Sexual Attraction, Sexual Identity, and Psychosocial Wellbeing

Transcription

Sexual Attraction, Sexual Identity, and Psychosocial Wellbeing
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
DOI 10.1007/s10964-012-9795-2
EMPIRICAL RESEARCH
Sexual Attraction, Sexual Identity, and Psychosocial Wellbeing
in a National Sample of Young Women During Emerging
Adulthood
Michelle Marie Johns • Marc Zimmerman
Jose A. Bauermeister
•
Received: 20 April 2012 / Accepted: 12 July 2012 / Published online: 31 July 2012
Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
Abstract Identity-based conceptualizations of sexual
orientation may not account adequately for variation in
young women’s sexuality. Sexual minorities fare worse in
psychosocial markers of wellbeing (i.e., depressive symptoms, anxiety, self esteem, social support) than heterosexual youth; however, it remains unclear whether these health
disparities exclusively affect individuals who adopt a sexual minority identity or if they also may be present among
heterosexually-identified youth who report same-sex
attractions. We examined the relationship between sexual
attraction, sexual identity, and psychosocial wellbeing in
the female only subsample (weighted, n = 391) of a
national sample of emerging adults (age 18–24). Women in
this study rated on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) their degree of sexual attraction to males and
females, respectively. From these scores, women were
divided into 4 groups (low female/low male attraction, low
female/high male attraction, high female/low male attraction, or high female/high male attraction). We explored the
relationship between experiences of attraction, reported
sexual identity, and psychosocial outcomes using ordinary
least squares regression. The results indicated sexual
attraction to be predictive of women’s psychosocial wellbeing as much as or more than sexual identity measures.
We discuss these findings in terms of the diversity found in
young women’s sexuality, and how sexual minority status
may be experienced by this group.
M. M. Johns (&) M. Zimmerman J. A. Bauermeister
Health Behavior and Health Education, School of Public Health,
University of Michigan, 1415 Washington Heights,
Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2029, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
123
Keywords Sexual orientation Emerging adulthood Women Mental health Measurement
Introduction
The period from adolescence to adulthood contains challenges unique to non-heterosexuals: coming out, facing
sexuality-related discrimination, and finding a place (or not)
in the larger lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) community
(Institute of Medicine (IOM) 2011). Navigating this time
period has many implications for the mental health of sexual
minorities–a term public health researchers use to represent
the full umbrella of non-heterosexual identities (Young and
Meyer 2005). Correspondingly, sexual minorities during this
developmental time period consistently report greater levels
of psychological distress than their heterosexual peers (Bos
et al. 2008; Cochran et al. 2003; IOM 2011). Lesbian, gay,
and bisexual identified adolescents and youth report more
symptoms of depression and higher levels of anxiety, and
lower levels of positive indicators of mental health like self
esteem than heterosexuals of the same age (IOM 2011). The
origins of these mental health disparities have been
explained through the sexual minority stress model, which
outlines how ownership of a sexual minority identity (i.e.,
lesbian, gay, bisexual) in a culture that privileges heterosexuality opens up the individual to a series of external (i.e.,
discrimination and prejudice) and internal (i.e., sexual
identity management and concealment) stressors that may
deplete mental health over time (Meyer 2003). Thus, young
sexual minorities encounter a climate which breeds these
disparities.
Pivotal in these social processes is the construct of social
support, as a sexual minority identity may influence the
amount of social support available within a social network,
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
and it may buffer the effect of sexual minority stress on
mental health outcomes (Meyer 2003). Ownership of a
sexual minority identity, for example, may lead some youth
to be rejected by their families and thus cut off from
familial support, and in turn, low levels of familial support
are associated with negative mental health outcomes like
depressive symptoms (Needham and Austin 2010; Ryan
et al. 2010; Williams et al. 2005). Similarly, sexual
minority youth may be less connected to their peers due to
their sexual orientation, and lack of connection can in turn
take a toll on mental health (Williams et al. 2005). Generalized measures of perceived social support have been
shown to mediate the relationship between sexual minority
status and mental health outcomes like depressive symptoms and self esteem in a sample of emerging adults
(Spencer and Patrick 2009). As such, social support from
family and friends is a critical component in the psychosocial well being of sexual minorities during emerging
adulthood.
While the validity and consistency of the relationships
between psychosocial wellbeing (e.g., depressive symptoms, anxiety, self esteem, and social support) and sexual
minority status are well acknowledged, less is known about
diversity in these relationships. Meyer’s (2003) sexual
minority stress model focuses on the centrality of a nonheterosexual sexual identity as the catalyst for disparities in
psychosocial well being; yet the psychosocial literature is
less clear about how these relationships may play out for
those whose sexuality falls outside of the heterosexual
mainstream, but who do not claim a sexual minority (e.g.,
lesbian, gay, bisexual) identity. The emerging adulthood
years are a time in which youth may be in the process of
exploring their sexuality (Brogan et al. 2001; IOM 2011;
Savin-Williams 2006), and a large body of literature suggests that female sexuality may be more fluid or plastic
than labels like straight, gay, or bisexual can accommodate
(Baumeister 2000; Diamond 2008; Diamond and SavinWilliams 2003; Russell and Consolacion 2003; Tolman
and McClelland 2011). In light of this information,
researchers who seek to understand psychosocial health
disparities among sexual minority women during the
emerging adulthood years may need to consider what
aspect of sexual orientation (i.e., identity, behavior, or
attraction) propels these differences.
Sexual Orientation in Health Research
Researchers have employed different operationalizations of
sexual orientation (IOM 2011; Narring et al. 2003; Saewyc
et al. 2004; Smith et al. 2003). Three primary dimensions
of sexual orientation are commonly used: disclosed sexual
behavior (i.e., male partners, female partners, both),
reported sexual identity labels (i.e., gay, lesbian, bisexual),
83
and expressed sexual attraction (i.e., male attracted, female
attracted, both) (IOM 2011; Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual
Youth Sexual Orientation Measurement Work Group (LGB
Measurement Work Group) 2003; Narring et al. 2003;
Saewyc et al. 2004; Smith et al. 2003). Each of these
dimensions has merits and deficits regarding who is
included and excluded in research, and who is placed in
one category or another for comparative analysis.
Sexual Behavior
A behavioral definition of sexual orientation has been
useful in the study of the transmission, diagnosis, and
treatment of sexually transmitted infections. This line of
sexual health inquiry distinguishes individuals engaging in
same-sex sexual behavior, regardless of their proclaimed
social identities, from those who engaged only in heterosexual sex. The value of this method of categorization can
be seen in examples such as an epidemiological study
comparing cross-sectional data from women who have sex
with women (WSW) and a control group of women who
were not WSW in order to assess the risk of contracting
sexually transmitted infections (Fethers et al. 2000), or
within the multitude of studies examining HIV transmission between male partners (Mustanski et al. 2011). From
an epidemiologic standpoint, the use of identity labels or
reported same-sex attractions would introduce bias into
studies of disease transmission, as those dimensions of
sexual orientation would exclude individuals who engage
in same-sex behavior but who do not identify as LGB, and/
or include individuals who harbor same-sex attractions yet
do not engage in same-sex behavior.
Notably, the aspects of behavioral measures of sexual
orientation that are ideal for examining STI transmission
are the same aspects that make behavior problematic for
consideration of the mental health of sexual minority
emerging adults. Developmentally, the teen and young
adult years are a time of marked sexual variation and
exploration, and a behavioral measure of sexual orientation
overlooks the complicated story around sexuality during
these years (Tolman and McClelland 2011). In places
where LGB communities are small or absent, for example,
emerging adults may claim a sexual minority identity label
like lesbian or gay, but not come into contact with any
potential romantic or sexual partners. Similarly, emerging
adults may harbor same-sex attractions, but not be out in
their identity or comfortable seeking out same-sex partners.
Both of these scenarios (among many other potential possibilities) describe emerging adults who would be constructing identities as sexual minorities, and thus would be
vulnerable to the processes of sexual minority stress, but
they would be excluded from a behavioral definition of
sexual orientation.
123
84
Sexual Identity
Identity labels are thought to avoid the reductive viewpoint
of sexuality that behavioral measures connote and account
for the personal salience and relevance of sexual orientation in the lives of sexual minorities. Young and Meyer
(2005) argue that when studying the health of sexual
minorities, the use of identity labels is essential, as any
other indicator risks erasing an individual’s self determined
sexual identity and overlooking the related social consequences of owning these often stigmatized identities. In
relation to mental wellbeing, sexual identity labels also
may help create bonds between individuals who share these
labels, facilitate access to social support, and promote the
development and visibility of the LGBT community (Doty
et al. 2010; Ramirez-Valles 2002). Doty et al. (2010), for
example, found knowing and interacting with other LGB
people to be associated with less psychological distress
among self-identified lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth. In
this study, use of sexual identity labels as the measure of
orientation was critical, as the process of identity affirmation and validation appeared to be driving these youths’
reported mental health status. Consequently, a sexual orientation measure based on behavior or attraction would
have been inappropriate for assessing these identity-based
relationships between social networks and mental health.
Of course, sexual identity labels also have their limitations. These identity labels reflect the current historical
understandings of gender and sexuality as defined by the
dominant cultural group, and thus these labels may not
represent the full scope of sexual minorities (LGB Measurement Work Group 2003). For example, some racial and
ethnic minorities in the US may be reticent to adopt labels
like lesbian and gay due to the fact that many report
experiencing racism from ‘‘mainstream’’ LGB culture as
well as homophobia among people of their own race/ethnicity—a dual burden of stigma that can create a barrier to
adoption of sexual minority identity labels (Be´rube´ 2001;
Dı´az et al. 2004). Alternatively, some researchers have
reported racial/ethnic differences in the coming out process; for instance, Black and Latino youth may come out or
adopt a sexual minority identity label later in life than their
White peers (Dube´ and Savin-Williams 1999). Thus, the
use of identity labels in sexuality surveys might inadvertently exclude Black and Latino sexual minorities during
emerging adulthood. Others have argued that, as social
acceptance for sexual diversity grows, some young people
may be turning away from traditional labels like lesbian
and gay, and either resisting labeling their sexuality altogether or creating new terms to denote non-heterosexual
identities (Savin-Williams 2006). In a recent sample of
teenagers involved in high school gay-straight alliances,
researchers found that the majority of non-heterosexual
123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
students still used traditional sexual identity labels like
lesbian, gay, and bisexual, but almost 30 % of the involved
teens categorized their sexual identity using language other
than LGB (Russell et al. 2009). In a cultural landscape
where these sexual identity designations carry multiple and
changing meanings, over-reliance on them to denote sexual
orientation may be limiting.
Sexual Attraction
The third approach to measuring sexual orientation, reported same-sex attraction, has been cited as capturing the
largest cross section of individuals, perhaps covering some
of the gaps left behind by behavior or identity measures
(Saewyc et al. 2004). In fact, several studies specifically
broaching this question of measurement with young people
found that attraction measures may be understood most
readily by this group (Austin et al. 2007; Friedman et al.
2004). In focus groups with adolescents, Friedman et al.
(2004) found that sexual attraction was discussed consistently as fundamental to the participants’ understanding of
sexual orientation, while both behavior and identity were
believed to be less relevant (i.e., persons could have sex
with someone to whom they were not attracted or claim an
identity for political reasons). In cognitive interviews with
youth, Austin et al. (2007) found that their participants
believed questions about attraction to males and females to
be self-explanatory in their intent and non-threatening in
their approach (i.e., people not wanting to claim a sexual
minority identity could still comfortably report some samesex attraction). Taken together, these studies appear to
indicate that attraction is a salient dimension of sexual
orientation among adolescents and emerging adults, and
thus explorations of attraction and health disparities may be
particularly well suited for these populations.
In 2003, the Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual (LGB) Youth
Sexual Orientation Measurement Work Group noted that
sexual attraction was a useful mechanism for investigating
mental health outcomes; however, to our knowledge, only
a few studies have undertaken this methodology for
examining sexual minority status and mental health among
emerging adults. Russell and Consolacion (2003) used
reported romantic attractions to same and other-sex partners along with relationship status to examine how these
two domains interacted to predict mental health outcomes
like depressive symptoms, anxiety, self esteem, and suicidal ideation. This study found that heterosexuallyattracted singles were (as expected) the most protected in
terms of mental health; however, same-sex attracted individuals who were in a same-sex relationship actually had
equivalently low levels of anxiety to heterosexuallyattracted singles (Russell and Consolacion 2003). The
results of this study give credence to attraction being useful
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
in the investigation of mental health outcomes, as well as
suggest that the social relationships of sexual minorities
may be important in shaping these outcomes; however, this
study did not differentiate between participants who were
exclusively same-sex attracted and those who harbored
attractions to both men and women. In a New Zealand
survey, Skegg et al. (2003) used participants’ reported
lifetime and current sexual attractions to divide their
sample of young adults into three groups (e.g., other-sex
attraction only, minor same-sex attraction, and persistent
major same-sex attraction) as a means to understanding
self-harm among sexual minorities. They found that persistent same-sex attraction (i.e., reporting same-sex
attraction both over the lifetime and currently) was related
to an increased likelihood of having inflicted self-harm.
Skegg et al.’s (2003) results lend further support to using
attraction as a marker of sexual minority status; however,
they too only investigated same-sex attraction as a unidirectional trait (i.e., present or absent). Furthermore, both of
these studies leave unexamined the question of whether it
is truly same-sex attraction that relates to psychological
wellbeing, or whether these findings would persist after
accounting for participants’ identity labels. The current
study attempts to further this line of research by differentiating between those with dual attraction to men and
women and those with exclusively same-sex attractions, as
well as determine whether the observable differences in
psychosocial well being associated with sexual attraction
are driven by sexual identity.
Given these considerations, researchers must take into
account the developmental reality of the segment of the
population with which they are working. Certainly, for
young people who still may be undergoing the processes of
sexual identity formation and coming out (Brogan et al.
2001; IOM 2011), identity labels may not be the most
appropriate means to measure sexual orientation. Behavior
measures may be similarly flawed in that young adults with
same-sex attractions may not yet be sexually active with
partners of the same-sex (IOM 2011). These gaps in
identity and behavioral measures suggest that another
measure such as same-sex attraction may be a useful tool in
assessing sexual orientation during the young adult years.
We address this issue by examining whether sexual
attraction was associated with psychological wellbeing,
after accounting for participants’ self-reported sexual
identity.
The Role of Gender
The inadequacies of identity or behavior-based approaches
to sexual orientation may be amplified when investigating
women at this developmental stage. Strong evidence suggests that the coming out process for women does not
85
follow a linear trajectory, and is distinct from that of men
in its timing and sequencing (Diamond 2008; Diamond and
Savin-Williams 2003). Women may come out at an older
age than men, may fluctuate between sexual identity labels
over time, and may go through periods of engaging in and
abstaining from same-sex sexual behavior (Diamond 1998,
2003, 2008; Diamond and Savin-Williams 2003). While
recent research focused on LGB-identified youths finds
women’s sexual identities to be more stable than males
(Rosario et al. 2006), studies of the general population (i.e.,
heterosexual and sexual minority females alike) suggest
greater variation. For example, Russell and Consolacion
(2003) found that heterosexually attracted adolescent girls
were seven times as likely to be in a same-sex relationship
as heterosexually attracted adolescent boys, results that
suggest that the boundaries of female sexuality may be
more nebulous than male sexuality. Naming this phenomenon erotic plasticity, Baumeister (2000) posits that
women’s sexual arousal and attractions may be more
flexible to social and cultural constraints than men’s. Given
young women’s high degree of fluidity in their sexual
identities (i.e., in this study we use the term fluidity to
describe both sexual identities that fall outside of rigid,
common cultural understandings of what it means to be
lesbian, bisexual, or heterosexual and ownership of different sexual identity labels over time) and variability in
their sexual behavior, health research relying solely on
measures of identity or behavior would be fated to miss
significant portions of young women; however, young
women’s reports of same-sex attractions might be inclusive
of a broader range of women.
Young women excluded by identity- or behavior-based
approaches to defining sexual orientation are often
neglected in research on sexual minorities even though
they may experience stress similar to more traditionally
defined minority status. In fact, evidence exists that these
women may experience more stress because of conflicts
between their identity or behavior, and their attractions. A
line of research exploring the social position of women
who identify their attractions as mostly heterosexual (i.e.,
thus not openly identifying as lesbian or bisexual, but
expressing some same-sex attraction) finds them to be a
population experiencing observable health disparities
(Corliss et al. 2009). Corliss et al. (2009) discovered that
women who reported their sexual attractions to be mostly
heterosexual also reported less social support and more
mental health and substance use problems than women
who reported their sexual attractions as exclusively heterosexual. This finding suggests that an individual need not
openly identify as lesbian and gay to be affected by some
of the consequences of sexual minority stress such as
depressive symptoms and substance use. Consequently, it
is important to examine whether women who have fluid
123
86
conceptions of sexuality are also vulnerable to sexualityrelated stress and decreased psychological wellbeing.
Current Study
Given the wide variation in how young women claim and
experience their sexuality, we examined whether the
attraction dimension of sexual orientation was related to
psychosocial wellbeing among women in their emerging
adulthood years (ages 18–24). First, we examined whether
participants’ attraction to men and women mapped onto
their self-reported identities and sexual behaviors. Consistent with prior findings (Baumeister 2000; Diamond 2008;
Diamond and Savin-Williams 2003; Russell and Consolacion 2003; Tolman and McClelland 2011), we hypothesized
that women’s self-rated attraction to males and females,
respectively, would map partially onto participants’ sexual
identities and behaviors. We then sought to understand how
reported same-sex attraction related to young women’s
psychosocial wellbeing (e.g., depressive symptoms, anxiety, self esteem, social support). We hypothesized that
female attracted women would have higher levels of mental
distress (e.g., depressive symptoms, anxiety) and lower
levels of self esteem and social support than women in the
male attraction group. Similarly, we hypothesized that
women attracted to males and females (bi-attraction) would
also have higher levels of mental distress and lower levels
of self esteem and social support than women who were
male attracted. Finally, we sought to examine whether these
relationships would persist even after accounting for participants’ self-reported sexual identities. Consistent with
prior literature suggesting that sexual identity measures
may be limited in their ability to describe female sexuality
(Diamond 1998, 2003, 2008; Tolman and McClelland
2011), we hypothesized that the observed relationships
between attraction and psychological well-being would
persist after accounting for sexual identity.
Methods
Sample
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
region of the U.S. to help insure a more nationally representative sample. The remainder of the sample was
recruited through referral chains from the original 22 seeds.
We computed a statistical weight to correct for the intraclass correlation that resulted from the network-referral
procedures (unweighted N = 3,448; weighted N = 829).
Description
For this analysis, we limited our analyses to the female subsample who had answered questions about sexual attraction
(weighted, n = 391; see Table 1). The mean age for this
group was 20.83 (SD = 1.82). Eight of these women identified as gay/lesbian, 23 as bisexual, and 357 as straight. Seventy
percent of the sample self-identified as White/European
American, 11 % Asian/Pacific Islander, 5 % Black/African
American, 9 % Hispanic/Latino, 1 % Native American, and
4 % Mixed Race/Other. The majority of our sample had some
college (51 %), a bachelor’s degree (16 %), or graduate
school (5 %) education. Thirty-six percent of the sample
resided in the Northeast of the United States, 22 % in the
Midwest, 28.9 % in the South, and 13.3 % in the West (i.e.,
categorized by US Census region and the state that participants reported currently residing in). See Table 2 for a
breakdown of these descriptive statistics by attraction group.
Procedure
Each prospective participant logged into the survey portal using
a unique identifying number (UID) and completed a short eligibility screener. Eligible participants read and consented to the
study, and completed the survey assessing their socio-demographic characteristics, Internet use, lifetime and recent alcohol
or other drug (AOD) use, lifetime and recent sexual behaviors,
and psychosocial variables (e.g., mental health, social support).
On average (median split), the questionnaire took 37 min to
complete. Participants received a monetary incentive for their
participation ($20 dollars) and were offered an additional $10
each for up to five additional young adults who were referred
into the study and completed the questionnaire. Incentives were
paid with a VISA e-gift card. Study data were protected with a
128-bit SSL encryption and kept on a secure firewalled server at
the University of Michigan. The study was approved by the
University of Michigan IRB.
Recruitment Strategy
Measures
Participants were recruited using Web-based respondent
driven sampling (webRDS) strategy (Bauermeister et al.
2012). To be eligible for the study, respondents had to be
between the ages of 18 and 24, live in the United States,
and have access to the Internet. The first wave of participants (seeds) was recruited through an online Facebook
advertisement and selected based on race/ethnicity and
123
Sexual Attraction
Our primary independent variable of interest was the
degree to which women reported being attracted to men
and women in the past year, respectively. Participants rated
on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) their degree
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
87
Table 1 Descriptive statistics of analytic variables by attraction (N = 391)
Total sample
Low attraction
Female attraction
Bi-attraction
Male attraction
Straight/heterosexual
357 (91.1 %)
149 (100.0 %)
37 (60.7 %)
46 (86.8 %)
125 (100.0 %)
Lesbian/homosexual
8 (2.0 %)
0 (0.0 %)
8 (13.0 %)
0 (0.0 %)
0 (0.0 %)
Bisexual/other
23 (5.9 %)
0 (0.0 %)
16 (26.2 %)
7 (13.2 %)
0 (0.0 %)
Sexual identity
Sexual behavior
Lifetime with a female
39 (9.9 %)
Lifetime with a male
273 (69.6 %)
30 days with a female
12 (3.1 %)
30 days with a male
188 (48.0 %)
4 (2.7 %)
22 (36.1 %)
10 (18.5 %)
3 (2.4 %)
84 (56.4 %)
44 (71.0 %)
47 (88.7 %)
98 (79.0 %)
0 (0.0 %)
10 (16.1 %)
2 (3.7 %)
0 (0.0 %)
52 (34.9 %)
26 (41.9 %)
33 (62.3 %)
77 (61.1 %)
Sexual attraction
Female attraction
Male attraction
1.52 (.97)
(1.00–5.00)
1.00 (.00)
2.94 (1.12)
2.54 (.83)
1.00 (.00)
4.06 (1.12)
3.26 (.97)
3.27 (.97)
5.00 (.00)
5.00 (.00)
1.90 (.52)
2.20 (.63)
2.12 (.64)
1.90 (.52)
1.91 (.80)
2.23 (.92)
2.24 (1.02)
1.96 (.80)
3.04 (.56)
2.86 (.57)
2.99 (.59)
3.12 (.52)
3.86 (1.20)
3.36 (1.28)
3.85 (1.17)
4.12 (1.08)
3.68 (.99)
3.65 (.95)
3.95 (.78)
4.13 (.79)
(1.00–5.00)
Psychological variables Depressive symptoms
1.98 (.57)
(1.00–4.00)
Anxiety
2.03 (.86)
(1.00–5.00)
Self-esteem
3.03 (.56)
(1.00–4.00)
Social support variables Maternal support
3.86 (1.19)
(1.00–5.00)
Peer support
3.86 (.92)
(1.00–5.00)
Values displayed in this descriptive table are the unstandardized means for each scale. In the regression models, all scale values were z-scored
Table 2 Additional descriptive statistics by attraction groups (N = 391)
Total sample
Low attraction
Female attraction
Bi-attraction
Male attraction
274 (100 %)
93 (33.9 %)
42 (15.3 %)
40 (14.6 %)
99 (36.1 %)
Race/ethnicity
White/Euro American
Black/African American
20 (100 %)
8 (40.0 %)
4 (20.0 %)
3 (15.0 %)
5 (25.0 %)
Hispanic/Latino
33 (100 %)
12 (36.4 %)
5 (15.2 %)
5 (15.2 %)
11 (33.3 %)
Asian/Pacific Islander
Other
45 (100 %)
18 (100 %)
28 (62.2 %)
8 (44.4 %)
8 (17.8 %)
3 (16.7 %)
4 (8.9 %)
1 (5.6 %)
5 (11.1 %)
6 (33.3 %)
Education level
High school or less
98 (100 %)
45 (45.9 %)
14 (14.3 %)
11 (11.2 %)
28 (28.6 %)
294 (100 %)
105 (35.7 %)
48 (16.3 %)
43 (14.6 %)
98 (33.3 %)
Northeast
139 (100 %)
54 (38.8 %)
24 (17.3 %)
16 (11.5 %)
45 (32.4 %)
Midwest
87 (100 %)
34 (39.1 %)
10 (11.5 %)
11 (12.6 %)
32 (36.8 %)
South
113 (100 %)
44 (38.9 %)
18 (15.9 %)
18 (15.9 %)
33 (29.2 %)
West
52 (100 %)
18 (34.6 %)
10 (19.2 %)
9 (17.3 %)
15 (28.8 %)
More than high school
US Census region
123
88
of sexual attraction to males and females on separate survey items (i.e., ‘‘During the past year, how sexually
attracted to (males/females) were you?’’). To address the
skewness in the attraction variables, we created attraction
categories that took into account participants’ relative
degree of sexual attraction to women and men as compared
to other women in the sample. Specifically, we standardized the male and female attraction values, respectively,
and mean split the z-scored attraction measures into high
and low attraction groups. Then, we utilized these cut offs
to place women into four groups: low attraction (low
female/low male attraction; n = 149), male attraction (low
female/high male attraction; n = 126), female attraction
(high female/low male attraction; n = 62), and bi-attraction (high female/high male attraction; n = 54).
Sexual Identity
Participants were also asked whether they identified as
straight/heterosexual, gay/lesbian/homosexual, bisexual, or
other. For analytic purposes, we collapsed bisexuals and
others into one category reflecting sexual identities other
than gay/lesbian or straight.
Sexual Behavior
Participants were asked two questions about their lifetime
sexual behavior with females and males, ‘‘Have you ever
had any sexual (genital) experiences with a female?’’ and
‘‘Have you ever had any sexual (genital) experiences with a
male?’’ Similarly, participants answered two questions
about their sexual behavior in the last 30 days, ‘‘During the
past 30 days, how many female sexual partners have you
had?’’ and ‘‘During the past 30 days, how many male
sexual partners have you had?’’ The responses to the sexual
behavior in the past 30 days questions were recoded into
dichotomous outcomes for both male and female partners:
Yes (male/female) sexual partners in the past 30 days; No
(male/female) sexual partners in the past 30 days.
Depressive symptoms
We examined participants’ degree of depressive symptoms
using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression
(CES-D) scale (Radloff 1977). The CES-D scale contains
eleven items designed to characterize symptoms of
depression (e.g., ‘‘I felt fearful’’ and ‘‘I could not ‘get
going’’’). Participants rated on a scale from 1 (rarely or
none of the time) to 4 (most or all of the time), how frequently they had experienced these feelings over the last
week. We took a mean score of the eleven items to use as
our outcome for depressive symptoms—high values indicated higher levels of depressive symptoms (a = .84).
123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
Anxiety
We evaluated anxiety using the Brief Symptom Inventory
(Derogatis and Melisaratos 1983). This scale contains six
items that describe common symptoms of anxiety (e.g.,
‘‘Nervousness or shakiness inside’’ and ‘‘Spells of terror or
panic’’). Participants rated on a scale from 1 (never) to 5
(very often) how often they had experienced these symptoms in the past week. The values of the six items were
mean scored to create a single item measure of anxiety
(a = .90)—high scores indicated greater levels of anxiety.
Self-Esteem
To measure self-esteem, we used the Rosenberg (1989)
Self-esteem scale, a ten item measure in which participants
rated their level of agreement regarding a series of statements designed to assess their feelings of self-worth on a
scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). We
mean scored these ten items, with higher scores indicating
more self-esteem (a = .89).
Maternal Support
We assessed maternal support using a 5-item perceived social
support measure, adapted from the Perceived Social Support
from Family Scale (PSS-Fa) (Procidano and Heller 1983).
Items included statements such as, ‘‘My mother or female
person who raised me enjoys hearing what I think,’’ and ‘‘I
have a deep sharing relationship with my mother or female
person who raised me.’’, and were also scored using a 5-point
scale (1 = not true to 5 = very true). We calculated maternal
support scores by taking the mean of these five items for each
participant. Higher scores indicated more support (a = .96).
Peer Support
We captured peer support using 5 items adapted from the
Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale (PSS-Fr)
(Procidano and Heller 1983). Participants completed this
scale, addressing relationships with friends (e. g., ‘‘I rely on
my friends for emotional support’’). This measure was
answered on a 5-point scale from 1 (Not true) to 5 (very
true). We calculated a mean score for peer support—higher
scores indicated more peer support (a = .92).
Data Analytic Strategy
We first examined and compared the three sexual orientation measures to each other using descriptive statistics. We
then ran a series of ordinary least squares (OLS) regression
models in order to examine the relationship between sexual
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
attraction and our social and psychological outcomes. OLS
was ideal for this analysis, as it provided a mechanism for a
multivariate assessment of several independent predictors
(i.e., participants’ attraction groups, participants’ self
reported identities) in relation to a single outcome variable
(i.e., depressive symptoms, anxiety, self esteem, social
support). For each outcome, we examined differences by
attraction categories (Model 1) using the male attraction
category as the referent group. In Model 2, we added
sexual identity variables as a second block, with heterosexual-identified women serving as a referent group. We
evaluated the standardized Beta coefficient to ascertain if
these two methods of capturing sexual orientation
explained different or overlapping portions of the variance
of our outcome variables. Due to concerns about powering
the analyses in Model 2 with the relatively small size of our
lesbian subgroup (n = 8), we also tested these associations
with lesbian and bisexual women collapsed into one sexual
minority category (n = 31). Because the results of the
sexual minority identity variable were consistent with our
original tests and we desired conceptual clarity around
lesbian and bisexual women possessing two distinct sexual
identities, we present only the results from the original
Model 2 structure. Furthermore, we also tested models that
examined social support as a potential mediator and moderator of the relationship between attraction and mental
health; however, none of these models were significant,
and we have omitted them from this discussion. Finally, we
conducted analyses to examine if the results changed by
including sociodemographic characteristics such as race/
ethnicity and education. We reran our regression models
including these domains, and found no meaningful relationships between these sociodemographic characteristics
and the outcomes, nor did the relationships between attraction, identity, and the outcomes vary once we accounted for
race/ethnicity and education. Thus, the final models presented exclude race/ethnicity and education variables. For
brevity, data from these additional models are not shown.
Results
Attraction as Measure of Sexual Orientation
We examined how the attraction categories (i.e., low
attraction, male attraction, female attraction, and bi-attraction) compared to other standardized markers of sexual
minority status (i.e., sexual identity, sexual behavior) (see
Table 1). Consistent with our first hypothesis, sexual identity
and sexual behavior variables mapped partially onto our
constructed attraction categories. The Pearson’s Chi-square
statistic of the comparison of attraction by reported identity
was statistically significant (v2 = 114.85, p \ .001, df = 6).
89
The low attraction and male attraction groups were composed entirely of women who identified themselves as
straight or heterosexual. Within the female attraction group,
61 % identified as straight or heterosexual, 13 % identified
as lesbian or homosexual, and 26 % identified as bisexual.
Within the bi-attraction group, 87 % identified as straight or
heterosexual and 13 % identified as bisexual.
Four separate analyses were run to examine attraction
groups by lifetime sexual behavior with females and males
respectively. Within the total sample, 10 % had some lifetime sexual experiences with women. Across our attraction
categories, 3 % of the low attraction women had some sexual
experience in their lifetime with women, versus 36 % of
female attraction women, 19 % of bi-attraction women, and
2 % of male attraction women. Within our total sample,
70 % of the women reported some lifetime sexual experiences with men. Across the attraction categories, 56 % of
low attraction women had some experiences with men,
versus 71 % of female attraction women, 89 % of
bi-attraction women, and 79 % of male attraction women.
Within the total sample, 3 % of participants had at least one
female sexual partner in the last 30 days. Across attraction
categories, 0 % of low attraction women, 16 % of female
attraction women, 4 % of bi-attraction women, and 0 % of
male attraction women had a female partner in the last 30 days.
In the entire sample, 48 % had at least one male sexual partner
in the last 30 days. By attraction category, 35 % of low
attraction women, 42 % of female attraction women, 62 % of
bi-attraction women, and 61 % of male attraction women had at
least one male partner in the last 30 days.
Depressive Symptoms
Consistent with our second and third hypotheses, both
bi-attraction and female attraction women on average
experienced more symptoms of depression than male
attraction women. When we tested the relationship between
attraction and depressive symptoms (see Table 3), we
found women in the bi-attraction group reported .44 standard deviations more depressive symptoms than women in
the male attraction group. Women in the female attraction
group also scored .55 standard deviations higher on the
CES-D than male attraction women. The addition of the
sexual identity variables in Model 2 did not change the
strength and direction of these relationships. As anticipated
in our fourth hypothesis, sexual identity was not uniquely
related to depressive symptoms after accounting for
attraction.
Anxiety
The anxiety results were similar to those for depressive
symptoms across attraction categories: the bi-attraction
123
90
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
Table 3 Mental health variables as a function of attraction and identity
Attraction model
Variables
Attraction and identity model
B
SE B
-0.15
0.09
b
B
SE B
-0.16
0.09
b
Depressive symptoms (n = 388)
Attraction
Constant
Low attraction (LM/LF)
0.03
0.12
0.01
0.03
0.12
0.01
Female attraction (LM/HF)
0.55***
0.15
0.2
0.45**
0.17
0.17
Bi-attraction (HM/HF)
0.44**
0.16
0.15
0.37*
0.16
0.13
0.08
0.38
0.01
0.32
0.22
0.08
Identity
Lesbian/homosexual
Bisexual/other
F
6.18***
4.10***
Anxiety (n = 389)
Attraction
Constant
-0.08
Low attraction (LM/LF)
-0.05
0.09
-0.08
0.09
0.12
-0.02
-0.05
0.12
-0.02
Female attraction (LM/HF)
0.32*
0.15
0.12
0.28
0.18
0.1
Bi-attraction (HM/HF)
0.34*
0.16
0.12
0.31
0.16
0.11
0.38
-0.03
0.23
0.06
Identity
Lesbian/homosexual
-0.2
Bisexual/other
F
0.23
3.46*
2.39*
Self-esteem (n = 387)
Attraction
Constant Low attraction (LM/LF)
0.15
0.09
–
-0.14
0.12
-0.07
0.15
0.09
–
-0.14
0.12
-0.07
Female attraction (LM/HF)
-0.45**
0.16
-0.16
-0.41*
0.18
-0.15
Bi-attraction (HM/HF)
-0.22
0.16
-0.08
-0.18
0.17
-0.06
Identity
Lesbian/homosexual
0.36
Bisexual/other
F
-0.3
2.85*
0.38
0.05
0.23
-0.08
2.39*
Male attraction (HM/LF) served as the referent group; * p \ .05; ** p \ .01; *** p \ .001
group scored .34 standard deviations higher on the anxiety
scale than participants in the male attraction group (see
Table 3). Participants in the female attraction group scored
.32 standard deviations higher on the anxiety scale than
those in the male attraction group. Yet, contrary to our
fourth hypothesis about attraction being independently
related to anxiety, when we added the sexual identity
variables to this model, the relationships between attraction
and anxiety disappeared.
this pattern would hold true for bi-attraction women as
well. In the model examining levels of reported self-esteem
across attraction (see Table 3), we found that female
attraction women reported .45 standard deviations lower
self-esteem than male attraction women. We noted no other
mean differences across attraction categories. Consistent
with our hypothesis about the role of sexual identity, the
strength and direction of these relationships did not change
with the addition of the sexual identity variables.
Self-Esteem
Maternal Support
In our examination of self esteem, we found support for our
second hypothesis that female attraction would predict
worse self esteem, but no support for our supposition that
We examined differences in the levels of maternal support
across attraction groups (see Table 4 for all social support
results). These results supported our ideas about female
123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
91
Table 4 Social support variables as a function of attraction and identity
Attraction model
Variables
B
Attraction and identity model
SE B
b
B
SE B
b
Maternal support (n = 384)
Attraction
Constant
0.20*
0.09
0.21*
0.09
Low attraction (LM/LF)
-0.21
0.12
-0.1
-0.21
0.21
-0.1
Female attraction (LM/HF)
-0.62***
0.15
-0.23
-0.43*
0.17
-0.16
Bi-attraction (HM/HF)
-0.21
0.16
-0.07
-0.15
0.16
-0.05
-0.39
0.38
-0.05
-0.53*
0.23
-0.13
Identity
Lesbian/homosexual
Bisexual/other
F
5.49***
4.46***
Peer support (n = 389)
Attraction
Constant
0.28***
0.09
0.28***
0.09
Low attraction (LM/LF)
-0.47***
0.12
-0.23
-0.47***
0.13
-0.23
Female attraction (LM/HF)
-0.51***
0.15
-0.19
-0.55**
0.17
-0.2
Bi-attraction (HM/HF)
-0.18
0.16
-0.06
-0.19
0.16
-0.07
Lesbian/homosexual
0.27
0.37
0.04
Bisexual/other
0.02
0.22
0.01
Identity
F
6.66***
4.09***
Male attraction (HM/LF) served as the referent group; * p \ .05; ** ; p \ .01; *** p \ .001
attracted women, but not about bi-attraction women. In
Model 1 (i.e., attraction variables alone), women in the
female attraction group reported lower maternal support
than women in the male attraction group (B = -0.62), but
we found no differences in maternal support for low
attraction or bi-attraction women. As anticipated, the
strength and direction of these relationships did not change
with the addition of the identity variables (Model 2);
however, unexpectedly, bisexual identity had a unique
effect on women’s reported level of maternal support:
those who reported their sexual identity as bisexual or other
had less maternal support than male attraction, heterosexual identified women (B = -0.53).
Peer Support
Finally, we modeled differences in levels of peer support
across attraction groups. In line with our a priori assumptions about the relationship between sexual attraction and
peer support, in Model 1 women in the female attraction
group reported lower levels of peer support than women in
the male attraction group (B = -0.51); however, we found
no significant differences in level of peer support for
bi-attracted women. Quite unexpectedly, women in the low
attraction group reported less peer support than women in
the male attraction group (B = -0.47). As predicted in our
hypotheses, in Model 2 the strength and direction of these
relationships did not change, once sexual identity was
included in the model.
Discussion
Young women experience their sexuality in multiple,
nuanced ways–exploring and defining burgeoning sexual
identities during emerging adulthood, owning different
sexual identities at different times, and contending with
sexual behaviors and attractions that may be seemingly
discordant with their current sexual identity (Baumeister
2000; Brogan et al. 2001; Diamond 2008; Diamond and
Savin-Williams 2003; IOM 2011; Rosario et al. 2006;
Russell and Consolacion 2003; Savin-Williams 2006).
While the health literature largely points to the fact that
sexual minorities during emerging adulthood experience
depleted psychosocial wellbeing as compared to heterosexual youth (Bos et al. 2008; Cochran et al. 2003; IOM
2011), less has been written on how the variation in young
women’s sexuality may influence these relationships. In
this study, we utilized young women’s self-rated sexual
attraction to women and men as a mechanism to expand
123
92
the scope of identity-based definitions of sexual orientation. By taking this approach, we found that psychosocial
wellbeing (i.e., depressive symptoms, anxiety, lower selfesteem, reduced social support) was compromised among
women who reported greater than average same-sex
attractions, and these effects appeared to operate independently from reported sexual identity. These results provide
evidence that, for young women, ownership of a sexual
identity may not be as relevant to sexuality-related disparities in psychosocial wellbeing as same-sex attraction,
and that ownership of same-sex attractions in a culture that
privileges other-sex attractions may be enough to compromise psychosocial wellbeing.
When examining our attraction measure of women’s
orientation to women’s reported sexual identity and sexual
behavior, we found that the sexual attraction groups we
created overlapped in theoretically sound ways with both
sexual identity and sexual behavior items. Lesbian identified women were all captured within our female attraction
group, while bisexual women were split between the
bi-attraction and female attraction groups. Similarly, most
individuals who reported sexual experiences with women
in their lifetime and in the past 30 days were categorized in
the female attraction and bi-attraction groups, though
notably, for the lifetime sexual behavior variable, some of
women who had female sexual partners could also be
found in our other two attraction categories—a finding that
validates the convention that women’s sexuality may be
more fluid or plastic than current explanatory categories of
sexuality permit (Baumeister 2000; Diamond 2008; Diamond and Savin-Williams 2003; Rosario et al. 2006;
Russell and Consolacion 2003). The degree of correspondence between our attraction categories and the other two
domains of sexual orientation (i.e., identity and behavior)
lends support to the predictive validity of these attraction
categories. Furthermore, by grouping women according to
whether they were on average more or less attracted to
male and female partners than other women in their age,
we appeared to expand the scope of the sexual orientation
measure. The accuracy and breadth of these attraction
categories may help account for some of the sexually fluid
women who do not identify with labels like lesbian or
bisexual and are not currently sexually active with female
partners (Diamond 2008).
Importantly, we found evidence that young women’s
possession of same-sex attraction opens them up to some of
the same disparities of psychosocial well being experienced by LGB identified people. Similar to the results of
Russell and Consolacion (2003), we found that women in
the female attraction and bi-attraction groups fared less
well than women in the male attraction group on measure
of depressive symptoms and anxiety, and women in the
female attraction group fared worse on measures of self-
123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
esteem. The current study also advances the literature on
sexual minority status and social support by demonstrating
that same-sex attraction among young women also relates
to differences in their social relationships during emerging
adulthood. Consistent with prior research with emerging
adults (Elkington et al. 2011), we noted that parental and
peer support had unique relationships with our outcomes of
interest. Furthermore, we noted disparities in peer and
parental support. In line with research on sexual minority
identities being associated with reduced social support
(Needham and Austin 2010; Ryan et al. 2010; Williams
et al. 2005), women in the male attraction group had the
highest levels of social support, and women in the female
attraction group had the lowest levels of social support.
The reflection of known trends in psychosocial disparities
among sexual minorities during emerging adulthood in this
study suggests that such an attraction measure of sexual
orientation does not weaken our ability to examine health
related consequences of sexuality-based stigma, and, in
fact, may capture young women who may not identify with
traditional lesbian or bisexual categories.
Indeed, what is compelling about our findings is the fact
that these trends in the connection between sexual minority
status and psychosocial outcomes were observable even
though the majority of women in our sample across the
female attraction and bi-attraction groups identified themselves as heterosexual. Previously, work on psychosocial
wellbeing among sexual minorities has assumed that the
ownership of a stigmatized sexual identity (e.g., lesbian,
bisexual) results in social stress, prejudice, and isolation
and, subsequently, to negative mental health sequelae
(Meyer 2003); however, the results of our study invite an
expanded explanation: an individual need not identify as
lesbian or bisexual to experience these phenomena. Samesex attractions, regardless of reported sexual identity,
appear to be associated with higher rates of mental distress.
In a heteronormative society where the expectation is that
sexual attraction is directed only at other-sex partners,
harboring more same-sex attraction than the majority of
your peers may result in psychological strain. This interpretation echoes what some researchers have discovered in
studying women who identify as mostly heterosexual in
that they experience many of the psychosocial phenomenon associated with sexual minority stress (Corliss et al.
2009).
Our results build on previous work utilizing attraction as
a method to examine psychosocial health disparities among
sexual minorities in that we simultaneously controlled for
participants’ sexual identities (i.e., lesbian, bisexual) as a
means to ensure that the relationships we observed could
not be better explained by these social identities (Russell
and Consolacion 2003; Skegg et al. 2003). In the majority
of our analyses, when we controlled for sexual identity, the
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
relationship between attraction and the psychosocial outcomes was frequently unchanged (as in the case of peer
support, depressive symptoms, self-esteem). In the case of
anxiety, the addition of sexual identity to the model erased
the significant relationships between attraction and anxiety;
however, none of the identity labels were related significantly to the outcome either, perhaps suggesting that
attraction or identity could be used to understand disparities of anxiety among sexual minority women. Consequently, for these outcomes, the possession of same-sex
attractions appears to be the fundamental driver of observable differences in psychosocial wellbeing among young
women during the emerging adulthood years, as much as
and in some cases more so than ownership of a sexual
minority identity.
The one exception to the lack of predictive power of the
sexual identity labels was found in the relationship between
maternal support, attraction, and identity. Initially, we
were interested in the unique mental health state of the
bi-attraction group, given the literature that expresses that
prejudice directed at bisexual people (i.e., bi-phobia or
bi-negativity) in heterosexual and LGB communities alike
may predispose bisexuals to worse health outcomes (Dodge
and Sandfort 2007; Klesse 2011). Interestingly, bi-attraction women in our sample reported equivalent degrees of
anxiety and depressive symptoms to female attraction
women, but fared better on self-esteem measures. This
result suggests that being attracted to men and women
while being heterosexually-identified does not predispose
women to the same degree of mental strain as those who
are predominantly attracted to women. Yet, in the maternal
support analysis, bisexual identity was related to less
maternal support, even after accounting for same-sex
attraction. Possibly, ownership of bisexual identity, which
may be stigmatizing in both straight and LGB contexts,
may carry burdens not accounted for by attraction variables
(Klesse 2011). Therefore, researchers interested in the
study of bisexuality may need to pay special attention to
the use of identity labels around sexual orientation.
Limitations and Strengths
This study had a few limitations. Given the skewness of the
attraction variables and the relatively few number of
women who reported high levels of same-sex attraction, we
were unable to use the attraction measures as continuous
scales. Instead, we opted to put women into categories
according to their relative degree of sexual attraction to
women and men as compared to other women their age.
This approach, while helpful in providing clarity in our
comparisons, may have masked some of the more nuanced
differences of women’s sexual fluidity as it relates to
psychosocial wellbeing. Future research might benefit from
93
testing the predictive properties of attraction scales as
continuous rather than categorical. A second limitation of
this inquiry is the fact that we were constrained to only a
quantitative exploration of the efficacy of attraction as a
measure of sexual orientation. In the future, a mixed
methods approach would be useful. Incorporating a qualitative component to the study would help explore the
meaning of the reported same-sex attractions of heterosexual-identified women in our female attraction and
bi-attraction groups. The integration of qualitative methods
with the quantitative approach would allow a deeper
understanding of the saliency of same-sex attraction in
these young women’s lives, especially in relation to the
way they perceive their support networks and day to day
emotional state. With regard to our social support variables, we were able to include measures of maternal and
peer support, but due to missing data could not explore the
relationship of sexual attraction and paternal support. This
area remains important and understudied, and we recommend research to reveal how sexual orientation may
interplay with social support from friends and family.
Finally, despite a purposeful sampling strategy designed to
illicit diversity across race and region in the US, our final
sample underrepresented Black/African–American and
Hispanic/Latino women. Continued inquiries into how
psychosocial wellbeing relates to sexual orientation in
these populations is warranted.
The above limitations notwithstanding, this study provides several unique contributions to our understanding of
same-sex attraction. First, our sampling strategy of webRDS aided in the recruitment of a national sample of
emerging adults. Consequently, our final sample of women
was diverse in age, region, and education—a unique asset
in the study of sexual minorities. Second, our data were
collected via web survey. Web data collection may
encourage the elicitation of honest responses to sensitive
subject matters such as sexual attractions, sexual behaviors,
and sexual identity (Pequegnat et al. 2007). Third, we
included psychometrically sound and widely used psychosocial measures which allow for useful comparisons
across groups, populations, and studies. Fourth, we controlled for identity measures to examine the unique effects
of our same-sex attraction measures. This decision may be
the most important contribution as it is one of the few
studies that included both measures in the same analysis
and in a nationally representative sample.
Conclusion
Our results invite a rethinking of how young women
experience their sexual orientation in relation to their
psychosocial well being. Previously, theorists and
researchers have surmised that ownership of a sexual
123
94
minority identity like lesbian, gay, or bisexual opens up an
individual to discrimination and stress that may deplete
psychosocial wellbeing (Meyer 2003). In our examination
of psychological well being and same-sex attraction in
sample of women during emerging adulthood, we found
that possession of greater than average same-sex attraction
was linked to disparities in psychosocial wellbeing even
when women self identified as heterosexual. These findings
point to two things. First, sexual attraction may be a more
all-inclusive means to understanding the full range of
diversity of young women’s expressions of sexuality
(Baumeister 2000; Diamond 2008; Diamond and SavinWilliams 2003; Tolman and McClelland 2011). Second,
the possession of same-sex attraction may reflect the
observed disparities in psychosocial well being across
sexual orientation among youth (Bos et al. 2008; Cochran
et al. 2003; IOM 2011). While certainly the identification
of oneself as lesbian, gay, or bisexual may open up a
person to external stressors that can negatively influence
psychosocial well being, the possession of same-sex
attractions may be the internal mechanism that leads
women of this age group to feel different from their peers,
regardless of how they choose to identify their sexual
orientation. Accordingly, we recommend that future
research into the psychosocial wellbeing of sexual minority
women during emerging adolescence consider use of samesex attraction as a more experientially accurate assessment
of sexual orientation for this group.
Acknowledgments Data for this project was collected as part of a
five-year National Institute of Mental Health grant, 1RC1DA02806101 (‘‘Virtual Network Influences on Young Adults’ Alcohol and Drug
Use’’), awarded to Drs. Zimmerman and Bauermeister. Dr. Bauermeister is supported by a Career Development Award from the
National Institute of Mental Health (K01-MH087242). Views
expressed in this manuscript do not necessarily represent the views of
the funding agencies.
References
Austin, S. B., Conron, K., Patel, A., & Freedner, N. (2007). Making
sense of sexual orientation measures: Findings from a cognitive
processing study with adolescents on health survey questions.
Journal of LGBT Health Research, 3(1), 55–65.
Bauermeister, J. A., Zimmerman, M. A., Johns, M. M., Glowacki, P.,
Stoddard, S. & Volz, E. (2012). Innovative recruitment using
online networks: Lessons learned from an online study of alcohol
and other drug use utilizing a web-based respondent driven
sampling (webRDS) strategy. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and
Drugs, 73(5).
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The
female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 347–374.
Be´rube´, A. (2001). How gay stays white and what kind of white it
stays. In B. B. Rasmussen (Ed.), The making and unmaking of
whiteness (pp. 234–265). Durham: Duke University Press.
123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
Bos, H. M. W., Sandfort, T. G. M., de Bruyn, E. H., & Hakvoort,
E. M. (2008). Same-sex attraction, social relationships, psychosocial functioning, and school performance in early adolescence.
Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 59–68.
Brogan, D., Frank, E., Elon, L., & O’Hanlan, K. A. (2001).
Methodologic concerns in defining lesbian for health research.
Epidemiology, 12(1), 109–113.
Cochran, S. D., Sullivan, J. G., & Mays, V. M. (2003). Prevalence of
mental disorders, psychological distress, and mental services use
among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(1), 53–61.
Corliss, H. L., Austin, S. B., Roberts, A. L., & Molnar, B. E. (2009).
Sexual risk in ‘‘mostly heterosexual’’ young women: Influence of
social support and caregiver mental health. Journal of Women’s
Health, 18(12), 2005–2010.
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom
inventory: An introductory report. Psychology and Medicine,
13(3), 595–605.
Diamond, L. M. (1998). Development of sexual orientation among
adolescent and young adult women. Developmental Psychology,
34(5), 1085–1095.
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A
biobehavioral model distinguishing romantic love and sexual
desire. Psychological Review, 110(1), 173–192.
Diamond, L. M. (2008). Sexual fluidity: Understanding women’s love
and desire. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Diamond, L. M., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2003). The intimate
relationships of sexual-minority youths. In G. R. Adams &
M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence
(pp. 393–412). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Dı´az, R. M., Ayala, G., & Bein, E. (2004). Sexual risk as an outcome
of social oppression: Data from a probability sample of Latino
gay men in three U.S. cities. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 10(3), 255–267.
Dodge, B., & Sandfort, T. G. M. (2007). A review of mental health
research on bisexual individuals when compared to homosexual
and heterosexual individuals. In B. A. Firestein (Ed.), Becoming
visible: Counseling bisexuals across the lifespan (pp. 28–51).
New York: Columbia University Press.
Doty, N. D., Willoughby, B. L. B., Lindahl, K. M., & Malik, N. M.
(2010). Sexuality related social support among lesbian, gay, and
bisexual youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39,
1134–1147.
Dube´, E. M., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (1999). Sexual identity
development among ethnic sexual- minority male youths.
Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1389–1398.
Elkington, K. S., Bauermeister, J. A., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2011).
Do parents and peers matter? A prospective socio-ecological
examination of substance use and sexual risk among African
American youth. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 1035–1047.
Fethers, K., Marks, C., Mindel, A., & Estcourt, C. S. (2000). Sexually
transmitted infections and risk behaviours in women who have
sex with women. Sexually Transmitted Infections, 76, 345–349.
Friedman, M. S., Silvestre, A. J., Gold, M. A., Markovic, N., SavinWilliams, R. C., Huggins, J., et al. (2004). Adolescents define
sexual orientation and suggest ways to measure it. Journal of
Adolescence, 27(3), 303–317.
Institute of Medicine (IOM). (2011). The health of lesbian, gay,
bisexual, and transgender people: Building a foundation for better
understanding. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Klesse, C. (2011). Shady characters, untrustworthy partners, and
promiscuous sluts: Creating bisexual intimacies in the face of
heteronormativity and biphobia. Journal of Bisexuality, 11(2/3),
227–244.
Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Youth Sexual Orientation Measurement
Work Group (LGB Measurement Work Group). (2003).
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:82–95
Measuring sexual orientation of young people in health
research. San Francisco, CA: Gay and Lesbian Medical
Association.
Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in
lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and
research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 674–697.
Mustanski, B. S., Newcomb, M. E., Du Bois, S. N., Garcia, S. C., &
Grov, C. (2011). HIV in young men who have sex with men: A
review of epidemiology, risk and protective factors, and
interventions. Journal of Sex Research, 48(2–3), 218–253.
Narring, F., Stronski Huwiler, S. M., & Michaud, P. A. (2003).
Prevalence and dimensions of sexual orientation in Swiss
adolescents: A cross sectional survey of 16 to 20-year-old
students. Acta Paediatrica, 92, 233–239.
Needham, B., & Austin, E. (2010). Sexual orientation, parental
support, and health during the transition to young adulthood.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(10), 1189–1198.
Pequegnat, W., Simon Rosser, B. R., Bowen, A. M., Bull, S. S.,
DiClemente, R. J., Bockting, W. O., et al. (2007). Conducting
internet-based HIV/STD prevention survey research: Considerations in design and evaluation. AIDS and Behavior, 11(4),
505–521.
Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social
support from friends and from family: Three validation studies.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1–24.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression
scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.
Ramirez-Valles, J. (2002). The protective effects of community
involvement for HIV risk behavior: A conceptual framework.
Health Education Research, 17(4), 389–403.
Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., Hunter, J., & Braun, L. (2006).
Sexual identity development among lesbian, gay, and bisexual
youths: Consistency and change over time. Journal of Sex
Research, 43(1), 46–58.
Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image, revised
edn. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.
Russell, S., Clarke, T., & Clary, J. (2009). Are teens ‘‘post-gay’’?
Contemporary adolescents’ sexual identity labels. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 38(7), 884–890.
Russell, S. T., & Consolacion, T. B. (2003). Adolescent romance and
emotional health in the United States: Beyond binaries. Journal
of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 32(4), 499–508.
Ryan, C., Russell, S. T., Huebner, D., Diaz, R., & Sanchez, J. (2010).
Family acceptance in adolescence and the health of LGBT young
adults. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing,
23(4), 205–213.
Saewyc, E. M., Bauer, G. R., Skay, C. L., Bearinger, L. H., Resnick,
M. D., Reis, E., et al. (2004). Measuring sexual orientation in
adolescent health surveys: Evaluation of eight school-based
surveys. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35(4), 345e1–345e15.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it matter? Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 15(1), 40–44.
Skegg, K., Nada-Raja, S., Dickson, N., Paul, C., & Williams, S.
(2003). Sexual orientation and self-harm in men and women.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 541–546.
Smith, A., Rissel, C. E., Richters, J., Grulich, A. E., & de Visser,
R. O. (2003). Sexual identity, sexual attraction and sexual
experience among a representative sample of adults. Australian
and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 27(2), 138–145.
95
Spencer, S., & Patrick, J. (2009). Social support and personal mastery
as protective resources during emerging adulthood. Journal of
Adult Development, 16(4), 191–198.
Tolman, D. L., & McClelland, S. I. (2011). Normative sexuality
development in adolescence: A decade in review, 2000–2009.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(1), 242–255.
Williams, T., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2005). Peer
victimization, social support, and psychosocial adjustment of
sexual minority adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
34(5), 471–482.
Young, R. M., & Meyer, I. H. (2005). The trouble with ‘‘MSM’’
and ‘‘WSW’’: Erasure of the sexual-minority person in public
health discourse. American Journal of Public Health, 95(7),
1144–1149.
Author Biographies
Michelle Marie Johns is a doctoral student in Health Behavior and
Health Education from the University of Michigan. She holds a MPH
from the University of Michigan and a BA in Psychology and Gender
Studies from Whitman College in Walla Walla, WA. She is a
Graduate Student Instructor in UM’s Department of Women’s
Studies, and an active member of Sexperteam, University of
Michigan’s peer sex education group. Her research interests focus
on health and wellbeing among women and LGBT people.
Marc Zimmerman is Chair and Professor in the Department of
Health Behavior and Health Education at the University of Michigan
School of Public Health. Dr. Zimmerman’s research focuses on
adolescent health and resiliency and empowerment theory. His work
on adolescent health examines how positive factors in adolescent’s
lives help them overcome risks they face. His research includes
analysis of adolescent resiliency for risks associated with alcohol and
drug use, violent behavior, precocious sexual behavior, and school
failure. He is also studying developmental transitions and longitudinal
models of change. Dr. Zimmerman’s work on empowerment theory
includes measurement and analysis of psychological and community
empowerment. The research includes both longitudinal interview
studies and community intervention research. Dr. Zimmerman is the
Director of the CDC funded Prevention Research Center of Michigan.
He is also the Principal Investigator for the CDC funded Youth
Violence Prevention Center. Dr. Zimmerman is the Editor of Youth &
Society and is a member of the editorial board for Health Education
Research.
Jose A. Bauermeister is the John G. Searle Assistant Professor in
Health Behavior and Health Education and Director of the Sexuality
& Health Lab (SexLab) at the University of Michigan’s School of
Public Health. His primary research interests focus on sexuality and
health, and interpersonal prevention and health promotion strategies
for high-risk adolescents and young adults. He is Principal Investigator of a NIH K01 grant to examine HIV/AIDS risk among young
men who have sex with men (YMSM), and Co-Principal Investigator
of the Virtual Network Study. Dr. Bauermeister is member of the
Editorial Board of the Journal of Youth & Adolescence, AIDS &
Behavior, and Archives of Sexual Behavior.
123