% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1,105] = 3.99, p < .05). Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 20% of the variance above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 103] = 13.58, p < .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment was found to significantly predict delinquency with more reported maltreatment experiences related to more reported arrests. PTSD also significantly predicted delinquency with fewer reported symptoms of PTSD related to higher reported arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 28. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in delinquency (F [1, 99] = 4.68, p < .05). Mother’s positive explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 30% of the variance in delinquency above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 22.11, p < .01). Callousunemotional psychopathy accounted for less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment was the only significant predictor of delinquency among males with more reported maltreatment experiences related to higher reported arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 29. 139 Table 28 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 -.12 .07 -.16 -.08 .07 -.12 -.03 .02 -.20* Social Desirability -.07 .07 -.09 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .02 -.05 Maltreatment .11 .02 .54** -.04 .02 -.28* Social Desirability -.07 .07 -.10 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .02 -.06 Maltreatment .11 .02 .55** PTSD -.04 .02 -.28* -.00 .03 -.02 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 Step 3 PTSD .20 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 140 Table 29 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 -.19 .09 -.21* -.18 .09 -.21* -.00 .03 -.02 -.07 .08 -.09 Mother’s Positive Parenting .03 .03 .10 Maltreatment .18 .03 .63** -.02 .02 -.15 Social Desirability -.06 .08 -.08 Mother’s Positive Parenting .03 .03 .11 Maltreatment .18 .03 .63** PTSD -.02 .02 -.15 .02 .04 .06 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 Social Desirability PTSD .30 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 141 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for 6% of the variance in delinquency (F [2, 89] = 3.01, p = .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained less than one percent of the variance above the other three variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Social desirability was a significant predictor of delinquency with higher social desirability scores related to lower reported number of arrests. Results of the regression are presented in Table 30. Positive Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, PTSD Symptoms, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Future Parenting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting Multiple regression analyses were performed to examine predictors of anticipated warmth in future parenting. Independent variables were perceptions of past parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms, and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability were entered in Step 1. Father’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step 3, Maltreatment and PTSD were entered. Callous-unemotional psychopathy was entered in Step 4. 142 Table 30 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Delinquency for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender .53 1.77 -.49 .21 Gender .75 1.84 .04 Social Desirability -.49 .21 -.25* -.03 .07 -.05 Gender .88 1.87 .05 Social Desirability -.48 .21 -.24* Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .09 -.02 .03 .07 .08 -.00 .05 -.02 .85 1.89 .05 Social Desirability -.48 .22 -.24* Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .09 -.02 Maltreatment .03 .07 .08 PTSD -.00 .05 -.02 .02 .11 .02 Social Desirability .06 .03 -.25* Step 2 Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .00 Step 4 Gender Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 91 143 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 5.17, p < .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 105] = 3.81, p = .05). Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 4% of the variance above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained an additional 4% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting and the change in R was significant (F [1,102] = 5.21, p < .05). Callous-unemotional psychopathy was found to significantly predict anticipated warmth in future parenting with higher callous-unemotional psychopathy scores related to lower anticipated warmth. Results of the regression are presented in Table 31. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance (F [1, 99] = 2.70, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained an additional 4% of the variance and the change in R approached significance (F [1, 98] = 3.62, p = .06). Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 7% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 3.90, p < .05). Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained an additional 1% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment was found to significantly predict anticipated warmth in future parenting with higher reported maltreatment experiences related to lower anticipated warmth. Results of the regression are presented in Table 32. 144 Table 31 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 .13 .06 .22* .11 .06 .18 .03 .02 .19* Social Desirability .11 .06 .18 Father’s Positive Parenting .01 .02 .12 -.03 .02 -.23* .02 .02 .15 Social Desirability .08 .06 .14 Father’s Positive Parenting .01 .02 .06 Maltreatment -.03 .02 -.21 .02 .02 .15 -.06 .03 -.22* Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .03 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .04 Step 4 PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .04 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 145 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability explained 1% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting (F [2, 93] = .49, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained less than one percent additional variance above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for 3% additional variance and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of anticipated warmth in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 33. The same regression analyses were performed with the mother’s positive parenting variable entered in Step 2 the place of father’s positive parenting. Independent variables were mother’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Mother’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callous-unemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 146 Table 32 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 .12 .07 .16 .08 .07 .12 .05 .03 .19 Social Desirability .04 .07 .06 Father’s Positive Parenting .03 .03 .12 -.06 .03 -.30* .00 .02 .03 Social Desirability .03 .07 .04 Father’s Positive Parenting .03 .03 .12 -.06 .03 -.29* .00 .02 .03 -.03 .03 -.11 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .04 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .07 Step 4 Maltreatment PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 147 Table 33 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.12 .51 -.03 -.06 .06 -.11 Gender -.15 .54 -.03 Social Desirability -.06 .06 -.11 .00 .02 .02 Gender -.24 .54 -.05 Social Desirability -.06 .06 -.10 Father’s Positive Parenting .00 .03 .01 Maltreatment -.02 .02 -.19 .01 .02 .09 Gender -.23 .55 -.05 Social Desirability -.06 .07 -.11 Father’s Positive Parenting .00 .03 .01 Maltreatment -.02 .02 -.18 .01 .02 .09 -.00 .03 -.03 Social Desirability .01 Step 2 Father’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .03 Step 4 PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 95 148 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 5% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 5.17, p < .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 4% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained an additional 4% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 102] = 5.03, p < .05). Callous-unemotional psychopathy was found to significantly predict anticipated warmth in future parenting with lower scores on callous-unemotional psychopathy related to higher anticipated warmth in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 34. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting (F [1, 99] = 2.70, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional 6% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 98] = 6.77, p = .01). Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 6% of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 3.59, p < .05). Callousunemotional psychopathy explained an additional 1% and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment was found to significantly predict anticipated warmth in future parenting with lower reported maltreatment experiences related to higher anticipated warmth. Results of the regression are presented in Table 35. 149 Table 34 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .05 .13 .06 .22* .11 .06 .17 .03 .02 .18 Social Desirability .10 .06 .17 Mother’s Positive Parenting .02 .02 .13 -.04 .02 -.24* .02 .02 .16 Social Desirability .08 .06 .14 Mother’s Positive Parenting .01 .02 .07 Maltreatment -.03 .02 -.22 PTSD .02 .02 .15 -.06 .03 -.22* Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .03 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .04 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .04 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 150 Table 35 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 .12 .07 .16 .06 .07 .09 .06 .03 .26* Social Desirability .02 .07 .04 Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 .03 .20* -.06 .03 -.29* .00 .02 .05 Social Desirability .01 .07 .03 Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 .03 .19 -.06 .03 -.28* .00 .02 .05 -.03 .03 -.09 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .06 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .06 Step 4 Maltreatment PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .01 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 151 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for one percent of the variance in anticipated warmth in future parenting (F [2, 94] = .45, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 3% of the variance above the other three variables and the change in R was not significant. Callousunemotional psychopathy explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of anticipated warmth in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 36. Positive Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, PTSD Symptoms, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine predictors of anticipated rejection in future parenting. Independent variables were father’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms, and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Father’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callousunemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 152 Table 36 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Warmth in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.10 .51 -.02 -.05 .06 -.10 Gender -.13 .53 -.03 Social Desirability -.05 .06 -.10 .00 .02 .02 Gender -.18 .53 -.04 Social Desirability -.05 .06 -.10 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .03 -.06 Maltreatment -.03 .02 -.21 .01 .02 .09 Gender -.17 .53 -.04 Social Desirability -.05 .06 -.10 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.01 .03 -.06 Maltreatment -.03 .02 -.21 PTSD .01 .02 .09 -.00 .03 -.02 Social Desirability .01 Step 2 Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .03 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 95 153 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 4% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 4.89, p < .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained less than one percent additional variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 4% of the variance above the first four variables and the change in R was significant (F [1, 102] = 4.74, p < .05). Callous-unemotional psychopathy significantly predicted anticipated rejection in future parenting with higher scores on callous-unemotional psychopathy related to higher anticipated rejection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 37. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for less than one percent of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [1, 99] = .03, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 10% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 5.52, p = .01). Callousunemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R approached significance (F [1, 95] = 3.54, p = .06). PTSD symptoms significantly predicted anticipated rejection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to higher anticipated rejection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 38. 154 Table 37 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .04 -.16 .07 -.21* -.13 .07 -.18 -.02 .02 -.15 Social Desirability -.14 .08 -.20 Father’s Positive Parenting -.03 .02 -.17 Maltreatment -.00 .03 -.01 -.01 .02 -.06 Social Desirability -.12 .07 -.16 Father’s Positive Parenting -.02 .02 -.12 Maltreatment -.00 .03 -.03 PTSD -.00 .02 -.05 .07 .04 .22* Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 PTSD .00 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .04 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 . 155 Table 38 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .00 .01 .09 .02 .05 .10 .06 -.05 .04 -.16 .07 .10 .08 -.02 .04 -.07 .03 .03 .11 .05 .02 .27* .09 .09 .11 Father’s Positive Parenting -.02 .04 -.07 Maltreatment .02 .03 .09 PTSD .05 .02 .27* .08 .04 .18 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting Maltreatment PTSD .10 Step 4 Social Desirability Callous-Unemotional Trait .03 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 156 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [2, 93] = 1.13, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 3% of the variance above the other three variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to significantly predict anticipated rejection in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 39. The same regression analyses were performed with mother’s positive parenting entered in Step 2 in the place of father’s positive parenting. Independent variables were mother’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms and callousunemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Mother’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callous-unemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 157 Table 39 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.82 .82 -.10 .08 .10 .09 -.51 .87 -.07 .12 .10 .12 -.04 .04 -.12 -.44 .87 -.06 .14 .10 .15 Father’s Positive Parenting -.02 .04 -.06 Maltreatment .00 .03 .00 .03 .03 .17 Gender -.54 .87 -.07 Social Desirability .16 .10 .17 Father’s Positive Parenting -.02 .04 -.07 Maltreatment -.00 .03 -.02 PTSD .03 .03 .19 .08 .05 .16 Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Gender Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .01 Step 3 Gender Social Desirability PTSD .03 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .03 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 96 158 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 4% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 4.89, p < .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained less than one percent additional variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 4% of the variance above the first four variables and the change in R was significant (F [1, 102] = 4.60, p < .05). Callous-unemotional psychopathy significantly predicted anticipated rejection in future parenting with higher scores on callous-unemotional psychopathy related to higher anticipated rejection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 40. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for less than one percent of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [1, 99] = .03, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional 4% of the variance and the change in R approached significance (F [1, 98] = 3.69, p = .06). Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 9% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 5.02, p = .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R approached significance (F [1, 95] = 3.17, p = .08). PTSD significantly predicted anticipated rejection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to higher anticipated rejection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 41. 159 Table 40 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .04 -.16 .07 -.21* -.13 .07 -.17 -.03 .02 -.16 Social Desirability -.14 .08 -.18 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.03 .02 -.17 .00 .02 .02 -.01 .02 -.06 Social Desirability -.12 .07 -.16 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.02 .02 -.12 Maltreatment -.00 .02 -.01 PTSD -.00 .02 -.05 .07 .04 .22* Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .00 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .04 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 160 Table 41 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .00 .01 .09 .02 .06 .10 .07 -.06 .03 -.20 Social Desirability .08 .10 .09 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.03 .04 -.10 .03 .03 .10 .05 .02 .26* Social Desirability .10 .10 .11 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.02 .04 -.08 Maltreatment .02 .03 .09 PTSD .05 .02 .27* .07 .04 .17 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 Step 3 Maltreatment PTSD .09 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .03 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 161 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in anticipated rejection in future parenting (F [2, 94] = 1.14, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for less than one percent additional variance above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 3% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained an additional 2% of the variance above the other variables and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables significantly predicted anticipated rejection in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 42. Positive Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, PTSD Symptoms, and CallousUnemotional Psychopathy as Predictors of Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting Multiple regression analyses were performed to examine predictors of anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Independent variables were positive parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms, and callous-unemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Father’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callous-unemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. 162 Table 42 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Rejection in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.81 .82 -.10 .09 .10 .10 -.69 .84 -.09 .08 .10 .10 -.02 .04 -.07 -.58 .84 -.08 Social Desirability .13 .10 .14 Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 .04 .01 Maltreatment .00 .03 .01 .03 .02 .19 -.71 .84 -.09 Social Desirability .15 .10 .15 Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 .04 .02 -.00 .03 -.00 .04 .02 .21 .07 .05 .16 Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Gender Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 Gender PTSD .03 Step 4 Gender Maltreatment PTSD Callous-Unemotional Trait .02 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 96 163 Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 1.84, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting explained an additional 1% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 2% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were significant predictors of anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 43. For males in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [1, 99] = 3.16, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 18% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 10.62, p < .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. PTSD was found to significantly predict anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to higher anticipated over protection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 44. 164 Table 43 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .02 .14 .10 .13 .16 .10 .15 -.02 .03 -.09 .15 .11 .14 -.04 .03 -.04 .04 -.16 .01 .03 .07 Social Desirability .14 .11 .14 Father’s Positive Parenting -.04 .03 -.16 Maltreatment -.04 .04 -.16 PTSD .01 .03 .07 -.01 .05 -.02 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .01 Step 3 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting -.15 Maltreatment PTSD .02 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 165 Table 44 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 .20 .11 .18 .19 .12 .16 .02 .04 .05 Social Desirability .21 .11 .19 Father’s Positive Parenting .06 .04 .15 Maltreatment .02 .04 .06 .10 .03 .41** Social Desirability .21 .11 .18 Father’s Positive Parenting .06 .04 .15 Maltreatment .02 .04 .06 PTSD .10 .03 .40** -.02 .05 -.04 Step 2 Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .18 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 166 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability explained 2% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [2, 93] = .69, p > .05). Father’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 1% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD explained an additional 9% of the variance above the other three variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 90] = 4.29, p < .05). Callousunemotional psychopathy accounted for another 5% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 89] = 4.97, p < .05). Maltreatment was found to significantly predict anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported maltreatment experiences related to lower anticipated over protection. PTSD also significantly predicted anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to more anticipated overprotection. Callous-unemotional psychopathy was also a significant predictor of anticipated overprotection with higher scores on callousunemotional psychopathy related to more anticipated overprotection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 45. 167 Table 45 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Father’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.53 .87 -.06 .08 .11 .09 -.25 .92 -.03 .12 .11 .12 -.03 .04 -.11 -.34 .90 -.04 .15 .11 .15 Father’s Positive Parenting -.01 .04 -.04 Maltreatment -.05 .03 -.21 .07 .03 .34* Gender -.47 .88 -.06 Social Desirability .18 .10 .18 Father’s Positive Parenting -.01 .04 -.05 Maltreatment -.05 .03 -.24* PTSD .07 .03 .36** .12 .05 .22* Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Gender Social Desirability Father’s Positive Parenting .01 Step 3 Gender Social Desirability PTSD .09 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .05 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 96 168 The same regression analyses were performed with mother’s positive parenting entered in Step 2 in the place of father’s positive parenting. Independent variables were mother’s positive parenting, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms and callousunemotional psychopathy. Social desirability was entered in Step 1. Mother’s positive parenting was entered in Step 2. In Step three, maltreatment and PTSD scores were entered and the callous-unemotional psychopathy score was entered in Step 4. Among females in the public school sample, social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [1, 106] = 1.84, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 1% of the variance above the first two variables and the change in R was not significant. Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained less than one percent additional variance and the change in R was not significant. None of the variables were found to be significant predictors of anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Results of the regression are presented in Table 46. For males, social desirability accounted for 3% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [1, 99] = 3.16, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting explained an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 17% of the variance above the first two variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 96] = 10.22, p < .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy explained an additional less than one percent of the variance and the change in R was not significant. PTSD was found to significantly 169 predict anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to higher anticipated overprotection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 47. Table 46 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Females Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .02 .14 .10 .13 .12 .10 .12 .01 .03 .05 Social Desirability .14 .11 .13 Mother’s Positive Parenting .01 .03 .05 Maltreatment -.02 .03 -.07 .02 .03 .10 Social Desirability .14 .11 .13 Mother’s Positive Parenting .01 .03 .05 Maltreatment -.02 .04 -.08 PTSD .02 .03 .10 .00 .05 .02 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .01 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 107 170 Table 47 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Public School Sample, Males Only ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Social Desirability .03 .20 .11 .18 .20 .12 .18 -.00 .04 -.01 Social Desirability .21 .11 .19 Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 .04 .12 Maltreatment .01 .04 .04 .11 .03 .41** Social Desirability .21 .11 .18 Mother’s Positive Parenting .04 .04 .11 Maltreatment .01 .04 .05 PTSD .11 .03 .41** -.01 .05 -.03 Step 2 Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .00 Step 3 PTSD .17 Step 4 Callous-Unemotional Trait .00 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01; N = 100 171 For the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender was included in Step 1 of the regression. Gender and social desirability accounted for 2% of the variance in anticipated overprotection in future parenting (F [2, 94] = .69, p > .05). Mother’s positive parenting accounted for an additional 2% of the variance and the change in R was not significant. Maltreatment and PTSD accounted for an additional 9% above the other three variables and the change in R was significant (F [2, 91] = 4.91, p = .01). Callous-unemotional psychopathy accounted for an additional 4% of the variance and the change in R was significant (F [1, 90] = 4.17, p < .05). Maltreatment was found to significantly predict anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported maltreatment experiences related to lower anticipated over protection. PTSD also significantly predicted anticipated overprotection in future parenting with more reported symptoms of PTSD related to more anticipated overprotection. Callous-unemotional psychopathy was also a significant predictor of anticipated overprotection with higher scores on callousunemotional psychopathy related to more anticipated overprotection. Results of the regression are presented in Table 48. 172 Table 48 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Mother’s Positive Parenting, Maltreatment Experiences, PTSD Symptoms, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Predicting Anticipated Overprotection in Future Parenting for the Juvenile Detention Center Sample ________________________________________________________________________ Variable ∆R² B SE B β ________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 Gender -.54 .86 -.07 .08 .10 .09 -.22 .89 -.03 .08 .10 .09 -.05 .04 -.15 -.23 .86 -.03 .13 .10 .13 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.06 .04 -.19 Maltreatment -.06 .03 -.28* .06 .02 .31* -.40 .85 -.05 Social Desirability .15 .10 .15 Mother’s Positive Parenting -.05 .04 -.17 Maltreatment -.07 .03 -.30* PTSD .07 .02 .34** .11 .05 .20* Social Desirability .02 Step 2 Gender Social Desirability Mother’s Positive Parenting .02 Step 3 Gender Social Desirability PTSD .09 Step 4 Gender Callous-Unemotional Trait .04 Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01. N = 96 173 The results of the current study offer support to existing research specifically related to the predictor and outcome variables examined. Additionally, these results contribute important knowledge to the processes involved in the intergenerational transmission of parenting styles. An overview of the findings of the current study are presented in Table 49. 174 - - Yes (-) Yes (-) - Callous-unemotional psych PS PS JDC Male female Yes (+) PS male Yes (-) Yes (+) Yes (-) PS female Delinquency JDC 175 Note. Direction of existing relationship is indicated by + or - C-U Psych PTSD Maltreatment Dad + Parenting Mom + Parenting Predictors PS female Yes (+) Yes (-) Yes (-) Yes (-) Yes (+) PS male Ant Warmth JDC Outcome variables Summary of Relationships Found Between Predictor and Outcome Variables Table 49 Yes (+) PS male Yes (+) PS female Ant Reject JDC Yes (+) PS male PS female Yes (+) Yes (+) Yes (-) JDC Ant Overprotect Chapter Five Discussion The purpose of the present study was to examine relationships among perceptions of past parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, PTSD symptoms, delinquency, callous-unemotional psychopathy, and anticipated future parenting. It was anticipated that past parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, and existing symptoms associated with PTSD would predict callous-unemotional psychopathy. Additionally, past parenting experiences, maltreatment experiences, existing symptoms of PTSD, and callous-unemotional psychopathy were expected to predict delinquency as well as anticipated future parenting. The results of the current study supported many of these predictions. Parenting Measures The current study utilized measures of past and future parenting that are currently in development. Past Parenting Experiences. The Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) was utilized in the current study. Overall reliability analyses of the 31 items within the measure revealed strong reliability (Cronbach alpha of .91). Reliability alphas were identical for both samples. The PPPS was developed to contain three major scales of 176 warmth, rejection, and overprotection. Reliability analyses revealed robust alphas (.8 or higher) for each of the scales of warmth, rejection, and overprotection across both samples. However, results of factor analyses did not reveal a distinct three-factor solution. A strong warmth dimension existed with all items loading on one factor within both mother and father domains for both public school and detention center data. Reliability alphas for father’s and mother’s warmth scales for both samples revealed strong reliability alphas (.90 or higher). Reliability analyses of father’s and mother’s rejection scales revealed adequate alphas of .7 or higher for both samples, with some variability in father’s rejection across samples; .8 and .7 for public school and Juvenile Detention Center respectively. Mother’s rejection alpha was .8 for both samples. For the overprotection scale, reliability alphas were also adequate and variable for both father’s and mother’s overprotection between samples, with father’s overprotection alphas of .7 and .9; mother’s overprotection alphas of .6 and .7 for public school and Juvenile Detention Center samples respectively. However, factor analyses revealed the structure of the scales of rejection and overprotection to be less distinct, with items within these scales loading across two factors, the warmth factor and an overprotection/rejection factor for both mother and father domains for both the public school and the detention center data. Because of this, one positive parenting dimension was created for purposes of major analyses by recoding negative items and summing all items into a total positive parenting variable for both father and mother. Anticipated Future Parenting. The Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API) utilized in the current study to measure anticipated future parenting was developed to mirror the three major scales within the PPPS of warmth, rejection, and overprotection. 177 Overall reliability analyses of the 31 items within the API revealed robust and consistent alphas between samples (.8 for both samples). Scale reliability was strong for the anticipated warmth dimension (.8 for both samples) with adequate reliability of .6 for anticipated rejection and anticipated overprotection scales across samples. Results of the factor analyses indicated a distinct warmth dimension with all items loading on one factor for both the public school and the Juvenile Detention Center data. A distinct overprotection scale also existed with most of the items loading on one factor for the data from the public school sample; only two of the eight items within the scale loaded on the “rejection” factor. However, the overprotection scale was not as distinct when analyzed using the Juvenile Detention Center data, with four items within the scale loading on the “overprotection” factor, three items loading on the “warmth” factor and one item on the “rejection” factor. Regarding the rejection scale, five of the nine items within the scale loaded on a distinct factor for the public school sample, with the other four items loading on the warmth factor. For the Juvenile Detention Center data, the rejection scale was less distinct, with four items loading on the rejection factor, three on the overprotection factor, and two on the warmth factor. Overall, these results suggest a strong three-factor structure within the API when the measure is used with a general population. The structure becomes less reliable when used on special populations (e.g., forensic, delinquent). This may be due to differing levels of motivation and/or effort with respect to fully reading each item, attending to content, and answering honestly. Differing interpretations of the content of individual items within each scale across samples may also be reflected in the inconsistent reliability and factor structure of the measures across samples. For instance, overprotection behaviors may be perceived as expressions of 178 warmth by participants in one sample and not the other, as reflected in many of the overprotection items loading on the warmth factor for the Juvenile Detention Center population. For the Juvenile Detention Center participants, any parental involvement may be viewed as positive particularly if negative or uninvolved parenting is more the norm among this population. Differing education and/or reading levels may also contribute to the differences that existed between samples regarding reliability and factor structure of the measures. Sample Comparisons Consistent with expectations, notable differences existed between the public school and Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) samples among the majority of the variables studied. Significant gender differences were also revealed within the public school sample. Due to the small number of females present within the Juvenile Detention Center sample, gender differences could not be examined in this sample. Previous research has demonstrated a higher prevalence of negative parenting experiences among delinquent youth (Caspi et al., 2004; Moris, Meesters, & van den Berg, 2003). The experience of more parental rejection and lack of warmth has been found to be a predictor of conduct problems, antisocial and delinquent behavior (Caspi et al., 2004; Moris et al., 2003; Palmer & Hollin, 1999; Ruchkin et al., 2001) as has the experience of too low or too high overprotection (Chambers et al., 2001). Similar findings regarding parenting experiences were identified in the current study as the public school sample reported significantly more positive parenting experiences from both father and mother overall than the Juvenile Detention Center sample. Public school participants reported experiencing significantly more of both father’s and mother’s 179 warmth; significantly less of both father’s and mother’s rejection; and more of father’s overprotection on average than the JDC participants. The experience of mother’s overprotection was not significantly different between the two groups. The potentially higher prevalence of residing in single parent homes and/or the absence of a father figure in the lives of many of the Juvenile Detention Center participants may be one explanation regarding the lower perceived levels of father’s warmth and overprotection and higher perceived father’s rejection overall among this population. Significant gender differences within the public school sample were not found for father’s or mother’s past parenting variables. JDC participants reported significantly more maltreatment experiences, which is consistent with other research that has found that offenders have experienced more negative and traumatic experiences in comparison to non-offenders, particularly negative parenting, maltreatment and witnessing of violence (Dixon et al., 2004). Behaviors associated with maltreatment, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, neglect, and emotional abuse, are typically linked to parent or caregiver and, thus, these outcomes can be viewed in general as an impact of parenting experiences. Maltreatment has been identified as a significant predictor of both internalizing and externalizing problems overall (Manly et al., 2001). Maltreated individuals have been found to display higher levels of aggression, oppositional, conduct, delinquent and antisocial behaviors (Bensley et al., 1999; Jaffee et al., 2004; Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001). A history of maltreatment has been found to predict aggression and criminal behavior by increasing levels of irritability, impulsivity, hypervigilance, and paranoia as well as affiliation with deviant peers (Kaplan et al., 1999; Knutson et al., 2004). 180 As expected, JDC participants also scored significantly higher on symptoms of PTSD. Higher rates of PTSD have often been found among delinquent populations (Ruchkin et al., 2002). As reported earlier, previous research and results of the current study have found higher rates of maltreatment among delinquent populations. Maltreatment experiences have been found to be related to symptoms of PTSD (Holden, 2003; Mabanglo, 2002; Rodriguez et al., 1998) with symptoms of PTSD being identified as one of the most commonly occurring outcomes of maltreatment experiences (Ackerman et al., 1998; Kendall-Tackett, 2002). Oppositional behaviors, conduct problems, and delinquent behaviors have been consistently found to occur at significantly higher rates among individuals with a history of maltreatment (Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999). Consistent with expectations based on prior research, higher levels of callousunemotional psychopathy were found among JDC participants than for the public school participants. In past research, delinquent adolescents, both males and females, have been found to score significantly higher on traits of psychopathy than non-delinquent populations (Salekin et al., 2004). Psychopathy traits have been found to be highly correlated with oppositional behaviors, conduct problems, aggression, and delinquent and offending behaviors (Gretton et al., 2004; Salekin et al., 2004; Vincent et al., 2003). Specific personality characteristics and emotional functioning associated with psychopathy that lead to antisocial activity and delinquent behavior have been found to be predicted by maltreatment and other traumatic and violent experiences (Ruchkin et al., 2002) and negative parenting experiences (Frick et al., 2003). These experiences have been found to be more predominant among delinquent populations. 181 Previous research has identified a strong intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors (Simons, Bearman, Conger, & Chao, 1992; Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Wu, 1991; Whitbeck et al., 1992). The persistence of parenting behaviors across generations has been viewed to be primarily the result of parental modeling (Serbin & Karp, 2003). Specifically, the modeling of negative parenting behaviors as well as aggressive and antisocial behaviors has been shown to persist and to be carried forth into adulthood and future parenthood. Results of the current study regarding anticipated future parenting support other research regarding the transmission of parenting behaviors across generations. Juvenile Detention Center participants reported significantly higher anticipated rejection and overprotection in future parenting than the public school participants. High levels of both rejection and overprotection in parenting have been found to be related to negative outcomes (Chambers et al., 2001). However, similar anticipated warmth was reported by both samples. The experience of negative parenting and the associated negative feelings related to these experiences may lead to Juvenile Detention Center participants’ desire to express more warmth to their children as future parents because it is something that they may have wanted more of from their parents. Significant gender differences existed regarding anticipated future parenting among public school sample participants. Males were higher in anticipated rejection in their future parenting than females. Females anticipated providing notably more warmth in future parenting in comparison to males. The potential for the societal environment to encourage males to be more masculine or tough and females to display more warmth and to be more accepting may have contributed to these responses. 182 Importance of Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, and PTSD for Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy Previous research has found positive relationships to exist between negative parenting behaviors (Frick et al., 2003; Kim & Rohner, 2003), maltreatment experiences (Shields & Cicchetti, 2001) and psychopathy among both males and females. In the current study, both mother’s and father’s parenting behaviors were found to predict psychopathy, specifically traits of callousness and unemotionality, only among female public school participants. For the females, experiencing of less father’s positive parenting and less mother’s positive parenting was found to predict higher callousunemotional traits, which are associated with psychopathy. Interestingly, none of the variables included in the study were found to play a role in callousness and unemotionality among male public school participants or Juvenile Detention Center participants. These results are unexpected and may be explained by inherent differences that exist between males and females as a result of differing societal sex-role expectations and resulting socialization approaches to the genders. For instance, the general attitude held by society that boys should be “tougher” and girls more vulnerable and emotionally expressive may lead to inherent differences in experiences between the genders. It is possible that some of this socialization of boys may foster traits that may be similar to callousness and unemotionality that may be more stable and less influenced by the predictor variables examined in the current study. Maltreatment experiences may not have been found in the current study to contribute significantly to callous-unemotional psychopathy because these types of experiences appear to have stronger implications for other problematic psychological 183 functioning, primarily internalizing problems, including depression (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Ruchkin et al., 2002; Safren et al., 2002), anxiety (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Ruchkin et al., 2002; Safren et al., 2002), PTSD (Muller et al., 2000; Ruchkin et al., 2002), and suicidal ideation (Johnson et al., 2002; Mabanglo, 2002). Importance of Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, PTSD, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for Delinquency Delinquent behavior, as indicated by self-reported number of arrests, was related to maltreatment experiences among both females and males in the public school sample. Males and females who reported a higher frequency of maltreatment related experiences (e.g., physical abuse, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional abuse) reported a higher number of arrests. These results are consistent with other research that has indicated that a relationship exists between physical abuse (Jaffee et al., 2004), sexual abuse (Bensley et al., 1999), neglect (Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004), and combined maltreatment experiences and delinquent behaviors (Benda & Corwyn, 2002; Kaplan, Pelcovitz, & Labruna, 1999; Knutson, DeGarmo, & Reid, 2004; Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001). Traumatic and violent experiences have been found to be related to specific personality characteristics and emotional functioning, such as lack of both empathy and guilt, that lead to antisocial activity (Ruchkin et al., 2002). For females in the public school sample, PTSD was also found to be related to delinquency, with fewer reported symptoms of PTSD associated with more reported arrests. Previous research has found a relationship between posttraumatic stress and behavior inhibition, particularly high harm avoidance and low self-directedness, and being more fearful, nervous, passive, and low in energy (Ruchkin et al., 2002). Because 184 symptoms of PTSD may serve as behavioral inhibitors, particularly for females, these symptoms may serve as a protective factor against delinquent behavior among females. Mother’s positive parenting was also indicated to play an important role in delinquency among females with experiences of more mother’s positive parenting related to fewer reported arrests. This relationship was no longer significant when the variables of maltreatment and PTSD were considered in addition to mother’s positive parenting, which suggests that maltreatment and PTSD may have introduced unrelated variance into the equation. Interestingly, results of the current study did not find a significant relationship between parenting experiences, specifically warmth, rejection, and overprotection, and delinquency among males, which is inconsistent with previous research that has consistently found a strong relationship to exist between these specific parenting experiences and delinquent behavior among both males and females (Caspi et al., 2004; Chambers et al., 2001; Kim et al., 2003; Knutson et al., 2004; Moris et al., 2003). These results may be explained by the important role of socially desirable responding among males in their reporting their delinquent behaviors. Among the Juvenile Detention Center participants, none of the variables were determined to predict delinquency with the exception of social desirability. Socially desirable responding was associated with fewer reported arrests among the Juvenile Detention Center population. It is likely that participants within the Juvenile Detention Center sample were motivated to respond in a socially desirable manner to the study questions, particularly related to delinquency, as a function of the environment and 185 participants’ perceived implications of honestly reporting the extent of their delinquent behavior. Callous-unemotional psychopathy was not significantly related to delinquency among participants within either sample, which is contradictory to research that has indicated a significant relationship between these two variables (Salekin et al., 2004; Gretton et al., 2004). In past research, negative parenting experiences, particularly low involvement; support; and supervision; and callous-unemotional psychopathy traits have been found to co-exist in children and adolescents with conduct problems (Wootton et al., 1997). Among the Juvenile Detention Center participants, this may be explained by the socially desirable responding style among these participants regarding their delinquent behaviors. Importance of Parenting Experiences, Maltreatment, PTSD, and Callous-Unemotional Psychopathy for Anticipated Future Parenting The current study also explored participants’ perceptions of their anticipated style of future parenting, specifically anticipated warmth, anticipated rejection, and anticipated overprotection in future parenting. As expected, many of the study variables were found to significantly predict anticipated style of future parenting. Previous research has implicated pathways of modeling, learning, and development of specific personality traits to have important implications for future parenting, increasing the transmission of parenting styles and behaviors (Simons et al., 1992; Whitbeck et al., 1992) and maltreatment (Knutson et al., 2004; Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2001) across generations. Research has also found symptoms of PTSD that are associated with maltreatment to predict the perseverance of maltreatment across generations through a compulsive need 186 to reenact the trauma through victimization of others or re-victimization of the self (Green, 1998). Anticipated Warmth. For females in the public school sample, callousunemotional psychopathy significantly predicted anticipated warmth in future parenting, with higher levels of callous-unemotional psychopathy related to lower anticipated warmth in future parenting. This is consistent with the emotional and behavioral functioning associated with callous (lack of empathy, manipulativeness) and unemotional (lack of guilt, emotional constrictedness) traits (Hare, 1998). These results may support callous-unemotional psychopathy as an important pathway in the transmission of parenting behaviors across generations among females. Previous research that has found negative parenting behaviors to contribute to psychopathy among both males and females (Frick et al., 2003; Kim & Rohner, 2003) and other results of the current study implicate both mother’s and father’s parenting behaviors as contributing to callous-unemotional psychopathy among females. Callous-unemotional psychopathy did not play a notable role in anticipated warmth among males. Gender-role expectations could result in more moderate levels among males of behaviors associated with expressions of warmth as reflected in the items within the anticipated warmth scale of the API. Father’s positive parenting was also found to significantly predict anticipated warmth in future parenting among public school females, with more father’s positive parenting experiences related to more anticipated warmth, though this relationship did not continue to be significant after inclusion of maltreatment and PTSD. 187 Among males, mother’s positive parenting was an important predictor of anticipated warmth, with more mother’s positive parenting experiences related to more anticipated warmth in future parenting, though the strength of this relationship was no longer significant after inclusion of callous-unemotional psychopathy. The addition of spurious variance or a non-linear relationship may explain these results. Additionally, among both females and males, a negative relationship existed between maltreatment experiences and anticipated warmth with more maltreatment experiences related to less anticipated warmth in future parenting. These results support previous research that has illustrated the negative impact of maltreatment and its related emotional and behavioral sequelae on future parenting (Knutson et al., 2004; StouthamerLoeber et al., 2001). However, among females, the relationship between maltreatment and anticipated warmth did not continue to be significant after inclusion of callousunemotional psychopathy. Introduction of spurious variance or a non-linear relationship may again be implicated in these results. Regarding the Juvenile Detention Center participants, none of the variables examined were found to be significant predictors of anticipated warmth in future parenting. It may be that the significantly different backgrounds and experiences of the participants within this sample may contribute to differing results between the samples regarding the importance of study variables on outcomes. In the JDC participants, lower reading levels may have compromised comprehension of items. Differing interpretations of items, lower motivation to carefully read and answer items and socially desirable response patterns may also have impacted their responses. Additionally, other variables not examined by the current study may be more important in predicting the outcome 188 variables examined due to the substantial differences, such as familial background and experiences, socioeconomic status, and history of trauma and violence, inherent within the Juvenile Detention Center sample. Anticipated Rejection. For females in the public school sample, as anticipated, callous-unemotional psychopathy was found to be significantly related to anticipated rejection in future parenting with higher levels of callous-unemotional psychopathy related to higher anticipated rejection in future parenting. Rejecting behaviors, as reflected in the items of the API, are consistent with the emotional and behavioral functioning associated with callous (lack of empathy, manipulativeness) and unemotional (lack of guilt, emotional constrictedness) traits (Hare, 1998). These results are similar to other results of this study that suggest callous-unemotional psychopathy as an important pathway in the transmission of parenting behaviors across generations among females. As noted earlier in this section, negative parenting behaviors have been found to predict psychopathy among both males and females (Frick et al., 2003; Kim & Rohner, 2003), which is consistent with other results of the current study that suggest a relationship between both mother’s and father’s parenting behaviors and callous-unemotional psychopathy among females. Among males, PTSD was found to be a significant predictor of anticipated rejection in future parenting with more symptoms of PTSD related to more anticipated rejection. The behavioral inhibition, emotional unavailability, and negativity that has been shown to be associated with symptoms of PTSD (Ruchkin et al., 2002), for males, may contribute to rejecting behaviors in anticipated future parenting. It may be possible that the PTSD symptoms reported by the males in the current study was related to 189 negative parenting experiences or maltreatment, which would demonstrate symptoms of PTSD as a pathway to intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors among males. For the Juvenile Detention Center participants, none of the variables investigated were found to significantly predict anticipated rejection in future parenting. Again, as noted elsewhere in this section, numerous factors may have contributed to this outcome. Anticipated Overprotection. PTSD was found to be a significant predictor of anticipated overprotection in future parenting among males in the public school sample with more symptoms of PTSD related to more anticipated overprotection. This finding is similar to other findings of this study that suggest a relationship between PTSD and anticipated future parenting among males. Behaviors associated with overprotection, as reflected within those items on the API, may be predicted by symptoms of PTSD. For instance, the high harm avoidance and being more fearful and nervous (Ruchkin et al., 2002), for males, may contribute to overprotecting behaviors in anticipated future parenting. Negative parenting experiences including maltreatment may be related to the symptoms of PTSD noted among males in the current study, and it may therefore be speculated that symptoms of PTSD may play an important role in the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors among males. Among females in the public school sample, none of the variables investigated were found to be significant predictors of anticipated overprotection in future parenting. This finding is unexpected based on prior research that has consistently implicated parenting experiences (Serbin & Karp, 2003; Simons et al., 1992; Whitbeck et al., 1992), maltreatment experiences (Cunningham, 2003; DeBellis, 2001; Locke & Newcomb, 190 2004) and symptoms of PTSD (Green, 1998) to predict anticipated future parenting style. Among females without severe conduct problems, behaviors associated with overprotection, as reflected in the items within the API, may be more stable and consistent with the level of involvement that may be perceived to be appropriate for motherhood, thus making this variable less influenced by the predictor variables examined in this study. Among the Juvenile Detention Center participants, maltreatment was a significant predictor of anticipated overprotection in future parenting, with more maltreatment experiences related to less anticipated overprotection. Maltreatment experiences appear to contribute to less anticipated involvement among this population as reflected by the negative relationship that existed between maltreatment experiences and overprotection. Other results of this study reflected the experience of significantly more negative parenting experiences and maltreatment among the Juvenile Detention Center participants. In addition to other negative parenting behaviors, the experience of too low or too high parental overprotection has been found to be related to increased conduct problems, antisocial and delinquent behavior (Chambers et al., 2001). In the current study, the JDC participants were found to experience significantly less overprotection from their fathers, which may be important for future parenting among this population. As anticipated, PTSD symptomatology was found to significantly predict anticipated overprotection among Juvenile Detention Center participants, with more symptoms of PTSD related to more anticipated overprotection in future parenting. Behaviors associated with overprotection, as reflected within those items on the API, may be predicted by symptoms of PTSD among these participants. The majority of the JDC 191 sample were males. The PTSD symptoms of high harm avoidance and being more fearful and nervous may contribute to overprotecting behaviors in anticipated future parenting particularly among males, as the results of this study have found PTSD to be related to anticipated overprotection among the males in both samples. Negative parenting experiences including maltreatment may be related to the symptoms of PTSD noted among males in the current study, suggesting that symptoms of PTSD may play an important role in the intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors among males. Callous-unemotional psychopathy also significantly predicted anticipated overprotection among Juvenile Detention Center participants with higher trait levels of callous-unemotional psychopathy predicting more anticipated overprotection in future parenting. These results are counterintuitive to expectations that high levels of callousunemotional traits would predict lack of overprotection due to lack of empathy and care and concern. Bivariate correlational analysis of the items within the anticipated overprotection scale and the items of the YPI callous-unemotional measure was performed to further explore this outcome. No correlations were found among items that would explain the relationship found between callous-unemotional psychopathy and overprotection among JDC participants. Experiment-wise error could explain these findings. Study Limitations The current study has several important limitations. The measures of past and future parenting, the PPPS and the API respectively, remain in development. Though psychometrics are fair to adequate for both measures, the use of measures that are in their infancy and still undergoing development may have implications for the current findings 192 and the extent to which conclusions can be drawn. The reliability and validity of measures that are early in their development do not have a solid body of research to support the accuracy of the measure in assessing the constructs targeted. Future research could further develop these measures through further examination of the appropriateness of current items and through inclusion of more items within each dimension. Aspects of the implementation of the study, particularly the number of measures administered to participants, should be noted as having a potentially detrimental effect on results. Eleven measures were administered to participants in one packet. The considerable number of questions and amount of time and effort required of each participant may have had a negative impact on motivation and amount of effort expended over the course of completion of the packet of questions. The order that the measures were placed in the packet was varied so as to minimize the impact of decreased effort on one study over the other. It happened that some participants were not able to complete all questions due to time constraints and not enough time to answer all of the numerous questions contained in the packet. This would have implications for the results of the current study because of the possible outcomes of inaccurate responding associated with decreasing effort, motivation, and concentration. Important information may not have been collected from those who were not able to complete all items or findings could have been compromised by response patterns that were not accurate due to the effects of effort, motivation, and concentration levels that were less than optimal. The comprehensive design of the current study is not without its influence on the limitations of the results. The number of predictor variables and outcome variables investigated made the interpretation of results complex. The number of predictor 193 variables included in the multiple regression analyses may have introduced spurious variance or resulted in nonlinear relationships, thus obscuring potentially important results. The notable sample differences that existed among the two populations sampled may have important implications for results. Differing familial environment and experiences, particularly more negative familial experiences overall as well as having numerous caregivers, likely influenced the lack of findings by the current study for the Juvenile Detention Center population. The lower educational level on average among the Juvenile Detention Center participants has important implications for reading levels and comprehension skills of those within this sample. JDC participants’ overall lower ability to read and comprehend items within the study questionnaires should be considered in interpretation of the results for this sample. The detention center environment and current situation of these participants likely may have had some important influence on results. For instance, fears of perceived potential consequences of honest reporting of delinquent behaviors may have resulted in a minimizing of actual legal involvement. Additionally, the increased prevalence of significant and likely untreated mental health related issues among delinquent populations may have consequences for motivation, effort, attention and concentration. Clinical Implications The results of the current research suggest that there are important relationships between past parenting, psychopathy, delinquency and anticipated future parenting. These findings can inform the focus of clinical attention to specific areas that will be more likely to have the most impact on preventing negative outcomes. 194 Negative parenting experiences (Caspi et al., 2004; Moris, Meesters, & van den Berg, 2003), maltreatment (Dixon et al., 2004), PTSD (Ruchkin et al., 2002), and callousunemotional psychopathy (Salekin et al., 2004) have been found to be more prevalent among delinquent populations. This was supported by the results of the current study, further demonstrating a relationship between these variables and delinquent outcomes. These results illustrate the importance of inclusion of interventions that address these variables as part of delinquency prevention and intervention. The important role of maltreatment in predicting delinquent behavior among both males and females was demonstrated by results of the current research. Additional results of the current research suggest that positive parenting experiences, particularly mother’s positive parenting for females, could be a protective factor in reducing delinquent behavior. These results amplify the importance of developing and implementing strategies to prevent maltreatment as well as to treat those who have endured maltreatment experiences. Because positive parenting experience may serve a protective function, strategies that foster positive parenting will also promote more positive outcomes. Support was found for the likely intergenerational transmission of parenting behaviors. Future parenting that was significantly more negative was found by the current research to be anticipated by those who demonstrated higher levels of each of the variables of negative parenting experiences, maltreatment, PTSD, and callousunemotional psychopathy. These results support the likely important role of these variables in the intergenerational transmission of negative parenting behaviors and maltreatment. For instance, the development of callous-unemotional psychopathy, traits 195 which have been found to be related to numerous negative outcomes, was found by the current study to be related to negative parenting experiences, particularly among females. Improving parenting behaviors could have a significant and protective impact, particularly among females, in overall psychological well-being and adjustment by possibly hindering the development of callous-unemotional traits. Reducing the prevalence of callous-unemotional psychopathy traits reduces the propensity for the negative outcomes associated with these traits. Because negative parenting may foster the development of callous-unemotional traits, focusing on supporting more positive approaches to parenting will be one important pathway to address in attempts to reduce the development of these traits. Additionally, callous-unemotional psychopathy may have a predictive role in anticipated future parenting styles that are more negative, specifically less anticipated warmth and more anticipated rejection in future parenting. Negative parenting by itself has been shown to be transferred across generations. Results of the current research suggest that the experience of negative parenting may possibly gather strength in its transmission through affecting the development of traits of callousunemotional psychopathy, traits which may negatively influence future parenting. Experience of father’s positive parenting among females and the experience of mother’s positive parenting among males may have important protective factors in supporting the transmission of positive parenting, specifically more anticipated warmth in future parenting as reflected in results of the current study. Maltreatment’s significant relationship to intergenerational transmission of negative parenting among both males and females further confirms the importance of addressing the issue of maltreatment in society, particularly because of the numerous negative sequelae associated with 196 experiences of maltreatment that compound the impact of these experiences. For example, among males, the presence of symptoms of PTSD, which are strongly associated with maltreatment, were found to predict anticipated negative parenting, specifically anticipated rejection and overprotection in future parenting. In summary, a strong and continuously growing evidence base exists to support the important influence of parenting on a wide range of outcomes. Altering the negative cycle of parenting through addressing the specific variables that are consistently found to predict negative parenting styles and the transmission of these forward into future parenting will have a positive impact on outcomes for future generations. 197 References Ackerman, P., Newton, J., McPherson, W., Jones, J., & Dykman, R. (1998). Prevalence of post traumatic stress disorder and other psychiatric diagnoses in three groups of abused children (sexual, physical, and both). Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 759774. Alessandri, S. & Lewis, M. (1993). Parental evaluation and its relation to shame and pride in young children. Sex Roles, 29(5), 335-343. Allaman, J., Joyce, C., & Crandall, V. (1972). The antecedents of social desirability response tendencies of children and young adults. Child Development, 43, 1135-1160. Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in nonreferred youths: A new assessment tool. In E. Blaauw & L. Sheridan (Eds.), Psychopaths: Current International Perspectives (pp. 131-158). The Hague: Elsevier. Arrindell, W., Emmelkamp, P., Monsma, A., & Brilman, E. (1983). The role of perceived parental rearing practices in the aetiology of phobic disorders: a controlled study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 183-187. Arrindell, W., Kwee, M., Methorst, G., van der Ende, J., Pol, E., & Moritz, B. (1989). Perceived parental rearing styles of agoraphobic and socially phobic in-patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 526-535. 198 Arrindell, W. A., Sanavio, E., Aguilar, G., Sica, C., Hatzichristou, C., Eisemann, M., Recinos, L. A., Gaszner, P., Peter, M., Battagliese, G., Kallai, J., & van der Ende, J. (1999). The development of a short form of the EMBU: its appraisal with students in Greece, Guatemala, Hungary, and Italy. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 613-628. Bao, W., Haas, A., & Pi, Y. (2004). Life-strain, negative emotions, and delinquency: an empirical test of general strain theory in the people’s republic of china. International Journal of Offender Therapy & Comparative Criminology, 48, 281297. Beers, S. & DeBellis, M. (2002). Neuropsychological function in children with maltreatment related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 483-486. Benda, B., & Corwyn, R. (2002). The effect of abuse in childhood and in adolescence on violence among adolescents. Youth and Society, 33, 339-365. Benjaminsen, S., Jorgensen, J., Kragh-Hansen, L., & Pedersen, L. (1984). Memories of parental rearing practices and personality features. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 69, 426-434. Bensley, L., Van Eenwyk, J., Spieker, S., & Schoder, J. (1999). Self-reported abuse history and adolescent problem behaviors: antisocial and suicidal behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 24, 163-172. Bernstein, D., Stein, J., Newcomb, M., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169-190. 199 Bogels, S., van Oosten, A., Muris, P., & Smulders, D. (2001). Familial correlates of social anxiety in children and adolescents. Behavior Research and Therapy, 39, 273-287. Bolger, K., Patterson, C., & Kupersmidt, J. (1998). Peer relationships and self-esteem among children who have been maltreated. Child Development, 69, 1171-1197. Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R., & Lavoie, F. (2001). Reactive and proactive aggression: predictions to physical violence in different contexts and moderating effects of parental monitoring and caregiving behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(4), 293-304. Carifio, J. (1994). Sensitive Data and Students’ Tendencies to Give Socially Desirable Responses. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 39, 74-84. Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Arseneault, L., Tully, L., Jacobs, C., Kim-Cohen, J., & Polo-Tomas, M. (2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts chidren’s antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotictwin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Developmental Psychology, 40, 149-161. Chambers, J., Power, K., Loucks, N., & Swanson, V. (2001). The interaction of perceived maternal and paternal parenting styles and their relation with the psychological distress and offending characteristics of incarcerated young offenders. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 209-227. Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 598-606. 200 Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2004). Perceived parental rearing style, self-esteem and selfcriticism as predictors of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 1-21. Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. (2001). The impact of child maltreatment and psychopathology on neuroendocrine functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 783-804. Cruise, K., Colwell, L, Lyons, P., & Baker, M. (2003). Prototypical analysis of adolescent psychopathy: investigating the juvenile justice perspective. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 21, 829-846. Cunningham, S. (2003). The joint contribution of experiencing and witnessing violence during childhood on child abuse in the parent role. Violence and Victims, 18, 619-639. DeBellis, M. (2001). Developmental traumatology: the psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 539-564. Dixon, A., Howie, P., & Starling, J. (2004). Psychopathology in female juvenile offenders. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45, 1150-1158. Edens, J., Skeem, J., Cruise, K., & Cauffman, E. (2001). The assessment of juvenile psychopathy and its association with violence: a critical review. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 19, 53-80. Edwards, V., Holden, G., Felitti, V., & Anda, R. (2003). Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: results from the adverse childhood experiences study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1453-1460. 201 Flisher, A., Kramer, R., Hoven, C., Greenwald, S., Alegria, M., Bird, H., Canino, G., Connell, R., & Moore, R. (1997). Psychosocial characteristics of physically abused children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 123-132. Frick, P., Kimonis, E., Dandreaux, D., & Farell, J. (2003). The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 21, 713-736. Glaser, D. (2000). Child abuse and neglect and the brain – a review. Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 97-116. Goldstein, N., Arnold, D., Weil, J., Mesiarik, C., Peuschold, D., Grisso, T., & Osman, D. (2003). Comorbid symptom patterns in female juvenile offenders. International Journal of Law & Psychiatry, 26, 565-582. Green, A. (1998). Factors contributing to the generational transmission of child maltreatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 1334-1336. Gretton, H., Hare, R., & Catchpole, R. (2004). Psychopathy and offending from adolescence to adulthood: a 10 year follow-up. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 72, 636-645. Hare, R. (1998). Psychopaths and their nature: implications for the mental health and criminal justice systems. In T. Millon, E. Simonson, M. Burket-smith, & R. Davis (Eds.) Psychopathy: antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 188212). New york: Guilford press. 202 Harrison, P., Fulkerson, J., & Beebe, T. (1997). Multiple substance use among adolescent physical and sexual abuse victims. Child Abuse and Neglect, 21, 529539. Higgins, D. & McCabe, M. (2003). Maltreatment and family dysfunction in childhood and the subsequent adjustment of children and adults. Journal of Family Violence, 18, 107-120. Holden, G. (2003). Children exposed to domestic violence and child abuse: terminology and taxonomy. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 151-160. Holmes, S. & Robins, L. (1987). The influence of childhood disciplinary experience on the development of alcoholism and depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(3), 399-415. Hunter, J., Figueredo, A., Malamuth, N., & Becker, J. (2004). Developmental pathways in youth sexual aggression and delinquency: risk factors and mediators. Journal of Family Violence, 19, 233-242. Jaffee, S., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 44-55. Johnson, J., Cohen, P., Gould, M., Kasen, S., Brown, J., & Brook, J. (2002). Childhood adversities, interpersonal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts during late adolescence and early adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 741-749. 203 Kaplan, S., Pelcovitz, D., & Labruna, V. (1999). Child and adolescent abuse and neglect research: a review of the past 10 years. Part I: physical and emotional abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1214-1222, Kendall-Tackett, K. (2002). The health effects of childhood abuse: four pathways by which abuse can influence health. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 715-729. Kerver, M., van Son, M., & de Groot, P. (1992). Predicting symptoms of depression from reports of early parenting: a one-year prospective study in a community sample. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 86, 267-272. Kim, I., Ge, X., Brody, G., Conger, R., Gibbons, F., & Simons R. (2003). Parenting behaviors and the occurrence and co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and conduct problems among african American children. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 571-583. Kim, S., & Rohner, R. (2003). Perceived parental acceptance and emotional empathy among university students in korea. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 34, 723-735. Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2004). Genetic and environmental processes in young children’s resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child Development, 75, 651-668. Kitahara, M. (1987). Perception of parental acceptance and rejection among Swedish university students. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 223-227. 204 Knutson, J., DeGarmo, D., & Reid, J. (2004). Social disadvantage and neglectful parenting as precursors to the development of antisocial and aggressive child behavior: testing a theoretical model. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 187-205. Koestner, R., Zuroff, D., & Powers, T. (1991). Family origins of adolescent selfcriticism and its continuity into adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(2), 191-197. Kokkevi, A. & Costas, S. (1988). Parental rearing patterns and drug abuse. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 78(344 suppl), 151-157. Lau, A., & Weisz, J. (2003). Reported maltreatment among clinic-referred children: implications for presenting problems, treatment attrition, and long-term outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 13271334. Leung, C., McBride-Chang, C., & Lai, B. (2004). Relations among maternal parenting style, academic competence, and life satisfaction in chinese early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 113-143. Libby, A., Orton, H., Novins, D., Spicer, P., Buchwald, D., Beals, J., & Manson, S. (2004). Childhood physical and sexual abuse and subsequent alcohol and drug use disorders in two American Indian tribes. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65, 74-83. Locke, T., & Newcomb, M. (2004). Child maltreatment, parent alcohol and drug related problems, polydrug problems, and parenting practices: a test of gender differences and four theoretical perspectives. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 120-134. 205 Lumley, M., Mader, C., Gramzow, J., & Papineau, K. (1996). Family factors related to alexithymia characteristics. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 211-216. Mabanglo, M. (2002). Trauma and the effects of violence exposure and abuse on children: a review of the literature. Smith Studies in Social Work, 72, 232-251. Magnus, K., Cowen, E., Wyman, P., Fagen, D., & Work, W. (1999). Parent-child relationship qualities and child adjustment in highly stressed urban black and white families. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 55-71. Manly, J., Kim, J., Rogosch, F., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 759-782. Maughan, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children’s emotional regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 1525-1542. McCreadie, R., Williamson, D., Athawes, R., Connolly, M., & TilakSingh, D. (1994). The Nithsdale Schizophrenia Surveys XIII. Parental rearing patterns, current symptomatology and relatives’ expressed emotion. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 347-352. McLean, L., & Gallop, R. (2003). Implications of childhood sexual abuse for adult borderline personality disorder and complex posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 369-371. 206 McMahon, J., & Clay-Warner, J. (2002). Child abuse and future criminality, the role of social service placement, family disorganization, and gender. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 1002-1019. Meesters, C. & Muris, P. (1996). The relationship between hostility and perceived parental rearing behaviours: a study of male myocardial infarction patients and healthy controls. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(2), 271-281. Meesters, C., Muris, P., & Esselink, T. (1995). Hostility and perceived parental rearing behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(4), 567-570. Moris, P, Meesters, C., & van den Berg, S. (2003). Internalizing and externalizing problems as correlates or self-reported attachment style and perceived parental rearing in normal adolescents. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 12, 171-183. Muller, R., Geobel-Fabbri, A., Diamond, T., & Dinklage, D. (2000). Social support and the relationship between family and community violence exposure and psychopathology among high risk adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 449464. Muris, P., Bogels, S., Meesters, C., van der Kamp, N., & van Oosten, A. (1996). Parental rearing practices, fearfulness, and problem behaviour in clinically referred children. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(5), 813-818. O’Connor, B., & Dvorak, T. (2001). Conditional associations between parental behavior and adolescent problems: a search for personality-environment interactions. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 1-26. 207 Palmer, E., & Hollin, C. (1996). Sociomoral reasoning, perceptions of own parenting and self-reported delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(2), 175-182. Palmer, E., & Hollin, C. (1997). The influence of perceptions of own parenting on sociomoral reasoning, attributions for criminal behaviour, and self-reported delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(2), 193-197. Palmer, E., & Hollin, C. (1999). An evaluation of the shortened EMBU scale in young offenders and non-offenders in England. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 171-179. Parish, T., & McCluskey, J. (1992). The relationship between parenting styles and young adults’ self-concepts and evaluations of parents. Adolescence, 27(108), 915-918. Pasold, T. (2003). Perceptions of Past Parenting, Emotional Intelligence, and Intentions for Future Parenting. Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio. Peretti, P., Clark, D., & Johnson, P. (1985). Affect of parental rejection on negative attention-seeking classroom behaviors. Education, 104(3), 313-317. Perrone, D., Sullivan, C., Pratt, T., & Margaryan, S. (2004). Parental efficacy, selfcontrol, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. International Journal of Offender Therapy & Comparative Criminology, 48, 298-312. 208 Pittman, L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. (2001). African American adolescent girls in impoverished communities: parenting style and adolescent outcomes. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11(2), 199-224. Pynoos, R., Rodriquez, N., Steinberg, A., Stuber, M., & Frederick, C. (1998). UCLA PTSD Index for DSM IV, Child Version, Revision 1. California: UCLA Trauma Psychiatry Service. Rey, J., & Plapp, J. (1990). Quality of perceived parenting in oppositional and conduct disordered adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(3), 382-385. Rodriguez, N., Vande Kemp, H., & Foy, D. (1998). Posttraumatic stress disorder in survivors of childhood sexual and physical abuse: a critical review of the empirical research. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 7, 17-44. Rohner, R. (1986). The Warmth Dimension. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Ross, M., Clayer, J., & Campbell, R. (1983). Parental rearing patterns and suicidal thoughts. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 429-433. Ruchkin, V., Eisemann, M., & Hagglof, B. (1998). Parental rearing and problem behaviours in male delinquent adolescents versus controls in northern Russia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 477-482. Ruchkin, V., Eisemann, M., & Hagglof, B. (1999). Coping styles in delinquent adolescents and controls: the role of personality and parental rearing. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28(6), 705-717. 209 Ruchkin, V., Koposov, R., Eisemann, M., & Hagglof, B. (2001). Conduct problems in Russian adolescents: the role of personality and parental rearing. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 19-27. Ruchkin, V., Schwab-Stone, M., Koposov, R., Vermeiren, R., & Steiner, H. (2002). Violence exposure, posttraumatic stress, and personality in juvenile delinquents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 322329. Safren, S., Gershuny, B., Marzoi, P., Otto, M., & Pollack, M. (2002). History of childhood abuse in panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 453-456. Salekin, R., Leistico, A., Neumann, C., DiCicco, T., & Duros, R. (2004). Psychopathy and comorbidity in a young offender sample: taking a close look at psychopathy’s potential importance over disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 416-427. Salzinger, S., Feldman, R., Ng-Mak, D., Mojica, E., & Stockhammer, T. (2001). The effect of physical abuse on children’s social and affective status: a model of cognitive and behavioral processes explaining the association. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 805-825. Serbin, L., & Karp, J. (2003). Intergenerational studies of parenting and the transfer of risk from parent to child. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 138142. 210 Shaw, B., Krause, N., Chatters, L., Connell, C., & Ingersoll-Dayton, B. (2004). Emotional support from parents early in life, aging, and health. Psychology and Aging, 19, 4-12. Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion dysregulation as risk factors for bullying and victimization in middle childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 349-363. Simons, R., Bearman, J., Conger, R., & Chao, W. (1992). Gender differences in the intergenerational transmission of parenting beliefs. Journal of Marriage and Family, 54(4), 823-836. Simons, R., Robertson, J., & Downs, W. (1989). The nature of the association between parental rejection and delinquent behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18(3), 297-310. Simons, R., Whitbeck, L., Conger, R., & Wu, C. (1991). Intergenerational transmission of harsh parenting. Developmental Psychology, 27, 159-171. Sohn, B. (2003). Are young people in correctional institutions different from community students who have never been convicted?: differences in internalizing and externalizing behaviors. British Journal of Social Work, 33, 739-752. Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., Homish, D., & Wei, E. (2001). Maltreatment of boys and the development of disruptive and delinquent behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 941-955. 211 Swanston, H., Parkinson, P., O’Toole, B., Plunkett, A., Shrimpton, S., & Oates, R. (2003). Juvenile crime, aggression and delinquency after sexual abuse: a longitudinal study. British Journal of Criminology, 43, 729-749. Teichman, M., & Kefir, E. (2000). The effects of perceived parental behaviors, attitudes, and substance-use on adolescent attitudes toward and intent to use psychoactive substances. Journal of Drug Education, 30(2), 193-204. Tousignant, M., Bastien, M., & Hamel, S. (1993). Suicidal attempts and ideations among adolescents and young adults: the contribution of the father’s and mother’s care and of parental separation. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 28, 256-261. Van IJzendoorn, M. (1992). Intergenerational Transmission of Parenting: A Review of Studies in Nonclinical Populations. Developmental Review, 12, 76-99. Vincent, G., Vitacco, M., Grisso, T., & Corrado, R. (2003). Subtypes of adolescent offenders: affective traits and antisocial behavior patterns. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 21, 695-712. Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. (2000). Influence of deviant friends on delinquency: searching for moderator variables. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(4), 313-325. Vrasti, R., Eisemann, M., & Bucur, M. (1993). Discriminative features of alcoholics classified according to family history. Journal of Substance Abuse,5, 145-155. Wagner, B., & Cohen, P. (1994). Adolescent sibling differences in suicidal symptoms: the role of parent-child relationships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22(3), 321-337. 212 Watson, P., Little, T., & Biderman, M. (1992). Narcissism and parenting styles. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 9, 231-244. Whitbeck, L., Hoyt, D., Simons, R., Conger, R., Elder, jr., G., Lorenz, F., & Huck, S. (1992). Intergenerational continuity of parental rejection and depressed affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 1036-1045. Winefield, H., Goldney, R., Tiggemann, M., & Winefield, A. (1989). Reported parental rearing patterns and psychological adjustment: a short form of the EMBU. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(4), 459-465. Wolfradt, U., Hempel, S., & Miles, J. (2003). Perceived parenting styles, depersonalization, anxiety and coping behavior in adolescents. Personality & Individual Differences, 34, 521-532. Wootton, J., Frick, P., Shelton, K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997). Ineffective Parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 301-308. Xia, G., & Qian, M. (2001). The relationship of parenting style to self-reported mental health among two subcultures of Chinese. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 251-260. Zemore, R., & Rinholm, J. (1989). Vulnerability to depression as a function of parental rejection and control. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21(4), 364-376. 213 APPENDICES 214 Appendix A DEMOGRAPHIC INFORMATION Please circle the letter or fill in the blank with information about yourself GENDER: A) Female AGE:__________ B) Male GRADE IN SCHOOL: ____________ ETHNICITY: A) White/European American B) Black/African American C) Hispanic American D) Asian American E) Biracial (please describe:__________) F) Other (please describe:___________) MOTHER/FEMALE GUARDIAN’S JOB: ___________________________________ MOTHER/FEMALE GUARDIAN’S EDUCATION LEVEL: A) Some high school B) Graduated from high school C) Some college D) Graduated from college E) Graduated with a post-graduate degree (Masters, Ph.D., M.D.) F) Don’t know/don’t have FATHER/MALE GUARDIAN’S JOB: ______________________________________ FATHER/MALE GUARDIAN’S EDUCATION LEVEL: A) Some high school B) Graduated from high school C) Some college D) Graduated from college E) Graduated with a post-graduate degree (Masters, Ph.D., M.D.) F) Don’t know/don’t have Have you ever been in trouble with the law or involved in juvenile court? YES Number of times in juvenile detention center:____________ Number of times you have moved:_______________ 215 NO Appendix B Lucas County Juvenile Detention Center: Informed Consent for Research Project Topic: Exploring Adolescents’ Parenting Experiences, Emotions, Behaviors, and Exposure to Violence Student Investigators: Jennifer Gunderson, M.A., Tracie L. Pasold, M.A. Faculty Advisor: Jeanne Funk, Ph.D. Affiliation: University of Toledo Psychology Department I, Judge James Ray of the Lucas County Juvenile Detention Center, agree to allow adolescents who give their consent to participate in projects examining the effects of media violence and parenting experiences on delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents, to be conducted by Jennifer Gunderson and Tracie L. Pasold, doctoral graduate students from the University of Toledo. I understand that participating adolescents will be asked to complete several questionnaires measuring exposure to violence (in the home environment, community, and through the media), parenting experiences, psychopathology, hostility, aggression, empathy, and socially desirable responding. This should take adolescents approximately 60 minutes total. Participant responses will remain anonymous and all information will be kept strictly confidential. I am aware of the risks involved in this study, specifically that some adolescents might become distressed while completing the questionnaires. I am also of aware of study benefits, including increasing our knowledge of the effects of violence. ___________________________________ Signature 216 __________________ Date of Signature Appendix C Public Schools: Informed Consent for Research Project Topics: Exploring Adolescents’ Parenting Experiences, Emotions, Behaviors, and Exposure to Violence Student Investigators: Jennifer Gunderson, M.A.; Tracie L. Pasold, M.A. Faculty Advisor: Jeanne Funk, Ph.D. Affiliation: University of Toledo Psychology Department ________________________________ school agrees to allow students who have parental consent and who give their consent themselves to participate in projects examining the effects of media violence and parenting experiences on delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents, to be conducted by Jennifer Gunderson and Tracie L. Pasold, Doctoral graduate students from the University of Toledo. I understand that participating students will be asked to complete several questionnaires measuring parenting experiences, emotions, behaviors, and exposure to violence. This should take students approximately 60 minutes total. Student responses will remain anonymous and all information will be kept strictly confidential. ___________________________________ Authorized Signature 217 __________________ Date of Signature Appendix D Parents… We need your help! You can help us understand more about adolescent experiences. A study will be conducted at your child’s school under the supervision of Dr. Jeanne Funk from the University of Toledo. We hope that all students will participate. Students who have permission and agree to participate will be asked to provide basic demographic information and will complete several questionnaires measuring parenting experiences, emotions, behaviors, and exposure to violence. This session, which will take place at your child’s school during or immediately following regular hours of attendance, will last about 60 minutes. All answers will be confidential and your child’s name will never be used in any research report. Participants will be entered into a drawing for a $25.00 Westfield Gift Card, which can be used at any Westfield location, such as the Franklin Park Mall. If your child wishes to stop participating, she or he may do so at any time, and still have a chance to win the gift card. There may be some minor risks associated with this study. It is possible that your child may feel anxious when asked to share negative experiences. If this does occur, students will be encouraged to speak with the school counselor or psychologist. The benefit of this project is that we may learn more about experiences of today’s youth, and how these may relate to hostility, aggression, empathy, and other emotional/personality functioning. This information may help us develop more effective prevention and treatment programs. If you choose not to participate, this will not affect you or your child’s relationship with the school or with the University of Toledo. If you permit your child to participate in this study, please sign below and ask your child to return it to her/his teacher. CONSENT: My son/daughter may participate in this study. ________________________________ Name of student (please print) ________________________________ Name of parent/guardian (please print) ________________________________ Signature of parent ________________________________ Date of signature I am interested in receiving a summary of the study results: yes no (circle one) Address to send results:____________________________________________________ * For more information, contact Tracie Pasold or Jen Gunderson at 419-530-2721. 218 Appendix E JDC Participants: Informed Consent for Research Student Investigators: Jennifer Gunderson, M.A.; Tracie L. Pasold, M.A. Faculty Advisor: Jeanne Funk, Ph.D. Affiliation: University of Toledo Psychology Department You are asked to participate in a study that looks at how your experiences at home and through the media (for example watching TV and movies) might be related to your feelings and behavior. If you agree to participate in this study, you will be asked to write down basic information about yourself and to complete several different questionnaires. There are no right or wrong answers, so please be honest on all questionnaires. These usually take 60 minutes to complete. You will not be writing your name on any of the questionnaires so all of your answers will be anonymous and confidential – this means no one will ever know how you answered the questions. We would like you to participate in this study, but you are free to say no or to stop working on questionnaires at any time. You do not have to answer questions that make you feel upset. You will not be punished if you decide you do not want to be in the study. There is a risk that you might feel upset when reading some of the questions (for example, about your feelings or experiences of violence). The benefit of this study is that you are helping us to learn more about teens’ experiences, feelings, and behaviors. To thank you for participating in this study, you will receive a piece of candy. If you decide to stop during the study, you can still choose a piece of candy. If you have any questions, please ask them now. If you would like to talk to someone before, during, or after the study, please ask a staff member to call the Rescue Mental Health Office (Robin Jackson, 419-255-9585). By signing below, you agree to participate in this study. _______________________________ Signature _____________ Date 219 Appendix F Public School Students: Informed Consent for Research Student Investigators: Jennifer Gunderson, M.A.; Tracie L. Pasold, M.A. Faculty Advisor: Jeanne Funk, Ph.D. Affiliation: University of Toledo Psychology Department You are asked to participate in a study that looks at how your experiences at home and through the media (for example, watching TV and movies) might be related to your feelings and behavior. If you agree to participate in this study, you will be asked to write down basic information about yourself and to complete several different questionnaires. There are no right or wrong answers, so please be honest on all questionnaires. These usually take 60 minutes to complete. You will not be writing your name on any of the questionnaires so all of your answers will be anonymous and confidential – this means no one will ever know how you answered the questions. We would like you to participate in this study, but you are free to say no or to stop working on questionnaires at any time. You do not have to answer questions that make you feel upset. You will not be punished if you decide you do not want to be in the study. There is a risk that you might feel upset when reading some of the questions (for example, about your feelings or experiences of violence). The benefit of this study is that you are helping us to learn more about teens’ experiences, feelings, and behaviors. To thank you for participating in this study, your name will be entered into a drawing to win a 25.00 Westfield gift card, which can be used at Franklin Park Mall. If you decide to stop during the study, you will still have a chance to win the gift card. If you have any questions, please ask them now. By signing below, you agree to participate in this study. _______________________________ Signature _____________ Date 220 Appendix G Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API) – Revised, Dissertation Version Each of us has certain ideas about what principles we anticipate we will use in parenting our children. Below are a number of questions concerning your anticipated style of future parenting. When filling out this questionnaire, it is essential that you try to think about how YOU would behave as a parent. For each question please circle the answer choice that most closely represents your own beliefs or perceptions about how you think you will behave towards your child(ren). Be careful not to leave any questions unanswered. 1. I will spend one-on-one time with my child(ren). No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 2. I will require my child(ren) to check in regularly when out. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 3. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she is not good enough. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 4. I will reward my child with money or material items. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 5. If my child(ren) does something wrong, I will ground him/her. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 6. I will try to instill fear in my child(ren) so that he/she will be afraid to do things. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 7. I will compare my child to siblings or other children. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 221 8. I will attend my child(ren)’s sporting events and other extracurricular activities. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 9. I will probably favor one child over another. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 10. I will insist on meeting and approving all of my child(ren)’s friends. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 11. It will be hard for me to let my child(ren) become independent. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 12. I will spank my child when he/she does something wrong. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 13. I will express affection and caring towards my child(ren) through words and gestures. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 14. My child(ren) will be expected to tell me who they are going to be with, where they are going, what they are planning to do, and why they want to do it before being allowed to go out. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 15. I will try to understand and support my child(ren)’s choices. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 16. I will use verbal statements to praise and encourage my child. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 222 17. My child(ren) will not be allowed to do things that other children are allowed to do because I will be afraid that something will happen to them. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 18. I will listen to my child(ren). No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 19. I will show interest in what my child(ren) is interested in. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 20. I will probably expect too much from my children. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 21. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she can’t handle things on his/her own. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 22. When my child does something wrong, I will discuss what was done wrong and why it was wrong with him/her. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 23. I will show interest in what is going on in my child’s life. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 24. I am likely to be overly worried that something might happen to my child(ren). No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 25. I will make decisions for my child. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 223 26. If my child does something wrong, I will throw away something that he/she really likes to punish him/her. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 27. I will give my child added chores to do as a means of punishment. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 28. I will make sure my child(ren) feels like he/she can come to me for anything. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 29. No matter how well my child does at something, I will tell them they could have done better. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 30. I will use a belt, paddle, or other object to punish my child. No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 31. I will listen to and try to understand my child(ren). No 1 Maybe 2 Yes 3 224 ITEM Warmth Scale 1. I will spend one-on-one time with my children 8. I will attend my child(ren)’s sporting events and other extracurricular activities 13. I will express affection and caring towards my child(ren) through words and gestures 16. I will use verbal statements to praise and encourage my child 18. I will listen to my child(ren) 23. I will show interest in what is going on in my child’s life 28. I will make sure my child(ren) feels like he/she can come to me for anything Overprotection Scale 2. I will require my child(ren) to check in regularly when out 6. I will try to instill fear in my child(ren) so that he/she will be afraid to do things 10. I will insist on meeting and approving all of my child’s friends 11. It will be hard for me to let my child(ren) become independent 14. My child(ren) will be expected to tell me who they are going to be with, where they are going, what they are planning to do, and why they want to do it before being allowed to go out Center Questionnaires 225 .34 .33 .49 .41 .32 .37 .38 .54 .67 .70 .73 .65 2.91 2.77 2.85 2.91 2.88 2.89 2.66 1.44 2.20 1.84 2.46 SD 2.91 M .20 -.06 .14 .02 .23 .63 .61 .62 .58 .64 .63 .65 1 -.05 .59 .11 -.00 .01 -.11 -.10 -.02 -.29 -.36 -.17 .05 2 Factors .58 .09 .57 .49 .55 .08 -.09 .11 .21 .22 .10 .13 3 Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of the Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API) Using Public School and Juvenile Detention Appendix H 226 17. My child(ren) will not be allowed to do things that other children are allowed to do because I will be afraid that something will happen to them 21. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she can’t handle things on his/her own 24. I am likely to be overly worried that something might happen to my child(ren) Rejection Scale 3. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she is not good enough 7. I will compare my child to siblings or other children 9. I will probably favor one child over another 15. I will try to understand and support my child(ren)’s choices (reverse scored) 19. I will show interest in what my child is interested in (reverse scored) 20. I will probably expect too much from my children 25. I will make decisions for my child 29. No matter how well my child does at something, I will tell them they could have done better 31. I will listen to and try to understand my child (reverse scored) Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .64 .59 .74 .46 .49 .43 .32 .41 .71 .66 .72 .35 1.63 1.32 2.08 1.13 1.21 1.15 1.10 1.17 1.72 1.77 1.51 1.08 -.24 -.73 -.69 -.40 -.07 .01 -.15 .04 -.26 .07 -.06 -.16 .44 .20 -.06 .33 .50 .35 .53 .62 .48 .13 .54 .23 .29 -.08 .06 -.31 .28 .27 -.00 -.12 -.01 .61 .11 .59 Warmth Scale 1. I will spend one-on-one time with my children 8. I will attend my child(ren)’s sporting events and other extracurricular activities 13. I will express affection and caring towards my child(ren) through words and gestures 16. I will use verbal statements to praise and encourage my child 18. I will listen to my child(ren) 23. I will show interest in what is going on in my child’s life 28. I will make sure my child(ren) feels like he/she can come to me for anything Overprotection Scale 2. I will require my child(ren) to check in regularly when out 6. I will try to instill fear in my child(ren) so that he/she will be afraid to do things 10. I will insist on meeting and approving all of my child’s friends 11. It will be hard for me to let my child(ren) become independent 14. My child(ren) will be expected to tell me who they are going to be with, where they are going, what they are planning to do, and why they want to do it before being allowed to go out ITEM .16 -.17 - .15 .04 -.25 -.22 -.22 .04 .67 -.01 .03 .03 .70 .52 .56 .64 .67 .67 .29 -.02 .12 .03 .22 227 3 1 .57 .12 .58 .56 .48 .01 -.03 .14 .13 .19 .18 -.02 .50 -.11 .45 .09 .44 .50 .56 .65 .54 .71 .64 .81 .07 .43 .26 -.13 .01 -.14 -.11 -.08 -.43 -.44 .05 -.09 2 Factors Factors 2 JDC Public School .69 1 Questionnaires Completed by Juvenile Detention Center (JDC) Participants Factor Loadings of the Anticipated Future Parenting Index (API) Using Questionnaires Completed by Public School Participants and Appendix I .25 .11 .36 .32 .24 .30 -.07 .01 .14 -.13 -.28 -.00 3 17. My child(ren) will not be allowed to do things that other children are allowed to do because I will be afraid that something will happen to them 21. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she can’t handle things on his/her own 24. I am likely to be overly worried that something might happen to my child(ren) Rejection Scale 3. I will make my child(ren) feel like he/she is not good enough 7. I will compare my child to siblings or other children 9. I will probably favor one child over another 15. I will try to understand and support my child(ren)’s choices (reverse scored) 19. I will show interest in what my child is interested in (reverse sc) 20. I will probably expect too much from my children 25. I will make decisions for my child 29. No matter how well my child does at something, I will tell them they could have done better 31. I will listen to and try to understand my child (reverse scored) Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .53 .12 .51 .64 .37 -.22 .24 .55 .30 .46 .27 -.14 .04 -.13 .08 -.52 -.57 -.45 -.16 -.03 -.19 -.71 228 .19 -.21 .20 -.09 .02 -.24 .26 .05 .14 -.19 .12 .66 -.12 .51 -.17 -.71 -.68 -.36 -.05 .23 -.17 -.25 .15 .14 -.00 .04 .35 .29 .18 .64 .02 .22 .69 .14 .72 -.22 .19 -.01 .35 -.09 .02 -.19 .66 .41 .05 .35 -.02 .61 .53 .61 Appendix J Perceptions of Parenting Scale – Dissertation Version Below are a number of questions concerning your ideas about your parents’ behavior towards you as you experience it. For each question, please circle the answer choice that applies to your own mothers’ and father’s behavior towards you. Try to answer all questions that apply. We are aware that certain questions are impossible to answer if you do not have any sister(s) or brother(s) or if you have been raised by one parent only. Read through each question carefully and consider which one of the possible answers applies to you. ANSWER SEPARATELY FOR YOUR MOTHER AND YOUR FATHER. Choose ONE person to be the person you will be thinking about when you answer each question about mother. Choose ONE person who you will be thinking about when you answer each question about father. 1. This form will be asking you questions about your experience of your mother. Who will you be thinking of when you answer the questions about your mom (i.e., real mom, stepmom, grandmother, aunt, foster mother, etc.)? Please indicate:_________________________________ 2. This form will be asking you questions about your experience of your father. Who will you be thinking of when you answer the questions about your dad (i.e., real dad, stepfather, grandfather, uncle, foster father, etc.)? Please indicate:_________________________________ 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough. No, never Father: 1 Mother: 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 229 6. When I do something right, my parents give me something like money. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 7. To punish me when I do something wrong, my parents ground me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 12. My parents want to know all of my friends. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 230 14. I get hit by my parents when I do something wrong. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me that I did a good job. No, never Father: 1 Mother: 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 20. My parents listen to me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 231 22. My parents expect too much from me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 24. When I do something wrong, my parents explain to me what was done wrong and why it was wrong. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 27. My parents try to make decisions for me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 28. When I do something wrong, my parents throw away something of mine that I really like to punish me. No, never Father: 1 Mother: 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 29. When I do something wrong, my parents make me do extra jobs at home. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 232 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 32. My parents hit me with a belt, paddle, or other object to punish me when I do something wrong. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me. Father: Mother: No, never 1 1 Sometimes 2 2 Yes, often 3 3 233 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends ITEM Juvenile Detention Center Questionnaires SD .78 .82 .84 .80 .81 .83 .84 .82 .66 .80 M 2.05 2.00 2.12 2.34 2.18 2.17 2.12 2.15 1.38 2.02 234 .07 .72 .69 .80 .80 .77 .81 .77 .68 .75 1 Factors -.05 .12 .13 .21 .23 .24 .18 .17 .31 .16 2 .46 .16 .18 -.16 -.11 -.08 -.14 -.07 -.03 .00 3 1.51 2.35 2.48 2.42 2.45 2.57 2.45 2.46 2.26 2.39 M .73 .72 .70 .76 .72 .65 .70 .73 .80 .70 SD -.17 .19 .16 .16 .19 .20 .21 .18 .23 .18 1 Factors -.11 .50 .63 .78 .77 .78 .76 .70 .62 .69 2 .32 .26 .08 -.16 -.11 -.13 -.17 -.10 -.02 -.06 3 FATHER MOTHER ________________________________ _________________________________ Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of all Items on the Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) Using Public School and Appendix K 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) 22. My parents expect too much from me 27. My parents try to make decisions for me 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .62 .81 .70 .68 .75 .73 .65 .68 .82 .75 .72 .70 .70 .81 1.32 2.08 1.55 1.46 1.77 1.53 1.38 1.39 1.85 2.00 1.70 1.66 1.54 1.80 235 -.84 .06 -.67 .09 .14 -.82 -.32 -.28 -.51 .45 -.02 .27 .73 .03 -.25 -.11 -.20 .12 -.03 -.21 -.16 -.10 -.05 .11 -.11 .05 .14 .03 .19 .54 .08 .55 .64 .11 .41 .50 .31 .51 .58 .53 .18 .56 1.53 1.63 1.93 1.78 1.88 1.55 1.46 1.44 1.46 2.08 1.61 1.72 2.39 1.49 .70 .75 .71 .73 .71 .72 .71 .71 .69 .75 .74 .72 .68 .71 -.21 -.09 -.11 -.09 -.14 -.18 -.21 -.15 -.14 -.03 -.08 -.02 .24 -.07 -.80 -.13 -.63 -.09 .02 -.78 -.44 -.37 -.45 .47 -.25 .29 .55 -.01 .24 .50 .13 .52 .59 .17 .40 .42 .43 .38 .49 .42 .19 .54 .79 .84 .85 .85 .84 .84 .80 .81 .83 .84 .82 .66 .80 .62 .81 .70 .68 .75 2.12 2.34 2.18 2.17 2.12 2.15 1.38 2.02 1.32 2.08 1.55 1.46 1.77 .34 .14 -.14 .67 .05 .67 -.05 .63 1 .76 .73 Father M SD 2.05 .78 2.00 .82 236 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me Questionnaires .68 .73 .63 .38 .47 .32 .63 .38 .04 .01 -.06 -.01 -.05 Factors 2 .11 .06 -.09 -.04 .24 -.11 .19 -.04 .11 -.11 -.12 .00 -.13 -.08 -.13 3 -.06 -.05 Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of the Father Items on the Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) Using All Completed Appendix L Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) 22. My parents expect too much from me 27. My parents try to make decisions for me 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .65 .68 .82 .75 .72 .70 .70 .81 1.38 1.39 1.85 2.00 1.70 1.66 1.54 1.80 237 .73 1.53 .01 -.88 -.21 -.24 -.86 -.73 .05 .02 -.49 .46 .03 .22 .09 .07 .04 .45 .76 .17 .37 .20 .81 .82 .12 .03 .53 .17 .44 238 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own Questionnaires .70 .80 .73 .65 .70 .72 .76 .70 .73 .72 .71 .68 .72 .74 2.39 2.26 2.46 2.57 2.45 2.45 2.42 2.48 1.51 2.35 1.49 2.39 1.72 1.61 Mother SD M .13 -.32 .49 -.05 .44 -.18 .50 .75 .79 .79 .80 .83 .70 .60 1 .08 .36 -.02 .53 .00 .23 -.21 -.09 -.19 -.23 -.15 -.16 -.13 -.16 Factors 2 .56 .43 .49 .09 .45 .58 .52 .03 .12 .01 .04 -.03 .11 .22 3 Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings of the Mother Items on the Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) Using All Completed Appendix M 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) 22. My parents expect too much from me 27. My parents try to make decisions for me 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. 239 .75 .69 .71 .71 .72 .71 .73 .71 .75 .70 2.08 1.46 1.44 1.46 1.55 1.93 1.78 1.88 1.63 1.53 -.12 -.86 -.19 -.28 -.78 -.69 -.06 -.13 -.37 .25 .53 .00 .82 .77 .24 .05 .61 .34 .63 -.04 .35 .14 -.07 -.08 -.04 .06 .37 .64 .12 .64 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent ITEM 2 .12 .30 .16 .30 .16 .24 .18 .14 -.09 .10 -.07 1 .71 .65 .72 .65 .77 .76 .77 .64 -.01 .69 -.00 Father Factors 240 .54 .40 .22 .23 -.20 -.17 -.11 -.20 -.12 -.04 .00 3 -.00 -.02 .28 .28 .16 .21 .22 .20 .21 .24 .23 1 -.23 -.23 .44 .52 .80 .76 .76 .78 .65 .62 .65 2 Mother Factors .53 .27 .33 .19 -.13 -.03 -.11 -.17 -.01 .02 - .03 3 Factor Loadings of all Items on the Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) Using Questionnaires Completed by Public School Participants Appendix N 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out .65 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me .07 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own -.22 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me .33 Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough -.48 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children -.42 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me -.38 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) -.79 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) -.59 22. My parents expect too much from me -.19 27. My parents try to make decisions for me -.06 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better -.14 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) -.79 Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .59 .49 .52 .46 .42 .41 .16 .08 .58 .67 .56 .28 .04 -.17 -.00 .02 -.12 -.10 -.20 -.17 .11 -.04 -.11 -.27 241 .19 .11 -.21 -.05 -.07 -.03 -.00 -.18 -.14 -.07 -.11 -.03 -.00 .02 .34 -.78 -.28 -.65 -.27 -.23 -.77 -.43 -.46 -.51 .27 -.50 .14 .45 .24 .44 .12 .46 .56 .09 .39 .35 .37 .42 .35 .48 .22 Warmth Scale 3. My parents spend one-on-one time with me 10. My parents come to my school activities and other activities 15. My parents express affection towards me by giving me hugs and saying they love me 18. When I do something right, my parents tell me I did a good job 20. My parents listen to me 25. My parents show interest in what is going on in my life 30. My parents make sure I feel like I can come to them for anything Overprotection Scale 4. My parents want to know where I am all the time and make me check in when I’m out 8. My parents try to scare me by telling me that bad things might happen so that I am afraid to do things 12. My parents want to know all of my friends 13. My parents try to stop me from becoming independent ITEM Center (JDC) Participants 2 .20 .31 .19 .20 .19 .21 .23 .08 -.05 .10 .11 1 .74 .68 .80 .85 .83 .84 .80 .73 .27 .71 .18 Father Factors 242 .52 .49 .08 .11 -.18 -.06 -.11 -.14 -.05 .02 -.02 3 -.06 -.17 .05 .07 .21 .19 .18 .23 .13 .15 .08 1 2 .20 -.01 .52 .72 .78 .77 .81 .75 .77 .65 .75 Mother Factors .53 .41 .31 .01 -.11 -.12 -.03 -.07 -.14 .05 .02 3 Factor Loadings of all Items on the Perceptions of Past Parenting Scale (PPPS) Using Questionnaires Completed by the Juvenile Detention Appendix O 16. I am expected to tell my parents who I am going to be with, where I am going, what I am planning to do, and why I want to do it before being allowed to go out .81 19. My parents do not allow me to do things that other children are allowed to do because they are afraid that something will happen to me .64 23. My parents try to make me feel like I can’t handle things on my own .28 26. My parents tend to be overly worried that something might happen to me .70 Rejection Scale 5. My parents make me feel like I am not good enough -.53 9. My parents think I am not as good as my brother or sister or other children -.05 11. My parents like my brother or sister more than they like me -.26 17. My parents try to understand me and what I want (reverse scored) -.84 21. My parents and I are interested in the same things (reverse scored) -.74 22. My parents expect too much from me .30 27. My parents try to make decisions for me .49 31. No matter how well I do at something, my parents tell me that I could have done better .43 33. My parents listen to and try to understand me (reverse scored) -.88 Note. Numbers in bold indicate loadings on that factor. .30 .60 .34 .21 .62 .45 .10 .13 .46 .47 .39 .10 .03 -.06 .17 -.17 -.12 -.28 -.21 -.20 .05 -.10 -.14 -.23 243 .17 .17 -.23 .06 -.09 -.08 -.13 -.17 -.24 -.13 -.14 .19 -.11 .11 .11 -.82 .00 -.64 .06 .26 -.80 -.49 -.33 -.42 .61 .04 .45 .65 .17 .50 -.00 .57 .58 .18 .43 .51 .54 .22 .60 .28 .33 Appendix P Traumatic Events Questionnaire Below are some questions about how some kids feel or about things that some kids have had happen to them. Please read each question, then mark how often it happens to YOU by circling the correct answer. Circle 1 if it never happens to you. Circle 2 if it happens rarely. Circle 3 if it happens sometimes. Circle 4 if it happens often. Please answer ALL of the questions. 1. I didn’t have enough to eat. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 2. I have been called mean names by people in my family. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 3. I have been scared of being hurt by someone in my family. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 4. I have felt like there was no one to take care of me and protect me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 5. I was punished with a belt, board, or other object. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 6. Someone tried to make me do or watch sexual things. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 7. Someone in my family hit me so hard that it left marks or bruises. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 244 Often True 4 8. Someone tried to make me touch them or they tried to touch me in a sexual way. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 9. I felt that someone in my family hated me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 10. My parents were too drunk or high to take care of the family. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 11. I had sex with someone a lot older than me (more than five years older). Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 12. Someone in my family said things that were hurtful or insulting to me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 13. I was hit or beaten badly enough that it was noticed by someone like a babysitter, teacher, or doctor. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 14. Someone made threats that they would hurt me or tell lies about me if I did not do sexual things with them. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 15. Someone in my family hit or beat me badly enough that I had to see a doctor. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 16. I was not taken to the doctor when I was sick or needed a checkup. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 245 Often True 4 Appendix Q YPI Version 3.0 Instructions This sheet consists of a number of statements that deal with what you think and feel about different things. Read each statement carefully and decide how well the particular statement applies to you. You can choose between four different alternatives on each statement. Answer each statement as you most often feel and think, not only how you feel right now. Example: I like reading books. Does not Does not apply at all apply well " " Applies fairly well Applies very well " " • Put a mark in the box that corresponds to how you feel. • Do not think too long on each statement. REMEMBER: • Answer ALL statements. • Do not put a mark between the alternatives. • Only one answer per statement. IMPORTANT!!! There are no answers that are ”Right” or ”Wrong”. You cannot score worse or better than anyone else. We are interested in what you think and feel, not in what is ”Right” or ”Wrong”. 1. I usually feel calm when other people are scared. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 2. I have the ability not to feel guilt and regret about things that I think other people would feel guilty about. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 246 3. I think that crying is a sign of weakness, even if no one sees you. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 4. When other people have problems, it is often their own fault, therefore, one should not help them. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 5. I seldom regret things I do, even if other people feel that they are wrong. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 6. It’s important to me not to hurt other people’s feelings. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 7. To be nervous and worried is a sign of weakness. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 8. When someone finds out about something that I’ve done wrong, I feel more angry than guilty. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 9. I often become sad or moved by watching sad things on TV or film. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 10. What scares others usually doesn’t scare me. Does not Does not Applies apply at all apply well fairly well " " " 247 Applies very well " 11. I don’t understand how people can be touched enough to cry by looking at things on TV or movie. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 12. To feel guilty and remorseful about things you have done that have hurt other people is a sign of weakness. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 13. I don’t let my feelings affect me as much as other people’s feelings seem to affect them. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 14. To feel guilt and regret when you have done something wrong is a waste of time. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 15. I usually become sad when I see other people crying or being sad. Does not apply at all Does not apply well Applies fairly well Applies very well " " " " 248 Appendix R Symptoms of Trauma Questionnaire Below is a list of problems people sometimes have after very bad things happen to them. Please read each question, then mark how often it happens to YOU in the past month by circling the correct answer. Circle 1 if it never happens to you. Circle 2 if it happens rarely. Circle 3 if it happens sometimes. Circle 4 if it happens often. Please answer ALL of the questions. 1. I watch out for danger or things that I am afraid of. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 2. I get very upset, afraid, or sad when something reminds me of a bad thing that happened to me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 3. Thoughts, pictures, or sounds of a bad thing that happened to me come into my mind when I don’t want them to and they upset me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 4. I feel grouchy, angry, or mad. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 5. I have dreams about a bad thing that happened to me or other bad dreams. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 6. I feel like I am back when the bad thing happened and living through it again. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 7. I feel like being by myself and not being with my friends. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 249 Often True 4 8. I feel alone inside and not close to other people. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 9. I try not to talk about, think about, or have feelings about a bad thing that happened to me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 10. I have trouble feeling happy or loved. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 11. I have trouble feeling sad or angry. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 12. I feel jumpy and easily startled, like when I hear a loud noise or when something surprises me. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 13. I have trouble getting to sleep or I wake up a lot during the night. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 14. I think that part of a bad thing that happened to me is my fault. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 15. I have trouble remembering parts of a bad thing that happened to me. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True 1 2 3 4 16. I have trouble concentrating or paying attention. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 17. I try to stay away from people, places, or things that make me remember a bad thing that happened. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 250 Often True 4 18. When something reminds me of a bad thing that happened, I have strong feelings in my body, like my heart beating fast, my head aches, or my stomach aches. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 Sometimes True 3 Often True 4 19. I think that I will not live a long life. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 20. I am afraid that a bad thing that happened to me will happen again. Never True 1 Rarely True 2 Sometimes True 3 251 Often True 4 Appendix S About Me Please circle either True or False for each statement as it applies to you. 1. I am always respectful to older people. True False 2. Sometimes I do not feel like doing what my teachers want me to do. True False 3. Sometimes I have felt like throwing things or breaking them. True False 4. I never talk back to my parent or guardian. True False 5. When I make a mistake, I always admit that I am wrong. True False 6. I sometimes feel like making fun of other people. True False 7. I always wash my hands before every meal. True False 8. Sometimes I wish I could just hang out instead of going to school. True False 9. I have never been tempted to break a rule or law. True False 10. Sometimes I dislike helping my parent/guardian even though I know they need my help around the house. True False 11. Sometimes I say things just to impress my friends. True False 12. I never shout when I feel angry. True False 252