edouard sequin
Transcription
edouard sequin
Discipline, liberty and pedagogy towards social and cultural independence through the regulation of activity and attention in a Montessori classroom Susan Feez University of New England 1 Discipline and liberty tend to be thought of as mutually exclusive in the domain of pedagogy. Disciplined children learning discipline knowledge, conventional wisdom suggests, cannot simultaneously be children free to engage in spontaneous activity following their own interests. That’s probably the one thing the back-to-basics traditionalists and the constructivist, progressive educators would agree on. Liberty and Discipline … discipline is very closely connected with freedom and action. It is generally thought that discipline and freedom are opposed to each other. The child has taught us, that they represent the two faces of the same coin, two faces of the same action. […] If there is no freedom there is no discipline; if there is no discipline there is no freedom. Thus freedom and discipline are the same thing […] If adults arrive at discipline and freedom in this fashion a great social problem will be solved. Maria Montessori (India, 1939) 2 Liberty and discipline, however, were considered by Dr Maria Montessori (1998 [1939], p. 41) to be inseparable; you can’t have one without the other, and this idea is embodied in the pedagogy she devised a century ago. Montessori (1946, p. 12) surrounded children with ‘things to handle which in themselves convey steps in culture’ because she observed that children have ‘natural aptitudes to easy acquisition of culture’. Designing objects to convey culture to children became her life’s work. Her legacy is a systematic and interrelated array of objects and graded exercises, covering all educational disciplines for children from infancy to the age of twelve years. This system, largely unaltered, continues to be used today in thousands of schools around the world. In Montessori classrooms children interact with the sets of objects, using precise movement and exact language to achieve specific purposes. The configuration of objects, movement and language can be thought of as multimodal ensembles of learning resources. Through their interaction with the Montessori ensembles, children learn both everyday and educational knowledge. The emphasis on liberty indicates that Montessori’s pedagogy has its origin in the Enlightenment, but the equal emphasis on discipline suggests its origin does not lie in the well-known Enlightenment tradition which begins with Rousseau’s famous story of the education of a fictional boy called Émile. Metalanguage Semiotic Mediation: a transdisciplinary view (Hasan) o Developmental psychology (Vygotsky (Vygotsky)) o Sociology of education (Bernstein (Bernstein)) o Social semiotics (Halliday (Halliday)) 3 The metalanguage I’ll be using to explore the socio-cultural evolution of Montessori pedagogy, and the use of the Montessori objects, is drawn from three disciplines: developmental psychology, following Vygotsky, sociology of education, following Bernstein, and social semiotics following Halliday. The unifying theme is Hasan’s transdisciplinary explanation of semiotic mediation, which brings together ‘the natural and the social’ in the process of human development (Hasan 2005 [1995], p. 107). 3 Developmental psychology(Vygotsky) o genetic accounts of human development across different spans of time (phylogenesis, socio-cultural genesis, ontogenesis, microgenesis) o scientific concepts 4 Development is modelled by Vygotsky as a process of adaption. The means of adaptation are (1) tools for interacting with and mastering the natural environment through practical action, and (2) signs for interacting with others in the social environment and for mastering oneself through meaning. In Vygotsky’s genetic account of development, changes in mediation mark critical steps or turning points. In phylogenesis, the domain of evolutionary time, the critical developmental step is the use of physical tools by primates to change their external physical world In socio-cultural genesis, historical time, the critical step is the use of signs by humans to master their behaviour, ie psychological tools to change their internal psychological world. In ontogenesis, the span of one life time, the critical step is the interweaving in childhood of two strands of development, the elementary biological strand and higher, cultural strand. There are a number of transitional forms between the elementary and higher functions’, transitions relating to the development of symbolic activity, from speech, to the use of signals and finally signs (Vygotsky and Luria 1994 [1930], p. 148). This developing symbolic activity frees the consciousness from the spatial and temporal limits of the immediate environment. The term microgenesis is not used by Vygotsky, but it has been coined by scholars modelling development within the timeframe of minutes or hours, in order to look closely, to use Vygotsky’s words, at the ‘process in flight’. This is the domain relevant to a child’s use of the Montessori ensembles of objects, movement and language. I have interpreted these ensembles as a type of semiotic mediation which sparks critical steps or turning points in the development of everyday and educational knowledge at the level of microgenesis. 4 Sociology of education (Bernstein) o categories of knowledge (everyday/educational) o recontextualisation of knowledge as pedagogic discourse o visible and invisible pedagogies 5 Bernstein’s sociology of pedagogy, in Hasan’s words foregrounds ‘the dialectic of the social and the semiotic’ (Hasan 2005a, p. 33). Most relevant to today’s talk about Montessori pedagogy is Bernstein’s metalanguage for talking about categories of knowledge, the recontextualisation of fields of knowledge as pedagogic discourse, in both its regulative and instructional aspects, and the distinction between visible and invisible pedagogies. Just as language imposes order and centres of relevance on an otherwise undifferentiated mass of potential thought and experience, in Bernstein’s model, pedagogy imposes order and centres of relevance on the otherwise undifferentiated mass of potential knowledge. Language has grammar to organise meaning into structures which can then be given reality in an expression form and pedagogy activates a system of principles through which knowledge is given reality in learning contexts. Today I want to draw attention to the system of principles which underpin Montessori pedagogy. 5 Social semiotics (Halliday) o language based theory of learning o text as unit of analysis (logogenesis) o grammatics 6 Social semiotics in Hasan’s words offers an analytical framework which goes ‘beyond action into interaction’ (Hasan 2005b, p. 146), enabling a close investigation of the mediating work of signs. Of most value to an exploration of Montessori pedagogy are first, Halliday’s language based theory of learning as a means of reinforcing and developing Vygotsky’s account of ontogenesis, second, the identification of text as the unit of analysis, combined with an expansive social semiotic metalanguage which can be used to describe the meaning relations within the multimodal configuration of objects, movement and language found in the ensembles of learning resources used in Montessori classrooms. The limits of time will mean only a brief glimpse into examples of the social semiotic analysis of the Montessori ensembles will be possible today. 6 Discipline in the Montessori tradition Discipline in the Montessori tradition encompasses both regulation and knowledge. o regulation ordering activity in the shared social space in which learning takes place o knowledge ordering what is to be learned, using knowledge structures as shared instruments of perception 7 Recontextualising both everyday and educational knowledge for learners in ways that capture attention and build concentration is the central concern of Montessori pedagogy. To achieve this, the pedagogy foregrounds disciplinarity, both as it relates to the learners themselves and to the instructional content. Even if the learners are very young, two and three years old, more emphasis is placed on discipline in Montessori settings than the more familiar early childhood emphasis on creativity, imagination and play. This may sound grim for little kids, but proponents of the Montessori approach argue that, in fact, discipline interlocked with liberty enhances learners’ capacity for playful, imaginative and creative interaction with their environment. Today I want to talk about the less visible Enlightenment tradition from which Montessori pedagogy is derived, a tradition with its origin in the work of educators trying to help real children overcome very real developmental challenges. 7 prepared environment society in embryo o distinctive materials (aesthetics+interest) o freedom of choice o blocks of unscheduled time o freedom to work indoors or outdoors o 3 year age span 8 First, I’m going to show you some clips from a video about Montessori pedagogy; the video is called Maria Montessori: Her life and legacy. It was prepared by Dr Annette Haines, who is a member of the Association Montessori Internationale pedagogical committee, based in Amsterdam (Haines 2004). [It is not possible to incorporate video clips into the pdf version of this presentation. Incomplete excerpts from the video can be seen on the following website: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=L2A3-z0qdCw ] In these clips you’ll see footage of well-established Montessori schools in the United States and hear the voice of Annette Haines. Dr Haines’s commentary exemplifies discourse used within the community of Montessori practitioners today, a discourse handed down in an oral tradition. I would like to emphasise that these are exemplary classrooms and edited footage. There are, obviously, among the many thousands of Montessori schools worldwide huge variations in classroom resources, teacher expertise and quality control. Some schools are better than others and Montessori teachers, like teachers everywhere, have better days than others. As important as variations in instances of Montessori practice are, today I would like to focus on the potential of this approach so we can consider some broader implications for teaching and learning in general. Each video clip draws your attention to key features of the Montessori tradition. Giving children opportunities build discipline in both the sense of self-regulation and knowledge, requires, in the Montessori tradition, a specially prepared environment, characterised by distinctive materials, choice, time and three year age range to enable the evolution of a mini-community. 8 sensory foundation of the intellect o ordering sensory impressions (contrast, grade) o language o abstract (portable) knowledge o observation judgement comparison 9 The development of the intellect has its origin in the ordering, categorising and cataloguing of sensory experience, ‘fixed’ by language. 9 mathematics and language o concrete abstract/symbolic o manipulative/ interactive o precise/exact o challenging 10 Educational (discipline) knowledge has its origin in ordered sensory experience, which makes way for abstract, symbolic expression. 10 liberty and discipline o grace and courtesy o freedoms (liberty) - movement - work - time - companions o limits (discipline) - physical space - knowledge structure (designed into materials) - demands of social group - lack of knowledge o judgement independence Willpower is built up by the routines of life itself, by the little decisions of daily living (Haines 2004) 11 The will power which underwrites the development of both liberty and discipline has its origin in daily social life. Following this video clip is a short clip from the same vidoe of a Montessori classroom prepared for children aged from six to twelve years. 11 Maria Montessori (1870-1952) 12 In the 1890s, in Rome, Maria Montessori trained first in mathematics and the physical sciences, then in the biological sciences as part of her medical degree. Later she studied, researched and practised in the fields of paediatrics, psychiatry and orthophrenics, or defectology, as Vygotsky called this field. As a young doctor Montessori came face to face with much human suffering in turn of the century Italy, particularly among women and children living in poverty, and the ‘defective’ children in the orthophrenic institute she directed. She was quick to realise that the long term solutions to these problems were social rather than medical. She campaigned for improvements in housing, sanitation, hygiene, ante-natal and post-natal care and the rights of women and children in general. Education became the treatment of choice for her ‘defectives’, and she succeeded in educating many to the point where they could pass the public exams. Very quickly she became famous, lecturing and campaigning throughout Europe. Montessori was a positivist scientist by training and the social science she turned to first to help her solve the social problems she observed around her was anthropology. Anthropology, as it was studied in Italy at the time, used physiological measurements as the means for studying human variation. Ironically, a leading figure in this field, drawing on body measurement data, had earlier tried to prevent Montessori from enrolling in medicine, because it was ‘against nature’ for a woman to undertake such study. To override this opposition, Maria Montessori, so the apocryphal story goes, appealed to the Pope, perhaps realising that the only way to trump science is with religion. On the completion of her medical degree she graduated at the top of her class. The positivist Italian anthropologists of the era used their measurements to come up with complex indices and ratios to identify and compare human types, for example, criminal types, the typology of Northern versus Southern Italians etc. The worrying direction in which this was heading is obvious from our standpoint in history, but many working in the field at the time held great hope that the data they were collecting, and the science they were developing, would help them mitigate, even prevent, social problems. When Montessori reviewed the physiological data relating to the criminal type, however, her medical training told her that these measurements were not innate characteristics of fixed human types but were largely the result of poverty, hunger and ill health. liberty as activity liberty is activity. […] If discipline is founded upon liberty, the discipline itself must necessarily be active. Maria Montessori (Rome, 1909) 13 Montessori merged her expertise in special education with her work in anthropology and began to lecture in a field she called ‘pedagogical anthropology’. In an era of increasing specialisation Montessori’s aim was to develop a science of pedagogy, with the human child as her object of study. Her link with positivist anthropology, however, was propelling her towards a crisis, the same crisis which two decades later Vygotsky (1986 [1934], pp. 13-15) would describe, with reference to the field of psychology, as being marked by ‘incongruity’ between the data and the theories which emerged to fit the data. When Montessori (1964 [1909/1912], pp. 14-15) finally rejected positivism, she cited the way body measurements were used to create desks and benches designed to force children to sit still in class, to become, in her words, ‘like butterflies mounted on pins’. ‘[W]ith such material as this’ she argued, ‘the experimental scientist can do nothing’. Observing immobile children to collect pedagogic data was equivalent in her mind to observing mounted butterflies to collect entomological data. For this reason, Montessori (1964 [1909/1912], p. 28) changed her object of study to the ‘spontaneous manifestations of the child’s nature’. This of course foreshadows the way Vygotsky would later address the crisis he perceived in psychology by using ‘ socially laden activity’ as the focus of his experiments. While Montessori certainly aimed to make schools far more humane than was common at the time, her reasons for insisting children should be given the freedom to engage in spontaneous activity were, thus, methodological, rather than an expression of the tradition associated with Rousseau. In 1907, the year Saussure delivered his lectures in Geneva, Maria Montessori opened her first school in Rome at the request of authorities who wanted to corral street children running wild and destroying buildings. In a few months the children became self-disciplined, had learned everyday activities such as washing, dressing, cleaning up, and had even begun to learn educational knowledge, such as writing, reading and mathematics, yet they were completely free. As a result of this experiment, duplicated in other schools, Montessori became an international household name. 13 Étienne Bonnot de Condillac (1715-1780) liberty through signs o visitor to Pereira’s school for the deaf o disciple of John Locke: ‘understanding’/consciousness not innate but learnt through experience via senses (empiricism) o modelled development of consciousness in terms of relations between sensory impressions, language, thought and knowledge (sensationalism) 14 Montessori achieved this apparent miracle by returning to the Enlightenment tradition which had been the source of her inspiration when teaching ‘defective’ children. This was the tradition, not of Rousseau, but of a contemporary, and we are told, a dinner companion of Rousseau, Diderot and the other philosophes in Paris at the time. He was an Abbé, Étienne Bonnot de Condillac. One of the major concerns of the group of philosophers to which Condillac belonged was the question of what it meant to be human and what the process of becoming human involved. Clues to the answers to these questions, they believed, were found in a Parisian school where deaf mutes at that time were successfully taught to speak by a teacher called Jacob Pereira. Because deaf mutes had no language, they were considered to be less than human. Pereira’s method was interactive and sensory-based. He is said to have, ‘practically made his pupils hear through the skin, and utter exactly what they so heard’. He also used grammar categories to teach what he called the force of different words, or as we might say, their function in meaning-making. His students became very prominent in Parisian society and letters. To contemporary observers, including Condillac, Rousseau and Diderot, teaching deaf mutes to speak gave them their humanity. Combining what he observed at the school with his interpretation of John Locke’s empiricism, Condillac argued that the senses are the doorways to the human mind, but movement through the doorway of sensory experience to the functions of the mind can only be achieved by using signs. This is because signs free humans from dependence on immediate sensory and temporal experience and give them control of their mental functions. The use of signs, according to Condillac, develops and refines attention and memory, and makes it possible to reorganise remembered experience in imaginative ways. Thus, the liberty of Condillac is the liberty endowed by the intentional use of socially-shared signs, an idea which is missing in the liberty Rousseau allows Émile. 14 knowledge structures as systems (Condillac) Condillac) a single principle successfully expanded 15 Condillac was also interested in systems, including bodies of knowledge. He described ‘a body of knowledge’ as a system with ‘mutually dependent parts, derived from a single principle successfully expanded’ (quoted in Knight 1968, p. 52). Such a system involves the data of empirical experience being organised and explained in conventional ways using language. The key to education for Condillac was gaining the learner’s attention, in playful ways adapted to the child’s level of development, then using signs in shared, social settings to control the child’s attention. The child’s experience is mediated through the senses and expressed as signs. Condillac applied his ideas in a curriculum he designed for the grandson of Louis XV, the Prince of Parma, to whom he was tutor for many years from the time the child was very young. In 1914 a contemporary and influential critic of Montessori, Boyd (1914, p. 45) noted, with great disapproval, that ‘Condillac attributed the whole of man’s ideas to social intercourse, ...’, and, for this reason, he argued, it was Rousseau who should be used as inspiration for new education in the twentieth century. Rousseau’s child, however, is left alone to make his choices in isolation based on his own impulsive response to experience. The child’s development is shaped by ‘sentiment’ (emotion), from within. For Condillac liberty is a function of social interaction and social conventions, represented in language, conventions which emerge from the distinctions made in the contexts of every day life. Thus, the development of Condillac’s child is shaped by social life, from outside. Condillac’s view of human development inspired the work of the two educators who would had the strongest influence on Maria Montessori, the nineteenth century doctors Jean Itard and Edouard Séguin. 15 Jean-Marc Gaspard Itard (1735-1838) sensory mediation o teacher of Victor, the wild boy of Averyon o identified Victor’s low level of functioning as the result of ‘cultural loss o playful activity involving repetition and imitation: natural life social life intellectual life 16 Jean Itard is famous as the teacher of Victor, the wild boy of Aveyron, a boy abandoned in a forest at about the age of four and found at about age twelve. Itard was a young doctor who rejected the assessment of others that Victor was organically impaired. He argued that the child suffered from ‘cultural loss’ and that education could redress this loss. When found, the child displayed limited abilities related to elementary functions, including movement, perception and attention. He could not, for example, distinguish hot from cold. He did respond to the sound of a walnut cracked behind him, this sound meant food, but not to the sound of pistol, which had no meaning for him. Using Condillac’s ideas, Itard prepared an environment that would meet the child’s wants, needs and interests and encourage him to develop new ones. Itard wrote: ‘Whenever his wants are concerned, his attention, his memory, and his intelligence seemed to raise him above himself’, an early foreshadowing of Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development extending beyond a learner’s present level of functioning. Itard prepared Victor’s environment, in Harlan Lane’s words, by ‘fabricating things, sights, and sounds to serve as vehicles of instruction’ (Lane 1976, p. 95). First, Itard worked to awaken the child’s senses, playfully using activities of daily life which he said ‘lead the mind to a habit of attention, by exposing the senses to the reception of the most lively impressions’ (Itard 1972 [1801], p. 106). Itard used sensory means to turn the child’s attention to differences in the environment, differences salient to the child’s needs and wants, and therefore meaningful to the child. Through sensory mediation Itard expanded the range of meaningful differences to differences which were also salient and significant to those who shared the child’s social context. He progressively drew attention to finer and finer contrasts, comparisons and distinctions eg from the contrast between the sounds of a drum and a bell to the distinctions between different phonemes. sensory mediation o sensory mediation: a first step towards mediation through signs (following Condillac) Was Victor’s potential limited because during critical periods the appropriate social context was missing? 17 The sensory mediation of experience, based on contrasting sensory perceptions in a closely-shared social context, opened up a pathway for Victor which led to the mediation of experience through signs. In this way Victor’s free and spontaneous activity was channelled towards the intentional control of attention, memory and imagination gained through sign-use. As signs took over, Victor let go the sensory cues. Without external order Victor became distressed and disoriented, so Itard drew outlines to help the child order the objects in his room on hooks. Itard later used the same technique to teach Victor geometric shapes and the letters of the alphabet. Then he devised a series of activities based on grammar categories to teach Victor to read and write. The outlines Itard drew for the objects Victor wanted to put in order were a memory aid which Victor was able to use once the number of objects became too many to order from memory. The grammatical categories Itard used to build Victor’s repertoire of signs are analogous, at a higher level of abstraction, to the outlines beside the hooks. In other words, grammatical categories were used by Itard as semantic outlines which could function as a type of memory aid to assist with the ordering of meaning. Montessori, building on the work of Itard’s student Séguin, elaborated and extended both the sensory matching of shapes to outlines and the use of grammatical categories in the teaching of reading. While Victor made extraordinary developmental steps under Itard’s tutelage, he never gained control of language and other social conventions to the extent considered normal for a child of his age. Itard reasoned that during his time in the forest the child had passed through critical developmental periods isolated from society, and therefore, was unable to exploit the developmental opportunities these periods provided. The child could never completely make up for the resulting ‘cultural loss’. Maria Montessori elaborated Itard’s conclusions into a key concept in her teaching methodology, the concept of the ‘sensitive period’, periods of heightened sensitivity and interest during which small children are able to make social and cultural advances, for example, learning language, unconsciously and with relative ease. After these periods have passed, the individual must acquire the same ability consciously, with greater difficulty and less perfectly, for example, the learning of second language in adulthood. Sensitive periods are used in Montessori pedagogy as a guide for teachers when matching activities to a child’s potential. Vygotsky (1986 [1934], p. 189) cites Montessori’s concept of the sensitive period as the starting point for his more theoretically developed description of the zone of proximal development. Itard’s account of Victor’s education, it could be argued, might provide more valuable reading in teacher training courses than the ubiquitous story of the imaginary Émile. Edouard Séguin (1812-1880) physiological pedagogy o collaborated with Itard o developed a sensory-based method to cure the social isolation and ‘intellectual deafness’ of children with developmental delay o ‘contrast is power’ o senses can be artificially augmented through physical and semiotic means 18 Late in his life Itard collaborated with Edouard Séguin, who elaborated Itard’s ideas to create schools for educating children who in those days were labelled ‘idiots’. The first step in Seguin’s method was to design materials and games the children could use to compare differences between sensory perceptions, to polarise their attention, first in terms of contrast and second in terms of similarity. He called the many games he designed a ‘language of action’, a sensory gateway to abstract meaning-making. For example, using active games in the footsteps of Itard, Séguin introduced the children in his care to grammatical categories they could use as semantic outlines, memory aids to support first steps in reading. These activities became the prototype of the Montessori grammar-based reading lessons. Most of all Séguin considered that teaching ‘idiot’ children involved training the will as a means of leading the children towards social life. “From collecting the sparse powers of muscles and nerves disconnected by the absence of will, he wrote, to the gathering of the faculties in the act of thinking, our progress has been a constant ascension on the steps leading from isolation to sociability” Séguin (1971 [1866], p. 209). Séguin (1971 [1866], pp. 198-200) described the foundation principle underpinning his pedagogy in the following way.: ... the law of evolution of the function of the senses ending in intellectual faculty [in which] [p]erception producing simple notion, faculty producing ideas more and more complex and abstract, are the extreme terms of the chain ... Seguin was an instrument maker, and believed that the senses can be artificially augmented through physical and semiotic means to overcome biological, spatial and temporal limits. While, for example, spectacles, telescopes and microscopes extend the capacity of the visual sense beyond the limits of impairment or the immediate visual field, mathematics, navigation and measurement are instruments which mediate and extend perception using the culture’s semiotic systems. 18 Séguin’ Séguin’s keystone o training of the will: - collaborative activity - control of movement and the mind o ensembles of learning resources - multimodal - redundant 19 It was Séguin’s method that Montessori had used so successfully in the 1890s to teach ‘deficient’ children at the orthophrenic institute in Rome. In 1907 when she applied the same method to the education of street children, she was amazed by what unfolded. Séguin’s approach, and Montessori’s interpretation, were judged by Montessori’s contemporaries working in the tradition of Rousseau, including followers of Dewey, as atomistic and lacking in creative and intellectual freedom. Within a couple of decades, however, Vygotsky (1993 [1932], p. 218), wrote of the ‘the profound intuition of Segen’s ... that the source of idiocy is solitude’. Vygotsky (1993 [1932], p. 218) continued: “The developmental path for a severely retarded child lies through collaborative activity, the social help of another human being, who from the first is his mind, his will, his activities. This proposition also corresponds entirely with the normal path of development for a child.” The ‘keystone’ in Séguin’s approach, as identified by Vygotsky (1993 [1935], p. 220), is ‘free will’. Like all higher functions, Vygotsky argues, if free will ‘is impossible on the level of individual development, it becomes possible on the level of social development’. To teach educational knowledge, Séguin combined concrete objects which embodied the knowledge to be taught, with a naming lesson, so the meanings were represented in multiple ways. The effect is to present the child with redundant expressions of the same meaning in ensembles of learning resources. Redundancy is a indexical feature of Montessori pedagogy inherited from Séguin. “This juxtaposition or even identification of the three, four, or five forms of things, i.e., their name written, printed, and pronounced, their images printed and carved, and their own selves in substance, such are the forcible instruments by which the first ideas may be forced through the senses into the mind” (Séguin 1971 [1866]). The naming lesson, called the three period lesson of Seguin, is still used in Montessori schools throughout the world today. 19 Ensembles of developmental resources • The term ensemble was used by Séguin (1971 [1866], p. 291) to describe the sets of educational resources which became the prototypes for the Montessori objects. • The term ensemble is used by Butt (2004, pp. 227-228), from a social semiotic perspective, to describe the semiotic resources teachers use to help students move from ‘local action towards human culture in general’. 20 The ensembles of learning resources and instructional sequences of Séguin’s method, elaborated in Montessori pedagogy, are located in the domain of microgenesis. These ensembles embody, in multimodal form, the description by Butt (2004, p. 236) of the teacher’s threefold task, that is, displacing ‘local representations’, disturbing what is perceived to be self-evident and commonsense, and trying ‘to reconcile ... abstraction with common, recognizable, experience’, a task which results in the ‘semantic complexity’ of pedagogic discourse. In Montessori pedagogy this complexity finds its expression in the design of the objects, the use of the objects and the accompanying language use. A key element of the ensembles is the deliberate representation of concepts in a form which captures students’ attention. This type of representation is identified by Butt (2004, p. 231) as a critical abstraction, which can be ‘recontextualised in a number of intellectual contexts and still retain its degree of fit’. In Butt’s account a critical abstraction represented verbally is typically a definition, but, when more than one mode is used, as in Montessori ensembles, the representation gains depth and power. Furthermore, a ‘cross-calibration’ of multiple modes of representation, Butt (2004, p. 233) argues, increases the potential for insight. A critical abstraction is described by Butt as having a two-way orientation: - external: abstract concept naturalised as recognisable local/commonsense knowledge - internal: commonsense knowledge ‘denaturalised’ to reveal ‘the conventional arrangements which underpin our intellectual tools’ Semiotic modes available to teachers to design critical abstractions include ‘verbal, iconic, numeric, indexical, kinaesthetic, geometric and musical’. 20 The Montessori Curriculum Prepared Paths to Culture Foundation (3 – 6 years) Exercises of Practical Life o the social and manual knowledge of the home and the workshop recontextualised into routinised activity sequences designed to develop conscious and voluntary control of attention and movement (lessons in grace and courtesy and practical life, extended into expressive knowledge of the creative arts) Exercises of the Senses o everyday empirical knowledge recontextualised into sets of objects which materialise this knowledge in the form of hierarchical, taxonomic systems, foreshadowing the systematic organisation of educational knowledge 21 Montessori pedagogy comprises an array of ensembles (objects, movement, language) systematised into a web of detailed instructional pathways known as Prepared Paths to Culture. These pathways are initiated by modelling to children everyday social, manual and expressive skills, and lead to the mastery of systematic, hierarchicallyorganised academic knowledge. The pathways are materialised in the classroom environment because each object, or set of objects, has a specified location on a shelf, the physical location reflecting its location in the child’s progression through the curriculum. The pathways originate in activities designed for infants and culminate in activities designed for twelve year old children. The web of pathways fans out into the various disciplines of educational knowledge from the starting point of two foundation areas of the Montessori curriculum located in the Children’s House environment for preschool age children. The progression from concrete experience to abstract principles is generalised across all areas of the Montessori curriculum. Alongside this progression, control of the regulative discourse of Montessori pedagogy is progressively handed over from teacher to child as the child’s knowledge and self-mastery increases. Everyday knowledge as preparation for interaction with educational knowledge. 21 Trivium (linguistic knowledge) Grammar (noun – verb) Rhetoric (question – response) Logic (meaning represented as geometry) 22 Condillac described ‘a body of knowledge’ as a system with ‘mutually dependent parts, derived from a single principle successfully expanded’. Identifying and embodying this single principle and then expanding it is the design principle at the heart of Montessori’s approach to recontextualising knowledge for young children. The organisation of knowledge in a Montessori classroom echoes the medieval European distinction between linguistic knowledge (the Trivium) and abstract knowledge of the physical world and mathematics (the Quadrivium). This organisation is described by Bernstein (2000, pp. 8-9) as the source of a specific European consciousness, a consciousness which pervades the discourse of Montessori pedagogy. Vygotsky’s discussion of scientific, or academic, concepts has its foundation in the same consciousness. Montessori considered knowledge of the Trivium and the Quadrivium to be an inheritance, a patrimonio in Italian, in which all children were entitled to share. This slide reveals the single contrast, or principle, used as the basis of the Montessori recontextualisation of the subjects which make up the Trivium. Montessori seems to have inherited the Enlightenment view of geometry as the ideal science. Is Geometry the ideal because it has the most hierarchical of knowledge structures? In Montessori pedagogy geometry links the logic of Trivium with the Quadrivium. 22 Quadrivium (abstract, scientific knowledge) Mathematics (base 10; the decimal system) Geometry (solid-surface-line-point; triangle-square- circle) Music (rhythm-pitch) Astronomy (all-nothing; star-planet; time-space) expanded to physics and chemistry (solid – liquid - gas) geography (land – water - air) biology (timeline of the evolution of life; animal-plant; vital functions of living things) history (timelines of humans and civilisations; fundamental human needs [spiritual-material] across time and space) 23 On this slide can be seen the foundation contrast, or principle, on which is based the Montessori recontextualisation of each of the subjects of the Quadrivium. Interestingly, in the Montessori curriculum, Astronomy is expanded to encompass the sciences and social sciences. The child’s liberty in a Montessori classroom is not as an expression of unmediated innate drives, but is the freedom gained by interacting, in developmentally appropriate ways, with communally-shared socio-cultural meanings. This is achieved by providing young children with objects they can use as external mediational means, objects which capture and regulate attention. I want to conclude with some illustrative examples of how this is done. 23 The Folding Cloths an exercise of practical life The parts of the folding cloths (shape, corners, colour, texture, embroidered line) A folding cloth dynamic ‘figure’ 24 The folding cloths are a Montessori exercise of practical life. It comprises four square cloths on which straight lines are embroidered. One cloth has one line parallel to two sides of the cloth and bisecting the adjacent sides. A second cloth has one embroidered diagonal. A third cloth has perpendicular lines bisecting the sides and intersecting in the centre of the cloth. A fourth cloth has both diagonals embroidered onto the cloth. The purpose of the exercise is to use embroidered lines as a guide to folding each cloth. Interpersonally the cloths, like all Montessori objects, can be thought of as an offer inviting the child to act. Experientially, a folding cloth and a hand movement combine dynamically into a figure, a dynamic quantum of ‘experiential change’ (Halliday 2004, p. 169). The elements of the figure are the cloths, the hands and the movement. The parts of the folding cloth with one embroidered diagonal line are shown in the illustration. During the folding exercises, the figures are tied together in a dynamic sequence, an activity sequence, to use Martin’s term, each figure in the sequence derived from taxonomies of ‘actions, people, places, things and qualities’ (Martin 1992, p. 292). The activity sequence in the folding cloths ensemble concludes with a synoptic final array of all the folded cloths, again typical of all Montessori ensembles. In the array the folded cloths are transformed into static elements related on the basis of comparative value (large, small) and class (rectangle, square, triangle). These final synoptic arrays so characteristic of the conclusion of Montessori ensembles can be thought of as entry-level knowledge systems. 24 Plane Geometry folding cloths from 2 ½ to 4 years Geometry Cabinet from 3 to 7 years Geometry Sticks from 7 to 9 years 25 The folding cloths, an exercise of practical life, represents a first step on a prepared Montessori pathway way to culture. Further steps in this pathway are illustrated on the slide, representing a developmental passage from concrete representation to abstract knowledge from the ages of about two and a half years to nine years. Children aged from about three to six work with the geometry cabinet, and accompanying cards, an exercise of the senses. The cabinet comprises several drawers in which a taxonomy of plane geometric shapes are materialised as wooden insets and frames. During the exercises the children match insets to frames in increasingly complex combinations. Later they match and grade the insets using increasingly abstract representations on the cards. As they undertake the matching and grading activities, children learn to name the shapes. Once children have entered the early years of schooling, they are introduced to the box of geometry sticks. Work with these sticks enables children to analyse , through concrete manipulation of a series of precisely calibrated, colour-coded sticks, the properties of plane shapes. 25 The Colour Tablets an exercise of the senses First period Second period Third period This is red. This is blue. This is yellow Give me the red (one). Which one is blue? (Can you) show me (which one is) the yellow (one)? Where is the red (one)? Put the blue (one) back in the box. (Can you) find the red one? (Would you like to) find the yellow one? What’s this? Do you remember what this is? Can you tell me what this is? 26 The Montessori exercises of the senses are designed as a culturally-relevant sensory ‘“alphabet” of the outer world’ for young children aged from about three to six. Once they have learnt the ‘alphabet’, children are able to classify their experience using these categories. That small children are interested in classifying their experience in accord with categories used in the cultural context has been demonstrated by Painter (1999, p. 78). In each set of graded objects, one sensory property, or quality, varies from object to object while all the other properties remain constant. The variations are given labels using the three period lesson of Seguin, a lesson which pivots on relational meanings First period: name (indication) Second period: recognise (collaboration) Third period: reproduce (independence) Note the potential for using the semantic feature [prefaced] in the second and third periods. The semantic feature [prefaced], as Hasan (2004) has demonstrated, is a discursive practice of groups of people with higher degrees of social and cultural autonomy. It represents an ‘invisible semiotic mediation’ through which children from these social groups achieve a mental disposition orienting them favourably to school education. In related work, the extensive use of the mental process remember in the prefacing of questions by mothers from higher autonomy social groups asking about their children’s memories of real and storybook events has been linked by Williams (2001) both to the role of memory in a young child’s thinking, as identified by Vygotsky (1978, p. 50), and to the development of the discourse of educational knowledge, or vertical discourse (Bernstein 2000). This type of semiotic mediation appears not to be experienced so frequently in everyday contexts by children from social groups with lower levels of social and cultural autonomy. The three period lesson of Seguin, however, makes this practice available to all children in an accessible form in a welldefined context. 26 From external to internal 27 The three period lesson of Séguin can be considered from three perspectives: - from indication via collaboration to independence. - from pseudo concept to true concept (following Vygotsky) - from signal to sign (following Hasan) When the three period lesson is used with sensory and kinaesthetic representations of educational knowledge, there is potential for powerful slippage between identifying and attributive relations and back again as children adjust the meanings from ‘signals’ to label the external world to true signs, or ‘symbols’ in an internalised meaning system. When a word is used as a ‘signal’, its use is ‘concrete’ and ‘context-bound’, referring, in Hasan’s words, to ‘something that is physically present to the senses’ (Hasan 2005 [1992], p. 81). When a word is used as a symbol, it signifies an internalised meaning, with the potential to refer to phenomena beyond the immediate sensory field. The noteworthy feature of Montessori pedagogy is that, wherever the child’s use of a meaning such as red is located on the developmental pathway from context-dependent signal for a physically present object to abstract, transportable attribute, the system of relational values which gives the abstract meaning its value is also physically present in a synoptic, idealised form, materialised in the array in which the object is located. Thus, the array of colour tablets representing the primary colours renders the values red, blue and yellow as a system of relations. When a child is given a word to signal one value in this materialised system, the child engages with the system’s meaning potential and takes a step towards eventually internalising the system as a whole, and with it the abstract values it carries. The next series of slides offer a brief look at some ensembles for teaching educational knowledge, in this case, grammar, knowledge used in Montessori pedagogy to teach reading. 27 Representing potential Using abstractions to represent abstraction Im Blau Kandinsky, 1925 28 In the 1920s Montessori designed a series of grammar-based reading games to lead children from the decoding of single, isolated, words to making meaning from extended , connected text, or discourse. Children use the symbols on this slide to play the grammar games, called by Montessori Functions of Words. Each symbol represents a different grammatical category (nine parts of speech). These symbols are first presented to children in 3D, each with a story woven around it to give it value. The basic contrast is between the noun (black square-based pyramid representing matter/stability/substantive) and the verb (red sphere representing the sun/energy/centrality). The symbols can be combined to create sub-categories, for example, the black triangle with a blue circle is used to represent abstract nouns (ie blue as in sky and the circle to recall the verbs many of abstract nouns are derived from), and the red circle with a white triangle is used to represent forms of the verb to be, and the work it does linking noun groups. When each word in a text is labelled with a symbol, the grammatical patterns of the text (the grammatical potential the text has emerged from) become visible in multimodal form above the text (an instance of that potential). In summary, Montessori used geometry, an abstract system, to represent in concrete, manipulable form abstract educational knowledge about grammar. Montessori developed this series of games during the 1920s, in the same decade as Kandinsky was creating the painting (see slide) as part of his exploration of abstraction. Montessori described her grammatics as ‘psico-grammar’, indicating that she believed that these knowledge structures had the potential to change consciousness. 28 The Functions of Words Grammatics as reading pedagogy Photo: Montessori East, Bondi NSW (2006) 29 This slide shows a completed synoptic array at the conclusion of a dynamic and active grammar game. This material is used by children in the age range from five to seven years. The games are active, sentence-building exercises, involving manipulable materials to mediate interactive engagement with the wording of the child’s language. 29 Sentence Analysis Exploring clauses in simple, compound and complex sentences 30 From six to eight years children play games with manipulable materials in order to analyse the structure of groups, phrases, clauses and sentences. The shapes and the colour coding of these materials is an extension from that learnt during the Functions of Words games. 30 Towards a science of pedagogy? ... if we showed them how to do something, this precision itself seemed to hold their interest. To have a real purpose to which the action was directed, this was the first condition, but the exact way of doing it acted like a support which rendered the child stable in his efforts, and therefore brought him to make progress in his development. Order and precision, we found, were keys to spontaneous work in the school. Maria Montessori 1949 31 With these words Montessori tells us that the key to regulating both activity and attention is discipline, ie order, precision, exactness in purposeful movement and in recontextualised knowledge structures. In Montessori pedagogy discipline knowledge is as integral to the regulative discourse of the classroom, as it is to the instructional discourse. Recontextualising the detail of educational knowledge into a visible pedagogy in ways which attract children’s interest and attention becomes part of the fabric of the regulative discourse. Drawing on the tradition of Condillac, Montessori made an art form of recontextualisation. The materialised abstractions she designed and integrated into ensembles of developmental resources render discipline knowledge as a regulator of attention, and of consciousness, in a very tangible way, in a way which children can literally grasp. 31 References 1 Bernstein, B. 2000. Pedagogy, symbolic control and identity: Theory, research, critique (Revised edition). Lanham, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers Inc Boyd, W. 1914. From Locke to Montessori: A critical account of the Montessori point of view. London: George G. Harrap & Company. Butt, D. 2004. How our meanings change: School contexts and semantic evolution. In G. Williams and A. Lukin (eds), The Development of language: Functional perspectives on species and individuals. London and New York: Continuum, 217-240. Feez, S. 2007. .Montessori’s mediation of meaning: a social semiotic perspective. Unpublished thesis. University of Sydne. [Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/2123/1859 ] Feez, S. 2008. Multimodal representation of educational meanings in Montessori pedagogy. In L. Unsworth (Ed.) Multimodal semiotics: functional analysis in contexts of education. Continuum. Haines, A. 2004. Maria Montessori: Her life and legacy. DVD. San Luis Obispo, California: Davidson Films. Halliday, M. A. K. H. 2004. An Introduction to Functional Grammar (Third edition). Revised by C.M.I.M., Matthiessen. London: Edward Arnold. Hasan, R. 2004. The world in words: semiotic mediation, tenor and ideology. In G. Williams and A. Lukin (eds) The development of language. London: Continuum, 158-181. Hasan, R. 2005a. Language, society and consciousness: transdisciplinary orientations and the tradition of specialisation. In Language, society and consciousness: The collected works of Ruqaiya Hasan Volume 1. Edited by J. J. Webster. London: Equinox, 3-17. Hasan, R. 2005b. Semiotic mediation and three exotropic theories: Vygotsky, Halliday and Bernstein. In Language, society and consciousness: The collected works of Ruqaiya Hasan Volume 1. Edited by J. J. Webster. London: Equinox, 130-156. Hasan, R. 2005 [1992]. Speech genre, semiotic mediation and the development of higher mental functions. In Language, society and consciousness: The collected works of Ruqaiya Hasan Volume 1. Edited by J. J. Webster. London: Equinox, 68-105. Hasan, R. 2005 [1995]. On the social conditions of semiotic mediation: the genesis of mind in society. In Language, society and consciousness: The collected works of Ruqaiya Hasan Volume 1 Edited by J. J. Webster. London: Equinox, 106-129. Itard, J. 1972 [1801]. On the first developments of the young savage of Averyon (1799, printed 1801). In L. Malson and J. Itard, 1972 [1964], Wolf Children; The Wild Boy of Aveyron. London: NLB, 91-140. 32 32 References 2 Knight, I. F. 1968. The geometric spirit: The Abbé de Condillac and the French Enlightenment. New Haven and London: Yale University Press. Lane, H. 1976. The wild boy of Aveyron. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Martin, J. R. 1992. English text: system and structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Montessori, M. 1946 Education for a New World. Madras, India: Kalakshetra. Montessori, M. 1964 [1909 (Italian)/1912 (English)]. The Montessori Method. New York: Schocken Books. Montessori, M. 1982 [1949]. The Absorbent Mind (Eighth edition). Madras, India: Kalakshetra Publications. Montessori, M. 1998 [1939]. Creative development in the child. Volume Two. Madras, India: Kalakshetra Press. Painter, C. 1999. Learning through Language in Early Childhood. London and New York: Continuum. Séguin, E. 1971 [1866]. Idiocy and its treatment by the physiological method. New York: Augustus M. Kelley. Vygotsky, L. S. 1978. Mind in Society: The development of higher psychological processes. M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner & E. Souberman (eds.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. (A collection of essays written between 1930 and 1935). Vygotsky, L. S. 1986 [1934]. Thought and Language. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. Vygotsky, L. S. 1993 [1932]. Introduction to E. K. Gracheva’s book: The Education and instruction of severely retarded children. In R. W. Rieber and A. S. Carton (eds.), The collected words of L. S. Vygotsky. Volume 2. The Fundamentals of Defectology (Abnormal psychology and learning disabilities). Translated by J. E. Knox and C. B. Stevens, New York and London: Plenum Press, 212-219. Vygotsky, L. and Luria, A. 1994 [1930]. Tool and symbol in child development. In R. van der Veer and J. Valsiner (eds.), The Vygotsky Reader. Oxford UK and Cambridge USA: Blackwell, 99-174. Williams, G. 2001. Literacy pedagogy prior to schooling: Relations between social positioning and semantic variation. In A. Morais, I. Neves, B.Davies and H. Daniels (eds). Towards a sociology of pedagogy: The contribution of Basil Bernstein to research. Oxford and New York: Peter Lang, 17-45. Williams G. 2005. Grammatics in schools. In J. Webster, C. Matthiessen and R. Hasan (eds.), Continuing Discourse on Language: Volume 1. London: Equinox, pp. 281-310. 33 33