Wellspring
Transcription
Wellspring
Wellspring Volume 15, Issue 1 Winter 2015 Committee Chairs Board of Directors Table of Contents President Margaret Rousser Oakland Zoo Page 1 President’s Corner – Margaret Rousser Page 2-12 Page 2-4 “Training Without the Trainer” Through Enrichment Tools and the Application of the Balance Index (IB) on Enrichment Use František Šusta Past President Heather Samper Mutts With Manners President-Elect Nicki Boyd San Diego Zoo 1st Vice President Annette Pederson Copenhagen Zoo Page 13-15 Animal Art: How Do They Do It? – Juliann Breeding Page 16-29 Fire Hoses Do More Than Fight Fires!- Micala Teetzen, Sedgwick County Zoo nd 2 Vice President Scott Trauger Lowry Park Zoo Chief Financial Officer Susie Ekard San Diego Zoo Safari Park Back Inside Cover Parting Shots Back Cover 2014 ABMA Conference Award Winners Behavior Management Fund Michelle Farmerie Genevieve Warner Conference Kirstin Anderson Hansen Conference Content Advisory Christine McKnight Conference Proceedings Jennifer Hickman Samantha Smith Education Missy Lamar Government Affairs Justin Garner Secretary Cinnamon Williams Kansas City Zoo Honors & Awards Scott Trauger Membership Amy Schilz Directors Christa Gaus National Aviary Merchandise Susie Ekard Jennifer Hickman Denver Zoo Nominations & Elections Heather Leeson Missy Lamar SeaWorld San Antonio Public Affairs Margaret Rousser Jay Tacey Busch Gardens Williamsburg Page 18-19 Publications Heidi Hellmuth Research &Evaluation Darren Minier Site Selection Kelly Elkins Sponsorship Cathy Schlott Website Heather Samper ‘Enrichment Eggstravaganza’ in the Primate House at the Saint Louis Zoo – photos by Joe Knobbe ABMA Wellspring - Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 Wellspring The Source of Behavioral Management Information Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 President’s Column What a great year for the Animal Behavior Management Alliance! Our membership is more diverse than ever before, with members on different continents from all over the world. 2015 brought ABMA’s first European conference in Denmark! Cohosts Copenhagen Zoo and Odense Zoo have planned an amazing week for us and I must give a shout out to Annette Pedersen and Kirstin Anderson Hansen for all of their hard work putting together the program. In 2016, ABMA will be heading to Tampa, FL. Cohosts Tampa’s Lowry Park Zoo and Busch Gardens, Tampa are looking forward to our visit and they have some great things planned for us while we are there, so save that date! We will be “Breaking down Barriers” to find New Possibilities in Animal Welfare. We will kick off the conference with a Keynote from Dr. Susie Ellis, the executive Director of the International Rhino Foundation. Our team on the ground in Tampa also has some new innovative ideas for this conference, so you won’t want to miss it! While conferences may be one of our favorite member benefits, they are far from the only ones. We are adding new content to our website all the time and keeping up with CollaborNation! Our team is vigilantly preparing new resources for our members. And let’s not forget the member benefit of AnimalProfessional.com! Even if you can’t make it to the conference every year, you can still see and access all of the amazing presentations just a few weeks afterwards with your ABMA membership. As I finish up my presidency, I am proud of this organization and honored to have served you! It has been my privilege to be your president and as I pass the baton on to our next president, Nicki Boyd, I feel confident that the ABMA will continue to realize its vision of being a global leader in animal welfare. Sincerely, Margaret Rousser ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 1 “Training Without the Trainer” Through Enrichment Tools and the Application of the Balance Index (IB) on Enrichment Use František Šusta Abstract: Enrichment and animal training are very powerful tools for helping to ensure animal welfare. Although for many of us they mean two separate activities, there are many scenarios when the training is enriching and enrichment can be used as training. Furthermore, understanding the principles of behavior management very often makes it more effective for us when using enrichment to solve behavioral concerns, At Institution 1 we manage a number of groups of animals. Aggression is natural behavior, however not something we wish to increase as a result of our presence during public presentations or shows, or as a result of our introducing enrichment. We have developed an analytical method which evaluates an individual animal’s potential to succeed in a training session. This has been used with great success particularly in our lemur shows. Recently we have used this method during enrichment. This has enabled us to effectively decrease aggression between conspecifics in a gorilla (Gorilla gorilla) group, lemur group, and increase our ability to manage feeding and medicating in a group of coatis (Nasua spp). Managing groups of animals is not easy. Keepers are often pressured to achieve this with little insight. When understanding the natural dominance over food resources of each group member, we can effectively manage behavior through elaborate enrichment tools, without losing keeper time or the necessity to separate the group members. To illustrate this principle, we use examples of a gorilla´s feeding time, ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta) show, and an experiment on a coati group enrichment. Introduction: Teaching behaviors to the animals in groups without the possibility of separating them is a very challenging job. In Institution 1 we have developed an analytic method called Balance Index (IB), presented at the ABMA conference in 2012, which helps us to maintain social harmony during training with a particular focus on individual animals without them being disturbed by others. But, this training still needs the active presence of an experienced trainer. Keepers are not always experienced enough to manage these scenarios, and their lack of time is also a compromising factor. For these reasons we decided to test the principles of group training using the IB model in the enrichment program. We would use specially prepared enrichment tools that had the potential for us to modify their difficulty relative to the needs of the individuals in the group in question. The IB model, described in “Methods” and in Susta (2011), is a mathematical model to analyze social situations in any group during training. The model determines the probability of the animal doing any behavior according to four variables. Two variables increase probability of the behavior occurring: K (does animal KNOW the behavior?) R (expected REWARD). Two variables do decrease probability: D (DIFFICULTY) PA (Potential “to be ASSAULTED” or aggressed towards) The formula is as follows: IB = (K×R)/(D×PA). From the six basic rules of the IB model, published in Susta et al. (2013), the important rules for potential enrichment use are: ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 2 1) The balance index (IB) for group behaviors can only be used for the groups where the dominance status of each member is clear to all other members – the group is stable (ideally the group members do live together). This can also be used only for situations when the group members compete for the same resource (such as food, the trainer, etc.). 2) All behaviors of individual animals in group must be understood as a part of a complex group behavior. 3) For each behavior in every moment, we have to manage the group so that the IB is the highest for the most dominant animal and decreases for the rest of group with their decreasing hierarchy status. Another situation will probably lead to less effectively maintained group behavior. (For a more detailed explanation see Susta (2011).) Dominant group members naturally tend to have the highest IB in all situations. This can be ensured by the highest level of K (the dominant animal Knows the behavior best), the highest R (expects a high level of Reward), the least D (tends to perform the easiest – least Difficult - behavior) or least PA (tends to choose the safest station or behavior, least Potential for Assault/aggression). When the dominant animal is assured of the highest IB, the rest of group members choose their behaviors relative to their hierarchy status – the IB of each member decreases in accordance with the decreasing status of that individual. At Institution 1, we respect and maintain the natural tendency of the group and always ensure the highest IB for the dominant group members. We have had success with this approach in that it has reduced stress in the group and enabled us to succeed in training behaviors for husbandry and shows with our groups of animals. We have recently successfully integrated this approach into our enrichment program. Here are three examples: GORILLA ENRICHMENT – Scent Target The problem we were facing was that when food enrichment was used with our gorilla group, we noted that the maximum time for the stimulation was 20 minutes; and the food enrichment also often resulted in high levels of aggression. After some simple “training through enrichment” we have increased the enrichment stimulation time to an hour of peaceful activity. This was done by teaching gorillas a “scent message”. Dominant gorilla male working of complicated puzzle feeders made of knotted fire hoses. Before using the “scent target” technique, the gorillas did not react to A variety of puzzle feeders were used. these feeders at all, after using the “scent target” they spend about one hour with of calm activity with these scented feeders Puzzles with food in them were scented with artificial almond scent. Puzzles with no scent had no food in them. More dominant animals were taught what message this almond scent meant and through this how to use the more difficult puzzle feeders. So their K variable was high which lead to high IB. Puzzle feeders were distributed throughout the enclosure, and feeding territories of the various animals were taken into account to determine where the more difficult puzzles would be placed. Before this approach was adopted, gorillas would try and steal all the puzzles and protect them while working them out, which is where the aggression initiated. After initiating this new approach, the gorillas were more engaged in choosing and working out the puzzles. It was also noted that they ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 3 worked out the easiest puzzles (those with the highest IB) first. This included the fact that they also searched the more difficult parts of the exhibit last (highest D) LEMUR ENRICHMENT We also directly used the IB model in our application of enrichment devices to a group of lemurs during their show. We developed a puzzle feeder for a dominant member of ring-tailed lemur group. The objective was to keep the animal on the ground during our exhibit shows. Over time, we were able to increase the complexity of the puzzle device. Initially it was very simple and when we gave it to the group, the dominant member naturally gained control of this food resource. Later we increased the difficulty more and more and the dominant member naturally became the only one in the group who was able to succeed (his variable K for this concrete behavior became the highest from the entire group). As a result he spends a long time on this puzzle feeder during the show and is thus engaged and so does not challenge or attack other group members during their feeding and training. We even noted that when we added new physically stronger lemurs of a different species, there was no problem for this lemur during show time, because these new dominant animals did not know how to open this feeder and so they did not attack him The dominant ring-tailed lemur Pancho works on his puzzle feeder during show. At the on the ground. It must be highlighted same time he allows the submissive lemurs to cooperate with the trainer and is not that more submissive animals in a afraid of attack by the potentially aggressive brown lemur on the ground next to him group of lemurs would naturally head up higher. The ground is reserved for more dominant, confident animals. Because of the success of using the IB method of enrichment we designed an experiment to assist us to answer the following question: Are we able to control the food and medication of individual group members without direct feeding or separation? Objective: The objective of our experiment was to test the possibility of using enrichment tools and the IB model to address concrete food or medication to concrete animal group members without separation of individuals or necessity of direct feeding. Positive result would be very helpful for managing shy animal groups where separation or direct feeding of individuals by keeper is not possible. Method: The experiment has been done in INSTITUTION 1, Europe, in July to August of 2012. The group of South American coatis (Nasua nasua) consisted of four individuals (2.2): Ďinďa (1.0; born July 2009, Prague, castrated), Stromovka (1.0; born April 2005, Prague, castrated), Anička (0.1; born July 2009, Prague), Smrž (0.1; born April 2001, Zoo Stutgart, mother of all the other individuals). We have prepared four similar puzzle feeders known to us as ‘desks’. These were constructed out of wood and plastic bins. They are 40x20x25 cm in The puzzle feeder experiment with coatis ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 4 size. The objective of the puzzle feeders was for coatis was to get the food out of a food bin between the two desks from a hole in the upper desk. Ten levels of difficulty are possible because of a series of wooden pickets situated between the two flat wood pieces that make up the desk. As first the hierarchy status of each individual was identified using the following methods: a) Dominance index (DI), b) Clutton-Brock Index (CBI) Ad a - The Dominance Index (DI) has been counted from the ratio of wins minus defeats to all decisive interactions defined by Bowen and Brooks (1978). DI= Σ (win+defeats/win-defeats) This index ranges from −1 (absolutely submissive) to +1 (absolutely dominant). Ad b - The Clutton-Brock Index was originally designed for use with red deer stags, Cervus elaphus. It measures an individual´s rank according to the ranks of his opponents and is based on the number of individuals an individual defeats, or is defeated by, adjusted by the number of individuals those individuals defeated, or are defeated by (Clutton-Brock et al., 1979). Clutton-Brock Index (CBI) for each member, i, of a group was calculated with the formula: CBI = (B + Σb + 1) / (L + Σl + 1) B represents the number of individuals that i defeated in one or more interactions, Σb represents the total number of individuals (excluding i) that those represented in B defeated, L represents the number of individuals by which i was defeated and Σl represents the total number of individuals (excluding i) by which those represented in L were defeated. After identifying the hierarchy status of each individual, and following the IB model, a prediction of how coatis will share daily food given them in sequence in four dishes, each one second after another, has been estimated and then simply compared with results of observation in real. The variables “number of attacks”, “number of escapes”, and “order of the food dish gained by the animal during daily feeding” has been tested by ANOVA. The attacks, escapes, and other dominance/submission interactions were recorded one hour before and after the normal daily feeding, when these interactions are mostly frequent. The normal daily feeding routine of the coati group using the four dishes. Each one takes one dish and we can see the hierarchy positions of each individual when sharing these food resources ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 5 After identifying the hierarchy status of individuals we provided the four puzzle feeders on a variety of difficulty settings and gave these feeders to the group many times. Following IB model we made predictions “which coati will take which feeder” and we statistically compared them with observed results. The values for all the four variables of IB model were the same as we use when assessing animal training scenarios (see below). During training we assign a numerical value based on the IB variables. This is only relevant for the behaviors we actually ask at any particular moment. For example if we want one animal to jump and the rest of the group just to sit and wait, we assign values of variables for “jumping” for one and for “sitting” for the rest of group. Variable 1: K - Does animal KNOW the behavior? 0 – does not know at all 1 –it knows only some basic steps 2 – it knows the full behavior, but not under stimulus control 3 – the desired behavior is under stimulus control Variable 2: R (expected REWARD) – the reward which is expected by the animal. 0 – no reward 1 – food reward, but not the favorite food 2 – ordinary food (ordinary used in normal daily feed ration, not in training) 3 – ordinary reward (not standard part of the daily feed ration, but the most frequently used during training 4 – “bonus” ( like a “weak jackpot”, for example a few more pieces of food used as rewards) 5 – “jackpot” (extra big reward) The value 4 or 5 we assign only when the animal has a good reason to expect it (for example because of reinforcement schedule). Variable 3: D (DIFFICULTY, also control over resources) How difficult is the behavior for the animal. From the animal’s point of view, this can be also understood as control over resources (food), because the behavior is the way to reach the reward – to get control over it. For example when animals are sitting around the trainer, the highest control over resources and so the smallest level of D can be found by the animal sitting nearest the pocket with rewards – the animal has the highest control. 1 – no difficulty (high control) 2 – small difficulty (high, but not maximum control) 3 – middle (middle control) 4 – high (small control) 5 – extremely high or impossible (no control over resources) Variable 4: PA – (Potential “to be ASSAULTED” or aggressed towards) For example with lemurs, the PA increases when the submissive animal is located right under the branch where a dominant aggressive animal is sitting. The designated values of these categories depend on the species ethology, as well as trainers knowledge of the animals. 1 – no potential (animal is not threatened by anyone in the group. May be the dominant animal) 2 – the potential would be real only when conditions will change (for example dominant primate sitting under the submissive one could be under threat only if it´s hierarchy will change) 3 – actually exists, but at a low level 4 – actually exists on high level 5 – “deadly endangered” The formula is IB = (K×R)/(D×PA) and the basic rule is “highest IB for the dominant group member and decreasing IB with decreasing hierarchy status each animal” (for other rules see above in Introduction and Susta (2011), Susta et al. (2013). ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 6 For different situations of inserting the four puzzle feeders to the group, (see picture 5) we predicted the following IB for different puzzle feeders and different coatis. The K (Knows) stayed always the same (value 2). For R (expected Reward) the value was always 3, because the rewards in the puzzle feeder remained the same and coatis can smell them. The D changed for different puzzle difficulties according to the previous success of the individual on that difficulty level: value 1 if the individual already successfully reached the food from that difficulty level, value 2 if the difficulty level is just a little above the one which coati already reached and value 3 if the difficulty level of the puzzle is more than one level above the last successful level. For PA (Potential to be Assaulted) we assigned values according to the hierarchy status of the individual – value 1 for the most dominant, value 2 for the second dominant and value The insertion of puzzle feeders during the experiment (in regular use 3 for the submissive animals, the distance between them was bigger) always endangered from the attacks of the others during feeding together. The PA must also be high for the entire group when the new enrichment is inserted, because this food resource will probably be understood as a potential danger. This is why the situation of first insertion of a new device has a different prediction way then the insertion of known devices. Example of doing prediction for a normal situation: We inserted four different feeders on difficulty levels c<d=d<e. According to the IB rules, the most dominant animal will choose first the feeder with highest IB for this individual. If this individual already knows from previous insertion how to reach food from difficulty level c, its D=1 for level c and D=2 for level d (which is only one level above the c), and D=3 for level e (two levels above the c). So for level c, his IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (2×3)/(1×1)=6, and for level d the IB= (2×3)/(2×1)=3, and for level e the IB= (2×3)/(3×1)=2. The dominant animal naturally chooses the level c since this one has the highest IB for him, and the rest of feeders remain for the other individuals. The second animal will choose from d=d< e, but for him they can have different values then for the first one, because this individual had another experience before. If he is already able to reach level f, levels d and e (with difficulty below the f) have D=1 for him and their both IBs are equal, IB= (2×3)/(1×2)=3. The PA=2 for the second animal, because he is potentially endangered from the first one when feeding together. This individual will probably take the feeder the furthest distance from the first one, not depending on difficulty level, because for him they are all the same. And there are two remaining feeders which submissive animals can use and their prediction we will do the same way, according to their experience and abilities known from previous feeders insertion (influences D) and their hierarchy status and proximity from dominant animals during feeding time (influences PA). Influences of the two, only changing variables D and PA, have been tested by contingency tables. Results: Hierarchy status of each individual: Dominance index (DI): (in DI the less number means higher hierarchy status of the individual) Ďinďa: 0,2 Stromovka: 0,9 Anička: impossible to calculate Smrž: impossible to calculate ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 7 Clutton-Brock Index (CBI) CBI -=(B+ Σb+1)/ (L+ Σl+1) (in CBI higher number means higher status) Ďinďa: 4,4 Stromovka: 1,25 Anička: 0,03 Smrž: 0,14 Both indexes do show that Dinďa is the most dominant animal of the group and Stromovka is the second one. Both females have lower hierarchy status, which were able to be calculated only by CBI index, and both are very low. Because of it we give them both the status 3-4. Prediction for sharing daily food dishes: When we give the group four food dishes with the same food, in sequence, with a one second delay between each one; it is clear that the first dish gives the dominant coati highest control over food resources (lowest D=1), then the second one has D=2 and so on due to their delay in time . All the other variables stay the same due to the same food, kind of bins, and others. So the IB status of each food dish is: IB of the first bin is : IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (2×2)/(1×1)=4 IB of the second bin is IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (2×2)/(1×2)=2 IB of the third bin is IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (2×2)/(1×3)=1,33 IB of the last bin is IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (2×2)/(1×4)= 1 Graph 1. The relationship between hierarchy status of the individual and orrder of the food dish the individual took According to the laws of IB model and according to the hierarchy status of each individual identified above, the prediction for sharing food dishes is, that the most dominant animal Dinďa will take always the first present dish, Stromovka the second, Anička and Smrž will share the rest. 6 5 Number of events The results are shown in graph 1. Dinďa, when present, always used bin number 1, Stromovka mostly number 2, and females really shared the rest of bins. If Dinďa is not present during feeding (two times), his dish is taken by Stromovka, and then the other individuals takes the nearest free dish with the highest IB according to the hierarchy status of that individual. So these results agree with our prediction. 7 4 1 - Dinďa 2 - Stromovka 3-4 Anička 3 3-4 Smrž 2 The ANOVA test for variables “number of attacks”, “number of escapes”, and “order of the food dish gained by the animal during daily feeding” has been found significant for all the three variables. 1 0 1 2 3 Order of the food dish ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 8 4 Table 1: The ANOVA test for variables “order of the dish”, “attacks”, “escapes ANOVA Sum of Squares Dishes Between Groups df Mean Square 22,403 3 7,804 25 Total 30,207 28 Between Groups 36,844 3 21,875 28 Total 58,719 31 Escapes Between Groups 12,250 3 4,083 Within Groups 29,250 28 1,045 Total 41,500 31 Within Groups Attacks Within Groups 7,468 F p 23,924 YES >0,001 0,312 12,281 15,720 YES >0,001 0,781 3,909 YES >0,01 Predictions for sharing the puzzle feeders: First insertion: For the first introduction to the group of a new, potentially dangerous food resource, with which the group has no previous experience, the PA must be very high at the beginning. The IB model predicts that all the variables (K –Know, R – expected Reward, D – Difficulty, PA – Potential to be Assaulted) are the same for all the group members. All the group members can know only basic steps (K=1) if the puzzle is easy enough. The expected reward is the same for all of them (R=3), the difficulty as well (D=2 if it is on the basic level) and all of these three variables stay the same all the time. But the PA, potential to be assaulted, decreases with time and with the habituation and desensitization of the animals. Then finally they will find this is not a danger and the PA decreases to PA=1 (no danger is expected from this new device). The numeral prediction for new food resources: IB for the feeder when first time inserted: IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (1×3)/(2×5)=0,3 IB for the feeder after short habituation: IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (1×3)/(2×3)=0,5 IB for the feeder after full habituation: IB = (K×R)/(D×PA)= (1×3)/(2×1)=1,5 It means that with decreasing PA during the habituation process, the IB increases over time. And because we know that dominant animals do naturally work on high levels of IB and submissive animals on lower values, the submissive animals will naturally tend to come first to potentially dangerous food resources, and the dominants will wait longer for better desensitization and lower PA, with respect to their higher IB. So the prediction for our group, according to previously found hierarchy status of individuals; is that Anička and Smrž will come first, later Stromovka, and the last one will be Dinďa - so the opposite order of their hierarchy status. Results: The order the coatis came to new feeders in this first meeting time was 1) Smrž, 2) Anička, 3) Stromovka 4) Dinďa . The situation is shown below. This result agrees with our prediction, but it can not be statistically proven once it was only one time (n=1´). ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 9 The first insertion of the new device into the exhibit. The dominant group members stay a safe distance on the rope and wait for habituation Consecutive insertions: After the group has been desensitized to the new devices, our prediction for each situation has been done as described above in Methods. The observed results have been found to agree with the predictions in most events, and the differences were only when some of coatis did not come to feeders due to their activity in another part of exhibit. But always, if any coati was absent, the next one on lower hierarchy level took his place. The relationship between variables “occurrence of predicted behavior” and “designated PA” for each individual is shown in graph 2. Analysis by contingency tables shows that predicted behavior occurred in 73,3%. The results show that coatis with lower PA show more predicted behavior. By Chi-Square Test the χ2=0,002, the same as Symmetric measures, which both means significant relationship between variables. Graph 2. Occurence of predicted behavior in relationship with PA of the individual 25 Occurence of predicted behavior 20 15 The predicted behavior did not occure 10 The predicted behavior occured 5 0 1 2 3 PA ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 10 The relationship between variables “occurrence of predicted behavior” and “designated D of each puzzle feeder for the individual” are shown by graph 3, which shows that more expected behavior is shown on feeders with lower values of variable D for the concrete animal working on the feeder. By contingency tables, the predicted behavior occurred in 73,3%. The results do show that coatis with lower PA do show more predicted behavior. By Chi-Square Test the χ2=0,003, the same as Symmetric measures, which both means significant relationship between variables. Discussion: Knowing the hierarchy status of a group or at least identifying the dominant animal in a group is very important information. By knowing and respecting this status, the keeper is able to more effectively manage the group without less aggression and potential for injury. Scientifically identifying the status of each animal by official methods following number of attacks and escapes is near impossible for a lot of keepers due to lack of time and difficulty of these methods. More effective ways of ascertaining the hierarchy that keepers can use is their noting individual animals’ dominance over food resources whenever keepers feed. Our results show that this way of identifying natural hierarchy in the group is possible and this can be effectively used without a great deal of a keeper´s time. When we put the new device into the exhibit, we found that the submissive animals used that food resource first, while dominant animals came to that potentially danger food resource last. This event is not surprising. This also fits with the prediction done by IB model rules and this is also known from the first steps of training new animal groups with positive reinforcement. Susta (2008) shows similar example on ring-tailed lemurs, red-ruffed lemurs and barbary sheep (Ammotragus lervia) in INSTITUTION 1. This is why this first moment of meeting new, potentially dangerous devices could be used in animal groups as a way to give the concrete food (or medicine) to a submissive animal. The only trouble is that we can never predict how much the new device is understood by the animals as “potentially dangerous” (how high the PA is). With frequent use of different devices, the group will also desensitize for a variety of new things, and with good desensitization the potential danger of the new device is minimal – then the PA=1 for all the individuals from the first insertion and the dominant group member will take it first. Using the IB variables D and PA when planning the use of puzzle feeders for animal groups has the potential for success to ensure that food and medication intake per individual is controlled without having to resort to separating animals, especially when individuals are shy and not able for hand feeding and separation. This would be managed as follows: The distance between each animal’s puzzle feeder would large enough to minimize the PA of submissive animals (potential to be assaulted by dominant group member). The actual abilities of each animal to solve the puzzle feeder would be known by keepers. All the individuals will have the same access to all the puzzle feeders at the beginning – the same distance between them, so the actual value of D for each animal on each feeder depends only on the previous experience of the individual with the tool and not on the distance from this resource. However this last requirement, together with requirement of large enough distance between puzzle feeders to minimize PA, is a big challenge in practice. So, to deliver food to the submissive group members, the effect of the first experience with new device described above, where submissive animals come first, gives us a better chance (the same D for everyone and changing PA in time). For dominant animals on the other hand, giving the concrete food to individuals is much more likely as a way of controlling the D variable and giving it through feeders after previous desensitization and training history. We also have to note that this one experiment is not enough and for rigid conclusions, more experiments will have to be done. Conclusion: In the gorilla scenario, while enriching the animals at the same time as using the IB variables, we effectively decreased aggression and increased enrichment stimulation time. In the lemur example, we use enrichment during our daily shows. The IB principles are employed in these scenarios and have enabled us to maintain harmony and productivity in these scenarios. ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 11 In the experiment with the coatis, we noted how much we can control the puzzle feeder scenario, ensuring that different animals stay at predicted feeders. This was done to see whether we could control the food and medication intake. We noted that with the application of the insight from the IB model, we can effectively predict and apply this system to achieve our goal. However this method, which could be useful for management of shy animal groups, needs more experiment and development to be rigid enough. References: Bowen, D.W., Brooks, R.J. (1978). Social organization of confined male collared lemmings (Dicrostonyx groenlandicus Traill). Animal Behavior, 26(4),1126 –1135. In Langbein, J, Puppe, B. (2004). Analysing dominance relationships by sociometric methods — a plea for a more standardised and precise approach in farm animals. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 87,293 – 315. Clutton-Brock, T. H, Albon, S. D, Gibson, R.M., Guinness, F.E. (1979). The logical stag: adaptive aspects of fighting in Red deer. Animal Behavior, 27(1),211 – 225. Šusta, F. (2008). (Displays of hierarchy inside Ring-Tailed Lemurs group during training). Gazela, Zoo Praha, 35, 135156 Šusta, F. (2011). Balance Index for Group Behaviors – A Mathematical Way of Finding “Where is the Trouble”. Wellspring, 12 (3, 4), 24-31. Šusta, F.; Drábová, J.; Holubová,, K.; Kamasová, K., Vodičková, K. (2013). New contexts for IB model- a mathematical way for group behaviors management. First international animal training conference, Twycross ZOO.in Press Key of Authors: František Šusta, Animal Training Specialist, Prague Zoo, Czech Republic, Europe Jana Drábová, University of South Bohemia in České Budějovice, Faculty of Agriculture, Czech Republic, Europe Kateřina Holubová, Charles University in Prague, Faculty of Arts, Czech Republic, Europe First author´s contact: RNDr. František Šusta, PhD. Animal Training Specialist of Prague Zoo, U Trojského zámku 120/3, Praha 7, Trója, 170 00, Czech Republic, Europe, [email protected]; +420 737 208 994 Key of institutions: Institution 1: Prague Zoo, U Trojského zámku 120/3, Praha 7, Trója, 170 00. Czech Republic, EUROPE *~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~*~* One of the core values of the ABMA states that: The sharing of knowledge and new ideas is fundamental to advancing animal behavior management. We do this in many ways, such as through our conferences, publications, and social media. Our written publications feature many fascinating and thought-provoking papers and articles. Some you may agree with, others may challenge your perceptions and ideas. And while the content that you read reflects the views of the author and does not necessarily represent the feelings of the ABMA or the board of directors, we think that the diversity of subjects and viewpoints represented by our members, at our conferences, in our publications, and via our social media outlets is one of the strengths of this organization. We encourage you to take in all that you read with an open mind, because you might be surprised by what you learn. Thank you and enjoy the publication! ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 12 Animal Art: How Do They Do It? Juliann Breeding Editor’s Note: This article is reprinted with permission from ZooNews – The Docent Newsletter for the Columbus Zoo Volunteer Community – Volume 45, Number 1 – Jan/Feb 2015. At Members’ Night at Wildlights, we played a guessing game. We asked the children and other members to guess who had created the animal paintings we had for sale. It was great fun to guess which animal might have made them and then try to figure out how they had done so. You may have played that game yourself, so here is a little more information about the processes the keepers go through with various animals. Artwork by Chris Lutmerding First though, a little history lesson: In 2005, keeper Alicia Shelley (North America) took notice that a zoo was having their animals paint, so she proposed that our animals and keepers try it. It took a few years to get it approved. Our first foray into animal painting was done by walking ponies and goats through paint and over a canvas board. Cher the swan painted the same way once, only for smelt. After that it was tried with other animals, using several techniques. Two main methods are used: One method is making a foot or handprint of an animal that is not easily handled, while it is under anesthesia for a medical procedure. Siamangs, gibbons, grizzly bear and kangaroos paint in this manner and several of our apes did early on. The second method is using food to motivate an animal to paint. The majority of our animals now paint in this way. Many animals move through the paint and then onto canvas, while some more purposely and actively paint; all usually with a food reward. Keepers try to use routines that are already part of the training or animals’ regular behavior. For example, Foli is our tiger who paints, and tigers are accustomed to transferring through the chute in the old lion building. Keepers gradually added the painting in small steps, such as first placing canvas on the floor of the chute, then paint and canvas. After the tiger was comfortable with walking through the paint and on the canvas once, the next time they asked him to transfer more than one time over the paint and canvas. Modifying a regular behavior is less scary for the animals than learning a whole new method. Amanda Schaffner (Asia Quest keeper) surmised Foli probably thought the keepers were a little crazy asking him to repeatedly transfer a few times. She related that as with any training, both the keepers and animals need to be comfortable before asking the animals to do a behavior, and it helps if the routine is already familiar. Of course the animal has to be in the mood to do so and sometimes the use of a snack such as a fish for an otter or seed for a cockatoo helps. Other animals that paint in this way are Photo of “Hanna’s Art” Courtesy of Grahm S. Jones red panda cubs, sun bears, red river hogs, Indian-crested porcupine, penguins, groundhog, prairie dogs, mountain lion, timber wolf, reindeer, and many of the snakes except one named Hanna. With Hanna the paint is placed on her scales and the canvas is stamped on her because she doesn’t move much. ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 13 The keepers try to take advantage of a normal behavior in an exhibit, but sometimes that doesn’t work, as with the kiwi. Dana Lintner (Australia keeper) mentioned that the kiwi generally walks the perimeter of the exhibit in the nocturnal building. So the keepers placed paint and canvas along the walls and the kiwi walked everywhere but where the paint and canvas were placed. Sometimes the keepers have to get very creative. When the keepers clean the feathertail gliders’ exhibit in the Roadhouse, they place the animals in a smaller enclosure. They placed paint and canvas on the floor of that enclosure. Ta-da! With the smaller space, many of the feathertail gliders walked across the paint and canvas to create a group painting. For painting with the polar and Alaskan brown bears, maintenance welded a special frame into which the keepers could place a canvas and paint which they could slide under the mesh, hoping the bears would put their feet on it but not pull it from the frame. Brutus and Buckeye and the polar bears did use it to paint without destroying it. The bats paint but don’t know it. The keepers set up the scale for a bat to climb down to, which is a regular training behavior. The other bats “know” they are supposed to let that bat alone to train. The keepers place a paint brush through a suet feeder and place a piece of fruit on the handle end of the paint brush. As the bat eats the fruit, it moves the brush loaded with paint and the keeper moves the canvas. Dude the Grand Cayman iguana and the Aldabra tortoises really seem to enjoy painting, Tara Archer (Shores keeper) thinks. They actively paint by moving through paint or pressing their feet, tails, or bodies in paint and against canvas boards held by keepers. When it comes to mandrills, vervet monkeys, and gorillas, they actively paint and make color choices. Within the gorilla group for example, Colo has definite color preferences and seems to like bright colors. Nia and Mo’ana are two of our best gorilla artists. Anakka likes just a few colors and licks the paints at first. Generally for these dexterous animals the paint and canvas are slid under the mesh and the animals use their fingers to trace through the paint and onto the canvas. Rosie and Kulinda black rhinoceros also paint. Rosie Photo of “Colo’s Art” Courtesy of Grahm S. Jones learned years ago at Zoo Atlanta. Mindi Scott (Pachyderms keeper) said that Kulinda has been trained to do so in the past year and she explained how it is done. These ladies use their pointed prehensile upper lips to paint. The rhino is asked to present her lip, the keeper applies the paint to it, and the keeper holds the canvas and gives a command to paint. Then the rhino moves her lip against the canvas and gets an apple chunk. The keeper washes the color off the lip and applies another color, using usually two or three colors for each painting. These artists have different styles, as the gorillas do. Kulinda is slow and methodical, making short broad strokes. Rosie is enthusiastic and moves both her lip and head, making longer strokes. Why do the keepers do this? Painting is an enrichment training event which helps the animals stay mentally active. To paint with the animals, first the keepers must have the time. It generally takes two keepers to create a painting, especially when an animal is moving and the canvas is manipulated, or if one keeper offers a treat and the other tends to the painting. The animal must also be in the mood as the keepers never force a behavior nor do they wish to stress any animal. The animal has to feel comfortable ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 14 with the keeper’s request, so baby steps are taken in training. The animals seem to enjoy the one-on-one attention and the keepers enjoy being able to have time with the animals. Photo of “Flamingo Art” Courtesy of Grahm S. Jones What do they use? Since many “painters” have sharp nails and teeth, canvas boards are used, which are less expensive and not as easily destroyed as canvas. Black background and burlap canvas are being tried but the colors don’t seem to show as well as on the white. Non-toxic kids’ paints and even glitter paints are used. The colors are chosen that don’t usually combine to make a brown color for obvious reasons. Also red is not used so visitors won’t think an animal is bleeding if every bit of color has not been washed off. After the paintings are completed, they are finished off with a clear spray to keep colors from running. Then a certificate is glued to the back. The certificate has a picture and name of the artist and some general biographical information about that animal. This touch was something one of our keepers noticed at a workshop in Los Angeles and thought would be nice to add. This great touch enabled us to play our guessing game with the children which in turn caught the interest of many parents. When are paintings sold and where does the sales money go? The funds raised by rhino paintings go to Bowling for Rhinos and Rhino Conservation. The funds raised at Wine for Wildlife go to the conservation project that is chosen for Wine for Wildlife that year. The paintings are also sold during Enrichment Day, Fall Fest, the last docent region meeting of the year, Members’ Night during Wildlights, and at the Business Office during December. Those funds go to the Jean Dixon Enrichment Fund which pays for many enrichment items for the animals. So as you can see, these activities benefit our animals and our Zoo in many ways. Stop and play our guessing game the next chance you get. Many thanks to the following people who helped provide information for this article: Enrichment Committee Members Dana Lintner, Australia and Tara Archer, Shores; Amanda Photo of “Cougar Art” Courtesy of Grahm S. Jones Schaffner, Asia Quest; Mindi Scott, Pachyderms; Samantha Frohlich, Heart of Africa; Alicia Shelley, North America. ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 15 Fire Hoses Do More Than Fight Fires! Micala Teetzen, Sedgwick County Zoo Enrichment Uses 1. Fire hose animals: Make heavy “carcasses” for predators such as large cats to carry around. Instructions are attached. 2. Fire hose hoops: Instructions are attached. 3. Boondoggles: These fire hose boondoggles stretch over time creating perfect places to hide treats, hang browse, etc. This was made using the "square stitch" (first you need to start with a “starter square stitch”). Visit www.boondoggleman.com for stitches. 4. Haynet: This is an elephant-size version that holds an entire bale of hay. 5. Spinning fire hose barrel: A simple enrichment device made by running a piece of fire hose through a hole in the bottom of an open-ended plastic barrel and tying a knot on each end. The fire hose should be a foot or two longer than the length of the barrel, so that the animal can grab either knot and run with the barrel spinning behind them. Fire hose animal Boondoggle Fire hose hoop Haynet Spinning barrel ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 16 Furniture Uses 1. Hammocks: Fire hose hammocks can be made in a wide variety of shapes and sizes. For example, there are tight-weave hammocks, loose-weave hammocks, square, rectangle, or triangle-shaped hammocks. Here are some of our unique designs: a. Papasan hammock: To create this hammock, the woven pieces are looped around the support section of hose so that they can slide (as opposed to attached in place along the cross piece). This allows them to sag together creating a comfy Papasan-like chair. b. Branch hammock: These have a slightly more natural appearance. Simply weave the hammock into a Y-shaped branch. This is perfect for smaller mammals such as lemurs, but can also be done on a larger scale for apes or cats. c. Helpful hint: PVC pipe can be inserted in the “arms” of hammocks to stiffen them if entanglement is a concern. This was done in our black bears’ (Ursus americanus) well-loved hammocks. Our male LOVES to play with his hammock and would potentially get his leg wrapped up in the long arms of his hammock if we hadn’t stiffened them with PVC. Triangle hammock Hammock with PVC in arms Branch hammock Slide PVC inside fire hose ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 17 2. Beds: Fire hose can be woven on a wood, metal, or PVC frame for a lightweight and comfortable bed. 3. Woven paths/vines a. Wide-width fire hose pathway: This is essentially a very long narrow hammock attached in several places along the length to create a rolling pathway. Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) use it for locomotion and napping. b. Woven vine path: This vine is made by weaving one fire hose back and forth in a figure eight between two straight fire hoses. c. “Browse hanger” weaving: This is an extended version of the “browse hanger weaving” developed by Keoni Pappas of Honolulu Zoo. Four full-length rolls of fire hose were used to create each section. Practice your weaving skills with ribbon or strips of paper. Instructions are attached. ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 18 d. Vines with leaves: Short pieces of fire hose are cut into leaf shapes and attached to the vine with a bolt, leaving the bottom side of the leaves open. The fire hose is then twisted to make it look more like a vine. Treats or diet items can be hidden inside the leaves to encourage foraging and/or they will randomly fall out when the vines are moved. Other Uses 1. Strip doors: Similar to refrigerator strip doors that keep the cold air in walk-in freezers, strips of fire hose are hung in doorways to off-exhibit holding areas. Weights can be added to the bottom of each strip to keep them taut. We bolt scraps of metal just inside the bottom of each strip for this purpose. These can be used with a variety of doors types (guillotine, sliders, Dutch doors, etc.). It is also easy to customize the strip doors depending on the size of the door opening and the species you are making it for. For example, we have tiny 30 cm x 30 cm doors into our heated Tammar wallaby (Macropus eugenii) houses that have strip doors made from lightweight fire hose. These strip doors serve numerous purposes including: • Keeping heat or cool air inside the buildings when the animals have access • Deterring birds or waterfowl from getting inside and eating grain or meat diets, building nests, or just leaving behind a mess • Fly spray can be sprayed on the strips to help keep flies out and the fly spray will also rub on the animals as they enter/exit through the strips. You will need to monitor new animals as they are introduced to strip doors to make sure they are comfortable entering the holding area. You may need to remove a few of the center strips until they are accustomed to the strip doors. Metal scrap weights Guillotine door ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 19 Slider door Dutch doors 2. Another way Sedgwick County Zoo uses fire hose is as a drain pipe to direct water away from our holding buildings. Tips for Working with Fire Hose: 1. A serrated knife cuts through fire hose like butter. It is far easier and safer than using a utility knife or sawzall and more practical than bandsaw. ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 20 2. A cordless drill is the easiest way to make holes in fire hose. However, it is very dangerous to use a drill if you are making a hole in the last few inches of a piece. The drill gets bound up and will wrench your wrist. For making a hole close to the end of a section of fire hose, sandwich the fire hose between two scraps of lumber, stand on the top of the lumber to hold it securely, then drill through the top piece (or a previous hole in the top piece) and the fire hose. Alternatively, a pocket knife blade also works to make a hole. Just make sure you have a blade that locks in position for safety. 3. You can “skin” double-layered fire hoses to remove the inner rubber layer. The advantages of this include: it is easier to work with, you will end up with a product that is much lighter in weight and it will prevent rubber ingestion in species where that is a concern. Steps to “skin” a fire hose: a. Clamp to the outer cloth layer to a sturdy structure such as a door frame. b. Use a second clamp to secure the inner rubber layer to the bed of a golf cart. c. Drive forward slowly pulling the layers apart as you go. A short piece of fire hose was used for demonstration purposes, but an entire 15.24 m roll can be skinned in this manner, you just need to make sure you choose a location where you can drive forward 30.5 m! ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 21 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 22 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 23 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 24 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 25 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 26 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 27 ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 28 References Pappas, K. (2005). Browse Hanger Weaving. Retrieved from http://www.honoluluzoo.org Teetzen, M. & Teetzen, D. (2003). Firehose Animals. Animal Keepers’ Forum, 30(5), 212-16. Weeks, D. (2010). Starter Square Stitch. Retrieved March 25, 2014, from http://www.boondoggleman.com/prj_starting_square.htm Weeks, D. (2010). The Square Stitch. Retrieved March 25, 2014, from http://www.boondoggleman.com/prj_square_stitch.htm ABMA Wellspring – Volume 15, Issue 1 – Winter 2015 – Page 29 Training & Enrichment at the Abilene Zoo 2014 Honors and Awards ++++++++++++++++++-+633333333 Behavior Management Welfare Award: Recognizes achievements that enhance animal welfare through specific environmental enrichment/conditioning techniques or programs – Fancy Footwork: Cheyenne Mountain Zoo’s Giraffe Herd Trains for Voluntary Farrier Work and X-rays. Amy Schilz – Cheyenne Mountain Zoo Behavioral Management Achievement Award: Recognizes an outstanding achievement in the application of behavior management techniques – Never Too Old: Voluntary Injection Training with a Geriatric Amur Tiger. Vicki Hardstaff – Toronto Zoo Behavioral Management Innovation Award: Recognizes an outstanding application of novel, unusual, or original behavior management techniques – Training Voluntary Reproductive Assessments and Artificial Insemination with African Elephants (Loxodonta Africana) in a Protected Contact Environment. Maura Middleton And Michael Burns – Tampa’s Lowry Park Zoo Impact Award: This award is chosen by all delegates at the end of the last formal presentation. Delegates may cast a vote for any paper, poster, or activity that they feel deserves special recognition – Training Voluntary Reproductive Assessments and Artificial Insemination with African Elephants (Loxodonta Africana) in a Protected Contact Environment. Maura Middleton And Michael Burns – Tampa’s Lowry Park Zoo Poster Presentation: Recognizes the best poster that represents an achievement in any of the award categories – “Stress Triangle” - The Four Questions for Which Animal Needs to Have an Answer. František Šusta – Prague Zoo, Gabrielle Harris – South African Association for Marine Biological Research, Tim Sullivan – Chicago Zoological Society – Brookfield Zoo Sharing the Knowledge: Recognizes achievements in behavioral management education to enhance the knowledge of professionals and/or the public to the benefit of animals in human care – Successful Reintroduction of a Beluga Whale and Calf Based on Observational Learning, Surrogate Rearing and Husbandry Training Techniques. Steven M. Aibel, Mark Galan – SeaWorld San Antonio Travel Scholarship Winner: A Tall List in Short Order: Developing a Positive Reinforcement Based Cheetah Training Program to Meet Programming and Collection Management Goals. Justin Garner – Busch Gardens Tampa
Similar documents
Wellspring
year. Sue was a charter member of ABMA and served on the Board of Directors for six years as President and Vice President. She was soft spoken, but a fountain of knowledge and a dedicated advocate ...
More information